You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jcbs

A review of anger, hostility, and aggression from an ACT perspective T


a,⁎,1 b c
Olga V. Berkout , Diana Tinsley , Maureen K. Flynn
a
Department of Psychology, Texas A&M-Corpus Christi, 6300 Ocean Drive, Bay Hall 316, Corpus Christi, TX 78414, USA
b
Department of Psychology, University of Mississippi, 207 Peabody Hall, University MS 38677, USA
c
Department of Psychology, Metropolitan State University of Denver, 890 Auraria Pkwy, Denver, CO 80204, USA

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: A growing body of literature has begun to examine anger, hostility, and aggression using the psychological
Psychological flexibility flexibility model among both youth and adults. This manuscript provides the first overview of this research.
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy Papers were included in this review if they were published in English, peer-reviewed, published through
Anger December 8th, 2017 on PsycInfo and PubMed, or were recommended during the review process. The research
Aggression
reviewed examines anger, hostility, and aggression in a variety of contexts, such as interpersonal difficulties,
Hostility
emotional difficulties (e.g., depression, posttraumatic stress disorder), impulse control, and externalizing. The
article also reviews Acceptance and Commitment Therapy-based interventions targeting problems related to
anger and aggression. Generally, there is support for the psychological flexibility model in this domain and
treatment studies have been demonstrated a beneficial impact of ACT on aggression and domestic violence in
adults. The literature in youth is extremely limited, although some supportive findings were demonstrated. More
extensive and methodologically stronger examinations would strengthen this area of study and are discussed.

1. Introduction (Cassiello-Robins & Barlow, 2016; Veenstra et al., 2018). Cognitive


Behavioral Therapy (CBT) has been the most widely examined ap-
Anger is often defined as a “negative” emotional reaction to per- proach to addressing problematic anger (Lee & DiGiuseppe, 2018). CBT
ceived provocation (Novaco, 2011). Anger is linked to aggression and focuses on reducing the experience and expression of anger through
interpersonal problems and emphasized in the symptomatology of a reducing arousal, altering anger-provoking thoughts, and providing
number of psychological disorders (Cassiello-Robbins & Barlow, 2016; alternative skills for navigating social situations and interpersonal
Fernandez & Johnson, 2016). Some scholars have used the term anger frustration (Kassinove & Tafrate, 2011). Although CBT based inter-
to broadly encompass emotional reaction, cognition, and behavioral ventions have received empirical support, these are not universally
response, whereas others more narrowly zeroed in on anger as an effective and treatment drop out continues to present difficulties (Berta
emotion (Eckhardt, Norlander, & Deffenbacher, 2004; Fernandez, Day, & Zarling, 2018). As such, scholars continue to examine other ap-
& Boyle, 2015). When more fine-grained distinctions are made, the proaches.
term hostility has been used to emphasize cognitive/attitudinal aspects, One promising alternative is the psychological flexibility model and
such as viewing others and the world antagonistically (Cassiello- its treatment application, Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT).
Robbins & Barlow, 2016; Eckhardt et al., 2004). Aggression, or beha- Psychological flexibility is defined as the ability to flexibly relate to
vior aimed at harming others, may be an expression of anger; however, private experiences and behave in a manner that is responsive to en-
it can also be used as a tool to achieve a goal (e.g., instrumental ag- vironmental contingencies and consistent with one's values (Hayes,
gression, Fontaine, 2007; Veenstra, Bushman, & Koole, 2018). Levin, Plumb-Vilardaga, Villatte, & Pistorello, 2013). Psychological
Although anger is associated with problematic behavior, it is also a flexibility is comprised of six processes: cognitive defusion, acceptance,
functional natural emotion, which can motivate defense against a rea- present moment focus, chosen values, committed action, and self as
listic threat or inspire communication about an interpersonal problem context (see Levin, Hayes, & Vilardaga, 2012 for a review). The con-
(Cassiello-Robins & Barlow, 2016; Deffenbacher, 2011). Anger is verse of psychological flexibility, inflexibility, has been linked to
viewed as problematic in the broader literature when it is excessive, emotional difficulties and dysfunctional behavior (Chawla & Ostafin,
expressed as aggression, or causes distress or functional impairment 2007). ACT differs from CBT and other dominant perspectives in its


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: oberkout@gmail.com (O.V. Berkout).
1
Present/permanent address: Texas A&M Corpus Christi, 6300 Ocean Drive, Corpus Christi, TX 78412, USA.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.12.001
Received 12 June 2018; Received in revised form 18 November 2018; Accepted 1 December 2018
2212-1447/ © 2018 Association for Contextual Behavioral Science. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

