You are on page 1of 19

REVIEW ARTICLE Sports Med 2003; 33 (1): 13-31

0112-1642/03/0001-0013/$30.00/0

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved.

The ‘Yips’ in Golf


A Continuum Between a Focal Dystonia and Choking
Aynsley M. Smith,1 Charles H. Adler,2 Debbie Crews,3 Robert E. Wharen,4
Edward R. Laskowski,1 Kelly Barnes,1 Carolyn Valone Bell,1 Dave Pelz,5 Ruth D. Brennan,1
Jay Smith,1 Matthew C. Sorenson1 and Kenton R. Kaufman6
1 Sports Medicine Center, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota, USA
2 Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona, USA
3 Department of Exercise Science, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, USA
4 Department of Neurosurgery, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, Florida, USA
5 Pelz Golf Institute, Austin, Texas, USA
6 Orthopedic Biomechanics Laboratory, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota, USA

Contents
Abstract . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
1. Dystonia, a Neurologic Disorder . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14
2. A Dystonia Perspective on the ‘Yips’ in Golf . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
3. Choking as a Phenomenon in Sport . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
4. A Choking Perspective of the ‘Yips’ in Golf . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
5. Choking vs Dystonia in Golf . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
6. Performance Anxiety in Golfers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
7. Neuroscience Considerations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20
8. Alcohol and β-Blockers to Reduce Performance Anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21
9. Golfers’ Perceptions of the ‘Yips’: An Investigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
9.1 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
9.2 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24
9.3 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26
9.4 Limitations and Conclusions of the Present Investigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28
10. Future Research Directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28
11. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29

Abstract The definition of the ‘yips’ has evolved over time. It is defined as a motor
phenomenon of involuntary movements affecting golfers. In this paper, we have
extended the definition to encompass a continuum from the neurologic disorder
of dystonia to the psychologic disorder of choking. In many golfers, the patho-
physiology of the ‘yips’ is believed to be an acquired deterioration in the function
of motor pathways (e.g. those involving the basal ganglia) which are exacerbated
when a threshold of high stress and physiologic arousal is exceeded. In other
golfers, the ‘yips’ seems to result from severe performance anxiety. Physically,
the ‘yips’ is manifested by symptoms of jerks, tremors or freezing in the hands
and forearms. These symptoms can result in: (i) a poor quality of golf performance
(adds 4.9 strokes per 18 holes); (ii) prompt use of alcohol and β-blockers; and
14 Smith et al.

(iii) contribute to attrition in golf. Golfers with the ‘yips’ average 75 rounds per
year, although many ‘yips’-affected golfers decrease their playing time or quit to
avoid exposure to this embarrassing problem. While more investigation is needed
to determine the cause of the ‘yips’, this review article summarises and organises
the available research. A small study included in this paper describes the ‘yips’
phenomenon from the subjective experience of ‘yips’-affected golfers. The sub-
jective experience (n = 72) provides preliminary support for the hypothesis sug-
gesting that the ‘yips’ is on a continuum. Based on the subjective definitions of
72 ‘yips’-affected golfers, the ‘yips’ was differentiated into type I (dystonia) and
type II (choking). A theoretical model provides a guide for future research on
golfers with either type I or type II ‘yips’.

Many golfers, athletes,[1-6] teaching profession- ence and leads to attrition in golf. A review of dys-
als,[7-10] sport scientists[11-29] and medical investi- tonia,[36-40] choking[10,13,14,28,41-47] and performance
gators and clinicians[30-35] are confused by the anxiety[29,47-51] contributes to our current under-
‘yips’, a putting problem which affects profes- standing of the ‘yips’.
sional and amateur golfers. The ‘yips’ is defined as Symptoms in golfers with the ‘yips’ are either:
a jerk, tremor or freezing in the distal upper ex- (i) attributable to a specific focal dystonia; (ii)
tremity[18,26,31-34] that interrupts the putting stroke. choking; or (iii) a consequence of the interaction
Some believe this is the result of choking (a psy- of features of both which renders the pathophysi-
chologic failure to perform), while others believe ology difficult to decipher. Symptoms may also
this is due to a focal, task-specific dystonia. One progress along the continuum just as the ‘yips’ in
teaching professional described the ‘yips’ as a the McDaniel[33] study progressed from one hand
‘fail-safe shutdown’ caused by a deterioration of to the other within 4 years. For example, some golf-
confidence rooted in unsound stroke mechanics.[7] ers may have symptoms of dystonia and become so
Our group recently described the ‘yips’ as a psy- anxious and obsessed about their putting problem
choneuromuscular impediment to execution of the that it is impossible to separate the factors in the
putting stroke in golf.[26] Sachdev[34] called the interaction. Conversely, chokers may also exhibit
‘yips’ an occupational cramp or dystonia. These the ‘double pull’ phenomenon, which mimics the
definitions of the ‘yips’, albeit confusing, describe agonist/antagonist behaviour reported in dystonia,
symptoms of both dystonia (a neurologic disorder) an intriguing neurologic disorder.
and choking (a psychologic disorder). We believe
that the ‘yips’ should be defined as a motor phe- 1. Dystonia, a Neurologic Disorder
nomenon of involuntary movements affecting Dystonia is a neurologic disorder characterised
golfers, with multiple possible aetiologies, span- by involuntary movements resulting in spasms,
ning a continuum from the neurologic disorder of twisting and posturing of a body part. The key fac-
dystonia to the psychologic disorder of choking tor that differentiates this movement disorder from
(table I). The pathophysiology of the ‘yips’ in others is that there is a sustained posture that occurs
many golfers may be an acquired deterioration at some time during the movement. It is estimated
in the function of the basal ganglia and a conse- that the prevalence of generalised dystonia (multi-
quence of extreme performance anxiety in others. ple body parts affected) is between 0.2–6.7 indi-
Symptoms are exacerbated when a threshold of viduals per 100 000 while the prevalence of focal
high stress and arousal is exceeded. Physically, dystonia (one body part affected) is at least 30 in-
the ‘yips’ is manifested by jerks, tremors or freez- dividuals per 100 000.[37] Dystonia may be classi-
ing that result in a poor motor performance. The fied by the age of onset, the aetiology or the distri-
‘yips’ reduces the quality of the golfing experi- bution. Dystonia may be focal: involving a single

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf
© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved.
Table I. Summary of comparison across ‘yips’ studies
‘Yips’ study Study design Definition of ‘yips’ Aetiology Recruitment and Eligibility Golf experience Putts most Conclusions
sample (y)/hcp troublesome
McDaniel et al.[33] Questionnaire Motor Increased Mailed to 1050 99% onset of ‘Yips’ (35.6); non- 0.3–2.4m (1–8 Anxiety
phenomenon of workload, 77% PGA, USGA and S&S during ‘yips’ (31.0) feet) from cup in exacerbates
involuntary performance LPGA golfers: tournaments, tournaments ‘yips’, but not
movements anxiety, basal 42% (n = 441) acquired S&S sole cause
affecting golfers ganglia, subcortical returned, of after 20 years
biochemical whom 93 golfers of golf
changes due to reported having
aging the ‘yips’
Sachdev[34] Psychometrics, Occupational From 19–20th Recruited from Golfers ‘Yips’ (35.1)/10.7. Tournaments Anxiety
medical cramp in golf. century ‘yips’ Australian golf acquired Added 4.7 strokes exacerbates
histories, neuro Term did not review: organic clubs, 20 golfers symptoms ‘yips’, but not
exam/RT appear until disorders, physical with ‘yips’, 20 affecting short sole cause
1977 causes, anxiety, golfers without game, normal
psychological ‘yips’, played neuro and
causes, idiopathic competitive golf muscle testing,
dystonia for 5 years at golfers had
high level ‘trick strategies’
Smith et al.[26] Phase I Psychoneuromotor Psychological 1042 returned Golfers ‘Yips’ (30.4)/4.45; 0.3–1.5m (1–5 The ‘yips’ is on a
questionnaire, impediment to (anxiety), questionnaires, requested to non-‘yips’ (30.8)/ feet) from cup in continuum of
phase II pilot execution of the neurological 461 ‘yips’ participate if 4.53 tournaments choking (anxiety)
study putting stroke in (dystonia) and retained they had been and focal dystonia
golf repetitive motor golfing a long
movements time and had a
(evidenced by hcp of under
acquired ‘yips’) 12. This was
done to ensure
the problem
was ‘acquired’
Present study Phase III a A motor Deterioration in the Recruited via Golfers ‘Yips’ hcp = 6.6 Tournaments Provides initial
questionnaire phenomenon of function of motor ‘yips’ website, requested to support for ‘yips’
involuntary pathways (e.g. direct mailings participate if continuum with
movements those involving the and flyers posted they had been anchors of focal
affecting golfers basal ganglia), in golf locker golfing a long dystonia and
that includes a exacerbated by rooms. Sample time and had a choking
continuum of those high arousal in was the analysis hcp of under
with dystonia and golfers with of questionnaires 12. This was
Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)

those who are dystonia; from 72 ‘yips’- done to ensure


chokers. The ‘yips’ symptoms affected golfers the problem
is manifested by attributed to severe who were invited was ‘acquired’
jerks, tremors and performance to participate
freezing anxiety in chokers
hcp = handicap; LPGA = Ladies Professional Golf Association; PGA = Professional Golf Association; RT = reaction time; S&S = signs and syptoms; USGA = United States Golf
Association.

