You are on page 1of 17

REVIEWS

The neuronal code for number


Andreas Nieder
Abstract | Humans and non-human primates share an elemental quantification system that
resides in a dedicated neural network in the parietal and frontal lobes. In this cortical network,
‘number neurons’ encode the number of elements in a set, its cardinality or numerosity,
irrespective of stimulus appearance across sensory motor systems, and from both spatial and
temporal presentation arrays. After numbers have been extracted from sensory input, they need
to be processed to support goal-directed behaviour. Studying number neurons provides insights
into how information is maintained in working memory and transformed in tasks that require
rule-based decisions. Beyond an understanding of how cardinal numbers are encoded, number
processing provides a window into the neuronal mechanisms of high-level brain functions.

Ordinal numbers
Number rules the universe. Pythagoras and wild animals (insects9,10, fish11,12, amphibians13,14,
Numbers that relate to the birds15,16 and mammals) exploit set size to arrive at adap-
empirical property of ‘rank’ One of the central concepts we use in thinking about tive decisions17–22. Three different processes, subitizing23,
in a sequence (for example, the world is the number concept. The number concept number estimation (also known as analogue magnitude
‘fifth place’).
encompasses cardinal numbers, which are the focus representation) and a texture-like mechanism are thought
Nominal numbers of this Review, ordinal numbers and nominal numbers1,2. to underlie the (nonverbal) representation of number
Strictly linguistic labels to Cardinality (also known as numerosity) corresponds to in humans and other animals24. Collectively, these find-
identify objects (for example, the empirical property of quantity, and is the number of ings argue that humans share a nonverbal quantification
‘Bus number 5’). countable elements in a given group (for example, five system with non-human animals.
Number estimation
runners). From basic needs, such as finding enough food This quantification system resides in a dedicated
(Also known as the analogue items, to more advanced requirements, such as calculat- parieto-frontal brain network in primates25,26. Over the
magnitude system and the ing the flight trajectories of spacecraft, survival depends past 15 years, studies in non-human primates have iden-
analogue number system). on processing numerical quantity. tified cellular mechanisms that give rise to numerical
A process of representing
Numbers are intriguing because they are abstract competence. This Review discusses how single ‘number
small and large set sizes that
becomes systematically less representations. When assessing numerosity, the sensory neurons’ in selected regions of the primate association
precise with increasing appearance of the elements is meaningless. For example, cortices encode the number of elements in a stimulus.
numbers. Thus, number three fingers, three calls and three hand movements can Beyond a mere understanding of how cardinal numbers
estimation obeys Weber’s law. all be classified by the cardinal number ‘three’. Numbers are represented, the investigation of number neurons
Subitizing
are also fascinating because they can be logically trans- elucidates the processing and transformation of numer-
(Also known as object file formed according to a finite set of rules; for example, two ical information in tasks that require cognitive control
representation or object plus two equals four. Such simple and many more com- as a pre­adaptation for arithmetic.
tracking system). The rapid plex operations are the building blocks of arithmetic, the
tracking for up to
elementary branch of mathematics. Numerical opera- Neurons represent abstract number
approximately four items
by assigning ‘files’ or ‘pointers’ tions require mental feats such as abstraction, memori- Coding across space, time and modality. Determining
to individual items. zation, rule following and decision making. Therefore, whether number representations are abstract or are
number processing provides a means to decipher the influenced by non-numerical variables is notoriously
neuronal mechanisms of cognitive control functions. difficult because number is intrinsically correlated with
Several lines of evidence suggest that number many other features of a physical stimulus. Varying the
Animal Physiology Unit, estimation is ubiquitous in the animal kingdom and number of items in a set inevitably changes spatial and/
Institute of Neurobiology, deeply rooted in human ancestry. First, numerate or temporal parameters, and fixing one parameter causes
University of Tübingen, humans who are presented with stimuli that are dis- another to vary with the number of items. For example,
Auf der Morgenstelle 28,
72076 Tübingen, Germany.
played too briefly to be counted can still estimate the total area covered by all items in an array can only be
andreas.nieder@ numerosity 3,4. Second, innumerate humans who have fixed if the size of the individual items is decreased for
uni-tuebingen.de never learned to count verbally are still able to process each item that is added to the array. Responses made by a
doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.40 numerosity 5,6. Third, prelinguistic infants can discrim- neuron or a subject to such an addition could in fact rep-
Published online 6 May 2016 inate the number of objects7,8, and, last, both trained resent changing item size rather than number. The best

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 1


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

way to tackle this problem is to control — unbeknown (visual or auditory) BOLD activation during numerosity
to the individual — one parameter after another in sepa- estimation has been observed in a right fronto-parietal
rate stimulus configurations. If a neuron (and the subject) network39. The same network was also identified dur-
responds equally to systematically varied (that is, con- ing verbal counting of visual and auditory items, with
trolled) numerosity stimuli, it is safe to conclude that the additional activation in prefrontal, parietal and premotor
neuron responds to number. Therefore, one important areas39. In contrast to monkeys, the human parietal lobe
past research agenda was to test the activity of neurons seems to be recruited more strongly with age and num-
to various number stimulus formats. ber proficiency 40,41, giving rise to supramodal (spoken or
In monkeys trained to judge cardinal values (FIG. 1a), written) and notation-independent (numerals or number
neurons in the lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) and the words) symbolic number activation42.
intraparietal sulcus (IPS) of the posterior parietal cor- The idea that numerical representations are abstract
tex (PPC) responded selectively to a specific number of has previously been rejected as premature based on the
items (that is, numerosity) in visual multiple-dot dis- behavioural differences that are found for the processing
plays27–31 (FIG. 1b,c), including a numerosity of zero32,33. of different number formats and number task-specific
Number neurons respond most strongly to their pre- BOLD responses43. Whether the issue of abstract rep-
ferred numbers, but they also respond to a lesser extent resentation can be resolved on the basis of behavioural
to adjacent numbers, and thus they have a bell-shaped outcome effects and the methodological limitations
(Gaussian) response function. The visual appearance of of BOLD signals is a question in itself 44. However, the
stimulus displays, such as the spatial arrangement, the idea of the abstractness of numbers does gain support if
density or the total area of the dots, had no effect on neu- ‘abstract representation’ is operationalized as “neuronal
ronal activity in the studies described above, confirming populations that code numerical quantity and are insensi-
that the neurons represented numerosity rather than the tive to the form of input in which the numerical informa-
co‑varying sensory features of the displays. tion was presented” (REF. 43). Number neuron responses
Number neurons not only signal the numerosity in in the PFC were generalized across spatial features in
simultaneously displayed visual items but also encode visual item arrays27,29,31, spatiotemporal visual presenta-
the number of elements that appear sequentially one by tion formats34, visuo-auditory presentation formats38, and
one, irrespective of the temporal intervals between the the number of dots and associated number signs45. That
elements34 (FIG. 1d). In functional MRI (fMRI), the acti- the extraction of numerosity is only minimally influ-
vation of number neurons is mirrored by bilateral blood enced by the processing of physical stimulus features
oxygen level dependent (BOLD) activation in the human has also recently been demonstrated in human imaging
IPS during numerosity estimation in both spatial (item studies46,47. Of course, if neurons operate on lower, more
arrays) and temporal (sequential items) modes of dis- sensory levels of the cortical hierarchy, such generalized
play 35. Sequential processing is particularly important responses cannot (and should not) be expected for all
in the motor system, and cells in the superior parietal selective neurons. Abstract number information could
lobule (SPL; area 5) are tuned to different numbers of also be extracted from population activity.
hand movements36. Transient pharmacological inactiva-
tion of neuronal activity in parietal area 5 led to individ- Computational advantage of abstract number neurons.
ual movements being omitted in monkeys that had been Before the first electrophysiological recordings of num-
trained to perform between one and five hand move- ber neurons, the influential triple-code model of number
ments, thus preventing the monkeys from performing processing 48 proposed that number representations
the correct number of movements37. Controls showed initially involve a lower step of modality-specific analy-
that the errors were not caused by motor deficits or an sis, followed by a higher processing stage in which these
impaired ability to select between actions. This finding representations reach an abstract, amodal format. The
demonstrates that the activity of number neurons is number-selective responses of supramodal neurons
causally related to number estimation. could easily be associated with arbitrary signs, such as
Given its abstract nature, a number needs to be visual shapes or auditory sounds, to establish symbolic
encoded not only across time and space in single sen- representations of numbers in humans through numer-
sory domains but also independently of the sensory als and number words40,49,50. Neurons in the PFC of mon-
modality (that is, encoding must occur supramodally). keys that were trained to associate the number of dots
In monkeys trained to assess both the number of sequen- with visual shapes (for example, with ‘3’) responded
tial visual dots and auditory sounds within the same ses- equally well to the cardinal values in both displays45. This
sion (FIG. 1d,e), neurons in the lPFC and in the ventral linking capacity of PFC neurons might constitute a
intraparietal (VIP) area of the IPS encoded the number pre-adaptation in monkeys that may have evolved to
Texture-like mechanism
of auditory pulses, the number of visual items, or both38. become symbolic linking in humans.
A mechanism that allows the
representation of very many Interestingly, many randomly selected neurons (11%
and densely packed items and of the total number) in the PFC responded to the same Number as a spontaneously represented category. For a
does not obey Weber’s law. number of items in both the visual and the auditory stim- long time, number neurons were investigated in monkeys
ulation protocols; that is, they responded supramodally to that had been trained to discriminate set sizes. Could it
Preadaptation
A trait that serves a different
number (FIG. 1f). In the VIP area, however, intermingled be that number neurons are only a by‑product of intense
purpose from the one for which neurons represented either visual or auditory numeros- experience rather than a reflection of a native quantita-
it evolved. ities, but not both38. In humans, modality-independent tive faculty that resides in the primate brain? After all,

2 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

a Delayed match-to-numerosity task 50% Match

Non-match Match

50%
Fixation Sample Memory delay Test 1 Test 2
0.5 s 0.5 s 1.0 s 1.2 s 1.2 s
Time
b Cell tuned to the number 2 c Cell tuned to the number 6
Sample Memory delay Sample Memory delay

30 30
28 28
26 26
24
Trials per number

Trials per number


24
22 22
20 20
18 18
16 16
14 14
12 12
10 10
8 8
6 6
4 4
2 2
1 1
100 20 1
2
Spike rate (Hz)

80
Spike rate (Hz)

4
60 6
10 8
40
10
20 20
30
0 0
0 500 1,000 1,500 2,000 0 500 1,000 1,500 2,000
Time (ms) Time (ms)
d Sequential visual number task Match

Item Pause Item Pause Item Pause Item

Non-match Match

1 2 3 4

Time
e Sequential auditory number task Match

Item Pause Item Pause Item Pause Item

Non-match Match

1 2 3 4

Time
f Supra-modal number-tuning curves g Number network in the monkey brain h Number network in the human brain
100
Normalized activity (%)

VIP
Area 5 IPS
area IPS
75 mPFC
lPFC
50

25

0
1 2 3 4
Number of items
Cell 1 (sounds) Cell 3 (sounds)
Cell 1 (dots) Cell 3 (dots)
Cell 2 (sounds) Cell 4 (sounds)
Cell 2 (dots) Cell 4 (dots)
lPFC
Nature Reviews | Neuroscience

