You are on page 1of 19

PEDIATRIC REHABILITATION, 2001, VOL. 4, NO.

3, 119 ±136

Assessing executive functions in children:


biological, psychological, and developmental
considerations{
VICKI ANDERSON

Keywords Executive function, children, development, assess- [4±6]. These cerebral regions are relatively immature
ment during childhood, with development thought to be a
protracted process which continues into early adoles-
Summary
cence [7±8]. Parallels between ongoing maturation of
Executive functions may be de®ned as those skills necessary the frontal lobes and the emergence of executive capa-
for purposeful, goal-directed activity, and are generally con- cities have been reported in a number of studies. These
sidered to be largely mediated by the frontal and prefrontal results suggest that, where developmentall y appropriate
cortices of the brain. These cerebral regions are relatively
assessment tools are employed, evidence of executive
immature during childhood, with development thought to be
a protracted process which continues into early adolescence. skills can be elicited in children as young as the age of
While early theorists suggested that executive skills were not 6 years [9±20].
functional until cerebral maturity, recent research provides There is now growing evidence that children sustain-
evidence that such skills can be elicited in early childhood. ing brain damage exhibit de®cits in executive skills. Such
The aim of this paper is to review current theories of develop-
problems may interfere with the child’s capacity to
ment of executive functions throughout childhood. In keeping
with contemporary approaches to child neuropsychology, develop normally and interact eVectively with the envir-
three critical dimensions will be evaluated; biological factors, onment, thus leading to ongoing cognitive, academic,
psychological dimensions, and developmental trajectories. In and social disturbances [21, 22]. Individual case studies
addition, the literature which addresses assessment of these provide anecdotal evidence of problems in planning,
functions will be examined, with reference to developmental
trajectories observed in normal populations, and in
problem solving, and abstract thinking in the day-to-
brain-damaged samples, where there may be disruption to day lives of these children [23±25]; however, such `execu-
the underlying neural substrates thought to be subsuming tive dysfunction’ is often di cult to detect using tradi-
these functions. tional assessment tools. Further evidence comes from a
handful of recent studies which have examined executive
functions following childhood brain injuries. Findings
Introduction suggest that residual de®cits in planning, problem solv-
ing, and adaptive behaviour are associated with head-
The concept of `executive function’, although vari- injury [26±29], hydrocephalus and spina bi®da [30], and
ously de®ned, is generally agreed to encompass the skills cranial irradiation for treatment of childhood cancers
necessary for purposeful, goal-directed activity [1±4]. [31, 32]. However, accurate and reliable identi®cation
Neuropsychologica l evidence suggests that these skills of such de®cits, both in clinical practice and research
may be largely mediated by the prefrontal cortex of endeavours, continues to be limited owing to the lack
the brain and related descending systems, providing a of developmentall y appropriate assessment tools.
framework within which stored information can be The aim of this paper is to review current theoretical
adaptively applied to novel, problem-oriented situations
models of executive function in children, from a neuro-
psychological perspective. In keeping with contempor-
Author: Vicki Anderson, Department of Psychology, ary approaches to child neuropsycholog y [33±35], three
University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia. critical dimensions will be evaluated; biological factors,
e-mail: vicki_anderson.psych@muwayf.unimelb.edu.au
yThis paper was originally published in Neuropsychological psychological processes, and developmental trends. In
Rehabilitation, 1998, 8(3), 319±349. Reproduced by permission addition, the literature which addresses assessment of
# 1998 Psychology Press Ltd. these functions will be examined, with reference to

Pediatric Rehabilitatio n ISSN 1363±8491 print/ISSN 1464±5270 online # 2001 Psychology Press Ltd
http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals
DOI: 10.1080 /1363849011009134 7
V. Anderson

developmental trajectories observed in normal popula- organization, di culties with generating and imple-
tions, and in brain-damage d samples where there may menting strategies for problem-solving, perseveration,
be disruption to the underlying neural substrates inability to correct errors of use feedback, and rigid or
thought to be subsuming these functions. concrete thought processes [6, 41]. Qualitative features
of executive dysfunction may include poor self-control,
impulsivity, erratic careless responses, poor initiation,
De®nitions of executive function
and in¯exibility [36]. While these behaviours are com-
Before embarking on a discussion of executive func- monly considered to be `deviant’ in adults, a similar
tion, issues of de®nition must be considered. Over time, interpretation may not always be warranted for chil-
there has been a gradual conceptual shift, with early dren. Before determining whether such behaviours are
notions describing a homogeneous set of processes, indicative of executive dysfunction in children, develop-
and not diVerentiating among individual sub-skills mental expectations need to be considered.
which might be incorporated by such a label.
Currently, while the range of skills included in speci®c
Frontal lobe function/executive function?
de®nitions varies, most authors agree that `executive
functions’ may be best understood as an umbrella The terms `executive function’ and `frontal lobe
term, encompassing a number of interrelated sub-skills, function’ have developed in parallel in the neuropsycho-
necessary for purposeful, goal-directed activity [4, 36]. logical literature. Furthermore, they are often employed
In a recent comprehensive review of neuropsycho- interchangeably, most likely due to observations of
logical assessment procedures, Lezak ([36] p. 42) states executive dysfunction in patients with frontal lobe
that executive functions are `capacities that enable a damage [5, 41, 44±47]. However, the practice of local-
person to engage successfully in independent, purpose- izing executive functions to the frontal lobes has been
ful, self-serving behaviours’. She suggests that they may questioned with similar patterns of behavioural disturb-
be conceptualized as having four components: (1) voli- ance identi®ed in patients where pathology is not
tion; (2) planning; (3) purposeful behaviour; and (4) restricted to these regions [38, 46, 48]. It may be argued
eVective performance, with each involving a distinctive that, while the frontal regions play a vital role in their
set of activity-relate d behaviours . Lezak [36] distin- mediation, the integrity of the entire brain is necessary
guishes between cognitive abilities, which may be seen for intact executive function. Alternatively, executive
as domain speci®c, and executive skills, which act more function may be interpreted purely as a psychological
globally and impact upon all aspects of behaviour. She concept, relating to a set of observable behaviours, with-
argues that the integrity of these functions is necessary out any reference to possible anatomical underpinnings
for appropriate , socially responsible conduct. Stuss [37] [37]. It is this latter psychological perspective which is
provides an integrated model of executive function, emphasized in the present discussion.
including a set of associated skills which allow the indi- There is a growing body of developmental research
vidual to develop goals, hold these goals in active mem- which describes a sequential improvement of perform-
ory, monitor performance, and control for interference ance on executive tasks through childhood, coinciding
in order to achieve these goals. Other authors include with growth spurts in frontal lobe development [18, 47,
focused and sustained attention, generation and imple- 49±51]. Such ®ndings have been interpreted as providing
mentation of strategies, monitoring, and utilization of support for the mediation of executive functions via
feedback under the umbrella term `executive functions’ anterior cerebral regions, and the prefrontal cortex
[6, 25, 38, 39]. Shallice [40] and Walsh [41] ®ne-tune the speci®cally. While this may be the case, these cerebral
concept further, arguing that executive functions are not regions are dependent upon other cerebral areas for
in maximal usage for the execution of routine, well- input, making it di cult to isolate frontal functions
learned behaviours, but are specially activated in novel from those of other developing cerebral areas. It may
or unfamiliar circumstances, where no previously estab- be that, as for research based on brain-damage d popu-
lished routines for responding exist. lations, the maturation of executive function re¯ects the
Such de®nitions are commonly operationalized, for integrity of cerebral development throughout the brain.
the purpose of neuropsychological assessment, to Similarly, from a cognitive perspective, the development
include planning, problem solving, abstract thinking, of executive functions may be inextricably associated
concept formation, self-monitoring, and mental ¯exi- with the gradual emergence of other cognitive capacities,
bility [5, 42, 43]. Thus, `executive dysfunction’ may be with ample evidence for associated gradual increments
re¯ected in test performances by poor planning and in skills such as language [5, 52, 53], attention [54, 55],

