You are on page 1of 13

Sex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos.

9/10, November 2005 (


C 2005)

DOI: 10.1007/s11199-005-7732-5

Body Dissatisfaction, Dating, and Importance of


Thinness to Attractiveness in Adolescent Girls

Susan J. Paxton,1,3 Melinda Norris,2 Eleanor H. Wertheim,1


Sarah J. Durkin,1 and Jenny Anderson2

The primary aim of this study was to test a multivariate model of predictors of body dissat-
isfaction in adolescent girls in which psychological variables, beliefs about the importance of
popularity with boys, and beliefs about the importance of thinness to attractiveness and dat-
ing were included. We also aimed to explore boys’ perceptions of the importance of thinness
for attractiveness. Grade 10 girls (n = 573) and boys (n = 145) completed questionnaires.
Path analysis provided partial support for the model proposed. Dating did not predict body
dissatisfaction, but a relationship between importance of popularity with boys and body dis-
satisfaction was fully mediated by the belief that boys see thinness as important in rating
girls’ attractiveness. Although girls underestimated the body size that is attractive to boys,
over 85% of boys reported a girl’s slimness influenced her attractiveness. Dating was not
correlated with body mass index.

KEY WORDS: dating; thinness; body dissatisfaction; dieting; adolescent girls and boys.

Past research has identified the importance of satisfaction, eating behaviors, and potential pressures
many sociocultural factors in the etiology of body associated with dating, such as the perception of the
dissatisfaction and disordered eating, including in- importance of thinness to have a boyfriend and dat-
ternalization of the thin ideal body image; percep- ing. In addition, there is a need to explore boys’ be-
tions of the importance of attractiveness for so- liefs about the importance of thinness in determining
cial success; media, peer, and family pressures; and attractiveness and whether they would ask a girl out
weight and shape teasing (e.g., Groesz, Levine, & on a date.
Murnen, 2002; Keery, van den Berg, & Thompson, The majority of previous research on dating and
2004; Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff- its relationship to body dissatisfaction and disordered
Dunn, 1999; Wertheim, Paxton, & Blaney, 2004). eating has centered on girls in their early teens and,
One specific group of pressures that may influence in particular, those who experienced synchronous on-
adolescent girls’ body image and eating behavior is set of menarche and dating experiences (Cauffman &
those related to the beginning of dating, an important Steinberg, 1996; Gralen, Levine, Smolak, & Murnen,
social activity that is typically first experienced in the 1990; Levine, Smolak, Moodey, Shuman, & Hessen,
teenage years. Although dating has been included as 1994; Smolak, Levine, & Gralen, 1993). In one study
a variable in past studies, there is a need for a deeper (Smolak et al., 1993), girls who began dating and
examination of the relationships between body dis- menarche at the same time were found to have higher
body dissatisfaction and disordered eating scores
1 La
than those who had not begun both at the same
Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.
2 University
time. In another study of 12- to 13-year-old girls
of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.
3 To whom correspondence should be addressed at School of Psy- (Cauffman & Steinberg, 1996), when age and menar-
chological Science, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Melbourne, chal status were controlled, girls who dated were sig-
Victoria 3086, Australia; e-mail: susan.paxton@latrobe.edu.au. nificantly more likely to diet than those who did not.

663 0360-0025/05/1100-0663/0 
C 2005 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
664 Paxton, Norris, Wertheim, Durkin, and Anderson

Using a scale that assessed a range of heterosocial be- ticipants in all of those studies were predominantly
havior from lower to higher involvement (i.e., from White, and thus their findings may not be generaliz-
no socializing with boys, meeting boys in groups, go- able to other ethnic or cultural groups.
ing out with a boy at night, to going steady with Beliefs about the importance of popularity to
a boy), Cauffman and Steinberg observed that the boys and the importance of thinness for attracting
greater the involvement in mixed-sex social activities, boys may themselves be influenced by psychological
the greater the level of dieting and disordered eating, well-being, in particular, by low self-esteem and
although dating was more strongly related to dieting depression. It is quite likely that the lower the self-
in girls who had recently begun to menstruate. How- esteem and the higher the depression, the more im-
ever, these relationships appear to be relatively short portant being popular with others, in this case boys,
term; puberty and dating are predictors of disor- might be as a means of obtaining self-validation.
dered eating and body dissatisfaction in younger girls Further, these attributes may contribute to greater
(grades 6–8) but not in mid-adolescent girls (grades sensitivity to socially endorsed notions of require-
9 and 10) (Gralen et al., 1990; Smolak et al., 1993). ments of attractiveness to others, for example,
Further research would be valuable to assess poten- thinness as a requirement of attractiveness to boys.
tial relationships between dating, body dissatisfac- Previous research has consistently indicated strong
tion, and eating behaviors in mid-adolescent girls. relationships between low self-esteem, depression,
Dating behavior itself is one factor that may be body dissatisfaction, and disordered eating in adoles-
related to body dissatisfaction and dieting; further cent girls (e.g., Paxton, Schutz, Wertheim, & Muir,
factors that may be related are beliefs regarding the 1999; Stice, Presnell, & Spangler, 2002; Wertheim
importance of (1) popularity with boys, (2) dating, et al., 1992). Thus, previously identified relationships
and (3) attractiveness to boys, which, together, was between dating behavior, dating concerns, a belief
described as “dating concern” by Gralen et al. (1990), that thinness is important in attractiveness to boys,
as these beliefs may put extra pressure on girls to and body dissatisfaction may also be influenced by
achieve the current slim beauty ideal. Gralen et al. re- psychological variables such as low self-esteem and
ported that dating concern rather than dating behav- depression in mid-adolescence. Finally, in Western
ior was a significant predictor of dieting frequency cultures, higher body mass index (BMI) consistently
and disordered eating in 6th and 10th grade adoles- predicts greater body dissatisfaction cross-sectionally
cents (but not in the 8th and 9th grades). They sug- and prospectively, most likely due to the negative
gested that dating concern and dating behavior were connotations of larger size in Western cultures
tapping different sources of variance. (Wertheim et al., 2004). Consequently, it is impor-
Alongside dating behavior and beliefs about tant to take BMI into consideration when exploring
the importance of popularity with boys, girls’ be- body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls.
liefs about the importance of thinness for attracting To date, multivariate models that integrate psy-
boys may be another perceived sociocultural pres- chological factors, BMI, and dating variables as
sure that influences body image and eating behaviors. predictors of body dissatisfaction, dieting, and bu-
In past research, adolescent girls have reported that limic symptoms have not been investigated. Drawing
attractiveness is important for success with boys, that on the theoretical and empirical material presented
body shape is an important component of attractive- above, the first aim of this research was to test a
ness, and that thinness is associated with dating suc- multivariate model of body dissatisfaction in mid-
cess (Damhorst, Littrell, & Littrell, 1988; Simmons adolescent girls, as shown in Fig. 1, using a path anal-
& Blyth, 1987). Interview studies have resulted in ysis approach. First, although it was not the main fo-
similar findings, where a substantial proportion of cus of this research, we drew on the dual pathway
girls (in one case almost 50%) held the belief that model of bulimic symptoms (e.g., Stice, 2001) and
boys preferred girls who were thin and that a girl proposed that dietary restraint and depression would
would have difficulty being popular with boys unless both directly predict bulimic symptomatology, and
she was thin (Nichter & Vuckovic, 1994; Wertheim, body dissatisfaction was proposed to be a direct pre-
Paxton, Schutz, & Muir, 1997). Lieberman, Gauvin, dictor of dietary restraint, as consistently found in
Bukowski, and White (2001) reported that attribu- previous research (e.g., Stice, 2002). Next, more in-
tions about the importance of weight and shape for volved and intimate dating behavior was proposed to
popularity and dating were important predictors of predict greater body dissatisfaction as it may be as-
body esteem and eating behavior. Notably, the par- sociated with more focus and concern on body size
Dating and Body Dissatisfaction 665

