You are on page 1of 4

508 Opinion TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.24 No.

9 September 2001

A TINS debate – Hindbrain versus the


forebrain: a case for cerebellar deficit
in developmental dyslexia
Progressive improvement in reading and writing skills, through school and beyond, is something that many of us take for granted.
However, for people suffering from dyslexia, these skills are not acquired in the usual manner. For many years it has been thought that
brain differences in the cortical areas related to language are the likely source of the problems. However, it has recently been
established that the problems associated with this syndrome go beyond reading-related problems: balance, motor skills and sensory
processes can also be affected. An explanation for this multitude of seemingly disparate problems has proved elusive. Roderick
Nicolson, Angela Fawcett and Paul Dean believe that a deficit in cerebellar performance might provide a complete explanation, and it is
this argument, presented in the first article below, that forms the focus of this debate. But can cerebellar deficit explain all deficits
experienced by dyslexics? Are those that experience cerebellar damage later in life similarly affected? Is the cerebellum the sole
contributor to dyslexia?
Two pairs of experts in this field, Thomas Zeffiro and Guinevere Eden, and Richard B. Ivry and Timothy C. Justus, discuss these and
other questions. It becomes clear during the debate that the acquisition of reading-related skills requires the co-ordination of many
areas of the brain involved in visual, motor and cognitive activities, and that an increased understanding of dyslexia could provide
insights far beyond the disorder itself. The conclusion to this debate is provided by Roderick Nicolson and his colleagues.

Developmental phonological deficit account2–4, which holds that the


reading difficulties derive initially from problems in
breaking spoken words down into their constituent

dyslexia: the cerebellar sounds (syllables or phonemes), and the


magnocellular deficit account, which holds that the
reading problems derive from impaired sensory

deficit hypothesis processing, caused by abnormal auditory5 and/or


visual6,7 magnocellular pathways.
Unfortunately, in spite of extensive research, these
approaches have failed to account for the full range of
Roderick I. Nicolson, Angela J. Fawcett difficulties established for dyslexic children. It is
therefore timely to present the case for our
and Paul Dean alternative hypothesis that the full range of deficits
might be accounted for in terms of cerebellar deficit.
There is no space here to survey the rich evidence
Surprisingly, the problems faced by many dyslexic children are by no means relating to alternative hypotheses. We fully expect the
confined to reading and spelling. There appears to be a general impairment in commentators to present cogent alternative data and
the ability to perform skills automatically, an ability thought to be dependent views. In preview, we claim that:
upon the cerebellum. Specific behavioural and neuroimaging tests reviewed
here indicate that dyslexia is indeed associated with cerebellar impairment in (1) The behavioural symptoms of dyslexia can be
about 80% of cases. We propose that disorders of cerebellar development can characterized as difficulties in skill automatisation8,9
in fact cause the impairments in reading and writing characteristic of (the process by which, after long practice, skills
dyslexia, a view consistent with the recently appreciated role of the become so fluent that they no longer need conscious
cerebellum in language-related skills. This proposal has implications for early control).
remedial treatment. (2) The pattern of difficulties in cognitive,
information processing and motor skills is predicted
Developmental dyslexia is traditionally defined1 as ‘a by the cerebellar deficit hypothesis9–12.
disorder in children who, despite conventional (3) Dyslexic adults showing the above behavioural
classroom experience, fail to attain the language manifestations of cerebellar impairment also show
skills of reading, writing and spelling commensurate direct neurobiological evidence of cerebellar
with their intellectual abilities’. Dyslexia researchers impairment13. This is consistent with other evidence
have focused on two alternative hypotheses: the of cerebellar abnormalities in dyslexia14.

http://tins.trends.com 0166-2236/01/$ – see front matter © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0166-2236(00)01896-8
Opinion TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.24 No.9 September 2001 509

(4) It is possible to present an ontogenetic causal (a) (b)


model for the development of the reading-related
problems and other problems of dyslexic children,
with the major causal factor being impaired implicit
learning as a result of cerebellar abnormality15.

