You are on page 1of 5

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/52009574

Complex Problem Solving

Chapter · January 2012


DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_685

CITATIONS READS

58 10,380

1 author:

Joachim Funke
Universität Heidelberg
288 PUBLICATIONS   4,842 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Validity of Minimal Complex Systems View project

Dorsch - Lexikon der Psychologie View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Joachim Funke on 21 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


682 C Complex Problem Solving

to help learners deal with the complexity of tasks, Reiser, B. J. (2004). Scaffolding complex learning: The mechanisms of
that is, to provide supports that enable them to deal structuring and problematizing student work. Journal of the
Learning Sciences, 13(3), 273–304.
with more complex content and skill demands than
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2007). Alternate models of instructional
they could otherwise handle. Moreover, provided design: Holistic design approaches and complex learning. In
guidance and support should gradually decrease in R. A. Reiser & J. V. Dempsey (Eds.), Trends and issues in instruc-
a process of “scaffolding,” as learners gain more exper- tional design and technology (pp. 72–81). Upper Saddle River:
tise (e.g., Reiser 2004). ▶ Cognitive load theory (van Pearson/Merrill Prentice Hall.
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Kirschner, P. A. (2007). Ten steps to
Merriënboer and Sweller 2005) explicitly studies
complex learning. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis.
methods that might help to reduce the high cognitive Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory
load that is imposed by rich learning tasks. Van and complex learning: recent developments and future direc-
Merriënboer et al. (2003), for example, describe on tions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177.
the basis of ▶ four-component instructional design Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking
methods that might help reduce high cognitive load: the load of a learners’ mind: instructional design for complex
learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 5–13.
(a) simple-to-complex sequencing of classes of equally
difficult whole tasks, (b) working from worked exam-
ples to conventional problems, (c) just-in-time presen-
tation of helpful information, and (d) provision of
part-task practice for routine aspects of tasks. Complex Problem Solving
With regard to learning outcomes, complex learn-
ing explicitly aims at ▶ transfer of learning, that is, the JOACHIM FUNKE
ability to apply what has been learned to unfamiliar Department of Psychology, Heidelberg University,
problems and/or in new situations. The main assump- Heidelberg, Germany
tion is that complex learning yields a highly integra-
ted knowledge base, organized in cognitive schemas,
which facilitates transfer (Gagné and Merrill 1990). Synonyms
On the one hand, particular types of learning tasks Dealing with uncertainty; Dynamic decision making;
(e.g., goal-free problems, worked examples, comple- Problem solving in dynamic microworlds
tion tasks), which are carefully tuned to the current
level of expertise of learners, contribute to the devel- Definition
opment of an integrated knowledge base and subse- Complex problem solving takes place for reducing the
quent transfer performance; on the other hand, barrier between a given start state and an intended goal
▶ variability of practice should ensure that the whole state with the help of cognitive activities and behavior.
set of learning tasks varies on all dimensions on which Start state, intended goal state, and barriers prove
tasks also differ from each other in the real world, complexity, change dynamically over time, and can be
including surface features and structural features, to partially intransparent. In contrast to solving simple
reach transfer (for an overview, see van Merriënboer problems, with complex problems at the beginning of
and Sweller 2005). a problem solution the exact features of the start state,
of the intended goal state, and of the barriers are
Cross-References unknown. Complex problem solving expects the effi-
▶ Cognitive Load Theory cient interaction between the problem-solving person
▶ Four-Component Instructional Design and situational conditions that depend on the task.
▶ Transfer of Learning It demands the use of cognitive, emotional, and
▶ Variability of Practice social resources as well as knowledge (see Frensch and
Funke 1995).
References
Gagné, R. M., & Merrill, M. D. (1990). Integrative goals for instruc- Theoretical Background
tional design. Educational Technology Research and Development, Since 1975 there has been started a new movement in
38(1), 23–30. the psychology of thinking that is engaged in complex
Complex Problem Solving C 683