15
16 Smith et al.

body part; segmental: involving adjacent body expanded upon by Uitti[52] who suggested that ac-
parts (face and neck); hemi: involving multiple tion dystonia implies an involuntary posturing
unilateral parts; multifocal: involving multiple ad- (dystonia) superimposed on a voluntary move-
jacent or non-adjacent parts; and generalised: in- ment. Task-specific limb dystonia, a subset of ac-
volving the legs as well as other body parts. tion dystonia, occurs primarily when a specific task
Generalised dystonias are usually hereditary or is performed. The upper limb is most often in-
drug-induced, while focal and multifocal dystonias volved. Tasks affected require either: (i) highly re-
are more often sporadic. Most adults afflicted with petitive movements; (ii) extreme motor precision
dystonia have a focal or segmental dystonia. Task- and an interplay between conscious, or at least
specific dystonias may affect writing (writer’s feedback-related modulation; and (iii) a repeti-
cramp), as well as activities performed by dentists, tively executed motor plan. Putting meets these cri-
musicians[38] and stenographers.[32] Individuals teria.
with writer’s cramp often can use their hands nor- The underlying pathophysiology of dystonia re-
mally for most activities except writing. When they mains unclear. Some investigators have implicated
begin to write, they will often describe an involun- an abnormality in the dopaminergic system in the
tary jerk or pull resulting in flexion or extension of basal ganglia, but neuropathologic studies have
one or more fingers or the wrist. Musicians may been inconclusive. Secondary, or symptomatic,
develop a similar involuntary flexion or extension dystonias may be caused by medications (espe-
of the fingers or wrist that interferes with playing cially the dopamine blocking drugs), metabolic
wind instruments, string instruments or using diseases, head injury and stroke.
drumsticks. Other professionals, including dentists
and surgeons, have developed an involuntary 2. A Dystonia Perspective on the ‘Yips’
movement that interferes with the fine movements in Golf
of the fingers or hands that most often occurs with In 1989, 42% of 1050 professional and amateur
specific motions or tasks. When the dystonia in- male golfers responded to a 69-item question-
volves multiple activities, it is called a generic limb naire,[33] 28% experienced the ‘yips’. Respondents
dystonia. averaged 36 years of age, 21 years of golf experi-
The ‘yips’ also appears in many golfers to be a ence prior to symptoms, and 60% reported freezing
task-specific dystonia.[37] The incidence of dysto- on short putts. Symptoms in the ‘yips’-affected
nia in professionals who are required to perform progressed from one hand to the other in 60% of
skilled, repetitive hand movements is about one in respondents in 1 year. Symptoms were most prob-
3400. Dystonia affects between one in 200 and one lematic during tournaments (99%), and a tendency
in 500 musicians.[38] The aetiology of dystonia toward obsessional thinking was reported.
may be primary, idiopathic, hereditary, sporadic or In a comprehensive study in 1992 of 20 Austra-
secondary;[39] yet, the actual pathophysiology is lian ‘yips’-affected golfers and their matched con-
not understood. Symptoms include a repetitive trols, no psychopathology was detected in the
twisting, turning and posturing that can be phasic ‘yips’-affected golfers.[34] Neuropsychologic tests
or tonic and, to date, there is no known, effective of mental and motor speed and visual motor coor-
treatment. dination did not differ between groups, and golfers
The Scientific Advisory Board of the Dystonia with ‘yips’ reported less alcohol use. A slightly
Medical Research Foundation, in 1984, agreed to higher tendency towards obsessional thinking and
define dystonia as a “syndrome of sustained mus- self-reported anxiety in the golfers with ‘yips’ was
cle contractions, frequently causing twisting or re- interpreted to be a consequence of the ‘yips’, not
petitive movements, or abnormal postures.”[52] In the cause.[33,34] Both studies[33,34] concluded the
1995, the definition specific to limb dystonia was ‘yips’ may be a focal dystonia exacerbated by, but

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 17

not caused by, anxiety (table I). The ‘yips’ in golf golfers. All golfers used an instrumented standard-
has been attributed to mechanisms such as: (i) bio- length putter, a standard grip (thumbs forward) and
chemical changes in the brain that accompany a non-compensated stance. HR, GF and EMG ac-
aging; (ii) excessive use or overuse of involved tivity in the wrist flexors and extensors were all
muscles; (iii) intense coordination and concentra- greater in ‘yips’-affected golfers.
tion demands; (iv) focal dystonia of the limb; and Putting performance in golfers with and without
(v) stress and obsessive thoughts.[32-34] In addition, ‘yips’[26] during specific scenarios difficult for the
electrophysiologic analysis of limb dystonia re- ‘yips’-affected, demonstrated that golfers with
veals co-contractions of agonist and antagonist ‘yips’ had greater error (distance ball ended up
muscles of the forearm which supports the notion from the cup), made fewer first putts and averaged
of a focal dystonia of the limb.[40] five of ten compared with nine of ten consecutive
In the first phase of a third study, a multidisci- putts made by golfers without ‘yips’. Whether
plinary team of investigators met with golf profes- these golfers experienced dystonia, were chokers
sionals in 1997 to discuss the ‘yips’.[26] A ques- or experienced symptoms of both was not clear.
tionnaire incorporating the opinions of teaching
professionals and the investigative team was 3. Choking as a Phenomenon in Sport
mailed to 2630 golfers with a handicap (hcp) of Because of the role of anxiety in sport and eval-
less than 12. To ensure the 1031 (39%) respon- uative performances, researchers have sought to
dents were good golfers who had acquired the understand the arousal and performance relation-
‘yips’ and not just bad putters, only males with a ship.[43-51,53] The cognitive appraisal model (figure
10 and under hcp (453 ‘yips’ and 393 non-‘yips’) 1) depicts some of the events that occur when a
and females under 12 hcp (23 ‘yips’ and 20 non- golfer chokes. For example, factors such as sup-
‘yips’) were included in the analysis (n = 889). portive audiences may negatively impact on per-
Golfers with and without ‘yips’ were similar in formance by causing athletes or performers to fo-
age, health status, average games played per sea- cus attention on themselves instead of on their
son, playing experience and hcp.[26] Inclusion cri- task.[43,44,53] Choking, an extreme manifestation of
teria of golfers with ‘yips’ ensured that only golf- performance anxiety, is a suboptimal performance
ers who had been good putters prior to their that results from self-focused or distracted atten-
episodic symptoms (average of 6 years’ duration) tion.[14] The golfer may focus inappropriately on
were retained in the study. The fact that they were biomechanics, the score or on his or her self image.
good golfers was supported by their average hcp Regardless of what the golfer focuses on, most im-
of 4.5. Symptoms were most apparent when put- portant is that self-attention may harm skilled per-
ting in tournaments at distances under 1.5m (5 feet) formance. Although choking[10,13,14,41-43,53] can
from the cup. Fast, downhill, left-to-right breaking occur in any sport or task when an individual wants
putts, playing in and/or leading a tournament, dif- to perform well, it has been studied comprehen-
ficult putts, specific competitors and easy putts sively in golf during putting.
also precipitated symptoms.
In a second phase of our research,[26] physical, 4. A Choking Perspective of the ‘Yips’
physiologic and performance differences in golf- in Golf
ers with and without ‘yips’ were measured.[18,23,26] Choking in golf is a phenomenon recently re-
Heart rate (HR), grip force (GF) and electromyo- ceiving research attention.[10,53] Several studies[8-
gram (EMG)[23] were recorded while ‘yips’ (n = 4) 10,13,14,28,53] on choking in golf have attempted to

and non-‘yips’ golfers (n = 3) putted difficult sce- identify learning styles and situations that might
narios. During putting, golfers with ‘yips’ had in- precipitate choking. Two mechanisms have been
creased HR and GF compared with non-affected proposed to account for choking: (i) distraction (cog-

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
18 Smith et al.

