You are on page 1of 9

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

published: 10 July 2018


doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00275

Females Are More Sensitive to


Opponent’s Emotional Feedback:
Evidence From Event-Related
Potentials
Xuhai Chen*, Hang Yuan , Tingting Zheng , Yingchao Chang and Yangmei Luo

Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Psychology in Shaanxi Province, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University,
Xi’an, China

It is widely believed that females outperformed males in emotional information


processing. The present study tested whether the female superiority in emotional
information processing exists in a naturalistic social-emotional context, if so, what
the temporal dynamics underlies. The behavioral and electrophysiological responses
were recorded while participants were performing an interpersonal gambling game
with opponents’ facial emotions given as feedback. The results yielded that emotional
cues modulated the influence of monetary feedback on outcome valuation. Critically,
this modulation was more conspicuous in females: opponents’ angry expressions
increased females’ risky tendency and decreased the amplitude of reward positivity
(RewP) and feedback P300. These findings indicate that females are more sensitive
to emotional expressions in real interpersonal interactions, which is manifested in both
early motivational salience detection and late conscious cognitive appraisal stages of
feedback processing.
Keywords: gender difference, interpersonal emotion, decision making, RewP, feedback P300
Edited by:
Xiaolin Zhou,
Peking University, China INTRODUCTION
Reviewed by:
Gennady Knyazev, Females are believed to have superiority in emotional competence such as understanding other
State Scientific-Research Institute of people’s emotions embedded in facial cues (Hall, 1978; Hall and Matsumoto, 2004; Kret and De
Physiology & Basic Medicine, Russia Gelder, 2012; Sawada et al., 2014; Weisenbach et al., 2014), even among adolescents and infants
Katharina Paul, (McClure, 2000; Lee et al., 2013). However, it remains unclear whether this advantage extends to
Ghent University, Belgium
real interpersonal interactions, as the participants in previous studies were required to recognize
*Correspondence: emotions from static images without a naturalistic social-emotional context (Hall, 1978; Filkowski
Xuhai Chen
et al., 2017). This is of great importance given that decoding of emotional information always takes
shiningocean@snnu.edu.cn
place in a specific context (Fukushima and Hiraki, 2006; Jack and Schyns, 2015; Wiggert et al.,
Received: 17 February 2018
2015; Pádua Júnior et al., 2016). Therefore, the present study approached this issue by recording
Accepted: 13 June 2018 behavioral and electrophysiological responses while participants performing an interpersonal
Published: 10 July 2018 gambling game with opponents’ facial emotions given as feedback (Chen et al., 2017).
Citation: It is widely reported that females are more sensitive to facial emotions in comparison with males
Chen X, Yuan H, Zheng T, Chang Y (McClure, 2000; Donges et al., 2012; Erol et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013; Weisenbach et al., 2014). For
and Luo Y (2018) Females Are More instance, females were more accurate in the categorization of fearful expressions relative to males
Sensitive to Opponent’s Emotional in facial emotion perception test (Weisenbach et al., 2014) and females’ judgments of distance
Feedback: Evidence From
Event-Related Potentials.
were more likely to be influenced by facial emotions (Kim and Son, 2015). And such behavioral
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:275. advantage was also observed in adolescence, with girls more sensitive to facial emotions than boys
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00275 (Lee et al., 2013). This female superiority in emotion decoding was also observed at subliminal level.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 1 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

