You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1991 by ihe American Psychological Associaiion, Inc.

1991, Vol.60, No. 2, 241-253 0022-3514/91/S3.O0

Close Relationships as Including Other in the Self


Arthur Aron, Elaine N. Aron, Michael Tudor, and Greg Nelson
University of California, Santa Cruz

The cognitive significance of being in a close relationship is described in terms of including other in
the self (in Lewin's sense of overlapping regions of the life space and in James's sense of the self as
resources, perspectives, and characteristics). Experiment 1, adapting Liebrand's (1984) decom-
posed-game procedures, found less self/other difference in allocations of money to a friend than to
a stranger, regardless of whether Ss expected other to know their allocations. Experiment 2, adapt-
ing Lord's (1987) procedures, found that Ss recalled fewer nouns previously imaged with self or
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mother than nouns imaged with a nonclose other, suggesting that mother was processed more like
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

self than a stranger. Experiment 3, adapting self-schema, reaction-time procedures (eg., Markus,
1977), found longer latencies when making "me/not me" decisions for traits that were different
between self and spouse versus traits that were similar for both, suggesting a self/other confusion
with spouse.

During the 1980s, close-relationship research expanded rap- tion of each member's two-person outcome matrix (that is, how
idly, rating its own Handbook (Duck, 1988) and its own Annual rewards or costs to partner are expected to affect self). Specifi-
Review of Psychology chapter (Clark & Reis, 1988). Much of the cally, Kelley and Thibaut argued that members of a close rela-
research in this area does not explicitly define what is meant by tionship each have a pattern of perceived interdependence of
a close relationship. However, the behavioral, systemic defini- outcomes in which partner's and joint benefits are expected in
tion offered by Kelley et al. (1983) has been widely influential. the long run to benefit self. Similarly, Clark and Mills (1979)
It focuses on mutual influence, interdependence, and degree of described a close relationship as having a communal character,
interconnectedness of activities. This approach recently served in which the partners are each motivated to act for the needs of
as the basis for the development of a measure of interpersonal- the other, regardless of the expected reciprocal outcome for the
closeness behavior (Berscheid, Snyder, & Omoto, 1989a, self. This general approach is also similar to the empathy model
1989b), which focuses on time spent together, diversity of shared for explaining prosocial behavior andfindingsshowing greater
activities, and perceived influence of other over one's own deci- helping for those with whom the person is in a close relation-
sions. (Maxwell, 1985, also developed a behavioral measure of ship (e.g., Clark, 1983). Wegner (1980) suggested that empathy
closeness, which is based on a more general review of the close- may "stem in part from a basic confusion between ourselves
relationship literature.) and others" (p. 133), which he considered may arise from an
There has been much less consensus about the cognitive sig- initial lack of differentiation between self and caregiver in in-
nificance of such behavioral interdependence for each person fancy (Hoffman, 1976).
in a close relationship. Yet a number of relevant phenomena 2. Closeness as a changed actor/observer perspective. Several
have been observed, mostly falling into one of three overlap- current social psychological approaches emphasize differences
ping categories: between the perspective people have of their own versus others'
1. Closeness as a changed resource allocation strategy. Kelley behavior; some of the associated research suggests that such
and Thibaut (1978; Kelley, 1983) saw the cognitive conse- differences are less when other is in a close relationship to self.
quences for the members of a close relationship as a transforma- For example, actor-observer discrepancies in attributional pro-
cesses (Jones & Nisbett, 1971) appear to be less when other is in
a close relationship to self (e.g., Sande, Goethals, & Radloff,
Preliminary analyses of some of the data in this article were pre- 1988). Another approach emphasizes the categorization of self
sented at the 1987 meeting of the Western Psychological Association in and close other(s) into a single cognitive category (e.g., Hogg &
Long Beach, California; the 1987 Iowa Conference on Personal Rela- Turner, 1987), extending ideas originally developed in the con-
tionships in Iowa City; the 1988 meeting of the Western Psychological text of the in-group/out-group distinction in intergroup rela-
Association in San Francisco; the 1988 International Conference on tions. A number of otherfindingsalso suggest a change in per-
Interaction and Close Relationships in Nags Head, North Carolina; spective regarding memory processes. Brenner (1973) found
and the 1988 International Conference on Personal Relationships in
Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. that memory of a mate's or romantic partner's performance in a
laboratory task was intermediate between memory of one's
We are grateful to Jennifer Collins, Mary Dundon, Frank Myers,
Elena Quintana, Paul Sanders, David Verdugo, and Tanya Webber for own and memory of a stranger's performance. Bower and Gilli-
their assistance in conducting this research and to Anthony Pratkanis gan (1979) found that associating trait adjectives with remem-
for his suggestions on the article. bered episodes in one's mother's life facilitated later recall
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to equally well as when they were associated with remembered
Arthur Aron, Kerr Hall, University of California, Santa Cruz, Califor- episodes in one's own life and that a comparison between associ-
nia 95064. ating traits with Walter Cronkite versus with self yielded much
241
242 ARON, ARON, TUDOR, AND NELSON

better recall for the self condition. Festinger, Pepitone, and


Newcomb (1952) found that ability to distinguish who said what
when remembering a group interaction is poorer when one is
ego involved in the group interaction. The more general idea
that people in close relationships experience a sense of we-ness
(eg., Hatfield, 1982; McDonald, 1981), an idea reminiscent of
Heider's (1958) concept of a cognitive "unit relation" is also
relevant.
3. Closeness as vicariously sharing other's characteristics.
This idea is traditionally emphasized in Freudian notions of
identification. Although identification is mainly associated Figure 1. Pictorial representation of including other in the self in a
with parent-child closeness, it has also been used in other con- close relationship. Adapted from Attraction in Relationship: A New
texts. For example, Bank and Kahn (1982) used identification Look at Interpersonal Attraction (p. 5) by G Levinger and J. D. Snoek,
1972, Marristown, New Jersey: General Learning Press. Copyright
to describe lifelong sibling relationships, and Reik (1944) ar-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1972 by Levinger and Snoek. Adapted by permission.


gued that people seek romantic partners who possess those
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

characteristics lacking in their own ego ideal to attain those


characteristics indirectly. In conceptually related social psycho-
logical work, Tesser (1988) showed that under conditions in "double being" and Schutz's (1970) reference to two people "liv-
which one is not competing with one's partner, one is more ing in each other's subjective contexts of meaning" (p. 167). In a
likely to reflect the pleasure of a partner's achievement if that similar vein, Bakan (1966) wrote about "communion" in the
partner is "close." And Deutsch and Mackesy (1985) interpreted context of his expansion on Buber's (1937) "I-thou" relation-
the established findings of self-partner similarities in close re- ship.
lationships as because of mutual influence on each other's self- The principle is that in a close relationship, the person acts as
schemata, creating an overlap of traits between them. if some or all aspects of the partner are partially the person's
In the preceding attempt to extract the existing literature's own. (There may in addition be some sense of a general in-
understanding of cognition in close relationships, we found ei- crease of fusion of self and other.) Aron and Aron (1986) empha-
ther approaches that emphasize interaction in close relation- sized that three aspects of self seem to be involved in this pro-
ships, with cognition as a secondary focus, or approaches that cess: resources, perspectives, and characteristics. This categori-
emphasize social cognition more generally, with cognition rele- zation relies in part on James's (1890/1948) influential division
vant to close relationships as a secondary focus. These ap- of the "empirical self " into material, social, and spiritual—the
proaches represent a considerable diversity of methodologies latter meaning typical styles of thinking or what Greenwald
and theoretical orientations. Yet, although it has not been recog- and Pratkanis (1984) described as self-processes or procedural
nized or articulated, there is a remarkable overarching commu- knowledge. These three categories of aspects of self correspond
nality of theme: In each case, these approaches yield results or in a general way to the three categories of cognitive implications
descriptions regarding close relationships that fall on a contin- of closeness suggested in the previous literature, as reviewed
uum, with their findings about self-cognition or typical de- earlier. Specifically, to the extent a partner is perceived as part
scriptions of self-cognition at one end and their findings or of one's self, allocation of resources is communal (because bene-
typical descriptions of cognitions about strangers at the other. fiting other is benefiting self), actor/observer perspective differ-
These in-between results and descriptions arise, we think, be- ences are lessened, and other's characteristics become
cause much of our cognition about the other in a close relation- one's own.
ship is cognition in which the other is treated as self or confused This approach is also generally consistent with much current
with self—the underlying reason being a self/other merging or, work on intimacy, a concept often considered virtually synony-
as Aron and Aron (1986) put it, "including others in the self" mous with closeness (e.g., Helgeson, Shaver, & Dyer, 1987). For
(P. 19). example, Reis and Shaver's (1988) integration of previous re-
Of course, this general idea has been expressed by a variety of search findings emphasized the centrality of the exchange of
theorists. For example, the idea of close relationships as includ- self-relevant information, and McAdam's (1988) integration ar-
ing other in the self is elegantly expressed in Levinger and gued that most definitions of intimacy "converge on the central
Snoek's (1972) Venn diagrams, an adapted version of which is idea of sharing that which is inmost with others" (p. 18).
shown in Figure 1. A more complex version was proposed by More generally still, a wide variety of thinkers on close rela-
Lewin (1948, p. 90), who diagrammed relationships within the tionships use metaphors such as union and attachment. Bataille
life space in terms of differing degrees of overlap between the (1962) expressed it dramatically: "Between one being and an-
differentiated region that represents the self and the region that other there is a gulf, a discontinuity" (p. 12). "What we desire is
to the self represents the partner. to bring into a world founded on discontinuity all the continuity
This general notion of overlapping selves involves an under- such a world can sustain" (p. 19). Jung (1925/1959) emphasized
standing similar to what Greenwald and Pratkanis (1984) call the role of relationship partners as providing unavailable
the "collective" aspect of self. It is also related to Ickes, Tookc, aspects of self, so as to help make the self whole. Maslow(1967)
Stinson, Baker, and Bissonnette's (1988) idea of "intersubjec- took it for granted that "beloved people can be incorporated
tivity," which Ickes and his colleagues made vivid by citing into the self " (p. 103). And McCall (1974) described attachment
Merleau-Ponty's (1945) description of a close relationship as a as "incorporation of . . . [the other's] actions and reac-
CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS AS INCLUDING OTHER IN THE SELF 243

