You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/230068121

The Frustration Discomfort Scale: development and psychometric


properties

Article  in  Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy · September 2005


DOI: 10.1002/cpp.465

CITATIONS READS

114 11,094

1 author:

Neil Harrington
University of Stirling
16 PUBLICATIONS   395 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Neil Harrington on 01 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy
Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)

Assessment The Frustration


Discomfort Scale:
Development and
Psychometric
Properties
Neil Harrington*
University of Edinburgh and Fife Health Board, UK

Frustration intolerance beliefs are hypothesized by rational–emotive


behaviour therapy (REBT) to form one of two major categories of
psychological disturbance, along with those referring to self-worth.
Although frustration intolerance is a central REBT concept its content
and factor structure are unclear. Therefore, a multidimensional
measure, the Frustration Discomfort Scale (FDS), was developed
based on REBT literature and theory. An exploratory factor analysis,
using a combined clinical and student population, indicated a four-
factor structure. These dimensions were labelled (I) emotional intol-
erance, involving intolerance of emotional distress; (II) entitlement,
involving intolerance of unfairness and frustrated gratification; (III)
discomfort intolerance, involving intolerance of difficulties and
hassles, and (IV) achievement, involving intolerance of frustrated
achievement goals. From these results, a simplified scale was devel-
oped, with confirmatory factor analysis supporting a four-factor solu-
tion. Both the preliminary and revised scales showed good internal
reliability, and evidence of discriminative validity. Copyright © 2005
John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

INTRODUCTION However, although frustration intolerance1 is a


central concept in REBT theory, and one of REBT’s
Rational–emotive behaviour therapy (REBT) is distinguishing features, investigation of these
unique in clearly distinguishing between two beliefs has been limited (Neenan & Dryden, 1999).
categories of dysfunctional belief (Ellis, 1979, 1980). Evidence from existing scales indicates that frus-
The first category involves the intolerance of frus- tration intolerance and self-worth do form separate
tration and discomfort, whilst the second involves categories (DiGiuseppe, Leaf, Exner, & Robin,
the global rating of self-worth. Whilst these two 1988). Yet beyond this, the content and factor struc-
categories of belief interact, they are also hypo- ture of frustration intolerance beliefs, and their
thesized to have an independent and distinct relationship with specific problems, has remained
relationship with specific psychological problems. largely unexplored.

1
Traditionally the term ‘low frustration tolerance’ has
* Correspondence to: Dr. Neil Harrington, Stratheden Hos- described this belief process. Since this has been criticized as
pital, Cupar, Fife, KY15 5RR, UK being ambiguous (Neenan & Dryden, 1999) ‘frustration
E-mail: Neil@nharr.freeserve.co.uk intolerance’ is used in this paper.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/cpp.465
Development of the Frustration Discomfort Scale 375

One reason is that many earlier irrational belief items were best grouped in terms of content (Shaw,
measures, such as the Belief Scale (Malouff & unpublished master’s thesis). In line with theoret-
Schutte, 1986), were based on inadequate and out- ical models (Walen, DiGiuseppe, & Dryden, 1992),
dated definitions of irrational belief, and gave these content factors reflected themes regarding
only an overall score. Contemporary REBT theory the need for achievement, approval, and comfort,
focuses on four belief processes, with demands with further items added later to form a fairness
(e.g. ‘I must do well’) as the central process, and sub-scale (Bernard, 1998). However, the General
self-worth, frustration intolerance, and awfulizing Attitude and Belief Scale has significant limita-
secondary to this belief. Some assessment scales, tions. First, the sub-scales do not clearly differenti-
such as the Survey of Personal Beliefs (Demaria, ate between frustration intolerance and self-worth
Kassinove, & Dill, 1989), have employed this beliefs, with some sub-scales including items from
model. However, by focusing on belief processes both categories. Second, the frustration intolerance
the content of beliefs has been largely ignored, content range is relatively limited with many
although recent evidence suggests that both belief similar worded items, features that are likely to
processes and their content are important. In par- distort the factor structure.
ticular, the series of experimental studies by Bond Therefore, Neenan and Dryden (1999) have high-
and Dryden indicates that the content of self-worth lighted the need to develop a multidimensional
and frustration intolerance beliefs, rather than measure of frustration intolerance. Likewise, from
demandingness, is central in determining psycho- a cognitive therapy perspective, Power et al. (1994)
logical disturbance (Bond & Dryden, 2000). have recommended the development of multi-
The focus on belief content, rather than ‘irra- dimensional belief scales, with content areas based
tional’ processes, brings REBT closer to other on theoretical as well as statistically grounds. The
cognitive therapy approaches. However, whilst authors argue that the failure to adequately assess
assessment scales in the general literature have specific belief domains is one reason for inconsis-
focused on belief content, they do not clearly dis- tent evidence linking core beliefs with enduring
tinguish between self-worth and frustration intol- vulnerability to depression. This is because only a
erance. For instance, achievement beliefs on the small sub-set of beliefs will be relevant to specific
Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS) (Weissman & problems, and therefore global measures of beliefs
Beck, 1978) almost entirely refer to issues of self- will merely act as general indicators of distress.
worth. Likewise, the Multidimensional Perfection- Similarly, a fundamental weakness of REBT theory
ism Scale (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, has been the difficulty in distinguishing between
1990) fails to separate self-worth and frustration distinct psychological problems in terms of specific
intolerance beliefs. In regard to perfectionism, irrational beliefs (Kendall et al., 1995).
REBT would argue that high standards may be The purpose of the present study was to develop
independently associated with self-worth (‘If I fail a multidimensional scale regarding the content
it means I’m a failure as a person’) and frustration of frustration intolerance beliefs, and to clarify its
intolerance beliefs (‘I can’t stand not achieving my factor structure. Two separate studies were
absolute best’). involved in the development of the scale, both
Even if irrational belief processes are fundamen- employing clinical and student samples. The first
tal to emotional problems, it may be argued that study reports the construction and exploratory
these are too generalized and all encompassing to factor analysis of a preliminary Frustration Dis-
enable specific predictions. Furthermore, although comfort Scale (FDS). Based on these results, a
frustration intolerance has been treated as unidi- revised scale was developed in the second study,
mensional, the range of frustration intolerance and the factor structure tested by confirmatory
beliefs described in the REBT literature suggests a factor analysis.
multidimensional construct. These beliefs include
the intolerance of hassles, effort, unfairness, and STUDY 1: PRELIMINARY FRUSTRATION
uncomfortable emotions (Dryden & Gordon, 1993). DISCOMFORT SCALE: DEVELOPMENT
However, there is no existing belief scale specifi- AND ANALYSIS
cally aimed at assessing the content of frustration
Method
intolerance beliefs. The General Attitude and Belief
Scale (GABS) (DiGiuseppe et al., 1988) was de- Participants
signed to assess both belief content and process, The clinical group comprised 242 respondents
and factor analysis of the GABS indicated that from a group of consecutive therapy referrals to an

