You are on page 1of 8

ARTICLE

The impact of daily sexual desire and daily sexual desire


discrepancy on the quality of the sexual experience in couples
Kristen P. Mark, PhD, MPH1
1 Department of Kinesiology & Health Promotion, College of Education, University of Kentucky

Recent research has found associations between sexual desire, desire discrepancy, and satisfaction
outcomes in individuals and couples on a broad level. The present study aimed to extend these findings
to the event level through examining daily experiences of sexual desire, sexual desire discrepancy, and
quality of the sexual experience in a sample of 87 mixed-sex couples (174 individuals) over a 30-day
period through daily electronic report. Participants were in their relationships for an average of 9.3 years.
Data were analyzed using over-time Actor Partner Interdependence Models (APIM). For women and men,
higher actor daily sexual desire predicted higher actor quality of the sexual experience. In addition, higher
partner daily sexual desire predicted higher actor quality of the sexual experience. Event-level desire dis-
crepancy between the couple was also a significant predictor of actor quality of the sexual experience for
women, though not for men. These results confirm that day-to-day sexual desire and desire discrepancy
are important indicators of quality of the sexual relationship and emphasize the importance of considering
event-level characteristics when examining sexual behaviour and couple dynamics. Implications and sugges-
tions for future research are also discussed.

KEY WORDS: sexual desire; sexual desire discrepancy; quality of sexual experience; sexual satisfaction;
couples; APIM; dyadic; daily diary; daily electronic report; sexual satisfaction

INTRODUCTION 2007; Davies, Katz, & Jackson, 1999; Mark, 2012; Mark &
Murray, 2012). These negative outcomes may be more pro-
Sexual desire is conceptualized as the experience of sexual nounced in relationships with longer duration than those
thoughts, fantasies, and urges to engage in sexual activity with shorter duration (Willoughby & Vitas, 2012). They may
(Basson, 2000) and is suggested to be the most universal sex- also impact women and men differently. For example, Davies
ual response experienced by both men and women (Regan & et al. (1999) found that women who had lower desire than
Atkins, 2006). Research indicates sexual desire is typically their partner were less relationally satisfied than women who
highest at the beginning of a relationship with declines as had similar or higher desire compared to their partner; not
relationship length increases (Murray & Milhausen, 2012; found in men. Similarly, Mark and Murray (2012) found
Sprecher & Regan, 1998). However, some research suggests that higher desire discrepancies were related to lower relation-
that sexual desire may ebb and flow rather than decline over ship satisfaction in women, but lower sexual satisfaction in
time (Acevedo & Aron, 2009; Ridley, Cate, Collins, Reesing, men. Desire discrepancy can arise for a variety of reasons in-
Lucero, Gilson, & Almeida, 2006), and this ebb and flow of cluding relationship factors, hormonal levels, medical issues,
sexual desire has not necessarily been captured in cross- stress, and conflicting schedules (Ellison, 2001). Desire dis-
sectional examinations of sexual desire. crepancy is the most common sexual complaint from women
When couples within a romantic relationship experience (Ellison, 2001) with consistent evidence to suggest it nega-
different levels of sexual desire relative to their partner, a sexual tively impacts sexual and relationship satisfaction (Bridges &
desire discrepancy is thought to exist (Zilbergeld & Ellison, Horne, 2007; Davies et al., 1999, Mark, 2012; Mark & Murray,
1980). This has been shown in the literature to be related to 2012).
negative sexual and relationship outcomes (Bridges & Horne,

Acknowledgements: The Patty Brisben Foundation for Women’s Sexual Health and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of
Canada supported this research. I would also like to acknowledge Dominique Zephyr from the Applied Statistics Lab at University of Kentucky for
data analysis assistance.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kristen P Mark, PhD, MPH, Department of Kinesiology & Health Promotion,
College of Education, 122 Seaton Building, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506 Email: kristen.mark@uky.edu

Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(1), 2014, pp. 27–33; doi:10.3138/cjhs.23.1.A2 27


