You are on page 1of 9

MIND, BRAIN, AND EDUCATION

Educational Neuromyths
Among Teachers in Latin
America
Ezequiel Gleichgerrcht1 , Benjamin Lira Luttges2 , Florencia Salvarezza3 , and Anna Lucia Campos2

ABSTRACT— Neuroscientific knowledge has undeniably strategies (Sigman, Pena, Goldin, & Ribeiro, 2014). Building
gained interest among educators worldwide. However, bridges between neuroscience and education is naturally
not all “brain facts” believed by teachers are supported by an ambitious process (Ansari, Coch, & De Smedt, 2011;
science. This study sought to evaluate the belief in these Devonshire & Dommett, 2010), filled with challenges of
so-called neuromyths among 3,451 Latin American teachers. varying nature. One such challenge involves the populariza-
We found that, consistent with prior research among teach- tion of misleading information, folk psychology, and general
ers in other geographic areas, teachers in Latin America misconceptions not only about the brain in general (e.g., “We
hold major misconceptions about neuroscience, especially only use 10% of our brains”), but also relating specifically to
as it relates to factual information about its structure and education (e.g., “This lesson is designed for the visual learn-
functioning. Differences across South American nations ers”) (Dekker, Lee, Howard-Jones, & Jolles, 2012; Pasquinelli,
were observed with moderate and slight effect sizes for gen- 2012). The Organization for Economic Co-operation and
eral knowledge of the brain and neuromyths, respectively. Development (OECD) took particular interest in this issue
Teachers working in higher education had slightly superior (2002) and, coining the term neuromyth to refer to any
performance. Teachers at all levels who reported knowing misconception resulting from intentional (e.g., commercial
more about the brain, however, were more likely to believe purposes) or unintentional (e.g., miscommunication) distor-
invalid assertions known as neuromyths. These results shed tion of neuroscientific findings to argue for the incorporation
light on trends in the field of education, hoping to encourage of brain research into classrooms and schools in general.
the development of strategies aimed at correcting the use of There are some fundamental concerns regarding the
science as it relates to education. The implications regarding proliferation of these so-called neuromyths. First, as it
teaching practice, policy, and teacher training are discussed. is usually the case with pseudoscience (e.g., Lilienfeld,
Ammirati, & David, 2012), neuromyths tend to be attractive
for the general public because they make sense and often
For the past two decades, we have witnessed an ever-growing rely on intuitive, friendly explanations to everyday issues
interest in neuroscience and knowledge about the brain, (Howard-Jones, 2014). For instance, Herculano-Houzel
both in and outside the laboratory. Education has not been (2002) demonstrated high rates of belief in popular state-
an exception to this phenomenon, and after centuries of ments about the brain that were scientifically unfounded
parallel work between (neuro)scientists and educators, we among the general population of Brazil. Not surprisingly,
now recognize the need for them to converge in an attempt these myths tend to become especially captivating for the
to design better, more efficient teaching and learning press, and get extensive attention from the media (Beck,
2010), consequently propagating at a notorious and concern-
ing rate (Goswami, 2006; Lilienfeld et al., 2012). The mass
media’s use of neuroscientific explanations also feeds on the
1 Department of Neurology, Medical University of South Carolina
2 Centro
public’s tendency to accept any fact or argument—despite
Iberoamericano de Neurociencia, Educación y Desarrollo
Humano—Cerebrum its possible lack of validity—if it is supposedly backed up
3 Institute of Cognitive Neurology by brain research (Weisberg, Keil, Goodstein, Rawson, &
Address correspondence to Dr. Ezequiel Gleichgerrcht, Department of
Gray, 2008), especially if accompanied by brain imaging
Neurology, Medical University of South Carolina, 96 Jonathan Lucas (McCabe & Castel, 2008). Second, there is high risk for
St. Suite CSB 301, Charleston, SC 29425; e-mail: gleichge@musc.edu. some of these myths to propagate among teachers, and even

170 © 2015 International Mind, Brain, and Education Society and Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Volume 9—Number 3
Ezequiel Gleichgerrcht et al.

