You are on page 1of 11

Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237

Motor awareness without perceptual awareness!


Helen Johnson, Patrick Haggard∗
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University College London,
Alexandra House, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK

Abstract

The control of action has traditionally been described as “automatic”. In particular, movement control may occur without conscious
awareness, in contrast to normal visual perception. Studies on rapid visuomotor adjustment of reaching movements following a target shift have
played a large part in introducing such distinctions. We suggest that previous studies of the relation between motor performance and perceptual
awareness have confounded two separate dissociations. These are: (a) the distinction between motoric and perceptual representations, and
(b) an orthogonal distinction between conscious and unconscious processes. To articulate these differences more clearly, we propose a new
measure of motor awareness, based on subjects’ ability to reproduce the spatial details of reaching movements they have just made. Here
we focus on the dissociation between motor awareness and perceptual awareness that may occur when subjects make rapid visuomotor
adjustments to reaching movements following a target shift. In experiment 1, motor awareness was dissociated from perceptual awareness of a
target shift during reaching movement. Participants’ reproduction of movement endpoints following visuomotor adjustment was independent
of whether they saw the target shift or not. Experiment 2 replicated this result, and further showed that neither motor awareness nor motor
performance were disrupted by TMS over the parietal cortex. The neural mechanisms underlying motor awareness, and the implications for
theories of consciousness, are discussed.
© 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Human; Reaching movement; Unconscious processing; Consciousness; Transcranial magnetic stimulation

1. Introduction work was continued over several years by Prablanc, Jean-


nerod and others, in an extensive series of studies using the
Studies of the neural mechanisms underlying coordinated “double-step paradigm” of visuomotor adjustments in reach-
action have traditionally occupied a curious middle ground ing movements (e.g. Pélisson, Prablanc, Goodale, & Jean-
between psychology and neuroscience. This has given the nerod, 1986; Prablanc & Martin, 1992). The double-step
field a very productive, interdisciplinary flavour, and allowed paradigm was initially studied in saccadic eye movements
researchers to raise strictly scientific questions, which at the (Hallet & Lightstone, 1976) but we focus here on the clas-
same time seem of fundamental importance to the wider view sic result in the case of reaching movements. Pélisson et al.
of the mind and human nature. This combination of scien- (1986) asked subjects to make rapid pointing movements to-
tific knowledge and a willingness to tackle major intellectual wards a visual target, without vision of the hand. In their
questions in a clear and practical way is very characteristic paradigm, subjects initially fixated a peripheral light, and
of the work of Marc Jeannerod, throughout his distinguished made a saccade to the target when it first appeared, and then
career. reached towards it (step 1). During the saccade, the target was
In this paper, we particularly focus on the relation between unpredictably shifted a small distance either to the left or to
skilled action and conscious experience. Marc Jeannerod was the right (step 2). Because of the known reduction in visual
among the first to investigate this relation scientifically. The perception during a saccade (Matin, 1974), subjects were not
perceptually aware that the target had shifted. Nevertheless,
! This study was carried out at the Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience.
inspection of the movement trajectories showed that subjects
∗ were able to correct their ongoing trajectories according to the
Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 20 7679 1151, fax: +44 20 7813 2835.
E-mail address: p.haggard@ucl.ac.uk (P. Haggard). direction of the visual target shift. That is, the brain systems

0028-3932/$ – see front matter © 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.009
228 H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237

