You are on page 1of 6

Epilepsy & Behavior 13 (2008) 614–619

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Epilepsy & Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yebeh

Residual cognitive effects of uncomplicated idiopathic and cryptogenic epilepsy q


Anne T. Berg a,*, John T. Langfitt b, Francine M. Testa c,d, Susan R. Levy c,d, Francis DiMario e,
Michael Westerveld f, Joseph Kulas e
a
Department of Biology, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL 60115, USA
b
Department of Neurology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14642, USA
c
Department of Pediatrics, Yale Medical School, New Haven, CT 06520, USA
d
Department of Neurology, Yale Medical School, New Haven, CT 06520, USA
e
Departments of Pediatrics and Neurology, University of Connecticut, and Connecticut Children’s Medical Center, Hartford, CT 06106, USA
f
Department of Neurosurgery, Yale Medical School, New Haven, CT 06520, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We assessed residual cognitive deficits in young people with idiopathic and cryptogenic epilepsy. In the
Received 18 May 2008 setting of an ongoing prospective study, we invited participants initially diagnosed and enrolled in the
Revised 16 July 2008 cohort 8–9 years earlier to undergo standardized neuropsychological assessment. Sibling controls were
Accepted 21 July 2008
invited when available. We analyzed 143 pairs in which cases had idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy
Available online 19 August 2008
and both case and control had normal intelligence. Compared with that for siblings, the Full Scale IQ
for cases was 3.3 points lower (P = 0.01) mainly due to slower processing speed, which was 5.6 points
Keywords:
lower (P = 0.0004). Word reading (P = 0.04) and spelling (P = 0.01), but not other scores, were also lower
Epilepsy
Neuropsychology
in cases. Remission status and drug use did not influence findings. In young people of normal intelligence
Cognitive deficit with idiopathic or cryptogenic childhood-onset epilepsy, substantial residual effects of epilepsy appear to
Children be confined largely to slower processing speed.
Epidemiology Ó 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Prognosis

1. Introduction tients typically were studied during the active phase of their
disorder, many while on AEDs [6,7,9–17]. Several studies enrolled
Epilepsy is associated with cognitive difficulties for a variety of prevalent cases with epilepsy, often from specialty centers, raising
reasons including the underlying symptomatic causes of epilepsy, concerns that more severe or intractable cases were overrepre-
the transient effects of seizures and interictal discharges, the sented [8,12,13,18], and some studies included individuals who
cumulative effects of repeated seizures on brain function and would be considered to have mild mental retardation (i.e., Full
structure, and the drugs used to suppress seizures [1–3]. The idio- Scale IQ < 70) [13,14] or did not state whether they were excluded
pathic and cryptogenic epilepsies may themselves be associated [15]. In contrast, the National Collaborative Perinatal Project
with some degree of behavioral and cognitive impairment inde- (NCPP) found little difference between cases and sibling controls
pendent of seizures and antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) and even prior tested at 7 years, provided both case and control were judged neu-
to the onset of seizures [4,5]. rologically normal [19].
It is not known whether the functional disturbances that cause It is worth noting that in small clinical studies, even syndromes
epilepsy also cause cognitive impairment, just by themselves. To such as benign rolandic epilepsy, a form of epilepsy with a near-
determine this, it is necessary to take into account the other known 100% chance of complete seizure remission [20], are associated with
causes of cognitive disturbance that occur with varying frequency evidence of subtle but significant cognitive difficulties [8–11,16].
in persons with epilepsy (e.g., perinatal stroke, other brain lesions, Current literature does not adequately address whether there are
mental retardation, active seizures, and AEDs). Many prior studies substantial differences among specific forms of idiopathic epilepsy
that performed detailed neuropsychological evaluations reported with respect to the nature and severity of cognitive difficulties.
significant case–control differences across broad cognitive do- It remains unclear whether there is an association between epi-
mains. These studies often focused on small groups [6–12]. Pa- lepsy and cognitive difficulties that is independent of the effects of
other associated causes of cognitive impairment, seizures, and
q
medications. To test whether such an association exists, we exam-
This work was funded by a grant from the National Institutes of Health, National
ined cognitive function in a sample of neurologically normal indi-
Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke RO1/R37-NS31146.
* Corresponding author. viduals with epilepsy and generally normal intelligence, who were
E-mail address: atberg@niu.edu (A.T. Berg). participating in a large prospective cohort study of individuals with

1525-5050/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.07.007
A.T. Berg et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 13 (2008) 614–619 615

childhood and adolescent-onset epilepsy. We used tests of general In secondary analyses, we included those pairs with uncompli-
intelligence, attention, and memory, cognitive domains that are of- cated epilepsy in which either the case or control had a FSIQ of 60–
ten affected by epilepsy and medications. 79, to compare our results with studies that included such patients
or did not specify IQ inclusion/exclusion criteria.

