You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/248577179

Evaluating aggressive behavior in dogs: a comparison of 3 tests

Article  in  Journal of Veterinary Behavior Clinical Applications and Research · July 2008


DOI: 10.1016/j.jveb.2008.04.001

CITATIONS READS

12 435

5 authors, including:

Maya Bräm Marcus G Doherr


Universität Bern Freie Universität Berlin
3 PUBLICATIONS   27 CITATIONS    312 PUBLICATIONS   5,090 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Daniel Mills
University of Lincoln
228 PUBLICATIONS   2,965 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Definition and management of human directed aggressive behaviour of dogs in the Western countries and Japan View project

Ethics of killing: a veterinary approach View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Daniel Mills on 23 February 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Veterinary Behavior (2008) 3, 152-160

RESEARCH

Evaluating aggressive behavior in dogs: a comparison


of 3 tests
Dr. Maya Bräma, Dr. Marcus G. Doherrb, Dr. Doris Lehmannc, Prof. Daniel Millsa,
Prof. Andreas Steigerc

a
Animal Behaviour and Welfare Group, Department of Biological Sciences, University of Lincoln, United Kingdom;
b
Department of Clinical Veterinary Science, Animal Neurology, University of Berne, Berne, Switzerland; and
c
Division of Animal Housing and Welfare, Vetsuisse Faculty, University of Berne, Berne, Switzerland.

KEYWORDS: Abstract This study assessed the consistency with which aggressive behavior occurred across 3 differ-
dog; ent provocation tests that are currently used in practice to evaluate the behavior and safety of dogs. The
aggression; aim of this study was not to validate the tests, but to evaluate tests that are not validated but are nev-
evaluation; ertheless being used in a legal context in Switzerland, by investigating the hypothesis that 3 different
tests; approaches, all claiming to correctly evaluate the behavior of dogs, should be expected to show signif-
comparison icant agreement. The same 60 dogs were tested in 3 behavioral tests being used in Switzerland at the
time of this study in the year 2003 (Test A: Test of the American Staffordshire Terrier Club; Test B:
Halterprüfung; Test C: Test of the Canton of Basel-Stadt). ‘‘Intraspecific behavior’’ and ‘‘interspecific
behavior toward humans’’ that might relate to potential aggressive behavior were of particular interest.
The observed agreement among the 3 tests was compared relative to chance using a k test. Signif-
icant but low levels of agreement were found among the 3 tests for the criterion ‘‘intraspecific behav-
ior’’ (k 5 0.133, P 5 .014), with the highest correlation between Tests A and B (k 5 0.345, P , .001)
and for the criterion ‘‘interspecific behavior’’ (k 5 0.135, P 5 0. 014), with Tests A and B (k 5 0.220,
P 5 .005) showing the highest correlation. However, significant absolute values of k were low in all
cases. In a further analysis, dogs evaluated to show no signs of potential aggression in the test situations
by all 3 tests were eliminated, and the results of the remaining dogs (‘‘interspecific behavior,’’ n 5 23;
‘‘intraspecific behavior,’’ n 5 29) were assessed for disagreement in pairwise combinations using a
McNemar chi-square test. No significant levels of disagreement were found for ‘‘intraspecific behav-
ior,’’ however, for ‘‘interspecific behavior,’’ Tests A and B (P 5 .035), and Tests B and C (P , .001)
differed significantly, with no significant difference between Tests A and B (P 5 0.11). The inconsis-
tency of the results from different tests suggests test bias at the very least and questions the validity of
these tests. Further work examining the validity of each individual test is warranted if they are to be
used in a legal context.
Ó 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Address reprint requests and correspondence to: Dr. Maya Bräm, Animal
Introduction
Behaviour and Welfare Group, Department of Biological Sciences,
University of Lincoln, Riseholme Park, Lincoln, LN2 2LG, UK, Tel: 144 Aggression is one of the most common reasons for
1522 895 481. the referral of dogs to behavior specialists (Blackshaw,
E-mail: mdbraem@smile.ch 1988; Beaver, 1994; Askew, 1996; Stafford, 1996;

1558-7878/$ -see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2008.04.001
Bräm et al Evaluating aggressive behavior in dogs 153

