You are on page 1of 6

J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat.

46 (2015) 115e120

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Behavior Therapy and


Experimental Psychiatry
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep

The approach-avoidance task as an online intervention in cigarette


smoking: A pilot study
Charlotte E. Wittekind a, *, Ansgar Feist a, Brooke C. Schneider a, Steffen Moritz a,
Anja Fritzsche b
a
University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Martinistraße 52, 20246 Hamburg, Germany
b
University Hamburg, Department of Psychology and Psychotherapy, Von-Melle-Park 5, 20246 Hamburg, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background and objectives: Dual-process models posit that addictive behaviors are characterized by
Received 18 March 2014 strong automatic processes that can be assessed with implicit measures. The present study investigated
Received in revised form the potential of a cognitive bias modification paradigm, the Approach-Avoidance Task (AAT), for
27 August 2014
retraining automatic behavioral tendencies in cigarette smoking.
Accepted 28 August 2014
Available online 16 September 2014
Methods: The study was set up as an online intervention. After completing an online survey, 257 smokers
were randomly allocated either to one of two experimental conditions (AAT) or a waitlist control group.
Participants responded to different pictures by pushing or pulling the computer mouse, depending on
Keywords:
Approach-avoidance task
the format of the picture. Pictures in portrait format depicted smoking-related items and were associated
Cognitive bias modification with pushing, pictures in landscape format depicted neutral items and were associated with pulling. One
Retraining version of the AAT provided individual feedback after each trial whereas the standard version did not.
Smoking After four weeks, participants were re-assessed in an online survey.
Cessation Results: Analyses revealed that the standard AAT, in particular, led to a significant reduction in cigarette
consumption, cigarette dependence, and compulsive drive; no effect was found in the control group.
Limitations: Interpretability of the study is constrained by the fact that no active control condition was
applied.
Conclusions: Notwithstanding the limitations, our findings indicate that the AAT might be a feasible
instrument to reduce tobacco dependence and can be applied as an online intervention. Future studies
should investigate whether the effects of behavior therapy can be augmented when combined with
retraining interventions.

© 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction was shown that compared to non-dependent individuals, drug-


dependent persons evaluate drug-related stimuli as more posi-
1.1. Information processing and approach-avoidance tendencies in tive, followed by behavioral approach tendencies (Stacy & Wiers,
addiction 2010). For example, behavioral approach tendencies were found
in alcohol dependence (e.g., Wiers, Rinck, Dictus, & van den
Despite being well aware of its negative consequences, Wildenberg, 2009) using an Approach-Avoidance Task (AAT,
continued substance use is typical of addictive behaviors in Rinck & Becker, 2007). The AAT is based on the well-established
smokers (Wiers & Stacy, 2006). This discrepancy can be reconciled finding that positive stimuli activate approach tendencies,
by dual-process models of addiction, which posit that in addictive whereas negative stimuli cause avoidance tendencies (e.g.,
disorders, there is an imbalance between automatic and reflective Neumann, Fo €rster, & Strack, 2003). The AAT measures duration of
systems in favor of automatic processes (Stacy & Wiers, 2010). It arm movements, with avoidance being characterized by reduced
response time of arm extending (pushing), and approach by
reduced response time of arm bending (pulling; Marsh, Kleck, &
Ambady, 2005).
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ49 40 7410 53522; fax: þ49 40 7410 57566. AAT tendencies have been examined among patients who abuse
E-mail address: c.wittekind@uke.uni-hamburg.de (C.E. Wittekind). various substances (e.g., Cousijn, Goudriaan, & Wiers, 2011; Wiers

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.08.006
0005-7916/© 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
116 C.E. Wittekind et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 46 (2015) 115e120