Empirical evidence
Behavioural symptoms of dyslexia
All major theories make a reasonable attempt at Fig. 1. Regions of significantly greater activation when learning a new
explaining the major behavioural symptoms – sequence. Location of significant differences in activation (P < 0.01,
corrected for multiple comparisons at P < 0.05) between dyslexic and
reading, writing and spelling. Consequently, crucial control subjects for the comparisons of New sequence with Rest. The
tests often derive from domains outside literacy. In images are integrated sagittal and horizontal projections of the
the studies mentioned below, the dyslexic subjects are statistical parametric maps (SPMs), using an 8 mm smooth. Images
produced by SPM96 (Wellcome Dept of Cognitive Neurology, 1996).
defined in terms of: significant reading delay (at least
The areas of significant difference are shown as light blobs
18 months); IQ of at least 90; without attention deficit superimposed on a T1-weighted magnetic resonance image
hyperactive disorder (ADHD) or serious emotional normalized to the same standard stereotactic space. The axis lines on
problems. Control subjects were matched with the each image indicate horizontal and vertical planes that project through
the position of the anterior commissure. Standard radiological
dyslexic subjects for age and IQ, and had no reading convention is used [right hemisphere in (a)]. The figure shows the
delay. In early work we assessed the ‘profile’ of regions where the dyslexic group showed significantly less relative
difficulties of dyslexic children by testing a range of activation compared with controls. The only regions of significantly
different relative activation are the right hemisphere of the cerebellum,
skills, within and outside the literacy domain.
together with the cerebellar vermis.
Interestingly, we established that the dyslexic
children tested showed difficulties ‘across the board’,
in information processing speed, memory, motor skill special school for dyslexic children12. A similar
and balance, in addition to phonological and literacy pattern of difficulties again occurred, with highly
skill9,16. This pattern was obtained not only for group significant deficits on balance and muscle tone
data but also for the individuals within the group. In comparable in magnitude to their reading and
particular, taking three disparate tests – balance spelling deficits, and greater than their deficits on
(while also undertaking a secondary task), phonemic segmentation and nonsense word repetition.
segmentation (e.g. “say ‘stake’ without the ‘t’’’) and Particularly noteworthy was that 51 of the 59 dyslexic
picture naming speed – 90% of the dyslexic children children were markedly impaired on muscle tone.
had ‘marked impairment’ (at least one SD below
normal performance) on at least two of the tests. We Direct tests of cerebellar function
concluded that the data supported our ‘dyslexic The above tests of cerebellar function were necessarily
automatization hypothesis’ – that dyslexic children indirect. In considering the design of a direct test, we
have difficulties automatizing skill, whether or not wished to implement a functional imaging study.
the skill is in the literacy domain8. However, rather than select one in the reading-related
domain (for which differences in performance affect
Behavioural tests of cerebellar function interpretation of imaging data) we preferred to study a
Problems in automatisation point to the cerebellum, task outside the literacy domain in which there was
which has traditionally been considered a motor clear evidence of strong cerebellar activation in
area17–20. We have established extensive multi- normal subjects. Fortunately, a PET study23 provided
disciplinary evidence directly consistent with the a perfect opportunity. Jenkins and colleagues had
cerebellar deficit hypothesis. An influential study21 their subjects learn a sequence of eight button presses
established that patients with acute cerebellar by trial and error using a four-key response board with
damage show a characteristic dissociation between one key per finger. They established clear increases in
time estimation and loudness estimation, with a cerebellar activation (compared with rest), both when
significant deficit only for the former. We established the subjects were executing a previously overlearned
that the same dissociation occurred for our dyslexic (automatic) sequence of presses and also when they
panel10. Next, we reimplemented the classic clinical were learning a new sequence of presses. We
tests of ‘cerebellar signs’ – both dystonia and undertook a precise replication, using the oldest
dyscoordination, described in Ref. 22, and applied members (now adult) of our dyslexic and control
them to our panels. The dyslexic children showed panels. Compared with the control subjects, our
highly significant impairments on all the cerebellar dyslexic subjects showed significantly less cerebellar
Roderick I. Nicolson*
Angela J. Fawcett
tests, and significant impairment compared even activation in the ipsilateral (right) hemisphere.
Paul Dean with reading-age controls on 13 of the 14 tasks, with Interestingly, similar results were obtained for both
Dept of Psychology, effect sizes equivalent to those found on the earlier tasks – executing the previously overlearned
University of Sheffield,
literacy-related tests11. The study was subsequently sequence, and learning the new sequence (Fig. 1).
Sheffield, UK S10 2TP.
*e-mail: R.Nicolson@ replicated with a larger sample of dyslexic children Overall the dyslexic group showed barely any increase
sheff.ac.uk taken from the whole cohort of 8–16-year-olds at a in activation in the right cerebellar hemisphere and

