You are on page 1of 14

Developmental Neurorehabilitation, February 2009; 12(1): 12–23

Predicting social and functional outcomes for individuals sustaining


paediatric traumatic brain injury

REBECCA WELLS1, PATRICIA MINNES2, & MARJORY PHILLIPS3


1
University of Waterloo, Psychology, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, 2Queen’s University, Psychology, Kingston, Ontario,
Canada, and 3Integra Foundation, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

(Received 17 January 2009; accepted 21 January 2009)

Abstract
Objective: To determine the efficacy of models employing the classification of severity of injury, based on either GCS scores
or clinical findings using standardized test scores for Occupational Therapy, Physiotherapy and Psychology in predicting
outcomes associated with paediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI).
Method: Medical records were reviewed to obtain GCS scores and standardized tests used in the clinically based
classification of severity of injury for 30 individuals who sustained a paediatric TBI and were patients of a brain injury
treatment programme. Interviews were conducted with parents to obtain current data on social participation, cognitive
functioning and environmental factors.
Results: Three variables emerged as significant predictors of outcome: age at injury, clinical ratings of injury severity and
environmental factors.
Conclusion: Findings offer preliminary support for the idea that a combination of factors, including age at injury, clinical
expertise and the environment, provide the best estimate of long-term outcome.

Keywords: Traumatic brain injury, paediatrics, clinicians, quality of life, severity, outcomes

Introduction behavioural sequelae noted after moderate-to-severe


brain injury, those behaviours related to memory
Empirically-based studies indicate that paediatric
and executive functioning appear to be the most
brain injury is more devastating than was once
commonly affected [11, 12].
thought [1–4]. Unlike adults, the effects of paediatric
In contrast, the empirical data reflecting brain
brain injury on brain function interact with the
injuries that are classified as ‘mild’ are less clear. In
maturation and development of the child. Skills that
a review of 40 research studies conducted on mild
are emerging or developing may be affected differ-
head injury in children and adolescents between
ently by brain injury than skills that are already
1970–1995, Satz et al. [13] found inconsistent
established. Because the child’s brain is still matur-
ing at the time of injury, the full impact of an insult evidence regarding neuropsychological, academic
to the brain may not become evident for many and psychosocial outcomes. More specifically,
months or even years [1, 3, 4]. Moreover, the rate although 33% of the studies reported adverse
and recovery of cognitive and behavioural deficits effects of mild brain injury, 45% reported no adverse
vary as a function of age at injury, severity of injury effects resulting from mild brain injury. The
and pre- and co-morbid factors [5]. remaining 22% of studies did not provide clear
Accurate identification of the severity of the contrasts between mild brain-injured groups and
traumatic brain injury (TBI) is important for the controls or more severely injured groups. This
development of timely and efficient rehabilitation review mirrors the findings of other studies in the
programmes. However, predicting which children TBI literature that highlight variability. In particular,
require acute and intensive rehabilitation, school and controlling for pre-injury characteristics, mild brain
community-based programming or monitoring is injury has been associated with hyperactivity,
an inexact science [6]. Severe TBI is most clearly inattention and conduct disordered behaviours in
associated with longer-term impairments in cogni- children between the ages of 10–13 years and
tive and behavioural functioning [7–10]. Of the adverse academic sequelae [14–16]. In contrast,

Correspondence: Ms Rebecca Wells, MA, University of Waterloo, Psychology, 200 University Ave W, Waterloo, Ontario, N2L3G1 Canada.
E-mail: remwells@uwaterloo.ca
ISSN 1751–8423 print/ISSN 1751–8431 online/09/010012–12 ß 2009 Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.
DOI: 10.1080/17518420902773109
Predicting social and functional outcomes for individuals sustaining paediatric TBI 13

arguing that associations between mild TBI and socioeconomic status (SES) and parental stress
deficits reflects problems in methodology, research- levels. In a study of 109 school-aged children with
ers have failed to find differences between children TBI, Donders and Nesbit-Greene [36] found that
with mild TBI and a control group on measures of demographic variables, including ethnicity and SES,
intellectual ability, receptive language and memory accounted for a significant portion of the variability
capacity [17–24]. in performance on neuropsychological tests such as
Classification of TBI into mild, moderate or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)
severe groups has traditionally relied upon measure- and the Children’s Category Test. Similarly, Hawley
ment of coma following insult to the brain [25]. [37] and Taylor et al. [10] found that parental
In particular, the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) was marital status and the family environment were
developed by Teasdale and Jennett [26] for an adult associated with behavioural problems up to 4 years
population to classify injuries by assessing three post-injury in samples of school-aged children
determinants: eye opening, verbal responses and sustaining TBI.
motor response. Individuals are considered to have One recent direction that is being taken in the
experienced a mild brain injury when their GCS study of paediatric TBI involves the use of neurop-
score is 13–15; moderate TBI (9–12); and severe sychological assessments to predict outcomes.
TBI (<8). Of note, the GCS was intended to be used Rather than relying solely on indices of severity of
serially over a period of time in order to capture injury such as the GCS or Post Traumatic Amnesia,
changes related to both the primary injury several studies have employed neuropsychological
(i.e. contusions, lacerations, diffuse axonal injury) test scores during the initial post-injury assessment
and secondary injury (i.e. oedema, intracranial phase to predict short- and long-term outcomes
infections) [27]. following a brain injury [9, 38]. In a study of 58
Although the GCS is widely used by medical children with TBI, Miller and Donders [9] found
personnel and in the bulk of research studies that T-scores below 45 on the California Verbal
investigating brain injury to determine severity of Learning Test-Children’s Version (CVLT-CV)
injury, research into its reliability and validity for resulted in an increase of up to 13 times that
this purpose has offered inconsistent results [28–32]. children would be placed in special education classes
There are few studies available that specifically within 2 years following injury. Kinsella et al. [38]
examine the utility of the GCS in assessing severity reported similar results in their study of 29 school-
of injury in paediatric populations [27, 33]. aged children sustaining a TBI. More specifically,
Furthermore, the majority of studies using severity lower scores on the Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning
of injury as a predictor of outcome use only one GCS Test and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test
score, including the best, the worst or the initial were predictive of placement in special education at
GCS, to determine injury severity. This practice is 2 years post-injury.
problematic not only because there is inconsistency In addition to the trend toward the utilization
across studies with regard to which GCS score is of standardized testing in determining outcomes,
used, but also in light of the fact that recent studies there is currently a movement toward evidence-
have reported that low GCS does not necessarily based practice in the health care system. Although
mean poor prognosis [34]. In fact, several studies not extensively tested, there is evidence to suggest
have reported that secondary injury factors may be that clinical expertise is important in determining
more important in predicting outcomes than the outcome [39]. The usefulness of clinical expertise in
primary injury [27, 33, 34]. predicting outcomes is based on the idea that experts
In those studies that attempt to predict outcomes are trained and experienced in recognizing mean-
following paediatric brain injury, a number of ingful patterns when assessing an individual.
variables have been suggested as possible confounds Additionally, experts acquire extensive knowledge
for the ability to make predictions based on severity and organize it in ways that allow for the retrieval of
of injury. Unlike TBI during adulthood, paediatric relevant knowledge automatically. It is easy to see
TBI occurs during a time of ongoing physical and how these abilities would be useful in the assessment
cognitive development [35]. Thus, in examining and treatment of brain injury when one considers
outcomes related to paediatric TBI, it is important how complex the relationship between injury char-
to consider variables such as age at injury which acteristics and outcomes can be. It is necessary to be
may impact findings regarding outcomes for these familiar with the variety of patterns that can emerge
individuals [4]. following an injury to the brain and to be able to
Another set of potentially confounding variables recognize variables that may directly or indirectly
involves the influence of demographic and environ- impact future functioning.
mental factors including gender, ethnicity and In addition to exploring alternate methods of
parental factors such as maternal education, operationalizing predictors, researchers have looked
14 R. Wells et al.

