You are on page 1of 7

Neuropsychology © 2017 American Psychological Association

2017, Vol. 31, No. 6, 682– 688 0894-4105/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000335

Triglycerides Are Negatively Correlated With Cognitive Function in


Nondemented Aging Adults

Vishnu Parthasarathy Darvis T. Frazier


University of California, San Diego University of California, San Francisco

Brianne M. Bettcher Laura Jastrzab and Linda Chao


University of Colorado University of California, San Francisco
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Bruce Reed and Dan Mungas Michael Weiner


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

University of California, Davis University of California, San Francisco

Charles DeCarli Helena Chui


University of California, Davis University of Southern California

Joel H. Kramer
University of California, San Francisco

Objective: Vascular risk factors like hyperlipidemia may adversely affect brain function. We hypothe-
sized that increased serum triglycerides are associated with decreased executive function and memory in
nondemented elderly subjects. We also researched possible vascular mediators and white matter micro-
structure as assessed with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Design/Method: Participants were 251
nondemented elderly adults (54% male) with a mean age of 78 (SD ⫽ 6.4; range: 62–94) years and a
mean education of 15.6 (SD ⫽ 2.9; range: 8 –23) years. Fasting blood samples were used to detect serum
triglyceride and low-density lipoprotein (LDL) levels along with ApoE4 status. DTI was used to
determine whole brain fractional anisotropy (FA). Composite executive and memory scores were derived
from item response theory. Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scores provided informant-based measures
of daily functioning. Results: Triglyceride levels were inversely correlated with executive function, but
there was no relationship with memory. Controlling for age, gender, and education did not affect this
correlation. This relationship persisted after controlling for vascular risk factors like LDL, total choles-

This article was published Online First June 12, 2017. Michael Weiner served on the scientific advisory boards for Pfizer;
Vishnu Parthasarathy, Department of Emergency Medicine, University BOLT International; Neurotrope Bioscience; Eli Lilly; University of Penn-
of California, San Diego; Darvis T. Frazier, Memory and Aging Center, sylvania’s Neuroscience of Behavior Initiative; National Brain Research
University of California, San Francisco; Brianne M. Bettcher, Anschutz Centre, India; Dolby Family Ventures, LP; and Alzheimer’s Disease Neu-
School of Medicine, University of Colorado; Laura Jastrzab, Memory and roimaging Initiative. He served on the editorial boards for Alzheimer’s &
Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco; Linda Chao, De- Dementia and Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Vishnu Parthasarathy, Darvis
partment of Radiology, University of California, San Francisco; Bruce T. Frazier, Brianne M. Bettcher, Laura Jastrzab, Linda Chao, Bruce Reed,
Reed and Dan Mungas, Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, University Dan Mungas, Charles DeCarli, Helena Chui, and Joel H. Kramer have no
of California, Davis; Michael Weiner, Center for Imaging of Neurodegen- disclosures to declare.
This study was funded by National Institutes of Aging, Bethesda, MD
erative Diseases, University of California, San Francisco; Charles DeCarli,
(AG123435, AG10129) and by the California Department of Health Services
Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, University of California, Davis;
Alzheimer’s Disease Program (Contracts 94 –20354, 94 –20355, 98 –14970,
Helena Chui, Department of Neurology, University of Southern California;
and 98 –14971). Brianne M. Bettcher was supported by National Institutes of
Joel H. Kramer, Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San
Health/National Institute on Aging (NIH/NIA) (K23AG042492). Michael
Francisco.
Weiner was supported by NIH/NIA/National Institute of Mental Health, U.S.
Statistical analysis was conducted by Vishnu Parthasarathy and Darvis Department of Defense, Alzheimer’s Association, Alzheimer’s Drug Discov-
T. Frazier. ery Foundation, Merck, Avid, and the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs
Vishnu Parthasarathy and Darvis T. Frazier: design, statistics, and man- (2U01AG024904, W81XWH-13-1-0259, W81XWH-12-2-0012, R01 AG10897,
uscript preparation; Brianne M. Bettcher: statistics; Laura Jastrzab: labo- 1P41 EB015904, P01 AG19724, R01 AG032306, R01A G03879, ADNI 2-12-
ratory analysis; Linda Chao: imaging data preparation and analysis; Bruce 233036, 20110506, R01 MH098062-01).
Reed: manuscript preparation; Dan Mungas: statistics; Michael Weiner: Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joel H.
imaging, data preparation and analysis; Charles DeCarli: manuscript prep- Kramer, Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Fran-
aration; Helena Chui: design; Joel H. Kramer: design and manuscript cisco, 1500 Owens Street 320, San Francisco, CA 94158. E-mail:
preparation. joel.kramer@ucsf.edu

682
TRIGLYCERIDES AND COGNITIVE FUNCTION IN AGING 683

terol, CDR and ApoE4 status. Lastly, adding whole-brain FA to the model did not affect the correlation
between triglycerides and executive function. Conclusion: Triglyceride levels are inversely correlated
with executive function in nondemented elderly adults after controlling for age, education, gender, total
cholesterol, LDL, ApoE4 status, CDR, and white-matter microstructure. The fact that the effect of
triglycerides on cognition was not clearly mediated by vascular risks or cerebrovascular injury raises
questions about widely held assumptions of how triglycerides might impact cognition function.

