You are on page 1of 5

833652

research-article2019
CDPXXX10.1177/0963721419833652Rosenbaum et al.Sooner Rather Than Later

ASSOCIATION FOR
Article PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Current Directions in Psychological

Sooner Rather Than Later: Precrastination Science


2019, Vol. 28(3) 229­–233
© The Author(s) 2019
Rather Than Procrastination Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0963721419833652
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419833652
www.psychologicalscience.org/CDPS

David A. Rosenbaum1, Lisa R. Fournier2, Shelly Levy-Tzedek3,4,5,


Dawn M. McBride6, Robert Rosenthal1, Kyle Sauerberger1,
Rachel L. VonderHaar6, Edward A. Wasserman7, and
Thomas R. Zentall8
1
Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside; 2Psychology Department, Washington
State University; 3Department of Physical Therapy, Recanati School for Community Health Professions;
4
Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev; 5Freiburg Institute for
Advanced Studies, University of Freiburg; 6Department of Psychology, Illinois State University;
7
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa; and 8Department of Psychology,
University of Kentucky

Abstract
Putting things off as long as possible (procrastination) is a well-known tendency. Less well known is the tendency to
attempt to get things done as soon as possible, even if that involves extra effort (precrastination). Since its discovery in
2014, precrastination has been demonstrated in humans and animals and has recently been revealed in an analogous
tendency called the mere-urgency effect. Trying to get things done as soon as one can may reflect optimal foraging,
but another less obvious factor may also contribute—reducing cognitive demands associated with having to remember
what to do when. Individual differences may also play a role. Understanding precrastination will have important
implications for explaining why hurrying happens as often as it does and may help reduce the chance that haste makes
waste.

Keywords
cognitive resources, foraging, hurrying, memory load, precrastination, procrastination, prospective memory

You have just driven home from the market with sev- possible, even if that involves extra effort. Precrastination
eral bags of groceries. You could carry one bag at a is illustrated in behaviors such as carrying too many
time in several trips or you could carry several bags bags in too few trips, answering e-mails immediately
at once in fewer trips. Which would you do? To get rather than waiting to answer them with more fore-
the job done quickly, you could carry many bags thought, or paying bills right away rather than collecting
simultaneously, but you might drop a bag or overtax interest on funds in the bank for as long as possible.
yourself physically. Alternatively, you could carry one Here, we review findings suggesting that precrastina-
bag at a time, but that would take longer. In such tion is an important phenomenon. The most interesting
circumstances, you might rush, but why would you finding about precrastination is that it is not simply a
do so? reflection of the desire to get instant gratification or
Consider the tendency to put things off as long as grab scarce resources (optimal foraging). Precrastina-
possible—procrastination. Procrastination generally tion also reduces cognitive demands.
applies to large-scale tasks such as writing term papers
or filing income taxes, not small-scale tasks such as car-
rying groceries. This article is about the opposite of
Corresponding Author:
procrastination—precrastination—a term coined by David A. Rosenbaum, University of California, Riverside, Department
Rosenbaum, Gong, and Potts (2014). Precrastination is of Psychology, 900 University Ave., Riverside, CA 92521
the tendency to hurry to get things done as soon as E-mail: david.rosenbaum@ucr.edu
230 Rosenbaum et al.

