You are on page 1of 10

J Neurosurg 109:583–592,

109:000–000, 2008

Intraoperative mapping of language functions: a longitudinal


neurolinguistic analysis
Clinical article

*
Josef Ilmberger, Ph.D.,1 Maximilian Ruge, M.D.,4 Friedrich-Wilhelm Kreth, M.D.,4
Josef Briegel, M.D., 2 Hans-Juergen Reulen, M.D., 3 and Joerg-Christian Tonn, M.D.4
Departments of 1Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 2Anesthesiology, and 3Neurosurgery, and
4
Neurosurgical Department, Ludwig Maximilians University, Munich, Germany

Object. This prospective longitudinally designed study was conducted to evaluate language functions pre- and
postoperatively in patients who underwent microsurgical treatment of tumors in close proximity to or within language
areas and to detect those patients at risk for a postoperative aphasic disturbance.
Methods. Between 1991 and 2005, 153 awake craniotomies with subsequent cortical mapping of language
functions were performed in 149 patients. Language functions were assessed using a standardized test battery. Risk
factors were obtained from multivariate logistic regression models.
Results. Language mapping was able to be performed in all patients, and complete tumor resection was achieved
in 48.4%. Within 21 days after surgery a new language deficit (aphasic disturbance) was observed in 41 (32%) of the
128 cases without preoperative deficits. There were a total of 60 cases involving postoperative aphasic disturbances,
including cases both with and without preoperative disturbances. Risk factors for postoperative aphasic disturbance
were preoperative aphasia (p < 0.0002), intraoperative complications (p < 0.02), language-positive sites within the
tumor (p < 0.001), and nonfrontal lesion location (p < 0.001). In patients without a preoperative deficit, a normal
(yet submaximal) naming performance was a powerful predictor for an early postoperative aphasic disturbance (p <
0.0003). Seven months after treatment 10.9% of the 128 cases without preoperative aphasic disturbances continued to
demonstrate new postoperative language disturbances. A total of 17.6% of all cases demonstrated new postoperative
language disturbances after 7 months. Risk factors for persistent aphasic disturbance were increased age (> 40 years,
p < 0.02) and preoperative aphasia (p < 0.001).
Conclusions. Every attempt should be undertaken to preserve language-relevant areas intraoperatively, even
when they are located within the tumor. New postoperative deficits resolve in the majority of patients, which may be
a result of cortical mapping as well as functional reorganization. (DOI: 10.3171/JNS/2008/109/10/0583)

Key Words      •      awake craniotomy      •      brain tumor      •      cortical mapping      •     


neurolinguistic analysis

I
ntraoperative stimulation mapping in the awake pa­ anesthesiological, and neuropsychological teams. Even
tient, as first described in larger patient series by Pen­ though numerous retrospective studies have reported on
field and Boldrey26 for sensory and motor functions the feasibility of this complex procedure and its possibly
and by Penfield and Rasmussen27 for language functions, is positive influence on clinical outcome scores,3,5–9,12,15,17,20,22
considered the gold standard for identifying eloquent cor­ ,24,28–30,34,39,43,45,48,49
detailed pre- and postoperative neurolin­
tical sites. Intraoperative stimulation mapping techniques guistic data are scarce for patients undergoing operations
are usually applied with the goal of reducing the frequency using this method. Moreover, uncertainties continue to ex­
of postoperative deficits and at the same time maximizing ist as to the prognostic importance of obtaining linguistic
the extent of tumor resection. In the absence of prospective data on language functioning preoperatively, intraopera­
data, however, the value of these techniques is difficult to tively, and in the early postoperative period after micro­
assess. This difficulty remains particularly true for intra­ surgical treatment.
operative mapping of language functions, which requires The current longitudinally designed prospective
a most demanding cooperation among the neurosurgical, study was conducted to evaluate language functions be­
fore and after open tumor resection using a standardized
test battery and to detect prognostic factors associated
Abbreviations used in this paper: AAT = Aachener Aphasia Test;
fMR = functional MR; KPS = Karnofsky Performance Scale; mRS =
with improved/stabilized or deteriorated language func­
modified Rankin scale; WHO = World Health Organization. tion at the time of early and late follow-up evaluation.
* Drs. Ilmberger and Ruge contributed equally to this study. Additionally, associations between intraoperative stimu­

