You are on page 1of 8

International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance, 2016, 11, 40  -47

http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/ijspp.2014-0464
© 2016 Human Kinetics, Inc. ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

A Comparison of the Perceptual and Technical Demands of Tennis


Training, Simulated Match Play, and Competitive Tournaments
Alistair P. Murphy, Rob Duffield, Aaron Kellett, and Machar Reid

Purpose: High-performance tennis environments aim to prepare athletes for competitive demands through simulated-match
scenarios and drills. With a dearth of direct comparisons between training and tournament demands, the current investigation
compared the perceptual and technical characteristics of training drills, simulated match play, and tournament matches. Meth-
ods: Data were collected from 18 high-performance junior tennis players (gender: 10 male, 8 female; age 16 ± 1.1 y) during
6 ± 2 drill-based training sessions, 5 ± 2 simulated match-play sessions, and 5 ± 3 tournament matches from each participant.
Tournament matches were further distinguished by win or loss and against seeded or nonseeded opponents. Notational analysis
of stroke and error rates, winners, and serves, along with rating of perceived physical exertion (RPE) and mental exertion was
measured postsession. Results: Repeated-measures analyses of variance and effect-size analysis revealed that training sessions
were significantly shorter in duration than tournament matches (P < .05, d = 1.18). RPEs during training and simulated match-
play sessions were lower than in tournaments (P > .05; d = 1.26, d = 1.05, respectively). Mental exertion in training was lower
than in both simulated match play and tournaments (P > .05; d = 1.10, d = 0.86, respectively). Stroke rates during tournaments
exceeded those observed in training (P < .05, d = 3.41) and simulated-match-play (P < .05, d = 1.22) sessions. Furthermore,
the serve was used more during tournaments than simulated match play (P < .05, d = 4.28), while errors and winners were
similar independent of setting (P > .05, d < 0.80). Conclusions: Training in the form of drills or simulated match play appeared
to inadequately replicate tournament demands in this cohort of players. Coaches should be mindful of match demands to best
prescribe sessions of relevant duration, as well as internal (RPE) and technical (stroke rate) load, to aid tournament preparation.

Keywords: training loads, periodization, long-term athlete development, tournament preparation

High-performance tennis athletes are exposed to a myriad of of TLs to mimic competition play, particularly in specific cohorts
training stimuli in preparation for tournaments, including techni- through early to late developmental stages.11
cal and tactical drills and simulated match play.1–4 While previous The TLs of different cohorts of tennis players completing
discrete3,4 and catalogued drill studies2 establish typical training training drills, simulated-match-play scenarios, and tournament
loads (TLs) associated with high-level tennis environments, com- play have been investigated through an assortment of internal (ie,
parisons of training sessions, simulated match play, and tourna- heart rate [HR], rating of perceived exertion [RPE], and mental
ments within the same cohort are nonexistent, with past research exertion) and external (ie, stroke rate) measures. Previously we have
inferring insight from data gathered from different competition described the internal and external TLs of various drill categories in
cohorts (ie, developmental vs elite).3,4 In a similar vein, given the a high-performance youth tennis population.2 Specifically, recovery/
significance attached to training during developmental years (ie, defensive drills were of greatest internal TL (HR, RPE, and mental
8–20 y),5 a more granular understanding of the actual training and exertion).2 Physiologically, more “open” drills were characterized
competitive demands imposed on this particular cohort of players by higher peak and mean HR, while match play and more “closed”
is needed.6,7 Ideally, training drills and simulated match play should or technical drills presented with lower peak and mean HR.2 Reid
mimic conventional or worst-case tournament scenarios (ie, highest et al3 earlier characterized 4 discrete, hand-fed drills including the
demands required during competition) depending on developmental Star, Box, Suicide and Big X. Internal TLs were reported using
stage of the involved players.2,8 In other sports (eg, rugby league), a HR (178–182 beats/min) and RPE (5.0–7.6 au), while external
primary objective of training is to replicate certain patterns of play, TLs were documented through stroke count (0.7–2.3 strokes/min),
enabling players to cope with the highest demands placed on them ball velocity (113–123 m/s), and distance covered (76–114 m) via
during competition.9,10 Unfortunately, in tennis, current literature global positioning system (GPS) measures.3 Furthermore, previous
has provided negligible insight into the appropriate prescription data suggest that stroke rates during point play and match play in
training are below those characterized in separately reported tour-
nament data (2.7 ± 2.2 to 4.7 ± 1.4 strokes/rally).2,12,13 Moreover,
Murphy is with the School of Human Movement Studies, Charles Sturt tournament RPE has been reported as ranging from 5 to 8 arbitrary
University, Bathurst, NSW, Australia. Duffield is with the Sport and Exer- units (au) (CR-10)14,15 and 10 to 16 (Borg 20-point).16–18 On the
cise Discipline Group, UTS: Health, University of Technology Sydney, surface, while this empirical backdrop appears extensive, it is the
Sydney, NSW, Australia. Kellett is with Tennis Australia, Melbourne, VIC, aggregate of independent, discrete training and competition insights,
Australia. Reid is with the School of Sport Science, Exercise and Health, and it lacks any consideration of training or matches within a single
University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia. Address author cohort, thereby placing practitioners in situations that require ongo-
correspondence to Alistair Murphy at amurphy@tennis.com.au. ing assumptions to inform TLs.