Fig. 1. Proposed path model predicting body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls.

and attractiveness. Belief that thinness is important that ranged in size from emaciated to obese. Women
in attractiveness to boys was proposed to predict were asked to select the figure that they thought men
body dissatisfaction directly as this belief is likely to would find most attractive. The figure women rated
provoke greater concerns about appearance. Impor- as being most attractive to the other sex was signifi-
tance of popularity to boys (dating concerns) was cantly thinner than the figure men actually preferred.
proposed to predict body dissatisfaction directly on This effect was replicated by Forbes, Adams-Curtis,
the basis of Gralen et al.’s (1990) finding. In addi- Rade, and Jaberg (2001) in college students and by
tion, however, we proposed that there would also be Cohn et al. (1987) in a sample of adolescent boys
a path between the importance of popularity with and girls (mean age 13 years, SD = .96). The sec-
boys and the belief that thinness is important in at- ond aim of the present study was not only to de-
tractiveness to boys, as a high belief in the impor- termine whether this effect would result when older
tance of popularity with boys could also contribute adolescents were asked the same question but also
to a greater endorsement of social stereotypes about to explore boys’ beliefs regarding the relationship
factors that make girls more attractive to boys, e.g., between thinness and attractiveness in girls. More
thinness. On the basis of previous research, low self- specifically, we were interested in whether a girl’s
esteem, depression, and BMI were each proposed thinness was important in making her appear attrac-
to predict body dissatisfaction directly. In addition, tive to boys and also whether it was important in de-
however, low self-esteem and depression were pro- termining whether a boy would ask a girl out on a
posed to predict the importance of popularity with date. To our knowledge, an examination of boys’ per-
boys, as popularity could be seen to reflect greater ceptions of girls’ attractiveness and dating potential
self-worth and social value. in relation to her thinness has not yet been described
Pressure toward thinness experienced by women in the literature.
is often ascribed to men’s preference for slender The final aim of the present study was to inves-
physiques (Cohn & Adler, 1992), but it is not clear tigate whether these presumed pressures for adoles-
whether women accurately perceive the degree of cent girls to be thin and attractive translated into ac-
slenderness desired by men and whether adolescents tual dating success. That is, we asked, are thinner
share the same perceptual patterns as older women. girls more likely to be involved in heterosocial activ-
Fallon and Rozin (1985) found that college women ities? Cawley (2001) examined a representative com-
misjudged men’s perceptions of what was the most munity sample of 12- to 16-year-old adolescents in
attractive figure. Both men and women were pre- the United States. Consistent with findings in adult
sented with a series of line drawn female figures women (Stake & Lauer, 1987), Cawley found that
666 Paxton, Norris, Wertheim, Durkin, and Anderson

higher weight lowered the probability of ever having the majority was born in Australia or New Zealand
dated and the probability of dating at least monthly (80.5%); the remaining boys were born in Asia
in the past year. Halpern and colleagues (Halpern, (5%), the Indian Subcontinent (2.8%), the United
Udry, Campbell, & Suchindran, 1999) conducted a 2- Kingdom (4.8%), European countries other than
year longitudinal study and found that higher body United Kingdom (1.4%), and other countries (5.5%).
fat was associated with a reduced probability of Only students from whom both student and parental
dating, even among nonobese adolescent girls. We consent were obtained and who were present on the
aimed to determine whether a similar relationship day of data collection participated in the research,
would be observed in an Australian sample. which resulted in a response rate of 85% of eligible
In summary, the present research was designed students.
to
Measures
(1) test a multivariate model of relationships be-
tween low self-esteem, depression, BMI, per- Separate questionnaire packages were prepared
ceived importance of popularity with boys, for girls and boys. The questionnaire for girls con-
belief that thinness is important in attractive- tained measures of dating behavior and beliefs, body
ness to boys, dating behavior, and body dis- image, eating, self-esteem, depression, and percep-
satisfaction in mid-adolescent girls (Fig. 1); tions of importance of thinness to attractiveness in
(2) examine how important a girl’s thinness is to girls to boys. The questionnaire for boys also con-
boys in determining a girl’s attractiveness and tained measures of importance of thinness to attrac-
dating potential; tiveness in girls to boys (as well as measures of dating
(3) examine how accurate mid-adolescent girls behavior, body image, health behaviors, and psycho-
are in their assessment of boys’ views of the logical well-being that were not the focus of this study
ideal female figure size; and and therefore not discussed here).
(4) identify whether dating behavior is related to
BMI in mid-adolescent girls. Dating Behavior and Beliefs