Landsberg et al., 1997; Overall, 1997) and for dogs to be the aim of this study was to assess the intertest reliability
surrendered to shelters (Christensen et al., 2007). Reports of these procedures.
and articles in the popular press about dangerous dogs The intertest reliability was studied by having the same
and dog attacks inevitably give rise to public concern, 60 dogs assessed for the 2 criteria ‘‘interspecific behavior’’
and such media reports can lead to political responses and ‘‘intraspecific behavior’’ by all 3 methods and by
aimed at controlling the problem owing to perceived public comparing the evaluations. The aim of this study was not to
pressure for such action. These decisions may not be based validate the tests, but to evaluate whether these methods
on scientific knowledge. In Great Britain, the Dangerous that all claim to accurately evaluate dog behavior come to
Dog Act of 1997 was enacted in response to such reports the same conclusion and to quantify their level of agree-
and later amended because of serious flaws (Klaassen ment or disagreement.
et al., 1996), as a result of its unusually quick passage
through parliament. In Germany, controversial lists of
‘‘dangerous’’ dog breeds have been established, and several Material and methods
behavioral tests have been developed, dog taxes raised, and
new laws passed. A similar situation exists in France. Swiss
Animals and persons
legislators have also acted on this problem in response to
public demands. In Switzerland, the Cantons of Basel-Stadt
Dogs were recruited through veterinarians, dog clubs,
and Basel-Land have introduced a breed list and have made
and animal behavior therapists in Switzerland and num-
an unvalidated test obligatory for several breeds. Other
bered according to their sequence of application. A total of
cantons are developing other solutions, such as evaluations
60 dogs belonging to 51 owners were tested in the 3 tests;
by individual behavior experts. However, the same question
42 of the owners participated with 1 dog, and 9 with 2 dogs.
is raised in all these countries and cantons: how and who is
As 69 dogs were enrolled, but as some owners withdrew,
to decide which dogs are dangerous?
the identity number of dogs does go beyond 60.
The current rather ad hoc situation, with a plethora of
Of the 60 dogs that participated in this study, 13
approaches and possible tests administered by a wide
belonged to the FCI (Fédération Cynologique Internatio-
variety of different kinds of ‘‘experts,’’ inspired us to look
nale) group 8 (retrievers, flushing dogs, and water dogs), 19
more closely at the methods used to assess ‘‘aggressive
to the FCI group 1 (sheepdogs and cattle dogs), 11 were
behavior.’’ Since 2000 several studies were run in Germany
mixed breeds, and 17 belonged to other pure breeds of the
focusing on the behavior tests which are partially compul-
other FCI groups. Twenty-six were males (11 neutered),
sory for certain breeds in certain ‘‘Bundesländer’’ (federal
and 34 were females (24 spayed). The ages ranged from 1.5
states) (Mittmann, 2002; Bruns, 2003). Mittmann (2002)
years to 11 years of age, with a mean age of 4.6 years and a
studied the occurrence of aggressive behaviors shown by
median age of 3.5 years.
14 listed breeds in the test of Niedersachsen (Niedersäch-
Although 2 of the 3 tests were originally designed for
sisches Ministerium für Ernährung, Landwirtschaft und
particular dog breeds belonging to the ‘‘potentially dangerous
Forsten, 2001) and states that although the behavior test
dog breeds’’ listed in some cantons of Switzerland, it was
is useful in triggering and evaluating aggressive behaviors
decided not to limit the test to any particular breeds. Svartberg
in dogs, she found that only 5% of the dogs belonging to
(2006) did find differences in behavior traits in breeds, how-
the 2 listed categories in Germany showed ‘‘inadequate
ever he states that these are independent of the original histor-
aggressive behaviors’’ in this test. No significant difference
ical function of the breed and are more likely to be related to
was found between the 2 categories of listed dogs, and
the current use of breeding stock. Not limiting the testing to
hence there was no basis to describe these breeds as
particular breeds additionally offered the opportunity of test-
‘‘more dangerous’’ nor prioritize them for testing. Bruns’s
ing a larger number of dogs more readily. As the focus of
(2003) results show that the probability of a dog reacting
this study was a comparison of the evaluations of responses
aggressively in everyday situations is strongly influenced
in the 3 tests, every dog was its own control, and so it is argued
by its handler and the handler’s knowledge about dogs.
that the participating breeds are not of primary importance.
At the time of this study, only 1 test was scientifically
validated to test for aggressive behavior in dogs (Netto and
Planta, 1997), but it had not been widely adopted. Three The 3 tests
other methods currently used to evaluate dog behavior in
Switzerland were chosen for evaluation in this study. Three tests were chosen that had similar aims, but were
Because none of these 3 methods had been scientifically somewhat different in structure. The 3 tests showed the
validated, the use of such tests to inform legal opinion is following similarities: (1) they all had the objective of
a cause for concern. Given the use of these tests in a legal identifying ‘‘potentially dangerous dogs’’; (2) they all
context, it is important that the information they provide is consider ‘‘aggressive behavior’’ as an undesired behavior
at least reliable, and so it was hypothesized that there in particular situations, because it is inappropriate or
should be substantial agreement between them. Therefore, unacceptable in our society and might pose some danger
154 Journal of Veterinary Behavior, Vol 3, No 4, July/August 2008