Kristen P. Mark

Sexual satisfaction in long-term relationships has been a 2006). In addition, some event-level research has pointed to
growing area of inquiry, in part due to its positive association a potential link between satisfaction and sexual goals related
to relationship satisfaction (Byers, 2005; Byers, Demmons, & to sexual desire. For example, Muise, Impett, and Desmarais
Lawrance, 1998; Schwartz & Young, 2009; Sprecher, 2002) (2013) examined dating couples and long-term couples using
and overall well-being (Apt, Hurlbert, Pierce, & White, 1996; a 21-day diary study to examine the link between sexual goals
Davison, Bell, LaChina, Holden, & Davis, 2009), but also due and satisfaction. They found that sexual desire mediated the
to the increase in attention paid to sexual health issues (e.g., significant relationships between sexual goals and satisfaction.
pharmacological innovations) and public health initiatives These findings indicate there may be something worthwhile
(Higgins, Mullinax, Trussell, Davidson, & Moore, 2011). In- to be learned from studying an event-level experience. How-
creasingly, clinical research and interventions are turning to ever, still unknown is the impact of daily levels of sexual
measures of sexual satisfaction as important clinical outcome desire for partner or sexual desire discrepancy on the quality
variables (McClelland, 2011). Sexual satisfaction has been con- of the specific sexual experience.
ceptualized as both evaluative and affective (Lawrance & Byers, As much of the desire discrepancy research conducted
1992), with an emphasis on positive affect, expectations, or a thus far has done, taking an interpersonal approach to examine
balance between positive and negative dimensions of satisfac- sexual desire and satisfaction offers benefits to taking an intra-
tion (McClelland, 2011). A recent study conducted by Pascoal, personal perspective in which one partner’s sexual desire is
Narciso, and Pereira (2014) examined coupled heterosexual lay linked to his or her own sexual or relationship outcomes
persons definitions of sexual satisfaction found that a crucial (Kane, Jaremka, Guichard, Ford, Collins, & Feeney, 2007).
component of sexual satisfaction was mutual pleasure and The context for understanding couple dynamics is greatly in-
the quality of the sexual experiences were primary to sexual creased when collecting both individual and partner percep-
satisfaction. The quality of individual sexual events over time tions of the variable of interest. This dyadic approach allows
may therefore be aggregated to form one’s overall sexual sat- for a shift in the diagnostic process from the level of the indi-
isfaction. In addition, McClelland (2011) found that partner’s vidual to the level of the couple to allow for the individual
level of sexual satisfaction was sometimes used as a proxy to with the lower level of desire to be understood within the
determine their own sexual satisfaction, further emphasizing context of the relationship rather than deemed pathological
the importance of couple dynamics. and dysfunctional (Davies et al., 1999).
Research has successfully integrated the links between Given the evidence suggesting sexual desire and sexual
individual sexual desire, desire discrepancy, and sexual satis- desire discrepancy impact sexual satisfaction on a broad level,
faction (Mark, 2012; Willoughby, Farero, & Busby, in press; it seems plausible this would translate to the event level.
Willoughby & Vitas, 2012). For example, Mark (2012) found Therefore, the current study aims to understand how sexual
that individual sexual desire for one’s partner significantly pre- desire and sexual desire discrepancy impact the quality of the
dicted sexual satisfaction for women and men, whereas sexual sexual experience on the event level. Specifically, the current
desire discrepancy significantly predicted sexual satisfaction paper seeks to answer the following three research questions:
for men only in a sample of college-age heterosexual couples.
Willoughby and Vitas (2012) examined dating couples and RQ1: Does daily sexual desire impact event-level quality of
found higher discrepancy between desired frequency of sex sexual experience?
and actual frequency of sex was associated with lower relation- RQ2: Does partner daily sexual desire impact event-level
ship stability. Negative relationship outcomes were particularly quality of sexual experience?
salient in couples with longer relationship length. In a sample RQ3: Does daily sexual desire discrepancy impact event-level
of married couples, Willoughby et al. (in press) found higher quality of sexual experience?
desire discrepancy related to negative relationship outcomes
including lower relationship satisfaction, lower stability, and
more conflict; these findings did not change based on relation- METHOD
ship length.
Procedure
The sexual desire discrepancy literature to date has been
confined to measuring constructs as a whole or over a period Participants were recruited to the study through online social
of specified time (e.g., over the previous four weeks) limiting media (Facebook and Twitter), email listservs, and fliers
our understanding of the individual experiences that combine posted around a college town in the Midwestern United
to create our overall level of satisfaction. Taking an event-level States. Interested participants who visited the study website
approach is especially interesting in the context of findings were provided additional information about the study. If the
from event-level prospective daily diary research that have participant was still interested, eligibility criteria were assessed
demonstrated the variability of pleasure and satisfaction both through a series of questions that ensured the participant was
within the person and across events with the same partner in a long-term relationship (3+ years) with someone of the
(Hensel, Stupiansky, Herbenick, Dodge, & Reece, 2012; Reece, opposite gender, not currently pregnant or with child under
Mark, Herbenick, Hensel, Jawed-Wessel, & Dodge, 2012; the age of one, and currently living with their partner. If they
Ridley, Cate, Collins, Reesing, Lucero, Gilson, & Almeida, met the eligibility criteria, the potential participant was asked

