You are on page 1of 11

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/7271672

Motor Development and Neuropsychological


Patterns in Persons with Down Syndrome

Article in Behavior Genetics · June 2006


DOI: 10.1007/s10519-006-9057-8 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

124 1,064

1 author:

Stefano Vicari
Ospedale Pediatrico Bambino Gesù
237 PUBLICATIONS 4,972 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Stefano Vicari on 23 May 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Behavior Genetics, Vol. 36, No. 3, May 2006 (Ó 2006)
DOI: 10.1007/s10519-006-9057-8

Motor Development and Neuropsychological Patterns


in Persons with Down Syndrome

Stefano Vicari1,2

Received 11 June 2005—Final July 7 2005

Neuropsychological research has permitted defining specific cognitive profiles among


individuals with mental retardation (MR) of different etiology. Namely, the cognitive profile
of people with Down syndrome (DS) is often reported to be characterized by a deficit in
language abilities that usually exceed impairments in visual–spatial capacities. However,
recent studies have demonstrated a more complex neuropsychological profile in this
population, with atypical development in the cognitive and in the linguistic domain. This
paper is dedicated to reviewing literature regarding motor, linguistic and cognitive abilities in
DS. Our aim is to present evidences supporting the hypothesis that individuals with these
syndrome exhibit a peculiar motor development and neuropsychological profile with some
abilities more preserved and others more impaired. This finding may have theoretical and
practical implications. In fact, a better definition of the cognitive pattern in DS may contribute
to understand the nature of MR in general and, also, it may suggests individualized
rehabilitation treatment protocols.
KEY WORDS: Brain development; cognition; Down syndrome; language; memory; Trisomy 21.

INTRODUCTION different laboratories have demonstrated a more


complex neuropsychological profile in this popula-
In the last years, neuropsychological research has
tion, with atypical development not only in the cog-
permitted defining different cognitive profiles among
nitive but also in the linguistic domain (for a review
subjects with mental retardation (MR) of different
Vicari et al., 2004a, b).
etiology. Numerous authors have stressed that cog-
A quite different pattern is often reported in
nitive profile of individuals with DS is characterized
other syndromes as Williams syndrome (WS). This is
by a remarkable deficit in language abilities that
another genetic condition, less frequent but equally
usually exceed impairments in visual–spatial abilities.
characterized by MR and typified by a number of
Several recent studies suggest, however, that a char-
severe medical anomalies, such as facial dysmor-
acterization of the cognitive profiles of DS children in
phology and abnormalities of the cardiovascular
terms of a dissociation between language and
system (Siegmuller and Bartke, 2004). Differently
visual–spatial abilities is too simplistic. Studies from
from DS, WS children often show marked impair-
ment in certain visual–spatial abilities (especially
1
Department of Neurology and Rehabilitation, Institute of
praxic-constructive) and relative preservation of both
Research and Clinical Care Bambino Gesù Children Hospital, productive and receptive language, at least concern-
Santa Marinella, Rome, Italy. ing the phonological elements (Vicari et al., 2004a,
2
To whom correspondence should be addressed at U.O.S. Neuro b). Moreover, different cognitive profiles have been
logia e Riabilitazione, IRCCS, Ospedale Pediatrico Bambino also described in individuals with comparable intel-
Gesù, Lungomare Guglielmo Marconi 36, I-00058, Santa Mari-
nella, Rome, Italy. Tel.: +39-0766-5244258; Fax: +39-0766-
lectual deficits or even with the same etiopathological
5244244; e-mail: vicari@opbg.net picture (Pezzini et al., 1999). All these observations
355
0001-8244/06/0500-0355/0 Ó 2006 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
356 Stefano Vicari

