You are on page 1of 13

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Behavior Therapy 43 (2012) 36 – 48


www.elsevier.com/locate/bt

Common Factors of Change in Couple Therapy


Sean D. Davis
Alliant International University

Jay L. Lebow
Family Institute at Northwestern University

Douglas H. Sprenkle
Purdue University

cate that these variables account for far more


Though it is clear from meta-analytic research that couple variance than the unique contributions of any
therapy works well, it is less clear how couple therapy particular model (Sprenkle & Blow, 2004). A
works. Efforts to attribute change to the unique ingredients more thorough understanding of these pantheore-
of a particular model have consistently turned up short, tical variables, generally referred to as “common
leading many researchers to suggest that change is due to factors,” how and when they are activated and
common factors that run through different treatment how they are woven into empirically based,
approaches and settings. The purpose of this article is to model-driven treatment could greatly simplify
provide an empirically based case for several common and refine couple therapy research and training,
factors in couple therapy, and discuss clinical, training, and and ultimately maximize the effectiveness of
research implications for a common factors couple therapy couple therapy. While the data point to the
paradigm. Critical distinctions between model-driven and importance of common factors, much of the
common factors paradigms are also discussed, and a relevant research in couple therapy has yet to be
moderate common factors approach is proposed as a more done. Nevertheless, enough data exist to make a
useful alternative to an extreme common factors approach. strong case for the ideas presented in this article.
The purpose of this article is to outline the main
tenets of a common factors paradigm, provide an
Keywords: couple therapy; common factors; outcome research overview of what is known about the common
factors, and to outline research, training, and
clinical implications. Interested readers can find a
AS DISCUSSED IN THE introduction to this special
more in-depth discussion of these issues in
section (Halford & Snyder, 2012-this issue), the
Sprenkle, Davis, and Lebow (2009).
efficacy of couple therapy is well established. What
is less clear, however, is how couple therapy Common Factors and Model-Driven Change:
works. Data suggest that successful couple therapy Two Paradigms of How Couples Change
consists of a complex interaction of numerous
The common factors paradigm stands as an
pantheoretical variables, and initial findings indi-
alternative to the model-driven change paradigm.
Though we believe there are significant differences
between the two paradigms, we also acknowledge
Address correspondence to Sean D. Davis, Alliant International that polarizing two paradigms overemphasizes
University, Marital and Family Therapy, 2030 West El Camino Avenue, differences and underemphasizes similarities. We
Suite 200, Sacramento, CA 95833; e-mail: sdavis2@alliant.edu.
outline below what we see as the polarities of these
0005-7894/xx/xxx-xxx/$1.00/0
© 2011 Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. Published by two paradigms, and discuss later our preferred
Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. “moderate” common factors approach. The
common factors of change in couple therapy 37

traditional model-driven paradigm assumes that the The State of the Research Surrounding
primary explanation for change rests within the Common Factors
unique elements and mechanisms of individual
Randomized clinical trials (RCTs) are widely
treatment models. Conversely, the common factors
considered the gold standard for establishing
paradigm assumes that common mechanisms of
treatment efficacy. In both individual and couple
change cut across all effective psychotherapies, and
therapy (though there are far fewer RCTs in couple
that models are the vehicles through which
therapy) RCTs routinely demonstrate that the
common factors are potentiated (Sprenkle &
model being tested outperforms treatment as usual
Blow, 2004).
and wait-list control conditions. Claims of efficacy
Taken to the extreme, the model-driven para-
solidify as RCTs accumulate for a particular model.
digm suggests that treatments are “dispensed” in
However, meta-analytic reviews of RCTs suggest
ways analogous to drugs and medical procedures.
that when RCTs are compared to each other,
In contrast, a common factors approach is more
differences in treatments largely disappear, partic-
contextual, emphasizing the interaction of com-
ularly when controlling for confounding variables
plex variables affecting treatment such as client
such as researcher allegiance (Wampold, 2001). In
and therapist variables, alliance, and expectancy.
other words, demonstrating efficacy relative to
These latter factors are viewed as more important
treatment as usual and wait-list controls does not
than the unique contributions of specific models.
equal efficacy relative to other models.
The model-driven change paradigm often deem-
phasizes the therapist's role in change, instead Meta-analytic research that calculates an effect size
focusing on the treatment dispensed. The com- by comparing diverse studies is sometimes criticized as
mon factors approach, on the other hand, confounding samples, treatments, presenting prob-
emphasizes that treatment models do not exist lems, and outcome measures. Inclusion of methodo-
in therapy outside of the therapist delivering logically weak studies can further confound meta-
them, and therefore the qualities of the therapist analytic results. Nevertheless, most meta-analytic
delivering the treatment are more important than reviews show few if any differences between treat-
the treatment itself. ments, and several show that this finding is as strong
The model-driven paradigm is more therapist- when comparing only methodologically sound
centric in that it emphasizes the importance of the studies as it is when including weak studies (Shadish
therapist performing therapy in a certain manner, & Baldwin, 2002; Smith & Glass, 1977). While there
and the client falling into step with what the are several possible explanations of this finding (e.g.,
therapist recommends. Conversely, the common different models may use different yet equally potent
factors paradigm is more client-centric in that change mechanisms), we believe the common factors
models—regardless of whether they are therapist hypothesis is the most consistent with existing data.
or client centered—are adapted to a client's Even most studies that represent the ideal situation—
unique needs and world views, and clients use an RCT with at least two established models that
whatever is offered in therapy in unique and controls for confounding variables such as researcher
idiosyncratic ways not necessarily predicted by allegiance—fail to show significant differences
the model. among treatments (Elkin et al., 1989), but showed
The model-driven change paradigm has always significant differences among such common factors
been the more prominent of the two approaches. as therapists characteristics (Blatt, Sanislow, Zuroff,
Models are more exciting and interesting than the & Pilkonis, 1996). Increasingly the debate is not over
factors that all good therapies possess. Perhaps it whether but how much common factors influence
is no surprise that most couple therapy training outcome (Chambless & Ollendick, 2001; Orlinsky,
programs emphasize models and rarely directly Grawe, & Parks, 1994).
address common factors. Today, most federal As research methods become more complex, so
research funding goes toward this paradigm, and may our understanding of the change process.
advocacy of “evidence-supported treatments” has Therefore, it may be better to expand RCTs rather
become commonplace. The common factors ap- than do away with them altogether. We believe that
proach, while enjoying intuitive appeal among an investigation of common factors and processes
many clinicians and a growing number of re- could be woven into an RCT, thus allowing for a
searchers, has a long way to go before it enjoys a more refined view of the change process. However,
similar seat at the cultural table as the model- although reliable differences in treatment models
driven change paradigm. We hope that this article may be discovered, we believe it is unlikely that one
will help bring the common factors paradigm into model will be shown to be universally more
the mainstream. effective than others. We believe it is more likely
38 davis et al.