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008 583


J. Ilmberger et al.

lation mapping results of language functions and the ex­ ysis of leads II and V5 and pulse oximetry. Spontaneous
tent of resection were analyzed. breathing was monitored using side-stream capnometry. A
flexible tube with a foam tip was placed in a nostril allowing
CO2 monitoring while the patient was breathing and speak­
Methods ing. In addition, continuous transcutaneous measurement of
PO2 and PCO2 was installed. A radial catheter was placed
Study Eligibility for invasive blood pressure monitoring and blood gas analy­
Patients were considered eligible for this prospec­ sis.
tive longitudinal study (conducted from 1991 to 2005) if Analgesia and sedation were induced and maintained
they: 1) had untreated or recurrent lesions near or within by continuous infusion of propofol (0.5–1.2 mg/kg) and
suspected language-related brain areas; 2) were between remifentanil (0.05–0.01 μg/kg/min) with the dose titrated
15 and 70 years of age; 3) were fluent in speaking and according to the patients’ requirements and guided by
understanding the German language; 4) did not display the parameters of the ventilation and gas exchange. Any
severe aphasia prior to surgery; 5) were not mentally dis­ increase in end-tidal PCO2 > 50 mm Hg was addressed
oriented; and 6) did not present with risk factors for an by reduction or cessation of the remifentanil and/or the
awake craniotomy (such as claustrophobia). propofol infusion. The procedure was designed to main­
tain an optimal and reproducible performance of the pa­
Study Protocol tient during each step of the test procedure.
In patients not yet treated with anticonvulsant drugs,
Preoperative and Postoperative Neurolinguistic Eval- 750 mg phenytoin was administered intravenously at the
uation. Formal neurolinguistic testing was performed once beginning of surgery and then tapered during the next 2
preoperatively (1–3 days before the operation) and twice postoperative days.
postoperatively (within 21 days and 1 year after surgery) Patient Positioning. The major goal of patient posi­
using the AAT.11 The AAT consists of 5 subtests: 1) the tioning was to maximize the intraoperative comfort of
Token Test, involving pointing to and manipulating geo­ each patient (to reduce distress and anxiety), to allow prop­
metric forms on verbal command; 2) repetition, consisting er visualization of the screen presenting the stimuli, and
of repeating phonemes, words, and sentences; 3) written to comply with the requirements of the surgeon. On the
language, consisting of reading and writing; 4) naming, operating room table, the patient was placed in a comfort­
involving naming visually presented objects, colors, and able position supported by pillows. The head was elevated
scenes; and 5) comprehension, involving choosing the right almost above the thorax level, and turned about 60° to the
item in a set with distractor items after spoken/written de­ right or left side. To avoid the usual head clamp, fixation of
scription. In each of these subtests, disturbances are classi­ the patient’s head and neck was achieved by using a vacu­
fied as none/minimal, mild, moderate, or severe according um pillow (Schmidt GmbH) comparable to those used for
to the norms of the AAT. For overall classification within stabilization of trauma patients. After disinfection, a local
the framework of this study, patients were classified as hav­ anesthetic (60–80 ml bupivacaine hydrochloride 0.25%)
ing an aphasic impairment whenever they showed at least was infiltrated at the site of the planned skin incision, in­
mild disturbance in 1 of the subtests, and severity of this cluding the supraorbital area and the zygomatic arc.
aphasic impairment was then rated according to the scores Drapes were fixed by sutures surrounding the opera­
achieved in the Token Test. If formal neurolinguistic test­ tive site to avoid misplacement in case of uncontrolled head
ing could not be performed at the time of follow-up evalua­ movement. After positioning the neuropsychological mon­
tion, language performance was classified (aphasia yes/no) itoring unit on the side contralateral to the patient’s lesion,
according to the clinical data. the drapes were arranged in such a way that the patient was
Patient Information and Preparatory Training. Con­ able to recognize the presented stimuli on a liquid crystal
frontation naming (naming of pictures of objects) was display monitor. In case of significant refraction anomalies,
the basic task used in our mapping procedures, because the earpiece of the patient’s glasses was unscrewed on the
naming is a core component in language abilities and has side of the craniotomy, allowing the use of glasses during
proven to be a reliable and robust method for identify­ surgery.
ing widespread essential language sites.20 Within 3 days
prior to surgery, patients were informed in detail about Intraoperative Cortical Stimulation
the intraoperative stimulation monitoring procedure by
the neuropsychologist/neurolinguist also responsible for An enlarged pterional craniotomy was performed.
intraoperative testing. The patient was familiarized with Numbers printed on sterilized paper (2 × 3 mm in size) were
the stimulus material and trained to perform the nam­ placed on the exposed cortical surface. Prior to stimulation
ing task. The intraoperatively applied stimulus material the operative site was documented using photography. For
was custom-tailored to the individual capabilities of each direct cortical stimulation we used a Nicolet Viking IV P
patient; if a patient had difficulties recognizing or nam­ 8-channel monitoring unit and bipolar stimulation forceps
ing certain objects, these items were removed from the (Fischer Leibinger). Electrophysiological parameters for
stimulus pool. cortical stimulation were square wave, 50/second, for 0.2
msec. Stimulation intensities ranged from 8 to 16 mA. In
Anesthesiological Monitoring and Management. Mon­ general, all sites were stimulated with the lowest intensity
itoring included electrocardiography with ST segment anal­ first; if necessary, stimulation intensity was incrementally

584 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008


Neurolinguistic analysis of intraoperative language mapping

in­creased by 2 mA. For validation of the mapping proce­ to be at least 1 cm. To ensure this distance, rectangular
dure, stimulation was repeated for all sites. Moreover, a cotton patties (10 × 5 mm) were placed with the short side
photograph of the cortical surface with the numbers in at the point of language response, thus indicating a line of
place was obtained immediately after the stimulation to 10 mm distant to it. Typically, resection of the tumor ad­
be able to identify sites of positive stimulation later on. In jacent to eloquent regions was terminated when a safety
cases in which seizures were elicited during cortex stimu­ distance of 5–10 mm was reached. The extent of tumor
lation, a 4°C saline solution (0.9%) was used to irrigate the resection (total vs subtotal) was determined by postopera­
cortex. After completion of the mapping procedure, the tive imaging (CT scans and MR imaging).
paper numbers were removed and the patient was sedated
for the microsurgical resection procedure. When resec­ Postoperative Adjuvant Therapy
tion was directed near language-relevant areas, sedation
was reduced, and the mapping procedure was repeated.28,29 Additional conventionally fractionated external beam
Stimulation of subcortical pathways was performed in se­ radiation (60 Gy tumor dose in 2 Gy fractions) was ad­
lected patients, but results of this stimulation technique are ministered to all patients with WHO Grade III gliomas or
not described in the current report. glioblastomas. In patients with WHO Grade II gliomas,
radiation therapy was only initiated at the time of tumor
Intraoperative Language Testing progression when reoperation resulted in incomplete tu­
mor resection or when the recurrent tumor was considered
Stimulus presentations were performed using a com­ ineligible for further surgical treatment. Chemotherapy
puter system via a liquid crystal display screen. After was only used as a second- or third-line treatment option
placement of the stimulation forceps at a cortical site, the in this series.
stimulus presentation was begun, which triggered elec­
trical stimulation. During each stimulation (4 seconds in Clinical Evaluation
duration), a line drawing of an object together with the
carrier phrase “Dies ist …” (“This is …”) was presented Follow-up evaluation was performed within 21 days
to the patient. The task was to produce the carrier phrase after surgery and then 3, 6, and 12 months later using the
in addition to the name of the object. The use of a carrier KPS and mRS.4,37,46 The mRS uses the following scoring
phrase allows discriminating language and speech errors. system: 0 = no symptoms at all; 1 = no significant disability
If the carrier phrase was produced correctly, 3 types of despite symptoms, able to carry out all usual duties and
errors were possible: 1) an aphasic arrest (the object name activities; 2 = slight disability, unable to carry out all pre­
was not produced at all); 2) an aphasic disturbance (seman­ vious activities, but able to look after own affairs without
tic or phonematic paraphasia instead of the object name); assistance; 3 = moderate disability, requiring some help,
or 3) a naming delay (the object name was produced only but able to walk without assistance; 4 = moderately severe
after a marked delay). Disorders of speech also concern disability, unable to walk without assistance and unable to
the production of the carrier phrase. If no utterance was attend to own bodily needs without assistance; 5 = severe
produced at all, it was classified as a speech arrest. If the disability, bedridden, incontinent and requiring constant
utterance was dysarthric or marked by a strain to speak, it nursing care and attention; and 6 = dead. Also, speech or
was classified as a speech disturbance. After occurrence language deficits that were apparent during the clinical
of an error, at least 1 stimulus presentation was performed evaluation of the patients were documented. Severe deficits
without stimulation to ensure that the patient was able to that required logopedic treatment were distinguished from
perform the task correctly again. mild disturbances that were mainly reported by the patient
(such as occasional word finding difficulties). Impairment
of speech (dysarthria) was documented separately.
Documentation and Analysis of Intraoperative Findings
Language and speech disturbances as well as motor Statistical Analysis for the Evaluation of Prognostic
or sensory phenomena were documented by the neuropsy­ Factors
chological/neurophysiological team. Fifty intraoperative Risk factors for postoperative aphasia were obtained
mapping protocols randomly selected from the first half from univariate and multivariate logistic regression mod­
(1991–1997, 25 protocols) and the second half (1998–2005, els. The following variables (either continuously scaled or
25 protocols) of the study period were analyzed regarding dichotomized) were used for prognostic evaluation: age (>
the number of stimulated sites, the number and intensity 40 years or ≤ 40 years); sex; preoperative aphasic distur­
of the applied stimulations, and the mode of the evoked bances (yes or no); preoperative KPS score (continuously
disturbances. scaled); histological results (low-grade vs high-grade tu­
mors); prior treatment; duration of initial symptoms (> 6
Surgical Strategy
months or ≤ 6 months); localization of the tumor (frontal
The surgical approach to the lesion was determined vs other); extent of resection (complete or incomplete);
by T1- and T2-weighted MR imaging data, supported by localization of language-relevant cortical areas revealed
information from neuronavigation and/or intraoperative by direct cortical stimulation (intratumoral localization
ultrasonography. The site of the corticotomy procedure vs other); language relevant sites remote from the tumor
was customized according to the results of the cortical (> 10 mm from the tumor) versus nearby (< 10 mm); in­
mapping procedure; the distance from the cortical inci­ traoperative complications (yes or no); and postoperative
sion to areas of positive motor or language response had neurological deficits (yes or no). The length of survival