emphasis on altering how one relates to thoughts, feelings, and other students in the Midwestern U.S. completed standardized measures of
private experiences, rather than attempting to change these (Hayes, psychological inflexibility, attachment anxiety (i.e., discomfort in in-
Levin, Plumb-Vilardaga, Villatte, & Pistorello, 2013; Hayes, Barnes- timate relationships), hostility, and interpersonal problems (i.e., cold-
Holmes, & Wilson, 2012). The focus of ACT treatment is increasing ness, avoidance, vindictiveness, and dominance). Psychological inflex-
flexible responding to the environment and living consistently with ibility was found to interact with hostility to predict dominating (B =
one's values, which is more strongly emphasized than distress reduction 0.00377, p < .01) and vindictiveness (B = 0.0025, p < .01), such that
(Ciarrochi, Bilich, & Godsel, 2010). relationships between hostility and these constructs were stronger at
The psychological flexibility perspective can be applied to anger, higher levels of inflexibility. Relationships between attachment anxiety,
hostility, and aggression. Eifert and Forsyth (2011) present a con- psychological inflexibility, and interpersonal problems were also de-
ceptual ACT model of anger difficulty. These scholars suggest that monstrated. Gerhart and colleagues (2014) suggested that participants
people who struggle with anger may be more strongly attached to anger might engage in problematic interpersonal behaviors to avoid the dis-
provoking thoughts (e.g., fusion) and view themselves as unable to comfort associated with hostility and that this may be particularly
control their anger (e.g., attachment to the conceptualized self). They challenging for those who are higher in psychological inflexibility.
propose that anger may provide a means to avoid other unpleasant Relationships between psychological inflexibility and anger ex-
private events, serving an experiential avoidance function. Eifert and pression have also been considered. Yavuz, Şahin, Ulusoy, Ipek, and
Forsyth (2011) further suggest that anger may make it difficult to en- Kurt (2016) explored the relationships between psychological inflex-
gage in behaviors consistent with one's values. Gardner and Moore ibility, anger, and interpersonal function among 34 men with Antisocial
(2008) have similarly proposed that aggression may allow individuals Personality Disorder (ASPD) and 32 men without psychiatric symp-
to avoid feeling threatened and vulnerable (their perspective is de- toms, recruited in Turkey. Participants completed a structured clinical
scribed as the Anger Avoidance Model), consistent with experiential interview and standardized questionnaires assessing anger, psycholo-
avoidance. gical inflexibility, and other aspects of social function. Psychological
An important point is that the psychological flexibility model con- inflexibility demonstrated relationships with all measures of anger in
siders anger, aggression, and hostility in terms of their function and the clinical group: it was negatively related to anger control (r =
does not view these as inherently problematic. For example, one could −0.50, p = .003) and positively correlated with trait anger (r = 0.54,
behave aggressively while saving a child from a rabid animal and this p = .001), anger internalization (r = 0.40, p = .021), and anger ex-
behavior would likely not be considered dysfunctional. The extent to pression (r = 0.35, p = .047). Conversely, only anger control was si-
which individuals are able to behave in a psychologically flexible milarly associated with psychological inflexibility among controls.
fashion, responding to the environment and leading values consistent Based on these findings, Yavuz and colleagues (2016) suggest that
lives, rather than the topography of the behavior itself is key (Eifert & psychological inflexibility is central in difficulties with anger and its
Forsyth, 2011). expression, particularly for clinical populations, such as ASPD.
Several studies focus on psychological inflexibility and intimate
2. Current review partner violence. Shorey et al. (2014) examined the association be-
tween psychological inflexibility and perpetration of dating violence
The purpose of this manuscript is to summarize the literature on among 109 male undergraduates at a Southeastern U.S. university.
psychological flexibility and ACT in relation to anger, hostility, and Participants completed standardized questionnaires assessing psycho-
aggression. English language based peer reviewed articles through logical inflexibility, alcohol use, relationship satisfaction, and perpe-
December 8th, 2017 on PsycInfo and PubMed were searched for the tration of psychological (e.g., threats), physical, and sexual intimate
terms: “psychological flexibility,” “experiential avoidance,” and partner violence. All forms of dating violence were positively related to
“Acceptance and Commitment Therapy” with “anger,” “aggression,” psychological inflexibility (psychological aggression r = 0.42,
“hostility,” and “violence.” Due to its emphasis on contextual beha- p < .001; physical aggression r = 0.24, p < .05; sexual coercion
vioral science, which includes the psychological flexibility model, the r = 0.36, p < .001). Psychological inflexibility remained predictive of
Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science was similarly reviewed for psychological aggression and sexual coercion, after controlling for age,
relevant articles. Articles presenting from these searches were ex- relationship satisfaction, and alcohol use. A similar relationship to
amined by the authors for association with ACT relevant constructs and physical aggression was not demonstrated after these variables were
focus on anger, hostility, and aggression. In addition to these efforts, we controlled for. Shorey and colleagues (2014) suggest that psychological
included two articles suggested during the review process, which fo- inflexibility appears linked to dating violence and that further ex-
cused on constructs of interest. We were broadly inclusive of studies, amination in larger samples may be helpful, particularly to help clarify
reviewing quantitative and qualitative data and case study and larger the relationship to physical aggression.
sample examinations. Both youth and adults were included with many Reddy, Meis, Erbes, Polusny, and Compton (2011) studied re-
samples being drawn from the United States (U.S.). Given the limited lationships between psychological flexibility and partner directed ag-
research in the area, we chose to include the construct of externalizing, gression among 49 American heterosexual military couples. Partici-
which encompasses aggression and hostility, as well as poor impulse pants completed standardized measures of psychological flexibility,
control and noncompliance for youth (Tully & Hunt, 2015). To our relationship adjustment, and psychological and physical aggression.
knowledge this review represents the first effort at providing an over- Men who had lower psychological flexibility were reported to be more
view of anger, hostility, and aggression within the ACT literature. physically aggressive by their partners (β = -0.21, p = .037) and they
reported experiencing more physical aggression from their partners
3. Findings on relationships with psychological inflexibility (β = -0.35, p = .032). Similar relationships were not demonstrated for
women, although the relationship between psychological flexibility and
3.1. Anger expression and interpersonal problems perpetration of physical aggression approached significance (β = -0.32,
p = .083). Negative associations between relationship adjustment and
Interpersonal problems, such as domestic violence, aggression, and psychological flexibility were also obtained. Reddy and colleagues
vindictiveness are the most commonly examined behaviors in relation (2011) suggested that aggressive behavior might be used to avoid ex-
to anger and psychological inflexibility. Gerhart, Baker, Hoerger, and cess arousal among individuals facing the stress of returning after de-
Ronan (2014) assessed the extent to which negative expectations about ployment.
social relationships account for the link between psychological inflex- Bell and Higgins (2015) examined relationships between intimate
ibility and interpersonal difficulties. In this study, 159 university partner violence, psychological inflexibility, and childhood

35
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

victimization. Participants were 232 Midwestern U.S. community fe- and ability to delay gratification. Scholars tested a path analysis model,
males who completed standardized measures of intimate partner vio- in which psychological inflexibility predicted delay of gratification,
lence, problem solving in social situations, experience of child abuse, both directly and through depression and anger. Psychological inflex-
and psychological inflexibility. Many participants endorsed physical ibility had significant direct effects on delay of gratification (β = -0.22,
intimate partner violence victimization (58% minor & 47% severe) and p = .001) and depression (β = 0.68, p = .001), as well as significant
perpetration (59% minor & 34% severe) over the course of the past indirect effects on anger via depression (β = 0.45, p = .001) and on
year. Path analysis was used to examine relationships. Childhood delay of gratification via depression and anger (β = -0.14, p = .001).
physical abuse was associated with perpetration of intimate partner Gerhart and colleagues (2014) suggested that challenges dealing with
violence. Childhood emotional abuse also had indirect effects via psy- common negative emotions associated with psychological inflexibility
chological inflexibility and problematic problem solving approaches might contribute to difficulties in delaying gratification and engaging in
(impulsivity/carelessness and negative problem orientation). Psycho- valued living.
logical inflexibility was associated with intimate partner violence per- Anger has been found to contribute to the relationship between
petration (β = 0.27) and had indirect effects on perpetration through PTSD symptoms and aggression perpetration (Novaco & Chemtob,
problematic approaches to social problem solving. An unexpected 2015). Scholars have begun examining the extent to which psycholo-
finding, however, was that negative problem orientation was inversely gical inflexibility plays a role in this relationship. Tull, Jakupcak,
related to perpetration. Scholars suggested that this relationship may be Paulson, and Gratz (2007) evaluated the extent to which psychological
reflective of a tendency to avoid problems and thus potential situations inflexibility contributes to anger and aggression among 113 student,
in which perpetration may occur. Bell and Higgins (2015) concluded staff, and faculty men exposed to situations with potential for trauma.
that psychological inflexibility was associated with perpetration of in- Participants provided self-report information on past experience of
timate partner violence, with some of its effects being obtained due to trauma, symptoms of PTSD, psychological inflexibility, the extent to
detrimental impact on problem solving in social situations. which they express emotions, anger, and aggressive behavior using
Researchers have examined relationships between psychological standardized measures. Psychological inflexibility was associated with
inflexibility, anger, hostility, aggression, and other interpersonal pro- both anger (r = 0.19, p < .05) and aggression (r = 0.33, p < .01) and
blems. Scholars have suggested that individuals who are less psycho- aggression was positively related to PTSD symptoms (r = 0.32,
logically flexible may respond with anger and aggression as means to p < .01). Furthermore, psychological inflexibility (β = 0.19, p < .05)
avoid other more uncomfortable private events (Eifert & Forsyth, 2011; and emotional inexpressivity (β = 0.18, p < .05) predicted aggression
Reddy et al., 2011). Consistently with this conceptualization, psycho- after PTSD symptoms and trait anger were accounted for. The re-
logical inflexibility has been linked with greater trait anger and greater lationship between PTSD symptoms and aggression was no longer sig-
expression of anger and aggression (Bell & Higgins, 2015; Reddy et al., nificant after these variables were included in the model, suggesting
2011; Shorey et al., 2014; Yavuz et al., 2016). Psychological inflex- that they serve as mediators. Tull and colleagues (2007) concluded that
ibility was also found to interact with hostility to predict problematic psychological inflexibility and emotional inexpressivity might account
interpersonal behaviors, such that these relationships were stronger for for aggressive behavior associated with PTSD.
those who were more inflexible (Gerhart et al., 2014). Generally the Scholars have considered relationships between social anxiety,
studies conducted in this area were consistent with the expected re- anger, and aggression. Kashdan and McKnight (2010) suggest that some
lationships from the psychological flexibility perspective. Methodolo- socially anxious individuals may become angry and aggressive in re-
gically, these studies have strengths and limitations. Populations ex- sponse to perceived rejection. Breen and Kashdan (2011) considered
amined included community adult, military, individuals diagnosed with the role of psychological inflexibility in the relationship between social
ASPD, and college students, avoiding the common limitation of un- anxiety and anger in response to rejection. Participants were 170 un-
dergraduate convenience samples. However, the majority also used dergraduates at a Mid-Atlantic U.S. university who completed stan-
cross-sectional self-report methodology and subsequent research would dardized measures of social anxiety, anger, and psychological inflex-
be strengthened by inclusion of longitudinal and more methodologi- ibility and responded to a vignette asking them to imagine social
cally varied examinations. rejection (including anger and experiential avoidance responses). Self-
reported psychological inflexibility was positively related to state
3.2. Relationships with other emotional difficulties (r = 0.41, p < .01) and trait anger (r = 0.37, p < .01), as well as the
tendency to hold anger in (r = 0.57, p < 0.01). Social anxiety symp-
Anger, hostility, and aggression may occur along with other emo- toms were positively associated with measures of state and trait anger
tional difficulties. For example, anger attacks, which present as intense and with anger and anger suppression in response to the rejection
anger accompanied by physiological arousal, appear common among vignette. The interaction between social anxiety and experiential
those struggling with depression and anxiety (Painuly, Grover, Gupta, & avoidance was a significant predictor of anger suppression in response
Mattoo, 2011). Physical and verbal aggression have also been linked to to imaged rejection. Participants low in social anxiety and low in ex-
depression (Leonhardt, Lysaker, Vohs, James, & Davis, 2018). Ad- periential avoidance engaged in less anger suppression in response to
ditionally, due to an established relationship between anger and Post- vignettes compared to participants high in social anxiety or experiential
traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), anger is included as a part of its di- avoidance. Breen and Kashdan (2011) suggested that individuals who
agnostic criteria (America Psychiatric Association, 2013). From a are higher in experiential avoidance may struggle more with adaptively
psychological flexibility perspective, these relationships may be con- handling anger and emphasized the extent to which flexible responding
sistent with the potential for anger to allow avoidance of emotional was important to address social challenges.
pain (Eifert & Forsyth, 2011). Scholars have begun to examine these Kashdan, Breen, Afram, and Terhar (2010) examined the relation-
associations and their links to potentially problematic expressions. ship between anger and psychological inflexibility among 148 under-
Gerhart, Heath, Fitzgerald, & Hoerger (2013) assessed the extent to graduates at a Mid-Atlantic U.S. university. Participants were asked to
which psychological inflexibility, anger, and depression play a role in write an autobiographical narrative associated with experiencing an-
contributing to difficulties with delaying gratification. Scholars ex- xiety, which was coded for experiential avoidance, coping, and emo-
pected that those who are less psychologically flexible would experi- tional experience. Participants completed standardized measures of
ence more negative emotions and would be more focused on short-term social anxiety, depression, anger, psychological inflexibility, and emo-
consequences, rather than long-term benefit. Two hundred and two tion regulation. Data were collected at both an initial session and a
undergraduates at a university in the Midwestern U.S. completed three-month follow up. Anger was unrelated to experiential avoidance
standardized measures of psychological inflexibility, anger, depression, coded from the narrative activity. Initial psychological inflexibility was