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 3


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

◀ Figure 1 | Representation of number in the primate brain. a | A delayed match‑to‑nu- VIP area neurons58. Cells in the IPS that were number-se-
merosity task is commonly used to explore the representation of numbers. A trial begins lective in both space and time34, and number cells in the
when the monkey grasps a lever and fixates at a central target on a screen. After fixation, PFC that integrate across modalities38, are probably neural
a sample stimulus comprising a number of dots (which varies between trials) is displayed substrates for cross-format and supramodal adaptation.
and the monkey memorizes this stimulus during a delay period. The monkey needs to
Collectively, these results from psychophysics, brain imag-
respond whenever the numerosity displayed in the sample phase is shown again in the
test phase. In this case, the test stimulus is a match in 50% of the cases. Trials are
ing and single-neuron recordings support the classic idea
pseudo-randomized and each numerosity is indicated with many different images. of a ‘sense of number’ (BOX 1).
b | Neurons of the primate association cortex (here the prefrontal cortex (PFC))
selectively respond to specific numbers. The time course of neuronal activity during the Cortical topography
trials shows that this example neuron preferentially responded to two dots (that is, it Hierarchical number processing. Across many studies
preferred the number 2). Note that this neuron signalled ‘2’ both during the sample in monkeys, the highest proportions of number neurons
presentation and during the following delay phase (that is, in working memory). have been found in the lPFC (20–30%) and in the VIP
Discharges to all even numbers (and 1) up to 30 for many stimulus repetitions are plotted area (15–20%) in the IPS27,29–31,34,45 (FIG. 1g). Both of these
as dot-raster histograms (top panel; each dot represents an action potential) and brain areas, which constitute the core number network,
averaged spike density functions (bottom panel; neural activity is averaged and
have also recently been activated in a monkey fMRI study
smoothed, and only a selection of the numbers are shown for clarity). Colours correspond
to specific tested numbers. c | Different neurons exhibit different preferred numbers.
of number processing 59. Neurons in the inferior temporal
This example neuron selectively responded to 6 items both during sample presentation lobe, however, do not seem to be involved in numer­osity
and memory delay. d | To assess how neurons encode the number of items presented over processing 29. The lPFC and PPC are anatomically 60–63
time, a sequential visual enumeration protocol has been used. In this task, the monkey and physiologically 64,65 interlinked, classic association
needed to enumerate items one by one and respond if the sample period and test display cortices. They are ideally positioned for number pro-
showed the same number of visual items, and it had to withhold a response if they did cessing because both the lPFC and the VIP area receive
not (probability = 50%). Sample numerosities ‘1’ to ‘4’ were cued by sequentially highly processed input from nearly all senses, which is
presented dots separated by blank displays. The temporal succession and duration of a pre­requisite for representing number abstractly across
individual dots were varied systematically within and across numerosities, thus multiple sensory modalities. In addition, both areas are
controlling for temporal cues. e | To find out whether number neurons generalized across
connected to premotor output structures, allowing them
sensory modalities, different numbers of sounds (indicated by speaker icons) had to be
enumerated by monkeys and matched to the number of visual items in multi-dot
to guide behaviour in an adaptive manner 66.
displays. This sequential auditory enumeration task was presented alternately with the Physiological parameters suggest that number infor-
sequential visual protocol (see part d) in a given session. In this way, the neuronal mation is processed hierarchically between the IPS and
responses of single neurons to both visual and auditory numbers could be tested. the lPFC. Simultaneous recordings from parietal and fron-
f | Supramodal number neurons responded to the same preferred numbers of visual dots tal lobes have repeatedly found that IPS neurons respond
and auditory sounds. Tuning functions (normalized average responses) of four example markedly earlier to number than do PFC cells29,30,53,67.
neurons with preferred numerosities of 1 to 4. Each neuron shows almost identical tuning In addition, single PFC neurons are less sensitive to co‑
to the number of auditory sounds or visual items. g | A frontolateral view of a monkey varying sensory features of numerosity displays than are
brain shows the areas of the number network with a high proportion of number neurons. IPS neurons29 and, in contrast to IPS neurons, PFC neu-
h | A fronto­lateral view of a human brain depicts areas that are consistently activated by
rons respond supramodally to numerosity 38. Finally, in
numbers in functional imaging studies. IPS, intraparietal sulcus; lPFC, lateral PFC; mPFC,
medial PFC; VIP, ventral intraparietal. Parts b and c are based on data from REF. 31. Part f
monkeys trained to associate cardinal values with visual
is based on data from REF. 38. Part h is adapted with permission from REF. 82, Elsevier. shapes, PFC neurons signify this association across time,
whereas IPS neurons do not 45. These findings suggest
that the IPS is the first cortical hub to extract quantita-
it is well known that neuronal responses in the associ- tive information. As a putative recipient of information
ation cortex can be modified by learning 51,52. However, about numerosity from the IPS, the PFC can then process
recordings in monkeys that had not been trained to numerical information in a goal-directed way.
judge number showed that single neurons in both the
VIP area and the lPFC spontaneously responded to Human number system. The cortical number network in
numer­osity and were tuned to preferred numerosities53. the non-human primate cortex is mirrored in the human
Similarly, recent imaging evidence has suggested that brain. Damage to parts of the association cortices causes
the direct and automatic extraction of numerosity also acquired deficits in processing numbers in humans68–70,
occurs in the human brain46,47. leading to a neuropsychological syndrome that is known
Strong support for the direct and spontaneous assess- as acalculia71–73. Acquired numerical impairments have
ment of number resulted from psychophysical experi­ revealed a crucial role for the parietal lobes in number
ments that showed that approximate visual number and calculation skills, besides other frontal and temporal
assessments are subject to adaptation (that is, set sizes associ­ation areas (reviewed in REFS 74,75). Collectively,
are underestimated after adaptation to a high numeros- recent neuropsychological studies in patients have sug-
ity)54,55. This adaptation is even present for sequentially gested a cortical number network rather than a one‑to‑one
presented items, across modalities and across spatio- correspondence between number abilities and one spe-
temporal presentation formats56. Because adaptation is a cific brain area76. In agreement with findings from lesion
characteristic of sense organs, number may be regarded as studies, it is thought that underdeveloped neuronal cir-
a sensory-like attribute54. Importantly, numerosity adapta- cuitries in the number network cause dyscalculia, which is
tion is spatially selective. Thus, adaptation may be medi- a developmental disorder that is characterized by impair-
ated by number-selective neurons that show a spatially ments in the ability to understand numbers and perform
restricted response field, such as lateral intraparietal57 or calculations77. Dyscalculia is associated with reductions

4 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Labelled-line rate code in grey matter volume in the right IPS78 and frontal areas, remote from the preferred one, the neuronal activity of
Relates to the discharge rates such as the anterior cingulum, the left inferior frontal single neurons progressively drops off, thus forming a
of neurons that belong to gyrus and the bilateral middle frontal gyri79. peak-tuned response curve27–31. Such a labelled-line rate
dedicated processing pathways Functional imaging studies in humans also suggest code for number has been shown to exist in the mon-
and that convey information
about specific stimulus
that frontal lobe areas and posterior parietal areas sub- key brain, across spatio­temporal presentation formats,
parameters (it is a variation serve the nonverbal and symbolic representation of quan- modalities and sensory-motor domains34,36,37,83.
of a rate code). tities80, with the posterior parietal areas even showing a The tuning functions of neurons explain the behav-
spatially organized numerosity map81. The findings of ioural phenomenon known as the numerical distance
dozens of fMRI studies exploring BOLD activation during effect 4,28, which is the finding that numerically distant
number tasks were summarized in a recent quantitative numbers are easier to discriminate (for example, 2 versus
meta-analysis82, which showed that numerical process- 6) than numerically closer numbers (for example, 5 ver-
ing is repeatedly and robustly associated with activation sus 6). This is because the read-outs from neurons that are
mainly in the inferior and superior parietal lobules and tuned to distant numbers are unequivocal given that the
the IPS, as well as in frontal lobe areas such as the inferior tuning functions hardly overlap. Neuronal coding prop-
and middle frontal gyrus of the lPFC, and the cingulate erties also explain the numerical size effect4,28, which cap-
gyrus of the medial PFC (mPFC) (FIG. 1h). tures the finding that it is easier to discriminate numbers
with low magnitudes (for example, 2 versus 3) than num-
Reading number from neuronal responses bers with high magnitudes (for example, 8 versus 9) at a
Labelled-line code for number. A number neuron shows given numerical distance (a distance of 1 in this example).
a maximum discharge rate to its preferred numerosity The numerical size effect is related to the finding that the
(FIG. 1b,c,f). When the presented number becomes more number-tuning curves (FIG. 2a) and population activity

Box 1 | A ‘sense of number’


The classic idea of a ‘sense of number’ (REFS 179,180) suggests that humans and animals are endowed with an innate
faculty to perceive the number of items in a set (that is, numerosity). This idea argues that numerosity is intuitively assessed
as a perceptual category by hard-wired sensory brain processes, without the need to be learned. Similar to perceptual
categories such as faces, numerosity is susceptible to adaptation54–56. For example, after adaptation to 400 dots, subjects
judge 100 dots as being equal to 30 dots (see the figure). Supporting sensory access to numerosity, visual numerosity
emerges as a statistical property of images in computational network models91.
Recently, this core idea of the ‘number sense’ was tested neurophysiologically by recording single-cell activity in
numerically naive monkeys. Numerosity-selective neurons were present both in the ventral intraparietal (VIP) area of the
posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) of the primate brain without numerical training53.
Moreover, recordings in naive monkeys revealed neurons that were spontaneously tuned to preferred numerosities. This
finding argues for a labelled-line code of numerosity and against summation coding. Finally, numerosity was encoded earlier
in the PPC, suggesting that numerical information is automatically extracted in the parietal cortex and then conveyed to
the frontal lobe. Indeed, when the
same initially numerically naive Before adaptation
monkeys were re‑trained to 30 dots 30 dots
actively discriminate visual
numerosity, contrasting neuronal
effects on PFC and VIP area
neurons were witnessed as a result
of numerosity training84: although Judged to
PFC neurons became more be equal to
responsive and more selective
during active numerosity
discrimination, none of these
effects was observed for VIP area
neurons. These data indicate the
elevated engagement of PFC
neurons for executive control, After adaptation to 400 dots
whereas the VIP area continues to 100 dots 30 dots
encode numerosity as a perceptual
category regardless of behavioural
relevance. Collectively, these
findings suggest that numbers
reflect a natural perceptual Judged to
category. The intuitive sense of be equal to
number may well be present at
birth in both humans and monkeys,
and number neurons probably
provide the neurobiological
substrate of this sense.