120
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

speed of processing [56, 57], and memory capacity Development of the frontal lobes also appears to
[58±61]. follow a hierarchical pattern, consistent with processes
such a myelination, which progress through a number of
stages, from primary and sensory areas to association
PHYSIOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT UNDERPINNING
areas and ®nally frontal regions [65, 72, 73]. Vestibular
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
and spinal tracts, related to basic postural control, are
Knowledge of central nervous system (CNS) matura- myelinated as early as at term. Midbrain corticalÐ
tion and related cognitive development is gradually visual pathways show evidence of myelination by 2±3
increasing, with advances in technical methodologies. months of age, and descending lateral cortical tracts
It is now well established that cerebral development is by the end of the ®rst year of life, when ®ne motor
ongoing during childhood. Brain weight increases from control appears [74]. Cerebellar±cerebral connections
¹ 400 grams at birth to 1500 grams at maturity in early are not myelinated until the second year of life, with
adulthood, although most maturation is thought to reticular tracts still maturing at school age and tracts
occur during the ®rst decade of life [62]. While pre- connecting speci®c and associative areas showing
natal development is primarily concerned with struc- ongoing development into adulthood [8].
tural formation, post-natal development is associated Results from the EEG studies also indicate CNS
with elaboration of the CNS [63, 64]. In particular, pro- changes through childhood. Thatcher [50, 51] has
cesses such as dendritic aborisation, myelination, and described a number of growth periods, the ®rst between
synaptogenesis have all been reported to progress during birth and 2 years, another from 7 to 9 years with a ®nal
early childhood, in a largely hierarchical manner, with spurt in late adolescence (16±19 years). These growth
anterior regions the last to reach maturity [65±68]. spurts are thought to be associated with increases in
Initially, developmental neuropsycholog y was in¯u- either the number or strength of cortical synaptic con-
enced by a view that the frontal lobes were `functionally nections. Consistent with Thatcher’s ®ndings, Hudspeth
silent’ in infancy and early childhood, with executive and Pribram [72] document EEG data which indicate
skills not measurable until the second decade of life maturational peaks and plateaux continuing through
[69]. A number of neropsychological studies now refute childhood and into adolescence. They report a diVeren-
this view, documenting frontal lobe activity even in tial progression of regional cerebral development, with
infancy. For example, Chugani et al. [70] measured simultaneous completion of maturation throughout the
local cerebral metabolic rates of glucose in infants and CNS. In frontal regions, they describe accelerated devel-
young children, and found evidence of frontal metabolic opment from 7±10 years, which then terminates syn-
changes in infants as young as 6 months of age. chronously with development of other brain regions.
Similarly, Bell and Fox [49] have documented changes Age-related pre-frontal ribonucleic acid and develop-
in scalp recorded electroencephalogram s (EEGs) in ment, through to ¹ 9 years of age [75], and changes in
frontal regions during the ®rst year of life, relating pattern of metabolic activity and levels of various
these to improvements in behavioural performance. enzymes [76], also support a hierarchical model of fron-
Many workers now support the notion that these bio- tal lobe development.
logical growth markers may explain some of the age- It may be that not all CNS development conforms to
related variation in `non-biological ’ development such this hierarchical model. An alternative argument sug-
as cognition [50, 51, 62]. gests that, while measurable parameters behave in a
It is generally agreed that the frontal lobes are hier- spurt-like fashion, underlying development is essentially
archically organized, with all areas receiving input from continuous [37]. For example, synaptogenesi s appears to
posterior and subcortical cerebral regions. In particular, be simultaneous in multiple areas and layers of the cor-
the prefrontal cortex, thought to be the primary media- tex [77], with neurotransmitte r receptors throughout the
tor of executive functions, receives input from all areas brain reported to mature at the same time [78]. Such
of the frontal and posterior neocortex [65, 71]. Thus ®ndings suggest concurrent development, where poster-
sensory and perceptual data are processed by the frontal ior and anterior structures develop along approximatel y
lobes, where actions are organized and executed. This the same time-table. Not all research supports this view
pattern of connectivity suggests that, while prefrontal of simultaneous maturation, even for neurochemical
regions may `orchestrate’ behaviour, they are also markers, with some arguing that this pattern, while
dependent on all other cerebral areas for input, with present in non-human species, may not hold for humans
e cient functioning reliant upon the quality of informa- [79]. Clearly, there is a need for further research to
tion received from other cerebral regions. delineate these complex issues.

121
V. Anderson

To summarize, these various lines of inquiry suggest human infants. However, such ®ndings need to be inter-
that cerebral development is likely to be primarily hier- preted cautiously, with the understanding that cross-spe-
archical, both within and across cerebral regions, with cies distinctions are problematic, as are comparisons
frontal areas reaching maturity relatively late, in early between the process of normal development and the
puberty. Further, there is substantial support for a step- eVects of acquired brain pathology.
wise model of development, rather than a gradual pro- Other studies have attempted to map developmental
gression, with convergent evidence that growth spurts trajectories for aspects of executive function in older
occur in early infancy, again around 7±10 years of age, children. Passler et al. [19] report one of the earliest
with a ®nal spurt during adolescence. studies employing this methodology. Using measures
of executive functioning adapted from adult neuro-
psychology, they have shown that children as young
P SYCHOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT
as 6 years are able to exhibit strategic and planful behav-
A number of parallels may be drawn between patterns iour. Their results suggest a stage-like progression of
of cognitive and biological development. Numerous executive skills, with mastery still not achieved by the
studies have now reported that executive functions pro- age of 12. In a follow-up study, Becker et al. [84] report
gress in a stage-like manner, consistent with growth a similar pattern of results, once again noting a failure to
spurts identi®ed within the CNS. Historically, cognitive achieve adult levels on executive measures by the age of
models have strongly supported such a hierarchical view 12. Using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test as their
of development. In particular, Piaget’s theory of cogni- measure of executive function, Chelune and Baer [85]
tive development [80], while providing no reference to report improvements in performance between 6 and 10
possible neural substrates, is highly compatible with cur- years, with adult performanc e achieved by 12 years.
rent understandings of cerebral development. Piaget Further, they observed that 6-year-old children demon-
describes four sequential cognitive stages including sen- strated di culties similar to those seen in adults with
sorimotor (birth±2 years), pre-operational (2±7 years), focal frontal lesions!
concrete operational (7±9 years) and formal operational More recently, a number of researchers have
(early adolescence). While more contemporary develop- employed a `battery model’, administering a range of
mental psychologists may dispute some of the principles tests purported to measure executive functions. Such
of Piagetian theory [81], it is worthy of note that the an approach, while providing developmental trajectories
hypothesized timing of transition between Piaget’s for each of these tasks, also enables investigation of
cognitive stages coincide quite closely with growth possible relationships among measures, thus addressing
spurts identi®ed within the CNS. the crucial issue of test validity. Levin et al. [18] eval-
Some researchers have employed Piagetian techniques uated 52 normal children and adolescents in three age
to investigate early cognitive development and its rela- bands, 7±8 years, 9±12 years, and 13±15 years. They
tionship with cerebral development. In a series of inves- administered a range of `executive’ measures and identi-
tigations, Diamond and Goldman-Raki c [14, 15, 82, 83] ®ed developmental gains across all tasks, re¯ecting pro-
employed the classic Piagetian object permanence para- gress in concept formation, mental ¯exibility, planning
digm, as well as an object retrieval task, to investigate and problem solving through childhood. Although their
goal-directed behaviours in infants. To establish links sample size was relatively small, they performed prin-
with possible cerebral substrates, they compared per- cipal components analysis on their data, identifying
formances of human infants to those of both adult three factors which they argued were associated with
and infant rhesus monkeys with focal lesions. They speci®c aspects of executive functions, as well as unique
found that infants as young as 12 months were able to developmental patterns. Factor 1 tapped semantic as-
exhibit object permanence, as were monkeys with par- sociation/concept formation and Factor 3 was primarily
ietal lesions. By contrast, older rhesus monkeys with concerned with problem solving, with each of these abil-
frontal lesions were unable to complete these tasks suc- ities showing a gradual progression over the three age
cessfully. Similarly, for objective retrieval tasks, human ranges. Factor 2 was related to impulse control and
infants showed age-related improvements in planning mental ¯exibility, and these behaviours were noted to
and self-control, mirroring those of normal infant mon- reach adult levels by the age of 12.
keys. Monkeys with frontal lesions were unable to mas- Welsh et al. [20] also studied a sample of normal
ter these tasks. These results have been interpreted as children, aged 3±12 years, using a series of measures
evidence that frontally mediated, goal-directed, planful of executive function. Consistent with previous ®ndings,
behaviour is present as early as 12 months of age in their results provide evidence for stage-like develop-

