You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [University College London]

On: 09 August 2011, At: 00:16


Publisher: Psychology Press
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Clinical and Experimental


Neuropsychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

Weak central coherence in eating disorders: A


step towards looking for an endophenotype of
eating disorders
a a b c
Carolina Lopez , Kate Tchanturia , Daniel Stahl & Janet Treasure
a
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London,
London, UK
b
Department of Biostatistics and Computing, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College
London, London, UK
c
Department of Academic Psychiatry, Guy's, King's, and St. Thomas's Medical
School, London, UK

Available online: 16 Dec 2008

To cite this article: Carolina Lopez, Kate Tchanturia, Daniel Stahl & Janet Treasure (2009): Weak central coherence
in eating disorders: A step towards looking for an endophenotype of eating disorders, Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 31:1, 117-125

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803390802036092

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions


This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution
in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the
contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug
doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any
loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising
directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
2009, 31 (1), 117–125

Weak central coherence in eating disorders:


NCEN

A step towards looking for an endophenotype


of eating disorders

Carolina Lopez,1 Kate Tchanturia,1 Daniel Stahl,2 and Janet Treasure3


Weak Coherence in Eating Disorders

1
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London,
UK
2
Department of Biostatistics and Computing, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London,
London, UK
3
Department of Academic Psychiatry, Guy’s, King’s, and St. Thomas’s Medical School,
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

London, UK

Previous work has found that women with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa have weak coherence. The aim
of this study was to examine whether women who had recovered from an eating disorder (ED) also had weak
coherence. A total of 42 recovered ED women and 42 healthy women were assessed with a battery of five
neuropsychological tests that measure aspects of global or local functioning. The recovered ED group showed
superior local processing and poorer global processing than the healthy group. These results are indicative of
weak coherence. The finding that weak coherence is a stable characteristic rather than a state effect suggests that it
may be an endophenotype for ED.

Keywords: Eating disorders; Central coherence; Recovery; Endophenotype; Cognition; Anorexia nervosa;
Bulimia nervosa; Detail focused processing; Global processing.

INTRODUCTION Another trait of recent interest in the field of ED


is weak central coherence (Gillberg, Rastam, Wentz,
Eating disorders (ED) are complex disorders that & Gillberg, 2007; Lopez et al., 2008a; Southgate,
affect mainly young females and can have severe Tchanturia, & Treasure, in press; Tokley & Kemps,
consequences (Hudson, Hiripi, Pope, & Kessler, 2007). Central coherence was first described by Frith
2007; van Hoeken, Seidell, & Wijbrand, 2003). (1989) as the natural tendency for typically devel-
ED have been traditionally conceptualized as caused oping adults to integrate incoming information
predominately by environmental and psycholog- into context, gestalt, and meaning. The concept of
ical factors. Recently the endophenotype concept weak central coherence was utilized to refer a bias
has been discussed in several ED reviews (Bulik towards local processing at the expense of global
et al., 2007; Steiger & Bruce, 2007; Treasure, 2007; meaning (Frith, 1989; Happé & Booth, 2008;
Treasure, Lopez, & Roberts, 2007). The cognitive Happé & Frith, 2006). This cognitive bias is the
feature of impaired set-shifting fulfils some of the predominant processing style among individuals
criteria for a potential endophenotype for ED with autism spectrum disorders (Happé & Frith,
(Holliday, Tchanturia, Landau, Collier, & Treasure, 2006; Shah & Frith, 1993). Following the latest
2005). revision of this concept in research (Happé &

This work was supported by the Nina Jackson Research Into Eating Disorders (RIED). Carolina Lopez was also funded by a
Chilean government scholarship.
Address correspondence to Carolina Lopez, Department of Academic Psychiatry, 5th Floor, Bermondsey Wing, Guy’s Hospital,
London, SE1 9RT, UK (E-mail: c.lopez@iop.kcl.ac.uk).

© 2008 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
http://www.psypress.com/jcen DOI: 10.1080/13803390802036092
118 LOPEZ ET AL.

Booth, 2008), in this paper we underline the inde- Participants in the EDRec group were assessed
pendence of the two dimensions of weak central with the semi-structured diagnostic interview
coherence: the bias towards superior local processing EATATE based upon the EDE (C. G. Fairburn
and the reduced tendency to global integration. & Cooper, 1993) encompassing lifetime diagnosis
There is some evidence that suggests that the for ED (Part I; Anderluh, Tchanturia, Rabe-
superiority in local processing is a familial trait in Hesketh, & Treasure, 2003). It produces a descrip-
the case of autism spectrum disorders (Baron-Cohen tion of eating disorder symptoms over the life
& Hammer, 1997; Baron-Cohen et al., 2006; Bolte course, which in this study helped to determine
& Poustka, 2006; Happé, Briskman, & Frith, 2001). the status of recovery of our participants. HC
In ED, we have reported that people with current completed the Eating Disorder Examination–
anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa have superior Questionnaire (EDE-Q; C. Fairburn & Beglin,
detail processing and weak global integration 1994) modified to provide a lifetime screening
(Lopez et al., 2008a; Lopez, Tchanturia, Stahl, & for eating disorder symptoms. Additionally,
Treasure, 2008b). Parts II and III of EATATE were administered
The aim of this study was to examine whether to all participants to examine for a history of
weak coherence, both superior detail processing other psychiatric problems.
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

