You are on page 1of 19

Journal of Management & Organization

http://journals.cambridge.org/JMO

Additional services for Journal of Management &


Organization:

Email alerts: Click here


Subscriptions: Click here
Commercial reprints: Click here
Terms of use : Click here

Emotional intelligence in teams: Development and initial


validation of the short version of the Workgroup
Emotional Intelligence Prole (WEIP-S)

Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

Journal of Management & Organization / Volume 15 / Issue 04 / September 2009, pp 452 - 469
DOI: 10.1017/S1833367200002546, Published online: 02 February 2015

Link to this article: http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S1833367200002546

How to cite this article:


Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence (2009). Emotional intelligence in teams: Development and
initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Prole (WEIP-S).
Journal of Management & Organization, 15, pp 452-469 doi:10.1017/S1833367200002546

Request Permissions : Click here

Downloaded from http://journals.cambridge.org/JMO, IP address: 138.251.14.35 on 19 May 2015


Copyright © eContent Management Pty Ltd. Journal of Management & Organization (2009) 15: 452–469.

Emotional intelligence in teams:


Development and initial validation
of the short version of the
Workgroup Emotional Intelligence
Profile (WEIP-S)
P ETER J J ORDAN
Centre for Work Organization and Wellbeing, Griffith University, Nathan QLD, Australia

S ANDRA A L AWRENCE
Centre for Work Organization and Wellbeing, Griffith University, Nathan QLD, Australia

ABSTRACT
Research reveals that emotional intelligence is an important factor in predicting performance in
teams. In this article, we initially outline a theoretical model for examining emotional intelligence
in teams. Using this model, we test a short version (16 items) of the self-report Workgroup Emo-
tional Intelligence Profile (WEIP). Evidence from three studies supports this model. Two samples of
620 and 217 employees support the hypothesized structure of the WEIP-S. Four distinct constructs
were derived: Awareness of own emotions; Management of own emotions; Awareness of others’
emotions; and Management of others’ emotions. The WEIP-Short Version (WEIP-S) scale, there-
fore, is based on abilities that are vital during the interaction of team members. Data from 99
employees provide evidence of test–retest stability for the WEIP-S across three time periods. Limita-
tions and potential uses in management research for this short-version scale are discussed.

Keywords: emotions; emotional intelligence; emotional awareness, emotional management, teams;


measurement

T he importance of emotions in work settings


has been established (Ashforth & Humphrey
1995; Jordan & Troth 2006; Weiss & Cropanzano
& Wolff 2001) and team performance (Jordan &
Troth 2004). Mayer and Salovey (1997) devel-
oped a model of emotional intelligence as a set of
1996). Emotional intelligence, a multi-dimension- emotion processing abilities that together con-
al construct that links emotion and cognition with tribute to improving social interactions. The four
the aim of improving human interactions (Mayer related emotion processing abilities are a) aware-
& Salovey 1997), has been linked to improved ness of own and others’ emotion, b) emotional
workplace behaviour (Aritzeta, Swailes & Senior facilitation, c) emotional understanding and d)
2007) and in particular team behaviour (Druskat management of own and others’ emotions (Mayer

452 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

& Salovey 1997). While other models of emotion- EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ABILITIES IN
al intelligence vary from this construct (e.g. Coop- WORK TEAMS
er & Sawaf 1997; Goleman 1995), the common The comprehensive theoretical framework devel-
point they share is a focus on emotional awareness oped by Tannenbaum, Beard and Salas (1992)
and emotional management as core abilities. contends that team performance (i.e. quality,
Given that emotion plays a prominent role quantity, outputs) is influenced by team input
in the workplace (Ashforth & Humphrey characteristics (individual characteristics, work
1995; Ashkanasy 2003) and that possessing structure, team characteristics, task characteris-
emotional intelligence is an advantage in a con- tics), team processes and organizational and situ-
text often requiring interpersonal communica- ational characteristics. Using Tannenbaum et
tion (Ashkanasy, Hartel & Zerbe 2000; Kelly al.’s (1992) team effectiveness framework, our
& Barsade 2001), it is surprising to find that article develops a theoretical model and argues
few measures of emotional intelligence examine that emotional intelligence is a team input char-
contextually specific abilities used by employ- acteristic.
ees in the workplace (Jordan, Ashton-James & Emotional intelligence comprises four abili-
Ashkanasy 2006). Van Rooy and Viswesvaran ties, emotional awareness (own and others),
(2004), in their meta-analysis of the research emotional management (own and others),
evidence on emotional intelligence, note that emotional understanding (understanding emo-
the construct is in the early stage of construct tional cycles and progressions) and emotional
development and measurement, and identify facilitation (generating emotions) (Mayer &
the need for more refined measures. Addition- Salovey 1997). Mooney, Holahan and Amason
ally, we argue that to maximise response rates (2007) argue that emotional interaction is
and survey completions in organisational sur- essential at a group level to enhance relation-
veys, workplace-relevant measures of emotional ships in those groups. In terms of understand-
intelligence should be valid, but as short as ing how emotional intelligence works in teams,
possible to minimize disruption to employees. we focus on abilities relating to dealing with
In this article, we examine both the theoretical your own emotions and abilities relating to
need for, and assessment of, a parsimonious dealing with other peoples’ emotions (see Fig-
model of emotional intelligence in work teams. ure 1). In both the theoretical development of
Our aim is to produce a theoretically driven and emotional intelligence (George 2000; Mayer &
psychometrically sound short measure that is Salovey 1997) and in empirical studies (e.g.
indicative of behaviours that constitute emotional Jordan & Troth 2004), researchers have indi-
intelligence in a team setting and hope that this cated that it is important to distinguish
measure will enhance future research between EI between abilities related to dealing with self
and behaviour/performance in teams. Initially, we (own emotions) and abilities relating to how
outline a theoretical model for examining emo- we deal with others (others’ emotions). Previ-
tional intelligence in teams and review existing ous research has shown clear differential effects
workplace-relevant measures of emotional intelli- for the focus on own and others’ emotions (see
gence. Using this model, we then develop and Jordan & Troth 2004). The abilities relating to
test a short-version (16 items) of the self-report own emotions involve intra-personal abilities
Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile including emotional self-awareness, while the
(WEIP) on work-based samples in three studies. abilities relating to deal with others draws on
We conclude the article by examining the limita- interpersonal abilities such as communication
tions and potential uses in management research abilities and conflict resolution abilities (Gard-
for this short-version scale. ner 1983).

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 453
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

Emotional Abilities Emotional


Awareness Management

Self Awareness of Management of


Own Emotions Own Emotions
Focus of
Attention

Awareness of Management of
Others Others’ Emotions Others’ Emotions

F IGURE 1: A MODEL OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ABILITIES IN TEAMS

Emotional awareness and emotional manage- dence has emerged which demonstrates a link
ment abilities have important consequences for between emotional self awareness and the ability
performance within teams, helping to maintain to respond to emotional cues (Lane, Reiman,
effective and appropriate relationships with fellow Axelrod, Yun, Holmes & Schwartz 1998)
workers (Jordan & Troth 2004), contributing to Emotional self-awareness has been identified
better information exchange and decision-making as contributing to leadership effectiveness (Sosik
in teams (Pelled, Eisenhardt & Xin 1999) and & Megerian 1999) and predicting team perform-
facilitating functional conflict resolution, rather ance (Jordan & Ashkanasy 2006). Silvia (2002)
than dysfunctional conflict resolution in teams has demonstrated that high emotional self-aware-
(Jehn & Mannix 2001; Jordan & Troth 2004). ness acts as a damper on individuals’ experience
In the next section, we examine these abilities of intense emotions, no matter whether these are
in more detail to reveal how each contributes to positive emotions or negative emotions. In other
better performance within a team context. We words, those individuals with high emotional
simultaneously reveal potential methods for self-awareness will experience more moderate
measuring these abilities through self report by emotional reactions in response to intense emo-
asking respondents to reflect on their behaviour tion triggering situations and, therefore, can com-
in teams, rather than on their general preferences municate more effectively with their team
or attitudes. members (Wolff, Pescosolido & Druskat 2002).

