You are on page 1of 6

C L I N I C A L A N D E X P E R I M E N T A L

Acuity predictions from VEP Heravian, Douthwaite and Jenkins


OPTOMETRY

Acuity predictions from visually evoked


potential to checkerboard pattern reversal
stimuli: the effect of reversal rate

Shandiz J Heravian* MSc PhD Introduction: A pattern reversal stimulus (check size 5.5 minutes of arc) was used to
WA Douthwaite† MSc PhD FCOptom elicit the visually evoked potential (VEP).
DCLP Methods: The peak to trough amplitude of the VEP wave was measured and compared
TCA Jenkins† MSc PhD FCOptom DCLP to subjective visual acuity (Landolt C). The recordings were made at three, six and 12
*Department of Optometry, Mashhad pattern reversals per second.
University of Medical Sciences Results: The correlation between VEP amplitude and visual acuity was found to decrease

Department of Optometry, University at higher temporal frequencies. The decrease is not confined to the use of a television
of Bradford system reversing checkerboard pattern stimulus as suggested by previous workers.
Discussion: It may well be concerned with a change in processing of the visual informa-
Accepted for publication: 19 October tion as could be expected at these higher frequencies.
1999 (Clin Exp Optom 1999; 82: 6: 244–249)

Keyboards: amplitude, reversal rate, visual acuity, visually evoked potential

The visually evoked potential (VEP) has the predicted acuity derived from a sin- of arc check used above. Shors and col-
been used by a number of investigators gle amplitude measurement of the small- leagues 10 suggested that the run-to-run
for non-invasive assessment of visual acuity est check size stimulus used in the inves- variability can be minimised by averaging
in human infants.1,2 Marg and coworkers,1 tigation (5.5 minutes of arc) was over a number of runs. Repeatability of
Towle and Harter 3 and Skalka 4 used a reasonable. It has been shown previously the VEP amplitude and subjective acuity
target where the detail size was reduced that when a small check stimulus of 5.5 has been studied by Douthwaite and col-
until a threshold was reached with the minutes of arc is used, there is a signifi- leagues. 11 They found a better repea-
visual signal no longer discernible from cant correlation between evoked potential tability at slow pattern reversal rates. In a
noise. One of the problems with this amplitude and subjective visual acuity in further study, Douthwaite and Jenkins12
approach is that including targets which non-amblyopic subjects. This correlation used the two check sizes of 5.5 and 40
produce noisy signals into a subsequent decreases when checks of increasing size minutes of arc and suggested that a single
regression will alter the slope of that are used.7 However, such measurements run of 65 sweeps produced a representa-
regression5 -7 and yield different resolu- have the benefit of needing only a single tive amplitude for a 40 minutes of arc
tions. Although there are techniques avail- observation, making them clinically fast check, whereas the 5.5 minutes of arc
able for dealing with this problem (for and easy to achieve. check required more runs to be averaged
example, sweep VEP), they require com- Amplitude measurements have been before the amplitude was stable. Jenkins
plex data processing. An alternative questioned in the past due to variability and colleagues7 noted that the correlation
approach 1,8,9 is to take supra-threshold on successive measurement, although between visual acuity and VEP wave
measurements of wave amplitudes at a generally these criticisms have been lev- amplitude decreased as the check size was
fixed checkerboard target size. elled at work that used check sizes three increased. This latter point must cast
Jenkins and colleagues7 discovered that or four times larger than the 5.5 minutes doubt on any approach that uses the VEP