2. Methods 2.4. Analytic approach and methods

2.1. Recruitment For both primary and secondary analyses, we performed a ser-
ies of matched tests to determine the presence and magnitude of
The Connecticut Study of Epilepsy prospectively enrolled chil- case–control difference for overall intelligence, index scores, other
dren (ages 1 month up to 16 years) during 1993–1997. Intensive cognitive functions, and school achievement. We also determined
follow-up has been ongoing since then. Etiology, seizure types, whether the case’s medication, remission status, or syndrome type
and epilepsy syndromes were classified according to internation- might explain any case–control differences observed.
ally recommended criteria at the beginning of the study [21,22] Matched t tests were used for bivariate analyses. Multiple linear
and reclassified over the years as new information accrued. Details regression techniques were used with the difference score as the
of the methods, including the initial response rate for recruitment outcome variable to determine whether there were case–sibling
into the cohort (80%), were reported previously [23,24]. Clinical differences, as represented by the intercept term in the model,
neuroimaging, particularly with MRI, was common in this cohort and the extent to which remission and treatment status might ex-
at the time of initial diagnosis [25]. Over the years more imaging plain those differences.
was done, and during the 9-year assessment phase, we offered a All tests had 90% power to detect an approximately one-third
research imaging study to cohort members. In all, 85% of the entire standard deviation difference between cases and controls with an
cohort had at least one MRI study. a error of P = 0.05. Because the cognitive measures are intercorre-
lated, two-tailed P values unadjusted for multiple testing are re-
2.2. Standardized cognitive assessments ported in the tables and text. They are conservatively interpreted,
however, in light of the multiple comparisons and with reference
During the period 2002–2006, at approximately 8–9 years after to the threshold needed with a Bonferroni correction.
initial study entry for each child, all families were invited to partici-
pate in a standardized assessment protocol that included a neuro- 2.5. Ethics approval
psychological test battery. When available, we also enrolled a
neurologically normal full-sibling control without epilepsy or All procedures were approved at each phase of the study by the
unprovoked seizures. Tests were administered by a licensed psy- institutional review boards of all participating institutions and
chologist or trained psychometrician. Either Wechsler’s child conform to all state and federal laws and international standards.
(Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III, WISC-III [26]) or adult Written informed consent parental permission and child assent
(Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales III, WAIS-III [27]) scale, as appro- or subject consent were obtained.
priate for the subject’s age, was used to measure general cognitive
function. We used an abbreviated form of the WISC-III that preserves 3. Results
the four-factor structure of the full battery [28] and, to facilitate
comparability, a modified form of the WAIS-III consistent with the A total of 613 children were originally enrolled in the cohort.
WISC-III short form to estimate IQ and index scores. Full Scale IQ Fig. 1 provides the derivation of the analytic sample for these anal-
(FSIQ) scores provide a global measure of intelligence. The index yses. During the time of recruitment for the testing, 530 cohort
scores assess four key components of intelligence, derived by factor members were actively followed. Of those, 389 (73%) had idio-
analysis performed by the test developers. Verbal memory was as- pathic or cryptogenic epilepsy and intellectual function known or
sessed with the California Verbal Learning Test (child [29] or adult estimated to be within or above the mildly retarded range
[30] version, CVLT); visual memory with the Rey Complex Figure test (IQ > 60), based on prior review of all information in their medical
(CFT) [31]; and attention with the Continuous Performance Test and education records to date [34]. Of these, 272 (70%) had neuro-
(CPT) [32]. The Wide Range Achievement Test, Revision 3 (WRAT- psychological testing. We compared the 272 eligible tested cases
3) [33], was used to measure academic achievement. Age-adjusted, with the 117 who were not tested (Table 1). These comparisons
standardized scores obtained from the test manuals were used in suggest that those who were tested were reasonably comparable
all analyses. to those who were not with respect to potentially important fac-
tors that we could reliably ascertain, although there was a ten-
2.3. Eligibility for analysis dency for participating compared with nonparticipating cases to
have parents who had attained higher levels of education.
Primary analyses involved pairs in which the case had uncom- Matched sibling controls were tested for 172 of 272 (63%) cases.
plicated epilepsy (i.e., idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy, no epilep- Controls for 100 cases were not obtained because there was no full
tic encephalopathy) and both case and control had intelligence in sibling (N = 38), the only sibling was too young (N = 5), or the only
the low-average range or higher (i.e., FSIQ P 80). Pairs were ex- sibling had epilepsy or another severe neurological condition
cluded from these analyses if the case had an epileptic encephalop- (N = 8). In the remaining 49 cases, there were only 7 absolute refus-
athy or remote symptomatic epilepsy or if either the case or als. In the other instances, controls lived out of state, worked, or were
control had a FSIQ < 80. Cases with structural abnormalities on in school (including college). Scheduling difficulties and travel con-
MRI (other than incidental findings) were by definition excluded cerns ultimately precluded their participation in neuropsychological
from the group with idiopathic and cryptogenic epilepsy. The FSIQ testing.
exclusion reflected our goal of assessing the effect of epilepsy inde- Of the 172 cases, all but 3 had undergone at least one MRI study.
pendent of other neurological conditions including coexisting In 154 instances (89.5%) this was a research MRI scan obtained un-
mental retardation. Because we used a short form of the IQ tests der a standardized seizure protocol.
and to be conservative, we excluded anyone whose FSIQ fell in Of the 172 matched pairs, 29 pairs had either a case (N = 23,
the borderline range (70–79) and below. 13%) or control (N = 9, 5%) with a FSIQ < 80. In three pairs both
616 A.T. Berg et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 13 (2008) 614–619