et al., 2009). Importantly, the AAT provided evidence that smoking consumption in comparison to the control group. Based on the
is associated with an approach bias toward smoking-related stimuli argumentation of the preceding paragraph, it is conceivable that
(Watson, de Wit, Cousijn, Hommel, & Wiers, 2013; Wiers et al., the modified AAT outperforms the standard AAT.
2013).
2. Material and methods
1.2. The AAT as an interventional technique
2.1. Recruitment
The bidirectional link between direction of physical movements
and evaluation processes has also been employed to modify atti- The study was set up as an online intervention using questback®
tudes, for example, in therapeutic contexts. Recent studies demon- (www.unipark.com/de). Invitations and a short description of the
strated that automatic action tendencies to approach alcohol could study were posted in several smoking-related internet forums. The
be retrained using a training variant of an alcohol AAT in hazardous rationale and the procedure of the study were explained to par-
drinkers and alcoholic inpatients (Wiers, Eberl, Rinck, Becker, & ticipants in written form both on the internet forums and on the
Lindenmeyer, 2011; Wiers, Rinck, Kordts, Houben, & Strack, 2010). introduction page of the survey. We refrained from using an
Furthermore, relapse rates were reduced in the AAT compared to the approach tobacco condition as it is conceivable that this condition
control group after one year (Eberl et al., 2013; Wiers et al., 2011). would increase cigarette consumption. The survey consisted of the
following sections: introduction, informed consent, sociodemo-
1.3. The present study graphic information, smoking behavior, psychiatric disorders, and
current medication. Subsequently, different questionnaires were
We adopted the AAT paradigm to smoking by presenting applied (see Section 2.7). Finally, participants were asked to provide
smoking-related and neutral pictures. Participants were allocated an email address.
to one of two slightly different versions of the smoking-related AAT
or a waitlist control group. In both versions, participants were 2.2. Participants
instructed to respond to the format of pictures which corresponded
to smoking-related (portrait) or neutral items (landscape), by The enrollment phase was terminated after 271 subjects had
pushing or pulling a joystick independent of the content of the completed the baseline survey. In total, 14 participants had to be
pictures. In the modified version, participants were shown their excluded from analyses (see Fig. 1). Participants were excluded if
reaction time (RT) after each trial. We decided to include the second they did not smoke within the last month or did not answer the
version because we wanted to examine whether immediate feed- survey honestly. No other exclusion criteria were applied.
back would increase the effectiveness of the program by activating
achievement motives (Sokolowski, Schmalt, Langens, & Puca, 2.3. Intervention
2000), thereby enhancing training compliance.
Although almost half of all smokers make an attempt to quit Based on the principle of the AAT (Rinck & Becker, 2007), the
(Shiffman, Brockwell, Pillitteri, & Gitchell, 2008), utilization of intervention was programmed using Visual Basic2010 Express
behavioral treatment is very low (Cokkinides, Ward, Jemal, & Thun, (Microsoft©) by AF. In total, 10 smoking-related and 10 neutral
2005; Shiffman et al., 2008). Frequent reasons for low treatment pictures were presented. Pictures were matched regarding the
utilization are fear of stigma, unsuccessful efforts to find adequate number of depicted items. After pressing a “start”-button, partici-
treatment, time constraints and the fact that smoking is not pants were asked to indicate their current urge to smoke. For the
considered a psychological problem but rather a bad habit. We training, smoking-related and neutral pictures were presented
therefore carried out our study via the Internet which offers the randomly. Smoking-related pictures were presented in portrait
possibility to reach people that would otherwise be reluctant to format and associated with “push”, neutral pictures were presented
attend intervention programs (for a discussion of (dis-)advantages in landscape format and associated with “pull”. After pressing the
see Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & John, 2004; Moritz, Timpano, start-button, a fixation cross appeared (500 ms) in the center of the
Wittekind, & Knaevelsrud, 2013). For the main outcome (ciga- screen, followed by either a neutral or a smoking-related picture.
rettes/day), we hypothesized that both versions of the AAT training Pulling led to an enlarged picture while pushing lead to a reduced
will lead to a significant decrease in participants' daily cigarette picture. Moving the computer mouse in the right direction led to

Excluded (n = 14)
Not meeting inclusion criteria (n = 2)
Not answering questions honestly (n = 2)
Entering invalid email address (n = 7)
Withdrawal of informed consent (n = 2) Allocated to
Randomization error (n = 1) standard AAT
(n = 87)

Pre-assessment Randomized Allocated to Post-assessment


(N = 271) (n = 257) modified AAT (n = 156)
(n = 85)

Allocated to
waitlist control
group (n = 85)

Fig. 1. CONSORT diagram.


C.E. Wittekind et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 46 (2015) 115e120 117

Table 1
Demographic and smoking-related information at baseline. Frequencies, means and standard deviations (in brackets).