36
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

associated with less outward anger expression (r = -0.21, p < .05) 4. ACT interventions findings
during the follow up assessment, however, this relationship was no
longer significant after initial anger expression, negative affect, and 4.1. Adults with anger and aggression difficulties
anxiety symptoms were controlled for. Scholars suggested that re-
lationships between anger and experiential avoidance as coded in the 4.1.1. Anger and aggression
narrative might have been weaker because the writing prompt focused Scholars have assessed the impact of ACT based interventions on
on anxiety, rather than anger. Kashdan and colleagues (2010) re- anger and aggression among adults. Donahue, Santanello, Marsiglio,
commended that more longitudinal and multimethod research on these and Van Male (2017) applied an ACT group intervention to 23 male
relationships be conducted. veterans in the Northwestern U.S. who reportedly struggled with anger
Overall, several studies examining psychological inflexibility, anger, and aggression using a single group design. The intervention targeted
and aggression in relation to other emotional difficulties have been mindfulness, acceptance, defusion, self as context, values, and com-
conducted. Generally, psychological inflexibility has been linked to mitted action, and consisted of 12 weekly 90-min group sessions. Par-
anger and aggression in these examinations (Breen & Kashdan, 2011; ticipants completed standardized measures of anger, physical and
Tull et al., 2007) and appears to interact with anger and depression in verbal aggression, psychological inflexibility, and quality of life at pre-
relation to difficulty in delaying gratification (Gerhart et al., 2014). treatment, post-treatment, and six-week follow up, and provided feed-
Additionally, psychological inflexibility was found to partially explain back on the intervention. Participants reported decreased physical ag-
the relationship between PTSD symptoms and aggression (Tull et al., gression (d = 0.53, p < .05) and psychological inflexibility (d = 0.65,
2007). Findings were generally supportive of the idea that inflexible p < .01) at post-treatment and decreased inflexibility (d = 0.86,
responding to private events may contribute to aggression and other p < .01) and difficulties with one's temper at follow up (d = 0.49,
problematic behaviors. However, the studies reviewed also had some p < .05). However, there were no significant changes in verbal ag-
limitations. With the exception of Tull and colleagues (2007) inclusion gression or quality of life. The rate of attrition was 30% and occurred
of other university employees, studies relied on college student sam- within the first three sessions; those who completed treatment tended to
ples. Examinations tended to be primarily cross-sectional (Breen & rate it as effective, a good use of their time, and endorse that they would
Kashdan, 2011; Gerhart et al., 2014; Tull et al., 2007) although recommend it to others. Moreover, they found that the protocol was not
Kashdan and colleagues (2010) studied relationships over a three- too long and many commented that they learned useful skills. Donahue
month period. Breen and Kashdan (2011) and Kashdan and colleagues and colleagues (2017) concluded that ACT presents a feasible treatment
(2010) included vignette response and narrative writing, broadening option for veterans with anger difficulties.
the methodology used. However, self-report methodology remained the Harvey, Henrickson, Bimler, and Dickson (2017) also studied the
primary approach and a limited number of studies have been con- effects of an ACT intervention on New Zealand military service per-
ducted. sonnel anger, aggression, distress, and use of drugs and alcohol. The
intervention targeted all six hexaflex processes over the course of a five-
day group treatment course. Two hundred and sixty two soldiers with
3.3. Psychological inflexibility and externalizing in youth psychological, social, occupational, or legal problems participated in
this study. Participants were referred by military command on the basis
Most studies of psychological inflexibility and anger, hostility, and of struggling with substance use difficulties. Soldiers were assigned to
aggression have been conducted in adult samples. Aggressive behavior the intervention or waitlist control group based on availability and
and hostility in youth is encompassed in the broader construct of ex- timing of the referral. Participants completed standardized measures of
ternalizing, which also includes difficulties with poor impulse control substance use, anger and aggression, stress, anxiety, locus of control,
and noncompliance (Tully & Hunt, 2015). Given the extremely limited and use of emotion regulation strategies at the beginning of treatment
literature on psychological inflexibility in youth, we chose to include and at one-month post intervention. Attrition rates were 66.7% for the
examinations of externalizing. A single study evaluating the relation- intervention and 44.6% for the waitlist control group. Compared to
ships between externalizing and experiential avoidance was identified. those in the waitlist control group, those who received the intervention
Shea and Coyne (2017) assessed relationships between experiential experienced a greater decrease in physical (d = −0.38, p < .05) and
avoidance, victimization by relational aggression (actions aimed at verbal (d = - 0.37, p < .05) aggression and perceived stress (d =
damaging social relationships), and psychological symptoms among −0.43, p < .05) and a greater increase in use of emotion management
fifty eight 11–14 year old girls from the Northeastern United States. (d = 0.69, p < .05). Additionally, greater decrease in perception of
Scholars proposed that those with a greater history of relational victi- powerful others having control over one's life was demonstrated for the
mization would endorse more experiential avoidance and psychological intervention group. There was a significant decrease in alcohol use for
difficulty. Participants completed standardized measures of experiential those in the intervention group, although the between group compar-
avoidance, victimization by relational aggression, and internalizing and ison of alcohol use change did not attain significance. Harvey and
externalizing difficulties, and a task assessing response to social ex- colleagues (2017) concluded that the study provided support for the use
clusion (Cyberball). History of victimization via relational aggression of an ACT intervention for military personnel and noted that partici-
was positively related to experiential avoidance. Experiential avoidance pants qualitatively reported positive perceptions of the intervention.
was found to predict externalizing behavior (β = 0.31, p < .001) after They suggested that larger more rigorous examinations using rando-
controlling for victimization and was similarly linked to greater inter- mized controlled trial methodology and long-term follow up would
nalizing symptoms. Experiential avoidance was additionally predictive strengthen the field.
of sad mood following exclusion in the behavioral task. Shea and Coyne Eisenbeck, Shietz, and Szekeres (2017) compared the impact of an
(2017) suggested that externalizing behavior could serve an avoidance ACT intervention to treatment as usual (TAU) among 17 Hungarian
function, allowing girls to evade unpleasant peer interactions by be- inmates charged with violent crimes and placed in pretrial detention.
having aggressively. A summary of youth and adult studies examining Participants completed standardized measures of psychological inflex-
relationships between anger, aggression, hostility, and related con- ibility, depression, anxiety, and valued living at pretreatment, post-
structs with psychological inflexibility is provided in Table 1. treatment, and three month follow up. Participants were randomly as-
signed to TAU (a CBT intervention) or an ACT based program targeting
all aspects of psychological flexibility. Both programs consisted of ten
weekly 90-min group sessions. Due to correctional transfer ten parti-
cipants (five in each group) remained at post-treatment. Scholars found

