You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/5886344

Improving blood donor recruitment and retention: Integrating theoretical


advances from social and behavioral science research agendas

Article  in  Transfusion · December 2007


DOI: 10.1111/j.1537-2995.2007.01423.x · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
109 3,268

5 authors, including:

Eamonn Ferguson Christopher R France


University of Nottingham Ohio University
294 PUBLICATIONS   7,809 CITATIONS    249 PUBLICATIONS   6,001 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Charles Abraham Paschal Sheeran


University of Exeter University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
302 PUBLICATIONS   19,215 CITATIONS    255 PUBLICATIONS   24,151 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

PREPARE study: Developing and evaluating HIV risk reduction interventions among adolescents in South Africa View project

Psychopathy View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Paschal Sheeran on 15 November 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


BLOOD DONORS AND BLOOD COLLECTION

Improving blood donor recruitment and retention: integrating


theoretical advances from social and behavioral science
research agendas

Eamonn Ferguson,1 Christopher R. France,2 Charles Abraham,3 Blaine Ditto,4 and Paschal Sheeran5

U
nderstanding and facilitating the recruitment
BACKGROUND: Increasing blood donor recruitment and retention of blood donors is the major
and retention is of key importance to transfusion ser- contribution that the social and behavioral
vices. Research within the social and behavioral sciences can make to transfusion medicine.
science traditions has adopted separate but comple- Facilitating retention and recruitment is important given
mentary approaches to addressing these issues. This that the number of donors may not meet the need of the
article aims to review both of these types of literature, health services due to either reduction in the number of
examine theoretical developments, identify commonali- active donors in the United Kingdom1 and United States2
ties, and offer a means to integrate these within a or increased demand relative to a stable donor profile.3
single intervention approach. Whether or not a decline in donors or failure to meet
STUDY DESIGN AND METHODS: The social and demands best represents underlying trends, there is a
behavioral science literature on blood donor recruitment clear need to enhance recruitment and retention of blood
and retention focusing on theory, interventions, and donors. In general, the social science literature has
integration is reviewed. focused on recruitment, whereas the behavioral sciences
RESULTS: The role of emotional regulation (anticipated have focused on retention. Thus, although complemen-
anxiety and vasovagal reactions) is central to both the tary, these agendas developed separately. It is argued here
behavioral and the social science approaches to that the goal of producing successful interventions will be
enhancing donor motivation, yet although intentions
are the best predictor of donor behavior, interventions
targeting enactment of intentions have not been used
to increase donation. Implementation intentions (that
ABBREVIATIONS: AT = applied tension; FIDT = foot-in-the-
is, if-then plans formed in advance of acting) provide
door; NSF = negative state relief; TPB = theory of planned
a useful technique to integrate findings from social
behavior.
and behavioral sciences to increase donor recruitment
and retention. From 1Risk Analysis, Social Processes and Health (RASPH)
CONCLUSION: After reviewing the literature, imple- Group, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Not-
mentation intention formation is proposed as a tech- tingham, UK; 2the Department of Psychology, Ohio University,
nique to integrate the key findings and theories from the Athens, Ohio; 3the Department of Psychology, University of
behavioral and social science literature on blood donor Sussex, Falmer, Brighton, UK; 4the Department of Psychology,
recruitment and retention. McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; and the 5Depart-
ment of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK.
Address reprint requests to: Eamonn Ferguson, Risk Analy-
sis, Social Processes and Health (RASPH) Group, School of Psy-
chology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK; e-mail:
eamonn.ferguson@nottingham.ac.uk.
This review arose form a symposium organized by EF and
CRF at the 26th International Congress of Applied Psychology;
July 16-21, 2006; Athens, Greece.
Received for publication February 26, 2007; revision
received April 24, 2007, and accepted April 25, 2007.
doi: 10.1111/j.1537-2995.2007.01423.x
TRANSFUSION **;**:**-**.

Volume **, ** ** TRANSFUSION 1


FERGUSON ET AL.

greatly advanced by integrating recent theoretical determinant of action and that, intention is, in turn, deter-
advances from both of these perspectives. This review mined by attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behav-
briefly describes the social and behavioral science ioral control. Attitude refers to the person’s anticipation of
research agendas, key theoretical models from each, their consequences following from action and evaluation of
main findings, how they have informed interventions, and these consequences (i.e., what will happen if I go to
how they can be integrated to produce more powerful donate blood and how will I feel about those outcomes).
interventions. Specifically, it is argued that interventions Subjective norm represents perceived social approval of
based on implementation intentions4 that focus on man- an action or nonaction (i.e., how much will people who are
aging emotional reactions that precede and accompany important to me approve of my becoming a blood donor),
donation could bridge this literature, enhance existing and perceived behavioral control refers to the perception
recruitment techniques, and produce better specified and that the action is feasible and under the person’s own
more successful interventions to increase donor recruit- control (i.e., it is entirely up to me whether or not I become
ment and retention. a blood donor). Tests of the TPB indicate that it typically
accounts for between 39 and 42 percent of the variance in
intention and between 27 and 34 percent of the variance
TWO RESEARCH AGENDAS
in measures of behavior16,18-20 with intentions the stron-
The social and behavioral science agendas can be distin- gest predictor of blood donor returns.5
guished in terms of the questions they address and the Applications of the TPB to blood donation motiva-
measures they apply. The social sciences agenda examines tion have suggested that it provides a good core model
cognitive (e.g., perceived control), motivational (e.g., in this context. Table 1 provides a basic description of
moral norms), and emotional (e.g., anticipated affect: that the main TPB variables (intentions, subjective norms,
is, emotion expected to be experienced in the future such attitudes, and perceived behavioral control) with respect
as anxiety or pleasure) factors with respect to predicting to the prediction of either donor intentions or actual
donor behavior and intentions (see Ferguson,5 Piliavin behavior.6,8,9,20-26
and Callero,6 and Piliavin,7 for reviews). This work has It has been argued that different variables are impor-
examined both recruitment of new donors (e.g., Lemmens tant for novice and experienced donors.9 To examine this,
et al.8) and retention of regular donors.6,9 By contrast, the Table 1 is further divided into novice and experienced
behavioral science agenda focuses on experimental inter- donors as defined within each study.6,8,9,20-26 The main
ventions, with a particular emphasis on exploring the problem with this distinction is that there is no consensus
impact of direct interventions (e.g., water or caffeine about what constitutes novice and experienced donors.
preload, applied muscle tension) on the management of Some researchers defined novices as four or fewer dona-
emotional and physiologic responses (e.g., symptoms of tions, experienced as five or more,9 and other as those who
vasovagal syncope) during the donation experience and have never donated versus those who have donated at
how changes in these responses affect donor retention.10-13 some time in the past.22 As such, novices have been
These two research agendas have over the past three defined as a mixture of those who have never donated,8,22
decades followed separate lines of enquiry. The issues
relating to recruitment and retention, however, are inevi-
tably interlinked5,6,14,15 so that making more explicit theo- TABLE 1. TPB measures as predictors of
retical links between this literature should enhance the intentions and behavior as a function of levels of
donor prior history*
potential for intervention.
Donation level Intentions Behavior
Novice PBC22 Norms23
Attitudes6,8,22,23 Intentions6,9,23,26
THE SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH Self-efficacy8 PBC26
AGENDA Subjective norms8
Experienced PBC22 Intentions9,23,26
This work has been primarily correlational, examining Attitudes22-24 Past behavior9,23
how variables from theoretical models of motivation and Self-efficacy24 PBC26
self-regulation predict donor intentions or attendance. Subjective norms24
Three main models have been used: 1) the theory of Whole sample Attitudes20,21,23,25 Intentions20,21,23,25
Subjective norm20,21,25 Attitudes20,25
planned behavior (TPB), 2) stage models, and 3) motiva-
PBC21,25 Self-efficacy20,21
tion (altruism). Self-efficacy20,21 Subjective norm20
* Novice is a mixture of never donated, 8,22 one to two previous
donations,23 first-time donors,26 and up to four donations.9
Theory of Planned Behavior Experienced is a mixture of ever donated,22,24,26 three or more
donations,23 and five or more donations.9
The TPB16 is the most widely applied model of cognitive PBC = perceived behavioral control.
antecedents of action.17 It proposes that intention is a key