that research will discover certain change processes feedback, which presupposes the flexibility discussed
and mechanisms that mediate change during certain above, has been shown to be a strong predictor of
stages of successful therapy, but that many different change (Halford et al., 2012-this issue). Some evidence
models can use specific methods to facilitate those suggests a better fit between certain clients and
change mechanisms. In fact, several couple therapy approaches, with lower-socioeconomic status clients
RCT studies that incorporate process research often preferring more directive, skills-focused ap-
suggest that similar processes such as softening of proaches than their higher-socioeconomic counter-
emotional expressiveness and responsiveness in parts (Cline, Mejia, Coles, Klein, & Cline, 1984).
emotionally focused therapy (EFT; Johnson & Matching the type and pacing of interventions with
Greenberg, 1985) and communication, behavior client characteristics also seems to be important, as
change in target problems, and acceptance of target emotionally reactive clients have been shown to prefer
problems in integrative and traditional behavioral inductive, eliciting approaches with high structure,
couple therapies (IBCT, TBCT, respectively; Doss, whereas less reactive clients may prefer less structure
Thum, Sevier, Atkins, & Christensen, 2005) (Beutler, Malik, & Alimohamed, 2004). Furthermore,
mediate change. Conceptual overlap between the some dyadic approaches do a better job of meeting
processes measured in both studies could lend certain unique challenges (e.g., substance abuse,
support to the common factors hypothesis that Powers, Vedel, & Emmelkamp, 2008; depression,
similar processes are impacted by different models Bodenman et al., 2008; intimate partner violence,
using different interventions. Stith, Rosen, McCollum, & Thomsen, 2004) than
Nuanced findings mentioned above may provide individual approaches, though little is known about
insight into which processes are at work at which how or why.
stage of therapy with which types of clients and Our moderate common factors approach suggests
presenting problems. Such research could suggest that because most models can facilitate similar
that the focus on processes or principles (e.g., couple processes, it is likely that most models will
facilitating softening) take precedence over a focus find a use either throughout or at different points in
on models. In other words, the model (or component the therapeutic process with most clients. This could
of the model) would be selected based on the degree explain why most tested couple therapy models are
to which it facilitated the requisite process, and the effective, but none significantly more than another—
requisite process would be determined by client because they all do an adequate job of invoking the
feedback (Halford et al., 2012-this issue), the stage of right change mechanisms at the right times. It could
therapy, unique issues relevant to the couple's also explain why treatment effects for many
presenting problem, and other variables discovered couples decrease significantly at 2-year follow-up
as research progressed. This client-centered ap- (Halford & Snyder, 2012-this issue)—because
proach would still be empirically informed, but treatment was not nuanced enough to adequately
much more flexible and adaptive (and therefore meet their long-term needs. It could also explain
presumably effective) than the dominant “hammer/ the wide variation in therapist effectiveness (Blatt
nail” approach in which a client is expected to et al., 1996)—some therapists may simply be better
conform to the efficacious treatment rather than vice at judging what clients need (or at eliciting client
versa (Blow, Sprenkle, & Davis, 2007). feedback about the same) at key times (Halford et
This approach to couple therapy could be similar al., 2012-this issue), and have a broader reservoir
to Prochaska's widely used model in substance of approaches to help treatment progress accord-
abuse treatment that focuses on selecting the type of ing to the client's needs. Future research is needed
treatment approach based on the client's “stage of to test these hypotheses.
change” (Prochaska & Norcross, 2009). A similar
integrative couple therapy approach has been Common Factors Shared by Individual and
advocated by Snyder (1999). We also applaud Couple Therapy
Christensen's (2010) similar efforts to develop a Most of the direct empirical support for common
treatment protocol that uses five general principles factors comes from the individual therapy literature.
to guide the selection of different methods from While much of what makes a good individual
effective couple therapy approaches throughout the therapist likely also makes a good couple therapist,
course of treatment. We believe process- and it is possible that there are differences (Blow et al.,
principle-based approaches such as these will be a 2007). However, in most cases the limited direct
promising major direction in future couple therapy inquiry into common factors in couple therapy tem-
practice, research, and training. pers assertions linking common factors in individual
Indirect research supports process- and principle- therapy to couple therapy. Research reviewed below is
based approaches. Adapting treatment based on client for individual therapy unless otherwise indicated.
common factors of change in couple therapy 39