Complicated
83 Lost to follow- N=31 (27%)
up (includes 13
deaths)
Uncomplicated
N=52 (63%)

613 originally
recruited Not tested
1993-97 N=80
Complicated
N=140 (26%)
Tested
530 actively N=60
followed in
2002-06 Case&control
Not tested FSIQ>=80
N=118 (30%) Sib-control N=143
Uncomplicated
N=390 (74%) N=172 (63%)
Tested
N=272 (70%) No control Case or control
N=100 (37%) FSIQ<80
N=29

Fig. 1. Derivation of analytic sample.

Table 1
Comparisons of potential differences between eligible cases who were tested and those who were not tested

Not tested (N = 118) Tested (N = 272) P value (df for v2)


Highest level of parental education
<High school (N = 12) 7 (6%) 5 (2%) 0.05 (2)
Completed high school (N = 210) 66 (56%) 144 (53%)
Completed college (N = 168) 44 (37%) 123 (45%)
Subject’s race/ethnicity
Caucasian (N = 333) 100 (85%) 232 (85%) 0.76 (3)
African-American (N = 33) 8 (7%) 25 (9%)
Hispanic (N = 21) 8 (7%) 13 (5%)
Other (N = 3) 1 (1%) 2 (1%)
Sex
Female (N = 193) 56 (47%) 137 (50%) 0.65 (1)
Male (N = 197) 61 (53%) 135 (50%)
Age at study entry
<5 years (N = 124) 33 (28%) 90 (33%) 0.19 (2)
5–9 years (N = 179) 62 (53%) 117 (43%)
P10 years (N = 87) 22 (19%) 65 (24%)
Epilepsy syndrome group
Idiopathic localization related 20 (17%) 43 (16%) 0.67 (3)
Cryptogenic focal 50 (42%) 133 (49%)
Idiopathic generalized 37 (31%) 79 (29%)
Undetermined 10 (8%) 17 (29%)
Any special education
No (N = 195) 61 (52%) 134 (49%) 0.60 (1)
Yes (N = 195) 56 (48%) 138 (51%)
Remission status*
<5 years seizure free (N = 128) 31 (26%) 96 (35%) 0.09 (1)
P5 years seizure free (N = 262) 86 (74%) 176 (65%)
Taking AEDsa
No (N = 275) 88 (75%) 187 (69%) 0.20 (1)
Yes (N = 115) 29 (25%) 85 (31%)
a
Assessed at the time of testing for those tested, or at the time of the global cognitive assessment if study neuropsychological testing was not performed.

