You are on page 1of 12

Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Children and Youth Services Review


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/childyouth

Attachment theory and change processes in foster care


David J. Tucker a,⁎, Michael J. MacKenzie b
a
School of Social Work, University of Michigan, United States
b
School of Social Work, Columbia University, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Despite wide acceptance in the multifaceted field of child care policy and practice, attachment theory has
Received 4 June 2012 found limited use in examining empirically the circumstances and conditions of special populations of
Received in revised form 24 July 2012 children. This inquiry addresses this limitation by elaborating attachment theory as a foundation for
Accepted 30 July 2012
contemporary foster care practice and policy. We focus on how caregiving contexts and the nature of their
Available online 9 August 2012
change selects certain characteristics and behaviors as relevant in explaining a child's risk of placement
Keywords:
change in, or exit from, foster care. We use data on a population of 3448 foster children over a 21-year period
Attachment theory to test arguments that children's strategies for dealing with change can be both resistant and adaptive, and
Behavioral inertia that self-perpetuating patterns of attachment can contribute to increasing rates of change in children's lives.
Foster care Results strongly support attachment theory as a transactional theory of change. Placement change not only
Placement change influences the hazard of exit in the manner predicted but also engenders a “liability of change,” with early
Internal working model change influencing the likelihood of future change independent of contextual and child characteristics. From
the perspective of this inquiry, future research that omits information on the history and timing of significant
changes in children's lives will be limited in its capacity to explain their current circumstances.
© 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction hypotheses designed to test AT's efficacy as an interpretive lens for


foster care dynamics; and then explore in detail the results and their
1.1. Purpose implications.

The relevance of insights from John Bowlby's attachment theory (AT) 1.2. Background
to child care policy and practice are so widely accepted that the need for
confirmation through empirical testing and analysis is only cursorily In foster care practice, Bowlby's insights undergird a strong
acknowledged. Significantly, such acceptance has helped promote two professional consensus regarding the importance of matching foster
complimentary limitations (Rutter & O'Connor, 1999). One is a tendency home strengths and capabilities with child need and pursuing quick-
to apply the principles of AT across differing populations of children as-viable family reunification (Pecora, Whittaker, Maluccio, Barth, &
without qualification. The complement is its limited use in examining DePanfilis, 2009; Steinhauer, 1991). Both practices are regarded as
the circumstances and conditions of special populations of children by important in minimizing potentially negative effects of relationship
elaborating relevant aspects of the theory in terms specific to such disruption on children (Rutter & O'Connor, 1999). Failing reunifica-
populations. tion, the consensus calls for creating conditions that AT predicts are
This inquiry seeks to address these limitations by exploring AT as a crucial to a child's healthy psycho-social development, stable and
foundation for contemporary foster care practice and policy. We focus secure social bonds with significant adult caregivers (Bowlby, 1988;
mainly on how caregiving contexts and the nature of their change D'Andrade, 2005; Steinhauer, 1991).
selects certain characteristics and behaviors as relevant in explaining In practice, foster care dynamics seldom yield conditions empirically
a child's risk of placement change in, or exit from, foster care. Within consistent with the implications of AT principles. Indeed, evidence of
this framework, we seek to determine, empirically not cursorily, constant-to-increasing duration times to foster care exit or family
whether AT can deepen understanding of the transactional nature of reunification indicate that the potentially negative effects for children of
contemporary foster care dynamics. To this end, we review recent relationship disruption are not sufficiently minimized (Child Welfare
work in the research tradition of AT and in foster care; posit several Information Gateway, 2010; Children's Bureau, 2010). Findings also
point to extended duration in care without movement to permanency,
and of comparatively frequent placement changes. Both support an
⁎ Corresponding author. expectation of proportionately higher numbers of children with behavior
E-mail address: celdjt@umich.edu (D.J. Tucker). problems (Children's Bureau, 2010).

0190-7409/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.07.020
D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219 2209

Motivated by these stubborn realities, researchers are working to with maintaining, rather than changing, existing behavioral reper-
identify relevant causal factors and conditions; recent studies show toires (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton & Waters, 1985; Crick & Dodge,
that such factors as age and nature of placement setting can predict 1994; Sroufe et al., 1999). This implies differential survival rates for
both duration to exit and risk of placement change (Akin, 2011; children's relationships with caregivers. On one hand, failure to
Connell et al., 2006; van Santen, 2010). Researchers thus suggest that, adapt by either or both parties could result in termination of a
if the effects of such factors can be mitigated, greater stability and relationship (Fichman & Levinthal, 1991). On the other, unilateral or
more continuity in child-caregiver relationships will result, thereby mutual adjustments could increase chances of relationship success.
producing more positive outcomes for children (Connell et al., 2006; This leads to the expectation that patterns of behavior evinced by
Pecora et al., 2009; Rutter & O'Connor, 1999). children in dealing with contexts of relationship change will reflect both
The research reported in this paper builds on and contributes to resistant and adaptive tendencies and that, in fact, self-perpetuating
this literature. However, our emphasis is not solely on examining patterns of attachment can actually contribute to increased rates of
relationships between rates of placement change or exit and child relationship change in children's lives.
characteristics. We are also concerned with the fact that relationships In this paper, we use the working model approach to children and
between child characteristics and rates of change and exit affect change to derive seven hypotheses particularized to children in
variation in the prevalence of child characteristics themselves. That is, foster care: the first three addresses how placement change affects
while current research treats child-specific variables like age as risk of exit from foster care; the remaining four consider the effects
influencing rates of change and exit, distributions of such character- of change itself on subsequent rates of placement change, indepen-
istics in observed populations also reflect effects of earlier changes in dent of the effects of contextual and child characteristics. While the
rates of change or exit (Swan & Sylvester, 2006). working model idea is owed to Bowlby, we expand its scope by
Thus, it is vital to understand how child characteristics influence risk incorporating aspects of representational theory, theory of mind,
of placement change or exit as they interact with the caregiving transactional theory, and organizational ecology. The result is a broader
contexts in which foster children are embedded. Accordingly, this study conception of children's working models which, combined with our
focuses less on how particular characteristics affect a child's propensity analysis, affirms the importance of conditioning interpretations of AT to
to exit care or sustain placement change. Rather it generates and tests the conditions and circumstances of special populations of children.
hypotheses about dynamic transactional processes underlying how
caregiving contexts select certain characteristics and behaviors (includ- 2. Hypotheses
ing the process of change itself) as important in explaining a child's risk
of change or exit. 2.1. Placement change and risk of exit

1.3. Views of the child in attachment theory Recent research on internal working models identified and described
their internal components, or schemas (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999;
As a “general theory” or “research tradition” (Laudan, 1977), AT has Bretherton & Waters, 1985; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). These
spawned a range of specific and sometimes conflicting ideas about how sequentially ordered structures contain information about how one
to answer questions and solve problems in the intersecting domains of interacts with specific others, under certain conditions and in specific
child development and behavior. situations, to achieve particular ends (Schank & Abelson, 1977). Schemas
One set of conflicting assumptions—those regarding children and define a child's knowledge of relationships and the actions and outcomes
change—are particularly important to this investigation. Some represent associated with the effectuation of that knowledge. While the idea of
children as malleable, able to adapt to the requirements of change, given working models as organized bundles of schemas implies differences
a supportive contemporaneous environment (Lewis, 2001). Others among children's working models, current literature identifies a limited
understand them as inflexible, based on arguments that attachment number of major forms of attachment organization (Crittenden, 1999;
patterns tend to be self-perpetuating and change resistant regardless of Main et al., 1985) as the range of possible caregiver responses to
context (Thompson & Raikes, 2003). The first view implies the viability attention-seeking behavior also is limited (Main et al., 1985). However,
of studying children's behavior using cross-sectional designs and static the idea of schema-based working models does give a more precise basis
forms of analysis. The second emphasizes children's development for thinking about how change will affect a child's relationship with its
histories and affirms longitudinal designs and dynamic forms of environment. Specifically, if stable, reproducible schemas are integral to
analysis. Following others (Bowlby, 1969; MacKenzie & McDonough, a child's attachment behavioral system and guide behavior, and if change
2009; Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990), we construe these views as calls into question the efficacy of these schemas, change is likely to
complimentary and adopt Bowlby's internal working model concept produce anxiety, defensive exclusion, and defensive misattribution that
as an organizing assumption (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980). Thus, we take interfere with the child's ability to contend constructively with its
into account both development history and current circumstance. immediate environment (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999). Within the
An internal working model—a relational network of internalized foster care system, these behaviors are likely to prove disruptive,
knowledge constructed on the basis of attachment experiences— lowering a child's chances of adapting well to a caring context and
encapsulates a child's expectations of external reality and its role in increasing the chances of being labeled problematic. This in turn likely
that reality. The child uses its internal working model to anticipate will lower the possibility, or risk, of exiting the foster care system. Thus:
and evaluate probable outcomes of purposive relationships, establish-
ing the security needed to form relationships. In time, cognitive Hypothesis 1. Placement change in foster care decreases a child's risk
repertoires associated with internal working models are routinized of exiting foster care, independent of the effects of other character-
(Bowlby, 1973; Crick & Dodge, 1994), and the child requires less istics and conditions.
conscious deliberation to invoke the working model. Thus the child Hypothesis 1 implies the effects of placement change are uniform
manages relationships more stably and efficiently. across children. We think this is unlikely in one important sense.
From the internal working model perspective, children approach Specifically, we expect child's age at time of placement change will
new situations not as blank slates but with “…certain preconceptions, affect subsequent risk of exit. How that effect will manifest is subject
behavioral biases, and interpretive tendencies” (Sroufe, Carlson, Levy, & to competing arguments.
Egeland, 1999, p. 5) and tend to interpret current environmental cues in a The first argument is consistent with Bowlby's claims (Bowlby,
manner consistent with past interpretations (Dodge, Pettit, Mcclaskey, & 1969, 1973, 1988) that a positive relationship exists between increasing
Brown, 1986). They also elicit reactions from the environment consistent age and increasingly inflexible patterns of behavior, and with Sroufe's
2210 D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219