The Discovery of Precrastination then pick up the near object on the way back. Fournier
et al. found that participants opted for choice (a). The
Precrastination was found by accident in a study of the researchers coded this choice as precrastination. In
planning and control of physical actions. Rosenbaum another experiment, Fournier et al. found that when a
et al. (2014) asked university students to walk down an detail of the task was changed so that the objects to be
alley and pick up either of two buckets: one some carried were cups filled with water that could easily be
distance away on the left edge of the alley or one some spilled but were supposed to be prevented from spill-
distance away on the right edge of the alley. The par- ing, the near object was seldom picked up first; option
ticipants were asked to take whichever bucket seemed (b) was now preferred over option (a). Fournier et al.
easier to carry to the end. It was expected that partici- concluded that precrastination is used to reduce cogni-
pants would pick up the bucket closer to the end of tive demands: When precrastination would have
the alley because that bucket would be carried a shorter increased cognitive demands (increasing attention to
distance. The researchers’ plan was to explore the bio- the carried objects if the objects were cups filled with
mechanical trade-offs between distance and weight: water), precrastination was eschewed.
How far would participants be willing to walk with a In another experiment, Fournier et  al. (2019) gave
relatively light bucket to avoid a relatively short walk participants a memorization task in conjunction with
with a heavier bucket? less attention-demanding object carrying. These partici-
Surprisingly, most participants chose the bucket that pants precrastinated at higher rates than participants
was closer to the starting point, not the one that was who were not given the added memorization. The
closer to the end of the alley. This was odd because researchers observed that this result was again consis-
the near-bucket choice resulted in a longer carrying tent with the hypothesis that precrastination is associ-
distance. When participants were interviewed about ated with reducing cognitive effort.
their choices, they said they chose the near bucket to In still other experiments, Fournier and her col-
get the task done as quickly as possible. Such a claim leagues (2018) showed that precrastination is likely to
made little sense from the point of view that the time be automatically invoked, though it can be inhibited if
would presumably have been the same if subjects had needed. Fournier et al. (2018) introduced manipulations
picked up the near bucket or the far bucket. On the that revealed that precrastination is so strong that it
basis of nine experiments designed to test alternative actually applies to the starting of tasks, not just the
hypotheses, Rosenbaum et al. (2014) were led to the completion of tasks. The results fit with an optimal-
hypothesis discussed in the next section. foraging view, in which expressing a bias to grab poten-
tially scarce resources tends to increase fitness and in
Precrastination Reduces Cognitive turn encourages quick action (Yoon, Geary, Ahmed, &
Shadmehr, 2018). An optimal-foraging view and a
Demands
cognitive-easing view are certainly not inconsistent with
Rosenbaum et al. (2014) suggested that the near-bucket one another. Indeed, having a clear (or cleared) mind
preference stemmed from a desire to reduce cognitive should help one forage more effectively.
demands. The idea was that picking up and carrying a VonderHaar, McBride, and Rosenbaum (2017) pro-
bucket is nontrivial from a cognitive perspective vided more evidence for the hypothesis that reducing
(Rosenbaum, 2017); witness the fact that robots still cognitive demands gets high priority. They asked uni-
have a very hard time with such tasks. In addition, versity students to complete a computerized box-
maintaining the intention to pick up a bucket loads moving task as well as a semantic item-generation task.
working memory. Therefore, picking up a bucket Their participants slid 10 boxes, one at a time, from
sooner rather than later enables people to rid them- one place to another on a computer screen and also
selves of the cognitive load sooner rather than later. generated items from various semantic categories. The
This interpretation was supported by Fournier, item generations had to occur without interruption but
Stubblefield, Dyre, and Rosenbaum (2018), whose par- could occur whenever participants preferred—before
ticipants performed a task similar to the one used by the first box move, before the second box move, before
Rosenbaum et al. (2014). Fournier et al.’s participants the third box move, and so on, all the way to after the
picked up two objects at two distances and brought 10th box move. A priori, the probability of doing the
them back to the start position in one trip. The question category-generation task at any serial position was
was whether the participants would (a) pick up the 1/11, but a full half of the 122 participants chose to do
near object, carry it out to the far object, then pick up all of the category generation before moving any box,
the far object, and then return with both objects or (b) an outcome whose probability was 1/1161, or astro-
walk past the near object, pick up the far object, and nomically small. Most other participants also did the
Sooner Rather Than Later 231

