You are on page 1of 430

Autism and Child Psychopathology Series

Series Editor: Johnny L. Matson

Johnny L. Matson
Editor

Handbook
of Childhood
Psychopathology
and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series

Series Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology
Louisiana State University
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/8665


Johnny L. Matson
Editor

Handbook of Childhood
Psychopathology
and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment
Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology
Louisiana State University
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

ISSN 2192-922X     ISSN 2192-9238 (electronic)


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series
ISBN 978-3-319-71209-3    ISBN 978-3-319-71210-9 (eBook)
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9

Library of Congress Control Number: 2017962862

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017


This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or
part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way,
and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software,
or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Printed on acid-free paper

This Springer imprint is published by Springer Nature


The registered company is Springer International Publishing AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Contents

History����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������    1
Johnny L. Matson and Claire O. Burns
 evelopmental Psychopathology and the Epidemiology
D
and Expression of Psychopathology from Infancy
Through Early Adulthood��������������������������������������������������������������������   11
Corina Benjet
 n Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis ����������������������������������   25
A
Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon, Julia L. Ferguson,
and Sara M. Weinkauf
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy��������������������������������������������������������������   43
Robert D. Friedberg and Micaela A. Thordarson
Parent Training Interventions��������������������������������������������������������������   63
Nicholas Long, Mark C. Edwards, and Jayne Bellando
 ognition and Memory��������������������������������������������������������������������������   87
C
Corey I. McGill and Emily M. Elliott
 andbook of Childhood Psychopathology
H
and Developmental Disabilities: Treatment����������������������������������������   97
Jason F. Jent, Tasha M. Brown, Bridget C. Davidson,
Laura Cruz, and Allison Weinstein
ADHD������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������  127
Johnny L. Matson and Jasper A. Estabillo
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders����������������������������������������  137
Michelle S. Lemay, Robert D. Rieske, and Leland T. Farmer
Treatment Strategies for Depression in Youth��������������������������������    159
Gail N. Kemp, Erin E. O’Connor, Tessa K. Kritikos,
Laura Curren, and Martha C. Tompson

v
vi Contents

 reating Bipolar Disorders ������������������������������������������������������������������  195


T
Johnny L. Matson and Claire O. Burns
Specific Phobias��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������  207
Peter Muris
 reatment of Anxiety Disorders������������������������������������������������������������  221
T
Maysa M. Kaskas, Paige M. Ryan, and Thompson E. Davis III
 ics and Tourette Syndrome ����������������������������������������������������������������  241
T
Denis G. Sukhodolsky, Theresa R. Gladstone,
Shivani A. Kaushal, Justyna B. Piasecka, and James F. Leckman
Treatment Approaches to Aggression and Tantrums
in Children with Developmental Disabilities ��������������������������������������  257
Abigail Issarraras and Johnny L. Matson
 elf-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual
S
and Developmental Disabilities: Current Practices
in Assessment and Treatment����������������������������������������������������������������  269
Casey J. Clay, Courtney D. Jorgenson, and SungWoo Kahng
Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disabilities��������������������������  287
Esther Hong and Dennis R. Dixon
 ocial Competence: Consideration of Behavioral,
S
Cognitive, and Emotional Factors��������������������������������������������������������  301
Karen Milligan, Annabel Sibalis, Ashley Morgan,
and Marjory Phillips
 eadache and Migraine������������������������������������������������������������������������  321
H
Tiah Dowell, Paul R. Martin, and Allison M. Waters
Eating Disorders������������������������������������������������������������������������������������  341
Juliet K. Rosewall, Janet D. Latner, Suman Ambwani,
and David H. Gleaves
Feeding Disorders����������������������������������������������������������������������������������  367
Jonathan K. Fernand, Krista Saksena, Becky Penrod,
and Mitch J. Fryling
 oilet Training: Behavioral and Medical Considerations������������������  393
T
Pamela McPherson, Claire O. Burns, Mark J. Garcia,
Vinay S. Kothapalli, Shawn E. McNeil, and Timothy Thompson

Index��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������  421
Contributors

Suman Ambwani  Department of Psychology, Dickinson College, Carlisle,


PA, USA
Jayne Bellando Department of Pediatrics, University of Arkansas for
Medical Sciences and Arkansas Children’s Hospital, Little Rock, AR, USA
Corina Benjet  National Institute of Psychiatry Ramón de la Fuente, Mexico
City, Mexico
Tasha M. Brown  University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami,
FL, USA
Claire O. Burns Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Joseph H. Cihon  Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, USA
Endicott College, Beverly, MA, USA
Casey J. Clay  Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri,
Columbia, MO, USA
Laura Cruz  University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL,
USA
Laura Curren  Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
Bridget C. Davidson University of Miami Miller School of Medicine,
Miami, FL, USA
Thompson E. Davis III Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Dennis R. Dixon  Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Woodland Hills,
CA, USA

vii
viii Contributors

Tiah Dowell  School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Mt Gravatt,


QLD, Australia
Mark C. Edwards Department of Pediatrics, University of Arkansas for
Medical Sciences and Arkansas Children’s Hospital, Little Rock, AR, USA
Emily M. Elliott Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Jasper A. Estabillo  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Leland T. Farmer Department of Psychology, Idaho State University,
Pocatello, ID, USA
Julia L. Ferguson  Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, USA
Jonathan K. Fernand Department of Psychology, University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL, USA
Robert D. Friedberg  Center for the Study and Treatment of Anxious Youth,
Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
Mitch J. Fryling  Division of Special Education & Counseling, California
State University, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Mark J. Garcia  Northwest Resource Center, Bossier City, LA, USA
Theresa R. Gladstone  Yale School of Medicine, Child Study Center, New
Haven, CT, USA
David H. Gleave School of Psychology, Social Work and Social Policy,
University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA, Australia
Esther Hong Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Woodland Hills,
CA, USA
Abigail Issarraras  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Jason F. Jent  University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL,
USA
Courtney D. Jorgenson Department of Special Education, University of
Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
SungWoo Kahng  Department of Health Psychology, University of Missouri,
Columbia, MO, USA
Maysa M. Kaskas  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Shivani A. Kaushal Yale School of Medicine, Child Study Center, New
Haven, CT, USA
Gail N. Kemp  Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
Contributors ix

Vinay S. Kothapalli Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center,


Shreveport, LA, USA
Tessa K. Kritikos  Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
Janet D. Latner  Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii, Honolulu,
HI, USA
Justin B. Leaf  Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, USA
James F. Leckman Yale School of Medicine, Child Study Center, New
Haven, CT, USA
Michelle S. Lemay Department of Psychology, Idaho State University,
Pocatello, ID, USA
Nicholas Long Department of Pediatrics, University of Arkansas for
Medical Sciences and Arkansas Children’s Hospital, Little Rock, AR, USA
Paul R. Martin School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Mt
Gravatt, QLD, Australia
Johnny L. Matson  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Corey I. McGill Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Shawn E. McNeil Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center,
Shreveport, LA, USA
Pamela McPherson Northwest Louisiana Human Services District,
Shreveport, LA, USA
Karen Milligan  Child Self-Regulation Lab, Ryerson University, Toronto,
ON, Canada
Ashley Morgan Child Development Institute, Integra Program, Toronto,
ON, Canada
Peter Muris Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht
University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Erin E. O’Connor  Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
Becky Penrod Department of Psychology, California State University,
Sacramento, Sacramento, CA, USA
Justyna B. Piasecka Yale School of Medicine, Child Study Center, New
Haven, CT, USA
Marjory Phillips  Child Development Institute, Integra Program, Toronto,
ON, Canada
Robert D. Rieske Department of Psychology, Idaho State University,
Pocatello, ID, USA
x Contributors

Juliet K. Rosewall Population Health Research Institute, St. George’s,


University of London, London, UK
Paige M. Ryan Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Krista Saksena Department of Psychology, California State University,
Sacramento, CA, USA
Annabel Sibalis  Child Self-Regulation Lab, Ryerson University, Toronto,
ON, Canada
Denis G. Sukhodolsky  Yale School of Medicine, Child Study Center, New
Haven, CT, USA
Timothy Thompson Department of School Psychology, University of
Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, MS, USA
Micaela A. Thordarson  ASPIRE Adolescent Intensive Outpatient Program,
Children’s Hospital of Orange County, Orange, CA, USA
Martha C. Tompson  Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
Allison M. Waters School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University,
Mt Gravatt, QLD, Australia
Sara M. Weinkauf  JBA Institute, Aliso Viejo, CA, USA
Allison Weinstein  University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami,
FL, USA
About the Editor

Johnny L. Matson, PhD  is professor and distinguished research master in


the Department of Psychology at Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge,
LA, USA. He has also previously held a professorship in psychiatry and clini-
cal psychology at the University of Pittsburgh. He is the author of more than
800 publications including 41 books. He served as the founding editor-in-­
chief for the journals Research in Developmental Disabilities (Elsevier) and
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders (Elsevier) and currently serves as the
editor-in-chief for the Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
(Springer).

xi
History

Johnny L. Matson and Claire O. Burns

Contents cure or means of supporting the child or their


Overview................................................................ .....  1 family.
Early attempts to develop effective classifica-
Early History.............................................................. 2
tion and treatment occurred in the late 1800s and
Conclusions................................................................. 8 early 1900s. Lightner Witmer, for example,
References................................................................... 8 established the first children’s clinic at the
University of Pennsylvania in 1896. The focus of
his efforts was on the assessment, diagnosis, and
intervention of children’s learning and behavior
Overview problems. Parry-Jones (1989) almost three
decades ago made several observations regarding
Disabilities, both physical and emotional/intel- the history of child mental health and develop-
lectual, have been known and reported since the mental movement that still hold true. He noted
beginning of recorded history. However, modern that developments were occurring but were
mental health care as we know it is of recent ori- slower and more discontinuous than in other sci-
gin. Prior to these modern developments, chil- entific fields. We would note the advances in
dren in ancient Greece and Rome were left computer science which have been dramatic and
outdoors to die. They were generally considered very rapid in comparison.
a burden to society and could not be supported. A variety of reasons may be at play in explain-
Explanations for these disabilities consisted of ing the rate of improvements. While computing is
punishment from the God(s) or demonic posses- a complex enterprise, to interface between human
sion. These nefarious causes resulted in even less genetics, biochemistry, and a vast number of
sympathy for these children and their ultimate environmental variables makes for a much more
fate. Treatments as we currently know them were diverse and complex set of problems in the field
unknown. Thus, many of these conditions had no of mental health. Second, there is a stigma asso-
ciated with intellectual disabilities and mental
health that does not exist in many other scientific
fields. This affects people who chose to work in
J.L. Matson • C.O. Burns (*)
the field and often resource issues. Third, far
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA fewer people are affected by these childhood
e-mail: cburn26@lsu.edu maladies than persons who use cell phones. Thus,

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 1


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_1
2 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

there is also an awareness problem with respect was devoted to developing educational methods
to the general population. Fourth, many of the aimed at improving the quality of life for handi-
problems in childhood mental health and devel- capped children (Malson, 1972). These efforts
opmental disabilities have only recently been began a trend toward efforts to care for disadvan-
defined. Fifth, the potential for profits is much taged youth.
less in mental health than in many other scientific The USA and Europe were leaders in the
fields. Funding obviously can help speed the development of the modern mental health move-
development of new knowledge but dispropor- ment as we know it. Much of the early clinical
tionately goes to other field. Sixth, the mental work focused on identifying children with learn-
health field is maintained by a high level of iner- ing issues. Most famously, Alfred Binet and
tia. Many methodologies such as psychoanalysis Theodore Simon set out to develop an intelli-
persist even though many other treatments with gence quotient test whose purpose was to distin-
better databases have long since overtaken them. guish children with intellectual disabilities from
Seventh, until the last half of the twentieth cen- typically developing children (Boake, 2002).
tury, many types of psychopathology were not Later Lewis Terman, a professor at Stanford
recognized as occurring in children. University, adapted the scale for an American
As recently as 1978, Monroe Lefkowitz and population. These developments constituted the
Nancy Burton argued that the diagnosis of pedi- beginning of the psychological testing movement
atric depression may be inappropriate. They (Mulberger, 2017).
argued that symptoms of depression were actu- A corollary of these developments was the
ally transitory developmental behaviors. More development of scales to assess adaptive behav-
recently, however, measures such as the Child ior. These scales have been established as a
Depression Inventory have been used success- means of evaluating another dimension of human
fully to identify childhood depression (Helsel & intelligence. They have largely been used to dif-
Matson, 1984). As recently as 1992, Harrington ferentiate people with typical cognitive develop-
asserted that the diagnosis of childhood depres- ment from persons with intellectual disabilities.
sion was controversial. He also asserted that Also, since these scales tap into a variety of skills
research was rapidly accumulating to demon- such as self-help, challenging behaviors, social
strate the veracity of this diagnosis. skills, and communication, they have been impor-
tant for developing treatment goals and for evalu-
ating intervention outcomes.
Early History The father of the adaptive behaviors scales
was Edgar Doll. The first rudimentary attempts to
The history of child mental health, specifically, tap into these skills involved a score card he
and psychology, in general, has been staples of developed in the 1920s. By 1936 Doll had devel-
the mental health field for many years. In fact, oped the Vineland Social Maturity Scale. This
courses on the history of psychology have been test had 117 items scored on a Likert format.
imbedded in graduate and undergraduate psy- Since that time, numerous other measures that
chology programs for many years (Hilgard, are similar have been developed to measure adap-
Leary, & McGuire, 1991). tive behavior and developmental milestones.
In the 1800s, the reporting of children with The development of assessment methods spe-
marked deficits in development was written about cific to children has continued, and the pace of
by professionals. The case of the Wild Boy of these developments has quickened with time.
Aveyron was perhaps the most famous of these Beginning with intelligence and achievement
reports. “Victor” was treated in an attempt to testing, the child assessment literature has
improve communication and socialization. resulted in general scales that measure a broad
Treatment was partially successful. Itard, the range of disorders or problems such as the Child
physician who treated Victor over a 5 year period, Behavior Checklist to more recent measures
History 3

s­ pecific to individual disorders. Thus, scales that programs and programs designed to treat a wide
only measure depression, phobias, autism, func- range of mental health and educational concerns.
tional assessment of challenging behaviors, As such, this clinic is representative of the evalu-
social skills, and ADHD have been published. ation of the child mental health field. Clinicians
Often several scales exist to address each of these and researchers now generally agree that comor-
problem areas. Meyer and colleagues (2001) con- bid or even multiple comorbid conditions can be
cluded that for clinical psychologists, assessment present for one child. Thus, comprehensive care
is second only to psychotherapy in professional includes the involvement of multiple professions
importance. This underscores how high a priority to provide holistic treatment.
these developments have become.
The term psychotherapy is more restrictive
than treatment. This latter label will be the focus Exploring Specific Disorders
of this volume. Treatment here is defined not just
by psychotherapy methods such as cognitive Intellectual disabilities, behavior problems, and
behavioral therapy. Also, covered here are educa- juvenile delinquency have been identified and
tional method and variations of applied behavior treated for over a century. However, many of the
analysis, as well as some focus on pharmacother- more specific neurodevelopmental disorders and
apy. This latter approach has been used primarily specific form of psychology are of much more
with severe psychopathology such as pediatric recent origin. For many years, most of the neuro-
bipolar disorder. developmental disorders were simply lumped
into the diagnostic label mental deficiency
(Wolff, 2004). Researchers in the mental health
Early History of Treatments field now realize that this diagnostic area is far
more complex than originally believed. There are
As noted earlier, Witmer is credited with devel- hundreds of genetic causes of intellectual dis-
oping the first treatment clinic for children in abilities. More disorders and causes will undoubt-
1897 at the University of Pennsylvania. The edly be discovered in the near future. This fact
focus was on behavioral (e.g., aggression, tan- applies to a number of well-recognized condi-
trums, noncompliance) and school-based learn- tions such as autism, ADHD, depression, and
ing problems. However, the greatest early factor anxiety disorders as well.
leading to the child mental health movement cen- For example, the origins of the modern defini-
tered on juvenile delinquency. The general view tion of autism and Asperger’s disorder date to the
of the judicial system a century ago was that 1940s (Asperger, 1944; Kanner, 1943). These
because youthful offenders were still develop- researchers were believed to have observed and
mentally immature, they had different needs than categorized this disorder independently.
adult offenders (Grisso, 2007). It was also recog- However, momentum built slowly. Initially, the
nized that these individuals had great potential to condition was described in children with little
get their lives back on track given their very recognition that it is a lifelong disorder. Also,
young age. autism was considered rare. Recognized in the
Judge Baker in Massachusetts ran a juvenile Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the
court in the early 1900s. His view was that these American Psychiatric Association, the definition
children had gotten into trouble largely due to has evolved since then, and autism is now consid-
poor home and school environments. A clinic ered to be a common ailment, occurring in 1 out
named after Judge Baker was established at of 66 people according to the American Center
Harvard University to provide outpatient care for for Disease Control.
children with mental health needs. The clinic, ADHD is of similarly recent origins. While
which is still in operation at the time of this writ- symptoms of inattention and overactivity have
ing, gradually expanded to include residential been described since the mid-1800s, the first
4 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

o­ fficial description of the condition was in the research on the topic had proliferated (Cicchetti
DSM-II (Wolraich, 2006). This manual was pub- & Toth, 1998).
lished in 1968. Treatment dates back a bit further Anxiety had by 1997, according to Dadds,
to Charles Bradley, a physician who reported on Spence, Holland, Barrett, and Laurens, been
the positive effects of stimulant medications for identified as a significant problem requiring more
children. attention from researchers and clinicians. Viken
From these early beginnings, ADHD has come (1985) stated that it was the most common men-
to be one of the most extensively studied child- tal health concern among children, and Mattison
hood disorders (Goldman, Genel, Bezman, & and Hooper (1992) noted that the disorder was
Slanetz, 1998). Additionally, research has related to significant and wide-ranging social
expanded to adults, since more recently the disor- impairments. Numerous studies using behavior-
der has been conceptualized as a lifelong condi- ally based intervention, most notably cognitive
tion. The diagnosis has become so prevalent that behavior therapy (CBT), have demonstrated the
ADHD now accounts for up to half of children in efficacy of psychologically based interventions.
some mental health populations (Cantwell, Early intervention and preventative programs
1996). These are remarkable numbers given that aimed at children who present risk factors for
ADHD was first described in the DSM in 1980 childhood and adolescent anxiety disorders have
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980). Since also proven to be useful in heading off anxiety or
that time, a steady increase in the use of medica- remediating the symptoms when they are present.
tion has occurred, along with dramatic increases These efforts, early on, can pay positive divi-
on add-on medications to stimulants in the form dends in terms of moderating adult anxiety disor-
of atypical antipsychotic medications. This latter ders as well (Chorpita & Barlow, 1998).
trend, in our view, is not particularly helpful. Researchers, in general, have accepted a devel-
Depression in children is another disorder that opmental model of psychopathology. That is,
is now receiving considerable attention. As with symptom complexes do not just rapidly appear.
autism and ADHD, the recognition of this prob- Rather, various risk factors collude to gradually
lem has been of recent origin. Lefkowitz and develop complex symptom clusters. Also, the pro-
Burton (1978), for example, in a frequently refer- fessional mental health community has come to
enced paper noted some skepticism regarding the realize that co-occurrence of various mental health
existence of this condition in children. A rapid and neurodevelopmental disorders is very com-
change in views on childhood depression mon. Estimates are that 70% of person with autism
occurred. By 2007, Bhatia and Bhatia asserted also meet criteria for intellectual disabilities.
that at a specific time, as many as 15% of chil- Depression and childhood anxiety disorders also
dren and adolescents presented with some symp- overlap a good deal. Again the rates of comorbidity
toms of depression. These authors also underscore are very high. A range of 20–70% has been reported
the need for screening and early detection. with the actual number perhaps somewhere in
Lefkowitz and Burton (1978) also decry the lack between (Angold, Costello, & Erkauli, 1999).
of instruments to establish that depressive symp- The notion of different age norms and symp-
toms were independent of normal childhood tom differences at various stages of childhood,
development. Within a few years, well-­ along with high rates of comorbid disorders, has
established assessment methods such as the Child changed the thinking about how childhood men-
Depression Inventory were in wide use (Bhatia & tal health and neurodevelopmental disorders are
Bhatia, 2007). Some differences do exist in adult assessed and treated. The field has evolved from
versus child symptoms. For example, prepubertal addressing one off disorders to the recognition
children exhibit more psychomotor agitation and that multiple disorders and age-related symptom
somatic complaints. Thus, in fairly short order presentation may vary considerably.
History 5

Approaches to Child Mental Health concerns have been critical to the development of
more comprehensive care by providing a com-
Initially, and for most of recorded history, the mon diagnostic language for identifying children
causes of mental health and neurodevelopmental in need of care.
conditions were attributed to supernatural causes Mental health has also been a concern of
and were usually lumped together. This approach national governments. The American National
was delayed for a time by Hippocrates who pos- Mental Health Act appeared in 1946 (Cutler,
ited a biological explanation revolving around Bevilacqua, & McFarland, 2003). The National
substances and their imbalance in the body. The Institute of Mental Health followed shortly there-
advent of the middle ages saw a return to the after. Another significant advancement supported
notion of external forcers aimed at punishing by the federal government occurred in 1963 when
individuals and/or their parents reemerged. the Community Mental Health Centers
Freud introduced modern thinking regarding Construction Act was passed. This resulted in
mental health issues. He popularized the notion outpatient mental health clinic in counties and
that these disorders had environmental causes possibly throughout the USA. Eventually, by the
which followed a developmental course. Research 1970s, the range of services provided included
on biochemical causes and treatments would fol- inpatient, outpatient, consultation, education, and
low in the 1950s, and soon after the rapid devel- emergency services across the age spectrum
opment of genetic factors which contribute to including children.
mental health and neurodevelopmental factors Stephan, Weist, Kataoka, Adelsheim, and
followed. Mills (2007) describe how schools are taking on
As the knowledge base regarding causes of a greater and greater role in the delivery of child
mental health issues in children matured and the mental health services. One of the major current
knowledge base began to expand exponentially, focuses is to expand the role of primary and sec-
service models and how to deliver services ondary schools in screening, identifying, and
expanded accordingly. Early in the chapter, we treating mental health problems of children and
noted the juvenile justice system and its import adolescents. Another focus has been to better
on youth services. However, other equally pow- coordinate between the family, the community
erful forces were also at work. One of these mental health, and the educational system. It is
trends was the institutionalization movement of generally believed that imbedding mental health
the late 1800s through the 1960s and 1970s. in this holistic manner can be useful in decreas-
Large congregate facilities for persons with ing the considerable stigma that is associated
mental health needs and separate facilities for with mental health considerations. Dealing with
people with intellectual disabilities across the stigma and substance abuse receives particular
USA and Europe and across the lifespan were emphasis. Weist, Lowie, Flaherty, and Pruitt
constructed. With time the institutionalization (2001) emphasize that school and community
movement gave way to the notion that persons programs are critical for expanding the type and
should be integrated into general society to the amount of mental health services that can be pro-
extent possible. For persons with intellectual dis- vided. Thus, integrating all the major environ-
abilities, this trend was called normalization. In ments in which the child lives has become a goal
the school environment, it was referred to as of intervention. This approach has been com-
mainstreaming. bined with the notion that holistic care also
The last few decades have seen additional involves including multiple disciplines to meet
expansion and change of mental health systems. the complex nature of current intervention. One
Egger and Emde (2011), for example, describe of the greatest challenges in implementing this
the development of diagnostic and classification service model is coordination. Often parents are
systems for early childhood. Developments of overwhelmed with the number of treatments,
criteria and means of classifying mental health both validated and not, which are available.
6 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

Professional services that exist to assist the fami- problems. The Center for Disease Control is a
lies in this critical task are needed. federal agency that has been instrumental in
studying this issue. Obviously, this information is
critical for planning service provision and estab-
Trends and  Topics lishing priorities.
Self-mutilation among adolescent psychiatric
The area of childhood education and mental inpatients has also received the attention of
health is a rapidly evolving topic. A few of these researchers and clinicians in the adolescent age
important developments are noted as a means of group. Nock and Prinstein (2004) note that ado-
adding context to relevant developments in the lescent begin these behaviors at early ages with
field. adolescence being a particularly strong risk fac-
International developments have been a cata- tor. The problem is considered to be very serious
lyst for issues being addressed in the child mental and pervasive among adolescents. For psychiatric
health literature. For example, Chaves and col- adolescent inpatients, rates of 40–60% for self-­
leagues (2017) write about the need for health mutilation have been reported. The etiology of
care for refugees to Australia. They argue that this self-mutilation is often believed to be auto-
within a month of arrival, children should have a matic reinforcement.
tailored assessment and initial treatment plan. Functional assessment to determine environ-
Turner and Mohan (2016) also discuss child mental factors that maintain challenging behavior
mental health services. The focus of their paper has become a major topic of study within the child
was on the stigma and attitudes of child mental mental health and developmental disabilities
health services among Asian-Indian parents liv- ­literature. Using these methods, which involve
ing in the USA. observation, checklists, and manipulating envi-
Another interesting paper on global child ronmental variables, has proven to be very useful.
mental health is described by Schneider, Okello, Common maintaining factors include inadvertent
and Lehmann (2016). They looked at the increase social or tangible reinforcement, escape from an
in health-care publications internationally over undesirable environment or task, and automatic
the last 10 years. They note a sevenfold increase reinforcement. This latter maintaining variable
over this time period with half of the papers suggests that the behavior is reinforcing in and of
addressing Africa; Asia was the topic of a third of itself (see self-mutilation above).
the paper, while only 11% focused on Central Transitioning children to adult mental health
and South America. Most of the papers from this services as they age has also been a topic of dis-
latter group focused on emerging nations, partic- cussion. Reid and Schraeder (2017) discuss this
ularly Brazil. Most papers were focused on gen- topic and note that many adolescents disengage
eral health issues such as maternal health, from the system during this transition period.
neonatal care, and reproductive health care. Only These authors note that this failure to transition is
4% of the papers were aimed at mental health, particularly acute for persons experiencing anxi-
showing that this topic is not a priority at this ety or depression where symptoms often wax and
time for underdeveloped countries. wane.
In the USA, another emerging trend is the Comorbidity of conditions has also become a
focus on mental health issues within the ethnic major area of interest (Ormel et al., 2017). This
minority community (Vega & Rumbaut, 1991). topic has become one of the most heavily
These authors list a number of groups under con- researched and reported on in recent years.
sideration, which reflect a broad international Comorbid internalizing disorders predicted psy-
heritage. One factor being addressed involved chological outcomes and drug use, while exter-
large epidemiological studies which can help elu- nalizing problems predicted health outcomes,
cidate the scope and nature of mental health according to these authors.
History 7

Caron and Rutter (1991) have also addressed long longitudinal study of 7–9-year-olds who had
the comorbidity issue. Their article underscores displayed disobedience, defiance of authority, fits
how thinking on this topic has changed in recent of temper, aggression, irritability, property
decades. They note that for many years, medical destruction, stealing, cruelty to others, and other
students were taught to use only one diagnosis antisocial behaviors which were collectively
when possible (Kendell, 1975). Caron and Rutter described as conduct disorders. These persons
(1991), to their credit, note that comorbid mental were followed up at 21–25 years of age. The
health conditions are very common in childhood sample was at particular risk for convicting
mental health. They also stress that failing to rec- crimes, mental health problems, substance
ognize comorbidities can result in incorrect dependence, and difficulties with partners’ rela-
assumption about the nature of the child’s prob- tionships. Thus, how childhood problems mani-
lems. We would also add that failure to recognize fest themselves in adulthood has been a central
comorbidities can result in insufficient and/or focus of the developmental psychopathology
poorly executed treatment. literature.
Several other topics have been trending in Academic skills constitute another important
recent years and are worth mentioning. area that continues to receive a great deal of
Tzoumakis and colleagues (2017) discuss the attention. As mentioned early on in this chapter,
impact on young children that parents with a his- academic problems and juvenile delinquency
tory of legal offenses can have on their children. were among the earliest topics addressed in mod-
They posit that there is a strong correlation ern child psychology. Children with mental
between these parental problems and childhood health problems and/or developmental disabili-
aggression. ties such as autism or intellectual disabilities are
Child sexual abuse has also become an impor- at particular risk for problems in this area
tant topic for study (Chiesa & Goldson, 2017). (Nelson, Benner, Lone, & Smith, 2004). These
These authors note that detecting physical signs concerns have led to the development of special-
of abuse may be very difficult. They also note ized supports and programs to deal with these
that fluids or other physical signs on clothing are disorder-specific concerns (Jent, Brown,
more likely to be the most likely evidence and Davidson, Cruz, & Weinstein, in press).
need to be detected within 72 h of the offense. Mental health professionals such as school
Obviously, developing accurate means of detect- and clinical psychologists are also important for
ing sexual abuse of children and then providing coordinating services. Jent and colleagues (in
effective interventions are very important. Better press) note that a variety of services may be pro-
technologies and procedures for detection are vided by these professionals such as developing
needed. psychologically based interventions. Treatments
Another important area that has become a include token economies, self-control proce-
focal point of the child mental health movement dures, means to enhance concentration, goal-­
in the past few decades is developmental psycho- setting, and related methods that can enhance
pathology. It is now recognized that mental health academic performance on overall adjustment to
problems are expressed in different ways at dif- school and home environments. Helping parents
ferent ages. Also, various disorders have onsets navigate this complex process is also a role often
that vary across the lifespan. Autism spectrum assumed by the mental health professional. The
disorders appear very early in life, while schizo- development of school guidelines at the local,
phrenia does not. Fergusson, Horwood, and state, and federal level have also helped to
Ridder (2005) provide one example of ongoing enhance and bolster these evermore complex and
research in this area. They conducted a 25-year-­ comprehensive treatment models.
8 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

Conclusions Boake, C. (2002). From the Binet-Simon to the


Wechsler-Bellevue: Tracing the history of intelli-
gence testing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Modern history regarding the treatment of devel- Neuropsychology, 24, 383–405.
opmental disabilities and mental health issues in Cantwell, D. P. (1996). Attention deficit disorder: A
children is of recent origin. The filed has adopted review of the last 10 years. Journal of the Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 978–987.
the notion of evidence-based treatment. This
Caron, C., & Rutter, M. (1991). Comorbidity in child psy-
move has been a very positive one since there are chopathology: Concepts, issues, and research strate-
so many unsubstantiated treatments that have gies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32,
been aimed at children. The 1970s and 1980s saw 1063–1080.
Chaves, N. J., Paxton, G. A., Biggs, B. A., Thambrian, A.,
the beginning of a rapidly expanding research
Garner, J., Williams, J., … Davis, J. S. (2017). The
base across a host of topics. Educational and Australasian society for infectious diseases and refu-
learning-based treatments (operant and classical gee health networks of Australia recommendations
condition) and the focus on cognitively based for health assessment for people from refugee-like
backgrounds: An abridged outline. Medical Journal of
treatments, particularly cognitive behavior ther-
Australia, 207(7), 310–315.
apy, have become very popular. Also, the use of Chiesa, A., & Goldson, E. (2017). Child sexual abuse.
psychotropic drugs has grown exponentially. The Pediatrics in Review, 38, 105–118.
number of drugs given to one child has increased Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). The development
of anxiety: The role of control in the early environ-
as have dosage levels. Children are being pre-
ment. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 3–21.
scribed these medications at younger and younger Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1998). The development of
ages. Often the research support for these medi- depression in children and adolescents. American
cation practices is minimal. Psychologist, 53, 221–241.
Cutler, D. L., Bevilacqua, J., & McFarland, B. H. (2003).
Overall, the resources allocated toward treat-
Four decades of community mental health: A sym-
ing childhood mental health conditions and neu- phony in four movements. Community Mental Health
rodevelopmental disorders are growing. Journal, 39, 381–398.
Emerging nations still devote the bulk of their Dadds, M. R., Spence, S. H., Holland, D. E., Barrett,
P. M., & Laurens, K. R. (1997). Prevention and early
resources to general health as compared to men-
intervention for anxiety disorders: A controlled trial.
tal health concerns. However, while the mental Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65,
health field is still not on a par with general health 627–635.
concerns, that priority is changing. From the Doll, E. A. (1936). The Vineland social maturity scale,
Publication of the Training School at Vineland
massive amount of information covered in this
Department Research Series, No. 3. New Jersey, MI:
volume, it is apparent that childhood adjustment American Guidance Service.
and learning is becoming a greater priority with Egger, H. L., & Emde, R. N. (2011). Developmentally
time. sensitive diagnostic criteria for mental health disor-
ders in early childhood: DSM-IV, RDC-PA, and the
revised DL:03. American Psychologist, 66, 95–106.
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Ridder, E. M.
References (2005). Show me the child at seven: The consequences
of conduct problems in childhood for prosocial func-
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic tioning in adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and
and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Psychiatry, 46, 837–849.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Goldman, L. S., Genel, M., Bezman, R. J., & Slanetz, P. J.
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkauli, A. (1999). (1998). Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/
Comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents.
Psychiatry, 40, 57–87. Journal of the American Medical Association, 279,
Asperger, H. (1944). Die “autiscihen psychopa- 1100–1107.
then” in kineralter. Archive Furpsychiatrie und Grisso, T. (2007). Progress and perils in the juvenile
Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136. justice and mental health movement. Journal of the
Bhatia, S. K., & Bhatia, S. C. (2007). Childhood and ado- American Academy Psychiatric Law, 35, 158–167.
lescent depression. American Family Physician, 75, Harrington, R. (1992). Annotation: The natural history and
73–80. treatment of child and adolescent affective ­disorders.
History 9

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, (2017). Functional outcomes of child and adolescent
1287–1292. mental disorders. Current disorder most important
Helsel, W., & Matson, J. L. (1984). Assessment of by psychiatric history matters as well. Psychological
depression in children: The internal structure of the Medicine, 47, 1271–1282. 
child depression inventory. Behaviour Research and Parry-Jones. (1989). Annotation: The history of child
Therapy, 22, 289–298. and adolescent psychiatry: Its present day relevance.
Hilgard, E. R., Leary, D. E., & McGuire, G. R. (1991). The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 3–11.
history of psychology: A survey and critical assess- Reid, G. J., & Schraeder, K. E. (2017). Who should tran-
ment. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 79–107. sition? Defining a target population of youth with
Jent, J. F., Brown, T. M., Davidson, B. C., Cruz, L., & depression and anxiety that will require adult mental
Weinstein, A. (in press). Academic skills. In J. L. health care. Journal of Behavioral Health Services
Matson (Eds.) Handbook of childhood psychopa- and Research, 44, 316–330.
thology and developmental disabilities: Treatment. Schneider, H., Okello, D., & Lehmann, V. (2016). The
New York: Springer. global pendulum swing towards community health
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con- workers in low- and middle-income countries. A scop-
tact. The Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. ing review of trends, geographical distribution, and
Kendell, R. E. (1975). The role of diagnosis in psychiatry. programmatic orientations, 2005 to 2014. Human
Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific. Resources for Health, 14, 65.
Lefkowitz, M. M., & Burton, N. (1978). Childhood Stephen, S. H., Weist, M., Kataoka, S., Adelsheim, S., &
depression: A critique of the concept. Psychological Mills, C. (2007). Transformation of children’s men-
Bulletin, 85, 716–726. tal health services: The role of school mental health.
Malson, I. (1972). Wolf children and the problem of Psychiatric Services, 58, 1330–1338.
human nature and Jean Itard the Wild Boy of Aveyron Turner, E. A., & Mohan, S. (2016). Child mental health
(trans: Fawett, E., Ayton, P., White, J.). London: NLB. services and psychotherapy attitudes among Asian
Mattison, R. E., & Hooper, S. R. (1992). The history of Indian parents: An exploratory study. Community
modern classification of child and adolescent psychi- Mental Health Journal, 52, 989–997.
atric disorders: An overview. In Hooper, S. R., Hynd, Tzoumakis, S., Dean, K., Green, M. J., Zheng, C., Kariuki,
G. W., & Mattison, R. E. (Eds). Child psychopathol- M., Harris, F., … Laurens, K. R. (2017). The impact
ogy: Diagnostic criteria and clinical assessment. New of parental offending on offspring aggression in early
Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.  childhood: A population-based record linkage study.
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 52,
Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., … Reed, G. M. (2001). 445–455.
Psychological testing and psychological assessment: Vega, W. A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (1991). Ethnic minorities
A review of the evidence. American Psychologist, 56, and mental health. Annual Review of Sociology, 17,
128–165. 351–385.
Mulberger, A. (2017). Mental association: Testing indi- Viken, A. (1985). Psychiatric epidemiology in a sample of
vidual differences before Binet. Journal of the History 1,510 ten-year-old children-I: Prevalence. Journal of
of the Behavioral Sciences, 53, 176–198. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 55–75.
Nelson, J. R., Benner, G. J., Lane, K. L., & Smith, B. W. Weist, M. D., Lowie, J. A., Flaherty, L. T., & Pruitt, D.
(2004). Academic achievement of K-12 students with (2001). Collaboration among the education, mental
emotional and behavioral disorders. Exceptional health, and public health systems to promote youth
Children, 71, 59–73. mental health. Psychiatric Services, 52, 1348–1351.
Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional Wolff, S. (2004). The history of autism. European Child
approach to the assessment of self-mutilative behav- and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 201–208.
ior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Wolraich, M. A. (2006). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity
72(5), 885. disorder: Can it be recognized and treated in children
Ormel, J., Oerlemans, A. M., Raven, D., Lacuelle, O. M., younger than 5 years? Infants and Young Children, 19,
Hartman, C. A., Veenstra, R., … Oldehinkel, A. J. 86–93.
Developmental Psychopathology
and the Epidemiology
and Expression
of Psychopathology from Infancy
Through Early Adulthood

Corina Benjet

Contents  hat Is Developmental


W
What Is Developmental Psychopathology?   11 Psychopathology?
Psychopathology in Infancy  13
Developmental psychopathology is a multidisci-
Psychopathology in Preschool/Toddlerhood  14 plinary field that examines how biological, psy-
Psychopathology in School-Aged Children  15 chological, and socio-contextual factors interact
to determine trajectories of continuity and dis-
Psychopathology in  Adolescence  16
continuity in adaptive and maladaptive function-
Psychopathology in Emerging Adulthood  17 ing over the life course (Cicchetti & Toth, 2009).
Anxiety Disorders from Childhood Development is conceived of as the progression
Through Adolescence and Emerging and organization of biological, social, affective,
Adulthood  18
and cognitive structures toward increased differ-
Depression from Childhood entiation and hierarchical integration and is
Through Adolescence and Emerging cumulative (Gottlieb, 1991; Sroufe, 2009). Thus,
Adulthood  18
in any transition or stage of reorganization, prior
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder structures are assimilated into subsequent struc-
Throughout Childhood, Adolescence,
and Emerging Adulthood  19
tures through hierarchical integration. In other
words, current adaptation is a product of current
Conclusion  20
circumstances (environment, epigenetics) and
References  20 prior circumstances (environment, genes) and
adaptations (past development). So while early
challenges accumulate over the life course which
may overload coping abilities for increasingly
complex demands, social contexts may change
biological processes such as through epigenetics,
changes in the neuroendocrine system, and ­neural
processing of social cues (Pollak, 2015). Further,
each developmental stage has its particular chal-
C. Benjet (*)
lenges or tasks. For example, among the develop-
National Institute of Psychiatry Ramón de la Fuente,
Mexico City, Mexico mental challenges during adolescence is
e-mail: cbenjet@imp.edu.mx adaptation to the physical changes of puberty,

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 11


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_2
12 C. Benjet

exploration of sexuality, increased responsibilities To exemplify, a teenage girl suffering from


and liberties, and identity exploration. Difficulties depression may have developed depression as a
handling these challenges specific to adolescence consequence of early sexual abuse, current school
are risk factors for the development or mainte- bullying, genetic vulnerability due to having a
nance of psychopathology during this stage and short allele of the HTTTP genotype, or early
are likely to be moderated by other factors, par- pubertal maturation or a combination of any of
ticularly prior adaptation and current the former, whereas an individual with early sex-
circumstances. ual abuse may develop during adolescence
Developmental psychopathology focuses on depression, conduct disorder, substance abuse, or
both normal and abnormal development as mutu- another disorder or no disorder at all when faced
ally informative. Psychopathology can only be with pubertal development and new interactions
understood in reference to normal development, with the opposite sex, depending on genetic vul-
especially since many psychiatric disorders are nerabilities, resilience factors, how the traumatic
not qualitatively different from normal experi- situation of sexual abuse was handled at the time,
ence but rather quantitatively different. For current peer group characteristics and dynamics,
example, the symptoms of attention deficit etc.
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) include “often Understanding the continuity and discontinu-
has difficulty sustaining attention in tasks or play ity of developmental pathways is important for
activities,” “Often leaves seat in situations when understanding the natural course of disorder and
remaining seated is expected,” and “often blurts changes and similarities of symptoms throughout
out an answer before a question has been com- development. Continuity of disorder does not
pleted” (APA, 2013). To judge if the frequency is mean simply the persistence of the disorder over
often enough to constitute abnormal behavior, time. Substantial comorbidity and fluidity of psy-
one must know the frequency that is normal for chiatric diagnosis especially in children and ado-
these behaviors at each developmental period. lescents have been reported (Costello, Copeland,
Similarly, a criterion of the newly incorporated & Angold, 2011; Wittchen, Lieb, Pfister, &
disruptive mood dysregulation disorder specifies Scuster, 2000). Thus the concept of homotypic
that “the temper outbursts are inconsistent with continuity versus heterotypic continuity has been
developmental level.” Thus an understanding of useful, homotypic continuity referring to a disor-
developmentally appropriate temper outbursts is der predicting itself over time (i.e., separation
essential to diagnosis. Conversely, given that anxiety disorder predicting later separation anxi-
psychopathology is conceived of as an exaggera- ety disorder) and heterotypic continuity referring
tion of the normal condition, understanding to a disorder predicting a different disorder over
abnormal development can provide important time (i.e., social anxiety disorder predicting later
insight into normal development as well (Toth & alcohol use disorder) (Costello et al., 2011).
Cicchetti, 2010). In this chapter, therefore, we aim to discuss
Transitions, or key life turning points, for the epidemiology and expression of psychopa-
example, school entry or pubertal maturation, are thology, both in terms of diagnostic categories
considered times when protective and risk factors and dimensionally, from infancy through emerg-
may act to change developmental risk trajectories ing adulthood from a developmental psychopa-
to a more adaptive or maladaptive pathway and thology perspective. It is important to note that
individuals may particularly benefit from thera- development does not end with emerging adult-
peutic interventions during these transitions hood, but the developmental stages of adulthood
(Elder, 1985; Toth & Cicchetti, 1999). An impor- are not the topic of this book. We will first discuss
tant principal of developmental psychopathology the developmental tasks and demands of each
is that there are multiple pathways to pathology stage. Then the epidemiology in terms of preva-
termed equifinality and any particular risk factor lence and risk factors by developmental stages
leads to multiple outcomes termed multifinality. will be discussed. Three specific types of disorders,
Developmental Psychopathology and the Epidemiology and Expression… 13

anxiety disorders, depression, and attention defi- Groh, Roisman, van Ijzendoorn, Bakermans-
cit hyperactivity disorder, will be exemplified Kranenburg, & Fearon, 2012; Humphreys &
over the course of childhood and adolescence. Zeanah, 2015; Montagna & Nosarti, 2016).
Infant temperament and attachment, in particular,
are two factors that have been studied in terms of
Psychopathology in Infancy predicting later psychopathology. For example,
the EDEN Mother-Child Cohort Study (Abulizi
Infancy, or the first 2 years of life, is the period et al., 2017) which followed mother-child pairs
of most rapid physical development (after the from 24 to 28 weeks of pregnancy over 5 years
prenatal period). Developmental tasks of infancy found that infants’ emotional temperament pre-
include, among others, gaining motor control to dicts emotional difficulties, conduct problems,
hold one’s head, sit, crawl, stand, walk, grasp, and symptoms of hyperactivity/inattention at age
and point; early language development and other 5.5, specifically active temperament predicting
cognitive abilities such as object permanence conduct problems and shyness predicting emo-
and becoming aware that one’s thoughts and tional problems. Various mechanisms have been
experiences are distinct from those of other peo- proposed to explain the association between tem-
ple emerge; social attachment is formed includ- perament and psychopathology: the risk model,
ing distinguishing the primary caregiver from the spectrum model, and the scar-effect model
others, forming a secure attachment to the pri- (Rettew & McKee, 2005). The risk model consid-
mary caregiver, and fear of strangers, both of ers temperament and psychopathology as qualita-
which lead to normative stranger and separation tively distinct. Certain temperaments contribute
anxiety; finally emotional development includes to a greater likelihood of developing a specific
learning to trust, quieting when comforted, self- disorder. Conversely, the spectrum model states
soothing, smiling to show pleasure, etc. The that psychiatric disorders are really extremes of
hypothalamic-­pituitary-­adrenocortical (HPA) temperament and thus are not taxonomically dis-
axis, which has an important role in stress regu- tinct. Finally the scar-effect model posits that
lation and in the development of psychopathol- temperament is an effect or consequence of the
ogy, is particularly responsive to external signals psychiatric disease. Rettew and McKee, upon
from in utero and in the first 3 months of life, reviewing the literature on this theme, conclude
whereas from the fourth and fifth months of life, that various mechanisms are viable to explain
the HPA axis becomes more regulated and less these associations and that conclusions from
reactive to small changes in stimulation suggest- individual studies tend to vary by the trait-disor-
ing a possible period of sensitivity to adverse der combination studied. However, they conclude
environments in the first few months of life that the most reasonable model is the risk model.
(Doom & Gunnar, 2013). Regardless of which model better explains these
While psychiatric disorders per se are rarely associations, early identification of temperamen-
manifested or diagnosed in infancy (with exception tal risks could lead to preventive interventions to
of some neurodevelopmental disorders), distur- promote more adaptive and less pathological
bances in meeting the developmental tasks of this developmental trajectories, particularly parent-
stage and experiences in infancy may influence child psychotherapy, for which there is evidence
or be early markers of later psychopathology. For of effectivity in infants (Barlow, Bennett,
example, insecure attachment, abuse and neglect, Midgley, Larkin, & Wei, 2015).
inadequate cognitive stimulation, low birth weight, Similarly, insecure attachment styles have
premature birth and birth complications, as well been found to be a risk factor for future psycho-
as genetic factors may impact the developmental pathology. Two meta-analyses (Fearon et al.,
trajectories of psychopathology (Brinksma 2010; Groh et al., 2012) found that insecure
et al., 2017; Fearon, Bakermans-­ Kranenburg, attachment styles, particularly disorganized and
van Ijzendoorn, Lapsley, & Roisman, 2010; avoidant attachment styles, were associated with
14 C. Benjet

externalizing problems in childhood and an child interventions are the most promising for
avoidant style with internalizing problems. this stage of life especially given that parent-child
Cassidy, Jones, and Shaver (2013) suggest that a relationship problems in the first 10 months of
promising explanation for the mechanisms of life were associated with more than twice the risk
association is via emotion regulation as a media- of a child disorder at 18 months.
tor given the relation between early attachment
and emotion regulation and stress responses and
the role of emotion dysregulation and the HPA  sychopathology in Preschool/
P
axis irregularities in many psychiatric disorders. Toddlerhood
Conversely, a secure attachment can buffer HPA
axis reactivity in infants and toddlers (Hostinar, The developmental tasks of the preschool years
Sullivan, & Gunnar, 2014). or toddlerhood, a period from approximately 2–4
Few epidemiologic studies have been con- or 5 years of age, include continued language
ducted to estimate the prevalence of psychopa- development, locomotion, fantasy play, self-­
thology in infancy. One of the few studies, the control, and sex-role identification toward the
Copenhagen County Child Cohort, which com- end of the stage or beginning of the next. Temper
prised all children born of mothers in a particular tantrums are common, attention span is short,
county of Copenhagen in the year 2000, reports and they begin to express new emotions such as
that for children 0–10 months of age, the most pride, shame, and jealousy.
common problems are feeding (30%), sleep Psychopathology at this stage is often a mal-
(20%), defecation (16%), gross motor function adaptive extreme of normative fears, emotional
(14.1%), general development (13%), infant lan- lability and reactivity, stubbornness, activity lev-
guage (11.7%), mother-children relation els, and attention deficits. Table 1 shows the
(10.1%), and the least frequent are tactile reac- Global Burden of Disease Study (GBD) esti-
tions (2.1%), parents way of speaking about the mates for mental and substance abuse disorders
child (3.7%), parents perception of contact with and self-harm by age group for the year 2015
the child (4.0%), and parents handling and care around the globe for both sexes combined.
(4.2%) (Meete Skovard, 2010). A subsample was Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) an indica-
evaluated at age one and a half using both tor that combines both the mortality and morbid-
International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) ity attributed to a disease and years lived with
and Diagnostic Classification Zero-to-Three disability (YLDs) are presented for distinct age
(D.C.: 0–3) diagnoses. The prevalence of any dis- groups from toddlerhood through emerging
order was 18%, the most frequent being parent-­ adulthood. As can be seen on the table, for
child relationship disturbances (8.5%) and youngsters aged 1–4, mental and substance use
regulatory disorders (7.1%). Furthermore, disorders account for 3.08% of YLDs and 0.51%
impairments in neurocognitive functions, lan- of DALYs with an additional 0.32% for self-­
guage development, disturbances in the child’s inflicted injuries and interpersonal violence. The
contact and communication, and relationship three disorders that contribute most to disability
problems at 0–10 months predicted any mental in this age group are autistic spectrum disorders
health disorder at 18 months. Neurodevelopmental (2.21%), idiopathic developmental intellectual
disorders were associated with pre- and perinatal disability (0.56%), and anxiety disorders
biological risks and were predicted by deviant (0.25%). These aforementioned estimates corre-
language development and impaired communica- spond to the percent of burden attributed to these
tion in the first 10 months of life, whereas risk disorders of all medical illnesses. However, for
factors for emotional, behavioral, eating, and prevalence estimates of mental disorders, there
sleeping disorders were parent-child disturbances are several community epidemiologic studies
and parental psychosocial adversities. Parent-­ that have been conducted on preschool children
Developmental Psychopathology and the Epidemiology and Expression… 15

Table 1  Global burden of disease 2015 estimatesa of fearful symptoms and hostile-aggressive behavior
years lived with disability and disability-adjusted life
in preschoolers were associated with twice the
years due to mental disorders from toddlerhood through
emerging adulthood risk of emotional difficulties at age 10–12
(Slemming et al., 2010). While an important pro-
Years lived Disability-­
with adjusted life Disability-­ portion of toddlers with psychopathology have
disability years adjusted life persistent psychopathology, it is important to
(YLDs) (DALYs) years (DALYs) remember that approximately half do not, such
attributed to attributed to attributed to
that protective factors and resilience are equally
mental and mental and self-harm and
Age substance use substance use interpersonal important to developmental pathways.
group disorders (%) disorders (%) violence (%)
1–4 3.08 0.51 0.32
5–9 10.32 5.00 0.45  sychopathology in School-Aged
P
10– 20.34 11.81 1.59 Children
14
15– 27.60 14.89 6.15
School entrance marks an important develop-
19
20– 31.81 16.16 8.03
mental transition with increased demands for
24 behavioral control and sustained attention.
a
Estimates from the Global Burden of Disease Study The developmental tasks of school-aged
interactive visualization tool https://vizhub.healthdata. children include cognitive tasks like achieving
org/gbd-compare/ concrete operations, friendship development,
skill learning, self-evaluation, and coopera-
tion. Once children begin school, they become
exposed to bullying, a common and chronic
(e.g., Egger & Angold, 2006; Gleason et al., stressor in school-aged children with impor-
2011; Keenan, Shaw, Walsh, Delliquadri, & tant mental health consequences (Moore et al.,
Giovannelli, 1997; Lavigne et al., 1996; 2017). Child maltreatment and childhood
Wichstrom & Berg-Nielsen, 2014) which report adversities in general are important risk fac-
prevalence rates ranging from 10.5% to 21.4% tors for the development of childhood
for any disorder, the most common disorders psychopathology.
being oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) in Mental and substance use disorders in chil-
some studies and generalized anxiety disorder dren aged 5–9 account for 10.32% of YLDs and
(GAD) in others. However these studies gener- 5.00% of DALYS with an additional 0.45% of
ally do not include autism spectrum disorders or DALYS due to self-harm and interpersonal vio-
intellectual disability. lence (Global Burden of Disease Study, 2015).
Research suggests that infant and toddler psy- The three types of disorders that account for the
chopathology is not transient such that of chil- greatest burden of disability in this age group are
dren who were studied when they were between conduct disorders (3.12%), autism spectrum dis-
12 and 40 months of age, 49.9% had persistent orders (2.63%), and anxiety disorders (2.54%).
psychopathology a year later which was pre- In the Great Smoky Mountains Study, a commu-
dicted by co-occurring problems, high family life nity survey of children from the Southeast of the
disruption, and parenting distress (Briggs-­ United States found that between 8.3% and
Gowan, Carter, Bosson-Heenan, Guyer, & 19.5% of 9–12-year-olds met criteria for any
Horwitz, 2006). Similarly another study reported 3-month disorder (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli,
that more than 50% of children who at age Keeler, & Angold, 2003). Rates were highest for
2–3 had a psychiatric disorder continued to 9–10-year-olds, descending thereafter until age
have a disorder 4 years later with greatest sta- 12 when rates begin to increase steadily there
bility for disruptive disorders (Lavigne et al., afterward. By age 16, 36.7% of children had
1998). A more recent study found that anxious- experienced at least one disorder.
16 C. Benjet

Some diagnostic continuity has been found Psychopathology in Adolescence


from preschool to school-aged children (Slemming
et al., 2010) and school-aged children to adoles- Adolescence is the next most rapid period of
cence (Copeland et al., 2013). An analysis of three growth in human development after infancy, requir-
prospective longitudinal studies covering the ages ing adaptive coping mechanisms, which can alter
9–32 found that having any childhood disorder the course of psychopathological development.
(those developing between 9 and 12 years of age) The developmental tasks of adolescence include
was associated with more than a threefold increase adaptation to pubertal maturation (i.e., secondary
in odds for having a disorder at any other point in sex characteristics), achieving formal operations,
time. While homotypic and heterotypic continuity emotional development, greater importance of the
was observed, the most persistent disorders were peer group, sexual exploration, psychological
behavioral disorders, particularly ADHD, which autonomy from parents, sex-role identity, internal-
tended to predict themselves over time, but not ized morality, and vocational decisions among the
later internalizing disorders, and anxiety and most important. Adolescents must contend with
depressive disorders tended to predict each other greater freedoms which test their decision-making
(Copeland et al., 2013). and expose them to risky situations while at the
Children with intellectual disabilities may be same time greater demands of responsibility.
at particular risk for additional psychopathology. The global burden of mental and substance
A 14-year longitudinal study of children and ado- use disorders increases substantially in adoles-
lescents with intellectual disabilities found that cence, accounting for 20.34% of YLDS in those
41% at wave I when they had an average age of aged 10–14 and 27.6% of those aged 15–19.
12 met criteria for comorbid psychopathology DALYs attributed to mental and substance use
and this rate declines slightly to 31% by early disorders are 11.81% in those aged 10–14 and
adulthood (Einfeld et al., 2006). While there 14.89% in older adolescents aged 15–19. The
were no initial differences in comorbid psycho- three types of disorders that cause most disability
pathology by level of intellectual disability, those in 10–14-year-olds are conduct disorders
with more mild intellectual disability and boys (6.35%), anxiety disorders (5.16%), and depres-
had a sharper decrease in comorbid psychopa- sive disorders (4.11%). In older adolescents aged
thology with age than those with severe or pro- 15–19, depressive disorders account for the
found intellectual disability. greatest amount of disability (9.46%), followed
Due to the greater dependence upon the fam- by anxiety disorders (5.67%) and conduct disor-
ily during childhood than adolescence, the results ders (4.21%); substance use disorders, bipolar
of one study found that family predictors were disorder, and schizophrenia also begin to contrib-
more strongly associated to childhood internal- ute to disability at this age. DALYs attributed to
izing symptoms (compared to adolescent inter- self-harm and interpersonal violence are esti-
nalizing symptoms), whereas peer victimization mated at 1.59% in 10–14-year-olds and climb to
was equally related to both childhood and adoles- 6.15% of 15–19-year-olds (Global Burden of
cent internalizing problems (Haltigan, Rosiman, Disease Study, 2015).
Cauffman, & Booth-LaForce, 2017). Many stud- Prevalence estimates of any psychiatric disor-
ies have found gene-environment interactions der in community surveys of adolescents range
predicting children’s mental health, particularly from 12% (Farbstein et al., 2010) to 50% (Shaffer
for interactions with child maltreatment and peer et al., 1996), with an average prevalence rate of
victimization and polymorphisms like the mono- 21.8% (Costello et al., 2011), and vary according
amine oxidase A, 5-HTTLPR, and BDNF, among to measurement method, diagnostic system,
others, though results have been mixed (Bellani, informant, ages included, diagnoses included,
Nobile, Bianchi, van Os, & Brambilla, 2012; and prevalence time period (lifetime, 12 months,
Benjet, Thompson, & Gotlib, 2010; Kim-Cohen 6 months, or current). From childhood to adoles-
et al., 2006). cence, there are an increase in rates of depres-
Developmental Psychopathology and the Epidemiology and Expression… 17

sion, substance use disorders, and some anxiety in some developing countries, the age of assuming
disorders (like panic disorder and agoraphobia) adult roles has increased contributing to a prolonged
but a decrease in other anxiety disorders (like period of emerging adulthood. This early adult
separation anxiety disorder) and attention deficit period is a highly heterogeneous life stage with a
hyperactivity disorder (Costello et al., 2011). diversity of life trajectories in terms of educational
Anxiety disorders are the most common during status and attainment, employment, economic inde-
this stage especially specific phobia and social pendence, living or not with the family of origin,
phobia followed by mood disorders and behav- etc. Arnett (2000) has proposed calling this stage
ioral disorders, while substance use disorders emerging adulthood with five defining psychologi-
begin to emerge at this stage (Benjet, Medina-­ cal characteristics: instability, possibilities, self-
Mora, Borges, Zambrano, & Aguilar-Gaxiola, focus, in-betweenness, and identity exploration.
2009; Merikangas et al., 2010). Bynner (2005) however argues that this stage of life
In the Great Smoky Mountain Study (Copeland is more greatly influenced by structural and social
et al., 2013), where homotypic and heterotypic factors such as employment and education opportu-
continuity of disorders from childhood to adoles- nities. These factors are likely to play an important
cence was studied, homotypic continuity from role in mental health trajectories.
childhood to adolescence was found to be stron- Mental and substance use disorders account
gest for substance use disorders and lowest for for nearly a third (31.81%) of YLDs in emerging
anxiety disorders. With regard to heterotypic adults aged 20–24 and 16.16% of DALYs, while
continuity, childhood depression predicted ado- self-harm and interpersonal violence account for
lescent anxiety disorders, whereas childhood 8.03% of DALYs, with the greatest burden for
anxiety disorders predicted adolescent depres- depression (16.76%), anxiety (4.87%), and drug
sion and adolescent substance use disorders. use (3.32%) disorders (Global Burden of Disease
Because adolescents are among the most Study, 2015). An 8-year prospective two-wave
physically healthy, compared to other age groups, panel study that evaluated youth between ages
most no longer go for checkups with pediatri- 12 and 17 at wave I and between 19 and 26 at
cians, and there are few services specially aimed wave II found an incidence of 37.9% for any dis-
at adolescents. They require an adult (e.g., par- order, the greatest incidence for substance use
ent, teacher) to recognize a problem and take disorders, specifically alcohol abuse, and the
them to treatment, but are less easily taken to lowest incidence for anxiety disorders given that
treatment if they do not wish it than children. As most individuals who will have an anxiety disor-
a result they are less likely to be seen in the health der have already developed one and there are few
sector. Thus it is important to take preventive who have first onsets during this period (Benjet
interventions and treatment strategies to them, et al., 2016). However, this latter finding is
and the most promising avenues for such inter- contrary to the finding of Copeland et al. (2014)
ventions are strategies online and on mobile that anxiety disorders had a large increase in
devices where adolescents are active and techno- the transition to adulthood. The incidence of
logically literate and may be more easily engaged. substance abuse disorder is salient during this
transition and is often preceded by other types of
­psychopathology in earlier stages of develop-
 sychopathology in Emerging
P ment (Costello, 2007).
Adulthood Like adolescents, emerging adults are among
the physically healthiest in society and are less
Among the primary tasks of early adulthood is the likely to have contact with the health sector, but
transition from school to work and may also include unlike adolescents they no longer require an adult
marriage and childbearing, though in many devel- to identify their problem and take them to
oped countries and among higher SES individuals treatment.
18 C. Benjet

 nxiety Disorders from Childhood


A attenuated extinction response compared to
Through Adolescence children and adults (Pattwell et al., 2012) and
and Emerging Adulthood thus are less likely to benefit from exposure-
based treatment which relies on fear extinction
Most anxiety disorders have very early ages of principles and is the only evidence-based treat-
onset; phobias and separation anxiety disorder ment for phobias.
have median ages of onset between 7 and Copeland and colleagues (2014) showed a
14 years of age, whereas generalized anxiety U-shaped curve for the 3-month prevalence rates
disorder, panic disorder, and posttraumatic of any anxiety disorder from age 9 to 26, with
stress disorder have later ages after childhood high rates in early childhood, sharply decreasing
(Kessler et al., 2007). Phobic disorders can be in middle childhood and then increasing again in
difficult to distinguish from the normative adolescence through emerging adulthood.
development of fears during childhood. Fear is a Females had higher rates than males in mid-­
necessary and protective emotion that is part of adolescence and emerging adulthood. However
normal development with evolutionary rele- the overall pattern of anxiety disorders over
vance. The object of one’s fears changes development varied by specific anxiety disorder.
throughout development. Young infants are Separation anxiety disorder that was common in
fearful of loud noises and sudden movements; in childhood becomes rare in early to mid-­
the second half of the first year of life, infants adolescence, whereas generalized anxiety disor-
begin to fear strange objects and persons, der was uncommon in childhood and increased in
heights, and separation from attachment figures. adolescence, and agoraphobia and panic disorder
Toddlers fear being alone, the dark, and ani- were uncommon in childhood and adolescence
mals; as children mature their fears extend to and increased in emerging adulthood. Specific
abstract and imaginary stimuli like monsters, phobia and social phobia were constant across
natural phenomenon, bad people, and being kid- this period. While some disorders like separation
napped; early adolescents begin to fear world anxiety disorder appear to be time-limited and
situations like war, economic difficulties, and may represent temporary fears that are resolved
questions related to self-image; older adoles- with development, others appear to be
cents continue to have social fears, sexual fears, persistent.
and fears regarding personal achievement
(Robinson & Rotter, 1991). While many of these
fears are transitory during development, some  epression from Childhood
D
may become maladaptive and persistent. An Through Adolescence
8-year longitudinal study from adolescence and Emerging Adulthood
through emerging adulthood found specific pho-
bia to persist in almost 18% of individuals with While the first episode of major depressive
this disorder at baseline which was predicted by disorder (MDD) is often during adolescence,
age of onset of the disorder in adolescence (ver- common symptoms of depression such as sad-
sus childhood), parental neglect, having a first- ness, irritability, and sleep problems and full
degree relative with specific phobia, and MDD can emerge much earlier. Because crying,
economic adversities (Albor, Benjet, Méndez, irritability in the form of tantrums, and diffi-
& Medina-­Mora, 2017). Additionally non- culty with sleep are frequent in young children,
comorbid-­specific phobia in adolescence pre- determining when these normative emotional
dicted the development of other anxiety experiences become pathological can be chal-
disorders and substance use disorders in emerg- lenging. Bufferd, Doughterty, and Olino (2017)
ing adulthood. A particular difficulty in the mapped the frequency and severity of depressive
treatment of phobias for adolescents is that behaviors in preschool-aged children providing
research has found that adolescents have an useful information for making this distinction.
Developmental Psychopathology and the Epidemiology and Expression… 19

They found that sadness, irritability, and tearful- genetic factors such as shortened telomerase
ness/sensitivity were normative, whereas low length in interaction with stressful/adverse
interest/pleasure, talking about death/suicide, environments and HPA dysregulation.
low self-worth, appetite/weight changes, and
difficulty concentrating/making decisions were
not normative in preschool-aged children. For  ttention Deficit Hyperactivity
A
example, tearfulness had to occur 32 times over Disorder Throughout Childhood,
14 days, and tantrums had to occur 9 times over Adolescence, and Emerging
14 days to be considered clinically relevant, Adulthood
whereas low interest/pleasure had to occur only
3–4 times and low self-worth 2 times over While high levels of activity can be observed in
14 days to be considered severe. infancy, symptoms of attention deficit hyperac-
The prevalence of depression increases with tivity disorder are generally first recognized dur-
age. In a community sample of 3-year-olds, the ing the preschool period or upon school entrance
prevalence was estimated at 1.8% (Bufferd et al., when the demands for attention and impulse
2017). In children 9–12, prevalence rates have control are increased. Childhood prevalence
been estimated at 2.8% (Costello et al., 2003), rates of ADHD are estimated between 5% and
and in adolescents the 12-month prevalence of 7% and fall to 3–4% among adolescents and
MDD ranges from 4.8% to 7.5% (Avenevoli, even lower in adults (2.8%) (Fayyad et al., 2017;
Swendsen, He, Burstein, & Merikangas, 2015; Polanczyk et al., 2007). Longitudinal studies of
Benjet et al., 2009), with important sex differ- the persistence of the disorder over the life span
ences emerging in adolescence which remain present a wide variety of estimates from 4% to
throughout the rest of the life course with females 76% persisting with full disorder from childhood
generally presenting twice the prevalence of to adulthood and depend upon informant source
MDD than males. A longitudinal study from age among other important differences between
8 to 18 found a surge in MDD in adolescence, studies (Caye et al., 2016). Consistent risk fac-
particularly from age 14, predicted by S carriers tors for persistence include the severity of
of 5-HTTLPR, higher peer chronic stress over ADHD, receiving treatment (presumably
3 years, the interaction of these two factors mod- because only the most severe receive treatment),
erated by age, and finally female gender and par- and comorbid conditions particularly comorbid-
ticularly females with greater chronic peer stress ity with conduct disorder and major depressive
(Hankin et al., 2015). disorder (Caye et al., 2016).
In another longitudinal study, an earlier onset The relative importance of symptoms also
of MDD was predicted by parental MDD and changes throughout childhood, adolescence, and
antisocial behavior, child negative emotionality adulthood. Inattentive symptoms are more stable
and externalizing behavior, and child maltreat- than hyperactive or impulsive symptoms. During
ment; on the other hand, lower positive emotion- preschool, hyperactivity peaks and is the most
ality, childhood maltreatment, and trait anxiety common ADHD symptom and then declines
predicted a recurrent course of MDD from late throughout childhood (Olson, 2002). Inattentive
childhood into adulthood (Wilson, Vaidyanathan, symptoms increase, become more noticeable as
Miller, McGue, & Iacono, 2014). children enter school, and remain stable (Hart,
Preschool, childhood, and adolescent-onset Lahey, Loeber, Applegate, & Frick, 1995). Martel
depression shows both homotypic and hetero- et al. (2016) found that the ADHD symptom
typic continuities. Preschool depression is a pre- structure became more differentiated over devel-
dictor of later childhood depression as well as opment, in other words there was a progressive
later childhood anxiety disorders and ADHD differentiation of symptoms as individuals age.
(Hankin, 2015). Persistent and recurrent MDD During preschool, all symptoms clustered tightly
throughout childhood and adolescence include together in a single cluster suggesting that
20 C. Benjet

during preschool those with ADHD manifest the normality of abnormality or rather the fluctua-
similar difficulties with all symptoms. During tions in emotional and behavioral adaptation as
childhood, the symptom structure most closely individuals face different challenges throughout
resembled the two-domain symptom structure the course of development. While the high preva-
defined in the DSM-5, two clusters, a tight cluster lence estimates reported in community surveys
of inattention symptoms and a separate but more may engender incredulity in some, or criticisms
disperse cluster of inattention and hyperactivity- as to the medicalization of normal suffering and
impulsivity symptoms. During adolescence three the human experience, this perspective discounts
clusters emerge, inattention and impulsivity the suffering of those who experience distress and
forming two separate and tightly formed clusters fails to promote actions to alleviate their distress
and a separate but less central cluster of hyperac- or to problem solve. So whether you accept the
tivity symptoms. By adulthood symptoms epidemiologic panorama which suggests that
become most differentiated, forming four distinct most people at some point in development will
clusters: mental effort, disorganization, motor have sufficient symptoms to meet diagnostic cri-
overactivity, and verbal impulsivity. Martel and teria for a psychiatric disorder or whether you
colleagues suggest that impulsivity is most consider this the universality of human suffering,
salient during adolescence possibly because of given that we have the means to alleviate suffering
the underdevelopment of the prefrontal cortex in for many of these conditions and alter the course
combination with greater freedom than children of developmental trajectories, we have the respon-
to make decisions, whereas verbal impulsivity sibility to do so. The most effective means for
emerges as salient in adulthood, possibly due to treatment will be the topic of subsequent
verbal impulsivity being the last aspect of chapters.
impulsivity to be reined under voluntary control.
Despite these changes in symptom structure
across development, two symptoms, “often easily References
distracted” and “difficulty sustaining attention,”
were core symptoms at each stage. And while Abulizi, X., Pryor, L., Michel, G., Melchior, M., van
der Waerden, J., & EDEN Mother-Child Cohort
there is substantial evidence that ADHD lessons Study Group. (2017). Temperament in infancy and
with age, it is unclear whether this is a true remis- behavioral and emotional problems at age 5.5: The
sion or only a reflection of the developmental EDEN mother-child cohort. PLoS One. https://doi.
insensitivity of diagnostic criteria (Faraone, org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171971
Albor, Y. C., Benjet, C., Méndez, E., & Medina-Mora,
Biederman, & Mick, 2006). M. E. (2017). Persistence of specific phobia from ado-
lescence to early adulthood: Longitudinal follow-up
of the Mexican adolescent mental health survey. The
Conclusion Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 78(3), 340–346.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Psychopathology is expressed from infancy Washington, DC: APA.
onward, with both homotypic and heterotypic Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of
continuities, as well as discontinuity, across development from the late teens through the twenties.
The American Psychologist, 55, 469–480.
developmental periods depending upon the com- Avenevoli, S., Swendsen, J., He, J. P., Burstein, M.,
plex interplay of biological, psychological, and & Merikangas, K. R. (2015). Major depression in
socio-contextual factors. Despite only 13.3% the national comorbidity survey-adolescent supple-
meeting criteria for a psychiatric disorder in any ment: Prevalence, correlates, and treatment. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
given 3-month period in childhood, over 60% met Psychiatry, 54(1), 37–44.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
full diagnostic criteria at some point by the age of jaac.2014.10.010
21, and another 21.4% had subclinical symptoms Barlow, J., Bennett, C., Midgley, N., Larkin, S. K., & Wei,
in the Great Smoky Mountains Study (Copeland, Y. (2015). Parent-infant psychotherapy for improving
parental and infant mental health. Cochrane Database
Shanahan, Costello, & Angold, 2011). This highlights
Developmental Psychopathology and the Epidemiology and Expression… 21

of Systematic Reviews, 1, CD010534. https://doi. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1–2), 16–25.
org/10.1002/14651858.CD010534.pub2.parent https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979.x
Bellani, M., Nobile, M., Bianchi, V., van Os, J., & Copeland, W., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., & Angold,
Brambilla, P. (2012). G X E interaction and neurode- A. (2011). Cumulative prevalence of psychiatric disor-
velopment I. Focus on maltreatment. Epidemiology ders by young adulthood: A prospective cohort analy-
and Psychiatric Sciences, 21(4), 347–351. https://doi. sis from the Great Smoky Mountains Study. Journal
org/10.1017/S2045796012000418 of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Benjet, C., Borges, G., Méndez, E., Albor, Y., Casanova, Psychiatry, 50(3), 252–261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
L., Orozco, R., … Medina-Mora, M. E. (2016). jaac.2010.12.014
Eight-­ year incidence of psychiatric disorders and Copeland, W. E., Adair, C. E., Smetanin, P., Stiff, D.,
service use from adolescence to early adulthood: Briante, C., Colman, I., … Angold, A. (2013).
Longitudinal follow-­up of the Mexican adolescent Diagnostic transitions from childhood to adolescence
mental health survey. European Child & Adolescent to early adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 25(2), 163–173. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Psychiatry, 54(7), 791–799.
s00787-015-0721-5 Copeland, W. E., Angold, A., Shanahan, L., & Costello,
Benjet, C., Medina-Mora, M. E., Borges, G., Zambrano, E. J. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of anxiety from
J., & Aguilar-Gaxiola, S. (2009). Youth mental health childhood to adulthood: The Great Smoky Mountains
in a populous city of the developing world: Results Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
from the Mexican adolescent mental health survey. Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(1), 21–33.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(4), Costello, E. J. (2007). Psychiatric predictors of adoles-
386–395. cent and young adult drug use and abuse: What have
Benjet, C., Thompson, R., & Gotlib, I. (2010). 5-HTTLPR we learned? Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88(S1),
moderates the effect of relational peer victimization 97–99.
on depressive symptoms in adolescent girls. Journal Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., &
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(2), 173–179. Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02149.x psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence.
Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Carter, A. S., Bosson-Heenan, J., Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(8), 837–844.
Guyer, A. E., & Horwitz, S. M. (2006). Are infant-­ Costello, J. E., Copeland, W., & Angold, A. (2011). Trends
toddler social-emotional and behavioral problems in psychopathology across the adolescent years: What
transient? Journal of the American Academy of Child changes when children become adolescents, and
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 849–858. when adolescents become adults? Journal of Child
Brinksma, D. M., Hoekstra, P. J., van den Hoofdakker, Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(19), 1015–1025.
B., de Bildt, A., Buitelaar, J. K., Hartman, C. A., & https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02446.x
Dietrich, A. (2017). Age-dependent role of pre- and Doom, J. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2013). Stress physiology
perinatal factors in interaction with genes on ADHD and developmental psychopathology: Past, present
symptoms across adolescence. Journal of Psychiatric and future. Developmental Psychopathology, 25(4 0
Research, 90, 110–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 2). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000667
jpsychires.2017.02.014 Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional
Bufferd, S. J., Dougherty, L. R., & Olino, T. M. (2017). and behavioral disorders in preschool children:
Mapping the frequency and severity of depressive behav- Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of
iors in preschool-aged children. Child Psychiatry & Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3/4), 313–337.
Human Development pub ahead of print. doi: https:// Einfeld, S. L., Piccinin, A. M., Mackinnon, A., Hofer,
doi.org/10.1007/s10578-017-0715-2. S. M., Taffe, J., Gray, K. M., … Tonge, B. J. (2006).
Bynner, J. (2005). Rethinking the youth phase of life-­ Psychopathology in young people with intellectual
course: The case for emerging adulthood? Journal of disability. JAMA, 296(16), 1981–1989. Global Burden
Youth Studies, 8(4), 367–384. of Disease Study. (2015). Interactive visualization tool
Cassidy, J., Jones, J. D., & Shaver, P. R. (2013). https://vizhub.healthdata.org/gbd-compare/
Contributions of attachment theory and research: A Elder, G. H. (1985). Perspectives on the life course. In
framework for future research, translation, and policy. G. H. Elder (Ed.), Life course dynamics: Trajectories
Development and Psychopathology, 25(4 0 2), 1415– and transitions, 1968–1980 (pp. 23–49). Ithaca, NY:
1434. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000692 Cornell University Press.
Caye, A., Spadini, A. V., Karam, R. G., Grevet, E. H., Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (2006). The
Rovaris, D. L., Bau, C. H., … Kieling, C. (2016). age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperac-
Predictors of persistence of ADHD into adulthood: A tivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies.
systematic review of the literature and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 36(2), 159–165.
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(11), Farbstein, I., Mansbach-Kleinfeld, I., Levinson, D.,
1151–1159. Goodman, R., Levav, I., Vograft, I., … Apter, A. (2010).
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements Prevalence and correlates of mental disorders in Israeli
and future promises of developmental psychopathol- adolescents: results from a national mental health sur-
ogy: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of vey. J Child Psychol Psychiatry., 51(5), 630–639.
22 C. Benjet

Fayyad, J., et al. (2017). The descriptive epidemiol- Keenan, K., Shaw, D. S., Walsh, B., Delliquadri, E., &
ogy of DSM-IV adult ADHD in the World Health Giovannelli, J. (1997). DSM-III-R disorders in pre-
Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Attention school children from low-income families. Journal
Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 9(1), 47–65. of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Psychiatry, 36, 620–627.
Ijzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A. M., & Roisman, Kessler, R. C., et al. (2007). Lifetime prevalence and age-­
G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attach- of-­onset distributions of mental disorders in the World
ment and disorganization in the development of chil- Health Organization’s World Mental Health Survey
dren’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Initiative. World Psychiatry, 6, 168–176.
Child Development, 81(2), 435–456. https://doi. Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B.,
org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01405.x Newcombe, R., Craig, I. W., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006).
Gleason, M. M., Zamfirescu, A., Egger, H. L., MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interac-
Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2011). tion predicting children’s mental health: New evidence
Epidemiology of psychiatric disorders in very young and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11(10),
children in a Romanian pediatric setting. European 903–913.
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(10), 527–535. Lavigne, J. V., Arend, R., Rosenbaum, D., Binns, H. J.,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-011-0214-0 Christoffel, K. K., & Gibbons, R. D. (1998). Psychiatric
Global Burden of Disease Study. (2015). Interactive disorders with onset in the preschool years: I. Stability
visualization tool https://vizhub.healthdata.org/ of diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of
gbd-compare/ Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 1246–1254.
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of Lavigne, J. V., Gibbons, R. D., Christoffel, K. K., Arend,
behavioral development: Theory. Developmental R., Rosenbaum, D., Binns, H., … Isaacs, C. (1996).
Psychology, 27, 4–13. Prevalence rates and correlates of psychiatric disorders
Groh, A. M., Roisman, G. I., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., among preschool children. Journal of the American
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Fearon, R. P. (2012). Academy of Childhood and Adolescent Psychiatry,
The significance of insecure and disorganized attach- 35(2), 204–214.
ment for children’s internalizing symptoms: A meta-­ Martel, M. M., Levinson, C. A., Langer, J. K., & Nigg, J. T.
analytic study. Child Development, 83(2), 591–610. (2016). A network analysis of developmental change
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01711.x in ADHD symptom structure from preschool to adult-
Haltigan, J. D., Roisman, G. I., Cauffman, E., & Booth-­ hood. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(6), 988–1001.
Laforce, C. (2017). Correlates of childhood vs. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702615618664
adolescence internalizing symptomatology from Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson,
infancy to young adulthood. Journal of Youth and S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., … Swendsen, J. (2010).
Adolescence, 46, 197–212. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. ado-
s10964-016-0578-z lescents: Results from the national comorbidity survey
Hankin, B. L. (2015). Depression from childhood through replication-adolescent supplement (NCS-A). Journal
adolescence: Risk mechanisms across multiple systems of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
and levels of analysis. Curr Open Psychol, 4, 13–20. Psychiatry, 49(10), 980–989.
Hankin, B. L., Young, J. F., Abela, J. R. Z., Smolen, A., Montagna, A., & Nosarti, C. (2016). Socio-emotional
Jenness, J. L., Gulley, L. D., … Oppenheimer, C. W. development following very preterm birth: Pathways
(2015). Depression from childhood into late adoles- to psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 80.
cence: Influence of gender, development, genetic https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00080
susceptibility, and peer stress. Journal of Abnormal Moore, S. E., Norman, R. E., Suetani, S., Thomas, H. J.,
Psychology, 124(4), 803–816. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Sly, P. D., & Scott, J. G. (2017). Consequences of bul-
abn0000089 lying victimization in childhood and adolescence:
Hart, E. L., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Applegate, B., & A systematic review and meta-analysis. World Journal
Frick, P. J. (1995). Developmental change in attention-­ of Psychiatry, 7(1), 60–76.
deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: A four-year Olson, S. L. (2002). Developmental perspectives. In S.
longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Sandberg (Ed.), Hyperactivity and attention disor-
Psychology, 23, 729–749. ders of childhood (2nd ed., pp. 242–289). Cambridge,
Hostinar, C. E., Sullivan, R. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2014). England: Cambridge University Press.
Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social Pattwell, S. S., Duhoux, S., Hartley, C. A., et al. (2012).
buffering of the HAP axis: A review of animal models Altered fear learning across development in both
and human studies across development. Psychological mouse and human. Proceedings of the National
Bulletin, 140(1). https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032671 Academy of Sciences, 109(40), 16318–16323.
Humphreys, K. L., & Zeanah, C. H. (2015). Polanczyk, G., et al. (2007). The worldwide prevalence
Deviations from the expectable environment in of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression
early childhood and emerging psychopathology. analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6),
Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(1), 154–170. https:// 942–948.
doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.165
Developmental Psychopathology and the Epidemiology and Expression… 23

Pollak, S. D. (2015). Developmental psychopathol- Sroufe, A. L. (2009). The concept of develop-
ogy: Recent advances and future challenges. World ment in developmental psychopathology. Child
Psychiatry, 14, 262–269. Development Perspectives, 3(3), 178–183. https://doi.
Rettew, D. C., & McKee, L. (2005). Temperament and org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00103.x
its role in developmental psychopathology. Harvard Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Developmental psy-
Review of Psychiatry, 13(1), 14–27. https://doi. chopathology and child psychotherapy. In S. Russ &
org/10.1080/10673220590923146 T. Ollendick (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapies with
Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Dulcan, M. K., Davies, M., children and families (pp. 15–44). New York, NY: Plenum.
Piacentini, J., … Regier, D. A. (1996). The NIMH Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). The historical ori-
Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version gins and developmental pathways of the discipline of
2.3 (DISC-2.3): Description, acceptability, prevalence developmental psychopathology. The Israel Journal of
rates, and performance in the MECA Study. Methods Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 47(2), 95–104.
for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Wichstrom, L., & Berg-Nielsen, T. S. (2014). Psychiatric
Disorders Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry., disorders in preschoolers: The structure of DSM-IV
35(7), 865–877. symptoms and profiles of comorbidity. European
Robinson, E., III, & Rotter, J. (1991). Children’s fears: Toward Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(7), 551–562.
a preventative model. School Counselor, 38(3), 187. Wilson, S., Vaidyanathan, U., Miller, M. B., McGue,
Skovard, A. M. (2010). Mental health problems and M., & Iacono, W. G. (2014). Premorbid risk factors
psychopathology in infancy and early childhood. An for major depressive disorder: Are they associated
epidemiological study. Danish Medical Bulletin, 57, with early onset and recurrent course? Development
B4193. and Psychopathology, 26(0), 1477–1493. https://doi.
Slemming, K., Sørensen, M. J., Thomsen, P. H., Obel, C., org/10.1017/S0954579414001151
Henriksen, T. B., & Linnet, K. M. (2010). The asso- Wittchen, H. U., Lieb, R., Pfister, H., & Scuster, P.
ciation between preschool behavioural problems and (2000). The waxing and waning of mental disorders:
internalizing difficulties at age 10–12 years. European Evaluating the stability of syndromes of mental dis-
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(10), 787–795. orders in the population. Comprehensive Psychiatry,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-010-0128-2 41(2), 122–132.
An Introduction to Applied
Behavior Analysis

Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon, Julia L. Ferguson,


and Sara M. Weinkauf

Contents atic approach to understanding behavior of social


What Is Applied Behavior Analysis?   25 interest. ABA is deeply rooted in the influential
work of individuals such as Edward Thorndike,
Basic Principles of ABA...........................................  26
John Watson, Ivan Pavlov, and B.F. Skinner, to
ABA-Based Procedures...........................................  27 name a few. In 1968, Baer, Wolf, and Risley out-
Conclusion................................................................  38 lined some of the defining characteristics research
References.................................................................  38
in ABA should exhibit in their seminal paper
“Some Current Dimensions of Applied Behavior
Analysis.” While there are many examples of
applied behavior analytic research prior to Baer
What Is Applied Behavior Analysis? et al. (e.g., Allen, Hart, Buell, Harris, & Wolf,
1964; Ayllon, 1963; Ayllon & Azrin, 1965; Ayllon
Applied behavior analysis (ABA) is one of the & Michael, 1959; Etzel & Gerwitz, 1967;
three branches of the science of behavior analy- Sherman, 1963; Wolf, Risley, & Mees, 1963), its
sis, the other two being the experimental analysis publication, along with the establishment of the
of behavior and behaviorism, or the philosophy Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, is com-
of behavior (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007). monly cited as what established the field of ABA.
As a science, ABA can be described as a system- Baer, Wolf, and Risley (1968, 1987) urged
research in the field of ABA to be applied, behav-
ioral, analytic, technological, conceptually sys-
tematic, effective, and generalizable; applied in
the sense that the subject matter is selected due to
J.B. Leaf (*) • J.L. Ferguson
its importance to the individual, community, and/
Autism Partnership Foundation,
Seal Beach, CA, USA or society. ABA research is behavioral in that the
e-mail: jblautpar@aol.com subject matter is observable, objectively defined,
J.H. Cihon and measurable. Research demonstrates the ana-
Autism Partnership Foundation, lytic dimension when there has been a believable
Seal Beach, CA, USA demonstration that the intervention, or
Endicott College, Beverly, MA, USA ­independent variable, is solely responsible for
S.M. Weinkauf changes in the behavior in question, or the depen-
JBA Institute, Aliso Viejo, CA, USA dent variable. This dimension is typically

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 25


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_3
26 J.B. Leaf et al.

assessed through the research design used in the 1987; Ivar Lovaas, Koegel, Simmons, & Long,
study. ABA research is technological when the 1973), as well as gerontology (e.g., Green, Linsk,
procedures are described completely to allow the & Pinkston, 1986), education (e.g., Hall, Lund, &
possibility of replication. To be conceptually sys- Jackson, 1968), juvenile delinquency (e.g.,
tematic, research in the field of ABA provides Phillips, Phillips, Fixsen, & Wolf, 1971), nonhu-
descriptions of interventions and changes in man welfare (e.g., Dorey, Rosales-Ruiz, Smith,
behavior that align with relevant principles of & Lovelace, 2009), healthcare (e.g., Lichtenstien,
behavior analysis. Baer et al. (1968, 1987) con- 1997), addiction (e.g., Silverman, Roll, &
sidered research that has demonstrated effects Higgens, 2008), relationships (e.g., Sanders,
that have practical value and are meaningful to 1999), and sustainability (e.g., Bekker et al.,
the participants as effective. Generality is demon- 2010).
strated when the results are lasting and occur
across different contexts (e.g., environments,
people, times of day, with different materials). Basic Principles of ABA
An additional important component of ABA,
while not included in Baer et al. (1968, 1987)’s As mentioned previously, ABA-based procedures
description of some of the dimensions of ABA, is are derived from the principles of the science of
social validity. The importance of which was dis- behavior analysis to allow for socially significant
cussed by Wolf (1978). Judgments on social behavior change to occur. Behavior can be
validity often involve inquiry on three factors: (1) defined as:
the significance of the goals selected, (2) the That portion of an organism’s interaction with its
appropriateness of the procedures utilized, and environment that is characterized by detectable
(3) the importance of the effects demonstrated displacement in space through time of some part of
(Wolf, 1978). Unlike most measures within the organism and that results in a measureable
change in at least one aspect of the environment.
behavior analytic work, social validity is often (Johnston & Pennypacker, 1993, p. 23)
subjective (e.g., done through questionnaires, rat-
ing scales, and interviews). Social validity mea- The principles of behavior analysis began
sures combined with objective measures allow their development from early work on respondent
researchers and practitioners to measure the and operant conditioning. In respondent condi-
effectiveness and social acceptability of tioning, behavior is elicited through a condi-
interventions. tioned or unconditioned stimulus. For example,
As a practice, ABA refers to the application of presenting food, an unconditioned stimulus, elic-
behavior analytic principles to improve socially its salivation, an unconditioned response. If a
important behaviors, for example, the use of light is paired with the onset of food, eventually
shaping to expand the food repertoire of an indi- the light alone will elicit salivation. While
vidual exhibiting food selectivity (e.g., Koegel respondent conditioning has been utilized within
et al., 2012). In this example, shaping, an empiri- ABA-based procedures and should be considered
cally evaluated behavioral technique, is employed in some contexts, the principles of operant behav-
to improve an assumed socially relevant diffi- ior are more common within practice.
culty. While the clinical application of ABA may Within the operant conditioning paradigm,
not require the experimental rigor common to behavior is changed through manipulating ante-
research in ABA, it still should align with the cedents and consequences (i.e., what comes
dimensions outlined at its conception. In prac- before and after the behavior in question).
tice, the principles of ABA have been employed Antecedent manipulation involves changes to the
across a wide spectrum of challenges. Some stimulus conditions prior to the potential onset of
examples include, but are not limited to, the treat- the targeted behavior. Consequent manipulation
ment of developmental disabilities, such as involves reinforcement and punishment.
autism spectrum disorder (ASD; e.g., Lovaas, Reinforcement occurs when a stimulus change
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 27

occurs contingent upon a behavior that results in Reed, Baez, & Maguire, 2011), conversation
a corresponding increase in the probability of skills (e.g., Ingvarsson & Hollobaugh, 2010), and
similar behavior occurring in similar situations in play and social skills (e.g., Nuzzolo-Gomez,
the future. Punishment occurs when a stimulus Leonard, Ortiz, Rivera, & Greer, 2002;
change occurs contingent upon a behavior that Shillingsburg, Bowen, & Shaprio, 2014).
results in a corresponding decrease in the proba- In a recent specific example, Conallen and
bility of similar behavior occurring in similar Reed (2016) used a DTT approach to teach sev-
situations in the future. eral children (ages 6–9 years), diagnosed with
What follows are brief descriptions and autism, to label the emotions of others. Situational
research examples of some procedures that uti- cards were placed in front of the participants that
lize the principles of ABA to modify behavior. depicted various scenarios that are likely to occa-
This list is not meant to be exhaustive, but rather sion a specific emotion (e.g., a boy at a birthday
a sample of some commonly used procedures party). The participant was then given a picture
within practice and research. Additionally, the of a boy displaying a happy, sad, or angry facial
research examples selected for each procedure expression and asked to match the card to the
were done to simply provide an example of the situational card. Following the match-to-sample
procedure used in the professional literature. condition, the researchers then presented each
These examples are not meant to be representa- participant with a situational card and asked a
tive of a review of the body of literature as a question related to that card (e.g., “It is his birth-
whole for any given procedure. day, how does he feel?”). The participants
answered by selecting the picture of the boy dis-
playing an emotion (i.e., happy, sad, or angry).
ABA-Based Procedures Conallen and Reed found that the procedure was
successful at teaching the participants to label
Discrete Trial Teaching emotions within this context. For a more in-depth
description of DTT, we refer the reader to Ghezzi
One of the most common approaches to teaching (2007), Leaf and McEachin (1999), Lerman,
within a behavior analytic framework is discrete Valentino, and LeBlanc (2016), Smith (2001),
trial teaching (DTT; Lovaas, 1981, 1987). This and Leaf, Cihon, Leaf, McEachin, and Taubman
systematic procedure is commonly used to teach (2016).
a variety of skills. Each discrete trial consists of
three primary components: (1) a discriminative
stimulus (e.g., an instruction from the interven- Prompting
tionist), (2) a response by the learner, and (3) a
consequence (i.e., reinforcement or punishment) To minimize errors, increase correct responding,
provided by the interventionist. An optional, but and increase the rate of reinforcement, prompts
common, fourth step involves providing a are often provided to assist the learner. Prompts
prompt, prior to the learner’s response, that are any antecedent behavior the interventionist
increases the likelihood of the learner responding engages in that alters stimulus conditions to
correctly. Other important components which increase the likelihood of the desired response
have been explored within experimental evalua- (Green, 2001; Grow & LeBlanc, 2013; MacDuff,
tions of DTT include inter-trial intervals, meth- Krantz, & McClannahan, 2001; Wolery, Ault, &
ods of data collection, and establishing operations Doyle, 1992). There are many ways an interven-
(EO; Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950; Michael, 1988). tionist can provide a prompt, which include, but
Researchers have demonstrated that DTT has is not limited to, pointing to the correct response
been an effective approach to teach a variety of (e.g., Soluaga, Leaf, Taubman, McEachin, &
skills such as receptive and expressive labels Leaf, 2008), physically guiding the learner to the
(e.g., Conallen & Reed, 2016; DiGennaro-Reed, correct response (e.g., Leaf, Sheldon, & Sherman,
28 J.B. Leaf et al.

2010), reducing the number of choices in the prompted trials or sessions, the interventionist
field (e.g., Soluaga et al., 2008), verbally model- implements time delay trials (e.g., 5 s delay). In
ing the correct response (e.g., Leaf, Sheldon, & time delay trials, the interventionist provides an
Sherman, 2010), or placing the target stimulus instruction to the learner (e.g., “Touch the ball”)
closer to the learner (e.g., Soluaga et al., 2008). followed by a brief time delay, typically ranging
Although researchers have shown that prompt- from 3 to 5 s, for the learner to respond to the
ing can be effective across multiple populations instruction.
and behaviors, it may be difficult for clinicians to There are many other types of prompting sys-
know when to prompt, fade prompts, and what tems which include graduated guidance (e.g.,
prompts to provide. Thus, researchers have eval- MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan, 1993),
uated various prompting systems to help guide simultaneous prompting (e.g., Leaf et al., 2010),
clinicians to effectively utilize prompts. One way and no-no prompting (e.g., Leaf et al., 2010). The
to provide and fade prompts is to develop a aforementioned studies typically have strict rules
prompting hierarchy. One method is known as and protocols for interventionists to follow. In
least-to-most prompting which starts with inter- contrast, flexible prompt fading (FPF; Soluaga
ventionist providing the least amount of assis- et al., 2008) is a prompting system which does
tance and gradually increasing the assistance not provide interventionists with strict protocols
based on learner responding. A second hierarchi- of when to prompt and when not to prompt, but,
cal prompting system is known as most-to-least instead, provides guidelines. In doing so, the
prompting which starts with the most assistive interventionist makes changes based upon in-the-­
prompt (e.g., full physical guidance), and, over moment assessment of several variables (e.g.,
successive trials or sessions, the interventionist current learner responding, affect, responses to
reduces the level of assistance. When using hier- previous prompts; Leaf, Cihon, Leaf, et al. 2016;
archical prompting systems, professionals typi- Leaf, Leaf, McEachin, et al. 2016). Within FPF
cally determine the number of steps in the the interventionist can use any and all prompt
prompting hierarchy, what types of prompts will types with the goal of keeping the learner averag-
be provided, the level of assistance, the criteria to ing 80% correct responding. In doing so, the
fade or reintroduce prompts, and what types of interventionist should always implement the least
reinforcers will be utilized for unprompted and assistive prompt whenever possible and fade
prompted responses. prompts as quickly as possible. To determine
A second way to provide and fade prompts is what prompt to provide, the interventionist must
based on manipulation of the time until a prompt factor in many variables including the learner’s
is provided. One common way to do this is to history, recent responding, any undesired behav-
implement a prompting system referred to as a ior, length of teaching session, what prompts
progressive time delay. During initial teaching typically have been successful, and what rein-
with progressive time delay prompting, the inter- forcers are currently motivating.
ventionist presents a set number of simultane- Researchers have shown that FPF has been
ously prompted trials (i.e., 0 s delay). After a set successful in teaching receptive and expressive
number of simultaneously prompted trials, the labels (e.g., Soluaga et al., 2008). Soluaga et al.
interventionist implements the time delay trials. (2008) provided the first study to measure FPF in
The amount of time systematically increases which the researchers compared a time delay
(e.g., from 1 to 2 s delay) until a terminal time prompt to FPF with five individuals diagnosed
criterion is met. A second way to provide prompt- with ASD. Time delay and FPF were effective,
ing in a time-based system is known as the con- but FPF was more efficient. Additional studies
stant time delay prompting system. During initial have shown that FPF was more effective than
teaching with constant time delay, the interven- most-to-least prompting (e.g., Leaf, Leaf,
tionist provides immediately prompted trials Alcalay, et al. 2016) and error correction (e.g.,
(i.e., 0 s delay). After a set number of immediate Leaf et al., 2014).
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 29

Incidental Teaching learner. The goal is the learner then imitates the
expanded model or provides the expanded
Incidental teaching is a procedure commonly response based on the prompt provided by the
used to expand language utilizing the principles interventionist. After the learner provides the
of behavior analysis. Incidental teaching has expanded response, the interventionist should
been used to teach conversation skills (e.g., Hart immediately provide the requested item/activity.
& Risley, 1975), play skills (e.g., Wong, Kasari, The requested item/activity should function as a
Freeman, & Paparella, 2007), complex language reinforcer and increase the likelihood of the
(e.g., Hart & Risley, 1978), social skills (e.g., expanded vocal response occurring on future
McGee, Almeida, Sulzer-Azaroff, & Feldman, occasions.
1992), receptive labels (e.g., McGee, Krantz, Teaching language through incidental teach-
Mason, & McClannahan, 1983), and early read- ing has several potential benefits including
ing skills (e.g., McGee, Krantz, & McClannahan, greater generalization compared to other proce-
1986). dures, less prompt dependence, and a variety of
Hart and Risley developed incidental teaching interventionists can easily implement the proce-
procedures in 1968 while working with children dure, including parents, teachers, and caregivers
from low-income families to increase the com- (McGee, Krantz, McClannahan, 1985, 1986;
plexity of their children’s language. Hart and McGee, Morrier, & Daly 1999).
Risley (1975) defined incidental teaching as “the
interaction between an adult and a single child,
which arises naturally in an unstructured situa- Token Economies
tion, which is used by the adult to transmit infor-
mation or give the child practice in developing a A token economy is a type of reinforcement sys-
skill” (p. 411). Hart and Risley (1968) found that tem in which the interventionist provides some
the incidental teaching method expanded chil- form of tokens (e.g., check marks, points, stick-
dren’s verbal communication skills and general- ers) contingent upon the learner engaging in a
ized into other settings. targeted response(s). Once the learner earns
Incidental teaching consists of four compo- enough tokens, she/he exchanges the tokens for a
nents: (1) environmental arrangement, (2) child preferred item or activity (e.g., toy, edible, game)
initiation, (3) elaboration, and (4) reinforcement. which presumably functions as a reinforcer.
Incidental teaching should take place in the learn- Since the acquisition of tokens is paired with the
er’s natural environment, but the environment delivery of a preferred item or activity, the tokens
should be arranged so that the learner needs to function as a conditioned reinforcer. This is con-
initiate and request desired items, activities, and sidered as a bridge in the gap to reinforcement as
any other materials (McGee et al., 1983). the delivery of tokens marks the occurrence of
Incidental teaching focuses on the learner’s inter- the desired behavior, but no preferred item or
ests and is dependent on the learner’s initiations. activity will be accessed until the learner has
Once the environment has been arranged appro- acquired a certain number of tokens. The applica-
priately, the interventionist should wait for the tion of token economies has a long history in
learner to initiate. The nature of the initiation will research and clinical practice within the field of
vary for each learner, which could be a gesture ABA.
toward an item or activity, a one-word request, a Ayllon and Azrin (1965) conducted a seminal
manual sign, a full sentence, etc. The interven- study in which they used a token economy to
tionist may then target an elaboration of the evaluate the effects of extrinsic reinforcement on
learner’s request. This could be in the form of a behavior that was presumed to be intrinsically
question (e.g., “What color paint?”) or a vocal motivating. The study consisted of six experi-
model (e.g., “I want the giraffe”). The form of the ments examining the effects of a token economy
elaboration should also be individualized for the and other operant procedures on the behavior of
30 J.B. Leaf et al.

adult patients, identified as psychotic, who Second, the form tokens will take must be
resided in a state hospital. The researchers imple- selected (e.g., points, stickers, check marks).
mented a token economy throughout all six Third, which preferred activities will be available
experiments in which tokens could be exchanged for exchange (e.g., toys, breaks, social praise,
for privacy, leave from the ward, social interac- edibles). Fourth, how many tokens must be
tion with staff, devotional opportunities, recre- earned before an exchange can occur. Fifth, if
ational opportunities, and commissary items. The tokens can also be lost (i.e., response cost;
dependent variables across the six experiments described later); sixth, how to fade the token sys-
were selection and engagement in various jobs tem; and, finally, how the token economy will be
inside and outside of the hospital. The contingent introduced should be planned. The final decision
application of the token economy system effected can often be the most important decision as prop-
choice of job as well as the patient’s performance erly introducing the token system is essential for
on the job. its success. Leaf, McEachin, and Taubman (2012)
Since Ayllon and Azrin’s (1965) seminal study have provided training materials on how to intro-
using a token economy, there have been several duce the token economy. Leaf and colleagues’
investigations across multiple populations (e.g., recommendation is to start with delivering tokens
developmental disabilities; Harchik, Sherman, & for a simple behavior (e.g., the learner placing his
Sheldon, 1992; juvenile delinquency; Phillips, or her hands in the lap) and gradually expanding
1968) and targeted responses (e.g., decreased in complexity. Additionally, Leaf and colleagues
symptoms of depression; Hersen, Eisler, Alford, recommended starting with the learner initially
& Agras, 1973; increased activity levels for only earning one token and then expanding to
chronic pain patients; Ritchie, 1976) on the more tokens before an exchange occurs. After
implementation of token economies. In one these decisions have been made by the clinician,
study, Charlop-Christy and Haymes (1998) eval- she/he can begin to implement the token
uated two variations of token economies for three economy.
individuals diagnosed with autism. One variation
used the participants’ perseverations as tokens
(e.g., if the perseveration was cars, then small toy Response Cost
cars were used as tokens). The second variation
used stars as tokens. The percentage of correct Another procedure which can be utilized to
responding during performance tasks was higher reduce the rate of undesired behavior is response
when perseverative objects were used as opposed cost. Response cost consists of the removal of a
to stars. In a more recent study, Dotson, Richman, reinforcing event contingent upon demonstration
Abby, Thompson, and Plotner (2013) evaluated a of an undesired behavior. This procedure is com-
class-wide token economy paired with the teach- monly used within a token economy (described
ing interaction procedure to teach job-related earlier) in which the interventionist removes
skills to eight adults with various developmental tokens (e.g., points, stickers); however, response
disabilities (e.g., intellectual disability, Down cost can occur in the absence of a token system
syndrome, and autism). The combination of the (e.g., removing certain tangible reinforcers con-
two procedures was successful in improving the tingent upon the learner engaging in an unde-
work-related behavior for all participants. sired behavior). Phillips et al. (1971) conducted
The research on token economies has helped a seminal study in which the researchers evalu-
lead to the procedures widespread clinical use. ated the effectiveness of a response cost system.
There are some variables that clinicians should In their study, all participants were part of
consider when implementing a token economy Achievement Place, a community-based treat-
that are worth noting. First, as with any reinforce- ment facility, and were considered predelinquent
ment system, what behavior will be reinforced youths. All youths participated in a token econ-
through the token economy must be determined. omy, in which participants could earn points for
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 31

engaging in appropriate behavior and exchange “reinforcing one response class and withholding
points earned for various reinforcers (e.g., reinforcement for another response class”
snacks, TV, allowances). Within this study, the (Cooper et al., 2007, p. 470) to “provide the
researchers showed that a token economy with strongest reinforcers for the best behaviors or
response cost could increase punctuality for performance” (Leaf & McEachin, 1999, p. 34).
meetings and answering questions correctly Differential reinforcement procedures have dem-
about an event that was just observed (e.g., onstrated effectiveness across a wide variety of
watching the news). Since this study, there have populations and target behaviors. Four common
been many evaluations of response cost which differential reinforcement procedures include
have included evaluating response cost with typ- differential reinforcement of other behavior
ically developing individuals (e.g., Tiano, (DRO), differential reinforcement of low rates of
Forston, McNeil, & Humphreys, 2005) and indi- behavior (DRL), differential reinforcement of
viduals diagnosed with attention deficit hyperac- incompatible behavior (DRI), and differential
tivity disorder (ADHD; e.g., McGoey & DuPaul, reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA).
2000), intellectual disabilities (e.g., Myers,
1975); ASD (Jowett, Dozier, & Payne, 2016), DRO  Within DRO, a reinforcing event is deliv-
developmental disabilities (e.g., Piazza, Fisher, ered contingent on the absence of a specific
& Sherer, 1997), and emotionally disturbed topography of response (Reynolds, 1960; Weiher
learners (e.g., Sprute & Williams, 1990). & Harman, 1975). The delivery of the reinforcing
Before a clinician uses response cost, there are event occurs based upon the absence of the tar-
several considerations that must be taken into geted response for a specified duration of time or
account. First, decide if response cost will be if the targeted response is not occurring at a spec-
paired with systematized reinforcement system ified time. There are several distinctions among
such as a token economy. Second, decide what DRO procedures based upon how the delivery of
behavior will result in a loss. Third, decide on the the reinforcing event is determined that are
cost (e.g., loss of a specific duration of time, loss beyond the scope of this chapter (see Cooper
of three tokens versus one token). This is an et al., 2007 for a detailed description). The effec-
important consideration, as the clinician needs to tiveness and variables affecting the effectiveness
ensure that the cost is high enough to have an of DRO procedures have been well documented
effect on the target behavior, but not too great within the research literature.
resulting in prolonged lapses in engaging in For instance, in a recent study, Heffernan and
appropriate behavior. Finally, decide if the con- Lyons (2016) examined the effectiveness of a
tingencies will be discussed with the learner DRO procedure to decrease the frequency of nail
before implementation. If the learner has the pre- biting for a 4-year-old boy diagnosed with ASD.
requisite skills required, discussing the system Prior to the onset of intervention, the researchers
with the learner may result in faster behavior conducted a functional behavior assessment
change. However, for some learners, discussing (FBA) and a preference assessment. Heffernan
the contingencies may not be appropriate and and Lyons identified several items that may pro-
should be avoided. vide similar sensory feedback to nail biting (e.g.,
containers of dry rice and pasta to run his fingers
through) that could potentially serve as reinforc-
Differential Reinforcement ers. Initially, the preferred items were available
following 20 s without nail biting. The interval
Differential reinforcement procedures are com- was reset each time nail biting occurred. The
mon for developing new behavior and decreasing intervention was successful at decreasing the fre-
the probability of undesired behavior. Broad defi- quency of nail biting and, throughout the course
nitions of differential reinforcement vary from of the intervention, the interval was increased to
32 J.B. Leaf et al.

60 min. For a detailed review of recent applied Within this procedure, reinforcement is contin-
literature utilizing DRO procedures, we refer the gent upon the occurrence of a predetermined
reader to Jessel and Ingvarsson (2016). response topography that is incompatible with
the undesired behavior that is targeted for
DRL  Ferster and Skinner (1957) first described decrease, however, not necessarily functionally
DRL as delivering a reinforcing event contingent equivalent. For example, if head hitting with
upon the lapse of a minimum amount of time one’s hand is the undesired behavior, hands in lap
without the occurrence of the target behavior and or in pockets could be selected for reinforcement
subsequent increasing of the periods of time because they are incompatible with head hitting.
between responses to further reduce the target Recent reviews of the empirical literature have
behavior. Another variation of DRL may also shown that DRI procedures are less common
involve a predetermined criterion level of among differential reinforcement procedures and
responding that must not be exceeded during a that positive treatment effects are commonly only
specified timeframe to receive access to a rein- observed when the DRI is paired with other pro-
forcing event (e.g., no more than three occur- cedures (Chowdhury & Benson, 2011).
rences of a target behavior in 10 min regardless For example, Neufeld and Fantuzzo (1987)
of the time between responses). Thus, DRL may examined the effectiveness of a DRI procedure to
not completely suppress the targeted response but decrease the frequency of self-injurious behavior
rather work toward systematically decreasing the (SIB) for three adults at a state hospital. The
target behavior to more appropriate or acceptable incompatible behavior selected during the inter-
levels. Since Ferster and Skinner’s first descrip- vention was placing rings onto a peg which was
tion, the DRL procedure has been utilized clini- related to the participants’ current habilitative
cally and evaluated empirically within the programming and incompatible with the
literature. SIB. Reinforcement was delivered at 10 s inter-
In one example, Austin and Bevan (2011) vals for engaging in the incompatible task. This
used a DRL procedure to reduce the frequency of DRI procedure was only partially effective as the
requests for interventionist attention with three rate of SIB still occurred at variable rates across
young children in an elementary school class- all three participants. However, when paired with
room in South Wales. Baselines were taken for contingent application of a helmet in combina-
all three participants to determine individual tar- tion with the DRI procedure, SIB was reduced to
get rates. To begin each session, boxes signifying near zero levels for all three participants.
the number of times the participant could request
attention were outlined on an index card, plus one DRA  DRA is similar to the DRI in that it speci-
additional box. For instance, if the targeted rate fies a response upon which reinforcement is con-
was three, that participant had four boxes on her tingent. However, unlike the DRI, the response
index card. Each time the participant requested selected within the DRA is not necessarily
attention, a box was checked. At the end of each incompatible with the undesired behavior.
session, the interventionist delivered a reinforcer Consider the head hitting example used in the
if the participant requested interventionist atten- description of the DRI above. Alternative
tion less often than the targeted rate (i.e., if all the responses for head hitting that are not necessarily
boxes were not checked). The results of a reversal incompatible with head hitting may be requesting
design showed that the DRL was effective at squeezes to the head, resting hand on the head, or
decreasing the rate of requests for attention for all asking for a break. DRA and DRO procedures are
three participants. the most commonly used differential reinforce-
ment procedures used among the literature
DRI  DRI differs from the DRO in that it speci- (Chowdhury & Benson, 2011).
fies the response topography upon which the In one example within the literature, Rehfeldt
delivery of reinforcement will be contingent. and Chambers (2003) utilized a DRA procedure
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 33

to decrease the frequency of perseverative verbal quent loud vocalizations and swearing behaviors
behavior and increase the frequency of appropri- participated in the first experiment. The research-
ate verbal behavior for a 23-year-old male diag- ers implemented a 2 min time-out procedure plus
nosed with autism and mental retardation. There a DRI (described previously). The time-out pro-
was no single appropriate verbal response cedure consisted of moving the participant to the
selected for reinforcement; rather, all appropriate corner of the room and placing her on the floor.
verbal responses were candidates for reinforce- The results of the study showed that the time-out
ment. Attention and eye contact (presumed rein- procedure resulted in an immediate change in the
forcing events) were delivered contingent upon participant’s behavior, with loud vocalizations
engaging in appropriate verbal behavior. The occurring at near zero rates. The same procedure
results indicated that the DRA procedure was was used in the second experiment with a 7-year-­
effective at increasing the frequency of appropri- old boy who engaged in frequent aggressive
ate verbal behavior and decreasing the frequency behavior. The results replicated those from the
of perseverative verbal behavior. first experiment with the rate of aggression
While differential reinforcement is commonly decreasing immediately and occurring at near
used for the reduction of the rates of undesired zero rates. Since this study, there have been
behavior, it is also used to strengthen response numerous investigations of time-out to decrease
classes and is a key component of shaping the severity of aberrant behavior across various
(described later). For an in-depth description of populations including typically developing chil-
the differential reinforcement procedures dren (e.g., Miller & Kratochwill, 1979), individu-
described here, we refer the reader to Cooper als diagnosed with ASD (e.g., Donaldson &
et al. (2007), Chowdhury and Benson (2011), and Vollmer, 2011), individuals diagnosed with atten-
Sulzer-Azaroff and Mayer (1977). tion deficit disorder (ADD) and ADHD (e.g.,
Fabiano et al., 2004), individuals diagnosed with
developmental disabilities (e.g., Mace & Heller,
Time-Out from Reinforcement 1990), and individuals diagnosed with intellec-
tual disabilities (e.g., Ritschl, Mongrella, Presbie,
Time-out from reinforcement is a procedure 1972).
which is used to decrease the rate of undesired There are several variables for clinicians to
behavior. When implementing time-out, the consider before implementing a time-out proce-
interventionist removes or delays reinforcement dure. First, define what behavior will result in
for a certain period of time contingent upon the time-out from reinforcement. In considering this,
learner engaging in undesired behavior. For the function of the behavior is important. The cli-
example, if one wants to reduce screaming while nician must ensure that the learner is not placed
playing a video game, one may pause or remove in a time-out when the function of the behavior is
the video game for a brief period of time. It to escape their present environment, as this would
should be noted that time-out from reinforcement have the opposite effect and would likely rein-
does not necessarily mean moving an individual force the behavior. Second, and perhaps most
from one area to another, as is commonly done in importantly, the clinician must ensure that the
mainstream society. Instead, time-out refers to time-in environment is reinforcing. If the time-in
temporarily removing access to reinforcement, environment is not reinforcing, then the cost for
the specifics of which are dependent on the nature leaving that environment will not result in the
of the reinforcement. desired behavior change. Third, decide the dura-
In a seminal study, Bostow and Bailey (1969) tion of time-out. Research on the amount of time
evaluated the implementation of a brief time-out a learner remains in time-out has been mixed
procedure to decrease undesired behavior for with some studies showing that a shorter duration
residents in a large state hospital. A 58-year-old is more effective (e.g., Pendergrass, 1971) and
woman, in a wheel chair, who engaged in fre- some studies showing a longer duration is more
34 J.B. Leaf et al.

effective (e.g., White, Nielsen, & Johnson, 1972). frames (i.e., without lenses) around the room
Fourth, decide the criteria for leaving time-out which, if the boy picked up, held, or carried the
(e.g., waiting for the learner to refrain from frames, a reinforcer would be delivered.
engaging in undesired behavior). Fifth, decide if Reinforcement was then delivered for bringing
time-out is to be exclusionary (i.e., the individual the frames closer to his eyes. Once the boy was
removed from all elements of the environment) putting his glasses on independently, the pre-
or non-exclusionary (i.e., only partial elements of scription lenses were introduced. Reinforcement
the environment removed). It is very important to was gradually faded, and the boy wore his glasses
ensure that all state laws, federal laws, and ethi- for approximately 12 h each day. Ricciardi,
cal codes are being followed in making such Luiselli, and Camare (2006) provided a more
decisions. Finally, decide what procedures (e.g., recent demonstration in which shaping was used
differential reinforcement, token economies, to increase the frequency of approach responses
prompting) to implement in conjunction with to electronic animated figures (e.g., dancing
time-out to ensure that the individual learns Elmo® doll) with an 8-year-old boy diagnosed
appropriate replacement behaviors. with autism. Preferred items were available for
maintaining the targeted distance from the ani-
mated figures. The distance started at 6 m and
Shaping gradually increased in steps to 1 m from the fig-
ures. The criterion distance was decreased upon
Shaping is usually described as differentially success with staying within the criterion distance
reinforcing (described previously) successive for 90% of intervals across two consecutive ses-
approximations toward a terminal response or sions. The results showed that the shaping proce-
goal (e.g., Cooper et al., 2007; Skinner, 1953). dure was successful at increasing approach
This leads to the common view that shaping is a responses to previously avoided electronic ani-
linear process in which the reinforcement of an mated figures.
approximation leads to another and another until
the terminal response is obtained. For instance,
when using shaping to improve upon selective  eaching Interaction Procedure/
T
eating, it is common to develop a set of steps Behavioral Skills Training
leading to consumption of a food (e.g., touch,
pick up, move toward mouth, touch to lips, hold Two procedures that use instruction, modeling,
between teeth, bite down, chew, swallow). practice, and feedback to teach a wide variety of
However, others have described the shaping pro- skills are behavioral skills training (BST;
cess as a method to expand general response Miltenberger, 2012) and the teaching interaction
classes, which, in turn, provide the shaper with procedure (TIP; Phillips, Phillips, Fixsen, &
more responses from which to select and the Wolf, 1974); however, some components between
learner with more responses in which to engage the two procedures differ. BST begins with the
(Bernal, 1972; Cihon, 2015). Take the aforemen- interventionist outlining the components of the
tioned approach to address food selectivity as an targeted skill. The interventionist provides a
example. A nonlinear shaping approach, such as model during or after this instruction. Following
Bernal (1972), would focus on expanding critical the model, the learners are provided with an
classes of responding (e.g., tolerating, interact- opportunity to practice. The interventionist pro-
ing, tasting). Shaping is frequently used within vides feedback during or after the practice. A TIP
practice and evaluated empirically to develop or begins with the interventionist labeling and iden-
expand upon a number of response classes. tifying the skill. Next, the interventionist pro-
In a classic demonstration, Wolf et al. (1963) vides meaningful rationales, followed by
used shaping to teach a 3-year-old boy to wear breaking the skill down into smaller steps (i.e., a
glasses. The researchers started by placing empty task analysis of the targeted skill). The interven-
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 35

tionist then demonstrates the correct and incor- strations of the correct way to engage in the tar-
rect way to engage in the targeted skill. Following geted skill to avoid the potential of imitating
this demonstration, the learner is provided with undesirable examples. The modified TIP was
opportunities to identify why the demonstration effective in teaching the targeted skills for all
was correct or incorrect. Next, the learner prac- four participants.
tices the targeted skill, while the interventionist
provides feedback. This last step continues until
the learner meets a specified criterion. The over- Functional Analysis
lap of the components within BST and the TIP
often leads to confusion (Leaf et al., 2015). The The analog functional analysis methodology
differences have been discussed at length else- developed by Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, and
where (e.g., Leaf et al., 2015) and will not be dis- Richman (1982, 1994) has become the standard
cussed here; however, the authors encourage approach when it comes to assessing and treating
interested readers to look at the corresponding aberrant behavior. Iwata et al. (1982, 1994)’s
literature. approach to treating aberrant behavior first exper-
BST and the TIP have been well documented imentally manipulates antecedents and conse-
to teach a wide variety of skills to a wide variety quences, in an analog setting, that may affect the
of learners. For instance, Gunby and Rapp (2014) occurrence of aberrant behavior, determining the
used BST to teach three children (ages 5–6 years) function that maintains the aberrant behavior and
diagnosed with autism to engage in behavior to then proceeding to treatment based upon these
prevent abduction from strangers. The interven- results. Once the function of the aberrant behav-
tion consisted of (1) a discussion of the safety ior is determined, an intervention is developed to
response and potential lures, (2) video models of teach a replacement behavior for the aberrant
potential scenarios and safe responses, and (3) behavior(s). It is common for targeted replace-
opportunities to practice the safety skills, fol- ment behaviors to be functional communicative
lowed by (4) feedback based on practice opportu- responses (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985; Hanley,
nities (corrective and reinforcing). The skills Sandy Jin, Vanselow, & Hanratty, 2014) which
were also probed within a high probability are commonly targeted using differential rein-
instructional sequence for each participant. A forcement, while the aberrant behavior is put on
multiple baseline across participants showed that extinction (Tiger, Hanley, & Bruzek, 2008).
BST was effective for teaching abduction preven- To determine the likely function of aberrant
tion skills for all three participants. behavior, Iwata et al. (1982, 1994) used four ana-
In another recent evaluation, Ng, Schulze, log conditions which were systematically alter-
Rudrud, and Leaf (2016) examined the effective- nated. Each condition manipulates antecedent
ness of a modified TIP to teach four individuals events that precede aberrant behavior and the
(9–15 years old) diagnosed with an ASD various consequences that follow. The attention condi-
social skills. At the time of the study, each par- tion assesses if the aberrant behavior is main-
ticipant had an IQ score less than 75. Targeted tained by social positive reinforcement. In the
social skills included providing help, negotiating, attention condition, the therapist ignores the indi-
giving a compliment, passing the phone, respond- vidual while typically occupying themselves
ing to offers of help, requesting without grab- with another activity (e.g., reading a magazine,
bing, and responding to comments. All teaching cleaning, etc.). Once the individual exhibits aber-
sessions occurred in a small group instructional rant behavior, the therapist provides attention. In
format. The TIP was modified to include the use the escape condition, the environment is arranged
of demonstrations of the rationales, picture to assess if negative reinforcement is the main-
prompts for identifying situations in which to taining function. In this condition, a task demand
engage in the skills, picture prompts to identify is continually presented; if the individual engages
the steps of the skills, and only providing demon- in aberrant behavior, the task demand is delayed
36 J.B. Leaf et al.

for a certain period of time. The alone condition mined, an appropriate communicative response
in an analog functional analysis is used to deter- can be taught that serves the same function as the
mine if the aberrant behavior is maintained by aberrant behavior.
automatic reinforcement. In the alone condition, For example, in Carr and Durand’s (1985)
the therapist and any other materials are not pres- hallmark study, four children with developmental
ent in the room. Additionally, no programmed disabilities were taught desired requests for
consequences are provided contingent on aber- escape from task demands (negative reinforce-
rant behavior. The play condition serves as a con- ment) or teacher attention (positive reinforce-
trol condition. Within the play condition, ment). Carr and Durand developed several
attention is given noncontingently on a predeter- conditions to determine the social function of
mined schedule, no task demands are placed, and each participant’s aberrant behavior (i.e., atten-
free access to toys is available. Another condition tion or escape from a demand). Once the func-
commonly used in an analog functional analysis tions were determined, Carr and Durand identified
is the tangible condition. Similar to the attention a communicative response that would serve as a
condition, the tangible condition is used to deter- replacement behavior for each of the participant’s
mine if positive reinforcement is the controlling aberrant behavior. To assess the importance of
contingency. In the tangible condition, a pre- functionally equivalent replacement behavior, the
ferred item and/or activity is present in the room experimenters taught each participant an irrele-
with the therapist which, contingent on aberrant vant communicative response that did not result
behavior, is provided to the individual (Rooker, in similar consequences to the aberrant behavior.
Iwata, Harper, Fahmie, & Camp, 2011). Functionally equivalent communicative
Since the landmark Iwata et al. study (1982, responses were taught through verbal prompts
1994), research in the area of analog functional and differential reinforcement. The aberrant
analyses has become a staple within behavior behaviors for each participant decreased once the
analytic research. Many different topics of functional communicative response was taught
research have stemmed from the initial research and the irrelevant communicative responses were
on the functional treatment of aberrant behavior not effective in reducing aberrant behavior.
including descriptive assessments (Anderson & Since Carr and Durand (1985), FCT has been
Long, 2002; Lerman & Iwata, 1993; Touchette, used to reduce a wide variety of aberrant behav-
MacDonald, & Langer, 1985), anecdotal assess- iors including aggression, self-injurious behav-
ments (Smith, Smith, Dracolby, & Pace, 2012; ior, vocal disruptions, property destruction,
Iwata, DeLeon, & Roscoe, 2013), brief func- elopement, body rocking, pica, and inappropriate
tional assessments (Bloom, Lambert, Dayton, & sexual behavior (Durand & Carr, 1991; Fisher
Samaha, 2013), interview-informed synthesized et al., 1993; Fyffe, Kahng, Fittro, & Russell,
contingency analysis (IISCA; Hanley et al., 2004; Hagopian, Fisher, Sullivan, Acquisto, &
2014), and functional analyses via telehealth LeBlanc, 1998; Wacker et al., 1990). FCT has
(Wacker et al., 2013). also been shown to be effective across a wide
range of populations including adults (Wacker
et al., 1990) and children diagnosed with
Functional Communication Training developmental disabilities (Durand & Carr,
­
1991), children with cerebral palsy (Durand,
Functional communication training (FCT) is an 1999), children with traumatic brain injury
intervention in which appropriate communicative (Fyffeet al. 2004), typically developing children
behavior is taught as a replacement for aberrant (Hanley, Heal, Tiger, & Ingvarsson, 2007), and
behavior (Cooper et al., 2007). For an FCT inter- children diagnosed with autism (Sigafoos &
vention to be successful, a functional assessment Meikle, 1996), among others.
must first occur to determine the function of the When implementing FCT several variables
aberrant behavior. After the function is deter- should be considered. First, the function of the
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 37

individual’s aberrant behavior should be identi- sequential order (Cooper et al., 2007). In order to
fied. This could be done through anecdotal ensure the task analysis is correct and complete,
assessments, descriptive assessments, or experi- the interventionist should validate the task analy-
mental functional analyses. After the function, or sis by observing the completion of the task by
hypothesized function, of the aberrant behavior is individuals who are fluent with the task, consult-
determined, a functionally equivalent communi- ing experts, or performing the skill using the task
cative response should be selected. analysis (Cooper et al., 2007; Sulzer-Azaroff &
Interventionists should consider response effort, Mayer, 1977).
the speed of response acquisition, and if the Teaching a behavioral chain is typically done
response taught will be recognized and rein- through forward chaining or backward chaining.
forced in other environments (Tiger et al., 2008). Forward chaining is when each response in the
When teaching the functional communicative behavioral chain is taught sequentially. For
response, the initial teaching location, the type of example, if hand washing was taught through
prompting system, how to fade prompts, and how forward chaining, then the first step taught would
to promote generalization should also be consid- be turning the faucet on, then putting hands
ered depending on the learner’s skill level (Tiger under the water stream, pumping the soap on to
et al., 2008). Finally, the interventionist should hands, rubbing hands together, etc. until hand
decide if the aberrant behavior in question will be washing was completed. Backward chaining is
put on extinction, if the aberrant behavior will be when the instructor completes the initial
reinforced during teaching, or if punishment will responses in the behavioral chain except for the
be utilized (Tiger et al., 2008). terminal response in the behavioral chain.
Reinforcement is then delivered contingent upon
the learner completing this final response. For
Chaining example, if backward chaining were used to
teach shoe tying, then the interventionist would
Chaining is a procedure used to teach new complete all the responses in the chain except for
responses by linking a sequence of discrete the last step (i.e., pulling the bow tight). If the
responses together to form a new behavior learner pulls the bow tight, then reinforcement
(Cooper et al., 2007). In a behavioral chain, each would be delivered. The interventionist would
discrete response produces a stimulus change then teach the learner the second to last step in
which then serves as a reinforcer for the response the behavioral chain (i.e., pulling loop through).
that produced it and serves as a discriminative The learner would then be responsible for com-
stimulus for the next response in the chain pleting the last two steps in the behavioral chain.
(Cooper et al., 2007). Chaining procedures have This process would be repeated until the learner
been used to teach shoe tying for individuals is completing all the responses in the behavioral
with ASD (Rayner, 2011), a sequence of dance chain independently.
moves (Slocum & Tiger, 2011), janitorial skills When using chaining procedures in a clinical
for individuals with intellectual disabilities setting, there are several variables to consider, for
(Cuvo, Leaf, & Barakove, 1978), adding with a instance, the length of the behavioral chain and
calculator and accessing a computer program length of the discrete responses. Depending on
(Werts, Caldwell, & Wolery, 1996), and swal- the learner’s skill level, longer chains with more
lowing liquids (Hagopian, Farrell, & Amari, complex individual responses may be too diffi-
1996), among many others. cult for the learner to master (Sulzer-Azaroff &
To teach a behavioral chain, a task analysis of Mayer, 1977). Utilizing responses already in a
the necessary steps in the chain must happen first. learner’s repertoire, or closer to the learner’s rep-
A task analysis involves breaking down a com- ertoire, may lead to faster acquisition of a behav-
plex skill (e.g., shoe tying) into smaller units in ioral chain.
38 J.B. Leaf et al.