Nature Reviews | Neuroscience

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 5


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Weber–Fechner law (FIG. 2b) become progressively broader and thus less selec- rates as a function of numerosity (that is, the ‘summa-
Classic psychophysical law tive with increasing magnitudes. Further evidence that tion code’)57,89–91. Summation units representing num-
about the perception of monkeys rely on the activity of number neurons to solve ber in a graded manner are also the basis of the serial
magnitudes in relation to the a trial comes from recordings during behavioural errors. mode-control model92,93, which was originally devel-
physical intensity of a stimulus;
it states that linear increments
The discharge rate to the preferred number is markedly oped to model animal timing behaviour. It suggests
in sensation S are proportional reduced in error trials compared with correct trials27,29–31,84. that accumulator units encode pacemaker impulses for
to the logarithm of stimulus Thus, if the neurons do not properly encode their pre- each element to be enumerated. Both the numerosity
magnitude I (S = k * log(I)). ferred number, the monkey is at risk of confusing numer- detector model and the mode-control model result in
osities. Together with the numerical deficits seen after analogue representations of number and predict the
pharmacological inactivation (described above)37, these Weber–Fechner law. So far, neurophysiological data for
findings suggest that the activity of number neurons is the assessment of number from spatial item arrays can
indispensable for numerical judgments. be reconciled well with the numerosity detector model.
Close examination of the shape of the tuning curves Whether this model could be adjusted to serially pre-
reveals asymmetric peak functions on a linear number sented items — a strength of the mode-control model
scale. The neuronal tuning functions (and the behav- — awaits further investigation.
ioural performance functions)4,85 are better described
(that is, symmetric) by a nonlinearly compressed, log- Population rate code. A problem of single-cell tuning
arithmic number scale (FIG. 2c) than a linear scale28,31 curves is that they only allow predictions about how neu-
(FIG. 2a). The logarithmic coding scheme resulting in rons will respond on average to a given number. The read-
bell-shaped curves has physiological advantages. First, outs for single trials of neuronal responses are noisy, and
it forms a scale-invariant set of neurons that have the smooth and unequivocal tuning functions only emerge
same widths of tuning for all numbers. Second, it ensures after the discharges of a cell to many identical trial rep-
that the variability of neuronal responses is independent etitions have been averaged (FIG. 1b,c), or after the tuning
of number preference. Third, logarithmic coding agrees functions of many equally tuned single cells have been
with the psychophysical Weber–Fechner law, which pre- pooled (FIG. 2b). The brain, however, needs to decipher a
dicts that the discrimination of stimuli diminishes in presented number within a single trial. To cope with noisy
proportion to the increasing quantity of the stimuli. discharge patterns, it must take the activity of a population
Logarithmic scaling may be a mathematical approxima- of other tuned neurons into account. The important infor-
tion, but so far this compression has provided the most mation in a population code is thus the relative, rather
symmetric number tuning functions in comparison than the absolute, amounts of activity in different neurons.
with other compression schemes, such as peak power Just like a democratic election involves many voters,
functions4,28,31. Symmetric Gaussian tuning functions are the coding of a particular number may involve many neu-
extensively used in neural models of information cod- rons, and the signal of each number neuron represents an
ing because they show computational advantages over independent vote for a specific number 94. The independ-
nonsymmetric functions and their analytical expression ent signals of number neurons may be overlapping and
can be easily manipulated in mathematical derivations86. may need to be averaged over large populations to clearly
Number neurons as described electrophysiologically indicate a number. When the activity profiles of all selec-
in the monkey brain have been postulated in humans87,88 tive neurons are grouped together, an array of overlapping
on the basis of BOLD signals in fMRI adaptation proto- number tuning functions emerges28,29 (FIG. 2a). This array
cols. This fMRI adaptation assumes that neurons tuned preserves the ordinal relationship of numerosities in rela-
to a preferred number should habituate (that is, they tion to one another. By reading out the activity of labelled
should decrease their discharge rates) with repeated lines across the population of cells, all numbers along the
presentations of the same preferred numerosity. Such number line can be deciphered. For example, if a stimulus
a habituation effect can then be read out by recording with two items is shown, the highest response is elicited
the BOLD activation to individual deviant numerosities, in the neuron tuned to 2 (FIG. 2a). However, depending
resulting in a release from adaptation to the preferred on the amount of tuning overlap, ‘2’ may also, to a lesser
numerosity and thus a BOLD recovery signal. The fMRI degree, activate neurons with a preference for adjacent
number-tuning curves in the human IPS were shown to numbers on the number line. Thus, the stimulation by
become symmetric and Gaussian only when plotted on specific numbers of items (‘2’ and ‘4’ in FIG. 2a) results in
a logarithmic scale87,88. Interestingly, this way of coding unique activation patterns in the neuronal population of
numerical information by number neurons showing log- number neurons for these numbers (FIG. 2b).
arithmically compressed tuning functions seems to have Clues to how the activity of number-selective neurons
evolved independently in vertebrate endbrains with very could be read out are provided by population-coding
different anatomies and thus is an example of convergent models developed for sensory and motor systems that
evolution (BOX 2). also rely on Gaussian-shaped tuning functions86. In the
Tuned and logarithmically scaled ‘numerosity detec- population vector approach applied to the coding of
tors’ were originally predicted by a numerosity detector sensory or movement direction information95–97, each
model89 that encoded numerosity from parallel inputs. neuron is assigned a vector — the length of which is pro-
Numerosity detectors may arise by combining the activ- portional to the activity of the neuron and the direction
ity from upstream ‘summation units’, which are neurons of which corresponds to its preferred orientation. All
that monotonically increase and decrease discharge the individual vectors are summed to form a population

6 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

vector that allows the preferred orientation to be esti- can be represented over time via the different spatial fir-
mated from its angle. As an alternative population code ing patterns of a neuronal population99. This dynamic
model, maximum likelihood estimators based on a tem- population coding for categories has been observed in
plate derived from the Gaussian-shaped tuning curves the monkey parietal cortex 100 and PFC101, and could be
of the cells have been suggested98. With adaptations, especially important for cognitive control functions that
standard models of population coding could be suited to are based on number 102.
modelling a population code for number representations.
Population coding has several advantages in coding Rate and time code. Can functionally overlapping
information compared with single neurons, including groups of parietal neurons provide sufficient informa-
the reduction of uncertainty caused by neuronal varia- tion for a monkey to make correct quantity judgments?
bility, relative insensitivity to the loss of cells, the ability To find out, a population decoding technique103 (that is,
to code different stimulus features simultaneously and a statistical classifier)104 was used to read out spike trains
the option to compute with representations, such as the that were recorded from behaving monkeys105. Indeed, a
transformation (for example, from specific to abstract) small group of numerosity-selective neurons conveyed
and the combination (for example, across modalities) categorical information about numerical quantity with
of internal representations86,97. Moreover, population high accuracy (FIG. 2d). These tuned neurons were behav-
activity can reflect changes in stimulus or task conditions iourally relevant, because the classifier was no longer
much faster than single-neuron rates, which need to be able to predict the numerosities that the monkey had
integrated over time, and the same sensory information seen based on the firing rates during error trials105.

a Visual number tuning b Population activity c Logarithmic number scale

Non-linear compression

100 100 100


Normalized mean

Normalized mean

Normalized mean
80 80 80
activity (%)

activity (%)

activity (%)
60 60 60
40 40 40
20 20 20
0 0 0
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Number of visual items Preferred number of Number of visual items
visual items (log scale)
Neurons tuned to '1' Neurons tuned to '2' Neurons tuned to '3' Neurons tuned to '4' Neurons tuned to '5'

d Classification with average firing rate e Classification with temporal firing pattern
0.6 0.6
Classification probablility
Classification probablility

0.4 0.4

0.2 0.2

0.0 0.0
–2 0 2 –2 0 2
Pr
Pr

ed
ed

s
s

as
as

ic
ic

cl
cl

te
te

e
e

d
d

u
u

Tr

cl
Tr

cl

as
as

s
s

Classification probablility

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8

Nature Reviews | Neuroscience

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 7


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Rate code Surprisingly, however, even populations of neurons (if a neuron is tuned to a magnitude and value that is
Relates to the information that were not numerosity-tuned based on classic spike- not currently relevant) or highly ambiguous (if a neuron
encoded by the number of count measures (that is, neurons that showed flat tuning encodes more than one magnitude).
spikes during an interval. functions) carried reliable information about quantity To disentangle the effects of two or more types of
Time code
category 105, albeit with less accuracy than the tuned neu- magnitudes on the response of a neuron, the aforemen-
Relates to the information rons (FIG. 2e). This suggests that the classifier extracted tioned concept of a population code can be extended. The
encoded by temporal additional information from the temporal structure of magnitude system would benefit from detecting patterns
patterns of action potentials the neuronal responses. These temporal discharge pat- in the relative responses of a population of neurons that
within an interval.
terns were behaviourally relevant, because the popula- are differently tuned to, for example, number, extent and
tion decoding technique was no longer able to classify duration, and on the basis of those patterns, the actual
numerosities based on spike trains during error trials. numerical value, length or time interval of a stimulus
Thus, in addition to a classical firing rate code, a tempo- could be determined. This distributed tuning of single
ral code (time code) provides supplementary information neurons probably causes the activation of overlapping
about number in the neuronal population. regions by different types of magnitudes in brain imaging
studies47,88,110,111 and could explain the well-known interac-
Discrete and continuous quantities. Many neurons in tions and interference effects between magnitudes. Based
the parietal and prefrontal association cortices respond on the commonalities between the neural and behavioural
not only to discrete numerical magnitudes but also to representations of number, space, time and other magni-
continuous magnitudes, such as length30,105, distance106 tudes, common processing mechanisms for different types
and time107,108. In both the VIP and PFC, anatomically of abstract magnitudes have been proposed112.
intermingled neurons have been found that encode
length, numerosity or both types of quantities30,109. Thus, Microcircuits sculpturing numerosity selectivity. Narrow
judging the type and value of magnitude to be processed numerosity tuning functions are advantageous for a pre-
from individual neurons alone would be not informative cise read-out of the population responses. At the level of
local neocortical microcircuits, inhibitory interneurons
have a vital role in sculpturing tuning curves, even though
◀ Figure 2 | Responses of neuron populations to number. a | Information about number
they are outnumbered 4:1 by excitatory neurons (mainly
is carried by neurons that form overlapping visual number-tuning functions. Normalized
and averaged (according to preferred visual numerosity) tuning functions of number pyramidal projection neurons)113,114. In extracellular PFC
neurons, each tuned to a different preferred numerosity. Different colours represent the recordings, pyramidal cells tend to exhibit broad action
distinct preferred numerosities of the neurons. The functions are overlapping and cover potential waveforms and low rates of discharge, whereas
the entire range of tested numerosities from 1 to 5. The stimulus display with two dots interneurons display narrow action potential waveforms
elicits the highest response in the neuron tuned to ‘2’, but also activates neurons tuned and high discharge rates115–119 (FIG. 3).
to all the other numerosities to a certain degree. Similarly, the stimulus display showing By exploiting the established differences between
four dots maximally excites the neuron tuned to ‘4’, but to a lesser extent also excites putative pyramidal cells and inhibitory interneurons
the other tuned neurons. b | Stimulation by specific numbers of items (‘2’ and ‘4’, that were recorded extracellularly in the PFC of behav-
respectively) results in unique activation patterns in the neuronal population of number
ing monkeys, the functional roles that these two classes
neurons shown in part a. c | Numerosity tuning functions shown in part a are not
symmetric on a linear number axis but show a shallower slope towards higher of neurons exhibit during numerical cognition in local
numerosities. After logarithmic transformation of the number axis, the tuning functions microcircuits have been addressed. Anatomically adja-
become more symmetric, as predicted by the Weber–Fechner law of logarithmic scaling cent pyramidal cells that were functionally connected
of perceived magnitudes. d | The average firing rate of a population of number-selective (indicated by temporally correlated discharges)120 showed
neurons conveyed robust information about number. A statistical classifier (neuronal similar numerosity tuning functions121 (FIG. 3a). The syn-
network algorithm) extracted information from neuronal responses recorded from chronous excitation of neighbouring pyramidal cells
monkeys discriminating numbers. Based on the information acquired from average probably reflects a shared excitatory input 116. By con-
firing rates of a population of number neurons, the classifier was able to predict the trast, functionally connected inhibitory interneurons
number judged by the monkey from novel recordings. The confusion matrix and pyramidal cells showed inverted numerosity tuning
(‘checkerboard’) describes the pattern of numerosity classification performed on
profiles that were negatively correlated in time (FIG. 3b).
neuronal population of tuned neurons. The rows in each confusion matrix represent the
true classes the monkey had seen and the columns correspond to the output of the That is, whenever an inhibitory interneuron discharged,
classifier. Colour codes the classification probability, and the four classes correspond to the pyramidal cell was markedly inhibited, and vice versa.
numerosity 1 to 4. The main diagonal shows how often the classifier correctly assigned A similar mechanism with opposite tuning of nearby
numerosity to their real category. Averaging the classification probabilities over the inhibitory interneurons and pyramidal cells accounts for
diagonal parallel to the main diagonal results in the average performance of the spatial tuning in the PFC115,117 (see REF. 116 for contrasting
classifier as a function of distance from the real numerosity, which is plotted as a tuning results), and blockade of GABAergic inhibition leads to
function at each end of the main diagonal. e | The temporal discharge pattern of a the broadening of the tuning profiles of these neurons122.
population of unselective neurons also conveyed reliable information about number. These findings suggest that inhibitory interneurons
Based on the information acquired from the temporal patterns of spikes of a population systematically inhibit numerosity-tuned pyramidal cells,
of neurons, the classifier was able to predict the number seen by the monkey from novel
as proposed by recurrent network models for spatial
recordings. Same analysis as in part d, but performed on neurons that were not tuned
based on firing rates. Parts a–c are based on data from REFS 28 and 29. Parts d and e are working memory 123. As the preferred numerosities of
adapted with permission from REF. 105, Tudusciuc, O. & Nieder, A. Neuronal population the inhibitory interneurons represent the non-preferred
coding of continuous and discrete quantity in the primate posterior parietal cortex. numerosities at the flanks of the tuning function of a
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 14513–14518 (2007). Copyright (2007) National Academy pyramidal cell, the shoulders of the tuning curve are low-
of Sciences, U.S.A. ered by this lateral inhibition, thus sharpening the tuning