28 Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(1), 2014, pp. 27–33; doi:10.3138/cjhs.23.1.A2


The Impact of Daily Sexual Desire and Daily Sexual Desire Discrepancy on the Quality of the Sexual Experience in Couples

to provide a valid email address for their partner. The partner ‘‘Thinking about this sexual experience that you just described,
was then sent an email by study personnel inviting them to would you say that it was’’: and participants chose from a 9-
learn more about the study and complete the same eligibility point scale ranging from ‘‘extremely bad’’ to ‘‘extremely
criteria checks the first member of the couple completed. If good,’’ or ‘‘extremely unpleasurable’’ to ‘‘extremely pleasur-
the partner also met the eligibility criteria, both members of able,’’ or ‘‘extremely good physically’’ to ‘‘extremely good
the couple were sent individual emails with a link to the study physically,’’ etc. with higher scores indicative of higher quality
that required them to enter their email address and create a of sexual experience. Based on a representative sample of the
password, which served as their login to the study. Once par- United States, this scale has demonstrated strong psychomet-
ticipants provided informed consent, they completed an online ric properties, with a Cronbach’s alpha of .96 and split-half
baseline survey that took approximately 30 minutes to com- reliabilities at .89 or higher (Sanders et al., 2013). In the cur-
plete. Participants were informed that they would receive an rent sample, the Cronbach’s alpha was .86 for men and .88
email the following morning to initiate the 30-day daily elec- for women. Scores ranged from 2 to 9 (M ¼ 7.44, SD ¼ .96;
tronic report portion of the study. The next morning, partic- Mwomen ¼ 7.35, SDwomen ¼ 1.06; Mmen ¼ 7.54, SDmen ¼ .83).
ipants were emailed a link to complete the first of 30 daily
surveys. Each daily electronic report took the participant ap- Participants
proximately three to five minutes to complete, depending on
A total of 93 couples created login information and provided
whether they engaged in sexual activity in the previous 24
informed consent. However, five of the couples dropped out
hours. Both members of the couple were instructed to com-
of the study before the end of the first week and their data
plete the daily electronic report independently of one another
was incomplete. Therefore, the analytic sample consisted of
each morning. All participants received a $40 Amazon gift
87 mixed-sex couples from the United States (n ¼ 71 couples;
card for participating in the study. All responses were kept
82%), Canada (n ¼ 14 couples; 16%), and the United Kingdom
confidential and the Indiana University IRB approved all pro-
(n ¼ 2 couples; 2%). The average age of participants was 32.46
cedures.
years (SD ¼ 10.25) for women and 33.67 years (SD ¼ 9.36)
for men. Participants were in a relationship with one another
Measures
for a minimum of three years and a maximum of 31 years
At the outset of the study, participants were asked to complete (M ¼ 9.33, SD ¼ 6.95) at the time of their participation. The
a general demographic and relationship questionnaire. These majority of participants were White (88.5%), though 3.4%
measures incorporated questions on age, sexual orientation, re- identified as multi-racial, 2.9% as Asian or Asian-American,
lationship length, geographic location, and education, among 1.7% as Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander, 1.2% as
others. In addition, participants were administered questions Black or African American, and 1.0% as American Indian or
regarding relationship dynamics such as sexual satisfaction, Native. All participants reported being in exclusive long-term
relationship satisfaction, and sexual behaviours engaged in relationships with someone of the opposite gender, though
during the previous week, month, and year. The measures not all orientation was reported as heterosexual: 90.1% of
collected every day for the 30-day daily electronic reports women and 98.5% of men identified as heterosexual; 6.9%
were as follows: of women and 1.5% of men identified as bisexual; and 2.5%
Sexual Desire. To assess sexual desire on the event level, of the women identified as uncertain or questioning.
participants were asked to answer the following question:
‘‘During the past day, did you feel sexual desire for your part- Analysis
ner?’’ on a 5-point scale ranging from 1(not at all) to 5 (ex-
The Actor-Partner Interdependence Model (APIM; Kenny,
tremely). Scale scores ranged from 1 to 5 (M ¼ 3.14, SD ¼
Kashy, & Cook, 2006) considers both the effect that a person’s
1.21; Mwomen ¼ 2.93, SDwomen ¼ 1.26; Mmen ¼ 3.36, SDmen ¼
independent variable has on their own dependent variable
1.12).
(actor effect) and the effect that a person’s independent vari-
Sexual Desire Discrepancy. The daily measure of sexual
able has on their partner’s dependent variable (partner effect),
desire discrepancy was calculated using the absolute differ-
and was used to guide the analysis. The APIM assumes that
ence between the partners’ daily sexual desire scores. That is,
the data from two members of a couple are not independent
a larger discrepancy score indicated more difference in desire
and treats the dyad as the unit of analysis allowing the re-
within each couple for each day. Couples with matching sex-
searcher to estimate actor and partner effects simultaneously.
ual desire on a given day received a discrepancy score of zero.
Data from romantic couples are not independent because
Scores ranged from 0 to 4 (M ¼ 1.05, SD ¼ .94).
there are strong correlations between variables of both male
Quality of Sexual Experience. The measure of quality
and female partners; the acts and characteristics of one partner
of sexual experience for each sexual event that occurred in
are likely to affect those of the opposite partner (Yucel &
the previous 24 hours was measured with a modified version
Gassanov, 2010). To anticipate unbiased statistical variances,
of the Quality of Sexual Experience Scale (QSE; Sanders,
the current analyses followed Kenny and colleagues’ (2006)
Herbenick, Reece, Schick, Mullinax, Dodge, et al., 2013), a 7-
suggestions to use the APIM in a two-level cross model with
item scale created to measure sexual quality on an event-
random intercepts where persons are nested within dyads,
specific basis. All questions began with the base question of
and days are crossed with persons to account for both members

Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(1), 2014, pp. 27–33; doi:10.3138/cjhs.23.1.A2 29


Kristen P. Mark

of the couple completing the surveys on the same days. All


daily-level predictors in the models were person-mean centred
to reflect associations between deviations from a person’s mean
score on daily sexual desire and the outcome measure, quality
of sexual experience. To answer the first two research questions
of interest, an over-time APIM using multi-level modelling was
run. To answer the third research question of interest, an
over-time APIM without measurement of the partner effects
(due to the couples-based discrepancy score) was run. All Figure 1. Conceptual model of actor-partner effects between
analyses were run using SAS 9.1.3 (SAS Institute, Inc.). sexual desire and quality of sexual experience

Table 1. Over-time APIM fixed actor and partner effects


RESULTS predicting quality of sexual experience with individual sexual
desire
The 87 couples (174 participants) reported engaging in a total
of 2,328 partnered sexual events (oral sex, vaginal sex, or anal Standard
sex) over the course of the 30 days. Sexual behaviour reports Estimate Error DF t
yielded a 98.6% consistency rate between members of the
Women
couple across the 30 days. Overall, there was variation on the
Actor Effect .52 .04 83 12.55***
sexual desire, sexual desire discrepancy, and the quality of
Partner Effect .13 .03 66 4.44***
sexual experience measures across the 30 days. On average, Men
across all days, women’s ratings of daily sexual desire (M ¼ Actor Effect .49 .04 77 12.64***
2.93, SD ¼ 1.26) were lower than men’s (M ¼ 3.36, SD ¼ Partner Effect .25 .03 82 7.75***
1.12), t(4783) ¼ 12.53, p < .001. In addition, on average,
across all days, women’s ratings of quality of sexual experience *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
(M ¼ 7.35, SD ¼ 1.06) were lower than men’s (M ¼ 7.54,
SD ¼ .83), t(1061) ¼ 3.26, p < .01. However, the effect size
for these differences was small to medium for sexual desire
(Cohen’s d ¼ .36) and small for the quality of sexual experi-
ence (Cohen’s d ¼ .20) (Cohen, 1988).
An over-time standard APIM was conducted to analyze
the first and second research questions of interest; the impact
of individual sexual desire and partner sexual desire on the
quality of sexual experience over time (see Figure 1). The in-
tercept was significant for women (Estimate ¼ 3.06, t(357) ¼ Figure 2. Conceptual model of actor effects between sexual
28.19, p < .001) and men (Estimate ¼ 2.83, t(267) ¼ 24.48, desire discrepancy and quality of sexual experience
p < .001). The actor effect was significant for women
(Estimate ¼ .52, t(83) ¼ 12.55, p < .001), such that for every
cept was significant for women (Estimate ¼ 3.21, t(303) ¼
one unit increase in daily sexual desire, women’s quality of
22.12, p < .001) and men (Estimate ¼ 3.03, t(314) ¼ 21.02,
the sexual experience increased by .52 units. The actor effect
p < .001). The actor effect was significant for women
was also significant for men (Estimate ¼ .49, t(77) ¼ 12.64,
(Estimate ¼ .10, t(79) ¼ 2.44, p < .05), such that with
p < .001), such that for every one unit increase in daily sexual
every one unit increase in daily desire discrepancy, women’s
desire, men’s quality of the sexual experience increased by .49
quality of sexual experience decreased by .10 of a unit. The
units. The partner effect was also significant for women
actor effect for men was non-significant ( p ¼ .89). See Table
(Estimate ¼ .13, t(66) ¼ 4.44, p < .001), such that for every
2 for all desire discrepancy model coefficients.
one unit increase in her partner’s daily sexual desire, women’s
quality of the sexual experience increased by .13 units. In
addition, the partner effect was significant for men
DISCUSSION
(Estimate ¼ .25, t(82) ¼ 7.75, p < .001), such that for every
one unit increase in his partner’s daily sexual desire, men’s This study provided insights into sexual desire and desire dis-
quality of sexual experience increased by .25 units. See Table crepancy on an event level as it relates to the quality of sexual
1 for all model coefficients. experience in a sample of couples. This extension of previous
An over-time APIM without measurement of the partner research, which has generally examined sexual desire and
effects (due to the couples-based discrepancy score) was con- desire discrepancy as a whole or over a period of specified
ducted to analyze the third research question of interest: the time, indicates that event-level links between these constructs
impact of couple sexual desire discrepancy on quality of the are significant and potentially important indicators for overall
sexual experience at the event level (see Figure 2). The inter- desire and satisfaction. In addition, this study indicates that

