You are on page 1of 19

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/236342821

The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

Article  in  Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange · June 2012


DOI: 10.18785/jetde.0501.06

CITATIONS READS

5 165

3 authors, including:

Lin Lin Lipsmeyer Jennifer Lee


Southern Methodist University University of North Texas
97 PUBLICATIONS   1,640 CITATIONS    16 PUBLICATIONS   220 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Examing the experience of being an educator View project

Impact of technology upon society View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Lin Lin Lipsmeyer on 30 November 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Lin, L., Robertson, A., & Lee, J. (2012). The impact of outcome intentions on reading and multitasking
performances. Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange, 5(1), 77-94.

The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and


Multitasking Performances

Lin Lin
University of North Texas

Adrian Robertson
Montana State University Billings

Jennifer Lee
University of North Texas

Abstract: This study investigated undergraduate students’ reading comprehension between


two outcome intentions and three media multitasking conditions. The two outcome intentions
were for accuracy and completion. The three multitasking conditions included silence, video
background, and video test conditions. One hundred thirty university students participated in
the study. Every participant completed two sets of reading, with two different intentions and in
two different conditions. Results showed that the participants performed better in reading (a)
when they strived for completion than for accuracy, (b) that the addition of an unobtrusive video
(video background) did not inhibit the processing of the primary reading task, and (c) that those
who strived for task accuracy might have actually benefited from the addition of the background
video. Implications of the results on multimedia design and student assessment are discussed.

Keywords: cognitive load, multitasking, outcome intention, multimedia learning

1. Introduction want to see greater focus applied to the most


important tasks at hand (Jackson, 2008). If our
We see students do it all the time: studying students are wavering in their attention while
while watching television or switching driving or are missing important information
between multiple programs on a computer. while studying, most would agree this presents
When asked how they accomplish what they a problem.
are doing, the typical response is some version
of “I multitask!” The propensity our students Whether one defines multitasking as the
have for juggling multiple lines of input has no splitting of attention or as the rapid switching
doubt been met with skepticism by those who of attention between tasks (Baddeley,

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 77


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

Chincotta, & Adam, 2001; Burgess, Veitch, 2. Theoretical Framework


de Lacy Costello, & Shallice, 2000; Monsell
& Driver, 2000), those who multitask are In this study, Cognitive Load Theory,
essentially trying to perform more task(s) Dual-Coding Theory, dual task, task switching,
over a shorter period of time. Usually one and Pareto Principle were referenced to help
of the tasks is considered more important: understand if students would perform reading
when watching television while studying, comprehension differently when instructed
for instance, gaining an understanding of the to apply cognitive resources with different
materials being studied would be the primary intentions and under different multitasking
focus and all other tasks would be secondary. conditions. Each is briefly described below.
Whatever the overall tasks-performance
outcome may be on all tasks combined, the Cognitive Load Theory (Sweller, 1988)
researchers in this study are concerned with suggests that an individual carry three forms
the outcome pertaining specifically to the cognitive load while processing resources
primary task at hand. Even if multitasking is during a learning process. The first is
successful in that more tasks are accomplished intrinsic load that is imposed by the nature
in less time, what happens to the primary and difficulty level of the material being
task? Even if there is an overall gain, what is presented. The second is extraneous load that
being lost on the most important task of the is imposed by the instructional methods and
moment? Answers to these questions have materials used. The third is germane load or
potentials to provide insights in important the mental process of taking new information
educational research and practices. The most and integrating it with old information so that
obvious is when a studying task is paired with learning occurs. The addition of intrinsic,
a competing task such as watching television. extraneous, and germane loads equals a
We need to understand whether a student learner’s Total Cognitive Load (TCL). This
is receiving sufficient information from the raises two primary arguments: (1) extraneous
materials being studied and to what extent load must be minimized to maximize the
they are mastering the materials. Another cognitive resources available to process the
situation is in the design of multimedia intrinsic and germane loads and (2) TCL
educational systems such as multi-user virtual cannot exceed the cognitive processing
environments (MUVEs) or computer-based resources of the learner, or the learner shuts
learning environments. We must consider the down under excessive load (Kirschner, 2002).
warnings raised by the rich media paradox
(Mayer & Clark, 2007) or the inclusion of Dual-Coding Theory assumes that
extraneous material (Nelson & Erlandson, “humans possess separate information
2008), and avoid distracting students from processing channels for visually represented
their primary learning tasks. A third situation material and auditorily represented material”
is in the assessment of student results, (Mayer, 2001, p. 46). While encountering a
particularly where there is a distinction new learning material, the learner has two
between gaining general knowledge versus primary channels of input available (Paivio,
gaining in-depth knowledge. We need to 1986). One is a verbal channel through which
ensure that the measurement of the outcomes the learner processes words while the other
in multimedia situations takes into account is an imagery channel through which the
the student’s learning outcome goals. Each of learner processes images. Multimedia design
these issues will be addressed in this paper. principles are intended to maximize the

