You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Sports Sciences

ISSN: 0264-0414 (Print) 1466-447X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific decision-


making skill

Mitchell R. Smith, Linus Zeuwts, Matthieu Lenoir, Nathalie Hens, Laura M. S.


De Jong & Aaron J. Coutts

To cite this article: Mitchell R. Smith, Linus Zeuwts, Matthieu Lenoir, Nathalie Hens, Laura M. S.
De Jong & Aaron J. Coutts (2016): Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific decision-making skill,
Journal of Sports Sciences, DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1156241

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1156241

Published online: 07 Mar 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rjsp20

Download by: [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] Date: 08 March 2016, At: 08:54
JOURNAL OF SPORTS SCIENCES, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1156241

ARTICLE

Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific decision-making skill


Mitchell R. Smitha,b, Linus Zeuwts b
, Matthieu Lenoirb, Nathalie Hensb, Laura M. S. De Jongc and Aaron J. Couttsa
a
Sport and Exercise Discipline Group, Faculty of Health, University of Technology Sydney (UTS) Sydney Cricket Ground, Moore Park, New South
Wales, Australia; bDepartment of Movement and Sports Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium;
c
Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


This study aimed to investigate the impact of mental fatigue on soccer-specific decision-making. Twelve Accepted 15 February 2016
well-trained male soccer players performed a soccer-specific decision-making task on two occasions,
KEYWORDS
separated by at least 72 h. The decision-making task was preceded in a randomised order by 30 min of Football; cognitive fatigue;
the Stroop task (mental fatigue) or 30 min of reading from magazines (control). Subjective ratings of team sport; visual search
mental fatigue were measured before and after treatment, and mental effort (referring to treatment)
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

and motivation (referring to the decision-making task) were measured after treatment. Performance on
the soccer-specific decision-making task was assessed using response accuracy and time. Visual search
behaviour was also assessed throughout the decision-making task. Subjective ratings of mental fatigue
and effort were almost certainly higher following the Stroop task compared to the magazines.
Motivation for the upcoming decision-making task was possibly higher following the Stroop task.
Decision-making accuracy was very likely lower and response time likely higher in the mental fatigue
condition. Mental fatigue had unclear effects on most visual search behaviour variables. The results
suggest that mental fatigue impairs accuracy and speed of soccer-specific decision-making. These
impairments are not likely related to changes in visual search behaviour.

Introduction (Boksem et al., 2006), and identify and respond to meaningful


cues in the visual search field of use for action preparation
Soccer match play demands frequent bouts of high-intensity
(Boksem et al., 2006; Lorist et al., 2000). Therefore, mental fatigue
activity, intermittently distributed among prolonged periods
may also impair soccer-specific decision-making, which relies on
of low-intensity activity (Mohr, Krustrup, & Bangsbo, 2005).
these cognitive abilities (Afonso, Garganta, & Mesquita, 2012).
This physically fatiguing activity profile contributes to reduc-
Previous research has shown that experienced soccer
tions in performance observed towards the end of a match
players make faster, more accurate decisions than their less-
(Rampinini, Coutts, Castagna, Sassi, & Impellizzeri, 2007;
experienced counterparts (Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams, Mazyn, &
Rampinini et al., 2008) and in subsequent matches (Mohr
Philippaerts, 2007). These discrepancies are partly determined
et al., 2005; Nédélec et al., 2012). Soccer is also cognitively
by differences in visual search behaviour between players of
demanding as it requires sustained attention to make quick,
different experience levels (Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams, Mazyn,
accurate decisions based on the retrieval and processing of
et al., 2007; Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams, & Philippaerts, 2007).
information from a dynamic environment (Nédélec et al., 2012;
Therefore, it is important to assess the impact of mental
Walsh, 2014). These demands suggest that soccer may also be
fatigue on decision-making skill and visual search behaviour
mentally fatiguing, with reductions in performance resulting
as these factors appear to be important contributors to suc-
from a combination of physical and mental fatigue. Indeed,
cessful soccer performance.
recent research has shown that mental fatigue impairs soccer-
To date however, no research has examined the effects of
specific physical and technical performance (Smith et al., 2016;
mental fatigue on soccer-specific decision-making. Previously,
Smith, Marcora, & Coutts, 2015).
Laborde and Raab (2013) revealed that both positive and
Mental fatigue is a psychobiological state induced by sustained
negative mood states impaired team handball players’ deci-
periods of demanding cognitive activity and characterised by
sion-making skill compared to a neutral mood control group.
feelings of tiredness and lack of energy (Boksem & Tops, 2008;
Additionally, Hockey, John Maule, Clough, and Bdzola (2000)
Marcora, Staiano, & Manning, 2009). The negative impact of men-
revealed a fatigue-related increase in riskiness of decisions in
tal fatigue on cognitive (Boksem, Meijman, & Lorist, 2005, 2006)
hypothetical, everyday situations. Together, these investiga-
and motor (Duncan, Fowler, George, Joyce, & Hankey, 2015; Lal &
tions indicate that mood can affect executive functioning,
Craig, 2001) performances is well established, and may be attribu-
justifying further research into the impact of psychological
table to an impaired capability to maintain attentional focus
manipulations on sport-specific decision-making.
(Boksem et al., 2005), monitor and adjust performance (Lorist,
The purpose of this investigation was to examine the effects
Boksem, & Ridderinkhof, 2005), react quickly and accurately
of induced mental fatigue on soccer-specific decision-making