at measures of outcome. Typically, standardized associated with paediatric brain injury. In light of
scores on tests of neurocognitive functioning are the recent shift toward the inclusion of clinical
used to operationalize outcome [10]. However, more expertise in determining outcomes and the lack of
recent approaches to understanding health and consistent support for the utility of the GCS in
disability view outcome from a functional perspec- predicting outcomes, it was hypothesized that
tive and include quality of life as an important aspect severity of injury classifications based on clinical
of functioning. Although quality of life can be expertise (Clinical Team Severity Rating) would
defined in a number of ways, it typically involves account for more variance in long-term social
being an active and contributing member of society participation outcomes than injury severity ratings
including at least some level of involvement in social based on either standardized test scores
activities [40]. As such, it is important to consider an (Standardized Test Severity Rating) or GCS scores
individual’s level of participation in social contexts (Glasgow Coma Scale Severity Rating). Similarly, it
when examining outcomes. Unfortunately, social was hypothesized that the Clinical Team Severity
competence is also an area in which survivors Rating (CTSR) would account for more variance in
of paediatric brain injury tend to do poorly. This long-term cognitive outcomes than either the
difficulty is due primarily to the cognitive and Standardized Test Severity Rating (STSR) or the
behavioural challenges that TBI survivors face, Glasgow Coma Scale Severity Rating (GCSR).
including difficulties with memory and language as Additionally, it was hypothesized that age at injury
well as their impaired ability to inhibit inappropriate and environmental factors would account for a large
behaviours. Unfortunately, individuals who have portion of the variance not accounted for by severity
sustained a paediatric TBI continue to face restric- of injury in the long-term social participation and
tions in their social participation for years following cognitive outcomes of paediatric brain injury.
injury [40]. Areas of social participation include
peer socialization, family activities such as decision-
making, structured community activities such as Method
sports leagues and school-based activities with other
children in the classroom. In their study of 60 Participants
school-aged children up to 6.7 years post-injury, Participants were 30 pairs, each consisting of a
Bedell and Dumas [40] found that 73% of their parent (27 female) and his/her child (12 female) who
sample experienced restrictions in social participa- had sustained a TBI between birth and 10 years of
tion in at least one area of participation. Other age (mean age at injury ¼ 5.32 years, SD ¼ 2.75
investigations focusing on individuals with develop- years). Each child was a current or former patient
mental disabilities or acquired brain injuries have at a Paediatric ABI Programme in Southeastern
employed the AIMS Interview which examines Ontario. The time since injury ranged from 3.92–
social participation from an acculturation perspec- 16.61 years (mean time since injury ¼ 10.05,
tive (i.e. identifying disability-related needs requiring SD ¼ 2.93 years).
support in order to promote community participa-
tion) [41–44]. More specifically, the AIMS examines
Measures
whether individuals’ needs are being identified and/
or supported in a way that promotes community Archival data. A review of consenting patients’
involvement. In a study of children and adolescents medical records allowed for the collection of data
with acquired brain injuries, Alvarez et al. [41] found obtained at the time of injury including available
that only 64% of the sample of 28 children and GCS, psychological test data including standardized
adolescents had their needs identified and sup- scores on memory tests, physiotherapy data consist-
ported. In a similar study of 19 children and ing of scores on the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of
adolescents with TBI, Vilela et al. [44] found that Motor Proficiency and occupational therapy data
although 74% of the sample was participating in consisting of scores on the Beery Developmental
social activities, only 16% of the sample had their Test of Visual-Motor Integration.
social participation needs identified and supported. Glasgow Coma Scale Severity Ratings (GCSR)
Additionally, 26% of the sample did not have their were assigned based on the first available GCS score
needs identified and were not participating in social obtained through the review of medical files. Based
activities. on the protocol established by Teasdale and Jennett
One goal of the present study was to determine the [26] and employed by numerous researchers, indi-
efficacy of additive models employing the classifica- viduals were classified as having a mild brain injury
tion of severity of injury based on either GCS scores, when their score was between 13–15. A score of
a combination of standardized test scores or clinical 9–12 was considered to indicate a moderate brain
expertise in predicting functional outcomes injury and a score of 8 or less was classified as a
Predicting social and functional outcomes for individuals sustaining paediatric TBI 15