78 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

flow of information through both channels is compelled to restart and refocus (Meyer &
in a manner that does not overload either Kieras, 1997). According to Just, Keller, and
one, or cause competition or redundancy Cynkar (2008), each time when one has this
between the information being processed, or alternation, there is a period in which one will
require excessive distance in terms of time make no progress on either task. However,
or space between the information processed neurological work over the last few decades
(Mayer & Moreno, 2003). Thus, good design also shows that our brains may be adapting to
minimizes extraneous cognitive load by the demands of simultaneous tasks (Diamond,
filling both channels with complementary 2002). Small and Vorgan (2009) reported that
information without redundancy, confusion, Internet use and web-browsing have evident
or extra burden on working memory (Miller, effects on our brains, which are much more
1956; Sweller, 1988). Poor design results changeable than most of us think, especially
in excessive extraneous processing and in the case of young people. Carr (2010)
leaves little room for the germane processing stated that our brains change in response to
required for learning. Mayer and colleagues our experiences and that the technologies we
categorized five types of cognitive overload use to find, store, and share information can
scenarios that would apply to a multimedia literally reroute our neural pathways. Other
learning situation (Mayer & Moreno, 2003). studies have also suggested that practices and
First, the visual channel is overloaded with training may increase brain processing speed,
essential information. Second, both the improve working memory, and improve our
visual and verbal channels are overloaded ability to multitask (Jaeggi, Buschkuehl,
by essential information. Third, one or both Jonides, & Perrig, 2008; Ruthruff, Van,
channels are overloaded by a mix of essential Johnston, & Remington, 2006). Some scholars
and incidental information. Fourth, one or both believe that digital technologies have changed
channels are loaded with essential information the way we retain and process information
that is presented in a confusing manner. Fifth, (Carr, 2010; Gee, 2003; Prensky, 2001).
one or both channels are loaded with essential
information that requires additional temporal This study was conducted under
storage and processing. It is important to this context. The researchers believe it is
examine these cognitive overload scenarios in important to understand what is involved
various levels of multitasking situations. and what is taking place when young people
conduct several tasks at the same time, and
Most studies focusing on multitasking to what extent their activities are beneficial
show that one’s ability to multitask is rather or detrimental to their learning. Because
limited (Lang, 2001). Further, multitasking our purpose was to investigate the cognitive
o v e r d i ff e r e n t t y p e s o f t a s k s r e d u c e s processing resources the young people applied
productivity (Just, Keller, & Cynkar, 2008) to the primary task, we set out to examine
and that one’s ability to perform concurrent how intentions affect reading performance in
mental operations is limited by the capacity of multitasking conditions. The Pareto Principle
the brain’s central mechanism (Schweickert & shed lights on how outcome intentions might
Boggs, 1984). Studies also find that it is more affect reading performance in a multitasking or
difficult to learn new things when one’s brain multimedia learning environment. The Pareto
is distracted by another activity (Poldrack & Principle was originally derived from Vilfredo
Foerde, 2007). Scholars believe that switching Pareto’s early-20th-century observation
between tasks wastes time because the brain that 80% of Italy’s wealth belonged to only

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 79


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

20% of the population (Rushton, Oxlet, & • What might be occurring with regards to
Croucher, 2000). This principle has later the attentive resources of the participants
been more broadly used in various fields when they multitask with different
including economics and systems science to outcome intentions?
describe that, for many events, 80% of the
effects are derived from 20% of the causes • If learning outcome intention affects
(Wilson, 1972). In terms of cognitive resource one’s performance, what does this mean
allocation, it is likely that people apply a small for student assessment in a multimedia
amount of attention to tasks where less-than- learning environment?
perfect outcomes are acceptable; expecting
that roughly 20% of their allocated attention The purpose of this study, therefore, was
will result in 80% of the total possible result. to understand what changes would occur in
In other words, if perfection is not required students’ focus and information acquisition
on the main task, a multitasking student might in Multitasking Conditions when they had
apply less attention to that task, and therefore, different learning Outcome Intentions.
have more attention available to apply to other
tasks than if a perfect outcome was required. 3. Method
Thus, it was expected that outcome intention
would have an effect on the participants’ 3.1. Participants
processing resource allocation. Johnson
Undergraduate students from eight classes
(2005) described it vividly as follows: “It
of the same course at a research university
usually involves skimming the surface of the
were invited to participate in this study during
incoming data, picking out the relevant details,
their regular class hours. The course was titled
and moving on to the next stream. You’re
“Computers in the Classrooms.” Students
paying attention, but only partially. That lets
were given extra credit for participating in
you cast a wider net, but it also runs the risk
the study. One hundred thirty-seven students
of keeping you from really studying the fish”
participated, but seven responses were
(p. 61). In addition to studying the effects of
determined to be unusable (2.5 standard
multitasking situations on the primary activity,
deviations or more below the mean number of
the researchers wanted to investigate whether
correct scores, with most questions simply left
there are differences in the attentive allocation
unanswered). This left 130 usable participant
of participants, under various multitasking
data-sets. The average age for the group
conditions, between those who were “casting
was 23.9 years. Female students accounted
a wider net” versus those who “really
for 90.7% of the participants. Male students
studied the fish.” The following are research
accounted for the other 9.3%. The gender
questions:
disparity was largely due to the fact that
• Does reading comprehension performance female students outnumbered male students in
differ when people intend to complete the the College of Education.
reading task as quickly and as completely
as possible (intention for completion) 3.2. Design and Procedure
as compared to doing it as accurately
Two sets of reading materials were
as possible (intention for accuracy) in
developed. Each set contained three articles:
different multitasking conditions?
one article in science, one in history, and one