CONTACT Mitchell Robert Smith Mitchell.Smith@uts.edu.au


© 2016 Taylor & Francis
2 M. R. SMITH ET AL.

skill. It was hypothesised that mental fatigue would impair participants were required to verbally respond to each word.
players’ decision-making speed and accuracy, and alter visual The correct response corresponded to the ink colour of the word
search patterns. (red, blue, green, yellow), rather than the words meaning.
However, if the ink colour of the word was red, the correct
response corresponded to the meaning of the word, rather
Methods than its printed colour. Verbal responses were monitored by a
Participants member of the research team who sat opposite the participants
throughout treatment. To increase motivation for the task, a
Twelve male soccer players (age: 19.3 ± 1.5 y; position: stri- competitive environment was created, whereby participants
ker = 1, central midfield = 8, central defence = 3) participated were challenged to successfully complete more words than
in this randomised, counter-balanced crossover investigation. other participants within the 30-min treatment period.
All players were currently competing in Belgian national (divi- The control treatment involved 30 min of reading from a
sions 3–4) or provincial (divisions 2–4) competitions and had selection of magazines. The magazines varied in theme, includ-
been playing soccer for at least 10 years (13.1 ± 2.6 y). ing sport, cars and travel, and were identified by pilot testing as
Participants signed an informed consent form outlining poten- emotionally neutral. Participants completed both treatments in
tial risks and describing the studies procedures which were the same room, under the supervision of the same researcher.
approved by the Ghent University Hospital Ethics Committee
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

(Project #: 2002052) and conformed to the Declaration of


Helsinki. Participants were provided with information outlining Subjective ratings
the study’s procedures but were not informed of its aims. Subjective ratings of mental fatigue, mental effort and motiva-
tion were recorded using the same 100-mm VAS as used in
Experimental overview previous research investigating the impact of mental fatigue
on soccer-specific performance (Smith et al., 2016). The scales
Participants completed one familiarisation, plus two testing were anchored at one end with “none at all” and at the other
sessions (control and mental fatigue). The familiarisation ses- end with “maximal”. Mental fatigue was measured pre- and
sion involved an explanation of all testing procedures and post-treatment as a manipulation check, and also after the
equipment. Participants were also provided instructions to decision-making task. Mental effort was assessed following
sleep for eight hours, refrain from consumption of alcohol treatment and the decision-making task, while motivation
and avoid vigorous exercise in the 24 h before each of the was only measured post-treatment.
testing sessions; avoid caffeine and demanding mental activity
the day of testing; and consume a reasonable meal approxi-
mately 1.5 h before the upcoming testing sessions which were Decision-making task
separated by at least 72 h. This study utilised the soccer-specific decision-making task pre-
Upon arrival for testing, participants completed a pre-test viously developed and validated by Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams,
checklist, assessing compliance with instructions, as well as a Mazyn, et al. (2007). This task required participants to observe
visual analogue scale (VAS) for the assessment of mental film-based simulations of offensive soccer play, projected onto a
fatigue. A standardised explanation of the decision-making 4.3 × 2.5-m screen positioned 4.4 m in front of the participant.
task was then displayed on a projector screen to reinforce Participants were required to imagine themselves as an offensive
familiarity for the upcoming test. Participants then walked to midfielder, playing in a central position, who were easily identifi-
an adjacent room where the control or mental fatigue treat- able on screen, as they wore a yellow vest over their playing
ment was administered. Immediately following treatment, par- jersey. Sequences of play varied in number, and position of
ticipants returned to the testing room where they completed players on screen (2 vs. 1, 3 vs. 1, 3 vs. 2, 4 vs. 3 and 5 vs. 3), as
VAS for subjective assessment of mental fatigue, mental effort well as in duration, ranging from 3.0 s to 9.7 s. Each sequence
and motivation. Participants were then fitted with a modified ended with a pass to the yellow player and film was occluded as
baseball cap housing the eye-tracking system. the yellow player received the ball.
Following calibration, participants completed four practice As the ball was passed to the yellow player on screen,
trials of the soccer-specific decision-making task, followed by participants were required to make quick and accurate decisions
33 testing trials, with an inter-trial interval of approximately by taking the appropriate action with the ball positioned in front
45 s. The calibration of the eye-tracking system was checked of them. In each sequence, participants had the option of pas-
following the four practice trials and after every 11 test trials. sing to any player on screen, shooting towards goal or dribbling
Participants again provided subjective ratings of mental fati- the ball “around a defender”. Participants were then required to
gue and mental effort following the decision-making task, verbalise their response to ensure that they had carried out the
which lasted approximately 35–40 min. intended action. Task performance was assessed using response
accuracy, response time and visual search data.
Treatment
Response accuracy and time
Mental fatigue was induced using a 30-min paper version of a
modified Stroop colour–word task. Four words (red, blue, green, Response accuracy was based on a scoring system from zero
yellow) were repeatedly printed in a random order, and to three points for each sequence. Therefore, accuracy was
JOURNAL OF SPORTS SCIENCES 3