further down from educators to learners. This may occur and training opportunities have emerged in educational neu-
both in the context of formal content (e.g., science lectures) roscience. We further sought to explore potential differences
and informally (e.g., “Let’s try to use more than 10% of our between countries in the region and compare trends with
brain for this course!”; “You should be more right-brained results from European teachers based prior research. More-
for this class”). Third, there is also a high risk for misuse over, we were interested in evaluating whether reading popu-
of financial, human, infrastructural, and time resources on lar science and primary scientific literature, as well as having
behalf of school administrators and teachers in the hopes of had access to neuroscientific material, was associated with
incorporating brain research in their everyday school activ- general knowledge about the brain and belief in neuromyths.
ities (Sylvan & Christodoulou, 2010). For instance, Dekker
et al. (2012) recently found that up to 98% of surveyed U.K.
teachers had engaged in programs on “learning styles,” 82% MATERIALS AND METHODS
on “brain gym,” and 71% on multiple intelligences, among
other figures of major concern. This reveals that while Participants
schools may be actively trying to build on knowledge gen- Participants were teachers of all levels currently working
erated from research in educational neuroscience, they are in Latin America. The total sample included 3,451 teachers
prone to becoming victims of pseudoscientific trends. This from Argentina (n = 551), Chile (n = 598), Peru (n = 2,222),
is especially important because reports have demonstrated and other Latin American countries (n = 80, including
that there is a widespread openness among educators, inter- Mexico, Nicaragua, Colombia, and Uruguay).
nationally, to learn about neurosciences and its potential
use in the classroom (Pickering & Howard-Jones, 2007). Procedure
Latin America is no exception to this eagerness of teach- Potential respondents were recruited at various events
ers to gain knowledge about the brain, as evidenced by an related to education (e.g., book fair, pedagogy training
increasing number of educational neuroscience initiatives, sessions, etc.), by word of mouth, and via email invita-
postgraduate courses, and conferences. The reality of Latin tions to databases of people who had previously enquired
America is, nonetheless, further complicated by several about information/courses on neuroscience and edu-
factors. On the one hand, the socioeconomic context of cation. Because of the diversity of outlets employed to
developing countries in the region raises questions as to recruit participants, some responses were delivered in
whether we should be focusing on building bridges between pen and paper form, and others completed on an online
neuroscience and education when there are some basic platform exclusively designed for the purposes of this
fundamental needs (e.g., infrastructure) that are still far study. A priori analyses revealed no significant differences
from being met for many schools throughout the continent. between teachers completing the survey in paper (56%)
On the other hand is the issue of language. A vast majority and those completing it online in terms of (a) response
of teachers speak limited or no English, and are thus unable rates (online: Argentina = 82%, Chile = 85%, Perú = 75%,
to easily access information about learning and the brain. and Others = 81%; paper: Argentina = 98%, Chile = 98%,
In fact, this further exacerbates the problem of pseudosci- Perú = 99%, and Others = n/a; χ2 = 1.05, df = 2, p = .59),
entific programs aimed at training teachers in the so-called as well as (b) in terms of responses to the questionnaire,
“latest trends in neuroscience,” because they are usually reason why all participants were pooled together for group
organized locally in their native language and translated by analysis. Mean completion time for both modalities was
nonexperts, many with ulterior motives. approximately 14 min.
Recently, a high prevalence of belief in neuromyths
has been documented among British, Dutch, Turkish,
Greek, and Chinese primary and secondary school teachers Measures
(Dekker et al., 2012; Howard-Jones, 2014). Their results Respondents initially gave their informed consent and
further revealed the worrying situation of neuroscientific provided demographic and professional background infor-
misconceptions among educators, especially as it relates to mation, including (a) age, (b) sex, (c) years of teaching
commercialized programs such as “brain gym” or “learning experience, (d) levels at which they teach (i.e., early child-
styles.” To the best of our knowledge, no previous study has hood, elementary school, high school, and/or higher
investigated the belief in neuromyths throughout the Latin education), (e) whether they read general related “pop
American region. Here, we sought to determine the preva- science” articles (yes/no), (f ) whether they read primary
lence of neuromyths in a large sample of teachers from Latin scientific literature (yes/no), and (g) whether they had ever
America. For the purposes of this study, we focused pri- had access to information about learning and the brain.
marily on teachers from Argentina, Chile, and Peru, which Participants then completed the survey. We used a
appear to be the countries in the region where most activities Spanish translation of the survey originally introduced by

Volume 9—Number 3 171


Educational Neuromyths Among Latin American Teachers

Dekker et al. (2012), which presents 32 statements about


brain and learning/education. Unaware to participants, 20
of these items were general assertions about the brain and 12
of these statements constituted neuromyths in education.1
In order to avoid response biases based on the order of
items, different versions with varying order of statements
were administered, at random, across participants. For
each item, participants were asked to determine whether
the statement was correct or incorrect. A third option,
do not know, was also available. For each participant, we
determined (a) percentage of correct responses to general
brain assertions (i.e. identifying valid assertions as correct
and invalid assertions as incorrect), (b) percentage of belief
in neuromyths (i.e. identifying neuromyths as true), and
(c) percentage of statements marked as do not know.