80 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

in politics. All of the articles were similar in condition. Because two out of three conditions
reading level, length, and format. Articles were involved videos, every participant watched at
selected based on the 8 th grade vocabulary least one video. When the video was played,
level according to the "Flesch–Kincaid Grade the participants were also asked to complete
Level Formula" (Kincaid, Fishburne, Rogers, the six questions related to that video. The
& Chissom, 1975). sequence of the conditions and the sequence
of the videos used were alternated and
Each article contained six multiple- balanced in the eight classes. For instance,
choice questions. A total of six articles with 36 if the previous class participated in the
questions were used to assess the participants’ silence condition during the first 16 minutes
reading comprehension for the study. The and the video background condition during
two sets of articles were equivalent in the the second 16 minutes, then the following
numbers of words, with 2,478 words in one participating class would participate in the
set and 2,439 words in the other set. Each set video background condition during the first
of articles was printed such that it contained 16 minutes and the silence condition during
the first article, questions for the first article, the second 16 minutes, and so forth. The
the second article, questions for the second same logic applied to the sequence of the
article, the third article, and questions for the videos used. If the previous class used the
third article. Within each set, the sequence of documentary video at the Video Background
the three articles was rotated during the data condition during the first 16 minutes of
collection process so that no particular article reading, then the following class would use the
was given priority. documentary video at the Video Background
condition during the second 16 minutes
In addition, two videos, each 16 minutes of reading. The purpose of alternating and
in length were developed. One video was a balancing the sequences of the conditions and
documentary on drunk-driving (Documentary), videos used was to prevent the sequence of
and another was a situational comedy conditions or videos from affecting the results
(Sitcom). Six questions accompanied each of the experiment.
video to measure the participants’ video
comprehension. The video comprehension In addition, the time was purposely set as
tests were used in the video-background and limited; the participants had to complete each
video-test conditions. Finally, a survey was set of reading materials in 16 minutes, with
administered to gather data on demographic or without a video. The participants were
information and the participants’ attention also instructed to finish one set of reading
allocations. The participants were tested on and questions as quickly and complete as
both reading and video comprehension so that possible (Intention for Completion), and to
an authentic multitasking condition would be finish the other set of reading and questions
replicated; namely, watching television while as accurately as possible (Intention for
reading or studying. Accuracy). Table 2 shows the numbers of
participants by Multitasking Conditions and
As shown in Table 1 below, students
Outcome Intentions.
in each class completed two sets of reading
comprehension tests in sequence, with one
set under one multitasking condition and
another set under a different multitasking

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 81


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

Table 1. Data Collection: Sequence of Multitasking Conditions, Video(s) Used, and Numbers of
Participants in Each Experimental Group

First set of reading Second set of reading


(16 minutes) (16 minutes) Numbers of
Groups Participants
Multitasking Condition (n=130 total)
Multitasking Condition (Video Used)
(Video Used)

1 Silence (None) Video Test (Documentary) 16

2 Video Test (Sitcom) Silence (None) 19

Video Background
3 Silence (None) 13
(Documentary)

4 Silence (None) Video Background (Sitcom) 17

5 Video Background (Sitcom) Video Test (Documentary) 19

6 Video Test (Sitcom) Video Background (Documentary) 17

Video Background
7 Video Test (Sitcom) 11
(Documentary)

8 Video Test (Documentary) Video Background (Sitcom) 18

Table 2. Numbers of Participants by Multitasking Conditions and Outcome Intentions

Multitasking Conditions No. of


Outcome
Silence Video background Video test participants
Intentions
Condition Condition (n=130)

Accuracy 33 48 49 130

Completion 32 47 51 130

82 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

Although the conditions were assigned at set of reading materials and questions (16
the existing class/group level, the Outcome minutes), the participants answered questions
Intentions were randomly assigned at the regarding the video that played (in both video
individual level with each class. That is, half background and video test conditions). They
of the class was randomly assigned “Intention were also asked to report how they split their
for Accuracy” and the other half was randomly attention between the reading and the video
assigned “Intention for Completion” during with the total attention equal to 100%.
the first 16 minutes of reading; those who
were assigned “Intention for Accuracy” during After a short break, the participants
the first 16 minutes of reading would switch to were given instructions with regards to the
“Intention for Completion” during the second Outcome Intentions for the second set of
16 minutes of reading and vice versa. This reading materials. If the participant had been
allowed all the students to participate in both in the Intention for Accuracy with the first set,
Outcome Intentions. The total length of time then he or she was instructed to assume the
each group of the participants took to finish Intention for Completion with the second set,
the experiment was about 45-50 minutes. and vice-versa. As before, if the reading was
The following details the process of the data not in the silence condition, then instructions
collection. were given regarding the video (video
background or video test). When the time was
With each of the eight participating up for the second set of reading and questions
groups, materials were distributed as (also 16 minutes), the participants answered
researchers explained what would be questions regarding the video and reported
asked of the participants. First, general their attention allocations between the reading
demographic information was collected. and video when the video was played. The
Second, instructions were given with regards Multitasking Condition used for the second
to the Outcome Intentions. With the first half of the experiment differed from that used
set of reading, half of the participants were in the first half. In fact, the three Multitasking
instructed to strive for Accuracy and the other Conditions, the two Outcome Intentions, the
half were instructed to strive for Completion. two sets of reading materials, and the three
Further, if the Multitasking Condition was articles within each set of reading materials,
“video background,” participants were told a all were fully cross-checked and distributed in
video would be playing while they read and the way no Condition, Intention, or article was
that they could either pay attention to it or favored in priority.
not. They were not told there would be any
questions asked of them concerning that video Every participant received three points
(although they were asked to answer the video for every correct answer, so getting an answer
questions for the experiment afterwards). In correct was always fully rewarded (and
the Multitasking Condition with the “video answering all questions correctly always
test,” participants were told a video would be provided the best result). However, when the
playing while they read and that they would be participants were assigned to the Intention
tested on the video: they had to pay attention for Accuracy, they were told they would lose
to both. The video (when used) played on one point for every incorrect answer, but
a large screen at the front of the classroom, would not lose a point if the question was
with speaker volume set at a comfortable left unanswered. When they were assigned
level. Once the time was up for the first to the Intention for Completion, they were
told they would lose one point for every