tions . . . into the content of one's various conceptions of the (1986) and others suggest to be included in the self in close
self" (p. 219). Finally, the related notion of "possessing" the relationships: resources, perspective, and characteristics. In
other (you are mine; I am yours) has been part of classic system- each case, we took a particular successful existing research par-
atic treatments of love (e.g., Berl, 1924; Freud, 1921/1951; adigm that emphasized self/other differences and adapted it to
Grant, 1976). As Reik (1949) put it, when we love we desire "to test whether the self/other difference was reduced when self and
own the other person" (p. 73). other were in a close relationship. Also, different types of close
Clearly there is ample precedent for thinking of close rela- relationships (friendship, parent-child, marriage) were used in
tionships as including other in the self. However, this idea has the several experiments.
not been made explicit and treated seriously as a conceptual Thus, Experiment 1 used a modified version of Liebrand's
framework for either forming an integrated understanding of (1984) decomposed-game measure, comparing allocations of
existing relevant social psychology research or for generation of money to self or other under conditions in which other was
new research. either a stranger, a friendly acquaintance, or a best friend. Ex-
In fact, in spite of the many roots of the idea and the large periment 2 adapted Lord's (1987) procedure, comparing free
body of data consistent with it, this understanding of close rela- recall of nouns originally imaged in connection with either self,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tionships as including other in the self represents a radically mother, or a nonclose other. (Lord, 1987, conceptualized self/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

different approach from the exchange, psychoanalytic, role and other differences in his paradigm as resulting from habitual
systems approaches that have overwhelmingly dominated differences infigure/groundperspective, so that our prediction
thinking in thefield(see Aron & Aron, 1986, for a review). And was that mother-imaged words would function most like self-
the data cited earlier that are consistent with this view were imaged words.) Finally, Experiment 3 adapted the reaction-time
from studies not explicitly designed to examine hypotheses de- procedure used by Markus (1977), Rogers, Kuiper, and Kirker
rived from this position and were each conducted and under- (1977), and others to compare latencies in making me/not me
stood within the confines of particular, different methodologi- choices for trait adjectives differentially relevant to self, spouse,
cal traditions. Thus the purpose of the present article is to illus- and a nonclose other. (The idea was that there should be more
trate the fruitfulness of this approach of considering close confusions, hence slower reaction times, for traits that are true
relationships in terms of including other in the self: to demon- of spouse but not of self or true of self but not of spouse.)
strate its ability to generate original hypotheses that can be Although it may seem possible to interpret the results of any
rigorously operationalized using highly diverse methodologies one of these experiments within a narrower theoretical context,
and its ability to yield results supportive of those hypotheses. it is the set of studies as a whole that is intended to show the
The present research effort was originally suggested by Aron utility of the inclusion-of-other-in-the-self approach.
and Aron's (1986) self-expansion model, which holds that peo-
ple are motivated to enter and maintain close relationships to Experiment 1
expand the self by including the resources, perspectives, and
characteristics of the other in the self. The model is primarily We argued earlier that to the extent other is included in the
motivational; previous research on the model (Aron, Aron, & self, other's resources are self *s resources. Thus people in a close
Allen, 1989; Aron, Dutton, Aron, & Iverson, 1989; McKenna, relationship should make less of a distinction between self and
1989; Paris, 1990; Rousar, 1990) has examined its implications other when allocating resources between self and other. Further-
in this context. Nevertheless, the model also clearly implies that more, because this lessened self/other difference is suggested to
a cognitive inclusion of other in the self actually occurs and result from perceiving other's resources as one's own (as op-
shapes the way information is processed about the develop- posed to expecting direct reciprocation of benefits), it should
ment, maintenance, and dissolution of relationships. However, occur regardless of whether self believes other will be aware of
this article is not specifically focused on the self-expansion the choices self makes.
model but on the general paradigm of viewing close relation- Experiment 1 explored these proposals using a modified ver-
ships in terms of including other in the self. sion of a procedure originally developed by Liebrand (1984).
The precise nature of what it means to have included other Liebrand^ decomposed-game procedure was intended to ex-
into self, in terms of cognitive structure, is probably multifold. tract the key elements of typical social psychology game inter-
For example, cognitive representations of self and other might action tasks. On a computer screen, subjects were presented
contain common elements or might occupy overlapping re- with a series of binary choices involving allocating money to
gions of a cognitive matrix, and access to them might follow themselves or another person. Each choice was between two
similar pathways. However, before sorting out these particulars scenarios, each scenario involving an allocation to self and
of how including other in the self comes about or even about its other. For example, thefirstcomputer screen might show Alter-
precise cognitive structure, thefirststep is to consider whether native A as self gaining $14.50 and other losing $3.90 and Alter-
an inclusion of other in the self—in the general sense we have native B as self gaining $16.00 and other losing $7.50. The series
described of a lessened self/other distinction influencing behav- of choices included a balanced set of alternatives presented in a
ior and cognition—actually occurs when examined with studies randomized order.
specifically designed for the purpose and using very diverse Using this procedure, Liebrand (1984) found that people clas-
methodologies. It was to this end that the present research was sified into different groups on the basis of their relative alloca-
addressed. tions to self and other behaved in the ways predicted from his
The experiments reported in this article focused on each of theory in laboratory 7-person and 20-person social dilemma
the three general aspects of the partner that Aron and Aron situations involving real money and a considerable amount of
244 ARON, ARON, TUDOR, AND NELSON

experimental realism. In another study, Liebrand, Jansen, Rij- Table 1


ken, and Suhre (1986) found that subjects in different groupings Mean Allocations to Self Minus Allocations to Other
behaved in predicted ways regarding reciprocation of an other's
Target person
strategy and emphasis on morality (vs. power) in a series of
ratings of self and other. Other researchers (e.g., Kuhlman & Best Friendly Disliked
Marshello, 1975) also used simplified game procedures with Condition friend acquaintance Stranger other
successful results.
Our adaptation of Liebrand's (1984) procedure did not use Experiment 1
his classifications but instead emphasized differences between Other will know -2.95 6.14 12.76
Other will not know -0.54 8.50 13.72
experimental conditions in which identifications of who the Follow-Up 1
other was and whether the other would know the subject's Other will know -1.05 15.16
choices were systematically manipulated. Following the logic Other will not know -0.74 18.81
noted earlier, we expected that the difference between self- and Follow-Up 2
Other will not know 2.92 20.52 24.64
other-allocations would be least when other was their best
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

friend, intermediate when other was a friendly acquaintance,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. The conditions varied regarding whether other would or would


and greatest when other was a stranger. We also expected that not know subject's allocations. For Experiment 1, n = 24. Follow-Up 1
the pattern would be largely unaffected by whether subjects involved real money (n = 13). Follow-Up 2 involved a disliked other
assumed other would or would not know the subject^ choices.