30 Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(1), 2014, pp. 27–33; doi:10.3138/cjhs.23.1.A2


The Impact of Daily Sexual Desire and Daily Sexual Desire Discrepancy on the Quality of the Sexual Experience in Couples

Table 2. Over-time APIM fixed actor effects predicting quality desire on a given day but engages in compliance sex anyway,
of sexual experience with sexual desire discrepancy perhaps they are less enthusiastic during the sexual experience,
in turn impacting the quality of the sexual experience for both
Standard
Estimate Error DF t self and partner. Alternately, if a partner does feel high sexual
desire for their partner that day, their enthusiasm for sex may
Women be emphasized and perhaps manifest itself through a higher
Actor Effect .10 .04 79 2.44* quality experience for the partner.
Men Third, the current study found that on days when the desire
Actor Effect .01 .04 70 .13 discrepancy was greater between individuals within a couple,
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 the female partner expressed lesser quality of the sexual experi-
ence, whereas on days when both members of the couple had
daily sexual desire and desire discrepancy are important indi- similar levels of sexual desire to one another, women’s quality
cators for the event-level quality of the sexual experience and of the sexual experience was greater. However, on greater dis-
partner effects point to the interdependent impact desire has crepant desire days, men’s quality of the sexual experience
on quality of the sexual experience. was not impacted. This is inconsistent with cross-sectional
First, the findings of this study indicated that on days when research that has found desire discrepancy to be a significant
women and men had greater sexual desire for their partner, the predictor of sexual satisfaction in men, but not women
quality of their sexual experience was also greater. This finding (Mark, 2012), but consistent with other research that has
was true for women and men and is consistent with research demonstrated this link in women (Bridges & Horne, 2007;
from studies that examined constructs as a whole or over a Mark & Murray, 2012). Perhaps men are less attuned than
period of specified time (Davies et al., 1999; Mark, 2012; women to the ebb and flow of sexual desire within couple
Mark & Murray, 2012). Daily sexual desire impacted the relationships, thereby not allowing these daily fluctuations
quality of the sexual experience for both men and women at (or perhaps not even being aware of these daily fluctuations)
a similar rate (for every one unit increase in daily sexual to impact their sexual quality on an event-level.
desire, women and men’s quality improved by around half of Interestingly, the current study found significant gender
a unit). Interpreted in the other direction, this finding indi- differences in sexual desire and quality of sexual experience.
cated that on days when desire for partner was lower but a As a whole, women rated their daily sexual desire for their
sexual event occurred, the quality of that sexual event suffered. partner and the quality of their sexual experience with their
Scholars have suggested that sexual desire does not have to partner significantly lower than men. To my knowledge, this
precede sexual arousal (Basson, 2001), and couples may prac- is the first study to report gender differences in sexual desire
tice sexual compliance by willingly engaging in sex with their and quality of sexual experience on a daily level. In terms of
partner in the absence of desire (Vannier & O’Sullivan, 2010). previous research that has examined these constructs as a
Although the work of these scholars implies that a lack of whole or over a period of specified time, this is consistent
desire may not negatively impact the couple relationship, the with some findings (Willoughby & Vitas, 2012; Willoughby
current findings indicate that although low desire on one day et al., in press) and inconsistent with others (Davies et al.,
will not drastically impact your quality of the experience (in 1999; Mark, 2012; Mark & Murray, 2012). Traditionally,
this sample, the sacrifice was approximately half of a unit in women have often been constructed as the member of the
quality), if sexual desire is routinely low and sex is engaged couple with lower sexual desire relative to their male partner
in anyway, this may add up over time to have a more drastic (see Baumeister, Catanese, and Vohs (2001) for a review of the
negative impact on quality. It would be interesting to examine research) and this has also been adopted as a cultural stereo-
the additive impact of low sexual desire in combination with type. There may be nuances with daily sexual desire and spe-
regular frequency of sex with a long-term partner and its cific to event-level quality of sexual experiences that resonate
impact on quality of the sexual experience and overall sexual with women and men differently. In addition, perhaps these
satisfaction. event-level ratings are used as an indication for more broad
Second, this study provided insight into partner effects that ratings of overall desire and satisfaction differently for women
have been less frequently studied in the literature. Willoughby and men. It is important to note that these significant gender
and Vitas (2012) and Willoughby and colleagues (in press) differences should be taken in the context of their small to
have examined partner effects and provided information on medium effect sizes and additional research should be done
perceived sexual desire discrepancy and its link to relational to examine both gender differences and similarities on these
outcomes such as relationship satisfaction and conflict as a constructs. This should be a future area of inquiry and further
whole. The current study was the first to link sexual desire to points to the need to consider gender differences and similar-
quality of sexual experience on an event level and found that ities by acknowledging that there may be as much variation
on days when a partner’s sexual desire is greater, the quality within each gender as there are between the genders when it
of the sexual experience for the self (or, the actor) was also comes to sexual desire.
greater. Alternately, on days when a partner’s sexual desire Some additional considerations should be made when
was lower, the quality of the sexual experience for the self deliberating these findings. Participants reported on their
suffered. It could be that when a partner has not felt sexual sexual desire after the sexual event occurred (recall that

Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(1), 2014, pp. 27–33; doi:10.3138/cjhs.23.1.A2 31


Kristen P. Mark

participants reported on their previous 24 hours of sexual that although sexual desire ebbs and flows, on days when sex-
activity). Research has shown that sexual desire does not ual desire for partner is expressed, it can strengthen the quality
always come before sexual activity but is often intertwined of the sexual experience for self and partner, and there may
with arousal (Basson, 2001), potentially creating a perception be benefits to learning how to improve daily sexual desire.
of sexual desire and desire discrepancy less notable after the
fact. However, significant findings were detected, so if desire
and desire discrepancy were less notable after the fact, the REFERENCES
strength of these associations may be stronger than reported
in the current study. In addition, these findings should only Apt, C., Hurlbert, D.F., Pierce, A.P., & White, C.L. (1996). Relation-
ship satisfaction, sexual characteristics and the psychological
be generalized to a population similar to the sample itself; a
well-being of women. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 5,
primarily White, middle to upper class, primarily heterosexual,
195–210.
long-term, mixed-sex couple sample. Further, these results gen-
Acevedo, B.P., & Aron, A. (2009). Does a long-term relationship kill
eralize based on 30 days of data across 87 couples and there
romantic love? Review of General Psychology, 13(1), 59–65.
may be interesting nuances that go undetected using this
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014226
approach. For example, there may be instances where women
Basson, R. (2000). The female sexual response: a different model.
have higher desire and higher quality than their male partners
Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 26(1), 51–65. http://dx.doi.org/
that are obscured by taking the average across the 30 days.
10.1080/009262300278641 Medline:10693116
Two methodological strengths of this research are the data
Basson, R. (2001). Human sex-response cycles. Journal of Sex &
collection from dyads and the daily event-level collection of
Marital Therapy, 27(1), 33–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
data. The dyadic approach is especially important since an 00926230152035831 Medline:11224952
overwhelming majority of sexual experiences take place within
Baumeister, R.F., Catanese, K., & Vohs, K. (2001). Is there a gender
the context of a partner (Impett, Muise, & Peragine, 2013).
difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual
The daily event-level collection of data offers more contextual
distinction, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality and
information that previous research examining the constructs Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 242–273. http://dx.doi.org/
of interest have been unable to provide (e.g., Mark, 2012; 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0503_5
Mark & Murray, 2012; Willoughby & Vitas, 2012; Willoughby
Bridges, S.K., & Horne, S.G. (2007). Sexual satisfaction and desire
et al., in press). Collecting sexual behavioural data on a daily discrepancy in same sex women’s relationships. Journal of Sex &
basis offers a valid approach to obtaining data about a specific Marital Therapy, 33(1), 41–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
event by nearly eliminating recall bias. 00926230600998466 Medline:17162487
These research findings inspire several ideas for future Byers, E.S. (2005). Relationship satisfaction and sexual satisfaction: a
research. For example, the current study examined sexual de- longitudinal study of individuals in long-term relationships.
sire and desire discrepancy during days when sexual activity Journal of Sex Research, 42(2), 113–118. http://dx.doi.org/
between partners occurred. It would be interesting to consider 10.1080/00224490509552264 Medline:16123841
days when sexual activity did not occur, particularly related to Byers, E.S., Demmons, S., & Lawrance, K. (1998). Sexual satisfaction
desire discrepancy and the overall satisfaction of the individ- within dating relationships: A test of the interpersonal exchange
uals within the couple. Based on findings from Impett and model of sexual satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
colleagues (2008) that engaging in sex for approach goals pro- tionships, 15(2), 257–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
tected against declines in desire over time and predicted higher 0265407598152008
sexual desire during a sexual event, it would also be interesting Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
to understand how the quality of the sexual event the day be- (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
fore impacts the desire for partner the next day. In addition, Davies, S., Katz, J., & Jackson, J.L. (1999). Sexual desire discrepan-
it would be beneficial to examine whether high quality of the cies: effects on sexual and relationship satisfaction in heterosexual
sexual experience for both members of the couple results in dating couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28(6), 553–567.
less desire discrepancy the following day. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1018721417683 Medline:10650441
Feelings of sexual desire have important implications for Davison, S.L., Bell, R.J., LaChina, M., Holden, S.L., & Davis, S.R.
sexual and relational quality. People who report higher sexual (2009). The relationship between self-reported sexual satisfaction
desire and lower desire discrepancy tend to be more satisfied and general well-being in women. Journal of Sexual Medicine,
sexually and relationally (Apt et al., 1996; Davies et al., 1999; 6(10), 2690–2697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-
Mark & Murray, 2012) and these individuals with high sexual 6109.2009.01406.x Medline:19817981
desire are less likely to leave their relationship (Regan, 2000). Ellison, C.R. (2001). A research inquiry into some American
The current study extends the literature in an important way women’s sexual concerns and problems. Women & Therapy,
that emphasizes the day-to-day components of sexual desire 24(1–2), 147–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J015v24n01_17
and sexual satisfaction. Researchers are increasingly demon- Hensel, D.J., Stupiansky, N.W., Herbenick, D., Dodge, B., & Reece,
strating that sexual desire ebbs and flows within a relationship M. (2012). Sexual pleasure during condom-protected vaginal sex
(Acevedo & Aron, 2009; Ridley et al., 2006). Examining sexual among heterosexual men. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9(5), 1272–
desire on an event level is one valuable way to contribute to 1276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02700.x
Medline:22781082
that line of research. The findings of the current study indicate

32 Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(1), 2014, pp. 27–33; doi:10.3138/cjhs.23.1.A2


The Impact of Daily Sexual Desire and Daily Sexual Desire Discrepancy on the Quality of the Sexual Experience in Couples