Conclusion field. These and many other procedures based


upon the science of ABA continue to make
ABA has come a long way in the past 50 plus socially significant gains in the lives of individu-
years. Our forefathers (e.g., B.F. Skinner, Donald als around the world. There is no doubt that the
Baer, Montrose Wolf, Todd Risley, James field of ABA will continue to make meaningful
Sherman, Ivar Lovaas, Sid Bijou, Ted Ayllon, contributions to society with a strong adherence
and Nate Azrin) and foremothers (e.g., Judith to the core principles of the science and contin-
Leblanc, Barbara Etzel, Sandra Harris, Beth ued development of meaningful solutions to soci-
Sulzer-Azaroff, Rosalie Rayner, Mary Cover etal challenges.
Jones) laid a strong foundation of methodology
which can be used to develop desired behavior
and decrease undesired behavior. Today the num- References
ber of professionals going into the field of ABA
Allen, K. E., Hart, B. M., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., &
continues to rise (Carr, Howard, & Martin, 2015), Wolf, M. M. (1964). Effects of social reinforcement
and the procedures based upon these principles on isolate behavior of a nursery school child. Child
are implemented in a wide variety of settings Development, 35, 511–518.
(e.g., home, school, clinic, university, residential, Anderson, C. M., & Long, E. S. (2002). Use of a struc-
tured descriptive assessment methodology to iden-
hospital, and community settings). Although tify variables affecting problem behavior. Journal of
many professionals in the field of ABA work Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 137–154.
with individuals diagnosed with ASD, ABA-­ Austin, J. L., & Bevan, D. (2011). Using differential rein-
based procedures are effective for a wide variety forcement of low rates to reduce children’s requests
for teacher attention. Journal of Applied Behavior
of populations. When the principles of ABA were Analysis, 44(3), 451–461.
first explored, they were being implemented with Ayllon, T. (1963). Intensive treatment of psychotic behav-
juvenile delinquents (Phillips et al., 1971), typi- iour by stimulus satiation and food reinforcement.
cally developing individuals (Hersen et al., 1973), Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1(1), 53–61.
Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. (1965). The measurement and
and children with intellectual disabilities (Ayllon reinforcement of behavior of psychotics. Journal of
& Azrin, 1968). the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8(6), 357–383.
There is no question that the field of ABA has Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. (1968). Reinforcer sampling:
made tremendous improvements in the lives of A technique for increasing the behavior of mental
patients. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1),
many individuals; however, there still remain 13–20.
areas in which the field may improve upon. For Ayllon, T., & Michael, J. (1959). The psychiatric nurse
instance, with the growing need for well-trained as a behavioral engineer. Journal of the Experimental
behavior analysts, it is imperative that education Analysis of Behavior, 2(4), 323–334.
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
and training is thorough, ongoing, and compre- current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
hensive (Ellis & Glenn, 1995; Shook, Ala’i-­ Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 91–97.
Rosales, & Glenn, 2002). As one can determine Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1987). Some
based on the content of this chapter, ABA and its still-current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20(4), 313–327.
applications are broad and require sophisticated Bekker, M. J., Cumming, T. D., Osborne, N. K. P.,
repertoires. Dependent upon the behavior ana- Bruining, A. M., McClean, J. I., & Leland, L. S.
lyst’s cliental, education and training should (2010). Encouraging electricity savings in a university
include the relevant procedures described residential hall through a combination of feedback,
visual prompts, and incentives. Journal of Applied
throughout this chapter in addition to the princi- Behavior Analysis, 43(2), 327–331.
ples of ABA, in-the-moment assessment, critical Bernal, M. E. (1972). Behavioral treatment of a child’s
thinking, clinical judgment, and problem eating problem. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
solving. Experimental Psychiatry, 3(1), 43–50.
Bloom, S. E., Lambert, J. M., Dayton, E., & Samaha,
ABA is a broad field with broad applications. A. L. (2013). Teacher-conducted trial-based func-
The procedures described in this chapter are sim- tional analyses as the basis for intervention. Journal of
ply an introduction to effective procedures in the Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 208–218.
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 39

Bostow, D. E., & Bailey, J. B. (1969). Modification of Maintenance and application in new settings. Journal
severe disruptive and aggressive behavior using brief of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(2), 251–264.
timeout and reinforcement procedures. Journal of Ellis, J., & Glenn, S. S. (1995). Behavior-analytic reper-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 2(1), 31–37. toires: Where will they come from and how can they
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior be maintained? The Behavior Analyst Today, 18(2),
problems through functional communication training. 285–292.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(2), 111–126. Etzel, B. C., & Gewirtz, J. L. (1967). Experimental modi-
Carr, J. E., Howard, J. S., & Martin, N. T. (2015). An fication of caretaker-maintained high-rate operant
update on the behavior analyst certification board. crying in a 6- and a 20-week-old infant (Infans tyran-
In Panel discussion presented at the Association for notearus): Extinction of crying with reinforcement
Behavior Analysis International 41st annual conven- of eye contact and smiling. Journal of Experimental
tion. Texas: San Antonio. Child Psychology, 5(3), 303–317.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1998). Using Fabiano, G. A., Pelham, W. E., Jr., Manos, M. J., Gnagy,
objects of obsession as token reinforcers for children E. M., Chronis, A. M., Onyango, A. N., et al. (2004).
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental An evaluation of three time-out procedures for chil-
Disorders, 28(3), 189–198. dren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Chowdhury, M., & Benson, B. A. (2011). Use of differ- Behavior Therapy, 35(3), 449–469.
ential reinforcement to reduce behavior problems in Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of rein-
adults with intellectual disabilities: A methodologi- forcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
cal review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Jefferson,
32(2), 383–394. G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional communication
Cihon, J. (2015). Yummy starts: A constructional approach training with and without extinction and punishment.
to food selectivity with children with autism (Master’s Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(1), 23–36.
thesis). Retrieved from: http://digital.library.unt.edu/ Fyffe, C. E., Kahng, S., Fittro, E., & Russell, D. (2004).
ark:/67531/metadc799526/ Functional analysis and treatment of inappropriate
Conallen, K., & Reed, P. (2016). A teaching procedure to sexual behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
help children with autistic spectrum disorder to label 37(3), 401–404.
emotions. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 23, Ghezzi, P. M. (2007). Discrete trials teaching. Psychology
63–72. in the Schools, 44(7), 667–679.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Green, G. (2001). Behavior analytic instruction for learn-
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle ers with autism advances in stimulus control tech-
River, NJ: Pearson. nology. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
Cuvo, A. J., Leaf, R. B., & Borakove, L. S. (1978). Disabilities, 16(2), 72–85.
Teaching janitorial skills to the mentally retarded: Green, G. R., Linsk, N. L., & Pinkston, E. M. (1986).
Acquisition, generalization, and maintenance. Journal Modification of verbal behavior of the mentally
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11(3), 345–355. impaired elderly by their spouses. Journal of Applied
DiGennaro Reed, F. D., Reed, D. D., Baez, C. N., & Behavior Analysis, 19(4), 329–336.
Maguire, H. (2011). A parametric analysis of errors of Grow, L., & LeBlanc, L. (2013). Teaching receptive
commission during discrete-trial training. Journal of language skills: Recommendations for instructors.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(3), 611–615. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 6(1), 56–75.
Donaldson, J. M., & Vollmer, T. R. (2011). An evalua- Gunby, K. V., & Rapp, J. T. (2014). The use of behavioral
tion and comparison of time-out procedures with and skills training and in situ feedback to protect children
without release contingencies. Journal of Applied with autism from abduction lures. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 44(4), 693–705. Behavior Analysis, 47(4), 856–860.
Dorey, N. R., Rosales-Ruiz, J., Smith, R., & Lovelace, Hagopian, L. P., Farrell, D. A., & Amari, A. (1996).
B. (2009). Functional analysis and treatment of self-­ Treating total liquid refusal with backward chaining
injury in a captive olive baboon. Journal of Applied and fading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Behavior Analysis, 42(4), 785–794. 29(4), 573–575.
Dotson, W. H., Richman, D. M., Abby, L., Thompson, S., Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto,
& Plotner, A. (2013). Teaching skills related to self-­ J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness of func-
employment to adults with developmental disabili- tional communication training with and without
ties: An analog analysis. Research in Developmental extinction and punishment: A summary of 21 inpatient
Disabilities, 34(8), 2336–2350. cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(2),
Durand, V. M. (1999). Functional communication training 211–235.
using assistive devices: Recruiting natural communi- Hall, R. V., Lund, D., & Jackson, D. (1968). Effects of
ties of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior teacher attention on study behavior. Journal of Applied
Analysis, 32(3), 247–267. Behavior Analysis, 1(r1), 1–12.
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Functional com- Hanley, G. P., Heal, N. A., Tiger, J. H., & Ingvarsson, E. T.
munication training to reduce challenging behavior: (2007). Evaluation of a class wide teaching program
40 J.B. Leaf et al.

for developing preschool life skills. Journal of Applied Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of
Behavior Analysis, 40(2), 277–300. psychology: A systematic text in the science of behav-
Hanley, G. P., Sandy Jin, C., Vanselow, N. R., & Hanratty, ior. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
L. A. (2014). Producing meaningful improvements Koegel, R. L., Bharoocha, A. A., Ribnick, C. B., Ribnick,
in problem behavior of children with autism via syn- R. C., Bucio, M. O., Fredeen, R. M., & Koegel, L. K.
thesized analyses and treatments. Journal of Applied (2012). Using individualized reinforcers and hierar-
Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 16–36. chical exposure to increase food flexibility in children
Harchik, A. E., Sherman, J. A., & Sheldon, J. B. (1992). with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
The use of self-management procedures by people with Developmental Disorders, 42(8), 1574–1581.
developmental disabilities: A brief review. Research Leaf, J. B., Cihon, J. H., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., &
in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Taubman, M. (2016). A progressive approach to
Journal, 13(3), 211–227. discrete trial teaching: Some current guidelines.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1975). Incidental teaching of lan- International Electronic Journal of Elementary
guage in the preschool. Journal of Applied Behavior Education, 9(2), 261.
Analysis, 8(4), 411–420. Leaf, J. B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M., Ala’i-­
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1978). Promoting productive Rosales, S., Ross, R. K., et al. (2016). Applied behav-
language through incidental teaching. Education and ior analysis is a science and, therefore, progressive.
Urban Society, 10, 407–429. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Establishing use of 46(2), 720–731.
descriptive adjectives in the spontaneous speech of Leaf, J. B., Leaf, R., Taubman, M., McEachin, J., &
disadvantaged preschool children. Journal of Applied Delmolino, L. (2014). Comparison of flexible prompt
Behavior Analysis, 1(2), 109–120. fading to error correction for children with autism
Heffernan, L., & Lyons, D. (2016). Differential reinforce- spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and
ment of other behaviour for the reduction of severe nail Physical Disabilities, 26(2), 203–224.
biting. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9(3), 253–256. Leaf, J. B., Sheldon, J. B., & Sherman, J. A. (2010).
Hersen, M., Eisler, R. M., Alford, G. S., & Agras, W. S. Comparison of simultaneous prompting and no-no
(1973). Effects of token economy on neurotic depres- prompting in two-choice discrimination learning with
sion: An experimental analysis. Behavior Therapy, children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
4(3), 392–397. Analysis, 43(2), 215–228.
Ingvarsson, E. T., & Hollobaugh, T. (2010). Acquisition Leaf, J. B., Townley-Cochran, D., Taubman, M., Cihon,
of intraverbal behavior: Teaching children with autism J. H., Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L., Kassardjian, A., et al.
to mand for answers to questions. Journal of Applied (2015). The teaching interaction procedure and behav-
Behavior Analysis, 43(1), 1–17. ioral skills training for individuals diagnosed with
Ivar Lovaas, O., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S. autism spectrum disorder: A review and commen-
(1973). Some generalization and follow-up measures tary. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental
on autistic children in behavior therapy1. Journal of Disorders, 2(4), 402–413.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(1), 131–165. Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (1999). A work in progress:
Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Roscoe, E. M. (2013). Behavior management strategies and a curriculum for
Reliability and validity of the functional analysis intensive behavioral treatment of autism. New York,
screening tool. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, NY: DRL Books.
46(1), 271–284. Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Taubman, M. (2012). A work in
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., progress: Companion series. New York: DRL.
& Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional Leaf, J. B., Leaf, J. A., Alcalay, A., Kassardjian, A.,
analysis of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Tsuji, K., Dale, S., … Leaf, R. (2016). Comparison
Developmental Disabilities, 2(1), 3–20. of Most-to-Least Prompting to Flexible Prompt
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Fading for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder.
Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis Exceptionality, 24(2), 109–122.
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive and
27(2), 197–209. experimental analyses of variables maintaining self-­
Jessel, J., & Ingvarsson, E. T. (2016). Recent advances injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
in applied research on DRO procedures. Journal of Analysis, 26(3), 293–319.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 991–995. Lerman, D. C., Valentino, A. L., & Leblanc, L. A. (2016).
Johnston, J. M., & Pennypacker, H. S. (1993). Strategies Discrete trial training. In: Early intervention for young
and tactics of human behavioral research (2nd ed.). children with autism spectrum disorder (pp. 47–83).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
Jowett Hirst, E. S., Dozier, C. L., & Payne, S. W. (2016). Lichtenstein, E. (1997). Behavioral research contributions
Efficacy of and preference for reinforcement and and needs in cancer prevention and control: Tobacco
response cost in token economies. Journal of Applied use prevention and cessation. Preventive Medicine,
Behavior Analysis, 49(2), 329–345. 26(5), S57–S63.
An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis 41

Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally disabled case study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 13(2–3),
children: The me book. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Books. 189–191.
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal Neufeld, A., & Fantuzzo, J. W. (1987). Treatment of
educational and intellectual functioning in young severe self-injurious behavior by the mentally retarded
autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical using the bubble helmet and differential reinforce-
Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. ment procedures. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. Experimental Psychiatry, 18(2), 127–136.
(1993). Teaching children with autism to use pho- Ng, A. H. S., Schulze, K., Rudrud, E., & Leaf, J. B.
tographic activity schedules: Maintenance and gen- (2016). Using the teaching interactions procedure to
eralization of complex response chains. Journal of teach social skills to children with autism and intellec-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(1), 89–97. tual disability. American Journal on Intellectual and
MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. Developmental Disabilities, 121(6), 501–519.
(2001). Prompts and prompt-fading strategies for Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., Leonard, M. A., Ortiz, E., Rivera,
people with autism. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & R. M. C. M., & Greer, R. D. (2002). Teaching children with
Foxx (Eds.), Making a difference behavioral interven- autism to prefer books or toys over stereotypy or pas-
tion for autism (1st ed., pp. 37–50). Austin, TX: Pro sivity. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions,
Ed. 4(2), 80–87.
Mace, F. C., & Heller, M. (1990). A comparison of Pendergrass, V. E. (1971). Effects of length of time-out
exclusion time-out and contingent observation for from positive reinforcement and schedule of appli-
reducing severe disruptive behavior in a 7-year-old cation in suppression of aggressive behavior. The
boy. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 12(1), Psychological Record, 21(1), 75–80.
57–68. Phillips, E. L. (1968). Achievement place: Token rein-
McGee, G. G., Almeida, M. C., Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & forcement procedures in a home-style rehabilitation
Feldman, R. S. (1992). Promoting reciprocal interac- setting for “pre-delinquent” boys. Journal of Applied
tions via peer incidental teaching. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(3), 213–223.
Behavior Analysis, 25(1), 117–126. Phillips, E. L., Phillips, E. A., Fixsen, D. L., & Wolf,
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., Mason, D., & McClannahan, M. M. (1971). Achievement place: Modification of
L. E. (1983). A modified incidental-teaching proce- the behaviors of pre-delinquent boys within a token
dure for autistic youth: Acquisition and generalization economy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 4(1),
of receptive object labels. Journal of Applied Behavior 45–59.
Analysis, 16(3), 329–338. Phillips, E. L., Phillips, E. A., Fixsen, D. L., & Wolf,
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. M. M. (1974). The teaching-family handbook (2nd
(1985). The facilitative effects of incidental teaching ed.). Lawrence, KS: University Press of Kansas.
on preposition use by autistic children. Journal of Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Sherer, M. (1997). Treatment
Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(1), 17–31. of multiple sleep problems in children with develop-
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. mental disabilities: Faded bedtime with response cost
(1986). An extension of incidental teaching procedures versus bedtime scheduling. Developmental Medicine
to reading instruction for autistic children. Journal of and Child Neurology, 39(6), 414–418.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 19(2), 147–157. Rayner, C. (2011). Teaching students with autism to tie
McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Daly, T. (1999). An a shoelace knot using video prompting and backward
incidental teaching approach to early intervention for chaining. Developmental Neuroehabilitation, 14(6),
toddlers with autism. Journal of the Association for 339–347.
Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24(3), 133–146. Rehfeldt, R. A., & Chambers, M. R. (2003). Functional
McGoey, K. E., & Dupaul, G. J. (2000). Token rein- analysis and treatment of verbal perseverations dis-
forcement and response cost procedures: Reducing played by an adult with autism. Journal of Applied
the disruptive behavior of preschool children with Behavior Analysis, 36(2), 259–261.
attention-­deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Reynolds, G. S. (1960). Behavioral contrast. Journal of
Psychology Quarterly, 15(3), 330–343. the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4(1), 57–71.
Michael, J. (1988). Establishing operations and the mand. Ricciardi, J. N., Luiselli, J. K., & Camare, M. (2006).
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 6, 3–9. Shaping approach responses as intervention for spe-
Miller, A. J., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1979). Reduction cific phobia in a child with autism. Journal of Applied
of frequent stomachache complaints by time out. Behavior Analysis, 39(4), 445–448.
Behavior Therapy, 10(2), 211–218. Ritchie, R. J. (1976). A token economy system for chang-
Miltenberger, R. G. (2012). Behavioral skills training ing controlling behavior in the chronic pain patient.
procedures, Behavior modification: principles and Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
procedures (pp. 251–269). Belmont, TN: Wadsworth, Psychiatry, 7(4), 341–343.
Cengage Learning. Ritschl, C., Mongrella, J., & Presbie, R. J. (1972). Group
Myers, D. V. (1975). Extinction, DRO, and response—cost time-out from rock and roll music and out-of-seat
procedures for eliminating self-injurious b­ ehavior: A
42 J.B. Leaf et al.

behavior of handicapped children while riding a Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1977). Applying
school bus. Psychological Reports, 31(3), 967–973. behavior analysis procedures with children and youth.
Rooker, G. W., Iwata, B. A., Harper, J. M., Fahmie, New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
T. A., & Camp, E. M. (2011). False-positive tangible Tiano, J. D., Fortson, B. L., McNeil, C. B., & Humphreys,
outcomes of functional analyses. Journal of Applied L. A. (2005). Managing classroom behavior of head
Behavior Analysis, 44(4), 737–745. start children using response cost and token economy
Sanders, M. R. (1999). Triple p-positive parenting pro- procedures. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior
gram: Towards an empirically validated multilevel Intervention, 2(1), 28–39.
parenting and family support strategy for the preven- Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional
tion of behavior and emotional problems in children. communication training: A review and practical guide.
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2(2), Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 16–23.
71–90. Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N.
Sherman, J. A. (1963). Reinstatement of verbal behavior (1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus control
in a psychotic by reinforcement methods. The Journal of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 28, 398–401. Analysis, 18(4), 343–351.
Shillingsburg, M. A., Bowen, C. N., & Shapiro, S. K. Wacker, D. P., Lee, J. F., Dalmau, Y. C. P., Kopelman,
(2014). Increasing social approach and decreasing T. G., Lindgren, S. D., Kuhle, J., et al. (2013).
social avoidance in children with autism spectrum dis- Conducting functional analyses of problem behavior
order during discrete trial training. Research in Autism via telehealth. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Spectrum Disorders, 8(11), 1443–1453. 46(1), 31–46.
Shook, G. L., Ala’i-Rosales, S., & Glenn, S. S. (2002). Wacker, D. P., Steege, M. W., Northup, J., Sasso, G.,
Training and certifying behavior analysts. Behavior Berg, W., Reimers, T., et al. (1990). A component
Modification, 26(1), 27–48. analysis of functional communication training
Sigafoos, J., & Meikle, B. (1996). Functional commu- across three topographies of severe behavior prob-
nication training for the treatment of multiply deter- lems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(4),
mined challenging behavior in two boys with autism. 417–429.
Behavior Modification, 20(1), 60–84. Weiher, R. G., & Harman, R. E. (1975). The use of omis-
Silverman, K., Roll, J. M., & Higgins, S. T. (2008). sion training to reduce self-injurious behavior in a
Introduction to the special issue on the behavior analy- retarded child. Behavior Therapy, 6(2), 261–268.
sis and treatment of drug addiction. Journal of Applied Werts, M. G., Caldwell, N. K., & Wolery, M. (1996). Peer
Behavior Analysis, 41(4), 471–480. modeling of response chains: Observational learn-
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. ing by students with disabilities. Journal of Applied
New York, NY: Free Press. Behavior Analysis, 29(1), 53–66.
Slocum, S. K., & Tiger, J. H. (2011). An assessment of White, G. D., Nielsen, G., & Johnson, S. M. (1972).
the efficiency of and child preference for forward Timeout duration and the suppression of deviant
and backward chaining. Journal of Applied Behavior behavior in children. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 44(4), 793–805. Analysis, 5(2), 111–120.
Smith, C. M., Smith, R. G., Dracobly, J. D., & Pace, A. P. Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Doyle, P. M. (1992). Teaching
(2012). Multiple-respondent anecdotal assessments: students with moderate to severe disabilities: Use
An analysis of interrater agreement and correspon- of response prompting strategies. New York, NY:
dence with analogue assessment outcomes. Journal of Long- man.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(4), 779–795. Wolf, M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. (1963). Application
Smith, T. (2001). Discrete trial training in the treatment of operant conditioning procedures to the behaviour
of autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental problems of an autistic child. Behaviour Research and
Disabilities, 16(2), 86–92. Therapy, 1(2), 305–312.
Soluaga, D., Leaf, J. B., Taubman, M., McEachin, J., & Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for sub-
Leaf, R. (2008). A comparison of flexible prompt fading jective measurement or how applied behavior analy-
and constant time delay for five children with autism. sis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(4), 753–765. Analysis, 11(2), 203–214.
Sprute, K. A., & Williams, R. L. (1990). Effects of a Wong, C. S., Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T.
group response cost contingency procedure on the (2007). The acquisition and generalization of joint
rate of classroom interruptions with emotionally dis- attention and symbolic play skills in young children
turbed secondary students. Child and Family Behavior with autism. Journal of the Association for Persons
Therapy, 12(2), 1–12. with Severe Handicaps, 32(2), 101–109.
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy

Robert D. Friedberg and Micaela A. Thordarson

Contents theory which facilitates the translation of bench


Introduction..............................................................  43 science to bedside clinical applications. The
approach is action-oriented and is committed to a
Historical Roots and Theoretical
Foundations..........................................................  43
problem-solving stance.
This chapter offers an overview of CBT with
Empirical Findings..................................................  45
youth. We begin with a discussion of the histori-
CBT with Youth: The Golden Nuggets...................  50 cal roots and theoretical underpinnings and, next,
Conclusion................................................................  55 the empirical findings supporting CBT for
depression, bipolar disorders, anxiety, obsessive-­
References.................................................................  55
compulsive disorder, trauma, disruptive behavior
disorder, and autism spectrum disorder. The ele-
ments, or golden nuggets of treatment, which are
Introduction common to all forms of CBT are described in the
third section.
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) with youth
is a well-recognized and widely adopted form of
treatment. March (2009) argued that “psychiatry  istorical Roots and Theoretical
H
will move to a unified cognitive-behavioral inter- Foundations
vention model that is housed within neuroscience
medicine” (p. 174). CBT bridges the research Contemporary CBT is rooted in learning theory
practice gap well. One of its strengths is a practical and information processing models. Classical,
operant, and social learning theory paradigms
provide the conceptual foundations for interven-
tion strategies. Classical and operant principles
R.D. Friedberg (*) load heavily on the behavioral (B) part of the
Center for the Study and Treatment of Anxious Youth, CBT equation. Historically, classical condition-
Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
ing formed the basis for understanding the
e-mail: rfriedberg@paloaltou.edu
­development and treatment of anxiety and enure-
M.A. Thordarson
sis in children (Benjamin et al., 2011). Bandura
ASPIRE Adolescent Intensive Outpatient Program,
Children’s Hospital of Orange County, (1977b) referred to classical conditioning as
Orange, CA, USA learning by antecedent determinants to ­emphasize

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 43


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_4
44 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

the crucial role of prediction in determining shaped the application of the approach with
behavior. Simply, in classical conditioning, indi- youth. Ellis’s rational emotive cognitive behav-
viduals learn which stimuli come to signal uncon- ioral therapy (RECBT) and Beck’s cognitive
ditioned responses. The basic notion states therapy (CT) both posit that behavioral and emo-
individuals learn that through repeated pairings tional difficulties are associated with faulty think-
with an inborn, pre-wired cue (unconditioned ing. Describing RECBT, Gonzalez et al. (2004)
stimulus), a previously neutral stimulus (condi- explained that “at the core of faulty thinking are
tioned stimulus) comes to elicit a reflexive, vesti- rigid and absolute beliefs (e.g., must, oughts) and
gial response. Benjamin et al. (2011) credit their derivatives (e.g., awfulizing)” (p. 222).
classical conditioning with providing the basis DiGuiseppe’s (1989) work on self-instruction
for processes such as extinction and habituation and Bernard and Joyce’s (1984) seminal text
as well as interventions such as countercondi- paved the way for RECBT applications with
tioning and exposure. youth.
Operant learning paradigms are often referred Gonzalez et al. (2004) completed a meta-­
to as Skinnerian learning models or learning by analysis evaluating RECBT’s effectiveness. Their
consequent determinants (Bandura, 1977b). The meta-analysis covered published studies from
four basic procedures (positive reinforcement, 1975 to 1998 and included 1021 children.
negative reinforcement, response cost, punish- Overall, they found the grand weighted Z was
ment) are familiar to most clinicians working 0.50 across all the studies, reflecting that children
with children. The operations explain how behav- treated with RECBT scored better than 69% of
ior is initiated, maintained, increased, and their cohorts on various outcome measures. More
reduced through contingencies. These processes specifically, RECBT had its greatest impact on
are fundamental to contingency contracting and disruptive behavior disorders.
most parent training/child management Aaron T. Beck stamped an indelible mark on
protocols. CBT with youth. Most modern cognitive behav-
Social learning theory also significantly ioral approaches are grounded in Beckian theory.
shaped CBT with youth. Bandura’s (1977a) In particular, Beck’s formative notion that an
notions of self-efficacy, reciprocal determinism, individual’s information processing system is
and observational learning are embedded within hierarchically ordered consisting of cognitive
current practices (Benjamin et al., 2011; Dobson processes, products, and structures is powerful
& Dozois, 2010; Scarpa & Lorenzi, 2013). (Beck & Clark, 1988; Ingram & Kendall, 1986).
Building young patients’ self-efficacy is integral Cognitive distortions were a major advance and
to good CBT. According to Bandura (1986), are recognized even by clinicians who are not
“perceived self-efficacy is defined as people’s CBT-inclined.
judgments of their capabilities, to organize and The content-specificity hypothesis (CSH) is
execute courses of action required to attain desig- certainly a less well-known component of
nated types of performance. It is concerned not Beckian CBT but nonetheless a clinically power-
with the skills one has but with judgments of ful one (Beck, 1976; Clark, Beck, & Alford,
what one can do with whatever skills one pos- 1999; Jolly, 1993; Jolly & Dyckman, 1994; Jolly
sesses” (p. 391). Self-efficacy theory (Bandura, & Kramer, 1994). The CSH posits that unique
1977a) states that genuine confidence in one’s cognitive content differentiates distinct emo-
coping capacity is most reliably determined by tional states. Simply, the same cognitions do not
performance attainments. Essentially, this forms denote various feelings. Daily thought diaries
the basis for homework assignments and expo- and cognitive restructuring depend on a full
sure treatment as well as many other CBT appreciation of the CSH. The cognitions that typ-
procedures. ify several emotional states are listed in Table 1.
Albert Ellis and Aaron Beck pioneered cogni- Finally, many specific clinical procedures and
tive approaches with adults and profoundly processes with youth such as guided discovery,
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 45

collaborative empiricism, session structure, and (RCT) (Kendall, 1994; Kendall et al., 1997;
rational analyses are founded on Beck’s work. Kendall, Hudson, Gosch, Flannery-Schroeder, &
Phil Kendall’s work catalyzed modern CBT Suveg, 2008) and obtained very strong results.
with youth. Impulsivity was a prime target in Modular CBT (mCBT) represents a natural
Kendall and colleagues’ initial work (Kendall & evolution in child psychotherapy (Chorpita &
Braswell, 1985; Kendall & Finch, 1976; Kendall Weisz, 2009; Friedberg, McClure, & Garcia,
& Wilcox, 1980). In an early single-case study, 2009). mCBT is a transdiagnostic approach that
Kendall and Finch (1976) reported that CBT distills and aggregates common powerful CBT
resulted in improvement on measures assessing interventions into meaningful units. The Modular
impulsivity in a 9-year-old boy. Further, in a Approach to Therapy for Children with Anxiety,
group comparison study, Kendall and Finch Depression, Trauma, or Conduct Problems
(1978) found that CBT was associated with better (MATCH-ADTC) includes 33 empirically sup-
performance on the Matching Familiar Figures ported procedures for treating children and ado-
Test and improved teacher ratings of impulsive lescents. MATCH-ADTC is gaining both popular
behavior in the classroom. appeal and empirical support (Bearman & Weisz,
Kendall is arguably most well-known for his 2015; Chorpita & Daleiden, 2009; Chorpita et al.,
cutting-edge Coping Cat protocol. The Coping 2011, 2015; Weisz & Chorpita, 2012; Weisz,
Cat (Kendall, 2012; Kendall, Furr, & Podell, Krumholz, Santucci, Thomassin, & Ng, 2015).
2010; Kendall & Hedtke, 2006) is a widely used Decades of precise theory building provides
and empirically sound approach for treating anx- CBT with a robust yet flexible conceptual plat-
ious children. Coping Cat has been implemented form. Pioneers such as Bandura, Skinner, Beck,
and tested in the USA, Canada, Australia, and the Kendall, and Chorpita paved the way for the
Netherlands (Beidas, Podell, & Kendall, 2008). empirical study of underlying principles support-
The treatment package effectively integrates skill ing the theory. The theoretical foundations
acquisition and application through the FEAR remain a reliable platform from which to launch
rubric (feeling frightened, expecting bad things multiple clinical interventions.
to happen, attitudes and actions that help, results
and rewards). The protocol has been investigated
through three major randomized clinical trials Empirical Findings

Table 1  Content-specificity hypothesis Foundational CBT research heavily investigated


Mood state Specific cognitive content the efficacy and effectiveness of the model. It was
Depression Negative view of self critical to determine whether CBT could success-
Negative view of others/experiences fully address the psychological needs of young
Negative view of the future patients. As protocols were tested, results were
Anxiety Overestimation of the probability of replicated: CBT works. At this point, CBT is
danger shown to be a broadly effective treatment for
Overestimation of the magnitude of the children and adolescents with psychological
danger
disorders.
Neglect of rescue factors
Ignoring coping resources
Anger Hostile attributional bias
Labeling others
Mood Disorders
Sense of complete unfairness
Violation of personal imperatives Depression  CBT is a well-established treatment
Panic Catastrophic misinterpretation of normal for depressive disorders in youth (David-Ferdon
bodily sensations & Kaslow, 2008; Weersing, Jeffreys, Do,
Social Fear of negative evaluation Schwartz, & Bolano, 2016). CBT reduced the
anxiety intensity and severity of depressive symptoms as
46 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

well as improved overall functioning (Weersing plus CBT condition. Regardless of the medica-
et al., 2016; Weisz, McCarty, & Valeri, 2006). tion used, the addition of CBT produced enhanced
Young patients treated with CBT also used less functioning and greater decreases in depressive
medication and sought fewer outpatient visits symptomology (Brent et al., 2008). CBT clearly
with their pediatricians (Clarke et al., 2005; addresses the impairments and deficits observed
March & Vitiello, 2009). CBT decreased suicidal in depression more effectively than medication
ideation in a time-effective manner (Weersing alone.
et al., 2016). Neither age, gender, nor ethnicity Although comparable in cost and acute out-
influenced the effectiveness of CBT for young come in many trials (Domino et al., 2009), CBT
patients (Weisz et al., 2006). Treatment was is still considered the best option to treat mild to
equally successful when delivered in either indi- moderate depression (Melvin et al., 2006) and an
vidual or group format (David-Ferdon & Kaslow, essential addition for the treatment of severe
2008). Recent effectiveness studies showed that depression (March & Vitiello, 2009). Medications
CBT continued to perform well in school and may at times demonstrate a reduction in depres-
community settings (David-Ferdon & Kaslow, sive symptoms faster than CBT (TADS Team,
2008; Eiraldi et al., 2016; Weersing et al., 2016). 2004). However, antidepressants reach the upper
CBT’s performance across a variety of contexts limit of effectiveness in a relatively short period
and conditions underscores its empirical strength. of time (12–18 weeks), and gains are not often
Most often, CBT is compared to or combined sustained after the completion of treatment (i.e.,
with antidepressant medications in studies of discontinuation of use) (TADS Team, 2009).
youth depression. The Treatment for Adolescent Benefits observed from a completed trial of CBT
Depression Study (TADS; TADS Team, 2004), a are maintained for a year or more after the com-
major, multisite trial, randomly assigned 439 pletion of treatment (Weisz et al., 2006; Weersing
adolescents ages 12–17 to be treated with sertra- et al., 2016). Vitiello et al. (2009) determined
line, a 12-week course of CBT, a combination CBT was an influential component of treatment
(medication and CBT), or a pill placebo. Data for adolescents who recently attempted suicide.
collected posttreatment initially suggested that Kennard et al. (2008) studied whether treatment
depression was best treated by either medication with CBT after successful response to an antide-
alone or a combination of medication and pressant served to protect against relapse. Youth
CBT. However, by week 18 (6 weeks after the who received CBT were eight times less likely to
conclusion of therapy), patients in the CBT group experience a return of clinically significant symp-
demonstrated improvement in depressive symp- toms of depression. Furthermore, youth treated
toms equal to those in the medication conditions. with CBT show continued improvement beyond
Indeed, CBT patients showed steady gains up to the completion of therapy (TADS Team, 2009).
36 weeks after the conclusion of care (TADS
Team, 2009). Although initially seeming to dem-
onstrate the superiority of medication, TADS Bipolar Disorder
revealed the power of CBT to effectively and
enduringly treat depression in adolescents. Although experienced at significantly lower inci-
The Treatment of Resistant Depression in dence than unipolar depression, bipolar disorders
Adolescents (TORDIA; Brent et al., 2008) proj- create debilitating impairments in functioning
ect, another major clinical study, sought to evalu- and yield significantly poorer prognosis (Pavuluri,
ate the added benefit of including CBT in Naylor, & Janicak, 2002). As such, effective
treatment after adolescents failed to respond to a treatments are desperately sought by families of
2-month trial of an SSRI. Of the 334 adolescents children diagnosed with bipolar disorders. CBT
between the ages of 12 and 18, some were packages adapted to specifically address family
assigned to one of two medication-only groups, variables are shown to produce meaningful out-
and others were assigned to a new medication comes for youth diagnosed with bipolar disorder.
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 47

Research demonstrates substantial decreases in combination treatment, or pill placebo condi-


symptoms of mania, depression, aggression, tions. At posttreatment, the combination condi-
ADHD, and psychosis, as well as improvements tion outperformed either monotherapy with
in sleep disturbance, global functioning, parent- respect to symptom reduction or diagnostic
ing skills, and family flexibility for youth treatedremission. Both sertraline and CBT alone sur-
with CBT (MacPherson, Weinstein, Henry, & passed placebo in effectiveness. No adverse
West, 2016; Pavuluri et al., 2004; Pavuluri, events related to suicide occurred; however, there
Birmaher, & Naylor, 2005). CBT for bipolar dis- were a number of reported undesirable side
orders addresses symptoms, deficits, and family effects for sertraline (e.g., fatigue, insomnia, rest-
functioning to give young patients the tools they lessness, and sedation; Walkup et al., 2008). The
need to alter the trajectories of their lives. CAMS arbitrarily shortened treatment from 16 to
14 sessions to better fit the study. Furthermore,
“posttreatment” measures were completed at
Anxiety Disorders week 12, neglecting the final two sessions for
CBT conditions. No follow-up data was pro-
CBT stands as the most steadfastly well-­ vided. Given the results of parallel trials for youth
established treatment for youth with anxiety dis- depression, it is reasonable to conclude that
orders (Higa-McMillan, Francis, Rith-Najarian, information collected in the months that followed
& Chorpita, 2016; Seligman & Ollendick, 2011). treatment would reveal superior long-term out-
Age, gender, symptom severity, and ethnicity do comes for CBT when compared with pharmaco-
not moderate treatment outcomes (Kendall & logical interventions. Despite methodological
Peterman, 2015; Walkup et al., 2008). CBT for flaws, CAMS continues to serve as definitive
child anxiety disorders reduces symptom sever- support of CBT as an effective treatment of child-
ity, improves global functioning, enhances social hood anxiety.
functioning, and reduces sleep-related problem The FRIENDS program (Shortt, Barrett, &
behaviors (Higa-McMillan et al., 2016; Pereira Fox, 2001) was designed as a 10-week family
et al., 2016; Peterman et al., 2016; Walkup et al., CBT program based on Coping Cat and modified
2008). CBT is an equally effective intervention in response to parent and child feedback. One-­
when delivered in group, individual, and family session treatment (OST) (Ollendick et al., 2015;
formats (Bennett et al., 2016; Kendall & Ost, 1997) addresses specific phobias in a single
Peterman, 2015). prolonged-exposure session. Social effectiveness
Effect sizes range from 0.99 to 1.31 by some training (SET-C; Beidel, Turner, & Morris, 2000)
estimates (Scaini, Belotti, Ogliari, & Battaglia, is an intensive program for youth with social pho-
2016). Bennett et al. (2016) completed an over- bia that combines group and individual sessions
view of systematic reviews and reported odds with a heavy emphasis on exposures and skill
ratios ranging from 3.27 to 7.85 in favor of CBT generalization.
when compared with passive controls. In other Brief cognitive behavioral therapy (BCBT) is
words, children who participated in CBT condi- a natural extension of the Coping Cat approach
tions were three to seven times more likely to (Beidas, Mychailyszyn, Podell, & Kendall,
show significant reduction in anxiety symptoms 2013). BCBT shortens the 16-session Coping Cat
after treatment. protocol to eight sessions and distills the approach
The Child/Adolescent Anxiety Multimodal down to the essential elements (psychoeducation,
Treatment Study (CAMS; Kendall et al., 2016; problem-solving, cognitive restructuring, expo-
Walkup et al., 2008) was a massive multisite sure). According to Beidas et al., “BCBT was
RCT studying the treatment of childhood anxiety developed primarily in response to a need for
that used Coping Cat in the CBT conditions. treatments whose dissemination and implemen-
Walkup and colleagues randomly assigned tation are more feasible in the community
488 youth ages 7–17 to either CBT, sertraline, given existing barriers to care” (p. 34). BCBT
48 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

steps both parents and children through the However, combination and CBT alone conditions
well-­established FEAR (feeling frightened, produced equal numbers of patients in remission
expec­ ting bad things to happen, attitudes and after 12 weeks.
actions that help, results and reward) plan. In POTS II was conducted to extend the findings
their single-­case study report, Beidas and col- of the first study (Freeman et al., 2009). Youth
leagues documented significant reductions in all ages 7–17 who failed a medication trial were
clinical indices. In an open pilot study evaluating assigned to an augmented medication trial, medi-
initial outcomes and feasibility with 26 children cation management plus basic instruction in CBT
ages 6–13 years, the results showed very favor- strategies, or medication management plus a
able outcomes (Crawley et al., 2013). BCBT was complete CBT package (Freeman et al., 2009).
seen as feasible, acceptable, and beneficial to The majority of patients (68.6%) who received a
vulnerable youth. complete course of CBT were considered
responders to treatment, more than double the
response rate of medication alone or medication
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder with the addition of CBT strategies (30% and
34%, respectively; Franklin et al., 2011). Youth
CBT is widely recognized as the effective psy- who receive CBT drop out of treatment much less
chosocial treatment for obsessive-compulsive often than those who were treated with medica-
disorder (OCD) in youth. In meta-analyses, the tion only (Ost et al., 2016). Combination treat-
only studies included in the review are those that ment does not consistently demonstrate any
involve CBT and medication singly or in combi- benefit beyond that accomplished by CBT alone
nation (Barrett, Farrell, Pina, Peris, & Piacentini, (Ost et al., 2016).
2008; Ost, Riise, Wergeland, & Hansen, 2016;
Wu, Lang & Zhang, 2016). Although many pro-
tocols include 12–16 sessions, one study reported Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
that 38.3% of patients were considered respond-
ers and 13.7% had already achieved remission at Similar to OCD, CBT is the single most effective
the midpoint of a 14-week program (Torp & treatment for children who experience post­
Skarphedinsson, 2017). Age, gender, and medi- traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Dorsey et al.
cation use prior to initiation of CBT did not mod- (2016) reviewed 37 published studies of treat-
erate outcomes (Piacentini, Bergman, Jacobs, ment of PTSD in youth and concluded CBT is
McCracken, & Kretchman, 2002). Research indi- the only well-established treatment for this pop-
cates 40–85% remission rates (Barrett et al., ulation. Across CBT protocols to treat PTSD,
2008) and marked reductions in symptoms in the there are four common elements: psychoeduca-
US and international samples (Wu et al., 2016). tion, relaxation and emotion regulation skills,
Treatment gains appeared durable, persisting exposure, and cognitive restructuring (Dorsey,
15 months or more (Barrett et al., 2008). Two Briggs, & Woods, 2011). Two branded CBT
major clinical trials investigated comparative packages (Trauma-Focused Cognitive Beha­
efficacy of CBT and medication to treat OCD in vioral Therapy (TF-CBT) and Cognitive-
young patients. Behavioral Intervention for Trauma in Schools
The Pediatric OCD Treatment Study (POTS; (CBITS)) boast extensive efficacy and effective-
POTS Team, 2004) randomized 112 youth ages ness. Trauma-Focused CBT (TF-CBT; Cohen,
7–17 to 12 weeks of medication management, Deblinger, Mannarino, & Steer, 2004) is a
CBT, combination, or placebo. In the posttreat- 12-week individual intervention that includes
ment evaluations, the combination condition parents; and Cognitive-Behavioral Intervention
demonstrated the greatest response, while CBT for Trauma in Schools (CBITS; Stein et al.,
and medication alone were equivalent and both 2003) is a 10-week group intervention designed
better at reducing symptoms than placebo. for delivery in the school setting.
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 49

TF-CBT was originally developed and tested talized on the ease of accessing services in the
on children with PTSD who suffered sexual school-based model and provided treatment to
abuse (Cohen et al., 2004). Over time, TF-CBT students affected by a major natural disaster
has been tested with youth who experienced sex- (Hurricane Katrina). Children were treated with
ual and physical abuse, neighborhood violence, either CBITS or TF-CBT; the outcomes follow-
assault, and natural disaster (Cohen et al., 2004; ing therapy were equivalent, illustrating the com-
Dorsey et al., 2016). TF-CBT reduced symptoms parable relative effectiveness of the protocols
of PTSD and comorbid depression in both chil- (Jaycox et al., 2010). In conclusion, CBT is a
dren and participating parents/caregivers (Neill, powerful tool to mollify symptoms and deficits
Weems, & Scheeringa, 2016; Nixon, Sterk, & associated with PTSD in children.
Pearce, 2012). TF-CBT also created clinically
significant reductions in behavioral problems,
overall anxiety levels, and feelings of shame as Disruptive Behavior Disorders
well as improvements in functioning (Cohen
et al., 2004; Cohen, Mannarino, Kliethermes, & In a review of 86 studies of psychosocial treat-
Murray, 2012; Dorsey et al., 2014). Studies inves- ments of disruptive behavior disorders (DBD),
tigating the effectiveness of TF-CBT typically McCart and Sheidow (2016) identified the well-­
include youth who are ethnically and racially established and probably efficacious models
diverse, who are in single-parent households or in CBT spectrum approaches. CBT protocols for
foster care placements, and whose families earn DBD reduce substance use, caregiver report of
below the federal poverty income level ( Dorsey externalizing behaviors, difficulties completing
et al., 2016; Scheeringa, Weems, Cohen, Amaya-­ homework, aggression, and impulsive behaviors
Jackson, & Guthrie, 2011). Age, gender, ethnic- (Boyer et al., 2016; Eyberg, Nelson, & Boggs,
ity, and severity of symptoms did not impact 2008; Froelich, Doepfner, & Lemkuhl, 2002
success of treatment (Dorsey et al., 2016). Youth Hogue et al., 2015). The most rigorously studied
in foster homes who completed TF-CBT were treatments involve youth who are incarcerated,
less likely to run away and experienced fewer on probation, and/or displaying severe degrees of
changes in placement over time (Cohen et al., antisocial behaviors (McCart & Sheidow, 2016).
2012). The addition of medication to therapy pro- Typically, CBT is conducted in individual, group,
vides no further emotional or behavioral benefit or family formats (Eiraldi et al., 2016; Henggeler
(Cohen, Mannarino, Perel, & Staron, 2007). et al., 2009; Hogue et al., 2015; Shin, 2009).
TF-CBT plainly generalizes across a wide-­ Lochman and Wells (2002, 2003) developed
ranging group of youth; thus, future efforts must the 12-week CBT Coping Power group program
attend to dissemination of services and eliminat- (CPP) to be delivered in schools to youth display-
ing barriers to care. ing DBD symptoms. In the pilot study, younger
CBITS demonstrates comparable results albeit students (fourth grade) were identified early by
with fewer studies. Stein et al. (2003) enrolled teachers as high risk for developing a DBD due to
126 sixth-grade students from local public school higher levels of observed aggression and impul-
in their initial CBITS trial. At the completion of sivity. Students then participated in CPP the fol-
the program, students displayed fewer symptoms lowing year. Students who participated in the
of PTSD and depression and lower levels of program exhibited lower levels of aggression,
impairment (Stein et al., 2003). Youth who par- substance use, and hostile attributions (Lochman
ticipated in CBITS also demonstrated higher aca- & Wells, 2002, 2003). Participants also showed
demic achievement by the end of the year higher social functioning and improved problem-­
(Kataoka et al., 2011). CBITS also successfully solving skills. Gains were maintained at 1-year
treated symptoms of PTSD and depression in a follow-up (Lochman & Wells, 2003). Eiraldi
school on a rural Native American reservation et al. (2016) delivered CPP to youth with a pri-
(Morsette et al., 2009). Jaycox et al. (2010) capi- mary diagnosis of DBD in urban school for stu-
50 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

dents between the kindergarten and eighth grade communicate when sensory input was distressing
levels. Over half (59%) of the group members them (Edgington et al., 2016).
displayed significant reductions in problem In conclusion, CBT effectively treats deficits
behaviors; both symptoms of oppositional defiant associated with ASD, conditions that co-occur
disorder and ADHD were addressed by with ASD, and does not require excessive sub-
CBT. CBT adequately addresses the symptoms stantial modification. Wolters, de Haan,
of DBD and proves able to divert youth from a Hogendoorn, Boer, and Prins (2016) stated that
risky trajectory. little to no special adaptation to conventional
CBT is needed to treat comorbid conditions
accompanying ASD. Moreover, they concluded
Autism Spectrum Disorder that symptom severity does not moderate treatment
effectiveness.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurode-
velopmental disorder that is characterized by
impairments in social functioning, communica-  BT with Youth: The Golden
C
tion, emotion regulation, dysregulated behaviors, Nuggets
sensory sensitivity, and rigidity (American
Psychiatric Association, 2013). ASD is also often There are several core elements, or golden nug-
accompanied by comorbid conditions, the most gets, which characterize CBT with young patients
common of which is anxiety (Joshi et al., 2010). (Chorpita & Weisz, 2009; Friedberg & Brelsford,
Accordingly the bulk of studies apply CBT to 2011; Nangle et al., 2016; Scarpa & Lorenzi,
comorbid ASD and anxiety disorders. White 2013). The essentials include emphasis on
et al. (2010) created a CBT program that empha- measurement-­ based care, adhering to session
sizes acquisition of social skills while simultane- structure, adopting a therapeutic stance, as well
ously addressing symptoms of anxiety. The as deploying psychoeducation, cognitive, and
treatment package included a combination of behavioral interventions.
individual and group therapy. Outcome data indi-
cated a clinically significant reduction in the
symptoms of anxiety as well as improvements in Measurement-Based Care
social and overall functioning (White et al., 2010,
2013). Parents reported they particularly liked Scott and Lewis (2015) parsimoniously described
homework assignments and being included in measurement-based care as the steady, routine
treatment (White et al., 2010). Sofronoff, tracking of treatment process, progress, and out-
Attwood, and Hinton (2005) reported that parents come using reliable and valid measures. “Let the
of children with ASD and anxiety observed data be your guide” is a familiar CBT aphorism.
increased friendships, higher confidence, and Collecting and using data to inform case concep-
improved emotional regulation skills in their tualization, collaborative clinical decision-­
children after treatment with CBT. making, and treatment planning is a long-standing
CBT is also used to treat sensory sensitivity tradition.
often seen in youth with ASD. Edgington, Hill, Numerous authors claim MBC offers multiple
and Pellicano (2016) noted that after treatment advantages to clinicians and patients (Bickman,
with school-based CBT, youth with ASD were 2008; Chorpita et al., 2011; Chorpita, Bernstein,
able to apply their coping skills to reduce distress & Daleiden, 2008; Chorpita & Daleiden, 2014;
in response to sensory input. Moreover, the learn- Chorpita, Daleiden, & Bernstein, 2016; Gondek,
ing generalized to contexts other than school set- Edbrooke-Childs, Fink, Deighton, & Wolpert,
tings. Children who participated in treatment 2016; Jensen-Doss, 2015; McLeod, Jensen-Doss
reported they liked learning more about how to & Ollendick, 2013). Overall, MBC shows favor-
able results in promoting treatment effectiveness,
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 51

efficiency, and collaborative involvement in ther- patient, family, and therapist are listed, and time
apy (Gondek et al., 2016). is allocated to discussing the identified topics.
MBC typically involves collecting data on After agendas are established, session content
functional improvements, reductions in symptom is therapeutically processed with an eye toward
scores, and patient satisfaction ratings (Scott & balancing content, structure, and process.
Lewis, 2015). Functional outcome metrics are Homework is assigned based on session content.
generally seen as the most persuasive type of data Finally, the patients’ perception of the treatment
(Chorpita, 2014). Functional indicators include process and the particular clinician is elicited.
outcomes such as reductions in medication dos-
ages, visits to school nurses, hospitalizations,
incarcerations, and school suspensions. Tracking Therapeutic Style and Stance
outcomes via symptom scores is relatively a
commonplace in clinical practice. Beidas et al. There are three central therapeutic processes that
(2015) provided comprehensive, low-cost, and flow through the course of CBT. These corner-
psychometrically sound methods for charting stones include collaborative empiricism, guided
progress. Patient satisfaction ratings round out discovery, and practicing flexibility within
choices for practitioners applying MBC. Most fidelity. The three clinical postures oxygenate
practitioners elect to use some combination of CBT’s lifeblood.
functional improvement, symptom, and patient Collaborative empiricism (CE) (Beck et al.,
satisfaction instruments. These measures are reg- 1979) explicitly addresses therapeutic relation-
ularly administered at intervals (1 week, 2 weeks, ship issues in CBT with youth. CE involves form-
monthly, etc.) collaboratively defined by clini- ing partnerships between children, families, and
cians and patients. Generally, the acuity and clinicians. The empiricism part of the CE equa-
severity determine how frequently these indices tion refers to the data-driven nature of treatment.
are readministered. CBT is tied to its phenomenological moorings
and relies on measures that monitor treatment
progress. Clinical improvement is regularly
Session Structure assessed, and feedback on therapeutic headway is
provided to patients. As Kendall et al. (1992)
There is a long-standing emphasis on applying a cogently explained, CBT uses an empirical
characteristic session structure in CBT (Beck, approach to evaluate the accuracy of beliefs.
2011; Beck, Rush, Shaw,& Emery, 1979). CBT Patients and therapists become thought detectives
sessions include components such as mood sorting through clues to evaluate patients’
check-ins, homework review, agenda setting, assumptions. Guided discovery is the process
processing session content, homework assign- built by therapists and patients to collect and con-
ment, and eliciting feedback. sider data. Socratic dialogues and behavioral
Mood check-ins can be conducted formally experiments are employed to cast doubt on chil-
and/or informally. Formal mood check-ins may dren’s beliefs rather than absolutely refuting or
involve patients completing a short symptom disputing them (Padesky, 1993).
checklist such as the Beck Depression Inventory Flexibility within fidelity is a clear guiding
(BDI-2; Beck, 1996) or Children’s Depression principle in CBT with youth (Kendall & Beidas,
Inventory-2 (CDI-2; Kovacs, 2010). Mood check-­ins 2008; Kendall, Gosch, Furr, & Sood, 2008).
could also be accomplished simply by asking the Simply, good CBT balances solid footing in
patients how they are feeling. Homework review social learning theory and CBT-based clinical
addresses patients’ completion of self-­help tasks practices with creativity. Innovative applications
between sessions or patients’ non-­ compliance of traditional methods must be grounded in a
with tasks. Agenda setting is a collaborative coherent conceptual model informed by learning
process where items of concern to both the theory in order to avoid going rogue. Indeed, con-
52 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

ceptualization is seen as the nucleus of CBT with butional methods all play a role in the treatment
youth (Friedberg, 2015). Clinicians who practice regime. These methods vary in complexity and
flexibility within fidelity clearly focus on cognitive their demand for in-depth rational analysis.
and behavioral mechanisms of change. Finally,
Kendall and colleagues (Kendall & Beidas, 2008; Problem-Solving  According to the seminal
Kendall et al., 2008) asserted a focus on action- paper by D’Zurilla and Goldfried (1971), “prob-
oriented treatment, and experiential application lem solving may be defined as a behavioral pro-
of techniques tether flexibility to a faithful cess, whether overt or cognitive in nature, which
adherence to CBT tenets. (a) makes available a variety of potentially effec-
tive response alternatives for dealing with the
problematic situation and (b) increases the prob-
Psychoeducation ability of selecting the most effective response
from among these various alternatives” (p. 108).
Psychoeducation involves teaching patients and Problem-solving methods are listed in the ten
their caregivers about symptoms, treatment alter- most frequently used procedures in clinical prac-
natives, and course of treatment (Ong & Caron, tice with youth (Chorpita & Daleiden, 2009). In
2008). In their review, Nangle et al. (2016) noted general, problem-solving approaches aim to
that psychoeducation empowers patients, instills increase young people’s alternatives and choices.
hope, enlists collaboration, normalizes distress, Crawley, Podell, Beidas, Braswell, and Kendall
and fosters engagement in treatment. Piacentini (2010) stated that teaching children to “solve
and Bergman (2001) asserted that psychoeduca- problems allows the children to gain confidence
tion should be accessible and understandable to in their ability to resolve daily struggles that once
patients and their families. Friedberg et al. (2009) may have seemed hopeless” (p. 379).
wrote, “the patient’s task includes reading and While there are a variety of problem-solving
personalizing information rather than simply rubrics, they all share basic properties (Chorpita
adopting it” (p. 56). & Weisz, 2009). The core components include
Psychoeducation is not a one-shot interven- clearly defining the problem, brainstorming pos-
tion. Rather, it should be part of ongoing treat- sible solutions, identifying the long-term positive
ment. There are multiple ways to deliver and negative consequences, choosing the best
psychoeducational material including pamphlets, alternative, and evaluating the outcome of the
books, websites, DVDs, games, TV shows, mov- new strategy. Many problem-solving interven-
ies, music, and other media. The various distribu- tions also include self-reward components for
tion options enable flexibility and responsiveness successfully implementing the process.
to individuals’ particular contexts (e.g., age, gen-
der, ethnicity, language, reading ability). Self-Instruction  Self-instruction earned its
Friedberg and colleagues (Friedberg et al., 2009; empirical stripes with impulsive youth
Friedberg, Gorman, Wilt, Biuckians, & Murray, (Meichenbaum & Goodman, 1971). Meichenbaum
2011) offer practitioners a plethora of psychoed- and Goodman found that self-­ instruction was
ucational resources to use with young patients effective in changing impulsive children’s behavior
and their families. as measured by psychometrics such as perfor-
mance IQ, cognitive impulsivity, and motor activ-
ity. More specifically, the impulsive youth
Cognitive Interventions employed self-instruction to orient, organize, regu-
late, and self-reward their behavior.
A variety of cognitive interventions distinguish a Self-instruction is a relatively straightforward
CBT approach. Problem-solving, self-­instruction, method used to change young patients’ habitual
as well as rational analysis techniques such as internal dialogues. Self-instruction works to
tests of evidence, decatastrophizing, and reattri- replace or exchange inaccurate dysfunctional
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 53

thoughts with counter-thoughts that propel more nations is assessed. Finally, young patients derive
adaptive functioning (Meichenbaum, 1985). a new conclusion. Consequently, the procedure
Effective self-instruction provides young patients fosters flexibility in thinking and consideration of
with a medium for translating distressing internal multiple alternatives.
instant messages into hopeful and adaptive men- Decatastrophizing targets children’s overesti-
tal dispatches. Friedberg and McClure (2015) mation of the magnitude and probability of vari-
offered several guidelines for self-instructional ous perceived dangers (Kendall et al., 1992).
interventions with children and adolescents. Decatastrophizing is generally accomplished via
First, self-instruction should be action-oriented, a series of Socratic questions including “What’s
and children’s attention is directed toward behav- the worst that could happen?”, ‘What’s the best
ing in a more productive fashion. Essentially, that could happen?”, and “What is the most likely
proper self-instruction involves making a “calm- thing to happen?” (Beck, 2011). Friedberg and
ing yet strategic statement” (Friedberg & McClure (2015) recommended adding a problem-­
McClure, 2015; p. 160). Additionally, self-­ solving component to the procedure.
instruction balances acceptance of distress with Rational analysis procedures are complex
plans for coping. Finally, self-instruction includes interventions and are difficult for the novice to
a self-reward component. learn quickly. However, there are many useful
workbooks/texts that break down this challeng-
Rational Analysis  Rational analysis procedures ing procedure into simple steps. The rational
are based on the Socratic method. Socratic meth- analysis interventions can be made more acces-
ods are rooted in the vestigial philosophies of sible to patients and clinicians through scaffold-
Socrates who argued that self-discovery is an ele- ing exercises. Various workbooks break rational
mental human endeavor (Overholser, 2010). The analysis into understandable steps for youth
goal of the Socratic method is for young patients (Friedberg, Friedberg, & Friedberg, 2001;
to come to their own conclusions. McLachlan, Kendall & Hedtke, 2006; Stallard, 2002).
Eastwood, and Friedberg (2016) wrote that
“Socratic questions facilitate children’s apprecia-
tion of hidden possibilities” (p. 106). Tests of evi- Behavioral Interventions
dence, reattribution, and decatastrophizing are
three major categories of rational analysis. Relaxation  Relaxation training is a very common
Tests of evidence (TOE) are staples in CBT with cognitive behavioral technique. The procedures
youth. Logical analysis is front and center in these and its variations are listed as the second most
procedures. When completing a TOE, patients and regularly used method for treating anxiety and
therapists look for facts supporting the belief and the ninth most customarily employed procedure
facts disconfirming the belief and form conclusions for depression (Chorpita & Daleiden, 2009). In
based on mindful deliberations regarding the con- fact, relaxation is part of the many empirically
firming and contradictory evidence. supported protocols for treating a variety of dis-
While TOE focuses on facts, reattribution cen- orders (Nangle et al., 2016). Nangle et al. asserted
ters on the explanations for facts. Reattributional that “relaxation training involves teaching
methods emphasize there are always multiple ­effective ways to reduce physiological arousal
explanations for events. Inaccurately jumping to related to tension and stress, thereby helping cli-
conclusions, overestimation of personal respon- ents achieve this freedom” (p. 114). Relaxation
sibility for negative events, and absolutistic procedures can be relatively simple involving
thinking are mitigated by this procedure. Similar controlled diaphragmatic breathing or more com-
to TOE, there are Socratic steps in reattribution. plicated including muscle tensing/relaxing cycles
First, alternate explanations are built (e.g., and images (Masters, Burish, Hollon, & Rimm,
“what’s another way of looking at this?”). Next, 1987). Additionally, Masters et al. explained that
the believability or plausibility of the new expla- relaxation can be implemented singly or in com-
54 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

bination with other techniques. Regardless, the Social Skills Training  Matson, Matson, and
goal for relaxation is providing young patients withRivet (2007) defined social skills as “interper-
a greater sense of self-­efficacy and self-control. sonal responses with specific operational defini-
tions that allow the child to adapt to the
Contingency Contracting  Tolin (2016) parsi- environment through verbal and non-verbal com-
moniously defined contingencies “as the context munication” (p. 685). Social skills training (SST)
in which a behavior occurs” (p.26). Antecedents is an ubiquitous group of interventions applicable
to and consequences of the behavior are essential to depression, anxiety, and autism spectrum and
components. Positive reinforcement, negative disruptive behavior disorders. Krumholz, Ugueto,
reinforcement, response cost, and punishment Santucci, and Weisz (2014) explained that “SST
represent contingencies. Accordingly, contin- typically addresses interpersonal engagement,
gency management techniques are based on building and maintaining friendships, communi-
operant conditioning principles and reflect cation and negotiation, assertiveness, and dealing
explicit agreements between caregivers and chil- with bullying” (p. 260). In order to accomplish
dren that are designed to increase desirable these goals, a multicomponent treatment package
behaviors (Wells & Forehand, 1981). These is indicated including behavioral skill training,
interventions specifically spell out desired behav- social perception instruction, teaching social
iors, their consequences, and the contexts in problem-solving skills and self-regulation, as
which these actions are expected to take place. well as contingency management (Spence, 2003).
Caregivers (e.g., teachers, parents) are taught to SST includes modeling, role-playing, provid-
give clear, specific requests, reinforce compli- ing positive and constructive feedback, problem-­
ance, and apply response cost procedures to non-­ solving obstacles, and assigning homework
compliant actions. Verbal praise, tangible (Nangle et al. 2016). Laugeson and colleagues
rewards, time-out, removing rewards and privi- (Laugeson & Frankel, 2011; Laugeson, Frankel,
leges, and ignoring are prototypical procedures. Gantman, Dillon, & Mogil, 2012) recommended
that SST protocols fit instruction to individual cir-
Pleasant Event Scheduling/Behavioral cumstances and include homework assignments.
Activation  Behavioral activation and pleasant Additionally, they emphasized that the skills must
activity scheduling are often used interchange- be ecologically valid so that strategies match what
ably. Both work to increase young patients’ youth who are successful in negotiating interper-
opportunities for positive reinforcement and sonal situations do in similar contexts.
decrease avoidance (Dimidjian, Barrera,
Martell, Munoz, & Lewinsohn, 2011; McCauley, Experiments/Exposures  Behavioral experi-
Schlordedt, Gudmudsen, Martell, & Dimidian, ments and exposures are experiential learning
2011; Mendlowitz, 2014). Activity scheduling, opportunities in CBT (Kendall, Robin, Hedtke,
problem-solving, and social skill training are Suveg, Flannery-Schroeder, & Gosch, 2006).
often included in behavioral activation (Kanter, Friedberg (2015) stated, “During exposures and
Rusch, Busch, & Sedivy, 2009). Clinicians work experiments, patients face down what they have
with young patients to collaboratively identify previously avoided owing to their anxiety, shame,
sources of positive reinforcement that are cur- depression, age or any other distressing emotion”
rently lost or avoided. Then, they develop a plan (p. 530). Behavioral experiments and exposures
for patients to re-engage in these pleasant activi- change emotional reactions, behavioral tenden-
ties. When obstacles emerge, patients apply cies, problem-solving strategies, and obdurate
problem-solving skills to address these barriers. beliefs (Rouf, Fennell, Westbrook, Cooper, &
If social deficits prevent engagement in pleasant Bennett-Levy, 2004). Genuine self-efficacy is
activities, social skills training is applied to nurtured through these performance attainments
these areas. because children obtain real-time disconfirming
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 55

Table 2  Sample creative exposures and the problems and effective intervention packages combine to
they target
make CBT a first-rate treatment approach.
Creative exposure Target problem Pioneers such as Skinner, Bandura, Beck,
Operation game (Peterman Intolerance for Ellis, Kendall, and Chorpita have forged new
et al. 2015) uncertainty, distress
trails. However, the work is not finished. CBT is
tolerance
Pop up monkeys Intolerance for frustration,
continuing to evolve from these initial paths to
(Friedberg et al. 2009) overvalued sense of construct vehicles carrying innovative treatments
control to young patients and their providers in various
Easter egg hunt (Peterman OCD contexts. Integrating CBT into primary care,
et al. 2015) developing technology-assisted procedures, and
Improvisational theater Social anxiety, tolerance discovering cost-effective ways to deliver these
games (Friedberg et al. for uncertainty
2009) interventions represent exciting new frontiers.
Sharing the Persian flaw Perfectionism
(Friedberg et al. 2009)
References
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
evidence regarding their predictions and assump-
and statistical manual (5th ed.). Washington, DC:
tions (Bandura, 1977a). These methods place less American Psychiatric Association.
emphasis on verbally mediated intervention and Bandura, A. (1977a). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying
rely heavily on action. theory of behavior change. Psychological Review, 84,
191–215.
The core procedures in exposures include cre-
Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. Englewood
ating a hierarchy, collaboratively implementing Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
the exposure in a stepwise manner, and process- Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and
ing/debriefing the experience (Nangle et al., action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Barrett, P. M., Farrell, L., Pina, A. A., Peris, T. S., &
2016). Most experiments and exposures are
Piacentini, J. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial
titrated from the least to most distressing. treatments for child and adolescent obsessive–compul-
Behavioral hierarchies are collaboratively cre- sive disorder. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
ated, and children work their way up the rungs of Psychology, 37(1), 131–155.
Beck, A. T. (1996). Beck depression inventory–II. San
the ladder in a stepwise fashion. There are many
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
creative ways to design and implement exposures Bearman, S. K., & Weisz, J. R. (2015). Review:
(Friedberg et al., 2009; Kendall, Robin, Suveg, Comprehensive treatments for youth co-­ morbidity:
Flannery-Schroeder, & Gosch, 2006; Peterman, Evidence guided approaches to a complicated prob-
lem. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 20,
Read, Wei, & Kendall, 2015). Table 2 lists several
131–141.
creative experiments/exposures and the targeted Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional
problems. disorders. New York, NY: International University
Press.
Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavior therapy: Basics
and beyond (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. (1988). Anxiety and depres-
Conclusion sion: An information processing perspective. Anxiety
Research, 1, 23–36.
From a small seed, a mighty trunk will grow. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).
Aeschuylus Cognitive therapy of depression. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Beidas, R. S., Mychailysysn, M. P., Podell, J. L., &
Aeschylus’s words describe the evolution of Kendall, P. C. (2013). Brief cognitive-behavioral ther-
CBT. CBT began as a small seed and now pro- apy for anxious youth: The inner workings. Cognitive
and Behavioral Practice, 20, 134–146.
vides a mighty trunk that supports clinical applica- Beidas, R. S., Podell, J. L., & Kendall, P. C. (2008).
tions with emotionally distressed youth in a variety Cognitive-behavioral treatment for child and ado-
of settings. Theory-building, empirical findings, lescent anxiety: The Coping Cat Program. In C. W.
56 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

LeCroy (Ed.), Handbook of evidence-based treatment in Mental and Mental Health Services Research, 43,
manuals for children and adolescents (pp. 405–430). 471–477.
New York, NY: Oxford Press. Chorpita, B. F., Daleiden, E. L., Ebsutani, C., Young, J.,
Beidas, R. S., Stewart, R. E., Walsh, L., Lucas, S., Downey, Becker, K. D., Nakamura, B. J., & Smith, R. I. (2011).
M. M., Jackson, K., … Mandell, D. S. (2015). Free, Evidence-based treatments for children and adoles-
brief, and validated: Standardized instruments for cents: An updated review of indicators of efficiency
low-resource mental health settings. Cognitive and and effectiveness. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Behavioral Practice, 22, 5–19. Practice, 18, 154–172.
Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., & Morris, T. L. (2000). Chorpita, B. F., Park, A., Tsai, K., Korathu-Larson,
Behavioral treatment of childhood social phobia. P., Higa-McMillan, C. K., Nakamura, B. J., …
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(6), The Research Network on Youth Mental Health.
1072–1080. (2015). Balancing effectiveness with responsive-
Benjamin, C. L., Puleo, C. M., Settipani, C. A., Brodman, ness: Therapist satisfaction across different treatment
D. M., Edmunds, J. M., Cummings, C. M., & Kendall, designs in the child STEPS randomized effectiveness
P. C. (2011). History of cognitive-behavioral therapy trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
in youth. Child and Psychiatric Clinics of North 83, 709–718.
America, 20, 179–190. Chorpita, B. F., & Weisz, J. R. (2009). Modular approach
Bennett, K., Manassis, K., Duda, S., Bagnell, A., Bernstein, to therapy for children with anxiety, depression,
G. A., Garland, E. J., … Wilansky, P. (2016). Treating trauma or conduct problems (MATCH-ADTC).
child and adolescent anxiety effectively: Overview of Satellite Beach, FL: Practicewise.
systematic reviews. Clinical Psychology Review, 50, Clark, D. M., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (1999).
80–94. Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy
Bernard, M. E., & Joyce, M. R. (1984). Rational-emotive of depression. New York: Wiley.
therapy with children and adolescents. New York: Clarke, G., Debar, L., Lynch, F., Powell, J., Gale, J.,
Wiley. O’connor, E., … Hertert, S. (2005). A randomized
Bickman, L. (2008). A measurement feedback system is effectiveness trial of brief cognitive-behavioral ­therapy
necessary to improve mental health outcomes. Journal for depressed adolescents receiving antidepressant
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent medication. Journal of the American Academy of
Psychiatry, 47, 1114–1119. Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(9), 888–898.
Boyer, B. E., Doove, L. L., Geurts, H. M., Prins, P. J., Van Cohen, J. A., Deblinger, E., Mannarino, A. P., & Steer,
Mechelen, I., & Van der Oord, S. (2016). Qualitative R. A. (2004). A multisite, randomized controlled trial
treatment-subgroup interactions in a randomized clini- for children with sexual abuse–related PTSD symp-
cal trial of treatments for adolescents with ADHD: toms. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Exploring what cognitive-behavioral treatment works Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(4), 393–402.
for whom. PLoS One, 11(3), e0150698. Cohen, J. A., Mannarino, A. P., Kliethermes, M., &
Brent, D., Emslie, G., Clarke, G., Wagner, K. D., Asarnow, Murray, L. A. (2012). Trauma-focused CBT for youth
J. R., Keller, M., … Birmaher, B. (2008). Switching with complex trauma. Child Abuse & Neglect, 36(6),
to another SSRI or to venlafaxine with or without 528–541.
cognitive behavioral therapy for adolescents with Cohen, J. A., Mannarino, A. P., Perel, J. M., & Staron,
SSRI-resistant depression: The TORDIA random- V. (2007). A pilot randomized controlled trial of
ized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical combined trauma-focused CBT and sertraline for
Association, 299(8), 901–913. childhood PTSD symptoms. Journal of the American
Chorpita, B. F., & Daleiden, E. L. (2014). Structuring Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(7),
the collaboration of science and service in pursuit 811–819.
of a shared vision. Journal of Clinical Child and Crawley, S. A., Kendall, P. C., Benjamin, C. L., Brodman,
Adolescent Psychology, 43, 323–338. D. L., Wei, C., Beidas, R. S., … Mauro, C. (2013).
Chorpita, B. F., Bernstein, A., & Daleiden, E. L. (2008). Brief cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxious youth:
Driving with roadmaps and dashboards: Using informa- Feasibility and initial outcomes. Cognitive and
tion resources to structure the decision model in service Behavioral Practice, 20, 123–133.
organizations. Administration and Policy in Mental and Crawley, S. A., Podell, J. L., Beidas, R. S., Braswell, L.,
Mental Health Services Research, 35, 114–123. & Kendall, P. C. (2010). Cognitive-behavioral ther-
Chorpita, B. F., & Daleiden, E. L. (2009). Mapping apy with youth. In K. S. Dobson (Ed.), Handbook of
evidence-­ based treatments for children and adoles- cognitive-behavioral therapies (3rd ed., pp. 376–410).
cents: Applications of the distillation and matching New York, NY: Guilford Press.
model to 615 treatment from 322 randomized trials. D’Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. R. (1971). Problem solv-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, ing and behavior modification. Journal of Abnormal
566–577. Psychology, 78, 107–126.
Chorpita, B. F., Daleiden, E. L., & Bernstein, A. (2016). David-Ferdon, C., & Kaslow, N. J. (2008). Evidence-­
At the intersection of health information technol- based psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent
ogy, and decision support: Measurement feedback depression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
systems… and beyond. Administration and Policy Psychology, 37(1), 62–104.
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 57

DiGuiseppe, R. A. (1989). Cognitive therapy with order treatment study II: Rationale, design and meth-
children. In A. Freeman & F. M. Dattilio (Eds.), ods. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental
Comprehensive casebook of cognitive therapy Health, 3(1), 4–19.
(pp. 515–533). New York, NY: Springer. Friedberg, R. D. (2015). Where’s the beef?: Concrete ele-
Dimidjian, S., Barrera, M., Martell, C., Munoz, R. F., ments when supervising cognitive-behavioral therapy
& Lewinsohn, P. (2011). The origins and status of with youth. Journal of the American Academy of Child
behavioral activation treatment for depression. Annual and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 537–541.
Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 1–38. Friedberg, R. D., & Brelsford, G. M. (2011). Core prin-
Dobson, K. S., & Dozois, D. J. A. (2010). Historical and ciples in cognitive therapy with youth. Child and
philosophical bases of the cognitive-behavioral thera- Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20,
pies. In K. S. Dobson (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive 369–378.
behavior therapy (pp. 3–38). New York, NY: Guilford Friedberg, R. D., Friedberg, B. A., & Friedberg, R. J.
Press. (2001). Therapeutic exercises for children. Sarasota,
Domino, M. E., Foster, E. M., Vitiello, B., Kratochvil, FL: Professional Resource Press.
C. J., Burns, B. J., Silva, S. G., … March, J. S. (2009). Friedberg, R. D., Gorman, A. A., Wilt, L. H., Biuckians,
Relative cost-effectiveness of treatments for adoles- A., & Murray, M. (2011). Cognitive behavioral
cent depression: 36-week results from the TADS ran- therapy for the busy child psychiatrist and other
domized trial. Journal of the American Academy of mental health professionals: Rubrics and rudiments.
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(7), 711–720. New York, NY: Routledge.
Dorsey, S., Briggs, E. C., & Woods, B. A. (2011). Friedberg, R. D., & McClure, J. M. (2015). Clinical prac-
Cognitive-behavioral treatment for posttraumatic tice of cognitive therapy with children and adoles-
stress disorder in children and adolescents. Child and cents: The nuts and bolts (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, Guilford Press.
20(2), 255–269. Friedberg, R. D., McClure, J. M., & Garcia, J. H. (2009).
Dorsey, S., McLaughlin, K. A., Kerns, S. E., Harrison, J. P., Cognitive therapy techniques for children and adoles-
Lambert, H. K., Briggs, E. C., … Amaya-­Jackson, L. cents. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
(2016). Evidence base update for psychosocial treat- Froelich, J., Doepfner, M., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2002). Effects
ments for children and adolescents exposed to trau- of combined cognitive behavioural treatment with par-
matic events. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent ent management training in ADHD. Behavioural and
Psychology, 1–28. Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30(01), 111–115.
Dorsey, S., Pullmann, M. D., Berliner, L., Koschmann, E., Gondek, D., Edbrooke-Childs, J., Fink, E., Deighton,
McKay, M., & Deblinger, E. (2014). Engaging foster J., & Wolpert, M. (2016). Feedback from outcome
parents in treatment: A randomized trial of supple- measures and treatment effectiveness, treatment
menting trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy efficiency, and collaborative practice: A systematic
with evidence-based engagement strategies. Child review. Administration and Policy in Mental Health
Abuse & Neglect, 38(9), 1508–1520. and Mental Health Services Research, 43, 325–343.
Edgington, L., Hill, V., & Pellicano, E. (2016). The Gonzalez, J. E., Nelson, J. R., Gutkin, T. B., Saunders,
design and implementation of a CBT-based interven- A., Galloway, A., & Shwery, C. S. (2004). Rational-­
tion for sensory processing difficulties in adolescents emotive therapy with children and adolescents: A
on the autism spectrum. Research in Developmental meta-analysis. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
Disabilities, 59, 221–233. Disorders, 12, 222–235.
Eiraldi, R., Power, T. J., Schwartz, B. S., Keiffer, Henggeler, S. W., Schoenwald, S. K., Borduin, C. M.,
J. N., McCurdy, B. L., Mathen, M., & Jawad, A. F. Rowland, M. D., & Cunningham, P. B. (2009).
(2016). Examining effectiveness of group cognitive-­ Multisystemic therapy for antisocial behavior in chil-
behavioral therapy for externalizing and internalizing dren and adolescents. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
disorders in urban schools. Behavior Modification, Higa-McMillan, C. K., Francis, S. E., Rith-Najarian, L.,
40(4), 611–639. & Chorpita, B. F. (2016). Evidence base update: 50
Eyberg, S. M., Nelson, M. M., & Boggs, S. R. (2008). years of research on treatment for child and adoles-
Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children cent anxiety. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
and adolescents with disruptive behavior. Journal Psychology, 45(2), 91–113.
of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37(1), Hogue, A., Dauber, S., Henderson, C. E., Bobek, M.,
215–237. Johnson, C., Lichvar, E., & Morgenstern, J. (2015).
Franklin, M. E., Sapyta, J., Freeman, J. B., Khanna, M., Randomized trial of family therapy versus nonfam-
Compton, S., Almirall, D., … Foa, E. B. (2011). ily treatment for adolescent behavior problems in
Cognitive behavior therapy augmentation of pharma- usual care. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
cotherapy in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder: Psychology, 44(6), 954–969.
The Pediatric OCD Treatment Study II (POTS II) ran- Ingram, R. E., & Kendall, P. C. (1986). Cognitive clinical
domized controlled trial. JAMA, 306(11), 1224–1232. psychology: Implications of an information processing
Freeman, J. B., Choate-Summers, M. L., Garcia, A. M., perspective. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.), Information pro-
Moore, P. S., Sapyta, J. J., Khanna, M. S., … Franklin, cessing approaches to clinical psychology (pp. 3–21).
M. E. (2009). The pediatric obsessive-compulsive dis- Orlando, FL: Academic.
58 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

Jaycox, L. H., Cohen, J. A., Mannarino, A. P., Walker, Kendall, P. C., & Finch, A. J. (1976). A cognitive-­
D. W., Langley, A. K., Gegenheimer, K. L., … behavioral treatment for impulsive control: A con-
Schonlau, M. (2010). Children’s mental health care trolled study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
following Hurricane Katrina: A field trial of trauma-­ Psychology, 44, 852–857.
focused psychotherapies. Journal of Traumatic Stress, Kendall, P. C., & Finch, A. J. (1978). A cognitive behav-
23(2), 223–231. ioral treatment for impulsivity: A group comparison
Jensen-Doss, A. (2015). Practical evidence-based clini- study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
cal decision-making: Introduction to the special issue. 46, 110–118.
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 22, 1–4. Kendall, P. C., Flannery-Schroeder, E., Panichelli-Mindel,
Jolly, J. B. (1993). A multi-method test of the cognitive S. M., Southam-Gerow, M., Henin, A., & Warman,
content-specificity hypothesis in young adolescents. M. (1997). Therapy for youths with anxiety disor-
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 7, 223–233. ders: A second randomized clinical trial. Journal of
Jolly, J. B., & Dyckman, R. A. (1994). Using self-report Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(3), 366–380.
data to differentiate anxious and depressive symptoms Kendall, P. C., Furr, J. M., & Podell, J. L. (2010). Child-­
in adolescents’ cognitive content-specificity and global focused treatment of anxiety. In J. R. Weisz & A. E.
distress. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 25–37. Kazdin (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies
Jolly, J. B., & Kramer, T. A. (1994). The hierarchical for children and adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 45–60).
arrangement of internalizing cognitions. Cognitive New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Therapy and Research, 8, 1–14. Kendall, P. C., Gosch, E., Furr, J., & Sood, E. (2008).
Joshi, G., Petty, C., Wozniak, J., Henin, A., Fried, R., Flexibility within fidelity. Journal of the American
Galdo, M., … Biederman, J. (2010). The heavy bur- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47,
den of psychiatric comorbidity in youth with autism 987–993.
spectrum disorders: A large comparative study of a Kendall, P. C., & Hedtke, K. A. (2006). Coping cat work-
psychiatrically referred population. Journal of Autism book. Ardmore, PA: Workbook Publishing.
and Developmental Disorders, 40(11), 1361–1370. Kendall, P. C., Hudson, J. L., Gosch, E., Flannery-Schroeder,
Kanter, J. W., Rusch, L. C., Busch, A. M., & Sedivy, S. E., & Suveg, C. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy
K. (2009). Validation of the Behavioral Activation for anxiety disordered youth: A randomized clinical
for Depression Scale (BADS) in a community sam- trial evaluating child and family modalities. Journal
ple with elevated depressive symptoms. Journal of of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(2), 282–297.
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-008-9088-y
36–42. Kendall, P. C., & Peterman, J. S. (2015). CBT for ado-
Kataoka, S., Jaycox, L. H., Wong, M., Nadeem, E., lescents with anxiety: Mature yet still developing.
Langley, A., Tang, L., & Stein, B. D. (2011). Effects American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 519–530.
on school outcomes in low-income minority youth: Kendall, P. C., Robin, J. A., Hedtke, K. A., Suveg,
Preliminary findings from a community-partnered C., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Gosch, E. (2006).
study of a school trauma intervention. Ethnicity & Considering CBT with anxious youth? Think expo-
Disease, 21(3 0 1), S1-71–S1-77. sures. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 12(1),
Kendall, P. C. (1994). Treating anxiety disorders in chil- 136–148.
dren: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal Kendall, P. C., & Wilcox, L. E. (1980). A cognitive
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 100–110. behavioral treatment for impulsivity: Concrete versus
Kendall, P. C. (2012). Anxiety disorders in youth. In P. C. conceptual training with non-self-controlled prob-
Kendall (Ed.), Child and adolescent therapy (4th ed., lem children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
pp. 143–187). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Psychology, 48, 80–91.
Kendall, P. C., & Beidas, R. S. (2008). Smoothing the Kennard, B. D., Emslie, G. J., Mayes, T. L., Nightingale-­
trail for dissemination of evidence-based practices Teresi, J., Nakonezny, P. A., Hughes, J. L., … Jarrett,
for youth: Flexibility within fidelity. Professional R. B. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy to prevent
Psychology: Research and Practice, 38, 13–20. relapse in pediatric responders to pharmacotherapy for
Kendall, P. C., & Braswell, L. (1985). Cognitive behav- major depressive disorder. Journal of the American
ioral therapy for impulsive children. New York, NY: Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(12),
Guilford Press. 1395–1404.
Kendall, P. C., Chansky, T. E., Kane, M. T., Kim, R. S., Kovacs, M. (2010). The Children’s depression inven-
Kortlander, E., & Ronan, K. R. (1992). Anxiety dis- tory–2 manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health
orders in youth: Cognitive-behavioral interventions. Systems.
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Krumholz, L. S., Ugueto, A. M., Santucci, L. C., & Weisz,
Kendall, P. C., Cummings, C. M., Villabø, M. A., Narayanan, J. R. (2014). Social skills training. In E. S. Sburlati,
M. K., Treadwell, K., Birmaher, B., … Gosch, E. (2016). H. J. Lyneham, C. A. Schiering, & R. M. Rapee (Eds.),
Mediators of change in the Child/Adolescent Anxiety Evidence-based CBT for anxiety and depression in
Multimodal Treatment Study. Journal of Consulting and children (pp. 260–274). New York, NY: Wiley.
Clinical Psychology, 84(1), 1–14.
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 59

Laugeson, E. A., & Frankel, F. (2011). Social skills for of cognitive-behavioral therapy, sertraline, and their
teenagers with developmental and autism spectrum combination for adolescent depression. Journal of the
disorders: The PEERS treatment manual. New York, American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
NY: Routledge. 45(10), 1151–1161.
Laugeson, E. A., Frankel, F., Gantman, A., Dillon, A. R., Mendlowitz, S. L. (2014). Changing maladaptive
& Mogil, C. (2012). Evidence-based social skills behaviors, part 2: The use of the behavioral activa-
for adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: tion and pleasant event scheduling with depressed
The UCLA PEERS program. Journal of Autism and children and adolescents. In E. S. Sburlati, H. J.
Developmental Disorders, 39, 596–606. Lyneham, C. A. Schniering, & R. M. Rapee (Eds.),
Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2002). The Coping Power Evidence-­ based CBT for anxiety and depression
program at the middle-school transition: Universal and in adolescence (pp. 208–224). West Sussex, UK:
indicated prevention effects. Psychology of Addictive Wiley.
Behaviors, 16(4S), S40–S54. Morsette, A., Swaney, G., Stolle, D., Schuldberg, D., van
Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2003). Effectiveness of den Pol, R., & Young, M. (2009). Cognitive behavioral
the Coping Power Program and of classroom interven- intervention for trauma in schools (CBITS): School-­
tion with aggressive children: Outcomes at a 1-year based treatment on a rural American Indian reserva-
follow-up. Behavior Therapy, 34(4), 493–515. tion. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
MacPherson, H. A., Weinstein, S. M., Henry, D. B., & Psychiatry, 40(1), 169–178.
West, A. E. (2016). Mediators in the randomized trial Nangle, D. W., Hansen, D. J., Grover, R. L., Kingery, J. N.,
of child-and family-focused cognitive-behavioral ther- & Suveg, C. (2016). Treating internalizing disorders in
apy for pediatric bipolar disorder. Behaviour Research children and adolescents: core techniques and strategies.
and Therapy, 85, 60–71. New York: Guilford.
March, J. S. (2009). The future of psychotherapy for men- Neill, E. L., Weems, C. F., & Scheeringa, M. S. (2016). CBT
tally ill children and adolescents. Journal of Child for child PTSD is associated with reductions in mater-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1–2), 170–179. nal depression: Evidence for bidirectional effects.
March, J. S., & Vitiello, B. (2009). Clinical messages from Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology.
the treatment for adolescents with depression study [Advanced On-Line Publication, https://doi.org/10.10
(TADS). American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(10), 80/15374416.2016.12123591].
1118–1123. Nixon, R. D. V., Sterk, J., & Pearce, A. (2012). A ran-
Masters, J. C., Burish, T. G., Hollon, S. D., & Rimm, D. C. domized trial of cognitive behaviour therapy and cog-
(1987). Behavior therapy: Technique and empirical nitive therapy for children with posttraumatic stress
findings. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. disorder following single-incident trauma. Journal of
Matson, J. L., Matson, M. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2007). Social Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(3), 327–337.
skills treatments for children with autism spectrum Ollendick, T. H., Halldorsdottir, T., Fraire, M. G., Austin,
disorders. Behavior Modification, 31, 682–707. K. E., Noguchi, R. J., Lewis, K. M., … Whitmore,
McCart, M. R., & Sheidow, A. J. (2016). Evidence-based M. J. (2015). Specific phobias in youth: A randomized
psychosocial treatments for adolescents with disrup- controlled trial comparing one-session treatment to
tive behavior. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent a parent-augmented one-session treatment. Behavior
Psychology, 45(5), 529–563. Therapy, 46(2), 141–155.
McCauley, E., Scholredt, K., Gudmudsen, G., Martell, Ong, S. H., & Caron, A. (2008). Family-based psycho-
C., & Dimidjian, S. (2011). Expanding behavioral education for children and adolescents with mood
activation to depressed adolescents: Lessons learned disorders. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 17,
in treatment development. Cognitive and Behavioral 809–822.
Practice, 18, 371–383. Öst, L. G. (1997). Rapid treatment of specific phobias. In
McLachlan, N. H., Eastwood, L., & Friedberg, G. C. L. Davey (Ed.), Phobias: A handbook of theory,
R. D. (2016). Socratic questions with children: research, and treatment (pp. 227–247). Chichester,
Recommendations and cautionary tales. Journal of UK: Wiley.
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 105–119. Öst, L. G., Riise, E. N., Wergeland, G. J., Hansen, B., &
McLeod, B. M., Jensen-Doss, A., & Ollendick, T. H. Kvale, G. (2016). Cognitive behavioral and pharma-
(2013). Diagnostic and behavioral assessment in chil- cological treatments of OCD in children: A system-
dren and adolescents: A clinical guide. New York, NY: atic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety
Guilford Press. Disorders, 43, 58–69.
Meichenbaum, D. H. (1985). Stress inoculation training. Overholser, J. (2010). Psychotherapy according to the
New York, NY: Pergamon. Socratic method: Integrating ancient philosophy with
Meichenbaum, D. H., & Goodman, J. (1971). Training contemporary cognitive therapy. Journal of Cognitive
impulsive children to talk to themselves. Journal of Psychotherapy, Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 24,
Abnormal Psychology, 77, 113–126. 354–363.
Melvin, G. A., Tonge, B. J., King, N. J., Heyne, D., Padesky, C. A. (1993, September). Socratic questioning:
Gordon, M. S., & Klimkeit, E. (2006). A comparison Changing minds or guided discovery. Keynote address
60 R.D. Friedberg and M.A. Thordarson

at the meeting of the European Congress of Behavioral S. W. White, & T. Attwood (Eds.), CBT for children
and Cognitive Therapies, London, UK. and adolescents with high functioning autism spec-
Pavuluri, M. N., Birmaher, B., & Naylor, M. W. (2005). trum disorders (pp. 3–26). New York: Guilford Press.
Pediatric bipolar disorder: A review of the past 10 Scheeringa, M. S., Weems, C. F., Cohen, J. A., Amaya-­
years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Jackson, L., & Guthrie, D. (2011). Trauma-focused
Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(9), 846–871. cognitive-behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress
Pavuluri, M. N., Graczyk, P. A., Henry, D. B., Carbray, disorder in three-through six year-old children: A ran-
J. A., Heidenreich, J., & Miklowitz, D. J. (2004). domized clinical trial. Journal of Child Psychology
Child-and family-focused cognitive-behavioral ther- and Psychiatry, 52(8), 853–860.
apy for pediatric bipolar disorder: Development and Scott, K., & Lewis, C. C. (2015). Using measurement-­
preliminary results. Journal of the American Academy based care to enhance any treatment. Cognitive and
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(5), 528–537. Behavioral Practice, 22, 49–59.
Pavuluri, M. N., Naylor, M. W., & Janicak, P. G. (2002). Seligman, L. D., & Ollendick, T. H. (2011). Cognitive-­
Recognition and treatment of pediatric bipolar disor- behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders in youth.
der. Contemporary Psychiatry, 1(1), 1–10. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
Pediatric OCD Treatment Study (POTS) Team. (2004). America, 20(2), 217–238.
Cognitive-behavior therapy, sertraline, and their com- Shin, S. K. (2009). Effects of a solution-focused program
bination for children and adolescents with obsessive-­ on the reduction of aggressiveness and the improvement
compulsive disorder: The Pediatric OCD Treatment of social readjustment for Korean youth probationers.
Study (POTS) randomized controlled trial. Journal Journal of Social Service Research, 35(3), 274–284.
of the American Medical Association, 292(16), Shortt, A. L., Barrett, P. M., & Fox, T. L. (2001).
1969–1976. Evaluating the FRIENDS program: A cognitive-­
Pereira, A. I., Muris, P., Mendonça, D., Barros, L., Goes, behavioral group treatment for anxious children and
A. R., & Marques, T. (2016). Parental involvement their parents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
in cognitive-behavioral intervention for anxious chil- 30(4), 525–535.
dren: Parents’ in-session and out-session activities Sofronoff, K., Attwood, T., & Hinton, S. (2005). A ran-
and their relationship with treatment outcome. Child domised controlled trial of a CBT intervention for anx-
Psychiatry and Human Development, 47(1), 113–123. iety in children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of
Peterman, J. S., Carper, M. M., Elkins, R. M., Comer, Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(11), 1152–1160.
J. S., Pincus, D. B., & Kendall, P. C. (2016). The Spence, S. H. (2003). Social skills training with children
effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy for youth anxi- and young people: Theory, evidence and practice.
ety on sleep problems. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 8(2), 84–96.
37, 78–88. Stallard, P. (2002). Think good, feel good: A cognitive
Peterman, J. B., Read, K. L., Wei, C., & Kendall, P. C. behavior workbook for children and young people.
(2015). The art of exposure: Putting science into Chichester, UK: Wiley.
practice. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 22(3), Stein, B. D., Jaycox, L. H., Kataoka, S. H., Wong, M., Tu,
379–392. W., Elliott, M. N., & Fink, A. (2003). A mental health
Piacentini, J., & Bergman, L. (2001). Developmental intervention for schoolchildren exposed to violence: A
issues in cognitive therapy for childhood anxiety randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American
disorders. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 15, Medical Association, 290(5), 603–611.
165–182. TADS Team, March, J., Silva, S., Petrycki, S., Curry, J.,
Piacentini, J., Bergman, R. L., Jacobs, C., McCracken, Wells, K., … Severe, J. (2004). Fluoxetine, cognitive-­
J. T., & Kretchman, J. (2002). Open trial of cogni- behavioral therapy, and their combination for adoles-
tive behavior therapy for childhood obsessive–com- cents with depression: Treatment for Adolescents With
pulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16(2), Depression Study (TADS) randomized controlled
207–219. trial. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Rouf, K., Fennell, M., Westbrook, D., Cooper, M., & 292(7), 807–820.
Bennett-Levy, J. (2004). Devising effective experi- Tolin, D. T. (2016). Doing CBT. New York, NY: Guilford
ments. In J. Bennett-Levy, G. Butler, M. Fennell, Press.
A. Hackmann, M. Mueller, & D. Westbrook (Eds.), Torp, N. C., & Skarphedinsson, G. (2017). Early respond-
Oxford guide to behavioral experiments in cognitive ers and remitters to exposure-based CBT for pediatric
therapy (pp. 21–58). Oxford, UK: Oxford University OCD. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related
Press. Disorders, 12, 71–77.
Scaini, S., Belotti, R., Ogliari, A., & Battaglia, M. Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study
(2016). A comprehensive meta-analysis of cognitive-­ (TADS) Team. (2009). The treatment for adolescents
behavioral interventions for social anxiety disor- with depression study (TADS): Outcomes over 1
der in children and adolescents. Journal of Anxiety year of naturalistic follow-up. American Journal of
Disorders, 42, 105–112. Psychiatry, 166(10), 1141–1149.
Scarpa, A., & Lorenzi, J. (2013). Cognitive-behavioral Vitiello, B., Brent, D. A., Greenhill, L. L., Emslie,
therapy with children and adolescents. In A. Scarpa, G., Wells, K., Walkup, J. T., … Coffey, B. (2009).
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 61

Depressive symptoms and clinical status during the Wells, K. C., & Forehand, R. (1981). Childhood
treatment of adolescent suicide attempters (TASA) behavior problems in the home. In S. M. Turner, K. S.
study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Calhoun, & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of clini-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(10), 997–1004. cal behavior therapy (pp. 527–567). New York, NY:
Walkup, J. T., Albano, A. M., Piacentini, J., Birmaher, B., Wiley.
Compton, S. N., Sherrill, J. T., … Iyengar, S. (2008). White, S. W., Albano, A. M., Johnson, C. R., Kasari,
Cognitive behavioral therapy, sertraline, or a combi- C., Ollendick, T., Klin, A., … Scahill, L. (2010).
nation in childhood anxiety. New England Journal of Development of a cognitive-behavioral intervention
Medicine, 359(26), 2753–2766. program to treat anxiety and social deficits in teens
Weersing, V. R., Jeffreys, M., Do, M. C. T., Schwartz, with high-functioning autism. Clinical Child and
K. T., & Bolano, C. (2016). Evidence base update Family Psychology Review, 13(1), 77–90.
of psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent White, S. W., Ollendick, T., Albano, A. M., Oswald, D.,
depression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Johnson, C., Southam-Gerow, M. A., … Scahill, L.
Psychology, 1–33. (2013). Randomized controlled trial: Multimodal
Weisz, J. R., & Chorpita, B. F. (2012). “Mod squad” for anxiety and social skill intervention for adolescents
youth psychotherapy: Restructuring evidence-based with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
treatment for clinical practice. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Developmental Disorders, 43(2), 382–394.
Child and adolescent therapy: Cognitive-behavioral pro- Wolters, L. H., de Haan, E., Hogendoorn, S. M., Boer, F.,
cedures (pp. 379–397). New York, NY: Guilford Press. & Prins, P. J. (2016). Severe pediatric obsessive com-
Weisz, J. R., Krumholz, L. S., Santucci, L., Thomassin, pulsive disorder and co-morbid autistic symptoms:
K., & Ng, M. Y. (2015). Shrinking the gap between Effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy. Journal
research and practice: Tailoring and testing youth psy- of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 10,
chotherapies in clinical care contexts. Annual Review 69–77.
of Clinical Psychology, 11, 39–63. Wu, Y., Lang, Z., & Zhang, H. (2016). Efficacy of
Weisz, J. R., McCarty, C. A., & Valeri, S. M. (2006). ­cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediatric obsessive-­
Effects of psychotherapy for depression in children compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Medical
and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Psychological Science Monitor: International Medical Journal of
Bulletin, 132(1), 132–149. Experimental and Clinical Research, 22, 1646–1653.
Parent Training Interventions

Nicholas Long, Mark C. Edwards,
and Jayne Bellando

Contents Over the past several decades parent training has


Example of a Hanf-Model Program: Helping become an increasingly popular intervention to
the Noncompliant Child (HNC)   64 address children’s behavior problems. While the
Widely Disseminated Parent Training broader dissemination of these programs is rela-
Programs   66 tively new, the origin of evidence-based parent
An Overview of the Empirical Support for training interventions dates back to the 1960s
Parent Training   67 (Kotchick, Shaffer, Dorsey, & Forehand, 2004). At
Immediate and Follow-Up Effects of Parent
that time, a paradigm shift was starting to occur in
Training   67 regard to the delivery of child therapy services.
Rather than therapists only working directly with
Parent Training and Autism Spectrum
Disorder (ASD)   76 the child, there was growing interest in therapists
training parents in the use of specific behavioral
Current and Anticipated Trends in Parent
Training   78 strategies that they could, in turn, use to change
their child’s behavior. That shift was based, in
Conclusion   82
large part, on early findings regarding the effec-
References   82 tiveness of behavior modification techniques in
changing child behavior (e.g., Williams, 1959)
and, subsequently, evidence that parents could
be taught to use such techniques effectively with
their own children (Hawkins, Peterson, Schweid,
& Bijou, 1966; Wahler, Winkel, Peterson, &
Morrison, 1965). This trend led to the develop-
ment of various parent training interventions that
utilized the triadic model (Tharp & Wetzel, 1969)
in the delivery of services. This model involves the
use of a therapist (consultant) to teach the parent
(mediator) to reduce a child’s (target) disruptive
N. Long (*) • M.C. Edwards • J. Bellando behavior (McMahon & Forehand, 2003).
Department of Pediatrics, University of Arkansas for Historically, the primary focus of most parent
Medical Sciences and Arkansas Children’s Hospital,
Little Rock, AR, USA training interventions has been to reduce
e-mail: longnicholas@uams.edu children’s disruptive behavior. This focus was
­

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 63


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_5
64 N. Long et al.

initially influenced heavily by Patterson (1982)  xample of a Hanf-Model Program:


E
and his research regarding the role of coercive Helping the Noncompliant Child (HNC)
parent-­child interactions in the development and
escalation of children’s disruptive behavior. In order to provide a more thorough understand-
Patterson’s model of reciprocal influences helped ing of the types of teaching strategies utilized and
explain how children’s disruptive behavior can skills taught in the Hanf-Model, what follows is
escalate while parent management tactics become an overview of the Helping the Noncompliant
more punitive and coercive (e.g., nagging, yell- Child (HNC) program (Forehand & McMahon,
ing). Ineffective parenting, especially in regard to 1981; McMahon & Forehand, 2003). Of the cur-
child compliance to parental directions during rent Hanf-Model parent training programs, HNC
the preschool years, was considered a primary is the most similar to Hanf’s original program.
catalyst for the development of these coercive HNC targets young children (3–8 years old) who
parent-child interactions (McMahon & Wells, exhibit high levels of noncompliance to parental
1998). The identification of specific aspects of directions. This clinic-based program involves a
parent-child interactions that were related to the therapist working with individual families. The
development and escalation of children’s disrup- child attends all sessions with their parent(s). The
tive behavior (e.g., reinforcing disruptive behav- primary goals of the program are to improve child
ior, the use of ineffective parental directions, and compliance to directions and to decrease disrup-
the failure to adequately reinforce appropriate tive behavior through teaching parents more
behavior) allowed these specific behaviors to be appropriate ways of interacting with their child.
targeted by behavioral parenting training The intervention consists of two major phases.
interventions. During Phase 1, differential attention skills are
In addition to the contributions of Gerald taught to improve the parent-child relationship as
Patterson, Constance Hanf (Reitman & McMahon, well as increase desirable behaviors. Phase 2
2013) also had a profound impact on the early involves compliance training skills that assist
development of behavioral parent training pro- parents in dealing with noncompliance and other
grams. She developed an innovative, two-­ stage problematic behavior. The instructional format
parent training model in the late 1960s (Hanf & for each session follows a standard process which
King, 1973) that targeted young children’s disrup- includes didactic instruction and discussion of a
tive behavior and included extensive modeling, specific skill, the therapist demonstrating the
role-playing, and parental practice of skills with skill through modeling and role-playing, the par-
therapist feedback. The primary skills taught to ent practicing the skill with the therapist, the skill
parents in her program included attending, reward- being introduced to the child, the parent subse-
ing, ignoring, giving clear instructions, and time- quently practicing the skill with the child while
out. Several of Hanf’s former trainees and the therapist provides cues/feedback, and finally
colleagues made different modifications to her a homework assignment given to allow the par-
program and have spent the past 40+ years study- ent to practice/utilize the skill at home.
ing their own “Hanf-Model” programs. These pro- Skills addressed in the program include attend-
grams have become some of best known and ing, rewarding, ignoring, giving directions, and
validated behavioral parent training programs in time-out. Phase 1 of the program involves teach-
use today. The “Hanf-Model” parent training pro- ing parents the effective use of the skills of attend-
grams include Sheila Eyberg’s Parent-Child ing, rewarding, and ignoring. Phase 2 involves
Interaction Therapy (PCIT), Rex Forehand and teaching parents to give effective d­ irections and
Robert McMahon’s Helping the Noncompliant how to use time-out appropriately. The clinical
Child (HNC), Carolyn Webster-­ Stratton’s program typically takes 8–12 sessions to com-
Incredible Years (IY), Russell Barkley’s Defiant plete. The number of sessions varies from family
Children (DC), and Charles Cunningham’s to family because HNC uses a competency-­based
Community Parent Education (COPE) Program. approach which requires parents to achieve a
Parent Training Interventions 65

certain level of competence with a skill before Differential Attention Plans  After the parent has
the next skill is introduced. Details regarding the mastered the skills of attending, rewarding, and
specific skills are provided below. ignoring, the therapist assists the parent in targeting
specific child behaviors to increase using differ-
ential attention. Parents use the skills taught in
Phase 1 (Differential Attention Skills) Phase 1 to implement differential attention plans
with guidance provided by the therapist.
Attending  Attending is a skill that parents can
use to help increase their child’s desirable behav-
iors. It also helps lay the groundwork for a more Phase 2 (Compliance Training Skills)
positive parent-child relationship. After discuss-
ing, modeling, and role-playing the skill with the The second phase of the program consists of teach-
parent(s), the therapist helps the parent master the ing parents two primary components of disciplinary
skill through practicing it in what is called the skills: how to give effective instructions to the child
“child’s game.” This is a time where the child and how to use a time-out procedure appropriately.
selects the play activity (e.g., playing with blocks)
and the parent is nondirective. The parent is taught Giving Effective Instructions  Parents are taught the
to simply describe a child’s activity while elimi- elements of giving effective instructions/commands
nating directions and questions addressed to the to their child. The parent practices giving instruc-
child. This practice allows the parent to master the tions to their child within the “parent’s game.”
skill of attending that will later be used to increase Unlike the “child’s game” which is used to teach
desirable behavior. This skill is the focus of the Phase 1 skills and involves the parent being nondi-
intervention until the parent demonstrates compe- rective, the “parent’s game” involves the parent tak-
tence. This competence is assessed using specific ing direction of the activities (e.g., the parent issues
behavioral criteria recorded during a structured frequent instructions/commands while directing the
observation. activity). The therapist provides feedback to the
parent regarding the directions being issued (e.g.,
Rewarding  The second skill involves teaching how they could be improved). The parent is also
the parent to praise or reward the child’s positive taught to attend to or praise their child’s compliance
behavior. This skill is taught using the same to their directions.
instructional procedures and is practiced using
the “child’s game.” The types of rewards that are Time-Out  Parents are taught a specific time-out
taught consist of labeled verbal (e.g., “I really procedure to use with their child. The child is also
like it when you pick up your toys!”) and physi- informed about the time-out protocol within the
cal (e.g., hug, pat) rewards. Parents are taught to session. The therapist provides guidance to the
focus on and reward prosocial behaviors rather parent regarding various issues related to time-
than negative behaviors. The parent has to dem- out. The therapist then helps the parent utilize a
onstrate competence before the next skill is clear instruction sequence that guides the parent
introduced. in how to consistently manage compliance and
noncompliance to parental directions.
Ignoring  The third component of the initial
phase of the program involves teaching a parent Standing Rules  Once the parent is effectively
to ignore minor unacceptable behavior, such as implementing the clear instruction sequence at
whining and fussing. Again, the standardized home, the use of standing rules is introduced.
instructional procedures are used. The parent is Standing rules are typically “If…then…” state-
taught an ignoring procedure that involves no ments (i.e., rules that specify the consequences for
eye, physical, or verbal contact when minor specific behavior). The therapist assists the parents
unacceptable behaviors occur. in developing appropriate standing rules.
66 N. Long et al.

Extending the Skills  The therapist discusses with & Reid, 2003). The goals of the parent training
the parent how they can use the skills they have been component are to promote parent competencies
taught to manage their child’s behavior outside of and strengthen families. This is a videotape mod-
the home and also to address other behaviors that eling and group discussion program. The BASIC
have not been directly targeted during the course parenting training program takes 26 h to com-
of the program. The goal is to teach the parent a plete (13 weekly 2 h group sessions). The video-
series of skills that they can utilize over time to tapes used in the program contain 250 short
address various behavioral issues that may arise vignettes (1–2 min each) of modeled parenting
in the future. skills. The vignettes are show to groups of 8–12
parents with a therapist leading group discussion.
The program focuses on teaching parents how to
 idely Disseminated Parent
W enhance the parent-child relationship through the
Training Programs use of child-directed interactive play, the use of
praise, and the use of incentives. The program
Parent-Child Interaction Therapy also teaches parenting techniques such as moni-
(PCIT) toring, ignoring, use of effective directions, time-
out, and natural and logical consequences.
PCIT (Brinkmeyer & Eyberg, 2003; McNeil & Webster-­ Stratton has also developed the
Hembree-Kigin, 2010), as a Hanf-Model pro- ADVANCE parent training program (Webster-
gram, is similar in many ways to the Helping the Stratton & Reid, 2003). This is 14-session video-
Noncompliant Child (HNC) program. Both pro- tape-based program that can be used following
grams focus on young children with disruptive completion of the BASIC program. The
behavior, have two phases, and are delivered to ADVANCE program has four primary compo-
individual families by a therapist. The two nents: (1) personal self-­control, (2) communica-
phases in PCIT are (1) child-directed interaction tion skills, (3) problem solving skills, and (4)
and (2) parent-directed interaction. Training is strengthening social support and self-care.
provided through didactic instruction, modeling,
role-­playing, and coaching. In PCIT, children
attend most, but not all, of the sessions with their Triple P
parents. Only the parents attend a single teaching
session at the beginning of each phase. During Triple P (Positive Parenting Program), developed
these teaching sessions, the parents are taught all by Sanders (Sanders, Kirby, Tellegen, & Day,
of the skills for that phase (whereas in HNC the 2014; Sanders & Ralph, 2004), is a unique parent
skills are taught sequentially within each phase). training program. Developed in Australia and cur-
PCIT also emphasizes the role of traditional play rently being used around the world, Triple P is a
therapy as part of their child-directed interaction multilevel parent training program that targets
phase. There is extensive evidence supporting children 2–12 years old. The program has five
the effectiveness of PCIT (see Brinkmeyer & levels. Level 1 is a universal parent information
Eyberg, 2003). strategy that makes general parenting information
available to all parents through the use of various
strategies including tip-sheets and promotional
The Incredible Years (IY) media campaigns. Level 2 consists of a brief one-
or two-session primary healthcare-­based parent-
IY training series (Metinga, Orobio de Castro, & ing intervention targeting children with mild
Matthys, 2013; Webster-Stratton & Reid, 2003) behavior problems. Level 3 is a four-­ session,
is a comprehensive program that has intervention more intensive parenting intervention that targets
components for parents, teachers, and young children with mild to moderate behavior prob-
children (2–8 years old). IY is an extremely lems. Level 4 is an 8–10 session individual or
well-­evaluated program (see Webster-Stratton group parent training program targeting children
Parent Training Interventions 67

with more significant behavior problems. Level 5 ent perception and well-being. The strength of
is an enhanced behavioral family intervention the evidence supporting certain types of parent
program that is utilized for significant behavior training rivals that of other evidence-based treat-
problems that are complicated by other factors ments. However, parent training also shares in
(e.g., marital conflict, high stress). some of the disappointments and challenges
These behavioral parent training programs, as common within the broader child psychotherapy
well as the others that target externalizing behavior enterprise, such as problems retaining families in
problems, continue to be the most commonly used treatment, poor treatment engagement, and atten-
and evaluated parent training interventions. uation of treatment effects over time (Assemany
However, there are many other types of parent & McIntosh, 2002). While the outcomes that will
training programs that utilize different strategies be demonstrated are a testament to the viability
and target concerns other than disruptive behavior of training parents as a prevention or intervention
(e.g., internalizing behavior problems). Next, we modality, broadly speaking, the variability of
will provide an overview of the empirical support program orientations and formats, target popula-
for the various types of parent training programs. tions, and outcomes, as well as the variability of
the research quality across studies, requires us to
ask more specific questions of the literature, such
 n Overview of the Empirical
A as what works for whom.
Support for Parent Training In this section, we will examine the evidence
of the immediate and follow-up effects of parent
The empirical literature evaluating programs training treatments across populations (ADHD,
designed to train parents to intervene with their disruptive behavior, other problems) and review
children’s problems is extensive. To illustrate the types. In addition, we will examine the review
scope of studies, a review of parent training out- literature related to the generalization and moder-
comes with disruptive behaviors identified 430 ating effects of training parents to intervene with
studies published in peer-reviewed journals their children.
between 1974 and 2003 (Lundahl, Risser, &
Lovejoy, 2006). No less than 53 reviews evaluat-
ing from 4 to 186 studies and 2 review-of-reviews I mmediate and Follow-Up Effects
have been published from 1972 to 2015. These of Parent Training
review types varied, with 16 utilizing meta-­
analyses exclusively or in combination with other The breadth of studies under the treatment cate-
review strategies, 30 using the systematic/critical gory of parent training presents a challenge for
review methods, and 8 conducting strength of evi- evaluating and summarizing the evidence. Given
dence1 reviews. The reviews focused on various the variability of meaningful dimensions across
child populations, including ADHD (9), clinical studies (e.g., study design, target population,
disruptive behaviors (24), mixed clinical prob- treatment format, treatment length, outcome
lems (10), and mixed clinical and general popula- measurement), as well as the various methods of
tion (9). The breadth of the literature makes parent reviewing studies (e.g., systematic/critical, meta-­
training the most studied child psychosocial inter- analysis, strength of evidence), any general sum-
vention modality to date. mary across all studies allows only for evaluation
As will be demonstrated, parent training pro- of parent training as a therapeutic treatment
grams have shown generally positive outcomes modality. To provide some specification, we will
across child and parent behavior, as well as par- examine three different methods of summarizing
the literature across target populations, as well as
assess programs across theoretical orientations.
Strength of evidence: a type of review that uses a system
1 
These methods are (1) meta-analytic reviews, (2)
for assessing the quality of studies and overall strength of
evidence – often used in evidence-based practice strength of evidence reviews, and (3) systematic/
guidelines. critical reviews.
68 N. Long et al.

Meta-analytic Reviews magnitude of effect sizes (d): 0.2 = small,


0.5 = medium, 0.8 = large. Below we will review
Meta-analysis (MA) provides a quantitative the effect sizes reported in meta-analytic reviews
method for summarizing studies that address across target populations.
similar questions. By combining multiple stud-
ies, MA can overcome some of the limitations of Disruptive Behaviors
single studies, such as low sample size and biases Six reviews conducted meta-analyses of parent
related to different study design characteristics. training for children with disruptive behavior
MA facilitates interpretation of findings across problems. These reviews included 155 studies
heterogeneous studies by using a common metric across study design types, spanning 1966–2011,
for evaluating results – an effect size – which and with children from preschool through
represents how many standard deviations the 18 years of age. Three of the reviews reported
average person in the treatment group changed outcomes on behavioral parent training (BPT)
compared to the average person in a control or treatments with children preschool through
comparison group. For example, an effect size of 18 years of age. Three reviews included out-
0.50 indicates that the score of the average per- comes for parent training treatments, regardless
son in the treatment group exceeded 0.50 stan- of theoretical orientation (see Tables 1 and 2).
dard deviations or 69% of the people in the The reviews that reported on BPT treatments
control/comparison group. For our purposes, we found small to large effect sizes for child out-
will adopt Cohen’s (1988) classification of the comes based on parent report (0.42–1.10) and

Table 1  Summary of meta-analytic reviews of parent training


Study designs included
Review TX Dates Age Na RTC B-S W-S SS
Disruptive behavior
Charach et al. (2013) BPT 1980–2011 <6 16 x x
Maughan et al. (2005) BPT 1966–2001 <18 79 x x x x (n = 15)
Serketich and Dumas (1996) BPT 1969–1992 P-E 26 x x
Dretzke et al. (2009) PT < 2006 <18 24 x
Bradley and Mandell (2005) PT 1990–2004 E  7 x
Lundahl et al. (2006) PT 1974–2003 P-E-M 63 x x x
ADHD
Lee et al. (2012) BPT 1970–2011 2.7–14.6b 47 x x
Zwi et al. (2011) PT <2011 5–18  3 x x
Pelham and Fabiano (2008) BPT 1997–2006 3–18 13 x x x
Corcoran and Dattalo (2006) BPT 1980–2003 <19 16 x x
Purdie et al. (2002) BPT 1990–1998 E-M  4 x x
Child abuse
Lundahl et al. (2006) PT 1970–2004 NR 23 x x x
Specific programs
Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck (2007) PCIT 1980–2004 3–12 13 x x
PPP 11
Cedar and Levant (1990) PET 1975–1990 NR 26 x x
Note: TX treatments, BPT behavioral parent training only, PT parent training regardless of theoretical orientation, PCIT
Parent Child Interaction Therapy, PPP Triple P: Positive Parenting Program, PET Parent Effectiveness Training, P
preschool age, E elementary age, M middle school age, NR not reported, RTC randomized control trials, B-S between-­
subjects (treatment and control/comparison groups), W-S within-subjects (one group, pretest/posttest), SS single
subject
a
Studies included in meta-analysis
b
Range of mean age
Parent Training Interventions 69

Table 2  Summary of effect sizes for parent training treatments across studies and outcomes
Child behavior PR Child behavior DO Parent outcome
Review Post FU Post FU Post FU
Disruptive behavior
Charach et al. (2013) 0.75 – – – 0.55 –
Maughan et al. (2005) 0.68/0.88a 0.69/0.92 0.36/0.56 0.10/0.46 – –
Serketich and Dumas (1996) 0.84/0.73b – 0.85 – 0.44 –
Dretzke et al. (2009) 0.62/0.67c – 0.44 – – –
Bradley and Mandell (2005) 1.1/0.25d – – – 0.88/0.25d –
Lundahl et al. (2006a) 0.42/0.44e 0.21/0.87f – – 0.45/0.66e 0.25/0.64f
ADHD
Lee et al. (2012) 0.65 0.30 0.68 −0.08 0.82/0.56/1.25g 0.75/0.24/0.56
Zwi et al. (2011) −0.32/−.48h – – – – –
Pelham and Fabiano (2008) – – – – – –
Corcoran and Dattalo (2006) 0.36/0.63/0.40i – – – – –
Purdie et al. (2002) 0.31/0.30/0.53j – – – – –
Child abuse
Lundahl et al. (2006b) – – – – 0.60/0.53/0.51k –
Specific programs
Thomas and Zimmer-­ −1.45/−1.31l −1.10n 0.11/−0.54 −0.43n −0.76/−1.46m −0.94n
Gembeck (2007) −0.69/−0.73m −0.70n −0.22/−0.61 −0.61n −1.07/−0.70m −0.69n
Cedar and Levant (1990) 0.03 0.53 – – 0.41/0.37o –
PR Parent reported, DO Direct observation
a
Between subjects/within subjects
b
Parent report/teacher report
c
Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI) Intensity Score/ECBI Frequency Score
d
Parent training (PT) alone/PT with child component
e
BPT programs/non-BPT programs
f
BPT programs only: between-subjects designs/within-subjects designs
g
Parent behavior questionnaire/parent behavior observation/parent perception
h
Externalizing/internalizing outcomes
i
Externalizing/internalizing/ADHD outcomes
j
Across all outcomes/ADHD/cognition
k
Abuse attitudes/emotional adjustment/child-rearing behavior
l
PCIT: independent groups (versus waitlist) comparisons/single group pre- to posttreatment
m
Triple P: independent groups (versus waitlist) comparisons/single group pre- to posttreatment
n
Single group pretreatment to follow-up
o
Parent attitude/parent behavior

small to large child outcomes based on direct Maughan, Christiansen, Jenson, Olympia, and
observations (0.36–0.85). Parent outcome effect Clark (2005) found medium overall effect sizes
sizes for BPT programs ranged from small to for BPT with children and adolescents across
medium (0.44–0.55). between-subjects (B-S; treatment versus control/
Charach and colleagues (2013) reviewed comparison), within-subjects (W-S; one group,
group studies of BPT with preschool-aged chil- pre-/posttest), and single-subject designs (0.58,
dren. They found medium effect sizes for child 0.74, 0.59, respectively). For group studies (B-S/
behavior outcomes based on parent report and W-S), they found medium to large effect sizes for
parenting skills outcomes across studies judged child behavior outcomes based on parent report –
to be of fair or good quality (−0.75 and 0.55, both at posttreatment (0.68/0.88) and long-term
respectively). They found a medium but slightly follow-up (0.69/0.92). Effect sizes for child out-
smaller effect size for child behavior when only come based on direct observation were small to
studies judged as good were included (−0.68). medium for posttreatment (0.36/0.56) and small
70 N. Long et al.

for follow-up (0.10/0.46). The consistently smaller  ttention Deficit Hyperactivity


A
effect sizes for B-S designs relative to W-S designs Disorder (ADHD)
suggest that later may inflate effect sizes. Five reviews conducted meta-analyses of BPT
Serketich and Dumas (1996) reported a large program for children with ADHD (see Tables 1
mean effect size for BPT on overall child out- and 2). A total of 63 individual studies were
come (0.86). The mean effect sizes for child out- included in all of these meta-analyses. Lee, Niew,
come based on parent, observer, and teacher were Yang, Chen, and Lin (2012) conducted the most
0.84, 0.85, and 0.73, respectively. The mean recent comprehensive review. They looked at 48
effect size for outcomes of parental adjustment studies between 1970 and 2011 of BPT only or
was medium at 0.44. Moderator analyses found BPT enhanced (integrated within a package of
smaller effects sizes for studies with larger sam- interventions). The overall effect size was
ple sizes, more accurate statistics reported, and medium-large (0.72) across child behavior and
younger subjects. parent behavior domains (questionnaire or obser-
Dretzke and colleagues (2009) reviewed 24 vation). A large effect was found in 20 studies
studies of parent training programs (20 BPT pro- that assessed parenting perception outcomes
grams, 4 non-BPT) for children less than 12 years (1.25). They found no significant differences in
of age. PT programs varied in length and format, effect sizes between type (BPT alone versus BPT
and studies represented community and clinical integrated with other interventions) or format of
populations. They reported medium effect sizes for program (group versus individually adminis-
child behavior outcomes (−0.62 to −0.67). Child tered). Higher quality studies and studies with a
behavior outcomes based on direct observation higher percentage of comorbid conditions were
showed a low to medium effect size (−0.44). associated with lower effect sizes. A small-­
Bradley and Mandell (2005) reviewed seven medium (0.35) overall follow-up effect (up to
randomized groups studies (2 non-BPT, 5 BPT) 3 years post-BPT) across child behavior and par-
and found large effect sizes on child home behav- ent behavior domains (−0.08–0.75) was found
ior outcomes and parenting stress for BPT pro- based on 17 studies.
grams alone (1.2, 0.88, respectively). The parent Zwi, Jones, Thorgaard, York, and Dennis
training (two non-BPT and one BPT) in combi- (2011) reviewed three randomized control trials
nation with other treatments showed small effect between 1993 and 2010 of parent training with
sizes for child home behavior and parenting children with ADHD (N = 284). Effect sizes for
stress (0.25, 0.25). externalizing and internalizing behaviors were
Lundahl, Risser, and Lovejoy (2006) reviewed small-medium (0.32; 3 studies) and medium
63 B-S and W-S studies of parent training pro- (0.48; 2 studies), respectively.
grams (49 BPT, 14 non-BPT). They found small Pelham and Fabiano (2008) conducted a
effect sizes for child behavior outcomes for both meta-­analysis of 13 studies of BPT from 1997 to
BPT and non-BPT programs (0.42, 0.44, respec- 2006 in the context of a broader review of psy-
tively) and small to medium effect sizes for par- chosocial interventions for children with ADHD,
ent behavior outcomes for BPT and non-BPT expanding on a previous strength of evidence
programs (0.45, 0.66, respectively). The BPT review (Pelham, Wheeler, & Chronis, 1998).
programs were found to have significantly higher BPT treatments compared to waitlist showed
methodological rigor than the non-BPT pro- effect sizes that ranged from 0.47 to 0.70, with
grams. Several factors made comparisons one exception (−0.02). One study showed BPT
between BPT and non-BPT programs difficult. to have a medium effect (0.66) compared to
The majority of BPT studies used clinical sam- nondirective/support intervention (Sonuga-
ples, whereas the majority of non-BPT studies Barke, Daley, Thompson, Laver-Bradbury, &
used nonclinical samples. Furthermore, programs Weeks, 2001). BPT combined with classroom
with different theoretical orientations tend to target behavioral interventions compared with commu-
different outcomes. nity (including medication) and medication
Parent Training Interventions 71

treatments showed effect sizes of −0.01 and ies evaluating the Parent-Child Interaction
−0.24 respectively. Therapy (PCIT) and Positive Parenting Program
Corcoran and Dattalo (2006) conducted a meta- (Triple P) programs – both BPT programs. They
analysis of “parent-involved” cognitive-­behavioral reviewed studies from 1980 through 2004,
treatments (not limited to parent training) for chil- including children 3–12 years of age. Both PCIT
dren (0–18 years) with ADHD. They included 16 and Triple P showed medium to large effects on
group studies between 1980 and 2003. They parent-reported child behavior and clinic-­
reported a small-medium (0.42) overall effect size observed parent behavior across study designs
across child outcomes. A medium effect size was (B-S/W-S). Small to medium effect sizes were
found for internalizing symptoms (0.63), and small- shown for direct observation of child behavior in
medium effect sizes were found for child behavior the clinic for both programs.
outcomes of ADHD (0.40) and externalizing (0.36). Cedar and Levant (1990) conducted a meta-­
Purdie, Hattie, and Carroll (2002) reviewed four analysis of studies evaluating the efficacy of
studies of parent training with ADHD children and the Parent Effectiveness Training program
found small effect sizes for overall (0.31) and (PET; Gordon, 1970) on the behavior and cog-
ADHD (0.30) outcomes and a medium effect size nitive adjustment of both children and parents.
for general cognition (0.54–2 studies). PET is based on a reflective/Rogerian approach.
Most of the studies were doctoral dissertations
 hild Abuse Risk
C rather than peer-reviewed journal articles.
Lundahl, Nimer, and Parsons (2006) evaluated the They examined 26 studies from 1975 to 1990.
effects of parent training programs on parent risk Their analyses found no to small effects on
factors related to child abuse and documented child behavior outcomes, small-medium
abuse. They identified 23 studies from 1970 to 2004 effects for child self-­esteem and parent atti-
that included 25 parent training treatment groups. tudes and behavior, and a large effect on out-
Of the 23 studies, 17 used W-S designs. The parent comes related to parental knowledge of course
training interventions used in these studies varied content (1.10).
on a number of characteristics, including theoretical
orientation (behavioral, non-behavioral, mixed),  ummary of Meta-analytic Reviews
S
location of intervention (home, office, mixed), While the volume of studies of parent training is
delivery mode (group, individual, mixed), and impressive, much of the research is of low qual-
number of sessions. Immediately following parent ity, particularly among non-behavioral programs.
training, parents showed medium improvements in Many of the reviews started with hundreds or
outcome variables: 0.60 for attitudes linked to thousands of studies only to be sharply narrowed
abuse, 0.53 for emotional adjustment, and 0.51 for by simple, reasonable inclusion criteria, and
child-rearing skills. Significant differences were while the studies that remained were of relatively
found between the effect sizes of studies with B-S high quality, they varied on multiple, meaningful
designs (n = 6; treatment versus control/compari- dimensions. However, overall, the MA reviews
son; 0.30) and studies with W-S designs (0.62) for indicate that many behavioral parent training
the emotional adjustment outcome variable, sug- programs tend to be at least moderately effective
gesting, at least for this variable, that the effects are (i.e., medium effect sizes on average). Meta-­
more in the small to medium range. Moderator analysis provides a useful method for managing
analyses showed a negative correlation between the biases and differences across multiple stud-
study rigor and outcomes (rs = −0.35 to −0.90). ies, placing MA data near the top of the evidence
hierarchy; however, it is not the only method for
 eta-analyses Related to Specific
M evaluating treatments. A more recent strategy
Programs focuses more on the quality or strength of the
Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck (2007) conducted available evidence as will be discussed in the
a systematic and meta-analytic review of 24 stud- next section.
72 N. Long et al.

Strength of Evidence Reviews Psychological Procedures, 1995). One of the


programs (Living with Children – the Oregon
In response to managed care, formal efforts were model of parent management training, Patterson,
made to identify empirically supported treat- Reid, Jones, & Conger, 1975) was classified as
ments (EST) to third-party payers and to inform well-­established, and the other six were classified
treatment guidelines (see Chambless & Ollendick, as probably efficacious (see Lonigan, Elbert, &
2001 for history of ESTs). These efforts utilized Johnson, 1998 for definitions).
systems for evaluating and categorizing the Chambless and Ollendick (2001) reviewed the
strength of evidence (SOE) of various treatments results of eight different task forces that catego-
across disorders. Treatments with the strongest rized the empirical support of child and adoles-
level of empirical support generally involve well-­ cent psychosocial treatments across disorders.
designed studies (randomized control trials) They developed their own three-point categorical
showing positive results that have been indepen- system to standardize the various criteria used
dently replicated. Six reviews evaluated the SOE across task forces. The Living with Children pro-
of BPT interventions within the context of gram was rated Category I (highest of three cat-
broader reviews of psychosocial treatments for egories of empirical support – at least two
children and adolescents. Three reviews evalu- positive Type 1 studies – randomized control tri-
ated BPT interventions across various disorders als (RTC) with clearly described statistical meth-
(Chambless & Ollendick, 2001; Chorpita et al., ods) by at least three workgroups, and Category
2002, 2011), two reviews evaluated BPT inter- II (at least one positive RTC) by at least one
ventions for disruptive behaviors (Brestan & workgroup. The Parent-Child Interaction
Eyberg, 1998; Eyberg, Nelson, & Boggs, 2008), Therapy program was rated as Category II by one
and one review evaluated BPT interventions for workgroup. Both of these treatment programs are
ADHD (Pelham et al., 1998). BPT treatments.

Disruptive Behaviors  ttention Deficit Hyperactivity


A
Three research groups evaluated the strength of evi- Disorder (ADHD)
dence of treatments involving parent training for Three research groups evaluated the strength of
children and adolescents with disruptive behaviors. evidence of treatments involving parent training
Chorpita et al. (2011) reviewed studies from 1965 for children and adolescents with ADHD.
to 2009 and utilized a five-level system (1 = Best Chorpita et al. (2011) evaluated 16 different
Support, 5 = No Support) based on the American treatment approaches for children with
Psychological Association (APA) Division 12 ADHD. They rated BPT alone, BPT with teacher
(Division of Clinical Psychology) standards to eval- training, and BPT with problem solving to be
uate 23 different treatment approaches for children Level 2: good support based on 5, 2, and 1 sup-
with disruptive behavior disorders. They rated BPT portive studies, respectively. Mean effect sizes
alone and BPT combined with a problem solving were 0.92, 0.80, and 0.68, respectively. In the
treatment to be Level I based on 41 and 3 supportive Chambless and Ollendick (2001) review, BPT
studies, respectively. BPT alone showed the highest was identified as a Category I treatment by one of
mean effect size (0.98) of the six treatments identi- the eight task forces.
fied as Level 1. Pelham et al. (1998) reviewed psychosocial
Eyberg et al. (2008) reviewed 15 studies from treatments for ADHD using the original APA
1966 through 2007 that utilized seven different two-level categorical system. They reviewed 17
BPT treatment programs (alone or in combina- studies of BPT and judged BPT to meet criteria
tion with other treatments). They utilized the for well-established treatment for ADHD, using
original APA two-level categorical system – liberal interpretations of the APA criteria. Their
well-established and probably efficacious (Task review showed seven group studies contributing
Force on Promotion and Dissemination of to the well-established criteria and five group
Parent Training Interventions 73

studies contributing to the probably efficacious Parent Training Within  and


criteria. They found three groups studies and Across Conditions
three single-subject studies that did not contrib- Several early reviews evaluated parent training
ute to the well-established or probably effica- research across conditions (Berkowitz &
cious criteria. In their follow-up study (Pelham & Graziano, 1971; Johnson & Katz, 1973;
Fabiano, 2008), they added three additional stud- Moreland, Schwebel, Beck, & Wells, 1982;
ies contributing to the well-established criteria O’Dell, 1974; Travormina, 1974; Wiese, 1992;
and judged BPT to clearly meet the criteria for Wiese & Kramer, 1988). For example, Graziano
well-established treatment. and Diament (1992) reviewed 186 empirical
studies that evaluate the efficacy of BPT with a
Autism variety of childhood problems. In addition to
One review (Chorpita et al., 2011) evaluated five problems with conduct and hyperactivity, studies
different treatment approaches for children with have examined BPT with children with intellec-
autism. They rated BPT alone to be Level 4 tual disabilities, physical disabilities, autism,
(Minimal Support based on 1 supportive study) overweight, enuresis, fears, and other specific
and a mean effect size of 0.55. behavioral problems. They concluded that the
BPT showed clear positive results for conduct
Summary of SOE Reviews problems and discrete child behavior problems
Based on these SOE reviews, BPT treatments (e.g., enuresis, fears, weight reduction), some
have been judged to have the highest to middle success with hyperactivity, and mixed results
level of empirical support for children and ado- with autism and intellectual disabilities. For the
lescents with disruptive behavior disorders and latter two conditions, they suggested that BPT
ADHD and minimal support for autism. The next might be more effective in improving parent
type of review we will examine is the systematic/ outcomes than child behavior. A RTC of parent
critical review. education and skills training interventions sup-
ports this notion (Tonge et al., 2006), showing
significant improvements in the functioning of
Systematic/Critical Reviews parents of young autistic children following
treatment relative to the control group.
The systematic/critical review method is high on Six reviews have critiqued parent training
the evidence hierarchy, on par with meta-­ treatments within a broader review of psychoso-
analysis. Thirty-six of the 53 reviews of parent- cial treatments with children with disruptive
ing training utilized the systematic/critical review behavior problems (Behan, 2000; Bryant, Vissard,
method alone or in combination with other Willoughby, & Kupersmidt, 1999; Dumas, 1989;
review strategies. This method can be used to Farmer, Compton, Burns, & Robertson, 2002;
evaluate the evidence in support of specific treat- Kazdin, 1987, 2001; McAuley, 1982; Webster-
ments across conditions by summarizing the Stratton, 1991). While all of the reviews presented
existing literature relative to treatment outcomes. generally positive posttreatment and follow-up
The systematic/critical review method is also findings relative to other psychosocial treatments,
useful for critiquing the quality of studies, high- each also critiqued areas in need of additional
lighting the different methods and outcomes research.
across studies (a nuance that can get obscured in Several studies focused their reviews specifi-
meta-analysis) and addressing specific concep- cally on parent training treatments for children
tual or policy questions. Below we will highlight with disruptive behaviors (Atkeson & Forehand,
aspects of parent training treatments that have 1978; Kazdin, 1997; Miller & Prinz, 1990;
been addressed through the systematic/critical Nixon, 2002). In reviewing various BPT treat-
method, both within and across conditions and ment formats for children with disruptive behav-
across theoretical orientations. ior problems, Nixon (2002) concluded that parent
74 N. Long et al.

training programs are promising but limited by in terms of how they relate to parent training
methodological problems. Using a vote counting treatment outcomes: (1) study quality; (2) con-
method in their systematic review of parent train- textual, child, and parent factors; and (3) pro-
ing treatments for children with disruptive behav- gram factors.
ior problems, Dretzke and colleagues (2009)
reported that 59% of 170 child outcomes across Study Quality
36 studies were statistically significant in favor As far as outcome evidence specific to treatment
of parent training treatment over controls. modality, the volume of studies of parent training
Reviews of parent training interventions with treatments is impressive; however, volume in
ADHD populations have concluded that more sys- itself is not an indicator of strength. The quality
tematic study is needed but that existing studies of study designs has been shown in many of the
provide sufficient evidence to consider parent reviews to be a moderator of treatment effects –
training an effective treatment for ADHD (Chronis, with a negative relationship shown between the
Chacko, Fabiano, Wymbs, & Pelham, 2004; Kohut quality of study designs and effect size (Cedar &
& Andrew, 2004; Pelham et al., 1998). Levant, 1990; Charach et al., 2013; Lee et al.,
2012). Poor quality studies tend to inflate out-
 arent Training Across Orientations
P come effects. The BPT programs were found to
Several older reviews have examined the method- have significantly higher methodological rigor
ology and efficacy of parent training programs than the non-BPT programs (Lundahl, Risser, &
from different theoretical orientations (Dembo, Lovejoy, 2006).
Sweitzer, & Lauritzen, 1985; Mooney, 1995;
Todres & Bunston, 1993, Travormina, 1974).  ontextual, Parental, and Child Factors
C
Several differences between BPT and non-BPT The role of various contextual, parental, and child
studies make comparisons difficult. The majority factors as likely mediators or moderators of par-
of BPT studies were more intervention focused ent training outcomes has been a persistent theme
and used clinical samples, whereas the majority discussed across the systematic/critical reviews.
of non-BPT studies were more prevention For example, Graziano and Diament (1992)
focused and used nonclinical samples. Programs examined the relationship between the factors of
with different theoretical orientations tend to tar- child age, child IQ, family’s socioeconomic sta-
get different outcomes, making direct compari- tus, parental social support, parental education
sons impossible. All of these reviews noted that level, parental functioning, family stress, and eth-
few studies met the criteria for well-designed nicity and parent training outcomes. The influ-
investigations, and the diverse methodologies ence of these factors is certainly not unique to
precluded direct comparisons of efficacy. All the parent training but shared by all psychosocial
reviews reported mixed results, with positive treatments.
findings following what would be expected from Factors identified in the quantitative reviews
the specific theoretical orientation. For example, include age and financial disadvantage. One
the Adlerian programs showed a greater percent- review (Lundahl, Risser, & Lovejoy, 2006)
age of positive findings in the outcome domain of found significantly higher effect sizes for chil-
parental attitudes and perceptions, while behav- dren than teens on child behavior outcomes but
ioral programs showed a greater percentage of no differences on parental behavior or percep-
positive findings on child behavior. tions. Two other reviews that included non-BPT
programs reported a positive relationship
between age and positive outcomes for parent
Moderators of Parent Training Effects training programs in general (Cedar & Levant,
1990; Serketich & Dumas, 1996). Many of the
Many of the reviews examined factors that mod- BPT programs were developed for children, with
erate the effects of parent training. In this sec- fewer programs targeting teens; consequently,
tion, we will focus on the following three factors the evidence is stronger for programs targeting
Parent Training Interventions 75

children younger than 12 than for those targeting Although the various reviews have shown
teens (Chronis et al., 2004). some support for child, parent, contextual, and
One study (Lundahl, Risser, & Lovejoy, 2006) program features as moderators of response to
found financial disadvantage to be the most parent training, most analyses are post-hoc and
salient moderator of parent training outcomes. correlational. Relatively little research has been
Children and parents from non-disadvantaged conducted where these characteristics have been
families benefited more across the child behav- studied as independent variables, which will be
ior, parent behavior, and parental perception out- needed to establish the validity of moderators.
come domains compared to disadvantaged
families. In addition, they found that marital sta-
tus was a moderator of child behavior outcomes.  eneralization of Parent Training
G
Reviews of BPT treatments and BPT outcomes Effects
with disruptive behavior problems and ADHD
(Assemany and McIntosh, 2002; Chronis et al., It is reasonable to assume that changing parents’
2004) reported on the moderating effects of mari- behavior would result in some generalization of
tal status on child behavior outcomes; studies treatment effects across time and settings and to
with a higher percentage of single parents did not untreated siblings. Some early, individual studies
show as much change as studies with a lower per- provide some support for the generalization of BPT
centage of single parents. effects to untreated siblings’ observed compliance
(Eyberg & Robinson, 1982; Humphreys, Forehand,
Program Factors McMahon, & Roberts, 1978) and deviant behavior
Several quantitative and systematic/critical (e.g., (Arnold, Levin, & Patterson, 1975; Wells,
Chronis et al., 2004) reviews have examined the Forehand, & Griest, 1980) at posttreatment. In one
association of different parent training program study, the improvements were maintained at a
features and parent training outcomes. The quan- 6-month follow-up (Arnold et al., 1975). Eyberg
titative reviews examined the association between and Robinson (1982) reported significant improve-
program format (e.g., individual versus group) ments in observed parent behavior with untreated
and program length and treatment outcomes. Two siblings and no significant reductions in the num-
reviews (Lee et al., 2012; Serketich & Dumas, ber or intensity of negative sibling behaviors.
1996) found no differences in effect sizes for the Two early, individual studies failed to show
overall child outcome and format of the treatment generalization of treatment effects from clinic to
(individual versus group). One review (Lundahl, school settings (Breiner & Forehand, 1981;
Risser, & Lovejoy, 2006) found no differences in Forehand et al., 1979). However, McNeil, Eyberg,
effect sizes between face-to-­face and self-directed Eisenstadt, Newcomb, and Funderburk (1991)
interventions; however, they reported that among reported significant improvements in teacher-
the 20 studies that treated financially disadvan- rated deviant behavior and observations of appro-
taged families, individual parent training resulted priate and compliant behaviors at school in ten
in significantly greater improvements in child children treated with a BPT program relative to
and parent behavior than group parent training; controls. In this study, they selected subjects who
no differences were found between individual showed high levels of behavior problems across
and group treatment in the parental perceptions home and school settings at pretreatment and who
outcome domain. One review (Lundahl, Nimer, all showed clinically significant improvements in
& Parsons, 2006) found that studies whose pro- home behavior after treatment.
grams were 12 or more sessions had greater While there is some supporting evidence for
improvements in parental attitudes linked to abuse generalization, confidence in the generalizability
compared to programs with fewer than 12 ses- of treatment effects would be increased with
sions and no differences in child-rearing behavior additional studies with improved methodology,
between programs with low and high number of such as larger sample sizes, multiple outcome
sessions. measures, and control groups.
76 N. Long et al.

 ummary of the Empirical Support


S with ASD is long-standing. The seminal research
for Parent Training done in the area of autism related interventions ini-
tially focused on therapist-­delivered services but
There have been a substantial number of studies eventually widened to include parents in thera-
evaluating parent training programs from differ- peutic approaches. The inclusion of parents is
ent theoretical orientations and across different seen in the work done by Lovaas, Koegel,
child problems. As a whole, the research is sup- Simmons, and Long (1973; McEachin, Smith, &
portive of the immediate effectiveness of parent Lovaas, 1993) and Pivotal Response Training
training across many parent and child outcome (Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987; Koegel,
domains. Data on the maintenance of effects is Koegel, Harrower, & Carter, 1999).
less consistent, with follow-up effect sizes rang- In recent years there has been a significant
ing from none to large. Parent training can be increase in studies examining the effectiveness of
considered at least moderately effective, which parent training with the ASD population
compares very favorably to the effects found for (Beaudoin, Sebire, & Coutre, 2014; Wallace &
other psychotherapy treatments. More specifi- Rogers, 2010). As a result, parent training has
cally, there is sufficient evidence to consider recently been listed as being an evidence-based
behaviorally oriented parent training programs intervention by the National Standards Project
efficacious in treating children with oppositional (NSP) (National Autism Center, 2015). The NSP
and ADHD problems. is an initiative funded by the National Autism
Center to provide evidence-based guidelines for
treatment in the area of ASD (National Autism
 arent Training and Autism
P Center, 2009, 2015). By a stringent review of the
Spectrum Disorder (ASD) body of literature on ASD interventions by the
NSP, there are published recommendations to
As previously discussed, the published reviews guide consumers about the “strength of research
of the data regarding the effectiveness of parent- support for educational and behavioral treatments
ing training programs targeting children with for core characteristics for individuals with ASD”
ASD (Chorpita et al., 2011) are limited and not (www.nationalautism center.org). The initial
very strong. However, it is important to consider, National Standards Project (NSP) published in
when reading a review focused on limited data, 2009 did not include parent training as one of the
whether there has been a significant growth in 11 evidence-based treatment approaches for
that area of research since the reviewed studies ASD. By the time the 2015 edition (NSP2, 2015)
were conducted. In the case of ASD, there has was published, they judged the current research
been a significant increase of studies examining evidence justified parent training now being listed
parent training in recent years. For that reason, as as an evidence-based treatment.
well as the growing concern regarding the need It is important to note that within the ASD lit-
for additional services targeting children with erature, the term “parent training” is often not
ASD, in this section, we will discuss some of the well defined (Bearss, Burrell, Stewart, & Scahill,
recent work in regard to parent training services 2015). This can lead to confusion in the literature
for this population. and in clinical practice as to what is meant by
To a large extent, the literature on parent train- “parent training” in the area of ASD. Although
ing for children with developmental disorders and the NSP lists parent training as an evidence-­
specifically autism has historically developed sep- based intervention, even when looking at the
arately from the parent training literature regard- studies included in their report, the studies
ing disruptive behavior disorders and ADHD included for review are a wide array of parent
(Brookman-Frazee, Stahmer, Baker-­ Ericzen & interventions from support groups with a primary
Tsai, 2006). Within the ASD literature, the involve- educational component all the way to in vivo
ment of parents in interventions for their children individual training taught and supervised by
Parent Training Interventions 77

licensed professionals. Unfortunately, the lack of 2006) to parent educational intervention (PEI)
clarity regarding how parent training is defined is (Breton & Tonge, 2005) in regard to changes in
a long-standing and still widespread issue that parent-child play interactions specific to parent
impacts not only the ASD literature but most responsivity and child joint attention. Parent-­
areas in which a wide array of parent training/ child dyads were randomly placed in one of the
education services are provided (Long, 1997). two interventions. Parents in the JASPER group
were found to have significantly increased their
responsive behavior to their children, and
 arent Support Versus Parent
P ­parent-­child joint engagement had significantly
Implementation improved at the end of the 10-week training com-
pared to the PEI group. In a related article by
Fortunately, Bearss and colleagues at the Marcus Kasari, Gulsrud, Paparella, Hellemann, and
Autism Center (2015) have proposed a taxonomy Berry (2015), additional analysis of data col-
for classifying parent training for children with lected in the randomized clinical trial showed
ASD. They separate parent training programs that parents in the PEI intervention did report a
into two categories: (1) parent support (which decrease in parenting stress over the 10-week
encompasses parent education and case coordi- program. The authors suggest that possibly a
nation) and (2) parent implementation (involves combination of the two interventions may pro-
training parents to work with the child rather than vide optimal support for families.
just providing education about a specific issue). A multi-site study led by the Marcus Autism
The “parent implementation” category would Center at Emory University (Bearss et al., 2015)
include interventions that most would consider to also compared parent training to parent education
be “parent training” interventions. in a randomized trial. In the parent training group,
Parent support services are no doubt important 89 parents received an individually delivered
in helping inform and support parents who are intervention that focused on teaching them strate-
rearing a child with ASD. For example, parents gies for handling disruptive behaviors. This inter-
who have a child diagnosed with ASD consis- vention included 11 core sessions, up to two
tently report that lack of good information about optional sessions, one home visit, and up to six
ASD is a problem (Hamilton, 2008; Lopez & parent-child coaching sessions over 16 weeks. The
Bellando, 2012). Fortunately, there has been a 91 parents assigned to the parent education group
recent increase in evidence-based information were provided information about autism but no
being made more readily available to parents. behavioral management strategies. This parent
Autism Speaks has created various toolkits that education intervention was provided through 12
are available for free on their website (www. sessions and one home visit. Two measures were
autismspeaks.org). These toolkits not only pro- used to measure change in regard to disruptive
vide information about ASD but also regarding behavior and noncompliance. At week 24 the par-
interventions for many common issues within the enting training group showed a larger decline in
ASD population including sleep issues, feeding irritable (47.7% decreased compared to a 31.8%
issues, toileting needs, and transition to adult ser- decrease in the education group). On the Home
vices (Bellando, Fussell, & Lopez, 2015). Situation Questionnaire-­PDD version (Chowdhury
A recent randomized control trial conducted et al., 2010) the parent training group also showed
by Shire, Gulsrud, and Kasari (2016) examines clinical improvement (55% decline in scores com-
the question of whether parenting support (i.e., pared to a 34.2% decline for the education group).
parent education) yields differing outcomes when The authors concluded that the parent training
compared to parent implementation models (i.e., group was more effective in reducing disruptive
parent training) with an ASD population. They behaviors than the parent education group but that
compared a parent-child interaction intervention the differences while statistically significant may
(JASPER model) (Kasari, Freeman, & Paparella, not be clinically different.
78 N. Long et al.

 arent Training to Help with Medical


P after VHS videotape technology became widely
Comorbidities Associated with ASD available in the early 1980s, this modality was uti-
lized in the Incredible Years parent ­training pro-
ASD is associated with various comorbidities gram (Webster-Stratton, Kolpacoff, &
(Lajonchere, Jones, Coury & Perrin, 2012). Parent Hollinsworth, 1988). In the 1990s CD-ROM tech-
training interventions are being developed to help nology was introduced and was used to deliver
address some for these related problems. For Parenting Wisely, an evidence-based self-­
example, feeding issues are a common comorbid administered interactive parent training program
condition for children with ASD (Williams, (Gordon, 2000). More recently, internet-based
Dalrymple, & Neal, 2000). Johnson, Foldes, parenting programs are being developed with
DeMand, and Brooks (2015) published a pilot increased frequency (McGrath et al., 2013;
study for a manualized behavioral parent training Sanders, Baker, & Turner, 2012). Technologies
program for feeding for children with ASD aged such as podcasts (Morawska, Tometzki, &
2–7. Parents of 14 children diagnosed with ASD Sanders, 2014) and smartphones (Jones et al.,
were enrolled in this 16-week program that pro- 2014) have also been utilized effectively to deliver
vided up to 9 individually administered sessions interventions and to enhance the effectiveness of
with a therapist trained in applied behavioral anal- existing evidence-based parent training programs.
ysis (ABA). Results showed significant decreases It is hard to predict the new technologies that will
in problems with mealtime behaviors, decreases be developed over the coming decades, but it is
in disruptive behaviors, and reduced parent stress. likely that they will be used to try to improve the
access, delivery, and effectiveness of parent train-
ing programs. It is also predicted that emerging
 urrent and Anticipated Trends
C technologies will be used with increased fre-
in Parent Training quency to train those that deliver parent training
interventions and facilitate the dissemination and
In this final section of the chapter, we will discuss scaling of evidence-based programs.
some current and anticipated trends in the area of
parent training. While there are many trends that
we could focus on, we have selected the following I ncreasing Focus on Transporting
five areas: (1) the use of technology to enhance Evidence-Based Parent Training
parent training, (2) transporting evidence-­ based Programs to Different Countries
parent training programs to other countries/cul- and Cultures
tures, (3) increasing focus on benefit-­cost analysis
and reducing the costs of implementing parent There has been a long-standing concern about the
training programs, (4) identifying key program relative lack of attention to the role that cultural
components and mediators of change, and (5) the context in behavioral parent training interven-
potential role that genetic and biological factors tions (Forehand & Kotchick, 1996). While there
may play in the future of parent training. is no doubt that this is an extremely important
issue worthy of significant study, there is recent
evidence suggesting that some evidence-based
I ncreasing Use of Technology parent training interventions can be successfully
to Enhance Parent Training implemented in other countries/cultures. A recent
review (Gardner, Montgomery, & Knerr, 2016)
There is little doubt that technology has had a pro- focused on the international implementation of
found influence on our lives over the past several four parent training programs that target the
decades. Technology has also had an increasing reduction of child behavior problems. These four
impact on the delivery of parent training services interventions (Incredible Years, Triple P, PCIT,
over the same time period (Long, 2004). Soon and PMTO) were developed in the United States or
Parent Training Interventions 79

Australia and have been evaluated in ten countries provide. As the healthcare system moves in this
within five regions of the world (Europe, Asia, direction, there will be a greater focus on ROI
North America, Middle East, and the Caribbean). (return on investment). Health and mental health
The interventions transported to “western” coun- systems of care will favor interventions that yield
tries demonstrated similar outcomes to those the greatest cost savings (e.g., reduced future
obtained in trials in their countries of origin. mental health costs) per dollar spent on the
Surprisingly, the effects were even stronger when intervention.
the interventions were implemented in the most As an example of the importance of ROI, the
culturally distant regions. Further, extensive cul- US Federal Government has recently funded a
tural adaptation of these interventions did not major national expansion of evidence-based
appear necessary to effectively transport these home visiting programs for parents with young
programs to these culturally different regions. children. The Department of Health and Human
This finding conflicts with the often held belief Services has provided several billion dollars for
that parenting interventions need to go through this expansion through the Maternal Infant and
extensive adaptations if they are to be effective in Early Childhood Home Visiting (MIECHV) pro-
different cultural contexts. The degree to which gram. This funding was a direct result of research,
these recent findings can be generalized to other primarily conducted by the Nurse Family
types of parent training programs and to other Partnership (NFP) program that demonstrated a
cultures (especially in low-­income countries) is significant ROI. As further indication that
yet to be determined. However, these early find- benefit-­cost evaluations will play an increasing
ings are intriguing and suggest that many pro- role funding decisions, the Institute of Medicine
grams may be more easily transported to many and the National Research Council (2014)
countries/cultures than previously thought. recently assembled researchers to help create
Further support for this view is the extensive dis- standards for conducting benefit-cost analyses
semination and acceptance of the Triple P related to prevention aspects of child, youth, and
throughout the world over the past 15 years family programs.
(Sanders & Murphy-Brennan, 2010). There is The pressure to provide effective parenting
little doubt that issue of effectively transporting interventions at lower costs will increase efforts
evidence-based parent training interventions for that explore the effectiveness of abbreviated par-
use within other cultures will be an area of grow- ent training interventions that could be imple-
ing interest given the increasing societal focus on mented in different settings (e.g., integrated
globalization. primary care pediatric settings). As an example
Berkovits et al. (2010) examined two abbreviated
versions of PCIT. One version consisted of a
I ncreasing Focus on Benefit-Cost four-session group intervention called Primary
Analyses and Reducing the Costs Care PCIT, and the other version was called PCIT
of Program Delivery Anticipatory Guidance and consisted of written
materials describing the use of PCIT techniques.
Given the escalating costs of health and mental These versions of PCIT were developed to
health services, policy makers and third-party address children’s subclinical behavior problems
payers will increasingly be concerned with rela- within pediatric primary care settings. Both
tive cost-benefit ratios of interventions. This will abbreviated versions of PCIT were found to be
be especially true as the healthcare system in the effective child behavioral concerns posttreatment
United States transitions from a fee-for-service and at 6-month follow-up, but the group inter-
system to a value-based system. In a value-based vention was not found to be more effective than
system, payment to providers/systems will no the self-directed approach which was based only
longer be based on the number of sessions/ on utilization of written materials. These results
services but rather on the value of the care they are consistent with a study comparing different
80 N. Long et al.

service modes for treating oppositional defiant I ncreased Focus on Key Program
disorder in primary care settings (Lavigne et al., Components and Mediators
2008). They compared a 12-session group inter- of Change
vention utilizing the Incredible Years (IY) pro-
gram (Webster-Stratton & Reid, 2003) to Given the growing number of evidence-based
bibliotherapy (just reading the parenting book parent training programs, there have been efforts
that accompanies the IY program). They found to identify the shared key components of effec-
improvement at posttreatment as well as at tive programs. For example, Kaminski and col-
12-month follow-up but no overall treatment leagues (2008) conducted a meta-analysis of 77
group effects. Additionally, Morawska and parent training studies focused on young children
Sanders (2006) examined the effectiveness of a in order to identify program content and program
self-administered version of the Triple P in com- delivery components that were consistently asso-
bination with weekly telephone support from a ciated with better outcomes. Based on this work,
counselling service provider. They found this the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
approach to improve parenting and reduce child (CDC) published the guideline Parent Training
behavior problems at 3-month follow-up. Programs: Insight for Practitioners (Centers for
The Helping the Noncompliant Child program Disease Control and Prevention, 2009). They
has also been adapted for delivery within a par- found that the program components most strongly
enting group format as well as for use as a self-­ associated with the acquisition of effective par-
directed written approach. The 6-week parenting enting skills were (1) teaching parents emotional
group program (total of 12 h) focuses on teaching communication skills, (2) teaching parents posi-
the core HNC skills as well as additional topics tive parent-child interaction skills, and (3) requir-
including creating a more positive home, improv- ing parents to practice with their child during
ing communication, developing more patience, program sessions. The program components
building positive self-esteem, and problem solv- most strongly related to decreases in children’s
ing. Evaluations of this parenting group format externalizing behavior were found to be (1)
suggest it is effective in improving parenting and teaching parents the correct use of time-out, (2)
in reducing child behavior problems (Conners, teaching parents to respond consistently to their
Edwards, & Grant, 2007; Forehand et al., 2011). child, (3) teaching parents to interact positively
The self-directed written approach involves a with their child, and (4) requiring parents to prac-
5-week strategy presented in the HNC-based tice with their child during program sessions.
book Parenting the Strong-Willed Child The increasing focus on delivering interven-
(Forehand & Long, 2010). Findings from a ran- tions in the most cost-effective manner will be a
domized trial (Forehand, Dorsey, Jones, Long, & motivating factor in expanding efforts to identify
McMahon, 2010) indicate that when parents read key components of effective parent training pro-
the book it was associated with decreases in child grams (e.g., Kaehler, Jacobs, & Jones, 2016).
behavior problems. Identifying components that are, and are not,
It is clear that various methods of teaching critical to effectiveness may help program devel-
parents can be used to effectively use core parent opers streamline their interventions and reduce
training skills. A major question that needs to be the costs of implementing the program (e.g.,
answered regards what level/method of interven- reducing the number of sessions necessary to
tion is most appropriate under what conditions achieve a specific level of outcome). It will also
(e.g., level/type of child problems, parent factors, help address the issue of maintaining adherence
etc.). Given the importance of this issue, it is (fidelity to the program’s key components) while
anticipated that this will be a continued focus of also allowing some degree of flexibility in deliv-
interest as the emphasis on benefit-cost issues ering the program (Forehand et al., 2010;
and ROI increases over the coming years. Mazzucchelli & Sanders, 2010).
Parent Training Interventions 81

In a related vein, it is predicted that greater that mothers with a specific genotype related to
attention will be focused on factors that mediate the DRD2 gene (about half of mothers) were sig-
the effectiveness of parent training. To what nificantly more likely to engage in harsh parent-
degree do changes in parenting skills versus other ing practices when exposed to economic adversity
factors mediate the positive outcomes of parent (an economic recession) than the other mothers
training programs? Surprisingly, relatively little (Lee, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Notterman, &
research has examined the putative mediators of Garfinkel, 2013). The DRD2 gene influences
effectiveness in parent training (Forehand, Lafko, dopamine which has been found to be involved in
Parent, & Burt, 2014). Future studies will, no regulating emotional and behavioral responses to
doubt, address these factors. environmental threats and rewards. Other studies
have found that children with specific genotypes
(also related to dopamine) to be more sensitive to
 he Role Genetic and Biological
T both negative and positive environments
Factors May Play in the Future (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van Ijzendoorn,
of Parent Training 2011). Specific gene variants (associated with
serotonin or dopamine) have also been found to
This final anticipated trend is much more specu- be related to how sensitive children are to rewards
lative and one we may not see realized for and punishments (Pedersen, 2013). Taken
decades. However, we do anticipate that at some together, these studies suggest that a parent’s
point in the future genetic information may be genotype puts her/him at risk for negative parent-
used to help select the most appropriate parent ing and that a child’s genotype might make him/
training intervention for a given family. We also her more or less sensitive to specific parenting
anticipate that in the future, biological indices interventions. So as information on our personal
will be used to assess the long-term impact of genome becomes more readily available and
parent training programs on health outcomes. Let known to healthcare providers, it is plausible that
us explain the basis for this speculation. this information could be used in helping to
As a result of the Human Genome Project’s determine what type of parent training program
sequencing of the human genome, “personalized might be best for whom.
medicine” has become a rapidly expanding area Another recent research study may also have
of medicine. Personalized medicine involves profound implications for the future of parent
providing customized healthcare for an individ- training in regard to future health issues that are
ual based on knowledge of his/her specific related to inflammation. Excessive inflammation,
genetic genome. Medical decisions and treatment a chronic over-activation of parts of the immune
will increasingly be made utilizing this informa- system, has been found to be related to a number
tion to optimize treatment effectiveness and out- of health problems later in life (e.g., heart dis-
comes (i.e., which medication/treatment has been ease, diabetes) as well as to depression and psy-
demonstrated to be most effective for individuals chosis (Khandaker, Pearson, Zammit, Lewis, &
with certain genotypes). Many would probably Jones, 2014). The intervention study of impor-
consider it a stretch to believe genetic informa- tance (Miller, Brody, Yu, & Chen, 2014) found
tion could be used effectively to help decide what that an intervention – delivered when children
type of parent training program might be most were 11 years old – that focused on improving
effective with a particular family or that parent parenting, parent-child communication, and
training programs could customized for parents helping children develop strategies for dealing
with specific genotypes. However, it may not be stressors actually resulted in reducing inflamma-
as much of a stretch as one might initially think. tion. This was a randomized controlled study
Gene variants are being found to be associated involving low-SES families in which the extent
with differences in behavior as the following of inflammation was assessed 8 years after the
study demonstrates. This large-scale study found intervention when the children were 19 years old.
82 N. Long et al.

