You are on page 1of 19

Predicting Marital Happiness and Stability from Newlywed Interactions

Author(s): John M. Gottman, James Coan, Sybil Carrere, Catherine Swanson


Source: Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 60, No. 1 (Feb., 1998), pp. 5-22
Published by: National Council on Family Relations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/353438
Accessed: 15/03/2010 10:26

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfr.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

National Council on Family Relations is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Marriage and Family.

http://www.jstor.org
JOHNM. GOTTMAN,JAMESCOAN, SYBILCARRERE,AND CATHERINESWANSON
University of Washington

Predicting MaritalHappiness and Stability from


Newlywed Interactions

A study with 130 newlywed couples was designed Recently some of our best scholars (e.g., Jacob-
to explore marital interaction processes that are son & Addis, 1993) have contended that marital
predictive of divorce or marital stability, processes therapy has relapse rates so high that the entire
that further discriminate between happily and un- enterprise may be in a state of crisis. Consistent
happily married stable couples. We explore seven with these conclusions, the recent Consumer Re-
types of process models: (a) anger as a dangerous ports study of psychotherapy (Seligman, 1995)
emotion, (b) active listening, (c) negative affect also reported that marital therapy received the
reciprocity, (d) negative start-up by the wife, (e) lowest marks from psychotherapy consumers.
de-escalation, (f) positive affect models, and (g) Marital therapy may be at an impasse because it
physiological soothing of the male. Support was is not based on a process model derived from
not found for the models of anger as a dangerous prospective longitudinal studies of what real cou-
emotion, active listening, or negative affect ples do that predicts if their marriages will wind
reciprocity. Support was found for models of the up happy and stable, unhappy and stable, or end
husband's rejecting his wife's influence, negative in divorce.
start-up by the wife, a lack of de-escalation of low Differential longitudinal prediction of marital
intensity negative wife affect by the husband, or a satisfaction and stability may be an essential step
lack of de-escalation of high intensity husband that has been omitted in designing marital thera-
negative affect by the wife, and a lack ofphysiolog- py. Building a process model of marriage using
ical soothing of the male, all predicting divorce. this prediction approach could turn out to be su-
Support was found for a contingent positive affect perior to building an intervention by imagining
model and for balance models (i.e., ratio models) what target populations in trouble may need or by
of positive-to-negative affect predicting satisfac- imagining it according to some theoretical posi-
tion among stable couples. Divorce and stability tion. What has happened in the field of marital
were predicted with 83% accuracy and satisfaction therapy is that a psychotherapy of marriage has
with 80% accuracy. been constructed by extending methods of psy-
chotherapy to the design of marital interventions,
instead of building a marital therapy from the way
people normally go about the process of staying
happily married. However, we should point out
Department of Psychology, Box 351525, University of that this is an assumption. This could be called
Washingon,Seattle,WA 98115 (johng@u.washington.edu). "the single theory assumption," which claims that
the functioning, dysfunctioning, and repair of
Key Words: divorce, gender, interaction, newlyweds, physiol-
ogy.
marital relationships can be explained using one

Journal of Marriage and the Family 60 (February 1998): 5-22 5


6 Journal of Marriage and the Family

theory. Alternatively, it might be the case that any need to apologize?Why do we raise our voices
process, such as active listening, might be a great whenit does no good?(p. 78)
intervention, even if people don't naturally do it.
Thus, to use an analogy, it might be the case that In contrast, Winkler and Doherty (1983) re-
more than one set of "orthopedic knowledge" is ported that, compared with Jewish couples in the
United States, Jewish Israeli couples did not asso-
necessary in marriage, one about the normally de-
veloping "bones" of marriage and another about ciate the amount of anger expressed in their mari-
how to repair "broken bones" in therapy. We do tal interaction with marital dissatisfaction.
not think that this will be the case. Gottman and Krokoff (1989) reported that, al-
We attempt to build such a process model of though anger was associated with lower concur-
rent marital satisfaction, it also was associated
marriage. What processes ought to be included
for investigation in building a process model? It with increases in marital satisfaction over time.
may be inadequate to simply describe what is Gottman, Katz, and Hooven (1997) studied what
"dysfunctional" when a marriage is ailing. It may they called the "meta-emotion" philosophy of
be necessary to describe what is "functional" parents-parents' emotional reactions and
when a marriage is working well. This may not be philosophies about anger and sadness. Parents'
redundantinformation. meta-emotions about anger and sadness were re-
lated to the amount of coaching they did with
their children, the way they taught their children a
"DYSFUNCTIONAL"
MODELSOF
new task, their child's regulatory physiology,
MARITALPROCESSES
their child's ability to focus attention, and a vari-
ety of child developmental outcomes.
Furthermore, in two longitudinal studies,
Two Models of Negative Affect: Anger Gottman (1994) reported that anger in marital in-
as a Destructive Emotion Versus
teraction did not predict divorce, whereas four
the "Four Horsemen"
processes that he called "The Four Horsemen of
The first set of processes concerns what might be the Apocalypse"-criticism, defensiveness, con-
called "the specificity of negativity hypothesis." tempt, and "stonewalling" (listener withdrawal)-
It addresses the question of whether all negative reliably did predict divorce. Subsequent research
affects are equally corrosive in marriages. A per- added "belligerence," a behavior that is provoca-
vasive model is that anger is destructive of mari- tive and that challenges the spouse's power and
tal relationships. For example, Hendrix (1988), authority. (For example: "What can you do if I do
author of a bestselling book on marriage, wrote in go drinking with Dave? What are you gonna do
a section titled "The Destructive Power of Anger": about it?") The current study with newlywed cou-
ples contrasted these two models of marital dys-
Angeris destructiveto a relationship,no matter function.
whatits form.Whenangeris expressed,the per-
son on the receivingend of the attackfeels bru-
Affect, Power, and Gender
talized,whetheror not therehas been any physi-
cal violence; the old braindoes not distinguish Here we also consider models that interface be-
betweenchoice of weapons.Further,becauseof tween affect and power. There are two models of
the strange workings of the unconscious, the
interest. The first we discuss as negative affect re-
personwho unleashesthe angerfeels equallyas-
saulted because on a deep level the old brain ciprocity versus accepting influence, and the sec-
ond we discuss as start-up-namely, who starts
perceivesall actionas inner-directed.
(p. 147)
the conflict discussion and how. We propose that
This view of anger also was expressed by Par- these processes are related and that they suggest
rott and Parrott (1995), who have a chapter titled the differential roles that husbands and wives play
"The Deadly Emotion of Anger." They wrote: in maintaining marital stability and happiness.

It would be tough to find anotheremotion that Negative reciprocity models and power. Using
has causedmarriedcouplesmore difficultythan observational data of marital interaction, the most
anger. Why do we get angry at the person we reliable empirical discriminator between happy
love the most? Why do we allow ourselves to and unhappy marriages across laboratories, within
get angrywhenwe knowin advancethatwe will the U.S. and cross-nationally (Germany, Holland,
Process Models of Marriage 7

