You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Sports Sciences

ISSN: 0264-0414 (Print) 1466-447X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

Motivational priming as a strategy for maximising


exercise outcomes: effects on exercise goals and
engagement

Christian Magaraggia, James Dimmock & Ben Jackson

To cite this article: Christian Magaraggia, James Dimmock & Ben Jackson (2014) Motivational
priming as a strategy for maximising exercise outcomes: effects on exercise goals and
engagement, Journal of Sports Sciences, 32:9, 826-835, DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2013.862841

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.862841

Published online: 10 Jan 2014.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 820

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rjsp20

Download by: [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] Date: 10 November 2017, At: 04:41
Journal of Sports Sciences, 2014
Vol. 32, No. 9, 826–835, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.862841

Motivational priming as a strategy for maximising exercise outcomes:


effects on exercise goals and engagement

CHRISTIAN MAGARAGGIA, JAMES DIMMOCK, & BEN JACKSON

School of Sport Science, Exercise and Health, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia

(Accepted 3 November 2013)


Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

Abstract
Using a scrambled sentence priming protocol, the first aim of this study was to investigate whether differences in novel
exercise-related goals existed between participants primed with motivational or non-motivational material (i.e. autonomous,
controlled or neutral primes). The second aim was to explore whether an indirect effect was present between priming
condition and the goal-related exercise sessions that individuals performed over the week following administration of the
prime. No effects were observed across priming conditions with respect to subjective vitality, goal concordance and the
frequency with which participants planned to exercise. However, autonomy-primed individuals set goals for their exercise
sessions that were significantly longer in intended duration (M = 57.72 min, SD = 37.42) than those set by their counter-
parts in both the controlled-prime condition (M = 44.12 min, SD = 27.78) and the neutral-prime condition
(M = 37.10 min, SD = 20.47). Bootstrapped analyses also revealed a significant indirect relationship between prime and
exercise behaviour, with the autonomy prime predicting longer goal-based exercise sessions, via the effect on the duration of
participants’ intended exercise sessions. These findings highlight the potential influence that priming autonomous motiva-
tion may have on individuals’ exercise aspirations, as well as the way in which primes may indirectly shape exercise
engagement.

Keywords: goal-setting, undergraduate, self-determination theory, goal concordance, intentions, priming

Research over the past few decades has repeatedly as autonomy and control) can give rise to beha-
demonstrated that behaviour and evaluations can be vioural outcomes.
“primed” (e.g. Carver, Ganellen, Froming, & Autonomous motivation reflects volitional invol-
Chambers, 1983). In other words, the mere percep- vement in an activity for self-determined reasons
tion of environmental stimuli can influence social (e.g. interest, enjoyment) and is characterised by a
behaviour and judgement without a person’s aware- perception of choice over one’s activities (Deci &
ness of this influence. In one seminal priming inves- Ryan, 2000). Controlled motivation, on the other
tigation, for example, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows hand, is generally associated with pressure that is
(1996) exposed a treatment group of participants to either internally or externally imposed (Ryan &
elderly related words, such as “old” and “wrinkle”, Deci, 2000). That being the case, when individuals
and found that individuals in this treatment group undertake an activity due to autonomous (rather
subsequently walked slower at the “completion” of than controlled) motives, behaviour will be perceived
the experiment than those in a control condition. as an energising and need fulfilling experience (Ryan
These findings suggest that perception of stimuli & Frederick, 1997; Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996).
associated with social groups can influence our beha- Likewise, the pursuit of goals that are consonant
viour in line with those groups. Aside from stereo- with internal core values (i.e. self-concordant) can
type-related priming, an abundance of evidence has satisfy basic psychological needs, as well as result in
indicated that goals and motivation may also be acti- higher levels of goal attainment and greater well-
vated and pursued automatically (e.g. Friedman, being (Sheldon, 2002; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999).
Deci, Elliot, Moller, & Aarts, 2010; Hodgins, In recent research, our laboratory group (Banting,
Yacko, & Gottlieb, 2006), and findings by Dimmock, & Grove, 2011) primed undergraduate
Levesque and Pelletier (2003) indicate that the prim- students in a cycling class with autonomous motiva-
ing of even complex motivational regulations (such tion, controlled motivation or no particular

Correspondence: James Dimmock, School of Sport Science, Exercise and Health, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia.
E-mail: james.dimmock@uwa.edu.au

