You are on page 1of 9

Reviews in Clinical Gerontology

http://journals.cambridge.org/RCG

Additional services for Reviews in Clinical Gerontology:

Email alerts: Click here


Subscriptions: Click here
Commercial reprints: Click here
Terms of use : Click here

Measuring loneliness in later life: a comparison of differing


measures

Christina Victor, Linda Grenade and Duncan Boldy

Reviews in Clinical Gerontology / Volume 15 / Issue 01 / February 2005, pp 63 - 70


DOI: 10.1017/S0959259805001723, Published online: 14 February 2006

Link to this article: http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0959259805001723

How to cite this article:


Christina Victor, Linda Grenade and Duncan Boldy (2005). Measuring loneliness in later life: a
comparison of differing measures. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 15, pp 63-70 doi:10.1017/
S0959259805001723

Request Permissions : Click here

Downloaded from http://journals.cambridge.org/RCG, IP address: 205.175.97.70 on 28 Mar 2015


Reviews in Clinical Gerontology 2005 15; 63–70
C 2006 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom doi:10.1017/S0959259805001723

Measuring loneliness in later life: a comparison


of differing measures
Christina Victor
School of Health and Social Care, University of Reading, UK

Linda Grenade and Duncan Boldy


Division of Health Sciences, Curtin University of Technology, Perth, Western Australia

Introduction of life in old age, currently a key policy objective


for older people.3 There is concern to promote
The social environment is one of the key factors
social engagement amongst older people that is
determining the quality of life of older people. Re-
manifest in local, national and international policy-
search has consistently demonstrated a strong and
makers’ interest in ideas such as social capital
positive relationship between social engagement in
and social exclusion. However, like the concept
all forms but especially participation within kin
of community care, notions of social capital, social
and wider social networks, and a high quality of
engagement and social exclusion are described in
life.1 However, this recognition of the importance
many different ways4,5 Scharf and Smith6 have
of this relationship is not new. In the early 1960s,
proposed a model of social exclusion in later life,
the ‘activity theorists’ of aging posited that the
which consists of five domains: exclusion from
key to a good old age was the maintenance of
social relations, civic activities, material resources,
high levels of activity, including social participation
basic services and neighbourhood. This definition
and the maintenance of kin- and friendship-based
of social exclusion provides a theoretical and
relationships. This resonates with the ideas of
policy-related framework within which to locate
Rowe and Kahn2 who suggest that a high level of
the study of social engagement and indicators of
social engagement is a key factor in achieving the
social impoverishment in later life, loneliness and
individually and socially desired goal of ‘successful
isolation.
aging’. Furthermore, they argue that, with adva-
It is evident from the policy, theoretical and
nced age, the social context, in combination with
research literature that a variety of different terms
the physical environment, exerts a more potent
and concepts are being used interchangeably to
influence upon the experience of later life than
describe notions of social engagement, exclusion
either intrinsic genetic or biological factors. Whilst
and participation by older people. These largely
the prescriptive strictures of activity theory and
reflect the dynamic and varying nature of the
successful aging remain the subject of debate, there
broader theoretical and policy context. Social
seems little doubt that the social environment
engagement is a broad and diverse concept with
continues to exert both a powerful influence upon,
different sub-divisions relating to notions such as
and provides a context within which people ex-
social capital, social participation, as measured by
perience old age, both individually and collectively.
activity and contact rates, and social networks, in-
A key element of current thinking on the
cluding notions of exchange relationships, intimate
promotion of quality of life in old age relates to
ties and roles and relationships.7 The predominant
notions of social engagement and social inclusion.
conceptualization of exclusion from social
Manipulation of the social environment by, for
relationships has been in terms of investigating the
example, interventions to combat isolation and
pathological end of the distribution, with a specific
promote social engagement may offer pathways
concentration upon isolation and loneliness. This
for the improvement and enhancement of quality
largely reflects an approach to the investigation of
social relationships in later life influenced by the
Address for correspondence: CR Victor, School of
study of social problems and, perhaps, too ready
Health & Social Care, University of Reading, Bulmershe an acceptance of the stereotype that the normal
Court, Reading, RG6 1HY, UK. experience of old age is of social neglect, isolation
64 C Victor, L Grenade and D Boldy