and Spain) has been the negative affect reciproci- How conflict begins. This model, the negative
ty model. (For a review, see Gottman, 1994.) If start-up model, is derived from of J. R. Patter-
one partner is negative (for example, angry), the son's writings about how coercive processes in
spouse is much more likely to be negative than he families start (e.g., Patterson, 1982). Start-up is
or she usually would be. We further distinguish operationally defined as the escalation of conflict
low-intensity negative affect reciprocity (sadness, from one partner's neutral affect to the other part-
anger, whining, tension or fear, domineering) ner's negative affect. We hypothesize a specific
from high-intensity negative affect reciprocity gender pattern based on the female-
(contempt, defensiveness, or belligerence). This is demand/male-withdraw pattern. (See Chris-
the first time this distinction has been made, and tensen & Heavey, 1990; Heavey, Christensen, &
it is possible because of the design of the Specific Malamuth, 1995; Heavey, Christopher, & Chris-
Affect Coding System (Gottman, McCoy, & tensen, 1993.) Women typically start most of the
Coan, 1996). This makes it possible to refine fur- conflict discussions in laboratories that use obser-
ther the negative affect reciprocity model by vational methods (Ball, Cowan, & Cowan, 1995;
breaking it into two types of hypotheses: negative Oggins, Veroff, & Leber, 1993). A graph of mo-
reciprocity in kind and escalating negativity. The ment-to-moment positive-minus-negative codes
first sequence is a reciprocation in kind. Anger is using the Rapid Couples Interaction Scoring Sys-
met with anger, for example. This model suggests tem found that the slope of these graphs was pre-
that marriages deteriorate because people cannot dictive a cascade toward divorce or marital stabil-
"put the brakes on" or regulate the reciprocation ity and happiness (Gottman & Levenson, 1992).
of negativity. These cycles of negativity charac- A quantitative analysis of the amount of negative
terize ailing marriages; they do not characterize interaction minus the amount of positive interac-
marriages that are working well. tion during these conversations (Gottman, 1994)
The second model is rapid escalation of the showed that for 96% of these interactions, if the
negativity in which any lower intensity negativity graphs began in the first few minutes with a posi-
like anger is met with an escalated negative affect tive or a negative slope, they were not reversed.
like belligerence, contempt, or defensiveness. We In only 4% of the interactions was there a major
recently have discovered that in the study of vio- change in slope during the 15-minute interaction.
lent marriages this latter pattern of the husband's Hence, the way the interactions start may be criti-
escalation of his wife's low-level negative affect cal in determining the couple's fate. The gen-
indexes violent males' rejection of influence from der-specific hypothesis here is that marriages
their wives (Coan, Gottman, Babcock, & Jacob- will work to the extent that women soften their
son, in press). This interpretationof the sequence start-up by not escalating from neutral to negative
also was supported by a recent dissertation in our affect.
laboratory by Rushe (1995). This form of the se-
quence of negative affect reciprocity operational- "FUNCTIONAL" OFMARITAL
MODELS PROCESSES
izes the hypothesis into a specific gender pattern
that is primarily about power, not affect. The hy- The Active Listening Model
pothesis is that marriages work to the extent that
men accept influence from women. This pattern The most influential process theory of what is
of functional marriage is indexed by men who do functional in the context of the resolution of con-
not escalate the low-intensity negative affect of flict in marriage may be called the active listening
women. Thus, it appears that issues of power model. This model also has been called a valida-
sharing are inherent in part of the pattern of nega- tion model (Gottman, Notarius, Gonso, & Mark-
tive affect reciprocity. We believe that it is impor- man, 1978; Markman, Stanley, & Blumberg,
tant to distinguish these two models. One model 1994; Notarius & Markman, 1993) and a mirror-
shows an "affect dysregulation" in ailing mar- ing model (Hendrix, 1988). It is the current way
riages (Hendrix, 1988; Markman, 1991). The of operationalizing empathy in marital interaction
other suggests that dimensions of power need to across a spectrum of perspectives, from the be-
interface with dimensions of affect to understand havioral perspective (Jacobson & Margolin,
how "dysfunctional" patterns work in ailing mar- 1979) to the psychoanalytic object-relations per-
riages. spective (Siegel, 1992). This model forms the
basis of most complex multi-component marital
treatments (e.g., see Jacobson & Gurman, 1995).
8 Journal of Marriage and the Family

In most marital therapies, the active listening Checklist, and a version of the Marital Interaction
model is reflected in some form of what has come Coding System to code general conversation
to be known as the listener-speaker exchange when they supposedly were setting up the equip-
(e.g., see Notarius & Markman, 1993). For exam- ment and the discussion of the Inventory of Mari-
ple, suppose the wife starts as the speaker, and the tal Conflict (IMC; Olson & Ryder, 1970). In the
husband is the listener. First, she is asked to state summary code of the Marital Interaction Coding
her complaints directly to the husband. Suppose System, the positive interactions were agreement,
she complains that she is hurt by the way he re- approval, humor, assent, laugh, positive physical
lates to their youngest child. She hates the way he contact, and smile. Distressed couples produced
ignores the boy and criticizes him. Then he is an average of 1.49 positive interations per minute;
asked to paraphrase, without defensiveness or nondistressed couples produced an average of
judgment, both the content and the feelings of his 1.93 positive interations per minute, a significant
wife's message and to check out his paraphrase. difference. In the home environment, distressed
Then he is asked to empathetically validate her partners recorded significantly fewer pleasing
feelings. The hypothesis may be drawn that sta- events and significantly greater displeasing events
ble, happy marriages are characterized by such than nondistressed partnersdid.
exchanges during conflict resolution and that ail- We are interested in two forms of the positive
ing marriages are characterized by the absence of affect model. One simply suggests that positive
these kind of exchanges. The intellectual history affect will be randomly distributed throughout the
of this approach to marital therapy is from client- conflict, without any connection to other process-
centered therapy, adapted by Guerney for couples es. This is a kind of noncontingent or good-will
(Guerey, 1982). However, there is a conceptual positive affect model. The second model of posi-
leap in Guerney's application of these ideas to the tive affect suggests that positive affect is used to
marital arena. In client-centered therapy the client de-escalate marital conflict. It is in the service of
is usually complaining about a third person, and moving the overall affect from a negative to a less
the therapist is empathizing with the client against negative (even a neutral affective) state. In this
a third party. When the client complains about the contingent positive affect model, the underlying
therapist, it is usually called "resistance." Yet in or related purpose of the positive affect involves
the marital arena, the spouse, even though the tar- de-escalation and possibly physiological soothing
get of these complaints, is expected to empathize of self or partner (Gottman, 1990). This model
with her or his partner. will be tested against the model of good-will pos-
itive affect, using structural equations modeling
De-Escalation Models in which the path coefficient between positive af-
fect and de-escalation will be compared across
The de-escalation model (Gottman, 1979) sug- newlyweds who eventually wound up in one of
gests that what is important in functional relation- three criterion groups: divorced, married but un-
ships is the de-escalation of conflict, which in- happy, or married and happy.
volves moving from one partner's negative affect
(either high or low intensity) to the other partner's Balance Models
neutral affect. Gottman found that husbands in
happy marriages were the ones most likely to de- One way of integrating negative and positive af-
escalate low-intensity negative affect, and fect models is to suggest the importance of a bal-
wives in happy marriages were most likely to ance of positive and negative affect. In these bal-
de-escalate high-intensity negative affect. ance models, the couple maintains a set point of
the ratio of positivity to negativity that is func-
Positive Affect Models tional if it is high or dysfunctional if it is low. The
balance models are not redundantbecause, for ex-
Another aspect of marital interaction that has re- ample, if only a negativity model were validated,
ceived scant attention are models of positive affect it would suggest that a goal of marital therapy
(such as humor, affection, and interest) in predict- ought to be declaring war on negative affect. Intu-
ing the eventual fate of marriages. An exception itively, such a conclusion seems absurd. A mar-
is Birchler, Weiss, and Vincent (1975), who used riage in which there is no negative affect and in
a self-report diary measure of "pleases" and "dis- which people were cheerful and positive all the
pleases," a precursor of the Spouse Observation time would seem to be one version of Sartre's re-
Process Models of Marriage 9

lationship hell, one that is reminiscent of the film has been considerable empirical support for this
The Stepford Wives in which suburban husbands contention, and it has been called the female-
conspire to murder their wives and replace them demand/male-withdraw pattern by Christensen
with cheerful robot copies. A balance model and his associates (e.g., Christensen, 1987, 1988,
would maintain that a marriage is like a stable, 1990; see also Gottman, 1994, on female criti-
predator-prey behavioral ecology, in which there cism and male stonewalling). There is also evi-
is a balance between the necessary predator of dence that babies do not socially reference their
negative affect and the necessary prey of positive unhappily married fathers but do continue to so-
affect. The ratio of pleases to displeases in Birch- cially reference their unhappily married mothers
ler, Weiss, and Vincent's study (1975) also dis- (Dickstein & Parke, 1988). The evidence for this
criminated between the groups. (The ratio was emotional withdrawal of the male in ailing fami-
29.66 for nondistressed couples and 4.30 for dis- lies is so widely recognized that it has become the
tressed couples.) We employed an observational subject of national conferences (e.g., the National
system that obtained considerably more detail and Conference on Men in Families in 1996). Writers
specificity than the Marital Interaction Coding have presumed that it is related to what has been
System of Birchler and colleagues obtained in the called "the absent father" and the widespread
realm of emotion, for both positive and negative abandonment of children by fathers after divorce.
affect (Gottman, 1996). Hence, another form of (See Griswold, 1993; Parke, 1996; Popenoe,
the positive affect model is the ratio model, which 1996.)
suggests that what is important is the relative Gottman and Levenson (1988) hypothesized
amount of positive to negative affect. Gottman that this gender difference is based, in part, on a
(1994) reported that in three types of stable mar- biological difference between the sexes. Their hy-
riages that he identified, the ratio of positive to pothesis was that men are in some ways more re-
negative interaction during conflict resolution active to stress than women, particularly in the
was 5 to 1, whereas the ratio was .8 to 1 in unsta- adrenergic parts of the cardiovascular system and
ble marriages. in the stress-related endocrine responses that ac-
company active coping, particularly the cate-
The Potential Importance of Physiological cholamines adrenaline and noradrenaline (Obrist,
Soothing of the Male 1981). There is some evidence to suggest that this
difference is found physiologically (in the auto-
Why might positive affect and de-escalation be nomic nervous system and endocrine responses)
related to the relapse effect in marital therapy? and in emotion-related behaviors. (For a review,
We propose that the link lies in the couple's abili- see Polefrone & Manuck, 1987.) Recent evidence
ty to physiologically soothe self and partner. In suggests that this gender difference may be par-
many marital therapies, partly through the influ- ticularly pronounced in the vigilance-startle sys-
ence of Murray Bowen's work (for a review, see tem (McCarter & Levenson, 1996) and that there
Papero, 1995), the therapist soothes the couple so is a greater male adrenergic-cardiovascular re-
that in the analysis of process they will rationally sponse to acoustic startling in terms of the reac-
examine their dysfunctional patterns of interac- tivity of the heart rate and vasoconstrictive re-
tion. The hypothesis is that if the therapist plays sponses.
this role instead of the couple, relapse may be the Because of the aversive nature of diffuse auto-
result. That is, we predict a good outcome for the nomic arousal, men may attempt to avoid nega-
couple to the extent that they, themselves, are tive affect in close relationships because it is
able to soothe self and partner. more physiologically punishing for them than for
There may be differences in how important women. Gottman (1994) reported that, emotional-
this physiological soothing is according to gen- ly, males are flooded by lower levels of negative
der. Gottman and Levenson (1988) reviewed evi- affect than females are. To evaluate this hypothe-
dence for the hypothesis that men and women dif- sis, in the study presented here, variables related
fer in their responses to negative affect in mar- to either partner's positive affect and to either
riage and in other close relationships. They partner's de-escalation of conflict were used as
proposed that the research suggests that in the cli- events in interrupted time-series analyses of
mate of negative affect that pervades unhappy heart-rate data collected synchronously with the
marriages, men withdraw emotionally, and video time code. The extent to which the soothing
women do not. Since their review appeared, there of either spouse occurred then was assessed and
10 Journal of Marriage and the Family