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
376 N. Harrington

adult clinical psychology department (39% men tionally described by the phrase ‘I can’t stand it’)
and 61% women). The mean age of the clinical and self-worth beliefs were separately rated for
group was 39 years (range 18–74). The sample was each demand statement:
recruited from a diverse geographical and social
area, and represented a broad range of non- I absolutely must not be kept waiting
psychotic problems, with anxiety disorders (32%),
Because I can’t tolerate waiting
depression (23%), and anger (14%) the most
0 1 2 3 4
common classifications. Problems were largely
It would totally lower my self-esteem
moderate to severe, with 50% of clients experienc-
0 1 2 3 4
ing these difficulties for three years or more
and 44% rating the effect on their everyday life
Reverse-scored items were not used, since the use-
as ‘severe’. There was a response rate of 44%.
fulness of this method of reducing response bias
A previous pilot study had indicated that non-
has been questioned (Schriesheim & Hill, 1981). In
responders did not differ from responders on
particular, it has been noted that only ten percent
diagnostic or demographic characteristics. The
of respondents are required to mistakenly answer
non-clinical group consisted of undergraduate
the reversed questions the ‘wrong’ way to produce
psychology students from three separate courses
a spurious reverse-scored factor (Schmitt & Stults,
in abnormal psychology (30% male and 70%
1985). Items referring to affect were avoided to
female).
reduce cross contamination and to insure the scale
Missing data was randomly spread across the
primarily assessed cognition rather than emotion
items, with a mean percentage missing values for
(Smith, 1989).
each item of 0.32%. Nine clinical cases and one
To enable adequate investigation of the underly-
student case with more than 10% of missing values
ing factor structure, and satisfy content validity,
were removed. With remaining cases containing
care was taken to include all relevant aspects
only a small percentage of missing data, a group
of frustration intolerance beliefs. Possible content
mean was employed. Six extreme outliers were
domains were suggested by REBT theory, and
also removed due to problematic patterns of
included demands for achievement, approval,
responses. Thus, following preliminary data analy-
comfort, fairness, immediate gratification, and
sis, 329 cases were included in the factor analysis:
control/certainty (Neenan & Dryden, 1999; Walen
242 in the clinical group and 87 in the student
et al., 1992). An item pool was created using exam-
group.
ples of frustration intolerance in the REBT litera-
ture (e.g. Dryden & Gordon, 1993; Neenan &
Dryden, 1999). Seventy-four items were selected
Scale Development
from this larger group to form the preliminary
It was important that the structure of items con-
Frustration Discomfort Scale. A small pilot study
formed to current REBT theory. Irrational beliefs
(n = 30) was conducted to allow revision of items
have been described as being best represented as a
that had poor comprehension or extreme fre-
compound sentence involving both a demand and
quency distributions. Content validity of the scale
a secondary belief (DiGiuseppe, 1996). It has also
was reviewed by Albert Ellis (personal communi-
been argued that the ‘absolute’ nature of the
cation, 1999), who commented that the important
demand statement needs to be emphasized,
aspects of the concept had been thoroughly
and that the word ‘should’ is insufficient in this
described.
regard (Robb & Warren, 1990). Following these
recommendations, the items were designed as
compound sentences with demand statements
Measures
prefaced by the phrase ‘absolutely must’. It was
also necessary to differentiate frustration intoler- Frustration Discomfort Scale (preliminary version).
ance from self-worth beliefs. This presented some This contained 74 items, rated on a five-point
difficulty, since these two categories of belief are Likert scale. Individuals were asked to rate the
closely interrelated. Therefore, Dryden (1996) has strength of belief, with the following anchors: (0)
argued that the strength of self-worth relative absent, (1) mild, (2) moderate, (3) strong, (4) very
to frustration intolerance needs to be assessed to strong. Using examples, the instructions high-
determine which type of belief is most salient. To lighted the difference between frustration and self-
address this, both frustration intolerance (tradi- esteem beliefs.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
Development of the Frustration Discomfort Scale 377

