You are on page 1of 5

Vision Res. Vol.35, No. 6, pp.

791-795, 1995
Pergamon 0042-6989(94)00157-X ElsevierScienceLtd.Printedin GreatBritain

The Effect of Perceived Distance on


Accommodation Under Binocular Steady-state
Conditions,*
J O H N C. K O T U L A K , # S T E P H E N E. M O R S E t
Received 21 March 1994; in revised form 30 June 1994

Spatiotopic cues, :such as perceived distance, have little effect on accommodation unless blur has been
reduced or eliminated. We investigated the effect of perceived distance on accommodation under
binocular steady-.,~tate conditions, about which little is known. Blur was reduced but not eliminated
by using a stimulus with a moderately low luminance. Accommodation was measured under two
conditions: (1) when cues from perceived distance, blur, and convergence were aligned; and (2) when
perceived distance was opposed by both blur and convergence. We found a significant difference in
accommodation between the two conditions, which we attribute to perceived distance.

Accommodation Perceived distance Binocular vision Autostereogram

INTRODUCTION distance) did affect accommodation, at least for some


observers. The purpose of the present work was to assess
Cognitive or higher order (spatiotopic) cues, such as
the influence of perceived distance on steady-state
perceived distance, have little impact on monocular
accommodation under binocular semiopen loop con-
steady-state accommodation when put into conflict with
ditions, which has not been previously investigated. The
the retinotopic cue of blur (Alpern, 1958; Morgan, 1968;
semiopen loop paradigm was selected because we were
Hennessy, 1975). Howe~cer, spatiotopic cues do affect
interested in natural viewing conditions and, in the light
monocular steady-state .accommodation when blur is
of previous work, it was felt that a closed loop approach
eliminated, such as by increasing the depth of focus of
would not be worth pursuing (Alpern, 1958; Morgan,
the eye with a pinhole pupil (Rosenfield & Gilmartin
1968; Hennessy, 1975).
1990; Rosenfield, Ciuffreda & Hung, 1991). The former
To ascertain the effect of perceived distance on accom-
(with-blur) condition is described as "closed loop", while
modation, we performed an experiment under two con-
the latter (without-blur) condition is referred to as "open
ditions. In the baseline case (Condition 1), there was
loop" (Morgan, 1968). tinder dynamic open loop con-
minimal conflict between perceived distance, blur, and
ditions, spatiotopic cues have been shown to influence
convergence. In Condition 2, perceived distance con-
accommodation during monocular (Kruger & Pola,
flicted, not only with blur, but also with convergence,
1987) and binocular viewing (McLin, Schor & Kruger,
i.e. blur and convergence were in harmony with each
1988).
other but both were opposed to perceived distance.
Recently, the term "semiopen" loop has been intro-
It was important to control for the effect of convergence
duced to describe an intermediate state in which the
because of its well known influence on accommodation
accommodative feedback loop is neither completely
(Fincham & Walton, 1957; Schor & Kotulak, 1986).
closed nor open (Kotulak, Morse & Wiley, 1994a, b).
All other things being equal, any change in accommo-
Under semiopen loop conditions, the stimulus is par-
dation between the two conditions must be due to the
tially degraded, such as through reduced contrast or
change in perceived distance. To create the semiopen
luminance, without complete removal of blur. Kotulak
loop condition we used a relatively low stimulus lumi-
et al. (1994a, b) found that under monocular semi-
nance. A control experiment was done to confirm that
open loop conditions, a spatiotopic cue (known object
this reduced the effect of blur.