Method
and stranger. Also as expected, neither the main effect nor in-
Twenty-four students participated in the experiment in return for teraction was statistically significant (both Fs < 1) for whether
research participation credit for a lower division psychology course. or not the other was assumed to know the subject's choices.
The computer presentation was based on a 24-choice version of the (Dawes's, 1980, review of the social dilemmas literature noted
decomposed-game procedure provided to us by Liebrand in the form
of a Pascal program on a computer disk. We modified the program
that some game-type studies have reported greater cooperation
slightly, so that the entire series was presented six times {each time with for public-disclosure than for anonymous conditions, but
the 24 choices in a different random order). Prior to each series, sub- Dawes also noted that such differences were typically small.)
jects were instructed about whom to think of as the other in their That these results were consistent with the expected pattern
choices—either their best friend, a particular friendly acquaintance, was particularly encouraging as a first examination of the util-
or a stranger—and whether they should assume this other would or ity of the inclusion-of-other-in-self approach to closeness be-
would not know their allocation choices. Thus each subject completed cause a continuum over three levels of closeness had been dem-
an entire set of 24 binary choices six times, making a 3 (other is close onstrated. Nevertheless, the procedures used were somewhat
friend, friendly acquaintance, or stranger) X 2 (other would or would abstract. Although scores on the Liebrand measure and related
not know subject's choices) within-subject design. Six sequences were
procedures have correlated with appropriate behaviors under
used: They included all the permutations of the 3 x 2 design that also
fitted the requirement (to minimize subjects' confusion) that subjects somewhat more realistic conditions, on the surface at least the
first always completed a friend, an acquaintance, and a stranger series procedure is fairly far removed from interactions involving real
(the order of the three varying over subjects), all three with other know- and significant resources. Furthermore, the manipulation of
ing (or other not knowing), then completed the remaining three series, whether other knows may have been weak, and there was no
keeping friend, acquaintance, and stranger in the same order, for other check of whether it was understood by the subjects.
does not know (or other does know). Subjects were randomly assigned
to sequences with the restriction that each of the six sequences was
Follow-Up Experiment Using Real Money
completed by two men and two women.
To address these issues, we attempted to replicate the experi-
Results and Discussion ment under more realistic conditions—specifically, using allo-
cations of real money to real others—plus giving more empha-
None of the various tests for order or gender interactions with sis to the instructions involving the manipulation of other
the main independent variables were significant. Thus all fur- knows or not, and we included checks on whether subjects un-
ther analyses were combined over order and gender, yielding a derstood the manipulation instructions.
3 x 2 repeated measures analysis of variance. The dependent The procedures used were identical to the original experi-
variable was allocations to self minus allocations to other. (Us- ment except for the followingfivechanges. First, subjects were
ing alternative measures—allocations to other alone, to self informed at the outset that their choices involved real money,
alone, or Liebrand's, 1984, "angle vector" measure—all yielded and various tactics were used to assure that subjects believed
the same patterns of means and statistical significance as the the payment would be made. (Subjects at the end of the study
self-minus-other measure used here.) were paid an average of $8.84 and the money sent to friends
Means are shown in Table 1. There was a clear main effect for averaged $5.10 per friend.) Second, to simplify the experiment,
type of other, F(2, 21) = 14.60, p < .01. Bonferroni ts (Dunn's the friendly acquaintance conditions were not included. Third,
test) confirmed the predicted pattern of least difference be- we used an elaborate procedure to make the manipulation for
tween self and best friend, intermediate difference between self other would know or other would not know more realistic. At
and friendly acquaintance, and greatest difference between self the outset, each subject addressed an envelope to his or her best
CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS AS INCLUDING OTHER IN THE SELF 245
friend living out of town. The experimenter then carefully ex- liking or similarity should somehow produce greater alloca-
plained to the subject that after the experiment this best friend tions to other. Another follow-up experiment addressed this
would be mailed a check with a letter breaking the total amount issue.
into (a) the amount the subject allocated to the best friend in the
condition in which the subject was told the friend would know
how much was allocated to the friend, (b) an amount allocated Follow-Up Experiment Using a Disliked Other
to the friend by another subject who did not know this friend
(this would be another subject's stranger-will-know condition), We reasoned that if our results were due to the friend being
and (c) an amount consisting of the sum of own friend-will-not- better liked or more similar, then a disliked other should be
know and another subject's stranger-will-not-know conditions, allocated as much less as a liked other is rewarded more. (Given
which would be described in the letter as coming from two Rosenbaum's, 1986, findings that dissimilarity has more nega-
subjects in the experiment, (The letters and checks were all tive impact on attraction than similarity has positive impact,
subsequently sent as promised.) Fourth, to reemphasize the real- theories associating reward allocation with similarity might
ity of the situation, the instructions prior to each series actually well predict a greater difference between a dissimilar versus
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

included the name of either their best friend or, in the stranger neutral other than between a similar versus neutral other.) On
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

condition, the best friend of another subject (we checked to be the other hand, if more money is allocated to a friend because
sure the subject did not know this person). Finally,fifth,checks the friend is included in the self, then there should be little
were made on whether the subject understood and believed the difference between a disliked other and a stranger, neither of
instructions. After each series, the instructions included a ques- whom are very much part of the self. Thus we conducted an
tion about whether on the previous series, the other would or experiment comparing allocations to a friend, stranger, and a
would not know how much the subject had allocated to him or disliked other.
her. (Every subject gave the correct answer on each trial to this In this experiment, we first had subjects select "the person
question.) Subjects were also telephoned a week later, before with whom you can work least well,** following the instructions
their friends were sent the letters and money. (All said they had in Fiedler and Chemers's (1974) Least Preferred Coworker
understood and believed what they had been told during the Scale. Subjects then completed the scale, to make this least
experiment.) preferred other vivid. Then, when subjects came to the disliked-
As in the main experiment, there were no significant order or other condition in the experiment, they were again reminded of
gender effects or interactions. And again, as shown in the mid- this person, were asked to write this person's initials, and were
dle rows of Table 1, the pattern of means was as predicted. asked to complete the next set of computerized allocation deci-
There was a main effect for best friend versus stranger, F(\, sions about this person. The only remaining difference from the
12) = 25.55, p < .001, with no significant main or interaction original experiment was that to save time, only the other-will-
effect for whether other would or would not know their choices not-know conditions were used. This created a three-condition
(bothi%<l). (friend vs. stranger vs. disliked other) within-subject design.
These data show that the suggested pattern holds using more The results of this follow-up experiment are shown in the
realistic conditions. They also give some added support to the bottom row of Table 1. Again, there were no interactions of the
assumption that subjects actually understood and believed that target-person factor with order or sex. And, again, there was a
the other would or would not know their decisions. clear overall main effect for who the other was, F(2, 100) -
Of course, allocating a greater share of money to friends than 79.63, p < .001. Also, all differences among the three means
strangers is also consistent with findings generated in the con- were significant. However, clearly consistent with the inclusion-
text of other theoretical orientations, such as the equity and of-other-in-the-self idea, but not so obviously with theories that
exchange approaches (e.g., Austin, 1980; Benton, 1971; Curtis, emphasize liking or similarity, the difference between friend
1979; Greenberg, 1978,1983; Lerner, 1974). However, in none and stranger was much greater than the difference between
of the previous studies we were able to locate did the subject disliked other and stranger, /(54) = 7,47, p < .001. The magni-
make allocations to real friends under conditions in which the tude of the difference is particularly evident if one compares
other would not know their choices. Furthermore, these studies effect sizes. Using the mean difference divided by the standard
were mostly with children and were conducted in the context of deviation of the difference scores gives an effect size of 1.31 for
allocating rewards for performance or as part of negotiation. the friend-versus-stranger comparison and an effect size of .37
In any case, the theoretical rationale in studies of this kind for the disliked-other-versus-stranger comparison. Using pro-
usually either (a) suggest that greater rewards to other result portion of variance accounted for as a measure of effect size
from other's potential reciprocity (an idea that does not seem to yieldedfiguresof .64 versus .12.
explain our data, in light of the failure to find any effects for There may well be other alternatives we have not considered.
expecting other to know of one's allocations) or (b) emphasizes a Nor is this second follow-up study itself free of alternative expla-
desire to maintain equity (which also does not seem a reason- nations. For example, the friends may be liked more than least
able explanation for our results, because the other in our experi- preferred coworkers are disliked—though it seems unlikely
ment was not perceived to have exerted any effort in the study). there would be as much difference as was seen in the effect-size
(Some researchers have also invoked a very unequity-like con- comparisons. Also, as noted earlier, there are reasons to expect
cept of identification with other as an explanation—an idea generally greater impacts of disliking than liking. More gener-
quite consistent with the inclusion-of-other-in-self paradigm.) ally, it is never made explicit, either in the present studies or in
However, another logic implicit in some of these studies is that the equity-type studies cited earlier, why liking or feeling simi-
246 ARON, ARON, TUDOR, AND NELSON