122
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

ment, with some components of executive function speci®c areas within the frontal lobes, also maturing at
maturing earlier than others, thus supporting a multi- diVerent rates. From a biological perspective, the poss-
dimensional notion of executive function. They argue ible in¯uence of ongoing development of other cerebral
for three distinct developmental stages, the ®rst com- areas also needs to be considered. For example, the
mencing around age 6, a second about age 10, and quality of neural transmission from posterior and sub-
®nal spurt in early adolescence. They suggest that the cortical regions may impact upon the functioning of the
ability to resist distraction is the ®rst skill to mature, at frontal and prefrontal cortices which have rich connec-
around age 6. Organized search, hypothesis testing, and tions with all cerebral areas. Maturation of these poster-
impulse control appear to reach adult levels at around ior regions may then enhance the functioning of
age 10, with verbal ¯uency, motor sequencing and plan- anterior cerebral areas. From a cognitive viewpoint,
ning skills not at adult levels at age 12. They further similar considerations are important. The gradual emer-
investigated possible associations among their measures, gence of greater memory capacity, more advanced lan-
and identi®ed three discrete factors: Factor 1, described guage skills, and faster speed of processing will all
as representing speeded responding; Factor 2, an indi- enhance the child’s capacity to function on measures
cator of hypothesis testing and impulse control; and of executive functioning. While tentative links have
Factor 3, re¯ecting planning ability. been established, there is still a way to go to de®ne
Anderson et al. [56] employed a similar methodology, relationships between development of executive skills
with the primary aim of providing normative data for a and frontal structures, or isolate cognitive gains speci®c
number of commonly used clinical tests, purported to to executive functions, divorced from lower-order
measure executive functions. Their sample included 376 cognitive capacities. Regardless, this convergence of
children aged 7±13 years, selected to be representative of evidence emphasizes the importance of close communi-
the general population with respect to social factors and cation among disciplines involved in improving one’s
gender. In line with the work of Levin [39] and Welsh et understanding of brain±behaviour relationships in the
al. [20], results suggest continued signi®cant improve- developing child.
ments in test performance through middle childhood,
indicating ongoing gains in executive functions. An ex-
Assessment of executive function
amination of correlations among these executive meas-
ures, outlined in table 1, suggests relatively strong The assessment of executive function is a topic which
associations between tasks tapping problem solving has received considerable attention in adult neuro-
and planning ability. By contrast, only a weak relation- psychology. Early, localizationist models designated
ship was found between these measures and tests of tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [86, 87]
concept formation, again suggesting that executive func- or the Complex Figure of Rey [88] as indicators of
tion is likely to incorporate a range of sub-skills. frontal lobe or executive function, based on poor per-
The parallels between the developmental trajectories formance by patients with frontal lobe pathology.
described in these studies, and those reported in neuro- Contemporary neuropsychologica l theory would argue
physiological research are di cult to ignore. However, that such an approach is too simplistic. The e ciency of
the situation is complex. While all measures employed in executive skills, and also of frontal lobe functioning, is
these studies provide evidence that executive function, necessarily mediated by lower-order processes. It is
rather than being a unitary concept, may be divided into important then to view executive functions in the con-
a number of speci®c categories, exhibiting diVerent text of these other functions, and to evaluate carefully
developmental trajectories, and maturing at diVerential assessment tools, considering the speci®c components of
rates. These varying patterns may re¯ect mediation by executive function they measure. In many cases this may

Table 1 Correlations among tests of executive function from Anderson et al. [56]

Measure Trails A Trails B CFR: copy CFR: organization TOL: correct

COWAT 0.20 0.06 70.11 70.11 0.40**


Trails A 0.37** 70.22 70.21 70.28**
Trails B 70.30** 70.32** 70.35**
CFR: copy 0.48** 0.37**
CFR: organization 0.31**

**p < 0:001.


COWAT: Controlled Oral Word Association Test; CFR: Complex Figure of Rey; TOL: Tower of London.

123
V. Anderson

be di cult to tease out, and often the `endpoint score’ is function in both groups. Such assumptions remain
not particularly sensitive to executive functions, as it untested, suggesting that caution is required when pro-
commonly summarizes performance on a variety of dif- viding localizing interpretations of test performance
ferent cognitive components. Assessment and isolation [92]. At a more practical level, these tests may be of little
of executive de®cits may rely on administration of mul- interest or relevance to young children, and frequently
tiple tests, each focusing on speci®c aspects of function, lack normative information with respect to developmen-
and sequentially ruling out skills as de®cient. tal expectations . Todd et al. [93] stressed the importance
A number of authors comment on the problems of of establishing expected normal levels of executive
assessing executive function. In addition to being acces- function in children and adolescents, when they com-
sible only through tests which include lower-order func- pared the results of head-injured and healthy adoles-
tions, de®cits in these skills are often di cult to detect cents on tests of planning ability. They found that,
within the clinical context, using standardized assess- while de®ciencies in planning were evident in their
ment tools. Typically, the neuropsychogica l assessment head-injured sample, this was also true for healthy ado-
is conducted in a well-structured clinic setting, where the lescents. The clinical implications of such ®ndings are of
examiner plans and initiates the majority of the evalua- signi®cance, when it may be that such de®cits are inac-
tion. Lezak [36] emphasizes these problems, noting that curately interpreted by clinicians as injury-related con-
de®cits in executive function are rarely re¯ected in test sequences, rather than developmentally appropriate
scores, as the majority of assessment tools are also levels of skill.
highly structured. Parker and Crawford [45], in a review To establish valid measures of executive function,
of assessment procedures, claimed to measure executive which overcome the problems described above, it is
functions, found `disappointingly few sensitive and essential to evaluate their capacity to measure the
reliable tests which the clinical neuropsychologis t can primary skills included in de®nitions of the concept:
depend upon’ (p. 286). They go further to argue that, planning, problem solving, abstract thinking, concept
when evaluating executive function, the clinician is often formation, self-monitoring, and mental ¯exibility [5,
forced to rely on qualitative observations and informed 42, 43, 47]. Walsh [41] argues that, in order to tap
judgement, as well as reports from family and social these skills eVectively, tasks require several characteris-
contexts. tics: novelty, complexity, and the need to integrate infor-
mation. In support of Walsh’s `formula’ for assessment
of executive functions, Shallice [40] states that routinized
Child assessment
tasks can be performed almost automatically, without
Whilst some researchers [69] have suggested that reference to executive skills. However, novel or complex
executive functions do not emerge until early adoles- tasks require the individual to develop new schema, for-
cence, recent studies provide compelling evidence that mulate new strategies, and monitor their eVectiveness,
these skills are assessable even in the preschool child, thus activating executive skills.
provided appropriate measures are employed. Given The most widely accepted measures of executive func-
the vulnerability of executive functions to early brain tion have been designed or borrowed from cognitive
damage [25], the importance of intact executive function psychology, with these basic requirements in mind.
to ongoing cognitive development [22], and to the suc- One of the greatest problems, however, is the lack
cess of treatment and rehabilitation programmes, there of consensus as to which of these measures is a valid
is a need to devise valid and well-standardized assess- indicator of executive function. A review of a number
ment measures, speci®cally designed for children, and of recent studies which have been designed to assess
based on current understandings of the nature of both executive skills shows that individual researchers
cerebral and cognitive development through childhood. vary in their understanding of which tests provide
There are many papers and even entire tests devoted the best measures of executive functions. Table 2
to neuropsychologica l assessment of children. A perusal lists the tests included in a number of recent child-
of chapter topics and subject indexes suggests that based studies, each of which employed multiple
executive function is frequently neglected [24, 34, 89± measures of executive functions. Clearly, some measures
91]. Further, most available or commonly utilized tests are more universally accepted. For example, the
purported to measure executive function in children Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT)
have been developed for use with adults, with their [52] was employed in half of the studies reviewed, and
inclusion in test protocols for children based on the was generally described as a measure of abstraction or
assumption that they will detect similarly localized dys- concept formation. Similarly, the Wisconsin Card

124
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

Table 2 Tests purported to measure executive functions in child-based studies

Trail
TOL/ Go/ Porteus Category Making TOVA/ Twenty Stroop
Study WCST TOH no go COWAT CFR mazes test test MFFT CPT questions test

[84] ± ± * ± ± ± ± ± ± ± ±
[33] ± ± ± ± ± ± * * ± ± ± *
[39] ± ± ± * * * ± ± ± ± * ±
[18] * * * * ± ± ± ± ± ± * ±
[20] * * ± * ± ± ± ± * ± ± ±
[94] ± * ± * ± ± ± ± ± ± ± ±
[95] * ± ± * * ± ± ± ± * ± *
[96] * ± ± * * * ± * ± * ± *
[56] ± * ± * * ± ± * ± ± ± ±
[97] ± ± ± * ± ± ± ± * ± * ±
[98] ± * ± ± * ± ± ± ± ± * ±
[99] * ± ± * * ± ± ± ± * ± ±

WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; TOL/TOH: Tower of London/Tower of Hanoi; COWAT: Controlled Oral Word Association Test; CFR:
Complex Figure of Rey; MFFT: Matching Familiar Figures Test; TOVA: Test of Visual Attention; CPT: Continuous Performance Test.

