You are on page 1of 7

Accident Analysis and Prevention 45 (2012) 759–765

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

The effects of on-street parking and road environment visual complexity on


travel speed and reaction time
Jessica Edquist ∗ , Christina M. Rudin-Brown, Michael G. Lenné
Human Factors group, Monash University Accident Research Centre, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: On-street parking is associated with elevated crash risk. It is not known how drivers’ mental workload
Received 15 July 2011 and behaviour in the presence of on-street parking contributes to, or fails to reduce, this increased crash
Received in revised form 14 October 2011 risk. On-street parking tends to co-exist with visually complex streetscapes that may affect workload and
Accepted 17 October 2011
crash risk in their own right. The present paper reports results from a driving simulator study examining
the effects of on-street parking and road environment visual complexity on driver behaviour and surro-
Keywords:
gate measures of crash risk. Twenty-nine participants drove a simulated urban commercial and arterial
Driving simulation
route. Compared to sections with no parking bays or empty parking bays, in the presence of occupied
Driving task difficulty
Risk compensation
parking bays drivers lowered their speed and shifted their lateral position towards roadway centre to
Road safety compensate for the higher mental workload they reported experiencing. However, this compensation
was not sufficient to reduce drivers’ reaction time on a safety-relevant peripheral detection task or to
an unexpected pedestrian hazard. Compared to the urban road environments, the less visually complex
arterial road environment was associated with speeds that were closer to the posted limit, lower speed
variability and lower workload ratings. These results support theoretical positions that proffer workload
as a mediating variable of speed choice. However, drivers in this study did not modify their speed suf-
ficiently to maintain safe hazard response times in complex environments with on-street parking. This
inadequate speed compensation is likely to affect real world crash risk.
© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction parking to increased crash rates was larger than that of road width.
This implies that factors other than road width must underlie the
On-street parking is frequently found in urban retail road envi- higher crash risk when on-street parking is present.
ronments, as it is convenient for shoppers and does not require In complex urban environments, drivers must monitor move-
the space used by a dedicated parking lot. Unfortunately, on-street ments of both pedestrians and vehicles. Parked cars may obstruct
parking is also associated with increased crash risk compared to the view of the road ahead, making it more difficult to see crossing
roads of the same category without on-street parking (Greibe, pedestrians. Parked cars may also suddenly become moving cars,
2003; Pande and Abdel-Aty, 2009; Roberts et al., 1995). It is impor- and rejoin the traffic stream. It has been found that the number
tant to understand the reasons behind this elevated crash risk in of (moving) vehicles in a scene negatively affects situation aware-
order to design appropriate countermeasures. ness and hazard avoidance (Gugerty, 1997), and parked vehicles
At a practical level, one potential reason for increased crash risk may contribute to this effect, as they require monitoring to deter-
is narrowed road width. Parked vehicles leave less space for travel- mine whether or not they are moving. The presence of parked cars
ling vehicles, forcing them to drive closer to vehicles in the next lane therefore increases the uncertainty, mental load and potential risk
(which may be travelling in the opposite direction). Similarly, nar- associated with the road environment.
row roads are associated with shifts in lateral position closer to the It would be surprising if drivers were oblivious to these changes
centreline (Lewis-Evans and Charlton, 2006), as well as higher crash and did not adapt their behaviour accordingly. In fact, traffic obser-
rates than standard roads (Greibe, 2003). Greibe’s (2003) modelling vations in residential areas have found that high parking densities
research found that both parking and road width were significant correlate with slower speeds (Daisa and Peers, 1997). Psychological
predictors of crash rates on road links, and that the contribution of research offers further insight into this adaptation. Chinn and Elliot
(2002) asked drivers questions about sketches of road scenes with
various features, and found that on-street parking increased ratings
∗ Corresponding author at: Building 70, Monash University, Victoria, 3800,
of tension and decreased the perceived safe speed. This is consis-
Australia. Tel.: +61 3 9905 5815; fax: +61 3 9905 4363.
tent with theories of driver behaviour that maintain drivers reduce
E-mail address: Jessica.edquist@monash.edu (J. Edquist). their speed when they perceive a road environment as being risky,

