You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Vol .25, No. 2, 1997, pp.

133-144

Language Abilities in Children with Attention Deficit


Hyperactivity Disorder, Reading Disabilities, and
Normal Controls

Karen L. Purvis1 and Rosemary Tannock2'3


Received August 22, 1995; revision received December 20, 1995;
accepted March 4, 1996

Research has demonstrated a high prevalence of language impairments (LI) and reading
disabilities (RD) in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Since RD
is also associated with LI, it is unclear whether the language impairments are specific to
ADHD or associated with comorbid RD. The language abilities of ADHD children with and
without RD were investigated in a task requiring recall of a lengthy narrative, and in tests
assessing knowledge of the semantic aspects of language. The study was conducted with 50
boys—14 ADHD, 14 ADHD + RD, 8 RD, and 14 normal controls, aged 7 to 11. Children
with ADHD (ADHD-only, ADHD + RD) exhibited difficulties in organizing and monitoring
their story retelling. Children with RD (RD-only, ADHD + RD) demonstrated deficits in
receptive and expressive semantic language abilities on the language processing tests. The
comorbid group (ADHD + RD) exhibited the deficits of both ADHD and RD children.
The deficiencies of ADHD children are consistent with higher-order executive function defi-
cits while the deficits of RD children are consistent with deficits in the basic semantics of
language processing.
KEY WORDS: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; reading disorder; language abilities; narratives.

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) communication/language disorders (Baker &


is characterized by persistent and developmentally in- Cantwell, 1992; Beitchman, Tuckett, & Batth, 1987;
appropriate levels of inattention, impulsivity, and hy- Cohen, Davine, Horodezky, Lipsett, & Isaacson,
peractivity (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). 1993; Love & Thompson, 1988; Tannock & Schachar,
However, ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder which 1996).
exhibits marked overlap with other disorders (see Bied- Many of the studies of the language abilities of
erman, Newcorn, & Sprich, 1991 for review). One class children with ADHD have not distinguished between
of disorders for which children with ADHD appear to children with ADHD alone (ADHD-only) and those
be at risk, yet which often remains unrecognized, is with comorbid disorders (e.g., Beitchman et al., 1987;
Love & Thompson, 1988), making it difficult to de-
termine whether the language impairments are spe-
1Doctoral candidate, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, cific to ADHD or whether they are more a function
Canada. of the comorbid disorder. In particular, a substantial
2Department of Psychiatry Research, Hospital for Sick Children, proportion of ADHD children also meet criteria for
Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5G 1X8. a diagnosis of reading disability (August & Garfinkel,
3Address all correspondence to Rosemary Tannock, Ph.D., De-
partment of Psychiatry Research, Hospital for Sick Children, 555 1989; Dykman & Ackennan, 1991; Faraone et al.,
University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario M5G 1X8 Canada. 1993; Lambert & Sandoval, 1980; Semrud-Oikeman

133
<X»l-0627/97A>4aH>13»113<VO C 1997 Plenum Publishing Corporation
134 Purvis and Tannock

et al., 1992) which are also associated with language sisted of children recruited from other medical out-
impainnents (Hallahan & Kaufman, 1976; Lapadat, patient clinics at the same hospital for minor medical
1991). problems or routine health checks. Only those chil-
The purpose of the present study was to examine dren without any history of behavioral or attentional
the pragmatic and semantic language abilities of chil- problems, nor any current medical, behavioral, or at-
dren with ADHD and to examine the impact of con- tentional problems were eligible for the comparison
current reading disabilities on their performance. The group. Any child whose estimated full-scale IQ was
inclusion of reading-disabled (RD) control groups less than 80 or who showed evidence of a neurologi-
(ADHD + RD, RD-only) permits an examination of cal disorder, poor physical health, uncorrected sen-
the deficits due to ADHD and/or comorbidity with sory impairments, or a history of psychosis was
RD. As well, the separate language measures should excluded. Four of the ADHD boys were receiving
permit us to examine the pragmatic communication psychostimulant medication. The remaining children
versus semantic language difficulties of ADHD chil- were being considered for an evaluation of stimulant
dren. The task of story retelling was chosen because treatment. Medications were discontinued 48 hours
it requires attention to the incoming information, ex- before the day of testing.
traction of meaning and relevance, encoding of the
input into memory, and reconstruction from memory Measures
using effort and judgment so that the information is
organized, coherent, and sensitive to the needs of the
Diagnostic Measures
listener. The ability to fulfil and coordinate the on-
going requirements while performing the task re-
quires, in addition to linguistic abilities, effortful The diagnosis of ADHD was based on informa-
organization, planning, and self-monitoring, which in- tion obtained from semi-structured interviews con-
volve higher-order cognitive skills, referred to as "ex- ducted with the child's parents [Parent Interview for
ecutive functions" (Douglas, 1988; Shallice, 1982). Child Symptoms—Revised (PICS-R); Schachar &
The two tests of receptive and expressive semantic Wachsmuth, 1989] and teacher [Teacher Telephone
language abilities were chosen as language measures Interview (TTI); Schachar & Tannock, 1990]. In both
which do not have the additional executive function the PICS-R and TTT, informants provided descrip-
requirements of planning and organization. tions of the child's behavior in various situations
specified by the interview (e.g., playing out of doors,
while watching television). The interviewer, rather
METHOD than the informant, made decisions about the pres-
ence of absence of symptoms by referring to specific
Subjects criteria regarding severity, frequency, age-appropri-
ateness, and level of disability resulting from the be-
A total of 50 boys, aged 7 to 11 years, partici- havior. Interrater reliability for the PICS is high, with
pated in the study. Of these, 14 boys had a confirmed agreement for diagnoses at 100%. Substantial inter-
diagnosis of ADHD, 14 met the diagnosis of ADHD rater agreement has also been obtained for diagnoses
and reading disability (ADHD + RD), eight had a based on the TTI: ADHD (K = .76), oppositional de-
diagnosis of RD alone, and 14 children had no di- fiant disorder (ODD) (K = .76), and conduct disorder
agnosis (normal comparison). The children were part (CD) (K = .83) (Schachar et al., 1995). To be classi-
of a larger study investigating the cognitive abilities fied as ADHD, children had to meet the severity and
of children with ADHD (Schachar, Tannock, Mar- age-of-onset criteria in the Diagnostic and statistical
riot, & Logan, 1995). The present study included manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.) (DSM-III-R;
70% of the sample from the larger study. Subjects American Psychiatric Association, 1987). ADHD was
were selected for the current study based upon age, diagnosed if eight or more ADHD symptoms were
IQ, and diagnosis. Children with ADHD and/or RD reported by either parents (PICS) or teacher (TTI)
were recruited from children referred for behavior with onset before 7 years of age and with persistence
problems to the psychiatric or pediatric outpatient of more than 6 months.
clinics at a children's hospital in a large metropolitan The diagnosis of reading disability was based on
urban area. The normal comparison group (NC) con- a combined formula approach recommended by
Language Abilities in ADHD and RD 135