Awareness of own emotions Management of own emotions


Awareness of own emotions involves being in The management of own emotions involves an
touch with our moment-to-moment feelings. individual’s ability to connect or disconnect from
Awareness of own emotions is often revealed in an emotion depending on its usefulness in any
an individual’s ability to discuss and disclose the given situation (Mayer & Salovey 1997). This has
emotions they are experiencing (Pennebaker & often been manifested in the individual’s ability to
Francis 1996). We contend that emotional aware- hold back on immediate reactions and delay
ness can be measured by asking respondents to judgements and then to express them in a more
reflect on the extent to which they are able to dis- considered manner. The advice to ‘think and then
cuss and disclose their emotions. Research evi- act’ and admonishments to ‘count to 10 before

454 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

replying’ and listening techniques such as ‘pause, skill is most commonly manifested in people’s
breathe and relax then respond’ are all attempts at ability to read faces and body language. While
managing emotions by expressing feelings and Ekman (1971) argues that emotion is most accu-
thoughts in an appropriate way when communi- rately read by examining the face, de Meijer
cating. Researchers have shown a clear link (1989) contends that the use of body language
between the experience of intense emotion and provides accurate information for assessing emo-
lowered cognitive functioning (Drevets & Raichle tions. We argue that asking a respondent to
1998). On this basis, delaying immediate respons- reflect on their ability to read faces and body lan-
es is a clear indication of emotional management guage is a method for measuring the awareness of
ability and we contend that asking respondents to others’ emotions.
reflect on their behaviour in delaying emotional Extensive research has been conducted on
reactions and forming more considered responses reading facial expressions (Ekman 1971; Izard,
(including emotional content) is a method of Wehmer, Livsey & Jennings 1965) and body lan-
measuring emotional self-management. guage (Van Dyne, Ang & Botero 2003) with a
Emotional self-management has been identi- view to improving interpersonal interactions.
fied as an important ability for employees. The Recognizing and analysing the sequence of emo-
expression of emotions in the workplace is a tions that emerge from one’s perceptions is an
stress-relieving action (Folkman & Lazarus 1988) important tool in overcoming negative responses
and is appropriate if the expression of emotion to emotions (Mischel & DeSmet 2000). In order
does not affect relationships. Mumby and Put- to address negative emotions, one first needs to
nam (1992) argue that the expression of emotion be able to accurately read the emotion and distin-
is only productive if emotional expression is con- guish between emotions to effectively respond to
strained to protect relationships. the emotion expressed (Jordan, Lawrence &
In teams, emerging situations such as differ- Troth 2006). Elfenbein, Polzer and Ambady
ences over values or goals, frustrations with prob- (2007) found that the ability of a team to recog-
lems, short timeframes for task performance, or nize teammates’ emotions significantly influenced
the entry of new members into the group, evoke team performance as measured by the goal
emotions that need controlling (Weiss & Cropan- achievement of the teams and team cohesiveness
zano 1996). Mischel and DeSmet (2000) note approximately one year later.
that self-regulation is an important ability in
resolving conflict. We argue that the self-regula- Management of others’ emotions
tion of emotions contributes to better team per- A more controversial ability in teams is the ability
formance. For instance, a team member who to manage other team members’ emotions. In
expresses anger or frustration at another employee some circumstances, emotions of other team
may jeopardize future working relationships members need to be managed to ensure that
(Mumby & Putnam 1992). Indeed, Jordan and working relationships are maintained. For
Troth (2004) demonstrated that emotional self- instance, immediately responding to an anger
management was a key predictor of team perform- episode from a fellow employ can result in retalia-
ance for a short-term problem solving exercise. tion which may exacerbate the situation. On the
other hand, managing this emotion by allowing
Awareness of others’ emotions the employee to vent and release the anger and
Recognizing emotional displays by others and then calming them down to discuss the situation
detecting false expressions of emotion is a funda- can result in a resolution of the anger episode.
mental ability involved in successfully dealing Research has shown that employees who manage
with other people (Mayer & Salovey 1997). This anger episodes in the workplace have a positive

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 455
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

impact on relationships in that workplace (Fitness The rationale for developing a short measure
2000). Encouraging positive emotions such as of emotional intelligence in teams is based on
enthusiasm may result in positive emotional con- theoretical and practical grounds. From a theoret-
tagion amongst workers, which, in turn, increases ical perspective, a short measure simplifies the
the positive interactions between team members understanding of the emotional intelligence abili-
(Barsade 2002) and has implications for motiva- ties that can be useful in teams into a two by two
tion (Christie, Jordan, Troth & Lawrence 2007). explanatory framework (focus of attention: own,
We argue that a way of measuring the ability to others; and ability: awareness, management) and
manage others’ emotions is to ask respondents to conforms to the theoretical framework of team
reflect on their behaviour in creating a positive effectiveness developed by Tannenbaum et al.
environment and their ability to use emotional (1992). Researchers argue the need for both par-
contagion to encourage the teams with whom simonious theory and measures (Morrell 2004).
they work (Kelly & Barsade 2001). In considering the practical issues around survey
administration, both researchers and practitioners
Summary are constantly looking for shorter, more parsimo-
In summary, we expect that individuals who have nious measures. The length of these question-
better emotional awareness and emotional man- naires, in particular, enhances the risk that
agement abilities will be more effective contribut- responses will be indifferent or incomplete, which
ing to their teams (Druskat & Wolff 2001), in turn, threatens the validity of results drawn
through their improved ability to communicate from such data (Periatt, LeMay & Chakrabarty
with their fellow team members and their ability 2004). Thus a valid, reliable and easy to use short
to ensure an effective emotional tone appropriate measure of emotional intelligence in teams would
to the work that needs to be completed. To contribute a valuable practical resource to
achieve this effective emotional tone, the individ- researchers and managers. We note that the cre-
uals need to focus both on themselves (own emo- ation of short forms of existing scales have been
tions) and on reacting effectively with their fellow seen as a valuable contribution to the discipline
team members (others’ emotions). In the next (e.g. Guppy et al. 2004; Rammstedt & John
section we outline how this model can be meas- 2007). We also note that this shortened format
ured using self report. complements other popular and widely used self
report measures in this field (e.g. Law, Wong &
MEASURING EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE Song 2004).
IN WORK TEAMS
We propose to develop a short measure of emo- Workplace measures of emotional
tional intelligence in teams that reflects four intelligence
broad self-reported emotional intelligence abili- A number of the emotional intelligence measures
ties that manifest as behaviours in work teams: used in organizational behaviour research do have
Awareness of own emotions (as revealed in their factors that approximate the Mayer and Salovey
ability to discuss and disclose emotions); Man- (1997) ability model of emotional intelligence
agement of own emotions (based on their ability (e.g. emotional self-awareness and management
to delay or withhold strong emotional reac- of emotions, as opposed to traits or preferences;
tions); Awareness of others’ emotions (as see Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 2001). A full review
revealed by the ability to read faces and body of these measures can be found in Jordan (2007).
language) and finally, Ability to manage others’ Of these measures, we have chosen to base our
emotions (based on the ability to positively short measure on the Workgroup Emotional
influence others’ emotions). Intelligence Profile (WEIP: Jordan, Ashkanasy,

