You are on page 1of 13

REVIEWS

Managing competing goals — a key


role for the frontopolar cortex
Farshad Alizadeh Mansouri1–3*, Etienne Koechlin4*, Marcello G. P. Rosa1,2
and Mark J. Buckley5
Abstract | Humans are set apart from other animals by many elements of advanced cognition and
behaviour, including language, judgement and reasoning. What is special about the human brain
that gives rise to these abilities? Could the foremost part of the prefrontal cortex (the frontopolar
cortex), which has become considerably enlarged in humans during evolution compared with
other animals, be important in this regard, especially as, in primates, it contains a unique
cytoarchitectural field, area 10? The first studies of the function of the frontopolar cortex in
monkeys have now provided critical new insights about its precise role in monitoring the
significance of current and alternative goals. In human evolution, the frontopolar cortex may
have acquired a further role in enabling the monitoring of the significance of multiple goals in
parallel, as well as switching between them. Here, we argue that many other forms of uniquely
human behaviour may benefit from this cognitive ability mediated by the frontopolar cortex.

Humans clearly stand apart from other animals in our prefrontal areas. Area 10 also shows late postnatal mat-
ability to engage in complex, organized, goal-directed uration, suggesting that its neurons undergo extensive
behaviours, and some of our advanced cognitive abilities experience-dependent structural changes that could be
1
Biomedicine Discovery may be uniquely human (for example, language, judge- essential for the development of higher cognitive func-
Institute and Department
ment, long-term planning and reasoning). Although tions12–16. For the purposes of this Review, we consider
of Physiology, Monash
University, Clayton, it is tempting to speculate that there might be some our region of interest, the frontopolar cortex, as over-
Victoria, 3800, Australia. underlying anatomical basis supporting these cogni- lapping primarily with cytoarchitectural area 10p (area
2
Australian Research Council, tive functions that separate us from other animals, few 10 polar) in the nomenclature proposed by Ongur and
Centre of Excellence in morphological differences have so far been identified collaborators17.
Integrative Brain Function,
that distinguish our brains from those of non-human Unlike the case for other prefrontal subregions, we
Clayton, Victoria, 3800,
Australia. primates. One notable exception is that in the primate do not have decades of research involving circumscribed
3
Cognitive Neuroscience evolutionary lineages leading to humans, the anterior animal lesion studies of the frontopolar cortex reveal-
Research Centre, Kerman parts of the frontal lobes (referred to as the prefrontal ing functions that necessarily depend upon it, nor do
University of Medical cortex) have expanded in volume relative to the poste- we have numerous cellular-activity recording studies
Sciences, Kerman, Iran.
rior cortical regions1. Moreover, in primates, the fore- revealing underlying neuronal mechanisms operating in
4
Institut National de la Santé
et de la Recherche Médicale, most part of the prefrontal cortex (the frontopolar the frontopolar cortex and mediating specific cognitive
Ecole Normale Supérieure, cortex) contains a cortical area, commonly referred to processes2,3. However, now that the first animal models
29, rue d’Ulm, 75005 Paris, as ‘cytoarchitectonic area 10’, that is of special interest. of area 10 have emerged18–24, it is timely and important
France.
Although area 10 is exclusive to primates, non-human to assess the critical new insights that these behavioural–
5
Department of
Experimental Psychology,
primates and humans show marked differences in this lesion and neuronal recording studies reveal about its
Oxford University, Oxford, brain region1 (FIG. 1a). Area 10 is proportionally larger in precise function. In particular, the first-ever circum-
OX1 3UD, UK. volume relative to the rest of the brain in humans than scribed behavioural–lesion studies of the frontopolar
*These authors contributed in non-human primates, and, in humans, its neurons cortex in monkeys have revealed cognitive abilities
equally to this work.
are more widely spaced, allowing more room for intra- that are spared, impaired or enhanced with lesions in
Correspondence to F.A.M. area and inter-area connectivity 1. The neuroanatomical this region23,24.
and E.K. 
farshad.mansouri@monash.
features of area 10 suggest it has a higher-order role in Specifying the contribution of the frontopolar cor-
edu; cognition2–7. These features include its unusual pattern tex to cognition is important not only for understand-
etienne.koechlin@upmc.fr of afferent connections, the vast majority of which ing the mechanistic basis of higher-order cognitive
doi:10.1038/nrn.2017.111 originate from multimodal and higher-order associa- functions in primates (possibly including some unique
Published online 29 Sep 2017 tion areas8–11 (FIG. 1b), which distinguishes it from other forms of intelligent human behaviour) but also because

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 1


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Human

Macaque Marmoset
1 cm

b ACC
DLPFC PCC

VLPFC MPFC RSC


STS/ OFC
STG
PHC
TP

Figure 1 | Neuroanatomy of the frontopolar cortex. a | The extent of the frontopolar cortex Nature Reviews | Neuroscience
(cytoarchitectural area 10p)
is shown in lateral (left) and medial (right) views of the right hemisphere in three species of simian primate. The extent of
area 10 is highlighted in red. In the human brain, the yellow region indicates the lateral subdivision of the frontopolar
cortex, which is hypothesised to subserve functions that monkeys are less capable of performing4,25. This comparison
illustrates the fact that the frontopolar cortex becomes larger not only in absolute terms but also in relative terms, as a
function of brain size1. b | The main sources of cortico-cortical afferent connections to the non-human primate frontopolar
cortex, here illustrated in a macaque brain. All major direct synaptic projections to the frontopolar cortex originate in
higher-order association areas8–10. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MPFC, medial
prefrontal cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; PHC, parahippocampal cortex; RSC,
retrosplenial cortex; STG, superior temporal gyrus; STS, superior temporal sulcus; TP, temporal pole; VLPFC, ventrolateral
prefrontal cortex.

this knowledge may help us to understand some of the enabling switching away from ongoing behaviour. We
underlying causes of the behavioural deficits associated theorise that in human evolution, the frontopolar cortex
with a range of neuropsychiatric disorders linked to has acquired additional related roles, both in enabling
disturbed prefrontal interactions. Here, we review the monitoring of the significance of several competing
extent to which a wide range of influential hypotheses goals in parallel and in switching between them. In par-
of human frontopolar cortex function, largely based on ticular, we propose that these new functions are linked to
human neuroimaging, are supported by new studies in a lateral subdivision of cytoarchitectural area 10 (FIG. 1),
animal models that address causality to a degree rarely which may not have a clear monkey counterpart 25. We
possible in human neuropsychology. We then outline propose that a range of unique human behaviours relate
how findings from a particular set of recent studies com- to this underlying advance in cognition mediated by the
bining human neuroimaging and computational mod- frontopolar cortex.
elling has, together with data from these recent animal
studies, provided a synergistic view of this brain region Pursuing goals in changing environments
while also revealing new insights about specific features In a complex and changing environment, the validity of
of the human frontopolar cortex. We conclude that a key tasks or goals might change in terms of their associated
specialization of primate frontopolar cortex is in manag- reward and cost, and we often face the necessity of mak-
ing competing goals, in part by keeping track of the sig- ing a strategic decision to adaptively shift between goals.
nificance of current and alternative goals, and therefore Imagine you are employed in a university but receive

2 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

a job offer from a different institution. The decision Models of frontopolar cortex function
to cease exploiting the current source of income and Studies involving non-invasive neuroimaging methods
shift to explore another would require assessing vari- such as functional MRI (fMRI) and positron emission
ous aspects of the cost and benefit of these alternative tomography (PET) in humans have reported correla-
opportunities based on your internal goals and plans tions between activity in the frontopolar cortex and
(FIG. 2). Adjusting the tendency for exploitation versus participant performance in a diverse range of cognitive
exploration is likely to be a fundamental aspect of for- tasks and experimental paradigms2–7. Although many
aging behaviour, to increase the chances of success and influential ideas about frontopolar cortex function in
survival in the long term. Humans are not the only spe- higher-order cognition have emerged from this correla-
cies that face such dilemmas in a changing environment. tional evidence (as reviewed below), no consensus has
Other species might also face the necessity of ceasing the yet been reached. Neuropsychological examination of
exploitation of a current source of reward and instead patients with lesions in the frontopolar cortex has also
exploring other resources in order to increase the chance so far provided limited insight, owing to the fact that
of their survival and reproduction. Such decisions entail selective and bilateral damage to the frontopolar cor-
assessment of the value (that is, the cost and the ben- tex is rare. Nonetheless, we know that lesions involving
efit) of the current and alternative reward resources this region do not impair overall intelligence, language
and associated goals for the individual, and, in socially ability or basic sensory and motor abilities 2,3,27–28.
advanced species, also for the group (FIG. 2). A distrib- Similarly, these patients do not exhibit deficits in stand-
uted neural network involving the prefrontal and medial ard neuropsychological tests or in the performance of
frontal cortices regulates the use of cognitive resources to executive control tasks, which are sensitive to lesions
optimize exploitation of current reward resources while in more posterior or medial regions of the prefrontal
minimizing the associated cost. This is referred to as cortex 2,3,27–28. Patients with frontopolar cortex lesions
executive control of goal-directed behaviour 26. Our main do, however, show profound impairments in managing
thesis is that recent studies in monkeys and humans sug- their daily life, particularly in dealing with open-ended,
gest that the most rostral part of the prefrontal cortex novel or changing situations2,3,27–28. As we argue below,
(the frontopolar cortex) has a crucial role in adjusting new findings in monkeys and humans, combined with
the tendency for exploitation versus exploration of other pioneering electrophysiological results, have allowed
alternative resources by assessing the value of alterna- a revised interpretation of the results of previous
tive tasks and goals and by re‑distributing our cognitive imaging and neuropsychological studies, leading to a
resources. Before reviewing evidence for this proposed new understanding of the unique role for the fronto­
model, we review previous models of frontopolar cortex polar cortex in the executive control of goal-directed
function in goal-directed behaviours. behaviour.