METHOD Simmons and Blyth’s (1987) Dating Behavior


Scale was used to assess a range of heterosocial ac-
Participants tivities of increasing intimacy from not going out in
the company of boys, to meeting a group of boys and
Participants were 573 girls (mean [M] age = girls at night, to having been out with a boy alone,
15.5 years, SD = .41, range = 14.4–16.4 years) and and to having a special boyfriend. As the scale as-
145 boys (M = 15.62 years, SD = .47, range = 14.3– sesses particular behaviors, it avoids the use of terms
16.9 years) in grade 10. The girls were drawn from such as “dating” that may have various meanings to
eight secondary schools (including private, public, respondents. It contains six items for which a score
coeducational, and single sex schools) in Melbourne, on a scale from 0–3 is derived such that higher scores
Australia, which encompasses a range of geographic are indicative of greater heterosocial involvement. A
and socioeconomic status areas. The majority of girls test–retest reliability of r = .75 was derived from a
was born in Australia or New Zealand (81%); the subset of the sample in the present study (N = 114).
remaining girls were born in Asia (9%), the Indian Participants were also asked whether they currently
Subcontinent (2%), the United Kingdom (1.6%), had and whether they ever had had a boyfriend or
European countries other than the United Kingdom girlfriend.
(1.6%), and other countries (5.8%). The mean BMI The importance of popularity with boys was as-
of the girls was 21.82 (SD = 3.41; BMI <19 [16.0%], sessed with an adaptation of the three-item Dat-
BMI = 19–25 [73.5%], BMI >25 [10.5%]). No signif- ing Concerns Scale of Gralen et al. (1990). These
icant differences between private and public or be- items were “Currently, how important is: (1) ‘hav-
tween coeducational and single sex schools were ob- ing a boyfriend,’ (2) ‘being popular with boys,’ and
served in the variables of interest. (3) ‘being attractive to boys?”’ The adaptation was
The smaller sample of boys was drawn from two the replacement of the word “dating” with having
coeducational schools (one public and one private), a boyfriend, as “dating” is not an expression widely
in middle socioeconomic areas. As with the girls, used by Australian adolescents. Items were rated
Dating and Body Dissatisfaction 667

from not at all important (1) to very important (5), Psychological Variables
and scores were summed. In the present study, a
Cronbach’s alpha of .81 and a test–retest reliability The 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory
of r = .80 were obtained. (Rosenberg, 1965) was used to assess self-esteem.
This is a widely used instrument in which statements
Body Dissatisfaction, Eating Behaviors, about the self such as “On the whole I am satisfied
and Body Mass Index with myself” are rated on a 4-point scale (1–4) that
ranges from strongly agree to strongly disagree. A
Body dissatisfaction was assessed using the 44- low score indicates low self-esteem. The inventory is
item Body Attitudes Questionnaire (Ben-Tovim & valid, has high reproducibility (Rosenberg), and had
Walker, 1991), which contains six subscales: attrac- a Cronbach’s alpha of .80 in the present sample.
tiveness, body disparagement, feeling fat, strength The short form of the Beck Depression Inven-
and fitness, lower body fat, and salience of weight tory (Beck & Beck, 1972) was used to measure de-
and shape. Each item is a statement about one’s atti- pression. This scale comprises 13 items, each of which
tude toward one’s body; items are rated on a 5-point has four alternative statements that vary in degree
Likert scale from definitely disagree (1) to definitely of depression expressed and that are rated from 1–4.
agree (5). Scores for each item are summed, such that Participants select the statement that best describes
a higher score indicates greater body dissatisfaction. the way they feel, and scores were summed. Higher
It has a previously demonstrated test–retest reliabil- scores were indicative of higher depression. The
ity and validity in adolescent girls (Byrnes, Burns, & short form has been found to be strongly correlated
Baur, 1995; Schutz, Paxton, & Wertheim, 2002) and with the long form (Beck, Rial, & Rickels, 1974),
in the present study Cronbach’s alpha = .87. which has been shown to have high validity and relia-
Dietary restraint was examined using the bility with adolescents (Teri, 1982). Cronbach’s alpha
10-item Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire- in the present study was .89.
Restraint subscale (Van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, &
Defares, 1986). Questions about restrained eating
behavior are rated on a 5-point Likert scale that Perceptions of the Importance of Thinness
ranges from never (1) to very often (5), and scores in Girls for Attracting Boys.
are summed such that higher scores indicate greater
dietary restraint. The DEBQ-R has high construct Girls’ perceptions of the importance of thinness
validity, test–retest reliability, and internal reliabil- for attracting boys were assessed with two items de-
ity in adolescent girls (Banasiak, Wertheim, Koerner, veloped for this research: “How important do you
& Voudouris, 2001; Laessle, Tuschl, Kotthaus, & believe being slim is. . .” (1) “to making girls attrac-
Pirke, 1989). Cronbach’s alpha in the present sample tive to boys?” and (2) “in determining whether a girl
was .94. has a boyfriend?” Items were rated on a scale from
The seven-item Eating Disorder Inventory- not at all important (1) to very important (5). The two
Bulimia subscale (Garner, Olmsted, & Polivy, 1983) items were summed to produce a score that describes
was used to assess bulimic symptomatology. Each girls’ beliefs about the importance of thinness for
item is rated on a 6-point scale, such that the least attracting boys. The items were strongly correlated
symptomatic response is given a score of 1 and the (r = .78).
most symptomatic a score of 6. The untransformed To assess beliefs held by boys about the impor-
scoring system, in which scores on each item are tance of a girl’s slimness in determining attractive-
summed, was used, as this has been found to be most ness to the boys themselves, two questions were rated
appropriate in nonclinical samples (Shoemaker, Van by boys on a 5-point scale that ranged from 1 (not at
Strien, & Van der Staak, 1994). The EDI-Bulimia all important) to 5 (very important). The questions
subscale has demonstrated validity and reliability in were “How important is slimness in making a girl
adolescent girls (Shoemaker et al., 1994; Shore & appear attractive?” and “How important is slimness
Porter, 1990) and in the present sample Cronbach’s in deciding to go out with a girl? (i.e., in having her
alpha was .83. as your girlfriend).” The two items were summed to
Weight and height were measured, and BMI produce a score that describes boys’ beliefs about
(weight [kg]/height2 [m]) was calculated (Keys, the importance of girls’ thinness for their attractive-
Fidanza, Karvonen, Kimura, & Taylor, 1972). ness. The items were strongly correlated (r = .65). In
668 Paxton, Norris, Wertheim, Durkin, and Anderson