and a weakness for global integration, was present The neuropsychological battery for all the partici-
in people with past history of ED, currently recovered pants consisted of several paradigms assessing aspects
(EDRec). Our hypothesis was that weak coherence of coherence.
would be an endophenotypical trait underpinning Tasks in which detail focused processing would
the development of ED and as such will be inde- benefit performance were: The Embedded Figures
pendent of acute illness state. Test–Form B (Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, & Karp,
1971) and the Unsegmented/Segmented Block Design
Test (Shah & Frith, 1993). These were used as
METHOD AND MATERIALS measures of speed and accuracy in local processing.
In the Embedded Figure Test participants are
The EDRec were 42 women who had a history of asked to find 12 simple shapes embedded in complex
anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa according designs within a time limit of 60 s. In this study, the
to DSM-IV criteria (Diagnostic and Statistical Man- original administration of the task was modified in
ual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition; American order to omit the memory element of the task by
Psychiatric Association, 1994), but had no remaining having the simple shape and the complex design
symptoms: body mass index (BMI) between 19 and displayed simultaneously. Total time and number
26 kg/m2 in the current year, with no binging, purg- of false claims (number of times the participant
ing, food restriction, or excessive exercise according wrongly claims to have found the figure) are
the EATATE interview (semi-structured diagnostic recorded. Shorter times and fewer false claims indicate
interview based upon the Eating Disorder Examina- a stronger local processing. In the Unsegmented/
tion instrument, EDE; see below). The healthy con- Segmented Block Design Test, participants are
trol (HC) group was the same sample as that used in asked to construct replicas of complex designs
our previous studies of coherence (Lopez et al., 2008a, presented on a computer screen, with four or nine
2008b), equivalent in age and intellectual ability to the wooden blocks. In the unsegmented trial designs
EDrec group. The inclusion criteria for healthy con- are presented as a whole whereas in the segmented
trols included a BMI between 19 and 26 kg/m2, no trial there is a gap between the four or nine constituent
psychoactive medicines, and no personal or family parts of the design. A time limit of 60 s for four-
history of ED or any other psychiatric illness. blocks designs and 180 s for nine-block designs are
Participants were recruited from the staff and allowed. Time and errors are recorded. Shorter
student population of the Institute of Psychiatry times indicate better local processing. More impor-
and King’s College of London, from advertise- tantly, people with weaker coherence are thought
ments on the Eating Disorders Unit website, and to show less benefit from segmentation (a differ-
from the local community. ence between the time spent in the unsegmented
The exclusion criteria for all participants were: and segmented trials).
use of psychoactive medication, non-native English Measures in which global processing would
speakers, a history of head injury, neurological benefit performance were: the Rey–Osterrieth
disease, psychosis, or learning disability. Complex Figure Test–recall form (Rey Figure;
Following a complete description of the study, Osterrieth, 1944), the Sentence Completion Task
written informed consent was obtained from all (Happé et al., 2001), and the Homograph Reading
participants. Task (Happé, 1997; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999).
WEAK COHERENCE IN EATING DISORDERS 119

The Rey Figure was used to measure visual spatial 2008b) and are available on request from the first
coherence. In this task, participants are asked to author.
copy and recall (after an interval of 20 minutes) a Participants also completed the National Adult
complex figure. The drawing process was video- Reading Test (NART; 2nd edition) to provide an
recorded. Accuracy of the drawing was scored estimate of premorbid intellectual ability (Nelson &
following Taylor’s (adapted from Osterrieth, 1944) Willison, 1991). As the most common concurrent
scoring procedure based on the drawing of 18 conditions of ED such as depressive, anxiety, and
elements of the figure awarded a 0 to 2 quality score obsessive-compulsive symptoms may persist after
(Spreen & Strauss, 1998). Higher scores mean better recovery (I. C. Gillberg, Rastam, & Gillberg, 1995;
accuracy (range 0–36). Additionally, percentage of Holtkamp, Müller, Heussen, Remschmidt, & Herpertz-
recall, a measure of recall corrected by accuracy in Dahlmann, 2005; von Ranson, Kaye, Weltzin, Rao,
the copy trial (recall/copy × 100) was calculated. & Matsunaga, 1999) and also can influence cogni-
Coherence of drawing style was measured using tive functioning (Castaneda, Tuulio-Henriksson,
two independent indices following Booth’s devel- Marttunen, Suvisaari, & Lönnqvist, 2008; Kuelz,
opment of the task (2006): order of construction Hohagen, & Voderholzer, 2004), symptom levels
index (order in which the elements of the figure were assessed in this study using the Hospital Anxiety
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

