You are on page 1of 12

EFFECTSOF INTERACTIVEVOCABULARY

INSTRUCTIONON THE VOCABULARY


LEARNINGAND READING
COMPREHENSIONOF JUNIOR-HIGH
LEARNINGDISABLED STUDENTS
Candace S. Bos and Patricia L. Anders

Abstract. Drawing upon theory-driven vocabulary instruction and the vocabulary-


reading comprehension connection, this study compared the effectiveness of three
interactive vocabulary strategies derived from the knowledge hypothesis with defini-
tion instruction derived from the access and instrumental hypotheses. Subjects were
61 learning disabled junior-high students. Using content-area texts, students par-
ticipated in one of three interactive strategies - semantic mapping (SM), semantic
feature analysis (SFA),and semantic / syntactic feature analysis (SSFA)- or in defini-
tion instruction (DI). Learning was measured both at short and long term by
vocabulary and comprehension multiple-choice items and written recalls. Results
from the multiple-choice items suggested that students participating in the inter-
active strategies demonstrated greater comprehension and vocabulary learning than
students receiving definition instruction. Results of the written recalls indicated
qualitatively and quantitatively greater recalls at long term for students in the SFA
and SSFA conditions compared with the DI condition. Implications for research and
practice are discussed.

Much currentreading research and theory sup- and for global measures of comprehension.
port reading as an interactiveprocess: The reader Building upon the work of Anderson and Free-
is characterizedas an active participantwho inter- body (1981), Mezynski (1983) suggested four
acts with the text to construct meaning. From this hypotheses that may explain the relationshipbe-
perspective, the reader uses prior knowledge as tween vocabulary and reading comprehension.
a frameworkto negotiate the meaning of the text One hypothesis, the aptitude hypothesis (e.g.,
(Rumelhart,1980). One of the most importantas- Hunt, 1978), purports that the relationship be-
pects of this priorknowledge is the reader'slabels tween vocabulary and reading comprehension is
for experiences or vocabulary knowledge (John- based on the reader'sunderlyingverbalabilityand
son & Pearson, 1984). Earlyfactor-analysis(e.g., implies that this aptitude is relatively immutable.
Davis, 1944, 1972) and correlationalstudies (e.g.,
Farr, 1969) highlighted the positive relationship
between vocabularyknowledge and readingcom-
prehension. Stahl and Fairbanks(1986) conducted CANDACES. BOS, Ph.D., is Associate Professor
a meta-analysis of studies concerned with the ef- of Special Education, University of Arizona.
fects of vocabularyinstructionon comprehension PATRICIAL. ANDERS, Ph.D., is Associate Pro-
and found a significant effect size for both com- fessor of Language, Reading, and Culture,Univer-
prehension of passages containingthe vocabulary sity of Arizona.

Volume 13, Winter 1990 31

Sage Publications, Inc.


is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve, and extend access to
Learning Disability Quarterly ®
www.jstor.org
A second hypothesis, the instrumentalhypothesis, 1984; Wong, 1980). Second, previousvocabulary
assumes that knowledge of individualword mean- research with learning disabled students (Bos,
ings is the majorfactorrelated to reading compre- Anders, Filip, & Jaffe, 1989; Pany & Jenkins,
hension. Therefore, increasing word knowledge 1978; Pany, Jenkins, & Schreck, 1982) has
by direct teaching of word meanings (cf. Becker, resulted in conflictingfindings in terms of the ef-
1977) would directlyincreasereadingcomprehen- fects of vocabulary instruction on reading com-
sion. Accordingto the thirdhypothesis, the access prehension. Bos et al. (1989) compared semantic
hypothesis, vocabularyknowledge is relatedto the feature analysis, a knowledge-based instruction,
reader'sabilityto access word meanings and use to a dictionarymethod (Gipe, 1978-79) and found
those meanings in text processing.This hypothesis that the interactiveinstructionresulted in greater
drawsheavilyupon notions of automaticityof word reading comprehension and vocabularylearning.
knowledge (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974; Lesgold In contrast, Pany et al. (1982) compared instru-
& Perfetti,1978). Instructionalimplicationsof this mental hypothesis-basedinstructionto a no-instru-
hypothesis would emphasize automatic decoding ction control condition with learningdisabled stu-
and definitiongenerationthroughsystematicprac- dents. While vocabulary learning was facilitated
tice. The fourth hypothesis, the knowledge by the instruction, comprehension, as measured
hypothesis, assumes that vocabularytermsare sur- by oral retellings and cloze tests of constructed
face representationsof underlyingconcepts repre- stories using the vocabulary, was not facilitated.
sented in the reader's knowledge structures or The only comprehension measure that demon-
schemata (Anderson & Freebody, 1981). Instruc- strated instructionaleffects was one that required
tionally, the knowledge hypothesis would imply studentsto answerfactualquestionscontainingthe
that to teach vocabulary, one must teach the vocabulary words.
underlyingconcepts and theirrelationshipsto one The current study directly compared know-
another. ledge-based and instrumental/ access-based vo-
An instructionalinterventioncan be generated cabularyinstructionon learningdisabledstudents'
by combining the instructionalimplicationsfrom reading comprehension. Specifically, this study
the access and instrumentalhypotheses whereas compared the effectiveness of three knowledge-
a contrastingintervention can be generated from based interactivevocabularystrategies- semantic
the instructional implications of the knowledge mapping, semantic feature analysis, semantic /
hypothesis. Specifically, an access / instrumental syntactic feature analysis--with an access/
intervention would focus on definitioninstruction instrumental-basedinstruction(definitioninstruc-
highlightingstudent learningof the vocabularyand tion) on the vocabularylearning,reading compre-
the related definitions to develop easy access, hension, and quality of written recalls of junior-
while a knowledge-basedinterventionwould focus high learning disabled students learning from
on the importance of activating student back- science text.
ground knowledge and developing conceptual
understandingand semantic relationshipsamong METHOD
the vocabulary. Subjects
The purpose of this investigation was to com- Subjects were 61 learning disabled (LD) stu-
pare the effectiveness of two types of theoretically dents recruited from middle- and lower-middle
based vocabularyinstruction:(a) interactivevocab- class junior-highschools in two school districtsin
ulary instructionbased on principlesof the know- a large, metropolitanarea in the Southwest. The
ledge hypothesis and (b) definition instruction 41 males and 20 females had been identified as
based on the access/instrumental hypotheses learning disabled and were receiving services in
using junior-high students identified as learning either resource or self-containedsettings. Eligibility
disabledstudyingscience text. Thispopulationwas criteria included the presence of a severe dis-
selected for two reasons. crepancybetween intellectualfunctioningand one
First,these students have been characterizedas or more academic areas and evidence of one or
not spontaneously generating vocabulary know- more deficitsin cognitive processing. These learn-
ledge (Simmons & Kameenui, 1988) and not ing problems could not be primarilyrelated to ex-
spontaneously employing comprehension and clusionary factors. Subjects were further defined
comprehension-monitoringstrategies(Bos & Filip, by the researchers as having at least normal in-