Inflammation was lowest among youth who Atkeson, B. M., & Forehand, R. (1978). Parent behav-
ioral training for problem children: An examination of
received, as a result of the intervention, more nur-
studies using multiple outcome measures. Journal of
turing-involved parenting and less harsh-­ Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 449–460.
inconsistent parenting. They also found that the Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H.
intervention was most effective in reducing (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environ-
ment depending on dopamine-related genes: New
inflammation with the most disadvantaged fami-
evidence and a meta-analysis. Development and
lies. If additional studies are able to replicate these Psychopathology, 23, 39–52.
findings with different parenting training pro- Bearss, K., Burrell, T. L., Stewart, L., & Scahill, L. (2015a).
grams, a profound impact will be realized in the Parent training in autism spectrum disorder: What’s in
a name? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review,
field. It is quite possible that specific parent train-
18, 170–182.
ing programs will be found to not only impact a Bearss, K., Johnson, C., Smith, T., Leavalier, L., Swiezy,
child’s behavior but also their future health. N., Aman, M., et al. (2015b). Effect of parent training
vs. parent education on behavioral problems in children
with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized trial.
Journal of the American Medical Association, 313,
Conclusion 1524–1533.
Beaudoin, A. J., Sebire, G., & Coutre, M. (2014). Parent
Parent training has made tremendous advances training interventions for toddlers with autism spec-
trum disorder. Autism Research and Treatment. https://
since the early work of Gerald Patterson and
doi.org/10.1155/2014/839890
Constance Hanf over 50 years ago. It continues to Bellando, J., Fussell, J., & Lopez, M. (2015). Autism
be a dynamic and ever-expanding intervention Speaks toolkits: Resources for busy clinicians.
across different target populations around the Clinical Pediatrics, 55(2), 171–175.
Berkovits, M. D., O’Brien, K. A., Carter, C. G., & Eyberg,
world. The research base supporting its effective-
S. M. (2010). Early identification and intervention for
ness also continues to expand; however, much behavior problems in primary care: A comparison of
work remains to be done. We need to better under- two abbreviated versions of parent-child interaction
stand the moderators and mediators that impact therapy. Behavior Therapy, 41, 375–387.
Bradley, M. C., & Mandel, D. (2005). Oppositional defi-
the effectiveness of parent training programs. We
ant disorder: A systematic review of evidence of
need to better understand what works best for intervention effectiveness. Journal of Experimental
whom. Programs also need to evolve to meet the Criminology, 1, 343–365.
changing needs and multiple-service delivery sys- Breiner, J., & Forehand, R. (1981). An assessment of the
effects of parent training on clinic-referred children’s
tems within our society. We also need to continue
school behavior. Behavioral Assessment, 3, 31–42.
to improve our understanding of how to more Brestan, E. V., & Eyberg, S. M. (1998). Effective psy-
effectively disseminate programs and take them chosocial treatments of conduct disordered children
to scale. and adolescents: 29 years, 82 studies, and 5,272 kids.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 180–189.
Overall, the future of parent training continues
Breton, A., & Tonge, B. (2005). Preschoolers with autism:
to look bright as the field explores ways to An education and skills training programme for par-
increase the effectiveness of an approach that is ents. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
still considered the treatment of choice for vari- Brinkmeyer, M. Y., & Eyberg, S. M. (2003). Parent-child
interaction therapy for oppositional children. In A. E.
ous child behavior problems.
Kazdin & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psycho-
therapies for children and adolescents (pp. 204–223).
New York, NY: Guilford.
Brookman-Frazee, L., Stahmer, A., Baker-Ericzen,
References M. J., & Tsai, K. (2006). Parenting interventions for
children with autism spectrum and disruptive behav-
Arnold, J. E., Levine, A. G., & Patterson, G. R. (1975). ior disorders: Opportunities for cross-fertilization.
Changes in sibling behavior following family inter- Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 9,
vention. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 43(5), 181–200.
683–688. Bryant, D., Vissard, L. H., Willoughby, M., & Kupersmidt,
Assemany, A. E., & McIntoh, D. E. (2002). Negative J. (1999). A review of interventions for preschool-
treatment outcomes of behavioral parent training pro- ers with aggressive and disruptive behavior. Early
grams. Psychology in the Schools, 39(2), 209–218. Education and Development, 10(1), 47–68.
Parent Training Interventions 83

Cedar, B., & Levant, R. F. (1990). A meta-analysis of the Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
effects of parent effectiveness training. The American 11(2), 130–137.
Journal of Family Therapy, 18(4), 373–384. Eyberg, S. M., Nelson, M. M., & Boggs, S. R. (2008).
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2009). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children
Parent training programs: Insight for practitioners. and adolescents with disruptive behavior. Journal of
Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control. Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37(1),
Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically 215–237.
supported psychological interventions: Controversies Farmer, E. M. Z., Compton, S. N., Burns, B. J., &
and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, Robertson, E. (2002). Review of the evidence
685–716. base for treatment of childhood psychopathology:
Charach, A., Carson, P., Fox, S., Alie, M. U., Beckett, J., Externalizing disorders. Journal of Consulting and
& Lim, C. G. (2013). Interventions for preschool chil- Clinical Psychology, 70(6), 1267–1302.
dren at high risk for ADHD: A comparative effective- Forehand, R., & Kotchick, B. A. (1996). Cultural diver-
ness review. Pediatrics, 131, e1584–e1604. sity: A wake-up call for parent training. Behavior
Chorpita, B. F., Yim, L. M., Donkervoet, J. C., Arensdorf, Therapy, 27(2), 187–206.
A., Amundsen, M. J., McGee, C., et al. (2002). Toward Forehand, R., & Long, N. (2010). Parenting the strong-­
large-scale implementation of empirically supported willed child (3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw Hill.
treatments for children: A review and observations Forehand, R. L., & McMahon, R. J. (1981). Helping the
by the Hawaii system of care. Clinical Psychology noncompliant child. New York, NY: Guilford.
Science and Practice, 9, 165–190. Forehand, R., Sturgis, E. T., McMahon, R. J., Aguar, D.,
Chorpita, B. F., Daleiden, E. L., Ebesutani, C., Young, Green, K., Wells, K. C., et al. (1979). Parent behavioral
J., Becker, K. D., Nakamura, B. J., et al. (2011). training to modify child noncompliance: Treatment
Evidence-based treatments for children and adoles- generalization across time and from home to school.
cents: An updated review of indicators of efficacy Behavior Modification, 3, 3–25.
and effectiveness. Clinical Psychology Science and Forehand, R., Dorsey, S., Jones, D., Long, N., &
Practice, 18, 154–172. McMahon, R. J. (2010). Adherence and flexibility:
Chowdhury, M., Aman, M. G., Scahill, L., et al. (2010). The They can (and do) co-exist! Clinical Psychology:
home situations questionnaire-PDD version. Journal of Science and Practice, 17(3), 258–264.
Intellectual Disabilities Research, 54, 281–291. Forehand, R., Merchant, M.J., Parent, J., Long, N., Linnea,
Chronis, A. M., Chacko, A., Fabiano, G. A., Wymbs, B. T., K., & Baer, J. (2011). An examination of a group cur-
& Pelham, W. E. (2004). Enhancements to the standard riculum for parents of young children with disruptive
behavioral parent training paradigm for families of chil- behavior. Behavior Modification, 35(3), 235–251.
dren with ADHD: Review and future directions. Clinical Forehand, R., Lafko, N., Parent, J., & Burt, K. B. (2014).
Child and Family Psychology Review, 7(1), 1–27. Is parenting the mediator of change in behavioral
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power and analysis for parent training for externalizing problems of youth?
the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 608–619.
Erlbaum. Gardner, F., Montgomery, P., & Knerr, W. (2016).
Conners, N. A., Edwards, M. C., & Grant, A. S. (2007). An Transporting evidence-based parenting programs for
evaluation of a parenting class curriculum for parents child problem behavior (age 3–10) between countries:
of young children: Parenting the strong-willed child. Systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 16, 321–330. Child & Adolescent Psychology, 45(6), 749–762.
Corcoran, J., & Dattalo, P. (2006). Parent involvement in Gordon, T. (1970). P.E.T.: Parent effectiveness training.
treatment for ADHD: A meta-analysis of the published New York, NY: Peter Wyden.
studies. Research on Social Work Practice, 16(6), Gordon, D. A. (2000). Parent training via CD-ROM:
561–570. Using technology to disseminate effective preven-
Dembo, M. H., Sweitzer, M., & Lauritzen, P. (1985). An tion practices. Journal of Primary Prevention, 21(2),
evaluation of group parent education: Behavioral, 227–251.
PET, and Adlerian programs. Review of Educational Graziano, A. M., & Diament, D. M. (1992). Parent
Research, 55(2), 155–200. behavioral training: An examination of the paradigm.
Dretzke, J., Davenport, C., Few, E., Barlow, J., Stewart-­ Behavior Modification, 16(1), 3–38.
Brown, S., Bayliss, S., et al. (2009). The clinical Hamilton, L. M. (2008). Facing autism: Giving parents
effectiveness of different parenting programmes for reasons for hope and guidance for help. New York,
children with conduct problems: A systematic review NY: Random House Digital.
of randomized controlled trials. Child and Adolescent Hanf, C., & Kling, J. (1973). Facilitating parent–child
Psychiatry and Mental Health, 3, 7. interaction: A two-stage training model. Unpublished
Dumas, J. E. (1989). Treating antisocial behavior in manuscript, University of Oregon Medical School,
children: Child and family approaches. Clinical Portland, Oregon.
Psychology Review, 9, 197–222. Hawkins, R. P., Peterson, R. F., Schweid, E., & Bijou, S. W.
Eyberg, S. M., & Robinson, E. A. (1982). Parent-child (1966). Behavior therapy in the home: Amelioration
interaction training: Effects on family functioning. of problem parent-child relations with the parent in
84 N. Long et al.

a therapeutic role. Journal of Experimental Child Khandaker, G. M., Pearson, R. M., Zammit, S., Lewis, G.,
Psychology, 4, 99–107. & Jones, P. B. (2014). Association of serum interleukin
Humphreys, L., Forehand, R., McMahon, R. J., & Roberts, 6 and c-reactive protein in childhood with depression
M. W. (1978). Parent behavioral training to modify and psychosis in young adult life: A population-­based
child noncompliance: Effects on untreated siblings. longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry, published
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental online August 13, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1001/
Psychiatry, 9(3), 235–238. jamapsychiatry.2014.1332
Institute of Medicine and National Research Council. Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. L., & Carter,
(2014). Considerations in applying benefit-cost analy- C. M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention 1:
sis to preventive interventions for children, youth, and Overview of approach. Journal of the Association for
families: Workshop summary. Washington, DC: The Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24, 174–185.
National Academies Press. Kohut, C. S., & Andrews, J. (2004). The efficacy of parent
Johnson, C. A., & Katz, R. C. (1973). Using parents as training programs for ADHD children: A fifteen-year
change agents for their children: A review. Journal of review. Developmental Disabilities Bulletin, 32(2),
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14(3), 181–200. 155–172.
Johnson, C. R., Foldes, E., DeMand, A., & Brooks, M. M. Kotchick, B. A., Shaffer, A., Dorsey, S., & Forehand,
(2015). Behavioral parent training to address feeding R. L. (2004). Parenting antisocial children and adoles-
problems in children with autism spectrum disorder: cents. In M. Hoghughi & N. Long (Eds.), Handbook of
A pilot trial. Journal of Developmental and Physical parenting: Theory and research for practice (pp. 256–
Disabilities, 27, 591–607. 275). London, UK: Sage.
Jones, D. J., Forehand, R., Cuellar, J., Parent, J., Honeycutt, Lajonchere, C., Jones, N., Coury, D., & Perrin, J. (2012).
A., Khavjou, O., … Newey, G. A. (2014). Technology- Leadership in health care, research, and qual-
enhanced program for child disruptive behavior disor- ity improvements for children and adolescents
ders: Development and pilot randomized control trial. with autism spectrum disorders: Autism Treatment
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Network and Autism Intervention Research Network
43(1), 88–101. on Physical Health. Pediatrics, 130, S62–S68.
Kaehler, L. A., Jacobs, M., & Jones, D. J. (2016). Lavigne, J. V., LeBailly, S. A., Gouze, K. R., Cicchetti,
Distilling common history and practice elements to C., Pochyly, J., Arend, R., … Binns, H. J. (2008).
inform dissemination: Hanf-model BPT programs as Treating oppositional defiant disorder in primary care:
an example. Clinical Child and Family Psychological A comparison of three models. Journal of Pediatric
Review, 19, 236–258. Psychology, 33(5), 449–461.
Kaminski, J. W., Valle, L. A., Filene, J. H., & Boyle, Lee, P., Niew, W., Yang, H., Chen, V. C., & Lin, K.
C. L. (2008). A meta-analytic review of components (2012). A meta-analysis of behavioral parent train-
associated with parent training program effectiveness. ing for children with attention deficit hyperactivity
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 567–589. disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33,
Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2006). Joint atten- 2040–2049.
tion and symbolic play in young children with autism: Lee, D., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., Notterman,
A randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of D., & Garfinkel, I. (2013). The great recession, genetic
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 611–620. sensitivity, and maternal harsh parenting. Proceedings
Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A., Paparella, T., Hellemann, G., of the National Academy of Sciences 2013. PNAS
& Berry, K. (2015). Randomized comparative effi- Early Edition accessed on 8/6/13 at www.pnas.org/
cacy study of parent-mediated interventions for tod- cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1312398110
dlers with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Long, N. (1997). Parent education/training in the USA:
Psychology, 83, 554–563. Current status and future trends. Clinical Child
Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behav- Psychology and Psychiatry, 2, 501–515.
ior in children: Current status and future directions. Long, N. (2004). E-parenting. In M. Hoghughi & N. Long
Psychological Bulletin, 102(2), 187–203. (Eds.), Handbook of parenting: Theory and research
Kazdin, A. E. (1997). Parent management train- for practice (pp. 369–379). London, UK: Sage
ing: Evidence, outcomes, and issues. Journal of Publications.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Lonigan, C. J., Elbert, J. C., & Johnson, S. B. (1998).
Psychiatry, 36, 1349–1356. Empirically supported psychosocial interventions
Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Treatment of conduct disorders. for children: An overview. Journal of Clinical Child
In J. Hill & B. Maughan (Eds.), Conduct disorders Psychology, 27(2), 138–145.
in childhood and adolescence. Cambridge, UK: Lopez, M., & Bellando, J. (2012). An informal survey
Cambridge University Press. on family/caregiver wishes for children with autism
Keogel, R. L., O’Dell, M. C., & Koegel, L. K. (1987). in Arkansas. The Journal of the Arkansas Medical
A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal Society., 109(7), 137–139.
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, R.
Disorders, 17, 187–200. (1973). Some generalizations and follow-up measures
Parent Training Interventions 85

on autistic children in behavior therapy. Journal of Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 131–166. 2014. PNAS Early Edition accessed on 7/21/14 at
Lundahl, B., Risser, H. J., & Lovejoy, M. C. (2006a). A www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1406578111
meta-analysis of parent training: Moderators and follow- Mooney, S. (1995). Parent training: A review of Adlerian,
up effects. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 86–104. parent effectiveness training and behavioral research.
Lundahl, B. W., Nimer, J., & Parsons, B. (2006b). The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for
Preventing child abuse: A meta-analysis of parent Couples and Families, 3(3), 218–230.
training programs. Research on Social Work Practice, Morawska, A., & Sanders, M. R. (2006). Self-administered
16(3), 251–262. behavioural family intervention for parents of toddlers:
Maughan, D. R., Christiansen, E., Jenson, W. R., Effectiveness and dissemination. Behaviour Research
Olympia, D., & Clark, E. (2005). Behavioral parent and Therapy, 44, 1839–1848.
training as a treatment for externalizing behaviors and Morawska, A., Tometzki, H., & Sanders, M. R. (2014). An
disruptive behavior disorders: A meta-analysis. School evaluation of the efficacy of a Triple P-Positive Parenting
Psychology Review, 34(3), 267–286. Program podcast series. Journal of Developmental and
Mazzucchelli, T. G., & Sanders, M. R. (2010). Facilitating Behavioral Pediatrics, 35(2), 128–137.
practitioner flexibility within an empirically supported Moreland, J. R., Schwebel, A. I., Beck, S., & Well, R.
intervention: Lessons from a system of parenting sup- (1982). Parents as therapists: A review of the behav-
port. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 17, ior therapy parent training literature-1975–1981.
238–252. Behavior Modification, 6(2), 250–276.
McAuley, R. (1982). Training parents to modify con- National Autism Center. (2009). National standards proj-
duct problems in their children. Journal of Child ect phase 1. Available from http://www.nationalau-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 23, 335–342. tismcenter.org/national-standards-project [internet
McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long-­ cited Oct 17, 2016).
term outcome for children with autism who received National Autism Center. (2015). National standards proj-
early intensive behavioral treatment. American ect phase 2. Available from http://www.nationalau-
Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(4), 359–372. tismcenter.org/national-standards-project [internet
McGrath, P. J., Lingley-Pottie, P., Ristkari, T., cited Oct 17, 2016].
Cunningham, C., Huttunen, J., Filbert, K., & Watters, Nixon, R. D. V. (2002). Treatment of behavior problems
C. (2013). Remote population-based intervention for in preschoolers: A review of parent training programs.
disruptive behavior at age four: Study protocol for a Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 525–546.
randomized trial of internet-assisted parent training NSP2. (2015). National Standards Project Phase 2.
(Strongest Families Finland-Canada). BMC Public Available from http://www.nationalautismcenter.org/
Health, 13, 985. national-standards-project [internet cited October 17,
McMahon, R. J., & Forehand, R. L. (2003). Helping the 2016]
noncompliant child: Family-based treatment for oppo- O’Dell, S. L. (1974). Training parents in behavior modifi-
sitional behavior. New York: Guilford. cation: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 418–433.
McMahon, R. J., & Wells, K. C. (1998). Conduct prob- Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene,
lems. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Treatment OR: Catalia.
of childhood disorders (2nd ed., pp. 111–207). Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., Jones, R. R., & Conger, R.
New York, NY: Guilford. (1975). A social learning approach to family interven-
McNeil, C. B., & Hembree-Kigin, T. L. (2010). Parent-­ tion (Vol. 1). Eugene, OR: Castalia.
child interaction therapy (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Pedersen, T. (2013). Brain chemical genes influence sensi-
Springer. tivity to reward, punishment. Psych Central. Retrieved
McNeil, C. B., Eyberg, S., Eisenstadt, T. H., Newcomb, on March 17, 2014, from http://psychcentral.com/
K., & Funderburk, B. (1991). Parent-child inter- news/2013/11/24/brain-chemical-genes-influence-
action therapy with behavior problem children: sensitivity-to-reward-punishment/62417.html
Generalization of treatment effects to the school set- Pelham, W. E., & Fabiano, G. A. (2008). Evidence-­
ting. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20(2), based psychosocial treatments for attention-deficit/
140–151. hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and
Metinga, A. T. A., Orobio de Castro, B., & Matthys, W. Adolescent Psychology, 37(1), 184–214.
(2013). Effectiveness of the incredible years par- Pelham, W. E., Wheeler, T., & Chronis, A. (1998).
ent training to modify disruptive and prosocial child Empirically supported psychsocial treatment for atten-
behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology tion deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical
Review, 33, 901–913. Child Psychology, 27(2), 190–205.
Miller, G. E., & Prinz, R. J. (1990). Enhancement of social Purdie, N., Hattie, J., & Carrol, A. (2002). A review of
learning family interventions for childhood conduct the research on interventions for attention deficit
disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 108(2), 291–307. hyperactivity disorder: What works best? Review of
Miller, G. E., Brody, G. H., Yu, T., & Chen, E. (2014). Educational Research, 72(1), 61–99.
A family-oriented psychosocial intervention reduces Reitman, D., & McMahon, R. J. (2013). Constance
inflammation in low-SES African American youth. “Connie” Hanf (1917–2002): The mentor and the
86 N. Long et al.

model. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 20, Tonge, B., Brereton, A., Kiomall, M., Mackinnon, A.,
106–116. King, N., & Rinehart, N. (2006). Effects on paren-
Sanders, M. R., & Murphy-Brennan, M. (2010). The tal mental health of an education and skills training
international dissemination of the Triple P – Positive program for parents of young children with autism: A
Parenting Program. In J. Weisz & A. Kazdin (Eds.), randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American
Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(5),
adolescents (pp. 519–537). New York, NY: Guilford 561–569.
Press. Travormina, J. B. (1974). Basic models of parent counsel-
Sanders, M. R., & Ralph, A. (2004). Towards a multi-­level ing: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 81(11),
model of parenting intervention. In M. Hoghughi & 827–855.
N. Long (Eds.), Handbook of parenting: Theory and Wahler, R. C., Winkel, G. H., Peterson, R. F., & Morrison,
research for practice (pp. 352–368). London, UK: D. C. (1965). Mothers as behavior therapists for their
Sage. own children. Behavior Research and Therapy, 3,
Sanders, M. R., Baker, S., & Turner, K. M. T. (2012). A 113–134.
randomized controlled trial evaluating the efficacy of Wallace, K. S., & Rogers, S. (2010). Intervening in
Triple P Online with parents of children with early-­ infancy: Implications for autism spectrum disorder.
onset conduct problems. Behaviour Research and The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Therapy, 50, 675–684. 51(12), 1300–1320.
Sanders, M. R., Kirby, J. N., Tellegen, C. L., & Day, J. J. Webster-Stratton, C. (1991). Annotation: Strategies for
(2014). The Triple P-Positive Parenting Program: A helping families with conduct disordered children.
systematic review and meta-analysis of a multi-level Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7),
system of parenting support. Clinical Psychology 1047–1062.
Review, 34(4), 337–357. Webster-Stratton, C., Kolpacoff, M., & Hollinsworth, T.
Serketich, W. J., & Dumas, J. E. (1996). The effective- (1988). Self-administered videotape therapy for fami-
ness of behavioral parent training to modify antiso- lies with conduct-problem children: Comparison with
cial behavior in children: A meta-analysis. Behavior two cost-effective treatments and a control group.
Therapy, 27, 171–186. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(4),
Shire, S. Y., Gulsrud, A., & Kasari, C. (2016). 558–566.
Increasing responsive parent-child interactions and Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M.J. (2003). The incred-
joint engagement: Comparing the influence of parent ible years: parents, teachers, and children training
mediated intervention and parent psychoeducation. series. In A.E. Kazdin & J.R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, based psychotherapies for children and adolescents
1737–1747. (pp. 224–240). New York: Guilford Press.
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Daley, D., Thompson, M., Laver-­ Wells, K. C., Forehand, R. L., & Griest, D. L. (1980).
Bradbury, C., & Weeks, A. (2001). Parent-based Generality of treatment effects from treated to
therapies for preschool attention-deficit/hyperactivity untreated behaviors resulting from a parent training
disorder: A randomized, controlled trial with a com- program. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
munity sample. Journal of the American Academy of Psychology, 9(3), 217–219.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(4), 402–408. Wiese, M. R. (1992). A critical review of parent training
Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination of research. Psychology in Schools, 29(3), 229–236.
Psychological Procedures. (1995). Training in and Wiese, M. R., & Kramer, J. J. (1988). Parent training
dissemination of empirically-validated psychologi- research: An analysis of the empirical literature from
cal treatments: report and recommendations. Clinical 1975–1985. Psychology in the Schools, 25(3), 325–330.
Psychology, 48(1), 3–23. Williams, C. D. (1959). The elimination of tantrum behav-
Tharp, R. G., & Wetzel, R. J. (1969). Behavior modifi- iors by extinction procedures. Journal of Abnormal
cation in the natural environment. New York, NY: and Social Psychology, 59, 269–270.
Academic. Williams, P. G., Dalrymple, N., & Neal, J. (2000). Eating
Thomas, R., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2007). habits of children with autism. Pediatric Nursing,
Behavioral outcomes of parent-child interaction 26(3), 259–264.
therapy and Triple P-Positive Parenting Program: A Zwi, M., Jones, H., Thorgaard, C., York, A., & Dennis,
review and meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child J. A. (2011). Parent training interventions for attention
Psychology, 35, 475–495. deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children 5 to
Todres, R., & Bunston, T. (1993). Parent education pro- 18 years. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,
gram evaluation: A review of the literature. Canadian (12), CD003018. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.
Journal of Community Mental Health, 12(1), 225–257. CD003018.pub3
Cognition and Memory

Corey I. McGill and Emily M. Elliott

Contents area of research focus. What is clear is that chil-


Cognitive Development............................................  87 dren become more organized and efficient with
their cognitive capabilities. In this chapter we
Functional Changes in Attention
and Strategy Use...................................................  87
will broadly review a few areas that have been
studied. These include functional improvements
Rehearsal...................................................................  90
in attention and strategy use, as well as potential
Structural Increases in Working Memory structural changes in the capacity of working
and Retrieval Efficiency in Long-Term memory, and the efficiency of retrieval from
Memory.................................................................  91
long-term memory.
Capacity Changes in Working Memory.................  91
Episodic Memory......................................................  92
Semantic Memory.....................................................  92
 unctional Changes in Attention
F
and Strategy Use
Final Conclusions and Directions
for Future Research..............................................  93
Attention
References.................................................................  94
One commonly asserted cause of cognitive
improvements across development is a greater
ability to avoid distraction from irrelevant stimuli
Cognitive Development in the environment. While attention as a construct
can be broadly conceived, the focus of this sec-
As children develop, their performance on a tion will be on selective attention, which we
number of cognitive tasks improves. Generally, define here as the ability to selectively attend to
this is due to an increase in cognitive functioning relevant stimuli while ignoring irrelevant stimuli.
as children age, but what elements specifically The ability to select which items a child pays
contribute to this development is still a major attention to can greatly impact their ability to
focus and complete tasks in the face of external
distracting stimuli.
C.I. McGill • E.M. Elliott (*)
One area of research has explored the early
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA development of attentional processes, starting
e-mail: eelliott@lsu.edu with infants. Evidence suggests that infants

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 87


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_6
88 C.I. McGill and E.M. Elliott

d­ emonstrate facilitative selective attention effects of ink a word is written in; however, the word
very early on, and as early as 4 months old, they itself may be congruent with the ink color or
also begin to develop a form of selective attention incongruent with the ink color. In order to com-
that includes both orienting to a target and sup- plete the task, individuals must hypothetically
pression of distractor information (Markant & suppress the automatic response that occurs when
Amso, 2016). For example, during a visual search reading a word and instead respond with the
task, infants are presented with a visual array, color the word is written in. One interpretation of
such as images of cats and dogs, and must iden- this finding is that the prepotent response sup-
tify a specific item in the array. During the task, pression requires that the individual control their
infants must focus their attention to identify the attention and avoid the automatic processing of
target rapidly and avoid searching the same area the irrelevant word for meaning (e.g., Wright,
for a target more than once. This attentional 2016). Children who have learned to read show
mechanism is known as inhibition of return, and significantly longer response times when com-
it has been demonstrated by studying the eye pared to adults, until they reach around age 8
movements of 4-month-old children (Markant & (Schiller, 1966). The work of Wright (2016) indi-
Amso). However, despite the impressive ability cated that children were more likely than adults
to demonstrate inhibition of return at a very to commit the error of inadvertent reading, which
young age, children continue to experience occurs when participants read the written word,
­significant improvements in selective attention rather than the task of naming the color of the
throughout development. word. This finding was interpreted with respect
to attentional control difficulties in children, rela-
tive to adults, who are better able to avoid inad-
Attention Capture vertently reading the word and maintain the task
goal of naming the word color (see also Kane &
There are multiple ways that attention can be ori- Engle, 2003; MacLeod & MacDonald, 2000).
ented from one target to another. Attention cap- Additional support for the development of
ture is the automatic focusing of attention on a selective attention abilities in children comes
stimulus. For example, pop-out effects occur from a paradigm known as the irrelevant sound
when a target in a visual search task has some effect (ISE). The ISE refers to the worsened per-
unique visual feature, distinct from nontargets in formance on serial order recall during the pres-
the array. In these instances, individuals are faster ence of changing auditory stimuli, relative to
to identify the unique target in the visual array silence. In adults, the ISE has been shown not to
than to identify the target when nontarget stimuli be caused by attention capture, in part due to
share many features. Additionally, the number of findings that illustrate the lack of a relationship
items in the array does not affect response time in with individual differences in a measure of atten-
pop-out trials, but does affect response times in tional control (e.g., working memory capacity;
control trials. These pop-out effects have been Elliott & Briganti, 2012). However, when chil-
shown in both children and adults, indicating that dren perform serial recall, they show markedly
the individual’s attention is being automatically larger effects of the irrelevant sound compared to
drawn to the visually distinct stimulus, even in adults. This developmental difference is thought
young children (Gerhardstein & Rovee-Collier, to be caused by an attentional effect occurring in
2002). children that is not occurring in adults (Elliott,
However, despite children showing a similar 2002; Elliott & Briganti; Elliott et al., 2016).
effect of attention capture when attention capture Children are unable to avoid the distracting
can facilitate performance and when attention effects of the irrelevant stimuli; thus they demon-
capture harms performance, children perform strate not only the typical ISE that is observed in
worse than adults. For example, on Stroop tasks, adults but also an additional effect of attention
individuals are typically asked to name the color capture. Once children mature beyond around
Cognition and Memory 89

age 7, the additional effect of attention capture Strategy Use


during the ISE is lessened, until the magnitude of
the disruptive effect on serial recall becomes Another area of well-known growth and change
adult-like (Elliott). is in the development of strategies to improve
Additionally, children with auditory process- performance on cognitive tasks. Strategies like
ing disorders (APD) demonstrate differential rehearsal, visual imagery, mnemonics, categori-
effects of auditory distractors than their typi- zation, and others all improve/develop with expe-
cally developing peers. While typically develop- rience. However, children have difficulty in
ing children show a smaller magnitude ISE transferring skills from the learning context to a
when the distracting auditory stimuli are tones, new context (Clerc, Miller, & Cosnefroy, 2014),
children with APD showed no difference but transfer of cognitive abilities from the learned
between tones and words as auditory stimuli in context to the real world is important for develop-
the ISE paradigm (Elliott, Bhagat, & Lynn, ment. This becomes an issue due to changes in
2007). These results suggested that children setting from acquisition of a strategy to use in
with APD process auditory stimuli differently, day-to-day life. Effective cognitive strategies are
without the same type of differentiation between often useful in a number of different settings, but
speech and tone stimuli seen in typically devel- these changes in setting may result in inconsis-
oping peers. Overall, the children showed sig- tent strategy usage and/or efficacy.
nificant effects of the distracting stimuli, which Two types of similarities across situations can
were irrelevant to the task. This pattern of irrel- impact the transfer from strategy learning to strat-
evant stimuli influencing children’s perfor- egy application. Structural similarities are simi­
mance occurs in visual stimuli as well and is not larities between the learned context and applied
restricted to the auditory domain (e.g., Enns & context that facilitate the transfer of a strategy.
Akhtar, 1989). Superficial similarities are similarities between the
Thus, it is clear that while children grow, their learned context and applied context that do not
attention develops and changes as well. For facilitate the transfer of a strategy. For example,
example, children and adults control attention to similar task demands would be an example of a
avoid repeated searching (Markant & Amso, structural similarity, while location may only be a
2016), and both have their attention captured by superficial similarity. Chi and VanLehn (2012) pro-
similar stimuli, as evidenced by the early onset of posed that children must evaluate a situation to
pop-out effects (Gerhardstein & Rovee-Collier, determine which similarities are structural and
2002). Furthermore, researchers have suggested which are superficial for successful strategy trans-
that children and adolescents with a diagnosed fer. Higher cognitive demands, such as an increased
anxiety disorder do not differ from matched con- working memory load within a given task, have
trols in their performance on a basic visual search been demonstrated to limit transfer effects, possi-
task and the pop-out effect (Lubow, Toren, Laor, bly by interfering with the ability to identify and
& Kaplan, 2000). However, attentional processes categorize similarities across contexts.
also go through a number of changes during However, once children identify the opportu-
development. Children show larger attention cap- nity to apply a previously learned strategy to a
ture effects to the same stimuli as adults in Stroop new context, they still must appropriately apply
tasks (Wright, 2016) or even show attention cap- that strategy. Strategy utilization deficiency
ture effects where adults show none, like in serial (Miller, 1990) occurs when children can appro-
recall tasks with irrelevant sounds (Elliott et al., priately identify a strategy to use but demonstrate
2016). Without these improvements of selective either no effect of the strategy on performance
attention throughout childhood, adults would be compared to control children who did not learn
easily distracted by environmental stimuli to a the strategy or do not show the expected increase
much larger degree. in performance typically shown in other children.
90 C.I. McGill and E.M. Elliott

Causes of strategy utilization difficulty include Recent research, however, has begun to ques-
high cognitive load, low intelligence, poor meta- tion this assertion of rehearsal development in
cognition, and a weak knowledge base. young children. First, a reexamination of the PSE
revealed that prior work using the raw difference
between phonologically similar and dissimilar
Rehearsal stimuli did not lead to an accurate representation
of the PSE (Jarrold & Citroën, 2013). By taking
One example of a commonly studied strategy into account baseline levels of recall, and reex-
during development is subvocal rehearsal. amining the PSE as a proportional effect, it was
Subvocal rehearsal is the covert recitation of rel- shown that children as young as 5 demonstrated
evant to-be-remembered stimuli. Flavell, Beach, the effect. Furthermore, the effect was equivalent
and Chinsky (1966) demonstrated that before age in magnitude to older children and adults.
7, most children do not demonstrate key indica- Additionally, even children as young as 5 have
tors of rehearsal. For example, kindergarten-aged been shown to demonstrate improvements due to
children performing a serial order recall task did training on rehearsal, and those improvements
not verbalize during the presentation of stimuli, are equivalent to the improvements demonstrated
during the delay before recall, or during the recall by 8- and 9-year-old children who are tradition-
period as often as second or fifth grade students ally believed to demonstrate rehearsal skills with-
(Flavell et al., 1966). Additionally, children who out being explicitly taught (Miller, McCulloch, &
were identified as producing those verbalizations Jarrold, 2015).
have been shown to perform significantly better Finally, research examining the PSE in typi-
on serial recall tasks. Training children to cally developing children, children with Down
rehearse produced levels of serial recall that were syndrome, and children with Williams syndrome
equivalent to those rehearsing originally (Keeney, revealed some unexpected findings (Danielsson,
Cannizzo, & Flavell, 1967). Henry, Messer, Carney, & Rönnberg, 2016).
The hypothesis that young children do not Although the verbal abilities of the children with
rehearse was further supported by evidence from Williams syndrome matched those of the typi-
research on the phonological similarity effect cally developing children, the results indicated
(PSE). The PSE is the lowered recall of to-be-­ no effect of phonological similarity on recall for
remembered items when those items share pho- the children with Williams syndrome. However,
nological characteristics with each other, such as both the typically developing children and the
lists of rhyming letters like “B,” “V,” and “G,” children with Down syndrome showed a signifi-
when compared to phonologically distinct items, cant PSE. The results of Danielsson et al. (2016)
such as “X,” “L,” and “R.” The multicomponent suggest that the link between phonological recod-
model of working memory (Baddeley, 2000) pro- ing, rehearsal abilities, and memory span perfor-
posed that the PSE is a rehearsal effect caused by mance in children with Down syndrome and
phonologically similar items interfering with Williams syndrome needs additional consider-
each other during rehearsal, because they are ation, especially as compared to typically devel-
being coded in a verbal or acoustically based oping peers.
form that relies upon the sound of the item. Research into strategy use in children has
Before age 7, children have been shown not to revealed a number of important findings regard-
demonstrate a PSE (Hitch, Halliday, Schaafstal, ing the ability of children to benefit from these
& Heffernan, 1991). Taken with the earlier work strategies, with a large number of studies con-
of Flavell and colleagues (Flavell et al., 1966; ducted in the areas of children’s rehearsal and
Keeney et al., 1967), this finding was tradition- metacognitive awareness of their own memory
ally seen as evidence for the development of performance (e.g., Baker-Ward, Ornstein, &
rehearsal as an effective strategy occurring Holden, 1984; Flavell, Friedrichs, & Hoyt, 1970;
around the age of 7. Miller et al., 2015). The consensus of these stud-
Cognition and Memory 91

ies suggests that very young children, such as the serial order of information while simultane-
3–4-year-olds, can engage in strategic behaviors ously processing stimuli (Foster, Shipstead,
to attempt to remember information and that Harrison, Hicks, Redick, & Engle, 2015). For
these behaviors reliably improve with increasing example, in the popular symmetry span task, a
age. However, the mechanisms underlying these participant would identify the vertical symmetry
developmental improvements remain an area for of a visual stimulus and then remember the serial
future research. As an interventional strategy for order of red squares on a 4 × 4 grid. This cycle
improving memory performance, overt training would repeat until three to seven squares were
of rehearsal-type skills seems to improve perfor- displayed. The processing component of com-
mance in both typically developing (Keeney plex span tasks (e.g., the symmetry judgment)
et al., 1967; Miller et al., 2015) and intellectually requires the individual to devote all or most of
disabled children (Belmont & Butterfield, 1971). their WM resources to the task and limits the
number of strategies the individual can use to
recall the stored items (e.g., the serial order of the
 tructural Increases in Working
S red squares).
Memory and Retrieval Efficiency Complex span tasks with stimuli that are
in Long-Term Memory designed for use in adults often produce floor
effects with children, so modified tasks are used
Working Memory (e.g., Case, Kurland & Goldberg, 1982; Kail &
Hall, 1999). The need for differential tasks sug-
Working memory (WM) is the currently activated gests a distinct developmental difference in WM
portion of memory at any given time. We use capacity as children age. One explanation for this
WM not only to briefly store information for change is an actual improvement in the size of
short-term memory tasks but also to store infor- WM capacity during development. As children
mation from long-term memory when the envi- develop, they improve on almost every measure
ronment elicits a specific memory. The of raw memory span from digit span (Cowan
complexity of WM lends itself to a number of et al., 2005) to visual arrays (Kuhn, 2016).
developmental changes throughout childhood. Additionally, Hitch, Towse, and Hutton (2001)
Additionally, performance on working memory demonstrated an increase in performance on two
tasks has been shown to predict numerous abili- distinct complex span tasks across children aged
ties and aptitudes, including children’s academic 9–11, indicating an increase in working memory
achievement on national tests (Gathercole, capacity as children age. As complex span tasks
Brown, & Pickering, 2003), classroom behavior are thought to limit the ability for strategies, such
(Gathercole, Lamont, & Alloway, 2006), and as rehearsal, to improve memory performance,
high school grade point average (Cowan et al., the fact that WM capacity improves during chil-
2005). dren’s development may suggest that WM capac-
ity is truly growing during childhood (e.g.,
Cowan, 2016b; Cowan, Ricker, Clark, Hinrichs,
 apacity Changes in Working
C & Glass, 2015). However, there are other possi-
Memory ble explanations for the improvements of WM
capacity during development.
WM capacity is measured by identifying the Another explanation for the increase of WM
amount of information an individual can hold in capacity is that individuals develop strategies and
mind, in an immediate sense (see Cowan, 2016a, long-term memory representations to facilitate
for more information on the measurement of recall of items during WM capacity tasks. For
WM capacity). One commonly utilized WM example, adults might be able to devote a small
capacity measure is the complex span task. portion of their resources on rehearsing the stored
Complex span tasks require individuals to store items even during the processing component of
92 C.I. McGill and E.M. Elliott

the task or use some type of semantic memory developing child must store. These memories of
strategy to facilitate later recall. As children are the past are stored in long-term memory.
in the process of discovering, perfecting, and However, while it is difficult to say when memo-
adapting cognitive strategies to improve their ries in WM move to long-term memory, most
memory in the real world (Clerc et al., 2014), cognitive models identify the two as separate
they may also be developing strategies to facili- (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 2016; Baddeley, 2000;
tate memory without tapping into the processes Cowan, 2016a). Long-term memory can be
blocked by the processing component of a com- divided into episodic and semantic memory, and
plex span task. The development of these strate- these constructs will both be discussed, as well as
gies would continue throughout normal a suggestion to improve learning of semantic
development and thus result in improved perfor- memory facts over the course of child
mance on complex span tasks without increased development.
capacity. Additionally, children may be relying
on superficial similarities in strategy selection
over structural similarities (Chi & VanLehn, Episodic Memory
2012), resulting in inefficient strategy utilization
during complex span tasks and lessening The form of long-term memory for personally
performance. experienced events is known as episodic memory.
While it is clear that working memory capac- We are consistently acquiring new episodic
ity increases with development, the precise cause memories, and these can guide the way we think
of this capacity change is debated. Some research- and act by providing important prior information
ers point to an increase in the actual capacity and contextual details. However, in children, epi-
itself (Cowan, 2016a, 2016b; Cowan et al., 2015), sodic memories are significantly less complex
while others suggest that factors such as the utili- (Yim, Dennis, & Sloutsky, 2013), which results
zation of strategies and the increased knowledge in a lessened impact of episodic memories on
base from long-term memory lead to the improve- future behavior. For example, if a child needs to
ments in memory performance (Clerc et al., access information from a particular event, like
2014). Finally, the executive attention view sug- where they left their backpack when they arrived
gests that what may appear as differences in WM home from school, they will have a less complex
capacity are actually differentially efficient pro- representation of arriving home from school and
cesses of WM. In other words, developmental may struggle to remember its location.
changes in WM capacity tasks may be the result Additionally, Yim et al. (2013) suggested that
of increased and improved attentional processes, children may suffer from more interference from
which lead to an improved ability to retain infor- similar episodic events, so children will be less
mation in the short term (Engle & Kane, 2004). able to distinguish their episodic memory for
While there is disagreement as to the cause of arriving home from school today from arriving
these improvements, many agree that the numer- home from school yesterday.
ous connections between the processes of atten-
tion, working memory, and long-term memory
are another key element of the improvements in Semantic Memory
cognitive functioning that are seen in children’s
development. In contrast to episodic memory, semantic mem-
ory is the general knowledge of the world. Adults
use this general world knowledge frequently and
Long-Term Memory efficiently, but children begin with little-to-no
semantic memory to draw upon. As children age,
With increasing life experiences come an they begin to build their semantic memory
increased number of prior memories which a through the use of semantic networks, or a net-
Cognition and Memory 93

work of interrelated concepts and ideas and their cated that children were able to integrate knowl-
meanings. For example, a semantic network of edge and correctly answer the integration
the concept “bird” would include required fea- questions under differing presentation condi-
tures of birds (feathers, wings, etc.), characteris- tions. Because the acquisition of knowledge
tic features of birds (flight), and all of the known through direct instruction is an important means
examples a person has of animals that fit into the of success in school children, it is important to
category of birds (robin, chicken, ostrich, etc.). study these other means of learning, such as the
These semantic networks are intertwined with self-generation of facts from previously learned,
each other and make up a majority of long-term but not previously integrated, information.
memory.
Possibly the most important use of semantic
networks is spreading activation or the idea that  inal Conclusions and Directions
F
when one item in a semantic network is activated, for Future Research
memory-related items also become activated to
facilitate memory of that item (Collins & This chapter cannot be concluded without a dis-
Quillian, 1969; Loftus, 1973). When an individ- cussion of the recent debates regarding the train-
ual thinks about the concept of bird, it is impor- ing of cognitive capabilities. The central
tant to also know the required and characteristic importance of working memory for many higher-­
features of a bird, as well as to have examples of order cognitive abilities has led many researchers
birds readily available in mind. However, despite to test the possibility of training working memory
the importance of semantic memory and spread- in children. If young children could be trained to
ing activation, in adulthood both heavily rely on increase their working memory capacity, the
language. In order to build an efficient semantic potential outcomes cannot be understated.
network, individuals use a linguistic system to However, despite the promise of this area of
organize the information. Despite not having lan- research, efforts have revealed a number of con-
guage to rely on, even infants demonstrate char- flicting findings. A recent issue of Psychological
acteristics of spreading activation (Barr, Walker, Science in the Public Interest was devoted
Gross, & Hayne, 2014). As children’s language entirely to the issue of “brain training,” which
matures, so do the complexity and sophistication extends beyond working memory training into a
of their semantic network, and children begin to larger class of training programs (Simons Boot,
rely on lexical information more to build and Charness, Gathercole, Chabris, Hambrick, &
access their semantic networks. Stine-Morrow, 2016). One of the main problems
Understanding semantic memory networks within this research is determining whether or
can be leveraged to help to teach children to not the trained abilities can transfer to other abili-
increase their ability to learn information and to ties, for example, if learning in one aspect of a
integrate semantic memory details. Research by working memory task could transfer to an entirely
Bauer, Blue, Xu, and Esposito (2016) has demon- new task. Furthermore, researchers have ques-
strated that 7–10-year-old children can learn new tioned if working memory training could even
information at one time and then learn related but lead to improvements in other areas of cognition,
different information at a later time and can dem- such as general fluid intelligence. The conclu-
onstrate “self-generation” of knowledge. For sions from the Simons et al. (2016) review sug-
example, children were exposed to facts, such as gested that there was little evidence of transfer
“A wombat is a marsupial” and “Marsupials keep beyond the tasks being trained. However, research
their babies in a pouch.” Then later, children were has suggested that the individual temperaments
asked through either open-ended questioning or of the children may be an important factor in the
forced-choice recall to answer an integration likelihood of transfer of working memory train-
question, such as “Where do wombats keep their ing to other abilities (Studer-Luethi, Bauer, &
babies?” The results of three experiments indi- Perrig, 2016). Additionally, researchers have
94 C.I. McGill and E.M. Elliott

begun to apply the concept of working memory aged children: Relations with reading comprehension
and deployment of cognitive resources. Developmental
training as an intervention for anxiety and depres-
Psychology, 52, 1024–1037. https://doi.org/10.1037/
sion (de Voogd, Wiers, Zwitser, & Salemink, dev0000130
2016), but this research is at an early stage, and Belmont, J. M., & Butterfield, E. C. (1971). Learning
more work needs to be done. strategies as determinants of memory deficiencies.
Cognitive Psychology, 2, 411–420.
The critical changes that occur in cognitive
Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982).
abilities over the course of child development Operational efficiency and the growth of short-­
help children to become efficient information term memory span. Journal of Experimental
processors as adults. Important functional Child Psychology, 33(3), 386–404. https://doi.
org/10.1016/0022-0965(82)90054-6
improvements in selective attention abilities and
Chi, M. H., & VanLehn, K. A. (2012). Seeing deep
strategy use are reflected in other aspects of the structure from the interactions of surface features.
cognitive system and are connected to develop- Educational Psychologist, 47, 177–188.
mental improvements in working memory capac- Clerc, J., Miller, P. H., & Cosnefroy, L. (2014). Young
children’s transfer of strategies: Utilization defi-
ity, efficient retrieval of facts from long-term
ciencies, executive function, and metacognition.
memory, and the ability to acquire new informa- Developmental Review, 34(4), 378–393. https://doi.
tion into adulthood. These topics are rich areas org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.10.002
for future study, and many scholars are drawing Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time
from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning
upon the well-tested paradigms within cognitive
and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–247. https://doi.
psychology to extend them to clinical popula- org/10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80069-1
tions and to expand the knowledge of the field of Cowan, N. (2016a). Working memory capacity. New York:
child development (e.g., Danielsson et al., 2016; Routledge. [Original edition 2005]: Psychology Press
and Routledge Classic Edition.
Elliott et al., 2007; Jarrold, 2017; Lubow et al.,
Cowan, N. (2016b). Working memory maturation: Can we
2000; Majerus & Cowan, 2016). Additional work get at the essence of cognitive growth? Perspectives on
is needed, with an emphasis on an interdisciplin- Psychological Science, 11, 239–264.
ary approach to unite researchers within the cog- Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Saults, J. S., Morey, C. C.,
Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., & Conway, A. A. (2005).
nitive and clinical traditions.
On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role
in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive
Psychology, 51(1), 42–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
References cogpsych.2004.12.001
Cowan, N., Ricker, T. J., Clark, K. M., Hinrichs, G. A.,
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2016). Human mem- & Glass, B. A. (2015). Knowledge cannot explain the
ory: A proposed system and its control processes. In developmental growth of working memory capacity.
R. J. Sternberg, S. T. Fiske, D. J. Foss, R. J. Sternberg, Developmental Science, 18, 132–145.
S. T. Fiske, & D. J. Foss (Eds.), Scientists making a Danielsson, H., Henry, L., Messer, D., Carney, D. P., &
difference: One hundred eminent behavioral and brain Rönnberg, J. (2016). Developmental delays in phono-
scientists talk about their most important contributions logical recoding among children and adolescents with
(pp. 115–118). New York: Cambridge University Press. down syndrome and Williams syndrome. Research in
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new com- Developmental Disabilities, 55, 64–76.
ponent of working memory? Trends in Cognitive de Voogd, E. L., Wiers, R. W., Zwitser, R. J., & Salemink,
Sciences, 4, 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/ E. (2016). Emotional working memory training as
S1364-6613(00)01538-2 an online intervention for adolescent anxiety and
Baker-Ward, L., Ornstein, P. A., & Holden, D. J. (1984). depression: A randomised controlled trial. Australian
The expression of memorization in early childhood. Journal of Psychology, 68, 228–238.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, Elliott, E. M. (2002). The irrelevant-speech effect and
555–575. children: Theoretical implications of developmental
Barr, R., Walker, J., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2014). change. Memory & Cognition, 30, 478–487. https://
Age-related changes in spreading activation during doi.org/10.3758/BF03194948
infancy. Child Development, 85, 549–563. https://doi. Elliott, E. M., Bhagat, S. P., & Lynn, S. D. (2007). Can
org/10.1111/cdev.12163 children with (central) auditory processing disorders
Bauer, P. J., Blue, S. N., Xu, A., & Esposito, A. G. (2016). ignore irrelevant sounds? Research in Developmental
Productive extension of semantic memory in school- Disabilities, 28, 506–517.
Cognition and Memory 95

Elliott, E. M., & Briganti, A. M. (2012). Investigating the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 70(9),
role of attentional processes in the irrelevant speech 1747–1767. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.12
effect. Acta Psychologica, 140, 64–74. 13869
Elliott, E. M., Hughes, R. W., Briganti, A., Joseph, T. N., Jarrold, C., & Citroën, R. (2013). Reevaluating key evi-
Marsh, J. E., & Macken, B. (2016). Distraction in ver- dence for the development of rehearsal: Phonological
bal short-term memory: Insights from developmental similarity effects in children are subject to propor-
differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 88, tional scaling artifacts. Developmental Psychology,
39–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.12.008 49, 837–847. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028771
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1999). Sources of developmen-
working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of tal change in children’s word-problem performance.
cognitive control. In B. H. Ross, & B. H. Ross (Eds.), Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 660–668.
The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.660
in research and theory, 44 New York: Academic Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory
Press. (pp. 145–199). doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/ capacity and the control of attention: The contribu-
s0079-7421(03)44005-x tions of goal neglect, response competition, and task
Enns, J. T., & Akhtar, N. (1989). A developmental study set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental
of filtering invisual attention. Child Development, 60, Psychology: General, 132, 47–70. https://doi.
1188–1199. org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. R., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Keeney, T. J., Cannizzo, S. R., & Flavell, J. H. (1967).
Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a Spontaneous and induced verbal rehearsal in a recall
function of age. Child Development, 37, 283–299. task. Child Development, 38, 953–966. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1126804 org/10.2307/1127095
Flavell, J. H., Friedrichs, A. G., & Hoyt, J. D. (1970). Kuhn, J. (2016). Controlled attention and storage: An
Developmental changes in memorization processes. investigation of the relationship between working
Cognitive Psychology, 1, 324–340. memory, short-term memory, scope of attention, and
Foster, J. L., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., intelligence in children. Learning and Individual
Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Shortened com- Differences, 52, 167–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
plex span tasks can reliably measure working mem- lindif.2015.04.009
ory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 43(2), 226–236. Loftus, E. F. (1973). Activation of semantic memory. The
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0461-7 American Journal of Psychology, 86, 331–337. https://
Gathercole, S. E., Brown, L., & Pickering, S. J. (2003). doi.org/10.2307/1421441
Working memory assessments at school entry as lon- Lubow, R. E., Toren, P., Laor, N., & Kaplan, O. (2000).
gitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attain- The effects of target and distractor familiarity on
ment levels. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, visual search in anxious children: Latent inhibition
109–122. and novel pop-out. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,
Gathercole, S. E., Lamont, E., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). 14, 41–56.
Working memory in the classroom. In S. Pickering MacLeod, C. M., & MacDonald, P. A. (2000).
(Ed.), Working memory and education (pp. 219– Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect:
240). London: Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi. Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of atten-
org/10.1016/B978-012554465-8/50010-7 tion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 383–391.
Gerhardstein, P., & Rovee-Collier, C. (2002). The Majerus, S., & Cowan, N. (2016). The nature of verbal
development of visual search in infants and very short-term impairment in dyslexia: The importance
young children. Journal of Experimental Child of serial order. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.
Psychology, 81(2), 194–215. https://doi.org/10.1006/ org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01522
jecp.2001.2649 Markant, J., & Amso, D. (2016). The development of
Hitch, G. J., Halliday, M. S., Schaafstal, A. M., & selective attention orienting is an agent of change in
Heffernan, T. M. (1991). Speech, ‘inner speech’, and learning and memory efficacy. Infancy, 21(2), 154–
the development of short-term memory: Effects of 176. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12100
picture-labelling on recall. Journal of Experimental Miller, P. H. (1990). The development of strategies of selec-
Child Psychology, 51, 220–234. https://doi. tive attention. In D. F. Bjorklund & D. F. Bjorklund
org/10.1016/0022-0965(91)90033-O (Eds.), Children’s strategies: Contemporary views of
Hitch, G. J., Towse, J. N., & Hutton, U. (2001). What limits cognitive development (pp. 157–184). Hillsdale, NJ:
children’s working memory span? Theoretical accounts Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
and applications for scholastic development. Journal Miller, S., McCulloch, S., & Jarrold, C. (2015). The devel-
of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 184–198. opment of memory maintenance strategies: Training
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.184 cumulative rehearsal and interactive imagery in children
Jarrold, C. (2017). Working out how working memory aged between 5 and 9. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 524.
works: Evidence from typical and atypical development. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00524
96 C.I. McGill and E.M. Elliott

Schiller, P. H. (1966). Developmental study of color-word depends on temperament. Memory & Cognition, 44,
interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 171–186.
72(1), 105–108. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023358 Wright, B. C. (2016). What Stroop tasks can tell us about
Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., selective attention from childhood to adulthood.
Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., & Stine-­Morrow, E. A. British Journal of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1111/
(2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psycho­ bjop.12230
logical Science in the Public Interest, 17, 103–186. Yim, H., Dennis, S. J., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2013). The
Studer-Luethi, B., Bauer, C., & Perrig, W. J. (2016). development of episodic memory: Items, contexts,
Working memory training in children: Effectiveness and relations. Psychological Science, 24, 2163–2172.
Handbook of Childhood
Psychopathology
and Developmental Disabilities:
Treatment

Jason F. Jent, Tasha M. Brown,
Bridget C. Davidson, Laura Cruz,
and Allison Weinstein

Contents
The Impact of Developmental Disabilities  he Impact of Developmental
T
and Psychopathology on Academic Disabilities and Psychopathology
and Social Outcomes............................................  97 on Academic and Social Outcomes
Definitions of Disorders...........................................  98
Academic skills are broadly defined as a collec-
Collaborative Consultation Within 
Academic Setting................................................  100 tion of study habits, oral and written communica-
tion skills, learning strategies, analytical thinking
Review of Federal Education Regulations...........  104
processes, and time management tools that help
Positive Behavior Interventions students learn and acquire new information.
and Supports at School......................................  106
Unfortunately, children with developmental dis-
Student Accommodations......................................  115 abilities, learning disorders, intellectual disabili-
Modifications..........................................................  117 ties, and/or externalizing/internalizing disorders
are at increased risk of poor academic outcomes
Summary.................................................................  118
and school dropout, if they do not receive appro-
References...............................................................  119 priate instructional and behavioral interventions,
modifications, and/or accommodations
(Hammond, Lipton, Smink, & Drew, 2007;
Nelson, Benner, Lane, & Smith, 2004). Chronic
difficulties with academics also increase risk for
long-term adverse outcomes during adolescence
and adulthood including mental health disorders,
criminal behaviors, economic hardship, occupa-
J.F. Jent (*) • T.M. Brown • B.C. Davidson tional instability, and substance abuse (Fergusson
L. Cruz • A. Weinstein
& Woodward, 2002; Nock, Holmberg, Photos, &
University of Miami Miller School of Medicine,
Miami, FL, USA Michel, 2007; Reef, Diamantopoulou, van
e-mail: jjent@med.miami.edu Meurs, Verhulst, & van der Ende, 2011; Vaughn,

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 97


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_7
98 J.F. Jent et al.

Salas-Wright, & Maynard, 2014). Given these expelled or suspended from school (Hinshaw,
long-term negative life outcomes, the need for 1992; Loe & Feldman, 2007; Owens, Holdaway
students to receive supports designed to promote et al., 2012). Students who exhibit oppositional
optimal learning and behavior within academic and conduct problems such as school refusal,
settings is critical. bullying, difficulty complying with requests, and
A number of different professionals in addi- aggressive behavior are more likely than their
tion to the child’s teachers may play a role in peers to experience school expulsion, persistent
helping to improve academic outcomes for stu- academic difficulties, and academic failure
dents with disabilities, disorders, and/or behav- despite adequate cognitive functioning
ioral problems by providing consultation within (Campbell, Spieker, Burchinal, & Poe, 2006;
an academic setting. These professionals include Robins, 1991). However, a comprehensive evalu-
but are not limited to psychologists, school psy- ation is needed to discern the origins and interac-
chologists, therapists, counselors, and clinical tions between externalizing behavior problems
social workers (referred to as clinicians and academic functioning (King et al., 2015).
hereafter). Noncompliance is defined as verbally refusing
The following sections will provide an over- to comply with teacher requests, ignoring
view of disabilities and psychopathologies that instructions, or becoming physically aggressive.
may impact a student’s academic performance, Noncompliant behavior negatively impacts
the consultation process within an academic set- academic functioning, interferes with student
ting, an overview of federal regulations related to learning, prevents teachers from carrying out
education, and examples of school-based inter- instructional tasks, and very often precedes and/
ventions, accommodations, and modifications or maintains disruptive classroom behavior
designed to promote academic success. (Cipani, 1992, 1993; Ford, Olmi, Edwards, &
Tingstrom 2001). Compliance in a classroom set-
ting allows for maximum educational opportuni-
Definitions of Disorders ties and benefits a student academically,
behaviorally, socially, and emotionally (Ritz,
Externalizing Behavior Disorders Noltemeyer, Davis, & Green, 2014).

Externalizing behavior disorders [e.g., attention-­


deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), conduct Internalizing Disorders
disorder (CD), oppositional defiant disorder
(ODD)] and academic difficulties have been cited Internalizing disorders such as anxiety and
as the most common problems in childhood depression are common, with prevalence rates of
(Metcalfe, Harvey, & Laws, 2013), and there is up to 20% in children prior to age 18 (e.g.,
high comorbidity between academic difficulties Chavira, Stein, Bailey, & Stein, 2004; Costello,
and behavior problems in the classroom (King, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003;
Lembke, & Reinke, 2016). Disruptive behaviors Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, 1998; Merry et al.,
such as noncompliance, inattention, aggression, 2012; Price et al., 2013; Werner-Seidler, Perry,
impulsivity, and hyperactivity interrupt the class- Calear, Newby, & Christensen, 2017). Children
room process and make it difficult for teachers to with internalizing disorders are at increased risk
carry out academic instruction (Owens, Holdaway for poorer social functioning and academic out-
et al., 2012), negatively impacting academic per- comes (Owens, Stevenson, Hadwin, & Norgate,
formance (Liu, 2004; Scope, Empson, McHale, 2012). However, due to the subtler classroom
& Nabuzoka, 2007). In fact, children who have a presentation of internalizing disorders as com-
diagnosis of ADHD are more likely to repeat a pared to disruptive behavior disorders, internal-
grade, exhibit noncompliance in the classroom, izing symptoms often go unrecognized and
receive special education services, and/or be unaddressed despite their detrimental impact.
Academic Skills 99

In the classroom setting, anxiety can lead to (ID; APA, 2013). Children as young as three with
inattention, reduced participation in discussions developmental disabilities have increased behavior
and/or activities, hesitation or avoidance of asking problems relative to their same-aged peers
for clarification or other assistance when needed, (Baker et al., 2002), and behavior problems in
missed assignments, social difficulties, and aca- children with developmental disabilities are
demic underperformance (Coplan, Girardi, predictive of academic failure (Baker et al., 2002;
Findlay, & Frohlick, 2007; Gimpel Peacock & Eisenhower, Baker, & Blacher, 2005).
Collett, 2010; Nelson, Rubin, & Fox, 2005). ASD is a developmental disability estimated
Anxious distraction impairs memory skills, mak- to affect 1 in 68 children in the United States
ing more effort necessary for school tasks (Owens, (Christensen, Baio, & Braun, 2016). ASD is
Stevenson, Norgate & Hadwin, 2008). Annually, characterized by persistent deficits in social com-
approximately 2–5% of students exhibit avoid- munication and the presence of restricted and
ance of school entirely, known as school refusal repetitive patterns of interests, behaviors, or
(Wimmer, 2003). Therefore, clinicians and fami- activities that can affect academic functioning in
lies working with children affected by anxiety many ways (APA, 2013). The presentation of
should provide school staff with psychoeducation ASD is heterogeneous, ranging from nonverbal
regarding anxiety and optimal school supports students with severe impairments in intellectual,
within a classroom setting. social, and adaptive functioning to high-­
Rates of depressive symptoms in preschool functioning students in gifted programs who have
children have been estimated to be about 5–6%, milder impairments in social communication.
while rates of depressive symptoms in school-­ Students with ASD with average or above intel-
aged children have been estimated to be 7–10% ligence have been found to have difficulties with
(Andreas et al., 2017). In an academic setting, listening comprehension, written or oral lan-
students with depression may demonstrate higher guage, reading comprehension, sensory function-
levels of inattention, irritability, school absences, ing, attention, problem-solving, and graphomotor
tardiness, social conflict, and risk for substance and organizational skills (Barnhill, Hagiwara,
abuse problems (Crundwell & Killu, 2007). Smith Myles, & Simpson, 2000; Dickerson
Further, children with depression are more likely Mayes & Calhoun, 2003a, 2003b, 2008;
to experience academic failure due to difficulties Goldstein, Minshew, & Siegal, 1994; Griswold,
completing assignments, persisting on tasks, Barnhill, Smith Myles, Hagiwara, & Simpson,
excessive focus on depressive cognitions, and 2002; Minshew, Goldstein, Taylor, & Siegel,
feeling academically incompetent and unmotivated 1994; Whitby & Mancil, 2009). Students with
to perform (American Psychiatric Association ASD may require more support to succeed in group
[APA], 2013; Hartlage, Alloy, Vázquez, & activities in school. Some students with ASD may
Dykman, 1993; Huberty, 2010). also have an ID that affects learning (APA, 2013).
ID is a type of developmental disability char-
acterized by deficits in cognitive functions such
Developmental Disabilities as reasoning, academic learning, problem solv-
ing, abstract thinking, and adaptive functioning
Developmental disabilities are lifelong condi- that begin during childhood (American
tions that negatively impact development, mani- Association on Intellectual and Developmental
fest early in life, and can cause impairments in Disabilities, 2017). ID results in failure to meet
academic, social, occupational, physical, and/or developmental and sociocultural standards for
intellectual functioning (Baker, Blachner, Crinic, independence and limited functioning in activi-
& Edelbrock, 2002). Examples of common ties of daily living (APA, 2013). As a result of
developmental disabilities include autism spec- cognitive impairments and deficits in adaptive
trum disorder (ASD) and intellectual disabilities functioning, children with ID are at an increased
100 J.F. Jent et al.

risk for academic failure, social-emotional con- behavior) within a system (e.g., school), with the
cerns, and behavior problems (McIntyre, Blacher, goal of helping the consultee and system in a spe-
& Baker, 2006). cific manner (Natale, Ulhorn, & Malik, 2012;
Rosenfield, 2008). Consultation relationships
are typically defined as professional peer rela-
Specific Learning Disorders tionships where parties have equal authority, but
the consultee is in need of specific assistance.
A specific learning disorder is characterized by Before initiating a consulting relationship, the
difficulties learning and using academic skills, clinician must identify and define the consultee(s)
despite intervention (APA, 2013). Difficulties in and members of the consultation team charged
learning include slow or inaccurate and effortful with determining, implementing, and monitoring
word reading, difficulties with reading compre- the needs and supports of the student. In addition
hension, poor written expression, spelling con- to identifying members of the consultation team,
cerns, poor understanding of numbers, and the clinician should define their specific role in
difficulties with mathematical reasoning. In order the consultation process. Clinicians may be uti-
to qualify as a learning disorder, academic skills lized to help with:
must be substantially and quantifiably below
what is expected for a child’s age and interfere • Facilitation of parent-teacher conferences
with academic performance. Children with learn- • Development and implementation of school-­
ing disorders may experience learning difficulties based interventions to manage child behavior
early in their schooling including understanding • Advocating for student accommodations or
new material, completing assignments, and stay- modifications
ing focused on tasks (Hinshaw, 1992). However • Modification of teacher response to child
these difficulties may not become problematic behavior
until academic demands exceed a child’s capac- • Restructuring of classroom environment to
ity to understand new concepts and complete optimize child learning and behavior
assignments (APA, 2013). In addition to aca- • Tailoring or adapting instructional
demic difficulties, children with learning disor- methodologies
ders are at increased risk for low self-esteem, • Improving skills and knowledge of parents
isolation, language deficits, and interpersonal dif- and professionals who work with the child
ficulties (Hinshaw, 1992). within the educational setting

Ethically, the clinician must assess their level


Collaborative Consultation of competence to provide school-based consulta-
Within Academic Setting tion for the child’s specific needs. Clinicians may
conduct a self-assessment of their competence to
Due to potential negative outcomes for students provide consultation by answering the following
with defined disorders, it is imperative for clini- questions:
cians to understand how to engage in collabora-
tive consultation relationships with the student’s 1. Have I worked with the presenting concern in
school and family. In this way, collaborators can the past?
determine the most appropriate evidence-based 2. Am I up to date on the existing literature in
intervention and/or accommodation to meet the this area?
student’s needs. Generally, consultation refers to 3. Am I familiar with this school setting/educa-
a process in which a professional assists a con- tional model/discipline model?
sultee (e.g., parent, teacher, speech therapist) 4. Do I have the resources and knowledge needed
with a problem (e.g., student performance or to provide consultation?
Academic Skills 101

If the clinician does not work within the school teachers’ willingness to engage in the consultation
setting, they should consider parents’ willingness process include culturally sensitive communica-
for the clinician to act on their behalf with school tions among the consultation team, perceptions of
personnel and what content will be shared the student’s locus of control, and teacher belief in
before proceeding with school consultation. the efficacy of psychotropic medication versus
The ­clinician should also consider the following behavioral interventions to manage behaviors.
before engaging in the consultation:

1. What is the existing relationship between par- Example Models of Consultation


ents and school teachers/personnel?
2. To what degree do parents feel comfortable Conjoint behavioral consultation (CBC; Sheridan
sharing that their child is receiving services & Kratochwill, 2008) is a collaborative consulta-
(e.g., therapy, medication management) out- tion model in which parents, teachers, school
side of the school setting? personnel, and a behavioral consultant (e.g., psy-
3. What will be the parents’ role in establishing chologist, school psychologist, therapist) partner
and/or maintaining the collaboration with the to promote positive outcomes related to a stu-
school? dent’s academic, behavioral, and socioemotional
4. How can the parent be empowered to main- functioning. The CBC model focuses on having
tain strategies and/or accommodations that all parties partner to enhance student outcomes
are implemented at school after the consulta- through collaborative problem solving, mutually
tion relationship with the school is developed intervention plans, operational defini-
terminated? tions of responsibilities for implementation of
5. To what degree are school administrators open plan at school and home, and goal progress moni-
to external consultants? toring (Sheridan, Ryoo, Garbacz, Kunz, &
6. What resources already exist within the school Chumney, 2013). The CBC model differs from
that may be important to any recommended traditional school consultation models in that
intervention, accommodation, and/or modifi- parents are taught and encouraged to implement
cation plans? strategies used at school in order to promote gen-
eralization of improvements across settings. The
Teacher resistance should be taken into consid- CBC model has been shown to be effective in
eration prior to initiating a consultation relation- improving student academic performance, behav-
ship. Teachers may question the validity of ior, and socioemotional functioning at home and
external experts with no direct classroom experi- school (Colton & Sheridan, 1998; Mautone et al.,
ence providing consultation (Spratt, Shucksmith, 2012; Murray, Rabiner, Schulte, & Newitt, 2008;
Philip, & Watson, 2006), and it is imperative for Power et al., 2012; Sheridan et al., 2012, 2013;
the clinician to view the teacher as the expert in Wilkinson, 2005).
providing instruction to children. However, when Instructional consultation is a consultee-­
teachers understand students’ cognitive and centered consultation approach that simultane-
behavioral deficits based on testing (Hart et al., ously focuses on “content (i.e., instructional
2016), and/or when teachers provide high levels assessment, evidence-based academic and behav-
of emotional support for students (Cappella et al., ioral interventions) and process (i.e., problem-­
2012), they are more likely to be receptive to con- solving steps, which include data collection, and
sultations. High levels of student poverty are the collaborative working relationship with the
related to limited receptiveness to school consul- classroom teacher; Rosenfield, Gravios, & Silva,
tation, likely due to the many other competing 2014; pp. 249).” Instructional consultation
demands and needs of these student populations focuses on addressing an instructional triangle
(Reinke, Stormont, Herman, Puri, & Goel, 2011). that evaluates the match between student knowl-
Other factors that may possibly contribute to edge, instruction approach, and assigned task
102 J.F. Jent et al.

expectations (Newman, Salmon, Cavanaugh, & defined problem behaviors (Iwata et al., 2000).
Schneider, 2014; Rosenfield, 2008). A data-­ An FBA is also utilized to develop a behavior
driven approach is taken to examine any potential intervention plan (BIP) that modifies variables
mismatches or gaps between the student’s current related to the problem behavior and teaches or
and expected performance, which may require allows opportunities for students to learn replace-
intervention. Through collaboration between the ment behaviors designed to improve the problem
teacher and clinician, evidence-based strategies behavior (Iwata et al., 2000). Within a school-­
that allow for progress monitoring are imple- based FBA, the consultation team is first charged
mented if mismatches exist in the instructional with operationally defining the problem
triangle (Rosenfield, 2008). Monitoring of imple- behavior(s) to be addressed in specific, measur-
mentation of evidence-based strategies is con- able, and observable terms. The examination of
ducted to determine if there are increases in antecedents or the events (e.g., interactions, set-
student performance following intervention or if ting, environmental change, external factors) that
troubleshooting is required. immediately precede the problem behavior help
The test drive model of consultation (Dart, the consultation team determine when the prob-
Cook, Collins, Gresham, & Chenier, 2012) was lem behavior is most likely to occur. Further,
developed and evaluated due to concern about examination of the student’s positive and/or neg-
teacher integrity and perceived acceptability of ative consequences following the problem behav-
interventions. Evaluation of this model indicated ior will help the team understand how the student
that when teachers are able to try out or test drive receives reinforcement. The FBA should also
different evidence-based interventions and then provide a better understanding of the desired
select the intervention they view as the most behaviors the student is not yet exhibiting.
acceptable, the intervention is implemented with The collection of direct and indirect multi-­
increased integrity (Dart et al., 2012). method and multi-reporter baseline data is imper-
ative to understand the function of the problem
behavior and develop and implement interven-
Consultation Process tion plans. Student performance can be evaluated
via universal screening or standardized testing,
Generally, consultation is utilized as a collabora- academic work samples, behavioral observa-
tive process between the clinician, teachers, and tions, clinical interviews, psychoeducational
family to help teachers modify instructional evaluations, objective and subjective parent and
behavior to promote optimal student outcomes teacher rating of student performance and emo-
(Rosenfield, 2008; Sheridan et al., 2013). The tional and behavioral functioning, student self-­
consultation process generally includes identify- report, and psychosocial history of family.
ing the problem, selecting an intervention, plan- Teacher-level data can be evaluated via self-­
ning the intervention, implementing and report of classroom instructional and behavioral
monitoring the plan, and terminating the consul- management practices (including strategies
tation relationship (Rosenfield et al., 2014). already used with the identified student) and
observation of teacher instruction. When con-
I dentifying the Problem Behavior ducting behavioral observations, the problem
In order to implement optimal interventions and behavior should be monitored until it has been
supports, the behavioral function of the individ- observed multiple times in each setting and situ-
ual student’s problematic or interfering behavior ation it occurs. Classroom environment data can
in the classroom must be understood (Hanley, be collected by examining the structure of the
Iwata, & McCord, 2003). Within school settings, classroom environment, including seat assign-
functional behavior assessments (FBA) are often ments, how desks are organized, and visual and
utilized to operationally define the antecedents, audio supports used during instruction. Through
functions, and maintaining consequences of this process, the consultation team should begin
Academic Skills 103

to identify targeted positive behaviors that the ing and problem-solving with the teacher related
child is not yet exhibiting but that would be a to implemented strategies may increase the suc-
desired outcome of any implemented interven- cess of the intervention while simultaneously
tion. While the baseline data available will vary building the capacity of the teacher. As a part of
for each consultation, it is imperative for the cli- implementation, the teacher, consultant, and par-
nician to feel confident that the data collected is ents need to collectively decide when the desired
representative of the student’s typical function- outcome of improved student academic perfor-
ing. Once appropriate levels of data are collected, mance has been met. This allows the consultation
the team should utilize data-informed decision-­ team to consider other interventions or accommo-
making to select evidence-based interventions dations if limited progress is observed or to deter-
and/or accommodations to meet the individual- mine when the consultant’s services are no longer
ized needs of the student. needed because defined outcomes for the student
have been met.
Intervention Planning
Once enough data is collected about the identi- Intervention Monitoring
fied behavior, a meeting should be arranged Intervention monitoring and subsequent adjust-
between all personnel involved in the student’s ments are typically conducted within the context
care in order to discuss the findings of the FBA of the response to intervention (RtI) system. RtI
and to develop a BIP. A BIP is a formal document is the systematic practice of an academic institu-
created by utilizing information and data obtained tion providing high quality instruction and inter-
through the FBA to guide in the design, imple- vention consistent with student needs (Batsche
mentation, and evaluation of behavioral interven- et al., 2005). Within RtI, students’ learning rate
tion strategies for a student. Students can have a and performance are measured over time at spec-
BIP in the general education setting, or the BIP ified time points in order to ensure that instruc-
can be part of an IEP for students in a special tion and interventions support students in
education program. The content includes target achieving optimal academic outcomes. As data is
behavior goals, intervention strategies to be collected over time, problem-solving is utilized
implemented, people responsible for implement- to make data-informed decisions about how to
ing interventions, measures used to monitor adjust or tailor instruction/intervention when stu-
progress, and a timeline for progress monitoring dents are not making adequate gains in perfor-
(Rosenfield, 2008). The BIP includes consider- mance (Tilly, 2008).
ation of proactive interventions designed to Monitoring requires consideration of several
reduce the likelihood of the problem behavior factors when making collaborative decisions
occurring, instructional interventions to replace regarding whether a student is making adequate
the problem behavior, and a reinforcement plan gains in performance. Factors include baseline
for when the student exhibits the adaptive data, selection of data progress monitoring sys-
replacement behavior. tem, selection of time points for evaluating prog-
ress, evaluation of school and teacher intervention
Intervention Implementation and accommodation integrity, and determination
Upon intervention implementation, it is important of maintenance procedures once child demon-
for the consultant to be available for initial techni- strates adequate progress. If implementation
cal assistance or coaching to ensure that the inter- fidelity is intact and the student is not making
vention is implemented with fidelity. However, progress, the team can elect to make modifica-
the consultant needs to maintain an approach of tions to interventions and/or accommodations to
building the teacher’s capacity to successfully meet the needs of the student. The progress mon-
implement an intervention that promotes the itoring process is then repeated until the student
teacher’s self-efficacy versus dependency on the demonstrates short-term and long-term achieve-
consultant (Natale et al., 2012). The use of coach-
104 J.F. Jent et al.

ment. Once goals are achieved, a plan for mainte- accommodations within the public educational
nance of improvements should be completed. setting (including charter schools and virtual
school). In fact, any student that has or is per-
 ermination of the Consultation
T ceived to have an impairment resulting from a
Process “physical or mental” disability may be eligible to
Following successful goal completion and imple- receive “reasonable accommodation” under
mentation of maintenance strategies, the consul- Section 504. Students would be eligible for
tant should formally notify the consultation team accommodations under a Section 504 plan if it is
when the consultation relationship is being termi- determined that a disability impairs their func-
nated (Rosenfield, 2008). When disengaging tioning in any major life activities such as walk-
from a consulting role, clinicians should ensure ing, speaking, or learning.
that the other parties involved (e.g., parents, Because a 504 plan is simply intended to pro-
teachers, other school personnel) have demon- vide students with necessary accommodations so
strated the independent ability to implement and that they can adequately participate and benefit
track interventions with fidelity for the student. from their educational environment, disabilities
While the consultation process can be volun- are more broadly defined under Section 504. As
tary, there are circumstances in which federal such, students with temporary impairments, such
regulations are in place to legally protect chil- a broken leg, can qualify for reasonable accom-
dren’s right to an optimal academic environment. modations under a 504 plan. However, students
Consultants need to be aware of these regulations experiencing longer-lasting behavioral and/or
to educate families when indicated regarding learning difficulties can also qualify for accom-
their child’s full range of options for interven- modations under Section 504. Examples of
tions and accommodations. accommodations afforded to students under
Section 504 can range from extended time during
tests, shortened assignments, enlarged print,
 eview of Federal Education
R adapted classroom equipment, preferential seat-
Regulations ing, etc.
Determination for 504 plan eligibility is usu-
There are several federal regulations related to ally made by a multidisciplinary team of profes-
providing optimal academic learning environ- sionals including teachers, parents, and the child
ments for school-aged children with disabilities. when appropriate. The multidisciplinary team
Specifically, the laws and regulations discussed must review evaluative data and information
herein are intended to provide children, who other- from multiple sources in order to determine if a
wise might encounter varying challenges within student meets eligibility for a 504 plan. Sources
the public school environment, access to an appro- of information considered for 504 plan eligibil-
priate education. As practitioners, it is important to ity may include standardized testing data, psy-
possess a general understanding of these federal chological or psychiatric reports, treatment
regulations to help guide families in collaborating summaries, physician’s statements, etc. Specific
with schools, which can help to ensure that chil- processes for eligibility determination, however,
dren are set up within an optimal learning environ- can vary between school districts.
ment for achieving academic success.

I ndividuals with Disabilities
Section 504 Education Act

Under Section 504 of the Student Rehabilitation Under the Individuals with Disabilities Education
Act of 1973 (hereafter Section 504), students Act (IDEA, 2004), children with a disability can
with a disability may be eligible to receive gain access to an individualized educational
Academic Skills 105

p­rogram (IEP) in order to meet their unique qualify for pull-out services which are often
­educational needs within the public school setting. provided by a special education teacher.
Eligibility for special education services under Students with significant disabilities that cannot
IDEA (2004) generally entails a more involved function successfully within the general educa-
identification process, including an initial evalua- tion setting may require a special class setting
tion following parental consent. Students with often consisting only of disabled peers and spe-
varying developmental disabilities and/or psycho- cially trained teachers, with a smaller student-
pathology often qualify for an IEP and receive to-teacher ratio. Generally, however, educators
services under a particular special education try to place a student in the least restrictive envi-
­program. Each special education program has a ronment (LRE) in which they can be successful
unique set of eligibility of criteria that generally (i.e., general education classes) per IDEA
requires evaluation data to support that a student (2004) regulations.
is unable to demonstrate adequate performance in
the school environment as a result of a disability.
Examples of special education programs include  avigating the Process of Obtaining
N
intellectual disability (InD), specific learning dis- an IEP or 504 Plan for Students
ability (SLD), gifted, autism spectrum disorder
(ASD), emotional behavioral disability (EBD, The first step in initiating the process of receiv-
which commonly includes children with diagno- ing necessary supports within the public school
ses of anxiety disorders, depressive disorders, and system involves the parent requesting a meeting
disruptive behavior disorders), and other health with school personnel in order to discuss con-
impaired (OHI, which commonly includes chil- cerns as well as develop a plan to address iden-
dren with ADHD and those with chronic medical tified areas of concerns. Federal regulations
issues). Although many special education pro- under IDEA (2004) outline the need for schools
grams are closely related to diagnoses, the intent to provide students demonstrating behavioral
is not to diagnose. Instead, eligibility determina- and/or academic concerns with evidenced-
tion for a special education program only implies based interventions prior to considering eligi-
that a student requires provision of federally man- bility for special education services. If a student
dated respective services and accommodations to fails to demonstrate sufficient progress despite
obtain an appropriate education. intervention (RtI process) or requires ongoing
Unlike a 504 plan, an IEP contains specific aca- intensive intervention supports to maintain
demic and/or behavioral goals for an individual gains, the school may proceed with a psycho-
based on a given student’s current functioning and educational evaluation for consideration for
further indicates how progress toward those goals special education (IDEA, 2004). Students
will be measured. Apart from accommodations evincing significant disabilities such as ID or
and modifications within the educational setting, ASD generally do not go through a process
the IEP may also outline other supports and ser- of intervention; they can often immediately
vices required to meet the needs of a given student receive an evaluation in order to determine eli-
(e.g., counseling, speech therapy, occupational gibility for special education services (IDEA,
therapy, etc.). An IEP must also note that impact of 2004). Once the evaluation is complete, the
a child’s disability on their functioning within the school team, including the parents of the stu-
general education setting as well as note the set- dent, must reconvene to discuss the results of
ting in which the child will be served (related to the evaluation and consider eligibility for spe-
how much time the student will participate with cial education (Klose, 2010). Should the school
nondisabled peers throughout the school day). team determine that the child does meet eligi-
If a student requires additional support out- bility for special education services, an IEP is
side of the general education setting, he/she can then developed (IDEA, 2004).
106 J.F. Jent et al.

 ositive Behavior Interventions


P a turn on the larger red slide” to a child with
and Supports at School lower receptive language abilities. Labeled
praises should be accompanied by visual cues or
IDEA (2004) suggests that schools “consider the gestures to increase the likelihood that child com-
use of Positive Behavioral Interventions and prehends the positive reinforcement.
Supports for any student whose behavior There are several key considerations that
impedes his or her learning or the learning of should be taken into account when using specific
others” (20 U.S.C. §1414(d)(3)(B)(i)). The following or labeled praise as a behavior management tool
section provides examples of evidence-based in the classroom. First, the clinician needs to con-
interventions that are designed to increase aca- sider the extent to which teachers view student
demic performance and/or behavioral function- extrinsic motivation for learning as potentially
ing within classroom settings. Brief overviews of harmful to students developing intrinsic motiva-
the interventions will be provided, including their tion for academic skills (e.g., student will only
applicability to various presenting concerns. In perform when external reinforcement is pro-
addition, brief overviews will be provided regard- vided). This may impact the teacher’s willingness
ing how to implement these strategies within the to strategically utilize praise within the classroom
classroom setting. as a targeted intervention (Bear, 2013). Therefore,
the teacher’s perceptions of the utility of praise
need to be understood before employing this
Specific or Labeled Praise intervention. Next, the consultation team should
create labeled praises for the positive opposite of
The use of consistent teacher labeled praise or operationally defined target problem behaviors.
specific positive verbal reinforcement following Strategies for praising approximations of desired
a desired appropriate behavior is an effective and behaviors the student is not yet exhibiting in the
empirically supported behavior management classroom setting should be discussed in order to
strategy (Lewis, Hudson, Richter, & Johnson, promote positive behavioral change. Clinicians
2004). Praise can be used to address a wide vari- should help teachers identify a continuum of
ety of externalizing and internalizing behaviors praises that can reinforce behaviors that the stu-
in an academic setting (Lewis et al., 2004). dent is already exhibiting which are approxima-
Children who exhibit externalizing behavioral tions of the targeted goal behavior (see Table 1
difficulties in a classroom frequently experience below).
negative interactions with their teachers and
peers (Partin, Robertson, Maggin, Oliver, &
Wehby, 2009). Due to frequent noncompliant and Planned Ignoring
off-task behavior, these students frequently
receive more attention or reinforcement for their Planned ignoring involves a teacher systemati-
inappropriate behavior than their prosocial cally withholding attention for a student’s identi-
behavior (Partin et al., 2009). Therefore, it is crit- fied negative attention-seeking behavior. When
ical to consider the use of praise as a strategy for used consistently, ignoring can result in the
increasing prosocial behavior in an academic extinction of disruptive and inappropriate
setting. attention-­
seeking behavior (e.g., making inap-
For children with developmental disabilities propriate noises or gestures, blurting out remarks;
and/or ID, it is especially important to consider Simonsen, Fairbanks, Briesh, Myers, & Sugai,
developmentally appropriate target behaviors and 2008). Planned ignoring can be paired with the
labeled praises to reinforce successive approxi- use of labeled praise. For example, while ignor-
mations toward goals. For example, a teacher ing a student’s blurting out of an answer, the
might say “good sharing” rather than “excellent teacher could ignore the student’s verbalization
job approaching your classmate and offering her while simultaneously praising the student for
Academic Skills 107

Table 1  Praising a continuum of positive opposite behaviors


Continuum of appropriate, specific, or labeled
Example behavior issues Positive opposite behavior teacher/caregiver praise
Inattention
Failing to pay close attention Paying close attention to tasks Thank you for sitting in your seat
to details/making careless and completing assignments You did a really great job taking out the correct
mistakes on school work correctly workbook
Good job turning to page five
I like how you are reading each item carefully
Very nice job answering number one correctly
You are doing an excellent job staying focused
on completing your assignment
Avoiding tasks that are Engaging in tasks that are I like how you got your pencil out for your
disliked, difficult, or require difficult, disliked, or require assignment
sustained attention sustained attention Great job starting your worksheet
Very nice job of staying focused on your
assignment even though you think it is difficult
You showed amazing effort working on that
really difficult question
Good job of asking me a question about how to
complete that difficult question on the
assignment
You are doing a nice job of completing your
assignment
Thanks for finishing up your assignment
Losing materials necessary Keeping track of all items Good job putting all your papers in your binder
to complete tasks or needed to complete tasks or You should be proud of yourself for keeping
activities activity track of your supplies
Excellent job checking to see if you have
everything you need to get your homework
done tonight
You are working really hard to keep your
belongings organized
Not listening when spoken to Listening when being spoken to I really like it when you look at me when I am
speaking to you
Good job stopping your work when you heard
me speaking to the class
Great job focusing on what I am telling you
Thanks for explaining to me what I just told you
Hyperactivity/impulsivity
Frequently getting out of seat Remaining seated Nice job of walking toward your chair
You are doing a great job sitting in your chair
Good job keeping your feet on the floor
I like it when you sit with your bottom on the
chair
Thank you for remaining seated during reading
Blurting out answers to Waiting to be called on before Thank you for raising your hand
questions answering a questions You did a great job waiting until I called on you
Thank you for sharing your answer when I
asked
It’s nice that you are letting your classmates
share their answers
(continued)
108 J.F. Jent et al.

Table 1 (continued)
Continuum of appropriate, specific, or labeled
Example behavior issues Positive opposite behavior teacher/caregiver praise
Noncompliance
Refusing to follow teacher Complying with teacher request Good job staying calm after I told you to
verbal instructions complete that task
Thank you for listening
I love it when you follow directions
I like it when you listen and follow directions
right way
You have been doing a great job following
instructions today
Often loses temper Remaining calm when not I can tell this is difficult. Good job taking a
getting own way deep breath to stay calm
Thank you for using your words to tell me what
was bothering you
I know this can be a really difficult situation;
thank you for walking away when you were
upset
Great job calming down so quickly
Thank you for staying calm
Anxiety
Leaving the classroom Remaining in the classroom It’s awesome that you sat through the entire
without permission due to a despite having a panic attack class today
panic attack Great job staying in the classroom even though
you felt uncomfortable
Repeatedly asking to call Waiting to speak to parents Great job staying focused on your assignment
parents due to separation until after school I am really proud of you for calming yourself
anxiety down even though you were upset that you
missed your dad
I like how you bossed back your anxiety and
waited to talk to your Mom until after class
You were impressively brave today by staying
in school all day without asking to call your
parents
Refusal to transition from Transitioning smoothly and Good job staying calm when getting in line for
one activity to another quickly between activities art class
You are an expert at getting from lunch to class
calmly
I really like that you were such a good helper by
going to that unexpected assembly
Depression
Minimal social interaction Engaging with peers It is great to see you playing with your
classmates
I like how you shared the materials with the
other students
Thank you for helping your friend with the
math problem
Physical look of sadness Expressions of pleasure or It’s nice to see you enjoying this activity
happiness It is so awesome to see you smiling and
laughing
It makes me glad to see you so happy about
going to recess today
(continued)
Academic Skills 109

Table 1 (continued)
Continuum of appropriate, specific, or labeled
Example behavior issues Positive opposite behavior teacher/caregiver praise
Social interactions
Poor eye contact Making eye contact Thank you for picking up your head up when I
asked you a question
I love it when you look in my direction
Thanks for looking at my eyes when you talk to
me
Being bossy toward others Taking turns with others I like it that you are allowing your friends to use
the colored pencils at the same time as you
You are doing a great job taking turns in the
group activity
It was nice of you to give John a chance to pick
what he wanted to do on the project
I like how you asked your friends what they
wanted to do during free time
Bullying, threating, or Engaging peers appropriately Good job keeping your hands to yourself
intimidating peers Thank you for talking/playing with to your
classmates nicely
You have been doing an excellent job
interacting with your friends

s­ itting nicely. This often immediately reduces the injurious behavior such as headbanging or biting
negative attention-seeking behavior because the that has an attention-seeking function (as deter-
child received attention for a positive behavior. mined by an FBA), the teacher can withdraw
Planned ignoring is not appropriate for external- attention to some extent (e.g., not talking to the
izing behaviors that are destructive, aggressive, child in the moment or providing reassurance),
or dangerous. It is important for the clinician to while also monitoring and ensuring the child’s
explain to the consultee that the problematic safety (Minshawi et al., 2013). Planned ignoring
behavior it is likely to experience an extinction may also be used for children with ASD who
burst in which the behavior temporarily increases exhibit inappropriate or repetitive behaviors
in magnitude, duration, and frequency because (Bearss, Johnson, Handen, Smith, & Scahill,
the student is used to receiving a response for the 2013). For example, the teacher might respond
behavior at least some of the time (Lerman, the first time a student makes a statement or ques-
Iwata, & Wallace, 1999). Otherwise, consultees tion and then implement active ignoring thereaf-
might quickly abandon implementation of this ter within the situation if the student perseverates
intervention (Quinn et al., 2000). on the topic. In these cases, differential reinforce-
Clinicians should help address concerns ment for alternative, appropriate behaviors is
teachers may have about classroom management critical for optimizing behavior change.
when ignoring and problem solve around manag-
ing how other students respond to disruptive
behavior. Planned ignoring can also be used to  oken Economies/Response Cost
T
diminish attention-seeking behaviors associated Interventions
with internalizing problems, such as crying
related to exposure to irrational fears or repeated Token economies and response cost interventions
reassurance-seeking statements. Planned ignor- are effective and empirically supported behavior
ing can be used with children with developmental interventions (Filcheck, McNeil, Greco, &
disabilities, including ID. For children with self-­ Bernard, 2004; McGoey & DuPual, 2000). Token
110 J.F. Jent et al.

economies are contingency management systems Table 2  Example token economy rewards
in which students earn reinforcers (e.g., tokens, Appropriate school Appropriate
points, stickers) for exhibiting targeted behaviors rewards home rewards
(e.g., sitting quietly, compliance) that are then Kindergarten Class helper Favorite snack
to 4th grade Eat with a friend Extra play time
exchanged at a later time for desired objects and/
or activities (e.g., extra screen time, movie night, Sticker Screen time
Line leader Choosing a
desired activity; Kazdin & Geesey, 1977;
special
McGoey & DuPaul, 2000; Quinn et al., 2000; breakfast
Zlomke & Zlomke, 2003). A response cost inter- Lunch with the Eating out
vention is a punishment procedure during which teacher
reinforcers (e.g., tokens) are taken away for an Draw a prize from Going to the
undesirable behavior. The removal of the rein- the class “prize park
box”
forcer decreases the likelihood that the targeted
Positive note sent Extra play time
behavior will occur (e.g., spitting, hitting; home with parent
Kazdin, 1972). These strategies lead to rapid 5th–8th grade Run classroom Screen time
behavior changes to address externalizing behav- errands
ioral concerns such as noncompliance, hyperac- Help classmate with Listen to music
tivity, inattention, and disruptive behavior in an academic activity
children ages 4 and older (Barkley, 1997; Help teacher present Chore pass
a lesson
McGoey & DuPaul 2000).
Praised on Eating out
When creating a daily token economy or school-wide
response cost intervention, there are several key announcement
steps that are essential to the success of the inter- Allow student to sit Special hang
vention. The FBA will guide this process. The anywhere in the out activity with
classroom parent
first step is to select the target behaviors to be
Draw a prize from Staying up late
addressed. The student’s behavior should then be the class “prize for an extra
assessed to estimate the frequency and/or dura- box” hour
tion the student is already engaging in targeted Having friends
behaviors. This must be done at several points over
during a baseline data collection period to obtain Going to a
friend’s house
an accurate estimate of the prevalence of the tar-
get behaviors. This allows for the frequency of a
goal behavior to be identified that is neither so
difficult that the student can never reach it or so to themselves, raising their hand, or staying
easy that it does not result in behavior change. focused on a task. When response cost interven-
Once completed, a reasonable criterion for the tions are used to address externalizing behavior,
number of times a student must exhibit the tar- students may lose reinforcers for inappropriate
geted behavior within a defined period of time to behavior such as breaking classroom rules,
receive a positive reward should be set. For the hitting, blurting out responses before being
student to be motivated to work toward the called on by their teacher, or getting out of their
defined goals, the student should be given the seat frequently (McGoey & DuPaul, 2000).
opportunity to create a menu of rewards from Token economies can also be implemented to
which to choose (e.g., daily rewards, weekly help anxious or depressed students to engage in
rewards; see Table 2 below). more behavioral activation, peer interactions,
Token economies are used for a broad range of group projects, or completion of classroom-based
problem behaviors. For externalizing behavior, exposure activities (Kendall & Hedtke, 2006).
students may earn rewards for appropriate behav- Token economies have been supported for use
ior such as following class rules, keeping hands with students with mild ID and/or ASD (see
Academic Skills 111

Matson & Boisjoli, 2009, for review). However, ties, behavior concerns (e.g., noncompliance,
they are less appropriate for students with more attentional difficulties, or aggression), or social
severe cognitive deficits (Matson & Boisjoli, difficulties in a classroom setting. Students are
2009). For students with ID, token economies provided immediate feedback about targeted
often focus on target behaviors related to conduct behaviors and receive praise from their teacher
(e.g., staying seated) and academic task (e.g., fin- for working toward and/or meeting behavioral
ishing portions of assignments on time; Matson goals directly to their daily report card (Owens,
& Boisjoli, 2009). For students with ASD, token Holdaway et al., 2012). The daily report card is
economies have been effectively used for social sent home daily, at which time caregivers review
behaviors (e.g., initiating social conversation or the daily report card with the student and provide
play; Kahng, Boscoe, & Byrne, 2003; Matson & rewards based on the student’s performance
Boisjoli, 2009). Finally, token economies can (Fabiano et al., 2010; Owens, Holdaway et al.,
also be used for adaptive behaviors related to 2012). Therefore, the daily report card can be a
activities of daily living (e.g., toilet training) for powerful tool to help teachers and caregivers
very young students and those with ID and/or maintain daily communication.
ASD (Matson & Boisjoli, 2009).
Once the token economy is created, it is
important to monitor the student’s progress and Emotion Regulation Strategies
modify as needed. However, token economies
often require some trial and error; therefore, it is Emotion regulation (ER) is comprised of the pro-
important to give the system time (enough data cesses that monitor, evaluate, and modify emo-
points related to implementation integrity and tional reactions, including intensity and temporal
student performance) to reliably determine if the features (Thompson, 1994). ER includes a con-
intervention is working before making modifica- stellation of skills that allow an individual to
tions. The system should be put into place until ­control their response to environmental stressors
the student is able to consistently meet criterion and manage and/or recover from this response
targeted behaviors. If the student fails to consis- (Keenan & Shaw, 2003). ER difficulties are com-
tently meet their goal, the criterion should be mon across a broad range of psychopathology
adjusted to an easier goal for the student, or the including developmental disorders, bipolar disor-
implementation of the system should be addressed ders, depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, and
if integrity issues are identified. Once a student is disruptive behavior disorders (Buckholtz &
consistently meeting the goals for the targeted Meyer Lindenberg, 2012; Nolen-Hoeksema &
behavior, the reinforcement for that behavior Watkins, 2011; Gross and Munoz, 1995; Keenan
should be removed gradually and another behav- & Shaw, 2003). Within school settings, children
ior should be added and targeted if needed. The can demonstrate ER difficulties in response to a
daily token economy or response cost interven- variety of settings related to academic instruction,
tion can be faded gradually when the student con- peer interactions, teacher-student interactions,
sistently exhibits all target goal behaviors. and performance-based tasks. Generally, children
are taught and practice effective ER strategies
 aily Report Card
D within outpatient evidence-based treatments (e.g.,
Daily report cards are a type of token economy cognitive behavior therapy for anxiety and/or
that are an empirically supported behavior modi- depression) or group school-based programs (e.g.,
fication tool for children who exhibit disruptive coping power, social problem-­solving skills train-
behavior and/or attentional difficulties in the ing, social skills training). Clinicians can advise
classroom (Fabiano et al., 2010; Owens, teachers on how to support the student’s use of
Holdaway et al., 2012). Daily report cards are effective ER within the context of skills the child
appropriate for students with academic difficul- is learning from other treatment modalities.
112 J.F. Jent et al.

 ffective Education to Promote


A 2007). Examples of somatic relaxation tech-
Emotion Regulation niques include progressive muscle relaxation,
Affective education teaches students how to more diaphragmatic breathing, and guided imagery
effectively engage in adaptive emotion regulation techniques.
(Schutz & Pekrun, 2007). Affective education is Within progressive muscle relaxation (PMR),
particularly relevant for students with anxiety dis- students are taught to tense a relax muscle groups
orders, disruptive behavior disorders, depressive throughout the body. PMR helps students become
disorders, impulse-control disorders, and ASD, more aware of how their body feels when they
many of whom exhibit emotion dysregulation are tense or stressed versus when they feel
(APA, 2013; Hofman, Sawyer, Fang, & Asnaani, relaxed. Diaphragmatic breathing includes
2012; Keenan, 2000; Mazefsky et al., 2013; Shaw, breathing in slowly through the nose until the
Stringaris, Nigg, & Leibenluft, 2015). Affective belly protrudes out, briefly holding the breath,
education first teaches students to better identify and then slowly exhaling by mouth. This breath-
their own emotions, as well as the emotions of ing process is repeated approximately five to ten
others. An example of an affective education cur- times or until the student feels relaxed. Within
riculum with tools for use in the classroom by guided imagery, students are taught to imagine
teachers is the Zones of Regulation, which uses they are in a relaxing place and to imagine feeling
colors to represent various emotional experiences calm. Once relaxed, students can return to the
(Kuypers, 2011). Affective education next teaches activity they were engaged in preceding the
students the cognitive-­ behavioral triad of how imagery. Guided imagery is often accompanied
their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are inter- by the use of diaphragmatic breathing and/or pro-
related (Stark, Herren, & Fisher, 2009; Stark gressive muscle relaxation.
et al., 2005). Students learn how interpretations For children with emotion dysregulation,
and responses to situations can vary and can affect somatic relaxation techniques provide the oppor-
thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. Finally, stu- tunity for students to produce a relaxed state
dents learn that specific strategies (e.g., cognitive while staying in the environment that is trigger-
restructuring, coping skills) can be used to modify ing the dysregulation. From a consultation per-
interpretations and responses to elicit more posi- spective, the consultation team should evaluate
tive thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. specific antecedents for when the student would
Teachers and other school personnel can benefit from utilizing somatic relaxation and
promote affect education in schools by model- develop a plan for cueing the student to use the
ing the labeling of emotions. For example, the technique prior to significant dysregulation. If
teacher can demonstrate labeling his or her own feasible, the student should be provided the
emotions in various situations and labeling stu- opportunity to receive positive reinforcement for
dents’ emotions. Teachers can prompt students daily practice or antecedent-specific use of
to label their own emotions or use fun games somatic relaxation. Reinforcement will increase
such as emotion charades to promote affective the student’s positive association with somatic
education. Teachers can provide positive rein- relaxation and increase the probability that the
forcement for students’ appropriate labeling of student utilizes the technique when indicated.
emotions (e.g., labeled praise, stickers) in
themselves and others. Social Problem-Solving
Although various models of social problem-­
Somatic Relaxation solving for children have been described, con-
Somatic relaxation interventions are utilized to ceptually, most of these models outline similar
increase adaptive emotion regulation and reduce processes. Components of social problem-­
stress through relaxing the body and have proven solving utilized by a wide variety of social com-
to reduce psychological distress and increase petence treatments including identifying the
individuals’ positive state of mind (Jain et al., problem, regulating affect, perspective taking,
Academic Skills 113

generating alternative solutions, evaluating pos- For children receiving social problem-solving
sible consequences for solutions, implementing skills training in other treatment modalities, cli-
solutions, evaluating specific solutions, and trou- nicians can reinforce and support implementation
bleshooting (D’Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; of this coping strategy within the child’s class-
Lochman & Wells, 2002; Lochman & Dodge, room by collaborating with the family and teacher
1994; Spivack & Shure, 1989). The underlying regarding building social problem-solving
approach of social problem-solving training is sequences for each identified antecedent. Once
not to teach children what to think, but rather to sequences are developed, the clinician can col-
teach children a problem-solving thinking style laborate with the teacher to provide student with
that allows them to address common interper- visual cues or reminders before a typical anteced-
sonal problems (Shure, 2001). ent is presented to utilize social problem-solving
Problem-solving deficits have been observed skills. Because most social problem-solving steps
at each stage of social-cognitive processing. At are internal, it may be difficult for the teacher to
the identification stage, aggressive and depressed identify when the student has successfully
children have been found to recall fewer relevant engaged in the social problem-solving process.
cues about events, base interpretations of events Therefore, the clinician and teacher should
on fewer cues, selectively attend to hostile rather develop plans for reinforcement whenever
than neutral cues, and attribute hostile bias to the students engage in the positive opposite
peers (Akhtar & Bradley, 1991; Lochman & following presentation of a typical triggering
Dodge, 1994; Lochman & Wells, 2002; Quiggle, antecedent.
Garber, Panak, & Dodge, 1992; Webster-Stratton
& Lindsay, 1999). At the solution generation
stage, children with aggression or impulsivity Mindfulness-Based Meditation
offer fewer competent verbal solutions, offer
more aggressive solutions, and generate fewer A more recent model that has been introduced to
and less effective solutions (Akhtar & Bradley, the school setting is mindfulness-based medita-
1991; Lochman & Wells, 2002; Shure, 2001; tion or mindfulness-based stress reduction
Webster-Stratton & Lindsay, 1999). Children (MBSR; Sibinga, Webb, Ghazarian, & Ellen,
with aggression produce more aggressive solu- 2016). Mindfulness-based interventions are built
tions in part because they believe that aggressive into multiple types of treatment programs or
behavior will result in desired outcomes (Akhtar classroom setting (Beauchemin, Hutchins, &
& Bradley, 1991; Lochman & Wells, 2002; Patterson, 2008; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2015).
Webster-Stratton & Lindsay, 1999). Children Mindfulness meditation programs implemented
with depression are less likely to generate asser- within school settings encompass the student
tive responses or to believe that assertive bringing his or her attention to the present in a
responses lead to positive outcomes (Quiggle moment-by-moment basis (e.g., selecting an
et al., 1992). attention anchor to focus on; Britton et al., 2014;
The adaptive use of social problem-solving Marlatt & Kristeller, 1999). The student is then
skills has been shown to be positively related to instructed to bring about emotional calmness by
better adjustment, the use of more effective focusing attention on specific sensations (e.g.,
­coping strategies, improvements in anger prob- sounds within the body) experienced in a
lems, depressive symptoms, and general well- moment. This process helps to bring about clarity
being (D’Zurilla, 1990; McGuire, 2001; Spivack and acceptance of the moment being experienced
& Shure, 1989; Stark, Reynolds, & Kaslow, in a nonjudgmental manner. Classroom-based
1987; Sukhodolsky, Kassinove, & Gorman, mindfulness programs also practice the tech-
2004; Vostanis, Feehan, Grattan, & Bickerton, niques during class and discuss how to apply the
1996). techniques within typical daily situations.
114 J.F. Jent et al.

Mindfulness-based interventions have been will not respond to requests to call parents
shown to have a positive impact on children’s frequently.
emotional and behavioral functioning with a par- Graduated exposure to school attendance has
ticular effectiveness for children with existing been recommended for older students and stu-
psychopathology (Zoogman, Goldberg, Hoyt, & dents with higher levels of anxiety related to the
Miller, 2015). When considering mindfulness to school environment (Kearney, 2008; Wimmer,
improve the behavioral and emotional f­ unctioning 2003; 2010). For example, graduated exposure to
of a student, the clinician should collaborate with school could start with defining a child’s hierar-
the teacher to determine potentially stressful chy of fears and levels of discomfort and then
events for the student which would allow the stu- approaching feared behaviors such as getting
dent the opportunity to practice mindfulness. ready for school, arriving at school, and atten-
dance in a preferred class, coupled with receiving
rewards (e.g., tangible or social) for approaching
Graduated Exposures and/or completing those behaviors. Rewards
could then be faded out for successful behaviors
Some students present with clinically significant as more difficult exposures are added and
levels of anxiety that warrant more targeted inter- rewarded with tokens.
vention through school-based or outpatient ther-
apy settings. Graduated exposures are a critical
component of most anxiety interventions, as they  ntecedent Strategies for Test
A
are highly effective in targeting avoidant coping Anxiety
(e.g., avoiding public speaking) and escape
behaviors (e.g., school refusal) over a relatively While intervention strategies listed for anxiety
brief period of time (Kendall et al., 2006). The are also recommended for reducing test anxiety,
clinician collaborates with the student’s teacher there are specific antecedent strategies for test
in supporting exposure activities once a fear hier- anxiety. Teachers working with students with test
archy (also known as a “ladder”) of exposure anxiety should inform them of the format of test
exercises according to the students’ expected in advance and provide practice tests when fea-
fear/distress/anxiety ratings is created. With sup- sible (Goonan, 2003). Simulation of testing con-
port from the teacher, the student approaches the ditions with a practice test serves as a form of
least anxiety-provoking situation and repeats the exposure to promote habituation of anxiety, as
exercise until habituation occurs (i.e., a signifi- well as practice to increase familiarity with test
cant reduction in anxiety) before moving on to format. Beyond interventions, there are several
the next exercise on the hierarchy. recommended accommodations for test anxiety,
Collaborative consultation is especially rele- including alternate presentation, alternate
vant when the teacher or school administration is responses, extended time, preferential seating,
needed to support the implementation of the and breaks (Hurren, Rutledge, & Garvin, 2006),
exposure activity. During graduated exposures, which are detailed below.
the teacher will have the student self-report level
of anxiety and provide encouragement without
reassurance when the student is engaged in the Peer-Mediated Instruction
exposure activity (Gillihan, Williams, Malcoun, and Intervention
Yadin, & Foa, 2012; Kuypers, 2011). For exam-
ple, a student with separation anxiety might fre- It may be helpful to pair some students with a
quently ask to call their parents. The teacher supportive and encouraging peer role model or
should let the child know that in order to get con- mentor, either in dyads or groups. This technique
trol over the anxiety, he/she is going to practice is formally known as peer-mediated instruction
waiting to talk to their parent and that the teacher and intervention (PMII) but often referred to as
Academic Skills 115

the “buddy system” (Hall & Stegila, 2003). PMII Preparing for  Transitions
should be used when both parties agree to this
arrangement, rather than forcing students to pair For some students, transitions between activities
who express opposition to the idea (Thelen & and changes in classroom routines can elicit anx-
Klifman, 2011). Specifically, the person selected ious, disruptive, and/or noncompliant behavior.
to serve as a “buddy” should be a peer or older Therefore, for students with transition difficulties
student volunteer who exhibits positive behavior it may be beneficial to create routines or provide
and academic functioning and is eager to help countdowns (e.g., 20 min, 10 min, and 5 min
others (Huberty, 2010). Students can be paired warnings before the end of an activity).
briefly during specific situations (class outings), Additionally, students with difficulties transition-
in rotations (e.g., monthly), or more long term ing may benefit from the use of rewards (e.g.,
(e.g., the school year). tokens) and praise (e.g., thank you for getting in
Peer-mediated approaches have been found to line) for successful transitions from activity to
be effective for children with ADHD and disrup- activity (Piffner, 2011).
tive behavior because they increase opportuni- For students with anxiety, notify and prepare
ties for engagement and active learning (Piffner, students in advance for significant changes in the
2011). For example, students with attentional classroom routine (e.g., substitute teachers,
difficulties who have trouble organizing tasks assemblies), in order to allow the student to plan
and preparing for activities may benefit from and proactively engage in adaptive coping strate-
being paired with a classmate who can provide gies. This is particularly important if there are
support in the areas of organization and prepara- identifiable antecedents or triggers for anxiety
tion (e.g., helping student pack homework at the that may occur (e.g., fire drills, active shooter
end of the day). It may also be beneficial to pair training drill, field trips, oral presentations).
students with attentional difficulties or reading A highly structured classroom routine with
disorders with peers to give students the oppor- transition supports can assist all students to
tunity to take turns reading passages or taking know what to expect (Thelen & Klifman, 2011)
the lead on problem-­ solving activities. Given but is particularly beneficial for those with ASD.
that some children with disruptive behaviors Visual schedules are recommended for students
have difficulties transitioning between class- with ASD (e.g., Dettmer, Simpson, Myles, &
room activities, it may be beneficial to pair stu- Ganz, 2000) but may also be helpful to other
dents with difficulty transitioning with peers students such as those with anxiety. Pairing
who can assist in helping transitions (e.g., help- visuals (e.g., picture schedule, visual timer)
ing student find correct workbook for upcoming with an auditory cue (e.g., ringing bell, rain-
lesson; Piffner, 2011). shaker) for transitions is also recommended,
There is support for pairing students to particularly for students with developmental or
improve social skills in young students with language delays who might have more difficulty
ASD (Chan et al., 2009; Laushey & Heflin, comprehending complex instructions or infor-
2000). Pairing students can also help to reduce mation regarding the planned sequence of events
anxiety and reduce disruptive behavior (e.g., (Thelen & Klifman, 2011).
specifically during transitions) in both regular
and special education classrooms (Jackson &
Campbell, 2009). To reduce the fear of rejection Student Accommodations
by peers for students with anxiety and/or depres-
sion, teachers should select small groups in The following section details accommodations
classrooms versus having students choose their that are typically included within 504 plan or
own groupings. IEP but can be voluntarily implemented. An
116 J.F. Jent et al.

accommodation is a support documented on a back, if they are anxious in crowds, or if they


student’s 504 plan or IEP that is intended to have incompatible sensory issues.
allow a student with a disability access to the
same curriculum as peers without disabilities
(IDEA, 2004). Although variability exists Extended Time on Tasks and Tests
regarding the provision of accommodations
between states, all states are required to provide Testing accommodations for students with behav-
eligible students with fair accommodations that ioral difficulties or disabilities are common, and
minimize the impact of a student’s disability in extended time is the most common accommoda-
their accessibility to the general education cur- tion (Lovett, 2010). With extended time, students
riculum (Hatcher & Waguespack, 2004). Most with a disability are allotted additional time to
states provide ­ accommodations related to the complete test and/or classroom assignments or
following areas: setting, time, presentation, and given alternate due dates (Hatcher & Waguespack,
response (Hatcher & Waguespack, 2004). 2004; Lovett, 2010). The amount of extended time
Clinicians are charged with understanding and allotted to students is dependent on their learning
advocating for accommodations that will assist and/or behavioral difficulties. Extended time may
students in achieving optimal outcomes. Below be recommended particularly for students with
are examples of these types of accommodations. attention difficulties that may interfere with their
test-taking, such as students with ADHD and/or
test anxiety. It is the most frequently used accom-
Preferential Seating/Limited modation for students with LD but should be con-
Distraction Setting sidered on an individual basis rather than assigned
based on LD diagnosis alone due to the heteroge-
When a student receives preferential seating or neity of this population (Fuchs & Fuchs, 2001). In
limited distraction seating, they are placed in a addition, students with internalizing symptoms
location within the classroom that best suits their such as test anxiety (Hurren et al., 2006) or depres-
learning style and/or behavioral difficulties sion (e.g., psychomotor slowing) may also benefit
(Hatcher & Waguespack, 2004). For example, the from extended time.
student is able to work in a place that is free from
distractions (e.g., peers, windows, busy areas of
the classroom), seated closer to the teacher dur- Alternate Presentation
ing instructional time, or allowed to leave the
classroom to complete assignments. Preferential As some students with disabilities may be permit-
seating may be beneficial for students with atten- ted to complete tasks via alternate methods,
tional difficulties who would benefit from fre- ­presentation of information can also vary for stu-
quent teacher cues to stay on task. Additionally, dents with disabilities. For instance, children with
students with frequent noncompliant or disrup- ­reading difficulties may benefit from having aca-
tive behavior may be placed near the teacher to demic material presented in a variety of ways, for
allow for the teacher to provide immediate rein- example, having test items and assignments read
forcement for appropriate behavior. For children aloud clearly and presented in writing (Hatcher &
with anxiety, preferential seating may be given to Waguespack, 2004). Another accommodation
place children near the teacher or supportive peer, related to presentation can include repetition or
closer to the bathroom (e.g., somatic complaints), various presentations of educational material or
and/or away from more boisterous children. multimodal provision of course content including
Children may also be given the option to select a oral, visual, and hands-on opportunities for learn-
comfortable seat in an auditorium, such as in the ing content (Hatcher & Waguespack, 2004).
Academic Skills 117

Alternate Responses tently reinforcing safety behaviors or maladap-


tive coping (e.g., anxious avoidance, such as
Alternate forms of responding to tasks may also escape during panic; Williams, Boyle, White, &
be permitted. For instance, some students with Sinko, 2010).
disabilities (e.g., dysgraphia, learning disorder in
written expression) may be allowed to respond
orally to test questions rather than via writing Modifications
(Hatcher & Waguespack, 2004) or complete an
oral presentation rather than a written report. Unlike accommodations, modifications are
Students with hearing loss, vision loss, and lan- changes to the curriculum content that students
guage or speech sound disorders might also ben- are expected to learn (Lee, Wehmeyer, Soukup,
efit from being permitted to respond via alternate & Palmer, 2010; Nolet & McLaughlin, 2005).
forms of communication, such as sign language, Modifications are usually implemented after
Picture Exchange Communication System, or attempts to use accommodations alone to allow
with other digital devices and communication the student to successfully access the standard
applications (assistive technology). curriculum content have been deemed unsuc-
cessful. Modifications are sometimes used in
combination with accommodations (e.g., a stu-
Chunking and Scheduled Breaks dent with alternate assessments who is also given
extended time).
Students with disabilities may also benefit from
chunking, which is when assignments are broken
into brief time periods or time or smaller portions Adapted Workload
(Hatcher & Waguespack, 2004). The consulta-
tion team through a FBA identifies how long a Depending on student functioning, modifica-
student can optimally work on an assignment tions to decrease or increase the student’s work-
before becoming fatigued, frustrated, and/or dis- load may be implemented. Students who have
tracted. For students with ADHD, ID, and/or LD, academic and/or behavioral difficulties that
chunking can help them complete tasks that are make it difficult for them to complete the
difficult, help them feel less overwhelmed, allow required coursework may receive a modified or
for more opportunities for positive reinforcement reduced course load to accommodate their
from teachers/caregivers, and reduce the argu- learning needs. For example, students with
mentativeness that is often accompanied with the given disabilities may receive shortened assign-
completion of long/difficult assignments ments or the ability to receive partial credit for
(Bulgren & Lenz, 1996; Gargiulo & Bouck, work completed (Hatcher & Waguespack,
2017; Mautone, Lefler, & Power, 2011). 2004). Teachers may also consider modifying
Scheduled breaks can also be beneficial for the amount of items students must complete
students with attentional or behavioral difficul- (e.g., by having them complete every other
ties, anxiety, and depression. Scheduled breaks problem; Hatcher & Waguespack, 2004).
are when students are allowed breaks when
needed and appropriate. A plan with clear expec-
tations for breaks should be formed between the Alternate Grading
teacher and student. This can include breaks
within the classroom (e.g., allowing the student When appropriate accommodations and modifi-
to engage in a brief break activity or put their cations are implemented and a student continues
head down briefly when feeling faint). Breaks to exhibit a pattern of failing grades, it may be
should be used with caution to prevent inadver- appropriate to use an individualized alternate
118 J.F. Jent et al.

grading system to optimize the student’s perfor- u­ nderstanding of how to engage in collaborative
mance and maintain the student’s motivation to consultation with the student’s family and school
work toward developmentally appropriate aca- personnel to determine the most appropriate
demic goals (Guskey & Bailey, 2001; Polloway evidence-­based intervention and/or accommoda-
et al., 1994). This generally applies to students tion to meet the student’s needs. IDEA (2004)
with more severe disabilities that require an indi- suggests that schools utilize evidence-based
vidualized curriculum. Options for grading mod- interventions and supports to improve students’
ifications include pass-fail systems, weighted academic skills and emotional and behavioral
grading systems, and separate grades for effort functioning within the classroom. Interventions
and achievement (Guskey & Bailey, 2001; Munk can be implemented voluntarily by the school
& Bursuck, 2004). or within the context of a 504 or IEP plan.
An accommodation is a support typically docu-
mented on a student’s 504 plan or IEP that is
Alternate Assessments intended to allow a student with a disability
access to the same curriculum as peers without
It is often deemed inappropriate for students with disabilities (IDEA, 2004). Modifications are
curriculum modifications to be assessed using changes to the curriculum content that students
standard tests or standardized state exams. If this are expected to learn.
is the case, the IEP will state that the student is Inter-professional collaboration and consulta-
not required to complete standardized exams, and tion for improving academic skills and outcomes
in some cases, may participate in an alternate for students with different needs have been shown
assessment. It is important to note that in some to have positive outcomes overall (Hurwitz,
states, there are certain grade levels at which Kratochwill, & Serlin, 2015). However, research
passing standardized testing is required, and in school consultation has been described as
therefore, decisions regarding alternate assess- underdeveloped and dated, with significant gaps
ment may need to be made early in elementary between research and practice (Erchul & Sheridan,
school. While it is possible to change testing des- 2008). For example, there is limited research sup-
ignations (e.g., from standard to alternate) in later port for successful evidence-based interventions,
school years, it may be difficult to meet require- accommodations, and modifications for particular
ments for a standard diploma once a student is student populations (e.g., students who are
opted out of standardized state testing (Thurlow retained and socially promoted, children with
& Johnson, 2000). depression) that warrant additional research
The requirements for each diploma type vary investigation (Rones & Hoagwood, 2000).
by state. It is common for commencement cere- Additionally, because the success of consultation
monies to be standardized to celebrate students’ has been shown to be dependent upon the indi-
achievements regardless of the type of diploma vidual collaborators, future research is needed to
awarded. However, the consideration of a nontra- understand individual clinician consultation train-
ditional diploma must be made carefully, as there ing models, characteristics, and/or interaction
is some evidence to suggest that these may limit styles that optimize collaborative consultation
future employment opportunities to some extent effectiveness (Hurwitz et al., 2015; Sheridan,
(Gaumer Erickson, Kleinhammer-Tramill, & Welch, & Orme, 1996). Videoconferencing is
Thurlow, 2007). increasingly utilized within behavioral health
interventions, and the acceptability and the utility
of videoconferencing for consultation and/or
Summary observation need to be further explored (Fischer
et al. 2016). Cultural issues are important to
It is imperative for clinicians working with ­consider when studying differences in individual
students with developmental, intellectual, or
­ success with collaborative consultation. However,
psychological challenges to have a strong
­ there is a dearth of literature exploring the
Academic Skills 119

i­ mplementation of collaborative, culturally compe- Barkley, R. A. (1997). Defiant children: A clinician’s
manual for assessment and parent training (2nd ed.).
tent school consultation (Ingraham, 2000; Newell
New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
et al., 2010). A particular struggle for clinicians is Barnhill, G., Hagiwara, T., Smith Myles, B., & Simpson,
the engagement of low income, low resource R. L. (2000). Asperger syndrome: A study of the cog-
schools where meeting students’ basic needs are nitive profiles of 37 children and adolescents. Focus on
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 15, 146–
the highest priority (Atkins, Frazier, Birman, Adil,
153. https://doi.org/10.1177/108835760001500303
Jackson, Graczyk, & McKay, 2006). Effective yet Batsche, G., Elliott, J., Graden, J., Grimes, J., Kovaleski,
sensitive strategies for engaging these school sys- J., Prasse, D., … Tilly, D. (2005). Response to
tems are needed. Intervention: Policy considerations and implementa-
tion. Alexandria, VA: National Association of State
Finally, academic success for all students is a
Directors of Special Education.
tremendous health protective factor. That is, aca- Bear, G. G. (2013). Teacher resistance to frequent rewards
demic success and risky health behaviors are and praise: Lack of skill or a wise decision? Journal of
strongly interrelated, and the implementation of Educational and Psychological Consultation, 23, 318–
340. https://doi.org/10.1080/10474412.2013.845495
effective interventions to support academic
Bearss, K., Johnson, C., Handen, B., Smith, T., & Scahill,
achievement is key to improving the well-being L. (2013). A pilot study of parent training in young
of not only youth but also adults (Bradley & children with autism spectrum disorders and disrup-
Greene, 2013). tive behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 43(4), 829–840.
Beauchemin, J., Hutchins, T. L., & Patterson, F. (2008).
Mindfulness meditation may lessen anxiety, pro-
References mote social skills, and improve academic perfor-
mance among adolescents with learning disabilities.
Akhtar, N., & Bradley, E. J. (1991). Social information Complementary Health Practice Review, 13, 34–45.
processing deficits of aggressive children: Present https://doi.org/10.1177/1533210107311624
findings and implications for social skills training. Bradley, B. J., & Greene, A. C. (2013). Do health and edu-
Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 621–644. https://doi. cation agencies in the United States share responsibil-
org/10.1177/0093854806295833 ity for academic achievement and health? A review
American Association on Intellectual and Developmental of 25 years of evidence about the relationship of ado-
Disabilities (AAIDD). (2017). Definition of intel- lescents' academic achievement and health behaviors.
lectual disability. Retrieved from web 3.28.17 Journal of Adolescent Health, 52(5), 523–532.
http://aaidd.org/intellectual-disability/definition#. Britton, W. B., Lepp, N. E., Niles, H. F., Rocha, T., Fisher,
WNqVJGe1thF N. E., & Gold, J. S. (2014). A randomized controlled
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic pilot trial of classroom-based mindfulness meditation
and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). compared to an active control condition in sixth-grade
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. children. Journal of School Psychology, 52, 263–278.
Andreas, A., Otto, Y., Stadelmann, S., Schlesier-Michel, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2014.03.002
A., von Klitzing, K., & Klein, A. M. (2017). Gender Buckholtz, J. W., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2012).
specificity of children’s narrative representations in Psychopathology and the human connectome: Toward
predicting depressive symptoms at early school age. a transdiagnostic model of risk for mental illness.
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(1), 148–160. Neuron, 74(6), 990–1004. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016-0533-3 neuron.2012.06.002
Atkins, M. S., Frazier, S. L., Birman, D., Adil, J. A., Bulgren, J., & Lenz, B. K. (1996). Strategic instruction
Jackson, M., Graczyk, P. A., & ... McKay, M. M. in the content areas. In D. D. Deshler, E. S. Ellis, &
(2006). School-Based Mental Health Services for B. K. Lenz (Eds.), Teaching adolescents with learning
Children Living in High Poverty Urban Communities. disabilities: Strategies and methods (pp. 409–473).
Administration And Policy In Mental Health And Denver, CO: Love Publishing Company.
Mental Health Services Research, 33(2), 146–159. Campbell, S. B., Spieker, S., Burchinal, M., & Poe,
http://doi.org/10.1007/s10488-006-0031-9 M. D. (2006). Trajectories of aggression from tod-
Baker, B. L., Blacher, J., Crnic, K. A., & Edelbrock, C. dlerhood to age 9 predict academic and social func-
(2002). Behavior problems and parenting stress in tioning through age 19. Journal of Child Psychology
families of three-year-old children with and with- and Psychiatry, 47(8), 791–800. https://doi.
out developmental delays. American Journal on org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01636.x
Mental Retardation, 107(6), 433–444. https://doi. Cappella, E., Hamre, B. K., Kim, H. Y., Henry, D. B.,
org/10.1352/0895-8017 Frazier, S. L., Atkins, M. S., & Schoenwald, S. K.
120 J.F. Jent et al.