Higgins, J.A., Mullinax, M., Trussell, J., Davidson, J.K., Sr., & Moore, Reece, M., Mark, K.P., Herbenick, D., Hensel, D.J., Jawed-Wessel, S.,
N.B. (2011). Sexual satisfaction and sexual health among univer- & Dodge, B. (2012). An event-level analysis of adding exogenous
sity students in the United States. American Journal of Public lubricant to condoms in a sample of men who have vaginal sex
Health, 101(9), 1643–1654. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/ with women. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9(3), 672–678. http://
AJPH.2011.300154 Medline:21778509 dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02605.x Medline:22239955
Impett, E.A., Muise, A., & Peragine, D. (2013). Sexuality in the con- Regan, P.C. (2000). The role of sexual desire and sexual activity in
text of relationships. In L.M. Diamond & D.L. Tolman (Eds.), dating relationships. Social Behavior and Personality, 28(1), 51–
APA handbook of sexuality and psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 269–316). 59. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2000.28.1.51
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Regan, P.C., & Atkins, L. (2006). Sex differences and similarities in
Impett, E.A., Strachman, A., Finkel, E.J., & Gable, S.L. (2008). Main- frequency and intensity of sexual desire. Social Behavior and
taining sexual desire in intimate relationships: the importance of Personality, 34(1), 95–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/
approach goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, sbp.2006.34.1.95
94(5), 808–823. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.808 Ridley, C.A., Cate, R.M., Collins, D.M., Reesing, A.L., Lucero, A.A.,
Medline:18444740 Gilson, M.S., & Almeida, D.M. (2006). The ebb and flow of
Kane, H.S., Jaremka, A.C., Guichard, M.B., Ford, N.L., Collins, B.C., marital lust: a relational approach. Journal of Sex Research, 43(2),
& Feeney, B.C. (2007). Feeling supported and feeling satisfied: 144–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490609552309
How one partner’s attachment style predicts the other partner’s Medline:16817061
relationship experiences. Journal of Social and Personal Relation- Sanders, S.A., Herbenick, D., Reece, M., Schick, V., Mullinax, M.,
ships, 24(4), 535–555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Dodge, B., & Fortenberry, J.D. (2013). The development and
0265407507079245 validation of a brief Quality of Sexual Experience (QSE) scale:
Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A., & Cook, W.L. (2006). Dyadic data results from a nationally representative sample of men and
analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press. women in the United States. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 10(10),
Lawrance, K., & Byers, E. S. (1992). Development of the Interpersonal 2409–2417. http://dx.doi.org/10.111/jsm.12198
Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction in long-term relationships. Medline:23679190
The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 1(4), 123–128. SAS Institute Inc., SAS Online Doc 9.1.3, Cary, NC: SAS Institute
Mark, K.P. (2012). The relative impact of individual sexual desire Inc., 2002–2005.
and couple desire discrepancy on satisfaction in heterosexual Schwartz, P., & Young, L. (2009). Sexual satisfaction in committed
couples. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 27(2), 133–146. relationships. Sexuality Research & Social Policy, 6(1), 1–17.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14681994.2012.678825 http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/srsp.2009.6.1.1