© 2013 Taylor & Francis


Motivational priming and goal-setting 827

motivation. In each condition, the priming material between intention and behaviour is minimised
involved a task in which participants were required (Ajzen, 2011). From a conceptual perspective,
to unscramble grammatically incorrect sentences there are compelling reasons to suggest that primes
(e.g. “feel are interested I usually” (autonomous); may influence conscious deliberation about an activ-
“feel are pressured I usually” (controlled); “book ity (e.g. goal-setting). In their dual-process model,
we the read top” (neutral)). This task was performed for instance, Strack and Deutsch (2004) proposed
while participants rode on stationary bicycles at a that the fast, automatic impulsive system influences
university fitness centre, and it was advertised as an “the differential accessibility of information about
investigation being conducted by the English behavioral options or about relevant aspects of the
Department at the University. Banting et al. discov- alternatives” (p. 230), which can bias reflective (i.e.
ered that participants primed with autonomous more conscious) operations. The work by Holland,
motivation experienced the cycling as more enjoy- Hendriks, and Aarts (2005) has also corroborated
able relative to controlled-primed participants. Also, the effects of environmental stimuli on goal activa-
autonomous-primed participants cycled for signifi- tion and subsequently planned behaviour.
cantly longer, at a greater percentage of their max- Participants in their treatment group, who had been
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

imum heart rate, and provided lower ratings of exposed to the scent of a cleaning product, were
perceived exertion, relative to their controlled- subsequently more likely to plan activities related to
primed counterparts. Other research has shown cleaning than participants in a control group. In a
that these motivational orientations can be primed similar work by Custers and Aarts (2007), partici-
via more naturalistic methods (than scrambled sen- pants who had been primed with the goal of being
tences), such as by acting in accordance with a par- “well-groomed” subsequently displayed a greater
ticular orientation (e.g. Friedman et al., 2010). likelihood of writing about neatness when asked to
Consequently, motivational priming, particularly write down their ideal or preferred physical appear-
with respect to autonomous motivation, could be ance characteristics. Taken together, these findings
effective as a strategic tool for use in goal-setting indicate that the mere perception of goal-related
programmes. stimuli can shape conscious and reflective goal-
related processes, even without participants’ aware-
ness of the influence.
The influence of primes on downstream con-
scious processing
The present study
Key findings in the study by Banting et al. (2011)
have already been discussed, but one other finding in It is possible that motivational primes offer an oppor-
their study is particularly noteworthy. More specifi- tunity for shaping individuals’ exercise-related
cally, Banting et al. discovered that, relative to the aspirations. However, researchers are yet to deter-
autonomy-primed and neutral-primed groups, the mine the extent to which abstract motivational
participants primed with controlled motivation dis- primes can influence one’s overt exercise goals.
played a significantly lower intention to exercise in Our research group (Banting et al., 2011) provided
the near future. Evidence is therefore emerging to preliminary evidence that primes reflecting autono-
demonstrate that motivational priming can yield mous or controlled motivation can influence exercise
overt effects, and that these effects might include intentions, but, despite these encouraging findings, a
changes to conscious intentions. formal goal-setting procedure was not utilised in this
Although a wealth of studies have documented investigation. Moreover, the participants were
non-conscious priming effects (e.g. Bargh et al., primed while they were concurrently undertaking
1996; Levesque & Pelletier, 2003), it is now exercise, and they utilised a generic two-item mea-
acknowledged that research in this field needs to sure of intention with responses on Likert scales.
broaden its scope to include higher-order, or “sec- Consequently, information about intended fre-
ond-generation”, questions (Bargh, 2006). One such quency and duration of exercise sessions could not
question relates to whether, and how, primes might be gleaned from these items.
interact with more conscious, deliberate ongoing Using an undergraduate cohort, the present study
processes (e.g. one’s stated intentions or goals; was designed to address these issues. Although the
Bargh, 2006). There is evidence to indicate that transition from high school to undergraduate studies
goal activation and pursuit can occur without con- is regularly accompanied by reductions in physical
scious intervention (see Bargh, 1990), but there also activity engagement (Pullman et al., 2009), as well as
remain many instances where consciousness is an one’s overall physical health and psychological well-
important regulator of behaviour. Overt intentions, being (e.g. Bray & Kwan, 2006; Jung, Bray, &
for example, often account for substantial variance in Martin Ginis, 2008), effective strategies for promot-
behaviour, particularly when the temporal disparity ing physical activity levels among newly enrolled
828 C. Magaraggia et al.

undergraduates are lacking (Bray & Born, 2004). Experimental design and procedures
That being the case, first-year undergraduates that
Having received ethical approval to conduct the
were in their first semester at university were
study, students were informed in a lecture that they
recruited to participate in the investigation, with the
could earn course credit in return for their participa-
aim of examining whether goals may support an
tion in two studies investigating comprehension abil-
indirect pathway between primes and subsequent
ity (study 1) and the types of exercise goals that
exercise behaviour. Relative to participants in the
undergraduates set (study 2). Although the studies
neutral- and controlled-prime groups, it was
were described as being entirely separate, the stu-
expected that autonomy-primed participants would
dents were informed that the investigations would be
create more self-concordant (see Sheldon & Elliot,
performed back-to-back in an upcoming tutorial. In
1999) exercise goals that were also reflective of
this lecture, two separate information sheets and
greater intended exercise engagement (i.e. higher
consent forms were distributed (reinforcing the
exercise duration and frequency), and that partici-
bogus cover story that two studies were being con-
pants setting goals under this condition would
ducted). One information sheet explained that the
experience higher subjective vitality (cf. Custers &
Faculty of English and Cultural Studies was collect-
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

