You are on page 1of 13

The Human Nature Review

ISSN 1476-1084
URL of this document
http://human-nature.com/nibbs/03/shanon.html

Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

Essay Review

Ayahuasca Variations

by

William L. Benzon

Review of The Antipodes of the Mind: Charting the Phenomenology of the Ayahuasca
Experience by Benny Shanon, Oxford University Press, 2002; ISBN: 0199252920

What the Brain Does pour in some milk, and begin beating the mix-
Sometime within the past two or three years ture, I am also thinking about Shanon’s book,
I came upon a paper by Eleanor Rosch (1997) Walter Freeman’s neurodynamic theories, Col-
in which she observed that “William James eridge’s drug-inspired “Kubla Khan” – and
speculated about the stream of consciousness at then! I look out the window and notice how
the turn of the century, and the portrayal of bright and sunny it is. I have little subjective
stream of consciousness has had various literary sense of doing this, then thinking about that,
vogues, but experimental psychology has re- and then back to doing this, and so forth. These
mained mute on this point, the very building seem to be simultaneous streams of attention,
block of phenomenological awareness.” My like two or three interacting contrapuntal voices
impression is that much the same could be said in a Bach fugue. If “the mind is what the brain
about the recent flood of consciousness studies. does” (Kosslyn and Koenig 1995, p. 4) then the
The authors of this work tend to treat con- conscious mind flits from one thing to another
sciousness as a homogenous metaphysical sub- in a most interesting way.
stance. They are quite interested in the relation- Walter Freeman (1999a, pp. 156-158; cf.
ship between this substance and the brain but Varela 1999) speculates that consciousness
show little interest in the varieties of conscious arises as discontinuous whole-hemisphere states
experience, in how consciousness evolves from succeeding one another at a “frame rate” of 6
one moment to the next. Hz to 10 Hz. Each attention stream would thus
How is it possible, for example, that I may consist of a set of discontinuous macroscopic
simultaneously prepare breakfast while thinking brain states interleaved with the states for the
about consciousness? Even as I break an egg other streams. As an analogy, imagine cutting
into a mixing bowl, add some pancake mix, three different films into short segments of no

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 239


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

more that a half dozen or so frames per seg- which he has little to say. By the same token, he
ment. Join the segments together so that each does not take these experiences at face value
second or two of projected film contains seg- either. Ayahuasca visions generally impress
ments from all three films. Now watch this in- themselves on people as sojourns in another
tercut film. Your mind automatically assigns world, a world that is at least somehow separate
each short segment to the appropriate stream so from the mundane world, if not ontologically
that you experience three non-interfering mov- superior to it (that is, more real). While Shanon
ies more-or-less at once. La Strada, Seven feels the pull of such a view, he knows that it is
Samurai, and Toy Story, as it were, unfold in not appropriate to a scientific study. Whatever
your mind each in its own context. is happening, it cannot usefully be explained by
That is the mind in action. Yet, as Rosch has appeals to the supernatural.
noted, cognitive science and consciousness Another World
studies have little to say about it. Benny While one can drink ayahuasca alone, most
Shanon’s The Antipodes of the Mind: Charting users seem to drink it in the company of others,
the Phenomenology of the Ayahuasca Experi- with a particularly experienced person presid-
ence brings this neglect into sharp relief by pre- ing. If the group is a religious one, then the
senting us with an account of the different whole proceeding will follow prescribed ritual,
modes of consciousness that emerge when one which often involves collective singing and
has taken ayahuasca. sometimes dancing as well. The music paces
Ayahuasca is a psychoactive drink con- the unfolding visions and shapes their content
cocted in South America that induces very (p. 189):
strong visions. Shanon is a cognitive psycholo-
gist who stumbled on the practice by accident The tempo and rhythm of the music one
and became interested in the nature of these vi- hears is often reflected in one’s visions.
sions. He started systematically exploring the Notably, the music determines the rhythm
experience, often through rituals organized by and movement of figures in visions as
indigenous animist groups and by more recent well as the rate of change between im-
syncretic groups (the Church of Santo Daime, ages. Thus, the circles of light one com-
União de Vegetal, Barquinha), but also with monly sees when intoxicated often pul-
less formal groups involving “independent sate to the rhythm of the singing one
drinkers” and in sessions by himself. Shanon hears and are co-ordinated with the
has participated in over 130 sessions himself movements of one’s body in dancing. . . .
and has interviewed 178 other users of which the chanting of ayahuasqueros [session
“16 were indigenous or persons of mixed race, leaders] is sensed as directing the course
106 were residents of urban regions of South of the visions of those who listen to it and
America, and 56 were foreigners (that is, per- as determining their general flavour and
sons residing outside South America)” (p. 44).1 colouring.
As the book’s subtitle indicates, it is mostly
an extensive investigation of what happens I can’t help but being reminded of Fantasia,
once one has ingested the brew. Much of it con- though the music there is purely abstract,
sists of more or less richly annotated lists of the whereas ayahuasca hymns have lyrics.
sorts of things one sees and experiences. One generally remains still during the vi-
Shanon is not interested in interpreting these sions, which may happen with one’s eyes
visions in the way a literary critic or a Jungian closed (most common) or with them open. It is
psychologist would. Nor is he interested in ex- even possible, though rare, to split the visual
plaining what happens in neural terms, about field (p. 200):

