You are on page 1of 15

ZDM Mathematics Education

DOI 10.1007/s11858-013-0519-1

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Challenges students identified with a learning disability


and as high-achieving experience when using diagrams
as a visualization tool to solve mathematics word problems
Delinda van Garderen • Amy Scheuermann •

Apryl Poch

Accepted: 7 July 2013


 FIZ Karlsruhe 2013

Abstract This article addresses a much understudied Behr, & Post, 1987) within mathematics, a representation
topic and concern regarding how students of varying ability could refer to both an internal and external manifestation of
levels employ visualization as a strategy in mathematics a mathematical idea or concept (Pape & Tchoshanov,
learning. The importance of this topic can be found in its 2001). Further, it could also refer to the act of taking an
connection to students’ ability to solve mathematical word internal representation or mental representation and exter-
problems. Many students, particularly students with nalizing it (Pape & Tchoshanov, 2001). Therefore, as noted
learning disabilities, often struggle to use visualization as a within the National Council for Teachers of Mathematics
strategy and this impacts their mathematics performance. [NCTM] Standards (2000) document, a representation
The purpose of this article is to present findings from a ‘‘refers both to process and to product—to the act of cap-
study that examined the challenges that students—those turing a mathematical concept or relationship in some form
identified as learning disabled and high-achieving—dis- and to the form itself’’ (p. 67).
played when using one visualization form, a diagram, to Many different representational forms exist (Zawojew-
solve mathematics problems. Overall, nine challenges ski & Lesh, 2003), and all, to some extent, can be used to
related to the use of diagram proficiency to solve problems solve word problems. However, one often recommended
were identified. Further, students with learning disabilities strategy for solving mathematical word problems is to use
were found to be more likely than their high achieving visual (external) representations—in particular, a diagram
peers to experience these challenges. Implications for (Lesh, 1999; Pape & Tchoshanov, 2001; Polya, 1957;
practice are provided. Shigematsu & Sowder, 1994). In studying diagrams, it is
useful to distinguish between different types that may be
Keywords Learning disability  Ability  Diagrams  used for problem solving (Zahner & Corter, 2010). The
Challenges first type is a diagram that frequently accompanies a text or
problem (e.g., Mayer, 1989; Mayer & Gallini, 1990) or
may support a particular type of mathematics problem
1 Introduction using a given schematic format (e.g., Jitendra et al., 2013).
The second type, and the focus of this study, is student-
Broadly defined a ‘‘representation’’ is a configuration that generated diagrams that are both prompted for and spon-
stands for something else (Goldin, 2002; Kaput, 1987). taneously initiated while engaged in problem solving
Although often considered an artificial dichotomy (Lesh, (similar to studies conducted by Diezmann, 2000; Schwartz
& Martin, 2004; Stylianou, 2002; Zahner & Corter, 2010).
Furthermore, we are particularly interested in the use of
D. van Garderen (&)  A. Poch student generated diagrams (e.g., tables, part-whole dia-
University of Missouri, Columbia, USA grams) that convey ‘‘information in a spatial layout’’
e-mail: vangarderend@missouri.edu
(Diezmann & English, 2001, p. 77) and cross various
A. Scheuermann mathematical domains (e.g., algebra, number and opera-
Minnesota State University, Mankato, USA tions, proportionality, probability).

123
D. van Garderen et al.

2 Use of a diagram to solve word problems 2.1 Proficiency for using diagrams to solve
mathematics problems
The use of diagrams can be an extremely ‘‘powerful’’
visual representation strategy when solving word problems. The ability to use a diagram as a tool for solving word
In the research, this is evidenced in two ways. First, problems should not be underestimated; it is a complex
numerous studies that incorporated the systematic use of a task. Representing the information from a mathematics
diagram have led to substantial improvements in mathe- problem as a diagram involves decoding the linguistic
matics achievement for students with disabilities (e.g., information that then is encoded into visual information
Butler et al. 2003; Jitendra et al. 2002; Maccini & Hughes, (Diezmann, 2000). This ‘‘translation’’ process depends on
2000; van Garderen, 2007; Witzel et al. 2003). Second, a the ability to selectively encode what is relevant, selec-
connection between the type of visual representation gen- tively combine the information into an integrated form, and
erated and overall problem solving performance has been selectively compare that information to identify new
established (Brown & Presmeg, 1993; Hegarty & Ko- knowledge and connect to prior knowledge about the
zhevnikov, 1999; Owens & Clements, 1998; Presmeg, problem and, of particular interest to this study, the dia-
1986a, 1986b; van Garderen & Montague, 2003). Specifi- gram (Sternberg, 1990, cited in Diezmann, 2000). Fur-
cally, the use of imagery considered schematic (images that thermore, encoding information from a mathematics
contain relational information of the problem) has been problem into a diagram requires an extensive knowledge
positively related to success in mathematical problem base. This includes knowledge related to the ability to
solving, whereas the use of imagery considered pictorial select, produce and productively use a diagram as a prob-
(images that depict the visual appearance of objects or lem solving tool as well as the ability to critique and
persons in a problem) has been negatively related to suc- modify or generate a new diagram where needed within the
cess in mathematical problem solving (Hegarty & Ko- context of a problem solving situation (Diezmann & Eng-
zhevnikov, 1999; van Garderen & Montague, 2003; van lish, 2001; diSessa, 2002; diSessa & Sherin, 2000). The
Garderen et al. 2012; Zahner & Corter, 2010). In addition, ability to use a diagram proficiently for solving a word
the more strategic ways in which a diagram was used (e.g., problem would be the goal.
not only to understand the problem but also solve and In examining what it means to be ‘‘mathematically pro-
monitor problem solving) has been positively correlated to ficient,’’ the National Research Council (NRC; 2001) sug-
higher performance for solving word problems (van gested that it involves five core strands or components:
Garderen et al. 2012). conceptual understanding, procedural fluency, strategic
Beyond the evidence related to mathematics perfor- competence, adaptive reasoning, and productive disposition.
mance, there are a number of other compelling reasons for Each strand provides a framework for ‘‘discussing the
using diagrams as a means to solve word problems. First, knowledge, skills, abilities, and beliefs that constitute
diagrams as a representation strategy demonstrate great mathematical proficiency’’ (NRC, 2001) as a whole, but also
versatility as they can be used for solving various types of to the various mathematical domains (e.g., algebra, geome-
problems for many topic areas (e.g., geometry, number and try, number and operations, etc.) (NRC, 2001, pp. 141–142).
operations, probability) and at all grade levels (Diezmann We also contend, however, that such a framework can also be
& English, 2001; Ives & Hoy, 2003; Novick et al. 1999; applied to using a mathematical process such as a diagram.
Zahner & Corter, 2010). Second, diagrams are powerful Figure 1 describes the five strands of ‘‘diagram proficiency’’
ways to facilitate communication about critical ideas in along with the knowledge, skills, abilities, and beliefs
mathematics as well as provide a platform for sharing associated with each strand. An important point to make
problem solving strategies with others (Stylianou, 2010). about these five strands is that they are interwoven and
As a result, both teachers and students can easily monitor interdependent. For example, to use a diagram in a strategic
the problem solving process with a diagram to evaluate and manner does require conceptual understanding of what a
improve upon its use, thereby increasing problem solving diagram is and how it may help to solve a problem. However,
performance (Kulm, 1994). Third, a diagram is a tool that this framework functions as a way to guide instruction and
can be used in multiple ways during the problem solving provides a way to analyze student proficiency (e.g., student
process. As Stylianou (2010, p. 329) notes, a representa- can generate a diagram accurately and efficiently for a given
tion—in our case a diagram—can serve (1) as a means to problem) (NRC, 2001).
understand the problem situation, (2) as a way to record
information both of the problem situation itself and of ideas 2.1.1 Conceptual understanding
as the problem is being solved, (3) as a tool to facilitate
exploration of critical concepts of the problem being Diezmann and English (2001) define a diagram as a ‘‘visual
solved, and (4) as a way to monitor and evaluate progress. representation that displays information in a spatial layout’’