The athlete
• Personality factors
• Skill
• Fitness
• Personal experience

Emotional or
The sport Cognitive
physiological Behaviour
situation appraisal
response
• Task difficulty • Of demands • Heart rate • Motor performance
• Demands • Of resources • Muscle tension • Decision making
• Of consequences • Brain waves • Perception
and their meaning • Skin conduction • Retention of
• Of bodily reactions learned material

Fig. 1. Factors that affect the arousal-performance relationship (reprinted from Williams,[54] with permission from The McGraw-Hill
Companies).

nitive load), which occurs when pressure distracts One golfer made eight fewer putts than at baseline,
golfers from the task at hand; and (ii) self-focus, one made ten more putts thus, earning $US300, and
when attention shifts inward to self-presentational anxiety failed to predict poor performance.[13]
concerns,[43] thereby disrupting performance. To The strongest predictor of poor performance
differentiate between these competing mecha- was the self-reported average golf score. Unsuc-
nisms 112 novice male golfers were divided into cessful putters (chokers) reported anticipated
two groups.[13] The first group learned to putt under scores of less than 90. Conversely, those who suc-
an increased cognitive load (i.e., counting back- ceeded reported anticipated scores over 90. Golf-
wards from 100 by two’s) and the other group ers reporting lower anticipated scores may have
learned under the condition of increased self- closely monitored their performances,[13] which re-
awareness (i.e., being videotaped) until they reached sulted in increased self-awareness and choking.
a specified criterion of accuracy. Both the distrac- Putters who were successful[13] were not burdened
tion group and the video group performed well by such high expectations.
under a high-pressure condition and did not Unsuccessful putting performance was asso-
choke.[14] Conversely, when a group was tested ciated with increased frontal theta, left hemi-
who had not adapted to increased self-focused at- sphere alpha, and beta electroencephalogram
tention or distraction, high pressure adversely af- (EEG) activity[10] that relate to increased anxiety
fected performance. Learning how to putt during and arousal.[10,27,55] These EEG patterns were rep-
distraction and videotaping helped to recruit licated when European tour players were studied
thoughts away from self and task from self-focus while performing poorly.[8] If observed during the
and distracting influences in order to focus on the ‘yips’ behaviour, such EEG activity would support
task at hand.[10,13] the contention that the ‘yips’ is exacerbated by
To learn more about who chokes and when, ten anxiety.[26,32-34] To date, there are no studies doc-
average golfers putted 20, 1.5m baseline putts, in- umenting EEG activity in ‘yips’-affected golfers
doors, while being videotaped with hidden cam- during unsuccessful putting. In summary, it seems
eras.[13] These golfers next putted on television that distraction from the golfer’s optimal psycho-
(NBC Dateline), with the opportunity to win logic and physiologic state potentially leads to a
$US300[10,13] if they matched or improved. They choking performance. The combination of in-
would lose $US100 if their objectives were not creased arousal and distracted attention preceded
met. Five golfers were successful and five choked. the poor performance often defined as choking. As

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 19

depicted, the interaction between cognitive ap- thus far that have examined the ‘yips’ phenomenon
praisal and physiologic response can negatively ef- have all reported that the ‘yips’ is worsened by
fect motor performance, decision making, percep- anxiety.[26,33,34] It should be noted that anxiety and
tion of the situation and recall or memory, if the stress worsens involuntary movements in many
athlete appraises the situation as stressful (figure neurologic disorders, such as Parkinson’s disease,
1). essential tremor and Tourette’s syndrome.
In addition to the presumed influence of person-
5. Choking vs Dystonia in Golf ality variables such as trait anxiety, obsessiveness
and confidence on the ‘yips’ putting behaviour, so-
Golfers who choke and golfers with a dystonia ciological and sport-specific situational factors
differ on the aetiology, age of acquisition, docu- also play a role and interact with emotional factors
mentation of EEG activity to date, strokes affected
to increase state anxiety. Of 435 ‘yips’-affected
and the treatment interventions likely to be suc-
golfers (mean hcp 4.5), most anxiety provoking
cessful (table II). While dystonia and choking dif-
situations were: (i) leading tournaments; (ii) tricky
fer in many respects, performance anxiety is expe-
putts (those putts with difficult lies or more than
rienced by both groups (figure 2). For golfers with
one change in inclination); (iii) specific competi-
dystonia, performance anxiety contributes to and
tors; and (iv) the need to make easy putts.[26] These
exacerbates the ‘yips’ symptoms. For chokers, per-
situations may interact with personality, cognitive
formance anxiety such as trait and high-state anx-
appraisal factors and golf-specific situations to
iety probably combine with self-focused attention
precipitate high arousal which, depending on the
and over-analysis to cause the ‘yips’.
individual, may facilitate or cause a deterioration
in optimal performance (figure 2).[23,45-48]
6. Performance Anxiety in Golfers
Although psychometric measures of anxiety
Although it is understood that performance anx- were not administered in the second phase of our
iety exacerbates both choking and dystonia, it is ‘yips’ research,[26] ‘yips’-affected golfers (n = 4)
important to consider both the cognitive and phys- experienced faster HR, greater GF and EMG activ-
iological perspectives. The term ‘yips’ has been ity in the wrist flexors and extensors, and putted
applied to both golfers who experience a focal dys- less well than the non-‘yips’ affected controls (n =
tonia and those who choke. Although no differ- 3). These measures may be physiologic and phys-
ences in state and trait anxiety were detected be- ical markers that are a consequence of perfor-
tween the golfers with and without ‘yips’, studies mance anxiety. Alternatively, it is also possible

Table II. A comparison of the aetiology, documentation of electroencephalogram (EEG), type of stroke affected and the type of treatment
appropriate in dystonia and choking in ‘yips’-affected golfers
Dystonia Choking
Aetiology Prolonged abnormal posture; fine motor skills affected Self-focused awareness; may cause generalised
muscle tension
Age Most golfers reporting the ‘yips’ are approximately 45 years Most golfers studied to test choking are between
of age 20–30 years of age
Acquired Only affects low handicap (mean 4.5) golfers after 20–30 Not acquired as it can affect a golfer early, in the
years of golf experience middle of, or late in career
EEG No EEG studies reported to date during putting and Left brain activity last second before putting supports
dystonia choking
Stroke Most often affects putting; fine motor skills May affect any stroke; studied most in golfers when
putting
Treatment Responds initially to new grip, long putter or stance, which Will not respond to physical changes; may benefit from
requires golfer to form a new template β-blockers, sport psychology, etc.

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
20 Smith et al.

Performance anxiety

Dystonia (type I) Non-categorised Choking (type II)


• Acquired • Self focused
• Affects low hcp • Not acquired
• Golfers • Psychologic
n = 40 n = 14 n = 16

Yips

Fig. 2. A model illustrating a continuum on which choking and dystonia anchor opposite ends. Of the 72 ‘yips’ golfers in the study
only the responses of 70 could be interpreted. Definitions seem to cluster along the continuum. Performance anxiety exacerbates
both types of ‘yips’. hcp = handicap.

that increased GF, EMG activity and HR may ‘yips’. For example, McNaughton and Gray[55]
cause high anxiety and a poor putting performance suggested that defensive avoidance, specifically
(figure 1). fear, is controlled by the amygdala. These authors
When performance anxiety is severe, it can lead suggest that in approach-avoidance situations, as-
to choking, which may result in the ‘yips’. Al- sessment of the conflict is controlled by the hippo-
though some golfers with high performance anxi- campus. McNaughton and Gray[55] studied the role
ety choke or develop a dystonia (table II), others of theta waves seen on EEG in the presence of high
execute their putts competently without ‘yip- arousal. They attribute a link between arousal and
ping’.[10] Conversely, it is unlikely that either the theta waves to the hippocampus reaction, and they
dystonia or choking symptoms of the ‘yips’ during concluded that in animals, anxiolytic drugs have a
putting occurs in the absence of performance anx- mild effect on memory while reducing the percep-
iety. tion of threat. Davis,[56] in a review article, sug-
To date, investigators interested in the ‘yips’ gested that fear activates the amygdala, the hypo-
have only acknowledged superficially that state thalamus and brain stem, which result in an
and trait anxiety (closely akin to fear) exacerbate elevated corticosteroid response. He also postu-
symptoms during putting. There has been little ac- lated that opiate and benzodiazepine receptors lo-
knowledgement of the potential contributions of cated in specific areas of the amygdala explain the
neuroscience, and especially the limbic system, to fear- and anxiety-reducing effects of various drugs
our understanding of cognitions and emotions such when given systemically.
as fear. Recent research being conducted in humans, ex-
amining meditation and neuroimaging, is complex
7. Neuroscience Considerations but is likely to evolve to further our understanding
of brain and behaviour.[57] For example, the meas-
Although much of the neuroscience research to urement of regional blood flow during meditation
date has examined the function of the amygdala has been examined using single proton emission
and hippocampus in animals,[55] some of these computed tomography (SPECT). Regional cere-
findings may improve our understanding of the bral blood flow, which correlates with cerebral ac-