Goal-directed behaviour
Exploration
Exploitation
Sustain current Explore Shift to alternative and/or most
task or goal alternative task valued (humans only) goal

Current task or goal, • Reward? Alternative


e.g. action selection, • Cost? task #1
Reward? Current Alternative
assessing behavioural
Cost? task value task value
outcome, reward • Reward? Alternative
prediction task #2
• Cost?
Frontal pole cortex:
• Estimates value of current and
Executive control network: alternative goals
Includes DLPFC, ACC, • Redistributes cognitive resources
OFC and parietal cortex to current or alternative goals

Nature Reviews | Neuroscience


Figure 2 | Adaptive goal-directed behaviour requires a balance between exploitation of the current goal and the
tendency to shift towards an alternative goal. Pursuing the current goal requires participation and coordination of
multiple cognitive functions, such as implementing a strategy or rule, representing spatiotemporal contingencies (task
context), action selection, mnemonic functions, assessing behavioural outcome, reward or risk prediction, and
task-related emotional and motivational regulations, to accomplish a task. An executive control network including
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) supports these
cognitive processes. The frontopolar cortex does not necessarily participate in the execution of well-learned tasks but
instead collects highly processed information regarding the value (cost and benefit) of the current and alternative tasks or
goals to adjust the balance between the tendency for exploitation of the current task and the drive for exploring
alternative reward sources or goals in the environment. Monkeys and humans might share the overall organization and the
neural substrate of such adaptive goal-directed behaviour; however, the human brain might have acquired additional
capacities to assess multiple goals and direct the exploratory shift towards the most advantageous one.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 3


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Gateway hypothesis. One cognitive ability that is individual operations2. This model suggests that the
assumed to be central to human mental processing is frontopolar cortex plays a unique role in integrating
the ability to control the extent to which we attend to the outcome of separate cognitive operations that are
stimuli and events in the external environment or to self-­ mediated by other cortical areas2. The model is not sup-
generated or self-maintained representations (that is, the ported by observations that many tasks that do depend
‘thoughts in our head’). The influential ‘gateway hypo­ on integrating outcomes of multiple cognitive processes
thesis’ maintains that the frontopolar cortex is positioned are not impaired either in patients3 or, as we will see later,
at the heart of such a mechanism, thus controlling transi- in monkeys24,25 with frontopolar cortex lesions.
tioning between the aptly named stimulus-oriented (SO)
and stimulus-independent (SI) modes of attention3,29–31. Mnemonic functions. Various mnemonic processes
This model is supported by imaging studies that showed support advanced human cognitive abilities. The
that sustained attention in either mode, or a transition most prominent mnemonic function associated with
between these two modes, correlates with frontopolar the human frontopolar cortex is episodic memory36,37.
cortex activation. Moreover, the model proposes that Activation of the frontopolar cortex has been reported
medial and lateral subdivisions of the frontopolar cortex during various tasks involving retrieval of episodic
support SO and SI attention, respectively 3,29–31. Although memory 36,37. The frontopolar cortex is also activated in
it is intuitive that the frontopolar cortex may have sub- prospective memory, which refers to the ability to main-
divisions, as its connections vary with location8,9, it is tain an intention to act in a certain way in the mind,
unclear how the SO and SI modes of attention relate usually after a delay filled with an unrelated ongoing
to anatomically defined subregions. In addition, other task3,38–41. Lesions including but not limited to the fronto­
areas, including those in the lateral prefrontal cortex, polar cortex have also been reported to lead to impair-
show similar SO‑associated and SI‑associated changes ment in prospective memory in humans3,42. Notably,
in activation, leaving open the question of which distinct however, it is time-based (not event-based) prospective
cognitive processing mode operates specifically in the memory that is associated with the frontopolar cortex
frontopolar cortex. Although deficits in multitasking and in voxel-based lesion analyses41, and a key element of
prospective memory in patients with frontopolar cortex such memory may be the relative value of two alternative
damage have been considered to support the gateway tasks (which changes continually over time as the time
hypothesis3, the fact that patients with frontopolar cor- for the prospective action approaches). As we will see,
tex damage do not show impairment in many other tasks the monkey lesion work discussed later in this review
that require a transition between SO and SI attention points to general impairments in exploring the value of
does not support this hypothesis2,3,32. a broad range of alternatives23.
There is a long history of association of prefron-
Mind-wandering and self-referential mental processes. tal cortex with working memory (that is, online mainte-
Another arguably unique human cognitive ability in nance of task-relevant information)26,43–46. Activation of
which the frontopolar cortex has been closely impli- the frontopolar cortex has also been observed in tasks
cated is the processing of self-referential knowledge, requiring working memory, leading to the suggestion
including knowledge of self, knowledge of the internal that the dorsolateral prefrontal and frontopolar corti-
mental world33,34, and evaluation of self-generated infor- ces form a hierarchical system that is specialized for the
mation35. Imaging studies that compared activation evaluation, monitoring and manipulation of information
between rest and task performance conditions showed held in working memory 46–48. Specifically, the dorsolat-
that several brain areas, including the frontopolar cortex, eral prefrontal cortex has been seen as supporting the
exhibit higher levels of activation during a rest period processing of externally generated information, whereas
or during tasks with minimal cognitive demands2,33,34. the frontopolar cortex would hold internally generated
Therefore, the frontopolar cortex has been associated information in memory 47,48. Although activation of the
with mind-wandering and engagement in the afore- frontopolar cortex in these and other mnemonic tasks
mentioned processes. However, this hypothesis remains indicates that this region might play a role in organiz-
Episodic memory poorly predictive without further specification of the cog- ing the retrieval, maintenance and processing of infor-
A memory of events enriched nitive processing that depends on the frontopolar cortex, mation, the exact function of the frontopolar cortex in
by contextual information such and it poses difficulties for explaining the activation of such diverse mnemonic processes remains unknown. As
as the associated emotion,
place and time.
this area in various cognitively challenging tasks2,4–7. we will see, the monkey lesion work discussed later in
this review reveals an absence of general impairments
Working memory Integration of the outcome of multiple cognitive pro- in working memory and retrieval of contextual informa-
A process of short-term cesses. Many advanced cognitive abilities depend on tion, pointing instead to specific deficits in rapid learning
storage of information to
integration of highly processed information that emerges about novel alternatives that impinge upon performance
support ongoing or upcoming
actions. from processing in distributed neural networks involv- across a range of diverse learning and memory tasks23,24.
ing different brain regions. Owing to its robust connec-
Analogical reasoning tions to supra-modal areas, the frontopolar cortex has Analogical, relational, transitive and abstract reasoning.
A process of reasoning based been proposed to sit atop a hierarchy of frontal brain Analogical reasoning49 and relational integration2,35,50 are
on comparison between
objects, events or models to
regions and to facilitate the transfer of highly processed clearly higher-order cognitive processes (though not nec-
help in understanding, learning information between discrete operational units located essarily uniquely human) that depend on the construc-
and decision-making. elsewhere to enable goals that cannot be achieved by tion of mental representations to facilitate comparisons