8 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Global neuronal workspace curve of the pyramidal cell (FIG. 3c). After a number has The reciprocally connected association areas that
(GNW). A framework for the been encoded, a population of pyramidal cells exhibits host the number network are particularly well suited
mechanism of consciousness. tuned persistent activity through recurrent excitations. to maintaining information across time and to exerting
It consists of a network of These tuned pyramidal cells activate local inhibitory cognitive control. The PPC and lPFC are connected not
distributed neurons with
long-distance connectivity
interneurons, which, in turn, inhibit pyramidal cells with only to one another but also to >12 other widely dis-
constituting a ‘global a different numerosity preference. Remarkably, the out- tributed cortical areas125, which are interconnected by
workspace’ that can potentially lined wiring scheme not only illustrates the encoding and a common thalamic input from the medial pulvinar
interconnect multiple sharpening of numerosity from sensory input but also nucleus126. The areas in this network display noncanon-
specialized brain areas in
suggests reverberation of numerosity-selective activity ical circuit properties127: in contrast to canonical circuits
a coordinated manner to
give rise to a subjective
after a stimulus has ceased based on positive feedback of the sensory and motor cortices, many connections
feeling of conscious effort. loops of activity between excitatory pyramidal cells. This is within this network lack a clear sensory-motor hierar-
the type of activity that is needed for working memory 124. chical polarity with consistent feedforward and feedback
Homology laminar termination patterns. Instead, the noncanonical
Refers to traits that share a
common ancestry but may
Robust numerical working memory network seems to be designed for parallel and re‑entrant
have different functions. The noncanonical association cortex. The controlled processing. It constitutes a global neuronal workspace
processing of numerical information towards a goal (GNW) that is thought to enable access to the conscious
Homoplasy requires, first and foremost, that number representa- processing of mental representations128,129. According to
Refers to traits with common
tions are actively maintained in our ‘mental sketchpad’. the GNW model, a behaviourally relevant representa-
functions but that are
associated with different
Working memory, which is the ability to briefly retain tion is maintained by a sudden, late and sustained acti-
underlying structures and and manipulate information, is the fundamental basis vation (conscious ‘ignition’ (REF. 129)) of a proportion
origins, and have evolved of cognition. It allows us to ‘think about’ quantitative of the GNW neurons, with only one such conscious
by convergent evolution. information, and to weigh and measure it in relation to representation being active at any given time.
previous experiences and current goals as a hallmark of Therefore, a key neurophysiological signature of
cognitive control. Consider the simple calculation ‘2 + 3’: recurrent loops of the noncanonical association cortex
it relies on the ability to mentally maintain ‘2’ before it is persistent (or sustained) neuronal activity. Persistent
can be incremented by ‘3’ to end up with the result, ‘5’. activity is typically witnessed during delayed response
Without working memory, we would be lost. tasks that include a short gap in time between a sensory

Box 2 | Convergent evolution of numerical representations


The sophisticated circuitries in the six-layered neocortex of the frontal and parietal lobes endow both humans and
non-human primates with numerical competence. Interestingly, this six-layered neocortex, which is thought to enable
primates with the highest levels of cognition, is not present in non-mammalian vertebrates such as birds. The last common
reptilian-like ancestor of mammals and birds lived 300 million years ago, at a time when the neocortex had not yet
developed181. Since then, mammals and birds have independently been through a parallel evolution of endbrains. The bulk
of the endbrain in both mammals and birds stems from the ontogenetic mantle, the pallium, and thus shares common
ancestry (homology)182,183. The overall architectures of the endbrains, however, are very different and developed
independently to give rise to similar functions owing to similar selection pressures (homoplasy)184,185. Despite these
structural differences, birds are known to rival primates in quantitative competence in the laboratory186–188 and in
the wild189–191.
To explore where and on which basis neuronal code numerosity is represented in avian brains, crows were trained to
discriminate the visual number of items in controlled displays192. At the same time, single-cell activity was recorded from an
endbrain association area that is known as the nidopallium caudolateral (NCL) in crows193, which is a brain area considered
to be the avian analogue of the primate prefrontal cortex194–196 (see the figure). The behavioural and neuronal data show an
impressive correspondence of neuronal mechanisms found in the avian brain with those described earlier in the primate
brain193: NCL neurons were tuned to individual preferred numerosities, and neuronal discharges proved to be relevant for
the correct performance of the crows in a delayed number discrimination task. Both the neuronal and the behavioural
tuning functions were best described on a logarithmic number line. This argues for a nonlinearly compressed coding of
numerical information, just as predicted by the psychophysical Weber–Fecher Law28, not only in primates but also
across vertebrates.
Even though crows and primates have
very differently organized endbrain PFC Nidopallium
structures, they show the same code for caudolaterale
number, which suggests that this way of
coding numerical information has
evolved based on convergent evolution
because it exhibits a superior solution to
a common computational problem.
A comparative approach is indispensable
for deciphering evolutionary stable
neuronal mechanisms and codes. This
proves true not only in the realm of
numerical competence but also for all
neurobiological questions.

Nature Reviews | Neuroscience


NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 9
©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

a Coupled pyramidal cells: b Coupled interneurons and pyramidal cells:


similar tuning opposite tuning
Putative pyramidal cell 1 Putative pyramidal cell 2 Putative pyramidal cell 1 Putative interneuron
12 60
7 3

Spike rate (Hz)

Spike rate (Hz)

Spike rate (Hz)

Spike rate (Hz)


10 52
6
2
5
8 44
1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4
Number of items Number of items Number of items Number of items

c Lateral inhibition sharpening number tuning

Glutamatergic GABAergic
pyramidal cell interneuron

1
1 1 3 3 3

Figure 3 | Microcircuit interactions of number neurons. a | Putative (excitatory) pyramidal Nature


cellsReviews Neuroscience
seem to |receive common
inputs about number information rather than being engaged in reciprocal interactions. This is suggested by functionally
connected and synchronously excited putative pyramidal cells (broad-spiking neurons) recorded simultaneously at the
same electrode tip, with both cells tuned to a numerosity of 1. b | The number tuning of putative (excitatory) pyramidal
cells seems to be shaped by inhibitory interneurons that show opposite number tuning. This is evidenced by putative
inhibitory interneurons (narrow-spiking neurons) and putative pyramidal cells (broad-spiking neurons) that were recorded
simultaneously at the same electrode tip and showed inverted (opposite) tuning profiles. These cells were functionally
coupled and inhibited each other. c | Putative schematic of the lateral inhibition microcircuit in the prefrontal cortex that
acts to sharpen number tuning. Tuned broad-spiking pyramidal cells excite each other through glutamatergic synapses on
the spines of basal dendrites, thus causing recurrent persistent excitation. Narrow-spiking inhibitory interneurons provide
lateral inhibition through GABAergic synapses. Numbers in each model neuron depict the respective preferred numbers.
Parts a and b are republished with permission of the Society for Neuroscience, from: Complementary contributions of
prefrontal neuron classes in abstract numerical categorization. Diester, I. & Nieder, A. J. Neurosci. 28 (31), 2008; permission
conveyed through Copyright Clearance Center, Inc.

stimulus and an instructed response130,131. By sustaining delay periods (FIG. 1b). In addition, many delay-selective
activity, neurons actively buffer and process information neurons integrate across spatial and temporal pres-
to bridge the gap until an adaptive output is selected132. entation formats34 and visual–auditory modalities38.
High-level cognitive functioning would be impossi- Therefore, numerical delay selectivity is abstract and
ble without persistent activity offering ‘freedom from is found irrespective of the exact spatial or temporal
immediacy’ (REF. 133). appearance of the memorized sets.
The delayed match‑to‑numerosity task (FIG. 1a) allows
the investigation of persistent activity during numerical Cognitive inhibition. Working memory has severely lim-
tasks because it requires animals to retain a number of ited capacity. Thus, transient representations of numerical
items over a delay period to subserve a future selection information in working memory are vulnerable to dis-
process. As a putative physical correlate of working tractions that potentially replace important representa-
memory for number, a notable proportion of neurons tions with different content encoded by another discrete
(20–30% in the PFC and 10–20% in the PPC) show combination of neurons in the workspace population.
persistent activity during delay periods when monkeys The process of restraining or overriding irrelevant rep-
memorize the numerosity that they have just sensed resentations to keep hanging on to relevant information
(FIG. 1b). Similar to neurons that are active during sensory is known as ‘cognitive inhibition’ (REF. 134). Crucial ques-
stimulation, delay-selective number neurons are tuned tions include where and how number representations
to the remembered numerosity 27–31. Neurons are even that each compete to gain access to the global workspace
tuned to the same numerosity in both the sample and the are selected.

10 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

a Number distractor task Match


50%

Non-match Match

50%
Fixation Sample 1. Memory delay Distractor 2. Memory delay Test 1 Test 2
0.5 s 0.5 s 1.0 s 1.5 s 1.0 s 1.2 s 1.2 s
Time

b Sorted by sample number c Sorted by distractor number d Information about number


Sample Distractor

Sample Distractor Sample Distractor 1.0


30 30

Information (PEV)
Spike rate (Hz)

Spike rate (Hz)

20 20 0.5

10 10
0

0 0
0 1,000 2,000 3,000 0 1,000 2,000 3,000 0 1,000 2,000 3,000
Time (ms) Time (ms) Time (ms)
1 2 3 4 Sample number information
Distractor number information
Significance threshold

Nature Reviews | Neuroscience


Figure 4 | Cognitive inhibition of a prefrontal cortex cell during numerical distraction. a | How neurons may cope
with the interference caused by an irrelevant number was studied in a modified delayed match‑to‑sample task
(see FIG. 1a). Here, a behaviourally irrelevant numerosity (distractor) presented in the prolonged delay period needed to
be ignored to solve the task correctly. Numerosities from 1 to 4 were shown both as the sample and as the distractor
numerosities, with all possible combinations used. b | An example neuron in the prefrontal cortex encoded a working
memory trace of the relevant sample numerosity even after a distracting numerosity had been shown in the
aforementioned distractor task. When trials are sorted by sample numerosity, the time course of activation (spike density
histogram) shows that the neuron was equally tuned to number 4 both before (1. memory delay) and after presentation of
a distractor number (2. memory delay). Interestingly, tuning was apparent during the distractor presentation. The plot is
temporally correlated with the task layout that is shown in part a. c | When the responses of the same neuron as in part b
were sorted by distractor numerosity, temporary tuning to the distractor numerosity during the end of the distractor
period was evident. d | The same neuron that is featured in parts b and c carried information about both the sample and
the distractor numerosity in different trial epochs. Information about either the sample or the distractor numerosity is
quantified by a sliding window percent explained variance (PEV; ω2) analysis, which is derived from an analysis of variance
(ANOVA). This analysis conveys how much of the discharge variation over time can be explained by the sample numerosity
or the distractor numerosity. Information about the sample numerosity (blue line) is encoded during the end of the first
memory delay and during the second memory delay, but is briefly lost at the end of the distractor phase. Conversely,
information about the task-irrelevant distractor numerosity emerges towards the end of the distractor phase and the
beginning of the second memory delay, but vanishes towards the end of the second memory delay. Thus, information
about target numerosity was not permanently replaced by irrelevant distractor information but regenerated in the second
delay after the distraction, just in time to solve the task. Dashed lines mark the significance threshold. Parts b–d are
adapted with permission from REF. 135, Elsevier.

