You are on page 1of 12

Psychology & Neuroscience

© 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 11, No. 3, 317–328


1983-3288/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pne0000118

Impact of Age and Schooling on Performance on the Brief


Cognitive Screening Battery: A Study of Elderly Residents in the
City of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Verônica C. Araujo, Flávia P. Furtado


Christina M. B. Lima, Secretaria Especial de Envelhecimento Saudável e
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and Eduarda N. B. Barbosa Qualidade de Vida (SESQV), Prefeitura da Cidade


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Helenice Charchat-Fichman
Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro

The objective of this study was to investigate the impact of age and schooling on Brief
Cognitive Screening Battery (BCSB) performance in elderly residents in the city of Rio de
Janeiro, Brazil. The sample was composed of 470 elderly individuals, all of whom were
evaluated using the BCSB. The results were analyzed by groups that were divided by age
and schooling. Analyses of variances (ANOVAs) were performed to examine differences
in performance between the age and schooling groups and interactions between age and
schooling. Correlations between cognitive variables, age, and schooling were also analyzed.
The sample consisted of 420 women (89.4%) and 50 men (10.6%). The mean age was
72.77 ⫾ 7.06 years, with average schooling of 9.54 ⫾ 5.32 years. The ANOVA showed
that elderly individuals who were older than 80 years of age presented worse performance
on memory tasks compared with the younger age groups. With regard to schooling, elderly
individuals with low schooling presented worse performance on the Verbal Fluency and
Clock Drawing tests and on the Mini Mental State Examination. The two-way ANOVA
revealed a significant Age ⫻ Schooling interaction for Mini Mental State Examination
scores. The Age ⫻ Schooling interaction impacted performance of the elderly in the
evaluation of global cognitive function. The present results indicated a dissociation, in
which age affected memory function, and schooling affected other cognitive functions.

Keywords: age, schooling, cognition, Brief Cognitive Screening Battery, elderly

The aging population is a worldwide phe- more rapidly and more recently than in devel-
nomenon, but it is more striking in developing oped countries (Figueiredo, Assis, Silva, Dias,
countries where this transformation is occurring & Mancini, 2013; Noroozian, Shakiba, & Iran-
nejad, 2014). This aging process has been ac-
companied by an increase in life expectancy and
This article was published Online First August 2, 2018. consequently a higher incidence of aging-
Verônica C. Araujo, Christina M. B. Lima, and Eduarda
N. B. Barbosa, Department of Psychology, Pontifícia Universi-
related diseases, especially neurodegenerative
dade Católica do Rio de Janeiro; Flávia P. Furtado, Secretaria diseases that have a tremendous economic and
Especial de Envelhecimento Saudável e Qualidade de Vida social impact (Figueiredo et al., 2013; Paddick
(SESQV), Prefeitura da Cidade do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, et al., 2014; Steibel, Olchik, Yassuda, Finger, &
Brazil; Helenice Charchat-Fichman, Department of Psychology;
Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro. Gomes, 2016).
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- A major focus of recent studies has been on
dressed to Verônica C. Araujo, Department of Psychology, the early detection of neurodegenerative dis-
Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro, R. Mar-
quês de São Vicente, 225 - Gávea, Rio de Janeiro - RJ
eases, such as Alzheimer’s disease. Early diag-
22430-060, Brasil. E-mail: veronica_c_araujo@yahoo nosis allows patients and caregivers to have a
.com.br better quality of life and allows access to treat-
317
318 ARAUJO ET AL.

ments that may slow the course of the disease 1994). The study by Brucki et al. (2003) also
(Chang et al., 2014; James, Grace, Thomas, & included youngsters in their sample, and the
Combrinck, 2015; Li & Hsu, 2015; Paddick et sample was divided by schooling (Brucki et al.,
al., 2014; Sposito, Neri, & Yassuda, 2015). 2003). The study by Laks et al. (2007) was
However, for a precise diagnosis to be made, it performed only with elderly individuals, and the
is necessary to know the ways in which cogni- sample was divided by age and schooling. How-
tive decline occurs during aging and the factors ever, the sample did not consist of individuals
that may influence performance on neuropsy- with more than 8 years of schooling, and the
chological tests in the elderly. elderly were older than 84 years of age (Laks et
Cognitive changes that occur as a conse- al., 2007). The creation of normative data that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

quence of aging are well reported in the litera- consider older ages, more years of schooling,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ture, such as changes in processing speed, work- and less broad groups of these variables is nec-
ing memory, and executive function. Recent essary for the early detection of cognitive de-
years have seen a significant increase in the cline in the Brazilian context.
prevalence of dementias (Castro-Costa et al., The Brief Cognitive Screening Battery
2011; Duff & Ramezani, 2015; Soares et al., (BCSB) was developed by Nitrini et al. (1994).
2012; Sposito et al., 2015). Schooling is another It is a tool for cognitive screening that is not
important factor that affects performance on greatly affected by schooling. In the study by
cognitive tests, which has been reported by sev- Nitrini et al. (1994), the performance of subjects
eral studies in various countries (Castro-Costa with mild or moderate dementia was compared
et al., 2011; James et al., 2015; Soares et al., with healthy subjects who were matched by age,
2012; Steibel et al., 2016). Formal education schooling, and gender. Participants were be-
leads to an increase in neuroplasticity and tween 50 and 85 years old and had at least 4
greater complexity of neural networks (Paddick years of schooling. The study concluded that the
et al., 2014). BCSB has a good ability to discriminate be-
Low levels of education and advanced age tween patients and controls (Nitrini et al.,
are considered risk factors for pathological cog- 1994). In another study, the BCSB was used
nitive decline, and these factors can, in many to differentiate subjects with Alzheimer’s dis-
cases, lead to false-positives on cognitive ease from controls. The average schooling of
screening exams (Chang et al., 2014; Li & Hsu, the participants was 11 years. The BCSB also
2015; Paddick et al., 2014; Soares et al., 2012). presented good diagnostic accuracy, mainly in
To prevent or reduce the incidence of such the late memory and verbal fluency tests (Nitrini
false-positives or false-negatives, valid screen- et al., 2007).
ing tests need to utilize normative data by con- Nitrini et al. (2008) evaluated the impact of
sidering age, schooling, or both. This strategy of age and schooling on BCSB performance. The
interpreting the results is even more relevant in study included adults who were 19 – 81 years of
countries with large sociodemographic hetero- age and excluded people who were illiterate. A
geneity (Duff & Ramezani, 2015; Steibel et al., negative correlation between BCSB perfor-
2016), such as Brazil. mance and age was found, and a positive cor-
Few studies of cognitive screening tests have relation between BCSB performance and
provided normative data that are stratified by schooling was found (Nitrini et al., 2008). In
age and schooling. In the case of the Mini another study, BCSB performance was evalu-
Mental State Examination (MMSE; i.e., the ated in elderly individuals with depression com-
most widely used screening test worldwide), the pared with elderly individuals with mild
main studies have been performed by Berto- Alzheimer’s disease and healthy elderly indi-
lucci, Brucki, Campacci, and Juliano (1994); viduals. The results demonstrated the ability of
Brucki, Nitrini, Caramelli, Bertolucci, and Oka- the BCSB to differentiate these three cognitive
moto (2003), and Laks, Baptista, Contino, de profiles (Novaretti, Radanovic, & Nitrini,
Paula, and Engelhardt (2007). Bertolucci et al. 2012). More recently, the BCSB was applied to
(1994) found no significant differences between frail elderly patients who were treated at a ge-
groups of subjects of different ages, including riatric outpatient clinic. In both studies, the
young adults, and the elderly; the sample was BCSB proved to be a good instrument to dif-
only divided by schooling (Bertolucci et al., ferentiate subjects with and without dementia
IMPACT OF AGE AND SCHOOLING ON BCSB 319

