You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 13 (2019) 103–108

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jcbs

From psychological distress to academic procrastination: Exploring the role T


of psychological inflexibility
Nikolett Eisenbecka,∗, David F. Carrenob, Rubén Uclés-Juárezb
a
Karoli Gaspar University of the Reformed Church in Hungary, Budapest, Hungary
b
Department of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain

ABSTRACT

Academic procrastination is generally understood as a problematic emotion regulation practice related to poor mental health. Previous studies have linked it to
higher levels of psychological inflexibility, defined as the rigid dominance of certain psychological reactions over personal values in guiding actions. In order to
discover the exact role of psychological flexibility in procrastination, a sample of 429 university students completed measures of academic procrastination, psy-
chological inflexibility and general psychological distress, comprising depression, anxiety and stress. As predicted, higher levels of procrastination were related to
elevated psychological distress. Both procrastination and psychological distress were associated with psychological inflexibility. Moreover, psychological inflexibility
mediated the relationship between general psychological distress and procrastination. This mediator role was observed in the case of each negative emotional state
(depression, anxiety and stress) that make up general psychological distress. These results indicate that negative emotional states and academic procrastination may
be linked through the problematic self-regulation pattern of psychological inflexibility. The findings thus support the scope of interventions that target both psy-
chological distress and academic procrastination by decreasing psychological inflexibility and promoting values-based actions.

1. Introduction Despite the amount of research dealing with the relationships be-
tween procrastination and psychological problems, their causes and
Academic procrastination can be defined as the voluntary delay of maintaining factors are still not completely understood (e.g., Steel,
beginning or completing important and timely academic tasks 2007). A number of recent studies define it as short-term emotional
(Schouwenburg, 2004; Ziesat, Rosenthal, & White, 1978). It is esti- regulation practice that is characterized by avoidant cognitive tenden-
mated that around 90–95% of the university population procrastinates cies of aversive experiences (e.g., Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996;
(Ellis & Knaus, 2002; Sommer & Haug, 2012), on average between 30% Sirois & Pychyl, 2013). This notion is supported by empirical findings:
and 60% of the time (Rabin, Fogel, & Nutter-Upham, 2011). According procrastination is related to frustration intolerance (Dryden & Dryden,
to Steel (2007), p. 50% of students report that they always delay aca- 2014; Harrington, 2005) and students are more likely to procrastinate if
demic tasks and recognize this behavior as problematic. Not only are they perceive greater control over their mood (Tice, Bratslavsky, &
these numbers alarming, but it has been observed that general pro- Baumeister, 2001). Furthermore, procrastinators appear to show a
crastination has significantly increased over the last few decades (Steel, disjunction between the present and the future self, being less focused
2007; Steel & Ferrari, 2013). on the future, which is partially due to their actual negative mood
Putting off a task may not be troublesome in itself, but problematic (Sirois, 2014a). They tend to turn their attention to experiences that are
procrastination is typically accompanied with the knowledge that one more immediately and emotionally rewarding in order to regulate their
will be worse off doing so (Steel, Brothen, & Wambach, 2001) and it is negative mood (Blunt & Pychyl, 2000; Sirois & Pychyl, 2013) and hence
associated with some type of internal discomfort (Lay & Schouwenburg, become more absorbed in the present moment (Sirois, 2014b). Conse-
1993). Thus, procrastination has found to be heavily linked to poor quently, procrastinators tend to experience an excessive discrepancy
mental health (e.g., Stead, Shanahan, & Neufeld, 2010). In academic between their work intentions and work actions (Steel et al., 2001).
settings, it has been associated with lower academic performance and As has been previously suggested (Glick, Millstein, & Orsillo, 2014;
higher levels of stress, anxiety, irritation, regret, despair and self-blame Scent & Boes, 2014), the above findings may be consistent with a broad
(Burka & Yuen, 1983; Pychyl, Lee, Thibodeau, & Blunt, 2000; theory of regulation of inner experiences, namely the theory of psy-
Rothblum, Solomon, & Murakami, 1986; Sirois, Melia-Gordon, & chological flexibility (e.g., Hayes, 2004; Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda,
Pychyl, 2003; Tice & Baumeister, 1997). & Lillis, 2006; Hayes, Pistorello, & Levin, 2012). In short, psychological