Aarts, 2007; Holland et al., 2005). It was also


ing normative data regarding comprehension ability
expected that individuals’ exercise goals would pre-
among university students. The second information
dict their subsequent exercise behaviour (cf. Ajzen,
sheet contained a basic overview of the goal-setting
2011). Overall, then, the aim of this study was to test
talk and goal-setting exercise, and outlined that after
if participants primed with autonomous, controlled
the talk students would be invited to set a new
or neutral material subsequently set different exer-
exercise goal for the following week. At this time,
cise-related goals, felt differently about those goals
students who consented to participate in the projects
and then subsequently acted on their exercise
completed a measure assessing their exercise-based
intentions.
behavioural regulation and initial levels of physical
activity (see the following section).
Method
Experimental session. Approximately two weeks later,
Participants
the participants were seated in a lecture room with a
A total of 137 first-year undergraduates were space between each student to minimise possible
recruited from degree programmes within the distraction and/or any cooperation on the tasks.
Faculty of Life and Physical Sciences at the authors’ The participants were then presented with a 10-min
institution. Major fields of study for participants talk relating to general recommendations for setting
included physiology, biology, psychology, physics, effective goals using information that was obtained
kinesiology and chemistry. Two participants were from a recognised undergraduate kinesiology text
removed as they reported suspicion that the (i.e. Weinberg & Gould, 2011). This talk made no
scrambled sentence task (i.e. priming procedure) reference to, nor did it include, any material that
may have influenced their responses on the goal- may have been associated with either controlled or
setting task. The remaining sample of 135 autonomous motivational orientations. Importantly,
(Mage = 18.64, SD = 2.11, nmale = 55, nfemale = 78, though, the participants were informed at the end of
nunknown = 2) completed the first stage of data collec- this talk that (a) including variety within one’s exer-
tion. Out of the 135 eligible participants who com- cise regime (in terms of activity modality) has been
pleted the initial session, 99 participants (nmale = 39, shown to support exercise maintenance, and (b)
nfemale = 60), with a mean age of 18.42 years focusing on a novel activity provides exercisers with
(SD = 2.00), responded to the follow-up question- an opportunity to incorporate all of the goal-setting
naire at time 2 (i.e. 73.33% retention rate). A one- recommendations that were included in the talk.
way MANOVA was performed to check for potential This was an important component of the design
background differences between those who did and because novel exercise goals would provide a more
did not provide complete data across both time robust test of goal adherence (i.e. avoiding the influ-
points, with background (i.e. motivation score, base- ence of other forms of physical activity on results),
line activity levels and age) and time 1 (i.e. intended and evidence indicates that adolescent students are
goal frequency and duration, subjective vitality and likely to be open-minded in relation to setting novel
goal concordance) indices as dependent variables. exercise goals (e.g. Eime, Payne, Casey, & Harvey,
A non-significant multivariate effect emerged 2010). After a short break of 5 min, the students
(F7,99 = 1.64, P = .13, η2P = .10, λ = .90), indicating were invited back into the auditorium and were
that “completers” and “dropouts” displayed no dif- asked to begin “study 1”, which consisted of a “com-
ferences on these variables. prehension” (i.e. scrambled sentence) task. The
Motivational priming and goal-setting 829

participants received one of three scrambled sen- ended questions about (a) whether they saw a link
tence tasks that had been designed to prime either between studies 1 and 2, and (b) whether they felt
autonomous motivation, controlled motivation or no their judgements or behaviours in study 2 were influ-
particular motivation. The dissemination of these enced by anything that occurred before the study.
tasks across participants was entirely random (i.e.
prior to their arrival, scrambled sentence sheets had
been randomly ordered in a pile, before being dis- Measures
tributed one-by-one among the students upon
Motivational orientation. Participants’ motivational
entry). Respondents were given 5 min to complete
orientations were assessed using the Behavioral
as much of their scrambled sentence task as possible.
Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire-2 (BREQ-2;
As the scrambled sentence materials were col-
Markland & Tobin, 2004; Mullan, Markland, &
lected, the experimenter simultaneously gave the
Ingeldew, 1997), which comprises 19 items that are
participants a goal-setting (“study 2”) survey. To
rated on a five-point response scale anchored at 0
reinforce the separation between prime and depen-
(not true for me) and 4 (very true for me), and is
dent variables, the experimenter thanked the partici-
designed to assess levels of amotivation, external
pants for completing the “first” comprehension
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