(Fichman et al., 2013; Fichman-Charchat et al., Instruments


2016). Two other epidemiological studies that
used the BCSB to identify and differentiate The BCSB consisted of the Memory of Fig-
cases of dementia were those of Herrera, Cara- ure test, Semantic Verbal Fluency test (animals
melli, Silveira, and Nitrini (2002) and Nitrini, category), Clock Drawing test. Lawton’s Instru-
Caramelli, Herrera, Bahia, et al. (2004). Both mental Activities of Daily Living Scale, the
studies were conducted in the city of Catan- Pfeffer Functional Activities Questionnaire, and
duva, Brazil. In addition to identifying cases of the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) were
dementia, these studies concluded that older age used to describe the functional profile of the
and lower schooling were associated with worse elderly. The Mini Mental State Examination
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cognitive performance in the elderly and a (MMSE) was used to measure general cognitive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

greater propensity to develop neurodegenera- functioning.


tive disease (Herrera et al., 2002; Nitrini, Cara- The Memory of Figures test consisted of a
melli, Herrera, Porto, et al., 2004). The objec- sheet of 10 simple drawings of common objects
tive of the present study was to investigate the (i.e., shoe, house, comb, key, airplane, bucket,
impact of age and schooling on performance on turtle, book, spoon, and tree) that should be
the BCSB in elderly residents in the city of Rio named by the examiner and then recalled im-
de Janeiro, Brazil. mediately by the subject. The subject was then
presented with the drawings two more times and
Method then asked each time to remember and say as
many drawings as possible. An interval of 5 min
Participants then elapsed when other tests were performed,
and the subject was asked again to remember
The sample consisted of 470 elderly individ- and say the figures. Finally, the recognition task
uals, ⬎60 years old and of both sexes, who was performed with a sheet of paper that con-
attended a social program that was offered by tained both the 10 figures above and 10 other
the government of Rio de Janeiro (Casas de distracting figures. The subject was asked to
Convivência e Lazer para Idosos). The subjects recognize which figures were presented previ-
were divided into five groups of schooling (0 ously (Nitrini et al., 1994).
years–illiterates, 1– 4 years, 5– 8 years, 9 –11 In the Semantic Verbal Fluency test (animals
years, and ⱖ12 years) and three age groups category), the subjects were asked to say as
(60 – 69 years, 70 –79 years, and ⱖ80 years). many animals as possible, as quickly as possi-
The inclusion criteria were participants who ble, in 1 min (Borod, Goodglass, & Kaplan,
were older than 60 years old, who agreed to 1980; Brucki, Fleury Malheiros, Okamoto, &
participate in the study, and who understood Bertolucci, 1997).
and signed an informed consent form that was In the Clock Drawing test, the subjects were
approved by the Research Ethics Committee asked to draw a clock with all the numbers
(opinion no. 965.264). The exclusion criteria inside and the watch hands marking 2:45 (2 h
were the existence of functional impairment and 45 min). Scores were calculated based on
(assessed by Lawton’s Instrumental Activities the criteria adapted from Sunderland and co-
of Daily Living Scale, score ⱕ5) and severe workers (1989; Mendes-Santos, Mograbi, Spen-
hearing or visual impairment that would make ciere, & Charchat-Fichman, 2015; Nitrini et al.,
communication difficult.
1994).
Procedures The MMSE has been used to assess global
cognitive function in Brazil (Brucki et al., 2003;
The evaluation lasted ⬃1 h and was held in a Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). In the pres-
quiet room. The questionnaires assessing the ent study, we used the score of 30 (MEEM-30)
independence of the elderly were duly filled out and 35 (MEEM-35). The score 35 was formed
by the accompanying persons who were indi- by the score of MEEM-30 added to the result of
cated by the subject. They participated in this the task of inverse spelling of the word
study as researchers, psychologists, and psy- “WORLD” (maximum of 5 points).
chology students, all attending weekly supervi- Lawton’s Instrumental Activities of Daily
sion. Living Scale and the Pfeffer Functional Activ-
320 ARAUJO ET AL.