Corresponding author. H-1037, Bécsi street 324, Budapest.
E-mail address: eisenbeck.nikolett@kre.hu (N. Eisenbeck).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.07.007
Received 6 February 2019; Received in revised form 16 June 2019; Accepted 31 July 2019
2212-1447/ © 2019 Association for Contextual Behavioral Science. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
N. Eisenbeck, et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 13 (2019) 103–108

flexibility can be defined as “the ability to contact the present moment that manifest themselves in problematic procrastination. That said,
more fully as a conscious human being, and to either change or persist there is no empirical evidence of this hypothesis so far. The present
when doing so serves valued ends” (Hayes et al., 2006, p. 5). At the study aims to investigate this gap in the literature by assessing the
other end of the spectrum, psychological inflexibility is “the rigid possible mediator role of psychological inflexibility between general
dominance of psychological reactions, over chosen values and con- psychological distress and academic procrastination and between each
tingencies, in guiding action” (Bond et al., 2011, p. 678). It involves of the negative emotional states (depression, anxiety and stress) that
reactions to certain thoughts and emotions (e.g., feelings like high an- constitute general psychological distress.
xiety levels about an exam that can be accompanied with a desire to
reduce them by for instance staying in bed and watching TV shows). 2. Method
Some reactions to these feelings can be problematic as they are in a way
incompatible with those actions that would bring the person closer to 2.1. Participants
what matters in life for them (e.g., spending the whole weekend lying in
bed and forgetting about one's responsibilities instead of studying for The study was conducted at the University of Almería, Spain. A total
the exam that would make the person closer to their dream of becoming of 447 undergraduate students filled in the survey package that con-
a lawyer). As such, psychological inflexibility is a problematic self- tained multiple self-report measures. Five participants did not respond
regulation approach that involves processes related to the inability to to all the questionnaires, meaning their data was not taken into ac-
contact the present more fully, avoidance of unwanted inner experi- count. The final sample consisted of 442 students, with ages ranging
ences, and lack of clarity and commitment to personal values. from 17 to 58 (M = 22.04, SD = 5.24) and comprised 76.2% females
There is growing literature supporting psychological inflexibility as (n = 337) and 23.8% males (n = 105). Most of the participants were
a possible trans-diagnostic process across several psychological dis- psychology majors (n = 331), while the rest of them were studying
orders (Bond et al., 2011; Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010; Levin et al., education (n = 106), history (n = 2) and electrical engineering (n = 3).
2014). Indeed, psychological inflexibility has been found to be func-
tionally associated with depression, anxiety and general psychological 3. Materials
distress, among others (e.g., Bond et al., 2011; Masuda & Tully, 2011;
Venta, Sharp, & Hart, 2012). Previous research has also supported that Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II. The Spanish version
it mediates the relationships between self-concealment (the predis- (Ruiz, Langer Herrera, Luciano, Cangas, & Beltran, 2013) of the Ac-
position of keeping distressing or potentially embarrassing personal ceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) was
information from others) and emotional distress in stressful inter- employed. The AAQ-II contains 7 items to measure psychological in-
personal situations (Masuda, Anderson, Wendell, Chou, Price, & flexibility, the inability to fully connect with the present moment and
Feinstein, 2011), between self-concealment and disordered eating persist in or change behavior in pursuit of personal goals and values.
symptoms (Masuda, Boone, & Timko, 2011), and (together with emo- Participants rate to what extent each statement is true for them, by
tion dysregulation) between neuroticism and depression (Paulus, using a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (never true) to 7 (always true).
Vanwoerden, Norton, & Sharp, 2016). Accordingly, total scores range from 7 to 49, with higher scores in-
Some preliminary data have demonstrated the relationship between dicating greater psychological inflexibility. The AAQ-II is a unidimen-
procrastination and psychological inflexibility. A recent study (Glick sional measure with good psychometric properties: it was significantly
et al., 2014) reported that psychological inflexibility was positively related to general psychopathology and quality of life measures and
associated with both trait anxiety and academic procrastination, adding showed good discriminant validity by differentiating between clinical
to the prediction of academic procrastination over trait anxiety. and nonclinical samples (Ruiz et al., 2013). It also showed a satisfactory
Gagnon, Dionne, and Pychyl (2016) found that general procrastination internal consistency of 0.88 (Ruiz et al., 2013). In the present study,
was related to general psychological distress, acceptance, cognitive Cronbach's alpha was .91, 95% CI [0.90, 0.92].
fusion, attention to the present-moment and committed action. More- Depression Anxiety Stress Scale. The Spanish version of the
over, one of the components of the psychological inflexibility model, Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21; Bados, Solanas, & Andrés,
mindfulness, has been found to mediate the effects of procrastination on 2005; original version by Brown, Chorpita, Korotitsch, & Barlow, 1997)
stress and perceived health (Sirois & Tosti, 2012). Another closely re- was implemented in the present study. Each item on this questionnaire
lated concept, self-compassion (an emotionally positive self-attitude describes a negative emotional state experienced in the last week and
that entails self-kindness, common humanity and mindfulness; Neff, rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (did not apply to me at
2003), was shown to mediate between trait procrastination and stress all) to 3 (applied to me very much, or most of the time). The measure
(Sirois, 2014c). Longitudinal studies have provided support to this ra- consists of 21 items organized into three subscales assessing depression,
tionale. Interventions targeting psychological inflexibility have brought anxiety and stress, with each subscale consisting of seven items. Sub-
about successful reduction in levels of psychopathology and procrasti- scales are created by adding the corresponding items together and
nation among university students (Dionne, 2016; Glick & Orsillo, 2015; multiplying the results by two. The three subscales can be added to-
Scent & Boes, 2014; Wang et al., 2017). These interventions mainly gether creating a score for general psychological distress. Maximum
used techniques based on Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; scores are 42 for the subscales and 126 for the general psychological
Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999), whose underlying mechanism of distress. Higher scores indicate more frequent symptoms. The DASS-21
change is to decrease psychological inflexibility (e.g., Ciarrochi, Bilich, has shown adequate correlations with other questionnaires of anxiety,
& Godsell, 2010). Generally, the aforementioned studies reported depression and positive/negative affect and satisfactory discriminant
moderate effect sizes. validity (Bados et al., 2005). It had good internal reliability with a
Taken altogether, previous data suggest that psychological distress, Cronbach's alpha of .84 for depression, 0.70 for anxiety and 0.82 for
academic procrastination and psychological inflexibility are related stress (Bados et al., 2005). In the present study, alpha values for de-
(e.g., Bond et al., 2011; Gagnon et al., 2016; Glick et al., 2014; Masuda pression, anxiety, stress and general psychological distress were 0.87,
& Tully, 2011; Venta et al., 2012). We hypothesize that the relationship 95% CI [0.85, 0.88], 0.83, 95% CI [0.80, 0.85], 0.83, 95% CI [0.80,
between psychological distress and procrastination is mediated by 0.85] and 0.93, 95% CI [0.91, 0.93], respectively.
psychological inflexibility. That is, higher presence/intensity of un- Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students. The Spanish version
wanted private events (psychological distress) makes it more likely that of the Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students (PASS; Natividad,
they would provoke avoidant tendencies and these inflexible reactions 2014; original version by Solomon & Rothblum, 1984) was used. The
towards unwanted feelings (not the feelings themselves) are the ones PASS is a 44-item self-report questionnaire that is divided into two