General Scientific Summary


In this cross-sectional cohort study, it was discovered that triglyceride levels are negatively associ-
ated with executive function scores and not memory scores in non-demented, aging adults. This
association lies independant of cardiovascular and Alzheimer’s disease risk factors and does not
appear to be mediated by changes in cerebral microstructure white-matter as measured by DTI
fractional anisotropy.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Keywords: triglycerides, dementia, executive function, aging, memory

In the United States, over 10% of those ages 65 and over are cognition in older adults. It is well established that chronic eleva-
cognitively impaired (Dimopoulos et al., 2007) with vascular risk tion of circulating triglycerides is closely associated with meta-
factors widely considered to be one of several potential contribut- bolic dysregulation and inflammation, risk factors of cognitive
ing factors (Hokanson & Austin, 1996; Luchsinger & Mayeux, impairment (Ford, DiGiovanni, & Hursting, 2013; Jung & Choi,
2004; Whitmer, Sidney, Selby, Johnston, & Yaffe, 2005). While 2014; Welty, 2013).
the effect of risk factors like hypertension and Type II diabetes on Biomarkers of metabolic dysregulation, including central obe-
cognitive function are well established in the literature, the rela- sity, hypertriglyceridemia, and insulin resistance, have consistently
tionship between triglycerides and cognition remains controversial shown to have inverse relationships with memory (Farr et al.,
(Luchsinger & Mayeux, 2004; Whitmer et al., 2005). Some studies 2008) and executive function systems (Frazier et al., 2015). Given
suggest that increased serum triglycerides are correlated with poor the significant associations between triglycerides and proinflam-
cognitive performance (Dong, Zhang, Lu, & Flaherty, 2009; Rog- matory markers, it is not surprising that declines in memory
ers, Meyer, McClintic, & Mortel, 1989), while others either find no (Gunstad, Paul, Cohen, Tate, & Gordon, 2006), executive function,
relationship (Dong et al., 2009; Van Exel et al., 2002) or suggest and processing speed performance (Marioni et al., 2009; Marsland
that low serum triglyceride levels lead to poorer memory (Dimo- et al., 2006) have often been observed in the setting of systemic
poulos et al., 2007; Lepara, Valjevac, Alajbegović, Zaćiragić, & inflammation. This relationship may be further modified by factors
Nakas-Ićindić, 2009). In addition, whereas most studies have fo- such as ApoE4 status (Schram et al., 2007), as well as cerebral
cused on the relationship between triglycerides and memory (Di- white-matter microstructure integrity (Bettcher et al., 2013). It is
therefore reasonable to propose that circulating serum triglycerides
mopoulos et al., 2007; Dong et al., 2009; Lepara et al., 2009;
may be related to memory (and executive function), and this
Luchsinger & Mayeux, 2004; Whitmer et al., 2005), few have
relationship may be influenced by the ApoE4 allele, as well as
explored the effects of triglycerides on other measures of cognitive
microstructural white-matter health in nondemented older adults.
function, particularly executive function.
The current investigation aims to address two specific questions:
With the advent of clinical biomarkers has come an impetus for
(1) Do serum triglycerides correlate with memory and executive
the detection of preclinical neurodegenerative disease. This has
functions in nondemented aging adults? (2) What might be the
been a mainstay in the ongoing development of disease-modifying
underlying mechanisms driving the relationship between triglyc-
therapies and preventive treatments. Use of cerebrospinal fluid, erides and cognition?
high-field structural and functional MRI, and inflammatory labo-
ratory panels has proven to be time consuming and costly. Blood-
based phospholipid panels are currently being investigated as a Method
viable alternative for predicting phenotypic neurodegenerative
conversion, and findings have been thus far encouraging (Map-
stone et al., 2014). Understanding how other potential blood-based
Participants
biomarkers may be used in the detection of early neurodegenera- Participants included 251, nondemented community-dwelling
tive disease and disease progression will be crucial in further older adults recruited to participate in a multisite study, “Aging
advancing the field of neurological treatment for these conditions. Brain: Vasculature, Ischemia, and Behavior” (VIB), designed to
Triglycerides have been well implicated in the pathogenesis of investigate changes in brain structure and function associated with
cardiovascular disease, and have been shown to mediate the uptake markers of atherosclerosis. Eligible subjects were over 60 years
of a number of gastrointestinal hormones such as leptin, ghrelin, old, spoke fluent English, and had either normal cognition or mild
and insulin across the blood– brain barrier (Banks, 2012; Urayama cognitive impairment which were denoted by a clinical dementia
& Banks, 2008). These findings have received a growing body of rating (CDR) of either 0 or 0.5, respectively. Exclusion criteria
interest, especially as it relates to the role of maintaining healthy included a current diagnosis of dementia, major depressive disor-
684 PARTHASARATHY ET AL.