Barkley (1990), which requires an achievement score dex (Dale & Chall, 1948). The number of units at each
of at least 1.5 SD below the mean for age, plus a of the four levels, from most to least important were
discrepancy of at least 1.0 SD between that achieve- 16,16,16, and 15, respectively, and occurred approxi-
ment score and the child's full scale IQ score. All mately equally often in each quarter section of the
subjects were administered the Vocabulary and Block story. Recall of important units would therefore not
Design subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for be contaminated by primacy or recency effects (Brown
Children—Revised (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1974) as an & Smiley, 1977).
estimate of verbal and nonverbal intelligence (Sat- Story Measures. Retelling a story involves two ba-
tler, 1988) as well as all three subtests of the Wide sic steps. First the child must attend and comprehend
Range Achievement Test—Revised (WRAT-R; Jastak the story. Second, he or she must try and reproduce
& Wilkinson, 1987). In this study, the Reading sub- the narrative. For this reason, the children's perform-
test of the WRAT-R was used to define reading dis- ances were assessed along these two basic dimen-
ability. sions, comprehension and production.
• Comprehension. Two measures were used to as-
Language Measures sess children's comprehension of the story. The
first was selective recall as a function of the pre-
Three measures of language ability were se- judged thematic importance of the story units.
lected. The first was a story retelling task which re- The second was a comprehension score based
quired the comprehension and use of extended on children's responses to a series of factual and
stretches of language. The other two measures were inferential questions which were derived from
standardized measures of receptive and expressive units of information central to the theme of the
abilities involved in the semantic aspects of language. story.
Story Retelling Task. One folk tale was selected Selective recall was defined as the num-
as stimulus material for this task. This story was un- ber of idea units correctly recalled at each of
familiar to the children in the study but had been the four levels of importance. This was con-
used in prior studies on story recall in normal chil- verted to a proportion of the total number of
dren (Brown, Day, & Jones, 1983; Brown & Smiley, units at each level, to remain consistent with
1977; Smiley, Oakley, Worthen, Campione, & Brown, previous research. The ability to focus on im-
1977) and ADHD children (Tannock, Purvis, & portant aspects of the story rather than non-
Schachar, 1993). The folk tale, "The Father, His Son essential material is an essential cognitive
and Their Donkey," originated from an oral tradition process for comprehension of written or audi-
and was intended to be read aloud to children. The torily presented information (Smiley et al.,
story was audiotaped at a normal rate for speaking 1977). Thus it is the pattern of recall (rather
or reading aloud, at approximately 100 words/minute than the absolute amount) which indicates
and narrated by a female voice. The story was 2 min, comprehension.
56 s, in length and involved multiple characters and Responses to the comprehension ques-
sequences of events which made it too long and de- tions were rated as correct (score 2),partially
tailed to be recalled verbatim. Thus retelling re- correct (score 1), or incorrect (score 0), yield-
quired the selection and organization of relevant ing a maximum score of 10. The questions
information. were included as a supplement to the selec-
The story had been divided into individual units. tive recall measure because it was considered
A unit was defined as one which contained an idea possible that the children could exhibit com-
and/or represented a pausal unit. Each unit was rated prehension by answering when prompted with
(using a 4-point scale) for its importance to the struc- a question but not spontaneously produce the
ture and theme of the story as a whole by Brown information during free recall of the story.
and colleagues in a previous study (Brown et al., Production. Two aspects of children's produc-
1983), following a procedure used by Brown and tion of narratives were assessed. The first was
Smiley (1977), developed by Johnson (1970). overall productivity (total amount recalled) and
The story contained 427 words, and 63 pausal the second was the adequacy of the children's
units, and scored 5.2 on the Dale-Chall Readability In- organization and self-monitoring of their output.
136 Purvis and Tannock