123
Challenges students identified with a learning disability and as high-achieving

Stran d Descrip ti on Ab i li t ie s
Conceptual Comprehension of what Can describe what a diagram is and
Understanding relationships a diagram can the ways a diagram can be used for
represent and how a diagram can be solving a problem.
used when solving a problem.
Procedural Skill in accurately and efficiently Can generate a diagram that represents
Fluency generating a diagram that represents the problem situation.
the problem situation.
Strategic Ability to represent and use a Can use a relevant diagram as a tool to
Competence diagram to solve a problem. represent, solve and monitor the
problem solving process.
Adaptive Ability to justify and communicate Can explain and justify how the
Reasoning an explanation of one’s use of a diagram that was used was appropriate
diagram to solve a problem. to help solve the problem.
Productive See a diagram as beneficial, Recognizes when a diagram can be
Disposition worthwhile and sensible for solving beneficial for solving a problem and
a problem coupled with a belief and will self-initiate use, with confidence,
confidence in one’s own ability to as needed.
use it.
Fig. 1 Strands of diagram proficiency. (Note. Adapted from NRC, 2001 p. 117)

(p. 77). Therefore, a diagram is not simply a picture or strategy is that it can take various forms depending on the
piece of artwork that portrays surface details; rather it type of problem to be solved (Diezmann & English, 2001;
depicts mathematical relations of the problem situation Novick et al. 1999). Novick et al. (1999), for example,
(Fosnot & Dolk, 2001; van Garderen, 2006). In addition to identified three general-purpose diagrams (line diagram,
understanding what a diagram is, knowing how it may be matrices, hierarchies) and Diezmann (1999) identified one
of use in the process of solving a problem is important. As general purpose diagram (part-whole) that can be used to
previously noted, diagrams can be used in multiple ways to solve certain types of problems (e.g., line diagram or net-
understand, solve, and monitor progress when solving a work that is best suited for problems that require objects or
problem (Stylianou, 2010). things to be put in order). While we acknowledge that
generating a relevant diagram for a given problem does
2.1.2 Procedural fluency depend on understanding the mathematics content in the
problem, we suggest that diagram strategic competence
Procedural fluency in mathematics is the knowledge of involves generating an appropriate diagram (i.e., using a
procedures first in terms of knowing when and how to form that best suits the quantitative relationships expressed
apply a procedure but also performing this procedure in the problem) and also using the diagram as a tool whether
flexibly, accurately, and efficiently (NRC, 2001). Likewise, to understand, solve, or monitor the problem solving
procedural fluency in terms of a diagram requires skill in process.
accurately and efficiently generating a diagram that rep-
resents the problem situation. This includes recognizing 2.1.4 Adaptive reasoning
that a diagram needs to depict key components of the
problem situation, but does not necessarily have to depict Presmeg (1986b) noted that for imagery to be useful it
all objects or things stated in a problem, nor does it need to needs to be coupled with analysis and thought. When using
look realistic. In addition, it should not take long to gen- a diagram to solve a problem, it should be used as a part of
erate which may mean using appropriate symbols or codes the reasoning process (i.e., ‘‘think logically about the
to represent the information (van Garderen, 2006). relationship among concepts and situations’’ [NRC, 2001,
p. 129]) in order to not only reach a solution, but also as a
2.1.3 Strategic competence way to explain and justify (i.e., ‘‘provide sufficient reason
for’’ [NRC, 2001, p. 130]) the solution. This suggests that
Critical to solving any problem is the ability to use a variety students recognize that a diagram is a tool and can explain
of strategies that might be useful for solving a specific and reason how they used it as a tool rather than just an
problem (NRC, 2001). The ‘‘strength’’ of a diagram as a ‘‘end product’’ that can aid them in the process of solving a