used as a predictor of marital outcome. The In the second phase of the study, 130 newly-
Gottman-Levenson hypothesis would be support- wed couples who represented an even distribution
ed only if the only predictors of marital outcomes of marital satisfaction were invited to participate
involved the physiological soothing of the male. in a marital interaction laboratory session and
To summarize, we are asking questions about complete additional questionnaires. These cou-
what interactive processes are dysfunctional and ples fit perfectly the demographic characteristics
what are functional in the context of a longitudi- of the major ethnic and racial groups in the
nal prediction study. We explore seven types of greater Seattle area, according to the Seattle City
models: (a) anger as the destructive emotion ver- Metropolitan Planning Commission Report. The
sus the Four Horsemen; (b) active listening; (c) demographic characteristics for these newly mar-
negative affect reciprocity: in kind (either low or ried couples were: wife's age = 25.4 years (SD =
high intensity) and husband escalation (indexing 3.5); husband's age = 26.5 years (SD = 4.2);
dimensions of power and gender, specifically the wife's marital satisfaction = 120.4 (SD = 19.7);
husband's refusal to accept influence); (d) nega- husband's marital satisfaction = 115.9 (SD =
tive start-upby the wife; (e) de-escalation: low in- 18.4).
tensity by the husband or high intensity by the
wife; (f) positive affect models: contingent, non- Marital Status and Criterion Groups
contingent, and ratio; (g) physiological soothing
of the male. We will ask these questions in the Once each year, the marital status and satisfaction
context of a representative sample of 130 newly- of the 130 couples in the study were assessed. At
weds from the Seattle area. We studied these 130 the end of the 6-year period (called Time 2 in this
couples over a 6-year period in cohorts of approx- article), there had been 17 divorces-six in the
imately 40 couples per cohort. Our follow-up pe- first cohort, six in the second, and five in the
riod has varied from 3 to 6 years. There were 17 third. The mean number of years married among
divorces in that time. We then took 20 compara- the divorced couples was 3 (SD = .79). The low-
ble couples with high marital satisfaction and 20 est of each couple's Time-2 scores on the Locke-
with low marital satisfaction as comparison Wallace marital satisfaction measure were used to
groups. form two similarly sized criterion groups of stable
couples-the 20 most happily married and the 20
METHOD most unhappily married. The mean score of mari-
tal satisfaction at Time 2 of the stable, happily
married group was 128.30 (SD = 27.65), and the
Participants
mean score of marital satisfaction at Time 2 of
Between 1989 and 1992, we used a two-stage the stable and unhappily married group was 90.70
sampling procedure to draw a sample of newly- (SD = 16.08).
wed couples from the Puget Sound area in Wash-
ington. Couples initially were recruited using Procedures and Measures
newspaper advertisements. To be eligible for the
study, the couples had to have married for the Behavioral observation. Two remotely controlled
first time within 6 months of participating in the VHS video cameras recorded both spouses during
study, and they had to be childless. Couples were the interaction sessions. The images from the two
contacted by phone and administered our tele- cameras were combined in a split-screen image.
phone version of the Marital Adjustment Test Microphones recorded the couple's audio interac-
(MAT; Krokoff, 1987; Locke & Wallace, 1959) tions. The computer synchronized physiological
and surveyed to determine their eligibility on the data with video data by utilizing the elapse-time
other criteria. The MAT measures marital satis- codes imposed on the video recordings. The Spe-
faction. Higher scores represent higher marital cific Affect Coding System (SPAFF; Gottman,
satisfaction. There were 179 newlywed couples McCoy, & Coan, 1996) was used to code the cou-
who met the research criteria and participated in ples' conflict interactions. The system indexed
the initial survey phase of the study. In this phase, specific affects expressed during the session of
husbands and wives separately were mailed a set problem resolution. SPAFF focuses solely on the
of questionnaires to fill out that included mea- affects expressed. The system draws on facial ex-
sures of demographic characteristics and indices pression (based on Ekman & Friesen's, 1978, sys-
about their marriage, well-being, and health. tem of facial action coding), vocal tone, and
Process Models of Marriage 11

speech content to characterize the emotions dis- marital discussion of areas of disagreement dur-
played. Coders categorized the affects displayed ing the marital interaction phase of the study.
using five positive codes (interest, validation, af- Couples used the inventory to rate the severity of
fection, humor, joy), 10 negative codes (disgust, issues in their marriage. Items include areas of
contempt, belligerence, domineering, anger, standard marital problems such as in-laws, fi-
fear/tension, defensiveness, whining, sadness, nances, and sex. Each item is rated on a scale of
stonewalling), and a neutral affect code. Every 0-100. Higher scores signify that the problem is
videotape was coded in its entirety by two inde- considered more severe.
pendent observers using a computer-assisted cod-
ing system that automated the collection of timing Laboratory Procedures
information. Each coder noted only the onset of
each code. A time-locked confusion matrix for The marital interaction assessment consisted of a
the entire videotape then was computed using a 1- discussion by the husband and wife of a problem
second window for determining agreement of that was a source of ongoing disagreement in
each code in one observer's coding against all of their marriage and two recall sessions in which
the other observers' coding. (See Bakeman & the couples viewed their discussion of their mari-
Gottman, 1986.) The diagonal versus the diago- tal disagreement. After the couple completed the
nal-plus-off-diagonal entries in these matrices CPI, the experimenter reviewed with the couple
then were entered into a repeated measures analy- the issues they rated most problematic and helped
sis of variance using the method specified by them to choose several to use as the basis for the
Wiggins (1977). We computed the Cronbach al- discussion. After choosing the topics for the dis-
phas for each code as the ratio of the mean square cussion, couples were asked to sit quietly and not
for observers minus the error mean square and the interact with each other during a 2-minute base-
mean square for observers plus the error mean line. The couples discussed their chosen topics for
square. (See also Bakeman & Gottman, 1986.) 15 minutes and then viewed the video recording
The Cronbach's alpha generalizability coeffi- of the interaction. In counterbalanced order, the
cients ranged between .651 and .992 and aver- husband and wife first viewed and rated their own
aged .907 for the entire coding of all 130 video- affect during the discussion and then viewed and
tapes. rated their spouse's affect. Both the husband and
wife used rating dials that provided continuous
Physiological measures. During the first year of self-report data. We collected continuous physio-
data collection, we measured physiological re- logical measures and video recordings during all
sponses with polygraphs and a computer to record of the interaction sessions, and data were aver-
and store the physiological data. The electrocar- aged over 1-second intervals.
diogram was collected, and interbeat intervals One way to operationalize the active listening
were calculated as the time in milliseconds be- sequences would be to look for sequences in
tween consecutive R-waves. General somatic ac- which one partner expressed negative affect and
tivity was indexed by an electromechanical trans- the other partnervalidated the negative affect. We
ducer under the platform supporting each cast a wider net than this and examined all se-
spouse's chair. quences in which one spouse first expressed low-
intensity or high-intensity negative affect, and
Self-report of affect. After the marital interaction, this was then followed by either interest, affec-
spouses viewed a videotape of it and were asked tion, humor, or validation by the partner. Thus,
to recall how they felt during the interaction. we searched for sequences that might be even
They used a rating dial to provide a continuous partially reflective of active listening-for exam-
self-report measure of their emotional evaluation ple, a sequence in which one partner complains
of the marital interaction. and the other partner responds warmly, perhaps
not exactly with validation, but with a great deal
Questionnaires.We used the MAT (Locke-Wallace, of active interest, affection, or shared humor. By
1959), as well as our telephone version of the low-intensity negative affect, we mean the
MAT, which has strong psychometric properties SPAFF codes anger, sadness, whining, disgust,
(Krokoff, 1987). The Couple's Problem Inventory tension and fear, or stonewalling. The SPAFF
(CPI; Gottman, Markman, & Notarius, 1977) was codes for high-intensity negative affect are bel-
filled out by the study participantsjust prior to the ligerence, contempt, or defensiveness. The higher-
12 Journal of Marriage and the Family