were drawn) and style index (continuity of drawing). and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond & Snaith,
A general coherence index was also derived 1983) and the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory–
and consists of a composite score of the previ- Revised (Foa et al., 2002; Foa, Kozak, Salkovskis,
ously mentioned two indices where higher scores Coles, & Amir, 1998).
mean a more coherent drawing style (Booth, 2006;
Lopez et al., 2008a). Organizational strategies as
Procedure
described by Savage and collaborators, which are
linked to executive functioning (Savage et al.,
The procedures for this study were approved by
1999), were examined as an extra measure of
the South London and Maudsley Trust Research
drawing style, in which six main elements of the
Ethics Committee. Weight and height were meas-
figure are scored for continuity of drawing (Sherman
ured on the day of testing. All the neuropsychological
et al., 2006).
measures were administered in the same session
The Sentence Completion Task and the Homo-
order: Rey Figure (copy), Sentences Completion
graph Reading Task were used as verbal coherence
Task, Embedded Figures Test, Homograph Reading
tasks designed to produce conflict between local
Task, Rey Figure (recall), Unsegmented Block
and global processing. In the Sentence Completion
Design, EATATE interview, and Segmented Block
Task participants are asked to finish off 25 sentences,
Design. Questionnaires were completed a week
of which 18 have a conflict between local and
before or after the testing session. The NART test
global completions. The sentences are presented on
was not obtained from the first 15 participants in
a digital recorder. Answers and times are recorded
the HC group and 3 in the EDRec group. We do
by computer software that allows for the determi-
not expect differences in the intellectual ability as
nation of the exact time spent between the end of
they were recruited from the same sources as the rest
the recorded sentence and the beginning of the
of the sample. A total of 3 women in the EDRec
participant’s answer. The number of local comple-
group and 1 in the HC did not return part of the
tions and time taken to find an appropriate ending
self-report measures. Their data were included in the
to the sentence are scored. Higher number of
analyses of the main outcomes measures.
local completions and longer hesitation times are
To ensure reliability in the scoring of neuropsy-
considered as indicators of either local processing
chological measures, some measures were video- or
bias or difficulties in verbal global processing. In
tape-recorded (Rey Figure and verbal tasks), and a
the Homograph Reading Task, participants are
proportion (30%) of them were scored in parallel
asked to read aloud 16 sentences that contain four
by a researcher blind to the condition of participants.
different homographs (e.g., tear, bow). In order to
There were good intraclass correlation coefficients
read them correctly, the context of the sentence
indicating high interrater agreement (all higher
should be considered. The task is tape-recorded,
than .89).
and the number of correct initial pronunciations
is scored. The number of errors is a marker of a
difficulty in verbal global processing. Data analysis
A full description of these tests can be found in
our previous studies of central coherence in people Sample size power calculation was conducted
with an active eating disorder (Lopez et al., 2008a, using NQuery 4.0 Advisor program (Elashoff,
120 LOPEZ ET AL.

2000) based on a pilot study for this project. It was RESULTS


found that a sample size of 42 participants in each
group would have an 80% power to detect clinical Demographic and clinical characteristics
significant differences (Cohen´s d = 0.60 in the
Embedded Figures Test, or higher) between groups The results of demographic and clinical character-
on the neuropsychological tests with a .05 two- istics are shown in Table 1.
tailed significance level. EDRec group was comparable to the HC in age,
Independent sample t tests were used for group years of education, and estimated intellectual ability
comparisons, and means (M) and standard devia- (all p > .05). Although they were recovered for a
tions (SD) are reported. For those measures without considerable time (M = 6.4 years, SD = 5.75), their
a normal distribution of the data, nonparametric BMI was still lower than that of the HC group,
Mann–Whitney U tests (MWU) were used, and t(82) = 0.21, p = .03.
medians (Mdn) and 25% and 75% quartiles (Q) The EDRec group consisted of 35 women who
are reported. To explore the relationship between had a history of anorexia nervosa (AN; 83.3%) and
neuropsychological, demographic, and clinical vari- 7 who had bulimia nervosa (BN) as their main life-
ables, Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) was used time diagnosis. A total of 12 (28.6%) had AN with
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

for normally distributed data, and Spearman’s rank bulimic symptoms (binge and purge); 9 (21.4%)
(rs) correlation was used otherwise. Cohen’s d was had purging AN (without binges); 14 (33.3%) had
calculated to obtain the effect size for each variable restricting AN; 5 (16.6%) had purging BN; and 2
using an effect size calculator (Wilson, 2001). For (4.8%) had nonpurging BN. A total of 37 out of
those variables with non-normally distributed data, the 42 EDrec (88.1%) had a period of AN at some
d was calculated by transforming the effect-size cor- point in their lives. The mean age of onset was
16.1 years (SD = 3.9), and their mean duration of
relation r = Z (from Rosenthal, 1991) in d using
N
illness was 6.1 years (SD = 4.57).
an effect size calculator. The EDRec group had higher levels of anxiety,
Analysis of covariance was carried out to evaluate depression, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms
any possible confounding effect of the demographic (see Table 1).
and clinical variables on differences between groups
in neuropsychological performance.
Two-tailed tests were used throughout. The Neuropsychological function
nominal significance level was chosen to be 5%.
Hochberg’s improved Bonferroni correction for The results from tasks thought to benefit from
multiple testing was applied on the main outcome detail processing are displayed in Table 2. These
measures (Hochberg, 1988). All analyses were carried results show that EDRec women excelled in the
out using SPSS 15.0. Embedded Figures Test (all p < .05). However,