regulation, introjected regulation, identified regula-


study at this time. The participants were then asked
tion and intrinsic motivation. Evidence in support of
to begin the goal-setting (i.e. “second”) study, with
the structural properties of BREQ-2 has been
particular emphasis on setting an exercise goal that
reported previously (Markland & Tobin, 2004),
“they are not currently pursuing but desired to com-
and in the current study, the intrinsic (α = .91),
plete over the course of the following week”. The
identified (α = .78), introjected (α = .81), external
questionnaire (comprising goal-setting task, suspi-
(α = .75) and amotivation (α = .72) subscales all
cion probe and other instruments; see the following
displayed adequate reliability. Scores on these sub-
section) took approximately 15 min to complete,
scales can be used to form a composite relative
after which time the participants were dismissed
autonomy index (RAI) to indicate the extent of indi-
from the laboratory. One week later, the participants
viduals’ autonomous (relative to controlled) regula-
were contacted via email and provided with a written
tion for exercise behaviour (Markland & Ingledew,
copy of their goal (as a reminder), and were then
2007). Prior to computing RAI scores, however, it is
asked to report the number and duration of goal-
necessary to demonstrate that subscale inter-correla-
related exercise sessions that they had performed
tions conform to a simplex pattern (e.g. Markland &
over the week. In a subsequent lecture, all students
Ingledew, 2007) (see Table I for BREQ-2 subscale
were debriefed as to the true nature of the
correlations). In this investigation, intrinsic motiva-
investigation.
tion displayed an acceptable pattern of correlations
with identified, introjected and external regulation.
Priming manipulation. A scrambled sentence task was
Identified regulation was also correlated as antici-
used to administer the prime, and a total of 20
pated with introjected and external regulation, and
sentences were included for each group. The sen-
the association between introjected and external sub-
tences for the autonomous and controlled motiva-
scales supported the expected pattern. Correlations
tion primes were guided by those used in previous
between amotivation scores and external regulation,
investigations (e.g. Banting et al., 2011; Hodgins
introjected regulation, identified regulation and
et al., 2006). To avoid arousing suspicion, a consis-
intrinsic motivation, however, did not conform to
tent number of identical neutral sentences (i.e. one
the forecasted simplex hypothesis. As a result, the
in every four) were included in the autonomous and
amotivation subscale was excluded when computing
controlled priming tasks. In all conditions, only four
RAI scores in this study, and the following RAI
words out of five were required to construct a gram-
calculation formula was used in line with the
matically correct sentence. Examples of the
procedures outlined by Mullan and Markland
scrambled sentences included “options have I two
and” (autonomous, i.e. “I have two options”), “this
do us must we” (controlled, i.e. “we must do this”) Table I. BREQ-2 subscale inter-correlations.
and “book we the read top” (neutral, i.e. “we read
Variable 2 3 4 5
the book”). All scrambled sentence material can be
found in the Appendix. A priming awareness (i.e. Intrinsic motivation .66*** .28** −.35*** −.30**
suspicion) probe was included in order to detect Identified motivation – .53*** −.10* −.53***
whether participants were aware as to the true pur- Introjected regulation – .14 −.08
External regulation – .06
pose of the scrambled sentence task. As per the Amotivation –
suspicion probe used in Dimmock, Jackson, and
Clarke (2013), the participants were asked open- Note: * P < .05, ** P < .01, *** P < .001.
830 C. Magaraggia et al.

(1997): (–2 × external regulation) + (–1 × introjected identified regulation and intrinsic motivation scores
regulation) + (1 × identified regulation) + (2 × intrin- and subtracting the introjected and external regula-
sic motivation). Higher scores on the RAI indicate a tion scores (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Accordingly, a
more autonomous (relative to controlled) regulation. higher goal concordance score reflects goals that are
valued and are pursued primarily due to personal
Exercise goal. The participants were asked to formu- interest (Sheldon, 2002).
late and record a new exercise-related goal for the
following week, and to structure their goal around Exercise behaviour. Initial exercise behaviour data
exercise that they had not undertaken in the previous were obtained by asking the participants to report
month. This instruction ensured that the partici- how many hours of physical activity they engaged in
pants did not list an exercise goal relating to an per week. These baseline levels of exercise were
activity in which they may already be engaged. The measured for use in preliminary analyses. One
participants reported how many exercise sessions week after completing the main procedure and set-
that they intended to complete as part of their exer- ting their new exercise goal, the participants were
cise goal, and they were also asked to record the contacted via email and asked to report the modality
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