ities Questionnaire assess the independence of (44.6%, n ⫽ 209) were widowed, 73 (15.3%)
the elderly with regard to instrumental activities were single, 97 (20.8%) were married, and 91
of daily living. For the Lawton scale, the elderly (19.3%) were divorced.
subjects and their companion responded to the With regard to age groups, 33.8% of the
questionnaire. For the Pfeffer questionnaire, sample were 60 – 69 years old, 48.87% were
only the elderly subject’s companion responded 70 –79 years old, and 17.4% were ⱖ80 years
(Dos Santos & Virtuoso Júnior, 2008; Dutra, old. With regard to schooling, 3.6% had 0 years
Ribeiro, Pinheiro, de Melo, & Carvalho, 2015; of schooling, 19.6% had 1– 4 years of school-
Lawton & Brody, 1969; Pfeffer, Kurosaki, ing, 18.7% had 5– 8 years of schooling, 17.4%
Harrah, Chance, & Filos, 1982). The GDS-15 is had 9 –11 years of schooling, and 40.6%
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

a shortened version of the original 30-item in- had ⱖ12 years of schooling. The means, SDs,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

strument, which has presented good sensitivity and percentiles for the BCSB for the total sam-
and specificity for screening depressive symp- ple are presented in Table 1. The means, SDs,
toms (Paradela, Lourenço, & Veras, 2005; Ye- and percentiles for the GDS, Lawton scale, and
savage et al., 1982). Pfeffer questionnaire are presented in Table 2.
The means and SDs for the BCSB stratified
Data Analysis
by age and schooling groups are presented in
The data were analyzed using SPSS 20.0 Tables 3 through 5. The data in Table 3 are from
software. Descriptive statistics were initially the 60- to 69-year-old group. The data in Table
performed. Frequencies, means, SDs, and per- 4 are from the 70- to 79-year-old group. The
centiles were analyzed according to age and data in Table 5 are from the ⱖ80-year-old
schooling groups for each BSCB measure. group.
Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) fol- The ANOVA indicated that some of the mem-
lowed by the Bonferroni post hoc test was used ory tasks were impacted solely by age. For inci-
to verify significant differences between age dental memory, immediate memory, learning, and
and schooling groups and interactions for each delayed recall memory test, the performance of
BSCB measure. Pearson’s correlation analysis the 60- to 69-year-old group was significantly
was performed to determine correlations be- different from the ⱖ80-year-old group (p ⬍ .01).
tween age, schooling, and cognition. Values of The 70- to 79-year-old group presented better
p ⬍ .05 were considered statistically significant. performance than the ⱖ80-year-old group on the
same tasks. For recognition, a significant differ-
Results ence was observed between the 60- to 69-year-old
group and ⱖ80-year-old group (p ⬍ .05). Pear-
The sample consisted of 420 women (89.4%) son’s correlation analysis indicated that incidental
and 50 men (10.6%). The mean age was 72.77 ⫾ memory, immediate memory, learning, delayed
7.06 years, and the mean schooling was 9.54 ⫾ recall memory test, and recognition were nega-
5.32 years. The majority of the elderly subjects tively correlated with age (p ⬍ .01; Table 6).

Table 1
Means, SDs, and Percentiles for the BCSB (n ⫽ 470)
Percentile Nom. I.M. Im.M. Lear. D.R.M. Rec. C.D.T. V.F. ME30 ME35
10 10.00 4.00 6.00 7.00 5.00 9.00 3.00 9.00 21.00 22.00
25 10.00 5.00 7.00 8.00 7.00 10.00 4.00 12.00 22.00 25.00
50 10.00 6.00 8.00 9.00 8.00 10.00 5.00 15.00 25.00 29.00
75 10.00 6.00 9.00 10.00 9.00 10.00 7.00 19.00 27.00 32.00
90 10.00 7.00 9.90 10.00 10.00 10.00 9.00 22.00 29.00 34.00
Mean 9.84 5.50 7.79 8.53 7.49 9.77 5.49 15.57 24.77 28.17
SD .65 1.46 1.48 1.38 2.10 .69 2.13 4.89 3.21 4.51
Note. BCSB ⫽ Brief Cognitive Screening Battery; Nom. ⫽ Nomination; I.M. ⫽ Incidental Memory; Im.M. ⫽ Immediate
Memory; Lear. ⫽ Learning; D.R.M. ⫽ Delayed Recall Memory test; Rec. ⫽ Recognition; C.D.T. ⫽ Clock Drawing test;
V.F. ⫽ Verbal Fluency test; ME30 ⫽ MMSE-30; ME35 ⫽ MMSE-35.
IMPACT OF AGE AND SCHOOLING ON BCSB 321

Table 2

Note. Nom. ⫽ Nomination; I.M. ⫽ Incidental Memory; Im.M. ⫽ Immediate Memory; Lear. ⫽ Learning; D.R.M. ⫽ Delayed Recall Memory test; Rec. ⫽ Recognition; C.D.T. ⫽
21.25 (2.60)
26.56 (4.16)
26.52 (3.65)
29.04 (3.85)
30.12 (3.23)
M (SD)
ME35
Means, SDs, and Percentiles for the Lawton Scale
and Pfeffer Questionnaire (n ⫽ 470)
Lawton Lawton
Percentile GDS elderly companion Pfeffer

21.00 (2.33)
23.92 (2.93)
23.72 (2.72)
25.37 (2.59)
26.18 (2.46)
10 .00 18.00 17.00 .00

M (SD)
ME30
25 1.00 20.00 19.00 .00
50 2.00 21.00 20.00 .00
75 4.00 21.00 21.00 1.00
90 7.00 21.00 21.00 4.00
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Mean 2.86 20.19 19.64 1.27

12.38 (1.77)
13.04 (4.50)
13.28 (4.63)
16.19 (4.84)
17.96 (4.76)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

SD 3.02 1.57 2.64 3.06

M (SD)
V.F.
Note. GDS ⫽ Geriatric Depression Scale.

There was age effect in the difference between

3.50 (1.31)
4.71 (2.10)
5.48 (2.24)
6.04 (2.14)
5.97 (2.03)
learning and delayed recall (F ⫽ 4.42, p ⬍ .05).