were borderline. Thus, there were 143 pairs for our primary anal- 9 (6.2%) cases were taking more than one AED. In the case
ysis of case–control differences in uncomplicated epilepsy in indi- group, 20 (14%) had idiopathic focal, 71 (50%) cryptogenic focal,
viduals with FSIQs P 80. 40 (28%) idiopathic generalized, and 12 (8%) unclassified
In these 143 pairs, the mean age at testing was 15.0 years epilepsy.
for cases and 15.6 years for controls. Females constituted 49% In 49 pairs, both case and control completed the WAIS-III; in 68
of the cases and 57% of the controls. Ninety-five (66%) cases pairs, both completed the WISC-III; in 26 pairs, they were discor-
were at least 5 years seizure free; 99 (69%) were no longer tak- dant for test version (in 17 pairs, case WISC-III/control WAIS-III;
ing AEDs; 88 (61.5%) were both seizure free and off AEDs. Only in 9 pairs the converse).
A.T. Berg et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 13 (2008) 614–619 617

3.1. Primary analyses: Case–control differences—uncomplicated worse than their sibling controls on most cognitive indices, mem-
epilepsy and FSIQ P80 (N = 143 pairs) ory tasks, and school achievement, but not on attention scores
(CPT). Even with a corrected critical value of 0.003, several of the
Cases scored slightly worse than controls on average on all of differences would still be considered significant. In a series of t
the 16 scales from five different tests that were compared. This dif- tests and multiple linear regression analyses, we tested whether
ference was substantial for processing speed only. Cases scored an remission status (<5 vs > 5 years seizure free) or current AED use
average of 5.6 points lower than sibling controls (P = 0.0004) (Table explained case–control differences, represented by the intercept
2a). The critical value for a Bonferroni correction is 0.003. The case, in the regression model. Remission status and AED use did contrib-
control, and differences scores were all consistent with a normal ute to some case–control differences. Cases still scored signifi-
distribution and the average difference was not due to a few outli- cantly worse than their controls even after adjustment for these
ers. In all, 29% of cases had scores that fell > 1 SD beneath control factors.
values, whereas only 10% of cases had scores > 1 SD above control
values. 4. Discussion
In a series of multiple regression analyses, we tested whether
the case–control differences were substantially influenced by No prior published study to our knowledge has determined
adjustment for remission status, AED status of the case, and the whether epilepsy alone has an impact on cognition, independent
case’s syndrome. None was. of associated factors and causes of epilepsy that can by themselves
We also considered whether test version might influence the adversely affect cognitive function. In young people of normal
findings. We first adjusted for whether the case and control within intelligence (FSIQ P80) and normal neurological status, we found
each pair underwent the same or different IQ tests. This did not little evidence indicating substantial residual impairment in either
influence results. We limited the analyses to pairs in which the general or specific cognitive functioning. Although overall intelli-
case and control were concordant for the IQ test version. None of gence, measured by the FSIQ, was significantly lower in cases than
the findings was substantially altered. Most importantly, the controls, almost all of this effect was due to processing speed,
case–control difference for processing speed did not vary signifi- which remained substantially lower in cases despite overall intel-
cantly as a function of test version ( 5.0, P = 0.04, for pairs tested ligence in the ‘‘normal range.” The cases’ remission and medication
with the WISC-III, and 6.3, P = 0.009, for pairs tested with the status did not explain this finding.
WAIS-III). In the subgroup in which cases were 5 years seizure free References to slowed mental processing have been noted since
and off AEDs (N = 90 pairs), this processing speed difference per- antiquity in descriptions of persons with epilepsy. Formal studies
sisted ( 6.0, P = 0.004). have documented slowed reaction time relative to controls, but
have been confounded by the effects of medications, which also
3.2. Secondary analyses: Inclusion of cases and controls with slow processing [35]. One study of children with recently diag-
borderline intelligence or mild mental retardation (FSIQ = 60–79) nosed epilepsy reported that psychomotor function, measured in
part by the coding subtest from the WISC, also used in processing
When we included the 29 pairs in which either the case or con- speed, was most affected [15]. Slowed processing speed is a non-
trol or both had a FSIQ of 60–79, we found much stronger evidence specific finding associated with diffuse brain dysfunction and one
of case–control differences (Table 2b). This is not surprising as of the earliest presenting neurocognitive deficits in disorders
more cases (13%) than controls (5%) fell in that range. Cases scored affecting primarily white matter early in the course of the disease

Table 2
Case–control differences when (A) both case and control have a FSIQ > 80 and (B) either case or control may have a FSIQ of 60–79