4 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

and judge similarities between episodes, to enhance and implicated the frontopolar cortex more gener-
learning and to make inferential judgements51. Reasoning ally in such higher-order executive control processes.
by transitive inference also requires the integration of mul- Specifically, the frontopolar cortex and intraparietal
tiple relations to reach a conclusion1,51,52. Activation of the sulcus were together more active during exploratory
frontopolar cortex has been reported in the context of behaviour, whereas regions of the striatum and ventro-
many tasks that require such abstract reasoning 2,35,50–52, medial prefrontal cortex were associated with exploita-
and a recent study also suggests that patients with lesions tive behaviour. The results suggest a highly interactive
extending to the frontopolar cortex show deficits in tests model of action selection under uncertainty that at least
that require analogical reasoning 53. Overall, these stud- implicates the frontopolar cortex in switching between
ies have suggested that the frontopolar cortex is involved exploratory and exploitative modes64. The animal work
when the processing and integration of abstract infor- reviewed later extends this idea of frontopolar cortex
mation guides decision-making and creativity, and involvement to broader kinds of exploratory behaviour.
that it supports emotional, affective and social cogni-
tive functions2,35,50–53. A hierarchical anterior–­posterior Planning. Planning is an important aspect of human
organization in the frontal cortex has also been proposed cognition, and imaging studies have revealed an acti-
based on the level of abstractness of representation upon vation change in the frontopolar cortex in the context
which control is exerted6,7,54,55 or on the level of actions of planning tasks, such as the Tower of London test. This
selected11, with the frontopolar cortex processing the led to the hypothesis that the frontopolar cortex plays a
highest level of abstraction and premotor cortex dealing crucial role in planning 65,66. However, these activations
with representation of concrete events or actions. Some were not specific to this region, and patients with fronto­
investigators have suggested an important role for the polar cortex lesions did not show deficits in performing
frontopolar cortex in other kinds of abstract reasoning, the Tower of London test 27; similarly, neuronal activity
including theory of mind, deception56,57 and moral judge- related to multistep planning is also observed outside the
ment58. However, in these cases, the observed activity was frontopolar cortex in monkeys67.
not specific to the frontopolar cortex — activity was also
seen in other prefrontal and parietal areas59–61. Cognitive branching or multitasking
Achieving goals in many tasks might require holding
Establishing task set and preparation for upcoming task information about a pending task in short-term memory
demands. The term ‘task set’ is used to refer to a config- while alternative (sub-)tasks are being completed. This
uration of cognitive processes that is actively maintained allows the individual to monitor and resume the main
for subsequent task performance, and this capability has task after completing the alternative tasks. This cognitive
also long been linked to the prefrontal cortex. However, ability has been considered to be cognitive branching or
the frontopolar cortex in particular has been proposed to multitasking 4–6,68. Some of the aforementioned cognitive
be involved in establishing task sets or shifting between abilities, such as planning, analogical and abstract rea-
them62. In one fMRI study, frontopolar (but not dorso- soning, and problem-solving, usually involve cognitive
lateral prefrontal) activity was more strongly correlated branching, and an influential hypothesis that emerged
with different posterior regions depending upon the from human imaging and computational studies sug-
rule the participants intended to apply after a delay 62. In gested that the frontopolar cortex plays an essential role
patients with rostral prefrontal lesions, the set-related in cognitive branching 4–6. Cognitive branching enables
delay-period activity of posterior areas (involved in each multitasking, which is conceptually different from dual-
task performance) was not affected, but the functional task performance. In dual-task performance, the subject
connectivity (a measure of synchronous activity that is switches back and forth between different tasks accord-
used to assess inter-region interactions) between these ing to the stimuli that are presented, without requiring
areas was markedly attenuated32. These patients showed the information for one task to be held in memory while
behavioural deficits when the task shifted, but they per- performing another task. Bilateral activation of the fron-
formed much better than the controls when the task topolar cortex has been observed in branching but not in
Transitive inference stayed the same32 (see later for details of related kinds dual-task conditions4–6. As only the branching condition
A process of reasoning based of improved performance in monkeys with frontopolar requires the additional process of retaining information
on relationships between cortex lesions). This finding was interpreted as the result about the main task while performing another task, it has
objects or events to help in
understanding, learning and
of patients following a default state or strategy when the been proposed that the cognitive branching function is
decision-making. task remains stable32; alternatively, the frontopolar cortex specific to the frontopolar cortex 4–6. Neuropsychological
may support the maintenance of the alternative set in a examination of patients with lesions confirmed that the
Theory of mind state of readiness, thereby facilitating transition between number of errors in the branching condition, but not in
Cognitive ability that allows
the sets without the necessity for re‑establishing it63. the dual-task condition, is proportional to the involve-
one to infer someone else’s
beliefs, intents, desires and ment of the frontopolar cortex 28,68. Further supporting
feelings. Exploratory decision-making. Optimum decision-­ evidence has come from an fMRI study in which partic-
making in uncertain environments requires balancing ipants recurrently chose between alternative courses of
Tower of London test the conflicting demands of gathering and exploiting action based on their expectations of a reward (which
A cognitive test of planning in
which participants must plan
information. An influential study highlighted the role was probabilistically changing and consequently uncer-
the order of balls on a peg of the frontopolar cortex in resolving this explore-­ tain)69. Frontopolar cortex activations signalled that
based on a given template. versus-exploit dilemma in a simulated gambling task64 the reward-based evidence had accumulated in favour

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 5


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

of the best unchosen course of action during the per- crucial role in a feedback-based learning process19–21.
formance of the chosen course of action69. This led to However, these kinds of neural signals appear to already
the suggestion that the frontopolar cortex encodes the be present in other prefrontal and medial frontal areas,
reward-based advantage of the unchosen, pending albeit, as mentioned above, embedded in a much richer
option and that when this advantage becomes positive, context21,26,43,70,72,73,80 (FIG. 3a). Consequently, the distinctive
the frontopolar cortex recruits posterior frontal and pari- information conveyed by neuronal activity in the fronto­
etal regions to implement the behavioural shift between polar cortex remains unclear. An alternative view of the
the two options69. Finally, frontopolar activations asso- electrophysiological results, which will be discussed in
ciated with cognitive branching appear to be located in the following sections, is that the modulation of fronto­
the lateral sector (FIG. 1), which seems to have no func- polar cortex cell activity confined around the feedback
tional equivalent in monkeys5,25. This suggests that, encodes the reward obtained from a successful decision
unlike humans, monkeys should have difficulty properly in the current task. Such information might reflect the
performing cognitive branching between tasks. overall relevance of performing the current task and facil-
itate monitoring the opportunity to stay versus to switch
The macaque frontopolar cortex away from the current task4 (FIG. 2).
Neuronal activity. To our knowledge, no published study
has reported recordings of neuronal activity in the human Behavioural consequences of lesions. Only two studies
frontopolar cortex. The first publications reporting the to date have examined the behavioural consequences of
results of recordings in the frontopolar cortex of monkeys bilateral selective frontopolar cortex lesions23,24. In one
have recently emerged18–21, and their findings proved to of these studies24, the monkeys performed a computer-
be remarkably unlike those of recordings in neighbour- ized analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
ing prefrontal and medial frontal regions (the dorso- (FIG. 3a), which provides a measure of cognitive flexibility
lateral prefrontal, orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate in learning and in switching between abstract rules (that
cortices)21. According to these reports, neuronal activity is, matching by colour or matching by shape). In contrast
in the frontopolar cortex is only modulated around the to the previously reported consequences of lesions in the
time a reward is delivered and when this reward results dorsolateral prefrontal, orbitofrontal, or anterior cingulate
from a successful self-generated (as opposed to directly cortices83, frontopolar cortex lesions did not impair basic
cued) decision. By contrast, recordings in adjacent perceptual-motor functions or the ability of the animals to
areas have shown that neurons encode a much wider adapt to rule changes in the WCST24 (FIG. 4). Together, the
range of parameters that may support the motivational absence of impairment in behavioural measures related
and higher-­order cognitive aspects of a task20,21,43–45,70–80, to the working memory of rules, selective attention to the
including working memory 26,43,44,46,76,77,79,80, prediction current rule, inhibition of a previously relevant rule, or
error 81, actions74, value75, risk78,81, outcomes26,78, rules assessment of the behavioural outcome to shift between
(FIG. 3a) and strategies26,43,79,80, and/or numerosity 82. abstract rules (to detect and adapt to unannounced rule
Frontopolar cortex neuronal activity has been changes) (TABLE 1) demonstrated a functional dissociation
recorded while monkeys performed a task in which between the fronto­polar cortex and these other prefrontal
cues instructed either a ‘stay’ or ‘shift’ decision19–21. and medial frontal cortical areas24 (FIG. 4).
Stay cues required a saccade in the same direction as The monkeys were also tested in another ver-
that chosen on the preceding trial, whereas shift cues sion of the task, in which the level of conflict between
required a saccade in the alternative direction. Thus, the rules changed trial by trial. Conflict cost24,45,71,80 (adverse
monkeys had to remember the direction of the saccade effects of conflict on performance in the current trial)
performed in the previous trial and also select the next was unimpaired in frontal-pole-lesioned monkeys, but
target according to the presented (stay or shift) cue. conflict adaptation25,45,71,80 (conflict-induced improved
Some test blocks involved a visual cue, whereas others performance in the following trial) was notably aug-
involved a specific pattern of delivery of fluid drops to mented. This was a distinctive behavioural alteration in
the mouth. A liquid reward (feedback) was given for frontal-pole-lesioned monkeys because lesions in dorso-
all correct responses. The main finding was that neu- lateral prefrontal cortex or orbitofrontal cortex cause a
ronal activity was only modulated around the feedback reduction in conflict adaptation, whereas lesions in the
period and only reflecting the action (saccade direction) cingulate cortex (anterior or posterior) have no signifi-
Conflict cost
The adverse effects on speed
performed in that trial19–21 (FIG. 3b). The conclusion that cant effect24,45,71,80. The better-than-control conflict adap-
and accuracy that arise as a the activity was bound to the feedback was based on tation indicated that the context-dependent allocation of
result of competition or conflict the fact that modulation in activity persisted during the cognitive resources to resolve the conflict was enhanced
between behavioural choices in feedback period, even when that feedback was delayed. in frontal-pole-lesioned monkeys. These findings led to
experimental tasks.
Intriguingly, the neuronal activity did not encode the the hypothesis that the frontopolar cortex is involved in
Conflict adaptation short-term memory of the preceding action, the cued redistributing cognitive resources away from the current
The behavioural effects of strategy (which was necessary for the task performance), task, towards alternative targets for attention24.
conflict that affect performance the anticipated or delivered reward, or the preparation To test this hypothesis, the monkeys were trained
in the subsequent trial, and execution of action19–21. Hence, the task-relevant to perform yet another version of the task (WCST-
where they are manifested as
a behavioural improvement if
activity of fronto­polar cortex neurons is indeed quite dis- interruption), in which intentional attempts were made
the subject is faced with tinct from those reported for other frontal areas. These to distract the animals from WCST trials. When perfor-
conflict again. findings were interpreted by the authors as reflecting a mance with one rule reached the shift criterion, instead