M (SD)
C.D.T.
The difference is higher for those aged 80 years
and over when compared with the other age strata
groups (p ⬍ .05).
The impact of schooling was evaluated in tasks

10.00 (.00)
9.92 (.40)
9.80 (.50)
9.89 (.32)
9.91 (.29)
M (SD)
that were related to executive function and global
cognitive function. In the Clock Drawing test, Rec.
significant differences were found between the
group with 0 years of schooling and the other
groups (all p ⬍ .01), with the exception of
8.63 (1.06)
7.28 (2.11)
7.60 (1.66)
8.15 (1.63)
7.88 (1.77)
Means and SDs for the 60 – 69 Year Old Group Stratified by Schooling (n ⫽ 159)

Clock Drawing test; V.F. ⫽ Verbal Fluency test; ME30 ⫽ MMSE-30; ME35 ⫽ MMSE-35.
M (SD)
D.R.M.

the group with 1– 4 years of schooling, in which


the difference only approached statistical signifi-
cance (p ⫽ .09). Performance on the Clock Draw-
ing test was significantly different between the
8.32 (1.31)
8.56 (1.16)

8.64 (1.46)

group with 1– 4 years of schooling and the group


8.88 (.83)

9.22 (.75)
M (SD)
Lear.

with 9 –11 years of schooling (p ⫽ .019) and


between the group with 1– 4 years of schooling
and the group with ⱖ12 years of schooling (p ⬍
.01). In the Semantic Verbal Fluency test, signif-
7.75 (1.04)
7.84 (1.55)
8.04 (1.27)
8.04 (1.29)
8.07 (1.55)
M (SD)

icant differences were found between the group


Im.M.

with ⱖ12 years of schooling and all of the other


groups (0 years, 1– 4 years, and 5– 8 years, all
p ⬍ .01; 9 –11 years, p ⫽ .03). Pearson’s corre-
lation analysis indicated that performance on both
5.44 (1.42)
5.84 (1.46)
5.44 (1.31)
5.84 (1.46)
6.00 (.76)
M (SD)

the Clock Drawing test and Semantic Verbal Flu-


I.M.

ency test was positively correlated with years of


schooling (p ⬍ .01).
Performance on the MMSE-30 indicated signif-
icant differences between all schooling groups
9.75 (.71)
9.76 (.52)
9.88 (.33)
9.96 (.19)
9.81 (.70)
M (SD)
Nom.

(p ⬍ .01), with the exception of the comparison


between the group with 5– 8 years of schooling
and the group with 9 –11 years of schooling (p ⫽
1.00). The MMSE-35 results were similar to the
9–11 (n ⫽ 27)
ⱖ12 (n ⫽ 74)
1–4 (n ⫽ 25)
5–8 (n ⫽ 25)

MMSE-30 but with a less marked difference be-


Schooling
(years)
0 (n ⫽ 8)

tween the group with 9 –11 years of schooling and


Table 3

the group with ⱖ12 years of schooling (p ⬍ .05).


Pearson’s correlation analysis indicated that per-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

322

Table 4
Means and SDs for the 70 –79 Year Old Group Stratified by Schooling (n ⫽ 229)
Schooling Nom. I.M. Im.M. Lear. D.R.M. Rec. C.D.T. V.F. ME30 ME35
(years) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
0 (n ⫽ 6) 9.50 (.84) 5.67 (1.86) 7.33 (1.97) 8.17 (1.17) 6.83 (1.72) 9.83 (.41) 3.17 (1.94) 12.00 (2.37) 18.83 (3.13) 19.83 (5.08)
1–4 (n ⫽ 50) 9.70 (1.02) 5.76 (1.65) 7.76 (1.56) 8.34 (1.60) 7.24 (2.34) 9.64 (1.03) 4.66 (1.88) 12.86 (4.06) 22.40 (3.04) 24.42 (4.49)
5–8 (n ⫽ 44) 9.86 (.77) 5.66 (1.22) 7.84 (1.43) 8.48 (1.17) 7.64 (2.06) 9.70 (.70) 5.50 (2.01) 15.14 (4.87) 24.95 (2.86) 29.05 (3.83)
9–11 (n ⫽ 39) 9.92 (.35) 5.38 (1.41) 8.05 (1.50) 8.67 (1.58) 7.46 (2.04) 9.85 (.43) 5.46 (2.22) 15.90 (5.23) 25.13 (2.68) 28.97 (3.74)
ⱖ12 (n ⫽ 90) 9.98 (.15) 5.29 (1.47) 7.88 (1.32) 8.78 (1.25) 7.88 (1.95) 9.81 (.60) 6.30 (2.25) 17.67 (4.57) 26.48 (2.74) 30.50 (3.46)
Note. Nom. ⫽ Nomination; I.M. ⫽ Incidental Memory; Im.M. ⫽ Immediate Memory; Lear. ⫽ Learning; D.R.M. ⫽ Delayed Recall Memory test; Rec. ⫽ Recognition; C.D.T. ⫽
Clock Drawing test; V.F. ⫽ Verbal Fluency test; ME30 ⫽ MMSE-30; ME35 ⫽ MMSE-35.
ARAUJO ET AL.

Table 5
Means and SDs for the ⱖ80 Year Old Group Stratified by Schooling (n ⫽ 82)
Schooling Nom. I.M. Im.M. Lear. D. R. M. Rec. C.D.T. V.F. ME30 ME35
(years) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
0 (n ⫽ 3) 9.67 (.58) 5.33 (2.52) 7.67 (2.08) 8.33 (.58) 7.00 (1.73) 10.00 (.00) 2.67 (2.08) 13.00 (4.58) 20.67 (4.51) 20.67 (4.51)
1–4 (n ⫽ 17) 9.47 (.94) 4.71 (1.45) 7.12 (1.58) 8.00 (1.90) 6.06 (2.90) 9.47 (1.18) 4.47 (1.46) 13.12 (4.20) 23.06 (3.13) 26.24 (4.70)
5–8 (n ⫽ 19) 9.84 (.38) 5.26 (1.15) 7.21 (1.58) 8.05 (1.39) 6.89 (1.70) 9.84 (.50) 5.05 (1.78) 14.11 (3.91) 24.26 (3.12) 27.00 (4.50)
9–11 (n ⫽ 16) 9.44 (1.55) 5.44 (1.67) 7.13 (1.41) 8.06 (1.18) 6.25 (2.75) 9.56 (.89) 5.25 (1.57) 14.81 (4.29) 24.13 (2.87) 27.75 (3.82)
ⱖ12 (n ⫽ 27) 10.00 (.00) 4.96 (1.58) 7.15 (1.68) 8.04 (1.53) 6.81 (2.65) 9.44 (1.28) 5.63 (1.86) 16.19 (4.16) 24.67 (3.54) 28.52 (4.55)
Note. Nom. ⫽ Nomination; I.M. ⫽ Incidental Memory; Im.M. ⫽ Immediate Memory; Lear. ⫽ Learning; D.R.M. ⫽ Delayed Recall Memory; Rec. ⫽ Recognition; C.D.T. ⫽ Clock
Drawing test; V.F. ⫽ Verbal Fluency test; ME30 ⫽ MMSE-30; ME35 ⫽ MMSE-35.
IMPACT OF AGE AND SCHOOLING ON BCSB 323