(1997) assertion that pathology is “… a pattern of adaptation reflecting care-giving environments depends, in part, on how secure similar
the totality of the developmental context to that point.” Applied to foster relationships were in earlier environments. Secure attachments' positive
care, this argument implies that as children age into foster care, patterns effects on confidence and self-image promote capacity for exploratory,
of behavior are likely to become less flexible, resulting in more consistent, autonomous behavior vis-à-vis current environments (Verschueren,
more apparent patterns of dysfunctional attachment or detachment Marcoen, & Schoefs, 1996). Children with secure earlier attachments
behaviors. In turn, such behaviors are likely to lead to assessments that thus have potential for greater sensitivity, or openness, to the specifics of
they remain in care. Overall, therefore, this argument suggests that the any new context. This implies that these children have a higher
disruptive effects of change will increase with age. Thus: probability of experiencing change and more opportunities to learn
about its meaning and management. Insecure attachments contribute
Hypothesis 2a. The older the child at the time of a placement change, to lower levels of confidence and a poorer self-image and thus, to less
the lower the subsequent risk of exit. openness to the specifics of a given context and more timid approach to
Crittenden's (1999, 2000) dynamic-maturational approach to attach- engaging it. Accordingly, children with insecure attachments who
ment suggests an alternative argument. Specifically, it cautions against experience change in a care-giving environment will experience it
overemphasizing security and underemphasizing danger as children's mainly as a negative external imposition, something to be resisted for
basis for working models. Danger has been a constant feature of human the costs it exacts, not embraced for the benefits it bestows. It is
environments, and it is reasonable to presume that, as a matter of reasonable that children with secure past attachments are more likely
survival, children construct self-protective strategies to deal with it. to adapt to the requirements of a new situation than those with a
These strategies are designed to influence caregiver behavior, rendering history of insecure attachments. Hence they are likely to have lower
it more predictable to the child in self-protective ways. For younger rates of change.
children, these strategies are simple and direct. However, with The capacity to adapt to new environments is not without limit.
maturation, children are increasingly able to consider and modify Indeed, we argue that children tend to evolve relatively stable
their own mental processes, resulting in more sophisticated and strategies of environmental engagement and are reluctant to change,
complex strategies characterized, among other things, by a “freeing even when confronted with contexts for which these strategies may
of mental activity from behavioral enactment” (Crittenden, 1999, p. not be highly adaptive. Clearly, a child's behavioral repertoire cannot
169). For example, “… a school age child may mislead others with be so flexible that it anticipates the requirements of adapting to the
regard to his intentions by hiding evidence of his mental function- full range of environmental contingencies to which it may be
ing,” or an adolescent may hide her feelings with one person but act exposed. By the same token, a one-size-that-fits-all strategy that
on them with another (Crittenden, 1999). depends on environmental constancy is not likely to function either.
Assuming that foster children generally consider themselves more It is more plausible that optimal environmental engagement re-
vulnerable when living among strangers ( Lyon, 1987; Mrazek & Mrazek, quires a balance between acknowledging the environment's com-
1987; Steele, 1986; Weinstein, 1960), children aging into foster care will plexity and treating it as an unchanging constant (Bowlby, 1969,
represent themselves behaviorally as adapted to that environment in the 1973). This strategy enhances efficiency and complexity. Based on
interest of increasing their chances of exiting foster care and returning past learning and experience, a child's behavior patterns become
home, or living independently. When presented with a change, they organized and consistent, strengthening its security and competence
could show themselves as coping well, thus increasing the prospects of in uncertain environments. However, this strategy also promotes
exiting. Empirically this manifests in a direct relationship between age behavioral inertia. Because routinized behaviors relieve a child of the
and risk of exit, suggesting: requirement to deal with each social interaction by conscious learning
and decision-making, routines are efficient and valuable. Hence, a child
Hypothesis 2b. The older the child at the time of a placement change,
may meet with resistance circumstances that need or demand behavioral
the higher the subsequent risk of exit.
change (Crick & Dodge, 1994, pp. 80–81).
For children persisting in foster care, effects of placement change are
Assuming a propensity to resist change requires attention to the
not likely to disappear immediately. Changes prompt circumstances and
timing of changes. Prospects for adapting to change improve if a child's
conditions that parallel repeated attempts to initiate establishment of
response matches the temporal pattern of change in its environment.
new relationships. Both theory and research (Jovanovic, 1979; Land,
However, our argument for behavioral inertia implies that this is
1984; March & March, 1977; Tucker, Hurl, & Ford, 1994; Vuchinich,
unlikely, suggesting that behavioral change tends to be slow relative to
Teachman, & Crosby, 1991) hold that relationships have the highest risk
environmental change. Behavioral inertia is indicated when a child's
of failure during initial formation, with risk of termination declining
adjustment lags substantially behind the rate of environmental change.
monotonically. That is, the longer a relationship lasts, the less likely it is to
Because of their greater environmental sensitivity and superior behav-
terminate. Given a degree of environmental predictability after change, a
ioral repertoires, we expect children with more secure attachment
person can rebuild and reorganize internal structures and processes,
histories to demonstrate lower levels of behavioral inertia vis-à-vis
stabilizing capacity to develop and maintain external relations (Bowlby,
environmental change—adjusting more quickly, thus successfully to
1988).
caregiving change—than children with insecure attachment histories.
For foster care, this implies that the longer a child's relationship
Foster care is often characterized by instability in care-giving
with a foster family, the child is more likely to fit with that context
environments brought about by frequent changes in children's place-
and less likely to be labeled problematic. Hence, we expect stability in
ments. Considering behavioral inertia, few things seem as adverse to a
placement after change to be associated with increased risk of exit.
child's wellbeing as the child committing to change only to find, upon
Thus:
changing, that the context requires different behavior! How is a child
Hypothesis 3. Disruptive effects of placement change attenuate with likely to react? How will he or she organize, behaviorally, to deal with
length of time since last change, resulting in a higher propensity to subsequent care-giving environments?
exit the system. We think the answer lies in recognizing that, when it comes to foster
care, the propensity toward behavioral inertia is different, in that there
are twin implications for change. As described above, behavioral inertia
2.2. Behavioral inertia and placement change occurs in a conventional sense when a child maintains a particular
pattern of behavior and resists change. But, there is another form of
According to the conventional conception of working models in inertia, momentum (Amburgey, Kelly, & Barnett, 1993), which occurs
AT, how children deal with changes in social relationships in current when the processes invoked for dealing with change are themselves
D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219 2211