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). This discriminative validity were used. First, sub-
scale was only completed in study I by the clinical statement scores on the FDS were examined to
group. It is a 10-item Likert-type scale with satis- screen for those items with higher self-worth sub-
factory internal reliability (0.87) and temporal statement scores relative to frustration intolerance
stability (0.86). Schmitt and Stults (1985) have scores. Paired t-tests indicated that only four items
highlighted the importance of careful preliminary had significantly higher self-worth sub-statement
screening and the deletion of ‘careless responses’ scores. Second, correlations between FDS items
due to reversed item response error. Following this and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale were exam-
recommendation, the present study identified out- ined. Comparison of corrected item–total correla-
liers in terms of the difference between the nega- tions showed that three items had significantly
tive and positive self-esteem sub-scale scores, lower correlations with remaining items on the
eliminating nine cases and leaving 232 for analy- FDS than with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale.
sis. The mean and standard deviation of the clini- Two of these items (intolerance of personal flaws
cal group was 23.39 (5.79), with higher scores and of upsetting others) were clearly associated
representing high self-esteem. with the concept of self-worth. These two items
and six further items displaying multiple problems
Design and Procedure were removed, leaving 66 items to be included in
A cross-sectional design was used. For the clini- the factor analysis.
cal group, questionnaires were included with
notification of first appointment with a stamped Exploratory Factor Analysis
addressed envelope for reply. For the student Since there is little empirical evidence regarding
group, questionnaires were completed during a the structure of frustration intolerance beliefs,
psychology workshop. the preliminary study used exploratory factor
analysis. Theoretically, frustration intolerance
dimensions were likely to be closely associated,
suggesting that an oblique rotation would be most
Results
appropriate. However, this can make interpreta-
Preliminary Analyses tion more complex. Therefore, a principal com-
A series of preliminary analyses were conducted ponents analysis employing a varimax orthogonal
to identify items that were inappropriately in- rotation was conducted first, and this was then
cluded in the scale. Frequency analysis examined compared with an oblique rotation.
the distribution of responses across the rating scale Careful consideration was given to the number
for each frustration question. Items were deemed of factors to retain. The eigenvalue greater than one
to have frequency problems if any two adjacent criterion can greatly overestimate the number of
scale points had fewer than 10% of responses. Reli- factors (Cattell, 1978), and with 15 factors above
ability analyses, carried out to identify items this criterion this appeared to be the case in the
not correlated with the scale, took the form of an present study. A scree plot analysis is a more accu-
iterative process. That is, items with the lowest rate decision method (Zwick & Velicer, 1982),
reliability were removed and corrected item–total and this indicated four factors. Therefore, solutions
correlations recalculated. Examination of these cor- involving four, five, and six factors were examined.
relations indicated two items below the 0.30 crite- The eigenvalues of these six factors were 19.43,
rion, these items involved being unable to ‘risk 3.46, 2.89, 2.31, 1.58 and 1.45. Examining the
upsetting others’ and the ‘need for excitement’. five-factor solution, the fifth factor was found to
It had been intended, by using both self-worth account for only a very small amount of additional
and frustration intolerance statements, to identify cumulative variance (2.7%), with just one variable
the most salient belief category. Several methods loading above 0.40. Likewise, in the six-factor solu-
of weighting frustration intolerance scores relative tion the last two factors were poorly defined, with
to self-esteem scores were investigated. However, the final factor having only one item with a sub-
these did not show meaningful results, and a sim- stantial loading, and little psychological coherence
plified scoring system was finally employed using between items.
only the frustration intolerance statement scores. Therefore, a four-factor solution was considered
Nevertheless, since a central aim of the FDS was to best represent the data. The cumulative variance
to measure frustration intolerance independently accounted for was 42.58%, with factor I accounting
from self-worth, other methods of determining for 13.96%, factor II for 11.35%, factor III for 10.25%,

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
378 N. Harrington

and factor IV for 7.02%. All items had salient load- approval and more with the control and sub-
ings on at least one factor. Reassuringly, the results servience of others. Indeed, compared with the
of the four-factor oblique rotation closely matched other FDS sub-scales, entitlement had the lowest
the orthogonal rotation. The pattern of factors and correlation with self-esteem (Table 1). The central
item order was essentially the same in both rota- theme of this factor might be summarized as the
tions. Attempts at oblique rotations with five and belief that one’s desires must be met and that other
six factors failed to converge after 50 iterations, people should indulge and not frustrate these
further supporting the reliability and robustness of desires.
the four-factor solution. The third factor, labelled discomfort intolerance,
referred to demands that life should be easy, com-
fortable, and free of hassle. The highest loading
Factor Labelling item was ‘Tasks that I attempt absolutely must
The first factor was labelled emotional intolerance, not be too difficult. Otherwise, I can’t stand doing
with items reflecting intolerance of emotional dis- them’ (0.680). Such beliefs have traditionally been
tress. The item with the highest loading on this central to the REBT concept of frustration intoler-
factor was ‘I absolutely must be free of distressing ance. Whilst in the GABS these beliefs were com-
feelings as quickly as I can. I can’t stand for them bined with emotional intolerance items to form a
to continue’ (0.716). Items included beliefs regard- ‘need for comfort’ sub-scale, the present analysis
ing the uncertainty, controllability, and aversive- indicated they were best represented by a separate
ness of emotion. REBT has long emphasized the factor.
importance of secondary disturbance, such as Factor four was labelled achievement, with items
anxiety about anxiety, and its relationship to frus- referring to task related frustration. The highest
tration intolerance (Ellis, 1979). loading item was ‘I absolutely must not leave work
The second factor was labelled entitlement. The unfinished. I can’t bear to leave work unfinished’
highest loading item was ‘I absolutely must not be (0.745). These types of belief have been included in
taken for granted. I can’t stand being unappreci- several belief scales, including the DAS achieve-
ated’ (0.661). Included within this factor were ment sub-scale and the multidimensional perfec-
demands for immediate gratification and for fair- tionism scales. However, the present achievement
ness. Whilst many of the items referred to personal items aimed to assess the intolerance of frustration,
relationships, analysis indicated that these beliefs as opposed to loss of self-worth, following achieve-
were not associated with self-worth or gaining ment goal failure. The relatively weak correlation
approval. Thus, the item regarding upsetting of the achievement sub-scale with self-esteem,
others had a negligible correlation (r = 0.09) with compared with its correlation with other FDS sub-
entitlement, suggesting that individuals scoring scales, supports the argument that this aim was
high on this factor were less concerned with successful (Table 1).