For example, in subliminal affective priming experiment, Donges The neurophysiological studies of feedback processing
et al. (2012) reported that females manifested greater affective focused on two event related potential (ERP) components. One
priming due to happy faces than males did. Likewise, Hoffmann is the frontocentral peaking component roughly 250–300 ms
et al. (2010) found that females were more accurate than males after feedback, which is thought to reflect early evaluation
in recognizing subtle facial displays of emotion. Moreover, such of performance feedback and action monitoring (Zhou et al.,
female advantage in facial emotion recognition extends to other 2010; Ullsperger et al., 2014; Proudfit, 2015; Sambrook and
materials like voice (Demenescu et al., 2014; Lambrecht et al., Goslin, 2015). It was originally linked to negative feedback
2014), point light displays (Alaerts et al., 2011), music (Hunter and referred as feedback related negativity (FRN; Gehring
et al., 2011) and multisensory emotion expressions (Collignon and Willoughby, 2002; Yeung et al., 2004), however, more
et al., 2010). recent research has indicated that the FRN effect may rather
Corresponding to the behavioral performance, the female be driven by a reward positivity (RewP), which attenuates
superiority in emotion decoding is associated with different a default frontocentral N2 component and which is present
neural pathways and varied neurodynamics. A recent for positive but not for negative outcomes (Proudfit, 2015;
meta-analysis study showed that the medial prefrontal cortex, Heydari and Holroyd, 2016). The other is feedback-related
anterior cingulate cortex, frontal pole and the thalamus were P300, a positive deflection with parietal distribution occurring
more recruited in men relative to women during emotion between 300 ms and 600 ms after feedback. This positive
perception, while women showed distinct activation in bilateral component, linking with a more elaborated and conscious
amygdala, hippocampus and some regions of the dorsal midbrain appraisal of the motivational significance of performance
(Filkowski et al., 2017), suggesting that males tend to recruit feedback, was reported to be larger for positive feedback in
bilateral prefrontal regions involved in rational thinking and comparison with negative feedback (Yeung et al., 2004; Leng
cognitive control whereas females tend to recruit bilateral and Zhou, 2010; Li et al., 2010; Ulrich and Hewig, 2014;
amygdala involved in quick emotional evaluation (AlRyalat, Mason et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2016). Moreover, these two
2017). Regarding neurodynamics, it was reported that females components are sensitive to both monetary and emotional
yielded significantly larger P100 to fearful faces than males in feedback, as the previous study reported that emotional
emotion discrimination task (Lee et al., 2017), and generated and monetary reward elicited morphologically similar RewP
longer latency and higher amplitude P450 component than (Ethridge et al., 2017) and feedback-related P300 (Oumeziane
males when explicitly detecting happy and sad faces among et al., 2017).
neutral faces (Orozco and Ehlers, 1998), suggesting that female Given that emotion cues can bias decision-making (van Kleef
advantage in emotion processing emerges in the early stage et al., 2004; Averbeck and Duchaine, 2009; Parkinson et al.,
of low-level visual feature processing and the late stage of 2012; Chen et al., 2017), we predicted that emotional cues would
indepth emotionality evaluation. Likewise, females (but not interact with monetary cues in feedback processing. Specifically,
males) yielded conspicuous N200 and P300 responses to opponents’ angry expressions should increase risky tendency and
moderately negative pictures (Li et al., 2008; Yuan et al., 2009) decrease RewP and feedback-related P300 associated with wins
and demonstrated enhanced N200 when viewed unpleasant and losses, while happy expressions demonstrate the opposite
stimuli (Lithari et al., 2010), implying that gender difference in effect if interpersonal emotion exerts its influence through
emotion decoding prevails in the initial perceptual coding and affective reaction (van Kleef, 2009). Moreover, considering that
the deliberative categorization of the emotional expressions. females outperform males in emotion decoding (Hall, 1978;
Furthermore, Güntekin and Başar (2007) found that females Hall and Matsumoto, 2004; Kret and De Gelder, 2012; Sawada
generated significantly larger occipital beta responses (15–24 Hz) et al., 2014; Weisenbach et al., 2014) and females are believed
than males during the presentation of face expressions and to be more interpersonally sensitive than men (Briton and Hall,
argued that beta synchronization might mediate the female 1995; Spence et al., 1975), we hypothesized the modulation of
advantage in emotion processing. interpersonal emotions was more conspicuous in females relative
The studies reviewed above revealed important insight into to males.
the female superiority in emotion processing. However, to the
best of our knowledge, the influence of contextual factors on MATERIALS AND METHODS
emotion processing has largely been neglected, despite these
factors exert great impact on how observers ultimately discern Participants
facial expressions (Barrett et al., 2011; Kring and Campellone, Fifty right-handed university students (25 females) were
2012). Therefore, we hope to shed light on this issue by using recruited to participate in this experiment. All participants
the interpersonal version of Gehring and Willoughby’s gambling reported normal auditory and normal or corrected-to-normal
task (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002; Chen et al., 2017), in which visual acuity and were free of neurological or psychiatric
participants chose between two monetary options and received problems. Four participants (two females) were excluded from
feedback orthogonally combined monetary cues and emotional analysis due to excessive EEG artifacts in the recordings. The
cues (Vrtička et al., 2014). Using this interpersonal paradigm, we remaining participants showed no significant difference between
hope to probe into the female advantage in emotion processing genders on age, personality and emotional intelligence (EI; see
in naturalistic context concurrent with emotional and monetary Table 1 for illustration). This study was carried out in accordance
feedback. with the recommendations of the Declaration of Helsinki. The

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 2 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

TABLE 1 | Age, personality and emotional intelligence∗ of the participants as a function of gender.

Male (n = 18) Female (n = 18) t P

Age 19.52 ± 1.13 19.08 ± 1.02 −1.42 0.16


Neuroticism 2.95 ± 0.49 2.68 ± 0.48 1.87 0.07
Extraversion 3.27 ± 0.51 3.32 ± 0.40 −0.34 0.73
Openness 3.18 ± 0.58 3.26 ± 0.68 −0.43 0.67
Agreeableness 3.19 ± 1.21 2.93 ± 0.61 0.52 0.61
Conscientiousness 3.34 ± 0.46 3.37 ± 0.41 −0.25 0.80
EI 3.71 ± 0.26 3.83 ± 0.34 −1.39 0.17

Personality were assessed with shortened Chinese version of the Costa and McCrae NEO-FFI (Yang et al., 1999), while emotional intelligence (EI) were assessed with
ESI (Schutte et al., 1998).