As challenging abstract categories, number rep- situation135. To that aim, a behaviourally irrelevant
resentations place high demands on cognitive inhi- number display was inserted as a distractor in the delay
bition. Consider a simple example: you look up your period of a delayed match-to-number task (FIG. 4a).
friend’s phone number to call for a chat but while you Because cognitive inhibition cannot entirely eradicate
pick up the phone someone utters an arbitrary num- distracting influences, a worsening of performance
ber. Only with considerable effort will you be able to reflects increased distraction. Indeed, the monkeys
ignore the distracting number and still manage to call managed to resist the distractor numbers most of
your friend’s number. Monkeys were confronted with the time but made slightly more errors than when no
a situation mirroring the abovementioned phone call distractors were present 135.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 11


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

While the monkeys performed this task, single-unit terms are not in brackets, in which case addition and
activity was simultaneously recorded from the PFC subtraction need to be calculated first. Because numer-
and the VIP area. After sample presentation, PFC neu- ical principles operate on abstract numerical categories,
rons encoded the relevant sample number during the quantitative rules in particular require high degrees of
first delay period and exhibited the well-known tun- cognitive structuring.
ing curves and persistent activity (FIG. 4b). However, A fundamental operation when dealing with num-
when the distractor number was subsequently shown, bers involves ‘greater-than’ and ‘fewer-than’ rules of the
the neurons did not resist interference. Rather, sam- form: if ‘x > y’ then ‘a’ versus if ‘x < y’ then ‘b’. These
ple information was overwritten and the PFC neurons relationships can also be mastered by non-human pri-
responded strongly and in a tuned manner to the dis- mates147–149. To gain insight into the neuronal process-
tractor numbers, sometimes with different preferred ing of numerical rules, rhesus monkeys were trained to
numerosities (FIG.  4c). Surprisingly, however, target flexibly switch between ‘greater-than’ and ‘fewer-than’
number representations were not permanently lost but rules148,150,151. In each trial (FIG. 5a), a sample stimulus
were regenerated in the second delay after the distrac- indicated the reference numerosity that the monkey
tion, just in time to solve the task (FIG. 4d). Neurons in needed to remember over a brief time interval (the
the VIP area, by contrast, were mostly unaffected by the ‘memory delay’). Next, a rule cue instructed the mon-
distractor numbers and continued to retain the working key to follow either a ‘greater-than’ or a ‘fewer-than’
memory representation of the sample numbers. rule. After a second delay (the ‘rule delay’), the monkey
These results were unexpected, given the findings needed to respond according to the currently valid rule
from previous studies in which neurons in the PFC and cue to a greater or fewer number of dots, respectively,
PPC were not activated during the memorization of than it had previously seen in the sample display. The
stimulus features but were instead activated by stimulus monkeys performed this task with different numbers of
location. In spatial working memory tasks, PFC neu- items and generalized to novel numerosities, indicating
rons only mildly respond to distractors, suggesting that that they had learned an abstract numerical principle148.
the suppression of distracting information in the PFC PFC neurons are known to flexibly group informa-
is the key mechanism that protects working memory tion into behaviourally meaningful categories according
from interference and gates access to it 136–139. By con- to task demands152–154, and they also encode these basic
trast, PPC neurons responded strongly to distractors mathematical rules. In the rule-switching task described
and encoded the physical properties of any stimulus above, approximately 20% of the PFC neurons reflected
irrespective of its relevance. Thus, the activity of PPC ‘greater-than’ or ‘fewer-than’ rules148 (FIG. 5b). Rule-
neurons was considered to be an inferior correlate of selective neurons responded strongly to the preferred
working memory 139–141. rule during the rule delay phase with an increase in
These new results point towards the PFC as a control discharge rate, irrespective of the particular sample
or selection stage in the global noncanonical network. numerosity to which the rule applied148. Importantly,
Activity in the PFC might be better understood not as a the sensory appearance of the rule cue had no impact
signature of memory storage per se142, but as a top-down on the activity of rule-selective neurons. Moreover,
signal143 that influences other members of the global responses in the rule delay could not be explained by
workspace, such as the VIP, where the actual working motor preparation, because the comparison number
memory representations seem to be maintained. To was still missing in this phase and therefore the mon-
adopt this position of a selection stage, the PFC needs key could not know which response was to be prepared.
representations of both relevant and not immediately Rule selectivity gradually evolved with time in the rule
relevant information to attentionally filter 144 and flexibly delay, with approximately one-half of the rule-selective
guide stimulus selection for the upcoming response later neurons preferring the ‘greater-than’ rule and the other
in the task. This could explain why the ability to resist one-half preferring the ‘fewer-than’ rule.
interfering stimuli is compromised in monkeys139,145 The responses of such rule-selective neurons corre-
and humans with lPFC lesions146 — not because stored lated with the behaviour of the monkeys: if the animals
information is lost, but because control and selection made wrong decisions, the responses of the neurons to
processes in the global workspace are dysfunctional. the preferred rule in the rule delay phase were mark-
edly reduced148. This observation indicates that there is
Rule-based decisions with numbers a direct relationship between the rule selectivity of the
Although they are important first steps, the representa- neurons and task performance. Rule-selective neurons
tion and memorization of numerical quantity alone do were prevalent in the frontal lobe but were also found,
not constitute behavioural advantages. After quantities although less frequently, in the VIP area of the IPS150.
have been extracted from sensory input and maintained ‘Greater-than–fewer-than’ rules can be applied not
in short-term memory, numbers need to be processed only to numbers but also to any magnitude. Could neu-
according to rules or general principles. Rules can be rons represent overarching ‘magnitude rules’ instead
understood as conditional ‘if–then’ statements that of rules applied to individual magnitudes? In mon-
determine the logic of a goal-directed task. Rules are cru- keys that applied these quantity rules to both numbers
cial to arithmetic: they constitute mathematical syntax. and the lengths of lines151, most rule-selective neurons
For example, in equations, we first solve multiplication responded only to the quantitative rules applied to one
and division terms, but only if addition and subtraction specific magnitude type: for example, to the longer lines

12 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

but not to more items. Such neurons can be regarded During mathematical problems, a limited set of neu-
as ‘rule specialists’. However, other neurons generalized rons needs to be able to solve several tasks throughout
the magnitude principle and encoded the overarching time and changing task demands. Thus, brain activity
concept of magnitude rules by equally representing the needs to be highly dynamic and neurons may adaptively
quantitative rules related to both magnitudes. Such neu- switch coding properties157 to join different populations
rons are known as ‘rule generalists’ (FIG. 5c). This indi- as time goes on102. Even though PFC neurons signal
cates that the primate brain uses a mixture of selective number, when appropriate, many of them may dynam-
neurons155. Rule generalist neurons provide a computa- ically switch to encoding cognitive control functions
tional advantage compared with rule specialist neurons such as rule-guided decisions. At the population level,
and could operate at a higher functional hierarchy 156; the temporal evolution of rule-selective activity can be
indeed, they allow the generalization and adaptation of expressed as a dynamic trajectory through activity state
quantitative rules to new circumstances. space (FIG. 5d). Once high-dimensional population data

1 2 3 4 5 6

a Numerical rule-switching task ‘Fewer-than’ rule d Population rule activity in 3D state space
50% 50%
Respond if 1 n = 380
‘fewer-than’
rule valid 0
6
2

PC 3
–1 4
1
Respond if
‘greater-than’ –2
rule valid 3
50% 50%
–3 5
‘Greater-than’ rule –2
–2
0 2 0
Fixation Sample Memory delay Rule cue Rule delay Choice PC 2 2 6 4 PC 3
500 ms 500 ms 1,000 ms 300 ms 1,000 ms 1,200 ms
‘Greater than’
‘Fewer than’
b Cell (same as part c) with ‘fewer-than’ rule activity e Rule-selective neurons in monkeys
20 mPFC IPS
Smaller
number
chosen
15
Spike rate (Hz)

10
Larger
number
5 chosen

lPFC
0
0 1,000 2,000 3,000
Time (ms)
c Cell (same as part b) with ‘shorter-than’ rule activity f fMRI activity during calculation in humans
IPS
20 mPFC

15 Shorter
Spike rate (Hz)

line
chosen
10

5 Longer
line
chosen
0
0 1,000 2,000 3,000
Time (ms) lPFC
‘Greater-than’ (cue 1) ‘Fewer-than’ (cue 1)
‘Greater-than’ (cue 2) ‘Fewer-than’ (cue 2)

Nature Reviews | Neuroscience

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 13


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

are compressed to a three-dimensional space, the dis- activity in addition to posterior parietal activity is readily
tinct trajectories resulting from PFC population activity observed when individuals perform arithmetic opera-
indicate robust differentiation of the ‘greater-than’ and tions (for example, addition or subtraction)82,161,162 (FIG. 5f),
‘fewer-than’ rules. (FIG. 5d). This does not apply only to particularly at young ages (around 10 years of age)163.
individual neurons — the entire population encodes Consistent with the general role of the PFC in exec-
quantitative rules dynamically. utive functions, the deficits that occur with numbers
Rule coding is under the influence of neuromodula- and calculation following frontal lesions are complex.
tors. In the PFC, the neuromodulator dopamine affects Luria164 suggested that the difficulty patients with fron-
the basic coding properties of neurons158,159 via the two tal lobe lesions experience in carrying out arithmeti-
major (D1 and D2) receptor families. Recent experiments cal operations stems from a disturbance of intellectual
probing single neurons by combining single-cell record- activity — a general problem of solving a complex
ings and simultaneous micro-iontophoretic drug applica- problem — rather than primary acalculia. Cognitive
tions showed that both the D1 and D2 dopamine receptor estimation requires quantitative reasoning, and cogni-
families cooperatively enhance number coding, albeit by tive estimation deficits in various quantitative domains
distinct physiological mechanisms160. Thus, numerical (size, weight, numerosity and time) have frequently
operations can be instrumental in deciphering the influ- been reported for patients with frontal lesions165–168. As
ence of neuromodulation in high-level processing. semantic number representations tend to be spared in
Numerical rule-related activity is most frequently such patients, these estimation deficits are attributed to
and selectively found in the lPFC and mPFC of mon- executive deficits that disrupt the translation of number
keys but can also be detected in the VIP area150 (FIG. 5e). representations to structured output. As a most pecu-
Corresponding brain areas show activity during calcu- liar deficit, ‘task-switching acalculia’ was reported in
lation tasks in humans (FIG. 5f). Pronounced prefrontal a patient with stroke who had brain lesions in the left
ventral and dorsolateral frontal lobe169. Interestingly,
the patient’s calculation ability per se was not affected,
◀ Figure 5 | Numerical rules. a | How primates and their neurons process numerical rules but he showed a specific deficit in switching between
has been investigated using a rule-switching task. Here, monkeys needed to decide
different operations in simple calculations; for exam-
whether there was a greater or fewer number of dots at the choice stage than the
number presented in a sample display (five different numerosities were used in the task). ple, from multiplication to addition or subtraction, and
A cue showed in the delay phase indicated whether a ‘greater-than’ (bottom) or a vice versa. This ‘task-switching acalculia’ is consistent
‘fewer-than’ rule (top) needed to be applied (the probability of each rule being displayed with the idea that frontal lesions lead to weak top-down
was 0.5). Each rule was signified by two pairs of different sensory cues (only colour cues control that is caused by an inability to switch between
shown). b | Neurons recorded in monkeys that were engaged in this numerical mathematical rules.
rule-switching task selectively responded to one or the other of these numerical rules.
The activity averaged over all trials (spike density histogram, smoothed with a 150 ms Conclusions
Gaussian kernel) of an example neuron was systematically higher during the rule delay The combined evidence from neurological, develop-
when the ‘fewer-than’ rule was cued, irrespective of the sensory features of the rule cue mental, imaging and neurophysiological studies points
(activity coded by cool colours). The plot is temporally correlated with the task layout
towards a dedicated, putatively homologous number
that is shown in part a. c | The neuron featured in part b also signalled a ‘fewer-than’ rule
applied to the length of lines. The task design was very similar to the numerical network in the primate brain that processes number
rule-switching protocol depicted in part a. In this task, however, monkeys saw lines of and other abstract magnitudes112,170–174. The reviewed
different lengths that they needed to process according to ‘shorter-than’ and studies highlight striking similarities between numer-
‘longer-than’ rules. This neuron showed elevated firing rates also to the ‘shorter-than’ ical representations in both non-human and human
rule, thus generalizing rules across magnitude types. The neuron shown in parts b and c primates. First, number neurons exist in comparable
was thus termed a ‘rule generalist’. d | The temporal dynamics of information about brain regions of the lPFC and IPS, with the IPS being
numerical rules during the numerical rule-switching task (as shown in part a) is encoded the first region to process number information in the
not only by single rule-selective neurons but also by an entire population of prefrontal neural number network. Second, number neurons in
cortex (PFC) neurons. Multi-dimensional state space analysis was applied to extracted both groups are tuned to preferred numbers. Third,
shared activity patterns in a population. At each time during a trial, population activity is
number neurons encode numerosity abstractly across
represented by an n‑dimensional vector in n‑dimensional state space (n = 380 dimensions
for 380 neurons). A principal component analysis was used to condense the population modalities and sensory-motor domains. Fourth, experi-
activity in a three-dimensional (3D) subspace. Red and blue lines show the time course of mental (in monkeys) or acquired (in humans) lesions in
average multidimensional dynamical trajectories during ‘greater-than’ and ‘fewer-than’ these cortical areas are detrimental to number process-
trials, respectively, with separated trajectories during the rule delay. Even if single ing. Fifth, active processing of numbers is not required
neurons adaptively switch tuning properties throughout an ongoing task, the entire for number neurons to exist and to respond to number.
population robustly differentiates between numerical rules. Numbers indicate trial Collectively, these similarities suggest that the parieto-
epochs depicted in part a. e | Numerical rule areas in the monkey brain. Frontolateral frontal network for nonverbal numerical information
view of a macaque brain showing areas in red that contain a relatively high proportion of constitutes an evolutionary pre-adaptation for semantic
number rule neurons. f | Frontolateral view of a human brain showing areas consistently number representations and mathematics in humans.
activated by calculations in functional imaging studies. IPS, intraparietal sulcus; lPFC,
Of course, this does not mean that symbolic and non-
lateral PFC; mPFC, medial PFC. Parts b and c are republished with permission of the
Society for Neuroscience, from: Representation of abstract quantitative rules applied to symbolic number representations need to be identi-
spatial and numerical magnitudes in primate prefrontal cortex. Eiselt, A.K. & Nieder, A. cal175–177. Even so, symbolic mathematical operations
J. Neurosci. 33 (17), 2013; permission conveyed through Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. may co‑opt or ‘recycle’ prefrontal and posterior parietal
Part d is based on data from REF. 160 and is provided courtesy of T. Ott. Part f is circuits178 to vastly enrich and enhance our symbolic
reproduced with permission from REF. 82, Elsevier. mathematical skills.