generating motor awareness clearly had access to informa- or subjective state. Dissociating processes which differ in
tion about the target location that did not enter into perceptual more than one respect is problematic: which dimension of
awareness. difference is responsible for the differences seen in experi-
Since this early work, several additional studies by mental data? Moreover, processes can only be dissociated if
Marc Jeannerod and others have replicated the basic result they were previously accepted as unitary. As the number of
(Castiello, Paulignan, & Jeannerod, 1991; Day & Brown, attributes along which the processes are accepted to differ
2001; Day & Lyon, 2000; Desmurget et al., 1999; Paulignan, increases, an approach based on dissociation becomes less
MacKenzie, Marteniuk, & Jeannerod, 1991; Prablanc & Mar- convincing.
tin, 1992). This work became widely known and had strong We therefore suggest that the scientific issues raised by the
impact on our way of thinking about the mind and brain in double-step paradigm can be broken down into investigations
general. For example, the basic finding drove the develop- of two separate dissociations. First, does motor performance
ment of the distinctions between the dorsal and ventral visual dissociate from motor awareness? That is, what do people
pathways (Goodale & Milner, 1992), and between the brain consciously know about their own visuomotor adjustments?
systems for semantics and pragmatics (Jeannerod, 1994). Second, does motor awareness dissociate from perceptual
Many studies using the double-step paradigm have trig- awareness? That is, can one be aware of a visuomotor ad-
gered the target shift directly from the saccade, in order to justment even when unaware of any target shift? We recently
prevent perceptual awareness of the target shift. Relatively gave an affirmative answer to the first question (Johnson, van
few have measured perceptual awareness directly, and even Beers, & Haggard, 2002). By asking subjects to reproduce
fewer have investigated what factors influence perceptual the spatial path of a movement they had just made in the
awareness. Therefore, the initial claim of dissociation be- double-step paradigm, we found that subjects typically un-
tween motor performance and perceptual awareness was not derestimated the latency and the gain of their own visuomotor
examined in detail. An important exception, however, was the adjustments. In an anti-point condition, however, both these
study by Castiello et al. (1991). They used the basic double- results were reversed. This pattern of results suggested that
step paradigm, as before. Unusually, they did not attempt the awareness subjects had of their visuomotor adjustment
to prevent vision of the target shift. Instead, they required depended strongly on the role of intention in generating the
subjects to respond to the target shift in different ways in adjustment. Motor awareness followed motor intention. We
each of three conditions. The first condition replicated the use the term “motor awareness” here to indicate subjects’
double-step paradigm: subjects responded to the target shift conscious representation of their own action, effectively as
by adjusting an ongoing reaching movement. In the second an opposite to the traditional use of the term “automatic” in
condition, subjects did not move their hand, but made a sim- the psychology of action.
ple vocal response on detecting the target shift. In the final Here, we consider the second of the two dissociations pro-
condition, subjects made both of these responses on each posed above: how is motor awareness related to perceptual
trial. Castiello et al. argued that the simple verbal response awareness? In the double-step reaching task, subjects may or
required perceptual awareness of the target shift, whereas a may not be aware of the target shift on any particular trial,
visuomotor adjustment did not. according to how successfully the paradigm implements sac-
The results showed that visuomotor adjustments occurred cadic suppression. At the same time, subjects may have full,
within approximately 100 ms of the target shift, while ver- limited, or no awareness that their movement has deviated
bal responses took over 300 ms. This latency difference was to one side (Johnson et al., 2002). In principle, these two
present whether the responses were made on the same trial varieties of awareness should be interdependent because the
(third condition) or individually (first and second conditions). spatial path of a movement towards the target physically de-
The authors interpreted the delay in the verbal response pends on the spatial location of the target. For example, if the
as reflecting the slow operation of the brain circuits me- subject sees the target jump to one side, and is trying to reach
diating perceptual awareness. The circuits underlying con- for that target, they should correct their movement, and should
scious processing are either additional to, or distinct from be aware that they have done so. Conversely, if the subject be-
and slower than, circuits underlying visuomotor responses. comes aware that their movement trajectory has veered to one
This result provides convergent behavioural evidence for side, it seems logical to conclude that the trajectory deviated
a dissociation between motor performance and perceptual in pursuit of a target, which had shifted to that side. In general,
awareness. a consciously perceived target shift should be associated with
While such studies have been very influential, and have awareness of any resulting motor adjustment. However, the
shed new light on the brain’s visuomotor systems, they do not psychological modules responsible for perceptual and motor
conclusively reveal the relations between action and aware- awareness may not follow the strict dependency of the spatial
ness. These studies appear to dissociate processes, which in world: indeed dissociative disorders are an established feature
fact differ in at least two respects. That is, motor performance of human consciousness (Farah & Feinberg, 2000). Motor
differs from perceptual awareness in that the former is mo- awareness and perceptual awareness could be a dissociable
tor and the latter perceptual, but also in that the former is a attributes of the human mind, subserved by separate brain
performance or behaviour, while the latter is an awareness, structures.
H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237 229