0001-4575/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.aap.2011.10.001
760 J. Edquist et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 45 (2012) 759–765

for example Wilde’s (1982) theory of Risk Homeostasis, Summala’s Dependent variables included subjective workload, vehicle con-
(1988) Zero Risk theory and Fuller’s (2005) and Fuller et al.’s (2008) trol (speed/headway, lateral position and variability in each of
Task-Capability Interface model. these), performance on a PDT, and response to a critical situation
Besides on-street parking, the roadside environment may also (in which the driver would collide with a simulated pedestrian
contribute to driver workload and perceived risk, and hence affect unless they took preventive action). PDT performance can be con-
travel speed. Chinn and Elliot (2002) found that the height and type strued as an objective measure of both hazard perception ability
(office vs. commercial) of buildings adjacent to the road lowered and situation awareness, two essential driver skills. Vehicle control
perceived safe speeds, although they did not change drivers’ ratings is also directly related to crash risk: a vehicle will crash if its posi-
of tension. Factors such as building setback (Martens et al., 1997; tion coincides with another object either laterally or longitudinally,
van der Horst and de Ridder, 2007), building height (Allsop, 1995; therefore the driver must maintain lateral control (lane position)
Elliot et al., 2003), and visual complexity (Horberry et al., 2006) and longitudinal control (speed, headway).
have all been found to affect speed on urban roads, presumably by
increasing the visual/cognitive workload of the driver. It is possible 2.2. Participants
that crash risk may also be affected by these roadside factors: Harms
(1986) found that, compared to rural areas, driving on highways Twenty-nine drivers (15 male) aged between 20 and 53 years
through village areas was associated with slower speeds and higher (mean = 28.0 years, SD = 7.6 years) were recruited from the Univer-
cognitive load, which was correlated with the number of reported sity community via an online advertisement. The average number
accidents. of years participants had been driving was 9.8 (SD = 8.0); all par-
Driver workload, the mediating variable between the difficulty ticipants were regular drivers with at least one year of licensed
of the driving task and performance, reflects the driver’s current (unsupervised) driving experience. Participants were compensated
ability to meet the demands of the task (De Waard, 1996). A driver’s $30 for their time. The study was approved by the University ethics
experience of increased workload would be expected to medi- committee.
ate compensation to a more difficult driving environment. Greater
workload is reflected in lower vehicle speed and longer headways 2.3. Equipment
to a lead vehicle, as drivers attempt to make the driving task less dif-
ficult. Variability in speed and headway has been found to increase The experiment used an EF-X by Eca-Faros driving simulator
with workload (Parkes et al., 2007), as drivers are less able to con- with modified software for research purposes. This consists of a
centrate on longitudinal vehicle control. Interestingly, variability in driver’s seat with dashboard, steering wheel, accelerator and brake
lateral position has been found to both increase and decrease with pedals, handbrake and gear shift. The simulated environment is
workload, depending on the source of the workload (De Waard, displayed on three screens providing 120◦ field of view including
1996; Engstrom et al., 2005). These variations may affect crash risk rear view mirror. Data is collected at 30 Hz.
if they result in lane excursions or very short headways. In addition,
if drivers’ adaptation to increased workload/task demand is inad- 2.4. Scenarios
equate (i.e. if they do not slow down sufficiently to compensate),
their reaction time to hazards will be slower, leaving less time to The simulated road had two lanes in each direction with a double
avoid a collision. white line (no median strip) in between. There were sidewalks with
The present study used a driving simulator to examine differ- pedestrians on both sides and occasional oncoming traffic. Lane and
ences in driver behaviour which may help to explain crash risk parking bay dimensions were based on information supplied by the
differences between different environments. In each road environ- local road authority. Fig. 1 shows examples of the simulated road
ment studied, the road geometry was held constant; however we environment.
varied the complexity of the road environment, and the presence of Scenarios were designed to replicate the four road environments
on-street parking. It was hypothesised that both road complexity (parking conditions) of interest. The four conditions comprised: the
and on-street parking would lead to increased subjective workload, Arterial No Parking condition, in which buildings were set far back
decreased vehicle speed (and possibly increased speed variability), from the road and there were no parking bays (see Fig. 1, top panel);
increased headway to a slower-moving lead vehicle (and pos- the Urban No Parking condition, a shopping-strip style road with
sibly increased headway variability), changes in lateral position shopfronts on low multi-storey buildings set close to the road on
(and possibly increased lane position variability), increased reac- both sides; the Urban Empty Parking condition, identical to Urban
tion time to safety-relevant peripheral detection task (PDT) targets No Parking but with the addition of empty parking bays marked
and more missed targets, and impaired responses to an unexpected in the kerbside lanes on each side of the road; and the Urban Full
pedestrian event. Parking condition (see Fig. 1, lower panel), identical to Urban Empty
Parking but with 90% of the parking bays occupied by vehicles.
These four conditions were each presented (as a 1 km-long block)
2. Method six times over the course of three experimental drives, in order
to minimise fatigue and order effects. Each drive contained eight
2.1. Design blocks: two Arterial No Parking blocks, and one block with car fol-
lowing and one block without for each of Urban No Parking, Urban
To examine the effects of road environment on driver behaviour, Empty Parking, and Urban Full Parking. The blocks were in a differ-
we compared a low complexity environment (the Arterial No Park- ent (randomised) order for each drive. In addition to these blocks,
ing condition) with three conditions involving a combination of each drive started and ended with a section similar to the Arterial
high complexity and parking factors in a single-factor, repeated No Parking condition that was not analysed.
measures ANOVA with four levels. Half of the urban blocks (i.e. Urban No Parking, Urban Empty
Within the higher complexity roadside environment, we exam- Parking and Urban Full Parking conditions) contained a car-
ined the effects of on-street parking with and without the presence following task, in which a lead vehicle turned into the road ahead
of traffic ahead. This analysis included three levels of parking (None, of the participant’s vehicle, travelling initially at the same speed as
Empty bays, Full bays) and two levels of traffic (lead vehicle present, the participant. After 160 m, this vehicle decelerated to 70% of the
lead vehicle absent – both levels contained oncoming traffic). participant’s speed (at that point) and remained at that speed for
J. Edquist et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 45 (2012) 759–765 761