intensity negative affect codes are more predic- most important determinant of sequences of both
tive of divorce (Gottman, 1994). Negative affect escalation of negative affect and emotional with-
reciprocity models mean the reciprocation of neg- drawal. This hypothesis suggests that marriages
ative affect in kind, either high intensity or low will work to the extent that they provide for
intensity. The refusing-to-accept-influence mod- soothing of the male. The soothing can imply
els involved the escalation of the intensity of neg- self-soothing, or it can imply soothing of the male
ative affect from one partner's low-intensity neg- by the female.
ative affect to the other partner's high-intensity
negative affect. Negative start-up involved a se- RESULTS
quence of one partner's neutral affect to the
other's negative affect, either high or low intensi- The analyses pursued two types of predictions: a
ty. De-escalation sequences involved the couple marital stability prediction in which we combined
moving from either low-intensity negative affect the two stable groups (happy and unhappy) and
to neutral affect or from high-intensity negative attempted to predict divorce or stability from their
affect to neutral affect. The positive affect models Time-i marital interaction (taken in the first 6
involved either the amount of positive affect or months of marriage), and a marital happiness pre-
the ratio of positive affect to positive plus nega- diction, in which, controlling for stability, the pre-
tive affect, all computed for one spouse. diction was to a couple's Time-2 marital happi-
To examine whether the positive affect events ness or unhappiness (from their Time-i marital
or the de-escalation sequences were related to interaction taken in the first 6 months of mar-
physiological soothing and whether, as predicted, riage). For all sequences, this procedure was fol-
physiological soothing involves primarily the lowed: A sequence first was examined that in-
male, we used the heart rate data that were time- dexed a particular process model. If the results
locked with the SPAFF codes. The analysis pro- turned out to be statistically significant, a covari-
ceeded in two phases. First, we used interrupted ance analysis also was conducted, using the fre-
time-series analyses to assess whether or not a quency of the consequent code as the covariate.
particular positive affective event (interest, ex- This was a stringent approach to the fundamental,
citement, affection, and humor by either spouse) sequential connection question, "Was the conse-
or a de-escalation event (by either spouse) result- quent more predictable from the antecedent than
ed in a statistically significant reduction in its base rate of occurrence?" (Gottman & Roy,
heart rate or a significant downturn in the 1990). Without this test, it cannot be known if a
heart rate slope of either spouse. De-escalation high joint frequency of occurrence was not due
was defined as negative affect by one spouse, fol- simply to a high base rate of the consequent, or if
lowed immediately in the next second by neutral a low joint frequency of occurrence is not due
affect from the partner. That neutral affect had to simply to a low base rate of the consequent. How-
last for at least 5 seconds. The Crosbie computer ever, not all laboratories agree that a covariance
program was used for these time-locked interrupt- analysis is the most meaningful way of analyzing
ed time-series analyses (Crosbie, 1993). A con- for sequential connection. For example, the Ore-
stant 5-second preevent interval and a constant 5- gon Social Learning Center employs only sequen-
second postevent interval was used for these anal- tial measures such as conditional probabilities be-
yses. Next, we computed the number of cause of the valid point that they are more clearly
statistically significant soothing events, compared interpretable. We present both analyses. Finally,
with all such events in a couple's record. These in our data analysis we construct a path analytic
frequencies then were entered into a log-linear model that attempts to link several theoretical
analysis to predict which of the three criterion process models to positive affect to determine if
groups-divorced, stable and happy, or stable and the positive affect model was contingent on or in-
unhappy-a couple was in. These data can predict dependent of other marital interaction processes.
marital outcomes in a number of ways. Many This analysis attempted to fit the same model to
models are possible. However, the most interest- all three criterion groups and then to compare the
ing model to us involves hypotheses of the impor- same model across groups.
tance to marital outcomes of the physiological Table 1 is a summary of means of anger and of
soothing of the male because Gottman and Lev- high- and low-intensity negative affect on the
enson (1988) hypothesized that it is the physio- SPAFF. Neither husband's nor wife's anger was
logical arousal of the male that is likely to be the predictive of divorce, nor did it predictively dis-
Process Models of Marriage 13

TABLE 1. TEST OF THE MODEL OF ANGER AS THE DESTRUCTIVE EMOTION IN MARRIAGES

Variable Stable Divorced F (1,55) Happy Unhappy F (1,38)


Husbandanger 17.88 24.88 .48 17.30 18.45 .01
Wife anger 23.30 54.70 3.52 16.45 30.15 1.14
Husbandlow negativity 54.45 102.53 2.31 32.75 76.15 1.64
Wife low negativity 72.20 161.24 7.05** 45.70 98.70 .07
Husbandhigh negativity 70.68 146.24 13.03*** 66.15 75.20 .22
Wife high negativity 60.58 121.00 6.76* 53.95 73.20 .93
Note: Means are in seconds
*p < .05. ** p < .01. ***p < .001.

criminate between happy and unhappy stable tivity and F(2,109) = 4.39, p = .038, for the wife's
marriages. However, in the model based on the reciprocation of negativity. Hence, in the first few
Four Horsemen, both husband's and wife's high- months of marriage, wives in marriages that are
intensity negative affect, (the sum of belligerence, going to be unstable are more likely to reciprocate
defensiveness, and contempt) did predict divorce, low-intensity negative affect than wives in mar-
but did not predictively discriminate between riages that are going to be stable. The respective
happy stable and unhappy stable marriages. The F ratios for the happiness analyses were 5.81, p =
wife's low-intensity negative affect-the sum of .0176, and 4.50, p = .0362. Conducting a covari-
whining, anger, sadness, domineering, disgust, ance analysis with the consequent as the covari-
fear, and stonewalling-did predict divorce, but it ate, however, we found that both F ratios became
did not predictively discriminate between happy nonsignificant, F(1,126) = 3.85, p = .052, and
and unhappy stable marriages. F(1,126) = 3.43, p = .066.
In the stability analyses, the active listening F
ratios for interest, affection, humor, and valida- Reciprocating negativity in kind: high-intensity
tion following the expression of negative affect negative affect. We examined the reciprocation of
by the partner were .06, .24, .12, and .00, respec- high-intensity negative affect, and the F ratios for
tively, all nonsignificant. These sequences oc- the stability analyses were 8.03, p = .006, for the
curred infrequently for all couples, totaling ap- husband's reciprocation of high-intensity negativ-
proximately 4 seconds out of 900. Using all four ity and 4.88, p = .031, for the wife's reciprocation
variables in a discriminant function analysis, x2 of high-intensity negativity, both significant. The
(4) = 3.92, ns. In the satisfaction analyses, the F unstable couples engaged in these sequences an
ratios were .77, .00, .02, and 3.92, respectively, average of 22.65 and 16.06 seconds, whereas the
all nonsignificant. Using all four variables in a stable couples engaged in these sequences an av-
discriminant function analysis, x2 (4) = .32, ns. erage of 6.78 and 6.90 seconds, for the husband's
These active listening exchanges hardly ever oc- and wife's reciprocation, respectively. Conduct-
curred, and they were not predictive of differen- ing a covariance analysis with the consequent as
tial marital outcomes. the covariate, however, we found that both F ra-
tios became nonsignificant, F(1,54) = 3.11, p =
Negative Reciprocity Models .083, and F(1,54) = .45, ns. The respective F ra-
tios for the happiness analyses were .90, ns, and
Reciprocating negativity in kind: low-intensity 1.64, ns, for the husband's and wife's reciproca-
negative affect. Examining the reciprocation of tion, respectively. Hence, reciprocating high in-
low-intensity negative affect, we found that the F tensity negativity in kind did not predict either
ratios for the stability analyses were 9.12, p = marital instability or unhappiness among the sta-
.003, for the husband's reciprocation of negativity ble couples.
and 12.10, p = .001, for the wife's reciprocation In eight predictions, in the covariance analyses
of negativity (both significant). The unstable cou- only one and one not anticipated-the wife's re-
ple engaged in these sequences an average of ciprocation of low-intensity negative affect in
10.65 and 11.59 seconds, whereas the stable cou- kind-was significant for the prediction of mari-
ples engaged in these sequences an average of tal instability.
4.09 and 3.95 seconds, respectively. We conduct-
ed a covariance analysis with the consequent as Refusing to accept influence: the escalation of neg-
the covariate, and the F ratios became F(2,109) = ative affect models. For the stability analyses, the F
2.54, ns, for the husband's reciprocation of nega- ratios were 17.40, p = .0001, for the husband reject-
14 Journal of Marriage and the Family

