You are on page 1of 13

The Journal of Educational Research, 105:404–415, 2012

Copyright 
C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0022-0671 print / 1940-0675 online
DOI:10.1080/00220671.2011.629693

Exploring the Value Added of a Guided,


Silent Reading Intervention: Effects on
Struggling Third-Grade Readers’
Achievement
D. RAY REUTZEL ALEXANDRA N. SPICHTIG
Utah State University Capella University

YAACOV PETSCHER
Florida State University

& Griffin, 1998). Fluent reading is essential because it is


ABSTRACT. The authors’ purpose was to explore the ef-
fects of a supplementary, guided, silent reading intervention the bridge between word recognition and reading compre-
with 80 struggling third-grade readers who were retained at hension processes (National Reading Panel, 2000; Rasinski,
grade level as a result of poor performance on the reading 1989; Reutzel & Hollingsworth, 1993; Samuels & Farstrup,
portion of a criterion referenced state assessment. The stu- 2006). The initial stages of reading fluency occur when stu-
dents were distributed in 11 elementary schools in a large, dents are able to automatically recognize words. Later on,
urban school district in the state of Florida. A matched,
quasi-experimental design was constructed using propensity automatic word recognition opens the potential to achieve
scores for this study. Students in the guided, silent read- reading’s ultimate goal, comprehension (Samuels, 2007;
ing intervention, Reading Plus, evidenced higher, statisti- Torgesen & Hudson, 2006). Topping (2006) described this
cally significant mean scores on the Florida Comprehensive later stage of fluency development when word recognition
Assessment Test criterion assessment measure of reading at bridges comprehension processes as “the extraction of max-
posttest. The effect size, favoring the guided, silent reading
intervention group was large, 1 full standard deviation, when imum meaning at maximum speed in a relatively continu-
comparing the 2 comparison groups’ mean posttest scores. ous flow, leaving spare, simultaneous processing capacity for
As such, the results indicate a large advantage for providing other higher order processes” (p. 107).
struggling third-grade readers guided silent reading fluency In its report, the National Reading Panel (2000) reviewed
practice in a computer-based practice environment. No sig- 77 studies of Guided Repeated Oral Reading (GROR) with
nificant difference was found between the treatment and con-
trol group on the Stanford Achievement Test–10 (SAT-10) Feedback. GROR with Feedback is an approach to reading
posttest scores, although posttest scores for the treatment fluency practice that includes oral, repeated readings of a
group trended higher than the control. After conducting a single grade or instructional level text (usually 3–5) while
power analysis, it was determined that the sample size (n = receiving feedback from a teacher or other more proficient
80) was too small to provide sufficient statistical power to readers. The National Reading Panel (2000) found GROR
detect a difference in third-grade students’ SAT-10 scores.
with Feedback to have substantial scientific evidence to sup-
Keywords: guided silent reading, program evaluation, port its efficacy for increasing students’ reading fluency.
struggling readers On the other hand, the National Reading Panel (2000)
report sparked considerable controversy when the panel
reported a lack of research supporting independent, silent

T eaching students to read has long been described reading practice as an effective means for developing
as the most important responsibility of elemen- students’ reading fluency (e.g., Silent Sustained Reading
tary schools (Boyer, 1995). Reading research has [SSR] or Drop Everything and Read [DEAR]; Allington,
recently produced an emerging consensus around several es- 2002; Coles, 2000; J. W. Cunningham, 2001; Edmondson
sential elements of beginning reading instruction to include
phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, com-
Address correspondence to D. Ray Reutzel, PhD, Emma Ec-
prehension, oral language, writing, concepts about print, cles Jones Center for Early Childhood Education, Utah State Uni-
and letter name knowledge (National Institute for Liter- versity, 2605 UMC, Logan, UT 84322-2605, USA. (E-mail:
acy, 2008; National Reading Panel, 2000; Snow, Burns, ray.reutzel@usu.edu)
The Journal of Educational Research 405

& Shannon, 2002; Krashen, 2002). Thus, one of the vide a causal research base showing that students’ reading
unintended consequences of the National Reading Panel’s comprehension, fluency, and achievement can be benefit-
report was to suppress the previously prevalent use of ted by the opportunity to read independently and silently
silent, independent reading practice to develop students’ when specific conditions of reading fluency practice are im-
reading fluency. Although silent reading practices such plemented. According to recent research it also appears that
as SSR had been generally criticized, sharp critiques of using independent, silent reading as the means to practice
independent, silent reading increased significantly after the fluency makes more sense developmentally and empirically
National Reading Panel’s (2000) report was released. In a for students who are older than age 8 years or at least in
contemporary era of high-stakes accountability, it becomes Grade 2 (Kuhn, 2004, 2005; Wright, Sherman, & Jones,
increasingly difficult for classroom teachers to justify the use 2004, 2010).
of instructional practices that do not have the imprimatur Today’s educational culture of increased accountability
of the National Reading Panel or are not sanctioned as has compounded teacher concerns regarding the use of inde-
evidence based by the federal government. pendent, silent reading to practice fluency, especially when
Consequently since the turn of the new millennia, GROR this method is used with low-achieving, struggling students.
with Feedback has become the dominant way in which A persistent fear among classroom teachers in an age of
teachers encourage students to practice their reading in increasing accountability about using independent, silent
classrooms to develop fluency. Once GROR with Feed- reading to practice reading fluency in classrooms, especially
back became the dominant method of classroom instruction when used with low-achieving, struggling students, is as-
used to develop fluency, it became more and more apparent suring that these students can actually read the books they
to teachers, administrators, and researchers that this par- have selected (Donovan, Smolkin, & Lomax, 2000; Fresch,
ticular mode of providing school reading fluency practice, 1995) and are keeping their eyes on the text (Hiebert,
oral and guided, was unrelated to the most common way Wilson, & Trainin, 2010). Students achieving in the bottom
in which most accomplished adolescent and adult readers quartile of their class have been shown to differ substantially
read—independently and silently. Although guided, oral re- when they read silently in an unguided context as com-
peated reading may be useful in school reading fluency prac- pared with a guided context (Hiebert et al., 2010; Trainen,
tice the long-term goal should be to help students become Wilson, Hiebert, Erickson, & Laughridge, 2007). When the
avid, competent independent silent readers. classroom setting for independent, silent reading fluency
practice lacked sufficient guidance and accountability, strug-
Silent Reading gling readers often failed to read. However, as reported in
several recent studies of independent silent reading, when
There is little question that the opportunity to read the challenge level of texts and the task of reading indepen-
whether silent or oral has been shown to be strongly associ- dently and silently were carefully scaffolded and guided by
ated with gains in students’ reading achievement (Allington, the teacher, even third-grade struggling readers were able to
2002; Guthrie, Schafer, & Huang, 2001; National Reading engage successfully (Bryan, Fawson, & Reutzel, 2003; Kamil,
Panel, 2000). In addition to the strong correlation evidence 2008; Kelley & Clausen-Grace, 2006, 2010; Reutzel, Fawson,
for time spent reading and reading achievement, causal evi- & Smith, 2008; Reutzel, Jones, et al., 2008; Reutzel et al.,
dence for the efficacy of engaging students in independent, 2010).
silent reading practice has been steadily growing over the Along with research on improving the outcomes of in-
past ten years since the publication of the report of the dependent, silent reading practice, over the past decade re-
National Reading Panel (2000). searchers also have been pushing forward on the scientific
In a large-scale experiment that provided students with frontiers of silent reading in other areas over the past decade.
an additional 20 min of independent, silent reading of trade Brenner and Hiebert (2010) recently reported research re-
books Block, Cleveland, and Reed (2006) found that the ad- lated to a professional development program intended to
ditional practice led to significant annual gains in students’ help teachers increase the amount of time students’ eyes
vocabulary, comprehension, and fluency. Similarly, Wu and were on text during silent reading. These researchers, among
Samuels’s (2004) reported results of a quasi-experiment that others who have recently examined the independent, silent
compared the reading comprehension and reading achieve- reading process, discovered that previous explanations of the
ment gains of third- and fifth-grade students who read in- eyes on text phenomenon had seemingly overlooked a fun-
dependently and silently for either 15 or 40 min. Students damental contributor to that process—the eyes (Samuels,
in the 40-min group evidenced significantly better reading Hiebert, & Rasinski, 2010).
achievement and comprehension than the group that read Rasinski, Samuels, Hiebert, Petscher, and Feller (2011)
for 15 min. These and other studies over the past decade (e.g., recently reported on research conducted with students in
Hiebert & Reutzel, 2010; Kamil, 2008; Kelley & Clausen- Grades 4–10 using a computer-based, guided silent read-
Grace, 2006, 2010; Kuhn & Schwanenflugel, 2008; Reutzel, ing fluency intervention known as Reading Plus. This in-
Fawson, & Smith, 2008; Reutzel, Jones, Fawson, & Smith, tervention makes use of infrared eye-movement photogra-
2008; Reutzel, Jones, & Newman, 2010) have begun to pro- phy assessment, placement tests, comprehension assessment,
406 The Journal of Educational Research