client variables (1997) found that EFT worked best with couples
We believe that clients are the most important who believed the tasks of EFT were related to their
variable in therapy. Bohart and Tallman (2010) problem. Jacobson and Christensen (1996) found
propose that the reason a number of approaches that traditional behavioral marital therapy worked
work equally well is because clients are adept at best with couples with high commitment and low
taking whatever a therapist offers and tailoring it to “traditionality” and similar goals for the marriage.
suit their needs. An excellent therapist can master- Some client characteristics appear to have an
fully deliver the most up-to-date treatment avail- influence on outcome. Individual comorbid psy-
able, but if the client lacks awareness, is unwilling chopathology has been associated with poorer
to take responsibility for changing, or otherwise outcomes in behavioral couple therapy treatments
does not engage in treatment, it will all be for of alcohol use disorder, though less so than
naught. Conversely, a highly motivated, proactive individual treatments for the same disorder
client who focuses on what their treatment has to (McGrady, Epstein, Cook, Jensen, & Hildebrandt,
offer rather than where it is lacking will likely shape 2009). Initial levels of relationship distress, sexual
whatever is offered to their betterment. dissatisfaction, and increased length of marriage
In keeping with our moderate common factors have been negatively related to outcome in integra-
approach (discussed later), we believe that therapists tive and traditional behavioral marital therapy
and models almost always do matter. Models (Atkins et al., 2005). As discussed earlier, some
provide a critical framework for treatment, without problems respond better to couples than individual
which many of the common factors may not be treatments. While conjoint treatment is clearly
potentiated. For example, client motivation may indicated for some problems, many conjoint
suffer if the therapist, as informed by their model, approaches could prove effective. In other words,
does not offer clients a more adaptive way of while some problems may benefit more from a
experiencing their problem. However, the ability to specialized class of approach, several treatments
convince the clients that the model is credible may be that share core characteristics could likely be
more important than the actual content of the model. developed for that class. For example, conduct-
The ability to present the approach convincingly disordered children and their families typically
would be the common factor. A poor therapist could respond best to approaches that are structured,
thwart a motivated couple, just as an excellent ecologically/systemically based, highly involved,
therapist could engage a couple with low motivation focused somewhat on the individual teenager, and
or present most models in such a way that fit with the use assertive methods of treatment engagement.
client's worldview. Conversely, a poor fit between However, there are at least four such empirically
the model and the client's worldview can lead to validated approaches, none of which have been
treatment dropout even if the clients are otherwise shown to be differentially effective relative to the
engaged and the therapist is typically effective others. While future research may shed light on
(Johnson & Talitman, 1997). Couples in which one which presenting problems or client characteristics
partner is “just visiting” or divorce is being actively benefit from a specialized focus as well as the
discussed often do poorly. Nonetheless, given that necessary elements of that focus, we believe it is
most therapists are at least somewhat effective and unlikely that there will ever be a problem or client
most models likely have something to offer most characteristic for which only one theoretical ap-
people, client characteristics remain the most potent proach is indicated.
determinant of outcome. Client motivation and engagement is likely to be
Despite this, research thus far sheds remarkably a more complex issue when dealing with two people
little light on client variables that affect couple with disparate levels of motivation using disparate
therapy outcome. Clinical trials research typically methods to work toward sometimes disparate
treats client variation as a source of error rather goals. Davis and Piercy (2007a, 2007b), in their
than an opportunity for discovery. Furthermore, qualitative study, outline several characteristics
studies often group couples based on diagnosis, exhibited by clients as they moved through the
ignoring individual characteristics that may be a process of successful couple therapy, as well as
larger contributor to change. Most research on what the therapists did to nurture these character-
client variables in couple therapy focuses on static istics and counter negative characteristics. These
characteristics such as age, gender, sexual orienta- included humility (operationalized as a willingness
tion, and race. Few studies focus on variables more to take personal responsibility for their role in the
likely related to outcome such as motivation, locus problems and a willingness to risk/be vulnerable),
of control, and engagement in treatment (Blow commitment to each other and the process of
et al., 2007). For example, Johnson and Talitman therapy, psychological and systemic awareness, and
40 davis et al.

hard work. Despite promising preliminary findings, therapist variables are ignored, which makes
we have much to learn about how client charac- differences among treatments appear larger than
teristics influence outcome in couple therapy, and they really are. Others (Blatt et al., 1996) report
if and/or how therapists can influence these similar findings. These are therapists in well-
characteristics. controlled outcome studies; it is likely that there is
far more variance among therapists in everyday
client feedback clinical practice.
Given the importance of the client's perception of There is so much variance among therapists that
therapy and the tendency of therapists to misjudge Blow et al. (2007) propose that it may be better to
the client's experience of therapy it follows that talk about empirically validated therapists than
formally eliciting feedback and making requisite empirically validated therapy. Therapeutic models
adjustments to the course of treatment is another come alive or die largely through the therapist. As
potential common factor (Halford et al., 2012-this common factors work through models, models
issue). Receiving formal client feedback about the work through therapists. What, then, are common
progress of therapy has been shown to improve factors of effective therapists? Surprisingly, little is
outcome regardless of whether that feedback is known about this question. Many of the variables
delivered via the clients themselves (Anker, Duncan, we review below account for relatively small
& Sparks, 2009) or a clinical supervisor systemati- amounts of the variance (Blow et al., 2007).
cally monitoring client progress (Lambert et al., Static characteristics (e.g., age, gender, race)
2001). Though the strong benefits of providing account for little variance, providing hope that a
treatment progress feedback is more established in competent therapist can overcome whatever initial
the individual therapy literature, initial findings in limitations may be presented by such variables
the couple therapy literature suggest a similar pattern (Blow et al., 2007). Experience level is another
(Halford et al.). Anker et al. found that couples variable that has surprisingly shown to not account
whose therapist received direct feedback about for much of the variance. In one study (Stolk &
treatment progress “achieved nearly 4 times the Perlesz, 1990), first-year students performed better
rate of clinically significant change, and maintained a than second-year students, possibly because first-
significant advantage on the primary measure at 6- year students focused more on forming an alliance
month follow-up while attaining a significantly and second-year students, having gotten more
lower rate of separation or divorce” (p. 693). comfortable in the room, focused more on tech-
nique at the expense of the alliance. It is also
therapist variables possible that experienced therapists work with
Buried among the data of what is likely the most more difficult cases.
methodologically sound psychotherapy outcome A friendly, positive therapist tends to get better
study to date, the National Institute of Mental results than a critical or hostile therapist, or a
Health Collaborative Depression Study (Elkin therapist who is defensive, particularly early in
et al., 1989) is a surprising finding: despite strong therapy. Effective therapists also tend to be sensitive
efforts to control for therapist factors (e.g., training, to the unique needs of their clients and flexible in
experience level, model allegiance), there was far their treatment approaches, offering insight-oriented
more variability in effectiveness across therapists interventions to more reflective clients, and skill-
than across treatment approaches. In fact, the most building interventions to more impulsive or aggres-
consistently effective therapist was a psychiatrist sive clients (Beutler et al., 2004). Effective couple
who split her work between the antidepressant drug therapists tend to be active enough to interrupt
clinical management and placebo clinical manage- destructive couple interaction, but not so active
ment conditions. She did not participate in either that couples do not learn how to communicate
the cognitive-behavioral or interpersonal therapy effectively on their own. Cultural and worldview
conditions; her “therapy” consisted of meeting sensitivity has also been shown to be important
briefly with clients with the main goal of being (Blow et al., 2007).
supportive (Blatt et al., 1996)!
Wampold (2001), in his major meta-analysis of therapeutic alliance
studies comparing bona fide treatments, found that The therapeutic alliance is the most studied
differences among therapists contribute more to common factor in couple therapy. A strong
outcome variance (effect size of 0.60) than the therapeutic alliance is generally seen as a necessary
treatment models they used (at most an effect size of component of effective therapy (Sprenkle et al.,
0.20, but probably closer to zero). Furthermore, 2009). Since the therapeutic alliance is more
Type 1 errors are significantly inflated when complex in couple than in individual therapy, and
common factors of change in couple therapy 41