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 21

tivity, was measured during meditation sessions of hemisphere dominance) during putting under
where practitioners increased their intensity of fo- conditions of stress. Although determining the
cus on a visualised image.[57] During meditation, benefits of pharmacologic manipulation must be
significantly increased cerebral blood flow was done in randomised, controlled trials; in the mean-
observed in the cingulate gyrus (the limbic sys- time, many ‘yips’-affected golfers seek relief from
tem). Interpretations of these findings are not yet the ‘yips’ using their own pharmacologic reme-
clear, however it seems likely that eventually they dies.
may be extrapolated to alternative hypotheses. For
example, opposite responses to meditation will 8. Alcohol and β-Blockers to Reduce
likely be found when golfers are studied in this Performance Anxiety
manner, during feared ‘yips’-provoking situations.
Newberg et al.[58] have suggested that in the Little is known about how ‘yips’-affected golf-
limbic system the amygdala serves as our emo- ers treat their symptoms. Reducing performance
anxiety[35,60-69] may be achieved through the use of
tional ‘watchdog’, the hippocampus is the ‘diplo-
alcohol and pharmacologic agents, such as β-
mat’ and the hypothalamus is the ‘master control-
blockers.[35,66-69] Musicians and athletes, includ-
ler’. While more empirical data are needed to
ing golfers, deal with performance anxiety through
support these statements, this oversimplified de-
either or both of these means.
scription serves as a useful interpretation of a com-
For example, 28% of musicians surveyed be-
plex neurophysiologic interaction. Fear, a primary
lieved that music majors had a problem with chem-
factor in ‘yips’-affected golfers, accompanied by
ical use, such as marijuana.[60] In a study of orches-
activation of the amygdala, plays a role in the cor-
tra members and music students, 34% admitted
ticosteroid release that often occurs during high
taking sedatives or alcohol to cope with music
arousal.[59] The qualitative assessment of fear of performance anxiety.[62] In a different study,[63]
failure beliefs provided by Davis et al.[59] is en- 20% of musicians were concerned about their
lightening and their integrated psychophysiologic substance use. It was also reported that 23% of
model shows the role of negative affect, anxiety musicians used β-blockers,[64] and in another study
and fear which may partially explain the experi- of 2122 sophisticated musicians, 27% were using
ence of ‘yips’-affected golfers. β-blockers.[65]
To date, our attempt to understand the ‘yips’ Physiologic symptoms of performance anxiety
phenomenon using a generic testing of anxiety[34] include tremors, shaking, palpitations and tachy-
has been too superficial to decipher which emo- cardia. In several studies,[66-68] β-blockers inhib-
tions (i.e. fear or frustration) are triggering the per- ited the increase in heart rate during performance
formance anxiety experienced. We have also been and reduced tremors and shaking compared with
unable to clearly identify the areas of the limbic the placebo groups. For example, a review of eight
system responsible for the performance anxiety studies[68] showed that most musicians reduced
experienced. We have also failed to clearly iden- their tremors with β-blockers compared with pla-
tify the areas of the limbic system responsible cebo controls.[68] On the other hand, although
for the performance anxiety experienced by ‘yips’- tremors were reduced, musicians reported drowsi-
affected golfers. ness.[68] β-Blockers are also used by individuals
Therefore, future research based on the model experiencing various forms of performance anxi-
(figure 3), must integrate the golfer’s subjective ety as well as by those with essential or hereditary
ratings of fear with objective physiologic data on tremors.[70-74]
the sympathoadrenomedullary axis (HR), the hy- Although β-blockers decrease performance
pothalamus-pituitary axis (salivary cortisol and anxiety[69,71-74] by blocking β-adrenergic recep-
testosterone) and EEG (waves and documentation tors,[74] they are minimally effective in treating fo-

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
22 Smith et al.

Dystonia (type I) Choking (type II)


• Patient history, • Sensation of stress
physical and Performance • Increased self-awareness
neurological exam anxiety • Increased EEG activity
• Acquired left brain last second
• Is this really a dystonia? prior to putt

Yips behaviour
Psychoneurologic
Type I study – test impediment primarily to Type II study – test
β-blockers, putting stroke in golf β-blockers effect on:
antileptics, BoNT • Psychology
• Changes in stance, Characterised by: Anxiety – social
grip force, putter • Jerk, twitch, tremor, desirability (SD),
during putting or freezing interaction obsessive-
• Test EEG while • Anxiety, fear of failure compulsiveness
putting • Poor performance • Physiologic
• Patient history Differentiate fear: HR, salivary cortisol
• Genetic influence • Excitement/approach and testosterone, EEG
of dystonia • Avoidance/flight • Physical
• Role of cortisol and • Relate to objective data EMG, grip force
testosterone Expected outcomes:
Expected outcomes: • Benefit from β-blockers
• β-blockers will have • Rank high on SD
no effect • Changes in stance
• BoNT and antileptics not effective
may help • EEG activity in left
• Changes in stance, brain greatest just
etc., will have initial before ball contact
effect, but not lasting
• EEG, not sure what
to anticipate

Fig. 3. A model based on the notion that there are at least two types of ‘yips’ (type I and type II) and suggesting logical directions for
future testing of both types. BoNT = botulinum toxin; EEG = electroencephalogram; EMG = electromyogram; HR = heart rate.

cal dystonia.[36,70] For example, when musicians • both groups perform intricate movements be-
are anxious, the anxiolytic property of β-blockers fore large evaluative audiences
reduces symptoms caused by high adrenaline lev- • the errors of both groups are immediately appar-
els.[74] ent to observers (e.g. audience, media and lis-
Symptoms of performance anxiety (choking) teners)
and dystonia described by musicians are compara- • both groups have much to gain or lose based on
ble to the ‘yips’ phenomenon experienced by golf- the quality of their performance
ers. For example: • a study in 1978[75] reported high pre-performance
• to achieve excellence, both musicians and golf- state anxiety in single-sport athletes and solo
ers assume an abnormal posture and perform musical performers (in middle school children)
repeated fine muscle movements over many prior to sport or music participation. Single-
years prior to onset of symptoms sport athletes and solo musical performers ex-

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 23

perienced higher state anxiety than team sport The purpose of the small study described in de-
athletes tail below was to quantify and qualify the self-
• both groups tend to over-practice perceptions of ‘yips’-affected golfers concerning
• approximately 30% of both groups have tried their putting problem. This study addressed: (i) if
β-blockers and/or alcohol to decrease symp- perceptions of ‘yips’-affected golfers support a
toms of performance anxiety. theory (figure 2) in which dystonia (type I) and
Dystonia in musicians affects the particular choking (type II) are on a ‘yips’ continuum; (ii) do
muscles being overused. For example, focal dys- type I and type II ‘yips’ have different causes; and
tonia affects cheek muscles in musicians who play (iii) are both types influenced by performance anx-
wind instruments and finger muscles in guitarists. iety?
On the other hand, it affects the wrist flexor and
extensor muscles in most golfers.
Since β-blockers decrease the physical and psy- 9.1 Methods
chologic symptoms of performance anxiety, sev-
eral sports (i.e. archery and shooting) have banned A preliminary golf questionnaire was devel-
their use during competition. To date, professional oped and posted on the Mayo Clinic website
golf organisations have not banned β-blocker use (www.mayo.edu/research/yips/). Fliers were also
in North America[35,69] even though players have mailed to several golf-teaching professionals iden-
discussed the positive effects of β-blockers on tified from the United States Golf Association ros-
their symptoms.[69] The use of β-blockers or other ter. Recipients were asked to post the question-
performance-enhancing agents in tournaments in naires in their golf club locker rooms. Website
the United Kingdom, such as the British Open, is visitors interested in participating in this project
forbidden.[35] Although, collectively, empirical downloaded the questionnaire, those who phoned
data about the ‘yips’ are accruing, there are only were mailed questionnaires, and all completed
occasional anecdotes that describe the subjective questionnaires were returned via US ground mail.
experience of the ‘yips’-affected. The questionnaire was completed by low hcp
golfers (n = 72) in the US and Canada in the first
9. Golfers’ Perceptions of the ‘Yips’: quarter of 2001. The questionnaire obtained de-
An Investigation mographics, a ‘yips’ history and the golfer’s
subjective perception or definition of the ‘yips’.
Interpretations of the ‘yips’ by non-‘yips’ af- The golfer’s perceptions were analysed qualita-
fected physicians, teaching professionals and sport tively[77-79] and quantitatively. Three investigators
scientists are respected; yet, rarely, are the ‘yips’- conducted the preliminary analysis. Golfer’s per-
affected golfers asked to subjectively describe ceptions were triaged into themes and were
their symptoms. Self-perception is the subjective categorised as representing physical symptoms
way in which we see and evaluate ourselves.[76] (dystonia: type I), psychosocial symptoms (chok-
Perception influences individual beliefs, which ing: type II), or a combination of both (non-
may influence the golfer’s decision about the type categorised). The preliminary analysis was then
of treatment to seek. For example, golfers with mailed to three physician co-investigators who
‘yips’ who believe their wrist jerk is an acquired were specialists in movement disorders and golf.
movement disorder, such as a dystonia, may seek The few definitions in each category which did not
medical advice, whereas golfers who attribute have 100% agreement were assigned to the classi-
their wrist jerk to poor putting technique will con- fication that received agreement by two of three
tact a teaching professional. Self-perception influ- physicians. This triangulation process validated
ences the golfer’s willingness to accept a diagno- the analysis and utilised the knowledge of the in-
sis, adhere to treatment and change behaviour. vestigators,[26] sources of data on perceptions and

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
24 Smith et al.