lar to someone should make us want to give them more money each, during which time subjects were instructed to "form as
when they do not even know we are giving it. It seems most vivid and interesting a mental image as possible of yourself [or a
reasonable that liked or similar others are perceived as some- target person] interacting with" (1987, p. 447) what the noun
how more closely associated with the self and that the enhance- referred to (for example, a mule). After the 10 s, the screen went
ment of the other enhances the self. blank, and subjects wrote down their image for 20 s before the
In any case, taking the original and the two follow-up experi- next slide appeared. Subjects followed along on a sheet which
ments together appears to give a reasonable basis of support for told them for each slide what target person to associate with the
the notion that for whatever reason, we treat close others as if slide (the order of target persons was randomized over subjects).
their resources were, to some extent, our own. Furthermore, After the entire series was presented, the sheet and the images
what makes these studies particularly salient and novel is that they wrote down were removed, and subjects were given 5 min
in conjunction with Experiments 2 and 3, which are quite dif- to write down, in any order, as many of the nouns as they could
ferent in methods, they all converge on this inclusion-of-other- recall. Lord found that nouns imaged interacting with self were
in-the-self theme. recalled significantly less well than nouns imaged interacting
with a well-known other such as Johnny Carson or then-presi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dent Ronald Reagan.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Experiment 2
Lord (1980,1987) interpreted these results in terms of a fig-
As noted in the Introduction, one aspect of including other in ure/ground difference between one's experience of self and
the self in a close relationship is viewing the world from the other when acting in the world. Of course, if this difference
perspective of the other (Aron & Aron, 1986); several theoreti- were entirely perceptual (as suggested, for example, by Storms's,
cal notions and some data support this view. For example, Nis- 1973, study), then there would be no reason to expect that even
bett, Caputo, Legant, and Marecek (1973, Study 3) found that a person with whom one was extremely close would be per-
the extent that people were willing to make dispositional attri- ceived differently than a prominent entertainment or political
butions about their friends was less the longer their relationship personality. However, we reasoned that if this figure/ground
with the close friend (r = —.45, p < .01). Another, more recent difference represented a different way of understanding and
study of actor/observer attributional differences, conducted by appreciating the world, then if other were included in one's
Sande et al. (1988). bears even more directly on the inclusion- inner world, other should become more like ground and less
of-other-in-self approach. These researchers first demonstrated like figure—that is, more like the self. Thus this figure/ground
an actor/observer difference in which people more often attrib- idea seems to capture very well the notion of including other's
uted to themselves than to others both poles of pairs of opposite perspective in the self.
traits (serious/carefree, energetic/relaxed, etc.) when given a Actually, in Lord's original study (1980, Experiment 2) one of
choice of the first, the second, both, or neither. They then pro- the targets that subjects used was their father. The mean recall
ceeded to show that this self/other distinction was less when of father-associated nouns was intermediate between self and
other was liked than when other was disliked. (Sande et al. also Walter Cronkite, as the inclusion-of-other-in-self paradigm
compared familiar versus unfamiliar others. There wasa differ- would suggest, but this difference was not statistically signifi-
ence between familiar and unfamiliar when both were liked but cant. However, we felt that father was not an ideal operational-
not when both were disliked.) We replicated Sande et al.'s proce- ization of a close other (nor had Lord intended it as such, of
dure in our own lab, modifying it to permit a comparison of self course), because college-student subjects in general may not be
to others that represented different degrees of liking (as op- very close to their father (\buniss & Smollar, 1985), particularly
posed to liking versus disliking). The 24 subjects we tested in an era in which a great many of them may have been raised in
(using the 11 adjective pairs in Sande et al.'s Experiment 2) chose mother-only households. Thus we reasoned that if a target per-
both poles for a mean of 4.50 adjective pairs for themselves, son were closer, the difference between a media personality and
3.46 for their best friend, and 2.71 for a friendly acquaintance, the close person might prove significant.
F(2, 4 6 ) - 6 . 0 2 , p<.0\. Therefore, Experiment 2 attempted to replicate Lord's (1980,
However, none of the existing studies relate as directly to 1987) procedures using as targets the subject's self, a prominent
closeness as might be desired for the present purposes (which entertainment personality, and their mother—a person who
they were not intended to serve). Furthermore, we felt that should be highly close for most subjects. And because college-
showing a decrease in a different and perhaps less obvious ac- age women are likely to be closer to their mother than are
tor/observer discrepancy would be a stronger demonstration of college-age men (Youniss & Smollar, 1985), only women were
the usefulness of the idea that in a close relationship the other's used. To further ensure closeness, we used only women who
perspective is included in the self. Lord's (1980,1987) paradigm had been living at home with their mother in the last 1.5 years
seemed tofitthis description. Lord showed that nouns remem- (the study was conducted in the fall quarter withfirst-and sec-
bered by constructing imagined scenes in which the self inter- ond-year students). The singer and movie actress Cher was se-
acts with the object the noun represents are recalled less well lected as the entertainment personality, since she was a woman
than nouns remembered by constructing such scenes involving and about the same age as most of the subjects' mothers. Cher
someone else. had performed in several successful recent movies and had won
Specifically, Lord (1980, Experiment 2; 1987) presented sub- an Academy Award that year. None of the lower division under-
jects with a series of 60 concrete nouns (taken from Craik & graduate women we pretested had any difficulty forming an
Tulving's, 1975, experiment) projected on the screen for 10 s image of Cher.
CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS AS INCLUDING OTHER IN THE SELF 247

Method makes such explanations unlikely but does not completely rule
them out—both because of the small sample size, which makes
Twenty female students served as subjects in return for research par-
ticipation credit for their introductory psychology course. All proce-
the difference between the correlations only marginally signifi-
dures were identical to those used by Lord (1987; personal correspon- cant, and because self-ratings of familiarity may not be accu-
dence, April 1988), except that the three target persons, as noted in the rate. On the other hand, one possible explanation for why famil-
previous paragraph, were self, mother, and Cher. Also, at the outset, iarity might create differences in the Lord task is that a familiar
subjects were asked if they were familiar with Cher and how she other would require less effort to image and therefore nouns
looked. (All said they were.) associated with the familiar other would be less well recalled.
This explanation, however, seems unlikely to us because it
should be easiest to form a vivid image of the entertainment
Results and Discussion personality who is known primarily as an image object (indeed
Order of presentation did not interact with target person. this is why the follow-up experiment was conducted). Another
The pattern of means for the different target persons was as possible familiarity explanation is that one's mother is easier to
predicted, with Cher (8.95 nouns recalled) greater than mother image because she has been observed more often in familiar
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(7.15 recalled), f(19) = 2.43, p < .05. Also, the difference be- surroundings. However, this explanation also seems unlikely
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tween mother and self (6.80 recalled) was so small that it was not because in the follow-up experiment, the comparison was to
statistically significant (/ < 1). mother's friend, who presumably would have been viewed in
the same familiar settings as the mother. Nevertheless, the fa-
miliarity explanation can probably not be discounted entirely
Follow-Up Experiment on the basis of the existing data. Of course, from the general
One potential alternative explanation is that more nouns point of view of demonstrating the usefulness of conceptualiz-
were recalled in the Cher condition because she is a more vivid ing closeness as including other in the self, it does not matter
image than one's mother. We were also interested in whether whether any such pattern is due to familiarity, because familiar-
the degree of mother/nonclose other difference would correlate ity is an almost universal concomitant of closeness. On the
with degree of perceived closeness to mother and whether any other hand, the likelihood in our view that familiarity does not
such relation could be sorted out from degree of perceived famil- explain the present results suggests that closeness may operate
iarity with or similarity to mother. more directly on cognitive processing.
Thus we conducted a follow-up experiment in which subjects In any case, these follow-up results together with the original
formed images of self, mother, and, instead of Cher, "a female Experiment 2 data suggest, once again, that subjects treated
friend or relative of your mother's who is about your mother's someone with whom they were in a close relationship more as if
age and to whom you are not very close." Also, at the end of this that person were somehow themselves. Moreover, this result is
experiment, subjects completed a brief questionnaire in which particularly interesting because it is about a nonobvious phe-
they indicated how close, familiar, and similar they felt to their nomenon that emphasizes the figure/ground difference be-
mother, each recorded on a 13-point scale ranging from very tween self's and other's perspective.
distant to very close (or very unfamiliar to very familiar, etc.) The
experiment was conducted during a regular class meeting of a
summer session lower division psychology class. Everyone in
Experiment 3
the class volunteered to participate (2 subjects, however, were The third aspect of the other suggested to be included in the
not included because their mothers were deceased). In all other self in a close relationship is the other's characteristics. A num-
respects (instructions, nouns, times, randomization of target- ber of studies support the idea that the self is a highly organized
person orders, etc.), the procedures were identical to the origi- cognitive structure, in part by demonstrating that in a reaction-
nal experiment. time situation, traits that are highly self-descriptive are recog-
Of the 14 subjects who participated, 7 had lived with their nized as self-descriptive more quickly than are traits that are of
mother within the last 2 years. The pattern of means for these 7 neutral self-descriptiveness (e.g., Markus, 1977). Similarly,
subjects was about the same as for the original experiment: Kuiper (1981) demonstrated that adjectives that were very true
friend of mother (6.86 nouns recalled) greater than mother (4.86 and very untrue of self were recognized as such more rapidly
recalled), ?(6) = 2.90, p < .05; no significant difference between than terms that were more intermediate. One standard proce-
mother and self (5.14 recalled; t < 1). dure in these kinds of studies is that people rate a list of adjec-
The mother/mother's friend difference in nouns recalled tives for their descriptive ness of themselves, take part in a dis-
tended to be strongly correlated with rated closeness to mother tracting filler task, then make a series of me/not me reaction-
(r = .56, p <. 10) but only weakly correlated with similarity (r = time choices (or yes/no choices regarding self-relevance). The
. 13) or familiarity (r= .16) with mother. effect of faster reaction times for recognizing self-descriptive
The possibility that the results of these experiments were due traits as true of self has been labeled a descriptiveness effect.
to familiarity bears some further discussion. Although Lord However, another line of research using similar methods (e.g.,
did not conceptualize his results as due to familiarity, the idea Mueller, Ross, & Heesacker, 1984; Ross, Mueller, & de la Torre,
has played a role in other studies, including the Sande et al. 1986) found that traits that are distinctive to self are recognized
(1988) study noted earlier. The low correlation of rated mother as self-descriptive more slowly than traits that are shared with
familiarity with the Cher/mother differences in words recalled others in general (e.g., most students). This has been called a
248 ARON, AROK, TUDOR, AND NELSON