der, history of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, seizures, Parkin- Assessment Scales (Williams, 1991), as well as the short and long
son’s disease, multiple sclerosis, head injury with loss of con- delayed free recall from the same test.
sciousness over 15 min, current drug or alcohol abuse, significant Scale construction methods for composite scores derived from a
hematologic or metabolic illness, and the use of medications that larger sample of 400 subjects and were guided by item-response
affect cognition (e.g., prescription-level pain medications). Our theory (IRT), a commonly used approach to large-scale psycho-
final sample consisted of 251 participants (49.7% male) with a metric test development. In short, IRT analyses produce scale-
mean age of 78 (SD ⫽ 6.4; range: 66 –95) years, and a mean level functions that characterize the inherent psychometric prop-
education of 15.7 (SD ⫽ 2.9, range: 8 –23) years. Participants erties of the measure, including a scale that captures the reliability
underwent neuropsychological assessment in addition to fasting at each point along the ability continuum, which is derived from
blood draws and an MRI scan. Initial analyses were based on a the testing information curve (TIC), as well as a scale that reflects
sample of 251; only a subset of 163 had whole-brain fractional the expected test score at each ability point, which is based on the
anisotropy (FA) data. Participant demographics are displayed in testing characteristic curve. The two composite measures had TICs
Table 1. Written informed consent was obtained from all partici- demonstrating high reliability (r ⱖ .90) from approximately 2.0
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

pants at each participating institution following the protocols ap- standard deviations (SD) below the mean of the overall develop-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

proved by the institutional review boards. ment sample to 2.0 SD above the mean. Consequently, EXEC and
MEM measures have a broad range of measurement, unencum-
bered by floor or ceiling effects for participants, as well as linear
Cognitive Assessment measurement properties across the broad ability range. In addition,
Standardized neuropsychological tests were administered to all the measures allow for parametric testing, as they were near-
participants. Psychometrically matched measures of executive normally distributed (Mungas, 2005).
function and verbal memory were created using item-response
theory, described in detail in a previous publication by Mungas, Blood Draws
Reed, and Kramer (2003). The executive functioning measure
(EXEC) is a composite derived from the Dementia Rating Scale Blood draws were performed on fasting participants at baseline.
Initiation—Perseveration subscale (Yamada, Mitsuno, Kato, & Fasting blood samples were used to detect serum levels of triglycer-
Hirano, 1997), the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised digit span ides and low-density lipoprotein (LDL) using standard laboratory
backward and visual span backward (Wechsler, 1987), and a protocols. Blood samples were also used for genetic testing to deter-
controlled oral word fluency task (F-A-S) (Benton & Hamsher, mine ApoE4 status. Interval times between blood draws, neuropsy-
1989). The memory measure (MEM) was derived using total recall chology testing, and neuroimaging did not exceed 30 days.
on Trials 2– 6 on the Word List Learning Test of the Memory
MRI Acquisition and Processing
Participants were scanned using a 3T or 4T MRI system at four
Table 1 different scanning sites—University of Southern California; Uni-
Participants (Valid n ⫽ 163) versity of California, Davis, research center; San Francisco De-
partment of Veterans Affairs Medical Center; and University of
Variable Mean (SD) % total Range
California, San Francisco, Neuroscience Imaging Center. The Uni-
Age (years) 78.2 (6.5) 66–92 versity of Southern California used a 3T General Electric Signal
Education (years) 15.7 (2.9) 9–24 HDx system with an 8-channel head coil. Acquired images in-
Global FA 0.4 (0.02) 0.32–0.44 cluded a T1-weighted volumetric SPGR scan (TR ⫽ 7 ms, TE ⫽
Triglycerides (mg/dL) 2.0 (0.2) 1.6–2.9
LDL (mg/dL) 94.6 (30.4) 25–202 2.9 ms, TI ⫽ 650 ms, with 1 mm3 isotropic resolution). The
Cholesterol 173.8 (37.3) 80–295 University of California, Davis (UC Davis) research center used a
MAP 94.4 (10.9) 61–128 3T Siemens Magnetom Trio Syngo system with an 8-channel head
FCRS 13.36 (8.3) 2–56 coil as well as a 3T Siemens Magnetom TrioTim system with an
SBP 139.0 (19.4) 96–206
DBP 72.4 (9.7) 42–100
8-channel head coil. Acquired images for all UC Davis participants
Memory score 98.6 (19.5) 52–141 included a T1-weighted volumetric MP-RAGE scan (TR ⫽ 2,500,
Executive score 94.1 (16.7) 48–129 TE ⫽ 2.98, TI ⫽ 1,100, with 1 mm3 isotropic resolution). Partic-
Sex (male) 57.9 ipants were scanned at the San Francisco Department of Veterans
CDR of 0.5 39.6 Affairs Medical Center using a 4T Siemens MedSpec Syngo
ApoE4 allele carriers 22.6
MI 14 System with an 8-channel head coil. A T1-weighted volumetric
Stroke 27.1 MP-RAGE scan (TR ⫽ 2,300, TE ⫽ 2.84, TI ⫽ 950, with 1 mm3
Diabetes 29.2 isotropic resolution) was acquired. The University of California,
Smoking 94.3 San Francisco, Neuroscience Imaging Center used a 3T Siemens
Anti-HTN 76.3
CABG 19.5
Magnetom TrioTim system with a 12-channel head coil. Acquired
images included a T1-weighted volumetric MP-RAGE scan (TR ⫽
Note. Global FA ⫽ global functional anisotropy; LDL ⫽ low-density 2,500, TE ⫽ 2.98, TI ⫽ 1,100, with 1 mm3 isotropic resolution).
lipoprotein; MAP ⫽ mean arterial pressure; FCRS ⫽ Framingham cardio-
vascular score; SBP ⫽ systolic blood pressure; DBP ⫽ diastolic blood
Inter- and intrareliability tests were run with preliminary experi-
pressure; CDR ⫽ clinical dementia rating; MI ⫽ myocardial infarction; mental subjects on all scanners involved. A protocol comparable to
Anti-HTN ⫽ antihypertensives; CABG ⫽ coronary artery bypass graft. that used in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
TRIGLYCERIDES AND COGNITIVE FUNCTION IN AGING 685