Mean sp
peed Ma
ax speed
70.0

65.0

Speed (km/h)
60.0

55.0

50.0

45.0
Arterial
r No Urban No U
Urban Emptty Urban F
Full
Parrking Park
P ing Parking Parking

Fig. 2. Mean and maximum speed (for non-following blocks only) by road environ-
ment and parking condition (bars represent 95% CIs).

across participants. At eight points (once each after blocks of each


Urban condition with and without car following, and twice after
Arterial No Parking blocks), participants stopped at a red traffic
light and completed a Mental Workload Questionnaire for the block
they had just driven though. The Mental Workload Questionnaire
was based on the NASA-TLX (Hart and Staveland, 1988); however
the question about physical demand was replaced with a question
about safety. Participants rated mental demand, time pressure, suc-
cess, effort, frustration, and safety (reverse scored) out of ten, and
these scores were summed to give an overall measure of work-
load (results for the two Arterial No Parking blocks were averaged,
after confirming that they were not significantly different from one
another).

2.6. Data analyses

After checking that there were no significant differences


between blocks of the same condition due to order effects, data
from different blocks of the same condition were averaged. Each of
Fig. 1. Screenshots of (a) Arterial No Parking and (b) Urban Full Parking conditions.
the dependent variables was analysed using a repeated-measures
analysis of variance (RMANOVA) with the within-subjects factor of
the rest of the block. Additionally, in one non-car-following block road environment (four levels: Arterial No Parking, Urban No Park-
for each of these three conditions, a pedestrian suddenly turned and ing, Urban Empty Parking, Urban Full Parking). For the dependent
walked onto the road in front of the subject vehicle. The pedestrian variables of workload rating, lateral position and peripheral detec-
turned and stepped into the road when the driver was 3.64 s away tion, an additional within-subjects factor was the presence/absence
from the potential collision site (based on the drivers’ speed, not a of a lead vehicle (these analyses do not include data from the
set distance), thus allowing plenty of time to avoid the collision if Arterial No Parking condition). For blocks that included the car-
the driver responded immediately. following task, data from the first 400 m and the last 200 m were
discarded to avoid the potentially confounding effects of the decel-
2.5. Procedure erating lead vehicle (at the start of the block) and red traffic signals
(at the start and end of the block). For blocks without this task, the
After completing a demographic and driving experience first and last 200 m were discarded.
questionnaire, participants performed a short practice drive to
familiarise them with the control dynamics of the simulated 3. Results
vehicle. Participants were instructed to practise accelerating and
braking gently, and to practise driving at a consistent speed of 3.1. Speed
60 km/h. This drive was repeated with the addition of the PDT; this
task involved the participant activating the left or right turn indica- As participants’ speed was restricted by the lead vehicle in
tor in response to a pedestrian icon which appeared at the far side the car-following blocks, the analysis of speed includes only data
of the left or right outer screens. The icon appeared at randomised from blocks without a lead vehicle present (4 × 1 RMANOVA for
intervals of 200, 300, 400, 500 or 600 m (12–36 s at 60 km/h), and each dependent variable). The main effect of road environment
disappeared after 100 m (6 s) if the driver failed to respond. Fol- was significant for mean speed, F(3,84) = 75.405, p < .001, 2 = .729;
lowing this practice, participants completed the three main drives maximum speed, F(3,84) = 24.404, p < .001, 2 = .466; and speed
(while also performing the PDT). The whole experiment took just variability, F(3,84) = 181.778, p < .001, 2 = .867. Mean and maxi-
over an hour. mum speed decreased with the complexity of the environment
In the main drives, participants were instructed to drive as they (Fig. 2): the Arterial No Parking and Urban Full conditions were
normally would, but remain in the centre lane, and try to main- significantly different from the other two conditions, while the
tain a consistent speed of around 60 km/h (the posted speed limit). Urban No Parking and Empty bay conditions were not significantly
The order of presentation of the three drives was counterbalanced different from each other.
762 J. Edquist et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 45 (2012) 759–765