seem to support a theoretical approach that considers frequently associated with the syndrome. The lack of
MR not as a mere slowing of normal cognitive muscle tone is certainly related to the reduced
development, but as distinct, individual profiles, that strength in children and adolescents even if the exact
can be qualitatively specified. In line with this theo- influence of hypotonia on motor development re-
retical point of view (which also suggests the need for mains to be determined. Davis and Kelso (1982)
strongly individualized rehabilitation treatment pro- noted, for example, that the most important muscular
tocols), many recent studies emphasized the need to dysfunction in DS is not really represented by
better define not only the impaired cognitive abilities hypotonia but by a lack of control of muscles stiff-
in each subject, but just as importantly, the respective ness. Therefore, although hypotonia is one of the
strengths, or relatively preserved abilities in children most frequent muscular characteristic in infants with
with MR. DS, its role in the early motor development of these
This paper is dedicated to reviewing the neuro- children is far to be determined.
psychological literature regarding cognitive, motor
and behavioral abilities of individuals with DS.
GLOBAL COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT:
According with the theoretical perspective reported
THE IQ LEVEL
above, our main goal is to report evidences supporting
the hypothesis that individuals with DS exhibit a IQ in people with DS is usually in the moderately
peculiar motor development and neuropsychological to severely retarded range (IQ=25–55) and Mental
profile with some abilities more preserved and others Age is rarely above of 8 years (Gibson, 1978). How-
more impaired. The possible correlation between ever, few individuals with DS have been reported to
neuropsychological profile and brain development in have IQ in the normal range (Epstein, 1989). In a re-
DS will be also presented and discussed. cent paper, Vicari et al. (2004a, b) reported IQ data
from a sample of 56 Italian individuals with DS. The
mean IQ value was 44.7 with a range varying from 28
MOTOR DEVELOPMENT
to 71. What is worthy to note is that, in DS differently
Several pioneer studies (e.g., Carr, 1970; Melyn from the typically developing children, IQ is not con-
and White, 1973) have documented that children with stant across the life but it progressively decreases with
DS usually do not acquire motor skills at the same age (Pennington et al., 2003). Consistently, Vicari
rate as their typically developing (TD) peers. How- et al. (2004a) reported IQ values ranging between 45
ever, children with DS may achieve rolling between 5 and 71 in children with DS of 6.5–8 years of chrono-
and 6.4 months, independent sitting between 8.5 and logical age. IQ was, instead, lower in adolescents and
11.7 months, thus suggesting the emergence of motor young adults (chronological age=12.2–25.9) varying
milestones are only slightly retarded in DS. None- between 28 and 47.
theless, the delays is greater for later developing It must be also remarked that IQ in adults with
motor skills: DS infants crawl on hands and knees DS may be also influenced by the increased risk of
between 12.2 and 17.3 months of age and walk be- early onset dementia of the Alzheimer type often
tween 15 and 74 months (Melyn and White, 1973). reported in this syndrome (Bush and Beail, 2004).
Moreover, it must be remarked the enormous range
acquisition in the motor milestones of children with
LINGUISTIC ABILITIES
DS (it may be useful to remember here that TD peers
walk independently earlier than 18 months). Despite rare exceptions (Vallar and Papagno,
Consistently with these evidences, children with 1993; Rondal, 1995; Papagno and Vallar, 2001),
DS seem to follow the same sequence of motor adolescents and young adults with DS usually exhibit
milestones of TD infants (Palisano et al., 2001) albeit very poor linguistic capacities. It is worthy to note
qualitative differences or atypical movement patterns that there is no definitive evidence that language
in order to maintain postural stability are often impairment in DS is merely a consequence of the
reported. For example, these children often sit with hearing loss. This is reported for 40–80% of indi-
legs spread extremely far away from each other, walk viduals (Davis, 1996) and, usually, it is a consequence
with a wide base, and to pass from a prone to a sitting of recurrent periods of otitis media that may deter-
position they spread the legs and push up with the mine a conductive loss varying from mild to moder-
hands (Lydic and Steele, 1979). Unusual postures ate and, less frequently, sensorineural loss in
may be also connected to the presence of hypotonia, young adults (Roizen et al., 1993). Studies specifically
Motor Development and Neuropsychological Patterns 357

conducted did not demonstrate a relationship be- associates (1978, 1988). Although they confirm that
tween hearing loss and linguistic disorders (Marcell, MLU is markedly delayed in children with DS, they
1995; Jarrold and Baddeley, 1997) or found it to also note that MLU is correlated with chronological
account for only a small percentage of variance, less age even within the DS population, which means that
than 10%, in expressive morphosyntax (Chapman it can be used as an index of grammatical change.
et al., 2000). Other studies focus on the potential dissociation
Impairment in language development of children between lexical and morphosyntactic abilities in
with DS is evident when they are compared with children with DS. Singer Harris et al. (1997) com-
typical developing peers of the same mental age. pared the communicative and linguistic development
Franco and Wishart (1995), Caselli et al. (1998), of two groups of children with mental retardation,
Singer Harris et al. (1997) and Iverson et al. (2003) DS and WS. The results of the comparison show an
have, for example, described a more extensive use of overall delay in the linguistic area for both groups.
communicative gestures compared with TD infants at Nevertheless, more detailed examination reveals a
the same stage of communicative-linguistic develop- number of differences: children with DS display
ment. This ‘‘advantage’’ in the gestural modality is specific problems in the knowledge and use of
interpreted by the researchers as an indirect sign of a grammatical features, whereas subjects with WS
verbal ability that is having trouble developing display levels of grammar appropriate for their level
adequately. The difficulties described in DS children of lexical development.
do not only emerge in comparison with TD children In front of such poor linguistic production,
of similar mental age (Fowler, 1990; Miller, 1992; comprehension is less impaired and a more pro-
Chapman, 1995), but are also particularly evident nounced deficit in verbal production versus compre-
when their performances are compared to those of hension seems to occur starting from a development
individuals with MR of different etiology such as age of about 18 months. Furthermore, while com-
Williams syndrome (Wang and Bellugi, 1994; Klein prehension initially seems to develop in parallel with
and Mervis, 1999; Mervis and Robinson, 2000; Vicari the global cognitive level, this linguistic ability grad-
et al., 2000a). ually lags behind the stage of cognitive development
Many studies examining the different compo- to which the children belong, even though it remains
nents of linguistic abilities in DS have pointed out better than the production domain.
marked difficulty in the area of linguistic production, In contrast with the generalized and compro-
which is often limited to a telegraphic type of utter- mised linguistic pattern observed in older children,
ance with a highly reduced use of function words early linguistic development in infants with DS pre-
such as articles, prepositions, pronouns, etc. (Rondal sents some surprises, with a much less even pattern.
et al., 1988; Miller, 1992; Rondal, 1993; Chapman, In fact, in a precocious phase of development, pro-
1995; Vicari et al., 2000a). Fabbretti et al. (1997) duction and lexical comprehension abilities appear
compared linguistic production of DS adolescents quantitatively and qualitatively comparable to those
and TD controls (matched on Mean Length of of TD children with the same mental age (Cromer,
Utterance, MLU) on a story description task. The 1987; Fowler, 1990). Vicari et al. (2000a) explored
results revealed strong individual differences in the the acquisition of language in children with DS
sample of individuals with DS. Data analysis focused focusing on the potential dissociation between men-
on a subset of lexical, morphological and syntactic tal age and specific aspects of language. Particular
aspects of language use. Albeit the two groups used a attention was given on the emergence of morpho-
similar lexical repertoire, individuals with Down syntactic abilities compared with the lexicon. Fifteen
syndrome made more omissions of free morphemes children with DS (varying in chronological age from
than TD children and used unusual syntactic and 4 to 7 years) and 15 normal controls matched on
pragmatic forms. Other research focused on the mental age participated in the study. The results
development of morphosyntactic skills has uncovered showed for children with DS a lower performance in
important and specific problems in this area. language abilities with respect to the TD controls. No
Chapman (1995) have shown how some morpho- dissociation was evident between lexical and cogni-
syntactic aspects are jeopardized to a greater extent tive abilities in either group, but specific morpho-
than other linguistic measures, such as MLU in syntactic difficulties emerged both in comprehension
morphemes. The development of MLU in DS chil- and production for the group formed by participants
dren was thoroughly investigated by Rondal and with DS.
358 Stefano Vicari