http://tins.trends.com
510 Opinion TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.24 No.9 September 2001

are all accounted for in different ways. It might be


Balance impairment useful to distinguish between direct and indirect
cerebellar causation. Cerebellar deficit provides a
Motor skill impairment Writing
natural, direct, explanation of the execrable quality of
Impaired Reduced handwriting frequently shown by dyslexic children.
phonological working Handwriting, of course, is a motor skill that requires
Cerebellar loop memory precise timing and coordination of diverse muscle
impairment groups. Literacy difficulties arise from several routes.
Articulatory Phonological Reading The central route is highlighted. If an infant has a
Cerebro- skill awareness
cerebellar impairment, initial direct manifestations
cerebellar
loop
will be a mild motor difficulty – the infant might be
Word
recognition slower to sit up and to walk – and crucially, the direct
module effect on articulation would suggest that the infant
might be slower to start babbling, and, later, talking.
Problems automatising Even after speech and walking emerge, one might
Spelling
skill and knowledge expect that the skills would be less fluent, less
TRENDS in Neurosciences
‘dextrous’, in infants with cerebellar impairment. If
articulation is less fluent than normal, then one
Fig. 2. A hypothetical causal chain. The abscissa represents both the passage of time (experience) indirect effect is that it takes up more conscious
and also the ways that difficulties with skill acquisition cause subsequent problems, leading to the resources, leaving fewer resources to process the
known difficulties in reading, writing and spelling. The text provides a fuller explanation of the
processes involved. Of particular interest is the progression highlighted as a central feature by the
ensuing sensory feedback. An additional indirect
box. Cerebellar abnormality at birth leads to mild motor and articulatory problems. Lack of effect is that reduced articulation speed leads to
articulatory fluency leads in turn to an impoverished representation of the phonological reduced effective ‘working memory’ as reflected in the
characteristics of speech, and subsequently to the well-established difficulties in phonological
‘phonological loop’24. This, in turn leads to difficulties
awareness at ∼5 years that lead to subsequent problems in learning to read. Other routes outline the
probable problems outside the phonological domain, and indicate that the difficulties in learning to in language acquisition25. Furthermore, reduced
read, spell and write might derive from a number of inter-dependent factors. quality of articulatory representation might lead
directly to impaired sensitivity to onset, rime, and the
vermis (~10% of the controls). This PET study phonemic structure of language26 – in short, one
therefore confirmed that the behavioural cerebellar would expect early deficits in phonological awareness.
signs of these subjects did indeed reflect abnormal Cerebellar impairment would therefore be predicted
cerebellar function, and therefore lends weight to the to cause, by direct and indirect means, the
above behavioural studies. ‘phonological core deficit’27 that has proved such a
Converging direct evidence of cerebellar dysfunction fruitful explanatory framework for many aspects of
is also provided by a recent study14 of metabolic dyslexia. For spelling, the third criterial skill,
abnormalities in dyslexic men. Rae and colleagues problems arise from several indirect routes – over-
obtained localized proton magnetic resonance spectra effort in reading, poor phonological awareness and
bilaterally from the temporo-parietal cortex and difficulties in automatising skills.
cerebellum of 14 dyslexic men and 15 control men of It is valuable to consider how the above framework
similar age. Bilateral MR spectroscopy indicated relates to alternative theoretical formulations for
significant differences in the ratio of choline-containing dyslexia. Note that there is a qualitative difference
compounds to N-acetylaspartate (NA) in the left between the three hypotheses discussed above: the
temporo-parietal lobe and the right cerebellum, magnocellular deficit and cerebellar deficit
together with lateralization differences in the hypotheses are both phrased in terms of an
cerebellum of the dyslexic men but not the controls. The underlying neural substrate – the ‘biological’ level,
authors concluded that ‘These differences provide whereas the phonological deficit hypothesis is framed
direct evidence of the involvement of the cerebellum in in terms of a non-biological theoretical mechanism –
dyslexic dysfunction’. the ‘cognitive’ level28.
At the biological level, cerebellar deficit is an
Toward a causal explanation alternative, or perhaps parallel, mechanism to
The above analyses indicate a correlation between magnocellular abnormality. It is possible that
dyslexia and abnormal cerebellar function in ~80% of dyslexic children might show either or both of these
the dyslexic children tested. A key question that abnormalities. This remains an open research issue.
arises is whether cerebellar impairment can provide a From the behavioural and functional data that we
causal explanation of the development of the specific have established, it would appear probable that the
cognitive difficulties of dyslexic children. majority of dyslexic children suffer from abnormal
Figure 2 (adapted from Ref. 15) outlines one cerebellar function.
hypothetical ontogenetic causal chain, linking At the cognitive level of explanation we have
cerebellar problems, phonological difficulties and outlined how cerebellar deficit accounts naturally for
eventual reading problems. Note that the three phonological deficit and for automatisation deficit. It
criterial difficulties of writing, reading and spelling also provides a natural explanation of the more recent