40
Comparison of Tennis Training, Match Play, and Tournaments   41

Nevertheless, based on a comparison of previously notated13 locations limits an ability to standardize environmental conditions
and perceived match demands14,15 with the observational data and hydration state. Accordingly, we recognize this as a limitation
describing typical training sessions,2 TLs in training and tournament of the current study.
play appear disparate. Indeed, this type of discrepancy may contrib-
ute to mismatches in the preparation of high-performance athletes Tournaments
for tournaments, albeit it is assumed—perhaps incorrectly—that
players are training at suitable intensities and durations. Given the Analysis of tournament TL (ie, match load) was carried out across
lack of empirical support for this assumption, the aim of this study 4 outdoor hard-court (ie, category 4 court surface) tournaments in
was to analyze the technical and perceptual characteristics of drill- Australia. Specifically, the first 2 tournaments were domestic Aus-
based training sessions in comparison with simulated match play tralian tournaments (national-title events in Sydney and Melbourne),
and tournaments in the same cohort of elite players. A secondary and the other 2 were junior ITF events (grades 1 and A) All matches
aim was to compare TLs in tournament matches won versus lost and followed ITF junior guidelines and were best of 3 sets, contested
against seeded versus nonseeded opponents to further explore the between 9:00 AM and 7:00 PM. Furthermore, tournament matches
nuances of TL responses related to match outcome. We hypothesized were distinguished by outcome (win or loss) and opponent (seeded
that training sessions would present lower TLs than simulated match or nonseeded) in separate analyses, with data obtained from Tennis
play, which would in turn be lower than tournament matches, and Australia and ITF Web sites.
that tournament demands would be elevated in matches lost and
against seeded opponents. Simulated Match Play
Downloaded by University of Exeter on 10/01/16, Volume 11, Article Number 1

Simulated match-play sessions were organized by assigned coaches,


Methods ensuring similar between-players capabilities. Sessions began with
coach instruction and session focus. However, aside from encourage-
Subjects ment, coaches observed but refrained from interfering with technical
Eighteen high-performance junior tennis players (gender: 10 male, or tactical feedback during the match. Each match was best of 3
8 female; age 16 ± 1.1 y, mass 63 ± 16.2 kg, height 171 ± 11.4 cm, sets, self-controlled using ITF rules, and conducted on an outdoor
Australian junior ranking 6 ± 5, and International Tennis Federation hard court (ie, category 4 court surface).
[ITF] junior ranking 85 ± 61) and their parents/guardians provided
written consent after full explanation of the study. The university Training Sessions
ethics in human research committee approved this investigation.
Athletes routinely trained 2 or 3 sessions/d, completing 96 ± 24 Training-drill sessions were selected for drills that were of open
matches for the year. This study involved collection of internal and nature only (in accordance with our previously reported data), as
external measures from at least 1 training session/d over a 10-week these drill types have the greatest physical and mental demands of
hard-court training period (December to February; Australian typical elite-oriented tennis drills.2 Specifically, these types of drills
summer). Athletes were well familiarized with each drill during consisted only of recovery/defensive, open-pattern, and 2-on-1 open
each session as a result of extensive exposure during previous drill categories. These drills were each typified by high strokes rates
training blocks. Training sessions were selected when at least 2 (>0.9 strokes/6 s), RPE (>5.5 AU), mental exertion (>5.8 AU), and
subjects were included in the session and coach-designed session % HRmax (>89%). Sessions were excluded from analysis post
plans involved open-nature drills (ie, higher physical demands) with hoc if the aforementioned criteria were not met. All sessions were
lesser emphasis on technical proficiency or outcomes. conducted on outdoor hard court (ie, category 4 court surface).