protocol was approved by the the Ethical Committee of Shaanxi


Normal University. All subjects gave written informed consent in
accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Procedure
Upon entry into the lab, the participant was introduced to
a confederate of the same gender who would perform as
opponent in a gambling game through a computer network.
They were told they would played as competitors, that is to
say, a loss for the participant means a win for his/her opponent
in the same amount, and vice versa. And then their facial
expressions (happy, angry and neutral) were recorded using a
Canon EOS 600D and used as feedback stimuli. Unbeknownst FIGURE 1 | Schematic diagram of an experimental trial in the interpersonal
to the participant, the facial expression of the confederate was gambling task. After a fixation, the computer selected performer (red square)
prerecorded and validated in advance. Immediately after giving and observer (green square) for each round of gambling randomly. The person
informed consent, participants were endowed with ¥40. They selected as the performer would view numeral 10 or 50 (cents) and make a
were told the money was theirs to risk during the study and choice by pressing the corresponding button as soon as possible. After the
choice presented for 300–1500 ms randomly, the observer saw the monetary
asked to place it in their wallets. Participants were told that outcome and chose one of his/her facial expressions to indicate his/her
additional rewards or punishments were given based on their attitude: happiness means he/she is happy with the outcome, anger means
performances. The actual earnings for the each participant he/she is angry with the outcome, neutral means no specific emotion. Then,
ranged from ¥30 to ¥50. the selected facial expression overlaid with the monetary cues (“+50” or
“−50”) on the forehead were presented as feedback for 1000 ms. While “+”
Following our previous study (Chen et al., 2017), the
means won, “−” means lose.
task in the present study was adapted from Gehring and
Willoughby gambling task (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002).
The key adaption was the interactivity (Chen et al., 2017) selected as performer two thirds of the trials and as observer the
and the feedback which orthogonally combined with monetary remaining third. Each participant received four types of feedback
and emotional cues (Vrtička et al., 2008, 2014; Chen et al., (happy-win, happy-lose, angry-win and angry-lose) equally with
2017). Figure 1 shows a schematic diagram of a trial in this 64 trials. To make the game more realistic, 32 neutral-win
task. Specifically, after a fixation period, the participants were and 32 neutral-lose trials were included as fillers, which were
told that the computer would select performer and observer not included in the data analysis. The whole experiment
for each round of gambling randomly. The person selected consisted of 448 trials, dividing into eight blocks with 56 trials
as the performer would view numeral 10 or 50 (cents) and each.
make a choice by pressing the corresponding button as soon
as possible. After the choice presented for 300–1500 ms
randomly, the observer saw the monetary outcome and chose EEG Recording
one of his/her facial expressions to indicate his/her attitude: EEG measurements were recorded at 64 scalp sites using tin
while happiness means he/she is happy with the outcome, electrodes mounted in an elastic cap (Brain Product, Munich,
anger means he/she is angry with the outcome, and neutral Germany) according to the modified expanded 10–20 system,
expressions means no specific emotions. Then, the selected each referenced online to FCZ. Vertical electrooculogram (EOG)
facial expression overlaid with the monetary cues (‘‘+50’’ or was recorded supra-orbitally and infra-orbitally from the right
‘‘−50’’) on the forehead were presented as feedback for 1000 ms. eye. The horizontal EOG was recorded as the left vs. right orbital
While ‘‘+’’ indicated that the performer won the points, rim. The EEG and EOG measurements were amplified using a
‘‘−’’ indicated the performer lose the points. Unbeknownst to 0.05–100 Hz bandpass and continuously digitized at 1000 Hz for
the participant, the monetary outcomes and affective responses offline analysis. The impedance of all electrodes was kept less
of the confederate were predetermined. Each participant was than 5 kΩ.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 3 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

FIGURE 2 | Behavioral performance. (A) Mean rates of risky choice, (B) the corresponding reaction times (RTs) and (C) rates of emotional feedback selection as a
function of conditions for females and males separately. Error bars indicate standard error.

Data Analysis (10 vs. 50) of the outcome was collapsed for conciseness. After
Preprocessing low-pass filtered at 30 Hz, extracted average waveforms for
The ‘‘10’’ is defined as the low-risk option (small potential win each participant and condition were used to calculate grand-
or loss) while the ‘‘50’’ is defined as the high-risk option (large average waveforms. For statistical analyses, following previous
potential win or loss). The risk-seeking preference was measured studies (Calvo and Beltrán, 2013; Chen et al., 2017), the
as the ‘‘risk ratio’’ by dividing the number of high-risk choices by mean amplitude between 220 ms and 280 ms over fronto-
the total number of choices. Following previous studies (Gehring central cluster (F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2)
and Willoughby, 2002; Chen et al., 2017), we analyzed the was calculated for RewP, whereas the mean activity between
preceding outcome on risky behavior in the current trial. Thus, 300 ms and 500 ms at the parietal cluster (P1, Pz, P2,
the risk ratio of the second trial during consecutive trials and PO3, POz, PO4) was calculated to assess feedback P300 (see
the corresponding reaction times (RTs; beyond three standard Figure 3).
deviations were excluded in RT calculation) were calculated as
the dependent variable (see Figure 2). Statistical Analysis
EEG data was preprocessed using EEGLAB (eeglab13_6_5b), We entered the behavioral data and ERP data into repeated
an open source toolbox running on the MATLAB platform measures ANOVAs, with the outcome valence (loss vs. win) and
(R2014a). First, the data were high pass filtered at 0.5 Hz, emotion (happy vs. angry) as within-subject factors and gender
and re-referenced offline to bilateral mastoid electrodes. The (male vs. female) as a between-subject factor. To examine how
data were segmented into epochs around the presentation of participants take the current interpersonal gambling task, the
outcome feedback stimuli (−200 to 800 ms post stimulus). rates of each emotional expressions they chosen as feedback were
The epoched data were baseline corrected using 200 ms before analyzed accordingly. The degrees of freedom of the F-ratio were
the onset of the feedback. EEG epochs with large artifacts corrected per the Greenhouse–Geisser method, and multiple
(exceeding ±100 µV) were removed, and channels with poor comparisons were Bonferroni adjusted if necessary. The effect
signal quality were interpolated spherically using EEGLAB sizes are shown as partial eta squared (ηp2 ).
toolbox (Perrin et al., 1989). Trials contaminated by eye
blinks and other artifacts were corrected using an independent RESULTS
component analysis algorithm (Delorme and Makeig, 2004).
There were on average 59.89 ± 4.17, 59.46 ± 3.71, 59.75 ± 4.48 Behavioral Performance
and 59.21 ± 4.90 artifact-free trials obtained for the lose- The participants selected more high-risk options after losses
angry, lose-happy, win-angry and win-happy conditions for (0.54 ± 0.02) than after wins (0.42 ± 0.03), (F (1,44) = 15.45,
females, while 58.83 ± 2.91, 58.61 ± 2.47, 57.56 ± 3.18 and p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.26). Moreover, there were marginal
58.44 ± 3.27 remained for males. Note that the magnitude significant two-way interaction between emotion and outcome