6 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

a Bar press
630 ms Liquid
Up to 10 s 700 ms Up to 2500 ms
reward
Correct
500 ms

Incorrect
Start cue Fixation Sample Decision

Error signal
DLPFC cell
9
Colour rule OFC cell: shape matching OFC cell: colour matching High conflict
Shape rule
18 18 Free reward
Low conflict
Spikes/s

Spikes/s

Spikes/s
Reward Reward

0 0 0
–1.3 0 1.3 –0.7 0 0.7 –0.7 0 0.7
Time (s) Time (s) Time (s)
Fixation Decision
Start cue Sample

Feedback
b

Fixation Cue Delay Go Saccade Pre-feedback Post-feedback


(1.5 s) (0.5 s) (1.0 –1.5 s) (0.5 –1.0 s) (0.5 s)
Feedback period

Cues Frontopolar cortex cell


10 n = 100 n = 100
Visual Liquid reward Preferred Left Right
Mean activity
(spikes per s)

8
(spikes per s)

decision 20
6
Activity

Stay Anti-preferred
4 decision 10
Shift 2
0 0
0.5 s on ff q k 0.5 s n ff k
e o Ac bac eo eo ac
Cu Cue ed Cu db
Fe Cu Fe
e

Figure 3 | Neuronal activity in posterior and anterior prefrontal cortex a correct decision. The response to the reward was also dependent on the
Nature Reviews | Neuroscience
of the macaque monkey. a | The monkeys performed a computerized level of conflict that the monkey experienced in the course of achieving
version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (upper panel). In each the reward. This conflict-related response to reward was seen when the
trial, the monkey was presented with a start cue, and if the monkey pushed shape-matching rule was used (middle) but not when the colour-matching
a bar, there was a fixation period followed by the appearance of a sample at rule was effective (right panel). These findings indicate that DLPFC and OFC
the centre of a touchscreen. If the monkey kept pressing the bar and cells represent rules or strategies and convey detailed information about
maintained eye fixation, three test items appeared surrounding the sample, feedback within the task context. b | The monkeys performed a strategy task
and the monkey had to touch one of the test items that matched the sample (upper panel) in which a ‘strategy cue’ indicated whether the monkey
in colour or shape. Cells in lateral and orbital regions of prefrontal cortex should ‘stay’ with the previously made saccade or ‘shift’ to the alternative
conveyed information about the details of the task while the monkeys were saccade direction. Strategy cues could be either visual objects or the
performing the WCST. The bottom row of panels shows the activities of two presence or absence of a liquid reward (juice) at the beginning of each trial
example cells from the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and dorsolateral (bottom left panel). The neuronal activity of a frontopolar cortex cell was
prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). The neuronal activity of the DLPFC neuron (left not modulated by the strategy cues or the actual strategy taken by the
panel) was modulated by the matching rule during the sample presentation monkey or the memory of the previous action but was modulated after a
period. The peri-stimulus time histogram shows the mean activity when the saccade around the feedback time (middle and right bottom panels). The
colour-matching or shape-matching rule was applied in correct trials. dashed vertical line indicates the target acquisition (Acq). Part a left panel
The OFC neuron (middle and right panels) showed anticipatory activity is republished with permission of Society for Neuroscience from REF. 43;
before reward delivery and then a strong increase in activity after the arrival permission conveyed through Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. Part a right
of the reward. However, the OFC cell did not respond to the free reward panel is republished with permission of Society for Neuroscience from
(a reward that was given randomly without performing the matching task), REF. 70; permission conveyed through Copyright Clearance Center, Inc.
indicating that this cell responded to the reward only when it resulted from Part b is from REF. 20, Nature Publishing Group.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 7


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

a b
FPC lesion
Lateral view Orbital view Medial view

Post-operative number of rule shifts as a


percentage of pre-operative number
100

PCC lesion DLPFC lesion ACC lesion OFC lesion

0
l C C C C C
ro FP PC PF AC OF
nt
Co DL

Figure 4 | Behavioural consequences of selective and bilateral lesions within the frontopolar Nature cortex.
ReviewsIn| Neuroscience
the
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), subjects should match visual items based on colour-matching or shape-matching
rules. The relevant rule is frequently changed without any notice, and the subjects should find the rule and its changes by
trial and error. The number of rule shifts in a daily session indicates the ability of monkeys to adapt to unannounced rule
shifts in the WCST. a | This chart shows the number of post-lesion rule shifts as a percentage of the pre-lesion level in
different groups of monkeys. A value of 100 on the vertical axis would mean that there was no change in the number of
rule shifts from preoperative to postoperative testing. The control group refers to the monkeys without any lesions.
Lesions in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) or the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)
impaired the ability of the monkeys to adapt to rule shifts; however, lesions in the frontopolar cortex (FPC) or posterior
cingulate cortex (PCC) had no marked effect on this ability, indicating a dissociation of function between the FPC and the
posterior parts of the PFC (the DLPFC, ACC and OFC). b | The schematics show the extents of the lesions (red) on different
aspects of the frontopolar cortex (upper row) and in other brain regions (lower row). Adapted with permission from REF. 24,
[Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences].

of a rule shift being implemented, the monkeys had to their superior performance. By contrast, control monkeys
perform two trials of an unrelated task (face-detection appeared to judge the free reward as altering the rele-
task) before returning to the WCST (still using the same vance of the current behaviour, thereby prompting the
rule). Whereas the performance of control monkeys exploration of alternative behavioural options24.
on this first trial after returning from the interruption In the second of the frontopolar cortex lesion studies23,
dropped to the chance level, the frontal-pole-lesioned the monkeys performed a series of learning and memory
monkeys still performed markedly better than the con- tasks (TABLE 1), the results of which confirmed that fronto­
trols. This result first shows that monkeys were unable to polar cortex lesions do not impair standard object work-
properly perform the cognitive branching between the ing memory tasks (that is, matching or non-­matching to
two tasks. Second, the diverting (albeit significant) task sample) or standard stimulus-reward associative learning
presumably triggered the control monkeys’ propensity tasks (that is, concurrent object discrimination learn-
to explore the importance of the new task, consequently ing). However, the frontal-pole-lesioned animals were
interfering with the maintenance of the currently relevant impaired (compared with control animals) in the rapid
rule during the interruption. The propensity to explore learning of the relative value of alternatives in three differ-
alternative goals when switching away from the ongo- ent tasks. First, in a standard concurrent object-in‑scene
ing task was probably diminished in lesioned animals24. learning task, the frontal-pole-lesioned animals exhibited
The hypothesis links the frontopolar cortex to redistrib- a highly specific deficit in rapid one-trial learning. That is,
uting cognitive resources over alternative goals, when they did not show the normal marked level of improve-
the current goal is deemed to be no longer relevant. The ment in performance accuracy from the first run through
hypo­thesis further predicts that monkeys with fronto­ a set of novel problems (in which their first choice was a
polar cortex lesions would be less concerned with any self-generated ‘guess’) to the second run (in which normal
events that bring into question the relevance of ongoing control animals show a distinct one-trial learning effect).
behaviour in general. To test this, in another version of This highly specific deficit is not seen after lesions in
the WCST, a free reward (a reward given to the animal other prefrontal regions, thereby further distinguishing
without the necessity to perform a task) was given during the frontopolar cortex from those areas. That the same
the inter-trial interval, after the monkeys reached the shift pattern of deficit specific to rapid early learning about
criterion. Again, the performance of the control animals novel alternatives was also observed in a very simple suc-
dropped to the level of chance in the first subsequent cessive single-problem learning task23 denies the impor-
trial, whereas that of the frontal-pole-­lesioned monkeys tance of scene-based and/or context-based learning for
did not24. Thus, monkeys with lesions in the frontopolar frontopolar cortex function. These selective deficits in
cortex remained focused on the main task, accounting for one-shot learning after frontopolar lesions are consistent