*The views,opinions, and/or findingscontained in this paper are those


of the authors, and should not be construed as an officialDepart- MAIN EXPERIMENT
ment of the Army position, unless so designated by other official
documentation. This experiment was done to assess the effect of
tVisual Sciences Branch, U.S. Army Aeromedical Research Labora- perceived distance on accommodation during binocular
tory, P.O. Box 620577, Fort Rucker, AL 36362-0577, U.S.A. semiopen loop viewing.
791
792 JOHN C. KOTULAKand STEPHEN E. MORSE

Apparatus the analog signals from the optometer and eye tracker
A relatively new type of random-dot stereogram were digitized at 20 Hz. A trial mean was the average
(autostereogram), composed of horizontally repeating of these 200 points. Three trial means were averaged
patterns of dots, was used to generate the visual stimuli for each condition. Accommodation was measured in
(Tyler & Clarke, 1990). The main differences between the left eye. To prevent optometer artifacts, the pupils of
an autostereogram and a conventional random-dot the subjects were dilated with two doses of 2.5% phenyl-
stereogram are: (1) an autostereogram can readily ephrine hydrochloride, administered at 5-min intervals,
elicit a stereoscopic depth percept without a separate and consisting of one drop each.
apparatus; and (2) in an autostereogram, complete Perceived distance was measured twice for each con-
information for the two eyes is contained in a single dition, and the two trials were averaged. The position of
printed sheet. The three-dimensional percept is visible the pointer was set randomly by the investigator prior to
only when stereoscopic fusion is obtained, just as in a each trial. The subject's task was to position a thimble
conventional random-dot stereogram. Stereoscopic in the exact location of the object of interest, using the
fusion occurs when the eyes are converged (or diverged) pointing finger of the (unseen) dominant hand (Foley
to a point not on the physical plane of the stereogram, & Held, 1972; Wallach et al., 1972; Fisher & Ciuffreda,
which creates a depth plane in front of (or behind) 1988). Between trials, the subject was required to alter-
the physical plane of the stereogram. When viewed nate fixation to various points around the room to avoid
without stereopsis, i.e. when the eyes are converged for the effects of oculomotor adaptation on perception
the physical plane of the stereogram, only the various (Wallach et al., 1972).
repeating patterns of random dots are perceived. Thus, For ease of comparison, we expressed accommo-
when an autostereogram is viewed alternately with dation, convergence, and perceived distance in numeri-
and without stereopsis, the perceived distance of the cally equivalent units, which were calculated from the
stimulus is varied without affecting its luminance, reciprocal of distance in meters. By convention, this unit
contrast, or spatial frequency spectrum, and without is referred to as diopters (D) for accommodation or
changing the dioptric stimulus to accommodation. distance, and as meter angles (MA) for convergence.
Unfortunately, the change in perceived distance is con- In this paper, all references to distance are in diopters
tingent upon a change in convergence, which introduces (including those from other studies in which distance was
a confounding variable. However, the confound can be measured on a linear scale).
overcome with prism of sufficient strength to restore
Subjects
convergence to the physical plane of the stereogram.
When this happens, the perception of depth is lessened, Eight volunteer subjects, who gave their written
but not eliminated. informed consent, were recruited for the study. The
Accommodation and convergence were measured subjects, whose mean S D age was 25.3 + 3.2 yr, had
with a dynamic infrared optometer (Cornsweet & Crane, unaided distance visual acuities of at least 20/20 in each
1970) and a dual-Purkinje-image infrared eyetracker eye and were free from eye disease and significant
(Crane & Steele, 1985), respectively. A beamsplitter oculomotor dysfunction.
permitted the subjects to view the autostereogram while
accommodation and convergence were recorded. A chin CONDITION 1: BASELINE
cup and forehead rest were used to minimize head
movements. Accommodation, convergence, and perceived distance
Perceived distance was measured with a pointing were measured under conditions designed to minimize
device that could be slid along a calibrated track located conflicts among the three.
just under the autostereogram (Foley & Held, 1972;
Methods
Wallach, Frey & Bode, 1972; Fisher & Ciuffreda, 1988).
The pointing device and track were housed such that The subject fixated the random dots on the stereogram
they could not be viewed by the subject. without attempting to elicit the depth effect. Perceived
distance was measured to the front surface of the
stereogram.
Procedures
The stimulus to blur-driven accommodation was the Results and discussion
autostereogram, and its distance from the subject's eyes M e a n _ SD accommodation, convergence, and per-
was 25 cm for all conditions. At this distance, the - 3 dB ceived distance were 3.87 + 0.37 D, 3.90 __+0.39 MA,
rolloff of the spatial frequency spectrum was at 5 c/deg, and 3.36_ 0.74D, respectively, for the target located
and the cutoff frequency was 11 c/deg. Luminance was at 25cm (4D or MA). Accommodation exhibited
1 cd/m< its characteristic steady-state error or lag (Kotulak &
Accommodation and convergence were measured for Schor, 1987). Convergence also was slightly inaccurate;
10 sec/trial. Short trials were used to prevent adaptation however, this was due mainly to a single subject who
effects (Wallach et al., 1972; Schor, Kotulak & Tsuetaki, underconverged, possibly due to the wallpaper illusion
1986). In addition, long intervals (5 min) were used (Brewster, 1844). When this subject's data were
between trials. Each trial yielded 200 data points since removed, mean + SD convergence was 4.02 + 0.23 MA.
EFFECT OF PERCEIVED DISTANCE ON ACCOMMODATION 793