Design Methodology
All training drills, simulated match play, and tournament matches All sessions were filmed using a video camera (DSR-PDX10P,
were completed on a Plexicushion tennis court. Athletes each Sony, Japan) positioned 10 m above and 6 m behind 1 baseline.
completed 6 ± 2 open-drill training sessions, 5 ± 2 simulated- The footage was later notated to establish stroke rate and unforced
match-play sessions, and 5 ± 3 tournament matches. Athletes in errors. Strokes were summated throughout the entire session or
the testing cohort were encouraged by coaching staff to standardize match involving any time in which the ball struck the racket face.
nutritional habits around training in preparation for competition. Errors in training sessions were distinguished inside the coach-
However, the inclusion of physical (strength and conditioning) prescribed constraints (if any) of the particular drill, which were
sessions in the training day meant that additional energy intake was clearly described by the assigned coach to both the athlete and the
required. Similarly, owing to the real-world settings characterized research team. Strokes, errors, winners, and serves were counted
by uncertain match start and finish times, travel demands, and the and analyzed relative to session/match duration (min). Work
variable selection and timing of meal/hydration options, nutritional durations—the effective playing time—were distinguished from
practices were not standardized. This approach aligns with previous the point of a successful serve until a winner or error (analyzed
match investigations15; however, due to the within-cohort analysis, only for simulated match play and tournaments). Rest durations
a similar approach was adopted for training also. However, while were then calculated as remaining time in simulated match play
conditions across all sessions were dry and relatively warm (Aus- or tournament matches (ie, changeover rest periods). Standard
tralian summer), these were unattainable across all training sessions match rules were implemented for errors in both simulated match
and matches, creating a limitation to the TL analysis. While the play and tournament play.19,20 Athletes were familiarized with
strength of the current investigation surrounds the within-cohort physical RPE and mental exertion as measures of internal load
comparisons, we recognize that the variety of training and match collected daily in their environment. Athletes provided RPE (Borg

IJSPP Vol. 11, No. 1, 2016


42  Murphy et al

CR-10)21 and mental-exertion evaluations (0–10 Likert scale) for in both matches won (13 ± 3.4 strokes/min) and matches lost (16
each drill session, simulated match play, and tournament match 30 ± 6.2 strokes/min) (P = .98, d = 0.63), as well as when playing a
minutes after completion.14,22 Session TL, as an arbitrary number seeded opponent (17 ± 8.2 strokes/min) compared with nonseeded
(au), was calculated through multiplication of duration and RPE.14 opponents (13 ± 4.2 strokes/min) (P = .95, d = 0.60). The work-rest
Mental-exertion rating (0–10 Likert scale) was used to establish a durations of simulated match play demonstrated large effects for
holistic rating of perceived mental intensity. Athletes rated based less work (ie, time in play) compared with tournament matches (P =
on descriptions of mental demand.22 All perceptual ratings were .29, d = 1.37) and significantly less rest (ie, stoppages) in simulated
provided privately to ensure no predisposition or bias. As RPE and match play than in tournaments (P < .05, d = 3.00). Within tourna-
duration are the main measures of the current investigation, it is ment matches, work-rest durations of matches won (29.6 ± 10.6
useful to note that previous research on adolescents has reported the min work, 51.7 ± 16.1 min rest) and lost (27.8 ± 14.3 min work,
correlation coefficient between RPE and HR as strong (r = .74).23 50.2 ± 17.3 min rest) were similar (P > .05, d < 0.80). Furthermore,
Furthermore, a trained analyst (coefficient of variation <2%) who there was a large effect observed for greater rest durations during
was familiarized with the notational analysis system (The Tennis matches against seeded (25.0 ± 11.1 min work, 44.7 ± 10.4 min
Analyst, V4.05.284, Fair Play, Australia) conducted all notational rest) than nonseeded opponents (31.7 ± 14.6 min work, 54.4 ± 20.3
analysis postsession. These measures are commonly used in train- min rest) (P > .05, d = 0.85).
ing and posttournament analysis to provide feedback and monitor In a more detailed analysis of stroke characteristics, the absolute
external TL.1,2 number of winners was similar between both simulated match play
and tournament match play (P = .92, d = 0.41). However, relative
Statistical Analysis to duration (Table 1), there was a large effect for more winners hit
Downloaded by University of Exeter on 10/01/16, Volume 11, Article Number 1