Variable Standard AAT (n ¼ 87) Modified AAT (n ¼ 85) Control group (n ¼ 85) Statistics (df ¼ 254)

Age (in years) 44.30 (9.62) 43.65 (11.63) 41.55 (11.73) F ¼ 1.45, p ¼ .24
Education (in years) 11.95 (1.58) 11.80 (1.68) 11.85 (1.77) F < 1, p ¼ .83
Gender (female/male) 54/33 56/29 47/38 c2(2) ¼ 2.06, p ¼ .36
Cigarettes per day 20.31 (9.00) 20.13 (10.86) 20.52 (9.97) F < 1, p ¼ .97
Tobacco dependenceb 5.05 (2.50) 4.80 (2.41) 4.76 (2.55) F < 1, p ¼ .72
Smoking duration (in years) 24.27 (10.34) 25.09 (10.75) 22.62 (11.94) F ¼ 1.11, p ¼ .33a
Completion rate (in %) 63.2 54.1 64.7 c2(2) ¼ 2.35, p ¼ .31
Note. AAT ¼ Approach-Avoidance Task; df ¼ degree of freedom.
a
df ¼ 253.
b
Total score of the Fagerstrøm Test for Nicotine Dependence (0e2 ¼ very low, 3e4 ¼ low; 5 ¼ medium, 6e7 ¼ severe, 8e10 ¼ very severe).

the disappearance of the picture. Errors (i.e., moving mouse in the intervention. If the intervention was used, several questions
wrong direction) had to be corrected. By pressing the start-button, pertaining to the program and other cessation strategies were
the next trial was initiated. Each participant performed 100 trials. assessed (see Table 4). After indicating whether all questions had
Subsequently, participants were again asked to indicate their urge been answered honestly, participants were thanked for their
to smoke. Finally, participants were provided with brief feedback. participation and links were provided to download both AAT par-
Apart from the “traditional” AAT, we set up a modified AAT. Here, adigms and a manual about progressive muscle relaxation.
upon disappearance of the picture, RT for each trial was shown (see
Section 1). 2.6. Questionnaires

2.4. Group allocation Participants were asked to complete the following question-
naires at both pre- and post-assessment.
Participants who completed the baseline survey were randomly
allocated to one of three conditions (standard AAT [sAAT], modified 2.6.1. Commitment to Quitting Smoking Scale (CQSS)
AAT [mAAT], waitlist control group) in pseudo-random order. The CQSS is an 8-item self-report instrument designed to
Experimental groups were sent an email with an instruction measure the commitment to quit smoking (internal consistency:
manual attached. The control group was sent an email explaining a ¼ .91). Studies suggest that greater commitment at baseline is
that participants would receive the program after the post- associated with a higher abstinence rate at follow-up (Kahler et al.,
assessment. Participants were informed that they could contact 2007).
the first author for questions.
2.6.2. Fagerstrøm Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND)
2.5. Post-assessment The 6-item FTND (Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker, &
Fagerstrøm, 1991) is the most frequently used measure to tap to-
Four weeks after the baseline assessment, participants were bacco dependence (Piper, McCarthy, & Baker, 2006) and has been
sent an email inviting them to participate in the post-assessment. shown to be a reliable instrument (internal consistencies range
The first page contained a short introduction explaining the from a ¼ .55 to a ¼ .74; de Meneses-Gaya, Zuardi, Loureiro, & de
importance of participation. Subsequently, participants were asked Souza Crippa, 2009).
to enter the same email address as in the baseline assessment in
order to match pre- and post-assessment data. The following in- 2.6.3. Cigarette Dependence Scale-12 (CDS-12)
formation was assessed: current alcohol consumption, information The 12-item CDS (Etter, Le Houezec, & Perneger, 2003) is a self-
about smoking behavior, current medication, questionnaires from report measure assessing addiction to cigarettes following the
the baseline assessment. Cigarette consumption was assessed with diagnostic criteria of the DSM-IV and the ICD-10. The questionnaire
item #2 of the Cigarette Dependence Scale (i.e., cigarettes per day). shows excellent reliability (internal consistency: a ¼ .90, test-retest
Participants in the intervention groups were asked if they had used reliability for full scale: r ¼ .84; Etter et al., 2003).