Table 1. Correlations between the revised FDS and Rosenberg Self-Esteem


Scale (preliminary FDS scores in brackets)

Rosenberg FDS
Self-Esteem Scale
Discomfort intolerance -0.43*** (-0.36***) 0.65*** (0.72***)
Entitlement -0.20*** (-0.17*) 0.67*** (0.73***)
Emotional intolerance -0.49*** (-0.44***) 0.70*** (0.69***)
Achievement -0.29*** (-0.18**) 0.64*** (0.57***)
Full four-factor scale -0.43*** (-0.36***)
Gratification -0.28*** 0.79***
Fairness -0.16* 0.68***
Full five-factor scale -0.40***
N = 232 (self-esteem), N = 242 (preliminary FDS).
N = 248 (self-esteem), N = 254 (revised FDS).
*** p < 0.001.
** p < 0.01.
* p < 0.05.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
Development of the Frustration Discomfort Scale 379

Sub-Scale Formation the remaining FDS items (i.e. minus the sub-scale
The final selection of items to form sub-scales in question). The discomfort and emotional intol-
was primarily based on relative loadings, using a erance sub-scales had the strongest correlations
cut-off point of >0.45. The need to take into account with self-esteem and entitlement and achievement
communalities and to include a representative the weakest.
content range led to three items below this loading Significant differences in FDS mean scores were
criterion being retained. Only three of the final sub- found between the clinical and student groups
scale items were classed as complex variables, with (Table 3). All sub-scales, except entitlement, were
loadings greater than 0.45 on other factors. Cor- higher in the clinical population. There was no sig-
rected item–total correlations were computed to nificant correlation between the full scale score and
highlight those items with poor reliability regard- age (r(240) = -0.02, ns) or gender (t(240) = 0.62, ns),
ing designated sub-scales. All items showed corre- or between these variables and individual sub-
lations above 0.45 on their own scale, and above scales. Although 15 final scale items did not sig-
0.30 with the full scale. Alpha values were (with the nificantly differentiate between the two groups, it
mean inter-item correlation in brackets) emotional was expected that some beliefs would have ele-
intolerance 0.91 (0.43), entitlement 0.88 (0.37), dis- vated scores in the student group. Thus, items
comfort 0.90 (0.40), achievement 0.82 (0.36), and referring to task obstruction, task hassle, task
full-scale score 0.95 (0.29). The full scale comprised interest, and task persistence all showed elevated
47 items, with each of the first three sub-scales con- scores, possibly reflecting increased concerns
taining 13 items, and the achievement sub-scale regarding academic work.
eight items. As expected, the FDS dimensions were
substantially intercorrelated (Table 2).
Discussion
Divergent and Discriminative Validity The results of this study support a model of
There were weak to moderate correlations frustration intolerance as a multidimensional
between the Frustration Discomfort sub-scales construct. The strong intercorrelations between
and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Table 1). sub-scales, and the weaker relationships with self-
However, these correlations with self-esteem were esteem, are consistent with the separation of frus-
substantially lower compared with the correlations tration intolerance beliefs from those of self-worth.
between the Frustration Discomfort sub-scales and Factor analysis indicated that a four-factor solution

Table 2. Revised FDS: sub-scale intercorrelation matrix (preliminary scale in


brackets)

Sub-scales 1 2 3 4
1 Discomfort intolerance
2 Entitlement 0.56 (0.65)
3 Emotional intolerance 0.62 (0.68) 0.56 (0.56)
4 Achievement 0.48 (0.47) 0.60 (0.55) 0.60 (0.51)
All correlations p < 0.001.

Table 3. Preliminary FDS: mean scores and standard deviations

Clinical Student t
Full scale 95.1 (34.5) 72.6 (27.1) 5.50***
Discomfort intolerance 22.4 (11.5) 16.6 (8.4) 4.31***
Entitlement 25.0 (10.9) 24.1 (9.3) 0.69
Emotional intolerance 30.8 (12.2) 18.5 (9.4) 8.64***
Achievement 16.9 (7.0) 13.5 (5.9) 4.04***
N = 242 (clinical). N = 87 (student).
*** p < 0.001.
** p < 0.01.
* p < 0.05.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
380 N. Harrington

best described the data, and yielded meaningful Scale. This suggested that the overlap with self-
sub-scales showing high internal consistency. esteem was relatively small. Therefore, it was
Additional factors appeared residual and poorly decided to use a single frustration intolerance
defined. However, the original content categories statement for each item and dispense with the
suggested by REBT theory were not entirely additional demand and self-worth statements:
replicated. Control beliefs were associated with a
number of content areas rather than forming a sep- I can’t stand doing things that involve a lot
arate certainty/control factor. Discomfort intoler- of hassle
ance and emotional intolerance were also found to 1 2 3 4 5
form separate factors.
It could also be argued that the equivalent These changes substantially simplified the ques-
dimension to the ‘need for approval’ within the tion structure and improved readability (Flesch
domain of frustration intolerance was that of enti- reading score of 84% compared to 64%). There
tlement. A further finding was that, rather than was also a reduction in missing data and spoiled
forming separate factors, items referring to imme- responses (2% compared to 5%). Furthermore, con-
diate gratification and fairness mainly loaded on siderably fewer responses in the ‘absent’ category
entitlement. Interestingly, whilst loading on enti- (12% compared to 21%) suggested greater ‘careless
tlement, the two positive gratification items (the responding’ on the preliminary scale, possible due
need for a ‘buzz’ or excitement) had weak rela- to item complexity.
tionships with remaining items on the FDS (0.34 All but two preliminary scale items were trans-
and 0.24 respectively). This was unexpected, since ferred to the new scale. Of 47 items included in the
short-term hedonism is described as a core aspect revised scale, 37 were used with relatively minor
of frustration intolerance (Dryden & Gordon, changes in wording. Overlapping and redundant
1993). On the other hand, the item referring to item content was reduced and the achievement
intolerance of gratification delay had a relatively scale was lengthened to make it equal with other
strong correlation (0.58) with the remaining sub-scales. Since it was unclear whether fairness
scale. One possible explanation is that the two and gratification beliefs were separate dimensions
‘excitement’ items reflected functional extraversion or facets of entitlement, further items were gener-
rather than dysfunctional impulsiveness. It might ated to enable entitlement to be split into two
further suggest that frustration intolerance items smaller sub-scales. Study II aimed to examine how
were better worded in terms of negative states, well the hypothesized factor structure fitted the
such as deprivation or aversiveness, rather than as data using a confirmatory factor analysis.
a need for positive gratification.