Therefore, we performed two experiments to study the Each trial had three phases. In the first, reaching phase,
relationship between perceptual awareness and motor aware- the subject initially fixated the peripheral spot for 2–3 s. The
ness in further detail. In experiment 1, we combined saccadic fixation spot disappeared and one of the three targets im-
suppression with a double-step reaching task. We asked sub- mediately appeared. Subjects looked at, and then reached
jects to reproduce each movement as a measure of motor for the illuminated target as quickly as possible. During the
awareness, and we considered how motor awareness var- saccade period (200–300 ms after target onset) the central
ied between trials where subjects had reported perceptual target might jump unpredictably by 4 cm to the left or right
awareness of the target shift and those where they had not. (Fig. 1). At the end of the movement, the target was extin-
In experiment 2, we attempted to disrupt visuomotor ad- guished and subjects returned to the starting position. This
justments using TMS over the parietal cortex in a similar initiated the second, reproduction phase of each trial. A 2 mm
paradigm. wide guideline was shown across the entire table from left to
right, at the level of the targets. Subjects were asked to re-
peat the spatial detail of the movement path that they had
2. Experiment 1 just made. They were specifically instructed to reproduce
the spatial features, without regard to movement speed or
2.1. Methods other temporal information. During the movement reproduc-
tion phase, a guideline at the Y location of the targets was
The details of the method and apparatus were based on pre- projected to indicate the appropriate movement amplitude,
vious studies (Desmurget et al., 1999; Pélisson et al., 1986). but no targets were visible. At the end of the reproduced
Fifteen right-handed subjects (10 female, mean age 26 years) movement, the guideline disappeared and subjects again re-
participated with informed consent. Subjects sat at a table in turned to the starting point. Third and finally, subjects were
a darkened room with their head on a chin rest. The right hand asked to verbally report any target jump (saying ‘left’, ‘right’
was occluded. Visual targets (7 mm diameter) were projected or ‘no’) seen during the reaching phase of the trial, as a mea-
onto a horizontal plane above the table, which the subject sure of perceptual awareness. The full sequence of events is
viewed via a mirror, so that they appeared to be in front of shown in Fig. 2.
the hand. The central (unperturbed) target was aligned in the
parasagittal plane 14 cm in front of the start position. Two
other targets were at 8 cm to either side of the central target 3. Experiment 1
in the frontoparallel plane. When the central target ‘jumped’
it moved 4 cm horizontally in either direction (see Fig. 1). 3.1. Results
An initial visual fixation point was shown 40 cm to the left
of the central target. Reaching movements were recorded at Subjects reported perceptual awareness of a target shift on
120 Hz with an electromagnetic tracker mounted on the index 37% of jump trials.
fingernail. Movement start and end were defined as the point Only trials in which the central target was initially illumi-
at which the velocity of the finger fell below 5 cm/s. nated were analysed kinematically. Of these 6.34% of trials
Subjects performed 10 practice trials, then 240 experi- were omitted due to equipment failure. A 2.11% of trials were
mental trials in blocks of 20. In 60 trials, the central target omitted due to pre-emptive eye or hand movements. A 7.8%
illuminated; in 50 trials, the left target illuminated; and in 50 of trials were omitted as false positives (in non-jump trials)
trials, the right target illuminated. In a further 40 trials, the or movement in the wrong direction (in jump trials). Exam-
centre target appeared and then jumped left, and in 40 trials, ple traces for one participant are shown in Fig. 3. The initial
it jumped right. Trial order was randomised across subjects. trial and the subsequent trajectory reproduction are shown

Fig. 1. Experimental set-up (note: targets were 7 mm in diameter, but are shown enlarged here for clarity).
230 H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237

Fig. 2. Events occurring in each trial.

for one trial on which the subject reported seeing the target to study the final hand positions, by which point the visuo-
shift, and a second trial on which they did not. The y compo- motor adjustment was easily detectable despite the bias. We
nent of the trajectories shows the smooth reaching movement discuss this bias in more detail below.
seen in earlier studies. The movement duration was typically Our main analysis therefore focussed on the final position
around 600 ms. The x component shows a clear movement ad- of the hand in the x direction. In the reaching phase of the
justment towards the target, which occurs during the reach- trial, this provides a measure of visuomotor adjustment fol-
ing movement, and without pause or reprogramming. This lowing a target jump. Trials in which the target jumped were
begins around 150 ms after movement onset. Note that the divided into those in which the subject detected the jump and
adjustment is similar in trials with and without perceptual those in which they did not (unaware), and these were anal-
awareness of the target shift. ysed separately. Endpoints of all jump trials can be seen in
We initially hoped to compare the full spatial paths of Table 1.
initial and reproduced movements, in order to investigate
whether motor awareness of visuomotor adjustments. How- 3.2. Endpoints of initial reaching movements
ever, inspection of the results revealed a strong rightward bias
in the initial reaching movements. This was substantially re- We first analysed the initial movements to detect whether
duced in the reproduced movements. The difference in bias visuomotor adjustments to the target jump occurred. The
tended to obscure the small lateral deviations, which mark the mean endpoint of each perturbed condition was compared
onset of the visuomotor adjustment. Therefore, we preferred to the mean endpoint of the equivalent unperturbed condi-

Table 1
Mean and S.D. across subjects of endpoint × deviation for initial and reproduced movements (mm)
Endpoint deviations Left (target = −40 mm) Centre (target = 0 mm) Right (target = 40 mm)

Aware Unaware n/a Aware Unaware


Initial movement
Mean (mm) −9 −11 25 57 63
S.D. (mm) 29 30 30 36 34
Reproduced movement
Mean (mm) −28 −32 14 55 58
S.D. (mm) 32 29 29 31 33
Note: Negative numbers indication mm left of centre, positive numbers are mm right of centre.
H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237 231