calculated as the percentage of points awarded out of a transformed when non-normally distributed. A contemporary
possible 99 points. Scores for each response were allocated magnitude-based approach was used to assess chances of
by a panel of coaches according to the following criteria: differences between experimental conditions being trivial or
true (greater than the smallest worthwhile change; 0.2 × the
● 3 points = The most goal-oriented action. between-subjects SD) (Hopkins, Marshall, Batterham, & Hanin,
● 2 points = An action “indirectly” leading to a goal-scor- 2009). Chances of true difference were qualitatively assessed
ing opportunity. as <0.5%, almost certainly not; <5%, very unlikely; <25%,
● 1 point = Maintenance of ball possession, not leading to unlikely; 25‒75%, possibly; >75%, likely; >95%, very likely
a goal scoring opportunity. and >99.5%, almost certainly. If the chances of a variable
● 0 points = Poor decision leading to loss of possession. having higher and lower differences were both >5%, the effect
was considered unclear. Effect sizes (ES) were calculated to
During each clip, participants stood on pressure-sensitive assess the magnitude of difference between groups and are
switches which were used to measure response time, defined reported ± 90% confidence limits. ES of <0.2, 0.2–0.6, 0.6–1.2,
as the time (in ms) from the start of the pass towards the 1.2–2.0 and >2.0 were considered trivial, small, moderate,
yellow player until the participant raised a foot off the pres- large and very large, respectively. If the 90% confidence inter-
sure switch to play the ball. val of the ES was greater than ± 0.2 in both directions, the
effect was considered unclear.
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