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008 585


J. Ilmberger et al.

and time to progression for different histological entities TABLE 2


were estimated using the Kaplan–Meier method. Histopathological findings

Results
Histopathological Findings No. of Cases

neoplastic lesions (WHO grade)


Patient Characteristics pilocytic astrocytoma (I) 1
gangliglioma (I) 1
From 1991 to 2005, 153 awake craniotomies were astrocytoma (II) 63
performed in 149 patients. Four patients underwent 2 sur­ oligodendroglioma (II) 10
gical procedures because of tumor recurrence. Clinical, oligoastrocytoma (II) 2
epidemiological, and histopathological data are summa­ astrocytoma (III) 44
rized in Tables 1 and 2. Patients with a nonfrontal tu­ oligodendroglioma (III)
oligoastrocytoma (III)
2
1
mor location had significantly larger tumor volumes (p gangliglioma (III) 2
< 0.05). Low- (WHO Grade II) and high-grade (WHO glioblastoma (IV) 14
Grade III or IV) gliomas were diagnosed in 74 (49.7%) gliosarcoma (IV) 2
and 62 (41.6%) patients, respectively, and the median du­ metastasis 2
ration of symptoms prior to surgery was 7 months. The other lesions
median KPS score prior to treatment was 90, and in 125
tumorlike lesion 1
cavernoma 4
cases (81.7%) patients were able to perform all usual ac­ cortical dysplasia 3
tivities at the time of the diagnosis (mRS score = 0 or 1). epidermoid 1
Focal seizures associated with language or speech distur­
bances were observed in 69 cases (45.1%). Preoperative
neurolinguistic testing identified 25 patients with mostly
mild aphasic disturbances (Fig. 1), whereas a language
and/or speech impairment was assumed clinically in 29 in another 8 cases (5.2%). The focal seizures were short-
cases (18.9%). No patient showed severe language impair­ lasting and could be controlled by irrigating the cortex
ment. with cold saline solution; the transient brain swelling was
caused by CO2 retention and successfully treated by the
Intraoperative Stimulation anesthesiologist.
Language relevant areas were found near the tumor
Intraoperative stimulation mapping was feasible in
margin (< 1 cm) in 114 cases, within the tumor in 17 cases,
all patients and generally well tolerated. Focal seizures
and remotely from the tumor in another 12 cases (a total
during the intraoperative stimulation mapping procedure
of 143 cases with language relevant areas). In 2 patients
occurred in 7 cases (4.6%) and a transient brain swelling
with previous open resections, stimulation was restricted
to the cortex above the lesion due to massive dural/pial ad­
hesions. Inadequate size of the craniotomy was probably
TABLE 1 the cause for negative intraoperative stimulation mapping
Clinical and epidemiological data in patients results in the remaining 8 patients.
undergoing awake craniotomies and cortical mapping The detailed analysis of 50 randomly selected map­
Variable Total ping protocols showed that between 10 and 48 sites (mean
21.3 sites) had been stimulated with stimulation intensities
no. of patients 149 ranging from 6 to 20 mA (mean 13.1 mA). A total of 1967
no. of awake craniotomies 153 stimulations at 1087 sites resulted in 536 disturbances,
which were classified as aphasic arrest (184 events), aphasic
sex (no. of pts)
male 70
female 79 disturbance (126 events), pronounced delay in naming (32
patient age (yrs) events), speech arrest (159 events), and speech disturbance
mean 39 (35 events). At 311 sites, subsets of multiple stimulations
median 38 were performed with constant stimulation intensities. Re­
sults of stimulation (no disturbance/disturbance) were con­
range 15–67
tumor laterality (no. of cases)
lt hemisphere 143 sistent in 81% of the cases (number of disturbances/number
rt hemisphere 10 of stimulations × 100). Stimulations at higher intensities (>
tumor localization (no. of cases) 12 mA) resulted in only slightly more disturbances (29%)
frontal 55 than stimulations at lower intensities (< 13 mA; 25%).
frontotemporal & insular 3
frontotemporal & parietal
parietal
4
13
Extent of Resection
temporal 55 Postoperative MR imaging revealed gross-total resec­
temporomesial
temporal & insular
8
3
tion in 74 (48.4%) of 153 cases. Gross-total resection was
temporoparietal 11 achieved in 36 (48.6%) of 74 cases involving WHO Grade
insular 1 II gliomas. The permanent morbidity rate was 3.9%. There
handedness (no. of cases) were no deaths. Gross-total resection was achieved signifi­
rt 135 cantly less often in patients with language-relevant areas
lt 18
within the tumor (p < 0.05).