category generation earlier rather than later. This out- an outcome that surprised us initially, considering
come fits with the view that there is a strong preference Fournier et al.’s (2018) conclusion that precrastination
to unload working memory as soon as possible. The is automatically invoked, subject to inhibition. Yet
conclusion supports the interpretations given above and Fournier and her colleagues found that precrastination
makes sense from the point of view that prospective is used strategically, which was also shown in another
memory is mentally taxing (Einstein & McDaniel, 2005; behavioral experiment by Rosenbaum and Sauerberger
see also Kool, McGuire, Rosen, & Botvinick, 2010). (2019) involving choosing buckets to be carried over
Yet another demonstration concerns a recently dis- long or short distances to platforms that were either
covered phenomenon called the mere-urgency effect high or low; subjects made sophisticated trade-offs
(Zhu, Bagchi, & Hock, 2018; Zhu, Yang, & Hsee, 2018). between the preference for the near bucket and aver-
Here, participants chose between tasks with short or sion to bending down at the end. In their questionnaire
long deadlines. All of the tasks could be completed study, Sauerberger, Rosenbaum, and Funder (2018)
within the available times, and the researchers showed found, in addition to the noneffect of impulsivity, a
that the participants believed that the short-deadline positive relation between precrastination and consci-
tasks were no harder than the long-deadline tasks. Nev- entiousness; people with high levels of conscientious-
ertheless, the participants preferred the short-deadline ness may want to get things done as soon as possible.
tasks. Zhu, Bagchi, and Hock (2018) and Zhu, Yang, Third, Sauerberger et al. (2018) found a positive relation
and Hsee (2018) suggested that participants preferred between precrastination and agreeableness. The basis
the shorter deadlines because those deadlines helped for this result is under investigation.
them avoid the need to monitor the passage of time.
Keeping track of time is cognitively taxing (Grondin,
2008), as is the need to remember to do upcoming tasks
Animal Studies
(Einstein & McDaniel, 2005). If precrastination is a basic phenomenon, one would
Consistent with this interpretation, findings from expect to see it in nonhuman animals. That expectation
another study (Levy-Tzedek, Ben Tov, & Karniel, 2011) has been confirmed. Wasserman and Brzykcy (2015)
revealed that participants “jumped the gun” by switching demonstrated precrastination in pigeons. Pigeons could
from one mode of manual behavior (a discrete mode) get food after making three pecks. The first peck was
to another (a continuous mode) rather than staying the made in a square in the center of a computer screen.
course in the discrete mode until forced to switch to the The second peck was made either in that same center
continuous mode. Premature switching was also observed square or in a square that randomly appeared to the
when the continuous mode was the first mode and the left or right. The third peck was made in the just-shown
discrete mode was the second. In other work, Levy- left or right square after a star appeared within it. Criti-
Tzedek (2017) showed, much as Fournier et al. (2019) cally, food was given after the final peck regardless of
did, that adding a cognitive task (counting backward) whether the second peck struck the center square or
affected the likelihood of precrastination. Levy-Tzedek side square. The pigeons overwhelmingly directed their
(2017) also found that older adults precrastinated less second pecks to the side square. In so doing, they
than younger adults, for reasons still to be explored. moved to the final position earlier rather than later,
though there was no obvious reason or extra reward
for doing so. By pecking on the side square, the birds
Individual Differences behaved in a manner consistent with precrastination.
Sauerberger, Rosenbaum, and Funder (2018) explored The result cannot be ascribed to reducing errors,
individual differences in precrastination. They replicated because no errors were possible with this procedure;
the original bucket-choice task of Rosenbaum et  al. reward was always obtained. The result also cannot be
(2014) and found that most (59%) of the university- ascribed to confusion, for then the birds would have
student participants chose the near bucket rather than responded equally often to the center and side squares.
the far bucket, a rate comparable with that seen by A related effect was found by Rayburn-Reeves, Molet,
Rosenbaum et al. (2014). Sauerberger et al. also found and Zentall (2011), who trained pigeons to peck a red
that participants’ choices were stable: 22% of the par- light for 40 trials and then peck a green light for the
ticipants always chose the first bucket, and 13% of the next 40 trials. Despite considerable training, the pigeons
participants never chose the first bucket; 55% deviated tended to switch to the green light before the reversal
once, at most, from their choices in all other trials. occurred. The pigeons precrastinated, anticipating the
These results indicate stable individual differences switch to the green light, though they would have been
whose bases were revealed through personality ques- more accurate (made only a single error) had they
tionnaires. Impulsivity did not predict precrastination, waited for the first trial in which a red-light peck would
232 Rosenbaum et al.