routinized. Applied in the context of this research, the momentum view relationship, and conveying a view of the child as remaining in the
of inertia allows that the way a child dealt with a particular kind of family (Tucker et al., 1994). Thus:
change in the past may be the way he or she manages it in the future.
Thus, the child uses change to deal with problems in the environment, Hypothesis 5. The probability of placement change decreases with
regardless of whether it solves those problems (Levitt & March, 1988). the time since the last placement change.
Concomitantly, the child's own behavior becomes a factor in explaining Along with the dynamics underpinning Hypothesis 5, a child's age
change (Sameroff & MacKenzie, 2003). also is likely to affect placement change. We expect that older children
Building on our inertia-momentum argument, we turn to Bowlby's will be more likely than younger children to experience placement
(1973) pathways model of personality development. Represented change. As children age they are treated differently by their environ-
metaphorically as a train on a main line switching to branch lines, it ments: in many ways, these environments become more complex and
elaborates how a child's prior experience with change may produce demanding. Since environmental change requires children to change
maladaptation in his or her current environment (Bowlby, 1973, p. 365, and adapt (Marvin & Britner, 1999), the nature and complexity of their
370). Specifically, maladaptive behavior emerges as successive “junction internal working models must change if they are to continue as
switches” that take a child farther from pathways leading to competent appropriate guides for the assessments of, and expectations in, given
social functioning. Sroufe (1997) affirmed this argument, observing that social interactions. For these change to occur, a child must have available
maladaptive behavior emerges from a succession of deviations from sufficient attentional resources to acknowledge, interpret, and encode
normative patterns. Each succeeding change lowers the child's proba- relevant socio-behavioral information from its environment (Crick &
bility of adapting positively to subsequent contexts (see also Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge et al., 1986; Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). In turn,
Dodge, 1994; Moffitt, 1993; Newton, Litrownik, & Landsverk, 2000). AT implies that, ceteris paribus, a child's attentional resources at any
In foster care, we expect this pattern to present in a child's given time are a function of current and past felt security.
increasingly detached behavior, treating caregivers as members of a By definition, foster care entails disruptions in family-based
singular, non-responsive category the child regards as not interested caregiving arrangements and placement in unfamiliar environ-
in, or committed to, social interaction that will advance positive ments. Moreover, many children have been exposed to problematic,
outcomes for him or her (Howes, 1999). Caregivers may experience perhaps frightening, conditions in family caregiving contexts (Dozier,
reciprocal effects, increasingly regarding children with frequent Albus, Fisher, & Sepulveda, 2002). This can have damaging consequences,
placement changes as inherently unmanageable and unrewarding including deficits in children's abilities to conceptualize, understand, and
because of their behavior and (perceived) limited prospects for make inferences about the thoughts, beliefs, and emotions of others
regaining positive, adaptive developmental trajectories. With this (Cicchetti, Rogosch, Maughan, Toth, & Bruce, 2003; Pears & Fisher, 2005).
mutual skepticism about the benefits of investing time, effort, and Hence many children may enter foster care with limited attentional
energy in relationship, child and caregiver commitments to relationship resources, primed to develop, if not already evidencing, maladaptive
become increasingly superficial; invoking placement change to manage behaviors. Accordingly they may be limited in their capacity to develop
problems of social interaction becomes easier and may be routinized in new and different relationships in current caregiving environments
the child's behavioral repertoire. In this way, once a child experiences (Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986) and may have problems negotiating subsequent
placement change, momentum for additional changes may develop developmental issues and changes (Crittenden, 1999; Sroufe, 1997). As
since change is part of what the child expects, knows how to do, and such, comparatively older foster children may emerge as problems for
may even work to invoke (Newton et al., 2000; Palmer, 1996). Thus: caregivers (James, 2004; Rhodes, Orme, & Buehler, 2001), which in turn
negatively influences foster families' ability or willingness to care for
Hypothesis 4. The probability of a placement change increases with them. Thus:
the number of previous placement changes.
Hypothesis 6. The probability of a placement change increases with
the age of the child.
2.3. The dynamics of placement change
Finally, we think age also is consequential vis-à-vis the timing of
placement change. Bowlby's pathways model (1973), refined by Sroufe
Hypothesis 4 implies something like a liability of change: Change
(1997), conceptualizes children's problems not as specific conditions
engenders change and thus a propensity not to persist in a given
endogenous to individuals but as maladaptations resulting from de-
context.
viations over time in successive adaptations to the environment. As such,
Yet, evidence indicates that children can move through periods of
it anticipates earlier failures to adapt as predictive of later failures
anxiety, change, and maladaptive behavior to achieve periods of
(Sroufe, 1997, p. 253). Considered in the context of our preceding
relative stability (Horwitz, Balestracci, & Simms, 2001; Howes &
arguments, this proclivity implies that placement change probability
Segal, 1993; Lewis, Feiring, & Rosenthal, 2000; Weinfield, Sroufe, &
depends on change frequency, and how recently it occurred, and on its
Egeland, 2000). Can this literature be reconciled with Hypothesis 4?
timing in a child's tenure in foster care. That is—accepting placement
We think the answer may lie in the dynamics of change itself. More
change as certainly stressful for a child and as indicative of some level of
specifically: the chance of a given change recurring depends on how
failure to adapt—the implication of the pathways model, ceteris paribus,
recently that type of change occurred. Foster care contexts provide
is that early change is likely to be followed by a propensity for further
different treatment over time than care received in an earlier context.
changes. Thus:
Thus a child's working model of attachment relationships is delayed but
nonetheless gradually updated and reorganized. This reorganizing Hypothesis 7. An early change in placement increases the probability
results in the foregrounding and perhaps generalization of more of subsequent placement changes.
recently developed and used schema. Assuming all other things as
equal, and accepting that children are more likely to invoke recently
used simulations of reality afforded by newer schemas than older and 3. Methods
less frequently used ones, we argue that the longer the time since the
last invocation of schemas promulgating change as an adaptive To test the seven hypotheses discussed above, we analyzed foster
strategy, the less likely they are to be accessed and used in the child exits and placement changes for 3448 children taken into care in
present. In foster care, the child's caregivers are likely to develop a major urban area in Ontario, Canada, between January 1970 and
routines that acknowledge and deal with the child's presence, both June 1990. Data sources were archival, and included confidential files
shaping and conforming to the child's expectations of a working on the children maintained by the local child welfare authority as well
2212 D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219

100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800


as various lists, documents, and reports from other sources—including
different levels and departments of government, local agencies, and other
special-purpose child welfare bodies. These data sources provided time-
varying information on both children and their environments. For each

Numbers of Changes
child, this included crucial timing information, recorded as to day, month
and year, on their entry into foster care, all placement changes, and their
final exit from the foster care system. We used these data to construct
event histories for each child in the form of a sequence of time periods, or
“spells,” separating each event. Each child's first spell began when he or
she first came into care and ended with the first change event the child
experienced—whether a placement change, meaning movement into a
new foster home, or an exit, meaning a termination of the child welfare
authority's responsibility to care for the child in a licensed foster care
facility. If the child persisted in foster care after the first change event, the

0
second spell began the day after the first event and ended with the
second event, and so on. We divided spells persisting for more than one 1970 1972 1974 1976 1978 1980 1982 1984 1986 1988 1990
year into annual increments ending with a dummy event recording Year
whether or not it involved a change in placement status. This method of
Fig. 2. Number of placement changes by six-month intervals 1970–1990.
organizing data allowed variable values to change over time and ensured
that time-varying covariates were updated annually.
Fig. 1 describes foster home exit levels by six-month periods. It and thus an increased risk of exiting the foster care system, as anticipated
shows an initial low rate of exit, increasing to a relatively stable level by by Hypothesis 1. The passage of time results in an increasing hazard and
the second half of 1971, dropping sharply in the second half of 1984 and thus a higher risk of exiting the system, as anticipated by Hypothesis 3.
first half of 1985. After this, exit levels compare to the pre-1984 period, On the other hand, Hypothesis 3 predicts that the duration clock will
coinciding with labor unrest in the child welfare authority that entailed carry a significant negative coefficient; after a change, the hazard of
a protracted strike. Thus, we include a period effect in our exit models to change increases initially but then declines over time.
control for low exit rates during this time. Evaluating Hypotheses 1 and 3 requires us to distinguish between
Fig. 2 describes the number of foster home placement changes by the effects of changing states and the effects of being in a specific state
six-month periods between 1970 and 1990. Prior to 1985, the overall (Amburgey et al., 1993). For this reason, we used gender to capture
pattern resembles the exit pattern—an increase in 1971 and relative the state-specific effect of being either male or female (Markovits,
stability until the 1984 decrease that corresponds the protracted Benenson, & Dolenszky, 2001).
strike. Thus, we include a period effect in change models to control for A dummy variable records whether or not a placement change
fewer placement changes. From 1985 until 1990, however, placement occurs, coded 0 until a change occurs and 1 thereafter, regardless of the
changes show a constant pattern of increase, a trend we explore in number of changes. Accordingly this variable captures the average
the discussion section. effect of change, net of other factors (Amburgey et al., 1993). To test
Hypotheses 2a, 2b and 7, we interacted this variable with the child's age.
3.1. Independent variables The interaction term takes a value of 0 prior to the change and the
child's age after that.
We constructed a duration “clock” variable, rescaled by dividing by Hypothesis 2a predict that the interaction term will have a significant
100 to avoid unwieldy numbers. It records elapsed time (in days) since negative coefficient, implying a decreasing hazard of exit, whereas
last placement change. It is 0 until a change, at which point it records Hypothesis 2b predicts a significant positive coefficient, implying an
the elapsed time since the change. If another change occurs, the clock is increasing hazard of change. For Hypothesis 7, the interaction term
reset to 0 and again records the elapsed time. These values were used to contributes nothing to the hazard of change prior to a placement change.
evaluate Hypotheses 1, 3, and 5. Hypotheses 1 and 3 predict that the If a change occurs early in a child's foster care tenure, the child's age will
duration clocks will have significant positive coefficients. A placement be lower, resulting in lower values for the hazard of change. By the same
change resets the time clock to 0, resulting in a drop in the hazard value token, if the first change occurs later in a child's tenure in foster care, age
is higher and the effects on the hazard of change greater. Conversely, for
Hypothesis 2a and 2b, if the first change occurs earlier in a child's tenure
20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180 200

when the age is lower, the effects on the hazard of exit will be greater. If
the first change occurs later when age is higher, the effects on the hazard
of exit will be less.
We used data on children's chronological ages, calibrated in days, to
Numbers of Exits

test Hypothesis 6. Similar to the duration clock variable, we rescaled age


by dividing by 100.
Finally, to evaluate Hypothesis 4, we created a variable counting
the cumulative occurrences of placement changes. This variable has a
value of 0 until the first change, of 1 until the second change, 2 until
the third change, and so on.