Table I. Means (SD) and Frequencies (Percentages) of Body Image, Disordered Eating, Dating
Variables, BMI, and Psychological Variables for Participants
Girls (n = 578) Boys (n = 145)
M (SD) M (SD) t-Value
Body dissatisfaction 120.60 (22.42)
Restraint 24.28 (9.61)
EDI-Bulimia 14.38 (6.06)
Dating behavior 1.40 (1.02) 1.63 (1.07) 2.40∗
Importance of popularity with boys/girls 8.90 (2.75) 9.88 (2.33) 4.03∗∗∗
Importance of thinness to boys 5.62 (2.09) 7.35 (1.67) 10.35∗∗∗
Attractive figure in girls 5.94 (1.72) 6.59 (1.16) 5.47∗∗∗
Body mass index 21.82 (3.41)
Depression 5.80 (6.40)
Self-esteem 16.21 (2.72)
% % χ2 -Value
Currently have a boy/girl friend 15 20 2.21
Ever had boy/girl friend in the past 62 86 29.23∗∗∗

Note. ∗ p ≤ .05. ∗∗ p ≤ .01. ∗∗∗ p ≤ .001.

addition to these questions regarding personal pref- to ensure that students’ understandings of these
erence, boys were asked about friends’ views to pro- words were clear and consistent.
vide an indication of perceived peer norms. These
items were rated on the same 5-point scale. The items RESULTS
were “Generally, how important is it to your friends
to have a girlfriend with a slim figure?” and “Gen- Test of Model of Predictors of Body
erally, how important do your friends think a slim Dissatisfaction in Adolescent Girls
figure is in determining a girl’s attractiveness?”
To assess both girls’ and boys’ perceptions of Descriptive characteristics of the sample are
the most attractive female figure, participants were presented in Table I. Prior to testing the proposed
presented with the set of nine female figures of model (Fig. 1), correlations among variables for the
graduated size used by Fallon and Rozin (1985). The female sample were examined. These are displayed
scale ranged from emaciated (1) to obese (17), with in Table II. Notably, there were no significant cor-
odd numbers associated with the figures and even relations between dating behavior and body dissat-
numbers associated with the midpoints between each isfaction, r = .06, p > .05, dietary restraint, r = .08,
two figures. Girls were asked to select the figure that p > .05, or bulimic symptoms, r = .08, p > .05. How-
they thought was most like their own (current figure) ever, other variables proposed to be associated were
and the figure they thought would be most attractive significantly correlated.
to the other sex. In addition, boys were asked to se- The proposed model was then tested using
lect the figure they found most attractive in the other a path analysis based on a series of multiple re-
sex. gression analyses. Figure 2 shows the standardized
coefficients of the significant paths observed, and
the findings provide strong, but not complete, sup-
Procedure port for the proposed model. As indicated above,
the proposed relationships with dating behavior
After informed consent was given, question- were not observed. Although it was predicted that
naires were completed by students in their normal there would be a direct relationship between
classes. Two researchers were in the classroom to importance of popularity with boys and body dis-
answer any questions during questionnaire comple- satisfaction, the relationship between these two
tion, and another measured participants’ height and variables was fully mediated by the belief that
weight in an adjacent private area. Students were thinness is important to boys. Whereas, as predicted,
provided with a glossary that contained definitions of self-esteem, depression, and BMI were direct pre-
words such as “calorie,” “laxative,” or “fluid tablet” dictors of body dissatisfaction, their effects were also
Dating and Body Dissatisfaction 669

Table II. Correlations Among Body Image, Heterosocial, and Psychological Variables in Adolescent Girls
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Body dissatisfaction 1.00
2. Dietary restraint .70∗∗∗ 1.00
3. EDI-Bulimia .48∗∗∗ .43∗∗∗ 1.00
4. Dating behavior .06 .08 .08 1.00
5. Importance of popularity with boys .23∗∗ .21∗∗∗ .23∗∗∗ .28∗∗∗ 1.00
6. Importance of thinness to boys .54∗∗∗ .46∗∗∗ .34∗∗∗ .05 .50∗∗∗ 1.00
7. Attractive figure in girls .01 −.06 −.10∗ −.07 −.10∗ −.07 1.00
8. Body mass index .57∗∗∗ .42∗∗∗ .17∗∗∗ .03 −.01 .25∗∗∗ .25∗∗∗ 1.00
9. Depression .43∗∗∗ .33∗∗∗ .47∗∗∗ .03 .16∗∗∗ .31∗∗∗ .06 .12∗∗ 1.00
10. Self-esteem −.33∗∗∗ −.28∗∗∗ −.28∗∗∗ −.03 −.16∗∗∗ −.30∗∗∗ .03 −.06 −.53∗∗∗
∗p ≤ .05. ∗∗ p ≤ .01. ∗∗∗ p ≤ .001.

partially mediated by the belief that thinness is im- Boys’ Perceptions of the Importance of Thinness
portant to boys. Further, it was proposed that there in Girls for Attracting Boys
would be a direct relationship between self-esteem
and importance of popularity with boys. However, The second research question (i.e., how impor-
the relationship between these two variables was tant is a girls’ thinness to boys in determining a girls’
fully mediated by depression. Finally, a relationship attractiveness and dating potential) was addressed by
between BMI and depression, which had not been a number of means. Initially, frequencies were cal-
predicted was observed. The full model, including culated for the boys’ responses on the importance of
both direct and indirect paths, accounted for a signif- body shape to boys measure. The proportions of boys
icant amount of the variance in body dissatisfaction, who responded in each category are presented in
R2 = .56, F (6, 513) = 109.01, p < .0001. Table III. Over 63% of boys indicated that thinness
As predicted, there were significant direct paths was fairly or very important in making a girl appear
from depression, body dissatisfaction, and dietary attractive and in deciding whether to go out with a
restraint to bulimic symptoms and the complete path girl. Notably, 88% of boys reported that their friends
model predicted a significant amount of variance considered a thin figure as fairly or very important in
in bulimic symptoms, R2 = .36, F (8, 496) = 34.44, determining a girl’s attractiveness, and ratings on this
p < .0001. item were significantly higher than the comparable