123
D. van Garderen et al.

problem (Pape & Tchoshanov, 2001). Further, this implies However, two studies, which focus on school-age students
a need for accuracy and precision when using a diagram as without disabilities, have examined student use of diagrams
a tool to aid the reasoning process (Diezmann & English, while solving problems and provide potential insights as to
2001; van Garderen, 2006). the difficulties students with LD may experience.
Diezmann (2000) examined 12 students’ (mean age of
2.1.5 Productive disposition 10 years 3 months) responses to five novel problems dur-
ing an interview and identified three categories of diffi-
Critical to the spontaneous use of diagrams to solve culties the students displayed. The first category was that
mathematics problems is the need for confidence, along students did not use a diagram. Reasons for this included a
with the perception that diagrams are ‘‘easy’’ to use and not lack of understanding the term and concept of a diagram,
just a teacher strategy (Uesaka et al. 2007). Therefore, a and that a diagram is a representation that utilizes a scale.
productive disposition towards diagrams includes the rec- The second category related to generic difficulties in the
ognition that a diagram can be beneficial and worthwhile to generation of a diagram. Specifically students drew dia-
use as a strategy for solving a problem along with the belief grams that were unusable as they were either too small or
and confidence in the use of a diagram. This also suggests untidy to allowed for modification or manipulation, or
that students will, as their conceptual, procedural, strategic, contained inaccuracies related to quantities. The third cat-
and adaptive reasoning abilities develop, self-initiate use of egory involved idiosyncratic difficulties related to the
a diagram when needed and appropriately select diagrams generation of a diagram. Specific difficulties included a
for a given problem-solving situation. lack of precision in the diagrams, overlooking the con-
straints in a diagram, incorrect labeling of the starting
2.2 Diagram proficiency of school-aged students position of a diagram, and incorrect portrayal of a rela-
with learning disabilities tionship within a diagram.
Smith (1999, cited in Smith, 2003) examined elemen-
While poor conceptual understanding of the mathematics tary-aged student responses focused on the creation and use
has been cited as a reason for interfering with mathematics of representations, in this case, diagrams, from a child’s
performance (e.g., Dufour-Janvier et al. 1987; van Gard- perspective to solve non-routine problems. In a summary of
eren, 2007), poor problem solving performance may also four children’s responses to problems involving portioning,
be the result of limited diagram proficiency (Diezmann, several interesting findings pertinent to the current study
2000; Dufour-Janvier et al. 1987; Larkin & Simon, 1987). were identified. First, it is possible to overemphasize the
If a student does not have an adequate proficiency for using context of a problem in the representation and not engage
diagrams, a diagram as a tool will have limited usefulness with the abstraction of mathematics (e.g., the quantitative
(Pape and Tchoshanov 2001). As Dufour-Janvier et al. relationship via an algorithm). However, it is also possible
(1987) noted, a representation can only be useful to the to overemphasize the abstraction of the mathematics to the
extent it has been ‘‘grasped’’ by the child. exclusion of the context in the process of examining the
From the limited research available focused on school- reasonableness of an answer. Finally, the use of represen-
aged children (K-12), three main findings emerge about the tations may be perceived as being designed only to solve a
difficulty students with a learning disability (LD) have particular problem, but not be seen as a tool for general use.
using visual representations, in particular diagrams. First,
they infrequently, if at all, use any visual representation
strategies when solving mathematical word problems 3 Purpose of the current study
(Montague & Applegate, 1993a, 1993b; van Garderen &
Montague, 2003). Second, when a diagram is generated, it Unfortunately, not all students with LD develop acceptable
is often poor in quality and lacking in important relational levels of proficiency in the use of a diagram (van Garderen
information (van Garderen & Montague, 2003; van Gard- & Montague, 2003; van Garderen et al. 2012). To over-
eren et al. 2012). Third, students with LD appear to use come this requires the provision of instruction that supports
their visual representations in fewer strategic ways (e.g., to the correct representation of problems (Rittle-Johnson et al.
organize, plan, monitor, compute, justify) than their peers 2001). However, exactly how this should be supported in
when solving word problems (van Garderen et al. 2012). the instruction is less clear. One particularly useful
Although it is clear that many students with LD appear approach to help guide instructional decision-making is
to struggle to use visual representations for solving word examining students’ thinking—in particular their errors
problems, to date, no studies have examined the specific (Ashlock, 2006; Fennema et al. 1993). While errors in and
challenges these students may experience as they use dia- of themselves are not bad as they can be seen as ‘‘an
grams as a tool to solve mathematics word problems. essential component of the construction of knowledge’’ and

123
Challenges students identified with a learning disability and as high-achieving

‘‘indicators that learning is taking place’’ (Knight, 2002, 2007). The KeyMath 3 is a norm-referenced measure of
p. 7), it is also possible that these errors may prevent or essential mathematical concepts and skills across the
misdirect mathematical learning that, if not addressed, mathematical strands.
become entrenched and accepted as accurate. Therefore, To maintain balance between the number of students
errors can also be viewed as diagnostic tools that provide with a LD and those identified as HA, the strongest three
insight into students’ processes and thinking and suggest students classified as HA in mathematics from each grade
a focus for remedial instruction (Pressley et al. 1992). level were examined. Students identified as HA had to have
Instructionally, these gaps and misconceptions could then scale scores of 15 or above on 2 or more of the KeyMath 3
be considered as ‘‘sites for learning’’ that are essential for subtests (numeration, applied problem solving, addition
the student’s growth as a mathematician (Hiebert et al. and subtraction, or multiplication and division). If more
1997). Additionally, many studies compare how students of than three individuals in a grade level met the scale score
exceptional ability versus students of lower ability solve criteria, the three with the highest total scores on the Non-
problems (e.g., Montague & Applegate, 1993a, 1993b). Routine Word Problem Assessment (NWPA; van Garderen
Through this type of comparison, it is possible to identify et al. 2012) were selected. Subject demographic data is
areas of weakness or challenge that the ‘‘novice’’ experi- presented in Table 1.
ences as compared to the preferred ways in which an
‘‘expert’’ would solve problems. 4.2 Research design and instruments
The purpose of this study is to identify and compare
what challenges students, specifically students identified as A qualitative analysis of the students’ responses to tran-
high achieving (HA) and students with LD, experience scribed interview questions about diagrams, their use when
when using diagrams to solve mathematics problems. solving mathematics word problems, and their actual
problem solving was completed in order to identify the
mathematical understandings and misconceptions students
4 Method with and without LD had. Data were collected from student
interviews in which the students were asked: ‘‘In mathe-
4.1 Participants and setting matics, what is a diagram?’’ and ‘‘In mathematics, why
would you use a diagram?,’’ along with student work
The data utilized in this study comes from a larger study samples including their explanations of their mathematical
that quantitatively examined the understanding and use of problem solving and diagram usage when solving both
diagrams to solve mathematics word problems. Participants prompted and unprompted word problems from the
included 95 fourth through seventh grade students with and NWPA. In this measure, the students needed to solve up to
without LD from Ohio and Missouri (see van Garderen 12 mathematical problems (minimum of 8) at various
et al. 2012). Every student who participated in the original levels and explain their processing (e.g., ‘‘Tell me how you
study returned a signed consent form and, according to solved this problem?’’, ‘‘In what way did you use a picture
school records, had English as his/her primary language. to solve this word problem?’’). For the last six problems the
For this study, the data from 28 students, 16 students with students were prompted to use a diagram when solving. All
LD and 12 students identified as HA in mathematics, was problems were read to the students and written responses
qualitatively examined in order to more clearly identify the were collected via student workbook while verbal respon-
challenges or misconceptions students experience when ses were audio recorded and then transcribed.
using a diagram to solve word problems. Students with LD
met their local district eligibility criteria that follows the 4.3 Data analysis
federal regulations (Federal Regulation 34 C.F.R § 300.8
(c)(10); see Appendix A for summary of criteria), and had a A content analysis approach was used to analyze the data.
full-scale IQ score of 80 or higher on the Wechsler Intel- The analysis itself involved several steps that followed a
ligence Scale for Children—IV (2003). The students with constant comparative data analysis procedure in which data
LD did not have to have a specific mathematics learning were analyzed systematically (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). At
disability to participate; all who were being served within each step, data were analyzed independently then com-
the category of learning disability were included. However, pared across researchers and re-analyzed to confirm find-
on average, all students with LD were lower performing in ings. To establish a general sense of the data, students’
mathematics than their peers without LD (see van Garderen written and verbal responses to each word problem, the unit
et al. 2012 for more details regarding their mathematical of analysis, were read multiple times by the second and
performance) based on assessment scores using the Key- third author. First, they independently analyzed the data
Math 3 Diagnostic Assessment (KeyMath 3; Connolly, collected only from the students with LD and generated