and computer-adapted levels of reading selections across a pling and matching process. A propensity score, as defined by
variety of genres to guide, monitor, and adjust the silent Rosenbaum and Rubin (1983), has a conditional probability
reading practice of school-aged students. Students’ initial of assignment to a particular treatment given a vector of ob-
placement and progressive reading levels are based on ongo- served covariates. More simplistically, a propensity score is
ing computerized feedback as students are provided with the probability of being in the treatment group derived from
visual and perceptual modeling practice through reading a logistic regression when accounting for important match-
texts that systematically increase in difficulty and length. ing variables. The primary objective for the researcher is
In their recent study, Rasinski et al. found a strong relation- to select a series of variables that would be considered to
ship between Grades 4 and 10 students who practiced silent be important for matching students. In traditional reading
reading using this intervention and subsequent gains in read- research, these variables might include race/ethnicity, so-
ing comprehension and general literacy achievement on a cioeconomic status, English learner status, primary excep-
state criterion- and normative-referenced national reading tionality status, gender, and some type of baseline measure
achievement test. of achievement (e.g., pretest). The main effects and inter-
Although these researchers found a relationship between actions among these and other variables would be included
the guided, silent reading practice with the Reading Plus pro- in a logistic regression to determine the probability of be-
gram of students in Grades 4–10 and the students’ gains in ing in the treatment when controlling for these important
reading comprehension and general reading achievement, matching covariates (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002).
much less is known about how such a supplementary inter- The resulting probabilities from the logistic regression may
vention program may influence the reading behaviors and then be used to match students who actually received the
achievement of struggling, younger readers. In today’s en- intervention with those who did not creating matched treat-
vironment of high accountability, there is a need for care- ment and control groups. In this way, students are more
fully constructed evaluations of commercially available sup- probabilistically matched at the pretest, and doing so allows
plementary intervention programs by credible organizations for stronger causal inferences regarding differences on the
such as the U.S. Department of Education’s What Works posttest or on gain scores than a simple comparison of all
Clearinghouse. Thus, studies such as the one reported by available students in a sample. There are several limitations
Rasinski et al. (2011) are of evaluative and practical im- that should be noted in regard to using propensity scores to
portance for classroom teachers and administrators who are construct an experimental sample such as that used in this
seeking guidance and evidence to support the selection study: (a) propensity scores tend to be most practically used
and use of available supplementary reading interventions with larger samples, (b) missing data can be problematic for
in classrooms. propensity analyses as the techniques are still relatively new,
Although previous studies have shown silent reading to and (c) propensity scores assume that no further confounds
be an effective way to read, more recent studies have shown exist that may predict the propensity. Nevertheless, despite
that silent reading practice conditions in school often re- these acknowledged limitations, propensity scores are now
sult in students acting like they are reading when they are viewed as one of the strongest quasi-experimental methods
not (Hiebert & Reutzel, 2010). As a result, we sought to for assessing relationships between treatments and outcomes
determine whether a computer-based, guided silent read- (Shadish et al., 2002).
ing fluency intervention using a combination of scaffolded
reading passages and comprehension questions can reliably Participants
increase struggling students’ reading achievement and com-
prehension by helping these readers keep their eyes on the Three criteria were used to select students for the con-
text during silent reading. trol and treatment group sample. First, students who were
selected were age 9 years or older. Second, students were
identified as being struggling readers by the scores from their
Research Question
end-of-year high-stakes assessments. The results from these
How does a guided silent reading fluency intervention, third-grade achievement tests put the students at risk for
Reading Plus, affect struggling third-grade students’ perfor- not being promoted to Grade 4 and the end of the Grade
mance on criterion- and norm-referenced tests of reading 3 school year. Finally, retained third-grade students were
comprehension and reading achievement? re-enrolled in Grade 3 classrooms during the implementa-
tion period, which ran from the beginning of the school
your through the administration of the Florida Comprehen-
Method sive Assessment Test (FCAT) and Stanford Achievement
Research Design Test–10 (SAT-10) in early March.
We selected struggling third-grade readers for participa-
We used a matched, quasi-experimental research de- tion in this study for two reasons. First, age 9 years (or Grade
sign. The study’s quasi-experimental control and treatment 3) has been shown by empirical studies to be an age and stage
groups were constructed by the use of a propensity score sam- of reading development where independent, silent reading
The Journal of Educational Research 407

becomes possible and advisable based on recent research recently received strong support in an empirical analysis of
findings (Hiebert & Reutzel, 2010). Second, the stage in its relationships with a variety of other reading comprehen-
reading development as third-grade students transition into sion, language, and basic reading measures (Schatschneider,
Grade 4 has been long associated with the Grade 4 read- Fletcher, Francis, Carlson, & Foorman, 2004).
ing slump or reading crisis (Chall, 2001; Chall, Jacobs, & The SAT-10 is approved for use by the U.S. Department
Baldwin, 1990). of Education and is constructed to determine if students are
The archival full student sample file available to re- meeting national or state standards in reading, mathemat-
searchers was 1,253 third-grade students enrolled in a large, ics, and language. The SAT-10 provides forms acceptable
urban public school system in the state of Florida. Of these for assessing students’ progress toward meeting state and na-
1,253 third-grade students, 158 represented the required spe- tional standards in Grades K–12. The reading section of the
cial case of 9-year-old students retained at the end of Grade 3. SAT-10 has an alpha reliability coefficient of .87, the math-
Thus, all 158 third-grade students in the study’s sample pop- ematics section .80–.87, and the language section .78–.84.
ulation were not promoted to Grade 4 based on their per- Alternate forms reliability coefficients ranged in the low
formance on Florida’s end-of-year high-stakes assessment, .90s for the total reading section. The SAT-10 by design
the FCAT. These 158 retained third-grade struggling read- evidences content and criterion related validity because its
ers attended 11 different elementary schools within this development is tied very closely to assessing progress toward
Florida urban school district. The final propensity score sam- meeting state and national standards in reading, mathemat-
ple constructed for this study’s matched, quasi-experimental ics, and language (Berk, 1998; Morse, 2005; Spies & Plake,
research design consisted of 40 students in the control and 2005).
40 students in the experimental treatment group with a total
number of 80 retained third-grade students. Control and Treatment Groups