(2012). Teacher consultation and coaching within Dettmer, S., Simpson, R. L., Myles, B. S., & Ganz, J. B.
mental health practice: Classroom and child effects (2000). The use of visual supports to facilitate tran-
in urban elementary schools. Journal of Consulting sitions of students with autism. Focus on Autism and
and Clinical Psychology, 80(4), 597–610. https://doi. Other Developmental Disabilities, 15(3), 163–169.
org/10.1037/a0027725 https://doi.org/10.1177/108835760001500307
Chan, J. M., Lang, R., Rispoli, M., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, Dickerson Mayes, S., & Calhoun, S. L. (2003a).
J., & Cole, H. (2009). Use of peer-mediated interven- Analysis of the WISC-III, Stanford-Binet: IV,
tions in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders: and academic achievement test scores in children
A systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 3(4), 876–889. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Disorders, 33, 65–80. https://doi.org/10.102
rasd.2009.04.003 3/A:1024462719081
Chavira, D. A., Stein, M. B., Bailey, K., & Stein, M. T. Dickerson Mayes, S., & Calhoun, S. L. (2003b). Ability
(2004). Child anxiety in primary care: Prevalent but profiles in children with autism. Sage Publications
untreated. Depression and Anxiety, 20, 155–164. and The National Autism Society, 6, 65–80. https://
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20039 doi.org/10.1177/1362361303007001006
Christensen, D. L., Baio, J., Braun, K. V., et al. (2016). Dickerson Mayes, S., & Calhoun, S. L. (2008). WISC-IV
Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum dis- and WIAT-II profiles in children with high function-
order among children aged 8 years – autism and devel- ing autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
opmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, Disorders, 38, 428–439. https://doi.org/10.1007/
United States, 2012. MMWR Surveillance Summary, s10803-007-0410-4
65(SS-3), 1–23. 10.15585/mmwr.ss6503a1 Eisenhower, A. S., Baker, B. L., & Blacher, J. (2005).
Cipani. (1992). A guide to developing language compe- Preschool children with intellectual disabil-
tence in preschool children with severe and moder- ity: Syndrome specificity, behavior problems,
ate handicaps (pp. 94–110). Springfield, IL: Charles and maternal well-being. Journal of Intellectual
C. Thomas. Disability Research, 49(9), 657–671. https://doi.
Cipani. (1993). The Cipani behavioral assessment and org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00699.x
diagnostic (C-BAD) system: Manual. Visalia, CA: Erchul, W. P., & Sheridan, S. M. (2008). The state of sci-
Cipani & Associates. entific research in school consultation. In Handbook of
Colton, D., & Sheridan, S. M. (1998). Conjoint behav- research in school consultation (pp. 3–12). New York,
ioral consultation and social skills training: Enhancing NY: Routledge. 
the play behavior of boys with attention deficit-­ Fabiano, G. A., Vujnovic, R. K., Pelham, W. E.,
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Educational and Waschbusch, D. A., Massetti, G. M., Pariseau,
Psychological Consultation, 9, 3–28. https://doi. M. E., … Volker, M. (2010). Enhancing the effec-
org/10.1207/s1532768xjepc0901_1 tiveness of special education programming for chil-
Coplan, R. J., Girardi, A., Findlay, L. C., & Frohlick, S. L. dren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(2007). Understanding solitude: Young children’s using a daily report card. School Psychology Review,
attitudes and responses toward hypothetical socially 39, 219–239.
withdrawn peers. Social Development, 16, 390–409. Fergusson, D. M., & Woodward, L. J. (2002). Mental
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00390.x health, educational, and social role outcomes of
Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & adolescents with depression. Archives of General
Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of Psychiatry, 59(3), 225–231. https://doi.org/10.1001/
psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. archpsyc.59.3.225
Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(8), 837–844. Filcheck, H. A., McNeil, C. B., Greco, L. A., & Bernard,
https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837 R. S. (2004). Using a whole-class token economy
Crundwell, R. M., & Killu, K. (2007). Understanding and and coaching of teacher skills in a preschool class-
accommodating students with depression in the class- room to manage disruptive behavior. Psychology in
room. Teaching Exceptional Children, 40(1), 48–54. the Schools, 41, 351–361. https://doi.org/10.1002/
https://doi.org/10.1177/004005990704000106 pits.10168
D’Zurilla, T. J. (1990). Problem-solving training for effec- Fischer, A. J., Dart, E. H., Leblanc, H., Hartman, K. L.,
tive stress management and prevention. Journal of Steeves, R. O., & Gresham, F. M. (2016). An inves-
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 4, 327–354. tigation of the acceptability of videoconferencing
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. R. (1971). Problem solv- within a school-based behavioral consultation frame-
ing and behavior modification. Journal of Abnormal work. Psychology in the Schools, 53(3), 240–252.
Psychology, 78, 107–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Ford, A. D., Olmi, D. J., Edwards, R. P., & Tingstrom,
h0031360 D. H. (2001). The sequential introduction of compli-
Dart, E. H., Cook, C. R., Collins, T. A., Gresham, F. M., ance training components with elementary-aged chil-
& Chenier, J. S. (2012). Test driving interventions to dren in general education classroom settings. School
increase treatment integrity and student outcomes. Psychology Quarterly, 16(2), 142–157. https://doi.
School Psychology Review, 41, 467–481. org/10.1521/scpq.16.2.142.18702
Academic Skills 121

Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2001). Helping teachers for- ior problems in summer settings: Results from a
mulate sound test accommodation decisions for stu- pilot evaluation in head start preschools. Journal
dents with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities of Early Intervention, 38, 92–117. https://doi.
Research and Practice, 16(3), 174–181. https://doi. org/10.1177/1053815116645923
org/10.1111/0938-8982.00018 Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vázquez, C., & Dykman, B.
Gargiulo, R. M., & Bouck, E. C. (Eds.). (2017). (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depres-
Instructional strategies for students with mild, moder- sion. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 247–278. https://
ate, and severe intellectual disability. New York, NY: doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.247
SAGE Publications. Hatcher, S. & Waguespack, A. (2004). Academic accom-
Gaumer Erickson, A. S., Kleinhammer-Tramill, J., & modations for students with disabilities. In A. Canter
Thurlow, M. L. (2007). An analysis of the relationship & L. Paige (Eds.), Helping children at home and
between high school exit exams and diploma options school II: Handouts for educators. Bethesda, MD:
and the impact on students with disabilities. Journal of National Association of School Psychologists.
Disability Policy Studies, 18(2), 117–128. https://doi. Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems
org/10.1177/10442073070180020201 and academic underachievement in childhood and
Gillihan, S. J., Williams, M. T., Malcoun, E., Yadin, E., adolescence: Causal relationships and underlying
& Foa, E. B. (2012). Common pitfalls in exposure mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 127–
and response prevention (EX/RP) for OCD. Journal 155. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127
of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 1(4), Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Fang, A., & Asnaani, A.
251–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2012.05.002 (2012). Emotion dysregulation model of mood and
Gimpel Peacock, G., & Collett, B. R. (2010). Collaborative anxiety disorders. Depression And Anxiety, 29(5),
home/school interventions: Evidence based solutions 409–416. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.21888
for emotional, behavioral, and academic problems. Huberty, T. (2010). Depression: Supporting students
New York, NY: Guilford Press. at school. In A. Canter, L. Paige, & S. Shaw (Eds.),
Goldstein, G., Minshew, N. J., & Siegal, D. J. (1994). Helping children at home and school III: Handouts
Age differences in academic achievement in high-­ for educators. Bethesda, MD: National Association of
functioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical School Psychologists.
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 671–680. Hurren, B. L., Rutledge, M., & Garvin, A. B. (2006). Team
https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639408402680 testing for individual success. Phi Delta Kappan,
Goonan, B. (2003). Overcoming test anxiety: Giving 87(6), 443–447.
students the ability to show what they know. In J. E. Hurwitz, J. T., Kratochwill, T. R., & Serlin, R. C. (2015).
Wall & G. R. Walz (Eds.), Measuring up: Assessment Size and consistency of problem-solving consultation
issues for teachers, counselors, and administrators outcomes: An empirical analysis. Journal of School
(pp. 2–18). Greensboro, NC: ERIC. Psychology, 53(2), 161–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Griswold, D. E., Barnhill, G. P., Smith Myles, B., jsp.2015.01.00
Hagiwara, T., & Simpson, R. (2002). Asperger syn- Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. (2004). 20
drome and academic achievement. Focus on Autism U.S.C. § 1400–1417.
and Other Developmental Disabilities, 17, 94–102. Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. (2011). 20
https://doi.org/10.1177/10883576020170020401 U.S.C. § 1401 et seq.
Gross, J. J., & Muñoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regu- Ingraham, C. L. (2000). Consultation through a multicul-
lation and mental health. Clinical Psychology: tural lens: Multicultural and cross-cultural consulta-
Science and Practice, 2(2), 151–164. https://doi. tion in schools. School Psychology Review, 29(3), 320.
org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00036.x Iwata, B. A., Wallace, M. D., Kahng, S., Lindberg, J. S.,
Guskey, T. R., & Bailey, J. M. (2001). Developing grading Roscoe, E. M., Conners, J., ... Worsdell, A. S. (2000).
and reporting systems for student learning. Thousand Skill acquisition in the implementation of functional
Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. analysis methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior
Hall, T., & Stegila, A. (2003). Peer mediated instruction Analysis, 33(2), 181–194. https://doi.org/10.1901/
and intervention. Wakefield, MA: National Center on jaba.2000.33-181
Accessing the General Curriculum. Jackson, J. M., & Campbell, J. N. (2009). Teachers
Hammond, C., Linton, D., Smink, J., & Drew, S. (2007). peer buddy selections for children with autism:
Dropout risk factors and exemplary programs. Social characteristics and relationship with peer
Clemson, SC: National Dropout Prevention Center, nominations. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Communities In Schools, Inc. Disorders, 39(2), 269–277. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). s10803-008-0623-1
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Jain, S. J., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S. C.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(2), 147– Mills, P. J., Bell, I., & Schwartz, G. E. R. (2007). A
185. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-147 randomized controlled trial of mindfulness medita-
Hart, K. C., Graziano, P. A., Kent, K. M., Kuriyan, A., tion versus relaxation training: Effects on distress,
Garcia, A., Rodriguez, M., & Pelham, W. E., Jr. positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction.
(2016). Early intervention for children with behav-
122 J.F. Jent et al.

Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 11–21. https://doi. with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 76(2), 213–
org/10.1207/s15324796abm3301_2 233. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440291007600205
Kahng, S., Boscoe, J. H., & Byrne, S. (2003). The use Lerman, D. C., Iwata, B. A., & Wallace, M. D. (1999).
of an escape contingency and a token economy to Side effects of extinction: Prevalence of bursting
increase food acceptance. Journal of Applied Behavior and aggression during the treatment of self-injurious
Analysis, 36(3), 349–353. https://doi.org/10.1901/ behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1),
jaba.2003.36-349 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-1
Kazdin, A. E. (1972). Response cost: The removal of con- Lewinsohn, P., Rohde, P., & Seeley, J. (1998). Major
ditioned reinforcers for therapeutic change. Behavior depressive disorder in older adolescents: Prevalence,
Therapy, 3(4), 533–546. https://doi.org/10.1016/ risk factors, and clinical implications. Clinical
S0005-7894(72)80001-7 Psychology Review, 18, 765–794. https://doi.
Kazdin, A. E., & Geesey, S. (1977). Simultaneous-­ org/10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00010-5
treatment design comparisons of the effects of earning Lewis, T., Hudson, S., Richter, M., & Johnson, N. (2004).
reinforcers for one’s peers versus for oneself. Behavior Scientifically supported practices in emotional and
Therapy, 8(4), 74–693. https://doi.org/10.1016/ behavioral disorders: A proposed approach and brief
S0005-7894(77)80200-1 review of current practices. Behavioral Disorders,
Kearney, C. A. (2008). Helping school refusing children 29(3), 247–259.
& their parents: A guide for school-based profession- Liu, J. (2004). Childhood externalizing behavior:
als. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Theory and implications. Journal of Child and
Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk fac- Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing: Official Publication
tor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: of the Association of Child and Adolescent
Science and Practice, 7(4), 418–434. https://doi. Psychiatric Nurses, Inc, 17(3), 93–103. https://doi.
org/10.1093/clipsy.7.4.418 org/10.1111/j.1744-6171.2004.tb00003.x
Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. S. (2003). Starting at the begin- Lochman, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). Social-cognitive
ning: Exploring the etiology of antisocial behavior in processes of severely violent, moderately aggres-
the first years of life. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, sive, and nonaggressive boys. Journal of Consulting
& A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and and Clinical Psychology, 62, 366–374. https://doi.
juvenile delinquency (pp. 153–181). New York, NY: org/10.1037//0022-006X.62.2.366
Guilford Press. Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2002). Contextual social-
Kendall, P. C., & Hedtke, K. (2006). Cognitive-behavioral cognitive mediators and child outcome: A test of
therapy for anxious children: Therapist manual (3rd the theoretical model of the coping power program.
ed.). Ardmore, PA: Workbook Publishing. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 945–967.
Kendall, P. C., Robin, J. A., Hedtke, K. A., Suveg, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579402004157
C., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Gosch, E. (2006). Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and edu-
Considering CBT with anxious youth? Think expo- cational outcomes of children with ADHD. Journal
sures. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 12(1), 136– of Pediatric Psychology, 32(6), 643–654. https://doi.
148. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(05)80048-3 org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsl054
King, K. R., Lembke, E. S., & Reinke, W. M. (2016). Lovett, B. (2010). Extended time testing accommodations
Using latent class analysis to identify academic for students with disabilities answers to five fundamen-
and behavioral risk status in elementary students. tal questions. Review of Educational Research, 80(4),
School Psychology Quarterly, 31(1), 43. https://doi. 611–638. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310364063
org/10.1037/spq0000111 Marlatt, G., & Kristeller, J. (1999). Mindfulness and med-
Klose, L. M. (2010). Special education: A basic guide itation. In W. Miller (Ed.), Integrating spirituality into
for parents. In A. Canter, L. Paige, & S. Shaw (Eds.), treatment: Resources for practitioners (pp. 67–84).
Helping children at home and school III: Handouts Washington, DC: American Psychological
for educators. Bethesda, MD: National Association of Association.
School Psychologists. Matson, J. L., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2009). The token econ-
Kuypers, L. (2011). The zones of regulation: A curriculum omy for children with intellectual disability and/
designed to Foster self-regulation and emotional con- or autism: A review. Research in Developmental
trol. San Jose, CA: Think Social Publishing. Disabilities, 30(2), 240–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Laushey, K. M., & Heflin, L. J. (2000). Enhancing social ridd.2008.04.001
skills of kindergarten children with autism through the Mautone, J. A., Lefler, E. K., & Power, T. J. (2011).
training of multiple peers as tutors. Journal of Autism Promoting family and school success for children with
and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 183–193. ADHD: Strengthening relationships while building
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005558101038 skills. Theory Into Practice, 50(1), 43–51. https://doi.
Lee, S. H., Wehmeyer, M. L., Soukup, J. H., & Palmer, org/10.1080/00405841.2010.534937
S. B. (2010). Impact of curriculum modifications on Mautone, J. A., Marshall, S. A., Sharman, J., Eiraldi, R. B.,
access to the general education curriculum for students Jawad, A. F., & Power, T. J. (2012). Development of
Academic Skills 123

a family–school intervention for young children with Nelson, J. R., Benner, G. J., Lane, K. L., & Smith,
ADHD. School Psychology Review, 41, 447–466. B. W. (2004). Academic achievement of K-12
Mazefsky, C. A., Herrington, J., Siegel, M., Scarpa, A., students with emotional and behavioral disor-
Maddox, B. B., Scahill, L., & White, S. W. (2013). ders. Exceptional Children, 71, 59–73. https://doi.
The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum dis- org/10.1177/001440290407100104
order. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Nelson, L. J., Rubin, K. H., & Fox, N. A. (2005). Social
Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(7), 679–688. https://doi. withdrawal, observed peer acceptance, and the devel-
org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006 opment of self-perceptions in children ages 4 to 7 years.
McGoey, K. E., & DuPaul, G. J. (2000). Token rein- Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20(2), 185–200.
forcement and response cost procedures: Reducing https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2005.04.007
the disruptive behavior of preschool children with Newell, M. L., Nastasi, B. K., Hatzichristou, C., Jones,
attention-­deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School J. M., Schanding, G. T., Jr., & Yetter, G. (2010).
Psychology Quarterly, 15(3), 330. https://doi. Evidence on multicultural training in school psy-
org/10.1037/h0088790 chology: Recommendations for future directions.
McGuire, J. (2001). What is problem-solving? A review School Psychology Quarterly, 25(4), 249. https://doi.
of theory, research, and applications. Criminal org/10.1037/a0021542
Behavior and Mental Health, 11, 210–235. https://doi. Newman, D. S., Salmon, D., Cavanaugh, K., & Schneider,
org/10.1002/cbm.397 M. F. (2014). The consulting role in a response-
McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J., & Baker, B. L. (2006). The to-intervention context: An exploratory study of
transition to school: Adaptation in young children instructional consultation. Journal of Applied School
with and without intellectual disability. Journal of Psychology, 30, 278–304. https://doi.org/10.1080/153
Intellectual Disability Research, 50(5), 349–361. 77903.2014.924456
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00783.x Nock, M. K., Holmberg, E. B., Photos, V. I., & Michel,
Merry, S. N., Hetrick, S. E., Cox, G. R., Brudevold-­ B. D. (2007). The self-injurious thoughts and
Iversen, T., Bir, J. J., & McDowell, H. (2012). behaviors interview: Development, reliability, and
­
Cochrane review: Psychological and educational validity in an adolescent sample. Psychological
interventions for preventing depression in children Assessment, 19, 309–317. https://doi.org/10.1037/
and adolescents. Evidence-Based Child Health: A 1040-3590.19.3.309
Cochrane Review Journal, 7(5), 1409–1685. https:// Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic
doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD003380.pub3 for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopa-
Metcalfe, L. A., Harvey, E. A., & Laws, H. B. (2013). thology: Explaining multifinality and divergent tra-
The longitudinal relation between academic/cogni- jectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6),
tive skills and externalizing behavior problems in pre- 589–609. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611419672
school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, Nolet, V., & McLaughlin, M. (2005). Accessing the gen-
105(3), 881–894. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032624 eral curriculum: Including students with disabilities
Minshawi, N. F., Hurwitz, S., Fodstad, J. C., Biebl, S., in standards-based reform (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
Morriss, D. H., & McDougle, C. J. (2013). The CA: Corwin Press.
association between self-injurious behaviors and Owens, J. S., Holdaway, A. S., Zoromski, A. K., Evans,
autism spectrum disorders. Psychology Research S. W., Himawan, L. K., Girio-Herrera, E., & Murphy,
and Behavior Management, 7, 125–136. https://doi. C. E. (2012). Incremental benefits of a daily report
org/10.2147/PRBM.S44635 card intervention over time for youth with bisruptive
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Taylor, H. G., & Siegel, behavior. Behavior Therapy, 4(43), 848–861. https://
D. J. (1994). Academic achievement in high func- doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2012.02.002
tioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Hadwin, J. A., & Norgate,
Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 261–270. https:// R. (2012). Anxiety and depression in academic
doi.org/10.1080/15377903.2014.924456 performance: An exploration of the mediating
Munk, D. D., & Bursuck, W. D. (2004). Personalized factors of worry and working memory. School
grading plans: A systematic approach to making the Psychology International, 33(4), 433–449. https://doi.
grades of included students more accurate and mean- org/10.1177/0143034311427433
ingful. Focus on Exceptional Children, 36(9), 1–11. Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Norgate, R., & Hadwin,
Murray, D., Rabiner, D., Schulte, A. C., & Newitt, K. J. A. (2008). Processing efficiency theory in chil-
(2008). Feasibility and effectiveness of a daily report dren: Working memory as a mediator between
card intervention for ADHD students. Child & Youth trait anxiety and academic performance. Anxiety,
Care Forum, 37, 111–126. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Stress, and Coping, 21(4), 417–430. https://doi.
s10566-008-9054-6 org/10.1080/10615800701847823
Natale, R., Uhlhorn, S., & Malik, N. (2012). Childcare Partin, T. C. M., Robertson, R. E., Maggin, D. M., Oliver,
center-based infant mental health program model. R. M., & Wehby, J. H. (2009). Using teacher praise and
Chapel Hill, NC: National Early Childhood Technical opportunities to respond to promote appropriate stu-
Assistance Center (NECTAC). dent behavior. Preventing School Failure: Alternative
124 J.F. Jent et al.

Education for Children and Youth, 54(3), 172–178. Psychologists. https://doi.org/10.1080/21683603.201


https://doi.org/10.1080/10459880903493179 3.790774
Piffner, L. J. (2011). All about ADHD: The complete Rosenfield, S. A., Gravios, T. A., & Silva, A. E. (2014).
practical guide for classroom teachers (2nd ed.). Bringing instructional consultation to scale. Handbook
New York, NY: Scholastic. of research in school consultation. Abingdon, UK:
Polloway, E. A., Epstein, M. H., Bursuck, W. D., Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203133170.
Roderique, T. W., McConeghy, J. L., & Jayanthi, M. ch10
(1994). Classroom grading: A national survey of poli- Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott,
cies. Remedial And Special Education, 15(3), 162–170. D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T. F., & Diamond,
http://doi.org/10.1177/074193259401500304 A. (2015). Enhancing cognitive and social-emo-
Power, T. J., Mautone, J. A., Soffer, S. L., Clarke, A. T., tional development through a simple-to-­ administer
Marshall, S. A., Sharman, J., ... Jawad, A. F. (2012). A mindfulness-­ based school program for elemen-
family–school intervention for children with ADHD: tary school children: A randomized control trial.
Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 52–66. https://doi.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 611–623. org/10.1037/a0038454
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028188 Schutz, P. A., & Pekrun, R. E. (2007). Emotion in educa-
Price, M., Higa-McMillan, C., Ebesutani, C., Okamura, tion. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. https://
K., Nakamura, B. J., Chorpita, B. F., & Weisz, doi.org/10.1016/B978-012372545-5/50002-2
J. (2013). Symptom differentiation of anxiety and Scope, A., Empson, J., McHale, S., & Nabuzoka, D.
depression across youth development and clinic-­ (2007). The identification of children with behavioural
referred/nonreferred samples: An examination of manifestations of inattention, hyperactivity and impul-
competing factor structures of the child behavior sivity, in mainstream school: The development of the
checklist DSM-oriented scales. Development and scope classroom observation checklist. Emotional &
Psychopathology, 25(4pt1), 1005–1015. https://doi. Behavioural Difficulties, 12(4), 319–332. https://doi.
org/10.1017/S0954579413000333 org/10.1080/13632750701664137
Quiggle, N. L., Garber, J., Panak, W. F., & Dodge, K. A. Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act, 29 U.S.C. § 701.
(1992). Social information processing in aggressive (1973).
and depressed children. Child Development, 63, 1305– Shaw, P., Stringaris, A., Nigg, J., & Leibenluft, E. (2015).
1320. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131557 Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperac-
Quinn, M. M., Osher, D., Warger, C., Hanley, T., Bader, tivity disorder. Focus, 14(1), 127–144. https://doi.
B., Tate, R., & Hoffman, C. (2000). Educational org/10.1176/appi.focus.140102
strategies for children with emotional and behav- Sheridan, S. M., Bovaird, J. A., Glover, T. A., Garbacz,
ioral problems. Washington, DC: Center for Effective S. A., Witte, A., & Kwon, K. (2012). A randomized
Collaboration and Practice. trial examining the effects of conjoint behavioral con-
Reef, J., Diamantopoulou, S., van Meurs, I., Verhulst, sultation and the mediating role of the parent–teacher
F. C., & van der Ende, J. (2011). Developmental relationship. School Psychology Review, 41, 23–46.
trajectories of child to adolescent externalizing https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2016.12.002
behavior and adult DSM-IV disorder: Results of a Sheridan, S. M., & Kratochwill, T. R. (2008). Conjoint
24-year longitudinal study. Social Psychiatry and behavioral consultation: Promoting family–school
PsychiatricEpidemiology, 46, 1233–1241. https://doi. connections and interventions. New York, NY:
org/10.1007/s00127-010-0297-9 Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-2512-4
Reinke, W. M., Stormont, M., Herman, K. C., Puri, R., Sheridan, S. M., Ryoo, J. H., Garbacz, S. A., Kunz, G. M.,
& Goel, N. (2011). Supporting children’s mental & Chumney, F. L. (2013). The efficacy of conjoint
health in schools: Teacher perceptions of needs, roles, behavioral consultation on parents and children in the
and barriers. School Psychology Quarterly, 26, 1–13. home setting: Results of a randomized controlled trial.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022714 Journal of School Psychology, 51, 717–733. https://
Ritz, M., Noltemeyer, A., Davis, D., & Green, J. (2014). doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2013.09.003
Behavior management in preschool classrooms: Sheridan, S. M., Welch, M., & Orme, S. F. (1996). Is
Insights revealed through systematic observation and consultation effective? A review of outcome research.
interview. Psychology in the Schools, 51(2), 181–197. Journal for Special Educators, 17(6), 341–354.
https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.21744 Shure, M. B. (2001). I can problem solve (ICPS): An
Robins, L. N. (1991). Conduct disorder. Journal of Child interpersonal cognitive problem-solving program for
Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 193–212. https://doi. children. Residential Treatment for Children & Youth,
org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00008.x 18(3), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1300/J007v18n03_02
Rones, M., & Hoagwood, K. (2000). School-based mental Sibinga, E. M., Webb, L., Ghazarian, S. R., & Ellen, J. M.
health services: A research review. Clinical Child and (2016). School-based mindfulness instruction: An
Family Psychology Review, 3(4), 223–241. RCT. Pediatrics, 137, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1542/
Rosenfield, S. (2008). Best practice in instructional con- peds.2015-2532
sultation and instructional consultation teams. Best Simonsen, B., Fairbanks, S., Briesch, A., Myers, D., &
practices in school psychology (pp. 1645–1660). Sugai, G. (2008). Evidence-based practices in class-
Bethesda, MD: National Association of School room management: Considerations for research to
Academic Skills 125

practice. Education and Treatment of Children, 31(3), in the United States. Journal of Public Health, 22,
351–380. https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0007 265–270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10389-014-0615-x
Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. (1989). Interpersonal cog- Vostanis, P., Feehan, C., Grattan, E., & Bickerton, W.
nitive problem-solving (ICPS): A competence-­ (1996). Treatment for children and adolescents with
building primary prevention program. Prevention depression: Lessons from a controlled trial. Clinical
in Human Services, 6, 151–178. https://doi. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1, 199–212. https://
org/10.1080/10852358909511187 doi.org/10.1177/1359104596012003
Spratt, J., Shucksmith, J., Philip, K., & Watson, C. Webster-Stratton, C., & Lindsay, D. W. (1999). Social
(2006). Interprofessional support of mental well-­ competence and conduct problems in young chil-
being in schools: A Bourdieuan perspective. Journal dren: Issues in assessment. Journal of Clinical Child
of Interprofessional Care, 20, 391–402. https://doi. Psychology, 28, 25–43. https://doi.org/10.1207/
org/10.1080/13561820600845643 s15374424jccp2801_3
Stark, K. D., Herren, J., & Fisher, M. (2009). Treatment Werner-Seidler, A., Perry, Y., Calear, A. L., Newby, J. M.,
of childhood depression. In M. J. Mayer, R. Van & Christensen, H. (2017). School-based depres-
Acker, J. Lochman, & F. M. Gresham (Eds.), Cognitive sion and anxiety prevention programs for young
behavioral interventions for students with emotional/ people: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
behavioral disorders (pp. 266–294). New York, NY: Clinical Psychology Review, 51, 30–47. https://doi.
Guilford. org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.005
Stark, K. D., Reynolds, W. M., & Kaslow, N. J. (1987). Whitby, P. S., & Mancil, G. R. (2009). Academic
A comparison of the relative efficacy of self-control achievement profiles in children with high function-
therapy and a behavioral problem-solving therapy ing autism and Asperger syndrome: A review of the
for depression in children. Journal of Abnormal literature. Education and Training in Developmental
Psychology, 15, 91–113. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Disabilities, 44(4), 551–560.
BF00916468 Wilkinson, L. A. (2005). An evaluation of con-
Stark, K. D., Simpson, J., Schnoebelen, S., Glenn, R., joint behavioral consultation as a model for sup-
Hargrave, J., & Molnar, J. (2005). ACTION workbook. porting students with emotional and behavioral
Ardmore, PA: Workbook Publishing. difficulties in mainstream classrooms. Emotional and
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Kassinove, H., & Gorman, B. S. Behavioural Difficulties, 10, 119–136. https://doi.
(2004). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anger in org/10.1177/1363275205054163
children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Aggression Williams, B. B., Boyle, K., White, J. M., & Sinko, A.
and Violent Behavior, 9, 247–269. https://doi. (2010). Children’s mental health promotion and sup-
org/10.1016/j.avb.2003.08.005 port: Strategies for educators. In A. Canter, L. Paige, &
Thelen, P., & Klifman, T. (2011). Using daily transition S. Shaw (Eds.), Helping children at home and school
strategies to support all children. Young Children, III: Handouts for educators. Bethesda, MD: National
66(4), 92–98. Association of School Psychologists.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme Wimmer, M. B. (2003). School refusal: Assessment and
in search of definition. Monographs of the Society intervention within school settings. Bethesda, MD:
for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3), 25–52. National Association of School Psychologists.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x Wimmer, M. (2010). School refusal: Information for
Thurlow, M. L., & Johnson, D. R. (2000). High-­ educators. In A. Canter, L. Paige, & S. Shaw (Eds.),
stakes testing of students with disabilities. Journal Helping children at home and school III: Handouts
of Teacher Education, 51, 305–314. https://doi. for educators. Bethesda, MD: National Association of
org/10.1177/0022487100051004006 School Psychologists.
Tilly, W. D., III. (2008). The evolution of school psychol- Zlomke, K., & Zlomke, L. (2003). Token economy plus
ogy to science-based practice: Problem ­solving and the self-monitoring to reduce disruptive classroom behav-
three-tiered model. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), iors. The Behavior Analyst Today, 4(2), 177–182.
Best practices in school psychology V (pp. 17–36). https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100117
Bethesda, MD: National Association of School Zoogman, S., Goldberg, S. B., Hoyt, W. T., & Miller,
Psychologists. L. (2015). Mindfulness interventions with youth: A
Vaughn, M. G., Salas-Wright, C. P., & Maynard, B. R. meta-analysis. Mindfulness, 6, 290–302. ­https://doi.
(2014). Dropping out of school and chronic disease org/10.1007/s12671-013-0260-4
ADHD

Johnny L. Matson and Jasper A. Estabillo

Contents medication. Often a second class of add-on medi-


Introduction............................................................  127 cations is prescribed as well. Generally, these
add-on drugs are atypical antipsychotic medica-
Symptoms................................................................  127
tions. Psychological treatments are also some-
Treatment................................................................  128 times used, often in combination with medication.
Multimodal Treatments.........................................  129 A review of common treatment protocols for
Parent Training......................................................  132
children and adolescents will be the primary
focus of this chapter. First, however, a brief over-
Treatment Issues....................................................  133 view of symptoms of ADHD will be discussed.
Conclusion..............................................................  134
References...............................................................  134
Symptoms

The core symptoms of ADHD are well-­


Introduction established and include inattention, off-task
behaviors, excessive fidgeting, restlessness, and
One of the most common and frequently excessive motor behaviors such as being out of
researched topics in the child mental health field seat, impulsivity, and excessive talking, as well as
is attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder frequently interrupting others. Categories of
(ADHD). Rates of the disorder in children have ADHD include primarily inattentive, primarily
been rising rapidly, and in recent years, large hyperactive/impulsive, or a combination of the
numbers of adults have also been diagnosed with two, with the latter condition being the most seri-
the disorder. These trends have led some research- ous and difficult to treat.
ers to refer to these developments as an epidemic It is also recognized that ADHD is a heteroge-
(Nielsen, 2017). In most cases, the person neous disorder that frequently overlaps with
diagnosed with ADHD is prescribed a stimulant other childhood mental health and/or neurodevel-
opmental conditions. This fact is obviously
important for diagnosis, but it also has important
J.L. Matson • J.A. Estabillo (*) implications for the development and implemen-
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA tation of treatment plans. Thus, investigators
e-mail: jestab1@lsu.edu have paid a great deal of attention to this topic.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 127


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_8
128 J.L. Matson and J.A. Estabillo

For example, Newcorn et al. (2001) note the medication management within this treatment.
potential effect of comorbid conditions on the They also recommended a clinic-based treatment
core symptoms of ADHD. They evaluated 498 which involves the family. A focus on imple-
children with ADHD. The participants were menting treatment strategies in the home envi-
divided into groups based on comorbidities. All ronment is also emphasized. These example
the children displayed high levels of inattention, studies point to the need to identify and treat
impulsivity, and disruptive errors. The ADHD comorbid problems along with core ADHD
plus oppositional defiant or conduct disorder symptoms. A more detailed review of a range of
group had especially high levels of impulsivity, treatment options follows next.
while the ADHD plus anxiety disorder group had
particular difficulties with inattentiveness. These
authors also found that boys had more severe Treatment
ADHD symptoms than girls.
Comorbid depression has also been studied Wolraich et al. (2011) present a series of treat-
with children diagnosed with ADHD. Daviss ment guidelines for the treatment of ADHD. They
(2008) noted that when this comorbidity is pres- make the point that school applications may be
ent, depression tends to emerge several years more difficult for adolescents since multiple
after ADHD symptoms emerge. Thus, the authors teachers are involved, which of course differs
also posit that chronic ADHD symptoms may be from primary school classes. These factors are
a primary cause of depression and the person’s also associated with cost. Medication alone is a
inability to cope with the ADHD symptoms when common choice of clinicians for the potential
they emerge. With respect to treatment, Daviss coordination problems noted above. Casadei and
recommends a biopsychosocial approach to colleagues (2017) note this treatment option (i.e.,
intervention. He also recommends an individual- medication) is cheaper than psychological inter-
ized approach that considers targeting specific ventions or multimethod approaches to interven-
symptoms as well as environmental factors that tion. Friedman, Banaschewski, Sikiricia, and
may contribute to the child’s mental health status. Chen (2017) further underscore the issue of
Where the symptom presentation is potentially scarce resources. They surveyed over 3600 care-
complex, Daviss recommends a broader array of givers regarding services for ADHD. Common
interventions which include medication along themes reported to the researchers included
with environmentally based treatments. This lat- underfunding and gaps in service provision.
ter group includes manualized therapies which The use of medication long term can become
have been developed for childhood depression. more expensive due to the laboratory tests and
He also stressed the value of including school addressing side effects. This point is underscored
personnel and parents in treatment planning and by the fact that more and more children are being
implementation. treated with medication. Additionally, children
Other problems that commonly co-occur with are being treated with these drugs for longer peri-
ADHD are oppositional defiant disorder and con- ods of time and with higher doses (Goldman
duct disorders. Husby and Wichstrom (2017) sug- et al., 1988). Because the most problematic
gest that oppositional defiant disorder symptoms aspects of medication occur months or years after
may predict conduct disorders, which have a later the treatment begins, these aspects of medica-
onset. They also note a strong relationship between tions are often underemphasized. The majority of
oppositional defiant disorder and ADHD. children diagnosed with ADHD are prescribed
Hogue, Evans, and Levin (2017) looked at the stimulant and/or non-stimulant drugs (Hauck,
relationship between ADHD and adolescents Lau, & Wing, 2017). Antidepressant medication
who have developed adolescent substance abuse. was prescribed for roughly 20% of children with
As an intervention strategy, they suggest using a ADHD and almost 5% of children without
family ADHD psychoeducation and embedding ADHD.
ADHD 129

Chen et al. (2017) have addressed a particu- clusions from Pelham et al. (2005). Methylphenidate
larly interesting positive effect of methylpheni- was used alone and paired with broad psychosocial
date. They looked at injuries, specifically bone treatment of parent training, counseling, social
fractures, and found that they occurred at greater skills training, psychotherapy, and academic train-
rates among individuals with ADHD. When the ing. These authors concluded that adding these
drug was administered for over 180 days, the psychological treatments did not improve ADHD
drug group suffered from fewer injuries than the oppositional defiant disorder symptoms over a
ADHD group who did not receive the treatment. 2-year period. There are at least two possible expla-
Jahangard et al. (2017) have also looked at the nations for these divergent results. Firstly is that the
effect of medication on noncore symptoms of specificity and consistency of treatment implemen-
ADHD. Over an 8-week period, methylphenidate tation may vary across studies. Secondly, psycho-
plus risperidone was administered for symptoms logical approaches may be more behavior specific.
of oppositional defiant disorder. They found that Thus, if the psychological treatment focused more
the combination of both drugs decreased both on social and academic skills, it would follow that
ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder symp- these skills would be impacted more than core
toms more than methylphenidate alone. symptoms of ADHD and oppositional defiant
Michaelson et al. (2001) treated 297 children behavior. If the former group of symptoms were
between 8 and 18 years of age who were diag- not measured, misjudgments of treatment efficacy
nosed with ADHD. Children and adolescents would follow. Another of these multimethod stud-
received 1.2 mg/kg/day and 1.8 mg/kg/day or ies is described by Reed, Jakubovski, Johnson, and
placebo. Medication was superior to placebo for Bloch (2017). They looked at school-based inter-
improving ADHD core symptoms as well as ventions on truancy and school fights. These
social and family functioning. authors stress that the failure to garner strong
parental involvement was a major predictor of psy-
chological treatment failure. Effective treatment
Multimodal Treatments did curb these problem behaviors.
Enns, Randall, and Smith (2017) also describe
A number of studies have employed combined a multimodal method to treat ADHD. They eval-
treatments. Pelham et al. (2005) described a uated 485 children and adolescents who had
study with 27 children diagnosed with ADHD received services for ADHD in Canada. They
who were 6–12 years of age. They used transder- reported positive effects associated with multi-
mal methylphenidate in combination with behav- modal therapy. Positive effects included higher
ior modification. The psychological intervention medication adherence and improved academic
consisted of a token economy with reinforcers performance. Also, when a multimodal strategy
plus response cost. Additionally, the children was employed, disparities based on family
were provided social skills training plus problem-­ income and ADHD-related problems were
solving. The kids also received noncontingent minimal.
reinforcement. Individualized behavioral treat- Miranda, Presentacion, and Soriano (2002)
ment plans were used for children engaging in describe a multicomponent school-based treat-
disruptive behavior. Reinforcement of positive ment for ADHD symptoms of children. Fifty-two
behaviors and the use of time-out were included children were assigned to either a treatment or a
in these plans. The combination of behavior control group. A preplanned intervention strategy
modification and medication proved to be more consisted of covering general information on
effective than either medication or psychological ADHD in session one. Topics covered were the
treatment used alone. nature, incidence, and effects of ADHD on learn-
Abikoff et al. (2004) describe the treatment of ing and behavior. The next two sessions focused
103 children diagnosed with ADHD who were on teaching general principles and procedures of
7–9 years of age. They drew many different con- behavior modification. Session four covered
130 J.L. Matson and J.A. Estabillo

instructions such as rearranging space, how to Jensen et al. (2001) describe a large-scale
use instructions, directions, and feedback with treatment study looking at singular and com-
respect to appropriate classroom performance. bined interventions. Five hundred seventy-nine
Also, there was a focus on the management of children 7–10 years of age were assigned to one
educational materials. Sessions five and six of four conditions of 14 months’ duration.
employed cognitive behavioral methods such as Children were not excluded if comorbid condi-
how to manage their child’s inappropriate behav- tions such as conduct disorder, internalizing dis-
iors, the use of self-instructions, using reinforce- orders, and specific learning disabilities were
ment during self-evaluation, and contingency diagnosed. Treatment one was methylphenidate
plans for the entire class. The “Think Aloud” while treatment two was behavior management.
program was also used. Self-instructions This latter intervention consisted of 35 sessions
included: (1) What is my problem? (2) What is to teacher/parent behavior management and
my plan? (3) Am I following my plan? (4) How coordinating with school services. Children in
did I do it? Considerable teacher support, rein- this condition also received summer school con-
forcement, encouragement, and feedback were sistent of day-­long sessions for 8 weeks. The
used. As the child became more self-sufficient, focus was on sports, social, and academic skills.
prompts by the teacher were faded. Specific The same therapist provided parent training and
behaviors that were targets included the follow- supervised their child’s counselor during the
ing: (1) avoid aggression toward others, (2) summer program. The therapist also consulted
respect teachers and classmates, (3) stay seated, with the child’s teacher during the regular
(4) remain quiet while seated, (5) attend to tasks, school year. They also supervised a teacher’s
and (6) follow teacher directions. A point system aide who worked with the child for 12 weeks in
was used to reward meeting appropriate behavior the fall. Treatment three combined the first
targets. These rules were taught by the teachers in two treatments. The final intervention group
a group context, and the points were applied indi- was called the community comparison group.
vidually. The treatment group did significantly Families in this group were provided a list of
better on academic performance and classroom referrals. Another option was to refer the family
conduct and improved teacher knowledge of the back to the original provider. Treatment in this
educational needs of their children. condition was heterogeneous and varied from
Richter and colleagues (1995) describe a mul- family to family dependent on whatever was
timodal treatment program goal. They note that available and what parents agreed to use. About
important questions regarding the treatment of 43 of the group received stimulant drugs.
ADHD in children remain. For example, what Drug and psychological interventions were
combination of treatment components best described as much less intensive than in groups
addresses differences in age, severity of core one to four.
ADHD symptoms, and comorbid disorders such The first three treatment groups described had
as conduct disorder, autism, intellectual disabili- superior effects to the community comparison
ties, or anxiety? These authors also note that group. These data underscore the value of
early studies focused almost exclusively on the ­interventions that are intensive and which are
use of stimulant medications on core inattention provided by specialists in ADHD with back-
and impulsivity symptoms. This approach has grounds in psychiatry and psychology. This is in
given way largely to psychosocial programs such contrast to the less effective method of general
as the one by Miranda et al. (2002). The combi- practitioners and generalists in the mental health
nation of medication with these psychological field. The combined treatment was superior to
treatments has also been discussed. These medication or intensive specialized psychologi-
authors’ overall conclusion is that combined cal intervention. Having said that, both of the sin-
treatments may produce better results than treat- gular treatments provided in groups one and two
ment models using a singular intervention. also result in benefits.
ADHD 131

Duric, Gundersen, Golos, and Elgen (2017) combined treatment or multimodal group. The
describe a treatment for ADHD that had a camp involved 3 h of manualized treatment each
6-month follow-up. They studied 285 children day of camp. A focus of this treatment was on
and adolescents diagnosed with ADHD in teaching children how to function as a group on
Norway with a mean age of 11–6 years. One establishing friendships. The remainder of the
treatment was described as non-pharmacological day, children participated in recreational activi-
(NF). The NF group employed scalp sensors to ties in groups. Psychologists and special educa-
measure brain waves. In an effort to shape brain tional teachers provided the care. Children
wave activity, kids played video games. Treatment participated in a response cost system where
was conducted three times a week for a total of points could be won or lost throughout the day.
30 sessions. A second condition consisted of They found that the addition of medication added
methylphenidate, while a third condition com- only marginal improvements over the behavioral-­
bined the first two treatments. Parents reported based school program on a range of measures.
change at a 6-month follow-up, but teachers did These measured included aggression and/or defi-
not. The NF group showed little change, but rela- ant behavior, interacting with peers, recreational
tively equal change was seen for the medication activities, and home settings. Other categories
group and the medication plus NF group. It included academic skills and general all-around
should be noted, however, that the psychological normal behaviors. They describe the 14-week
intervention was nonconventional and may plain program as producing very large gains.
the results in this paper. Bikic, Reichow, McCauley, Ibrahim, and
Swanson et al. (2008) took a more conven- Sukhodolsky (2017) provide a review of organi-
tional approach. They compared stimulant medi- zational skills training for children with
cation to behavior therapy in a review of papers ADHD. Skill building methods have been used to
on the topic. These authors draw a number of teach organization of materials. In some of the
conclusions. (1) Intensive drug management therapy models, time management and goals for
using a stimulant medication algorithm should be treatment were broken down into steps and com-
a first-line treatment. This approach is recom- bined with prompts, instructions, and reinforce-
mended with or without behavior therapy. (2) ment. Another program these authors review
Growth suppression of 20% accumulates due to includes the Homework, Organization, and
stimulant medication over a 2-year period with Planning Skills (HOPS). As with the program
height reductions of up to 2 cm. Rebound is pos- components reviewed alone, this is a skills-based
sible once medication is discontinued. (3) Long-­ treatment that was provided after school for
term treatment effects defined as 2 years were 8 weeks. Organizational checklists and methods
evident with the stimulant medication algorithm. to track homework were used. The Child Life and
(4) Relative superiority of stimulant medication Attention Skills (CLAS) program is another of
may be temporary for 2/3 of children, and these these organization treatments for children with
effects will be evident by 3 years of drug use. (5) ADHD. Major aspects of this program include
More needs to be learned about these children teacher report cards on the child, guidance in task
who receive medications and why some children completion, improving executive functioning,
stop medication use for ADHD. and dealing more effectively with problems. The
Pelham et al. (2000) have also addressed the Family School Success (FSS) is yet another of
issue of combined treatments for ADHD symp- these programs. The primary goal of this
toms of children. They treated 117 children who approach to the treatment of ADHD is to enhance
range in age from 7 to 9 years. Children were positive parental involvement with the school
assigned to a behavior therapy summer camp. process. One of the primary targets includes inat-
Children at the camp were diagnosed with ADHD tention on the parent of the child. All of these
and were divided into placebo or medication organizational programs proved to be useful in
groups. This latter group was described as the enhancing attention and academic skills. Many of
132 J.L. Matson and J.A. Estabillo

these programs also involve a parental compo- to produce these skills were presented. A parent
nent. Frequently used strategies involve provid- component was also employed. Parents were pro-
ing structure to the child’s routine to enhance vided education about ADHD and applied behav-
organizational skills. This structure, which is ior analysis. Among the techniques used were
planned and supported by parents and teachers, is behavioral contracts, token systems, and differ-
further aided by the instructional methods such ential reinforcement of other behavior.
as performance feedback and reinforcement sys- Compliance and eye contact were among the
tems. These approaches tend to be independent behaviors that were trained. Significant improve-
of medication-based treatments for ADHD. ments in all measured of executive function were
Tamm et al. (2017) target a different set of noted.
behaviors for children with ADHD. Their focus Sibley, Comer, and Gonzalez (2017) address
was specific to word reading difficulties of 216 the problem of poor treatment utilization among
children in grades 2–5. Groups consisted of med- adolescents with ADHD. These authors used
ication and parent training focused on ADHD parent-teen video conferencing for ADHD. The
treatment, a treatment condition consisting of focus was on motivating the adolescents to meet
reading instruction, and a third condition which goals as well as a focus on how parents could
combined treatments in group 1 and group 2. implement strategies to support their children.
Treatment outcome measures consisted of ratings The authors note that this learning format results
on ADHD scales by parents and teacher and on in very good adherence to treatment and satisfac-
assessment of word reading and/or decoding. As tion with the program.
would be expected, inattention and hyperactivity/
impulsivity symptoms responded best to the
medication plus parent training condition and the Parent Training
combined condition. The two groups with great-
est improvements on ADHD symptoms did not Chronis, Chacko, Fabiano, Wymbs, and Pelham,
differ markedly. Word reading and decoding Jr. (2004) review a number of studies that focus
improvements were greatest for the reading on behavioral parent training (BPT). These
instruction and combined conditions. These data authors present an excellent review. One of the
suggest that interventions need to be symptoms most significant points they cover are common
specific. This finding is in tune with our earlier components of parent training. The typical
remarks that some interventions may appear less sequence involves (1) an overview of behavior
effective because the behavior being affected by management principles and ADHD symptoms;
the intervention may not be addressed in some (2) establishing home-school notes, home behav-
studies. Measurement issues of this sort may lead ior checklists, and various reward systems; (3)
investigators to make incorrect assumptions attending to (reinforcing) appropriate behavior
regarding the effectiveness of various while ignoring minor transgressions; (4) giving
treatments. appropriate commands and reprimands; (5)
Shuai et al. (2017) describe a Chinese sample defining and reinforcing contingencies; (6)
­
of 44 children with ADHD and 88 children with employing time-out; (7) establishing home token
typical development. Five of the children in the systems; (8) enforcing contingencies outside the
ADHD groups were on medication for their living environment; (9) problem-solving meth-
symptoms. The authors provided a skills-based ods; and (10) maintenance after active treatment
intervention focused on executive function. has been terminated.
Specific skills that were targeted included plan- The authors point out that over the last two
ning and organizing, theory of mind, time man- decades, a major move in parent treatment has
agement, and emotion regulation. These skills been from the clinic to group school-based pro-
were taught to the children. The learning environ- grams. Many of these programs are manualized.
ment was structured so that ample opportunities One of the best known of these is parent-child
ADHD 133

interaction therapy. The focus of this intervention Treatment Issues


is a series of conventional behavior therapy strat-
egies such as in vivo practice, modeling, and per- Sikirica, Gustafsson, and Makim (2017) con-
formance feedback. Initially, this therapeutic ducted a retrospective study of 1794 children and
model was implemented on a family-by-family adolescents 6–17 years of age in Sweden. They
basis. More recently group strategies have been looked at treatment patterns for these clients,
employed to decrease costs and enhance effi- defined as the first administration of stimulant
ciency of the training. Videotaped modeling of medication. For those with comorbidities, more
skills has also been introduced as a means of medications were used. Also, there were, as
enhancing treatment efficiency. would be expected, greater overall resource use
Other manualized programs have also been and higher cost.
devised as a means of enhancing childhood cop- Colaneri, Klein, and Adesman (2017) have
ing skills. These treatment models also use vid- also addressed the issue of medication utilization
eotaped modeling, group discussion, role-playing, for persons diagnosed with ADHD. They
and performance feedback. One of these pro- addressed the issue of physician practices aimed
grams is the Incredible Years (Webster-Stratton, at presenting the misuse of stimulant mediations.
1996). Chronis et al. (2004) state that this train- The most common practice was to use long-­
ing model is promising for families of children acting medications versus immediate-release
with ADHD. We underscore that originally this drugs. Another strategy has been to prescribe
treatment was designed for oppositional defiant non-stimulant drugs for ADHD since other psy-
disorder and conduct disorder. It should also be chotropic drug classes are less likely to be used
underscored that these two latter disorders are recreationally. Medical contracts with clients
also comorbid with ADHD at high levels. seemed to be moderately effective and were used
Another of these well-established training but less frequently than the other two methods
models is the Community Parent Education mentioned. Print materials on the potential mis-
Program (COPE; Cunningham, Brenner, & use of medication were also mentioned but rarely
Secord, 1998). Similar behavior therapy strate- used. Another method that has been suggested
gies to the Incredible Years program are used. but which also has gained little traction was to
There is also a focus on parent-led small groups prescribe smaller numbers of pills. The authors
with a particular emphasis on problem-solving. point out that physicians must take greater
Chronis-Tuscano, Wang, Woods, Strickland, and responsibility for drug misuse by employing the
Stein (2017) underscore the importance of parent strategies noted above more frequently and
involvement in ADHD interventions for their systematically.
children. They also make the point that ¼ to ½ of Sleath et al. (2017) have also addressed the
these parents have also experienced ADHD. It issue of drug adherence. They studied children
seems logical to conclude, therefore, that these and adolescents between 7 and 17 years of age.
interventions may benefit the parents as well. Also, parents completed questionnaires. Seventy
Haack, Villodas, McBurnett, Hinshaw, and families participated in the project. One-third of
Pfiffner (2017) have also looked at the benefits the children and adolescents desired more infor-
of parent treatment in children with ADHD, mation on ADHD from mental health profession-
predominately inattentive type. They employed als. The average number of questions was 8. With
the multifaceted psychosocial skills program respect to nonadherence, being nonwhite and
CLAS. They treated 7- to 11-year-old children being older were the greatest risk factors.
with the latter intervention or a more parent-­ Compliance to psychosocial treatment is also
focused treatment. Both treatments resulted in a major issue of concern. Schultz, Evans,
improvements in negative parenting, while Langberg, and Schoemann (2017) addressed this
the CLAS group also improved positive issue in a large multisite study called the
parenting. Challenging Horizons Program. This program
134 J.L. Matson and J.A. Estabillo

covered one school year and focused on aca- Bikic, A., Reichow, B., McCauley, S. A., Ibrahim, K., &
Sukhodolsky, D. G. (2017). Meta-analysis of organi-
demic and social skills of adolescents diagnosed
zational skills interventions for children and adoles-
with ADHD. Those who complied with the treat- cents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
ment showed improvements in challenging Clinical Psychology Review, 52, 108–123. https://doi.
behaviors, being organized, and completing org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.12.004
Casadei, G., Reale, L., Costantino, M. A., & Bonati, M.
homework. Little impact on social skills was
(2017). Italian regional health service costs for diag-
observed, however. An interesting finding was nosis and 1-year treatment of ADHD in children
that participants most in need of ADHD social and adolescents. International Journal of Mental
services were most likely to comply. Health Systems, 11(1.) https://doi.org/10.1186/
s13033-017-0140-8
Chen, V. C.-H., Yang, Y.-H., Liao, Y.-T., Kuo, T.-Y., Liang,
H.-Y., Huang, K.-Y., … Lin, T.-C. (2017). The asso-
Conclusion ciation between methylphenidate treatment and the
risk for fracture among young ADHD patients: A
nationwide population-based study in Taiwan. PLoS
A marked evaluation in treatment and service pro-
One, 12(3), e0173762. https://doi.org/10.1371/jour-
vision has occurred in those with ADHD. While nal.pone.0173762
not specifically addressed here, due to the scope Chronis, A. M., Chacko, A., Fabiano, G. A., Wymbs,
of the book’s topics, adults are being diagnosed B. T., & Pelham, W. E. J. (2004). Enhancements to
the behavioral parent training paradigm for families of
with ADHD at much higher rates in recent years.
children with ADHD: Review and future directions.
Other trends have been ever-­increasing numbers Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 7(1),
of children being diagnosed with 1–27.
ADHD. Comorbid disorders are being diagnosed Chronis-Tuscano, A., Wang, C. H., Woods, K. E.,
Strickland, J., & Stein, M. A. (2017). Parent ADHD
at much higher rates and being treated much more
and evidence-based treatment for their children:
frequently as well. Also, behavioral interventions Review and directions for future research. Journal of
are recommended for treating hyperactivity/ Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(3), 501–517. https://
impulsive behavior even when criteria for ADHD doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0238-5
Colaneri, N., Keim, S., & Adesman, A. (2017). Physician
are not met (Wolraich et al., 2011).
practices to prevent ADHD stimulant diversion and
Abuse of stimulant drugs is a major topic as misuse. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 74,
well. Better drug adherence and proactive efforts 26–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2016.12.003
for mental health professionals are urgently Cunningham, C. E., Bremmer, R. B., & Secord-Gilbert,
M. (1998). COPE, the community parent education
needed. More and better implementations of psy-
program: A school based family system oriented work-
chosocial treatments are also needed. There is shop for parents of children with disruptive behavior
sufficient evidence to conclude that behaviorally disorders (Leader’s manual). Hamilton, ON: COPE
based methods implemented at school and with Works.
Daviss, W. B. (2008). A review of co-morbid depres-
parents paired with stimulant drugs produce the
sion in pediatric ADHD: Etiologies, phenomenol-
best results. More emphasis on how to expand ogy, and treatment. Journal of Child and Adolescent
this approach in the broadest real-world context Psychopharmacology, 18(6), 565–571. https://doi.
is needed. org/10.1089/cap.2008.032
Duric, N. S., Assmus, J., Gundersen, D., Duric Golos, A.,
& Elgen, I. B. (2017). Multimodal treatment in chil-
dren and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactiv-
References ity disorder: A 6-month follow-up. Nordic Journal of
Psychiatry, 71(5), 386–394. https://doi.org/10.1080/0
Abikoff, H., Hechtman, L., Klein, R. G., Weiss, G., Fleiss, 8039488.2017.1305446
K., Etcovitch, J., … Pollack, S. (2004). Symptomatic Enns, J. E., Randall, J. R., Smith, M., Chateau, D.,
improvement in children with ADHD treated with Taylor, C., Brownell, M., … Nickel, N. C. (2017).
long-term methylphenidate and multimodal psycho- A multimodal intervention for children with ADHD
social treatment. Journal of the American Academy reduces inequity in health and education outcomes.
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(7), 802–811. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 62(6), 403–412.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000128791.10014.ac https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743717692301
ADHD 135

Fridman, M., Banaschewski, T., Sikirica, V., Quintero, J., Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(6), 547–563.
& Chen, K. S. (2017). Access to diagnosis, treatment, https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194020350060601
and supportive services among pharmacotherapy-­ Newcorn, J. H., Halperin, J. M., Jensen, P. S., Abikoff,
treated children/adolescents with ADHD in Europe: H. B., Arnold, L. E., Cantwell, D. P., … Vitiello,
Data from the caregiver perspective on pediat- B. (2001). Symptom profiles in children with
ric ADHD survey. Neuropsychiatric Disease and ADHD: Effects of comorbidity and gender.
Treatment, 13, 947–958. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
S128752 Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(2), 137–146. https://doi.
Goldman, L. S., Genel, M., Bezman, R. J., & Slanetz, P. J. org/10.1097/00004583-200102000-00008
(1988). Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/ Nielsen, B. (2017). Imitations and transformations:
hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. On side effects of the ADHD epidemic. Medical
JAMA, 279(14), 1100–1107. https://doi.org/10.1001/ Anthropology, 36(3), 246–259. https://doi.org/10.108
jama.279.14.1100 0/01459740.2016.1239618
Haack, L. M., Villodas, M., McBurnett, K., Hinshaw, Pelham, W. E., Burrows-Maclean, L., Gnagy, E. M.,
S., & Pfiffner, L. J. (2017). Parenting as a mecha- Fabiano, G. A., Coles, E. K., Tresco, K. E., …
nism of change in psychosocial treatment for youth Hoffman, M. T. (2005). Transdermal methylphe-
with ADHD, predominantly inattentive presentation. nidate, behavioral, and combined treatment for
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(5), 841– children with ADHD. Experimental and Clinical
855. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0199-8 Psychopharmacology, 13(2), 111–126. https://doi.
Hauck, T. S., Lau, C., Wing, L. L. F., Kurdyak, P., & org/10.1037/1064-1297.13.2.111
Tu, K. (2017). ADHD treatment in primary care: Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., Hoza, B.,
Demographic factors, medication trends, and treatment Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., … McBurnett, K.
predictors. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 62(6), (2000). Behavioral versus behavioral and pharmaco-
393–402. https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743716689055 logical treatment in ADHD children attending a sum-
Hogue, A., Evans, S. W., & Levin, F. R. (2017). A clini- mer treatment program. Journal of Abnormal Child
cian’s guide to co-occurring ADHD among adolescent Psychology, 28(6), 507–525
substance users: Comorbidity, neurodevelopmental Reed, M. O., Jakubovski, E., Johnson, J. A., & Bloch,
risk, and evidence-based treatment options. Journal of M. H. (2017). Predictors of long-term school-­
Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 26(4), 277–292. based behavioral outcomes in the multimodal
https://doi.org/10.1080/1067828X.2017.1305930 treatment study of children with attention-­ deficit/
Husby, S. M., & Wichstrøm, L. (2017). Interrelationships hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and
and continuities in symptoms of oppositional defiant Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 27(4), 296–309.
and conduct disorders from age 4 to 10 in the com- https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2015.0168
munity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(5), Richters, J. E., Arnold, L. E., Jensen, P. S., Abikoff, H.,
947–958. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0210-4 Conners, C. K., Greenhill, L. L., … Swanson, J. M.
Jahangard, L., Akbarian, S., Haghighi, M., Ahmadpanah, (1995). NIMH collaborative multisite multimodal
M., Keshavarzi, A., Bajoghli, H., … Brand, S. (2017). treatment study of children with ADHD: I. Background
Children with ADHD and symptoms of oppositional and rationale. Journal of the American Academy of
defiant disorder improved in behavior when treated Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(8), 987–1000.
with methylphenidate and adjuvant risperidone, https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199508000-00008
though weight gain was also observed – results from a Schultz, B. K., Evans, S. W., Langberg, J. M., &
randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical Schoemann, A. M. (2017). Outcomes for adoles-
trial. Psychiatry Research, 251, 182–191. https://doi. cents who comply with long-term psychosocial treat-
org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.010. ment for ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., Greenhill, Psychology, 85(3), 250–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/
L. L., Conners, C. K., Arnold, L. E., … Wigal, T. ccp0000172
(2001). Findings from the NIMH multimodal treatment Shuai, L., Daley, D., Wang, Y.-F., Zhang, J.-S., Kong,
study of ADHD (MTA): Implications and applications Y.-T., Tan, X., & Ji, N. (2017). Executive function
for primary care providers. Journal of Developmental training for children with attention deficit hyperactiv-
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22(1), 60–73 ity disorder. Chinese Medical Journal, 130(5), 549–
Michelson, D., Faries, D., Wernicke, J., Kelsey, D., 558. https://doi.org/10.4103/0366-6999.200541
Kendrick, K., Sallee, F. R., & Spencer, T. (2001). Sibley, M. H., Comer, J. S., & Gonzalez, J. (2017).
Atomoxetine in the treatment of children and adoles- Delivering parent-teen therapy for ADHD through vid-
cents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A eoconferencing: A preliminary investigation. Journal
randomized, placebo-controlled, dose-response study. of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment.
Pediatrics, 108(5), E83. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-017-9598-6
Miranda, A., Presentación, M. J., & Soriano, M. (2002). Sikirica, V., Gustafsson, P. A., & Makin, C. (2017).
Effectiveness of a school-based multicomponent Treatment patterns among children and adolescents
program for the treatment of children with ADHD. with attention-deficit/hyperactivity his order with
136 J.L. Matson and J.A. Estabillo

or without psychiatric or neurologic no morbidities word reading difficulties: A randomized clinical trial.
in Sweden: A retrospective cohort study. Neurology Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85(5),
and Therapy, 6(1), 115–130. https://doi.org/10.1007/ 434–446. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000170
s40120-017-0066-8 Webster-Stratton, C. H. (1996). Early intervention
Sleath, B., Carpenter, D. M., Sayner, R., Thomas, K., with videotape modeling: Programs for families
Mann, L., Sage, A., … Sandler, A. D. (2017). Youth of children with oppositional defiant disorder or
views on communication about ADHD and medication conduct disorder. In E. D. Hibbs & P. S. Jensen
adherence. Community Mental Health Journal, 53(4), (Eds.), Psychosocial treatments for child and ado-
438–444. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10597-016-0078-3 lescent disorders: Empirically based strategies for
Swanson, J., Arnold, L. E., Kraemer, H., Hechtman, L., clinical practice (pp. 435–474). Washington, DC:
Molina, B., Hinshaw, S., … Wigal, T. (2008). Evidence, American Psychological Association. https://doi.
interpretation, and qualification from multiple reports org/10.1037/10196-017
of long-term outcomes in the multimodal treatment Wolraich, M., Brown, L., Brown, R. T., DuPaul, G., Earls,
study of children with ADHD (MTA): Part I: Executive M., Feldman, H. M., … Visser, S. (2011). ADHD:
summary. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12(1), 4–14. Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evalu-
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054708319345 ation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactiv-
Tamm, L., Denton, C. A., Epstein, J. N., Schatschneider, ity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics,
C., Taylor, H., Arnold, L. E., … Vaughn, A. (2017). 128(5), 1007–1022. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.
Comparing treatments for children with ADHD and 2011-2654
Treatments for Autism Spectrum
Disorders

Michelle S. Lemay, Robert D. Rieske,
and Leland T. Farmer

Contents Introduction
Introduction............................................................  137
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurode-
History.....................................................................  138
velopmental disorder that presents with restricted
Classification/Diagnosis.........................................  139 and repetitive behaviors and interests as well as
Early Treatment Requires Early Diagnosis.........  140 deficits in social communication abilities and
Measuring Empirical Support..............................  140
sensory abnormalities. When discussing treat-
ments available for individuals with ASD, there
Non-efficacious but Generally Benign are many considerations that need to be taken
Treatments..........................................................  142
into account before selecting an appropriate
Non-efficacious and Potentially Harmful intervention. No single intervention will be maxi-
Treatments..........................................................  145
mally effective for all clients and intervention
Potentially Efficacious Treatments.......................  148 planning that is individualized to the client based
Empirically Validated Treatments........................  149 on specific factors will help to improve treatment
Experimental Treatments......................................  151
outcomes. Some of these factors include the
intellectual functioning of the individual, the
Treatment of Comorbid Psychopathology...........  152 severity of autistic symptoms, as well as any
Future Research Directions...................................  152 comorbid disorders (both medical and psycho-
Conclusion..............................................................  153 logical) that may affect treatment.
When discussing level of functioning for indi-
References...............................................................  153
viduals with ASD, many often confuse this with
the severity of autism symptoms; rather, these are
two distinct characteristics. While it is true that a
majority on the autism spectrum also have an
intellectual disability, this is not necessarily tied
to severity of autism symptoms, and, in fact,
many individuals with ASD are considered “high
functioning” (i.e., no intellectual disability diag-
M.S. Lemay • R.D. Rieske (*) • L.T. Farmer
nosis), and some exhibit intellectual functioning
Department of Psychology, Idaho State University,
Pocatello, ID, USA well above the average range. Because of this
e-mail: riesrobe@isu.edu wide range of intellectual functioning across the

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 137


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_9
138 M.S. Lemay et al.

autism spectrum, differing treatments should be the word “autism” was intended to represent
considered. For example, cognitive-behavior one’s self-absorbed retreat into an idiosyncratic
therapy (CBT) may be a useful tool in treating an mental state of fantasy, which he thought to be
individual with ASD with average or above aver- tied to symptoms of a schizophrenic trait
age intellectual functioning but may not be as (Goldstein & Ozonoff, 2009). He reported that
useful for an individual with an intellectual dis- those with schizophrenia oftentimes display
ability; for these individuals a more behavioral behaviors such as daydreaming and imaginative
approach may be more effective. Clinicians and play but that as long as the “autistic thinking” of
families need to be aware of these issues and the schizophrenic individual remains intact, so
make appropriate decisions that will be most does their ability to think reasonably and ratio-
impactful for their specific client. nally. However, if the thinking is challenged, then
In this chapter, we will briefly discuss the his- the individual displays illogical and irrational
tory of ASD including classification changes as thinking patterns (Bleuler, 1913). Bleuler’s focus
this is important to understand when working on the sensory abnormalities in terms of autism
with and treating this population. We will then led many children from the 1930s to 1960s with
discuss the importance of early intervention in schizophrenia to be thought to actually have
working with children with ASD as well as what autism. Leo Kanner made the distinction between
makes an intervention “evidence-based.” The autism and schizophrenia in children, reporting
focal point of this chapter will present many of that autism was due to a deviation from typical
the prominent interventions that are available to neurodevelopment (Goldstein & Ozonoff, 2009).
individuals with ASD with respect to their level However, this led to some confusion among pro-
of empirical support (or lack thereof) and in some fessionals, and the distinction between disorders
cases the potential for harm. This is not meant to has continually been debated, although there is
be an exhaustive review of all available interven- significant evidence and criteria supporting a
tions/treatments available for ASD (as there are clear distinction (Cohen, Paul, & Volkmar, 1986;
many) but rather a review of some of the most de Bruin, de Nijs, Verheij, Hartman, & Ferdinand,
popular and well research (which are not always 2006; Towbin, Dykens, Pearson, & Coehn, 1993).
one and the same) treatments for ASD. The features of autism were initially outlined
by Kanner (1944, 1954), describing a series of
“core symptoms.” These symptoms included an
History inability to form appropriate social relationships
with those around them, an odd use of language
ASD is a relatively new diagnosis compared to or general speech/language deficits, and an
others in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual excessive desire for a “preservation of sameness”
of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). that were present from birth onward, although
While ASD remains somewhat new and under- they became more noticeable during childhood
studied, it is now becoming a more prominent (Kanner, 1943, 1944). Additionally, he noted that
neurodevelopmental disorder, receiving a large autism was present more often in boys than girls,
amount of attention from clinicians and families these children more often have enlarged heads,
alike. Although parents and physicians are now and their parents tended to be more intelligent
more aware of signs and symptoms to be looking with stronger interests in science and arts than in
for, the history and development of the disorder other people (Kanner, 1943, 1954; Kanner &
remain predominantly unknown by most. Eisenberg, 1957). Interestingly, Hans Asperger, a
German researcher, was also investigating these
behaviors around the same time as Kanner. In
First Use fact, the ideas and observations are so similar and
so close in temporal proximity that some
The term “autism” first came into use in 1911 by researchers argue that Kanner knew about
Eugen Bleuler, a Swiss psychiatrist. His use of Asperger’s work but chose to ignore it. It is also
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 139

possible that the two researchers truly were not attachments to objects, onset prior to 30 months
aware of each other’s studies due to WWII pre- of age, and the absence of schizophrenic features
venting any exchange of information (Deisinger, such as hallucinations and delusions. Then, in the
2011). However, his work went mostly unnoticed DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association.
until it was translated to English in 1991. The Work Group to Revise DSM-III, American
work of Asperger led to a wider array of disor- Psychiatric Association 1987), the disorder was
dered symptoms to be classified within the spec- renamed to “Autistic Disorder” with some
trum of autism, thus allowing individuals with changes in the criteria including the removal of
less severe impairments to receive psychological age of onset and a requirement of 8 out of 16
and other therapeutic interventions. items with at least 2 impairments in reciprocal
interaction, 1 communication impairment, and 1
restricted interest or activity. The diagnostic cri-
Classification/Diagnosis teria changed very little between DSM-III-R,
DSM-IV, and DSM-IV-TR, with the exception of
In 1972, Michael Rutter reported that childhood the inclusion of Asperger’s disorder, which
schizophrenia, the disorder label used to describe required six criteria instead of the eight required
autistic symptoms that lumped children with for autism. However, it was not until DSM-5 that
schizophrenia and autism together, was no longer the disorder of autism saw a significant change.
helpful. He believed that “childhood schizophre-
nia” was being used as a catchall for the above-­
described symptoms and it no longer held the DSM-5
same level of clinical utility (Rutter, 1972). He
based this decision off a previous publication The DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association.
from himself and colleagues that described a tri- DSM-5 Task Force, & American Psychiatric
axial classification system that separated intellec- Association, 2013) shifted the diagnostic criteria
tual disabilities and clinical problems (Rutter so that it emphasized deficits in two core areas
et al., 1969). This model began to gain more sup- associated with autism: social communication
port from clinicians as time went on, and eventu- and restricted/repetitive behaviors/interests.
ally included psychiatric and intellectual axes, Within the social communication domain, indi-
but split the etiological factors into biological viduals must exhibit impairments in a total of
factors and psychosocial factors. His work pro- three areas, including social and emotional reci-
pelled autism into the next phase for diagnosis procity (e.g., struggling with social communica-
and treatment, the DSM. tion, impairment in showing and sharing of
interests or emotions, or failure to engage in other
social interactions), nonverbal social
 SM-III, DSM-III-R, DSM-IV,
D ­communication (e.g., poor eye contact, impaired
and DSM-IV-TR use and understanding of gestures, and impaired
use of facial expressions), and deficits in devel-
A description of the diagnosis for autism first oping and maintaining social relationships. They
appeared in the third edition of the Diagnostic must also exhibit impairments in at least two of
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders the following restricted/repetitive behaviors or
(DSM) as infantile autism and was listed in a new interests including stereotyped/repetitive motor
category: pervasive developmental disorders movements, use of objects, or speech and insis-
(American Psychiatric Association 1980). The tence on sameness or over-adherence to rituals or
criteria included a pervasive lack of response to routines, restricted interest in objects or subject
others, deficits in language development, odd use matter, or exhibit sensory abnormalities such as
of language or pronoun reversals if speech is over- or under-reactivity to different sensory
present, insistence on sameness, odd interests or input. Additionally, the individuals must
140 M.S. Lemay et al.

e­xperience significant impairments in areas of a diagnosis or treatment for fear of stigmatizing


adaptive functioning. their child. The other issue here is that many
One of the major changes from prior DSM underserved or lower socioeconomic families are
editions to the DSM-5 is the elimination of undereducated on potential symptoms to be look-
Asperger’s disorder and the change of “Autistic ing for and therefore do not realize that their
Disorder” to “Autism Spectrum Disorder” with child may be on the spectrum. Additionally, once
the implementation of severity levels. This allows a diagnosis is obtained, there is generally an
clinicians to provide a severity rating on a three-­ additional latency period in finding and setting up
point scale that describes the level of support intervention services. Depending on the city in
needed. Additionally, the DSM-5 provided a which the family is living, there could be as little
series of specifiers to describe impairments that as one or no professionals who specialize in pro-
commonly co-occur in individuals with autism viding treatment to children with autism. Then,
including intellectual disability, language impair- even if there are specialists in the area, parents
ments, or other neurodevelopmental, mental, or often struggle with lengthy wait lists.
behavioral disorders or catatonia. Furthermore, Multiple studies have implicated the impor-
the DSM-5 eliminated the age limitation which tance of early diagnosis and early treatment in the
allows for additional inclusion of individuals. positive outcome of children with ASD
However, due to the elimination of Asperger’s (McNamara, 2003). For example, in a study
disorder, those who display autistic traits that are investigating the outcome of 4-year-olds diag-
higher functioning are more likely to not receive nosed at age 2, they found that treatment
a diagnosis. improved symptoms to the point that many of the
children included in the study with a diagnosis of
ASD at age 2 were functioning at the level of a
 arly Treatment Requires Early
E typically developing child at age 4 (Sutera et al.,
Diagnosis 2007). In contrast, children diagnosed and pro-
vided treatment later in life displayed higher
The ability for a mental health professional or rates of “emotional difficulty,” and an overall
physician to diagnose a child with ASD is tied to “poor” or “fair” overall outcomes (Eaves & Ho,
the age of the child as well as the severity of 2008). It’s obvious that the earlier a child is diag-
symptoms. Children can be diagnosed as early as nosed, the better the outcome, but there are other
12 months of age, but these infants typically have factors, such as lower symptom severity, parent
more severe and noticeable differences such as participation, and higher maternal education that
language regression, unusual mannerisms, severe are associated with better outcomes as well
developmental delays, or having relatives with (Mhatre, Bapat, & Udami, 2016). These critical
ASD (Valicenti-McDermott, Hottinger, Seijo, & periods of diagnosis and treatment aid in
Shulman, 2012). Typically, ages 4–5 are when ­providing the child with a better grasp of verbal
the majority of children are diagnosed with ASD, and social skills to improve their overall quality
as it coincides with the commencement of pre- of life as they mature and adjust developmental
school or kindergarten when these children are trajectories.
expected to socialize with their peers but rather
begin displaying certain behavioral differences.
One of the main issues with the process of Measuring Empirical Support
diagnosis and treatment for children with ASD is
the latency periods. There is a range in time The empirical basis concerning ASD treatment
between when a parent notices potential symp- is both diverse and extensive. Interventions that
toms and when they receive a formal diagnosis, are considered “evidence based” in the treatment
often lasting 6 months or more. Additionally, of ASD symptoms have been shown to improve
many parents will intentionally avoid seeking out deficits in adaptive, cognitive, and social
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 141

f­ unctioning. There is a growing body of evidence s­tudies and substantial increases in IQ for
to support the efficacy of intensive behavioral ­participants receiving more intensive intervention.
interventions, which are typically one-on-one Based on many of the same principles, the
with the client and therapist and are carried out Early Start Denver Model (ESDM) is another
over 25–40 h per week. The interventions are intervention program geared specifically for
structured and designed with lessons broken young toddlers. The ESDM model has exhibited
down into simple repeated tasks or trials. In each significant improvements in autistic-related
trial, the client is presented with a stimulus and symptomology (Dawson et al., 2012; Estes et al.,
encouraged through the use of positive rein- 2015), and the practice seeks to promote the
forcement (Lovaas, 1987). Clients receiving development of neural systems for social interac-
intensive behavioral interventions have shown tion and consequently elevate the child’s social
improvements in their performance on measures motivation. The practice also tries to facilitate
of intellectual abilities, language skills, and active attention to faces by increasing a child’s
adaptive functioning (Howard, Sparkman, exposure to interpersonal exchanges. In addition
Cohen, Green, & Stanislaw, 2005). The follow- to significant gains in socio-communicative func-
ing will discuss seminal studies that have influ- tion, long-term participation in ESDM has
enced empirically based interventions used seemed to normalize electrophysiological brain
today. responses to facial stimuli in young children with
Empirically supported treatments are theory-­ autism (Dawson et al., 2012).
driven and testable treatments intended for a spe- Once behavioral interventions were identified
cific population or disorder. This led to some as empirically supported treatments, researchers
issues in the early to mid-1900s as there were still began investigating new methods of treatment
few clinicians that agreed on the diagnosis of that were more generalizable to natural environ-
autism, which made identifying a theoretical ments. For example, Koegel, O’Dell, and Koegel
platform for treatment implementation difficult. (1987) developed a treatment intended to improve
However, one of the first studies implementing a verbal abilities in nonverbal children with autism.
theory-driven empirically-based treatment was His study showed such improved gains that other
conducted by Lovaas and colleagues. His study, researcher began using the same “naturalistic”
termed the UCLA Young Autism Project, pro- approach with observed gains in areas such as
vided empirical support of interventions for indi- symbolic play, joint attention, disruptive behav-
viduals with ASD (Lovaas, 1987). In this study, ior, and social behavior to name a few (Koegel,
they incorporated behavioral analysis to evaluate Koegel, & Camarata, 2010). These studies
the educational effects of a behaviorally based brought about a new form of treatment modality,
intervention for young children diagnosed with referred to as Pivotal Response Treatment (dis-
ASD. The children received 10–40 h of interven- cussed in further detail below), which is a
tion per week at varying degrees of intensity. The scientifically-­
based approach to treatment that
outcome of the study showed that children receiv- allows for scientific review.
ing more frequent and intense interventions dis- In summary, the research literature shows
played significantly greater gains in terms of strong support for intensive behavioral interven-
intellectual ability. tion in the treatment of children with ASD. A
Additional studies have replicated these find- number of research studies strongly favor the use
ings (e.g., Sheinkopf & Siegel, 1998), and a meta- of intensive behavioral interventions with evi-
analysis of 34 behavioral intervention studies dence that is compelling in both its consistency
conducted by Eldevik et al. (2009) and later 16 and scope. However, research varies in efficacy
group design treatment studies (Eldevik et al., and the level of potential harm within different
2010) supported the use of intensive behavioral forms of treatment ranging from non-efficacious
intervention as the treatment of choice based on to efficacious and empirically validated. While
the superior outcomes found reliably across this chapter does not seek to review all
142 M.S. Lemay et al.

e­vidence-­based interventions (see Wong et al., (1998) studied participants using a randomized,
2015 for a more extensive review of evidence double-blind trial and found the use of vitamins
based practices) or unsubstantiated and poten- and other supplements displayed no significant
tially harmful treatments (see Foxx & Mulick, difference from controls. More recently, vitamin
2016 for an extensive resource on controversial supplements have been in use as a complimen-
therapies), many of the common treatments will tary or alternative medical treatment for ASD.
be discussed. Vitamin C is not commonly used as an iso-
lated treatment for ASD, but it is frequently com-
bined with other vitamins and given to children
 on-efficacious but Generally
N with ASD. Researchers studied the efficacy of
Benign Treatments vitamin C and reported a decrease in stereotyped
behavior in a double-blind trial of children with
Many therapies and interventions that are avail- ASD (Dolske, Spollen, McKay, Lancashire, &
able to families and individuals with ASD lack Tolbert, 1993); however, the results of this study
research support for their effectiveness in the have not been replicated.
actual treatment of ASD symptoms. Some of Although vitamins and minerals are a recom-
these are due to a lack of research in the area; mended part of a healthy diet, suggesting that
however, many of these have been found to be vitamins or minerals should be used as the pri-
ineffective through rigorous studies. Many of the mary treatment for ASD is unfounded and has
treatments/interventions included in this section little support. Vitamins and minerals do not have
are generally benign meaning they cause no sig- an effect on the impairment in verbal or nonver-
nificant harm to the individual or family but at the bal communication, imagination, reciprocal
same time offer little to no benefit in regard to social interaction, or developmental delays in
improving ASD symptoms or outcomes. ASD. Therefore, vitamins and minerals should
Regardless, many of these interventions are not be viewed as an alternative medical treat-
widely used and supported by family members, ment; instead vitamins and minerals should be
clinicians, and caregivers in spite of its unproven viewed as complimentary dietary supplements.
efficacy.

Camels’ Milk
Vitamin Supplements
Along the same vein of vitamins and minerals,
Linus Pauling introduced orthomolecular psy- milk is typically thought of as containing impor-
chiatry in the late 1960s, which argues for treat- tant nutrients for human nourishment. Camels’
ing mental illness using vitamins and minerals. milk is believed to have some potential ­therapeutic
Pauling believed that inadequate levels of vita- effects in diseases such as diabetes and hepatitis
mins and minerals could produce a number of B (Shabo, Barzel, Margoulis, & Yagil, 2005).
mental defects. Pauling’s theory has been used to Recently, some parents have used camels’ milk
support vitamin treatment for autism spectrum as an alternative treatment for children with
disorder. However, the use of vitamins in psycho- ASD because of the perceived therapeutic
logical and developmental disorders is not sup- capabilities.
ported in a number of well-designed studies Initially, experimentation using animals has
(Kozlowski, 1992). shown that a powerful opioid, casomorphin,
Over the past 20 years, B6 and magnesium causes autistic-like symptoms in animals (Shabo
have been a popular choice for ASD. However, & Yagil, 2005). This opioid is believed to ini-
there is still not enough evidence to support the tially cause cognitive and behavioral symptoms,
use of supplements for treatment of eventually leading to brain damage in animals.
ASD. Rimland, Findling, Maxwell, and Witnitzer Consequently, Shabo and Yagil suggest that we
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 143

restrict cows’ milk products because it can lead c­hildren without GI abnormalities, but this is
to the formation of casomorphin. Instead, Shabo strongly tied to parent’s perception of improve-
and Yagil recommend camels’ milk as it does not ment (Pennesi & Klein, 2012). Conversely,
contain beta-lactoglobulin or beta-casein like other research indicates that a gluten- and
cow’s milk. They also believed that there is addi- casein-free diet is not scientifically supported
tional benefit to camels’ milk in the form of and that these restrictive diets can cause stigma-
immunoglobulins, which are necessary for main- tization and reduced cortical bone thickness
taining the immune system and for healthy brain (Mulloy et al., 2010). Limiting a child’s diet is
development. not only suggested to be ineffective but can also
While it is true that camels’ milk lacks beta-­ become problematic as children with ASD
lactoglobulin and beta-casein, which are two already tend to have a limited food preference;
powerful allergens in cow’s milk, this only makes so by reducing that even more, major health
camels’ milk more appealing to children suffer- concerns may arise.
ing from allergies related to cow’s milk. However, Overall, the results of special diets are prob-
this would not necessarily mean a reduction in lematic because most of the studies rely on parent
ASD symptomology such as impairment in ver- report of the outcome variables. This method of
bal or nonverbal communication, reciprocal treatment is often seen as a simple solution for
social interaction, and developmental delays parents that is more cost-effective than the major-
(Bashir & Al-Ayadhi, 2013). Children with ity of the other treatments, as well as less harm-
severe food allergies may benefit from the unique ful. Therefore, parents become hopeful and
properties of camels’ milk, but it should be biased in their assessment of the effectiveness of
viewed as a complementary dietary supplement special diets when, in fact, there is little eviden-
for the general population and not as a primary tial support for the mechanism of change with
treatment option for individuals with ASD this treatment.
(Bashir & Al-Ayadhi).

Equine-Assisted Activities
Special Diets and Therapies

Some pediatricians and general physicians will Animal-assisted therapies have been used to
inform parents or patients that special diets can influence physiological factors including lower-
aid in autistic symptoms. More specifically, a ing blood pressure, regulating heart rate, and
casein- and gluten-free diet has had an increase in decreasing anxiety levels (Morrison, 2007).
popularity in recent years, boasting the curative Equine-assisted activities and therapies (EAAT)
effects of a clean and healthy diet on pain toler- are a subtype of animal-assisted therapy, which
ance, self-injurious behaviors, delayed or lost some have tried as an alternative treatment for
language, and attention. Other diets promoted individuals with ASD. The purpose of EAAT is
include the “Whole Food Diet” that claims to to use horseback riding activities to improve bal-
help with picky eating and hyperactivity, the ance, posture, and mobility of the client (Parish-­
“Specific Carbohydrate Diet,” which is suggested Plass, 2013). Another important goal of EAAT is
to help with distension, sleep, and attention, and to develop a therapeutic relationship or bond
the “Body Ecology Diet,” that is gluten free, between the client, therapist, and horse (All et al.,
casein free, and sugar free, with improvements 1999). Proponents of EAAT claim that it stimu-
in digestion, distension, and sleep (Ackerman, lates multiple domains of functioning for chil-
2015). dren with ASD. This therapy targets children
In children with ASD and gastrointestinal with neurological disorders who generally pres-
(GI) abnormalities, a gluten- and casein-free ent with a combination of motor, cognitive, and
diet can be beneficial compared to similar social disabilities (Fine, 2006).
144 M.S. Lemay et al.

In EAAT therapy, the horse represents a mul- esized that children with ASD would better focus
tisensory organism, which can assist children their attention to the stimuli in their environment,
with ASD to better utilize their own senses if a dolphin was present. That is because the dol-
(McDaniel, Osmann, & Wood, 2015). EAAT is phin would give the child an incentive, which
believed to help each child understand how their Nathanson thought would motivate the child to
bodies relate to external stimuli (Parish-Plass, complete each task and to give the appropriate
2013). Another potential benefit is that a child response in the therapy session (Nathanson,
with ASD may feel compelled to communicate 1998).
with the horse or the instructor during the therapy There are no set criteria for what constitutes
session because of the excitement they feel from DAT, and there are no regulations (Marino &
the experience (All et al., 1999). However, cau- Lilienfeld, 2007). Some proponents of DAT have
tion should be used with regard to the claims suggested that the technique works through sono-
EAAT and other animal-assisted therapies make. phoresis, in which the dolphins’ ultrasonic echo-
Replication and further research are needed to location influence is thought to cause positive
assess the claims and therapeutic effects of EAAT chemical and electrical changes in the individu-
with better controls (McDaniel et al., 2015). als in close proximity (Brensing & Linke, 2003).
To date, there is not sufficient evidence to However, the claim that echolocation serves as
show that EAAT benefits the cognitive, psycho- the therapeutic instrument in DAT has no scien-
logical, or social domains of individuals with tific merit (Humphries, 2003).
ASD (McDaniel et al., 2015). The only existing Psychologists have cautioned that DAT is not
meta-analysis of EAAT was presented to the effective for any known condition. While we
Society for the Study of Occupation. The paper included DAT in the benign category, DAT pres-
reviewed existing studies and noted that research ents considerable potential risks to the humans
is still inconclusive in regard to the effects of and the captive dolphins involved in the therapy
EAAT. Therefore, EAAT should not be consid- (Marino & Lilienfeld, 2007). DAT has been
ered a best practice or primary treatment option strongly criticized as having no long-term benefit
for individuals with ASD. and being based on flawed observations
(Humphries, 2003). For these reasons, the pur-
portedly therapeutic procedure of DAT should
Dolphin-Assisted Therapy not be considered a primary treatment option for
individuals with ASD.
Dolphin-assisted therapy (DAT) refers to the
implementation of therapy while swimming with
dolphins. DAT involves a child receiving the Hyperbaric Oxygen Therapy (HBOT)
reward of swimming with a dolphin after suc-
cessfully completing a therapy session Hyperbaric oxygen therapy (HBOT) is consid-
(Nathanson, 1998). The child’s success in the ered an alternative medicine treatment in which
therapy session is usually attributed to the moti- patients breathe pure oxygen in a pressurized
vating effects of swimming with a dolphin. chamber. While this treatment is considered a
Proponents of DAT claim extraordinary results well-established treatment for medical condi-
and breakthroughs for their clients (Marino & tions such as decompression sickness, arterial gas
Lilienfeld, 2007). The objective of DAT is to help embolisms, and carbon monoxide poisoning and
individuals with ASD by aiding in motor func- to aid in the healing of wounds, it has also been
tion, attention span, and language development used in treating individuals with ASD.
(Humphries, 2003). Some researchers have reported potential
David Nathanson, a clinical psychologist, improvements in behavioral functioning in chil-
studied DAT, and he is responsible for much of dren with ASD. For example, one study reported
the existing research on DAT. Nathanson hypoth- improvement on both clinician and parent reports
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 145

of behavior (Bent, Bertoglio, Ashwood, Nemeth, compared to other treatments discussed within
& Hendren, 2012); however, this study lacked this chapter.
any type of control group or objective measures
of behavior. Few rigorous studies have been con-
ducted utilizing HBOT, and of those that have  on-efficacious and Potentially
N
employed more rigorous methods, results sug- Harmful Treatments
gest no improvements in any ASD symptoms,
cognitive functioning, or common comorbid While there are many therapies that have been
symptoms (Xiong, Chen, Luo, & Mu, 2016). In proven ineffective or lack evidence supporting
fact, in a study of 16 children utilizing multiple their efficacy, there are also therapies within this
baseline methods (and 5 replications of those distinction that are not only ineffective but are
multiple baselines), researchers reported no sig- potentially dangerous and harmful. These are
nificant or consistent findings which demon- therapies which parents, caregivers, individuals,
strated the ineffectiveness of HBOT in treating clinicians, and others should avoid. In these
ASD (Jepson et al., 2011). While no studies have cases, the risks greatly outweigh any potential
found HBOT to be particularly harmful when benefits, and empirical studies have shown these
used correctly (although some studies have therapies/interventions to be ineffective.
reported mild ear barotrauma), the cost of HBOT
can be exorbitant and takes resources away from
more evidence-based practices. Miracle Mineral Solution (MMS)

Miracle Mineral Solution (MMS) was developed


Craniosacral Therapy by Jim Humble and is sold as a “miracle” cure for
almost any medical issue. Jim Humble does not
The theory behind craniosacral therapy was possess any credentials relating to ASD, medical
developed by John Upledger based on his biology, or chemistry, and furthermore, the ingre-
research at Michigan State University from 1975 dients associated with MMS are not medicinal in
to 1983 (Upledger, 2017). He believed that the nature, but rather the ingredients included in
reason children were experiencing autistic symp- MMS are toxic to the human system.
toms was due to a buildup of cerebrospinal fluid One of the known components of MMS is
in the brain, causing increases in pressure (Offit, industrial bleach, which makes up 28% of the
2010). Craniosacral therapy is a form of massage mixture and causes nauseas, severe vomiting,
that claims to use manipulation of the cranial lowered blood pressure, and fever (Williams,
bones and sutures to redirect the flow of cerebro- Dawling, & Seger, 2009). Proponents of MMS
spinal fluid (Vanes, 2013). Although this form of assure potential clients that the side effects are
therapy has been used for roughly three decades, part of a detox process that is both natural and
clinical trials have been limited. Research against healthy to undergo. These claims are false and
craniosacral therapy purports that there is no evi- dangerous. People experiencing negative symp-
dence to suggest that these children have any spi- tomology from the use of MMS should instead
nal fluid buildup. However, a recent study seek immediate medical attention (Williams
consisting of 124 patients found that craniosacral et al., 2009).
therapy shows improvement in cognitive func- Proponents of MMS have claimed that MMS
tion, communication, and social skills (Kratz, is effective in treating ASD, HIV, colon prob-
Kerr, & Porter, 2016); however, these results lems, brain cancer, heart disease, and skin dis-
have not been replicated. More research is needed eases and in regenerating the liver. Some people
to further support the use of this therapy though have even advocated for the use of MMS to treat
the recent study is suggestive of positive out- malaria, a claim that has been refuted by the
comes with fewer harmful side effects when International Federation of Red Cross and Red
146 M.S. Lemay et al.

Crescent Societies (IFRC). MMS is not approved Chelation Therapy


by the FDA or regulated in any way by the gov-
ernment. Unfortunately, MMS is still somewhat Chelation therapy follows a similar hypothesis
of an attractive option to parents seeking poten- with chemical castration such that there is too
tial solutions and alternative treatments for ASD much mercury in the blood of children with
(Brown, 2011). MMS is administered using sev- autism, and therefore it needs to be removed. The
eral different methods; parents are told to mix process of this therapy involves injecting a syn-
bleach in their child’s juices, baby bottles, and thetic solution (ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid,
even administer via enemas. To date, there are no EDTA) into the bloodstream to remove heavy
empirical journal articles that legitimize the metals or minerals. Although the Geiers (see
claims of MMS proponents or support the use of “Chemical Castration” section) were the first to
MMS as an effective treatment for ASD or any hypothesize chelation therapy could be an effec-
other condition. tive treatment for autism, it was J. B. Handley
who received credit for this treatment. He
founded Generation Rescue, an organization
Chemical Castration intended to provide parents of children with
autism the truth about their disorder and heal
One of the more harmful and less efficacious their children from mercury poisoning (Offit,
forms of treatment for individuals with ASD is 2010). However, results from randomized control
chemical castration, or the administration of studies are mixed at best, with most studies
medications that reduces the amount of testoster- expressing concern for the safety of the children
one and estrogen in the body. The most frequently (Sinha, Silove, & Williams, 2006). Then, in 2008,
used medication is Lupron, which is designed for the Journal of the American Medical Association
the treatment of prostate cancer, to suppress pro- published an article stating that the federal gov-
duction of testosterone. The theory behind ernment has halted any trials testing chelation
administration of the drug in treating ASD is that therapy, stating that the “studies had little scien-
it helps to remove mercury from the body, as high tific merit and exposed participants to unaccept-
mercury levels are thought by some parents to able safety risks” (Mitka, 2008). Much like many
cause autistic symptoms, while simultaneously other harmful treatments for autism, chelation
decreasing the child’s aggressive and sexual therapy has been deemed harmful and
behaviors. However, many medical experts argue non-efficacious.
against the claims that the medication, or mer-
cury in general, has anything to do with ASD
(Heasley, 2010). In fact, chemical castration Medicinal Marijuana
made the Forbes list of “5 Scariest Autism
Treatments,’” right after Miracle Mineral Some in the medical field argue that use of
Solution, (Willingham, 2013). medicinal marijuana is a potentially useful alter-
Chemical castration, developed by Mark and native treatment option for individuals with ASD
David Geier, was believed to decrease testoster- (Lucido, 2004). These advocates suggest that
one that was tied to mercury making it more medicinal marijuana has the ability to control
difficult to chelate. However, little research was epileptic seizures, relieve nausea, stimulate appe-
found in support of this treatment, and in fact, tite, help metabolism, decrease anxiety, ease
more research states that it has potentially pain, combat muscle spasms, and improve certain
harmful side effects. Unfortunately, the treat- autoimmune disorders (Cohen, 2009; Gilman,
ment had so much advertisement from the 2005; Levisohn, 2007; Martín-Sánchez,
Geiers that many unsuspecting parents are still Furukawa, Taylor, & Martin, 2009). However,
using chemical castration as a means to treat the degree to which medicinal marijuana helps
their children. with each of the aforementioned problems is still
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 147

highly debatable (Anderson, Hansen, & Rees, & Hansen, 2010). Increasingly, adverse events
2015). have been associated with spinal manipulation,
There are also a number of negative side including subarachnoid hemorrhage, vertebral
effects associated with the use of medicinal mari- dislocation, and quadriplegia (Vohra, Johnston,
juana that persons administering the drug must Cramer, & Humphreys, 2007). Furthermore, the
take into consideration before prescribing the cervical spine in children is fragile and particu-
drug. For instance, the human brain is not fully larly vulnerable to injury from the use of chiro-
developed in adolescence, and the brain does not practic manipulation (Akins et al., 2010).
fully develop until a person has reached their mid Proponents of chiropractic care suggest that
to late 20s (Anderson et al., 2015). The habitual cranial misalignments contribute to ASD symp-
use of marijuana is also associated with negative toms (Khorshid, Sweat, Zemba, & Zemba, 2006).
residual neuropsychological effects (Anderson According to these proponents, when the skull is
et al., 2015). Medicinal marijuana is believed to misaligned, the part of the brain adjacent to the
cause cognitive dysfunction and significant misalignment will suffer a greater pressure and a
pathology and may cause structural changes in decreased blood and nerve supply, which results
the brain (Brooks, 2012). Therefore, adolescents in hypoxia to that part of the brain (Wong &
and children are especially vulnerable to the neg- Smith, 2006). This is believed to adversely affect
ative effects of medicinal marijuana (Brooks, how that part of the brain functions (Khorshid
2012). et al., 2006).
The consequences of prolonged use of mari- The health claims made by chiropractors
juana are disconcerting, and each prescriber must regarding the application of manipulation as a
cautiously weigh the costs and benefits of its use. health care intervention for individuals with ASD
For these reasons, medical marijuana will likely continues to lack scientific support and credibil-
remain a questionable alternative treatment ity (Akins et al., 2010). To date, there are no pub-
option until further research is compiled to better lished randomized studies comparing the effects
assess its efficacy. Additionally, medicinal mari- of chiropractic manipulation in children with
juana should not be prescribed as the primary ASD (Gotlib & Rupert, 2008). Methodological
treatment for ASD because medicinal marijuana weaknesses including lack of a control group,
does little to nothing to combat impairment in unclear criteria for ASD diagnosis, and use of
verbal or nonverbal communication, reciprocal invalidated tools for measurement of ASD
social interaction, and developmental delays in improvement are present in a number of mislead-
ASD (Hadland, Knight, & Harris, 2015). ing findings that support the use of chiropractic
manipulation (Akins, et al., 2010). Therefore, the
chiropractic manipulation remains an unsafe and
Chiropractic Manipulation non-efficacious treatment option for individuals
with ASD.
A relatively new and increasingly popular alter-
native treatment for ASD is chiropractic manipu-
lation. An estimated 10% of parents of children Facilitated Communication (FC)
with ASD have used chiropractic care for their
children in the hopes of alleviating ASD-related Many individuals with ASD are also nonverbal or
symptoms (Hanson et al., 2007; Wong & Smith, non-communicative. In fact, it is estimated that
2006). approximately one-quarter of individuals living
Chiropractic manipulation of the spine is used with ASD are nonverbal with even higher rates of
by chiropractors to treat a variety of medical con- those that have significant communication defi-
ditions including ASD. The exact rationale for its cits. Many therapies have been developed to help
use in the treatment of core symptoms of ASD is augment or assist individuals in their ability to
unknown and not supported (Akins, Angkustsiri, communicate their wants and needs with others.
148 M.S. Lemay et al.

Facilitated communication (FC) is a technique Floortime (DIR)


which utilizes a trained facilitator to physically
aid the nonverbal individual in typing out their The developmental individual difference
thoughts. This was brought to the United States relationship-­based model (DIR) is described as a
by Douglas Biklen from Syracuse University in functional developmental approach, which exam-
the early 1990s but was quickly debunked in the ines how children integrate their motor, cogni-
mid-1990s by several researchers (Jacobson, tive, language, spatial, and sensory abilities to
Mulick, & Schwartz, 1995; Shane & Kearns, carry out emotionally meaningful goals
1994). Although the technique was thought to (Greenspan & Wieder, 1999). DIR is used to treat
have been disproven, the prevalence of FC con- children with a variety of developmental prob-
tinues to this day and the School of Education at lems, including children with ASD (Prelock &
Syracuse University and others continue to pro- McCauley, 2012). This approach, which is often
vide trainings for FC. referred to as “floortime,” remains popular with
FC is also used in many clinical applications families of children with ASD through the distri-
throughout the United States and internationally bution of publications, seminars, and websites
despite well-structured experiments that have (Prelock & McCauley).
invalidated the use of this method. FC can The model is based on Greenspan and
become dangerous in such instances of abuse Wieder’s (1999) affect diathesis hypothesis,
allegations or any situation in which the author- which suggests that children with symptoms of
ship of the communication comes into question ASD may have a biologically based processing
(Todd, 2012). FC has resulted in situations where deficit involving the connection of affect, symbol
families have been falsely accused of sexual or formation, motor planning, and sequencing
physical abuse as well as family-trusted clini- capacities (Wieder, 2013). According to the
cians betraying that trust and gaining false con- authors, the ability to connect affect to motor
sent for sexual relationships with a function and symbolic representation is a critical
non-communicative client (as is the case of Anna skill that develops in the second year of life
Stubblefield of Rutgers-Newark). Due to the lack (Greenspan & Wieder, 1999).
of evidence supporting FC in actual communica- The DIR approach appears attractive to par-
tion authored by the individual, but rather ents because of its unique aspects of emotional
authored by the facilitator, and the potential for reciprocity, which is not typically associated
serious harm, FC is considered an ineffective and with ASD. With its focus on emotional develop-
potentially dangerous treatment method. ment, the model gives hope to parents that nor-
mal social interactions can be achieved through
the use of DIR (Stephenson, Carter, & Kemp,
Potentially Efficacious Treatments 2012). Additionally, proponents of DIR have
accused behavioral interventions of not focusing
The research base of effective autism treatments on the emotional development of their client
is continually growing and maturing. That being base (Kalyva, 2011). These advocates then mis-
said, there are several treatments and interven- represent the DIR approach as unique in its abil-
tions that show promise in improving ASD symp- ity to provide parents with a warm relationship
toms or improving comorbid impairments but with their child with ASD (Carr & LeBlanc,
lack evidence to be included in our last category 2007). Despite emotional appeal and popularity
of empirically validated treatments. These of DIR, no well-controlled studies have been
include under-researched interventions relying published to document the effects of DIR (Carr
solely on small-case designs or lack controls to & LeBlanc). Overall, DIR lacks the necessary
prove the efficacy of these treatments but show empirical support to be considered a validated
significant potential based on the research that is treatment for ASD (Carr & LeBlanc; Stephenson
currently available. et al., 2012).
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 149

Pivotal Response Training (PRT) grow, there are several useful tools that have been
proven to be effective in improving outcomes for
Pivotal response training (PRT) is a behavioral individuals with ASD and should be considered
treatment method based loosely on the method of the first choice treatments of clinicians and fam-
applied behavior analysis (ABA) with a more ily members. Early intensive behavioral interven-
naturalistic teaching perspective and focus on tions that are based on applied behavior analysis
increasing motivation of the individual and (ABA) are currently viewed as the most effica-
implementation by parents/caregivers. These cious treatments and will not be discussed in
“pivotal responses” are behaviors that are consid- elsewhere. However, there are several treatments
ered essential to stimulating learning and moti- that incorporate some ABA methods that will be
vating responses in nontarget behavioral areas discussed here.
such as language, social interaction, play skills,
and other areas. It was created and validated for
use with children between 2 and 16 years of age Behavior Therapy
and has a relatively strong research base support-
ing its use and effectiveness. Mixed results exist in the effectiveness of behav-
As with other ABA-based behavioral inter- ioral treatments for the socially significant behav-
ventions, earlier intervention has been strongly iors of those with autism. One of the most
tied to better outcomes, especially when imple- well-known treatments for ASD is applied behav-
mented before 5 years of age. In addition to ior analysis (ABA), which is discussed further in
improvements in the target behaviors, one study another chapter. ABA is an applied science
observed increased verbal utterances from base- devoted to understanding the ways in which the
line measurements of 10–30% prior to PRT to environment affects one’s behavior to better
70–80% posttreatment (Bryson et al., 2007). understand behaviors and ways to improve
Likewise, another study observed significant socially relevant difficulties (Vismara & Rogers,
increases in all measured areas of adaptive and 2010). Other forms of efficacious behavioral
independent behavior as a result of a community-­ treatments for ASD fall under the realm of dis-
based PRT intervention across all age groups crete trial training (DTT), which breaks down
(Baker-Ericzén, Stahmer, & Burns, 2007). While complex skills to teach behaviors in a sequential
the evidence base for PRT relies primarily on building-block process (Lovaas, 1981). Each step
small case designs, a recent meta-analysis sug- consists of a specific set of instructions and goals,
gests outcome effectiveness in PRT models that oftentimes with the therapist modeling the appro-
focused on at least two of the three core features priate action or response. This form of behavioral
of ASD (Bozkus-Genc & Yucesoy-Ozkan, 2016). treatment has been found to increase IQ by an
Although current evidence suggests general effi- average of 20 points, and when an intensive form
cacy of PRT, further research (and more system- of DTT is applied to children under the age of 7,
atic and rigorous methods) is needed to compare their scores on standardized tests significantly
PRT against other well-validated ABA-based increase (Cohen, Amerine-Dickens, & Smith,
interventions (Cadogan & McCrimmon, 2015). 2006; Howard et al., 2005; Sallows & Graupner,
2005). However, DTT has been criticized for
being too restrictive in its presentation and use of
Empirically Validated Treatments stimuli, making it less generalizable to the indi-
vidual’s natural environment (Koegel et al., 1987;
Our final category includes treatments and inter- Schreibman, 1997).
ventions for ASD that have been shown through In response to the negative aspects of DTT,
rigorous and well-constructed studies and meta-­ new behavioral therapies came to light including
analyses to be well-validated and effective treat- incidental teaching and milieu teaching (Kaiser
ments. While we hope that this area continues to & Hester, 1996; McGee, Feldman, & Chernin,
150 M.S. Lemay et al.

1991). These new behavioral therapies are all verbal development (Vivanti & Dissanayake,
intended to create teaching opportunities within 2016). This supports the view that these skills
the individual’s naturally occurring events with develop in a very sensitive and critical period of
explicit prompting, reinforcing attempts, and nat- development that is specifically targeted by this
ural reinforcement. These forms of therapy intervention. Given this, meta-analyses support
increase applicability in natural settings, are the effectiveness of ESDM in treating young chil-
reported to be less aversive to individuals with dren with ASD (Canoy & Boholano, 2015), but
autism, and are more easily taught to caregivers, the effectiveness of this intervention is highly
thus reducing the frequent need of therapists. A dependent on the age of the child (earlier inter-
third form of behavioral treatment is the Early vention generally gives way to better outcomes)
Start Denver Model. and the intensity of the intervention (increased
hours generally correlated with better
outcomes).
Early Start Denver Model (ESDM)

Early treatment has been shown to be crucial to Social Skills Interventions


more positive outcomes for individuals with
ASD. One treatment model that has focused on Social skills interventions are treatments that
the early intervention of children between 12 and focus primarily on facets of one’s social behav-
48 months of age is the Early Start Denver Model ior. Individuals with autism frequently struggle
(ESDM). This treatment focuses on improving with socially related behaviors, and therefore
developmental trajectories by focusing on skills social skills interventions are applied to focus on
during sensitive periods of development (Rogers eye contact, appropriate content of speech (e.g.,
& Dawson, 2010). While ESDM was originally saying please and thank you), appropriate speech
developed to be conducted in a 1:1 setting, intonation, number of words spoken, appropriate
researchers have found that a group-based deliv- facial affect, appropriate motor movements, ver-
ery of ESDM has also been effective over other bal disruptions, and actions such as leaving a
group-based interventions (Vivanti et al., 2014). group or participating in conversational speech to
The intervention is based on practices of ABA name a few. The primary focus of these interven-
but incorporates a relationship-focused develop- tions is most often initiating conversation and
mental model to provide a more naturalistic making eye contact. These social skills are most
learning approach. This intervention helps to often taught in school settings, which allows for
address some of the criticisms of ABA methods increases in applicability and generalizability to
such as discrete trial training which have been the individual’s natural environment (Matson,
denigrated based on a lack of naturalistic Matson, & Rivet, 2007). Techniques imple-
learning. mented in social skills training include peer-­
While this method has not been shown to sig- mediated approaches, peer tutoring, social games,
nificantly reduce those symptoms specific to self-management, video modeling, direct instruc-
ASD (Rogers & Dawson, 2010; Vivanti & tion, visual cuing, circle of friends, and social-­
Dissanayake, 2016), it has been shown to be skills groups.
effective in increasing cognitive, adaptive, and Modeling and reinforcement are the most
language abilities in young children with long-­ applied form of social skills training and typi-
term gains maintained (Estes et al., 2015). The cally include an individual and confederate in
ESDM model has been demonstrated to be ben- which a social situation aimed at eliciting a
eficial to children over 48 months of age; how- response from the target individual is presented.
ever, superior gains have been found in children The response is rated, and feedback is provided
receiving this intervention before 48 months of on successes and ways in which they can still
age, especially with respect to language and over- improve. Social and edible rewards are also
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 151

p­rovided as reinforcement for appropriate Researchers believe that by mediating the sup-
responding. However, this method of social skills pressive immune activity, mesenchymal stem
training is more effective for higher functioning cells could be able to restore balance in the
individuals. In peer-mediated interventions, the immune system (Hu et al., 2011).
peers (most often children) are trained in how to Although mesenchymal stem cells or other
model and prompt appropriate social behaviors medical research options may 1 day be viable
(Matson et al., 2007). Unfortunately, this becomes treatment options for individuals with ASD, there
very limited in younger children because teach- is not currently enough medical research to sup-
ing them how to model appropriate behaviors and port the use of mesenchymal stem cells in ASD
prompt is not easily done. There are additional populations (Siniscalco et al., 2012).
ways of doing social skills training, including at
home with parents and with a psychologist,
speech therapist, or occupational therapist, all of Oxytocin Inhalation
which have been proven to be effective (Matson,
et al., 2007). Although this intervention is Oxytocin, a neuropeptide believed to be respon-
included in the “Empirically Validated sible for increased social interactions, has
Treatments” section, it should be noted that not received growing interest in autism research. It
all social skills interventions have been proven to is theorized that this naturally occurring hor-
be efficacious. mone, which is now in synthetic form, can be
administered through the nasal cavity of chil-
dren and adolescents with ASD to increase their
Experimental Treatments social interactions. However, in a randomized
control trial testing the effects of oxytocin on
With the development of new information in emotion regulation, social interaction, and gen-
ASD, treatments continue to form based on these eral behavioral adjustment in children diag-
findings. Treatments that are still undergoing nosed with ASD, results did not indicate any
research but have not had significant support for significant improvements in the oxytocin group
or against their efficacy or harm will be discussed (Lin et al., 2014). Additionally, oxytocin did not
below. improve emotion recognition or repetitive
behaviors in this sample. Conversely, other
results suggest that oxytocin can increase visual
Stem Cells scanning of faces, including the eye area,
increased social playing and interaction, and
Mesenchymal stem cells and cord blood cells improved ability to process socially relevant
have been suggested as a method to treat ASD, cues and acquire their meaning in an interacting
but this proposal has not been fully tested (Ichim context (Andari et al., 2010).
et al., 2007). Some researchers believe that mes- From a neurological level, the administration
enchymal stem cells may represent a significant of oxytocin into patients with ASD increases the
future treatment option for individuals with ASD blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) activa-
(Hu et al., 2011). Since immune system deregula- tion in visual areas of the brain selectively for
tion has been associated with ASD, mesenchy- faces. Oxytocin also activated the anterior orbital
mal stem cells show reasonable potential as a frontal cortex, a region of the brain known for its
treatment for the disorder. That is because indi- role in reward value representation, during a
viduals with ASD tend to have an imbalance in T social reciprocity task (Andari, Richard, Leboyer,
lymphocytes cells and natural killer (NK) cells & Sirigu, 2016). Although this displays the effect
(Siniscalco et al., 2012). In addition, peripheral of oxytocin at a neurological level, research is
blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) overproduce still inconclusive for the effectiveness at the
leukocytic pyrogen (Ichim et al., 2007). behavioral level.
152 M.S. Lemay et al.

 reatment of Comorbid
T Cognitive Behavior Therapy
Psychopathology
Individuals diagnosed with autism oftentimes
Patients with ASD will oftentimes display comor- will have a series of comorbid disorders includ-
bid psychological disorders such as intellectual ing mood disorders, anxiety, and obsessive-­
disabilities, ADHD, depression, anxiety, and compulsive disorder (OCD). One way in which
other psychological disorders tied to behavior these disorders are treated is through the use of
such as obsessive-compulsive disorder or con- cognitive behavior therapy (CBT). Although
duct disorder. Because of that, treatments have CBT is used in this population, the treatment is
been created with the primary aim of treating focused more on the comorbid disorders instead
these comorbid disorders. of the symptoms of ASD itself. Treatments have
been modified for those with autism to focus on
the thoughts and behaviors associated with OCD,
Medication mood disorders, and anxiety and have shown to
be highly effective in higher functioning individ-
Pharmacotherapy for the treatment of ASD would uals (Krebs, Murray, & Jassi, 2016; Lang,
appear to be a simple solution for a lifetime dis- Regester, Lauderdale, Ashbaugh, & Haring,
order. However, as of yet, there exists no medica- 2010; Scattone & Mong, 2013). However, many
tion to effectively treat the disorder itself. Much of the studies investigating CBT with autism
like the use of antipsychotic medications to treat spectrum disorders focused on those with
disorders such as schizophrenia, medication used Asperger’s and/or generally lower severity of
in patients with autism is only effective for cer- symptoms. More research is therefore required
tain “problem behaviors” and varies on an indi- for the application of CBT more broadly.
vidual basis. A series of medications including
fluvoxamine, sertraline (a selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitor), clomipramine (a tricyclic Future Research Directions
antidepressant), and risperidone and ziprasidone
(atypical antipsychotics) have been applied to With the shift in psychological research toward a
patients with an ASD diagnosis for the treatment more genetic or biological model, more studies
of symptoms including aggressive behavior, are focusing on neurological and biological com-
repetitive behavior, and self-injurious behaviors ponents of autism (Muotri, 2016). By investigat-
(Sawyer, Lake, Lunsky, Liu, & Desarkar, 2014). ing the molecular compounds and development
In a review of medication efficacy, Sawyer et al. of the disorder, researchers will be able to
(2014) found that fluvoxamine and risperidone increase knowledge regarding effective medica-
were the most effective in treating repetitive tion for treatment of the disorder. That being said,
behavior, and risperidone and ziprasidone were obtaining an adequate sample size is important to
the most effective in treating self-injurious assess for generalizability. One of the limitations
behavior. Additionally, the summation of research within current treatments today is that much of
indicates that certain Federal Drug Administration the literature is based on DSM-IV diagnoses
(FDA)-approved medications such as risperi- which separate Asperger’s from autism. With the
done, fluvoxamine, and ziprasidone are effective implementation of a spectrum for diagnosis
in treating aggressive behaviors, whereas sertra- based on severity of symptoms and functioning,
line was noted to cause increases in aggression. future research and treatment should be looking
Research therefore indicates that certain forms of at differences in treatment within this spectrum.
medication can be effective in treating some of In terms of research-specific directions, ani-
the challenging behaviors associated with ASD, mal models continue to be perfected in mimick-
but no such medication currently exists to treat ing the symptoms of autism, while psychotropic
the core symptoms of the disorder. medications continue to be developed to target
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 153

changes in brain functioning (Damiano, endeavor. While future research is needed in the
Mazefsky, White, & Dichter, 2014). However, treatment of ASD, especially for adolescent and
due to the fact that approximately 50% of indi- adults, there are several resources at the hands of
viduals with a diagnosis of ASD display positive well-trained clinicians to improve social func-
improvements as a result of receiving an tioning, adaptive living skills, communication,
evidence-­based treatment, we see an increased and the myriad of comorbid issues that often
need for improving our understanding of the dis- accompany an ASD diagnosis. As researchers
order and its functioning. As the rates of ASD and clinicians who work with individuals with
increase, identifying effective and efficacious ASD, we need to place greater focus and value in
treatments becomes more imperative. empirically validated and evidence-based treat-
As discussed earlier within this chapter, early ments and better inform clients and families
identification of symptoms or early diagnosis is regarding the vast array of unsubstantiated and
directly related to improved outcomes later in potentially harmful treatments that continue to
life. However, many children are not diagnosed persist among the options for treatment.
until they begin school, if not later. This issue
could be mended by improving screenings for
ASD in pediatric settings and a better way to cat- References
egorize children who do not yet meet criteria for
a diagnosis but still exhibit symptoms (Goin-­ Ackerman, L. (2015). Special Diets for ASD. Talk About
Kochel, Mackintosh, & Myers, 2006). Similarly, Curing Autism (TACA). Retrieved from: https://www.
studies should look at risk factors related to the tacanow.org/family-resources/special-diets-for-asd/
Akins, R. S., Angkustsiri, K., & Hansen, R. L. (2010).
development of ASD as a means of identifying Complementary and alternative medicine in
children that may potentially develop the disor- autism: An evidence-based approach to ­negotiating
der later on. safe and efficacious interventions with families.
Lastly, much of the research on treatments for Neurotherapeutics, 7(3), 307–319.
All, A. C., Loving, G. L., & Crane, L. L. (1999). Animals,
individuals with ASD is targeted primarily on horseback riding, and implications for rehabilitation
infants and children. This leads to issues with therapy. Journal of Rehabilitation, 65(3), 49.
adolescents and adults because it is difficult to American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic
transform the evidence-based treatments so that and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
they apply to an older demographic. In fact, a American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5 Task Force, &
review of literature conducted in 2013 discovered American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
that there were only 13 randomized controlled and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-­
trials of interventions for adults with ASD 5 (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Association.
(Bishop-Fitzpatrick, Minshew, & Eack, 2013). American Psychiatric Association. Work Group to Revise
By conducting more studies with an adolescent DSM-III, & American Psychiatric Association.
and adult population, mental health professionals (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-
will be better able to implement effective treat- tal disorders: DSM-III-R (3rd ed.). Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Association.
ments depending on the severity of symptoms. Andari, E., Duhamel, J., Zalla, T., Herbrecht, E., Leboyer,
M., Sirigu, A., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2010). Promoting
social behavior with oxytocin in high-­ functioning
Conclusion autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 107(9), 4389–4394. https://doi.org/10.1073/
The treatment of ASD is a complicated and often pnas.0910249107
slowly progressing task. However, with careful Andari, E., Richard, N., Leboyer, M., & Sirigu, A. (2016).
treatment planning and reliance on those inter- Adaptive coding of the value of social cues with
oxytocin, an fMRI study in autism spectrum disor-
ventions that are empirically validated, the task der. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the
of improving the lives of individuals with ASD Nervous System and Behavior, 76, 79–88. https://doi.
(and their families) can be a highly rewarding org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.010
154 M.S. Lemay et al.

Anderson, D. M., Hansen, B., & Rees, D. I. (2015). Medical Cohen, D. J., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. R. (1986). Issues in
marijuana laws and teen marijuana use. American Law the classification of pervasive and other developmental
and Economics Review, 17(2), 495–528. disorders: Toward DSM-IV. Journal of the American
Baker-Ericzén, M. J., Stahmer, A. C., & Burns, A. Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, 213–220.
(2007). Child demographics associated with out- Cohen, H., Amerine-Dickens, M., & Smith, T. (2006).
comes in a community-based pivotal response Early intensive behavioral treatment: Replication of
training program. Journal of Positive Behavior the UCLA model in a community setting. Journal
Interventions, 9, 52–60. of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2),
Bashir, S., & Al-Ayadhi, L. Y. (2013). Effect of camel S145–S155.
milk on thymus and activation-regulated chemokine Cohen, P. J. (2009). Medical marijuana: The conflict
in autistic children: Double-blind study. Pediatric between scientific evidence and political ideology.
Research, 75(4), 559–563. Part one of two. Journal of Pain & Palliative Care
Bent, S., Bertoglio, K., Ashwood, P., Nemeth, E., & Pharmacotherapy, 23(1), 4–25.
Hendren, R. (2012). Brief report: Hyperbaric oxy- Damiano, C. R., Mazefsky, C. A., White, S. W., & Dichter,
gen therapy (HBOT) in children with autism spec- G. S. (2014). Future directions for research in autism
trum disorder: A clinical trial. Journal of Autism & spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Child &
Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1127–1132. https:// Adolescent Psychology, 43(5), 828–843. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1337-3 org/10.1080/15374416.2014.945214
Bishop-Fitzpatrick, L., Minshew, N. J., & Eack, S. M. Dawson, G., Jones, E. J., Merkle, K., Venema, K.,
(2013). A systematic review of psychosocial inter- Lowy, R., Faja, S., & Webb, S. J. (2012). Early
ventions for adults with autism spectrum disorders. behavioral intervention is associated with normal-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, ized brain activity in young children with autism.
687–694. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Bleuler, E. (1913). Autistic thinking. American Journal of Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(11), 1150–1159. https://
Insanity, 69, 873–886. doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.018
Bozkus-Genc, G., & Yucesoy-Ozkan, S. (2016). Meta-­ De Bruin, E. I., de Nijs, P. F. A., Verheij, F., Hartman,
analysis of Pivotal Response Training for children with C. A., & Ferdinand, R. F. (2006). Multiple complex
autism spectrum disorder. Education and Training in developmental disorder delineated from PDD-NOS.
Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 51, 13–26. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37,
Brensing, K., & Linke, K. (2003). Behavior of dolphins 1181–1191.
towards adults and children during swim-with-dolphin Deisinger, J. A. (2011). History of autism spectrum disor-
programs and towards children with disabilities during ders. In A. F. Rotatori, F. Obiakor, & J. Bakken (Eds.).,
therapy sessions. Anthrozoös, 16(4), 315–331. 2011 History of special education. Bradford: Emerald
Brooks, J. (2012). Interview with U.S. Attorney Haag on Group Publishing Limited.
pot operations: ‘If it’s close to children, that’s a line Dolske, M. C., Spollen, J., McKay, S., Lancashire, E.,
we’re going to draw.’ KQED News. Retrieved from: & Tolbert, L. (1993). A preliminary trial of ascorbic
http://blogs.kqed.org/newsfix/2012/03/15/interview- acid as supplemental therapy for autism. Progress
w-us-attorney-haag-on-potoperations-if-its-close-to- in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological
children-thats-a-line-were-going-to-draw/ Psychiatry, 17(5), 765–774.
Brown, J. A. (2011). Toxicity from the use of miracle min- Eaves, L. C., & Ho, H. H. (2008). Young adult outcome
eral solution (sodium chlorite). Clinical Toxicology, of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
49(3), 205. and Developmental Disorders, 38, 739. https://doi.
Bryson, S. E., Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Openden, org/10.1007/s10803-007-0441-x
D., Smith, I. M., & Nefdt, N. (2007). Large scale Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E.,
dissemination and community implementation of Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2009). Meta-analysis of
pivotal response treatment: Program description of early intensive behavioral intervention for children
preliminary data. Research & Practice for Persons with autism. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
with Severe Disabilities, 32, 142–153. Psychology, 38(3), 439–450.
Cadogan, S., & McCrimmon, A. W. (2015). Pivotal Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E.,
response treatment for children with autism spectrum Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2010). Using partici-
disorder: A systematic review of research quality. pant data to extend the evidence base for intensive
Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 18, 137–144. behavioral intervention for children with autism.
Canoy, J. P., & Boholano, H. B. (2015). Early start American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
DENVER model: A meta-analysis. Journal of Disabilities, 115(5), 381–405.
Education and Learning, 9, 314–327. Estes, A., Munson, J., Rogers, S. J., Greenson, J., Winter,
Carr, J. E., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2007). Autism spectrum dis- J., & Dawson, G. (2015). Long-term outcomes of
orders in early childhood: An overview for practicing early intervention in 6-year-old children with autism
physicians. Primary Care; Clinics in Office Practice, spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy
34(2), 343–359. of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(7), 580–587.
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 155

Fine, A. H. (2006). Handbook on animal-assisted ther- p­ seudoscience, and antiscience science working group
apy: Theoretical foundations and guidelines for prac- on facilitated communication. American Psychologist,
tice (2nd ed.). Academic Press. 50, 750–765.
Foxx, R. M., & Mulick, J. A. (Eds.). (2016). Controversial Jepson, B., Granpeesheh, D., Tarbox, J., Olive, M., Stott,
therapies for autism and intellectual disabilities: Fad, C., Braud, S., & ... Allen, M. (2011). Controlled evalu-
fashion, and science in professional practice (2nd ed.). ation of the effects of hyperbaric oxygen therapy on the
New York: Routledge. behavior of 16 children with autism Spectrum disor-
Gilman, V. (2005). The marijuana debate: Healing herb ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
or dangerous drug? National Geographic News. 41(5), 575–588. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/
Retrieved from: http://news.nationalgeographic.com/ s10803-010-1075-y
news/2005/06/0621_050621_marijuana.html Kaiser, A. P., & Hester, P. P. (1996). How everyday envi-
Goin-Kochel, R. P., Mackintosh, V. H., & Myers, B. J. ronments support children’s communication. In L. K.
(2006). How many doctors does it take to make an Koegel & R. L. Koegel (Eds.), Positive behavioral
autism spectrum diagnosis? Autism, 10, 439–451. support: Including people with difficult behavior in
Goldstein, S., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Historical perspec- the community (pp. 145–162). Baltimore: Brookes.
tive and overview. In S. Goldstein, 1952, J. A. Naglieri, Kalyva, E. (2011). Autism: Educational and therapeutic
& S. Ozonoff (2009). Assessment of autism spectrum approaches. Portland, OR: Sage Publications.
disorders. New York: Guilford Press. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con-
Gotlib, A., & Rupert, R. (2008). Chiropractic manipula- tact. The Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
tion in pediatric health conditions–an updated system- Kanner, L. (1944). Early infantile autism. The Journal of
atic review. Chiropractic & Osteopathy, 16(1), 1. Pediatrics, 25, 211–217.
Greenspan, S., & Wieder, S. (1999). A functional devel- Kanner, L. (1954). To what extent is early infantile autism
opmental approach to autism spectrum disorders. determined by constitutional inadequacies? Research
Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Publications - Association for Research in Nervous
Handicaps, 24(3), 147–161. and Mental Disease, 33, 378–385.
Hadland, S. E., Knight, J. R., & Harris, S. K. (2015). Kanner, L., & Eisenberg, L. (1957). Early infantile autism,
Medical marijuana: Review of the science and implica- 1943–1955. Psychiatric Research Reports, 7, 55–65.
tions for developmental behavioral pediatric practice. Khorshid, K. A., Sweat, R. W., Zemba, D. A., & Zemba,
Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics: B. N. (2006). Clinical efficacy of upper cervical versus
JDBP, 36(2), 115. full spine chiropractic care on children with autism:
Hanson, E., Kalish, L. A., Bunce, E., Curtis, C., McDaniel, A randomized clinical trial. Annals of Vertebral
S., Ware, J., & Petry, J. (2007). Use of complementary Subluxation Research, 9, 1–7.
and alternative medicine among children diagnosed Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., & Camarata, S. M. (2010).
with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Definitions of empirically supported treatment.
Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 628–636. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Heasley, S. (2010). Chemical castration drug peedled 40(4), 516–517. http://dx.doi.org.unr.idm.oclc.
as autism treatment. Disability Scoop. Retrieved org/10.1007/s10803-009-0933-y
from https://www.disabilityscoop.com/2010/08/03/ Koegel, R. L., O’Dell, M. C., & Koegel, L. K. (1987).
lupron-autism/9631/ A natural language paradigm for teaching non-verbal
Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G., autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
& Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive Disorders, 17, 187–199.
behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young Kozlowski, B. W. (1992). Megavitamin treatment of
children with autism. Research in Developmental mental retardation in children: A review of effects
Disabilities, 26(4), 359–383. on behavior and cognition. Journal of Child and
Hu, Y., Li, J., Liu, P., Chen, X., Guo, D. H., Li, Q. S., Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 2(4), 307–320.
& Rahman, K. (2011). Protection of SH-SY5Y Kratz, S. V., Kerr, J., & Porter, L. (2016). The use of
neuronal cells from glutamate-induced apopto- CranioSacral therapy for autism spectrum disorders:
sis by 3, 6′-disinapoyl sucrose, a bioactive com- Benefits from the viewpoints of parents, clients,
pound isolated from radix polygala. Journal of and therapists. Journal of Bodywork and Movement
Biomedicine & Biotechnology, 2012, 1–5. https://doi. Therapies. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbmt.2016.06.006
org/10.1155/2012/728342 Krebs, G., Murray, K., & Jassi, A. (2016). Modified cog-
Humphries, T. L. (2003). Effectiveness of dolphin-­ nitive behavior therapy for severe, treatment-­resistant
assisted therapy as a behavioral intervention for young obsessive-compulsive disorder in an adolescent
children with disabilities. Bridges, 1(6), 1–9. with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical
Ichim, T. E., Solano, F., Glenn, E., Morales, F., Smith, L., Psychology, 72, 1162–1173.
Zabrecky, G., & Riordan, N. H. (2007). Stem cell therapy Lang, R., Regester, A., Lauderdale, S., Ashbaugh,
for autism. Journal of Translational Medicine, 5(1), 1. K., & Haring, A. (2010). Treatment of anxiety in
Jacobson, J. W., Mulick, J. A., & Schwartz, A. A. (1995). autism spectrum disorders using cognitive behav-
A history of facilitated communication: Science, iour therapy: A systematic review. Developmental
156 M.S. Lemay et al.

Neurorehabilitation, 13(1), 53–63. https://doi. Mulloy, A., Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni,
org/10.3109/17518420903236288 G., & Rispoli, M. (2010). Gluten-free and casein-free
Levisohn, P. M. (2007). The autism-epilepsy connection. diets in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders:
Epilepsia, 48(s9), 33–35. A systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum
Lin, I., Kashino, M., Ohta, H., Yamada, T., Tani, M., Disorders, 4, 328–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Watanabe, H., … Kato, N. (2014). The effect of rasd.2009.10.008
intranasal oxytocin versus placebo treatment on the Muotri, A. R. (2016). The human model: Changing
autonomic responses to human sounds in autism: A focus on autism research. Biological Psychiatry, 79,
single-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, cross- 642–649.
over design study. Molecular Autism, 5(1), 20–20. Nathanson, D. E. (1998). Long-term effectiveness of
doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-20 dolphin-assisted therapy for children with severe dis-
Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally dis- abilities. Anthrozoös, 11(1), 22–32.
abled children: The ME book. Baltimore: Univ. Park Offit, P. A. (2010). Autism’s false prophets: Bad science,
Press. risky medicine, and the search for a cure (Paperback
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal ed.). Chichester, UK: Columbia University Press.
educational and intellectual functioning in young Parish-Plass, N. (2013). Animal-assisted psychotherapy:
autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Theory, issues, and practice. West Lafayette, IN:
Psychology, 55, 3–9. Purdue University Press.
Lucido, F. (2004). Implementation of the compassionate Pennesi, C. M., & Klein, L. C. (2012). Effectiveness of
use act in a family medicine practice: Seven years clin- the gluten-free, casein-free diet for children diag-
ical experience. Retrieved from https://drfranklucido. nosed with autism spectrum disorder: Based on paren-
com/implementation-of-the-compassionate-use-act- tal report. Nutritional Neuroscience, 15(2), 85–91.
in-a-family-medical-practice-seven-years-clinical- https://doi.org/10.1179/1476830512Y.0000000003
experience/ Prelock, P. A., & McCauley, R. J. (Eds.). (2012). Treatment
Marino, L., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Dolphin-assisted of autism Spectrum disorders: Evidence-­based inter-
therapy: More flawed data and more flawed conclu- vention strategies for communication and social inter-
sions. Anthrozoös, 20(3), 239–249. actions. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.
Martín-Sánchez, E., Furukawa, T. A., Taylor, J., & Martin, Rimland, B., Findling, R. L., Maxwell, K., & Witnitzer,
J. L. R. (2009). Systematic review and meta analysis M. (1998). High dose vitamin B6 and magnesium in
of cannabis treatment for chronic pain. Pain Medicine, treating autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
10(8), 1353–1368. Disorders, 28(6), 581–582.
Matson, J. L., Matson, M. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2007). Social-­ Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). Early start Denver
skills treatments for children with autism spectrum model for young children with ASD: Promoting lan-
disorders: An overview. Behavior Modification, 31(5), guage, learning, and engagement. New York: Guilford
682–707. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445507301650 Press.
McDaniel, B. C., Osmann, E. L., & Wood, W. (2015). Rutter, M. (1972). Childhood schizophrenia reconsidered.
A systematic mapping review of equine-assisted Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2(3),
activities and therapies for children with autism: 315–337.
Implications for occupational therapy. Paper presented Rutter, M., Lebovici, S., Eisenberg, L., Sneznevskij, A. V.,
at the Annual Research Conference of Society for the Sadoun, R., Brooke, E., & Lin, T. Y. (1969). A tri-axial
Study of Occupation: USA. classification of mental disorders in childhood. An
McGee, G. C., Geldman, R. S., & Chernin, L. (1991). A international study. Journal Of Child Psychology and
comparison of emotional facial display by children Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 10, 41–61.
with autism and typical preschoolers. Journal of Early Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Intensive
Intervention, 15, 237–245. behavioral treatment for children with autism: Four-­
McNamara, D. (2003). Early diagnosis optimizes autism year outcome and predictors. American Journal of
outcomes: Merits of the Miami model. Family Practice Mental Retardation, 110, 417–438.
News, 33(22), 41. Sawyer, A., Lake, J., Lunsky, Y., Liu, A., & Desarkar, P.
Mhatre, D., Bapat, D., & Udani, V. (2016). Long-term (2014). Psychopharmacological treatment of challeng-
outcomes in children diagnosed with autism spec- ing behaviours in adults with autism and intellectual
trum disorders in India. Journal of Autism and disabilities: A systematic review. Research in Autism
Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 760–772. https://doi. Spectrum Disorders, 8(2014), 803–813.
org/10.1007/s10803-015-2613-4 Scattone, D., & Mong, M. (2013). Cognitive behavior
Mitka, M. (2008). Chelation therapy trials halted. therapy in the treatment of anxiety for adolescents and
JAMA, 300(19), 2236–2236. https://doi.org/10.1001/ adults with autism spectrum disorders. Psychology
jama.2008.607 in the Schools, 50, 923–935. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Morrison, M. L. (2007). Health benefits of animal-assisted pits.21717
interventions. Complementary Health Practice Schreibman, L. (1997). Theoretical perspectives on
Review, 12(1), 51–62. behavioral intervention for individuals with autism.
Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders 157

In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of Vanes, D. (2013). Craniosacral therapy. In D. Vanes (Ed.),
autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd Taber’s Cyclopedic Medical Dictionary (22nd ed.,
ed., pp. 920–933). New York: Wiley. p. 584). Philadelphia: F. A. Davis Company.
Shabo, Y., Barzel, R., Margoulis, M., & Yagil, R. (2005). Vismara, L. A., & Rogers, S. J. (2010). Behavioral
Camel milk for food allergies in children. The Israel treatments in autism spectrum disorder: What do
Medicine Association Journal, 7(12), 796. we know? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,
Shabo, Y., & Yagil, R. (2005). Etiology of autism and 6(1), 447–468. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
camel milk as therapy. International Journal on clinpsy.121208.131151
Disability and Human Development, 4(2), 67–70. Vivanti, G., & Dissanayake, C. (2016). Outcome for chil-
Shane, H. C., & Kearns, K. (1994). An examination of dren receiving the early start Denver model before and
the role of the facilitator in facilitated communication. after 48 months. Journal of Autism and Developmental
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 3, Disorders, 46(7), 2441–2449. https://doi.org/10.1007/
48–54. s10803-016-2777-6
Sheinkopf, S. J., & Siegel, B. (1998). Home-based behav- Vivanti, G., Paynter, J., Duncan, E., Fothergill, H.,
ioral treatment of young children with autism. Journal Dissanayake, C., & Rogers, S. (2014). Effectiveness
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(1), 15–23. and feasibility of the early start Denver model imple-
Sinha, Y., Silove, N., & Williams, K. (2006). Chelation mented in a group-based community childcare set-
therapy and autism. BMJ [British Medical Journal], ting. Journal Of Autism & Developmental Disorders,
333(7571), 756. 44(12), 3140–3153. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Siniscalco, D., Sapone, A., Cirillo, A., Giordano, C., Maione, s10803-014-2168-9
S., & Antonucci, N. (2012). Autism spectrum disorders: Vohra, S., Johnston, B. C., Cramer, K., & Humphreys,
Is mesenchymal stem cell personalized therapy the K. (2007). Adverse events associated with pediatric
future? Journal of Biomedicine and Biotechnology, 2012, spinal manipulation: A systematic review. Pediatrics,
1–6. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/480289 119(1), e275–e283.
Stephenson, J., Carter, M., & Kemp, C. (2012). Quality Wieder, S. (2013). Developmental, individual difference,
of the information on educational and therapy inter- relationship-based (DIR) model. In Encyclopedia of
ventions provided on the web sites of national autism autism spectrum disorders (pp. 895–905). New York:
associations. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Springer.
6(1), 11–18. Williams, S. R., Dawling, S., & Seger, D. L. (2009). Severe
Sutera, S., Pandey, J., Esser, E. L., Rosenthal, M. A., hemolysis in pediatric case after ingestion of miracle
Wilson, L. B., Barton, M.,… Fein, D. (2007). mineral solution (TM). Clinical Toxicology(47, 7,
Predictors of optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed 737–737). New York: Informa Healthcare.
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Willingham, E. (2013). The 5 scariest autism ‘treatments.’
Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 98–107. doi:https:// Forbes (Pharma & Healthcare). Retrieved from http://
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0340-6 www.forbes.com/sites/emilywillingham/2013/10/29/
Todd, J. (2012). The moral obligation to be empirical: the-5-scariest-autism-treatments/#26c2789b5fc7
Comments on Boynton’s “Facilitated Communication-­ Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig,
what harm it can do: Confessions of a former facilita- A., Kucharczyk, S., & Shultz, T. R. (2015). Evidence-­
tor.”. Evidence-Based Communication Assessment based practices for children, youth, and young
and Intervention, 6, 36–57. adults with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehen-
Towbin, K. E., Dykens, E. M., Pearson, G. S., & Cohen, sive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
D. J. (1993). Conceptualizing “borderline syndrome of Disorders, 45, 1951–1966.
childhood” and “childhood schizophrenia” as a devel- Wong, H. H., & Smith, R. G. (2006). Patterns of com-
opmental disorder. Journal of the American Academcy plementary and alternative medical therapy use in
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 775–782. children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders.
Upledger, J. (2017). Discover craniosacral therapy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Upledger Institute International. Retrieved from www. 36(7), 901–909.
upledger.com/therapies Xiong, T., Chen, H., Luo, R., & Mu, D. (2016). Hyperbaric
Valicenti-McDermott, M., Hottinger, K., Seijo, R., & oxygen therapy for people with autism spectrum
Shulman, L. (2012). Age at diagnosis of autism spec- disorder (ASD). Cochrane Database of Systematic
trum disorders. The Journal of Pediatrics, 161(3), Reviews, 11. ­https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.
554–556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.05.012 CD010922.pub2
Treatment Strategies
for Depression in Youth

Gail N. Kemp, Erin E. O’Connor, Tessa K. Kritikos,


Laura Curren, and Martha C. Tompson

Contents correlates, and etiology of youth depression.


Despite the many questions that remain, the field
Critical Issues in Treatment of Depressed
Youth...................................................................  160 has moved forward in the development of effica-
cious treatment strategies. In the treatment of
Efficacy of Treatments for Youth Depression......  166
adolescent depression, selective serotonin reup-
Directions for Future Research.............................  184 take inhibitors (SSRIs) have demonstrated lim-
Conclusions.............................................................  185 ited efficacy, but tricyclic antidepressants have
References...............................................................  185
not (Bridge et al., 2007; Cipriani et al., 2016).
Given the rates of adverse events in clinical trials
with youth, their limited efficacy (Cipriani et al.,
2016), and concerns about the potential for
There is a well-developed literature on depres- increased risk of self-harm associated with SSRIs
sion in adults, including treatment, risk factors, in youth (US Food and Drug Administration,
phenomenology, course, and biological sub- 2004), there is a strong need to continue to
strates (Kessler et al., 2003; Saleh et al., 2017). develop and test effective psychosocial treat-
Examination of both psychopharmacologic and ments as alternatives and supplements to medica-
psychosocial treatments is extensive (Cuijpers, tion in the comprehensive treatment of depressed
van Straten, van Oppen, & Andersson, 2008), youth.
and treatment guidelines have been developed to Although depression appears to be relatively
direct clinicians’ interventions (American Psychi­ rare prior to adolescence, representing only 3%
atric Association, 2006). However, examination of school-aged youth (Costello et al., 1996), the
of depression in youth has commenced much prevalence increases significantly postpuberty
more recently, and the research literature to guide (Costello et al., 1996; Lewinsohn, Hops, Roberts,
treatment is less well-developed. The last three Seeley, & Andrews, 1993; Avenevoli, Knight,
and a half decades have seen a surge in our Kessler, & Merikangas, 2008) to 4.6% among
understanding of the phenomenology, course,
­ adolescent boys and 5.9% among adolescent girls
(Costello, Erkanli, & Angold, 2006). The limited
literature to date suggests potentially important
differences in adolescent-onset and preadolescent-­
G.N. Kemp • E.E. O’Connor • T.K. Kritikos
onset depression. First, earlier onset depression
L. Curren • M.C. Tompson (*)
Boston University, Boston, MA, USA may be associated with a more pernicious course
e-mail: mtompson@bu.edu than later onset depression (Kovacs et al., 1984).

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 159


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_10
160 G.N. Kemp et al.