32 Learning Disability Quarterly


Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Student Characteristics (N= 61)
Characteristics Mean Standard
Deviation

Age 13.80 .93

Full-ScaleIQ 91.97 8.95

VerbalIQ 88.67 10.05

PerformanceIQ 98.00 10.65

ReadingAchievement 81.30 11.20


(Woodcock-Johnson)

PriorKnowledgeof Topic 13.31 2.84


(TotalPossible = 30)

PriorInterestin Topic 22.90 7.69


(TotalPossible =35)

telligence as indicated by either verbal, perfor- with the correct choices balanced across the test.
mance, or full-scale IQ standard scores of at least To determine students' prior knowledge as re-
85 and reading identified as a remediation focus. lated to the experimental passage, a prior-know-
Subject characteristicsare presented in Table 1. ledge assessment was constructed using the 30
Students were randomlyassigned to one of four items from the experimental passage test and 10
intervention conditions and instructed in groups items fromthe practicepassage test (servingas dis-
of 6 to 12. Analyses of variance on the student- tractors). A 7-item topic interest inventory ascer-
characteristic data revealed no significant dif- tained students' prior interest in the topic. Each
ferences among the conditions. item represented a superordinate or coordinate
Materials concept presented in the experimental passage.
Assessment materials. To measure learning, Students rated each statement on a 5-point Likert
a 30-item multiple-choicetest for the experimental scale measuring their interestin learning about it.
passage and a similartest for the practicepassage Instructional materials. The instructional
were developed. Test construction was based on reading materials consisted of a practice and an
content analyses of the passages (Frayer,Fred- experimental passage. Passages were selected
erick, & Klausmeier, 1969), and each test con- from text and trade books used in junior-high
sisted of 15 vocabulary and 15 comprehension science classes. The experimental passage was a
items. The vocabulary items measured students' 1,300-word passage on the topic of fossils with an
knowledge of the context-relatedmeanings of the estimated fifth-to sixth-gradereadabilitylevel (Fry,
vocabulary presented in the passage; the com- 1977). The passage's text structure was mixed,
prehension items, in turn, measured students' beginningwith a narrativeabout a young boy who
understanding of the passage or their ability to is interested in fossils and becomes a geologist.
apply the concepts presented to novel situations. Within the narrative,information about fossils is
Each item on the objectivetest offeredfive options, provided. After the narrative introduction, the