Clinical and Experimental Optometry 82.6 November–December 1999


244
Acuity predictions from VEP Heravian, Douthwaite and Jenkins

METHODS

Visual acuity assessment


Landolt Cs were used to measure the sub-
ject’s visual acuity.7 A walk-back technique
was employed. The Landolt Cs were pre-
sented in a rectangular matt white field,
of size 14 by 10 degrees. Each Landolt C
test card consisted of one row of 10 Cs with
each C separated by a space one C wide.
The direction of the gap was confined to
the four cardinal positions. The back-
Figure 1. A typical set of three VEP recordings made from an ground luminance of the test card, and
individual subjects showing P1and N2 for the waves analysed the screen in which it was held, was
in this study. A is the three reversals per second wave, B is the 258 cd/m 2. The mean contrast of the
six reversals per second wave, C is the 12 reversals per second Landolt C targets was 85 per cent (where
wave. The waves have been displaced vertically for clarity. Note contrast = L max - L min/L max + L min).
the isolated (transient) waveform in A and the steady state Subjects were required to estimate their
waveform in C. own threshold for a trial test card by ad-
justing the target-to-eye distance until they
felt they were at the limit of their acuity.
The measuring procedure then started
under forced-choice conditions, although
no limit to time was given. The distance
response to large check sizes (generated and 12 reversals per second) found a good of the subject was increased (in steps,
by visual cells eccentric to the fovea) to correlation between the P1N2 amplitude which produced five seconds of arc
estimate visual acuity. Thus, any regression and visual acuity at three and six reversals changes in the gap size of the Cs) in a
technique using supra-threshold measure- per second, but a lower correlation at 12 bracketing method until they failed to call
ments that involves large checks for the reversals per second. the pass criterion of eight correct out of a
purpose of predicting visual acuity via a Lovasik and Ahmedbhai 14 described row of 10 Cs. The test card was changed
regression line or curve and an intercept VEP artefacts arising from the mean at each viewing distance. In this fashion,
is questionable. screen luminance variations with time the resolution threshold was determined
The optimal temporal characteristics of which occurred with their television stimu- and was considered to be equal to the
the VEP stimulus used for acuity assays lus. On such a screen, the check reversal angular subtense of the gap in the small-
have received comparatively little atten- does not occur at the same time in all parts est C that produced a pass.
tion. Regan13 reviewed studies by inde- of the screen as it takes a finite time for
pendent workers which suggested that each frame to be presented. We wanted The VEP measurement
spatial selectivity in infants may be con- to determine if the decrease in correla- The VEP stimulus was a black and white
founded with temporal selectivity. His own tion with the fast reversal rate found by pattern reversal checkerboard produced
work 13 suggests that infants as old as Douthwaite and colleagues11 using a mov- by a moving mirror back projection system
four to six years possess pattern-evoked ing mirror stimulus, which reverses all the (Digitimer Model No 112) with a mean
responses tuned to temporal frequencies checks simultaneously, might resolve the screen luminance of 130 cd/m2 and a con-
well outside the adult range. Sokol2 sup- uncertainty. Hence, the purpose of this trast of 80 per cent. A single check size of
ported the notion that the temporal char- study was to determine the relationship 5.5 minutes of arc was used over an 8° x 7°
acteristics of the visual system of infants between visual acuity and VEP amplitude field at a working distance of 2.19 m. The
must be considered when investigating its for a 5.5 minutes of arc checkerboard tar- small 5.5 minutes of arc check encourages
spatial properties. The latencies of the get at three temporal frequencies. We also a response mainly from the central five
infant waves were found to be longer than considered it necessary to repeat the degrees of the visual field.7 The evoked
those of the adult but decrease with the Douthwaite and colleagues11 experiment potential was recorded on a Digitimer
passage of time and reach adult levels in order to establish whether their results D200 averager and signal analyser with the
around the age of six years. Douthwaite could only be applied to the television analysis time of 300 ms. The band pass fil-
and colleagues11 using a 5.5 minutes of arc stimulus or if the results apply equally well ters were set at 0.16 Hz and 300 Hz with a
check and three reversal rates (three, six to a moving mirror stimulus. sensitivity of 100 mv per volt. The active