have been wrong. Recall that Levy-Tzedek et al. (2011) that may lead to injury? If precrastination reflects stable
obtained a similar result. individual differences, one would expect such gener-
Zentall, Case, and Andrews (2018) got a somewhat alization. Personnel-selection studies might be designed
different result, which, interestingly, they took to reflect accordingly. Cognitive abilities should also affect the
procrastination rather than precrastination. They gave likelihood of precrastination according to the main
pigeons a choice between (a) a 15-s green light fol- hypothesis offered here.
lowed by a 5-s red light signaling food or (b) a 5-s blue Finally, practical questions arise. Are accidents due
light followed by a 15-s yellow light signaling the same to procrastination, and how can they be prevented? Is
food. All durations were fixed-interval schedules that the fast-food industry a reflection of precrastination?
required pigeons to peck immediately at the end of Do people interrupt others in conversation to reduce
each interval. The pigeons preferred the long-short mental workload and not just to exert power over them?
sequence even though the total time from choice to Do people multitask because of precrastination? Finally,
food was the same in both instances. Furthermore, the do people precrastinate to different degrees in different
pigeons pecked the terminal 5-s red light at twice the cultures and social settings? These are all important
rate of the terminal 15-s yellow light. Zentall et al. sug- questions. We expect answers to be sought for them
gested that it may be especially rewarding to have a sooner rather than later.
short cue immediately signaling reinforcement even if
one has to wait longer to receive it (cf. Fantino, 1969; Recommended Reading
Fantino & Abarca, 1985). Rosenbaum, D. A., Gong, L., & Potts, C. A. (2014). (See
Wasserman and Zentall here hypothesize that organ- References). The first article to report on and name pre-
isms will precrastinate if reward will be available soon crastination.
(as found by Wasserman & Brzykcy, 2015) but will Wasserman, E. A. (2018). (See References). A review of
procrastinate if reward will be available later (as found evidence concerning precrastination that connects that
by Zentall et al., 2018). This view, which was inspired evidence with a long history of interest in anticipatory
learning, sensitivity to stimuli originating outside of the
by and echoed by Rachlin and Green (1972), Rung and
body, and brain evolution.
Madden (2018), and Wasserman (2018), was previewed Zhu, M., Yang, Y., & Hsee, C. K. (2018). (See References).
earlier, when we indicated that one might hurry to bring A description of the mere-urgency effect—the tendency
in groceries (precrastination) but delay working on to choose less important tasks that must be completed
term papers (procrastination). immediately rather than more important tasks that can
be completed later.
Conclusions and Future Directions
Action Editor
Precrastination is the tendency to hurry to get things Randall W. Engle served as action editor for this article.
done and even to start as soon as possible, even if that
involves extra effort. As shown here, the tendency is Acknowledgments
strong. It may be triggered automatically but can be
This article summarizes the results of a workshop on precras-
inhibited. The drive to act quickly may optimize forag-
tination held at the University of California, Riverside (UCR)
ing. More interestingly, however, precrastination on August 3, 2018, at which the authors were presenters. We
reduces cognitive demands. The desire to unload work- thank Randall W. Engle, Gil Einstein, and two anonymous
ing memory may be so strong that one is willing to reviewers for helpful comments.
work hard physically to do so.
Questions remain. One pertains to cognitive-demand Declaration of Conflicting Interests
reduction in animals. The studies described above
The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest
showed differential reliance on precrastination and pro- with respect to the authorship or the publication of this
crastination in animals, but cognitive demands were article.
not varied. Tools for doing so have been developed
(Wasserman & Zentall, 2006). It will be interesting to Funding
see whether precrastination is undertaken to reduce The workshop from which this article was adapted was sup-
cognitive demands in animals. ported by a University of California, Riverside (UCR) Teaming
A second question is whether the tendency to pre- Workshop Grant. The work was also supported by a Guggenheim
crastinate generalizes over tasks. If someone picks up Fellowship and a UCR Committee on Research grant (to D. A.
near buckets, will he or she answer e-mails immedi- Rosenbaum) and by the Helmsley Charitable Trust Agricultural,
ately, finish work too soon, and hurry on physical tasks Biological and Cognitive Robotic Initiative, the Marcus
Sooner Rather Than Later 233