104
N. Eisenbeck, et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 13 (2019) 103–108

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations between variables.
PASS AAQ-II DASS-21 DASS-D DASS-A DASS-S Age

PASS (12–60)
AAQ-II (7–49) .335∗∗
DASS-21 (0–126) .228∗∗ .612∗∗
DASS-D (0–42) .188∗∗ .572∗∗ .881∗∗
DASS-A (0–42) .199∗∗ .541∗∗ .898∗∗ .701∗∗
DASS-S (0–42) .219∗∗ .511∗∗ .878∗∗ .629∗∗ .699∗∗
Age .059 .001 .081 .185 .148 .064
Gender .022 .034 .126∗∗ .064 .086 .184∗∗ -.093
M 35.11 22.74 37.98 11.24 10.41 16.32 22.04
SD 7.65 9.36 25.69 10.01 9.15 9.87 5.24
Cronbach's alpha [ 95% CI] .80 [ .77, .83] .91 [ .90, .92] .93 [ .91, .93] .87 [ .85, .88] .83 [ .80, .85] .83 [ .80, .85]

Note: N = 442; *p < .050; **p < .001. All p values are two-tailed.
PASS = Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students; AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; DASS-21 = General psychological distress measured by the
Total Depression Anxiety Stress Scale; DASS-D = Depression subscale of the DASS-21; DASS-A = Anxiety subscale of the DASS-21; DASS-S = Stress subscale of the
DASS-21. Gender: 1 = male, 2 = female.