Fig. 3. Typical perturbation (“jump”) trials for one participant showing the trajectories for their initial and subsequently reproduced (“copied”) movements.
The upper row shows a trial after which the subject reported seeing the target shift. In the trial in the lower row, the subject reported no change in target position.

tion. Family wise Bonferroni corrections were performed, but also clear evidence of a normal visuomotor adjustment
and so the significance level for the following t-tests is 0.025. pattern in the initial movements.
All movement endpoints differed significantly from the un-
perturbed trials regardless of whether the subject was aware 3.3. Endpoints of reproduced movements
(left: t14 = 11.43, P > 0.001, right: t14 = −8.07, P > 0.001)
or unaware (left: t14 = 16.75, P > 0.001, right: t14 = −21.41, We then investigated whether subjects were aware of their
P > 0.001) of the target shift. The effect of awareness on ad- visuomotor adjustments by repeating the same comparisons
justment amplitude was then considered. No significant dif- for the reproduced movements. The reproduced endpoints of
ference was found between the endpoints of aware and un- each perturbed condition were compared to the reproduced
aware trials for jumps to either the left (t14 = 1.54, P = 0.15) endpoints in the unperturbed condition. Following family
or the right (t14 = −0.58, P = 0.57). That is, subjects were wise Bonferroni corrections, the significance level for the
just as efficient in adjusting their movements following tar- following t-tests is again 0.025. All reproduced endpoints
get jumps regardless of whether they were aware of the jump on perturbed trials differed significantly from those on un-
or not: motor performance was independent of perceptual perturbed trials regardless of whether the subject was aware
awareness. The average endpoints to targets that jumped to (left: t14 = 10.06, P > 0.001, right: t14 = −11.36, P > 0.001)
the left were 10 mm to the left of centre. Average endpoints or unaware (left: t14 = 17.26, P > 0.001, right: t14 = −15.78,
to targets that jumped to the right were 60 mm to the right of P > 0.001) of the target shift. The effect of awareness on
centre. Thus, there was an overall rightward bias (see above), the extent of the reproduced adjustments was then consid-
232 H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237

ered. No significant difference was found between the repro- endpoint of the central trials. That is, subjects performed
duced endpoints of aware and unaware trials for jumps to functional visuomotor adjustments following the target shift.
either the left (t14 = −1.20, P = 0.25) or the right (t14 = 1.28, However, no significant difference was found between the
P = 0.22). endpoints for those trials in which the subject was aware of
The relationship between initial endpoints and reproduced the target shift and those in which they were not. This con-
movements in jump trials gives a measure of motor awareness firms previous findings that efficient adjustments can be made
(see Table 1). Inspection of the table shows that the subjects to an ongoing movement in response to a target shift regard-
accurately reproduced the direction of their visuomotor ad- less of whether the subject is aware of that target shift or not
justments in the immediately preceding movement. (Day & Lyon, 2000; Desmurget et al., 1999; Paulignan et al.,
To investigate the relation between perceptual awareness 1991; Pélisson et al., 1986). The above studies have generally
and motor awareness, a 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA was conducted on studied manual responses to targets that shift during saccadic
the perturbation trial data (see Fig. 4). The factors were direc- suppression of conscious experience. Therefore, the dissoci-
tion of target jump (left, right), perceptual awareness (aware, ation between manual performance and visual awareness that
unaware) and phase of trial (initial movement, reproduced they report correlates with a difference between effectors: the
movement). hand and arm in one case, and the oculomotor system in the
There was a significant effect of side (F1,14 = 265.96, other. However, the dissociation between performance and
P > 0.001). On average, endpoints to leftward target shifts awareness can be found even in a single effector. Gaveau et al.
were 20 mm left of centre, and 59 mm right of centre for (2003) recently reported that subjects adjusted saccade kine-
rightward shifts. There was no significant effect of aware- matics for small changes in visual target position of which
ness (F1,14 = 0.35, P = 0.56) There was a significant effect of they were perceptually unaware.
trial phase (F1,14 = 22.49, P > 0.001). This occurred because Returning to the present experiment, initial movements in
the rightward bias previously mentioned in the initial move- all conditions were subject to a rightward bias of approxi-
ments was reduced in the reproduced movements. A signif- mately 2 cm. Such biases have also seen in other similar ex-
icant interaction between side and movement (F1,14 = 24.1, periments (Desmurget et al., 1999). We believe it may reflect
P > 0.001) indicated that this reduction was most marked for biomechanical factors (Carey et al., 1996; Carey & Otto de
leftward target jumps. There was no significant interaction Hart, 2001), coupled with the lack of visual feedback of hand
between trial phase and awareness (F1,14 = 2.92, P = 0.11), position. We speculate that the visual guide presented during
nor awareness and side (F1,14 = 1.91, P = 0.19). Critically, the reproduction phase of the movement may have helped to
there was no interaction between side, awareness and trial reduce this bias.
phase (F1,14 = 0.015, P = 0.91). That is, subjects’ ability to
reproduce the spatial detail of visuomotor adjustments was 4.1.2. Reproduced movements and motor awareness
equivalent in trials with and without perceptual awareness of The most important results concern motor awareness. Sub-
the target jump. jects reproduced the spatial details of the visuomotor adjust-
ment on the immediately preceding trial. A similar finding
had been reported previously (Johnson et al., 2002). How-
4. Experiment 1
ever, in that earlier study, subjects had full vision of the limb
4.1. Discussion throughout both the initial and the reproduced movement.
Therefore, the reproduced movements in that study could
4.1.1. End points of initial movements reflect a visual memory of limb positions during the initial
The mean endpoint of the movements during which the movement, as well as, or instead of a representation truly
target jumped were significantly different from the mean linked to movement. Moreover, that study focussed on the
perceived latency of visuomotor adjustments, whereas the
present analysis of endpoints focuses on perceptual gain. Our
measures of motor awareness did not differ significantly ac-
cording to whether subjects were aware of the target shift or
not. This demonstrates the novel finding that motor awareness
can dissociate from perceptual awareness. That is, despite re-
porting that the target did not shift, subjects were neverthe-
less aware that their movement contained a lateral deviation
in the appropriate direction. They are able to report their mo-
tor awareness of their movement despite having no conscious
knowledge of the target event that drove it. This pattern of
results seems quite paradoxical. The subjects clearly knew, in
some sense, that their initial movements contained a lateral
deviation. It is tempting to speculate on how subjects recon-
Fig. 4. Mean endpoints in perturbation (“jump”) trials (mm). cile this awareness of their own motor adjustment deviations
H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237 233