3.2. Control variables

We incorporated a number of realistic but otherwise extraneous


0

1970 1972 1974 1976 1978 1980 1982 1984 1986 1988 1990 control variables in our models to evaluate whether or not our
Year hypotheses are supported despite the presence of other plausible
sources of explanation. Two variables—local unemployment and local
Fig. 1. Number of foster home exits by six-month intervals 1970–1990. labor force participation rates of women—refer to general economic
D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219 2213

conditions that may have implications for foster care dynamics. Women times into our exit models. We anticipate that the linear term will carry
historically have been the main source of childcare labor in families. a negative sign, indicating an initial declining risk of exit, and that the
Accordingly, their increased employment outside the home without quadratic term will carry a positive sign, indicating an increasing rate.
simultaneous change in the distribution of responsibility in the home We also anticipate some underlying duration effect for change; an
may engender conditions that dampen the return of children to their increasing proportion of older, problematic foster children may be
own homes. Women's increased out-of-home employment may also associated with increasing risks of change for these children. It is not
create conditions supportive of increased movement of children from clear why this pattern may invert in time (as is the case for exits).
one foster care placement to another. On the other hand, women's place Accordingly, we attempt to control for this effect in our change models
in the economy is more tenuous than men's. Hence, in times of increased by introducing a linear, but not a quadratic, term. We anticipate this
unemployment they are more likely to be fired or laid off, thereby linear term will carry a positive sign, indicating an underlying pattern of
possibly making more labor available to care for children. The increasing risk of change.
possible consequences include increased foster care exit rates and
lower placement change rates. 3.3. Models and analysis
We included two control variables measuring possible effects of
policy shifts aimed at instituting economic incentives and institutional We tested the hypotheses using parametric models of exit and
change in the child welfare authority. Each was treated as a historical placement change processes (Amburgey et al., 1993, p. 62; Carroll &
event, coded 1 when the policy is in effect and 0 otherwise. Each was Hannan, 2000, pp. 371–374), which were defined operationally in
intended to have, and can be construed as having, effects on the exit and terms of the instantaneous rate:
change rates of children in care. Economic incentives refers to a period h i
beginning in 1973 when foster home rates were increased, and policies r jk ðt Þ ¼ lim qj ðt; t þ Δt Þ=Δt ;
aimed at establishing fostering as an economically rewarding career
option were introduced. Institutional change refers to a period beginning where qj is the discrete probability of a child experiencing a type k
in 1978 when formal legislative expectations concerning relationships event (exit or placement change) between t and Δt, conditional on
among foster homes, foster children, and the agencies were changed the child being at risk for the event at time t. The rates of placement
such that the rights and needs of foster children achieved increased change and of exit are assumed to be a loglinear function of the
priority against the collective rights and needs of foster homes. Further, covariates to preclude negative predicted outcomes (Blossfeld &
foster homes were constrained by these changes to behave less as Rohwer, 2002), initially giving models of the general form:
autonomous self-directing family units and more as partners, cooperat-
ing with child welfare staff in setting goals and implementing plans for r j kðt Þ ¼ exp½βX t ;
the children in care (Tucker & Hurl, 1992).
Our next two variables refer first to the period of labor unrest and where Xt is a vector of time-varying variables, and β is a vector of
second to a method of foster care practice. Labor unrest was measured parameters indicating the effects of the variables on the rate of
as a period effect, coded 1 when in effect and 0 otherwise. The method occurrence.
of practice, matching, emphasized the importance of workers making Models of this form make two potentially problematic assumptions
every effort, when placing children in foster homes, to ensure a good fit relevant to testing our hypotheses, viz., the timing and occurrence of
between a child's needs and foster home characteristics and capabili- placement changes for each child are independent of each other, and
ties. We measured this indirectly as the ratio in any given year of the potentially important but unmeasured covariates are not excluded from
numbers of children in care to the number of licensed foster homes. the analysis. A moment's reflection suggests these assumptions probably
There are conflicting arguments about the possible effects of this are not viable. For placement changes, the second and subsequent
ratio. On the one hand, it can be assumed that a high ratio, indicating placement changes likely are influenced by, and therefore are different
proportionately more children than foster homes, means fewer choices from, the initial and subsequent changes. For unmeasured covariates,
for workers and thus the potential for poor matches, resulting in lower there may be factors intrinsic to individual children—aspects of behavior
propensity to exit and a higher propensity to change. Alternatively, it or quality of foster home placements—for which we lack data but which
can be assumed that a high ratio implies more control by foster homes bear on placement change or exit. As recent methodological work shows
over what child they will accept into their care. Hence, they are more (Box-Steffensmeier, De Boef, & Zorn, 2007; Hougaard, 1991, 1999, 2000;
invested in the decision, once it is made, resulting in a lower propensity Kelly & Lim, 2000), not controlling for these possible effects could result
for children to change placements and a higher propensity for them to in misleading or spurious findings because of incorrect estimates of
exit. standard errors, biases in the estimated effects of measured covariates, or
Our final control variable is overall duration dependence, or the misleading estimates of duration dependency (Box-Steffensmeier &
possible effect of total time spent in care on a child's hazard of change Jones, 2004).
or exit at any given point. We include this variable because general Accordingly, we fit our exponential models to the data as shared
models are exponential models and therefore do not assume a frailty models. Such models address the problem of within-subject
particular functional form for the underlying pattern of duration correlation by introducing a random covariate or “error term” (Blossfeld
dependence. We would argue that in the case of the exit from care or & Rohwer, 2002) into the equation. Some individuals are intrinsically
changes in placement, this likely is not correct. The least problematic more prone to experience the event of interest than others, and
children will likely exit earlier; over time proportionately more distribution of these individual-specific effects can be approximated by
children will persist in care who, due to circumstances and behavior, assigning a specific distribution presumed to characterize the “error
are less likely to exit. Accordingly, the pattern of exits will decline term” (Box-Steffensmeier et al., 2007). We follow this convention here.
over time. Accordingly the general form for our models is re-specified as:
However, a child's time in foster care is time limited, usually not
extending beyond age 18. It is not unusual for older foster children to r j kðt Þ ¼ exp½βX t þ Z i ω;
receive emancipation and move to independent living arrangements
outside foster care. These considerations suggest that in time the exit where ω corresponds to the random effect, defined by the gamma
curve will begin to turn up; thus the underlying exit curve for foster distribution, the most frequently used distribution in applied research
children will be an inverted U shape. We attempt to capture these (Box-Steffensmeier & Jones, 2004). It is presumed to capture a given
effects by introducing linear and quadratic terms for total duration observation's unobserved propensity towards the event of interest. Zi
2214 D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219