study was executed by Michael Weiner for MRI harmonization for diffusion tensor imaging fractional anisotropy values. Analyses
and calibration between and within sites. were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sci-
ences (SPSS version 20; IBM, Chicago, IL) and Statistical Anal-
ysis System (SAS version 9.2; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).
Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) Acquisition
and Processing
Results
DTI data were derived from skull stripping, motion, and eddy-
current correction with FSL package and geometric distortion In our first model, cross-sectional analysis of triglycerides and
correction (Tao, Fletcher, Gerber, & Whitaker, 2009). FA images executive function showed an inverse correlation between triglyc-
for each subject were generated and all FA images were resliced to erides and executive scores (p ⬍ .01), but no significant relation-
the SPM8 white-matter template using a rigid-body transformation ship with memory (p ⫽ .29). In the second model (controlling for
(dimensions: 121 ⬎ 145 ⬎ 121 voxels; resolution: 1.5 mm3). age, education, and sex), higher triglycerides remained correlated
A probabilistic FA atlas including anatomical labels of 50-deep with worse executive functions (p ⫽ .004). In addition to demo-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

white-matter (WM) regions of interest (ROIs) from the DTI of 200 graphic factors, we also tested a third model in which we con-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

subjects was taken from the Johns Hopkins University (JHU) trolled for vascular and Alzheimer’s disease risk factors that could
ICBM-DTI-81 atlas package (Oishi et al., 2008) and subsequently also be contributing to the variance of our model. Even after
imported into SPM8 WM template. The resliced FA images were adding LDL, cholesterol, FCRS score, and ApoE4 status, in addi-
normalized to an intensity-averaged FA template using a diffeo- tion to age, education, sex, and CDR score, higher triglycerides
morphic registration algorithm (DARTEL) (Ashburner, 2007). The remained significantly correlated with poorer executive function
template was generated from 182 research subjects from the Aging (p ⫽ .03).
Brain study (mean age ⫽ 78 ⫾ 6 years) who had no infarcts and In our fourth model, we controlled for DTI fractional anisotropy
whose white-matter hyperintensity (WMH) burden volume was values using a smaller subset of patients to determine whether the
less than 0.5% of intracranial volume, indicating lack of significant relationship between triglycerides and executive function was due
WMH pathology (DeCarli et al., 1995). Individual FA images and in part to microstructural white-matter abnormalities. We chose to
the JHU ICBM-DTI-81 FA atlas were warped to the common use DTI because it is a sensitive measure of white-matter health
space of the normalized FA template generated from 23 VIB and can provide inferences about cerebrovascular mechanisms as
participants whose DTI data, other than being used in creation of well as the microstructure within the neural network (Charlton,
the intensity-averaged FA template, were not part of the current Schiavone, Barrick, Morris, & Markus, 2010). The effect of trig-
analyses. DTI values were taken from voxels where FA was lycerides on EXEC remained significant, suggesting that triglyc-
greater than 0.20, in efforts to avoid including surrounding gray erides do not affect executive function through white-matter alter-
matter or cerebral spinal fluid. We selected global brain FA as our ations (p ⫽ .02; see Table 2 for the fourth model).
primary predictor, which was generated by calculating the geo- In our final model, we controlled for specific cardiovascular risk
metric mean of the following: projection, association, limbic, and factors including the individual components of the FCRS score to
commissural fiber tracts; corona radiata, internal and external screen for potential confounders that could potentially explain the
capsule, corpus callosum, fornix, cingulum bundle, parahippocam- relationship between triglycerides and executive function (see Ta-
pal white matter, posterior thalamic radiation, inferior and superior ble 3). Variables in this model included age, education, gender,
frontal-occipital fasciculus, superior longitudinal fasciculus, sagit- CDR score, systolic and diastolic blood pressures, history of
tal striatum, and uncinate fasciculus.