37
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

Table 1
Summary of relationships between anger, hostility, aggression, and psychological inflexibility.
Study Sample Age: Mean (SD) Measurement Design Relationship to Inflexibility

Gerhart et al. (2014) 159 primarily female 24.00 (6.00) AAQ-II Cross-sectional survey Stronger relationships for hostility with
university students AQ-H interpersonal problems at higher level
Yavuz et al. (2016) 34 men with ASPD & 32 men ASPD: 31.60 AAQ-II Cross-sectional survey and Relates negatively to anger control and
without (7.78) clinical interview positively to anger and expression for
Without: 36.56 STAXI ASPD
(9.74)
Shorey et al. (2014) 109 male undergraduates 18.44 (0.75) AAQ-II Cross-sectional survey Positively related to psychological
CTS−2 aggression and sexual coercion
Reddy et al. (2011) 49 heterosexual military Soldiers: 35.00 AAQ-II Cross-sectional survey & Males higher in inflexibility reported to be
couples partners (7.39) partner report more aggressive
Partners: 34.00 CTS−2
(8.43)
Bell and Higgins (2015) 232 community females 31.96 (10.09) AAQ Cross-sectional survey Related to greater perpetration of IPV
CTS−2
Gerhart, Heath, Fitzgerald, and 202 primarily female 19.50 (3.00) AAQ-II Cross-sectional survey Positive effect on anger and negative effect
Hoerger (2013) undergraduates PED-A on delay of gratification
Tull et al. (2007) 113 men exposed to trauma 26.41 (5.00) AAQ Cross-sectional survey Predicted aggression over anger and PTSD
BDHI symptoms
STAXI
Breen and Kashdan (2011) 170 primarily female 21.29 (5.19) AAQ-II Cross-sectional survey and Positively related to anger and anger in
undergraduates STAXI-II vignette response
Kashdan, Breen et al. (2010) 148 primarily female Age not reported AAQ Longitudinal survey & No significant relationships
undergraduates MAI narrative
Shea and Coyne (2017) 58 adolescent girls 12.26 (0.98) AFQ-Y CBCL-YSR Cross-sectional survey & Experiential avoidance associated with
behavioral task greater externalizing

Note: AAQ=Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes et al., 2004); AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (Bond et al., 2007, 2011); AFQ-
Y= Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire for Youth (Greco, Lambert, & Baer, 2008); AQ-H = Aggression Questionnaire Hostility (Buss & Perry, 1992); BDHI = Buss-
Durkee Hostility Inventory (Buss & Durkee, 1957); CBCL-YSR = Child Behavior Checklist – Youth Self-Report (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001); CTS-2 =Revised
Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996); MAI = Multidimensional Anger Inventory (Siegel, 1986); PED-A= PROMIS Emotional
Distress-Anger (Cella et al., 2010); STAXI = State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (Spielberger, 1988); STAXI-II = State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-II
(Spielberger, 1999). Mean and standard deviation (SD) for age provided when available.

that self reported values consistent behaviors increased for participants delivered in groups. The ACT intervention addressed mindfulness, de-
in ACT from pre to post-treatment and that these participants were fusion, acceptance, values, and committed action and was provided via
higher on this measure compared to TAU at post-treatment (η2 = 0.54, 12 weekly sessions lasting two hours each. Participants completed
p = 0.02) and at three month follow up (η2 = 0.73, p = 0.02). No other standardized measures of psychological and physical aggression in in-
differences were obtained. Scholars suggested that changes in values timate relationships, psychological inflexibility, and difficulties in
consistency, rather than psychological inflexibility, may have been re- emotion regulation. Seventy two percent of participants in ACT and
flective of greater focus on values over the course of the intervention about 72.5% of participants in the control condition completed post-
(Eisenbeck, et al., 2017). treatment assessments. Participants in ACT reported less psychological
Razzaque (2012) studied the effectiveness of an ACT intervention on (d = 0.40) and physical aggression (d = 0.36) at post-treatment and six
three patients in a British psychiatric intensive care unit using a case month follow up (psychological aggression d = 0.96 and physical ag-
study design. The intervention included defusion, self as context, va- gression d = 0.79). Additionally, participants in ACT evidenced a
lues, and mindfulness delivered via 20-min daily sessions over the greater reduction in psychological and physical aggression from pre-
course of two to three weeks. Participants had lengthy histories of self- treatment to six-month follow up. Participants in ACT, but not the
harm or violence towards others; two were diagnosed with Bipolar control condition, experienced declines in psychological inflexibility
Disorder and one with Schizoaffective Disorder. Nursing staff assessed and difficulties in emotion regulation. Psychological inflexibility
observed violent behaviors and verbal expressions of the desire to self- mediated reductions in psychological and physical aggression, sug-
harm were assessed through interviews; both of these metrics did not gesting theoretically consistent therapeutic change. Zarling and col-
use standardized assessment. Clinical Global Index, a structured rating leagues (2015) suggested that findings were supportive of applicability
scale, was additionally used to evaluate illness severity. All participants of ACT to the treatment of intimate partner violence and that change
were rated as decreasing violent behavior and reported other positive appeared to occur in a theoretically consistent fashion.
symptom change (e.g., fewer derogatory auditory hallucinations, In another examination, Zarling, Bannon, and Berta (2017) com-
greater confidence in impulse control), although this rating did was not pared an ACT based program, Achieving Change Through Values-Based
made using a standardized measure limiting conclusions. All partici- Behavior (ACTV) to a traditional CBT/Duluth model treatment, which
pants received lower illness severity ratings after treatment. Razzaque focused on changing problematic beliefs around masculinity and power
(2012) suggested that ACT was a promising intervention for reducing and control of women. Data from 3474 men, court ordered to partici-
aggression among seriously mentally ill. pate in a batterer intervention program, were analyzed in this study.
The study was conducted in Iowa and participants were assigned to
4.1.2. Intimate partner violence conditions based on availability and scheduling considerations. The
Several studies have evaluated the potential of ACT to reduce en- ACTV intervention includes a focus on values, committed action, ac-
gagement in intimate partner violence. Zarling, Lawrence, and ceptance, mindfulness, defusion, and an emphasis on behavioral skills,
Marchman (2015) examined the effects of a group ACT intervention in such as assertiveness and conflict resolution. Both interventions con-
a randomized controlled trial among 101 male and female treatment sisted of 24 weekly group sessions that were an hour and a half to two
seeking participants. Participants were randomly assigned to receive hours long. Criminal justice data on the occurrence and frequency of
ACT or an attention control supportive intervention; both were violent offenses during and after completion of the program were