parts. Only the first part was implemented in this study, measuring biserial correlation coefficient. Gender differences were also assessed
procrastination in six academic areas (writing a term paper, studying with independent sample t-tests and Cohen's d effect sizes were calcu-
for an exam, keeping up with weekly reading assignments, performing lated.
administrative tasks, attending meetings and performing academic Mediation was assessed by a series of multiple regression analyses
tasks in general). Students are asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale on unstandardized variables, using PROCESS, option 4 (Hayes, 2012).
the frequency with which they procrastinate (1 = never procrastinate, We aimed to evaluate whether general psychological distress had a
5 = always procrastinate) and the degree to which procrastination significant indirect effect on procrastination through psychological in-
creates a problem for them (1 = not at all a problem, 5 = always a flexibility (Model 1); depression on procrastination through psycholo-
problem). A total procrastination score is generated by adding together gical inflexibility (Model 2); anxiety on procrastination through psy-
these two subscales (ranging from 12 to 60), with higher scores in- chological inflexibility (Model 3); and stress on procrastination through
dicating greater levels of problematic academic procrastination. The psychological inflexibility (Model 4). The mediation analyses were
third subscale regarding the intention of reducing procrastination was performed using the bootstrapping method with bias-corrected con-
not included, similarly to previous studies (e.g., Glick et al., 2014). The fidence estimates (MacKinnon, Lockwood, & Williams, 2004; Preacher
PASS has shown to be related to student's grades (e.g., Natividad, 2014) & Hayes, 2004). In the present analysis, the 95% confidence interval of
and to levels of depression, anxiety and low self-esteem (Solomon & the indirect effects was obtained with 5000 bootstrap resamples (see:
Rothblum, 1984). It also had good internal consistency of 0.86 (Garzon- Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Bootstrapping is a preferred method for
Umerenkova & Gil-Flores, 2017). Cronbach's alpha of the scale for the mediation analyses, because it overcomes certain limitations, such as
present sample was .80, 95% CI [0.77, 0.83)]. those associated with sampling distribution assumptions (Shrout &
Bolger, 2002).
3.1. Procedure
4. Results
Participants were recruited through in-class announcements. At the
end of each class, students were asked to stay and participate in a short, 4.1. Academic procrastination: prevalence and correlations
15-min study in which they would fill in paper-and-pencil ques-
tionnaires in the classroom. After providing their informed consent, Results showed that procrastination was high in our sample: 32.8%
participants completed a set of questionnaires for demographic data of the students reported almost always or always procrastinating (an-
(age, gender and major), AAQ-II, DASS-21 and PASS. They received no swering 4 or 5) on writing papers, 37.3% on studying for an exam,
compensation for their collaboration and were fully debriefed at the 37.1% on assigned readings, 16.8% on performing administrative tasks,
end of their participation. All procedures were approved by the host 32.3% on attending meetings and 23.5% on performing academic tasks
institution. in general. A total of 61.3% students found their procrastinatory be-
havior almost always or always a problem in at least one of the aca-
3.2. Data analysis demic areas. Total procrastination scores (frequency and problem levels
added together) ranged from 12 to 60, with an average of 35.24
First, we evaluated the required sample size for the mediational (SD = 7.65). These levels are consistent with results found in previous
analyses. According to Fritz and MacKinnon (2007), for the joint sig- studies among college students (e.g., Glick et al., 2014; Natividad,
nificance test, a sample size of 405 or larger was required for 0.8 power. 2014; Onwuegbuzie, 2004; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). Results on
This criterion was met, as our sample size was 442. AAQ-II and DASS-21 scales did not deviate from normative data in the
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS, version 24 (IBM sample (see Bond et al., 2011; Henry & Crawford, 2005).
Corp., New York). Missing data were 0.12% and were classified as being Means scores, standard deviations and inter-correlations for PASS,
‘missing completely at random’ (Little's Missing Completely at Random AAQ-II, DASS-21, age and gender are shown in Table 1. Strong asso-
Test: χ2 = 670.58, df = 689, p =.685). Missing data were replaced ciations were observed between the variables. In particular, higher le-
with the Expectation-Maximization algorithm for each subscale. Cron- vels of psychological inflexibility were related to increased levels of
bach's alphas were computed at 95% confidence intervals to evaluate general psychological distress, depression, anxiety, stress and procras-
the internal consistency of the instruments. Descriptive statistics tination. Also, higher levels of procrastination were associated with
(means, standard variations) and Pearson’ correlation coefficients were elevated levels of general psychological distress, depression, anxiety
calculated. Gender was dichotomously categorized (1 = male, 2 = fe- and stress. No differences were found for age in the assessed variables.
male) for Pearson correlation analyses in order to estimate point Women, however, showed more stress, t(440) = −4.21, p < .001,