Method
Participants and Procedure
The participants and procedure followed that
STUDY II: REVISED FRUSTRATION
of the preliminary study. The clinical sample con-
DISCOMFORT SCALE: DEVELOPMENT
sisted of 254 patients, with the response rate (45%)
AND ANALYSIS
comparable to the previous study. Mean age of the
Scale Redesign clinical group was 38.6 (41% male and 59% female).
Six cases were removed due to spoilt or missing
The preliminary scale had been designed follow-
data. The non-clinical population (n = 79) came
ing REBT theory that proposed irrational beliefs
from two classes of undergraduate psychology
were best represented by combining a primary
students (17% male and 83% female). This gave an
demand with a secondary self-worth or frustration
overall sample of 333 cases for inclusion in the con-
intolerance belief. However, subsequent research
firmatory factor analysis. The mean percentage of
indicated that secondary beliefs may constitute the
missing values for each item was 0.48%, and these
primary mechanism leading to disturbance, and
were replaced by the group mean.
were best used on their own in assessment (Bond
& Dryden, 2000). The preliminary scale analysis
Measures
also indicated that all but two frustration intoler-
ance items had substantially higher correlations Frustration Discomfort Scale (revised version). The
with the FDS than with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem initial scale consisted of 47 items rated on a five-

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
Development of the Frustration Discomfort Scale 381

point Likert scale with the following anchors: (1) had substantially higher correlations with the
absent, (2) mild, (3) moderate, (4) strong, (5) very overall FDS than with self-esteem. Final selection
strong. The instructions were as follows: ‘Listed for sub-scales took into account the strength of cor-
below are a number of common thoughts and rected item-total correlations and the need to have
beliefs that people may have when they are dis- an acceptable range of item content.
tressed or frustrated. Please read each statement Sub-scale alpha coefficients were (mean inter-
and decide how well this usually describes your item correlation in brackets) discomfort intolerance
own beliefs. Circle the number that best indicates 0.88 (0.50), entitlement 0.85 (0.42), emotional intol-
the strength of this belief’. Following analysis, the erance 0.87 (0.49), and achievement 0.84 (0.43),
final number of items was reduced to 28 items on with gratification 0.84 (0.43), and fairness 0.83
the four-factor scale, with each sub-scale contain- (0.42). The full-scale mean inter-item correlation
ing seven items. The five-factor scale contained for the four-factor scale was 0.94 (0.34) and the five-
seven additional items to form separate gratifica- factor scale 0.95 (0.33). These values indicate good
tion and fairness sub-scales. internal consistency, with the mean inter-item cor-
relation giving a clearer measure of item homo-
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. The Rosenberg scale geneity since this is not influenced by scale length.
was completed by clinical and student groups. Although the sub-scales were designed to measure
Preliminary screening resulting in the removal of relatively narrow factors, no mean inter-item cor-
three clinical cases showing ‘careless responding’, relation exceeded 0.50, which represents the item
leaving 248 clinical, and 77 student replies, for redundancy cut-off point suggested by Briggs and
further analysis. Cheek (1986). Furthermore, the full-scale scores are
within the optimal range of item homogeneity
Preliminary Analyses and Sub-Scale Formation (0.20–0.40) that Briggs and Cheek suggest offers the
Inappropriate items and cases were eliminated best compromise between bandwidth and fidelity.
using the same criteria as in the preliminary study. Finally, all items had corrected item–total correla-
No items showed frequency problems and all tions above 0.48 with the full scale, and above 0.50
items had corrected item–total correlations above with sub-scales (Table 4). Overall, these results
0.45. For the initial scale, internal reliabilities were suggest the revised scale has captured the range
high with a full-scale alpha of 0.96 and sub-scale and complexity of frustration intolerance beliefs
alpha values between 0.92 and 0.85. However, within an acceptable level of item redundancy.
a high alpha may simply reflect high inter-
correlations among items, indicating overlapping Confirmatory Factor Analysis
or redundant item content. Therefore, particular Both clinical and student FDS scores were
importance was placed on reducing redundancy normally distributed, and examination of individ-
and conceptual overlap when deciding item selec- ual item distributions did not indicate marked
tion for the final scale. Examination of the sub-scale deviations from normality. Therefore, confirmatory
intercorrelation matrix indicated only one pair of factor analysis was conducted employing the
items with an intercorrelation >0.70, referring to maximum likelihood method and using the AMOS
intolerance of disturbed feelings and of disturbing 4 program (Arbuckle & Wothke, 1999). Five alter-
thoughts (r = 0.73). However, it was decided that native models were compared. The first model was
conceptually these beliefs were sufficiently distinct a unidimensional model in which all the items
to warrant inclusion. Five items with intercorrela- were assumed to load on a single frustration intol-
tions above 0.60 were removed, and two other erance dimension. A two-factor model had dis-
items with overlapping content. Similarly, five comfort intolerance and emotional intolerance
items with relatively low correlations with their forming a ‘discomfort’ factor, and entitlement
designated sub-scale were also removed. Cor- and achievement forming a ‘frustration’ factor. The
rected item–total correlations were recalculated four-factor model was the solution most strongly
after the removal of each item. supported by the exploratory factor analysis. A
As in the preliminary study, the relationship five-factor model split the entitlement sub-scale
between individual items and the Rosenberg Self- into two further sub-groups representing fairness
Esteem Scale was examined. Only two items (intol- and immediate gratification.
erance of disturbing thoughts and continuing The adequacy of the competing models was
situation) had correlations above 0.50, and these evaluated using five different fit indices: (1) the
two items, along with all other items on the scale, model chi-square, a measure of overall fit, with a