456 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

Hartel & Hooper 2002) for two reasons. First, assess the behavioural use of particular emotional
other measures have been developed as uni- intelligence abilities in a team context. Scoring
dimensional constructs with no differentiation of can be performed to produce either a single score
subscales (e.g. Self Report Emotional Intelligence of individual emotional intelligence, 2 broad
Test (SREIT): Schutte et al.1998). For instance, scales (Ability to deal with own emotions, Ability
the SREIT contains 33 items with a Cronbach’s to deal with others’ emotion), or 7 subscales
alpha of .89 (Schutte et al. 1998) that provides a (Awareness of own emotions; Ability to discuss
single measure of emotional intelligence based on own emotions, Ability to use own emotions to
Mayer and Salovey’s framework. As outlined facilitate thinking; Ability to recognize other’s
above, we argue that it is important to assess the emotions, Ability to detect false displays of emo-
contribution of each emotional intelligence abili- tion, Empathy; and Ability to manage other’s
ty on interpersonal behaviours and performance emotions). The WEIP has been a valid and reli-
within teams. able measure, particularly within student sample
Second, other measures assess general abilities populations. Jordan and colleagues validated the
and skills and the results of the tests are then measure’s subscale factor structure using
extrapolated to specific research contexts (e.g. exploratory factor analysis and provided evidence
MSCEIT: Mayer et al. 2001; Wong & Law Emo- of convergent validity with trait mood and con-
tional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS): Law, et al. current validity with self-monitoring, empathy,
2004). Thus, for example, the ability to read creative thinking and emotional control (Jordan
facial expressions is taken to indicate that an et al. 2002). Further, there is evidence of predic-
employee behaviourally uses this ability (aware- tive validity with other-rated team process per-
ness of others’ emotions) in the workplace. We formance and other-rated team goal focus
argue that it is important to assess abilities as (Jordan et al. 2002), positive team behaviours
expressed as actual behaviour in a specific context and course grade (Sue-Chan & Latham 2004),
(in this case the team) and, therefore, we require career advancement (Donohue & Stevensen
a measure that can identify the specific abilities 2006), and a positive relationship between leaders
that are actually being used in those particular and followers emotional intelligence scores (Kel-
contexts. This approach is supported by Mischel lett, Humphrey & Sleeth 2006). Reliability statis-
and Shoda (1998), who argue that actual behav- tics reported for the WEIP demonstrate adequate
iour is a product of both personal attributes (e.g. internal consistency for the scale overall (α = .86
personality and abilities) and the context in to .93), the two broad scales (α =.76 to .85) and
which those behaviours emerged. Moreover, the the 7 subscales (α =.58 to .85; Jordan et al. 2002;
episodic and situationally specific nature of emo- Moriarty & Buckley 2003; Sue-Chan & Latham
tions (Ortony, Clore & Collins 1988) provides a 2004).
framework for measuring emotional abilities Subsequent refinement of the WEIP (WEIP-
within particular contexts, particularly in busi- 6; Jordan & Troth 2004) increased the number of
ness settings where emotions can be used both to items to 30 (using the original item pool) to
engender and to decrease performance (Ashforth improve the reliability of the measure and the
& Humphrey 1995). number of subscales was reduced to 5 (Empathy
Aligned with the Mayer and Salovey (1997) was removed and Ability to detect false emotions
framework for emotional intelligence, the WEIP was merged with the Ability to recognize others’
(Jordan et al. 2002) is a complex measure of self- emotions) to reflect the redevelopment of the
reported individual emotional intelligence within emotional intelligence model in Mayer and
a team context. From an original item pool of 52, Salovey (1997). Following this refinement, the 5
the final measure consists of 27 items. Items subscales became: Ability to recognize own emo-

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 457
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

tions, Ability to discuss own emotions, Ability to STUDY 1: SCALE EVALUATION


manage own emotions; Ability to recognize oth-
ers’ emotions, Ability to manage others’ emotions DEVELOPMENT OF THE WEIP-SHORT
(WEIP-6; Jordan & Troth 2004). Jordan & Troth VERSION
(2004) reported adequate reliability statistics for The initial survey used 25 items selected from the
the scale overall (α =.80), the two broad scales 30 item WEIP-6 (Jordan & Troth 2004), using a
(Ability to deal with own emotions, α = .79, 7-point Likert response format ranging from 1
Ability to deal with others’ emotion, α = .80) and (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) for each
the 5 subscales (α = .71–.80). item that conforms to the theoretical model
The WEIP and WEIP-6, in particular, are use- described earlier in this article (Figure 1). In this
ful measures as they assess (in part) dimensions of instrument, there were 5 items relating to ability
emotional intelligence by both ability (awareness, to discuss own emotions (Own Aware; e.g. ‘I can
management) and focus of attention (own, oth- discuss the emotions I feel with other team mem-
ers). As we argue above, our framework examines bers’), 8 items relating to the ability to control
abilities in relation to the individual’s themselves emotional responses (Own Manage; e.g. ‘I respect
and their abilities in dealing with other team the opinions of team members, even when I
members as this differentiation allows us to think they are wrong’), 7 items relating to the
examine both intra-personal and interpersonal ability to recognize others’ feelings (Other Aware;
abilities in a team context (Gardner 1983). More- e.g. ‘I am able to describe accurately the way oth-
over, as the measures assess emotional intelligence ers in the team are feeling’) and 5 items relating
behaviours used in team contexts, they are the ability to positively influence others’ emo-
aligned with our argument that it is important to tional states (Other Manage; e.g. ‘My enthusiasm
examine the effects of emotional intelligence abil- can be contagious for members of a team’). Each
ities in particular contexts (e.g. within teams) to of the 25 items had prima facie validity conform-
more accurately understand processes that are ing to the theoretical model of emotional intelli-
occurring within these contexts. gence in teams (see Figure 1).
However, both the WEIP and WEIP-6 The instructions to respondents read: ‘Please
could not be viewed as short measures. indicate your level of agreement with each of the
Researchers such as Gosling, Rentfrow & following statements about your feelings when
Swann (2003) have expressed the advantage of working in your team. When thinking about
short measures that can be used in longer your team, please think of your immediate work
research surveys and be easily converted to a unit.’ A self-report format was adopted for two
self-scoring format for self-development pur- reasons. First, this format is relatively simple to
poses. Our aim, therefore, is to use a selection complete (15 minutes). Secondly, as a measure
of the behaviourally-based ability items listed that has been designed to examine behaviours
in the revised WEIP-6 (Jordan & Troth 2004) within a specific context (i.e. the respondent’s
to develop a new short-version of the scale: the work team), we considered that self-report based
WEIP-Short (WEIP-S). Scale evaluation of the on the reflections of the respondent behaviours is
inventory is presented in Study 1, followed by a reasonable method to assess the respondent’s
two validation tests: scale replication in Study abilities within that context (see also Jordan et al.
2 and test–retest reliability in Study 3. All three 2002). The method of questioning for this
studies use data obtained from working popu- instrument asks respondents to recall specific
lations and the measure’s structural validity was actual behaviours they have exhibited while
assessed using the statistically rigorous method working in their teams, rather than providing
of confirmatory factor analysis. responses to preferred behaviours. While there