Fig. 2. Path model predicting body dissatisfaction from belief that thinness is important to boys, impor-
tance of popularity with boys, depression, self-esteem, and body mass index (∗ indicates p < .05, ∗∗ indicates
p < .01, ∗∗∗ indicates p < .005).
670 Paxton, Norris, Wertheim, Durkin, and Anderson

Table III. Percentage of Boys and Girls Giving Different Ratings on Items Assessing Importance of Thinness for Attractiveness in Girls
Not at all Not very A little bit Fairly Very
important (%) important (%) important (%) important (%) important (%)
How important is slimness in making a girl Boys 1 10 26 47 16
appear attractive? Girls 10 18 32 26 13
How important is slimness: in deciding to go Boys 2 10 24 48 17
out with a girl; in determining whether a girl Girls 22 31 27 15 5
had a boyfriend?
Generally, how important is it to your friends Boys 9 7 28 38 18
to have a girlfriend with a slim figure?
Generally, how important do your friends Boys 0 1 11 61 27
think a slim figure is in determining a girl’s
attractiveness?
Do you feel any pressure from your friends Boys 25 18 27 21 9
only to show an interest in slimmer girls?

item related to own attitude, Wilcoxon Z = −2.25, was not significantly correlated with dating behav-
p > .02. As indicated in Table I, the mean rating of ior score, r = .03, p > .05. Girls were also classified
boys on the importance of thinness to boys scale was into three BMI groups: BMI <19 (15% of girls), BMI
7.35, where the maximum rating was 10, which in- ≥19<25 (73%), or BMI ≥25 (12%). An analysis of
dicates that boys generally perceived thinness as at variance indicated there was no significant difference
least moderately important in girls’ attractiveness. between the groups on mean dating behavior scores
(BMI <19 mean = 1.32 [SD = 1.00]; BMI ≥19<25
mean = 1.44 [SD = 1.02]; BMI ≥25 mean= 1.33 [SD
Accuracy of Girls’ Assessments of Boys’ Attitudes
=1.09]), F(2, 566) = .75, p > .05. However, there was
a significant difference in the proportion of girls who
To address the third research question (regard-
reported ever having had a boyfriend according to
ing accuracy of girls’ assessments of boys’ ideal body
BMI category; with 48%, 65%, and 63% of those cat-
figure in a girl), the mean attractive figure for girls—
egorized as BMI ≤19, BMI ≥19<25, and BMI ≥25,
girl-rated score was examined and compared to the
respectively, had ever a boyfriend, χ2 (2) = 8.85, p <
mean attractive figure in girls—boy-rated score us-
.05. Thus, the thinnest girls were less likely than the
ing an independent t-test (see Table I). A significant
others to report ever having had a boyfriend. To ex-
difference was found between the mean scores such
plore further whether girls who had a more “steady”
that girls believed that the attractive figure to boys
boyfriend differed across BMI categories, responses
was significantly smaller than that reported by boys
to the item in the dating scale that assessed whether
on the same measure. However, it is of interest that
a girl saw a particular boy once a week or more
an independent t-test that compared girls’ and boys’
were examined. There was no difference between
ratings on the importance of (girls’) thinness to boys
BMI categories and frequency of contact; 18%, 19%,
indicated that boys’ ratings were significantly higher
and 20% of those categorized as BMI ≤19, BMI
on this measure (Table I). Thus, it appears that, al-
≥19<25, and BMI ≥25, respectively, reported seeing
though boys view thinness in girls as important, their
a boyfriend once a week or more, χ2 (2) = .11, p >
perception of what constitutes thinness is larger than
.05.
girls believe it to be. The mean current figure of girls,
M = 7.95, SD = 2.74, was significantly higher than the
mean figure rated as most attractive to boys by both
DISCUSSION
girls, M = 5.94, SD = 1.72, t = 69.55, df = 574, p <
.001, and boys, M = 6.59, SD = 1.16, t = 5.78, df =
In the present research, we tested a model of
716, p < .001.
relationships between dating behavior, beliefs about
the importance of popularity with boys and having
Dating Behavior and BMI a boyfriend, beliefs about the importance of thin-
ness in girls’ attractiveness to boys, body dissatisfac-
Finally, the relationship between girls’ BMI and tion, and eating behavior in grade 10 girls. Partial
their dating was examined. Table II shows that BMI support for the proposed model was observed. Of
Dating and Body Dissatisfaction 671