Second, early-onset depression frequently Developmental Context


disrupts the negotiation of important develop-
­
mental tasks with deleterious impact on psycho- It has been well established that rates of depres-
social competence (Puig-Antich et al., 1985a, sion rise significantly as children reach adoles-
1985b; Puig-Antich et al., 1993). Thus, compared cence (Hankin et al., 2015), with the greatest
to nondepressed school-aged youth, depressed spikes occurring in girls (Hankin et al., 2015;
school-aged youth are more likely to enter Hankin, Mermelstein, & Roesch, 2007). This
­adolescence with fewer skills to cope with an demonstrates a clear need to understand the impact
increasingly demanding environment. Third, of developmental processes on depression. Risk
while research generally supports the role of neg- factors that seem related to depression symptoms
ative attributional processes in adolescent depres- across development include greater impact of cog-
sion (Garber & Flynn, 2001), the data on the role nitive vulnerabilities, increased exposure to stress-
of cognitions in preadolescent depression is less ful life events (particularly interpersonal stress),
clear (Lakdawalla, Hankin, & Mermelstein, and the intersection with ongoing developmental
2007). Fourth, while the data suggest strong con- processes (Abela & Hankin, 2008).
tinuity between adolescent and adult depression Cognitive vulnerability theories of depression
(Bardone, Moffitt, Caspi, & Dickson, 1996; posit that an individual may be predisposed to
Fleming, Boyle, & Offord, 1993; Lewinsohn, perceiving, judging, and processing information
Rohde, Klein, & Seeley, 1999; Melvin et al., 2013; in a way that is negatively biased and overly self-­
Pine, Cohen, Gurley, Brook, & Ma, 1998; Weissman referential (Abela & Hankin, 2008). This nega-
et al., 1999), studies of preadolescent-­onset depres- tive pattern can trigger a cascade of depressogenic
sion suggest high rates of ongoing maladjustment thinking, which can lead to a depressive state. For
and psychiatric disorders, but less specificity for example, cognitive vulnerability theories empha-
later depression. In sum, there is evidence that ado- sizing the role of hopelessness focus on the idea
lescent-onset and preadolescent-­ onset depression that the individual sees negative events as having
differ in important ways. For these reasons, we have global and stable causes that lead to catastrophic
chosen to examine treatments for adolescents and outcomes and trigger negative self-views (Abela
preadolescent depression separately. & Hankin, 2008). Since children have an under-
In this chapter, we describe the primary psy- developed concept of the future and a lower
chosocial treatment approaches that have been capacity to generalize concepts across various
investigated for depression in youth. First, we situations, this type of negatively biased informa-
outline some of the critical issues to consider in tion processing is likely to require a more
treatment for youth depression. Second, we advanced stage of cognitive development. There
review the literature on treatment efficacy. Third, is some support for the idea that children display
we suggest directions for future research. less hopelessness than adolescents (Stark, Sander,
& Hauser, 2006; Weiss & Garber, 2003), and
there is a weaker relationship between cognitive
 ritical Issues in Treatment
C vulnerabilities and depression in the face of
of Depressed Youth stressors for children (Lakdawalla et al., 2007).
As youth reach later developmental stages, they
It is important to identify the factors that contribute are able to make extrapolations across time and
to and impact youth depression to clarify potential circumstances. As a result, the cognitive vulner-
etiological processes, which can inform treatment ability may then be applied more broadly, in
research and prevention efforts. In this section, we accord with these advances in development
will briefly discuss the relation between develop- (Abela & Hankin, 2008; Kaslow, Adamson, &
mental context, comorbidity, familial processes, Collins, 2000).
stress, cultural background, and neurobiological In addition to the more globally applied cogni-
factors on childhood depression. tive vulnerability, youth face more stressful life
Depression in Youth 161

events as they advance in age, particularly in the Proffitt, & LeGagnoux, 1997) finding that ­therapy
realm of interpersonal stress (Shih, Eberhart, was more effective for children (ages 4–12) than
Hammen, & Brennan, 2006). As youth enter ado- for adolescents (ages 13–18). However, a review
lescence, the peer context becomes increasingly of 150 different studies of the effects of psycho-
salient, and youth experience more peer-related therapy with children and adolescents found that
stress as they transition from being family ori- treatment outcomes were superior for adoles-
ented to peer oriented (Tompson, Boger, & cents (mean effect size of 0.65) compared to chil-
Asarnow, 2012). For girls, this rise in interper- dren (mean effect size of 0.48) (Weisz, Weiss,
sonal stress may be steeper than for boys, and Han, Granger, & Morton, 1995). Another meta-
girls may experience a stronger reactivity to this analysis found that the effect size for studies of
type of stress (Hankin et al., 2007), placing them youth under age 13 was not significantly different
at greater risk for depression. These risk pro- from the effect size for treatment of adolescents
cesses may in part account for the particularly (0.41 versus 0.33) when trials with mixed child
steep increases in rates of depression in postpu- and adolescent samples were excluded (Weisz,
bertal females. McCarty, & Valeri, 2006). However, the effect
The more stabilized and entrenched cognitive size for the younger children (vs. adolescents)
vulnerabilities, along with the increases in peer-­ was based on a very small number of trials (n = 7)
related stress, all occur within the context of all of which were selected based on depressive
other developmental factors, such as the greater symptoms vs. diagnoses, which likely led to less
egocentricity of adolescence (Elkind, 1978). This severe depression in the child samples.
heightened self-referential focus may serve as the Altogether, this research highlights a clear
platform for the assumption and expectation that need for a better understanding of the relation-
one’s negative self-views are also endorsed and ship between age and treatment outcomes in
actively held by others within the social setting youth depression. The extant literature points to
(Lakdawalla et al., 2007). The expectation of a distinct differences in the impact of risk factors
persistently negative imagined audience may fur- that vary across development (Harter, 2012).
ther exacerbate the vulnerability toward a depres- Effective treatment strategies should therefore be
sive episode. tailored to the specific socialization needs and
The rise in rates of depression with age has cognitive capacities of youth at different develop-
prompted a significant focus on understanding mental stages. More research is needed on youth
adolescent depression. While this emphasis is depression generally and childhood depression
understandable, it is important not to neglect particularly. Such research should be informed
childhood depression, which has received less by the developmental changes associated with
attention. The gap between research on adoles- preadolescence and adolescence and the ways in
cent depression and childhood depression is par- which they interact with various treatment
ticularly stark in relation to treatment studies. approaches. Additionally, while developmental
There have been few randomized control trials considerations are typically examined according
(RCTs) that exclusively targeted preadolescents to age, there is a strong need to investigate the
with diagnosed depression (Dietz, Weinberg, ways in which these risk factors impact individu-
Brent, & Mufson, 2015; Luby, Lenze, & Tillman, als with developmental delays who are experi-
2012; Tompson, Sugar, Langer, & Asarnow, encing, or at risk for developing, depressive
2017b). In addition, most treatment trials focused symptomology (D’Angelo & Augenstein, 2012).
on children with elevated depressive symptoms
as opposed to meeting criteria for a clinical diag-
nosis of a depressive disorder. More research is Comorbidity
needed to understand the role of age and develop-
ment on treatment outcomes, as the data on Youth depression is most commonly accompanied
potential age effects appears to be mixed with by anxiety and behavior disorders (Kovacs,
Weisz and colleagues (Weisz, Thurber, Sweeney, Obrosky, & George, 2016). Research has estimated
162 G.N. Kemp et al.

that more than half of the youth with diagnosable the genetic and biological risk factors (Hankin
depression almost meet criteria for another Axis I et al., 2015) that may account for these associa-
disorder (Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seely, 1998). The tions, psychosocial factors in families may also
odds ratios between depression and ADHD, con- contribute (Goodman & Gotlib, 2002). Two areas
duct disorder, and anxiety are 5.5, 6.6, and 8.2, of familial processes that have received attention
respectively (Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, 1999). in the literature include parenting behaviors and
Additionally, youth with developmental disabilities family conflict.
may be at enhanced risk for the development of Harsh, or aversive, parenting practices
depression. The limited research suggests that include behaviors such as overly negative criti-
youth with an intellectual disability have greater cism, lack of warmth, and parent-child conflict.
odds of having depression (OR = 1.7; Emerson, There is consistent evidence that suggests that
2003), and students with learning disabilities score parental depression interrupts a caregiver’s
somewhat higher on measures of depressive symp- ability to care successfully for the child. Parents
toms (Maag & Reid, 2006). Children with autism who are depressed exhibit overly harsh parent-
spectrum disorders also have an increased risk for ing behaviors (Lovejoy, Graczyk, O’Hare, &
depression symptoms (Ghaziuddin, Ghaziuddin, & Neuman, 2000; Wilson & Durbin, 2010) and
Greden, 2002; Matson & Nebel-Schwalm, 2007; display greater disengagement and negativity
Saulnier & Volkmar, 2007; Strang et al., 2012). than nondepressed mothers (Lovejoy et al.,
Thus, depression is not only difficult for children 2000). Apter-­ Levi et al. (2016) found that
and adolescents because of its symptoms in isola- depressed mothers displayed more negative
tion, but also because of the vast risk it poses to the parenting and that maternal depression was
child with its high comorbidity (Archie, Kazemi, & associated with reduced cortisol variability in
Akhtar-Danesh, 2010). their children, which predicted higher child
The greatest risk and most dangerous associ- psychopathology and social withdrawal. This
ated behavior that depression presents to children study provides support for a pathway of vulner-
and adolescents is suicide. In the USA, suicide is ability in which maternal depression may nega-
the third leading cause of death among youths tively impact children’s HPA system functioning
ages 10–14 and second among those ages 15–24 and thus, their social-­ emotional adjustment
(CDC, 2015). Furthermore, in 80–90% of cases (Apter-Levi et al., 2016). In addition, offspring
of adolescent suicide, a psychiatric disorder was are at increased risk for developing an insecure
present (Bridge, Goldstein, & Brent, 2006), with attachment (Milan, Snow, & Belay, 2009). All
mood disorders being primarily present, espe- of these factors are associated with increased
cially in girls (Shaffer et al., 1996). risk for depression in offspring (Yap & Jorm,
2015).
In addition to parenting factors, the overall
Familial Processes family environment may confer additional risk
for youth depression. Families of children with
Familial processes are also key factors associated depression have higher levels of conflict, includ-
with youths’ risk for and vulnerability to depres- ing marital discord, both of which are linked to
sion. Parental psychopathology has often been increases in depressive symptoms (Kane &
associated with youth depression (Beardslee, Garber, 2004; Mark Cummings, Keller, &
Gladstone, & O’Connor, 2011; Tompson, Asarnow, Davies, 2005; Rabinowitz, Drabick, & Reynolds,
Mintz, & Cantwell, 2015). Findings from multi- 2016; Rice, Harold, Shelton, & Thapar, 2006;
ple studies show that offspring of depressed par- Sheeber, Hops, & Davis, 2001). Not only does
ents have higher rates of depression diagnosis, family conflict impact the depressed child, but
recurrence, and chronicity than those of nonde- there is a bidirectional effect, with the depressed
pressed parents (Goodman et al., 2011; Murray child displaying more negative affect that impacts
et al., 2011; Weissman et al., 2016). In addition to family conflict (Kelly et al., 2016).
Depression in Youth 163

This research underscores the need to life stressors (e.g., those over which the individual
u­ nderstand the complex role of the family in the has little to no influence) are correlated with
etiology and maintenance of youth depression. depression, with some evidence supporting a link
Such understanding should inform treatment (Aber, Brown, & Jones, 2003; Hammen et al.,
planning in terms of determining the target of 2012), and other researchers finding no signifi-
treatment (child, parents, parents and children, or cantly predictive association between these types
families) and the most appropriate treatment of events and depression (Rudolph et al., 2000).
strategies (medication, family-focused treatment, More consistent research has pointed to a link
or individual treatment). between youth depression and dependent life
stress (events that a youth with depressive symp-
toms may influence) (Hankin et al., 2007) as well
Stress as interpersonal stress. Compared to nonde-
pressed youth, those with depressive symptoms
Research has indicated that stress is one of the experience more peer-related stress and family
predominant pathways to the development and conflict, which predict heightened depressive
manifestation of youth depression (Stark et al., symptoms, particularly when cognitive or genetic
2005; Stark et al., 2006; Young & Dietrich, 2015). vulnerabilities are present (Hamilton, Stange,
Knowledge of the role of stress is therefore cru- Abramson, & Alloy, 2015; Hammen, 2006; Liu
cial in designing and implementing effective & Alloy, 2010; Rice et al., 2006; Rose, Glick,
treatments. Stressful life events may play a role Smith, Schwartz-Mette, & Borowski, 2016;
both in the etiology and maintenance of youth Sheeber, Davis, Leve, Hops, & Tildesley, 2007).
depression. In terms of etiology, the diathesis-­ In addition to experiencing heightened stress,
stress model posits that there are underlying depressed children and adolescents are also more
vulnerabilities (the diathesis) predisposing the likely to use avoidant coping strategies to manage
­
individual to depression such as genetic factors stress (Rodríguez-Naranjo & Caño, 2016).
(Hankin et al., 2015) and cognitive vulnerabili- Conversely, children’s use of more adaptive cop-
ties (Abela & Hankin, 2008). According to this ing strategies for managing stressors is associ-
model, these predispositions, when coupled with ated with fewer depressive symptoms (Burwell &
stressors, increase the likelihood of symptom Shirk, 2007; Rodríguez-Naranjo & Caño, 2016).
onset (Lakdawalla et al., 2007). There is evidence Although there is more research needed,
to suggest that youth at risk for depression may stressors that are interpersonal and dependent
experience more stressful life events (Hammen, seem to play a definitive, but complex, role in the
Hazel, Brennan, & Najman, 2012) and may be onset and course of youth depression, and inde-
more sensitive to the effects of this stress pendent stressful life events may also have an
(Hammen, 2002). impact. Treatment planning and research should
In terms of maintenance of depression, stress involve considerations of these contextual factors
generation models of depression speak to the and the ways in which the attitudes, cognitive
bidirectional relationship between depressive style, and actions of depressed youth in stressful
symptoms and life stress. In these models, symp- situations may influence symptomatology.
toms of depression increase interpersonal stress,
which in turn contributes to ongoing depression
symptoms. For example, a depressed youth exp­ Cultural Considerations
ec­ ting rejection may withdraw from social
­interactions, increasing the chances that social Cultural background is another key factor that
isolation will result and contributing to greater relates to the manifestation of youth depression
depressive symptoms over time. Indeed, when the and its treatment. Existing research on ethnicity
nature of a particular life stressor has been eva­ and depression indicates that various racial
luated, there are mixed findings that independent groups experience differing levels of depression
164 G.N. Kemp et al.

severity, varied symptom expression, and more likely to drop out of treatment early.
­different likelihoods of receiving treatment (Stark Researchers have found a similar trend for
et al., 2006). depression treatment seeking behavior more spe-
In terms of depressive symptoms’ rates, cifically (Lindsey, Chambers, Pohle, Beall, &
research has demonstrated that Chinese Lucksted, 2013). Some have suggested that
American, Filipino American, Japanese higher rates of perceived mental health stigma
American, and Native Hawaiian youth experi- may account for this discrepancy. In a study of
ence higher rates of depressive symptoms than stigma among African American youth and
white youth (Okamura et al., 2016). However, the depression severity, researchers found that
highest rates of depressive symptoms, when greater depression symptom severity was associ-
compared to other racial/ethnic groups, occur ated with elevated perceptions of stigma (Rose,
among Hispanic youth (Céspedes & Huey, 2008) Joe, & Lindsey, 2011). An additional noteworthy
and particularly Hispanic girls (McLaughlin, finding in this study was that youth with higher
Hilt, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2007). There is some rates of depression accurately identified their
evidence to suggest that cultural clashes between need for treatment (Rose et al., 2011). This sug-
the high emphasis on individuation in western gests that efforts to increase treatment seeking for
cultures and the emphasis on family (“familismo”) depressed adolescents may not require more psy-
predominant in Hispanic/Latino cultures may choeducation on the need for treatment, but a
partially account for the higher depressive symp- greater emphasis on decreasing stigma associated
tom rates (McLaughlin et al., 2007). with treatment seeking.
Not only do rates of symptoms vary for differ- Taken together, these findings indicate that
ent ethnic minority groups, but research suggests response to treatment might differ according to
variation in the nature of symptoms endorsed as the client’s ethnic background. Current clinical
well. For example, Iwata, Turner, and Lloyd trials may not generalize to underrepresented
(2002) found that African American, US-born minorities (Huey & Polo, 2008; Kataoka, Zhang,
Hispanic, non-US-born Hispanic, and non-­ & Wells, 2002; Miranda et al., 2005). Additionally
Hispanic white adolescents and young adults concerning is the paucity of data to draw ­definitive
provided different responses to symptoms as conclusions on the cross-cultural generalizability
reflected on the Center for Epidemiologic Studies of treatments (Kataoka, Novins, & DeCarlo
Depression Scale (CES-D). More specifically, Santiago, 2010). Clearly, depressed youths (and
they found that the African American respon- likely their treatment providers) hold beliefs and
dents scored low on depressed affect symptoms values about psychopathology and treatment that
and high on somatic symptoms, while the are influenced by their various cultural back-
US-born Hispanic respondents scored low on grounds (Weisz, Jensen Doss, & Hawley, 2005).
the interpersonal symptoms, but lower positive Therefore, a sensitive understanding of these dif-
affect. ferences is essential to accurate assessment and
In addition to recognizing differences in effective planning of treatment and treatment
symptom expression, it is also important to pay research.
attention to the intersection between culture and
help-seeking behaviors. Researchers have found
variations in the level of mental health service Neurobiological Factors
utilization across racial groups. For instance,
Cuffe and colleagues (Cuffe, Waller, Cuccaro, Given that not all youth respond to current
Pumariega, & Garrison, 1995) found that while depression treatments, whether they are psycho-
African American adolescents had higher scores social or psychopharmacological, there has been
on a measure of depression, they were less likely a recent push to understand underlying neurobio-
than European American adolescents to receive logical processes in youth depression that may
outpatient treatment for any disorder and were lead to more targeted and effective interventions.
Depression in Youth 165

Processes that have received recent attention A number of recent efforts have attempted to
include reward processing, heart rate variability, identify how best to target low positive affect,
and sleep. We will briefly discuss several of these reward processing, and emotion dysregulation in
neurobiological mechanisms as they relate to the depression, both through intervention programs
treatment of child and adolescent depression, but and the identification of individual differences in
will not include an exhaustive review. these systems. Hankin and colleagues (Hankin
et al., 2011) found that youth carrying short
Reward Processing  Based on a number of recent alleles of 5-HTTLPR displayed lower positive
studies in youth with depressive disorders, reward emotion in the context of unsupportive parenting
processing may be a promising endophenotype for and higher positive emotion in the context of
risk for depression and could have important treat- ­supportive parenting (Hankin et al., 2011). As
ment implications. A number of depressive symp- such, future interventions could target the family
toms may be related to reward processes, resulting environment in order to upregulate youth positive
in the low positive affect and difficulties with emo- affect and enhance salience of anticipated and
tion regulation, often seen in depression (Forbes, received rewards in the context of behavioral
2009; Forbes & Dahl, 2005; Hankin, Wetter, & activation and emotion regulation processes
Flory, 2012; Sheeber et al., 2009; Silk, Davis, (Hankin et al., 2012). Kovacs et al. (2006) pilot
McMakin, Dahl, & Forbes, 2012). When the tested one such contextual emotion regulation
reward system is suppressed, depression and low therapy for child depression, which focused on
positive affect are thought to result (Davey, Yücel, the self-regulation of distress and dysphoria in
& Allen, 2008; Hankin et al., 2012). Indeed, it has children ages 7–12. Although a small pilot trial,
been theorized that certain depressive symptoms at a 12-month follow-up, 92% of the children had
such as anhedonia, social withdrawal, and psycho- achieved remission of dysthymia (Kovacs et al.,
motor retardation may in part result from reduced 2006). Although promising, more research is
reward seeking behavior, decreased motivation to needed in order to understand how and when in
obtain reward, and disruption in the experience development to target low positive affect and
of rewarding outcomes (Forbes & Dahl, 2005; reward processing in existing depression inter-
Hankin et al., 2012).  ventions, which could be key to both intervention
Recent research provides support for the role and prevention efforts (Silk et al., 2012).
of reward processing and emotion regulation in
depression. Specifically, interventions for depres- Heart Rate Variability  Heart rate variability
sion have been linked to activity in several brain (HRV) is an index of parasympathetic nervous
regions associated with such processes (Straub system function that is considered a psycho­
et al., 2015; Tao et al., 2012). In a study of CBT physiological marker of emotion regulation
group treatment for adolescent depression, Straub (Beauchaine & Thayer, 2015; Koenig, Kemp,
et al. (2015) found that treatment response was Beauchaine, Thayer, & Kaess, 2016). Specifically,
significantly associated with changes in brain low resting high-frequency HRV (HF-HRV) has
activation in areas implicated in reward process- been associated with a range of psychopathology
ing and emotion regulation, including the left in children and adolescents (Beauchaine, Gatzke-­
hippocampus, the left amygdala, and the bilateral Kopp, & Mead, 2007; Crowell et al., 2005) and
subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC). has more generally been linked to emotion regu-
Activation of these areas is frequently cited as lation deficits (Beauchaine & Thayer, 2015).
relevant to depression processes (Arnone et al., With regard to depression, compared to healthy
2012; Gotlib et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2010). controls, depressed children and adolescents
These results suggest that psychotherapy may be have lower resting state HF-HRV (Koenig et al.,
able to affect processes involved in emotion 2016). However, unlike in adult samples, a recent
regulation.  meta-analysis found no association between
166 G.N. Kemp et al.

HF-HRV and depression severity in depressed depression are complex, but it is believed that in
youth, although these associations were limited part it is due to the role that sleep plays in emo-
to nonclinical samples (Koenig et al., 2016). tion regulation as well as involvement of the HPA
Although research on HRV in youth depression is axis (Clarke & Harvey, 2012). 
in its infancy, current findings suggest HRV could There is promising evidence that sleep distur-
be used as a marker of treatment response, with bances may be effectively treated and result in
Koenig et al. (2016) suggesting that such bio- depression symptom improvement in adults
markers may be useful assessment tools, especially (Manber et al., 2008). Unfortunately, very few
for children who have difficulty reporting symp- studies have examined sleep interventions with
tomatic distress. Additionally, treatment options depressed youth. Preliminary treatment studies
designed to increase or alter vagal activity may for adolescents incorporate CBT techniques to
be particularly effective, such as physical activ- improve the response rate to traditional psycho-
ity, diet changes, and potentially more invasive therapy for youth depression (Clarke & Harvey,
vagus nerve stimulation (Koenig et al., 2016). 2012). Given the promising results in the adult
literature and the pervasive sleep disturbances in
Sleep  The relationship between sleep difficul- depression and youth, particularly in adoles-
ties and depression in youth is a complicated one. cence, the treatment of comorbid sleep distur-
During adolescence major changes in both sleep bance and depression may be a fruitful avenue for
patterns and rates of depression occur (Costello, increasing the effectiveness of current evidence-­
Copeland, & Angold, 2011; Lovato & Gradisar, based treatments.
2014). Among adolescents in a clinical sample,
increased depressive symptoms are associated
with sleep problems, although this association Efficacy of Treatments for Youth
has not been evident among children under age Depression
12 (Alfano, Zakem, Costa, Taylor, & Weems,
2009). The association is further complicated by Adolescent Depression
the fact that the majority of depressed adoles-
cents report sleep complaints (Liu et al., 2007), As reviewed briefly above, research on psychoso-
and its role as an etiological factor or correlate of cial correlates of depression in youth emphasizes
depression remains unclear. Some evidence sug- its association with negative cognitions (review,
gests that sleep disturbance in depressed adoles- Garber & Flynn, 2001), disturbed interpersonal
cents may increase the risk for a subsequent relationships (review, Kaslow, Jones, Palin,
depressive episode (Roane & Taylor, 2008; Pinsof, & Lebow, 2005), and stress (Rudolph
Roberts & Duong, 2013), contribute to the main- et al., 2000). Accordingly, evidence-based treat-
tenance of depression, interfere with treatment, ments for adolescents can be broadly delineated
and persist after remission of a depressive ­episode into either cognitive behavioral or interpersonal
(Clarke & Harvey, 2012; Emslie et al., 2001). For approaches. Studies vary in their inclusion of
instance, Emslie et al. (2012) found that adoles- subjects with diagnosed depressive disorders ver-
cents who reported more substantial insomnia sus subjects with high levels of depressive symp-
were less likely to respond to fluoxetine com- toms. It is not clear the degree to which findings
pared to adolescents who reported less insomnia. from studies of youth with high depressive symp-
However, this was not the case with children, toms generalize to youth with a diagnosable
such that children who reported more substantial depressive disorder. Table 1 includes studies con-
insomnia were more likely to respond to fluox- ducted with youth with diagnosed depressive dis-
etine compared to those who did not report higher orders. The 21 studies include 12 with a cognitive
levels of insomnia (Emslie et al., 2012). The behavioral intervention condition, five with an
mechanisms by which sleep disturbance plays a interpersonal therapy condition, two with social
role in the development and maintenance of skills conditions, and three with family therapy
Table 1  Randomized clinical interventions trials for adolescents with diagnosed depression
Treatment Intervention Posttreatment
Reference Subjects Diagnostic/risk assessment format(s) type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Asarnow Ages 13–21 Either: (1) Endorsed “stem items” for Individual (1) 6-month Immediate Intervention patients, compared with usual
et al. (2005) (n = 418) MDD or DD from the CIDI-12, 1 week quality care patients, reported significantly higher
or more of past-month depressive improvement mental health care utilization, fewer
Depression in Youth

symptoms, and a total CES-D intervention depressive symptoms, higher mental


score>16, or (2) CES-D score>24 (2) Usual care health-related quality of life, and greater
satisfaction with mental health care
Brent et al. Ages 13–18 Diagnosis of MDD based on K-SADS Family (1) Systematic Immediate The CBT group had faster response, fewer
(1997) (n = 107) interview and BDI ≥13 behavior family cases of diagnosable MDD at the end of the
therapy treatment, and a lower number of depressive
Individual (2) CBT symptoms and was more likely to be
(3) Supportive remitted than other groups. No difference
therapy between family and supportive therapies
Clarke, Ages 14–18 Diagnosis of MDD or DD based on the Group (1) Adolescent Immediate; CBT was associated with higher depression
Rohde, (n = 123) K-SADS interview coping with 12 months; recovery rates (66.7% vs. 48.1% in wait list
Lewinsohn, depression course 24 months condition) and greater reduction in
Hops, and (CWD-A) depressive symptoms. Addition of parent
Seeley (1999) (2) CWD-A with group had no significant effect. Booster
nine-session parent sessions accelerated recovery among youth
group still depressed at the end of acute treatment
(3) Wait list but did not reduce recurrence
control
Clarke et al. Ages 13–18 Diagnosis of DSM-III-R MDD and/or Group (1) Usual care plus Immediate; No significant differences between CBT and
(2002) (n = 88) DD based on the K-SADS interview group CBT 12 months; usual care, either for depression diagnoses,
program 24 months continuous depression measures,
(CWD-A) nonaffective mental health measures, or
(2) Usual care functioning outcomes
Clarke et al. Ages 12–18 Diagnoses of DSM-IV MDD based on Individual (1) Brief CBT plus Immediate; CBT program showed advantages on the
(2005) (n = 152) the K-SADS-PL (Present and Lifetime treatment as usual 26 weeks; Short-Form-12 Mental Component Scale
Version) interview (primarily SSRI) 52 weeks and reductions in treatment as usual
(2) Treatment as outpatient visits and days’ supply of all
usual medications. No effects were detected for
MDD episodes; a nonsignificant trend
favoring CBT was detected on the CES-D
(continued)
167
168

Table 1 (continued)
Treatment Intervention Posttreatment
Reference Subjects Diagnostic/risk assessment format(s) type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Diamond, Ages 13–17 Diagnoses of DSM-III-R MDD based Family (1) Attachment-­ Immediate; At posttreatment, 81% treated no longer met
Reis, (n = 32) on the K-SADS based family 6 months criteria for MDD vs. 47% of patients in the
Diamond, therapy (ABFT) waitlist group. The ABFT patients showed
Siqueland, (2) Minimal-­ greater reduction in depressive and anxiety
and Isaacs contact, waitlist symptoms and family conflict. At follow-up,
(2002) control group 87% of the ABFT patients continued to not
meet criteria for MDD
Emslie et al. Ages 7–18 Diagnosis of DSM-IV MDD with Individual Acute treatment: 12 weeks Following acute treatment 51% adolescents
(2008) (n = 168) CDRS-R ≥40 and CGI ≥4  (1) Fluoxetine (acute) and 71% of children (ages 7–11) entered
 (2) Placebo 6 months continuation. In continuation treatment
(responders fluoxetine was superior to placebo in
6-month
only) preventing relapse (42% versus 69%) and in
continuation:
increasing time to relapse in children and
 (1) Fluoxetine adolescents
 (2) Placebo
Fine, Forth, Ages 13–17 Diagnosis of MDD or DD based on Group (1) Therapeutic Immediate; At posttest both groups improved; TSG
Gilbert, and (n = 66) K-SADS interview support group 9 months significantly more effective than SSG in
Haley (1991) 83% female (TSG) vs. reducing depression on K-SADS with more
(2) Social skills subjects in nonclinical range. Group
group (SSG) differences disappeared at follow-up
Goodyer Ages 11–17 Diagnosis of MDD on Individual (1) SSRI and Immediate; There was no difference in effectiveness
et al. (2007) (n = 208) K-SADS-PL (Present and Lifetime routine care 28 weeks between groups. No evidence that CBT in
Version) interview (2) SSRI, routine addition to SSRI and routine care is more
care, and CBT effective than SSRI and routine care alone
Lewinsohn, Ages 14–18 Diagnosis of major, minor, or Group (1) Adolescent-­ Immediate; Significantly fewer youths in the treatment
Clarke, Hops, (n = 59) intermittent depression based on only CBT training 1 month; groups met criteria for depressive disorders
and Andrews K-SADS interview with mother and group 6 months; after treatment and at follow-up.
(1990) adolescent Family (2) Adolescent-­ 12 months; Significantly improved on self-reported
parent CBT 24 months depression, anxiety, number of pleasant
training groups activities, and depressogenic thoughts.
(3) Wait list Trend for adolescent-parent condition to
control outperform adolescent-only group
G.N. Kemp et al.
Treatment Intervention Posttreatment
Reference Subjects Diagnostic/risk assessment format(s) type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Melvin et al. Ages 12–18 Diagnosis of DSM-IV MDD, DD, or Individual (1) CBT Immediate; All groups showed significant improvement
(2006) (n = 73) DDNOS based on the K-SADS (2) Antidepressant 6 months on outcome measures and this was
medication maintained at follow-up. Combined group
(Sertraline) was not superior to monotherapy. CBT alone
(3) Combined was superior to medication alone
CBT and
Depression in Youth

medication
Mufson, Ages 12–18 Clinician diagnosis of MDD based on Individual (1) Interpersonal Immediate IPT-A patients reported greater decrease in
Weissman, (n = 48) the HRSD psychotherapy for depressive symptoms, improved social
Moreau, and depressed functioning, and improved problem-solving
Garfinkel adolescents skills compared to controls. In the IPT-A
(1999) (IPT-A) condition, 74% recovered compared to 46%
(2) Clinician in the control condition
monitoring
Mufson et al. Ages 12–18 DSM-IV diagnosis of MDD, DD, Individual (1) IPT-A Immediate IPT-A associated with fewer clinician-­
(2004) (n = 63) adjustment disorder with depressed reported depression symptoms on the
mood, or DDNOS and HRSD>10 and HAMD, better functioning on the C-GAS,
a C-GAS score<65 (2) Treatment as better overall social functioning on the
usual Social Adjustment Scale-Self-Report,
greater clinical improvement, and greater
decreases in clinical severity on the Clinical
Global Impressions scale
O’Shea, Ages 13–19 DSM-IV diagnoses of MDD based on Individual (1) IPT-I Immediate; Improved depression, anxiety, and overall
Spence, and (n = 33) the K-SADS-E (epidemiological Group (2) IPT-G 12 months functioning. No difference between IPT-I
Donovan version) and IPT-G. Treatment gains maintained at
(2015) 12-month follow-up
Reed (1994) Ages 14–19 Clinician diagnosis of MDD or DD Group (1) Social skills Immediate; Skills group participants scored significantly
(n = 18) training 6–8 weeks higher on clinicians’ rating of improvement.
(2) Attention Male subjects improved, but female subjects
placebo control deteriorated
Rohde, Ages 13–17 DSM-IV diagnoses of MDD and Group (1) CWD-A Immediate; Posttreatment MDD recovery rates better in
Clarke, (n = 91) conduct disorder based on the (2) Life skills 6 months; CWD-A group (36%), compared to life skills/
Mace, K-SADS-E-5 (epidemiological tutoring/control 12 months tutoring (19%). CWD-A participants reported
Jorgensen, version) reductions in BDI-II and HDRS scores and
and Seeley improved social functioning posttreatment.
(2004) Group differences in MDD recovery rates at
follow-up were nonsignificant
(continued)
169
Table 1 (continued)
170

Treatment Intervention Posttreatment


Reference Subjects Diagnostic/risk assessment format(s) type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Rosselló and Ages 13–18 Diagnosis of MDD, DD, or both Individual (1) CBT Immediate; Both active treatments were associated with
Bernal (1999) (n = 71) (2) IPT 3 months significant reductions in depression when
(3) Wait list compared to wait list. IPT was superior to
control CBT in enhancing social functioning and
self-esteem
Rosselló, Ages 12–18 Diagnosis of DSM-III-R MDD using Individual (1) CBT-I Immediate Individual format and group produced
Bernal, and (n = 112) the DISC or CDI >13 Group (2) CBT-B similar reductions in depressive symptoms.
Rivera (2008) (3) IPT-I CBT overall resulted in greater
improvements in depressive symptoms and
(4) IPT-G
self-concept
TADS Team Ages 12–17 DSM-IV diagnosis of MDD based on Individual Twelve weeks of: Immediate There were significant differences between
(2004) (n = 439) the K-SADS-PL (Present and Lifetime  (1) Fluoxetine combination treatment and placebo on the
Version) alone CDRS-R. Combined treatment was superior
 (2) CBT alone when compared with fluoxetine alone and
CBT alone. Fluoxetine alone was superior to
 (3) CBT with
CBT alone
fluoxetine
 (4) Placebo
Vostanis, Ages 8–17 Diagnosis of MDD, DD, or minor Individual (1) Depression Immediate; No difference in remission rates; remission
Feehan, (n = 56) depression based on K-SADS treatment program 9 months rates were high in both groups
Grattan, and (2) Attention
Bickerton placebo
(1996)
Wood, Ages 9–17 Diagnosis of MDD or RDC minor Individual (1) CBT Immediate; Posttest revealed greater reductions in
Harrington, (n = 48) depression based on K-SADS 6 months depressive symptoms and an advantage in
and Moore interview with both parent and child (2) Relaxation overall outcome in the CBT group. At
(1996) training follow-up, group differences were
attenuated
MDD major depressive disorder, DD dysthymic disorder, DDNOS depressive disorder not otherwise specified, CIDI Composite International Diagnostic Interview, K-SADS
Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for school-aged children, BDI Beck Depression Inventory, CDI Children’s Depression Inventory, CGAS Children's Global
Assessment Scale, GAF Global Assessment of Functioning Scale, CES-D Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale, CDRS-R Revised Children’s Depression Rating
Scale, RADS Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale, BID Bellevue Index of Depression, DISC Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, HRSD Hamilton Rating Scale for
Depression
G.N. Kemp et al.
Depression in Youth 171

conditions. Five studies include comparison with & Labus, 2014) underscore the importance of
medication conditions. Table 2 includes interven- understanding what participants are receiving in
tions conducted with youth experiencing high “usual care” conditions. Five studies have com-
levels of depressive symptoms and reviews ten pared CBT to other psychosocial treatments, and
interventions, eight of which are cognitive behav- it has been shown to be superior to systemic
ioral in their approach and two of which investi- ­family therapy, supportive therapy (Brent et al.,
gate interpersonal therapy. 1997), relaxation training (Wood et al., 1996),
and life skills training (Rohde et al., 2004).
Cognitive Behavioral Approaches  As illus- However, in one study comparing it to inter­
trated in Tables 1 and 2, cognitive behavioral personal psychotherapy (IPT), IPT had a larger
interventions have been more thoroughly investi- effect size and greater enhancements in social
gated than any other intervention approach for functioning and self-esteem (Rosselló & Bernal,
adolescent depression. The specific cognitive 1999). In the four studies that included medica-
interventions used have varied across treatment tion arms, one was not designed to compare the
studies. These studies have compared cognitive two interventions (Asarnow et al., 2005), one
behavioral treatment with different conditions, found that the addition of CBT to treatment with
examined its delivery in different formats (group medication had no benefit over treatment with
versus individual), looked at longer-term follow-­ medication alone (Goodyer et al., 2007), one
ups, and examined the role of parallel parent found medication alone to be superior to CBT
groups in enhancing treatment efficacy.  (TADS team, 2004), and one found CBT to be
Of the 16 studies of diagnosed depressed superior to medication intervention (Melvin
youth that included a cognitive behavioral et al., 2006). In the study by Asarnow et al.
­treatment condition, 11 support the superiority of (2005), 418 adolescents in primary care settings
CBT in comparison to control conditions. The (ages 13–21) were randomly assigned to a
efficacy of cognitive behavioral interventions has 6-month “quality improvement” intervention or
been demonstrated when compared to waitlist or usual care. Those in the quality improvement
no intervention conditions in four studies. CBT intervention had access to a care manager, who
showed superiority in four studies comparing it educated them about depression and treatment
to waitlist control (Clarke et al., 1999; Lewinsohn options, and participants could select medication
et al., 1990; Rosselló & Bernal, 1999; Smith or CBT treatments. Although the study was not
et al., 2015) and showed equal effects in one designed to evaluate the relative efficacy of CBT
study when compared with a monitor and control and medication, the quality improvement inter-
condition (Poppelaars et al., 2016). Yet CBT vention overall was associated with significantly
showed superiority in only one of the four studies lower depressive symptoms, and adolescents
comparing it to usual care (Asarnow et al., 2005). were somewhat more likely to prefer CBT. In the
In one of the studies in which CBT did not show study by Goodyer et al. (2007), 208 adolescents
an advantage (Clarke et al., 2005), the usual care (ages 11–17) with major or probable major
consisted primarily of medication (SSRI) inter- depression were randomly assigned to one of two
vention. In a second study in which CBT did not groups: treatment with an SSRI, CBT, and clini-
show an advantage (DePrince & Shirk, 2013), cal care or treatment with SSRI and clinical care
CBT for adolescent depression was modified to without CBT. The ongoing clinical care provided
address history of interpersonal trauma as well. to both groups consisted of psychoeducation and
The usual care (UC) therapists were predomi- attention to family or peer group conflicts. The
nantly client-centered, psychodynamic, and fam- authors found that receiving CBT in conjunction
ily systems oriented and employed treatment with an SSRI and ongoing clinical care had no
strategies common to their clinical practice. benefit over treatment with an SSRI and ongoing
Studies with an undefined UC component (Clarke clinical care alone. In the study conducted by
et al., 2002, 2005; Shirk, DePrince, Crisostomo, Melvin et al. (2006), 73 adolescents (ages 12–18)
Table 2  Randomized clinical interventions trials for adolescents with depression symptoms or risk factors for depression
172

Diagnostic/risk Treatment Post-­intervention


Reference Subjects assessment format(s) Intervention type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Ackerson, Ages CDI >10 and HRSD Self-­ (1) Cognitive bibliotherapy Immediate; Treatment produced statistically and
Scogin, 14–18 >10 administered (reading “Feeling Good”) and 1 month clinically significant improvements in
McKendree-­ (n = 22) weekly monitoring phone depressive symptoms that were
Smith, and calls maintained at follow-up. And a
Lyman (1998) (2) Delayed-­treatment control significant decrease in dysfunctional, but
not in negative automatic, thoughts
Clarke et al. 9th and CES-D >23 but does Group (1) CWD-A Immediate; Significantly fewer CWD-A adolescents
(1995) 10th not meet criteria for (2) No intervention 6 months; diagnosed MDD or DD. Higher GAF and
graders MDD or DD 12 months lower CES-D for CWD-A group at
(n = 150) (K-SADS) posttest but no differences at follow-up
Clarke et al. Ages Symptomatic Group (1) Usual HMO care plus Immediate; Group intervention decreased depression
(2001) 13–19 adolescent offspring group cognitive therapy 12 months; symptoms and episode rates to the
(n = 94) (CES-D >24) of (2) Usual HMO care 24 months community-normal range and decreases
recently depressed in the incidence of MDD at follow-up
parents, assessed
using the F-SADS
Kerfoot, Mean age Contact with social Individual (1) Brief CBT 17 weeks after No significant differences between
Harrington, 13.9 (1.9) services within the (2) Routine care initial groups in depression or global
Harrington, (n = 52) previous 2 years; assessment; adjustment. At posttreatment, 77% of the
Rogers, and mood and feelings 33 weeks after CBT group and 80% of the routine care
Verduyn depression initial group had residual depressive symptoms
(2004) questionnaire>23 assessment or disorder
Marcotte and Ages CDI >15 on two Group (1) Rational emotive Immediate; No difference between the two
Baron (1993) 14–17 administrations and (2) No treatment 8 weeks treatments: Depressive symptoms
(n = 25) elevated score on reduced at posttreatment in both groups
semi-structured
interview focusing on
depressive symptoms
Poppelaars Girls, mean RADS-2 >59 Computerized (1) School-­based CBT 1 week; No difference between four groups;
et al. (2016) age 13.35 prevention program 3 months; depressive symptoms decreased in all
(0.64) (2) Computerized CBT 6 months; conditions
(n = 208) (3) Combination of school- 12 months
based and computerized CBT
(4) Monitoring control
G.N. Kemp et al.

condition
Reynolds and 9th–12th (1) BDI score >11; Group (1) CBT Immediate; Both active treatments showed significant
Coats (1986) graders (2) RADS >71; (2) Relaxation training 5 weeks decreases in depressive symptoms and
(n = 30) (3) BID >20; (3) Wait list control improved academic self-concept
compared to wait list. Relaxation
(4) No other current
associated with reductions in anxiety as
treatment
well
Depression in Youth

Smith et al. Ages MFQ-C >20 Computerized (1) School-­based Immediate; Relative to being on a waiting list,
(2015) 12–15 computerized CBT 3 months; C-CBT was associated with statistically
(n = 112) (2) Waiting list 6 months significant improvements in symptoms of
depression and anxiety according to
adolescent self- report
Young, 7th–10th (1) CES-D scores Group (1) IPT-AST (adolescent Immediate; IPT-AST group had significantly less
Mufson, and graders between 16 and 39 skills training) 3 months; depressive symptoms and overall
Davies (2006) (n = 41) (2) At least 2 (2) SC (school counseling) 6 months improved functioning at post and
subthreshold or follow-up periods. There were fewer
threshold symptoms depression diagnoses at any point in the
on the K-SADS study period for the IPT-AST group
(3) No present Major (3.7%) as compared to the SC group
Depressive Episode (28.6%)
(MDE)
Young, Ages (1) CES-D scores Group (1) IPT-AST (adolescent Immediate; Decreased depression symptoms and
Mufson, and 13–17 between 16 and 39 skills training) 6 months; improved overall functioning in IPT-AST
Gallop (2010) (n = 57) (2) At least 2 (2) SC (school counseling) 12 months group at post and 6 months. At
subthreshold or 12 months, no significant differences
threshold symptoms between the groups on either depression
on the K-SADS symptoms or overall functioning
(3) No present Major
Depressive Episode 
(MDE)
MDD major depressive disorder, DD dysthymic disorder, K-SADS Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for school-aged children, BDI Beck Depression Inventory,
CBT cognitive behavioral therapy, CDI Children’s Depression Inventory, GAF Global Assessment of Functioning Scale, CES-D Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression
Scale, CDRS-R Revised Children’s Depression Rating Scale, RADS Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale, BID Bellevue Index of Depression, HRSD Hamilton Rating Scale
for Depression, MFC-Q Mood and Feelings Questionnaire-­Child, IPT-AST Interpersonal Psychotherapy-Adolescent Skills Training, SC school counseling
173
174 G.N. Kemp et al.

were randomly assigned to the CBT alone, depressive disorders. In four of the studies,
medication alone (sertraline), or a combined
­ ­cognitive behavioral treatment was superior to a
CBT and medication intervention. CBT alone no-­treatment comparison group. Clarke et al.
was superior to medication alone and the com- (1995, 2001) examined the 15-session Coping
bined treatment was not superior to either treat- with Depression Course (CWD) in two studies.
ment alone. The authors have noted that the poor In the first study ninth and tenth graders, who had
showing for medication in this study may be a high CES-D scores, but did not meet criteria for a
function of inadequate dosing. Finally, the depressive disorder, were randomly assigned to
Treatment of Adolescent Depression Study (2004) CWD or a no intervention group and followed up
compared CBT alone, fluoxetine alone, CBT at 6- and 12-month intervals after treatment com-
combined with fluoxetine, and placebo. Although pletion. In the second study, youth ages 13–19,
the combination treatment was superior to all whose parents had recently been depressed and
other conditions, fluoxetine was superior to CBT were themselves currently symptomatic (but did
alone, and CBT alone was not significantly better not meet criteria for major depressive or dysthy-
than pill placebo. It has been noted that the effect mic disorders) were assigned to usual care or
size for the CBT intervention in the TADS study usual care plus CWD and followed at 12 and
was significantly smaller than in other studies of 24 months after treatment was completed. In both
CBT, and the version used may have been “low studies, the rates of depressive disorders at fol-
potency” (Weisz et al., 2006). In fact, other inves- low-­up were significantly lower in the CWD-­
tigators have questioned the implementation of treated groups and comparable to community
the specific CBT intervention in the TADS study, rates of depression. In a small study of youth
suggesting it may have been overly structured with mild to moderate depressive symptoms,
and not have allowed the flexibility necessary for Ackerson and colleagues (Ackerson et al., 1998)
maximally impactful implementation (Hollon, found a significant advantage of implementing
Garber, & Shelton, 2005). At this time, given the cognitive bibliotherapy over no treatment, sug-
conflicting findings, the role of medication-­ gesting the possible utility of brief, inexpensive,
psychotherapy combination treatments for ado- cognitively focused interventions for milder
lescent depression remains unresolved. depressive symptoms. Finally, Reynolds and
Six studies have examined group cognitive Coats (1986) compared cognitive behavioral
behavioral interventions in adolescents with high therapy to relaxation training and to no interven-
levels of depressive symptoms rather than diag- tion. Although the cognitive behavioral interven-
nosed depressive disorders. In the studies where tion was superior to the no-treatment condition, it
treatments have been applied to participants who showed no advantage over relaxation training.
are not diagnosed, samples were frequently het- Overall, these studies of cognitive behavioral
erogeneous. Using cutoffs on continuous mea- interventions in youth with elevated depressive
sures of depressive symptoms, some researchers symptoms provide support for these treatments;
may include many youth with diagnosable however, they are unable to address whether psy-
depression (Kerfoot et al., 2004; Marcotte & chosocial treatments generally or cognitive
Baron, 1993; Reynolds & Coats, 1986), but for behavioral treatments specifically are superior to
practical reasons are unable to provide specific no-treatment conditions. 
diagnoses. Others, in an effort to focus on the Two studies found no advantage for cognitive
secondary prevention, studies in at risk-samples treatments. Kerfoot et al. (2004) found no differ-
have purposely excluded diagnosed youth and ences between brief cognitive behavioral therapy
focused on subsyndromal depression (Clarke and routine care in a sample of youth seen
et al., 1995, 2001). Due to this heterogeneity, it is through social service agencies. However, chro-
difficult to determine whether findings from nicity, comorbidity, and residential instability led
these studies can effectively be compared to one to high levels of treatment non-completion in this
another or generalized to youth with diagnosable challenging sample. Indeed, fewer than one half
Depression in Youth 175

of participants in the study completed four factors in the development of new computer
sessions of cognitive behavioral therapy.
­ delivered interventions. 
Marcotte and Baron (1993) compared rational While the immediate effects of cognitive
emotive therapy to no treatment in a small sam- behavioral interventions for youth depression
ple of teens with high depressive symptoms. have been evaluated, fewer studies have exam-
Symptoms reduced in both groups posttreatment; ined longer-term impact. Indeed, as revealed in
however, the extremely small sample and limited Tables 1 and 2, a limited number of studies have
statistical power make it difficult to draw mean- provided follow-up evaluations. Clarke and
ingful conclusions.  ­colleagues have included the longest follow-up
Cognitive interventions have been imple- intervals, ranging from 12 months (Rohde et al.,
mented in different formats, including individual 2004) to 24 months (Clarke et al., 1999; Clarke
and group. Of those studies conducted with diag- et al., 2002; Clarke et al., 2005; Lewinsohn et al.,
nosed depressed adolescents, nine were con- 1990). Results are mixed. In two studies in which
ducted in an individual format and four in a group there were initial group differences, treatment
format, and both approaches were associated with effects were maintained throughout a 24-month
significant improvements in depressive symp- follow-up in one (Lewinsohn et al., 1990), but
toms. Of those studies of adolescents with high attenuated by 12 months in another (Rohde et al.,
depressive symptoms, four used a group format 2004). Two studies showed no difference between
and three found significant advantage to CBT CBT and usual care conditions either immedi-
over a no intervention control (Clarke et al., 1999; ately or at a 24-month follow-up (Clarke et al.,
Clarke et al., 2001; Reynolds & Coats, 1986). 2002, 2005). One study examined the use of
Indeed, meta-analysis supports the observation booster sessions to promote recovery in youth
that both formats may be useful in the treatment who remained depressed following the 8-week
of adolescent depression (Weisz et al., 2006).  group CBT intervention and to prevent recur-
Computerized CBT programs for depressed rence in recovered youth (Clarke et al., 1995).
adolescents have recently been developed with Booster sessions significantly reduced time to
the aim of more broadly disseminating evidence-­ recovery in symptomatic youth, but failed to pre-
based treatments, especially to adolescents who vent recurrence. Alternatively, in a small pilot
would not otherwise have access to treatment. study, Kroll, Harrington, Jayson, Fraser, and
Between two studies included in the cognitive Gowers (1996) found much lower rates of relapse
behavioral treatment condition, the results are among youth receiving continuation CBT com-
mixed. A multisite, school-based randomized pared to an historical control group. Overall, the
controlled trial compared computerized CBT degree to which CBT interventions are sustained
program (C-CBT) to a waitlist condition for over time is not clear, and future studies need to
youth ages 12–16 with significant depressive include substantial follow-up periods. 
symptoms (Smith et al., 2015). Results demon- Two studies have examined the role of parent
strated that compared to waitlist control, C-CBT involvement in cognitive behavioral therapy.
was associated with statistically significant Each study compared the Adolescent Coping
improvements in depressive symptoms, main- with Depression (CWD) course alone with CWD
tained at 6-month follow-up, according to ado- supplemented with cognitive behavioral training
lescent self-report. In a second RCT (Poppelaars for parents and to waitlist control. In both stud-
et al., 2016), for 208 female adolescents with ies, treated groups had higher rates of recovery
depressive symptoms, there was no difference in from depression and greater reductions in depres-
change in symptomatology between a CBT pre- sive symptoms. However, there was not strong
vention program, a computerized CBT program, support for the addition of parental involvement.
a combination of the two, and a monitoring con- One of these studies found no difference between
trol condition. Going forward, establishing reli- CWD alone and CWD with the supplemental
ability and cultural competency will be critical parent group (Clarke et al., 1999), and the other
176 G.N. Kemp et al.

revealed only a slight trend for the adolescent-­ group to a therapeutic support group. Although
parent condition to outperform the adolescent-­ both groups had improved significantly posttreat-
only condition (Lewinsohn et al., 1990). Thus, while ment, contrary to expectation, the therapeutic
it is generally agreed that parent involvement in support group was superior in reducing depres-
youth treatments is important, extensive parent sive symptoms to the nonclinical range. Second,
involvement in the delivery of cognitive behav- Reed (1994) compared social skills training to an
ioral interventions is not supported.  attention placebo control condition. Although
Since Weisz et al. (2006) reported large effects participants in the overall skills group showed a
of cognitive behavioral treatments for youth greater improvement in clinicians’ ratings, there
depression across 31 RCTs, two meta-analyses were significant gender effects with boys show-
have supported CBT, both as an effective treat- ing some improvements and girls deteriorating.
ment for adolescent depression (Klein, Jacobs, & The small sample size in this study (18 partici-
Reinecke, 2007) and also as an effective interven- pants) makes it is difficult to draw firm conclu-
tion for adolescent depression prevention sions. Overall, the limited available data do not
(Hetrick, Cox, & Merry, 2015). It is important to suggest that social skills training alone is an effi-
note that the treatment effects of CBT have cacious treatment for adolescent depression. 
decreased from the large effects seen in early tri- Interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT) has recei­
als, most likely due to methodological differ- ved a fair amount of attention in the literature and
ences, including greater methodological rigor demonstrates promising results. In IPT clinicians
(Klein et al., 2007). focus on reducing depressive symptoms and
Hetrick et al. (2015) observe that across the 43 enhancing interpersonal functioning using an
trials included in the adolescent depression pre- active, collaborative approach and focusing on
vention meta-analysis, the only feature common one or two primary interpersonal problem areas.
to every program was some form of cognitive A recent meta-analysis combining research on
restructuring. The heterogeneity of interventions adults and adolescents (Cuijpers et al., 2011)
under the umbrella of CBT makes it difficult to found that IPT produced strong results when
pinpoint the mechanisms and process variables compared to waitlist control or treatment as
that are most effective for treatment. The field is usual, with a mean effect size (Cohen’s d) of
calling for further research to identify compo- 0.63. They also found that pharmacotherapy was
nents of treatment that are most strongly associ- slightly more efficacious than IPT, although the
ated with clinical improvements in adolescent combination of the two may produce stronger
depression (Klein et al., 2007; Webb, Auerbach, results. There was no effect of age group (adult
& DeRubeis, 2012; Weersing et al., 2016). versus adolescent) on these results (Cuijpers
et al., 2011). 
Interpersonal Approaches  Therapies focused We examined five RCTs that investigated the
on enhancing interpersonal functioning vary impact of IPT for the treatment of adolescent
widely and include group-based social skills depression and all show strong support for this
training, individually based interpersonal psy- intervention. In an initial study, Mufson and col-
chotherapy, and family-based interventions. leagues (Mufson et al., 1999) compared IPT to
While they share common goals of improving clinician monitoring in 48 depressed adolescents.
interpersonal relationships, decreasing social iso- Those patients in IPT showed greater improve-
lation, and enhancing interpersonal skills, these ments in depressive symptoms, social function-
interventions vary greatly in their formats, tech- ing, and problem-solving compared to those in
niques, and foci.  the clinical monitoring condition. Significantly
Two studies examining the efficacy of social more patients in IPT showed recovery from
skills training for depressed adolescents have depression as well. Second, Rosselló and Bernal
yielded mixed results. First, Fine et al. (1991) (1999) adapted the IPT model specifically for
compared a 12-session social skills training Puerto Rican adolescents. They compared IPT to
Depression in Youth 177

CBT and waitlist control conditions. Both CBT Two studies examined IPT for adolescents
and IPT were superior to the waitlist in reducing with elevated depressive symptoms. Young et al.
depression, and IPT was associated with greater (2006) examined a group IPT treatment for
gains in social functioning and self-esteem com- ­adolescents as compared to school counseling.
pared to the waitlist condition and exhibited a Students were eligible if they had scores between
larger overall effect size (0.73) than did CBT 16 and 39 on the Center for Epidemiologic
(0.43). Third, Mufson et al. (2004) replicated Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) and at least
their original findings by comparing IPT to treat- two subthreshold or threshold symptoms on the
ment as usual in a school-based health clinic. K-SADS-PL. There were 41 students enrolled in
Those receiving IPT showed greater reductions the study who were in grades 7–10. Adolescents
in symptoms of depression and improvements on in the IPT-AST group had lower depressive
social and global functioning. Fourth, Rosselló symptoms at all follow-ups (post, 3 months, and
et al. (2008) compared the impact of IPT and 6 month). Furthermore, fewer participants in the
CBT in a group and individual formats, with both IPT-AST group met criteria for depression at any
approaches culturally adapted for a predomi- point in the study (3.7%) than in the SC group
nantly Latino sample. They found similar results (28.6%). In a follow-up study, investigators
regardless of whether treatment was delivered in enrolled 57 participants in a similarly designed
a group versus individual format, with a slightly study, but this time extended the follow-up period
greater impact of individual treatment. This out to 12 months (Young et al., 2010). Eligibility
­finding is important given the cost reductions criteria and the interventions remained the same.
associated with group treatment as compared to As compared to the SC group, participants in the
individual. While both conditions resulted in IPT-AST group again displayed fewer depressive
improvements in depressive symptoms, the CBT symptoms as well as improved overall function-
group experienced greater reductions and incre­ ing at the posttreatment assessment as well as at
ased positive self-concept, with 62% of partici- 6 months. It is interesting to note, however, that at
pants functioning in the nonclinical range the 12-month follow-up, the difference in depres-
compared to 57% of the IPT group. Lastly, sive symptoms and overall functioning between
O’Shea et al. (2015) compared IPT delivered in a the groups was no longer significant (Young
group versus individual format. They did not et al., 2010). 
have another type of treatment since the goal of The importance of family support and involve-
the study was to determine if the two formats ment in youth treatment is often considered a
produced similar results. Using intent-to-treat
­ clinical given, but few studies have examined the
analysis, their results demonstrated significant role of family treatments in adolescent depres-
improvements in depression, as well as anxiety, sion. Brent, Poling, McKain, and Baugher (1993)
at the posttreatment assessment. There was no demonstrated that a two-hour psychoeducational
significant difference between the two formats session for parents was associated with their
and gains were maintained at 12 months. Overall, greater knowledge and fewer dysfunctional
IPT appears to be a powerful treatment for beliefs about depression and its treatment. Most
depression in youth that is flexible in its adapta- parents (97%) found such psychoeducation to be
tion to other cultural contexts. Given its focus on worthwhile. Indeed, in their large adolescent
interpersonal functioning, IPT necessarily depression treatment study, Brent et al. (1997)
empha­ sizes family relationships and often included brief family psychoeducation in all
includes parents in some sessions (Mufson et al., treatment conditions with the goal of minimizing
2004). While long-term effects of IPT were dropout and supporting treatment. However, the
reported in one study, more research is needed. outcomes of more extended interventions aimed
Most follow-ups occurred only immediately at altering family relationships have been more
(Mufson et al., 1999, 2004) or up to 3 months fol- mixed. First, Brent et al. (1997) compared
lowing treatment completion.  systemic-­behavioral family therapy to individual
178 G.N. Kemp et al.

CBT and individual nondirective supportive was found to be more tolerable than duloxetine
therapy for the treatment of adolescents with
­ and imipramine (Cipriani et al., 2016). There
major depressive disorder. The systemic-behav- have also been concerns over the past decade
ioral family therapy focused on altering family about the potential for increased risk of self-harm
interaction patterns through the use of reframing associated with SRRIs in youth (US Food and
and communication and problem-solving skills Drug Administration, 2004). Although the over-
interventions. However, this family therapy was all risk for treatment-emergent suicidal ideation/
­significantly less effective than CBT and compa- attempt is fairly low (less than 1% in one meta-­
rable to nondirective supportive therapy. Second,analysis), there is a small, but increased, risk fol-
Diamond and colleagues developed and exam- lowing SSRI use in youth (Bridge et al., 2007).
ined attachment-based family therapy (ABFT; Cipriani et al. (2016) found that imipramine,
Diamond et al., 2002), a family treatment model duloxetine, and venlafaxine had more adverse
for depressed adolescents that is derived from events than did placebo groups, with venlafaxine,
attachment theory. This treatment focuses on a selective serotonin noradrenergic reuptake
building alliances between the therapist and bothinhibitor (SNRI), significantly increasing the risk
the parent and the adolescent, repairing the for suicidal ideation/behavior in younger chil-
parent-­child bond, and (with parental support) dren. Additionally, although there are fewer med-
building the adolescent’s competencies. An ini- ication trials with younger children, studies have
tial evaluation of this model, comparing it to a indicated that placebo response rates tend to be
higher among youth under age 12 (Bridge et al.,
waitlist control group, found substantially greater
rates of recovery (81% versus 47%) from dep­ 2007), with the exception of fluoxetine (Mayes
ression; these recovery rates were maintained et al., 2007). Therefore, further research is neces-
at 6-month follow-up. Results from an RCT sary in order to better understand the differences
revealed that ABFT was more efficacious than in antidepressant medication response in children
enhanced usual care in reducing suicidal ideationcompared to adolescents. 
and depressive symptomatology in adolescents Given that only approximately 60% of adoles-
(Diamond et al., 2010).  cents respond to initial SSRI treatment, one study
examined the most efficacious treatment strategy
Overall, interventions that focus on interper-
sonal functioning appear promising in the treat- for adolescents who do not initially respond to an
ment of adolescent depression. However, the initial adequate SSRI treatment (Brent et al.,
appropriate role of family involvement has yet to 2008). Adolescents in the study who did not
be clarified in treating depression during this respond to an initial trial of SSRI treatment were
developmental period, and clinicians tread a dif- randomized to one of four groups that included
ficult path in balancing the need to enhance fam- treatment with a different SSRI alone, venlafax-
ily support and functioning while supporting the ine alone, or one of the two plus CBT. After
adolescent’s burgeoning autonomy. 12 weeks of intervention, CBT plus either a dif-
ferent SSRI or venlafaxine showed a greater
Medication Trials  Although the use of antide- response rate than a switch to medication alone
pressants, specifically selective serotonin reup- (Brent et al., 2008). By 24 weeks of treatment,
take inhibitors (SSRIs), is commonly used in the approximately 60% of participants achieved
treatment of depressive disorders in youth, effi- remission, although initial treatment assignment
cacy as a stand-alone treatment is questionable. did not predict likelihood of remission (Vitiello
Recent studies indicate that SSRIs are a rather et al., 2010). However, the SSRI group had a
heterogeneous group, with some consistently more rapid decline in self-reported depressive
outperforming others. Several recent meta-­ symptoms and suicidal ideation compared to
analyses demonstrated that fluoxetine was the those in the venlafaxine group (Vitiello et al., 2010).
only SSRI more efficacious than a placebo group Moreover, likelihood of remission and time to
(Bridge et al., 2007; Cipriani et al., 2016) and remission were more likely if demonstrated after
Depression in Youth 179

12 weeks of treatment, suggesting that early promising results with diagnosed school-aged
intervention may be particularly important youth (Flory, 2004; Kaslow, Baskin, Wyckoff, &
among a non-responder group (Emslie et al., Kaslow, 2002; Tompson et al., 2007), and one
2010).  with preschool-aged youth (Lenze, Pautsch, &
As these findings demonstrate, the relative effi- Luby, 2011), but the majority of RCTs have
cacy of a combined treatment of SSRI and CBT focused on preadolescents with high levels of
compared to monotherapies remains unclear, depressive symptoms, with only three studies
although the combination of fluoxetine and CBT that exclusively targeted preadolescents with
may be more effective than fluoxetine or CBT diagnosed clinical depression (Dietz et al., 2015;
alone in treatment-resistant depression (Brent Luby et al., 2012; Tompson et al., 2017). More­
et al., 2008). However, given the evidence for the over, of the studies including preadolescents,
effectiveness of psychotherapy for the treatment ­several have not separated adolescents and pre-
of child and adolescent depression (described ear- adolescents. Three of the studies discussed under
lier in this chapter) and the unclear findings adolescent depression treatments (Goodyer et al.,
regarding the use of combination or mono-phar- 2007; Vostanis et al., 1996; Wood et al., 1996)
macological interventions in treating child and include children under the age of 12 and up to the
adolescent depression, it stands that psychother- age of 17. Three of the studies we included in our
apy may be the best choice for a first line treat- preadolescent review also included children
ment. Currently, in line with the findings presented 12 years and older (Dietz et al., 2015; Gillham
above, fluoxetine is the only SSRI approved by et al., 2006; Tompson et al., 2017).These issues
the FDA for the treatment of child and adolescent of diagnosis and heterogeneity make it difficult to
depression, although it should be noted that there parse out the exact roles that development and
have been fewer studies assessing its use specifi- depression status may play in treatment efficacy.
cally with children under the age of 12. Current Of the 13 studies listed in Table 3, ten used a
practice parameters do not include the use of tri- group format. These ten either include a CBT
cyclic antidepressants in the treatment of youth intervention explicitly or examine a particular
depression, given lack of efficacy and side effect component of this approach. However, unlike
profiles in comparison to SSRIs. interventions for adolescent depression, these
interventions focused heavily on improving inter-
personal functioning and building interpersonal
Preadolescent Depression skills, combining cognitive behavioral and inter-
personal approaches. The types of skills included
Although CBT is the most widely and thoroughly problem-solving, self-monitoring, and social
investigated intervention approach for adolescent ability. For example, Asarnow and colleagues
­
depression, far less evidence exists for any inter- (Asarnow et al., 2002) have a specific portion of
vention for childhood depression (Forti-Buratti, the protocol devoted to helping children build
Saikia, Wilkinson, & Ramchandani, 2016), and friendships in a developmentally appropriate way
most studies are conducted with youth with high that acknowledges the social challenges of late
depressive symptoms rather than those with diag- elementary and middle school youth. Compared to
nosed depressive disorders. Table 3 illustrates the interventions for adolescents, those designed for
small literature that exists on RCTs of inter­ children have more incorporation of parents in ses-
ventions for preadolescent depression. The most sions. For example, the intervention elaborated by
commonly examined intervention was CBT. Gillham et al. (2006) added a parent component on
Several studies examined social skills approaches, the basis that children learn interpretive and cop-
one assessed family-based IPT (FB-IPT) and ing styles from their parents and caregivers. Across
another investigated an adapted form of parent- six sessions, parents learned the core skills of the
child interaction therapy (PCIT). Some prelimi- program their children received and learned how
nary treatment development studies have shown to incorporate the skills into their parenting.
180

Table 3  Randomized clinical interventions trials for preadolescents with depression


Diagnostic/risk Treatment Post-­intervention
Reference Subjects assessment format(s) Intervention type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Asarnow, Scott, 4th–6th graders School screening; CDI Group (1) CBT and family Immediate Children in the intervention group were
and Mintz (n = 23) education more likely to show reductions in
(2002) (2) Waitlist control depressive symptoms, negative
cognitions, and internalizing coping
Butler, Miezitis, 5th–6th graders Teacher referral; high Group (1) Role-play Immediate Role-play group showed significant
Friedman, and (n = 56) scores on CDI problem-solving reduction on CDI and improved
Cole (1980) (2) Cognitive classroom functioning. One of two
restructuring groups in cognitive restructuring showed
(3) Attention control significant reductions on CDI
De Cuyper, Ages 10–12 (n = 20) CDI score >11 and/or Group (1) CBT program Immediate; Four-month follow-up comparisons with
Timbremont, T score >23 on CBCL (“taking action”) 4 months; baseline measures showed significant
Braet, De internalizing and (2) Waitlist control 12 months improvement on the CDI and the
Backer, and anxious/depressed group self-perception profile only for CBT
Wullaert (2004) subscale; at least one group. At the 12-month follow-up, CBT
MDD criterion but group showed further improvement and
without other apparent significant decreases on the CDI, STAI,
Axis I and CBCL
Dietz et al. Ages 7–12 (n = 42) DSM-IV diagnoses of Family (1) FB-IPT Immediate Greater depression remission rates in
(2015) MDD (2) CCT FB-IPT group (66%) as compared to
CCT group (31%). FB-IPT participants
also had significantly lower depressive
symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and
interpersonal impairment as compared to
the CCT group
Gillham et al. Ages 11–12 (n = 44) School-based sample Group (1) School-based Immediate; Students in intervention arm reported
(2006) selected for higher CDI cognitive behavioral 6 months; lower levels of depressive symptoms
scores (mean = 10.56; depression 12 months over the follow-up period. The
SD = 5.99) prevention program intervention effect was not significant at
with parent post-assessment but was significant at
intervention 6- and 12-month follow-ups
component
(2) No intervention
control
G.N. Kemp et al.
Diagnostic/risk Treatment Post-­intervention
Reference Subjects assessment format(s) Intervention type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Jaycox, Reivich, Ages 10–13 (n = 143) Z-scores on CDI + Group (1) Cognitive Immediate; No differences between treated groups
Gillham, and Child Perception (2) Social 6 months; who had fewer depressive symptoms at
Seligman Questionnaire >0.50 problem-solving 12 months; posttest and at follow-up and improved
(1994), and (3) Combined (both 18 months; classroom behavior (teacher report) than
Gillham, above treatments) 24 months untreated groups. Effects more
Reivich, Jaycox, pronounced among children from
(4) Wait list control
and Seligman high-conflict homes. Follow-up revealed
Depression in Youth

(1995) (5) No participation even greater group differences in


control depressive symptoms over time
Kahn, Kehle, Ages 10–14 (n = 68) Multistage gating: Group (1) Cognitive Immediate; All active treatment groups showed
Jensen, and Stage 1: CDI >14; behavioral 1 month significant improvement in depression
Clark (1990) RADS >71. Stage 2: (2) Relaxation compared to control. Most children in
Reassessment 1 month training CBT and relaxation groups went from
later with CDI and (3) Self-modeling dysfunctional to functional range on
RADS. Stage 3: depressive symptoms; self-modeling
(4) Wait list control
Interview, BDI>19. No group less improved than other groups
other depression
treatment
King and KG- 4 graders (n = 135) Children who scored Group (1) Social skills Immediate Combined program showed reduced
Kirschenbaum above a cutoff on the training plus depression as compared to consultation
(1990) activity mood consultation with only. Multidimensional ratings of
screening questionnaire parents and teachers behavior and skills improved across both
(2) Consultation groups
only
Liddle and Ages 7–11 (n = 31) CDI ≥19 Group (1) Social Immediate; No group differences at pretest, posttest,
Spence (1990) competence training 3 months or follow-up. All groups declined on
CDRS-R ≥40 (2) Attention placebo CDI scores and increased on teacher’s
(3) Waitlist control reports of problem behavior
(continued)
181
Table 3 (continued)
182

Diagnostic/risk Treatment Post-­intervention


Reference Subjects assessment format(s) Intervention type(s) assessment Impact of treatment
Luby et al. Ages 3–7 (n = 54) Research diagnostic Parent (1) PCIT-ED Immediate Both groups showed significant
(2012) DSM-IV criteria for training (2) Psychoeducation decreases in depression severity. There
MDD as assessed by parent group on were no differences in PAPA MDD
the PAPA child development severity scores between groups. The
PCIT-ED group showed significant
improvement in a larger number of
clinical symptom categories. PCIT-ED
resulted in statistically significant
improvements in ratings of executive
functioning and emotion regulation
Stark, Reynolds, 4th–5th graders CDI scores >12 on 2 Group (1) Behavioral Immediate; Both active treatment groups showed
and Kaslow (n = 29) administrations problem-solving 8 weeks significant reductions in depressive
(1987) (2) Self-control symptoms; however, in behavioral
(3) Waitlist control problem-solving, both mothers and
children reported differences, whereas in
self-control only children reported
differences
Weisz et al. 3rd–6th graders (n = 48) CDI ≥10 and/or Group (1) Primary and Immediate; At posttest and follow-up, treated group
(1997) identified by teachers/ secondary control 9 months showed significantly greater reductions
counselor as depressed; enhancement training on both CDI and CDRS-R
and CDRS-R interview (2) No-treatment
score ≥34 control
Tompson et al. Ages 7–14 (n = 134) Diagnosis of MDD, DD, (1) (1) Supportive Immediate FFT-CD was associated with greater
(2017b) or DDNOS based on Individual Therapy adequate clinical response compared to
K-SADS-PL interview (2) Family (2) FFT-CD (Family- individual; FFT-CD parents reported
Focused Treatment for more understanding of youth depression
Childhood and more ability to help their child;
Depression) FFT-CD children reported more ability to
deal with problems
BDI Beck Depression Inventory, BID Bellevue Index of Depression, CCT child-centered therapy, CDI Children’s Depression Inventory, CDRS-R Revised Children’s Depression
Rating Scale, CES-D Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale, DD dysthymic disorder, DDNOS depressive disorder not otherwise specified, FB-IPT family-based
IPT, GAF Global Assessment of Functioning Scale, K-SADS Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for school-aged children, MDD major depressive disorder,
PCIT-ED Parent-Child Interaction Therapy-Emotion Development, PAPA Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment, RADS Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale
G.N. Kemp et al.
Depression in Youth 183

Of the ten CBT intervention studies presented, Three studies incorporated interventions that
treated groups showed significant improvements included work with parents and families. In the
over untreated groups in reduction of depressive first study, Dietz et al. (2015) conducted an
symptoms in nine of the studies (Asarnow et al., ­intervention trial comparing family-based inter-
2002; Butler et al., 1980; De Cuyper et al., 2004; personal therapy (FB-IPT) and child-centered
Gillham et al., 2006; Jaycox et al., 1994; Kahn therapy (CCT). They enrolled 42 children
et al., 1990; King & Kirschenbaum, 1990; Stark between the ages of 7 and 12 who met DSM-IV
et al., 1987; Weisz et al., 1997). Liddle and criteria for a depressive disorder. At posttreat-
Spence (1990) found that children treated with ment, children in the FB-IPT condition experi-
social competence therapy, attention control, and enced greater remission rates (66%) than those in
no treatment all improved over time and showed the CCT group (31%). Also, FB-IPT was associ-
no group differences. ated with significantly less depressive symptoms,
Despite positive post-intervention results and anxiety symptoms, and interpersonal impairment
preliminary evidence of good maintenance of (Dietz et al., 2015).
treatment gains (De Cuyper et al., 2004; Gillham In the largest study to examine treatment for
et al., 1995; Jaycox et al., 1994; Weisz et al., preadolescent youth with depression, Tompson
1997), there is currently no evidence of relative et al. (2017) conducted a clinical trial comparing
superiority of one type of CBT-oriented psy­ family-focused treatment for childhood depres-
chotherapy over another for child depression. sion (FFT-CD) to an individual client-centered
Five of the group-based CBT (and CBT compo- therapy (IP). The FFT-CD involved parents in 15
nent) studies listed in Table 3 compared different sessions focused on enhancing family relation-
treatments to one another and three of those ships and building skills for communicating and
included a full CBT component. In terms of CBT solving problems (Tompson, Langer, Hughes, &
efficacy, this small literature points to no significant Asarnow, 2017a). Although youth in both
advantage of CBT over role-play problem-­solving treatment conditions demonstrated significant
­
(Butler et al., 1980), social problem-­ solving improve­ment over time on depressive symptoms,
(Gillham et al., 1995; Jaycox et al., 1994), or comorbid problems, and functional outcomes,
relaxation training (Kahn et al., 1990). depression response was greater in the FFT-CD
The role of the family in the treatment of condition (78%) than in the IP group (60%).
depression in school-aged youth remains to be Finally, after an open trial to adapt parent-­
clarified. Although the interventions examined at child interaction therapy (PCIT) for preschool-­
this point have focused on group formats, several aged children, meeting criteria for depression
have included family involvement (Asarnow (Lenze et al., 2011) Luby et al. (2012) conducted
et al., 2002; Stark, 1990). Given the embedded- a pilot RCT to address the need for research on
ness of school-aged youth within their families, depression interventions for this younger age
there are strong reasons to believe that family-­ group. PCIT was developed as a therapeutic
based approaches may be particularly potent approach that uses therapist coaching to help par-
­during this developmental period. Indeed, in a ents handle difficult child externalizing behaviors
study of family intervention for childhood anxi- by enhancing the parent-child relationship and
ety disorders comparing individual CBT, CBT teaching parents more effective behavior man-
plus family treatment, and a waitlist control agement (Eyberg, 1988). Partly based on PCIT
group, Barrett, Dadds, and Rapee (1996) found a adaptations to address child anxiety (Pincus,
significant age effect; younger children showed Eyberg, & Choate, 2005), study investigators
better outcomes in CBT plus family treatment, adapted PCIT to treat preschool-onset depression
whereas older children did equally well in both by incorporating an emotion development (ED)
active treatments. These findings highlight the component. Therapists coach parents on support-
importance of examining family-based treat- ing the child’s emotion regulation, using tech-
ments in school-aged youth. niques such as relaxation training, labeling and
184 G.N. Kemp et al.

validating emotions, identifying triggers, and optimistically suggested very large effect sizes
addressing parental factors that may hinder their (Lewinsohn & Clarke, 1999; Reinecke, Ryan, &
ability to respond consistently and calmly to DuBois, 1998), more recent meta-analyses with
intense child emotions (Lenze et al., 2011; Luby more comprehensive inclusion of treatment stud-
et al., 2012). Fifty-four children who met criteria ies indicate modest effect sizes (Forti-Buratti
for MDD based on clinical assessment using the et al., 2016; Weisz et al., 2006). Medication
Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA) interventions appear equally limited (Cipriani
­
were enrolled in the trial. The comparison inter- et al., 2016). A substantial portion of clinically
vention was a parent psychoeducation group that depressed youth fail to show significant recovery
provided parents with information on child social or remission in trials of either psychosocial or
and emotional development. Children in both pharmacologic treatments. These findings high-
groups experienced reductions in depression light the critical need to develop new efficacious
severity; however, as a small pilot RCT, the study treatment strategies and enhance the impact of
was not powered to detect statistically significant current treatments.
differences. Nonetheless, the PCIT-ED group Second, there remains a need to understand
exp­erienced improvements in a larger number of the mechanisms underlying effective treatment.
symptom areas and improved executive function- Mediators are variables that may explain changes
ing (based on parental report on the BRIEF mea- in treatment that are essential to influencing out-
sure of executive functioning) as compared to the comes of interest. By understanding mediation
psychoeducation group (Luby et al., 2012). These we can discover how treatments operate to effect
two studies provide preliminary data showing change. Unfortunately, few RCTs have ade-
that incorporating parents and families in treat- quately evaluated mediation. Several have exam-
ment is likely important, especially for pre­ ined cognitive changes as a mediator in trials of
adolescents and preschool-aged children. The CBT for depression, but findings have been
paucity of research in this area supports the need equivocal. Understanding mechanisms underly-
for more studies on the development of family ing effective treatment may help us to design and
and parental interventions for children with a alter interventions to maximize their impact.
depression diagnosis. Third, special populations of youth with
depression have received inadequate attention.
For example, compared to work on adolescent
Directions for Future Research depression treatment, far fewer studies have
focused on preadolescent depression. Develop­
Treatment research to date supports the value of mental considerations during early, middle, and
psychosocial approaches for the treatment of late childhood, including greater dependence on
youth depression. However, many issues remain parents and rapidly changing cognitive capacity,
and further research is necessary to determine point to a need for developmentally informed
optimal strategies for the comprehensive treat- treatment specifically for children. Recent clini-
ment of depression. Areas of concern include cal trials provide support for integrating families
incomplete treatment efficacy, lack of adequate in treatment specifically focused on interpersonal
understanding of the mechanisms of effective coping. As another example, there is a paucity of
treatment, paucity of research examining specific research on depression in individuals with devel-
populations of youth with depression, and l­ imited opmental and intellectual disabilities. Although
data examining treatments in “real world” clinical limited, studies suggest that individuals with
settings. developmental disabilities and/or intellectual dis-
First, treatments to date, both pharmacologi- abilities can experience high levels of depressive
cal and psychosocial, have demonstrated limited symptoms (Magnuson & Constantino, 2011), and
efficacy. Although earlier meta-analytic studies those on the autism spectrum with higher levels
of psychosocial treatments for youth depression of insight may be at particular risk (Ghaziuddin
Depression in Youth 185

et al., 2002; Matson & Nebel-Schwalm, 2007; References


Saulnier & Volkmar, 2007). There are no clinical
trials examining depression treatments for these Abela, J. R., & Hankin, B. L. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook
special p­ opulations, and there is a strong need of depression in children and adolescents. New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
to develop and test treatments for these Aber, L. J., Brown, J. L., & Jones, S. M. (2003).
populations. Developmental trajectories toward violence in
Finally, although RCTs indicate efficacy for middle childhood: Course, demographic differ-
CBT and other interventions for depressed youth, ences, and response to school-based intervention.
Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 324–348. https://
effectiveness trials in which these interventions doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.324
have been implemented in usual care settings Ackerson, J., Scogin, F., McKendree-Smith, N., &
(i.e., community clinics, pediatric settings) have Lyman, R. D. (1998). Cognitive bibliotherapy for mild
produced mixed results. Several studies have and moderate adolescent depressive symptomatology.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(4),
indicated weak effects when CBT was imple- 685–690.
mented in community mental health clinics Alfano, C. A., Zakem, A. H., Costa, N. M., Taylor, L. K.,
(Clarke et al., 2002; Clarke et al., 2005; TADS, & Weems, C. F. (2009). Sleep problems and their
2004; Weisz et al., 1995), while others have indi- relation to cognitive factors, anxiety, and depressive
symptoms in children and adolescents. Depression
cated positive impact in these clinics (Weersing and Anxiety, 26(6), 503–512. https://doi.org/10.1002/
& Weisz, 2002) and in primary care (Asarnow da.20443
American Psychiatric Association. (2006). American psy-
et al., 2005; Richardson et al., 2014). Interestingly,
those in primary care were part of an integrated chiatric association practice guidelines for the treat-
ment of psychiatric disorders: Compendium 2006.
collaborative care model, suggesting the impor- Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
tance of considering a more comprehensive treat- Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999).
ment delivery system. Overall, findings highlight Comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and
the need to both examine and enhance depression Psychiatry, 40(01), 57–87.
Apter-Levi, Y., Pratt, M., Vakart, A., Feldman, M., Zagoory-­
treatment in real-world settings. Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2016). Maternal depres-
sion across the first years of life compromises child
psychosocial adjustment; relations to child HPA-axis
Conclusions functioning. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 64, 47–56.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.11.006
Archie, S., Kazemi, A. Z., & Akhtar-Danesh, N. (2010).
Our understanding of depression and its treat- Comorbid depression and binge drinking: Risk
ment in youth has advanced significantly in the for suicidality among youth. Journal of Affective
past 20 years, and guidelines for clinical practice Disorders, 122, S32–S41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
have been developed. In addition to medication jad.2010.02.005
Arnone, D., McKie, S., Elliott, R., Thomas, E. J.,
strategies, interpersonal interventions, family-­ Downey, D., Juhasz, G., … Anderson, I. M. (2012).
based strategies, and both individually based and Increased amygdala responses to sad but not fearful
group-based cognitive behavioral interventions faces in major depression: Relation to mood state
are treatment options for depressed youth. How­ and pharmacological treatment. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 169(8), 841–850. https://doi.org/10.1176/
ever, additional research is required to enhance appi.ajp.2012.11121774
and develop treatment approaches, to understand Asarnow, J. R., Jaycox, L. H., Duan, N., LaBorde, A. P.,
underlying mechanisms, and to address the needs Rea, M. M., Murray, P., … Wells, K. B. (2005).
of specific populations of depressed youth. Effectiveness of a quality improvement intervention
for adolescent depression in primary care clinics: A
Importantly, the effective treatments described in randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American
this chapter need to be more widely available Medical Association, 293(3), 311–319. https://doi.
across settings and in care models that will opti- org/10.1001/jama.293.3.311
mize outcomes for youth. Much work remains in Asarnow, J. R., Scott, C. V., & Mintz, J. (2002). A combined
cognitive-behavioral family education intervention
effectively intervening to enhance the lives and for depression in children: A treatment development
futures of youth suffering from depression and study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26(2), 221–
their families. 229. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014573803928
186 G.N. Kemp et al.

Avenevoli, S., Knight, E., Kessler, R. C., & Merikangas, Burwell, R. A., & Shirk, S. R. (2007). Subtypes of rumina-
K. R. (2008). Epidemiology of depression in children tion in adolescence: Associations between brooding,
and adolescents. In J. Z. Abela, B. L. Hankin, J. Z. reflection, depressive symptoms, and coping. Journal
Abela, & B. L. Hankin (Eds.), Handbook of depres- of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(1),
sion in children and adolescents (pp. 6–32). New York, 56–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410709336568
NY: Guilford Press. Butler, L., Miezitis, S., Friedman, R., & Cole, E. (1980).
Bardone, A. M., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., & Dickson, N. The effect of two school-based intervention programs
(1996). Adult mental health and social outcomes of on depressive symptoms in preadolescents. American
adolescent girls with depression and conduct disor- Educational Research Journal, 17, 111–119.
der. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 811–829. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007446 (2015). Suicide: Facts at a glance. http://www.cdc.
Barrett, P. M., Dadds, M. R., & Rapee, R. M. (1996). gov/violenceprevention/pdf/suicide-datasheet-a.pdf
Family treatment of childhood anxiety: A controlled Céspedes, Y. M., & Huey, S. J., Jr. (2008). Depression
trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, in Latino adolescents: A cultural discrep-
64(2), 333–342. ancy perspective. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Beardslee, W. R., Gladstone, T. R., & O’Connor, E. E. Minority Psychology, 14(2), 168–172. https://doi.
(2011). Transmission and prevention of mood dis- org/10.1037/1099-9809.14.2.168
orders among children of affectively ill parents: A Cipriani, A., Zhou, X., Del Giovane, C., Hetrick, S. E.,
review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Qin, B., Whittington, C., … Cuijpers, P. (2016).
Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(11), 1098–1109. https:// Comparative efficacy and tolerability of antidepres-
doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.07.020 sants for major depressive disorder in children and
Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. adolescents: A network meta-analysis. The Lancet,
(2007). Polyvagal theory and developmental psy- 388(10047), 881–890. https://doi.org/10.1016/
chopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct S0140-6736(16)30385-3
problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological Clarke, G., Debar, L., Lynch, F., Powell, J., Gale, J.,
Psychology, 74(2), 174–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. O'Connor, E., … Hertert, S. (2005). A randomized
biopsycho.2005.08.008 effectiveness trial of brief cognitive-behavioral ther-
Beauchaine, T. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2015). Heart rate vari- apy for depressed adolescents receiving antidepres-
ability as a transdiagnostic biomarker of psychopa- sant medication. Journal of the American Academy
thology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(9), 888–898.
98(2), 338–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0890-8567(09)62194-8
ijpsycho.2015.08.004 Clarke, G., & Harvey, A. G. (2012). The complex role of
Brent, D., Emslie, G., Clarke, G., Wagner, K., Asarnow, sleep in adolescent depression. Child and Adolescent
J., Keller, M., … Birmaher, B. (2008). Switching to Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 21(2), 385–400.
another SSRI or to venlafaxine with or without cog- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2012.01.006
nitive behavioral therapy for adolescents with SSRI-­ Clarke, G. N., Hawkins, W., Murphy, M., Scheeber,
resistant depression: The TORDIA randomized L. B., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1995).
controlled trial. JAMA, 299(8), 901–913. https://doi. Targeted prevention of unipolar depressive disor-
org/10.1001/jama.299.8.901 der in an at-risk sample of high school adolescents:
Brent, D. A., Holder, D., Kolko, D., Birmaher, B., A randomized trial of a group cognitive interven-
Baugher, M., Roth, C., … Johnson, B. A. (1997). A tion. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
clinical psychotherapy trial for adolescent depression Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(3), 312–321. https://doi.
comparing cognitive, family, and supportive therapy. org/10.1097/00004583-199503000-00016
Archives of General Psychiatry, 54(9), 877–885. Clarke, G. N., Hornbrook, M., Lynch, F., Polen, M., Gale,
Brent, D. A., Poling, K., McKain, B., & Baugher, M. J., Beardslee, W., … Seeley, J. (2001). A randomized
(1993). A psychoeducational program for families trial of a group cognitive intervention for preventing
of affectively ill children and adolescents. Journal depression in adolescent offspring of depressed par-
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent ents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(12), 1127–
Psychiatry, 32(4), 770–774. 1134. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1127
Bridge, J. A., Goldstein, T. R., & Brent, D. A. (2006). Clarke, G. N., Hornbrook, M., Lynch, F., Polen, M.,
Adolescent suicide and suicidal behavior. Journal of Gale, J., O’Connor, E., … Debar, L. (2002). Group
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3–4), 372–394. cognitive-behavioral treatment for depressed adoles-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01615.x cent offspring of depressed parents in a health mainte-
Bridge, J. A., Iyengar, S., Salary, C. B., Barbe, R. P., nance organization. Journal of the American Academy
Birmaher, B., Pincus, H. A., … Brent, D. A. (2007). of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(3), 305–313.
Clinical response and risk for reported suicidal ide- https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200203000-00010
ation and suicide attempts in pediatric antidepressant Clarke, G. N., Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Hops,
treatment: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled H., & Seeley, J. R. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral
trials. JAMA, 297(15), 1683–1696. https://doi.org/ treatment of adolescent depression: Efficacy
10.1001/jama.297.15.1683 of acute group treatment and booster sessions.
Depression in Youth 187

Journal of the American Academy of Child and to interpersonal trauma: A case study with two teens.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(3), 272–279. https://doi. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 20(2), 189–201.
org/10.1097/00004583-199903000-00014 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2012.07.001
Costello, E., Erkanli, A., & Angold, A. (2006). Is there an Diamond, G. S., Reis, B. F., Diamond, G. M., Siqueland,
epidemic of child or adolescent depression? Journal of L., & Isaacs, L. (2002). Attachment-based family ther-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(12), 1263–1271. apy for depressed adolescents: A treatment develop-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01682.x ment study. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Costello, E. J., Angold, A., Burns, B. J., Stangl, D. K., & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1190–1196. https://
Tweed, D. L., Erkanli, A., & Worthman, C. M. doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200210000-00008
(1996). The Great Smoky Mountains Study of Youth: Diamond, G. S., Wintersteen, M. B., Brown, G. K.,
Goals, designs, methods, and prevalence of DSM-­ Diamond, G. M., Gallop, R., Shelef, K., & Levy, S.
III-­R disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, (2010). Attachment-based family therapy for adoles-
53(12), 1129–1136. https://doi.org/10.1001/ cents with suicidal ideation: A randomized controlled
archpsyc.1996.01830120067012 trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Costello, E. J., Copeland, W., & Angold, A. (2011). Trends Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(2), 122–131.
in psychopathology across the adolescent years: What Dietz, L. J., Weinberg, R. J., Brent, D. A., & Mufson,
changes when children become adolescents, and L. (2015). Family-based interpersonal psychother-
when adolescents become adults? Journal of Child apy for depressed preadolescents: Examining effi-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(10), 1015–1025. cacy and potential treatment mechanisms. Journal
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02446.x of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., McCauley, E., Smith, Psychiatry, 54(3), 191–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
C. J., Stevens, A. L., & Sylvers, P. (2005). Psycho­ jaac.2014.12.011
logical, autonomic, and serotonergic correlates of Elkind, D. (1978). Understanding the young adolescent.
parasuicide among adolescent girls. Development and Adolescence, 13(49), 127–134.
Psychopathology, 17(4), 1105–1127. https://doi.org/1 Emerson, E. (2003). Prevalence of psychiatric disor-
0.10170S0954579405050522 ders in children and adolescents with and with-
Cuffe, S. P., Waller, J. L., Cuccaro, M. L., Pumariega, A. J., out intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual
& Garrison, C. Z. (1995). Race and gender differences Disability Research, 47(1), 51–58. https://doi.
in the treatment of psychiatric disorders in young org/10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00464.x
adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Emslie, G. J., Armitage, R., Weinberg, W. A., Rush, A. J.,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(11), 1536–1543. Mayes, T. L., & Hoffmann, R. F. (2001). Sleep poly-
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199511000-00021 somnography as a predictor of recurrence in children
Cuijpers, P., Geraedts, A. S., van Oppen, P., Andersson, and adolescents with major depressive disorder. The
G., Markowitz, J. C., & van Straten, A. (2011). International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology,
Interpersonal psychotherapy for depression: A meta-­ 4(2), 159–168. https://doi.org/10.1017/
analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(6), 581– S1461145701002383
592. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10101411 Emslie, G. J., Kennard, B. D., Mayes, T. L., Nakonezny,
Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., van Oppen, P., & Andersson, P. A., Zhu, L., Tao, R., … Croarkin, P. (2012).
G. (2008). Are psychological and pharmacologic Insomnia moderates outcome of serotonin-selective
interventions equally effective in the treatment of adult reuptake inhibitor treatment in depressed youth.
depressive disorders? A meta-analysis of comparative Journal of child and adolescent psychopharma-
studies. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(11), cology, 22(1), 21–28. https://doi.org/10.1089/
1675–1685. cap.2011.0096
D’Angelo, E. J., & Augenstein, T. M. (2012). Develop­ Emslie, G. J., Mayes, T., Porta, G., Vitiello, B., Clarke,
mentally informed evaluation of depression: Evidence- G., Wagner, K. D., … Kennard, B. (2010). Treatment
based instruments. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric of Resistant Depression in Adolescents (TORDIA):
Clinics of North America, 21(2), 279–298. https://doi. Week 24 outcomes. American Journal of Psychiatry,
org/10.1016/j.chc.2011.12.003 167(7), 782–791. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.
Davey, C. G., Yücel, M., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The emer- 2010.09040552
gence of depression in adolescence: Development of Emslie, G. J., Kennard, B. D., Mayes, T. L.,
the prefrontal cortex and the representation of reward. Nightingale-Teresi, J., Carmody, T., Hughes, C.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(1), 1–19. W., … Rintelmann, J. W. (2008) Fluoxetine Versus
De Cuyper, S., Timbremont, B., Braet, C., De Backer, Placebo in Preventing Relapse of Major Depression
V., & Wullaert, T. (2004). Treating depressive symp- in Children and Adolescents. American Journal
toms in school children: A pilot study. European Child of Psychiatry, 165(4):459–467.  http://dx.doi.org.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 105–114. https://doi. ezproxy.bu.edu/10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07091453
org/10.1007/s00787-004-0366-2 Eyberg, S. (1988). Parent-child interaction therapy:
DePrince, A. P., & Shirk, S. R. (2013). Adapting cognitive-­ Integration of traditional and behavioral concerns.
behavioral therapy for depressed adolescents exposed Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 10(1), 33–46.
188 G.N. Kemp et al.

Fine, S., Forth, A., Gilbert, M., & Haley, G. (1991). Review, 14(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Group therapy for adolescent depressive disor- s10567-010-0080-1
der: A comparison of social skills and therapeutic Goodyer, I., Dubicka, B., Wilkinson, P., Kelvin, R.,
support. Journal of American Academy of Child Roberts, C., Byford, S., … Harrington, R. (2007).
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 79–85. https://doi. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and
org/10.1097/00004583-199101000-00012 routine specialist care with and without cognitive
Fleming, J. E., Boyle, M. H., & Offord, D. R. (1993). The behaviour therapy in adolescents with major depres-
outcome of adolescent depression in the Ontario child sion: Randomised controlled trial. British Medical
health study follow-up. Journal of the American Academy Journal, 335, 142–146. https://doi.org/10.1136/
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(1), 28–33. https:// bmj.39224.494340.55
doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199301000-00005 Gotlib, I. H., Sivers, H., Gabrieli, J. D., Whitfield-Gabrieli,
Flory, V. (2004). A novel clinical intervention for severe S., Goldin, P., Minor, K. L., & Canli, T. (2005).
childhood depression and anxiety. Clinical Child Subgenual anterior cingulate activation to valenced
Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 9–23. emotional stimuli in major depression. Neuroreport,
Forbes, E. E. (2009). Where’s the fun in that? Broadening 16(16), 1731–1734.
the focus on reward function in depression. Biological Hamilton, J. L., Stange, J. P., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy,
Psychiatry, 66(3), 199–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. L. B. (2015). Stress and the development of cognitive
biopsych.2009.05.001 vulnerabilities to depression explain sex differences
Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2005). Neural sys- in depressive symptoms during adolescence. Clinical
tems of positive affect: Relevance to understand- Psychological Science, 3(5), 702–714. https://doi.
ing child and adolescent depression? Development org/10.1177/2167702614545479
and Psychopathology, 17(3), 827–850. https://doi. Hammen, C. (2002). Context of stress in families of
org/10.1017/S095457940505039X children with depressed parents. In S. H. Goodman
Forti-Buratti, M. A., Saikia, R., Wilkinson, E. L., & & I. H. Gotlib (Eds.), Children of depressed par-
Ramchandani, P. G. (2016). Psychological treatments ents: Mechanisms of risk and implications for treat-
for depression in preadolescent children (12 years ment (pp. 175–199). Washington, DC: American
and younger): Systematic review and meta-analysis Psychological Association.
of randomised controlled trials. European Child & Hammen, C. (2006). Stress generation in depression:
Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(10), 1045–1054. https:// Reflections on origins, research, and future directions.
doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0834-5 Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(9), 1065–1082.
Garber, J., & Flynn, C. (2001). Vulnerability to depres- https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20293
sion in childhood and adolescence. In R. E. Ingram & Hammen, C., Hazel, N. A., Brennan, P. A., & Najman,
J. M. Price (Eds.), Vulnerability to psychopathology: J. (2012). Intergenerational transmission and continu-
Risk across the lifespan (pp. 175–225). New York, NY: ity of stress and depression: Depressed women and
Guilford Press. their offspring in 20 years of follow-up. Psychological
Ghaziuddin, M., Ghaziuddin, N., & Greden, J. (2002). Medicine, 42(5), 931–942. https://doi.org/10.1017/
Depression in persons with autism: Implications for S0033291711001978
research and clinical care. Journal of Autism and Hankin, B. L., Mermelstein, R., & Roesch, L.
Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 292–306. https://doi. (2007). Sex differences in adolescent depres-
org/10.1023/A:1016330802348 sion: Stress exposure and reactivity models.
Gillham, J., Reivich, K., Jaycox, L. H., & Seligman, Child Development, 78(1), 279–295. https://doi.
M. (1995). Prevention of depressive symptoms org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x
in school children: Two year follow-up. Psycho­ Hankin, B. L., Nederhof, E., Oppenheimer, C. W., Jenness,
logical Science, 6(8), 343–351. https://doi.org/ J., Young, J. F., Abela, J. R. Z., ... Oldehinkel, A. J.
10.1016/0005-7967(94)90160-0 (2011). Differential susceptibility in youth: Evidence
Gillham, J. E., Reivich, K. J., Freres, D. R., Lascher, that 5-HTTLPR x positive parenting is associated with
M., Litzinger, S., Shatté, A., & Seligman, M. E. positive affect ‘for better and worse’. Translational
(2006). School-based prevention of depression and Psychiatry, 1(10). https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2011.44
anxiety symptoms in early adolescence: A pilot of a Hankin, B. L., Wetter, E. K., & Flory, K. (2012). Appetitive
parent intervention component. School Psychology motivation and negative emotion reactivity among
Quarterly, 21(3), 323–348. remitted depressed youth. Journal of Clinical Child
Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002). Children of & Adolescent Psychology, 41(5), 611–620. https://doi.
depressed parents: Mechanisms of risk and impli- org/10.1080/15374416.2012.710162
cations for treatment. Washington, DC: American Hankin, B. L., Young, J. F., Abela, J. R. Z., Smolen, A.,
Psychological Association. Jenness, J. L., Gulley, L. D., … Oppenheimer, C. W.
Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, (2015). Depression from childhood into late adolescence:
M. R., Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal Influence of gender, development, genetic susceptibility,
depression and child psychopathology: A meta-­ and peer stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4),
analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology 803–816. ­https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000089
Depression in Youth 189

Harter, S. (2012). The construction of the self: Develop­ Kataoka, S. H., Zhang, L., & Wells, K. B. (2002). Unmet
mental and sociocultural foundations. New York, NY: need for mental health care among US children:
Guilford Press. Variation by ethnicity and insurance status. American
Hetrick, S. E., Cox, G. R., & Merry, S. N. (2015). Where Journal of Psychiatry, 159(9), 1548–1555. https://doi.
to go from here? An exploratory meta-analysis of the org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.9.1548
most promising approaches to depression prevention Kelly, A. B., Mason, W. A., Chmelka, M. B., Herrenkohl,
programs for children and adolescents. International T. I., Kim, M. J., Patton, G. C., … Catalano, R. F.
Journal of Environmental Research and Public (2016). Depressed mood during early to middle
Health, 12(5), 4758–4795. https://doi.org/10.3390/ adolescence: A bi-national longitudinal study of the
ijerph120504758 unique impact of family conflict. Journal of Youth
Hollon, S. D., Garber, J., & Shelton, R. C. (2005). and Adolescence, 45(8), 1604–1613. https://doi.
Treatment of depression in adolescents with cognitive org/10.1007/s10964-016-0433-2
behavior therapy and medications: A commentary on Kerfoot, M., Harrington, R., Harrington, V., Rogers, J., &
the TADS project. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, Verduyn, C. (2004). A step too far? Randomized trial
12(2), 149–155. of cognitive-behaviour therapy delivered by social
Huey, S. J., Jr., & Polo, A. J. (2008). Evidence-based psy- workers to depressed adolescents. European Child
chosocial treatments for ethnic minority youth. Journal and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13(2), 92–99. https://doi.
of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37(1), 262– org/10.1007/s00787-004-0362-6
301. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410701820174 Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz,
Iwata, N., Turner, R. J., & Lloyd, D. A. (2002). Race/ D., Merikangas, K. R., … Wang, P. S. (2003).
ethnicity and depressive symptoms in community-­ The ­ epidemiology of major depressive disorder:
dwelling young adults: A differential item function- Results from the National Comorbidity Survey
ing analysis. Psychiatry Research, 110(3), 281–289. Replication (NCS-­R). Journal of the American
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00102-6 Medical Association, 289(23), 3095–3105. https://doi.
Jaycox, L. H., Reivich, K. J., Gillham, J. E., & Seligman, M. E. org/10.1001/jama.289.23.3095
P. (1994). Prevention of depressive symptoms in school- King, C. A., & Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1990). An experi-
children. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 32(8), 801– mental evaluation of a school-based program for chil-
816. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)90160-0 dren at-risk: Wisconsin early intervention. Journal
Kahn, J. S., Kehle, T. J., Jensen, W. R., & Clark, E. (1990). of Community Psychology, 18(2), 167–177. https://
Comparisons of cognitive-behavioral, relaxation, and doi.org/10.1002/1520-6629(199004)18:2<167::AID-
self modeling interventions for depression among JCOP2290180208>3.0.CO2-Z
middle school students. School Psychology Review, Klein, J. B., Jacobs, R. H., & Reinecke, M. A. (2007).
19(2), 196–211. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adolescent depres-
Kane, P., & Garber, J. (2004). The relations among sion: A meta-analytic investigation of changes in effect-­
depression in fathers, children’s psychopathology, size estimates. Journal of the American Academy of
and father–child conflict: A meta-analysis. Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(11), 1403–1413.
Psychology Review, 24(3), 339–360. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1097/chi.0b013e3180592aaa
org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.03.004 Koenig, J., Kemp, A. H., Beauchaine, T. P., Thayer, J. F.,
Kaslow, N. J., Adamson, L. B., & Collins, M. H. (2000). & Kaess, M. (2016). Depression and resting state heart
A developmental psychopathology perspective on the rate variability in children and adolescents—A sys-
cognitive components of child and adolescent depres- tematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology
sion. In Handbook of developmental psychopathol- Review, 46, 136–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.
ogy (pp. 491–510). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer 2016.04.013
Academic Publishers. http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.bu. Kovacs, M., Feinberg, T. L., Crouse-Novak, M. A.,
edu/10.1007/978-1-4615-4163-9_26 Paulauskas, S. L., Pollock, M., & Finklestein, R.
Kaslow, N. J., Baskin, M. L., Wyckoff, S. C., & Kaslow, (1984). Depressive disorders in childhood. Archives
F. W. (2002). A biopsychosocial treatment approach for of General Psychiatry, 41(3), 643–649. https://doi.
depressed children and adolescents. In Comprehensive org/10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790140019002
handbook of psychotherapy: Integrative/eclectic (Vol. Kovacs, M., Sherrill, J., George, C. J., Pollock, M.,
4, pp. 31–57). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Tumuluru, R. V., & Ho, V. (2006). Contextual
Kaslow, N. J., Jones, C. A., Palin, F., Pinsof, W. M., & emotion-­ regulation therapy for childhood depres-
Lebow, J. L. (2005). A relational perspective on sion: Description and pilot testing of a new interven-
depressed children: Family patterns and interventions. tion. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(8), 892–903. https://doi.
Kataoka, S., Novins, D. K., & DeCarlo Santiago, C. org/10.1097/01.chi.0000222878.74162.5a
(2010). The practice of evidence-based treatments Kovacs, M. Obrosky, S., & George, C. (2016). The
in ethnic minority youth. Child and Adolescent course of major depressive disorder from childhood
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 19(4), 775–789. to young adulthood: Recovery and recurrence in a
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2010.07.008 longitudinal observational study. Journal of Affective
190 G.N. Kemp et al.

Disorders, 203:374–381. ­http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy. Liu, X., Buysse, D. J., Gentzler, A. L., Kiss, E., Mayer,
bu.edu/10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.042 L., Kapornai, K., … Kovacs, M. (2007). Insomnia and
Kroll, L., Harrington, R., Jayson, D., Fraser, J., & hypersomnia associated with depressive phenomenol-
Gowers, S. (1996). Pilot study of continuation ogy and comorbidity in childhood depression. Sleep,
cognitive-­
­ behavioral therapy for major depres- 30(1), 83–90.
sion in adolescent psychiatric patients. Journal Lovato, N., & Gradisar, M. (2014). A meta-analysis and
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent model of the relationship between sleep and depression
Psychiatry, 35(9), 1156–1161. https://doi.org/ in adolescents: Recommendations for future research
10.1097/00004583-199609000-00013 and clinical practice. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 18(6),
Lakdawalla, Z., Hankin, B. L., & Mermelstein, R. (2007). 521–529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2014.03.006
Cognitive theories of depression in children and Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O’Hare, E., & Neuman,
adolescents: A conceptual and quantitative review. G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behav-
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10(1), ior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology
1–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-006-0013-1 Review, 20(5), 561–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Lenze, S. N., Pautsch, J., & Luby, J. (2011). Parent– S0272-7358(98)00100-7
child interaction therapy emotion development: A Luby, J., Lenze, S., & Tillman, R. (2012). A novel early
novel treatment for depression in preschool children. intervention for preschool depression: Findings from
Depression and Anxiety, 28(2), 153–159. https://doi. a pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child
org/10.1002/da.20770 Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(3), 313–322. https://
Lewinsohn, P. M., Clarke, F. N., Hops, H., & Andrews, doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02483.x
J. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral treatment for Maag, J. W., & Reid, R. (2006). Depression among stu-
depressed adolescents. Behavior Therapy, 21, 385– dents with learning disabilities assessing the risk.
401. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80353-3 Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(1), 3–10.
Lewinsohn, P. M., & Clarke, G. N. (1999). Psychosocial Magnuson, K. M., & Constantino, J. N. (2011).
treatments for adolescent depression. Clinical Psy­ Characterization of depression in children with autism
chology Review, 19, 329–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/ spectrum disorders. Journal of Developmental and
S0272-7358(98)00055-5 Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP, 32(4), 332–340. https://
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., Klein, D. N., & Seeley, doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e318213f56c
J. R. (1999). Natural course of adolescent depres- Manber, R., Edinger, J. D., Gress, J. L., San Pedro-Salcedo,
sion. Journal of the American Academy of Child M. G., Kuo, T. F., & Kalista, T. (2008). Cognitive
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(1), 56–63. https://doi. behavioral therapy for insomnia enhances depression
org/10.1097/00004583-199901000-00020 outcome in patients with comorbid major depressive
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., & Seely, J. R. (1998). Major disorder and insomnia. Sleep, 31(4), 489–495.
depressive disorder in older adolescents: Prevalence, Marcotte, D., & Baron, P. (1993). L’efficacite d’une
risk factors, and clinical implications. Clinical stategie d’intervention emotion-rationelle aupres
Psychology Review, 18(7), 765–794. https://doi. d’adolescents depressifs du milieu scolaire (The
org/10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00010-5 efficacy of a school-based rational-emotive interven-
Lewinsohn, P. M., Hops, H., Roberts, R. E., Seeley, J. R., & tion strategy with depressive adolescents). Canadian
Andrews, J. A. (1993). Adolescent ­psychopathology: Journal of Counseling, 27, 77–92.
I. Prevalence and incidence of depression and Mark Cummings, E., Keller, P. S., & Davies, P. T. (2005).
other DSM-III—R disorders in high school stu- Towards a family process model of maternal and
dents.  Journal Of Abnormal Psychology, 102(1), paternal depressive symptoms: Exploring multiple
133–144. http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.bu.edu/10.1037/ relations with child and family functioning. Journal
0021-843X.102.1.133 of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(5), 479–489.
Liddle, B., & Spence, S. H. (1990). Cognitive-behaviour https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00368.x
therapy with depressed primary school children: A Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. S. (2007). Comorbid
cautionary note. Behavioral Psychotherapy, 18(2), psychopathology with autism spectrum disorder in
85–102. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0141347300018218 children: An overview. Research in Developmental
Lindsey, M. A., Chambers, K., Pohle, C., Beall, P., & Disabilities, 28(4), 341–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Lucksted, A. (2013). Understanding the behavioral ridd.2005.12.004
determinants of mental health service use by urban, Mayes, T. L., Tao, R., Rintelmann, J. W., Carmody, T.,
under-resourced Black youth: Adolescent and care- Hughes, C. W., Kennard, B. D., … Emslie, G. J.
giver perspectives. Journal of Child and Family (2007). Do children and adolescents have differential
Studies, 22(1), 107–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/ response rates in placebo-controlled trials of fluox-
s10826-012-9668-z etine? CNS Spectrums, 12(2), 147–154.
Liu, R. T., & Alloy, L. B. (2010). Stress generation McLaughlin, K. A., Hilt, L. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S.
in depression: A systematic review of the empiri- (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in internalizing and
cal literature and recommendations for future study. externalizing symptoms in adolescents. Journal of
Clinical Psychology Review, 30(5), 582–593. https:// Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(5), 801–816. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.010 doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9128-1
Depression in Youth 191

Melvin, G. A., Dudley, A. L., Gordon, M. S., Ford, S., Psychiatry, 55(1), 56–64. https://doi.org/10.1001/
Taffe, J., & Tonge, B. J. (2013). What happens to archpsyc.55.1.56
depressed adolescents? A follow-up study into early Poppelaars, M., Tak, Y. R., Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A.,
adulthood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151(1), Engels, R. C. M. E., Lobel, A., Merry, S., … Granic,
298–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.012 I. (2016). A randomized controlled trial comparing
Melvin, G. A., Tonge, B. J., King, N. J., Heyne, D., two cognitive-behavioral programs for adolescent
Gordon, M. S., & Klimkeit, E. (2006). A comparison girls with subclinical depression: A school-based
of cognitive-behavioral therapy, sertraline, and their program (Op Volle Kracht) and a computerized pro-
combination for adolescent depression. Journal of the gram (SPARX). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 80,
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.03.005
45(10), 1151–1161. https://doi.org/10.1097/01. Puig-Antich, J., Kaufman, J., Ryan, N. D., Williamson,
chi.0000233157.21925.71 D. E., Dahl, R. E., Lukens, E., … Nelson, B. (1993). The
Milan, S., Snow, S., & Belay, S. (2009). Depressive symp- psychosocial functioning and family environment of
toms in mothers and children: Preschool attachment depressed adolescents. Journal of the American Academy
as a moderator of risk. Developmental Psychology, of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(2), 244–253.
45(4), 1019–1033. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016164 https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199303000-00003
Miranda, J., Bernal, G., Lau, A., Kohn, L., Hwang, W. C., Puig-Antich, J., Lukens, E., Davies, M., Goetz, D.,
& LaFromboise, T. (2005). State of the science on psy- Brennan-Quattrock, J., & Todak, G. (1985a). Psy­
chosocial interventions for ethnic minorities. Annual chosocial functioning in prepubertal major depressive
Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 113–142. https:// disorders. I. Interpersonal relationships during the
doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143822 depressive episode. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Mufson, L., Dorta, K. P., Wickramaratne, P., Nomura, Y., 42, 500–507.
Olfson, M., & Weissman, M. M. (2004). A random- Puig-Antich, J., Lukens, E., Davies, M., Goetz, J.,
ized effectiveness trial of interpersonal psychother- Brennan-Quattrock, J., & Todak, G. (1985b).
apy for depressed adolescents. Archives of General Psychosocial functioning in prepubertal depressive
Psychiatry, 61(6), 577–584. https://doi.org/10.1001/ disorders. II. Interpersonal relationships after sus-
archpsyc.61.6.577 tained recovery from affective episode. Archives of
Mufson, L., Weissman, M. M., Moreau, D., & Garfinkel, General Psychiatry, 42, 511–517.
R. (1999). Efficacy of interpersonal psychotherapy for Rabinowitz, J. A., Drabick, D. A., & Reynolds, M. D.
depressed adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, (2016). Family conflict moderates the relation
56(6), 573–579. https://doi.org/10-1001/pubs.Arch between negative mood and youth internalizing and
Gen Psychiatry-ISSN-0003-990x-56-6-yoa8163 externalizing symptoms. Journal of Child and Family
Murray, L., Arteche, A., Fearon, P., Halligan, S., Goodyer, Studies, 25(12), 3574–3583. https://doi.org/10.1007/
I., & Cooper, P. (2011). Maternal postnatal depression s10826-016-0501-y
and the development of depression in offspring up to Reed, M. K. (1994). Social skills training to reduce
16 years of age. Journal of the American Academy depression in adolescents. Adolescence, 29, 293–302.
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(5), 460–470. Reinecke, M. A., Ryan, N. E., & DuBois, D. L. (1998).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.02.001 Cognitive-behavioral therapy of depression and
O’Shea, G., Spence, S. H., & Donovan, C. L. (2015). depressive symptoms during adolescence: A review
Group versus individual interpersonal psychotherapy and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy
for depressed adolescents. Behavioural and Cognitive of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(1), 26–34.
Psychotherapy, 43(01), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/ https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199801000-00013
S1352465814000216 Reynolds, W. M., & Coats, K. I. (1986). A comparison
Okamura, K. H., Ebesutani, C., Bloom, R., Higa-­ of cognitive-behavioral therapy and relaxation training
McMillan, C. K., Nakamura, B. J., & Chorpita, B. F. for the treatment of depression in adolescents. Journal
(2016). Differences in internalizing symptoms across of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(5), 653–
specific ethnic minority groups: An analysis across 660. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.54.5.653
Chinese American, Filipino American, Japanese Rice, F., Harold, G. T., Shelton, K. H., & Thapar, A.
American, native Hawaiian, and white youth. Journal (2006). Family conflict interacts with genetic liabil-
of Child and Family Studies, 1–14. https://doi. ity in predicting childhood and adolescent depres-
org/10.1007/s10826-016-0488-4 sion. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Pincus, D. B., Eyberg, S. M., & Choate, M. L. (2005). Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(7), 841–848. https://doi.
Adapting parent-child interaction therapy for young org/10.1097/01.chi.0000219834.08602.44
children with separation anxiety disorder. Education Richardson, L. P., Ludman, E., McCauley, E.,
and Treatment of Children, 28(2), 163–181. Lindenbaum, J., Larison, C., Zhou, C., … Katon,
Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., Gurley, D., Brook, J., & Ma, W. (2014). Collaborative care for adolescents with
Y. (1998). The risk for early-adulthood anxiety depression in primary care: A randomized clinical
and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxi- trial. JAMA, 312(8), 809–816. https://doi.org/10.1001/
ety and depressive disorders. Archives of General jama.2014.9259
192 G.N. Kemp et al.

Roane, B. M., & Taylor, D. J. (2008). Adolescent insom- of General Psychiatry, 53(4), 339–348. https://doi.
nia as a risk factor for early adult depression and sub- org/10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830040075012
stance abuse. Sleep, 31(10), 1351–1356. Sheeber, L., Hops, H., & Davis, B. (2001). Family pro-
Roberts, R. E., & Duong, H. T. (2013). Depression and cesses in adolescent depression. Clinical Child and
insomnia among adolescents: A prospective perspec- Family Psychology Review, 4(1), 19–35. https://doi.
tive. Journal of Affective Disorders, 148(1), 66–71. org/10.1023/A:1009524626436
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.049 Sheeber, L. B., Allen, N. B., Leve, C., Davis, B., Shortt,
Rodríguez-Naranjo, C., & Caño, A. (2016). Daily stress J. W., & Katz, L. F. (2009). Dynamics of affective experi-
and coping styles in adolescent hopelessness depres- ence and behavior in depressed adolescents. Journal of
sion: Moderating effects of gender. Personality and Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(11), 1419–1427.
Individual Differences, 97, 109–114. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02148.x
org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.027 Sheeber, L. B., Davis, B., Leve, C., Hops, H., & Tildesley,
Rohde, P., Clarke, G. N., Mace, D. E., Jorgensen, J. S., E. (2007). Adolescents’ relationships with their moth-
& Seeley, J. R. (2004). An efficacy/effectiveness study ers and fathers: Associations with depressive disor-
of cognitive-behavioral treatment for adolescents with der and subdiagnostic symptomatology. Journal of
and without comorbid major depression and conduct Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 144–154. https://doi.
disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.144
& Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(6), 660–668. https://doi. Shih, J. H., Eberhart, N. K., Hammen, C. L., & Brennan,
org/10.1097/01.chi.0000121067.29744.41 P. A. (2006). Differential exposure and reactivity to
Rose, A. J., Glick, G. C., Smith, R. L., Schwartz-Mette, interpersonal stress predict sex differences in ado-
R. A., & Borowski, S. K. (2016). Co-rumination lescent depression. Journal of Clinical Child and
exacerbates stress generation among adolescents Adolescent Psychology, 35(1), 103–115. https://doi.
with depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3501_9
Child Psychology, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Shirk, S. R., DePrince, A. P., Crisostomo, P. S., & Labus,
s10802-016-0205-1 J. (2014). Cognitive behavioral therapy for depressed
Rose, T., Joe, S., & Lindsey, M. (2011). Perceived adolescents exposed to interpersonal trauma: An
stigma and depression among black adolescents in ­initial effectiveness trial. Psychotherapy, 51(1), 167–
outpatient treatment. Children and Youth Services 179. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034845
Review, 33(1), 161–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Silk, J. S., Davis, S., McMakin, D. L., Dahl, R. E.,
childyouth.2010.08.029 & Forbes, E. E. (2012). Why do anxious children
Rosselló, J., & Bernal, G. (1999). The efficacy of become depressed teenagers? The role of social evalu-
cognitive-­behavioral and interpersonal treatments for ative threat and reward processing. Psychological
depression in Puerto Rican adolescents. Journal of Medicine, 42(10), 2095–2107. https://doi.org/10.1017/
Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 67(5), 734–745. S0033291712000207
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006X.67.5.734 Smith, P., Scott, R., Eshkevari, E., Jatta, F., Leigh, E.,
Rosselló, J., Bernal, G., & Rivera, C. (2008). Randomized Harris, V., … Yule, W. (2015). Computerised CBT
trial of CBT and IPT in individual and group format for depressed adolescents: Randomised controlled
for depression in Puerto Rican adolescents. Cultural trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 73, 104–110.
Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.07.009
234–245. Stark, K. D. (1990). Childhood depression: School-based
Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., Burge, D., Lindberg, N., intervention. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Herzberg, D., & Daley, S. E. (2000). Toward an inter- Stark, K. D., Hoke, J., Ballatore, M., Valdez, C.,
personal life-stress model of depression: The develop- Scammaca, N., & Green, J. (2005). Treatment of child
mental context of stress generation. Development and and adolescent disorders. In Psychosocial treatments
Psychopathology, 12(2), 215–234. for child and adolescent disorders (2nd ed., pp. 239–
Saleh, A., Potter, G. G., McQuoid, D. R., Boyd, B., 265). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Turner, R., MacFall, J. R., & Taylor, W. D. (2017). Association.
Effects of early life stress on depression, cognitive Stark, K. D., Reynolds, W. M., & Kaslow, N. J. (1987). A
performance and brain morphology. Psychological comparison of the relative efficacy self-control therapy
Medicine, 47(1), 171–181. https://doi.org/10.1017/ and a behavioral problem-solving therapy for depres-
S0033291716002403 sion in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Saulnier, C., & Volkmar, F. (2007). Mental health prob- 15, 91–113. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00916468
lems in people with autism and related disorders. In Stark, K. D., Sander, J., & Hauser, M. (2006). Depressive
N. Bouras & G. Holt (Eds.), Psychiatric and behav- disorders during childhood and adolescence. In
ioural disorders in intellectual and developmental Treatment of childhood disorders (3rd ed., pp. 336–
disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 215–224). New York, NY: 407). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Cambridge University Press. Strang, J. F., Kenworthy, L., Daniolos, P., Case, L.,
Shaffer, D., Gould, M. S., Fisher, P., Trautman, P., Moreau, Wills, M. C., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2012).
D., Kleinman, M., & Flory, M. (1996). Psychiatric Depression and anxiety symptoms in children and
diagnosis in child and adolescent suicide. Archives adolescents with autism spectrum disorders without
Depression in Youth 193

intellectual disability. Research in Autism Spectrum Vostanis, P., Feehan, C., Grattan, E., & Bickerton, W.
Disorders, 6(1), 406–412. ­https://doi.org/10.1016/j. (1996). A randomized controlled out-patient trial of
rasd.2011.06.015 cognitive-behavioural treatment for children and ado-
Straub, J., Plener, P. L., Sproeber, N., Sprenger, L., lescents with depression: 9-month follow-up. Journal
Koelch, M. G., Groen, G., & Abler, B. (2015). Neural of Affective Disorders, 40(1–2), 105–116. https://doi.
correlates of successful psychotherapy of depression org/10.1016/0165-0327(96)00054-7
in adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 183(1), Webb, C. A., Auerbach, R. P., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2012).
239–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.020 Processes of change in CBT of adolescent depression:
Tao, R., Calley, C. S., Hart, J., Mayes, T. L., Nakonezny, Review and recommendations. Journal of Clinical
P. A., Lu, H., … Emslie, G. J. (2012). Brain activ- Child & Adolescent Psychology, 41(5), 654–665.
ity in adolescent major depressive disorder before https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2012.704842
and after fluoxetine treatment. American Journal of Weersing, V. R., Rozenman, M., Gonzalez, A., Jeffreys,
Psychiatry, 169(4), 381–388. https://doi.org/10.1176/ M., Porta, G., & Brent, D. A. (2016). 48.5 Brief behav-
appi.ajp.2011.11040615 ioral therapy for pediatric anxiety and depression in pri-
Tompson, M. C., Asarnow, J. R., Mintz, J., & Cantwell, mary care: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of the
D. P. (2015). Parental depression risk: Comparing American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
youth with depression, attention deficit hyperactivity 55(10). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.07.406
disorder and community controls. Psychology and Weersing, V. R., & Weisz, J. R. (2002). Community clinic
Psychotherapy, 5(4), 193–201. treatment of depressed youth: Benchmarking usual
Tompson, M. C., Boger, K. D., & Asarnow, J. R. (2012). care against CBT clinical trials. Journal of Consulting
Enhancing the developmental appropriateness of & Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 299–310. https://doi.
treatment for depression in youth: Integrating the fam- org/10.1037//0022-006X.70.2.299
ily in treatment. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Weiss, B., & Garber, J. (2003). Developmental differences
Clinics of North America, 21(2), 345–384. https://doi. in the phenomenology of depression. Development
org/10.1016/j.chc.2012.01.003 and Psychopathology, 15(2), 403–430. https://doi.
Tompson, M. C., Pierre, C. B., McNeil Haber, F., Fogler, org/10.1017/S0954579403000221
J. M., Groff, A., & Asarnow, J. R. (2007). Family-­ Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Gameroff, M. J.,
focused treatment for childhood-onset depressive Warner, V., Pilowsky, D., Kohad, R. G., … Talati, A.
disorders: Results of an open trial. Clinical Child (2016). Offspring of depressed parents: 30 years later.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 12(3), 403–420. https:// American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(6), 1001–1008.
doi.org/10.1177/1359104507078474 https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15101327
Tompson, M. C., Langer, D. A., Hughes, J. L., & Asarnow, Weissman, M. M., Wolk, S., Wickramaratne, P., Goldstein,
J. R. (2017a). Treatment of preadolescent depres- R. B., Adams, P., Greenwald, S., … Steinberg,
sion: Model and case illustrations. Cognitive and D. (1999). Children with prepubertal-onset major
Behavioral Practice, 24, 269–287. depressive disorder and anxiety grown up. Archives
Tompson, M. C., Sugar, C., Langer, D. A., & Asarnow, of General Psychiatry, 56(9), 794–801. https://doi.
J. R. (2017b). A randomized clinical trial comparing org/10.1001/archpsyc.56.9.794
family focused treatment and individual supportive Weisz, J. R., Jensen Doss, A. J., & Hawley, K. M. (2005).
therapy for depression in childhood and early adoles- Youth psychotherapy outcome research: A review
cence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and and critique of the evidence base. Annual Review of
Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(6), 515–523. https://doi. Psychology, 56, 337–363. https://doi.org/10.1146/
org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.018 annurev.psych.55.090902.141449
Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study Weisz, J. R., McCarty, C. A., & Valeri, S. M. (2006).
(TADS) Team. (2004). Fluoxetine, cognitive-­ Effects of psychotherapy for depression in chil-
behavioral therapy, and their combination for ado- dren and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Psycho­
lescents with depression. Journal of the American logical Bulletin, 132(1), 132–149. https://doi.org/
Medical Association, 292(7), 807–820. https://doi. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.132
org/10.1001/jama.292.7.807 Weisz, J. R., Thurber, C., Sweeney, L., Proffitt, V., &
United States Food and Drug Administration. (2004). LeGagnoux, G. (1997). Brief treatment of mild to
Labeling change request letter for antidepressant moderate child depression using primary and sec-
medications. Accessed 30 Nov 2004 [On-line]. ondary controlled enhancement training. Journal of
Available: http://www.fda.gov/cder/drug/antidepres- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(4), 703–707.
sants/SSRIlabelChange.htm https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.65.4.703
Vitiello, B., Emslie, G., Clarke, G., Wagner, K. D., Weisz, J. R., Weiss, B., Han, S. S., Granger, D. A., & Morton,
Asarnow, J. R., Keller, M. B., … Mayes, T. L. (2010). T. (1995). Effects of psychotherapy with children and
Long-term outcome of adolescent depression initially adolescents revisited: A meta-analysis of treatment
resistant to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treat- outcome studies. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 450–
ment: A follow-up study of the TORDIA sample. 468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.450
The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72(3), 388–396. Wilson, S., & Durbin, C. E. (2010). Effects of pater-
https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09m05885blu nal depression on fathers’ parenting behaviors:
194 G.N. Kemp et al.