Table 2
Group comparisons and results of the ANCOVAs for the outcome measures for pre, post, and across time, per protocol analyses.

Variable/time Pre per protocol Per protocol Post per protocol Per protocol between-group
between-group difference pre-post, ANCOVA
Waitlist Modified Standard Waitlist Modified Standard
difference pre
(n ¼ 55) AAT AAT (n ¼ 55) AAT AAT
(df ¼ 123)
(n ¼ 33) (n ¼ 38) (n ¼ 33) (n ¼ 38)

Cigarettes per day (n) 20.76 (9.95) 19.48 (7.13) 19.97 (8.93) F < 1, p ¼ .80 20.20 (10.55) 17.91 (7.35) 16.97 (10.07) F(2, 122) ¼ 3.55, p ¼ .032; h2p ¼ .06
FTND 4.55 (2.52) 4.67 (2.29) 4.89 (2.45) F < 1, p ¼ .79 4.47 (2.50) 4.42 (2.17) 4.08 (2.67) F(2, 122) ¼ 2.96, p ¼ .056, h2p ¼ .05
CQSS 26.96 (6.69) 25.27 (5.95) 25.97 (6.55) F < 1, p ¼ .48 27.62 (7.90) 24.15 (6.16) 25.03 (6.48) F(2, 122) ¼ 2.18, p ¼ .12, h2p ¼ .04
CDS-12 45.25 (9.17) 46.09 (5.81) 47.05 (8.31) F < 1, p ¼ .58 44.73 (9.97) 45.03 (5.49) 42.92 (9.92) F(2, 122) ¼ 5.62, p ¼ .005, h2p ¼ .08
OCSS total 15.98 (6.97) 18.94 (5.89) 19.05 (6.28) F ¼ 3.34, p ¼ .039 15.85 (6.77) 17.67 (4.84) 16.45 (7.31) F(2,122) ¼ 1.73, p ¼ .18, h2p ¼ .03
OCSS e preoccupation 8.31 (4.97) 10.30 (4.35) 10.24 (4.74) F ¼ 2.64, p ¼ .076 8.20 (4.67) 10.00 (3.74) 8.97 (4.96) F(2, 122) < 1, p ¼ .41, h2p ¼ .01
OCSS e Compulsive 7.67 (2.69) 8.64 (2.03) 8.82 (1.93) F ¼ 3.30, p ¼ .04 7.65 (2.74) 7.67 (1.67) 7.47 (2.81) F(2, 122) ¼ 3.32, p ¼ .039, h2p ¼ .05
drive

Note. df ¼ degree of freedom; AAT ¼ Approach-Avoidance Task; FTND ¼ Fagerstrøm Test for Nicotine Dependence (0e2 ¼ very low, 3e4 ¼ low; 5 ¼ medium, 6e7 ¼ severe,
8e10 ¼ very severe); CQSS ¼ Commitment to Quitting Smoking Scale; CDS-12 ¼ Cigarette Dependence Scale-12 (12 ¼ low dependence up to 60 ¼ high dependence);
OCSS ¼ Obsessive Compulsive Smoking Scale.
118 C.E. Wittekind et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 46 (2015) 115e120

Table 3
Group comparisons and results of the ANCOVAs for the outcome measures for pre, post, and across time, last observation carried forward.

Variable/time Pre LOCF LOCF between-group Post LOCF LOCF between-group difference
difference pre pre-post, ANCOVA
Waitlist Modified Standard Waitlist Modified Standard
(df ¼ 254)
(n ¼ 85) AAT AAT (n ¼ 85) AAT AAT
(n ¼ 85) (n ¼ 87) (n ¼ 85) (n ¼ 87)