Instrumentation All 80 retained third-grade students in the control


(n = 40) and treatment (n = 40) groups followed the state-
The FCAT is a major component of Florida’s test- approved Comprehensive Core Reading Program (CCRP)
ing effort to assess student achievement in reading, writ- adopted by this large, urban Florida school district. The
ing, mathematics, and science as represented in Florida’s CCRP delineated specific protocols unique to the third-
Sunshine State Standards (SSS; Florida Department of grade retained students, requiring schools to provide a
Education, 2001). The SSS reading portion of the FCAT dedicated and uninterrupted 2-hr block of classroom instruc-
is a group-administered, criterion-referenced test consisting tional time for reading instruction for all students. Whole-
of six to eight narrative or informational reading passages, group explicit reading instruction was provided daily for the
where students respond to between six and 11 multiple- first 30–40 min using Houghton Mifflin’s Reading Treasures
choice items per passage. Embedded within these six to 11 (Cooper & Pikulski, 2006) comprehensive core reading pro-
multiple choice questions are four content clusters: (a) ref- gram. Thirty minutes of the 2-hr block were dedicated to
erence and research, (b) words and phrases in context, (c) writing instruction. For the remainder of the 2-hr reading
the main idea, and (d) comparison and cause and effect. instructional block teachers differentiated instruction using
Based on students’ scores, they are placed into one of five small groups and center rotations. This time was used for
performance levels on a scaled score ranging from 100 to students to practice, demonstrate, and extend skills previ-
500. Levels 1 and 2 reflect below grade-level performance in ously taught during the teacher-led explicit reading instruc-
reading, with Level 1 being the lowest indication of reading tion. Approved supplemental reading intervention programs
performance. Levels 3 and higher represent proficiency in could be used at this time. Some of these included Quick-
reading comprehension at or above grade-level standards. Reads (repeated oral reading of the same passage), Elements of
Students who score below Level 1 proficiency on the FCAT Reading: Vocabulary (oral vocabulary instructional program),
in Grade 3 must be retained for another year according to and the supplemental activities provided with Houghton Mif-
Florida law. If they can demonstrate the required reading flin’s Reading Treasures. Retained students were required to
level or proficiency through an approved alternate test (the receive intensive intervention in areas of demonstrated defi-
SAT) or through a student portfolio they can be granted a ciency during the mandated 2-hr reading instructional block.
good cause exemption and be promoted to Grade 4. Thus, In addition to the dedicated 2-hr block instructional time
the students selected into this study represented the highest that all control and treatment students received, the par-
risk segment of the overall Grade 3 population. The internal ticipating students received an additional 30 min of supple-
consistency reliability for the FCAT-SSS has been shown to mental reading instruction every day. Supplemental read-
be .90 (Cronbach’s alpha); moreover, test score content and ing programs included Soar to Success, Essential Elements of
concurrent validity have been established through a series of Reading: Vocabulary, Voyager Passport, Earobics, and Read-
expert panel reviews and data analyses (Florida Department ing Plus. Treatment group students engaged in Reading Plus
of Education, 2001). The construct validity of the FCAT- while the control group used one of the other three supple-
SSS as a comprehensive assessment of reading outcomes mentary reading interventions for third-grade students. The
408 The Journal of Educational Research

programs differed in that Soar to Success contained instruc- Part 2 is designed for second-grade students and older who
tion in four of the essential components of reading outlined are struggling with fluency. Students may engage in software
by the National Reading Panel and Reading First (phon- games that target phonemic awareness and phonics skills or
ics, fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension) and Passport teachers may provide explicit instruction in language enrich-
Voyager and Earobics in five of the components (phone- ment, phonemic awareness, letter–sound correspondences,
mic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, and compre- decoding, and early reading and writing.
hension) while treatment intervention focused primarily on
fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension development. Guided Silent Reading Fluency Intervention
(Treatment Group)
Reading Acceleration Programs (Control Group)
An equal number of selected struggling third-grade stu-
Selected struggling third-grade students (n = 40) received dents (n = 40) received the comparison treatment, Reading
one of three accelerated reading treatments during three Plus, a guided, silent reading supplementary intervention.
30-min sessions per week. Other programs, utilized by the Students involved in this guided, silent reading interven-
control groups, were implemented according to publisher tion participated in a series of online, computer-based ses-
guidelines. sions that included a specific sequence of daily activities. As
struggling students participated in this guided, silent read-
Soar to Success. Soar to Success is a program designed ing intervention, the difficulty level of the reading materials
to accelerate students’ reading ability and help them was adjusted as a function of a student’s progress based on
quickly and easily apply comprehension and decoding strate- reading comprehension and reading rate analyses. Students
gies to other content area texts through the use of re- began the intervention by completing a reading assessment
ciprocal teaching; an instructional technique that uses (Reading Placement Appraisal [RPA]) to establish the ini-
teacher–student dialogue to teach students to use cog- tial placement level within the guided, silent reading supple-
nitive strategies of summarizing, clarifying, questioning, mentary intervention program. This 20-min placement test
and predicting. Each 30-min lesson consists of five parts: assessed independent reading level, rate, comprehension,
(a) revisiting—students reread self-selected Soar to Success and vocabulary to determine the most appropriate practice
books for fluency development, (b) reviewing—students re- starting level. The RPA consisted of three parts. Part I pre-
view strategies and summarize what was read utilizing graphic sented students with several 100-word selections that each
organizers, (c) rehearsing—students follow a teacher-guided were followed by a set of literal recall questions. Content dif-
or independent preview of the daily reading, (d) reading ficulty was adjusted according to a student’s comprehension
and reciprocal teaching—students read silently followed performance and reading rate mastery to ascertain a stu-
by the use of four reciprocal teaching strategies (sum- dent’s tentative independent reading level. Part II with its
marizing, clarification, questioning, and predicting), and 300-word selections and diverse comprehension questions
(e) responding–reflecting—students complete written re- served to confirm the independent reading level. Part III as-
flections and engage in discussions to bring closure to the sessed a student’s vocabulary level. From the three-part RPA
daily activity (FCRR, n.d.). assessment, an instructional reading level was established for
individual students, and they were then placed at appropri-
ate levels of reading challenge within each component of
Essential Elements of Reading: Vocabulary, Voyager the program. Students continued to be assessed on similar
Passport. Essential Elements of Reading: Vocabulary, Voy- tasks throughout the intervention period with appropriate
ager Passport is a program designed to accelerate reading adjustments made to the level of reading selections as a re-
growth and assist students in reaching grade-level expec- sult of their performances on these formative assessments.
tations through the use of teacher modeling, guided and As students participated in this supplementary silent read-
independent practice, and immediate corrective feedback. ing fluency intervention, they were provided reading lessons
The program consists of daily lessons that are taught in small and continuous feedback about their silent reading in an
groups. A typical 30-min lesson for third-grade students con- individual computer-based, online environment.
sists of advanced vocabulary word analysis, fluency-building Each lesson began with a perceptual accuracy and visual
passage reading, and comprehension strategies. Third-grade efficiency (PAVE) warm-up. This activity consisted of two
students in need of additional support in word study may en- parts, scan and flash. In the scan activity, students scanned
gage in an optional targeted word study component (FCRR, the computer screen to count the number of times a tar-
n.d.). get letter or number appeared on the screen. The target and
other letters or numbers were flashed in a left-to-right presen-
Earobics. Earobics is a program designed to help students tation. The presentation speed increased in accordance with
develop foundational skills to become successful readers the student’s proficiency. In the second activity, flash, a series
though the use of software, teacher-directed activities, ma- of letters or numbers ranging in length from two to 12 de-
nipulatives, and books. The program consists of two parts: pending on the students’ placement level were flashed (one
Part 1 is designed for first- and second-grade students and sixth of a second per flash, which does not permit moving
The Journal of Educational Research 409