458 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

may be an amount of ego protection and social ing for respondent errors and omissions was con-
desirability in the responses, we don’t anticipate ducted prior to analysis. Missing data for any
this would be any more than for any other ques- scale item were random (less than 2%; Cohen &
tionnaire that asked respondents to reflect on Cohen 1983). Missing data were replaced by val-
their behaviour or emotions (e.g. PANAS; Wat- ues estimated by the Expectation-Maximization
son, Clark & Tellegan 1988). (EM) imputation method in SPSS 10.0.5 (see
Kline 1998).
Method All model estimations in this paper were con-
ducted on covariance matrices, using the Maxi-
Participants and procedure
mum Likelihood (ML) procedure in EQS 6.1
A survey containing the WEIP and several meas- (Bentler & Wu 2005). Fit indexes and cutoff val-
ures of employee attitudes and organizational cli- ues used when conducting confirmatory factor
mate was distributed through a large public sector analyses adhered to recommendations of Hu and
organization’s internal mail system to 640 partici- Bentler (1999). The fit indexes used in all analy-
pating staff and returned via a postage-paid return ses were the CFI, IFI, NFI, NNFI, RMSEA and
envelope. Participation in the survey was volun- the SRMR. Hu and Bentler (1999) argue that for
tary and anonymous. From this sample (Sample analyses using the ML method, good model fit is
1), 620 respondents completed usable surveys demonstrated when the CFI, IFI and NNFI are
(response rate 97%). This high response rate was close to .95, the RMSEA is close to .06 and the
achieved through gaining the full commitment of SRMR is close to .08.
senior management of the organization and a Examination of the data revealed some evi-
process of advertising the project prior to the sur- dence of univariate skewness and kurtosis for sin-
vey going out. We also attribute the high return gle items relating to own manage, other aware
rate to the fact that the survey was administered as and other manage. Corrected test statistics (the
a part of a training course in which all employees Satorra-Bentler rescaled chi-square statistic and
participated. Of the respondents, 299 (48.2%) the CFI Robust) are therefore reported when
were male, the mean age was 40 years, ranging assessing model fit involving those constructs, in
from 18 to 66 years, with 578 (93.2%) being full- order to control for the degree of non-normality
time employees. Four hundred and forty-nine of the data (Kline 1998). Anderson and Gerbing’s
(72%) respondents worked in administrative roles, (1988) recommended two-step procedure was
44 (7%) in technical roles, 87 (14%) in profes- used to conduct the confirmatory factor analyses.
sional roles and 15 (2%) in senior managerial Step One consisted of separately assessing the fit
roles. The average size of the teams that respon- of the four emotional intelligence constructs.
dents worked in was 10 employees. Analysis of the Step Two of the analysis involved assessment of
demographics reveals this is a representative sam- these measurement models together, represented
ple for the organization. as four correlated factors, where each factor repre-
sented a distinct emotional ability (see Table 1).
Results Analysis of the a priori measurement models
Construct validity tests via confirmatory factor for Awareness of Own Emotions (Own Aware)
analyses were conducted to evaluate the emotion- and Management of Own Emotions (Own Man-
al intelligence scale and to evaluate subsequent age) revealed an acceptable (the RMSEA only
revisions to the measure. Model comparisons reached mediocre levels; Brown & Cudeck 1993)
were also conducted to verify that the four-factor and poor fit of the model, respectively (see Table
structure, based on the modified solution, was 1). An examination of the factor loadings for the
the best representation of the data. Data screen- two models also revealed non-significant load-

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 459
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

TABLE 1: I NVENTORY EVALUATION CONFIRMATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS RESULTS a

Source Models χ2 p df Δχ 2 Δdf CFI IFI NFI NNFI RMSEA SRMR

Own Aware A priori 39.80 < .001 5 —- —- .97 .97 .96 .94 .11 .03
Modified 33.28 < .001 2 6.52 3 .97 .97 .97 .91 .12 .03
Own Manage b A priori 128.85 < .001 20 —- —- .90 .90 .88 .85 .09 .05
Modified 32.77 < .001 9 96.08*** 11 .97 .97 .95 .94 .07 .04
Other Aware b A priori 53.00 < .001 14 —- —- .96 .96 .95 .95 .07 .04
Other Manage b A priori 39.15 < .001 5 —- —- .96 .96 .95 .92 .10 .04
Combined Model b 548.21 < .001 203 —- —- .92 .92 .88 .91 .05 .05
Modified Combined Model 271.33 < .001 98 276.88*** 105 .95 .95 .92 .94 .05 .05
a n = 620. *** p < .001
b Robust statistics reported for χ2 and CFI.

ings. Alternatively, analysis of the a priori meas- Step Two of the analysis involved assessment of
urement models for Awareness of Other’s Emo- the Step 1 final measurement models together,
tions (Other Aware) and Management of Other’s represented as four correlated factors, where each
Emotions (Other Manage) items revealed a good factor represented a distinct emotional ability and
and acceptable fit of the model, respectively. responses to the relevant inventory items repre-
From the results above, respecification was sented the factor indicators (see Table 1; Ander-
necessary on both the Own Aware and Own son & Gerbing 1988). This model was consistent
Manage models to attain better fit. Respecifica- with our conceptualization of emotional abilities
tion decisions were based both on content (face and how they are interrelated. Analysis of the
validity) and statistical considerations (standard- model revealed a mediocre fit. The SRMR and
ized residuals, Cronbach’s alpha statistics and RMSEA both reached .06, however, the CFI, IFI,
Wald and LM tests). Deleting problem indicators NFI and NNFI did not reach the .95 cutoff.
is the preferred solution for poor fitting models Respecification was conducted in order to
(Anderson & Gerbing 1988). On this basis, one improve the fit of the model. On this basis, six
indicator was dropped from the Own Aware indicators were dropped, two from the Own
measurement model and two from the Own Manage factor, three from the Other Aware factor
Manage measurement model. The modified and one from the Other Manage factor. The
measurement model for Own Aware did not sta- resulting 16-item modified, combined measure-
tistically improve fit, as compared to the a priori ment model revealed a substantially improved fit
measurement model (Δχ2 (3) = 6.52, p > .05; see as compared to the a priori model (Δχ2 (66) =
Table 1). However, the model now only had sig- 206.74, p < .001). Analysis of the modified com-
nificant, moderately high factor loadings and as bined model (see Table 1), combined with an
this model demonstrated acceptable fit (Anderson examination of factor loadings and Cronbach’s
& Gerbing 1988; Browne & Cudeck 1993; Hu alphas (see Table 2), revealed a good overall fit.
& Bentler 1999), it was retained as a more viable Table 2 presents construct labels, items, stan-
solution than the a priori model. The modified dardized coefficient factor loadings and Cron-
measurement model for Own Manage revealed a bach’s alpha coefficients for the modified
substantially improved fit, as compared to the a inventory. Table 3 describes the means, standard
priori measurement model (Δχ2 (11) = 96.08, p < deviations and correlations for the WEIP-S sub-
.001) and given the combined fit indices, factor scales. The four emotional intelligence constructs
loadings and reliability, was deemed to demon- have moderate correlations with one another (see
strated good fit. Table 3). To verify that the four-factor structure,

460 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

TABLE 2: S TANDARDIZED CONFIRMATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS AND CRONBACH ’ S ALPHA COEFFICIENTS FOR
FINAL INVENTORY ITEMS (WEIP–S)

Standardized Cronbach’s
coefficient alpha
Subscale labels and items factor loadings coefficients

Awareness of Own Emotions (Own Aware) .85


1. I can explain the emotions I feel to team members. .77
2. I can discuss the emotions I feel with other team members. .87
3. If I feel down, I can tell team members what will make me feel better. .66
4. I can talk to other members of the team about the emotions I experience. .76
Managgement of Own Emotions (Own Manage) .77
5. I respect the opinion of team members, even if I think they are wrong. .66
6. When I am frustrated with fellow team members, I can overcome my frustration. .70
7. When deciding on a dispute, I try to see all sides of a disagreement before
I come to a conclusion. .62
8. I give a fair hearing to fellow team members’ ideas. .71
Awareness of Others’ Emotions (Other Aware) .81
9. I can read fellow team members ‘true’ feelings, even if they try to hide them. .81
10. I am able to describe accurately the way others in the team are feeling .78
11. When I talk to a team member I can gauge their true feelings from their
body language. .68
12. I can tell when team members don’t mean what they say. .62
Managem ment of Others’ Emotions (Other Manage) .81
13. My enthusiasm can be contagious for members of a team. .71
14. I am able to cheer team members up when they are feeling down. .68
15. I can get fellow team members to share my keenness for a project. .70
16. I can provide the ‘spark’ to get fellow team members enthusiastic. .79
Note: All factor coefficients significant at p < .001.