Visual search
Visual search data were recorded using an IviewX Head
mounted Eye-tracking Device (SMI, Teltow, GER). Participants
Results
wore a modified baseball cap, housing an infra-red eye cam- Subjective ratings
era, which recorded left eye movements at 50 Hz using pupil
Subjective ratings of mental fatigue almost certainly increased
position and corneal reflex. A second camera recorded images
more following the Stroop task, than the control treatment
of the scene at 25 Hz. Both cameras were connected via USB
(ES = 1.23 ± 0.26; Figure 1). Follow-up analysis revealed no
cable to a notebook which was placed in a small backpack
clear difference in subjective ratings of mental fatigue pre-
worn by the participants. Data from both cameras were com-
treatment, but an almost certainly higher rating following the
bined using BeGaze 3.0 software (SMI, Teltow, GER), which has
Stroop task (ES = 1.50 ± 0.38). Furthermore, changes from pre-
a spatial accuracy of 1°. Visual search data was analysed frame
to post-treatment were likely trivial following the control
by frame using video analysis software (Kinovea open source
treatment (ES = 0.08 ± 0.24) and almost certainly increased
project, www.kinovea.org). Technical and calibration issues
following the Stroop task (ES = 1.85 ± 0.43). Subjective ratings
resulted in poor tracking of gaze behaviour during some test-
of mental fatigue very likely increased more following the
ing sessions. Therefore, visual search data are presented for
decision-making task in the control condition
eight participants.
(ES = −1.03 ± 0.53); however, no clear differences between
Search rate was assessed using the mean number of visual
conditions existed following the decision-making task
fixations per second (n · s−1), and mean fixation duration. A
(ES = 0.19 ± 0.51). Subjective ratings of mental fatigue possibly
fixation was defined as a period where the eye remained on
increased following the decision-making task in the control
one area of interest for equal to or greater than 120 ms. The
condition (ES = 0.29 ± 0.60), but very likely decreased in the
percentage of total viewing time (per trial) spent in fixation
mental fatigue condition (ES = −1.10 ± 0.76).
(fixation %) was also calculated, and further broken down into
measurement of fixation % for different locations. Fixations
were allocated to one of nine areas: yellow player (YP); player
with ball (PB); ball (B); unmarked attacker (A); closely marked
attacker (A/D); defender (D); goalkeeper (K); free space (FS)
and an unspecified category for fixations that could not be
categorised under the aforementioned locations (U).
Fixation order refers to the sequence of searching utilised
by the participants. As in previous investigations (Vaeyens,
Lenoir, Williams, Mazyn, et al., 2007; Williams, Davids,
Burwitz, & Williams, 1994), fixation order was measured as
the number of times per second (n · s−1) that participants
alternated their gaze between the player with the ball,
another area of the display and then back to the player with
the ball.

Statistical analysis Figure 1. Subjective ratings of mental fatigue.


Data presented as mean ± SD. ↑ increase, ↓ decrease, * possible, *** very likely,
Data are presented as mean ± SD unless otherwise stated. **** almost certain change; ### very likely, #### almost certain difference
Prior to analysis, data were tested for normality and log- between conditions.
4 M. R. SMITH ET AL.

Post-treatment ratings of mental effort were almost cer- Overall response time was likely slower in the mental fati-
tainly higher following the Stoop task (79.5 ± 13.3 AU) than gue condition (768 ± 134 ms) compared to the control condi-
the control treatment (28.8 ± 15.0 AU; ES = 3.13 ± 0.75), and tion (685 ± 156 ms; ES = 0.49 ± 0.47). Response time was very
possibly trivial or possibly higher following the decision-mak- likely slower for 2 vs. 1 formations (ES = 1.37 ± 1.03), possibly
ing task in the mental fatigue condition (53.7 ± 27.5 AU) than slower for 3 vs. 1 formations (ES = 0.25 ± 0.43) and likely lower
the control condition (47.7 ± 25.8 AU; ES = 0.22 ± 0.47). for 3 vs. 2 (ES = 0.40 ± 0.57), 4 vs. 3 (ES = 0.57 ± 0.54) and 5 vs.
Differences in motivation to perform the decision-making 3 formations (ES = 0.34 ± 0.40; Figure 2B).
task were possibly trivial or possibly higher in the mental
fatigue condition (79.0 ± 18.6 AU) than the control condition
(74.5 ± 12.6 AU; ES = 0.33 ± 0.52). Visual search
Overall, no clear differences existed between conditions for
mean number of fixations per second (control: 2.07 ± 0.25
Response accuracy and time
n · s−1; mental fatigue: 2.09 ± 0.30 n · s−1; ES = 0.41 ± 0.79),
Overall response accuracy was very likely lower in the mental or mean fixation duration (control: 447 ± 58 ms; mental fati-
fatigue condition (80.9 ± 6.4%) compared to the control condi- gue: 450 ± 81 ms; ES = 0.05 ± 0.54). However, for 2 vs. 1
tion (85.7 ± 4.9%; ES = −0.89 ± 0.73). Response accuracy was formations, mean number of fixations per second were likely
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

likely lower for 3 vs. 1 formations (ES = −0.49 ± 0.87), possibly lower (ES = −0.38 ± 0.41; Figure 2C), and mean fixation dura-
lower for 3 vs. 2 formations (ES = −0.37 ± 0.71) and very likely tion was likely higher (ES = 0.38 ± 0.34; Figure 2D) in the
lower for 5 vs. 3 formations (ES = −1.00 ± 0.96; Figure 2A) in the mental fatigue condition. No clear differences in fixations per
mental fatigue condition. No clear differences in response accu- second or fixation duration existed between conditions for any
racy existed between conditions for 2 vs. 1 or 4 vs. 3 formations. other formations.