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 83


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

unanswered question, but would not lose a answers, and significant differences for the
point if the question was incorrectly answered. number of blank answers. Further, each of these
These point scores were only used to award tests was completed separately for the data
a few small gifts, and thereby, motivate the under each of the three Multitasking Conditions
participants toward a particular Outcome (silence, video background, and video test)
Intention (being accurate or being complete as with similar results. This set of tests indicated
directed) that would provide the researchers that the Outcome Intention manipulation was
with the opportunity to identify possible successful, and it was successful regardless of
connections between the learners’ reading the Multitasking Conditions.
performance and their Outcome Intentions.
Data used for the analysis of this research To check on the manipulation of the two
however, was based upon the actual number of Multitasking Conditions (video background
correct, blank, and incorrect answers. and test conditions), the researchers looked
at the percentage of attention the participants
3.3. Manipulation Check self-reported as having dedicated to the video
versus the reading under the two conditions:
Before moving forward with data those in the video test condition should report
analysis, manipulations were checked to see a higher percentage of attention applied to
if the experimental design worked. The first the video than those in the video background
manipulation was between the two Outcome condition. While self-reports of attention
Intentions (Intention for Accuracy versus percentages may not be as reliable as measured
Intention for Completion) and the second was data, the researchers believe the participants’
between the two Multitasking Conditions “subjective mental workload” to be reliable
(Video Background and Video Test Conditions). and accurate in its own right (Kalyuga,
Chandler, & Sweller, 2000; Paas, Tuovinen,
In the Outcome Intention design, the Tabbers, & van Gerven, 2003; Tuovinen
researchers encouraged the participants to & Paas, 2004). Those under the Video
strive for either Accuracy or Completion. Background Multitasking Condition reported a
The researchers expected that the participants lower percentage of attention paid to the video
who strived for Accuracy would have a lower playing (25.98%) than did those under the
percentage of wrong answers while those who Video Test Multitasking Condition (38.63%).
strived for Completion would have a lower Both a student’s t-test (p<.001) and a Mann-
percentage of blank answers. Therefore, to Whitney ranking test (p<.001) between the
check the Outcome Intentions, the researchers two conditions indicated the difference was
examined the patterns of wrong and blank significant. In addition, the pattern of correct
answers in the Intention for Accuracy and answers on the video tests was examined to
in the Intention for Completion scenarios. see how attentive the participants were to the
The data showed that the participants had a videos in these two conditions. As expected,
lower percentage of wrong answers (12.3% when watching a video under the video
versus 21.4%) and a higher percentage of background condition participants had a lower
blank answers (20.3% versus 1.2%) under the percentage of video correct answers (54.6%)
Intention for Accuracy than under the Intention than when watching a video under the test
for Completion. Logistic regression (p<.01) condition (66.2%). Logistic regression (p<.01)
and Chi-square analysis (p<.01) indicated and Chi-square analysis (p<.01) indicated
significant differences for the number of wrong significant differences for the number of video

84 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

correct answers between the two conditions, In addition, because eight existing classes
giving us confidence that the participants were used in the study, it was necessary
were paying more attention to the video under to address the concern whether or not the
the Video Test Multitasking Condition than participants in one condition performed
under the Video Background Condition. These significantly better or worse because of their
evidences gave us confidence that our Video paired condition. This concern was addressed
Background and Video Test multitasking through a series of t-tests on the numbers of
manipulation was also successful. correct answers on reading comprehension by
the participants. Table 3 displays the results
from the t-tests.