Volume 13, Winter 1990 33


structure switches to descriptive expository. The intervention.The definitioninstruction(DI)activity
practice passage, on the topic of forming ice, was consisted of directlyteaching the definitionsof the
of similar length and readabilityutilizinga com- vocabulary terms (Engelmann & Carnine, 1982;
parison/ contrast text structure. Pany et al., 1982). The method emphasizes oral
The concept-related vocabularyfrom the read- recitation,correctand automaticpronunciationof
ing passages served as the instructionalfocus for each vocabulary word or phrase, and memoriza-
the four interventionconditions. The concept-re- tion of concise context-related definitions. The
lated vocabularywas selected using the same con- vocabulary was learned and reviewed in groups
tent analysis as for the test construction. Context- of five with initial intensive practice followed by
grounded definitionswere generated and verified spaced practice and review. The three interactive
by content experts. For the definition instruction conditions employed interactive, discussion-ori-
condition, the instructionalmaterialsconsisted of ented strategies(Bos & Anders, in press) designed
a writtenlist of the vocabularyand definitions.For to assist students in activatingpriorknowledge, in-
the semanticmappingcondition,instructional mater- stantiatingknowledge, and predictingand drawing
ials consisted of a written list of the vocabulary relationshipsamong the concepts. Forthe seman-
used by the teacher and students to generate a tic mapping (SM) condition, the researcher and
semantic map (Pearson & Johnson, 1978). The students constructed a hierarchical relationship
map was a visual representationof the vocabulary map from the vocabularylist. In the semantic fea-
and the relationshipsamong the differentvocab- ture analysis (SFA) and the semantic/syntactic
ulary.A relationshipmatrix(Anders& Bos, 1986; feature analysis (SSFA)conditions, the researcher
Johnson & Pearson, 1984) was constructed for and students predicted the relationships among
use in the semantic feature analysis condition. To concepts using the relationshipmatrix.The resear-
prepare the matrix, vocabulary representing the cher and students in the SSFA condition also
superordinate concept served as the title, with predictedthe answers for the cloze-typesentences
vocabulary representing the coordinate concepts using their matrix as a guide.
placed along the top of the matrix, and the vo- Duringthe second day of instruction,the resear-
cabulary representing the subordinate concepts cher and students set purposes for readingaccord-
along the side. For the semantic-syntacticfeature ing to theirinstructionalinterventionand students
analysiscondition, cloze-type sentences were writ- then readthe passage. Afterreading,the researcher
ten based on the matrix.Both the relationshipma- and students met as a group in a postreading ac-
trix and the sentences were used during this in- tivity and again discussed their matrix and maps
structional condition. or practicedand reviewedthe vocabularyand their
Procedures definitions. During this time, students were en-
Five researchers served as teachers. All were couraged to use the text to confirm definitions
trained in the instructionalinterventions used in and / or relationships.
the study and randomly assigned to instructional On the third day of instruction, students were
groups with each researcherteaching at least two given 10 minutes with their instructional aide
different conditions and groups. (definitions,map, matrix,or matrixand sentences)
Intervention consisted of eight 50-minute ses- to study for the posttest.They were then instructed
sions over a span of approximatelyseven weeks. to complete writtenrecallsby writing"allyou know
Firststudents were given the prior-knowledgetest about the topic"of the passage includingwhat they
and topic interest inventory.Two weeks later,stu- read, what they learned during instruction, or
dents and their assigned researcher participated other informationthey knew about the topic. Stu-
in three 50-minute practice sessions followed ap- dents were instructedthatthey would not be judged
proximately two weeks later by the three 50- for spellingor grammar,but on the content.Twenty
minute experimentalsessions. Finally,four weeks minutes were providedfor writing,whereupon the
later an additional session was held to collect the multiple-choicetest was administered.To measure
follow-up measures. long-termlearning,students again completed writ-
Duringthe firstday of both the practiceand the ten recalls and the objective test four weeks later.
experimental sessions, students were introduced Data Scoring
to the study whereupon they completed a pre- Each objective test item was computer scored
readingactivitydesigned around theirinstructional as corrector incorrect,and a vocabularyscore and