consistent with our conceptualization, was the four emotional ability dimensions. Similarly, the
best representation of the items in the WEIP-S, three-factor models represented all possible three-
all possible alternative models were estimated way combinations of the four dimensions. The
based on an item-set derived from the modified evaluation of these models and the chi-square dif-
combined solution (see Mathieu & Farr 1991). ference tests conducted to compare all of the
The models included a one-factor, 3 two-factor alternative models to the four-factor solution, are
and 6 three-factor dimensions. The two-factor shown in Table 4. Results from the alternative
models represented all possible parings of the model estimations and chi-square difference tests

TABLE 3: STUDY 1 – MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS , CORRELATIONS AND INTER -ITEM RELIABILITIES FOR WEIP–Sa
Variables M SD 1 2 3 4

1. Own Aware 4.14 1.15 (.85)


2. Own Manage 5.32 .79 .38*** (.77)
3. Other Aware 4.45 .93 .53*** .43*** (.81)
4. Other Manage 4.71 .88 .58*** .46*** .54*** (.81)
a n = 620. *** p < .001. Inter-item reliabilities in brackets.

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 461
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

clearly defended a four-factor structure for the ple 2), taken from a general population. The
team-based emotional skills. The four-factor solu- modified 16-item, four-factor model derived in
tion in each case was significantly better (p < Study 1 was replicated using Sample 2 data to
.001) than any other of the alternative models verify the construct validity and reliability of the
presented and the difference in CFI ranged from four dimensions of the WEIP-S.
.05 to .19. Respondents did distinguish between
the emotional abilities they perceived they uti- Method
lized in work teams. Further, the model compari-
Participants and procedure
son analysis substantiates the notion that there
are four conceptually distinct emotional abilities Data for this study were collected in a national web
that participants utilize in team-based work. survey. Participants were recruited via advertise-
ments in national and local print and radio media
WEIP-S VALIDATION and were asked to complete the survey anony-
To provide further evidence of the construct mously and voluntarily. As such the data in this
validity of the WEIP-S, we conducted additional analysis are drawn from a convenience sample.
analyses including a model replication analyses in Two hundred and seventeen working respon-
Study 2 (using a general working population dents provided usable responses to all items in the
sample), and test-retest reliability in Study 3 web survey. Of these, 63 (29%) were male, 154
(using a sample from a single organization). (71%) were female and their mean age was 36 to
40 years, ranging from under 20 to over 65.
STUDY 2: REPLICATIVE CONFIRMATORY Ninety-nine (46%) had tertiary qualifications; 76
FACTOR ANALYES had completed graduate studies (35%); 26 had
To assess whether the analysis results are a perva- completed a diploma, certificate or apprentice-
sive phenomenon and not an artefact of the par- ship training; and 16 (7%) had completed high
ticular sample used during data collection (see school. Respondents worked in a broad spectrum
DeVellis 1991), the confirmatory factor analyses of industries (e.g. but not limited to, manufactur-
were repeated on data using the 25 WEIP item ing, wholesale trade, finance & insurance, educa-
instrument obtained from a second sample (Sam- tion, health & community services) and the mean

TABLE 4: R ESULTS OF EMOTIONAL ABILITIES MODEL COMPARISONS USING INVENTORY EVALUATION MODIFIED
MODEL ITEMS a

Models χ2 p df Δχ 2b Δdf CFI Δ CFI IFI NFI NNFI RMSEA SRMR


c
4-factor: OwnA OwnM OthA OthM 271.33 < .001 98 —- —- .95 —- .95 .92 .94 .05 .05
3-factor: (OwnA/OwnM) OthA OthM 599.77 < .001 101 328.44*** 3 .85 .09 .85 .83 .82 .09 .08
3-factor: (OwnA/OthA) OwnM OthM 546.86 < .001 101 275.53*** 3 .87 .07 .87 .84 .84 .08 .07
3-factor: (OwnA/OthM) OwnM OthA 507.21 < .001 101 235.88*** 3 .88 .06 .88 .85 .85 .08 .06
3-factor: OwnA (OwnM/OthA) OthM 526.85 < .001 101 255.52*** 3 .87 .07 .87 .85 .85 .08 .07
3-factor: OwnA (OwnM/OthM) OthA 488.25 < .001 101 216.92*** 3 .88 .06 .89 .86 .86 .08 .07
3-factor: OwnA OwnM (OthA/OthM) 482.35 < .001 101 211.02*** 3 .89 .05 .89 .86 .86 .08 .06
2-factor: (OwnA/OwnM) (OthA/OthM) 810.68 < .001 103 539.35*** 5 .79 .15 .79 .77 .75 .11 .09
2-factor: (OwnA/OthA) (OwnM/OthM) 761.44 < .001 103 490.11*** 5 .80 .14 .80 .78 .77 .10 .08
2-factor: (OwnA/OthM) (OwnM/OthA) 759.17 < .001 103 487.84*** 5 .80 .14 .81 .78 .77 .10 .08
1-factor: (OwnA/OwnM/OthA/OthM) 952.76 < .001 104 681.43*** 6 .75 .19 .75 .73 .71 .12 .09

a n = 620.
b Critical χ2 (df = 3, p < .001) = 16.27, χ2 (df = 5, p < .001) = 20.52, χ2 (df = 6, p < .001) = 22.46
c OwnA = Own Aware, OwnM = Own Manage, OthA = Other Aware, OthM = Other Manage

462 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

number of years respondents had worked in their As for the inventory evaluation analyses, the four
organization was 6 years. One hundred and fifty- emotional skill constructs have moderate correla-
two respondents were employed in full-time tions with one another (see Table 6). Rather than
employment (70%); 24 (11%) were employed in conducting model comparison analyses, a simpler
full-time contract positions; and 41 (19%) were test of discriminant validity amongst the factors in
employed in part-time or casual positions. the measurement model was conducted. In such an
analysis, the correlations between the four factors
Results were fixed at 1.0 and a chi-square difference test
Data screening for respondent errors and omis- was used to compare the constrained and uncon-
sions was conducted using the previously stated strained models (Bagozzi & Phillips 1982). The test
procedure. As was the case for the Sample 1, revealed that the modified measurement model fit-
some of the individual items showed evidence of ted the data significantly better than the con-
univariate skewness and kurtosis. When assessing strained model (Δχ2 (6) = 24.04, p < .001). Thus,
the combined four-factor model, corrected test respondents distinguished amongst the emotional
statistics (the Satorra-Bentler rescaled chi-square skills they utilized in work teams. Accordingly, a
statistic and the CFI Robust) were reported to four-factor structure measuring the proposed emo-
control for this non-normality. tional skill constructs was deemed appropriate.
The analysis assessing the modified 16-item,
four-factor model revealed comparable results to STUDY 3: TEST–RETEST RELIABILITY
the evaluation analysis of Sample 1. The four-fac- The purpose of Study 3 was to investigate whether
tor measurement model revealed a good fit (see the four emotional intelligence constructs in the
Table 5). The CFI, IFI and NNFI reached or was WEIP-S demonstrated stability over time.
close to .95, the SRMR and RMSEA were both
below cutoff values, the factor loadings were Method
moderately high and each scale had a moderate
Participants and procedure
or high level of reliability (see Table 6). Again,
although the NFI was below .90, the measure- A survey containing WEIP-S items was distributed
ment model shows good overall fit, given the to 560 employees at three points in time, 4 months
combined results. apart as a part of their normal mail distribution.