distinctiveness effect. One study by Mueller, Thompson, and where they completed the reaction-time portion of the task at a com-
Dugan (1986) demonstrated that both the descriptiveness and puter terminal.
distinctiveness effects operated independently (i.e., additively). The initial questionnaire had subjects rate on separate pages each of
A possible explanation of the distinctiveness effect is that the three target persons for how much each of 90 traits applied to that
cognitive structure of the self overlaps with the cognitive struc- person. The trait adjectives were selected from Anderson's (1968)
norms. They included 20 with likableness ratings above 4.5, 30 with
ture about the other (or, in the studies in the previous para- ratings between 2.0 and 3.8, and 30 with ratings below 1.7. The 90 were
graph, overlaps with the cognitive structure held for people in randomly ordered on the page. Subjects responded to each item on a
general). Thus when a trait is descriptive of self but not other, 7-point scale from extremely like {\) the target person to unhkeil). The
there is a bit of confusion in deciding whether it actually repre- target persons were self, spouse, and Bill Cosby. Questionnaires using
sents the self. We take this line of reasoning one step further. In the three possible orders of target persons were randomly assigned to
the case of a close relationship, in which we suggest other's traits subjects. (However, each subject's order of response was accidentally
are included in the self to a substantial extent, there should be a not recorded when the response pages were reordered to facilitate data
greater overlap of cognitive structures—hence more confusion entry, so no tests of order effects could be conducted for this experi-
and slower response times. ment.)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Experiment 3 examined this suggestion of slower reaction The computer instructions explained to subjects that they would be
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

presented with a series of descriptors. Their task was to decide as


times for traits distinctive from close others. We adapted proce-
quickly as possible for each trait whether that trait applied to them-
dures from the various studies cited earlier of response times to selves or not by pressing the A key for "me" or the L key for "not me."
self-referent traits. Subjects first rated a series of trait adjectives Ten example adjectives were given, then the set of 90 adjectives was
for their descriptiveness of themselves, their spouse, and an presented three times, each time in a different random order. Each
entertainment personality. After a distracting intermediate adjective remained on the screen until the subject pressed one of the
task, they made a series of me/not me reaction-time choices to keys. The computer recorded responses and latencies.
these trait words. The prediction was that there would be most
confusion, thus longer response latencies, for trait words that Results and Discussion
were different between self and spouse. (Note that in this study,
all responses in the reaction-time task were in the context of Adjective ratings were divided at the median, so that those
self. This should not be confused with a number of previous with ratings of 4 or higher were considered not descriptive (or
studies, e.g., Keenan & Baillet, 1980, which compared reaction false) of the target person and those less than 4 as descriptive (or
times for words in which the context of the choice was some- true) of the target person. Next the adjectives for each subject
times self and sometimes some other person.) were divided into categories according to their pattern of trues
Testing this prediction required dividing traits into groups and falses for the three target persons. Ten (2 men and 8
according to whether they were descriptive or not of self, women) of the 13 subjects had usable responses to at least one of
spouse, and the entertainment personality. Of the eight possible the traits in each of the four critical categories. Average re-
combinations of true and false of each of the three people, four sponse latencies to traits in each of these four categories were
combinations were of interest: (a) traits that were rated as true of then computed for each of these subjects.
self and spouse but not of entertainment personality (TTF), (b) Table 2 contains the mean response latencies in each cate-
traits that were true of self, false of spouse, and true of the gory. A 2 X 2 repeated measures analysis of variance yielded a
entertainment personality (TFT), (c) traits that were false of self main effect for true versus false, F(l, 9) = 7.82, p < .05, but no
and spouse but true of the entertainment personality (FFT), significant interaction, (F < 1). This main effect indicates that
and (d) traits that were false of self, true of spouse, and false of subjects were faster at responding to traits that were true of
the entertainment personality (FTF). (The other four combina- themselves than ones that were false of themselves. This is con-
tions—TTT, FFE TFF; and FTT—were not of interest because sistent with previous research (Kuiper, 1981; Mueller et aL,
they confounded whether a trait was true or false of spouse with
whether it was true or false of the entertainment personality)
The prediction was that subjects would have more confusion, Table 2
hence slower reaction time, for the traits in which they differed Mean Response Times and Errors for Traits Differing
from spouse: (b) TFT and (d) FTF. in Their Descriptiveness of Selfand Spouse
Trait
Method
Response time (ms) Proportion of errors
Subjects were married graduate students attending a social psychol-
ogy seminar at a freestanding psychology professional school. The Descriptiveness Same Different Same Different
research was presented as a nonrequircd opportunity to experience
social cognition research methods. There were 17 students in the semi- For self
nar, of which all 13 who were married agreed to participate. Their True 1,023 1,096 .069 .177
mean age was 38.4; they had been married a mean of 6.45 years. False 1,094 1.150 .252 .438
Participants first completed an initial questionnaire as they were Total 1,059 1,123 .161 .308
waiting for the seminar to begin. Sometime after (allowing 15 min to 2
hr of seminar interaction, functioning as a distracting task), students Note. Same and different refer to descriptiveness of trait for self and
were called out of the seminar individually and sent to another room spouse.
CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS AS INCLUDING OTHER IN THE SELF 249
1986). Because there was no interaction, the overall analysis have had to be one in which (a) the high similarity traits were
relevant to our predictions about including the other in the self more likely to be rated as similar both in terms of their presence
could be collapsed into a t test for dependent means between and their absence (because both the FFTs and the TTFs
response latencies to traits in which self and spouse were similar counted as similar) and (b) the difference between traits with
(TTF and FFT) versus not similar (TFT and FTF). The result, high and low self/spouse similarity was not due to ratings on the
/(9) = 2.21, p < .05, indicates that subjects were slower respond- low similarity traits being less reliable (because in all four pat-
ing to traits that differed between self and spouse. This finding terns used in the comparisons there were two Ts and one F or
is consistent with the prediction and can be interpreted as one T and two Fs). For example, the possibility that traits se-
showing that self and spouse are more closely integrated cogni- lected as more similar between self and spouse are more mun-
tively than are self and the entertainment personality. dane does not seem to fit with criterion (a): A subject may well
We also analyzed the data for response errors. (A response be more likely to have mundane traits that their spouse also has,
was counted as an error if the way the subject had rated it as but if so they should be less likely to be similar in the sense of
self-descriptive or not on the questionnaire received the oppo- not having mundane traits that their partner also does not have.
site corresponding me or not me key press.) The analyses for (This mundaneness argument, in particular, also seems un-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