2 TRANSFUSION Volume **, ** **


DONOR RECRUITMENT AND RETENTION

made one to two previous donations,23 first-time donors,26 leagues8 found that attitude was a strong correlate of
and up to four donations.9 Experienced donors have been donation intentions explaining 24 percent of the variance
defined as a mixture of those who have ever donated,22,24,26 in intention scores and, moreover, that 45 percent of the
made three or more donations,23 or five or more dona- variance in donation attitudes could be explained by two
tions.9 Research needs to empirically resolve how novice beliefs, namely, the extent to which respondents believed
and experienced are defined. Within this context, one that blood and needles were frightening and the extent to
study27 showed that donors’ self-categorization indicated which they expected to feel nervous and tense before
a split into nondonors, first-time donors, occasional donation. These findings also highlight the importance of
donors (median of four donations), and experienced anticipated emotional responses to our understanding of
donors (median of five donations; see also Ferguson and donation motivation and suggest that campaigns should
Bibby9). Table 1 also contains studies that reported on probably not highlight the role of needles and blood in
a whole sample without dividing donors into different donation. For example, images of blood bags on red back-
categories. grounds, which have featured in some recruitment cam-
Examining Table 1 shows that the main difference in paigns, could be counterproductive because they prompt
predictors is between intention and behavior. (The lack of anticipated feelings found to undermine donation moti-
difference between novice and experienced donors may vation. Although reference to blood and the procedures is
be due to confounding in relation to the manner that dif- difficult to avoid when developing recruitment cam-
ferent studies define experienced and novice donors.) paigns, it is suggested that such campaigns could focus
Attitudes, perceived behavioral control, and subjective less on the paraphernalia of blood process and more on
norms predict intentions across experience, and intention donors themselves, for example, by reassuring potential
is the most consistent predictor of behavior. It has been donors that current donors do not feel nervous and tense
shown, however, that past behavior and not intention is before or after donation. Further experimental work is
a better predictor of future donations in experienced required to confirm the efficacy of focusing on these key
donors (five or more donations), with intention and not beliefs, but work of this type identifies potential cognitive
past behavior the best predictor in novice donors (four or targets for future recruitment campaigns.
less donations).9
Ajzen16 noted that the TPB could be extended if
further constructs were found to enhance the prediction Stage models
of intention or behavior. Research supports three key Stage models of self-regulation propose that people pass
modifications in the context of donor motivation. First, through a number of decision making stages as they
the construct of perceived behavioral control is closely attempt to change their behavior and that the transition
related to Bandura’s28 self-efficacy construct, which refers from one stage to the next is facilitated by different stage-
to one’s confidence in being able to successfully under- specific cognitive or behavioral processes.30 There is evi-
take a proposed action or sequence of action. Conner and dence that there may be qualitative differences not only
Sparks29 suggest that both perceived behavioral control between nondonors and donors, but also among donors
and self-efficacy should be measured when applying the with varying degrees of experience,6,9 and this has inspired
TPB and, interestingly, when the two constructs have been initial work on stages models of blood donation.27 A reli-
compared in the context of blood donation motivation able distinction has been drawn between occasional (one
(see Table 1), self-efficacy has been found to be a stronger to four donations) and regular (five or more donations)
correlate of intention.21 Certainly, self-efficacy should be donors.5,9 This work has particular relevance to issues of
included in models of the antecedents of blood donation. retention but is still very much in its infancy, and no inter-
Second, personal moral norms (i.e., feeling that one is ventions based on these distinctions have been con-
morally obliged to undertake a behavior) are important to ducted. These findings, however, indicate that donors
donation motivation. Both Armitage and Conner20 and should not be treated as a homogeneous group and that
Lemmens and coworkers8 found that measures of per- donor experience should be taken into account both in
sonal moral norm predicted donation intentions. Third, research and the evaluation of interventions.
the extent to which one identifies with donation or sees
donation as a compatible component of one’s identity
predicts donation.6,20,21 Thus an extended TPB for use in Motivation and/or altruism
blood donation contexts should include measures of self- Cross-sectional surveys of donor motivation indicate that
efficacy, moral norms, and blood donation identity. donors are motivated by altruistic motives and/or beliefs
Research into beliefs underlying attitudes toward (i.e., that donating will benefit society31-36), and many
donation is an important first step toward designing large-scale recruitment campaigns focus on altruism.
appropriately targeted motivational interventions. For Unfortunately, such findings may reflect a shared stereo-
example, in a study on nondonors, Lemmens and col- type of the blood donation as socially desirable rather

Volume **, ** ** TRANSFUSION 3


FERGUSON ET AL.