Cigarettes per day (n) 20.52 (9.97) 20.13 (10.86) 20.31 (9.00) F < 1, p ¼ .97 20.09 (10.40) 19.38 (10.95) 18.46 (9.84) F(2, 253) ¼ 3.85, p ¼ .023; h2p ¼ .03
FTND 4.76 (2.55) 4.80 (2.41) 5.05 (2.50) F < 1, p ¼ .72 4.71 (2.58) 4.66 (2.35) 4.45 (2.72) F(2, 253) ¼ 5.01, p ¼ .007, h2p ¼ 04
CQSS 26.62 (6.71) 26.62 (7.01) 26.46 (6.45) F < 1, p ¼ .98 26.89 (7.58) 26.49 (7.27) 26.25 (6.71) F(2, 253) < 1, p ¼ .79, h2p ¼ 00
CDS-12 45.69 (9.17) 45.51 (7.53) 46.80 (8.76) F < 1, p ¼ .56 45.31 (9.70) 45.15 (7.01) 44.16 (9.83) F(2, 253) ¼ 6.38, p ¼ .002, h2p ¼ 05
OCSS total 17.11 (6.82) 18.47 (6.81) 18.71 (6.70) F ¼ 1.36, p ¼ .26 16.75 (6.85) 17.54 (7.03) 16.84 (8.22) F(2, 253) ¼ 2.28, p ¼ .10, h2p ¼ .02
OCSS e preoccupation 9.22 (4.76) 10.01 (5.06) 10.16 (4.94) F < 1, p ¼ .41 9.04 (4.54) 9.69 (5.01) 9.31 (5.30) F(2, 253) ¼ 1.02, p ¼ .36, h2p ¼ .01
OCSS e Compulsive 7.88 (2.76) 8.46 (2.40) 8.55 (2.49) F ¼ 1.73, p ¼ .18 7.87 (2.79) 7.85 (2.65) 7.67 (3.09) F(2, 253) ¼ 4.18, p ¼ .016, h2p ¼ .03
drive

Note. df ¼ degree of freedom; AAT ¼ Approach-Avoidance Task; FTND ¼ Fagerstrøm Test for Nicotine Dependence (0e2 ¼ very low, 3e4 ¼ low; 5 ¼ medium, 6e7 ¼ severe,
8e10 ¼ very severe); CQSS ¼ Commitment to Quitting Smoking Scale; CDS-12 ¼ Cigarette Dependence Scale-12 (12 ¼ low dependence up to 60 ¼ high dependence);
OCSS ¼ Obsessive Compulsive Smoking Scale.

2.6.4. Obsessive Compulsive Smoking Scale (OCSS) 2001). If the main analyses yielded a significant result, Bonferroni
The OCSS was designed to assess compulsive smoking (Hitsman corrected post-hoc tests were conducted by comparing both AAT
et al., 2010). It contains 10 items that are allocated to the subscales training conditions against the waitlist control group. Intention-to-
(1) preoccupation with smoking and (2) compulsive drive. Internal treat (ITT) and per protocol (PP) analyses were performed. For ITT
consistency was excellent, both for the full (a ¼ .89) and the sub- analyses, the data of all participants who completed the baseline
scales (preoccupation: a ¼ .87, compulsive drive: a ¼ .85). survey and met inclusion criteria were analyzed using the last
observation carried forward (LOCF) procedure. For PP, only data
2.7. Strategy of data analysis from completers and participants who used the training at least
once were considered.
All participants who withdrew informed consent or did not
answer the questions honestly were excluded from analyses. 3. Results
Analysis of the primary outcome (cigarettes/day) and all secondary
analyses were conducted using analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs). 3.1. Baseline
Difference scores (pre-post) served as dependent variables and
mean baseline scores as covariates (Field, 2009). This approach was Baseline demographic and smoking-related characteristics of
chosen to avoid regression to the mean and to control for baseline the groups are shown in Table 1. As can be seen, randomization was
differences (Borm, Fransen, & Lemmens, 2007; Vickers & Altman, successful: groups did not differ in any demographic or smoking-
related variable.

Table 4
Subjective appraisal of the programs for both intervention groups.
3.2. Group comparisons