14 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

Comparisons of the response properties of number an advantage during learning, for example, when linking
neurons in different brain areas help to clarify the respec- numerosities with arbitrary shapes as a first step towards
tive contributions of the PPC and PFC in number pro- learning number symbols40,45,49,50. Number neurons also
cessing. They show that neurons in the PFC operate at support the learning and application of task rules, so
a more abstract level than do PPC neurons. By abstract- that neurons selective to quantitative rules provide easy
ing number and disposing of specific sensory informa- readout signals for rule-based decisions and action selec-
tion during encoding and memorizing sets, the brain tion. Because numbers are abstract categories and their
expands its computational power. It reduces the com- processing relies on derived principles, they constitute
plexity of neuronal signals that can then be processed ideal stimuli to probe sophisticated executive control
more rapidly and more easily. In particular, this provides functions of the association cortices.

1. Wiese, H. Numbers, Language, and the Human Mind. 23. Kaufman, E. L. & Lord, M. W. The discrimination of 40. Ansari, D., Garcia, N., Lucas, E., Hamon, K. &
(Cambridge Univ. Press, 2003). visual number. Am. J. Psychol. 62, 498–525 (1949). Dhital, B. Neural correlates of symbolic number
2. Nieder, A. Counting on neurons: the neurobiology 24. Anobile, G., Cicchini, G. M. & Burr, D. C. Number as a processing in children and adults. Neuroreport. 16,
of numerical competence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, primary perceptual attribute: a review. Perception 45, 1769–1773 (2005).
177–190 (2005). 5–31 (2016). 41. Ansari, D. & Dhital, B. Age-related changes in the
3. Whalen, J., Gallistel, C. R. & Gelman, R. Nonverbal 25. Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L. & Wilson, A. J. activation of the intraparietal sulcus during
counting in humans: the psychophysics of number Arithmetic and the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, nonsymbolic magnitude processing: an event-related
representations. Psychol. Sci. 10, 130–137 (1999). 218–224 (2004). functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn.
4. Merten, K. & Nieder, A. Compressed scaling of 26. Nieder, A. & Dehaene, S. Representation of number Neurosci. 18, 1820–1828 (2006).
abstract numerosity representations in adult humans in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 185–208 42. Eger, E., Sterzer, P., Russ, M. O., Giraud, A. L. &
and monkeys. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 333–346 (2009). Kleinschmidt, A. A supramodal number representation
(2009). 27. Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J. & Miller, E. K. in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 37, 719–725
5. Gordon, P. Numerical cognition without words: Representation of the quantity of visual items in the (2003).
evidence from Amazonia. Science 306, 496–499 primate prefrontal cortex. Science 297, 1708–1711 43. Cohen Kadosh, R. & Walsh, V. Numerical
(2004). (2002). representation in the parietal lobes: abstract or not
6. Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V. & Dehaene, S. Exact and This was the first report of neurons in the PFC abstract? Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 313–328 (2009).
approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene responding to the number of visual items monkeys 44. Nieder, A. The number domain — can we count on
group. Science 306, 499–503 (2004). were trained to discriminate. parietal cortex? Neuron 44, 407–409 (2004).
7. Siegler, R. S. & Opfer, J. E. The development of 28. Nieder, A. & Miller, E. K. Coding of cognitive 45. Diester, I. & Nieder, A. Semantic associations between
numerical estimation: evidence for multiple magnitude: compressed scaling of numerical signs and numerical categories in the prefrontal
representations of numerical quantity. Psychol. Sci. information in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron cortex. PLoS Biol. 5, e294 (2007).
14, 237–243 (2003). 37, 149–157 (2003). 46. Park, J., DeWind, N. K., Woldorff, M. G. &
8. Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S. & Spelke, E. Core systems 29. Nieder, A. & Miller, E. K. A parieto-frontal network for Brannon, E. M. Rapid and direct encoding of
of number. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 307–314 (2004). visual numerical information in the monkey. Proc. Natl numerosity in the visual stream. Cereb. Cortex 26,
9. Dacke, M. & Srinivasan, M. V. Evidence for counting Acad. Sci. USA 101, 7457–7462 (2004). 748–763 (2016).
in insects. Anim. Cogn. 11, 683–689 (2008). 30. Tudusciuc, O. & Nieder, A. Contributions of primate 47. Leibovich, T., Vogel, S. E., Henik, A. & Ansari, D.
10. Gross, H. J. et al. Number-based visual generalisation prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices to length Asymmetric processing of numerical and nonnumerical
in the honeybee. PLoS ONE 4, e4263 (2009). and numerosity representation. J. Neurophysiol. 101, magnitudes in the brain: an fMRI study. J. Cogn.
11. Agrillo, C., Piffer, L. & Bisazza, A. Number versus 2984–2994 (2009). Neurosci. 28, 166–176 (2016).
continuous quantity in numerosity judgments by fish. 31. Nieder, A. & Merten, K. A labeled-line code for small 48. Dehaene, S. Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition
Cognition 119, 281–287 (2011). and large numerosities in the monkey prefrontal 44, 1–42 (1992).
12. Bisazza, A., Tagliapietra, C., Bertolucci, C., Foà, A. & cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 5986–5993 (2007). 49. Ansari, D. Effects of development and enculturation
Agrillo, C. Non-visual numerical discrimination in a 32. Okuyama, S., Kuki, T. & Mushiake, H. Representation on number representation in the brain. Nat. Rev.
blind cavefish (Phreatichthys andruzzii). J. Exp. Biol. of the numerosity ‘zero’ in the parietal cortex of the Neurosci. 9, 278–291 (2008).
217, 1902–1909 (2014). monkey. Sci. Rep. 5, 10059 (2015). 50. Piazza, M. Neurocognitive start‑up tools for
13. Uller, C., Jaeger, R., Guidry, G. & Martin, C. 33. Ramirez-Cardenas, A., Moskaleva, M. & Nieder, A. symbolic number representations. Trends Cogn. Sci.
Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: Neuronal representation of numerosity zero in the 14, 542–551 (2010).
rudiments of number in an amphibian. Anim. Cogn. 6, primate parieto-frontal number network. Curr Biol. 51. Freedman, D. J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T. &
105–112 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.052 (2016). Miller, E. K. Categorical representation of visual
14. Krusche, P., Uller, C. & Dicke, U. Quantity 34. Nieder, A., Diester, I. & Tudusciuc, O. Temporal and stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 291,
discrimination in salamanders. J. Exp. Biol. 213, spatial enumeration processes in the primate parietal 312–316 (2001).
1822–1828 (2010). cortex. Science 313, 1431–1435 (2006). 52. Roy, J. E., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T. & Miller, E. K.
15. Koehler, O. Can pigeons “count”? Zeitschrift 35. Castelli, F., Glaser, D. E. & Butterworth, B. Discrete Prefrontal cortex activity during flexible
Tierpsychol. 1, 39–48 (in German) (1937). and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: categorization. J. Neurosci. 30, 8519–8528
16. Emmerton, J. in Avian Visual Cognition (ed. a functional MRI study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, (2010).
Cook, R. G.) [online], http://www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/ 4693–4698 (2006). 53. Viswanathan, P. & Nieder, A. Neuronal correlates
avc/emmerton/ (Comparative Cognition Press, 2001). 36. Sawamura, H., Shima, K. & Tanji, J. Numerical of a visual “sense of number” in primate parietal and
17. McComb, K., Packer, C. & Pusey, A. Roaring and representation for action in the parietal cortex of the prefrontal cortices. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110,
numerical assessment in contests between groups of monkey. Nature 415, 918–922 (2002). 11187–11192 (2013).
female lions, Panthera leo. Animal Behav. 47, 379–387 This was the first report of neurons in the parietal Visual number neurons were spontaneously
(1994). lobe responding to the number of hand movements present in monkeys that had never been
18. Brannon, E. M. & Terrace, H. S. Ordering of the that monkeys were trained to perform. trained to judge numerosity, arguing that the
numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science 282, 746–749 37. Sawamura, H., Shima, K. & Tanji, J. Deficits in action primate brain is hard-wired to assess
(1998). selection based on numerical information after numerical quantity.
Using painstakingly controlled visual stimuli and inactivation of the posterior parietal cortex in 54. Burr, D. & Ross, J. A visual sense of number.
elegant protocols, this seminal behavioural study monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 902–910 (2010). Curr. Biol. 18, 425–428 (2008).
showed that monkeys grasp the cardinal and Transient pharmacological inactivation of neuronal 55. Ross, J. & Burr, D. C. Vision senses number directly.
ordinal meaning of numbers. activity in parietal area 5 prevented monkeys J. Vis. 10, 10.1–10.8 (2010).
19. Cantlon, J. F. & Brannon, E. M. How much does from performing the correct number of 56. Arrighi, R., Togoli, I. & Burr, D. C. A generalized
number matter to a monkey (Macaca mulatta)? movements, while sparing general motor functions sense of number. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281, 20141791
J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 33, 32–41 and action selection. This demonstrates that the (2014).
(2007). activity of number neurons is causally related to Impressive psychophysical experiments reporting
20. Jordan, K. E., Maclean, E. L. & Brannon, E. M. number estimation. the generalization of number adaptation across
Monkeys match and tally quantities across senses. 38. Nieder, A. Supramodal numerosity selectivity of modalities and spatio-temporal formats,
Cognition 108, 617–625 (2008). neurons in primate prefrontal and posterior parietal suggesting a perceptual system to encode an
21. Beran, M. J. Quantity judgments of auditory and cortices. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 11860–11865 abstract sense of number.
visual stimuli by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). (2012). 57. Roitman, J. D., Brannon, E. M. & Platt, M. L.
J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 38, 23–29 Neurons in the PFC encode the same number of Monotonic coding of numerosity in macaque lateral
(2012). visual or auditory events in behaving monkeys. intraparietal area. PLoS Biol. 5, e208 (2007).
22. Wilson, M. L., Kahlenberg, S. M., Wells, M. & 39. Piazza, M., Mechelli, A., Price, C. J. & Butterworth, B. 58. Colby, C. L., Duhamel, J. R. & Goldberg, M. E. Ventral
Wrangham, R. W. Ecological and social factors affect Exact and approximate judgements of visual and intraparietal area of the macaque: anatomic location
the occurrence and outcomes of intergroup encounters auditory numerosity: an fMRI study. Brain Res. 1106, and visual response properties. J. Neurophysiol. 69,
in chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 83, 277–291 (2012). 177–188 (2006). 902–914 (1993).