586 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008


Neurolinguistic analysis of intraoperative language mapping

Fig. 1.  Chart showing the results of pre- and postoperative neurolinguistic testing in cases with and without apha­
sic disturbances. The classifications of mild, moderate, and severe refer to severity of aphasic disturbances in neuro­
linguistic testing. C = clinical evaluation only; T = formal neurolinguistic testing.

Neurolinguistic Outcome forming a profile for the varying language abilities as mea­
Results of postoperative neurolinguistic testing are sured by the Token Test and the naming, written language,
summarized in Fig. 1. If patients were dismissed or trans­ repetition, and comprehension tests (Fig. 2). Def­icits were
ferred very quickly or language impairments were too se­ most pronounced in the Token Test and the naming subtest
(70–80% subnormal scores), whereas performance in the
vere for formal testing, only clinical testing was performed.
subtests of written language, repetition, or comprehension
A quick transfer sometimes was performed when a bed be­
was impaired in only ~ 50% of these patients.
came unexpectedly rapidly available at the initial referring Tumor recurrence did not affect the resolution profile
hospital; if patients did not undergo a second test, it was of the deficit; that is, none of those patients with a persis­
mainly due to organizational difficulties in patients with tent new deficit developed a tumor recurrence within the
very remote places of residence. time frame of neurolinguistic longitudinal analysis.
The first testing took place after a median interval
of 7.0 days after surgery, whereas the second testing was
performed after a median interval of 29.5 weeks. At the
first testing, we found new language impairment in 41
cases (34 by formal testing, 7 by clinical report). Twenty-
five of these patients improved and had no disturbance
at the second postoperative testing (20 with formal test­
ing, 5 by clinical report); 14 patients continued to show
a language impairment (11 by formal testing, 3 by clini­
cal report), and 2 patients had missing data. Language
disturbances were mostly mild in this group of patients.
In those patients with preoperative deficits, 6 showed no
disturbance postoperatively whereas 19 continued to have
language deficits that were somewhat more pronounced
than those observed preoperatively (16 by formal testing,
3 by clinical report). Of these 19 cases, 13 continued to
show aphasic disturbances at the second postoperative
testing (8 with formal testing, 5 by clinical report), and
only 3 pa­tients were free of any disturbance at that time.
Fig. 2.  Bar graph showing the percentage of cases with mild,
Three pa­tients had missing data. modest, severe, or no aphasic disturbances on the subtests of the
In 41 patients with no preoperative deficits but with aphasia battery at the first postoperative test. COM = comprehen­
new deficits at the first postoperative testing, we analyzed sion; NAM = naming; REP = repetition; TT = Token Test; WRIT
these deficits with regard to the subtests of the AAT, thus = written language.

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008 587


J. Ilmberger et al.

Risk Factors for a Postoperative Language Disturbance


Univariate and multivariate logistic regression anal­
ysis showed that preoperative aphasia (p < 0.0002), an
intraoperative complication (p < 0.02), language-positive
stimulation sites within the lesion (p < 0.001), and non­
frontal lesion locations (p < 0.001) were risk factors for a
language disturbance in the early postoperative interval.
Fifty-three percent of the 17 patients with positive stimu­
lation sites within the tumor developed an early new post­
operative language-related deficit. Age, KPS score, sex,
duration of symptoms, symptoms at presentation, histo­
logical results of the lesion, surgery for a recurrent tu­mor,
and extent of resection were nonsignificant covariates.
Neither the number of stimulation sites outside the le­
sion nor their relationship to the lesion (remote or nearby)
gained significant influence in the prognostic models.
At the time of late neurolinguistic evaluation (7 months
after surgery) only preoperative aphasia (p < 0.001) or alter­
natively early postoperative aphasia (which was pos­itively
intercorrelated with preoperative aphasia) and in­creased age
(> 40 years of age, p < 0.02) were associated with persistent Fig. 3.  Graph showing the impact of preoperative naming ca­
language impairment. The prognostic impact of tumor lo­ pabilities on early postoperative neurolinguistic outcome. Nor­mal
cation, intraoperative complications, and positive stimula­ (but submaximal) naming performance on the naming subtest,
tion sites within the tumor was no longer significant. al­though in the normal range (111–120), is negatively correlated
A separate multivariate analysis of those patients with the percentage of patients suffering from early postoperative
harboring no language disturbance before treatment con­ aphasic disturbances.
firmed the unfavorable influence of the following inde­
pendent variables: 1) language-positive stimulation site or Time to Progression
sites within the lesion (p < 0.002); 2) intraoperative com­ The 5-year progression-free survival rate for patients
plications (p < 0.05); and 3) nonfrontal lesion locations (p with WHO Grade II and III gliomas was 63 and 57%,
= 0.003). Moreover, a submaximal performance on both respectively (Fig. 4). This difference was statistically not
the Token Test and the naming subtest (but still within the significant. Patients with glioblastomas did significantly
normal range) was significantly associated with the oc­ worse than patients with other types of tumors (median
currence of an early postoperative aphasic disturbance af­ progression free survival = 16 months, p < 0.001).
ter univariate analysis (p < 0.003 and p < 0.0007, respec­
tively); that is, the lower the performance score on either
of these subtests, the greater the risk of an early postop­ Discussion
erative language disturbance in this patient series. This Language-related brain regions have been shown to
correlation is depicted for the naming subtest in Fig. 3. be embedded in complex and highly interconnected net­
After adjustment for the effects of the other 3 covariates works,31,35 which do not permit one to establish unequivo­
noted above, the influence of the Token Test was no lon­ cal associations between a specific lesion site and aphasic
ger significant, whereas the influence of the naming sub­ symptoms.50 Accordingly, uncertainties continue to exist
test scores was maintained (p < 0.0003). After univariate as to what extent the impairment of a single intraoper-
analysis, a submaximal preoperative performance in the atively detected language-relevant area and its cortical
naming subtest also remained a statistically significant
(p < 0.02) prognostic risk factor for the late neurolinguis­
tic evaluation. TABLE 3
Clinical status evaluation as estimated using the mRS*
Clinical Evaluation
mRS Testing Point
Evaluation of the patients’ clinical status before and
after treatment, as estimated using the mRS, is summa­ Category Preop Postop 3 mos 6 mos 12 mos
rized in Table 3. Eighty-two percent of the cases presented mRS Score 1 125 (81.7) 90 (58.8) 94 (72.9) 88 (75.2) 80 (76.2)
with an mRS score of 1 before treatment. Immediately mRS Score 2 13 (8.5) 23 (15.0) 22 (17.1) 19 (16.2) 16 (15.2)
postoperatively there was a significant clinical deteriora­ mRS Score 3 15 (9.8) 28 (18.3) 10 (7.8) 5 (4.3) 7 (6.7)
tion (p < 0.0001), which continuously improved at 3, 6, and mRS Score 4
total
0
153
12 (7.8)
153
3 (2.3)
129
5 (4.3)
117
2 (1.9)
105
12 months postoperatively to near initial values (p = 0.12). lost to FU 18 8 8
One year after treatment, however, 1.9% of the cases pre­ excluded due 6 10 14
sented with an mRS score of 4. The median KPS score was to POD
90 prior to surgery as well as at 3, 6, and 12 months after * Values given are number of cases (%). Abbreviations: FU = follow-up;
treatment. POD = progression of disease.