34 Learning Disability Quarterly


a comprehension score were computed for each recall, an overall quality rating from 0 to 3 was
student. assigned, with 3 representing the richest use of
The written recalls were scored using a variety scriptal knowledge. Finally, a holistic rating was
of procedures. First, they were analyzed for the generated using a 6-point scale rangingfrom 0 to
vocabulary used in the recall. A list of text-related 5 (Irwin& Mitchell, 1983).
vocabularywas generated using the listof instruc- Written recalls were analyzed and scored by
tional vocabulary,their correspondingdefinitions, trainedscorers knowledgeable in discourse analy-
and the content words from the passage. Withthis sis. For each score, the scorers were trained until
list as a template, each recall was scored for the a .85 reliabilitylevel was reached. Afterinitialscor-
number of text-relatedvocabulary.Content words ing, a second scorerrandomlyselected and scored
in the student recalls that were not text-related 15% of the scored recalls. Reliabilitiesfor the dif-
were scored as eitherstudent-relevantvocabulary ferent scores ranged from .86 to .97. The excep-
or student-irrelevantvocabulary. tion to this scoring scheme was the holisticrating.
Second, the conceptual units recalled were In this case, holisticscoringprocedures were used
analyzed and tallied. Conceptual units, adapted (Cooper, 1977; Myers, 1980), includingselection
from Frederiksen's(1975) propositional analysis, of anchored recalls,trainingof raters,and multiple
were defined as ideas that convey meaning. For ratings of each recall.
example, in the sentence, "Fossilscan be bones Data Analysis
from animals who lived long ago,"three concep- Data were analyzedto determinethe short-and
tual units are present (i.e., fossils can be bones; long-term learning effects of the four instructional
bones are from animals; animals lived long ago). conditions on LD students'learningusing a 4 x(2)
Similar to the vocabulary scoring, each concep- mixed design (Lindquist,1953). With the four in-
tual unit was scored as text-related, student- structionalconditions as the between factor, time
relevant, student-irrelevant,or student-inaccurate. of the test (posttest and follow-up) served as the
A conceptual unit was judged as inaccurate if it within factor.The score for the experimental pas-
could be directlydisproven by the text or instruc- sage items on the prior-knowledge assessment,
tional materials. the score for the topic-interestinventory, and the
It was hypothesized that, compared to their full-scale IQ standard score (see Table 1) served
peers, students who received interactive instruc- as covariates in the analyses. These three factors
tion would integrate more elaborated prior or were selected due to theirtheoreticallinkingto the
scriptalknowledge into their written recalls since dependent measures (Anderson,Reynolds, Schal-
these teaching strategiesemphasizedthe activation lert, & Goetz, 1977; Osako & Anders, 1983; Tor-
of prior knowledge (Anders & Bos, 1986). To in- gesen, 1987).
vestigatethis hypothesis, the student-relevantcon-
ceptual units were analyzed for quality of scriptal RESULTS
knowledge generated. This qualityratingwas ob- Covariates
tained by first counting the number of student- The role the covariates played in each analysis
relevant conceptual units. To ascertain quality, varied across the reading test and the written re-
each unit was categorized as elaborate, specific, calls. Althoughpriorknowledge served as a signifi-
or restrictive.This categorizationwas adapted from cant covariate for both the vocabulary score, F
Langer (1982): elaborate scriptal knowledge (1, 54) = 20.43, p < .001, and the comprehen-
represented creative extensions, linkings, analog- sion score, F(1, 54) = 20.00, p < .001, on the
ies, definitions, enriched information,or a predic- readingtest, priorknowledge was not a significant
tion regardingpropositionsin the text (e.g., "Fossils covariatein any of the written-recallanalyses. Prior
are the blueprintsof ancient life.");specific scriptal interestin the topic and IQ were not significantco-
knowledge included attributes, describing char- variates in any of the analyses associated with
acteristics,examples, processes or actions specific either the reading test or the written recalls.
to one proposition (e.g., "Fossilsare big or small."); Reading Test
and restrictivescriptalknowledge contained very Results from the reading test were analyzed for
limited information (e.g., "Fossils can be very both the vocabularyand the comprehensionscore.
small"). Based on both the amount and type of The adjusted means and standard deviations at
scriptal knowledge integrated into the written posttest and follow-up for both scorers are pre-

Volume 13, Winter 1990 35


sented in Table 2. tests for condition, F(3, 54) = 6.29, p < .001,
Vocabulary. For the vocabulary score, results and time, F(1, 57) = 17.66, p < .001, being sig-
of the main-effecttests indicateda significanteffect nificant.Although the interactionfollowed the pat-
for condition, F(3, 54) = 5.37, p < .003, and tern of the vocabularyscores, it was not significant,
for time, F(1, 57) = 59.97, p < .001. A signifi- F(3, 57) = 2.69, p < .06. At posttest, post-hoc
cant interactionbetween condition and time also analyses indicated that students in the interactive
resulted, F(3, 57) = 5.88, p < .002. Post-hoc conditionsscored significantlyhigherthan students
analyses using the Tukey method (Glass & Hop- in the definitioninstructioncondition.At follow-up,
kins, 1984) with a pooled errorterm (Kirk,1968) students in the SSFA conditionscored significantly
and a critical alpha level of .05 showed that at higher than those in the DI condition. Neither at
posttest students participatingin the SFA and SM posttestnor follow-upwere theredifferencesamong
instructionalconditions had learned more vocab- the three interactive instructionalconditions.
ularythan participantsin the DI condition.In terms Written Recalls
of long-term learning (follow-up test), students in Data relatedto the writtenrecallswere analyzed
the three interactiveinstructionalconditions out- using the same design as for the readingtests. Ad-
performedsubjectsreceivingdefinitioninstruction. justed means and standarddeviationsfor the rele-
Neither short- nor long-term learningshowed dif- vant vocabulary generated and the relevant con-
ferences among the three interactiveinstructional ceptual units are presented in Table 3; adjusted
conditions. means and standard deviations for the scriptal-
Comprehension. Results for the comprehen- knowledge quality rating and holistic rating are
sion items were similar,with both the main-effect shown in Table 4.

Table 2
Adjusted Means and Standard Deviations for the Reading Test Vocabulary and
Comprehension Scores (N = 61)
Definition Semantic Semantic Semantic,
Instruction Mapping Feature Syntactic
Analysis Feature
Analysis
(n= 11) (n= 19) (n= 17) (n= 14)
Vocabulary

Posttest 8.02 10.58 10.66 9.46


(2.6) (1.5) (2.4) (1.8)

Follow-Up 5.93 8.11 7.72 9.25


(2.8) (1.9) (2.5) (1.9)

Comprehension

Posttest 7.46 9.98 10.60 10.14


(2.2) (1.6) (2.0) (2.2)

Follow-Up 6.92 8.09 8.19 9.79


(2.1) (2.6) (3.2) (1.9)

36 Learning Disability Quarterly


Table 3
Adjusted Means and Standard Deviations for the Relevant Vocabularyand Relevant
Conceptual Units Generated on the Written Recalls (N = 61)
Definition Semantic Semantic Semantic /
Instruction Mapping Feature Syntactic
Analysis Feature
Analysis
(n = 11) (n= 19) (n = 17) (n = 14)
RelevantVocabulary