http://tins.trends.com
Opinion TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.24 No.9 September 2001 511

‘double deficit’ hypothesis29. This is based on the cerebellum in articulation-related cognitive skills is
established difficulties that dyslexic children have on directly consistent with recent evidence of its role in
‘rapid automatised naming’ tasks30, in which the child speech-related cognitive tasks.
has to name as rapidly as possible a page full of (4) Finally, we provided a plausible, albeit
common pictures or standard colours, and suggests speculative, causal analysis that explains the
that dyslexia is characterized by a deficit not only in difficulties in reading, writing and spelling within a
phonological skills but also in naming speed consistent and coherent developmental framework.
(reflecting a lower speed of processing). Naming speed Furthermore, two of the major alternative cognitive-
difficulties are precisely those predicted by the level explanations of dyslexia, namely the phonological
cerebellar deficit hypothesis, given its established deficit hypothesis and the double-deficit hypothesis,
role in speech, inner speech and speeded processing. might be integrated naturally within this framework.
Consequently, all three cognitive level hypotheses
appear to be directly consistent with, and indeed, We would like to conclude by emphasizing that the
subsumed by, the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. cerebellar deficit hypothesis should be seen as
speculative at this stage, because the dyslexia-related
Summary and conclusions data provided are mostly from small scale studies in
In summary, we have argued the following points. our own laboratory. One important research
requirement therefore is to establish the extent to
(1) A high percentage of diagnosed dyslexic children which other groups of dyslexic children show
show behavioural evidence of abnormal cerebellar ‘cerebellar signs’. The approach raises many further
function – in skill automatisation, in time estimation, theoretical questions: are there subtypes of dyslexia
balance and the classic cerebellar signs of dystonia. corresponding to different loci of abnormality in the
(2) In the dyslexic adults tested, the behavioural cerebellum; to what extent do cerebellar and
evidence of cerebellar abnormality was accompanied by magnocellular deficits co-occur; and how do these
direct evidence of abnormal cerebellar function, both specific issues relate to underlying genetic
for executing an ‘automatic’sequence of button presses endowment33,34? We consider these are all potentially
and for learning a new sequence of button presses. fruitful research issues, and we consider their
(3) The difficulties in skill automatisation investigation will continue to illuminate the complex
correspond directly to the traditional role of the interplay between the brain, the environment and
cerebellum31,32. The hypothesised role of the behaviour, in both normal and abnormal development.
References 12 Fawcett, A.J. and Nicolson, R.I. (1999) early school years: a longitudinal study. Dev.
1 World Federation of Neurology (1968) Report of Performance of dyslexic children on cerebellar Psychol. 28, 887–898
research group on dyslexia and world illiteracy and cognitive tests. J. Mot. Behav. 31, 68–78 26 Snowling, M. and Hulme, C. (1994) The
WFN 13 Nicolson, R.I. et al. (1999) Association of abnormal development of phonological skills. Phil. Trans.
2 Bradley, L. and Bryant, P.E. (1983) Categorising cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties Roy. Soc. London Series B 346, 21–27
sounds and learning to read: a causal connection. in dyslexic adults. Lancet 353, 1662–1667 27 Stanovich, K.E. (1988) Explaining the differences
Nature 301, 419–421 14 Rae, C. et al. (1998) Metabolic abnormalities in between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor
3 Shankweiler, D. et al. (1995) Cognitive profiles of developmental dyslexia detected by H-1 magnetic reader: the phonological-core variable-difference
reading-disabled children – comparison of resonance spectroscopy. Lancet 351, 1849–1852 model. J. Learn. Dis. 21, 590–604
language-skills in phonology, morphology, and 15 Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (1999) 28 Morton, J. and Frith, U. (1995) Causal modelling:
syntax. Psychol. Sci. 6, 149–156 Developmental dyslexia: the role of the a structural approach to developmental
4 Stanovich, K.E. (1988) The right and wrong places cerebellum. Dyslexia: Int. J. Res. Prac. 5, 155–177 psychopathology. In Manual of Developmental
to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability. 16 Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (1994) Reaction times Psychopathology (D. Cicchetti and D.J. Cohen
Ann. Dyslexia 38, 154–177 and Dyslexia. Quart. J. of Exp. Psychol. 47A, 29–48 eds), Wiley
5 Tallal, P. et al. (1993) Neurobiological basis of 17 Holmes, G. (1917) The symptoms of acute cerebellar 29 Wolf, M. and Bowers, P.G. (1999) The double-
speech - a case for the pre-eminence of temporal injuries due to gunshot injuries. Brain 40, 461–535 deficit hypothesis for the developmental
processing. Ann. New York Acad. Sci. 682, 27–47 18 Eccles, J.C. et al. (1967) The Cerebellum as a dyslexias. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 415–438
6 Eden, G.F. et al. (1996) The visual deficit theory of Neuronal Machine. Springer–Verlag 30 Denckla, M.B. and Rudel, R.G. (1976) Rapid
developmental dyslexia. NeuroImage 4, S108–S117 19 Ito, M. (1984) The Cerebellum and Neural ‘Automatized’ naming (R.A.N.). Dyslexia
7 Stein, J. and Walsh, V. (1997) To see but not to Control. Raven Press differentiated from other learning disabilities.
read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends 20 Stein, J.F. and Glickstein, M. (1992) Role of the Neuropsychologia 14, 471–479
Neurosci. 20, 147–152 cerebellum in visual guidance of movement. 31 Marr, D. (1969) A theory of cerebellar cortex.
8 Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (1990) Physiol. Rev. 72, 972–1017 J. Physiol. (London) 202, 437–470
Automaticity: a new framework for dyslexia 21 Ivry, R.B. and Keele, S.W. (1989) Timing functions 32 Albus, J.S. (1971) A theory of cerebellar function.
research? Cognition 35, 159–182 of the cerebellum. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 1, 136–152 Math. Biosci. 10, 25–61
9 Nicolson, R.I. and Fawcett, A.J. (1994) 22 Dow, R.S. and Moruzzi, G. (1958) The Physiology 33 Gayan, J. et al. (1999) Quantitative-trait locus for
Comparison of deficits in cognitive and motor- and Pathology of the Cerebellum. University of specific language and reading deficits on
skills among children with dyslexia. Ann. Minnesota Press chromosome 6p. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 64,
Dyslexia 44, 147–164 23 Jenkins, I.H. et al. (1994) Motor sequence 157–164
10 Nicolson, R.I. et al. (1995) Time-estimation learning – a study with Positron Emission 34 Grigorenko, E.L. et al. (2000) Chromosome 6p
deficits in developmental dyslexia – evidence for Tomography. J. Neurosci. 14, 3775–3790 influences on different dyslexia-related cognitive
cerebellar involvement. Proc. R. Soc. London 24 Baddeley, A.D. et al. (1975) Word length and the processes: further confirmation. Am. J. Hum.
Series B 259, 43–47 structure of short term memory. J. Verb. Learn. Genet. 66, 715–723
11 Fawcett, A.J. et al. (1996) Impaired performance Verb. Behav.14, 575–589 35 Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology
of children with dyslexia on a range of cerebellar 25 Gathercole, S.A. et al. (1992) Phonological (1996) Statistical Parametric Mapping SPM96.
tasks. Ann. Dyslexia 46, 259–283 memory and vocabulary development during the (ICN, London)

http://tins.trends.com

You might also like