to other individuals. By contrast, in some tests, such as the On the road (dog on leash):
test for German shepherd dogs in Switzerland, a certain 7. The owner and dog are passed by a cyclist, then a jogger
degree of aggressive behavior is desired (Fuchs et al., from behind and then from the front.
2005); (3) they were all developed by people familiar and 8. The owner and dog pass a ‘‘stimulus’’ dog (9 dogs
experienced with canine behavior; and (4) they were and shared this job during the test) with a handler standing
are currently (2007) used in Switzerland. The 3 tests are to the left. The dog and owner being evaluated turn
structured differently, for example, concerning test location around and walk back the way they came, thereby pass-
(open field, fenced area, kennels), duration of the test, and ing the ‘‘stimulus’’ dog and handler on their right.
degree of security for the participants. 9. The dog is tied to a pole and the owner disappears out of
sight of the dog. The expert walks past the dog noisily
several times.
Test A: Test of the American Staffordshire
Terrier Club (ASTC) of Switzerland Test A lasted about 15 minutes in this study.

This test was designed to recognize potentially danger- Test B: The ‘‘Halterprüfung,’’ Switzerland
ous American Staffordshire terriers or dog–owner teams,
namely, those dogs showing ‘‘undesirable aggressive be- The ‘‘Halterprüfung’’ (‘‘dog handler test’’) is not an
havior’’ and inappropriate owner behavior. In the regula- obligatory test for any dog breed or population category.
tions of this test, the following characteristics are The primary aim of this test is to find out if the owner has
mentioned as ‘‘undesirable’’ for the dogs: aggression, his or her dog under control in everyday situations
disobedience, fearfulness, and low or too low threshold of (‘‘guideability’’). The second goal is to be able to assess
reaction leading to slow or no recovery at all after stress. the basic character/nature of the dog and consequently to be
The following behaviors are considered as ‘‘undesirable’’ able to decide whether the animal is a risk to its surround-
for the owner: uncontrolled and insecure behavior toward ings. The test is divided into 2 parts: Part 1, in which the
his/her dog, and violation of the animal protection laws. present state of the dog is determined before it can pass into
There are 2 parts to this test; the first is located within a the various test conditions of the Part 2, where the dog is off
fenced area where the dog is mostly off-leash, and the lead at all times. Within the aspect of ‘‘guideability’’ the
second is on a quiet road bordering the fenced area. The following points are evaluated: (1) general ‘‘guideability’’;
owners may motivate their dogs as they usually do, that is, (2) ‘‘guideability’’ and behavior under distraction: (3)
using voice, treats, or toys. In the fenced area: ‘‘guideability’’ and behavior in everyday situations; and
(4) ‘‘guideability’’ and behavior in the presence of conspe-
1. The dog is let free to wander around and investigate the
cifics. Within the aspect of character/nature, the following
fenced area (for 1 to 2 minutes), then the owner calls the
points are evaluated: (1) the dog’s reaction to stimuli from
dog back.
the environment (noise, fast movements, sudden influences,
2. The owner motivates the dog to play (with a toy). Dog
objects); (2) the dog’s fearfulness; (3) ‘‘nerve stability’’ in
and owner play for about half a minute, then the dog
everyday situations; (4) the type of risk the dog poses to its
should stop playing when the owner gives the command.
environment; and (5) remedial measures and recommenda-
On command of the expert, the owner resumes playing
tions. The procedure emphasizes obedience and how well
with the dog.
the handler has his or her dog under control, in normal
3. The owner and the dog (off leash) walk through a group
situations and especially in more stressful situations.
of 6 to 9 people moving around.
Part 1 involves:
4. The group of people forms a circle around the owner and
dog. The circle of people first closes, then opens around 1. Walking on-lead
the dog–owner team, walking slowly the first time, and 2. Walking off-lead
running the second time. 3. ‘‘Down’’ out of a movement and ‘‘down’’ with
5. The owner walks the dog back and forth on the leash. After distraction
walking in one direction, the owner asks the dog to sit, 4. Being called back out of a game
then continues to walk with the dog. The owner walks
Part 2 of the test took place on an open field and on a
the dog (on the leash) back and forth a second time. On
path through the woods. The dog is off-lead during the
the way back, the expert walks toward the dog and owner
whole test and is put into the following situations:
and first greets the owner by handshake, then greets the
dog. 1. The dog is confronted with unknown animals (chicken
6. The owner walks the dog to the end of the area and puts the in a pen, 2 goats tied to a tree trunk).
dog into a down position or asks it to sit and gives it the 2. The dog encounters a solitary person riding a bike and
command to stay. The owner walks about 20 steps away 1 person jogging.
from the dog. The expert walks past and around the wait- 3. The dog passes through an active group of people in a
ing dog several times. The owner goes and gets the dog. stressful context (ie, several people forming a circle, in
Bräm et al Evaluating aggressive behavior in dogs 155