response errors (means are shown in Table 2) yielded the same likely because all the traits were common adjectives.) Indeed, it
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

pattern of findings as the response latency data: a true-versus- is difficult to think of any possible characteristics of trait adjec-
false-of-self main effect, F(l,9)= 14.85, p< .01; no significant tives that would meet these criteria and still make the adjectives
interaction (F < 1) and a significant difference between traits in likely to be rated as similar between self and spouse and that
which self was similar versus different from spouse. t(9) = 2.45, could be reasonably related to reaction-time responses.
p < .05. This last finding means that there were more errors in
responding to traits on which self and spouse differed—again
consistent with the notion that self and spouse are more closely Follow-Up Experiment
integrated cognitively, leading to confusions, than are self and An alternative interpretation that did particularly concern us
the entertainment personality. was the possibility that the present results could have benefited
Note again that these results are different from the findings from using a population of clinical psychology graduate stu-
demonstrating better memory or faster reaction time for words dents who might be unusually sensitive to trait similarities and
associated with close others than with more distant others differences with their spouse. Also, we wanted to examine
(Bower & Gilligan, 1979; Ferguson, Rule, & Carlson, 1983; whether the degree of response-time difference between
Keenan & Baillet, 1980; Kuiper & Rogers, 1979). Although the spouse-similar and spouse-different traits would vary in rela-
results of those studies are also generally consistent with the tion to perceived closeness to spouse. Finally, we were con-
closeness-as-inclusion-of-other-in-self paradigm, the present cerned that the present results might somehow be dependent on
findings make a more direct case for an actual overlap or confu- using an entertainment personality for the nonclose-other trait
sion of cognitive structures. ratings.
Note also that the present findings cannot be accounted for Thus we conducted a follow-up experiment using a different
by there being more positive traits among those attributed to population, included a measure of closeness, and used friend of
spouse. The product of Anderson's (1968) likableness norms by spouse for the nonclose-other ratings. For this experiment we
subjects' ratings for each of the 90 traits yielded no significant contacted people who had responded to a newspaper survey
difference between spouse and entertainment personality about marital activities (McKenna, 1989) and who had included
</<l). a note with their survey response indicating a willingness to
Nor can these findings be accounted for by the traits in the participate in further research (a recruiting procedure sug-
spouse-similar categories having more extreme ratings of de- gested by Shaver & Rubenstein, 1983). Sixteen of those who
scriptiveness of self. (This would be a confound, because responded and came to our laboratory for testing had been
Mueller et al., 1986, found faster reaction times in self-descrip- married at least a year and were in either their first or second
tive situations for words that were previously rated as either very marriage. Of these, 10 (6 men and 4 women; mean age, 47.0
descriptive or very nondescriptive of self.) If this pattern held, years; mean length of marriage, 16.1 years) had usable re-
then a 2 X 2 repeated measures analysis of variance comparing sponses to at least one trait in each of the four critical catego-
the self-ratings of the words in the four crucial categories should ries. The only other difference from the procedures of the origi-
have yielded a significant True/False X Similarity-to-Spouse nal experiment was that because of practical limitations in the
interaction effect. In fact, the interaction was not significant circumstances of testing, we were able to include only a brief
(F<i). distractor task (completing a questionnaire).
More generally, the question arises as to whether these results The pattern of means on response times was the same as in
could be due to some other difference in subjects' responses to the original experiment. And again, the main effect for true/
the different trait adjectives. For example, those traits rated as false was significant, F(\, 9) = 5.48, p < .05; and the interaction
similar to spouse might be more mundane than those rated as with whether or not the trait was similar for self and spouse was
similar to the entertainment personality, and greater mundane- not significant (F < 1). The comparison between traits in which
ness might lead to faster decisions. However, to have influenced self and spouse were similar versus dissimilar approached but
the results of Experiment 3, any characteristic of traits that was did not quite attain statistical significance, t(9) = 1.58, p < . 10.
consistently associated with self/spouse similarity would also Of course, given the small sample size, this result had a substan-
250 ARON, ARON, TUDOR, AND NELSON

tial effect size (the mean difference divided by the standard to explain thefindingsin other studies for self-and-other-trait-
deviation of the difference scores was .50). It also represented a rating reaction times (e.g., Kuiper & Rogers, 1979). Also, Srull
replication of an earlier experiment (the effect size for the corre- and Gaelick (1983) used familiarity to explain their findings,
sponding comparison was .70), Thus, it seemed not unreason- which are conceptually related to the present study. Srull and
able to consider the overall set of results to be supportive of the Gaelick assessed the degree of symmetry between similarity
prediction and to conclude that with regard to response laten- ratings when self versus other is the reference point (how similar
cies, it probably did not matter a great deal whether the sample is other to you vs. how similar are you to other). In one of their
was a group of psychologists or not or whether the nonclose studies, they found that symmetry was greater when the self/
other was an entertainment personality or not. It may matter, other similarity ratings were for mother or father than when
however, whether the sample included mostly women. In the they were for a media personality In their study, familiarity was
original experiment, 8 of 10 were women; in the follow-up, only a quite reasonable explanation because the greater information
4 of 10 were women. Considering just the women in the follow- available about a familiar other makes it easier (as compared
up experiment, even with only 4 subjects, there was a clearly with a stranger) for unique differences to come to mind when
significant effect (in the expected direction) for response time to other is thefocusand harder for unique differences to come to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

traits in which spouse and self were similar versus dissimilar, mind when self is the focus.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

t(5) = 3.53, p < .05. On the other hand, there was no obvious In the present case, the potential influence of familiarity
theoretical rationale for expecting a gender difference in this would seem to be less clear. One reasonable possibility might be
case, so that the slight statistical tendencies were probably best that familiar/familiar comparisons could be more difficult,
not taken very seriously unless subsequent studies replicate the making decisions slower, with more errors. Note, however, that
pattern. this way of thinking implies that when deciding on whether a
In this follow-up experiment, we also administered the Inclu- trait is true or not of themselves, subjects implicitly made a
sion of Other in the Self (IOS) Scale (Aron, Aron, & Smolan, comparison with their spouse.
1990), which measures perceived closeness. The IOS Scale asks Thus, this kind of familiarity explanation implies a rather
subjects to select one of seven diagrams of overlapping circles special cognitive relationship between self and other that goes
(like Figure 1), representing different degrees of overlap, as beyond the models of mere familiarity proposed in some
most descriptive of their relationship. Aron et al. (1990) found previous studies.
that the IOS Scale had high levels of reliability, correlated with In sum, Experiment 3 and its follow-up seem to provide yet
other measures of closeness, and predicted whether dating cou- another illustration of the potential hypothesis-generating
ples would break up 3 months later. McKenna (1989) and Grif- value of considering that in a close relationship, people process
fin (1990) both reported that the IOS Scale also correlates information as if the other is partially included in the self. The
highly with standard measures of marital quality, even after present experiment is especially interesting because it applied
controlling for measures of social desirability. the reaction-time social-cognition approach to a topic that had
Including the IOS Scale permitted us to correlate the self/ in the past been studied mainly in the context of clinical theory
spouse similar/dissimilar difference in response times with per- and personality testing. And even among studies that focus on
ceived closeness. The resulting correlation of r = .59, p < .05, reaction-time or memory differences in relation to own and
suggests that the more perceived closeness, the greater the over- other's traits, it was thefirstto look directly at implicit compari-
lap of cognitive structures between self and spouse—again, a sons or selfybther confusions when making only self-ratings—
result quite consistent with the inclusion-of-other-in-self per- an approach we suspect would not have been generated within
spective. Finally, we examined response errors. This time the the theoretical atmosphere in which most social-cognition stud-
similar/dissimilar difference, although in the predicted direc- ies have been conducted.
tion, did not even approach statistical significance (/ < 1). Nor
could this result be explained by the smaller number of women
in the sample. However, there was a tendency for closeness to be General Discussion
correlated in the expected direction with the self/spouse simi- Taken together, these various studies, applying a variety of
lar/dissimilar error differences (r - .50, p <. 10). Thus, subjects methods and using different types of close relationships, illus-
in the follow-up may not have been as close to their spouse as trate the utility of viewing close relationships as including other
were those in the original experiment. Or this inconsistency in the self. The results of Experiment 1 and its follow-up experi-
with the original experiment may be due to subjects in the ments suggest that regardless of whether the other will know of
follow-up going almost immediately from the rating task to the self's decision, differences in allocation of money to self and
reaction-time task, so that they were more likely to try to re- other decrease as other is closer to the self, comparing others
member their responses on the rating tasks. Finally; this group, who are best friend, acquaintance, and stranger. The results of
which did not consist of psychology graduate students, did Experiment 2 and its follow-up suggest that differences in mem-
make more errors overall; these may have been largely caused ory that is based on images of self versus others interacting with
by unfamiliarity with the traits themselves, so that these errors an object noun to be remembered are less when other is close to
masked the systematic distribution of errors by target person. self. (Other was varied by comparing images made with mother
In considering both Experiment 3 and its follow-up, the ques- versus either an entertainment personality or a friend of one's
tion of the possible role of familiarity arises again here, as it did mother.) The results of Experiment 3 and its follow-up suggest
in discussing the results of Experiment 2. Familiarity might that cognitive representations of self and other are more closely
seem particularly salient in this case because familiarity is used interconnected when other is in a close relationship, as shown
CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS AS INCLUDING OTHER IN THE SELF 251
by patterns of response latencies in making me/not me deci- which both partners initially saw themselves as similar in terms
sions about traits previously rated as descriptive of self, spouse, of liking the outdoors (because compared with most people,
or either an entertainment personality or a friend of spouse. they know they both feel largely the same way), after living
Together thesefindingslend support to the idea that the cogni- together some years, any slight difference between them might
tive implications of being in a close relationship are that other is repeatedly show itself, for example, one might always want to go
included in self, taking into account the three central aspects of camping, but the other might always resist it. Thus this small
self originally emphasized by James (1890/1948) and described difference could become identified in the world of the couple as
by Aron and Aron (1986) in terms of resources, perspectives, a major point of conflict. However, such situations arise, in our
and characteristics. We want to emphasize that what is impor- view, precisely because the self/other merging in close relation-
tant here is not so much the results of each study individually, ships makes any self/other differences both more likely to be
but of the set as a whole. noticed and more likely to matter. More generally, we would
The procedures used in the experiments reported here also expect self/other differences in close relationships to operate
suggest new ways to look at closeness—as cognitive closeness, like contradictions among one's own behaviors and characteris-
rather than objective interaction patterns (Berscheid et al., tics. This idea is implicit in cognitive dissonance theorizing.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1989a, 1989b; Maxwell, 1985). They also provide theoretically Dissimilarity between self and a friend's attitudes is considered
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