intended duration of these sessions (in minutes). and frequency (i.e. number) of goal-related exercise
Finally, the modality of the exercise goal (running, sessions they had completed over the last week, as
walking and swimming) was also ascertained. well as the duration (i.e. minutes) of each of these
sessions. The participants were asked to only report
Subjective vitality scale. The subjective vitality scale the exercise that they had completed which was
(Ryan & Frederick, 1997) was used to assess the directly related to the goal they had set the previous
degree to which the participants felt energised/vita- week and were asked to exclude any exercise that
lised after the goal-setting procedure. The partici- they had performed which was unrelated to their
pants completed an instrument consisting of seven goal. Responses to these questions were encouraged
items rated on a seven-point Likert scale ranging in an open-ended format (i.e. after writing down
from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true), with items their exercise modalities, the participants noted
such as, “At this time, I have energy and spirit” and how many times they undertook each form of exer-
“I feel energised right now”. Bostic, Rubio, and cise and how long they spent in each session). In
Hood (2000), however, presented evidence that a line with previous research investigating exercise
six-item measure (which excluded one reverse- behaviour (Ishikawa-Takata, Ohta, & Tanaka,
scored item from the original instrument) was super- 2003; Juneau et al., 1987), and in order to ensure
ior to the original; therefore, the six-item version was concordance between goal- and activity-related
used for all analyses in the current study. This measures, exercise duration (minutes per exercise
six-item measure displayed acceptable reliability session) and frequency (sessions per week) were
(α = .91) in this investigation. included as measures of exercise engagement.
Notably, these measures have been recognised as
Goal concordance rating. Goal concordance was mea- important predictors of health outcomes associated
sured by replicating the method used by Sheldon with physical activity (Ballor & Keesey, 1991;
and Elliot (1999), which involved participants rating Shephard, 1968).
their reasons for pursuing each goal on a nine-point
Likert scale anchored at 1 (not at all for this reason)
and 9 (completely for this reason). Items in this instru- Results
ment correspond to one of four behavioural regula-
Preliminary analyses
tions, namely, external, introjected, identified and
intrinsic (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Items included, Descriptive statistics for background and dependent
“you pursue this striving because somebody else variables are displayed in Table II, and correlations
wants you to or because the situation demands it” between all variables are presented in Table III. A
(external regulation), “you pursue this striving MANOVA was used to explore whether background
because you would feel ashamed, guilty or anxious differences existed on participants’ self-reported
if you didn’t” (introjected regulation), “you pursue motivational orientations (i.e. RAI scores derived
this goal because you really believe it’s an important from BREQ-2) and baseline activity levels (i.e. time
goal to have” (identified regulation) and “you pursue spent participating in sport/exercise each week)
this striving because of the fun and enjoyment that it according to priming condition. Analyses conducted
provides you” (intrinsic regulation). A single goal on time 1 data revealed that there was no multi-
concordance index was created by weighing the variate effect (F4,208 = .86, P = .49, η2P = .01, λ =
four behavioural regulations (see Sheldon & Elliot, .97), indicating that there were no background dif-
1999 for a full description), before summing the ferences between those in the three priming
Motivational priming and goal-setting 831

Table II. Descriptive statistics according to priming condition.

Autonomy Controlled Neutral

M SD M SD M SD

Background variables
RAI score 5.95 2.86 5.36 3.33 5.78 2.59
Physical activity (hours/week) 5.88 3.19 5.86 3.50 5.10 2.46
Dependent variables
Exercise goal frequency 3.74 2.02 4.14 1.76 3.28 1.48
Exercise goal duration 57.72 37.42 44.12 27.78 37.10 20.47
Goal concordance 1.66 1.20 1.47 1.36 1.55 1.22
Subjective vitality 2.91 1.28 3.04 1.33 3.17 1.23
Actual exercise frequency (no. of sessions) 3.05 1.95 2.75 1.40 2.58 2.30
Actual exercise duration (minutes/session) 51.77 29.87 41.31 30.68 35.00 25.71

Notes: Higher RAI scores denote relatively more autonomous (versus controlled) motives for exercise participation. Exercise goal frequency
and duration reflect intended number of exercise sessions and intended duration of each of those sessions (over the coming seven days).
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

Higher goal concordance score reflects goals that are valued and pursued primarily due to personal interest. Subjective vitality measured 1 to
7, where higher scores denote more positive perceptions. Actual exercise frequency and duration represent the number of goal-related
exercise sessions participated in during the week following administration of the prime, as well as the average duration of each of the sessions.

Table III. Correlation matrix for all background and dependent variables.

Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

RAI score .12 .03 .02 .44*** .34*** .01 .09


Physical activity – .12 .29* .07 .45*** .18 .31**
Exercise goal frequency – −.04 .20* .11 .37*** .08
Exercise goal duration – .06 .15 .17 .64***
Goal concordance – .33*** .09 .17
Subjective vitality – .09 .14
Actual exercise frequency (no. of sessions) – .14
Actual exercise duration (minutes/session) –

Note: * P < .05, ** P < .01, *** P < .001.