38
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

et al., 1996); DAR = Dimensions of Anger Reactions Scale (Novaco, 1975); VLQ= Valued Living Questionnaire (Wilson, Sandoz, Kitchens, & Roberts, 2011). Mean and standard deviation (SD) for age provided when
2008); AQ = Aggression Questionnaire (Buss & Perry, 1992); AQ-SF = Buss Perry Aggression Questionnaire Short Form (Diamond & Magaletta, 2006); CTS-2-PA = Conflict Tactics Scale-2 Physical Assault Scale (Straus
Note: AAQ = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes et al., 2004); AAQ=II = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (Bond et al., 2011); AFQ-Y = Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire for Youth (Greco et al.,
compared. During the intervention, significantly fewer ACTV vs. CBT/

Participant highlighted group connection as contributing to

Low rate of domestic violence recidivism and decrease in


Duluth participants committed offenses that led to charges (6.4% vs.
12.3%). Within twelve months after treatment completion a markedly

Lower violent recidivism for those receiving ACT


Lower psychological and physical aggression and
Decreased physical and verbal aggression in ACT
lower percentage of ACTV participants (7.6%) engaged in general

Increase in values consistent behaviors in ACT


Decreased physical aggression & inflexibility
violent recidivism compared to those in the Duluth/CBT (12.1%) con-
dition. Similarly, a smaller proportion of those in ACTV (5.3%) had
domestic assault charges compared to those in the CBT/Duluth (7.7%)

psychological inflexibility for ACT


group over the year following treatment. Treatment dropout rates of
38.9% for ACT and 27% for the CBT/Duluth condition were obtained.

Decrease in violent behavior


Zarling and colleagues (2017a) concluded that ACTV presents a pro-

experiential avoidance
treatment engagement
mising alternative to the traditional Duluth/CBT treatment and may be
more effective in lowering recidivism rates.
Zarling and Berta (2017) provide a qualitative case study high-

Major Finding
lighting the experiences of Ed, a participant receiving treatment for
interpersonal violence perpetration in the Midwestern United States. Ed
reported high alcohol use and frequent interpersonal difficulties with
his girlfriend, which resulted in an altercation with police involvement.
As a result, he received charges of domestic assault and was court or-

ACT vs. waitlist control, nonrandom

Randomly assigned to ACT vs. CBT


dered to attend ACTV. Although Ed was resistant to treatment at first,

ACT vs. Duluth/CBT, nonrandom


Single group ACT intervention

Single group ACT intervention


Randomly assigned to ACT vs.
he later made a connection with one of the other participants and over
time reported that he began to realize that there were problems in his
current relationship. At the end of treatment, Ed acknowledged that he
gained insight into his behaviors and emotions and had a better un-

attention control
Case description

Case description
derstanding of his values. Zarling and Berta (2017) further note that

assignment

assignment
more randomized controlled studies are needed to understand factors
that influence treatment and how much and what quality of training is
Design
necessary for ACTV group facilitators.
Berta and Zarling (In Press) examined the impact of ACTV among 23

No formal measures
male domestic violence perpetrators in the Midwestern United States in

AFQ-Y Recidivism
Staff observation
a single group design. Participants in this study were incarcerated to
permit them to complete treatment after a fourth contempt of court

CTS− 2-PA
Recidivism
Measures

charge for failing to complete a community domestic violence program.


AAQ-II

AAQ-II

AAQ-II
AQ-SF
DAR

VLQ
The majority of participants (52%) identified as Native American and
AQ

all were male. ACTV was delivered via 24 sessions lasting two hours
each, which were provided three times per week. Concurrent to the
Primarily 20–24; range < 20

ACTV program, participants received treatment as usual via meetings


with a psychiatrist and counselor. Participants completed standardized
Age data not provided

measures of experiential avoidance, psychopathy, adverse childhood


Age: Mean (SD)

experiences (such as abuse and living with household members who


54.83 (14.57)

33.45 (10.25)

36.00 (10.00)
26.47 (8.06)

31.45 (7.39)

were struggling with mental illness or substance abuse), and attach-


to > 40

Late 30's

ment. Legal records on recidivism for one-year post treatment com-


pletion were examined. Participants were comparable to other domestic
violence samples on attachment and exhibited higher psychopathy
scores. They additionally reported an average of 3.94 adverse child-
3474 men court ordered to batterer intervention
17 male inmates charged with violent offenses

hood experiences. Scholars found that experiential avoidance decreased


101 primarily female adults with relationship
23 male veterans with anger and aggression

262 primarily male military personnel with

over the course of treatment (d=-4.73). One participant received a


Single male in treatment for interpersonal

23 incarcerated male domestic violence

domestic violence charge and 24% a violent criminal charge over the
year following program completion. Scholars highlighted the beneficial
Three patients in psychiatric ICU

impact of treatment on experiential avoidance and the low rate of do-


mestic violence recidivism (4.5%) as strengths of the intervention.
substance use difficulties

Overall, these studies suggested that ACT interventions offer a


violence perpetration

promising approach to addressing aggressive behavior (Donahue et al.,


2017; Harvey et al., 2017; Razzaque, 2012) and its occurrence in in-
timate partner relationships (Berta & Zarling, In press; Zarling et al.,
perpetrators
Summary of adult intervention studies.

difficulties

2015; Zarling et al., 2017; Zarling & Berta, 2017). Additionally, ACT
program
Sample

based interventions appear promising among legally involved in-


dividuals (Eisenbeck et al., 2017; Zarling et al., 2017; Berta & Zarling,
In press), which may be more challenging than those who are vo-
Zarling, Bannon et al. (2017)

luntarily seeking treatment. However, the literature remains limited.


Zarling and Berta (2017)
Eisenbeck et al. (2017)
Donahue et al. (2017)

Few studies have been conducted and only two (Zarling et al., 2015;
Harvey et al. (2017)

Zarling et al. (2015)

Berta & Zarling ( )

Eisenbeck et al., 2017) used randomized controlled trial methodology,


Razzaque (2012)

with one of these being a small sample of 17 participants (Eisenbeck


et al., 2017). More extensive examination and independent replication
available.

of findings would strengthen empirical support for ACT for aggressive


Table 2

Study

behavior. Adult intervention study characteristics, measures, and key


outcomes are summarized in Table 2.