of the eyes and thus provides single fixation training). The son, 1993; Eder & Felmlee, 1984; Hiebert, 1983; Hoffman,
amount of numbers or letters is increased in response to 1984; Wonder-McDowell, Reutzel, & Smith, 2011).
the students’ ability to correctly recreate the sequence. This This supplementary, guided silent reading intervention
warm-up activity aimed to increase students’ visual percep- provides approximately 600 reading selections ranging from
tion, attention, and automaticity in the discrimination and preprimer to adult-level texts, including high-interest and
recognition of print. Studies conducted by numerous re- low-readability selections for older struggling students. Se-
searchers (e.g., Brenner & Hiebert, 2010; Mirsky, 1999; lections represent a wide range of genres, such as “Miguel’s
Torgesen & Hudson, 2006) suggested that one of the defin- Big Day,” a family life story; “The Lighthouse Visitor,” a
ing characteristics of a proficient reader is the ability to sus- mystery; and “Looking at Clouds,” a science/nature story.
tain attention and keep their eyes on the text. According to As students progress through the varied guided silent read-
Pikulski (2006), “instant, accurate, and automatic access to ing levels, text becomes longer and more challenging, and
all these dimensions of a printed word is the needed elements content choices become more informational. Lesson texts
of fluency that will allow readers to focus their attention on were presented within both a guided silent reading format
comprehension rather than on decoding” (p. 75). (a moving window guides students’ eyes across lines of print
The next part of the guided, silent reading session pro- from left-to-right) and an independent reading format with-
vided students with extensive structured silent reading prac- out any left-to-right guidance. Regardless of the nature of the
tice to build fluency within an authentic reading experience lesson or activity, the text was presented within a controlled
where students read for meaning. During guided silent read- format and rate parameter for each student in the online
ing sessions, involving timed, guided, left-to-right reading environment. Dynamically controlled by individual student
practice, students read texts selected from a diverse collec- performance, comprehension questions were either inter-
tion of narrative and expository texts at each student’s inde- spersed between individual reading segments or followed at
pendent instructional reading level. The work of O’Connor the conclusion of the story. All comprehension questions
et al. (2002), as reported by Allington (2006), showed that were electronically coded by the system to continuously
providing daily intervention lessons using grade-level texts track student performance with 25 comprehension skills
was not nearly as successful as providing daily lessons using based on Bloom’s taxonomy, including literal understanding,
texts matched to the instructional reading levels of strug- interpretation, analysis, evaluation, and appreciation. The
gling readers. O’Connor et al. argued that selecting texts of format (wide vs. repeated readings) and rate at which text
appropriate challenge should be a first step in the design of ef- was presented on screen was then incrementally increased as
fective supplementary reading instruction and intervention. a function of students’ performance on these comprehension
This is no less true when designing effective silent reading questions and reading rate performances during the reading
practice for regular education students in elementary class- events. As students progressed through the levels, the texts
rooms (Reutzel, Jones, et al., 2008). read became progressively longer and more challenging. The
Lesson text selections were matched to struggling readers’ intent of the guided silent reading lesson is to provide stu-
independent reading levels using Spache (1953), Dale and dents with authentic reading experiences that build compre-
Chall (1948a, 1948b), and Fry (1968) readability formulas. hension, fluency and stamina at a level of difficulty that will
The guided, silent reading supplementary intervention com- provide them the maximum acceleration of progress. Ad-
puter environment was programmed to continuously and ditionally, given that the difficulty of texts was established
dynamically monitor students’ performance using reading using the Spache (for primary-level texts) and Dale–Chall
rate measures and responses to comprehension questions, (middle-level texts), both of which rely on high-frequency
adjusting the reading content level to match each student’s word lists, students have considerable opportunity to develop
progress. In addition, the guided silent reading intervention fluency with a core group of high frequency words reading
program used a mix of instructional formats and scaffolds these texts. Torgesen and colleagues (Rashotte, MacPhee,
to further match individualized needs and rates of progress. & Torgesen, 2001; Torgesen & Hudson, 2006) argued that
These included variation in the presentation of text, the limited sight vocabularies are a principal characteristic of
length of reading segments, the location and number of students with reading disabilities beyond the initial phase of
comprehension questions, and the use of repeated readings. learning to read.
Thus, students were able to progress through levels of reading The guided silent reading component of the guided, silent
challenge in this intervention based on several factors. Stu- reading supplementary intervention was also followed by the
dents had to be able to read passages at their present levels cloze vocabulary component. The cloze component used
with grade-appropriate rates and good comprehension be- structured contextual analysis activities to assist struggling
fore they were advanced to subsequent levels. Past research students develop comprehension competency. These cloze
on reading fluency and comprehension development has fur- exercises are intended to encourage students to use context
ther demonstrated that struggling readers are the least likely clues to complete the meaning of sentences as well as longer
to engage in effective silent reading practice that would passages. Students also practice deriving the meaning of diffi-
provide them with the opportunity to integrate the varied cult or unfamiliar word meanings by analyzing the surround-
reading instruction interventions they receive (Allington, ing context in these cloze activities, thus potentially enhanc-
2006; Chinn, Waggoner, Anderson, Schommer, & Wilkin- ing wide reading vocabulary learning strategies and skills.
410 The Journal of Educational Research

Performance scores within each practice module, the


interconnectedness of the various practice modules, inte- TABLE 1. Demographic Comparison of Treatment and
grated formative assessments following each lesson, and a Matched Control Students
highly sophisticated operating system inform just-in-time in-
structional decisions that are sensitive to student character- Treatment Control
Demographics (n = 40) (n = 40)
istics such as age, reading level, performance, progress, and
instructional trajectory. The system not only dynamically % Black 65 58
adjusts each student’s differentiated lesson format within % White 0 8
each practice module but also provides unique adjustments % Latino 35 35
% ELL 15 13
for daily practice sessions. The integration of these modules
% ESE 5 25
allow for the system to provide each student with a practice
environment that uniquely addresses his or her individual Note. ELL = English language learner; ESE = exceptional student
silent reading development needs at any moment in time education.
during the implementation period.