Clinical and Experimental Optometry 82.6 November–December 1999


245
Acuity predictions from VEP Heravian, Douthwaite and Jenkins

electrode was positioned 2.5 cm above the to generate a VEP wave from a 5.5 minute Table 1 shows the mean and standard
inion on the midline and referenced to check stimulus. However, many of the sub- deviations of resolution (seconds of arc)
the centre of the forehead with a ground jects who took part in the experiment had and peak to trough amplitudes and the
electrode on the right of the forehead. An acuities of 6/6 or better and to allow a peak and trough latencies for all subjects
interelectrode impedance of five Kohm, better approximation of the regression, for three, six and 12 reversals per second.
or less, was acceptable. blurring lenses, +0.25 to +0.75 dioptre From this table, it can be seen that the
At the start of recording, the subject was were used to obtain a spread of acuities. mean amplitude for the three reversal rate
instructed to fixate the centre of the field The acuities at the lower end of the range is 5.32 mv and that for the six reversal rate
and to concentrate on keeping the pat- arose from subjects with a defocusing lens it is 5.06 mv. The 12 reversal rate has a
tern clear. All the recordings were mo- before the right eye only. Defocusing was mean amplitude of 4.25 mv which indi-
nocular, and the eye not being investi- achieved with spherical convex lenses cates a reduction in amplitude at this re-
gated was covered by a white septum which placed in the trail frame and the lenses versal rate (see Figure 1 waveform C). The
gave a homogenous field to the occluded were worn throughout the procedure absence of values for the latencies at 12
eye, and that adapted it to approximately described below. The lenses were carefully reversals per second is due to the fact that
the same light level as the eye under test. centred to avoid induced prismatic effects. the response at this temporal frequency
The occluder was placed as close to the is a steady-state response and in these con-
eye as possible. All subjects were volun- ditions the temporal measurements made
RESULTS
teers who were fully informed of the on the waves are those of a phase angle.
nature of the experiments. The pattern Figure 1 illustrates a typical set of results When considering the complete set of
reversal alternation rates used were three, from an individual subject with peak and results there seemed to be a wide range
six and 12 reversals per second (1.5, 3, 6 trough of interest labelled to indicate of amplitudes produced for any single
Hz). The extreme values were chosen to where the measurements were made. The acuity level. This variation in amplitude
give a definite transient and a definite VEP wave generally consisted of a nega- is due to a number of factors such as
steady-state VEP response, with the inter- tive trough N1 (peak latency around 88 ms), fatigue and concentration and will be
mediate temporal frequency lying in the followed by the positive peak P1 (122 ms), discussed later.
boundary region between the other two. followed by a negative trough N2 (172 ms). It was sometimes difficult to find the
Three runs of approximately 85 sweeps Figure 1 waveform A shows the VEP re- exact positions of peak-to-trough ampli-
were recorded to give a 256 sweep result. cording obtained with three reversals per tude for each wave, as some waves had very
The pattern reversal rate was changed at second. Figure 1 waveform B obtained at small signal/noise ratios. The smallest
this point and the procedure was re- six reversals per second and Figure 1 wave- amplitude obtained for the 12 reversal
peated. The three, six and 12 reversal rates form C is the 12 reversals per second wave- rate was 1.05 mv and was more likely to
examined in this work were selected in a form. The waveforms have been displaced be noise rather than a visual response. As
random sequence. vertically for clarity. Note the steady-state the high frequency signals contain the
The peak-to-trough amplitude (P1N2) C waveform in Figure 1. smallest amplitudes, these can be ex-
with the peak and trough latencies (P1 Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4 show pected to provide the least reliable
and N2) were measured for the 256 sweep the scatter plots for peak-to-trough ampli- estimates for acuity as is evident by the
waves. The P1N2 amplitudes were used in tudes against Landolt C acuities. The cor- large spread in their confidence limits.
this experiment because the negative relation coefficient for the three revers- The measured peak-to-trough latencies
trough has been observed to be sensitive als per second rate was r = -0.56 (p < 0.001) showed no significant correlation with
to contour density (detail size) and im- and the intercept on the X axis was 133 subjective visual acuity for all three
age clarity when using small checks.15,16 seconds of arc, which is equivalent to reversal rates.
This trough has a latency of around 150 6/13.3, the 95 per cent confidence limits
msec under these recording conditions. for the slope -0.76 < r < -0.27. For the six
DISCUSSION
reversals per second rate the correlation
coefficient was r = -0.40 (p < 0.01) with an The technique described in this paper
SUBJECTS
intercept on the X axis of 158 seconds of represents a rapid and convenient method
Thirty-three subjects were recruited; their arc equivalent to 6/15.8. The 95 per cent for predicting the corrected visual acuity
ages ranged from 19 to 38 years (median, confidence limits for this slope were -0.66 in an eye capable of near normal resolu-
23 years). The right eye from each sub- < r < -0.07. The correlation coefficient tion. It should provide a useful and easy
ject was used, after a precise refraction, for the 12 reversals per second rate was screening tool for amblyopia or for the
with the refractive correction worn r = -0.32 (p < 0.5) with an intercept on measurement of potential acuity.
throughout. The visual acuity range for the the X axis of 206 seconds of arc equiva- This work supports the findings of
group was from 6/3 to 6/9 (Landolt C), lent to 6/20.6. The 95 per cent confidence Jenkins and colleagues7 that the peak-to-
which was restricted to acuity levels likely limits for this slope were -0.60 < r < 0.02. trough amplitude measurement from a