588 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008


Neurolinguistic analysis of intraoperative language mapping

Fig. 4.  Line graph showing the progression-free survival rate of patients after open tumor resection according to
WHO tumor grade.

and/or subcortical representation during tumor resection imaging (as compared with intraoperative stimulation
contribute to postoperative language-related outcome map­ping) still remains difficult. The transfer of preop­
scores. Taking into account these uncertainties, every at­ eratively generated fMR imaging data into a neuronav-
tempt was undertaken to preserve those language-relevant igation system easily resolves mathematically induced
areas that were detected in the current patient series, and in­consistencies of the spatial resolution, which might be
resection of a tumor adjacent to language-relevant areas fur­ther aggravated by brain-shift effects after opening of
was usually discontinued when a safety margin of 5–10 the dura.19,36
mm was reached. Using this surgical strategy, gross-total New fMR imaging approaches with the aid of wa­
resection was achieved in 48% of all treated patients with ter diffusion tensor imaging permit more direct explora­
tumors (and in 48% of the subpopulation of patients with tion of tissue metabolism (such as the study conducted by
low-grade tumors as well). This approach, however, by no Mori and Zhang18) than traditional blood oxygen level–
means indicates that the extent of resection was optimally dependent monitoring and deserve further evaluation.
adjusted in every single patient of this study. For a more Ad­ditionally, white matter tracts can be visualized by
refined elucidation of structure–function relationships, ra­diological tractography obtained during diffusion ten­
however, pre- and postsurgical functional neuroimag­ sor MR imaging. However, these data have not yet been
ing results and their correlation with both intraoperative functionally validated and cannot be reliably integrated
stimulation mapping and language functioning of the pa­ into neuronavigational systems.14,16 Thus, given these and
tient over time appears to be essential. other limitations in the fascinating field of functional im­
Functional MR imaging analysis, however, which aging, open tumor resection in the vicinity of language-
traditionally exploits the blood oxygen level–dependent relevant areas should still rely initially on the results of
effect for delineation of activated cortical regions during intraoperative stimulation mapping.
execution of specific tasks, has been shown to be strongly In the current patient series, a highly selected patient
influenced by subject-specific parameters, task generic population was addressed. Patients were young, exhib­
parameters, cortical regions analyzed, and the statisti- ited excellent performance scores before treatment, and
cal analysis of the data (such as the choice of thresh­ the vast majority (82%) were able to perform all usual
old); accordingly only a moderate test–retest reliability activities at the time of the diagnosis. Open tumor resec­
of the fMR imaging mapping results has been observed tion alone (applied to all patients with WHO Grade II
so far.10,25,38,41 Moreover, the congruence of fMR imag­ gliomas) or in combination with external beam radiation
ing and intraoperative stimulation mapping data remains therapy (applied to all patients with high-grade tumors)
unsatisfying. Although very optimistic reports with sensi­ resulted in extremely favorable outcome data, which
tivity values as high as 100% have been reported for fMR were in the upper range of those reported in the litera­
imaging,32 the occurrence of missed sites (cortical sites ture (a 5-year progression-free survival rate for patients
found to be essential by intraoperative stimulation map­ with WHO Grade II and III gliomas of 63 and 57%, re­
ping but not identified by preoperative imaging as report- spectively). Apparently, eligibility for complex surgical
ed by Roux et al.40) has precluded the use of imag­ing procedures such as intraoperative stimulation mapping
data for guiding resections until now. Validation of fMR of language function appears to be intrinsically associ­