2.0 --
The discrepancy between actual and perceived distance
is typical. Foley and Held (1972) reported that subjects 1.5 m
y = 0.55x- 0.03
R = 0.88
invariably underestimate (dioptric scale) distance with o
'2 P = 0.004
o~
the measurement technique that was used in the present 1.0 --
N=8 O
study. E
0.5 --

CONDITION 2: CONFLICTING CUES e~


0 -
o
Accommodation was raeasured while perceived dis-
-0.5 --
tance conflicted with both blur and convergence. The
latter two cues were in harmony with each other, but -1.0
I I L I I
both were opposed to perceived distance. -0.5 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0

Change in perceived distance (MA)


Methods
FIGURE 2. Differencein accommodation with (Condition 2) and
To obtain stereopsis, the subjects converged for a without (Condition 1) conflictingcues as a function of the analogous
point approximately twice the distance of the stereo- differencein perceiveddistance.
gram. Then, base-out prism was added on a trial-and-
error basis until convergence approximated the amount
measured under Condition 1 ( + 0.1 MA). The mean _ The change in accommodation between Conditions 1
SD amount of prism was 15.1+ 1.6 A D (approx. and 2 varied considerably among subjects. This varia-
2.52 MA). The stereoscopic percept, obtained in this tion was not random, but was closely related to the
manner, appeared behind the plane of the stereogram. analogous variation in perceived distance. Figure 2
The fixation point was a mountain peak within the shows that those subjects whose accommodation
stereoscopic percept, which was a nature scene contain- changed little between Conditions 1 and 2 were the ones
ing birds, mountains, trees, etc. The mountain peak also whose perceived distance also changed little, and vice
was the point from which perceived distance was versa.
measured.