Table III. Definitions of the peceptions of the ‘yips’ (classified as dystonia [type I]), as defined by 40 of 72 ‘yips’-affected golfers
Uncontrollable movement on short putts with hands/wrist producing jerking motion/stabbing of putt vs a smooth stroke
Last second jerk of the club and turn of face of the putter
Complete inability to make a proper stroke with the putter. Inability to keep the wrists from snapping, the shorter the putt, the worse the
problem
Freezing on short putts or twitching/jerking on short putts
Involuntary movement of body/arms/hands not in coordination with desired movement
Involuntary spasm or nerve reaction
Involuntary motion of the hands during a stroke
Inability to make a smooth stroke on putts (especially short putts)
Neurologically generated problem that disrupts communication between nerves/muscle and brain
‘Freeze’ over the ball and often make spastic jerky putts
Involuntary aberration in usual putting stroke, especially on short putts
Uncontrollable movement in the hands that causes the putter blade to turn
‘Flinch’ or ‘jerk’ that occurs at or just prior to impact on an actual golf shot
Involuntary twitch in the right wrist that destroys both distance and direction in short game shots
Involuntary jerking of hands when putting
Jerky, uncontrollable swing with the putter
Flinching on impact
Failure to control club face at contact
When you can’t take the putter back; or when you finally get it to move, you take it back short and then putt the ball going forward
While in the process of stroking the ball, there is a sudden jerk or movement that alters the normally expected swing pattern. It is
involuntary
Inability to bring the club back and/or jabbing-type blow at the ball
Your brain will not let you control your hands
Tendency to jerk putter or freeze over shortish putts and chips
Twitch of the hands at a putt
When putting, just before contacting the ball, having a spasm causing the player to miss the putt
Involuntary motion or freeze up while putting
When at the point of contact with the ball, your muscles explode uncontrollably
Involuntary flinching of the nerves that you can’t control
Uncontrolled flinch at impact causing you to miss a shot that you would normally make
Total loss of control when trying to perform a stroke
Involuntary movement of the wrist only when the ball is in play
An involuntary twitching of the right hand on every putt
Sudden unexpected movement
Uncontrollable flinch
Hands take on a mind of their own and refuse to complete stroke or chip. Jerk out or across the ball; at worst, can’t even bring putter back
Nervous twitch or jerk which inexplicably occurs during the putting stroke
Loss of control of my right wrist during putting
Inability to draw putter back on a 1-footer. Uncontrollable muscle spasm in short putting stroke and chipping
Uncontrolled movement of the arms/hands just before making contact with the ball. The movement is like a twitch or jerk
An involuntary flinching during a short motion

the opinions of expert colleagues (R Wharen, per- (mean hcp of 13.1, ranging between 7–17) com-
sonal communication).[70,79] pleted questionnaires to assess ‘yips’ symptoms.
Of these 72 ‘yips’-affected golfers, 62 (85%) had
9.2 Results hcps of 10 and under. Most had achieved a high
Subjective descriptions of the ‘yips’ were an- level of golf proficiency (e.g. were on golf schol-
alysed from the questionnaires returned by the first arships, etc.) before acquiring the ‘yips’.[34]
72 interested golfers. Sixty-nine ‘yips’-affected The mean age of the 72 golfers was 52 years,
male golfers (mean hcp of 6.7, ranging between mean golf experience was 36 years and golfers av-
0–16) and three ‘yips’-affected female golfers eraged 75 rounds of golf per year. Golfers with

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 25

Table IV. Definitions of the ‘yips’ (classified as choking [type II]), as defined by 16 of 72 ‘yips’-affected golfers
Inability to make simple short putts when you need to, as if paralysed
Inability to commence the putting stroke finally followed by a ‘herky-jerky’ putting stroke. The entire predicament is probably caused by
negative feelings; wondering how you will miss the putt rather than the feeling of making the putt
The feeling that you cannot make a putt regardless of how easy it is. Nervousness, tension and jumpiness
Can’t make short putts and lose confidence and then get nervous over them
Nervousness over short range putts. Sometimes making it difficult to draw the putter back. Causes loss of confidence
Involuntary mental response in putting that you feel overrides technique and ability
Not being able to make the same stroke when competing or just playing that you can make when practising
Can’t start the golf swing
Inability to repeat swing or stroke that would be made with no pressure
Lack of confidence, particularly short putts
When the adrenaline gets going and nervousness sets in at certain times
Inability to consistently make a 3–4 foot putt while putting under pressure
Your mind tells you that you can’t make short putts, thus you either push or pull the putt
Inability to think in a relaxed, positive, quiet manner, and the inability to execute during period of slightest pressure
Nervousness and tight feeling in the body prior and during the putt
Mental freeze up that does not allow your muscles to freely work

‘yips’ averaged 20 years of club tournament expe- a manner that failed to describe their symptoms at
rience with 66 (90.4%) playing in local and 19 all.
(26%) in national tournaments. ‘Yips’ symptoms In the dystonia category (type I), definitions fo-
were episodic in 54% and affected putting only in cused on physical characteristics of the ‘yips’ such
53.4%. as “an involuntary spasm or nerve reaction” (table
When ‘yips’-affected golfers defined the ‘yips’, III). Within this category, golfers mentioned a jerk
it was assumed they were describing the ‘yips’ (n = 28), freezing (n = 10) and a tremor (n = 6).
symptoms they experienced. Three primary inves- Type I golfers tried to treat their symptoms by a
tigators reduced the definitions into the two themes change in either grip (n = 17), stance (n = 8), putter
of dystonia (n = 40) and choking (n = 16) [see table length (n = 14) or swing (n = 2). Sixteen golfers
III and table IV]. Fourteen had symptoms of both (40%) self-medicated with alcohol, four (10%)
themes and were determined to be non-categorical self-referred to a sport psychologist and seven
(see table V). Two golfers described the ‘yips’ in (17.5%) tried relaxation (n = 3), music (n = 2),

Table V. Non-categorised definitions of the ‘yips’, as defined by 14 of 72 ‘yips’-affected golfers


A physical reaction to a mental/physical condition which causes a jerky motion at impact causing the putt to be pulled/pushed off line
Tighten up and your stroke gets short and choppy. Sometimes you flinch
Inability to make a normal smooth stroke under pressure with a standard-size putter
Involuntary collapse during a putting stroke
Muscles tighten up and the mind shuts down
Anything that interferes with the natural free flow of the putting stroke (tightness, freezing over putt)
Flinching at impact. The desire to make a smooth, complete motion at the golf ball and the inability to do so. Lack of confidence in ability,
especially under pressure
Uncontrollable nervosa, mini panic attacks, like a lightning rod of hypertension right in the middle of the stroke
Physical reaction to performance anxiety
When your nerves affect your putting stroke, preventing you from obtaining a smooth stroke
Nervous reaction of the wrist as you are about to hit the ball
Loss of ability to putt, caused by a lack of ‘feeling’ in your hands, as a result of fear of missing putts
Medically: a psychoneuromuscular reaction experienced during short putts. Socially: a horrifying affliction prompting avoidance behaviour
in others. A ‘career killer’
Inability to putt the ball. Look over it, but feel you can’t pull the trigger, in a freeze mode

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
26 Smith et al.

medications (n = 1) or exercise (n = 1). Although egories) will be positioned in the centre of the con-
some improvement may have occurred, it is note- tinuum depicted on the theoretical model (figure
worthy that these golfers still registered for the 2).
‘yips’ investigation, so their symptoms had not These results support research reported earlier
completely resolved as a result of the aforemen- by these investigators[26] on criteria qualifying
tioned interventions. most cases of the ‘yips’ as a dystonia. Criteria of
The choking category (n = 16) included defini- the ‘yips’ were that golfers should have been good
tions of psychologic distress such as “inability to putters and thus have acquired the ‘yips’ problem.
make simple short putts when you need to, as if The ‘yips’ symptoms should be episodic, prompt-
paralysed” (table IV). Non-categorised definitions ing golfers to alter their grip, putter length or
(n = 14) which contained characteristics of both stance, changes that most often result in temporary
dystonia and choking such as “muscles tighten up improvement.
and the mind shuts down” were included in table Of 200 golfers[26] examined retrospectively for
V. Definitions (n = 2) such as “a nasty monster” these criteria in a previously reported study, 136
and the “inability to make a proper putting stroke (68%) met three or four criteria of dystonia and 64
under any number of conditions” were too vague (32%) met two or fewer. In the present study, 38
to be interpreted and were not included in the anal- of 40 (95%) type I dystonia golfers described them-
ysis. selves as good putters prior to their ‘yips’ symp-
From the standpoint of performance, the 40 type toms supporting the notion that the symptoms were
I dystonia golfers had a mean hcp of 6.6, compared acquired. Eighteen of the 40 (42.9%) said their
with a hcp of 6.2 for the type II golfers classified symptoms were episodic and all (100%) had
as chokers. The choking group experienced less changed their grip, stance, putter length or swing.
range in their hcps (table VI). Using a qualitative analysis of the definitions[77-79]
provided by these ‘yips’-affected golfers, 40 (55%)
9.3 Discussion were classified as dystonia, compared with 136
(68%) who met the criteria reported earlier.[26]
It is evident that a golfer’s perception of what is
The high incidence of ‘yips’-affected golfers
happening in the mind, body or both can help in-
who meet the inclusion criteria used to categorise
vestigators determine the aetiology and sub-
sequently the appropriate treatment for the ‘yips’. a dystonia is surprising. This incidence is higher
In this study, the ‘yips’ descriptors ranged on a than expected when one considers that the inci-
continuum from symptoms of dystonia (55%) to dence of focal dystonia (30/100 000) reported in
choking (22%). Nineteen percent (19.4%) of the 72 the general population is fairly low.[37] Even the
golfers provided definitions that contained symp- increased incidence of dystonia reported by musi-
toms of both dystonia and choking (table V) and as cians (1 in 200 to 1 in 500)[38] is likely less than
such were not included in either category (figure what we are reporting in these competitive low hcp
2). Perhaps as more empirical data are obtained, golfers (we do not have a denominator necessary
the non-categorised group (symptoms of both cat- to accurately calculate true incidence).
Musicians, like golfers, repetitively perform
fine motor skills under conditions of high arousal,
Table VI. Summary of handicaps for type I and type II ‘yips’-affected experience intense concentration demands and fear
golfers
not performing well. Supporting the high preva-
Type of ‘yips’ Mean Standard Mode Range
deviation
lence of dystonia-like symptoms in golfers is the
Type I: dystonia 6.8 4.6 7 0–17.4 fact that accomplished pianists and guitarists, who
Type II: choking 6.2 3.2 8 2–8 perform repetitive hand movements, experience
Mean 6.5 3.9 7.5 0–17.4 dystonia more often than other musicians. The