In summary: with increasing age, the split be- Hitch, 1974; Baddeley, 1986). WM is not sub-served
tween lexical and morphosyntactic abilities decreases by a unitary store but by the co-operation of two
and a generalized picture of linguistic difficulty major systems. The first is a Central Executive Sys-
emerges (Fowler, 1990; Miller, 1992; Fabbretti et al., tem, a limited capacity central processor able to
1997; Vicari et al., 2004a). All these findings seem to temporarily store and process information from
suggest that linguistic characteristics of DS may de- many modalities. The second major system of the
velop differently along distinct developmental tra- WM model actually consists of a number of periph-
jectories. As Paterson et al. (1999) have argued, eral Slave Systems, or limited capacity systems, which
linguistic and cognitive skills in adolescents are not temporarily store and rehearse information belonging
predictable on the basis of the pattern exhibited at to a single modality when the flow of data surpasses
younger ages and, consequently, researchers ‘‘... the capacity of the Central Executive System. The
cannot rely on phenotypic outcomes to make gener- Articulatory Loop is a two-component system spe-
alizations about impaired or intact modules in the cialized for the temporary storage of verbal material.
initial state.’’ One component is devoted to the passive mainte-
nance of verbal information in a phonological code
(Phonological Store). The other component (Articu-
VISUAL–SPATIAL ABILITIES
latory Rehearsal) prevents the decay of material
In the visual–spatial domain individuals with DS stored in the Phonological Store by refreshing the
usually perform consistently to their mental age. This memory trace. Moreover, it is involved in the
relative strength in DS contrast with a greater general re-coding of visually presented verbal material into a
difficulty on spatial constructive processing often phonological format (Baddeley, 1986).
reported in children with WS. However, WS people is The Articulatory Loop model can account for
usually relatively preserved in visual–perceptual two robust experimental findings in verbal span: the
abilities such as, for example, facial recognition phonological similarity effect and the word-length
(Mervis et al., 1999; Pezzini et al., 1999; Bellugi and effect. The first effect refers to the phenomenon that
St. George, 2001). strings formed by phonologically similar words (e.g.,
In a very recent study, Vicari et al. (2005) rat, bat, cat, mat) are more difficult to recall imme-
described a discrepancy in the performance level diately after presentation than strings formed by
achieved by individuals with DS in the visual and phonologically dissimilar words (e.g., fish, girl, bus,
spatial domains. The group with DS were signifi- hand). The hypothesis that verbal material is held in
cantly poorer than mental age TD matched individ- an acoustic format in the Phonological Store and that
uals when visual perceptual and imagery material had as a consequence acoustically similar words form less
to be processed; however, when the task involved the distinctive memory traces may explain this finding.
processing of spatial data, performance of DS and The word-length effect refers to the finding that
TD participants did not significantly differ. To our memory span is longer for strings of short words
knowledge, this is the first description of reduced (e.g., bus, pig, car, tree) than for lists of long words
visual but relatively preserved spatial abilities in DS. (e.g., banana, elephant, policeman, kangaroo)
(Baddeley and Hitch, 1974). This finding is com-
monly interpreted as evidence of the contribution of
MEMORY
articulatory rehearsal to verbal span since long words
Studies from memory gave us further informa- take longer to be rehearsed than short words.
tion about cognitive and linguistic capabilities in DS, The visual–spatial sketchpad is the second
contributing to a better definition of their neuropsy- peripheral slave system and it is specialized for the
chological profile. temporary storage of visual material. Although the
Many previous studies have documented an functioning of this system has been far less investi-
impairment in verbal short-term and working mem- gated than that of the Articulatory Loop, there is
ory, measured by digit or word span, in individuals reason to believe that also here there is an internal
with DS compared to groups of mental age-matched fractionation of structure and functioning. Indeed,
controls (for a review Vicari and Carlesimo, 2002). clinical and experimental data support the hypothesis
Working memory (WM) is defined as a limited that temporary memory for visual-object information
capacity system for the temporary storage of infor- (such as registering colors and shapes) and for the
mation held for further manipulation (Baddeley and visual–spatial location of objects are processed by
Motor Development and Neuropsychological Patterns 359