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 4 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

FIGURE 3 | Neurophysiological results. (A) Group-averaged event related potential (ERP) voltage waveforms over Cz, (B) scalp topography (top view shown) and (C)
bar plots of mean ERP values for reward positivity (RewP) and P300 during the selected time window as function of conditions. Error bars indicate standard error.

(F (1,44) = 3.29, p = 0.08, ηp2 = 0.06), and marginal significant (0.38 ± 0.04, p = 0.01) expressions if opponent won the game
two-way interaction between emotion and gender (F (1,44) = 3.88, (see Figure 2C).
p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.08). Simple effect analysis indicated that the
difference in risky selection was more conspicuous following Neurophysiological Performance
opponents’ angry expressions (win: 0.43 ± 0.04 vs. lose: The analysis of RewP showed a main effect of emotion
0.56 ± 0.03, p < 0.001) in comparison with happy expressions (F (1,44) = 9.32, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.18), a significant interaction
(win: 0.41 ± 0.04 vs. lose: 0.51 ± 0.03, p = 0.02; see of emotion × gender (F (1,44) = 4.42, p = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.09),
Figure 2A). Additionally, while females’ choices tended to be and a marginal significant interaction of emotion × outcome
affected by opponents’ expressions (happy: 0.45 ± 0.03 vs. angry: valence (F (1,44) = 3.32, p = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.07). Simple effect
0.48 ± 0.03, p = 0.05), males’ choices were immune to opponents’ analysis (see Figure 3) indicated females differentiated the angry
expressions (happy: 0.50 ± 0.03 vs. angry: 0.49 ± 0.03, (−0.32 ± 0.68 µV) from happy (0.69 ± 0.73 µV, p < 0.01)
p = 0.39). The analysis on RTs only showed a significant expressions, while males failed to do this (angry: 0.63 ± 0.68 µV
main effect of emotion (F (1,44) = 5.29, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.11), vs. happy: 0.81 ± 0.73 µV, p = 0.53). Moreover, the RewP
with the RTs longer following opponents happy expressions was more positive going following happy expressions (0.84 ±
(805 ± 22 ms) relative to angry expressions (788 ± 22 ms; see 0.57 µV) relative to angry expressions (−0.09 ± 0.46 µV, p =
Figure 2B). 0.002) if participants won the game, whereas the RewP was hardly
The analysis on rates of emotional expression only differentiated between emotions (happy: 0.66 ± 0.49 µV vs.
showed a two-way interaction between emotion and outcome angry: 0.40 ± 0.52 µV, p = 0.31) if participants lost the game.
(F (2,88) = 10.11, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.19). Simple effect analysis The analysis of P300 amplitudes showed a main effect of
indicated that the angry expressions (0.21 ± 0.03) were less outcome valence (F (1,44) = 25.22, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.37), and a
selected than both happy (0.43 ± 0.03, p = 0.01) and neutral main effect of emotion (F (1,44) = 7.77, p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.15).
(0.36 ± 0.04, p = 0.03) expressions if opponent lost the Also significant were the interaction of emotion × outcome
game, in contrast, happy expressions (0.21 ± 0.03) were less valence (F (1,44) = 12.98, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.23), and a significant
selected than both angry (0.42 ± 0.04, p = 0.01) and neutral interaction of outcome valence × gender (F (1,44) = 4.63, p = 0.03,

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 5 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