Organization and self-monitoring were assessed and semantic abilities. Five of the six available sub-
in terms of unrepaired errors made by children tests were administered to the children. These in-
during their story reconstructions. Errors left un- cluded (1) Associations, where the subject must
repaired reflect a lack of ongoing self-monitor- choose the one word from four that does not belong
ing of the accuracy of the child's own output. and then explain his/her choice in relation to the
Four types of errors were analyzed: (a) errors category of the other three words; (2) Synonyms,
in sequencing story events, (b) ambiguous ref- where the subject must express a one-word synonym
erences, (c) misinterpretations, and (d) inappro- for each stimulus item; (3) Semantic Absurdities,
priate word substitutions. The number of errors which measures the ability to identify and express
of a specific type were computed as a proportion what is wrong with an absurd statement; (4) Anto-
of the total number of story units recalled by nyms, where the child must give a one-word opposite
the child to control for differences in total out- for each stimulus item; and (5) Multiple Definitions,
put. Definitions and examples of each of these where the child must give two meanings for each test
coding categories are provided in the appendix. word.
Retelling story events out of sequence re- Language Processing Test. The Language Process-
flects a breakdown in the global organization ing Test (LPT, Richard & Hanner, 1985) is a stand-
of the story theme. Referring back to prior ardized measure of the semantic aspects of language
events via the use of pronominal references which taps the ability to analyze, organize, and asso-
requires a complex organization of ideas to ciate linguistic units. Three of the six available sub-
maintain the distinction between characters, tests were chosen, including (1) Similarities, where the
events, and their relationships, for the clari- subject must compare the properties of two items,
fication of the listener (Liles & Purcell, 1987). identity the primary common aspects, and describe
Ambiguous references thus reflect cohesion how they are alike; (2) Differences, which taps the
problems with the local organization of infor- ability to contrast two items by identifying the primary
mation across sentences (Fine, 1985; Liles & aspects of item variance and tell how the two objects
Purcell, 1987). Misinterpretations are seman- are not alike; and (3) Multiple Meanings, which re-
tic departures from the original text that vio- quires the child to recognize and define a word in
late text veracity (i.e., inaccurate information varying contexts using language strategies to select ap-
that alters the text's primary meaning). These propriate synonyms or definitions by giving up to four
reflect a breakdown in the process of self- definitions for each stimulus word.
monitoring the accuracy of the information.
However, they must be interpreted in light of
Procedure
the child's comprehension of the story. That
is, if comprehension is poor, misinterpreta-
Children were tested individually in one session
tions may in fact be due to poor comprehen-
in a small quiet room. The instructions for the story
sion. However, if comprehension is adequate,
retelling task were to listen carefully to the story so
misinterpretations may be viewed as deficits
that each child could retell it in his or her own words
in self-monitoring of the output at the propo-
for the next child coming to the study. The children
sition level. Inappropriate word substitutions
were also told that their stories would be recorded
(e.g., nuns substituted for maidens) also re-
and that afterwards they would be asked questions
flect a breakdown in self-monitoring of the
about the story. Details of the procedure and data
accuracy of information but is at the word
preparation including transcription and coding can be
level rather than the proposition level. Leav-
found in Tannock et al. (1993). Interrater agreement
ing unrepaired these inaccuracies that violate
for the story units, based on point-to-point correspon-
text veracity reflects a failure to actively
dence and averaged across importance levels, was .91.
monitor meanings within and across sen-
Interrater reliability for story errors was .95. All dis-
tences (Liles & Purcell, 1987).
agreements were resolved through discussion prior to
The Word Test. The Word Test (WT, Jorgeson, data entry and analysis. The children were then ad-
Barrett, Huisingh, & Zachman, 1981) is a stand- ministered the subtests of The Word Test and Lan-
ardized measure of children's expressive vocabulary guage Processing Test. Diagnostic interviews with
Language Abilities in ADHD and RD 137

Table I. Selection Criteria and Clinical Characteristics of the Sample0


F-values and significance
(df = 1,46)"
Controls RD ADHD ADHD + RD ADHDx
(14) (8) (14) (14) ADHD RD RD

Age 9.2 (1.4) 9.3 (1.9) 8.7 (1.4) 8.7 (1.3)


WISC-R
Estimated IQ 109.0 (8.1) 102.8 (11.4) 104.9 (9.4) 101.6 (12.2)
Vocabulary 11.6 (1.8) 8.1 (2.8) 10.6 (2.2) 9.2 (2.6) 12.5d
Block Design 11.6 (Zl) 12.9 (3.3) 11.1 (2.1) 11.4 (2.7)
WRAT-R
Reading 100.9 (9.8) 71.0 (7.1) 101.1 (9.5) 68.9 (9.4) 129.5*
Spelling 91.1 (11.9) 74.2 (6.5) 92.6 (14.3) 68.6 (9.3) 40.0"
Arithmetic 95.3 (13.9) 87.2 (7.3) 93.3 (7.6) 81.6 (15.7) 7.T1
Parent and Teacher Diagnostic Variables and Cutoff Scores

Parent
Hyperactivity 0.6 (0.7) 1.8 (2.5) 11.0 (2.4) 9.1 (4.2) 92.4'
Oppositional defiant 0.7 (1.1) 0.6 (1.5) 4.7 (2.O) 32 (Z8) 25.5*
Conduct disorder 0.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.4) 1.6(1.4) 1.8(1.7) 16.4B
Teacher
Hyperactivity 0.7 (1.0) 3.7 (1.7) 11.4 (2.2) 9.4 (Z8) 130.8* ViSf-"
Oppositional defiant 0.8 (1.9) 0.5 (1.2) 3.6 (2.3) 3.1 (3.4) 14.6'
Conduct disorder 0.0 (0.0) 0.0 (0.0) 1.4 (1.6) 1.5 (1.9) 8.&
'Values given as means and standard deviations (in parentheses). ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, RD = reading
disability.
6Missing data for some subjects; for parent ratings (df = 1,40), controls, n = 9, RD, n = 7, ADHD, n = 14, ADHD + RD, « = 14;
for teacher ratings; (df = 1,38), controls, n = 9,, RD, n = 6, ADHD, n = 14, ADHD + RD, n = 13.
cPost hoc comparison of RD and contro groups, and post hoc comparison of ADHD and ADHD + RD groups not significant.
dp < .01.
'p < .001.