Data Analysis
Differences between countries were analyzed using analysis
of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni post hoc compar-
isons. Contingency coefficients were computed to compare
Fig. 1. Mean (SEM) percent correct responses to general state-
categorical variables. The α value for all statistical tests was
ments about the brain (a) across teachers in different countries; (b)
set at 0.05, two-tailed, but given the large sample size of across teachers working in different levels (EC = early childhood;
participants in the study, differences are shown as Cohen’s ES = elementary school; HS = high school; HE = higher education);
d scores because the size of the effect when comparing and (c) between teachers who stated they have read general science
groups is more important than the degree of significance texts (GST), primary scientific literature (PSL), or neuroscientific
per se. Effect sizes for correlations are reported as R2 . material (NM), and those who did not.
When interpreting results, we followed Cohen’s (1992)
classification of effect sizes as small/slightly (d = .10–.39), difference, F(3,3447 ) = 29.0, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.32, was
medium/moderately (d = .40–.69), or large/substantially found across countries for their general knowledge about
(d > .70). Effect sizes for analyses of categorical data are the brain, with Peruvian respondents exhibiting the lowest
expressed in terms of Cramér’s V score (Cramér, 1999). scores compared to all other countries (p < .001). Post hoc
comparison of performance between all other countries was
not significant (all p > .05). As shown in Figure 1b, a signif-
RESULTS
icant difference was also found between teachers working
Sociodemographic and Professional Profile at different levels, F(3,3447 ) = 22.3, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.28.
Specifically, we found that professors in higher education
Mean (SD) age of teachers participating in this study was
had higher rates of correct responses to general statements
43.4 (10.5) years and they had 17.8 (10.5) years of experience
on average. A total of 24.1% worked at the early childhood than all other education levels (all p < .001), while the latter
level, 54.8% in elementary school, 22.1% in high school, and did not differ significantly from one another (all p > .05).
8.4% in higher education. Of these, 9.4% had experience Teachers who stated they read general texts about science,
in more than one level of education. A total of 60.9% of t(2892 ) = 8.94, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.33, and primary scien-
teachers reported reading general articles about science and tific literature, t(2916 ) = 6.71, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.25, per-
71% stated they read primary scientific articles. A total of formed significantly better than those who did not frequently
55% also reported having had previous access to information engage in such readings. Similarly, teachers who had previ-
about learning and the brain. ously had access to material about learning and the brain
exhibited significantly higher rates of correct responses
t(2755 ) = 11.5, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.44, than participants
General Knowledge About the Brain who were naïve to neuroscientific material (Figure 1c). Sig-
For the overall sample, we found an average rate of 66.7% nificant associations between percent of correct responses
(SD = 13.5%) correct responses to general statements about and age, r = −.09, p < .001, R2 = .01, and years of experience
the brain. Of these statements, 12.5% (SD = 13.2%) were as a teacher, r = −.07, p < .001, R2 = .005, had completely
marked as do not know. As shown in Figure 1a, a significant negligible correlation coefficients.

172 Volume 9—Number 3


Ezequiel Gleichgerrcht et al.

Table 1
Percent Correct Answers to General Statements About the Brain Sorted by Overall Increasing Performance