Item Standard Modified 3.2.1. Completion


AAT AAT
(n ¼ 38) (n ¼ 33)
The post-assessment was completed by 156 of the initial 257
participants at baseline (60.7%). Completion rate did not signifi-
Satisfaction with the program in generala 52.6% 42.4%
cantly differ between groups (see Table 1). Non-completers did not
Will you use the program in the future?b 68.4% 48.5%
Do you smoke less?c 28.9% 15.2% differ from completers on any of the variables (all ps > .1) except for
Would you recommend the program …d the FTND which was lower in completers than non-completers at
…to a light smoker? 84.2% 69.7% baseline, t(255) ¼ 2.65, p ¼ .009.
…to a medium smoker? 78.9% 63.6%
…to a strong smoker? 65.8% 51.5%
The program is appropriate for 68.4% 69.7% 3.2.2. PP analyses
self-administration.e Seventeen (of 55) participants of the sAAT and 13 (of 46) par-
My urge to smoke was reduced by 23.7% 9.1% ticipants of the mAAT group had to be excluded due to non-usage of
using the program.e
the training, although they completed both surveys. Groups did not
The description of the program was 94.8% 84.8%
written comprehensively.e differ as to the use of other cessation strategies. Four participants of
I had to overcome myself to use the 36.9% 42.5% the mAAT and five participants of the sAAT group used another
program regularly.e technique in addition to the AAT, c(1) < 1, p ¼ .90.
The program was more helpful than 23.7% 24.2% Results for the main analyses (group comparisons) are sum-
other cessation methods.e
The program was not applicable for me.e 15.8% 36.4%
marized in Table 2. There was a significant improvement across
The design of the program was appealing.e 42.1% 45.5% time in both AAT groups relative to the control group for the pri-
a mary outcome (see Table 2). Post-hoc tests revealed that partici-
Very satisfied, rather satisfied, rather unsatisfied, very unsatisfied, percent are
summed for “satisfied” and “rather satisfied”. pants of the sAAT group smoked significantly fewer cigarettes than
b
Yes; rather yes, rather no, no, percent are summed for “yes” and “rather yes”. the control group, p ¼ .026, whereas there were no differences
c
% yes. between the mAAT and the control group, p ¼ .822. Comparisons
d
I would definitely not recommend it, I would rather not recommend it, I would within groups using one-sample t-tests against zero revealed a
recommend it conditionally, I would recommend it, percent are summed for
“conditionally recommend” and “recommend”.
meaningful reduction in cigarette smoking within the sAAT,
e
true, rather true, rather untrue, untrue, percent are summed for “rather true” t(37) ¼ 2.93, p ¼ .006, and the mAAT group, t(32) ¼ 2.95, p ¼ .006,
and “true”. but not within the control group, t(54) ¼ 1.26, p ¼ .212.
C.E. Wittekind et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 46 (2015) 115e120 119