it is so strongly related to outcome, we will discuss hope/expectancy


the alliance in greater detail later. Although called by different terms, many empirically
Though the alliance is clearly related to outcome, validated couple or family therapy models focus
it is less clear whether that relationship is correla- initial efforts largely on fostering hope (Sprenkle &
tional or causal. It is possible that if therapy is Blow, 2004). In couple therapy, the relationship of
proceeding well, clients may report a more positive hope to outcome is largely theoretical; we know of no
alliance. Clients may also report a more positive quantitative studies investigating the relationship
alliance based on a third variable, such as changes between hope and outcome. Ward and Wampler
in his or her life outside of therapy, or the strength (2010) and Davis and Piercy (2007b) used qualitative
of the referral source. Future research is needed to methods to investigate hope. Ward and Wampler
determine the direction and strength of the rela- define hope as “a belief and feeling that a desired
tionship between alliance and outcome. outcome is possible” (p. 216). Results of interviews
with several marriage and family therapists suggest
therapist/researcher allegiance that hope consists of the partially overlapping
concepts of options (belief in one's ability to choose
If a therapist or researcher strongly believes in the
a path to desired outcomes), evidence (can clients see
approach he or she is using or studying, that belief
that the desired outcome is likely to occur?),
itself may increase the likelihood of success. This
connection (hope instilled by a connection to other
passion likely spreads to the couple, increasing the
human beings, a higher power, etc.), and actions
likelihood that the couple views the approach as a
(one's willingness to act to reach desired outcomes).
credible path to relational health (Davis & Piercy,
Ward and Wampler discuss a general trajectory of
2007a). People are more likely to “buy” something
hope across successful treatment (from the therapist's
the person “selling” it believes in. In other words,
perspective), including pantheoretical interventions
the therapist may be more important than the
couple therapists can use to facilitate hope at
therapy (Blow et al., 2007).
different stages of treatment.
Researchers conducting outcome studies typically
Davis and Piercy (2007b) found several concepts
choose models they either developed or are passion-
related to hope in their interviews with couple
ate about and typically recruit therapist participants
therapy model developers, their former students,
who share their passion, and that passion influences
and their former clients. Trusted referral sources led
outcome. In a study of psychotherapies, Luborsky
to more hope that clients could be helped, which led
et al. (2002) coded allegiance effects three different
them to give therapists more benefit of the doubt in
ways across 29 different outcome studies, finding
the crucial early sessions of therapy. This trust in
that “The correlation between the mean of 3
the referral source was amplified by the therapist's
measures of the researcher's allegiance and the
actual competence, as defined by the ability of the
outcome of the treatments compared was a huge
therapist to fit the model to each client's worldview
Pearson's r of .85 for a sample of 29 comparative
in a way that provided a credible road map out of
treatment studies” (p. 5). Wampold (2001) presents
their problems. Motivational beliefs, metaphors,
similar evidence suggesting that what outcome
and experiences were used throughout to help keep
researchers think are treatment effects may rather
the larger picture in mind when working through
be allegiance effects.
difficult issues in therapy.

organization and coherence provided non-model-specific mechanisms


by a model of change
What is it about a therapeutic model that facil- Non-model-specific mechanisms of change exist
itates healing? We concur with Frank's assertion across different models regardless of model-specific
(Frank & Frank, 1991) that the organization, theoretical assumptions and techniques. They are
coherence, and healing rituals provided by the sometimes referred to as “narrow” (as a subset of
model are far more important than the actual “broad” common factors, such as the alliance)
content of the model. If a model provides ground- common factors (Sprenkle & Blow, 2004). These
ing in something the client's culture values as mechanisms exist at a lower level of abstraction
relevant to healing (for couple therapy, scientifi- than theory and a higher level of abstraction than
cally based principles of healthy relationships), technique. For example, while narrative and
provides credible healing rituals (i.e., interven- cognitive-behavioral couple therapists have differ-
tions), and a structure within which these rituals ent conceptual lenses and employ different tech-
make sense to the client, the model will likely help niques (e.g., re-storying and the downward arrow,
clients be successful. respectively), both achieve the same goal of helping
42 davis et al.