severe brain injury. Although only 15 participants level of internal consistency (Cronbach’s ¼ 0.82)
had actual GCS scores recorded in their files, the and good test–re-test reliability (r ¼ 0.89).
other 15 participants were not assessed using the The Clinical Team Severity Ratings (CTSR) were
GCS because their injuries did not warrant assess- determined within 1 year of the injury by the ABI
ment. These were cases in which there was no loss Programme team, which included a physician,
of consciousness or memory disturbance noted. a psychologist, a physiotherapist, an occupational
Thus, for the purposes of the present study, these therapist and a speech-language pathologist. To
participants were deemed to have had a GCS score determine severity of injury, each team member
of 15 and were classified as having mild injuries. independently rated the patient according to the
Psychological, occupational therapy and physical severity of the team member’s unique area of
therapy assessment data collected within 1 year of function (i.e. cognitive, language, gross motor,
the date of injury were combined to determine the fine motor/skills of independent living, behaviour).
Standardized Test Severity Rating (STSR). More Severity was interpreted in the context of a perceived
specifically, based on clinical practices of members change in function from estimated pre-accident
of the ABI Programme team, standardized test levels. The team then met together to review the
scores falling more than 2 SD below the mean individual ratings and to achieve a consensual rating
indicated impairment in the area being measured of severity for each patient. Thus, the CTSR were
(i.e. intellectual functioning, gross motor ability and partially based on the standardized test scores, used
fine motor ability). Furthermore, a lack of impair- to determine STSR, as well as GCS scores; however,
ment in any area was classified as a mild injury, additional test data and the clinicians’ expertise in
impairment in any one area was classified as a the area of brain injury were also used to provide
moderate injury and impairment in two or more additional information that these scores alone might
areas was classified as a severe injury. not provide.
Memory is one of the most commonly reported
deficits associated with TBI [36] and standardized
Current data. An interview was conducted with one
memory tests were among the most consistently used
parent of each of the children included in the study.
measures during the psychological assessment of the The interview was completed over the telephone
children and adolescents in this study. Impairment following the collection of archival data from patient
of intellectual functioning was therefore determined medical records. Questionnaires administered in
using the ‘general memory’ standardized score from the telephone interview included the Demographic
either the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Questionnaire, the Child and Adolescent Scale of
Learning (WRAML) [45] or the Children’s Memory Environment (CASE), the Child and Adolescent
Scale (CMS) [46]. Psychometric testing of both Factors Inventory (CAFI) and the Child and
measures indicated a high level of internal consis- Adolescent Scale of Participation (CASP) [49].
tency (WRAML: Cronbach’s ¼ 0.93; CMS: Demographic data collected included the age and
Spearman-Brown split half reliability ¼ 0.91) and sex of the brain injury survivor and parent, the date
good test–re-test reliability (WRAML: mean interval and type of injury, the annual household income and
49 days; r ¼ 0.81; CMS: mean interval 60 days; the parent’s highest level of education completed.
r ¼ 0.84). The Child and Adolescent Scale of Environment
Standardized scores from the Bruininks-Oseretsky (CASE) consists of 18 items pertaining to problems
Test of Motor Proficiency (BOTMP) [47] were used experienced with physical, social and attitudinal
to determine impairment in participants’ gross motor aspects of the environment as well as problems
functioning and standardized scores on the Beery related to the quality and availability of services or
Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration assistance that children with disabilities may need
(VMI) [48] were used to determine fine motor [49]. The CASE was developed as an inventory
impairment, as they were the most commonly of related and unrelated items pertaining to the
reported scores in the physiotherapy and occupa- environment. A factor analysis conducted by
tional therapy reports, respectively. For the purposes G. Bedell (personal communication, 20 June 2006)
of the present study, the gross motor composite score identified six factors in the CASE (school support/
as measured by the physiotherapist was used to assess attitudes, home and community support/attitudes,
gross motor impairment. Psychometric testing of the family stress/finances, physical design of buildings,
BOTMP indicated a high level of internal consistency crime/violence and transportation) accounting for
(Cronbach’s ¼ 0.86). The fine motor composite 79% of the variance explained (Cronbach’s
VMI score as measured by the occupational therapist ¼ 0.86). For the present study, only those items
was used to determine fine motor impairment. related to school, home and community support
Psychometric testing of the VMI indicated a high and attitudes as well as family stress and finances
16 R. Wells et al.