TABLE 5: I NVENTORY REPLICATION CONFIRMATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS RESULTS ab

Models χ2 p df Δχ 2b Δdf Δ CFI CFI IFI NFI NNFI RMSEA SRMR

Combined 4-Factor Model 151.54 < .001 98 —- —- —- .95 .95 .87 .94 .05 .06
Constrained Combined Model 175.58 < .001 104 24.04*** 6 .02 .93 .93 .84 .92 .06 .20

a n = 217. Critical χ2 (df = 6, p < .001) = 22.46


b Robust statistics reported for χ2 and CFI.

TABLE 6: S TUDY 2 – M EANS , STANDARD DEVIATIONS , CORRELATIONS AND INTER -ITEM RELIABILITIES WEIP–S a
Variables M SD 1 2 3 4

1. Own Aware 4.40 1.31 (.86)


2. Own Manage 5.66 0.86 .45*** (.76)
3. Other Aware 4.87 1.15 .37*** .35*** (.86)
4. Other Manage 4.97 1.13 .48*** .46*** .46*** (.86)
a n = 217. *** p < .001. Inter-item reliabilities in brackets.

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 463
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

The employees worked at the organization used to .02 to .19, the standard deviation difference for a
collect data in Study 1, but were not part of the particular construct ranged from .01 to .09.
Study 1 sampling frame. The surveys were complet- None of the means for a construct were signifi-
ed voluntarily and anonymously by the staff and cantly different from one another. All Cronbach’s
returned to the researcher by mail via a postage-paid alphas for the four constructs ranged from .73 to
return envelope. Three hundred and twenty-five .88, with an average reliability of .82. Across time
respondents (58%) completed surveys at Time 1, periods, the matched construct variable correla-
263 (47%) respondents completed surveys at Time tions ranged from .47 to .66 with a mean of .59.
2 and 227 (41%) respondents completed surveys at The alphas and bivariate variable correlations
Time 3. Combined, 99 respondents completed suggest a moderate to high level of internal con-
usable surveys across all three time periods, reflect- sistency. Test–retest reliabilities between the three
ing an 18% response rate. Of these respondents, 58 time periods were moderate to high and reflective
(59%) were male with a mean age of 40 years, rang- of good levels of stability across time for the
ing from 18 to 62 years. Eighty-one (82%) were WEIP-S constructs. These findings provide evi-
full-time employees with 63 (64%) in administra- dence of the reliability and, by extension, the
tive roles, 7 (7%) in technical roles, 10 (10%) in construct validity of the WEIP-S.
professional roles and 4 (4%) in senior managerial
roles. Again, the average size of the teams that SUMMARY DISCUSSION
respondents worked in was 10 employees. In this article, we developed a model demonstrat-
ing the types of emotional intelligence abilities
Results that may be useful in teams. We also have out-
Table 7 demonstrates the consistency of the lined three studies aimed at validating a short-
WEIP-S across three points in time. The mean version of the WEIP, an inventory that measures
difference for a particular construct ranged from self reported emotional awareness and manage-
TABLE 7: S TUDY 3 – D ISTRIBUTIONAL PROPERTIES , RELIABILITIES AND TEST – RETEST RELIABILITY
CALCULATIONS FOR THE WEIP-S CONSTRUCTS
Test–Retest Reliability
M (SD ) Cronbach’s Alpha Time 1&2 Time 1&3 Time 2&3

Own Aware
Time1 4.03 (1.21) .88 .76 .80
Time2 4.22 (1.27) .87 .81
Time3 4.16 (1.18) .87
Own Manage
Time1 5.40 (.76) .74 .67 .64
Time2 5.30 (.78) .77 .77
Time3 5.28 (.73) .73
Other Aware
Time1 4.50 (1.00) .85 .71 .77
Time2 4.61 (1.04) .85 .83
Time3 4.59 (1.01) .84
Other Manage
Time1 4.57 (.93) .80 .78 .79
Time2 4.70 (.97) .84 .82
Time3 4.59 (.96) .83
n = 99.

464 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

ment abilities within team settings. The final this self report measure has shown the importance
WEIP-S inventory consists of 16 items, 4 items of distinguishing between abilities relating to self
for each of the four emotional abilities. Evidence (intrapersonal abilities) and abilities relating to
of construct validity was provided for the final how we deal with others (interpersonal abilities).
inventory though a series of tests, using different Although the emotional intelligence construct is
samples: scale evaluation, discriminant validity, still in early theoretical development, the idea of
construct replication across samples, reliability measuring abilities in relation to own emotions
and test–retest stability. In Study 1, the derived and abilities in relation to other’s emotions there
16-item scale demonstrated acceptable to good has not been broadly articulated before. Figure 1
model fit for each of the emotional intelligence contributes to this understanding.
constructs and a good fit for the combined model In terms of practical applications, WEIP-S
consisting of all four self reported emotional abil- provides a short, easy to use public domain self
ities. Comparison with all possible alternative report, workplace-based measure of emotion
models in Study 1 confirmed that our conceptu- intelligence that can be utilized to assess other
alization of four, distinct self reported emotional team based behaviours and attitudes. This meas-
abilities best represented the data. The replication ure, which relies on a reflection method to collect
study (Study 2) also confirmed a four-factor solu- self reported behaviourally-based responses to
tion for the WEIP-S. It is important to acknowl- items, enables the WEIP-S to be used for staff
edge, however, that the results also indicate that development purposes. The format also enables
there is some empirical overlap (and therefore the instrument to be extended from its current
conceptual overlap) between the constructs, as self-reporting format to a peer-reporting format.
evidenced by moderate bi-variate correlations in
Tables 3 and 6. Internal consistency reliability LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
statistics for the resulting four self reported emo- The first and most obvious limitation of the pres-
tional abilities constructs were moderate to high ent study is that the WEIP-S data relies on self-
across all three studies. Moreover, in Study 3, the report. There is a debate over the best method of
four constructs demonstrated test–retest stability assessing emotional intelligence with significant
over three time periods. criticism of measures that use a self-report format
(see Mayer et al. 2001; Roberts, Zeidner &
CONTRIBUTION TO THEORY AND Matthews 2001). This criticism is based on the
PRACTICE assertion that if emotional intelligence is really
As noted earlier, the rationale for developing a intelligence, then it needs to be tested as other
short measure of emotional intelligence in teams intelligences are tested, using ability testing and
was a based on theoretical and practical grounds. not self-report. In response to this criticism, we
From a theoretical perspective, this article has note that the WEIP-S uses a format in which
contributed to understanding the way in which respondents are asked for responses based on a
emotional intelligence abilities may be used in reflection of their behaviours while working in
teams and conforms to the theoretical framework their team, rather than for behavioural prefer-
of team effectiveness developed by Tannenbaum ences. While this method of questioning may be
et al. (1992). The WEIP-S devised in this paper effected by social desirability and ego protection
demonstrated further evidence for the conceptu- responses, we argue that as a self-development
alization of emotional intelligence abilities within tool it is entirely appropriate for individuals to
team settings as distinguished by both focus of reflect on their situational behaviour. As noted
attention (own, others) and ability (awareness, earlier, there is evidence that the WEIP can pre-
management). In particular, the development of dict performance and that it can differentiate