during simulated match play than during tournaments (P > .05, d


External- and internal-TL data were reported as mean ± SD unless = 0.90). Similarly, there were no differences observed between
otherwise specified. Comparison of external- and internal-TL winners hit during tournament matches won (11 ± 4.9 total, 0.4 ±
responses between different scenarios—training, simulated match 0.3 winners/min) or lost (13 ± 6.9 total, 0.5 ± 0.3 winners/min) (P
play, or tournaments—was undertaken by repeated-measures 2-way > .05, d < 0.80). There was also no difference in absolute winners
(session mode × measure) ANOVAs with Tukey HSD post hoc hit during matches played against seeded (12 ± 6.8 total; 0.5 ± 0.3
tests to locate differences. Statistical significance was set at P < winners/min) and nonseeded opponents (12 ± 5.0 total, 0.5 ± 0.3
.05. Cohen d effect-size analysis established the magnitude of dif- winners/min) (P > .05, d < 0.80). Absolute and relative serve counts
ference in TL. Effect-size results were interpreted as described by (serves/min) of simulated match play (46 ± 12.5 total, 2.6 ± 1.3
Christensen and Christensen,24 with effect sizes of <0.2 classified as serves/min) and tournament play (90 ± 16.6 total, 3.4 ± 0.8 serves/
small, 0.4 to 0.6 as medium, and >0.8 as large. Statistical analyses min) was higher during tournament matches than during simulated
were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences match play (P < .05, d = 4.28; P = .26, d = 1.03, respectively),
(SPSS) (Version 20, SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). while absolute serve volume was similar between matches won (88
± 15.8 total, 3.2 ± 0.9 serves/min) and lost (93 ± 27.4 total; 3.7 ±
1.2 serves/min), as well as between seeded opponents (80 ± 13.6
Results total, 3.6 ± 1.3 serves/min) and nonseeded opponents (96 ± 27.5
Figure 1 shows session duration and the internal-TL measures (ses- total, 3.4 ± 1.0 serves/min) (P > .05, d < 0.80). Finally, error rates
sion TL, RPE, and mental exertion) for training drills, simulated (errors/min) were significantly lower in training drills and simulated
match play, and tournament play. Furthermore, session duration match play than in tournament matches (P < .05, d > 1.00), while
and internal-TL measures for tournament matches won versus lost errors were similar between drill sessions and simulated match
and against seeded versus nonseeded opponents are presented in play (P > .05, d < 0.80). Within tournament matches, there were
Figures 2 and 3, respectively. No significant differences in session no significant differences in error rates (P > .05, d < 0.80) between
TL were present between session types (P > .05, d < 0.80). However, matches won (1.7 ± 0.6 errors/min) and lost (1.8 ± 0.7 errors/min)
there was significantly lower session duration evident for training or against seeded (1.7 ± 0.7 errors/min) and nonseeded opponents
than for tournament play (P < .05, d = 1.18). Simulated-match-play (1.7 ± 0.5 errors/min).
durations were similar to those of both training and tournaments
(P > .05, d < 0.80). Large effects indicated a greater session RPE
in tournaments than in training (P > .05, d = 1.26) and simulated
Discussion
match play (P > .05, d = 1.05). Matches within tournaments that An important component of the prescription of TLs is to ensure
were won were perceived to be of significantly greater exertion that athletes are exposed to match-like demands in training. As
than matches lost (P < .05, d = 1.40). Furthermore, large effect with other sports, tennis uses on-court training drills and simulated
sizes suggest that a greater mental exertion was perceived during match play for such preparation, generally alternating training at,
simulated match play and tournaments than during training drills above, or below match intensities. However, currently no literature
(P > .05; d = 1.10, d = 0.86, respectively). has concurrently compared the demands of common training drills,
External TLs (ie, stroke rate and work/rest durations) and simulated match play, and tournaments within a homogeneous
technical outcomes (winners, serves, and errors) of drills, simulated group. Accordingly, the current findings indicate that both session
match play, and tournaments are presented in Table 1 (relative duration and RPE during training tends to be lower than those typi-
to session durations). Stroke rates (strokes/min) in training were cal of tournament play. A comparison of work-rest durations also
significantly lower than in both simulated match play and tourna- revealed simulated match play to be less intensive (ie, less work,
ments (P < .05; d = 0.98, d = 3.41 respectively). Tournament stroke less recovery). Furthermore, training sessions elicited less mental
rates were also significantly greater than in simulated match play exertion than both simulated match play and tournaments. From
(P < .05, d = 1.22). Within tournaments, stroke rates were similar a technical standpoint, tournament stroke rates exceeded those in

IJSPP Vol. 11, No. 1, 2016


Comparison of Tennis Training, Match Play, and Tournaments   43
Downloaded by University of Exeter on 10/01/16, Volume 11, Article Number 1

Figure 1 — (A) Training load, (B) session duration, (C) session rating of perceived exertion (RPE), and (D) mental exertion between training, match
play and tournament play, mean ± SD. AU indicates arbitrary units. *Large effects for lower interaction effect than tournaments (d ≥ 0.8). #Large effects
for lower interaction effect than match play (d ≥ 0.8). §Significantly lower interaction effect than tournaments (p < .05).