factors (i.e. Questions 4–7, 9, 10 and 12–15) were All questionnaires were scored according to the
included in the analyses, as the remaining factors procedures determined by the original authors.
were not highly endorsed by participants. Questions CASP, CAFI and CASE summary scores were
are worded ‘Does your child currently experience calculated by adding the scores from each applicable
any problems with . . .’. Each item is rated on a 3- item and dividing by the total maximum possible
point scale: no problem (1), little problem (2) or big score based on the number of items rated. These
problem (3). Additionally, each item has a ‘not numbers were then multiplied by 100 to conform to
applicable’ response option. Psychometric testing a 0–100 point scale. Higher CASP scores indicate a
of the CASE indicates a high level of internal greater degree of age-expected participation, higher
consistency (Cronbach’s ¼ 0.91). CAFI scores indicate more child-related problems, a
The Child and Adolescent Scale of Participation greater impact of problems or a combination of the
(CASP) assesses social functioning of children and two and higher CASE scores reflect more environ-
youth within the context of the WHO ICF [49]. ment problems, a greater impact of problems or
The CASP consists of 20 items pertaining to home, a combination of the two.
school and community life for children and youth
[40, 49]. Questions are worded ‘Compared to other
children your child’s age, what is your child’s current Results
level of participation in . . .?’ and include items
pertaining to social, play or leisure activities, family Demographic data indicated that 25 participants
chores, academic activities and money management. were living in a 2-parent home with annual house-
Each item is rated on a 4-point scale: age expected hold incomes ranging from $15,000–$200,000
(4), somewhat limited (3), very limited (2) or with a mean of $68,965.52 (SD ¼ $40,247.87).
unable (1). Additionally, each item has a ‘not Ten parents had completed high school, 12 had
applicable’ response option. A factor analysis con- completed college, three had obtained an under-
ducted by G. Bedell (personal communication, graduate degree and one had obtained a graduate
20 June 2006) identified two factors (social/leisure/ degree. Only four parents had not completed high
communication (SLC) and activities of daily living/ school.
personal care) accounting for 68% of the variance Table I provides the overall means and standard
explained (Cronbach’s ¼ 0.96). As the purpose of deviations for all continuous variables as well as the
the current study was examining social participation, means and standard deviations for each continuous
only those questions pertaining to the first factor, variable broken down by injury severity within each
SLC (i.e. questions 1, 2, 6–8, 10, 12 and 15), were severity of injury rating method: Glasgow Coma
included in the analyses. Psychometric testing of the Scale Severity Rating (GCSR), Standardized Test
CASP indicates a high level of internal consistency Severity Rating (STSR) and Clinical Team Severity
(Cronbach’s ¼ 0.98). Rating (CTSR). One Way ANOVAs revealed no
The Child and Adolescent Factors Inventory significant group differences between mean scores
(CAFI) assesses cognitive and behavioural function- on any of the measures.
ing of children and youth within the context of the In calculating the STSR it was determined that
WHO ICF [49]. The CAFI consists of 15 items 24 children experienced impairments in cognitive
pertaining to impairments and child-related factors functioning, 22 experienced impairments in gross
that are typically of concern for family caregivers of motor functioning and 26 experienced impairments
children with TBI [40, 49]. The CAFI was devel- in fine motor functioning. An examination of CASP
oped as an inventory of related and unrelated items scores revealed that 80% of the sample was not
pertaining to cognitive and behavioural functioning. participating in age-expected levels of social partici-
A factor analysis conducted by G. Bedell (personal pation across settings (i.e. home, school or commu-
communication, 20 June 2006) identified three nity). Additionally, 100% of the sample was found
factors (cognitive/behavioural/speech (CBS), move- to be experiencing difficulties in two or more areas
ment/vision/health and hearing) accounting for of cognitive functioning (CAFI), with memory and
62% of the variance explained (Cronbach’s learning being the most commonly reported diffi-
¼ 0.86). For the present study, only items pertain- culties. Similarly, 100% of the sample reported
ing to CBS (i.e. questions 1–7) were included in experiencing problems with support and attitudes
the analyses, including problems with attention in the environment (CASE) with the most common
and concentration, motivation and judgement. difficulties being attitudes of people at school and
Questions are worded ‘Does your child currently in the community.
have any difficulty . . .’. Each item is rated on a A correlational analysis was conducted to examine
3-point scale: no problem (1), little problem (2) or the relationships between the dependent variables
big problem (3). (Child and Adolescent Scale of Participation (CASP)
Predicting social and functional outcomes for individuals sustaining paediatric TBI 17

Table I. Means and standard deviations of dependent and independent variables.

Injury age (years) CASE CASP CAFI

n M SD M SD M SD M SD

Overall sample 30 5.32 2.75 44.72 13.92 88.89 10.73 60.56 15.81
GCSR
Mild 17 5.09 2.58 45.58 15.90 90.07 10.55 58.82 16.59
Moderate 3 7.82 2.51 38.89 9.62 91.67 11.83 65.28 15.77
Severe 10 4.95 2.97 45.00 11.92 86.04 11.30 62.08 15.65
STSR
Mild 20 5.27 2.43 45.42 15.17 90.31 10.43 58.33 17.36
Moderate 4 7.60 1.20 45.83 14.43 87.50 7.22 68.75 7.98
Severe 6 3.95 3.76 41.67 10.54 85.07 13.99 62.50 13.69
CTSR
Mild 8 4.72 1.02 43.75 13.18 93.36 6.98 53.13 18.33
Moderate 11 5.41 2.75 42.42 12.05 90.06 9.46 59.85 15.40
Severe 11 5.67 3.33 47.73 16.70 84.47 13.09 66.67 12.91

Table II. Correlations among dependent and independent age at injury was entered in the first block; a rating
variables (n ¼ 30). of severity of injury (GCSR, STSR or CTSR) was
CASP CAFI entered in the second block and environment
(CASE) scores were entered in the third block.
GCSR 0.17 0.10 Current environment was used as an independent
STSR 0.20 0.15
CTSR 0.34 0.34
variable as it is a factor external to the child and
CASE 0.64** 0.55** environment has been shown to impact on children’s
Number of parents in home 0.05 0.06 functioning [37]. Similarly, as the focus of the
Household income 0.04 0.12 present study was examining children’s functional
Parent education 0.13 0.10
outcomes, social participation and cognitive func-
Child age at injury 0.37* 0.15
Time since injury 0.16 0.03 tioning were used as dependent variables. The
outcome variable was dependent on the hypothesis
**Significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed); *Significant at the in question and was either social participation
0.05 level (2-tailed).
(CASP) scores or cognitive functioning (CAFI)
scores.
and Child and Adolescent Factors Inventory (CAFI))
and the independent variables (Child and Adolescent
Scale of Environment (CASE), Glasgow Coma Scale Severity of injury ratings as predictors
Severity Rating (GCSR), Standardized Test Severity of social participation
Rating (STSR), Clinical Team Severity Rating
(CTSR) and age at injury). The results of this ana- It was hypothesized that Clinical Team Severity
lysis are presented in Table II. Results indicated that Rating (CTSR) would account for more variance
age at injury was positively correlated with CASP in social participation than the Standardized Test
scores. Thus, younger age at injury was related to Severity Rating (STSR) and Glasgow Coma Scale
lower current social participation. Additionally, Severity Rating (GCSR). Forced entry hierarchical
CASE scores were negatively correlated with CASP regression analyses were conducted with Child and
scores and positively correlated with CAFI scores. Adolescent Scale of Participation (CASP) scores
More specifically, better environments were related entered as the dependent variable. R2 change scores
to higher levels of social participation and lower for the GCSR (R2 ¼ 0.028, p ¼ 0.347) and STSR
levels of cognitive difficulty. Conversely, parent (R2 ¼ 0.026, p ¼ 0.369) were non-significant (see
education, household income and injury severity Tables III and IV). The R2 change score for CTSR
ratings were not significantly related to either of the (R2 ¼ 0.153, p ¼ 0.023), however, was significant,
outcome measures. However, there were trends with with CTSR accounting for 15.3% of the variance in
respect to the CTSR with both CASP and CAFI social participation outcomes (see Table V). Further
such that greater severity of injury was associated examination of the regression equation revealed that
with lower levels of social participation and higher age at injury was acting as a suppressor. That is, even
levels of cognitive functioning difficulties. though the bivariate correlation between CTSR and
Forced entry hierarchical regression analyses were CASP was non-significant, the inclusion of age at
conducted to test all hypotheses. In each analysis, injury removed some of the error variance in CTSR
18 R. Wells et al.