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 240


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

A Brazilian with extensive involvement tion in the scene.]


with Ayahuasca told me that he once ex- 14. Supreme light.
perienced a split of his visual field. On
one side, he had powerful visions; on the Shanon goes on to assert (p. 296):
other side, he observed the real world
around him, just as it was. Further, he . . . not all types appear in all sessions.
could navigate between the two realms The most common types are (1), (2), (3),
and readily shift between observing the (5), (6), (7), and (8). Next in line is type
visions, on the one hand, and acting in the (9), whereas types (10), (11), (12), (13),
real world and interacting with other (ac- and (14) might not be reached at all. . . .
tually present) persons, on the other hand. Overall, types (1), (2), and (5) are charac-
teristic of the beginnings of sessions.
By and large, the content of these visions can Types (6), (7), (8), and (9) are usually as-
be anything known to the visualizer – incidents sociated with the middle part of sessions.
from their personal life, natural and human his- . . . Types (11), (12) and (14) are charac-
tory, culture and geography, mythology – and teristic of the later part of sessions.
much that seems invented.
Shanon lists the following types of visual By and large, the more complex and sophisti-
scenes (p. 293), all of which are discussed (the cated visions require experience with ayahua-
numbered list is Shanon’s, the explanatory sca. One has to learn how to deploy the drug’s
comments are mine): visionary power. The ability to act in a scene
comes with experience. Note, however, that
1. Bursts, puffs, and splashes of colour. while one can act in scenes, and even become
2. Repetitive, multiplying non-figurative fully immersed in them, one cannot will images
elements. or scenes into being (p. 307).
3. Geometric designs and patterns. Experienced users come to think of the drug
4. Designs with figures. [Figures are vis- as a teacher and ayahuasca sessions as lessons
ual elements that look like things, plant, offering insight into personal, cultural, ethical,
animal, human, and so forth.] political, and metaphysical matters. Traditional
5. Rapid figural transformations healers may encounter “beings who instruct
6. Kaleidoscopic images. them how to go about treating their patients” (p.
7. Well-defined, stable, single figurative 173). Beyond the visions, one often feels great
images. empathy for and insight into the people with
8. Proto-scenes. whom one is sharing the ayahuasca ritual.
9. Full-fledged scenes. Some Temporal Effects
10. Interactive scenes. [The visionary has While visions seem to be the most salient ef-
limited interaction with things in the fect of ayahuasca intoxication, it also has pro-
scene.] found temporal effects, to which Shanon de-
11. Scenes of flight. [The visionary is fly- votes a chapter (pp. 226-241). Thus he reports
ing over the scene and has a subjective that, in his first session he “decided to be cau-
sense of flight, though he or she is, in tious and close my eyes for only a limited pe-
fact, immobile. One may become trans- riod of time. Feeling that so much had hap-
formed into a bird.] pened, I reopened my eyes only to discover –
12. Celestial and heavenly scenes. by looking at my watch – that barely two min-
13. Virtual Reality. [One moves from be- utes had elapsed” (p. 227). His subjective sense
ing a spectator to full immersion and ac- of flow had been altered; sometimes time seems