training and in simulated match play, while greater (relative and rent RPE responses in simulated match play (6 ± 0.9 au) and in
absolute) serve loads were observed during tournaments than in tournaments (6 ± 0.8 au) were not dissimilar to those in previous
simulated match play. Consequently, it is clear that the physical and discrete investigations.1–3,7,14,25,26 However, simulated match-play
technical TLs of training drills and simulated match play warrant and tournament match-play RPE exceeded training-drill RPEs (5
ongoing scrutiny to assist with the prevention of overtraining or ± 0.8 au). Previously, after examination of training sessions, we
undertraining, thereby ensuring that the long-term consequences suggested that RPE is greatest in drills that most closely mimic
of poor training intensity are avoided. match worse-case scenarios or extreme time-pressure situations (ie,
As previously mentioned, training drills were selected spe- recovery/defensive drills, 6.5 ± 1.8 au).2 To provide clearer context,
cifically due to the associated physical demands demonstrated in we have also previously shown that closed technical drills are char-
previous studies.2 As such, we are confident that any bias toward acterized by low RPEs (4.6 ± 1.9 au).2 Before those studies, Reid
technical foci during training was minimized. In any case, the cur- et al3 described discrete, work:rest-ratio-driven conditioning drills

IJSPP Vol. 11, No. 1, 2016


44  Murphy et al
Downloaded by University of Exeter on 10/01/16, Volume 11, Article Number 1

Figure 2 — (A) Training load, (B) session duration, (C) session rating of perceived exertion (RPE), and (D) mental exertion between tournament
matches won and lost, mean ± SD. AU indicates arbitrary units. *Large effects for lower interaction effect than tournament matches won (d ≥ 0.8).
§Significantly lower interaction effect than tournament matches won (p < .05).

(ie, suicide, 7.6 ± 1.1 au, and Big X, 7.6 ± 1.0 au) of much greater ent developmental stages of players, as many other factors might
RPE. As to be expected, they also found that drill duration (ie, 30 influence overall perception of effort during this type of tourna-
s vs 60 s) was pivotal in the distinction of RPE for drills, a concept ment play (ie, prize money, media scrutiny, spectators), potentially
relevant to the interpretation of all training-drill analyses.3 Thus, increasing the RPE. Furthermore, it is unclear to what extent these
prescription of sessions to mimic tournament demands must take influences may or may not affect the RPEs reported in tournaments
into consideration not only the RPE of drills but also the duration where rankings are in dispute as compared with other competition/
of drills and work:rest ratios involved. tournament formats. Nevertheless, in the current study, the RPEs
Notwithstanding the body of work that has reported internal reported in training could be interpreted to reflect a mismatch with
load in competitive match play and invitational tournaments, few tournament demands. As such, care should be taken to ensure that
researchers have specifically explored the RPEs of athletes after athletes are exposed to match-like physical intensities at some stage
completion of tournament matches in which international junior or in preparatory training blocks.
senior ranking points are in dispute. Indeed, the work of Coutts et Despite the aforementioned widespread use of perceptual
al14 represents a rare investigative foray in this regard, describing load-monitoring measures in tennis, mental exertion is an area that
the RPE and session TL of a top-level player from the 2008 Roland TL-monitoring literature has seldom investigated—particularly
Garros. Those researchers reported match RPEs ranging from 5 to in tennis. Admittedly, this may be confounded by the validity of
7 (au), with a weekly competition TL of 2908 (au), ~18% greater the construct, yet its simplicity is instructive in high-performance
TL than during the final week of tournament preparation (2380 junior tennis environments. Indeed, in the current investigation it
au).14 However, caution is required in comparing between differ- is evident that a discrepancy exists between the perceived mental

IJSPP Vol. 11, No. 1, 2016


Comparison of Tennis Training, Match Play, and Tournaments   45
Downloaded by University of Exeter on 10/01/16, Volume 11, Article Number 1

Figure 3 — (A) Training load, (B) session duration, (C) session rating of perceived exertion (RPE), and (D) mental exertion between tournament
matches against seeded and nonseeded opponents, mean ± SD. AU indicates arbitrary units.

Table 1  Relative External-Load Measures for Shot demands of training, simulated match play, and tournament match
play. Both simulated match play and tournament matches required
Rate, Winners, Serves, Errors, Work Duration, and Rest
similar perceived mental intensity, which is considerably more than
Duration, Mean ± SD that of training. Previously, we have shown discrepancies between
Simulated certain drill categories, with drills of greater focus (accuracy drills)
Training match play Tournament and physical intensity (recovery/defensive drills) being character-
Shot rate (/min) 7 ± 1.0 10 ± 5.1* 14 ± 3.6*#
ized by significantly greater mental exertion than closed technical
drills.2 Keeping in mind that the current investigation controlled
Winners /min) 0.6 ± 0.2 0.5 ± 0.2§ for intensity of training drills, it can be interpreted that in order for
Serves (/min) 2.6 ± 1.3 3.4 ± 0.8§ mental exertion to approximate tournament match play, simulated
Errors (/min) 1.0 ± 0.2 1.1 ± 0.4 1.7 ± 0.5*#^§ competition or pressures (ie, targets, time pressure) must be incor-
porated in training.
Work duration (min) 20 ± 7.0 29 ± 9.8§
Given that drills were selected due to their prominent physical
Rest duration (min) 30 ± 8.6 51 ± 11.1#§ intensity rather than technical focus, the current findings identify a
Work:rest ratio 0.7: 1 0.6: 1 somewhat perplexing disparity between stroke rates of training ses-
sions (7 ± 1.0 strokes/min), simulated match play (10 ± 5.1 strokes/
*Significantly greater than training external load (P < .05). #Significantly greater
than simulated-match-play external load (P < .05). ^Large effects for greater external min), and tournament matches (14 ± 3.6 strokes/min). Despite
load than training (d ≥ 0.8). §Large effects for greater external load than simulated similar durations observed between simulated and tournament
match play (d ≥ 0.8). matches, less work was completed in simulated match play than in