Clinical and Experimental Optometry 82.6 November–December 1999


246
Acuity predictions from VEP Heravian, Douthwaite and Jenkins

Figure 2. The scatter plot of peak to trough amplitude Figure 3. The scatter plot of peak to trough amplitude (P1N2) against
(P1N2) against visual acuity at three reversals per second visual acuity at six reversals per second for 33 subjects. The equation of
for 33 subjects. A linear regression gives Y = -0.7 X + 9.32, the regression line is Y = -0.05 X + 8.03, where Y is the amplitude in mv
where Y is the amplitude in mv and X is the resolution in and X is the resolution in sec arc, r = -0.40.
sec arc with r = -0.56.

Figure 4. The scatter plot of peak to trough amplitude (P1N2) against visual
acuity at 12 reversals per second for 33 subjects. The equation of the regression
line is Y = -0.03 X + 6.25, where Y is the amplitude in mv and X is the resolution
in sec arc, r = -0.32.

Reversals Spatial acuity Standard Mean peak Standard Mean latency Standard Mean latency Standard
(per sec ) (sec arc ) deviation to trough deviation of positive deviation of negative deviation
amplitude (mv) peak (m sec) trough (m sec)
3 60.15 ±19 5.32 ± 2.25 122.5 ± 16 177.4 ± 18
6 5.06 ± 2.2 113.5 ± 14 175.4 ± 19
12 4.25 ± 2.0

Table 1. Mean and standard deviation of spatial resolution and VEP peak to trough amplitude (P1N2) and the peak and trough latency
for the 33 subjects at the three reversal rates