and a reliance upon fragile social networks.8 There usually distinguished in terms of their differing
has been a concentration in research upon peer- methodological approach and theoretical positions
group studies in terms of empirical investigations of concerning the nature of loneliness.
loneliness,9,10,11,12 social isolation and exclusion.6 Cognitive theories of loneliness, based upon
By contrast, other perspectives such as describing personal assumptions, such as the discrepancy
current experience within a life-course or bio- between desired and available relationships, giving
graphical context have received relatively little rise to maladaptive patterns of thinking which
attention. can generate feelings of loneliness, underpin the
Perhaps the greatest emphasis in both research self-rating scales approach to measurement. From
and policy terms has been upon the examination this perspective, loneliness is considered to be a
of loneliness in later life, although loneliness is es- state that can be manipulated, hence the interest in
timated to be commoner and more intense in adol- intervention studies.17
escence. Despite the extensive research literature, Self-report measures are simple to use, appear
however, there are still problems in defining and highly acceptable to research participants and ask
measuring this concept13,14 reflecting the theoret- directly about feelings of loneliness. This approach
ical diversity underpinning notions of loneliness. At presumes that loneliness is a unidimensional
the fundamental level, loneliness is concerned with concept, and that the primary variation between
how individuals evaluate their overall level of social individuals is in the intensity of the experience.
interaction, and describes a state in which there is Such questions require respondents to rate their
a deficit between the desired and actual quality levels of loneliness from ‘never’ to ‘always’ with
and quantity of social engagement. Loneliness a variety of intermediate gradations of response.18
may thus be identified as the emotional response This simple scale has been found to adapt better
to the discrepancy between desired and available to the oldest age group, although it does not elicit
relationships and is described by Fees et al.15 as ‘a information about the amount, nature, value or
concept that relies on comparisons’. meaning of loneliness, nor about its causes or
The development and acceptance of theories of consequences.15 However its simplicity has been
loneliness, and its measurement within populations seen as a weakness, in that the question presumes a
has been hampered by the fact that loneliness is common understanding of the concept by study
often masked by clinical syndromes: there is a participants and, as such, is highly culturally
strong association between loneliness and depres- specific.19 More fundamentally, loneliness may be
sion.16 There is an obvious and consistent link seen as a stigmatizing concept compromising the
between loneliness and depression – indeed depres- identity of individuals so that older people may
sion indices often used to include questions about choose not to define themselves in this way and may
loneliness. Depression is a problem that often downshift their loneliness rating. Consequently,
accompanies loneliness, and depressive symptomo- study participants may express a ‘public account’
tology such as withdrawal, anxiety, lack of and present what they assume the interviewer may
motivation and sadness both mimic and mask want to hear, or not admit to feelings of loneliness,
the manifestations of loneliness. In such cases, for fear of compromising their autonomy and self-
people are often treated for depression without identity.
consideration of the possibility that loneliness A key objection to the use of direct questions to
may be a contributory and sustaining factor measure the extent of loneliness is that they will
in their condition. Consequently, loneliness has not elicit a true response: they will generate only
often been subsumed under depression, anxiety or a publicly acceptable [socially desirable] response.
social isolation, rather than being recognized as a Hence a variety of scales have been developed
distinct problem. Thus loneliness and depression which approach the topic indirectly. One widely
are characteristics of people who have recently used scale is the University of California, Los
suffered the death of a close family member or Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale,20 a 20-item
friend. Likert scale. De Jong Gierveld12 has also developed
There are two main approaches towards the a scale for use with older people which meets
empirical measurement of this phenomenon; self- the criteria of the Rasch measurement model and
report measures and the development of scales has both positive and negative dimensions. It also
or aggregate measures.13 These measures are reflects the de Jong Gierveld proposition14 that
Measuring loneliness in later life 65