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 241


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

to speed up, sometimes it seems to slow down I suggest that what Shanon is seeing during
(pp. 230 ff.). The most interesting effect, how- these abrupt movements are these anticipatory
ever, is that “drinkers may feel that they are al- preafferents, which are generated as part of the
together outside time” (p. 234), that “they have act of movement. That is why those abstract
experienced eternity” (p. 236; cf. Benzon 2001, visual elements track his movement exactly;
pp. 164-166). they were generated to do just that. So, even as
Rather than reviewing Shanon’s discussion his brain directs his body to move, it also gen-
of these temporal effects, however, I want to erates anticipatory signals to the visual system
consider a specific observation that may afford and these signals wipe-out whatever vision had
some insight into the underlying neural proc- been evolving to that point.
esses (p. 314): Assume this speculation is correct. As for-
mulated, it is, at best, only a first step toward an
Abruptly, I moved my body (for instance, explanation. For it does not tell us why, in these
in order to check my tape recorder) and circumstances, he should see those preafferents.
found myself out of my previous medita- Normally they are invisible. You can test this
tive balance. With this, my inner visual by sitting comfortably, closing your eyes, and
field was inundated with repetitive non- then standing up. Do you see lights in your vis-
figurative elements whose overall contour ual field that track your motion? I do not. When
traced the pattern of my movement. I close my eyes I see a faint negative after-
image of the scene that had been in front of me.
Why should abrupt movement have this effect, When I stand up, that after-image remains fixed
both to wipe out the ongoing vision, whatever it in my visual field. Ayahuasca changes that, but
may have been, and to replace it with abstract we do not why or how that happens.
visual elements tracing that movement? While I Let us now push this line of speculation a lit-
certainly do not know what is going on, an ob- tle farther. We know that both animals and hu-
vious speculation suggests itself. mans make extensive use of dead reckoning
That suggestion is grounded in the sensory (also known as path integration) in navigating
requirements of physical movement. Given that from one place to another (Golledge 1999, Re-
our eyes move as we move, it follows that the dish 1999). In dead reckoning you estimate dis-
visual world will change in a way that is pre- tance traveled along a given heading as a func-
cisely matched to our path of motion. As we tion of velocity and elapsed time. Dead reckon-
move straight ahead small distant objects will ing thus requires accurate time-telling. How-
become larger and larger, thereby taking up ever, during ayahuasca sessions body move-
more and more of our visual field, until they ment is minimized. One might travel great (vir-
reach their maximum apparent size and then tual) distances in visions, but that travel hap-
disappear off the margin of the visual field. pens without the legs walking or running any-
Similarly, if we move our head to the right, say, where. Thus a critical component of the naviga-
to track a running horse, stationary objects will tion system is non-functional. Could this affect
move to the left in the visual field while the one’s subjective sense of elapsed time?
horse itself will be relatively stable in the cen- I do not know. Shanon does not himself dis-
ter. Since organizing a sharp visual image takes cuss this; but he does cite rather different but
precious fractions of a second, the brain primes classic work by Fraisse, Ornstein, and others on
the visual system for these movements by send- time perception. This work seeks to relate our
ing preafferent signals anticipating the visual subjective sense of time to information load
effects of our movement (Freeman 1999a, during intervals of elapsed time. Shanon finds
1999b, 2000; cf. Gibson 1979, 206 ff., 227 ff.). this suggestive – for information load does

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 242


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

seem to change under ayahuasca – but not en- flight, or perhaps a musician’s masterful
tirely satisfactory. It is in that spirit that I have playing (sic) of his or her instrument.
offered my speculation about time and naviga-
tion. I think it is a useful starting point, but no The surfing image conveys a sense that one is
more. carried, that one only gives a nudge in this or
Beyond this I would like to suggest that that direction, allowing the board and the waves
anyone with a reasonable background in psy- do the rest. That is certainly how improvisation
chology will find their own points of entry into feels when it is going well – and by that I mean
the material Shanon presents. It is rich and routinely well, what an experienced player can
various, and will require us to construct a rich achieve at will. You head off in this or that mu-
psychology. sical “direction” and expect hands, breath, and
Musical Performance fingers to take care of all the details automati-
As I indicated in the beginning, music seems cally; you do not have time to think things
to be quite important in the ayahuasca experi- through at the level of specific notes and
ence. Where the experience is structured by phrases. Further, you intend to hear certain
formal ritual, music is a part of the ritual. The sounds rather than intending to execute certain
assembled celebrants will sing hymns together motor actions. Motor execution is automatic. It
and will listen to music sung by leaders and is only when something goes wrong that you
others. Music shapes the visions. may start thinking about what your lips, hands,
Judging from Shanon’s accounts, much of lungs, and feet should be doing.
this music is prepared beforehand (though not Given musical knowledge, on the one hand,
necessarily rehearsed, Shanon does not say) – and a willingness to allow the music to happen,
certainly that is the case with the hymns sung in on the other hand, one can improvise. That will-
standard rituals. But some of this music is im- ingness, that attitude, is as important as the mu-
provised, and that is what concerns me now. In sical knowledge. With that in mind, consider
fact, what really concerns me is the similarity the following story Shanon tells about a private
between the dynamics of ayahuasca experience, ayahuasca session:
as Shanon has described it, and the dynamics of
skilled improvisation – as I understand it from In an amateur fashion, I have been play-
my own fairly extensive experience and as ing the piano since childhood. I have
those dynamics are presented in the literature played only classical music, always from
(Bailey 1992, Berliner 1994, Nettl and Russell the score, never improvising . . . Once
1998, cf. Benzon 2001, pp. 69-71, 93-95). during a private Ayahuasca session, I saw
As you read the following statement by the piano in front of me. A score of a
Shanon (pp. 351-352), recall Freeman’s specu- Bach prelude was there. I played the
lation that consciousness occurs as a serious of piece repeatedly and felt I was entering
discontinuous whole-hemisphere brain states: into a trance. Then, I left the score aside
and began to improvise. I played for more
. . . under the intoxication, drinkers may than an hour, and the manner of my play-
move back and forth smoothly between ing was different from anything I have
radically different states of mind. Such ever experienced. It was executed in one
movement need not be either erratic or unfaltering flow, constituting an ongoing
chaotic, and for the experienced drinker it narration that was composed as it was be-
may even have the feel of playfulness. As ing executed. It appeared that my fingers
I have suggested earlier, I would liken it just knew where to go. Throughout this
to surfing the waves of the sea, or a bird’s act, my technical performance astounded