No lead vehicle Slow lead vehicle


.200

right
.100

(metres)
.000

-.100

left
-.200

-.300
Arterial No Urban No Urban Empty Urban Full
Parking Parking Parking Parking
Fig. 3. Standard deviation of speed over several 1 km blocks, by road environ- Fig. 4. Mean lateral position by road environment, parking condition and presence
ment and parking condition (bars represent 95% CIs) *Significant difference between of lead vehicle; 0 represents the centre of the driver’s lane, with negative values
‘Urban No Parking’ and ‘Urban Full Parking’. representing positions left of the centre (bars represent 95% CIs).

Speed variability increased with environmental complexity


targets (less than 2% of the overall sample) were deleted from the
(Fig. 3): the Arterial No Parking condition was associated with
subsequent analyses of reaction time.
significantly less variable speeds than all other conditions, while
Reaction time to peripheral targets during the blocks without
the Urban Full Parking condition was associated with significantly
any lead vehicles was analysed in a repeated measures ANOVA
more variable speeds than the Urban No Parking condition (the
with road environment as a 4-level, within-subjects factor. The
Urban Empty Parking condition was not significantly different from
main effect of road environment was significant, F(3,84) = 45.917,
either the Urban Full Parking or Urban No Parking conditions).
p < .001, 2 = .621. Pairwise comparisons showed that PDT reac-
tion time during the Urban Full Parking condition was significantly
3.2. Headway to lead vehicle slower than during all other conditions, which did not differ from
each other (Fig. 5).
A 3 × 1 RMANOVA for each dependent variable revealed that A second RMANOVA without the data from the Arterial No
there were no significant effects of road environment on mean Parking condition included parking as a 3-level, within-subjects
time headway (F < 1), minimum time headway (F < 1), or time head- factor, and lead vehicle as a 2-level, within-subjects factor. The
way variability (F < 2). Mean overall headway was 2.7 s, minimum main effect of parking condition was significant (F(2,56) = 58.273,
(averaged across participants) was 2.1 s, and variability was 0.37 s. p < .001, 2 = .675). The main effect of the presence of a lead vehicle
was borderline significant, F(1,28) = 4.031, p = .054, 2 = .126. There
3.3. Lateral position was no interaction between parking condition and presence of a
lead vehicle in terms of PDT reaction time.
For mean lateral position data, the blocks without a lead vehicle
were analysed in a repeated measures ANOVA with road environ-
ment as a 4-level, within-subjects factor. The main effect of road 3.5. Response to unexpected pedestrians
environment was significant, F(3,84) = 42.003, p < .001, 2 = .600.
Pairwise comparisons showed that participants drove closest to the In one block each of the Urban No Parking, Empty Parking and
left (kerbside) edge in the Arterial No Parking condition and fur- Full Parking conditions (always with no lead vehicle present), a
thest from the kerb in the Urban Full Parking condition, with the pedestrian suddenly turned from the footpath and walked onto the
Urban No Parking and Empty conditions being in between these road in front of the subject vehicle.
two extremes (Fig. 4). When the blocks with lead vehicles were
included (and the Arterial No Parking condition excluded) in a 3 × 2
2.00
RMANOVA, parking condition was significant, F(2,56) = 56.346,
p < .001, 2 = .668. Presence of a lead vehicle was also significant, No vehicle Slow lead vehicle
Response time (seconds)