123
D. van Garderen et al.

Table 1 Demographics of participants by ability level and means and categorization (Patton, 1990). Patterns related to student
standard deviations for IQ and KeyMath3 subtests difficulties and barriers regarding diagrams emerged and
Variable Ability level were categorized into challenges. As the challenges were
established, parallels to the five strands of diagram profi-
High Learning Overall
achieving disability (n = 28) ciency became apparent. These strands then became the
(n = 12) (n = 16) overall themes by which the data was organized.
The second step involved the first and second author
Age
independently confirming the alignment of the challenges
M (years) 11.17 11.31 11.25
within the five strands of diagram proficiency and finding
SD 1.53 1.58 1.53
corroborating examples. At this point, the challenges were
Gender further refined.
Male 6 (50 %) 10 (62.5 %) 16 (57.1 %) The third step then involved the second and third authors
Female 6 (50 %) 6 (37.5 %) 12 (42.9 %) examining the data from the students identified as HA.
Ethnicity These data were then used to find additional challenges that
White 11 (91.7 %) 9 (56.3 %) 20 (71.4 %) may not have emerged from the data on students with LD
African–American – 6 (37.5 %) 6 (21.4 %) and to find examples that matched the challenges already
Hispanic – 1 (6.3 %) 1 (3.6 %) identified.
Asian 1 (8.3 %) – 3 (3.6 %) In the final step, all of the authors identified which
Free and/or reduced lunch students exhibited each challenge within the five strands of
Yes 2 (16.7 %) 13 (81.3 %) 15 (53.6 %) diagram proficiency generated from the data of both stu-
No 10 (83.3 %) 3 (18.8 %) 13 (46.4 %) dents identified as HA and students with LD in order to
Grade level compare how the groups differed across the various
Fourth grade 3 (25.0 %) 7 (43.8 %) 10 (35.7 %) strands. During this step, the challenges were further
Fifth grade 3 (25.0 %) 3 (18.8 %) 6 (21.4 %) refined and collapsed.
Sixth grade 3 (25.0 %) – 3 (10.7 %)
Seventh grade 3 (25.0 %) 6 (37.5 %) 9 (32.1 %)
IQ scoresa
5 Findings
M 131.86 94.06 105.57
SD 14.44 8.11 20.44
The purpose of this study was to first, identify what chal-
KeyMath 3b lenges students experienced related to diagram use and
Numeration understanding, and second, to compare these challenges
M 15.33 6.81 10.46 experienced between students identified as LD or HA in
SD 1.50 1.87 4.62 mathematics. In the end, nine challenges were identified
Addition and subtraction that aligned to the five strands of diagram proficiency (see
M 13.58 6.19 9.36 Fig. 1). We first report the different challenges that
SD 2.43 2.34 4.40 emerged from the examined data. Following that discus-
Multiplication and division sion, we compare how the two groups of students differed
M 13.33 6.19 9.25 on those challenges.
SD 2.27 1.56 4.05
Applied problem solving 5.1 Challenges students experienced
M 16.25 6.63 10.75
SD 1.29 1.82 5.10 5.1.1 Challenges related to conceptual understanding
a
Mean of 100 and SD of 15
b
Means calculated on scale score (M = 10, SD = 3) Two challenges related to conceptual understanding
emerged from the data. First, students were unable to
define a diagram, and, in some cases, appeared confused by
codes to categorize the data into frequently occurring
the term. For example, in response to the question ‘‘In
challenges. While the solution accuracy of each problem
mathematics, what is a diagram?’’ students responded:
was noted, the primary focus of their analysis was the
difficulties and barriers these students displayed while I haven’t studied them yet. Diagram? Like those little
using diagrams. The independent analyses were then dots? Oh, those are decimals. Sorry. I don’t know
shared and discussed by all of the researchers, who together what they are, but I think that they help you when
identified shared similarities and differences in coding and you’re doing a math problem.