Sorting Test (WCST) [87], variations of the Tower of Block Design [41], Picture Arrangement [45] and
London (TOL) [2], the Complex Figure of Rey (CFR) Similarities have been earmarked as measures of
[88], and Trail Making Test [100] were also commonly planning, problem solving, and abstraction, respect-
included in batteries purported to tap executive skills in ively. Even more recently developed, process-based
children. Details of each of these tests are provided in assessment batteries (e.g. McCarthy Scales of
the following discussion. Children’s Abilities [106]; Kaugman Ability Battery for
Children [107]) include no speci®c analysis of these
skills. Lezak [36] argues that, in general, these tests
METHODS OF ASSESSMENT may be too highly structured to tap executive functions
Standardize d measures accurately, which may be best identi®ed on the basis of
careful observation of qualitative features of test
There are a range of child-based battery-style performance and from information provided by family
psychometric tests available; however, they commonly and educational sources with respect to day-to-day
pay scant attention to the measurement of executive functioning.
function (e.g. Halstead Reitan Neuropsychologica l
Test Battery for Children [101]; Reitan±Indiana
Neuropsychologica l Test Battery for Younger Children
Individual test procedures
[102]; Luria±Nebraska Neuropsychologica l Battery
[103]). For the Wechsler scales (Wechsler Intelligence One method of rationalizing the current relatively
Scale for ChildrenÐIII [104]; Wechsler Preschool arbitrary use of individual tests purported to measure
and Primary Scale of IntelligenceÐRevised [105]), executive function is to categorize each test (or test
there is a similar lack of focus on measures of executive component) according to the skills they are thought to
function, although, from adult samples, it has measure. Table 3 provides one possible example, listing
been argued that individual subtests may measure commonly used measures, according to the primary
aspects of executive function. For example, aspects of executive function involved in each task.

Table 3 Tests purported to tap speci®c aspects of executive function

Executive function Test Standard administration Normative data

Planning and organization Complex Figure of Rey Yes For 6 years and older
Porteus Mazes Yes No
Problem solving Tower of London Several versions For 6 years and older
Tower of Hanoi No No
Abstraction/concept formation Controlled Oral Word Association Yes For 6 years and older
Twenty Questions Several versions in use No
Mental ¯exibility Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Yes Yes
Trail Making Test Yes For 6 years and older
Stroop Test Yes No

125
V. Anderson

ployed in copying the ®gure. This procedure is based


on scoring the copy as the child is drawing, noting
the order in which component parts are produced, and
whether the child has employed a logical, planful
approach, or a more piecemeal random method. As
with other organizational scores, there is a gradual
improvement in organizationa l level with age. Further,
and perhaps not surprisingly, there is a close relation-
ship between quality of organization of copy and quality
of recall, emphasizing the importance of intact planning
for day-to-day functioning.
A number of clinical studies have included the CFR
in their test protocols, with de®cient performances
observed in a range of patient populations, including
learning-disabled children [111], head-injured children
[98], post-meningitic children [112], and cranially irra-
Figure 1 The Complex Figure of Rey. diated children [31, 113]. However, while these ®ndings
provide support that the CFR is a useful measure of
planning ability in children with both developmental
What follows is a brief description of some of these
and acquired CNS disorders, there is no evidence as
measures, with discussion restricted to those tests for
yet that performance on the task is speci®cally related
which there are standardized administration procedures
to frontal lobe dysfunction.
and adequate developmental norms.
Maze Tasks (Porteus Mazes [114]; Mazes, Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for ChildrenÐIII [104]; Wechsler
Preschool and Primary Scale of IntelligenceÐRevised
PLANNING ABILITY
[105]) have also been employed to assess planning and
The Complex Figure of Rey (CFR) [88] requires the foresight in children. Speci®c maze formats and admin-
child to copy a complex geometric design, illustrated in istration procedures vary across tests, but each provide
®gure 1, and then redraw it from memory following a 3- some developmental data, enabling interpretation of
minute delay. It is generally described as a test of plan- performance with respect to age expectations. As for
ning and organization skills; however, other lower-order the CFR, the evaluation of executive functions via
skills are also important to e cient performance. These maze tasks is confounded by other skills required for
skills include visual perception, visuo-motor skills, ®ne e cient performance, including ®ne motor skills, visual
motor skills, and visual memory. All might contribute to perception, and speed of response.
overall performance. Scores have been devised for copy
accuracy, recall, and organizationa l e ciency, with a
PROBLEM SOLVING
number of references providing scoring protocols and
developmental norms [56, 108, 109]. Accuracy, recall, The Tower of London (TOL) [2] measures problem-
and organizationa l scores have been shown to improve solving aspects of executive functioning. The task in-
through childhood, as illustrated in ®gure 2 [56], and volves 12 items, with each requiring children to rear-
®gure 3 provides examples of copies produced by range three coloured balls to a con®guration presented
normal children of varying ages, emphasizing individual on a stimulus card, and in a prescribed number of
diVerences in performance, even within age bands. moves. Where a child fails to complete an item correctly,
With respect to executive functions, one of the great- the balls are replaced in their original con®guration, and
est breakthroughs has been the development of organ- the child has the opportunity to try again. Scores include
izational measures, rather than simple accuracy and number of items correct, numer of failed attempts,
recall indices. Waber and Holmes [109] devised one of planning time, and time taken to complete each item.
the earliest versions of organizationa l scoring, employ- An example of one of the items in the test is shown in
ing a 5-point scale to rate productions in terms of out- ®gure 4.
come. Because of perceived limitations in this method, Shallice [2] argues that the TOL involves minimal
Anderson et al. [110] have developed an alternative contribution of lower-order skills, such as perceptual
model which speci®cally addresses the `process’ em- and motor abilities, short-term memory, and sustained

126
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

Figure 2 Development trajectories for the Complex Figure of Rey.

attention, based on low associations noted between the tal data [9, 115]. These ®ndings describe continued
TOL and other cognitive measures. If this is the case, improvements in test performance through childhood,
then the TOL may overcome some of the di culties of with some evidence for developmental spurts at around
other executive tests. In addition, the test may be utilized age 8±9 and again around age 10±11. Developmental
to tap a number of components of executive function, trajectories for the TOL from the Anderson et al. [9]
including planning speed, impulsivity, and ¯exibility. sample are shown in ®gure 5. Finally, the TOL is one
While originally designed for use with adults, the TOL of the few executive measures which has been directly
has a number of features that allow it to be readily related to frontal lobe damage in children. Levin et al.
applicable to pediatric populations. First, the task is [116] found de®cient performanc e on the TOL to be
generally challenging and attractive to children of vary- associated with head injury in children. In addition,
ing ages. In addition it incorporates a range of di culty they identi®ed a speci®c contribution of frontal lobe
levels, so that even young children are able to complete damage to performanc e levels, and in particular to the
initial items. frequency of rule-breaking behaviours.
Despite its potential advantages , clinical use of the The Tower of Hanoi (TOH) [16, 61] is a similar disk-
TOL has been restricted, owing to the lack of standar- transfer task, requiring a child to solve a problem to
dized administration protocols and normative data. which the solution is not immediately apparent, but
However, two recently published studies provide alter- requires the formulation of a new plan. Similar to the
native testing protocols, and both include developmen- TOL, the child is directed to plan a sequence of legal

127
V. Anderson

Figure 3 Examples of normal children’s productions of the Complex Figure of Rey. (a) 6-year-old children, (b) 8-year-old children, and (c) 12-year-
old children.

128
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

moves in order to transfer the initial con®guration into a


target con®guration, using as few moves as possible.
There are several versions of this task; however, no
uniform administration procedures or normative data
are available, limiting its use in child assessment.

ABSTRACTION AND CONCEPT FORMATION

Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) [52]


is a measure of verbal ¯uency and the ability to generate
words based on a set of arbitrary rules. Children are
asked to generate words beginning with a certain letter
in a 1-minute period. These words must be selected with
consideration of two rules: (1) No words must be begin
with a capital letter, and (2) each word must be used
only once. Scores include total number of words gener-
ated over three letter trials, number of words repeated,
Figure 4 The Tower of London. and number of rule breaks. While some normative data

Figure 5 Developmental trajectories for the Tower of London.