than an underlying motivational explanation. The lack of Paulhus and colleagues43 manipulated messages
prospective studies relating altruistic beliefs and motives given to donors at a donor drive, that either focused on the
to blood donation is of concern given the prominence societal duty (altruism) or personal benefits (blood
afforded to altruistic motives in national recruitment cam- typing). They showed that those exposed to the altruistic
paigns. For example, in the United Kingdom, the national message were more likely to intend to donate in the future.
television and radio campaigns focus on celebrities who Reich and associates44 exposed participants (first-time
relate how they or one of their relatives were helped by donors) to either patient-focused (altruistic) or donor-
donated blood. The accompanied by-line is “Do some- focused (egoistic) messages and found that the patient-
thing amazing. Give blood.” In the United States, the most focused message increased the number of donations.
recent campaign is based on a cartoon superhero—The Although these studies seem to support the utility of altru-
Red Defender—who states that he cannot save all lives by istic messages, their design or interpretation did not seem
himself and he needs our help in terms of donating blood. to take full account of theories of altruism. The altruism
These campaigns are predicated on the motivating power message of Paulhus and coworkers43 concerns social
of altruism. duty—not sacrificing oneself to help others—whereas the
Without denying the possibility of “pure” altruism, egoism message focuses on one aspect of egoism—
evidence indicates that helping behavior may be driven by hedonism (personal gain only). Theories of altruism and
egoistic and/or selfish concerns.37-40 Because human egoism would see these as polar extremes40 with other
cooperation is associated with activation of neurologic aspects of egoism characterized as combinations of these
reward centers,41 it is entirely plausible that blood dona- extremes (benevolence and kinship). Indeed, there is evi-
tion is driven by a sense of personal reward combined with dence that hedonism does not predict blood donor behav-
the desire to help—the so-called benevolent response42 ior.47 Moreover, “social duty” is closer to the concept of
—rather than pure altruism. moral norm than altruism, and evidence suggests that
moral norms predict donor intentions.8 Thus, the findings
of Paulhus and colleagues43 could be interpreted as
Summary support for the role of moral norms, and indirectly the
The brief review above is consistent with other reviews of TPB, rather than the motivating capacity of altruism.
the social science approaches to understanding donation Reich and coworkers44 manipulated patient focus
(e.g., Ferguson,5 Piliavin and Callero,6 and Piliavin7), indi- (altruism) with a story about a transplant patient in need
cating that models based on the TPB are useful in predict- of a blood transfusion. The donor-focused manipulation
ing donor intentions and that donor intentions predict emphasized donor self-esteem and the fact that the donor
donation (see Table 1). The review adds to the literature by will be given their blood type—a manipulation of hedo-
summarizing current advances with respect to extensions nism. The altruism manipulation was designed to create
to the TPB within the context of blood donation and high- empathy and emotional involvement within the recipient.
lighting the potential for stage models. This research also The effect for altruism can be explained via the negative
emphasizes that anticipated emotions (especially antici- state relief (NSR) model of altruism.37,38 That is, people
pated anxiety about donation) are also important to develop negative emotional reactions when considering
donors’ decision making. How has this basic research someone in need and help others to reduce this negative
been translated into clinical practice and interventions? affect and make themselves feel better. Thus, these results
can be interpreted as support for the role of egoism
because the mechanism underlying helping behavior
INTERVENTIONS BASED ON SOCIAL
described in the NSF implies that one’s own mood and/or
SCIENCE
affect regulation drives helping.
Interventions derived from the social science research
agenda can be grouped into four types (altruism, remind-
ers, motivational, and intention-based). Reminders and commitments
There is considerable and consistent evidence that
reminders have a positive effect on attendance and com-
Altruism-egoism pliance across a number of clinical settings.48 The same is
A number of studies have examined altruism with respect true of blood donation where reminders have been shown
to blood donation.43-46 Two of these are of particular inter- to be successful with respect to increasing donor
est as they compare manipulations of altruism and ego- recruitment.49-51 The mechanism by which reminders
ism.43,44 This distinction is important because it has been work remains unclear, however. One possible mechanism
suggested that blood donor behavior is driven by altruism might be that reminders heighten negative emotion and,
rather than egoism. Other studies have compared altru- consistent with NSR model of altruism, this emotion
ism to incentives45 or altruism to fear appeals.46 could motivate helping behavior.32 Another possibility is

4 TRANSFUSION Volume **, ** **


DONOR RECRUITMENT AND RETENTION

that reminders activate (increase the accessibility of) rel- providing a repeat donation, the experience of vasovagal
evant attitudes and intentions. Most reminders focus on reactions (e.g., dizziness, weakness, lightheadedness)
retention and are often sent out close to the time of the during the donation process is a particularly important
blood drive. Unfortunately, although interventions to impediment to retention of whole-blood donors.7,13,59-63
improve compliance with medical appointment keeping For example, in a retrospective survey of more than 30,000
have been systematically reviewed,48 there are no data on: blood donors,62 donors’ perception of physical well-being
1) how sustainable these effects are and 2) the type of during or after donation was the single strongest predictor
commitment (e.g., time, place, duration) is most effective. of intention to donate again in the future. Donors who
rated their physical well-being as “fair to poor” reported
an anticipated attrition rate that was six times higher than
Foot-in-the-door those who rated their well-being as “good to excellent.”
Related work has compared foot-in-the-door (FIDT, that Direct evidence of the importance of vasovagal reactions
is, ask for a small commitment first and, if agreed, then ask has also been obtained by comparing phlebotomist
for a larger one) to the door-in-the-face (that is, ask for a records of donor reactions to actual donor return behav-
big commitment, and when it is refused, then ask for a ior. For example, France and colleagues64 conducted a
smaller one) techniques. Evidence suggests that the FIDT large-scale, retrospective assessment of repeat donations
approach is more effective.52-54 The FIDT may work by for approximately 90,000 allogeneic blood donors and
engendering positive mood—people continue to donate demonstrated that phlebotomist ratings of reactions was a
to maintain this mood and avoid negative moods.55-57 The significant predictor of donor return. These findings are
central role for affect and/or mood suggested here is consistent with similar results obtained in other regions of
wholly consistent with the NSR model of helping.38 the United States.65,66 Because donor perceptions may be
as important, if not more important, than phlebotomist
ratings of vasovagal experiences, France and colleagues67
Intentions and response format examined the relationship between subjective reports of
Although intention to donate blood is the strongest pre- vasovagal reactions to blood donation in a sample of 1,052
dictor of donor behavior, only one study has manipulated Canadian blood donors. Consistent with the findings
intentions.58 This study explored how the response obtained from phlebotomist ratings, higher self-reports of
options to intentions questions affected behavior. Find- reactions were associated with a significantly lower likeli-
ings showed that participants who were invited to report hood of repeat donation. Interestingly, studies of both
their negative intention (not to donate blood) were more phlebotomist and donor ratings indicate that vasovagal
likely to make a subsequent donation compared to par- reactions have a deleterious influence on retention for
ticipants invited to report their positive intention (to both novice and experienced blood donors.64,67 These
donate), positive or negative intention, and various findings indicate that distress while donating in terms of
control conditions. These findings come about because feeling faint has deleterious effects on donor retention.
negative intention participants were liable to suspend Interestingly, recent work shows that vasovagal reactions
their donation decision (not respond to the question- also reduce return rates in donors who elect to undergo
naire) but subsequently came around to the idea and automated double red blood cell collections,68 suggesting
actually gave blood. that new collection technology does not eliminate the
deterrent effect of negative reactions. In sum, interven-
tions that can reduce vasovagal reactions are a key focus
Summary of the social science agenda for behavioral scientists working on blood donation.
Although intentions are the best predictor of blood donor
behavior, the focus of social science–based interventions
BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE INTERVENTIONS
has been on altruism, reminders, or FIDT. Across these
interventions the role of emotions and emotional regula- During blood donation, a variety of physiologic and psy-
tion appears to be a key mechanism and one that should chological challenges can operate in isolation or in com-
be considered in intervention development. bination to produce acute decreases in blood pressure.
Given a sufficient reduction in cerebral blood flow, the
donor will be susceptible to reactions that can range from
BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE AGENDA
mild dizziness to unconsciousness. In principle, strategies
This work has primarily examined how the experience that prevent acute decreases in blood pressure should
of negative reactions during the donation process may decrease the likelihood of unpleasant physical reactions
dissuade future donation and how different behavioral and intervention studies suggest that there are several
strategies can be applied to enhance donor retention. simple and cost-effective methods that can be used to
Although a number of factors can dissuade donors from reduce donor distress.