with manifest unawareness of any reason why the movement motor adjustments in our first experiment might not arise in
required adjusting. Subjects did not seem at all troubled by the parietal structures that generate the adjustment itself, but
this incompatibility, and did not seem to find the motor ad- in a separate cortical pathway. This reasoning suggested the
justments phenomenologically remarkable in the way that intriguing possibility that parietal TMS might suppress the
non-intentional movements evoked by transcranial magnetic visuomotor adjustment itself, while leaving the subject with
stimulation, or by Kohnstamm’s manoeuvre often are. the (erroneous) conscious awareness that they had adjusted
their movement. Experiment 2 sets out to investigate this pos-
sibility.
5. Experiment 2

5.1. Introduction 6. Experiment 2

Our initial aim in this experiment was to try to dissociate 6.1. Method
motor performance from motor awareness. It is widely ac-
knowledged that much, even most, motor function does not The method for experiment 2 was identical to that of ex-
require conscious thought (Haggard, 2001). The term “auto- periment 1, which in turn was partly based on the study of
matic” is often used in such cases. Therefore, motor perfor- Desmurget et al. (1999). Subjects again made reaching move-
mance is often dissociated from motor awareness in everyday ments to targets that were unpredictably perturbed. In this
life. However, to understand why motor performance some- experiment, however, TMS was delivered on 50% of trials
times does enter awareness and sometimes does not, an inter- selected at random. On those trials, TMS was delivered at the
ventive experimental approach is required, in which the ex- start of movement when the hand velocity exceeded 5 cm/s.
perimenter can manipulate whether a given feature of move- A focal TMS coil was held by the experimenter over the
ment either does or does not enter awareness. One previous left PPC. The exact position was localised according to the
attempt at this approach was the work of Haggard and Magno method of Desmurget et al. (1999); 4.5 cm caudal and 0.5 cm
(1999). They used the well-established delay in reaction times medial from the hand motor area. Output was set at 120%
caused by appropriately timed transcranial magnetic stimu- of the subject’s motor threshold. Horizontal EOG was mea-
lation (TMS) over the motor cortex to delay subjects’ motor sured with surface electrodes. Target jumps were triggered at
performance (Day et al., 1989). Their subjects additionally a random 25–100 ms interval after the onset of EOG activity
judged the perceived time of their reactions, using the method associated with the large saccade from peripheral fixation to
of Libet and colleagues (Libet, Gleason, Wright, & Pearl, the central target. The methods were otherwise as for exper-
1983). The results showed that subjects were largely unaware iment 1.
of the performance delay produced by cortical TMS, suggest- Five new right-handed subjects (4 female, mean age 26
ing at least a partial dissociation between motor performance years) participated in the experiment.
and motor awareness.
More recently, Desmurget et al. (1999) reported that visuo-
motor adjustments in the double-step task could be abolished 7. Experiment 2
by TMS over the posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Moreover,
several authors have additionally proposed that the parietal 7.1. Results
cortex acts as an “auto-pilot” for aimed movement (Pisella
et al., 2000) and does not contribute to conscious awareness. Subjects reported perceptual awareness of a target shift on
We therefore reasoned that the motor awareness of visuo- 47% of jump trials in which TMS was delivered, and 47%