problems in contrast to simple problems. Essential more reliable assertions. Complexity demands
impulses for this development came from external, from the problem solver a simplification through
shocking events like the oil crisis or the first analyses reduction to the essential.
of the “Club of Rome” at that time, which showed the 2. Connectivity between involved variables. Needless to
constraints of growth and which made humanity- say, it is not the pure number of variables that is C
threatening problem fields visible. Besides that, the decisive for the workload on the problem-solving
dissatisfaction about the nonpredictability of relevant person, but the connectivity between these. Assum-
characteristics like professional, economical, or politi- ing that in a system of 100 variables every variable is
cal success based on classical intelligence tests led to connected to only exactly one other, the connectiv-
a search of alternative measurements for the assessment ity is lower than in a system in which all variables
of the way humans deal with complex situations, are connected to each other. For making mutual
a search for “operative intelligence,” as it was coined dependencies understandable, a model of the con-
by Dietrich Dörner. nectivity is required from the problem solver.
As an alternative, the use of computer-simulated 3. Dynamics of the situation. This feature explains the
scenarios was proposed. Such “microworlds” allow fact that interventions into a complex, networked
experimental research of complex problems under con- system might activate processes whose impact was
trolled conditions (Brehmer and Dörner 1993). For possibly not intended. A unique variant is the own
example, the scenario “Lohhausen” (Dörner 1997) (intern) dynamic (“eigen-dynamics”). It signifies
simulated the events in a fictitious village. The subject that in a lot of cases the problem does not wait for
had to act as the mayor of a small city for simulated the problem-solving person and his/her decisions,
10 years (essentially reduced to nearly 10 h of gaming but the situation changes itself over time. Dynamic
time) and had to care about the well-being of requires from the problem solver the consideration
the community and its financial wealth. For this task, of the factor “time.”
the fictitious mayor could control the events and shape 4. Intransparency concerning the variables involved
the town according to her or his visions. Based on the and concerning the definition of the goal. In an
data from successful and less successful subjects in this intransparent situation, not all required informa-
scenario, interesting hypotheses about the conditions tion about variables and possible goals are given.
of success and failure in dealing with uncertainty and Intransparency requires from the problem solver
complexity have been formulated. the active acquisition of information.
Since that early start of this research program with 5. Polytely. In a complex situation, reaching goals can
“Lohhausen” in the mid-1970s, numerous scenarios be complicated. Usually there is more than one
with varying extent and from different domains (e.g., goal in a complex situation that has to be consid-
economy, ecology, policy, technology) have been ered. Conflicts due to antagonistic goals require
developed and applied in both basic and applied the forming of compromises and the definition
research. In the following sections, I will outline char- of priorities.
acteristics of complex problems, describe tendencies in
Two approaches concerning research with complex
research, illustrate empirical results, and discuss prob-
problems differentiate with respect to procedures and
lems and perspectives of this approach.
to goals:
Characteristics of complex problems considerably
differ from requirements of simple problems. Five ● The experimental approach: “Systematic manip-
features have been differentiated traditionally (Funke ulation of scenarios.” Essential features of this
2003): approach are the experimental manipulation of
the stimuli (the complex systems) and its condition
1. Complexity of the problem situation. Traditionally, of presentation. Particularly the systematic manip-
complexity is defined based on the number of ulation of scenarios (or system features) became
variables in the given system. Surely, this is only a characteristic of this approach: degree of connec-
a first orientation for the estimation of problem tivity, presence or absence of eigen-dynamics, or the
difficulty, but additional characteristics permit degree of time delays show influences on knowledge
684 C Complex Problem Solving

acquisition (= identification of systems) and With respect to Situational Aspects, according to


knowledge application (= control of systems). early studies, transparency of a system leads to easier
● The correlational approach: “Search for information processing and increasing efficacy of
interindividual differences.” Essential features of intelligence concerning the success of problem solving.
this approach are the search for interindividual However, this moderator function of transparency is
differences and the search for correlations of suc- questioned repeatedly by current research.
cess and failure. Systems attributes were kept con- Passive observing of a system or active intervention
stant to see the space of behavioral possibilities. are two situational requirements, which lead to differ-
Additionally, individual trajectories through com- ent acquirements. While pure observing delivers
plex systems were analyzed and correlated with structural knowledge about the problematic system,
constructs like test intelligence, personality char- control knowledge arises out of intervention condi-
acteristics, and so on. tions (Osman 2010). An increase in training also leads
to improvement under complex conditions. However,
Important Scientific Research and there are certain conditions (e.g., existence of time
Open Questions delays), which do not profit from it.
Many empirical results for solving complex problems The semantic appearance of a system is very
are reviewed by Funke (2003) in detail. Here, only important, since several prior knowledge structures
selected but important results are presented. They are are activated and can be used. However, prior knowl-
ordered by their focus. edge is not always beneficial, especially if activated
With respect to Personality Aspects, general intelli- prior knowledge fitting only on the surface does not
gence measured by tests seemed to be an inappropriate correspond to deeper structures.
predictor for handling complex problems according With respect to System Aspects, the type of feedback
to previous research. However, by today’s knowledge is important for the success in solving the problem.
it seems clear that specific components of intelli- Generally one can say: the more indirect and delayed
gence (like processing capacity) are predictive for the a feedback for a certain condition of the system, the
successful handling of complex problems (Wenke more difficult the controlled intervention. Formal fea-
et al. 2005). Besides that, there are several forms of tures of systems also have proven their influential status
knowledge (e.g., system knowledge, control knowl- concerning identification (knowledge acquisition) and
edge, strategic knowledge) that have to be taken controlling (knowledge implementation) within the
into account. process of complex problem solving (for a review, see
The role of motivational parameters becomes Osman 2010).
apparent in the fact that problems which are considered Problems within complex problem-solving research
as more important get more attention (e.g., the differ- deal with the following issues:
ent handling of a simulated epidemic situation based
on deadly smallpox or innocuous influenza). As a con- ● Identifying the quality of solution. A decision about
sequence, there are changes in strategies of information the quality of simple problem solving is easily
processing. If really high-stake problems are dealt with, possible, because the criteria for success are
the search for risk-defusing operators increases. transparent. For complex problems the situation is
Emotional effects find expression, for example, in different, because mostly there are no obvious goal
“emergency reactions” of the cognitive system. After conditions. A one-dimensional evaluation is not
perceived failure of problem solving a decrease in possible in that case. Problems arise if success of
intellectual level follows, which is accompanied by handling complex problems is used for diagnostic
a tendency for fast acting and for degenerated hypothesis statements about the acting person.
generation. Also, the emotion regulation during com- ● Context effects. One of the most impressive abilities
plex problem solving plays an important role. Experi- of human cognition is its enormous context sensi-
ments showed that complex problem-solving situations tivity. Structural similar tasks are treated differently
with negative feedback of results lead to a higher infor- in different semantic contexts. Different contexts
mation retrieval and to a better performance. also become apparent in processing the same
Complex Problem Solving C 685