Table 3. T-tests on the Number of Correct Answers in Reading Comprehension under Each
Multitasking Condition

Number of Correct
Paired Conditions t-tests
Answers
All Silence and Video Background Conditions (Groups
13.93 (in Silence) df=63
#3 + #4 in Table 1)
t=1.288
All Silence and Video Test Conditions (Groups #1 p=.203
12.89 (in Silence)
+ #2 in Table 1)

All Video Background and Silence Conditions (Groups 13.55 (in Video
#3 + #4 in Table 1) Background) df=96
t=.065
All Video Background and Video Test Conditions 13.30 (in Video p=.948
(Groups #5 + #6 + #7 + #8 in Table 1) Background)

All Video Test and Silence Conditions (Groups #1 + #2 12.48 (in Video
in Table 1) Test) df=98
t=.731
All Video Test and Video Background Conditions 11.97 (in Video p=.467
(Groups #5 + #6 + #7 + #8 in Table 1) Test)

A s s h o w n i n Ta b l e 3 , w h e n t h e significantly different in the Silence condition


participants did reading comprehension in the in the two paired conditions; df=63, t=1.288,
Silence and the Video Background conditions, p=.203. When the participants did reading
they received an average of 13.93 correct comprehension in the Video Background
answers in reading comprehension in the and Silence conditions, they received an
Silence condition. In comparison, when the average of 13.55 correct answers in reading
participants did reading comprehension in comprehension in the Video Background
the Silence and Video Test conditions, they condition. In comparison, when the
received an average of 12.89 correct answers participants did reading comprehension in the
in reading comprehension in the Silence Video Background and Video Test conditions,
condition. An independent-samples t-test they received an average of 13.30 correct
indicates the participants’ scores were not answers in reading comprehension in the

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 85


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

Video Background condition. An independent- conditions; df=98, t=.731, p=.467. Therefore,


samples t-test indicates the participants’ it was concluded that the participants were
scores were not significantly different in not significantly affected by the multitasking
the Video Background condition in the two condition pairing to which they were assigned.
paired conditions; df=96, t=.065, p=.948.
Finally, when the participants did reading 4. Results
comprehension in the Video Test and Silence
conditions, they received an average of 12.48 The results (shown in Table 4) indicated
correct answers in reading comprehension in that overall the participants scored higher in
the Video Test condition. In comparison, when their reading comprehension under the video
the participants did reading comprehension background condition (average 75.4%) than
in the Video Test and Video Background under the silence (average 74.3%) or the video
conditions, they received an average of 11.97 test condition (average 68.3%) conditions.
correct answers in reading comprehension in Logistic regression (p<.01) and Chi-square
the Video Test condition. An independent- analysis (p<.01) indicate that the difference
samples t-test indicates the participants’ in the number of correct answers between the
scores were not significantly different in background (average 75.4%) and test (average
the Video Test condition in the two paired 68.3%) conditions was statistically significant.

Table 4. Reading Comprehension Performances: Percent Average of Correct, Wrong, and Blank
Answers by Multitasking Conditions and Outcome Intentions

Multitasking Conditions

Video background Video test All


Outcome Silence condition
condition condition conditions
intentions
% % % % % % % % % % % %
Correct Wrong Blank Correct Wrong Blank Correct Wrong Blank Correct Wrong Blank

Accuracy 67.3 9.6 23.1 70.0 13.2 16.8 65.0 13.2 21.8 67.4 12.3 20.3

Complet-
81.4 17.9 0.7 80.8 18.0 1.2 71.6 26.8 1.6 77.4 21.4 1.2
ion
All
74.3 13.6 12.1 75.4 15.5 9.1 68.3 20.1 11.6 72.4 16.8 10.8
Intentions

The pattern that the participants’ reading answers respectively. With the Intention
comprehension scored better in video for Completion however, the participants
background condition than in silence and video did almost equally well under the silence
test conditions stood out prominently with (average of 81.4%) and video background
the Intention for Accuracy, with 70% average (80.8%) conditions. The participants had
correct answers in background as compared higher reading comprehension scores with
to 67.3% and 65% of average correct the Intention for Completion (average 77.4%)

86 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

than with the Intention for Accuracy (average their adjusted scores with the Intention for
67.4%), although there is an initial possible Accuracy with the additional bonus credits
explanation. Because the instructions for (72.2%). Further, while both groups showed
participants under the Intention for Completion their lowest reading comprehension scores
was to guess if necessary, it stands to reason under the video test condition, those trying to
that approximately 25% of their guessed- be complete (average 71.6%) had much better
at answers would result in a correct answer scores than those trying to be accurate (average
due to random chance (each test question 65.0%, difference of 6.6%, p<.01). In addition,
had four possible answers). Therefore, the the participants’ reading comprehension score
participants’ scores were adjusted and they gap between the Intention for Completion and
were given credit for 25% of the questions Intention for Accuracy was smaller under the
they simply did not answer when they worked video test condition than under the silence or
with the Intention for Accuracy. This would video background condition. The differences
reflect what they would have received by just in the latter two conditions between the two
guessing the answers. However, the results Outcome Intentions continued to be significant
indicated that the participants still had a even when the participants’ scores were
higher percentage of correct answers with adjusted with 25% credit for blank answers
the Intention for Completion (77.4%) than (see Figure 1 below).