the center of which stand the owner and the dog, the 3. Owner lets his or her dog off the leash, and both walk
people in the circle all jump up into the air at the back and forth past the kennels again.
same time and shout when landing). To relax the situa- 4. A stimulus dog (3 intact males shared this job in this
tion, they crouch down afterwards and initiate friendly study) is let out into the second kennel; owner and
physical contact with the dog. dog walk past the kennels again.
4. The dog is confronted with everyday situations such as a 5. The expert leaves the kennel and reads the microchip on
noisy group of people (shouting, singing, shaking tin the dog’s left shoulder.
cans filled with pebbles, etc.) that is walking toward 6. The owner puts his or her dog on the leash again, and
the dog and its owner; the dog must follow its owner they walk through a gate and a narrow passageway.
through this group. The group lines up to form a passage
The duration of this test during this study was about
for the dog and owner and when they pass through, the
5 minutes per dog.
persons drop their noisy objects (bottles filled with peb-
bles, tin cans).
5. The dog is confronted with conspecifics: the dog and its Time schedule
owner must walk through a group of people playing with
their dogs. One test was run per dog per day, with several dogs
assessed each day. The tests were run in a time period of
This test lasted around 10 minutes in this study. 2 weeks in the month of March 2002 on a dog training
grounds in Düdingen, FR, Switzerland, and on the campus
of the School of Veterinary Medicine, Berne, Switzerland.
Test C: The Test of the Canton of Basel-Stadt, The dogs were organized into 6 groups of 10 dogs each. To
Switzerland control for any possible influence of learning on the dogs’
and handlers’ behaviors, the dogs went through the tests in
The aims of this test are: different sequences according to a Latin square design. The
1. To recognize potentially dangerous dogs of 6 nominally owners were not informed about the test results until after
‘‘dangerous dog breeds’’ and dogs of other breeds who they had taken the last test, and the assessors were asked
have shown dangerous behavior. This test has the great- not to give the owners any information on how they had
est legal force in the canton of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland. performed during the test, so as to avoid influencing the
2. To protect humans and other animals from dog owners’ behavior in subsequent tests.
aggression.
3. To protect the dog itself from being kept in conditions Evaluation
not appropriate for its species (Kantonales Veterinäramt
Basel-Stadt, 2001). The 3 participating tests had different ways of evaluating
(ie, different test situations) and scoring (ie, numbering,
Behaviors of both the dog and of the owner are taken into
pass vs non-pass, etc.) to assess different behaviors in dogs,
account in all situations. The following dog behaviors are
but they were all forms of a provocation test. It was
noted: normal; aggressive; threatening; fearful; biting with
therefore proposed to use a common outcome assessment to
or without threatening; the dog leading its owner; aggressive
compare the results of the 3 tests, based on 1-0 recording at
behavior toward the owner; and good, acceptable, or bad
the end of the test of the dog’s response. Two assessors
obedience. Concerning the owner, the following are noted:
were used for each test, with only 1 present in any test
no reaction to the dog’s behavior; correction of the dog
situation, hence no interobserver comparisons were done
(verbally or physically); fearful, relaxed, insecure, or
within the tests. These individuals were chosen because
dominant behavior; the owner has to get the dog because
they were the usual experts in these tests in the everyday
it does not react to the ‘‘come’’ command; and no random or
situation. The experts were therefore very familiar with the
fearful reaction to or any correction of the dog showing
test procedures and experienced with the evaluation of dog
aggressive behavior toward its owner. Great emphasis is put
behavior. To avoid bias of their observations, the assessors
on the security of the experts and dogs present by minimiz-
were not present at any of the other tests and were not
ing possible contact between dog and experts. The experts
informed about the evaluations in the other tests. These six
and the stimulus dog are both protected by a kennel, and the
experts (2 experts per test) were asked to evaluate the dogs’
owner and dog that are being tested pass by outside these
behavior in both intraspecific and interspecific (toward
kennels. The test consists of the following situations:
humans) contexts throughout the test with the following
1. First contact of expert and dog–owner team, separated scoring system (Figure 3): (1) open, friendly, neutral behav-
by a gate. The expert goes into a kennel and asks the ior; (2) dominant behavior or mistrustful, fearful behavior,
team to enter the fenced-in area. without aggression; (3) threatening, warning; (4) overt
2. Owner and dog on the leash walk past the kennels as if aggression/attack/biting with threatening/warning; (5) overt
on a normal walk. aggression/attack/biting without threatening/warning. This
156 Journal of Veterinary Behavior, Vol 3, No 4, July/August 2008