loneliness can be perceived as a multidimensional (ONS) Omnibus Survey and funded by the ESRC
phenomenon comprising three distinct dimensions; Growing Older Programme and Medical Research
a deprivation component that relates to the loss Council Health Services Research Collaboration.
of an intimate attachment; a temporal perspective, This is based on face-to-face interviews with
raising the question of the extent to which the approximately 2000 adults aged 16 years and
state of being lonely might be prone to change; over in their own home. Researchers can purchase
and a range of emotional aspects of loneliness, specific modules/questions on the survey, or use
such as sadness, guilt, frustration and desperation. the survey as a mechanism to identify eligible
Wenger21 also developed an 8-item scale, which research participants, as was the case in this study.
views loneliness from the perspective of friendship Respondents aged 65 and over participating in the
and family connections: here questions probe Omnibus Survey were invited to participate in a
the pervasiveness of loneliness in older people, Quality-of-life module that was administered at a
while only including the word ‘loneliness’ once. second interview.27,28,22 To control for seasonality
However, all scales demonstrate cultural specificity and to generate a sample of sufficient statistical
and rely upon measuring loneliness via indirect power, all 1598 participants aged 65 years and over
questions relating to direct social engagement. who were interviewed for the April, September,
Hence they make theoretical assumptions about November 2000 and January 2001 Omnibus
the definition and meaning of loneliness and the Surveys were invited to participate in the Quality-
link between, in the case, for example, of of-Life survey and 1323 agreed to do so. At
the Wenger index,13 social engagement and follow-up, 24 of these addresses were subsequently
loneliness. However the indirectness of these found to be ineligible, leaving a potential study
measures offers a way of exploring the ‘private’ population of 1299. Of this number, 243 (19%)
expressions of loneliness, in that they do not declined to participate and 57 (4%) were not
directly ask about or require older people to contactable. This yielded a total study population
give direct responses to potentially stigmatizing or of 999 respondents, a response rate of 77% of
compromising questions. In this paper we compare those identified as eligible for the study and 63%
the utility of the differing approaches towards of those who participated in the index waves of the
the measurement of loneliness, via the results Omnibus Survey.
generated by two different measures of loneliness.
The Perth, Western Australia Study
Sources of data
This study was funded by ‘Healthway’ (the
The data for this analysis is provided by two Western Australia Health Promotion Foundation)
surveys: one conducted in the UK and the second and was based in three districts of the Perth
in Perth, Western Australia. The two surveys metropolitan area and stratified to reflect different
used different methods to determine the extent socioeconomic levels: City of Melville (high SES),
of loneliness amongst older people and different Canning City Council (intermediate SES), and
modes of data collection. The British survey took City of Fremantle (low SES). Respondents were
the form of a face-to-face interview and employed identified from the electoral information data
a direct question asking respondents to rate their provided by the Australian Electoral Commission
level of loneliness22 whilst the Perth study used and consisted of people aged 65 years and over
a postal questionnaire which included the same living in the community. The sample was stratified
direct question, as well as the indirect 11-item de according to age and sex within five-year age
Jong Gierveld scale.23,24,25 The self-report question bands. Equal numbers of men and women were
and the de Jong scale that elicited the data reported sampled in each age group. The study used
here are shown in Figure 1. a postal questionnaire supported by telephone
follow-up and support. Initially, 625 individuals
were identified but 78 (12.5%) were excluded
The British Survey
as no telephone number could be traced for
The first dataset is derived from a national survey them. Questionnaires were sent to 547 potential
of households26 in Great Britain conducted using participants and 353 were returned: a response
the United Kingdom Office for National Statistics rate of 65% (or 56% of the initial sample). The
66 C Victor, L Grenade and D Boldy

A. The self-rating scale


‘Would you say that you are’
Always lonely
Often lonely
Sometimes lonely
Never lonely

B. The items included in the de Jong Gierveld scale


There is always someone I can talk to about day-to-day problems
I miss having a really close friend
I experience a general sense of emptiness
There are plenty of people I can lean on when I have problems
I miss the pleasure of the company of others
I find my circle of friends and acquaintances too limited
There are many people I can trust completely
There are enough people I feel close to
I miss having people around
I often feel rejected
I can call on my friends whenever I need them

Figure 1 Methods of measuring loneliness used in the two studies

major reasons for non-response were: refusal (80), current levels of loneliness on a four-point scale:
ill-health (39), and problems with literacy (24). ‘never’/ ‘sometimes’/ ‘often’/‘always’, in effect, a
public declaration of a negative and potentially
Results stigmatizing experience. All respondents in the
The results are presented in three sections, firstly, British survey and all but 10 in the Australian
description of the samples. We then examine the sample answered the question. The results of the
public representation of loneliness as measured two studies are remarkably similar (Table 2),
by our direct self-rating question and then move which is particularly interesting, considering that
on to consider the private accounts of loneliness the Perth study used a postal questionnaire and the
produced by our study participants via the use of British survey a direct interview. The majority of
the de Jong scale. respondents in both studies, about 60%, reported
that they were never lonely. In both populations
Sample description a minority of respondents (8–9%) reported being
often or always lonely and approximately one-
The profile of the two samples is broadly similar
third reported that they sometimes experienced
in terms of sex distribution, chronic illness and
loneliness.
household living arrangements. The Perth sample
is ‘older’ than the British sample, whilst the British
sample has a much larger percentage of widowed Loneliness: the ‘private’ experience
respondents (Table 1). To explore the potentially unexplored (and
different) ‘private’ representation of loneliness,
Loneliness: the ‘public’ account
we compared the levels of loneliness generated
Both studies used a direct self-rating question, from our direct question with those derived
which required study participants to rate their from the use of the de Jong scale in the Perth
Measuring loneliness in later life 67