different, but functionally related, subsystems (Logie, Fowler, 1995). In fact, a more general relationship
1995; Della Sala and Logie, 2002; Vicari et al., 2003). between language development and phonological
Hulme and Mackenzie (1992) made a qualitative memory is well-demonstrated in children with DS
analysis of the verbal short-term memory (STM) (Laws and Gunn, 2003; Laws, 2004) as well as in TD
disorder in DS in a study based on Baddeley’s model children (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1993). However,
of working memory. These authors administered a recent study by Vicari et al. (2004b) provided little
span tests for phonologically similar and dissimilar support for the hypothesis that defective functioning
verbal sequences and for words of increasing length of the Phonological Store component of the Articu-
to groups of DS, MR individuals of various etiology, latory Loop is responsible for poor verbal span in
mental age-matched TD children. They also assessed individuals with DS. Indeed, these authors docu-
articulatory speed using vocalization tasks of syllable mented analogous susceptibility to phonological
sequences. The results showed that individuals with similarity in a word span test in participants with DS
MR (Down and non-Down) had reduced spans than and TD controls.
TD children. Moreover, MR groups failed to show So far, few data have indicated a malfunctioning
phonological similarity and word length effects as central executive as the origin of poor verbal STM in
well as significant correlation between articulatory individuals with DS. In a previous study, Vicari et al.
speed and span size which, instead, was significant in (1995) presented forward Digit and Spatial (Corsi’s
the group of TD children. Hulme and Mackenzie block) span tasks and backward Digit and Spatial
(1992) interpreted these data as evidence of a reduced span tasks (in which the subject had to repeat the
contribution of the articulatory loop to the verbal numbers or reproduce the spatial sequence in the
span of persons with MR. In particular, the disabled reverse order) to groups of individuals with DS, with
persons would tend not to repeat the verbal sequences MR of various etiology and to mental age-matched
which, as a result, would decay rapidly from the TD children. Results showed a specific performance
phonological store. decay in backward span tasks in individuals with DS
At partial variance with Hulme and MacKen- compared to both typically-developing children and
zie’s results, Jarrold et al. (2000) and Kanno and to persons with MR of various etiology, thus sug-
Ikeda (2002) documented a significant word-length gesting that reduced resources of the executive system
effect in children with DS and mental age-matched are responsible for the particularly poor STM in
TD children. However, no correlation was found persons with DS.
between span extension and speech rate. In conclusion, compared to mental age-matched
These results raise a great deal of perplexity TD children, individuals with DS show poor verbal
about the mechanisms underlying the word-length STM on span tasks. This deficit seems to be inde-
effect in the verbal span of individuals with DS and of pendent of the articulatory difficulty they often
very young TD children. According to Jarrold et al. present. Instead, greater responsibility should be
(2000), the better memory for strings of short than of attributed to a poorly functioning phonological buf-
long words in these individuals cannot be the effect of fer or, even more, to deficits of the central executive
articulatory rehearsal (neither of the two groups system.
spontaneously engaged in rehearsal) but rather of There are very few data available on the func-
more time needed by the examiner to pronounce tioning of the visual–spatial sketchpad (the slave
strings of long than of short words, with greater de- system of the WM model devoted to the processing
cay from the phonological buffer of the former than of visual material) in children with mental retarda-
the latter. The non-use of the rehearsal mechanism in tion in general and with DS in particular. Wang
very young TD children (forming the control groups and Bellugi (1994) compared nine individuals with
in the above-mentioned studies) is also at odds with DS and 10 with WS on digit and Corsi span and
the hypothesis that defective functioning (or a lack of found that those with WS performed better on the
spontaneous utilization) of the rehearsal mechanism verbal short-term memory task but, at the same
is at the base of the poorer verbal span exhibited by time, had lower scores on the visual–spatial short-
DS individuals. term memory task. Indirectly, this suggests a rela-
Involvement of the phonological buffer in the tive advantage for DS participants in visual–spatial
verbal STM deficit of children with DS can be rather than in verbal span. Similar results were also
hypothesized on the basis of their difficulties in obtained by Jarrold et al. (1999) and by Laws
auditory phonological analysis (Chapman, 1995; (2002).
360 Stefano Vicari

Recently, Vicari et al. (2005) compared DS, WS were better than DS. On the other hand, the perfor-
and typically developing children matched for mental mances of the three groups did not differ in an im-
age in a visual and spatial span test. The two plicit memory test (repetition priming); both groups
tests involved studying the same complex, non- with MR performed as well as mental age matched
verbalizable figures and using the same response controls. These results confirmed a dissociation be-
modality (pointing to targets on the screen). The tween explicit and implicit memory in people with
crucial experimental variable was that in one case the MR.
position where the figure appeared on the screen had In the last few years, some experimental data has
to be recalled; in the other case, the physical aspect of been reported regarding the possible extension to
the figure studied had to be recalled. Results docu- individuals with MR of the dissociation between ex-
mented that people with DS showed reduced per- plicit and implicit memory processes so frequently
formance in both tests. Instead, individuals with WS described in brain damaged adults with memory
exhibited specific difficulties in the visual–spatial, but disorders. As for repetition priming, studies investi-
not the visual-object, working memory task. How- gating facilitation in identifying perceptually de-
ever, while the observed selective deficit in individuals graded pictures induced by the previous exposure to
with WS persisted even when perceptual abilities were the same pictures have consistently reported a com-
taken into account, the deficits in individuals with DS parable priming effect in individuals with MR and
were compensated when their scores were adjusted TD children matched for chronological or mental age
for perceptual levels. Indeed, after covarying for (for a review, Vicari and Carlesimo, 2002). Instead, a
performance level on the visual perceptual tasks, quite complex and somewhat contradictory pattern
performance of the DS participants and the TD of results emerged from studies investigating repeti-
children no longer differed on the working memory tion priming for verbal material. Most of these
tasks. These results suggest that working memory is studies were based on the Stem Completion proce-
not uniformly compromised in DS. Although this has dure in which subjects are requested to complete a list
been well established for verbal material, it is far less of stems (i.e., the first three letters) with the first word
sure in the visual–spatial domain where impairment that comes to mind. In this test, the priming effect is
in perceptual analysis rather than in memory pro- revealed by a bias in completing the stems with words
cesses is likely responsible for DS individuals’ poor that have been previously studied over unstudied
performance. ones. Carlesimo et al. (1997) and Vicari et al. (2000a,
Verbal and visual–spatial long-term memory has b, 2001) reported a priming effect with this procedure
been also extensively investigated in persons with in various groups of individuals with MR (etiologi-
mental retardation, and particularly with DS, both in cally unspecified, DS and WS) comparable to that of
the explicit and in the implicit component (Vicari and mental-age matched TD children.
Carlesimo, 2002). Explicit memory concerns inten- Less experimental works have been devoted to
tional recalling or recognition of experiences or investigate the ability to learn visuo-motor or cognitive
information. Implicit memory is manifested as a skills in individuals with MR and with DS in partic-
facilitation (that is an improvement in performance) ular. Vicari and co-workers suggested an intriguing
in perceptual, cognitive and motor tasks, without any difference in the skill learning abilities of DS and WS.
conscious reference to previous experiences. Explicit In the first study (Vicari et al., 2000b), a group
memory deficits in persons with MR and, particu- of individuals with DS showed the same rate of
larly, with DS have also been extensively docu- improvement as a group of mental-age matched TD
mented. According to recent studies, due to this children across successive trials of the Tower of
diffuse impairment of mnesic abilities, persons with London test and in the comparison of the repeated
MR should show a relative preservation of implicit vs. random blocks of a facilitated version of the Serial
memory. Reaction Time test (requiring implicit learning of the
Carlesimo et al. (1997) described long-term sequential order of a series of visual events), devised
memory abilities in persons with DS and in others by Nissen and Bullemer (1987). Instead, in the second
with MR of unknown etiology, comparing them with study, a group of children with WS showed signifi-
typically developing children of similar mental age. cantly less procedural learning than TD children on
The performance of the TD children in explicit both of these tests (Vicari et al., 2001).
memory tests was significantly better than those of In summary, in comparison with TD control of
children with MR of unknown etiology, and the latter comparable mental age, persons with DS usually
Motor Development and Neuropsychological Patterns 361