8 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Table 1 | Cognitive functions after selective bilateral lesions in the frontopolar cortex in monkeys
Task Effect of lesion on behaviour Key cognitive processes involved
in behaviour
Object-in‑scene learning Impairment of 1‑trial learning Rapid reinforcement-based learning
Concurrent object discrimination None Slow, gradual reinforcement-based
learning learning
Successive single-problem learning Impairment of 1‑trial learning Rapid reinforcement-based learning
Standard WCST None Rapid switching between
well-established abstract rules
WCST-conflict Enhancement of conflict-induced • Resolving conflict between
behavioural adaptation well-established behavioural rules
• Conflict-induced behavioural
adjustment
WCST-interruption Enhancement of rule memory after Maintaining information of a pending
the interruption task while an alternative task is
performed for successful resumption
of the pending task
WCST-free reward Enhancement of rule memory after Remaining focused on maintaining
the distracting free reward information of the ongoing task
despite extra-task salient events
Delayed matching to sample None Working memory for stimulus
Delayed non-matching to sample None Working memory for stimulus
Learning new abstract rule (‘smaller Failure to learn the new rule in the Learning new rules
than’) first three days
Applying two rules (‘smaller than’ and None • Applying two abstract rules at the
‘same identity’ at the same time) same time
• Combining the result of multiple
cognitive operations
WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The table is based on findings from REFS 23,24.

with the results described in the preceding section from a current task or goal, whereas those in the frontopolar
WCST tasks in that they may similarly reflect impaired cortex may instead be monitoring the environment for
consideration of alternative behavioural options; this is indications that potential alternative goals may be more
because in these one-shot learning tasks, the learning advantageous and, as such, these neurons may come to
rate of the monkeys (which, based on their learning-rate trigger disengagement in favour of a new task or goal
curves, are clearly not perfect learners23) are presumably (FIG. 2) when switch conditions are favourable23,24. Hence,
enhanced if animals learn not only about the value of the this model also proposes a balance between ‘exploitatory’
chosen stimulus in each trial but also about the inferred drive from other frontal areas, which allocate attentional
relative value of the unchosen stimulus. This benefit and control resources in service of the current task or
in rapid acquisition diminishes with problem repeats, goal, and an ‘exploratory’ drive from the frontopolar
as then slower, frontopolar-­independent 23 learning by cortex, which allows redistributing these resources23,24,64
repeated reinforcement comes to predominate. Hence, (FIGS 2,5). Based on activation pattern of frontopolar cortex
frontopolar cortex may therefore provide primates with in imaging studies, other investigators have also suggested
a mechanism for rapid learning when faced with novel that the left lateral human frontopolar cortex is involved
alternatives. Taken together with a significant deficit in the stimulus-driven allocation of attentional resources
in rapid learning of new rules after frontopolar cortex away from a currently attended visual dimension or
lesions, the findings instead point to a general deficit in spatial location to a new dimension or location84,85.
exploring novel behavioural options in the early stages of At first glance, the better-than-control performance
learning 22,23, as previously observed in humans3. In sum- observed in monkeys24 (and humans32) with frontopolar
mary, these findings (TABLE 1) across both studies indicate cortex lesions might appear counterintuitive. However, in
that the frontopolar cortex plays a general and crucial role real-life environments, which may be uncertain, changing
in favouring the exploration of the value of alternative and open-ended, focusing exclusively on optimal perfor-
behavioural options23,24. mances in an ongoing task may be detrimental. In such
environments, the relevance of tasks or goals may be sub-
Frontopolar cortex function in monkeys ject to change according to new external contingencies.
The aforementioned frontopolar cortex lesion stud- For example, imagine a monkey engaged in grooming
ies together suggest the hypothesis (FIG. 5) that neurons her young; it would be maladaptive to allocate all atten-
in prefrontal areas generally interact with each other to tional resources to this task at the expense of potentially
maximise the value or benefit that can be derived from missing cues to predators, food or social opportunities24.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 9


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Arcuate
Principal Anterior prefrontal cortex cortex lesions induce no impairments in the ability to
sulcus
sulcus Disengaging cognitive control from the concurrently consider several different attributes of
current task and redistributing cognitive stimuli or in the ability to implement well-learned com-
resources to other potential goals
existing in the environment. plex rules. We may then reasonably infer that, if these cog-
nitive processes are independent of the integrity of the
Exploration drive frontopolar cortex in monkeys, the human frontopolar
Goal-directed behaviour cortex is unlikely to specifically subserve these processes.
Consequently, these findings provide little support for
Exploitation drive models that assume that the human frontopolar cortex has
Posterior prefrontal cortex an essential role in mnemonic and particularly working
Recruiting and implementing the memory functions, in establishing and shifting between
Lateral cognitive control to optimize cognitive sets or in relational integration or integration of
sulcus the performance of the current task
the outcome of multiple cognitive processes.
Second, studies in monkeys support the idea that
Nature Reviews | Neuroscience
Figure 5 | Functional model of the frontopolar cortex in monkeys. We propose that the function of the frontopolar cortex is to redistribute the
the frontopolar cortex (area 10 cortex, depicted in red colour) and posterior parts of the allocation of attentional and executive resources away
prefrontal cortex (including dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices, from a default behavioural strategy, which is driving
orbitofrontal cortex, area 8 within the peri-arcuate region and the anterior cingulate ongoing behaviour. This function is likely to be inher-
cortex) have complementary but dissociable roles in adjusting the distribution of ited in the human frontopolar cortex. This may explain
cognitive control. In this model, there is a balance between the ‘exploitation’ drive from the critical involvement of the human frontopolar
the posterior parts of the prefrontal cortex and the ‘exploration’ drive from the
cortex in cognitive abilities such as mind-wandering,
frontopolar cortex that limits the focus on the current task and redistributes some
cognitive resources to other potential goals. To redistribute cognitive resources for the planning, abstract reasoning, multitasking and cog-
exploration of other potential tasks and goals, the frontopolar cortex may construct and nitive branching, which require switching away from
monitor an estimated value for the relevance of the current behavioural strategy based an ongoing behavioural option, considering multiple
on posterior prefrontal representations that control this strategy and trigger exploration behavioural options or exploring new ones.
when the strategy is deemed irrelevant. Adapted with permission from REF. 24, Third, a striking finding was that frontal-pole-lesioned
[Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences]. macaques perform better than controls when returning to
an interrupted task after being diverted by behaviourally
meaningful events (rewards) or the execution of a sec-
Thus, evolutionary pressure may have favoured the ondary task24. Interestingly, this behavioural paradigm
development of an additional system centred on involves cognitive branching (TABLE 1) and suggests that,
the frontopolar cortex to redistribute and limit allocation unlike humans, monkeys have difficulty properly per-
of attentional/control resources to a single endeavour 23,24 forming true cognitive branching. Although the finding
(FIGS 2,5). Such a strategic balance between exploitation reveals a critical role of the macaque frontopolar cortex
and exploration may be a fundamental aspect of adaptive in redistributing cognitive resources and in exploring
behaviour, particularly in socially advanced species6,46,86,87. options beyond the ongoing behavioural strategy 24, it
suggests that normal monkeys are unable to hold relevant
Human frontopolar function information and simply resume the task after completing
What can be learned about human frontopolar cortex or exploring other options. This is in contrast with human
function from monkey studies? Interspecies compari- cognition, in which branching is an inherent aspect of
sons often require analysis of subtle differences. This is goal-directed behaviour. Cognitive branching in humans
particularly true in the case of the anterior prefrontal is specifically associated with activation of a region in the
cortex, for which there is evidence of anatomical differ- lateral frontopolar cortex (FIG. 1a) that appears not to exist
ences between monkeys and humans1,25. Nevertheless, in monkeys25. In contrast to monkeys, humans with lateral
some reasonable inferences can be made from monkey frontopolar lesions show impaired performance in cogni-
studies about human frontopolar cortex function. tive branching 28. Thus, lesion studies in monkeys, human
First, the monkey studies found that, unlike the imaging studies and comparative-anatomical studies pro-
multi­faceted neuronal representations in posterior pre- vide convergent evidence that a higher capability for cog-
frontal and medial frontal regions, frontal pole cell activ- nitive branching may have emerged in human evolution
ity appears to be unrelated to the prospective memory of in association with the enlargement and development of
cued upcoming actions, to the retrospective memory the lateral frontopolar cortex 1,5,6.
of previously performed actions or to the formation of
and shifting between cognitive sets, even though all Human models
these components were required in the tasks the animals Recent studies combining computational modelling,
performed. Moreover, lesions in monkeys have revealed behavioural tests and fMRI experiments have proposed
that, in contrast to posterior lesions, those in the fronto­ a model that describes arbitration processes between
polar cortex induce no notable impairments in working exploitation and exploration behaviours (FIGS 2,6) and
memory for task rules, selective attention to ongoing formalizes aspects of the evolution of the frontal pole
task rules, inhibition of previously relevant rules, or the function from monkeys to humans5,6,87. This model dis-
assessment of behavioural outcomes to detect and adapt tinguishes two arbitration systems. The first is a basic
to unpredictable rule changes. Furthermore, frontopolar system that monitors online the relevance (‘absolute