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 243


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

me. At times, I felt that a force was upon had been written out; he thus had no choice but
me and that I was performing at its com- to play entirely “by ear.” It was some of the
mand. (pp. 220-221) best improvisation he had ever done.
What these incidents have in common is that
Someone who had been present during this per- they involve experienced musicians playing in
formance remarked that “It seemed that the an unaccustomed way. It is clear that, in some
Muses descended upon you.” Shanon has sub- sense, both of these men already had a certain
sequently continued to improvise without par- capability in their heads and fingers, but did not
taking of ayahuasca, though he remarks that ordinarily access it. My acquaintance was ac-
“the quality of this playing is not like that under customed to improvisation, but his level of per-
the intoxication.” formance on these occasions was elevated
Now, compare Shanon’s experience with the above his norm. Shanon’s case is more extreme
following one, reported to me by an acquaint- in that he had never before improvised; that is,
ance: he had never before assumed the attitude of an
improviser.
I had a gig playing piano on New Year's It is not at all clear what made the difference
Eve at the cocktail lounge of the most for my acquaintance. Something just happened.
prestigious resort hotel in town. I took in Nor, for that matter, is it clear what happened to
my drum/bass machine, but had to play Shanon. Yes, he had taken ayahuasca; but we
the house piano [which had an unusually do not know enough about the brain and the
sluggish action]. For the first half hour of drug for that information to count as an expla-
the gig, I struggled with the molasses-like nation of what happened. How did ayahuasca
keys and was about to quit in frustration, unlock the knowledge held latent in Shanon’s
when a man at the bar made a request for brain and fingers? How did it give him the atti-
a tune I particularly like to play, Monk's tude of an improviser?
“Round Midnight.” Suddenly I found that Given the knowledge and the attitude, one
the same stiff keys that were giving me finds oneself able to revise one’s musical inten-
fits seemed to melt under my touch and I tions from moment to moment. Frequently
played with great feeling. The man left, notes come out that are not consistent with your
but I continued on my roll; within a half current intention. You learn to put these “mis-
hour, people who had stepped out of the takes” to creative use by reconceiving the im-
main ballroom where a big band was provisation and sending it in a different direc-
playing began to gather around to listen tion (cf. Berliner 1994). Similarly, skilled im-
and dance. This fueled me further and I provisers pick up rhythms and phrases from
was able to reach even greater heights of accompanying musicians and incorporate them
performance. . . . I was giddy with confi- into their performance. In the extreme, one can
dence and inspiration.2 even incorporate rude intrusions into the musi-
cal flow, as the following story about Wynton
This same person reported a similar incident at Marsalis illustrates (Hadju 2003):
a band rehearsal. He was playing the “ride” or
“jazz” chair in the trumpet section – that is, he He played a ballad, “I Don't Stand a
was the player designated to take the impro- Ghost of a Chance With You,” unaccom-
vised solos – as the band was rehearsing a new panied. . . . When he reached the climax,
arrangement for the first time. When it came Marsalis played the final phrase, the title
time for him to play a solo he noticed that he statement, in declarative tones, allowing
had lost the page on which the chord changes each successive note to linger in the air a