Posttest 24.82 19.77 22.97


22.25
(13.8) (8.6) (6.9) (6.4)

Follow-Up 6.25 11.35 14.30 16.83


(5.0) (5.0) (6.3) (6.2)

RelevantConceptualUnits

Posttest 17.74 19.83 15.27 17.82


(15.7) (8.6) (8.1) (7.1)

Follow-Up 2.56 9.04 9.98 13.46


(2.7) (5.5) (5.4) (6.1)

Vocabulary generated. The recalls were for the relevant, accurate conceptual units gener-
analyzed in terms of the relevant vocabularygen- ated, includingthe concepts that were represented
erated including the text-related vocabulary and in the instructionalmaterialsand text and those
the student-relevant vocabulary. The irrelevant that were generatedby the student. Student-irrele-
vocabulary was also scored but not analyzed be- vant conceptual units and student-inaccuratecon-
cause the instances of irrelevantvocabularyin the ceptual units were not analyzed, since the number
writtenrecalls averaged less than one per written of occurrencesper writtenrecallaveraged less than
recall. one.
Results of the analysis revealed significant ef- The main-effects tests showed a significantdif-
fects for time, F(1, 57) = 108.92, p < .001, and ference across posttest and follow-up,F(1, 57) =
the interaction,F(3, 57) = 6.96, p < .001, be- 63.73, p < .001, as well as a significantinteraction,
tween condition and time. Post-hoc analyses on F(3, 57) = 4.61, p < .006. Like the vocabulary
the adjusted means (see Table 3) indicated that generated, post-hoc analyses of the adjusted
although differences were not evident among the means (see Table 3) revealed no significant dif-
four instructionalconditionsimmediatelyfollowing ference among the four instructionalconditions at
instruction,a month later students in the DI con- posttest. At follow-up, the findings also paralleled
dition generated significantlyless vocabularythan the findingsfor relevantvocabularygenerated.The
students in either the SSFA or SFA conditions. No SSFA and SFA students generated significantly
statisticaldifferenceswere evident among the three more conceptual unitsthan subjectsin the DI con-
interactive conditions. dition. No differences were evident among the
Conceptual units. Recallswere also analyzed three interactive conditions.

Volume 13, Winter 1990 37


Scriptal knowledge. Based on the interactive Holistic rating. The same pattern is evident
model, it was predicted that the quality of scriptal with regard to the traditional holistic rating (see
knowledge would be higher for students partici- Table4), with a significanteffect for time, F(1, 57)
pating in the interactivestrategies. The quality of = 65.03, p < .001, and for the interaction, F
scriptalknowledge in the student retellingswas as- (3, 57) = 4.34, p < .005. Accordingto post-hoc
certained by assigning an overall quality rating analyses, no differences were found among the
based on the amount and type of scriptalknow- four conditions at posttest. However, at follow-up
ledge generated: elaborate, specific, or restrictive. students participatingin the SSFA and SFA con-
Although a significanteffect was not evident for ditions generated significantlyhigher holistic rat-
condition, there was a significant effect for time, ings than DI students. No differenceswere evident
F(1, 57) = 11.60, p < .001, and for the interac- among the interactive conditions.
tion of condition and time, F(3, 57) = 3.45,
p < .05. Post-hoc analyses for the adjustedmeans DISCUSSION
(see Table4) indicatethat no differenceswere evi- Previous research with learning disabled
dent among the groups on the ratings measuring students has resulted in conflicting findings with
short-termlearning.However, for long-termlearn- regardto the vocabulary-comprehensionconnec-
ing, students in all three interactiveconditionsgen- tion. Instrumental/ access-based interventions
erated a higher quality of scriptal knowledge in (Pany et al., 1982) have not demonstrateda clear
their recalls than students in the DI condition. No effect on reading comprehension compared with
differences emerged among the interactive a no-instructioncontrol condition; however, inter-
strategies. active or knowledge-based interventions (Bos et

Table 4
Adjusted Means and Standard Deviations for the Quality of Scriptal Knowledge and
the Holistic Rating of the Written Recalls (N-=61)
Definition Semantic Semantic Semantic/
Instruction Mapping Feature Syntactic
Analysis Feature
Analysis
(n= 11) (n= 19) (n= 17) (a = 14)
Qualityof ScriptalKnowledge

Posttest 1.78 2.18 1.68 1.75


(1.0) (1.1) (1.2) (1.2)

Follow-Up .33 1.44 1.56 1.61


(.6) (1.0) (1.3) (1.1)

HolisticRating

Posttest 5.27 6.20 5.17 5.44


(2.9) (1.9) (2.1) (1.9)

Follow-Up 1.73 3.62 4.11 4.09


(1.5) (1.9) (2.2) (1.5)