exhibit peculiar memory patterns. In short-term The neuropsychological profiles we described in


memory tasks, people with DS obtain lower perfor- DS might rely upon the difference within cortical and
mance scores than TD children processing both subcortical structures here observed. For example, in
verbal and visual–spatial material. However, in the agreement with Fabbro et al. (2002), the lower per-
visual–spatial domain impairment in perceptual formances of DS in linguistic tasks may be partially
analysis rather than in memory processes is likely explained in terms of impairment of the frontocere-
responsible for DS individuals’ poor performance. bellar structures involved in articulation and verbal
In long-term memory, people with DS exhibit working memory. Similarly, the reduced long-term
different patterns in explicit and implicit memory memory capacities may be related to the temporal
domains. Albeit individuals with DS are usually and, specifically, hippocampal dysfunction which
poorer than TD mental age control in verbal and seems to be one of the more relevant characteristic of
visual–spatial explicit memory tasks, in the implicit people with DS (Pennington et al., 2003).
memory domain comparable results may be observed Several recent observations suggest as the dorsal
between the two groups in repetition priming tasks as areas of the parietal cortex (besides the frontal ones)
well as in procedural learning. are markedly involved in the mediation of spatial
It is worthy to note that the memory profile processing; in contrast, the temporal ventral (and
observed in DS is not shared with other genetic perhaps frontal) areas intervene in working memory
syndromes also characterized by mental retardation. for objects and faces and, more generally, in the
We have, indeed, reported the case of William syn- processing of visual material (Courtney et al., 1996;
drome which is characterized by a relative strengths Nelson et al., 2000). Therefore, persons with Down
in verbal an visual short-term memory but impair- syndrome could present a relatively preserved matu-
ments in spatial working memory. A similar pattern ration of the dorsal compared to the ventral com-
is observed in explicit long-term memory domain, ponent of the visual system and thus perform
while an impairment in the ability to learn implicitly relatively better on visual-object than on visual–
new procedures has been reported in adolescents with spatial memory tests.
WS. Finally, concerning implicit memory, both neu-
ropsychological (Molinari et al., 1997) and functional
neuroimaging (Van Der Graaf et al., 2004) data
NEUROBIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES
assign a critical role to basal ganglia and cerebellum
The cognitive profile we have described in people in the implicit learning of visuo-motor skills. As
with DS presumably results from some specific reported above, the brains of individuals with Down
characteristics of their anomalous brain development. syndrome exhibit severe cerebellar hypoplasia with
However, any attempt to identify which neuroana- normal morphology of basal ganglia (Jernigan et al.,
tomical structures are specifically involved in the 1993).
cognitive impairment displayed by people with DS is Differently from the brain features reported in
speculative, and it must necessarily be based on DS, brains from individuals with WS usually exhibit
qualitative comparisons of their deficit with that a remarkable atrophy of the posterior region of the
displayed by patients with acquired brain lesions. In brain as well as of the basal ganglia (Jernigan et al.,
this regard, it is worth noting that autopsy observa- 1993; Bellugi et al., 1999). Moreover, albeit cerebellar
tions in people with DS report a lower brain weight volume is relatively preserved (Jernigan et al., 1993;
with particularly small cerebellum, frontal and tem- Bellugi et al., 1999) a neurochemical alteration
poral lobes (Wisniewski, 1990). Consistent with this, (reduction of the neurotransmitter N-acetylaspartate)
volumetric MRI studies of individuals with DS have has been demonstrated in the cerebellum (Rae et al.,
documented reduced overall brain volumes, with 1998).
disproportionately smaller volumes in frontal, tem- A further characteristic of the WS brain, more
poral (including uncus, amygdala, hippocampus and recently described, is a reduced volume in the pos-
parahippocampal gyrus) and cerebellar regions (Pin- terior regions of the corpus callosum (Tomaiuolo
ter et al., 2001). In contrast, brains of people with DS et al., 2002). This hypoplasia of the corpus callosum
usually show relatively preserved volume of subcor- may determine a defective callosal transfer of infor-
tical areas, such as lenticular nuclei (Bellugi et al., mation, thus effecting an insufficient integration and
1999) as well as posterior (parietal and occipital) coordination of the activity of both cerebral hemi-
cortical gray matter (Pinter et al., 2001). spheres. On the basis of all these observations, the
362 Stefano Vicari