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 244


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

bit longer. “I don't stand ... a ghost ... of cally. I would guess the whole process of arriv-
... a ... chance ...” The room was silent ing back at the concluding line of the original
until, at the most dramatic point, some- tune required relatively little deliberate inter-
one's cell phone went off, blaring a rapid vention – most likely for the key changes Hadju
singsong melody in electronic bleeps. . . . mentions.
Marsalis paused for a beat, motionless, This same adaptability is characteristic of
and his eyebrows arched. . . . Still frozen experienced users of ayahuasca. Thus Shanon
at the microphone, Marsalis replayed the reports (p. 346) that “when, during a session . . .
silly cell-phone melody note for note. I would wish to invest my visions with new
Then he repeated it, and began improvis- momentum, I would open my eyes and look
ing variations on the tune. The audience around. Closing my eyes again, the new percep-
slowly came back to him. In a few min- tions from the real world would be incorporated
utes he resolved the improvisation— into the images that appeared before me and
which had changed keys once or twice further progressions in the visioning would be
and throttled down to a ballad tempo— experienced.” In both cases, improvising and
and ended up exactly where he had left ayahuasca visions, a coherent flow is stitched
off: “with ... you ...” The ovation was tre- together moment by moment. What is puzzling
mendous. is how the overall flow can be coherent despite
the sporadic presence of extreme discontinuities
I do not know what Marsalis was consciously from one moment to the next. What is it that
regarding when he heard the cell phone melody, one has to learn – about music, the ayahuasca
but his decision to take up that melody has to world, oneself – to be comfortable embracing
have been a quick one. It may even have been the discontinuities?
made during the interrupting melody itself. Xanadu
Let us return to Freeman’s speculation that Now let us take a brief look at the most fa-
consciousness is organized as short discontinu- mous drug-induced poem in the English lan-
ous whole-hemisphere states. While Marsalis guage, “Kubla Khan” by Samuel Taylor Col-
was improvising, almost all of those states eridge. Though all or part of the poem may
would have been focused on the improvisation have been written as early as 1797, when Col-
itself; if he was operating at a level of concen- eridge published it in 1816 he provided it with a
tration that seems relatively rare, then all of preface in which he claimed that a poem of
those “frames” would have been given over to “from two to three hundred lines” came to him
the music.3 When the cell phone jingle inter- over the course of a three-hour opium-induced
rupted – during a brief pause in Marsalis’s mu- reverie. During this process “all the images rose
sic – some of these conscious states would have up before him as things, with a parallel produc-
focused on it. The decision to take up the jingle tion of the correspondent expressions, without
might have intruded on a state or three or its any sensation or consciousness of effort.” Upon
own, or it might have been blended in with awakening he started transcribing the poem but,
states otherwise given over to the jingle. The no sooner had he started, he was interrupted for
decision would not have been a matter of ex- about an hour. Upon returning to his room he
plicit deliberation and planning; rather, it would discovered that “though he still retained some
have been more like a bicyclist evading an un- vague and dim recollection of the general pur-
expected pothole than Gary Kasparov trying to port of the vision, yet, with the exception of
out-think an opponent. Once the decision had some eight or ten scattered lines and images, all
been made the variations on that melody would the rest had passed away like the images on the
have composed themselves all but automati- surface of a stream into which a stone has been

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 245


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

cast, but, alas! without the after restoration of slanted


the latter!” The fifty-four lines Coleridge pub- Down the green hill athwart a cedarn
lished are all that is left of the three-hundred cover!
line poem that came to him in this vision.4 (ll. 12-13)
For reasons that are too complex to review
here, scholars have come to doubt the truth of The shadow of the dome of pleasure
various of the assertions that Coleridge makes Floated midway on the waves;
in this preface. Yet Coleridge’s assertion that (ll. 31-32)
“the images rose up before him as things” is
quite similar to the phenomenology of ayahua- There is no sense that the poet (that is to say,
sca visions – though we should note that opium the voice that is producing these words) has any
is quite different pharmacologically – as is the role in this world; it is simply there. This too is
asserted fragility, that Coleridge was unable to similar to many ayahuasca visions. The vision
reconstruct the full vision once he had been in- simply unfolds, scene by scene.
terrupted. The exotic nature of the poem’s con- Things change dramatically with the begin-
tent – a fantastic oriental garden and palace de- ning of the second part of the poem. There is an
creed by an Oriental ruler – is also consistent abrupt change of scene and we are no longer
with themes and motifs Shanon has reported. looking at Xanadu. Such change is, again, char-
Beyond this we should note that “Kubla acteristic of ayahuasca visions. It is not at all
Khan” is unique in Coleridge’s output. His col- clear where we are as the second part opens:
lected poems fill a single volume of almost 500
pages, and none of them are similar to “Kubla A damsel with a dulcimer
Khan.”5 However this poem came into being, In a vision once I saw:
Coleridge was not able to repeat that process. It was an Abyssinian maid,
On the whole I am inclined to believe that he And on her dulcimer she played,
did have a vision and that this poem is what he Singing of Mount Abora.
was able to make of it. Perhaps the published (ll. 37-41)
poem was quickly written down more or less as
published, as he asserted. But I can also imag- The exoticism remains, but the poet has now
ine that he worked with the raw material quite a entered the poem – “In a vision once I saw” –
bit to give it final form. We have no strong evi- and will remain in it until the end. He goes on
dence either way. to assert that, if he could revive the maid’s song
Turning to the poem itself, the first thirty-six he would then use “music loud and long” (l. 45)
lines (of fifty-four) present us with the world of to “build that dome in air” (l. 46). The perform-
Xanadu. It is laid before us, impersonally: ance would be so convincing that onlookers
would actually see “That sunny dome! those
In Xanadu did Kubla Khan caves of ice!” (l. 47). Notice that the priority of
A stately pleasure-dome decree. music over vision suggests the role that music
(ll. 1-2) plays in forming ayahuasca visions. The poet
sings a song and the audience sees the things he
So twice five miles of fertile ground sings about, namely the pleasure palace and the
With walls and towers were girdled caves. These listeners respond by distancing
round: themselves from the poet and his vision:
(ll. 5-6)
And all should cry, Beware! Beware!
But oh! that deep romantic chasm which His flashing eyes, his floating hair!