ing influence from his wife, and 5.02, p = .029, for Positive Affect Models
the wife rejecting influence from her husband.
Next we examined positive affect. The amount of
However, the covariance analyses yielded F(1,126)
= 10.53, p = .002, and F(1,126) = .98, ns, respec- positive affect was significantly related to stabili-
tively. For the happiness analyses, the F ratios ty, with F ratios 7.67, p = .008, for husband posi-
were .30, ns, and 1.18, ns, for husbands and wives, tive affect, and 7.64, p = .008, for wife positive
affect (stable husbands = 62.23 seconds, unstable
respectively. Hence, as expected, the evidence
shows that divorce was predicted only by the hus- husbands = 31.76 seconds; stable wives = 59.05
band's refusing to accept influence from his wife. and unstable wives = 31.23 seconds), discrimi-
nant function x2 (2) = 8.74, p = .013. Positive af-
fect also was related to happiness, with F ratios
Negative Start-Up Model 5.27, p = .027, for the husband's positive affect,
The F ratios for stability were 6.83, p = .01, for and 8.25, p = .007 for the wife's positive affect,
the husband's start-up and 8.79, p = .004, for the X2(2) = 8.14,p = .017.
wife's start-up. However, the covariance analysis
revealed that F(1,126) = 1.03, ns, for the husband's Positive-to-negative affect ratio model. We com-
start-up and F(1,126) = 10.81, p = .0001, for the puted the ratio of positive affect to the sum of
wife's start-up. Hence, as expected, there was evi- positive and negative affect for each wife and
dence only for the wife's start-up predicting di- husband. For the stability analyses, the F ratios
vorce. For the happiness analyses, F ratios were were 9.56, p = .003 for the wife's ratio, and 9.96,
3.64, p = .059, and .13, ns, respectively. The co- p = .003 for the husband's ratio, respectively, x2
variance analyses for the marginally significant (2) = 10.57, p = .0051. (M for stable husbands =
effect revealed that F(1,109) = .08, ns, respectively. .42, unstable husbands = .16; M for stable wives
= .38, unstable wives = .15.) For the happiness
De-Escalation Models analyses, the F ratios were 5.88, p = .020, and
1.61, ns, respectively, for husband and wife ra-
De-escalating low intensity negativity. The F ra- tios, with discriminant function %2(2) = 5.36, p =
tios for stability were 7.30, p = .009, for the hus- .069. (M for happy husbands = .49, unhappy hus-
band's de-escalation and 3.81, p = .056, for the bands = .36; M for happy wives = .48, unhappy
wife's de-escalation. In the covariance analyses, wives = .27.)
for the wife's de-escalation, F(1,126) = 1.35, ns,
but for the husband's de-escalation, F(1,126) = Contingent versus noncontingent positive affect
6.47, p = .014. Hence, as expected, there was evi- models. The analyses we conducted revealed that
dence that the husband's de-escalation of low- positive affect was the only variable that predict-
intensity negative affect predicts marital stability, ed marital stability and also was able to discrimi-
which is consistent with results reported by nate between stable, happily married couples and
Gottman (1979). For the happiness analyses, the stable, unhappily married couples. To understand
F ratios were 2.69, ns, and 1.86, ns, respectively. the role of positive affect, we constructed a path
analytic model among the following variables:
De-escalating high-intensity negative affect. The positive affect, the sum of husband and wife bat-
second de-escalation sequence involved the cou- 'em-back sequences, the sum of husband and wife
ple moving from high-intensity negative affect to de-escalation sequences, and the sum of husband
neutral affect. The F ratios for stability were 3.47, and wife negative reciprocity sequences (combin-
p = .068, for the husband's de-escalation, and ing reciprocity of low- and high-intensity nega-
2.53, ns, for the wife's de-escalation. The F ratios tive affect).
for the covariance analyses were 3.29, p = .072, The model Figure 1 constructed, using the
for the husband's de-escalation and 2.12, ns, for Bentler computer program EQS (1989), was suc-
the wife's de-escalation. The absence of a stability cessful in fitting the covariance matrices of all
effect for the wife's de-escalation of high- three groups. For the stable and happily married
intensity negative affect is inconsistent with re- group, X2 (1) = .35, p = .55, Bentler-Bonett
sults reported by Gottman (1979). For the happi- Normed Index (BBN) = .992, Comparative Fit
ness analyses, the F ratios were .56, ns, and .07, Index (CFI) = 1.00; for the stable and unhappily
ns, respectively, and the discriminant function married group, x2 (1) = .25, p = .62, BBN = .991,
was X2(2) = .88, ns. CFI = 1.00, and for the divorced group, x2 (1) =
Process Models of Marriage 15

FIGURE 1: COMPARISON OF PATH MODELS FOR THE STABLE AND HAPPILY MARRIED, THE
STABLE AND UNHAPPILY MARRIED, AND THE DIVORCED

STABLE,HAPPY

STABLE,UNHAPPY

DIVORCED

1. NEGATIVE
RECIPROCITY

.41
.41,32
16 Journal of Marriage and the Family

FIGURE 2. PROBABILITY THAT HUMOR BY WIFE de-escalation, the Four Horsemen, and total posi-
SIGNIFICANTLY DECREASED HUSBAND'S HEART RATE
tive affect, was employed in making predictions
about both divorce and satisfaction. For the di-
.35- vorce prediction, the canonical correlation was
.30- .69, with a correct stability-divorce prediction of
.25- 82.5%. For the satisfaction prediction among the
^ .20- stable couples, the canonical correlation was .60,
,-
.15-
with a correct stability-divorce prediction of 80%.
.

.10-
The only one-tailed statistically significant
predictions of marital outcome involved the phys-
0c

.05-
0 iological soothing of the male by either the male
Divorced
himself or by the female. The events that were in-
Happy Unhappy
volved in this prediction were de-escalation by
the husband, G2 (2) = 9.20, p < .01; humor by the
.40, p = .53, BBN = .970, CFI = 1.00. We then
wife, G2 (2) = 3.80, p < .05; validation by the hus-
compared the models across all three groups. The band G2 (2) = 10.60, p < .01; affection by the hus-
omnibus test showed that the path coefficients for
the three groups were significantly different, band, G2 (2) = 8.10, p < .01. Figures 2-5 summa-
rize these statistically significant predictions of
X2(11) = 22.87, p = .018. Testing specific path co- marital outcome obtained by the probabilities that
efficients with cumulative multivariate statistics
these positive events would result in significant
showed that the paths between variables 3 and 4
were significantly different across groups, as physiological soothing of the male. Figure 2
shows that the probability that humor by the wife
were the paths between 2 and 4 and between 2
and 3. (Model specifications are available from significantly decreased the husband's heart rate
was significantly higher in couples who eventual-
the author.) Only in the stable, happy group was
there a positive association between de-escalation ly wound up stable and happy than in the other
two groups of couples. Figure 3 shows that the
and positive affect (variables 3 and 4). In the
other two groups, the path coefficient was nega- probability that de-escalation by the husband sig-
tive. The relationship between escalation (vari- nificantly decreased his own heart rate was signif-
able 2, bat-'em-back) and positive affect (variable icantly higher in couples that eventually wound
up stable and happy than in the other two groups
4) was more negative in the stable, happy group of couples. Figure 4 shows that the probability
than in the stable, unhappy group and in the di-
that validation by the husband significantly de-
vorced group. The relationship between escala-
creased his heart rate was significantly higher in
tion (variable 2) and de-escalation (variable 3)
was stronger in the stable, happy group than in couples that eventually wound up stable and
the other two groups. happy than in the other two groups of couples.
Figure 5 shows that the probability that affection
by the husband significantly decreased his heart
Strength of the Predictions rate was significantly higher in couples who
The same discriminant function model, combin- eventually wound up stable and happy than in the
other two groups of couples.
ing the models of start-up, high and low intensity The same discriminant function model was
FIGURE 3. PROBABILITY THAT DE-ESCALATION BY HUSBAND
employed in making predictions of both divorce
SIGNIFICANTLY DECREASED HUSBAND'S HEART RATE
and satisfaction. For the divorce prediction, the
canonical correlation was .69, with a correct
.35
stability-divorce prediction of 82.5%. For the
.30- .........
satisfaction prediction among the stable couples,
.25-............ the canonical correlation was .60, with a correct
.20- ........ stability-divorce prediction of 80%.
.......1
15..
.10.. ....... DISCUSSION
Consistent with Gottman (1994), we found no ev-
0
idence in the study of newlyweds to support the
Happy Unhappy Divorced model of anger as the destructive emotion in mar-
ProcessModelsof Marriage 17

FIGURE 4. PROBABILITY THAT VALIDATION BY HUSBAND modelsthatpredicteddivorce.The patternpredic-