possible role played by the reduced posterior regions domains, falling further behind in others, or flatten-
of brain and corpus callosum in the visual–spatial ing out at a developmental ceiling that children with
difficulties in individuals with WS is, although spec- severe learning disabilities cannot surpass.
ulative, very suggestive. Thus, the more pronounced From a theoretical point of view, the results of
difficulty in managing visual–spatial material re- the experimental literature reviewed above are rele-
ported in individuals with Williams syndrome could vant to students working in the fields of both normal
be related to a particularly delayed maturation of the cognitive development and neuropsychological out-
dorsal visual system. come following cerebral damage. The former can
Concerning visuo-motor skill learning, it may be compare ‘‘normal’’ cognitive development with that
tentatively conclude that difficulties reported in peo- of people with DS in an attempt to dissociate distinct
ple with Williams syndrome are related to the defi- functional components of the cognitive and linguistic
cient maturation of striatal circuits known to be systems on the basis of discrepant developmental
critical for this ability. trends. The latter can attempt to apply the same
Studies conducted in genetic syndromes describ- theoretical and experimental approaches already used
ing their neuropsychological profiles in relationship in the investigation of neuropsychological disordered
with specific brain characteristics are just at the adults to the deficits exhibited by people with DS to
beginning. However, it looks a very fascinating topic gain new insights on the neural substrate and the
to better understand the biological nature of behavior basic mechanisms of normal and pathological
and to interpret the cognitive and linguistic differ- cognitive development. From a practical point of
ences usually observed among people with mental view, these data can provide invaluable information
retardation. for educational psychologists and teachers for plan-
ning rationally grounded interventions to alleviate
the learning difficulties and social maladjustment of
CONCLUSION
these individuals.
Down syndrome is characterized by a complex
neuropsychological profile with some abilities more
impaired than others. In particular, motor, language
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
and specifically morpho-syntax, verbal short-term
memory, explicit long-term memory are usually im- The financial support of Telethon-Italy (Grant
paired and visual–spatial short-term memory and No. E.C. 685), of CNR (Grant CNRC002D39_001
implicit long-term memory are relatively preserved. ‘‘Clinical tools for the identification and the early
This cognitive pattern is sustained by a particular diagnosis of language disorders in infancy’’) and
brain development with a reduced volumes in frontal, FIRB/MIUR (Grant No. RBNE01SZB4) are grate-
temporal and cerebellar regions and a relative pres- fully acknowledged. I would like to thank Maria
ervation in subcortical areas, such as lenticular nuclei Cristina Caselli, Virginia Volterra and Giovanni
as well as posterior (parietal and occipital) cortical Augusto Carlesimo. Their friendly and precious ef-
gray matter. forts in cooperating in more than 15 years let possible
However, every theory of the neural substrates all the studies here reported.
of people with DS have to consider that neuropsy-
chological profiles change from infancy to adoles-
cence in this population. There are at least two
possible explanations. First, the neural differences REFERENCES
between the population may be established at birth, Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H.
but respond differentially to the various linguistic and Bower (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. New
York: Academic Press, pp. 47–90.
non-linguistic tasks that are used. This would occur Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. London: Oxford Uni-
because developmental changes in task difficulty versity Press.
require the recruitment of different neural systems Bellugi, U., Mills, D., Jerningan, T., Hickok, G., and Galaburda, A.
(1999). Linking cognition, brain structure, and brain func-
(e.g., different substrates are required for ‘‘easy’’ tion in Williams syndrome. In H. Tager-Flusberg (ed.),
items passed by younger children and ‘‘hard’’ items Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp.
passed by older children). Second, behavior and brain 111–136.
Bellugi, U., and St. George, M. (2001). Journey from Cognition to
may change together dynamically across the course of Brain to Gene. Perspectives from Williams Syndrome. Cam-
development, reflected in ‘‘catching up’’ in some bridge MA: MIT Press.
Motor Development and Neuropsychological Patterns 363