105
N. Eisenbeck, et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 13 (2019) 103–108

Table 2 In the case of Model 1 (see Table 2 and Fig. 1 for unstandardized
Mediation analyses. coefficients), general psychological distress was significantly related to
a path (X → b path (M → Direct effect (c’ Indirect effect (ab psychological inflexibility (a path) and psychological inflexibility was
M) Y) path) 95% CI path) 95% CI also related to procrastination (b path). As the direct effect was not
significant and the 95% confidence interval for indirect effect did not
Model 1: X (DASS-21) → Y (PASS) mediated by M (AAQ-II)
include zero, we can conclude indirect-only mediation of the psycho-
R2 adj = .11
F (2, 439) = 27.90**
logical distress’ effect on problematic procrastination through psycho-
b .22 .25 .01 [- .02, .04] .06 [.04, .08] logical inflexibility. Further analyses showed that psychological in-
SE .01 .05 .02 .01 flexibility mediated the relationship between depression and
t 16.23** 5.48** .66 procrastination (Model 2), between anxiety and procrastination (Model
Model 2: X (DASS-D)→ Y (PASS) mediated by M (AAQ-II)
2 3) and between stress and procrastination (Model 4). However, each
R adj = .11
F (2, 439) = 27.66** model explains a relatively low percent of variance (11%) of academic
b [CI] .54 .26 - .00 [- .09, .08] .15 [.10, .20] procrastination (see Table 2).
SE .04 .05 .04 .03
t 14.64** 6.15** - .10
5. Discussion
Model 3: X (DASS-A) → Y (PASS) mediated by M (AAQ-II)
2
R adj = .11
F (2, 439)= 27.78** The present study aimed to explore procrastination among under-
b [CI] .55 .26 .02 [- .07, .11] .15 [.10, .20] graduate students and evaluate the relationships between academic
SE .04 .04 .04 .03 procrastination, psychological distress and psychological inflexibility,
t 13.51** 6.00** .48
the latter being proposed as a mediator of the relationship between the
Model 4: X (DASS-S) → Y (PASS) mediated by M (AAQ-II)
R2 adj = .11 first two.
F (2, 439) = 28.52** The participants showed high levels of academic procrastination,
b [CI] .48 .25 .05 [- .03, .13] .12 [.08, .17] the majority of them reaching problematic levels, indicating that aca-
SE .04 .04 .04 .02 demic procrastination is a widespread problem among undergraduate
t 12.47** 5.77** 1.24
students (e.g., Glick et al., 2014; Natividad, 2014; Onwuegbuzie, 2004;
Notes: N = 442; *p < .050; **p < .001. All p values are two-tailed. Coefficients Solomon & Rothblum, 1984; Steel, 2007). Academic procrastination
are unstandardized. PASS = Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students; AAQ-II was found to be strongly correlated to general psychological distress
= Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; DASS-21 = General psychological and its subscales, namely depression, anxiety and stress (for similar
distress measured by the Total Depression Anxiety Stress Scale; DASS- results, see Beck, Koons, & Milgrim, 2000; Bui, 2007; Dryden, 2012;
D = Depression subscale of the DASS-21; DASS-A = Anxiety subscale of the Gagnon et al., 2016; Glick et al., 2014; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). In
DASS-21; DASS-S = Stress subscale of the DASS-21. this sample, levels of stress and general psychological distress were
higher among females than males, consistent with earlier data
(Rosenfield & Mouzon, 2013). In line with previous studies, psycholo-
gical inflexibility was related both to general psychological distress and
its subdivisions, depression, anxiety and stress (Bond et al., 2011) as
well as to procrastination (Gagnon et al., 2016; Glick et al., 2014).
In addition, the study provided evidence for the first time in the
literature that psychological inflexibility mediated the relationship
between general psychological distress and academic procrastination.
This mediation effect was also found for each of the subscales that
comprise general psychological distress, namely depression, anxiety
and stress. These findings suggest that the relationship between these
negative emotional states and academic procrastination may be estab-
Fig. 1. Indirect effect of general psychological distress on procrastination lished through psychological inflexibility, that is, psychological distress
through psychological inflexibility. *p < .050; **p < .001. is only related to procrastination if it the latter serves inflexible ten-
dencies of dealing with the distress.
These results are consistent with the literature on psychological
d = 0.44, 95% CI [0.22, 0.66] and general psychological distress, t inflexibility. According to this approach, responding in an inflexible
(440) = −2.67, p = .008, d = 0.30 95% CI [0.08, 0.52], than men (for manner implies not accepting one's own experiences as they are and
mean values, see Table 1). The findings that stress levels and the living a life that is not in line with what the person truly desires. In this
combined scores of depression, anxiety and stress (general psycholo- regard, what ultimately accounts for the lack of committed actions to-
gical distress) are higher for women are consistent with the literature as wards one's values are not the negative feelings and thoughts per se, but
women tend to suffer more from internalizing disorders, such as de- the way one responds to them (e.g., Hayes et al., 2006). Students en-
pression, anxiety and stress (e. g., Rosenfield & Mouzon, 2013). gaging in problematic procrastination might be delaying their academic
tasks not only because they experience negative emotional states, but
4.2. Psychological inflexibility as a mediator between general psychological because they have a pattern of reacting to such experiences with
distress and academic procrastination avoidance. Thus, the psychological inflexibility account also explains
why there are some students who do not engage in problematic pro-
To test whether the association between mental health problems crastinatory behaviors, despite feeling depressed, anxious or stressed.
and academic procrastination could be explained by psychological in- These students may respond to their negative emotions with accep-
flexibility, several mediation analyses were carried out. Model 1 re- tance, sustaining their focus on what really matters in their lives despite
presents the analysis assessing the mediator role of psychological in- the presence of psychological distress.
flexibility between general psychological distress and procrastination; Nonetheless, as this was a cross-sectional study, the causal direction
Model 2 between depression and procrastination; Model 3 between of the relationships cannot be confirmed. It could be argued that the
anxiety and procrastination; and Model 4 between stress and procras- relationship between psychological problems and academic procrasti-
tination. nation mediated by psychological inflexibility can be bidirectional. In