identifies observations that share a common frailty because they Table 1


pertain to the same child. We estimated the parameters of the models Descriptive statistics for all spells (N = 20,997), terminal spells (N = 3448) and gender
(N = 3448).
using maximum likelihood methods implemented in Stata (Cleves,
Gould, & Gutierrez, 2002, pp. 292–298; Gutierrez, 2002; StataCorp, Variables Mean s.d.
2003a,b). Unemployment 6.633 1.946
Labor force participation 48.074 7.369
4. Results Institutional change 0.692 0.462
Economic incentives 0.921 0.27
Labor unrest 0.01 0.098
Table 1 presents selected descriptive statistics on the study Matching ratio 2.253 0.151
population, including means and standard deviations for all spells Age clock 8.32 0.464
and terminal spells, as well as percent distribution for gender (53.34% Change 0.756 0.429
male). Table 2 gives an all-spells intercorrelation matrix of indepen- Cumulative change 5.851 9.464
Change clock 2.591 2.855
dent and control variables. It suggests that although the majority of
Age × Change 6.414 3.565
the correlation coefficients are positive and significant, multicolli- Terminal spells (n = 3448)
nearity does not appear to be a problem. Age in days (logged) 8.248 0.566
Table 3 presents information on the pattern of placement changes. Cumulative change 3.295 6.719
It shows that the majority of children, approximately 44%, experi-
Gender Male Female
enced only one change during their tenure in foster care. Approxi- 53.34 46.66
mately 79% had five or fewer changes. Of the remaining 720 children,
42.6%, or 307 children, experienced more than 10 placement changes,
with 112 of these having more than 20 changes. change, followed by a more rapid decline than would be expected
Table 4 displays estimated coefficients for exit rate models and because of aging. While these changes show a pattern of repetitive
information relevant to assessing heterogeneity in the data. Model I is momentum slowed by endogenous time decay in risk of exit, they
the baseline model. It gives the coefficients for gender, the control also show that effects of increasing age amplifies the overall pattern
variables, the dummy variable for whether or not there has been a of change. Considered together, therefore, these findings support
placement change, and the age clock. Model II adds elapsed time from Hypotheses 5 and 6, which proposed, respectively, that probability of
last change (change clock), plus the interaction term for placement placement change would decrease with time since the last change,
change and age. and that probability of placement change would increase with age of
Starting with Hypothesis 1 and the question of how placement child.
change affects exits rates, estimates in model I indicate that, as predicted Finally, the interaction term for change and age is positive and
by Hypothesis 1, children with at least one placement change had lower significant, indicating that early placement change increases likelihood
risk of exit compared to those with no changes. In model II, the of another change, thereby increasing momentum for further change.
interaction term for change and age is positive and significant, indicating This supports the argument for Hypothesis 7, which predicted that
that as a child ages into foster care, change is likely to be associated early placement change increases prospects for subsequent place-
within increasing risk of exit. This contradicts Hypothesis 2a, which ment changes.
anticipated that increasing age would act to lower the exit rate, but Similar to the exit models I and II in Table 4, the likelihood ratio test
supports Hypothesis 2b, which made the opposite prediction. Finally, the of theta= 0 for the change model is strongly statistically significant,
coefficient for the change clock in model II is positive and significant, indicating unobserved heterogeneity in the data. This finding also
indicating that, after a change, the risk of exit increases over time. Thus, supports that the effects associated with the measured covariates in
as anticipated by Hypothesis 3, the longer the time since the last change, Table 5 are not spurious. As a result, estimates reported in this table can
the higher the risk of exit. be construed as providing strong support for Hypotheses 4, 5, 6, and 7.
Regarding heterogeneity, the likelihood ratio test of theta= 0 is
strongly statistically significant for both models, indicating that consid- 5. Discussion
erable unobserved heterogeneity exists in the data, but this heteroge-
neity is not captured by the covariates in the models. The presence of We began by proposing to test AT as transactional and process
statistically significant effects for the relevant covariates described in the theory of change, particularized to foster children and to the way
preceding paragraph also tells us that controlling for this heterogeneity change occurs in their lives. In constructing this test, we presumed
does not eliminate our findings. As Blossfeld and Rohwer (2002, p. 269) that the effects of change processes in foster care work independently
point out: “The utility of estimating a mixture (frailty) model lies not in from the effects of child and other contextual attributes. Our analysis
separating an error term from a ‘true’ transition rate, but in providing strongly supports the efficacy of treating AT, in the context of foster
some means for assessing the robustness of estimates for the observed care, as a transactional process theory of change. For all hypotheses
covariates.” Accepting this argument implies that, overall, findings except Hypothesis 2a, the predicted effects of change were in the
presented in Table 4 strongly support Hypotheses 1, 2a, and 3, but not expected direction.
Hypothesis 2a. Hypothesis 2a predicted that older age at time of placement
Table 5 displays the results for the rates-of-change analysis. The change would be associated with a decreasing risk of exit. The
coefficient for cumulative placement changes is positive and signif- theoretical basis for this hypothesis was a fairly literal interpretation
icant, indicating that one placement change increases likelihood of of Bowlby's assertion of a positive relationship between children's
another placement change, and that effects are cumulative. This increasing age and increasingly inflexible behavior patterns. The
supports Hypothesis 4, which predicted that probability of placement competing hypothesis, Hypothesis 2b—based on Crittenden's dy-
change increases with number of previous placement changes. namic maturational model of attachment—predicted an increasing
Second, the change clock coefficient is significant and negative; risk of exit, and was supported by the analysis. Our finding of support
the main effect of age is positive, indicating that increasing propensity for Hypothesis 2b, and thus Crittenden's argument, is important for
for placement change as children age. These findings indicate that the two reasons. First, Crittenden does not dispute Bowlby's central claim
rate of these changes shows repetitive momentum, and that aging that children seek relational security in their lives, or the assumption
augments this pattern. The significant negative coefficient for the that the basis and orientation of a child's current behavior is strongly
change clock indicates an initial jump in the likelihood of another connected to the basis and orientation of earlier behavior. Hence, it does
D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219 2215

Table 2
Intercorrelation matrix all observations (N = 20,997).

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Gender
2. Unemployment − 0.0186⁎
3. Labor force − 0.0325⁎ 0.1381⁎
participation
4. Institutional change − 0.0273⁎ 0.4104⁎ 0.7960⁎
5. Economic incentives − 0.0217⁎ 0.2860⁎ 0.4125⁎ 0.4400⁎
6. Labor unrest − 0.0026 0.1491⁎ 0.0408⁎ 0.0662⁎ 0.0291⁎
7. Matching ratio − 0.0244⁎ 0.1617⁎ 0.7010⁎ 0.6561⁎ 0.4687⁎ 0.0755⁎
8. Age clock 0.1515⁎ 0.1014⁎ 0.2095⁎ 0.2178⁎ 0.1668⁎ − 0.0788⁎ 0.1621⁎
9. Duration 0.1284⁎ 0.0948⁎ 0.2147⁎ 0.2115⁎ 0.1296⁎ − 0.0476⁎ 0.1645⁎ 0.9344⁎
10. Duration squared − 0.0865⁎ 0.0181⁎ 0.1957⁎ 0.1457⁎ 0.1138⁎ − 0.0322⁎ 0.1437⁎ 0.2268⁎ 0.2088⁎
11. Change 0.0062 0.0836⁎ 0.0976⁎ 0.0997⁎ 0.0632⁎ 0.0033 0.0828⁎ 0.0360⁎ 0.0108 0.0284⁎
12. Cumulative change − 0.0633⁎ 0.0196⁎ 0.3671⁎ 0.2761⁎ 0.1540⁎ − 0.0614⁎ 0.2698⁎ 0.2714⁎ 0.2288⁎ 0.3133⁎ 0.1580⁎
13. Change clock 0.0937⁎ 0.0476⁎ − 0.0809⁎ − 0.0324⁎ 0.0469⁎ − 0.0756⁎ − 0.0449⁎ 0.4010⁎ 0.2609⁎ − 0.0301⁎ − 0.0461⁎ − 0.1735⁎
14. Age × Change − 0.0447⁎ 0.0876⁎ 0.2900⁎ 0.2488⁎ 0.2233⁎ − 0.1531⁎ 0.2380⁎ 0.3756⁎ 0.2884⁎ 0.6093⁎ 0.1976⁎ 0.4451⁎ 0.0583⁎
⁎ p b .05.