Bush, A., and Beail, N. (2004). Risk factors for dementia in people syndrome and typically-developing children in the early stages
with Down syndrome: issues in assessment and diagnosis. Am. of communicative development. Int. J. Lang. Communicat.
J. Mental Retard. 109:83–97. Disorders 38:179–197.
Carlesimo, G. A., Marotta, L., and Vicari, S. (1997). Long-term Jarrold, C., and Baddeley, A. D. (1997). Short-term memory for
memory in mental retardation: evidence for a specific verbal and visuo-spatial information in Down’s syndrome.
impairment in subjects with Down’s Syndrome. Neuropsy- Cognit. Neuropsychiat. 1:101–122.
chology 35:71–79. Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., and Hewes, A. K. (1999). Genetically
Carr, J. (1970). Mental and motor development in young mongol dissociated components of working memory: evidence from
children. J. Mental Defic. Res. 14:205–220. Down and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology 37:637–651.
Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., Longobardi, E., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C., and Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., and Hewes, A. K. (2000). Verbal
Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in early development of short-term memory deficits in Down syndrome: a consequence
children with Down syndrome. J. Speech Hear. Res. of problems in rehearsal?. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat.
41:1125–1135. 40:233–244.
Chapman, R. S. (1995). Language development in children and Jernigan, T. L., Bellugi, U., Sowell, E., Doherty, S., and Hesselink, R.
adolescents with Down syndrome. In P. Fletcher and B. (1993). Cerebral morphologic distinctions between WS and
MacWinney (eds.), The Handbook of Child Language. Oxford: DS. Archiv. Neurol. 50:186–191.
Blackwell. Kanno, K., and Ikeda, Y. (2002). Word-length effect in verbal
Chapman, R. S., Seung, H. K., Schwartz, S. E., and Kay-Raining short-term memory in individuals with Down’s syndrome.
Bird, E. (2000). Language production in children and adoles- J. Intellect. Disab. Res. 46:613–618.
cents with Down syndrome: the role of comprehension. Klein, B. P., and Mervis, C. B. (1999). Cognitive strength and
J. Speech, Lang. Hear. Res. 43:340–350. weaknesses of 9- and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome or
Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Keil, K., and Haxby, J. V. Down syndrome. Develop. Neuropsychol. 16:177–196.
(1996). Object and visual working memory activate separate Laws, G. (2002). Working memory in children and adolescents
neural systems in human cortex. Cereb. Cortex 6:39–49. with Down syndrome: evidence from a colour memory
Cromer, R. (1987). The cognition hypothesis revisited. In F. Kessel experiment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 43:353–364.
(ed.), The Development of Language and Language Researches. Laws, G. (2004). Contributions of phonological memory, language
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 48–65. comprehension and hearing to the expressive language of
Davis, B. (1996). Auditory disorders. In B. Stratford and P. Gunn adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. J. Child
(eds.), New Approaches to Down Syndrome. London: Cassell, Psychol. Psychiat. 45:1–11.
pp. 100–121. Laws, G., and Gunn, D. (2003). Phonological memory as a pre-
Davis, W. E., and Kelso, J. A. S. (1982). Analysis of ‘‘invariant dictor of language comprehension in Down syndrome: a five-
characteristics’’ in motor control of Down’s syndrome and year follow-up study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 44:1–11.
normal subjects. J. Mot. Behav. 14:194–212. Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-Spatial Working Memory. Hove: Law-
Della Sala, S., and Logie, R. H. (2002). Neuropsychological rence Erlbaum Associates.
impairments of visual and spatial working memory. In A. D. Lydic, J. S., and Steele, C. (1979). Assessment of the quality of
Baddeley, B. Wilson and M. Kopelman (eds.), Handbook of sitting and gait patterns in children with Down syndrome.
Memory Disorders. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons, pp. Phys. Therapy 59:1489–1494.
271–292. Marcell, M. M. (1995). Relationships between hearing and audi-
Epstein, C. J. (1989). Down syndrome. In C. R. Scriver, A. L. tory cognition in Down’s syndrome youth. Down’s Syndrome:
Beaudet, W. S. Sly and P. Valle (eds.), The Metabolic Basis of Res. Pract. 3:75–91.
Inherited Disease. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 291–396. Melyn, M. A., and White, D. T. (1973). Mental and developmental
Fabbretti, D., Pizzuto, E., Vicari, S., and Volterra, V. (1997). A story milestones of non-istitutionalized Down’s syndrome children.
description task in children with Down syndrome: lexical and Pediatrics 52:542–545.
morphosyntactic abilities. J. Intell. Disab. Res. 41:165–179. Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Bertrand, J., and Robinson, B. F.
Fabbro, F., Alberti, A., Gagliardi, C., and Borgatti, R. (2002). (1999). Williams syndrome: findings from an integrated pro-
Differences in native and foreign language repetition task be- gram of research. In H. Tager-Flusberg (ed.), Neurodevelop-
tween subjects with Williams and Down syndromes. J. Neu- mental Disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 65–110.
roling. 15:1–10. Mervis, C. B., and Robinson, B. R. (2000). Expressive vocabulary
Fowler, A. E. (1990). Language abilities in children with Down ability of toddlers with Williams syndrome or Down syn-
syndrome: evidence for a specific delay. In D. Cicchetti and M. drome: a comparison. Develop. Neuropsychol. 17:111–126.
Beeghley (eds.), Children with Down Syndrome: A Develop- Miller, J. F. (1992). Development of speech and language in
mental Perspective. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University children with Down syndrome. In I. T. Lott and E. E. Mc Loy
Press. (eds.), Down Syndrome. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss, pp.
Fowler, A. E. (1995). Language variability in persons with Down 39–50.
syndrome. In L. Nadel and D. Rosenthal (eds.), Down Syn- Molinari, M., Leggio, M. G., Solida, A., Ciorra, R., Misciagna, S.,
drome: Living and Learning in the Community. New York: and Silveri, M. (1997). Cerebellum and procedural learning:
Wiley-Liss. evidence from focal cerebellar lesions. Brain 120:1753–1762.
Franco, F., and Wishart, F. (1995). The use of pointing and other Nelson, C. A., Monk, C. S., Lin, J., Carver, L. J., Thomas, K. M.,
gestures by young children with children with Down syn- and Truwit, C. L. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of spatial
drome. Am. J. Mental Retard. 100:160–182. working memory in children. Develop. Psychol. 36:109–116.
Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working Memory Nissen, M. J., and Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of
and Language. Hove (UK): Lawrence Erlbaum. learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognit. Psy-
Gibson, D. (1978). Down Syndrome: The Psychology of Mongolism. chol. 19:1–32.
London: Cambridge University Press. Palisano, R. J., Walter, S. D., Russell, D. J., Rosenbaum, P. L.,
Hulme, C., and Mackenzie, S. (1992). In (eds.), it Working Mem- Gémus, M., and Galuppi, B. E. (2001). Gross motor function
ory and Severe Learning Difficulties. Hove: Lawrence Erl- in children with Down syndrome: creation of motor growth
baum Associates curves. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabilit. 82:494–500.
Iverson, J., Longobardi, E., and Caselli, M. C. (2003). The rela- Papagno, C., and Vallar, G. (2001). Understanding metaphors and
tionship between gestures and words in children with Down idioms: a single-case neuropsychological study in a person
364 Stefano Vicari