129
V. Anderson

Figure 6 Developmental trajectories for the Controlled Oral Word Association Test.

were included in the original description of this task, elephant or uncle when asked to generate words com-
subsequent studies have provided more substantial nor- mencing with the letter A, and concept of `capital
mative data [53, 56], making it a more useful clinical letters’ may not be adequately established for some age
tool. Changes in performances with age, as reported groups. Thus, the interpretation of this measure as one
by Anderson et al. [56], are given in ®gure 6. of executive function needs to be made cautiously, with
The COWAT is one of the most commonly used such developmental considerations taken into account.
measures of executive function in pediatric research. To emphasize this point, a number of studies of reading-
De®cient performances on this test have been described disabled children, who are thought to experience speci®c
following pediatric head injury [117], and childhood phonological de®cits, have reported de®cits on the
meningitis [118], as well as for children with Tourette COWAT [119, 120].
syndrome [99], although not for focal frontal involve- Twenty Questions [121] is a test of a child’s ability to
ment. Interpretation of the COWAT is problematic utilize feedback and re-evaluat e goals to reach a correct
within the pediatric population. The lower-order skills response. The child is shown a card with 42 hand-draw n
required for the task include phonological awareness, pictures which may be grouped into various categories
which is not well developed in younger children. It is (for example, animals, plants, utensils). The child is
not uncommon for children to provide words such as asked to identify which picture the examiner has in

130
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

mind, and is able to ask 20 questions to do so. Only from the examiner. The test continues until six correct
questions necessitating a yes or no response are allowed. categories have been achieved, or until all cards are
Scoring may include time taken to detect target and sorted. Scores derived from the test include number of
total questions asked. Unfortunately , developmental categories achieved, total number of errors, and percen-
norms are not available for this task, and administration tage of perseverative errors.
protocols vary considerably. The task can, however, Chelune and Baer [85] provide normative data for
provide qualitative information regarding the child’s children aged between 6±12 years on the WCST,
concept-formatio n strategies, by dividing questions indicating improvements in performance throughout
into speci®c categories. For example, hypothesis-seeking childhood, re¯ecting increasing abilities in concept for-
approaches such as `is it the pot plant?’ are ine cient as mation and ability to shift a set ¯exibly. The test has
they only eliminate one of the pictures. By contrast, also been employed with a number of clinical samples,
constraint-seeking questions, such as `is it alive’, will suggesting that the task is sensitive to the generalized
eliminate several possibilities, and thus are more e - impact of head injury [117, 122], cranial irradiation
cient. Levin et al. [18] utilized this measure in their [118], as well as developmental disorders including
study of executive function, and found that older chil- Tourette syndrome [99] and attention de®cit hyperactiv-
dren needed to ask fewer questions to identify target ity disorder [123, 124]. Currently, there is no evidence
pictures, suggesting better capacity to form concepts that the test is able speci®cally to diVerentiate frontal
and utilize feedback. Garth et al. [98] employed the lobe dysfunction in children.
Twenty Questions test with children who had sustained Trail Making Test [100] measures speed of visual
frontal lobe damage. They found no diVerences between search, attention, mental ¯exibility, and visuo-motor
a clinical group and controls on summary measures of function. The children’s version of the task has two
total questions asked and time to completion. Using a levels. Trails A consists of 15 circled numbers randomly
qualitative analysis of the nature of questions posed, positioned on a page. Children are required to connect
they identi®ed less e cient performance by children the numbers in order by making pencil lines. This level
with frontal lesions. This group exhibited higher provides a baseline indication of speed of visual search
frequencies in the hypothesis-seekin g and pseudo- and visuo-motor functioning. The second level, Trails B,
constraint-seeking categories and lower frequencies of requires children to connect 15 numbers and letters in
the more eVective constraint-seeking questions. These alternating order. This level incorporates an additional
®ndings provide some initial support for a link between component of shift or mental ¯exibility. Completion
performance on Twenty Questions and speci®c frontal time and number of errors are recorded for each level.
lobe involvement. There are a handful of clinical studies which have
employed this task and identi®ed reduced performances
associated with developmental disorders and generalized
MENTAL FLEXIBILITY
cerebral dysfunction [96, 117, 120, 122]. Age-related per-
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) [86, 87] is also formance patterns for normal children are illustrated in
commonly considered to tap aspects of executive skill, in ®gure 7.
particular the ability to form abstract concepts and shift Stroop Test [125] is a popular measure of cognitive
and maintain set. The test begins with the child being ¯exibility, tapping a child’s ability to shift a cognitive
presented with four stimulus cards, the ®rst showing a set to conform with changing demands, and suppress a
red triangle, the second two green stars, the third three habitual response in favour of a more novel one. The
yellow crosses, and the fourth four blue circles. The task includes three subtests: First, the `Word’ condition,
child is then given 128 response cards, each with con®g- where the child reads out a list of colour names printed
urations similar to those appearing on the stimulus in black type; secondly, the `Colour’ condition where the
cards, but each varying with respect to colour, number, child must name the colour of groups of dots; and
and geometric shape. The child is directed to match each thirdly, the `Colour/Word’ task, where colour names
card to one of the four stimulus cards and informed that are presented in coloured ink, with the two dimensions
they will be told whether or not each response is correct. not always compatible. The requirement is to ignore the
Initially, the correct sorting criteria is colour, and once verbal content and name the colour in which the words
the child has achieved 10 correct responses, the examiner are printed. For each condition, number of errors is
shifts the target dimension to form. Subjects are not recorded, and if a time-limited administration is not
informed directly of the particular dimensions for sort- employed, time taken to completion may also be scored.
ing, but must determine this on the basis of feedback There are several published versions of the Stroop

131
V. Anderson

Figure 7 Developmental trajectories for the Trail Making Test.

paradigm, with one designed speci®cally for children groups, with some success. Outside the neuropsycho-
[126]; however, normative data are limited. Despite its logical literature, several researchers have investigated
popularity, many child-based studies report that this functional planning skills in children and adolescents
measure is not a good indicator of cerebral dysfunction [132, 133]. In these studies, information such as the
in general or frontal lobe damage in particular [96, use of strategies, presence of purpose, and assistance
127]. gained by aids has been analysed to determine what
constitutes good planning. Some researchers have
begun to use these approaches in clinical populations
QUALITATIVE APPROACH [93, 98, 134, 135] to fractionate aspects of executive skills
While a number of these traditional executive meas- exhibited by children with disabilities. Information
ures employ `endpoint’ or summary scores as indicators obtained from such qualitative analysis of behaviour
may be usefully employed in the design of rehabilitation
of performance, it may be argued that qualitative
programmes and remedial interventions.
aspects of performance or `microanalysis’ of individual
skills required for adequate performance provides a
more accurate picture of executive functioning. In the
Future directions
adult literature, a number of such approaches has been
developed, and found to be sensitive to subtle de®cits in Several lines of evidence now provide a picture of
executive function not apparent on standard measures ongoing development of executive functions through-
[128, 129]. Discourse analysis procedures [130, 131], out childhood. Physiological research describes sub-
involving ®ne-grained analysis of language transcripts, stantial CNS development continuing at least into
have also been employed to investigate subtle diVerences early adolescence, with anterior cerebral regions matur-
in conversation patterns between clinical and control ing relatively late, and showing a series of growth spurts.