not contradict our argument that children tend toward relative is amplified by the effects of increasing age, and the main effect of age
behavioral inertia. Second, Crittenden supports the idea that, in dealing significantly increases likelihood of change.
with variation over time in their felt-sense of security, children evolve a Hypotheses 7, which predicted that early change would increase
broader range of attachment strategies than anticipated by the fourfold probability of subsequent changes, was also supported by our analysis.
classification system used to characterize the attachment strategies of An early change added an increment to the likelihood of further changes,
very young children (Thompson & Raikes, 2003). Thus, Crittenden's and this increment increased with age of child. The Bowlby/Sroufe
argument affirms our emphasis on conceptualizing AT as a lifespan pathways model, which provides the theoretical basis for this hypothesis,
theory of change with potential to deepen understanding of children's
behavior at least to young adulthood. Table 4
Hypotheses 1 and 3 also support the effects of change on propensity Effects of placement change on exit●.
to exit. For Hypothesis 1, the disruptive effects of change for a child's Variable Model
internal working model increase the prospects for that child being
I II
labeled problematic, thus decreasing risk of exit. In contrast, Hypothesis
3 finds that, providing that the child stays in the same placement, the Gender − 0.181⁎⁎⁎ − 0.181⁎⁎⁎
(0.060) (0.063)
disruptive effects of change attenuate over time and increase chances of
Unemployment rate 0.140⁎⁎⁎ 0.138⁎⁎⁎
exit. (0.016) (0.017)
This latter finding raises a troubling issue. Specifically, a placement Women in labor force 0.010 0.013⁎
persists after a change because the child and the foster family have (0.007) (0.007)
achieved some level of mutual accommodation, resulting in the child Legal change 0.072 0.111
(0.100) (0.103)
being labeled non-problematic. Thus, conditions are created that support Economic incentives 0.673⁎⁎⁎ 0.683⁎⁎⁎
the child leaving foster care and returning to his or her own home. (0.099) (0.103)
Current policies and practices define this kind of effect as normatively Labor unrest − 0.932⁎⁎⁎ − 0.821⁎⁎⁎
appropriate. Reflection on the theory and findings reported in this paper, (0.265) (0.280)
Matching ratio − 0.022 − 0.144
however, suggests that it is not transparently clear that this is always in
(0.229) (0.237)
the child's best interests. We explore this issue in greater detail in the last Time − 2.245⁎⁎⁎ − 2.914⁎⁎⁎
paragraph of this discussion section. (0.086) (0.116)
Hypotheses 4, 5 and 6 support a “liability of change” argument. That Time squared 0.458⁎⁎⁎ 0.596⁎⁎⁎
is, change tends to engender change, and children tend to move from one (0.024) (0.029)
Age clock 0.007⁎⁎⁎ 0.004⁎⁎
context to another once change has been initiated. In addition, a process (0.002) (0.002)
of repetitive momentum operates for placement changes; more frequent Placement change − 5.086⁎⁎⁎ − 5.366⁎⁎⁎
experience with change increases the probability of its recurrence. At the (0.106) (0.112)
same time, this repetitive momentum is dampened somewhat between Change clock 0.302⁎⁎⁎
(0.028)
changes by a propensity for risk of change to decline with the passage of
Change × Age 0.005⁎⁎⁎
time. This overall pattern of change can be visualized as a series of jumps (0.002)
with a declining propensity to change following each jump. The pattern Constant − 5.508⁎⁎⁎ − 5.240⁎⁎⁎
(0.510) (0.535)
Theta 0.878⁎⁎ 1.056
Table 3 (0.047) (0.056)
Total number of placement experiences by children in care (N = 3448). Chi-squared 8057.62 8185.41
Degrees of freedom 11 13
Total number of placements Number of children % of Children Cumulative %
Number of children 3448 3448
1 1525 44.23 44.23 Log likelihood −3759.50 − 3695.60
2 429 12.44 56.67 Number of events 3170 3170
3 372 10.79 67.46 Number of observations 20,997 20,997
4 237 6.87 74.33 Likelihood-ratio test of theta = 0 733.22 712.46
5 165 4.79 79.12
⁎ p b .10.
6–10 413 11.98 91.1
⁎⁎ p b .05.
11–20 195 5.66 96.75
⁎⁎⁎ p b .01.
> 20 112 3.25 100 ●
Standard errors are in parentheses.
2216 D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219

Table 5 more in relational terms (Sameroff & Emde, 1989); and the efficacy
Effects of change and time since change on rates of change●. of taking explicit account of attachment histories when assessing
Variable Model problems and developing both preventive programs and treatment
plans.
Gender 0.012
(0.044) Despite consistent support for our hypotheses, the analysis nonethe-
Unemployment rate 0.019⁎⁎⁎ less results in some qualification to our argument of behavioral inertia—
(0.007) at least as this inertia overtly manifests. Specifically, the findings show
Women in labor force − 0.053⁎⁎⁎
that risks of exit and change both depend on age; age moderates
(0.004)
Legal change − 0.191⁎⁎⁎ between change and exit, and between earlier and later change. In the
(0.052) case of exits, the main effect of age is positive; for change, it is negative.
Economic incentives − 0.182⁎⁎⁎ As a consequence, effects of age are more complex than anticipated.
(0.060) Older children are more likely to exit and, therefore, have a higher base
Labor unrest − 0.180⁎
hazard rate. As a result, the net effect of change is likely to be quite
(0.111)
Matching ratio − 0.284⁎⁎ powerful. It produces a more marked change in an older child's risk of
(0.113) exiting compared to the effects of similar change in a younger child,
Time 0.618⁎⁎⁎ because the latter has a lower base hazard rate. On the other hand,
(0.022)
when it comes to change, the situation is reversed. Older children are
Age clock − 0.040⁎⁎⁎
(0.002) less likely to experience change since they have a lower base hazard
Cumulative placement changes 0.008⁎⁎⁎ rate. Accordingly, the net effect on rates of change is likely to be stronger
(0.001) among younger children, who start from a higher base hazard rate.
Change clock − 0.345⁎⁎⁎ One plausible interpretation of this pattern, in line with the earlier
(0.011)
discussion of Crittenden's dynamic maturational model of attachment, is
Change × Age 0.004⁎⁎⁎
(0.001) that increasing age brings increased sophistication by children adapting
Constant 9.595⁎⁎⁎ to and/or influencing their context. They may use this to negotiate exit
(0.268) from foster care at opportune times, or to facilitate heightened stability
Theta 0.758⁎⁎⁎
of placement when they persist in care.
(0.043)
Chi-squared 3723.19
In a related vein, the significance of elapsed time between successive
Degrees of freedom 12 changes also merits consideration. When the change clock is reset by a
Number of children 3448 change, the elapsed time since the last change is a stronger indicator of
Log likelihood 897.09 the cumulative effects of being in care than age is. Accordingly, change
Number of events 11,443
has long-term effects on the hazard rate on which age-associated effects
Number of observations 20,997
Likelihood-ratio test of theta = 0 2264.42 are based. This suggests that underlying processes associated with
children's placement patterns and disposition are not simply condi-
⁎ p b .10.
⁎⁎ p b .05. tioned by chronological aging; they also strongly reflect the influence
⁎⁎⁎ p b .01. that change experiences have on children.

Standard errors are in parentheses. Interestingly, this pattern of change and exit, and the potential
significance of relational experience in explaining it, is in line with
arguments and research concerning self-fulfilling prophecies in close
relations (Downey, Freitas, Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998; Sroufe, 1990). In
has helped support and rationalize a widely accepted approach to the brief, arguments assert that, conditioned by their relational experiences,
study of childhood problems. This approach focuses on identifying and people's internal working models of relationships incorporate expecta-
understanding factors and processes that, over time, augment a child's tions of acceptance and rejection, which then shape their relationships in
chances of moving to maladaptive pathways (leading to behavioral the manner of a self-fulfilling prophecy. Sensitivity to expectations of
disturbances) or deflecting them away from such pathways (Schofield rejection by others leads people to behave in ways that solicit such
& Beek, 2005). rejection. This affirms and perpetuates a disposition to expect, perceive,
Significantly, however, we are not aware that the underlying premise and overreact to rejection. Conversely, expectations of acceptance lead to
of this model—an initial deviant pathway change leads to successive behaviors more solicitous of being accepted by others, and less to a
deviant changes that take children away from pathways leading to disposition to behave as if expecting rejection.
competent social functioning—has been tested empirically. To the extent Applying this self-fulfilling argument to AT as a process theory of
that early placement change in foster care can be construed as reflecting change, it is plausible that, for many children, being taken from their
maladaptive behavior patterns (we would argue they can), our finding families into foster care is a powerful rejection experience. It may
for Hypothesis 7 may be the first empirical test of the validity of this compound intra-familial rejection experiences that made child
underlying premise. welfare authority action necessary in the first place. Hence, if it is
Accepting ours as the first affirming empirical test suggests at not already present, it is probable that many foster children start
least two things. First, some additional confidence can be placed in foster care tenure with a heightened sensitivity to the possibility of
earlier and future research findings testing empirical implications rejection. Subsequent placement changes, particularly if place-
based on the pathways developmental model. Process, it seems, does ments are short, likely will be perceived as affirming the necessity
count, making it possible, at least in the case of foster children, to for rejection sensitivity, thereby orienting internal working models
predict outcomes based on knowledge of their context and patterns of relationships to defensively expect rejection from others. This
of change without explicit and detailed knowledge of their personal augments the probability of confirmatory behavior from others, and
attributes. Second, it adds significance to Sroufe's (1997) arguments conditions appropriate for the emergence of the self-fulfilling
regarding the potential importance of adopting a developmental prophecy described above are put in place.
approach to psychopathology in children instead of relying almost Empirically this would manifest in the pattern of findings reported
exclusively on the dominant medical model. Among other things, in this research. Based on the arguments presented above, assuming
this suggests need for expanded study of continuity and change in that children in foster care vary in their levels of rejection sensitivity
children's lives; the relevance of conceptualizing their problems implies that those with lower levels will be less likely to perceive
D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219 2217