A ­meta-­analytic review. Clinical Psychology analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175(1), 424–
Review, 30(2), 167–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.050
cpr.2009.10.007 Young, C. C., & Dietrich, M. S. (2015). Stressful life
Wood, A., Harrington, R., & Moore, A. (1996). Controlled events, worry, and rumination predict depressive and
trial of a brief cognitive-behavioural intervention in anxiety symptoms in young adolescents. Journal of
adolescent patients with depressive disorders. Journal Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 28(1),
of Child Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37(6), 35–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcap.12102
737–746. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996. Young, J. F., Mufson, L., & Davies, M. (2006).
tb01466.x Efficacy of interpersonal psychotherapy-adolescent
Yang, T. T., Simmons, A. N., Matthews, S. C., Tapert, S. F., skills training: An indicated preventive interven-
Frank, G. K., Max, J. E., … Wu, J. (2010). Adolescents tion for depression. Journal of Child Psychology
with major depression demonstrate increased amyg- and Psychiatry, 47(12), 1254–1262. https://doi.
dala activation. Journal of the American Academy of org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01667.x
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(1), 42–51. https:// Young, J. F., Mufson, L., & Gallop, R. (2010). Preventing
doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2009.09.004 depression: A randomized trial of interpersonal psy-
Yap, M. B. H., & Jorm, A. F. (2015). Parental factors chotherapy and adolescent skills training. Depression
associated with childhood anxiety, depression, and and Anxiety, 27(5), 426–433. https://doi.org/10.1002/
internalizing problems: A systematic review and meta-­ da.20664
Treating Bipolar Disorders

Johnny L. Matson and Claire O. Burns

Contents and Bipolar II fall under this category as do


Bipolar Disorder Defined......................................  195 cyclothymic disorder, substance-/medication-­
induced bipolar disorder, bipolar and related dis-
Prevalence...............................................................  196
orders due to another medical condition, other
Risk Factors............................................................  196 specified bipolar and related disorders, and
Service Models........................................................  198 unspecified bipolar and related disorder. For the
Medications and Supplements..............................  199
purposes of this chapter, our discussion will be
confined to Bipolar I and Bipolar II disorder. As
Antipsychotics........................................................  199
the reader may be aware, a Bipolar I diagnosis
Other Medications.................................................  200 must meet criteria for a manic episode which
Supplements...........................................................  201 consists of elevated, expansive, or irritable mood
for at least a week and for most of the day every
Psychological Treatments......................................  202
day. Before or after the manic episode, a hypo-
Conclusions.............................................................  203 manic or major episode should be present.
References...............................................................  204 Symptom presentation for a hypomanic episode
is similar to a manic episode, but of shorter dura-
tion (i.e., 4 days). A major depressive episode
must occur for at least 2 weeks, but the symptoms
vary markedly from hypomanic and manic
Bipolar Disorder Defined behaviors. Diminished interest or pleasure in
activities, less energy, insomnia, feelings of use-
Bipolar disorder and related disorders, as they are lessness or being slowed down, fatigue, feelings
referenced in DSM-5  (American Psychiatric of worthlessness, diminished ability to concen-
Association [APA], 2013), are severe forms of trate, and suicidal ideation are among the symp-
psychopathology that are distinct from schizo- toms that are present.
phrenia and other forms of psychosis. Bipolar I Bipolar II differs in that no manic episode has
ever been present. The person will have experi-
enced at least one hypomanic episode and one
J.L. Matson • C.O. Burns (*)
major depressive episode. Symptoms for the
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA hypomanic and major depressive episodes will
e-mail: cburn26@lsu.edu mirror the symptoms noted in Bipolar I. For this

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 195


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_11
196 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

chapter, bipolar disorder will be used, and no dis- ity, euphoric/elevated mood, pressured speech,
tinction between Bipolar I and II will be noted. hyperactivity, racing thoughts, poor judgment,
This approach is applicable since interpretation grandiosity, inappropriate laughter, decreased
strategies do not vary markedly between Bipolar need for sleep, and flight of ideas. In our view,
I and Bipolar II. Similarly, these distinctions are this broad list of symptoms, many of which do
not evident in much of the published treatment not map precisely onto the DSM-5 diagnostic cri-
literature. terion for bipolar disorder, may at least in part
account for this marked increase in children diag-
nosed with the condition. This approach may also
Prevalence account for the views of some practitioners that
pediatric bipolar disorder is overdiagnosed.
For many years, bipolar disorder was considered Vedel Kessing, Vradi, and Andersen (2015)
to be largely present exclusively among adults. underscore another factor that may add to the
However, more recently, this thinking has increased prevalence of pediatric bipolar disor-
changed, with greater and greater numbers of der: long-term involvement with the mental
children being identified for treatment. health system. They used ICD-10 and reported
Additionally, age of onset is becoming earlier on children and adolescents below age 19 who
and earlier, with children as young as 4 years of received a bipolar diagnosis in Denmark. Of the
age being diagnosed. Also, age of onset, preva- 354 individuals they studied who had received a
lence, and comorbid conditions are greater diagnosis of bipolar disorder, 144 were given the
among children in the USA versus Europe. Post diagnosis at first contact. For the remainder,
and colleagues (2016) compared offspring of 132 about 1 year of treatment occurred before the
parents and their children from Germany and the diagnosis was given. Why a subgroup of individ-
Netherlands to 356 parents from the USA. The uals did not receive a diagnosis early on is not
children of the US adults were more likely to known but warrants further study.
express depression, bipolar disorder, drug abuse, Whatever the reason may be, researchers are
and “other illnesses” than their European aware that pediatric bipolar disorder has become
counterparts. one of the more commonly diagnosed childhood
Stebbins and Corcoran (2016) have also conditions. Obviously, these increased rates have
addressed what they characterized as the “con- important implications for treatment.
siderable rise in pediatric bipolar disorder.” They Additionally, much of this intervention is geared
interviewed ten child psychiatrists regarding this toward psychotropic medications.
issue. The professionals were of the opinion that
bipolar disorder in children was often misdiag-
nosed and/or overdiagnosed. The need for more Risk Factors
and better diagnostic methods and more unifor-
mity in diagnostic practices may also be a con- Prevention and early intervention are important
tributing factor. Having said that, much more for all mental health issues. However, this topic is
research on this topic is warranted. no more critical than for children who may be
As noted, the diagnosis of bipolar disorder in experiencing severe psychopathology. These def-
children is controversial (Van Meter, Burke, icits markedly impede normal development and
Kowatch, Findling, & Youngstrom, 2016). In can have a lifetime impact on adjustment across
their paper, the authors reviewed 20 studies on social contact, work, family relationships, and a
bipolar disorder in youth (N = 2226). They noted host of other factors.
that symptoms used for diagnosis varied widely Betts, Williams, Naiman, and Alati (2016)
across studies. The most commonly reported conducted a longitudinal study with children who
symptoms include increased energy, irritability, were assessed for a range of neurodevelopmental
mood lability, distractibility, goal-directed activ- factors at age 5. At age 21 these same people
Treating Bipolar Disorders 197

were evaluated for symptoms of mental health severe, psychopathology. Of course, where
problems. Developmental delay predicted manic parental bipolar disorder is present, a chicken and
symptoms, while behavior problems predicted egg analogy will most likely be present with
depression symptoms as well as behavioral respect to the extent of genetics versus parenting
descriptors of psychosis. More research of this which may influence the manifestation of bipolar
type is needed. Identifying reliable risk factors disorder. However, it is known that increased
can be very useful for establishing prevention duration of exposure to a parent with bipolar dis-
programs. order increases the risks of bipolar disorder for
Biederman and colleagues (2009) report that a the offspring (Doucette, Horrocks, Grof, Keown-­
diagnosis of bipolar disorder in mothers was a Stoneman, & Duffy, 2013). Thus, it is possible
strong predictor of bipolar disorders in offspring. that genetic factors are embellished when long-­
These data are underscored by Hunt, Schwartz, term environmental stressors are also present.
Nye, and Frazier (2016) who emphasize the Benarous, Mikita, Goodman, and Stringaris
importance of addressing offspring of bipolar (2015) looked at a different set of factors that
parents as a “high-risk cohort.” Insel (2012) may be associated with pediatric bipolar disor-
asserts that this phenomenon may result in clini- der, social aptitude (skills). This factor is an
cal symptoms being present years before a for- important consideration in the evaluation and
mal diagnosis is made. However, the study of this treatment of pediatric bipolar disorder, yet it has
topic is in its infancy at present. rarely been studied to date. These authors mea-
These authors argue for the need to identify sured skills that involved identifying social and
symptoms likely to lead to pediatric bipolar dis- emotional cues in everyday social interactions.
order as early as possible so that early interven- Specific behaviors assessed included being easy
tion may be initiated. As noted however, these to talk to, being able to read between the lines,
symptoms early on may be nonspecific to bipolar and judging emotions. As might be expected,
disorder. Therefore, these nonspecific behaviors children with parent reported manic symptoms
do not always lead later on to a diagnosable bipo- scored lower on social skills than the general
lar disorder using standardized tests and diagnos- population. These data are important, since
tic symptoms. For example, children with anxiety teaching social coping skills would seem to be an
and sleep problems have a significant likelihood important element of a comprehensive treatment
of developing serious emotional disorders. plan for pediatric bipolar disorder.
However, in addition to bipolar disorder, major Selten, Lundberg, Rai, and Magnusson (2015)
depression and schizoaffective disorder are also examined a very large group of children and ado-
implicated (Duffy, Aldo, Hajek, Sherry, & Grof, lescents under the age of 17 diagnosed with
2010; Ritter et al., 2012). autism spectrum disorder (N = 9062). The intent
Duffy, Jones, Goodday, and Bentall (2015) was to determine the rate at which bipolar disor-
assert that pediatric bipolar disorders are likely der occurred in this group. These authors note
caused by a combination of genetic, environmen- that children with autism spectrum disorder are at
tal, and epigenetic variables. This theory is a greater risk compared to the general population
good bet since current thinking on a broad range for the development of bipolar disorder. They
of neurodevelopmental disorders asserts similar also theorize that some genetic overlap exists
broad risk factors. Also, an important takeaway between these conditions, but their study did not
from current causative models is that disorders confirm this assumption.
develop gradually over time until a diagnostic Another study looking at the potential comor-
threshold is reached versus rapid onset. This bidity of bipolar disorder compared it to ADHD
model, which again applies to a wide range of in a nationwide study conducted in Taiwan. A
neurodevelopmental conditions (e.g., autism, total of 144,920 children diagnosed with ADHD
ADHD), has led investigators to hunt for these were studied. Wong et al. (2016) found that chil-
more subtle early warning signs of later, more dren with ADHD when compared to the general
198 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

population were seven times more likely to require treatment is essential. Various service
develop bipolar disorder. Also, age of first diag- models on hard to treat pediatric bipolar disor-
nosis of bipolar disorder was earlier for the ders as proposed by researchers follow.
ADHD group. Children treated with methylphe-
nidate were less likely than others with an ADHD
diagnosis to receive a bipolar diagnosis. Service Models
Researchers have attempted to link immuno-
logical problems to bipolar as well. Chen and Fristad and colleagues (2012) describe an outpa-
colleagues (2015) employed an insurance tient program to screen for manic symptoms.
research database of 9506 children and adoles- They evaluated 707 6–12-year-olds using the
cents 12–18 years of age. These participants, Parent General Behavior Inventory. This scale
who were from Taiwan, all had been diagnosed was a ten-item mania scale. In addition to this
with allergic rhinitis. These individuals were sig- measure, parent history of mania and parental
nificantly more likely to develop bipolar disorder stress were also evaluated. They particularly
compared to controls. Of course, most of these underscore the need for a careful, detailed assess-
studies are correlational at this point, so specific ment since most of the risk factors they addressed
causality is unknown. were not particularly accurate for predicting
Duffy et al. (2015) provide an excellent review mania.
of multiple risk factors for bipolar disorder in Potter and colleagues (2009) also discuss the
children. They make several general conclusions. treatment of pediatric bipolar disorder in a spe-
Increased risk of psychopathology and early cialty outpatient clinic. They evaluated 53 chil-
exposure to stressors such as parental illness and dren using the Clinical Global Impression Scales
neglect from the child’s mothers were associated (Severity and Improvement). The average num-
with bipolar disorder. Other markers of vulnera- ber of medications per person was three. The
bility include reward sensitivity, disturbances in authors attribute this phenomenon to the fact that
circadian rhythm, unstable self-esteem, immune 68% of the children diagnosed with pediatric
dysfunction, rumination, and positive bipolar disorder had at least one comorbid mental
self-appraisal. health condition. They underscore the need to
Environmental risk factors have also been employ psychological interventions as a method
hypothesized as possible factors associated with to minimize total drug use. Similar drug use pat-
juvenile bipolar disorder. Marangoni, Hernandez, terns and recommendations for psychosocial
and Faedda (2016) suggested that environmental treatment for pediatric bipolar disorder were
risk factors could be grouped into three catego- noted by Vande Voort and colleagues (2016).
ries: neurodevelopmental, encompassing having However, in the 85 youth with bipolar disorder, a
the flu while pregnant and/or fetal development; cautionary note was added. They underscore that
substance abuse of alcohol, cannabis, cocaine, for both medication and psychosocial treatments,
opioids, stimulants, sedative, or tranquilizers; visits to the clinic were only once every 2 months.
and psychological/physiological stressors such This may be associated with the fact that almost
as parental loss, abuse, and brain injury. All of half of the sample experienced relapses.
these variables were associated with pediatric Benarous, Consoli, Milhiet, and Cohen (2016)
bipolar disorder. emphasize the need for early intervention pro-
These studies demonstrate robust initial efforts grams for children at high risk for bipolar disor-
to establish factors associated with pediatric der. They note that the lack of specific diagnostic
bipolar disorder. Risk factors are helpful for early markers does make it difficult to identify pre-
identification of the condition. In other cases, the teens. Additionally, for young children, there do
ability to identify and track persons likely to be at not appear to be well-delineated intervention
risk for developing pediatric bipolar disorder or a packages for these children. Researchers do
group of subclinical symptoms that may also know that medications, particularly lithium and
Treating Bipolar Disorders 199

antipsychotic drugs, along with “supportive” par- cebo for treating bipolar mood disorder. However,
ent and child therapy are treatments which are there are several reservations to the current
emphasized. Other components of intervention research. Many of the studies are open-label
such as social skills, adaptive skills, and coping (noncontrolled case studies). We do not believe
skills should also be considered. studies of this type provide adequate method-
ological controls to consider the results valid.
Lithium has a narrow therapeutic window, and
Medications and Supplements toxicity is always an issue. This latter point is
particularly critical with children where monitor-
Singh, Ketter, and Chang (2010) stress that medi- ing may be more difficult and where the child
cation in combination with psychologically based may have difficulty in accurately self-reporting
therapies is essential in the treatment of chil- side effects. This problem is underscored by
dren’s bipolar disorder. They recommend these Landersdorfer, Findling, Frazier, Kafantaris, and
treatments for acute mania or mixed symptoms. Kirkpatrick (2016) who point out there is a dearth
These authors stress that this approach is useful of information on lithium dosage levels for chil-
for stabilizing symptoms. dren. Given the substantial research database
Lithium has the best track record for stabiliz- using lithium to treat bipolar disorder in adults,
ing symptoms of bipolar disorder for adults. This further study is warranted. However, given the
medication has also been used with childhood current database, clinicians should proceed with
populations. Findling, Kafantaris, and colleagues caution when addressing the pediatric bipolar
(2013) treated 41 outpatients between 7 and population.
17 years of age. Additionally, for 25 of these indi-
viduals, antipsychotic drugs were used as an add-
­on therapy. Dependent variables consisted of the Antipsychotics
Young Mania Rating Scale and the Clinical
Global Impression-Improvement. They note that Antipsychotic medications, and particularly the
children who initially responded to lithium ther- atypical antipsychotics, have been used in many
apy for acute manic symptoms also found the studies of pediatric bipolar disorder. For exam-
drug to be effective for maintenance. Side effects ple, in one chart review study of nine, 14–17-year-­
noted were vomiting, headache, abdominal pain, olds diagnosed with bipolar disorder were
and tremor. prescribed paliperidone, palmitate, risperidone,
Another study using lithium and add-on medi- fluphenazine, or aripiprazole (Pope & Zaraa,
cations to treat pediatric bipolar disorder is 2016). Similarly, in a review of existing studies,
described by Geller and colleagues (2012). They Doey (2012) reported that there were 140 book
treated 279 children, 6–15 years of age for ele- chapters and articles describing aripiprazole to
vated mood and ideas of grandiosity. All of the treat schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or psycho-
children displayed these symptoms, while almost sis in children and adolescents. Only seven of
80% of the population evinced symptoms of psy- these papers were double-blind controlled studies
chosis. Over 90% of the sample also shared with the bulk of the papers being chart reviews,
mixed mania and daily rapid cycling. The drug case studies, or meta-analyses. Given the potency
treatment was lithium, risperidone, or divalproex of these medications and the vulnerability of
sodium. Children treated with risperidone had a pediatric populations, researchers in the field
better response than persons receiving lithium or need to do better. A much more extensive data-
divalproex sodium. There was no difference in base using rigorous experimental designs is
treatment response for the latter two drugs. needed.
In a review of lithium treatment for children, Youngstrom and colleagues (2013) tested
Naguy (2016) reviewed several studies that dem- aripiprazole and discussed the need to have
onstrate the superiority of this drug over a pla- appropriate, reliable, and valid methods of evalu-
200 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

ating treatment effectiveness for children and Risperidone was compared to divalproex.
adolescents with Bipolar I disorder. Among the Risperidone was the more effective drug for
measures they used were the Clinical Global decreasing bipolar symptoms, although 24% of
Impression-Bipolar Disorder, the Children’s the sample dropped out before the study was
Global Assessment Scale, and the Young Mania completed.
Rating Scale. The Young Mania Rating Scale Haas and colleagues (2009) also tested risper-
was the most sensitive measure of the three with idone for 169 10–17-year-olds with a DSM-IV
respect to monitoring treatment effects in this diagnosis of Bipolar I. Of their sample, 58
study. received a placebo while 50 received doses of ris-
Findling, Correll, and colleagues (2013b) peridone at 0.05–2.5 mg per day, and a third
reported a double-blind, 30-week study for chil- group (n = 61) received at 3–6 mg per day. The
dren and adolescents 10–17 years of age. The two risperidone groups produced significantly
participants (N = 296) were diagnosed with lower scores on the Young Mania Rating Scale
Bipolar I disorder with or without psychotic fea- compared to the placebo group.
tures. Two hundred and ten of these individuals Other new-generation antipsychotics have
completed the aripiprazole treatment. The authors been employed to treat pediatric bipolar disorder.
report that medication was superior to placebo Detke, DelBello, Landry, and Usher (2015)
for decreasing symptoms measured on the Young treated 10–17-year-olds with olanzapine/fluox-
Mania Rating Scale, the Global Assessment of etine (n = 170) or a placebo (n = 85). The active
Functioning Scale, and the Clinical Global drug group produced significantly greater reduc-
Impression-Bipolar scale. In a review of studies, tions in symptoms on the Children’s Depression
using aripiprazole as a treatment for pediatric Rating Scale-Revised and the Young Mania
bipolar disorder largely using double-blind stud- Rating Scale.
ies further supports the efficacy of this medica-
tion (Uttley, Kearns, Ren, & Stevenson, 2013).
Risperidone has also been studied in the treat- Other Medications
ment of pediatric bipolar disorder in a number of
studies. Carlson, Potegal, Margulies, Basile, and Other medications have also been used to treat
Gutkovich (2010) studied 49 of 151 5–12-year-­ pediatric bipolar disorder. Carbamazepine is one
olds treated for “rage.” A subgroup of these chil- of these. Findling and Ginsberg (2014) describe
dren possibly also evince bipolar disorder, an “open-label” study (no experimental controls).
according to the authors. The procedure was one One hundred and fifty-seven children and adoles-
use of seclusion for the first episode of rage, fol- cents between 10 and 17 years of age diagnosed
lowed by medication when a second episode with acute manic or mixed episodes of pediatric
occurred. The restrictive nature of the treatments, bipolar disorder were studied. The dependent
particularly given that the children were so measure was the Young Mania Rating Scale (for
young, suggests the need for a different first step. which lower scores indicate less symptomology).
Clinicians should consider using a functional Average scores went from 28.6 before
assessment, followed by behaviorally based ­intervention to 13.8 at the end of the 26-week
interventions such as teaching triggers to the treatment phase.
challenging behaviors, relaxation and other cop- Another open-label carbamazepine study was
ing skills, modifying stressful situations, and reported for 16 children who completed an
teaching replacement behaviors. 8-week treatment regimen. All children were
Pavuluri and colleagues (2010) have also diagnosed with pediatric bipolar disorder.
treated pediatric bipolar disorder with risperi- Dependent variables for the studies included the
done. They describe a double-blind randomized Young Mania Rating Scale, the Children’s
outpatient experiment. There were 66 children in Depression Rating Scale, and the Brief
the study with an age range of 8–15 years. Psychiatric Rating Scale. Joshi and colleagues
Treating Bipolar Disorders 201

(2010) reported that the medication trial was gen- with mania or hypomania. Their primary depen-
erally beneficial as assessed with these measures dent variables were the Children’s Depression
(e.g., improved scores). Rating Scale-Revised and the Young Mania
Azorin and Findling (2007) provide a review Rating Scale. They reported improvement in
of yet another medication used for pediatric bipo- about three-fourth of the participants, with fur-
lar disorder, valproate. They reviewed papers that ther gains noted during the remission period.
used randomized controlled trials when valproate Another medication used for children and
was used alone or in combination with an atypi- adolescents diagnosed with bipolar disorder is
cal antipsychotic medication. A positive aspect divalproex sodium. Redden and colleagues
of these papers is that the Young Mania Rating (2009) used the slow-release form of the drug in
Scale was used. Thus, the indirect comparison of an open-label study with 226 children and ado-
effectiveness across medications for pediatric lescents 9–17 years of age. One hundred and nine
bipolar disorder can be made. They note that participants completed the 6-month study. From
these studies support the efficacy of this treat- pretest to posttest, Young Mania Rating Scale
ment approach. scores dropped on average 12.4, a marked reduc-
Quetiapine for children with bipolar disorder tion in symptoms.
have also been reported. Scheffer, Tripathi,
Kirkpatrick, and Schultz (2010) studied rapid
quetiapine loading for 75 children and adoles- Supplements
cents 6–16 years of age who were diagnosed with
pediatric bipolar disorder. They used specific cut- In addition to conventional mental health medi-
off scores on the Young Mania Rating Scale and cations, other treatments have also been tried.
the Clinical Global Impression-Improvement Gracious, Chirieac, Costescu, Youngstrom, and
scale that the authors defined as remission. The Hibbeln (2010) prescribed flax oil which con-
researchers reported a 70% remission rate tains omega-3 fatty acid. Children and adoles-
6 months after initiating the drug trial. cents diagnosed with bipolar disorder who were
Joshi and colleagues (2012) in another study 6–17 years old (N = 51) were given 12 capsules a
testing quetiapine treated 49 children and adoles- day for 16 weeks. Outcome measures included
cents aged 4–15 years. Thirty-four of the partici- the Young Mania Rating Scale, the Children’s
pants completed the drug trial. It was reported Depression Rating Scale-Revised, and the
that these individuals with bipolar disorder sig- Clinical Global Impressions-Bipolar scale. No
nificantly improved on scores with the Young significant therapeutic effects were detected.
Mania Rating Scale. The authors also report sig- More positive effects were noted by Wozniak
nificant weight gain among those individuals and colleagues (2015). They used a randomized
who were treated. double-blind experimental design over 12 weeks
Findling, Pathak, Earley, Liu, and DelBello with 5–12-year-old children diagnosed with
(2014) studied quetiapine extended-release in bipolar disorder. Seven children received inosi-
193 10–17-year-olds who had been diagnosed tol, seven received omega-3 fatty acid, and ten
with acute bipolar depression. One hundred children received both. A little over half of the
forty-four patients completed the study. This persons treated completed the drug trial. The
study had good experimental control and used the greatest improvements as measured with the
Children’s Depression Rating Scale-Revised as Children’s Depression Rating Scale were the
the primary outcome measure. They reported that children receiving both treatments.
the clinical trial demonstrated the efficacy of this What is clear at this point is that the bulk of
treatment. the intervention attempts have focused on medi-
Lamotrigine has also been used to treat pediat- cation and supplements for pediatric bipolar dis-
ric bipolar disorder (Pavuluri et al., 2009). These order. More needs to be done to enhance the
authors used a 14-week open trial for 46 children specificity and sensitivity of the diagnostic pro-
202 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

cess itself. It is laudable that a few standard (2015). The participants were 12–18 years old,
checklist measures have been consistently used and all of the individuals had a primary diagnosis
to evaluate treatment outcome and are rarely of bipolar disorder. Clients were from an outpa-
reported. However, more sensitive observational tient clinic and were randomly assigned to the
measures of target behaviors should be used to behavior therapy (n = 14) or psychosocial treat-
augment these normed scales. Also, standardized ment as usual condition (n = 6). All participants
methods of evaluating drug side effects are needed. also were receiving medication management
This latter point is particularly salient with highly independent of the study. Dialectic behavior ther-
vulnerable populations such as children. apy consisted of 36 sessions, half of which
Many of the studies involve open-label focused on family skills. The treatment as usual
research designs. This approach is not particu- condition consisted of “eclectic” psychotherapy
larly productive. Using controlled quasi-­ that was primarily supportive in nature. Target
randomized double-blind studies is needed, symptoms included suicidal ideation, self-injury,
particularly as a means of comparing various and emotional dysregulation. The dialectic
drug treatments that are showing some promise. behavior therapy groups improved more than
Also, more emphasis on psychologically based controls on manic symptoms and emotional
intervention is urgently needed. Trials of drug dysregulation.
psychological intervention as combined treat- Young et al. (2016) also describe a treatment
ments are needed as well. At present the available using psychoeducational methods and omega-3
research is still somewhat preliminary in nature. supplements. They studied 94 children 7–14 years
of age in an open-label (nonrandomized or con-
trolled) study. Outcome measures included the
Psychological Treatments Clinical Global Impression-Improvement,
Children’s Depression Rating Scale-Revised, and
Early in the chapter the authors lamented the lack Young Mania Rating Scale. The authors con-
of studies using psychological intervention plus cluded that this approved may have some benefit
medication for pediatric bipolar disorder. An for bipolar disorder, particularly for externalizing
exception to this problem is a paper by Fristad symptoms.
and colleagues (2015). They report a controlled As just noted, the efforts to develop psycho-
trial of omega-3 fatty acids plus family psycho- logically based treatment for children diagnosed
educational psychotherapy. A 12-week trial was with bipolar disorder are very limited, much
run on several conditions. The groups included more so than research on medications. A few
omega-3 plus psychoeducational treatment efforts have been described in the empirical lit-
(n = 5), omega-3 and active monitoring (n = 5), erature. Cognitive or cognitive behavior therapy
placebo plus psychoeducational psychotherapy is the descriptors most commonly used for these
(n = 7), and placebo plus active monitoring psychological approaches. Typical of this meth-
(n = 6). The 7–14-year-olds were all diagnosed odology is a paper by Strawn and colleagues
with bipolar disorder with dependent variables (2016). They studied nine children and
consisting of the Kiddie Schedule for Affective ­ adolescents 11–15 years of age. The authors
Disorder-Depression and Mania subscales, the employed what they described as a mindfulness-
Children’s Depression Rating Scale-Revised, and based cognitive behavior therapy (CBT)
the Young Mania Rating Scale. Over four-fifths approach. They suggest that the treatment they
of the sample completed the 12-week trial. Some used enhanced brain activity in those anxious
improvement was noted across groups, but not at youth with parents who were diagnosed with
statistically significant levels. bipolar disorder. The problem here is the sample
A treatment based on dialectic behavior ther- size, the experimental design, and the indirect
apy is described by Goldstein and colleagues nature of the dependent measures.
Treating Bipolar Disorders 203

Another CBT study is described by Knutsson, ery from core symptoms. These improvements
Backstrom, Daukantaite, and Lecerof (2016). continued over the following year.
Seven adolescents diagnosed with pediatric bipo- Miklouitz and colleagues (2011) conducted
lar disorder and 11 parents, all in Sweden, par- another study with a similar methodology. They
ticipated. Using a case series design, the studied 13 children and adolescents 10–17 years
researchers focused on improving social skills, of age. All the participants had a parent with bipo-
parental knowledge about how to cope with their lar disorder, while all of the participants were
children’s problems, and family communication. diagnosed with bipolar disorder, major depres-
The authors conclude that CBT may help in deal- sion, or cyclothymic disorder. The 12-­ session
ing with the problem areas noted alone. They also manualized treatment described in the previous
note that this intervention can be provided on an study was used here. The authors note improve-
outpatient basis. ments in depression, hypomania, and social
Dickstein, Cushman, Kim, Weissman, and behavior. A major problem with the study was
Wegbreit (2015) advocate for using CBT in the that psychotropic medications were “adjusted” as
treatment of pediatric bipolar disorder. In their needed during the course of the study. In our view
review, particular emphasis is placed on training this renders the results uninterpretable.
response inhibition, face processing, and cogni- A concern with this intervention and for most
tive flexibility. When looking at these studies, the of the psychologically based interventions
goals for intervention vary a good deal. Having a reported is their brevity. Given the chronic nature
template of psychological treatment goals would of bipolar disorder, more extended interventions
be very useful. of a year or more may be in order, at least for
Cotton and colleagues (2016) also talk about some of the more treatment-resistant cases. Also,
using CBT. They follow a mindfulness model. going from weekly sessions and abruptly stop-
These authors treated ten children and adoles- ping intervention may not be the most effective
cents using 12 weekly sessions. Cognitive behav- model in all instances. Gradually titrating ses-
ior therapy and mindfulness were used to enhance sions from weekly to biweekly to monthly might
attention and nonjudgmental acceptance of be another approach worth considering as a
thoughts. They reported decreases in childhood means of maximizing treatment gains over time.
anxiety and improved parent emotional regula-
tion. However, this was an uncontrolled study,
and the results therefore must be interpreted with Conclusions
caution.
Early intervention for children at risk for pedi- Peruzzolo, Tramontina, Rohde, and Zeni (2013)
atric bipolar disorder is also a topic that has note that there has been increased attention and
received some attention. Miklouitz and col- research on pediatric bipolar disorder. Most of
leagues (2013) studied 40 children and adoles- the research has been on medication, and as these
cents 9–17 years of age who had been diagnosed authors note, the most researched area has been
with bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, the use of second-generation antipsychotics.
or cyclothymic disorder. The primary outcome These medications have potentially very serious
measure was the Young Mania Rating Scale. side effects, particularly when given at high doses
Participants were designed on educational con- and/or over long time periods. Several issues
trol/treatment as usual or a 12-session psychoed- apply with respect to this drug class in particular
ucational program that was more intensive and but also to medication in general.
focused on teaching communication and First, we recognized that when pediatric bipo-
problem-­solving skills. This more intensive and lar disorder is present, the use of medication may
structured intervention resulted in greater be unavoidable. There is enough data available at
improvements. Benefits were more rapid recov- this point to suggest that a number of medications
204 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

appear to have some positive effects on core Benarous, X., Mikita, N., Goodman, R., & Stringaris, A.
(2015). Distinct relationships between social aptitude
symptoms of bipolar disorder.
and dimensions of manic-like symptoms in youth.
Second, make sure the diagnosis is accurate European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. https://doi.
and err on the side of under- versus overdiagnos- org/10.1007/s00787-015-0800-7
ing. This point is particularly salient when con- Betts, K. S., Williams, G. M., Naiman, J. M., & Alati,
R. (2016). Predicting spectrums of adult mania, psy-
sidering the data of Karanti and colleagues (2016)
chosis, and depression by prospectively ascertained
who discuss prescription patterns for adults with childhood neurodevelopment. Journal of Psychiatric
bipolar disorder. They report a dramatic uptick in Research, 72, 22–29.
the use of second-generation antipsychotics and Biederman, J., Petty, C.R., Wilens, T.E., Spencer, T.,
Henin, A., Farone, S.V. … Wozniak, J. (2009).
decrease in the use of mood stabilizers alone.
Examination of concordance between maternal and
These patterns of drug prescription seem to apply youth reports in the diagnosis of pediatric bipolar dis-
to children and adolescents as well (Peruzzolo order. Bipolar Disorder, 11, 298–306.
et al., 2013). Carlson, G. A., Potegal, M., Margulies, P., Basile, J., &
Gutkovich, Z. (2010). Liquid risperidone in the treat-
Third, to minimize risks, better assessment of
ment of rages in psychiatrically hospitalized children
drug side effects is needed. Also needed are pro- with possible bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 12,
tocols to minimize dosing and to titrate and ter- 205–212.
minate drug trials. This point can’t be emphasized Chen M-H., Lan, W-H.., Hsu, J-W., Huang, K-L.., Chen,
Y-S., Li, C-T. … Bai Y-M.. (2015). Risk of bipo-
enough because often in clinical practice, once
lar disorder among adolescents with allergic rhi-
started, medications continue with no systematic nitis: A nationwide longitudinal study. Journal of
attempt to follow up and adjust medications Psychosomatic Research, 79, 533–536.
based on a systematic review of symptoms and Cotton, S., Luberto, C.M., Sears, R.W., Strawn, J.R.,
Stahl, L., Wasson, R.S … DelBello, M.P. (2016).
their exposure to treatment.
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for youth with
Fourth, where are the psychological interven- anxiety disorders at risk for bipolar disorder: A pilot
tions? Very little has been done on this front, but trial. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 10. doi:https://
the meager results available to date appear to be doi.org/10.1111/eip.12216
Detke, H. C., DelBello, M., Landry, J., & Usher, R. W.
promising. Treatment development in this area
(2015). Olanzapine/fluoxetine combined in children
should be a top priority. and adolescents with bipolar I depression: A random-
Pediatric bipolar disorder has gone from a ized double-blind placebo-controlled trial. Journal
rarely diagnosed condition to one of the high-­ of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 54, 217–224.
profile diagnoses in children’s mental health.
Dickstein, D. P., Cushman, G. K., Kim, K. L., Weissman,
Given the severity and chronicity of the condition A. B., & Wegbreit, E. (2015). Cognitive remediation:
and the intrusive nature of preferred treatments, Potential novel brain-based treatment for bipolar dis-
much more focus on how to enhance therapeutic order for children and adolescents. CNS Spectrums,
20, 382–390.
effects while minimizing harm should be
Doey, T. (2012). Aripiprazole in pediatric psychosis and
considered. bipolar disorder: A clinical review. Journal of Affective
Disorders, 138, 515–521.
Doucette, S., Horrocks, J., Grof, P., Keown-Stoneman, C.,
& Duffy, A. (2013). Attachment and temperament pro-
References files among the offspring of a parent with bipolar dis-
order. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150, 522–526.
Azorin, J. M., & Findling, R. L. (2007). Valproate use in Duffy, A., Aldo, M., Hajek, T., Sherry, S. B., & Grof, P.
children and adolescents with bipolar disorder. CNS (2010). Early stages in the development of bipolar dis-
Drugs, 21, 1019–1033. order. Journal of Affective Disorders, 121, 127–135.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic Duffy, A., Jones, S., Goodday, S., & Bentall, R. P. (2015).
and Statistical Manual of Mental disorders (DSM-5). Candidate risks indicators for bipolar disorder:
Washington, DC. Early intervention opportunities in high-risk youth.
Benarous, X., Consoli, A., Milhiet, V., & Cohen, D. (2016). International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology,
Early interventions for youths at high risk for bipolar 19, pyv071. http://dx.doi.org/10.10931ijnplpyvo71
disorder: A developmental approach. European Child Findling, R. L., Correll, C.U., Nyilas, M., Forbes, R.A.,
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 217–233. McQuade, R.D., Jin, N. … Carlson, G.A. (2013).
Treating Bipolar Disorders 205

Aripiprazole for the treatment of pediatric bipolar I Insel, T. R. (2012). Next generation treatments for ­mental
disorder: A 30-week, randomized, placebo-controlled disorders. Science Translational Medicine, 4, 155ps19.
study. Bipolar Disorders, 15, 138–149. Joshi, G., Petty, C., Wozniak, J., Faraone, S.V., Doyle,
Findling, R. L., & Ginsberg, L. D. (2014). The safety R., Georgiopoulos, A … Biederman, J. (2012). A pro-
and effectiveness of open-label extended-release car- spective open-label trial of quetiapine monotherapy
bamazepine in the treatment of children and adoles- in preschool and school age children with bipolar
cents with bipolar I disorder suffering from a manic spectrum disorder. Journal of Affective Disorder, 136,
or mixed episode. Neuropsychiatric Disease and 1143–1153.
Treatment, 10, 1589–1597. Joshi, G., Wozniak, J., Mick, E., Hammerness, P.,
Findling, R. L., Kafantaris, V., Pavuluri, M., McNarmara, Gerogiopoulos, A. … Biederman, J. (2010). A pro-
N. K., Frazier, J. A., Sikich, L., & Taylor-Zapata, P. spective open-label trial of extended-release carbam-
(2013). Post-acute effectiveness of lithium in pediat- azepine monotherapy in children with bipolar disorder.
ric bipolar I disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology,
Psychopharmacology, 23, 80–90. 20, 7–14.
Findling, R. L., Pathak, S., Earley, W. R., Liu, S., & Knutsson, J., Backstrom, B., Daukantaite, D., & Lecerof,
DelBello, M. P. (2014). Efficacy and safety of F. (2016). Adolescent and family-focused cognitive-­
extended-release quetiapine fumarate in youth with behavioural therapy for paediatric bipolar disorders:
bipolar depression: An 8 week, double-blind placebo-­ A case series. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy.
controlled trial. Journal of Child and Adolescent https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2027
Psychopharmacology, 24, 325–335. Karanti, A., Kardell, M., Lundberg, U., Landén, M.
Fristad, M.A., Frazier, T.W., Youngstrom, E.A., Mount, (2016). Changes in mood stabilizer prescription
K., Fields, B.W. … Findling, R.L. (2012). What dif- patterns in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective
ferentiates children visiting outpatient mental health Disorders, 195, 50–56.
services with bipolar spectrum disorder from children Landersdorfer, C. B., Findling, R. L., Frazier, J. A.,
with other psychiatric diagnoses? Bipolar Disorders, Kafantaris, V., & Kirkpatrick, C. M. J. (2016).
14, 497–506. Lithium in paediatric patients with bipolar disorder:
Fristad, M.A., Young, A.S., Vesco, A.T., Nader, E.S., Implications for selection of dosage regimens via
Healy, K.Z., Gardner, W … Arnold, L.E. (2015). A population pharmacokinetics/pharmacodynamics.
randomized controlled trial of individual family psy- Clinical Pharmacokinetics. https://doi.org/10.1007/
choeducational psychotherapy and omega-3 fatty acids s40262-016-0430-3
in youth with subsyndromal bipolar disorder. Journal Marangoni, C., Hernandez, M., & Faedda, G. L. (2016).
of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 25, The role of environmental exposures as risk factors for
764–774. bipolar disorder: A systematic review of longitudinal
Geller, B., Luby, J.L., Joshi, P. Wagner, K.D., Emslie, studies. Journal of Affective Disorders, 193, 165–174.
G., Walkup, J. T … Lavori, P. (2012). A randomized Miklouitz, D.J., Schneck, C.D., Singh, M.K., Taylor,
controlled trial of risperidone, lithium, or divalproex D.O., George, E.L., Cosgrove, V.E … Chang, K.D.
sodium for initial treatment of bipolar I disorder, (2013). Early intervention for symptomatic youth at
manic or mixed phase, in children and adolescents. risk for bipolar disorder: A randomized trial of family-­
Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 515–528. focused therapy. Journal of the American Academy of
Goldstein, T.R., Fersch-Podrat, R.K., Rivera, M., Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 121–136.
Axelson, D.A., Merranko, J., Yu, H … Birmaher, Miklowitz, D. J., Chang, K. D., Taylor, D. O., George,
B. (2015). Dialectical behavior therapy for adoles- E. L., Singh, M. K., Schneck, C. D., … & Garber,
cents with bipolar disorder: Results from a pilot J. (2011). Early psychosocial intervention for youth at
randomized trial. Journal of Child and Adolescent risk for bipolar I or II disorder: A one-year treatment
Psychopharmacology, 25, 140–149. development trial. Bipolar Disorders, 13(1), 67–75.
Gracious, B. L., Chirieac, M. C., Costescu, S., Youngstrom, Naguy, A. (2016). Lithium is clearly underutilized in child
E. A., & Hibbeln, J. R. (2010). Randomized, placebo-­ psychiatry. Chinese Medical Journal, 129, 376.
controlled trial of flax oil in pediatric bipolar disorder. Pavuluri, M.N., Henry, D.B., Findling, R.L., Parnes,
Bipolar Disorders, 12, 142–154. S., Carbray, J.A., Mohammed, T. … Sweeney, J.A.
Haas, M., DelBello, M. P., Pandina, G., Kushner, S., Van (2010). Double-blind randomized trial of Risperidone
Hove, I., Augustyns, I., & Kusumakar, V. (2009). versus Divalproex in pediatric bipolar disorder.
Risperidone for the treatment of acute mania in chil- Bipolar Disorders, 12, 593–605.
dren and adolescents with bipolar disorder: A random- Pavuluri, M. N., Henry, D. B., Moss, M., Mohammed, T.,
ized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Bipolar Carbray, J. A., & Sweeney, J. A. (2009). Effectiveness
Disorders, 11, 687–700. of lamotrigine in maintaining symptom control in
Hunt, J., Schwartz, C. M., Nye, P., & Frazier, E. (2016). Is pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal of Child and
there a bipolar prodromal among children and adoles- Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 19, 75–82.
cents? Current Psychiatry Reports, 18, 35.
206 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

Peruzzolo, T. L., Tramontina, S., Rohde, L. A., & Zeni, Neural function before and after mindfulness-based
C. P. (2013). Pharmacotherapy of bipolar disor- cognitive therapy in anxious adolescents at risk for
der in children and adolescents: An update. Revisita developing bipolar disorder. Journal of Child and
Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 35, 393–405. Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26, 372–379.
Pope, S., & Zaraa, S. G. (2016). Efficacy of long-­acting Uttley, L., Kearns, B., Ren, S., & Stevenson, M. (2013).
injectable antipsychotics in adolescents. Journal Aripiprazole for the treatment and prevention of acute
of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26, manic and mixed episodes in bipolar I disorder in
391–394. children and adolescents: A NICE single technology
Post, R.M., Altshuler, L.L., Kupka, R., McElroy, S.L., appraisal. PharmacoEconomics, 31, 981–990.
Frye, M.A., Rowe, M … Nolen, W.A. (2016). More Van Meter, A. R., Burke, C., Kowatch, R. A., Findling,
illness in offspring of bipolar patients from the U.S. R. L., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2016). Ten-year updated
compared to Europe. Journal of Affective Disorders, meta-analysis of the clinical characteristics of pedi-
191, 180–186. atric mania and hypomania. Bipolar Disorders, 18,
Potter, M. P., Liu, H. Y., Monuteaux, M. C., Henderson, 19–32.
C. S., Wozniak, J., Wilens, T. E., & Biederman, Vande Voort, J. L., Singh, A., Bernardi, J., Wall, C. A.,
J. (2009). Prescribing patterns for treatment of pedi- Swintak, C. C., Schak, K. M., & Jensen, P. S. (2016).
atric bipolar disorders in a specialty clinic. Journal Treatments and services provided to children diag-
of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 19, nosed with bipolar disorder. Child Psychiatry and
529–538. Human Development, 47, 494–502.
Redden, L., DelBello, M., Wagner, K.D., Wilens, T. E., Vedel Kessing, L., Vradi, E., & Andersen, P. K. (2015).
Malhorta, S., Wozniak, P … Saltarelli, M. (2009). Diagnostic stability in pediatric bipolar disorder.
Long-term safety of divalproex sodium extended Journal of Affective Disorders, 172, 417–421.
release in children and adolescents with bipo- Wong, L.-J., Shyu, Y.-C., Yuan, S.-S., Yang, K.-C., Lee,
lar I disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent T.-L., & Lee, S.-Y. (2016). Attention-deficit hyper-
Psychopharmacology, 19, 83–89. activity disorder, its pharmacotherapy and the risk of
Ritter, P. S., Marx, C., Lewtschenko, N., Pfeiffer, S., developing bipolar disorder: A nationwide population-­
Leopold, K., Bauer, M., & Pfenning, A. (2012). The based study in Taiwan. Journal of Psychiatric
characteristics of sleep in patients with manifest Research, 72, 6–14.
bipolar disorder, subjects at high risk of develop- Wozniak, J., Faraone, S.V., Chan, J., Tarko, L., Hernandez,
ing the disease and health control. Journal of Neural M. Davis, J … Biederman, J. (2015). A randomized
Transmission, 119, 1173–1184. clinical trial of high eicosapentaenoic acid omega-3
Scheffer, E., Tripathi, A., Kirkpatrick, F. G., & Schultz, fatty acids and inositol as monotherapy and in com-
T. (2010). Rapid quetiapine loading in youths with bination in the treatment of pediatric bipolar spec-
bipolar disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent trum disorder: A pilot study. The Journal of Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 20, 441–445. Psychiatry, 76, 1548–1555.
Selten, J.-P., Lundberg, M., Rai, D., & Magnusson, C. Young, A. S., Meers, M. R., Vesco, A. T., Seidenfeld,
(2015). Risk for nonaffective psychotic disorder and A. M., Arnold, L. E., & Fristad, M. A. (2016).
bipolar disorder in young people with autism spectrum Predicting therapeutic effects of psychodiagnostic
disorder: A population-based study. JAMA Psychiatry, assessment among children and adolescents par-
72, 483–489. ticipating in randomized controlled trials. Journal of
Singh, M. K., Ketter, T. A., & Chang, K. D. (2010). Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. https://doi.
Atypical antipsychotics for acute manic and mixed org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1146992
episodes in children and adolescents with bipolar dis- Youngstrom, E., Zhao, J., Mankoski, R., Forbes, R.A.,
order. Drugs, 70, 433–442. Marcus, R.M., Carson, W … Findling, R.L. (2013).
Stebbins, M. B., & Corcoran, J. (2016). Pediatric bipolar Clinical significance of treatment effects with aripip-
disorder: The child psychiatrist perspective. Child and razole versus placebo in a study of manic or mixed
Adolescent Social Work Journal, 33, 115–122. episodes associated with pediatric bipolar I disorder.
Strawn, J.R., Cotton, S., Luberto, C.M., Patino, R.L., Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology,
Stahl, L.A., Weber, W.A. … DelBello, M.P. (2016). 23, 72–79.
Specific Phobias

Peter Muris

have put a fence around it, but only after we have


Contents checked it carefully. We cannot take her to the
Introduction............................................................   207 town center for shopping or to a park, and our last
holiday on a campsite was a complete disaster.
Phenomenology......................................................   208
Epidemiology..........................................................   209 This letter written by a mother provides a
Etiology...................................................................   210 nice example of a child with a specific phobia.
Treatment................................................................   213
Kim displays marked and persistent fear and
anxiety, and this extreme emotional reaction
Conclusion..............................................................   216 does not become manifest in relation to all types
References...............................................................   216 of stimuli and situations but specifically occurs
when being confronted with dogs. In addition,
fear and anxiety hinder Kim in her daily func-
tioning, giving fear a pathological flavor and
Introduction justifying the diagnosis of a clinical disorder.
With a mean age of onset of 10 years (Kessler
Dear doctor, all of my children were wary of dogs et al., 2007), specific phobias typically appear
when they were younger, but the fear of my young-
est daughter, Kim (11 years), is far more extreme.
early in life. Thus, it makes sense that when
Whenever she sees one of these animals, she will interested in the pathogenesis and treatment of
start to yell and cry hysterically, clinging to me like this anxiety disorder, one should focus on the
a little baby. Over the years, the problem has gotten childhood years (Ollendick & Muris, 2015). In
worse. Her fear of dogs is currently so extreme that
she does not dare to go on the street anymore. If
this chapter, the knowledge on specific phobias
she has to go out, for example on school days, we in children and adolescents that has accumu-
have to survey the area first before she can cross lated over the years will be summarized. First,
the street (luckily our house is opposite to the the phenomenology of specific phobias will be
school). She is able to go in our garden since we
addressed, followed by a brief exposé of the epi-
demiology of this anxiety disorder in youths. In
the next section, factors involved in the etiology
P. Muris (*) of specific phobias in children and adolescents
Department of Clinical Psychological Science, will be discussed. Finally, an overview will be
Maastricht University, Maastricht,
The Netherlands given of the most commonly used treatments of
e-mail: peter.muris@maastrichtuniversity.nl this condition.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 207


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_12
208 P. Muris

Phenomenology disorder), separation from home or attach-


ment figures (as in separation anxiety disor-
The term phobia stems from the Greek word der), or social situations (as in social anxiety
“phobos,” meaning fear. Since the beginning of disorder).
the nineteenth century, the term phobia was
increasingly used in its current sense to denote an The DSM discerns five subtypes of specific
intense fear that is out of proportion to the stimu- phobias, namely animal type (e.g., dogs, snakes,
lus or situation that provokes it. Only in 1952, spiders), blood-injection-injury type (e.g., sight
phobias became a diagnostic category in the of blood, needles, invasive medical procedures),
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental natural environment type (e.g., heights, thunder-
Disorders (DSM). In the early editions of this storms, deep water), situational type (e.g., air-
psychiatric classification system, all phobias planes, elevators, tunnels), and other type (e.g.,
were grouped together, but from DSM-III choking, costumed characters, loud sounds).
onward, simple phobias were regarded as a sepa- There is some empirical support for the subtyp-
rate disorder that was distinct from other phobic ing of specific phobias in young people. For
disorders such agoraphobia and social phobia. In example, in a factor analytic study performed on
DSM-IV simple phobias were relabeled as spe- the fear ratings of 996 children and adolescents
cific phobias, but the criteria for defining this aged between 7 and 19 years, Muris, Schmidt,
anxiety disorder have remained more or less the and Merckelbach (1999) noted that fears clus-
same. In the current edition of the DSM (i.e., tered in three primary factors. The first factor
DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association consisted of animal phobias, the second factor
[APA], 2015), the following criteria need to be contained blood-injection-injury phobias,
met: whereas the third factor was a combination of
natural environment and situational phobias.
A. Marked fear or anxiety about a specific object Phobias present themselves in three response
or situation. systems (Lang, 1968). That is, the marked fear
B. The phobic object or situation almost always and anxiety are typically accompanied by (1)
provokes immediate fear or anxiety. subjective feelings of apprehension (e.g., fear of
C. The phobic object or situation is actively harm or injury, fear of losing control), (2) physi-
avoided or endured with intense fear or cal symptoms (e.g., heart rate acceleration,
anxiety. sweating, increased respiration), and (3) avoid-
D. The fear or anxiety is out of proportion of the ance or escape behavior (e.g., evading the phobic
actual danger posed by the specific object or stimulus, running away, staying close to a famil-
situation and to the sociocultural context. iar person). There may be differences in symp-
E. The fear, anxiety, or avoidance is persistent, tom presentation across various subtypes of
typically lasting for 6 months or more. specific phobias (LeBeau et al., 2010). For
F. The fear, anxiety, or avoidance causes clini- instance, although all phobias are accompanied
cally significant distress or impairment in by subjective feelings of fear and anxiety, in
social, occupational, or other important areas some animal and blood-injection-injury phobias,
of functioning. strong feelings of disgust and revulsion are also
G. The disturbance is not better explained by the present (Olatunji & McKay, 2009). In a similar
symptoms of another mental disorder, includ- vein, whereas all specific phobias are character-
ing fear, anxiety, and avoidance of situations ized by cognitions related to fear of harm or
associated with panic-like symptoms or other injury, there are several phobias, especially situ-
incapacitating symptoms (as in agoraphobia), ational phobias (e.g., claustrophobia), that
objects or situations related to obsessions (as involve additional anxiety expectations such as
in obsessive-compulsive disorder), reminder fear of going crazy or fear of losing control
of traumatic events (as in posttraumatic stress (Craske, Mohlman, Yi, Glover, & Valeri, 1995).
Specific Phobias 209

Further, confrontation with the phobic stimulus of 3.6% for specific phobias. The National
usually elicits sympathetic arousal (tachycardia Comorbidity Survey-Replication that was con-
or increased heart rate) in most phobia subtypes, ducted in the United States recently documented
but in blood-injection-injury phobia, a biphasic a lifetime prevalence rate of 20% among 13- to
physiological response pattern is typically noted 17-year-olds (Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson,
(i.e., initial tachycardia followed by a bradycar- Zaslavsky, & Wittchen, 2012). Specific phobias
dia or heart rate slowing; Page, 1994). of animals such as dogs, spiders, and snakes are
There may also be differences in the respon- quite frequent, and the same is true for phobias of
sivity of the three systems among phobic youths. blood, injections, and injuries and environmental
In a study by Ollendick, Allen, Benoit, and phobias of heights and thunderstorms (Benjet,
Cowart (2011), 73 clinically referred children Borges, Stein, Mendez, & Medina-Mora, 2012).
and adolescents with various types of specific Specific phobias tend to be more prevalent
phobias provided subjective fear ratings, partici- among girls than boys (with a rate of approxi-
pated in a behavioral approach test (BAT), and mately 2:1; APA, 2015) and in older than younger
underwent a physiological recording of their children (e.g., Ollendick, King, & Muris, 2002).
heart rate. The results showed that although vari- Further, in nonclinical samples, specific phobias
ous indices of fear were significantly correlated often occur in isolation without the presence of
with each other, there was also quite some varia- other comorbid problems. This is different in
tion with some youths being concordant and oth- clinical populations where the majority of the
ers being discordant across the three response youngsters with specific phobias also meet the
systems. Ollendick et al. (2011) found some evi- diagnostic criteria of other psychiatric disorders.
dence to suggest that concordant activation of all For example, in an older study by Last, Strauss,
systems is indicative for the severity of the disor- and Francis (1987), it was found that 64% of the
der. Another example of individual differences in children and adolescents with a primary diagno-
fear responses can be found when looking at the sis of a specific phobia also presented with one or
behavioral system. Whereas most phobic youths more additional diagnoses including generalized
will try to avoid or escape from the stimulus or anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder,
situation that they fear, there is a subgroup of obsessive-­ compulsive disorder, panic disorder,
children expressing their fear or anxiety by cry- major depressive disorder, dysthymia, and oppo-
ing, tantrums, freezing, or clinging (APA, 2015). sitional defiant disorder.
Again, it might be the case that these atypical The results regarding the continuity of child-
behaviors are mainly found in youths with severe hood specific phobias indicate that this anxiety
specific phobias, but there may also be a link with problem is not always stable over time. For
age/developmental level, children’s tempera- example, in a study by Last, Perrin, Hersen, and
ment, and characteristics of the phobic stimulus Kazdin (1996), it was found that 31% of the chil-
or situation (e.g., imminence of threat). dren and adolescents with an initial diagnosis of
a specific phobia on time 1 no longer fulfilled the
diagnostic criteria at a 3- to 4-year follow-up.
Epidemiology However, in comparison with other anxiety dis-
orders (such as separation anxiety disorder and
Anxiety disorders in general are one of the most social anxiety disorder), this recovery percentage
prevalent types of psychopathology in youths was quite modest, leading the authors to con-
(Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, clude that specific phobias represent one of the
2003), and among the anxiety disorders specific most persistent anxiety problems. Recent evi-
phobias are most common. Using the data of 14 dence also showed that specific phobias in young
epidemiological studies that included children people are a “precursor” disorder predicting the
and adolescents of various ages, Costello, Egger, subsequent onset of other types of psychopathol-
and Angold (2004) found a point-prevalence rate ogy (Lieb et al., 2016).
210 P. Muris

Etiology ally found in comparable studies conducted in


adult populations (Van Houtem et al., 2013) and
Some scholars assume that specific phobias are indicate that specific phobias are at least in part
inborn, evolutionary prepared responses that pro- heritable.
tect children against environmental threats.
Briefly, this preparedness account assumes that Aberrant Brain Processes  In recent decades,
evolution has selected for fear and avoidance of considerable advancements have been made with
certain stimuli (e.g., snakes, spiders, water, the study of brain processes in individuals suffer-
heights) to protect the defenseless young off- ing from specific phobias. This research has
spring of human beings (Seligman, 1971). mainly been conducted with adults using a symp-
Although this theory has attracted a lot of tom provocation procedure. During such a proce-
research attention (McNally, 1996), questions dure, the phobic individual is exposed to, for
remain about its empirical validity. An alternative example, a picture of fear-relevant stimulus,
account for the etiology of specific phobias while a scanner assesses the activation in various
adopts a developmental psychopathology per- areas of the brain. By comparing these data to
spective. In this view, children’s phobias are in those obtained for healthy control participants, it
essence normal developmental fears that due to is possible to detect aberrations in the brain pro-
an interaction of genetic vulnerability and detri- cessing of fear. Typically, this type of studies
mental learning experiences have radicalized into demonstrate that individuals with a specific pho-
a persistent anxiety problem (Muris, 2007; Muris bia show more activation in the brain areas that
& Field, 2011; Muris & Merckelbach, 2001; are involved in the perception and early amplifi-
Muris, Merckelbach, De Jong, & Ollendick, cation of fear – including the formation of an
2002). It is important to keep in mind that the autonomic arousal response – such as the amyg-
origins of specific phobias cannot be explained dala, anterior cingulate cortex, thalamus, and
by a single process. This is illustrated in the next insula. At the same time, brain areas in the pre-
paragraphs of this section in which I will discuss frontal cortex, which are normally activated in
a number of pathogenic factors, including genetic healthy controls to regulate fear, are less activated
influences, aberrant brain processes, tempera- in individuals with a specific phobia (see for a
ment, negative learning experiences, avoidance, review Del Casale et al., 2012). Altogether, this
and cognitive biases. suggests that at a structural brain level, individu-
als with a specific phobia are more easily and
Genetic Influences  Few behavioral-genetic stud- more strongly aroused when being confronted
ies have been conducted to explore the role of with fear-relevant stimuli, while they have less
heritability in specific phobias of children and capability to regulate this hyperactivation.
adolescents. A notable exception is the investiga-
tion by Lichtenstein and Annas (2000) who Temperament  Behavioral inhibition refers to the
employed parent report data of specific fears and habitual tendency of some youths to interrupt
phobias in 1106 pairs of 8–9-year-old Swedish ongoing behavior and to react with distress and
twins. The results indicated that the total concor- withdrawal when confronted with unfamiliar
dance of specific phobia was significantly higher stimuli and situations (Kagan, 1994). For chil-
in monozygotic than in dizygotic twins (52% ver- dren many stimuli and situations are novel, and
sus 25%), which produced an overall heritability so it is well-conceivable that those who score
estimate of 65%. Further analysis conducted on high on behavioral inhibition are particularly
the separate phobia types revealed that the genetic prone to show more intense fear reactions and to
contribution was larger in animal (58%) and situ- engage in avoidance. An investigation by
ational/environmental phobias (50%) than in Biederman, Rosenbaum, Bolduc, Faraone, and
blood-injection-injury phobias (28%). These Hirshfeld (1991) has indicated that young chil-
results are reasonably in line with what is gener- dren with this temperamental disposition indeed
Specific Phobias 211

are at higher risk for developing anxiety prob- fear for this animal. This changed after the exper-
lems and that this is also true for specific phobias. imenters paired the presentation of the rat with an
In this study, a structured clinical interview was aversive loud noise (the unconditioned stimulus
used to assess anxiety disorders in two samples or UCS) that was produced by striking a steel bar
of children: an at-risk sample of children of adult hideously behind the boy’s back, and which
patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia and caused him great fright (the unconditioned
a longitudinal cohort of children who had been response or UCR). After five such experiences,
followed from a very young age. In both samples, Albert became very upset (the conditioned
it was found that inhibited children showed sig- response or CR) by the sight of the white rat,
nificantly more anxiety disorders than the unin- even without the presentation of the loud noise.
hibited children, and the difference was Obviously, the fear originally associated with the
particularly clear-cut for generalized anxiety dis- loud noise was now elicited by the previously
order (in the at-risk sample) and specific phobias neutral stimulus, the white rat (now the condi-
(in the longitudinal cohort). Interestingly, when tioned stimulus or CS).
the combined samples were reassessed at a fol- There are certainly some children for which
low-­up of 3 years, it was noted that inhibited chil- their specific phobia can be explained by a dis-
dren displayed a marked increase in specific tinct aversive conditioning event (Dadds, Davey,
phobias and other anxiety disorders (Biederman & Field, 2001), but in many cases the learning
et al., 1993). In another study by Muris, history of a phobia is much more complicated.
Merckelbach, Wessel, and Van der Ven (1999), This is nicely illustrated in a study by Ten Berge,
adolescents aged 12–14 years were provided Veerkamp, and Hoogstraten (2002) who
with a definition of behavioral inhibition and then explored the treatment history of children with
asked to identify themselves as low, middle, or varying levels of dental fear. The results indi-
high on this temperament characteristic. In addi- cated that high and low fearful children did not
tion, the young adolescents completed a stan- differ with regard to the number of aversive,
dardized questionnaire of anxiety disorder curative treatments (potential conditioning
symptoms that included separate scales for the events). The most important difference was that
main types of specific phobias (i.e., animal, situ- low fearful children had experienced more
ational/environmental, blood-injection-injury). innocuous dental visits before their first curative
The results indicated that children in the high treatment took place as compared to high fearful
behavioral inhibition group more often reported children. Apparently, previous neutral experi-
specific phobia symptoms than did children in ences with a CS immunize against the impact of
the low and middle behavioral inhibition groups. an aversive event, a phenomenon that has been
It is good to keep in mind that behavioral inhibi- labeled as latent inhibition. Further, Ten Berge
tion might be the observable manifestation of the et al. noted that children’s personal emotional
genetic/biological liability to specific phobias reactions to the curative treatment enhanced the
that has been described in previous paragraphs. aversiveness of the UCS (UCS inflation), which
suggests that subjective perceptions of threat
Conditioning and Other Learning Experi­ play a decisive role during fear conditioning.
ences  Environmental influences are also thought Both latent inhibition and UCS inflation fit well
to play a crucial role in the etiology of specific with the current theoretical perspective that fear
phobias. In this context, conditioning experi- conditioning should not be viewed as simple,
ences are particularly relevant. The well-­known reflex-like stimulus-response learning but rather
case study of Little Albert (Watson & Rayner, as a process during which individuals learn that
1920) nicely demonstrated that it is possible to one stimulus (the CS) is likely to predict the
instill a phobia in a healthy child via classical occurrence of another stimulus (the UCS),
conditioning. An 11-month-old boy was exposed which in turn will elicit a CR under certain con-
to a white rat and initially the toddler showed no ditions (Field, 2006).
212 P. Muris

Classical conditioning requires that the person contact with the phobic stimulus or situation and
has had direct experience with the CS and the hence does not allow the phobic person to learn
UCS, but fears and phobias can also be learned that the CS is in fact harmless. Meanwhile, by
by observing other people’s responses to a stimu- evading the phobic stimulus or situation, subjec-
lus or situation (modeling) or by hearing that a tive feelings of fear and physiological arousal
stimulus or situation might be dangerous or has decrease, thereby negatively reinforcing the
some other negative connotation (negative infor- avoidance behavior (Ollendick, Vasey, & King,
mation transmission; Rachman, 1991). Evidence 2001). Especially in children, parents seem to
for these indirect learning pathways mainly play an important role in either the continuation
comes from experimental laboratory studies. For or elimination of avoidance behavior of their off-
example, Gerull and Rapee (2002) examined the spring. For example, Ollendick, Lewis, Cowart,
role of modeling in children’s fear acquisition in and Davis (2012) found that clinically referred
15- to 20-month-old toddlers who were con- youths with a specific phobia, who had to
fronted with novel rubber toy spiders and snakes. approach the phobic object in a stepwise manner
During the experiment, mothers were also pres- (i.e., behavioral approach task), were in general
ent and instructed to display either a positive or a less avoidant when one of their parents was pres-
negative facial expression, while their offspring ent as compared to when they had to conduct the
was exposed to the toy animals. The results approach task on their own. However, parent
showed that toddlers whose mothers displayed a characteristics had an impact on children’s per-
negative facial expression were more fearful and formance: when their parent was less warm and
more hesitant to approach the toy animals than less involved during the task, children displayed
toddlers whose mothers had exhibited a positive higher levels of avoidance.
facial expression. Cognitive biases are also thought to be
Field, Argyris, and Knowles (2001) were the involved in the maintenance of specific phobias.
first to investigate the negative information path- These biases reflect enhanced processing of fear-­
way using a paradigm in which children aged related information, which fuel the phobic fear
7–9 years received either negative or positive over and over again. A nice example is attention
information about two unknown monster dolls. bias, which refers to phobic individuals’ hyper-
Results indicated that fear-related beliefs changed vigilance in the perception of threat cues. Martin,
as a function of the verbal information: Negative Horder, and Jones (1992) were one of the first to
information increased children’s self-reported demonstrate that this type of bias is present in
fear, whereas positive information decreased phobic youths. Using a modified version of
their fear level. Subsequent research has shown Stroop task, these researchers found that spider
that negative information transmission has fear-­ fearful children, as compared to non-fearful con-
enhancing effects in all fear modalities (i.e., sub- trols, displayed slower response times when they
jective, physical, behavioral; Muris & Field, were asked to color-name spider-related words
2010) and is also involved in the transfer of fear (e.g., “web”) but not when they had to color-­
from parents to offspring (Muris, Van Zwol, name neutral words (e.g., “fly”). Another type of
Huijding, & Mayer, 2010). However, for both bias that operates in specific phobias is covaria-
modeling and negative information transmission, tion bias, which is concerned with a tendency to
it remains to be demonstrated that they are suffi- overestimate the association between fear- and
ciently powerful to produce clinical levels of phobia-related stimuli (CS) and negative out-
phobic symptoms. comes (UCS). In an experiment conducted by
Muris, Huijding, Mayer, Den Breejen, and
Avoidance and Cognitive Biases  Behavior thera- Makkelie (2007), first evidence was obtained for
pists assume that avoidance is the key mechanism the existence of this type of bias in youths.
in the conservation of phobic fear. That is, avoid- Children and adolescents aged 8–16 years played
ance serves to minimize direct and prolonged a computer game during which they were exposed
Specific Phobias 213

to pictures of spiders (i.e., negative fear-relevant ation response. Briefly, Wolpe assumed that a
stimulus), guns (i.e., negative fear-irrelevant response antagonistic to anxiety (e.g., physiolog-
stimulus), and flowers (i.e., neutral control stimu- ical relaxation) inhibits the emotional fear
lus). Following each picture, a smiley was gener- response. Various studies have demonstrated that
ated by the computer signaling an outcome: a systematic desensitization yields positive effects
happy smile indicated that the child had won when treating phobic youths, and this is espe-
three pieces of candy, and a sad smiley indicated cially true when real-life exposure to the phobic
that the child had lost three pieces of candy, stimulus is used to provoke fear and anxiety dur-
whereas a neutral smiley signaled no positive or ing the therapeutic procedure. For example,
negative consequences. The pictures were shown Ultee, Griffoen, and Schellekens (1982) divided
in a random order, and the computer game was 24 water-phobic children aged between 5 and
programmed in such a way that each picture type 10 years in 3 groups: (1) an in vitro desensitiza-
was equally often followed by a happy, sad, or tion group in which children received gradual
neutral smiley. After the computer game, chil- imaginal exposure to fear-evoking stimuli plus
dren were asked to provide estimates on the relaxation, (2) an in vivo desensitization group in
observed contingencies between the three picture which children were treated with gradual real-life
types and various outcomes. Results indicated exposure in combination with relaxation, and (3)
that children in general displayed an inclination a no-treatment control group. The results indi-
to link the negative outcome to negatively cated that both desensitization procedures were
valenced pictures (i.e., they believed that they effective in reducing children’s fear of water,
had more often lost candy following pictures of whereas no such effect could be observed in the
spiders and guns). Most importantly, the findings no-treatment condition. Further, evidence was
also demonstrated that this covariation bias was found showing that in vivo exposure yielded bet-
modulated by fear. That is, spider fearful youths ter treatment effects than in vitro exposure. In
showed a stronger tendency to specifically asso- spite of the fact that various other studies have
ciate the spider pictures with a negative outcome documented positive effects of systematic desen-
(i.e., losing candy). sitization in the treatment of childhood phobias,
this type of intervention seems somewhat out-
dated. This is because research has demonstrated
Treatment that Wolpe’s (1958) basic ideas about the under-
lying mechanism of systematic desensitization
Exposure is generally regarded as the key ele- are not correct. In fact, there is clear evidence
ment in the treatment of specific phobias, and showing that the relaxation component of this
there is sufficient evidence from controlled treat- treatment is not necessary to achieve the positive
ment outcome research indicating that exposure-­ effects of the intervention.
based treatments are indeed effective in reducing Systematic desensitization pairs exposure
fear and anxiety in phobic children and adoles- with relaxation, but it is good to note that for
cents. Exposure-based treatment of childhood some specific phobias, this type of treatment is
phobias can be delivered in many ways, and less applicable. As noted earlier, blood-injection-­
below I will summarize a number of commonly injury phobias are typically accompanied by a
employed variants. biphasic physiological response pattern (i.e., ini-
tial tachycardia followed by a bradycardia or
Systematic Desensitization  Based on the idea heart rate slowing), which may result in a fall of
that two emotional states cannot occur simultane- blood pressure and ultimately fainting. For these
ously, Wolpe (1958) developed the treatment phobias, it appears preferable to combine expo-
approach of “systematic desensitization,” during sure with muscle tension exercises. This treat-
which fear and anxiety elicited by a phobic stim- ment, also known as applied tension, prevents the
ulus are terminated by a previously learned relax- blood pressure drops and the individual is
214 P. Muris

capable of maintaining the exposure to blood and which children can acquire a fear or phobia.
other prototypical stimuli (e.g., needles; Öst & However, modeling can also be exploited thera-
Sterner, 1987). In children and adolescents, there peutically by asking children to observe a non-­
is still little evidence for the efficacy of the fearful person who approaches the phobic
applied tension technique. A protocol for treating stimulus without displaying any fear. This proce-
blood-injection-injury phobias in youths (Oar, dure can be conducted in various ways. The first
Farrell, & Ollendick, 2015) prescribes psycho- way is filmed modeling, during which the child
education about fainting and the use of simple watches a film in which a model interacts with
coping strategies such as lying down, drinking the phobic stimulus. The second way is live mod-
cool water, and wiggling one’s toes as ways of eling: the phobic child observes a real model
dealing with the physiological symptoms of these interacting and dealing with the phobic stimulus.
phobias. Obviously, this intervention might fur- Finally, during participant modeling, the child
ther profit from the inclusion of applied tension and the model work together: the model demon-
exercises. strates how to approach and deal with the phobic
stimulus and then instructs the child to imitate
Emotive Imagery  Although the therapeutic pro- this behavior. There is some research comparing
cedure of systematic desensitization is less fre- the relative efficacy of these modeling variants.
quently employed nowadays, it is of interest to For example, Menzies and Clarke (1993)
note that there is an age-downward variant that assigned 3- to 8-year-old children with water
may still be feasible to apply, in particular when phobia to various interventions involving expo-
working with younger children. This technique sure, live modeling, or a combination of these
has been described as “emotive imagery” two procedures (which can best be viewed as a
(Lazarus & Abramovitz, 1962). An important variant of participant modeling). Most impor-
feature of the emotive imagery procedure is that tantly, this study demonstrated that modeling
the child identifies himself with a “personal hero” merely yielded significant treatment effects when
(usually a person or cartoon character seen on combined with exposure exercises. Clearly, this
television) and then makes up a narrative, in finding can be taken as support for the notion that
which the phobic stimulus is gradually intro- participant modeling is more effective than live
duced. After the imaginal exposure, during which modeling (Ollendick, Davis, & Muris, 2004).
the child – supported by the personal hero –
effectively deals with the phobic stimulus, he/she Reinforced Practice  During “reinforced prac-
is encouraged to apply these newly learned skills tice” (also known as “contingency manage-
in real-life situations (King, Molloy, Heyne, ment”), an attempt is made to weaken the negative
Murphy, & Ollendick, 1998). In an attempt to associations with the phobic stimulus that result
examine the effectiveness of emotive imagery, in avoidance behavior by strengthening positive
Cornwall, Spence, and Schotte (1996) assigned associations through reinforcement of approach
24 clinically referred 7- to 10-year-old children behavior. This is achieved via exposure exercises
with a severe darkness phobia to either emotive during which successful approaches of the pho-
imagery treatment or a waiting-list control condi- bic stimulus are reinforced by means of rewards.
tion. The results demonstrated that children in the There is ample evidence supporting the efficacy
emotive imagery group showed significant reduc- of reinforced practice in treating phobic children
tions in self-reports of darkness fear and clear and adolescents. For example, Silverman et al.
improvement on a darkness tolerance test, (1999) treated 33 6- to 16-year-old youths with
whereas no such effects were observed in the phobic disorders by means of a reinforced prac-
waiting-list control group. tice program during which children had to per-
form increasingly difficult exposure tasks that
Participant Modeling  As noted in the Etiology were reinforced by their parents every time they
section, modeling represents one way through completed a task successfully. Results revealed
Specific Phobias 215

that this treatment program was equally effective forcement following successful approach behav-
in reducing fear and anxiety levels as a cognitive-­ ior. Öst, Svensson, Hellstrom, and Lindwall
behavioral intervention. Further, it was found that (2001) tested the effectiveness of one-session
the positive treatment effects of reinforcement therapy in a large sample of children and adoles-
practice were largely maintained at a 1-year fol- cents (N = 60) with various types of specific pho-
low-­up. In terms of clinical significant improve- bias. For this purpose, youths were randomly
ment, it was found that more than half of the assigned to (1) regular one-session therapy, (2)
youths (55%) no longer met the diagnostic crite- one-session therapy with one of the child’s par-
ria of a phobic disorder after the completion of ents present, or (3) a waiting-list control group.
the treatment. Various outcome measures were used including
self-report inventories, independent assessor rat-
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT)  CBT is an ings, a behavioral approach test, and physiologi-
intervention that has been originally developed cal indexes (e.g., blood pressure, heart rate), most
for children and adolescents with anxiety disor- of which were obtained at pretreatment, post-
ders in general, but that can also be employed in treatment, and 1-year follow-up. The results con-
youths with specific phobias (Kendall, 1994). A sistently showed that one-session therapy
key element of this treatment is exposure to the produced significantly better results than the
feared stimuli and situations, but CBT also incor- waiting-list control condition. Further, both vari-
porates a range of other behavioral and cognitive ants of the one-session therapy did equally well
techniques (including relaxation, positive self-­ on most outcome measures, indicating that the
talk, cognitive restructuring, reinforcement, etc.) presence of a parent did neither promote nor hin-
that may be helpful to reduce fear and anxiety. der the treatment effects. Finally, the treatment
Although CBT is thought to be particularly effects of OST were maintained at a follow-up of
appropriate for major anxiety disorders (e.g., 1 year. Other studies have shown that this type of
social anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disor- treatment is also more effective than other psy-
der, and generalized anxiety disorder; Rapee, chological interventions (Muris, Merckelbach,
Schniering, & Hudson, 2009), this intervention Holdrinet, & Sijsenaar, 1998; Ollendick et al.,
has certainly proven useful in the treatment of 2009), and so the conclusion seems justified that
children and adolescents with specific phobias this type of intervention is highly effective for
(Ollendick & King, 1998). treating phobias in children and adolescents
(Davis, Jenkins, & Rudy, 2012). 
One-Session Therapy (OST)  OST consists of a A final note on treatment concerns the involve-
single, 3-h session of graduated hierarchical ment of parents in the intervention of children
exposure in combination with elements of psy- with specific phobias. The evidence described in
choeducation, participant modeling, reinforced the section on the etiology clearly suggests that
practice, skills training, and cognitive restructur- parents can be involved in the onset and mainte-
ing (Öst, 2012). The therapist first provides a nance of specific phobias, and so it seems logical
rationale for the treatment and identifies the child to assume that this family factor also needs to be
and therapist as a “team” who are working addressed in the treatment of this anxiety prob-
together to overcome the child’s fear. Treatment lem. However, up till now, there is little support
is comprised of a series of behavioral “experi- for this proposition. For instance, a study by
ments” during which the child is encouraged to Ollendick et al. (2015) compared the efficacy of
approach the feared stimulus while thinking of regular OST (that only focused on the child) with
him- or herself as a “scientist” or “detective” who that of a parent-augmented OST in 97 youths
is testing out phobic cognitions. The therapist aged 6–15 years diagnosed with at least one spe-
acts as a model demonstrating how to handle the cific phobia. Both treatment conditions produced
fearful situation, kindly encouraging the child to similar outcomes, with approximately 50% of the
participate in the exercises, and providing rein- children and adolescents being diagnosis-free
216 P. Muris

and judged to be much to very much improved. t­herapist’s instructions, her parents encouraged
At a 6-month follow-up, the child-only OST was her to approach the animals, which she occasion-
found to be slightly better that the intervention ally (but not always) did.
that also involved parents, but the main conclu-
sion was that “parent augmentation of OST pro-
duced no appreciable gains in treatment Conclusion
outcomes” (p. 141). 
Back to the case of Kim, the 11-year-old girl Specific phobias comprise a class of anxiety dis-
described in the introduction, after making the orders that frequently occur in children and ado-
classification of a specific phobia – animal type – lescents. This psychiatric condition is
it was decided to treat the girl with OST. The characterized by marked fear of a specific stimu-
therapist first explained the basic idea behind the lus or situation, which are typically linked to a
intervention and then together with Kim created a number of select categories (i.e., animal, blood-­
fear hierarchy consisting of dreaded situations injection-­injury, situational, environmental, and
with dogs. Treatment started with a small mild-­ other). The fear manifests itself in three response
tempered dog (a Bichon Frisé): a series of steps systems (i.e., subjective/cognitive, physiological,
was carried out, beginning with entering the and behavioral) and is excessive and unreason-
room and standing 3 m from the animal that was able, thereby hindering the young person’s daily
caged in a bench and ending with standing next functioning. Specific phobias tend to have a
to the dog and petting it. Each step was first mod- multi-facet origin, involving various genetic/bio-
eled by the therapist, who while performing the logical, temperamental, and environmental risk
step carefully described the animal’s benign and vulnerability factors, and are maintained by
behavior in an attempt to correct Kim’s dysfunc- avoidance and various types of cognitive biases.
tional thoughts about dogs. Next, the therapist The effective treatment is straightforward and
encouraged the girl to conduct the step herself, usually involves some kind of exposure to the
and if she succeeded in doing so, she received a feared stimulus or situation, preferably in vivo. In
small reward (i.e., a sticker; at the end of the this way, young people like Kim can be success-
intervention, these stickers were awarded with a fully rescued from their phobic fear, ultimately
price, a stuffed toy dog). After a hesitating start, raising their chances to have a normal life and a
Kim successfully completed the full hierarchy more healthy development toward adulthood.
with the small dog within 1 h. Following this, a
larger dog (a golden retriever) was introduced
and again the full fear hierarchy was conducted. References
It took Kim 45 min to carry out all the steps.
During the final part of the OST, Kim did some American Psychiatric Association. (2015). Diagnostic
additional exercises with the purpose to general- and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
ize the newly acquired skills with dogs to other Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
Benjet, C., Borges, G., Stein, D. J., Mendez, E., &
more “natural” situations, for example, entering a Medina-Mora, M. E. (2012). Epidemiology of fears
room with an unleashed dog, walking the dog, and specific phobias in adolescence: Results from the
staying calm when the dog starts barking, and Mexican Adolescent Mental Health Survey. Journal of
demonstrating her father and mother how she Clinical Psychiatry, 73, 152–158.
Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Bolduc, E. A., Faraone,
played with the dog. Within one 3-h session, Kim S. V., & Hirshfeld, D. R. (1991). A high risk study
was capable of “handling” both animals. Three of young children of parents with panic disorder and
months later, a telephone call revealed that the agoraphobia with and without comorbid major depres-
girl’s phobic complaints were still largely in sion. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37, 333–348.
Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Bolduc-Murphy, E. A.,
remission: she dared to go alone outside on the Faraone, S. V., Chaloff, J., Hirshfeld, D. R., & Kagan,
street and did not panic when she was unexpec­ J. (1993). A 3-year follow-up of children with and
tedly confronted with dogs. Following the without behavioral inhibition. Journal of the American
Specific Phobias 217

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, and mood disorders in the United States. International
814–821. Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21,
Cornwall, E., Spence, S. H., & Schotte, D. (1996). The 169–184.
effectiveness of emotive imagery in the treatment of King, N. J., Molloy, G. N., Heyne, D., Murphy, G. C., &
darkness phobia in children. Behaviour Change, 13, Ollendick, T. H. (1998). Emotive imagery treatment
223–229. for childhood phobias: A credible and empirically
Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2004). validated intervention? Behavioural and Cognitive
Developmental epidemiology of anxiety disorders. In Psychotherapy, 26, 103–113.
T. H. Ollendick & J. S. March (Eds.), Phobic and anxi­ Lang, P. J. (1968). Fear reduction and fear behaviour:
ety disorders in children and adolescents. A clinician’s Problems in treating a construct. In J. M. Schlein
guide to effective psychosocial and pharmacological (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy (Vol. 3, pp. 90–103).
interventions (pp. 61–91). New York, NY: Oxford Washington, DC: American Psychological
University Press. Association.
Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Last, C. G., Perrin, S., Hersen, M., & Kazdin, A. E.
Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of (1996). A prospective study of childhood anxiety dis-
psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. orders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837–844. Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1502–1510.
Craske, M. G., Mohlman, J., Yi, J., Glover, D., & Valeri, Last, C. G., Strauss, C. C., & Francis, G. (1987). Comor­
S. (1995). Treatment of claustrophobia and snake/ bidity among childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of
spider phobias: Fear of arousal and fear of context. Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 726–730.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 197–203. Lazarus, A. A., & Abramovitz, A. (1962). The use of
Dadds, M. R., Davey, G. C. L., & Field, A. P. (2001). ‘emotive imagery’ in the treatment of children’s pho-
Developmental aspects of conditioning processes in bias. Journal of Mental Science, 108, 191–195.
anxiety disorders. In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds LeBeau, R. T., Glenn, D., Liao, B., Wittchen, H. U.,
(Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxi­ Beesdo-Baum, K., Ollendick, T. H., & Craske, M. G.
ety (pp. 205–230). New York, NY: Oxford University (2010). Specific phobia: A review of DSM-IV specific
Press. phobia and preliminary recommendations for DSM-V.
Davis, T. E., Jenkins, W. S., & Rudy, B. M. (2012). Depression and Anxiety, 27, 148–167.
Empirical status of one-session treatment. In T. E. Lichtenstein, P., & Annas, P. (2000). Heritability and
Davis, T. H. Ollendick, & L. G. Öst (Eds.), Intensive prevalence of specific fears and phobias in childhood.
one-session treatment of specific phobias (pp. 209– Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41,
226). New York, NY: Springer. 927–937.
Del Casale, A., Ferracuti, S., Rapinesi, C., Serata, D., Lieb, R., Miché, M., Gloster, A. T., Beesdo-Baum, K.,
Piccirilli, M., Savoja, V., … Girardi, P. (2012). Meyer, A. H., & Wittchen, H. U. (2016). Impact of
Functional neuroimaging in specific phobia. specific phobia on the risk of onset of mental disor-
Psychiatric Research: Neuroimaging, 202, 181–197. ders: A 10-year prospective-longitudinal community
Field, A. P. (2006). Is conditioning a useful framework study of adolescents and young adults. Depression
for understanding the development and treatment of and Anxiety, 33, 667–675.
phobias? Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 857–875. Martin, M., Horder, P., & Jones, G. V. (1992). Integral
Field, A. P., Argyris, N. G., & Knowles, K. A. (2001). bias in naming of phobia-related words. Cognition and
Who’s afraid of the big bad wolf: A prospective para- Emotion, 6, 479–486.
digm to test Rachman’s indirect pathways in children. McNally, R. J. (1996). The legacy of Seligman’s “Phobias
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1259–1276. and preparedness” (1971). Behavior Therapy, 47,
Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: 585–594.
Effects of maternal modelling on the acquisition of Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1993). A comparison
fear and avoidance behaviour in toddlers. Behaviour of in vivo and vicarious exposure in the treatment
Research and Therapy, 40, 279–287. of childhood water phobia. Behaviour Research and
Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy. Temperament in Therapy, 31, 9–15.
human nature. New York, NY: Basic Books. Muris, P. (2007). Normal and abnormal fear and anxiety
Kendall, P. C. (1994). Treating anxiety disorders in chil- in children and adolescents. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
dren: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal Muris, P., & Field, A. P. (2010). The role of verbal threat
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 100–110. information in the development of childhood fear.
Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., “Beware the Jabberwock!”. Clinical Child and Family
Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Üstün, T. B. (2007). Age of Psychology Review, 13, 129–150.
onset of mental disorders: A review of recent litera- Muris, P., & Field, A. P. (2011). The normal development
ture. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 359–364. of fear. In W. K. Silverman & A. P. Field (Eds.), Anxiety
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, disorders in children and adolescents: Research,
A. M., & Wittchen, H. U. (2012). Twelve-month and assessment and intervention (2nd ed., pp. 76–89).
lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
218 P. Muris

Muris, P., Huijding, J., Mayer, B., Den Breejen, E., & disorders: Current status. Journal of Clinical Child
Makkelie, M. (2007). Spider fear and covariation bias Psychology, 27, 156–167.
in children and adolescents. Behaviour Research and Ollendick, T. H., King, N. J., & Muris, P. (2002). Fears
Therapy, 45, 2604–2615. and phobias in children: Phenomenology, epidemi-
Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (2001). The etiology of ology, and aetiology. Child and Adolescent Mental
childhood specific phobia: A multifactorial model. Health, 7, 98–106.
In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The develop­ Ollendick, T. H., Lewis, K. M., Cowart, M. W., & Davis,
mental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 355–385). T. E. (2012). Prediction of child performance on a
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. parent-­child behavioral approach test with animal
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., De Jong, P. J., & Ollendick, phobic children. Behavior Modification, 36, 509–524.
T. H. (2002). The aetiology of specific fears and phobias Ollendick, T. H., & Muris, P. (2015). The scientific leg-
in children: A critique of the non-­associative account. acy of Little Hans and Little Albert: Future directions
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 185–195. for research on specific phobias in youth. Journal
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Holdrinet, I., & Sijsenaar, M. of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44,
(1998). Treating phobic children: Effects of EMDR 689–706.
versus exposure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Ollendick, T. H., Öst, L. G., Reuterskiold, L., Costa, N.,
Psychology, 66, 193–198. Cederlund, R., Sirbu, C., … Jarrett, M. A. (2009). One-­
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Wessel, I., & Van der Ven, M. session treatment of specific phobias in youth: A ran-
(1999). Psychopathological correlates of self-reported domized clinical trial in the United States and Sweden.
behavioural inhibition in normal children. Behaviour Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77,
Research and Therapy, 37, 575–584. 504–516.
Muris, P., Schmidt, H., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The Ollendick, T. H., Vasey, M. W., & King, N. J. (2001).
structure of specific phobia symptoms among children Operant conditioning influences in childhood anxiety.
and adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The develop­
37, 863–868. mental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 231–252).
Muris, P., Van Zwol, L., Huijding, J., & Mayer, B. (2010). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Mom told me scary things about this animal! Parents Öst, L. G. (2012). One-session treatment: Principles and
installing fear beliefs in their children via the ver- procedures with adults. In T. E. David, T. H. Ollendick,
bal information pathway. Behaviour Research and & L. G. Öst (Eds.), Intensive one-session treatment of
Therapy, 48, 341–346. specific phobias (pp. 59–95). New York, NY: Springer.
Oar, E. L., Farrell, L. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2015). One-­ Öst, L. G., & Sterner, U. (1987). Applied tension: A spe-
session treatment for specific phobias: An adaptation cific behavioral method for treatment of blood phobia.
for paediatric blood-injection-injury phobia in youth. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 25–29.
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 18, Öst, L. G., Svensson, L., Hellstrom, K., & Lindwall, R.
370–394. (2001). One-session treatment of specific phobias
Olatunji, B. O., & McKay, D. (2009). Disgust and its in youths: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of
disorders. Theory, assessment, and treatment impli­ Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 814–824.
cations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Page, A. C. (1994). Blood-injury phobia. Clinical
Association. Psychology Review, 14, 443–461.
Ollendick, T. H., Allen, B., Benoit, K. E., & Cowart, Rachman, S. (1991). Neoconditioning and the classi-
M. W. (2011). The tripartite model of fear in phobic cal theory of fear acquisition. Clinical Psychology
children: Assessing concordance and discordance Review, 17, 47–67.
using the behavioral approach test. Behavior Research Rapee, R. M., Schniering, C. A., & Hudson, J. L. (2009).
and Therapy, 49, 459–465. Anxiety disorders during childhood and adolescence:
Ollendick, T. H., Davis, T. E., & Muris, P. (2004). Origins and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical
Treatment of specific phobia in children and ado- Psychology, 5, 311–341.
lescents. In P. M. Barrett & T. H. Ollendick (Eds.), Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness.
Handbook of interventions that work with children Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.
and adolescents: Prevention and treatment (pp. 273– Silverman, W. K., Kurtiness, W. M., Ginsburg, G. S.,
299). New York, NY: Wiley. Weems, C. F., Rabian, B., & Serafini, L. T. (1999).
Ollendick, T. H., Halldorsdottir, T., Fraire, M. G., Austin, Contingency management, self-control, and education
K. E., Noguchi, R., Lewis, K. M., … Whitmore, M. J. support in the treatment of childhood phobic disorders:
(2015). Specific phobias in youth: A randomized A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and
controlled trial comparing one-session treatment to Clinical Psychology, 67, 675–687.
parent-augmented one-session treatment. Behavior Ten Berge, M., Veerkamp, J. S. J., & Hoogstraten,
Therapy, 46, 141–155. J. (2002). The etiology of childhood dental fear: The
Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1998). Empirically sup- role of dental and conditioning experiences. Journal of
ported treatments for children with phobic and anxiety Anxiety Disorders, 16, 321–329.
Specific Phobias 219

Ultee, C. A., Griffoen, D., & Schellekens, J. (1982). phobia subtypes and corresponding fears. Journal of
The reduction of anxiety in children: A comparison Anxiety Disorders, 27, 379–388.
of the effects of ‘systematic desensitization in vitro’ Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emo-
and ‘systematic desensitization in vivo’. Behaviour tional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
Research and Therapy, 20, 61–67. 3, 1–14.
Van Houtem, C. M. H. H., Laine, M. L., Boomsma, D. I., Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy and reciprocal inhibi­
Ligthart, L., Van Wijk, A. J., & De Jongh, A. (2013). A tion. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
review and meta-analysis of the heritability of specific
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders

Maysa M. Kaskas, Paige M. Ryan,
and Thompson E. Davis III

Contents ble outcomes of the feared stimuli, environment,


Treatment of Anxiety Disorders............................  221 or situation) all interact to produce the anxiety
and fear response. These components may occur
Developmental and (Trans)diagnostic
Considerations....................................................  223
together in varied intensities. When individuals
experience severe anxiety and fear that impairs
Etiology of Anxiety and Anxiety Disorders..........  223
daily functioning, these problems are classified
Maintaining Factors of Anxiety............................  226 as anxiety disorders. Individuals with anxiety
Translating Research into Practice......................  228 disorders experience excessive fear or worry,
usually an immediate physiological response
Components of Efficacious Treatments
for Childhood Anxiety Disorders......................  232 (e.g., increased heart rate), dysfunctional beliefs,
and avoidance of certain situations and/or stim-
Conclusions and Future Directions......................  236
uli. These disorders impact the ability to function
References...............................................................  237 properly in several contexts (e.g., family, school,
peer relationships, occupation; American
Psychiatric Association, 2013). The lifetime
prevalence of any anxiety disorder is estimated to
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders be approximately 30%, and the 12-month preva-
lence rate for children and adolescents is about
Anxiety is a normal emotional and behavioral 1–7%, with variability across the different anxi-
response that occurs across development and ety disorders (Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson,
serves an adaptive function. According to Lang’s Zaslavsky, & Wittchen, 2012). Thus, the demand
(1979) tripartite model, physiological responses for effective treatments is incredibly high. In
(e.g., sweating, elevated heart rate), behaviors order to better understand and effectively treat
(e.g., enduring a situation with distress, avoiding anxiety, research initiatives, including those to
situations that provoke anxiety), and negative recommend “evidence-based practice” (EBP)
cognitions (e.g., thoughts about the worst possi- guidelines in psychology and to further identify
“empirically supported treatments” (EST), have
begun to systematically gather and evaluate the
M.M. Kaskas • P.M. Ryan • T.E. Davis III (*) evidence for psychological treatments of indi-
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA viduals affected by anxiety disorders. This chap-
e-mail: ted@lsu.edu ter will focus on treatments for some of the

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 221


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_13
222 M.M. Kaskas et al.

DSM-5 categories for anxiety and obsessive-­ in impairment. This finding has important
compulsive disorders (i.e., separation anxiety implications for treatment, as it suggests that
­
disorder, social anxiety disorder, selective mut- when treating individuals with comorbid anxiety
ism, panic disorder, agoraphobia, generalized disorders, clinicians may wish to focus on treat-
anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder; ment of generalized anxiety disorder symptoms
for information on specific phobia, see chapter first. Clinicians may also treat symptoms of gen-
“Tics and Tourette Syndrome”). eralized anxiety transdiagnostically in order to
Children with anxiety disorders are likely to have the largest effects on severity and impair-
experience significant impairments in broad areas ment of functioning.
of functioning. Many children are initially
referred to treatment due to concerns about aca- Table 1 Characteristic symptoms of select anxiety
demic functioning, which is significantly corre- disorders
lated with anxiety severity across race, gender,
Disorder Description
and age (Nail et al., 2015). Academic functioning Separation Persistent worry, disproportionate
is also positively correlated with global function- anxiety concern, and distress about separating
ing, suggesting that children’s academic func- disorder from an attachment figure or from the
tioning may greatly impact other areas of their home; worries may include harm
befalling on the parents, the child him-/
functioning. Notably, both anxiety severity and herself, or the home when separated or
academic impairments have been found to fears that the parent will never return
decrease with treatments such as cognitive behav- Social Intense distress/fear in or anxiety about
ioral therapy (Nail et al., 2015). anxiety social situations or performances in
It has also been noted that the level of impair- disorder which the primary concern is negative
evaluation from others, typically leading
ment may differ not only across anxiety severity to the avoidance of those situations
but also across disorders (see Table 1 for more Generalized Persistent, uncontrollable worry across
information on select anxiety disorders). anxiety multiple domains (e.g., doing things
Researchers have found that both adults and chil- disorder perfectly, making good impressions on
others, performance, world events)
dren with certain anxiety disorders [i.e., general-
occurring more days than not that is
ized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder, and associated with the presence of at least
panic disorder] have less satisfaction with their one physical symptom (e.g., aches and
overall quality of life as compared to non-­anxious pains, feelings of restlessness, trouble
concentrating)
individuals (Barrera & Norton, 2009; Ramsawh
Panic Usually unexpected anxiety reaction
& Chavira, 2016). Naragon-Gainey, Gallagher, &
disorder (i.e., panic attack) leading to intense
Brown (2014) assessed the degree to which physiological symptoms (e.g., heart
symptoms of anxiety interfered with five differ- palpitations, sweating, shortness of
ent areas of daily functioning (i.e., private lei- breath); the person may experience
significant worry and concern about
sure, work, household tasks, social leisure, family these reactions and avoid situations
relationships) throughout a period of 2 years. where these reactions could occur
When comparing treatment-seeking individuals Selective Consistent failure to speak in certain
with diagnoses of generalized anxiety disorder, mutism situations where the person is required
social anxiety disorder, and/or panic disorder, to do so due to debilitating fear; the
behavior causes significant impairment
results suggested that individuals with a primary in academic, occupational, and/or social
diagnosis of generalized anxiety disorder experi- settings
enced the most impairment at baseline. While Obsessive-­ Persistent and distressing obsessions
anxiety symptoms and impairment in all groups compulsive (thoughts or impulses) and/or
declined over time with treatment, change in disorder compulsions (repetitive compensatory
behaviors) that significantly impair
severity of symptoms of generalized anxiety functioning for at least an hour per day
disorder was most specifically related to change
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 223

 evelopmental and (Trans)
D maintenance of anxiety disorders not only across
diagnostic Considerations child development but also across the life-span.
Thus, these biological, cognitive, and environ-
The prevalence, phenomenology, and etiology of mental factors are important to dissect individu-
a child’s anxiety are often dependent on the ally in order to further understand their relations
child’s developmental stage. For example, to each other across development and to treat-
younger children are much more likely to develop ment outcome.
separation anxiety than a more generalized worry
due to developmental (particularly cognitive)
constraints (Gullone, 2000). Younger children Temperamental Differences
may experience fear only in the moment of sepa-
ration without also experiencing the typically Behavioral inhibition, or the tendency to with-
occurring persistent worry following the separa- draw from or be fearful in unfamiliar situations,
tion. As these children get older and their cogni- is often discussed as a contributing factor to the
tive abilities become more advanced, development of anxiety disorders. Biederman
future-oriented worries about specific dangers and colleagues (2001) suggested that children
(e.g., accidents, kidnapping, death) or vague con- aged 2–6 years who were classified as “behavior-
cerns about families not reuniting may emerge. ally inhibited” were more likely to develop anxi-
In general, children tend to move from more spe- ety disorders than their peers without behavioral
cific, concrete fears (e.g., separation, loud noises) inhibition. Children are more likely to be behav-
to more abstract anxiety and worries (e.g., criti- iorally inhibited if one or more parent(s) has an
cism, anticipatory worries, war) as their cognitive anxiety disorder (e.g., agoraphobia and/or panic
abilities develop (Gullone, 2000). Another disorder) themselves (Hirshfeld, Biederman,
important developmental consideration includes Brody, Faraone, & Rosenbaum, 1997; Rosenbaum
the child’s language abilities, which may facili- et al., 1992). These results are an example of the
tate or contribute to anxiety in social situations. high rates of parental psychopathology seen
For example, selective mutism often has an early across children with anxiety disorders, present-
onset (i.e., on or near initial entry into school), ing questions about the roles of genetics and par-
and research has suggested that children with enting styles.
selective mutism may experience expressive lan-
guage deficits compared to their peers, contribut-
ing to anxiety in social situations (McInnes, Biological Differences
Fung, Manassis, Fiksenbaum, & Tannock, 2004).
Similarly, the onset of social anxiety disorder Genetic  There is substantial evidence that chil-
(formally known as social phobia) is usually dren who have a parent (or parents) with anxiety
much later in childhood, when peers are of are more likely to develop anxiety disorders
greater importance to the child and peer rejection themselves. Studies on the etiology of
becomes an increasingly salient stressor (Coie, internalizing disorders suggest that approxi-
­
Dodge, & Kupersmidt, 1990). mately 30% of variance in anxiety can be
explained by genetic factors (Schrock &
Woodruff-Borden, 2010). Large-scale studies
 tiology of Anxiety and Anxiety
E have identified genetic factors that account for
Disorders about half of the genetic variance across anxiety
disorders (Middledorp, Cath, van Dyck, &
Most researchers agree that anxiety disorders Boomsa, 2005). Notably, the level of genetic vul-
develop through a complex integration of bio- nerability is inconsistent across studies of broad
logical, cognitive, and environmental factors. anxiety disorders, as well as within different anx-
These factors may change in prominence in the iety disorders. Some researchers have been able
224 M.M. Kaskas et al.

to find specific candidate genes in specific anxi- disorder may exhibit different physiological
ety disorders (e.g., associates with the serotoner- responses associated with fear pathways com-
gic (5-HT) and dopaminergic pathways in social pared to non-anxious children. For example, chil-
anxiety disorder, SAPAP3 variants in obsessive- dren with social anxiety disorder display
compulsive disorder; Boardman et al., 2011). indicators of higher baseline sympathetic activity
However, similar studies have been unsuccessful and lower parasympathetic activation (Krämer
in finding the same specific genes associated with et al., 2012; Schmitz, Krämer, Tuschen-Caffier,
specific disorders; due to this inconsistency, some Heinrichs, & Blechert, 2011). Similarly, children
researchers believe these genes reflect the general with anxiety disorders have even been found to
familial predisposition to anxiety as opposed to a have lower scores on IQ tests than children with-
specific gene-to-disorder match (Knappe, out psychopathology (Davis, Ollendick, &
Sasagawa, & Creswell, 2015). Within genetic Nebel-Schwalm, 2008), and the combination of
studies, there are underlying biological and tem- anxiety and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
peramental vulnerabilities that should be noted der has been shown to lead to greater impairment
(e.g., Hirshfeld et al., 1997; Rosenbaum et al., than simply attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
1992; the abovementioned studies that found that der alone (e.g., working memory impairments;
behavioral inhibition is more common in chil- Jarrett, Wolff, Davis, Cowart, & Ollendick,
dren of parents with anxiety disorders). 2016).

Neurological  Youth with anxiety disorders may


show a biological predisposition to an increased Environmental Differences
vigilance for threat, which can be demonstrated
through neuroimaging research. Monk et al. The differences in environment between youth
(2008) found that children and adolescents with a with and without anxiety disorders have also
diagnosis of generalized anxiety disorder were been the focus of much research. Specifically, the
more likely than comparison peers without a psy- family environment has been of great interest due
chological/psychiatric diagnosis to show right to both the amount of time children spend in the
amygdala hyperactivation in response to rapidly home and the significant influence family envi-
presented threatening stimuli. Although such ronment has on the child. This environment may
studies are still preliminary in nature, there is be even more influential if the environment is
some evidence to suggest that youth with a characterized as “high risk” (Burt, 2009).
“behaviorally inhibited” temperament show Relevant factors within the family environment
heightened striatal reactivity in the caudate to consider include parental anxiety, parental
nucleus and the putamen region, suggestive of control, parental modeling, attachment styles,
increased sensitivity to rewards, heightened vigi- and stressors such as low socioeconomic status
lance toward evaluation their performance, and (Ollendick & Benoit, 2012).
excessive valuing of positive outcomes (Caouette
& Guyer, 2014). Individuals with anxiety disor- Parental Anxiety  An anxious parenting style,
ders may also have deficits in inhibitory learning, including displaying excessive worry and con-
which prevents the extinguishing of an anxious cern about children’s safety, may increase chil-
response, even after repeated experiences with dren’s symptoms of anxiety; however, this effect
the object of the fear or anxiety (Craske, Treanor, was only found when children reported perceiv-
Conway, Zbozinek, & Vervliet, 2014). These ing their parents’ anxiety level (Grüner, Muris, &
findings might explain the maintenance of anxi- Merckelbach, 1999). Researchers have investi-
ety if and when youth experience a negative gated the impact of parental anxiety alone on
interaction with an anxiety-provoking stimulus child anxiety, after controlling for family size,
and additionally suggest a powerful target for socioeconomic status, child birth order, parental
treatment. Similarly, children with social anxiety education level, and parental occupation. Results
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 225

suggest that parental anxiety is associated with 2008). While parents may wish to protect their
less parental communication with children and a children from anxiety-provoking or distressing
more controlling parenting style. Thus, the situations, children need opportunities to test
parent-­child relationship was less warm and hypotheses and coping strategies; after all, it is
affectionate, and children reported more feelings only through experience that children learn what
of disappointment, rejection, and unhappiness. situations they can and cannot navigate indepen-
Children with anxious parents also exhibited dently. For example, a child with a predisposition
more withdrawn behavior than children without to fear dogs may not have the opportunity to
anxious parents; this withdrawn behavior had a interact with dogs, understand what distinguishes
negative impact on academic functioning, extra- a friendly dog from an unfriendly dog, and test
curricular activities, and social skills. These find- out his own capacity to tolerate fear if the child’s
ings suggest that parental anxiety can even affect overcontrolling parent limits or abbreviates the
the child’s ability to establish future secure rela- child’s interactions with dogs due to a desire to
tionships (Rao & Ram, 1984). reduce the child’s anxiety-related distress (cf.
Anxiety in adults may hinder the development Davis, Ollendick, & Öst, 2009, 2012).
of adaptive coping skills, which may cause There is a strong theoretical foundation for the
anxiety-­enhancing parenting behaviors such as linkages between parental anxiety, child anxiety,
modeling avoidance of unknown or unfamiliar and parental control, suggesting that parents who
situations (Ginsburg & Schlossberg, 2002). are anxious seek to exert control in unknown or
Additionally, anxious parents may have a ten- uncertain situations, thereby increasing the
dency to interpret ambiguous or novel situations child’s level of anxiety in these situations (Wood,
as threatening for their children, perhaps leading McLeod, Sigman, Hwang, & Chu, 2003). A
to increased control as a means of ensuring avoid- meta-analysis of 17 studies examined the con-
ance of these situations (van der Bruggen, Stams, nection between child anxiety and parental con-
& Bögels, 2008). Rapee (2001) suggested par- trol, finding a medium-to-large and significant
ents may grow accustomed to making decisions effect size of d = 0.58 (CI 0.51 < d < 0.64; van der
for their anxious children, eventually exerting Bruggen et al., 2008). These findings strongly
control in anticipation of their anxious child’s suggest higher levels of child anxiety are associ-
distress. While these parental behaviors prevent ated with higher levels of parental control.
the child from experiencing anxiety-related dis- Further research has indicated that parenting
tress, they also serve to exacerbate and maintain variables, such as parental control and parental
the child’s anxiety. warmth, may account for approximately 4% of
the variance in child anxiety (McLeod, Weisz, &
Parental control  Parental control may contrib- Wood, 2007).
ute to the development and/or maintenance of
childhood anxiety by increasing the child’s per- Parental modeling  Parents may model anxiety
ception of threat; for example, children may for their children through their own behaviors
observe their parent(s) taking over for them in (e.g., worrying aloud; screaming, crying, or pan-
situations and conclude that the situation must be icking when encountering feared stimuli or situa-
too dangerous or risky for them to navigate on tions; avoiding feared, unfamiliar, or uncertain
their own (Rapee, 2001). Subsequently, high lev- stimuli or situations), through relaying negative
els of parental control may reduce the child’s per- information (e.g., suggesting an abundance of
ceived control over threat, decreasing the child’s threat, providing incomplete or skewed informa-
confidence and perceived competence. tion on the likelihood that a negative outcome
Additionally, parental overcontrol reduces the might occur), and/or through reinforcing their
number of opportunities a child has to explore children’s avoidant behavior (e.g., allowing their
their environment and obtain and practice appro- children to escape or avoid encountering feared,
priate coping skills (van der Bruggen et al., unfamiliar, or uncertain stimuli or situations).
226 M.M. Kaskas et al.

This theory of parental modeling may account Maintaining Factors of Anxiety


for anxiety in children, particularly for social
worries (Fisak & Grills-Taquechel, 2007). Cognitive Theories
Anxious parents may model specific behaviors
such as social inhibition for their children, lead- Negative patterns of thinking, such as conscious
ing to the development of anxiety disorders such perceptions of sensations, stimuli, and situations
as selective mutism (Kristensen & Torgersen, and expectancies for outcomes, are significant
2001). Parental modeling and its relation to factors in the maintenance of anxiety. Therefore,
restriction of appropriate learning experiences these mechanisms of change are important to
has also been examined. Findings suggest that understand in order to effectively treat anxiety.
families with anxious parents behave differently Individuals with anxiety disorders often perceive
than non-anxious controls even during routine objectively neutral or ambiguous stimuli and sit-
activities: although anxious parents did not uations as threatening, which triggers a maladap-
directly restrict their children’s activities, they tive cycle of thoughts, perceptions, and behaviors.
did display visible distress, while their children This preoccupation in turn interferes with the
engaged in both routine tasks and structured play child’s ability to focus attention on the details of
activities. Through observational learning, chil- the stimuli/situation that are nonthreatening (e.g.,
dren may begin to associate such typical, age-­ noticing the peers who are kindly smiling or who
appropriate activities with a stress response that are not even focused on the child), further exacer-
their parent(s) previously displayed, increasing bating and maintaining the anxiety (Alfano,
the likelihood of an anxiety disorder (Turner, Beidel, & Turner, 2006).
Beidel, Roberson-Nay, & Tervo, 2003). Overall, there is an evidence to show that chil-
Thus far, child temperament, parental anxiety, dren and adolescents with anxiety disorders, par-
parental overcontrol, and parental modeling and ticularly those with generalized anxiety disorder
their relationships to the development and main- and/or social anxiety disorder, experience more
tenance of child anxiety have been discussed. negative cognitions than their non-anxious peers
Although these factors were introduced some- both before entering a situation/encountering a
what separately, it is important to note that they feared stimulus (i.e., anticipatory cognitions
are not orthogonal but rather are related in several about the possible negative outcomes of situa-
specific ways. Ollendick and Benoit (2012) iden- tions, underestimating their ability to cope with a
tified a five-risk factor model that explains a large negative outcome) and while engaged in a situa-
proportion of the variance in the development of tion/facing a feared stimulus (i.e., more negative
social anxiety disorder: the child’s level of cognitions regarding the quality of their own per-
“behavioral inhibition” or anxious temperament formance, more negative self-talk during a task
(as discussed above), parental anxiety, attach- or while in a situation; Alfano et al., 2006; Blöte,
ment process, information processing biases, and Miers, Heyne, Clark, & Westenberg, 2014;
parenting practices (including modeling). While Bögels & Zigterman, 2000; Castagna, Davis, &
behavioral inhibition is often thought to be Lilly, in press; Kley, Tuschen-Caffier, &
genetic and stable, it can be further reinforced by Heinrichs, 2012; Ranta, Tuomisto,
parenting practices. For example, anxious par- ­Kaltiala-­Heino, Rantanen, & Marttunen, 2014).
ents can reinforce behavioral inhibition in their These negative cognitions have also been found
children through modeling of avoidance and pro- to persist after children or adolescents with anxi-
viding negative information about sources of fear ety disorders exit a situation/experience with a
or worry. feared stimulus, in the form of more negative
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 227

post-event processing, excessive rumination, and (In-Albon, Kossowsky, & Schneider, 2010;
persistently negative, distorted self-perceptions Seefeldt, Krämer, Tuschen-Caffier, & Heinrichs,
(Hodson, McManus, Clark, & Doll, 2008; 2014). However, this phenomenon reduces sig-
Schmitz, Krämer, Blechert, & Tuschen-Caffier, nificantly after the provision of treatment for
2010; Spence & Rapee, 2016). This cycle of neg- anxiety disorders; for example, a sample of 18
ative cognitions increases the likelihood that children with separation anxiety disorder showed
youth encountering a similar situation/stimulus significant improvement in both symptomology
in the future will again experience a significant and hypervigilance toward threat after cognitive
anxiety response or avoid the encounter entirely, behavioral therapy (CBT). This finding lends
and both responses work to maintain and exacer- strong support to the efficacy of treatment for
bate anxiety. This makes cognitions a powerful both the apparent and automatic symptoms of
target for treatment. anxiety disorders (In-Albon & Schneider, 2012).

Safety behaviors  Avoiding feared situations or


Behavioral Theories stimuli completely is not always a possibility,
especially for children and adolescents who may
Avoidance  Anxiety often manifests behavior- be unable to control their environments. When
ally as general avoidance of the feared stimulus avoidance is not possible, youth with anxiety dis-
or situation; for example, a child who fears nega- orders may engage in safety behaviors, which are
tive evaluation from peers may withdraw from or strategies that individuals engage in the presence
avoid situations involving peer interactions or of the feared stimulus or situation in order to
performance evaluation, choosing not to read reduce feelings of anxiety and the possibility of a
aloud in class or play on a sports team. Avoidance negative outcome. A child who is afraid of
may reduce a child’s distress in the short term, heights might close his eyes while going down a
but avoidant behavior actually maintains anxiety slide, a child with anxiety about speaking to
by restricting the possibility of a positive or neu- adults might avoid eye contact, or a teenager with
tral experience with the feared situation/stimulus, social concerns might cover her face while speak-
reducing the opportunities to challenge dysfunc- ing to disguise perceived flushing or sweating.
tional, negative cognitions about the situation/ Although youth with anxiety disorders engage in
stimulus, and decreasing the occasions the child safety behaviors with the intention to prevent
has to practice appropriate coping skills or strate- negative outcomes, the safety behaviors typically
gies (Rapee, 2001). As well, the decrease in have the opposite effect, by drawing more atten-
physiological sensations accompanying with- tion to the youth or increasing the likelihood of a
drawal from an anxiety-provoking situation is negative outcome (e.g., covering one’s face may
often observed to be negatively reinforcing itself be perceived as unusual). Additionally, safety
(Mowrer, 1947). behaviors typically prime anxious individuals to
Research in cognitive psychology has led to attribute any positive or neutral outcome to the
some interesting findings regarding anxious pop- safety behavior rather than to any fact of the situ-
ulations and avoidant behavior. The vigilance-­ ation/stimulus or to the individuals’ own skills or
avoidance model states that anxiety dictates knowledge. This prevents full exposure to the
initial vigilant attention (often hypervigilance) stimulus or situation and reduces the likelihood
toward the threatening stimulus followed by a that the experience will generalize (Kley et al.,
rapid avoidance of attention from the stimulus. 2012). Thus, safety behaviors help to maintain
This effect has been demonstrated through eye threat expectancies and other negative cognitions
tracking technology in both adults and children about the situation/stimulus, even in the absence
with anxiety disorders, including separation of negative experiences (Spence & Rapee, 2016).
anxiety disorder and social anxiety disorder The extinction of safety behaviors should be a
­
228 M.M. Kaskas et al.

part of treatment, as they serve to increase the both efficacy (i.e., evaluating evidence on causal
likelihood of negative outcomes, facilitate further relationships between treatment and disorders)
avoidant behavior, and hamper realistic interpre- and clinical utility (i.e., assessing evidence from
tations of positive or neutral experiences. research and clinical experiences on the general-
izability, feasibility, client preferences, and a
Skill deficits  In some circumstances, youth with cost/benefit analysis of each intervention) in
anxiety disorders may be exhibiting avoidance or order to determine whether a particular interven-
safety behaviors due to skill deficits, which also tion approach may be considered evidence-based.
need to be addressed in treatment (Davis & The APA’s determination of which interventions
Ollendick, 2005). For example, social skills defi- qualify as EBPP has had significant reverbera-
cits (e.g., poor eye contact, poor reciprocal con- tions in healthcare policy, insurance coverage,
versation skills) may have led to negative prior and media exposure. For example, specific states
experiences with peers and contributed to the require mental health interventions to be listed as
development of symptoms of social anxiety. EBPP in order to be covered under Medicaid pro-
Without remedy, those social skill deficits may grams (American Psychological Association
maintain or contribute to increases in anxiety Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based
(Miers, Blöte, de Rooij, Bokhorst, & Westenberg, Practice [APA Presidential Task Force], 2006).
2013). Additionally, specific skill deficits such as While the EBPP movement is not without contro-
poor eye contact and limited verbal initiations in versy, it has made significant progress in consoli-
conversation are also associated with behavioral dating the available evidence from research and
inhibition, which may interact to further interfere practice to determine that psychological inter-
with functioning, facilitate avoidant behaviors, ventions are both safe and effective for all age
and contribute to negative cognitions (Rapee, groups and for a variety of psychological and
2002). Given that these skill deficits can impede relational concerns (Kazdin & Weisz, 2003;
the progress of treatment, targeted skill training Weisz, Hawley, & Doss, 2004). Additionally, the
as a part of anxiety treatment may increase the EBPP movement has determined that psycholog-
likelihood of treatment success and also improve ical therapy often pays for itself in terms of medi-
maintenance of treatment gains. cal cost offset, increased productivity (e.g.,
regained or improved occupational, relational,
academic, social competencies), and quality of
Translating Research into Practice life (Chiles, Lambert, & Hatch, 2002; Yates,
1994). However, the quality and efficacy of psy-
Evidence-Based Practice chological interventions are of paramount impor-
in Psychology (EBPP) tance in order to see these benefits.

Information on the phenomenology, epidemiol- Evaluating research evidence and clinical


ogy, etiology, and maintenance of anxiety disor- expertise  While EBPP evaluated scientific
ders in children and adolescents is important to results from multiple research designs (e.g., clini-
understand in the development and implementa- cal observation, systematic case studies, experi-
tion of efficacious treatments. One particular mental designs, public health studies,
research-based initiative, evidence-based prac- meta-analyses), randomized controlled trials
tice in psychology (EBPP), began in 2005 when (RCTs) were weighted most heavily in drawing
the American Psychological Association (APA) conclusions about intervention efficacy.
was charged with developing treatment guide- According to the APA Presidential Task Force
lines for practitioners. In order to better facilitate (2006), RCTs “are the most effective way to rule
efficacious treatment of psychological disorders, out threats to internal validity [i.e., avoiding con-
the APA assembled a task force to integrate founding variables] in a single experiment”
research evidence with clinical expertise and cli- (p. 275). In evaluating the available evidence, it
ent preferences. This entailed examination of was found that the type of treatment, qualities
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 229

related to the practitioner him-/herself (e.g., lar psychological disorder or class of disorders. If
training and competencies), the therapeutic rela- there is no sufficient or strong research support
tionship, and variables associated with the client on a particular disorder or clinical need, clini-
him-/herself are all related to the efficacy of cians should exercise judgment in interpreting
interventions. The APA Presidential Task Force extant evidence and applying the best available
(2006) did not rely on only empirical evidence; intervention while carefully monitoring client
they also worked with professionals to identify progress and modifying treatment as needed
relevant tips for treatment coming from clinical (Lambert, Harmon, Slade, Whipple, & Hawkins,
expertise (i.e., competencies acquired through 2005). Finally, clinicians must critically evaluate
formal education, training, and experiences). The their own levels of training and expertise—a cli-
committee recommended that psychologists nician without the proper competencies to imple-
avoid errors in judgment such as overgeneraliza- ment a specific treatment must refer the client
tions or confirmatory biases based on previous elsewhere or seek consultation from other profes-
experiences by being cognizant of the limitations sionals (APA Presidential Task Force, 2006). A
of one’s knowledge and skills, learning about core set of therapeutic competencies have been
heuristics and biases in judgment, seeking con- identified by Sburlati, Schniering, Lyneham, and
sultation or supervision from other professionals, Rapee (2011); most centrally, these competen-
and obtaining systematic feedback from the cli- cies include understanding of and adherence to
ent (APA Presidential Task Force, 2006). professional, legal, and ethical codes in addition
to the following abilities: accessing, critically
Recommendations before beginning treat- evaluating, and using research to advise practice,
ment  According to the APA Presidential Task honestly judging one’s own skill levels, integrat-
Force (2006), expert clinicians must be able to ing multiple methods (e.g., diagnostic interviews,
conduct a sound assessment before beginning questionnaires, observations) and multiple infor-
treatment, including developing a clear case con- mants (e.g., child, parent, teacher) in conducting
ceptualization based upon information about the a thorough psychological assessment, engaging
client/client’s family sociocultural context, and maintaining a positive therapeutic alliance
assessment of psychopathology and strengths with the client, communicating rationale for spe-
(i.e., clinically relevant strengths that can be cific techniques and skills chosen in treatment,
incorporated in or used to reinforce treatment), and pacing and structuring a session appropri-
and an accurate profile of diagnostic impressions. ately for a child’s developmental level and skill-
Additionally, it is crucial to continually revisit set (e.g., using age-appropriate worksheets, token
and amend the case conceptualizations through- economies, interactive activities).
out the treatment process. In selecting an inter- It is recommended that clinicians incorporate
vention strategy for children and adolescents clients in planning intervention by asking clients
with anxiety disorders, clinicians should first to identify their goals for treatment (APA
understand the clients’ needs, skill level, and Presidential Task Force, 2006). When working
strengths. According to the APA Presidential with children and adolescents, it may be helpful
Task Force (2006)’s compendium of research on to ask incorporate family members in the goal-­
interventions, many other client-related variables setting process, as youth may have limited insight
may significantly influence treatment implemen- into the interference caused by their anxiety and/
tation and outcomes; these variables are dis- or limited ability to articulate what they would
played in Table 2 along with corresponding like to see change in their lives. Clinicians may
examples of modifications that may be ask youth specific questions to guide their think-
beneficial. ing and goal setting, such as: “Is there anywhere
Second, clinicians should select a treatment that you cannot go because you get nervous or
by balancing knowledge of client preferences and scared?,” “Does being nervous ever get in the
unique characteristics, professional judgment, way of doing the things that you want or need to
and the available research evidence for a particu- do?,” and/or “If we could wave a magic wand and
230 M.M. Kaskas et al.

Table 2  Client variables to consider when choosing and implementing intervention


Client-related variable Example of modification
Level of functioning (e.g., cognitive, adaptive, A child or adolescent functioning at a lower level may require
executive, academic) more concrete visual stimuli (e.g., pictures, visual reminders,
charts) during treatment as well as a slower pace of treatment
with more time dedicated to review and practice of skills
Readiness for change and motivation for treatment A child or adolescent with low motivation may benefit from
reevaluation of goals for treatment and implementation of a
reward system contingent on desired behaviors (e.g., game or
activity before session for completing homework, sticker chart
during session for effort)
Level of social support (e.g., strength of peer and A child or adolescent who is experiencing peer victimization
familial relationships, experience of peer may require additional sessions on communication training
victimization, community membership) (e.g., assertive communication) and problem-solving skills
Variations in presenting problems, etiology of A child or adolescent who is depressed in addition to being
symptoms, and comorbidity highly anxious may benefit from adding behavioral activation
to the treatment plan
Behavioral difficulties, such as disruptive A child or adolescent with disruptive or defiant behaviors may
behaviors or defiance require supplemental strategies such as parent management
training, particularly if these behaviors interfere with treatment
Chronological age, developmental status, A child typically requires simpler language and more concrete
developmental history, and life stage examples than an adolescent
Sociocultural factors (e.g., gender, gender identity, A child or adolescent from a marginalized group may benefit
race, ethnicity, social class, language acquisition from more extensive rapport building in order to address
status) concerns such as medical mistrust based on power differentials
in therapy
Environmental context (e.g., safety of home and A child or adolescent from a turbulent home environment may
neighborhood, access to resources) and current benefit from more practice with relaxation and coping skills
stressors (e.g., life events such as moves, deaths in
the family, parent unemployment, divorce)
Personal preferences, values, and expectations for A child or adolescent may benefit from more explicit norm
treatment setting at the beginning of treatment to build realistic
expectations for how therapy sessions are structured, what is
expected of each person, and how much can change through
intervention

get rid of your worries right now, how would your by research and a desire for better client out-
life change?.” These goals should be revisited with comes, they differ in approach. EBPP is a more
the client throughout treatment and considered an holistic, comprehensive approach to using
important outcome of intervention success along research to improve clinical practice; it incorpo-
with overall symptom reduction, prevention of rates a broad range of clinical activities, begin-
future episodes, quality of life, and adaptive ning with psychological assessment, delving
functioning across domains. into case formulation and treatment planning,
and including aspects of the therapeutic rela-
tionship, which permeate the treatment process.
Empirically Supported Treatments EBPP begins by examining a particular type of
client and questions what evidence may guide
Often compared (and often confused) with the practitioner in achieving the best outcome
EBPP are empirically supported treatments for the particular client. Through the process of
(ESTs). While the two initiatives are both fueled EBPP, a practitioner is (ideally) able to shape
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 231

the entire scope of intervention, making Table 3  Criteria for evaluating empirically supported
treatments
­decisions about which treatment(s) to select and
how to implement and modify treatment through Level of
evidence Criteria
the use of an integrated stream of esearch, clini-
Level I: A. A minimum of two good between-
cal judgment and previous e­xperiences, and
well-­ group design experiments
knowledge of patient characteristics and prefer- established demonstrating efficacy in one or more
ences (APA Presidential Task Force, 2006). treatment of these ways:
In contrast, EST is more focused on the treat-  1. Statistically superior to
ment itself; in fact, it begins with a particular type psychological placebo, pill, or
another treatment
of treatment (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy)
 2. Equivalent to an already
and then examines the extant evidence on that established treatment in experiments
treatment approach, using the research to guide with adequate statistical power (i.e.,
recommendations on how practitioners may about 30 participants per group)
improve client outcomes using that treatment. Or
Therefore, specific psychological treatments B. A large series of single-case design
experiments (n > 9) demonstrating
which have been found to be efficacious in con-
considerable efficacy using good
trolled clinical trials are said to be ESTs (APA experimental design and comparing the
Presidential Task Force, 2006; Ollendick, King, & intervention to another treatment
Chorpita, 2006). There are four categories of  All well-established treatments must
treatment efficacy, Level I, well-established treat- have:
ments; Level II, probably efficacious treatments;    A treatment manual
   Reliably defined inclusion criteria
Level III, possibly efficacious treatments; and
   Clearly described sample
Level IV, experimental treatments, as described in characteristics
Table  3. In evaluating treatments, the task force    Reliable and valid outcome
behind ESTs emphasizes “good” experimental measure
designs, that is, experiments where variables are    Appropriate data analyses
controlled so that effects observed may reason-  Effects must have been demonstrated
ably be attributed to the intervention (rather than by at least two independent
researchers or research teams
changes due to chance, the passage of time, or
Level II: A. At least two experiments showing the
another confounding variable). Therefore, there is probably experiment is superior to a waitlist
a heavy focus on evaluating RCTs to determine efficacious control group
ESTs, similar to the EBPP approach. However, treatment Or
there are few RCTs that focus on individual child- B. At least one experiment meeting the
hood anxiety disorders (Ollendick et al., 2006). Level I criteria with the exception of
having been conducted in at least two
independent research settings and by
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy  According to independent investigatory teams
Davis, May, and Whiting (2011), cognitive Level III: At least one good study showing the
behavioral therapy (CBT) meets criteria for a possibly treatment to be efficacious in the
well-established treatment for both general child- efficacious absence of conflicting evidence
treatment
hood anxiety disorders and obsessive-compulsive
Level IV: Treatments which have not yet been
disorder. A recent landmark RCT ­ (Child/ experimental tested in trials which meet criteria for
Adolescent Anxiety Multimodal Study) com- treatments methodology, including newly
pared outcomes for 488 youth with diagnoses of developed treatments
either separation anxiety disorder, generalized Adapted from Chambless and Hollon (1998)
232 M.M. Kaskas et al.

anxiety disorder, or social anxiety disorder who  omponents of Efficacious


C
were randomized to one of the following groups: Treatments for Childhood Anxiety
CBT alone, medication alone, combination of Disorders
CBT and medication, or placebo (i.e., pill pla-
cebo with “medication management” check-ins As CBT is likely the most efficacious course of
with a provider; Compton et al., 2010). When treatment for youth with anxiety disorders (Davis
treatment response, remission rates, and changes et al., 2011; Piacentini et al., 2014; Read, Puleo,
in anxiety severity were compared between Wei, Cummings, & Kendall, 2013), this chapter
groups, the combination of CBT and medication will largely focus on components of CBT. CBT
slightly outperformed both monotherapies, while addresses three core areas: distorted and cata-
CBT alone and medication alone provided statis- strophic cognitions, dysfunctional behaviors
tically equivalent benefits. Notably, however, at (e.g., avoidance), and identification of emotions
follow-up, the differences between the three and physiological symptoms (Chorpita, 2007;
active treatment groups (i.e., CBT alone, medica- Davis & Ollendick, 2005; Friedberg & McClure,
tion alone, and combined) were no longer signifi- 2015). While individual treatment manuals may
cant, and all three groups showcased sustained differ in ordering and pacing of components, the
benefit from treatment. This provides strong following steps are recommended in treating
evidence for the efficacy of CBT across anxiety anxious children: psychoeducation, relaxation
disorders and is particularly significant given the training/somatic management, cognitive restruc-
common negative side effects associated with turing, problem solving, and exposure tasks
medication (Piacentini et al., 2014). (Read et al., 2013). Additional components or
alterations may be beneficial based on a client’s
Incorporating Family Members  CBT with the specific characteristics, including presence of
addition of family anxiety management (a com- comorbidities, level of functioning, and motiva-
ponent wherein parents/caregivers receive train- tion for treatment (refer to Table 2 for examples
ing to identify and target their own anxiety, learn of treatment modifications).
communication and problem-solving skills, and
are trained in contingency management strategies
to improve their responses to their child’s anxi- Specific Components of CBT
ety) meets criteria for a probably efficacious
treatment at this time (Davis et al., 2011; Kendall Exposure  Behavioral avoidance is an extremely
& Suveg, 2006; Ollendick et al., 2006). However, common and impairing feature of anxiety disor-
the evidence on incorporating family members ders, and it works to maintain problematic fear
into treatment is mixed and inconclusive at this and anxiety responses. Exposure is a systematic
time, with some studies showing incremental and hierarchical presentation of feared situations/
benefits and others suggesting no difference stimuli that provides clients with the opportunity
between parent-augmented CBT and CBT alone to practice learned skills and cope with their fear/
(as noted in Kendall & Suveg, 2006). Inclusion of anxiety without resorting to avoidance. Exposure
family members in the child’s anxiety treatment is used in approximately 80% of all anxiety treat-
through separate parent training sessions, sepa- ment models (CBT and other models; Chorpita &
rate adult anxiety management sessions, and/or Daleiden, 2009). When conducting exposure, the
active involvement in the child’s sessions may first step is building a fear hierarchy using the cli-
benefit some clients and not others; for example, ent’s ratings of feared situations/stimuli, from the
highly anxious parents, parents who lack founda- least feared (and likely easiest step) to the most
tional behavior management skills, or children catastrophic (and likely most impairing) fear.
who lack insight may benefit from such modifi- Younger or lower functioning children may have
cation of CBT. more difficulty with the abstract nature of this
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 233

process; therefore, the parents’ ratings of the exposures and sets the stage for more challenging
child’s fears are typically incorporated. During items on the hierarchy (Davis, 2009). Tracking
the exposures, clients test out their coping skills exposures using progress monitoring sheets can
(i.e., relaxation, somatic management, cognitive improve clients’ feelings of autonomy and self-­
restructuring, problem solving) and differentiate efficacy. It is recommended that, as appropriate,
between their anxious perceptions and the facts exposure exercises occur in multiple settings to
about the actual threat. Through exposure exer- facilitate generalization of learning and coping
cises, clients discover that the expected/feared (Chorpita, 2007).
outcome (e.g., “Everyone will laugh at me”) does
not match the actual outcome, finding that they Variations on exposure  Although exposure exer-
are able to successfully manage fear and cope cises may look very different from client to cli-
with the eventual outcome. The goal is for clients ent, many forms of exposure have been found to
to have successful experiences dealing with dis- be efficacious. A meta-analysis concluded that
tressing situations without avoidance or safety interventions with in vivo exposures outperform
behaviors (Chorpita & Daleiden, 2009; Read alternative forms of exposure (e.g., virtual reality
et al., 2013). exposure, imaginal exposure) directly after treat-
There are two primary methods of conduct- ment; however, the discrepancy was absent at
ing exposures: feared situations/stimuli can follow-up, suggesting that alternative modes of
either be directly experienced in session (in exposure are as effective as direct exposures in
vivo) or imagined (imaginal; in vitro). Imaginal maintaining treatment gains (Wolitzky-Taylor,
exposures are helpful when the object of fear or Horowitz, Powers, & Telch, 2008). Exposures
anxiety is difficult to procure (e.g., worries using virtual reality, for example, have been
about traveling in airplanes) or unsafe or imprac- found to be efficacious in treating anxiety disor-
tical to simulate in a session (e.g., being in a ders and well-tolerated by clients (Parsons &
burning building, getting a serious illness). In Rizzo, 2008). Another variant of exposure inte-
imaginal exposures, clinicians and clients col- grates specific therapeutic strategies to enhance
laborate to depict an intense, detailed scene of inhibitory learning in anxious children and adults.
the feared situation/environment/stimuli; this For example, therapists may continue exposure
may be enhanced through manipulating the exercises until the client’s expectancy of an
room’s lighting and use of an anxiety-inducing adverse outcome significantly drops (i.e., as
audiotape of the situation, videos, and/or pic- opposed to measuring fear reduction or habitua-
tures (Davis, 2009). Clinicians may prompt cli- tion) and increase variability throughout expo-
ents throughout the exposure to report on their sures (e.g., conducting exposures in a random
level of distress and subjective anxiety using a order as opposed to following a specific fear hier-
simple system, such as scale of varying degrees archy; Craske et al., 2014). Variants of exposure
of smiling/frowning faces or a numerical scale also include changing the pacing of exposure
from 0 to 10. This helps clinicians structure within sessions and spacing between sessions.
exposure sessions (e.g., knowing when the cli- Although there is no evidence to support a differ-
ent has habituated and exposure can end) and ence in efficacy between gradual exposure (i.e.,
helps reinforce for clients that anxiety levels can working through the fear hierarchy) and flooding
and will decrease over the course of the expo- (i.e., beginning exposure exercises with the most
sure (Friedberg & McClure, 2015). challenging step in the hierarchy), gradual expo-
Exposures are repeated in order to allow cli- sure is typically regarded to be more tolerable
ents to build a history of adaptive coping, accru- and ethical than flooding, reducing likelihood of
ing more positive or neutral experiences with the attrition or loss of motivation (Davis, 2009). The
once-feared situation/stimuli. This also allows most important aspect of exposure timing may be
clients to habituate more quickly to successive allowing clients adequate time to consolidate the
234 M.M. Kaskas et al.

learned experience, not the number of sessions or rapport with clients. While praise and rewards
the spacing between them (Moscovitch, Antony, work well throughout treatment, it may be par-
& Swinson, 2009). Notably, a recent research ticularly important to praise clients’ effort and
trend involves a single exposure-based treatment progress during challenging activities, such as
session (i.e., one-session treatment, OST), with exposures. However, it is recommended that cli-
exposures typically paced over a single 3-h session. nicians use reinforcement on a contingent basis;
The efficacy of OST has been well documented that is, clients should earn reinforcers such as
across specific phobias in youth; however, OST verbal praise or rewards (e.g., break time to play
has not yet been systematically evaluated in a game, small trinket like a decorative pencil,
broad categories of anxiety disorders (Davis piece of candy) for desired behavior (e.g., com-
et al., 2011; Davis & Ollendick, 2005). pleting progress monitoring worksheets, partici-
pating or providing examples in session,
Modeling  For each skill, it is helpful for clini- mastering a step on an exposure hierarchy).
cians to operate as “coping models” for clients, Additionally, clinicians must take care not to
explaining and demonstrating skills for clients inadvertently reinforce anxious or avoidant
before requesting that clients join them in prac- behaviors (Davis, 2009).
ticing skills. This has dual benefits: it can help
children visualize appropriate coping strategies Psychoeducation  In most CBT manuals, practi-
and makes rehearsal and role-playing experi- tioners begin by teaching clients about the nature
ences less threatening. For example, the clinician of the anxiety response (i.e., physiological, cog-
self-disclosure during the psychoeducation step nitive, and behavioral components) and the fac-
can be an appropriate method to normalize fear tors that contribute to the maintenance of anxiety
and anxiety, build rapport, set the tone for an (e.g., distorted cognitions, avoidance). At this
open discussion of feelings, and model coping time, clients learn that anxiety is neither some-
skills for the client (Kendall & Suveg, 2006). An thing that is defective within them nor a response
example of appropriate self-disclosure follows: that is unique to them but rather a universal and
“Everyone in the world gets scared or nervous often adaptive reaction to stressors. Children
sometimes. You know, I used to get really ner- often benefit from the inclusion of family mem-
vous on rollercoasters. Sometimes, I would even bers at this stage in order to better understand the
refuse to watch while my friends rode roller- environmental triggers, physiological responses,
coasters! Of course, that didn’t help me feel bet- and factors that maintain their problems with
ter about rollercoasters, and it actually stopped anxiety (Chorpita, 2007). Clinicians working
me from having fun with my friends. Have you with younger children or lower functioning youth
ever felt so nervous about something that it may incorporate more concrete language, pic-
stopped you from doing something you wanted to tures or videos, developmentally appropriate
do?.” Additionally, therapists should take advan- metaphors (e.g., anxiety as false alarms), and
tage of incidental opportunities for modeling additional practice with emotional identification
coping skills in uncertain or distressing situa- and establishing connections between cognitions,
tions—for example, if the session is interrupted emotions, and behaviors (Davis, 2009; Read
by the loud sound of a door slamming in the hall- et al., 2013).
way, clinicians may use the opportunity to model
realistic thinking skills (e.g., “These doors can be Relaxation and somatic management  This
really heavy. It sounds like someone accidentally aspect of intervention often includes both muscle
let the door slam behind them”). relaxation and breathing exercises. Here, it is
helpful to begin by reviewing the physiological
Praise and rewards  Many clinicians naturally components of anxiety and probing for the cli-
integrate praise throughout the intervention pro- ent’s specific symptoms. Youth then learn to tense
cess as a mechanism of building and maintaining and relax specific muscle groups successively;
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 235

the duration and scope of this step may be varied and catastrophic cognitions (i.e., feared outcome
according to the specific client’s needs or the par- or the worst possible thing that could happen,
ticular treatment manual chosen. For example, such as “If I fall on my face, everyone will laugh
some muscle relaxation techniques focus on a at me, and no one will play with me”); and gener-
specific part of the body (e.g., tensing the hands ate alternative and realistic interpretations
into fists, relaxing them, tensing them again), (Kendall & Suveg, 2006). Clients are encouraged
while others focus on the client’s entire body to “think like detectives,” using clues to chal-
(e.g., starting with the muscles in their toes, lenge the nature of their anxious conclusions—
working up to their foreheads, and then working for example, the above child who is anxious
back down to the toes). Analogies may be used to about going down the slide may be asked ques-
increase attention and comprehension of the task: tions to challenge his/her unrealistic threat expec-
for example, children may be asked to squeeze tancies (e.g., “What usually happens when people
their fists tightly, as if they are squeezing all of go down slides?,” “What happened the last time
the juice out of a lemon. Deep diaphragmatic you saw someone go down a slide?,” “What else
breathing is also taught as a mechanism to coun- could happen other than falling on your face?”)
ter the rapid and shallow breaths that accompany and catastrophic cognitions (e.g., “What usually
anxious physiological arousal (Read et al., 2013). happens when people fall down?,” “If you do fall
This step provides clients with skills to amelio- down, what else could happen other than every-
rate the uncomfortable physiological sensations one laughing at you?,” “How likely is it that all of
(e.g., muscle tension, hyperventilation) caused your friends will stop playing with you if you fall
by anxiety. These skills may be used as part of the down?”). Clinicians may incorporate discussions
intervention (e.g., if a client becomes too anxious and activities on cognitive distortions, such as
during an exposure) or added to the client’s rep- “all-or-nothing” thinking or “fortune-telling,” to
ertoire or “toolkit” of coping skills (Kendall & help clients identify patterns in their thoughts and
Suveg, 2006). generate alternatives (Read et al., 2013).

Cognitive restructuring  By this time, clini- Problem solving  Many clients may erroneously
cians have already covered the important connec- characterize their anxious distress as uncontrol-
tions between cognitions, emotions, and lable, inevitable, and catastrophic in nature.
behaviors; thus, clients are able to understand Clients are taught to view anxiety as a problem
that addressing biased “self-talk” (i.e., what chil- that can be solved. Clinicians and clients act as
dren think and say to themselves when they feel teams to brainstorm ideas on how to decrease dis-
anxious) is one way to tackle anxiety. After tress and effectively master daily challenges. The
establishing the rationale for this step of treat- first step involves problem orientation and identi-
ment, clinicians should lead clients in identifying fication, acknowledging that everyone encoun-
thoughts which may facilitate and maintain their ters problems sometimes and that it is important
anxious feelings (e.g., subjective feelings of fear to work to solve problems rather than avoiding or
or nervousness) and physiological hyperarousal ignoring them and selecting a problem to work
(e.g., racing heart, tense muscles). Next, clients on (Kendall & Suveg, 2006). The second step
begin to identify and categorize patterns in their involves thoroughly defining the problem (e.g.,
biased thinking. Clinicians are encouraged to asking clients “Why is this a problem?,” “When
teach clients cognitive coping strategies to reduce does this problem happen?,” “What do you usu-
negative self-cognitions (i.e., self-referential ally do when faced with this problem?”). Next, it
thoughts, such as “I always do something embar- is recommended that clinicians encourage clients
rassing”); challenge unrealistic self-cognitions, to brainstorm many possible alternative solutions
threat expectancies (i.e., estimated probability to solve the identified problem without yet judg-
that something bad will happen, such as “If I go ing or filtering out impractical solutions. Here,
down this slide, I will definitely fall on my face”), clinician modeling, participation, and positive
236 M.M. Kaskas et al.

reinforcement may be particularly beneficial in nizes the components of anxiety treatment using
helping clients generate their own solutions with- the FEAR acronym. Step one, F: Feeling fright-
out self-criticism. Last is the evaluation phase, in ened?, focuses on teaching anxious youth emo-
which clinicians encourage clients to systemati- tional identification skills, including how to
cally “test” the viability (e.g., consider likely out- recognize physiological symptoms of anxiety.
comes or consequences) of each possible solution Step two, E: Expecting bad things to happen?,
in order to achieve the best outcome (Friedberg & covers identification of anxious thoughts, includ-
McClure, 2015). Some children might have dif- ing unrealistic threat expectancies and cata-
ficulty with this skill or be overwhelmed by the strophic cognitions. Step three, A: Attitudes and
process; in these circumstances, therapists should actions that can help, involves the development
use concrete examples of problems (e.g., looking of healthy coping strategies, such as problem
for a lost toy) to explain the steps of problem solving, cognitive restructuring, and relaxation
solving (Read et al., 2013). techniques. Finally, step four, R: Results and
rewards, teaches children how to realistically
Assigning “homework”  In order to master evaluate their progress and reward themselves
techniques and generalize learned coping skills, accordingly (Kendall & Suveg, 2006). The
clients must practice outside of the weekly ses- Coping Cat program has also been adapted as an
sions. Therefore, it is recommended that clini- interactive computer-assisted treatment (Camp
cians assign “homework” to be completed Cope-A-Lot) for anxious children aged
throughout the week between therapy sessions. 7–13 years (Khanna & Kendall, 2010). Due to
Of course, many children react negatively to the the wide range of symptoms in anxiety disorders,
term “homework”; therefore, alternative terms some treatment manuals specifically target cer-
(e.g., practice, “Show That I Can” tasks) are tain disorders (e.g., Talking Back to OCD
encouraged to increase motivation and rapport describes a specific program for children with
(Kendall & Suveg, 2006). These assignments are obsessive-compulsive disorder; March, 2006).
essential for monitoring client progress, holding Flexible approaches to CBT, including Modular
clients accountable for incorporating the skills Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Childhood
learned in sessions in their daily lives, and foster- Anxiety Disorders by Chorpita (2007), allow
ing client feelings of autonomy and mastery in clinicians to customize treatment with additional
treatment. Assignments should be explained to modules based on the client’s presenting con-
youth before the end of each session, and it is cerns (e.g., modules on improving child motiva-
often helpful to do an example or two with chil- tion or disruptive behavior).
dren to ensure comprehension. Finally, the com-
pleted assignments must be reviewed and
incorporated into the following week’s treatment Conclusions and Future Directions
(Friedberg & McClure, 2015).
Anxiety disorders, if left untreated, can be per-
Pulling it all together  These components of sistent and extremely impairing. At present,
CBT are combined to create a cohesive and inte- there is a significant movement in clinical psy-
grated skills-based treatment program. Although chology to identify evidence-based practices
individual treatment manuals may vary pacing and empirically supported treatments in order to
within or between therapy sessions, sessions typ- ameliorate symptoms, improve quality of life,
ically last 1 h per week with homework assigned and reduce functional impairment for individu-
for completion between sessions. Most treatment als with anxiety disorders. Although CBT has
programs recommend a total of 15–20 sessions in been identified as a well-established treatment,
order to allow the child to learn, practice, and the highest level of EST, there is still work to be
exhibit mastery over skills covered in therapy. done in differentiating between treatment
One common CBT program, Coping Cat, orga- responders and nonresponders (e.g., identifying
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 237

variables that may affect treatment outcomes) as Burt, A. (2009). A mechanistic explanation of popu-
larity: Genes, rule breaking, and evocative gene–­
well as identifying new methods and applica-
environment correlations. Journal of Personality and
tions of delivering treatment (e.g., teleconfer- Social Psychology, 96(4), 783–794.
encing, augmented reality treatments, online Caouette, J. D., & Guyer, A. E. (2014). Gaining insight
therapies). Additionally, it is important to note into adolescent vulnerability for social anxiety from
developmental cognitive neuroscience. Developmental
that systematic evaluations of modification
Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 65–76.
trends (e.g., simplifying language, incorporat- Castagna, P., Davis III, T. E., & Lilly, M. (in press).
ing more review and practice) have not been Behavioral avoidance tasks with anxious youth: A
conducted. Future studies should also target review of procedures, properties, and criticisms.
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review.
specific anxiety disorders in order to identify
Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining
specific components of treatment or alternative empirically supported therapies. Journal of Consulting
forms of treatment that may be efficacious. and Clinical Psychology, 66(1), 7–18.
Chiles, J. A., Lambert, M. J., & Hatch, A. L. (2002).
Medical cost offset: A review of the impact of psy-
chological interventions on medical utilization over
References the past three decades. In N. A. Cummings, W. T.
O’Donohue, & K. E. Ferguson (Eds.), The impact
Alfano, C. A., Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2006). of medical cost offset on practice and research
Cognitive correlates of social phobia among chil- (pp. 47–56). Reno, NV: Context Press.
dren and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Chorpita, B. F. (2007). Modular cognitive-behavioral
Psychology, 34(2), 182–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/ therapy for childhood anxiety disorders. New York,
s10802-005-9012-9 NY: Guilford Press.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic Chorpita, B. F., & Daleiden, E. L. (2009). Mapping
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). evidence-­ based treatments for children and adoles-
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. cents: Application of the distillation and matching
American Psychological Association Presidential Task model to 615 treatments. Journal of Counseling and
Force on Evidence-Based Practice. (2006). Evidence-­ Clinical Psychology, 77(3), 566–579.
based practice in psychology. American Psychologist, Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1990).
61(4), 271–285. Peer group behavior and social status. In S. R. Asher
Barrera, T. L., & Norton, P. J. (2009). Quality of life & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood
impairment in generalized anxiety disorder, social pho- (pp. 17–59). New York, NY: Cambridge University
bia, and panic disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, Press.
23(8), 1086–1090. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Compton, S. N., Walkup, J. T., Albano, A. M.,
janxdis.2009.07.011 Piacentini, J. C., Birmaher, B., Sherrill, J. T., ...,
Biederman, J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Iyengar, S. (2010). Child/adolescent anxiety multi-
Herot, C., Friedman, D., Snidman, N., …, Faraone, modal study (CAMS): Rationale, design, and meth-
S. V. (2001). Further evidence of association between ods. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental
behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. Health, 4(1), 1–15.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(10), 1673–1679. Craske, M. G., Treanor, M., Conway, C. C., Zbozinek, T.,
Blöte, A. W., Miers, A. C., Heyne, D. A., Clark, D. M., & & Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy:
Westenberg, P. M. (2014). The relation between social An inhibitory learning approach. Behavior Research
anxiety and audience perception: Examining Clark and and Therapy, 58, 10–23.
Wells’ (1995) model among adolescents. Behavioral Davis, T. E., III. (2009). PTSD, anxiety, and phobia. In
& Cognitive Psychotherapy, 42(5), 555–567. https:// J. L. Matson, F. Andrasik, & M. L. Matson (Eds.),
doi.org/10.1017/S1352465813000271 Treating childhood psychopathology and develop-
Boardman, L., van der Merwe, L., Lochner, C., Kinnear, mental disabilities (pp. 183–220). New York, NY:
C. J., Seedat, S., Stein, D. J., …, Hemmings, S. J. Springer.
(2011). Investigating SAPAP3 variants in the etiology Davis, T. E., III, May, A. C., & Whiting, S. E. (2011).
of obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania Evidence-based treatment of anxiety and phobia in
in the South African white population. Comprehensive children and adolescents: Current status and effects
Psychiatry, 52(2), 181–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. on the emotional response. Clinical Psychology
comppsych.2010.05.007. Review, 31, 592–602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Bögels, S. M., & Zigterman, D. (2000). Dysfunctional cpr.2011.01.001
cognitions in children with social phobia, separation Davis, T. E., III, & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). Empirically
anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. supported treatments for specific phobia in children:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(2), 205– Do efficacious treatments address the components of
211. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005179032470 a phobic response? Clinical Psychology: Science and
238 M.M. Kaskas et al.