10 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

reliability’, in the model terminology) of the ongoing keep track of and test several behavioural hypotheses
behavioural strategy (that is, the ‘actor’) and triggers simultaneously and, as a special case, perform cognitive
undirected exploration when this strategy is deemed branching.
irrelevant. Relevance is inferred from the predictability Unlike other computationally meaningful adaptive
of action outcomes and the occurrence of contextual behaviour models, the model integrating the two sys-
cues. Undirected exploration is conceived as the emer- tems accounts for human sequential choices in uncertain,
gence of a new cognitive set that serves as the actor. This changing, recurrent or open-ended environments4–6,87,88.
is initially built from long-term memory of previously Furthermore, fMRI studies in humans reveal that mon-
learned, contextually relevant strategies and is subse- itoring the relevance of strategies based on the predict-
quently adjusted to external contingencies. The rele- ability of action outcomes is associated with anterior
vance of this cognitive set is monitored online and may prefrontal activations, with medial ones involved in
eventually be consolidated in long-term memory, when monitoring the actor strategy and lateral ones in mon-
it is deemed relevant. itoring alternative strategies5,6. Intriguingly, this latter
The second is an add‑on system that further mon- activation is located in the frontopolar cortex region pre-
itors online the relevance of a few alternative behav- viously identified as subserving cognitive branching 4–6,
ioural strategies. These strategies were previously used which may have no homologue in monkeys25. Consistent
as relevant-­a ctor strategies but were subsequently with these findings, there is convergent evidence from
deemed irrelevant. Critically, this system allows one of functional and morphometric MRI, neuro­psychological
these alternative strategies to replace the current-actor examination of patients, as well as transcranial magnetic
strategy when the latter is deemed irrelevant (by the stimulation studies89–91, that human subjective reports
basic system) and the former is deemed relevant again about their confidence in chosen actions is associated
(by the add‑on system) (FIG. 6). The add‑on system thus with anterior prefrontal function. Moreover, single-cell
enables directed exploration — that is, the ability to activity recorded in the monkey frontopolar cortex follow-
ing action choices and preceding positive outcomes19–21
also suggests that the frontopolar cortex monitors the
Premotor relevance of the actor strategy 4 and supports human con-
Medial frontal
fidence judgements regarding current behaviour. These
Lateral prefrontal
results, together with those of macaque studies reviewed
above, thus suggest that the monkey frontopolar cortex
Frontopolar subserves the basic system that monitors the relevance of
the current behavioural strategy and arbitrates between
pSMA exploiting this strategy and undirected exploration of
Undirected exploration alternative ones. The human lateral frontopolar cortex
Medial frontopolar cortex further features a new functional region that may support,
Monitoring the current goal through interaction with other regions, the add‑on system
for possibly redistributing that enables directed exploration of concurrent alternative
cognitive control resources
to other potential goals strategies5,6,87. If this is the case, then this function may
explain the activation of the lateral frontopolar cortex in
Directed exploration
the various cognitive paradigms reviewed above, such as
Lateral frontopolar cortex
Monitoring a few alternative tasks/ exploration, cognitive branching, abstract reasoning and
goals for possibly re-engaging one as problem-solving.
replacement of the current task/goal
Exploitation
Conclusions and future directions
Posterior prefrontal cortex
Recruiting and implementing cognitive control to We propose that there is a balance between an ‘exploita-
optimize the performance of the current goal tory’ drive from the posterior parts of the prefrontal cor-
Goal-directed
behaviour tex, which allocates cognitive resources to an ongoing
task, and an ‘exploratory’ drive from the frontopolar cor-
Nature Reviews | Neuroscience tex, which promotes the redistribution of such resources
Figure 6 | Functional model of the frontopolar cortex in humans. We propose that away from the current task, when other potential goals
the monkey frontopolar cortex function has evolved in humans through the might appear behaviourally more relevant as a result of
development of a lateral frontopolar cortex sector. Accordingly, the function of the changes in the environment (FIGS 5,6). Modulating the
monkey frontopolar cortex, mainly involved in monitoring the relevance of the current balance between exploratory and exploitatory drives is a
task or goal and regulating the redistribution of cognitive resources away from this task shared cognitive challenge for humans and non-­human
or goal according to changes in internal and external contingencies, and referred to as
primates and is a core function in the regulation of for-
undirected exploration, is inherited by the medial sector of human frontopolar cortex
(red). The lateral sector of human frontopolar cortex (yellow) is associated with the
aging and appetitive behaviours with direct relevance
development of a more elaborated exploration function, referred to as directed to survival and fitness (FIG. 2). However, the volume of
exploration, monitoring the relevance of a few alternative tasks or goals and the the frontopolar cortex in humans surpasses that of the
opportunity to redistribute cognitive resources towards one of these. As in monkeys, corresponding areas in monkeys1,17,92,93 (FIG. 1a), prob-
the posterior prefrontal cortex (blue) mainly serves to control the execution of the ably enabling unique cognitive functions that are inac-
current task or goal according to the context in which the subject is acting cessible to non-human primates but might nonetheless
(exploitation). pSMA, pre-supplementary motor area. be underscored by the frontal pole’s contribution to the

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 11


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

management of competing goals. Indeed, the fronto­ have evolved to acquire additional cognitive capacities for
polar cortex has already been linked with several argu- the directed exploration of concurrent alternative strate-
ably uniquely human forms of intelligent behaviour, but gies (FIGS 2,5,6). Our model stresses interactions between
this new view that the frontopolar cortex supports the the frontopolar cortex and posterior cortical areas (FIG. 2).
management of competing goals may offer a unique uni- Recent studies indicate that alterations in frontopolar
fying perspective. For example, reasoning requires men- cortical activation are associated with functional impair-
tal consideration of competing abstract thoughts and ment in neuropsychiatric disorders94. Testing the role of
projections. Even episodic memory and its associated the frontopolar cortex in humans and monkeys using
introspection and ‘mental time travel’86 are now defined similar cognitive tasks while recording from multiple
specifically with respect to future planning, the enhance- interacting areas, in both the presence and absence of
ment of which may involve representing and simulating interventions using newly developed techniques95,96, will
or considering imagined future alternatives to plan most likely herald important future insights regarding how
effectively. Whether the human frontopolar cortex con- neuropsychiatric diseases like schizophrenia, bipolar dis-
tains a subregion that has no analogue in monkeys, as order and obsessive compulsive disorder 97–99 and related
indicated by imaging studies25, is also highly intriguing, behavioural deficits stem from dysfunctional interacting
but this issue ultimately requires further clarification frontal regions.
through studies that do not confound state (resting versus The complexity of a changing environment, par-
anaesthetized) across species, as is currently the case, and ticularly for species with advanced social behaviour, is
ideally that compare functional connectivity during the likely to have led to strong evolutionary pressure for
performance of similar cognitive tasks. the development of an alternative neural system for the
We argue that understanding the role of the fronto­ disengagement and redistribution of executive control
polar cortex in non-human primates provides key between potential options and goals. This in turn might
insights into fundamental aspects of primate cognition. have led to the emergence of higher-level cognitive abili-
The exploratory role of the frontopolar cortex in mon- ties that underlie advanced intelligence and the creativity
keys may have been preserved in humans but may also of the human mind.

1. Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., 11. Petrides, M. & Pandya, D. N. Efferent association 23. Boschin, E. A., Piekema, C. & Buckley, M. J. Essential
Zilles, K. & Van Hoesen, G. W. Prefrontal cortex in pathways from the rostral prefrontal cortex in the functions of primate frontopolar cortex in cognition.
humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 27, 11573–11586 Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, E1020–E1027
Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 114, 224–241 (2001). (2007). (2015).
2. Ramnani, N. & Owen, A. M. Anterior prefrontal cortex: 12. Dumontheil, I. Development of abstract thinking This is a pioneering study investigating the
insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. during childhood and adolescence: the role of behavioural consequences of lesions of the
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 184–194 (2004). rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. frontopolar cortex in monkeys.
This comprehensive review proposes that the 10, 57–76 (2014). 24. Mansouri, F. A., Buckley, M. J., Mahboubi, M. &
frontopolar cortex is involved in integrating the 13. Dumontheil, I., Burgess, P. W. & Blakemore, S. J. Tanaka, K. Behavioral consequences of selective
outcomes of multiple cognitive processes in humans. Development of rostral prefrontal cortex and damage to frontal pole and posterior cingulate
3. Burgess, P. W., Gilbert, S. J. & Dumontheil, I. Function cognitive and behavioural disorders. Dev. Med. Child cortices. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112,
and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area Neurol. 50, 168–181 (2008). E3940–E3949 (2015).
10). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 14. Burman, K. J., Lui, L. L., Rosa, M. G. & Bourne, J. A. This is another pioneering study investigating the
887–899 (2007). Development of non-phosphorylated neurofilament behavioural consequences of lesions of the
This comprehensive review proposes that the role protein expression in neurones of the New World frontopolar cortex in monkeys.
of frontopolar cortex can be better explained by monkey dorsolateral frontal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25. Neubert, F. X., Mars, R. B., Thomas, A. G., Sallet, J. &
‘gateway’ and ‘prospective memory’ hypotheses. 25, 1767–1779 (2007). Rushworth, M. F. Comparison of human ventral frontal
4. Koechlin, E. Frontal pole function: what is specifically 15. Sowell, E. R. et al. Longitudinal mapping of cortical cortex areas for cognitive control and language with
human? Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 241 (2011). thickness and brain growth in normal children. areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron 81, 700–713
This study presents the first account relating J. Neurosci. 24, 8223–8231 (2004). (2014).
monkey and human frontopolar function. 16. Miller, D. J. et al. Prolonged myelination in human This study shows evidence that a region in the
5. Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S. & neocortical evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, lateral sector of the human frontopolar cortex has
Grafman, J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex 16480–16485 (2012). no counterpart in monkeys.
in human cognition. Nature 399, 148–151 (1999). 17. Ongür, D., Ferry, A. T. & Price, J. L. Architectonic 26. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of
This is a pioneering fMRI study that revealed the subdivision of the human orbital & medial prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24,
specific role of the frontopolar cortex in cognitive prefrontal cortex. Comp. J. Neurol. 460, 425–449 167 (2001).
branching. (2003). This study provides an explanation of the role of
6. Donoso, M., Collins, A. G. & Koechlin, E. Human 18. Mitz, A. R., Tsujimoto, S., Maclarty, A. J. & Wise, S. P. prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and flexibility.
cognition. Foundations of human reasoning in the A method for recording single-cell activity in the 27. Hoffmann, M. & Bar‑On, R. Isolated frontopolar cortex
prefrontal cortex. Science 344, 1481–1486 (2014). frontal-pole cortex of macaque monkeys. J. Neurosci. lesion: a case study. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 25, 50–56
This computational and fMRI study reveals the Methods 177, 60–66 (2009). (2012).
critical role of anterior prefrontal regions in 19. Tsujimoto, S. & Genovesio, A. Firing variability of 28. Dreher, J. C., Koechlin, E., Tierney, M. & Grafman, J.
monitoring the relevance of current and alternative frontal pole neurons during a cued strategy task. Damage to the fronto-polar cortex is associated with
behavioural strategies and arbitrating between them. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 25–36 (2017). impaired multitasking. PLoS ONE 3, e3227 (2008).
7. Bludau, S. et al. Cytoarchitecture, probability maps 20. Tsujimoto, S., Genovesio, A. & Wise, S. P. Evaluating 29. Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D. & Burgess, P. W. Involvement
and functions of the human frontal pole. Neuroimage self-generated decisions in frontal pole cortex of of rostral prefrontal cortex in selection between
93 (Pt 2), 260–275 (2014). monkeys. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 120–126 stimulus-oriented and stimulus-independent thought.
8. Burman, K. J. et al. Subcortical projections to the (2010). Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 1423–1431 (2005).
frontal pole in the marmoset monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. This pioneering study describes the activity of 30. Gilbert, S. J., Simons, J. S., Frith, C. D. &
34, 303–319 (2011). frontopolar cortex cells in monkeys performing Burgess, P. W. Performance-related activity in medial
9. Burman, K. J., Reser, D. H., Yu, H. H. & cognitive tasks. rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low-demand
Rosa, M. G. P. Cortical input to the frontal pole of the 21. Tsujimoto, S., Genovesio, A. & Wise, S. P. Neuronal tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 32,
marmoset monkey. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1712–1737 activity during a cued strategy task: comparison of 45–58 (2006).
(2011). dorsolateral, orbital, and polar prefrontal cortex. 31. Gilbert, S. J. et al. Functional specialization within
This paper is a comprehensive investigation and review J. Neurosci. 32, 11017–11031 (2012). rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta-analysis.
of the cortical connections of the frontopolar cortex. 22. Boschin, E. A. & Buckley, M. J. Differential J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 932–948 (2006).
10. Barbas, H. & Pandya, D. N. Architecture and intrinsic contributions of dorsolateral and frontopolar cortices 32. Rowe, J. B. et al. Is the prefrontal cortex necessary for
connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus to working memory processes in the primate. establishing cognitive sets? J. Neurosci. 27,
monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 286, 353–375 (1989). Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, 144 (2015). 13303–13310 (2007).