Table 2
Mean and S.D. across subjects of endpoint × deviation (mm), for initial and reproduced movements with and without TMS
Position of target Left (target = −40 mm) Centre (target = 0 mm) Right (target = 40 mm)

TMSa No TMSa TMSa No TMSa TMSa No TMSa

Awareb Unawareb Awareb Unawareb n/ab n/ab Awareb Unawareb Awareb Unawareb
Initial movement
Mean (mm) −12 −18 −15 −17 22 21 64 62 62 62
S.D. (mm) 46 39 44 44 32 35 52 50 47 54
Reproduced movement
Mean (mm) −30 −24 −30 −28 18 19 72 67 69 67
S.D. (mm) 36 38 29 36 34 38 45 46 39 52
Note: Negative numbers indication mm left of centre, positive numbers are mm right of centre.
a Presence or absence of TMS.
b Subjective awareness of target jump.
234 H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237

icant difference was found for leftward (t4 = 0.39, P = 0.71)


or rightward (t4 = 0.45, P = 0.68) adjustments. The pattern of
results seen in experiment 1 was therefore replicated in the
non-TMS trials of experiment 2.

7.1.1. No effect of TMS on visuomotor adjustment


A further series of comparisons focussed on effects of
TMS on visuomotor adjustment. The endpoints of the TMS
jump trials were compared with the endpoints of the unper-
turbed TMS centre point trials. Family wise Bonferroni cor-
rections were again performed, and the significance level for
all of the following t-tests is 0.025.
All of the TMS jump trials differed significantly from the
unperturbed TMS trials regardless of whether the subject was
aware (left: t4 = 7.88, P = 0.001, right: t4 = −6.08, P = 0.004)
or unaware (left: t4 = 8.90, P = 0.001, right: t4 = −7.51,
P = 0.002) of the target shift. Therefore, visuomotor adjust-
ments were clearly present in trials where TMS was applied
to the PPC.
Fig. 5. Mean endpoints in perturbation (“jump”) trials (mm). Although TMS did not abolish visuomotor adjustments
it might nevertheless have subtly changed their kinematic
of jump trials without TMS. A 2.7% of trials were elimi- pattern. Therefore, the initial movement endpoints of jump
nated from the kinematic analysis because of pre-emptive trials in which TMS was administered were compared with
eye or hand movements, 10.6% because of equipment fail- the endpoints of no TMS jump trials. No significant differ-
ure, and 8.2% because of movements in the wrong direction ences were found regardless of whether subjects were aware
with respect to the target shift. The endpoints for initial and (left: t4 = −1.87, P = 0.14, right: t4 = −0.38, P = 0.73) or un-
reproduced perturbed movements are shown in Table 2 and aware (left: t4 = 0.24, P = 0.82, right: t4 = 0.19, P = 0.86) of
Fig. 5. the target shift.
We first analysed the no TMS trials to assess whether the We also speculated that TMS could have been effective in
pattern of results found in experiment 1 was replicated in just a small subset of trials. However, plotting the endpoints
experiment 2. To establish whether visuomotor adjustments for individual trials showed essentially overlapping endpoint
had been made, the endpoints in each target jump condition distributions for TMS and non-TMS trials (Fig. 6).
were compared with the endpoints of the unperturbed centre Finally, although it would have been contrary to our pre-
target trials. dictions, it could still have been possible that TMS had af-
Family wise Bonferroni corrections were performed, and fected motor awareness, despite motor performance remain-
the significance level for all of the following t-tests is 0.025. ing unaffected. Therefore, the reproduced endpoints of the
All of the non-TMS jump trials differed significantly from TMS jump trials were compared with their non-TMS coun-
the unperturbed trials regardless of whether the subject was terparts. No significant differences were found (all t4 < 2, all
aware (left: t4 = 10.85, P < 0.001, right: t4 = −8.69, P = 0.001) P > 0.20).
or unaware (left: t4 = 8.30, P = 0.001, right: t4 = −7.19,
P = 0.002) of the target shift. This confirms that visuomo-
tor adjustments were being made regardless of perceptual 8. Experiment 2
awareness.
To establish whether these adjustments entered into mo- 8.1. Discussion
tor awareness, as they had in experiment 1, the reproduced
endpoints of jump trials were compared with the reproduced Three features of experiment 2 are noteworthy. First, we
endpoints from the centre point condition. In the reproduced replicated in a further group of subjects the dissociation
movements, jump trials again differed significantly from un- between motor awareness and perceptual awareness seen in
perturbed trials regardless of whether the subject was aware experiment 1. Again, we observed that subjects reproduced
(left: t4 = 6.28, P = 0.003, right: t4 = −10.32, P < 0.001) or un- motor adjustments following target jumps of which they were
aware (left: t4 = 7.84, P = 0.001, right: t4 = −6.56, P = 0.003) perceptually unaware. The use of EOG to trigger the target
of the target shift. jump in the present experiment is a methodological improve-
Finally, to confirm that perceptual awareness had no effect ment which makes the lack of any perceptual awareness
on motor awareness reproduced endpoints of jump trials in effects more convincing, if anything, than in experiment 1.
which subjects were aware of the target shift were compared Second, we entirely failed to obtain the suppression
with those on trials in which they were unaware. No signif- of visuomotor adjustments by parietal TMS reported by
H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237 235