Table III. Social participation as predicted by injury age, Glasgow coma scale severity rating (GCSR)
and child and adolescent scale of environment (CASE).

Model Predictor B R2 R2 p

1 Injury age 89.181 0.138 0.138 0.043 0.372


2 Injury age 84.578 0.166 0.028 0.347 0.372
GCSR 0.168
3 Injury age 107.838 0.519 0.353 <0.001 0.270
GCSR 0.187
CASE 0.603

R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by the variable entered
on that block. R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by all variables
entered to that point.

Table IV. Social participation as predicted by injury age, standardized test severity rating (STSR) and
child and adolescent scale of environment (CASE).

Model Predictor B R2 R2 p

1 Injury age 81.181 0.138 0.138 0.043 0.372


2 Injury age 84.809 0.164 0.026 0.369 0.353
STSR 0.162
3 Injury age 110.182 0.538 0.374 <0.001 0.240
STSR 0.236
CASE 0.625

R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by the variable entered
on that block. R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by all variables
entered to that point.

Table V. Social participation as predicted by injury age, clinical team severity rating (CTSR) and child
and adolescent scale of environment (CASE).

Model Predictor B R2 R2 p

1 Injury age 81.181 0.138 0.138 0.043 0.372


2 Injury age 91.160 0.291 0.153 0.023 0.425
CTSR 0.395
3 Injury age 109.900 0.577 0.286 <0.001 0.321
CTSR 0.312
CASE 0.549

R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by the variable entered
on that block. R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by all variables
entered to that point.

that was not correlated with CASP scores, thereby dependent variable. Again, R2 change scores for
increasing the multiple correlation in this model. the GCSR (R2 ¼ 0.011, p ¼ 0.585) and STSR
(R2 ¼ 0.018, p ¼ 0.484) were non-significant (see
Tables VI and VII); however, the R2 change score
Severity of injury ratings as predictors of long-term
for the CTSR (R2 ¼ 0.135, p ¼ 0.048) was signifi-
cognitive outcomes
cant, accounting for 13.5% of the variance in CAFI
It was hypothesized that Clinical Team Severity scores (see Table VIII). Further examination of the
Rating (CTSR) would account for more variance in regression equation revealed that age at injury was
long-term cognitive outcomes than the Standardized acting as a suppressor. As with the CASP model,
Test Severity Rating (STSR) and Glasgow Coma even though the bivariate correlation between CTSR
Scale Severity Rating (GCSR), R2 change scores and CAFI was non-significant, the inclusion of age
from the first block (age at injury) to the second at injury increased the multiple correlation in the
block (severity rating) were compared for each model, suggesting that age at injury was suppressing
rating system when Child and Adolescent Factors some of the error variance in CTSR that was not
Inventory (CAFI) scores were entered as the correlated with CAFI scores.
Predicting social and functional outcomes for individuals sustaining paediatric TBI 19

Table VI. Cognitive functioning as predicted by injury age, Glasgow coma scale severity rating (GCSR)
and child and adolescent scale of environment (CASE).

Model Predictor B R2 R2 p

1 Injury age 64.986 0.021 0.021 0.445 0.145


2 Injury age 61.868 0.032 0.011 0.585 0.145
GCSR 0.105
3 Injury age 30.885 0.320 0.288 0.003 0.053
GCSR 0.122
CASE 0.545

R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by the variable entered
on that block. R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by all variables
entered to that point.

Table VII. Cognitive functioning as predicted by injury age, standardized test severity rating (STSR)
and child and adolescent scale of environment (CASE).

Model Predictor B R2 R2 p

1 Injury age 64.986 0.021 0.021 0.445 0.145


2 Injury age 60.536 0.039 0.018 0.484 0.130
STSR 0.135
3 Injury age 26.728 0.345 0.306 0.002 0.027
STSR 0.201
CASE 0.565

R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by the variable entered
on that block. R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by all variables
entered to that point.

Table VIII. Cognitive functioning as predicted by injury age, clinical team severity rating (CTSR) and
child and adolescent scale of environment (CASE).

Model Predictor B R2 R2 p

1 Injury age 64.986 0.021 0.021 0.445 0.145


2 Injury age 51.201 0.156 0.135 0.48 0.195
CTSR 0.370
3 Injury age 26.265 0.389 0.233 0.004 0.101
CTSR 0.295
CASE 0.496

R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by the variable entered
on that block. R2 reflects the amount of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by all variables
entered to that point.