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 246


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

Weave a circle round him thrice, rately structured. Its temporal evolution is there
And close your eyes with holy dread, on the page, awaiting our analysis. This is not
For he on honey-dew hath fed, the place to detail that structure, which is a long
And drunk the milk of Paradise. and often tedious task (see Benzon 1985 for
(ll. 49-54) details); but we can review some general as-
pects.
As this is but the hypothetical consequence of Each of the poem’s two parts can be divided
the poet being able to sing his song, it involves into three sections according to content. The
what Shanon has called a second-order vision. first section of the first part (ll. 1 – 11) concerns
The poet (base vision) imagines what would Kubla’s decree and is visual and spatial in char-
happen if he became inspired (second-order vi- acter. The second section (ll. 12 – 30) is audi-
sion). Shanon has had only one second order tory and kinesthetic and is focused on a foun-
vision himself, and no informant has reported tain giving birth to a river. The third section (ll.
one. In Shanon’s base vision (p. 103) he was in 31-36) is both visual and auditory. In a similar
a palace where he met an person “whom I fashion, the second part of the poem also has a
found impressive.” This person invited him on ternary structure. The first section is about
an “adventure that . . . would involve a radical poet’s vision of the damsel (ll. 37-41); the sec-
change in my state of consciousness.” Shanon ond section involves the hypothetical creation
then imagines that adventure, making it a sec- of the “dome in air” (ll. 42-50); while the third
ond-order vision within the base vision of the section (ll. 51-54) gives the magical incantation
palace and the impressive man. In this hypo- of the poet’s audience.
thetical vision Shanon becomes transformed We can extend this structural analysis down
into a puma who is then devoured by other Pu- a level, subdividing the three sections of the
mas acting at the behest of the impressive man. poem’s first part into subsections and then do-
Needless to say, the Shanon in the base vision ing the same with the poem’s second part. We
declined the invitation to undergo this further find that the two parts of the poem have the
visionary opportunity. same structure at this level. Expressed in out-
In both cases, the second-order vision de- line form, that common structure looks like this
picts what would happen if a figure in the base (with line numbers to the right):
vision (the poet, Shanon) went into an inspired
mental state. In both cases the result was ag- 1st Part 2nd Part
gression directed against the inspired figure. In
Coleridge’s case the aggression is rather mild: A. First Section 1-11 37-41
the audience makes magical noises to protect 1. subdivision 1-5 37-38
themselves from the wild-eyed poet. In 2. subdivision 6-11 39-41
Shanon’s case the aggression was far more di- B. Second Section 12–30 42-50
rect: being eaten by pumas. 1. subdivision 12–16 42-44
Above and beyond matter of thematic con- 2. subdivision 17-24 45-48
tent perhaps the most interesting reason for 3. subdivision 25-30 49-50
looking at the poem is that it is highly struc- C. Third Section 31-36 51-54
tured. Shanon gives relatively few accounts of 1. subdivision 31-34 51-52
complete vision sequences, and the ones he 2. subdivision 35-36 53-54
gives are necessarily sketchy. Thus we have
little sense of the exact time-course of an un- Notice that, just as the two parts of the poem
folding vision. Coleridge’s vision is vividly re- are divided into three sections, so the middle
alized and, above all else, the poem is elabo- sections of those three is itself divided into

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 247


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

three subdivisions while the first and third sec- don’t know.
tions are divided only into two subdivisions. We do not know how the brain evolves from
Given that the content of the two parts is so state to state under the influence of ayahuasca.
very different, this level of structural similarity For obvious reasons, we cannot get real-time
is quite striking. accounts of ayahuasca visions; the very act of
Thus the poem does not simply present us reciting what is happening would influence the
with one thing after another. Rather, the vision experience. We do not have any second-by-
unfolds in scenes that have a rich hierarchical second accounts of these visions.
structure. Do ayahuasca visions have such So, the poem and the visions and the under-
structure? lying mental acts are not directly comparable.
What is even more striking is that that final But we can analyze and study the poem and
line of the poem’s first part recurs in the middle people’s response to it more readily than we
of the middle of the poem’s second part: can ayahuasca visions. While the ubiquitous
PET scanners are too sluggish to capture infor-
Line 36: A sunny pleasure-dome with mation about moment-by-moment brain activ-
caves of ice! ity, we might be able to use EEG (or other)
Line 47: That sunny dome! those caves of methods to obtain useful information about the
ice! brain activity of people as they read the poem
themselves, or as they listen to someone recite
If we think of the mind as evolving through it. We could then begin correlating that brain
some discontinuous set of states during line 36, activity with the text of the poem and so begin
what sequence of states does it evolve through to understand how the mind evolves such
during line 47? Is it the same sequence or not? I meanings from moment to moment. Once we
do not know, nor even know how to find out. get that far I cannot help but believe that we
That question arises naturally from a thorough will have learned something that is applicable
analysis of “Kubla Khan” (see Benzon 1985); to unraveling the mysteries of ayahuasca,
but that is only one of many questions arising though I cannot predict what that might be.
from such analysis. What are the correlative Shanon argues that “the Ayahuasca experi-
questions to be asked about ayahuasca visions? ence is one of generation and creation. Rather
One could reasonably reply that there are no than being a discoverer or subject to a passive
such questions as the situations are rather dif- process of discovery, the Ayahuasca drinker
ferent. After all, Coleridge had not drunk aya- should be viewed as an imaginative creator” (p.
huasca, he had taken tincture of opium. Further, 383). If this is so, then studying people’s re-
even if we believe that “Kubla Khan” reflects sponse of poems, such as “Kubla Khan,” should
an opium-induced vision, we have no good rea- give us clues to the workings of ayahuasca.
son to think the poem is a direct read-out of that Sensing the Real
vision. It may reflect a certain amount deliber- Perhaps the most puzzling aspect of ayahua-
ate shaping by Coleridge. This does not neces- sca visions, above and beyond their fantastic
sarily mean, for example, that he self- variety and detail, is their sense of being real (p.
consciously schemed to bring the poem’s two 65):
parts into structural alignment; but he might
very well have been guided by intuitions about Often, the things I saw under the intoxi-
aesthetic “rightness.” Thus we have no positive cation impressed me as being so real that
reason to believe that this poem’s complexity is the conclusion seemed to be unavoidable:
a direct mirror of some complexity that evolved truly existing other realities are being re-
during a vision. It may or may not. We simply vealed. Believing that this is the case is