Table 1. Key sociodemographic characteristics of were more likely to be female and older, but
the two samples (%) were similar in terms of marital status and
household composition. Table 2 illustrates that
Perth, WA Great Britain∗
the overall classification of respondents was
% %
remarkably similar in the de Jong scale and the self-
Age rating questionnaire, especially at the severe end
65–74 40 58
of the distribution, where the prevalence estimates
75–84 42 34
85+ 18 8 are broadly consistent at about 8–9%. The scale
Sex did, however, generate a higher percentage of
Male 51 47 participants in the intermediate loneliness category
Female 49 53 and a subsequent decrease in the ‘never’ lonely
Marital status group.
Single 2 7 For 293 participants in the Perth survey, we can
Married/cohabiting 65 46 directly compare their loneliness ratings (Table 3).
Widowed 26 39
This confirms the variability in the responses in the
Separated/divorced 7 8
Living arrangements ‘never’/intermediate loneliness category between
Lives alone 31 37 our two measures. Overall 63% of respondents
Lives with others 69 63 had the same classification on both measures. Of
Health status the 108 where there was a difference between
% reporting longstanding 57 62 measures, 74 were rated as more severe on the de
illness Jong measure and the majority of these were on the
% reporting limiting 37 ‘never’/intermediate loneliness boundary.
longstanding illness
∗ Source: Victor et al (2005) Discussion
Measuring loneliness is problematic.13 Two main
sample. As the scale is calculated on the basis approaches towards the empirical measurement of
of responses to 11 questions, because of missing loneliness may be identified: self-report measures
values to one or more, there were 53 respondents and the development of scales or aggregate
for whom we could not calculate a score: they measures. The use of a single-question self-rating

Table 2. Reported levels of loneliness in the two studies using differing methods of measurement (%)

Great Britain Perth, WA Perth, WA


% respondents % respondents % respondents
Self rating scale rated as rated as De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale classed
Do you feel lonely?
Never 61 62 Not lonely (score of 0–2) 52
Sometimes 32 32 Moderately lonely (score of 3–8) 39
Often 7 6 Severely lonely (score of 9–10) 7
Always 2 2 Very severely lonely (score of 11) 2

Table 3. Comparison of loneliness classification for individuals in the Perth study: cross-tabulation
of responses to the self-rating scale and classification on the de Jong scale

Self-rating loneliness scale responses

de Jong Never Sometimes Often Always Total


Not lonely (0–2) 125 25 1 0 151
Moderately lonely (3–8) 58 48 6 4 116
Severely lonely (9–10) 0 11 9 0 20
Very severely lonely (11) 0 3 2 1 6
Total 183 87 18 5 293
68 C Victor, L Grenade and D Boldy