132
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

Neuropsychologica l studies have also identi®ed `growth References


spurts’ represented by distinct improvements in per- 1. LURIA, A. R.: Higher cortical functions in man (New York: Basic
formance on tests purported to measure executive Books), 1966.
2. SHALLICE, T.: Speci®c impairments of planning. Philosophical
functions. Further, there is a suggestion that these phy- Transcripts of the Royal Society of London, 298: 199±209, 1982.
siological and cognitive `spurts’ may coincide, with tran- 3. SPREEN, O., R ISSER, A. H. and EDGELL, D.: Developmental
sitions in cognitive development re¯ecting ongoing Neuropsychology (New York: Oxford University Press), 1985.
4. STUSS, D. T. and BENSON, D. F.: The frontal lobes (New York:
cerebral development.
Raven Press), 1986.
The immaturity of these executive skills through 5. LURIA, A. R.: The working brain (New York: Basic Books), 1973.
childhood suggests that they may be vulnerable to the 6. STUSS, D. T. and BENSON, D. F.: The frontal lobes and control of
impact of early cerebral insult, where emerging and cognition and memory. In: E. Perecman (editor) The frontal lobes
revisited (New York: IRBN Press), pp. 141±158, 1987.
developing abilities have been noted to be particularly 7. RABINOW ICZ , T.: Morphological features of the developing brain.
at risk [22]. The presence of `dysexecutive syndrome’ In: M. A. B. Brazier and F. Coceani (editors) Brain dysfunction in
early in life may have important implications for infantile febrile convulsions (New York: Raven), pp. 1±23, 1976.
8. YAKOVLEV, P. I. and LECOURS, A. R.: The myelogenetic cycles of
ongoing cognitive development. The lack of ability to regional maturation of the brain. In: A. Minkiniwski (editor)
plan, reason, abstract, and think ¯exibly may impinge Regional development of the brain in early life (Oxford, UK:
on a child’s capacity to learn and bene®t from the envir- Blackwell), pp. 3±70, 1967.
9. ANDERSON, P., A NDERSON, V. and LAJOIE, G.: The Tower of
onment. Clinical observations of brain-injured children
London Test: validation and standardization for pediatric popula-
support this notion, with recent research also demon- tions. The Clinical Neuropsychologis t, 10: 54±65, 1996.
strating the presence of residual executive dysfunction 10. BRUNER, J.: Organization of early skilled action. Child
following early cerebral insult. To describe accurately Development, 44: 1±11, 1973.
11. DELOACHE, J. S. and BROW N, A. L.: Where do I go next?
the range of de®cits associated with childhood brain Intelligent searching by very young children. Developmental
insult, and provide appropriate treatment, neuro- Psychology, 20: 37±44, 1984.
psychologists need to include measures tapping these 12. DENNIS, M.: Frontal lobe function in childhood and adolescence:
a heuristic for assessing attention regulation, executive control,
skills in their assessment protocols. At present, there and the intentional states important for social discourse.
are few useful tests of executive function available for Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 7: 327±358, 1991.
childhood populations. Of those currently employed, 13. DIAMOND, A.: The development of neural bases of memory func-
tion as indexed by the AB and delayed response tasks in human
most have been designed for adult populations. Many
infants and rhesus monkeys. In: A. Diamond (editor) The devel-
lack standardized administration and scoring pro- opment of neural bases of higher cortical function (New York: New
cedures. Few have adequate developmental norms, pre- York Academy of Science Press), pp. 267±317, 1990.
cluding accurate interpretation of developmentall y 14. DIAMOND, A. and G OLDMAN-R AKIC, P. S. Comparison of human
infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget’s AB task: evidence for
appropriate levels of performance. As a consequence, dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental
clinicians are often required to base their analysis of Brain Research, 74: 24±40, 1989.
executive function on qualitative observation and con- 15. GOLDMAN-RAKIC, P. S.: Development of cortical circuitry and
cognitive function. Child Development, 58: 601±622, 1987.
textual data. 16. KHLAR, D.: Goal formation, planning, and learning by preschool
In order to establish valid and reliable assessment problem solvers or: `My socks are in the dryer’. In: R. Seigler
of executive function in children, further research is (editor) Children’s thinking: What develops? (Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.), pp. 181±211, 1978.
essential. First, traditional tests need to be normed for 17. KLAHR, D. and R OBINSON, M.: Formal assessment of problem
children, to provide information with respect to age- solving and planning processes in preschool children. Cognitive
appropriate test performances, thus enabling detection Psychology, 13: 113±148, 1981.
of deviant results. Secondly, rather than restricting 18. LEVIN, H. S., C ULHANE, K. A., HARTMANN, J. et al.: Devel-
opmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal
interpretation to summary scores, maximum informa- lobe functioning. Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 7: 377±395,
tion may be obtained from these tests through a micro- 1991.
analysis of test performance, where individual test 19. PASSLER, M. A., ISAAC, W. and H YND, G. W.: Neuro-
psychological development of behavior attributed to frontal lobe
variables are considered and interpreted with respect functioning in children. Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 1: 349±
to speci®c components of executive function. Thirdly, 370, 1985.
drawing on both adult tests and developmental psychol- 20. W ELSH, M. C., PENNINGTON, B. F. and G ROISSER, D. B.: A
normative±developmental study of executive function: a window on
ogy, new `child-friendly’ procedures need to be designed prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7:
and validated. Further, current ®ndings are largely 131±149, 1991.
based on cross-sectional studies. Longitudinal studies, 21. ANDERSON, V. and M OORE, C.: Age at injury as a predictor of
mapping developmental trajectories for individual chil- outcome following pediatric head injury. Child Neuropsychology,
1: 187±202, 1995.
dren, may provide additional insights into the pattern of 22. DENNIS, M.: Language and the young damaged brain. In: T. Boll
development of executive skills through childhood. and B. K. Bryant (editors) Clinical neuropsycholog y and brain func-

133
V. Anderson

tion: Research, measurement and practice (Washington: American 45. P ARKER, D. M. and C RAW FORD, J. R.: Assessment of frontal lobe
Psychological Association), pp. 89±123, 1989. dysfunction. In: J. R. Crawford, D. M. Parker and W. W.
23. A NDERSON, V.: Recovery of function in children: the myth of McKinlay (editors) A handboo k of neuropsychologica l assessment
cerebral plasticity. In: M. Matheson and H. Newman (editors) (Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd), pp. 267±294,
Brain Impairment. Proceedings from the Thirteenth Annual Brain 1992.
Impairment Conference, Sydney, pp. 223±247, 1988. 46. W ALSH, K. W.: Understandin g brain damage: A primer of neuro-
24. EW ING-COBBS, L. and FLETCHER, J. M.: Neuropsychological psychologica l evaluation (New York: Churchill Livingston), 1985.
assessment of head injury in children. Journa l of Learning 47. W ELSH, M. C. and P ENNINGTON, B. F.: Assessing frontal lobe
Disabilities, 20: 526±535, 1987. functioning in children: views from developmental psychology.
25. M ATEER, C. A. and W ILLIAMS, D.: EVects of frontal lobe injury in Development Neuropsychology, 4: 199±230, 1988.
childhood. Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 7: 69±86, 1991. 48. ALBERT, M. S. and KAPLAN, E. F.: Organic implications of neuro-
26. A SARNOW , R. F., SATZ , P., LIGHT, R. et al.: Behavior problems psychological de®cits in the elderly. In: L. W. Poon, J. Fozard, L.
and adaptive functioning in children with mild and severe closed Cermak et al. (editors) New directions in memory and ageing:
head injury. Journal of Paediatric Psychology, 16: 543±555, 1991. Proceedings of the George A. Talland Memorial Conference
27. FLETCHER, J. M., EWING-C OBBS, L., M INER, M. E. et al.: (Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.), pp. 403±432,
Behavioral changes after closed head injury in children. Journal 1980.
of Consultin g and Clinical Psychology, 58: 93±98, 1990. 49. BELL, M. A. and FOX, N. A.: The relations between frontal brain
28. P ENTLAND, L., TODD, J. A. and A NDERSON, V.: The impact of electrical activity and cognitive development during infancy. Child
head injury severity on planning ability in adolescence: a func- Development, 63: 1142±1163, 1992.
tional analysis. Neuropsychologica l Rehabilitation, 8: 319±335, 50. THATCHER, R. W.: Maturation of the human frontal lobes.
1998. Physiological evidence for staging. Developmental Neuro-
29. P ERROTT, S. B., TAYLOR, H. G. and M ONTES, J. L.: psychology , 7: 397±419, 1991.
Neuropsychological sequelae, familial stress, and environmental 51. THATCHER, R. W.: Cyclical cortical reorganization during early
adaptation following pediatric head injury. Developmental neuro- childhood. Brain and Cognition, 20: 25±50, 1992.
psychology, 7: 69±86, 1991. 52. GADDES, W. H. and C ROCKETT, D. J.: The Spreen Benton
30. LANDRY, S. H., J ORDAN, T. and FLETCHER, J. M.: Developmental Aphasia Tests: normative data as a measure of normal language
outcomes for children with spina bi®da and hydrocephalus. In: development. Brain and Languag e, 2: 257±279, 1975.
M. G. Tramontana and S. R. Hooper (editors) Advances in child 53. HALPERIN, J. M., HEALEY, J. M., ZEITCHIK, E. et al.: Develop-
neuropsychology, Volume 2 (New York: Springer-Verlag), pp. 85± mental aspects of linguistic and mnestic abilities in normal children.
118, 1994. Journa l of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11: 518±
31. BROUW ERS, P., RICCARDI, R., P OPLACK, D. Attention de®cits in 528, 1989.
long-term survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. 54. M CKAY, K. E., HALPERIN, J. M., SCHWARTZ , S. T. et al.:
Journa l of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6: 325±336, 1984. Developmental analysis of three aspects of information processing:
32. STEHBENS, J. A., KALEITA, T. A., NOLL, R. B. et al. : CNS sustained attention, selective attention, and response organization.
prophylaxis of childhood leukemia: what are the long term Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 10: 121±132, 1994.
neurological, neuropsychological and behavioral eVects? 55. M ILLER, P. H. and W EISS, M. G.: Children’s attentional allocation,
Neuropsychologica l Review, 2: 147±177, 1991. understanding of attention, and performance on the incidental
33. HOLMES-BERNSTEIN, J. and WABER, D. P.: Developmental neuro- learning task. Child Development, 52: 1183±1190, 1981.
psychological assessment: the systemic approach. In: A. A. 56. ANDERSON, V., LAJOIE, G. and BELL, R.: Neuropsychological
Boulton, G. B. Baker and M. Hiscock (editors) Neuromethods: assessment of the school-age d child (Australia: Department of
Volume 17: Neuropsycholog y (Clifton, NJ: The Humana Press Psychology, University of Melbourne), 1995.
Inc), pp. 311±371, 1990. 57. HOWARD, L. and P OLICH, J.: P300 altency and memory span
34. R OURKE, B. P., FISK, J. L. and STRANG, J. D.: Neuropsychological development. Developmental Psychology, 21: 283±289, 1985.
assessment of children: A treatment oriented approach (New York: 58. BADDELEY, A.: Working memory (Oxford: Oxford University
Guilford), 1986. Press), 1986.
35. TAYLOR, H. G. and SCHATSCHNEIDER, C.: Child neuropsycho- 59. C ASE, R.: Intellectual development: Birth to adulthoo d (Orlando,
logical assessment: a test of basic assumptions. The Clinical FL: Academic Press), 1985.
Neuropsychologist, 6: 259±275, 1992. 60. P ARIS, S. G. and LINDAUER, B. K.: The role of inference in chil-
36. LEZ AK, M.: Neuropsychological assessment (New York: Oxford), dren’s comprehension and memory for sentences. Cognitive
1993. Psychology , 8: 217±227, 1976.
37. STUSS, D. T.: Biological and psychological development of execu- 61. SIMON, H. A.: The functional equivalence of problem solving
tive functions. Brain and Cognition, 20: 8±23, 1992. skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7: 268±288, 1975.
38. GLOSSER, G. and GOODGLASS, H.: Disorders in executive control 62. C AESER, P.: Old and new facts about perinatal brain development.
functions among aphasic and other brain damaged patients. Journa l of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34: 101±109, 1993.
Journa l of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12: 485± 63. ORZHEKHOVSKAYA, N. S.: Fronto-striatal relationships in priamte
501, 1990. ontogeny. Neuroscience and Behavioural Physiology, 11: 379±385,
39. LEVIN, B.: Organizational de®cits in dyslexia: possible frontal lobe 1981.
dysfunction. Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 6: 95±110, 1990. 64. YAKOVLEV, P. I.: Morphological criteria of growth and matura-
40. SHALLICE, T.: From neuropsycholog y to mental structure (New tion of the nervous system in man. Research Publications
York: Cambridge University Press), 1990. Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 39: 3±
41. W ALSH, K. W.: Neuropsychology : A clinical approac h (New York: 46, 1962.
Churchill Livingston), 1998. 65. FUSTER, F.: Frontal lobes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3:
42. DUNCAN, J.: Disorganization of behavior after frontal lobe 160±165, 1993.
damage. Cognitive Neuropsycholog y, 3: 271±290, 1986. 66. JERNIGAN, T. L. and TALLAL, P.: Late childhood changes in brain
43. NEISSER, U.: Cognitive psycholog y (New York: Appleton-Century- morphology observable with MRI. Developmental Medicine and
Crofts), 1967. Child Neurology, 32: 379±385, 1990.
44. BENTON, A. L.: DiVerential behavioral eVects of frontal lobe dis- 67. KOLB, B. and FANTIE, B.: Development of the child’s brain and
ease. Neuropsychologia, 6: 53±60, 1968. behaviour. In: C. Reynolds and E. Fletcher-Janzen (editors)