Test score A. Case and control FSIQ > 80 (N = 143 pairs) B. Case or control FSIQ < 80 (N = 172 pairs)
a
Case mean (SD) Control mean (SD) P value Case mean (SD) Control mean (SD) P valuea
b
FSIQ 104.3 (13.9) 107.6 (12.7) 0.01 99.4 (17.6) 104.9 (14.3) <0.0001
Index scoresb
Verbal comprehension 108.2 (15.2) 110.5 (14.9) 0.09 103.6 (17.8) 108.0 (16.7) 0.001
Perceptual organization 106.4 (14.5) 106.8 (14.3) 0.78 101.8 (17.9) 104.4 (15.4) 0.09
Working memory 101.9 (14.5) 104.2 (13.2) 0.11 98.2 (16.4) 102.3 (13.8) 0.004
Processing speed 99.1 (15.2) 104.7 (13.6) 0.0004 96.4 (16.3) 103.4 (14.0) <0.0001
CVLT
List A 1–5c 52.6 (10.9) 53.5 (11.2) 0.47 51.0 (11.5) 53.1 (11.1) 0.07
Short-delay free recalld 0.10 (1.00) 0.18 (0.98) 0.45 0.08 (1.13) 0.15 (0.94) 0.03
Long-delay free recalld 0.12 (0.98) 0.27 (0.91) 0.20 0.01 (1.03) 0.23 (0.88) 0.03
Rey Complex Figure
Immediate recall 49.4 (14.4) 51.1 (14.1) 0.30 46.7 (15.4) 50.7 (14.1) 0.01
Delayed recall 48.7 (14.3) 50.0 (13.8) 0.43 45.8 (15.4) 49.6 (13.7) 0.008
CPT
Omissions 49.7 (15.4) 48.2 (9.2) 0.28 50.3 (15.0) 48.6 (9.8) 0.14
Commissions 50.6 (11.8) 49.3 (11.6) 0.33 50.9 (11.7) 49.9 (11.6) 0.39
Reaction time 46.9 (10.4) 46.6 (10.8) 0.79 47.5 (10.6) 46.9 (10.9) 0.52
WRAT
Reading 103.4 (12.9) 105.7 (9.0) 0.04 100.1 (14.8) 104.0 (10.1) 0.0005
Spelling 102.9 (13.1) 105.9 (10.5) 0.01 99.7 (14.9) 104.3 (11.1) 0.0001
Arithmetic 98.2 (18.0) 100.9 (13.3) 0.09 94.7 (19.0) 99.8 (13.3) 0.0009
a
Based on matched t test.
b
Scaled to a mean of 100 and SD of 15.
c
T scores, mean = 50, SD = 10.
d
Z score, mean = 0, SD = 1.
618 A.T. Berg et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 13 (2008) 614–619