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 465
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

between different types of performance (e.g. team be compared to a peer assessment measure cur-
and individual; Jordan & Troth 2004). This sug- rently in development. Research has shown that
gests that it is a useful measure for tapping into shorter questionnaires have more potential to
some human potential in terms of emotion and improve the accuracy of peer-self evaluations
how we conceptualise emotion when working (Jordan & Ashkanasy 2006; Rammstedt & John
with others. Both Damasio (1994) and Calne 2007) and the WEIP-S allows us to take advan-
(1999) have pointed out that both intelligence tage of its shorter format to test it against peer
and emotion are required to generate behaviour. reports of an individual’s behaviour.
The aim of the research program outlined in this As mentioned above, the WEIP- S requires
article is not to resolve the issue of self report and extensive testing on its predictive validity in
ability testing in emotional intelligence, but rather applied settings. Researchers can examine the effi-
to offer a theoretically driven and psychometrical- cacy of the WEIP-S for predicting both task and
ly sound short measure that is indicative of emo- contextual performance in teams, at multiple lev-
tional intelligence being used in a team setting. els of analysis. One area of research that needs
A potential limitation of Study 2 is that a con- more examination at the individual level of analy-
venience sampling method was used to collect the sis, for example, is the idea that teams may
data. While this type of sampling prevents us require different emotional intelligence abilities
from drawing conclusions about the generaliz- depending on the type of work to be completed
ability of these data, we collected these data for a by the team. For instance, are different emotional
replication study to provide evidence of the sta- intelligence abilities required for creative research
bility of the WEIP-S between groups and the and development teams when compared with
obtained data from this sampling method did sales teams? The WEIP-S will be useful in
provide us with a reasonably heterogeneous sam- extending this research.
ple. Respondents worked in a broad spectrum of
industries (e.g. manufacturing, wholesale trade, CONCLUSION
finance & insurance, education, health & com- In this article we have developed and tested a
munity services) and, therefore, it could be short measure of emotional intelligence that can
argued that the sample was more heterogeneous be used to measure the emotional intelligence of
than the sample in Study 1, where the partici- members of teams. Our findings provide initial
pants were drawn from a single and possibly cul- support for the WEIP-S as a valid and reliable self
turally homogeneous, organization. report measure of emotional intelligence that
The present research is only the beginning of a assesses emotional intelligence on 4 dimensions:
larger program to confirm the predictive poten- Awareness of Own Emotions, Management of
tial for this short measure of emotional intelli- Own Emotions, Awareness of Others’ Emotions
gence abilities in teams. In the current article, we and Management of Others’ Emotions. We antic-
have not presented evidence of the convergent ipate that this measure will be useful to
validity, concurrent validity or predictive validity researchers examining the impact of emotional
of the short version of the WEIP. Further research intelligence on team attitudes, team behaviours
is required to establish that the dimensions of the and team performance.
WEIP-S can be discriminated from both concep-
tually similar and divergent constructs. Such evi- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
dence will provide further support for our claim This paper was supported by grants from the
that the WEIP-S is a valid and reliable measure of Australian Research Council. The authors would
emotional intelligence. We also note that the like to thank Jane Murray for her work in collect-
short version of the WEIP will enable results to ing the data used in Studies 1 and 2.

466 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

References DeVellis RF (1991) Scale development: Theory and


Anderson JC and Gerbing DW (1988) Structural applications, Sage, Newbury Park, CA.
equation modelling in practice: A review and Donohue R and Stevensen L (2006) July The
recommended two-step approach, Psychological relationship between emotional intelligence and
Bulletin 103: 411-423. individual advancement and the mediating role of
Aritzeta A, Swailes S and Senior B (2007) The team transformational leadership ACREW Conference
role self-perception inventory: Development, Tuscany: Italy
validity and applications for team building, Drevets WC and Raichle ME (1998) Reciprocal
Journal of Management Studies 44: 96-118. suppression of regional cerebral blood flow during
Ashforth BE and Humphrey RH (1995) Emotion emotional versus higher cognitive processes:
in the workplace: A reappraisal, Human Implications for interactions between emotion and
Relations 48: 97-125. cognition, Cognition and Emotion 12: 353-386.
Ashkanasy NM (2003) Emotions in organizations: Druskat VU and Wolff SB (2001) Building the
A multilevel perspective’, in Dansereau F & emotional intelligence of groups, Harvard
Yammarino FJ (Eds) Research in multi-level Business Review 79: 80-91.
issues, vol 2: Multi-level issues in organizational Ekman P (1971) Universals and cultural differences
behavior and strategy, pp 9-54, Elsevier Science, in facial expressions of emotion’, in J Cole (Ed ),
Oxford, UK. Nebraska symposium on motivation, pp 207-283,
Ashkanasy NM, Härtel CEJ and Zerbe WJ (Eds) University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NE.
(2000) Emotions in the workplace: Research, theory Elfenbein HA, Polzer JT and Ambady N (2007) Team
and practice Quorum Books, Westport, CT. emotion recognition accuracy and team perform-
Bagozzi RP and Phillips LW (1982) Representing ance in Härtel CEJ, Ashkanasy NM and Zerbe WJ
and testing organizational theories: A holistic (Eds) Research on Emotion in Organizations: Funct-
construal, Administrative Science Quarterly, 27: ionality, Intentionality and Morality, 3: 87-119.
459-489. Fitness J (2000) Anger in the workplace: An emotion
Barsade SG (2002) The ripple effect: Emotional script approach to anger episodes between workers
contagion and its influence on group behavior, and their superiors co-workers and subordinates,
Administrative Science Quarterly 47: 644-675 Journal of Organizational Behavior 21: 147-162
Bentler PM and Wu EJC (2005) EQS 6 1 - Folkman S and Lazarus RS (1988) The relationship
Structural equation modelling software for windows between coping and emotion: Implications for
Multivariate Software Inc, Encino, CA. theory and research, Social Science and Medicine
Browne MW and Cudeck R (1993) Alterative ways 26: 309-317.
of assessing model fit’, in Bollen K A & Long J Gardner H (1983) Frames of mind: The theory of
S (Eds) Testing structural equation models, pp multiple intelligences, Basic Books, New York.
136-162, Sage, Newbury Park, CA. George JM (2000) Emotions and leadership: the
Calne DB (1999) Within reason: Rationality and role of emotional intelligence, Human Relations
human behavior, Pantheon Books, New York. 53: 1027-1055.
Christie A, Jordan PJ, Troth AC and Lawrence SA Goleman D (1995) Emotional intelligence: Why it
(2007). Testing the links between emotional can matter more than IQ, Bantam, New York.
intelligence and motivation. Journal of Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ and Swann WB Jr (2003)
Management & Organization 13: 212 – 226. A very brief measure of the Big Five personality
Cohen J and Cohen P (1983) Multiple domains, Journal of Research in Personality 37:
regression/correlation for the behavioral sciences (3rd 504 – 538.
ed) Erlbaum, Mahwah NJ. Guppy A, Edwards JA, Brough P, Peters-Bean KM,
Cooper RK and Sawaf A (1997) Executive EQ, Sale C and Short E (2004) The psychometric
Emotional intelligence in leadership and properties of the short version of the Cybernetic
organizations, Grossett/Putnam, New York. Coping Scale: A multigroup confirmatory factor
Damasio AR (1994) Descartes, error: Emotion, reason analysis across four samples, Journal of Occupat-
and the human brain, Grossett/Putnam, New York. ional and Organisational Psychology, 77: 39-62.
de Meijer M (1989) The contribution of general Hu L & Bentler PM (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit
features of body movement to the attribution of indexes in covariance structure analysis:
emotions, Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 13: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives,
247-268. Structural Equation Modeling 6: 1-55.