partners see each other in a “softer” light, which same as comparing conjoint vs. individual treat-
will in turn help them treat each other more kindly. ments), the clear effectiveness of systemic approaches
“Changing the viewing” is the common change that embrace this common factor relative to individ-
mechanism. Sprenkle and Blow modified Karasu's ual approaches provides strong indirect evidence.
(1986) list of nonspecific change mechanisms to the Some issues reviewed earlier achieve a more durable
following three categories: changing the doing, resolution in systemic treatment. In their review of 20
changing the thinking, and changing the feeling. meta-analyses of couple and family therapy, Shadish
Common change mechanisms account for the and Baldwin (2002) note that the average effect size
overlap among models in their natural form. If an for couple therapy is 0.84, with the average for
emotionally focused therapist, for example, is trying family therapy being 0.58. The couple therapy effect
to adapt his or her approach to a more cognitively size is strong (stronger than many medical treat-
oriented couple, his or her work may start to look ments); the family therapy effect size is moderate,
very similar to a cognitive-behavioral therapist and possibly due to the difficulty of getting everyone in a
vice versa for a cognitive-behavioral therapist family happy about a particular outcome or the
working with a more affectively oriented couple. difficulty of problems typically addressed in family
We commend recent efforts by model developers to therapy.
discuss their models in more pluralistic terms, and
believe that the more this happens, the easier it will disrupting dysfunctional relational
be for therapists to master more than one approach patterns
and ultimately better adapt to a diverse clientele. If conceptualizing problems in terms of relational
Dattilio (2010), for example, goes to great lengths patterns is a common factor of effective couple
to incorporate emotions into a cognitive-behavioral therapy, it follows that disrupting those patterns is
framework, helping his approach resonate with a also a common factor. Similar indirect evidence
wider range of therapists. exists for this common factor as exists for concep-
tualizing problems in relational terms. Davis and
Common Factors Unique to Couple Therapy Piercy (2007a, 2007b) found that therapists using
Many of the common factors reviewed above apply different couple therapies described conceptualizing
to individual therapy as well as couple therapy. problems in terms of interactional cycles (i.e.,
There may be a few important differences, however, relational patterns), and focused the majority of
between common factors in individual and couple their interventions on altering these patterns.
therapy (Sprenkle et al., 2009). Furthermore, interactional cycles were found to
consist of cognitive, behavioral, and affective
conceptualizing difficulties in domains, all of which were targets of intervention.
relational terms For example, a wife thinks her husband does not
Many symptoms become understandable responses care about her when he comes home late (cognition)
to untenable circumstances when a therapist steps so she gets angry (affect) and yells at him every
back and looks at a couple's larger context. If a time he is late (behavior). Her husband thinks his
therapist only heard the husband's story about his wife is a nag (cognition) and feels intimidated by her
nagging wife, the therapist may ultimately side with (affect) so he comes home late each night (behav-
the husband, downplaying his role in the couple's ior). A therapist would have six possible points
distress and unfairly demonizing the wife. Such an of intervention into this interactional cycle, all of
approach could unwittingly undermine the mar- which could shift the entire pattern. If the wife
riage and subtly lead the husband toward separa- realized that she felt abandoned rather than angry,
tion or divorce. However, once the therapist and she was able to express that to her husband
brought the wife into treatment, her nagging will rather than nagging, he may soften toward her and
likely become an understandable response to the stop thinking she's a nag and start coming home on
husband's withdrawing, for example. These recip- time, to which she may respond more kindly. Or if
rocal interactions in which one person's symptoms the therapist helped the husband start coming home
become understandable only in the context of their on time no matter what, it may soften the wife's
partner's symptoms, and each are presumed to thoughts about and feelings toward him, which
exacerbate and maintain the other, is a hallmark of would shift the cycle, and so forth. While some
systemic couple therapy (with possible exceptions, models focus more on one aspect of the cycle than
such as domestic violence). others (e.g., EFT focuses on emotions more than
While there is little direct evidence for this common CBT, which focuses more on cognitions and
factor (i.e., comparing relationship conceptualiza- behaviors), most systemic models focus on each
tion to individual conceptualization, which is not the aspect to a degree.
common factors of change in couple therapy 43

expanded direct treatment system Extreme Versus Moderate Common Factors


A systemically oriented therapist typically works with Approaches
as many members of a client's system as possible. If Although there is general agreement on what
working with an individual, the same therapist will common factors are, there is disagreement about
still incorporate a thorough understanding of the the clinical and research implications of common
client's systems into assessment, conceptualization, factors. Many scholars have taken what we consider
and intervention. Symptoms are seen as best under- an extreme “either/or” stance on common factors.
stood in the context of the client's interactions with For example, it is common to hear “All models work
micro (e.g., spouse or family) and/or macro (e.g., the same; therefore models do not matter,” “A strong
larger cultural and political) systems. The more therapeutic alliance is all that is needed for success,”
people in the system who change, the more durable or “Because meta-analyses reveal that no significant
that change is likely to be. Greater efficacy for several differences between treatments exist, comparative
problems discussed earlier provides substantial direct efficacy research is useless and should stop.” We
evidence for this common factor. reject these and similar extreme statements as reach-
expanded therapeutic alliance ing beyond what the data justify, and believe they run
the risk of throwing the proverbial baby out with the
There is ample evidence supporting the therapeutic
bath water. Our “both/and” moderate common
alliance as a potent common factor. A couple
factors approach (Sprenkle et al., 2009) takes a
therapist must monitor not only his or her alliance
more centrist stance, and draws five important
with each partner, but their alliance with each other
distinctions.
and the overall alliance as a group working
together. This is particularly challenging when belief that one treatment is as good
working in the highly emotional climate that so as another versus claims of relative
often characterizes couple therapy in which one or efficacy
both partners is prone to interpret therapist Many common factors proponents cite meta-
attempts to validate the partner as a personal analytic research in which no model has consis-
rejection. When the alliance is strong, it tends to tently shown to be more efficacious than another
amplify the effects of treatment. When it is weak, (Shadish & Baldwin, 2002) as evidence that all
therapy often does not last long enough for models are the same. “The dodo bird verdict”—a
interventions to take effect (Sprenkle et al., 2009). phrase borrowed from Alice in Wonderland in
Therefore, monitoring, maintaining, and repairing which all have won and must have prizes—is often
the alliance is a critical task of couple therapy. used to make this point (Luborsky et al., 2002).
The alliance is more than the emotional “bond” Taken to the extreme, the dodo bird renders models
between clients and therapist, including also tasks irrelevant, and implies that it does not matter what
and goals (Horvath, 1994). In addition to feeling you do in therapy provided you have a good
emotionally connected to their therapist (bond), alliance, a motivated client, and so forth.
clients and therapist have to agree regarding where We believe the data support claims that among
they are headed (goals) and how to get there (tasks). efficacious couple therapies, there is little evidence for
In relational therapy, the relationship is the client differences in treatment outcome (Shadish & Baldwin,
rather than the individuals within the relationship. 2002). However, just because tested models have not
This requires nuanced skills at balancing the been shown to be differentially or relatively efficacious
alliance so each partner feels validated as the vis-à-vis each other does not mean they are irrelevant.
therapist listens to them, yet not threatened while On the contrary, most tested couple therapy models
the therapist validates his or her partner, who is show significant improvement, and differences in
often saying inflammatory things. There is some outcome between the model group and treatment as
research suggesting that balancing the alliance is usual group in most RCTs suggest that the model adds
even more important than the strength of the something essential to treatment (Shadish & Baldwin,
alliance (Sprenkle & Blow, 2004). The alliance in 2002). Using a well-defined, tested couple therapy
couple therapy is very complex. Space limitations approach such as EFT (Johnson, 2004) ensures a
prevent a thorough discussion of all the nuances of consistently applied effective therapy, whereas the
forming, monitoring, maintaining, and repairing impact of untested or atheoretical “improvisational”
the alliance in couple therapy. Brief practical therapy remains unknown. It may be that some of
guidance for maintaining a systemic alliance is those therapies do not include the core effective
found later in the “Clinical Implications” section. ingredients of successful couple therapy.
Interested readers are referred to Sprenkle et al. We believe it likely from the findings of meta-
(2009) for a more thorough review. analyses (Shadish & Baldwin, 2002) that successful
44 davis et al.