106
N. Eisenbeck, et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 13 (2019) 103–108

other words, procrastinating, when it serves inflexible tendencies, direction as it has provided novel empirical evidence of psychological
paradoxically can increase the level of unwanted thoughts and feelings inflexibility as an underlying mechanism of procrastination.
(for similar explanations see Sirois, 2004; Sirois & Pychyl, 2013; Sirois
& Tosti, 2012; Steel, 2007). It is quite possible that high levels of psy- Funding
chological inflexibility can create a vicious circle dominated by un-
wanted experiences and their avoidance, preventing the person from This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in
living a meaningful life. the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Previous empirical findings have provided partial support of this
theory. Psychological flexibility and values-based actions have been Conflicts of interest
found to be predictors of procrastination (Gagnon et al., 2016; Glick
et al., 2014) and interventions aiming to increase psychological in- Nikolett Eisenbeck, David F. Carreno and Rubén Uclés-Juárez de-
flexibility in order to reduce procrastination were successful (Dionne, clare that they have no conflict of interest.
2016; Mullen, 2014; Wang et al., 2017). Moreover, there is extensive
literature which perceives procrastination as a short-term mood re- Ethical approval
pairing self-regulatory strategy that is likely to have negative con-
sequences for the future (e.g., Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996; Sirois, All procedures performed in studies involving human participants
2004; Sirois & Pychyl, 2013; Steel, 2007). There is some support that were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/
procrastination is linked to mental health problems, but only when or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration
procrastinators react to their feelings and thoughts with maladaptive and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
control strategies. Among these strategies are, for instance, high levels
of absorption (Sirois, 2014b), negative self-judgments (Sirois, 2014c), Informed consent
low levels of certain executive functions (Rabin et al., 2011), and low
levels of mindfulness (Sirois & Tosti, 2012). The psychological inflex- Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants in-
ibility approach may form a theoretical framework that integrates the cluded in the study.
set of previous psychological processes and may explain their connec-
tions to procrastination. Psychological inflexibility refers not only to the References
general inability to coexist with unwanted experiences, but it also pays
special attention to the role of commitment to personal values, by Bados, A., Solanas, A., & Andrés, R. (2005). Psychometric properties of the Spanish
providing a theory of how the relationship that one establishes with version of depression, anxiety and stress scales (DASS). Psicothema, 17(4).
Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: An overview.
their own inner experiences can ultimately encourage or impede the Psychological Inquiry, 7(1), 1–15.
living of a meaningful life. Beck, B. L., Koons, S. R., & Milgrim, D. L. (2000). Correlates and consequences of beha-
Despite the statistically significant and theoretically supported re- vioral procrastination: The effects of academic procrastination, self-consciousness,
self-esteem and self-handicapping. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 15(5), 3.
sults, there are a number of limitations of the study that warrant Blunt, A. K., & Pychyl, T. A. (2000). Task aversiveness and procrastination: A multi-di-
mention. Common method variance, which is attributable to the mea- mensional approach to task aversiveness across stages of personal projects.
surement method, is a potential limitation of this paper similarly to Personality and Individual Differences, 28(1), 153–167.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., ...
other studies in the field of behavioral research. External validity of the Zettle, R. D. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the acceptance and ac-
findings may be limited, as the sample was mainly composed of female tion questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experi-
psychology students. More research is needed regarding whether these ential avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 676–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.
2011.03.007.
results are replicable in different student populations (e.g., males, other
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Psychometric
university mayors or high school students) or in different contexts of properties of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) in clinical samples. Behaviour
procrastination (implementing for example Lay's General Research and Therapy, 35(1), 79–89.
Procrastination Scale; Lay, 1986). Bui, N. H. (2007). Effect of evaluation threat on procrastination behavior. The Journal of
Social Psychology, 147(3), 197–209. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.147.3.197-209.
The use of self-report measures and the cross-sectional nature of the Burka, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (1983). Procrastination: Why you do it, what to do about it.
study makes it difficult to establish causal links and directionality be- Reading, PA: Addison-Wesley.
tween the analyzed variables. Future studies may favor the use of Ciarrochi, J., Bilich, L., & Godsel, C. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a mechanism of
change in acceptance and commitment therapy. In R. Baer (Ed.). Assessing mindfulness
longitudinal design, experimental settings and behavioral measures in and acceptance: Illuminating the processes of change (pp. 51–76). Oakland, CA: New
order to determine causality. With the use of different self-report Harbinger Publications, Inc.
questionnaires with regard to depression, anxiety and stress, different Dionne, F. (2016). Using acceptance and mindfulness to reduce procrastination among
university students: Results from a pilot study. Revista Prâksis, 1, 8–20.
results might have been obtained. Likewise, it may be relevant to in- Dryden, W. (2012). Dealing with procrastination: The REBT approach and a demon-
troduce more questionnaires to the analysis that measure concepts stration session. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 30(4),
potentially linked either to psychological inflexibility or academic 264–281.
Dryden, P. P. S. W., & Dryden, W. (2014). Dealing with procrastination: An integrated cbt
procrastination. approach. London: Routledge.
In spite of these limitations, findings of this study have several Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. J. (2002). Overcoming procrastination. New York: New American
implications. As psychological inflexibility is associated with academic Library.
Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated
procrastination and general psychological distress, its measurement
effect. Psychological Science, 18(3), 233–239. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.
may help to identify those students at risk of both. It may encourage the 2007.01882.x.
design of interventions that target psychological inflexibility among Gagnon, J., Dionne, F., & Pychyl, T. A. (2016). Committed action: An initial study on its
college students in order to reduce procrastination and at the same time association to procrastination in academic settings. Journal of Contextual Behavioral
Science, 5(2), 97–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.04.002.
promote mental health. The few ACT and mindfulness-based interven- Garzon-Umerenkova, A., & Gil-Flores, J. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Spanish
tions in existence have shown promising results in diminishing pro- version of the test procrastination assessment scale-students (PASS). Revista
crastination, but taking into account its prevalence and the possible Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación, 1(43), 149–163.
Glick, D. M., Millstein, D. J., & Orsillo, S. M. (2014). A preliminary investigation of the
serious health and economic consequences, more studies are necessary. role of psychological inflexibility in academic procrastination. Journal of Contextual
In order to effectively tailor these interventions, it is crucial to under- Behavioral Science, 3(2), 81–88.
stand the characteristics of procrastination and its maintaining factors. Glick, D. M., & Orsillo, S. M. (2015). An investigation of the efficacy of acceptance-based
behavioral therapy for academic procrastination. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
The authors consider that the present study is an important step in this