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 248


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

very common with the drinkers of Aya- have to imagine where it is going to be
huasca. Both during the course of the in- after I have moved it. I have to imagine
toxication and afterwards the question re- what is not now true — a contrafactual. I
peatedly forces itself: Does this really ex- understand where the glass now is — the
ist? . . . The things seen with Ayahuasca reality of the glass—by noting where it is
often strike people as so different from not. Having moved the glass, I know
anything they have seen or known that where it is by remembering where it was,
they cannot be the products of their own again something no longer the fact.
intellect.
We are now in the same conceptual territory we
Where does the sense of reality come from? entered into when we began speculating about
How does the brain engender that? time, where we were concerned about sensory
This is the kind of question that drove Des- preafference attendant upon moving. While I do
cartes: How can you tell that the scene before not think we can leave matters rest there —
you is real, rather than being a phantasm placed surely there is more work to be done, both em-
in your mind by a malignant being? The prob- pirically and conceptually — I am content for
lem is that there is nothing in the visual field now with the general notion that our sense of
that serves as a reality indicator. You cannot, time and our sense of reality may be bound up
for example, look to the lower left visual field in the same neural mechanisms, namely those
and see flashing lights indicating reality status: responsible for moving us reliably from one
one light indicating real perception, two lights place to another in a complex world.
indicating a dream, or three lights indicating a If so, then one might think that, to under-
hallucination. Perception is not like that. One stand the effects of ayahuasca, we need only
just knows that things are real, immediately and understand how those mechanisms — surely
surely – and sometimes wrongly as well. Simi- they are ancient ones — operate. It may well be
larly, the ayahuasca voyager knows that the that the drug’s fundamental effects involve
world of her visions is real, immediately and changes in only a few basic physiological pa-
surely. Thus the sense of reality would seem to rameters, but those changes cascade through the
be an elementary sensation rather than the out- entire brain. One takes ayahuasca and voilà! the
come of a sophisticated inferential process. Of world changes. I suspect, however, that we will
what could such a sensation possibly consist? only understand how the whole world changes
I do not know, but Norman Holland (2003) when we understand the whole of the brain in
has made a useful suggestion while considering which it changes. That is a formidable task.
a similar problem: Why is it that we invest lit-
erary works with sufficient reality that we often William L. Benzon, Ph.D., 708 Jersey Avenue,
feel strong emotion in response to them? Ob- 2A, Jersey City, NJ 07302, USA. Email: bben-
serving that “brains serve one overarching pur- zon@mindspring.com.
pose,” namely, “to move a body,” Holland goes
on to observe that we are generally static when Notes
“absorbed in a movie or play or book” and that:
1
N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT) is the major,
Reality-testing, it turns out, is also related but by no means only, psychoactive chemical in
to planning movement and action. ayahuasca. It affects the brain’s serotonin sys-
To intend to act, to plan a movement, tem. There is a fair amount of information
we imagine the outcome. If I plan to about ayahuasca available on the web, includ-
move that glass of water on the table, I ing first-person accounts of visions. Two places