meaningful experimental procedures that could serve as crite- dissonant in the same way that holding opposite attitudes
ria for validity testing of measures of closeness. Finally, we hope within oneself is considered dissonant—and, we would add, the
that this study will inspire a larger number of researchers to degree of dissonance should be a function of the closeness of
respond to Clark and Reis's (1988) call for more experiments the relationship (i.e., the degree to which the other is included in
that use laboratory methods in the study of ongoing close rela- the self). It is certainly a testable point, as is the implication
tionships. from a more psychodynamic perspective, that if the other is
The inclusion-of-other-in-the-self approach to closeness sug- included in the self and if self/other differences in close relation-
gests many other hypotheses that might be tested in further ships function like self/self conflicts, then the closer the rela-
research. Obviously many more self/other discrepancies in the tionship, the more self/other differences should be either (a)
social psychology research literature could be examined to see highly emotionally charged or (b) very uncharged (because of
if the discrepancy was less when other was in a close relation- their being integrated into an understanding that sees them no
ship to self. But numerous, less obvious implications could also longer really contradictory but as complementary). In other
be tested: For example, we are currently conducting studies of words, in close relationships, we would expect greater variance
the cognitive effects of the loss of a close relationship, of the in emotional charge associated with self/other differences.
difference in the cognitive significance of the use of the terms Thus the present experiments only begin to explore the cog-
loving and being in love, and of fear of intimacy in relation to nitive structure of close relationships in terms of the self. But
self-boundaries. they are a beginning, we hope, that illustrates the potential
Future research might also examine more closely the specific utility of the approach as a complement to those that have as-
cognitive mechanisms through which other is included in the sisted the study of close relationships up to now.
self. The model of including other in the self as used in the
experiments reported here might be best described in Lewin's
References
(1948) terms as overlapping differentiated regions within the
life space. A more precise notion, of overlapping prototypes (or Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likahleness ratings of 555 personality-trait
schemata) of self and other is also attractive and seems likely to words. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 272-279.
be at least a subclass of the general idea of including other in the Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1986). Love as the expansion of self: Under-
self. But as Greenwald and Pratkanis (1984) note, there seem to standing attraction and satisfaction. New York: Hemisphere.
be more aspects to the self than can be fully accounted for by Aron, A., Aron, E. N, & Allen, J. (1989, May). The motivation for
the cognitive representation approach. One such area of diffi- unrequited love: A self expansion perspective. Paper presented at the
Second Iowa Conference on Personal Relationships, Iowa City, IA.
culty involves the issue of identity: Do people actually confuse
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smolan, D. (1990, July). Assessing closeness as
their own identity with the close other (or, alternately, base their including other in the self: The IOS Scale. Paper presented at the
identity on that other)? Yet another line of research would focus International Conference on Personal Relationships, Oxford, En-
on the process by which other becomes included in the self (or gland.
even by which we see ourselves as included in the other). Aron, A., Dutton, D. G., Aron, E. N., & Iverson, A. (1989). Experiences
Afinalpoint about the idea of closeness as including other in of falling in love. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 6,
the self: In some cases, being in a close relationship might well 243-257.
make one appear to treat the other as more different from self Austin, W (1980). Friendship and fairness: Effects of type of relation-
rather than less. For example, Tesser (1988) showed that when ship and task performance on choice of distribution rules. Personal-
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 402-408.
self and other are competing for an outcome relevant to one's
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: Isolation and com-
self-esteem, one is more likely to be jealous of someone with mitment in Western man. Boston: Beacon Press.
whom one is close than with a stranger (although Tesser's defini- Bank, S. P., & Kahn, M. D. (1982). The sibling bond. New York: Basic
tion of closeness in his various studies overlaps with similarity). Books.
More generally, to the extent the other is highly salient in the life Bataille, G. (1962). Eroticism (M. Dalwood, Trans.). London: Calder.
space of the individual, other's characteristics are especially (Original work published 1957)
available for social comparison. For example, in a marriage in Benton, A. A. (1971). Productivity, distributive justice, and bargaining
252 ARON, ARON, TUDOR, AND NELSON

among children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, In M. Fisher&G. Strieker (Eds.), Intimacy(pp. 267-292). New York:
68-78. Plenum Press.
Berl, E. (1924). The nature of love (F. Rothwell, Trans.). London: Chap- Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York:
man & Hall. (Original work published 1923) Wiley.
Berscheid, E., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (1989a), Issues in studying Helgeson, V S., Shaver, P., & Dyer, M. (1987). Prototypes of intimacy
close relationships: Conceptualizing and measuring closeness. In C. and distance in same-sex and opposite-sex relationships. Journal of
Hendrick(Ed-), Review ofpersonality and social psychology (Vol. 10, Social and Personal Relationships, 4,195-233.
pp. 63-91). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathy, role taking, guilt, and development
Berscheid, E., Snyder, M, & Omoto, A. M. (1989b). The Relationship of altruistic motives. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and
Closeness Inventory: Assessing the closeness of interpersonal rela- behavior. New York: Holt, Rinchart & Winston.
tionships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 792-807. Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1987). Intergroup behaviour, self-stereo-
Bower, G. H., & Gilligan, S. G. (1979). Remembering information re- typing and the salience of social categories. British Journal ofSocial
lated to one's self. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 420-432. Psychology, 26, 325-340.
Brenner, M. (1973). The next-in-line effect. Journal of Verbal Learning Ickes, W, Tooke, W., Stinson, L., Baker, V, & Bissonnette, V (1988).
and Verbal Behavior, 12, 320-323. Naturalistic social cognition: Intersubjectivity in same-sex dyads.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Buber, M. (1937). landthou. New York: Scribners.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 58-84.