Table 1 Descriptive Results of the Comparison of the Three Tests A, B and C, Intraspecific Behavior towards other Dogs
Absolute values Percentage of total minus missing values
tests same diff missing Total same diff Total
A-C 34 24 2 60 59% 41% 100%
A-B 41 13 6 60 76% 24% 100%
C-B 32 22 6 60 59% 41% 100%
A-C-B 25 29 6 60 46% 54% 100%
Same 5 same answers, diff 5 different answers, missing 5 missing values.

scale was newly created in collaboration with all 6 experts this analysis, the software STATA 7 (www.stata.com) was
and based on an analysis of several tests (among others, the used.
test of Niedersachsen, Germany [Niedersächsisches Minis- To provide a more rigorous assessment of dogs who
terium für Ernährung, Landwirtschaft und Forsten, 2001], reacted with aggression to a test situation in some way,
the test for aggressive behavior by Planta and Netto, the that is, those showing any evidence of aggressive poten-
Netherlands [1997; 1999; 2001], and the 3 participating tial (including fearful and mistrustful behaviors), we
tests). The assessors used this scoring system to evaluate cross-tabulated the data relating to any dog who was
the dogs only for this study, hence the usual scoring recorded to have reacted in an aversive way in any of the
systems for the tests were not of relevance for this study. tests (ie, any dog that was evaluated by at least 1 test to
With every case, the assessors had the option to add not show open, friendly, neutral behavior), and we used
comments. McNemar’s chi-square test of association to undertake
pairwise comparisons. The exclusion of dogs who showed
open, friendly, neutral behaviors in all tests reduced the
Statistical Analysis risk of error because a large proportion of the population
consistently showed no aggressive potential as a result of
For the comparison of the results of the 3 tests among sample bias.
the 60 dogs, a k test statistic was used. This statistic
assesses the level of agreement between raters (in this
study, the 3 tests are considered ‘‘raters’’) that is beyond Results
what would be expected by chance alone. An extension of k
within the statistics software STATA 7 can be used to study The level of agreement between tests is shown in Tables
the agreement of 3 assessments, by calculating a weighted 1 and 2. Figures 1 and 2 show the results for all dogs. Miss-
average of each individual k. Absolute k values can range ing data, as a result of dogs not showing up for a particular
from 0 to 1. Conventions for the interpretation of k suggest test or assessors not scoring a dog, are listed as ‘‘missing
less than 0.2 indicates slight agreement, values between values’’ in Tables 1 and 2. The 3 tests appear to agree on
0.21 and 0.40 indicate fair agreement, values between 0.41 dogs that were evaluated to show open, friendly, neutral
and 0.60 indicate moderate agreement, values between 0.61 behavior (score 1 on the evaluation scale). These were
and 0.80 indicate substantial (high) agreement, and values the majority of the subjects. Slight agreement between
over 0.81 indicate very high agreement between 2 raters the results of the 3 tests was suggested by the weighted av-
(Thursfield, 1995). Two sets of tests were run: (1) a pair- erage of the individual k for both the criteria ‘‘intraspecific
wise comparison of the agreement of the individual tests, behavior’’ (k 5 0.133, P 5 .014) and ‘‘interspecific behav-
and (2) a comparison of the agreement of all 3 tests. For ior toward humans’’ (k 5 0.135, P 5 .014).

Table 2 Descriptive Results of the Comparison of the Three Tests A, B and C, Interspecific Behavior towards Humans

Absolute values Percentage of total minus missing values


tests same diff missing Total same diff Total
A-C 44 13 3 60 77% 23% 100%
A-B 35 19 6 60 65% 35% 100%
C-B 36 19 5 60 65% 35% 100%
A-C-B 31 25 4 60 55% 45% 100%
Same 5 same answers, diff 5 different answers, missing 5 missing values.
Bräm et al Evaluating aggressive behavior in dogs 157

Interspecific aggression 1-29


5

aggression scale
4

3 test A
test B
2 test C

0
1 2 5 6 7 8 9 10 12 14 15 16 17 19 20 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 30 31 32 33 34 35 36
dog number

Interspecific aggression 30-60


5
aggression scale

3 test A
test B
2 test C

0
37 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69
dog number
A = Test of the American Staffordshire Terrier Club, B = “Halterprüfung”, C = Test of the Canton of Basel-Stadt
1 = open, friendly, neutral behavior; 2 = dominant behavior, mistrustful, fearful behavior without aggression,
3 = threatening behavior, 4 = attack with threat, 5 = attack without threat
NB: the numbers labeling the individual dogs are not equal to the total number in the population due to some of the dogs not participating

Figure 1 Interspecific behavior towards humans.