Figure 1. Comparisons of percent correct answers in reading comprehension between the


Intention for Completion, Intention for Accuracy, and the Intention for Accuracy with the
adjusted credit points given to 25% of blank answers

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 87


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

Table 5 shows the percentage of correct samples student’s t-test, a logistic or linear
answers on the reading tests under silence and regression test, and a Mann-Whitney’s U
background conditions, between Intentions ranking procedure as indicators for the strength
for Completion and Accuracy, and their of these differences.
differences. It also shows the result of a paired-

Table 5. Percent Average of Correct Answers in Reading Comprehension by Outcome Intentions:


Actual and Adjusted Results under the Silence and Video Background Conditions

Actual Results Adjusted Results


Outcome
Intentions Video Video
Silence Difference Silence Difference
background background

Accuracy 67.3 70.0 - 2.7 (ns) 72.9 73.9 - 1.0 (ns)

Completion 81.4 80.9 .5 (ns) 81.6 81.1 .5 (ns)

Difference - 14.1 - 10.9 - 8.7 - 7.2

Student’s T p<.01 p<.01 p<.01 p<.01

Regression p<.01 p<.01 p<.01 p<.01

Mann-
p<.01 p<.01 p<.01 p<.01
Whitney’s U

As shown in Table 5, participants striving Accuracy as compared to when they strived


for completion ended with approximately the for Completion. Under the Intention for
same score whether they operated in silence Completion, the percentage of wrong answers
(average 81.4%) or with a video playing in between the silence and video background
the background (80.9%, a drop of just .5%, conditions were nearly identical, but rose
p=ns), but while striving for accuracy, the dramatically under the test condition. It
participants scored higher while a video played seemed that the participants with the Intention
in the background (silence average 67.3%, for Completion were fairly unaffected by the
background average 70.0%, an increase of difference between silence and background,
2.7%, p=ns). but they appeared bothered by the video test
condition in that they made more mistakes.
Under the Intention for Accuracy, the Figure 2 below provides the comparison of the
participants reported nearly identical attention reading scores.
allocations on the video when they strived for

88 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

Figure 2. The participants’ reading comprehension scores (the percentage of correct, wrong, and
blank answers) under three Multitasking Conditions and two Outcome Intentions

5. Discussion and Implication intended learning from the materials (Nelson


& Erlandson, 2008). In a more traditional
One of the benefits of MUVEs is the scenario, educators are concerned that the
richness and complexity of the information students, while studying, may be distracted by
they can display, creating something closer the background television or the conversations
to a real-world environment (Dede, 2003; with friends they constantly switch to through
Dede, Ketelhut, & Reuss, 2003; Nelson, text messaging or chat programs (Jackson,
Ketelhut, Clarke, Bowman, & Dede, 2005), 2008). This concern is extended to the
utilizing ill-defined learning outcomes and increasing acceptance of laptop computers
processes (Barab, Thomas, Dodge, Carteaux, in the classrooms (Fried, 2008). This study
& Tuzan, 2005; Jonassen, 1999), and requiring targets these concerns in an experiment
a complex set of interactions (Bruckman, designed to look at the effects of various
2000) to increase participant engagement and levels of video interference on a reading-to-
cognitive processing. Similarly, computer- learn situation and how these effects may
based learning environments incorporate interact with two different outcome intentions
text, video, and pictures to load the learner’ educators could be expecting from their
s input channels in a complementary manner students. Reading comprehension was used
and enrich the learner’s experiences (Clark & to fill the participants’ verbal channel and
Mayer, 2003). One of the concerns educators the video to fill the participants’ non-verbal
have about these systems is the possibility that or imagery channel. Such a setting allowed
multimedia streams, not directly supporting mimic of the actual cases to be replicated;
the material, could have a distracting effect namely, watching television while doing
on the learner and actually impede the homework, communicating with friends while

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 89


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

studying, or using written materials along be more disappointing (Hackman, 1992).


with video materials in a multimedia learning The message for designers of multimedia
presentation (Bruckman, 2000; Dede, 2003; systems is that multitasking may be more
Foehr, 2006). suitable when aiming for an overview of the
information to be presented than when an
Findings indicated that students were, exacting knowledge of the information is
indeed, diverting attention away from their required. The participants operating under the
reading materials when they were required to Intention for Completion generated almost
pay attention to a video at the same time (the identical results when under the silence and
video test condition). This result not only is video background conditions. It appears
in line with evidences that have been shown they were neither harmed nor helped by the
in various dual-task or tasking-switching addition of a background video. This should
studies, but also supports the Cognitive Load give some comfort to those who are concerned
Theory in that the tested video increased about students studying at home with the
cognitive load, and as a result, negatively television on or with the unrelated music
affected the learners’ ability to process the playing while doing homework. As long as the
information. What is of note however, is the learner is seeking a general knowledge of the
difference between the reading performances subject and the distraction can be ignored as
when the learner strived for accuracy as needed, the learner seems to be able to allocate
compared to completion, and when there was attentive resources. The learner can switch
a penalty compared to no penalty for wrong attention to the main task when required for
answers. When the students were encouraged something more intense or difficult, but apply
to complete as many questions as they could his or her attentive resources elsewhere when
and were given the freedom to make mistakes less attention is required on the main task.
without penalty, they did better. When required
to answer each question accurately, such that a In addition, it appears that when the
penalty would be applied for wrong answers, participants worked on their reading task
the participants’ scores fell. This difference in silence with the Intention for Accuracy,
persisted even when the participants were some of their attention simply drifted away
given a 25% credit on the blank answers in the to something else. They needed to think of
Intention for Accuracy. something else and needed a “Daydreaming
Attention Manager” of some kind to find
In this preliminary study, every participant something else to think about; all these took
was asked to engage in both Outcome away their attentive resources. However,
Intentions for Completion and Accuracy, with the “Daydreaming Attention Manager”
two sets of reading materials of equivalent was not needed while the video was played
difficulty, length, and format. Assuming that in the background. The participants were
the participants’ intrinsic cognitive loads were already given the “something else” for their
equivalent between the tasks, more stringent daydreaming or wayward attention. Therefore,
the outcome intention appeared to be increased the attention resources that would have been
extraneous cognitive load and decreased the spent by the Manager were available for their
resources available for completing a reading- reading task. An analogy to this can be seen
to-learn task over a set period of time. In in people who prefer to study in a noisy café
many ways this result is intuitive: if fear rather than in a quiet library. The background
of failure alone builds a greater resistance video may have helped the participants to use
to learning, then the outcomes will likely