Volume **, ** ** TRANSFUSION 5


FERGUSON ET AL.

Distraction context of blood donation, recent studies have assessed


For many years, patients have been encouraged to divert the ability of this technique to prevent negative reactions
their attention from stressful medical procedures as a in blood donors. Ditto and colleagues84 showed inexperi-
means of reducing pain and distress. Empirical evidence enced blood donors an instructional video on muscle
suggests that many such diversions can have significant tension immediately before their donation. Compared
benefits. For example, patients undergoing dental restora- with a control group of inexperienced blood donors who
tion report less pain and discomfort when listening to did not watch the video, donors who learned the muscle
music69 or watching videos70 and less distress when tensing technique reported significantly lower levels of
playing video games.71,72 Video game play can also reduce subjective physiologic reactions to blood donation and
levels of distress and unpleasant treatment side effects in manifested reactions similar to those of more experienced
patients undergoing chemotherapy.73-75 In the context of donors. A subsequent, larger study of more than 1000
blood donation, Kaloupek and colleagues59,76,77 reported blood donors demonstrated that those who used muscle
that donors who engage in coping strategies that involve tension during donation 1) reported a significantly lower
either thinking about being elsewhere or explicitly trying levels of negative reactions, 2) were more likely to provide
to divert attention away from the donation procedures a full unit of blood, and 3) expressed greater confidence
experience less distress. Conversely, those who fail to that they would donate blood again in the future.85 From a
engage in avoidant coping behavior report a decreased physiologic perspective, the success of muscle tension
likelihood of making future donations.77 Based on these may be due to the ability of rhythmic contractions to
findings, a recent study examined audiovisual distraction increase the return of venous blood toward the heart by
as a potential method of reducing negative reactions in “milking” the large veins of the arms and legs. Thus, this
first-time blood donors.10 Because different people may procedure may offset the postural effects that can lead to
prefer different coping styles when confronting stressors, decreased blood pressure and blood flow to the brain.86
this study also compared donors who indicated that they Benefits of muscle tension may, for some donors,
typically cope with stress through avoidance strategies however, derive from the opportunity to engage in an
(e.g., turning away, preventing sight or sound of stressors, activity that helps distract the donor from the ongoing
preferring not to discuss the stressor) with donors who blood collection procedures. Thus, this technique may
typically prefer more vigilant coping styles (e.g., attending have both physiologic benefits (prevention of blood
to the stressor, seeking out additional information). pooling) and psychological benefits (reduction of anxiety
Results indicated that donors who prefer avoidant coping through distraction) that combine to reduce the risk of
strategies were less likely to experience negative reactions negative reactions.
when they watched a three-dimensional movie while
giving blood. Those who preferred vigilant coping were
neither helped nor harmed by watching the movie.10 Caffeine and water loading
Individual differences in coping have also been shown It has been suggested that donors at increased risk for
to moderate the effect of background music on donor reactions should drink beverages, possibly containing caf-
experience.78 Ferguson and colleagues78 observed that the feine, shortly before they donate.87 The suggestion to
presence of “easy listening” background music at the increase fluid intake is based on evidence that healthy
donation site is associated with a complex mix of detri- men and women show significant cardiovascular changes,
mental and beneficial effects upon donor mood and including increases in vascular constriction and blood
appraisals, depending on donation experience and vigi- pressure and arterial constriction, that peak approxi-
lance coping. In sum, additional research is required to mately 30 minutes after consuming water.88-90 These
further explore how distraction may combine with various changes have been shown to increase orthostatic toler-
donor characteristics to maximize satisfaction with the ance,89 suggesting that this effect may help compensate
donation experience and likelihood of repeat donation. for the reduction in blood volume that occurs when
healthy individuals move from a seated to upright posture.
To examine the effect of predonation hydration on the
Muscle tension experience of vasovagal reactions, Hanson and France91
Repeated, rhythmic contraction of major muscle groups asked healthy young blood donors to drink either 500 mL
of the arms and legs has been used for decades to treat of bottled water or no water approximately 30 minutes
fainting reactions in individuals with blood and injury before donation.91 Results revealed that acute hydration
phobia.79-82 Phobic individuals learn to make voluntary was associated with significantly fewer adverse reactions
muscular contractions when faced with feared stimuli, in both male and female donors. Further, there was no
and these actions can increase blood flow to the brain83 relationship between baseline total body water levels and
and effectively prevent faintness.80,81 Because the muscle reported reactions, suggesting that the benefit of water
tension technique is simple to learn and safe to use in the loading arises from acute rather than chronic physiologic