IJSPP Vol. 11, No. 1, 2016


46  Murphy et al

tournament matches. This was despite greater rest periods during enced by junior high-performance athletes. While no other literature
tournaments—further highlighting a disconnect in the intended has investigated the discrepancies in TL between tennis matches
training prescription from a physical perspective. We admit, how- won and lost, it is conceivable that developing high-performance
ever, that such disparity may be due to the technical/tactical focus players are perhaps more “invested” physically for matches where a
or development during simulated match play (ie, tactical patterns, or winning outcome is possible or perceive matches won as more taxing
stroke technique) in the corresponding training block. Alternatively, as they produced a winning performance—despite no difference in
the observed increased winner rate during simulated match-play external TL. Internal TL measures comparing seeded and nonseeded
sessions may indicate more aggressive mindsets during these ses- matches also found no key differences, but a moderate effect sug-
sions. This is according to the similar ratings of mental exertion gests that potentially greater mental exertion was required during
perhaps corresponding to alternative pressures during tournament matches against seeded opponents. Furthermore, stroke rate, win-
matches (ie, match outcome/consequences). Nevertheless, with ners, and serves indicate limited differences between match types
such disparity between simulated match play and tournaments, it (despite a relatively low sample size). In summary, it appears that
is advisable that high-intensity drills be implemented in close prox- neither training drills nor simulated match play equals the duration
imity to simulated match play to compliment or elevate simulated or perceptual or technical TL of tournament matches.
match-play demands. Previously reported tournament demands
have been noted to reach 0.81 ± 0.04 strokes/s for men and 0.76 ±
0.03 strokes/s for women in matches during the Australian Open Practical Applications
in 1997 to 1999.27 Such discrepancies highlight the difference in
Current comparison of training drills, simulated match play, and
elite tournament match intensity and that of the current developing
Downloaded by University of Exeter on 10/01/16, Volume 11, Article Number 1

tournament matches reveals that the demands placed on tennis play-


player cohort (0.23 strokes/s).
ers in training are not necessarily of sufficient physical requirement
In certain situations, increased error rates in matches may
to prepare for tournament match play. Coaches should be aware of
increase match durations, alter the work:rest ratio, and affect the
reduced internal (RPE) and technical (stroke rate) demands in train-
mental state of players.28 As such, error rates become of interest to
ing to ensure appropriate stimuli, aiming for preparations to be as
coaches when preparing players for competition demands. The cur-
similar as possible to competition. Specifically, stroke rates during
rent findings suggest that tournament matches result in greater error
tournaments exceeded those observed in training and simulated
rates than both training and simulated match play. We postulate that
match-play sessions. In addition, coaches should be mindful that
such elevated error rates in simulated match play and tournaments
even the most physically demanding training sessions and simulated
may be due to increases in mental exertion compared with drills.
match play were of lower RPE than tournament matches. With
Player anxiety is perhaps increased in simulated and competitive
mental exertion in training also lower than both simulated match
situations where opposition or situational pressure (ie, increased
play and tournaments, it appears that tournament match play is cur-
strokes rates, physical demand, or importance of result) hinders an
rently difficult to replicate during training. Given the disagreement
athlete’s ability to perform well. Earlier, descriptive analysis of train-
of internal and external TLs between training drills, simulated match
ing drills found that drills of technical focus and extreme physical
play, and tournament match play, periodization of training needs to
(end-range) requirements induced higher error rates.2 Furthermore,
be clearly driven toward the demands of competition at the athletes’
a recent tennis investigation found increased somatic and cognitive
stage of development.
state anxiety and lower self-confidence precompetition on match
day than on training day.29 Moreover, after matches, somatic and
cognitive anxiety (associated with consequence of failure) were still Conclusions
elevated compared with training days.29 Therefore, during simulated
match and training sessions in which error-ameliorating practice Session durations and RPE during training and simulated match
is desired, the effect that both physical and mental exertion have play do not match those of tournament matches. Similarly, mental
on error rates should be acknowledged. In addition, to effectively exertion in training was not comparable to that in simulated or tour-
prepare athletes for the mental demands of competition, there appear nament match play. From a technical standpoint, stroke rates during
to be limited alternatives other than through tournament matches. tournament matches exceed those of both training and simulated
Having acknowledged this, however, we note that a limitation of match play, suggesting again that training and simulated-match-play
the current investigation is the lack of obtainable context in which intensity is mismatched. Furthermore, match-specific measures
matches were played—environmental conditions, opposition, and of the serve (relative and absolute) demonstrate greater incidence
ranking points needed/on offer. Accordingly, it should be acknowl- during tournament matches than simulated match play. Likely due
edged that these factors might also affect both physical effort and to the increased stroke volume, tournament error rates are also far
mental perception of the on-court demands. greater than those of simulated match play, which are greater again
Notably, these findings are certainly subject to the constraints than in training sessions. Despite greater stroke counts during tour-
of individual matches and sessions. Accordingly, a secondary aim nament play, comparison of work–rest durations reveals simulated
was to compare the technical and perceptual TLs between tourna- match play to be less intensive (ie, less work, less recovery). Sec-
ment matches won and lost, as well as against seeded and nonseeded ondary analysis suggests that TLs of matches won are perceived as
opponents. The findings highlight that TLs of matches won are of requiring greater physical exertion, notwithstanding similar mental
greater RPE than matches lost, notwithstanding similar mental per- perception. Notably, through an attempt to present an ecologically
ception and match durations. Furthermore, similar stroke and work valid and concurrent comparison between training and competition,
rates in matches lost were apparent. A recent tennis investigation our analysis was limited to a small cohort of players and a host of
observed greater postmatch salivary cortisol response and anxiety in uncontrolled external influences—the interaction of which might be
losers than winners, while winners reported higher self-confidence.29 more closely examined in future research. Nonetheless, it appears
As such, we suggest that a final positive (or potentially positive) that both training drills and simulated match play do not match the
outcome may provide a buffering effect on the mental stress experi- perceptual or external TLs of tournament matches. Coaches should