ηp2 = 0.10). Critically, the three-way interaction of emotion × as social feedback, could bring in similar effect as monetary
outcome valence × gender is significant (F (1,44) = 4.94, p = 0.03, feedback (Vrtička et al., 2014; Ethridge et al., 2017; Oumeziane
ηp2 = 0.10). Simple effects analysis yielded that, for the females, et al., 2017), we speculate that the current modification result
the wins (4.70 ± 0.46 µV) elicited larger P300 amplitudes than from the interaction of two types feedback cues. Moreover,
the losses (3.86 ± 0.43 µV, p = 0.001) when accompanied with combined with the influence of emotional cues on RTs, the
happy expressions, while the P300 differences between wins current finding supported the assumption that interpersonal
(4.32 ± 0.51 µV) and losses (4.43 ± 0.47 µV, p = 0.44) were emotions bias ones’ decision making (van Kleef et al., 2004;
diminished when accompanied by angry expressions. In contrast, Averbeck and Duchaine, 2009; Parkinson et al., 2012; Chen et al.,
for the males, the wins elicited larger P300 than losses when 2017).
accompanied by both angry (5.92 ± 0.51 µV vs. 5.17 ± 0.47 µV, Consistent with these behavioral findings, we observed
p < 0.001) and happy (5.58 ± 0.46 µV vs. 4.60 ± 0.42 µV, p < conspicuous interaction between monetary and emotional cues
0.001) expressions. on both the RewP and feedback P300. This finding replicated
the previous finding that opponent’s angry expressions reversed
DISCUSSION the differentiation pattern of RewP/FRN and diminished
feedback P300 difference associated with losses and wins (Chen
To examine the female superiority of emotion decoding et al., 2017). Following previous studies (Chen et al., 2017;
in a real social context, this study required participants Proudfit, 2015; Heydari and Holroyd, 2016), we speculated
to play an interpersonal gambling game with monetary that this phenomenon might result from that opponents’ angry
and emotional cues orthogonally combined as feedback. expressions were taken as negative feedback and thus reduced the
The results yielded that participants selected more happy positive-going deflection elicited by wins. Moreover, the current
expressions for opponents’ losses but more angry expressions for finding was in accordance with the previous studies showing
opponents’ wins. Moreover, participants selected more high-risk that interpersonal emotional expressions affect negotiation
options following losses relative to wins, and such effect was (van Kleef et al., 2004), dispute resolution (Friedman et al.,
more conspicuous when accompanied with opponents’ angry 2004), cooperation (Krumhuber et al., 2007), and prosocial
expressions. Additionally, while females’ risky tendency was behaviors (van Doorn et al., 2015). Taken together, these
affected by opponents’ emotional feedback, males showed no findings provided evidence for the assumption that emotional
such tendency. Corresponding to these behavioral results, RewP information biases decision-making (Averbeck and Duchaine,
and feedback P300 for females was influenced by opponents’ 2009; Evans et al., 2011; Parkinson et al., 2012; Aïte et al.,
emotional feedback, but not for males. The significance of these 2013). Complement to the previous studies, the current findings
findings will be addressed as the following. depicted the neurodynamics of the impact of interpersonal
According to Emotions as Social Information Model (van emotions. Given that RewP/FRN is associated with early
Kleef, 2009), emotional expressions affect observers’ behavior evaluation of performance feedback and action monitoring
by triggering inferential processes and/or affective reactions (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002; Holroyd et al., 2008; Ullsperger
in them, consequently, emotional expressions can be used as et al., 2014; Proudfit, 2015) while P300 reflected elaborated
strategy to influence observers’ behavior (Xiao and Houser, appraisal of the motivational significance of outcome (Yeung
2005). The participants in current study chose more happiness et al., 2004; Leng and Zhou, 2010; Li et al., 2010; Ulrich
for opponents’ losses but more anger for opponents’ wins, and Hewig, 2014; Mason et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2016),
indicating that they were aware of the setup of the experiment the current findings suggested that interpersonal emotions
and used emotional expressions as tactics to affect opponents. might affect outcome processing during both early stage of
In turn, we assumed that participants would take opponents’ motivational salience monitoring and late stage of cognitive
emotional feedback seriously given that they performed appraisal processing.
strategically when they had the right to give emotional feedback. More critical to the current study, we found that females were
And thus, this result can evidence the good validity of our more prone to be influenced by opponents’ emotional feedback.
interpersonal gambling game. That is, opponents’ angry expressions increased females’ risk
In line with previous studies (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002; tendency, decreased RewP and feedback P300 in comparison
Yeung et al., 2004; Chen et al., 2017), participants were more with happy expressions. Given that angry expressions have been
likely to gamble on risky outcomes if on the previous trial they used as a negative social feedback (Vrtička et al., 2014; Ethridge
had lost the points. This might due to that participants were more et al., 2017; Oumeziane et al., 2017) and elicited smaller RewP
willing to anticipate larger monetary rewards in order to reduce (Ethridge et al., 2017) and feedback P300 (Oumeziane et al.,
negative consequences. By contrast, they were more prone to 2017), the current findings suggested that females are highly
protect the money they had and thus showed more conservative susceptible to emotional feedback, and consquently modified the
behavior when faced with rewarding feedback. Complement amplitude of RewP and P300. Based on the modulation on RewP
to the previous studies, the current study showed that the and feedback P300, it seems the impact of angry expressions can
willingness to engage in risky choice following losses was affected even overshadow the influence of monetary cues during both
by opponents’ emotional feedback. Specifically, opponents’ angry early stage of motivational salience monitoring and late stage
expressions enlarged the risky tendency relative to happy of cognitive appraisal processing for females. In contrast, for
expressions. Given that angry and happy expressions, used males, emotional feedback only counteracted effect of monetary

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 6 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