F(1,28) = 7.057, p = .013, 2 = .201; participants drove further away


from the kerb in the presence of a lead vehicle. There was no sig-
1.50
nificant interaction between lead vehicle and parking.
There was no significant main effect of parking condition on
lane position variability. Lateral position variability was signifi-
cantly lower in the presence of a lead vehicle: F(1,28) = 35.758,
p < .001, 2 = .561; the standard deviation of lane position (SDLP) 1.00
was 0.144 m (SE = 0.008) without the lead vehicle present and
0.123 m (SE = .007) with the lead vehicle present.

0.50
3.4. Peripheral detection task
Arterial No Urban No Urban Empty Urban Full
Parking Parking Parking Parking
There were only 34 missed PDT targets of a total of 1740 target
presentations across all participants. Thirty-two of these missed Fig. 5. Response time to peripheral icon by road environment, parking condition,
targets (94%) occurred in the Urban Full Parking condition. Missed and presence of lead vehicle (bars represent 95% CIs).
J. Edquist et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 45 (2012) 759–765 763

Table 1
Response to unexpected pedestrian event.

Measure F(2,56) P 2 Difference

Time to accelerator release 39.491 <.001 .585 Full > No park/Empty


Time to brake 32.515 <.001 .537 Full > No park/Empty
Maximum brake pressure 11.691 <.001 .295 Full > No park/Empty
Minimum distance 20.401 <.001 .421 Full < No park/Empty
Minimum time-to-collision (TTC) 20.056 <.001 .417 Full < No park/Empty
Number of collisions 2 (2) = 21.46 <.001 Full > No park/Empty

Dependent measures of participants’ responses to the unex- A second RMANOVA without the Arterial No Parking condition
pected pedestrian event included: time to accelerator release, time involved parking as a 3-level within-subjects factor, and the pres-
to brake, maximum brake pressure, minimum time to collision, ence of a lead vehicle as a 2 level within-subjects factor. The main
minimum distance to the pedestrian, and whether or not the par- effect of parking was significant, F(2,52) = 17.612, p < .001, 2 = .404.
ticipant’s vehicle collided with the pedestrian. The first five of these The Urban No Parking and Empty conditions were not significantly
measures were included in a repeated measures multivariate anal- different from one another, but the Full condition was rated by par-
ysis of variance (MANOVA). The main effect of parking condition ticipants as significantly more demanding in terms of workload
was significant, F(10,106) = 6.063, p < .001, 2 = 364 (using Pillai’s (Fig. 6). Neither presence of a lead vehicle, nor the interaction of
trace); and univariate analyses showed that it was significant for lead vehicle and parking, was significant.
every measure. Table 1 presents the results of the analyses. In every
case, there was a significant difference between the Urban Full Park- 4. Discussion
ing condition and the Urban No Parking/Empty conditions, which
do not differ from each other. As expected, vehicles parked on-street in a complex, urban
There was only one collision during the Urban Empty Parking environment influenced driver behaviour in several ways: drivers
block, after which the driver said that he had seen the pedestrian reported experiencing higher workload, reduced their vehicle
(and braked) but expected that the pedestrian would stop, so had speed, positioned the vehicle further from the kerb towards
not come to a full stop. There were no collisions during the Urban oncoming traffic, reacted more slowly to a peripheral target, and
No Parking block, while 11 of the 29 drivers collided with the pedes- responded more slowly to an unexpected pedestrian when driving
trian during the Urban Full Parking block. in the Urban Full Parking condition compared to the Urban Empty
Time to brake was almost twice as long in the Urban Full Park- condition. Although there was evidence of some compensation by
ing condition (2.00 s) compared to the Urban No Parking condition drivers in response to the increased workload in terms of reduced
(1.12 s). Assuming a friction coefficient of 0.7 during braking, ini- speed and a change in lane position further from the parked vehi-
tial travel speed in the Urban Full Parking condition would need to cles, these changes were not sufficient to protect drivers from an
be 47 km/h (instead of 60 km/h) to achieve the same stopping dis- increase in crash risk. The combination of behaviour changes seen
tance as in the Urban No Parking condition (based on the equation in the driving simulator is therefore likely to translate to higher
of motion: velocity squared = initial velocity squared plus 2 times crash rates in real world, urban areas where vehicles are parked
acceleration times distance, solving for distance, where accelera- on-street.
tion is gravity times friction coefficient). The pattern of compensation to on-street parking is interesting:
of those variables which were significantly affected by parking, sub-
3.6. Mental workload jective workload and speed variability increased the least between
the Urban No Parking and Full Parking conditions, and by about the
Blocks without any lead vehicle present were analysed in a 4 × 1 same amount as speed itself reduced. These results support the con-
RMANOVA with road environment as a within-subjects factor. The cept of mental workload as an important mediator of speed control
main effect of road environment was significant, F(3,84) = 11.948, (e.g. Fuller, 2005; Hoyos, 1988). Reaction time on the PDT increased
p < .001, 2 = .299. Pairwise comparisons showed that the Arterial more than did workload, with response time to the unexpected
No Parking condition was associated with significantly lower work- pedestrian almost doubling in the Urban Full Parking condition
load ratings than all other conditions, and the Urban Full Parking compared to the Urban No Parking condition. There appears to be
condition received significantly higher workload ratings than all a separation between the effects of on-street parking on perceived
other conditions, while Urban No Parking and Urban Empty Parking workload and speed control, and its effects on drivers’ perception-
workload ratings were not significantly different from one another. response time to hazards. This has negative implications for road
safety. Perceived workload could be more dependent on drivers’
30 ability to maintain lateral control, rather than on their ability to
Workload sum (out of 60)