Clinical and Experimental Optometry 82.6 November–December 1999


247
Acuity predictions from VEP Heravian, Douthwaite and Jenkins

5.5 minute check can be used as an indi- quency mechanisms provide information a true assay of the acuity effect. They sug-
cator of subjective visual acuity for peo- only about motion or temporal change. gest that it may be unwise to infer that this
ple with acuities within the range 6/3 to Their experimental work demonstrated situation provides any useful information
6/9 (Landolt C). However, the results of that although these high temporal fre- for the prediction of corrected visual acu-
the experiment suggest that there was a quency detectors can also relay spatial in- ity where the optical image may be formed
lower correlation between the VEP wave formation, it was much poorer at spatial clearly on the retina of a visual system that
amplitude and subjective visual acuity discrimination than was the low temporal has reduced resolving ability. Obviously,
(r = - 0.56) for all three reversal rates com- frequency system. This finding is consist- such a relationship will only apply if the
pared to previous work.11 Douthwaite and ent with proposals that temporal modula- cause of acuity loss is optical in nature.
coworkers11 found much higher correla- tion is served by at least two distinct sets Studies show that VEP amplitudes de-
tions of r = - 0.81 for three and six reversal of mechanisms. It is likely that the VEP crease in an orderly fashion as retinal blur
rates with a lower correlation of r = - 0.54 waveforms obtained in this study at the increases up to three dioptres where a dif-
for the 12 reversal rates. Nevertheless, the slow flicker rates (three reversals per ferent relationship exists.15,24,25 Waveforms
same relation between the reversal rate second) reflect the high acuity sustained can still be detected in the presence of
and the acuity in the two studies still holds channels described above. large amounts of blur and these are due
true. Although there is some dispute about to the flicker component of the reversing
Douthwaite and coworkers11 and oth- the precise mechanism involved, it is con- pattern. However, the amount of blurring
ers17 suggested that the lower correlation ceivable that the poor correlation at the used in this experiment ranged from
to the faster reversal rate might be due to 12 reversals per second rate may be due +0.25 to +0.75 D and this is unlikely to
artefacts produced by the television screen to a significant contribution from the tran- alter amplitude significantly. Thus, the
such as unequal luminance, unequal sient system, which is less effective at the subjects in the experiment described
number of black and white checks, distor- slower reversal rate. Stromeyer and above are mixed in the sense that some
tion at the edge of the television screen coworkers21 suggested that there is a shift use blurring lenses representing optical
and different phases at the top and bot- from form to movement mechanisms defocus but others do not, and this would
tom of the screen.17 However, the present when the reversal rate is increased from have reduced the true correlation coeffi-
study used a moving mirror back projec- 5.4 reversals per second to 16 reversals per cient, consistent with our finding. It is also
tion system to eliminate the artefacts pro- second and this is evident in experiments possible that a number of other factors will
duced by a television system. Despite using reported by Panish and colleagues.22 They have affected the results. These include
this equipment, there was still a reduction showed that there is a dissociation in the noise, attention, accommodation, eye
in correlation for the 12 reversals per sec- processing of spatial and temporal detail movements, artefacts, electrode place-
ond stimulus. Therefore, it may be that as reversal rate increases. Thompson 23 ment and trend (an increase or decrease
this reduction in correlation is concerned proposed that a change occurs in the in amplitude with repeated measure-
with factors other than the stimulus limi- dominance of these two independent ments).26 Variability in amplitude between
tations in the earlier experiments. Towle channels and that this change becomes runs on the same occasion has been
and Harter 3 acquired a correlation coeffi- manifest at eight reversals per second. studied by Fagan and colleagues 26 They
cient of r = -0.86 when comparing their Thus, the reduction in the correlation concluded that the major contribution to
VEP acuity prediction with subjective coefficient between slow and fast reversal amplitude variability arises from noise,
visual acuity. Weiner and colleagues 18 rates in the present work is in agreement with sequential (trend) effects also mak-
found a value of r = -0.98 for the same with the psychophysical evidence. ing a significant contribution. Weiner and
relation. However, in both cases a wider However, although this cause will apply coworkers18 found that the amplitude and
spread of visual acuities was used and this to all studies, the correlations obtained for threshold of the wave can be altered by
will inevitably improve the correlation this study (r = -0.56) were considerably fatigue. The effect of this factor on our
coefficient where a linear relationship is lower than those (r = -0.81) obtained by readings has not been established.
present. Douthwaite and colleagues.11 The reason
There are physiological reasons, which for this could be due to the fact that the
CONCLUSION
may be responsible for the loss of correla- previous work used subjects with natural
tion with fast reversal rates. Kulikowski acuities ranging from 6/2.5 to 6/8.5, It has been found that the amplitude of a
and Tolhurst19 proposed the concept of whereas some of the subjects in this VEP waveform elicited by a 5.5 minutes
sustained and transient processing by the experiment were artificially blurred to of arc check at three reversals per second
visual system, in which spatial patterns are obtain a range of acuities from 6/3 to 6/9. correlates with the subjective visual acuity
considered to be processed by sustained According to Jenkins and coworkers7 the of people with relatively good spatial reso-
channels whereas the transient channels use of a blurring lens means that the in- lution and is consistent with the findings
process movement. Watson and Robson20 vestigation is concerned with the relation- of others.1,7,11 This correlation decreases
have argued that the high temporal fre- ship between the VEP and blur and is not at higher temporal frequencies and this