123
Challenges students identified with a learning disability and as high-achieving

The distance, height, and length. … Surface area, I Likewise, reasons given for how a diagram could be of
don’t know. benefit frequently lacked depth and were primarily limited
S: I don’t know. to one main idea. For example, the majority of students
I: You don’t know? Have you heard that word suggested that a diagram is a good way to organize and see
before? information to understand and remember the problem.
S: Yes, in class, but I don’t know what it is. When asked how a diagram could help, one student stated,
I: Can you remember when or why she used that ‘‘So that you can organize your thoughts and you don’t get
word? numbers lost.’’ A few students indicated that it could be
S: When, was before yesterday. And then she used it used to solve a problem and/or check work such as, ‘‘To
on the board and I did not know what she meant on help me solve problems, so I could find the right answer.
the board. And also to check my work.’’
Further, several students were unable to provide any
reason or were confused as to why a diagram could be used 5.1.2 Challenge related to procedural fluency
to solve a mathematics problem and, how it could help.
When asked how a diagram could help one student Within procedural fluency, one primary challenge emerged—
responded, ‘‘Because you need it to answer a problem, and students drew diagrams that contained inaccuracies when
if you don’t, you can’t finish the problem. You don’t know compared to the information found in the problem. This
what the answer is.’’ Another stated, ‘‘Maybe to measure included notation of incorrect numbers and quantities or the
things.’’ absence of key specific details central to solving the problem
Although some students could provide a definition of a as seen in the examples found in Fig. 2. As Diezmann (2000)
diagram and a reason for using them, a second challenge notes, while this may reflect carelessness, because these
involving the conceptual understanding of a diagram that inaccuracies were not detected and resulted in an incorrect
emerged was a lack of depth or understanding of the answer, it is considered an irrecoverable error.
mathematical use of the term diagram. In many of their
definitions, students simply referenced a diagram form 5.1.3 Challenges related to strategic competence
such as a ‘‘table’’, ‘‘chart’’, or ‘‘graph’’. Others indicated it
was a ‘‘picture’’ or ‘‘drawing,’’ lacking any connection to a Two main challenges emerged within strategic compe-
representation that depicts relational elements. Several tence. First, students did not use a diagram to understand,
students suggested it was something that organizes or gives solve, or monitor the process of solving one or more
you information. For example, ‘‘A diagram is like a gra- problems. Figure 3 provides examples of this challenge.
phic, a visual, that organizes data.’’ Most notably, when prompted to use a diagram to solve a

Fig. 2 Inaccuracies in diagram generation

123
D. van Garderen et al.

Fig. 3 Did not use the diagram to understand, solve, or monitor while solving the problem

problem, some students generated it after they solved the S: I don’t know that one, that one is hard.
problem or only when they recognized they were having I: What about if you used a picture to solve this?
difficulty solving the problem using other strategies. S: It still won’t help.
Interestingly, when some students recognized the problem I: Why not?
was difficult to solve, a diagram was not seen as a tool that S: Because I don’t know the problem, so I don’t know
could be used to help even to understand the problem. As how to do a picture.
one student expressed in response to a series of questions I: Can you tell me what you don’t know about the
when he was having difficulty solving a problem: problem? Tell me what you tried here.

123
Challenges students identified with a learning disability and as high-achieving

Fig. 4 Inappropriate diagrams for the problems they are solving

S: I tried counting down to see if I got a low numbers, I: Why do you think a picture won’t work?
and that’s too high to divide it from all 4 friends. So I S: I don’t have the answer to draw the picture.
knew that one wasn’t going to work. Then I tried that
Second, students did not generate an appropriate dia-
one.
gram to solve the problem as they either used the same
I: What this one?
diagram form (e.g., table/chart) to solve all the problems
S: 64 minus 4. And I knew that wasn’t going to work.
they were presented with or resorted to a specific inap-
I: A picture?
propriate form as they considered them easier to use. See
S: A picture isn’t going to work either.
Fig. 4 for examples. In addition, some students tended to
I: Are you seeing something in your mind?
think that specific diagram forms were the only forms to be
S: No.

123
D. van Garderen et al.

Fig. 5 Unable to explain how the diagrams helped solve the problem

used when solving specific types of problems. For example, appears that they understood what they were solving for. In
one student explained: some cases, students got confused within or ‘‘lost’’ when
using the diagram to explain their problem solving process
We learned in third grade that if you have a fraction
as they had difficulty keeping track of what they were
problem, any type of fraction problem, you can draw
solving for or the diagram became too unwieldy as seen in
a pizza and cut it into the amount it was.
the example of the student attempting to solve the Hal-
loween party problem found in Fig. 6. Alternatively, as
5.1.4 Challenges related to adaptive reasoning they used the diagram to explain their problem solving
process, some students overly focused on the diagram and
Two main challenges related to adaptive reasoning emerged. did not connect back to the problem. To illustrate, a student
First, although students used a diagram to solve the problem drew a 30-foot rope and segmented the rope into sections,
whether in a limited or extensive way, many students were but stopped drawing because ‘‘there was no more space in
unable to provide an explanation as to how a diagram was the 30 feet rope’’ and incorrectly stated the answer was 6 in
used to solve the problem even when use of the diagram was the rope problem found in Fig. 6. Finally, some students
perceived as being helpful. Rather, students often referred to used their diagram incorrectly even though they had gen-
the computation they used in the process of solving the erated an appropriate diagram as they reasoned about the
problem. Several examples are provided in Fig. 5. relationships between the concepts and situation while they
Second, students had difficulty using the diagram as a solved the problem. This is seen in the example of the
tool to reason with as they solved the problem even when it student solving the card problem, where the student

123
Challenges students identified with a learning disability and as high-achieving

Fig. 6 Difficulty using a diagram as a tool to reason with as they solved the problem

represents the total number of cards (64) as six groups of do when they were ‘‘little’’ or in elementary school.
ten and four more. Each group of 10 is assigned to a friend Although, in some cases, generating a diagram was not
and 12 (a 10 and 2) are circled. The student then incorrectly necessary as the student could solve the problem accurately
splits the remaining cards among the friends resulting in an and efficiently without it; some students did not think they
incorrect answer. needed to use a diagram at all because a different strategy
(typically computation) was easier to use even when it was
clearly apparent that they did not understand the problem.
5.1.5 Challenges related to productive disposition
Examples of this include:
Within productive disposition, two main challenges S: Because I don’t really use pictures in my math, I
emerged. First, some students perceived diagrams not to be just use pictures to draw what I like to draw. I don’t
useful or worthwhile for solving the problems. Often stu- use pictures in my math.
dents indicated that diagrams were something they used to I: Did you find it helpful to draw the picture?