Table 6
BCSB Performance Stratified by Age (n ⫽ 470)
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3
Cognitive variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Performance
Nomination 9.84 (.56) 9.87 (.63) 9.73 (.85) G2 ⬎ G1 and G3
Incidental memory 5.72 (1.40) 5.49 (1.47) 5.09 (1.50) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3ⴱ
Immediate memory 8.01 (1.43) 7.86 (1.44) 7.17 (1.56) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3ⴱ
Learning 8.69 (1.28) 8.59 (1.38) 8.05 (1.47) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3ⴱ
Delayed recall memory 7.82 (1.77) 7.59 (2.07) 6.57 (2.48) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3ⴱ
Recognition 9.89 (.35) 9.76 (.71) 9.59 (1.02) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3ⴱ
⬎ ⬎ G3
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Clock Drawing test 5.59 (2.15) 5.56 (2.22) 5.07 (1.79) G1 G2


Verbal Fluency test 15.87 (5.08) 15.68 (4.95) 14.68 (4.22) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

MMSE-30 25.04 (2.91) 24.86 (3.35) 23.99 (3.31) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3


MMSE-35 28.36 (4.17) 28.35 (4.68) 27.26 (4.60) G1 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G3
Note. BCSB ⫽ Brief Cognitive Screening Battery; ME30 ⫽ MMSE-30; ME35 ⫽ MMSE-35. Group 1, 60 – 69 years old;
Group 2, 70 –79 years old; Group 3, ⱖ80 years old.

p ⬍ .05, significant difference between groups (one-way analysis of variance).

formance on the MMSE-30 and MMSE-35 was in elderly residents in the city of Rio de Janeiro.
positively correlated with years of schooling (p ⬍ Most studies that have used the BCSB included
.01; Table 7). outpatient populations or mixed elderly and
The ANOVA indicated an Age ⫻ Schooling young adults in the same sample. Few studies
interaction only for global cognitive function on have included elderly participants who were
the MMSE-35 (F(8, 471 ⫽ 2.30, p ⫽ .02). Figure stratified by levels of schooling. The present
1 shows the results of the multifactorial analysis of study evaluated BCSB performance in commu-
the BCSB data that indicated the effects of the nity-dwelling elderly individuals with all levels
Age ⫻ Schooling interaction on global cognitive of schooling.
function. We found that sociodemographic factors,
such as age and schooling, differentially impact
Discussion
cognitive function. Age affected immediate
The present study investigated the effects of memory but had no effect on executive function
age and schooling on performance on the BCSB or measures of global cognitive function. One

Table 7
BCSB Performance Stratified by Schooling (n ⫽ 470)
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 Group 4 Group 5
Cognitive variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Performance
Nomination 9.65 (.70) 9.67 (.89) 9.86 (.59) 9.84 (.74) 9.92 (.45)
G2 ⬍ G5ⴱ
Incidental memory 5.76 (1.48) 5.48 (1.59) 5.63 (1.28) 5.41 (1.41) 5.46 (1.51)
G1 ⬎ G3 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G5 ⬎ G4
Immediate memory 7.59 (1.50) 7.66 (1.56) 7.76 (1.44) 7.87 (1.45) 7.85 (1.49)
G4 ⬎ G5 ⬎ G3 ⬎ G2 ⬎ G1
Learning 8.53 (.94) 8.27 (1.58) 8.41 (1.22) 8.73 (1.33) 8.62 (1.39)
G4 ⬎ G5 ⬎ G1 ⬎ G3 ⬎ G2
Delay Recall memory 7.71 (1.61) 7.03 (2.42) 7.47 (1.88) 7.45 (2.16) 7.73 (2.02)
G5 ⬎ G1 ⬎ G3 ⬎ G4 ⬎ G2
Recognition 9.94 (.24) 9.68 (.94) 9.76 (.61) 9.80 (.53) 9.79 (.67)
G1 ⬎ G4 ⬎ G5 ⬎ G3 ⬎ G2
Clock Drawing test 3.24 (1.60) 4.64 (1.85) 5.40 (2.01) 5.61 (2.08) 6.08 (2.12)
G1 ⬍ G3, G4, and G5ⴱ
G2 ⬍ G4 and G5ⴱ
Verbal Fluency test 12.35 (2.42) 12.96 (4.16) 14.39 (4.63) 15.78 (4.90) 17.57 (4.60) G1 ⬍ G4, and G5ⴱ
G2 ⬍ G4, and G5ⴱ
G3 ⬍ G5ⴱ
MMSE-30 20.18 (3.00) 22.93 (3.07) 24.45 (2.90) 25.01 (2.69) 26.10 (2.81) G1 ⬍ G2 ⬍ G3 ⬍ G4 ⬍ G5ⴱ
MMSE-35 20.65 (3.74) 25.34 (4.51) 27.89 (4.06) 28.76 (3.78) 30.07 (3.59) G1 ⬍ G2 ⬍ G3 ⬍ G4 ⬍ G5ⴱ
Note. BCSB ⫽ Brief Cognitive Screening Battery; ME30 ⫽ MMSE-30; ME35 ⫽ MMSE-35.Group 1, 0 years of
schooling; Group 2, 1– 4 years of schooling; Group 3, 5– 8 years of schooling; Group 4, 9 –11 years of schooling; Group
5, ⱖ12 years of schooling.

p ⬍ .05, significant difference between groups (one-way analysis of variance).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