Models to predict social participation For the STSR (Table IV), adding severity of injury
in the second block resulted in a non-significant
It was hypothesized that age at injury and Child and
R2 change score; however, adding CASE scores in
Adolescent Scale of Environment (CASE) scores
the third block accounted for an additional 37.4%
would account for a large portion of the variance in
of the variance in CASP scores.
Child and Adolescent Scale of Participation (CASP)
Finally, for the model employing CTSR
scores not accounted for by severity of injury ratings.
(Table V), adding severity of injury ratings in the
As can be seen in Tables III–V, age at injury added
second block accounted for an additional 15.3% of
in the first block uniquely accounted for 13.8%
the variance in CASP scores. Additionally, adding
of the variance in CASP scores, regardless of which
CASE scores in the third block accounted for an
severity of injury rating system was employed.
additional 28.6% of the variance in CASP scores.
However, as can be seen in Table III, for the
model employing the GCSR, adding the severity
Models to predict cognitive functioning
of injury rating in the second block and CASE scores
in the third block resulted in non-significant R2 It was hypothesized that age at injury and Child and
change scores for both. Adolescent Scale of Environment (CASE) scores
20 R. Wells et al.

would account for a large portion of the variance cognitive outcomes, do not support reliance on a
in Child and Adolescent Factors Inventory (CAFI) single GCS score as a valid predictor of future social
scores not accounted for by severity of injury ratings. participation and cognitive outcome for children.
As can be seen in Tables VI–VIII, age at injury Findings from the present study also support
added in the first block did not significantly account the idea that outcomes are not merely the product
for any of the variance in CAFI scores, regardless of the severity of the primary physical injury itself,
of the severity of injury rating system employed. but that additional factors play a role in determining
For the GCSR model (Table VI), adding severity long-term functional outcomes [4, 13]. For example,
of injury ratings in the second block resulted in a age at injury appears to play an important role in
non-significant R2 change score. However, adding predicting outcomes. This makes sense when con-
CASE scores in the third block accounted for 28.6% sidering that child and adolescent brains are still
of the variance. developing at the time of injury. Thus, the injury
Similarly, for the STSR model (Table VII), disrupts both current abilities and ongoing develop-
adding severity of injury ratings in the second ment. As the findings of the present study suggest,
block resulted in a non-significant R2 change score; the older children are when they sustain their brain
however, adding CASE scores in the third block injury, the fewer restrictions they experience in social
accounted for 30.6% of the variance. participation. There are several reasons that this may
Conversely, for the CTSR model (Table VIII), be the case. First, a child who sustains an injury
adding severity of injury ratings in the second block at an older age has had more of an opportunity to
accounted for an additional 13.5% of the variance learn appropriate social behaviours than a toddler
in CAFI scores. Furthermore, adding CASE scores or infant who sustains an injury. Although some
in the third block accounted for an additional 23.3% of the social skills may be lost or impaired, the older
of the variance in CASP scores. child may still retain some knowledge and/or skill
related to social conduct, whereas the toddler or
infant would have to learn these skills. Additionally,
Discussion the acquisition of social skills may be negatively
affected by the cognitive and behavioural impair-
The purpose of the present study was two-fold: The ments typically associated with TBI. More specifi-
first goal was to examine the amount of variance in cally, children with TBI often experience both
functional outcomes accounted for by each of three expressive and receptive language delays [22]. For
methods of classifying severity of injury: GCS scores example, children with TBI may have difficulty
(GCSR), Standardized Test scores (STSR) and interpreting words with more than one meaning or
Clinical Team ratings (CTSR). The second goal may have difficulty communicating their thoughts.
was to determine the efficacy of additive models, They also frequently experience difficulty controlling
each employing one of the three methods of impulsive behaviour, which could lead to difficulties
classification, in predicting long-term social and in social situations [23].
cognitive outcomes associated with paediatric TBI. In addition to age at injury, the environment in
The results of this study indicated that clinical which the child lives appears to be related to social
team ratings accounted for more variance in both participation and cognitive functioning. That is, if
social and cognitive outcomes than more standar- a child is being reared in a nurturing, supportive
dized ratings (i.e. GCSR or STSR). This finding environment and is accepted by members of the
is important in light of the evidence that decisions community they are more likely to be receiving the
regarding the allocation of services and supports support necessary to participate in age-appropriate
following an injury are often made in the early stages social activities as well as assistance with improving
of recovery and are predicated on the basis of cognitive functioning that may have been impaired
severity of injury. This finding also lends support through the brain injury.
to the recent move toward employing clinicians’ Consistent with the findings of Bedell and
expertise in predicting health care outcomes [39]. Dumas [40], 80% of the participants in the present
Furthermore, despite the fact that the GCS was study experienced at least some level of restriction
developed to be used serially in adult populations in their social participation, with the most common
to track the depth and duration of coma and restrictions being faced in social activities with same-
impaired consciousness following an injury to the aged peers. Additionally, all participants reported
brain, single GCS scores have consistently been used experiencing some level of cognitive impairment,
as the measure of severity of injury in both treatment with motivation and judgement being the most
planning and research with children [13]. However, frequently reported problems. These findings lend
the results of the present study, in which GCS support to the call for increased efforts in addressing
scores did not account for any variance in social or social participation needs and cognitive functioning
Predicting social and functional outcomes for individuals sustaining paediatric TBI 21