Table 2
Statistical Analysis Cross-Sectional Analysis of the Effect of Triglycerides on
A linear regression model was used to study the cross-sectional Dependent Variable Executive Function While Controlling for
correlation between serum triglycerides and cognitive function. Demographic Factors and Vascular Risk Factors, and
Triglyceride values were logarithmically transformed to achieve Microstructural White-Matter Abnormalities
normality and constant variance for our regression model. Depen-
Variable ␤ SE t p
dent variables included composite scores for executive function
(EXEC) and memory (MEM). Age, sex, and years of education Age ⫺0.46 0.20 ⫺2.27 .03
were used to control for demographic factors in our sample. Education 1.40 0.43 3.34 .01
Clinical dementia rating (CDR) scores were also added to control Gender 4.77 2.87 1.66 .10
LDL 0.10 0.09 1.09 .28
for severity of functional impairment. To address our question of CHOL ⫺0.11 0.08 ⫺1.46 .14
whether other vascular risk factors could be responsible for the MAP ⫺0.61 0.12 ⫺0.52 .61
correlation between triglycerides and executive function, we added FCRS ⫺0.01 0.20 ⫺0.03 .98
LDL, total serum cholesterol, mean arterial pressure, and the ApoE4 status ⫺1.43 2.89 ⫺0.49 .62
CDR ⫺20.60 4.91 ⫺4.19 .00
participant’s Framingham cardiovascular risk score (FCRS; which Global FA ⫺17.70 50.90 ⫺0.35 .73
includes diabetes) to our demographic and CDR covariates. Triglycerides ⫺13.20 6.03 ⫺2.19 .03
APOE4 status, a measure of Alzheimer’s disease risk, was also
Note. LDL ⫽ low-density lipoprotein; CHOL ⫽ cholesterol; MAP ⫽
included in the model. To determine if the relationship between mean arterial pressure; FCRS ⫽ Framingham cardiovascular score;
triglycerides and cognition could be mediated by white-matter CDR ⫽ clinical dementia rating; Global FA ⫽ global functional anisot-
microstructure, we repeated the linear regression model, adjusting ropy.
686 PARTHASARATHY ET AL.

Table 3 pathway. Serum triglyceride levels have been shown to regulate


Cross-Sectional Analysis of the Effect of Triglycerides on blood– brain barrier transport of insulin and other gastrointestinal
Dependent Variable Executive Function While Controlling for hormones, which may have downstream effects on cognition
Specific Cardiovascular Risk Factors (Banks, 2012; Urayama & Banks, 2008). Recent studies have
suggested systemic inflammation, known to be elevated in obese
Variable ␤ SE t p individuals (Gregor & Hotamisligil, 2011), as a potential mediator
Age ⫺0.31 0.17 ⫺1.83 .07 of obesity-associated cognitive decline (Trollor et al., 2012). We
Education 1.28 0.357 3.58 .00 acknowledge, however, that our fractional anisotropy data in-
CDR ⫺10.17 4.47 ⫺2.27 .02 volved a smaller subset of patients, and our analysis may have
Stroke 8.20 2.53 3.25 .00 succumbed to low statistical power. In addition, we used whole-
MI 0.96 3.41 0.28 .78
CABG 0.98 2.97 0.33 .74 brain FA as our predictor; but it is plausible that looking at more
Gender 2.65 2.11 1.25 .211 specific regions of interest like subcortical-frontal or frontoparietal
Diabetes 1.16 2.31 0.50 .62 tracts may have yielded different results. Further studies will be
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

SBP 0.06 0.06 0.91 .37 required to confirm or refute this relationship.
DBP ⫺0.37 0.13 ⫺3.00 .00
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