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008 589


J. Ilmberger et al.

ated with a favorable pattern of prognostic factors and serves further prospective evaluation and might become a
the obtained results should not be extrapolated to all pa­ major factor for individual patient counseling and risk esti­
tients with low- and high-grade gliomas. The aim of the mation. The unfavorable prognostic impact of preoperative
current prospective longitudinal study was, for the first aphasia has already been reported by other groups (such as
time, to evaluate language function before and after in­ Bello et al.2) and has now been incorporated more clearly
traoperative stimulation mapping–guided surgery using a into the perspective of an unfavorable long-term prognosis
standardized neurolinguistic test battery. The study was for a considerable number of patients. The somewhat di­
designed to identify prognostic factors and patients at vergent prognostic findings of early and late neurolinguis­
risk for surgery-related language impairment. Given the tic examinations might indicate the effects of functional
methodological limitations of intraoperative stimulation reorganization during the follow-up period, which could
mapping techniques of language functioning, no attempt involve perilesional areas and/or distant ipsilateral struc­
was made to focus on structure–function relationships in tures, and/or the activation of homologous contralateral
this patient series. regions. One might speculate whether potentially favorable
effects of functional reorganization are less pronounced in
Risk Factors for Early and Late Postoperative Language the older subpopulation (> 40 years old) and whether those
Impairment effects had been already exhausted in those patients pre­
Early follow-up evaluation (within 21 days) revealed senting with phasic disturbances before treatment. More
a language-related deterioration rate of 32%. Risk factors longitudinally obtained functional image-based data and
associated with early language impairment were preop­ their correlation with pre- and postsurgical neurolinguistic
erative aphasia, language positive stimulation sites within status are necessary to more accurately elucidate the re­
the tumor, a nonfrontal tumor location, and intraoperative covery mechanisms of aphasia after open tumor resection
complications. The unfavorable impact of a nonfrontal and to adjust findings of intraoperative stimulation map­
tu­mor location—which has not yet been reported in the ping more precisely to long-term neurolinguistic outcome,
literature—might be explained by its significant associa­ particularly in low-grade glioma surgery.
tion with larger tumor volumes. More data are necessary Clinical follow-up evaluation results in this study (as
to elucidate the impact of tumor size. Patients who pre­ measured by the mRS) paralleled neurolinguistic out­
sented with a suboptimal but still normal performance in come measurements: early evaluation indicated a signifi­
the naming test preoperatively experienced early postop­ cant clinical deterioration, which continuously improved
erative disturbances significantly more often even after at 3, 6, and 12 months postoperatively. Significant differ­
adjustment for the effects of the other 4 risk factors. ences, as compared with the preoperative findings, were
Removal of positive stimulation sites within the tu­ no longer significant 3 months after surgery.
mor, as was done were during the initial phase of this
series, was explained by the assumption of the treating Neurolinguistic Considerations
surgeons that intratumoral language positive sites might Confrontation naming is the most widely intraopera­
no longer or might not relevantly be involved in the lan­ tively used task and has proven to be a robust method for
guage network. This assumption was found to be incor­ identifying essential language sites also outside the clas­
rect in the early phase of this patient series when this sic language zones. Additional tasks such as reading or
was not yet common knowledge. The deficits following calculation may be added in individual cases, for example
resection or cortical undermining of 1 or several sites in when parietal areas are to be mapped. To achieve high
4 patients, even when further language-positive stimu­ reliability in mapping, we believe it is absolutely neces­
lation sites were found outside the tumor, demonstrated sary to use custom-tailored stimulus material that has
that such sites still bear important language functions and been selected preoperatively in a training session with the
are essential for favorable early neurolinguistic outcome patient. No stimulus material that the patient is unfamil­
scores. This finding has been confirmed by other inves­ iar with or tasks the patient has not shown to be able to
tigative groups.1,23,42,44 However, in 2 of these 4 patients master should be used intraoperatively to avoid confusion
the initial new postoperative deficits resolved completely and delays during the mapping procedure. In our experi­
within 12 months, and the remaining 2 patients regained ence, a set of at least 20 items that the patient is able to
their preoperative status that had involved slight language name consistently in preoperative training is necessary
impairments. Thus, the resolution profile seemingly did to avoid too many repetitions and thus automatization of
not differ from all other patients. responses during stimulation mapping.
At the time of late neurolinguistic evaluation (7 Despite those precautions, the detailed analysis of
months after surgery) preoperative aphasia or alternatively mapping protocols in a random sample of 50 patients
early postoperative aphasia (which was positively intercor­ showed that results of intraoperative mapping are not al­
related with preoperative aphasia) and increased age (> 40 ways consistent at specific sites, that is, errors are not al­
years) were associated with persistent language impair­ ways or never evoked at a specific site during repeated
ment. Also, after univariate analysis, performance on the testing. Whitaker and Ojemann47 described this phenom­
naming subtest remained a prognostic factor. As naming enon as graded localization; Ojemann21 believes it is an
capabilities might be closely associated with verbal intel­ indication of “graded transition from essential to non-es­
ligence, it could be assumed that patients with a high verbal sential cortex.” Within the connection of a network, some
intelligence level are less prone to neurolinguistic deterio­ nodes would have weaker connections, and stimulations
ration after microsurgical treatment. This hypothesis de­ of such nodes would produce disturbances on a stochastic