Practice, 12(2), 144–160. https://doi.org/10.1093/ (ADHD) and comorbid anxiety. Journal of


clipsy/bpi018 Attention Disorders, 20, 636–644. https://doi.
Davis, T. E., III, Ollendick, T. H., & Nebel-Schwalm, org/10.1177/1087054712452914
M. (2008). Intellectual ability and achievement in Kazdin, A. E., & Weisz, J. R. (Eds.). (2003). Evidence-­
anxiety-disordered children: A clarification and exten- based psychotherapies for children and adolescents.
sion of the literature. Journal of Psychopathology New York, NY: Guilford Press.
and Behavioral Assessment, 30, 43–51. https://doi. Kendall, P. C., & Suveg, C. (2006). Treating anxiety dis-
org/10.1007/s10862-007-9072-y orders in youth. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Child and ado-
Davis, T. E., III, Ollendick, T. H., & Öst, L. G. (2009). lescent therapy: Cognitive-behavioral procedures (3rd
Intensive treatment of specific phobias in children and ed., pp. 243–296). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
adolescents. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16, Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky,
294–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.cbpra.2008.12.008 A. M., & Wittchen, H. U. (2012). Twelve-month and
Davis, T. E., III, Ollendick, T. H., & Öst, L. G. (Eds.). (2012). lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety
Intensive one-session treatment of specific phobias. and mood disorders in the United States. International
New York, NY: Springer Science and Business Media, Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(3),
LLC. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-3253-1 169–184.
Fisak, B., & Grills-Taquechel, A. E. (2007). Parental mod- Khanna, M. S., & Kendall, P. C. (2010). Computer-­
eling, reinforcement, and information transfer: Risk assisted cognitive behavioral therapy for child anxi-
factors in the development of child anxiety. Clinical ety: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of
Child and Family Psychology Review, 10(3), 213–231. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 737–745.
Friedberg, R. D., & McClure, J. M. (2015). Clinical prac- Kley, H., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Heinrichs, N. (2012).
tice of cognitive therapy with children and adoles- Safety behaviors, self-focused attention, and nega-
cents: The nuts and bolts (2nd ed.). New York, NY: tive thinking in children with social anxiety disorder,
Guilford Publications. socially anxious, and non-anxious children. Journal of
Ginsburg, G. S., & Schlossberg, M. C. (2002). Family-­ Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43(1),
based treatment of childhood anxiety disorders. 548–555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.07.008
International Review of Psychiatry, 14(2), 143–154. Knappe, S., Sasagawa, S., & Creswell, C. (2015).
https://doi.org/10.1080/09540260220132662 Developmental epidemiology of social anxiety and
Grüner, K., Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The social phobia in adolescents. In K. Ranta, A. M. Greca,
relationship between anxious rearing behaviors and L. J. Garcia-Lopez, & M. Marttunen (Eds.), Social
anxiety disorders symptomatology in normal chil- anxiety and phobia in adolescents: Development,
dren. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental manifestation, and intervention strategies (pp. 39–70).
Psychiatry, 30(1), 27–35. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
Gullone, E. (2000). The development of normal fear: Krämer, M., Seefeldt, W. L., Heinrichs, N., Tuschen-­
A century of research. Clinical Psychology Review, Caffier, B., Schmitz, J., Wolf, O. T., & Blechert,
20(4), 429–451. J. (2012). Subjective, autonomic, and endocrine reac-
Hirshfeld, D. R., Biederman, J., Brody, L., Faraone, S. V., tivity during social stress in children with social pho-
& Rosenbaum, J. F. (1997). Expressed emotion toward bia. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(1),
children with behavioral inhibition: Associations with 95–104.
maternal anxiety disorder. Journal of the American Kristensen, H., & Torgersen, S. (2001). MCMI-II person-
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), ality traits and symptom traits in parents of children
910–917. with selective mutism: A case-control study. Journal
Hodson, K., McManus, F., Clark, D., & Doll, H. (2008). of Abnormal Psychology, 110(4), 648–652. https://doi.
Can Clark and Wells’ (1995) cognitive model of social org/10.1037/0021-843X.110.4.648
phobia be applied to young people? Behavioral and Lambert, M. J., Harmon, C., Slade, K., Whipple, J. L.,
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36(4), 449–461. & Hawkins, E. J. (2005). Providing feedback to psy-
In-Albon, T., Kossowsky, J., & Schneider, S. (2010). chotherapists on their patients’ progress: Clinical
Vigilance and avoidance of threat in the eye move- results and practice suggestions. Journal of Clinical
ments of children with separation anxiety disor- Psychology, 61, 165–174.
der. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(2), Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emo-
225–235. tional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.
In-Albon, T., & Schneider, S. (2012). Does the vigilance-­ March, J. S. (2006). Talking back to OCD: The program
avoidance gazing behavior of children with separation that helps kids and teens say no way – and parents say
anxiety disorder change after cognitive-behavioral way to go. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
therapy? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, McInnes, A., Fung, D., Manassis, K., Fiksenbaum, L., &
40(7), 1149–1156. Tannock, R. (2004). Narrative skills in children with
Jarrett, M., Wolff, J., Davis, T. E., III, Cowart, M., & selective mutism: An exploratory study. American
Ollendick, T. H. (2016). Characteristics of chil- Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13(4), 304–
dren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity d­isorder 315. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2004/031)
Treatment of Anxiety Disorders 239

McLeod, B. D., Weisz, J. R., & Wood, J. J. (2007). Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(3),
Examining the association between parenting 297–310.
and childhood anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Ramsawh, H. J., & Chavira, D. A. (2016). Association
Psychology Review, 27(8), 155–172. https://doi. of childhood anxiety disorders and quality of life in
org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.001 a primary care sample. Journal of Developmental and
Middledorp, C. M., Cath, D. C., van Dyck, R., & Boomsa, Behavioral Pediatrics, 37(4), 269–276. https://doi.
D. I. (2005). The co-morbidity of anxiety and depres- org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000296
sion in the perspective of genetic epidemiology: A Ranta, K., Tuomisto, M. T., Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rantanen,
review of twin and family studies. Psychological P., & Marttunen, M. (2014). Cognition, imagery and
Medicine, 35, 611–624. coping among adolescents with social anxiety and
Miers, A. C., Blöte, A. W., de Rooij, M., Bokhorst, C. L., phobia: Testing the Clark and Wells model in the pop-
& Westenberg, P. M. (2013). Trajectories of social ulation. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 21(3),
anxiety during adolescence and relations with cogni- 252–263.
tion, social competence, and temperament. Journal of Rao, V. N., & Ram, P. K. (1984). Impact of disturbed
Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(1), 97–110. https:// parents on the children. Child Psychiatry Quarterly,
doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9651-6 17(4), 133–138.
Monk, C. S., Telzer, E. H., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Mai, Rapee, R. M. (2001). The development of generalized
X., Louro, H. M., ..., Pine, D. S. (2008). Amygdala and anxiety. In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The
ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 481–
angry faces in children and adolescents with general- 503). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
ized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, Rapee, R. M. (2002). The development of and modi-
65(5), 568–576. fication of temperamental risk for anxiety disor-
Moscovitch, D. A., Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. ders: Prevention of a lifetime of anxiety? Society of
(2009). Exposure-based treatments for anxiety disor- Biological Psychiatry, 52, 947–957.
ders: Theory and process. In M. M. Antony & M. B. Read, K. L., Puleo, C. M., Wei, C., Cummings, C. M., &
Stein (Eds.), Oxford handbook of anxiety and related Kendall, P. C. (2013). Cognitive–behavioral treatment
disorders (pp. 461–475). New York, NY: Oxford for pediatric anxiety disorders. In Pediatric anxiety
University Press. disorders (pp. 269–287). New York, NY: Springer.
Mowrer, O. H. (1947). On the dual nature of learning: A Rosenbaum, J. F., Biederman, J., Bolduc, E. A., Hirshfeld,
reinterpretation of conditioning and problem solving. D. R., Faraone, S. V., & Kagan, J. (1992). Comorbidity
Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102–148. of parental anxiety disorders as risk for childhood-­
Nail, J. E., Christofferson, J., Ginsburg, G. S., Drake, onset anxiety in inhibited children. American Journal
K., Kendall, P. C., McCracken, J. T., … Sakolsky, D. of Psychiatry, 149(4), 475–481.
(2015). Academic impairment and impact of treat- Sburlati, E., Schniering, C., Lyneham, H., & Rapee, R.
ments among youth with anxiety disorders. Child (2011). A model of therapist competencies for the
and Youth Care Forum, 44(3), 327–342. https://doi. empirically supported cognitive behavioral treat-
org/10.1007/s10566-014-9290-x ment of child and adolescent anxiety and depressive
Naragon-Gainey, K., Gallagher, M. W., & Brown, disorders. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
T. A. (2014). A longitudinal examination of psy- Review, 14(1), 89–109. https://doi.org/10.1007/
chosocial impairment across the anxiety disorders. s10567-011-0083-6
Psychological Medicine, 44(8), 1691–1700. https:// Schmitz, J., Krämer, M., Blechert, J., & Tuschen-
doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713001967 Caffier, B. (2010). Post-event processing in chil-
Ollendick, T. H., & Benoit, K. E. (2012). A parent–child dren with social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Child
interactional model of social anxiety disorder in youth. Psychology, 38(7), 911–919. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15(1), s10802-010-9421-2
81–91. Schmitz, J., Krämer, M., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Heinrichs,
Ollendick, T. H., King, N. J., & Chorpita, B. F. (2006). N., & Blechert, J. (2011). Restricted autonomic flex-
Empirically supported treatments for children and ibility in children with social phobia. Journal of Child
adolescents. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Child and adoles- Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(11), 1203–1211.
cent therapy: Cognitive-behavioral procedures (3rd Schrock, M., & Woodruff-Borden, J. (2010). Parent-child
ed., pp. 492–520). New York, NY: Guilford Press. interactions in anxious families. Child and Family
Parsons, T. D., & Rizzo, A. A. (2008). Affective outcomes Behavior Therapy, 32(4), 291–310. doi.org/10.1080/
of virtual reality exposure therapy for anxiety and spe- 07317107.2010.515523
cific phobias: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Seefeldt, W. L., Krämer, M., Tuschen-Caffier, B., &
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 250–261. Heinrichs, N. (2014). Hypervigilance and avoid-
Piacentini, J., Bennett, S., Compton, S., Kendall, P., ance in visual attention in children with social pho-
Birmaher, B., Albano, A., ..., Walkup, J. (2014). 24-and bia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
36-Week outcomes for the Child/Adolescent Anxiety Psychiatry, 45(1), 105–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Multimodal Study (CAMS). Journal of The American jbtep.2013.09.004
240 M.M. Kaskas et al.

Spence, S. H., & Rapee, R. M. (2016). The etiology of and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America,
social anxiety disorder: An evidence-based model. 13, 729–815.
Behavior Research and Therapy, 86, 50–67. https:// Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Horowitz, J. D., Powers,
doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.06.007 M. B., & Telch, M. J. (2008). Psychological
Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., Roberson-Nay, R., & Tervo, appro­aches in the treatment of specific phobias: A
K. (2003). Parenting behaviors in parents with anxiety meta-­analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(6),
disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(5), 1021–1037.
541–554. Wood, J. J., McLeod, B. D., Sigman, M., Hwang, W.-C.,
van der Bruggen, C. O., Stams, G. J., & Bögels, S. M. & Chu, B. C. (2003). Parenting and childhood anxi-
(2008). The relation between child and parent anxi- ety: Theory, empirical findings, and future direc-
ety and parental control: A meta-analytic review. tions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 44(1), 134–151. https://doi.
and Allied Disciplines, 49(12), 1257–1269. doi. org/10.1111/1469-7610.00106
org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01898.x Yates, B. T. (1994). Toward the incorporation of costs,
Weisz, J. R., Hawley, K. M., & Doss, A. J. (2004). cost-effectiveness analysis, and cost– benefit analy-
Empirically tested psychotherapies for youth internal- sis into clinical research. Journal of Consulting and
izing and externalizing problems and disorders. Child Clinical Psychology, 62, 729–736.
Tics and Tourette Syndrome

Denis G. Sukhodolsky, Theresa R. Gladstone,
Shivani A. Kaushal, Justyna B. Piasecka,
and James F. Leckman

Contents Abbreviations
Clinical Characteristics of Tics
and Tourette Syndrome.....................................  242 A-TAC  Autism—Tics, AD/HD, and other
Comorbidities inventory
Premonitory Urges.................................................  243
ADHD Attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Contextual Factors and Tic Expression...............  243 disorder
The Prevalence of Tics and TS..............................  244 BOLD Blood oxygenation level-dependent
Co-occurrence of TS with Other Psychiatric
CBIT Comprehensive behavioral interven-
Disorders.............................................................  244 tion for tics
CBT Cognitive-behavioral therapy
Anger and Rage Attacks........................................  245
CSTC Corticostriatal-thalamo-cortical
Neuropsychological Function in TS......................  245 CTD Chronic tic disorder
Neurobiology of  TS................................................  245 D2 Dopamine 2
Clinical Assessment................................................  247 EMG Electromyography
HRT Habit reversal training
Treatment................................................................  248
MOVES Motor tic, Obsessions and com­
Behavior Therapy for Tics.....................................  249 pulsions, Vocal tic Evaluation
Behavioral Therapy for Irritability Survey
and Explosive Outbursts...................................  250 OCD Obsessive-compulsive disorder
Pharmacotherapy of  Tics......................................  251 PMd Dorsal premotor cortex
PMT Parent management training
Conclusions and Clinical Implications.................  251
PPI Prepulse inhibition
References...............................................................  252 PUTS Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale
SMA Supplementary motor area
Chapter for “Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology STSS Shapiro TS Severity Scale
and Developmental Disabilities” Johnny L. Matson, TODS Tourette’s Disorder Scale
Editor, Springer.
TS Tourette syndrome
D.G. Sukhodolsky (*) • T.R. Gladstone TS-CGI Tourette Syndrome Clinical Global
S.A. Kaushal • J.B. Piasecka • J.F. Leckman
Impression
Yale School of Medicine, Child Study Center,
New Haven, CT, USA VSP Visuospatial priming
e-mail: denis.sukhodolsky@yale.edu YGTSS Yale Global Tic Severity Scale

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 241


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_14
242 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

 linical Characteristics of Tics


C tent (chronic) motor or vocal tic disorder, provi-
and Tourette Syndrome sional tic disorder, and the other specified and
unspecified tic disorders (American Psychiatric
Tics are sudden, purposeless, repetitive, and ste- Association, 2013). Diagnosis of any tic disorder
reotyped movements and vocalizations which can is based on the presence of tics, duration of tic
be characterized by their anatomical location, symptoms, age of onset before 18 years, and
number, frequency, intensity, and complexity absence of any known medical causes. The his-
(Leckman, Bloch, Sukhodosky, Scahill, & King, torical distinction between the diagnoses of TS
2013). For example, blinking, nose twitching, and and CTD is the presence of chronic tics in one or
rapid jerking of any part of the body are common two (motor and phonic) modalities. However,
motor tics, and throat clearing, coughing, and distinctions between motor and vocal tics have
grunting are simple phonic tics. The complexity been questioned, as vocal tics are due to muscle
of tics ranges from brief and meaningless to lon- contractions of the oropharynx or diaphragm
ger and seemingly purposeful behaviors. Some (Leckman et al., 2013) and some experts in the
examples of complex motor tics are facial ges- field suggested that neurologically these disor-
tures, touching objects or people, thrusting arms, ders are the same (McNaught & Mink, 2011).
gyrating, and bending. Complex phonic tics are Because tics tend to wax and wane in number and
also diverse and may include syllables, words, severity and some individuals may have tic-free
phrases, and speech atypicalities such as sudden periods for weeks and months, the 1-year mini-
changes in pitch or volume, echolalia (repeating mum duration of tics assures a sufficient period
another person’s words), and coprolalia (uttering of time to confirm the persistent nature of tic dis-
obscene or inappropriate words and phrases). orders. Frequency and intensity of tics can vary
Regarding diagnostic criteria and features, in considerably among individuals with TS, and so
1885 Gilles de la Tourette described nine patients can the level of impairment associated with the
with motor and phonic tics and noted that tics tics. In some cases, tics are frequent and forceful,
were characterized by childhood-onset, lifelong resulting in social impairment or, rarely, physical
duration, and a waxing and waning course. These disability. However, in some individuals tics may
characteristics have been since confirmed in a be frequent but may go unnoticed and do not
large number of clinical series worldwide (Lin interfere with daily living (Coffey et al., 2004).
et al., 2002; Robertson, Trimble, & Lees, 1988). Overall impairment, however, may not be directly
Of note, coprolalia, a feature that has become related to tic severity. Some patients with TS and
engraved in the public view of TS (Olson, 2004), mild tics may be distressed and impaired whereas
is present only in 15–20% of cases in clinical some patients are seemingly unaffected by their
samples (Freeman et al., 2009). Since the intro- more prominent tics. Consequently, the tic-­
duction of the DSM-III (American Psychiatric related impairment is not part of the current diag-
Association, 1980), motor or phonic tics that are nostic criteria for TS. However, individuals who
present for 2 weeks but less than 12 months are meet some but not all criteria for TS or CTD but
diagnosed as Transient Tic Disorder. In Chronic present with clinically significant distress or
Tic Disorder (CTD) the motor or phonic tics last impairment can be diagnosed with unspecified tic
for more than a year. Tourette disorder or Tourette disorder (American Psychiatric Association,
syndrome (TS) is diagnosed when there are mul- 2013).
tiple motor tics and at least one phonic tic that Regarding developmental course, motor tics
persist for more than a year. These diagnostic cat- usually appear between the ages of 3 and 8 with
egories of tic disorders have been largely pre- the average onset at approximately 6 years of age
served in the DSM-5 which contains four (Leckman et al., 1998). Phonic tics may present
diagnostic categories: Tourette disorder, persis- first, but typically they appear several years after-
Tics and Tourette Syndrome 243

ward with the average age of onset of 11 years. tic. Despite the growing consensus that the pre-
Fewer than 5% of patients have phonic tics in the monitory urges trigger performance of the tics,
absence of motor tics (Leckman, King, & Cohen, the mechanisms of premonitory urges remain
1999). Most children become aware of premoni- poorly understood (Leckman, Bloch, Scahill, &
tory sensory urges by the age of 10. Tics follow a King, 2006; Woods, Piacentini, Himle, & Chang,
well-described waxing and waning course, a tem- 2005). A closely related phenomenological
poral pattern of remissions and exacerbations aspect of TS is the often reported capacity to sup-
that occur over a course of several weeks or press tics, at least temporarily (Leckman,
months (Lin et al., 2002). On a shorter timescale Vaccarino, Kalanithi, & Rothenberger, 2006).
of hours and minutes, tics also occur in bouts Even though tics are involuntary, they can be sup-
with periods of bursting followed by periods of pressed for minutes or even hours, which may
relative quiescence (Peterson & Leckman, 1998). result in uncertainty regarding the voluntary con-
Over the course of the disorder, tics multiply and trol of tics. Many patients report that the intensity
worsen in number and complexity with the period of premonitory urges increases during tic
of worst severity estimated to be between 9 and suppression.
12 years. At this time, tics may interfere with
adaptive functioning and school work. Follow-up
studies of clinically referred samples indicate  ontextual Factors and Tic
C
significant decline in tics in up to 80% of the Expression
patients by late adolescence (Bloch et al. 2006).
Another important consideration in assessment
and management of tics is the possible association
Premonitory Urges of tic expression with situational or contextual
factors. Many studies have documented that tics
Although TS is defined by motor and phonic tics, can be both attenuated and exacerbated by ante-
individuals with TS also experience premonitory cedents and consequences that can be either inter-
urges, recurrent unpleasant sensations associated nal states or external stimuli (Conelea & Woods,
with the tics. The urges are commonly described 2008). For example, doing something that requires
as discomfort, pressure, or tingling localized in focused attention and motor control such as play-
the muscles involved in the performance of the ing a musical instrument has been associated with
tics. These premonitory sensations prompt the reduction of tics. In contrast, unstructured activi-
performance of the tic, which is followed by ties such as waiting or watching TV can be associ-
momentary relief of the associated discomfort. ated with greater tics. Social situations such as
Up to 90% of individuals with TS report the being in a group of peers or sometimes just the
experience of premonitory urges (Banaschewski, presence of another person in the same room can
Woerner, & Rothenberger, 2003) and some be associated with either decrease or increase in
describe the urges as more bothersome than the tics (Himle et al., 2014). Remarkably, some tics
tics themselves (Hollenbeck, 2001). Tics involv- can be exacerbated while others are attenuated by
ing head, neck, and shoulder movements are the same situation. For example, vocal tics may
associated with particularly prominent urges become more prominent during social interaction
(Leckman, Walker, & Cohen, 1993). It has been while motor tics can be suppressed without obvi-
argued that tics may represent a voluntary ous effort or awareness of the individual with
response aimed at reducing the discomfort asso- TS. Environmental consequences for displaying
ciated with premonitory urges (Lang, 1991). As tics, such as receiving accommodations or atten-
with tics, the occurrences of urges vary in their tion from others, can also affect tic expression and
frequency, intensity, and duration. The intensity have been reported to have a greater influence on
of the urge can vary from fleeting and easily children’s tic severity than emotional factors
ignored to irresistible and inevitably leading to a (Eaton et al., 2017). Internal states such as stress
244 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

and anxiety have all been linked to increased like- the prevalence of TS was reported to be 14 per
lihood of tic expression (Conelea, Woods, & 1000 children (Scahill, Dalsgaard, & Bradbury,
Brandt, 2011; Lin et al., 2010). The recognition of 2013). Tic disorders are three to four times more
the role of antecedents and consequences in common in boys than in girls (Centers for
behavioral interventions for tics has been led to Disease Control and Prevention, 2009).
adding functional assessment of tics as part of a
comprehensive behavioral intervention for tics
(CBIT) that is discussed at length later in this  o-occurrence of TS with Other
C
chapter. In addition, recognition of situational and Psychiatric Disorders
internal factors associated with tics is an impor-
tant part of educating patients with tics and their TS is often associated with psychiatric comor-
families. For example, many parents report exac- bidity, most notably obsessive-compulsive disor-
erbation of their child’s tics at home after school der (OCD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity
which is often attributed to children’s effort to disorder (ADHD), with a lifetime prevalence of
suppress their tics at school and “letting the tics any 1 psychiatric disorder over 85% and over
out” at home. This interpretation can be true if half of the population with 2 or more disorders.
confirmed by child report on a careful clinical The time of greatest risk of onset for most
interview. However, an alternative explanation of psychiatric comorbidities is early childhood,
­
the relative increase in tics at home after school between the ages of 4 and 12 years (Hirschtritt
can be the tendency of tics to be reduced during et al., 2015). Clinically ascertained cases of TS
activities that require concentration such as doing may be associated with mood and anxiety disor-
schoolwork and increased by unstructured activi- ders (Coffey, Biederman, Geller, et al., 2000;
ties such as relaxing at home after school. This Robertson, Banerjee, Eapen, & Fox-Hiley,
latter explanation is also supported by experimen- 2002), disruptive behavior (Sukhodolsky et al.,
tal studies that showed no tic exacerbation or 2003), and learning disabilities (Yeates &
“rebound effect” after relatively short (minutes to Bornstein, 1996). Studies of clinically referred
hours) periods of tic suppression (Specht et al., samples reveal that 60–90% of children and ado-
2013; Verdellen, Hoogduin, & Keijsers, 2007; lescents with TS also have ADHD (Coffey,
Woods, Himle, et al., 2008). Biederman, Smoller, et al., 2000; Robertson
et al., 2002). The co-occurrence of TS and
ADHD is somewhat lower in community ascer-
The Prevalence of Tics and TS tained samples and ranges from 8% to 58%
(Hornsey et al., 2001; Wang & Kuo, 2003).
Community studies indicate that transient tics Several controlled studies, including our own
are relatively common and affect 10–20% of work, documented the negative impact of co-­
school-­age children (Cubo et al., 2011; Snider occurring ADHD on psychopathology and func-
et al., 2002). Epidemiological studies show that tioning in children with TS (Hoekstra et al.,
the prevalence of TS in children and adolescents 2004; Sukhodolsky et al., 2003). A recent
varies in the range from 0.15% to 3.0% in the ­prospective longitudinal study of 314 children in
general population (Hornsey, Banerjee, Zeitlin, the age range from 5 to 19 years who were re-
& Robertson, 2001; Khalifa & von Knorring, evaluated at follow-up 6 years later reported that
2003; Kraft et al., 2012). These estimates vary reduction in tic severity at follow-up was paral-
depending on the age of the sample as well as leled by reduction of symptom severity of co-
the ascertainment methods. Higher estimates occurring ADHD and OCD (Groth, Mol Debes,
are observed in samples of younger children and Rask, Lange, & Skov, 2017). However, 63% of
in studies that use multiple informants and participants continue to have comorbid psychiat-
direct observation. The best current estimate of ric disorders and only 37% had pure TS.
Tics and Tourette Syndrome 245

Anger and Rage Attacks Neuropsychological Function in TS

Anger outbursts and disruptive behavior have Neuropsychological functioning in TS has a long
long been recognized as common clinical fea- history of research in processes that are presumed
tures of TS, and some of the more serious forms to be associated with abnormal movement of the
of behavioral problems have been described as involuntary tics and their cognitive control. Thus,
“rage attacks” or “rage storms” (Budman, Bruun, children and adults with TS have been reported to
Park, & Olson, 1998), terms that are attributed to have deficits in fine-motor skills and in visual-­
the sudden onset and high intensity that charac- motor integration (Schultz et al., 1998). Fine-­
terize such episodes. Rage attacks may be further motor skills’ deficits in childhood were also shown
characterized by verbal and/or physical aggres- to predict tic severity in adulthood (Bloch,
sion, especially to a degree that is out of propor- Sukhodolsky, Leckman, and Schultz, 2006).
tion to the situation at hand (Budman, Bruun, Deficits in executive functioning, a broad domain
Park, Lesser, & Olson, 2000). Further, these covering planning, goal-directed behavior, inhibi-
attacks commonly occur toward a family member tory control, attention, and self-regulation
and are often described as appearing out-of-­ (Diamond, 2013), have also been reported in chil-
control and out of character to the otherwise dren and adults with TS. Some studies reported
good-­ natured personality of the child. that TS might be characterized by a selective defi-
Understandably, recurrences of these outbursts cit in inhibitory control, the ability to suppress an
are described by parents as extremely impairing activated response and avoid interference, which is
to child and family functioning (Dooley, Brna, & measured by tasks such as the Stroop (Channon,
Gordon, 1999). The extent to which rage attacks Pratt, & Robertson, 2003; Marsh, Zhu, Wang,
in TS represent a feature of associated psychopa- Skudlarski, & Peterson, 2007), Flanker (Crawford,
thology or an emotion regulation deficit unique to Channon, & Robertson, 2005), Go-NoGo (Müller
TS is not well understood. On a behavioral level, et al., 2003), and visuospatial priming (Swerdlow,
aggression in children with TS has been associ- Magulac, Filion, & Zinner, 1996) tasks. However,
ated with the presence of co-occurring ADHD, other studies suggested that deficits in executive
though not necessarily associated with the sever- functioning in children with TS might be due
ity of tics (Budman et al., 2000; Sukhodolsky to co-occurring ADHD (Sukhodolsky,
et al., 2003). There are studies that have reported Landeros-­
­ Weisenberger, Scahill, Leckman, &
positive correlation of tic severity with explosive Schultz, 2010).
outbursts in children with TS (Chen et al., 2013)
and with irritability in adults with TS (Cox &
Cavanna, 2015). Similarly, neurocognitive defi- Neurobiology of  TS
cits in response inhibition and cognitive control
have been associated with co-occurring ADHD The basal ganglia model of TS suggests that tics
and are not impaired in children with TS without are caused by the aberrantly active striatal neu-
co-­occurring conditions. Thus, it is possible that rons which inhibit basal ganglia output neurons,
the emotion regulation deficits which have been which in turn leads to disinhibition, via the thala-
well described in children with ADHD are also at mus, of cortical motor areas (Albin & Mink,
work on children with TS+ADHD. Because of 2006). This model is supported by animal studies
their significant impact on psychological and of stereotypies in rodents and nonhuman primates
adaptive functioning, several studies have tested which arise from the imbalance in metabolic
behavioral interventions for anger control in chil- activity between medium spiny neurons in the
dren and adolescents with TS (Scahill, striosomes and matrix compartments of the stria-
Sukhodolsky, et al., 2006; Sukhodolsky et al., tum (Balleine & O’Doherty, 2010; Worbe et al.,
2009). This work is described in the later section 2009). In the largest MRI study to date, basal gan-
on treatment. glia volumes were examined in 154 children and
246 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

adults with TS and 130 matched healthy controls with TS (Baym, Corbett, Wright, & Bunge, 2008;
(Peterson et al., 2003). The volumes of the cau- Jackson et al., 2011). It was suggested that
date nucleus were decreased in all subjects with increased activation of the frontal areas during
TS. The volumes of the putamen and globus palli- tasks requiring cognitive control of motor
dus were decreased in adults with TS but not in responses represents a manifestation of a neural
children, suggesting that the smaller lenticular compensatory mechanism that develops in chil-
nuclei may reflect a neuroregulatory predisposi- dren with TS as a result of ongoing efforts to
tion to continuing tics into adulthood. Consistent inhibit involuntary tics (Marsh et al., 2007;
with this suggestion, reduced caudate volumes in Serrien, Orth, Evans, Lees, & Brown, 2005).
childhood were shown to predict severity of tics Pathophysiology of TS remains the focus of
in adults with TS (Bloch, Leckman, Zhu, & active neuroimaging research. Many recent fMRI
Peterson, 2005). Recent postmortem and brain studies of TS have used tasks involving action
imaging studies have also provided evidence that inhibition to better understand the role of inhibi-
the pathogenesis of TS may depend on significant tory processes in TS. In one recent fMRI study of
cross talk between neural and immune pathways action inhibition using a stop-signal paradigm,
which is consistent with observations in other patients with TS showed atypical activation in
neurodevelopmental disorders (Martino, Zis, & motor and premotor regions and had higher reac-
Buttiglione, 2015). While anatomical and func- tion times and lower accuracy on stop (response
tional abnormalities in the basal ganglia are impli- inhibition) trials than healthy controls (Thomalla
cated in the generation of tics, cortical regions are et al., 2014). Another study used a stop-signal
considered to be involved in the regulation and reaction time task in adult TS patients and healthy
suppression of tics. Tic suppression, an act of controls during fMRI but adjusted intervals for a
stopping a tic, is thought to rely upon the neural 50% inhibition rate. They found that TS patients
circuitry that regulates response inhibition and showed greater dorsal premotor cortex (PMd)
cognitive control of motor behavior (Leckman, activation in go trials, while healthy controls
Bloch, Smith, Larabi, & Hampson, 2010). showed greater PMd activation in stop trials.
Although the causes of age-­related tic reduction Furthermore, there was a significant positive cor-
are unknown, it is likely to be associated with relation between motor tic frequency and
increased functional capacity of the frontal cortex ­activations in the right supplementary motor area
subserved by an increased myelinization (Salat (SMA)-proper during successful stop trials in
et al., 2005) and compensatory increased postna- patients, suggesting a common neural substrate
tal generation of inhibitory interneurons (Moll, for action inhibition and tic suppression and
Heinrich, Gevensleben, & Rothenberger, 2006). involving a global inhibitory mechanism (Ganos
This suggestion is in line with the finding of larger et al., 2014). Another measure of response inhibi-
prefrontal cortices in children with TS, which was tion called the visuospatial priming (VSP) task
interpreted as an adaptive change enabling suc- was used in a recent fMRI study before and after
cessful tic regulation (Peterson et al., 2001). treatment with CBIT. For TS subjects, research-
Using the Stroop task in an event-related fMRI ers found a significant decrease in striatal (puta-
study, Marsh and colleagues reported that com- men) activation from pre- to posttreatment. VSP
pared to unaffected controls, children and adults task-related activation from pre- to posttreatment
with TS had normal behavioral performance but in the inferior frontal gyrus was negatively cor-
increased activation in the frontostriatal circuitry related with changes in tic severity. The authors
of response inhibition including right inferolateral suggest that CBIT may promote normalization of
prefrontal cortex and left dorsolateral prefrontal aberrant corticostriatal-thalamo-cortical (CSTC)
cortex. Similarly, increased activation in prefron- associative and motor pathways in TS patients
tal cortex during the task-switching paradigm (Deckersbach et al., 2014).
requiring cognitive control of motor responses To probe the mechanisms of core motoric
was reported in two smaller studies of children manifestations of TS, researchers have also uti-
Tics and Tourette Syndrome 247

lized tasks of voluntary motor execution. that decreased sensorimotor gating in boys with
Zapparoli and colleagues used a task involving TS is associated with reduced utilization of brain
executed and imagined movements and also regions implicated in the higher-order integration
examined how neural patterns correlated with of somatosensory stimuli (Buse, Beste,
severity of TS. TS patients showed hyperactivation Herrmann, & Roessner, 2016).
in the premotor and prefrontal areas for executed
motor tasks, as anticipated, but additional hyper-
activation was seen in rostral prefrontal and tem- Clinical Assessment
poroparietal regions of the right hemisphere
during imagined motor tasks. Additionally, blood Comprehensive assessment of individuals with TS
oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) responses should include careful assessment of tics and co-
in the premotor cortex during motor imagery occurring psychiatric disorders as well as detailed
tasks were significantly correlated with Yale assessment of adaptive functioning across devel-
Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS) scores. These opmentally relevant domains. Clinical assessment
findings suggest a distinct system of motor con- of TS should include detailed evaluation on tics
trol in TS patients that is separate from the actual including their current and past features, age of
execution of motor acts and along with other stud- onset, developmental course, and presence of pre-
ies could provide further insight into compensa- monitory urges. For children with TS who present
tory mechanisms used by TS patients (Zapparoli with academic difficulties, psychoeducational
et al., 2016). Another fMRI study of voluntary assessment is recommended to characterize
motor control was conducted in younger TS strengths and weaknesses in learning skills and
patients using a finger-tapping paradigm and assure optimal educational planning.
revealed distinct motor network recruitment from A number of psychometric instruments can be
control participants. The most prominent differ- helpful for assessment of tic severity (Martino
ences arose from utilizing a nonpreferred hand, et al., 2017). The most comprehensive, valid, and
which resulted in decreased activation in the con- reliable instrument is the Yale Global Tic Severity
tralateral sensorimotor cortex and greater recruit- Scale (YGTSS) (Leckman et al., 1989). This
ment of premotor and prefrontal regions along instrument besides being most commonly used
with the left inferior parietal lobule (Roessner has been recommended by TS international
et al., 2013). Across studies, the left prefrontal guidelines. The YGTSS assesses tic dimensions
cortex also seems to be more active in TS patients including frequency, intensity, complexity, distri-
during voluntary movement, suggesting that these bution, as well as interference and impairment.
movements are more cognitively demanding for Although relatively longer to administer, this
TS patients (Zapparoli, Porta, & Paulesu, 2015). scale highlights relevant exacerbations that can
Attention in neuroimaging research has also aid in treatment (Lin et al., 2002). The Tourette
turned to altered sensorimotor gating, a hypothe- Syndrome Clinical Global Impression (TS-CGI)
sized core issue in TS. In a study of 22 patients and the Shapiro TS Severity Scale (STSS) are
with TS and 22 healthy controls (all males), Buse quicker to administer but do not include all the tic
and colleagues found that prepulse inhibition dimensions. The TS-CGI overall assesses impact
(PPI) of the startle response, collected by electro- of tics on the client’s life. The STSS includes
myography (EMG) during fMRI, was lower in assessment of intensity and interference of tics.
participants with TS than in healthy controls. To determine severity of TS, the Tourette’s
There was also decreased BOLD activity in the Disorder Scale (TODS) measures tics along with
middle frontal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, superior common comorbid symptoms including inatten-
parietal cortex, cingulate gyrus, and caudate body tion, hyperactivity, obsessions, compulsions,
in participants with TS, and PPI of the startle aggression, and emotional symptoms (Shytle
response was positively correlated with PPI-­ et al., 2003). The Premonitory Urge for Tics
related BOLD activity. These results indicated Scale (PUTS) measures and quantifies sensory
248 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

phenomena that many individual experience Treatment


before tics. This self-report scale is only valid for
individuals older than 10 years old (Woods et al., Tics can range considerably in terms of severity
2005). and impairment and treatment is warranted only
The two main screening instruments used if tics are a source of impairment and interference
for tics include the Motor tic, Obsessions and in the patient’s everyday life. Usual clinical
compulsions, Vocal tic Evaluation Survey practice focuses initially on educational and sup-
(MOVES) and the Autism—Tics, AD/HD, and portive interventions (Lebowitz & Scahill, 2013).
other Comorbidities inventory (A-TAC). Both Given the waxing and waning course of the disor-
instruments include a range of abnormal behav- ders, it is likely that whatever is done (or not
iors, including tics. The MOVES includes five done) will lead in the short term to some improve-
subscales: motor tics, vocal tics, obsessions, com- ment in tic severity. The decision to employ tar-
pulsions, and associated symptoms including geted behavioral interventions or psychoactive
echolalia, echopraxia, coprolalia, and copropraxia medications is usually made after the educational
(Gaffney, Sieg, & Hellings, 1994). The A-TAC is a and supportive interventions have been in place
screening interview for several disorders including for a period of months, and it is clear that the tic
autism and ADHD; however, the tic module may symptoms are persistently severe and are them-
be administered independently (Hansson et al., selves a source of impairment in terms of self-­
2005). Table 1 outlines the most used and vali- esteem, relationships with the family or peers, or
dated instruments for assessing TS severity. school performance.

Table 1  Instruments assessing TS severity


Time of Method of
Instrument Assessment targets Benefits administration administration
Yale Global Tic Severity Tic symptoms Identifies exacerbations 15–20 min Clinician
Scale (YGTSS) that direct treatment
The Tourette Syndrome Adverse impact of tics Quick to administer <2 min Clinician
Clinical Global
Impression (TS-CGI)
Tourette’s Disorder Scale Tics and comorbid Includes assessment of >20 min Parent or
(TODS) symptoms the severity of common clinician
comorbid behaviors
Shapiro Tourette Intensity and Quick to administer <5 min Clinician
Syndrome Severity Scale interference of tics
(STSS)
Premonitory Urges for Premonitory urges Quick to administer, only 5–10 min Patient
Tics Scale (PUTS) validated scale to measure
tic-related premonitory
urges
Autism—Tics, AD/HD, Screening instrument Quick to administer, <2 min for tic Clinician
and other Comorbidities for autism, ADHD, tics, interviewer may be module
inventory (A-TAC) and other comorbid nonexpert
disorders
Motor tic, Obsessions and Screening instrument Quick to administer <5 min Clinician
compulsions, Vocal tic for motor tics, vocal
Evaluation Survey tics, obsessions, and
(MOVES) compulsions
Tics and Tourette Syndrome 249

Behavior Therapy for Tics least distressing. As a rule, more distressing tics


are addressed first although tics with more read-
Since the seminal work on habit reversal training ily identifiable competing responses can be
(HRT) by Azrin and Nunn (1973), considerable addressed first too (McGuire et al., 2015).
progress has been made in the development and Awareness training and competing response
testing on behavioral interventions for tics training are then implemented and practiced dur-
(McGuire et al., 2014). A treatment program ing CBIT sessions one tic at a time. For example,
entitled comprehensive behavioral intervention a child with a neck-jerking tic may be taught to
for tics (CBIT) (Woods, Piacentini, et al., 2008) look forward with his chin slightly down while
has received rigorous testing in two randomized gently tensing neck muscles for 1 min or until the
controlled trials, one in children (Piacentini et al., urge goes away. Current guidelines suggest that
2010) and another in adults with TS (Wilhelm the competing response does not have to be phys-
et al., 2012). Medium effect sizes were found in ically incompatible with the targeted tic to be
both the pediatric (ES = 0.68) and the adult effective, and any voluntary movement can
(ES = 0.57) trials, and based on these studies, reduce the desire to perform the tics. This obser-
CBIT is now considered to be the first-line treat- vation is consistent with the commonly reported
ment for tics. The data from both child and adult reduction of tics during periods of goal-directed
CBIT studies were recently combined to examine behavior, especially those that involve both
moderators of treatment response. These analy- focused attention and fine-motor control, as what
ses revealed that presence of co-occurring occur in musical and athletic performances.
ADHD, OCD, or anxiety disorders did not mod- Table 2 contains example of competing responses
erate response to CBIT. There was a moderating for common tics.
effect of tic medication such that all participants In addition to HRT techniques, CBIT also
showed improvement after CBIT but the differ- includes functional assessment and intervention
ence between CBIT and PST was greater for for tics. The purpose of functional assessment is
participants who were not on tic-suppressing
­ to identify situational factors that may contribute
medication (Sukhodolsky et al., 2017). to the performance or worsening of tics. Adding
The key component of CBIT is HRT which functional assessment as a CBIT component was
involves teaching individuals with TS to detect based on the observation that tics can be wors-
the initial signs of tics and then performing a ened by specific situations such as being in pub-
“competing response” instead of the tic until the lic and by activities such as watching TV
urge to tic dissipates. The treatment starts with tic (Conelea & Woods, 2008; Himle et al., 2014).
awareness training which entails self-monitoring Functional assessment is conducted as an inter-
of current tics while focusing on the premonitory view with parent and child where the clinician
urge or other early signs that a tic is about to asks whether each of the antecedent and conse-
occur. When the patient is able to detect the first quence items is associated with improvement or
sign of the tics, they are taught to perform volun- worsening of each tic. Upon completion of the
tary behaviors that are physically incompatible assessment, an intervention plan is developed for
with the tic (i.e., competing responses). tics that have identifiable situational factors. For
Competing-response training is different from tic example, if a child’s throat-clearing tics become
suppression that many individuals may attempt more frequent during mealtimes and are associ-
on their own in that it teaches the patient to per- ated with siblings’ comments and requests to
form a specific voluntary movement when they stop, a functional intervention may include ask-
notice that a tic is about to occur. CBIT starts ing the child with tics to practice a relaxation
with an assessment of tics that is used to create a exercise for several minutes before dinner and
tic hierarchy where tics are listed from most to explaining to the siblings that their reactions
250 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

may inadvertently strengthen the tics. Particular usual. Similarly, parent management training
attention is given to evaluating and, if needed, (PMT), which focuses on the prevention and
eliminating opportunities for escape and avoid- effective response to problem behavior via oper-
ance conditioning in which tics may be rein- ant principles (Barkley, 2013), has also been
forced by escaping unpleasant situations. applied to parents of youth with TS with success
Because stress may be associated with exacerba- (Scahill, Sukhodolsky, et al., 2006). In a sample
tion of tics (Lin et al., 2007), relaxation training of 24 children, ratings of disruptive behavior
is often used as an auxiliary technique to manage decreased 51% in the PMT group versus 19% for
worsening of tics that may be triggered by situ- treatment-as-usual.
ational anxiety. Finally, behavioral reward sys- Based on these studies, a cognitive-behavioral
tems can be used to encourage children’s approach that combines principles of parent
engagement in CBIT and practicing tic manage- training with teaching skills for managing frus-
ment strategies at home. tration and improving social functioning can be
recommended for managing moderate levels of
irritability and noncompliance in children and
Behavioral Therapy for Irritability adolescents with TS (Sukhodolsky & Scahill,
and Explosive Outbursts 2012). The treatment manual of CBT for anger
and noncompliance consists of 10–12 sessions
Targeted behavioral treatments can be helpful for grouped into three modules: emotion regulation,
addressing disruptive behaviors in the context of problem-solving, and social skills training. The
TS. Anger control training is a type of cognitive-­ emotion regulation module is dedicated to learn-
behavioral therapy (CBT) that encourages tar- ing about anger triggers and physiological cues
geted skills to decrease anger, such as identifying associated with anger, as well as practice of anger
anger cues and practicing replacement behaviors, management skills such as relaxation and cogni-
and has been shown to significantly reduce prob- tive reappraisal. Because anger outbursts are
lem behaviors in youth with TS (Sukhodolsky most likely to occur in social interaction, children
et al., 2009). Among 26 participants, parent rat- also practice social problem-solving skills,
ings of disruptive behavior decreased 52% in including identifying the consequences of
active treatment versus 11% in treatment-as-­ choices taken in common anger-provoking situa-
tions and generating alternate solutions based on
those consequences. The last portion of the child-­
Table 2  Example of tics and competing responses focused treatment is dedicated to practicing the
Common tics Possible competing responses core anger management skills through role-play
Head jerking or Tilt head down and tense neck of typical anger-provoking situations involving
nodding muscles peers, parents, and teachers. After each session,
Eye blinking or Controlled, slow blinking or children are asked to practice the skills they have
eye rolling staring ahead while focusing on the
same spot learned and complete logs describing their expe-
Facial grimaces Relax facial muscle to produce rience of successfully managing their anger over
neutral facial expression the previous week using the skills they have
Lip rolling or lip Purse lips together or push tongue learned. Parents also participate in the treatment,
licking in the roof of the mouth with each session including a parent check-in
Shoulder Fold arms across chest or push during which the week is discussed, contents of
shrugging and elbows to the sides
arm tics the session are reviewed, and the clinician trou-
Body jerks Tighten stomach and back muscles bleshoots any stumbling blocks the family may
Tightening Rhythmic, diaphragmatic breathing be encountering. Parenting skills, including iden-
stomach muscles tifying observable behaviors and learning about
Vocal tics such as Relaxed breathing or purposeful antecedents and consequences that can be used to
throat clearing “hard” swallowing strengthen appropriate behavior, are also taught
Tics and Tourette Syndrome 251

to maximize the child’s opportunities for success & Cohen, 1987). Atypical neuroleptics such as
in decreasing the frequency and intensity of their risperidone and ziprasidone were also shown to
aggression and angry outbursts. be superior to placebo and resulted in a 30 to 40%
reduction of tics (Sallee et al., 2000; Scahill,
Leckman, Schultz, Katsovich, & Peterson, 2003).
Pharmacotherapy of  Tics Side effects noted in these controlled studies of
risperidone included weight gain and sedation
A wide variety of therapeutic agents are now (Scahill, Erenberg, et al., 2006). The benzodiaz-
available to treat tics (Murphy, Lewin, Storch, & epines, such as clonazepam, are occasionally
Stock, 2013; Weisman, Qureshi, Leckman, used as an adjunctive treatment for tics, though
Scahill, & Bloch, 2013). Clonidine and guanfa- they have not been well studied (Scahill,
cine are potent α2-receptor agonists that are Erenberg, et al.) and are associated with side
thought to function by stimulating post-synaptic effects including sedation, short-term memory
alpha-2A receptors on dendritic spines of the pre- problems, depression, and addiction.
frontal cortical pyramidal cells and by increasing
the functional connectivity of the prefrontal cor-
tical networks (Arnsten, 2010). A recent meta-­  onclusions and Clinical
C
analysis of six randomized, placebo-controlled Implications
trials demonstrated that alpha-2 agonists had a
medium effect size (ES = 0.68) in reducing tic In addition to the evaluation of tics and possible
symptoms in trials in which participants also had co-occurring disorders, a comprehensive clinical
ADHD. However, in the absence of ADHD, the evaluation of TS should involve a detailed discus-
efficacy of these agents was small (ES = 0.15) sion of social, family, and adaptive functioning.
and nonsignificant (Weisman et al., 2013). Whenever possible, the clinical evaluation should
Although this finding calls into question existing be complemented by psychometric tools, some of
pharmacological treatment guidelines for TS that which are listed in this chapter. Many families
recommend alpha-2 agonists as first-line pharma- can benefit from a continuing discussion over the
cological treatment of tics, the available studies course of several visits about the relative contri-
for review were few in number and sample sizes butions of tics versus ADHD, anxiety, or disrup-
were small. Consequently, firm conclusions can- tive behavior disorders to the impairments in
not be drawn due to the less than adequate state daily life and adaptive functioning. Behavioral
of current evidence. Guanfacine is generally pre- and pharmacological interventions can be helpful
ferred to clonidine because it is less sedating and for reduction of tics that result in distress or
not associated with rebound hypertension follow- impairment. At present, behavioral interventions
ing withdrawal (Leckman & Bloch, 2015). (i.e., CBIT) are recommended as a first-line treat-
Dopamine 2 (D2) receptor blocking agents ment in multiple practice guidelines (Murphy
have been the mainstay of treatment for tics. The et al., 2013; Robertson et al., 2017; Verdellen,
typical neuroleptics such as haloperidol and van de Griendt, Hartmann, Murphy, & Group,
pimozide have been the best studied, and on aver- 2011). However, more focused interventions
age, these medications show a 50% or greater such as problem-solving training or academic
reduction in tics in controlled studies (Sallee, skills counseling can be helpful to address prob-
Nesbitt, Jackson, Sine, & Sethuraman, 1997; lems in social, family, and school functioning.
Shapiro et al., 1989). Despite the use of relatively There is evidence that tics may be sensitive to
low doses, unwanted side effects of typical neu- environmental events, worsening during times of
roleptics may include sedation, cognitive dulling, stress and fatigue and improving during engage-
dystonia, dyskinesias, parkinsonism, akathisia, ment in activities that require mental focus or
weight gain, and, rarely, tardive dyskinesia fine-motor skills. This can inform discussions
(Bruun, 1988; Riddle, Hardin, Towbin, Leckman, with children and their families about the choice
252 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

of hobbies and sports, scheduling of day-to-day Bloch, M. H., Sukhodolsky, D. G., Leckman, J. F., &
Schultz, R. T. (2006). Fine-motor skill deficits in child-
activities, and utilizing stress management strate-
hood predict adulthood tic severity and global psycho-
gies that may improve tics as well as optimize social functioning in Tourette’s syndrome. Journal of
development across other important areas of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 47(6), 551–559.
functioning. Focusing on personal strengths and Bruun, R. D. (1988). Subtle and under recognized
side effects of neuroleptic treatment in children
building resilience toward the goal of improving
with Tourette’s disorder. The American Journal of
quality of life and well-being should inform clin- Psychiatry, 145(5), 621–624.
ical care for children with TS and their families. Budman, C. L., Bruun, R. D., Park, K. S., Lesser, M.,
& Olson, M. (2000). Explosive outbursts in children
with Tourette’s disorder. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(10),
References 1270–1276.
Budman, C. L., Bruun, R. D., Park, K. S., & Olson, M. E.
Albin, R. L., & Mink, J. W. (2006). Recent advances in (1998). Rage attacks in children and adolescents with
Tourette syndrome research. Trends in Neurosciences, Tourette’s disorder: A pilot study. The Journal of
29(3), 175–182. Clinical Psychiatry, 59(11), 576–580.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic Buse, J., Beste, C., Herrmann, E., & Roessner, V. (2016).
and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Neural correlates of altered sensorimotor gating in
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. boys with Tourette syndrome: A combined EMG/fMRI
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic study. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry,
and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edi- 17(3), 187–197. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622975.20
tion, (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric 15.1112033
Publishing. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2009).
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2010). The use of α-2A adrener- Prevalence of diagnosed Tourette syndrome in per-
gic agonists for the treatment of attention-deficit/ sons aged 6–17 years – United States, 2007. MMWR
hyperactivity disorder. [Review]. Expert Review of Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 58(21),
Neurotherapeutics, 10(10), 1595–1605. https://doi. 581–585.
org/10.1586/ern.10.133 Channon, S., Pratt, P., & Robertson, M. M. (2003).
Azrin, N. H., & Nunn, R. G. (1973). Habit reversal: Executive function, memory, and learning in Tourette’s
A method of eliminating nervous habits and tics. syndrome. Neuropsychology, 17(2), 247–254.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 11(4), 619–628. Chen, K., Budman, C. L., Diego Herrera, L., Witkin, J. E.,
Balleine, B. W., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and Weiss, N. T., Lowe, T. L., et al. (2013). Prevalence and
rodent homologies in action control: Corticostriatal clinical correlates of explosive outbursts in Tourette
determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. syndrome. Psychiatry Research, 205(3), 269–275.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 48–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.09.029
Banaschewski, T., Woerner, W., & Rothenberger, A. Coffey, B. J., Biederman, J., Geller, D., Frazier, J.,
(2003). Premonitory sensory phenomena and suppress- Spencer, T., Doyle, R., et al. (2004). Reexamining
ibility of tics in Tourette syndrome: Developmental tic persistence and tic-associated impairment in
aspects in children and adolescents. Developmental Tourette’s disorder findings from a naturalistic follow-
Medicine and Child Neurology, 45(10), 700–703. ­up study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
Barkley, R. A. (2013). Defiant children: A clinician’s 192(11), 776–780.
manual for assessment and parent training (3rd ed.). Coffey, B. J., Biederman, J., Geller, D. A., Spencer,
New York: The Guilford Press. T. J., Kim, G. S., Bellordre, C. A., et al. (2000).
Baym, C. L., Corbett, B. A., Wright, S. B., & Bunge, S. A. Distinguishing illness severity from tic severity in chil-
(2008). Neural correlates of tic severity and cognitive dren and adolescents with Tourette’s disorder. Journal
control in children with Tourette syndrome. Brain, of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
131(1), 165–179. Psychiatry, 39(5), 556–561.
Bloch, M. H., Leckman, J. F., Zhu, H., & Peterson, Coffey, B. J., Biederman, J., Smoller, J. W., Geller, D. A.,
B. S. (2005). Caudate volumes in childhood predict Sarin, P., Schwartz, S., et al. (2000). Anxiety disor-
symptom severity in adults with Tourette syndrome. ders and tic severity in juveniles with Tourette’s dis-
Neurology, 65(8), 1253–1258. order. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Bloch, M. H., Scahill, L., Otka, J., Katsovich, L., Zhang, Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(5), 562–568.
H., Leckman, J. F., et al. (2006). Adulthood outcome Conelea, C. A., & Woods, D. W. (2008). The influence of
of tic and obsessive-compulsive symptom sever- contextual factors on tic expression in Tourette’s syn-
ity in children with Tourette syndrome. Archives of drome: A review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 160(1), 65–69. 65(5), 487–496.
Tics and Tourette Syndrome 253

Conelea, C. A., Woods, D. W., & Brandt, B. C. (2011). Psychiatric telephone interview with parents for
The impact of a stress induction task on tic frequencies screening of childhood autism – tics, attention-­
in youth with Tourette syndrome. [Article]. Behaviour deficit hyperactivity disorder and other comorbidities
Research and Therapy, 49(8), 492–497. https://doi. (A-TAC): Preliminary reliability and validity. The
org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.05.006 British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, 262–267. https://
Cox, J. H., & Cavanna, A. E. (2015). Irritability symptoms doi.org/10.1192/bjp.187.3.262
in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. [Article]. Journal Himle, M. B., Capriotti, M. R., Hayes, L. P., Ramanujam,
of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 27(1), K., Scahill, L., Sukhodolsky, D. G., et al. (2014).
42–47. Variables associated with tic exacerbation in children
Crawford, S., Channon, S., & Robertson, M. M. (2005). with chronic tic disorders. Behavior Modification,
Tourette’s syndrome: Performance on tests of behav- 38(2), 163–183. doi:0145445514531016 [pii]
ioural inhibition, working memory and gambling. 381177/0145445514531016.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(12), Hirschtritt, M. E., Lee, P. C., Pauls, D. L., Dion, Y.,
1327–1336. Grados, M. A., Illmann, C., et al. (2015). Lifetime
Cubo, E., Trejo Gabriel Y Galán, J. M., Villaverde, prevalence, age of risk, and genetic relationships of
V. A., Sáez Velasco, S., Delgado Benito, V., Vicente comorbid psychiatric disorders in Tourette syndrome.
MacArrón, J., et al. (2011). Prevalence of tics in [Article]. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(4), 325–333. https://
schoolchildren in central Spain: A population-based doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2650
study. [Article]. Pediatric Neurology, 45(2), 100–108. Hoekstra, P. J., Steenhuis, M. P., Troost, P. W., Korf, J.,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2011.03.003 Kallenberg, C. G., & Minderaa, R. B. (2004). Relative
Deckersbach, T., Chou, T., Britton, J. C., Carlson, L. E., contribution of attention-deficit hyperactivity disor-
Reese, H. E., Siev, J., et al. (2014). Neural correlates der, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and tic severity
of behavior therapy for Tourette’s disorder. Psychiatry to social and behavioral problems in tic disorders.
Research, 224(3), 269–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
pscychresns.2014.09.003 25(4), 272–279.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review Hollenbeck, P. J. (2001). Insight and hindsight into
of Psychology, 64, 135–168. Tourette syndrome. Advances in Neurology, 85,
Dooley, J. M., Brna, P. M., & Gordon, K. E. (1999). 363–367.
Parent perceptions of symptom severity in Tourette’s Hornsey, H., Banerjee, S., Zeitlin, H., & Robertson,
syndrome. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 81(5), M. (2001). The prevalence of Tourette syndrome in
440–441. 13-14-year-olds in mainstream schools. Journal of
Eaton, C. K., Jones, A. M., Gutierrez-Colina, A. M., Ivey, Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(8), 1035–1039.
E. K., Carlson, O., Melville, L., et al. (2017). The Jackson, S. R., Parkinson, A., Jung, J., Ryan, S. E.,
influence of environmental consequences and inter- Morgan, P. S., Hollis, C., et al. (2011). Compensatory
nalizing symptoms on children’s tic severity. [Article]. neural reorganization in tourette syndrome. Current
Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 48(2), Biology, 21(7), 580–585. doi:S0960-9822(11)00238-7
327–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-016-0644-5 [pii] 441016/j.cub.2011.02.047.
Freeman, R. D., Zinner, S. H., Müller-Vahl, K. R., Fast, D. K., Khalifa, N., & von Knorring, A. L. (2003). Prevalence
Burd, L. J., Kano, Y., et al. (2009). Coprophenomena of tic disorders and Tourette syndrome in a Swedish
in Tourette syndrome. [Article]. Developmental school population. Developmental Medicine and
Medicine and Child Neurology, 51(3), 218–227. Child Neurology, 45(5), 315–319.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03135.x Kraft, J. T., Dalsgaard, S., Obel, C., Thomsen, P. H.,
Gaffney, G. R., Sieg, K., & Hellings, J. (1994). The Moves – Henriksen, T. B., & Scahill, L. (2012). Prevalence
a self-rating scale for Tourettes-syndrome. Journal of and clinical correlates of tic disorders in a community
Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 4(4), sample of school-age children. [Article]. European
269–280. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.1994.4.269 Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 21(1), 5–13. https://
Ganos, C., Kuhn, S., Kahl, U., Schunke, O., Feldheim, J., doi.org/10.1007/s00787-011-0223-z
Gerloff, C., et al. (2014). Action inhibition in Tourette Lang, A. (1991). Patient perception of tics and other
syndrome. Movement Disorders, 29(12), 1532–1538. movement disorders. Neurology, 41(2 I), 223–228.
https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.25944 Lebowitz, E. R., & Scahill, L. (2013). Psychoeducational
Groth, C., Mol Debes, N., Rask, C. U., Lange, T., & Skov, interventions: What every parent and family member
L. (2017). Course of Tourette syndrome and comor- needs to know. In D. Martino & J. F. Leckman (Eds.),
bidities in a large prospective clinical study. [confer- Tourette syndrome. New York: Oxford University Press.
ence paper]. Journal of the American Academy of Leckman, J. F., & Bloch, M. H. (2015). Tic disorders. In
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(4), 304–312. A. Thapar, D. S. Pine, J. F. Leckman, S. Scott, M. J.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.01.010 Snowling, & E. Taylor (Eds.), Rutter’s child and ado-
Hansson, S. L., Svanstrom Rojvall, A., Rastam, M., lescent psychiatry (6th ed., pp. 757–773). New York:
Gillberg, C., Gillberg, C., & Anckarsater, H. (2005). Wiley.
254 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

Leckman, J. F., Bloch, M. H., Scahill, L., & King, R. A. Marsh, R., Zhu, H., Wang, Z., Skudlarski, P., & Peterson,
(2006). Tourette syndrome: The self under siege. B. S. (2007). A developmental fMRI study of self-­
Journal of Child Neurology, 21(8), 642–649. regulatory control in Tourette’s syndrome. The
Leckman, J. F., Bloch, M. H., Smith, M. E., Larabi, D., American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 955–966.
& Hampson, M. (2010). Neurobiological substrates of Martino, D., Pringsheim, T. M., Cavanna, A. E., Colosimo,
Tourette’s disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent C., Hartmann, A., Leckman, J. F., et al. (2017).
Psychopharmacology, 20(4), 237–247. https://doi. Systematic review of severity scales and screening
org/10.1089/cap.2009.0118 instruments for tics: Critique and recommendations.
Leckman, J. F., Bloch, M. H., Sukhodosky, D. G., Scahill, Movement Disorders, 32(3), 467–473. https://doi.
L., & King, R. A. (2013). Phenomenology of tics and org/10.1002/mds.26891
sensory urges: The self under siege. In D. Martino & Martino, D., Zis, P., & Buttiglione, M. (2015). The role
J. F. Leckman (Eds.), Tourette syndrome. New York: of immune mechanisms in Tourette syndrome. Brain
Oxford University Press. Research, 1617, 126–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Leckman, J. F., King, R. A., & Cohen, D. J. (1999). Tics brainres.2014.04.027
and tic disorders. In J. F. Leckman & D. J. Cohen McGuire, J. F., Piacentini, J., Brennan, E. A., Lewin, A. B.,
(Eds.), Tourette’s syndrome-tics, obsessions, compul- Murphy, T. K., Small, B. J., et al. (2014). A meta-
sions: Developmental psychopathology and clinical analysis of behavior therapy for Tourette syndrome.
care (pp. 23–42). New York: Wiley. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 50(1), 106–112.
Leckman, J. F., Riddle, M. A., Hardin, M. T., Ort, S. I., McGuire, J. F., Piacentini, J., Scahill, L., Woods,
Swartz, K. L., Stevenson, J., et al. (1989). TheYale global D. W., Villarreal, R., Wilhelm, S., et al. (2015).
tic severity scale: Initial testing of a clinician-rated Bothersome tics in patients with chronic tic disorders:
scale of tic severity. Journal of the American Academy Characteristics and individualized treatment response
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4), 566–573. to behavior therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-198907000-00015 70, 56–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.05.006
Leckman, J. F., Vaccarino, F. M., Kalanithi, P. S. A., & McNaught, K. S. P., & Mink, J. W. (2011). Advances in
Rothenberger, A. (2006). Tourette syndrome: A relent- understanding and treatment of Tourette syndrome.
less drumbeat driven by misguided brain oscillations. [Review]. Nature Reviews Neurology, 7(12), 667–676.
Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 47(6), https://doi.org/10.1038/nrneurol.2011.167
537–550. Moll, G. H., Heinrich, H., Gevensleben, H., &
Leckman, J. F., Walker, D. E., & Cohen, D. J. (1993). Rothenberger, A. (2006). Tic distribution and inhibi-
Premonitory urges in Tourette’s syndrome. American tory processes in the sensorimotor circuit during ado-
Journal of Psychiatry, 150(1), 98–102. lescence: A cross-sectional TMS study. Neuroscience
Leckman, J. F., Zhang, H., Vitale, A., Lahnin, F., Lynch, Letters, 403(1–2), 96–99.
K., Bondi, C., et al. (1998). Course of tic severity in Müller, S. V., Johannes, S., Münte, T. F., Wieringa, B.,
Tourette syndrome: The first two decades. Pediatrics, Weber, A., Kolbe, H., et al. (2003). Disturbed monitor-
102(1 Pt 1), 14–19. ing and response inhibition in patients with Gilles de la
Lin, H., Katsovich, L., Ghebremichael, M., Findley, D. B., Tourette syndrome and co-morbid obsessive compul-
Grantz, H., Lombroso, P. J., et al. (2007). Psychosocial sive disorder. Behavioural Neurology, 14(1–2), 29–37.
stress predicts future symptom severities in chil- Murphy, T. K., Lewin, A. B., Storch, E. A., & Stock, S.
dren and adolescents with Tourette syndrome and/ (2013). Practice parameter for the assessment and
or obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Child treatment of children and adolescents with tic disor-
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, ders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
48(2), 157–166. Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(12), 1341–1359.
Lin, H., Williams, K. A., Katsovich, L., Findley, D. B., Olson, S. (2004). Making sense of Tourette’s. Science,
Grantz, H., Lombroso, P. J., et al. (2010). Streptococcal 305, 1390–1392.
upper respiratory tract infections and psychosocial Peterson, B. S., & Leckman, J. F. (1998). The temporal
stress predict future tic and obsessive-compulsive dynamics of tics in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome.
symptom severity in children and adolescents with Biological Psychiatry, 44(12), 1337–1348.
Tourette syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disor- Peterson, B. S., Staib, L., Scahill, L., Zhang, H., Anderson,
der. Biological Psychiatry, 67(7), 684–691. https:// C., Leckman, J. F., et al. (2001). Regional brain and
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.020 ventricular volumes in Tourette syndrome. Archives of
Lin, H., Yeh, C. B., Peterson, B. S., Scahill, L., Grantz, H., General Psychiatry, 58(5), 427–440.
Findley, D. B., et al. (2002). Assessment of symptom Peterson, B. S., Thomas, P., Kane, M. J., Scahill, L.,
exacerbations in a longitudinal study of children with Zhang, H., Bronen, R., et al. (2003). Basal Ganglia
Tourette’s syndrome or obsessive-compulsive disor- volumes in patients with Gilles de la Tourette syn-
der. Journal of the American Academy of Child and drome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(4), 415–
Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(9), 1070–1077. https://doi. 424. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.4.415
org/10.1097/00004583-200209000-00007
Tics and Tourette Syndrome 255

Piacentini, J., Woods, D. W., Scahill, L., Wilhelm, S., trial of parent management training in children with
Peterson, A. L., Chang, S., et al. (2010). Behavior ther- tic disorders and disruptive behavior. Journal of Child
apy for children with Tourette disorder: A randomized Neurology, 21(8), 650–656.
controlled trial. JAMA, 303(19), 1929–1937. Schultz, R. T., Carter, A. S., Gladstone, M., Scahill, L.,
Riddle, M. A., Hardin, M. T., Towbin, K. E., Leckman, Leckman, J. F., Peterson, B. S., et al. (1998). Visual-­
J. F., & Cohen, D. J. (1987). Tardive dyskinesia fol- motor integration functioning in children with Tourette
lowing haloperidol treatment in Tourette’s syndrome. syndrome. Neuropsychology, 12(1), 134–145.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 44(1), 98–99. Serrien, D. J., Orth, M., Evans, A. H., Lees, A. J., &
Robertson, M. M., Banerjee, S., Eapen, V., & Fox-Hiley, Brown, P. (2005). Motor inhibition in patients with
P. (2002). Obsessive compulsive behaviour and Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: Functional activation
depressive symptoms in young people with Tourette patterns as revealed by EEG coherence. Brain, 128(1),
syndrome. A controlled study. European Child & 116–125.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 11(6), 261–265. Shapiro, E., Shapiro, A. K., Fulop, G., Hubbard, M.,
Robertson, M. M., Eapen, V., Singer, H. S., Martino, D., Mandeli, J., Nordlie, J., et al. (1989). Controlled study
Scharf, J. M., Paschou, P., et al. (2017). Gilles de la of haloperidol, pimozide and placebo for the treat-
Tourette syndrome. Natural Review Disease Primers, ment of Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Archives of
3, 16097. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrdp.2016.97 General Psychiatry, 46(8), 722–730.
Robertson, M. M., Trimble, M. R., & Lees, A. J. (1988). Shytle, R. D., Silver, A. A., Sheehan, K. H., Wilkinson,
The psychopathology of the Gilles de la Tourette B. J., Newman, M., Sanberg, P. R., et al. (2003).
syndrome. A phenomenological analysis. The British The Tourette’s disorder scale (TODS): Development,
Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 383–390. reliability, and validity. Assessment, 10(3), 273–287.
Roessner, V., Wittfoth, M., August, J. M., Rothenberger, https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191103255497
A., Baudewig, J., & Dechent, P. (2013). Finger Snider, L. A., Seligman, L. D., Ketchen, B. R., Levitt,
tapping-­ related activation differences in treatment-­ S. J., Bates, L. R., Garvey, M. A., et al. (2002). Tics
naive pediatric Tourette syndrome: A comparison of and problem behaviors in schoolchildren: Prevalence,
the preferred and nonpreferred hand. Journal of Child characterization, and associations. Pediatrics, 110(2
Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(3), 273–279. https:// Pt 1), 331–336.
doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02584.x Specht, M. W., Woods, D. W., Nicotra, C. M., Kelly,
Salat, D. H., Tuch, D. S., Hevelone, N. D., Fischl, B., L. M., Ricketts, E. J., Conelea, C. A., et al. (2013).
Corkin, S., Rosas, H. D., et al. (2005). Age-related Effects of tic suppression: Ability to suppress,
changes in prefrontal white matter measured by diffu- rebound, negative reinforcement, and habituation to
sion tensor imaging. Annals of the New York Academy the premonitory urge. [Article]. Behaviour Research
of Sciences, 1064, 37–49. and Therapy, 51(1), 24–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Sallee, F. R., Kurlan, R., Goetz, C. G., Singer, H., Scahill, brat.2012.09.009
L., Law, G., et al. (2000). Ziprasidone treatment of Sukhodolsky, D. G., Landeros-Weisenberger, A.,
children and adolescents with Tourette’s syndrome: A Scahill, L., Leckman, J. F., & Schultz, R. T. (2010).
pilot study. Journal of the American Academy of Child Neuropsychological functioning in children with
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(3), 292–299. Tourette syndrome with and without attention-­deficit/
Sallee, F. R., Nesbitt, L., Jackson, C., Sine, L., & hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American
Sethuraman, G. (1997). Relative efficacy of halo- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
peridol and pimozide in children and adolescents 49(11), 1155–1164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.
with Tourette’s disorder. The American Journal of 2010.08.008
Psychiatry, 154(8), 1057–1062. Sukhodolsky, D. G., & Scahill, L. (2012). Cognitive-­
Scahill, L., Dalsgaard, S., & Bradbury, K. (2013). The behavioral therapy for anger and aggression in chil-
prevalence of Tourette syndrome and its relationship dren. New York: Guilford Press.
to clinical features. In D. Martino & J. F. Leckman Sukhodolsky, D. G., Scahill, L., Zhang, H., Peterson,
(Eds.), Tourette syndrome (pp. 121–136). New York: B. S., King, R. A., Lombroso, P. J., et al. (2003).
Oxford University Press. Disruptive behavior in children with Tourette’s syn-
Scahill, L., Erenberg, G., Berlin, C. M., Jr., Budman, C., drome: Association with ADHD comorbidity, tic
Coffey, B. J., Jankovic, J., et al. (2006). Contemporary severity, and functional impairment. Journal of the
assessment and pharmacotherapy of Tourette syn- American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
drome. NeuroRx: Journal of the American Society for 42(1), 98–105.
Experimental NeuroTherapeutics, 3(2), 192–206. Sukhodolsky, D. G., Vitulano, L. A., Carroll,
Scahill, L., Leckman, J. F., Schultz, R. T., Katsovich, L., D. H., McGuire, J., Leckman, J. F., & Scahill, L.
& Peterson, B. S. (2003). A placebo-controlled trial of (2009). Randomized trial of anger control training
risperidone in Tourette syndrome. Neurology, 60(7), for adolescents with Tourette’s syndrome and
1130–1135. ­disruptive behavior. Journal of the American
Scahill, L., Sukhodolsky, D. G., Bearss, K., Findley, D. B., Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(4),
Hamrin, V., Carroll, D. H., et al. (2006). A randomized 413–421.
256 D.G. Sukhodolsky et al.

Sukhodolsky, D. G., Woods, D. W., Piacentini, J., Wilhelm, S., Woods, D. W., Himle, M. B., Miltenberger, R. G., Carr,
Peterson, A. L., Katsovich, L., et al. (2017). Moderators J. E., Osmon, D. C., Karsten, A. M., et al. (2008).
and predictors of response to behavior therapy for tics Durability, negative impact, and neuropsychological
in Tourette syndrome. Neurology, 88(11), 1029–1036. predictors of tic suppression in children with chronic
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003710 tic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Swerdlow, N. R., Magulac, M., Filion, D., & Zinner, 36(2), 237–245.
S. (1996). Visuospatial priming and latent inhibi- Woods, D. W., Piacentini, J., Himle, M. B., & Chang, S.
tion in children and adults with Tourette’s disorder. (2005). Premonitory urge for tics scale (PUTS): Initial
Neuropsychology, 10(4), 485–494. psychometric results and examination of the premonitory
Thomalla, G., Jonas, M., Baumer, T., Siebner, H. R., urge phenomenon in youths with tic disorders. Journal
Biermann-Ruben, K., Ganos, C., et al. (2014). Costs of of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26(6),
control: Decreased motor cortex engagement during a 397–403.
Go/NoGo task in Tourette’s syndrome. Brain, 137(Pt Woods, D. W., Piacentini, J. C., Chang, S. W., Deckersbach,
1), 122–136. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt288 T., Ginsburg, G. S., Peterson, A. L., et al. (2008).
Verdellen, C., van de Griendt, J., Hartmann, A., Murphy, Managing Tourette syndrome: A behavioral interven-
T., & Group, E. G. (2011). European clinical guide- tion. New York: Oxford University Press.
lines for Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders. Worbe, Y., Baup, N., Grabli, D., Chaigneau, M.,
Part III: Behavioural and psychosocial interventions. Mounayar, S., McCairn, K., et al. (2009). Behavioral
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(4), 197– and movement disorders induced by local inhibitory
207. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-011-0167-3 dysfunction in primate striatum. Cerebral Cortex,
Verdellen, C. W. J., Hoogduin, C. A. L., & Keijsers, G. P. 19(8), 1844–1856.
J. (2007). Tic suppression in the treatment of Tourette’s Yeates, K. O., & Bornstein, R. A. (1996). Neuro­
syndrome with exposure therapy: The rebound phe- psychological correlates of learning disability sub-
nomenon reconsidered. Movement Disorders, 22(11), types in children with Tourette’s syndrome. Journal
1601–1606. of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(5),
Wang, H.-S., & Kuo, M.-F. (2003). Tourette’s syndrome 375–382.
in Taiwan: An epidemiological study of tic disorders Zapparoli, L., Porta, M., Gandola, M., Invernizzi, P.,
in an elementary school at Taipei County. Brain and Colajanni, V., Servello, D., et al. (2016). A functional
Development, 25(Supplement 1), S29–S31. magnetic resonance imaging investigation of motor
Weisman, H., Qureshi, I. A., Leckman, J. F., Scahill, L., & control in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome during
Bloch, M. H. (2013). Systematic review: Pharmacological imagined and executed movements. The European
treatment of tic disorders – Efficacy of antipsychotic and Journal of Neuroscience, 43(4), 494–508. https://doi.
alpha-2 adrenergic agonist agents. Neuroscience and org/10.1111/ejn.13130
Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(6), 1162–1171. Zapparoli, L., Porta, M., & Paulesu, E. (2015). The
Wilhelm, S., Peterson, A. L., Piacentini, J., Woods, D. W., anarchic brain in action: The contribution of task-­
Deckersbach, T., Sukhodolsky, D. G., et al. (2012). based fMRI studies to the understanding of Gilles
Randomized trial of behavior therapy for adults with de la Tourette syndrome. Current Opinion in
Tourette’s disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, Neurology, 28(6), 604–611. ­https://doi.org/10.1097/
69(8), 795–803. WCO.0000000000000261
Treatment Approaches
to Aggression and Tantrums
in Children with Developmental
Disabilities

Abigail Issarraras and Johnny L. Matson

Contents family; these behaviors are highly stigmatized,


Introduction............................................................ 257 which can cause social exclusion or isolation,
restrict access to services and adequate learning
Characteristics and Prevalence.............................  258
environments, leave children vulnerable to the
Treatment Approaches...........................................  259 use of physical restraints, and even reduce future
Caregiver, Professional Staff, and  opportunities for independent living and personal
Clinician Attitudes Toward Aggression............  264 relationships with others (Machalicek, O’Reilly,
Supporting Families...............................................  264 Beretvas, Sigafoos, & Lancioni, 2007; Matson,
Future Directions...................................................  265
Dixon, & Matson, 2005). Many children who
have these severe forms of challenging behavior
Conclusion..............................................................  266
lack close friendships with their peers in school
References...............................................................  266 or community settings, which may add to their
feelings of isolation. Addressing a child’s aggres-
sive and tantrum behaviors has lasting impact
Introduction across many domains of their daily functioning.
Aggression and tantrum behaviors take many
Aggression and tantrums occur at high rates in forms. One distinction often made is between
the developmental disabilities population. aggressive behavior directed towards other indi-
Though many children may exhibit such behav- viduals (i.e. physical or verbal aggression) and
iors as they grow and develop, approaching such aggressive behavior directed toward objects
behaviors in children with developmental dis- (i.e., property destruction). Physical aggression
abilities differs from typically-developing chil- is defined as hitting, pushing, scratching, kick-
dren because often their intensity and chronicity ing, hair pulling, biting, and any other behavior
persists well into adulthood (Matson & Neal, that would injure another person. Verbal aggres-
2009; Murphy et al., 2005). Often a child’s sion takes the form of yelling or threatening
aggression or severe tantrum behavior can have someone. Some examples of property destruc-
various consequences for both the child and their tion include breaking, throwing, and/or hitting
objects. Some of these behaviors may have top-
ographic overlap with more severe tantrums,
A. Issarraras (*) • J.L. Matson
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
which can include extreme bouts of crying/
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA screaming, falling to the ground, and other
e-mail: aissar1@lsu.edu related behaviors.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 257


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_15
258 A. Issarraras and J.L. Matson

Historically the fields of psychology, applied constitutes more of an “impulsive unintended


behavior analysis (ABA), and psychiatry have reaction” rather than an intention of doing harm.
each addressed the management and treatment of This understanding is further supported by evi-
aggression and tantrum behaviors differently. As dence from Dominick et al. (2007), whose find-
Mace and Critchfield (2010) discuss in their ings suggested that aggression was the only
review of early ABA studies, much of the early behavior to show a significant relationship with
work in this field focused on increasing positive both cognitive and language measures. If chil-
behaviors through reinforcement. Other studies, dren do not have a means of communicating
such as one by Risley (1968) aimed to understand clearly, their difficulty and frustration in trying to
how punishment procedures (i.e. aversive shocks) express their wants and needs may also lend itself
could decrease problem behavior. Meanwhile, to more aggressive behaviors.
psychiatrists have usually used the means of their Tantrum behaviors refer to a group of chal-
training (i.e. medications) to address the issues lenging and externalizing behaviors that may
presented to them. In order to assist clinicians in manifest as crying, becoming upset, displaying
navigating the treatment of these behaviors with anger, property destruction, and noncompliance
this population, this chapter provides an over- (Goldin, Matson, Tureck, Cervantes, & Jang,
view of aggression and tantrums in developmen- 2013). Tantrums can also include the aggressive
tal disabilities, discusses two main avenues of behaviors discussed previously. It is important to
treatment found in the literature, and considers note that children of all ability levels occasion-
other issues related to managing and treating ally exhibit tantrum behavior even in the absence
these behaviors. of psychopathology and/or developmental dis-
abilities (Bhatia et al., 1990). However, as Bhatia
and colleagues also report, these behaviors tend
Characteristics and Prevalence to decrease as typically-developing children
grow older. This is not always the case for chil-
Researchers and clinicians must often take extra dren with developmental disabilities. In a study
caution when discussing aggressive behaviors in comparing rates of tantrum behavior in children
children. As Gendreau and Archer (2005) dis- with comorbid autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
cuss, “aggressive” is an emotionally charged and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
term, involving a social and moral judgment. (ADHD), Konst, Matson, and Turygin (2013) did
Adding to this complexity, aggressive behavior is not see a significant influence of age on the exhi-
often described in terms of an individual’s inten- bition of tantrum behaviors. However, they did
tion to do harm to others. This issue of intent find a positive correlation between ASD sympto-
becomes especially concerning when children mology and elevations in the severity of such
with developmental disabilities or delays are con- behaviors (Konst et al., 2013).
sidered. Often these children do not have the This relationship between aggression and tan-
emotional or cognitive capacity to exhibit a typi- trum behaviors and an ASD diagnosis persists
cal understanding of intent. Because of this, a fair throughout the literature. Dominick et al. (2007)
definition of aggressive behavior in this popula- found that 70% of children with autism in their
tion is behavior that results or may potentially study had experienced a period of severe tan-
result in harm to another person or property (i.e. trums either in the past or in the present.
biting, hitting, kicking, throwing objects, etc.). Furthermore, 60% of these children exhibited
Aggressive behavior typically has an onset in the tantrum behavior on a daily basis and so these
toddler years, though onset does continue well behaviors were considered a constant problem
past age 5 (Dominick, Davis, Lainhart, Tager-­ (Dominick et al., 2007). Findings were similar
Flusberg, & Folstein, 2007). As Tremblay (2000) for rates of aggressive behavior. Dominick and
pointed out, it’s possible that aggressive behavior colleagues report that in 88% and 75% of cases,
Aggression and Tantrums 259

parents and siblings respectively were the targets Treatment Approaches


of the aggressive behavior; teachers were also
targets of aggression in 70% of the cases. Two of the most widely researched approaches to
Aggression was directed toward more than one aggressive behavior and tantrums are behavioral
person in over 90% of the cases observed treatments and pharmacotherapy. While these
(Dominick et al.). Another study by Kanne and two are among the most accepted approaches to
Mazurek (2011) also found that, among a sample aggression and tantrums, there are more issues to
of children and adolescents with autism spectrum consider than some clinicians may realize. First,
disorder, 56% engaged in some form of physical all treatment decisions must consider the client’s
aggressive behavior towards caregivers and dignity; children and adults with developmental
another 32% of children and adolescents engaged disabilities deserve to be treated with respect
in physical aggression towards non-caregivers. while their families seek treatment for the chal-
Higher rates of aggressive behavior were also lenging behavior. The Association for Behavior
seen in a study by Farmer and Aman (2011) in Analysis states that a clinician’s responsibility is
children with comorbid ASD and attention defi- to ensure their client’s right to effective treat-
cit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and in a study ment; this includes the right to services whose
by McClintock, Hall, and Oliver (2003) in indi- goal is the individual’s personal welfare and the
viduals with comorbid ASD and intellectual dis- right to the most effective interventions available
ability (ID). These high rates of aggressive (Association for Behavior Analysis, 1989).
behavior are also found in young toddlers. For However, family differences in culture, socioeco-
example, Fox, Keller, Grede, and Bartosz (2007) nomic status, rural areas, and other such factors
found that 24% of children (age birth to three) could impact which treatment the family feels is
referred to early intervention services in their best for their child and thus the strength of the
study displayed aggressive behavior, while 41% treatment and treatment integrity. For example,
of these children exhibited tantrum behaviors. while some families may embrace medication for
Treating aggression and tantrum behaviors their children’s behavior, other families may be
must be a priority for clinicians working with hesitant to have their young children dependent
children with disabilities. These behavior prob- on medication for a variety of personal reasons.
lems put the child at risk for limited community Similarly, access to treatment centers can be an
involvement, long-term inpatient care, and issue for both children from low-income families
restricted learning environments, as well as put and children living in rural areas. These just
the child and caregivers at risk for harm. Not sur- scratch the surface of the complex issues families
prisingly then, this challenging behavior has also must manage while also pursuing the best treat-
been found to be a strong predictor of parental ments for their child. Therefore, this review of
stress (Baker, Blacher, Crnic, & Edelbrock, 2002; treatment approaches to aggression and tantrums
Emerson, 2003). Gardner and Moffatt (1990) will prioritize not only individual client dignity
also state that the presence of aggressive behavior and personal welfare, but family considerations
is one of the strongest predictors of overall qual- as well.
ity of life for the child with developmental dis-
abilities. Both Murphy et al. (2005) and Matson
and Neal (2009) found that unless effectively Behavioral Approach
treated, these challenging behaviors would
remain chronic among children and adolescents. Assessment
The persistence and severity of aggression and As Brosnan and Healy (2011) establish in their
tantrum behaviors across the lifespan necessitate review of behavioral treatments, aggression is
the use of the most powerful evidence-based fundamentally a learned set of behaviors related
interventions (Matson, 2009). to the consequences which reliably follow it.
260 A. Issarraras and J.L. Matson

If interventions do not accurately alter these ior (“B”) and the immediate consequence of the
consequences, the aggressive behavior may be behavior (“C”) (Kozlowski & Matson, 2012) and
further reinforced. One review by Matson et al. is one of the most commonly used direct observa-
(2011) of 173 studies in which functional tion method. Observing a behavior using this
assessment techniques were used to determine method can help a clinician formulate their
the environmental variables related to the chal- hypothesis for the function of a behavior. For
lenging behavior of people with ASD revealed example, imagine a child engages in an extreme
that for many of these participants, attention or tantrum behavior at the grocery store. The ante-
escape from demands maintained their chal- cedent (“A”) of this behavior may have been a
lenging behaviors. However, as Matson and parent requesting the child stay next to the cart
Nebel-Schwalm (2007) state, though these and keep quiet. The tantrum behavior (“B”)
challenging behaviors may appear straightfor- might then be followed by the parents picking up
ward, often their topography does not ade- the child and leaving the store (“C”). Thus, a cli-
quately reflect the complex patterns of behavior nician might hypothesize that the tantrum behav-
maintaining them. Children with developmen- ior is maintained by escape from certain
tal disabilities often are unable to communi- environments. Understanding why a child exhib-
cate their wants and needs, and so often cannot its a behavior then helps a clinician plan the best
explain why they engaged in certain behaviors. treatment to address the function of the
So, in order to alter the consequences that behavior.
address the learned aggressive behaviors, often Another method for identifying the function
clinicians will conduct a functional behavioral of behavior is an experimental functional analy-
assessment. A functional behavioral assess- sis (EFA). EFA is a method that involves the sys-
ment (FBA) describes the process of gathering tematic manipulation of antecedents and
and interpreting data related to the function of consequences associated with the target chal-
a problem behavior (O’Neill, Albin, Storey, lenging behavior (O’Neill et al., 2014). However,
Horner, & Sprague, 2014). FBA is extremely though this approach gives the best insight into
valuable when designing an effective and effi- the function of a behavior, these procedures are
cient behavioral intervention as it provides often long and require a large number of
information about what reinforcers to change resources. The use of experimental functional
to reduce aggressive or tantrum behavior analysis with severe aggressive behavior espe-
through an individualized treatment. There are cially should be carefully considered. This is
many studies which have demonstrated FBAs because in an EFA, a clinician manipulates the
as effective in identifying the reinforcers that antecedents to provoke the target behavior, which
maintain challenging behavior (see Didden, may have harmful effects in these circumstances.
Duker, & Korzilius, 1997; Iwata et al., 1994; Though these experimental manipulations could
Roane, Fisher, & Carr, 2016). give valuable insight and inform more effective
As discussed, when using a behavior approach intervention strategies, there are potential risks to
to aggression, the first step in treatment is to iden- escalating an individual to the extent that they
tify the function maintaining the problem behav- display severe aggression. Not only could some-
ior. One method for identifying the function of one get injured if the behavior is not quickly
behavior is through a direct observation method. managed, but the issue of client dignity would
Systematic, direct observation of the aggressive also be overlooked. Matson (2009) therefore
behavior and the environmental variables related advises the use real-time data recording such as
to the behavior then provides a descriptive analy- the “ABC” approach in evaluating more serious
sis of the behavior’s function (O’Neill et al., and severe aggressive behaviors over a compli-
2014). The “ABC” approach, collects data cated/potentially harmful manipulation such as
regarding the antecedent event (“A”), the behav- an EFA.
Aggression and Tantrums 261

Treatment understood by their parents, caregivers, and


Once the clinician has a better understanding of teachers.
what is reinforcing the aggressive or tantrum Differential reinforcement is another
behavior, then a plan for treatment can be formu- extremely effective treatment in changing aggres-
lated. Treatments aimed at managing aggressive sive behavior (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007).
behaviors should be based upon the descriptive The differential reinforcement procedure consists
information gathered from the direct observation of removing reinforcement for the aggressive
methods or EFA previously discussed. When behavior and establishing a provision of rein-
treatment procedures address the function of forcement either based on an acceptable, posi-
behavior they are more likely to result in success- tive, alternative behavior or a decrease in the
ful treatment outcomes. The most commonly undesired behavior (Cooper et al., 2007).
used treatments for the developmental disabili- Differential reinforcement has many forms which
ties population and aggressive behavior are func- differ in terms of the form of the reinforcement of
tional communication training, differential the alternative behavior. In terms of treating chal-
reinforcement of behavior, and extinction proce- lenging behavior, there are four types of differen-
dures (Matson et al., 2005). Most often, a combi- tial reinforcement: differential reinforcement of
nation of these different procedures is used to incompatible behavior (DRI), differential rein-
address the target behavior. forcement of alternative behavior (DRA), differ-
In Functional Communication Training ential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO),
(FCT), the child’s communication of a want or and differential reinforcement of low rates
need is reinforced as an alternative to the aggres- (DRL).
sive or tantrum behavior they are engaging in. Differential reinforcement of incompatible
This allows the individual to gain control over behavior (DRI) and differential reinforcement of
access to their reinforcer, which varies greatly alternative behavior (DRA) procedures are simi-
from individual to individual. By giving the indi- lar in that appropriate behavior is reinforced
vidual more control, FCT aims to increase the while reinforcement is withheld for the target
use of this functional communication more and aggressive or tantrum behavior (Brosnan and
decrease the frequency of the aggressive behav- Healy, 2011). Because DRA and DRI procedures
ior until it is completely gone (Chezan, Drasgow, reinforce the alternative behavior, often they are
& Martin, 2014). This communication behavior conducive to the acquisition of new skills in chil-
can be a verbal response (e.g. “I want to play on dren. Specifically, in a DRI procedure, an incom-
my iPad”), a signed request (e.g. signing “done” patible behavior to the aggressive behavior, in
when a child is ready to leave an environment), or which simultaneous occurrence would be impos-
can also involve the use of a communication sible, is reinforced (Cooper et al., 2007). This can
device, such as a Dynavox or PECS system, in be as simple as reinforcing a child’s hands
which the child can either have their device speak together in their lap, a behavior that would be
or present their caregiver with a card displaying incompatible with hitting and pinching. Similarly,
their desired reinforcer. Using FCT can be incred- a DRA procedure involves reinforcing of an
ibly effective for both children and adults with acceptable alternative behavior, but this behavior
developmental disabilities (Chezan et al., 2014), does not have to be incompatible with the prob-
but the child’s cognitive and adaptive functioning lem behavior.
must be considered when choosing an appropri- Another type of differential reinforcement is
ate communicative alternative. It would be inap- differential reinforcement of other behavior
propriate to select a communicative alternative (DRO). Unlike the DRI and DRA procedures, a
outside of the child’s skill set, as this could just DRO procedure tries to reduce the rate of occur-
increase the child’s frustration and engagement rence of the aggressive or tantrum behavior by
in the problem behavior. Ideally, this alternative focusing on reinforcing its absence (Cooper
would be easily accessible to the child and easily et al., 2007). This absence of the problem behav-
262 A. Issarraras and J.L. Matson

ior could be at a specific time or during an entire with these severe behaviors, which is an increase
time interval. For example, a child could be rein- in the behavior immediately following the extinc-
forced after a full afternoon without displaying tion procedures (Cooper et al., 2007). This burst
aggressive behavior. One can see how often these in aggressive behavior can have obvious and
procedures are used in conjunction with others, severe consequences for all involved.
as they may not be as effective on their own. Additionally, extinction procedures may actually
There is also a final type of differential reinforce- lead to increases in aggression (Lerman, Iwata, &
ment of low rates (DRL). As Cooper et al. (2007) Wallace, 1999). It’s important that this procedure
describe, a DRL procedure delivers reinforce- be used in conjunction with other behavioral
ment following the occurrence of a problem methods, such as the ones previously described.
behavior, then gradually increases the length of Matson et al. (2005) also found that successful
time between displays of problem behavior for interventions often utilized antecedent altera-
reinforcement to be delivered. A DRL procedure tions, reinforcement-based strategies and conse-
can be effective at reducing rates of behavior, but quence manipulations.
it works more slowly than other procedures, mak-
ing it less appropriate for use with severe aggres-
sion and tantrum behaviors that must be addressed Pharmacotherapy Approach
quickly.
Finally, there is the procedure of extinction. Due to increased collaboration among interdisci-
Aggressive and tantrum behaviors are said to be plinary teams of clinicians, children with devel-
“put on extinction” when a behavior no longer opmental disabilities will often have large team
produces the reinforcing consequences it previ- of specialists addressing different aspects of their
ously produced (Cooper et al., 2007). In an day-to-day life. In the field of developmental dis-
extinction procedure, the relationship between abilities, pharmacotherapy has been researched
whatever environmental variable that was main- most extensively for aggression in children and
taining this behavior through reinforcement and adults with ASD (Martin, Koenig, Anderson, &
the aggressive behavior itself has been broken. A Scahill, 2003). In terms of aggression and tan-
simple example of this could be if a child’s tan- trum behavior, psychiatrists and/or pediatricians
trum behavior was maintained by the attention may be sought to help manage the presenting
from a teacher in the classroom. In this case, problem behavior, in which case an atypical anti-
attention could be in the form a reprimand from psychotic medication is often prescribed to treat
the teacher. An extinction procedure would the aggressive behavior. There are several rea-
involve a plan to no longer provide this attention sons why the continued use of psychotropic med-
to the child when the tantrum behavior occurs. ications is problematic at best.
Removing reinforcement for the tantrum behav- Two drugs, risperidone and aripiprazole, are
ior then decreases the likelihood that the behavior FDA-approved for use in children and adoles-
will occur again. Instead of engaging in the tan- cents diagnosed with ASD (United States Food
trum behavior, the child could also use an alter- and Drug Administration, 2006, 2009). Both ris-
native behavior (such as raising their hand) to peridone and aripiparazole are atypical antipsy-
access the same reinforcement for teacher atten- chotics known to have adverse effects such as
tion, which is why extinction is often an impor- weight gain, increased appetite, sedation, tired-
tant procedure to implement in treatment. ness, drooling, and tremor (Deb et al., 2014). As
However, there are many situations where Deb et al. (2014) describe, these types of medica-
extinction would not be an appropriate procedure tions target specific neurotransmitters to try and
to implement. In the previous example, if the stabilize dopamine production. It is important to
child were biting another student, then ethically note that their use is specifically for the symptom
the teacher could not simply ignore the behavior. of “irritability” and not any of the core symptoms
Also, there is the possibility of extinction bursts of ASD. Even more troubling, Adler and col-
Aggression and Tantrums 263

leagues (2015) found that for risperidone and 2015; Cohen et al., 2013; Matson & Wilkins,
aripiprazole, again the only two drugs approved 2008; Tyrer et al., 2008), a great number of chil-
by the US Food and Drug Administration, most dren, adolescents, and adults with disabilities are
challenging behaviors frequently remained drug still prescribed these psychotropic medications.
refractory to both drugs. So, not only did these Matson and Dempsey (2008) noticed that many
drugs show negative side effects for the partici- children on these anti-psychotic medications are
pants, even in the short trial period, but the drugs as young as 4 and 5 years old. Finding this unac-
overall frequently did not decrease or address the ceptable, they suggest that aggression and “prob-
aggressive behavior (Adler et al., 2015). This is lem [behaviors] should be chronic, severe and
especially troubling considering the high rates of unresponsive to psychological and educationally
children with ASD, intellectual disability, or based treatments” before addressing the behavior
other developmental disabilities found to be on with medication. Because children and even
these medications. One study by Spencer et al. adults with profound or severe intellectual dis-
(2013) reported as high as 64% of children with ability are often cared for by their legal guard-
ASD were prescribed at least one antipsychotic ians, they are not legally able to consent to the
medication. use of psychotropic medications. When using
Matson and Jang (2014) describe the current medications for behavioral management, it is
prescription drug dilemma as “compromised”. therefore of the utmost importance that practitio-
Their inspection of the use of prescriptions found ners must consider the individual’s dignity, as
that often many drugs often do not target the well as use of less restrictive methods first.
mechanisms they suggest they do, instead simply Beyond the reasons discussed previously, the
sedating the individual into a state of compliance. evidence that does exist for the use of these med-
This is most likely because little is understood ications often do not answer important research
about the neural mechanisms involved in the questions to improve their claims. For example,
aggressive behavior. Additionally, these com- one leading limitation of any pharmacological
plexities are what make this area of treatment dif- research, especially in this population, is the lack
ficult to completely disregard. Some studies have of long-term studies investigating the benefits
shown that children and adolescents treated with and costs of long-term drug treatment. It is ethi-
appropriate doses of psychotropic medications cally understood why such studies would be dif-
(including risperidone) did show reductions in ficult to conduct, because these medications are
aggression, tantrums, or other types of challeng- already known to have adverse side effects.
ing behavior (Shea et al., 2004). Meanwhile, Similarly, research on these anti-psychotic medi-
Tyrer et al. (2008) found that individuals assigned cations often fails to follow up with children as
to a placebo showed a similar decrease in aggres- they mature, to investigate how the behavior
sive behavior as those given risperidone. Much of symptoms continue to be controlled (or not).
the research falls between these two extremes. As There is also little research comparing pharma-
Farmer, Thurm, and Grant (2013) find in their cological approaches to their alternative, namely
review, there is simply no evidence that defini- behavioral approaches as those previously
tively supports the use of pharmacological treat- described. These important questions only
ments as effective or efficacious. Though there scratch at the surface of what remains to be
might be some evidence that drug treatments answered regarding the use of psychotropic
work for some children with developmental dis- medications in children with developmental dis-
abilities, it simply is not enough to justify such abilities. However, an unfortunate truth as
widespread use of antipsychotic medications. pointed out by Matson and Konst (2015), is that
Further complicating the subject, although these research studies are often funded by the
many studies continue to show the instability and very people and corporations who stand to ben-
unreliability of drug treatments (Adler et al., efit from their success – the pharmaceutical
264 A. Issarraras and J.L. Matson

manufacturers themselves. This bias, intentional behaviors are put in the more restrictive envi-
or not, complicates the issues surrounding phar- ronments. Perhaps implementing mindfulness
macological research and interventions even more. training for those who work closely with these
students can also be helpful to reduce aggressive
behaviors in the school system. Additionally,
 aregiver, Professional Staff,
C having typically developing students understand
and Clinician Attitudes why their peers engage in these challenging
Toward Aggression behaviors could also lead to a more empathetic
learning environment, creating a bridge to begin
Often professionals who work with children with building the social supports and friendships
developmental disabilities and aggressive behav- often missing from children with developmental
iors do not realize the emotional toll managing disabilities experience.
challenging behaviors can place on themselves.
Knotter, Wissink, Moonen, Stams, and Jansen
(2013) suggest physical aggression often contrib- Supporting Families
utes to the individual having a “reputation” for
“being” aggressive; caregivers may then associ- Often families are most overwhelmed by the
ate aggressive behavior as a personal attack severe aggressive behaviors their children dis-
against them, which can influence their own play. As Lecavalier, Leone, and Wiltz (2006)
emotional reaction to the situation (Knotter et al., found, even though caregivers may differ on the
2013). These negative attitudes, though initially rates of aggressive behavior, there is a strong
toward the aggression, may affect their day to relationship with the stress an individual care-
day management of the client’s behavior. This giver experiences. Further, they found that stress
study of staff and client interactions by Knotter and aggressive behavior often exacerbate each
et al. (2013) found that negative attitudes towards other, which can leave parents and caregivers in
aggression within a team were strongly associ- what can seem like a never-ending cycle
ated with more frequent use of coercive measures (Lecavalier et al., 2006). Huge tantrum behaviors
such as physical restraints. Physical restraints are in public places can also make parents feel inad-
undignified and often traumatic experiences for equate and judged by their peers. These fears
the individual being restrained. It is important for may not be unwarranted. For example, the con-
all those who work with children with develop- cept of the “refrigerator mother” dates back to the
mental disabilities to decrease the use of physical late 1960s and was used to explain why some
restraints as much as possible. The implications children were “autistic”, essentially blaming
for these findings strongly suggest that training their mothers as cold women who did not show
staff to decrease negative attributions with enough love and affection to their children.
aggression could enhance treatment and the Unfortunately, remnants of the “refrigerator
entire environment for both staff and the children mother” era in the thoughts of others in their
or adults being served. communities may also leave parents feeling as if
In a similar approach, Singh et al. (2006) their child’s aggression and tantrum behavior is
found that including mindfulness training their “fault”. As clinicians, it is important to
increased the staff member’s ability to effec- understand families’ struggle with these types of
tively manage the aggressive behavior and behaviors in their lives and the many ways it can
learning of the individuals. This evidence sug- affect them. Often parents and siblings feel alien-
gests that attitudes towards aggressive behavior ated and unsupported in caring for a child with
are not only important and have consequences, aggressive behaviors; these feelings can be
but also that they can be managed. There are exacerbated when the child grows up and
­
also larger implications for this in school set- becomes stronger, leaving them more exhausted
tings where often children with aggressive and desperate for alternatives.
Aggression and Tantrums 265

Though it is important to be respectful of a are described, there is no consistent means of


families’ culture and beliefs, it is also necessary reporting them, and sometimes abstract terms,
to be weary of certain unsubstantiated treatments such as “low rates”, are used. Addressing these
like the ones described by Green et al. (2006). limitations would add to the strengths of current
Parents often undertake unusual treatment meth- treatments for children with developmental
ods such as special diets, aromatherapy, acupunc- disabilities.
ture, and other treatments that do not have strong Also, though much research has been done
evidence supporting their use; these types of on decreasing aggressive behavior over time,
treatments tend to be sought out more often by more studies must be done on effective trainings
families with a child with severe autism and for managing severe aggressive behavior in the
aggressive behaviors (Green et al., 2006). moment it is already happening, and how to best
However, if parents are continuously pulled train parents and caregivers in managing them.
between overmedicating their child or imple- For example, if the child pulls hair and bites to
menting treatments that require lengthy amounts escape a situation, parents, caregivers, teachers,
of time to take effect, then it is understandable and other individuals who work with children
why any alternative treatment offering a quick with developmental disabilities need to know
relief would be welcomed. Guiding parents to the the safest and best approaches to de-escalating
most appropriate evidence-based treatment these highly stressful situations, such as proper
should always be done respectfully while provid- blocking techniques, how to manage their own
ing them correct and comprehensive information stress, etc. One study by Calabro, Mackey, and
regarding the effectiveness and potential side Williams (2002) looked at two programs – The
effects of such treatments. Nonviolent Crisis Intervention® (CPI),
(National Crisis Prevention Institute, Inc.,
Brookfield, WI) and Handle with Care (Handle
Future Directions with Care Behavioral Management System, Old
Bridge, NJ). Both programs are designed to
The past 50 years have been prolific for research teach mental health facilities staff about how to
into the function of challenging behaviors, the prevent and control disruptive behavior of cli-
best approaches to treatment, and many aspects ents, including self-­defense skills for staff to use
of caring for a child with developmental disabili- when encountering an aggressive client.
ties with aggression and/or tantrum behaviors. However, more information is needed on
However, there are still questions to be answered. whether these programs can be also be taught
In the field of developmental disabilities, assess- and utilized by families of children with devel-
ment of individuals with profound and severe opmental disabilities and aggressive behavior at
intellectual disability has traditionally been dif- a low cost and in a timely manner. Though many
ficult because these individuals lack functional clinicians who work with children with develop-
language and may have many issues in their mental disabilities can also have experience
adaptive functioning. As Smith and Matson managing aggressive behavior, most parents or
(2010) recognize, behavior problems commonly teachers do not. Even if a parent is implement-
occur among individuals with ID, and so it would ing new behavioral treatments, the aggressive
be extremely beneficial for clinicians to under- behavior could still occur. More research on
stand this population’s specific needs. Even so, effective behavior management programs is
there is still a large disparity in the development needed to ensure families are equipped to han-
of assessment techniques appropriate for this dle these aggressive behaviors as they occur.
population (Smith & Matson, 2010). As far as the limitations of many pharmaco-
Additionally, many studies fail to report any fol- logical studies already discussed, future research
low-up measures and whether generalization of could also compare different types of treatment
treatment gains occurred. When treatment effects with how they relate to a child’s gender, the
266 A. Issarraras and J.L. Matson

socioeconomic status of their family, parental developmental delays. American Journal of Mental
Retardation, 107(6), 433–444.
education, region where they currently receive
Bhatia, M. S., Dhar, N. K., Singhal, P. K., Nigam, V. R.,
services, and various other social markers that Malik, S. C., & Mullick, D. N. (1990). Temper tan-
could inform other disparities that the field is trums. Prevalence and etiology in a non-referral outpa-
only recently beginning to address. tient setting. Clinical Pediatrics (Phila), 29, 311–315.
Brosnan, J., & Healy, O. (2011). A review of behavioral
interventions for the treatment of aggression in indi-
viduals with developmental disabilities. Research in
Conclusion Developmental Disabilities, 32(2), 437–446.
Calabro, K., Mackey, T. A., & Williams, S. (2002).
Evaluation of training designed to prevent and man-
Aggressive and tantrum behaviors are often the
age patient violence. Issues in Mental Health Nursing,
most challenging behaviors that clinicians work- 23, 3–15.
ing with children with developmental disabilities Chezan, L. C., Drasgow, E., & Martin, C. A. (2014).
and their families face. These behaviors co-occur Discrete-trial functional analysis and functional
communication training with three adults with intel-
at high rates and tend to be chronic without inter-
lectual disabilities and problem behavior. Journal of
vention. The most effective approach to treat- Behavioral Education, 23(2), 221–246.
ment for aggression and tantrums is a behavioral Cohen, D., Raffin, M., Canitano, R., Bodeau, N., Bonnot,
analysis approach. There are many procedures O., Périsse, D., … Laurent, C. (2013). Risperidone or
aripiprazole in children and adolescents with autism
that can be implemented that will target the prob-
and/or intellectual disability: A Bayesian meta-­
lem behavior and can also work to improve alter- analysis of efficacy and secondary effects. Research in
native and accepted behaviors. Clinicians must Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(1), 167–175.
be careful in relying on anti-psychotic medica- Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle
tion to treat certain symptoms of developmental
River, NJ: Pearson.
disabilities as research has shown it to be increas- Deb, S., Farmah, B. K., Arshad, E., Deb, T., Roy, M., &
ingly problematic. Not only are these treatments Unwin, G. L. (2014). The effectiveness of aripiprazole
often ineffective unless paired with a behavioral in the management of problem behaviour in people
with intellectual disabilities, developmental disabili-
plan, but they may place the individual at greater
ties and/or autistic spectrum disorder – a systematic
risk for harm. Combining an effective behavioral review. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
plan with a well-trained staff is the best founda- 35(3), 711–725.
tion for success in treatment of aggression and Didden, R., Duker, P. C., & Korzilius, H. (1997). Meta-­
analytic study on treatment effectiveness for problem
tantrum behaviors. Clinicians and researchers
behaviors with individuals who have mental retarda-
still have much to investigate and learn in this tion. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 101,
area of developmental disabilities, and there is 387–399.
still a need for more research on efficient, Dominick, K. C., Davis, N. O., Lainhart, J., Tager-­
Flusberg, H., & Folstein, S. (2007). Atypical behav-
evidence-­based, and dignified treatments.
iors in children with autism and children with a history
of language impairment. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 28(2), 145–162.
Emerson, E. (2003). Mothers of children and adolescents
References with intellectual disability: Social and economic situa-
tion, mental health status, and the self-assessed social
Adler, B. A., Wink, L. K., Early, M., Shaffer, R., Minshawi, and psychological impact of the child’s difficulties.
N., McDougle, C. J., & Erickson, C. A. (2015). Drug- Journal of Intellectual Disability Research: JIDR,
refractory aggression, self-injurious behavior, and 47(Pt 4–5), 385–399.
severe tantrums in autism spectrum disorders: A chart Farmer, C., Thurm, A., & Grant, P. (2013).
review study. Autism, 19(1), 102–106. Pharmacotherapy for the core symptoms in autistic
Association for Behavior Analysis. (1989). The right to disorder: Current status of the research. Drugs, 73(4),
effective behavioral treatment. Retrieved from www. 303–314.
abainternational.org/ABA/statements/treatment.asp Farmer, C. A., & Aman, M. G. (2011). Aggressive behav-
Baker, B. L., Blacher, J., Crnic, K. A., & Edelbrock, C. ior in a sample of children with autism spectrum dis-
(2002). Behavior problems and parenting stress in orders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1),
families of three-year old children with and without 317–323.
Aggression and Tantrums 267

Fox, R. A., Keller, K. M., Grede, P. L., & Bartosz, A. M. students with autism spectrum disorders. Research in
(2007). A mental health clinic for toddlers with devel- Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(3), 229–246.
opmental delays and behavior problems. Research in Martin, A., Koenig, K., Anderson, G. M., & Scahill, L.
Developmental Disabilities, 28(2), 119–129. (2003). Low-dose fluvoxamine treatment of children
Gardner, W. I., & Moffatt, C. W. (1990). Aggressive behav- and adolescents with pervasive developmental dis-
iour: Definition, assessment, treatment. International orders: A prospective, open-label study. Journal of
Review of Psychiatry, 2(1), 91–100. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(1), 77–85.
Goldin, R. L., Matson, J. L., Tureck, K., Cervantes, P. E., Matson, J. (2009). Aggression and tantrums in children
& Jang, J. (2013). A comparison of tantrum behavior with autism: A review of behavioral treatments and
profiles in children with ASD, ADHD and comor- maintaining variables. Journal of Mental Health
bid ASD and ADHD. Research in Developmental Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 2(3), 169–187.
Disabilities, 34(9), 2669–2675. Matson, J. L., & Dempsey, T. (2008). Autism spectrum
Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly, disorders: Pharmacotherapy for challenging behaviors.
M., & Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treatments Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
used by parents of children with autism. Research in 20(2), 175–191.
Developmental Disabilities, 27(1), 70–84. Matson, J. L., Dixon, D. R., & Matson, M. L. (2005).
Gendreau, P., & Archer, J. (2005). Subtypes of aggression Assessing and treating aggression in children and ado-
in humans and animals. In R. Tremblay, W. Hartup, & lescents with developmental disabilities: A 20-year
J. Archer (Eds.), The developmental origins of aggres- overview. Educational Psychology, 25, 151–181.
sion (pp. 25–46). New York: Guilford Press. Matson, J. L., & Jang, J. (2014). Treating aggression in
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & persons with autism spectrum disorders: A review.
Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(12),
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 3386–3391.
27(2), 197–209. Matson, J. L., & Konst, M. J. (2015). Why pharmacother-
Kanne, S. M., & Mazurek, M. O. (2011). Aggression in apy is overused among persons with autism spectrum
children and adolescents with ASD: Prevalence and disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 9,
risk factors. Journal of Autism and Developmental 34–37.
Disorders, 41(7), 926–937. Matson, J. L., Kozlowski, A. M., Worley, J. A.,
Knotter, M. H., Wissink, I. B., Moonen, X. M. H., Stams, Shoemaker, M. E., Sipes, M., & Horovitz, M. (2011).
G. J. J. M., & Jansen, G. J. (2013). Staff’s attitudes and What is the evidence for environmental causes of
reactions towards aggressive behaviour of clients with challenging behaviors in persons with intellectual dis-
intellectual disabilities: A multi-level study. Research abilities and autism spectrum disorders? Research in
in Developmental Disabilities, 34(5), 1397–1407. Developmental Disabilities, 32(2), 693–698.
Konst, M. J., Matson, J. L., & Turygin, N. (2013). Matson, J. L., & Neal, D. (2009). Psychotropic medica-
Comparing the rates of tantrum behavior in children tion use for challenging behaviors in persons with
with ASD and ADHD as well as children with comor- intellectual disabilities: An overview. Research in
bid ASD and ADHD diagnoses. Research in Autism Developmental Disabilities, 30(3), 572–586.
Spectrum Disorders, 7(11), 1339–1345. Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. (2007). Assessing
Kozlowski, A., & Matson, J. L. (2012). Interview and challenging behaviors in children with autism spec-
observation methods in Functional Assessment. In trum disorders: A review. Research in Developmental
J. L. Matson (Ed.), Functional assessment for chal- Disabilities, 28(6), 567–579.
lenging behaviors (pp. 105–124). New York: Springer Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2008). Antipsychotic drugs
Science & Business Media. for aggression in intellectual disability. The Lancet,
Lecavalier, L., Leone, S., & Wiltz, J. (2006). The impact 371, 1–2.
of behaviour problems on caregiver stress in young McClintock, K., Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (2003). Risk mark-
people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of ers associated with challenging behaviours in people
Intellectual Disability Research, 50(3), 172–183. with intellectual disabilities: A meta-analytic study.
Lerman, D. C., Iwata, B. A., & Wallace, M. D. (1999). Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47(6),
Side effects of extinction: Prevalence of bursting 405–416.
and aggression during the treatment of self-injurious Murphy, G. H., Beadle-Brown, J., Wing, L., Gould, J.,
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), Shah, A., & Holmes, N. (2005). Chronicity of chal-
1–8. lenging behaviours in people with severe intellectual
Mace, F. C., & Critchfield, T. S. (2010). Translational disabilities and/or autism: A total population sample.
research in behavior analysis: Historical traditions and Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
imperative for the future. Journal of the Experimental 35(4), 405–418.
Analysis of Behavior, 93(3), 293–312. O’Neill, R. E., Albin, R. W., Storey, K., Horner, R. H., &
Machalicek, W., O’Reilly, M. F., Beretvas, N., Sigafoos, Sprague, J. R. (2014). Functional assessment and pro-
J., & Lancioni, G. E. (2007). A review of intervention gram development for problem behavior: A practical
to reduce challenging behavior in school settings for handbook (3rd ed.). Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning.
268 A. Issarraras and J.L. Matson

Risley, T. R. (1968). The effects and side effects of pun- Spencer, D., Marshall, J., Post, B., Kulakodlu, M.,
ishing the autistic behaviors of a deviant child. Journal Newschaffer, C., Dennen, T., … Jain, A. (2013).
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(I), 21–34. Psychotropic medication use and polypharmacy in
Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., & Carr, J. E. (2016). Applied children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics,
behavior analysis as treatment for autism spectrum 132(5), 833–840.
disorder. The Journal of Pediatrics, 175, 27–32. Tremblay, R. (2000). The development of aggressive
Shea, S., Turgay, A., Carroll, A., Schulz, M., Orlik, H., behaviour during childhood: What have we learned in
Smith, I., & Dunbar, F. (2004). Risperidone in the the past century? International Journal of Behavioral
treatment of disruptive behavioral symptoms in chil- Development, 24, 129–141.
dren with autistic and other pervasive developmental Tyrer, P., Oliver-Africano, P. C., Ahmed, Z., Bouras,
disorders. Pediatrics, 114(5), e634–e641. N., Cooray, S., Deb, S., … Crawford, M. (2008).
Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Winton, A. S. W., Curtis, Risperidone, haloperidol, and placebo in the treatment
W. J., Wahler, R. G., Sabaawi, M., & McAleavey, K. of aggressive challenging behaviour in patients with
(2006). Mindful staff increase learning and reduce intellectual disability: A randomised controlled trial.
aggression in adults with developmental disabili- The Lancet, 371(9606), 57–63.
ties. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27(5), U. S. Food and Drug Administration/Center for Drug
545–558. Evaluation and Research. (2006). FDA approves the
Smith, K. R. M., & Matson, J. L. (2010). Behavior prob- first drug to treat irritability associated with autism,
lems: Differences among intellectually disabled adults Risperdal. FDA News.
with co-morbid autism spectrum disorders and epi- U. S. Food and Drug Administration/Center for Drug
lepsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(5), Evaluation and Research. (2009). Aripiprazole clinical
1062–1069. addendum. Washington, DC: Otsuka Pharmaceutical.
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children
with Intellectual
and Developmental Disabilities:
Current Practices in Assessment
and Treatment

Casey J. Clay, Courtney D. Jorgenson,
and SungWoo Kahng

Contents
Introduction
Introduction   269
Prevalence   270 One of the first studies on treating self-injury was
Topographies   270
conducted by Tate and Baroff (1966) with a
9-year-old boy. Tate and Baroff defined self-­
Measurement   270
injurious behavior (SIB) as behavior which pro-
Assessment   271 duces physical injury to the individual’s own
Causes (Why Does SIB Occur?)   274 body. The young boy had a 6-year history of
engaging in SIB, which consisted of face slap-
Treatment   275
ping and banging his head forcefully against
Conclusion   281 doors. He had developed bilateral cataracts and a
References   281 complete detachment of the left retina likely
caused from his head-directed SIB. The research-
ers decreased the SIB of the young boy via con-
tingent withdrawal of physical contact and later
with response-contingent electric shock. Since
the original Tate and Baroff study, multiple
reviews (e.g., Carr, 1977; Iwata, Pace, Dorsey,
et al., 1994; Johnson & Baumeister, 1978; Kahng,
Iwata, & Lewin, 2002a, 2002b; Matson &
LoVullo, 2008; Richman, 2008) on the causes
and treatment of SIB in persons with intellectual
and developmental disabilities (IDD) have high-
C.J. Clay • S. Kahng (*) lighted the emergence of multiple, effective
Department of Health Psychology,
assessments and treatments for SIB.
University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
e-mail: Kahngs@health.missouri.edu The dangerous nature of SIB makes immedi-
acy and effectiveness of the intervention critical.
C.D. Jorgenson
Department of Special Education, University of Additionally, intervening earlier is important as a
Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA longer history of SIB may result in greater tissue

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 269


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_16
270 C.J. Clay et al.

damage (Newell, Challis, Boros, & Bodfish, (Griffin et al., 1987; Kahng et al.; Kurtz et al.;
2002). It is also important to identify and inter- Matson & LoVullo, 2008). Multiple reviews have
vene in SIB to avoid worsening the problem also reported that individuals typically engage in
behavior (e.g., greater intensity, additional topog- multiple forms of SIB at a time (Hyman et al.,
raphies) and to minimize the possibility of treat- 1990; Kahng et al., 2002b; Kurtz et al.; Oliver
ment resistance (Schroeder et al., 2014). This et al., 1987). Categorization of SIB has also
chapter will describe effective assessment and occurred by using severity as an indicator.
treatment procedures in children with IDD. Severity may be difficult to define; therefore spe-
cific measurement procedures are necessary (e.g.,
Iwata, Pace, Kissel, Nau, & Farber, 1990). These
Prevalence will be discussed in the following section.

The prevalence of SIB in individuals with IDD


has been found to be higher in children than Measurement
adults. Estimates from client databases receiving
IDD services in California and New York (N = 89, Identifying a method of measuring SIB must take
415) showed 15.5% of children under the age of place in order to begin treatment. The many
20 with IDD engaged in SIB compared to 9.05% dimensions (e.g., frequency, intensity, magnitude
of reported adults ages 20–45 (Rojahn, or force, size, location) of SIB have been mea-
Borthwick-Duffy, & Jacobson, 1993). Oliver, sured multiple ways. Measurement of SIB is
Murphy, and Corbett (1987) examined the preva- typically done through direct observation by
lence of SIB among 596 individuals with DD and counting and recording the number of times the
found that the prevalence of SIB was higher in behavior occurs (e.g., Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone,
late childhood and adolescence (i.e., between 10 Smith, & Mazaleski, 1993) or through recording
and 30 years old), and individuals who exhibited the duration of continuous SIB (e.g., Goh et al.,
more severe SIB were more likely to be signifi- 1995) and bouts of SIB (e.g., Kroeker, Touchette,
cantly younger. Data showing a higher preva- Engleman, & Sandman, 2004). These types of
lence of SIB in children is concerning and measures are most commonly used during assess-
provides more evidence that effective interven- ment and treatment. Other measures such as the
tions be implemented as soon as possible. severity of SIB provide descriptive information
but are used less often.
Objective measures of SIB severity may be
Topographies difficult to obtain due to the individualized nature
of individuals who engage in SIB. For example, a
Multiple reviews on SIB of persons with IDD scratch may be a relatively minor injury. However,
have reported a range of ways SIB has been clas- for an individual with a blood clotting disorder, it
sified. Oliver et al. (1987) categorized SIB into may be life threatening. Severity could vary
19 different topographies. Kahng et al. (2002a) based on different topographies of SIB, which
identified 13 topographies reported in the treat- may cause different amounts of tissue damage.
ment research literature. Although classification For example, head-directed SIB in which the
is relatively arbitrary, the most common topogra- individual targets hard, sharp corners versus head
phies of SIB tend to be head-directed SIB hitting with an open palm might lead to more
(Hyman, Fisher, Mercugliano, & Cataldo, 1990; severe tissue damage. Measures of SIB severity
Kahng et al., 2002a; Kurtz et al., 2003) such as have been taken from medical literature that take
head banging. Some other topographies included into account the number of injuries, the location
self-biting, body hitting, pica, scratching, skin of the injuries, and the type of injury (Iwata,
picking, hair pulling, pinching, bruxism, hand Pace, Kissel, et al., 1990; Wilson, Iwata, &
mouthing, eye gouging, and orifice digging Bloom, 2012). Other studies on prevalence of
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 271

SIB have classified severity based on frequency observe SIB. This information can quantify pre-
of the occurrence of SIB (e.g., Cooper, 1998; vious injuries, which may help to determine
Smith, Branford, Collacott, Cooper, & future risk. The SIB scale may also be a useful
McGrother, 1996). Generally, SIB that is head-­ permanent product measure, which can be used
directed, frequent, and creates larger amounts of to measure treatment effects.
tissue damage is classified as more severe. Recently, Wilson et al. (2012) described a
Another possible measure of severity of SIB more objective measure of SIB injury. They used
could be through identifying overt signs of pain. a computer-assisted measurement tool to mea-
Work by McGrath, Rosmus, Canfield, Campbell, sure wound surface area. This technique involved
and Hennigar (1998) and Breau, McGrath, photographing the injured area and uploading the
Camfield, Rosmus, and Finley (2000) has con- photograph into a computer program, which ana-
tributed to the Non-Communicating Children’s lyzed (measure) the surface area of the wound.
Pain Checklist (NCCPC), which is a reliable and Wilson et al. used wound surface area as their
validated rating scale that covers the presence dependent variable to measure treatment effects
and intensity of observational pain across seven on covert SIB of a woman with Prader-Willi syn-
subscales including vocal, facial expression, eat- drome. Computer programs have also been used
ing/sleeping, social/personality, facial expres- to analyze videotapes depicting SIB and compute
sion, activity, body/limbs, and physiological. the force of impact of self-striking (Newell et al.,
This checklist involves retrospective report and 2002).
direct observation to determine pain in children
and adults with IDD typically following an injury
(e.g., burns, falls, injury), but may also provide Assessment
some indication of pain and severity of SIB. For
example, Breau et al. (2003) were able to delin- Vollmer, Sloman, and Borrero (2009) separated
eate different forms of SIB (e.g., targeting differ- SIB assessment methods into two main catego-
ent locations on the body) based on results of the ries: indirect and direct. Indirect assessment does
checklist. not require information recording to occur at the
Multiple procedures to measure SIB before, same time the SIB is occurring. For example,
during, and after it has occurred have been devel- information gathered from caregivers before the
oped. However, it is sometimes difficult, or SIB has occurred (e.g., interviews and question-
impossible, to observe the behavior as it is occur- naires) provide the clinician with information on
ring (e.g., environments with limited data record- topography and severity they might expect to see
ing availability or SIB that occurs when no one is when conducting direct assessment. Similarly,
present). When this is the case, measurement measurement of permanent products (e.g.,
must take place after the behavior has occurred wounds, abrasions, tissue damage) after the
and has left some evidence of occurrence (e.g., occurrence of SIB can lead to valuable informa-
wound). This is referred to as permanent product tion when examining the effect of the treatment,
measurement. This measurement system is con- or to identify correlated variables in the environ-
venient for the data collector if the SIB occurs ment to inform direct assessment procedures.
infrequently, or covertly, because the data collec- Direct assessment occurs when SIB is being
tor does not have to continuously monitor the directly observed and typically involves data
individual to record SIB. For example, the Self-­ being recorded at the time the SIB is taking place.
Injury Trauma Scale (SIT; Iwata, Pace, Kissel, For example, experimenter observation of clients
et al., 1990) provides information on number, in their typical setting when SIB is occurring pro-
type, location, and severity of visible tissue dam- vides information on the variables surrounding
age. The SIT scale is completed by caregivers how the SIB is occurring and a more precise
who can conduct a visual check of the individual. description of topography. Clinical manipulation
However, it does not require caregivers to directly of the variables as part of assessment (e.g., func-
272 C.J. Clay et al.

tional analysis) is also part of direct assessment for conducting an interview; they might serve as
and can lead to more precise intervention proto- the basis of future interviews that could provide
cols. In the following sections, indirect and direct more specific information (Iwata et al., 2013).
assessment of SIB will be discussed in detail. A major disadvantage of indirect assessments
is that they do not specifically identify cause-and-­
effect variables through controlled manipulation
Indirect Assessment (Hall, 2005; Iwata et al., 2013; Thompson &
Borrero, 2011). This may create a problem lead-
Indirect assessments use methods that do not ing to false-positive or false-negative findings
require the assessor to be present while SIB from indirect assessments when trying to deter-
occurs. For this reason, indirect assessments fre- mine the function of SIB. To address false-­
quently involve the use of rating scales (e.g., positive results, researchers have compared
Durand & Crimmins, 1988; Iwata, Pace, Kissel, results from indirect assessments to direct assess-
et al., 1990; Rojahn, Matson, Lott, Esbensen, & ments, and the findings are mixed. Some studies
Smalls, 2001), interviews (e.g., O’Neill, Albin, have found higher correspondence (Arndorfer,
Horner, Storey, & Sprague, 2015), checklists Miltenberger, Woster, Rortvedt, & Gaffaney,
(e.g., Paclawskyj, Matson, Rush, Smalls, & 1994; Cunningham & O’Neill, 2000; Durand &
Vollmer, 2000; Van Houten & Rolider, 1991), and Crimmins, 1988; Hall , 2005; Lewis, Mitchell,
questionnaires (e.g., Lewis, Scott, & Sugai, 1994). Harvey, Green, & McKenzie, 2015), whereas
Some advantages include time efficiency as well other studies have found low correspondence
as less training as compared to direct assessments, between indirect and direct assessments
which allows for greater access for clinicians, (Crawford, Brockel, Schauss, & Miltenberger,
teachers, and aides (Vollmer et al., 2009). 1992; Lerman & Iwata, 1993; Paclawskyj et al.,
Another advantage of indirect assessment is 2000).
that it provides preliminary information to be Another limitation of indirect assessments is
used in direct assessments. For example, the that they rely on human report, which can be sub-
Functional Analysis Screening Tool (FAST; jective and prone to bias, and may take place long
Iwata, DeLeon, & Roscoe, 2013) is an interview after the SIB has occurred. Additionally, teach-
used to identify factors that may influence SIB ers, staff, or caregivers reporting SIB may have
and other problem behaviors. The FAST was cre- limited experience with the child. For example, a
ated specifically for use only as a screening tool teacher may only work with a child for few
as part of a comprehensive functional analysis of months and may have only had a few experiences
behavior. The Behavior Problems Inventory observing the behavior and may not fully know
(BPI-01; Rojahn et al., 2001) is another tool that what to report. Furthermore, bias may lead the
has been used to indirectly assess SIB. This teacher to report that the SIB occurred because
respondent-based behavior rating instrument can the students did not want to do their work, as
provide information on frequency, severity, and opposed to reporting the consequence (i.e., the
topography of SIB. This information could be student was given a break) that occurred; the
useful for epidemiological reasons or for admin- teacher may also forget about instances of the
istrative decision-making in regard to persons behavior that have happened in the past. Due to
with IDD that engage in SIB. Furthermore, mea- the serious limitations of indirect assessments,
surement of SIB using this tool might allow for multiple authors have recommended using direct
monitoring of changing behavior problems or the assessments in conjunction with indirect assess-
emergence of new SIB in longitudinal assess- ments (Beavers, Iwata, & Lerman, 2013; Iwata
ments (Rojahn et al.). Rating scales and question- et al., 2013; Smith, Smith, Dracobly, & Pace,
naires like the FAST and the BPI-01 are also 2012; Vollmer et al., 2009; Zarcone, Rodgers,
useful because they provide a consistent format Iwata, Rourke, & Dorsey, 1991).
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 273

Direct Assessment program for antecedent or consequence changes


to test for function; instead the recording is event
Direct assessment approaches involve direct driven, occurring whenever a problem behavior
observations that provide information about a or episode of problem behavior occurs.
person’s behavior as it occurs (Miltenberger, Descriptive analyses have limitations that
2011). Direct assessment is commonly used to might lead to false-positive conclusions. One
assess why a child is engaging in SIB by directly way this could occur is if a particular conse-
observing the child in their typical setting with quence commonly followed SIB, but was not a
naturally occurring interactions. Direct assess- reinforcer for SIB. For example, Thompson and
ment is usually more accurate than indirect Iwata (2001) found attention of caregivers com-
assessment because you do not have to rely on monly follows dangerous behavior even if the
other caregiver’s memories or subjective reports attention may not function as a reinforcer. St.
(Miltenberger, 2011). Peter et al. (2005) demonstrated events that fol-
Direct assessment can be carried out in the low problem behavior (including SIB) do not
form of descriptive analyses in which environ- necessarily maintain or cause the problem behav-
mental variables are recorded as they happen, or ior. They found that attention was highly corre-
through experimental functional assessment (i.e., lated with problem behavior, following
functional analysis—to be discussed later), in observation and computing of matching relations
which variables in the environment are manipu- for three participants, despite functional analyses
lated by the professional. Descriptive analyses ruling out attention as a reinforcer.
typically include the observer taking data on Functional analysis (FA) is a type of direct
antecedent-behavior-consequence interactions in assessment, which involves directly manipulat-
the child’s environment and then inferring func- ing the variables (i.e., antecedent and conse-
tion of the behavior based on correlations in the quence events) to identify the variable(s) that
data. For example, Lerman and Iwata (1993) may maintain SIB. This type of assessment is the
observed subjects during 15-min sessions once or most common form of assessment to identify the
twice a day for several weeks and recorded natu- function of SIB (Kahng et al., 2002a). In a semi-
rally occurring sequences of subject and staff nal article on assessment of SIB, Iwata, Dorsey,
behavior including SIB as the target behavior. Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982/1984)
They then calculated conditional probabilities to manipulated social (i.e., social disapproval, aca-
identify relevant antecedent and consequent demic demand, unstructured play) and nonsocial
events. variables (i.e., leaving participant alone) to assess
A common descriptive analysis tool is the environmental functional relationships with
Functional Assessment Observation Form SIB. They found higher levels of SIB were asso-
(FA; O’Neill et al., 2015). When using the FAOF ciated with exposure to specific variables relative
observers identify topography of target behav- to others, suggesting they were able to identify
iors, predictors, and perceived functions and then the variables causing and maintaining the
record across the day the time and frequency of SIB. Since this study, over 200 studies have been
when all of these events occur. It is assumed that published using functional analysis to assess SIB
patterns that may appear in the recording will (Beavers et al., 2013).
reveal what the antecedents and consequences of Due to the nature of SIB, it is important to
the problem behavior are, due to the frequency consider potential risk before conducting an FA
with which they show up on the form as correlat- and consult medical personnel (Iwata et al.,
ing with the problem behavior. These data can 1982/1994; Vollmer et al., 2009). However, this
then be used to inform planning for treatment should not limit whether or not an FA is con-
based on the identified function of the behavior. ducted. Kahng et al. (2015) reviewed records of
The FAOF is considered a naturalistic observa- 99 inpatients who engaged in SIB and found that
tion tool because observers do not manipulate or conducting FAs on SIB was relatively safe when
274 C.J. Clay et al.

appropriate precautions were taken. It has been engages in SIB and delays or removes the instruc-
suggested by multiple researchers and clinicians tion by the caregiver to get dressed.
that conducting an FA is best practice when Social positive reinforcement involves a
determining the function of problem behavior socially mediated stimulus being presented fol-
including SIB (Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003; lowing an individual engaging in SIB, leading to
Kahng et al., 2015; Vollmer et al., 2009). maintenance or increase of SIB by that individ-
ual. The delivery of attention following SIB is a
common socially mediated event that occurs. For
Causes (Why Does SIB Occur?) example, a teacher offers a statement of concern,
“Don’t do that, you’ll hurt yourself,” following a
There are multiple hypotheses about why SIB student engaging in SIB. The attention delivered
occurs. Symons and Thompson (1997) identified by the caregiver, although well-meaning, could
seven: psychodynamic hypothesis, biological or inadvertently reinforce the SIB.
organically based hypotheses, developmental Automatic reinforcement is said to maintain
hypothesis, side effect of minor illness hypothe- SIB when SIB occurs independent of social con-
ses, self-stimulation hypothesis, learned behavior sequences. That is, the behavior is a direct result
hypothesis, and neurochemical hypothesis. of one’s own behavior and does not involve rein-
Although many hypotheses exist, research sug- forcement mediated by another person (Vaughan
gests that the majority of SIB occurs due to envi- & Michael, 1982). For example, relieving an
ronmental events that have produced a learning itching sensation through scratching would be
history (Hagopian, Rooker, & Zarcone, 2015; considered automatic reinforcement. Rincover
Iwata, Kahng, Wallace, & Lindberg, 2000; Iwata, (1978) proposed automatic reinforcement occurs
Pace, Cowdery, & Miltenberger, 1994; Iwata through sensory stimulation and as such could be
et al., 2000; Kahng et al., 2002a; Matson & eliminated through blocking the cause of stimu-
LoVullo, 2008; Vollmer et al., 2009). The idea lation (i.e., extinction). Other theories for how
that SIB is a learned behavior is supported by the automatic reinforcement maintains problem
number of effective treatments that are based on behavior such as SIB refer to covert physiologi-
manipulating antecedents and consequences in cal actions including pain attenuation (Cataldo &
the environment (see Kahng et al. for review). Harris, 1982) and the production of endogenous
That is, environmental control of variables can opioids (Sandman, 1990, 2009). Automatic rein-
reduce SIB, so it is likely that these same vari- forcement, through covert mechanisms, may be
ables cause or maintain SIB. more difficult to treat because the reinforcers
Although specific environmental variables maintaining behavior are not within the control
controlling SIB are sometimes idiosyncratic, in of the practitioner (Vollmer, 1994). However,
previous reviews the most common behavioral treatments do exist for SIB maintained by auto-
functions of SIB have been divided into four cat- matic reinforcement, as will be discussed later.
egories: social negative reinforcement, social It has recently been suggested that automati-
positive reinforcement, automatic reinforcement, cally maintained SIB is further subtyped due to
and multiple functions (Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, distinct functional properties of SIB (Hagopian
et al., 1994; Kahng et al., 2002a). et al., 2015). Hagopian et al. (2015) identified
Social negative reinforcement involves a three subtypes of automatically maintained SIB
socially mediated stimulus being removed fol- among 39 individuals based on patterns of
lowing an individual engaging in SIB leading to responding during the FA (e.g., levels of
maintenance or increase of SIB by that individ- responding were high in the alone condition and
ual. This is commonly referred to as escape from low in the play condition). They also imple-
a demand. For example, a caregiver may ask a mented treatments for all of the individuals
child with IDD to get dressed. The child then to identify relationships between treatment
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 275

outcome and SIB subtypes. They found signifi- variables were able to be identified for 92% of the
cant differences across subtypes of automati- individuals (Kahng et al., 2002b).
cally maintained SIB on effectiveness of Identifying why SIB occurs is critical to
treatment outcome. That is, interventions failed, inform the treatment process. Multiple authors
and more intensive interventions were required have suggested the process of identifying the
for Subtypes 2 and 3 (SIB that was high and function of SIB and then applying a function-­
undifferentiated across functional analysis con- based intervention is necessary to effectively
ditions and SIB including self-­restraint, respec- treat SIB (Carr, 1977; Hanley, 2012; Iwata, Pace,
tively). This study offers a subtyping model that Kalsher, Cowdery, & Cataldo, 1990; Kahng
may be useful for practitioners trying to reduce et al., 2002b). Additionally, school districts in the
automatically reinforced SIB based on patterns United States are legally mandated to conduct
of responding in the FA. functional behavior assessments for individuals
It may also be the case that the function of the with ongoing behavior problems that preclude
SIB is controlled by multiple sources, or a clear them from school according to revisions to the
function is unable to be identified (e.g., Smith, Individuals with Disabilities Improvement
Iwata, Vollmer, & Zarcone, 1993; Vollmer, Education Act of 2004 (IDEA, 2004). Once a
Marcus, & LeBlanc, 1994). In these cases, fur- clinician or researcher identifies the function,
ther assessment may be necessary. For example, there is a range of behavioral treatment options
Smith et al. (1993) sequentially exposed partici- available.
pants to multiple treatments each tailored to
address a different function of multiple controlled
SIB. They found that multiple different treat- Treatment
ments were required to reduce SIB for two of
three subjects, thus confirming the results of the This section describes examples of behavioral
previous FA. However, a single treatment was treatments that have successfully reduced SIB in
able to reduce SIB for one of the participants, individuals with IDD following a functional anal-
suggesting spurious results of the initial FA. ysis. Although medical and pharmacological
Reviews identifying the prevalence of differ- treatments (e.g., electroconvulsive therapy, typi-
ent functions of SIB have found consistent and cal and atypical antipsychotics, selective sero-
interesting results. Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al. tonin reuptake inhibitors, naltrexone, lithium,
(1994) summarized functional analysis data from and antiepileptic drugs) have been used with vari-
152 cases of individuals who engaged in SIB and ous degrees of efficacy (for review, see Minshawi,
found social negative reinforcement functioned Hurwitz, Morriss, & McDougle, 2014), discuss-
as the reinforcer maintaining the majority of the ing them in depth is beyond the scope of this
cases (38.1%). This finding corresponded to chapter.
Kahng et al. (2002b) in which they also found Current behavioral treatments tend to be func-
social negative reinforcement to be the function tion based, meaning they rely on the results of a
for the majority (31.3%) of the cases they functional analysis or functional assessment to
reviewed. Furthermore, there was close corre- determine the function of a behavior and develop
spondence among the percentage of cases across a treatment that is designed to address it. As dis-
all of the functions of SIB for both studies. cussed previously, the results of a functional
However, the disparity between the functions was analysis determine what events maintain a behav-
small (i.e., 31.3%, 26.4%, and 27.5% for social ior. The treatments are guided based on this infor-
negative reinforcement, social positive reinforce- mation and may include access to tangible items,
ment, and automatic reinforcement, respec- access to attention, and escape. Treatments for SIB
tively). This suggests SIB is not overwhelmingly found to be automatically maintained are also
maintained by access to a specific consequence described. General suggestions, including con-
for individuals with IDD; however, maintaining siderations for ensuring the safety of individuals
276 C.J. Clay et al.

who engage in SIB throughout assessment and SIB has also been treated through differential
treatment, and a summary of some punishment-­ reinforcement of behavior that does not involve
based treatments, are discussed at the end of the reinforcing a specific alternative response.
chapter. Differential reinforcement of other behavior
(DRO) involves reinforcing the absence of the
target behavior (Pierce & Cheney, 2013).
Function-Based Treatment Reinforcement is provided after a predetermined
for Socially Mediated SIB interval if the individual has not engaged in the
target behavior during that time, regardless of
Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior other behavior. Kahng et al. (2002a) found that
(DRA) is a procedure that involves reinforcing DRO was the most commonly used intervention
behavior that is different from the behavior tar- for behavioral treatment of SIB from 1964 to
geted for reduction (Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). One 2002. Kurtz et al. (2003) used DRO, which
of the most common forms of DRA involves the involved delivering the reinforcer maintaining
teaching of a communication response or func- SIB contingent on the absence of SIB, with 4 of
tional communication training (FCT; Carr & their 24 participants. For example, case 13 in
Durand, 1985). FCT involves teaching the indi- their study specifically required DRO for escape-­
vidual to appropriately request the reinforcer maintained SIB. More recently researchers have
found to be maintaining SIB (Tiger, Hanley, & used DRO procedures to treat automatically
Bruzek, 2008). For example, if a child engages in maintained SIB in children with a nonfunction-­
SIB maintained by access to tangible items, the based treatment (e.g., Cowdery, Iwata, & Pace,
child may be taught to exchange a picture card to 1990; Paisey, Whitney, & Wainczak, 1993; Tiger,
receive an item instead of engaging in SIB. Kurtz Fisher, & Bouxsein, 2009; Toussaint & Tiger,
et al. (2003) pointed out that FCT is often paired 2012). More will be discussed on SIB maintained
with other components. Of 24 total participants by automatic reinforcement in the next section.
in their summary cases of SIB in children, 8 were Differential reinforcement has been found to
successfully treated using FCT plus extinction. be more effective when SIB can be placed on
However, successful treatment for 9 of the 24 extinction (Fisher et al., 1993; Volkert, Lerman,
participants required a punishment component. Call, & Trosclair-Lasserre, 2009). However,
FCT is a good treatment choice when the implementing extinction is not always possible
behavior is socially mediated, and there are sev- (Athens & Vollmer, 2010; Hagopian, Fisher,
eral ways individuals can be taught to request for Sullivan, Acquisto, & LeBlanc, 1998). For exam-
the reinforcer maintaining their SIB. Danov, ple, implementing escape extinction (i.e., not
Hartman, McComas, and Symons (2010) com- allowing an individual to access a break or have
pared FCT of different request topographies with demands briefly removed) may not be feasible
a 3-year-old boy with autism whose SIB was because the caregiver has to block the SIB, thus
maintained by access to tangible items. They first producing a brief access to escape from demands.
conducted a preference assessment to identify An alternative intervention for decreasing SIB
highly preferred items. Next, they compared FCT without extinction is noncontingent reinforce-
using a vocal request to FCT using a picture card. ment (NCR; Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). This proce-
They found that both the vocal response and pic- dure involves providing access to a reinforcer
ture card reduced SIB; however, the child did not continuously or on a predetermined schedule,
independently request items during the vocal regardless of other behavior (Pierce & Cheney,
request condition. This has an important implica- 2013). One benefit of NCR is that it does not
tion—choosing a response modality that the par- require constant monitoring of the individual.
ticipant can use independently is an essential Wilder, Normand, and Atwell (2005) used NCR
consideration when choosing FCT as treatment. to treat the SIB of a 3-year-old female with
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 277

autism, gastroesophageal reflux, and food involving no contingency for task completion,
allergies who engaged in pinching, scraping, and negative reinforcement only for task completion,
scratching her own skin and hitting her head and negative reinforcement plus positive rein-
against the ground and other objects. The results forcement for task completion. They found that
of a functional analysis showed that the child SIB occurred at high rates and fewer tasks were
engaged in SIB maintained by escape from completed during the condition in which there
demands, specifically the presentation of food. was no reinforcement for task completion. More
Treatment included NCR in the form of continu- tasks were completed, and problem behavior was
ous access to a highly preferred video during reduced in the negative reinforcement plus posi-
sessions, and SIB resulted in a 15-s break during tive reinforcement condition. This study suggests
treatment sessions. This treatment resulted in that combining positive reinforcement and nega-
a decrease of SIB from 44% of intervals in the tive reinforcement contingencies may be most
initial baseline phase to 6% of intervals during effective for decreasing problem behaviors while
treatment. increasing desired behaviors. This study is also
Vollmer, Marcus, and Ringdahl (1995) treated different from many others because the treatment
SIB in two male children with developmental did not place problem behavior on extinction.
disabilities using NCR during demand situations, Problem behavior was reinforced with a 30-s
which they termed noncontingent escape (NCE). break. Even without using extinction, positive
Vollmer et al. provided escape (a break from results of the treatment were maintained as the
demands) on a fixed-time schedule. There were number of tasks to be completed increased and
no programmed consequences for SIB, and the schedule of reinforcement was thinned. This is
frequency of SIB did not affect the frequency of promising for implementation of treatment out-
breaks. NCE was successful in reducing SIB for side of clinical settings, where placing a behavior
both children from median rates of 1.6 and 3.2 on extinction may not be possible.
during baseline to zero for both participants dur- Antecedent manipulations (i.e., altering what
ing NCE treatment. The researchers were also happens in the environment before the behavior
able to systematically increase the intervals occurs) have also been used to reduce
between breaks. This intervention was successful SIB. O’Reilly, Sigafoos, Lancioni, Edrisinha,
not only in decreasing SIB but also in increasing and Andrews (2005) rearranged a schedule of
the amount of work the participants completed. activities to address the SIB of a 12-year-old boy
NCE might be a good choice in environments with autism and intellectual disabilities. Their
where frequent demands are presented (e.g., functional analysis showed that the SIB occurred
classrooms). in the demand condition and seemed to carry
Another way to avoid the use of extinction is over into other conditions that followed. During
to combine reinforcement contingencies. In one treatment analysis in a classroom, O’Reilly et al.
study, researchers compared the efficacy of three (2005) compared a schedule that included 5 min
treatment conditions for an 11-year-old female of work, followed by 5 min of no interaction and
who was diagnosed with autism and engaged in then 5 min of play to the normal classroom
hand biting maintained by escape from demands schedule. Results showed that the antecedent
(Hoch, McComas, Thompson, & Paone, 2002). manipulation of altering the activity schedule of
SIB resulted in a 30-s break from demands in all the child decreased SIB and increased his engage-
three conditions. In one condition, there was no ment in classroom activities.
programmed consequence for task completion. In addition to determining the function of SIB,
In another condition, task completion resulted in researchers have addressed behaviors that consis-
escape from demands. In the final condition, task tently occur before the SIB. This is known as tar-
completion resulted in escape from demands and geting the precursor behavior. This is particularly
access to a preferred activity. The researchers relevant when addressing attention-maintained
were comparing the efficacy of a treatment problem behavior because it may be the case that
278 C.J. Clay et al.

delivering attention following SIB is unavoid- by the clinician or researcher. Delivery of stimuli
able. This might occur in cases of severe SIB that function as reinforcers for behavior that
where the behavior must be blocked, for exam- competes with SIB has been used in these
ple, head banging on sharp corners or glass situations.
­surfaces. In these cases, researchers have con- Noncontingent reinforcement has also been
ducted functional analyses of precursor behavior used to treat SIB that is automatically main-
and designed treatment based on those results. tained. Researchers conducted modified FAs
This was done with an older individual (a with 64 individuals, including adolescents and
29-year-old man) with an IDD who engaged in adults, who engaged in chronic hand mouthing,
face slapping, face punching, and head banging and found that this behavior was automatically
(Dracobly & Smith, 2012). An unstructured maintained for all but one participant (Roscoe,
observation was conducted to identify precursor Iwata, & Zhou, 2013). Treatment evaluation was
behaviors, which were used as the target behavior conducted for 14 individuals and showed that
within a functional analysis. The functional anal- NCR alone (e.g., delivery of a leisure item) was
ysis determined access to attention to be main- successful in reducing SIB for 6 individuals.
taining the precursor behavior. Treatment Other components, including response blocking,
consisted of providing attention for the precursor DRA, and brief manual restraint, were necessary
behavior and withholding attention for the for successful treatment for the other eight indi-
SIB. The participant’s SIB was successfully viduals. The use of these other components may
reduced and eventually eliminated through the not be feasible or practical in all cases. For exam-
assessment and treatment of precursor behavior. ple, response blocking would not be possible
An advantage of this type of assessment and when the person implementing the intervention
treatment is that it allows for addressing a target cannot stay close enough to the individual to
behavior while reducing the risk of harm for the block all SIB attempts, as would likely be the
participant (Dracobly & Smith). case in classrooms and other group settings.
To summarize, we have identified examples of Additionally, manual restraint may not be allowed
effective interventions to reduce SIB that is in all settings.
socially mediated. It is important to point out that In another example of the application of NCR,
although one intervention (e.g., FCT) might have researchers compared the efficacy of NCR alone,
been used to reduce one type of socially mediated response blocking alone, and NCR plus response
SIB (e.g., SIB maintained by access to attention), blocking for reducing the SIB of a 4-year-old
the same intervention has been successful at male with autism (Saini et al., 2016). This child
reducing other types of socially mediated prob- engaged in automatically maintained SIB in the
lem behavior (e.g., access to escape). form of self-biting. Blocking alone resulted in
reductions of the SIB, but the reductions were not
clinically significant. Treatment significantly
 reatment for SIB That Is Maintained
T reduced the occurrences of SIB only when NCR
by Automatic Reinforcement and response blocking were combined. Saini
et al. (2016) noted that a modified blocking pro-
Researchers have identified and discussed the cedure was put in place in the NCR plus response
difficulties in treating automatically maintained blocking condition due to the high rate of SIB
problem behavior (e.g., LeBlanc, Patel, & Carr, and the therapist’s inability to block all attempts
2000; Vollmer, 1994). One of the main difficul- using the original blocking procedure. However,
ties lies in not being able to manipulate the func- this modified blocking procedure was never
tional reinforcer because it is not a socially tested alone.
mediated reinforcer. That is, reinforcement gen- To increase the efficacy of NCR, some
erated by the behavior itself cannot be accessed researchers have employed the use of a competing
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 279

stimulus assessment (CSA). A typical competing noncontingent access to toys, which were unsuc-
stimulus assessment (CSA) involves the compar- cessful in reducing SIB. Hagopian et al. modified
ison of 15 stimuli and a control condition across their intervention to include response blocking
2-min sessions (Piazza et al., 1998). The ther- for precursor behavior. Although there was no
apist gives the participant one stimulus to be functional analysis of the precursor behavior
evaluated at the beginning of each session. The itself, the addition of response blocking of the
therapist ensures that the item is still within reach precursor behavior reduced eye poking from
if the participant sets it down and rates of target occurring in 38% of intervals on average in the
behavior and duration of stimulus contact are original treatment to only 7.7% of intervals in the
recorded. modified treatment. Not only did SIB decrease,
Jennett, Jann, and Hagopian (2011) compared but it also decreased the number of times the ther-
a typical CSA with two other CSAs: one in which apist was required to response block by 80%,
the item was re-presented to the participant if she which made the intervention easier and more
set it down and one in which the item was re-­ practical to implement.
presented and response blocking was in place for Protective equipment can be used to
SIB. The participant in this study was a 3-year-­ decrease SIB maintained by automatic rein-
old female diagnosed with cerebral palsy, IDD, forcement. One way the application of protec-
and shunted hydrocephalus. She engaged in SIB tive equipment might reduce SIB is through
in the form of head hitting, finger and hand bit- increasing the response effort of the SIB. Zhou,
ing, and head banging. Results of a functional Goff, and Iwata (2000) increased the response
analysis suggested that this behavior was main- effort of SIB by having participants (four
tained by automatic reinforcement. This partici- women with IDD) wear flexible arm splints.
pant engaged in lower levels of SIB and higher Results showed that providing access to a rein-
levels of stimulus contact in the CSA, which forcer can reduce SIB, even if the reinforcer is
included re-presentation of the item and response less preferred than SIB, when response effort
blocking for SIB. No stimuli reduced levels of for SIB is increased.
SIB to a clinically acceptable level during the Protective equipment has also been used to
typical CSA. A treatment evaluation showed that terminate the behavior-consequence relation in
the CSA plus re-presentation and response block- SIB through sensory extinction to decrease auto-
ing was the most effective treatment. Although matically maintained SIB. Moore, Fisher, and
providing a competing stimulus is a practical Pennington (2004) applied protective equipment
treatment choice for a group setting, results of to relative body parts of a 12-year-old female
this study show that additional treatment compo- who engaged in over 18 topographies of auto-
nents may be necessary for significant reductions matically maintained SIB. These topographies
of SIB. Conducting a brief comparison of CSAs were grouped into three categories: shoulder-­
with other components may help to predict the directed, hand-directed, and leg-directed. Results
success of these treatments. showed that applying the protective equipment to
Treatment of automatically maintained SIB relevant body parts decreased SIB to near-zero
has also involved addressing precursor behavior. rates. These decreases occurred for each of the
Hagopian, Paclawskyj, and Kuhn (2005) con- three groups of topographies only when the pro-
ducted a functional analysis of SIB (eye poking) tective equipment was placed on areas of the par-
exhibited by a 16-year-old male diagnosed with ticipant’s body where that topography of SIB was
stereotypical movement disorder, IDD, and directed. Researchers identified sensory extinc-
Down syndrome. The functional analysis showed tion (e.g., the protective equipment decreased or
that the behavior was maintained by automatic removed the sensory stimulation gained from
reinforcement. Previous treatments included SIB) as the most likely explanation for the effec-
response blocking, protective eye goggles, and tiveness of this treatment.
280 C.J. Clay et al.