cues during the early stage of salience monitoring, but not the from participants and confederates surely increase the ecological
late stage of in-depth valuation. This finding was in accordance validity, however, the external validity might be constrained.
with the neuroanatomical findings that while males tend to be Second, although we found conspicuous gender difference in
rational by recruiting bilateral prefrontal regions, females tend to interpersonal emotion decoding after controlling age, personality
be emotional by recruiting bilateral amygdala when facing with and EI, we did not take sex hormonal levels and menstrual cycle
emotional information (AlRyalat, 2017; Filkowski et al., 2017). into consideration. Given that sex hormones and cycle phases are
Actually, females have long been believed to outperform males implicated in sexual dimorphism in facial emotion recognition
at recognizing emotions expressions (McClure, 2000; Li et al., (Derntl et al., 2008; Guapo et al., 2009), future studies should
2008; Yuan et al., 2009; Donges et al., 2012; Erol et al., 2013; take these factors into consideration. Third, whether biological
Lee et al., 2013; Weisenbach et al., 2014; Mason et al., 2016), sex or psychological gender identity matter in gender difference
and more prone to be influenced by emotional information (Bourne and Maxwell, 2010) in interpersonal emotion processing
(Schirmer et al., 2002, 2004; Kim and Son, 2015). The current is also an interesting topic in future studies.
conspicuous female advantage of emotion decoding during both
early stage of motivational salience monitoring (RewP) and CONCLUSION
late stage of cognitive appraisal processing (feedback P300) was
in line with the findings that gender difference in emotions The present study examined the gender difference in
processing emerges at early stage of emotion extraction (Lee et al., interpersonal emotions processing. Participants were asked
2017) and late stage of emotion in-depth processing (Orozco to perform interpersonal gambling task with opponents’
and Ehlers, 1998). Taken together, the current study provided emotional expressions presented as feedbacks. It was found that
convergent evidence for the gender difference in interpersonal opponents’ angry expressions increased females’ risky tendency
emotion decoding, adding new knowledge to this area by taking and decreased the amplitude of RewP and feedback P300. These
the contextual factors into consideration (Barrett et al., 2011; findings indicate that females are more sensitive to emotional
Kring and Campellone, 2012). expressions in interpersonal interactions, which is manifested
Although the explanation of the influence of angry expression during early stage of motivational salience monitoring and late
on feedback P300 is quite reasonable, the reverse of RewP/FRN stage of conscious appraisal of outcomes.
for losses and wins is still elusive. However, this phenomenon
seems to be robust, as we observed this pattern again (Chen AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
et al., 2017). One quite possible reason is the congruency between
emotional and monetary cues: the incongruence might result XC, HY and TZ designed the study and discussed the results. HY,
in more negative valence. To support this speculation, a study TZ and YC organized the studies, analyzed the data and wrote a
employed similar design reported that right inferior frontal gyrus first draft of the article, which was revised by YL.
was more activated for incongruent feedback than for congruent
feedback (Vrtička et al., 2014). Another possible reason is that FUNDING
emotional feedback is so salient that overshadow the influence
of monetary feedback. The big facial expressions overlaid with This work was supported by Funded projects for the Academic
small monetary cues in the current study might also boost Leaders and Academic Backbones, Shaanxi Normal University
this tendency. However, all these speculations still need further 16QNGG006, Fundamental Research Funds for the Central
studies. Universities under grant GK201603124, and Major project
Despite the contributions of this study, some limitations of medicine science and technology of PLA (Grant no.
should be noted. First, the use of emotional expression taken AWS17J012).

REFERENCES gender identity matter? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 48, 1289–1294. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2009.12.032
Aïte, A., Borst, G., Moutier, S., Varescon, I., Brown, I., Houdé, O., et al. (2013). Briton, N. J., and Hall, J. (1995). Gender-based expectancies and observer
Impact of emotional context congruency on decision making under ambiguity. judgments of smiling. J. Nonverbal. Behav. 19, 49–65. doi: 10.1007/bf02173412
Emotion 13, 177–182. doi: 10.1037/a0031345 Calvo, M. G., and Beltrán, D. (2013). Recognition advantage of happy faces: tracing
Alaerts, K., Nackaerts, E., Meyns, P., Swinnen, S. P., and Wenderoth, N. (2011). the neurocognitive processes. Neuropsychologia 51, 2051–2061. doi: 10.1016/j.
Action and emotion recognition from point light displays: an investigation of neuropsychologia.2013.07.010
gender differences. PLoS One 6:e20989. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020989 Chen, X., Zheng, T., Han, L., Chang, Y., and Luo, Y. (2017). The neural dynamics
AlRyalat, S. A. (2017). Gender similarities and differences in brain activation underlying the interpersonal effects of emotional expression on decision
strategies: voxel-based meta-analysis on fMRI studies. J. Integr. Neurosci. 16, making. Sci. Rep. 7:46651. doi: 10.1038/srep46651
227–240. doi: 10.3233/JIN-170015 Collignon, O., Girard, S., Gosselin, F., Saint-Amour, D., Lepore, F., and
Averbeck, B. B., and Duchaine, B. (2009). Integration of social and monetary Lassonde, M. (2010). Women process multisensory emotion expressions
factors in decision making. Emotion 9, 599–608. doi: 10.1037/a0016509 more efficiently than men. Neuropsychologia 48, 220–225. doi: 10.1016/j.
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., and Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion neuropsychologia.2009.09.007
perception. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 286–290. doi: 10.1177/096372141 Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for
1422522 analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component
Bourne, V. J., and Maxwell, A. M. (2010). Examining the sex difference in analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.
lateralisation for processing facial emotion: does biological sex or psychological 10.009

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 7 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