No vehicle Slow lead vehicle


maintain a safe stopping distance. This is supported by findings
25
such as Brooks et al. (2011) simulator study of curve navigation
20 in fog. Although they did not collect workload data, they found
that participants did not slow down until visibility was massively
15 reduced, well below the level at which they could stop in time
10 to avoid hitting a pedestrian hidden in the fog. Further research
is required to confirm this suggestion, and to determine the most
5 appropriate countermeasure.
It should be noted that the effects of parked vehicles on reac-
0
tion time to the PDT and to the unexpected pedestrian were found
Arterial No Urban No Urban Empty Urban Full
even though drivers had slowed down. If response times had been
Parking Parking Parking Parking
adjusted to account for the increased time available when partic-
Fig. 6. Workload ratings by road environment and parking condition (bars represent ipants drove more slowly (as was done by Trick et al., 2010), the
95% CIs). results would have been even more marked.
764 J. Edquist et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 45 (2012) 759–765

The slower free speeds chosen by drivers in the Urban Full Park- Another limitation of the study is that with a moderate fidelity
ing condition were not reflected in longer time headways when simulator there are questions around the extent to which the speed
car-following, perhaps because the time headways drivers chose perception cues available in real world are present. Such cues may
in this study were very conservative (an average of 2.7 s across all help drivers on real roads to slow down more in conditions of
conditions; the standard guideline is 2 s). Lateral position shifted high roadside object density and low pavement width, such as
towards the centre of the lane, which probably reflects the use of the Urban Full Parking condition. Ideally, future research would
the lead vehicle’s position (centred in the lane) as a guide. This may include a wider field of view and more on-road measures to deter-
also account for the finding that lane position variability decreased mine if the effects are of the same magnitude. However, the fact
when a lead vehicle was present. Alternatively, this finding may that the present study showed changes in driving performance and
reflect the amount of attention drivers had available to concentrate workload in the absence of these cues is a testament that useful
on the lateral control portion of the driving task. In the presence of results can be obtained even without the highest degree of simula-
a lead vehicle, longitudinal control could be maintained by moni- tor fidelity.
toring the closeness of the lead vehicle (while looking ahead) rather The present study examined driver behaviour under the instruc-
than the speedometer (requiring a downward glance); in addition, tion to drive at 60 km/h (the speed limit for most connector roads
the lower speed imposed by the lead vehicle would have made the in Australian cities). It would be useful to understand how drivers
task of maintaining a consistent lateral position easier. adapt to on-street parking under a range of speed restrictions (i.e.
Attentional allocation can also explain the variability of lateral speed limits). Previous research has shown that drivers under load
and longitudinal control in the Urban Full Parking condition. In the from a secondary task actually drive faster than when not under
presence of parked cars, which essentially narrowed the available load, if the speed they are asked to drive at is lower than the speed
road width, participants may have paid more attention to main- they would habitually drive at (Lewis-Evans et al., 2011). It is there-
taining their lateral position; thus SDLP remained low compared to fore important to understand how workload induced by the road
the Urban No Parking condition, while speed variability increased. environment interacts with speed restrictions, particularly where
Road environment and complexity appear to influence driving the speed limit is lower than drivers would usually choose in simi-
performance. A number of performance measures showed differ- lar situations (for example, variable speed limits). Further research
ences between the Arterial No Parking condition (where buildings could also examine whether traffic calming devices and/or visual
were few and set far back from the road, and the roadside envi- treatments can successfully change behaviour to match risk levels
ronment was simple) and the Urban No Parking/Empty Parking posed by factors such as on-street parking.
condition (where the roadside environment was visually complex,