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 467
Peter J Jordan and Sandra A Lawrence

Izard CE, Wehmer CM, Livsey W and Jennings IR interaction between emotion and attention in
(1965) Affect awareness and performance in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive
Tomkins SS & Izard CE (Eds) Affect, cognition Neuroscience 10: 525–535.
and personality pp 2-41, Springer, New York. Law KS, Wong CS and Song LJ (2004) The construct
Jehn KA and Mannix EA (2001) The dynamic and criterion validity of emotional intelligence and
nature of conflict: a longitudinal study of its potential utility for management studies,
intragroup conflict and group performance, Journal of Applied Psychology 89: 483-496.
Academy of Management Journal 44: 238-251. Mathieu JE and Farr JL (1991) Further evidence for
Jordan PJ (2007) Emotional intelligence at work: A the discriminant validity of measures of organiz-
review of research. In Glendon, I Myors, B & ational commitment, job involvement and job
Thompson B (Eds), Advances in organisational satisfaction, Journal of Applied Psychology 76:
psychology: An Asia-Pacific perspective. pp. 355-370, 127-134.
Australian Academic Press, Brisbane, Australia. Mayer JD, Caruso DR and Salovey P (2001)
Jordan PJ and Ashkanasy NM (2006) Emotional Selecting a measure of emotional intelligence:
intelligence, emotional self-awareness and team The case for ability scales, in Bar-On R and
effectiveness’, in Druskat, VU Sala, F & Mount Parker JDA (Eds), The handbook of emotional
GJ (Eds) Linking emotional intelligence and intelligence: Theory, development, assessment and
performance at work pp 145-164, Jossey-Bass, application at home, school and in the workplace,
San Francisco. pp 320-342, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco.
Jordan PJ, Ashkanasy NM, Härtel CEJ and Hooper Mayer J and Salovey P (1997) What is emotional
GS (2002) Workgroup emotional intelligence: intelligence? in Salovey P and Sluyter D (Eds)
Scale development and relationship to team Emotional development and emotional intelligence:
process effectiveness and goal focus, Human Implications for educators pp 3-31, Basic Books,
Resource Management Review 12: 195-214. New York.
Jordan PJ, Ashton-James CE and Ashkanasy NM Mayer JD, Salovey P and Caruso DR (2000)
(2006) Evaluating the claims in Murphy K R Competing models of emotional intelligence, in
(Ed) A Critique of emotional intelligence: What are Sternberg R (Ed) Handbook of intelligence pp
the problems and how can they be fixed?, pp 189- 396-420, Cambridge, New York.
210, Erlbaum, Mahwah NJ. Mischel W and DeSmet AL (2000) Self-regulation
Jordan PJ, Lawrence SA and Troth AC (2006) The in the service of conflict resolution in Deutsch M
impact of negative mood on team performance, & Coleman PT (Eds) The handbook of conflict
Journal of Management & Organization 12: resolution: Theory and practice, pp 256-275,
131-145. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco.
Jordan PJ and Troth AC (2004) Managing emotions Mischel W and Shoda Y (1998) Reconciling
during team problem solving: Emotional processing dynamics and personality dispositions,
intelligence and conflict resolution, Human Annual Review of Psychology 49: 229–258.
Performance 17: 195-218. Mooney AC, Holahan PJ and Amason AC (2007)
Jordan PJ and Troth AC (2006) Emotions and Don’t take it personally: Exploring cognitive
coping with conflict: An Introduction, Journal conflict as a mediator of affective conflict,
of Management & Organization 12(2): 98-100. Journal of Management Studies 44: 733-758.
Kellett JB, Humphrey RH and Sleeth RG (2006) Moriarty P and Buckley F (2003) Increasing team
Empathy and the emergence of task and relations emotional intelligence through process, Journal
leaders, The Leadership Quarterly 17: 146-162. of European Industrial Training 27: 98-110.
Kelly JR and Barsade SG (2001) Mood and Morrell K (2004) Decision-making and business
emotions in small groups and work teams, ethics: The implications of using image theory in
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision preference to rational choice, Journal of Business
Processes 86: 99-130. Ethics 50: 239-252.
Kline RB (1998) Principals and practice of struct- Mumby DK and Putnam LA (1992) The politics of
ural equation modelling, The Guilford Press, emotion: A feminist reading of bounded rational-
New York. ity, Academy of Management Review 17: 465-486.
Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes Ortony A, Clore GL and Collins A (1988) The
A and Schwartz GE (1998) Neural correlates of cognitive structure of emotions, Cambridge
levels of emotional awareness: Evidence of an University Press, New York

468 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009
Development and initial validation of the short version of the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP-S)

Pelled LH, Eisenhardt KM and Xin KR (1999) and team performance: A study of the mediating
Exploring the black box: an analysis of work effects of cognitive ability and emotional
group diversity conflict and performance, intelligence, International Journal of Selection and
Administrative Science Quarterly 44: 1-28. Assessment 12: 312-320.
Pennebaker JW and Francis ME (1996) Cognitive: Tannenbaum SI, Beard RL and Salas E (1992) Team
emotional: and language processes in disclosure, building and its influence on team effectiveness:
Cognition and Emotion 10: 601-626. An examination of conceptual and empirical
Periatt JA LeMay SA & Chakrabarty S (2004) The developments, in K. Kelley (Ed) Issues, theory and
selling orientation-customer orientation (SOCO) research in industrial/organizational psychology, pp
scale: Cross-validation of the revised version, 117-153, Amsterdam, Elsevier Science.
Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management Van Dyne L, Ang S and Botero IC (2003)
24: 49-54. Conceptualizing employee silence and employee
Rammstedt B and John OP (2007) Measuring per- voice as multidimensional constructs, Journal of
sonality in one minute or less: A 10-item short Management Studies 40: 1359-1392.
version of the Big Five Inventory in English and Van Rooy DL and Viswesvaran C (2004)
German, Journal of Research in Personality 41: Emotional intelligence: A meta-analytic
203-212. investigation of predictive validity and
Roberts RD, Zeidner M and Matthews G (2001) nomological net, Journal of Vocational Behavior
Does emotional intelligence meet traditional 65: 71-95.
standards for an intelligence? Some new data and Watson D, Clark LA and Tellegan A (1988)
conclusions, Emotion 1: 196-231. Development and validation of brief measures of
Schutte NS, Malouff JM, Hall LE, Haggerty DJ, Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales,
Cooper JT, Golden CJ and Dornheim L (1998) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54:
Development and validation of a measure of 1063-1070.
emotional intelligence, Personality and Weiss H and Cropanzano R (1996) Affective events
Individual Differences 25: 167-177. theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure,
Silvia PJ (2002) Self-awareness and emotional causes and consequences of affective experiences
intensity, Cognition and Emotion 16: 195-216. at work, Research in Organizational Behavior 18:
Sosik JJ and Megerian LE (1999) Understanding 1-74.
leader emotional intelligence and performance: Wolff SB, Pescosolido AT and Druskat VU (2002)
The role of self-other agreement on Emotional intelligence as the basis of leadership
transformational leadership perceptions, Group emergence in self-managing teams, Leadership
and Organization Management 24: 367-390. Quarterly 13: 505-522.
Sue-Chan C and Latham GP (2004) The
situational interview as a predictor of academic Received 7 August 2008 Accepted 15 July 2009

N O W A V A I L A B L E
The Leading Way of Changing Meaning Awakening-Struggle: Towards
(2007) By Sandra Sytsma Buddhist Critical Social Theory (2004)
In studying leading as a way of changing By Robert Hattam
meaning, this research documents a The ground-breaking book offers the
journey of inner exploration amongst five first extensive comparison of critical
self-nominated leaders in education. theory with socially-engaged Buddhism.
ISBN: 978-1-921214-26-4 ISBN: 978-1-876682-57-6

Following Vygotsky to a Learner-Centred SoulWork: Finding the Work You Love,


School (2006) By Robert G Grandin Loving the Work You Have (2007)
The author describes his experiences in By Deborah P Bloch, Lee J Richmond
attacking the problem of individuality in Relates career to spiritual themes, and
learning. provide advice and support to people in
ISBN: 978-1-876682-96-5 working through their personal choices.
ISBN: 978-0-9775742-3-0
eContent Management Pty Ltd, PO Box 1027, Maleny QLD 4552, Australia
Tel.: +61-7-5435-2900; Fax. +61-7-5435-2911; subscriptions@e-contentmanagement.com
www.e-contentmanagement.com

Volume 15, Issue 4, September 2009 JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT & ORGANIZATION 469

You might also like