last factor. Story errors were analyzed using a 2


parents were conducted on the same day as the chil-
(ADHD) x 2 (RD) multivariate analysis of variance
dren's testing. Telephone interviews with teachers
(MANOYA) on the four error types. Scores on the
were conducted within 1 to 2 weeks before or after
two standardized language tests were analyzed sepa-
the testing session. Testing was conducted blind to
rately in 2 (ADHD) x 2 (RD) x 5 (WT subtests) and
each child's diagnosis.
2 (ADHD) x 2 (RD) x 3 (LPT subtests) MANOVAS.
Univariate analyses and post-hoc comparisons of
Analysis pairs of group means (Tukey Honestly Significant
Difference Test; Keppel, 1982) were conducted to
Diagnostic and sample characteristics were ana- clarify any main effects and interactions.
lyzed using the analysis of variance (ANOVA) pro-
gram in the windows version of the Statistical
Package for the Social Sciences with two between- RESULTS
subjects factors: 2 (ADHD) x 2 (RD). Analysis of
comprehension (proportion of idea units recalled as Age and IQ
a function of their structural level of importance) was
analyzed using a 2 (ADHD) x 2 (RD) x 4 (Impor- The four groups did not differ in age, estimated
tance Level) ANOVA with repeated measures on the IQ, or Block Design scores of the WISC-R. A sig-
138 Purvis and Tannock

Fig. 1. Mean proportion of story units recalled by normal control, RD, ADHD, ADHD +
RD groups as a function of prejudged level of thematic importance. RD = reading disability,
ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

nificant main effect of RD was found on the Vocabu-


of ADHD symptomatology. Similarly, the RD and
lary scores of the WISC-R, indicating that the RD
NC groups did not differ significantly from each
groups (RD and ADHD + RD) scored lower on this
other. These findings validate our diagnostic proce-
subtest than the non-RD groups (NC and ADHD).
dures and indicate that comorbidity was not con-
Means, standard deviations, F-values, and signifi-
founded with severity.
cance for these data and diagnostic measures are
Similar main effects of ADHD diagnosis were
presented in Table I.
found for severity of ODD and CD symptomatology
(Table I). Thus the ADHD groups differed from the
Diagnostic Measures non-ADHD groups in the severity of ODD/CD
symptomatology. In the pure-ADHD group, three
As expected, children who were selected to be children met criteria for a diagnosis of ODD, and six
in the ADHD groups (ADHD, ADHD + RD) had met criteria for a diagnosis of CD. In the ADHD +
significantly higher ADHD scores on both the parent RD group, one child met the diagnosis for ODD and
and teacher measures than children in the non- eight children met criteria for CD. A chi-square test
ADHD groups (RD and normal controls) (Table I). for the difference in the distribution of ODD/CD di-
The average number of positive symptoms (out of a agnoses between the ADHD and ADHD + RD
total of 14) for the ADHD group was 11 on both groups revealed no significant differences. No child
parent and teacher interviews. The average number in either the normal controls or the pure-RD group
of positive symptoms for the ADHD + RD group met criteria for a further diagnosis.
were nine on both interviews. No interaction between With respect to academic achievement, signifi-
ADHD and RD status was found for parent meas- cant main effects of RD were found for the three
ures; however, an interaction was found for teacher subtests of the WRAT-R (Reading, Spelling, and
scores of ADHD symptoms. Post-hoc Tukey analysis Arithmetic), indicating the RD groups (RD and
did not reach significance, but an examination of the ADHD + RD) scored significantly lower than the
means revealed that the pure-RD children had non-RD groups (ADHD and normal controls) on all
slightly more teacher-identified symptoms of ADHD three subtests. No significant interactions were
than normal controls. Thus the ADHD and ADHD found. These results again validate the diagnostic
+ RD groups did not differ significantly in severity procedures by indicating that academic achievement,
Language Abilities in ADHD and RD 139

fig. 2. Mean proportion of errors produced by all groups during story recall. RD =
reading disability; ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

particularly reading, was equated within the RD and cant difference in the recall of levels 4 and 3, or be-
non-RD groups. tween levels 2 and 1.
The mean comprehension question scores (out
of a possible 10) for the ADHD, ADHD + RD, RD,
Story Retelling—Comprehension and control groups were 5.4,5.8, 4.9, and 6.4, respec-
tively. Although the RD-only children obtained the
The primary measure of comprehension was se-
lowest scores, the analysis revealed no significant dif-
lective recall. The mean proportion of units recalled
ferences between the groups.
by the four groups of children at each level of im-
portance are illustrated in Fig. 1. This figure shows
that the control, ADHD, and ADHD + RD groups Story Retelling—Production
exhibited a similar pattern of recall, although the
normal children tended to recall more at each level Verbal Productivity. As illustrated in Fig. 1, the
of importance. Thus these three groups recalled normal controls appeared to recall proportionately
more of the theman'calry important information than more total idea units than the other three groups.
the minor details. The pure-RD group, as shown in The proportion of idea units for the ADHD, ADHD
Fig. 1, appeared to have a different pattern of recall + RD, RD, and NC groups were .27, .28, .24, and
from the other three groups, recalling fewer of the .36, respectively. This trend was indicated by a non-
most important units of information (level 4). They significant interaction between the ADHD and RD
did not recall increasingly more information units as groups [F(l, 46) = 3.40, p = .072].
importance level increased. Organization and Monitoring. The mean propor-
Statistical analysis of these results revealed no tion of error types as a function of diagnostic group
significant interactions or main effects for group. As are illustrated in Fig. 2. The overall MANOVA indi-
expected, a significant effect of level [F(3, 138) = cated a three-way interaction between ADHD, RD,
29.00, p < .001] was found. Post-hoc Tukey compari- and error type [F(3,138) = 4.69, p < .01]. Therefore
sons among the means for importance levels revealed errors were analyzed separately in 2 (ADHD) x 2
that children recalled proportionately more of the (RD) ANOVAs. Significant main effects of ADHD
most important units (levels 4 and 3) than the least diagnosis were found for sequence errors [F(l, 46)
important (levels 2 and 1) but there was no signifi- = 5.7, p < .05], misinterpretations [F(l, 46) = 4.5,
140 Purvis and Tannock