IJSPP Vol. 11, No. 1, 2016


Comparison of Tennis Training, Match Play, and Tournaments   47

be aware of the disparity in TL and aim to adequately prepare ath- 14. Coutts AJ, Gomes RV, Viveiros L, Aoki MS. Monitoring training
letes for tournament requirements. loads in elite tennis. Rev Bras Cineantropom Desempenho Hum.
2010;12(3):217–220.
15. Gomes RV, Coutts AJ, Viveiros L, Aoki MS. Physiological
Acknowledgments
demands of match-play in elite tennis: a case study. Eur J Sport Sci.
The authors would like to thank Tennis Australia for their support in testing, 2011;11(2):105–109. doi:10.1080/17461391.2010.487118
as well as the tennis players who participated in this study and assigned 16. Mendez-Villanueva A, Fernandez-Fernandez J, Bishop D, Fernandez-
coaches who allowed the integrated testing design. Further thanks to Dan­ Garcia B, Terrados N. Activity patterns, blood lactate concentra-
ielle Gescheit for assistance in data analysis. tions and ratings of perceived exertion during a professional singles
tennis tournament. Br J Sports Med. 2007;41(5):296–300. PubMed
doi:10.1136/bjsm.2006.030536
References 17. Mendez-Villanueva A, Fernandez-Fernandez J, Bishop D, Fernandez-
Garcia B. Ratings of perceived exertion–lactate association during
1. Murphy AP, Duffield R, Kellett AD, Reid M. Comparison of athlete– actual singles tennis match play. J Strength Cond Res. 2010;24(1):165–
coach perceptions of internal and external load markers for elite junior 170. PubMed doi:10.1519/JSC.0b013e3181a5bc6d
tennis training. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2014;9(5):751–756. 18. Fernandez-Fernandez J, Sanz-Rivas D, Fernandez-Garcia B, Men-
PubMed http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/IJSPP.2013-0364 dez-Villanueva A. Match activity and physiological load during a
2. Murphy AP, Duffield R, Kellett AD, Reid M. A descriptive analy- clay-court tennis tournament in elite female players. J Sports Sci.
sis of internal and external loads for elite-level tennis drills. Int J 2008;26(14):1589–1595. PubMed doi:10.1080/02640410802287089
Downloaded by University of Exeter on 10/01/16, Volume 11, Article Number 1