For this project, we were interested in studying heterogeneous


Anti-HTN 4.4 2.6 1.73 .09
Smoking ⫺1.14 4.22 ⫺0.27 .79 nondemented adults with a range of vascular risk factors and
Triglycerides ⫺10.47 4.82 ⫺2.17 .03 cognitive capacities. Our cohort, therefore, included individuals
who, while not considered demented, nonetheless report a change
Note. CDR ⫽ clinical dementia rating; MI ⫽ myocardial infarction;
CABG ⫽ coronary artery bypass graft; SBP ⫽ systolic blood pressure; in cognition from baseline. Given this, our results may reflect a
DBP ⫽ diastolic blood pressure Anti-HTN ⫽ antihypertensives. snapshot of preclinical dementia, highlighted primarily by a
triglyceride-associated decline in executive function. This theory is
consistent with recently published cross-sectional data by Har-
stroke, diabetes, myocardial infarction, coronary artery bypass rington et al. (2013) that showed that executive function changes
graft, and the use of antihypertensive medication. Within this preceded memory in patients with preclinical Alzheimer’s pathol-
model, the effect of triglycerides on executive function remained ogy. Additionally, our data also adds validity to previous findings
significant, thereby suggesting the presence of an alternate path- of a relationship between triglycerides and decreased global cog-
way, independent of the above cardiovascular mediators, by which nitive ability and is consistent with Rogers et al.’s (1989) finding,
triglycerides may exert its influence. which showed that treating patients with lipid-lowering drug Gem-
fibrozil improved cognitive performance as measure by Cognitive
Capacity Screening Exam (Farr et al., 2008). The authors report
Discussion
that lowering lipid levels improved cerebral blood flow, suggesting
The two major findings of this study are (1) higher levels of a relationship among cerebral perfusion, triglycerides, and cogni-
serum triglycerides are significantly related to worse executive tive function. While we controlled for vascular risk factors and
functions, but not memory in nondemented community dwelling fiber tract integrity in our models, we did not control for cerebral
older adults; and (2) this relationship is independent of other perfusion in our study.
vascular and Alzheimer’s disease risk factors, as well as cerebral The results of this study specifically stress the importance of
white-matter microstructure. triglycerides as a cognitive risk factor in aging adults. Monitoring
Executive functions enable us to organize simple ideas and and maintaining triglyceride values within normal ranges may
thought processes into complex goal directed activities, and facil- prove instrumental in preventing cognitive decline and maintain-
itate execution of activities of daily living. The observed associa- ing healthy cognitive function in our elderly population. We ac-
tion between triglycerides and executive function supports recent knowledge that our study is cross-sectional, giving us only a single
studies showing an inverse correlation between other cardiovas- snapshot in time. Therefore, a longitudinal study of the relation-
cular risk factors and executive function in younger adult popula- ship between triglycerides and cognitive function in aging adults
tions (Yaffe et al., 2014; Nishtala et al., 2014), and Perlmuter et al. will be the next important step in capturing the interplay between
(1988) have previously reported a link between triglyceride levels lipid status and the cognitive aging process.
in older diabetics.
Our second key finding was that the relationship between trig-
References
lycerides and executive functioning was not mitigated by other
vascular risk factors or even a marker of white-matter microstruc- Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm.
ture. This finding raises questions about the widely held assump- NeuroImage, 38, 95–113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007
tion that the impact of vascular risks on cognition is mediated by .07.007
cerebrovascular changes. This lack of association is consistent Banks, W. A. (2012). Role of the blood– brain barrier in the evolution of
with recent studies that have shown that higher body mass index feeding and cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
1264, 13–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06568.x
correlates with deficits in cognition independent of its relationship
Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. D. (1989). Controlled oral word association
to cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease (Cournot et al., test, multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.
2006; Elias, Elias, Sullivan, Wolf, & D’Agostino, 2003; Elias, Bettcher, B. M., Walsh, C. M., Watson, C., Miller, J. W., Green, R., Patel,
Elias, Sullivan, Wolf, & D’Agostino, 2005; Sabia, Kivimaki, Shi- N., . . . Kramer, J. H. (2013). Body mass and white matter integrity: The
pley, Marmot, & Singh-Manoux, 2009) Perhaps the association we influence of vascular and inflammatory markers. PLoS ONE, 8(10),
see between triglycerides and cognition is mediated by an alternate e77741. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077741
TRIGLYCERIDES AND COGNITIVE FUNCTION IN AGING 687