with Down syndrome. J. Internat. Neuropsychol. Soc. Van Der Graaf, F. H., De Jong, B. M., Maguire, R. P., Meiners, L.
7:516–528. C., and Leenders, K. L. (2004). Cerebral activation related to
Paterson, S., Brown, J. H., Johnson, M. C., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. skills practice in a double serial reaction time task: striatal
(1999). Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science involvement in random-order sequence learning. Brain Res.
17:2355–2358. Cognit. Brain Res. 20:120–131.
Pennington, B. F., Moon, J., Edgin, J., Stedron, J., and Nadel, L. Vicari, S., Carlesimo, G. A., and Caltagirone, C. (1995). Short-
(2003). The neuropsychology of Down syndrome: evidence for term memory in persons with intellectual disabilities and
hippocampal dysfunction. Child Develop. 74:75–93. Down syndrome. J. Intellect. Disab. Res. 39:532–537.
Pezzini, G., Vicari, S., Volterra, V., Milani, L., and Ossella, M. T. Vicari, S., Caselli, M. C., and Tonucci, F. (2000a). Early language
(1999). Children with Williams syndrome: is there a single development in Italian children with Down syndrome: asyn-
neuropsychological profile?. Develop. Neuropsychol. chrony of lexical and morphosyntactic abilities. Neuropsy-
15:141–155. chology 38:634–644.
Pinter, J. D., Eliez, S., Schmitt, J. E., Capone, G. T., and Reiss, A. Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2000b). Implicit and
L. (2001). Neuroanatomy of Down’s syndrome: a high-reso- explicit memory: a functional dissociation in persons with
lution MRI study. Am. J. Psychiat. 158:1659–1665. Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia 38:240–251.
Rae, C., Lee, M. A., Dixon, R. M., Blamire, A. M., Thompson, C. Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2001). Procedural
H., and Styles, P. (1998). Brain biochemistry in Williams learning deficit in children with Williams syndrome. Neuro-
syndrome: evidence for a role of the cerebellum in cognition?. psychology 39:665–677.
Neurology 51:33–40. Vicari, S., and Carlesimo, G.A. (2002). Children with intellectual
Roizen, N. J., Wolters, C., Nicol, T., and Blondis, T. A. (1993). disabilities. In A. Baddeley, B. Wilson, and M. Kopelman
Hearing loss in children with Down syndrome. J. Ped. (eds.), Handbook of Memory Disorders. John Wiley & Sons,
123:S9–S12. Ltd., pp. 501–518.
Rondal, J. A. (1978). Developmental sentence scoring procedure Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2003). Visual and
and delay-difference question in language development of spatial working memory dissociation: evidence from a genetic
Down’s syndrome children. Ment. Retard. 16:169–171. syndrome. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. 45:269–273.
Rondal, J. A. (1993). Exceptional cases of language development Vicari, S., Bates, E., Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P., Gagliardi, C.,
in mental retardation: the relative autonomy of language as a and Tonucci, F. (2004a). Neuropsychological profile of Ital-
cognitive system. In H. Tager-Flusberg (ed.), Constraints on ians with Williams syndrome: an example of a dissociation
Language Acquisition: Studies of Atypical Children. NJ: Erl- between language and cognition?. J. Internat. Neuropsychol.
baum Hillsdale, pp. 155–174. Soc. 10:862–876.
Rondal, J. A. (1995). Exceptional Language Development in Down Vicari, S., Marotta, L., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2004b). Verbal
Syndrome. England: Cambridge University Press. short-term memory in Down’s syndrome: an articulatory loop
Rondal, J. A., Ghiotto, M., Brédart, S., and Bachelet, J. F. (1988). deficit?. J. Intellect. Disab. Res. 48:80–92.
Mean length of utterance of children with Down syndrome. Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2005). Evidence from
Am. J. Ment. Retard. 93:64–66. two genetic syndromes for the independence of spatial and
Siegmuller, J., and Bartke, S. (2004). Williams syndrome from a visual working memory. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. in press
clinical perspective. In J. Siegmuller and S. Bartke (eds.), Wang, P. P., and Bellugi, U. (1994). Evidence from two genetic
Williams Syndrome Across Languages. Philadelphia, USA: syndromes for dissociation between verbal and visual–spatial
John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 9–37. short-term memory. J. Clin. Exper. Neuropsychol. 16:317–322.
Singer Harris, N. G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., and Rossen, Wisniewski, K. E. (1990). Down syndrome children often have
M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language in children with brain with maturation delay, retardation of growth, and
Williams and Down syndromes. Develop. Neuropsychol. cortical digenesis. Am. J. Med. Genet. (Suppl). 7:274–281.
13:345–370.
Tomaiuolo, F., Di Paola, M., Caravale, B., Vicari, S., Petrides, M.,
and Caltagirone, C. (2002). Morphology and morphometry of Edited by Pierre L. Roubertoux
the corpus callosum in Williams syndrome: a magnetic
resonance imaging analysis. NeuroReport 13:1–5.
Vallar, G., and Papagno, C. (1993). Preserved vocabulary acqui-
sition in Down’s syndrome children: the role of phonological
short-term memory. Cortex 29:467–483.

View publication stats

You might also like