Data Analyses for the treatment and matched controls groups is reported
in Tables 1 and 2.
To assess the value-added of this silent reading fluency
As can be seen by the reported indices, the two groups
intervention with third-grade struggling readers, a propen-
were reasonably matched from the propensity analysis. The
sity score analysis was used to match the 40 students from
mean pretest score for the matched control group on the
the sample of 158 who did not receive this supplementary
FCAT-SSS was 814.90 (SD = 217.92) compared with the
silent reading fluency intervention to a group of 40 stu-
treatment group mean of 845.50 (SD = 117.69), correspond-
dents who were similar with regard to demographics, prior
ing to a standardized coefficient of g = 0.17. Similarly, the
FCAT achievement, and performance on the SAT-10 in
mean pretest score on the SAT-10 for the treatment group
this study. The 40 struggling students who received supple-
was 575.75 (SD = 16.04), compared with the control group
mental reading instruction with this silent reading fluency
mean of 570.73 (SD = 18.90), and corresponded to a stan-
intervention completed an average of 71 lesson units dur-
dardized coefficient of g = 0.28.
ing the study. The logistic regression used in this study to
Because students who participated in the program were
construct the propensity scores predicted group membership
from different classes and schools, and the analysis was based
with race/ethnicity, limited English proficiency status, pri-
on available, archival data, the ratio of students to classes
mary exceptionality status, and reading performance on the
was small, precluding a mixed-effects modeling of the data
previous year’s FCAT-SSS and the SAT-10. Prior technical
to account for clustering at the classroom and school levels.
reports have indicated that the correlation between FCAT
A one-way ANOVA was used to assess the extent to
scores from year to year is approximately .75 in elementary
which the treatment and matched control students were
schools (Florida Department of Education, 2001); moreover,
statistically differentiated on the posttest scores for both
the correlation between the FCAT-SSS and the SAT-10 in
the FCAT-SSS and the SAT-10. To control for the false
Grade 3 is .78. As such, while a strong correlation exists be-
discovery rate, a linear step-up procedure was used for any
tween the two assessments of reading comprehension, it was
statistically significant finding.
important to capture the unexplained covariance in scores.
By using both measures in the propensity score matching,
greater specificity could be attained. Resulting propensity
scores were used in a secondary analysis to match students
TABLE 2. Descriptive Statistics for the Control and
based on their designation as having received treatment or Treatment Groups on the FCAT and SAT-10 Pre- and
not. Once students were appropriately matched, they were Posttest Scores
designated to receive or not receive the supplementary silent
reading fluency intervention (control vs. treatment group). Control group Treatment group
After a full year, the resulting student scores were analyzed
Measure M SD M SD
using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a lin-
ear step-up to control for the false-discovery rate (Benjamini FCAT Pretest 845.50 117.69 819.90 217.92
& Hochberg, 1995). FCAT Posttest 1012.33 357.46 1322.63 171.24
SAT-10 Pretest 570.03 18.90 575.75 16.04
SAT-10 Posttest 597.93 34.95 608.53 24.43
Results
Note. FCAT = Florida Comprehensive Assessment Test; SAT-10
A summary of the demographics (see Table 1) and descrip- = Stanford Achievement Test–10.
tive statistics (see Table 2) for the FCAT and SAT-10 scores
The Journal of Educational Research 411

FCAT-SSS results indicated that a significant effect ex- Discussion


isted for treatment, F(1, 79) = 24.52, p < .001, suggesting
that treatment students’ scores on the posttest were signifi- Providing the highest quality reading instruction for all
cantly higher than the matched control. The mean posttest students is a central focus of current educational research.
score for the silent reading fluency intervention students was With the advent of the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001
1322.63 (SD = 171.24) compared with the matched con- (2002) and increasing use of response to intervention models
trol’s mean of 1012.33 (SD = 357.46). A more appropriate in classrooms and schools, the urgency of identifying effec-
way to contextualize these results is to calculate an effect tive instructional interventions and practices to help strug-
size, which communicates, in standard deviation units, how gling readers succeed has also increased. Both groups in this
large the differences were between the means of the two study of retained, struggling third-grade readers, treatment
groups regardless of sample size. A standardized effect size and control, were provided access to comprehensive core
value g = 1.09 was estimated, indicating that the mean for and supplementary reading instruction in all five essential
the students who were receiving the silent reading fluency elements as identified by the National Reading Panel (2000)
intervention were performing one full standard deviation and as a part of the mandated 2-hr reading instruction block
above the mean for the matched controls. In context, Co- in their school district. Much of past silent reading research
hen (1988) provides guidelines stating that an effect size of has focused on comparing the results obtained from silent
0.80 would be considered large. In practical terms, 80% of the independent versus oral guided reading practice. Such stud-
treatment students who received the supplementary guided, ies have typically found that guided oral reading practice is
silent reading fluency intervention in this study achieved more effective for students and is also preferred by teach-
reading proficiency as measured by the FCAT (achievement ers. This is the case because guided oral reading provides
levels 3 or higher) and were promoted to the next grade a check on whether students are actually reading and how
level, as compared to 32% of the of the matched control well they do so. Prior to the turn of the millennia, studies of
students. guided oral compared to independent silent reading practice
Conversely, no statistically important findings were ob- contributed little to an understanding of how silent reading
served for the SAT-10 differences in the ANOVA, F(1, 79) practice might become more effective.
= 2.59, p = .11, despite a higher posttest SAT-10 score for Instead of providing yet one more comparison of inde-
students receiving the guided, silent reading supplementary pendent, silent reading versus other, largely guided oral ap-
intervention (M = 608.53, SD = 23.43) compared with the proaches to reading practice, we examined how changing
matched controls (M = 597.83, SD = 34.95). Important silent reading practice conditions from silent independent
components to consider in these seemingly conflicting to silent guided affected the reading comprehension and
findings are the issues of power and baseline equivalence. reading achievement of struggling third-grade readers. In
Given the present total sample size in the design (i.e., n = this study, the focus was on comparing the reading compre-
80), a potential reason for the lack of statistical significance hension and achievement of third-grade struggling readers
in the SAT-10 analysis is due to a small effect size that who received guided practice during silent reading using
could be observed, and not a sufficient sample size to detect Reading Plus to a group of matched control students who
it. Indeed, a power analysis with n of 80, Cronbach’s alpha received a combination of other school district approved
of .05, and power of 0.80 indicates that the minimum supplementary reading interventions—Early Success, Soar to
detectable effect size would be 0.63. As such, in the case of Success, Essential Elements of Reading: Vocabulary, and Voy-
the FCAT, a statistically significant finding was observed ager Passport—using largely oral, guided reading practice.
with an estimated effect size difference of over 1.0. With The goal of using the treatment intervention was to provide
the SAT-10 data, a quick calculation of the posttest mean struggling third-grade students with sufficient guidance, in-
differences would yield a standardized coefficient of 0.30, tensity, consistency, and appropriateness of silent reading
yet with minimum detectable effect size of only 0.67, it practice in an online environment to substantially increase
would not be possible to obtain a statistically meaningful their reading comprehension and reading rate achievement.
finding with this group. However, this does not imply that if Guided silent reading practice as provided in the treatment
the sample size was larger or the baseline effect was smaller, intervention was continuously adapted for format of reading
a statistically significant effect would be obtained, as the practice (repeated vs. wide readings, short or long passages),
pretest differences suggest that a more diverse sample could pace and level of structure during reading (guided vs. inde-
be used to provide a more accurate match. Notwithstanding pendent reading) and level of reading challenge (readability
this limitation, these results represent preliminary evidence and genre types) via the use of reading efficiency measures
that a moderate to strong relationship between the value- and comprehension assessments during online silent reading
added of the guided silent reading fluency intervention, practice with leveled texts. Using guided silent reading prac-
Reading Plus, and student performance in reading exists for tice not only frees the teacher to provide more instruction
retained third grade students in Florida, given the measured and assistance to targeted students during reading practice
outcomes. sessions but also assures that when the teacher is not present,
412 The Journal of Educational Research