Charlton, R. A., Schiavone, F., Barrick, T. R., Morris, R. G., & Markus, tion, insulin resistance, dyslipidemia and nonalcoholic fatty liver dis-
H. S. (2010). Diffusion tensor imaging detects age related white matter ease. International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 15, 6184 – 6223.
change over a 2-year follow-up which is associated with working mem- http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijms15046184
ory decline. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 81, Lepara, O., Valjevac, A., Alajbegović, A., Zaćiragić, A., & Nakas-Ićindić,
13–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2008.167288 E. (2009). Decreased serum lipids in patients with probable Alzheimer’s
Cournot, M., Marquie, J., Ansiau, D., Martinaud, C., Fonds, H., Ferrieres, disease. Bosnian Journal of Basic Medical Sciences/Udruzenje Basicnih
J., & Ruidavets, J. (2006). Relation between body mass index and Mediciniskih Znanosti ⫽ Association of Basic Medical Sciences, 9,
cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology, 215–220.
67, 1208 –1214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000238082.13860.50 Luchsinger, J. A., & Mayeux, R. (2004). Cardiovascular risk factors and
DeCarli, C., Murphy, D. G., Tranh, M., Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Gillette, Alzheimer’s disease. Current Atherosclerosis Reports, 6, 261–266.
J. A., . . . Schapiro, M. B. (1995). The effect of white matter hyperin- http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11883-004-0056-z
tensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance, and cerebral Mapstone, M., Cheema, A. K., Fiandaca, M. S., Zhong, X., Mhyre, T. R.,
metabolism of glucose in 51 healthy adults. Neurology, 45, 2077–2084. MacArthur, L. H., . . . Federoff, H. J. (2014). Plasma phospholipids
http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.45.11.2077 identify antecedent memory impairment in older adults. Nature Medi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Dimopoulos, N., Piperi, C., Salonicioti, A., Psarra, V., Mitsonis, C., cine, 20, 415– 418. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nm.3466
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Liappas, I., . . . Kalofoutis, A. (2007). Characterization of the lipid Marioni, R. E., Stewart, M. C., Murray, G. D., Deary, I. J., Fowkes,
profile in dementia and depression in the elderly. Journal of Geriatric F. G. R., Lowe, G. D. O., . . . Price, J. F. (2009). Peripheral levels of
Psychiatry and Neurology, 20, 138 –144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ fibrinogen, C-reactive protein, and plasma viscosity predict future cog-
0891988707301867 nitive decline in individuals without dementia. Psychosomatic Medicine,
Dong, C. H. B., Zhang, H. W. Y., Lu, J. W. Z., & Flaherty, J. H. (2009). 71, 901–906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181b1e538
Association of Cognitive Impairment with Serum Lipid/Lipoprotein Marsland, A. L., Petersen, K. L., Sathanoori, R., Muldoon, M. F., Neu-
among Chinese. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 27, 111– mann, S. A., Ryan, C., . . . Manuck, S. B. (2006). Interleukin-6 covaries
116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000194660 inversely with cognitive performance among middle-aged community
Elias, M. F., Elias, P. K., Sullivan, L. M., Wolf, P. A., & D’Agostino, R. B. volunteers. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 895–903. http://dx.doi.org/10
(2003). Lower cognitive function in the presence of obesity and hyper- .1097/01.psy.0000238451.22174.92
tension: The Framingham heart study. International Journal of Obesity, Mungas, D. (2005). Contributions of subcortical lacunar infarcts to cogni-
27, 260 –268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.802225 tive impairment in older persons. In R. H. Paul, R. Cohen, B. R. Ott, &
Elias, M. F., Elias, P. K., Sullivan, L. M., Wolf, P. A., & D’Agostino, R. B. S. Salloway (Eds.), Vascular dementia (pp. 211–222). Totowa, NJ:
(2005). Obesity, diabetes and cognitive deficit: The Framingham Heart Humana Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1385/1-59259-824-2:211
Study. Neurobiology of Aging, 26(Suppl. 1), 11–16. http://dx.doi.org/10 Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., & Kramer, J. H. (2003). Psychometrically
.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.08.019 matched measures of global cognition, memory, and executive function
Farr, S. A., Yamada, K. A., Butterfield, D. A., Abdul, H. M., Xu, L., for assessment of cognitive decline in older persons. Neuropsychology,
Miller, N. E., . . . Morley, J. E. (2008). Obesity and hypertriglyceridemia 17, 380 –392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.380
produce cognitive impairment. Endocrinology, 149, 2628 –2636. http:// Nishtala, A., Preis, S. R., Beiser, A., Devine, S., Hankee, L., Seshadri, S.,
dx.doi.org/10.1210/en.2007-1722 . . . Au, R. (2014). Midlife cardiovascular risk impacts executive func-
Ford, N. A., DiGiovanni, J., & Hursting, S. D. (2013). Metabolic pertur- tion: Framingham offspring study. Alzheimer Disease and Associated
bations associated with adipose tissue dysfunction and the obesity– Disorders, 28, 16 –22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0b013e31
cancer link. In Adipose tissue and cancer (pp. 1–14). New York, NY: 82a715bc
Springer. Oishi, K., Zilles, K., Amunts, K., Faria, A., Jiang, H., Li, X., . . . Mori, S.
Frazier, D. T., Bettcher, B. M., Dutt, S., Patel, N., Mungas, D., Miller, J., (2008). Human brain white matter atlas: Identification and assignment of
. . . Kramer, J. H. (2015). Relationship between insulin-resistance common anatomical structures in superficial white matter. NeuroImage,
processing speed and specific executive function profiles in neurologi- 43, 447– 457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.009
cally intact older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychologi- Perlmuter, L. C., Nathan, D. M., Goldfinger, S. H., Russo, P. A., Yates, J.,
cal Society, 21, 622– 628. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S135561771 & Larkin, M. (1988). Triglyceride levels affect cognitive function in
5000624 noninsulin-dependent diabetics. Journal of Diabetic Complications, 2,
Gregor, M. F., & Hotamisligil, G. S. (2011). Inflammatory mechanisms in 210 –213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0891-6632(88)80011-4
obesity. Annual Review of Immunology, 29, 415– 445. http://dx.doi.org/ Rogers, R. L., Meyer, J. S., McClintic, K. M. K., & Mortel, K. F. (1989).
10.1146/annurev-immunol-031210-101322 Reducing hypertriglyceridemia in elderly patients with cerebrovascular
Gunstad, J., Paul, R. H., Cohen, R. A., Tate, D. F., & Gordon, E. (2006). disease stabilizes or improves cognition and cerebral perfusion. Angiol-
Obesity is associated with memory deficits in young and middle-aged ogy, 40, 260 –269.
adults. Eating and Weight Disorders, 11(1), e15– e19. http://dx.doi.org/ Sabia, S., Kivimaki, M., Shipley, M. J., Marmot, M. G., & Singh-Manoux,
10.1007/BF03327747 A. (2009). Body mass index over the adult life course and cognition in
Harrington, M. G., Chiang, J., Pogoda, J. M., Gomez, M., Thomas, K., late midlife: The Whitehall II Cohort Study. American Journal of
Marion, S. D., . . . Fonteh, A. N. (2013). Executive function changes Clinical Nutrition, 89, 601– 607. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2008
before memory in preclinical Alzheimer’s pathology: A prospective, .26482
cross-sectional, case control study. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e79378. http://dx Schram, M. T., Euser, S. M., de Craen, A. J. M., Witteman, J. C., Frölich,
.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079378 M., Hofman, A., . . . Westendorp, R. G. J. (2007). Systemic markers of
Hokanson, J. E., & Austin, M. A. (1996). Plasma triglyceride level is a risk inflammation and cognitive decline in old age. Journal of the American
factor for cardiovascular disease independent of high-density lipoprotein Geriatrics Society, 55, 708 –716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415
cholesterol level: A meta-analysis of population-based prospective stud- .2007.01159.x
ies. Journal of Cardiovascular Risk, 3, 213–219. http://dx.doi.org/10 Tao, R., Fletcher, P. T., Gerber, S., & Whitaker, R. T. (2009). A variational
.1097/00043798-199604000-00014 image-based approach to the correction of susceptibility artifacts in the
Jung, U. J., & Choi, M. S. (2014). Obesity and its metabolic complications: alignment of diffusion weighted and structural MRI. In J. L. Prince,
The role of adipokines and the relationship between obesity, inflamma- D. L. Pham, & K. J. Myers (Eds.), Information processing in medical
688 PARTHASARATHY ET AL.