Figure 2. Effects of mental fatigue on decision-making accuracy (A), response time (B), fixations/s (C), fixation duration (D), fixation % (E) and fixation order (F).
Data presented as mean ± SD. # possible, ## likely, ### very likely difference between conditions.
JOURNAL OF SPORTS SCIENCES 5

Overall fixation % was likely lower in the mental fatigue Follow-up analysis revealed that percentage of time fixat-
condition (90.1 ± 2.8%) compared to the control condition ing on D was possibly lower in the mental fatigue condition
(91.3 ± 1.3%; ES = −0.81 ± 1.00). For 3 vs. 2 formations, fixation for 2 vs. 1 formations (ES = −0.38 ± 0.53; Figure 3B), but likely
% was very likely lower in the mental fatigue condition higher for 3 vs. 1 (ES = 0.83 ± 0.64; Figure 3C) and 3 vs. 2
(ES = −0.94 ± 0.73); however, no clear differences existed for (ES = 0.73 ± 0.88; Figure 3D) formations, and possibly higher
other formations (Figure 2E). Differences in fixation order for 5 vs. 3 formations (ES = 0.26 ± 0.43; Figure 3F).
between conditions were possibly trivial or possibly lower in Furthermore, for 3 vs. 1 formations, percentage of time fixat-
the mental fatigue condition (0.17 ± 0.11 n · s−1) compared to ing on A was possibly lower (ES = −0.38 ± 0.59), and percen-
the control condition (0.17 ± 0.08 n · s−1; ES = −0.19 ± 0.33). tage of time fixating on FS was likely lower (ES = −0.41 ± 0.48)
No clear differences in fixation order existed between condi- in the mental fatigue condition (Figure 3C).
tions for any specific formations (Figure 2F).
No clear differences between conditions existed for over-
all percentage of time fixating on YP, A/D, K or U Discussion
(Figure 3A). Differences in overall percentage of time fixat-
The aim of the current investigation was to determine the
ing on PB and FS were possibly trivial or possibly lower in
impact of mental fatigue on soccer-specific decision-making,
the mental fatigue condition (PB: ES = −0.16 ± 0.30; FS:
and gaze behaviour. In agreement with our primary hypoth-
ES = −0.16 ± 0.19). Overall percentage of time fixating on
esis, mental fatigue impaired both speed and accuracy of
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

D was likely higher in the mental fatigue condition


soccer-specific decision-making. However, only minor differ-
(ES = 0.67 ± 0.71), while differences for B were possibly
ences in gaze behaviour were observed between conditions.
trivial or possibly higher in the mental fatigue condition
Similar to previous investigations (Rozand, Lebon,
(ES = 0.29 ± 0.24).
Papaxanthis, & Lepers, 2015; Smith et al., 2016), subjective

Figure 3. Effects of mental fatigue on fixation location in all (A), 2 vs. 1 (B), 3 vs. 1 (C), 3 vs. 2 (D), 4 vs. 3 (E) and 5 vs. 3 (F) formations.
Data presented as mean ± SD. # possible, ## likely, ### very likely difference between conditions.
6 M. R. SMITH ET AL.

ratings of mental fatigue almost certainly increased following in their decision-making, leading to slower and less accurate
the Stroop task, yet remained stable following the control. This responses.
is likely due to the almost certainly higher mental effort In contrast to our hypothesis, mental fatigue had minimal
required by the Stroop task. impact on visual search behaviour during the soccer-specific
Again in line with previous research (Brownsberger, decision-making task. Between-condition differences in search
Edwards, Crowther, & Cottrell, 2013; Marcora et al., 2009; rate were observed only in 2 vs. 1 formations and percentage
Pageaux, Marcora, Rozand, & Lepers, 2015; Smith et al., of time fixating was different only in 3 vs. 2 formations.
2016), participants in the current investigation reported simi- Furthermore, while overall fixation order was possibly lower
lar levels of motivation for the soccer-specific decision-mak- in the mental fatigue condition, the size of the effect was
ing task, following both treatments. These findings appear unclear, and no true between-condition differences existed
to contradict the well-established inverse relationship for specific formations. However, when mentally fatigued,
between mental fatigue and motivation (Boksem & Tops, players appeared to spend a higher percentage of time fixat-
2008), and reward-based theories of cognitive control ing on defensive players and a lower percentage fixating on
(Botvinick & Braver, 2015; Hagger, Wood, Stiff, & offensive players (Figure 3A).
Chatzisarantis, 2010). However, Inzlicht, Schmeichel, and Previous research (Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams, & Philippaerts,
Macrae (2014) provide a plausible explanation for the similar 2007) identified similar results, with successful decision-
levels of motivation, suggesting that acts of self-control makers fixating more on the player with the ball and attacking
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