scale to measure loneliness appears to have this variability may reflect either statistical noise
been first reported by Sheldon29 and has in the scale, the inexact nature of the response
subsequently been widely used in Britain,30,31,21, categories used in the rating scale or indeed, some
Continental Europe,11 and North America.32 Self- combination of the two.
report measures are simple to use, appear to Pepper35 reported that respondents’ interpreta-
be highly acceptable to research participants, tions of the term ‘sometimes’ are variable; with
in that they generate few ‘missing items’ either many people understanding it to be about 20%
in direct interviews or via postal surveys, and of the time, while others understood it to mean
ask directly about feelings of loneliness, which about half the time. Schaeffer36 recommends that
is the specific area of interest. However their the relative frequency responses currently used in
use presupposes a common understanding of many scales be replaced by absolute frequency
the term ‘loneliness’ amongst respondents. In response options, such as ‘every day’ or ‘once
research with older people, participants may a week’. By using absolute frequency response
present to the survey interviewer only their ‘public’ options, the possibility that comparisons between
accounts (Cornwell33 ) and fail to affirm the groups would be valid reflections of the concept,
existence of socially undesirable or stigmatizing and not caused by differences in how groups
concepts such as loneliness in their lives. As interpret the response option terms may increase.
loneliness may be seen as a ‘stigmatizing’ concept If, however, we wish to identify a wider group of
compromising the identity of individuals, these older people for whom low-level interventions may
measures may thus capture only the ‘public’, be appropriate then it may be more desirable to
socially desirable account. An alternative approach adopt a scale approach (perhaps in addition to a
to the measurement of loneliness has been the direct question or questions with a less ambiguous
development of specific scales such as those devised set of response options).
by de Jong Gierveld12 and Wenger.13 These do not Loneliness is an intensely personal, subjective
ask directly about loneliness and, perhaps, offer feeling for which there are no observable signs
a way of capturing a ‘private’ account by asking or symptoms. Due to the inherent problems
indirectly about loneliness via questions relating to in defining loneliness, the discrepancies between
social networks and the availability of confiding theoretical perspectives, and the problems of
relationships. (All these scales have undergone distinguishing loneliness from related states such
varying degrees of psychometric testing of validity as depression, the problem of trying to measure the
and reliability). phenomenon is challenging. To measure loneliness,
The ‘public’ account of loneliness articulated in researchers must rely on a person’s statement
our studies falls within the pattern expected from about their internal experiences, and attempt to
the published literature, as our prevalence of 8–9% ‘diagnose’ loneliness from symptoms described
reporting that they were ‘often’/‘always’ lonely is or from self-reported questionnaires. People are
consistent with previous research.34 The similarity ‘labelled’ as lonely because they say they are,
of our responses is notable, given that the British or because they relate to the indirect indicators
study used direct interview whilst the Australian of loneliness. It has been suggested37 that the
data was generated by a postal survey. However, self-report measures are more appropriate for
here we have had the opportunity to compare use with older people than composite scales.
both public and private accounts by comparing a However, we would suggest that this conclusion
direct question and an indirect scale. Again, the is premature. Our studies certainly suggest that
most immediate conclusion was in the similarity self-rated questions need to be modified to
of the overall estimates of loneliness generated by include less ambiguous response categories, and
the two methods, especially at the severe end of that more complex rating scales may be more
the distribution Where there was variability, it appropriate in screening-type studies of older
was on the ‘never lonely’/intermediate loneliness people.
boundary, with as many as 32% (58/183) of There is clearly scope for further research
those ‘publicly’ indicating that they were never investigating the relationship between the different
lonely, being ‘privately’ identified as ‘moderately’ ways of measuring loneliness and how older people
lonely. Given that the rationale for the use of the define and conceptualize this important aspect of
intervals for categories used in the scale is unclear, quality of life.
Measuring loneliness in later life 69