Fig. 6. Endpoints of perturbed and unperturbed trials for each subject, with and without TMS (mm). The target was located at (0, 240).

Desmurget et al. (1999) despite using comparable methods. Third, we found no effects of parietal TMS on motor
We speculate on two possible reasons for this. Although we awareness, measured as the discrepancy between initial and
used their procedure for localising the TMS site, Desmurget reproduced visuomotor adjustments. This is, of course, a null
et al. subsequently confirmed the location of their stimula- result, and we had not predicted any such effects. Moreover,
tion using MRI. They also noted that one of their subjects one might argue that the absence of any TMS effect on motor
who showed a slightly different pattern of results had been performance would make effects on motor awareness un-
stimulated more medially than the others. We did not con- likely. Nevertheless, we note that our failure to alter motor
firm our stimulation site with MRI, so anatomical variations awareness is at least consistent with the view that the PPC
or incorrect localisation might have resulted in inappropriate provides an “auto-pilot” for on-line movement control, (Gréa
stimulation sites for some of our subjects. On the other hand, et al., 2002; Pisella et al., 2000) independent of conscious ex-
one of their subjects who did not show the inhibitory effect perience.
of TMS, did appear to have been stimulated appropriately.
Therefore, there may be substantial individual differences in 8.2. General discussion and conclusions
responsiveness to parietal TMS. Finally, the TMS coil and
stimulator in their study may have been more powerful than Previous studies have suggested that the motor system can
ours. In particular their stimulation may have been effective respond to information of which we are perceptually unaware
at deeper sites within the intraparietal sulcus than ours. (Pélisson et al., 1986) Our own work had further shown that
236 H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237