Table II. Mean Scores on the Word Test (WT) and Language Processing Testa
F-values and significance (WT, df
= 1,37; LPT, df = 1,36)
ADHDx
Controls RD ADHD ADHD + RD ADHD RD RD
The Word Test
Associations 51.9 (63) 44.2 (8.8) 50.1 (2.8) 47.8 (7.6)
Synonyms 50.8 (6.0) 43.8 (45) 50.6 (4.0) 47.9 (4.0)
Semantic Absurdities 51.6 (4.6) 42.7 (6.1) 49.8 (5.2) 47.8 (3.4)
Antonyms 48.6 (4.3) 44.0 (4.3) 51.4 (4.4) 47.4 (2.8)
Multiple Definitions 50.4 (4.6) 41.0 (5.4) 48.4 (4.6) 46.6 (4.3)
Mean score 605 (3.9) 43.2 (5.0) 50.0 (22) 47.6 (3.2) 18.6e 45C
Language Processing Test
Similarities 52.7 (65) 45.0 (9.9) 51.8 (6.8) 49.5 (9.9)
Differences 50.9 (5.3) 43.8 (9.4) 50.8 (8.1) 46.5 (8.9)
Multiple Meanings* 48.9 (10.3) 40.5 (85) 46.3 (5.2) 425 (3.6)
Mean score 50.8 (5.4) 43.1 (8.6) 49.6 (5.0) 46.2 (6.1) 7.3d
"Values given as cans and standard deviations (in parentheses) T-scores, mean = 50, SD = 10 (WT); 15 (LPT. Missing data for some
subjects; the Word Test controls, n = 9., RD, n = 4, ADHD, n = 14, ADHD, n = 14; Language Processing Test ADHD + RD, n
= 13. RD = reading disability; ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
^Scores on this test significantly lower than the combined scores of Similarities and Differences.
cp < .05.
*p < .01.
'p < .001.

with the pure-RD children scoring lower than the


p < .05], and substitutions [F(l, 46) = 5.2, p < .05],
normal controls. A significant main effect for RD
indicating that the ADHD groups (ADHD, ADHD
\F(1, 37) = 18.65, p < .001] was also found, indicat-
+ RD) made more of these errors than the non-
ing that the RD groups (RD and ADHD + RD)
ADHD groups. A significant ADHD x RD interac-
scored significantly lower than the non-RD groups
tion was found for ambiguous references [F(l, 46) -
(normal controls and ADHD). No other main effects
7.6, p < .01]. Post-hoc analyses of the four groups
or interactions were found.
revealed that the pure-ADHD and pure-RD groups
Language Processing Test. Again, a significant
made significantly more ambiguous references than
main effect for reading disability [F(l, 36) = 7.3, p
normal controls. No differences in ambiguous refer-
< .05] was found, indicating that the RD groups (RD
ences were found between the two ADHD groups
and ADHD + RD) scored significantly lower than
(ADHD and ADHD + RD) or between the two RD
the non-RD groups (normal controls and ADHD).
groups (ADHD + RD and RD).
No interactions between ADHD and RD diagnostic
status or subtests were found. A significant main ef-
The Word Test and Language Processing Test fect of subtest was found [F(2, 72) = 6.0, p < .01].
Post-hoc tests found that all children scored lower on
Word Test. Means and standard deviations for the Multiple Meanings subtest than on the Similari-
The Word Test and Language Processing lest are ties and Differences subtests.
presented in Table II. The overall MANOVA for The
Word lest revealed a significant ADHD x RD inter-
action [F(l, 37) = 4.5, p < .05]. Although post-hoc DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS
Tukey tests for this interaction did not reach signifi-
cance, an examination of the means suggests that the The results revealed two central findings con-
interaction occurred within the non-ADHD groups, cerning the language abilities of ADHD children
Language Abilities in ADHD and RD 141