comprising continuous shopfronts at a small distance from the road Role of funding source
edge). In the latter conditions, subjective workloads were higher,
speed was lower and more variable, and lateral position was fur- This project was funded through MUARC’s Baseline Research
ther away from the road edge. These are the same effects as those Program for which grants have been received from: the Victorian
found when comparing the Urban No Parking/Empty Parking to the Department of Justice, VicRoads, and the Transport Accident Com-
Urban Full Parking condition, and imply that, in addition to char- mission (TAC). The sponsors gave input into the overall direction
acteristics of the road itself, the visual complexity of the roadside of the research (investigating crashes in urban environments); the
environment is an important contributor to driver workload and design, analysis, interpretation and writing were all performed by
performance. Interestingly, PDT performance remained constant the researchers.
between the Arterial No Parking and the Urban No Parking/Empty
Parking conditions, but was impaired in the Urban Full Parking con- Acknowledgements
dition. Previous research has shown that PDT response times are
affected by environmental complexity in terms of traffic (Nunes The authors would like to thank Mr Ashley Verdoorn for pro-
and Recarte, 2005) and level of visual information (Stinchcombe gramming the driving simulator scenarios and Ms Karen Stephan
and Gagnon, 2010); further research is required to clarify the effect for her contribution towards study design.
of complex backgrounds on peripheral hazard detection.
It is noteworthy that there were no significant differences in References
any of the dependent measures between the Urban No Parking and
Empty Parking conditions. These conditions were identical except Allsop, R., 1995. Reducing Traffic Injuries Resulting From Excess and Inappropriate
for the presence of road markings implying that vehicles were per- Speed. European Transport Safety Council, Brussels.
Brooks, J.O., Crisler, M.C., Klein, N., Goodenough, R., Beeco, R.W., Guirl, C., et al., 2011.
mitted to park on the street. Drivers in the present study were never Speed choice and driving performance in simulated foggy conditions. Accident
directed to park, so it appears that they interpreted these markings Analysis & Prevention 43 (3), 698–705.
as being irrelevant in the absence of any vehicles actually parked Chinn, L., Elliot, M.A., 2002. The effect of road appearance on perceived safe travel
speed: Final report (No. PA3828/02): Transport Research Laboratory.
there. This is consistent with previous research, which has found Daisa, J.M., Peers, J.B., 1997. Narrow residential streets: do they really slow down
little effect on speed of similar markings such as bus lanes and cycle speeds? In: ITE Annual Meeting Compendium, Washington DC: Institute of
lanes unless they are occupied (Elliot et al., 2003). Transportation Engineers, pp. 546–551.
De Waard, D., 1996. The measurement of drivers’ mental workload. The Uni-
versity of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands. From http://dissertations.ub.
4.1. Limitations and future research rug.nl/faculties/ppsw/1996/d.de.waard/.
Elliot, M.A., Mccoll, V.A., Kennedy, J.V., 2003. Road design measures to reduce drivers’
speed via ‘psychological’ processes: A literature review (No. TRL564): Transport
While the present study included both male and female drivers,
Research Laboratory.
of a range of different ages and experience levels, all were car Engstrom, J., Johansson, E., Ostlund, J., 2005. Effects of visual and cognitive load in
drivers who primarily drove as commuters. It is possible that differ- real and simulated motorway driving. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic
Psychology and Behaviour 8 (2), 97–120.
ent risk factors will affect certain driver groups to different extents.
Fuller, R., 2005. Towards a general theory of task behaviour. Accident Analysis &
For example, Tarko (2009) found that pavement width had more Prevention 37, 461–472.
effect on speed for truck drivers than car drivers. Speed selec- Fuller, R., McHugh, C., Pender, S., 2008. Task difficulty and risk in the determina-
tion may be different for drivers such as a taxicab driver, a young tion of driver behaviour. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/European
Review of Applied Psychology 58 (1), 13–21.
driver who drives for excitement, or a cautious older driver who Greibe, P., 2003. Accident prediction models for urban roads. Accident Analysis &
self-restricts, to give a few examples. Prevention 35, 273–285.
J. Edquist et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 45 (2012) 759–765 765