Twenty-three dogs showing some signs of potential behavior’ (ie, scores 2 to 5 in the original assessment).
aggression in an interspecific context in at least 1 of the 3 The distribution of subjects between tests was assessed
tests and 29 dogs showing some signs of potential aggres- using McNemar’s test for pairwise comparisons. As the
sion in an intraspecific context were evaluated as a separate sample size is rather small (n 5 23 for interspecific
population owing to their potential significance. The score behavior, n 5 29 for intraspecific behavior), P values of
of the dogs was then collapsed into 2 categories: ‘open, around .1 or less were considered to be of interest. A signif-
friendly, neutral behavior’ and ‘potentially aggressive icant difference was found between Tests A and B (P 5

Intraspecific aggression 1-29


5
aggression scale

3 test A
test B
2 test C

0
1 2 5 6 7 8 9 10 12 14 15 16 17 19 20 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 30 31 32 33 34 35 36
dog number

Intraspecific aggression 30-60


5
aggression scale

3 test A
test B
2 test C

0
37 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69
dog number
A = Test of the American Staffordshire Terrier Club, B = “Halterprüfung”, C = Test of the Canton of Basel-Stadt
1 = open, friendly, neutral behavior; 2 = dominant behavior, mistrustful, fearful behavior without aggression,
3 = threatening behavior, 4 = attack with threat, 5 = attack without threat
NB: the numbers labeling the individual dogs are not equal to the total number in the population due to some of the dogs not participating

Figure 2 Intraspecific behavior towards other dogs.


158 Journal of Veterinary Behavior, Vol 3, No 4, July/August 2008

Criteria Answers and define behavior terms in the same way and whether it is
Intraspecific aggression 1 open, friendly, neutral
2 dominant behavior or mistrustful, fearful, without
possible to generalize a dog’s behavior from one day to
aggression another. The question of being able to generalize behaviors
3 threatening, warning
Interspecific aggression 4 overt aggression / attack / bite with threatening / noted during temperament tests in everyday situations has
warning
5 overt aggression / attack / bite without been investigated by Christensen et al. (2007). They ran a
threatening / warning prospective study on 67 dogs that had successfully passed
Figure 3 Evaluation score. a temperament test at an animal shelter, after these dogs
were re-homed. They found that certain types of aggressive
tendencies (territorial, predatory, and intraspecific aggres-
.035) and between Tests B and C (P , .001) for the crite- sion, in particular) were not detected efficiently. Other
rion ‘‘interspecific behavior,’’ and the difference between studies have found that certain tests are predictive with par-
Tests A and C (P 5 .109) was inconclusive. The criterion ticular personality traits but less so with others (Svartberg,
‘‘intraspecific behavior’’ showed no convincing evidence 2005).
of significant disagreement in all 3 comparisons (P . .15 One possible explanation for the higher agreement of
in all cases). Tests A and B might relate to the way the dog’s behavior
toward other dogs was evaluated in the tests. Tests A and B
both applied no restrictions on the sex of the stimulus dog,
whereas Test C required that only an intact male dog could
Discussion be the stimulus dog. The developers of this test argue this is
There have been several studies evaluating individual tests sufficient, as this is where most of the problems occur.
(Serpell and Hsu, 2001; Mittmann, 2003; Bruns, 2003; However, it was in this test that a 4-year-old female intact
Fuchs, 2005), but to the authors’ knowledge, this is the first dog (dog number 41, Figure 2) passed the test without prob-
report of any attempt to practically compare different forms lems but later attacked another female neutered dog in-
of tests currently used in the field to evaluate potential ag- volved in the experiment.
gressive behaviors in dogs, despite the widespread use of The surroundings of Test C also differ markedly from
such tests to make important decisions of both legal and those of the other 2 tests, as the whole test is held within an
welfare significance. Diederich et al. (2006) have reviewed enclosed area, and the tested dog is confronted with a
different methodologies used in behavioral evaluations and person or dog in a kennel, thereby is separated always from
found that, at least on a theoretical basis, there is little con- them by bars. This situation, on the one hand, increases the
sensus on the parameters and procedures used to evaluate security of all individuals involved and makes it easier to
behavior in dogs. Taylor and Mills (2006) have emphasized standardize the test, but on the other hand, it is somewhat
the need for tests to be validated and have raised concern artificial and might decrease the capacity to observe the
over the quality of published tests. These results support dog’s behavior in detail.
this growing expression of concern over the caliber of
many tests in current use.
Interspecific behavior of the tog toward human
beings
Intraspecific behavior of the dog
Overall agreement between the tests was significantly
The weighted average of the individual k over the 3 tests better than chance and slightly higher for ‘‘interspecific
suggests the 3 tests are not entirely independent, but the behavior toward human beings’’ compared to ‘‘intraspecific
absolute level of agreement is low. Considering that these behavior,’’ however the values are still low and there were
tests all evaluated the same 60 dogs and all of the tests significant differences between the tests, with Test B
claim to be able to evaluate correctly the behavior of the appearing less similar to the other tests (65% agreement
dogs, an overall level of agreement of less than 0.2 is of with both A and C). The 3 tests agreed overall in 55% of the
concern even without significant differences between the cases, and this result was 73% between Tests A and C.
tests. The level of agreement between Tests A and B at 76% Nonetheless, the McNemar test shows a significant differ-
is markedly higher than the agreement of either of these ence between Tests A and C and Tests B and C in the
tests with Test C (both 59%), suggesting that Test C may be evaluations of the dogs that did not show open, friendly,
quite different than the other two. The agreement across all neutral behavior in all 3 tests. In 77% (n 5 10) of the 13 cases
3 tests is even lower (46%). Of further concern is that it is in which Tests A and C did not agree (Figures 3 and 4), Test A
evident that disagreement mainly concerns dogs that were evaluated the dog higher on the scale, whereas the opposite
evaluated as showing some signs of potential aggressive was the case in only 23% (n 5 3). This finding suggests
behavior by at least one of the tests, that is, unreliability is that Test A was perhaps more provocative than Test C. Test
greatest with the dogs of potential interest for public safety. B evaluated the dogs higher on the scale than Test C in
This finding questions whether the raters evaluate behavior 33% of the cases, with the opposite found in only 2% of the
Bräm et al Evaluating aggressive behavior in dogs 159