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
382 N. Harrington

non-significant c2 indicating good fit; (2) the c2 correlations with remaining FDS items were sub-
divided by the degrees of freedom, with a ratio of stantially higher than with self-esteem, supporting
between two and three suggesting a good fit the divergent validity of the scale. Whilst the com-
(Carmines & McIver, 1981); (3) the comparative fit parisons in Table 1 refer to clinical groups, the rela-
index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), with values above 0.95 tionship between the FDS sub-scales and student
indicating a good fit; (4) the root mean square of self-esteem scores showed evidence of even greater
approximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck, 1993), separation (correlations with remaining FDS items
with values over 0.10 leading to rejection of the in brackets): discomfort intolerance -0.38 (0.57),
model, those from 0.05 to 0.08 acceptable, and entitlement -0.11 (0.67), emotional intolerance
values below 0.05 indicating a good fit; (5) the -0.38 (0.70), achievement -0.24 (0.68).
Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis, 1973), Means and standard deviations for both the four
which takes into account the degree of parsimony, and five-factor models are presented in Table 6.
with scores of above 0.90 regarded as a reasonable Independent t-tests indicated significant differ-
fit. ences in FDS scores between the clinical and
Fit indices are presented in Table 5. Although all student groups. As might be expected, emotional
models had significant c2 values, this frequently intolerance showed the greatest separation and
overstates the lack of fit in larger samples. achievement frustration the least. Analysis of indi-
However, the one- and two-factor models also dis- vidual items indicated that nine items did not
played a poor fit on the other fit indices, with both distinguish between the two groups. As with the
the c2 ratio and the RMSEA values failing to reach preliminary scale, these items largely reflected
acceptable levels. On the other hand, the four- and the increased importance of task orientation in a
five-factor models were both good fits to the data. student population (Q2, 4, 5, 8, 9, 10, 16, 24, and
The CFI and TLI values indicate a better fit for the 25). Four of the seven additional items included
four-factor model, although the difference was rel- in the fairness and gratification sub-scales also
atively small. All items loaded on their designated failed to discriminate between clinical and student
factors. The standardized factor loadings for both groups (Q30, 31, 32, and 33). Interestingly, the grat-
the five- and four-factor models were all substan- ification sub-scale had the strongest correlation
tial and significant, ranging from 0.554 to 0.826 with remaining FDS items, underlining the impor-
(Table 4). tance of these beliefs.
There were no gender differences for the FDS
Sub-Scale Intercorrelations total score in the clinical or student groups. How-
The examination of the intercorrelations between ever, the emotional intolerance sub-scale was
sub-scales was helpful in comparing the four- and significantly higher for females in the clinical
five-factor models (Table 2). The correlation cor- group (t(252) = 2.53, p < 0.05). Unlike the prelimi-
rected for attenuation due to unreliability should nary scale, there was a weak but significant nega-
be larger than the sub-scale reliabilities, and if this tive relationship between the FDS total score and
is not the case then the two scales lack discri- age (r(248) = -0.15, p < 0.05), with age significantly
mination. All four sub-scales, although moderately correlating with entitlement (r(248) = -0.24, p <
intercorrelated, met this criterion. However, the 0.001) and emotional intolerance (r(248) = -0.15, p
fairness and gratification scales had corrected < 0.05). This indicates that entitlement and emo-
correlation (0.87) higher than their respective reli- tional intolerance decrease with greater maturity.
abilities (0.83 and 0.84), suggesting that these two
scales are practically indistinguishable.
Discussion
Divergent and Discriminative Validity
Correlations between the FDS and Rosenberg The confirmatory factor analysis supported the
Self-Esteem Scale are presented in Table 1. Com- multidimensional model of frustration intolerance
parison with the preliminary scale results shows an described in study I. Although the four- and five-
increased association with between the revised factor models both fitted the data well, the magni-
scale and self-esteem, with the full-scale correla- tude of the difference in fit between these two
tion increasing from r = 0.36 to 0.43. However, models was small and the degree of interrelation-
apart from the achievement sub-scale, which con- ship large. Furthermore, the fairness and gratifica-
tained a wider range of items in the revised scale, tion sub-scales had corrected correlations higher
the increase is relatively small. Moreover, sub-scale than their reliability coefficients. This suggests

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
Table 4. Revised FDS: Standardized factor loadings and corrected item-total correlations

Factor Factor Factor Factor Factor Factor Corrected


I II III IV II (a) II (b) item–total
correlation
Factor I Discomfort intolerance
21 I can’t stand doing things that involve a lot of hassle 0.784 0.724
13 I can’t stand having to push myself at tasks 0.758 0.701
25 I can’t stand having to persist at unpleasant tasks 0.704 0.637
5 I can’t stand doing tasks that seem too difficult 0.702 0.666
17 I can’t stand the hassle of having to do things right now 0.672 0.633
1 I need the easiest way around problems; 0.669 0.602

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


I can’t stand making a hard time of it
9 I can’t stand doing tasks when I’m not in the mood 0.666 0.623
Development of the Frustration Discomfort Scale