Sports Physiol Perform. 2014;9(5):863–870. PubMed doi:10.1123/ 19. Reid MM, Duffield R, Minett G, Sibte N, Murphy A, Baker J. Physi-
ijspp.2013-0452 ological, perceptual and technical responses to on-court tennis training
3. Reid M, Duffield R, Dawson B, Baker J, Crespo M. Quantification of on hard and clay courts. J Strength Cond Res. 2013;27(6):1487–1495.
the physiological and performance characteristics of on-court tennis PubMed doi:10.1519/JSC.0b013e31826caedf
drills. Br J Sports Med. 2008;42(2):146–151. PubMed doi:10.1136/ 20. Johnson CD, McHugh MP, Wood T, Kibler WB. Performance demands
bjsm.2007.036426 of professional male tennis players. Br J Sports Med. 2006;40(8):696–
4. Bekraoui N, Fargeas-Gluck M-A, Léger L. Oxygen uptake and heart 699. PubMed doi:10.1136/bjsm.2005.021253
rate response of 6 standardized tennis drills. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 21. Borg G. Subjective aspects of physical and mental load. Ergonomics.
2012;37(5):982–989. PubMed doi:10.1139/h2012-082 1978;21(3):215–220. PubMed doi:10.1080/00140137808931715
5. Ford P, De Ste Croix M, Lloyd R, et al. The long-term athlete devel- 22. Hart SG. NASA-Task Load Index (NASA-TLX); 20 years later. Paper
opment model: physiological evidence and application. J Sports Sci. presented at: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting;
2011;29(4):389–402. PubMed doi:10.1080/02640414.2010.536849 October 2006; Santa Monica, CA. doi:10.1177/154193120605000909
6. Reid M, Quinlan G, Kearney S, Jones D. Planning and periodization 23. Eston RG, Williams J. Exercise intensity and perceived exertion
for the elite junior tennis player. Strength Cond J. 2009;31(4):69–76. in adolescent boys. Br J Sports Med. 1986;20(1):27–30. PubMed
doi:10.1519/SSC.0b013e3181afc98d doi:10.1136/bjsm.20.1.27
7. Fernandez-Fernandez J, Mendez-Villanueva A, Fernandez-Garcia B, 24. Christensen JE, Christensen CE. Statistical power analysis of health,
Terrados N. Match activity and physiological responses during a junior physical education, and recreation research. Res Q. 1977;48(1):204–
female singles tennis tournament. Br J Sports Med. 2007;41(11):711– 208. PubMed
716. PubMed doi:10.1136/bjsm.2007.036210 25. Novas AM, Rowbottom DG, Jenkins DG. A practical method of
8. Torres-Luque G, Cabello-Manrique D, Hernández-García R, Garata- estimating energy expenditure during tennis play. J Sci Med Sport.
chea N. An analysis of competition in young tennis players. Eur J 2003;6(1):40–50. PubMed doi:10.1016/S1440-2440(03)80007-5
Sport Sci. 2011;11(1):39–43. doi:10.1080/17461391003770533 26. Girard O, Lattier G, Micallef JP, Millet GP. Changes in exercise charac-
9. King T, Jenkins D, Gabbett T. A time motion analysis of professional teristics, maximal voluntary contraction, and explosive strength during
rugby league match-play. J Sports Sci. 2009;27(3):213–219. PubMed prolonged tennis playing. Br J Sports Med. 2006;40(6):521–526.
doi:10.1080/02640410802538168 PubMed doi:10.1136/bjsm.2005.023754
10. Gabbett TJ. Science of rugby league football: a review. J Sports Sci. 27. O’ Donoghue P, Ingram B. A notational analysis of elite tennis
2005;23(9):961–976. PubMed doi:10.1080/02640410400023381 strategy. J Sports Sci. 2001;19(2):107–115. PubMed doi:10.1080/
11. Reid M, Duffield R. The development of fatigue during match- 026404101300036299
play tennis. Br J Sports Med. 2014;48(Suppl 1):i7–i11. PubMed 28. Duffield R, Murphy AP, Kellett AD, Reid M. Recovery from
doi:10.1136/bjsports-2013-093196 repeated on-court tennis sessions: combining cold-water immersion,
12. Mendez-Villanueva A, Fernandez-Fernandez J, Bishop D. Exercise- compression, and sleep interventions. Int J Sports Physiol Perform.
induced homeostatic perturbations provoked by singles tennis match 2014;9(2):273–282. PubMed http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/IJSPP.2012-
play with reference to development of fatigue. Br J Sports Med. 0359 \
2007;41(11):717–722. PubMed doi:10.1136/bjsm.2007.037259 29. Fernandez-Fernandez J, Boullosa DA, Sanz-Rivas D, Abreu L, Filaire
13. Hornery DJ, Farrow D, Mujika I, Young W. An integrated physiologi- E, Mendez-Villanueva A. Psychophysiological stress responses during
cal and performance profile of professional tennis. Br J Sports Med. training and competition in young female competitive tennis players.
2007;41(8):531–536. PubMed doi:10.1136/bjsm.2006.031351 Int J Sports Med. 2015;36(1):22–28. doi:10.1055/s-0034-1384544

IJSPP Vol. 11, No. 1, 2016

You might also like