Clinical and Experimental Optometry 82.6 November–December 1999


248
Acuity predictions from VEP Heravian, Douthwaite and Jenkins

decrease is not confined to the use of a 14. Lovasik JV, Ahmedbhai N. Stimulus contami-
television system reversing checkerboard nants in visual electrophysiology. Amer J
Optom Physiol Opt 1985; 62: 334-343.
pattern stimulus. It may be due to a
15. Ellenberger C, Shuttlesworth DE. Electrical
change in the processing of the visual in- correlates of normal binocular vision. Arch
formation known to occur at these higher Neurol 1978; 35: 834-837.
temporal frequencies. We propose that 16. Harter MR, Seipk WH, Salmon LE. Evoked
such a technique can be used as a first cortical responses to dichoptically patterned
light flashes: interocular interaction. Life Sci
estimate of potential acuity in normal
1972; 2: 27-33.
observers. As a consequence, it would pro- 17. Faulkner DJ, Holden AL. Stimulus contami-
vide a useful screening test for amblyopia. nants in checkerboard patterns. Amer J
Optom Physiol Opt 1986; 63: 86-88.
REFERENCES 18. Wiener DE, Wellish K, Nelson JI,
1. Marg E, Freeman DN, Peltzman P, Goldstein Kupersmith MJ. Comparisons among Snel-
PJ. Visual acuity development in human len, psychophysical, and evoked potential
infants: evoked potential measurements. visual acuity determinations. Amer J Optom
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1976; 15: 150-153. Physiol Opt 1985; 62: 669-679.
2. Sokol S. Measurement of infant visual 19. Kulikowski JJ, Tolhurst DJ. Psychophysical
acuity from pattern reversal evoked evidence for sustained and transient detec-
potentials. Vision Res 1978; 18: 33-39. tors in human vision. J Physiol (Lond) 1973;
3. Towle VL, Harter MR. Objective determi- 232: 149-162.
nation of human visual acuity: pattern 20. Watson AB, Robson JG. Discrimination at
evoked potentials. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci threshold: labelled detectors in human
1977; 16: 1073-1076. vision. Vision Res 1981; 21: 1115-1122.
4. Skalka HW. Comparison of Snellen acuity, 21. Stromeyer CF, Madsen JC, Klein S, Zeevi YY.
VER acuity, and Arden grating scores in Movement - selective mechanisms in human
macular and optic nerve diseases. Brit J vision sensitive to high spatial frequencies.
Ophthalmol 1980; 64: 24-29. J Opt Soc Amer 1978; 68: 1002-1005.
5. Fagan JE, Yolton RL. Theoretical reliability 22. Panish SC, Swift DJ, Smith RA. Two crite-
of visual evoked response-based acuity rion threshold techniques: evidence for
determinations. Amer J Optom Physiol Opt separate spatial and temporal mechanisms?
1985; 62: 95-99. Vision Res 1983; 23: 1519-1525.
6. Jenkins TCA, Douthwaite WA. An objective 23. Thompson P. Discrimination of moving
VER assessment of visual acuity compared gratings at and above detection threshold.
with subjective measures. Amer J Optom Vision Res 1983; 23: 1533-1538.
Physiol Opt 1988; 65: 957-961. 24. Berman MS, Seki S. Blur-induced changes
7. Jenkins TCA, Douthwaite WA, Peedle JE. in the visual evoked potential. Amer J Optom
The VER as a predictor of normal visual Physiol Opt 1982; 59: 556-560.
acuity in the adult human eye. Ophthal 25. Katsumi O, Hirose T, Sakaue H, Mehta M,
Physiol Opt 1985; 5: 441-449. Rosenstein RB. Effect of optical defocus on
8. Odom JV, Hoyt CS, Marg E. Eye patching the steady state pattern reversal visual
and visual evoked potential acuity in chil- evoked response. Ophthal Res 1990; 22:
dren four months to eight years old. Amer J 383-390.
Optom Physiol Opt 1982; 59: 706-717. 26. Fagan JE, Allen RG, Yolton RL. Factors con-
9. Stadler G, Muller J. Determination of visual tributing to amplitude variability of the
acuity with pattern evoked cortical re- steady state visual evoked response. Amer J
sponses. Doc Ophthalmol Proc Ser 1982; 31: Optom Physiol Opt 1984; 61: 453-464.
437-442. 27. Heravian JS, Jenkins TCA, Douthwaite WA.
10. Shors TJ, Ary JP, Eriksen KJ, Wright KW. Binocular summation in visually evoked
P100 amplitude variability of the pattern responses and visual acuity. Ophthal Physiol
visual evoked potential. Electroencephalogr Opt 1990; 10: 257-261.
Clin Neurophysiol 1986; 65: 316-319.
11. Douthwaite WA, Jenkins TCA, Taylor AJ.
Author’s address:
Visual acuity prediction using the VER—
temporal aspects. Amer J Optom Physiol Opt Dr J Heravian Shandiz
1987; 64: 888-896. Department of Optometry
12. Douthwaite WA, Jenkins TCA. Amplitude Pirapezeshki Faculty
variability of transient visual evoked re- Mashhad University of Medical Sciences
sponse. Ophthal Physiol Opt 1988; 8: 221-226. Ahmadabad St
13. Regan D. Assessment of visual acuity by
evoked potential recording: ambiguity caused
Parastar Ave
by temporal dependence of spatial frequency Mashhad
selectivity. Vision Res 1978; 18: 439-443. IRAN

Clinical and Experimental Optometry 82.6 November–December 1999


249

You might also like