Overall Peru Argentina Chile Other χ2 Cramér’s V

3. Boys have bigger brains than girls 14.1 14.8 15.1 11 12.5 6.02 0.04
23. Circadian rhythms (“body clock”) shift during 36.6 31 49.4 45.3 38.3 88.4*** 0.16
adolescence, causing pupils to be more tired during the
first lessons of the school day
6. When a brain region is damaged other parts of the brain 37.9 30.9 48.6 50.3 66.2 140.2*** 0.2
can take up its function
10. The brains of boys and girls develop at the same rate 38.7 38.7 49 31.3 23.8 60.4*** 0.09
8. The left and right hemispheres of the brain always work 45.1 51 39.6 28.3 46.2 106.0*** 0.18
together
14. Learning is not due to the addition of new cells to the 46.5 40 52.3 63.4 60 119*** 0.19
brain
26. Extended rehearsal of some mental processes can change 51.2 51 47.7 55 51.2 81.1*** 0.11
the shape and structure of some parts of the brain
24. Regular drinking of caffeinated drinks reduces alertness 55.7 47 70.4 71.6 76.2 191.5*** 0.24
18. Normal development of the human brain involves the 61.8 59 65.5 67.2 73.8 22.6*** 0.08
birth and death of brain cells
17. Academic achievement can be affected by skipping 64.3 60.9 65.7 74.9 70 41.9*** 0.11
breakfast
29. Production of new connections in the brain can continue 66.2 61.6 78.8 71.9 81.2 81.9*** 0.15
into old age
20. Vigorous exercise can improve mental function 68.6 73.5 58.4 61.4 58.8 69.2*** 0.14
13. Information is stored in the brain in a network of cells 75.8 77.8 73.1 70.2 80 17.7** 0.07
distributed throughout the brain
16. Learning occurs through modification of the brains’ 76.5 76.2 73.7 78.4 88.8 10.5* 0.06
neural connections
11. Brain development has finished by the time children 82.4 82.6 85.5 78.3 87.5 12.2** 0.06
reach secondary school
19. Mental capacity is hereditary and cannot be changed by 83.1 78.5 90.4 92.1 92.5 94.2*** 0.17
the environment or experience
31. There are sensitive periods in childhood when it is easier 89.7 89.7 84.2 94 92.5 30.4*** 0.09
to learn things
1. We use our brains 24 hr a day 92.5 91.1 94.9 94.6 97.5 17.5** 0.07
32. When we sleep, the brain shuts down 93.2 90.6 97.1 98.5 98.8 66.7*** 0.14
27. Individual learners show preferences for the mode in 96.1 96.1 94 97.7 97.5 10.6* 0.06
which they receive information (e.g., visual, auditory,
kinesthetic)

Note: Correct and incorrect statements were balanced. Higher values indicate better performance (i.e., correct statements marked as correct and incorrect statements
marked as incorrect). Item order numbers correspond to original list by Dekker et al. (2012).
*p = .05. **p = .01. ***p < .001.

Table 1 summarizes the overall and by-country percent with the worst performance. Besides an expected degree of
of correct answers for each general statement. The general variability, no striking differences were found in the patterns
observation was that performance on specific facts about of response across the countries.
brain anatomy and functioning (i.e., items 3, 6, 8, 10, 14, 23)
was generally poor, with less than 50% of the teachers giving
Neuromyths
the correct answer. On the contrary, teachers obtained
For the overall sample, we found that teachers failed to iden-
much higher scores on items related with everyday effects tify 50.7% (SD = 13.8%) of neuromyths on average, 11.3%
likely to be observable by educators, including exercise, (SD = 13.4%) of which were marked as do not know. As
breakfast, and learning/development, among others. The shown in Figure 2a, a significant difference, F(3,3447 ) = 76.4,
most remarkable differences between the answers of teach- p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.52, was found across countries for
ers from different countries, as revealed by effect sizes, were the number of neuromyths identified as valid, with Peruvian
on assertions about brain damage (item 6), neurogenesis respondents exhibiting the highest rate of errors compared
(item 14), left-right lateralization (item 8), and biological with all other countries (p < .001). Post hoc comparison of
clock (item 23). Coincidentally, these were some of the items performance on neuromyth statements between all other