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 27

‘yips’-affected were good golfers (mean hcp nied by performance anxiety, by the fear of recur-
4.5[26] and 6.6 in the present study). Golfers with rence and by a loss of confidence. In the ‘yips’ type
‘yips’ who have had 20–30 years’ experience re- I, it may be that a nerve conduction pathway, pre-
petitively practice putting while maintaining the viously highly functional, wears out (R Wharen,
prolonged abnormal hand and wrist posture in- personal communication). To date, golfers with
herent in the putting grip, a factor believed requi- the ‘yips’ (type I) do not recover, but re-train them-
site to the onset of dystonia symptoms. selves, with the help of a teaching professional, to
A possible explanation for why the majority of acquire expertise on a long putter, a new grip or
golfers affected describe symptoms of dystonia stance. If ‘yips’ type I golfers return to their pre-
might be that the ‘yips’-affected prefer to interpret vious putting style, the ‘yips’ symptoms usually
their signs and symptoms as a physical and not a return.
psychologic impediment. Alternatively, the ‘yips’- The ‘yips’ type II golfers experience choking,
affected are likely to experience a transitory task- a consequence of self-focused attention,[8,13,14,43,
specific dystonia in high-pressure situations (i.e. 44,53,76] performance anxiety and possibly over-

golf tournaments). Therefore, symptoms of dysto- analysis.[10,27] Preliminary EEG studies by Crews
nia may not affect execution of tasks in their daily et al.[8,27,28] on choking suggest that both success-
lives. As a consequence, ‘yips’-affected golfers ful and non-successful golfers experienced anxi-
may not seek medical attention. Thus, by avoiding ety, but successful golfers may have stopped an-
a neurologic diagnosis, they are not included in the alysing and balanced the EEG activity of both
epidemiology reports on focal dystonia. hemispheres just before the putt (in the last 3 sec-
The taxonomy differentiating the ‘yips’ into onds).[8] Although provocative, this work needs
dystonia (type I) and choking (type II) has appeal. replication. Interventions suggested to relieve
Self-perceptions of the ‘yips’-affected provide ini- choking range from cognitive strategies,[19] use
tial empirical support for the model (figure 2) and of mental imagery[80-83] and a visualisation pro-
may reduce the lay public’s confusion about the gramme specific to activating the right hemisphere
‘yips’. Figure 3 provides structure for future re- of the brain.[10] The latter may move the focus of
search testing of both the aetiology and the effec- attention away from left brain analysis to promote
tiveness of interventions specific to the ‘type’ of automaticity. The effectiveness of these interven-
‘yips’ experienced. tions on choking, at levels of performance anxiety
While some ‘yips’ behaviour results from chok- sufficient to elicit the ‘yips’, await empirical test-
ing, not all choking results in the ‘yips’. Putts that ing.
fail to go in the cup attributed to high anxiety may The pathophysiology and aetiologies of the
not involve a tremor, jerk, twitch or freezing. The ‘yips’ type I and type II are incomplete. Other ex-
ball may fail to drop due to mild performance anx- planations postulated to cause the ‘yips’ have been
iety or a slight error in judgement, but the golfer suggested. Explanations include a psychiatric
may not have truly ‘yipped’. block which impedes retrieval of a normal, stored
The ‘yips’ type I resembles focal dystonia, the motor engram for putting.[30] Alternatively, the re-
possible aetiological explanations of which were trieval process may occur normally but yield a
listed earlier in the paper. It is said that dystonias faulty, motor engram characteristic of the
affect the same muscles individuals use to make a ‘yips’.[21] It is also possible that golfers who are
living. The more one practices golf, the more ‘yips’-affected have a genetic predisposition to
rounds played per year, the more clinics, years ex- dystonia or choking not yet identifiable.
perience, intensity, concentration and tension, the A provocative, unsubstantiated psychophysio-
more likely it is that the golfer will acquire the logic hypothesis that merits research attention in-
‘yips’ type I. Once started, the ‘yips’ is accompa- volves the role of testosterone in contributing to

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
28 Smith et al.

the ‘yips’ of both types. In college tennis players, use alcohol and β-blockers to reduce performance
salivary testosterone was significantly higher in anxiety.
winners than losers.[84] Testosterone remained
high while players continued winning, which led 9.4 Limitations and Conclusions of the
investigators to conclude that elevated testosterone Present Investigation
was associated with confidence. ‘Yips’ symptoms, • This present investigation is the first reported
reported most often in men, occur at 45–55 years analysis of the golfer’s perception of their ‘yips’
of age,[85,86] when testosterone may decline. Lower symptoms. These data provide initial support
levels of testosterone, associated with decreased for the proposed model (figure 2). The results
confidence, might exacerbate the putting problem, of this study support previous investigations
a premise to be tested in subsequent research.[20] that lean toward the diagnosis of a focal dysto-
Another explanation for the ‘yips’ type II has a nia for the majority of ‘yips’-affected golf-
psychosocial foundation. Golfers at middle age, ers.[26,33,34] The ‘yips’ taxonomy (type I and
with the expertise necessary to have a low hcp, may type II) provides organisation for what, to date,
have their self-identity highly invested in golf. has been a disorganised picture of the ‘yips’.
Such a scenario would predispose competent Future research will hopefully progress in ac-
golfers to ‘acquire’ the type II ‘yips’ which also cordance with the guidelines provided in figure
has a mean age of onset in the late 40s or early 3.
50s.[26,33,34] • A strength and a limitation of this research is
Finally, Dave Pelz,[7] a teaching professional at that it was based upon the golfer’s perception of
the ‘yips’ behaviour. Ideally, a neurologist
the Pelz Golf Institute, has observed that golfers
would clinically confirm the ‘yips’ classifica-
who become ‘yips’-affected are very intelligent
tion. The questionnaires were returned from all
and committed to their sport. Future research needs
across the US and Canada so that was not pos-
to be done to substantiate his observation. It also
sible. As with our earlier study[26] on the ‘yips’,
may be that golfers with ‘yips’ are compulsive, the ‘yips’ respondents were primarily male,
over practice, overanalyse and/or take themselves which limits the applicability of the results to
too seriously. These interpretations remain conjec- females.
ture until empirical data are obtained. Perhaps in • It is likely that psychosocial influences of peers
the future, a ‘yips’ personality profile will identify and the media have skewed the subjective re-
those at risk for developing this frustrating prob- porting of symptoms and the ‘yips’ experience.
lem and strategies can be identified to prevent the Care must be taken to distinguish between the
onset of symptoms. type of ‘yips’ reported, and to relate both sub-
Golfer’s self-perceptions provide insight into jective and objective findings.
what is experienced when the ‘yips’ occurs. Describ-
ing the ‘yips’ as a “horrifying affliction prompt- 10. Future Research Directions
ing avoidance behaviour” or a “career killer” con- • Subsequent studies should ensure that ‘yips’-
veys the distress experienced by golfers who affected golfers are examined clinically to de-
are ‘yips’-affected. It is worrying when golfers termine the congruity between the golfer’s
treat their ‘yips’ symptoms with alcohol or self- perception of the ‘yips’ and the ‘yips’ classifi-
medication. The incidence of reported alcohol cation assigned by the neurologist. Future stud-
use to treat ‘yips’ symptoms ranged from those ies will compare the psychometrics, stress
categorised as dystonia (type I: 39%) to those in hormones, HR, GF, EMG and EEG relative to
the choking group (type II: 33%), percentages the performance of ‘yips’-affected golfers dur-
which are similar to the numbers of musicians who ing baseline, placebo and β-blocker conditions