Clark, M. S. (1983). Reactions to aid in communal and exchange rela- James, W (1948). Psychology. Cleveland, OH: Fine Editions Press.
tionships. In J. D. Fisher, A. Nadler, & B. M. DePaulo (Eds.), New (Original work published 1890)
directions in helping (Vol. 1, pp. 281 -304). San Diego. CA: Academic Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. (1971). The actor and the observer: Diver-
Press. gent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. Kan-
Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange ouse, H. Kelley, R. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner(Eds.), Attribution:
and communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 79-94). Morristown, NJ: Gen-
chology, 37, 12-24. eral Learning Press.
Clark. M. S., & Reis, H. T. (1988). Interpersonal processes in close Jung, C. G. (1959). Marriage as a psychological relationship. In V S.
relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 609-672. DeLaszlo (Ed.), The basic writings ofC. G. Jung(R. F. C. Hull, Trans.;
Craik. F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the pp. 531-544). New York: Modern Library. (Original work published
retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psy- 1925)
chology: General, 104, 268-294.
Keenan, J. M., & Baillet, S. D. (1980). Memory for personally and
Curtis, R. C. (1979). Effects of knowledge of self-interest and social socially significant events. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and
relationship upon the use of equity, utilitarian, and Rawlsian princi- performance (Vol. 8, pp. 651-669). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ples of allocation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 9, 165-
Kelley, H. H. (1983). The situational origins of human tendencies: A
175.
further reason for the formal analysis of structures. Personality and
Dawes, R. M. (1980). Social dilemmas. Annual Review of Psychology,
Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 3-30.
31,169-193.
Deutsch, F. M, & Mackesy, M. E. (1985). Friendship and the develop- Kelley, H. H., Berscheid, E., Christensen, A., Harvey, J. H., Huston,
ment of self-schemas: The effects of talking about others. Personality T. L., Levinger, G, McCHntock, E., Peplau, L. A., & Peterson, D. R.
and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1L 399-408. (1983). Analyzing close relationships. In Close relationships (pp. 20-
67). San Francisco: Freeman.
Duck, S. (1988). Handbook of personal relationships: Theory, research
and interventions. New York: Wiley. Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W (1978). Interpersonal relations: A theory
Ferguson, T. J., Rule, B. G., & Carlson, D. (1983). Memory for person- of interdependence. New York: Wiley.
ally relevant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Kuhlman, D. M., & Marshello, A. E J. (1975). Individual differences in
ogy, 44,251-261. game motivation as moderators of preprogrammed strategy effects
Festinger, L., Pepitone, A., & Newcomb, T. (1952). Some consequences in prisoner's dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
of deindividuation in a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- 32, 922-931.
chology, 47, 382-389. Kuiper, N. A. (1981). Convergent evidence for the self as a prototype:
Fiedler, E E., & Chemers, M. M. (1974). Leadership and effective man- The "inverted-U RT effect" for self and other judgments. Personality
agement- Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 438-443.
Freud, S. (1951). Group psychology and the analysis of the ego (J. Stra- Kuiper, N. A., & Rogers, T. E. (1979). Encoding of personal informa-
chey, Trans). New York: Liveright. (Original work published 1921) tion: Self-other differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Grant, V (1976). Ealling in love: The psychology of the romantic emotion. chology, 37, 499-514.
New York: Springer. Lerner, M. J. (1974). The justice motive: "Equity'1 and "parity" among
Greenberg, J. (1978). Allocator-receptor similarity and the equitable children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 539-550.
division of rewards. Social Psychology Quarterly, 41, 337-341. Levinger, G., & Snoek, J. D. (1972). Attraction in relationship: A new
Greenberg, J. (1983), Equity and equality as clues to the relationship look at interpersonal attraction. Morristown, NJ: General Learning
between exchange participants. European Journal ofSocial Psychol- Press.
ogy, 13, 195-196. Lewin, K. (1948). The background of conflict in marriage. In G. Lewin
Greenwald, A. G., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. Wyer& T. (Ed.), Resolving social conflicts: Selected papers on group dynamics
K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 129-178). (pp. 84-102). New York: Harper.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Liebrand, W B. G. (1984). The effect of social motives, communication
Griffin, J. M. (1990). Sexual fantasy, extramarital affairs and marriage and group size on behavior in an A^-person mufti-stage mixed-mo-
commitment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California Gradu- tive game. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 239-264.
ate School of Family Psychology, San Rafael, CA. Liebrand, W B. G., Jansen, R. W T. L., Rijken, V M., & Suhre, C. J. M.
Hatfield, E. (1982). Passionate love, companionate love, and intimacy. (1986). Might over morality: Social values and the perception of
CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS AS INCLUDING OTHER IN THE SELF 253

other players in experimental games. Journal ofExperimental Social Reik, T. (1949). Oflove and lust: On the psychoanalysis ofromantic and
Psychology, 22, 203-215. sexual emotions. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Lord, C. G. (1980). Schemas and images as memory aids: Two modes of Reis, H. T, & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as interpersonal process. In S.
processing social information. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psy- Duck (Ed.), Handbook ofpersonal relationships: Theory, research and
chology, 38, 257-269. interventions (pp. 367-389). New \brk: Wiley.
Lord, C. G. (1987). Imagining self and others: Reply to Brown, ICeenan, Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N, A., & Kirker, W S. (1977). Self-reference and
and Potts. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 53,445-450. the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about Social Psychology, 35, 677-688.
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78. Rosenbaum, M. E. (1986). The repulsion hypothesis: On the nonde-
Maslow, A. H. (1967). A theory of metamotivation: The biological velopment of relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
rooting of the value-life. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 7, 93- ogy, 51M56-1166.
127. Ross, M. 1, Mueller, J. H., & de la Torre, M. (1986). Depression and
Maxwell, G. M.(I985). Behaviouroflovers: Measuring the closenessof trait distinctiveness in the self schema. Journal ofSocial and Clinical
relationships. Journal of Personal and Social Relationships, 2, 215- Psychology, 4, 46-59.
238. Rousar, E. (1990). Valuings role in romantic love. Unpublished doctoral
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dissertation, Pacific Graduate School of Psychology, Palo Alto, CA.


McAdams, D. P. (1988). Personal needs and personal relationships. In
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships: Theory, research Sande, G. N., Goethals, G. R., & Radloff, C E. (1988). Perceiving one's
and interventions (pp. 7-22). New York: Wiley. own traits and others': The multifaceted self. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54,13-20.
McCall, G. X (1974). A symbolic interactionist approach to attraction.
Schutz, A. (1970). On phenomenology and social relations. Chicago:
In T. L. Huston (Ed.), Foundations of interpersonal attraction (pp.
Chicago University Press.
217-231). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Shaver, P., & Rubenstein, C. (1983). Research potential of newspaper
McDonald, G, W (1981). Structural exchange and marital interaction. and magazine surveys. In H. Reis (Ed.), New directions for methodol-
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 43, 825-839. ogy ofsocial and behavioral science: Vol.l 5. Naturalistic approaches to
McKenna, C. (1989). Marital satisfaction and sensation seeking in the studying social interventions (pp. 7 5-91). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
first ten years of marriage: Self-expansion versus boredom. Unpub- Srull, T. K.., & Gaelick, L. (1983). General principles and individual
lished doctoral dissertation, California Graduate School of Family differences in the self as a habitual reference point: An examination
Psychology, San Rafael, CA. of self-other judgments of similarity. Social Cognition, 2, 108-121.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phenomenologie de la perception [Phenom- Storms, M. D. (1973). Videotape and the attribution process: Reversing
enology of perception]. Paris: Gallimard. actors' and observers' point of view. Journal ofPersonality and Social
Mueller, J. H., Ross, M. I, & Heesacker, M. (1984). Distinguishing me Psychology, 27,165-175.
from thee. Bulletin ofthe Psychonomic Society, 22, 79-82. Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of so-
Mueller, J. H., Thompson, W. B., &. Dugan, K. (1986). Trait distinctive- cial behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
ness and accessibility in the self-schema. Personality and Social Psy- psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 181-227). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
chology Bulletin, 12, 81-89. Wegner, D. M. (1980). The self in prosocial action. In D. M. Wegner&
Nisbett, R. E., Caputo, C, Legant, P., & Marecek, J. (1973). Behavior as R. R. Vallacher (Eds.), The self in social psychology (pp. 131-157).
seen by the actor and as seen by the observer. Journal of Personality New York: Oxford University Press.
and Social Psychology, 27,154-164. Youniss, X, & Smollar, X (1985). Adolescent relations with mothers, fa-
Paris, M. (1990). Falling in love and the transformation of self-concept. thers, and friends. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California Graduate School of
Family Psychology, San Rafael, CA. Received March 24,1989
Reik, T. (1944). A psychologist looks at love. New York: Farrar & Rine- Revision received August 25,1990
hart. Accepted August 29,1990 •

You might also like