in individuals who have sustained a TBI [40]. of severity of injury, there are a number of variables
As expected, mean social participation scores thought to be involved in outcomes that could
decreased as the severity of injury based on clinical not be assessed. For example, research has shown
team ratings increased. Similarly, when examining that it is important to consider data related to an
severity of injury based on clinical team ratings, individual’s pre-injury functioning as it permits more
individuals experienced greater frequency and/or accurate measures of the effects of injury by allowing
intensity of cognitive difficulties when their injuries an opportunity to control for pre-existing confounds
were rated as more severe. However, this was not the [4, 52]. As the present study relied on the availability
case when severity of injury was based on Glasgow of data collected at the time of (or shortly after)
Coma Scale scores or standardized test ratings, injury there was no opportunity to gather valid and
suggesting that the addition of clinical expertise reliable data regarding pre-injury functioning.
may provide a better indication of future outcomes. Similarly, the present study employed a measure
of the current environment. Although current
Limitations and future directions environment is an important factor to be considered
when examining issues in social participation, several
This type of study has not been conducted pre-
studies have reported significant environmental
viously and provides a foundation for future
changes, including divorce and income loss, within
research. The findings are interesting but prelimin-
the months and years following TBI [38]. Thus,
ary and therefore must be interpreted with caution.
including a measure of environment at or shortly
Additionally, there are several limitations that must
after injury may allow for the development of a
be taken into consideration when interpreting the
more predictive model of outcome following TBI.
results.
Measuring social participation and environmental
change over time might allow for a more in depth
Sample size and power. All participants in this study understanding of factors that contribute to children
were recruited from the same treatment facility and and adolescents’ engagement in social activities.
live within a small geographical area. Furthermore,
participants were from a population that had actively
sought information and resources to assist them in Conclusion
the rehabilitation process. Unfortunately, there are
many other families living with the effects of TBI Overall the findings of the present study offer
who are unaware of the resources available to assist preliminary support for the idea that a single GCS
them. Thus, the sample may not be representative score or even a given set of neuropsychological tests
of the general population of children and adolescents do not reliably predict outcomes for children and
with TBI. Additionally, only 43% of the potential adolescents who sustain a TBI. Rather, it appears
sample was included in the analyses. It is possible necessary to apply clinical expertise in the context
that those individuals who declined participation of a biopsychosocial model in order to determine
were feeling overwhelmed by their current situation how to provide supports and resources that will
and participation in this study may have been allow for the best possible quality of life following
perceived as an additional burden that they did a paediatric TBI.
not need.
Although the sample size is comparable to other
Declaration of interest: The authors report no
clinical research samples in the TBI research
conflicts of interest. The authors alone are respon-
literature [50, 51] the sample size was small
sible for the content and writing of the paper.
(n ¼ 30), yielding relatively low power (0.713). It is
possible, therefore, that smaller effects were not
detected. Obtaining larger, truly representative
samples is difficult given the nature of research; References
however, future research should attempt to obtain 1. Bond Chapman S, McKinnon L. Discussion of developmental
larger samples and include those individuals not plasticity: Factors affecting cognitive outcome after pediatric
receiving information and support as well as those traumatic brain injury. Journal of Communication Disorders
individuals who are doing well but believe they have 2000;33:333–334.
2. Brooks BM, Rose FD, Johnson DA, Andrews TK,
nothing to offer to the study.
Gulamali R. Support for children following traumatic brain
injury: The views of educational psychologists. Disability and
Rehabilitation 2003;25:51–56.
Design issues (i.e. archival data). Although data 3. Gil AM. Neurocognitive outcomes following pediatric
regarding the injury and impairments provided a brain injury: A developmental approach. Journal of School
basis for examining differences in the classification Psychology 2003;41:337–353.
22 R. Wells et al.