Acknowledgements 13 Wenger GC. Loneliness: a problem of


measurement. In : Jerrome D ed. Ageing in
The British study was funded by the Economic and Modern Society. Beckenham, Kent: Croom Helm:
Social Research Council as part of its Growing 1983, 145–67.
Older Programme (award numbers L480254042 14 de Jong Gierveld, J. A review of loneliness:
and L480254022). Relating to the Perth study, concepts and definitions, causes and consequences.
LG and DB would like to acknowledge their co- Rev Clin Gerontol 1998; 8: 73–80.
investigators Helena Iredell, Thérèse Shaw, Prof 15 Fees B, Martin PI, Poon I. A model of loneliness in
Peter Howat and Dr Ros Morrow. The study was older adults. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 1999; 54b:
funded by Healthway – the Western Australian 231–39.
Health Promotion Foundation. We are all especi- 16 Adams KB, Sanders S, Auth EA. Loneliness and
depression in independent-living communities: risk
ally grateful to all those older people who gave so
and resilience factors. J Aging Mental Health
freely of their time to participate in these studies.
2004; 8, 6: 475–85.
17 Cattan M, White M, Bond J, Learmouth A.
References Preventing social isolation and loneliness among
older people: a systematic review of health
1 Bond J, Corner L. Quality of life and old age. promotion interventions. Ageing Soc 2005; 25:
Buckingham: Open University Press, 2004. 41–67.
2 Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Successful aging The 18 Victor CR, Scambler S, Bond J, Bowling A.
Gerontologist 1997; 37: 433–40. Loneliness in later life: preliminary findings from
3 Department of Health. National Service the Growing Older Project. Qual Ageing 2001; 3:
Framework for Older People. London: HMSO, 34–41.
2001. Available at http://www.dh.gov.uk/ 19 Jylha M. Old age and loneliness: cross-sectional
assetRoot/04/07/12/83/04071283.pdf and longitudinal analyses in the Tampere
4 Scharf T, Phillipson C, Kingston P, Smith AE. longitudinal study on aging. Can J Aging, 2005;
Social exclusion and ageing. Ed Ageing 2000; 16: 23: 157–168
303–20. 20 Russell, D. The UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version
5 Smith A, Phillipson C, Scharf T. Social capital: 3): Reliability, validity and factor structure. J Pers
concepts, measures and the implications for urban Assess 1996; 66: 20–40.
communities. Working paper number 9. Keele 21 Wenger, GC. The supportive network. London:
University: Centre for Social Gerontology, 2002. George Allen and Unwin, 1984.
6 Scharf T, Smith AE. Older people in urban 22 Victor CR, Scambler SJ, Bowling A., Bond J. The
neighbourhoods: addressing the risk of social prevalence of, and risk factors for, loneliness in
exclusion in later life In: Phillipson C, Allan G, later life: a survey of older people in Great Britain.
Morgan D eds. Social networks and social Ageing Soc 2005; 25: 357–75.
exclusion. Aldershot: Ashgate, 2004: 162–79. 23 de Jong Gierveld, J, Kamphuis F. The development
7 Phillipson C, Allan G, Morgan D eds. Social of a Rasch-type loneliness scale. Appl Psychol
networks and social exclusion. Aldershot: Meas 1985; 9: 289–99.
Ashgate, 2004: 162–79. 24 Iredell H, Grenade L, Boldy D et al. Coping with
8 Victor CR, Scarf T. Social isolation and loneliness. loneliness and social isolation in later life: a pilot
In Walker A ed. Understanding quality of life in study. Perth, WA: Freemasons Centre for Research
old age. Maidenhead: Open University Press, into Aged Care Services, Curtin University of
2005: 100–116. Technology, 2003.
9 Wenger GC, Davies R, Shahtahmesebi S, Scott A. 25 Scharf T, Phillipson C, Smith AE, Kingston P.
Social isolation and loneliness in old age: review Growing older in socially deprived areas: social
and model refinement. Ageing Soc 1996; 16: exclusion in later life. London: Help the Aged,
333–58. 2002.
10 Holmen K, Ericsson K, Andersson L, Winblad B. 26 Walker A. et al. Living in Britain: results of the
Loneliness among elderly people in Stockholm: 2001 General Household Survey. London:
a population study. J Adv Nurs 1992; 17: 43–51. HMSO, 2002.
11 Holmen K, Ericsson K, Andersson L, Winblad B. 27 Bowling A, Banister D, Sutton S, Evans O,
Loneliness and living conditions of the oldest old. Windsor J. A multidimensional model of the
Scand J Soc Med 1994; 22: 15–19. quality of life in older age. Aging Mental Health,
12 de Jong Gierveld, J. Developing and testing a 2002; 6: 355–71.
model of loneliness. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987; 53: 28 Ayis S, Gooberman-Hill, R, Ebrahim, S.
119–28. Longstanding and limiting illness in older people:
70 C Victor, L Grenade and D Boldy

associations with chronic diseases, psychosocial 33 Cornwall J. Hard-earned lives: accounts of health
and environmental factors, Age Ageing, 2003; 32: and illness from East London. London: Tavistock
265–72. 1984.
29 Sheldon JH. The social medicine of old age. 34 Victor CR, Scambler S, Bond J, Bowling A. Being
Oxford: Oxford University Press, alone in later life: loneliness, isolation and living
1948. alone in later life. Rev Clin Gerontol, 2000; 10:
30 Victor CR, Scambler S, Shah S. et al. Has 407–17.
loneliness amongst older people increased? An 35 Pepper, S. Problems in the quantification of
investigation into variations between cohorts, frequency expressions. In: Fiske DW ed. New
Ageing Soc 2002; 22: 1–13. directions for methodology of social and
31 Victor CR, Scambler SJ, Bowling A, Bond, J. behavioural science: problems with language
The prevalence of, and risk factors for, imprecision. San Francisco. Josey-Bass: 1981,
loneliness in later life: a survey of older people pp 25–41.
in Great Britain. Ageing Soc 2005; 25: 36 Schaeffer NC. Hardly ever or constantly? Group
357–75. comparisons using vague quantifiers. Pub Opin Q
32 Mullins LC, Elston CH, Gutkowski SM. Social 1991; 55: 395–423.
determinants of loneliness among older 37 Holmen K, Furukawa H. Loneliness, health and
Americans. Genet Soc Gen Psych Monogr 1996; social network among elderly people – a follow-up
122: 453–73. study. Arch Geront Geriatr 2002; 35: 261–71.

You might also like