Volume 9—Number 3 173


Educational Neuromyths Among Latin American Teachers

were related to the supposedly brain-enhancing effect of


stimulus-rich environments (item 21), the positive impact
of coordination exercises on literacy (item 25) and left-right
hemispheric integration (item 30). In relation with the latter,
approximately 3 in 4 teachers still believe in the distinction
of right versus left brain learners (item 9) and almost 7 in
10 teachers support the idea that we only use 10% of our
brain (item 7). Quite remarkably, 9 of the 12 myths that were
actually inaccurate were believed by more than half of the
teachers.
We retrieved the rates of belief for each neuromyth from
the United Kingdom and Netherlands samples of teachers
surveyed by Dekker et al. (2012) and obtained an average
for comparison purposes with our sample (see rightmost
column of Table 2). We identified an overall similar transcon-
tinental pattern of responses. The most remarkable differ-
ences relate to second-language acquisition (item 2), with
more Latin American than European teachers believing that
simultaneous bilingualism may be detrimental for language
Fig. 2. Mean (SEM) percent neuromyths identified as valid (i.e., development; on the contrary, more European than Latin
worse performance) (a) across teachers in different countries; (b) American respondents believe education may be unable to
across teachers working in different levels (EC = early childhood; remediate developmental learning difficulties (item 28).
ES = elementary school; HS = high school; HE = higher education);
and (c) between teachers who stated they have read general science
texts (GST), primary scientific literature (PSL), or neuroscientific DISCUSSION
material (NM), and those who did not.
This study investigated neuroscientific literacy and belief in
countries was not significant (all p > .05). As shown in neuromyths among 3,451 Latin American teachers, espe-
Figure 2b, teachers working at different levels of education cially from Argentina, Chile, and Peru, but also from other
had similar performance, and the significant difference countries in the region, looking at overall and by-country
found had a remarkably smaller effect size, F(3,3447 ) = 5.67, trends. The association of brain knowledge with teachers’
p = .001, Cohen’s d = 0.14. The comparison of neuromyths access to popular science readings, primary scientific liter-
correctly identified by teachers who stated they read gen- ature, and neuroscientific material was also examined.
eral texts about science, t(2892 ) = 3.23, p = .001, Cohen’s Concerning general knowledge about the brain, we found
d = 0.12, or primary scientific literature, t(2916 ) = 2.48, that more than 50% of teachers responded to 2 out of 3 asser-
p = .01, Cohen’s d = 0.09, rendered significant differences tions correctly, mostly those featuring concepts or obser-
with negligible effect sizes. Importantly, having had previous vations that relate to everyday experiences they may be
access to material about neuroscience had no effect on the able to gather from the classroom. Instead, more factual
ability to correctly identify neuromyths, t(2755 ) = 1.5, p = .16, information about brain structure and functioning proved
Cohen’s d = 0.05. No significant correlations were found to be more challenging. For neuromyths, we found that
between percent of neuromyths identified and age, r = .03, more than 50% of the teachers failed to identify 9 of the 12
p = .09, R2 < .0001, or years of experience as a teacher, r = .03, invalid statements as such. Overall, these figures are sim-
p = .09, R2 < .0001. A small significant correlation was found ilar to the patterns found by Dekker et al. (2012) among
between performance in general statements about the brain British and Dutch teachers, with a 70% correct response rate
and belief in neuromyths, r = .21, p < .001, R2 = .045. This found for general brain knowledge, and teachers believing in
same pattern was observed individually for each country, more than half of the myths. They also match response pat-
Argentina: r = .18, p < .001, R2 = .032; Chile: r = .15, p < .001, terns recently reported among Greek, Turkish, and Chinese
R2 = .021; and Peru: r = .30, p < .001, R2 = .09. teachers (Howard-Jones, 2014).
Table 2 summarizes the overall and by-country percent Relative to European countries, the Latin American sam-
of belief for each myth. Remarkably, myths about learning ple performed somewhat worse, likely as a result of limited
styles had very similar answers across countries, revealing access to material in Spanish. Within the region, Peruvian
regional trends concerning this topic. Other neuromyths teachers were at a relative disadvantage for general neuro-
that had strong belief rates and transnational consensus science literacy compared with colleagues from other Latin

174 Volume 9—Number 3


Ezequiel Gleichgerrcht et al.

Table 2
Percent Belief for Each Neuromyth Sorted by Decreasing Strength of Belief

Overall Peru Argentina Chile Other χ2 Cramér’s V Dekker et al.