38 Learning Disability Quarterly


al., 1989) have led to greatercomprehension than ing a context within which to predict definitions
a dictionary method of teaching vocabulary. The and provide definitional information.
currentstudy compared an instrumental/ access- Results from the written recalls demonstrated
based intervention with three knowledge-based a consistent pattern across the various measures,
interventions. but this patternwas not congruent with the results
The results of the reading test and more specif- of the reading test. For the written recalls, dif-
ically the comprehension items indicate that stu- ferences among the four instructionalconditions
dents in the interactiveinterventionsscored higher were not evident when the students were asked
than students engaged in definition learning.' to writeon the day followinginstruction.However,
These findings parallelthe conclusions drawn by one month later, students in the semantic feature
Stahl and Fairbanks(1986) in their meta-analysis analysis and semantic/syntactic feature analysis
of vocabularyinstruction.They concluded that the conditions performed significantlybetterthan stu-
instructionalmethods producing higher compre- dents in the definition instruction condition on
hension effects involved students in deeper pro- vocabularygenerated, conceptual unitsgenerated,
cessing of definition and contextual information and the holistic rating. For the quality of scriptal
and provided students more than one or two ex- knowledge used in the follow-up recall, all three
posures to the to-be-learned words. interactiveconditions resulted in a higher quality
Comparisonof the definitioninstructionand the of recall compared to definition instruction.
interactive instruction reveals clear differences One explanation for the lack of differences in
among the methods in terms of depth of process- short-term recall between the access/instru-
ing and use of definition and contextual informa- mental-basedinstructionand the knowledge-based
tion. Definition instruction focused on correctly instruction may be the nature of written recalls.
pronouncingthe vocabularyand accuratelymem- Most studies with elementary and middle-school
orizing content-related definitions of the words, students have employed oral retellings (e.g.,
more indicative of surface processing. In this in- Adams, Carnine, & Gersten, 1982; McKeown,
structionalconditionstudentswere not encouraged Beck, Omanson, & Pople, 1985; Pany et al.,
to "thinkabout7how the vocabularyrelatedto their 1982), requesting students to retellwhat they just
currentunderstandingor how the concepts might read. One exception was a study of fifth graders
be related one to another. Each vocabulary/ (Wixson, 1986) whose findings were similar to
definition was taught as a separate piece of infor- those of the present investigation;differenceswere
mation (except that students were told they all evident among the vocabulary-instructionme-
related to the superordinate concept of "fossils"). thods for the measure employing comprehension
Students were leftto inferthe relationshipsamong questions but not for the writtenrecalls. The pre-
the vocabularyand their priorknowledge. In con- sent study not only employed written recalls but
trast, the interactiveinterventions highlightedthe also gave students more latitudeto utilizea variety
semantic relationshipsamong the vocabularyand of information including "informationyou have
encouraged students to "thinkabout" what they read about, we have talked about, and other in-
already knew about the concept. formation you may know about the topic" Such
Differencesare also evident in terms of the type broad directions may have served as a cueing
of information highlighted. Definition instruction device for students in the definition-instruction
emphasized relatively context-free definitions. condition, therebystimulatingrecallsthat mirrored
Even though each definitionwas content-related, those provided by students in the interactivein-
it did not provide a richcontext for the definitional structional conditions.
information. This finding is again consistent with At follow-up, results suggest that the quality of
Stahl and Fairbanks'(1986) meta-analysis. These the recalls decreased, but that the decrease dif-
authors noted that methods providingonly defini- fered across the instructionalconditions. The re-
tional informationdid not produce reliableeffects calls reflect a higher quality of long-term learning
on comprehension. In contrast, the interactive for students in the instructional conditions that
strategies highlighted contextual information. employed a relationship matrixcompared to de-
Through discussion, students were encouraged to finition instruction.The use of the matrixguided
activate,share, and elaborateon theirpriorknow- the students and teacher to discuss, predict, and
ledge concerning the vocabulary,thereby provid- confirmthe definitionfor each vocabularyand the