For the CDS-12, the main analysis also achieved significance: the reduction in craving which, however, was not directly assessed in
sAAT group endorsed significantly fewer symptoms than the con- the present study.
trol group, p ¼ .005, there were no differences between the mAAT It is unlikely that significant effects are attributable to the
and the control group, p > .9. One sample t-tests against zero simultaneous usage of other cessation programs as only five par-
confirmed that only within the sAAT group improvement was ticipants in the sAAT and four participants in the mAAT group
beyond chance, t(37) ¼ 3.78, p ¼ .001 (other groups, ps > .1). applied additional cessation techniques. Nevertheless, it cannot be
The sAAT group also improved more on the OCSS subscale entirely ruled out that the effects were due to some extraneous
Compulsive Drive. The sAAT outperformed the control group, factors unrelated to treatment.
p ¼ .042, and the mAAT group did not differ from the control group, With regard to the efficacy of the two AAT versions, the study
p ¼ .313. There were no other significant group differences (see revealed that the sAAT outperformed the mAAT for most measures.
Table 2). For tobacco dependence as measured with the FTND, One possible explanation is that direct feedback after each trial
significance was bordered, p ¼ .056, h2p ¼ .05. interrupted the continuous movement thereby preventing a
reduction in associative strength. On the other hand, it could be
3.2.3. ITT analyses that perceived poor performance discouraged participants from
Results of the main analyses remained significant for the main further training leading to less favorable cessation results. Unfor-
outcome as well as the CDS-12 (see Table 3). Participants in the tunately, both versions did not differ significantly in the ITT ana-
sAAT group had a significantly greater reduction in the number of lyses which might be explained by the high attrition rate. However,
cigarettes/day than controls, p ¼ .026. For the CDS-12, the sAAT to fully establish an effect, ITT analyses are crucial.
group showed significantly greater improvement compared to the First, we have to acknowledge that no active control group was
control group, p ¼ .007; however, the mAAT group did not included; however; there were no significant differences between a
demonstrate significant improvement compared to the control sham and control condition in one study (Wiers et al., 2011)
group, p > .9. In addition, the sAAT group changed significantly in rendering a placebo effect unlikely. Moreover, we did not assess
the OCSS subscale Compulsive Drive as well as the FTND compared behavioral tendencies before and after the training. Although
to the control group (post-hoc: p ¼ .014 and p ¼ .011, see Table 3). behavioral changes are more important than changes in cognitive
No other group differences emerged (see Table 3). biases, they become more convincing when mediated by changes in
bias scores (cf. Eberl et al., 2013). This constitutes a serious limita-
3.3. Reliability tion, however, due to the set-up of the study, behavioral tendencies
could not be assessed. Second, individual use was not tracked as the
Internal consistencies (Cronbach's a) for all scales at pre- intervention was conducted via self-application and frequency of
assessment (N ¼ 257) ranged from a ¼ .69 to .91. Re-test reli- use was not assessed in the post-assessment. One further limitation
ability was determined for the waitlist group. All scales achieved that constrains the explanatory power of our results is that we did
satisfactory re-test reliabilities (cigarettes per day: r ¼ .95; CQSS: not obtain follow-up data. Finally, the absence of biochemical
r ¼ .73; FTND: r ¼ .89; CDS: r ¼ .90; OCSS total: r ¼ .79; OCSS verification further limits the validity of the data.
preoccupation: r ¼ .74; OCSS compulsive drive: r ¼ .78).
5. Conclusions
4. Discussion
As this was the first study that employed the AAT as a retraining
The aim of the present study was to investigate whether a
instrument in tobacco dependence, results need to be replicated
therapeutic adaptation of the AAT can reduce cigarette smoking.
with an active control condition before strong conclusions can be
The PP analyses revealed that relative to the waitlist control group
drawn. Given the small effects, it would be interesting to investi-
both training versions of the AAT led to a significantly greater
gate whether the training provides surplus benefit as an adjunct
reduction in cigarette consumption. Our results are in line with
treatment, a precursor to other smoking cessation interventions or
previous studies that successfully retrained action tendencies in
whether the training yields lower relapse rates when combined
alcohol-dependent individuals (Wiers et al., 2011, 2010). As the
with treatment as usual (cf. Eberl et al., 2013; Wiers et al., 2011).
study differed from previous studies in several regards (e.g., drug
Overall, our preliminary findings provide a first hint that the
under investigation, treatment goal), the significant effect provides
training version of the AAT might be a valuable instrument in
further evidence for a generalized effect of the AAT training.
treating tobacco dependence.
However, results need to be interpreted carefully as the study lacks
a placebo control condition and one could argue that participants
who engaged in treatment were more motivated to change. Acknowledgments
Notwithstanding, the fact that the two versions of the AAT yielded
different outcomes tentatively speaks against the possibility that We would like to thank all participants of the study. We would
improvement reflected a placebo effect alone. Furthermore, in the also like to thank the reviewers for their constructive feedback and
study of Wiers et al. (2011) there were no significant differences valuable comments.
between the sham and the no training condition which renders a
placebo effect unlikely. Future studies should apply a more strategic
References
control group with 50% approach and 50% avoidance movements to
all images. Borm, G. F., Fransen, J., & Lemmens, W. A. (2007). A simple sample size formula for
In comparison to the mAAT and the waitlist control group, both analysis of covariance in randomized clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Epidemi-
ology, 60, 1234e1238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.02.006.
cigarette dependence and Compulsive Drive significantly decreased
Cokkinides, V. E., Ward, E., Jemal, A., & Thun, M. J. (2005). Under-use of smoking-
in the sAAT group. Both of these results conform, at least theoret- cessation treatments: results from the National Health Survey, 2000. Amer-
ically, to changes within the dual-process system. The latter finding ican Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28, 119e122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
suggests that the sAAT training increased the perceived control j.amepre.2004.09.007.
Cousijn, J., Goudriaan, A. E., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). Reaching out towards cannabis:
over smoking urges (see Hitsman et al., 2010, p. 378, for a definition approach-bias in heavy cannabis users predicts changes in cannabis use.
of compulsive drive) and could tentatively be interpreted as a Addiction, 106, 1667e1674. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03475.x.
120 C.E. Wittekind et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 46 (2015) 115e120