21. Environments that are rich in stimulus 92.2 91.4 87.8 98.5 97.5 52.6*** 0.12 75.5
improve the brains of pre-school children
15. Individuals learn better when they receive 90.5 90.6 85.8 95.2 86.2 30.7*** 0.09 94.5
information in their preferred learning style
(e.g., auditory, visual, and kinesthetic)
25. Exercises that rehearse coordination of 86 88.3 77.5 86.8 75 51.7*** 0.12 70.5
motor-perception skills can improve
literacy skills
30. Short bouts of coordination exercises can 77.9 77.8 73 81.3 87.5 16.0** 0.07 85
improve integration of left and right
hemispheric brain function
9. Differences in hemispheric dominance (left 73.3 74.7 57.9 81.3 73.3 245.8*** 0.19 88.5
brain, right brain) can help explain
individual differences among learners
5. It has been scientifically proven that fatty 71.1 76 58.3 66.6 58.8 80.6*** 0.15 61.5
acid supplements (omega-3 and omega-6)
have a positive effect on academic
achievement
12. There are critical periods in childhood 68.8 67 71 74.2 66.2 13.2 ∗∗ 0.06 42.5
after which certain things can no longer be
learned
7. We only use 10% of our brain 61 67.5 56.1 41.5 60 141*** 0.2 47
22. Children are less attentive after consuming 51.4 56.3 31 51.5 55 113.3*** 0.18 56
sugary drinks and/or snacks
2. Children must acquire their native language 38.9 50 15.6 19.7 31.4 336.4*** 0.31 21.5
before a second language is learned. If they
do not do so neither language will be fully
acquired
28. Learning problems associated with 22.6 27.6 18.5 9.5 10 102.5*** 0.17 47.5
developmental differences in brain function
cannot be remediated by education
4. If students do not drink sufficient amounts 9.4 11.2 5.6 6 15 31.2*** 0.1 22.5
of water (= 6–8 glasses a day) their brains
shrink

Note: Higher values indicate stronger beliefs in each myth. Item order numbers correspond to original list by Dekker et al. (2012).
**p = .01. ***p < .001.

American countries. The poorer performance of Peruvian literacy is higher than in teachers who do not engage in
teachers was nonetheless mild, and it may be reflecting the such readings, but this does not translate to reduced belief
more diverse array of teachers surveyed in the larger sample in neuromyths. What is more, a significant association was
of participants recruited from that country. found between performance on general statements about
Professors working in higher education had slight supe- the brain and belief in neuromyths, revealing that teachers
rior performance compared to teachers in early childhood, who know more about the brain are also the ones who
elementary, and high school levels. This is possibly due to will more likely believe in invalid assertions about brain
the fact that most higher education teachers in our sample research and its applications. Importantly, this correlation
were from psychology and related degree programs and was replicated when looking at each country independently,
had previously undertaken courses in basic neuroscience. but characterized by very small effect sizes, suggesting that
Remarkably, however, teachers in higher education did not the association between these two variables may not be
exhibit superior performance in responding to neuromyths. as direct. Still, this finding is in line with previous studies
The same pattern was observed for teachers who stated they (Dekker et al., 2012) reporting that the best predictor of
read popular science articles, primary scientific articles, or belief in neuromyths was general knowledge about the
even neuroscience material specifically: their neuroscience brain. What these findings are revealing is that teachers are