imaging. IPMI 2009. Lecture notes in computer science (vol. 5636, pp. Whitmer, R. A., Sidney, S., Selby, J., Johnston, S. C., & Yaffe, K. (2005).
664 – 675). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3- Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late life.
642-02498-6_55 Neurology, 64, 277–281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL
Trollor, J. N., Smith, E., Agars, E., Kuan, S. A., Baune, B. T., Campbell, .0000149519.47454.F2
L., . . . Sachdev, P. (2012). The association between systemic inflam- Williams, J. (1991). Memory assessment scales. Odessa, FL: Psychological
mation and cognitive performance in the elderly: The Sydney Memory Assessment.
and Ageing Study. Age, 34, 1295–1308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ Yaffe, K., Vittinghoff, E., Pletcher, M. J., Hoang, T., Launer, L., Whitmer,
s11357-011-9301-x R., . . . Sidney, S. (2014). Early adult to mid-life cardiovascular risk
Urayama, A., & Banks, W. A. (2008). Starvation and triglycerides reverse factors and cognitive function. Circulation, 129, 1560 –1567. http://dx
the obesity-induced impairment of insulin transport at the blood– brain
.doi.org/10.1161/circulationaha.113.004798
barrier. Endocrinology, 149, 3592–3597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/en
Yamada, M., Mitsuno, S., Kato, S., & Hirano, H. (1997). Dementia rating
.2008-0008
scale. Nippon Rinsho. Japanese Journal of Clinical Medicine, 55(Suppl.
Van Exel, E., de Craen, A. J., Gussekloo, J., Houx, P., Bootsma-van der
1), 310 –314.
Wiel, A., Macfarlane, P. W., . . . Westendorp, R. G. (2002). Association
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

between high-density lipoprotein and cognitive impairment in the oldest


old. Annals of Neurology, 51, 716 –721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ana
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.10220
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. New York, NY:
Psychological Corporation.
Welty, F. K. (2013). How do elevated triglycerides and low HDL- Received November 17, 2015
cholesterol affect inflammation and atherothrombosis? Current Cardi- Revision received September 26, 2016
ology Reports, 15, 400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11886-013-0400-4 Accepted October 18, 2016 䡲

Members of Underrepresented Groups:


Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and
Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the
publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C
Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate
more in this process.

If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write APA Journals at Reviewers@apa.org.
Please note the following important points:

• To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The
experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective
review.

• To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most
central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently
published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission
within the context of existing research.

• To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information.
Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you
are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example,
“social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude
change” as well.

• Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to
review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript
thoroughly.

APA now has an online video course that provides guidance in reviewing manuscripts. To learn
more about the course and to access the video, visit http://www.apa.org/pubs/authors/review-
manuscript-ce-video.aspx.

You might also like