cases (Figures 3 and 4). Test B also has the tendency to eval- Conclusions
uate the dogs higher on the scale than Test A. In Test B, there
are several situations where the interactions are potentially In spite of the homogeneity of the population tested, the
quite stressful (involving loud noise, people forming a circle reliability and hence the validity of these behavioral tests
around the dog, people jumping up and down). These stress- used for assessing behavior in dogs in Switzerland should be
ful situations may be more likely to elicit fearful, defensive questioned with regard to their significance in evaluating
aggressive behavior and have a cumulative effect, increasing aggressive behavior. We suggest that validity should not be
the score of the dogs. Evidence of significant differences be- assumed from the outset. Rather, we suggest validity should
tween the tests in the assessment of potential aggressive be- be a prerequisite of use, especially in legal contexts, and at the
havior toward human beings is of particular public health very least additional methods of evaluation, such as history of
concern. However, the question again arises whether any of the dog, its lifestyle, the owner’s behavior, and so on, should
these tests is predictive of real risk in the wider community. be taken into account when making an assessment.
As Christensen et al. (2007) and Svartberg (2005) have
shown, predictability varies with the behavior being evalu-
ated in the tests, and care must be taken to generalize to a Acknowledgments
dog’s temperament or character based on the results of a sin-
gle test without taking the dog’s history and behavior in ev- We thank the Margaret and Francis Fleitmann Founda-
eryday situations into consideration. The variability of tion, Switzerland for financing this project.
these results supports this caution in the applicability of
single-test results.
The dogs of most interest to public safety are probably References
those that show some sort of averse behavior, since
aggression is frequently a response to perceived aversion. Askew, H.R., 1996. Treatment of Behavioral Problems in Dogs and Cats:
A Guide for the Small Animal Veterinarian. Blackwell Scientific Pub-
As all the dogs were recruited by word of mouth or through lications, Oxford.
dog training schools, the selection of dogs that participated Beaver, B.V., 1994. Owner complaints about canine behavior. J. Am. Vet.
in this study is not representative of the general population. Med. Assoc. 204, 1953-1955.
It is likely that the population used for this study was biased Blackshaw, J.K., 1988. Abnormal behaviour in dogs. Aust. Vet. J. 65,
in the direction of open, friendly, neutral dogs whose 393-394.
Bruns, S., 2003. Fünf Hunderassen und ein Hundetypus im Wesenstest
owners actively work with them, hence the elimination of nach der Niedersächsischen Gefahrtier-Verordung vom 05.07.2000:
all the dogs evaluated by all 3 tests to show open, friendly, Faktoren, die beissende von nicht-beissenden Hunden unterscheiden,
neutral behavior. For the remaining dogs, the McNemar test Inaugural-Dissertation zur Erlangung des Grades einer Doktorin der
suggests a significant disagreement between Tests A and B Veterinärmedizin, Hannover.
and Tests B and C and marginal disagreement between Christensen, E., Scarlett, J., Campagna, M., Houpt, K.A., 2007. Aggressive
behavior in adopted dogs that passed a temperament test. Appl. Anim.
Tests A and C. This level of disagreement raises the Behav. Sci. 106, 85-95.
question of at least one and possibly all 3 tests being Diederich, C., Giffroy, J., 2006. Behavioural testing in dogs. A review of
invalid. This question is of importance, because the welfare methodology in search for standardisation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.