(such as the Stroop task) produce lower motivation for players, and less time fixating on the ball and defensive
“have-to” tasks, and higher motivation for “want-to” tasks. players. In contrast to these previous results, in the current
This may have resulted in reduced motivation to perform investigation there were very few between-condition differ-
the Stroop task, but a possibly higher motivation to perform ences for search rate, fixation percentage and fixation order.
the soccer-specific decision-making task. Nevertheless, moti- This may be due to the limited sample size of the current
vational differences cannot explain any performance differ- investigation. Nevertheless, it is difficult to determine whether
ences in the current investigation as participants were highly these small changes in visual search behaviour were respon-
motivated to perform the soccer-specific decision-making sible for the observed decline in decision-making performance
task in both conditions. in mentally fatigued players.
The primary finding of the current investigation was that In soccer, successful decision-making also relies on a
mental fatigue impaired accuracy and speed of soccer-specific player’s capability to identify, select and then programme
decision-making. This impaired decision-making skill in men- the correct action in response to opposition or teammate
tally fatigued athletes is not surprising, as previous investiga- postural cues, recognise meaningful patterns in play and
tions have revealed the negative impact of mental fatigue on determine situational probabilities (Roca, Ford, McRobert, &
reaction time and accuracy in cognitive tasks (Boksem et al., Williams, 2013; Williams, Ford, Eccles, & Ward, 2011). In the
2005; Lorist et al., 2005), and the negative impact of mood current investigation, it is possible that despite minimal differ-
manipulation on decision-making skill in team handball ences in visual search behaviour, players’ information proces-
players (Laborde & Raab, 2013). sing was less effective, resulting in impaired decision-making
One potential explanation for the mental-fatigue-induced skill. Indeed, mental fatigue has been shown to impair execu-
impairment in decision-making skill is that players may have tive functioning and information-processing during cognitive
been unable to effectively utilise environmental cues to pre- tasks (Meijman, 1997; Van der Linden, Frese, & Meijman, 2003),
pare for their upcoming decisions. Indeed, previous investiga- which may partly be due to the effects of mental fatigue on
tions using computer-based cognitive tasks have revealed that attention.
mental fatigue negatively impacts preparation and planning Mental fatigue has been associated with changes in atten-
processes (Boksem et al., 2006; Lorist et al., 2000). In a soccer- tional focus from goal-directed to stimulus-driven attention
specific context, this may manifest as a reduced capability to (Ackerman, 2011; Boksem et al., 2005), which could lead to
interpret and/or anticipate the movement of players and the inattentional blindness, whereby observers fail to notice
ball. Future research utilising electroencephalography may important cues such as unmarked teammates (Memmert &
provide further insight on this hypothesis, as specific changes Furley, 2007). Therefore, fatigue-related impairments to deci-
in brain activity are associated with preparatory and anticipa- sion-making in the current investigation may partly be
tory processes (Boksem et al., 2006). explained by ineffective information-processing and changes
Decision-making ability of team sport athletes is affected in attentional focus, rather than altered visual search patterns.
by preferences for intuition and deliberation. Raab and Indeed, mentally fatigued participants in a previous examina-
Laborde (2011) found that team handball players who pre- tion did not report difficulties with visual perception, but did
ferred intuitive decision-making made faster and better deci- report more attentional difficulties (Van der Linden & Eling,
sions than those who preferred deliberative decision-making. 2006). As previously suggested (Smith et al., 2016), these
Indeed, when athletes are under time pressure to choose from potential changes in attentional focus may be more significant
multiple plausible options, taking the first option often results in ecological settings such as full-sized or small-sided games,
in the fastest and most accurate decision (Laborde & Raab, where the number of irrelevant stimuli is greater than that of a
2013; Raab & Laborde, 2011). Although decision-making pre- controlled setting.
ferences were not measured in the current investigation, it is Although the players in the current investigation were well-
possible that mentally fatigued players were more deliberative trained, it is unknown whether similar results would be
JOURNAL OF SPORTS SCIENCES 7