TABLE 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

N Test statistic

EDRec HC EDRec HC group t testa MWb p-value Effect size (d)

Age (years)b 42 42 25.00 (21.0–31.0) 26.00 (21.6–29.0) na 858.5 .833 0.04


Number years of educationb 41 42 16 (14–17.5) 17 (14.75–19) na 432.0 .123 0.32
Estimated intellectual abilitiesa 39 27 113.6 (5.3) 112.2 (5.4) –1.08 na .281 0.26
BMI (kg/m2)a 42 42 20.9 (2.2) 21.9 (2.7) 0.211 na .032 0.41
HADS Anxietya 41 41 9.9 (3.0) 6.0 (3.2) –5.67 na <.001c 1.26
HADS Depressionb 41 41 9.0 (7–9) 2.0 (1–3) na 52.5 <.001c 2.5
OCI-Rb 39 42 16 (9–25) 6.5 (3–10) na 307.5 <.001c 1.25

Note. Anxiety, depression, and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) symptoms are measured in terms of level of severity, and they do
not indicate a clinical diagnosis. EDRec = eating disorders, currently recovered. HC=healthy controls. MW = Mann–Whitney. BMI =
body mass index. HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. OCI-R = Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised.
at test statistics for t test pairwise comparisons for data normally distributed, mean values displayed (standard deviations in

parentheses) (na = not applicable).


b
Test statistics for Mann–Whitney U for data not normally distributed, median values displayed with upper and lower quartiles (na =
not applicable).
c
Results that remained significant (p < .05) after Hochberg correction for multiple testing (Hochberg, 1988).
WEAK COHERENCE IN EATING DISORDERS 121

TABLE 2
Neuropsychological tasks benefited by detail focused processing

Test statistic
EDRec group HC group
(N = 42) (N = 42) t testa MWb p-value Effect size (d)
b c
EFT total time taken (medians) 6.7 (3.7–10.2) 12.2 (7.9–15.3) na 419.0 <.001 1.01
EFT number time out failuresb 0 (0–1) 1 (0–2) na 560.0 .002c 0.72
EFT false claimsb 1 (0–2) 2 (0–3) na 647.5 .030 0.49
Unsegmented Block Designb 47.3 (38.1–66.0) 44.9 (32.3–62.7) na 875.0 .950 0.01
Segmented Block Designb 28.9 (24.7–33.1) 27.7 (25.5–31.5) na 826.5 .620 0.11
Benefit from segmentation (%)a 38.3 (14.9) 40.9 (16.3) .74 na .459 0.17

Note. EDRec = eating disorders, currently recovered. HC = healthy controls. MW = Mann–Whitney. EFT = Embedded Figures Test.
a
t test statistics for t test pairwise comparisons for data normally distributed, mean values displayed (standard deviations in parentheses;
df = 82) (na = not applicable).
b
U test statistics for Mann–Whitney U for data not normally distributed, median values displayed with 25 and 75 percentiles (na = not
applicable).
cResults that remained significant (p < .05) after Hochberg correction for multiple testing (Hochberg, 1988).
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

TABLE 3
Neuropsychological tasks benefited by global integration

Test statistic
EDRec group HC group
(N = 42) (N = 42) t testa MWb p-value Effect size (d)

Rey Figure
Ordera 2.2 (0.7) 2.5 (0.4) 2.01 na .048 0.53
Stylea 1.4 (0.5) 1.7 (0.3) 3.30 na .002 c 0.73
CC indexa 1.4 (0.4) 1.6 (0.3) 3.03 na < .001 c 0.57
SCT
Local processing scoreb 1.5 (1–3) 0 (0–1) na 391.0 < .001 c 1.15
Number of local completions b 0 (0–0) 0 (0–0) na 819.0 .370 0.18
HRT total scoreb 15 (14–15) 15 (14–16) na 691.5 .075 0.39

Note. CC = Coherence index. EDRec = eating disorders, currently recovered. HC = healthy controls. MW = Mann–Whitney. SCT =
Sentence Completion Task. HRT = Homograph Reading Task.
a
t test statistics for t test pairwise comparisons for data normally distributed (df = 82), mean values displayed (standard deviations in
parentheses) (na = not applicable).
b
U test statistics for Mann–Whitney U for data not normally distributed, median values displayed with 25 and 75 percentiles (na = not
applicable).
c
Results that remained significant (p < .05) after Hochberg correction for multiple testing (Hochberg, 1988).

no differences were found in both the fused and at both visuospatial and verbal levels than did the
segmented parts of the Block Design Test in terms HC group. These results are shown in Table 3.
of time spent on the task as in the percentage of Additional aspects of the cognitive processing
advantage when designs were segmented. were obtained from the Rey Figure Test. The
In terms of global processing, the EDRec group EDRec group obtained poorer scores in copy,
showed poorer global integration in the drawing t(82) = 6.9, p < .001, and recall accuracy, t(82) = 3.7,
process of the Rey Figure than did a HC group, p < .001, than did the HC group. However, no
with low scores on the coherence indices (style, significant differences were found in percentage of
order of construction, and coherence index). In the recall (how much they recall from their first drawing)
verbal domain, the EDRec group took longer to nor in organizational strategy.
produce coherent sentences in the Sentence Com- Most of the significant results reported above
pletion Task. In the Homograph Reading Task the maintained their formal significance after correc-
EDRec group made more errors but this did not tion for multiple testing (see Tables 2 and 3).
reach formal levels of significance (p = .07). Explorative correlation analyses revealed a large
Together these findings indicate that the EDRec positive relationship between scores in the accuracy
groups had more difficulties in global integration copy and recall in both groups (r = .61 in the HC
122 LOPEZ ET AL.