6 TRANSFUSION Volume **, ** **


DONOR RECRUITMENT AND RETENTION

effects of hydration. These findings have since been repli- off interventions such as applied tension (AT) are known
cated in a much larger sample of approximately 9000 high to be effective with respect to anticipatory emotions,
school donors.92 Predonation consumption of caffeine has donors may not use this technique every time. If donors
also been shown to be effective at reducing risk of reac- do not use AT each time they donate, they may experience
tions. With a randomized, double-blind design, Sauer and distress and may not return. Thus an important question
France13 had first-time blood donors drink 280 mL of for the behavioral sciences is: How do you get donors to
water and consume a pill that contained 0, 125, or 250 mg use AT every time they donate? Similarly, social science
of caffeine shortly before they donated for the first time. research indicates that intentions to donate predict dona-
Results indicated that predonation caffeine consumption tion but that such intentions may become derailed by
was associated with fewer severe reactions, fewer inter- anticipated anxiety about donation. How can we increase
ventions by phlebotomists for reactions, and a greater behaviors such as AT and also to help to manage antici-
willingness on the part of these first-time donors to give pated emotions associated with donation? One theoretical
blood again in the future. Further, these effects showed a development from the social sciences that meets these
dose–response relationship, with the highest dose leading requirements is the formation of implementation inten-
to the greatest benefit relative to placebo, and the 125-mg tions. Implementation intention formation is an effective
dose showing an intermediate effect. Another encourag- technique for increasing compliance.4
ing result was that even at the highest dose, equivalent to
approximately two to three cups of coffee, donors did not
report significantly higher levels of anxiety; this result is Implementation intentions
important because too much caffeine could increase Implementation intentions are if-then plans (“If it is situ-
anxiety and hence the likelihood of reactions. Although ation X, then I will do Y to reach goal Z”) that facilitate the
this study provided a direct test of caffeine’s ability to translation of goal intentions into action (e.g., help to
decrease vasovagal reactions, presumably via a pharma- ensure that one’s intention to donate blood results in
cologically mediated increase in resting blood pressure blood donation behavior). To form an implementation
levels, it is also possible that additional beneficial effects intention, the person should specify a good opportunity to
were obtained as a result of the small amount of water act in the if-part of the plan, and specify a response that
consumed by all donors. moves one toward the goal in the then-part of the plan.
Thus, the goal intention of getting screened for cervical
cancer (“I intend to obtain screening”) could be furnished
Summary by an implementation intention that specifies when,
where, and how to make an appointment with one’s
The above evidence clearly shows that simple behavioral
medical practitioner (e.g., “If I have finished coffee on
techniques can reduce distress and fainting in donors and
Monday at 11:10 am then I will phone the surgery.”94). By
in so doing increase the likelihood of return donations.
forming an if-then plan, the person delegates control of
behavior from the self to the situational cues specified in
INTEGRATING THE AGENDAS: MANAGING the plan; when the person subsequently encounters these
EMOTIONAL REACTIONS WITH cues, the behavior is elicited automatically. Considerable
IMPLEMENTATION INTENTION FORMATION evidence attests to the idea that forming implementation
intentions increases attendance for medical screening
Emotions and intentions and promotes performance of health behaviors compared
Two constructs, 1) emotional responding (anticipated to merely forming relevant goal intentions.4 Techniques
anxieties, vasovagal reactions) and 2) intentions to for forming implementation intention are fairly straight-
donate, emerge from the above brief review as key to forward. Participants state their goal intention (i.e., to
improving donor recruitment and retention. donate blood) and then prompting them to state where
Throughout this review we have referred to antici- and when the will undertake particular subgoals associ-
pated emotions and experienced vasovagal reactions. ated behavior (such as drinking water before donation).
Loewenstein and associates93 have highlighted how the Gollwitzer and Sheeran4 argue that goal attainment (in
distinction between anticipatory emotions (immediate this case, donating blood) can be blocked by three key
visceral reactions, in this case vasovagal reactions) and the self-regulatory problems: 1) failing to get started, 2)
anticipated emotions (those not experienced in the imme- getting derailed (behaviors and feelings that hinder an
diate environment but expected to be experienced in the ongoing behavioral goal), and 3) not calling a halt (trying
future, in this case, anxiety) is centrally important to deci- to continue in the face of disappointing feedback).
sion making. As such, both need to be considered. The Forming appropriate implementation intentions to help
behavioral sciences have examined anticipatory emotions to resolve Problems 1 to 3 should facilitate positive dona-
and the social sciences anticipated emotions. While one- tion experiences.

Volume **, ** ** TRANSFUSION 7


FERGUSON ET AL.

The successful application of this technique depends

If I feel faint after donation, then I will tell myself this


I will not worry about this and see it as something

I will not worry about this and see it as something


then I will change my diet, in preparation to make

then I will change my diet, in preparation to make


crucially on identifying critical subgoals that will facilitate

If I am unable to donate after the anemia test, then

If I am unable to donate after the anemia test, then


If I feel faint after donation, then I will tell myself
that happens from time to time! If I am anemic

that happens from time to time! If I am anemic


performance.4 For example, careful pilot work needs to be
conducted with representative subsamples of donors at
different stages of the donor career cycle5 or who have
experienced different types of donation experience

Not calling a halt


(feeling faint, deferred). Appropriate specification and
phrasing of the implementation intentions can be under-
taken on the basis of this work. If this stage of intervention

a donation in the future!

a donation in the future!


development is not conducted properly, the resultant
intervention will be unlikely to succeed. Examples of some

this is normal!
types of implementation that might be considered are
described in the next section. It needs to be emphasized,

is normal.
however, that these are only examples; identifying the
most effective for implementation requires pilot work
focusing on the real opportunities and impediments
facing donors.

drink a cup or coffee or a large glass of water and

If I am donating blood then I will use muscle tension


use breathing and imagery exercises to stay calm
If I am going to give blood, then before I go I will
Implementation intentions and donor recruitment

TABLE 2. Example implementation intentions


and retention
Donor recruitment. The social science literature
shows that goal intentions are the strongest predictor of

Getting derailed
attendance5 but also that fear of negative emotional con-

and relaxation techniques!


sequence acts as an inhibitor. Both the social and the
behavioral science data demonstrate that negative
emotion experience reduces the likelihood of donor
returns and that this is especially the case for first time
donors.65,68 The behavioral science data also show that and relaxed!