results will also emerge from many (but not all) as largely political—they also form the foundation for
yet untested couple therapies such as narrative the meta-analytic studies that led to the common
therapy and others. To the extent that such theories factors paradigm. Furthermore, while many com-
are based on sound psychological and systemic mon factors cannot be experimentally manipulated,
principles, provide adequate organization and some (e.g., therapist factors, client motivation,
coherence, share common elements, and are imple- therapist belief in and degree of adherence to the
mented by competent therapists, they would likely model) could easily be added to the list of variables
(but not certainly) be proven efficacious. Rarely is a measured in a clinical trial. While many of these
model meeting those criteria for bona fide treat- variables are best suited for quantitative and
ments found not to be efficacious in clinical trials qualitative process research designs, clinical trials
(Wampold, 2001). Nevertheless, we remain open to could still provide useful information about common
the possibility that differences may be discovered as factors. Again, we stress the need not to throw the
research methodologies and questions become baby out with the bath water.
more refined.
“either–or” versus “both–and”
disparages rather than supports As discussed earlier, we are open to contemporary
treatment models common factors claims being subject to future
We believe that well-defined models are an indis- refinement or disconfirmation as research method-
pensible component of good therapy. Models help a ologies become more complex. Some of this is
therapist know how and when to intervene and to already happening; in individual therapy there
what purpose. Models provide necessary order and appears to be reasonable evidence that certain
structure to therapy. Models are like a road map that specific problems such as phobias, panic disorder,
directs therapists through the maze of information and compulsions respond better to behavioral and
present in therapy and orients therapists and clients cognitive approaches, and that these results cannot
to what is wrong, proposes viable alternatives for be explained by the usual mediating and moderat-
helping things go right, and techniques, skills, and ing variables in outcome research (Sprenkle &
rituals for helping clients go from one to the other as Blow, 2004). Similarly, certain types of family
they adapt the therapy's offerings to their needs in problems respond better to certain types of therapy,
their own way. Our only contention is with the claim though there are many similarities among the
that models work primarily through mechanisms effective therapies for this population. Similarly,
that are unique to that model. Rather, we believe couple therapies that have been shown to be
effective models are the vehicles through which effective are all structured, include a caring,
common factors operate. involved therapist who monitors and works with
the interactions, builds skills, promotes emotional
primarily emphasizes the therapeutic engagement, provides a calm holding environment
relationship rather than views the for dealing with negative highly charged feelings,
relationship as only one aspect promotes communication, and targets the building
of change of the relationship system. We believe that such
Common factors proponents often say “The common factors account for more variance than
therapeutic alliance is all that matters.” We believe specific factors, though we acknowledge this may
that while the therapeutic alliance is a necessary not be the case for every population.
factor of successful therapy, it is not sufficient, in
part because it does not stand separate from other A Common Factors Couple Therapy
variables in therapy. For example, whether the Metamodel
client views a therapist's theory and interventions as The common factors movement is sometimes
credible has an influence on the alliance (Sprenkle criticized as providing little more than lists of
& Blow, 2004). variables without any guidance as to how and when
these variables interact to produce change (Sexton
minimizes rather than supports & Ridley, 2004). Similar to the principle- and
clinical trials research process-based approaches discussed earlier, a com-
As discussed earlier, we are more optimistic about the mon factors metamodel could address this problem
usefulness of clinical trials research than some of our by outlining a framework of conceptual principles
common factors colleagues whose work we other- and mechanisms of change that could be super-
wise admire (Wampold, 2001). Not only do clinical imposed over diverse clinical approaches, allowing
trials establish the efficacy of therapy to third-party therapists to move between models as indicated by
stakeholders—an important contribution, even if general principles and client needs.
common factors of change in couple therapy 45

Davis and Piercy (2007a, 2007b) used qualitative clients. Participants also focused on their subjective
grounded theory methodology to develop a prelim- memory of therapy rather than objective observa-
inary metamodel. Diverse couple therapy model tions of the therapy by trained coders. Despite the
developers and proponents, their former students, limitations inherent in this study specifically, and in
and former clients of both were asked what they qualitative research generally, we believe that
thought made therapy successful. Participants formal inductive inquiry is the best way to generate
described couples as entering therapy without a relevant hypotheses during the formative stages of
clear problem definition or a workable plan for developing a new approach such as a common
resolving their problems, leading them to feel factor metamodel.
hopeless. Conversely, the therapist described hav- Training, Clinical, and Research Implications of
ing hope and direction in the form of their therapy
model and experience. Clients reported increased a Moderate Common Factors Approach
hope as they adopted their therapist's way of clinical implications
viewing their situation. This adoption seemed to Providing clinical guidance for each common factor
happen provided that (a) the therapist's explana- is beyond the scope of this article. Interested readers
tion of their difficulties fit the client's experience, can find such guidance in Sprenkle et al. (2009). We
(b) the therapist was viewed as competent (which limit our discussion to the therapeutic alliance
seemed to be related to the first concept), (c) the because it is one of the most robust yet complex
therapist proposed ideas in a way that minimized common factors in couple therapy. Boszormenyi-
client resistance, and (d) the clients were at least Nagy coined the term “multidirectional partiality”
somewhat willing to take responsibility for their (Boszormenyi-Nagy & Spark, 1984) to describe a
problems. process whereby the therapist actively takes each
Though most interventions were unique to the partner's side, pleading his or her case to his or her
therapist's model, they seemed to follow similar partner in language that is easier to hear. By taking
patterns (Davis & Piercy, 2007a, 2007b). For each person's side, both partners feel accepted and
example, therapists described helping clients start to entertain the possibility that there is validity
become aware of their interactional cycles by to what their partner is saying. As the therapist
slowing down the cycle, thus helping each partner reframes each partner's comments in ways that are
step away from their struggles to change each easier to hear, they learn to say things without
other. As couples slowed down the cycle, attacking and listen without defending. It is
therapists reported helping clients stand “above” important to spend roughly equal session time
the cycle, thus seeing their and their partner's role with each partner during this process. When it is
in the cycle in a different light. Experiencing their necessary to focus more on one partner, it can be
partner differently seemed to make it easier for helpful to clarify that the relationship is the client,
each partner to take responsibility for their role in so for the good of the relationship the focus may be
their difficulties and take the recommended steps momentarily imbalanced. While the balance of
to exit the cycle. As therapy neared completion, attention can vary from session to session, it is still
clients described a softening toward each other. important to keep the alliance balanced over the
They also said they stopped trying to control, course of therapy.
choosing instead to enjoy each other regardless of Split alliances, in which the therapeutic alliance is
differences. They described feeling more confident stronger with one partner than the other, present
and relaxed. These processes are similar to another challenging situation. Shifting from the
softening in EFT (Johnson, 2004) and acceptance engaged to disengaged partner too abruptly may
in IBCT (Jacobson & Christensen, 1996). Partic- end therapy prematurely. However, therapy may
ipants described changes at different stages of not progress unless the disengaged partner engages.
therapy as being mediated by therapist and client Shifting positions solely for the sake of the alliance
variables, the alliance, expectancy/motivational is likely to fail except in the case of ethical or social
factors, and other variables as operationalized in justice issues. Rather, therapists are encouraged to
Davis and Piercy. use dialogue, empathy, and styles of relating that
There are several limitations inherent in the engage the disengaged partner while still validating
design of the Davis and Piercy (2007a, 2007b) the engaged partner.
study. First, the goal was an in-depth description of
a process rather than a statistical investigation of training implications
causal links between variables. Furthermore, the Given the importance of adapting an approach to a
sample was a small, carefully chosen group of client's culture and worldview, it follows that a
highly skilled practitioners who recruited successful therapist should achieve competence in diverse
46 davis et al.