Factor II Entitlement
10 I can’t bear it if other people stand in the way 0.756 0.683
of what I want
6 I can’t stand it if other people act against my wishes 0.716 0.632
18 I can’t stand having to give in to other people’s demands 0.707 0.625
22 I can’t stand having to change when others are at fault 0.697 0.665
26 I can’t tolerate criticism especially when I know I’m right 0.627 0.581
14 I can’t tolerate being taken for granted 0.600 0.547
2 I can’t stand having to wait for things I would like now 0.559 0.499

Factor III Emotional intolerance


19 I can’t bear disturbing feelings 0.826 0.756
11 I can’t bear to have certain thoughts 0.799 0.728
3 I must be free of disturbing feelings as quickly as possible; 0.702 0.646
I can’t bear if they continue
15 I can’t stand situations where I might feel upset 0.685 0.613
27 I can’t stand to lose control of my feelings 0.636 0.585
7 I can’t bear to feel that I am losing my mind 0.632 0.600
23 I can’t get on with my life, or be happy, 0.619 0.570
if things don’t change

Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)


383
384

Factor IV Achievement
28 I can’t tolerate any lapse in my self-discipline 0.672 0.588
24 I can’t stand feeling that I’m not on top of my work 0.666 0.602
20 I can’t stand doing a job if I’m unable to do it well 0.660 0.581
4 I can’t stand being prevented from achieving 0.654 0.604
my full potential
8 I can’t bear the frustration of not achieving my goals 0.652 0.604
16 I can’t bear to move on from work 0.651 0.591
I’m not fully satisfied with
12 I can’t tolerate lowering my standards even when 0.617 0.565

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


it would be useful to do so

Factor II (a) Gratification


10 I can’t bear it if other people stand in the way of what I want 0.743 0.677
6 I can’t stand it if other people act against my wishes 0.713 0.621
18 I can’t stand having to give into other people’s demands 0.688 0.591
29 I can’t tolerate being overlooked 0.658 0.594
34 I can’t bear being deprived now of things I lacked in the past 0.612 0.563
32 I can’t stand giving up immediate pleasures for 0.587 0.536
the sake of a distant goal
2 I can’t stand having to wait for things I would like now 0.579 0.559

Factor II (b) Fairness


22 I can’t stand having to change when others are at fault 0.757 0.691
14 I can’t tolerate being taken for granted 0.716 0.651
33 I can’t tolerate being treated with disrespect 0.689 0.635
26 I can’t tolerate criticism especially when I know I’m right 0.641 0.552
35 I can’t tolerate other people’s bad or stupid behaviour 0.592 0.529
31 I can’t stand being left in the dark with no explanations 0.585 0.521
30 I can’t bear to have been treated unjustly 0.554 0.494

Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)


N. Harrington
Development of the Frustration Discomfort Scale 385

Table 5. Confirmatory factor analysis: fit indices

Model c2 df c2/df CFI TLI RMSEA


Single factor 1531.51 350 4.38 0.949 0.940 0.101
Two factors 1224.90 349 3.51 0.962 0.956 0.087
Four factors 758.46 344 2.21 0.982 0.979 0.060
Five factors 1208.37 550 2.20 0.977 0.974 0.060

Table 6. Revised FDS: means and standard deviations

Clinical Student t
Four-factor score 90.8 (20.7) 75.7 (15.9) 6.82***
Discomfort intolerance 20.4 (6.5) 17.6 (4.7) 4.22***
Entitlement 22.1 (6.2) 19.6 (4.4) 3.87***
Emotional intolerance 25.6 (6.6) 18.3 (5.1) 10.39***
Achievement 22.7 (6.1) 20.2 (5.2) 3.28***
Five-factor score 113.2 (25.3) 95.4 (19.4) 6.57***
Gratification 19.9 (6.3) 17.2 (4.1) 4.43***
Fairness 24.6 (5.9) 22.2 (5.1) 3.27***
N = 254 (clinical group). N = 79 (student group).
*** p < 0.001.

that they are correlated to such an extent that little ination between the clinical and student groups
would be gained by their separation, and that they was also improved, with all sub-scales significantly
are best interpreted as highly related facets of enti- higher in the clinical group.
tlement (John & Benet-Martinez, 2000). However, These results are of clinical and theoretical
whilst the four-factor model is preferred on the importance. Regarding the REBT theory of psy-
grounds of parsimony it may be premature to con- chological disturbance, they support a model of
clude that the distinction between gratification irrational belief in which frustration intolerance
and fairness is unimportant. One limitation of the and self-worth beliefs form separate categories of
present study is that, whilst representing a wide disturbance (DiGiuseppe, 1996). However, com-
range of disorders, the numbers of individuals in parison of the preliminary and revised scales indi-
some specific diagnostic categories was relatively cates that restricting the wording of irrational
small. Problems expected to have particular beliefs to frustration intolerance statements, and
associations with frustration intolerance, such as omitting absolute demandingness, did not change
alcohol and substance abuse, were underrepre- the factor structure or reduce the relationship
sented, and it would be useful for further research with psychological disturbance. Whilst this does
to determine the generalizability of these factors to not imply that imperative demands are irrelevant
different populations. to disturbance, it does suggest that other belief
The revised scale did show an increased correla- processes may be equally important. This is also
tion with self-esteem, suggesting that the more consistent with recent evidence that irrational
complex preliminary scale structure helped to sep- beliefs may be best phrased in terms of secondary
arate these belief categories. However, the increase belief content (Bond & Dryden, 2000).
in correlation is relatively small, and balanced From a clinical perspective, effective therapy
against this the revised scale shows improvement depends on being able to identify which cognitions
in readability, acceptability, and psychometric are related to what problems. However, although
properties. Since individuals with low frustration frustration intolerance beliefs are hypothesized as
tolerance would be less likely to complete difficult being central to many difficulties, such as therapy
tasks, these are not minor considerations. Discrim- resistance (Ellis, 1985), the empirical evidence is