placement change defensively. Thus, in the longer run, they will be Another refinement to future foster care research, consistent with
more likely to solicit acceptance behaviors from caregivers, leading to this paper's view of AT as a life span theory, is suggested by recent
placement stability. This will be reflected empirically in longer times theoretical and philosophical analyses of temporality in social research
in placements and lower rates of change. (Abbott, 2001; Clemens, 1999). It supports conceptualizing a child's life
Conversely, those with higher levels of rejection sensitivity will be trajectory as a sequence of nested durations marked by events that
more prone to perceive change defensively and overreact to it, thereby reach backward and forward in their causes and effects (Abbott, 2001:
prompting confirmatory responses. Empirically, this would manifest in 226). From this perspective, and for example, adoption and reunifica-
progressively shorter times in placements and an increasing propensity tion are not outcomes or “ends” in a foster child's life trajectory. Instead
for one change to be followed by another. they are “middles” with something before and something after (Abbott,
Though we cannot test the validity of this explanation here, it does 2001, 2005; MacKenzie & McDonough, 2009). This dialectical issue is
suggest an important approach for future research that—building on the obscured by the periodization of foster care to durations from entry to
contributions of this and other research—could illuminate the mecha- event-specific exits such as adoption or reunification conceptualized as
nisms and processes underlying how the experience of change influences outcomes of the foster care system. Hence, future research into the
children's life trajectories and outcomes. To ensure a strong test of dynamics of foster care should expand its scope to include questions
the theory, such research should not only account for rejection and about how events such as adoption and reunification affect a child's
acceptance expectancies of children, but the rejection and accep- subsequent development and life chances.
tance expectancies of caregivers and professionals who work with While the results of this study do contribute to attachment and
the children and are involved in planning changes in their lives. change-and-stability literature, certain cautions are in order. In modeling
Children come with histories. Considered in the context of the arguments our data, we were constrained to assume that the error term in our eqs.
and findings presented in this research, how children's significant others could be accurately described using a gamma distribution. Because
interpret those histories may be a crucial element in determining the introducing a different distribution may have given different results,
nature and direction of the developmental pathways that subsequently there are limitations to the authoritativeness of our findings. In addition,
evolve. our emphasis has been on exploring AT usefulness in deepening
Returning now to the promised discussion of Hypothesis 3, the issue understanding of special populations of children, in this case, foster
concerned implications of the finding that increased stability in foster children. Hence, while we would construe our findings as applicable
care increased the likelihood of the child exiting foster care. Are such to foster children generally, we can make no such claim in the case of
transitions in the child's best interests? Normatively current policies and other populations of children.
practices give an affirmative answer. We think the theory and findings of We are limited in drawing theoretical inferences about the meaning
this research suggest caution in applying this view. Specifically, stability of our findings. The results of this study show that a child's age is an
in a placement indicates the presence of a relationship between the important consideration in explaining the frequency and magnitude of
foster child and caregivers. Moving the child out of this environment change. However, a substantial literature suggests that aging in children
disrupts that relationship, regardless of the purpose of the move. In is more than a chronological process specific to the individual (Dannefer,
addition to disrupting a relationship, it also transitions the child to a 1984; Featherman & Lerner, 1985; Laz, 1998; Neugarten, Moore, & Lowe,
context that, while entailing a historical connection, may now appear as 1965; Riley, 1987). It is social, contextual, and has strong potential to
new or strange, requiring the renegotiation of old relationships or even influence children's developmental pathways. We lacked the informa-
the establishment of new ones. tion needed to disentangle these effects, suggesting a less precise
Our theory suggests that this combination of disrupting old analysis of their comparative influences than perhaps should be the case
relationships and the renegotiating or creating new ones is likely to (Riley, 1987). In a related vein, we also lacked direct measures of
induce anxiety, potentially leading to child behaviors inimical to schemas and, therefore, of possible changes to internal working models
persistence in this new environment, with a subsequent return to as a result of changes in schema organization patterns. This persistent
foster care. Considered in the context of findings and discussion problem in attachment research is worth noting. Without its resolution,
presented in this study, the implications are that, all other things we continue to struggle to differentiate empirically between internal
assumed equal, such transitions carry the potential to do more harm and external effects and consequences in studies that accord central
than good. Because of the strong preference afforded this approach, explanatory roles to internal working models.
transitions that move children from stable foster home environ- We began this paper with two critical claims. First, modeling
ments to their home environments seem a significant topic for future variation over time in foster child exit and placement change rates, as
research. explained by such demographic attributes as age, was complicated
by the fact the same kinds of connections also could explain the
distribution of such attributes in the study population. Second, AT
6. Contribution, limitations, and conclusions was a relevant framework for explicating and understanding the
dynamic, reciprocal processes implied as underlying patterns of
The results of this study are important for several reasons. They foster child exit and change. The results of this research strongly
affirm the importance and relevance of studying effects of change affirm the latter observation. By giving insight into children's
processes, not just the content of change, in children's lives. While developmental processes, AT enabled us to show, for our study
characteristics of children and their contexts did have effects, the population, that how children are affected by change is crucially
consequences of changes examined did not depend only on these determined by when it happens. Moreover, by giving a basis for
characteristics and how they changed over time. deriving this answer, AT also provides practical insight for those
Significantly, the effects produced by change operated indepen- who work with foster children. Like history, change counts. And,
dently of the effects of these characteristics during the time children for this vulnerable group, its effects should never be treated as
were in care. In addition, results suggest that a strong historical benign.
dependency exists in the trajectory of children's lives. This finding
clearly aligns with arguments that history matters in accounting for References
current child behavior. From the perspective of this research, future
research that omits information about the history of significant Abbott, A. (2001). Time matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Abbott, A. (2005). Process and temporality in sociology: The idea of outcome in US
changes in children's lives, and how such changes occur, will be sociology. In G. Steinmetz (Ed.), The politics of method in the human sciences
limited in its capacity to explain their current circumstances. (pp. 392–426). Durham: Duke University Press.
2218 D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219