struggling readers who were reading silently, were on task make significant progress. Holding students accountable for
and their eyes were on the page. their time spent reading by measuring students’ reading rates,
Statistically significant differences were identified in fa- responses to comprehension questions, and cloze passages
vor of the guided silent reading treatment group on strug- lets students know they are going to be monitored for the
gling third-grade students’ reading comprehension and read- time spent in reading practice. Accountability assured that
ing achievement scores on the FCAT test. The effect sizes students’ eyes were on the text, which has been shown to
were large, slightly greater than a full standard deviation, predict student reading achievement (Brenner & Hiebert,
favoring the guided, silent reading supplementary inter- 2010). Finally, the guided, silent reading supplementary in-
vention as compared with other school district–approved tervention tested in this study focused more time and prac-
supplementary reading interventions for use with these re- tice on developing students’ fluency, vocabulary, and com-
tained third-grade struggling readers. prehension skills than did the control programs that gave
The statistically significant findings and large effect sizes considerable time and practice to increasing students’ word
favoring the guided, silent reading practice provided to strug- recognition automaticity through decoding practice. In fo-
gling third-grade readers can be at least partially explained cusing students’ practice on fluency, vocabulary, and com-
by turning to other research on effective approaches for pro- prehension may have transported students more efficiently
viding silent reading practice to students in schools. First, over the fluency bridge from decoding to comprehension
this type of guided, silent reading intervention increased than did more decoding practice.
this sample of third-grade struggling readers’ opportunities No significant differences were found between the con-
to read. It did so in a number of ways. First, in past research trol and treatment groups of retained, struggling third-grade
on silent reading, struggling readers often selected books readers on the SAT-10 nationally norm-referenced reading
that were too difficult for them to read fluently. The guided, achievement text and reading comprehension subtest. With
silent reading supplementary intervention computer envi- respect to the SAT-10 findings, locating a sample size of
ronment monitored students’ comprehension of texts and retained third-grade struggling readers sufficiently large to
then automatically and continuously adjusted the format of power the analysis of SAT-10 reading scores proved to be
practice, genre, and level of challenge to match the students’ daunting, even with an initial sample of over 1,200 third-
abilities to comprehend the texts they were reading silently. grade students. A post hoc power analysis of the sample size
When students cannot read the texts they have selected for for this study (n = 80) determined that the obtained sample
silent reading they do not read much. Allington (1977) re- size was too small to provide sufficient statistical power to
minded that “if they don’t read much, how are they ever detect a difference in third-grade students’ SAT-10 reading
gonna get good?” When students do not read much during comprehension and achievement scores. As a result we can-
silent or oral reading practice time, they do not benefit in not be sure that this guided silent reading intervention was
terms of achievement from the time allocated. When strug- any more or less effective than other supplementary reading
gling readers cannot or do not read silently, they find it interventions provided to this sample of struggling third-
difficult to keep their eyes on the text and focus attention grade readers as measured by the SAT-10 although posttest
(Trainin, Wilson, Hiebert, Erikson, & Laughridge, 2007). SAT-10 reading comprehension mean scores trended higher
The guided, silent reading intervention used in this study for the guided, silent reading supplementary intervention
assured that students’ eyes were on the text. It accomplished treatment group than for the control group. However, these
this aim by providing visual and perceptual modeling prac- results can also be used to argue that the guided silent reading
tice, monitoring their comprehension responses to the read- intervention used in this study was at least as useful as were
ing of increasingly challenging and longer text selections, the other school district approved supplementary reading
and continuously adjusting the level of text and question interventions provided to this group of struggling readers.
challenge based on these indicators. As a result, the evidence presented in this study demon-
The guided, silent reading supplementary intervention strates that providing struggling third-grade readers with a
used in this study also promoted student motivation. Be- guided, silent reading intervention in an online environ-
cause students are provided with a selection of appropriately ment via the Reading Plus supplementary reading inter-
leveled texts from which they can choose stories that most vention used in this study yielded large effects on reading
interest them (Fawson, Reutzel, Read, Smith, & Moore, achievement and comprehension scores on a high stakes
2009; Swan, Coddington, & Guthrie, 2010). In addition, state administered test, the FCAT, which is used by schools
students who read widely learn more vocabulary word mean- to determine both individual student progress and school
ings through reading and increase their abilities to man- progress toward meeting the requirements of Adequate
age and comprehend a variety of text structures and genres Yearly Progress (AYP).
(Cunningham & Allington, 2010; Pressley, 2002). Provid-
ing struggling students with continuous feedback on their Limitations
reading performance in terms of rates and comprehension
was also helpful to students as a part of designing effective The results of this study, comparing a matched sample of
guided, silent reading practice conditions. Adjusting passage struggling third-grade readers who were retained in grade for
and lesson difficulty also seemed to help struggling students poor reading performance, was limited by the total sample
The Journal of Educational Research 413

size (n = 80). The criterion used to select struggling readers reading intervention for struggling readers. Other supple-
for this study was poor performance on the FCAT test at mentary intervention programs could also be used in future
the end of Grade 3 resulting in retention in that grade for comparisons of the efficacy of the Reading Plus guided, silent
another year. These criteria were fairly narrow as compared reading intervention used in this study. Future evaluations of
to those used in other research focused on struggling readers. this intervention’s efficacy could also be assessed with var-
Very often struggling readers are selected based on perfor- ied reading and achievement assessment instruments that
mance that is at least one standard deviation below the mean would provide both more sensitive measurement as well as
on traditional reading or achievement measures. The study multiple, converging data points. Future researchers should
was also limited to a comparison of a single, guided silent also investigate the use of wave or growth modeling to ex-
reading intervention, Reading Plus, with a variety of other amine the build-up effects for this intervention to determine
nationally marketed supplementary reading programs. It was optimal length of use to achieve maximum improvements in
not the purpose of this study to compare Reading Plus with reading comprehension and achievement.
any other specific interventions. Therefore, nothing can be Despite these improvements and previously noted limita-
said about this individual intervention’s efficacy in compari- tions, this study provides important evidence supporting the
son with other interventions not evaluated in this study. The efficacy of a supplementary, guided, silent reading interven-
study was also limited by its geographical location and de- tion with a sample of matched third-grade struggling readers
mographics. The study took place in a large, predominantly who were retained at grade level. The guided, silent reading
Hispanic, southeastern urban school district environment. intervention not only afforded this group of struggling third
Therefore the results of this study may not be generalizable grade students with appropriately challenging and varied
to other regions, types or sizes of school districts and to reading genres to be motivating and within their reach, but
other ethnic groups across the nation. The tests used in this it also resulted in the great majority of these students mak-
study were also a limitation. Although the use of criterion- ing sufficient progress so as to be promoted to the next grade
referenced state reading tests have become the standard by level. This guided, silent reading intervention in this study
which most schools are judged for achieving AYP under provided students guidance in terms of visual and percep-
the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (2002), the FCAT tual modeling and rate management during silent reading,
represents only one of many such tests used nationally and formatting their reading practice individually, adapting the
may be more or less technically and psychometrically sound text to be read by type, genre, and level of challenge contin-
in comparison with other such tests used across the nation. uously monitoring their performance during silent reading
Similarly, the SAT-10 is only one of many psychometrically practice. This combination of guided, silent reading inter-
sound, nationally distributed, and norm-referenced reading vention elements nested within an adaptive online presen-
achievement tests available and sold nationally. Finally, the tation environment demonstrated efficacy with this group
design of the study was a limitation as well. Even though the of struggling third-grade readers on the FCAT test after a
use of propensity scores provides a more exacting approach full-year trial. Thus, the results of this study indicate that a
for matching student characteristics to form experimental guided, silent reading intervention employing a suite of in-
groups, it is nevertheless limited by the characteristics se- structional elements described can offer classroom teachers
lected by the researchers for doing so. It is not as strong a a potentially useful and efficacious tool for providing strug-
research design for making inferences as a true, randomized, gling third-grade student effective, supplementary, guided
controlled experimental study. silent reading practice at school.