107
N. Eisenbeck, et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 13 (2019) 103–108

General, 144(2), 400–409. Rosenfield, S., & Mouzon, D. (2013). Gender and mental health. In C. S. Aneshensel, J. C.
Harrington, N. (2005). It's too difficult! Frustration intolerance beliefs and procrastina- Phelan, & A. Bierman (Eds.). Handbook of the sociology of mental health (pp. 277–296).
tion. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(5), 873–883. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.
j.paid.2004.12.018. Rothblum, E. D., Solomon, L. J., & Murakami, J. (1986). Affective, cognitive, and beha-
Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and the new behavior vioral differences between high and low procrastinators. Journal of Counseling
therapies: Mindfulness, acceptance and relationship. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette, & Psychology, 33(4), 387.
M. Linehan (Eds.). Mindfulness and acceptance: Expanding the cognitive behavioral tra- Ruiz, F. J., Langer Herrera, A. I., Luciano, C., Cangas, A. J., & Beltran, I. (2013).
dition (pp. 1–29). New York: Guilford Press. Measuring experiential avoidance and psychological inflexibility: The Spanish ver-
Hayes, A. F. (2012). Process: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, sion of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire - II. Psicothema, 25(1), 123–129.
moderation, and conditional process modeling [white paper]. Retrieved from http:// https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2011.239.
www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf. Scent, C. L., & Boes, S. R. (2014). Acceptance and commitment training: A brief inter-
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and vention to reduce procrastination among college students. Journal of College Student
commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Psychotherapy, 28(2), 144–156.
Therapy, 44(1), 1–25. Schouwenburg, H. C. (2004). Academic procrastination: Theoretical notions, measure-
Hayes, S. C., Pistorello, J., & Levin, M. E. (2012). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy ment, and research. In C. H. Schouwenburg, C. Lay, T. A. Pychyl, & J. R. Ferrari
as a unified model of behavior change. The Counseling Psychologist, 40(7), 976–1002. (Eds.). Counseling the procrastinator in academic settings (pp. 3–17). Washington, DC:
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An American Psychological Association.
experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press. Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental stu-
Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short‐form version of the Depression Anxiety dies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7(4), 422–445.
Stress Scales (DASS‐21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non‐clinical Sirois, F. M. (2004). Procrastination and counterfactual thinking: Avoiding what might
sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(2), 227–239. have been. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43(Pt 2), 269–286. https://doi.org/10.
IBM Corp Released (2016). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 24.0. Armonk, NY: IBM 1348/0144666041501660.
Corp. Sirois, F. M. (2014a). Out of sight, out of time? A meta-analytic investigation of pro-
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect crastination and time perspective. European Journal of Personality, 28(5), 511–520.
of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 865–878. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1947.
2010.03.001. Sirois, F. M. (2014b). Absorbed in the moment? An investigation of procrastination, ab-
Lay, C. H. (1986). At last, my research article on procrastination. Journal of Research in sorption and cognitive failures. Personality and Individual Differences, 71, 30–34.
Personality, 20(4), 474–495. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.016.
Lay, C. H., & Schouwenburg, H. C. (1993). Trait procrastination, time management, and Sirois, F. M. (2014c). Procrastination and stress: Exploring the role of self-compassion.
academic behavior. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 8(4), 647. Self and Identity, 13(2), 128–145.
Levin, M. E., MacLane, C., Daflos, S., Seeley, J. R., Hayes, S. C., Biglan, A., et al. (2014). Sirois, F. M., Melia-Gordon, M. L., & Pychyl, T. A. (2003). “I'll look after my health, later”:
Examining psychological inflexibility as a transdiagnostic process across psycholo- An investigation of procrastination and health. Personality and Individual Differences,
gical disorders. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3(3), 155–163. https://doi. 35(5), 1167–1184. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00326-4.
org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.06.003. Sirois, F. M., & Pychyl, T. A. (2013). Procrastination and the priority of short‐term mood
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the regulation: Consequences for future self. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate 7(2), 115–127.