Akin, B. A. (2011). Predictors of foster care exits to permanency: A competing risks Howes, C., & Segal, J. (1993). Children's relationships with alternative caregivers: The
analysis of reunification, guardianship, and adoption. Children and Youth Services special case of maltreated children removed from their homes. Journal of Applied
Review, 33(6), 999–1011. Developmental Psychology, 14, 71–81.
Amburgey, T. L., Kelly, D., & Barnett, W. P. (1993). Resetting the clock: The James, S. (2004). Why do foster care placements disrupt? An investigation of reasons
dynamics of organizational change and failure. Administrative Science Quarterly, for placement change in foster care. The Social Service Review, 78(4), 601–627.
38(1), 51–73. Jovanovic, B. (1979). Job matching and the theory of turnover. Journal of Political
Blossfeld, H. P., & Rohwer, G. (Eds.). (2002). Techniques of event history modeling: New Economy, 87, 972–990.
approaches to causal analysis (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesMahwah, NJ. Kelly, P. J., & Lim, L. L. Y. (2000). Survival analysis for recurrent event data: An
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books. application to childhood infectious diseases. Statistics in Medicine, 19(1), 13–33.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Separation (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books. Land, K. C. (1984). Markovian transition rates models of macro social change. In A.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Loss. New York: Basic Books. Diekmann, & P. Mitter (Eds.), Stochastic modeling of social processes (pp. 215–243).
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent–child attachment and healthy human Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
development. New York: Basic Books. Laudan, L. (1977). Progress and its problems: Towards a theory of scientific growth.
Box-Steffensmeier, J. M., De Boef, S., & Zorn, C. J. W. (2007). Event dependence and Berkeley: University of California Press.
heterogeneity in duration models: The conditional fraility model. Political Analysis, Laz, C. (1998). Act your age. Sociological Forum, 13(1), 85–113.
15(3), 237–256. Levitt, B., & March, J. G. (1988). Organizational learning. Annual Review of Sociology, 14,
Box-Steffensmeier, J. M., & Jones, B. S. (2004). Event history modeling: A guide for social 319–340.
scientists. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lewis, M. (2001). Issues in the study of personality development. Psychological Inquiry,
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working models in attachment 12(2), 67–83.
relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy, & P. R. Saver (Eds.), Handbook of Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Attachment over time. Child Development,
attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 89–111). New York: The 71(3), 707–720.
Guilford Press. Lyon, T. D. (1987). Angelino foster care children's understanding of their removal from
Bretherton, I., & Waters, E. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. home. Unpublished Report. Southern California Studies Center, University of
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1–2), 3–35. Southern California.
Carroll, G., & Hannan, M. T. (2000). The demography of corporations and industries. MacKenzie, M. J., & McDonough, S. C. (2009). Transactions between perception and
Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. reality: Maternal beliefs and infant regulatory behavior. In A. J. Sameroff (Ed.), The
Child Welfare Information Gateway (2010). Foster Care Statistics 2009. Retrieved Transactional Model of Development: How children and contexts shape each other
February 2, 2011, from. U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, Adminis- (pp. 35–54). Washington: APA Books.
tration for Children and Families http://www.childwelfare.gov Macrae, C. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking categorically
Children's Bureau (2010). Child welfare outcomes 2004–2007: Report to Congress. about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93–120.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health & Human Services. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood:
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Maughan, A., Toth, S. L., & Bruce, J. (2003). False belief A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in
understanding in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 15(4), Child Development, 50(1–2), 66–104.
1067–1091. March, J. C., & March, J. G. (1977). Almost random careers: Wisconsin school
Clemens, E. S. (1999). Continuity and coherence: Periodization and the problem of superintendency, 1940–1972. Administrative Science Quarterly, 22(3), 377–409.
institutional change. In F. Engelstad, & R. Kalleberg (Eds.), Social time and social Markovits, H., Benenson, J., & Dolenszky, E. (2001). Evidence that children and
change: perspectives on sociology and history (pp. 62–83). Oslo: Scandinavian adolescents have internal models of peer interactions that are gender differenti-
University Press. ated. Child Development, 72(3), 879–886.
Cleves, M. A., Gould, W. W., & Gutierrez, R. G. (2002). An introduction to survival analysis Marvin, R. S., & Britner, P. A. (1999). Normative development: The ontogeny of
using STATA. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation. attachment. In J. Cassidy, & P. R. Saver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
Connell, C. M., Vanderploeg, J. J., Flaspohler, P., Katz, K. H., Saunders, L., & Kraemer research and clinical applications (pp. 44–67). New York: The Guilford Press.
Tebes, J. (2006). Changes in placement among children in foster care: A Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial-behavior: A
longitudinal study of child and case influences. The Social Service Review, 80(3), developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100(4), 674–701.
398–418. Mrazek, P. J., & Mrazek, D. A. (1987). Resilience in child maltreatment victims: A
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social conceptual exploration. Child Abuse & Neglect, 11(3), 357–366.
information-processing mechanisms in children's social-adjustment. Psychological Neugarten, B. L., Moore, J. W., & Lowe, J. C. (1965). Age norms, age constraints, and
Bulletin, 115(1), 74–101. adult socialization. The American Journal of Sociology, 70(6), 710–717.
Crittenden, P. M. (1999). Danger and development: The organization of self-protective Newton, R. R., Litrownik, A. J., & Landsverk, J. A. (2000). Children and youth in foster
strategies. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 145–171 care: Disentangling the relationship between problem behaviors and number of
(Chap. 3). placements. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(10), 1363–1374.
Crittenden, P. M. (2000). Dynamic-maturational exploration of the meaning of security Palmer, S. E. (1996). Placement stability and inclusive practice in foster care: An
and adaptation: Empirical, cultural and theoretical considerations. In P. M. empirical study. Children and Youth Services Review, 18(7), 589–601.
Crittenden, & A. H. Claussen (Eds.), The organization of attachment relationships Pears, K. C., & Fisher, P. A. (2005). Emotion understanding and theory of mind among
(pp. 358–383). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. maltreated children in foster care: Evidence of deficits. Development and
D'Andrade, A. C. (2005). Placement stability in foster care. In G. P. Mallon, & P. M. Hess Psychopathology, 17(1), 47–65.
(Eds.), Child welfare for the 21st century: A handbook of practices, policies, and Pecora, P. J., Whittaker, J. K., Maluccio, A. N., Barth, R. P., & DePanfilis, D. (2009). The
programs (pp. 608–622). New York: Columbia University Press. child welfare challenge: Policy, practice, and research. New Brunswick, N. J: Aldine
Dannefer, D. (1984). The role of the social in life-span developmental-psychology, past Transaction Publishers.
and future: Rejoinder. American Sociological Review, 49(6), 847–850. Rhodes, K. W., Orme, J. G., & Buehler, C. (2001). A comparison of family foster parents
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Mcclaskey, C. L., & Brown, M. M. (1986). Social competence in who quit, consider quitting, and plan to continue fostering. The Social Service
children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51(2), 1–85. Review, 75(1), 84–114.
Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy Riley, M. W. (1987). On the significance of age in sociology. American Sociological
in close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Review, 52(1), 1–14.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 545–560. Rutter, M., & O'Connor, T. G. (1999). Implications of attachment theory for child care
Dozier, M., Albus, K., Fisher, P. A., & Sepulveda, S. (2002). Interventions for foster policies. In J. Cassidy, & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory, research,
parents: Implications for developmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, and clinical applications (pp. 823–844). New York: Guilford Press.
14(4), 843–860. Sameroff, A. J., & Emde, R. N. (Eds.). (1989). Relationship disturbances in early childhood:
Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (1985). Ontogenesis and sociogenesis: Problematics A developmental approach. New York: Basic Books.
for theory and research about development and socialization across the lifespan. Sameroff, A. J., & MacKenzie, M. J. (2003). Research strategies for capturing
American Sociological Review, 50(5), 659–676. transactional models of development: The limits of the possible. Development
Fichman, M., & Levinthal, D. A. (1991). Honeymoons and the liability of adolescence: A and Psychopathology, 15(3), 613–640.
new perspective on duration dependence in social and organizational relation- Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding an inquiry
ships. Academy of Management Review, 16, 442–468. into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.
Gutierrez, R. G. (2002). Parametric frailty and shared frailty survival models. The Stata Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2005). Risk and resilience in long-term foster-care. British
Journal, 2(1), 22–44. Journal of Social Work, 35(8), 1283–1301.
Horwitz, S. M., Balestracci, K. M. B., & Simms, M. D. (2001). Foster care placement Sroufe, L. A. (1990). An organizational perspective on self. In D. Cicchetti, & M. Beeghly
improves children's functioning. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 281–307). Chicago: University
155(11), 1255–1260. of Chicago Press.
Hougaard, P. (1991). Modeling heterogeneity in survival-data. Journal of Applied Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and
Probability, 28(3), 695–701. Psychopathology, 9(2), 251–268.
Hougaard, P. (1999). Fundamentals of survival data. Biometrics, 55(1), 13–22. Sroufe, L. A., Carlson, E. A., Levy, A. K., & Egeland, B. (1999). Implications of attachment
Hougaard, P. (2000). Analysis of multivariate survival data: Statistics for biology and theory for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology,
health. New York: Springer. 11(1), 1–13.
Howes, C. (1999). Attachment relationships in the context of multiple caregivers. In J. Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The fate of early experience following
Cassidy, & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory, research, and clinical developmental-change: Longitudinal approaches to individual adaptation in
applications (pp. 671–687). New York: Guilford Press. childhood. Child Development, 61(5), 1363–1373.
D.J. Tucker, M.J. MacKenzie / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 2208–2219 2219

Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of relationships. In Tucker, D. J., Hurl, L. F., & Ford, H. (1994). Applying organizational ecology to the
W. W. Hartup, & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and development (pp. 51–71). family: The case of who persists in providing foster-care. Journal of Marriage and
Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates. the Family, 56(4), 1005–1018.
StataCorp (2003a). Survival analysis and epidemiological tables reference manual: van Santen, E. (2010). Predictors of exit type and length of stay in non-kinship family
Release 8. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation. foster care: The German experience. Children and Youth Services Review, 32(10),
StataCorp (2003b). Stata statistical software: Release 8. College Station, TX: StataCorp 1211–1222.
LP. Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal working model of the
Steele, B. F. (1986). Notes on the lasting effects of early child-abuse throughout the self, attachment, and competence in five-year-olds. Child Development, 67(5),
life-cycle. Child Abuse & Neglect, 10(3), 283–291. 2493–2511.
Steinhauer, P. D. (1991). The least detrimental alternative: A systematic guide to case Vuchinich, S., Teachman, J., & Crosby, L. (1991). Families and hazard rates that change
planning and decision-making for children in care. Toronto: University of Toronto over time: Some methodological issues in analyzing transitions. Journal of Marriage
Press. and the Family, 53(4), 898–912.
Swan, C. A., & Sylvester, M. S. (2006). The foster care crisis: What caused caseloads to Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from infancy to early
grow? Demography, 43(2), 309–335. adulthood in a high-risk sample: Continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates.
Thompson, R. A., & Raikes, H. A. (2003). Toward the next quarter-century: Conceptual Child Development, 71(3), 695–702.
and methodological challenges for attachment theory. Development and Psychopa- Weinstein, E. A. (1960). The self-image of the foster child. New York: Russell Sage
thology, 15(3), 691–718. Foundation.
Tucker, D. J., & Hurl, L. F. (1992). An ecological study of the dynamics of foster home
entries. The Social Service Review, 66(4), 617–641.

You might also like