90 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

more resources on the primary task because having to be spent finding such a place. Or
the secondary task was a given and did not this may be because the additional stimulation
have to be found. In fact, a recent study by forced the students to try harder to focus
Andrade (2010) showed a similar result of the and get better results on their primary task
beneficial effect of a secondary task (doodling) when distractions are competing for their
on a primary task in that doodling as the attentive resources. Obviously, more research
secondary task facilitated the primary task is necessary to examine the real cause of the
by reducing daydreaming. The background issue. The third implication is related to the
video was suspected to have also served consideration test-designers need to give to
as something to prevent participants from the output intention projected within a time-
daydreaming when they were completing their limited test. The deteriorating effect of the
reading tasks. Intention for Accuracy appears to operate
regardless of multitasking conditions, and
In summary, three issues deserve further assessment results should be interpreted in
attention and discussion. The first is the need light of the outcome intention thrust upon the
for multimedia system designers to consider test-takers.
the level of the information presented (detailed
versus general) when loading both of the 6. Conclusion
learner’s input channels (verbal and non-
verbal)with essential information, the second
Multitasking is an increasingly popular
type of cognitive overload scenario presented
activity in the way we live our lives, so
by Mayer and Moreno (2003). Multitasking
it would benefit us to understand how
may be more suitable when a general than
multitasking works, its outcomes, and
a detailed level of information is to be
moderators of its effectiveness. There are
assimilated. The second implication is the use
many powerful outcomes at stake: the car that
of a secondary non-essential task as a means
loses its guidance on the highway, the student
to focus more of the learner’s resources on the
who does not learn what he or she must know
primary task. According to Mayer and Moreno
for the next major challenge, or the learning
(2003), learners experience the third type of
system that was a great idea, but just did not
cognitive overload when a mix of essential
work. This study extended our understanding
and incidental information is presented during
of the impact of outcome intentions on
a learning process. Our study indicates that
students’ reading comprehension and
the issue may be more complex. Although
multitasking abilities. Our data indicate that
it is too soon to jump to the conclusion,
students performed better with the Intention
the result of the study suggested that the
for Completion than with the Intention
incidental information might sometimes help
for Accuracy, and that the unobtrusive
the acquisition of the essential information
background video likely reduced daydreaming
when the information was channeled through
and helped students focus better in processing
different senses. That is, it may happen that
their primary reading tasks. These findings
our students study better when the television
will hopefully help inform the researchers,
is playing in the background. This may be
designers, and educational practitioners
because the incidental information and the
to conceptualize, design, and construct
stimulation of different senses allowed the
multimedia learning environments that will
part of attention that would daydream to find
help improve students’ ability to focus, read,
a place to operate without attentive resources
understand, and learn.

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 91


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

References adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy,


and biochemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R.
Andrade, J. (2010). What does doodling T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of Frontal
do? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, Lobe Function. Oxford, England: Oxford
100-106. University Press.
Baddeley, A., Chincotta, D., & Adlam, A. Foehr, U. G. (2006). Media multitasking
(2001). Working memory and the control among American youth: Prevalence,
of action: evidence from task switching. predictors, and pairings. Menlo Park, CA:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Kaiser Family Foundation.
General, 130(4), 641-657. Fried, C. B. (2008). In-class laptop use and its
Barab, S. A., Thomas, M., Dodge, T., effects on student learning. Computers &
Carteaux, R., & Tuzan, H. (2005). Making Education, 50, 906-914.
learning fun: Quest Atlantis, a game Gee, J. P. (2003). What video games have to
without guns. Educational Technology, teach us about learning and literacy. New
Research, and Development, 53(1), York: Palgrave Macmillan.
86-108. Hackman, J. R. (1992). Group influences
Bruckman, A. (2000). Uneven achievement on individuals in organizations. In
in a constructivist learning environment. M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough
Paper presented at the International (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and
Conference on Learning Sciences, Ann Organizational Psychology (Vol. 3)
Arbor, MI. (pp. 199-268). Palo Alto: Consulting
Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., de Lacy Costello, Psychologists Press, Inc.
A., & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive Jackson, M. (2008). Distracted: The Erosion
and neuro-anatomical correlates of of Attention and the Coming Dark Age.
multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), New York: Prometheus Books.
848-863. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J.,
Carr, N. (2010). The Shallows: What the & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid
Internet Is Doing to Our Brains. NYC: intelligence with training on working
W.W. Norton & Company. memory. Proceedings of the National
Clark, R. C. & Mayer, R. E. (2003). E-learning Academy of Sciences, USA. 105(19),
and the science of instruction: Proven 6829-33.
guidelines for consumers and designers Johnson, S. B. (2005). Everything bad is good
of multimedia learning. San Francisco: for you: How today’s popular culture is
Pfeiffer. actually making us smarter. New York:
Dede, C. (2003). Multi-user virtual The Penguin Group.
environments. Educause Review, 38(3), Jonassen, D. H. (1999). Designing
60-61. constructivist learning environments. In
Dede, C., Ketelhut, D. J., & Reuss, K. (2003). Reigelugh (Ed.), Instructional design
Motivation, usability, and learning theories and models: A new paradigm
outcomes in a prototype museum-based of instructional theory (Vol. 2) (pp.
multi-user virtual environment. Paper 217-239). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum
presented at the Fifth International Associates, Inc.
Conference of the Learning Sciences. Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., & Cynkar, J. A.
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development (2008). A decrease in brain activation
of prefrontal cortex from birth to young associated with driving when listening to