Behavioral Research, 39(1), 99–128. Sirois, F. M., & Tosti, N. (2012). Lost in the moment? An investigation of procrastination,
Masuda, A., Anderson, P. L., Wendell, J. W., Chou, Y.-Y., Price, M., & Feinstein, A. B. mindfulness, and well-being. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior
(2011). Psychological flexibility mediates the relations between self-concealment and Therapy, 30(4), 237–248.
negative psychological outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(2), Solomon, L. J., & Rothblum, E. D. (1984). Academic procrastination: Frequency and
243–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.037. cognitive-behavioral correlates. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31(4), 503–509.
Masuda, A., Boone, M. S., & Timko, C. A. (2011). The role of psychological flexibility in https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.503.
the relationship between self-concealment and disordered eating symptoms. Eating Sommer, L., & Haug, M. (2012). What influences implementation intentions in an aca-
Behaviors, 12(2), 131–135. demic learning context-the roles of goal intentions, procrastination, and experience.
Masuda, A., & Tully, E. C. (2011). The role of mindfulness and psychological flexibility in International Journal of Higher Education, 1(1), 32–61.
somatization, depression, anxiety, and general psychological distress in a nonclinical Stead, R., Shanahan, M. J., & Neufeld, R. W. J. (2010). “I’ll go to therapy, eventually”:
college sample. Journal of Evidence-based Complementary & Alternative Medicine, 17(1), Procrastination, stress and mental health. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(3),
66–71. https://doi.org/10.1177/2156587211423400. 175–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.028.
Mullen, A. (2014). The role of psychological flexibility in procrastination. (master's thesis) Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of
Lafayette, LA: University of Louisiana at Lafayette. quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 65–94. https://
Natividad, L. A. (2014). Análisis de la procrastinación en estudiantes universitarios. (doctoral doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65.
dissertation)Valencia, Spain: Universitat de València. Steel, P., Brothen, T., & Wambach, C. (2001). Procrastination and personality, perfor-
Neff, K. (2003). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude mance, and mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(1), 95–106. https://doi.
toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2(2), 85–101. org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00013-1.
Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Academic procrastination and statistics anxiety. Assessment & Steel, P., & Ferrari, J. (2013). Sex, education and procrastination: An epidemiological
Evaluation in Higher Education, 29(1), 3–19. study of procrastinators' characteristics from a global sample. European Journal of
Paulus, D. J., Vanwoerden, S., Norton, P. J., & Sharp, C. (2016). Emotion dysregulation, Personality, 27(1), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1851.
psychological inflexibility, and shame as explanatory factors between neuroticism Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, perfor-
and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 376–385. https://doi.org/10. mance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science,
1016/j.jad.2015.10.014. 8(6), 454–458. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00460.x.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation
effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, 36(4), 717–731. takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it!. Journal of Personality
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and Social Psychology, 80(1), 53–67.
and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Venta, A., Sharp, C., & Hart, J. (2012). The relation between anxiety disorder and ex-
Methods, 40(3), 879–891. periential avoidance in inpatient adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 24(1), 240.
Pychyl, T. A., Lee, J. M., Thibodeau, R., & Blunt, A. (2000). Five days of emotion: An Wang, S., Zhou, Y., Yu, S., Ran, L., Liu, X., & Chen, Y. (2017). Acceptance and commit-
experience sampling study of undergraduate student procrastination. Journal of Social ment therapy and cognitive–behavioral therapy as treatments for academic pro-
Behavior & Personality, 15(5), 239. crastination: A randomized controlled group session. Research on Social Work Practice,
Rabin, L. A., Fogel, J., & Nutter-Upham, K. E. (2011). Academic procrastination in college 27(1), 48–58.
students: The role of self-reported executive function. Journal of Clinical and Ziesat, H. A., Jr., Rosenthal, T. L., & White, G. M. (1978). Behavioral self-control in
Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 344–357. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395. treating procrastination of studying. Psychological Reports, 42(1), 59–69. https://doi.
2010.518597. org/10.2466/pr0.1978.42.1.59.

108

You might also like