590 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008


Neurolinguistic analysis of intraoperative language mapping

basis only. An alternative explanation of these inconsis­ be most sensitive for the detection of tumor-related apha­
tencies would be that specific sites show category speci­ sic disturbances and were able to identify patients at risk
ficity, that is, that specific sites are differentially involved before treatment; normal (but suboptimal) preoperative
in the processing of items from different semantic catego­ naming capacities are an indication of a higher risk of
ries (such as living vs nonliving things). Category-specific early postoperative language impairment. Further devel­
effects are well documented in functional imaging and in opments should include even more extended pre- and in­
lesion studies, and Ilmberger and associates13 were able to traoperative testing to further elucidate the nature of risk
demonstrate the influence of using items from different factors related to verbal intelligence and to strengthen the
semantic categories on errors evoked during stimulation consistency of intraoperative mapping results, such as by
by subdural electrodes in epilepsy patients. Also, Pou­ systematic testing of different semantic categories.
ratian et al.33 identified a category-specific naming area
us­ing intraoperative stimulation monitoring in a single Acknowledgments
patient. To our knowledge, however, no other studies have
We gratefully acknowledge the editorial and secretarial sup­
focused on semantic criteria for the selection of items in port of Ms. I. Anders during preparation of this manuscript. The
stimulation mapping, and this would be a promising vari­ co­operative effort of Dr. W. Eisner (Innsbruck, formerly of Ludwig
able to be included in future work. Maximilians University, Munich) and Ms. E. Schröck during the ini­
tial phase of the language-mapping project is appreciated.
Patient Tolerance and Commitment of the Neurosurgical Hans-Juergen Reulen, M.D., is Professor Emeritus in the De­­
Team part­ment of Neurosurgery, Ludwig Maximilians University, Mu­nich,
Ger­many.
Earlier reports have shown that intraoperative stimu­
lation mapping is tolerated well by the vast majority of Disclaimer
patients and that the experience of this procedure does
The authors report no conflict of interest concerning the mate­
not lead to psychological trauma.6,49 In our experience, it rials or methods used in this study or the findings specified in this
is extremely important that the patient and the neurosur­ paper.
gical team develop some form of mutual trust in working
together in the operating room. Penfield and Boldrey26 References
commented on this issue in a beautiful way: “It is essen­
  1.  Atlas SW, Howard RS, Maldjian J, Alsop D, Detre JA, Listerud
tial that the patient should be in sympathy with operator J, et al: Functional magnetic resonance imaging of regional
and anaesthetist, and it is an interesting comment on the brain activity in patients with intracerebral gliomas: findings
bravery and fortitude of mankind that almost without ex­ and implications for clinical management. Neurosurgery 38:
ception these subjects have gone through the ordeal of op­ 329–338, 1996
eration patiently and intelligently…” The neuropsycholo­   2.  Bello L, Gallucci M, Fava M, Carrabba G, Giussani C, Acerbi F,
gist or neurolinguist plays a key role in the entire process, et al: Intraoperative subcortical language tract mapping guides
because during the few minutes of mapping it is his or her removal of gliomas involving speech areas. Neurosur­gery 60:
job to guide the patient through the procedure. Enough 67–82, 2007
  3.  Berger MS, Ojemann GA: Intraoperative brain mapping tech­
time should be given to the preoperative dialogue to make niques in neuro-oncology. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg 58:
sure that all the patients’ questions are answered and that 153–161, 1992
the patient and neuropsychologist feel confident that the   4.  Bonita R, Beaglehole R: Modification of Rankin scale: recovery
situation will be mastered. A very important piece of in­ of motor function after stroke. Stroke 19:1497–1500, 1988
formation for the patient to know is the types of phenom­   5.  Danks RA, Aglio LS, Gugino LD, Black PM: Craniotomy under
ena elicited by the stimulations (movements, sensations, local anesthesia and monitored conscious sedation for the re­
speech, and language disturbances). section of tumors involving eloquent cortex. J Neurooncol 49:
131–139, 2000
  6.  Danks RA, Rogers M, Aglio LS, Gugino LD, Black PM: Pa­
Conclusions tient tolerance of craniotomy performed with the patient un­
der local anesthesia and monitored conscious sedation. Neu-
Awake craniotomy with intraoperative stimulation rosurgery 42:28–36, 1998
mapping of language function for microsurgical treat­   7.  Duffau H, Capelle L, Denvil D, Sichez N, Gatignol P, Tailland­
ment of complex tumors located in close proximity to ier L, et al: Usefulness of intraoperative electrical subcortical
or within speech areas is a safe and economic procedure mapping during surgery for low-grade gliomas located within
eloquent brain regions: functional results in a consecutive se­
that is well tolerated by the patient. Given the uncertain­ ries of 103 patients. J Neurosurg 98:764–778, 2003
ties as to what extent a single intraoperatively detected   8.  Duffau H, Lopes M, Arthuis F, Bitar A, Sichez JP, Van Effen­
language-relevant area contributes to language-related terre R, et al: Contribution of intraoperative electrical stimu­
outcome scores, every attempt should be undertaken to lations in surgery of low grade gliomas: a comparative study
preserve language-relevant areas intraoperatively. The between two series without (1985-96) and with (1996-2003)
occurrence of early clinical and neurolinguistic deterio­ functional mapping in the same institution. J Neurol Neuro-
ration after surgery in 32% of the patients in this study surg Psychiatry 76:845–851, 2005
  9.  Eisner W, Reulen HJ, Ilmberger J, Swozil U, Bise K: Intraop­
did not predict long-term outcome scores; the majority of erative mapping of eloquent brain areas. Front Radiat Ther
these patients either improved to the preoperative status Oncol 33:28–36, 1999
(61%) or showed only mild impairment (22%) within 3 10.  Havel P, Braun B, Rau S, Tonn JC, Fesl G, Brückmann H, et
months after treatment. The naming subtest and the Token al: Reproducibility of activation in four motor paradigms: an
Test of the AAT neurolinguistic test battery were found to fMRI study. J Neurol 253:471–476, 2006