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 29

(figure 3). Golfers experiencing type II ‘yips’ nostic category. Those golfers who receive appro-
may show greater symptom reduction after re- priate treatment expediently will be less likely to
ceiving a β-blocker, whereas those with type I abuse alcohol or non-prescription medications.
‘yips’ are less likely to find symptom relief. The proposed model provides an organisation
• It should be noted that because dystonia symp- for future research. Results of subsequent investi-
toms are greatly exacerbated by anxiety, even gations on the effectiveness of treatment will not
those ‘yips’-affected golfers classified as hav- only help relieve the distress experienced by com-
ing a dystonia may benefit from a β-blocker, if petent golfers but will also be applicable to musi-
it sufficiently reduces anxiety. ‘Yips’-affected cians,[52] dentists and other similarly affected pop-
golfers with symptoms of dystonia should be ulations and professions.
studied during putting to determine EEG activ-
ity in the right and left hemisphere. Such results Acknowledgements
can be compared with the EEG data obtained
from successful and unsuccessful putters (chok- These authors would like to acknowledge the Johannson-
Gund Endowment Fund, Lila Burnett, and Joseph Roberts
ers).
for their assistance with this paper. The authors have no
• Future studies must delineate the behavi- conflicts of interest directly relevant to the contents of this
our produced by a differentiation of fear and review.
anxiety. Fear-related measures of avoidance
(flight), excitement (approach) and freezing References
may predominate in type II golfers rather than 1. Blundell N. To ‘yips’ or not to ‘yips’. Golf Victoria 1990; 31
in dystonia type I golfers. Type II chokers may (5): 17-9
2. Blundell N. A cure for the ‘yips’: a case study. Golf Victoria
exhibit anxiety manifested by stimuli from a 1990; 31 (5): 11-4
conflict between threat and curiosity.[55] Partic- 3. Moody L. Master class: the mental and physical condition
known as the yips. Cricketer Int 1993; 74 (2): 36-7
ipating in a ‘yips’ putting tournament may ide- 4. Murphy A. A serious case of the ‘yips’. Sports Illustrated 1996;
ally stimulate the conflict of interest. Such a 84 (5): 30
research emphasis will help translate the exten- 5. Snead S. My side-saddle style can work for you, too. Golf Di-
gest 1982; 33 (9): 60-1
sive body of research on animals[55] to an in- 6. Slingerland G. Beat the ‘yips’ with your eyes closed. Golf Mag
creased understanding of human behaviour 1979; Mar: 80-1
7. Pelz D. Putting like the pros. New York: Harper Collins Pub-
under pressure. Outcomes could then be inter- lishers, 1989: 201-11
preted in light of the psychophysiology research 8. Crews DJ, Lutz R, Nilsson P, et al. Psychophysiological indi-
on adaptation to stress by Dienstbier.[87] cators of confidence and habituation during golf putting. In:
Farrally MR, Cochran AJ, editors. Science and Golf III: Pro-
ceedings of the World Scientific Congress of Golf. Cham-
11. Conclusion paign (IL): Human Kinetics, 1999: 158-65
9. Boutcher SH, Crews DJ. The effect of a preshot attentional
routine on a well-learned skill. Int J Sport Psychol 1987; 18
Although this paper has advanced our under- (1): 30-9
standing of the ‘yips’ beyond work reported ear- 10. Abrahams J. And the winner is . . . . Golf Mag 2001; Mar: 92-7
lier,[26] the two categories of ‘yips’, type I and type 11. Murphy SM, Woolfolk RL. The effects of cognitive interven-
tions on competitive anxiety and performance on a fine motor
II, are not yet adequately described. Interventions skill accuracy task. Int J Sport Psychol 1987; 18 (2): 152-66
must next be evaluated for effectiveness. 12. Cook DL, Horvath MJ, Connelly D. The effects of an imagery
audiotape on putting a golf ball. J Appl Res Coaching Athlet-
The golfer’s perception of the ‘yips’ will deter- ics 1989; 4 (3): 195-205
mine the type of care the golfer seeks for relief of 13. Linder D, Lutz R, Crews DJ, et al. Who chokes and when:
symptoms. The nomenclature discussed in this pa- situational and dispositional factors in failure under pressure.
In: Farrally MR, Cochran AJ, editors. Science and Golf III:
per, once disseminated to golfers, physicians and Proceedings of the World Scientific Congress of Golf. Cham-
teaching professionals, will result in a more paign (IL): Human Kinetics, 1999: 207-12
14. Lewis BP, Linder DE. Thinking about choking: attentional pro-
prompt recognition of the ‘yips’ symptoms. Golf- cesses and paradoxical performance. Personality Soc Psychol
ers may be triaged promptly into the correct diag- Bull 1997; 23: 937-44

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
30 Smith et al.

15. Beauchamp PH. Psychological influences on golfers’ putting per- 35. Plank E. The golf doc: health, humor, and insight to improve
formance [dissertation]. Montreal: University of Montreal, your game. Sundbury (MA): Jones and Bartelett Publishers,
1996. Dissertation Abstracts International 1996; 57: 2918 2000
16. Beauchamp PH. Peak putting performance: player skills and 36. Adler CH. Management of dystonia: strategies for controlling
strategies utilized by Pro Tour golfers. In: Farrally MR, involuntary movements. Postgrad Med 2000; 108: 151-60
Cochran AJ, editors. Science and Golf III: Proceedings of the 37. Nutt JG, Muenter MD, Melton LJ, et al. Epidemiology of dys-
World Scientific Congress of Golf. Champaign (IL): Human tonia in Rochester, Minnesota. Adv Neurol 1988; 50: 361-5
Kinetics, 1999: 181-9 38. Tubiana R, Chamagne P. Prolonged rehabilitation treatment of
17. Cook SG. Quantification of the ‘yips’ phenomenon during golf musician’s focal dystonia: medical problems of the instru-
putting [dissertation]. California American University. Dis- mentalist musician. London: Martin Dunitz Ltd, 2000: 369-78
sertation Abstracts International 1993; 54: 1660 39. Fahn S, Bressman SB, Marsden CD. Classification of dystonia.
18. Smith AM, Malo SA, Laskowski ER, et al. A multidisciplinary Adv Neurol 1998; 78: 1-10
model to solve the mystery of the ‘yips’ in golf: a focal dys- 40. Ghez C, Gordon J, Hening W. Trajectory control in dystonia.
tonia? Symposium presentation at the Association for the Ad-
Adv Neurol 1988; 50: 141-55
vancement of Applied Sport Psychology; 1998 Sep 26; Cape
41. Nideffer RM, Sagal MS. Concentration and attention control
Cod (MA). J Appl Sport Psychology Proc 1998; Sep: S61-3
training. In: Williams JM, editor. Applied sport psychology:
19. Owens D, Kirchenbaum D. Smart golf: how to simplify and
personal growth to peak performance. Mountain View (CA):
score your mental game. San Francisco (CA): Jossey-Bass
Inc. Publishers, 1998 Mayfield, 1998: 296-315
20. Giannettino B. ‘Golfers’ twitch’ strikes 25% of serious players. 42. Brewer BD, Van Raalte JL, Linder DE, et al. Peak performance
Sports Primary Care 2001; 1 (3): 1, 14 and the merits of retrospective introspection. J Sport Exerc
21. Christina RW. The 1991 C.H. McLoy research lecture: unrav- Psychol 1991; 13: 227-38
eling the mystery of the response complexity effect in skilled 43. Butler JL, Baumeister RF. The trouble with friendly faces:
movements. Res Q Exerc Sport 1992; 63 (3): 218-30 skilled performances with a supportive audience. J Pers Soc
22. Boutcher SH, Zinsser NW. Cardiac deceleration of elite and Psychol 1998; 75: 1213-30
beginning golfers during putting. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1990; 44. Baumeister RF, Steinhilber A. Paradoxical effects of supportive
12 (1): 37-47 audiences on performance under pressure: the home field dis-
23. Sabick MB, Smith AM, Laskowski ER, et al. EMG patterns advantage in sports championships. J Pers Soc Psychol 1984;
during putts in golfers with and without the ‘yips’: pilot data. 47: 85-93
Presented at the International Society of Biomechanics Con- 45. Weinberg RS, Gould D. Foundations of sport and exercise psy-
gress; 1998 Aug 8-13; Calgary (AB) chology: sport and exercise psychology. Champaign (IL): Hu-
24. Delay D, Nougier V, Orliaguet P, et al. Movement control in man Kinetics, 1995: 332-4
golf putting. Hum Mov Sci 1997; 16 (5): 597-619 46. Spielberger CD. Manual for the state anxiety inventory. Palo
25. Gwyn RG, Patch CE. Comparing two putting styles for putting Alto (CA): Consulting Psychologists, 1983
accuracy. Percept Mot Skills 1993; 76 (2): 387-90 47. Gould D, Krane V. The arousal-athletic performance relation-
26. Smith AM, Malo SA, Laskowski ER, et al. A multidisciplinary ship: current status and future directions. In: Horns TS, editor.
study of the ‘yips’ phenomenon in golf: an exploratory anal- Advances in sport psychology. Champaign (IL): Human Ki-
ysis. Sports Med 2000; 6: 423-37 netics, 1992: 119-42
27. Crews DJ, Landers DM. Electroencephalographic measures of 48. Smith AM, Stuart MJ, Fish KN. Psychological, physiological, and
attentional patterns prior to the golf putt. Med Sci Sports Ex- performance variables in goalies during hockey games. In: As-
erc 1993; 25: 116-26 hare AB, editor. Safety in ice hockey. West Conshohocken (PA):
28. Crews DJ. EEG insights into attentional patterns associated American Society Testing Materials, 2000: 220-35
with stress and golf putting. Symposium presentation at the 49. Smith AM, Sim FH, Smith HC, et al. Psychological, situational,
Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychol- and physiological variables and on-ice performance of youth
ogy; 1998 Sep 26; Cape Cod (MA). J Appl Sport Psychology hockey goalkeepers. Mayo Clin Proc 1998; 73 (1): 17-27
Proc 1998; Sep: S61-3
50. Smith AM, Ortiguera SA, Laskowski ER, et al. A preliminary
29. Landers DM, Arendt SM. Applied Sport Psychology. In: Wil-
analysis of psychophysiological variables and nursing perfor-
liams JM, editor. Personal growth to peak performance. 4th
mance in situations of increasing criticality. Mayo Clin Proc
ed. Mountain View (CA): Mayfield Publishing, 2001: 206-28
2001; 76: 275-84
30. Morley DC. When the putting fails: a psychiatrist tries to ex-
plain why so many great golfers lose their touch on the greens. 51. Landers DM, Boutcher SH. Arousal-performance relationships.
Golf Mag 1981; 23 (1): 80-2 In: Williams JM, editor. Applied sport psychology: personal
31. Laskowski ER. The biomechanics and kinematics of the golf growth to peak performance. 3rd ed. Mountain View (CA):
swing: kinetic chain implications for the ‘yips’ phenomena. Mayfield Publishing, 1998: 197-218
Abstract presentation at the Association for the Advancement 52. Wilson FW. Current controversies on the origin, diagnosis and
of Applied Sport Psychology; 1998 Sep 26; Cape Cod (MA). management of focal dystonia. In: Tubiana R, Amadio PC,
J Appl Sport Psychology Proc 1998; Sep: S61-3 editors. Medical problems of the instrumentalist musician.
32. The ‘yips’: an uncontrollable movement that can plague your London: Martin Dunitz, 2000: 311-27
putting game. Mayo Clin Health Lett 1990; Apr: 3 53. Beilock SL, Carr TH. On the fragility of skilled performance:
33. McDaniel KD, Cummings JL, Shain S. The ‘yips’: a focal dys- what governs choking under pressure? J Exp Psychol Gen
tonia of golfers. Neurology 1989; 39: 192-5 2001 Dec; 130 (4): 701-25
34. Sachdev P. Golfers’ cramp: clinical characteristics and evi- 54. Williams J. Applied sport psychology: personal growth to peak
dence against it being an anxiety disorder. Mov Disord 1992; performance. Mountain View (CA): Mayfield Publishing,
7: 326-32 1998: 1999