324
ARAUJO ET AL.

indicated the effects of the Age ⫻ Schooling interaction on global cognitive function.
Figure 1. Multifactorial analysis of the Brief Cognitive Screening Battery (BCSB) data that
IMPACT OF AGE AND SCHOOLING ON BCSB 325

explanation for the impact of age on memory ospatial strategies, planning, and semantic lex-
task performance is the negative effect of age on ical access, all of which are necessary for good
immediate memory and learning (Castro-Costa performance on the Clock Drawing test, Seman-
et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2011; Sposito et al., tic Verbal Fluency test, and assessments of
2015; Tripathi, Kumar, Bharath, Marimuthu, & global cognitive function, such as the MMSE
Varghese, 2014). Such deleterious effects on (Mokri et al., 2012; Moraes et al., 2013). Years
memory may be explained by decreases in brain of schooling can also improve synaptic-
volume, neurotransmitter levels, or concentra- dendritic connections, myelination, and neuro-
tions of brain metabolites (Collinson, Fang, genesis (Noroozian et al., 2014); thus, leading
Lim, Feng, & Ng, 2014). Immediate memory is to better cognitive skills, such as language, se-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

essential for learning, and immediate memory mantic memory, attention, mental organization,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

deficits have a negative impact on learning. In and processing speed (Nielsen & Jørgensen,
the present study, performance on the memory 2013). These abilities are strongly related to
tasks was only significantly different for the executive function, which may explain the
oldest group (ⱖ80 years of age). The subjects in worse performance in less-educated elderly in-
this group were as old as 96 years of age, which dividuals.
is 20 years older than the majority of the sam- Cognitive reserve is another important factor
ple. that is related to schooling. Older people with
Previous studies have reported that learning more years of schooling have more cognitive
ability worsens as individuals age (Avila, reserve that may protect them against the dele-
Lopes, Nakano, & Bottino, 2016; Soares et al., terious effects of aging and neurodegenerative
2012). In Avila et al. (2016), standards were disease. Cognitive reserve is not fully formed in
defined for memory assessment using the Fuld school; it develops and improves throughout
Object Memory Evaluation (that was developed life. People with more years of schooling also
to be less influenced by schooling and can mea- tend to have jobs and engage in activities that
sure learning ability and episodic memory with- stimulate cognition; thus, increasing their cog-
out the use of word lists) and Memory of Fig- nitive reserve with advancing age (Noroozian et
ures Test. The results of Avila et al. (2016) al., 2014; Paddick et al., 2014). In the present
indicated a significant effect of age, but unlike study, Pearson’s correlation analysis indicated a
in the Memory of Figures Test, schooling was strong influence (all p ⬍ .01) of schooling on
also a determining factor in the outcome. performance on tasks that require visuocon-
Soares et al. (2012) used the BCSB to eval- structive skills, planning, mental organization,
uate the impact of sociodemographic vari- and a broad vocabulary. Mokri et al. (2012)
ables on cognition in a sample of community- compared performance in illiterate seniors with
dwelling elderly individuals. They also found older people who had learned to read and write,
an effect of age, which predicted worse per- regardless of whether they attended school. The
formance in memory tasks. Similar to the results of the Clock Drawing test, Verbal Flu-
present study, memory was unaffected by ency test, and MMSE indicated that elderly
schooling. Other studies investigated the ef- individuals with relatively low levels of school-
fects of schooling on BCSB performance, in- ing performed significantly better than elderly
dicating that the Memory of Figures Test is individuals who were illiterate (i.e., 0 years of
valid for populations with different levels of schooling). One explanation for this is that the
schooling, including illiterate individuals ability to read and write facilitates performance
with no schooling (Fichman-Charchat et al., on written tasks that require the use of a pencil
2016; Nitrini, Caramelli, Herrera, Porto, et (Mokri et al., 2012; Soares et al., 2012).
al., 2004; Takada et al., 2006). Notably, in the present study, the Age ⫻
In contrast to other studies, in the present Schooling interaction affected only global cog-
study, performance on tasks that evaluated ex- nitive function, evaluated by the MMSE-35. In
ecutive function (i.e., Clock Drawing test and the other tasks, cognitive reserve that was gen-
Semantic Verbal Fluency test) and global cog- erated by schooling may have protected the
nitive function (i.e., MMSE-30 and MMSE-35) subjects against the negative effects of aging on
was only affected by schooling and not by age. cognition, and younger individuals, even those
Schooling, especially literacy, improves visu- who had only a few years of schooling, may
326 ARAUJO ET AL.

have performed better because they were still ency]. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 55, 56– 61.
young relative to elderly individuals who were http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X199700
older than 80 years of age. 0100009
One limitation of the present study was the Brucki, S. M., Nitrini, R., Caramelli, P., Bertolucci,
relatively low number of illiterate elderly indi- P. H., & Okamoto, I. H. (2003). Sugestões para o
uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil
viduals with 0 years of schooling and very old
[Suggestions for utilization of the mini-mental
individuals (i.e., ⱖ80 years of age). A relevant state examination in Brazil]. Arquivos de Neuro-
factor may be the lack of available tools to Psiquiatria, 61, 777–781. http://dx.doi.org/10
assess the rate of illiteracy that have been .1590/S0004-282X2003000500014
adapted and validated for the Brazilian popula- Castro-Costa, E., Dewey, M. E., Uchôa, E., Firmo,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion. Another limitation may be that some of the J. O., Lima-Costa, M. F., & Stewart, R. (2011).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