123
D. van Garderen et al.

Table 2 Percent and number of students with challenges by ability not to a draw a diagram for one or more of the problems
Strand Challenge LD HA %
that they were asked to solve.
% (n) (n)
5.2 Comparison of the students across the challenges
Conceptual Unable to define 37.5 8.3
experienced
(6) (1)
Unable to provide a reason 43.8 0.0
(7) (0) Using the five strands of diagram proficiency, several dif-
Limited definition 62.5 91.7 ferences between the students classified as LD and HA
(10) (11) were identified for each challenge. A summary of these
Limited reason(s) 56.3 100.0 differences for each challenge by ability level can be found
(9) (12) in Table 2. Several interesting findings between the two
Procedural Inaccurate diagram 31.3 0.0 groups deserve to be highlighted.
(5) (0) First, a higher percentage of students with LD than
Strategic Did not use the diagram to 18.8 50.0 students identified as HA were unable to provide a defi-
understand, solve, or monitor while (3) (6)
solving the problem
nition or reason for using a diagram. Notably, unlike the
students with LD, all of the students identified as HA were
Inappropriate diagrams for the 25.0 0.0
problems (4) (0) able to give at least one reason for using a diagram. Sec-
Adaptive Unable to explain, despite perceived 31.3 8.3 ond, no student identified as HA generated an inaccurate
reasoning helpfulness, how the diagram was (5) (1) diagram whereas 31 % of students with LD did. Third, no
used student identified as HA had difficulty generating a dia-
Difficulty using a diagram as a tool 31.3 41.7 gram considered appropriate for the problem situation
to reason with (5) (5)
whereas 25 percent of students with LD did. Interestingly,
Productive Diagram not perceived as useful or 50.0 41.7 however, 50 % of the students identified as HA did not use
disposition worthwhile to use (8) (5)
a diagram in the process of solving a problem as compared
Diagram not used:
to only 18 % of students with LD. Fourth, a higher per-
For any unprompted problems 68.8 50.0
(11) (6) centage of students identified as LD than HA perceived the
When prompted, for at least one 25.0 8.3
diagram as helpful, but had difficulty explaining how a
problem. (4) (1) diagram helped them solve a problem. However, both
groups of students struggled to use a diagram to reason
with as they solved the problem. Finally, within both
S: Not really, because I don’t use pictures and it groups of students, a similar percentage of students did not
doesn’t help me anymore. perceive the diagrams to be worthwhile; however, students
I: What do you mean by it doesn’t help you anymore? with LD were more likely than students identified as HA to
S: A long time ago I used to use pictures because I not use a diagram to solve problems.
didn’t get math as much. But now I’m really good at
it, so I don’t need pictures anymore.
[After incorrectly solving the problem]
I: Can you tell me why you didn’t use one [a 6 Discussion
diagram]?
S: It would be easier to make 30 plus 4, because it According to the Standards for Mathematical Practice in
would be easier instead of making a line that’s 30 feet the Common Core State Standards for Mathematics
long on the piece of paper. (National Governor’s Association, & State Education
I: How did you know what to do here? [Referring to Chiefs, 2010), all students are expected to be able to model
the diagram the student drew] with mathematics using a tool such as a diagram. Specifi-
S: I did it when I was little. cally, it is expected that students
I: But you don’t do that anymore?
… are able to identify important quantities in a
S: No.
practical situation and map their relationships using
Second, students did not self-initiate the strategy of such tools as diagrams …. [Using the model, t]hey
using a diagram when solving a problem. In fact, without can analyze those relationships mathematically to
prompting 17 out of the 28 students did not use a diagram draw conclusions. They routinely interpret their
as a strategy to solve a single problem. Further, even with mathematical results in the context of the situation
prompting to use a diagram, six students (out of 28) chose and reflect on whether the results make sense,

123
Challenges students identified with a learning disability and as high-achieving

possibly improving the model if it has not served its be worthwhile. In some cases, students indicated they did
purpose. (p. 7) not need it as they had already solved the problem using
another strategy, resorted to other ways (e.g., computation)
However, as the findings of this study suggest, not all
to solve the problem, or the problem was too easy and thus
students are necessarily proficient in using a tool such as a
did not warrant a diagram. A productive disposition towards
diagram and may experience any number of challenges
mathematics is critical (NRC, 2001); therefore, if students
when using them. Based on the findings of the study, two
do not see value or use in a strategy, such as a diagram, it
main conclusions can be drawn.
may not be implemented most effectively or students may
First, students of all abilities experienced a range of
not find it useful to spend time learning to or improving on
challenges while using diagrams. The challenges that were
their use of diagrams. Third, a higher percentage of students
identified were varied and of concern. It is clear that not all
identified as HA than students with LD had difficulty using
students were able to identify what a diagram is and how it
a diagram to reason with as they solved the problem. It is
could be of benefit. For a number of students, generating
unclear as to why this may be the case. However, many
and using a diagram as a tool to solve a problem was
students with LD had difficulty within the procedural flu-
difficult. Further, many of the students did not perceive
ency and strategic competency strands that precede adap-
diagrams to be a worthwhile tool to use. Similar to Diez-
tive reasoning and, therefore, more students identified as
mann’s (2000) conclusions, drawing a diagram, despite its
HA got to this stage. It is also possible that reasoning with a
potential, is not necessarily an effective problem-solving
diagram is a difficult process that students may need more
tool for many students. This is of concern as a diagram, as
time and experience to develop.
has been previously noted, can be a powerful tool given its
Although several interesting findings were revealed in
versatility and has been demonstrated to aid in problem
this study, there are a number of limitations that need to be
solving performance.
acknowledged. First, it is possible that there are additional
Second, while both students identified as LD and HA
challenges that students might experience beyond those
experienced challenges using diagrams, it appears that
described in this study. Diezmann (2000) for example,
students with LD are more likely to experience these
notes other challenges specific to using diagrams classified
challenges. For example, more students with LD had dif-
as networks that were not identified here. Clearly, there is a
ficulty defining a diagram and providing reasons for using a
need to verify the current findings as well as look for other
diagram. The findings in this study also suggest that stu-
possible challenges. Second, the data was collected in
dents with LD may experience a greater range of chal-
individual sessions not representative of a typical mathe-
lenges as compared to their HA peers. Various studies have
matics classroom setting. It would be interesting to
suggested that students with LD differ in the quality of how
examine how the students use diagrams when solving
they use their problem solving strategies rather than in the
problems in the classroom as additional challenges may
quantity (e.g., Montague & Applegate, 1993a, 1993b; van
arise that were not found in the current data. Third, no
Garderen et al. 2012). Students with LD in those studies
attempt was made to break down and examine differences
typically use strategies in a less effective and efficient
of diagram use by age (or grade) of the students. Differ-
manner. In this study, this also appears to be the case.
ences in challenges that students experience may vary
Unlike other studies, however, the findings of this study
depending on age. Further research is needed to closely
provide more detail as to what explicit challenges they
examine how use of a diagram may gradually develop over
might experience when using diagrams. Specifically, stu-
time as a problem solving strategy (e.g., idiosyncratic to
dents with LD are more likely to have difficulty generating
conventional application) along with how tasks may impact
a diagram that accurately represents the information pre-
that development (e.g., learning trajectory).
sented in a problem and frequently struggle to generate an
appropriate diagram for a given problem.
There were three interesting exceptions to the second
main finding that should be noted. First, a higher percentage 7 Implications for practice
of HA students than students with LD did not consistently
utilize a diagram as a strategy to solve a problem. However, A highly recommended strategy to use in mathematics
a number of those students also indicated that they did not instruction for both struggling learners and students with
need to use the diagram, as they, in most cases, correctly disabilities is the use of visual representations such as a
solved the problem. This is not surprising, as a problem diagram (Gersten et al. 2009). Although this is preferable,
situation must warrant (e.g., be of sufficient challenge) the for some students, in particular students with LD, it needs
use of a given strategy. Second, a high percentage of stu- to be recognized that they may lack knowledge about
dents identified as LD and HA did not perceive diagrams to diagrams, one visual representation form, which may