SIGNIFICANTLY DECREASED HUSBAND'S HEART RATE tive of divorcewas negativestart-upby the wife,
refusal of the husbandto accept influence from
.35
his wife, wife's reciprocationof low intensity
.30-.......................................................................
negativityin kind,andthe absenceof de-escalation
of low intensitynegativityby thehusband.The only
.25-........... ....... variablethat predictedboth maritalstabilityand
maritalhappinessamong stable couples was the
.20-- -
amountof positive affect in the conflict.Through
pathmodeling,we foundthatonly in coupleswho
eventually wound up stable and happy was this
0 :
positiveaffect contingent,actingin the serviceof
Happy Unhappy Divorced the de-escalationof negativity.Also, only in this
group, if couples refused to accept influence,
riages.Instead,we foundevidencethatreplicated were they also likely to de-escalate.The relation-
the Gottmanfindingsthatcontempt,belligerence, ship betweenescalation(bat-'em-back)and posi-
and defensiveness were the destructivepatterns tive affect was more negative in the stable and
duringconflictresolution.Ouranalysessuggested happygroupthanin the stableandunhappygroup
thatthe active listeningmodel, which is the most andin the divorcedgroup.Furthermore, we found
commoncomponentof currentmodels of marital supportfor the hypothesisthatpositive affect and
therapy,occurredinfrequentlyin the resolutionof de-escalationare predictiveof positive outcomes
maritalconflict and was not predictiveof differ- in the marriageto the extentthatthe positiveaffect
entialmaritaloutcomes. or de-escalationinvolvedthe physiologicalsooth-
We needto convey how shockedandsurprised ing of the maleeitherby his partneror himself.
we were by these results for the active listening We concludethatthe marriagesthatwoundup
model. We expectedthatthe validationsequence happy and stable had a softened start-upby the
would predict positive outcomes in marriages. wife, that the husbandaccepted influence from
We have recommended this intervention her, that he de-escalatedlow-intensity negative
(Gottman,1979;Gottman,1994;Gottman,Notar- affect, that she was likely to use humorto effec-
ius, Gonso, & Markman,1978). To deal with our tively soothe him, and that he was likely to use
shockandsurprise,we conducteda seriesof qual- positive affect and de-escalation to effectively
itative analyses on the data from this study and soothe himself. The alternativeto the active lis-
from anotherlongitudinalcohort we have been tening model suggested by these analyses is a
following for 13 years. We examined in detail model of gentleness,soothing,and de-escalation
every videotape and transcriptof every stable, of negativity (negativity by one spouse is fol-
happycouple.It was not a big surpriseto find that lowed by the partner'sneutralaffect). In contrast
couples were not paraphrasing theirspousesvery to this gentle de-escalationand soothing,the ac-
often. We also found thatthey were not summa- tive listening model is a more confrontational
rizing their partner'sfeelings (e.g., "Soundslike model in the sense that it expects people to be
this makesyou prettymad"),nor even summariz- able to be empatheticin the face of negative af-
ing the content of their spouse's statements fect directedat them by their spouses. Based on
("You'd like it if we saved more money each FIGURE 5. PROBABILITY THAT AFFECTION BY HUSBAND
month").Furthermore, they almostnevervalidat- SIGNIFICANTLY DECREASED HUSBAND'S HEART RATE
ed theirspouse's feelings (e.g., "I can understand
why this wouldmakeyou upset").Furtherexami- .40

nation of the validationcode of the SPAFF re- .35 ......... ........................................................................


vealed that our observerswere using our valida-
tion code exclusively for what Duncanand Fiske .30----Z
- / /
(1977) had called "listenerbackchannels."These
are the usual brief vocalizations,head nods, and
facial movementsthatconvey to the speakerthat o ''' ' A:'' ' -- - _ % -
the listeneris tracking.
Instead,in the quantitativeanalysesof process,
we found that there was a particularpatternof Happy Divorced
18 Journal of Marriage and the Family

our analyses, we are led to the hypothesis that the therapeutic use of the active listening model. At
active listening model may be expecting a form of the outset of the complaint-validation sequence,
emotional gymnastics from people who, at that clients are admonished to state their complaints in
moment in that relationship, are somewhat emo- terms of what they want, instead of the partner's
tionally disabled by conflict. We found that even failing. Wile (1995) wrote:
couples in stable, happy marriages do not do this
active listening naturally. Active listening, al- It is impossible to make "I-statements"when
though it may make intuitive sense as a model of you are in the "hating-my-partner, wanting-re-
empathy, may be misguided. After all, even in venge, feeling-stung-and-needing-to-sting-back"
stateof mind.At such a momentyou cannotre-
client-centered, individual therapy, the client is memberwhat an "I-statement" is, and, frankly,
generally complaining about a third party, and the you do not care.(p. 2)
therapist empathizes. In some senses, the promi-
nent active listening model of marital therapy But this is precisely when therapists have sug-
may amount to a kind of celebration of conflict. gested that making I-statements, being nondefen-
Hendrix (1988) has suggested that the active lis- sive, being empathetic, and paraphrasing one's
tening model is the way to "understand one's partnerare necessary. They are unnecessary when
partner's inner world" (p. 118). The suggestion things are going well.
often has been made in advice books that conflict The findings in these data suggest that nega-
is, in fact, the road to intimacy in marriages. For tive affect reciprocity in kind, because it is so
example, the subtitle of Wile's (1988) book is characteristic of all marriages, may be something
"How Conflict Can Improve Your Relationship." that therapists can afford to ignore. It is not the
Markman, Stanley, and Blumberg (1994) suggest problem. The problem, our data suggest, is the es-
that, because conflict is inevitable, managing con- calation of negativity and only by the husband,
flict is an essential communication skill and that which we believe is an index of the husband's re-
the active listening model "may be very different fusal to accept influence from his wife. We are
from the way you normally might do things, but
quite confident that this sequence indexes this
that's the point. Like any skill, these new sugges-
process. Our confidence comes from a detailed
tions become easier with practice" (p. 9). Other observational analysis of transactions of power
writers also have raised questions about how dif- and influence in a recent dissertation on violent
ficult active listening may be for couples in their
couples in our laboratory by Regina Rushe (1995)
everyday lives in the midst of conflict (e.g., Wile, and a subsequent analysis of the same sequence in
1988; N. S. Jacobson, personal communication, the same couples. We are, however, planning to
June 4, 1996). code this pattern of rejecting and accepting influ-
The generally negative results of our analyses ence directly with a new observational coding
in the models of negative affect reciprocity in
system, but the process will take several years to
kind show that negative affect reciprocity in kind
complete.
does not, in general, predict negative marital out- If we assume that this sequence is, in fact, an
comes. There was only evidence that in the first index of the refusal of some husbands to share
few months of marriage the wife in a marriage
power with their wives by rejecting the demands
that is going to be stable is more likely to recipro- she makes, then the issue in therapy becomes not
cate low-intensity negative affect than is the case one of getting the couple to apply the brake in the
in a marriage that is going to be unstable. This re- face of negative affect reciprocity, but in getting
sult was weak in terms of the size of the F ratio, husbands to share power with their wives. Usual-
F(2,109) = 4.39, p = .038. This means that, by ly the wife brings marital issues to the table for
and large, it is characteristic of all marriages,
discussion, and she usually brings a detailed anal-
even happy, stable ones, for people to reciprocate
ysis of the conditions in which this problem oc-
most negativity, even high intensity negativity. In
curs, its history, and suggestions for a solution.
happy and stable marriages husbands do de-esca- Some men, those whose marriages wind up stable
late low intensity negativity, but wives do not, and happy, accept influence from their wives, and
and no one does so when the negativity escalates, some do not. Most sociological analyses of mar-
even in happy, stable marriages. This suggests
riage emphasize the loss of power that men have
that, for the most part, there is not much of a experienced in the last 40 years with the loss of
brake in most marriages once negativity begins. I- the breadwinner role and with women's emer-
statements also have been characteristic of the
gence in the workplace (e.g., Schwartz, 1994).
Process Models of Marriage 19