with and without reading disabilities. First, children Malone, & Roberts, 1994; Zentall, Gohs, & Culatta,
with ADHD, regardless of RD status, exhibited dif- 1983) have been demonstrated in ADHD children.
ficulties in organizing and monitoring their verbal The pragmatic difficulties of ADHD children
productions. Second, children with RD, regardless of may be related to the higher-order cognitive deficits
ADHD status, demonstrated deficits in receptive and referred to as "executive functions" (Barkley, 1994;
expressive semantic language abilities. In other Benson, 1991; Douglas, 1988; Hamlett, Pellegrini, &
words, difficulties in story organization and self- Connors, 1987; Mattes, 1980; Pennington, Groissser,
monitoring of the accuracy of the information as re- & Welsh, 1993; Schachar, Tannock, & Logan, 1993;
flected in story errors were affected by ADHD status, Shue & Douglas, 1992). Executive functions are self-
whereas deficits in semantic aspects of language were regulatory processes which are responsible for the or-
affected by RD status. The comorbid group (ADHD ganization and monitoring of information processing,
+ RD) generally exhibited the deficits of both pure mobilizing attention, and inhibiting responding
groups (ADHD-only and RD-onty). Douglas, 1988; (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Luria, 1966).
The difficulties of ADHD children in organizing The problems in self-regulation are not problems of
their productions as reflected in story errors are con- knowing what to do, but rather in doing what is known
sistent with previous research (Tannock et al., 1993). (Kinsbourne, 1989). The story used in this study was
The ADHD groups exhibited a higher frequency of complex (many characters and events) and was not
sequence errors, which reflects a breakdown in the logically sequenced. This increases the demands on
global organization of story theme. The pure-ADHD the child's executive processes, requiring self-directed
group had difficulties in local organization, reflected effort, planning, and organization. Thus it may be
in ambiguous references. These are failures in cohe- deficits in executive function rather than deficits in
sion which make it difficult for the listener to follow the fundamental subsystems of language (i.e., pho-
the speaker's train of thought Ambiguous references nology, semantics, and syntax) that accounted for the
can result from a failure to organize and monitor the pragmatic difficulties of the ADHD children (Tan-
cohesion between sentences, as well as from a failure nock & Schachar, 1996).
to take into account the needs of the listener. Both Impairments in these basic language subsystems
the pure-ADHD and ADHD + RD groups were are more closely associated with RD than with
more likely to misinterpret information and to use ADHD (Ackerman, Dykman, & Gardner, 1990; Au-
inappropriate word substitutions. These two error gust, 1987; August & Garkinkel, 1989; Felton, Wood,
types occurred in the absence of comprehension Brown, Campbell, & Harter, 1987; Tannock, Corkum,
problems; therefore they are interpreted to reflect a Schachar, & Purvis, 1994; Tannock et al., 1993;). The
failure to monitor the accuracy of the information. findings of deficits on the language processing tests
These errors reflect pragmatic difficulties with lan- supported this general conclusion, since children with
guage use. The inability to relate ideas specifically a diagnosis of RD (ADHD + RD, RD-onty), scored
and accurately in a logical sequence results in dis- significantly lower than children with adequate read-
unity in discourse and makes comprehension by the ing skills (normal controls, ADHD).
listener difficult (Prutting & Kirchner, 1987). In this study, no differences were found between
In contrast to the RD groups (RD and ADHD groups in comprehension of the story as reflected in
+ RD), the ADHD-only group did not show deficits the selective recall of levels of importance or in the
in the receptive or expressive, semantic aspects of comprehension questions scores. Previous narrative
language. Unlike the narrative task, The Word lest studies have found a lack of sensitivity to the impor-
and the Language Processing lest did not require tance of information in children with learning dis-
lengthy responses with demands for self-organization. abilities (Hansen, 1978; Smiley et al., 1977; Worden
Thus the language deficits of ADHD children with- & Nakamura, 1982), but not in ADHD children
out RD seem to reflect difficulties with language use (O'Neill & Douglas, 1991; Tannock et al., 1993; Zen-
(i.e., pragmatics) rather than deficits in the basic sub- tall, 1988). The present findings are consistent with
systems of language (i.e., phonology, semantics and previous research, with the pure-RD children recall-
syntax). A wide range of pragmatic difficulties, such ing fewer of the most important units of information;
as difficulties in maintaining a conversation or turn however, the sample size may have been too small
taking during conversations (Humphries, Koltun, to permit detection of differences.
142 Purvis and Tannock

In contrast to the language deficits on the lan- a current study with a similar design and comparable
guage processing tests, the children with RD did not populations.
exhibit difficulties organizing their story retelling in From a clinical perspective, these results suggest
an accurate, logical sequence. However, the pure-RD a need to emphasize different aspects of language in
children did have particular difficulties with referen- the instruction and remediation of children in the
tial cohesion as reflected in the number of ambigu- different diagnostic categories. In particular, pure-
ous references. Previous research has supported the ADHD children would benefit from instruction in
finding that children with RD and language-disor- self-monitoring and awareness of language use in
dered children have particular difficulties with refer- language contexts which are beyond the sentence
ential cohesion even in the absence of level. Children with RD would benefit from instruc-
comprehension difficulties (Lapadat, 1991), but no tion in the semantic aspects of language, including
difficulties with sequencing story information when the ability to analyze, categorize, and associate the
the story is meaningfully structured (Feagans & meanings of words in a language. The finding that
Short, 1984; Graybeal, 1981; Montague, Maddus, & children with both ADHD and RD exhibited the
Dereshivksy, 1990; Worden & Nakamura, 1982). deficits of both pure groups suggests that remedia-
The deficits of the comorbid group (ADHD + tion should be directed to both types of difficulties.
RD) relative to the two pure groups allows for an
examination of the basis for comorbidity. There is
considerable debate in the literature as to whether
this group has the core cognitive deficits of RD-onty
children, ADHD-only children, or both (i.e., Fergus-
son & Horwood, 1992; Pennington et al., 1993; Shay-
witz et al., 1995). In this study, the ADHD + RD APPENDIK
children were found to have the most of the deficits
of both pure groups. However, there was some indi- Error code Explanation
cation that this group was less impaired on the lan- Sequence error Idea unit is retold in a different order than
guage measures than pure-RD children. The ADHD in the original story,
so that the story theme is affected (e.g.,
+ RD group did not differ from normal controls in "The father gets off the
ambiguous references and the pure-RD children donkey and then hoists his son up behind
seemed to be the most impaired on The Word lest. him"). It is not scored
if an item is simply omitted and the story
In addition, the RD group tended (albeit nonsignifi- continues in order.
cantly) to be the least sensitive to importance levels
Misinterpretation Meaning of an action or event is
in the story retelling task. It is possible that the basis incorrectly interpreted, (e.g., in
for the reading disability in ADHD + RD children the original story the father hoists his son
is somewhat different than for its presence in pure- up behind him on the
donkey, whereas the child interprets the
RD children. One limitation of this study was that it event as the father gets
could not measure phonological processing. off and then the son gets on).
Phonological abilities are widely recognized as the Substitution A word for an object, character, or event
core deficit in reading disabilities (e.g., Stanovich, in the original story is
1988) and may have differentiated between the replaced with an inappropriate word (i.e.,
not a synonym). For
ADHD + RD and pure-RD groups. example, nuns is substituted for maidens, pond
A recent study which addressed issues relating is substituted for aver.
to classification and definition with a large cohort of Ambiguous A pronoun is not linked to a specific
RD, ADHD, and ADHD + RD children found that reference character or event so that its
children with both ADHD and RD did indeed ex- referent is ambiguous. For example, in the
sentence "The father
hibit difficulties within the language system (includ- and son were on the donkey and then he
ing phonological awareness) characteristic of got off," it is not
children with RD only. Thus it appears that many known whether the pronoun "he" refers to
the father or the son.
children with comorbid ADHD and RD diagnoses
exhibit the characteristics of both pure disorders.
This issue is also being addressed by the authors in
Language Abilities in ADHD and RD 143