123
D. van Garderen et al.

interfere with their development and understanding of key Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research.
mathematical ideas and problem solving performance. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Diezmann, C. (1999). Assessing diagram quality: Making a difference
Therefore, as Rubenstein and Thompson (2012) note, to representation. In Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Conference
‘‘When we acknowledge that reading [and, we contend, of Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia, (pp.
using] charts, diagrams, graphs, and other visual repre- 185–191), Adelaide, Australia.
sentations may be less transparent to students than to us as Diezmann, C. M. (2000). The difficulties students experience in
generating diagrams for novel problems. In T. Nakahara & M.
teachers, we can begin to identify and adopt strategies to Koyama (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th annual conference of the
support students’ ‘‘reading’’ [and ‘‘use’’] of those displays’’ international group for the psychology of mathematics education
(p. 550). (pp. 241–248). Japan: Hiroshima.
While many students, given sufficient time and oppor- Diezmann, C. M., & English, L. D. (2001). Promoting the use of
diagrams as tools for thinking. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio
tunities, develop diagram proficiency, for some students (Eds.), The roles of representation in school mathematics: 2001
additional targeted, explicit instruction for developing yearbook (pp. 77–89). Reston: National Council of Teachers of
diagram proficiency may be necessary. In this study, we Mathematics.
identified nine possible challenges that students may diSessa, A. A. (2002). Students’ criteria for representational ade-
quacy. Mathematics Education Library, 30, 105–130.
experience that could be areas for instruction. For example, diSessa, A. A., & Sherin, B. L. (2000). Meta-representation: An
instruction may be needed to help develop a clear under- introduction. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 19, 385–398.
standing of what a diagram is and how a diagram may be Dufour-Janvier, B., Bednarz, N., & Belanger, M. (1987). Pedagogical
used to solve mathematics problems. As Diezmann (2000) considerations concerning the problem of representation. In C.
Janvier (Ed.), Problems of representation in the teaching and
poignantly notes, ‘‘Advocating that students draw a dia- learning of mathematics (pp. 109–122). Hillsdale: Lawrence
gram without addressing their difficulties and educating Erlbaum Associates.
them about diagrams is quite simply the waste of a very Fennema, E., Franke, M. L., Carpenter, T. P., & Carey, D. A. (1993).
good tool!’’ (p. 247). Using children’s mathematical knowledge in instruction. Amer-
ican Educational Research Journal, 30(3), 555–583.
Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young mathematicians at work:
Constructing number sense, addition, and subtraction. Westport:
Appendix A Heinemann.
Gersten, R., Beckmann, S., Clarke, B., Foegen, A., Marsh, L., Star, J.
R., & Witzel, D. (2009). Assisting students struggling with
Learning disabilities: mathematics: Response to intervention for elementary and
middle schools (NCEE2009-4060). Washington, DC: National
1. a disorder in one or more of the basic psychological
Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance,
processes, including visual, auditory, or language Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
processes; http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/publications/practiceguides/.
2. academic achievement significantly below the stu- Goldin, G. A. (2002). Representation in mathematical learning and
problem solving. In L. English (Ed.), Handbook of international
dent’s level of intellectual functioning;
research in mathematics education (pp. 197–218). Mahwah:
3. learning problems that are not due primarily to other Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
disabilities; and Hegarty, M., & Kozhevnikov, M. (1999). Types of visual-spatial
4. ineffectiveness of general education alternatives in representations and mathematical problem solving. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 91, 684–689. doi:0022-0663/99/S3.00.
meeting the student’s educational needs.
Hiebert, J., Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Fuson, K. C., Wearne, D.,
Murray, H., et al. (1997). Making sense: Teaching and learning
mathematics with understanding. Portsmouth: Heinemann.
Ives, B., & Hoy, C. (2003). Graphic organizers applied to higher-level
References secondary mathematics. Learning Disabilities Research &
Practice, 18(1), 36–51.
Ashlock, R. B. (2006). Error patterns in computation: Using error Jitendra, A., DiPipi, C. M., & Perron-Jones, N. (2002). An explor-
patterns to improve instruction (9th ed.). Upper Saddle River: atory study of schema-based word-problem-solving instruction
Pearson education, Inc. for middle school students with learning disabilities: An
Brown, D. L., & Presmeg, N. C. (1993). Types of imagery used by emphasis on conceptual and procedural understanding. The
elementary and secondary school students in mathematical Journal of Special Education, 36, 23–38.
reasoning. In Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting of the Jitendra, A. K., Rodriguez, M., Kanive, R., Huang, J., Church, C.,
International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Educa- Corroy, K. A., et al. (2013). Impact of small-group tutoring
tion (Vol. II, pp. 137–144), Tsukuba. interventions on the mathematical problem solving achievement
Butler, F. M., Miller, S. P., Crehan, K., Babbitt, B., & Pierce, T. of third-grade students with mathematical difficulties. Learning
(2003). Fraction instruction for students with mathematics Disability Quarterly, 36(1), 21–35. doi:10.1177/073194871
disabilities: Comparing two teaching sequences. Learning Dis- 2457561.
abilities Research and Practice, 18, 99–111. Kaput, J. J. (1987). Representation systems and mathematics. In C.
Connolly, A. J. (2007). KeyMath-3 diagnostic assessment. San Janvier (Ed.), Problems of representation in the teaching and
Antonio: Pearson. learning of mathematics (pp. 19–26). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