39
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

Note: AFQ-Y = Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire for Youth (Greco et al., 2008); PEQ-R = Revised Peer Experiences Questionnaire (Prinstein, Boergers, & Vernberg, 2001); RSQ = Responses to Stress Questionnaire
4.2. Interventions with youth

Children experienced less difficulty with anger and Although ACT has not been extensively examined in the treatment

Decreased frequency, intensity, and duration of


of anger, hostility, and aggression in youth, a small number of studies
Increase in active problem solving in ACT have been conducted in this area. Chang and Hwang (2017) explored
whether an ACT-based protocol could decrease anger and anxiety in
children training in the martial art of Taekwondo. This study was ex-
ploratory in nature and did not use standardized measures. The authors
suggested that, due to its competitive and aggressive nature, partici-
pation in martial arts could evoke anger and anxiety. Nine elementary
school aged children in Seoul, Korea received eight daily hour and a
half intervention sessions. The intervention was based on an ACT and
Major Findings

mindfulness approach and included acceptance, defusion, present mo-


irritability

outbursts

(Connor-Smith, Compas, Wadsworth, Thomsen, & Saltzman, 2000). Demographic information on gender & age mean and standard deviation (SD) provided when available.

ment focus, and value clarification. Participants provided information


on their emotional experience and perception of the intervention via
unstructured self-report and providing ratings after each session. These
Randomly assigned to ACT vs. waitlist control

evaluations indicated that children endorsed more positive attitudes


towards Taekwondo and experienced less difficulty with anger and ir-
ritability, although standardized measures were not used in the eva-
Single group ACT/mindfulness based

luation (Chang & Hwang, 2017). While interesting, this study was
limited in its description of methodology. Evaluation of improvement
appears to have been subjective rather than informed by statistical
evaluation.
Theodore-Oklota, Orsillo, Lee, and Vernig (2014) evaluated the
Single case design

impact of a three-session ACT-based intervention on relational aggres-


sion. The ACT-based intervention included mindfulness, defusion, ac-
intervention

ceptance, and values focused exercises and consisted of three sessions


Design

delivered during 48-min class periods over the course of two weeks.
Participants were 210 seventh graders at a New England middle school,
whose classrooms were randomized to treatment or waitlist control.
Behavioral observation of anger episodes

Participants completed standardized measures on social interactions,


experiential avoidance, and psychopathology before starting the pro-
gram and were assessed again at three-month follow up. The ACT in-
tervention was found to have a positive impact on the use of active
problem solving in peer conflict (β = 0.15, p = 0.03), although rela-
Measurement unclear

tional aggression, experiential avoidance, and psychopathology symp-


toms were not affected. Theodore-Oklota and colleagues (2014) sug-
gested that the program may have been too brief to have an impact on
Measures

experiential avoidance, but noted that the positive impact on active


AFQ-Y
PEQ-R

problem solving to address conflict was promising.


RSQ

Ruiz and Perete (2015) examined the application of an ACT in-


formed protocol in a single case of a five-year old boy with problematic
Age: Mean (SD)

12.45 (0.51)

expressions of anger (e.g., derogatory comments to peers when he was


jealous, yelling, and crying). The protocol consisted of four 20-min
intervention sessions implemented over a three-week period. Inter-
9.89

5.00

vention sessions included components focused on increasing psycho-


logical flexibility drawing on Relational Frame Theory perspective
taking. Additionally, exercises, such as holding a difficult posture, or
210 primarily male eight graders

abruptly ending a video game and then mindfully reflecting on the


Nine elementary school aged

experience were used to increase flexible responding. A token economy


Single case male child

was also used to help reduce angry outbursts. Effectiveness was mea-
sured through frequency, duration, and intensity of outbursts as re-
corded by his mother. During treatment, the child evidenced sig-
nificantly fewer anger outbursts (Tau-U = .542, p = .004) with lower
Summary of youth intervention studies.

children

intensity (Tau-U = .476, p = .014) and shorter duration (Tau-U =


Sample

.574, p = .002). Scholars suggested that ACT presents a promising


treatment approach to problematic childhood anger. Notably, in this
Theodore-Oklota et al. (2014)

study Samuel's mother, who is also the second author of this manu-
script, delivered the intervention, while being supervised by the first
Chang and Hwang (2017)

Ruiz and Perete (2015)

author. Although parent-training interventions commonly include par-


ents in at home skill practice, it is possible that this dual role may have
influenced interpretation.
Very limited research has been conducted on ACT interventions for
youth anger and aggression difficulties. Although the authors including
Table 3

Study

these studies are breaking new ground, methodological improvements


would strengthen conclusions that could be drawn. For example, Chang

40
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

and Hwang (2017) and Ruiz and Perete (2015) did not use standardized other measures that better capture other psychological flexibility pro-
measures in their examinations, which may limit the ability to make cesses in future research would be beneficial. Additionally varied
comparisons with future studies. Additionally, Ruiz and Perete had a measures were used to assess psychological inflexibility, anger, hosti-
parent serve as both an author and the deliverer of the intervention. lity, and aggression, potentially tapping into different aspects of these
Potentially this dual role may have influenced the author's perspective. constructs. Given the fairly limited literature, this aspect has the po-
Theodore-Oklota and colleagues (2014) used both standardized mea- tential to influence findings, although the relationships obtained were
sures and random assignment, but did not find treatment group dif- generally consistent.
ferences in experiential avoidance and relational aggression. However,
this study also found that participants in ACT used more positive pro- 5.3. Impact of ACT interventions
blem solving, suggesting that some benefit was obtained (Theodore-
Oklota et al., 2014). As noted by Theodore-Oklota and colleagues Scholars have examined the impact of ACT on anger and aggression.
(2014) the brevity of the intervention may not have been sufficient to ACT appears to decrease engagement in aggressive behavior (Donahue
demonstrate lasting benefit after a three-month period. Key character- et al., 2017; Harvey et al., 2017; Razzaque, 2012) and intimate partner
istics of youth intervention studies are summarized in Table 3. violence (Zarling et al., 2015; Zarling et al., 2017; Zarling & Berta,
2017; Berta & Zarling, In Press). Examinations of ACT for aggression
5. Discussion outside the domestic violence context have demonstrated small to
medium effect sizes but fairly high dropout rates (30%, Donahue et al.,
5.1. Relationships with psychological inflexibility 2017; 66.7%, Harvey et al., 2017). The particularly high drop out rate
in Harvey and colleagues (2017) study may be attributable to use of a
The psychological flexibility model emphasizes flexible responding population who also struggled with substance use. Medium to large
to private events and value consistent behavior (Hayes et al., 2013). effect sizes for aggression and decreased recidivism were obtained in
Conceptual applications of this perspective have emphasized the po- examinations of ACT for domestic violence (Zarling et al., 2015; Zarling
tential for anger and aggression to allow individuals to avoid un- et al., 2017). Dropout rates of about 28% (Zarling et al., 2015) and
pleasant private events (e.g., experiential avoidance; Eifert & Forsyth, about 38.9% (Zarling et al., 2017) were obtained in these studies,
2011). Individuals who are more inflexible might be expected to ex- which are lower than typical for domestic violence intervention pro-
perience anger and aggression more due to the tendency towards ex- grams (Berta & Zarling, In press). Of additional importance, many ex-
periential avoidance. The literature was generally consistent with this aminations also found theoretically consistent changes in psychological
prediction: individuals who were higher in inflexibility tended to report flexibility processes over the course of ACT based interventions
more anger and aggression (Bell & Higgins, 2015; Reddy et al., 2011; (Donahue et al., 2017; Eisenbeck et al., 2017; Zarling et al., 2015; Berta
Shorey et al., 2014; Yavuz et al., 2016). Experiential avoidance was & Zarling, In press).
similarly linked to youth externalizing behavior (Shea & Coyne, 2017). Examinations among youth are much more limited, although there
Of note, the psychological flexibility model emphasizes values con- are some positive findings. Scholars have found that ACT based inter-
sistent behavior, more so than psychological distress (Ciarrochi et al., ventions appear to decrease aggression (Chang & Hwang, 2017; Ruiz &
2010). Response to private events, rather than whether they are ex- Perete, 2015) and increase adaptive social problem solving (Theodore-
perienced is ultimately most important: some support for the role of Oklota et al., 2014). However, support for the intervention is not uni-
psychological flexibility in adaptive responding has been obtained. Tull versal and the sole group examination using statistical analyses and
and colleagues (2007) found that psychological inflexibility mediated random assignment failed to find group differences in experiential
the link between PTSD symptoms and aggression. Although not all avoidance, relational aggression, or psychopathology (Theodore-Oklota
examinations demonstrated expected theoretically consistent relation- et al., 2014). It is difficult to make conclusive statements given the
ships (e.g., Kashdan et al., 2010), support for the psychological flex- extremely limited literature and more studies of ACT in youth are
ibility model has been obtained. needed.