particular note, although dating behavior was not as- beauty ideal. Future research is needed to explore
sociated with body image and eating variables, belief this possibility further.
that thinness was important to girls’ attractiveness to The model supported in this research indicates
boys strongly predicted body dissatisfaction. This be- a relationship between the importance of popularity
lief fully mediated the relationship between belief in with boys (or dating concerns) and body dissatisfac-
the importance of popularity with boys and body dis- tion, as observed by Gralen et al. (1990). However,
satisfaction. The impacts of low self-esteem and de- our data suggest that this is an indirect relationship
pression on body dissatisfaction were also partially mediated by belief in the importance of thinness in
mediated by beliefs about importance of popularity being attractive to boys. Therefore, if a girl is particu-
to boys and beliefs about thinness being seen by boys larly concerned about popularity with boys, she may
as important in girls’ attractiveness. The significant be more sensitive to cultural suggestions regarding
and high amount of variance in body dissatisfaction factors that make a girl more appealing to boys. Hav-
accounted for by the full model (56%) supports the ing a negative view of one’s self-worth as reflected in
predictive value of the model. lower self-esteem and higher depression scores ap-
This research also revealed that the majority pears to generalize to body dissatisfaction. However,
of boys do believe that thinness is an important the impact of those general well-being variables on
attribute in attractiveness in girls and that they body dissatisfaction is also mediated by girls’ belief
believe that thinness in girls is important to their that thinness is important for attractiveness to boys
male friends as well. However, consistent with and, in the case of depression, by girls’ belief that
previous research, boys’ assessment of what is an popularity with boys is important. It is possible that
ideal size in a girl is larger than girls’ assessments girls with lower self-esteem and higher depression
of boys’ ideal. Finally, no association was observed are more susceptible to internalizing beliefs that are
between dating behavior score and BMI, although, strongly endorsed by Western culture about how to
contrary to expectations, it was observed that those be acceptable to others. Finally, a direct relationship
in the thinnest category (BMI ≤19) were less likely between BMI and body dissatisfaction has been con-
to report ever having had a boyfriend. sistently observed in past research (e.g., Paxton et al.,
Our findings supported those of Gralen et al. 1999; Wertheim, Koerner, & Paxton, 2001). How-
(1990) that by mid-adolescence dating behavior in ever, the model supported in this research indicates
girls is not associated with body dissatisfaction or eat- that a higher BMI is associated with a greater belief
ing behaviors. Consequently, it does not appear that that thinness is important for attractiveness to boys.
increasing intimacy with boys increases body image Therefore, the greater the departure from the cur-
concerns at this age. Rather than actual dating be- rent beauty ideal, the greater the exposure to nega-
havior, the direct relationship between body dissat- tive comments and teasing, which may act to increase
isfaction and a girl’s belief that thinness is important awareness of cultural ideals of attractiveness and
for attractiveness to boys suggests that this belief may thus facilitate internalization of these ideals. Overall,
sensitize a girl to perceived failings in her body. Be- the model supported by our data enhances our un-
lief in the importance of thinness to boys may be part derstanding of relationships between dating beliefs,
of a wider schema about the importance of thinness dating behaviors, body dissatisfaction, and eating
generally. Thompson and Stice (2001) have explored behaviors. However, the reason for the relationships
the concept of internalization of the beauty ideal, or between variables requires further exploration.
the extent to which an individual endorses the beauty The second research question asked how impor-
standards of his or her culture. They have proposed tant a girl’s thinness is to boys in determining a girl’s
that higher internalization of the thin ideal is a risk attractiveness and deciding whether boys wanted to
factor for body dissatisfaction, and this is also sup- date her. The fact that over 60% of boys indicated
ported by experimental research (Durkin & Paxton, that thinness is important to making a girl appear
2002). Although the wider concept of internalization attractive and in deciding to go out with a girl does
of the thin ideal was not assessed in the present re- suggest that this is indeed an important feature of
search, thinness as a key to attractiveness to boys is attractiveness in the current environment. A further
widely promoted in media and advertising (Levine & finding in support of this observation is the fact
Harrison, 2004). Consequently, a high belief in the that 88% of boys indicated that they believed their
importance of thinness in determining attractiveness friends viewed a thin figure as important in determin-
may be closely associated to the internalization of the ing a girl’s attractiveness. Thus, it appears that boys
672 Paxton, Norris, Wertheim, Durkin, and Anderson

do endorse the current thin beauty ideal for women. thinness is associated with attractiveness in women
This is not surprising given that boys are exposed and that attractiveness is related to dating frequency
as much as girls are to media presentations of ideal (Bersheid, Dion, Walster, & Walster, 1971; Cawley,
women that endorse thinness as an essential quality. 2001; Halpern et al., 1999; Stake & Lauer, 1987).
It is not clear why boys believe thinness is important Perhaps the finding by Stake and Lauer, that aver-
to their friends to a greater extent than it is to them- age weight women reported dating more frequently
selves, as previous research suggests that adolescents than overweight women, was because their over-
and their friends typically have similar attitudes (e.g., weight participants were at the extreme end of the
Paxton et al., 1999). It may be that they exaggerate BMI continuum. The present sample showed less ex-
the importance of thinness to their friends. This treme BMI values than those of Stake and Lauer,
view is consistent with the contention of Sanderson, but there was still a quite substantial range (BMI
Darley, and Messinger (2002) that attitudes opposed 15–40).
to the thinness norm may not be expressed, as they Data from the present study suggest that, al-
are believed to be counter normative, and conse- though the majority of both girls and boys believe
quently the perceived norm does not accurately that thinness in a girl is at least a little bit important in
represent the private attitudes of group members. determining her dating success, in real terms a girl’s
However, it is also possible that these responses may thinness does not appear to impact on frequency of
reflect a socially desirable reporting bias when boys her dating behavior. This suggests that pressure to
are describing their own beliefs (as they believe the become thin to ensure dating success does not re-
thinness requirement is superficial and should not be sult from observations made in the real world, but
important) that result in an underestimation of their instead comes from other sources, such as media and
attitudes that is not present when they reflect on their peers. Further work is needed to identify the sources
friends’ beliefs. The present data do not allow us to of these messages. In addition, our research does not
resolve this issue, but they do indicate endorsement provide information about whether girls of a particu-
of the thin ideal and that the friendship environment lar body size were more likely to go out with the more
of many boys may reinforce attitudes toward thin- or less popular boys. In relation to the finding that
ness and attractiveness in girls. This finding supports those in the lowest BMI category (<19) were actually
the idea that prevention work with adolescents less likely ever to have had a boyfriend, it is possible
should not simply be focused on the girls themselves, that these thinnest girls were not as developmentally
but rather on the broader social environment. or heterosocially mature as those in the other BMI
Despite apparent endorsement of the thin ideal groups. This finding needs to be confirmed in future
in girls, boys’ definitions of thinness may be differ- research.
ent from that of girls. In the present study, girls sig- In addition, it would be interesting to mea-
nificantly underestimated the body figure size rated sure the BMI of boys in future studies to determine
most attractive by boys. This finding is consistent whether there were similarities in the BMI scores of
with those of both Fallon and Rozin (1985) and Cohn boys and their girl friends. Further, it would be in-
et al. (1987) who studied college aged and adolescent teresting to determine whether more “popular” boys
samples, respectively. Why do girls misjudge the level tended to date thinner girls.
of thinness rated as attractive by boys? Perhaps girls Although primarily of theoretical interest, our
are responding to pressures in addition to that of the findings also have practical implications. In relation
pressure to meet the standard of thinness preferred to the prevention of body dissatisfaction, the mis-
by boys, such as pressures from female peers and the match between boys’ and girls’ perceptions of a thin
media. The sources of information that communicate body could be highlighted in prevention programs.
to girls the degree of slimness desired by boys (e.g., This is especially important in light of the fact that
media images) may be exaggerating. The reason for boys on the whole do appear to endorse the thin
this discrepancy requires further examination. ideal; suggestions to the contrary within an adoles-
In answer to our final research question, no neg- cent environment are not likely to be credible. The
ative linear relationship was found between dating finding that there is not a relationship between dat-
behavior and BMI. Contrary to expectations, the ing behavior and BMI, as often assumed, may also
thinnest BMI group were less likely than the others help dispel the view that thinness is crucial to dating
to report ever having had a boyfriend. These results success and, consequently, assist in alleviating some
were unexpected as past research has suggested that of the pressure to be thin. However, the benefits
Dating and Body Dissatisfaction 673