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Dale, E, & Chall, J. S. (1948). A formula for predicting readabil-


ity. Educational Research Bulletin, 27, 11-21, 37-54.
Doughs, V I. (1988). Cognitive deficits in children with attention
This research was supported by grants from deficit disorder with hyperactivity. In L. M. Bloomingdale &
Health and Welfare Canada (NHRDP) awarded to G. Swanson (Eds.), Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
5, (Book Suppl.), 65-81.
Drs. Schachar, Tannock, and Logan, and from the Dykman, R. A., & Ackerman, P. T. (1991). Attention deficit disorder
Medical Research Council of Canada awarded to and specific reading disability: Separate but often overlapping dis-
Drs. lannock, Schachar, and Logan. The study is orders. Journal of Learning Disabilies, 24, 96-103.
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Lehman, B. K., Spencer, T, Norman,
based in part on a Master's thesis completed by D., Seidman, L. J., Kraus, I., Perrin, J., Chen, W. J., & Tsuang,
Karen Purvis at the University of Toronto. The M. T (1993). Intellectual performance and school failure in
authors gratefully acknowledge Michael Marriott and children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and in
their siblings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 616-623.
Patricia Fulford for their assistance with data collec- Feagans, L., & Short, E. (1984). Developmental differences in the
tion. comprehension and production of narratives by reading-dis-
abled and normally achieving children. Child Development, 55,
1727-1736.
Felton, R. H., Wood, E B., Brown, I. S., Campbell, S. K., & Harter,
REFERENCES M. R. (1987). Separate verbal memory and naming deficits
in attention deficit disorder and reading disability. Brain and
Ackerman, P. T., Dykman, R. A., & Gardner, M. Y. (1990). ADD Language, 31, 171-184.
students with and without dyslexia differ in sensitivity to Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L, J. (1992). Attention deficit and
rhyme and alliteration. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, reading achievement. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
279-283. try, 33, 375-385.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical Fine, J. (1985). Cohesion as an index of social-cognitive factors:
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Oral language of the reading disabled. Discourse Processes, 8,
Author. 91-112.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Development of cortical circuitry and
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: cognitive function. Child Development, 58, 601-622.
Author. Graybeal, C (1981). Memory for stories in language-impaired chil-
August, C. J. (1987). Production deficiencies in free recall: A com- dren. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 269-283.
parison of hyperactive, learning disabled and normal children. Hallahan, D. P., & Kaufman, J. M. (1976). Introduction to learning
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 429-440. disabilities: A psychobehavioral approach. Englewood Cliffs,
August, G. L., & Gaifinkel, B. D. (1989). Behavioral and cognitive NJ.: Prentice-Hall.
subtypes of ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child Hamlett, K. W., Pellegrini, D. S., & Conners, C. K. (1987). An
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 739-748. investigation of executive processes in the problem solving of
Baker, L., & Cantwell, D. £ (1992). Attention deficit disorder and attention deficit disorder-hyperactive children. Journal of Pe-
speech/language disorders. Comprehensive Mental Health diatric Psychology, 12, 227-240.
Care, 2, 3-16. Hansen, C L. (1978). Story retelling used with average and learn-
Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A ing disabled readers as a measure of reading comprehension.
handbook for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 76-77). New York: Learning Disability Quarterly, 1, 62-69.
Guilford Press. Humphries, T, Koltun, H., Malone, M., & Roberts, W. (1994).
Barkley, R. A. (1994). Delayed responding and attention deficit Teacher-identified oral language difficulties among boys with
hyperactivity disorder A unified theory. In D. K. Routh (Ed), attention problems. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
Disruptive behavior disorders in children: Essays in honor of 15, 92-98.
Herbert Quay. New York: Plenum Press. Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1987). Manual for the Wide Range
Beitchman, J. H., Tuckett, M., & Battb, S. (1987). Language delay Achievement Test (revised). Wilmington, DE: Jastak.
and hyperactivity in preschoolers: Evidence for a distinct sub- Johnson, R. & (1970). Recall of prose as a function of the struc-
group of hyperactives. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 32, 683- tural importance of the linguistic units. Journal of Verbal
687. Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 12-20.
Benson, D. F. (1991). The role of frontal dysfunction in attention- Jorgeson, C, Barrett, M., Huisingh, R., & Zachman, L. (1981).
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, 6, The Word Test: A test of expressive vocabulary and semantics.
9-12. Moline, Illinois: Linguisystems Inc.
Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. E. (1991). Comorbidity Keppel, G. (1982). Design and analysis: A researcher's handbook
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). American (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Inc.
Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 564-577. Kinsboume, M. (1989). Control system instability in hyperactive
Brown, A. L., Day, J. D., & Jones, R. S. (1983). The development children. In L M. Bloomingdale (Ed), Attention deficit disor-
of plans for summarizing texts. Child Development, 54, 968- der (Vol. 3, pp. 104-106). New York: Pergamon Press.
979. Lambert, N. M., & Sandoval, J. (1980). The prevalence of learning
Brown, A. L., & Smiley, S. S. (1977). Rating the importance of disabilities in a sample of children considered hyperactive.
structural units of prose passages: A problem of metacognitive Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 8, 33-50.
development. Child Development, 48, 1-8. Lapadat, J. C. (1991). Pragmatic language skills of students with
Cohen, N., Davine, M., Horodezky, N., Lipsett, L., & Isaacson, L. language and/or learning disabilities: A quantitative synthesis.
(1993). Unsuspected language impairment in psychiatrically Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 147-158.
disturbed children: Prevalence and language and behavioral Liles, B. Z., & Purcell, S. (1987). Departures in the spoken nar-
characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and ratives of normal and language-disordered children. Applied
Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 595-603. Psycholinguistics, 8, 185-202.
144 Purvis and Tannock