Gugerty, L.J., 1997. Situation awareness during driving: explicit and implicit knowl- Traffic and Transport Psychology Theory and Application: Proceedings of the
edge in dynamic spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied ICTTP 2004. Elsevier, pp. 339–354.
3 (1), 42–66. Pande, A., Abdel-Aty, M., 2009. A novel approach for analyzing severe crash patterns
Harms, L., 1986. Drivers’ attentional response to environmental variations: a dual- on multilane highways. Accident Analysis & Prevention 41 (5), 985–994.
task real traffic study. In: Gale, A.G., Freeman, M.H., Haslegrave, C.M., Smith, P., Parkes, A.M., Luke, T., Burns, P.C., Lansdown, T., 2007. Conversations in cars: the
Taylor, S.P. (Eds.), Vision in Vehicles, I. North-Holland, pp. 131–138. relative hazards of mobile phones (No. 664): Transport Research Laboratory.
Hart, S.G., Staveland, L.E., 1988. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Roberts, I., Norton, R., Jackson, R., Dunn, R., Hassall, I., 1995. Effect of environmental
results of empirical and theoretical research. In: Hancock, P.A., Meshkati, factors on risk of injury of child pedestrians by motor vehicles: a case-control
N. (Eds.), Human Mental Workload. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, pp. study. British Medical Journal 310, 91–94.
139–183. Stinchcombe, A., Gagnon, S., 2010. Driving in dangerous territory: complexity
Horberry, T., Anderson, J., Regan, M.A., Triggs, T.J., Brown, J., 2006. Driver distraction: and road-characteristics influence attentional demand. Transportation Research
the effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 13 (6), 388–396.
on driving performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention 38 (1), 185–191. Summala, H., 1988. Risk control is not risk adjustment: the zero-risk theory of driver
Hoyos, C.G., 1988. Mental load and risk in traffic behaviour. Ergonomics 31 (4), behaviour and its implications. Ergonomics 31 (4), 491–506.
571–584. Tarko, A.P., 2009. Modeling drivers’ speed selection as a trade-off behavior. Accident
Lewis-Evans, B., Charlton, S., 2006. Explicit & implicit processes in behavioural adap- Analysis & Prevention 41 (3), 608–616.
tation to road width. Accident Analysis & Prevention 38, 610–617. Trick, L.M., Toxopeus, R., Wilson, D., 2010. The effects of visibility conditions, traffic
Lewis-Evans, B., de Waard, D., Brookhuis, K.A., 2011. Speed maintenance under cog- density, and navigational challenge on speed compensation and driving perfor-
nitive load – implications for theories of driver behaviour. Accident Analysis & mance in older adults. Accident Analysis & Prevention 42 (6), 1661–1671.
Prevention 43 (4), 1497–1507. van der Horst, R., de Ridder, S., 2007. Influence of roadside infrastructure on driving
Martens, M.H., Comte, S., Kaptein, N.A., 1997. The effects of road design on speed behavior: driving simulator study. Transportation Research Record 2018 (2018),
behaviour: a literature review: TNO Human Factors Research Institute. 36–44.
Nunes, L.M., Recarte, M.A., 2005. Speed, traffic complexity and visual performance: Wilde, G.J.S., 1982. The theory of risk homeostasis: implications for safety and health.
a study on open road. In: Underwood, G., Rothengatter, T., Huguenin, R.D. (Eds.), Risk Analysis 2, 209–225.

You might also like