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 15


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

59. Wang, L., Uhrig, L., Jarraya, B. & Dehaene, S. frontal and parietal neurons. Curr. Biol. 25, 1259–1269 110. Pinel, P., Piazza, M., Le Bihan, D. & Dehaene, S.
Representation of numerical and sequential (2015). Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations
patterns in macaque and human brains. Curr. Biol. 85. Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Spelke, E. & Pica, P. Log or of number, size, and luminance during comparative
25, 1966–1974 (2015). linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in judgments. Neuron 41, 983–993 (2004).
60. Barbas, H. & Mesulam, M. M. Cortical afferent input Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science 111. Kaufmann, L. et al. A developmental fMRI study of
to the principalis region of the rhesus monkey. 320, 1217–1220 (2008). nonsymbolic numerical and spatial processing. Cortex
Neuroscience 15, 619–637 (1985). 86. Pouget, A., Dayan, P. & Zemel, R. Information 44, 376–385 (2008).
61. Petrides, M. & Pandya, D. N. Projections to the frontal processing with population codes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 112. Walsh, V. A theory of magnitude: common cortical
cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus 1, 125–132 (2000). metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci.
monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 228, 105–116 (1984). 87. Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D. 7, 483–488 (2003).
62. Cavada, C. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Posterior parietal & Dehaene, S. Tuning curves for approximate 113. Markram, H. et al. Interneurons of the neocortical
cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for segregated numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. inhibitory system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 793–807
corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic Neuron 44, 547–555 (2004). (2004).
areas with the frontal lobe. J. Comp. Neurol. 287, Using fMRI adaptation, the authors retrace 114. Wonders, C. P. & Anderson, S. A. The origin and
422–445 (1989). logarithmically scaled number-tuning functions specification of cortical interneurons. Nat. Rev.
63. Lewis, J. W. & Van Essen, D. C. Corticocortical in the human parietal lobe. Neurosci. 7, 687–696 (2006).
connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal 88. Jacob, S. N. & Nieder, A. Tuning to non-symbolic 115. Wilson, F. A., O’Scalaidhe, S. P. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S.
processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque proportions in the human frontoparietal cortex. Functional synergism between putative
monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 428, 112–137 (2000). Eur. J. Neurosci. 30, 1432–1442 (2009). γ-aminobutyrate-containing neurons and pyramidal
64. Quintana, J., Fuster, J. M. & Yajeya, J. Effects of 89. Dehaene, S. & Changeux, J. P. Development of neurons in prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
cooling parietal cortex on prefrontal units in delay elementary numerical abilities: a neural model. 91, 4009–4013 (1994).
tasks. Brain Res. 503, 100–110 (1989). J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 390–407(1993). 116. Rao, S. G., Williams, G. V. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S.
65. Chafee, M. V. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Inactivation of 90. Verguts, T. & Fias, W. Representation of number Isodirectional tuning of adjacent interneurons and
parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence in animals and humans: a neural model. J. Cogn. pyramidal cells during working memory: evidence for
of neural activity during memory-guided saccades. Neurosci. 16, 1493–1504 (2004). microcolumnar organization in PFC. J. Neurophysiol.
J. Neurophysiol. 83, 1550–1566 (2000). 91. Stoianov, I. & Zorzi, M. Emergence of a ‘visual 81, 1903–1916 (1999).
66. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of number sense’ in hierarchical generative models. 117. Constantinidis, C. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Correlated
prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, Nat. Neurosci. 15, 194–196 (2012). discharges among putative pyramidal neurons and
167–202 (2001). 92. Meck, W. H. & Church, R. M. A mode control model of interneurons in the primate prefrontal cortex.
67. Vallentin, D. & Nieder, A. Representations of visual counting and timing processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 3487–3497 (2002).
proportions in the primate posterior parietal and Behav. Process 9, 320–334 (1983). 118. Johnston, K. & Everling, S. Task-relevant output
prefrontal cortices. Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 1380–1387 93. Gallistel, C. R. & Gelman, R. Preverbal and verbal signals are sent from monkey dorsolateral prefrontal
(2010). counting and computation. Cognition 44, 43–74 cortex to the superior colliculus during a
68. Berger, H. Über Rechenstörungen bei (1992). visuospatial working memory task. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
Herderkrankungen des Großhirns. Archiv. Psychiatrie 94. DeCharms, R. C. & Zador, A. Neural representation 21, 1023–1038 (2009).
Nervenkrankheiten 78, 238–263 (in German) (1926). and the cortical code. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 119. Merchant, H., de Lafuente, V., Peña-Ortega, F. &
69. Hécaen, H., Angelergues, R. & Houillier, S. Les variétes 613–647 (2000). Larriva-Sahd, J. Functional impact of interneuronal
cliniques de acalculies au cours de lesions 95. Georgopoulos, A., Kalaska, J. & Caminiti, R. On the inhibition in the cerebral cortex of behaving animals.
retrorolandiques: approche statistique du problème. relations between the direction of two-dimensional Prog. Neurobiol. 99, 163–178 (2012).
Revue Neurol. 105, 85–103 (in French) (1961). arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor 120. Epping, W. J. & Eggermont, J. J. Coherent neural
70. Lemer, C., Dehaene, S., Spelke, E. & Cohen, L. cortex. J. Neurosci. 2, 1527–1537 (1982). activity in the auditory midbrain of the grassfrog.
Approximate quantities and exact number 96. Salinas, E. & Abbot, L. Vector reconstruction from J. Neurophysiol. 57, 1464–1483 (1987).
words: dissociable systems. Neuropsychologia 41, firing rate. J. Comput. Neurosci. 1, 89–108 (1994). 121. Diester, I. & Nieder, A. Complementary contributions
1942–1958 (2003). 97. Sanger, T. Probability density estimation for the of prefrontal neuron classes in abstract numerical
71. Lewandowsky, M. & Stadelmann, E. Über einen interpretation of neural population codes. categorization. J. Neurosci. 28, 7737–7747 (2008).
bemerkenswerten Fall von Hirnblutung und über J. Neurophysiol. 76, 2790–2793 (1996). 122. Rao, S. G., Williams, G. V. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S.
Rechenstörungen bei Herderkrankung des Gehirns. 98. Deneve, S., Latham, P. E. & Pouget, A. Reading Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by
J. Psychol. Neurol. 11, 249–265 (in German) (1908). population codes: a neural implementation of ideal disinhibition: GABAA blockade of prefrontal cortical
72. Henschen, S. E. Über Sprach-, Musik-und observers. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 740–745 (1999). neurons engaged by working memory. J. Neurosci. 20,
Rechenmechanismen und ihre Lokalisation im 99. Stokes, M. G. ‘Activity-silent’ working memory in 485–494 (2000).
Großhirn. Zeitschrift Gesamte Neurol. Psychiatrie 52, prefrontal cortex: a dynamic coding framework. 123. Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P. S. &
273–298 (in German) (1919). Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 394–405 (2015). Wang, X.‑J. Synaptic mechanisms and network
73. Ashkenazi, S., Henik, A., Ifergane, G. & Shelef, I. Basic 100. Crowe, D. A., Averbeck, B. B. & Chafee, M. V. Rapid dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a
numerical processing in left intraparietal sulcus (IPS) sequences of population activity patterns dynamically cortical network model. Cerebral Cortex 10, 910–923
acalculia. Cortex 44, 439–448 (2008). encode task-critical spatial information in parietal (2000).
74. Cohen, L. & Dehaene, S. in The Behavioral and Cognitive cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 11640–11653 (2010). 124. Durstewitz, D., Seamans, J. K. & Sejnowski, T. J.
Neurology of Stroke. 2nd edn (ed. Godefroy, O.) 101. Meyers, E. M., Freedman, D. J., Kreiman, G., Neurocomputational models of working memory.
101–113 (Cambridge University Press, 2013). Miller, E. K. & Poggio, T. Dynamic population coding of Nat. Neurosci. 3, S1184–S1191 (2000).
75. Cipolotti, L. & van Harskamp, N. Handbook of category information in inferior temporal and prefrontal 125. Selemon, L. D. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Common
Neuropsychology. 2nd edn Vol. 3 (ed. Berndt, R.S.) cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 1407–1419 (2008). cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral
305–334 (Elsevier Science, 2001). 102. Stokes, M. G. et al. Dynamic coding for cognitive prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus
76. Cappelletti, M. The Oxford Handbook of Numerical control in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 78, 364–375 monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network
Cognition (ed. Cohen Kadosh, R. & Dowker, A.) (2013). subserving spatially guided behavior. J. Neurosci. 8,
808–836 (Oxford University Press, 2015). Population activity of PFC neurons experiences 4049– 4068 (1988).
77. Butterworth, B., Varma, S. & Laurillard, D. Dyscalculia: dynamic neuronal state transitions during the 126. Grieve, K. L., Acuña, C. & Cudeiro, J. The primate
from brain to education. Science 332, 1049–1053 course of a cognitive task, indicating that neural pulvinar nuclei: vision and action. Trends Neurosci.
(2011). tuning profiles adapt to accommodate changes in 23, 35–39 (2000).
78. Isaacs, E. B., Edmonds, C. J., Lucas, A. & Gadian, D. G. behavioural context. 127. Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Topography of cognition: parallel
Calculation difficulties in children of very low 103. Averbeck, B. B., Latham, P. E. & Pouget, A. Neural distributed networks in primate association cortex.
birthweight: a neural correlate. Brain 124, 1701–1707 correlations, population coding and computation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 137–156 (1988).
(2001). Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 358–366 (2006). 128. Baars, B. J. A cognitive theory of consciousness.
79. Rotzer, S. et al. Optimized voxel-based morphometry 104. Kohonen, T. Self-Organizing Maps (Springer-Verlag (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1989).
in children with developmental dyscalculia. Berlin Heidelberg, 1997). 129. Dehaene, S. & Changeux, J. P. Experimental and
Neuroimage 39, 417–422 (2008). 105. Tudusciuc, O. & Nieder, A. Neuronal population coding theoretical approaches to conscious processing.
80. Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D. & Dehaene, S. A of continuous and discrete quantity in the primate Neuron 70, 200–227 (2011).
magnitude code common to numerosities and number posterior parietal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 130. Fuster, J. M. & Alexander, G. E. Neuron activity
symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 53, 104, 14513–14518 (2007). related to short-term memory. Science 173, 652–654
293–305 (2007). 106. Genovesio, A., Tsujimoto, S. & Wise, S. P. Prefrontal (1971).
81. Harvey, B. M., Klein, B. P., Petridou, N. & cortex activity during the discrimination of relative 131. Kubota, K. & Niki, H. Prefrontal cortical unit activity
Dumoulin, S. O. Topographic representation of distance. J. Neurosci. 31, 3968–3980 (2011). and delayed alternation performance in monkeys.
numerosity in the human parietal cortex. Science 341, 107. Leon, M. I. & Shadlen, M. N. Representation of time J. Neurophysiol. 34, 337–347 (1971).
1123–1126 (2013). by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the 132. Merten, K. & Nieder, A. Active encoding of decisions
82. Arsalidou, M. & Taylor, M. J. Is 2 + 2=4? Meta- macaque. Neuron 38, 317–327 (2003). about stimulus absence in primate prefrontal cortex
analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and 108. Mita, A., Mushiake, H., Shima, K., Matsuzaka, Y. neurons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 6289–6294
calculations. Neuroimage 54, 2382–2393 (2011). & Tanji, J. Interval time coding by neurons in the (2012).
83. Nieder, A. Coding of abstract quantity by ‘number presupplementary and supplementary motor areas. 133. Shadlen, M. N. & Gold, J. I. in The Cognitive
neurons’ of the primate brain. J. Comp. Physiol. A Nat. Neurosci. 12, 502–507 (2009). Neurosciences. 3rd edn (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.)
Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 199, 1–16 109. Eiselt, A. K. & Nieder, A. Single-cell coding of sensory, 1229–1241 (MIT Press, 2004).
(2013). spatial and numerical magnitudes in primate 134. MacLeod, C. M. Inhibition in cognition. (eds
84. Viswanathan, P. & Nieder, A. Differential impact of prefrontal, premotor and cingulate motor cortices. Gorfein, D. S. & MacLeod, C. M.) 3–23 (American
behavioral relevance on quantity coding in primate Exp. Brain Res. 234, 241–254 (2016). Psychological Association, 2007).