models. We reiterate what Davis and Sprenkle have research


said elsewhere with Blow: We suggest there is a need for far greater focus
in research on the common factors in effective
“[Common factors ideas] suggest a shift in our
treatment. Clinical trials research can be quite
thinking away from encouraging a student to be
useful in this regard, and could be greatly enhanced
passionate about a theory towards being passio-
by incorporating a wider variety of independent
nate about theory. We believe that encouraging a
variables discussed earlier (e.g., therapist and client
therapist to be passionate about only one theory
variables) into the design. The ability of process/
may unwittingly give a therapist the proverbial
observational and outcome research to shed light
hammer with which he or she must turn every
on the how of change in couple therapy make them
client into a nail before treatment can proceed. We
promising research designs for the common factors
advocate that a more client-centered approach
paradigm. In addition to the research directions
would be to encourage the therapist to have a
suggested at the beginning of this article, qualitative
thorough familiarity with several diverse models
methodologies also could be useful, particularly
so that he or she can adapt to his or her client's
because there are so many unanswered questions
contexts rather than vice-versa. . . . Clients should
in the current common factors paradigm that at
not have to add ‘figure out how to adapt to my
this point could benefit from a discovery form of
therapist’ to their already lengthy list of chal-
investigation. Such designs could shed light on
lenges.” (Blow et al., 2007, pp. 309–310, emphasis
largely unanswered yet critical questions such as
in original)
“How do therapists balance the alliance in a way
As reviewed earlier, whereas more research is that allows disparate views to be validated simul-
needed to substantiate this claim, indirect evidence taneously?” “What client characteristics are related
suggests that therapists who tailor their treatment to outcome and how can a therapist influence
based on client feedback enjoy significantly better them?” “What therapist behaviors or characteris-
results (Halford et al., 2012-this issue). The ability tics facilitate versus thwart client motivation?”
to do this presupposes the knowledge of different “What—if any—common change mechanisms are
models, or at least the ability to present the same indicated for different problems (e.g., in what ways,
model in widely varying ways. if any, is helping a couple through an affair different
The daunting task of learning seemingly diverse than helping them through domestic violence?)?”
models can be made easier with an understanding and so forth.
of the nonspecific mechanisms of change discussed
earlier. When a therapist learns to view models as Conclusion
more overlapping than distinct, moving between We believe that the moderate common factors
approaches can become more intuitive (Blow et al., paradigm provides the best explanation of the data
2007). Realizing the overlap among seemingly suggesting few differences between couple therapy
disparate models can also help therapists become approaches (Shadish & Baldwin, 2002). While
passionate about diverse models, thus increasing much of the relevant research into common factors
their chances of success. In fact, it could be argued in couple therapy remains to be done, we never-
that many models of couple therapy were devel- theless believe there is enough to suggest a strong
oped by people who had a thorough grasp of and basis for the ideas we have covered in this article.
appreciation for diverse models. Examples include We hope that research into common factors in
EFT, which draws upon structural–strategic and couple therapy will continue to gain momentum.
experiential therapies (Johnson, 2004) and IBCT,
which incorporates emotionally focused principles
into a cognitive-behavioral framework (Jacobson References
& Christensen, 1996).
We also believe that therapists should receive Anker, M. G., Duncan, B. L., & Sparks, J. A. (2009). Using
client feedback to improve couple therapy outcomes: A
specific training in common factors such as establish- randomized clinical trial in a naturalistic setting. Journal of
ing, maintaining, and repairing the therapeutic Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 693–704.
alliance, seeking and responding appropriately to Atkins, D. C., Berns, S. B., George, W., Doss, B., Gattis, K., &
client feedback, nurturing hope, and facilitating Christensen, A. (2005). Prediction of response to treatment
motivation. Furthermore, we believe training pro- in a randomized clinical trial of marital therapy. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 893–903.
grams should more deliberately help students develop Beutler, L. E., Malik, M. L., & Alimohamed, S. (2004).
personal therapeutic attributes. State licensure boards Therapist variables. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.). Bergin and
could also require similar training in the continuing Garfield's handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change
education requirements for license renewal. (pp. 227–306). New York: Wiley.
common factors of change in couple therapy 47