Demenescu, L. R., Mathiak, K. A., and Mathiak, K. (2014). Age- and gender-related Kring, A. M., and Campellone, T. R. (2012). Emotion perception in schizophrenia:
variations of emotion recognition in pseudowords and faces. Exp. Aging Res. 40, context matters. Emot. Rev. 4, 182–186. doi: 10.1177/1754073911430140
187–207. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2014.882210 Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. L., and
Derntl, B., Kryspin-Exner, I., Fernbach, E., Moser, E., and Habel, U. (2008). Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and
Emotion recognition accuracy in healthy young females is associated with cycle cooperative behavior. Emotion 7, 730–735. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.730
phase. Horm. Behav. 53, 90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.006 Lambrecht, L., Kreifelts, B., and Wildgruber, D. (2014). Gender differences in
Donges, U. S., Kersting, A., and Suslow, T. (2012). Women’s greater ability to emotion recognition: impact of sensory modality and emotional category.
perceive happy facial emotion automatically: gender differences in affective Cogn. Emot. 28, 452–469. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.837378
priming. PLoS One 7:e41745. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041745 Lee, S. A., Kim, C. Y., Shim, M., and Lee, S. H. (2017). Gender differences in neural
Erol, A., Putgul, G., Kosger, F., and Ersoy, B. (2013). Facial emotion recognition responses to perceptually invisible fearful face-an ERP study. Front. Behav.
in schizophrenia: the impact of gender. Psychiatry Investig. 10, 69–74. Neurosci. 11:6. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00006
doi: 10.4306/pi.2013.10.1.69 Lee, N. C., Krabbendam, L., White, T. P., Meeter, M., Banaschewski, T.,
Ethridge, P., Kujawa, A., Dirks, M. A., Arfer, K. B., Kessel, E. M., Klein, D. N., et al. Barker, G. J., et al. (2013). Do you see what I see? Sex differences in the
(2017). Neural responses to social and monetary reward in early adolescence discrimination of facial emotions during adolescence. Emotion 13, 1030–1040.
and emerging adulthood. Psychophysiology 54, 1786–1799. doi: 10.1111/psyp. doi: 10.1037/a0033560
12957 Leng, Y., and Zhou, X. (2010). Modulation of the brain activity in outcome
Evans, S., Fleming, S. M., Dolan, R. J., and Averbeck, B. B. (2011). Effects evaluation by interpersonal relationship: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 48,
of emotional preferences on value-based decision-making are mediated by 448–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.002
mentalizing and not reward networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2197–2210. Li, P., Jia, S., Feng, T., Liu, Q., Suo, T., and Li, H. (2010). The influence of the
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21584 diffusion of responsibility effect on outcome evaluations: electrophysiological
Filkowski, M. M., Olsen, R. M., Duda, B., Wanger, T. J., and Sabatinelli, D. evidence from an ERP study. Neuroimage 52, 1727–1733. doi: 10.1016/j.
(2017). Sex differences in emotional perception: meta analysis of divergent neuroimage.2010.04.275
activation. Neuroimage 147, 925–933. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016. Li, H., Yuan, J., and Lin, C. (2008). The neural mechanism underlying the female
12.016 advantage in identifying negative emotions: an event-related potential study.
Friedman, R., Anderson, C., Brett, J., Olekalns, M., Goates, N., and Lisco, C. C. Neuroimage 40, 1921–1929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.033
(2004). The positive and negative effects of anger on dispute resolution: Lithari, C., Frantzidis, C. A., Papadelis, C., Vivas, A. B., Klados, M. A., Kourtidou-
evidence from electronically mediated disputes. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 369–376. Papadeli, C., et al. (2010). Are females more responsive to emotional stimuli? A
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.2.369 neurophysiological study across arousal and valence dimensions. Brain Topogr.
Fukushima, H., and Hiraki, K. (2006). Perceiving an opponent’s loss: gender- 23, 27–40. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0130-5
related differences in the medial-frontal negativity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. Mason, L., Trujillo-Barreto, N. J., Bentall, R. P., and El-Deredy, W. (2016).
1, 149–157. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl020 Attentional bias predicts increased reward salience and risk taking in bipolar
Gehring, W. J., and Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 311–319. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.014
the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 295, 2279–2282. McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial
doi: 10.1126/science.1066893 expression processing and their development in infants, children, and
Guapo, V. G., Graeff, F. G., Zani, A. C. T., Labate, C. M., dos Reis, R. M., adolescents. Psychol. Bull. 126, 424–453. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.424
and Del-Ben, C. M. (2009). Effects of sex hormonal levels and phases of the Orozco, S., and Ehlers, C. L. (1998). Gender differences in electrophysiological
menstrual cycle in the processing of emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology responses to facial stimuli. Biol. Psychiatry 44, 281–289. doi: 10.1016/s0006-
34, 1087–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.007 3223(97)00487-3
Güntekin, B., and Başar, E. (2007). Gender differences influence brain’s β Oumeziane, B. A., Schryer-Praga, J., and Foti, D. (2017). ‘‘Why don’t they
oscillatory responses in recognition of facial expressions. Neurosci. Lett. 424, ‘like’ me more?’’: comparing the time courses of social and monetary reward
94–99. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.07.052 processing. Neuropsychologia 107, 48–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychol. Bull. 85, 2017.11.001
845–857. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.85.4.845 Pádua Júnior, F. P., Prado, P. H. M., Roeder, S. S., and Andrade, E. B. (2016). What
Hall, J. A., and Matsumoto, D. (2004). Gender differences in judgments of multiple a smile means: contextual beliefs and facial emotion expressions in a non-verbal
emotions from facial expressions. Emotion 4, 201–206. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542. zero-sum game. Front. Psychol. 7:534. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00534
4.2.201 Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., and Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: adult social
Heydari, S., and Holroyd, C. B. (2016). Reward positivity: reward prediction referencing in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion
error or salience prediction error? Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 53, 1185–1192. 12, 817–826. doi: 10.1037/a0026434
doi: 10.1111/psyp.12673 Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O., and Echallier, J. (1989). Spherical splines
Hoffmann, H., Kessler, H., Eppel, T., Rukavina, S., and Traue, H. C. (2010). for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
Expression intensity, gender and facial emotion recognition: women recognize Neurophysiol. 72, 184–187. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90180-6
only subtle facial emotions better than men. Acta Psychol. 135, 278–283. Proudfit, G. H. (2015). The reward positivity: from basic research on reward to
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.012 a biomarker for depression. Psychophysiology 52, 449–459. doi: 10.1111/psyp.
Holroyd, C. B., Pakzad-Vaezi, K. L., and Krigolson, O. E. (2008). The feedback 12370
correct-related positivity: sensitivity of the event-related brain potential to Sambrook, T. D., and Goslin, J. (2015). A neural reward prediction error revealed
unexpected positive feedback. Psychophysiology 45, 688–697. doi: 10.1111/j. by a meta-analysis of ERPs using great grand averages. Psychol. Bull. 141,
1469-8986.2008.00668.x 213–235. doi: 10.1037/bul0000006
Hunter, P. G., Glenn Schellenberg, E., and Stalinski, S. M. (2011). Liking and Sawada, R., Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., et al.
identifying emotionally expressive music: age and gender differences. J. Exp. (2014). Sex differences in the rapid detection of emotional facial expressions.
Child. Psychol. 110, 80–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.001 PLoS One 9:e94747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094747
Jack, R. E., and Schyns, P. G. (2015). The human face as a dynamic tool for Schirmer, A., Kotz, S. A., and Friederici, A. D. (2002). Sex differentiates the role
social communication. Currt. Biol. 25, R621–R634. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015. of emotional prosody during word processing. Cogn. Brain Res. 14, 228–233.
05.052 doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00108-8
Kim, N.-G., and Son, H. (2015). How facial expressions of emotion affect distance Schirmer, A., Zysset, S., Kotz, S. A., and Yves von Cramon, D. (2004). Gender
perception. Front. Psychol. 6:1825. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01825 differences in the activation of inferior frontal cortex during emotional speech
Kret, M. E., and De Gelder, B. (2012). A review on sex differences in perception. Neuroimage 21, 1114–1123. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.048
processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia 50, 1211–1221. doi: 10.1016/j. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T.,
neuropsychologia.2011.12.022 Golden, C. J., et al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 8 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275