Volume 13, Winter 1990 39


relationshipof each coordinate concept with each tie between research and practice. Can such in-
subordinate concept. Such systematic discussion teractivetechniques be employed by practitioners,
utilizingstudent background knowledge and text and does their use of the strategies lead to similar
informationmay be particularlyfruitfulin terms of findings? Research-based strategies that lack ef-
generating long-term learning. ficacyfor teachersin theirclassroomsare of limited
Instructionally,this study lends support to the use to students, teachers, and intervention
notion that rich, elaboratedvocabularyinstruction researchers.
based on implicationsfrom the knowledge hypo- In the last several years there has been an in-
thesis can facilitate reading comprehension for creasing call for theory-driven intervention re-
learningdisabled students. Such findingshold im- search in learning disabilities(e.g., Wong, 1987).
plications for practitionersand researchers alike. The currentresearchprovidesevidence for interac-
From a practitioner orientation, this study and tive practices that are theoretically derived. By
those described in the Stahl and Fairbanks(1986) more closely examining theory-related practices
meta-analysis and the Weisberg (1988) review such as these, researchersand practitionersalike
challenge the use of practicesthat focus on teach- should find cause to increase their confidence in
ing definitionsisolated from content-area context. the outcome of instructionand their understand-
Learning,especially long-term learning, seems to ing of the tie between research and practice.
occur under conditions that provide adequate op-
portunitiesfor students to (a) activate and instan-
tiate prior knowledge, (b) share that knowledge REFERENCES
with each other, (c) make predictions concerning Adams,A., Carnine,D., & Gersten,R. (1982).Instruc-
the relationships among concepts, and (d) read tionalstrategiesforstudyingcontentareatextin the
to confirm and justify their predictions. If new intermediategrades.Reading ResearchQuarterly,18,
27-55.
learning is based on old learning, it is imperative
that practitionersconsider the curriculumavailable Anders,P.L., & Bos, C.S. (1986). Semanticfeature
to students in content areas. A curriculumof ideas analysis:An interactivestrategyfor vocabularyde-
velopment and text comprehension.Journal of
based on successive lessons and student experi-
Reading, 29, 610-616.
ences is needed to develop sufficient schema for Anderson,R.C., & Freebody,P. (1981). Vocabulary
negotiating meanings from content-area text. The knowledge.InJ.T.Guthrie(Ed.),Comprehension
and
results of this study seem to promote the notion teaching:Researchreviews (pp. 77-117). Newark,DE:
of engaging students in ideas, thereby facilitating International
ReadingAssociation.
long-term learning that becomes schema upon Anderson,R.C., Reynolds,R.E., Schallert,D.L., &
which new learning can occur. Goetz,E.T.(1977). Frameworksforcomprehending
For researchers, these findings raise several discourse. American Educational Research Journal,
questions. First, the lack of congruence between 14, 367-382.
the comprehension and composition measures Becker,W.C.(1977). Teachingreadingand language
- Whatwe have learnedfrom
to the disadvantaged
during short-term learning poses a challenging field research. Harvard Educational Review, 46,
question. To what degree must strategy-based 518-543.
writinginstruction(e.g., Englert& Raphael, 1988) Bos, C.S.,& Anders,P.L.(inpress).Towardan interac-
be paired with vocabulary instructionto bringthe tive model:Teachingtext-basedconceptsto learning
two processes closer together? Observationsdur- disabledstudents. In H.L. Swanson & B. Keogh
ing instruction and teacher reports made it clear (Eds.), Learningdisabilities;Theoreticaland research
that writtenrecalls did not constitute a frequently issues. Hillsdale,NJ: ErlbaumAssociates.
used means of measuring content knowledge. Bos, C.S., Anders,P.L.,Filip,D., & Jaffe,L.E.(1989).
Would students'comprehension be quantitatively The effectsof an interactiveinstructionalstrategyfor
and /or qualitativelydifferentif composing was a enhancinglearningdisabledstudents'readingcom-
more conventionalinstructionalexperience?A sec- prehensionand content area learning.Journalof
Learning Disabilities, 22, 384-390.
ond question focuses on the discussionsthat occur
Bos, C.S.,& Filip,D. (1984).Comprehension
monitor-
during instruction.What are the characteristicsof ingin learningdisabledandaveragestudents.Journal
the discussion duringinteractivestrategiesthat are of Learning Disabilities, 17, 229-233.
distinguishablefrom discussion when definitions Cooper,C.R. (1977). Holisticevaluationof writing.In
are emphasized ? A third question deals with the C.R. Cooper& L. Odell (Eds.),Evaluatingwriting