Love, A. J., & Thompson, M. G. (1988). Language disorders and Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., Schneider, A. E.,
attention deficit disorders in young children referred for psy- Marchione, K. E., Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J., Shankweiler,
chiatric services. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 58, 52- D. P., Katz, L., Liberman, I. Y., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1995).
64. Interrelationships between reading disability and attention-
Luria, (1966). Higher cortical function in man. New York: Basic deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 1, 170-
Books. 186.
Mattes, J. A. (1980). The role of frontal lobe dysfunction in child- Shue, K. L., & Douglas, V I. (1992). Attention deficit hyperactivity
hood hyperkinesis. Comprehensive Psychiatry 21, 358-369. disorder and the frontal lobe syndrome. Brain and Cognition,
Montague, M., Maddus, C. D., & Dereshrwsky, M. I. (1990). Story 20, 104-124.
grammar and comprehension and production of narrative Smiley, S., Oakley, D, Worthen, D., Campione, J., & Brown, A.
prose by students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learn- (1977). Recall of thematically relevant material by adolescent
ing Disabilities, 23, 190-197. good and poor readers as a function of written versus oral
O'Neill, M. E., & Douglas, V I. (1991). Study strategies and story presentation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 381-387.
recall in attention deficit disorder and reading disability. Jour- Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 671-692. dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonologi-
Pennington, B. F., Groisser, D., & Welsh, M. C. (1993). Contrast- cal-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Dis-
ing cognitive deficits in attention deficity hyperactivity disor- abilities, 21, 590-604.
der versus reading disability. Developmental Psychology, 29, Tannock, R., Corkum, P., Schachar, R., & Purvis, K. (1994).
511-523. Phonological processing in children with attention deficit hyper-
Prurting, C A., & Kirchner, D. M. (1987). A clinical appraisal of activity disorder. Poster presentation at the Annual Child Psy-
the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of Speech and chiatry Day, The Hospital for Sick Children, Children,
Hearing Disorders, 52, 105-119. Tannock, R., Purvis, K. L., & Schachar, R. J. (1993). Narrative
Richard, G., & Hanner, M. (1985). Language Processing Test. abilities in children with attention deficit disorder and normal
Moline, Illinois: Linguisystems Inc. peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 103-117.
Sattler, J. M. (1988). Assessment of Children (3rd. ed). San Diego,
CA: J. M. Sattler. Tannock, R., & Schachar, R. (1996). Executive dysfunction as an
Scbachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1990). Teacher Telephone Interview underlying mechanism of behavior and language problems in
for Disruptive Behavior Disorders (DSM-III-R). Unpublished attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In J. H. Beitchman,
manuscript, Hospital for Sick Children, Department of Psy- N. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas, & R. Tannock (Eds.), Lan-
chiatry Research, Toronto. guage learning and behavior disorders: Developmental, biologi-
Schachar, R., Tannock, R., & Logan, G. (1993). Inhibitory control, cal, and clinical perspectives (pp. 128-155). Cambridge,
impulsiveness and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. England: Cambridge University Press.
Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 721-739. Voelker, S. L, Carter, R. A., Sprague, D. J., Gdowski, C. L, &
Schachar, R, Thnnock, R., Marriott, M, & Logan, G. (1995). De- Lachar, D. (1989). Developmental trends in memory and
ficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity dis- metamemory in children with attention deficit disorder. Jour-
order. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 411-437. nal of Pediatric Psychology, 14, 75-88.
Schachar, R. J., & Wachsmuth, R. (1989). Parent Interview for Child Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Symptoms—Revised DSM-III-R. Unpublished manuscript, Children—Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Hospital for Sick Children, Department of Psychiatry, Worden, P. E., & Nakamura, G. V (1982). Story comprehension
Toronto. and recall in learning disabled vs. normal college students.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Biederman, J., Sprich-Buckminster, S., Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 633-641.
Lehman, B., Faraone, S., & Norman, D. (1992). Comorbidity Zentall, S. S. (1988). Production deficiencies in elicited language
between ADDH and learning disability: A review and report but not in the spontaneous verbalization of hyperactive chil-
in a clinically referred sample. Journal of the American Acad- dren. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 657-673.
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 439-448. Zentall, S. S., Gohs, D. E., & Culatta, B. (1983). Language and
Shallicc, T (1982). Specific impairments in planning. Royal Society activity of hyperactive and comparison children during listen-
of London, B298, 199- 209. ing tasks. Exceptional Children, 50, 255-266.

You might also like