conditions (parenthetically, this non-significant mul- the dependent variables. A Bonferroni correction
tivariate effect was also apparent when examining (i.e. α = .0125) was used to interpret significance at
only those who provided time 2 data, F4,176 = .78, the univariate level for this analysis. The analysis
P = .54, η2P = .01, λ = .96). This indicated that revealed a statistically significant multivariate effect
students in the different conditions did not display for priming condition (F8,256 = 2.50, P = .012, η2P =
differences on exercise-related motivational regula- .073, λ = .86). A significant main effect was found
tions or the average time they spent participating in for intended goal duration (F2,131 = 5.54, P = .005,
sport/exercise each week. Subsequently, a η2P = .08); however, no significant main effects were
MANOVA was also used to determine whether found for intended goal frequency (F2,131 = 2.53,
intended goal duration, intended goal frequency, P = .08, η2P = .04), intended goal concordance
goal concordance and subjective vitality differed (F2,131 = .27, P = .76, η2P = .004) or subjective
according to gender. This analysis was also non-sig- vitality (F2,131 = .42, P = .66, η2P = .006). In relation
nificant (F4, 127 = .97, P = .43, η2P = .03, λ = .97), to intended goal duration (see Figure 1), autonomy-
and as a result, gender was not included as a covari- primed participants planned to exercise for signifi-
ate or independent variable in subsequent analyses. cantly longer within each session (M = 57.72 min,
SD = 37.42) than their controlled- (M = 44.12 min,
SD = 27.78; Mean diff = 13.60, SE = 6.17, P = .03)
and neutral-primed counterparts (M = 37.10 min,
Main analyses
SD = 20.47; Mean diff = 20.63, SE = 6.36, P =
Exercise goals. A MANOVA was conducted, in which .001). There were no significant intended goal dura-
priming condition was entered as the independent tion differences between those in the controlled- and
variable, with intended goal frequency, intended goal neutral-primed groups (Mean diff = 7.02, SE = 6.39,
duration, goal concordance and subjective vitality as P = .27).
832 C. Magaraggia et al.

indicate that there was a significant indirect prospec-


60.00 tive pathway from prime to goal-based exercise ses-
sion duration, via the duration of participants’
Mean of intended goal duration (minutes)

planned goal-based sessions.


40.00

Discussion
These results indicated that the presentation of an
20.00
autonomous (relative to controlled and neutral)
motivational prime may influence the characteristics
of individuals’ exercise goals, at least in terms of
0.00 intended goal duration. Importantly, the goals that
Autonomy Control Neutral
Condition were set by the participants in the study were pre-
dictive of exercisers’ subsequent behavioural pat-
Figure 1. Means and standard error for goal duration across
priming conditions.
terns (i.e. the length of exercise sessions carried
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

out). Findings from the study therefore corroborate


and extend results from other research in which
Exercise behaviour. To test for the indirect effect motivational primes have influenced consciously
between the prime and subsequent exercise session planned goals (e.g. Banting et al., 2011). In an
duration (via intended goal duration), bootstrap- attempt to explain the effects in the present study,
ping-based analyses were performed using the it is noteworthy that scrambled sentence primes for
INDIRECT macro for SPSS, developed by abstract motivational orientations are “action-neu-
Preacher and Hayes (2008). This procedure tests tral” and are not themselves associated with beha-
each of the individual pathways specified within the vioural representations. In other words, unlike
indirect model, as well as providing estimates and primes that reflect specific goals (e.g. to memorise),
significance levels of total, direct and indirect effects the primes presented in this study reflect broad and
for the predictor on the outcome. Duration of goal- abstract constructs with more distal links to beha-
based exercise sessions was entered as the dependent viour. Consequently, it is likely that the primes in
variable, intended goal duration was entered as the this study were used to interpret an individual’s
proposed indirect mechanism and priming condition current circumstances, which may have been respon-
was entered as a categorical independent variable sible for the observed responses on goal content (see
(coded autonomy = 1, controlled = 2, neutral = 3). Dijksterhuis, Preston, Wegner, & Aarts, 2008).
The bootstrap analysis used 5000 samples to build In terms of goal-related influences, it was interest-
non-parametric sampling distributions of the ing that the intended duration, but not the intended
hypothesised indirect effect, and the indirect effect frequency, of exercise sessions differed between
is considered to be significant (using α = .05) if the priming conditions. This priming effect is particu-
95% confidence interval excludes zero. larly noteworthy given that no differences existed
Analyses indicated that the relationship between between the participants in terms of their exercise-
priming condition and intended goal duration was related motivational regulation (i.e. before the
significant (b = –12.10, SE = 4.02, t98 = –3.01, P = administration of the prime), and the specific influ-
.003), insofar as an autonomous prime was predic- ence on goal duration might reflect the greater per-
tive of greater intended session duration. There was ceived malleability of exercise session duration in
also a significant predictive effect between intended comparison to exercise frequency. In fact, goal dura-
goal duration and subsequent exercise session dura- tion is often considered the most controllable of the
tion (b = .56, SE= .07, t98 = 7.50, P < .001), which three components of exercise (i.e. intensity, duration
indicated that greater duration intentions were and frequency) (e.g. Courneya & McAuley, 1994). It
related to greater actual session duration. Taken is possible that logistical constraints, such as study or
together, these predictors accounted for 39.24% of work commitments, are more likely to impinge on
the variance in actual exercise session duration changes to goal frequency than they are to goal
(F2, 95 = 32.38, P < .001). Most importantly, the duration. In other words, autonomy-primed partici-
bootstrapped total indirect effect from prime to ses- pants may have been more likely to seek to extend
sion duration (through intended goal duration) was each of their planned exercise sessions, as opposed
also significant (estimate = –6.80, SE = 2.31, to attempting to schedule additional sessions into
95% bias corrected and accelerated confidence inter- their weekly routine. Somewhat less intuitive,
val –12.11, –2.85), and the overall normal theory test though, was our finding that priming groups did
associated with the indirect effect was significant not differ in terms of subjective vitality or goal con-
(z = –2.81, P = .005). Collectively, these findings cordance perceptions. One possible explanation for
Motivational priming and goal-setting 833