requirements in different cultures. Cultural com- tasks set by the different scenarios. Thereby, one
parison does not mean changing between nations would get from blanket description to precise testi-
or continents, but could happen simply on the level monies. Scenarios have to be analyzed in form and
of “subcultures.” Assessing how variations in con- content. It has to be explained properly what is
text lead to variation in strategies and subjectively measured. C
constructed problem spaces within the process of ● Characteristics of the problem-solving process. Once
problem solving might be an important task of the requirements are known, cognitive processes
future research. within the acting person can be focused in detail.
● Training and the question of domain specificity or Particularly the differentiation between implicit
generalizability. The question of domain specificity and explicit processes and their relation to the dis-
of problem-solving activities is closely related to the tinction between novice and expert problem solving
issue of context sensitivity. In case of research in could be of peculiar interest. Based on this research,
complex problem solving, the question is one of training procedures could be designed. Existing
transfer of knowledge and strategies between spe- dynamic scenarios contributed to this purpose
cific scenarios. It is generally accepted that confron- already because of their differentiation between
tation with different scenarios leads to an extension different forms of knowledge, of strategies, and of
of the realm of experience – however, there are metacognition.
no empirical evidences. The simple repetition ● Heuristics. It seems promising to transfer our
of processing the same scenario leads to learning knowledge about heuristics found in research on
effects, but training itself means more: the acquisi- decision making to the field of complex problem
tion of strategic competences universally applicable. solving. Possibly simple heuristics control the
Finding rules for unpredictable situations could be processing of complex problems, an idea which
the squaring of a circle. Concerning application would be helpful for finding a global theory.
aspects, there is a huge challenge of psychological
research in problem solving. Cross-References
● Missing theory. The major problem of current ▶ Complex Problem Solving
research is the lack of a firm theory about dealing ▶ Learning and Thinking
with complex problems. It is not even clear if there ▶ Problem Solving
is a need for another theory besides a theory for ▶ Simulation and Learning: The Role of Mental
solving simple problems. Indeed a global theory of Models
cognition that describes and explains dealing with ▶ Simulation-Based Learning
all forms of problems is needed. But such a “unified
theory of cognition” (Alan Newell) does not seem
References
Brehmer, B., & Dörner, D. (1993). Experiments with computer-
to appear on the horizon.
simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the
Perspectives. Within the major area called “psychol- laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study. Computers in
Human Behavior, 9, 171–184.
ogy of thinking and reasoning,” the exploration of
Dörner, D. (1997). The logic of failure. Recognizing and avoiding error
complex problems represents a question that is of in complex situations. New York: Basic Books.
great significance beyond our discipline. Thereby, a Frensch, P. A., & Funke, J. (Eds.). (1995). Complex problem solving:
chance appears to devote psychology on a basis of The European perspective. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum
verified findings to a field of application within areas Associates.
like politics and business consulting (“give psychology Funke, J. (2003). Problemlösendes Denken. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.
Osman, M. (2010). Controlling uncertainty: A review of human
a-way”). For this reason, more intensive data pooling
behavior in complex dynamic environments. Psychological Bul-
and the refinement of appropriate theoretical approaches letin, 136, 65–86.
are needed. Interesting developments could be expected Wenke, D., Frensch, P. A., & Funke, J. (2005). Complex problem
in following areas: solving and intelligence: Empirical relation and causal direction.
In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Pretz (Eds.), Cognition and intelligence:
● Task and requirement analysis. It seems profitable to Identifying the mechanisms of the mind (pp. 160–187). New York:
undergo an analysis of requirements concerning the Cambridge University Press.

View publication stats

You might also like