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008 591


J. Ilmberger et al.

11.  Huber W, Poeck K, Weniger D, Willmes K: Aachener Aphasie 33.  Pouratian N, Bookheimer SY, Rubino G, Martin NA, Toga
Test. Göttingen: Hogrefe, 1983 AW: Category-specific naming deficit identified by intraop­
12.  Ilmberger J, Eisner W, Schmid U, Reulen HJ: Performance in erative stimulation mapping and postoperative neuropsycho­
picture naming and word comprehension: evidence for com­ logical testing. J Neurosurg 99:170–176, 2003
mon neuronal substrates from intraoperative language map­ 34.  Pouratian N, Cannestra AF, Bookheimer SY, Martin NA,
ping. Brain Lang 76:111–118, 2001 Toga AW: Variability of intraoperative electrocortical stimu­
13.  Ilmberger J, Rau S, Noachtar S, Arnold S, Winkler P: Naming lation mapping parameters across and within individuals. J
tools and animals: asymmetries observed during direct electri­ Neurosurg 101:458–466, 2004
cal cortical stimulation. Neuropsychologia 40:695–700, 2002 35.  Pulvermüller F: Words in the brain’s language. Behav Brain
14.  Kamada K, Todo T, Masutani Y, Aoki S, Ino K, Takano T, et Sci 22:253–336, 1999
al: Combined use of tractography-integrated functional neu­ 36.  Quinones-Hinojosa A, Ojemann SG, Sanai N, Dillon WP,
ronavigation and direct fiber stimulation. J Neurosurg 102: Berger MS: Preoperative correlation of intraoperative cortical
664–672, 2005 mapping with magnetic resonance imaging landmarks to pre­
15.  Keles GE, Berger MS: Advances in neurosurgical technique dict localization of the Broca area. J Neurosurg 99:311–318,
in the current management of brain tumors. Semin Oncol 31: 2003
659–665, 2004 37.  Rankin J: Cerebral vascular accidents in patients over the age
16.  Kinoshita M, Yamada K, Hashimoto N, Kato A, Izumoto S, of 60. II. Prognosis. Scott Med J 2:200–215, 1957
Baba T, et al: Fiber-tracking does not accurately estimate size 38.  Rau S, Fesl G, Bruhns P, Havel P, Braun B, Tonn JC, et al: Re­
of fiber bundle in pathological condition: initial neurosurgical producibility of activations in Broca area with two language
experience using neuronavigation and subcortical white mat­ tasks: a functional MR imaging study. AJNR Am J Neuro-
ter stimulation. Neuroimage 25:424–428, 2005 radiol 28:1346–1353, 2007
17.  Meyer FB, Bates LM, Goerss SJ, Friedman JA, Windschitl 39.  Reulen HJ, Schmid DU, Ilmberger J, Eisner W, Bise K: [Tumor
WL, Duffy JR, et al: Awake craniotomy for aggressive resec­ surgery of the speech cortex in local anesthesia. Neuropsy­
tion of primary gliomas located in eloquent brain. Mayo Clin chological and electrophysiological monitoring during opera­
Proc 76:677–687, 2001 tions in the dominant hemisphere.] Nervenarzt 68:813–824,
18.  Mori S, Zhang J: Principles of diffusion tensor imaging and its 1997 (Ger)
applications to basic neuroscience research. Neuron 51:527– 40.  Roux FE, Boulanouar K, Lotterie JA, Mejdoubie M, LeSage JP,
539, 2006 Berry I: Language functional magnetic resonance im­aging in
19.  Nimsky C, Ganslandt O, Cerny S, Hastreiter P, Greiner G, Fahl­ preoperative assessment of language areas: correlation with di­
busch R: Quantification of, visualization of, and compensation rect cortical stimulation. Neurosurgery 52:1335–1347, 2003
for brain shift using intraoperative magnetic resonance imag­ 41.  Rutten GJ, Ramsey NF, van Rijen PC, van Veelen CW: Re­
ing. Neurosurgery 47:1070–1080, 2000 producibility of fMRI-determined language lateralization in
20.  Ojemann G, Ojemann J, Lettich E, Berger M: Cortical language individual subjects. Brain Lang 80:421–437, 2002
localization in left, dominant hemisphere. J Neurosurg 71:316– 42.  Schiffbauer H, Ferrari P, Rowley HA, Berger MS, Roberts TP:
326, 1989 Functional activity within brain tumors: a magnetic source
21.  Ojemann GA: Organization of language cortex derived from in­ imaging study. Neurosurgery 49:1313–1321, 2001
vestigations during neurosurgery. Semin Neurosci 2:297–305, 43.  Signorelli F, Guyotat J, Isnard J, Schneider F, Mohammedi R,
1990 Bret P: The value of cortical stimulation applied to the surgery
22.  Ojemann GA, Whitaker HA: Language localization and vari­ of malignant gliomas in language areas. Neurol Sci 22:3–
ability. Brain Lang 6:239–260, 1978 10, 2001
23.  Ojemann JG, Miller JW, Silbergeld DL: Preserved function in 44.  Skirboll SS, Ojemann GA, Berger MS, Lettich E, Winn HR:
brain invaded by tumor. Neurosurgery 39:253–259, 1996 Functional cortex and subcortical white matter located within
24.  Ojemann JG, Ojemann GA, Lettich E: Cortical stimulation gliomas. Neurosurgery 38:678–685, 1996
mapping of language cortex by using a verb generation task: 45.  Taylor MD, Bernstein M: Awake craniotomy with brain map­
effects of learning and comparison to mapping based on ob­ ping as the routine surgical approach to treating patients with
ject naming. J Neurosurg 97:33–38, 2002 supratentorial intraaxial tumors: a prospective trial of 200
25.  Otzenberger H, Gounot D, Marrer C, Namer IJ, Metz-Lutz cases. J Neurosurg 90:35–41, 1999
MN: Reliability of individual functional MRI brain mapping 46.  Van Swieten JC, Koudstaal PJ, Visser MC, Schouten HJ, van
of language. Neuropsychology 19:484–493, 2005 Gijn J: Interobserver agreement for the assessment of handi­
26.  Penfield W, Boldrey E: Somatic motor and sensory represen­ cap in stroke patients. Stroke 19:604–607, 1988
tation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical 47.  Whitaker HA, Ojemann GA: Graded localisation of naming
stimulation. Brain 60:389–443, 1937 from electrical stimulation mapping of left cerebral cortex.
27.  Penfield W, Rasmussen T: The Cerebral Cortex of Man. Nature 270:50–51, 1977
New York: Macmillan Co., 1950 48.  Whittle IR, Borthwick S, Haq N: Brain dysfunction following
28.  Peraud A, Ilmberger J, Reulen HJ: Surgical resection of gli­ ‘awake’ craniotomy, brain mapping and resection of glioma.
o­mas WHO grade II and III located in the opercular region. Br J Neurosurg 17:130–137, 2003
Acta Neurochir (Wien) 146:9–18, 2004 49.  Whittle IR, Midgley S, Georges H, Pringle AM, Taylor R:
29.  Peraud A, Meschede M, Eisner W, Ilmberger J, Reulen HJ: Patient perceptions of “awake” brain tumour surgery. Acta
Surgical resection of grade II astrocytomas in the superior Neurochir (Wien) 147:275–277, 2005
frontal gyrus. Neurosurgery 50:966–977, 2002 50.  Willmes K, Poeck K: To what extent can aphasic syndromes
30.  Picht T, Kombos T, Gramm HJ, Brock M, Suess O: Multimod­ be localized? Brain 116:1527–1540, 1993
al protocol for awake craniotomy in language cortex tumour
surgery. Acta Neurochir (Wien) 148:127–138, 2006
31.  Poeppel D, Hickok G: Towards a new functional anatomy of Manuscript submitted October 4, 2007.
language. Cognition 92:1–12, 2004 Accepted February 5, 2008.
32.  Pouratian N, Bookheimer SY, Rex DE, Martin NA, Toga AW: Address reprint requests to: Joerg-Christian Tonn, M.D., Neu­ro­
Utility of preoperative functional magnetic resonance imag­ surgical Department, Klinikum Grosshadern, Ludwig Maximilians
ing for identifying language cortices in patients with vascular University, Marchioninistrasse 15, 81377 Munich, Germany. email:
malformations. J Neurosurg 97:21–32, 2002 Joerg.Christian.Tonn@med.uni-muenchen.de.

592 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 109 / October 2008

You might also like