12 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

33. Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L. & 55. Badre, D., Doll, B. B., Long, N. M. & Frank, M. J. 80. Mansouri, F. A., Tanaka, K. & Buckley, M. J. Conflict-
Raichle, M. E. Medial prefrontal cortex and self- Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex and individual induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the
referential mental activity: relation to a default mode differences in uncertainty-driven exploration. Neuron executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. Nat. Rev.
of brain function. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 73, 595–607 (2012). Neurosci. 10, 141–152 (2009).
4259–4264 (2001). 56. Ganis, G., Kosslyn, S. M., Stose, S., Thompson, W. L. & 81. Yoshida, W. & Ishii, S. Resolution of uncertainty in
34. Mason, M. F. et al. Wandering minds: the default Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. Neural correlates of different prefrontal cortex. Neuron 50, 781–789 (2006).
network and stimulus-independent thought. Science types of deception: an fMRI investigation. Cereb. 82. Nieder, A. Coding of abstract quantity by ‘number neurons’
315, 393–395 (2007). Cortex 13, 830–836 (2003). of the primate brain. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens.
This study shows that some brain regions, including 57. Karim, A. A. et al. The truth about lying: inhibition of Neural Behav. Physiol. 199, 1–16 (2013).
the frontopolar cortex, show higher activation the anterior prefrontal cortex improves deceptive 83. Buckley, M. J. et al. Dissociable components of rule-
levels during rest periods, which correlates with the behavior. Cereb. Cortex 20, 205–213 (2010). guided behavior depend on distinct medial and
level of mind-wandering. 58. Moll, J., Eslinger, P. J. & Oliveira-Souza, R. prefrontal regions. Science 325, 52–58 (2009).
35. Christoff, K., Ream, J. M., Geddes, L. P. & Frontopolar and anterior temporal cortex activation in 84. Pollmann, S. Switching between dimensions, locations,
Gabrieli, J. D. Evaluating self-generated information: a moral judgment task: preliminary functional MRI and responses: the role of the left frontopolar cortex.
anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. results in normal subjects. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 59, Neuroimage 14, S118–124 (2001).
Behav. Neurosci. 117, 1161–1168 (2003). 657–664 (2001). 85. Pollmann, S. Frontopolar resource allocation in human
36. Dobbins, I. G., Foley, H., Schacter, D. L. & 59. Sommer, M. et al. Neural correlates of true and false and nonhuman primates. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20,
Wagner, A. D. Executive control during episodic belief reasoning. Neuroimage 35, 1378–1384 (2007). 84–86 (2016).
retrieval: multiple prefrontal processes subserve 60. Ray, K. L. et al. Co‑activation based parcellation of the This paper proposes that frontopolar cortex is involved
source memory. Neuron 35, 989–996 (2002). human frontal pole. Neuroimage 123, 200–211 in allocation of attention between various tasks.
37. Bunge, S. A., Burrows, B. & Wagner, A. D. Prefrontal (2015). 86. Raja Beharelle, A., Polanía, R., Hare, T. A. & Ruff, C. C.
and hippocampal contributions to visual associative 61. Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J. D. & Transcranial stimulation over frontopolar cortex
recognition: interactions between cognitive control Mattingley, J. B. Understanding the minds of others: a elucidates the choice attributes and neural
and episodic retrieval. Brain Cogn. 56, 141–152 neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. mechanisms used to resolve exploration-exploitation
(2004). 65, 276–291 (2016). trade-offs. J. Neurosci. 35, 14544–14556 (2015).
38. Burgess, P. W., Gonen-Yaacovi, G. & Volle, E. 62. Sakai, K. & Passingham, R. E. Prefrontal set activity 87. Koechlin, E. An evolutionary computational theory of
Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective predicts rule-specific neural processing during prefrontal executive function in decision-making. Philos.
memory: what have we learnt so far? subsequent cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 26, Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 369, 20130474 (2014).
Neuropsychologia 49, 2246–2257 (2011). 1211–1218 (2006). This paper contains the first theory formalizing the
39. Benoit, R. G., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D. & 63. Benoit, R. G. The role of rostral prefrontal cortex in evolution of the prefrontal function in mammals from
Burgess, P. W. Rostral prefrontal cortex and the focus establishing cognitive sets: preparation or rodents to humans.
of attention in prospective memory. Cereb. Cortex 22, coordination? J. Neurosci. 28, 3259–3261 (2008). 88. Wan, X., Cheng, K. & Tanaka, K. The neural system of
1876–1886 (2012). 64. Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B. & postdecision evaluation in rostral frontal cortex during
40. Okuda, J. et al. Thinking of the future and past: the Dolan, R. J. Cortical substrates for exploratory problem-solving tasks. eNeuro http://dx.doi.
roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal decisions in humans. Nature 441, 876–879 (2006). org/10.1523/ENEURO.0188-16.2016 (2016).
lobes. Neuroimage 19, 1369–1380 (2003). 65. Dagher, A., Owen, A. M., Boecker, H. & Brooks, D. J. 89. Rahnev, D. et al. Causal evidence for frontal cortex
41. Volle, E., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., Costello Ade, L., Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET organization for perceptual decision making. Proc.
Gilbert, S. J. & Burgess, P. W. The role of rostral activation study with the Tower of London task. Brain Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 6059–6064 (2016).
prefrontal cortex in prospective memory: a voxel-based 122, 1973–1987 (1999). 90. Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Nagy, Z., Dolan, R. J. & Rees, G.
lesion study. Neuropsychologia 49, 2185–2198 66. Nitschke, K., Kostering, L., Finkel, L., Weiller, C. & Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in
(2011). Kaller, C. P. A. Meta-analysis on the neural basis of brain structure. Science 329, 1541–1543 (2010).
42. Umeda, S., Kurosaki, Y., Terasawa, Y., Kato, M. & planning: activation likelihood estimation of functional 91. Fleming, S. M., Ryu, J., Golfinos, J. G. &
Miyahara, Y. Deficits in prospective memory following brain imaging results in the Tower of London task. Blackmon, K. E. Domain-specific impairment in
damage to the prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 396–413 (2016). metacognitive accuracy following anterior prefrontal
49, 2178–2184 (2011). 67. Mushiake, H., Saito, N., Sakamoto, K., Itoyama, Y. & lesions. Brain 137, 2811–2822 (2014).
43. Mansouri, F. A., Matsumoto, K. & Tanaka, K. Tanji, J. Activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex reflects 92. Chaplin, T. A., Yu, H. H., Soares, J. G., Gattass, R. &
Prefrontal cell activities related to monkeys’ success multiple steps of future events in action plans. Neuron Rosa, M. G. P. A conserved pattern of differential
and failure in adapting to rule changes in a Wisconsin 50, 631–641 (2006). expansion of cortical areas in simian primates.
Card Sorting Test analog. J. Neurosci. 26, 68. Roca, M. et al. The role of Area 10 (BA10) in human J. Neurosci. 33, 15120–15125 (2013).
2745–2756 (2006). multitasking and in social cognition: a lesion study. 93. Takizawa, R. et al. Reduced frontopolar activation
44. Mansouri, F. A., Rosa, M. G. P. & Atapour, N. Working Neuropsychologia 49, 3525–3531 (2011). during verbal fluency task in schizophrenia: a multi-
memory in the service of executive control functions. 69. Boorman, E. D., Behrens, T. E. & Rushworth, M. F. channel near-infrared spectroscopy study. Schizophr.
Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, 166 (2015). Counterfactual choice and learning in a neural Res. 99, 250–262 (2008).
45. Mansouri, F. A., Buckley, M. J. & Tanaka, K. network centered on human lateral frontopolar cortex. 94. Suddendorf, T., Addis, D. R. & Corballis, M. C. Mental
Mnemonic function of the dorsolateral prefrontal PLoS Biol. 9, e1001093 (2011). time travel and the shaping of the human mind. Phil.
cortex in conflict-induced behavioral adjustment. 70. Mansouri, F. A., Buckley, M. J. & Tanaka, K. The Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1317–1324 (2009).
Science 318, 987–990 (2007). essential role of primate orbitofrontal cortex in 95. Eldridge, M. A. et al. Chemogenetic disconnection of
46. Fuster, J. M. Upper processing stages of the conflict-induced executive control adjustment. monkey orbitofrontal and rhinal cortex reversibly disrupts
perception-action cycle. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 143–145 J. Neurosci. 34, 11016–11031 (2014). reward value. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 37–39 (2016).
(2004). 71. Mansouri, F. A., Egner, T. & Buckley, M. J. Monitoring 96. Whissell, P. D., Tohyama, S. & Martin, L. J. The use of
47. Braver, T. S. & Bongiolatti, S. R. The role of demands for executive control: shared functions DREADDs to deconstruct behavior. Front. Genet. 7, 70
frontopolar cortex in subgoal processing during between human and nonhuman primates. Trends (2016).
working memory. Neuroimage 15, 523–536 Neurosci. 40, 15–27 (2017). 97. Schultz, C. C. et al. Reduced cortical thickness in first
(2002). 72. Lee, D., Rushworth, M. F., Walton, M. E., Watanabe, M. episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 116, 204–209
48. Kim, C., Kroger, J. K., Calhoun, V. D. & Clark, V. P. The & Sakagami, M. Functional specialization of the (2010).
role of the frontopolar cortex in manipulation of primate frontal cortex during decision making. 98. Jogia, J., Dima, D., Kumari, V. & Frangou, S.
integrated information in working memory. Neurosci. J. Neurosci. 27, 8170–8173 (2007). Frontopolar cortical inefficiency may underpin reward
Lett. 595, 25–29 (2015). 73. Watanabe, M. & Sakagami, M. Integration of cognitive and working memory dysfunction in bipolar disorder.
49. Volle, E., Gilbert, S. J., Benoit, R. G. & Burgess, P. W. and motivational context information in the primate World J. Biol. Psychiatry 13, 605–615 (2012).
Specialization of the rostral prefrontal cortex for distinct prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17 (Suppl. 1), 99. den Braber, A. et al. Brain activation during cognitive
analogy processes. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2647–2659 101–109 (2007). planning in twins discordant or concordant for obsessive-
(2010). 74. Tanji, J. & Hoshi, E. Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex compulsive symptoms. Brain 133, 3123–3140 (2010).
50. Dusek, J. A. & Eichenbaum, H. The hippocampus and in executive behavioral control. Physiol. Rev. 88,
memory for orderly stimulus relations. Proc. Natl 37–57 (2008). Acknowledgements
Acad. Sci. USA 94, 7109–7114 (1997). 75. Tsutsui, K., Grabenhorst, F., Kobayashi, S. & We would like to thank Keiji Tanaka (at RIKEN Brain Science
51. Wendelken, C. & Bunge, S. A. Transitive inference: Schultz, W. A dynamic code for economic object Institute) for his contribution to our proposed functional
distinct contributions of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex valuation in prefrontal cortex neurons. Nat. Commun. model of the frontopolar cortex in monkeys. We would like to
and the hippocampus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 7, 12554 (2016). thank R. Tweedale for editorial suggestions..
837–847 (2010). 76. Funahashi, S. & Takeda, K. Information processes in the
52. Green, A. E., Fugelsang, J. A., Kraemer, D. J., primate prefrontal cortex in relation to working memory Author contributions
Shamosh, N. A. & Dunbar, K. N. Frontopolar cortex processes. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 313–345 (2002). The authors all wrote the article, and all authors researched
mediates abstract integration in analogy. Brain Res. 77. Wang, L. et al. Differential roles of delay-period neural data for it. The authors all made a substantial contribution to
1096, 125–137 (2006). activity in the monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the discussion of content and reviewed and/or edited the man-
53. Urbanski, M. et al. Reasoning by analogy requires visual-haptic crossmodal working memory. Proc. Natl uscript before submission.
the left frontal pole: lesion-deficit mapping and Acad. Sci. USA 112, E214–E219 (2015).
clinical implications. Brain 139, 1783–1799 78. Schultz, W., O’Neill, M., Tobler, P. N. & Kobayashi, S. Competing interests statement
(2016). Neuronal signals for reward risk in frontal cortex. The authors declare no competing interests.
54. Bahlmann, J., Blumenfeld, R. S. & D’Esposito, M. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1239, 109–117 (2011).
The rostro-caudal axis of frontal cortex Is sensitive to 79. Wallis, J. D., Anderson, K. C. & Miller, E. K. Single Publisher’s note
the domain of stimulus information. Cereb. Cortex 25, neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional
1815–1826 (2015). Nature 411, 953–936 (2001). claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 13


©
2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.

You might also like