123
Challenges students identified with a learning disability and as high-achieving

Knight, J. (2002). Crossing boundaries: What constructivists can Pressley, M., Harris, K. R., & Marks, M. B. (1992). But good strategy
teach intensive-explicit instructors and vice versa. Focus on instructors are constructivists! Educational Psychology Review,
Exceptional Children, 35(4), 1–16. 4(1), 3–31.
Kulm, G. (1994). Mathematics assessment: What works in the Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001).
classroom. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in
Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of Educational
worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99. doi:10. Psychology, 93(2), 346–362.
1016/S0364-0213(87)80026-5. Rubenstein, R., & Thompson, D. (2012). Reading visual representa-
Lesh, R. (1999). The development of representational abilities in tions. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 17(9),
middle school mathematics. In Irving E. Sigel (Ed.), Develop- 544–550.
ment of mental representation: Theories and application (pp. Schwartz, D. L., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future
323–349). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student
Publishers. production in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction,
Lesh, R., Behr, M., & Post, T. (1987). Rational number relations and 22(2), 303–317.
proportions. In C. Janvier (Ed.), Problems of representation in Shigematsu, K., & Sowder, L. (1994). Drawings for story problems:
the teaching and learning of mathematics (pp. 41–58). Hillsdale: Practices in Japan and the United States. Arithmetic Teachers,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 41, 544–547.
Maccini, P., & Hughes, C. A. (2000). Effects of a problem-solving Smith, S. P. (2003). Representation in school mathematics: Children’s
strategy on the introductory algebra performance of secondary representations of problems. In J. Kilpartrick, W. G. Martin, &
students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to principles and
Research and Practice, 15, 10–21. standards for school mathematics (pp. 263–274). Reston:
Mayer, R. (1989). Systemic thinking fostered by illustrations in National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
scientific text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), Stylianou, D. (2002). On the interaction of visualization and
240–246. analysis—The negotiation of a visual representation in problem
Mayer, R., & Gallini, J. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten solving. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 21(3), 303–317.
thousand words? Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), Stylianou, D. (2010). Teachers’ conceptions of representation in
715–726. middle school mathematics. Journal of Mathematics Teacher
Montague, M., & Applegate, B. (1993a). Mathematical problem- Education, 13(4), 325–434. doi:10.1007/s10857-0109143-y.
solving characteristics of middle school students with learning Uesaka, Y., Manalo, E., & Ichikawa, S. (2007). What kinds of
disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 27, 175–201. perceptions and daily learning behaviors promote students’ use
doi:10.1177/002246699302700203. of diagrams in mathematics problem solving? Learning and
Montague, M., & Applegate, B. (1993b). Middle school students’ Instruction, 17(322), 335. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.02.
mathematical problem solving: An analysis of think-aloud 006.
protocols. Learning Disability Quarterly, 16, 19–32. doi:10. van Garderen, D. (2006). Spatial visualization, visual imagery, and
2307/1511157. mathematical problem solving of students with varying abilities.
National Governor’s Association, & State Education Chiefs. (2010). Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(6), 496–506.
Common core state standards for mathematics. http://www. van Garderen, D. (2007). Teaching students with LD to use diagrams
corestandards.org/. to solve mathematical word problems. Journal of Learning
National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up: Helping children Disabilities, 40(6), 540–553. doi:10.1177/00222194070400060501.
learn mathematics. J. Kilpatrick, J. Swafford, & B. Findell van Garderen, D., & Montague, M. (2003). Visual-spatial represen-
(Eds.), Mathematics Learning Study Committee, Center for tation, mathematical problem solving, and students of varying
Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and abilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 18,
Education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 246–254. doi:10.1111/1540-5826.00079.
Novick, L. R., Hurley, S. M., & Francis, M. (1999). Evidence for van Garderen, D., Scheuermann, A., & Jackson, C. (2012). Examining
abstract, schematic knowledge of three spatial diagram repre- how students with diverse abilities use diagrams to solve
sentations. Memory & Cognition, 27, 288–308. doi:10.3758/ mathematics word problem. Learning Disability Quarterly,.
BF03211413. doi:10.1177/0731948712438558.
Owens, K. D., & Clements, M. A. K. (1998). Representations in Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th
spatial problem solving in the classroom. Journal of Mathemat- ed.). San Antonio: PsychCorp, Pearson Assessment.
ical Behavior, 17, 197–218. Witzel, B. S., Mercer, C. D., & Miller, M. D. (2003). Teaching
Pape, S. J., & Tchoshanov, M. A. (2001). The role of representa- algebra to students with learning difficulties: An investigation of
tion(s) in developing mathematical understanding. Theory into an explicit instruction model. Learning Disabilities Research
Practice, 40, 118–127. doi:10.1207/s15430421tip4002_6. and Practice, 18(2), 121–131.
Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods. Zahner, D., & Corter, J. E. (2010). The process of probability problem
Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. solving: Use of external visual representations. Mathematical
Polya, G. (1957). How to solve it: A new aspect of mathematical Thinking and Learning, 12, 177–204. doi:10.1080/109860
method (2nd ed.). Garden City: Doubleday & Company, Inc. 61003654240.
Presmeg, N. C. (1986a). Visualization and mathematical giftedness. Zawojewski, J. S., & Lesh, R. (2003). A models and modeling
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 17, 297–311. perspective on problem solving. In R. Lesh & H. M. Doerr
Presmeg, N. C. (1986b). Visualization in high school mathematics. (Eds.), Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling perspec-
For the Learning of Mathematics, 6, 42–46. doi:10.1007/ tives on mathematics problem solving (pp. 317–328). Mahwah:
BF00305075. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

123

You might also like