Endowment Fund at Ben-Gurion University, the Borten Family Rosenbaum, D. A., Gong, L., & Potts, C. A. (2014). Pre-
Foundation, the Promobilia Foundation, the Israeli Science crastination: Hastening subgoal completion at the expense
Foundation (Grants 535/16 and 2166/16), and the European of extra physical effort. Psychological Science, 25, 1487–
Union’s Horizon 2020 Marie Skłodowska-Curie Grant 754340 1496. doi:10.1177/0956797614532657
(to S. Levy-Tzedek). Rosenbaum, D. A., & Sauerberger, K. S. (2019). End-state
comfort meets pre-crastination. Psychological Research.
Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s00426-018-
References 01142-6
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective mem- Rung, J. M., & Madden, G. J. (2018). Experimental reductions of
ory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in delay discounting and impulsive choice: A systematic review
Psychological Science, 14, 286–290. and meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Fantino, E. (1969). Choice and rate of reinforcement. Journal General, 147, 1349–1381. doi:10.1037/xge0000462
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 723–730. Sauerberger, K. S., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Funder, D. C.
Fantino, E., & Abarca, N. (1985). Choice, optimal foraging, (2018, March). When doing things later is the best choice:
and the delay-reduction hypothesis. Behavioral & Brain Precrastination as an individual difference. Poster ses-
Sciences, 8, 315–330. sion presented at the 2018 Annual Meeting of the Society
Fournier, L. R., Coder, E., Kogan, C., Raghunath, N., Taddese, for Personality and Social Psychology, Atlanta, GA.
E., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2019). Which task will we choose VonderHaar, R. L., McBride, D. M., & Rosenbaum, D. A.
first? Precrastination and cognitive load in task order- (2017, November). Precrastination effects in a prospective
ing. Attention, Perception, & Performance, 81, 489–503. memory task. Poster session presented at the 2017 Annual
doi:10.3758/s13414-018-1633-5 Meeting of the Psychonomics Society, Vancouver, British
Fournier, L. R., Stubblefield, A. M., Dyre, B. P., & Rosenbaum, Columbia, Canada.
D. A. (2018). Starting or finishing sooner? Sequencing pref- Wasserman, E. A. (2018). Precrastination: The fierce urgency
erences in object transfer tasks. Psychological Research. of now. Learning & Behavior. Advance online publication.
Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s00426-018-1022-7 doi:10.3758/s13420-018-0358-6
Grondin, S. (Ed.). (2008). Psychology of time. Bingley, Wasserman, E. A., & Brzykcy, S. J. (2015). Pre-crastination
England: Emerald. in the pigeon. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1130–
Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. 1134. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0758-3
(2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive Wasserman, E. A., & Zentall, T. R. (Eds.). (2006). Comparative
demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelli-
139, 665–682. gence. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Levy-Tzedek, S. (2017). Changes in predictive task switch- Yoon, T., Geary, R. B., Ahmed, A. A., & Shadmehr, R. (2018).
ing with age and with cognitive load. Frontiers in Aging Control of movement vigor and decision making during
Neuroscience, 9, Article 375. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2017.00375 foraging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
Levy-Tzedek, S., Ben Tov, M., & Karniel, A. (2011). Early USA, 115, E10476–E10485. doi:10.1073/pnas.181297
switching between movement types: Indication of pre- 9115
dictive control? Brain Research Bulletin, 85, 283–288. Zentall, T. R., Case, J. P., & Andrews, D. M. (2018).
doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.11.010 Procrastination in the pigeon: Can conditioned rein-
Rachlin, H., & Green, L. (1972). Commitment, choice and self- forcement increase the likelihood of human procrasti-
control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, nation? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 1952–1957.
17, 15–22. doi:10.1901/jeab.1972.17-15 doi:10.3758/s13423-017-1409-2
Rayburn-Reeves, R. M., Molet, M., & Zentall, T. R. (2011). Zhu, M., Bagchi, R., & Hock, S. J. (2018). The mere dead-
Simultaneous discrimination reversal learning in pigeons line effect: Why more time might sabotage goal pur-
and humans: Anticipatory and perseverative errors. suit. Journal of Consumer Research, 45, 1068–1084.
Learning & Behavior, 39, 125–137. doi:10.1093/jcr/ucy030
Rosenbaum, D. A. (2017). Knowing hands: The cognitive psy- Zhu, M., Yang, Y., & Hsee, C. K. (2018). The mere urgency
chology of manual control. New York, NY: Cambridge effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 3, 673–690.
University Press. doi:10.1093/jcr/ucy008

You might also like