Mark, K.P., & Murray, S.H. (2012). Gender differences in desire dis- Sprecher, S. (2002). Sexual satisfaction in premarital relationships:
crepancy as a predictor of sexual and relationship satisfaction in a associations with satisfaction, love, commitment, and stability.
college sample of heterosexual romantic relationships. Journal of Journal of Sex Research, 39(3), 190–196. http://dx.doi.org/
Sex & Marital Therapy, 38(2), 198–215. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1080/00224490209552141 Medline:12476266
10.1080/0092623X.2011.606877 Medline:22390532 Sprecher, S., & Regan, P.C. (1998). Passionate and companionate
McClelland, S. (2011). Who is the ‘‘self ’’ in self reports of sexual love in courting and young married couples. Sociological Inquiry,
satisfaction? Research and policy implications. Sexuality Research 68(2), 163–185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-
& Social Policy, 8(4), 304–320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13178- 682X.1998.tb00459.x
011-0067-9 Vannier, S.A., & O’Sullivan, L.F. (2010). Sex without desire: charac-
Muise, A., Impett, E.A., & Desmarais, S. (2013). Getting it on versus teristics of occasions of sexual compliance in young adults’ com-
getting it over with: sexual motivation, desire, and satisfaction in mitted relationships. Journal of Sex Research, 47(5), 429–439.
intimate bonds. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490903132051 Medline:19662565
39(10), 1320–1332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490963 Willoughby, B.J., Farero, A.M., & Busby, D.M. (2013) (in press).
Medline:23812928 Exploring the effects of sexual desire discrepancy among married
Muise, A., Impett, E.A., Kogan, A., & Desmarais, S. (2013). Keeping couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
the spark alive: Being motivated to meet a partner’s sexual needs s10508-013-0181-2 Medline:24045904
sustains sexual desire in long-term romantic relationships. Social Willoughby, B.J., & Vitas, J. (2012). Sexual desire discrepancy: the
Psychological and Personality Science, 4(3), 267–273. effect of individual differences in desired and actual sexual
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550612457185 frequency on dating couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(2),
Murray, S.H., & Milhausen, R.R. (2012). Sexual desire and relation- 477–486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9766-9
ship duration in young men and women. Journal of Sex & Medline:21573707
Marital Therapy, 38(1), 28–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Yucel, D., & Gassanov, M.A. (2010). Exploring actor and partner
0092623X.2011.569637 Medline:22268980 correlates of sexual satisfaction among married couples. Social
Pascoal, P.M., Narciso, I.S., & Pereira, N.M. (2014). What is sexual Science Research, 39(5), 725–738. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
satisfaction? Thematic analysis of lay people’s definitions. Journal j.ssresearch.2009.09.002
of Sex Research, 51(1), 22–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Zilbergeld, B., & Ellison, C.R. (1980). Desire discrepancies and
00224499.2013.815149 Medline:24070214 arousal problems in sex therapy. In S.R. Leiblum & L. Pervin
(Eds.), Principles and practice of sex therapy (pp. 65–106). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.

Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 23(1), 2014, pp. 27–33; doi:10.3138/cjhs.23.1.A2 33


Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without
permission.

You might also like