this lack of effect is that in the current study all exercise goals, which may subsequently align with
experimental groups may have felt external pressures desirable behavioural outcomes. To the extent that
to create an exercise goal due to the nature of the this influence is non-conscious, clients are unlikely
experimental manipulation, and perceived little voli- to deem the efforts of the practitioner as manipula-
tion over the scope of the goal-setting (e.g. Judge, tive or divisive.
Bono, Erez, & Locke, 2005). Future work is encour-
aged to overcome this problem by investigating the
impact of primes in self-selected goal-setting
programmes. References
Countless studies have supported the notion that Ajzen, I. (2011). The theory of planned behavior: Reactions and
goals influence behaviour, but what is important to reflections. Psychology and Health, 26, 1113–1127.
establish is whether goals set under priming condi- Ballor, D. L., & Keesey, R. E. (1991). A meta-analysis of the
tions retain their motivational force. This study pro- factors affecting exercise-induced changes in body mass, fat
mass and fat-free mass in males and females. International
vided indirect evidence to suggest that prime- Journal of Obesity, 15, 717–726.
influenced goals still predict behaviour. This finding Banting, L. K., Dimmock, J. A., & Grove, J. R. (2011). The
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

was particularly noteworthy given that no differences impact of automatically activated motivation on exercise-
were apparent between priming groups in terms of related outcomes. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33,
baseline exercise behaviour, and is particularly rele- 569–585.
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of
vant for this population in light of the decrements in social interaction. In T. E. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),
physical activity and health status that are often Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social beha-
observed during the transition from high school to vior (Vol. 2, pp. 93–130). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
undergraduate studies (e.g. Bray & Kwan, 2006). Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years?
Although it is not clear whether these findings may On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of non-con-
scious social behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology,
generalise across other populations (e.g. high school 36, 147–168.
students) and contexts (e.g. community samples), Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of
the indirect prospective effect that emerged provides social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype
encouraging evidence for the role of priming-based activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
manipulations in promoting exercise-related out- 71, 230–244.
Bostic, T. J., Rubio, M. D., & Hood, M. (2000). A validation of
comes among a first-year undergraduate cohort. the Subjective Vitality Scale using structural equation model-
One limitation of the current study, however, was ling. Social Indicators Research, 52, 313–324.
the self-report nature of the exercise engagement Bray, S. R., & Born, H. A. (2004). Transition to university and
variable, and in future, researchers are encouraged vigorous physical activity: Implications for health and psycho-
to consider the extent to which these primes may logical well-being. Journal of American College Health, 52, 181–
188.
indirectly support physical activity when assessed Bray, S. R., & Kwan, M. Y. W. (2006). Physical activity is asso-
using more objective (e.g. accelerometer-based) ciated with better health and psychological well-being during
methods. Also, an assessment was not made in the transition to university life. Journal of American College Health,
present study about the extent to which the new 55, 77–82.
goal-related exercise activities operated as substitutes Carver, C. S., Ganellen, R. J., Froming, W. J., & Chambers, W.
(1983). Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibil-
or as additional activities to participants’ normal ity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 403–421.
exercise behaviour. Research on naturalistic primes, Courneya, K. S., & McAuley, E. (1994). Are there different
another area of inquiry described by Bargh (2006) as determinants of the frequency, intensity, and duration of phy-
an important “second-generation” issue, also repre- sical activity? Behavioral Medicine, 20, 84–91.
sents a worthwhile extension to this work. As Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2007). Goal-discrepant situations prime
goal-directed actions if goals are temporarily or chronically
opposed to the use of scrambled-sentence tasks, accessible. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 623–
creating an autonomous or controlled learning envir- 633.
onment in a laboratory prior to a goal-setting activity Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motiva-
might be one “naturalistic” way to investigate this tions: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary
issue. Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67.
Dijksterhuis, A., Preston, J., Wegner, D. M., & Aarts, H. (2008).
Health promotion practitioners often lament the Effects of subliminal priming of self and god on self-attribution
challenges inherent in changing clients’ exercise of authorship for events. Journal of Experimental Social
behaviour. Obtaining clients’ commitment to engage Psychology, 44, 2–9.
in recommended exercise is often difficult, and even Dimmock, J. A., Jackson, B., & Clarke, D. (2013). “Warm inter-
when this commitment is obtained, clients some- actions” and “cold streaks”: The influence of temperature word
primes on interpersonal warmth and competence-related out-
times do not act upon their intentions. It is encoura- comes in a dyadic sport task. Sport, Exercise, & Performance
ging, then, that subtle features of the environment Psychology, 2, 102–116.
may be used alongside more traditional cognitive Eime, R. M., Payne, W. R., Casey, M. M., & Harvey, J. T.
behavioural approaches to foster more adaptive (2010). Transition in participation in sport and unstructured
834 C. Magaraggia et al.