16 | ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION www.nature.com/nrn


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
REVIEWS

135. Jacob, S. N. & Nieder, A. Complementary roles for 154. Mansouri, F. A., Buckley, M. J. & Tanaka, K. 176. Bulthé, J., De Smedt, B. & Op de Beeck, H. P. Visual
primate frontal and parietal cortex in guarding Mnemonic function of the dorsolateral prefrontal number beats abstract numerical magnitude: format-
working memory from distractor stimuli. Neuron 83, cortex in conflict-induced behavioral adjustment. dependent representation of Arabic digits and dot
226–237 (2014). Science 318, 987–990 (2007). patterns in human parietal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
This single-cell study points to the PFC as a 155. Rigotti, M. et al. The importance of mixed selectivity 27, 1376–1387 (2015).
selection stage for goal-directed number in complex cognitive tasks. Nature. 497, 585–590 177. Lyons, I. M., Ansari, D. & Beilock, S. L. Qualitatively
processing that represents behaviourally relevant (2013). different coding of symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers
as well as transiently irrelevant numerical 156. Dehaene, S. & Changeux, J. P. The Wisconsin Card in the human brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 475–488
information, whereas distractor-resistant working Sorting Test: theoretical analysis and modeling in a (2015).
memory representations seem to be maintained in neuronal network. Cereb. Cortex 1, 62–79 (1991). 178. Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. Cultural recycling of cortical
parietal VIP. 157. Duncan, J. The structure of cognition: attentional maps. Neuron 56, 384–398 (2007).
136. Anderson, M. C. & Green, C. Suppressing unwanted episodes in mind and brain. Neuron 80, 35–50 179. Danzig, T. Number — The Language of Science
memories by executive control. Nature 410, 366–369 (2013). (The Free Press, 1930).
(2001). 158. Seamans, J. K. & Yang, C. R. The principal features 180. Dehaene, S. The Number Sense. 2nd edn (Oxford
137. McNab, F. & Klingberg, T. Prefrontal cortex and and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the University Press, 2011).
basal ganglia control access to working memory. prefrontal cortex. Prog. Neurobiol. 74, 1–58 (2004). 181. Evans, S. E. in Evolutionary Developmental Biology
Nat. Neurosci. 11, 103–107 (2008). 159. Jacob, S. N., Ott, T. & Nieder, A. Dopamine regulates of the Cerebral Cortex (eds Bock, G. & Cardew, G.)
138. Feredoes, E., Heinen, K., Weiskopf, N., Ruff, C. & two classes of primate prefrontal neurons that represent 109–113 (Wiley, 2000).
Driver, J. Causal evidence for frontal involvement sensory signals. J. Neurosci. 33, 13724–13734 182. Dugas-Ford, J., Rowell, J. J. & Ragsdale, C. W. Cell-
in memory target maintenance by posterior brain (2013). type homologies and the origins of the neocortex.
areas during distracter interference of visual 160. Ott, T., Jacob, S. N. & Nieder, A. Dopamine receptors Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 16974–16979
working memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, differentially enhance rule coding in primate prefrontal (2012).
17510–17515 (2011). cortex neurons. Neuron 84, 1317–1328 (2014). 183. Dugas-Ford, J. & Ragsdale, C. W. Levels of homology
139. Suzuki, M. & Gottlieb, J. Distinct neural mechanisms 161. Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R. & and the problem of neocortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
of distractor suppression in the frontal and parietal Tsivkin, S. Sources of mathematical thinking: 38, 351–368 (2015).
lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 98–104 (2013). behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science 284, 184. Jarvis, E. D. et al. Avian brains and a new
140. Constantinidis, C. & Steinmetz, M. A. Neuronal 970–974 (1999). understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nat. Rev.
activity in posterior parietal area 7a during the delay 162. Gruber, O., Indefrey, P., Steinmetz, H. & Kleinschmidt, A. Neurosci. 6, 151–159 (2005).
periods of a spatial memory task. J. Neurophysiol. 76, Dissociating neural correlates of cognitive components 185. Butler, A., Reiner, A. & Karten, H. J. Evolution of the
1352–1355 (1996). in mental calculation. Cereb. Cortex 11, 350–359 amniote pallium and the origins of mammalian
141. Bisley, J. & Goldberg, M. Neural correlates of (2001). neocortex. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1225, 14–27 (2011).
attention and distractibility in the lateral intraparietal 163. Rivera, S. M., Reiss, A. L., Eckert, M. A. & Menon, V. 186. Scarf, D., Hayne, H. & Colombo, M. Pigeons on par
area. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 1696–1717 (2006). Developmental changes in mental arithmetic: evidence with primates in numerical competence. Science 334,
142. Postle, B. R. Working memory as an emergent for increased functional specialization in the left 1664 (2011).
property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience 139, inferior parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1779–1790 187. Bogale, B. A., Kamata, N., Mioko, K. & Sugita, S.
23–38 (2006). (2005). Quantity discrimination in jungle crows, Corvus
143. Lara, A. H. & Wallis, J. D. The role of prefrontal cortex 164. Luria, A. R. Higher Cortical Functions in Man. macrorhynchos. Anim. Behav. 82, 635–641 (2011).
in working memory: a mini review. Front. Syst. Neurosci. (Tavistock, 1966). 188. Moll, F. W. & Nieder, A. The long and the short of it:
9, 173 (2015). 165. Shallice, T. & Evans, M. E. The involvement of the rule-based relative length discrimination in carrion
144. Lennert, T. & Martinez-Trujillo, J. Strength of response frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex 14, crows, Corvus corone. Behav. Processes 107, 142–149
suppression to distracter stimuli determines 294–303 (1978). (2014).
attentional-filtering performance in primate prefrontal 166. Smith, M. L. & Milner, B. Differential effects of frontal- 189. Lyon, B. E. Egg recognition and counting reduce costs
neurons. Neuron 70, 141–152 (2011). lobe lesions on cognitive estimation and spatial of avian conspecific brood parasitism. Nature 422,
145. Malmo, R. B. Interference factors in delayed response in memory. Neuropsychologia 22, 697–705 (1984). 495–499 (2003).
monkeys after removal of frontal lobes. J. Neurophysiol. 167. Della Sala, S., MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., 190. Templeton, C. N., Greene, E. & Davis, K. Allometry
5, 295–308 (1942). Sacco, L. & Spinnler, H. The role of semantic of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode
146. Chao, L. L. & Knight, R. T. Contribution of human knowledge on the cognitive estimation task — information about predator size. Science 308,
prefrontal cortex to delay performance. J. Cogn. evidence from Alzheimer’s disease and healthy adult 1934–1937 (2005).
Neurosci. 10, 167–177 (1998). aging. J. Neurol. 251, 156–164 (2004). 191. Hunt, S., Low, J. & Burns, K. C. Adaptive numerical
147. Cantlon, J. F. & Brannon, E. M. Semantic congruity 168. Revkin, S. K. et al. Verbal numerosity estimation competency in a food-hoarding songbird. Proc. Roy.
affects numerical judgments similarly in monkeys and deficit in the context of spared semantic Soc. B 275, 2373–2379 (2008).
humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 16507–16511 representation of numbers: a neuropsychological 192. Ditz, H. M. & Nieder, A. Numerosity representations
(2005). study of a patient with frontal lesions. in crows obey the Weber–Fechner law. Proc. R. Soc. B
148. Bongard, S. & Nieder, A. Basic mathematical rules Neuropsychologia 46, 2463–2475 (2008). 283, 20160083 (2016).
are encoded by primate prefrontal cortex 169. Domahs, F., Benke, T. & Delazer, M. A case of ‘task- 193. Ditz, H. M. & Nieder, A. Neurons selective to the
neurons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 2277–2282 switching acalculia’. Neurocase 17, 24–40 (2011). number of visual items in the corvid songbird endbrain.
(2010). 170. Vallentin, D. & Nieder, A. Behavioural and prefrontal Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 7827–7832 (2015).
Populations of rule-selective neurons in the PFC representation of spatial proportions in the monkey. This was the first study reporting number neurons
of behaving monkeys signal abstract ‘greater-than’ Curr. Biol. 18, 1420–1425 (2008). in a non-mammalian species, and a species without
or ‘fewer-than’ rules applied to numbers. 171. Tudusciuc, O. & Nieder, A. Comparison of length a neocortex: the crow.
149. Okuyama, S., Iwata, J., Tanji, J. & Mushiake, H. judgments and the Müller-Lyer illusion in monkeys and 194. Divac, I., Mogensen, J. & Björklund, A. The prefrontal
Goal‑oriented, flexible use of numerical operations humans. Exp. Brain Res. 207, 221–231 (2010). “cortex” in the pigeon. Biochemical evidence. Brain Res.
by monkeys. Anim. Cogn. 16, 509–518 (2013). 172. Dehaene, S. & Brannon, E. M. Space, Time and 332, 365–368 (1985).
150. Vallentin, D., Bongard, S. & Nieder, A. Numerical Number in the Brain: Searching for the 195. Güntürkün, O. The avian “prefrontal cortex” and
rule coding in the prefrontal, premotor, and Foundations of Mathematical Thought (Academic cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 686–693
posterior parietal cortices of macaques. J. Neurosci. Press, 2011). (2005).
32, 6621–6630 (2012). 173. Jacob, S. N. & Vallentin, D. & Nieder, A. Relating 196. Veit, L. & Nieder, A. Abstract rule neurons in the
151. Eiselt, A. K. & Nieder, A. Representation of abstract magnitudes: the brain’s code for proportions. Trends endbrain support intelligent behaviour in corvid
quantitative rules applied to spatial and numerical Cogn. Sci. 16, 157–166 (2012). songbirds. Nat. Commun. 4, 2878 (2013).
magnitudes in primate prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 174. Genovesio, A., Wise, S. P. & Passingham, R. E.
33, 7526–7534 (2013). Prefrontal-parietal function: from foraging to foresight. Acknowledgements

152. Wallis, J. D., Anderson, K. C. & Miller, E. K. Single Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 72–81 (2014). The author thanks H. Ditz, T. Ott, L. Veit and S. Westendorff
neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. 175. Bulthé, J., De Smedt, B. & Op de Beeck, H. P. for valuable comments on the manuscript. H. Ditz helped with
Nature 411, 953–956 (2001). Format‑dependent representations of symbolic and Figures 1b and 1c, and T. Ott provided Figure 5d.
153. Genovesio, A., Brasted, P. J., Mitz, A. R. & Wise, S. P. non-symbolic numbers in the human cortex as
Prefrontal cortex activity related to abstract response revealed by multi-voxel pattern analyses. Neuroimage Competing interests statement
strategies. Neuron 47, 307–320 (2005). 87, 311–322 (2014). The author declares no competing interests.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION | 17


©
2
0
1
6
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.

You might also like