Women not only work but tend to have careers as riages (Jacobson, Gottman, Waltz, Rushe, & Bab-
meaningful to them as their husbands' careers, al- cock, 1994), and that this arousal is predictive of
beit they are earning a fraction of what men earn the illness of women (Gottman, 1994). For both
for the same work. Our data suggest that only newlywed men and women, marital conflict also
newlywed men who accept influence from their is associated with reduced serum prolactin and
wives are winding up in happy and stable mar- higher levels of the stress-related hormones,
riages. epinephrine, norepinephrine, ACTH, and growth
We conducted exploratory analyses of the cor- hormone, but not cortisol (Malarkey, Kiecolt-
relates of the husband's rejection of influence Glaser, Pearl, & Glaser, 1994), and associated
from the wife. These analyses capitalize on with the suppression of the immune system
chance, and we need to be cautious about accept- (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 1993). Marital separation is
ing these results. The preliminary results suggest associated with even greater immuno-suppression
that the husband's rejection of influence from his in women than in men (Kiecolt-Glaser et al.,
wife is unrelated to the husband's age, income, 1987, 1988). Also, Gottman (1994) reported that
occupation, or educational level. However, we a spouse's contempt was predictive of increased
found that the husbands who are more likely to infectious illness for women, but not for men. The
reject influence from their wives are high on the relationship between marital conflict and illness
MMPI hostility subscale (which assesses suspi- for men, but not for women, was mediated
cious hostility), are smokers, are more likely to through loneliness. Marital conflict, peripheral
regularly use cocaine, were rated by observers as autonomic physiology, cardiovascular reactivity,
dominating their wives in the discussion in our the endocrine system, the immune system, and ill-
oral history interview, make the major decisions ness all are associated in similar and different
in the family, have suffered financial or emotional ways for men and women, and the final assess-
hardships in the marriage, are physically shorter, ment is not yet in. It is likely that the biological,
report being healthier, and are more physically stress-related response of men is more rapid and
active in one-on-one competitive sports than men recovery is slower than that of women, and that
who accept influence from their wives. this response is related to the greater emotional
The data suggest that, though asymmetric, withdrawal of men than women in distressed fam-
gender roles during conflict resolution are a two- ilies. Levenson, Carstensen, and Gottman (1994)
way street. Only newlywed women who are able suggested that the general psychophysiological
to soften their start-up of conflict wind up in finding of men's greater awareness of their car-
happy and stable marriages. The view recom- diovascular physiology may imply that men are
mends a model of marital therapy based on gen- more likely than women to look at their bodily
tleness, on a softened start-up by the wife, on the cues for signs of emotion, whereas women are
male's acceptance of influence, of de-escalation more likely than men to look at the social envi-
of low intensity negativity by the male, high ra- ronment. Hence, men are more likely than women
tios of positivity to negativity, and the use of pos- to withdraw when aroused physiologically. How-
itive affect by both partnersto de-escalate conflict ever, it is likely that physiological soothing (by
and to physiologically soothe the male. Perhaps self and partner) of both spouses will turn out to
these approaches to conflict represent a psycho- be an important component of marital therapy.
logically less costly approach than approaches Many prominent writers on marital therapy
currently taught to distressed couples, and per- have proposed ways of having the therapy do the
haps this less costly approach to conflict is less work of softening confrontation in marriages. For
likely to relapse after treatment. example, Hendrix (1988) has suggested that mari-
We need to comment on the results of physio- tal hostility is actually the result of "childhood
logically soothing the male in marriages that turn wounds," and he speaks of training couples to
out to be stable and happy. Although there is evi- "develop an X-ray vision" that allows them to see
dence for the Gottman-Levenson hypothesis, the wounds behind the hostility. This approach is
there is also evidence that distressed marriages likely to help soften the partner's response to hos-
and high levels of marital conflict are chronically tility when the therapist is present and can help
and diffusely physiologically arousing for reframe a perceived attack as the result of an un-
women, as well as for men (Gottman, 1990; resolved wound. Hendrix even has suggested that
Gottman & Levenson, 1992), that this arousal is one task of marriage is to heal childhood wounds.
especially marked for women in battering mar- This view is consistent with object relations theo-
20 Journal of Marriage and the Family

ry, which also sees marital hostility as primarily how they are established and maintained. A po-
the result of projective identification. Other mari- tentially profitable avenue of investigation would
tal therapies have provided similar mechanisms of involve asking what is the etiology of the dys-
softening confrontation from within the thera- functional patterns predictive of divorce in these
py. Johnson and Greenberg's (1988) "emotion- data, such as the wife's negative start-up and the
focused" therapy, which is based on attachment husband's refusing to accept influence. We ex-
theory, views marital hostility as the result of an pect that the answers to both questions are found
"underlying" insecurity. The therapist helps the in patterns of emotional engagement and respon-
couple reframe hostility in the context of what siveness during everyday interaction. Negative
they call the more "vulnerable" emotions. In this wife start-up may be an escalated complaint, and
way, anger and contempt are reframed as the re- it may be escalated because the lower intensity
sult of an emotion that is probably easier for the complaint was previously ignored by her husband
partner to be empathetic to, namely the spouse's in the nonconflict context. Hence, we predict that
fear. In an examination of who profits most from engagement and responsiveness in neutral affec-
this therapy, Johnson and Greenberg suggested tive contexts and engagement and responsiveness
that it is couples who are able to soften their com- to both positive and negative affect in nonconflict
plaints. In fact, the term "softening" for changing contexts will predict both the wife's negative hard
the hardness of the confrontation should be at- start-up and the husband's refusal to accept her
tributed to them (p. 188). In a recent proposal of a influence. To investigate this, we constructed an
new integrative therapy, Jacobson and Chris- apartment laboratory where couples lived for 24
tensen (1996) suggested an "acceptance" therapy hours (and were videotaped for 12 hours) and
based on helping partners accept their spouses as were instructed to act as they normally would at
they are, instead of demanding change, although home. A subsample of 50 newlywed couples from
they suggest that, paradoxically, acceptance is this study went through this procedure. Currently,
also the basis for obtaining behavior change. The we are coding the 600 hours of videotape from
therapy also includes a set of techniques for the this part of the study to assess whether engage-
therapist to reduce direct confrontation by refram- ment and responsiveness during interactions in
ing the "harder"emotions (anger, hostility) as the these nonconflict contexts are related to the pat-
"softer" emotions (fear, sadness). terns of conflict resolution that we now have
Thus, these therapies are, to some degree, identified.
proposing mechanisms that appear to be aimed at If we are right, then the implications are that
producing results similar to what the newlyweds marital therapy needs to do two things. First, it
in this study who wound up in stable, happy mar- needs to abandon the active listening model in
riages do. Some things may be hard to reprogram, favor of a model of increased softening and gen-
however. For example, it seems unlikely that cou- tleness in start-up, de-escalating, changing the
ples will be able to follow an admonition to in- balance of power in favor of the husband's in-
crease the amount of positive affect in the service creased acceptance of influence from his wife,
of de-escalation. Interest, humor, and affection and increasing physiological soothing (by self and
need to occur naturally, particularly in the context partner). Second, it also may need to find ways of
of conflict resolution. The admonition to be funny working with the couple's patterns of emotional
or to enjoy a partner's attempts at humor are engagement and responsiveness in contexts other
probably self-destructive mechanisms. than conflict resolution, as well as changing the
Of course, we need to raise again the caveat way couples resolve disagreements, which ap-
that it is not logical that, to be effective, therapeu- pears to be the sole preoccupation of all extant
tic interventions for ailing marriages need to be marital therapies.
based on an empirical analysis of what naturally
occurs in well-functioning marriages. There could REFERENCES
be two kinds of lawful relationships, one for mar-
riages that function well and another for mar- Ball, F. L. J., Cowan,P., & Cowan,C. P. (1995).Who's
riages that were ailing but then are repaired. We got the power?Genderdifferencesin partner'sper-
do not think that this will turn out to be the case. ceptionof influenceduringmaritalproblem-solving
discussions.FamilyProcess,34, 303-321.
In our view, what is needed is an empirically Bakeman,R., & Gottman,J. (1986). Observinginterac-
based theory of how positive processes are con- tion: An introductionto sequential analysis. New
nected in marriages that are functioning well- York:CambridgeUniversityPress.
Process Models of Marriage 21