and r = .53 in the EDRec group, p < .001) and a DISCUSSION


moderate correlation between coherence index and
recall accuracy (r = .37 in the HC and r = .41 in the The aim of this study was to examine the concept
EDRec group, p < .05). of central coherence in people with a lifetime
diagnosis of ED, currently healthy (EDRec). Our
A comparison between healthy controls hypothesis, which was that women with EDRec
and people with a lifetime history of would present a performance profile
anorexia nervosa with enhanced detail processing and weak global
coherence, was mainly confirmed.
When only individuals who had anorexia as
EDRec women performed extremely well in a
the main past diagnosis were considered, the signif-
task that benefited from enhanced detail function—
icant difference seen in the whole group in terms of
namely, the Embedded Figure Test—where they
order index of Rey Figure fell to trend levels only.
were fast and showed the error-free style described
However, the difference in the Homograph Reading
also in acute AN (Lopez et al., 2008a). Their func-
Task became significant (p < .05) with an increased
tioning in the Block Design Test, which is thought
effect size of d = 0.47.
to benefit from piecemeal processing, was compa-
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

rable to that of HC. On the other hand, the recov-


Relationship between demographic, ered group displayed weaker performance in tasks
comorbid, and clinical variables and that require a global strategy (Rey Figure, Sentence
neuropsychological performance Completion Task, and—although less impaired—
the Homograph Reading Task). These results
Correlation analyses were performed separately together raise the possibility that our secondary
for each group to explore potential confounder hypothesis, which was that weak coherence might
variables on neuropsychological testing. Only sig- be an endophenotype for ED, is correct.
nificant results in each group are described. In the Importantly, indices of weak coherence (strengths
EDRec group those who were recovered for a in detail processing and weak integration) were
longer time spent more time in the Unsegmented not associated with comorbid symptoms or BMI
Block Design trial (rs = .44, p = .003). Intellectual in EDRec group. In the acute illness, anxiety
ability as measured by NART was negatively was associated with measures of coherence in
associated with accuracy in the Rey Figure copy bulimia nervosa (Lopez et al., 2008b). The results
trial (r = .32, p=.05). Older people performed less presented here suggest that weak coherence is
well in accuracy recall in the Rey Figure (r = .30, p relatively independent from transitory comorbid
= .05) and in the Segmented Block Design (rs = conditions.
.32, p = .04). Finally, those with higher scores in There are some, albeit minor, differences
obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCI-R) scored between the neuropsychological performance of
lower in copy accuracy of the Rey Figure (rs = .42, these recovered women and those with acute ED
p < .01). described in our previous work (Lopez et al.,
In the HC group, anxiety was negatively corre- 2008a, 2008b). In general, the groups at normal
lated with poorer organizational strategies in the weight (bulimia nervosa and recovered) had an
Rey Figure (rs = .33, p = .03) and longer times in intermediate profile between those with an acute
the Segmented Block Design Test (rs = .36, p = .02). anorexia and HC. The recovered group was
The number of errors in the Embedded Figures even more enhanced in detail processing than
Test was linked with lower intellectual ability and those with active anorexia nervosa (e.g., the
years of education (rs =.42 and .45, respectively, all times on the Embedded Figures Test are
p < .03). Finally, in HC, BMI was positively associ- shorter). In addition, the EDRec group displays
ated with the time taken in the Unsegmented Block better visual coherence (higher coherence scores
Design Test (rs =.34, p < .03). on the Rey Figure Test) than those with acute
Correlation analyses involving verbal coherence anorexia nervosa. This evidence suggests that
tasks were not performed due to the limited vari- weight loss accentuates the difficulty in global
ance of scores on these verbal central coherence integration and may weaken efficient detail
tasks. processing.
Analyses of covariance to examine potential Regarding other aspects of cognitive functioning
confounder effects of relevant demographic, measured by these tasks it was found that organi-
comorbid, and clinical differences between groups zational strategy in the Rey Figure, which is
in neuropsychological performance were carried thought to be linked to executive functioning,
out, and no effects were found. was similar to that of controls. This finding adds
WEAK COHERENCE IN EATING DISORDERS 123