(e.g., multiple sclerosis, multi-infarct dementia) [36–38]. Poten- and also provide an analysis of factors that might raise concerns
tially, the disturbances themselves that lead to idiopathic and about selection bias. Although individuals could self-select to par-
cryptogenic epilepsy persist in the form of relative inefficiency of ticipate in the cognitive assessments, there is little evidence of ma-
processing, even in cognitively and neurologically normal individ- jor differences between participating and nonparticipating cases.
uals whose epilepsy is in remission. Despite the prospective nature of this study, we do not have
These findings differ from those in the current literature that longitudinal data with standardized neurocognitive testing done
suggest substantial cognitive impairments in young people with at initial diagnosis and then years later. We do not know if cogni-
idiopathic and cryptogenic epilepsy. There are several potential tive function changed from what it was 8–9 years earlier; for some,
reasons for this. this would have been during infancy and very early childhood.
First, by using full siblings as controls we were able to assess the Studies of children with new-onset epilepsy report that they score
impact associated with epilepsy after adjustment for the two significantly worse then controls [14,15]. These studies examine
greatest determinants of cognitive function, genetic and environ- children on medication, with recent seizures, and in the active
mental factors [39]. This was not the case in other studies with stages of their disorder. Had our study subjects been tested at on-
the exception of one with several methodological similarities to set, a similar pattern of findings might have been observed. We
our own. In that study, after excluding those with IQ scores <80, think this would be unlikely. If the cases’ current levels of function
no difference was found in FSIQ when comparing cases with reflected substantial loss over 8–9 years that would mean that they
matched sibling controls [19]. would have had to be functioning at a substantially higher level
Second, most of the literature focuses on patients with active than their sibling controls prior to onset, because the current dif-
epilepsy on treatment. In one often-cited study of 251 prevalent ference between cases and controls is so modest. On the other
cases, 94% of patients were on treatment, with 58% taking two or hand, cases whose FSIQ was in the borderline to very mild mental
more drugs [13]. In a study of 53 children with recent-onset epi- retardation range, the ones we excluded from the primary analy-
lepsy, 77% of cases were taking AEDs when tested (15). Antiepilep- ses, may represent a subgroup that experienced declines since on-
tic drugs have a substantial impact on cognitive function [3,40–43]. set. In that regard, some studies considered changes in cognitive
Additionally, interictal abnormalities, even without seizures, may function from before or at onset [19,46] to about 3 years later or
impair cognitive function transiently [1]. This suggests that the ac- during the course of chronic epilepsy [47] and found no evidence
tive stage of epilepsy may be the most vulnerable period for expe- of change. Nonetheless, declines in cognitive function cannot be
riencing cognitive deficits. fully ruled out. They have been seen in studies that focus on severe
Third, some studies included subjects with FSIQs < 80. We ini- epilepsy and institutionalized patients [48]. Determining the exact
tially excluded these to isolate the effects of epilepsy itself from determinants of such changes may not be a simple matter.
other factors that might cause both seizures and subnormal intel- These findings should be reassuring for the more than half of
lectual function. When we later included them, our results were individuals with uncomplicated childhood-onset epilepsy and nor-
more consistent with published reports. This suggests that some mal intelligence. If the findings for processing speed reflect true,
findings in the literature may reflect inclusion of individuals whose residual cognitive comorbidity associated with epilepsy, this may
overall level of cognitive function falls below the normal range. have implications for schooling and employment. We caution,
It is possible that some studies included children with structural however, that these findings should not be used to minimize the
brain abnormalities, the presence of which is often not suspected importance of cognitive comorbidity in young people with epi-
prior to the onset of seizures and which cannot be determined with- lepsy. In a separate analysis we reported that a quarter of the entire
out appropriate neuroimaging. For example, Byars et al. recently cohort had borderline to severe intellectual impairment [34]. Fur-
demonstrated that children with such previously unsuspected le- ther, even if the effects of epilepsy appear relatively mild 8 to 9
sions performed less well on a battery of neuropsychological tests years later in individuals with overall normal cognitive function,
than did children with negative imaging studies [44]. the impact may have been much greater during the active phase
Epidemiologists consider mental retardation alone as sufficient of the disorder; a large proportion of the idiopathic and crypto-
grounds for classifying the cause of epilepsy as ‘‘remote symptom- genic cases received special education services [49]. Our study is
atic” (symptomatic) [22]. Technically, this includes anyone with a not designed to test whether such interventions are effective; how-
FSIQ < 70. The basis for this is an older retrospective, chart review ever, we strongly caution that our findings should not be used to
study of patients diagnosed from 1935 to 1974 in which mental argue that they are unnecessary. Older studies with prolonged fol-
retardation was defined as a FSIQ < 70. No information was avail- low-up of social and employment outcomes in adulthood have
able, however, about whether cognitive testing was performed or suggested that young adults with childhood-onset epilepsy are rel-
if this designation was simply based on clinical impressions at atively impaired in these domains compared with their peers
the time of initial diagnosis. Infants and young children with mild [50,51]. Long-term follow-up of our cohort has the potential to
mental retardation would not have been tested and detected. The bridge the gap between manifestations of the disorder in childhood
study, for the most part, considered only severe mental retarda- and consequences in adulthood.
tion, often in association with cerebral palsy [45]. Because IQ can
reflect environmental factors as well as the effects of seizures Acknowledgments
and medications, we required the FSIQ to be < 60 to use IQ alone
(absent abnormal motor examination, abnormal imaging, etc.) as We thank all the physicians in Connecticut who have allowed
the basis for designating the underlying cause as ‘‘symptomatic” us to recruit and follow their patients. Eugene Shapiro provided
[34]. essential administrative help throughout. Shlomo Shinnar partici-
The prospective identification and intensive follow-up of our pated in earlier phases of this study. Above all, the study would
community-based cohort generally diminish concerns over selec- not have been possible without the generous help of the many
tion bias that can be caused by prevalence sampling methods or families who have participated over the years.
by studies that do not report their response rates at all, a failing
common in the clinical literature in this field. We were not able
References
to test all study subjects or recruit controls for those that we did.
Unlike most studies, however, we have been able to describe the [1] Binnie CD. Cognitive impairment during epileptiform discharges: is it ever
derivation of our analytic sample, demonstrate high response rates, justifiable to treat the EEG? Lancet Neurol 2003;2:725–30.
A.T. Berg et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 13 (2008) 614–619 619