Implications REFERENCES

This study provided emerging evidence supporting the use Allington, R. L. (1977). If they don’t read much, how they ever gonna get
good? Journal of Reading, 21, 57–61.
of a guided, silent reading intervention known as Reading Plus Allington, R. L. (Ed.). (2002). Big Brother and the national reading curriculum:
for improving the reading comprehension and achievement How ideology trumped evidence. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann Educational
scores of struggling third-grade readers on the FCAT. It did Books.
Allington, R. L. (2006). Fluency: Still waiting after all these years. In
not provide similar evidence for the use of this guided, silent S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say about
reading intervention for improving the reading comprehen- fluency instruction (pp. 94–105). Newark, DE: International Reading
sion and achievement scores of struggling third-grade readers Association.
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery
on the SAT-10. Future researchers may want to broaden the rate- a new and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the
criteria used to select struggling readers in order to enlarge Royal Statistical Society B, 57, 289–300.
the sample size. To increase the ability to generalize findings Berk, R. A. (1998). Review of the Stanford Achievement Test, Ninth
Edition. In J. C. Impara & B. S. Plake (Eds.), The thirteenth mental mea-
to other groups in others schools and classes across the na- surements yearbook (pp. 925–928). Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental
tion, struggling readers should be selected from more than Measurements.
a single grade level, a single school district, and a single re- Block, C. C., Cleveland, M. D., & Reed, K. M. (2006). When twenty
minutes of literacy instruction is added to the day: Which learning en-
gion of the country. For future researchers, a randomized, vironments increase students’ overall achievement, vocabulary, compre-
controlled trial would provide stronger evidence for making hension, fluency, and affective development? In Scholastic classroom books
inferences about the potential efficacy of this guided, silent compendium of research (pp. 18–36). New York, NY: Scholastic.
414 The Journal of Educational Research

Boyer, E. L. (1995). The basic school: A community for learning. Princeton, Hoffman, J. V. (1984). Guided oral reading and miscue focused verbal
NJ: Carnegie Foundation for the Advancement of Teaching. feedback in second-grade classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 19,
Brenner, D., & Hiebert, E. F. (2010). The impact of professional de- 367–384.
velopment on students’ opportunity to read. In E. H. Hiebert & D. Kamil, M. L. (2008). How to get recreational reading to increase reading
R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting silent reading: New directions for teach- achievement. In Y. Kim, V. J. Risko, D. L. Compton, D. K. Dickinson,
ers and researchers (pp. 198–220). Newark, DE: International Reading M. K. Hundley, R. T. Jiménez, . . . D. Well Rowe (Eds.), 57th yearbook of
Association. the National Reading Conference (pp. 31–40). Oak Creek, WI: National
Bryan, G., Fawson, P. C., & Reutzel, D. R. (2003). Sustained silent read- Reading Conference.
ing: Exploring the value of literature discussion with three non-engaged Kelley, M., & Clausen-Grace, N. (2006). R5: The sustained silent read-
readers. Reading Research and Instruction, 43(1), 47–73. ing makeover that transformed readers. The Reading Teacher, 60, 148–
Chall, J. S. (2000). The academic achievement challenge: What really works in 157.
the classroom. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kelley, M., & Clausen-Grace, N. (2010). R5: A sustained silent reading
Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why makeover that works. In E. H. Hiebert & D. R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting
poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Press. silent reading: New directions for teachers and researchers (pp. 168–180).
Chinn, C. A., Waggoner, M. A., Anderson, R. C., Schommer, M., & Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Wilkinson, I. A. (1993). Situated action in the small-group reading les- Krashen, S. (2002). More smoke and mirrors: A critique of the Na-
son: A microanalysis of oral reading error episodes. American Educational tional Reading Panel Report on Fluency. In R. L. Allington (Ed.), Big
Research Journal, 30, 361–392. Brother and the national reading curriculum: How ideology trumped evidence
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd (pp. 112–124). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann Educational Books.
ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kuhn, M. R. (2004). Helping students become accurate, expressive readers:
Coles, G. (2000). Misreading reading: The bad science that hurts students. Fluency instruction for small groups. The Reading Teacher, 58, 338–344.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Kuhn, M. R. (2005). A comparative study of small group fluency instruction.
Cooper, J. D., & Pikulski, J. J. (2006). Houghton Mifflin’s Reading Treasures. Reading Psychology, 26, 127–146.
Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin. Kuhn, M. R., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2008). Fluency in the classroom. New
Cunningham, J. W. (2001). The National Reading Panel report. Reading York, NY: Guilford Press.
Research Quarterly, 30, 326–335. Mirsky, A. F. (1999). Disorders of attention: A neuropsychological perspec-
Cunningham, P. M., & Allington, R. L. (2010). Classrooms that work: They tive. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and
can all read and write (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education. executive function (pp. 71–96). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Dale, E., & Chall, J. S. (1948a). A formula for predicting readability. Morse, D. T. (2005). The sixteenth mental measurements yearbook. Lincoln,
Educational Research Bulletin (Ohio State University), 27(1), 11–20. NE: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
Dale, E., & Chall, J. S. (1948b). A formula for predicting readability: National Institute for Literacy. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of
Instructions. Educational Research Bulletin (Ohio State University), 27(2), the National Early Literacy Panel. Jessup, MD: Author.
37–54. National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teach-
Donovan, C. A., Smolkin, L. B., & Lomax, R. G. (2000). Beyond the ing children to read. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health
independent-level text: Considering the reader-text match in first- and Human Development.
graders’ self-selections during recreational reading. Reading Psychology: No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub. L. No. 107–110, § 115, Stat. 1425
An International Quarterly, 21, 309–333. (2002).
Eder, D., & Felmlee, D. (1984). The development of attention norms in O’Connor, R. E., Bell, K. M., Harty, K. R., Larkin, L. K., Sackor, S. M.,
ability groups. In P. L. Peterson, L. C. Wilkinson, & M. Hallinan (Eds.), & Zigmond, N. (2002). Teaching reading to poor readers in the inter-
The social context of instruction: Group organization and group processes mediate grades: A comparison of text difficulty. Journal of Educational
(pp. 189–208). New York, NY: Academic Press. Psychology, 94, 474–485.
Edmondson, J., & Shannon, P. (2002). The will of the people. In Pikulski, J. J. (2006). Fluency: A developmental and language perspec-
R. L. Allington (Ed.), Big Brother and the national reading curricu- tive. In S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say
lum: How ideology trumped evidence (pp. 224–231). Portsmouth, NH: about fluency instruction (pp. 70–93). Newark, DE: International Reading
Heinemann. Association.
Fawson, P. C., Reutzel, D. R., Read, S., Smith, J. A., & Moore, S. A. (2009). Pressley, M. (2002). Comprehension strategies instruction: A turn-of-the-
Reading attitudes: The influence of differing the paths to an incentive century status report. In C. C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension
on third-graders’ recreational and academic reading. Reading Psychology: instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 11–27). New York, NY: Guil-
An International Quarterly, 30, 564–583. ford Press.
Florida Center for Reading Research. (n.d.). Florida Center for Read- Rashotte, C. A., MacPhee, K., & Torgesen, J. K. (2001). The effectiveness
ing Research: FCRR reports: Soar to success. Voyager passport. Re- of a group reading instruction program with poor readers in multiple
trieved from http://www.fcrr.org/FCRRReports/PDF/Soar Success.pdf or grades. Learning Disability Quarterly, 24, 119–134.
Vpassport final.pdf Rasinski, T. V. (1989). Fluency for everyone: Incorporating fluency instruc-
Florida Department of Education. (2001). FCAT handbook: A resource for tion in the classroom. The Reading Teacher, 43, 690–693.
educators. Tallahassee, FL: Author. Rasinski, T. V., Samuels, S. J., Hiebert, E., Petscher, Y., & Feller, K. (2011).
Fresch, M. J. (1995). Self-selection of early literacy learners. The Reading The effects of silent reading fluency instructional protocol on students’
Teacher, 49, 220–227. reading comprehension and achievement in an urban school setting.
Fry, E. (1968). A readability formula that saves time. Journal of Reading, 11, Reading Psychology, 32(1), 75–97.
513–516. Reutzel, D. R., Fawson, P. C., & Smith, J. A. (2008). Reconsidering
Guthrie, J. T., Schafer, W. D., & Huang, C. (2001). Benefits of opportunity silent sustained reading: An exploratory study of scaffolded silent reading
to read and balanced instruction on the NAEP. The Journal of Educational (ScSR). The Journal of Educational Research, 102, 37–50.
Research, 94, 145–162. doi:10.1080/00220670109599912. Reutzel, D. R., & Hollingsworth, P. M. (1993). Effects of fluency training on
Hiebert, E. H. (1983). An examination of ability grouping for reading second grade students’ reading comprehension. The Journal of Educational
instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 231–255. Research, 86, 325–331.
Hiebert, E. H., & Reutzel, D. R. (2010). Revisiting silent reading in 2020 Reutzel, D. R., Jones, C. D., Fawson, P. C., & Smith, J. A. (2008). Scaffolded
and beyond. In E. H. Hiebert & D. R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting silent silent reading (ScSR): An alternative to guided oral repeated reading that
reading: New directions for teachers and researchers (pp. 290–299). Newark, works! The Reading Teacher, 62, 194–207.
DE: International Reading Association. Reutzel, D. R., Jones, C. D., & Newman, T. (2010). Scaffolded silent read-
Hiebert, E. H., Wilson, K. M., & Trainin, G. (2010). Are students ing: Improving the conditions of silent reading practice in classrooms. In
really reading in independent reading contexts? An examination of E. H. Hiebert & D. R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting silent reading: New direc-
comprehension-based silent reading rate. In E. H. Hiebert & D. tions for teachers and researchers (pp. 129–150). Newark, DE: International
R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting silent reading: New directions for teach- Reading Association.
ers and researchers (pp. 151–167). Newark, DE: International Reading Rosenbaum, P. R., & Rubin, D. B. (1983). The central role of the propensity
Association. score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika, 70, 41–55.
The Journal of Educational Research 415