of dogs incorrectly evaluated to be a risk is compromised 97, 51-72.
by potentially imposing unnecessary measures on them, Fuchs, T., Gaillard, C., Gebhardt-Henrich, S., Ruefenacht, S., Steiger, A.,
2005. External factors and reproducibility of the behaviour test in
depriving them of important needs, which potentially also German shepherd dogs in Switzerland. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 94,
indirectly influences the owners’ well-being. On the other 287-301.
hand, animals falsely evaluated to be ‘‘safe’’ are a source of Kantonales Veterinäramt Basel-Stadt, 2001. Pressekonferenz ‘‘Potentiell
danger to the public if these tests create a false sense of gefährliche Hunde: Erfahrungen mit der revidierten Hundegesetzge-
security. One aspect not considered in this study is the bung,’’ 1. Oktober 2001, http://www.veterinaeramt.bs.ch.
Klaassen, B., Buckley, J.R., Esmail, A., 1996. Does the Dangerous Dog
repeatability of the tests, that is, whether the results are Act protect against animal attacks: a prospective study of mammalian
similar when the test is repeated. This issue poses some bites in the Accident and Emergency department. Injury. 27, 89-91.
difficulties when testing behavior, as it is very difficult to Landsberg, G., Hunthausen, W., Ackerman, L., 1997. Handbook of behav-
eliminate the influence of the participating animals learning ior problems of the dog and cat. Butterworth-Heinemann, Oxford.
from one session to the next. A larger population of dogs, if Mittmann, A., 2002. Untersuchung des Verhaltens von 5 Hunderassen und
einem Hundetypus im Wesenstest nach den Richtlinien der Nieder-
possible balanced for breed, age, sex, training experience, sächsischen Gefahrtierverordnung, Hannover.
and so on would be necessary to test the repeatability of Netto, W.J., Planta, D.J.U., 1997. Behavioural testing for aggression in the
these tests. Further evidence of the validity, repeatability, domestic dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 52, 243-263.
and usefulness of specific tests to evaluate aggressive Niedersächsisches Ministerium für Ernährung, Landwirtschaft und
behavior and potential dangerousness in dogs is necessary Forsten, 2001. Wesenstest für Hunde, Zusammenfassung der Referate
des Vets2001 Kongresses, Fribourg, Schweiz, I – XXIX. http://www.
and urgently required, given current political trends. The gstsvs.ch.
interested reader may refer to Taylor and Mills (2006) for a Overall, K.L., 1997. Clinical Behavioral Medicine for Small Animals.
guide to the development of valid tests. Mosby, Inc., St. Louis, MO.
160 Journal of Veterinary Behavior, Vol 3, No 4, July/August 2008

Serpell, J.A., Hsu, Y., 2001. Development and validation of a novel method Svartberg, K., 2006. Breed-typical behaviour in dogsdhistorical remnants
for evaluating behavior and temperament in guide dogs. Appl. Anim. or recent constructs? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 96, 293-313.
Behav. Sci. 72, 347-364. Taylor, K., Mills, D.S., 2006. The development and assessment of temper-
Stafford, K.J., 1996. Opinions of veterinarians regarding aggression in dif- ament tests for adult companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res.
ferent breeds of dogs. N Z Vet. J. 44, 138-141. 105, 358-368.
Svartberg, K., 2005. A comparison of behaviour in test and in everyday Thursfield, M., 1995. Veterinary Epidemiology, 2nd Ed.. Blackwell Science,
life: evidence of three consistent boldness-related personality traits London.
in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 91, 103-128. http://www.wolfsprung-kennels.ch, May 17, 2003.

View publication stats

You might also like