5.2. Methodological considerations in examinations of relationships 5.4. Methodological considerations in intervention studies

As common for emerging areas in psychology, many of the studies Overall stronger support for ACT as an intervention for aggression
reviewed used cross-sectional self-report methodology to assess con- has been obtained among adults, however, the need for further research
structs of interest. Self-report methodology can permit scholars to ex- remains. The majority of these evaluations used quasi-experimental
plore new areas with limited expenditure of resources. These studies designs and only two randomized controlled examinations have been
have generally demonstrated expected relationships in the areas of conducted (Eisenbeck et al., 2017; Zarling et al., 2015). As research in
anger, hostility, and aggression. However, greater integration of other this area is limited, quasi-experimental studies have contributed con-
methods, such as behavioral tasks, legal records, and longitudinal ex- siderably to beginning to establish ACT as a promising intervention for
aminations would strengthen the field. Additionally, although other aggression. However, a greater number of randomized controlled trials
populations were included, college students, who may not be re- or carefully conducted single case designs supporting the intervention
presentative of the general population, were the most common group would provide stronger support and permit for systematic reviews
studied. A single study was identified examining relationships among consistent with contemporary recommendations for identifying evi-
youth. Continued efforts to broaden the populations studied would dence based treatments (Tolin, McKay, Forman, Klonsky, & Thombs,
improve the literature base. 2015). Additionally, the populations examined have been primarily on
An important methodological consideration is measurement of military individuals (Donahue et al., 2017; Harvey et al., 2017) and
psychological flexibility and its processes. With a few exceptions, the domestic violence perpetrators (Berta & Zarling, In press; Zarling et al.,
Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) and 2017; Zarling et al., 2015). Determining the extent to which ACT based
its prior versions were used to evaluate psychological flexibility. AAQ-II treatments are beneficial in other groups would improve the current
has been described as a measure of psychological inflexibility by its literature base. Measurement of outcomes and processes used varied
developers (Bond et al., 2011). However, scholars have argued that the methodology. Inclusion of legal records in addition to self-report data is
AAQ-II focuses primarily on experiential avoidance and fusion and does a strength of several studies. However variability in the measures used
not adequately represent all of the components of psychological flex- to assess constructs of interest may also influence findings. As the lit-
ibility (Francis, Dawson, & Golijani-Moghaddam, 2016). Inclusion of erature expands, the impact of measurement variability may be

41
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

clarified. The youth intervention literature remains extremely limited Therapy to problem anger. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 18(2), 241–250. https://
and would be strengthened by clear descriptions of methodology, in- doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2010.04.004.
Eisenbeck, N., Sheitz, K., & Szekeres, B. (2017). A brief Acceptance and Commitment
clusion of standardized measures, and examinations including random Therapy-based intervention among violence-prone male inmates delivered by novice
assignment. therapists. Psychology, Society, & Education, 8(3), 187. https://doi.org/10.25115/
psye.v8i3.173.
Fernandez, E., Day, A., & Boyle, G. J. (2015). Measures of anger and hostility in adults.
5.5. Conclusions and future directions Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs, 74–100. https://doi.org/
10.1016/b978-0-12-386915-9.00004-8.
Although research on the psychological flexibility model as applied Fernandez, E., & Johnson, S. L. (2016). Anger in psychological disorders: Prevalence,
presentation, etiology and prognostic implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 46,
to anger and aggression remains limited, support for this perspective 124–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.012.
has been obtained. Relationships with anger, hostility, and aggression Fontaine, R. G. (2007). Disentangling the psychology and law of instrumental and re-
have overall been consistent with the psychological flexibility model active subtypes of aggression. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 13(2), 143–165.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.13.2.143.
and ACT interventions appear promising. However, the body of litera-
Francis, A. W., Dawson, D. L., & Golijani-Moghaddam, N. (2016). The development and
ture remains limited and would be enhanced by future research em- validation of the Comprehensive assessment of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
ploying stronger methodology and a broader set of populations. processes (CompACT). Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 5(3), 134–145.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.05.003.
Gardner, F. L., & Moore, Z. E. (2008). Understanding clinical anger and violence. The
References anger avoidance model. Behavior Modification, 32, 897–912. https://doi.org/10.
1177/0145445508319282.
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and Gerhart, J. I., Heath, N. M., Fitzgerald, C., & Hoerger, M. (2013). Direct and indirect
profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth associations between experiential avoidance and reduced delay of gratification.
and Families. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 2(1–2), 9–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.
America Psychiatric Association (2013). (DSM-5)Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental 2013.02.001.
disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. Gerhart, J. I., Baker, C. N., Hoerger, M., & Ronan, G. F. (2014). Experiential avoidance
Bell, K., & Higgins, L. (2015). The impact of childhood emotional abuse and experiential and interpersonal problems: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Contextual
avoidance on maladaptive problem solving and intimate partner violence. Behavioral Behavioral Science, 3(4), 291–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.08.003.
Sciences, 5(2), 154–175. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs5020154. Greco, L. A., Lambert, W., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Psychological inflexibility in childhood
Berta, M., & Zarling, A. (In Press). A preliminary trial of an Acceptance and Commitment and adolescence: Development and evaluation of the Avoidance and Fusion
Therapy-Based program for incarcerated domestic violence offenders. Violence and Questionnaire for Youth. Psychological Assessment, 20(2), 93–102. https://doi.org/10.
Victims. 1037/1040-3590.20.2.93.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K., Orcutt, H. K., & Zettle, R. D. (2007). Harvey, S. T., Henricksen, A., Bimler, D., & Dickson, D. (2017). Addressing anger, stress,
Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire—II: and alcohol-related difficulties in the military: An ACT intervention. Military
A revised measure of psychological flexibility. Unpublished manuscript. Psychology, 29(5), 464–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/mil0000173.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., & Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., &
Zettle, R. D. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and McCurry, S. M. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a
Action Questionnaire–II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and ex- working model. The Psychological Record, 54(4), 553–578. https://doi.org/10.1007/
periential avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 676–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. bf03395492.
beth.2011.03.007. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Contextual behavioral science:
Breen, W. E., & Kashdan, T. B. (2011). Anger suppression after imagined rejection among Creating a science more adequate to the challenge of the human condition. Journal of
individuals with social anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(7), 879–887. https:// Contextual Behavioral Science, 1(1–2), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2012.09.
doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.04.009. 004.
Buss, A. H., & Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Hayes, S. C., Levin, M. E., Plumb-Vilardaga, J., Villatte, J. L., & Pistorello, J. (2013).
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 21(4), 343–349. https://doi.org/10. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and Contextual Behavioral Science: Examining
1037/h004690. the progress of a distinctive model of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy. Behavior
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Therapy, 44(2), 180–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2009.08.002.
Social Psychology, 63(3), 452–459. https://doi.org/10.1037/t00691-000. Kashdan, T. B., & McKnight, P. E. (2010). The darker side of social anxiety. Current
Cassiello-Robbins, C., & Barlow, D. H. (2016). Anger: The unrecognized emotion in Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 47–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/
emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 23(1), 66–85. https:// 0963721409359280.
doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12139. Kashdan, T. B., Breen, W. E., Afram, A., & Terhar, D. (2010). Experiential avoidance in
Cella, D., Riley, W., Stone, A., Rothrock, N., Reeve, B., Yount, S., & Hays, R. (2010). The idiographic, autobiographical memories: Construct validity and links to social an-
Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS) developed xiety, depressive, and anger symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(5), 528–534.
and tested its first wave of adult self-reported health outcome item banks: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.03.010.
2005–2008. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 63(11), 1179–1194. https://doi.org/10. Kassinove, H., & Tafrate, R. C. (2011). Application of a flexible, clinically driven approach
1016/j.jclinepi.2010.04.011. for anger reduction in the case of Mr. P. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 18(2),
Chang, D., & Hwang, S. (2017). The development of anger management program based 222–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.08.008.
on acceptance and commitment therapy for youth taekwondo players. Journal of Lee, A. H., & DiGiuseppe, R. (2018). Anger and aggression treatments: A review of meta-
Exercise Rehabilitation, 13(2), 160–167. https://doi.org/10.12965/jer.1732882.441. analyses. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.
Chawla, N., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional 2017.04.004.
approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, Leonhardt, B. L., Lysaker, P. H., Vohs, J. L., James, A. V., & Davis, L. W. (2018). The
63(9), 871–890. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20400. experience and expression of anger in posttraumatic stress disorder: The relationship
Ciarrochi, J., Bilich, L., & Godsel, C. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a mechanism of with metacognition. Journal of Mental Health, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1080/
change in Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. In R. Baer’s (Ed.). Assessing mind- 09638237.2018.1466036.
fulness and acceptance: Illuminating the processes of change (pp. 51–76). Oakland, CA: Levin, M. E., Hayes, S. C., & Vilardaga, R. (2012). Acceptance and commitment therapy:
New Harbinger Publications, Inc. Applying an iterative translational research strategy in behavior analysis. In J. G.
Connor-Smith, J. K., Compas, B. E., Wadsworth, M. E., Thomsen, A. H., & Saltzman, H. Madden (Ed.). APA handbook of behavior analysis, Vol. 2: Translating principles into
(2000). Responses to stress in adolescence: Measurement of coping and involuntary practice (pp. 455–479). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
stress responses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(6), 976–992. Novaco, R. W. (1975). Anger control: The development and evaluation of an experimental
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.68.6.976. treatment. Oxford, England: Lexington.
Deffenbacher, J. L. (2011). Cognitive-Behavioral conceptualization and treatment of Novaco, R. W. (2011). Perspectives on anger treatment: Discussion and commentary.
anger. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 18(2), 212–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 18(2), 251–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.
cbpra.2009.12.004. 2010.11.002.
Diamond, P. M., & Magaletta, P. R. (2006). The short-form Buss-Perry Aggression Novaco, R. W., & Chemtob, C. M. (2015). Violence associated with combat-related
Questionnaire (BPAQ-SF): A validation study with federal offenders. Assessment, posttraumatic stress disorder: The importance of anger. Psychological Trauma: Theory,
13(3), 227–240. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191106287666. Research, Practice, and Policy, 7(5), 485–492. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000067.
Donahue, J. J., Santanello, A., Marsiglio, M. C., & Van Male, L. M. (2017). Acceptance and Painuly, N. P., Grover, S., Gupta, N., & Mattoo, S. K. (2011). Prevalence of anger attacks
Commitment Therapy for anger dysregulation with military veterans: A pilot study. in depressive and anxiety disorders: Implications for their construct? Psychiatry and
Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 47(4), 233–241. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Clinical Neurosciences, 65(2), 165–174. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2010.
s10879-017-9361-z. 02177.x.
Eckhardt, C., Norlander, B., & Deffenbacher, J. (2004). The assessment of anger and Prinstein, M. J., Boergers, J., & Vernberg, E. M. (2001). Overt and relational aggression in
hostility: A critical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9(1), 17–43. https://doi. adolescents: Social-psychological adjustment of aggressors and victims. Journal of
org/10.1016/s1359-1789(02)00116-7. Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 30(4), 479–491. https://doi.org/10.1207/
Eifert, G. H., & Forsyth, J. P. (2011). The Application of Acceptance and Commitment s15374424jccp3004_05.
Razzaque, R. (2012). An acceptance and commitment therapy based protocol for the