observed in elite players. Indeed, considering their familiarity Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental
with highly demanding cognitive activity during competition fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107–
116. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011
(Walsh, 2014), it is possible that elite players would be less Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2006). Mental fatigue,
susceptible to the mental fatigue induced by the Stroop task. motivation and action monitoring. Biological Psychology, 72(2), 123–
However, it is also possible that the higher importance of elite 132. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.007
competition places greater cognitive demands on elite players Boksem, M. A. S., & Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits.
as they regulate emotions prior to a match. Therefore, elite Brain Research Reviews, 59(1), 125–139. doi:10.1016/j.
brainresrev.2008.07.001
players may experience mental fatigue during competition, Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: From
and therefore may be affected by the negative impact of behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 83–
mental fatigue on soccer-specific decision-making skill 113. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015044
observed in this investigation. Brownsberger, J., Edwards, A., Crowther, R., & Cottrell, D. (2013). Impact of
While the current investigation clearly reveals the negative mental fatigue on self-paced exercise. International Journal of Sports
Medicine, 34(12), 1029–1036. doi:10.1055/s-00000028
impact of mental fatigue on accuracy and speed of soccer- Duncan, M. J., Fowler, N., George, O., Joyce, S., & Hankey, J. (2015).
specific decision-making, some limitations should be acknowl- Mental fatigue negatively influences manual dexterity and anticipa-
edged. Due to technical difficulties, visual search behaviour tion timing but not repeated high-intensity exercise performance in
was only assessed in eight participants. It is possible that trained adults. Research in Sports Medicine, 23(1), 1–13. doi:10.1080/
additional data may have provided a clearer assessment of 15438627.2014.975811
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2010). Ego deple-
visual search patterns and potential mechanisms underlying tion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis.
performance changes. However, this limitation does not Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 495–525. doi:10.1037/a0019486
negate the substantial impairments to soccer-specific deci- Hockey, R. J., John Maule, A., Clough, P. J., & Bdzola, L. (2000). Effects of
sion-making induced by mental fatigue. Furthermore, future negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition
research should take a more practical approach to assessing and Emotion, 14(6), 823–855. doi:10.1080/02699930050156654
Hopkins, W., Marshall, S., Batterham, A., & Hanin, J. (2009). Progressive
the impact of mental fatigue on soccer performance. Indeed, statistics for studies in sports medicine and exercise science. Medicine &
although anecdotal reports exist, it is unknown whether Science in Sports & Exercise, 41(1), 3–13. doi:10.1249/
match play or common pre-match activities induce mental MSS.0b013e31818cb278
fatigue. If so, players and staff should reassess pre-match Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control
routines and implement strategies that may attenuate the seems (but may not be) limited. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3),
127–133. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009
negative impact of mental fatigue on soccer performance. Laborde, S., & Raab, M. (2013). The tale of hearts and reason: The influence
of mood on decision making. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
35(4), 339–357.
Conclusions Lal, S. K. L., & Craig, A. (2001). A critical review of the psychophysiology of
driver fatigue. Biological Psychology, 55(3), 173–194. doi:10.1016/S0301-
This investigation demonstrates that mental fatigue impairs 0511(00)00085-5
both accuracy and speed of soccer-specific decision-making Lorist, M. M., Boksem, M. A. S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Impaired
skill. It is unlikely that these impairments are related to cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue.
changes in visual search behaviour. Alternatively, it is possible Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 199–205. doi:10.1016/j.
that mental fatigue impaired players’ capability to utilise envir- cogbrainres.2005.01.018
Lorist, M. M., Klein, M., Nieuwenhuis, S., Jong, R., Mulder, G., & Meijman, T.
onmental cues and/or resulted in changes to attention and F. (2000). Mental fatigue and task control: Planning and preparation.
decision-making strategies. Nevertheless, the results of the Psychophysiology, 37(5), 614–625. doi:10.1111/psyp.2000.37.issue-5
current investigation have significant implications for soccer, Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., & Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs
where the capability to make quick and accurate decisions is physical performance in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology, 106(3),
critical for success. 857–864. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008
Meijman, T. F. (1997). Mental fatigue and the efficiency of information
processing in relation to work times. International Journal of Industrial
Ergonomics, 20(1), 31–38. doi:10.1016/S0169-8141(96)00029-7
Disclosure statement
Memmert, D., & Furley, P. (2007). “I Spy With My Little Eye!”: Breadth of
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. attention, inattentional blindness, and tactical decision making in team
sports. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29(3), 365.
Mohr, M., Krustrup, P., & Bangsbo, J. (2005). Fatigue in soccer: A brief
review. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23(6), 593–599. doi:10.1080/
ORCID
02640410400021286
Linus Zeuwts http://orcid.org/0000-0003-2911-6085 Nédélec, M., McCall, A., Carling, C., Legall, F., Berthoin, S., & Dupont, G.
(2012). Recovery in soccer: Part I-post-match fatigue and time course of
recovery. Sports Medicine, 42(12), 997–1015.
References Pageaux, B., Marcora, S. M., Rozand, V., & Lepers, R. (2015). Mental
fatigue induced by prolonged self-regulation does not exacerbate
Ackerman, P. L. (2011). 100 years without resting. In P. L. Ackerman (Ed.), central fatigue during subsequent whole-body endurance exercise.
Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(67), 1–12. doi:10.3389/
future applications (pp. 11–43). Washington, DC: American fnhum.2015.00067
Psychological Association. Raab, M., & Laborde, S. (2011). When to blink and when to think:
Afonso, J., Garganta, J., & Mesquita, I. (2012). Decision-making in sports: Preference for intuitive decisions results in faster and better tactical
The role of attention, anticipation and memory. Revista Brasileira de choices. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82(1), 89–98.
Cineantropometria & Desempenho Humano, 14(5), 592–601. doi:10.1080/02701367.2011.10599725
8 M. R. SMITH ET AL.