people’s awareness of their own motor output may involve brain processes and the conscious experience of being an
an attenuated or an exaggerated perception of their actual agent.
movement, essentially depending on the extent to which the
movement was intended or not (Johnson et al., 2002). In the
present study, we have investigated the link between these two Acknowledgements
approaches, by investigating whether perceptual awareness of
a target shift modulates motor awareness of the visuomotor HJ was supported by a BBSRC studentship. Further sup-
adjustment to that shift during reaching movements. port was provided from a BBSRC Project grant to PH, a Lev-
Our results show that awareness of adjustment to ongoing erhulme Research Fellowship awarded to PH, and a Royal
movements is independent of perceptual awareness of any tar- Society Equipment grant.We are grateful to Louise Whiteley
get displacement necessitating such adjustment. Therefore, for assistance with preparing this work for publication.
we have shown that motor awareness and perceptual aware-
ness can be dissociated, at least in the double-step task. This
dissociation appears relatively complete: the magnitudes of References
lateral deviations in reproduced movements were numeri-
cally close to those in the corresponding initial movements, Carey, D. P., Hargreaves, E. L., & Goodale, M. A. (1996). Reaching
and not just attenuated versions of the original movements. To to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor
conclude, we comment on some psychologically important processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control.
Experimental Brain Research, 112, 496–504.
features of this dissociation. Carey, D. P., & Otto de Hart, E. G. (2001). Hemispatial differences in
Our result suggests a paradox or incompatibility at the visually guided aiming are neither hemispatial nor visual. Neuropsy-
heart of subject’s experience of their own action. In some tri- chologia, 39, 885–894.
als, our subjects reported a stationary target on the one hand, Castiello, U., Paulignan, Y., & Jeannerod, M. (1991). Temporal dissocia-
yet a clearly deviated trajectory on the other. At face value, tion of motor responses and subjective awareness. A study in normal
subjects. Brain, 114, 2639–2655.
resolution of these incompatible situations would seem possi- Day, B. L., & Brown, P. (2001). Evidence for subcortical involvement in
ble only if subjects generate an alternative explanation, other the visual control of human reaching. Brain, 124, 1832–1840.
than a displacement of the target, of why they made a tra- Day, B. L., & Lyon, I. N. (2000). Voluntary modification of automatic
jectory deviation. For example, subjects could conceivably arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target. Experimental
attribute the deviation of their initial movement to an execu- Brain Research, 130, 159–168.
Day, B. L., Rothwell, J. C., Thompson, P. D., Maertens de Noordhout,
tion error that accidentally arose in the course of a movement A., Nakashima, K., Shannon, K., et al. (1989). Delay in the execution
to an apparently stationary target. They would then faith- of voluntary movement by electrical or magnetic brain stimulation in
fully reproduce this error. Two reasons lead us to reject this intact man. Brain, 112, 649–663.
idea. First, our subjects did not show the sort of frustration Desmurget, M., Epstein, C. M., Turner, R. S., Prablanc, C., Alexander,
and expletives typically associated with the experience of G. E., & Grafton, T. (1999). Role of the posterior parietal cortex in
updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nature Neuroscience,
movement error. Second, subjects presumably knew that their 2, 563–567.
lateral deviations were larger than typical execution errors Farah, M. J., & Feinberg, T. E. (2000). Disorders of perception and
(van Beers, Haggard, & Wolpert, 2004). Instead, we think awareness. In M. J. Farah & T. E. Feinberg (Eds.), Patient-based Ap-
the mind does not even attempt to resolve lack of percep- proaches to Cognitive Neuroscience. Issues in Clinical and Cognitive
tual awareness with positive motor awareness. Rather, motor Neuropsychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gaveau, V., Martin, O., Prablanc, C., Pelisson, D., Urquizar, C., &
awareness and perceptual awareness might be quite different Desmurget, M. (2003). On-line modification of saccadic eye move-
and independent processes in the brain, which have different ments by retinal signals. Neuroreport, 14(6), 875–878.
neural substrates and different natural time courses. In some Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for
respects, this position recalls the concept of multiple “micro- perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20–25.
consciousnesses” within the visual system (Zeki & Bartels, Gréa, H., Pisella, L., Rossetti, Y., Desmurget, M., Tilikete, C., Grafton,
S., et al. (2002). A lesion of the posterior parietal cortex disrupts
1999). on-line adjustments during aiming movements. Neuropsychologia, 40,
We end by noting a possible relation between our con- 2471–2480.
cept of motor awareness and the concept of agency used by Haggard, P. (2001). The psychology of action. British Journal of Psy-
Jeannerod (2002). The sense of agency derives from an ability chology, 92, 113–128.
to compare motor commands with reafference from the body Haggard, P., & Magno, E. (1999). Localising awareness of action with
transcranial magnetic stimulation. Experimental Brain Research, 127,
movements and external events caused by the commands. 102–107.
Our data suggests that subjects can have a clear conscious Hallet, P. E., & Lightstone, A. D. (1976). Saccadic eye movements to
awareness of their own movement patterns even when the flashed targets. Vision Research, 16, 107–114.
external stimulus event causing the movement is unclear. We Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor
believe the dissociation between perceptual awareness and intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–245.
Jeannerod, M. (2002). La nature de l’esprit: Sciences Cognitives et
motor awareness may be an important clue towards under- Cerveau. Paris: Odile Jacob.
standing how the brain distinguishes between self-generated Johnson, H., van Beers, R. J., & Haggard, P. (2002). Action and awareness
and external events, and towards understanding both the in pointing tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 451–459.
H. Johnson, P. Haggard / Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 227–237 237

Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time Pisella, L., Grea, H., Tilikete, C., Vighetto, A., Desmurget, M., Rode, G.,
of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activities et al. (2000). An automatic pilot for the hand in human posterior pari-
(readiness potential): The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary etal cortex: Toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nature Neuroscience,
act. Brain, 106, 623–642. 3, 729–736.
Matin, E. (1974). Saccadic suppression: Review and an analysis. Psycho- Prablanc, C., & Martin, O. (1992). Automatic control during reaching at
logical Bulletin, 81, 899–917. undetected two-dimensional target displacements. Journal of Neuro-
Paulignan, Y., MacKenzie, C., Marteniuk, R., & Jeannerod, M. (1991). physiology, 67, 455–469.
Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. van Beers, R. J., Haggard, P., & Wolpert, D. M. (2004). The role of
Experimental Brain Research, 83, 502–512. execution noise in movement variability. Journal of Neurophysiology,
Pélisson, D., Prablanc, C., Goodale, M. A., & Jeannerod, M. (1986). 91, 1050–1063.
Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb. Zeki, S., & Bartels, A. (1999). Toward a theory of visual consciousness.
Experimental Brain Research, 62, 303–311. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 225–259.

You might also like