[2] Dulac O. Epileptic encephalopathy. Epilepsia 2001;42(Suppl. 3):23–6. [26] Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children. third ed. San Antonio,
[3] Meador KJ. Cognitive and memory effects of the new antiepileptic drugs. TX: Psychological Corp.; 1991.
Epilepsy Res 2006;68:63–7. [27] Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale. revised ed. San Antonio,
[4] Austin JK, Harezlak J, Dunn DW, Huster GA, Rose DF, Ambrosius WT. Behavior TX: Psychological Corp; 1981.
problems in children before first recognized seizures. Pediatrics [28] Donders J. A short form of the WISC-III for clinical use. Psychol Assess
2001;107:115–22. 1997;9:15–20.
[5] McAfee AT, Chilcott KE, Johannes CB, Hornbuckle K, Hauser WA, Walker AM. [29] Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. CVLT-C: California Verbal Learning
The incidence of first provoked and unprovoked seizure in pediatric patients Test, children’s version. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1994.
with and without psychiatric diagnoses. Epilepsia 2007;48:1075–82. [30] Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California verbal learning test. research ed. San
[6] Sturniolo MG, Galletti F. Idiopathic epilepsy and school achievement. Arch Dis Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1987.
Child 1994;70:424–8. [31] Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Rey Complex figure test and recognition trial. Lutz,
[7] Neyens LGJ, Aldenkamp AP, Meinardi HM. Prospective follow-up of intellectual FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995.
development in children with recent onset of epilepsy. Epilepsy Res [32] Conners CK. Conners’ continuous performance test (CPT-II). North Tonawanda,
1999;1999:85–90. NY: Multi-Health Systems; 2000.
[8] Piccirilli M, D’Alessandro P, Tiacci C, Ferroni A. Language lateralization in [33] Wilkinson GJ. The wide range achievement test. third ed. Wilmington,
children with benign partial epilepsy. Epilepsia 1988;29:19–25. DE: Jastak; 1993.
[9] Metz-Lutz MN, Kleitz C, t Martin A, Massa R, Hirsch E, Marescaux C. Cognitive [34] Berg AT, Langfitt JT, Testa FM, et al. Global cognitive function in children with
development in benign focal epilepsies of childhood. Dev Neurosci epilepsy: a community-based study. Epilepsia 2008;49:608–14.
1999;21:182–90. [35] Trimble MR, Thompson PJ. Neuropsychological aspects of epilepsy. In: Grant I,
[10] Staden U, Isaacs E, Boyd SG, Brandl U, Neville BG. Language dysfunction in Adams KM, editors. Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric
children with rolandic epilepsy. Neuropaediatrics 1998;29:242–8. disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 1986. p. 321–46.
[11] Lundberg S, Frylmark A, Eeg-Olofsson O. Children with rolandic epilepsy have [36] Schulz D, Kopp B, Kunkel A, Faiss JH. Cognition in the early stage of multiple
abnormalities of oromotor and dichotic listening performance. Dev Med Child sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006;253:1002–10.
Neurol 2005;47:603–8. [37] Sanfilipo MP, Benedict RH, Weinstock-Guttman B, Bakshi R. Gray and white
[12] Henkin Y, Sadeh M, Kivity S, Shabtai E, Kishon-Rabin L, Gadoth N. Cognitive matter brain atrophy and neuropsychological impairment in multiple
function in idiopathic generalized epilepsy of childhood. Dev Med Child Neurol sclerosis. Neurology 2006;66:685–92.
2005;47:126–32. [38] Peters N, Opherk C, Danek A, Ballard C, Herzog J, Dichgans M. The pattern of
[13] Bulteau C, Jambaque I, Viguier D, Kieffer V, Dellatolas G, Dulac O. Epileptic cognitive performance in CADASIL: a monogenic condition leading to
syndromes, cognitive assessment and school placement: a study of 251 subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162:2078–85.
children. Dev Med Child Neurol 2000;42:319–27. [39] Gray JR, Thompson PM. Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics. Nat
[14] Oostrom K, Schouten A, Kruitwagen C, Peters A, Jennekens-Schinkel A. Rev Neurosci 2004;5:471–82.