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)
‘Yips’ in Golf 31

55. McNaughton N, Gray JA. Anxiolytic action on the behavioral 73. Meibach RC, Dunner D, Wilson LG, et al. Comparative effi-
inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute cacy of propranolol, chlordiazepoxide and placebo in the
to anxiety. J Affect Disord 2000; 61: 161-76 treatment of anxiety: a double-blinded trial. Clin Psychiatry
56. Davis M. The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu 1987; 48 (9): 355-8
Rev Neurosci 1992; 15: 353-75 74. Faigel HC. The effect of beta-blockade on stress-induced cogni-
57. Newberg A, Alavi A, Baime M, et al. The measurement of tive dysfunction in adolescents. Clin Pediatr 1991; 30 (7): 441-5
regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive 75. Simons JA, Martens R. Children’s anxiety in sport and non-
task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry sport evaluative activities. J Sport Psychol 1979; 1: 160-9
Res 2001; 106 (2): 113-22 76. Roediger HL, Capaldi ED, Paris SC, et al. Psychology. 3rd ed.
58. Newberg A, D’Aquili E, Rause V. Why God won’t go away. New York: Harper Collins Publishers, 1991: 630
New York: Random House Inc., 2001: 42-6 77. Rovegno I. Teaching within a curricular zone of safety: school
59. Davis H, Botterhill C, MacNeill K. Mood and self-regulation culture and the situated nature of student teachers’ pedagogical
changes in under-recovery: an intervention model. In: Kell- content knowledge. Res Q Exerc Sport 1994; 65 (3): 269-79
man M, editor. Enhancing recovery: preventing underper- 78. Locke LF. Qualitative research as a form of scientific inquiry
formance in athletes. Champaign (IL): Human Kinetics, in sport and physical education. Res Q 1989; 60 (1): 1-20
2002: 161-79 79. Thomas JR, Nelson JK. Qualitative research in physical activ-
60. Hipple J, Chesky K, Young J. Alcohol use patterns and percep- ity. 3rd ed. Champaign (IL): Human Kinetics, 1996: 365-82
tion of substance abuse among university music majors: a 80. Martin KA, Hall CR. Using mental imagery to enhance intrinsic
preliminary investigation. Texas Counseling Assoc J 2000; motivation. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1995; 17: 54-69
28 (2): 102-10 81. Beauchamp PH. Peak putting performance: psychological
61. Chesky KS, Hipple J. Performance anxiety, alcohol-related skills and strategies utilized by PGA tour golfers. In: Farrally
problems, and social/emotional difficulties of college stu- MR, Cochran AJ, editors. Science and Golf III: Proceedings
dents: a comparative study between lower-division music and of the World Scientific Congress of Golf. Champaign (IL):
non-music majors. Med Probl Perf Art 1997; 12: 126-32 Human Kinetics, 1999: 181-9
62. Steptoe A. Stress, coping and stage fright in professional musi- 82. Woolfolk RL, Parrish MW, Murphy SM. The effects of positive
cians. Psychol Musicians 1989; 17: 3-11 and negative imagery on motor skill performance. Cognit
63. Young JA, Hipple J. Social/emotional problems of university Ther Res 1985; 9 (3): 335-41
music students seeking assistance at a student counseling cen- 83. Williams JM, Harris DV. Relaxation and energizing techniques
ter. Med Probl Perf Art 1996; 11: 123-36 for regulation of arousal. In: Williams JM, editor. Applied
64. Salmon P, Shook CP, Lombart KG. Performance impairments, sport psychology: personal growth to peak performance. 4th
injuries, and stress hardiness in a sample of keyboard and ed. Mountain View (CA): Mayfield Publishing Company,
other instrumentalists. Med Probl Perf Art 1995; 10: 140-6 2001: 229-46
65. Fishbein M, Middlestadt SE, Ottati V, et al. Medical problems 84. Booth A, Shelley G, Mazar A, et al. Testosterone and winning and
among ICSOM musicians: overview of a national survey. losing in human competition. Horm Behav 1989; 23: 556-71
Med Probl Perf Art 1988; 8: 73-6 85. Malo SA. A cognitive approach to understanding the devasta-
66. Neftel KA, Adler RH, Kappeli L. Stage fright in musicians: a ting ‘yips’ in golf. Symposium presentation at the Association
model illustrating the effects of beta-blockade. Psychosom for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology; 1998 Sep
Med 1982; 44 (5): 461-9 26; Cape Cod (MA). J Appl Sport Psychology Proc 1998;
67. James IM, Pearson QM. Reducing the somatic manifestations Sep: S62
of anxiety by beta blockage: a study of stage fright. J Psy- 86. Smith AM. An exciting multidisciplinary approach to a com-
chosom Res 1978; 22: 327-37 plicated problem: golf and the ‘yips’. Symposium presenta-
68. Nube J. Beta-blockers: effects on performing musicians: a fear of tion at the Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport
failure or a fear of success? Med Probl Perf Art 1991; 5: 61-8 Psychology; 1998 Sep 26; Cape Cod (MA). J Appl Sport
69. Voy R. Drugs, sports and politics. Champaign (IL): Human Psychology Proc 1998; Sep: S62
Kinetics, 1991: 51-3 87. Dienstbier RA. Arousal and physiological toughness: impli-
70. Adler CH, Ahlskog JE, editors. Parkinson’s disease and move- cations for mental and physical health. Psychol Rev 1993;
ment disorders: diagnosis and treatment guidelines for the 96: 579-89
practicing physician. Totowa (NJ): Humana Press Inc., 2000
71. Hartley LR, Ungapen S, Davies I. The effect of beta-adrenergic
blocking drugs on speaker’s performance and memory. Br J
Psychiatry 1983; 142: 512-7
Correspondence and offprints: Aynsley M. Smith, Desk L-C,
72. Arcier A. Stage fright. Tubiana R, Camadio PC, editors. Med- Sports Medicine Center, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN
ical problems of the instrumentalist musician. London: Mar- 55905, USA.
tin Dunitz, 2000: 507-20 E-mail: smith.aynsley@mayo.edu

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med 2003; 33 (1)

You might also like