134
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

Handboo k of clinical child neuropsycholog y (New York: Plenum), 89. HYND, G. W., SNOW , J. and BECKER, M. G.:
pp. 17±39, 1989. Neuropsychological assessment in clinical child psychology. In:
68. RISSER, A. H. and EDGELL, D.: Neuropsychology of the develop- B. B. Lahey and A. E. Kazden (editors) Advance in Clinical Child
ing drain: implications for neuropsychological assessment. In: M. Psychology, Volume 9 (New York: Plenum Press), pp. 35±86,
G. Tramontana and S. R. Hooper (editors) Assessment issues in 1986.
child neuropsycholog y (New York: Plenum), pp. 41±60, 1988. 90. O BRZ UT, J. E. and HYND, G. W.: Child neuropsychology , Volume
69. GOLDEN, C. J.: The Luria±Nebraska Children’s Battery: theory 2: Clinical practice (New York: Academic Press), 1986.
and formulation. In: G. W. Hynd and J. E. Obrzut (editors) 91. TRAMONTANA, M. G. and HOOPER, S. R. (editors) Assessment
Neuropsychologia l assessment of the school-age d child (New issues in child neuropsycholog y (New York: Plenum), 1988.
York: Grune & Stratton), pp. 277±302, 1981. 92. TAYLOR, H. G. and FLETCHER, J. M.: Neuropsychological assess-
70. C HUGANI, H. T., P HELPS, M. E. and MAZZIOTTA, J. C.: Positron ment in children. In: G. Goldstein and M. Herson (editors)
emission tomography study of human brain functional develop- Handboo k of psychologica l assessment, 2nd edn (New York:
ment. Annals of Neurology, 22: 287±297, 1987. Pergamon), pp. 228±255, 1990.
71. BARBAS, H.: Architecture and cortical connections of prefrontal 93. TODD, J. A., A NDERSON, V. A. and LAW RENCE, J.: Planning skills
cortex in the rhesus monkey: In: P. Chauvel and A. Delgado- in head injured adolescents and their peers. Neuropsychological
Esceuta (editors) Advances in neurology (New York: Raven Rehabilitation, 6: 81±99, 1996.
Press), pp. 91±115, 1992. 94. GYNS, J. A. and WILLIS, G. W.: Validation of executive function
72. HUDSPETH, W. and P RIBRAM, K.: Stages of brain and cognitive tasks with young children. Developmental Psychology, 7: 487±501,
maturation. Journa l of Educational Psychology, 82: 881±884, 1990. 1991.
73. STAUDT, M., SCHROPP , C., STAUDT, F. et al.: Myelination of the 95. R EADER, M. J., H ARRIS, E. L., SCHUERHOLZ , L. J. et al.:
brain in MRI: a staging system. Pediatric Radiolog y, 23: 169±176, Attention de®cit hyperactivity disorder and executive dysfunc-
1993. tion. Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 10: 493±512, 1994.
74. C AESAR, P. and LAGAE, L.: Age speci®c approach to neurological 96. BARKLEY, R. A. and GRODZ INSKY, G.: Are tests of frontal lobe
assessment in the ®rst year of life. Acta Pediatrica Japonica, 33: functions useful in diagnosis of attention de®cit disorder? The
125±138, 1991. Clinical Neuropsychologis t, 8: 121±139, 1994.
75. UEMURA, E. and H ARTMAN, H. A.: RNA content and volume of 97. W EYANDT, I. L. and W ILLIS, W. G.: Executive functions in
nerve cell bodies in human brain: I. Prefrontal cortex in aging school-aged children: potential e cacy of tasks in discriminating
normal and demented patients. Journal of Neuropatholog y and clinical groups. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10: 27±38, 1994.
Experimental Neurology, 37: 487±496, 1978. 98. GARTH, J., ANDERSON, V. and W RENNALL, J.: Executive func-
76. KENNEDY, C., SAKURADA, O., SHINOHARA, M. et al.: Local cere- tions following moderate to severe frontal lobe injury: impact of
bral glucose utilization in the newborn macaque monkey. Annals injury and age at injury. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 1: 99±108, 1997.
of Neurology, 12: 333±340, 1982. 99. HARRIS, E. L., SCHUERHOLZ , L. H., SINGER, H. S. et al.:
77. RAKIC, P., BOURGEOIS, J-P., ECKENHOFF, et al.: Concurrent over- Executive function in children with Tourette syndrome and/or
production of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral attention de®cit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the Inter-
cortex. Science, 232: 232±235, 1986. national Neuropsychologica l Society, 1: 511±516, 1995.
78. LIDOW , M. and GOLDMAN-RAKIC, P. S.: Synchronised overpro- 100. Army Individual Test Battery: Manual of directions and scoring
duction of neurotransmitter receptors in diverse regions of the (Washington, DC: War Department, Adjunct General’s O ce),
primate cerebral cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of 1944.
Science, 88: 10218±10221, 1991. 101. R EITAN, R. M. and DAVISON, L. A. (editors): Clinical neuropsy-
79. GIBSON, K. R.: Myelination and behaviour development: a chology: Current status and applications (Washington DC:
comparative perspective on questions of neoteny, altriciality, and Winston), 1974.
intelligence. In: K. R. Gibson and A. C. Petersen (editors) Brain 102. R EITAN, R. M.: Manual for administration of neuropsychologica l
maturation and cognitive development: Comparative and cross-cul- test batteries for adults and children (Indianapolis, IN: Author),
tural perspectives (New York: Aldine De Gruter), pp. 29±64, 1991. 1969.
80. P IAGET, J.: The origins of intelligence in children (New York: 103. GOLDEN, C. J.: Manual for the Luria±Nebraska Neuropsycho-
W. W. Norton), 1963. logical Battery: Children’s Revision (Los Angeles, CA: WPS),
81. FLAVELL, J. H.: Cognitive development: past, present, and future. 1986.
Developmental Psychology, 28: 998±1005, 1992. 104. W ECHSLER, D.: Manual for the Wechsler Scale of Children’s
82. DIAMOND, A.: DiVerences between adult and infant cognition: is IntelligenceÐIII (New York: Psychological Corporation), 1991.
the crucial variable presence or absence of language? In: L. 105. W ECHSLER, D.: Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary
Weiskrantz (editor) Thought without language (New York: Scale of IntelligenceÐRevised (New York: Psychological
Oxford University Press), pp. 337±370, 1988. Corporation), 1989.
83. DIAMOND, A. and GOLDMAN-RAKIC, P. S.: Evidence for in- 106. M CC ARTHY, D.: McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (New
volvement of prefront cortex in cognitive changes during the York: Psychological Corporation), 1972.
®rst year of life: comparison of human infants and rhesus mon- 107. KAUFMAN, A. S. and K AUFMAN, N. L.: The Kaufman assessment
keys on a detour task with transparent barrier. Neurosciences battery for children: Interpretive manual (Cirlce Pines, MN:
Abstracts (Pt. II), 11: 832, 1985. American Guidance Services), 1983.
84. BECKER, M. G., ISAAC, W. and H YND, G.: Neuropsychology devel- 108. KOLB, B. and W ISHAW , Q.: Fundamentals of human neuropsy-
opment of non-verbal behaviours attributed to the frontal lobes. chology, 3rd edn (New York: W. H. Freeman), 1990.
Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 3: 275±298, 1987. 109. W ABER, D. P. and HOLMES, J. M.: Assessing children’s copy pro-
85. C HELUNE, G. J. and BAER, R. A.: Developmental norms for the ductions of the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure. Journal of
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journa l of Clinical and Experimental Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7: 264±280, 1985.
Neuropsychology, 8: 219±228, 1986. 110. A NDERSON, P., A NDERSON, V. and G ARTH, J.: A process-oriented
86. BERG, E. A.: A simple objective treatment for measuring ¯exibility approach to scoring the Complex Figure of Rey, The Clinical
in thinking. Journa l of General Psychology, 39: 15±22, 1948. Neuropsychologist (in press).
87. HEATON, R. K.: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Odessa, 111. W ABER, D. P. and BERNSTEIN, J. H.: Performance of learning
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources), 1981. disabled and non-learning disabled children on the Rey-
88. REY, A.: L’examen clinique en psychologi e (Paris: Presses Osterreith Complex Figure. Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 11:
Universitaires de France), 1964. 237±252.