Chen et al. Female Advantage in Interpersonal Emotion Decoding

emotional intelligence. Pers. Individ. Dif. 25, 167–177. doi: 10.1016/S0191- emotional social interactions. Front. Psychol. 6:1372. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.
8869(98)00001-4 01372
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., and Stapp, J. (1975). Ratings of self and peers on sex Xiao, E., and Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment
role attributes and their relation to self-esteem and conceptions of masculinity behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 7398–7401. doi: 10.1073/pnas.
and femininity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 32, 29–39. doi: 10.1037/h0076857 0502399102
Ullsperger, M., Fischer, A. G., Nigbur, R., and Endrass, T. (2014). Neural Yang, J., McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., Dai, X., Yao, S., Cai, T., et al. (1999). Cross-
mechanisms and temporal dynamics of performance monitoring. Trends. cultural personality assessment in psychiatric populations: the NEO-PI—R in
Cogn. Sci. 18, 259–267. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.009 the people’s republic of china. Psychol. Assess. 11, 359–368. doi: 10.1037/1040-
Ulrich, N., and Hewig, J. (2014). A miss is as good as a mile? Processing of 3590.11.3.359
near and full outcomes in a gambling paradigm. Psychophysiology 51, 819–823. Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error
doi: 10.1111/psyp.12232 detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev.
van Doorn, E. A., van Kleef, G. A., and van der Pligt, J. (2015). How emotional 111, 931–959. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.111.4.939
expressions shape prosocial behavior: interpersonal effects of anger and Yuan, J. J., Luo, Y. J., Yan, J. H., Meng, X. X., Yu, F. Q., and Li, H. (2009). Neural
disappointment on compliance with requests. Motivat. Emot. 39, 128–141. correlates of the females0 susceptibility to negative emotions: an insight into
doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9421-6 gender-related prevalence of affective disturbances. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 30,
van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life: the emotions as 3676–3686. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20796
social information (EASI) model. Curr. Dir. in Psychol. Sci. 18, 184–188. Zhao, D., Gu, R., Tang, P., Yang, Q., and Luo, Y. J. (2016). Incidental
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01633.x emotions influence risk preference and outcome evaluation. Psychophysiology
van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K., and Manstead, A. S. (2004). The interpersonal 53, 1542–1551. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12694
effects of anger and happiness in negotiations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 57–76. Zhou, Z., Yu, R., and Zhou, X. (2010). To do or not to do? Action enlarges the
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.57 FRN and P300 effects in outcome evaluation. Neuropsychologia 48, 3606–3613.
Vrtička, P., Andersson, F., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., and Vuilleumier, P. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.010
(2008). Individual attachment style modulates human amygdala and striatum
activation during social appraisal. PLoS One 3:e2868. doi: 10.1371/journal. Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
pone.0002868 conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
Vrtička, P., Sander, D., Anderson, B., Badoud, D., Eliez, S., and Debbané, M. be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
(2014). Social feedback processing from early to late adolescence: influence of
sex, age, and attachment style. Brain Behav. 4, 703–720. doi: 10.1002/brb3.251 Copyright © 2018 Chen, Yuan, Zheng, Chang and Luo. This is an open-access article
Weisenbach, S. L., Rapport, L. J., Briceno, E. M., Haase, B. D., Vederman, A. C., distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
Bieliauskas, L. A., et al. (2014). Reduced emotion processing efficiency in The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
healthy males relative to females. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 316–325. original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original
doi: 10.1093/scan/nss137 publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.
Wiggert, N., Wilhelm, F. H., Derntl, B., and Blechert, J. (2015). Gender differences No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these
in experiential and facial reactivity to approval and disapproval during terms.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 9 July 2018 | Volume 12 | Article 275

You might also like