Blatt, S. J., Sanislow, C. A., Zuroff, D. C., & Pilkonis, P. A. Horvath, A. O. (Ed.). (1994). Empirical validation of Bordin's
(1996). Characteristics of effective therapists: Further pantheoretical model of the alliance: The Working Alliance
analyses of data from the National Institute of Mental Inventory perspective. Oxford, England: Wiley.
Health treatment of depression collaborative research Jacobson, N. S., & Christensen, A. (1996). Integrative couple
program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, therapy: Promoting acceptance and change. New York:
64, 1276–1284. Norton.
Blow, A. J., Sprenkle, D. S., & Davis, S. D. (2007). Is who Johnson, S., & Greenberg, L. (1985). The differential effective-
delivers the treatment more important than the treatment ness of experiential and problem solving interventions in
itself?: The role of the therapist in common factors. Journal resolving marital conflict. Journal of Consulting and
of Marital and Family Therapy, 33, 298–317. Clinical Psychology, 53, 175–184.
Bodenman, G., Plancherel, B., Widmer, K., Meuwly, N., Johnson, S. M. (2004). The practice of emotionally focused
Hautzinger, M., Beach, S. R. H., Gabriel, B., et al. (2008). marital therapy: Creating connection (2nd ed.). New York:
Effects of coping-oriented couples therapy on depression: A Brunner/Mazel.
randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Johnson, S. M., & Talitman, E. (1997). Predictors of success in
Psychology, 76(6), 944–954. emotionally focused marital therapy. Journal of Marital and
Bohart, A. C., & Tallman, K. (2010). Clients: The neglected Family Therapy, 23, 135–152.
common factor in psychotherapy. In B. L. Duncan, S. D. Karasu, T. B. (1986). The specificity versus nonspecificity
Miller, B. E. Wampold, & M. A. Hubble (Eds.). The heart dilemma: Toward identifying therapeutic change agents.
and soul of change: Delivering what works in therapy American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 687–695.
(2nd ed., pp. 83–112). Washington, DC: American Lambert, M. J., Whipple, J. L., Smart, D. W., Vermeersch, D. A.,
Psychology Association. Nielsen, S. L., & Hawkins, E. J. (2001). The effects of
Boszormenyi-Nagy, I., & Spark, G. M. (1984). Invisible providing therapists with feedback on patient progress during
loyalties. New York: Routledge. psychotherapy: Are outcomes enhanced? Psychotherapy
Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically Research, 11, 49–68.
supported psychological interventions: Controversies Luborsky, L., Rosenthal, R., Diguer, L., Andrusyna, T. P.,
and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, Berman, J. S., Levitt, J. T., et al. (2002). The dodo bird
685–716. verdict is alive and well—mostly. Clinical Psychology:
Christensen, A. J. (2010). A unified protocol for couple therapy. Science and Practice, 9, 2–12.
In K. Halweg, M. Grawe-Gerber, & D. H. Baucom (Eds.). McCrady, B. S., Epstein, E. E., Cook, S., Jensen, N., &
Enhancing couples. Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe. Hildebrandt, T. (2009). A randomized trial of individual
Cline, V. B., Mejia, J., Coles, J., Klein, N., & Cline, R. and couple behavioral alcohol treatment for women. Journal
(1984). The relationship between therapist behaviors of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77(2), 243–256.
and outcome for middle- and lower-class couples in Orlinsky, D. E., Grawe, K., & Parks, B. K. (1994). Process and
marital therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, outcome in psychotherapy: Noch einmal. In A. E. Bergin, &
691–704. S. L. Garfield (Eds.). Handbook of psychotherapy and
Dattilio, F. M. (2010). Cognitive-behavioral therapy with behavior change (4th ed., pp. 270–376). Oxford, England:
couples and families: A comprehensive guide for clinicians. Wiley.
New York: Guilford Press. Powers, M. B., Vedel, E., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (2008).
Davis, S. D., & Piercy, F. P. (2007). What clients of MFT Behavioral couples therapy (BCT) for alcohol and drug use
model developers and their former students say about disorders: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review,
change, Part I: Model dependent common factors across 28(6), 952–962.
three models. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33, Prochaska, J. O., & Norcross, J. C. (2009). Systems of
318–343. psychotherapy: A transtheoretical analysis (7th ed.). New
Davis, S. D., & Piercy, F. P. (2007). What clients of MFT model York: Brooks Cole.
developers and their former students say about change, Part Sexton, T. L., & Ridley, C. R. (2004). Implications of a
II: Model independent common factors and an integrative moderated common factors approach: Does it move the
framework. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33, field forward? Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 30,
344–363. 159–164.
Doss, B., Thum, Y., Sevier, M., Atkins, D., & Christensen, A. Shadish, W. R., & Baldwin, S. A. (2002). Meta-analysis of
(2005). Improving relationships: Mechanism of change in MFT interventions. In D. H. Sprenkle (Ed.). Effectiveness
couple theraphy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical research in marriage and family therapy (pp. 339–370).
Psychology, 73, 624–633. Alexandria, VA: American Association of Marriage and
Elkin, I., Shea, T., Watkins, J. T., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, S. M., Family Therapy.
Collins, J. F., et al. (1989). National Institute of Mental Smith, M. L., & Glass, G. V. (1977). Meta-analysis of
Health treatment of depression collaborative research psychotherapy outcome studies. American Psychologist,
program: General effectiveness of treatments. Archives of 32(9), 752–760.
General Psychiatry, 46, 971–982. Snyder, D. K. (1999). Affective reconstruction in the context of
Frank, J. D., & Frank, J. B. (1991). Persuasion and healing: A a pluralistic approach to couples therapy. Clinical Psychology:
comparative study of psychotherapy (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Science and Practice, 6, 348–365.
Johns Hopkins University Press. Sprenkle, D. H., & Blow, A. J. (2004). Common factors and our
Halford, W. K., Hayes, S., Christensen, A., Lambert, M., sacred models. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 30,
Baucom, D. H., & Atkins, D. (2012). Towards making 113–130.
progress feedback an effective common factor in couple Sprenkle, D. H., Davis, S. D., & Lebow, J. (2009). Common
therapy. Behavior Therapy, 43, 49–60 (this issue). factors in couple and family therapy: The overlooked
Halford, W., & Snyder, D. (2012). Special series: Universal processes foundation for effective practice. New York: Guilford Press.
and common factors in couple therapy and relationship Stith, S. M., Rosen, K. H., McCollum, E. E., & Thomsen, C. J.
education-Introduction. Behavior Therapy, 43, 1–12 (this issue). (2004). Treating intimate partner violence within intact
48 davis et al.

couple relationships: Outcomes of multi-couple versus Ward, D. B., & Wampler, K. S. (2010). Moving up the
individual couple therapy. Journal of Marital and Family continuum of hope: Developing a theory of hope and
Therapy, 30(3), 305–318. understanding its influence in couples therapy. Journal of
Stolk, Y., & Perlesz, A. J. (1990). Do better trainees make worse Marital and Family Therapy, 36, 212–228.
family therapists?: A follow-up study of client families.
Family Process, 29, 45–58. R E C E I V E D : August 17, 2010
Wampold, B. E. (2001). The great psychotherapy debate: A C C E P T E D : January 5, 2011
Models, methods, and findings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Available online 6 June 2011

You might also like