92 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012


The Impact of Outcome Intentions on Reading and Multitasking Performances

someone speak. Brain Research, 1205, MIT Press.


70-80. Nelson, B., & Erlandson, B. E. (2008).
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Sweller, J. (2000). Managing cognitive load in educational
Incorporating learner experience into multi-user virtual environments:
the design of multimedia instruction. reflection on design practice. Educational
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), Technology, Research, and Development,
126-136. 56, 619-641.
Kincaid, J. P., Fishburne, R. P., Rogers, R. Nelson, B., Ketelhut, D., Clarke, J., Bowman,
L., & Chissom, B. S. (1975). Derivation C., & Dede, C. (2005). Design-based
of new readability formulas (Automated research strategies for developing a
Readability Index, Fog Count and Flesch scientific inquiry curriculum in a multi-
Reading Ease Formula) for Navy enlisted user virtual environment. Educational
personnel). Research Branch Report 8-75, Technology, 45(1), 21-27.
Millington, TN: Naval Technical Training, Paas, F. G., Tuovinen, J. E., Tabbers, H., &
U. S. Naval Air Station, Memphis, TN. van Gerven, P. W. M. (2003). Cognitive
Kirschner, P. A. (2002). Cognitive load theory: load measurement as a means to advance
Implications of cognitive load theory cognitive load theory. Educational
on the design of learning. Learning & Psychologist, 38(1), 63-71.
Instruction, 12(1), 1-10. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations.
Lang, A. (2001). The limited capacity model New York: Oxford University Press.
of mediated message processing. Journal Poldrack, R. A., & Foerde, K. (2007).
of Communication, 46-70. Category learning and the memory
Mayer, R. (2001). Multimedia Learning. systems debate. Neuroscience and
Cambridge University Press. Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 197-205.
Mayer, R. E., & Clark, R. C. (2007). Using Prensky, M. (2001). Digital game-based
rich media wisely. In R.A. Reiser & J.V. learning. Columbus, OH: McGraw-Hill.
Dempsey (Eds.) Trends and issues in instructional Rushton, A., Oxley, J., & Croucher, P. (2000).
design and technology (pp. 311-322). New The handbook of logistics and distribution
Jersey: Pearson Education, Inc. management (2nd ed.). London: Kogan Page.
Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways Ruthruff, E., Van, S. M., Johnston, J. C., &
to reduce cognitive load in multimedia Remington, R. (2006) How does practice
learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), reduce dual-task interference: Integration,
43-52. automatization, or just stage-shortening?
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A Psychological Research, 70, 125–142.
Computational Theory of Executive Schweickert, R., & Boggs, G. J. (1984).
Cognitive Processes and Multiple-task Models of central capacity and
Performance: Part 1. Basic Mechanisms. concurrency. Journal of Mathematical
Psychological Review, 104, 229-233. Psychology, 28, 223--281.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number Small, G., & Vorgan, G. (2009). iBrain:
seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on Surviving the Technological Alteration
our capacity for processing information. o f t h e M o d e r n M i n d . N e w Yo r k :
Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. HarperCollins Publisher.
Monsell, S., & Driver, J. (2000). Control Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during
of cognitive processes: attention and problem solving: Effects on learning.
performance XVIII. Cambridge, Mass: Cognitive Science, 12(2), 257-285.

Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012 93


Journal of Educational Technology Development and Exchange

Tuovinen, J., & Pass, F. G. W. C. (2004. Contact the Authors


Exploring multidimensional approaches to
the efficiency of instructional conditions. Lin Lin
Instructional Science, 32, 133-152. University of North Texas
Wilson, R. (1972). Social choice theory Email: Lin.Lin@unt.edu
without the Pareto Principle. Journal of
Economic Theory, 5(3), 478-486. Adrian Robertson
Montana State University Billings1
Email: tip.robertson@msubillings.edu

Jennifer Lee
University of North Texas
Email: Jennifer.Lee@unt.edu

94 Volume 5, No. 1, October, 2012

View publication stats

You might also like