4. Taylor HG, Alden J. Age-related differences in outcomes 22. Leahy LF, Holland AL, Frattalli CM. Persistent deficits
following childhood brain insults: An introduction and following closed-head injury. Journal of Clinical and
overview. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Experimental Neuropsychology 1987;9:34–41.
Society 1997;3:555–567. 23. Light R, Asarnow R, Satz P, Zaucha K, McCleary C,
5. Beaulieu CL. Rehabilitation and outcome following pediatric Lewis R. Mild closed-head injury in children and adolescents:
traumatic brain injury. Surgical Clinics of North America Behavior problems and academic outcomes. Journal of
2002;82:1–14. Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1998;66:1023–1029.
6. Cassidy LD, Potoka DA, Adelson PD, Ford HR. 24. Winogron HW, Knights RM, Bawden HN. Neuropsycho-
Development of a novel method to predict disability after logical deficits following head injury in children. Journal of
head trauma in children. Journal of Pediatric Surgery Clinical Neuropsychology 1984;6:269–286.
2003;38:482–485. 25. Degeneffe CE. Family caregiving and traumatic brain injury.
7. Braga LW, Da Paz AC, Ylvisaker M. Direct clinician- Health and Social Work 2001;26:257–269.
delivered versus indirect family-supported rehabilitation of 26. Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment of coma and impaired
children with traumatic brain injury: A randomized con- consciousness: A practical scale. Lancet 1974;2:81–83.
trolled trial. Brain Injury 2005;19:819–831. 27. Lieh-Lai MW, Theodorou AA, Sarnaik AP. Limitations of
8. Ewing-Cobbs L, Fletcher JM, Levin HS, Francis DJ, the Glasgow Coma Scale in predicting outcome in children
Davidson K, Miner ME. Longitudinal neuropsychological with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Pediatrics 1992;120:
outcome in infants and preschoolers with traumatic brain 195–199.
injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological 28. Coppens P. Subpopulations in closed-head injury:
Society 1997;3:581–591. Preliminary results. Brain Injury 1995;9:195–208.
9. Miller LJ, Donders J. Prediction of educational outcome after 29. Gupta A, Ghai D. Memory and information processing
pediatric traumatic brain injury. Rehabilitation Psychology capacity after closed head injury. Indian Journal of Clinical
2003;48:237–241. Psychology 1996;23:170–177.
10. Taylor HG, Yeates KO, Wade SL, Drotar D, Stancin T, 30. McMillan TM, Jongen EL, Greenwood RJ, Assessment
Minich N. A prospective study of short- and long-term of post-traumatic amnesia after severe closed head injury:
outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children: Behavior Retrospective or prospective? Journal of Neurology,
and achievement. Neuropsychology 2002;16:15–27. Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 1996;60:422–427.
11. Slomine BS, Gerring JP, Grados MA, Vasa R, Brady KD, 31. Nelson LD, Adams KM. Challenges for neuropsychology in
Christensen JR, Denckla MB. Performance on measures of the treatment and rehabilitation of brain-injured patients.
‘executive function’ following pediatric traumatic brain Psychological Assessment 1997;9:368–373.
32. Vilkki J, Ahola K, Holst P, Öhman J. Prediction of
injury. Brain Injury 2002;16:759–772.
psychosocial recovery after head injury with cognitive tests
12. Wassenberg R, Max JE, Lindgren SD, Schatz A. Sustained
and neurobehavioral ratings. Journal of Clinical &
attention in children and adolescents after traumatic brain
Experimental Neuropsychology 1994;16:325–338.
injury: Relation to severity of injury, adaptive functioning,
33. Grewal M, Sutcliffe AJ. Early prediction of outcome
ADHD and social background. Brain Injury 2004;18:
following head injury in children: An assessment of the
751–764.
value of Glasgow Coma Scale score trend and abnormal
13. Satz P, Zaucha K, McCleary C, Light R. Mild head injury
plantar and papillary light reflexes. Journal of Pediatric
in children and adolescents: A review of studies (1970–1995).
Surgery 1991;26:1161–1163.
Psychological Bulletin 1997;122:107–131.
34. Hackbarth RM, Rzeszutko KM, Sturm G, Donders J,
14. Brown G, Chadwick O, Shaffer P, Rutter M, Traub M.
Kuldanek AS, Sanfilippo DJ. Survival and functional out-
A prospective study of children with head injuries.
come in pediatric brain injury: A retrospective review and
Psychological Medicine 1981;11:63–78.
analysis of predictive factors. Critical Care Medicine
15. Horowitz I, Costeff H, Sadan N, Abraham E, Geyer S,
2002;30:1630–1635.
Najenson T. Childhood head injuries in Israel: Epidemiology
35. Levin HS. Predicting the neurobehavioral sequelae of closed
and outcome. International Rehabilitation Medicine 1983;
head injury. In: Wood RL, editor. Neurobehavioural sequelae
4:32–36.
of traumatic brain injury. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis;
16. Segalowitz SJ, Brown D. Mild head injury as a source of
1990. pp 89–109.
developmental disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities
36. Donders J, Nesbit-Greene K. Predictors of neuropsychologi-
1991;24:551–559. cal test performance after pediatric traumatic brain injury.
17. Anderson V, Catroppa C, Morse S, Haritou F, Rosenfeld J. Assessment 2004;11:275–284.
Outcome from mild head injury in young children: A 37. Hawley CA. Behaviour and school performance after brain
prospective study. Journal of Clinical & Experimental injury. Brain Injury 2004;18:645–659.
Neuropsychology 2001;23:705–717. 38. Kinsella G, Ong B, Murtagh D, Prior M, Sawyer M. The
18. Bijur PE, Haslum M, Golding J. Cognitive and behavioural role of the family for behavioral outcome in children and
sequelae of mild head injury in children. Pediatrics adolescents following traumatic brain injury. Journal of
1990;86:337–344. Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1999;67:116–123.
19. Butterbaugh G. Traumatic brain injuries in adolescents: 39. American Psychological Association. Evidence-based practice
A psychosocial and neuropsychological review. In: Singer M, in psychology. American Psychologist 2006;61:271–285.
Singer L, Anglin T, editors. Assessing adolescents at risk. 40. Bedell GM, Dumas HM. Social participation of children and
Lexington: Lexington Publishing; 1993. pp.293–332. youth with acquired brain injuries discharged from inpatient
20. Casey R, Ludwig S, McCormick MC. Morbidity rehabilitation: A follow-up study. Brain Injury 2004;
following minor head trauma in children. Pediatrics 1986; 18:65–82.
78:497–502. 41. Alvarez A, Benn K, Minnes P. Community integration of
21. Knights RM, Ivan LP, Ventureyra EC, Bentivoglio C, children with developmental disabilities and children with
Stoddart C, Winogron W, Bawden HN. The effects of head acquired brain injury: Caregivers’ perspectives. Poster pre-
injury in children on neuropsychological and behavioural sented at Ontario Association on Developmental Disabilities
functioning. Brain Injury 1991;5:339–351. Conference 2003; Barrie, ON.
Predicting social and functional outcomes for individuals sustaining paediatric TBI 23

42. Minnes PM, Buell KB, Nolte ML, McColl MA, Carlson P, 48. Beery KE. The Beery developmental test of visual-motor
Johnston J. Defining community integration of persons integration, 4th ed. New Jersey: Modern Curriculum
with brain injuries as acculturation: A Canadian perspective. Press; 1997.
Neurorehabilitation 2001;14:1–8. 49. Bedell GM. Developing a follow-up survey focused on
43. Solish A, Minnes P, Kupferschmidt A. Community integra- participation of children and youth with acquired brain
tion of children with developmental disabilities in social injuries after discharge from inpatient rehabilitation.
activities. Journal on Developmental Disabilities 2003; NeuroRehabilitation 2004;19:191–205.
10:115–122. 50. McAllister TW, Sparling MB, Flashman LA, Guerin SJ,
44. Vilela T, Phillips M, Minnes P. Is the grass necessarily Mamourian AC, Saykin SJ. Differential working memory
greener? What families of children with ABI tell us about load effects after mild traumatic brain injury. NeuroImage
stress and coping with and without access to third party 2001;14:1004–1012.
funding. Paper presented at the Fifth InterUrban Brain Injury 51. Watt S, Shores EA, Kinoshita S. Effects of reducing
Conference 2005; Peterborough, ON. attentional resources on implicit and explicit memory after
45. Sheslow D, Adams W. Wide range assessment of memory severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 1999;13:
and learning. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates; 1990. 338–349.
46. Cohen MJ. Children’s memory scale (CMS). San Antonio, 52. Arroyos-Jurado E, Paulsen JS, Merrell KW, Lindgren SD,
TX: Psychological Corporation; 1997. Max JE. Traumatic brain injury in school-age children:
47. Bruininks RH. Bruininks-Oseretksy test of motor proficiency. Academic and social outcome. Journal of School Psychology
Minnesota: American Guidance Service; 1978. 2000;38:571–587.

You might also like