Results and discussion CONTROL E X P E R I M E N T


Mean_+SD accommodation and perceived dis- This experiment was done to determine if our lumi-
tance were 3.52 _+ 0.56 and 2.57 _ 0.44 D, respectively. nance of I cd/m 2 was sufficiently low to create the
Figure 1 shows how accommodation and perceived semiopen loop condition that we desired. Previous
distance in the present condition varied with the research suggests that it was. Kotulak and Schor (1987)
analogous values in Condition 1. Perceived distance and found that accommodation typically was more accurate
accommodation were significantly less in Condition 2 at I0 than at 1 cd/m 2 for spatially bandpass filtered
than baseline as detenrtined by paired t-test [for targets when the center spatial frequencies were between
perceived distance, t(7) =: 3.19, P < 0.02; for accommo- 3.2 and 12.8 c/deg. This spatial frequency range overlaps
dation, t ( 7 ) = 2.62, P < 0.04]. Given that the stimulus that of the autostereogram. In addition, Kotulak and
to blur-driven accommodation and the degree of Morse (1994) reported that there was significantly more
convergence were the sarae for Conditions 1 and 2, the accommodation at 10 than at 1 cd/m 2 when the eyepiece
difference in accommodation between the two probably focus settings of an optical instrument were similar.
is due to the difference in perceived distance.
Methods
4.0 --
Our approach was to determine whether the accom-
I-"7 Condition 1
modative stimulus-response functions were different
above and below the luminance used in the main exper-
177771 C o n d i t i o n 2
iment (1 cd/m2). If so, this would demonstrate that
3.5 accommodative accuracy was not optimal at 1 cd/m 2, i.e.
that the loop was semiopen. To do so, we measured
'z stimulus-response functions at 4 and 0.4cd/m 2. We
recorded accommodation under binocular conditions
3.0
to match the conditions of the main experiment.
The accommodative stimulus values ranged from 0 to
- 1 . 5 D in 0.5 D steps. The visual stimuli were high
2.5 F7777] contrast letters whose size approximated the resolution
Accommodation Perceived
distance
limit for the test luminance. Accommodation otherwise
was measured as in the main experiment. Luminance and
F I G U R E 1. Comparison o f accommodation and perceived distance
with (Condition 2) and without (Condition 1) conflicting cues. The lens power were controlled by an optical device, known
stimulus for blur-driven accommodation was 4 D in both conditions. as a stimulus deflector, which attaches to the eyetracker
Convergence was the same in Condition 2 as it was in Condition 1. (Crane & Clark, 1978).
VR 35/6~2
794 JOHN C. K O T U L A K and STEPHEN E. M O R S E

1.50
A 4.0 cd/m 2 distance of the physical plane of the stereogram from the
y = - 0 . 3 4 x + 0.74 eyes, which was a constant 25 cm for both conditions.
1.25 - ~ R = 0 . 9 8 , P<0.02
In addition, other factors that are known to affect
reflex accommodation, such as luminance, spatial fre-
1.00 - • quency, and contrast (Kotulak & Schor, 1987) did
not vary between the two conditions. Equal convergence