drinking coffee before donation reduces the chances of


feeling faint and having a negative emotional experi-
ence.13 There is also evidence that convenience and time
management are important predictors of donor behav-
ior.5 Thus, to aid recruitment and manage the donation
then I will ensure that I have enough time to do so

then I will ensure that I have enough time to do so

experience, three implementation intentions should be


If it is the day I have decided to go and give blood,

If it is the day I have decided to go and give blood,


and arranged any necessary child care or cover

and arranged any necessary child care or cover

formed: 1) one that focuses on scheduling (getting


started), 2) one that focuses on coffee intake (getting
derailed), and 3) one that focuses on managing any
negative emotional experiences or consequence of tem-
porary deferral (not calling a halt). Table 2 provides
Getting started

some examples of the types of implementation inten-


tions that could be formed by donors to achieve each of
these.
These donors are likely to be first-time donors and
could also be taught applied muscle tension techniques.
National advertising campaigns could not just ask people
needed!

needed!

to donate but ask them to think about how, where, and


when to donate to get them to form an if-then plan.
Potential donors could also be asked to form an imple-
mentation intention around preparing for their donation
by drinking sufficient fluid such as coffee and using
Recruitment
Donor cycle

relaxation if they feel anxious. Donors should also be told


Retention

not merely to consider donation as an act that helps


others, but to consider also what they might get out of it
themselves in terms of a sense of personal achievement.

8 TRANSFUSION Volume **, ** **


DONOR RECRUITMENT AND RETENTION

Implementation intentions,
specific to deferral reason

BEHAVIORAL AND SOCIAL Deferral


SOCIAL COGNITION SOLUTIONS
BARRIERS
SOLUTIONS
TRADITIONAL
Hydration
- Fear of fainting Donation without reactions
- Fear of fainting + implementation intentions
Reminders - Scheduling
- Scheduling
- Motivation
- Motivation Applied Tension
+ implementation intentions Vasovagal reaction

Implementation intentions,
specific to contributors to
vasovagal reactions

RECRUITMENT PERIOD ARRIVAL AT DONATION SITE DONATION

Fig. 1. Donor cycle and the integration of traditional and new social science and behavioral science interventions.

Donor retention. The work from the behavioral sci- SUMMARY AND AGENDA FOR FUTURE
ences shows that techniques such as AT can reduce symp- RESEARCH
toms of feeling faint and increase goal intentions to make
This review highlights the utility of integrating social
future donations,85 as can preloading with water91 and caf-
science and behavioral science research agendas within
feine.13 Implementation intentions offer a technique that
transfusion medicine to increase the recruitment and
enables continual use of techniques like AT. Once AT has
retention of blood donors. Although this literature has
been demonstrated and used, the donor can be requested
developed separately, their integration is timely and
to form an implementation intentions about the use of AT
worthwhile. Most important, this review points to several
along with an implementation intention to donate next
possibilities for integrative interventions that can, and
time. The donation implementation plan would focus on
should, be tested in future studies. In particular, experi-
when and where they would next donate whereas the
mental tests of the application of implementation inten-
focus of the AT plan would be on when and where to
tion prompts to promote scheduling and instruction in AT
perform AT (e.g., from registration through to recovery in
and predonation hydration in donor recruitment and
the donation location). These if-then plans could be made
retention materials are warranted.
at the end of a donor session as well as being part of the
reminder letters that are sent out. In this way, the power of
a simple behavioral technique can be strengthened with a
social science technique that ensures that the behavioral
technique is used every time, and at the right time, that
REFERENCES
people donate. Implementation intentions for scheduling, 1. Ferriman A. Decline in altruism threatens blood supplies.
feeling faint, and deferral would also be pertinent to this BMJ 1998;317:1405.
group. 2. Simon TL. Where have all the donors gone? A personal
reflection on the crisis in America’s volunteer blood
program. Transfusion 2003;42:273-9.
3. Sullivan MT, Cotton R, Read EJ, Wallace EL. Blood collec-
Augmenting existing techniques tion and transfusion in the United States in 2001. Transfu-
Figure 1 provides a schematic representation of the donor sion 2007;47:385-94.
cycle from recruitment, to donation and/or deferral, to 4. Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P. Implementation intentions and
retention. As can be seen in Figure 1, social science tech- goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes.
niques (e.g., implementation intentions) and behavioral Adv Exp Soc Psychol 2006;38:69-119.
science interventions (e.g., AT) may be used at different 5. Ferguson E. Predictors of future behaviour: a review of the
points in the cycle to promote donor participation and psychological literature on blood donation. Br J Health
retention. Psychol 1996;1:287-308.

Volume **, ** ** TRANSFUSION 9


FERGUSON ET AL.