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 249


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

to start exploring this information are Erowid “Christabel” as three “poems of the fantastic,”
and Lycæum: which they are. The other two poems, however,
http://www.erowid.org/chemicals/ayahuasca/ay are long narratives, and so are constructed quite
ahuasca.shtml differently from “Kubla Khan,” though sharing
http://leda.lycaeum.org/Preparations/Ayahuasca its exoticism.
.4445.shtml
The Church of Santo Daime has a website: Acknowledgements
http://www.santodaime.org/indexy.htm
2
This person had been a student of mine about a I would like to thank Howard Rheingold and
decade ago and the account is one he had sub- Alexander Shulgin for their comments on an
mitted as part of his coursework. I have since earlier draft. I take full responsibility for the
lost touch with him and so have been unable to published essay.
obtain his permission to publish his words here.
I do note, however, that his account was circu- References
lated to everyone in the class and thus publish-
ing it here does not seem to be a violation of Bailey, D. (1992). Improvisation: Its Nature
privileged communication. and Practice in Music. New York, Da Capo
3
I am, of course, guessing about this. My guess Press.
is based on my own experience and on various Benzon, W. L. (1985). "Articulate Vision: A
subjective reports I have read, a number of Structuralist Reading of "Kubla Khan.""
which are scattered throughout my book (Ben- Language and Style 18: 3-29.
zon 2001). In general, one’s mind can easily flit Benzon, W. L. (2001). Beethoven's Anvil: Mu-
about to other matters while one is playing. The sic in Mind and Culture. New York, Basic
more involved one is with the music, however, Books.
the less the mind flits about. Complete and utter Berliner, P. (1994). Thinking in Jazz: The Infi-
absorption into the music seems relatively rare, nite Art of Improvisation. Chicago, Univer-
but I do not know of any systematic attempts to sity of Chicago Press.
gather a large corpus of subjective accounts that Freeman, W. J. (1999a). Consciousness, Inten-
would support a more reasoned and subtle tionality and Causality. Reclaiming Cogni-
statement. tion. R. Núñez and W. J. Freeman. Thover-
4
”Kubla Khan” is available online; I recom- ton, Imprint Academic: 143-172.
mend the lightly annotated version made avail- Freeman, W. J. (1999b). How Brains Make Up
able by the University of Virginia: Their Minds. London, Weidenfeld and
http://etext.lib.virginia.edu/stc/Coleridge/poems Nicholson.
/Kubla_Khan.html Freeman, W. J. (2000). "Perception of time and
For a review and critique of the major interpre- causation through the kinesthesia of inten-
tations of the poem, see Tsur 1987; Hogsette tional action." Cognitive Processing 1: 18-
1997 has an interesting approach to the poem’s 34.
puzzling preface. Finally, you might wish to Golledge, R. G. (1999). Wayfinding Behavior:
consult Book Four of John Livingston Lowes, Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Proc-
The Road to Xanadu (1927). Here Lowes pre- esses. Baltimore, The Johns Hopkins Uni-
sents the results of his search through Col- versity Press.
eridge’s notebooks for the sources of themes Hadju, D. (2003). "Wynton's Blues." The At-
and images in “Kubla Khan”. lantic Monthly 291(2): 43-58.
5
”Kubla Khan” is often grouped together with http://www.theatlantic.com/issues/2003/03/h
“The Rime of the Ancient Mariner” and ajdu.htm

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 250


Human Nature Review 3 (2003) 239-251

Hogsette, D. S. (1997). "Eclipsed by the Pleas- University of Chicago Press.


ure Dome: Poetic Failure in Coleridge's 'Ku- Rosch, E. (1997). Transformation of the Wolf
bla Khan'." Romanticism On the Net 5. Man. The Authority of Experience: Essays
http://users.ox.ac.uk/~scat0385/eclipsed.htm on Buddhism and Psychology. J. Pickering.
l Surrey, Curzon Press. Available online at:
Holland, N. N. (2003). "The Willing Suspen- http://cogweb.ucla.edu/Abstracts/Rosch_97.
sion of Disbelief: A Neuro-Psychoanalytic html
View." PsyArt: A Hyperlink Journal for the Redish, D. A. (1999). Beyond the Cognitive
Psychological Study of the Arts: article Map. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
020919. Siegel, R. K. and L. J. West (1975). Hallucina-
http://www.clas.ufl.edu/ipsa/journal/2003/ho tions. New York, John Wiley & Sons.
llan06.htm Tsur, R. (1987). The Road to "Kubla Khan": A
Kosslyn, S. M. and O. Koenig (1995). Wet Cognitive Approach. Jerusalem, Israel Sci-
Mind: The New Cognitive Neuroscience. ence Publishers.
New York, Free Press. http://www.tau.ac.il/~tsurxx/KublaProspect_
La Barre, W. (1972). The Ghost Dance. New 2.html
York, Dell Publishing Co., Inc. Varela, F. J. (1999). The Specious Present: A
Lowes, J. L. (1927). The Road to Xanadu: A Neurophenomenology of Time Conscious-
Study in the Ways of the Imagination. Bos- ness. Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in
ton, Houghton Mifflin Company. Contemporary Phenomenology and Cogni-
Nettl, B. and M. Russell, Eds. (1998). In the tive Science. J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pa-
Course of Performance: Studies in the choud and J.-M. Roy. Stanford, Stanford
World of Musical Improvisation. Chicago, University Press: 266-314. .

Human Nature Review, Volume 3, 2003, 251

You might also like