support to the proposed relative independence of has produced some interesting findings. Longitudinal
central coherence from executive functions (Happé studies would add to the interpretation of these
& Booth, 2008). The EDRec group had lower studies. Finally, ceiling effects were observed in
scores in accuracy in copy and recall trials of the the Homograph Reading Task, which might preclude
Rey Figure than did healthy controls, although the possibility of finding differences between the
both groups were comparable in terms of the recovered and control groups.
memory component of the task (percentage of recall). We propose that weak coherence may trigger
We are uncertain as to why the recovered group core behavioral and cognitive traits of ED such as
had lower accuracy in the Rey Figure than both perfectionism, fear of mistakes, and change. Also
HC and those with acute anorexia nervosa (Lopez the relative difficulty in integrative processing may
et al., 2008a). It is possible that the high-detail explain in part the neglect of long-term health
focus may interfere with the task. Replication of consequences. These findings therefore may have
this study may help to clarify this effect. The lack implications for clinical practice and future research.
of difference in percentage of recall between Integrating these observations into treatment with
EDRec and HC groups suggests that the loss of motivationally enhanced neuropsychological feed-
memory was not totally mediated by central coher- back (Lopez, Roberts, Tchanturia, & Treasure,
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

ence indices in the EDRec as seen in anorexia ner- in press; Treasure et al., 2007) or with cognitive
vosa (Lopez et al., 2008a). remediation therapy (CRT) to enhance global
Cross-sectional studies that have examined the processing strategies (Baldock & Tchanturia, 2007;
basic neuropsychological performance of long-term Tchanturia, Davies, & Campbell, in press) may
recovered eating disorder samples have demon- improve metacognitive skills and moderate the bias
strated that some neuropsychological anomalies towards detail-focused processing of information.
found in the active phase of anorexia are present Further study to clarify the relationship between
in those recovered whereas other studies suggest weak central coherence, eating disorder psychopa-
that certain functions are restored. For instance, thology, recovery, and the interplay with other cog-
aspects of set-shifting difficulties (Pendleton Jones, nitive abnormalities in ED is needed. For example,
Duncan, Brouwers, & Mirsky, 1991; Roberts, as recovered people have also been shown to have
Tchanturia, Stahl, Southgate, & Treasure, 2007; difficulties with set-shifting (Roberts et al., 2007;
Tchanturia et al., 2004; Tchanturia, Morris, Surguladze, Tchanturia et al., 2004) it is possible that the con-
& Treasure, 2002), immediate memory, and perform- junction between weak central coherence and diffi-
ance in Morse finger-tapping task (Bosanac et al., culties in set-shifting together may make recovery
2007) seem to remain impaired in recovery. In more difficult.
terms of restored functions, Tchanturia et al. As a final point, this study adds to the evidence
(2007) found no deficits in decision making in supporting a common endophenotype between
recovered women as opposed to those with acute autism spectrum disorders and ED. These findings
anorexia. Indeed, decision-making abilities has give some empirical endorsement to the suggestion
been described as a good predictor of weight recovery that ED form a diagnostic cluster in association
(Cavedini et al., 2006). Bosanac et al. (2007) found with obsessive-compulsive disorders (Hollander,
normal attention in recovered anorexia. Finally, Kim, Khanna, & Pallanti, 2007). Obsessive-
Pendleton Jones et al. (1991) did not find statistical compulsive personality traits were found to contribute
differences between the performance of a recov- to poor global processing in ED (Tokley &
ered and a healthy control group on tasks measur- Kemps, 2007). On the other hand, people with
ing vigilance, memory, executive functions, and obsessive-compulsive disorders have also been
verbal and visuospatial abilities. found to have a bias towards detail processing
This study has several limitations. The cross- of information in neuropsychological testing as
sectional design is somewhat unsatisfactory in that did women with anorexia nervosa (Savage et al.,
it is questionable whether those who are able to 1999; Sherman et al., 2006). In autism spectrum
achieve recovery are representative of the eating disorders weak coherence is a potential endophe-
disorder population as a whole or whether they are notype as several studies have found that the first-
an atypical subgroup, perhaps less severe, of the degree relatives are better in tasks that require
eating disorder population. This, however, would a detail strategy (Baron-Cohen & Hammer,
err on the side of reducing any trait effect. How- 1997; Baron-Cohen et al., 2006; Bolte & Poustka,
ever, the inclusion of a recovered group has been 2006; Happé et al., 2001). Also, one study showed
widely employed in the eating disorder literature to difficulties in global processing in parents of peo-
examine for trait versus state markers (Bailer et al., ple with these disorders as measured by the Sen-
2007; Wagner et al., 2008; Wagner et al., 2007) and tence Completion Test (Happé et al., 2001).
124 LOPEZ ET AL.