6. Piliavin JA, Callero P. Giving blood: the development of an soned action in the prediction of repeated behavior. Soc
altruistic identity. Baltimore John Hopkins University Psychol Q 1988;51:303-17.
Press; 1991. 24. France JL, France CR, Himawan LK. A path analysis of
7. Piliavin JA. Why do they give the gift of life: a review of intention to redonate among experienced blood donors:
research on blood donors since 1977. Transfusion 1990;30: an extention of the theory of planned behavior. Transfu-
444-59. sion 2007;47:1006-13.
8. Lemmens KP, Abraham C, Hoekstra T, Rutter RA, De Kort 25. Giles M, Cairns E. Blood donation and Ajzen’s theory of
WL, Brug J, Schaalma HP. Why don’t young people volun- planned behaviour: an examination of perceived behav-
teer to give blood? An investigation of the correlates of ioural control. Br J Soc Psychol 1995;34:173-88.
donation intentions among young nondonors. Transfusion 26. Godin G, Conner M, Sheeran P, Belanger-Gravel A,
2005;45:945-55. Germain M. Determinants of repeated donation among
9. Ferguson E, Bibby PA. Predicting future blood donor new and experienced donors. Transfusion 2007;47:000-0.
returns: past behavior, intentions and observer effects. 27. Ferguson E, Chandler S. A stage model of blood donor
Health Psychol 2002;21:513-8. behaviour: assessing voluntary behaviour. J Health Psychol
10. Bonk VA, France CR, Taylor BK. Distraction reduces self- 2005;10:359-72.
reported physiological reactions to blood donation on 28. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York:
novice donors with a blunting coping style. Psychosom Freeman; 1997.
Med 2001;63:447-52. 29. Conner M, Sparks P. Theory of planned behaviour and
11. Ditto B, France CR, Lavoie P, Roussos M, Adler SJ. Reduc- health behaviour. In: Conner M, Norman P, editors.
ing reactions to blood donation with applied muscle Predicting health behaviour. Berkshire: Open University
tension: a randomized controlled trial. Transfusion 2003; Press; 2005.
43:1269-75. 30. Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC, Norcross JC. In search of
12. Ditto B, France CR. The effects of applied tension on how people change: applications to addictive behaviors.
symptoms of French-speaking blood donors: a randomized Am Psychol 1992;47:1102-14.
trial. Health Psychol 2006;25:433-7. 31. Glynn SA, Klienman SH, Schreiber GB, Zuck T, McCombs
13. Sauer LA, France CR. Caffeine attenuates vasovagal reac- S, Bethel J, Garratty G, Williams AE. Motivations to donate
tions in female first-time donors. Health Psychol 1999;18: blood: demographic comparisons. Transfusion 2002;42:
403-9. 216-21.
14. James R, Matthews D. The donation cycle: a framework for 32. Misje AH, Bosnes V, Gasdal O, Heier HE. Motivation,
the measurement and analysis of blood donor and return recruitment and retention of voluntary non-remunerated
behavior. Vox Sang 1993;64:47-2. blood donors: a survey based questionnaire study. Vox
15. Lowe KC, Ferguson E. Benefit and risk perceptions in Sang 2005;89:236-44.
transfusion medicine: blood and blood substitutes. J Int 33. Oswalt RM, Gordon M. Motivations of blood donors and
Med 2003;253:498-507. nondonors. J Appl Psychol 1993;59:122-4.
16. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Org Behav Hum 34. Suarez IM, Fernandez-Montoya A, Fernandez AR, et al.
Dec Process 1991;50:179-211. How regular blood donors explain their behaviour. Trans-
17. Ajzen I. Nature operation attitudes. Annu Rev Psychol fusion 2004;44:1441-6.
2001;52:27-58. 35. Titmuss RM. The gift relationship: from human blood to
18. Armitage CJ, Conner M. Efficacy of the theory of planned social policy. New York: Pantheon; 1971.
behaviour: a meta-analytic review. Br J Soc Psychol 2001; 36. Trimmel M, Lattacher H, Janda M. Voluntary whole-blood
40:471-99. donors, and compensated platelet donors and plasma
19. Godin G, Kok G. The theory of planned behavior: a review donors: motivation to donate, altruism and aggression.
of its applications to health related behaviors. Am J Health Transfus Apher Sci 2005;33:147-55.
Promot 1996;11:87-98. 37. Baumann DJ, Cialdini RB, Kenrick DT. Altruism as
20. Armitage CJ, Conner M. Social cognitive determinants of hedonism: helping and self-gratification as equivalent
blood donation. J Appl Soc Psychol 2001;31:1431-57. responses. J Pers Soc Psychol 1981;40:1039-46.
21. Giles M, McClenahan C, Cairns E, Mallet J. An application 38. Maner JK, Luce CL, Neuberg SL, Cialdini RB, Brown S,
of the theory of planned behaviour to blood donation: Sagarin BJ. The effects of perspective taking on motiva-
the importance of self-efficacy. Health Educ Res 2004;19: tions for helping: still no evidence for altruism. Pers Soc
380-91. Psychol Bull 2002;11:1601-10.
22. Godin G, Sheeran P, Conner M, Germain M, Blondeau D, 39. Rachlin H. Altruism and selfishness. Behav Brain Sci 2002;
Gagne C, Beaulieu D, Naccache H. Factors explaining the 25:239-96.
intentions to give blood among the general population. 40. Sober E, Wilson DS. Unto others: the evolution and psy-
Vox Sang 2005;89:140-9. chology of unselfish behaviour. London: Harvard Univer-
23. Charng HW, Piliavin JA, Callero PL. Role identity and rea- sity Press; 1998.