subjects in the present study who were not con- Trajectories of cognitive decline over 10 years in a
sidered illiterate for group assignment were not Brazilian elderly population: The Bambuí Cohort
actually able to understand text that was slightly Study of Aging. Cadernos de Saude Publica,
more complex than, for example, “Close your 27(Suppl. 3), S345–S350. http://dx.doi.org/10
eyes” on the MMSE. Lastly, the recruitment of .1590/S0102-311X2011001500004
subjects may have been limited by accessibility, Chang, J., Tse, C. S., Leung, G. T., Fung, A. W.,
in which some elderly individuals are unable to Hau, K. T., Chiu, H. F., & Lam, L. C. (2014). Bias
attend activities in an autonomous and indepen- in discriminating very mild dementia for older
adults with different levels of education in Hong
dent way; thus, making this age group less
Kong. International Psychogeriatrics, 26, 995–
accessible for recruitment. 1010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1041610214
The present results indicate that the BCSB is 000234
a good tool for cognitive screening that is gen- Collinson, S. L., Fang, S. H., Lim, M. L., Feng, L., &
erally not influenced by interactions between Ng, T. P. (2014). Normative data for the repeatable
age and schooling. Normative data that are battery for the assessment of neuropsychological
stratified by age and schooling are needed to status in elderly Chinese. Archives of Clinical Neu-
better understand the performance of older ropsychology, 29, 442– 455. http://dx.doi.org/10
adults with different levels of schooling and .1093/arclin/acu023
improve the accuracy of this instrument. Dos Santos, R. L., & Virtuoso Júnior, J. S. (2008).
Confiabilidade da versão Brasileira da Escala de
Atividades Instrumentais de Vida Diária [Reliabil-
References ity of the Brazilian version of the Scale of Instru-
mental Activities of Daily Living]. Revista Brasil-
Avila, R., Lopes, M. A., Nakano, E. Y., & Bottino,
eira em Promoção da Saúde, 21, 290–296. http://
C. M. (2016). Normative data of Fuld Object
dx.doi.org/10.5020/18061230.2008.p290
Memory Evaluation test for Brazilian elderly pop-
ulation. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 74, 138– Duff, K., & Ramezani, A. (2015). Regression-based
144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282X20 normative formulae for the Repeatable Battery for
150200 the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status for
Bertolucci, P. H., Brucki, S. M., Campacci, S. R., & older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychol-
Juliano, Y. (1994). O Mini-Exame do Estado Men- ogy, 30, 600– 604. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/
tal em uma população geral. Impacto da escolari- acv052
dade [The Mini-Mental State Examination in an Dutra, M. C., Ribeiro, R. S., Pinheiro, S. B., de Melo,
outpatient population: influence of literacy]. Ar- G. F., & Carvalho, G. A. (2015). Accuracy and
quivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 52, 1–7. http://dx.doi reliability of the Pfeffer Questionnaire for the Bra-
.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001 zilian elderly population. Dementia & Neuropsy-
Borod, J. C., Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1980). chologia, 9, 176–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/
Normative data on the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia 1980-57642015DN92000012
Examination, parietal lobe battery, and the Boston Fichman, H. C., Fernandes, C. S., Oliveira, R. M.,
Naming Test. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychol- Caramelli, P., Aguiar, D., & Novaes, R. (2013).
ogy, 2, 209–215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Predomínio de Comprometimento Cognitivo Leve
01688638008403793 Disexecutivo em idosos atendidos no ambulatório
Brucki, S. M. D., Malheiros, S. M., Okamoto, I. H., da geriatria de um hospital público terciário na
& Bertolucci, P. H. (1997). Dados normativos para cidade do Rio de Janeiro [Disexecutive Mild Cog-
o teste de fluência verbal categoria animais em nitive Impairment predominance in elderly as-
nosso meio [Normative data: Category verbal flu- sisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio
IMPACT OF AGE AND SCHOOLING ON BCSB 327

de Janeiro]. Revista Neuropsicologia Latinoameri- Mendes-Santos, L. C., Mograbi, D., Spenciere, B., &
cana, 5, 31– 40. Charchat-Fichman, H. (2015). Specific algorithm
Fichman-Charchat, H., Miranda, C. V., Fernandes, method of scoring the Clock Drawing Test applied
C. S., Mograbi, D., Oliveira, R. M., Novaes, R., & in cognitively normal elderly. Dementia & Neuro-
Aguiar, D. (2016). Brief Cognitive Screening Bat- psychologia, 9, 128–135. http://dx.doi.org/10
tery (BCSB) is a very useful tool for diagnosis of .1590/1980-57642015DN92000007
probable mild Alzheimer=s disease in a geriatric Mokri, H., Ávila-Funes, J. A., Le Goff, L., Ruiz-
clinic. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 74, 149– Arregui, L., Gutierrez Robledo, L. M., & Amieva,
154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282X2015 H. (2012). Self-reported reading and writing skills
0202 in elderly who never attended school influence
Figueiredo, C. S., Assis, M. G., Silva, S. L., Dias, cognitive performances: Results from the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

R. C., & Mancini, M. C. (2013). Functional and Coyoacán cohort study. The Journal of Nutrition,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cognitive changes in community-dwelling elderly: Health & Aging, 16, 621– 624. http://dx.doi.org/10
Longitudinal study. Brazilian Journal of Physical .1007/s12603-012-0070-8
Therapy, 17, 297–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/ Moraes, A. L., Guimarães, L. S., Joanette, Y., de
S1413-35552012005000094 Mattos Pimenta Parente, M. A., Fonseca, R. P., &
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. Almeida, R. M. (2013). Effect of aging, education,
(1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method reading and writing, semantic processing and de-
for grading the cognitive state of patients for the pression symptoms on verbal fluency. Psicologia:
clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, Reflexão e Crítica, 26, 680– 690. http://dx.doi.org/
189–198. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956 10.1590/S0102-79722013000400008
(75)90026-6 Nielsen, T. R., & Jørgensen, K. (2013). Visuocon-
Herrera, E., Jr., Caramelli, P., Silveira, A. S., & structional abilities in cognitively healthy illiterate
Nitrini, R. (2002). Epidemiologic survey of de- Turkish immigrants: A quantitative and qualitative
mentia in a community-dwelling Brazilian popu- investigation. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 27,
lation. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disor- 681– 692. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13854046
ders, 16, 103–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ .2013.767379
00002093-200204000-00007 Nitrini, R., Brucki, S. M. D., Smid, J., Carthery-
James, K. A., Grace, L. K., Thomas, K. G., & Com- Goulart, M. T., Renato, A., Areza-Fegyveres, R.,
brinck, M. I. (2015). Associations between CAM- . . . Mansur, L. L. (2008). Influence of age, gender
COG-R subscale performance and formal educa- and educational level on performance in the Brief
tion attainment in South African older adults. Cognitive Battery-Edu. Dementia & Neuropsycho-
International Psychogeriatrics, 27, 251–260. logia, 2, 114–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1041610214002233 S1980-57642009DN20200007
Laks, J., Baptista, E. M., Contino, A. L., de Paula, Nitrini, R., Caramelli, P., Herrera, E., Jr., Bahia,
E. O., & Engelhardt, E. (2007). Mini-Mental State V. S., Caixeta, L. F., Radanovic, M., . . . Taka-
Examination norms in a community-dwelling sam- hashi, D. Y. (2004). Incidence of dementia in a
ple of elderly with low schooling in Brazil. Cad- community-dwelling Brazilian population. Alzhei-
ernos de Saude Publica, 23, 315–319. http://dx.doi mer Disease and Associated Disorders, 18, 241–
.org/10.1590/S0102-311X2007000200007 246.
Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment Nitrini, R., Caramelli, P., Herrera, E., Jr., Porto, C. S.,
of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental Charchat-Fichman, H., Carthery, M. T., . . . Lima,
activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9, E. P. (2004). Performance of illiterate and literate
179–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/9 nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-
.3_Part_1.179 term memory. Journal of the International Neuro-
Li, C. L., & Hsu, H. C. (2015). Cognitive function psychological Society, 10, 634– 638. http://dx.doi
and associated factors among older people in Tai- .org/10.1017/S1355617704104062
wan: Age and sex differences. Archives of Geron- Nitrini, R., Caramelli, P., Porto, C. S., Charchat-
tology and Geriatrics, 60, 196–200. http://dx.doi Fichman, H., Formigoni, A. P., Carthery-Goulart,
.org/10.1016/j.archger.2014.10.007 M. T., . . . Prandini, J. C. (2007). Brief cognitive
Liu, K. P., Kuo, M. C., Tang, K. C., Chau, A. W., Ho, battery in the diagnosis of mild Alzheimer’s dis-
I. H., Kwok, M. P., . . . Chu, L. W. (2011). Effects ease in subjects with medium and high levels of
of age, education and gender in the Consortium to education. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 1, 32–
Establish a Registry for the Alzheimer’s Disease 36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642008
(CERAD)-Neuropsychological Assessment Bat- DN10100006
tery for Cantonese-speaking Chinese elders. Inter- Nitrini, R., Lefèvre, B. H., Mathias, S. C., Caramelli,
national Psychogeriatrics, 23, 1575–1581. http:// P., Carrilho, P. E., Sauaia, N., . . . Scaff, M. (1994).
dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1041610211001153 Testes neuropsicológicos de aplicação simples
328 ARAUJO ET AL.