Behavioral problems in children with newly diagnosed idiopathic or [40] Meador KJ, Loring DW, Moore EE, et al. Comparative cognitive effects of
cryptogenic epilepsy attending normal schools are in majority not phenobarbital, phenytoin, and valproate in healthy adults. Neurology
persistent. Epilepsia 2003;44:97–106. 1995;45:1494–9.
[15] Hermann B, Jones J, Sheth R, Dow C, Koehn M, Seidenberg M. Children with [41] Meador KJ. Cognitive side effects of antiepileptic drugs. Can J Neurol Sci
new-onset epilepsy: neuropsychological status and brain structure. Brain 1994;21(Suppl. 3):S12–6.
2006;129:2609–19. [42] Meador KJ, Loring DW, Vahle VJ, et al. Cognitive and behavioral effects of
[16] Giordani B, Caveney AF, Laughrin D, et al. Cognition and behavior in children lamotrigine and topiramate in healthy volunteers. Neurology
with benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes (BECTS). Epilepsy Res 2005;64:2108–14.
2006;70:89–94. [43] Meador KJ, Loring DW, Ray PG, et al. Differential cognitive and behavioral
[17] Parrish J, Geary E, Jones J, Seth R, Hermann B, Seidenberg M. Executive effects of carbamazepine and lamotrigine. Neurology 2001;56:1177–82.
functioning in childhood epilepsy: parent-report and cognitive assessment. [44] Byars AW, de Grauw TJ, HJohnson CS, et al. The association of MRI findings and
Dev Med Child Neurol 2007;49:412–6. neuropsychological functioning after the first recognized seizure. Epilepsia
[18] Bailet LL, Turk WR. The impact of childhood epilepsy on neurocognitive and 2007;48:1067–74.
behavioral performance. a prospective longitudinal study. Epilepsia [45] Annegers JF, Hauser WA, Elveback LR. Remission of seizures and relapse in
2000;41:426–31. patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia 1979;20:729–37.
[19] Ellenberg JH, Hirtz DG, Nelson KB. Do seizures in children cause intellectual [46] Oostom KJ, van Teeseling H, Smeets-Schouten A, Peters ACB, Jennekens-
deterioration? N Engl J Med 1986;314:1085–8. Schinkel A. Three to four years after diagnosis: cognition and behavior in
[20] Bouma PA, Bovenkerk AC, Westendorp RG, Brouwer OF. The course of benign children with ‘epilepsy only’: a prospective, controlled study. Brain
partial epilepsy of childhood with centrotemporal spikes: a meta-analysis. 2005;128:1546–55.
Neurology 1997;48:430–7. [47] Aldenkamp AP, Alpherts WCJ, De Bruine-Seeder D, Dekker MJA. Test–retest
[21] Commission on Classification and Terminology of the International League variability in children with epilepsy: a comparison of WISC-R profiles. Epilepsy
Against Epilepsy. Proposal for revised classification of epilepsies and epileptic Res 1990;7:165–72.
syndromes. Epilepsia 1989;30:389–99. [48] Dodrill C, Ojemann GA. Do recent seizures and recent changes in antiepileptic
[22] Commission on Epidemiology and Prognosis, International League Against drugs impact perfomance on neuropsychological tests in subtle ways that
Epilepsy. Guidelines for epidemiologic studies on epilepsy. Epilepsia might easily be missed? Epilepsia 2007;48:1833–41.
1993;34:592–6. [49] Berg AT, Smith SN, Frobish D, et al. Special education needs in children with
[23] Berg AT, Shinnar S, Levy SR, Testa FM. Newly diagnosed epilepsy in children: newly diagnosed epilepsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47:749–53.
presentation at diagnosis. Epilepsia 1999;40:445–52. [50] Camfield C, Camfield P, Smith B, Gordon K, Dooley J. Biologic factors as
[24] Berg AT, Vickrey BG, Testa FM, et al. How long does it take epilepsy to become predictors of social outcome of epilepsy in intellectually normal children: a
intractable? A prospective investigation. Ann Neurol 2006;60:73–9. population-based study. J Pediatr 1993;122:869–73.
[25] Berg AT, Testa FM, Levy SR, Shinnar S. Neuroimaging in children with [51] Shackleton DP, Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite DGA, de Craen AJM, Vandenbroucke JP,
newly diagnosed epilepsy: a community-based study. Pediatrics Westendorp RGJ. Living with epilepsy: long-term prognosis and psychosocial
2000;106:527–32. outcomes. Neurology 2003;61:64–70.

You might also like