40 Learning Disability Quarterly


(pp.3-31). Buffalo,NY:NationalCouncilof Teachers McKeown,M.G.,Beck, I.L.,Omanson,R.C.,& Pople,
of English. M.T.(1985).Some effectsof the natureandfrequency
Davis,F.B.(1944).Fundamental factorsof comprehen- of vocabularyinstructionon the knowledgeand use
sion in reading.Psychometrika,9, 185-197. of words.ReadingResearchQuarterly, 20, 522-535.
Davis,F.B.(1972). Psychometric researchon compre- Mezynski, K. (1983). Issues the
concerning acquisition
hensionin reading.ReadingResearchQuarterly,7, of knowledge: Effectsof vocabularytrainingon
628-678. reading comprehension.Review of Educational
Engelmann,S., & Carnine,D.W.(1982). Theoryof in- Research,53, 253-279.
struction:Principlesand applications.New York: Myers,M.A.(1980).A procedureforwriting assessment
Irvington. andholisticscoring.Urbana,IL:ERICClearinghouse,
Englert,C.S., & Raphael,T.E. (1988). Constructing NationalInstituteof EducationandNationalCouncil
well-formedprose: Process, structure,and meta- of Teachersof English.
cognitive knowledge. ExceptionalChildren,54, Osako, G.N., & Anders, P.L. (1983). The effect of
513-520. reading intereston comprehensionof expository
Farr,R. (1969). Reading: Whatcan be measured? materialswithcontrolsfor priorknowledge.In J.A.
Newark,DE: International ReadingAssociation. Niles& L.A. Harris(Eds.),Searchesfor meaningin
Frayer,D.A., Frederick,W.C., & Klausmeier,H.J. reading/languageprocessingandinstruction (Thirty-
(1969). A schema for testingthe level of concept second yearbook,pp. 56-60). Rochester,NY:Na-
mastery (WorkingPaper No. 16). Madison:The tionalReadingConference.
Universityof Wisconsin,WisconsinResearchand Pany,D., & Jenkins,J.J. (1978).Learningwordmean-
DevelopmentCenterfor CognitiveLearning. ings:A comparisonof instructional proceduresand
Frederiksen,C.H. (1975). Representinglogical and effectson measuresof readingcomprehensionwith
semanticstructuresof knowledgeacquiredfromdis- learning disabled students. Learning Disability
course. CognitivePsychology,7, 371-458. Quarterly,1(2), 21-32.
Fry, E. (1977). Fry'sreadabilitygraph:Clarifications, Pany,D.,Jenkins,J.J.,& Schreck,J. (1982).Vocabulary
validity and extensions to level 17. Journal of instruction:Effectson wordknowledgeand reading
Reading,21, 242-252. comprehension.LearningDisabilityQuarterly,5,
Gipe,J.P.(1978-79).Investigating techniquesforteach- 202-215.
ingwordmeanings.ReadingResearchQuarterly, 14, Pearson, P.D., & Johnson, D.D. (1978). Teaching
624-644. readingcomprehension.New York:Holt, Rinehart,
Glass,G.V.,& Hopkins,K.D.(1984).Statistical methods & Winston.
in educationand psychology(2nded.). Englewood Rumelhart, D.E.(1980).Schemata:Thebuildingblocks
Cliffs,NJ: Prentice-Hall. of cognition.InR.J.Spiro,B.C.Bruce,& W.EBrewer
Hunt,E. (1978). Mechanicsof verbalability.Psycho- (Eds.),Theoretical issuesin readingcomprehension
logicalReview,85, 109-130. (pp. 33-58). Hillsdale,NJ: ErlbaumAssociates.
Irwin,P.A., & Mitchell,J.N. (1983). A procedurefor Simmons,D.C.,& Kameenui,E.J.(1988).Learning dis-
assessingthe richnessof retellings.Journalof Read- abledand normalachievers'vocabularyknowledge:
ing, 26, 391-396. A quantitativeand qualitativeanalysis. In J.E.
Johnson,D.D.,& Pearson,P.D.(1984).Teachingread- Readence& R.S.Baldwin(Eds.),Dialoguesin literacy
ing vocabulary(2nded.). New York:Holt,Rinehart, research (Thirty-seventh yearbook,pp. 133-140).
& Winston. Chicago:NationalReadingConference.
Kirk,R.E.(1968).Experimental design:Proceduresfor Stahl,S.A., & Fairbanks, M.M.(1986). The effectsof
the behavioral sciences.Belmont,CA:Brooks/ Cole. vocabulary instruction:A mode-basedmeta-analysis.
LaBerge,D.,& Samuels,S.J. (1974).Towarda theory Reviewof EducationalResearch,56, 72-110.
of automaticinformation processingin readingcom- Torgesen,J.K. (1987). Thinkingabout the futureby
prehension.CognitivePsychology,6, 293-323. distinguishingbetweenissues that have resolutions
Langer,J.A. (1982). Facilitating text processing:The and those that do not. In S. Vaughn& C.S. Bos
elaboration of priorknowledge.InJ.A.Langer& M.T. (Eds.),Researchin learningdisabilities: Issues and
Smith-Burke(Eds.),Readermeetsauthor/bridging futuredirections(pp.55-64). San Diego:College-Hill
the gap (pp. 149-162). Newark,DE: International Press.
ReadingAssociation. Weisberg,R. (1988). 1980s:A changein focusof read-
Lesgold,A.M.,& Perfetti,C.A. (1978).Interactive pro- ing comprehensionresearch:A reviewof reading/
cesses in readingcomprehension.DiscoursePro- learningdisabilitiesresearchbasedon an interactive
cesses, 1, 323-326. modelof reading.LearningDisabilityQuarterly,11,
Lindquist,E.R. (1953). Design and analysisof ex- 149-159.
perimentsin psychology and education. Boston: Wixson,K.K.(1986).Vocabulary instructionandchild-
HoughtonMifflin. ren's comprehension of basal stories. Reading

Volume13, Winter1990 41
Research Quarterly,21, 317-329. tional differencesbetween the three interactivestrategies.
Wong, (1980). Activatingthe inactivelearner:Use
BY.L.
of questions/prompts to enhance comprehension This research was funded through the Interactive
and retention of implied informationin learning dis- Teaching Project (G008630125) from the Office of
abled children. Learning Disability Quarterly,3(1), Special Education and Rehabilitative Services, U.S.
20-37. Department of Education.
Wong, B.Y.L.(1987). Conceptual and methodological
issues in interventionswith learning-disabledchildren We wish to thank the special education teachers and
and adolescents. In S. Vaughn & C.S. Bos (Eds.), theirstudents at Doolen, FlowingWells, Magee, Secrist,
Research in learning disabilities:Issues and future and UtterbackJunior High Schools for allowing us to
directions (pp. 185-196). San Diego: College-Hill work in their schools. A special thanks to Adela Allen,
Press. Judy Mitchell, Grace Duran, MargaretGallego, Elba
FOOTNOTES Reyes, and Dave Scanlon for their assistance.
'The lack of statistical differences between the three
interactive strategies may be related to a Type II error Requests for reprintsshould be addressed to: Candace
due to insufficient power associated with the small Bos, Division of Special Education and Rehabilitation,
number of students in the interventionconditions. This College of Education,Universityof Arizona,Tucson, AZ
limitationdoes not allowfor discussionrelatedto instruc- 85721.

42 Learning Disability Quarterly

You might also like