of challenging the norms that girls hold about to body ideal, including media, peers, and family. This
boys’ perceptions of thinness should be done with research sheds light on a further potential source of
care, as Mutterperl and Sanderson (2002) have found influence: boys’ perceptions of the most attractive
that challenging norm misperceptions about eating body size for girls. Our data suggest that dating be-
behaviors in college women can result in women havior itself was not related to body dissatisfaction
with greater internalization of the thin ideal show- and eating behaviors in mid-adolescent girls. How-
ing greater disordered eating. The social world of ever, belief that thinness is important for attractive-
adolescents is complex, and further research is re- ness to boys was a direct predictor of body dissatis-
quired to determine the best way to communicate faction, and it partially mediated the effects of low
this information. self-esteem and depression on body dissatisfaction
A number of factors limit the interpretation of and fully mediated the relationship between belief
our findings. Dating patterns are likely to vary across in the importance of popularity with boys and body
cultures and, indeed, across subcultures. Thus, the dissatisfaction. Although the data appear to provide
generalization of these findings to other populations some validity for girls’ belief, as boys typically did
cannot be assumed. A further limitation to the gen- report that thinness is an important feature in at-
eralizability of the findings is that, although the re- tractiveness in girls, the ideal figure selected by boys
sponse rate was quite high (85%), we do not have in- was larger than that believed by girls to be ideal to
formation on nonresponders, and thus it is not clear boys. Further, dating behavior was not significantly
whether or not we had a biased sample. There are associated with body size. These latter findings pro-
also a number of limitations related to the measure- vide potential ammunition for challenging prevail-
ment instruments we used. In particular, self-report ing beliefs that contribute to body dissatisfaction in
measures of dating activities were used and their ac- mid-adolescent girls.
curacy cannot be verified. It would also have been
valuable to include a measure of menarche in this
research, as in younger adolescent girls, timing of ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
menarche has been associated with body dissatisfac-
tion. However, due to sensitivities within the school This research was funded by grants from
communities about collecting such personal data, this the Australian Rotary Health Research Fund and
information was not obtained. Although research Victorian Health Promotion Foundation.
suggests that timing of menarche is not associated
with body dissatisfaction in mid-adolescence (Gralen
et al., 1990; Smolak et al., 1993), it would have been
valuable to be able to confirm that in the present REFERENCES
study.
Measurement issues are also pertinent in rela- Banasiak, S. J., Wertheim, E. H., Koerner, J., & Voudorouris, N. J.
(2001). Test-retest reliability and internal consistency of a va-
tion to assessment of body size and figure ratings. riety of measures of dietary restraint and body concerns in
Weight and height were measured in a private room a sample of adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating
during questionnaire completion, and, although par- Disorders, 29, 85–89.
Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients
ticipants had previously agreed to weight and height in family practice: A rapid technique. Postgraduate Medicine,
measurements, this may have increased awareness of 52, 81–85.
body image issues and affected later responses on Beck, A. T., Rial, W. Y., & Rickels, K. (1974). Short form of
the Depression Inventory: Cross validation. Psychological Re-
our measures. In relation to assessment of body fig- ports, 34, 1184–1186.
ure size preferences in boys particularly, social de- Ben-Tovim, D. I., & Walker, K. (1991). The influence of age and
sirability reporting bias may have influenced results. weight on women’s body attitudes as measured by the Body
Attitudes Questionnaire (BAQ). Journal of Psychosomatic
This is suggested by the difference between ratings of Research, 38, 477–481.
the importance of thinness to attractiveness to them- Bersheid, E., Dion, K., Walster, E., & Walster, G. W. (1971). Phys-
selves as compared to the perceived importance of ical attractiveness and dating choice: A test of the match-
ing hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7,
thinness to their friends in determining a girl’s attrac- 173–189.
tiveness. Further research is required to clarify these Byrnes, S. E., Burns, C., & Baur, L. A. (1995, October). A valida-
issues. tion study of the Ben-Tovim-Walker Body Attitudes Question-
naire in girls 12–16 years. Paper presented at the Australian
A range of sociocultural factors have been iden- Society for the Study of Obesity Conference, Melbourne,
tified as potential sources of pressure to fit the thin Australia.
674 Paxton, Norris, Wertheim, Durkin, and Anderson