para o diagnóstico de demência [Brief and easy- poverished sub-district of São Paulo in Brazil.
to-administer neuropsychological tests in the diag- Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 54, e187–
nosis of dementia]. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquia- e192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2011.11
tria, 52, 457– 465. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/ .014
S0004-282X1994000400001 Sposito, G., Neri, A. L., & Yassuda, M. S. (2015).
Noroozian, M., Shakiba, A., & Iran-nejad, S. (2014). Cognitive performance and engagement in physi-
The impact of illiteracy on the assessment of cog- cal, social and intellectual activities in older adults:
nition and dementia: A critical issue in the devel- The FIBRA study. Dementia & Neuropsychologia,
oping countries. International Psychogeriatrics, 9, 270–278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-
26, 2051–2060. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ 57642015dn93000010
S1041610214001707 Steibel, N. M., Olchik, M. R., Yassuda, M. S., Finger,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Novaretti, T. M. S., Radanovic, M., & Nitrini, R. G., & Gomes, I. (2016). Influence of age and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(2012). Screening for cognitive impairment in late education on the Rivermead Behavioral Memory
onset depression in a Brazilian sample using the Test (RBMT) among healthy elderly. Dementia &
BBRC-Edu. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 6, Neuropsychologia, 10, 26–30. http://dx.doi.org/10
85–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-5764201 .1590/s1980-57642016dn10100005
2DN06020004 Sunderland, T., Hill, J. L., Mellow, A. M., Lawlor,
Oliveira, E. M., Silva, H. S., Lopes, A., Cachioni, M., B. A., Gundersheimer, J., Newhouse, P. A., &
Falcão, D. V., Batistoni, S. S., . . . Yassuda, M. S. Grafman, J. H. (1989). Clock drawing in Alzhei-
(2015). Atividades Avançadas de Vida Diária mer’s disease. A novel measure of dementia sever-
(AAVD) e desempenho cognitivo entre idosos ity. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
[Advanced Activities of Daily Living (AADL) and 37, 725–729. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-
cognitive performance among older adults]. Psico- 5415.1989.tb02233.x
USF, 20, 109–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/ Takada, L. T., Caramelli, P., Fichman, H. C., Porto,
1413-82712015200110 C. S., Bahia, V. S., Anghinah, R., . . . Nitrini, R.
Paddick, S. M., Longdon, A., Gray, W. K., Dotchin, (2006). Comparison between two tests of delayed
C., Kisoli, A., Chaote, P., & Walker, R. (2014). recall for the diagnosis of dementia. Arquivos de
The association between educational level and de- Neuro-Psiquiatria, 64, 35– 40. http://dx.doi.org/10
mentia in rural Tanzania. Dementia & Neuropsy- .1590/S0004-282X2006000100008
chologia, 8, 117–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/ Tripathi, R., Kumar, K., Bharath, S., Marimuthu, P.,
S1980-57642014DN82000006 & Varghese, M. (2014). Age, education and gen-
Paradela, E. M., Lourenço, R. A., & Veras, R. P. der effects on neuropsychological functions in
(2005). Validação da escala de depressão geriátrica healthy Indian older adults. Dementia & Neuro-
em um ambulatório geral [Validation of geriatric psychologia, 8, 148–154. http://dx.doi.org/10
depression scale in a general outpatient clinic]. .1590/S1980-57642014DN82000010
Revista de Saude Publica, 39, 918–923. http://dx Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O.,
.doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89102005000600008 Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V. O. (1982).
Pfeffer, R. I., Kurosaki, T. T., Harrah, C. H., Jr., Development and validation of a geriatric depres-
Chance, J. M., & Filos, S. (1982). Measurement of sion screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal
functional activities in older adults in the commu- of Psychiatric Research, 17, 37– 49. http://dx.doi
nity. Journal of Gerontology, 37, 323–329. http:// .org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronj/37.3.323
Soares, L. M., Cachioni, M., Falcão, D. V., Batistoni,
S. S., Lopes, A., Neri, A. L., & Yassuda, M. S. Received June 6, 2017
(2012). Determinants of cognitive performance Revision received October 19, 2017
among community dwelling older adults in an im- Accepted October 31, 2017 䡲

You might also like