Bentler, P. M. (1989). EQS: Structural equations pro- Gottman, J. M., Markman, H., & Notarius, C. (1977).
gram manual. Los Angeles: BMDP Statistical Soft- The topography of marital conflict: A sequential anal-
ware. ysis of verbal and nonverbal behavior. Journal of
Birchler, G. R., Weiss, R. L., & Vincent, J. P. (1975). Marriage and the Family, 39, 461-477.
Multimethod analysis of social reinforcement ex- Gottman, J. M., McCoy, K. & Coan, J. (1996). The Spe-
change between maritally distressed and nondis- cific Affect Coding System. In Gottman (Ed.), What
tressed spouse and stranger dyads. Journal of Person- predicts divorce? The measures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
ality and Social Psychology, 31, 349-360. baum.
Christensen, A., & Heavey, C. L. (1990). Gender and Gottman, J. M., Notarius, C., Gonso, J., & Markman, H.
social structure in the demand/withdraw pattern of (1978). A couple's guide to communication. Cham-
marital conflict. Journal of Personality and Social paign, IL: Research Press.
Psychology, 59, 73-82. Gottman, J. M., & Roy, A. K. (1990). Sequential analy-
Christensen, A. (1987). Detection of conflict patterns in sis: A guide for behavioral researchers. New York:
couples. In K. Hahlweg & M. J. Goldstein (Eds.), Un- Cambridge University Press.
derstanding major mental disorder: The contribution Giswold, R. L. (1993). Fatherhood in America. New
of family interaction research (pp. 250-265). New York: Basic Books.
York: Family Process Press. Guerney, B. (1982). Relationship enhancement. San
Christensen, A. (1988). Dysfunctional interaction pat- Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
terns in couples. In P. Noller & M. A. Fitzpatrick Heavey, C. L., Christensen, A., & Malamuth, N. M.
(Eds.), Perspectives on marital interaction (pp. 31- (1995). The longitudinal impact of demand and with-
52). Avon, England: Multilingual Matters. draw during marital conflict. Journal of Consulting
Christensen, A. (1990). Gender and social structure in and Clinical Psychology, 63, 797-801.
the demand/withdrawal pattern of marital conflict. Heavey, C. L., Christopher, L., & Christensen, A.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, (1993). Gender and conflict structure in marital inter-
73-81. action: A replication and extension. Journal of Con-
Coan, J., Gottman, J., Babcock, J. & Jacobson, N. (in sulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 16-27.
press). Battering and the male rejection of influence Hendrix, H. (1988). Getting the love you want: A guide
from women. Aggressive Behavior. for couples. New York: Henry Holt & Co.
Crosbie, J. (1993). Interruptedtime-series analysis with Jacobson, N. S., & Addis, M. E. (1993). Research on
brief single subject data. Journal of Consulting and couples and couple therapy. What do we know?
Clinical Psychology, 61, 966-974. Where are we going? Journal of Consulting and Clin-
Dickstein, S. & Parke, R. D. (1988). Social referencing ical Psychology, 61, 85-93.
in infancy: A glance at fathers and marriage. Child Jacobson, N. S., & Christensen, A. (1996). Integrative
Development, 59, 506-511. couple therapy: Promoting acceptance and change.
Duncan, S. D. Jr., & Fiske, D. W. (1977). Face-to-face New York: W.W. Norton.
interaction: Research methods and theory. Hillsdale, Jacobson, N. S., Gottman, J. M., Waltz, J., Rushe, R., &
NJ: Erlbaum. Babcock, J. (1994). Affect, verbal content, and psy-
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. (1978). The Facial Action chophysiology in the arguments of couples with a vi-
Coding System. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholo- olent husband. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psy-
gists' Press. chology. 62, 982-988.
Gottman, J. (1979). Marital interaction: Experimental Jacobson, N. S., & Gurman, A. S. (1995). Clinical
investigations. New York: Academic Press. handbook of couple therapy. New York: Guilford.
Gottman, J. (1990). How marriages change. In G. R. Jacobson, N. S., & Margolin, G. (1979). Marital
Patterson (Ed.) New directions in family research: therapy. New York: Brunner-Mazel.
Depression and aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Johnson, S. M., & Greenberg, L. S. (1988). Emotionally
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Hills- focused therapyfor couples. New York: Guilford.
dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Malarkey, W. B., Chee, M. A., &
Gottman, J. M. (1994). Why marriages succeed or fail. Newton, T. (1993). Negative behavior during marital
New York: Simon & Schuster. conflict is associated with immunological down-
Gottman, J. M. (Ed.) (1996). What predicts divorce?: regulation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 395-409.
The measures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Fisher, B. S., Ogrocki, P., Stout,
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). J. C., Speicher, C. E., & Glaser, R. (1987). Marital
Meta-emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. quality, marital disruption, and immune function.
Gottman, J. M., & Krokoff, L. J. (1989). The relation- Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 13-33.
ship between marital interaction and marital satisfac- Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Kennedy, S., Malkoff, S., Fisher,
tion: A longitudinal view. Journal of Consulting and L., Speicher, C. E., & Glaser, R. (1988). Marital dis-
Clinical Psychology, 57, 47-52. cord and immunity in males. Psychosomatic
Gottman, J. M. & Levenson, R. W. (1988). The social Medicine, 50, 213-229.
psychophysiology of marriage. In P. Noller & M. A. Krokoff, L. (1987). Anatomy of negative affect in
Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Perspectives on marital interaction working-class marriages. Dissertation Abstracts In-
(pp. 182-200). Philadelphia: Multilingual matters. ternational, 45,7A. (University Microfilms No. 84-22
Gottman, J. M. & Levenson, R. W. (1992). Marital pro- 109).
cesses predictive of later dissolution: Behavior, phys- Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., & Gottman, J. M.
iology, and health. Journal of Personality and Social (1994). Influence of age and gender on affect, physi-
Psychology, 63, 221-233. ology, and their interrelations: A study of long-term
22 Journal of Marriage and the Family

marriages. Journal of Personality & Social Psycholo- Parke, R. D. (1996). Fatherhood. Cambridge, MA: Har-
gy, 67, 56-68. vard University Press.
Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital ad- Parrott, L., & Parrott, L. (1995). Becoming soul mates:
justment and prediction tests: Their reliability and va- Cultivating spiritual intimacy in the early years of
lidity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251-255. marriage. Grand Rapids, MI: Zondervan Publishing
Malarkey, W. B., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Pearl, D., & House.
Glaser, R. (1994). Hostile behavior during marital Patterson, J. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eu-
conflict alters pituitary and adrenal hormones. Psy- gene, OR: Castalia.
chosomatic Medicine, 56, 41-51. Polefrone, J. M., & Manuck, S. B. (1987). Gender dif-
Markman, H. J. (1991). Constructive marital conflict is ferences in cardiovascular and neuroendocrine re-
NOT an oxymoron. Behavioral Assessment, 13, 83- sponse to stressors. In R. C. Barett, L. Biener, &
96. G. K. Baruch (Eds.), Gender and stress pp. 13-38.
Markman, H., Stanley, S., & Blumberg, S. L. (1994). New York: The Free Press.
Fighting for your marriage. San Francisco: Jossey- Popenoe, D. (1996). Life without father. New York:
Bass. Free Press.
McCarter, L. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1996, October). Rushe, R. (1995). Tactics of power and influence in vio-
Sex differences in physiological reactivity to the lent marriages. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
acoustic startle. Paper presented at the Society for University of Washington, Seattle.
Psychophysiological Research, Vancouver, Canada. Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). The effectiveness of psy-
Notarius, C., & Markman, H. (1993). We can work it chotherapy: The Consumer Reports study. American
out: Making sense of marital conflict. New York: Psychologist, 50, 965-974.
Putnam. Siegel, J. (1992). Repairing intimacy: An object rela-
Obrist, P. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology. tions approach to couples therapy. Northvale, NJ:
New York: Plenum. Jason Aronson.
Oggins, J., Veroff, J., & Leber, D. (1993). Perceptions Schwartz, P. (1994). Peer marriage. New York: The
of marital interaction among Black and White newly- Free Press.
weds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Wiggins, J. (1977). Personality and prediction. New
65,494-511. York: Addison-Wesley.
Olson, D. H., & Ryder, R. G. (1970). Inventory of Mari- Wile, D. B. (1988). After the honeymoon: How conflict
tal Conflicts (IMC): An experimental interaction pro- can improve your relationship. New York: Wiley.
cedure. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 32, 443- Wile, D. B. (1995). After the fight: A night in the life of
448. a couple. New York: Guilford.
Papero, D. V. (1995). Bowen family systems and mar- Winkler, I., & Doherty, W. J. (1983). Communication
riage. In N. S. Jacobson & A. S. Gurman (Eds.), Clin- styles and marital satisfaction in Israeli and American
ical handbook of couple therapy (pp. 11-30). New couples. Family Process, 22, 229-237.
York: Guilford.

We've Expanded Our Product Line!


NCFR
uProudlyAnnounces
Historical Perspectives on Families andJustice in Dist-ressed
Its Latest Collection
Communities (V)
JacquelineJones
J.cq-oi-oJ:oo
Resources:
Regardlessof their divergentcollective histories,and their raio
identification,Americanfamiliesin distressedconr
of Family
i Resources.
adaptivestrategiesin their strugglesto resist the d
Approx.60 ill.
94V3 NCRMember
The Intentional Family: How
$39.95
s
Over 150 products-39 NEW items-arranged by topic:
World(B)
WilliamJ. Dohey
Dr. Dohertyoffers provenstr
Adolescents,& Families*Diversity
* Children, &Families ?
amidstt.day's huriedandr * Divorce& Families
* FamilyLife Education* Grief,Death,&Loss* Health& Families
*
familiesdrift towarda I
sat'egiesfor beco.i * Midlife,Aging, & Families* Policy & Families* Relationships,
rituals. Ta
Fathr's'1 * Marriage,& Families*Therapy& Families*Violence& Families* Work& Families?

Obtainyourfree copy of our 39-page Catalog. Special discountprices for NCFR members.
National Council on Family Relations
3989 CentralAve. NE, Suite 550
Minneapolis,MN 55421
NCFR
NCFR
Toll Free: 888-781-9331 * Phone: 612-781-9331
FAX: 612-781-9348 * E-mail: ncfr3989@ncfr.com
Visitour Websiteat www.ncfr.com JMF298

You might also like