Volume 9—Number 3 175


Educational Neuromyths Among Latin American Teachers

having a difficult time identifying valid and invalid assertions neuroscience content is written in English, and English pro-
about the brain. ficiency in Latin America is relatively low. At least partly,
This finding may be, in part, the result of teachers’ the proliferation of neuromyths can be explained by lack of
eagerness to increase their neuroscience literacy (Pickering access to quality research, which is almost always written in
& Howard-Jones, 2007). In our study, 60.9% of teachers English. This highlights the need for (1) more funding for
claimed that they read general articles about science and local research and (2) the translation into Spanish of qual-
71% say they read primary scientific articles. In addition, ity research written in English. However, it must be noted
more than half of the sample stated that they have had access that our findings reflect patterns also found in English speak-
to material on the brain and learning, specifically. These ing countries (Dekker et al., 2012). Thus, while developing
figures are without a doubt inflated and shed light on part of language-accessible materials to members of non-English
the problem: teachers are still struggling to identify the qual- speaking cultures is an important goal, it is by no means a
ity of their sources. From our experiences working in over panacea for a lack of scientific literacy or overly technical and
a dozen countries in Latin America, it is extremely unlikely exclusionary scientific jargon in this area for materials aimed
that almost 3 in 4 teachers are reading primary scientific at teachers and educational policy makers.
articles, for example. Databases for journal access are gener- There are some caveats to this study that should be taken
ally restricted and unavailable to teachers, most articles are into consideration when contemplating future research in
published in English, and the majority of teachers have not this line. Perhaps the most obvious limitation is related to
had training in research methods or how to read scientific the sample: we focused primarily on three countries to infer
reports. What material these teachers are actually reading generalizations about a whole continent. For example, we
is perhaps the key factor to explain some of the response purposefully excluded Brazilian teachers, whose dominant
patterns found in this study and deserves further research. language is Portuguese rather than Spanish, to avoid the
The overall findings of this study nonetheless demonstrate effect of any linguistic differences in the survey used for
that accessing neuroscientific-related information provides this study. Our sample size is nonetheless considerable and
no advantage in terms of neuroscientific literacy for teachers our results consistent with other international samples.
in Latin America, despite the source of information being Nonetheless, we encourage researchers to work with other
accessed, and their ability to identify said source as primary, populations to incorporate more diversity to their partici-
secondary, or tertiary literature. pant samples. Another important issue to consider for future
These results therefore raise the issue of the scientific studies relates to the marked asymmetries in rural versus
illiteracy of the teacher population in Latin America. The urban settings in many Latin American countries. We used
growing interest of teachers in informing their pedagogical the survey originally designed by Dekker et al. (2012) so we
practice with scientific evidence from brain research high- could compare belief in neuromyths with European teachers,
lights the need to redesign teacher training programs in but future research should delve deeper into issues that may
Latin America to include basic science literacy and a basic be associated with misconceptions as a result of local trends.
understanding on how to read science literature. Those We thus invite researchers and teachers to work together to
abilities could help teachers gain critical thinking skills that identify local factors that could influence beliefs of teachers
would allow them, in turn, to approach science with better and design ad hoc measures to test the propagation of
preparation and therefore base their practice on sound such ideas.
scientific evidence rather than simpler, more attractive and It is also important to consider certain response biases
easier-to-understand—but inaccurate—information. inherent to the structure of the questionnaire used in our
These results also raise the issue of the inadequate study, and the strategies we implemented in order to min-
communication between research and practice, between imize such biases. For instance, respondents were offered
scientists and teachers, and between neuroscience and the do not know choice for all items, in order to minimize
education. They imply the need to find proper transla- a forced choice (a.k.a. insufficient category) bias. We also
tional efforts that can build bridges between neuroscientific randomized the order of items across participants, so that
research and the pedagogical practice. In many cases, response fatigue would have less of an impact on specific
these neuromyths are the result of the lack of translational items presented toward the end of the questionnaire. In this
efforts (Daniel, 2012) or of inadequate translational efforts sense, we celebrate recent studies confirming high preva-
(Daniel & Poole, 2009). Therefore, the need for transla- lence of neuromyths among teachers using new instruments
tion that (1) is accessible to teachers and (2) does not lose (Tardif, Doudin, & Meylan, 2015).
any of the meaning of the original research it synthesizes Crucially, two sources of concern arise for the demo-
is highlighted. graphic/background questions, particularly those referring
The lack of access to content in Spanish may also be to teachers’ access to literature. One the one hand is the
a contributor to this lack of adequate knowledge. Most fact, as discussed above, that teachers may not know

176 Volume 9—Number 3


Ezequiel Gleichgerrcht et al.