Collaborative research work would help to clarify disorders with a focus on young adults. Journal of
this hypothesis. Affective Disorders, 106, 1–27.
Cavedini, P., Zorzi, C., Bassi, T., Gorini, A., Baraldi, C.,
In summary, the results of this study support the
Ubbiali, A., et al. (2006). Decision-making functioning
potential trait hypothesis of weak coherence in as a predictor of treatment outcome in anorexia
ED, as opposed to a state, since this characteristic nervosa. Psychiatry Research, 145, 179–187.
was present in recovered people. Research into Elashoff, J. (2000). nQuery Advisor (Version 4.0)
unaffected family members of people with ED and [Computer software]. Los Angeles: Statistical Solu-
tions Ltd.
genetics are needed to explore the endophenotype
Fairburn, C., & Beglin, S. (1994). Assessment of eating
hypothesis further. disorders: Interview or self-report questionnaire? Inter-
national Journal of Eating Disorders, 16, 363–370.
Original manuscript received 8 October 2007 Fairburn, C. G., & Cooper, Z. (1993). The eating disor-
Revised manuscript accepted 3 March 2008 der examination. In C. G. Fairburn & G. T. Wilson
First published online 4 June 2008 (Eds.), Binge eating: Nature, assessment and treatment
(12th ed., pp. 317–331). New York: Guilford Press.
Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Langner, R., Leiberg, S.,
REFERENCES Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., et al. (2002). The Obsessive-
Compulsive Inventory: Development and validation
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14,
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). 485–496.
Washington, DC: Author. Foa, E. B., Kozak, M. J., Salkovskis, P. M., Coles,
Anderluh, M., Tchanturia, K., Rabe-Hesketh, S., & M. E., & Amir, N. (1998). The validation of a new
Treasure, J. (2003). Childhood obsessive-compulsive obsessive-compulsive disorder scale: The Obsessive-
personality traits in adult women with eating dis- Compulsive Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 10,
orders: Defining a broader eating disorder 206–214.
phenotype. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford,
242–247. UK: Basil Blackwell.
Bailer, U. F., Frank, G. K., Henry, S. E., Price, J. C., Gillberg, I., Rastam, M., & Gillberg, C. (1995). Ano-
Meltzer, C. C., Becker, C., et al. (2007). Serotonin rexia nervosa 6 years after onset: Part I. Personality
transporter binding after recovery from eating disor- disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 36, 61–69.
ders. Psychopharmacology, 195, 315–324. Gillberg, I., Rastam, M., Wentz, E., & Gillberg, C.
Baldock, E., & Tchanturia, K. (2007). Translating labo- (2007). Cognitive and executive functions in anorexia
ratory research into clinical practice: Foundations, nervosa ten years after onset of eating disorder. Jour-
functions and future of cognitive remediation therapy nal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29,
for anorexia nervosa. Therapy, 4, 285–292. 170–178.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997). Parents of Happé, F. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind
children with Asperger syndrome: What is the cogni- in autism: Reading homographs in context. British
tive phenotype? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 1–12.
548–554. Happé, F., & Booth, R. (2008). The power of the posi-
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Chitnis, X., Wheelwright, tive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum
S., Gregory, L., Williams, S. C. R., et al. (2006). disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
fMRI of parents of children with Asperger syndrome: chology, 61, 50–63.
A pilot study. Brain and Cognition, 61, 122–130. Happé, F., Briskman, J., & Frith, U. (2001). Exploring
Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2006). The broader cognitive the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak “central
phenotype of autism in parents: How specific is the coherence” in parents and siblings of children with
tendency for local processing and executive dysfunc- autism: I. Experimental tests. The Journal of Child
tion? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 299–307.
639–645. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence
Booth, R. (2006). Local-global processing and cognitive account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism
style in autism spectrum disorders and typical develop- spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Develop-
ment. London: King’s College London. mental Disorders, 36, 5–25.
Bosanac, P., Kurlender, S., Stojanovska, L., Hallam, K., Hochberg, Y. (1988). A sharper Bonferroni procedure
Norman, T., McGrath, C., et al. (2007). Neuropsy- for multiple test of significance. Biometrika, 75,
chological study of underweight and “weight-recov- 800–802.
ered” anorexia nervosa compared with bulimia Hollander, E., Kim, S., Khanna, S., & Pallanti, S.
nervosa and normal controls. International Journal of (2007). Obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-
Eating Disorders, 40, 613–621. compulsive spectrum disorders: Diagnostic and
Bulik, C. M., Hebebrand, J., Keski-Rahkonen, A., dimensional issues. CNS Spectrums, 12(2, Suppl. 3),
Klump, K. L., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Mazzeo, 5–13.
S. E., et al. (2007). Genetic epidemiology, endophe- Holliday, J., Tchanturia, K., Landau, S., Collier, D. A.,
notypes, and eating disorder classification. Interna- & Treasure, J. (2005). Is impaired set-shifting an
tional Journal of Eating Disorders, 40(Suppl. 3), endophenotype of anorexia nervosa? American Jour-
S52–S60. nal of Psychiatry, 162, 2269–2275.
Castaneda, A. E., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Marttunen, M., Holtkamp, K., Müller, B., Heussen, N., Remschmidt, H.,
Suvisaari, J., & Lönnqvist, J. (2008). A review on & Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2005). Depression, anxiety,
cognitive impairments in depressive and anxiety and obsessionality in long-term recovered patients
WEAK COHERENCE IN EATING DISORDERS 125

with adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa. European Steiger, H., & Bruce, K. R. (2007). Phenotypes,
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 14, 106–110. endophenotypes, and genotypes in bulimia spectrum
Hudson, J. I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H. G., & Kessler, R. C. eating disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 52,
(2007). The prevalence and correlates of eating disor- 220–227.
ders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Tchanturia, K., Davies, H., & Campbell, I. C. (in press).
Biological Psychiatry, 61, 348–358. Cognitive remediation therapy for patients with ano-
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central rexia nervosa: Preliminary findings. Annals of General
coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-func- Psychiatry, 6. Epub ahead of print: doi:10.1186/1744–
tioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: Is 1859X-1186–1114.
local coherence impaired? Cognition, 22, 149–185. Tchanturia, K., Morris, R., Anderluh, M., Collier, D.,
Kuelz, A. K., Hohagen, F., & Voderholzer, U. (2004). Nikolaou, V., & Treasure, J. (2004). Set shifting in
Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive anorexia nervosa: An examination before and after
disorder: A critical review. Biological Psychology, 65, weight gain, in full recovery and relationship to child-
185–236. hood and adult OCPD traits. Journal of Psychiatric
Lopez, C., Roberts, M. E., Tchanturia, K., & Treasure, J. Research, 38, 545–552.
(in press). Using neuropsychological feedback thera- Tchanturia, K., Morris, R., Surguladze, S., & Treasure, J.
peutically in treatment for anorexia nervosa: Two (2002). An examination of perceptual and cognitive
illustrative case reports. European Eating Disorders set shifting tasks in acute anorexia nervosa and
Review. Epub ahead of print: doi: 10.1002/erv.866. following recovery. Journal of Eating and Weight
Downloaded by [University College London] at 00:16 09 August 2011

Lopez, C., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., Booth, R., Disorders, 7, 312–315.
Holliday, J., & Treasure, J. (2008a). An examination Tchanturia, K., Pei-Chi, L., Uher, R., Lawrence, N.,
of the concept of central coherence in women with Treasure, J., & Campbell, I. C. (2007). An investigation
anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating of decision making in anorexia nervosa using the Iowa
Disorders, 41, 143–152. Gambling Task and skin conductance measurements.
Lopez, C., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., & Treasure, J. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,
(2008b). Central coherence in women with bulimia 13, 635–641.
nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41, Tokley, M., & Kemps, E. (2007). Preoccupation with
340–347. detail contributes to poor abstraction in anorexia nervosa.
Nelson, H. E., & Willison, J. R. (1991). The Revised Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29,
National Adult Reading Test–test manual (NART). 734–741.
Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson Publishing Co. Treasure, J. (2007). Getting beneath the phenotype of
Osterrieth, P. (1944). Test of Copying a Complex Figure: anorexia nervosa: The search for viable endopheno-
Contribution to the study of perception and memory. types and genotypes. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry,
Archives de Psychologie, 30, 206–356. 52, 212–209.
Pendleton Jones, B., Duncan, C., Brouwers, P., & Treasure, J., Lopez, C., & Roberts, M. (2007). Endophe-
Mirsky, A. (1991). Cognition in eating disorders. notypes in eating disorders: Moving toward etiologically
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, based diagnosis and treatment focused on pathophys-
13, 711–728. iology. Pediatric Health, 1, 171–181.
Roberts, M. E., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., Southgate, L., & van Hoeken, D., Seidell, J., & Wijbrand, H. (2003).
Treasure, J. (2007). A systematic review and meta- Epidemiology. In J. Treasure, U. Schmidt, & E. van
analysis of set shifting ability in eating disorders. Furth (Eds.), Handbook of eating disorders (2nd ed.,
Psychological Medicine, 37, 1075–1084. pp. 11–34). London: Wiley and Sons Ltd.
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social von Ranson, K., Kaye, W., Weltzin, T., Rao, R., &
research (revised). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Matsunaga, H. (1999). Obsessive-compulsive
Savage, C. R., Baer, L., Keuthen, N., Brown, H. D., disorder symptoms before and after recovery from
Rauch, S. L., & Jenike, M. A. (1999). Organizational bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry,
strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in 156, 1703–1708.
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, Wagner, A., Aizenstein, H., Mazurkewicz, L., Fudge, J.,
45, 905–916. Frank, G. K., Putnam, K., et al. (2008). Altered
Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals insula response to taste stimuli in individuals recovered
show superior performance on the block design task? from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsy-
The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, chopharmacology, 33, 513–523.
1351–1364. Wagner, A., Aizenstein, H., Venkatraman, V. K.,
Sherman, B. J., Savage, C. R., Eddy, K. T., Blais, M. A., Fudge, J., May, J. C., Mazurkewicz, L., et al. (2007).
Deckersbach, T., Jackson, S. C., et al. (2006). Altered reward processing in women recovered from
Strategic memory in adults with anorexia nervosa: Are anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry,
there similarities to obsessive compulsive spectrum 164, 1842–1849.
disorders? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39, Wilson, D. B. (2001). Effect size determination program
468–476. [Computer software]. College Park, MD: University
Southgate, L., Tchanturia, K., & Treasure, J. (in press). of Maryland.
Information processing bias in anorexia nervosa. Witkin, H., Oltman, P., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. (1971). A
Psychiatry Research. Epub ahead of print: doi: manual for the Embedded Figure Test. Palo Alto, CA:
10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.017. Consulting Psychologists Press.
Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neu- Zigmond, A., & Snaith, R. (1983). The hospital anxiety
ropsychological tests: Administration, norms and com- and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
mentary (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. 67, 361–370.

You might also like