10 TRANSFUSION Volume **, ** **


DONOR RECRUITMENT AND RETENTION

41. Fehr E, Rockenbach B. Human altruism: economic, neural 60. Ogata H, Iinuma N, Nagashima K, Akabane T. Vasovagal
and evolutionary perspectives. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2004; reactions in blood donors. Transfusion 1980;20:679-83.
14:1-7. 61. Staallekker LA, Stammeijer RN, Dudok de Wit C. A Dutch
42. Nunney L. Group selection, altruism, and structured-deme blood bank and its donors. Transfusion 1980;20:66-70.
models. Am Naturalist 1985;126:212-30. 62. Thomson RA, Bethel J, Lo AY, Ownby HE, Nass CC,
43. Paulhus DL, Shaffer DR, Downing LL. Effects of making Williams AE. Retention of “safe” blood donors. The Retrovi-
blood donation motive salient upon donor retention: rus Epidemiol Donor Study. Transfusion 1998;38:359-67.
a field experiment. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1977;3:99-102. 63. Trouern-Trend JJ, Cable RG, Badon SJ, Newman BH, Pop-
44. Reich P, Roberst P, Laabs N, Chinn A, McEvoy P, Hirschler ovsky MA. A case-controlled multicenter study of vasovagal
N, Murphy EL. A randomized trial of blood donor recruit- reactions in blood donors: influence of sex, age, donation
ment strategies. Transfusion 2006;46:1090-6. status, weight, blood pressure, and pulse. Transfusion
45. Royse D. Exploring ways to retain first-time volunteer 1999;39:316-20.
blood donors. Res Soc Work Pract 1999;9:76-85. 64. France CR, Rader A, Carlson B. Donors who react may not
46. Ferrar JR, Leippe MR. Noncompliance with persuasive come back: analysis of repeat donation as a function of
appeals form a prosocial, altruistic act: blood donation. phlebotomist ratings of vasovagal reactions. Transfus
J Appl Soc Psychol 1992;22:83-101. Apher Sci 2005;33:99-106.
47. Ferguson E, Farrell K. Egoistic not altruistic motives predict 65. Gorlin JB, Petersen J. Reactions in first-time high school
blood donation [abstract]. Proc Br Psychol Soc 2005;13:67. blood donors. Transfusion 2004;44:463.
48. Macharia WM, Leon G, Rowe BH, Stephenson BJ, Haynes 66. Newman BH, Ahmad R, Newman DT. Reply to letter.
RB. An overview of interventions to improve compliance Transfusion 2004;44:463.
with appointment keeping for medical services. JAMA 67. France CR, France JL, Roussos M, Ditto B. Mild reactions
1992;267:1813-7. to blood donation predict a decreased likelihood of donor
49. Lipsitz A, Kallmeyer K, Ferguson M, Abas A. Counting on return. Transfus Apher Sci 2004;30:17-22.
blood donors: increasing the impact of reminder calls. 68. Rader AW, France CR, Carlson B. Donor retention as a
J Appl Soc Psychol 1989;19:1057-67. function of donor reaction to whole blood and automated
50. Pittman TS, Pallak MS, Riggs MS, Gotay CC. Increasing double red cell collections. Transfusion 2007;47:in press.
blood donor pledge fulfillment. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1981; 69. Anderson R, Baron RS, Logan H. Distraction, control, and
7:195-200. dental stress. J Appl Soc Psychol 1991;21:156-71.
51. Ferrari JR, Barone RC, Jason LA, Rose T. The effects of a 70. Seyrek SK, Corah NL, Pace LF. Comparison of three dis-
personal phone call prompt on blood donor commitment. traction techniques in reducing stress in dental patients.
J Commun Psychol 1985;13:295-8. J Am Dent Assoc 1984;108:327-9.
52. Cialdini RB, Ascani K. Test of concession procedures for 71. Corah NL, Gale EN, Illig SJ. Psychological stress reduction
inducing verbal, behavioral and further compliance with during dental procedures. J Dent Res 1979;58:1347-51.
a request to give blood. J Appl Psychol 1976;61:295-300. 72. Corah NL, Gale EN, Illig SJ. The use of relaxation and
53. Foss RD, Dempsey CB. Blood donation and the foot-in- distraction to reduce psychological stress during dental
the-door technique: a limiting case. J Pers Soc Psychol procedures. J Am Dent Assoc 1979;98:390-4.
1979;37:580-90. 73. Kolko DJ, Rickard-Figueroa JL. Effects of video games on
54. Hayes TJ, Dwyer FR, Greenwalt TJ, Coe NA. A comparison the adverse corollaries of chemotherapy in pediatric oncol-
of two behavioral influence techniques for improving ogy patients: a single-case analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol
blood donor recruitment. Transfusion 1984;24:399-403. 1985;53:223-8.
55. Gorassini DR, Olson JM. Does elf-perception change 74. Redd WH, Jacobsen PB, Die-Trill M, Dermatis H, McEvoy
explain the foot-in-the-door effect? J Pers Soc Psychol M, Holland JC. Cognitive/attentional distraction in the
1995;69:91-105. control of conditioned nausea in pediatric cancer patients
56. O’Keefe DJ, Figge M. A guilt-based explanation of the door- receiving chemotherapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987;55:
in-the-face influence strategy. Hum Commun Res 1997;24: 391-5.
64-81. 75. Vasterling J, Jenkins RA, Tope DM, Burish TG. Cognitive
57. Burger JM, Guadagno RE. Self-concept clarity and the foot- distraction and relaxation training for the side effects due
in-the-door procedure. Basic Appl Soc Psychol 2003;25:79- to cancer chemotherapy. J Behav Med 1993;16:65-80.
86. 76. Kaloupek DG, Scott JR, Khatami V. Assessment of coping
58. Cioffi D, Garner R. The effects of response options on deci- strategies associated with syncope in blood donors. J Psy-
sions and subsequent behavior: sometimes inaction is chosom Res 1985;29:207-14.
better. Per Soc Psychol Bull 1998;24:463-72. 77. Kaloupek DG, White H, Wong M. Multiple assessment of
59. Kaloupek DG, Stoupakis T. Coping with a stressful medical coping strategies used by volunteer blood donors: implica-
procedure: further investigation with volunteer blood tions for preparatory training. J Behav Med 1984;7:
donors. J Behav Med 1985;8:131-48. 35-60.

Volume **, ** ** TRANSFUSION 11


FERGUSON ET AL.

78. Ferguson E, Singh A, Cunningham-Snell N. Stress and cerebral tissue oxygenation and cerebral artery blood
blood donation: effects of music and previous donation velocity. Stroke 2001;32:1546-51.
experience. Br J Psychol 1997;88:277-94. 87. Kasprisin DO, Glynn SH, Taylor F, Miller KA. Moderate and
79. Kozak MJ, Montgomery GK. Multimodal behavioral treat- severe reactions in blood donors. Transfusion 1992;32:23-6.
ment of recurrent injury-scene-elicited fainting (vasode- 88. Routledge HC, Chowdhary S, Coote JH, Townend JN.
pressor syncope). Behav Psychother 1981;9:316-21. Cardiac vagal response to water ingestion in normal
80. Ost LG, Fellenius J, Sterner U. Applied tension, exposure in human subjects. Clin Sci 2002;103:157-62.
vivo, and tension-only in the treatment of blood phobia. 89. Schroeder C, Bush VE, Norcliffe LJ, Luft FC, Tank J, Jordan
Behav Res Ther 1991;29:561-74. J, Hainsworth R. Water drinking acutely improves orthos-
81. Ost LG, Sterner U. Applied tension: a specific behavioral tatic tolerance in healthy subjects. Circulation 2002;106:
method for treatment of blood phobia. Behav Res Ther 2806-11.
1987;25:25-9. 90. Scott EM, Greenwood JP, Gilbey SG, Stoker JB, Mary DA.
82. Ost LG, Sterner U, Fellenius J. Applied tension, applied Water ingestion increases sympathetic vasoconstrictor
relaxation, and the combination in the treatment of blood discharge in normal human subjects. Clin Sci 2001;100:
phobia. Behav Res Ther 1989;27:109-21. 335-42.
83. Foulds J, Wiedmann K, Patterson J, Brooks N. The effects of 91. Hanson SA, France CR. Predonation water ingestion
muscle tension on cerebral circulation in blood-phobic attenuates negative reactions to blood donation. Transfu-
and non-phobic subjects. Behav Res Ther 1990;28:481-6. sion 2004;44:924-8.
84. Ditto B, Wilkins JA, France CR, Lavoie P, Adler PS. On-site 92. Newman BH, Tommolino E, Andreozzi C, Joychan S,
training in applied muscle tension to reduce vasovagal Pocedic J, Herringhausen J. The effect of a 16-oz. water
reactions to blood donation. J Behav Med 2003;26: drink on blood donor reaction rates in high-school
53-65. students: two independent studies combined. Transfusion
85. Ditto B, France CR, Lavoie P, Roussos M, Adler PS. Reduc- 2006;46:S80A.
ing reactions to blood donation with applied muscle 93. Loewenstein GF, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as
tension: a randomized controlled trial. Transfusion 2003; feelings. Psychol Bull 2001;127:267-86.
43:1269-75. 94. Sheeran P, Orbell S. Using implementation intentions to
86. van Lieshout JJ, Pott F, Madsen PL, van Goudoever J, increase attendance for cervical cancer screening. Health
Secher NH. Muscle tensing during standing: effects on Psychol 2000;19:283-9.

12 TRANSFUSION Volume **, ** **

View publication stats

You might also like