what “primary scientific articles” involves, or the use of and to responsibly translate promising scientific findings to
“information on learning and the brain” may be too vague. usable knowledge for education.
On the other hand is the issue of response desirability,
which may have inflated teachers’ positive responses to
their culture of accessing neuroscience-related informa- NOTE
tion. We believe these are important measures that are
1 Here, we note a slight variation from the nomenclature
worthy of further research, as understanding teachers’
used by the authors of the original questionnaire. In their
attitudes toward scientific information can help design
study, Dekker et al. (2012) present 15 neuromyths, three
more efficient strategies aimed at improving teacher
of which are “correct.” In our interpretation of the defini-
training.
tion established by the OECD (2002), a neuromyth is per
se an incorrect statement; for this reason we decided to
just consider the 12 neuromyths that were actually incor-
CONCLUSIONS
rect for that category.
Taken together, the results from our study confirm a high
prevalence of neuromyths among teachers in Latin America, REFERENCES
consistent with patterns observed among educators in other
regions (Dekker et al., 2012; Howard-Jones, 2014; Tardif Ansari, D., Coch, D., & De Smedt, B. (2011). Connecting education
and cognitive neuroscience: Where will the journey take us?
et al., 2015). These findings should not go unnoticed, as the
Educational Philosophy and Theory, 43(1), 37–42.
misleading use of science in education comes at a cost. In Beck, D. M. (2010). The appeal of the brain in the popular press.
terms of resources, for example, it means that governments Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 762–766.
and schools may be investing time, money, and infrastruc- Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1),
ture in fostering programs that are based on pseudoscience. 155–159.
At the individual level, this also translates to educators Cramér, H. (1999). Mathematical methods of statistics. Princeton,
spending money—most often out of their own pocket—to NJ: Princeton University Press.
receive training in topics such as multiple intelligences Daniel, D. B. (2012). Promising principles: Translating the science
of learning to educational practice. Journal of Applied Research
and learning styles, among others, hoping that these will
in Memory and Cognition, 1, 251–253.
help them better understand “the science” underlying their Daniel, D. B., & Poole, D. A. (2009). The ecology of pedagogy: How
students’ performance and behavior. This is especially wor- collaborative research can prevent us from harming students.
risome in the context of what appears to be a prolific market Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(1), 91–96.
offering educators what allegedly appears to be the key to Dekker, S., Lee, N. C., Howard-Jones, P., & Jolles, J. (2012). Neu-
better teaching practices. This has become apparent again romyths in education: Prevalence and predictors of miscon-
and again in our work with thousands of teachers across the ceptions among teachers. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 429.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00429
continent, who voice their frustration about having invested
Devonshire, I. M., & Dommett, E. J. (2010). Neuroscience: Viable
time and money in training on pseudoscience-based pro- applications in education? The Neuroscientist, 16, 349–356.
grams and adapting their teaching in the hopes that, by doi:10.1177/1073858410370900
being backed up by what they supposed was research-based Goswami, U. (2006). Neuroscience and education: From research
findings, they would help their students learn better. The to practice? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 406–411.
eventual result of these efforts may serve to dampen the doi:10.1038/nrn1907
positive potential impact of neuroscience on educational Herculano-Houzel, S. (2002). Do you know your brain? A survey on
practice and policy as the field matures. public neuroscience literacy at the closing of the decade of the
brain. The Neuroscientist, 8(2), 98–110.
These are all important hazards associated with the pop-
Howard-Jones, P. A. (2014). Neuroscience and education: Myths
ularization of neuromyths in the world of education, but and messages. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 817–824.
they also leave room for optimism. The openness of teachers doi:10.1038/nrn3817
to incorporate new information, even after years of work in Lilienfeld, S. O., Ammirati, R., & David, M. (2012). Distinguish-
the classroom, and their willingness to receive training in ing science from pseudoscience in school psychology:
a field that can be intimidating and challenging is not only Science and scientific thinking as safeguards against
reassuring, but also inspiring. In a way, it is a double-edged human error. Journal of School Psychology, 50(1), 7–36.
doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2011.09.006
sword, as enthusiasm for gaining access to new information
McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believing: The
can make teachers prone to encountering pseudoscience. It effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning.
is this same interest and eagerness that we must leverage, Cognition, 107, 343–352. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017
however, to help teachers better identify reliable sources of OECD (2002). Understanding the brain: Towards a new learning
scientific information that can be relevant in their practice science. Paris, France: OECD.

Volume 9—Number 3 177


Educational Neuromyths Among Latin American Teachers

Pasquinelli, E. (2012). Neuromyths: Why do they exist and persist? Sylvan, L. J., & Christodoulou, J. A. (2010). Understanding the role
Mind, Brain, and Education, 6, 89–96. of neuroscience in brain based products: A guide for educators
Pickering, S. J., & Howard-Jones, P. (2007). Educators’ views on the and consumers. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4, 1–7.
role of neuroscience in education: Findings from a study of UK Tardif, E., Doudin, P. A., & Meylan, N. (2015). Neuromyths among
and international perspectives. Mind, Brain, and Education, 1, teachers and student teachers. Mind, Brain, and Education, 9,
109–113. 50–59.
Sigman, M., Pena, M., Goldin, A. P., & Ribeiro, S. (2014). Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray,
Neuroscience and education: Prime time to build the J. R. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience expla-
bridge. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 497–502. doi:10.1038/ nations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 470–477.
nn.3672 doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20040

178 Volume 9—Number 3

You might also like