physical activity for rural living adolescent girls. Health Mullan, E., Markland, D., & Ingeldew, D. K. (1997). A graded
Education Research, 25, 282–293. conceptualisation of self-determination in the regulation of
Friedman, R., Deci, E. L., Elliot, A. J., Moller, A. C., & Aarts, H. exercise behaviour: Development of measures using confirma-
(2010). Motivational synchronicity: Priming motivational tory factor analytic procedures. Personality and Individual
orientations with observations of others’ behaviors. Motivation Differences, 23, 745–752.
and Emotion, 34, 34–38. Preacher, K., & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling
Hodgins, H. S., Yacko, H. A., & Gottlieb, E. (2006). Autonomy strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in
and nondefensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 283–293. multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40,
Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Smells like 879–891.
clean spirit: Non-conscious effects of scent on cognition and Pullman, A. W., Masters, R. C., Zalot, L. C., Carde, L. E.,
behavior. Psychological Science, 16, 141–154. Saraiva, M. M., Dam, Y. Y., … Duncan, A. M. (2009).
Ishikawa-Takata, K., Ohta, T., & Tanaka, H. (2003). How much Effect of transition from high school to university on anthropo-
exercise is required to reduce blood pressure in essential hyper- metric and lifestyle variables in males. Applied Phsyiology,
tensives: A dose–response study. American Journal of Nutrition, and Metabolism, 34, 162–171.
Hypertension, 16, 629–633. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez, A., & Locke, E. A. (2005). Core the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and
self-evaluations and job and life satisfaction: The role of self- well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
concordance and goal attainment. Journal of Applied Psychology, Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

90, 257–268. health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being.


Juneau, M., Rogers, F., De Santos, V., Yee, M., Evans, A., Bohn, A., Journal of Personality, 65, 529–565.
& Debusk, R. F. (1987). Effectiveness of self-monitored, home- Sheldon, K. M. (2002). The self-concordance model of healthy
based, moderate-intensity exercise training in middle-aged men goal striving: When personal goals correctly represent the per-
and women. The American Journal of Cardiology, 60, 66–70. son. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-
Jung, M. E., Bray, S. R., & Martin Ginis, K. A. (2008). Behavior determination research (pp. 65–86). Rochester, NY: University
change and the freshman 15: Tracking physical activity and of Rochester Press.
dietary patterns in 1st-year university women. Journal of Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satis-
American College Health, 56, 523–530. faction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance
Levesque, C., & Pelletier, L. G. (2003). On the investigation of model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
primed and chronic autonomous and heteronomous motiva- 482–497.
tional orientations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R., & Reis, H. T. (1996). What makes for
1571–1584. a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and in the
Markland, D., & Ingledew, D. K. (2007). The relationships person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1270–
between body mass and body image and relative autonomy 1279.
for exercise among adolescent males and females. Psychology Shephard, R. (1968). Intensity, duration and frequency of exercise
of Sport and Exercise, 8, 836–853. as determinants of the response to a training regime. European
Markland, D., & Tobin, V. (2004). A modification to the beha- Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 26,
vioral regulation in exercise questionnaire to include an assess- 272–278.
ment of amotivation. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive deter-
191–196. minants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology
Mullan, E., & Markland, D. (1997). Variations in self-determina- Review, 8, 220–247.
tion across the stages of change for exercise in adults. Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. (2011). Foundations of sport and
Motivation and Emotion, 21, 349–362. exercise psychology (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Motivational priming and goal-setting 835

Appendix: autonomous, controlled and neutral


scrambled sentences

Autonomous Controlled Neutral

options have I two and do we to this must by people walk some now
dollars salad on costs two do I should to homework book we read the top
feel are choiceful I usually dollars salad costs two for dollars salad costs two for
is to this opportunity my to I smile ought and the a brown was dog
to we choose so leave for required to I’m study fence they but saw the
enjoy I freedom my to work to with obligated I’m two so had he hats
very coffee the is hot meet we on deadlines must plant I like obvious that
in we autonomous often are for boss coerced my me was sign a there too
have for preference a we is to here served lunch is to here served lunch
to go and I decided and obey we’re compelled to porch the far white was
to our we classes selected compulsory to attendance is our soft indirect is light to
Downloaded by [Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam] at 04:41 10 November 2017

apple was to the delicious apple was to the delicious apple was to the delicious
on choice we a have giving in to necessary is the walk fish swims slowly
we today unconstrained were for manipulates my to me boss tall is Julia quite but
can self-regulate to usually I so behaviour my they restrict pictures and our good were
is to here served lunch So coffee the is hot so coffee the is hot
actions and my are independent forced by to study I’m I student am a how
at the new computer is at the new computer is at the new computer is
now to I unrestricted am the by limits constrained us are pencils hers the it
am I still for self-determined very are we pressured that am citizen from a I

You might also like