Rampinini, E., Coutts, A. J., Castagna, C., Sassi, R., & Impellizzeri, F. M. (2007). decision-making skill in youth soccer players. Journal of Sport and
Variation in top level soccer match performance. International Journal of Exercise Psychology, 29(2), 147.
Sports Medicine, 28(12), 1018–1024. doi:10.1055/s-2007-965158 Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A. M., & Philippaerts, R. M. (2007).
Rampinini, E., Impellizzeri, F. M., Castagna, C., Azzalin, A., Bravo, D. F., & Mechanisms underpinning successful decision making in skilled youth
Wisloff, U. (2008). Effect of match-related fatigue on short-passing soccer players: An analysis of visual search behaviors. Journal of Motor
ability in young soccer players. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, Behavior, 39(5), 395–408. doi:10.3200/JMBR.39.5.395-408
40(5), 934–942. doi:10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181666eb8 Van Der Linden, D., & Eling, P. (2006). Mental fatigue disturbs local proces-
Roca, A., Ford, P. R., McRobert, A. P., & Williams, A. M. (2013). sing more than global processing. Psychological Research, 70(5), 395–
Perceptual-cognitive skills and their interaction as a function of 402. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0228-7
task constraints in soccer. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, Van Der Linden, D., Frese, M., & Meijman, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and
35(2), 144–155. the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and plan-
Rozand, V., Lebon, F., Papaxanthis, C., & Lepers, R. (2015). Effect of mental ning. Acta Psychologica, 113(1), 45–65. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(02)
fatigue on speed-accuracy trade-off. Neuroscience, 297, 219–230. 00150-6
doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.03.066 Walsh, V. (2014). Is sport the brain’s biggest challenge? Current Biology, 24
Smith, M. R., Coutts, A. J., Merlini, M., Deprez, D., Lenoir, M., & Marcora, S. (18), R859–R860. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.003
M. (2016). Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific physical and technical Williams, A. M., Davids, K., Burwitz, L., & Williams, J. G. (1994). Visual search
performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 48(2), 267–276. strategies in experienced and inexperienced soccer players. Research
doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000000762 Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65(2), 127–135. doi:10.1080/
Smith, M. R., Marcora, S. M., & Coutts, A. J. (2015). Mental fatigue impairs 02701367.1994.10607607
intermittent running performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Williams, A. M., Ford, P. R., Eccles, D. W., & Ward, P. (2011). Perceptual-
Downloaded by [Orta Dogu Teknik Universitesi] at 08:54 08 March 2016

Exercise, 47(8), 1682–1690. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000000592 cognitive expertise in sport and its acquisition: Implications for applied
Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A. M., Mazyn, L., & Philippaerts, R. M. cognitive psychology. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(3), 432–442.
(2007). The effects of task constraints on visual search behavior and doi:10.1002/acp.1710

You might also like