135
Assessing executive function in rehabilitation

112. G RIMW OOD, K., A NDERSON, V. A., BOND, L. et al.: Adverse out- 124. CHELUNE, G. J., FERGUSON, W., K OON, R. et al.: Frontal lobe
comes of bacterial meningitis in school-age survivors. Pediatrics, disinhibition in attention de®cit disorder. Child Psychiatry and
95: 646±656, 1995. Human Development, 16: 221±234, 1986.
113. W ABER, D. P., BERNSTEIN, J. H., KAMMERER, B. L. et al.: 125. STROOP , J. R.: Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction.
Neuropsychological diagnostic pro®les of children who received Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18: 643±662, 1935.
CNS treatment for acute lymphoblastic leukemia: the systemic 126. GOLDEN, C. J.: Stroop Color and Word Test (Chicago, IL:
approach to assessment. Developmental Neuropsycholog y, 8: 1± Stoelting Co.), 1978.
28, 1992. 127. CHADWICK, O., RUTTER, M., SHAFFER, D.: A prospective study
114. P ORTEUS, S. D.: Porteus maze test: Fifty years’ application (New of children with head injuries: IV. Speci®c cognitive de®cits.
Journal of Clinical Neuropsycholog y, 3: 101±120, 1981.
York: Psychological Corporation), 1965.
128. SHALLICE, T. and BURGESS, P.: De®cits in strategy application
115. K RIKORIAN, R., BARTOK, J. and GAY, N.: Tower of London pro-
following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114: 727±741, 1981.
cedure: a standard method and developmental data. Journal of 129. LEVINE, B., STUSS, D. T. and M ILBERG, W. P.: Concept genera-
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsycholog y, 16: 840±850, 1994. tion: validation of a test of executive functioning in a normal
116. LEVIN, H. S., MENDELSOHN, D., LILY, M. A. et al.: Tower of aging population. Journa l of Clinical and Experimental
London performance in relation to magnetic resonance imaging Neuropsychology, 17: 740±758, 1995.
following closed head injury in children. Neuropsychology, 8: 130. DENNIS, M. and BARNES, M. A.: Knowing the meaning, getting
171±179, 1994. the point, bridging the gap, and carrying the message: aspects of
117. BASSETT, S. S. and SLATER, E. J.: Neuropsychological function in discourse following closed head injury in childhood and adoles-
adolescents sustaining mild closed head injury. Journal of cence. Brain and Language, 39: 428±446, 1990.
Paediatric Psychology, 15: 225±236, 1990. 131. HARTLEY, L. and JENSENS, P.: Three discourse pro®les of closed-
118. A NDERSON, V., GODBER, T., S MIBERT, E. et al.: Neurobehav- head-injury speakers: theoretical and clinical implications. Brain
ioural sequelae following cranial irradiation and chemotherapy Injury, 6: 271±282, 1991.
in children: an analysis of risk factors. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 132. HAYES-R OTH, B. and H AYES-ROTH, F.: A cognitive model of
1: 63±76, 1997. planning. Cognitive Science, 3: 275±310, 1979.
119. A NDERSON, V. and STANLEY, G. V.: Ability pro®les of learning 133. CHALMERS, D. and LAW RENCE, J. A.: Investigating the eVects of
disabled children. Australian Psychologis t, 27: 48±51, 1992. planning aids on adults’ and adolescents’ organisation of a com-
120. BEERS, S. R., G OLDSTEIN, G. and KATZ , L. J.: Neuropsy- plex task. Internationa l Journa l of Behavioral Development, 16:
chological diVerences between college students with learning 191±214, 1993.
134. CONDOR, A., A NDERSON, V. and SALING, M.: Do reading dis-
disabilities and those with mild head injury. Journal of Learning
abled children have planning problems? Developmental Neuro-
Disabilities, 27: 315±324, 1994.
psychology , 11: 485±502, 1996.
121. D ENNY, D. R. and D ENNY, N. W.: The use of classi®cation for 135. WANSART, W.: Learning to solve a problem: a microanalysis of
problem solving: a comparison of middle and old age. the solution strategies of children with learning disabilities.
Development Psychology, 9: 275±278, 1973. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23: 164±170, 1990.
122. P ENTLAND, L. and ANDERSON, V.: Attention skills in head injured 136. ANDERSON, V., BOND, L., C ATROPP A, C. et al.: Childhood bac-
adolescents and their peers. Journal of the Internationa l terial meningitis: impact of age at illness and medical compli-
Neuropsychological Society, 1: 330, 1995. cations on long term outcome. Journal of the Internationa l
123. BOUCUGNANI, L. L. and JONES, R. W.: Behaviors analogous to Neuropsychological Society, 3: 147±158, 1997.
frontal lobe dysfunction in children with attention de®cit disor- 137. PIAGET, J.: The construction of reality in the child (New York:
der. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 4: 161±173, 1989. Basic Books), 1954.

136
Copyright of Pediatric Rehabilitation is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied
or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like