42
O.V. Berkout et al. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 11 (2019) 34–43

management of acute self-harm and violence in severe mental illness. Journal of Tolin, D. F., McKay, D., Forman, E. M., Klonsky, E. D., & Thombs, B. D. (2015).
Psychiatric Intensive Care, 9(02), 72–76. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Empirically supported treatment: Recommendations for a new model. Clinical
s1742646412000258. Psychology: Science and Practice, 22(4), 317–338. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.
Reddy, M. K., Meis, L. A., Erbes, C. R., Polusny, M. A., & Compton, J. S. (2011). 12122.
Associations among experiential avoidance, couple adjustment, and interpersonal Tull, M. T., Jakupcak, M., Paulson, A., & Gratz, K. L. (2007). The role of emotional in-
aggression in returning Iraqi war veterans and their partners. Journal of Consulting expressivity and experiential avoidance in the relationship between Posttraumatic
and Clinical Psychology, 79(4), 515–520. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023929. stress disorder symptom severity and aggressive behavior among men exposed to
Ruiz, F. J., & Perete, L. (2015). Application of a relational frame theory account of psy- interpersonal violence. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 20(4), 337–351. https://doi.org/10.
chological flexibility in young children. Psicothema, 27(2), 114–119. https://doi.org/ 1080/10615800701379249.
10.7334/psicothema2014.195. Tully, L. A., & Hunt, C. (2015). Brief parenting interventions for children at risk of ex-
Shea, S. E., & Coyne, L. W. (2017). Reliance on experiential avoidance in the context of ternalizing behavior problems: A systematic review. Journal of Child and Family
relational aggression: Links to internalizing and externalizing problems and dys- Studies, 25(3), 705–719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-015-0284-6.
phoric mood among urban, minority adolescent girls. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Veenstra, L., Bushman, B. J., & Koole, S. L. (2018). The facts on the furious: A brief review
Science, 6(2), 195–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.03.001. of the psychology of trait anger. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 98–103. https://
Shorey, R. C., Elmquist, J., Zucosky, H., Febres, J., Brasfield, H., & Stuart, G. L. (2014). doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.014.
Experiential avoidance and male dating violence perpetration: An initial investiga- Wilson, K. G., Sandoz, E. K., Kitchens, J., & Roberts, M. (2011). The Valued Living
tion. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3(2), 117–123. https://doi.org/10. Questionnaire: Defining and measuring valued action within a behavioral framework.
1016/j.jcbs.2014.02.003. The Psychological Record, 60(2), 249–272. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03395706.
Siegel, J. M. (1986). The Multidimensional Anger Inventory. Journal of Personality and Yavuz, K. F., Şahin, O., Ulusoy, S., Ipek, O. U., & Kurt, E. (2016). Experiential avoidance,
Social Psychology, 51(1), 191–200. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.1.191. empathy, and anger-related attitudes in antisocial personality disorder. Turkish
Spielberger, C. D. (1988). Professional manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory Journal of Medical Sciences, 46, 1792–1800. https://doi.org/10.3906/sag-1601-80.
(STAXI). Odessa, FL, USA: Psychological Assessment Resources. Zarling, A., Lawrence, E., & Marchman, J. (2015). A randomized controlled trial of ac-
Spielberger, C. D. (1999). State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-Second Edition. Odessa, FL, ceptance and commitment therapy for aggressive behavior. Journal of Consulting and
USA: Psychological Assessment Resources. Clinical Psychology, 83(1), 199–212. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037946.
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The Revised Zarling, A., Bannon, S., & Berta, M. (2017a). Evaluation of Acceptance and Commitment
Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS–2). Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283–316. https://doi. Therapy for domestic violence offenders. Psychology of Violence Advance. https://doi.
org/10.1177/019251396017003001. org/10.1037/vio0000097 Online Publication.
Theodore-Oklota, C., Orsillo, S. M., Lee, J. K., & Vernig, P. M. (2014). A pilot of an Zarling, A., & Berta, M. (2017b). An Acceptance and Commitment Therapy approach for
acceptance-based risk reduction program for relational aggression for adolescents. partner aggression. Partner Abuse, 8(1), 89–109. https://doi.org/10.1891/1946-
Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3(2), 109–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 6560.8.1.89.
jcbs.2014.03.001.

43

You might also like