de Meneses-Gaya, I. C., Zuardi, A. W., Loureiro, S. R., & de Souza Crippa, J. A. (2009). Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., & Baker, T. B. (2006). Assessing tobacco dependence: a
Psychometric properties of the Fagerstrøm test for nicotine dependence. Bra- guide to measure evaluation and selection. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 8,
zilian Journal of Pulmonology, 35, 73e82. 339e351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14622200600672765.
Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. Journal
(2013). Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 105e120. http://dx.doi.org/
replicate and for whom does it work best? Developmental Cognitive Neurosci- 10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.001.
ence, 4, 38e51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.002. Shiffman, S., Brockwell, S. E., Pillitteri, J. L., & Gitchell, J. G. (2008). Use of smoking-
Etter, J. F., Le Houezec, J., & Perneger, T. V. (2003). A self-administered questionnaire cessation treatments in the United States. American Journal of Preventive Med-
to measure dependence on cigarettes: the cigarette dependence scale. Neuro- icine, 34, 102e111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2007.09.033.
psychopharmacology, 28, 359e370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300030. Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H.-D., Langens, T. A., & Puca, R. M. (2000). Assessing
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once: the Multi-Motive Grid
Publishers. (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74, 126e145. http://dx.doi.org/
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we trust web- 10.1207/S15327752JPA740109.
based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet Stacy, A. W., & Wiers, R. W. (2010). Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for
questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59, 93e104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ explaining paradoxical behavior. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6,
0003-066X.59.2.93. 551e575. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131444.
Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., & Fagerstrøm, K. O. (1991). The Vickers, A. J., & Altman, D. G. (2001). Statistics notes: Analysing controlled trials
Fagerstrøm test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrøm tolerance with baseline and follow up measurements. British Medical Journal, 323,
questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 1119e1127. 1123e1124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.323.7321.1123.
Hitsman, B., Shen, B.-J., Cohen, R. A., Morissette, S. B., Drobes, D. J., Spring, B., et al. Watson, P., de Wit, S., Cousijn, J., Hommel, B., & Wiers, R. W. (2013). Motivational
(2010). Measuring smoking-related preoccupation and compulsive drive: mechanisms underlying the approach bias to cigarettes. Journal of Experimental
evaluation of the obsessive compulsive smoking scale. Psychopharmacology, 211, Psychopathology, 4, 250e262. http://dx.doi.org/10.5127/jep.030512.
377e387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1910-z. Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining
Kahler, C. W., LaChance, H. R., Strong, D. R., Ramsey, S. E., Monti, P. M., & Brown, R. A. automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients' approach bias for
(2007). The commitment to quitting smoking scale: Initial validation in a alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science, 22, 490e497.
smoking cessation trial for heavy social drinkers. Addicitve Behaviors, 32, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611400615.
2420e2424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.04.002. Wiers, C. E., Kühn, S., Javadi, A. H., Korucuoglu, O., Wiers, R. W., Walter, H., et al.
Marsh, A. A., Kleck, R. E., & Ambady, N. (2005). The effects of fear and anger facial (2013). Automatic approach bias towards smoking cues is present in smokers
expression on approach- and avoidance-related behavior. Emotion, 5, 119e124. but not in ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 229, 187e197. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.119. 10.1007/s00213-013-3098-5.
Moritz, S., Timpano, K., Wittekind, C. E., & Knaevelsrud, C. (2013). Harnessing the Wiers, R. W., Rinck, M., Dictus, M., & van den Wildenberg, E. (2009). Relatively
web: Internet and self-help therapy for people with obsessive compulsive strong automatic appetitive action-tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 G-
disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. In E. A. Storch, & D. McKay (Eds.), allele. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 8, 101e106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-
Handbook of treating variants and complications in anxiety disorders (pp. 183X.2008.00454.x.
375e397). New York, NY: Springer Science. Wiers, R. W., Rinck, M., Kordts, R., Houben, K., & Strack, F. (2010). Retraining
Neumann, R., Fo €rster, J., & Strack, F. (2003). Motor compatibility: the bidirectional automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers.
link between behavior and evaluation. In J. Musch, & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The Addiction, 105, 279e287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02775.x.
psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. Wiers, R. W., & Stacy, A. W. (2006). Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction.
371e391). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers.

You might also like