0.75- accommodation between the two conditions was
• 0.4 cd/m 2 achieved by the use of prism and was confirmed by direct
y = - 0 . 2 0 x + 0.68 measurement. Changes in tonic accommodation between
0.50 - -
R = 0.98, P < 0.02
conditions due to adaptation (Schor el al., 1986) were
0.25 I I I I controlled by using short trials (10 sec), long intervals
-2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0 between trials (5 min), and by requiring the subjects
Stimulus (D) to alternate fixation to various points around the
FIGURE 3. A c c o m m o d a t i v e s t i m u l u s - r e s p o n s e u n d e r b i n o c u l a r
room between trials (Wallach et al., 1972). Since we
conditions at luminances roughly a half log unit above and below that controlled for all the known factors which could influ-
o f the m a i n e x p e r i m e n t . ence accommodation, we conclude that the change in
accommodations across conditions was due to perceived
distance.
Further evidence for this comes from Fig. 2, which
Subjects relates the change in accommodation between Con-
Sixteen volunteer subjects, who gave their written ditions 1 and 2 for each subject to the analogous change
informed consent, were recruited for the study. The in perceived distance. If the decrease in accommodation
subjects, whose mean + SD age was 25.4 ___3.0 yr, had between Conditions 1 and 2 were due to some factor
unaided distance visual acuities of at least 20/20 in each other than perceived distance, one would expect these
eye and were free from eye disease and significant accommodative changes to be poorly correlated with the
oculomotor dysfunction. corresponding changes in perceived distance. However,
this is not the case. The changes in accommodation
Results and discussion between Conditions 1 and 2 were not haphazard but
The accommodative stimulus-response functions for were highly correlated with analogous changes in
0.4 and 4.0 cd/m z are displayed in Fig. 3. The functions perceived distance (P < 0.004).
were significantly different between the two luminances The x-axis values in Fig. 2 ranged from - 0 . 1 7
by analysis of variance of regression coefficients to 1.19 D. For changes in perceived distance greater
[F(2,4) = 22.84, P < 0.007]. Since the slope was notably than our maximum, it is uncertain whether the corre-
steeper (by a factor of 1.7) for the higher luminance, lation would persist. Based on the results of a monocular
it confirms that at 1 cd/m 2 accommodative accuracy semiopen loop experiment, a saturation effect is likely
was not optimal. This is consistent with Kotulak and to occur at higher levels of blur (Kotulak et al.,
Schor (1987) and with Kotulak and Morse (1994), and 1994a, b).
it provides evidence that the accommodative feed- The effect of perceived distance on accommodation is
back loop was at least partially open during the main mirrored by a reciprocal effect of accommodation on
experiment. perceived distance, in which judgements of egocentric
distance are biased by the amount of blur-driven accom-
modation (Fisher & Ciuffreda, 1988). Thus, the relation-
GENERAL DISCUSSION
ship between accommodation and its spatiotopic cues
Accommodation was significantly less (P <0.04) appears to be similar to the relationship between accom-
during the conflicting-cue condition of the main exper- modation and convergence (Fincham & Walton, 1957;
iment (Condition 2) than during baseline (Condition 1). Schor & Kotulak, 1986). Fisher and Ciuffreda (1988)
Can we legitimately attribute this difference to the reported that, on average, a 1 D change in accommo-
significant change in perceived distance (P < 0.02) that dation elicited a 0.27 D change in perceived distance.
occurred between the two conditions? To do so, we must In the present study, a 1 D change in perceived distance
be sure that other factors which influence accommo- elicited a 0.55 D change in accommodation. It is poss-
dation were sufficiently controlled. ible that this difference reflects an anisotrophy in the
Heath (1956) classified the factors that have substan- relationship between accommodation and perceived
tial effects on accommodation into four components: distance. Besides Fisher and Ciuffreda (1988), there is
proximal or psychic, reflex, convergence, and tonic. additional evidence that the precision of accommodation
Proximal or psychic accommodation is that aroused by as a rangefinder is limited (Richards & Miller, 1969;
spatiotopic cues, such as perceived distance. The latter Kfinnapas, 1968; Crannel & Peters, 1970; Foley, 1977).
was the independent variable of our main experiment, This could be due to the inherent inaccuracy of accom-
and was manipulated by viewing the autostereogram modation (i.e. its steady-state error), which increases
alternately with and without stereoscopic fusion. Reflex under degraded stimulus conditions (Johnson, 1976;
or blur-driven accommodation was determined by thc Kotulak & Schor, 1987).
EFFECT OF PERCEIVED DISTANCE ON ACCOMMODATION 795