Cauffman, E., & Steinberg, L. (1996). Interactive effects of menar- Lieberman, M., Gauvin, L., Bukowski, W., & White, D. (2001).
cheal status and dating on dieting and disordered eating Interpersonal influence and disordered eating behaviors in
among adolescent girls. Developmental Psychology, 32, 631– adolescent girls: The role of peer modeling, social reinforce-
635. ment, and body-related teasing. Eating Behaviors, 2, 215–
Cawley, J. (2001). Body weight and the dating and sexual behav- 236.
iors of young adolescents. In R. T. Michael (Ed.), Social awak- Mutterperl, J. A., & Sanderson, C. A. (2002). Mind over matter:
ening: Adolescent behavior as adulthood approaches (pp. 174– Internalization of the thinness norm as a moderator of respon-
198). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. siveness to norm misperception education in college women.
Cohn, L. D., & Adler, N. E. (1992). Female and male percep- Health Psychology, 21, 519–523.
tions of ideal body shapes: Distorted views among Caucasian Nichter, M., & Vuckovic, N. (1994). Fat talk: Body image among
college students. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 16, 69– adolescent females. In N. Sault (Ed.), Many mirrors: Body im-
79. age and social relations (pp. 109–131). New Brunswick, NJ:
Cohn, L. D., Adler, N. E., Irwin, C. E., Jr., Millstein, S. G., Kegeles, Rutgers University Press.
S. M., & Stone, G. (1987). Body-figure preferences in male Paxton, S. J., Schutz, H. K., Wertheim, E. H., & Muir, S. (1999).
and female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, Friendship clique and peer influences on body image con-
276–279. cerns, dietary restraint, extreme weight-loss behaviors, and
Damhorst, M., Littrell, J., & Littrell, M. (1988). Age differences binge eating in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
in adolescent body satisfaction. Journal of Psychology, 121, ogy, 108, 255–266.
553–562. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Measurement of self-esteem. In M. Rosen-
Durkin, S. J., & Paxton, S. J. (2002). Predictors of vulnerability to berg (Ed.), Society and the adolescent self image (pp. 297–307).
reduced body image satisfaction and psychological well-being Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
in response to exposure to idealized female media images in Sanderson, C. A., Darley, J. M., & Messinger, C. S. (2002).
adolescent girls. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 995– “I’m not as thin as you think I am”: The development
1005. and consequences of feeling discrepant from the thinness
Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in perceptions of norm. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 172–
desirable body shape. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 183.
102–105. Schutz, H. K., Paxton, S. J., & Wertheim, E. H. (2002). Investiga-
Forbes, G. B., Adams-Curtis, L. E., Rade, B., & Jaberg, P. (2001). tion of body comparison among adolescent girls. Journal of
Body dissatisfaction in women and men: The role of gender- Applied Psychology, 32, 1906–1937.
typing and self-esteem. Sex Roles, 44, 461–484. Shoemaker, C., Van Strien, T., & Van Der Staak, C. (1994).
Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development Validation of the Eating Disorder Inventory in a non clin-
and validation of a multidimensional Eating Disorder Inven- ical population using transformed and untransformed re-
tory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal sponses. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 387–
of Eating Disorders, 2, 15–34. 393.
Gralen, S. J., Levine, M. P., Smolak, L., & Murnen, S. K. (1990). Shore, R. A., & Porter, J. E. (1990). Normative and reliability
Dieting and disordered eating during early and middle ado- data for 11 to 18 year olds on the Eating Disorder Inventory.
lescence: Do the influences remain the same? International International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 201–207.
Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 501–512. Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987). Moving into adolescence:
Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The experi- The impact of pubertal change and school context. Hawthorne,
mental presentation of thin media images on body dissatisfac- NY: Aldine Press.
tion: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Eating Smolak, L., Levine, M. P., & Gralen, S. (1993). The impact
Disorders, 31, 1–16. of puberty and dating on eating problems among middle
Halpern, C., Udry, R., Campbell, B., & Suchindran, C. school girls. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 355–
(1999). Effects of body fat on weight concerns, dating, 368.
and sexual activity: A longitudinal analysis of Black and Stake, J., & Lauer, M. (1987). The consequences of being over-
White adolescent girls. Developmental Psychology, 35, 721– weight: A controlled study of gender differences. Sex Roles,
736. 17, 31–47.
Keery, H., van den Berg, P., & Thompson, J. K. (2004). An evalu- Stice, E. (2001). A prospective test of the dual-pathway model
ation of the tripartite influence model of body dissatisfaction of bulimic pathology: Mediating effects of dieting and neg-
and eating disturbance with adolescent girls. Body Image, 1, ative affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 124–
237–251. 135.
Keys, A., Fidanza, F., Karvonen, M. J., Kimura, N., & Taylor, H. Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathol-
L. (1972). Indices of relative weight and obesity. Journal of ogy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825–
Chronic Diseases, 25, 329. 848.
Laessle, R. G., Tuschl, R. J., Kotthaus, B. C., & Pirke, K. M. Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge
(1989). A comparison of the validity of three scales for the eating onset in adolescent girls: A 2-year prospective investi-
assessment of dietary restraint. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- gation. Health Psychology, 21, 131–138.
ogy, 98, 504–507. Teri, L. (1982). The use of the Beck Depression Inventory with
Levine, M., & Harrison, K. (2004). Media’s role in the perpetua- adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 277–
tion and prevention of negative body image and disordered 284.
eating. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Handbook of eating dis- Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S.
orders and obesity (pp. 695–717). New York: John Wiley & (1999). Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of
Sons, Inc. body image disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
Levine, M. P., Smolak, L., Moodey, A. F., Shuman, M. D., logical Association.
& Hessen, L. D. (1994). Normative developmental chal- Thompson, J. K., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internalization:
lenges and dieting and eating disturbances in middle school Mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body-image dis-
girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 11– turbance and eating pathology. Current Directions in Psycho-
20. logical Science, 10, 181–183.
Dating and Body Dissatisfaction 675

Van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., & Defares, P. B. book of eating disorders and obesity (pp. 463–494). New York:
(1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) Wiley.
for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating Wertheim, E. H., Paxton, S. J., Maude, D., Szmukler, G. I., Gib-
behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295– bons, K., & Hillier, L. (1992). Psychosocial predictors of ex-
315. treme weight loss behaviors and binge eating in adolescent
Wertheim, E. H., Koerner, J., & Paxton, S. J. (2001). Longitudinal girls and boys. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 12,
predictors of restrictive eating and bulimic tendencies in three 151–160.
different age groups of adolescent girls. Journal of Youth and Wertheim, E. H., Paxton, S. J., Schutz, H. K., & Muir, S. (1997).
Adolescence, 30, 69–81. Why do adolescent girls watch their weight? An interview
Wertheim, E. H., Paxton, S. J., & Blaney, S. (2004). Risk factors for study examining sociocultural pressures to be thin. Journal of
body image dissatisfaction. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Hand- Psychosomatic Research, 42, 345–355.

You might also like