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
386 N. Harrington

sparse. In particular, it is unclear whether the ing. Poster session presented at the World Congress of
dimensions of frustration intolerance are uniquely Behaviour Therapy, Edinburgh.
related to particular problems or if these beliefs Dryden, W. (1996). When musts are not enough. In
W. Dryden (Ed.), Inquiries in rational emotive therapy
have a more general influence. There is initial evi- (pp. 47–50). London: Sage.
dence from further validation data that specific Dryden, W., & Gordon, W. (1993). Beating the comfort trap.
dimensions of frustration intolerance are associ- London: Sheldon.
ated with distinct emotional problems, indepen- Ellis, A. (1979). Discomfort anxiety: A new cognitive
dent of any shared relationship with self-esteem behavioral construct. Part I. Rational Living, 14, 3–8.
(Harrington, unpublished doctoral thesis). It is Ellis, A. (1980). Discomfort anxiety: A new cognitive
behavioral construct. Part II. Rational Living, 15, 25–30.
hoped that the development of a multidimensional
Ellis, A. (1985). Overcoming resistance: Rational emotive
measure will encourage the further investigation of therapy with difficult patients. New York: Springer.
these beliefs. Frost, R.O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R.
(1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468.
John, O.P., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Measurement:
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Reliability, construct validation, and scale construc-
This paper is based on research submitted to the tion. In H.T. Reis, & C.M. Judd (Eds), Handbook of
research methods in social and personality psychology (pp.
University of Edinburgh in part fulfilment of a
339–369). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Doctorate of Philosophy degree. I would like to Kendall, P.C., Haaga, D.A.F., Ellis, A., Bernard, M.,
thank Professor Mick Power for his help and DiGiuseppe, R., & Kassinove, H. (1995). Rational–
encouragement throughout the research. emotive therapy in the 1990 and beyond: Current
status, recent revisions, and research questions. Clini-
cal Psychology Review, 15, 169–185.
Malouff, J.M., & Schutte, N.S. (1986). Development and
REFERENCES validation of a measure of irrational belief. Journal of
Arbuckle, J.L., & Wothke, W. (1999). Amos 4.0 users guide. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 860–862.
Chicago, IL: SPSS. Neenan, M., & Dryden, W. (1999). Rational emotive behav-
Bernard, M.E. (1998). Validation of the General Attitude iour therapy: Advances in theory and practice. London:
and Belief Scale. Journal of Rational–Emotive and Whurr.
Cognitive–Behavior Therapy, 16, 183–196. Power, M.J., Katz, R., McGuffin, P., Duggan, C.F., Lam,
Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural D., & Beck, A.T. (1994). The Dysfunctional Attitude
equation models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246. Scale (DAS): A comparison of forms A and B and pro-
Bond, W.B., & Dryden, W. (2000). How rational beliefs posals for a new sub-scaled version. Journal of Research
and irrational beliefs affect people’s inferences: An in Personality, 28, 263–276.
experimental investigation. Behavioural and Cognitive Robb, H.B., & Warren, R. (1990). Irrational belief tests:
Psychotherapy, 28, 33–43. New insights, new directions. Journal of Cognitive Psy-
Briggs, R.S., & Cheek, J.M. (1986). The role of factor chotherapy, 4, 303–311.
analysis in the development and evaluation of per- Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
sonality scales. Journal of Personality, 54, 106–148. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways Schmitt, N., & Stults, D.M. (1985). Factors defined
of assessing model fit. In K.A. Bollen and J.S. Long by negatively keyed item: The result of careless
(Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). respondents? Applied Psychological Measurement, 9,
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 367–373.
Carmines, E.G., & McIver, J.P. (1981). Analyzing models Schriesheim, C.A., & Hill, K.D. (1981). Controlling acqui-
with unobserved variables. In G.W. Bohrnstedt, & E.F. escence response bias by item reversals: The effect
Borgatta (Eds), Social measurement: Current issues. on questionnaire validity. Educational and Psychological
Beverly Hills: Sage. Measurement, 41, 1101–1114.
Cattell, R.B. (1978). The scientific use of factor analysis. New Smith, T.W. (1989). Assessment in rational emotive
York: Plenum. therapy: Empirical access to the ABCD model. In
Demaria, T.P., Kassinove, H., & Dill, C.A. (1989). Psy- M. Bernard, & R. DiGiuseppe (Eds), Inside rational
chometric properties of the survey of personal beliefs: emotive-therapy: A critical appraisal of the theory and
A rational–emotive measure of irrational thinking. therapy of Albert Ellis (pp. 135–153). San Diego:
Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 329–341. Academic.
DiGiuseppe, R. (1996). The nature of irrational and Tucker, L.R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient
rational beliefs: Progress in rational emotive behavior for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika,
therapy. Journal of Rational Emotive and Cognitive– 38, 1–10.
Behavior Therapy, 14, 5–28. Walen, S.R., DiGiuseppe, R., & Dryden, W. (1992). Prac-
DiGiuseppe, R., Leaf, R.C., Exner, T., & Robin, M. (1988). titioner’s guide to rational–emotive therapy (2nd ed.).
The development of a measure of irrational/rational think- Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
Development of the Frustration Discomfort Scale 387

Weissman, A., & Beck, A.T. (1978). Development and vali- Zwick, W.R., & Velicer, W.F. (1982). Factors influencing
dation of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Paper pre- four rules for determining the number of components
sented at the Annual Convention of the Association for to retain. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 17, 253–269.
Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Chicago, IL.

Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 12, 374–387 (2005)
View publication stats

You might also like