Samuels, S. J. (2007). The DIBELS tests: Is speed of barking at print what Wonder-McDowell, C., Reutzel, D. R., & Smith, J. A. (2011). Does in-
we mean by fluency? Reading Research Quarterly, 42, 563–566. structional alignment matter: Effects on struggling second-grade readers’
Samuels, S. J., & Farstrup, A. E. (2006). What research has to say about achievement. The Elementary School Journal, 112, 259–279.
fluency instruction. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Wright, G., Sherman, R., & Jones, T. B. (2004). Are silent reading behav-
Samuel, S. J., Hiebert, E. H., & Rasinski, T. V. (2010). Eye movements make iors really silent? The Reading Teacher, 57, 546–553.
reading possible. In E. H. Hiebert & D. R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting silent Wright, G., Sherman, R., & Jones, T. B. (2010). Developmental consid-
reading: New directions for teachers and researchers (pp. 24–44). Newark, erations in transferring oral reading skills to silent reading. In E. H.
DE: International Reading Association. Hiebert & D. R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting silent reading: New directions for
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C., & Foorman, teachers and researchers (pp. 57–66). Newark, DE: International Reading
B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal Association.
comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282. Wu, Y., & Samuels, S. J. (2004, May). How the amount of time spent in
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental independent reading affects reading achievement: A response to the National
and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston, MA: Reading Panel. Paper presented at the 49th annual convention of the
Houghton Mifflin. International Reading Association, Reno, NV.
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. N., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties
in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Spache, G. D. (1953). A new readability formula for primary-grade reading AUTHORS NOTE
materials. Elementary School Journal, 53, 410–413.
Spies, R. A., & Plake, B. S. (Eds.). (2005). The sixteenth mental measurements D. Ray Reutzel is the Emma Eccles Jones Endowed Chair
yearbook. Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
Swan, E. A., Coddington, C. S., & Guthrie, J. T. (2010). Engaged silent Professor of Early Literacy at Utah State University. He is a
reading. In E. H. Hiebert & D. R. Reutzel (Eds.), Revisiting silent reading: member of the Reading Hall of Fame. His research addresses
New directions for teachers and researchers (pp. 95–111). Newark, DE: issues in early literacy instruction primarily in comprehen-
International Reading Association.
Topping, K. J. (2006). Building reading fluency: Cognitive, behavioral, sion and fluency development.
and socioemotional factors and the role of peer-mediated learning. In Yaacov Petscher is an Associate Research Professor at
S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say about Florida State University. Dr. Petscher is a member of the
fluency instruction (pp. 106–129). Newark, DE: International Reading
Association. Learning Systems Institute, Florida Center for Reading Re-
Torgesen, J. K., & Hudson, R. F. (2006). Reading fluency: Critical issues search (FCRR). His research expertise is in research design,
for struggling readers. In S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What analysis, and methodology.
research has to say about fluency instruction (pp. 130–158). Newark, DE:
International Reading Association. Alex N. Spichtig recently completed her PhD in educa-
Trainin, G., Wilson, K. M., Hiebert, E., Erickson, J., & Laughridge, V. tion at Capella University in Minnesota. She is a research
(2007, April). An examination of silent reading rates and comprehension: associate at Taylor and Associates and has an interest in
Tasks, proficiency levels, and length of text. Poster presented at the annual
meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, measuring reading fluency development with eye movement
IL. photography.
Copyright of Journal of Educational Research is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like