REFERENCES Kotulak, J. C., Morse, S. E. & Wiley, R. W. (1994b). The effect of


knowledge of object distance on accommodation during instrument
Alpern, M. (1958). Vergence and accommodation: Can change in size viewing. Perception. In press.
induce vergence eye movements? Archives of Ophthalmology, 60, Kiinnapas, T. (1968). Distance perception as a function of available
355-357. visual cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 523-529.
Brewster, D. (1844). On the knowledge of distance given by binocular Kruger, P. & Pola, J. (1987). Dioptric and non-dioptric stimuli for
vision. Transactions of the Royal Society of Edinburgh, 15, 663~74. accommodation: Target size alone and with blur and chromatic
Corusweet, T. N. & Crane, H. D. (1970). Servo-controlled infrared
aberration. Vision Research, 27, 555-567.
optometer. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 60, 548-554. McLin, L. N., Schor, C. M. & Kruger P. B. (1988). Changing size
Crane, H. D. & Clark, M. R. (1978). Three-dimensional visual stimulus
(looking) as a stimulus to accommodation and vergence. Vision
deflector. Applied Optics, 17, 706--714.
Research, 28, 883-898.
Crane, H. D. & Steele, C. M. (1985). Generation-V dual-Purkinje- Morgan M. W. (1968). Accommodation and convergence. American
image eyetracker. Applied Optics, 24, 527-537.
Journal of Optometry and Archives of the American Academy of
Crannell, C. W. & Peters, G. (1970). Monocular and binocular
Optometry, 7, 417-454.
estimations of distance when knowledge of the relevant space is Richards, W. & Miller, J. F. (1969). Convergence as a cue to depth.
absent, Journal of Psychology, 76, 157-167.
Perception & Psychophysics, 5, 317-320.
Fincham, E. H. & Walton, J. (1957). The reciprocal actions of
Rosenfield, M. & Gilmartin, B. (1990). Effect of target proximity on
accommodation and converg,mce. Journal of Physiology, London,
the open-loop accommodative response. Optometry and Vision
137, 488-508. Science, 67, 74-79.
Fisher, K. S. & Ciuffreda, K. J. (1988). Accommodation and apparent
Rosenfield, M., Ciuffreda, K. J. & Hung, G. K. (1991). The linearity
distance. Perception, 17, 609-621.
of proximally induced accommodation and vergence. Investigative
Foley, J. M. (1977). Effect of distance information and range on two
Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 32, 2985-2991.
indices of visually perceived distance. Perception, 6, 449-460.
Schor, C. M. & Kotulak, J. C. (1986). Dynamic interactions between
Foley, J. M. & Held, R. (1972). Visually directed pointed as a function
accommodation and convergence are velocity sensitive. Vision
of target distance, direction, and available cues. Perception &
Research, 26, 927-942.
Psychophysics, 12, 263-268.
Schor, C. M., Kotulak, J. C. & Tsuetaki, T. (1986). Adaptation to
Heath, G. G. (1956). Components of accommodation. American
tonic accommodation reduces the lag of accommodation and is
Journal of Optometry and Archives of the American Academy of
masked in darkness. Investigative Ophthalmology and Vision Science,
Optometry, 33, 569-579.
27, 820 827.
Hennessy, R. T. (1975). Instrument myopia. Journal of the Optical
Tyler, C. W. & Clarke, M. B. (1990). The autostereogram. In
Society of America, 65, 1114--1120.
Proceedings of the Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation Engin-
Johnson, C. A. (1976). Effects c,f luminance and stimulus distance on
eers, stereoscopic displays and applications (Vol. 1256, pp. 182-197).
accommodation and visual re~;olution. Journal of the Optical Society
Bellingham, Wash.: Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation En-
of America, 66, 138-142. gineers.
Kotulak, J. C. & Schor, C. M. (1987). The effects of optical vergence,
Wallach, H., Frey, K. J. & Bode, K. A. (1972). The nature
contrast, and luminance on the accommodative response to spatially
of adaptation in distance perception based on oculomotor cues.
bandpass filtered targets. Vision Research, 27, 1797-1806.
Perception & Psychophysics, II, 110-116.
Kotulak, J. C. & Morse, S. E. (1994). Relationship among accommo-
dation, focus, and resolution with optical instruments. Journal of the
Optical Society of America A, 11, 71-79.
Kotulak, J. C., Morse, S. E. & Wiley, R. W. (1994a). Accommodation
during instrument viewing can be influenced by knowledge of object Acknowledgements--The authors wish to thank Chris Tyler and Cliff
distance. In Vision science and its applications, 1994 technical digest Schor for helpful discussions; Tom Harding and Heber Jones for
series (Vol. 2, pp. 175-178). 'Washington, D.C.; Optical Society of assistance with the Fourier analysis of the stimulus; and Mark Kenzie
America. and Jennifer Ardouin for aid with data collection and analysis.

You might also like