Once behavior has been reduced or eliminated,  he Role of Punishment in Treatment


T
fading protective equipment or restraints out of of SIB
the treatment may be considered. As an example,
restraint fading has been implemented to decrease As reported in the review of behavioral treat-
the automatically maintained hand-­to-­head SIB of ments of SIB from 1964 to 2000 (Kahng et al.,
a 16-year-old female with IDD (Kahng, Abt, & 2002a), there was an increase in the number of
Wilder, 2001). In this study, the participant wore studies using reinforcement-based interventions
mechanical arm splints t­hroughout the day. The beginning in the early 1990s. As such, there were
splints were systematically faded contingent on few recent studies that exclusively used punish-
low rates of SIB by reducing the amount they ment to decrease SIB. It is important to note that
restricted movement. Although the splints in this chapter, punishment refers to the addition
decreased her hand-to-head SIB, rates of other or removal of a stimulus that results in a decrease
topographies of SIB (e.g., arm- or shoulder-­to-­ of behavior. Although the studies included in this
head, head-to-surface) increased. Kahng et al. chapter primarily involve reinforcement-based
(2001) then implemented environmental enrich- procedures, it is worth noting that many included
ment by providing access to highly preferred response blocking, which could be seen as a pun-
items during the sessions, but this had little impact ishing component (e.g., Hagopian et al., 2005;
on SIB. Finally, they added a punishment compo- Jennett et al., 2011; Roscoe et al., 2013) and,
nent, contingent exercise, in which the therapist similarly, brief manual restraint (e.g., Kurtz et al.,
guided the child to touch her toes repeatedly for 2003; Roscoe et al.).
30 s following all instances of SIB. This resulted A common punishment treatment is response
in immediate suppression of the SIB. It is impor- interruption. Response interruption has been suc-
tant to note that researchers were able to fade the cessfully used to suppress problem behaviors
mechanical restraints to flexible sleeves in 175 such as pica (e.g., Hagopian & Toole, 2009;
sessions over 1.5 months. This was done while Hagopian, Gonzàlez, Rivet, Triggs, & Clark,
maintaining low to zero levels of SIB during the 2011) and stereotypy (e.g., Ahrens, Lerman,
combined intervention of restraints, contingent Kodak, Worsdell, & Keegan, 2011; Brusa &
exercise, and environmental enrichment. Richman, 2008; Giles, St. Peter, Pence, &
Removal of protective equipment has also Gibson, 2012). However, Lydon, Healy, O’Reilly,
been incorporated into a contingency as a treat- and McCoy (2013) reviewed research on response
ment to reduce SIB. This was done with an interruption for treating challenging behaviors in
8-year-old boy who engaged in SIB in the form individuals with IDD and cautioned that response
of closed-fist punches directed toward his head interruption is unlikely to completely eliminate
(Magnusson & Gould, 2007). Results of a func- the targeted behavior. Given the harmful nature
tional analysis showed this behavior was main- of SIB, this might not be a suitable intervention
tained by automatic reinforcement. Magnusson for addressing this behavior.
and Gould (2007) compared a condition in which One recent study that used punishment alone
the child had continuous access to a soft foam used response interruption to treat SIB in a
karate helmet to a condition in which the helmet 14-year-old female diagnosed with Rett syn-
was removed contingent on each instance of drome (Roane, Piazza, Sgro, Volkert, & Anderson,
SIB. When the helmet was removed, the child 2001). This child’s exhibited hand mouthing and
was prompted to fold his hands and count out a functional analysis showed that it was main-
loud to ten. Access to the helmet was given again tained by automatic reinforcement. The research-
once the child counted to ten without engaging in ers implemented a 5-s hands down procedure in
SIB. Results showed that the child’s SIB remained which the child’s hands were held down for 5 s
high in the continuous access condition. SIB was contingent on hand mouthing attempts. Although
reduced to near-zero levels immediately in the treatment reduced SIB, it still occurred up to 1.1
contingent removal condition. responses per min during treatment.
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 281

Safety and Treatment of SIB atic, socially acceptable, and accurate identifica-


tion of functions and treatment for SIB.
Many studies we reviewed commented on client
safety during assessments and interventions.
Prioritizing client safety is always important. References
This is especially true when assessing and treat-
ing SIB. We suggest having predetermined ses- Ahrens, E. N., Lerman, D. C., Kodak, T., Worsdell, A. S.,
& Keegan, C. (2011). Further evaluation of response
sion termination criteria in place throughout interruption and redirection as treatment for ste-
assessments and treatments. Kahng et al. (2015) reotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44,
conducted a review to examine injuries related to 95–108.
functional analysis of SIB as compared to other Arndorfer, R. E., Miltenberger, R. G., Woster, S. H.,
Rortvedt, A. K., & Gaffaney, T. (1994). Home-based
settings. They found that injuries were relatively descriptive and experimental analysis of problem
infrequent and rarely severe. Overall, this review behaviors in children. Topics in Early Childhood
stresses the relative safety of functional analyses Special Education, 14, 64–87.
of SIB as long as appropriate precautions are Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An investigation
of differential reinforcement of alternative behav-
taken. In their seminal work on conducting a ior without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior
functional analysis of SIB, Iwata et al. Analysis, 43, 569–589.
(1982/1994) laid out the following safeguards: Beavers, G. A., Iwata, B. A., & Lerman, D. C. (2013).
(1) each participant’s current physical health was Thirty years of research on the functional analysis
of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
assessed by a physician; (2) the physician for Analysis, 46, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.30
each participant recommended termination crite- Breau, L. M., Camfield, C. S., Symons, F. J., Bodfish,
ria based on risk of physical injury; (3) if termi- J. W., MacKay, A., Allen, F., & McGrath, P. J. (2003).
nation criteria were met at any time for a Relation between pain and self-injurious behavior
in nonverbal children with severe cognitive impair-
participant, the session was terminated, SIB was ments. Journal of Pediatrics, 142, 498–503. https://
blocked using restraint, and the participant was doi.org/10.1067/mpd.2003.163
assessed by a nurse or physician; (4) after four Breau, L. M., McGrath, P. J., Camfield, C., Rosmus,
sessions, each participant was examined by a C., & Finley, G. A. (2000). Preliminary validation
of an observational pain checklist for persons with
nurse; and (5) each participant’s case was cognitive impairments and inability to communi-
reviewed at least once a week by several mem- cate verbally. Developmental Medicine and Child
bers of a multidisciplinary team. Neurology, 42, 609–616. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0012162200001146
Brusa, E., & Richman, D. (2008). Developing stimulus
control for occurrences of stereotypy exhibited by a
Conclusion child with autism. International Journal of Behavioral
Consultation and Therapy, 4, 264–269.
The behavioral treatment of SIB in children with Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-­ injurious
behavior: A review of some hypotheses.
disabilities has been extensively researched since Psychological Bulletin, 84, 800–816. https://doi.
the early 1960s. Effective procedures have org/10.1037/0033-2909.84.4.800
evolved and developed since that time. Arguably, Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior
the inclusion of functional behavior assessment problems through functional communication training.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126.
has likely had the biggest impact in changing and Cataldo, M. F., & Harris, J. (1982). The biological basis
improving intervention in SIB. This stems from for self-injury in the mentally retarded. Analysis and
the data that SIB is largely a learned behavior and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 21–39.
can be intervened upon by manipulating anteced- https://doi.org/10.1016/0270-4684(82)90004-0
Cooper, S. A. (1998). Behaviour disorders in adults
ents and consequences in the environment. with learning disabilities: Effect of age and differen-
Advances in assessment and intervention pro- tiation from other psychiatric disorders. Irish Journal
cedures have led to the reduction in the usage of of Psychological Medicine, 15, 13–18. ­https://doi.
punishment procedures as well as more system- org/10.1017/S0790966700004614
282 C.J. Clay et al.

Cowdery, G. E., Iwata, B. A., & Pace, G. M. (1990). Hagopian, L. P., Paclawskyj, T. R., & Kuhn, S. C. (2005).
Effects and side effects of DRO as treatment for The use of conditional probability analysis to iden-
self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior tify a response chain leading to the occurrence of eye
Analysis, 23, 497–506. poking. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26,
Crawford, J., Brockel, B., Schauss, S., & Miltenberger, 393–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2003.09.002
R. G. (1992). A comparison of methods for the func- Hagopian, L. P., Rooker, G. W., & Zarcone, J. R. (2015).
tional assessment of stereotypic behavior. Journal of Delineating subtypes of self-injurious behavior main-
the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, tained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied
17, 77–86. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-497 Behavior Analysis, 48, 523. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Cunningham, E., & O’Neill, R. E. (2000). Comparison jaba.236
of results of functional assessment and analysis meth- Hagopian, L. P., & Toole, L. M. (2009). Effects of response
ods with young children with autism. Education and blocking and competing stimuli on stereotypic behav-
Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental ior. Behavioral Interventions, 24, 117–125.
Disabilities, 35, 406–414. Hall, S. S. (2005). Comparing descriptive, experimental
Danov, S. E., Hartman, E., McComas, J. J., & Symons, and informant-based assessments of problem behav-
F. J. (2010). Evaluation of two communicative iors. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 514–
response modalities for a child with autism and self-­ 526. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2004.11.004
injury. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology & Hanley, G. P. (2012). Functional assessment of problem
Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 70–79. behavior: Dispelling myths, overcoming implemen-
Dracobly, J. D., & Smith, R. G. (2012). Progressing tation obstacles, and developing new lore. Behavior
from identification and functional analysis of precur- Analysis in Practice, 5, 54–72.
sor behavior to treatment of self-injurious behavior. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003).
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 361–374. Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-361 Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.
Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1988). Identifying https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-147
the variables maintaining self-injurious behavior. Hoch, H., McComas, J., Thompson, A., & Paone, D.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, (2002). Concurrent reinforcement schedules: Behavior
18, 99–117. change and maintenance without extinction. Journal
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Cataldo, M. F., Harrell, R., of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 155–169.
Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com- Hyman, S. L., Fisher, W., Mercugliano, M., & Cataldo,
munication training with and without extinction and M. F. (1990). Children with self-injurious behavior.
punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Pediatrics, 85, 437–441.
26, 23–36. Individuals with Disabilities Improvement Education
Giles, A. F., St. Peter, C. C., Pence, S. T., & Gibson, Act of 2004. (2004). 20 U.S.C. § 1415 procedural
A. B. (2012). Preference for blocking or response safeguards.
redirection during stereotypy treatment. Research in Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Roscoe, E. M. (2013).
Developmental Disabilities, 33, 1691–1700. Reliability and validity of the functional analysis
Goh, H. L., Iwata, B. A., Shore, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., screening tool. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Lerman, D. C., Ulrich, S. M., & Smith, R. G. (1995). 46, 271–284. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.31
An analysis of the reinforcing properties of hand Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
mouthing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, & Richman, G. S. (1982/1994). Toward a functional
269–283. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-269 analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
Griffin, J. C., Ricketts, R. W., Williams, D. E., Locke, Analysis, 27, 197–209.
B. J., Altmeyer, B. K., & Stark, M. T. (1987). A Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S. W., Wallace, M. D., & Lindberg,
community survey of self-injurious behavior among J. S. (2000). The functional analysis model of behav-
developmentally disabled children and adoles- ioral assessment. In J. Austin & J. E. Carr (Eds.),
cents. Psychiatric Services, 38, 959–963. https://doi. Handbook of applied behavior analysis (pp. 61–89).
org/10.1176/ps.38.9.959 Reno, NV: Context Press.
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto, Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness of func- R. G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An
tional communication training with and without analysis of procedural form and function. Journal of
extinction and punishment: A summary of 21 inpa- Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 131–144. https://doi.
tient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-131
211–235. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone,
Hagopian, L. P., Gonzàlez, M. L., Rivet, T. T., Triggs, J. R., Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., … Willis, K. D.
M., & Clark, S. B. (2011). Response interruption and (1994). The functions of self-injurious behavior: An
differential reinforcement of alternative behavior for experimental-epidemiological analysis. Journal of
the treatment of pica. Behavioral Interventions, 26, Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240. ­https://doi.
309–325. org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-215
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 283

Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery, G. E., Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive and
& Cataldo, M. F. (1990). Experimental analysis and experimental analyses of variables maintaining self-
extinction of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 11–27. https://doi. Analysis, 26, 293–319. https://doi.org/10.1901/
org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-11 jaba.1993.26-293
Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kissel, R. C., Nau, P. A., & Lewis, T. J., Mitchell, B. S., Harvey, K., Green, A., &
Farber, J. M. (1990). The self-injury trauma (SIT) McKenzie, J. (2015). A comparison of functional
scale: A method for quantifying surface tissue dam- behavioral assessment and functional analysis meth-
age caused by self-injurious behavior. Journal of odology among students with mild disabilities.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 99–110. https://doi. Behavioral Disorders, 41, 5–20.
org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-99 Lewis, T. J., Scott, T. M., & Sugai, G. (1994). The problem
Jennett, H., Jann, K., & Hagopian, L. P. (2011). Evaluation behavior questionnaire: A teacher-based instrument to
of response blocking and re-presentation in a compet- develop functional hypotheses of problem behavior
ing stimulus assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior in general education classrooms. Diagnostique, 19,
Analysis, 44, 925–929. 103–115.
Johnson, W. L., & Baumeister, A. A. (1978). Self-injurious Lydon, S., Healy, O., O’Reilly, M., & McCoy, A. (2013).
behavior a review and analysis of methodological A systematic review and evaluation of response redi-
details of published studies. Behavior Modification, 2, rection as a treatment for challenging behavior in indi-
465–487. https://doi.org/10.1177/014544557824002 viduals with developmental disabilities. Research in
Kahng, S., Abt, K. A., & Wilder, D. A. (2001). Treatment Developmental Disabilities, 34, 3148–3158.
of self-injury correlated with mechanical restraints. Magnusson, A. F., & Gould, D. D. (2007). Reduction of
Behavioral Interventions, 16, 105–110. https://doi. automatically-maintained self-injury using contingent
org/10.1002/bin.86 equipment removal. Behavioral Interventions, 22,
Kahng, S., Hausman, N. L., Fisher, A. B., Donaldson, 57–68.
J. M., Cox, J. R., Lugo, M., & Wiskow, K. M. (2015). Matson, J. L., & LoVullo, S. V. (2008). A review of
The safety of functional analyses of self-injurious behavioral treatments for self-injurious behav-
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, iors of persons with autism spectrum disorders.
107–114. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.168 Behavior Modification, 32, 61–76. https://doi.
Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., & Lewin, A. B. (2002a). Behavioral org/10.1177/0145445507304581
treatment of self-injury, 1964 to 2000. American McGrath, P. J., Rosmus, C., Canfield, C., Campbell,
Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, 212–221. https:// M. A., & Hennigar, A. (1998). Behaviours caregiv-
doi.org/10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0212:BTOSIT ers use to determine pain in non-verbal, cognitively
>2.0.CO;2 impaired individuals. Developmental Medicine and
Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., & Lewin, A. B. (2002b). Child Neurology, 40, 340–343.
The impact of functional assessment on the treat- Miltenberger, R. G. (2011). Behavior modification:
ment of self-injurious behavior. In S. R. Schroeder, Principles and procedures. Belmont, CA: Cengage
M. L. Oster-Granite, & T. Thompson (Eds.), Self-­ Learning.
injurious behavior: Gene-brain-behavior relation- Minshawi, N. F., Hurwitz, S., Morriss, D., & McDougle,
ships (pp. 119–131). Washington, DC: American C. J. (2014). Multidisciplinary assessment and treat-
Psychological Association. ment of self-injurious behavior in autism spectrum
Kroeker, R., Touchette, P. E., Engleman, L., & disorder and intellectual disability: Integration of psy-
Sandman, C. A. (2004). Quantifying temporal dis- chological and biological theory and approach. Journal
tributions of self-injurious behavior: Defining of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 1541–
bouts versus discrete events. American Journal 1568. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2307-3
on Mental Retardation, 109, 1–8. https://doi. Moore, J. W., Fisher, W. W., & Pennington, A. (2004).
org/10.1352/0895-8017(2004)109<1:QTDOSB>2.0 Systematic application and removal of protective
.CO;2 equipment in the assessment of multiple topographies
Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S. F., of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
O’Connor, J. T., Paclawskyj, T. R., & Rush, K. S. 37, 73.
(2003). Functional analysis and treatment of self-­ Newell, K. M., Challis, J. H., Boros, R. L., &
injurious behavior in young children a summary of Bodfish, J. W. (2002). Further evidence on the
30 cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, dynamics of self-injurious behaviors: Impact
205–219. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-205 forces and limb motions. American Journal on
LeBlanc, L. A., Patel, M. R., & Carr, J. E. (2000). Recent Mental Retardation, 107, 60–68. https://doi.
advances in the assessment of aberrant behavior main- org/10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0060:FEOTDO>2
tained by automatic reinforcement in individuals .0.CO;2
with developmental disabilities. Journal of Behavior O’Neill, R. E., Albin, R. W., Horner, R. H., Storey, K.,
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 31, 137–154. & Sprague, J. R. (2015). Functional assessment
284 C.J. Clay et al.

and ­program development (3rd ed.). Stamford, CT: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 181–198.
Cengage Learning. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.14
O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Edrisinha, C., Saini, V., Greer, B. D., Fisher, W. W., Lichtblau, K. R.,
& Andrews, A. (2005). An examination of the effects DeSouza, A. A., & Mitteer, D. R. (2016). Individual and
of a classroom activity schedule on levels of self-­ combined effects of noncontingent reinforcement and
injury and engagement for a child with severe autism. response blocking on automatically reinforced problem
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49,
305–311. 693–698. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.306
Oliver, C., Murphy, G. H., & Corbett, J. A. (1987). Self-­ Sandman, C. A. (1990). The opiate hypothesis in autism
injurious behaviour in people with mental handicap: A and self-injury. Journal of Child and Adolescent
total population study. Journal of Mental Deficiency Psychopharmacology, 1, 237–248. https://doi.
Research, 31(Pt 2), 147–162. org/10.1089/cap.1990.1.237
Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y., Sandman, C. A. (2009). Efficacy of opioid antagonists in
& Vollmer, T. R. (2000). Questions about behavioral attenuating self-injurious behavior. In R. L. Dean III,
function (QABF): A behavioral checklist for func- E. J. Bilsky, & S. S. Negus (Eds.), Opiate receptors
tional assessment of aberrant behavior. Research in and antagonists (pp. 457–472). New York: Humana
Developmental Disabilities, 21, 223–229. https://doi. Press. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/
org/10.1016/S0891-4222(00)00036-6 chapter/10.1007/978-1-59745-197-0_24
Paisey, T. J. H., Whitney, R. B., & Wainczak, S. M. Schroeder, S. R., Marquis, J. G., Reese, R. M., Richman,
(1993). Case study: Noninvasive behavioral treat- D. M., Mayo-Ortega, L., Oyama-Ganiko, R., …
ment of self-injurious hand stereotypy in a child with Lawrence, L. (2014). Risk factors for self-injury,
Rett syndrome. Behavioral Residential Treatment, 8, aggression, and stereotyped behavior among young
133–145. children at risk for intellectual and developmental
Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A., disabilities. American Journal on Intellectual and
Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. E., & Keeney, K. M. Developmental Disabilities, 119, 351–370. https://doi.
(1998). Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of org/10.1352/1944-7558-119.4.351
its reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior Smith, C. M., Smith, R. G., Dracobly, J. D., & Pace, A. P.
Analysis, 31, 165–189. (2012). Multiple-respondent anecdotal assessments:
Pierce, D. W., & Cheney, C. D. (2013). Behavior analysis An analysis of interrater agreement and correspon-
and learning (5th ed.). New York: Psychology Press. dence with analogue assessment outcomes. Journal of
Richman, D. M. (2008). Annotation: Early intervention Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 779–795. https://doi.
and prevention of self-injurious behavior exhibited org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-779
by young children with developmental disabilities. Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52, 3–17. (1993). Experimental analysis and treatment of multi-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.01027.x ply controlled self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
Rincover, A. (1978). Sensory extinction: A procedure for Analysis, 26, 183–196. https://doi.org/10.1901/
eliminating self-stimulatory behavior in developmen- jaba.1993.26-183
tally disabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Smith, S., Branford, D., Collacott, R. A., Cooper, S. A., &
Psychology, 6, 299–310. McGrother, C. (1996). Prevalence and cluster typology
Roane, H. S., Piazza, C. C., Sgro, G. M., Volkert, V. M., & of maladaptive behaviors in a geographically defined
Anderson, C. M. (2001). Analysis of aberrant behav- population of adults with learning disabilities. The
iour associated with Rett syndrome. Disability and British Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 219–227. https://
Rehabilitation, 23, 139–148. doi.org/10.1192/bjp.169.2.219
Rojahn, J., Borthwick-Duffy, S. A., & Jacobson, J. W. St. Peter, C. C., Vollmer, T. R., Bourret, J. C., Borrero,
(1993). The association between psychiatric diagno- C. S. W., Sloman, K. N., & Rapp, J. T. (2005). On
ses and severe behavior problems in mental retarda- the role of attention in naturally occurring matching
tion. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry: Official Journal relations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38,
of the American Academy of Clinical Psychiatrists, 5, 429–443. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.172-04
163–170. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00924733 Symons, F. J., & Thompson, T. (1997). A review of self-­
Rojahn, J., Matson, J. L., Lott, D., Esbensen, A. J., & injurious behavior and pain in persons with develop-
Smalls, Y. (2001). The behavior problems inventory: mental disabilities. International Review of Research
An instrument for the assessment of self-injury, ste- in Mental Retardation, 21, 69–111.
reotyped behavior, and aggression/destruction in indi- Tate, B. G., & Baroff, G. S. (1966). Aversive control of
viduals with developmental disabilities. Journal of self-injurious behavior in a psychotic boy. Behaviour
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 577–588. Research and Therapy, 4, 281–287. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013299028321 org/10.1016/0005-7967(66)90084-2
Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Zhou, L. (2013). Thompson, R. H., & Borrero, J. C. (2011). Direct obser-
Assessment and treatment of chronic hand mouthing. vation. In W. Fisher, C. Piazza, & H. Roane (Eds.),
Self-Injurious Behavior in Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities… 285

Handbook of applied behavior analysis (pp. 191–205). ferential reinforcement of other behavior. Journal
New York: The Guilford Press. of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 9–21. https://doi.
Thompson, R. H., & Iwata, B. A. (2001). A descriptive org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-9
analysis of social consequences following problem Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & LeBlanc, L. (1994).
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, Treatment of self-injury and hand mouthing fol-
169–178. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-169 lowing inconclusive functional analyses. Journal of
Tiger, J. H., Fisher, W. W., & Bouxsein, K. J. (2009). Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 331–344. https://doi.
Therapist- and self-monitored DRO contingencies org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-331
as a treatment for the self-injurious skin picking of Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Ringdahl, J. E. (1995).
a young man with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Noncontingent escape as treatment for self-injurious
Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 315–319. https://doi. behavior maintained by negative reinforcement.
org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-315 Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 15–26.
Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & Borrero, C. S. W.
communication training: A review and practical guide. (2009). Behavioral assessment of self-injury. In
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23. J. L. Matson, F. Andrasik, & M. L. Matson (Eds.),
Toussaint, K. A., & Tiger, J. H. (2012). Reducing covert Assessing childhood psychopathology and devel-
self-injurious behavior maintained by automatic rein- opmental disabilities (pp. 341–369). New York:
forcement through a variable momentary DRO pro- Springer. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/
cedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, chapter/10.1007/978-0-387-09528-8_12
179–184. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-179 Van Houten, R., & Rolider, A. (1991). Applied behav-
Vaughan, M. E., & Michael, J. L. (1982). Automatic ior analysis. In J. L. Matson & J. A. Mulick (Eds.),
reinforcement: An important but ignored concept. Handbook ofmental retardation (2nd ed., pp. 569–
Behavior, 10, 217–227. 585). New York: Pergamon
Volkert, V. M., Lerman, D. C., Call, N. A., & Trosclair-­ Wilder, D. A., Normand, M., & Atwell, J. (2005).
Lasserre, N. (2009). An evaluation of resurgence dur- Noncontingent reinforcement as treatment for food
ing treatment with functional communication training. refusal and associated self-injury. Journal of Applied
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 145–160. Behavior Analysis, 38, 549–553.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-145 Wilson, D. M., Iwata, B. A., & Bloom, S. E. (2012).
Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic rein- Computer-assisted measurement of wound size
forcement: Implications for behavioral research associated with self-injurious behavior. Journal of
in developmental disabilities. Research in Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 797–808. https://doi.
Developmental Disabilities, 15(3), 187–207. https:// org/10.1901/jaba.45-797
doi.org/10.1016/0891-4222(94)90011-6 Zarcone, J. R., Rodgers, T. A., Iwata, B. A., Rourke,
Vollmer, T. R., & Iwata, B. A. (1992). Differential rein- D. A., & Dorsey, M. F. (1991). Reliability analysis of
forcement as treatment for behavior disorders: the motivation assessment scale: A failure to replicate.
Procedural and functional variations. Research in Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 349–360.
Developmental Disabilities, 13, 393–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/0891-4222(91)90031-M
Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. G., Zhou, L., Goff, G. A., & Iwata, B. A. (2000). Effects of
& Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of attention in increased response effort on self-injury and objectma-
the treatment of attention-maintained self-­ injurious nipulation as competing responses. Journal of Applied
behavior: Noncontingent reinforcement and dif- Behavior Analysis, 33, 29–40.
Pica in Individuals
with Developmental Disabilities

Esther Hong and Dennis R. Dixon

Contents was reclassified under the “Feeding and Eating


Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disorders” category in the Diagnostic and
Disabilities............................................................  287 Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth
Assessment..............................................................  289
Edition (DSM-5; APA, 2013), which broadened
the onset criteria to individuals of all ages. Pica is
Treatment of Pica...................................................  290
a significant challenging behavior in that it can
Antecedent Modification Interventions................  291 lead to serious medical problems and, in some
Response-Contingent Interventions......................  292 cases, even death. Due to the severe medical con-
sequences of pica, some researchers have classi-
Punishment-Based Interventions..........................  293
fied pica as self-injurious behavior (SIB; Call,
Discussion................................................................  295 Simmons, Lomas Mevers, & Alvarez, 2015;
References...............................................................  297 Williams & McAdam, 2016).
According to the DSM-5, symptoms must per-
sist for over a period of at least 1 month, be inap-
propriate to the developmental level of the
 ica in Individuals
P individual, and not part of a culturally supported
with Developmental Disabilities or socially normative practice (APA, 2013).
Commonly reported types of pica include geoph-
Pica is the consumption of nonfood, nonnutritive agy (consumption of earth; Johnson, 1990),
substances (American Psychological Association chthonophagy (consumption of dirt; Johnson,
[APA], 2013) and is the most commonly observed 1990), lithophagy (consumption of stone or
feeding disorder among children and adolescents gravel; Johnson, 1990), coprophagy (consump-
with developmental disabilities (Barrett, 2008). tion of feces; Foxx & Martin, 1975), and other
Pica has historically been treated as a feeding and nonfood items (e.g., chalk, paper, paint chips,
eating disorder specific to infancy or early child- cigarette butts). Although the DSM-5 states that
hood (APA, 2000). More recently though, pica pica is of “nonfood” items, some researchers
have broadened pica topography to include non-
nutritive food consumption, such as amylophagy
E. Hong (*) • D.R. Dixon
(consumption of raw starches; Johnson, 1990)
Center for Autism and Related Disorders,
Woodland Hills, CA, USA and pagophagy (consumption of ice; Miao,
e-mail: e.hong@centerforautism.com Young, & Golden, 2015), or to include food items

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 287


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_17
288 E. Hong and D.R. Dixon

that are retrieved from inappropriate places (e.g., 2008; Johnson, 1990). Pica is commonly comor-
floor, trash; Hirsch & Smith-Myles, 1996). bid with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and ID
Pica is common among certain cultures. An and less commonly comorbid with schizophrenia
individual does not meet the DSM-5 criteria for and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; APA,
pica if feeding behaviors are supported by cul- 2013). Kinnell (1985) found that 60% of the ASD
tural practices (APA, 2013). For example, chil- group engaged in pica, while only 4% of the
dren in sub-Saharan Africa (Nchito, Geissler, Down’s syndrome group engaged in pica.
Mubila, Friis, & Olsen, 2004) and pregnant Individuals with profound ID are most likely to
women across Africa commonly engage in be affected by pica (Ali, 2001; APA, 2013), and
geophagy (28–100%; Young et al., 2010). In prevalence among individuals with ID appears to
areas where geophagy was less common, amy- increase with severity of ID. Although pica is not
lophagy was much more frequently reported commonly reported among individuals with ID
(Young et al. 2010). In the United States, pica is in the community, high rates of pica are found in
more common in rural (Johnson, 1990) and clinics and institutions (5.7–25.8%; Ashworth,
underdeveloped areas, with reports of pagophagy Hirdes, & Martin, 2009), with one report finding
among pregnant, African-American women the prevalence of pica to be as high as 25.8% in
(Edwards et al., 1994) and geophagy among institutionalized people with ID (Danford &
pregnant women in the rural South (Johnson, Huber, 1982). It should be noted that pica is com-
1990). monly missed and underreported (Rose, Porcelli,
Pica is sometimes considered not to be a major & Neale, 2000), thus, the prevalence is difficult
behavioral or medical problem (McAlpine & to ascertain (APA, 2013). Underreporting is a
Singh, 1986; Williams & McAdam, 2012). problem and difficult to control. Fear of chastise-
Consequently, this behavior is commonly missed, ment may contribute to this underreporting
and individuals with this disorder engage in this (Young et al., 2010). When pica occurs in the
potentially dangerous behavior for several years context of other mental disorders, particularly
before it is detected (McAlpine & Singh, 1986). ASD, it is imperative to seek clinical and/or
Pica has many medical consequences including behavioral treatment (APA, 2013).
intestinal obstruction or puncture, acute weight Several theories regarding the etiology of
loss, poisoning, dental health problems, infec- pica, including behavioral or nutritional causes
tion, and gastrointestinal parasites (Call et al., (Bugle & Rubin, 1993; Chisholm & Martin,
2015; Foxx & Martin, 1975). These complica- 1981; Lofts, Schroeder, & Maier, 1990), have
tions may result in emergency surgery and even been evaluated. Some research has indicated that
death (APA, 2013; Bell & Stein, 1992; McAdam, pica may be caused due to micronutrient defi-
Sherman, Sheldon, & Napolitano, 2004). In addi- ciencies (e.g., iron, zinc), hunger, gastrointestinal
tion, ingestion of certain nonfood items (e.g., distress, and protection from pathogens and tox-
paint chips, soil) can impair intellectual and ins (e.g., Rose et al., 2000; Young et al., 2010).
physical development. Thus, detection of pica One meta-analysis found that compared to indi-
and subsequent treatment should be a high viduals without pica, those with pica were more
priority. likely to have anemia, low hemoglobin concen-
Pica affects people across ages, gender, geo- tration, low hematocrit concentration, and low
graphic location, and socioeconomic status plasma zinc concentration (Miao et al., 2015).
(Sayetta, 1986; Young et al., 2010). Pica is fre- Researchers have hypothesized that these factors
quently reported among pregnant women and result in nutritional deficiencies. Further, they
less frequently among young, typically develop- theorize that individuals experience cravings and
ing children. In very young children, the behavior engage in inappropriate feeding behavior in order
is not considered true pica but rather a habitual to satisfy the cravings and eliminate the nutri-
mouthing behavior, which diminishes with age tional deficiencies (reviewed by Barrett, 2008).
and is virtually nonexistent by age 2 (Barrett, However, it is unclear if the nutritional deficien-
Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disabilities 289

cies were the cause or the result of the pica. Feeding Problems (STEP; Matson & Kuhn,
Nonetheless, among young children without 2001), the Autism Spectrum Disorders-­
intellectual impairments, a nutritional-deficiency Comorbidity for Children (ASD-CC; Matson &
approach was found to be the most common Gonzalez, 2007), and the Behavior Problems
treatment applied (McAdam et al., 2004). Inventory (BPI; Rojahn, Matson, Esbensen, &
However, Rose and colleagues (2000) found no Smalls, 2001).
significant differences in nutritional deficiencies The STEP (2001) is a 23-item questionnaire
between children and adolescents with pica and that screens for feeding problems presented by
those without. Overall, there is some evidence to persons with ID. The questions are designed
support this theory; however, data are limited and using a Likert-type format and assess for the fre-
much more research needs to be conducted. quency and severity of feeding problems. This
While the direct causes of pica remain unclear, screening tool represents five categories of feed-
there has been significant support for a behav- ing problems. These categories include aspira-
ioral etiology (Favell, McGimsey, & Schell, tion risk, selectivity, skills, food refusal-related
1982; Smith, 1987). Pica is found to be most behavior problems, and nutrition-related behav-
commonly maintained by automatic reinforce- ior problems. Items that may be associated with
ment (Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003; Williams pica such as “he/she eats or attempts to eat items
& McAdam, 2012). Further, there is support for that are not food” are included in the nutrition-­
pica to be maintained by social variables. In a related behavior problems category.
study of institutionalized adults with ID The ASD-CC (2007) is an 84-item question-
(N = 1008), Ashworth and colleagues (2009) naire that screens for comorbid conditions with
found that pica was significantly associated with ASD, including depression, conduct disorder,
the absence of a strong and supportive relation- attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),
ship with family and reduced social contact with tic disorder, OCD, specific phobia, and eating
family and/or friends (i.e., visit, overnight stay, or
difficulties. In regard to pica, informants are
other types of interaction within the last 30 days).instructed to rate the feeding item (i.e., eats things
These participants had spent an average of that are not meant to be eaten [e.g., eats paint
41.6 years in an institutional setting, and 71.7% chips, dirt, hair, cloth, etc.]) for the extent that it
of participants had left their family home before is a recent problem. While this tool is not a
the age of 10. The inverse relationship between screening tool specific to the assessment of pica,
pica and social interaction suggests that decreased it may be an important first step in assessing pica
levels of social interaction were associated with in individuals with ASD.
increased rates of pica (Ashworth et al., 2009). The BPI (2001) is 52-item questionnaire that
These findings warrant further investigation of screens for problem behaviors (i.e., SIB items,
the behavioral and social variables that maintain stereotypic behavior items, and aggressive/
pica. destructive behavior items) in individuals with
ID. Pica is included within the SIB section and is
defined as the “mouthing or swallowing of
Assessment objects which should not be mouthed or swal-
lowed for health or hygiene reasons (non-food
Professionals who treat individuals with develop- items such as feces, grass, paper, garbage, hair).”
mental disabilities are recommended to screen The BPI is one of the few instruments that
for pica by reviewing medical history, interview- ­specifically assesses for the frequency and sever-
ing caregivers, observing behavior, and/or imple- ity of pica.
menting challenging behavior screening scales The STEP, ASD-CC, and BPI are efficient
(Williams & McAdam, 2012). However, well-­ tools for assessing pica in individuals with ID or
developed assessment scales designed to detect ASD. All of these scales have demonstrated good
pica are limited but include the Screening Tool of reliability and validity (Gonzalez et al., 2009;
290 E. Hong and D.R. Dixon

Kuhn & Matson, 2002; Matson, LoVullo, Rivet, Toyer, 2000). Due to these issues, biological
& Boisjoli, 2009). These indirect methods of interventions have insufficient evidence to sup-
assessment can be useful in identifying the pres- port their use as treatments for pica (reviewed by
ence of pica. Matson et al., 2013).

Treatment of Pica Behavioral Interventions

The treatment of pica has received significant A key aspect of most behavioral interventions is
attention over the years and has been the subject to first identify the operant function of the chal-
of a number of reviews (Hagopian, Rooker, & lenging behavior. Functional analysis procedures
Rolider, 2011; Matson, Hattier, Belva, & Matson, are typically categorized as either direct or indi-
2013; McAdam et al., 2004). Overall, most stud- rect (Dixon, Vogel, & Tarbox, 2012). Once the
ies have focused on behavioral treatments; how- function of the behavior is identified, the clini-
ever, some few researchers have address cian is able to choose the most appropriate inter-
biological treatments such as nutritional supple- vention to address the variables that maintain the
ments. These treatments are discussed in turn. behavior.
Indirect functional analysis methods consist
of gathering information about the person and
Biological Interventions potential environmental factors that may be serv-
ing to reinforce the behavior. Typically, a clini-
A number of researchers have discussed biologi- cian may distribute a questionnaire or interview
cal variables in regard to the etiology of pica the caretaker of the individual with pica regard-
(Barrett, 2008; McAdam, Briedbord, Levine, & ing the frequency, severity, and contexts of
Williams, 2012). However, studies that have eval- behavior. An indirect functional analysis can help
uated biological treatment based upon these etio- identify the topography and function of behavior,
logical explanations have been scarce. In general, but some researchers have argued that they may
biological treatments have exclusively focused not always be sufficient for determining the oper-
on the use of nutritional supplements to treat the ant function of a challenging behavior (Williams
hypothesized cause of the challenging behavior & McAdam, 2016). It should also be noted
(Matson et al., 2013; McAdam et al., 2012). though that indirect functional analyses may be
Biological interventions such as providing more effective at identifying the function of
nutritional supplements (e.g., iron supplement, behavior when the behavior is of a low frequency
multivitamins) have received some attention and unlikely to be observed within typical obser-
from researchers (e.g., Bugle & Rubin, 1993; vation periods.
Gutelius, Millican, Layman, Cohen, & Dublin, Direct methods of functional assessment
1962; Pace & Toyer, 2000); however, empirical include experimental approaches to identifying
evaluations of these treatments have not found the variables that maintain the SIB (i.e., Iwata,
evidence to support their use. For example, in a Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982).
controlled trial, Gutelius and colleagues (1962) During a direct functional analysis, a clinician
found that iron supplementation did not yield any will take an experimental approach to identify the
clinically significant differences between groups. variables that maintain the behavior. By using
More recent studies have had significant limita- safe, baiting methods (i.e., presentation of food
tions such as an uncontrolled research design and nonfood items) to manipulate the ­antecedents
(Bugle & Rubin, 1993), small sample size (Pace and consequences of behavior, researchers can
& Toyer, 2000), and variable results (Pace & identify under which contingencies the behaviors
Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disabilities 291

are maintained. A direct functional analysis may Antecedent Modification


provide more detailed information regarding the Interventions
function of a behavior. However, an experimental
functional analysis may require several hours per Noncontingent reinforcement (NCR) is the most
day, over a period of 2–3 weeks, across clinicians commonly used intervention for pica (McAdam
(Matson, Bamburg, Cherry, & Paclawskyj, 1999). et al., 2004). NCR is a well-established treatment
Therefore, conducting an experimental func- that can be useful for interrupting or preventing
tional analysis can be very time and resource automatically maintained behavior by providing
intensive and also may yield results lacking in alternative sources of reinforcement (Favell et al.,
reliability and validity (Matson et al., 1999; 1982). During NCR procedures, a reinforcer (e.g.,
Sturmey, 1995). Instead, practitioners may utilize toy, food, attention) is presented independent of a
reliable, indirect functional analyses such as the response (Cooper, Heron, & Heyward, 2007).
Questions About Behavioral Function (QABF; Reinforcement can be delivered on a fixed-time
Matson & Vollmer, 1995), which was found to schedule (e.g., reinforcement provided every
predict the function of behavior in 75% of cases 5 min) or variable-time schedule (e.g., reinforce-
(Hall, 2005). Following the functional analysis, ment provided on average of every 5 min; Huete,
the contributing variables are altered during treat- Schmidt, & Lopez-Arvizu, 2014). In addition, the
ment to reduce the challenging behavior (Hanley reinforcer does not need to be functionally related
et al., 2003). to the challenging behavior. NCR has been effec-
The application of functional analyses has led tive in reducing challenging behaviors maintained
to significant advancements in the assessment by automatic reinforcement (e.g., oral self-stimu-
and treatment of pica in individuals with develop- lation) because alternative sources of reinforce-
mental disabilities. Studies have found that pica ment are provided (Favell et al., 1982).
is most commonly maintained by sensory or Several studies have found that NCR was suc-
automatic reinforcement (e.g., oral stimulation; cessful in reducing pica. The noncontingent pre-
Delaney et al., 2015; McAdam et al., 2004) and sentation of food or toys that can be safely
less commonly maintained by social variables, mouthed have been effective in reducing pica
such as access to tangible items and/or attention, because the alternative objects provides access to
or physiological variables, such as the addictive the same source of stimulation and thus provides
effect of nicotine in cigarettes (Piazza et al., the same, or similar, reinforcement (McAdam
1998). et al., 2004). Favell and colleagues (1982) pro-
Once the function of the behavior is identified, vided popcorn and toys to three adolescent par-
treatment may be implemented to reduce pica ticipants with profound ID, whose behavior was
and generalize results across settings. Meta-­ hypothesized to be maintained by gustatory rein-
analyses have found that comprehensive behav- forcement. Pica was reduced to 0% in two study
ioral interventions are well-established and have participants and to 5% in one study participant.
been highly effective in treating pica (Call et al., In another study, the cigarette pica of a 17-year-­
2015), with several studies reporting more than a old male with severe ID and ASD was hypothe-
90% reduction of pica (Hagopian et al., 2011). sized to be maintained by physiological variables
Behavioral treatments that have been effective in (i.e., nicotine). Piazza, Hanley, and Fisher (1996)
reducing or eliminating pica will be described in found that the participant’s pica was maintained
turn. Interventions have been categorized under when the environment was baited with cigarettes
(a) antecedent modification interventions, (b) that contained tobacco with nicotine but was not
response-contingent interventions, and (c) maintained when baited with cigarettes that con-
punishment-­based interventions. tained herbs without nicotine. Treatment consisted
292 E. Hong and D.R. Dixon

of the noncontingent presentation of preferred 21-year-old female with profound ID and epi-
foods and a contingent verbal i­nterruption, “no lepsy was taught to discriminate between food
butts.” Following treatment, the participant’s pica and nonfood items and subsequently place the
was reduced to 0 responses per min (Piazza et al., nonedible items in the trash (Bogart, Piersel, &
1996). In a separate study by Piazza and col- Gross, 1995).
leagues (1998), a functional analysis of pica was Response effort manipulations have been
conducted for three participants with intellectual found to be effective in reducing pica. Piazza,
disabilities. For two of the three study partici- Roane, Keeney, Boney, and Abt (2002) manipu-
pants, a 5-year-old male with moderate ID and a lated response effort among three participants
4-year-old female with profound ID, pica was whose pica was maintained by automatic rein-
found to be maintained by social and automatic forcement. When response effort to engage in
reinforcement. The noncontingent presentation pica was low or medium, with no access to alter-
of attention and continuous access to tangible native items, pica level was the highest. When
reinforcement led to significant reduction of pica. response effort to engage in pica was high, with
Environmental enrichment procedures utilize no access to alternative items, pica was still
the participant’s environment to reduce pica by reduced. When response effort for alternative
ensuring the environment includes items hypoth- items was increased, pica increased and effort to
esized to compete with pica (e.g., preferred food, engage with alternative items decreased. Lowest
toys, and activities; Call et al., 2015). By increas- levels of pica were found when response effort to
ing the available preferred items in the environ- engage in pica was high and alternative, preferred
ment, the potential for reinforcement is items were available. These findings indicate that
maximized (Williams & McAdam, 2016). In a increasing the effort required to engage in pica, in
participant group of young children, the addition addition to NCR, may reduce the behavior.
of play and other recreational activities resulted
in decreased levels of pica (Madden, Russo, &
Cataldo, 1980). Similar results were found when Response-Contingent Interventions
leisure activities were provided for an adult male
participant with developmental disabilities Differential reinforcement (DR) is used to
(Burke & Smith, 1999). Enrichment of foods increase desired behaviors through reinforcement
(e.g., using highly spiced, flavored foods with and to decrease challenging behaviors through
meals/snacks, teaching participants to exchange extinction (Cooper et al., 2007). In differential
nonfood items for preferred food items) was also reinforcement of incompatible behavior (DRI),
found to reduce pica (Baker, Valenzuela, & behaviors that are incompatible with, or cannot
Wieseler, 2005). occur at the same time as, the problem behavior
Discrimination training procedures aim to are reinforced. In differential reinforcement of
prevent pica by teaching individuals to correctly alternative behavior (DRA), alternative, appro-
discriminate edible versus nonedible food items. priate behaviors are reinforced (Huete et al.,
However, discrimination training alone cannot 2014). Typically, during DRI and DRA proce-
eliminate pica and thus are applied in conjunc- dures, the challenging behavior is placed on
tion with a response-contingent intervention fol- extinction. In some cases, extinction may not be
lowing pica attempts. Johnson, Hunt, and Siebert possible depending on the severity of the chal-
(1994) taught two male teens with profound ID to lenging behavior and may require additional
only eat food placed on a specific placement. manipulation of the environment to increase
When pica occurred, the participants were opportunities for appropriate behavior (Athens &
required to spit out the nonfood item and wash Vollmer, 2010).
their faces for 15 s. Pica was reduced across set- Studies have found that DRI and DRA proce-
tings (e.g., dining room, alone, group activity dures led to a reduction of pica (Call et al., 2015;
room) in both participants. In another study, a Donnelly & Olczak, 1990; Goh, Iwata, & Kahng,
Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disabilities 293

1999; Smith, 1987). Contingent on an attempt of ings suggest that both social interaction and
pica, Smith (1987) provided a verbal prompt not response-blocking equipment may influence the
to ingest the inedible object (e.g., paper clip, reduction of pica (Mace & Knight, 1986).
paper, bottle caps) and instructed the participant During visual screening procedures, a type of
to remain on task. Verbal praise and tokens were blindfold is placed over the participant’s eyes
provided when the participant performed the immediately following the pica attempt. Singh
incompatible behavior (i.e., keeping hands on and Winston (1984) found that visual screening
work materials). Following DRI intervention, for 1 min resulted in significant reduction of pica
pica occurrence decreased from 21.3 occurrences in a 24-year-old female with profound ID. In
of pica per day to 3.7 occurrences per day. In another study of three toddlers with profound ID
DRA procedures to treat pica, alternative behav- and pervasive developmental disorders, pretreat-
iors (e.g., handing pica item to a clinician, dis- ment assessments suggested that facial screening
carding pica item in the trash, engaging in a would be an effective intervention (Fisher et al.,
leisure activity) were reinforced (Call et al., 1994). Participants were verbally reprimanded
2015). DRA was found to be effective in reduc- following each occurrence of pica and his/her
ing cigarette pica in 38-year-old and 44-year-old eyes covered for 30 s. Pica was reduced and, fur-
males with profound ID. Attempts of pica were ther, remained at low levels at a 9-month follow-
interrupted, and a reinforcer was provided con- ­up. Contingent upon pica, visual screening (i.e.,
tingent upon alternative behavior (i.e., chewing covered face with bib) and physical restraint (i.e.,
sugarless mint gum). held hands to side for 15 s) were implemented.
Response-blocking procedures are time and Pica decreased from 25 occurrences per month to
staff intensive in that they require a caregiver to 12 occurrences per month (Bogart et al., 1995).
provide constant supervision and remain in close
proximity to the participant in order to physically
block access to pica items. Response blocking Punishment-Based Interventions
can only reduce pica if each attempt of pica is
consistently interrupted (McCord, Grosser, In punishment-based procedures, an environmen-
Iwata, & Powers, 2005). Consequently, response-­ tal stimulus is either provided following a behavior
blocking procedures are rarely used as a sole (i.e., positive punishment) or removed following a
intervention and instead are included as part of an behavior (i.e., negative punishment; Lerman &
intervention package (Williams & McAdam, Vorndran, 2002). Positive punishment procedures
2016). Mechanical restraint (i.e., face mask, hel- include overcorrection, aversive stimuli, and phys-
met, and arm restraints) and response blocking ical restraint. Negative punishment procedures
(i.e., pica item pushed down before entry into the include response cost and time-out. A limitation of
mouth) were both found to be effective in reduc- punishment-based procedures is that they do not
ing pica in a 4-year-old girl with profound ID teach appropriate or replacement behaviors (Huete
(LeBlanc, Piazza, & Krug, 1997). LeBlanc and et al., 2014). As a result, punishment-­based inter-
colleagues (1997) concluded that response block- ventions should be implemented in conjunction
ing was the preferable intervention since it was with an antecedent-based intervention, which rein-
less restrictive, resulted in fewer negative vocal- forces and teaches appropriate behavior.
izations, and increased opportunities for social In a review by McAdam and colleagues
interaction. In a study evaluating the effective- (2004), overcorrection was the most commonly
ness of a protective equipment (i.e., helmet) as a used punishment-based intervention. However, a
response-blocking tool, Mace and Knight (1986) study using overcorrection treatment methods
found that the helmet alone did not prevent or has not been published in almost 30 years.
reduce pica. Rather, lower levels of interaction Overcorrection, a type of “work and effort” pro-
(i.e., staff-participant interaction) with no helmet cedure, is considered a mild punishment proce-
resulted in the lowest levels of pica. These find- dure that once was favored because it required
294 E. Hong and D.R. Dixon

the participant to make restitution for the chal- pins, cigarette butts; Paisey & Whitney, 1989). In
lenging behavior and to exhibit more appropriate a 16-year-old female with multiple disabilities,
behaviors (Matson et al., 2013). Overcorrection the presentation of water mist in the face or sniff
procedures vary across studies and range from of aromatic ammonia resulted in a significant
simple overcorrection procedures (e.g., spit reduction in pica occurrence. In spite of these
object out and wash the mouth with washcloth successes, treatment of pica has moved away
for 15 s; Kalfus, Fisher-Gross, Marvullo, & Nau, from aversive stimuli to more socially accepted
1987) to multistep, complex procedures. In an interventions (Matson et al., 2013).
overcorrection procedure created by Foxx and Physical and mechanical restraints are also
Martin (1975) to eliminate coprophagy, contin- controversial methods of treatment but are still
gent on a pica attempt, the trainer manually implemented given the high potential for serious
guided the participant toilet bowl and verbally medical complications following each instance
instructed the client to spit the feces into the toi- of pica (Matson et al., 2013). Physical restraint
let. Next, the participant was required to brush procedures (e.g., holding down arms to side) had
their mouth, teeth, and gums with a toothbrush high rates of success (up to 97.3% reduction of
soaked in oral antiseptic. The participant was pica; Call et al., 2015) when applied in conjunc-
then required to wash their hands and scrub fin- tion with other reinforcement-based interven-
gernails for 10 min. In addition, the participant tions. In a comparison study between physical
was required to clean their anal area with a cloth restraint and overcorrection interventions, physi-
and then briefly wash their hands. Lastly, the par- cal restraint was found to be more effective in
ticipant was guided back to the area where they reducing pica than overcorrection (Singh &
were discovered engaging in pica and required to Bakker, 1984). Several studies found that imple-
either mop up the area with a disinfectant. If the menting physical restraint procedures for various
participant had obtained feces from an unflushed time intervals (e.g., 10 s, 15 s, 30 s; 1 min;
toilet, the participant was required to flush all Williams & McAdam, 2016) reduced pica.
unflushed toilets. This overcorrection procedure However, it is unclear whether brief intervals or
created by Foxx and Martin (1975) took approxi- longer intervals of physical restraint are most
mately 30 min to complete. After just 2 weeks of effective in reducing pica or if treatment out-
this overcorrection intervention, the target behav- comes maintain over time.
iors had decreased to 0%. In addition, the elimi- Time-out procedures have not been commonly
nation of pica was maintained at the 7-week implemented to treat pica. Therefore, there is
follow-up. Although overcorrection methods are insufficient evidence for the efficacy of this inter-
effective, they are time and staff-intensive and vention to reduce pica. However, a study by
thus limited to institution or hospital settings. Ausman, Ball, and Alexander (1974) found that
The presentation of aversive stimuli has also pica was reduced to 0% following a time-out pro-
been used as a punishment-based intervention to cedure. Contingent on the behavior, the study
limit and reduce pica. Aversive stimuli are pro- participant, a 14-year-old male with severe ID,
vided contingent upon the occurrence of problem was told “don’t eat that” and required to wear a
behavior. Commonly reported aversive stimuli time-out helmet for 15 min.
included a squirt of water mist on face or a squirt A number of behavioral interventions are
of lemon juice in mouth (Paisey & Whitney, effective to reduce pica. These interventions have
1989; Rojahn, McGonigle, Curcio, & Dixon, been grouped as (a) antecedent modification
1987). These methods have been viewed as con- interventions, (b) response-contingent interven-
troversial but were effective in the reduction of tions, and (c) punishment-based interventions.
pica. Following aversive stimuli treatment of Well-established interventions include NCR
squirting lemon juice in his mouth, a 16-year-old (Favell et al., 1982; Mace & Knight, 1986), envi-
male with profound ID no longer engaged in the ronmental enrichment (Madden et al., 1980), and
ingestion of nonedible objects (e.g., broken glass, overcorrection (Foxx & Martin, 1975).
Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disabilities 295

Interventions such as response effort manipula- Some researchers have hypothesized that the
tions (Piazza et al., 2002) and response blocking etiology of pica is due to nutritional deficiencies,
(McCord et al., 2005) have limited evidence but such as anemia or low iron and zinc levels, and
warrant additional research. that individuals engage in pica to eliminate the
nutritional deficiencies. However, there is insuf-
ficient evidence to support a nutritional explana-
Discussion tion. From a behavioral perspective, pica is most
commonly maintained by automatic reinforce-
As previously noted, pica is a challenging behav- ment, followed by social variables (Matson et al.,
ior that can lead to severe medical complications, 2013). Several behavioral interventions are well-­
including death. There are many different types established and have been found to reduce pica in
and forms of pica, including geophagy, amyloph- individuals with ID and ASD.
agy, chthonophagy, lithophagy, pagophagy, and Another limitation of current research is that
coprophagy. Some types of pica, including there are few standardized assessments available
geophagy and amylophagy, are culturally to screen and detect pica. While there are a lim-
accepted practices and as such, do not meet the ited number of assessments available, the STEP,
criteria for the diagnosis of pica. However, the ASD-CC, and BPI are well-established tools for
ingestion of a nonfood or inappropriate food the detection of pica. Given that pica is com-
items should be considered a major medical and monly comorbid with ID and ASD, all clinicians
behavioral problem because it can lead to medi- treating individuals with ID and ASD should rou-
cal complications and impair intellectual and tinely screen for pica. The existing screening
physical development. assessments for pica can be conducted relatively
A continued issue is that the prevalence of quickly; screening should not be overly
pica is difficult to ascertain because pica is com- burdensome.
monly undetected and underreported. Pica is Given the lack of empirical support for the
commonly comorbid with ID and ASD, with biological causes of inappropriate feeding behav-
prevalence of pica increasing with severity of ior, researchers have focused on developing
ID. The majority of the study participants in the function-­based treatments for pica (Piazza et al.,
existing literature included individuals with 1998). As noted, it is essential to identify the
ID. For example, Kinnell (1985) found that as variables that maintain the challenging behavior
many as 60% of individuals with ASD engaged (Iwata et al., 1982). Direct and indirect functional
in pica. However, McAdam and colleagues analyses have both strengths and weaknesses,
(2004) found that only 4 of the 44 participants and no one approach is recommended over the
included in their review had ASD, while 32 par- other (Tarbox et al., 2009). Following the assess-
ticipants had profound ID. Additional research is ment, the clinician should determine the appro-
needed to evaluate the effects on behavioral treat- priate behavioral intervention based upon the
ments of pica in individuals with ASD and other nature and function of behavior. There is no sin-
comorbid disorders. In addition, the sample size gle method of treatment that most effectively
of study participants should be increased. In a eliminates pica. Rather, an individualized, com-
review of treatments for pica, Hagopian and col- prehensive treatment plan which includes multi-
leagues (2011) found that the average number of ple elements of behavioral procedures (i.e.,
participants per study was 1.92 (range 1–4). Due antecedent modification, reinforcement, conse-
to the underreporting of pica, it may be challeng- quence) may optimize treatment outcomes.
ing for researchers to increase the number of Antecedent modification interventions for the
study participants per study. Consistent use of treatment of pica include NCR, environmental
screening tools such as the STEP, ASD-CC, and enrichment, discrimination training, and response
BPI should improve the accuracy of these preva- effort procedures. NCR and environmental
lence estimates. enrichment interventions appear to be the most
296 E. Hong and D.R. Dixon

effective in reducing pica maintained by auto- shown as ineffective. However, these extreme
matic reinforcement or social attention because procedures may be warranted given the life-­
they provide alternative sources of reinforce- threatening nature of pica.
ment. In order for NCR and environmental Overall, behavioral interventions have
enrichment to be effective, clinicians need to resulted in a 70–90% reduction in pica. In an
identify preferable items (e.g., toys, food, activi- analysis of the treatment of pica in an intensive
ties) that provide the same or similar type of rein- day-­ treatment clinical setting, Call and col-
forcement. In addition, opportunities for leagues (2015) found that only 25% of partici-
reinforcement should be maximized. As such, pants had a 100% reduction of pica. Given that
antecedent modification interventions may be just one instance of pica can lead to a serious
time and staff intensive. Discrimination training health complications, the ultimate goal of treat-
and response effort procedures should not be ment interventions should be to completely
used as the sole intervention for treating pica. eliminate pica, not just to reduce it. A number of
Rather, they should be applied as a component of study limitations may affect the variation in pica
treatment in an intervention package, in conjunc- reduction levels across studies.
tion with a reinforcement system such as DR. A general limitation to behavioral intervention
Within the category of response-contingent research was pointed out by McAdam and col-
interventions, DR, response blocking, and visual leagues (2004), who noted that only 11 of the 26
screening procedures have been found to reduce studies evaluated generalization of behavior. Of
pica. Response-contingent interventions provide the 11 studies, 10 studies reported successful
individuals with a clear consequence of behavior, generalization across behaviors (n = 2), behavior-­
and inappropriate behaviors are reduced. change agents (n = 4), settings (n = 2), and
Response-contingent interventions are successful behavior-­ change agents and settings (n = 1).
in reducing pica only if the strict schedule of Despite reports of generalization, none of the
reinforcement is implemented (i.e., during DRA) studies included measures of long-term mainte-
or each pica attempt is consistently interrupted nance or generalization that are required to meet
(i.e., during response blocking). As a result, the criteria for most evidence-based practice
response-contingent procedures are time and standards. This is not surprising given that most
staff intensive and are rarely used as the sole of the study settings were limited to institutional
intervention. settings or clinical settings (e.g., inpatient clinic
Punishment-based interventions include over- at a medical school; McAdam et al., 2004). Thus,
correction, aversive stimuli, physical and it is unclear if individuals living in the commu-
mechanical restraint, and time-out procedures. nity would demonstrate treatment outcomes as
Punishment procedures are among the oldest study participants in the existing research. Only 3
methods of treatment and have been found to of the 26 studies were conducted in a community
reduce or eliminate pica. However, punishment-­ setting (e.g., classroom). Institutional and hospi-
based interventions do not teach appropriate, tal settings typically have constant, individual-
alternative behaviors and thus should be imple- ized staff supervision and are not representative
mented in conjunction with antecedent modifica- of the environments of many individuals with
tion interventions. Following the development of pica. Thus, study settings should be expanded to
more socially acceptable interventions, community-based and/or home settings in order
punishment-­ based interventions are less fre- to increase generalization of learned behavior.
quently evaluated in research studies. While Overall, behavioral treatments are effective at
punishment-­based interventions have a long his- significantly reducing pica. Given this, it is essen-
tory of use, these methods should be carefully tial to routinely screen for pica in order to assess
considered before implementing and only be and treat pica as soon as it detected. Using reli-
used once other non-aversive methods have been able methods to detect pica and implementing
Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disabilities 297

effective function-based treatments, the serious Chisholm, J. D., & Martin, H. L. (1981). Hypozincemia,
ageusia, dysomia, and toilet tissue pica. Journal of the
medical consequences of this challenging behav-
National Medical Association, 73, 163–164.
ior should be reduced. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heyward, W. L. (2007).
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH:
Pearson.
Danford, D. E., & Huber, A. E. (1982). Pica among men-
References tally retarded adults. American Journal of Mental
Deficiency, 87, 141–146.
Ali, Z. (2001). Pica in people with intellectual disability: A Delaney, C. B., Eddy, K. T., Hartmann, A. S., Becker,
literature review of aetiology, epidemiology, and com- A. E., Murray, H. B., & Thomas, J. J. (2015). Pica
plications. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental and rumination behavior among individuals seeking
Disability, 26, 205–215. treatment for eating disorders or obesity. International
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic Journal of Eating Disorders, 48, 238–248.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Dixon, D. R., Vogel, T., & Tarbox, J. (2012). A brief his-
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. tory of functional analysis and applied behavior analy-
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic sis. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Functional assessment for
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). challenging behaviors (pp. 3–24). New York: Springer.
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. Donnelly, D. R., & Olczak, P. V. (1990). The effects of
Ashworth, M., Hirdes, J. P., & Martin, L. (2009). The differential reinforcement of incompatible behaviors
social and recreational characteristics of adults with (DRI) on pica for cigarettes in persons with intellec-
intellectual disability and pica living in institutions. tual disability. Behavior Modification, 14, 81–96.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 512–520. Edwards, C. H., Johnson, A. A., Knight, E. M., Oyemade,
Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An investigation U. J., Cole, O. J., Westney, O. E., … Westney, L. S.
of differential reinforcement of alternative behav- (1994). Pica in an urban environment. The Journal of
ior without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Nutrition, 124(6 Suppl), 954S–962S.
Analysis, 43, 569–589. Favell, J. E., McGimsey, J. F., & Schell, R. M. (1982).
Ausman, J., Ball, T. S., & Alexander, D. (1974). Behavior Treatment of self-injury by providing alternative
therapy of pica with a profoundly retarded adolescent. sensory activities. Analysis and Intervention in
Mental Retardation, 12, 16–18. Developmental Disabilities, 2, 83–104.
Baker, D. J., Valenzuela, S., & Wieseler, N. A. (2005). Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C., Bowman, L. G., Kurtz, P. F.,
Naturalistic inquiry and treatment of coprophagia Sherer, M. R., & Lachman, S. R. (1994). A prelimi-
in one individual. Journal of Developmental and nary evaluation of empirically derived consequences
Physical Disabilities, 17, 361–367. for the treatment of pica. Journal of Applied Behavior
Barrett, R. P. (2008). Atypical behaviors: Self-injury and Analysis, 26, 24–26.
pica. In M. L. Wolraich, D. D. Drotar, P. H. Dworkin, Foxx, R. M., & Martin, E. D. (1975). Treatment of scav-
& E. C. Perrin (Eds.), Developmental and behavioral enging behavior (coprophagy and pica) by overcorrec-
pediatrics: Evidence and practice (pp. 871–885). tion. Behavior Research and Therapy, 13, 153–126.
Philadelphia: Mosby Elsevier. Goh, H. L., Iwata, B. A., & Kahng, S. W. (1999).
Bell, K. E., & Stein, B. M. (1992). Behavioral treatments Multicomponent assessment and treatment of cigarette
for pica: A review of empirical studies. International pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 297–315.
Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 377–389. Gonzalez, M. L., Dixon, D. R., Rojahn, J., Esbensen,
Bogart, L. C., Piersel, W. C., & Gross, E. J. (1995). The A. J., Matson, J. L., Terlonge, C., & Smith, K. R.
long-term treatment of life-threatening pica: A case (2009). The behavior problems inventory: Reliability
study of a woman with profound mental retardation and factor validity in institutionalized adults with
living in an applied setting. Journal of Developmental intellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research
and Physical Disabilities, 7, 39–50. in Intellectual Disabilities, 22(3), 223–235.
Bugle, C., & Rubin, H. B. (1993). Effects of a nutritional Gutelius, M. F., Millican, F. K., Layman, E. M., Cohen,
supplement on coprophagia: A study of three cases. G. J., & Dublin, C. C. (1962). Nutritional studies of
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 14, 445–456. children with pica. 1. Controlled study evaluating
Burke, L., & Smith, S. L. (1999). Treatment of pica: nutritional status. Pediatrics, 29, 1012–1023.
Considering least intrusive options when work- Hagopian, L. P., Rooker, G. W., & Rolider, N. U. (2011).
ing and live in a community setting. Developmental Identifying empirically supported treatments for pica
Disabilities Bulletin, 27, 40–46. in individuals with intellectual disabilities. Research
Call, N. A., Simmons, C. A., Lomas Mevers, J. E., & in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2114–2120.
Alvarez, J. P. (2015). Clinical outcomes of behavioral Hall, S. S. (2005). Comparing descriptive, experimental,
treatments for pica in children with developmental and informant-base assessments of problem behaviors.
disabilities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 1514–1526.
Disorders, 45, 2105–2114.
298 E. Hong and D.R. Dixon

Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Matson, J. L., & Gonzalez, M. L. (2007). Autism spectrum
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. disorders – comorbidity-child version. Baton Rouge,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. LA: Disability Consultants, LLC.
Hirsch, N., & Smith-Myles, B. S. (1996). The use of a Matson, J. L., Hattier, M. A., Belva, B., & Matson,
pica box in reducing pica behavior in a student with M. L. (2013). Pica in persons with developmental
autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental disabilities: Approaches to treatment. Research in
Disabilities, 11(4), 222–225. Developmental Disabilities, 34, 2564–2571.
Huete, J., Schmidt, J., & Lopez-Arvizu. (2014). Behavioral Matson, J. L., & Kuhn, D. E. (2001). Identifying feeding prob-
disorders in young children with autism spectrum dis- lems in mentally retarded persons: Development and reli-
order. In J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, P. Sturmey, & J. L. ability of the screening tool of feeding problems (STEP).
Matson (Eds.), Handbook of early intervention for Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22, 165–172.
autism spectrum disorders: Research, policy, and Matson, J. L., LoVullo, S. V., Rivet, T. T., & Boisjoli,
practice (pp. 717–752). New York: Springer. J. A. (2009). Validity of the autism spectrum disorder-­
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., comorbid for children (ASD-CC). Research in Autism
& Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional Spectrum Disorders, 3(2), 345–357.
analysis of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Matson, J. L., & Vollmer, T. (1995). The questions about
Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20. behavioral function (QABF) user’s guide. Baton
Johnson, B. E. (1990). Pica. In H. K. Walker, W. D. Hall, Rouge, LA: Scientific Publications.
& J. W. Hurst (Eds.), Clinical methods: The his- McAdam, D. B., Briedbord, J., Levine, M., & Williams,
tory, physical, and laboratory examinations (3rd ed., D. E. (2012). Pica. In P. Sturmey & M. Hersen (Eds.),
pp. 709–710). Boston: Butterworths. Handbook of evidence-based practice in clinical psy-
Johnson, C. R., Hunt, F. M., & Siebert, M. J. (1994). chology (pp. 303–317). New York: Wiley.
Discrimination training in the treatment of pica and McAdam, D. B., Sherman, J. A., Sheldon, J. B., &
food stealing. Behavior Modification, 18, 214–229. Napolitano, D. A. (2004). Behavioral interventions
Kalfus, G. R., Fisher-Gross, S., Marvullo, M. A., & Nau, to reduce the pica of persons with developmental dis-
P. A. (1987). Outpatient treatment of pica in a devel- abilities. Behavior Modification, 28(1), 45–72.
opmentally delayed child. Child and Family Behavior McAlpine, C., & Singh, N. N. (1986). Pica in institution-
Therapy, 9, 49–63. alized mentally retarded persons. Journal of Mental
Kinnell, H. G. (1985). Pica as a feature of autism. The Deficiency Research, 30, 171–178.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 80–82. McCord, B. E., Grosser, J. W., Iwata, B. A., & Powers,
Kuhn, D. E., & Matson, J. L. (2002). A validity study L. A. (2005). An analysis of response blocking param-
of the screening tool of feeding problems (STEP). eters in the prevention of pica. Journal of Applied
Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, Behavior Analysis, 38, 391–394.
27(3), 161–167. Miao, D., Young, S. L., & Golden, C. D. (2015). A meta-­
LeBlanc, L. A., Piazza, C. C., & Krug, M. A. (1997). analysis of pica and micronutrient status. American
Comparing methods for maintaining the safety Journal of Human Biology, 27, 84–93.
of a child with pica. Research in Developmental Nchito, M., Geissler, P. W., Mubila, L., Friis, H., & Olsen,
Disabilities, 18, 215–220. A. (2004). Effects of iron and multimicro-nutrient
Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2002). On the sta- supplementation on geophagy: A two-by-two facto-
tus of knowledge for using punishment: Implications rial study among Zambian schoolchildren in Lusaka.
for treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine
Behavior Analysis, 35, 431–464. and Hygiene, 98, 218–227.
Lofts, R. H., Schroeder, S. R., & Maier, R. H. (1990). Pace, G. M., & Toyer, E. A. (2000). The effects of a vita-
Effects of serum zinc supplementation on pica behav- min supplement on the pica of a child with severe
ior of persons with mental retardation. American mental retardation. Journal of Applied Behavior
Journal of Mental Retardation, 95(1), 103–109. Analysis, 33, 619–622.
Mace, F. C., & Knight, D. (1986). Functional analysis and Paisey, T. J., & Whitney, R. B. (1989). A long-term case
treatment of severe pica. Journal of Applied Behavior study of analysis, response suppression, and treat-
Analysis, 19(4), 411–416. ment maintenance involving life-threatening pica.
Madden, N. A., Russo, D. C., & Cataldo, M. F. (1980). Behavioral Residential Treatment, 4, 191–211.
Behavior treatment of pica in children with lead poi- Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A.,
soning. Child Behavior Therapy, 2, 67–81. Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. E., & Keeney, K. M.
Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., Cherry, K. E., & Paclawskyj, (1998). Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of
T. R. (1999). A validity study on the questions about its reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior
behavioral function (QABF) scale: Predicting treat- Analysis, 31, 165–189.
ment success for self-injury, aggression, and stereo- Piazza, C. C., Hanley, G. P., & Fisher, W. W. (1996).
typies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20(2), Functional analysis and treatment of cigarette pica.
163–175. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 437–450.
Pica in Individuals with Developmental Disabilities 299

Piazza, C. C., Roane, H. S., Keeney, K. M., Boney, B. R., & incompatible behavior. Journal of Behavior Therapy
Abt, K. A. (2002). Varying response effort in the treat- and Experimental Psychology, 18(3), 285–288.
ment of pica maintained by automatic ­reinforcement. Sturmey, P. (1995). Analog baselines: A critical review
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 233–246. of the methodology. Research in Developmental
Rojahn, J., Matson, J. L., Esbensen, A. J., & Smalls, Y. Disabilities, 16, 269–284.
(2001). The behavior problems inventory: An instru- Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles,
ment for the assessment of self-injury, stereotyped R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., … Tia, B.
behavior, and aggression/destruction in individuals (2009). Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experi-
with developmental disabilities. Journal of Autism and mental functional assessments of challenging behavior
Developmental Disorders, 31(6), 577–588. in children with autism. Journal of Developmental and
Rojahn, J., McGonigle, C., Curcio, C., & Dixon, M. J. Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514.
(1987). Suppression of pica by water mist and aro- Williams, D. E., & McAdam, D. (2012). Assessment,
matic ammonia. Behavior Modification, 11, 65–74. behavioral treatment, and prevention of pica: Clinical
Rose, E. A., Porcelli, J. H., & Neale, A. V. (2000). Pica: guidelines and recommendations for practitio-
Common, but commonly missed. Journal of the ners. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33,
American Board of Family Practice, 13(5), 353–358. 2050–2057.
Sayetta, R. B. (1986). Pica: An overview. American Williams, D. E., & McAdam, D. (2016). Pica. In N. N.
Family Physician, 33, 181–185. Singh (Ed.), Handbook of evidence based practices in
Singh, N. N., & Bakker, L. W. (1984). Suppression of pica intellectual and developmental disabilities (715–726).
by overcorrection and physical restraint: A compara- Switzerland, Cham: Springer.
tive analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Young, S. L., Khalfan, S. S., Farag, T. H., Kavle, J. A., Ali,
Disorders, 14, 40–45. S. M., Hajji, H., … Stoltzfus, R. J. (2010). Association
Singh, N. N., & Winston, A. S. (1984). Effects of screening of pica with anemia and gastrointestinal distress
procedures on pica and collateral behaviors. Journal among pregnant women in Zanzibar, Tanzania.
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 15, American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene,
59–65. 83(1), 144–151.
Smith, M. D. (1987). Treatment of pica in an adult
­disabled by autism by differential reinforcement of
Social Competence: Consideration
of Behavioral, Cognitive,
and Emotional Factors

Karen Milligan, Annabel Sibalis, Ashley Morgan,


and Marjory Phillips

Contents l­ong-­term positive outcomes (Rose & Asher,


Social Competence.................................................. 301 2017). For example, a positive relationship has
been found between social competence and aca-
Social Competence Interventions.......................... 308
demic achievement in school-age children (Del
I ntegra Social ACES Program: A Social Prette, Del Prette, de Oliviera, Gresham, &
Competence Intervention for LD........................... 311
Vance, 2012; Elias & Haynes, 2008; Shek &
Conclusion............................................................... 316 Leung, 2016). Del Prette et al. (2012) suggested
References................................................................ 316 that this relation may reflect a stronger sense of
belonging in students who are more academically
engaged and motivated, and positive relation-
ships with peers may promote better problem-
Social Competence solving and peer collaboration which may
positively influence academic outcomes. Along
Social competence is considered an important the same line of reasoning, higher levels of social
resilience factor that increases positive develop- competence have also been associated with better
mental outcomes, even in the face of risk (Reich, career success in the long term (Amdurer,
2016). Friendships are thought to enhance knowl- Boyatzis, Saatcioglu, Smith, & Taylor, 2014).
edge about social situations, as well as provide Social competence is also identified as a pro-
emotional support, instrumental aid, affection, tective factor for good mental health (Alduncin,
self-validation, companionship, and opportuni- Huffman, Feldman, & Loe, 2014). It helps us to
ties to learn conflict resolution skills in a support- develop strong social supports and to work effec-
ive environment (Rose-Krasner, 1997). tively with others. More and more, we live in a
The ability to form and maintain friendships complex and connected world, and the ways in
and social relationships is associated with which we connect are increasingly fast paced and
fragmented. The challenges of social media, liv-
K. Milligan (*) • A. Sibalis ing away from extended relatives and familiar
Child Self-Regulation Lab, Ryerson University,
communities, having to form new social sup-
Toronto, ON, Canada
e-mail: Karen.milligan@psych.ryerson.ca ports, and having to work with groups of people,
all add to the need for high levels of social
A. Morgan • M. Phillips
Child Development Institute, Integra Program, competence. Social competence mitigates the
­
Toronto, ON, Canada impact of adverse events, such as maltreatment

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 301


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_18
302 K. Milligan et al.

(Schultz et al., 2009). Conversely, low social ing out social skills. Rather, the emphasis is on
competence is associated with negative out- the performance of complex and interconnected
comes, including school failure and dropout, skills within interpersonal environments (Lillvist,
alcohol and substance use, social rejection, and Sandberg, Bjorck-Akesson, & Granlund, 2009).
delinquency (Parker & Asher, 1987). Social com- Attempts have been made to disentangle and
petence deficits are associated with lower social identify the complex and interconnected set of
supports and higher risk factor for physical dis- skills that enables us to navigate social interac-
ease (Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, 2002). tions and initiate and maintain relationships with
others (Stichter, O’Connor, Herzog, Lierheimer,
& McGhee, 2012). These skills are thought to
What Is Social Competence? include communication (making eye contact,
taking turns, appropriate tone of voice), coopera-
While there is agreement about the importance tion (helping others, sharing materials, following
of social competence, what constitutes social directions), assertion (requesting information
competence is less clear, with an abundant array from others, introducing oneself, responding to
of operational definitions used in the extant lit- the actions of others), empathy (showing concern
erature (Rantanen, Eriksson & Neimenen, 2012; for another, taking the perspective of another),
Rose-Krasner, 1997). For example, in her review engagement (joining ongoing activities, making
of the use of term social competence, Rose-­ friends, interacting with others), and self-control
Krasner (1997) concluded that the key emphasis (taking turns, compromising, responding appro-
is on positive social outcomes and effectiveness. priately to conflict; Lyons, Huber, Carter, Chen,
Social competence is defined as “the ability to & Asmus, 2016). To be considered socially com-
achieve personal goals in social interaction petent, one needs to use social skills in a way that
while simultaneously maintaining positive rela- adheres to social conventions and that responds
tionships over time and across situations” appropriately to others’ emotions and thoughts
(Rubin & Rose-Krasner, 1992, p. 4). Arthur, (Stichter et al., 2012).
Bochner, and Butterfield (1999), in contrast, Each of these social skills can be seen at a
took a developmental approach to the construct behavioral level. Social interactions, however,
and defined social competence as reflecting the are complex and rarely is one enacting a single
evolving understanding of self and others and social behavior in isolation. One must attend to
the ability to form meaningful relationships and process context cues, as well as verbal and
with peers. Gresham (2001) defined social com- nonverbal cues from social partners. This infor-
petence as the degree to which children and mation must then be integrated and compared
youth are able to establish and maintain satis- with previous experiences and knowledge. The
factory interpersonal relationships, gain peer child must decide what information is key to
acceptance, make friendships, and terminate responding, make a plan, draw on their verbal
negative or pernicious interpersonal relation- and behavioral skill repertoire, and implement.
ships. The importance of perceiving and This complex cognitive and behavioral process is
responding appropriately to the emotional com- further complicated in the context of strong emo-
ponents of social interactions was highlighted tion (e.g., fear, anger, excitement), which is often
by Halberstadt, Denham, and Dunsmore (2001) present in human interactions. Emotions can
in their understanding of social competence. hijack cognitive processes, making it harder to
More recently, the ability to regulate emotions is perspective-take, problem-solve, and behave in a
considered to be an important component of manner that takes into account all the complex
social competence (Blair & Raver, 2015). cues of social situations (Zelazo & Lyons, 2012).
Examining all of these definitions suggests As such, there has been movement within
that there is general agreement that social compe- the social competence field away from a social
tence reflects more than just learning and carry- skills perspective that focuses on behavior to
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 303

Fig. 1 Behavioral,
cognitive, and emotional
factors interact to Behavioral
support development Social skills such as
and enactment of social taking turns, making
competence eye contact, sustaining
a conversation,
negotiating conflict

Cognitive
Knowledge of social Emotional
situations, perspective- Emotion
taking, attributions, understanding and
neuropsychological regulation
cognitive processes

an integrative perspective that accounts for the  ognition: Thought Processes Related
C
complex interaction of cognitive and emotional to Social Competence
processes that support social competence at a
behavioral level (Beauchamp & Anderson, Schemas reflect knowledge about the rules/
2010; Milligan, Phillips, & Morgan, 2016). expectations of social situations. They may be
See Fig. 1. developed based on a child or adolescent’s per-
sonal experience or the observation or experi-
ences of others and essentially help children (and
 ognitive Factors and Social
C adults) in predicting what will occur in a given
Competence social situation so that they don’t have to experi-
ence a situation as novel every time it is encoun-
Cognitive factors involved in social competence tered. An example of this would include a child
can be viewed from two interrelated perspec- knowing broadly what is expected when they
tives. At one level, cognition reflects thoughts, play a board game (e.g., sit down, take turns, fol-
including one’s knowledge of social situations low rules, etc.).
(e.g., what is expected in terms of behavior, Related to schemas are attributions, cognitive
content, and different roles) and one’s interpre- processes that reflect a child’s perception of the
tation of situations (e.g., perspective-taking and cause or intent of another’s behavior (Weiner,
attributions about the cause of events or behav- 1985). Most of the research on attributions and
iors). At a more basic level, cognition also social behavior has focused on hostile attribu-
embodies neuropsychological cognitive abili- tions (i.e., the tendency to attribute negative
ties, including but not limited to attention, exec- intent in a benign situation). Hostile attributions
utive functions, processing speed, and are explained within the context of the social
visual-spatial processing. Importantly, these information processing model (SIP; Crick &
two levels of cognition are not independent, but Dodge, 1994). SIP breaks social problem-solv-
rather, they interact in a transactional manner to ing down into a series of steps which include
support social competence (Crick & Dodge, interpreting cues, clarifying goals, generating
1994; Dodge, 1986; Galway & Metsala, 2011; alternative responses, selecting and implement-
Gifford-Smith & Rabiner, 2004; Lemerise & ing a specific response, and evaluating the out-
Arsenio, 2000). come. These steps are executed rapidly and the
304 K. Milligan et al.

process is far from linear, with steps overlapping likely to make joint proposals and to assign roles
and numerous feedback loops as information is for themselves and their playmates when engaged
processed within the child’s knowledge and in pretend play. These results suggest that chil-
beliefs about social situations (e.g., Crick & dren with superior perspective-taking skills are
Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1986; Gifford-Smith & more aware that their partners’ beliefs about the
Rabiner, 2004; Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). pretend situation may differ from their own and
Research has consistently shown that children accordingly make their beliefs and intentions
who are socially rejected and/or engage in more explicit to their partner. Understanding of
heightened levels of aggression are more likely others’ beliefs has also been related to connect-
to attribute hostile or negative intent in benign edness of communications between friends
social situations. Further, they are more likely to (Slomkowski & Dunn, 1996) and successful
experience challenge in understanding and/or communication bids and cooperative play (Dunn
performing the SIP steps (Dodge & Coie, 1987; & Cutting, 1999).
Dodge & Feldman, 1990; Dodge, Murphy, & The studies reviewed examine the relation
Buchsbaum, 1984; Dodge & Newman, 1981; between ToM and social competence in the pre-
Dodge & Tomlin, 1987). school period. Most typically developing chil-
Theory of mind (ToM) is another area of cog- dren have developed false-belief understanding
nitive understanding that has been linked with by middle childhood, and as such there is less
social competence. ToM reflects the ability to variation found in social competence by false-­
infer beliefs, thoughts, and desires (i.e., mental belief understanding. However, it is possible that
states) to another person and to be able to see that individual differences in the flexible and appro-
these may differ from one’s own mental states priate implementation of ToM may play a role in
(Milligan, Astington, & Dack, 2007). The rela- social competence during this period as well.
tion between ToM and social competence is well While less commonly examined, advanced ToM
established (for a review, see Astington, 2003). tasks that are passed later in childhood (e.g.,
The false-belief task is the gold standard task for Little & Nettle, 2006) have been associated with
assessing ToM in the preschool period. This task, social competence outcomes, including the num-
passed by most children by age 5, assesses a ber of friends in a child’s social network (Stiller
child’s ability to reason about the behavioral con- & Dunbar, 2007) and teacher-rated social compe-
sequences of holding a mistaken belief. Thus, by tence (e.g., Little & Nettle, 2006). This suggests
age 5 most children can act in a way that a continued role of ToM for school-age social
­acknowledges that mental representations impact competence. This is an area in need of further
on what a person says or does, even in cases exploration, particularly with neurodevelopmen-
where they are mistaken about the situation in tal samples who may present with more chal-
reality (Milligan et al., 2007). To become socially lenges with ToM.
skilled, children must understand that desires and
beliefs held by peers influence their behavioral
and emotional responses (Slomkowski & Dunn, Neuropsychological Processing
1996). This knowledge assists children in under- Abilities
standing the social behavior and verbal commu-
nications of their peers and guides their behavior Disorders associated with neuropsychological or
in social interactions, thereby enabling them to cognitive-executive weaknesses, such as schizo-
regulate and coordinate their interactions phrenia, specific and nonverbal learning disabili-
(Astington & Gopnik, 1991; Baron-Cohen, 1994; ties (LD, Galway & Metsala, 2011; Milligan
Lalonde & Chandler, 1995). One of the first stud- et al., 2016), autism spectrum disorders (Gates,
ies to examine this relation, completed by Kang, & Lerner, 2017), and traumatic brain
Astington and Jenkins (1995), found that chil- injury (Tlustos et al., 2016), have been associated
dren who passed false-belief tasks were more with greater social competence challenge. Within
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 305

these disorders, research has highlighted the key social competence are seen in both those with
role of processing deficits in social competence ADHD-inattentive and ADHD-combined (inat-
challenge. While an exhaustive review of all neu- tentive and hyperactive-impulsive) subtypes, sug-
ropsychological cognitive processes involved in gesting that the variance in social competence is
social competence is beyond the scope of this likely related to inattention and not solely due to
chapter, we will explore the impact of attention challenges with hyperactivity/impulsivity. In fact,
control and executive functions, processing research suggests that children with ADHD-­
speed, and visual-spatial processing to exemplify inattentive subtype are more likely to show defi-
the impact of processing on social competence. cits in the performance of socially competent
behavior (similar to combined type) but experi-
ence even more challenge in the acquisition of
Attentional Control social skills, possibly due to the critical role atten-
tion plays in learning (Wheeler & Carlson, 1994).
Attentional control reflects the ability to orient
and sustain attention while filtering out irrelevant
stimuli (Derakshan & Eysenck, 2009). Challenges Executive Functions
with attention have been associated with behav-
ioral challenges in social interactions (Andrade, Closely associated with attentional control are
Brodeur, Waschbusch, Stewart, & McGee, 2009). executive functions (EF), which are the higher-­
Challenges with attentional control can impact order cognitive processes that support purposeful
on learning social skills and developing one’s and effortful goal-directed behaviors (Pennington
knowledge of social situations and situational & Ozonoff, 1996). The model of EF proposed by
norms. Attentional deficits are also intricately Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, and
involved in the relation between attributions and Howerter (2000) suggests that the ability to con-
social competence. For example, research sug- trol impulses, respond flexibly (or adjust one’s
gests that children who exhibit aggressive behav- approach, behavior, attention, or thinking based
ior demonstrate biased attention toward threat on feedback from the environment), and keep
cues. For example, aggressive children have dif- information in mind while working with that
ficulty attending to and remembering all impor- information (i.e., working memory) are the pri-
tant aspects of a social interaction and encode mary processes within the broad EF construct.
fewer social cues (with preference for those that These EF assist with problem-solving in every-
may be most recent) before making causal attri- day life and, as such, are considered pivotal to
butions about the hostile intent of another person successful social interaction. Children and ado-
(Milich & Dodge, 1984). lescents with weaknesses in EF experience chal-
Research examining social competence in lenge with knowing what social information to
ADHD populations (where deficits in attentional focus on, developing plans for social interactions,
control are considered central) exemplifies the executing their plans, controlling their behavior
role of attention in social competence. Children in keeping with the social/situational demands,
and adolescents with ADHD experience higher monitoring the success of their behavior, and
rates of rejection, fewer friends, and lower levels flexibly shifting their behavioral approach based
of social support compared to non-ADHD peers on feedback from peers and the broader environ-
(Humphreys, Galán, Tottenham, & Lee, 2016). ment (Clark, Prior, & Kinsella, 2002; Nigg,
In addition to improving indices of attentional Quamma, Greenberg, & Kusche, 1999; Dennis,
control, stimulant medication has been associated Brotman, Huang, & Gouley, 2007; Riggs,
with improvements in social functioning at home Greenberg, Kusché, & Pentz, 2006).
and school, with notable medium to large effect Similar to attention, EF is related to thought
sizes (van der Oord, Prins, Oosterlaan, & processes, such as ToM and hostile attributions.
Emmelkamp, 2008). Importantly, challenges with There is a small to moderate association between
306 K. Milligan et al.

ToM and EF (d = 0.38, Devine & Hughes, 2014). processing may ultimately result in children hav-
It is possible that EF enables children to attend to ing to narrow their field of perception in order to
and reflect upon the mental states of others, successfully process information, resulting in
thereby improving social competence. information loss and heightened possibility of
Within typically developing samples, the social errors.
association between EF and social competence Certainly, research with clinical populations
appears to decrease as children age (small effect with marked processing speed challenges high-
size, Devine, White, Ensor, & Hughes, 2016). lights the relation between processing speed and
For example, Harms, Zayas, Meltzoff, and social competence (e.g., schizophrenia; Bowie
Carlson (2014) found that EF at 8 and 12 years et al., 2008; traumatic brain injury, Rassovsky
was not significantly related to social compe- et al., 2006). Backenson et al. (2015) have high-
tence as rated by teachers at age 12. However, lighted that learning disabilities marked by sig-
within populations of children with significant nificant processing speed challenges have a
EF deficits (e.g., traumatic brain injury, disrup- greater impact on adaptive functioning (includ-
tive behavior disorder), the association appears to ing social) than learning disabilities associated
be maintained across developmental periods. For with working memory or executive functions.
example, in adolescents who have experienced a Similarly, adolescents with ADHD marked by
traumatic brain injury, parent ratings of EF were sluggish cognitive tempo, which reflects symp-
significantly negatively associated with social toms such as drowsiness, daydreaming, lethargy,
competence (Tlustos et al., 2016). Further, rat- and slowed processing speed (e.g., Barkley,
ings of EF were found to moderate the impact of 2011; Becker & Langberg, 2014), also have been
a social competence intervention, suggesting that shown to have lower levels of social competence
EF may be a resilience factor that supports learn- challenge than those without these symptoms
ing and performance of socially competent (Becker & Langberg, 2014). Research suggests
behaviors (Tlustos et al. 2016). the sluggish cognitive tempo accounts for chal-
EF deficits also appear to moderate the impact lenges in initiation and working memory (EF),
of hostile attributions. For example, in their study and this may be one pathway by which process-
of 83 boys, Ellis, Weiss, and Lochman (2009) ing speed influences social competence.
found that boys who presented with both hostile
attributions and EF challenges in planning and
inhibition exhibited higher rates of reactive Visual-Spatial Processing
aggression but that EF challenges alone did not
lead to increased rates of reactive aggression. Children and adolescents with visual-spatial
This underscores the importance of examining processing deficits may also be more likely to
the interaction of thought processes and neuro- experience challenges with social competence
psychological cognitive factors on social compe- (Galway & Metsala, 2011; Petti, Voelker,
tence, rather than each in isolation. Shore, & Hayman-Abello, 2003). Effective
social interactions depend upon the ability to
attend to and rapidly process and integrate
Processing Speed multiple, often subtle, nonverbal social cues,
as well as determine their relative salience.
The speed at which children and adolescents pro- This information assists individuals in under-
cess visual and verbal information also has standing emotional states and intentions of
important implications for social competence others (Nowicki & Duke, 1992). Research
(Anderson, 2008). If it takes a child longer to take examining children with nonverbal learning
in, process, and respond in a social context, this disabilities (NLD) who present with core
may impact on their ability to follow conversa- weaknesses in visual-spatial processing has
tions, to formulate responses, and to deliver highlighted that, in comparison to a typically
responses in a timely manner. Further, slowed developing control group, children with NLD
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 307

encode fewer social cues and have more diffi- Emotion understanding is a broad multidimen-
culty detecting and inferring emotion based on sional construct that reflects emotion recognition
nonverbal social cues. As such, it is possible and emotional knowledge (i.e., the ability to attri-
that children and adolescents with visual pro- bute emotions to oneself and others based on
cessing challenges may become overwhelmed knowledge about emotion-eliciting situations), as
by the amount/type of social information to well as the integration across the skill areas
encode, leading to a narrowed focus that may (Castro, Halberstadt, & Garrett-Peters, 2016).
distort understanding of a social situation. This Emotion understanding develops across childhood
may result in challenges in understanding the with emotion recognition skills developing in the
emotional aspects of a situation that require preschool years and emotion knowledge develop-
more inference and integration of information. ing in the school-age years. More complex emo-
Research suggests that children with NLD are tion understanding (e.g., mixed emotions) also
able to generate competent/assertive responses develops during the school-age years as develop-
to social challenges at levels that are commen- ing cognitive abilities facilitate the ability to ana-
surate with typically developing peers; how- lyze, interpret, and integrate emotional information
ever, they are less likely to believe that (see Castro et al., 2016 for review). A well-estab-
enactment of these responses will lead to posi- lished base of research support exists for the rela-
tive outcomes (Galway & Metsala, 2011). It is tion between emotion understanding and social
possible that the generation responses, while competence (e.g., Heinze, Miller, Seifer, Dickstein,
potentially accurate or competent, may be & Locke, 2015; Miller et al., 2005; Ornaghi,
associated with a sense of overload or anxiety. Grazzani, Cherubin, Conte, & Piralli, 2015). For
This, in turn, may impact on performance of example, Castro et al. (2016) found that emotional
the response and/or attributions of success. knowledge about the experience of emotion across
Further research is needed into what specific situations supported positive social competence
aspects of visual-spatial processing (vs. a outcomes in grade 3 students.
broad diagnosis such as NLD) impact on the While understanding emotions in self and oth-
different components of the social interaction ers provides essential information for social
process and how these challenges combine problem-solving, enacting behaviors and thought
with other neuropsychological cognitive pro- processes associated with social competence is
cesses to impact social behavior. dependent, in part, on emotion regulation.
Emotion regulation is defined as the “extrinsic
and intrinsic processes responsible for monitor-
Emotion Regulation ing, evaluating, and modifying emotional reac-
tions, especially in their intensive and temporal
Social competence is not just a cognitive and features, to accomplish one’s goals” (Thompson,
behavioral process. Social interactions are emo- 1994, pp. 27–28). Emotion regulation is associ-
tional by nature, and emotion has the potential to ated with both cognitive processes related to
impact on learning social skills, perspective-­ attributions and perspective-taking and neuro-
taking and problem-solving, and performance of psychological cognitive factors.
behaviors. Children who are better able to regu- Emotion regulation is a significant contributor
late their emotions are more likely to experience to effective social information processing. In a
positive social outcomes, including positive study of 100 grade 4–6 boys, Bauminger and
engagement with peers, greater acceptance by Kimhi-Kind (2008) found that children with LD
peers, and a higher quality of friendships experienced significant challenge with social
(Eisenberg et al. 2000). As such, a comprehen- information processing, including hostile attribu-
sive understanding of social competence must tions. Moreover, emotion regulation was found to
include factors relating to emotion understanding moderate the strength of this relation, with those
and emotion regulation, given the central role of with emotion regulation challenges experiencing
emotion in social interaction. more social information processing deficits.
308 K. Milligan et al.

Emotional regulation is also significantly asso-  ocial Competence Interventions


S
ciated with neuropsychological cognitive processes for Typically Developing Populations
(e.g., attention, language, flexibility, processing
speed, inhibition; see Diamond, 2013 for review). Numerous universal social competence interven-
Certainly, children and adolescents with neurode- tions have been developed for children and ado-
velopmental disorders, such as LDs, are at increased lescents without specific cognitive, emotional, or
risk for emotion regulation deficits (Bauminger & behavioral challenges. The content and delivery
Kimhi-Kind, 2008) and associated co-occurring characteristic of programs in the extant literature
mental disorders (Milligan, Badali, & Spiroiu, appear to be moderated by age. Social compe-
2015). From a neurobiological perspective, the tence interventions designed for children ages 10
presence of a strong emotional response limits a and under (including those for preschool-age
child’s ability to fully engage their cognitive pro- and kindergarten-age children) nearly exclu-
cesses and behavioral skills (e.g., impulse control, sively focus on the content at a behavioral level,
cognitive flexibility, social knowledge, perspective- highlighting simple, physical social actions such
taking abilities, social skills; Zelazo & Lyons, as sharing toys, initiating conversations, listen-
2012). For children and adolescents who already ing quietly when others speak, and promoting
present with challenges in these areas, emotion helping behavior (e.g., Battistich, Solomon,
may serve to magnify these challenges. As such, Watson, Solomon, & Schaps, 1989; Boyle et al.,
many children and adolescents may cope with 1999; Ialongo, Poduska, Werthamer, & Kellam,
social challenge by engaging in fight (e.g., aggres- 2001; Stanton-Chapman, Walker, & Jamison,
sion) or flight (e.g., avoidance) behaviors to regu- 2014; Stevahn, Johnson, Johnson, Oberle, &
late strong emotions (Milligan et al., 2015). Further Wahl, 2000). As children age, the content of
research is needed to better understand the interac- social competence programs mirrors the
tion between cognitive, behavioral, and emotional advances they are making in terms of cognitive
factors and the manner in which they impact on the and emotional processes (Beelmann, Pfingsten,
trajectory of the social interaction process. & Loesel, 1994), as well as the growing com-
plexity and prominent importance of social
interactions (Brown & Larson, 2009). More spe-
Social Competence Interventions cifically, interventions designed for youth in
grades 5 and higher begin to incorporate emo-
Given that social competence develops and is fur- tional facets of social competence. The focus
ther refined over the course of childhood and appears to shift from behavioral aspects of social
adolescence, and its well-documented contribu- skills to understanding the feelings experienced
tion to resilience, a number of universal social by oneself as well as others. In fact, the majority
competence programs have been developed, of interventions targeting middle- and high
schools being the primary setting in which these school-age youth in the extant literature contain
interventions have been implemented and some component reflecting socio-emotional
evaluated. understanding and self-presentation, for exam-
In this next section of the chapter, we take a ple, emotion regulation (stress management;
critical look at the extent to which the social calming down when frustrated; expressing anger
competence programs in the extant literature tai- appropriately), communicating feelings and
lor their content or delivery of the program to desires to others, social assertiveness and resist-
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional processes. ing peer pressure, and empathy and perspective-
First, we examine interventions for typically taking (e.g., Caplan et al., 1992; Holsen, Smith,
developing children and adolescents, followed by & Frey, 2008; Kimber, Sandell, & Bremberg,
an examination of programs for clinical popula- 2008; O’Hearn & Gatz, 1999; Sarason &
tions with specific challenges in behavioral, cog- Sarason, 1981; Taylor, Liang, Tracy, Williams,
nitive, and emotional processes. & Seigle, 2002).
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 309

One example of a social competence program recent studies replicate these results. Training in
for typically developing children is the Second social competence has led to more positive social
Step program (Committee for Children, 1997). interactions with peers, as rated by children and
An in-class, manualized program presented by their teachers, as well as improvements in self-
classroom teachers, the program is adjustable for esteem, decreases in internalizing and external-
three different age groups: early learning (pre- izing problems, and, for younger intervention
school), elementary (kindergarten to grade 5), and participants, increases in social initiations and
middle school (grades 6–8). Depending on par- cooperative play (Holsen et al., 2008; Kimber
ticipant age, the program is 22–28 weeks in et al., 2008; Stanton-Chapman et al., 2014).
length, with 20–40-min lessons. Across all age While research supports the benefit of
groups, participants are presented with four core social competence programs, effect sizes are
units: skills for learning (listening, focusing atten- small. While social skills are a focus across
tion, self-talk, being assertive), empathy (identify- childhood and adolescence, and emotion under-
ing feelings, showing care and compassion, standing and regulation, as well as cognitive-
helping others), emotion management (managing perspective,appear to be more of a focus after
anxiety, disappointment, and anger), and prob- age 10, there is little emphasis on neuropsycho-
lem-solving (playing fairly, thinking of solutions, logical cognitive processes. These processes
taking responsibility). Specific content is adjusted are important to consider given their role in
for age and level of cognitive ability of partici- learning (Milligan et al., 2015). In particular,
pants, with attention to what would be develop- executive functions are still under development
mentally appropriate or salient at a given age. For throughout the childhood and adolescent peri-
example, in the emotion management unit, pre- ods, and social competence groups that tailor
schoolers discuss managing waiting, while grade content and delivery to the specific level of EF
5 students address avoiding making assumptions. within a class may be more successful in
Additional units addressing bullying prevention, enhancing social competence. It is also impor-
substance abuse prevention, and goal setting are tant to recognize that universal programs may
added to the program beginning in grade 6. be associated with smaller effect sizes because
There is support for the broad benefit of social many of the students may already possess
competence interventions for typically develop- appropriate levels of competence, leaving little
ing youth, across age groups. In a meta-analysis room for improvement on outcome measures.
of 213 studies examining social competence Regardless, it may be beneficial to explore if
interventions for typically developing children pre-intervention EF (e.g., working memory,
completed between 1955 and 2007, Durlak, impulse control, flexibility) moderates the
Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, and Schellinger impact of social competence programs. If dif-
(2011) found that participation in interventions ferences do exist, future research that informs
led to moderate improvement in social and emo- tailoring of social competence program curric-
tional skills (d = 0.57), as rated by participants ulum to support the development of these exec-
themselves, their parents, or their teachers, as utive functions or accommodate for weaknesses
well as small improvements in self-esteem and in executive functions may improve the strength
self-efficacy (d = 0.23) and a small improvements of the observed effect.
in the level of positive social behaviors such as
cooperation with peers (d = 0.24; Durlak et al.,
2011). In addition, participants showed a small  ocial Competence Interventions
S
reduction in conduct problems (d = 0.22) and for Neurodevelopmental Disorders
reduced emotional distress (d = 0.24; Durlak
et al. 2011). Intervention participation was also The most common neurodevelopmental disor-
predictive of small improvements in academic ders for which social competence interventions
performance (d = 0.27; Durlak et al. 2011). More have been developed are autism spectrum
310 K. Milligan et al.

d­isorder (ASD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity integrated. Similar to other social competence


disorder (ADHD), and learning disability (LD). programs, program delivery capitalizes on mul-
Review of this literature suggests that both con- tiple instruction methods, including didactic
tent and program delivery attend more explicitly instruction, discussion, modeling, and peer-based
to behavioral, cognitive, and emotional factors practice. However, it is adapted to the neuropsy-
associated with social competence, with the chological processing profiles of children with
specific focus depending on the central deficits ASD, with particular attention to challenges with
associated with each disorder. For example, the executive function of cognitive flexibility. For
social deficits are central to the diagnosis of example, (1) all sessions follow a strictly consis-
ASD. Children with ASD have difficulties expe- tent outline in order to accommodate the need for
riencing and displaying empathy and engaging inflexible routines; (2) acknowledge and antici-
in reciprocal social interactions (APA, 2013). pate transitional difficulty when changing from
Often, these difficulties lead to a lack of behav- one activity to the next, setting aside time between
iors necessary to build and maintain social inter- tasks and providing transitional support; and (3)
actions, such as maintaining eye contact, predictability is increased by pre-teaching activi-
displaying engaged or welcoming body lan- ties and breaking them down into simpler steps.
guage, responding to direct or indirect social A similar program, Social Competence
advances, and engaging in cooperative play and Intervention (SCI; Stichter et al., 2010), was
activities. Additionally, children with ASD can developed for children and adolescents with ASD
display restrictive and repetitive behaviors and and includes three separate curricula specified for
interests – for example, repetitive motor move- children (ages 6–10), adolescents (ages 11–14),
ments such as hand flapping or obsession with and high school students (ages 14–18). All pro-
cars – and tend to be inflexible regarding grams are 10 weeks (1 h/week) in length and
changes to established routines (APA, 2013). school-based. The adolescent curriculum focuses
Such behaviors or obsessive interests can be on recognizing facial expressions, communica-
seen as confusing, frightening, or off-putting by tion skills such as eye contact, and nonverbal
peers (Swaim & Morgan, 2001). Further, comor- cues, turn-taking in conversation, recognizing
bid social anxiety is highly prevalent, affecting emotions in oneself and others, stress and anxiety
up to 84% of children with ASD (White et al., management, and problem-solving. As in SCEP,
2009) and further impairing children from SCI is adapted to suit the needs of ASD partici-
engaging in social situations. pants, using small group sizes (maximum six par-
Review of the social competence programs ticipants/group) to avoid overstimulation and
designed for ASD suggests that both the content minimize social anxiety and adhering to a strictly
and the method of delivery take into account structured lesson format that always begins with
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional processes the practice of acknowledging, greeting, and
involved in learning and performing socially making eye contact with all participants. A study
competent behavior. One exemplar intervention of 27 SCI participants showed that participation
is the Social Competence Enhancement Program was associated with improvements in parent-­
(SCEP; Cotugno, 2009) for elementary school-­ rated social skills and executive functioning and
age children with ASD. This 30-week (1 h/week) improved performance on measures of facial
program focuses on eye contact and gaze sharing expression recognition and theory of mind
with others (behavioral), social initiations and (Stichter et al. 2010).
social responding (behavioral), joint attention ADHD is associated with a different profile of
with others (cognitive-attention), and flexibility neuropsychological cognitive challenge. Children
and transitioning between thoughts and activities often have difficulty remaining focused on the
(cognitive-executive functioning). Anxiety and task at hand or understanding and sticking with
stress management strategies, such as visualiza- difficult tasks or problems (e.g., playing a com-
tion and breathing (emotion regulation), are also plex game, engaging in school group projects),
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 311

which often impairs cooperative work and play disorder (Cragar & Horvath, 2003; Rao, Beidel, &
with peers (Wehmeier, Schacht, & Barkley, Murray, 2008), this appears to be inconsistently put
2010). Due to distractibility and/or hyperactivity, into practice. While certain interventions may
children with ADHD often have difficulty wait- address the global deficits of the population they
ing their turn in conversation or acknowledging a seek to serve (e.g., eye contact in children with
peer’s thoughts and ideas, which can hinder con- ASD), most interventions do not take into account
versations or budding friendships (Wehmeier the specific needs of the subgroup attending the
et al., 2010). Finally, children with ADHD may intervention, and how this subgroup’s abilities and
be prone to outbursts of frustration (APA, 2013), deficits may vary slightly from the disorder as a
which may alienate peers. whole (Cragar & Horvath, 2003; Rao et al., 2008).
Similar to ASD, the social competence inter- Researchers propose that by tailoring interventions
ventions for ADHD for children 6–12 tailor the more specifically to the participants attending
content and the delivery of the program to the them, interventions may have a greater positive
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional processes of impact (Attwood, 2000; Rao et al. 2008).
social competence. For example, the Therapeutic The Integra Social ACES (Awareness,
Summer Day Camp for Children with ADHD Competence, Engagement, Skills) program is a
(Hantson et al., 2012) is a 2-week social skills program that aims to advance social competence
training program offered in the milieu of a sum- in learning-disordered children by introducing
mer day camp that aims to increase understand- social abilities in light of differing ability levels
ing and labeling feelings, emotional self-control, of participants and deficits in underlying cogni-
and positive approaches to deal with anger and tive abilities. The program will be outlined here
frustration (e.g., response to teasing and avoid- as a model for social competence programming
ance of verbal and physical confrontations). that successfully integrates emotional, cognitive,
Specific skills addressed include introducing and processing facets of social competence.
oneself (behavioral), joining social situations
(behavioral), anger management (emotional),
and using self-control (emotional/cognitive-­ Integra Social ACES Program:
executive functioning). Program delivery is tai- A Social Competence Intervention
lored to provide a mix of active and calm activities for LD
in order to keep children engaged and introduce
and practice skills across domains. Concurrent While we often think of LDs in the context of
parent training is provided to support generaliza- academic achievement, challenges experienced
tion to home (e.g., effective praise and rewards, by children and adolescents extend beyond the
providing a structured day schedule, building a classroom, with approximately 75% of students
positive parent-child relationship). Participation with LDs having lower levels of social compe-
in the program has been shown to be associated tence than typically developing children, as
with parent-rated improvements in peer relations, assessed by teachers, peers, and children them-
as well as behavioral and emotional problems selves (Forness & Kavale, 1996). Further,
(Hantson et al. 2012). approximately 50% of children with LDs are
Meta-analyses examining the impact of social rejected, neglected, or victimized by peers
competence interventions for neurodevelopmental (Baumeister, Storch, & Geffken, 2008; Mishna,
disorders suggest that the strength of the observed 2003), and many have impoverished and unstable
effect is small (d = 0.199, Quinn, Kavale, Mathur, friendships (Wiener & Schneider, 2002; Wiener
Rutherford, & Forness, 1999; PND = 69%, low or & Sunohara, 1998).
questionable effectiveness, Bellini, Peters, Benner, While there is considerable current debate
& Hopf, 2007). Despite multiple researchers not- how LDs should be defined, we will use the con-
ing the need for intervention programs to specifi- sensus definition of the LDAO (2001), which
cally cater to the neuropsychological deficits of a defines LDs as a disorder that (1) affects how
312 K. Milligan et al.

individuals acquire, understand, retain, or orga- child’s learning profile, self-regulation, and emo-
nize information, (2) results in specific rather tion regulation skills, in addition to their baseline
than global deficits in individuals with average to level of social competence, are taken into account.
above average intelligence, and (3) results from Children are categorized into group profiles on
impairments in one or more psychological pro- the basis of clinician ratings of social competence
cesses related to learning (e.g., language process- and emotion regulation, further delineated by age
ing, visual-spatial skills, processing speed, and gender, to ensure that children with compat-
memory, and attention). ible goals are placed together and to provide a
framework for tailoring group activities and les-
sons. See Table 1.
Overview of the Program Children are then matched carefully according
to their individual treatment goals with consider-
The Integra Social ACES (Awareness, ation of each child’s self-regulation and emotion
Competence, Engagement, and Skills) program regulation abilities and how these abilities may
is a strength-based, client-centered, and experi- positively or negatively affect the group process
ential program intended to provide children and and opportunities for learning for the children in
youth with LDs with a positive social experience the group. For example, a child who needs to
and increase their social competence. Unlike work on basic social competence, such as turn-­
many manualized social skills treatment pro- taking, eye contact, and basic conversational
grams, the Integra Social ACES program tailors skills, may be placed with other children with
the curriculum to the child’s and group’s treat- similar social competence treatment goals. The
ment goals and takes an individualized approach level of self-regulation may help to determine the
in terms of flexibility of content, therapeutic pacing and nature of the group activities. For
stance, and group matching (Integra Program, instance, children with low levels of regulation
2016). A key component of the Integra Social may need a faster pace of activities and less talk-
ACES program is the tailoring of group activities ing and processing of the activities in order to
to accommodate group participants’ neuropsy- sustain their attention and focus. Groups vary in
chological processing deficits. This is accom- size from three to eight children or youth and are
plished through careful group matching and matched according to age, developmental stage,
informed by a review of each participant’s learn- and gender.
ing profile (based on a comprehensive psycho-
educational assessment).
Group Content

Group Matching One of the key features of the Integra Social


ACES program is that there is less of an empha-
Through a multisource assessment informed by sis on teaching social skills in a didactic man-
the child’s psychological assessment report, clin- ner. Rather, the program content largely consists
ical observations of the child in an assessment of games and activities (e.g., tabletop games,
group, as well as clinician and parent report, a drama activities, teamwork-based activities)

Table 1  Group matching by social competence and emotion regulation


Social competence
Low Medium High
Regulation Low Low social competence/low Medium social competence/low High social competence/low
regulation regulation regulation
High Low social competence/high Medium social competence/high High social competence/high
regulation regulation regulation
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 313

that provide naturalistic and engaging opportu- check-in involves having the group participants
nities for participants to practice their skills. demonstrate a particular movement with their
Children learn from each other and are directly bodies while the other group members mirror the
coached by adult facilitators. This encourages movement. This type of check-in works best for
children to approach social situations that they children with self-regulation difficulties who
may normally avoid and to learn to manage the would benefit from having an opportunity to
associated e­ motion. In addition, the games and release excess energy from their bodies, allowing
activities allow for “in-the-moment” teaching them to experience improved self-regulation dur-
opportunities, group discussion of the skills ing the rest of the group. An active check-in also
learned to “real-­world” situations, and direct encourages the group to tune in to the participant
modeling and coaching by the group leading the movement, which involves visual
facilitators. tracking and shifting one’s attention to the par-
ticipant, important skills in social interactions.
For children with slower processing speed, group
 ample Group Session: Skills,
S leaders will ensure that each child has an oppor-
Information Processing Deficits, tunity to engage in the movement by adjusting
and Accommodations the pace of the check-in. A feelings check-in
often involves having the participants discuss
Given that each child brings to the group a unique their current feeling state and briefly explain their
set of social competence strengths and needs choice to the group. Feelings check-ins are often
across behavioral, cognitive, and emotional adapted by having a visual component that
areas, treatment goals differ by group and no two includes a card with a picture of an animal and an
groups are structured in the same exact manner. associated feeling label (e.g., a bear is associated
However, the groups follow a general structure with irritable). This accommodation supports
with the common elements of a form of check-in participants who may learn and express them-
and a time for “snack and chat” at the end of the selves best with visual rather than verbal infor-
group. The specific group activities that make up mation. Feelings check-ins promote emotional
the content of the group are based on several fac- awareness and conversation skills, including
tors including, but not limited to, individual par- visual tracking, sharing about oneself, the oppor-
ticipant treatment goals, group treatment goals, tunity for group participants to ask follow-up
stage of treatment, and progression toward goals. questions, and opportunities to demonstrate
Decisions regarding the specific content of each empathy. Throughout a verbally based check-in,
group session also account for the participants’ group leaders will scaffold for group members
specific information processing deficits. For how to show good listening skills, how to ask
example, all group sessions begin with an over- follow-up questions to demonstrate appropriate
view of the group agenda, as well as a visual listening skills, and may provide direct feedback
schedule to accommodate participants with regarding making eye contact for participants
memory difficulties, such that they know the plan who struggle with eye contact, for example. 
for the group and can refer back to the schedule The following content activities are examples
to know what is coming next. Providing a visual of activities that could be used to target skills
schedule also helps to support group members such as taking turns, compromising, and
who struggle with transitions as they know what cooperation.
to expect and what is expected of them.
Squiggle Game  The squiggle game involves
Group Check-In  Most group sessions begin having the group members draw a simple squig-
with an active check-in or a feelings check-in as gle on a piece of paper and passing the paper to
a way to ground and reconnect the participants someone else. The next group member will turn
since their last group session together. An active the squiggle into a drawing, while the original
314 K. Milligan et al.

participant who drew the squiggle has to watch skill. Group leaders will provide immediate and
their squiggle transform into something new. The direct feedback during the negotiation part of this
children who are not involved in drawing are activity to support children who have executive
encouraged to ask questions and show an interest functioning difficulties.
in the drawing. For the child who is drawing,
visual-motor integration difficulties may make Snack and Chat  Each group session ends with
this activity particularly challenging. To accom- “snack and chat,” a structured time during which
modate for visual-motor integration difficulties, the group members are supported to practice
group leaders may provide suggestions for how their conversational skills while having a snack.
to turn the squiggle into a drawing or may subtly Specific skills targeted during the snack and chat
provide a concrete example to assist the child in include asking on-topic questions, sharing about
visualizing a potential drawing. For the group oneself, making on-topic comments to build on
member who originally drew the squiggle, execu- the conversation, and complimenting. Depending
tive functioning difficulties may impact on their on the level of social competence of the group
ability to regulate their reaction and shift their members, more or less scaffolding is provided by
expectations. Group leaders may provide verbal the group leaders during snack and chat. For
feedback to the group member who is having a example, a group with overall low levels of social
hard time seeing their squiggle turned into some- competence may require more explicit direction,
thing unexpected by labeling their feelings and modeling, and coaching to practice asking ques-
praising them for regulating their emotional tions of one another to keep the conversation
response. going. Over time, group participants build their
skills in this area with the highest level of social
Change the Room  In this activity, one group competence being a conversation that begins and
member will leave the room while the remaining is maintained with minimal facilitation on the
group members change three things about the part of the group leaders. In addition to explicit
room. The group member who left the room has instruction on how to maintain conversation, as
a few guesses to figure out what is different upon well as opportunities to role-play these skills and
reentering the room. This game fosters compro- practice them in a naturalistic context, group
mise, negotiation skills, and cooperation and leaders will accommodate for memory difficul-
involves visual attention to detail. A common ties, slow processing speed, and executive func-
accommodation for the participant who is guess- tion difficulties by moderating the pace of the
ing which changes have been made is that the rest conversation, providing scaffolding to group
of the group members will indicate “hot” or members, and adjusting their tailored feedback
“cold” if they are getting closer to the vicinity of according to the group member’s level of diffi-
the change. This accommodation is only pro- culty with the skill. See Table 2 for a summary of
vided with the permission of the guessing partici- the skills targeted related to the activity, the infor-
pant. Group members are instructed to discuss mation processing deficits that may interfere with
each change with one another and to ensure that the activity, and the accommodations that are
all group members contribute and approve each often provided in the Social ACES program.
change, thereby promoting the skills of negotia- Another key component of the Social ACES
tion, cooperation, and compromise. Children program is its focus on self-regulation and
with executive functioning difficulties may strug- emotion regulation and how these affect the
gle with the emphasis on compromise involved in acquisition and development of social skills.
this activity as a result of their difficulty shifting. Children with self-regulation difficulties may
To accommodate for this executive functioning struggle with monitoring and controlling their
difficulty, group leaders will have introduced the energy level, maintaining focused attention
skill of compromising prior to this activity and during social interactions, or providing conver-
provided them with opportunities to practice this sational space for others to participate, for
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 315

Table 2  Sample activity and associated skills and neuropsychological cognitive processes and accommodations
Neuropsychological
cognitive processes
Activity Skills targeted involved Accommodations
Feelings Emotional awareness Language processing Modeling from group leaders (sharing their
check-in Conversation skills Visual-spatial processing internal feeling state as well as asking
Processing speed follow-up questions to engage others in
conversation)
Visual images to represent and match feelings
listed on cards
Allowing enough time for group members to
respond (i.e., group leaders moderate pace of
turn-taking)
Squiggle Communication Language processing Visual activity for children who may have
game Creativity Executive functioning difficulty with language-based activities
Expression through art (e.g., shifting) Scaffolding by group leaders to support group
Adapting to change and Visual-motor integration members to “let it go”/adapt to unexpected
compromising skills changes
Repetition of instructions related to activity
provided to group members
Check-in with group members to ensure their
understanding
Extra time (related to visual-motor integration
difficulties)
Change Negotiation Language processing Scaffolding by group leader to support group
the room Compromise Processing speed members to ask each other questions and tune
Cooperation Executive functioning in to others
Memory Ensuring that each group has a chance to
ToM contribute their ideas
Group leaders ensuring enough time for group
member who is guessing the changes
Repetition of rules to reduce memory demands
Didactic lesson on what it means to
compromise
Snack and Conversation skills, Language processing Didactic lesson related to how to keep a
chat including asking on-topic Processing speed conversation going
questions Memory Role-plays to allow group participants to
Sharing about oneself Attention practice the skills
Making on-topic Executive functioning Modeling from group leaders
comments to build on the skills Scaffolding (i.e., group leader asks, “does
conversation anyone have any questions about that?”)
Complimenting

example. A child’s regulation may significantly Similarly, for children with higher levels of
impact their ability to actively participate in social competence and low levels of self-­
group process and may impact their social regulation, a focus of intervention is on improv-
interactions. Due to difficulties with self-regu- ing their awareness of the impact of their actions
lation, a child’s ability to attend to and follow on others and reducing silliness. To address dif-
conversation may be affected. For children who ficulties with self-regulation, the Social ACES
have some social competence yet who struggle program uses a tool called the silly-serious scale.
with self-regulation, their difficulties may The goal of introducing the silly-serious scale is
impede performance of their social skills. such that group members will learn that different
Often, a focus of intervention for these children activities and situations require different levels of
is on improved awareness of self and others, as silliness or seriousness and develop the skills to
well as monitoring their self-regulation. self-monitor and adjust their behavioral output
316 K. Milligan et al.

accordingly. In introducing the silly-serious Conclusion


scale, group leaders will elicit from the group
participants what are acceptable energy levels for This chapter highlights the complexity of
particular activities (e.g., watching television social competence, both in terms of its devel-
requires a relatively calm energy level, while opment and its enactment. We have provided a
playing outside at recess can involve more silli- possible framework for understanding the
ness and less regulation). Once this tool has been interaction of behavioral, cognitive, and emo-
introduced in the context of a group, it is referred tional factors in social competence. Review of
to throughout the group so that the group partici- the extant literature suggests that cognitive and
pants gradually build their awareness related to emotional factors are not consistently attended
their energy level and its impact on others. to in the curriculum or delivery of social com-
In addition to difficulties with behavioral self-­ petence interventions and that differences in
regulation, children with LDs often have difficulty targeting these factors may depend, at least in
managing and regulating emotional reactivity due part, on the age and clinical characteristics of
to their executive functioning deficits. Their diffi- the group the intervention is designed for.
culties with emotion regulation may impact them Effect sizes for social competence interven-
socially as they are more likely to struggle with tions are small. It is possible that attending to
managing their reactions to others and perspective- behavioral, cognitive, and emotional factors in
taking, for example. The Social ACES program our interventions, with flexibility to individual-
pays particular attention to children who demon- ize to participants in groups (as is done in the
strate rigidity and low frustration tolerance as these Integra Social ACES program), may enhance
characteristics can significantly impact on a child’s the effectiveness of our social competence
ability to engage in and benefit from the interven- interventions.
tion. For example, children who exhibit extreme
rigidity regarding rules of a game or the concept of
fairness will benefit most from opportunities to References
interact with peers who model flexibility and who
will be tolerant of the group member’s rigidity. Alduncin, N., Huffman, L. C., Feldman, H. M., & Loe,
I. M. (2014). Executive function is associated with
Evaluation research of the Integra Social social competence in preschool-aged children born
ACES programs attests to its promise in enhanc- preterm or full term. Early Human Development, 90,
ing the social skills of children with learning dis- 299–306.
abilities and co-occurring mental health issues Amdurer, E., Boyatzis, R. E., Saatcioglu, A., Smith,
M. L., & Taylor, S. N. (2014). Long term impact of
(Milligan et al., 2016). The program was associ- emotional, social and cognitive intelligence compe-
ated with significant gains in initiation and tencies and GMAT on career and life satisfaction and
engagement in positive social interactions, foun- career success. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–15.
dational skills that support improvement in social American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
competence. Effect sizes ranged from d = 0.40– Washington, DC: Author.
0.59, which reflects larger effects than seen in Anderson, P. J. (2008). Towards a developmental model of
previous research (Quinn et al., 1999) and effects executive function. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, & P. J.
that approach or are medium in strength. Anderson (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal
lobes (pp. 3–21). New York: Psychology Press.
Qualitative interviews with parents, children, and Andrade, B. F., Brodeur, D. A., Waschbusch, D. A.,
teachers suggested improvements in social self-­ Stewart, S. H., & McGee, R. (2009). Selective and
concept, initiation, and emotion regulation. sustained attention as predictors of social problems in
Tailoring treatment to the child’s information children with typical and disordered attention abilities.
Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 341–352.
processing and emotion regulation abilities, as Arthur, M., Bochner, S., & Butterfield, N. (1999).
well as “in-the-moment” feedback, was reported Enhancing peer interactions within the context of play.
to support gains made and contributed to partici- International Journal of Disability, Development and
pants having a positive social experience. Education, 46(3), 367–381.
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 317

Astington, J., & Jenkins, J. (1995). Theory of mind devel- Bowie, C. R., Leung, W. W., Reichenberg, A., McClure,
opment and social understanding. Cognition and M. M., Patterson, T. L., Heaton, R. K., & Harvey,
Emotion, 9, 151–165. P. D. (2008). Predicting schizophrenia patients’ real
Astington, J. W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never world behavior with specific neuropsychological and
sufficient: False-belief understanding and social functional capacity measures. Biological Psychiatry,
competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), 63(5), 505–511.
Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications Boyle, M. H., Cunningham, C. E., Heale, J., Hundert, J.,
for typical and atypical development (pp. 13–38). MacDonald, J., Offord, D. R., et al. (1999). Helping
New York: Psychology Press. children adjust – a tri-ministry study: I. Evaluation
Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Theoretical expla- methodology. Journal of Child Psychology and
nations of children’s understanding of the mind. British Psychiatry, 40(7), 1051–1060.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 7–31. Brown, B. B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships
Attwood, T. (2000). Strategies for improving the social in adolescence. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg
integration of children with Asperger syndrome. (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.,
Autism, 4(1), 85–100. pp. 74–103). New York: Wiley.
Backenson, E. M., Holland, S. C., Kubas, H. A., Fitzer, Caplan, M., Weissberg, R. P., Grober, J. S., Sivo, P. J.,
K. R., Wilcox, G., Carmichael, J. A., … Hale, J. B. Grady, K., & Jacoby, C. (1992). Social competence
(2015). Psychosocial and adaptive deficits associated promotion with inner-city and suburban adolescents:
with learning disability subtypes. Journal of Learning Effects on social adjustment and alcohol use. Journal
Disabilities, 48, 511–522. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(1), 56–63.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can Castro, V. L., Halberstadt, A. G., & Garrett-Peters, P.
read minds: cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. (2016). A three-factor structure of emotion under-
Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology standing in third-grade children. Social Development,
of Cognition, 13, 513–552. 25(3), 602–622.
Barkley, R. A. (2011). Sluggish cognitive tempo is a distinct Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relation-
disorder from ADHD in adults. The ADHD Report, ship between executive function abilities, adaptive
19(5), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1521/adhd.2011.19.5.1 behaviour, and academic achievement in children with
Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Watson, M., Solomon, J., & externalizing behaviour problems. Journal of Child
Schaps, E. (1989). Effects of an elementary school Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 785–796.
program to enhance prosocial behavior on children’s Committee for Children. (1997). Second step: A violence-­
cognitive-social problem-solving skills and strategies. prevention curriculum. Seattle, WA: Committee for
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, Children.
147–169. Cotugno, A. J. (2009). Social competence and social skills
Baumeister, A., Storch, E., & Geffken, G. (2008). Peer training and intervention for children with autism spec-
victimization in children with learning disabilities. trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 25, 11–23. Disorders, 39(9), 1268–1277.
Bauminger, N., & Kimhi-Kind, I. (2008). Social infor- Cragar, D. E., & Horvath, L. S. (2003). The application
mation processing, security of attachment, and emo- of social skills training in the treatment of a child
tion regulation in children with learning disabilities. with Asperger’s disorder. Clinical Case Studies, 2(1),
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 315–332. 34–49.
Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and refor-
integrative framework for the development of social mulation of social information-processing mecha-
skills. Psychological Bulletin, 136(1), 39–64. nisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological
Becker, S. P., & Langberg, J. M. (2014). Attention-deficit/ Bulletin, 115, 74–101.
hyperactivity disorder and sluggish cognitive tempo Del Prette, Z. A. P., Del Prette, A., de Oliviera, L. A.,
dimensions in relation to executive functioning in Gresham, F. M., & Vance, M. J. (2012). Role of
adolescents with ADHD. Child Psychiatry and Human social performance in predicting learning problems:
Development, 45(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Prediction of risk using logistic regression analysis.
s10578-013-0372-z School Psychology International, 33(6), 615–630.
Beelmann, A., Pfingsten, U., & Loesel, F. (1994). Effects Dennis, T. A., Brotman, L. M., Huang, K.-Y., & Gouley,
of training social competence in children – a meta-­ K. K. (2007). Effortful control, social competence, and
analysis of recent evaluation studies. Journal of adjustment problems in children at risk for psycho-
Clinical Child Psychology, 23(3), 260–271. pathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Bellini, S., Peters, J. K., Benner, L., & Hopf, A. (2007). Psychology, 36, 442–454.
A meta-analysis of school-based social skills inter- Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety,
ventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. processing efficiency, and cognitive performance:
Remedial and Special Education, 28(3), 153–162. New developments from attentional control theory.
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and European Psychologist, 14, 168–176.
self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological Devine, R.  T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between
approach. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711–731. false belief understanding and executive function in
318 K. Milligan et al.

early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, Gates, J. A., Kang, E., & Lerner, M. D. (2017). Efficacy of
85(5), 1777–1794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237 group social skills interventions for youth with autism
Devine, R. T., White, N., Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2016). spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-­
Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 52, 164–181.
associations with executive function and social com- Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Social
petence. Developmental Psychology, 52, 758–771. information processing and children's social adjust-
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review ment. In J. B. Kupersmidt & K. A. Dodge (Eds.),
of Psychology, 64, 135–168. Children’s peer relations: From development to
Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information-­processing intervention (pp. 61–79). Washington, DC: American
model of social competence in children. In Psychological Association.
M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota symposium on child Gresham, F. M. (2001). Assessment of social skills in chil-
psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77–125). Hillsdale, NJ: dren and adolescents. In J. Andrews, D. Saklosfske,
Erlbaum. & H. Janzen (Eds.), Handbook of psychoeducational
Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-­ assessment (pp. 326–356). San Diego, CA: Academic.
processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C.
in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and (2001). Affective social competence. Review of Social
Social Psychology, 53(6), 1146–1158. Development, 10(1), 79–119.
Dodge, K. A., & Feldman, E. (1990). Issues in social cog- Hantson, J., Wang, P. P., Grizenko-Vida, M., Ter-­
nition and sociometric status. In A. R. Asher & J. D. Stepanian, M., Harvey, W. J., & Joober, R. (2012).
Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood (pp. 119– Effectiveness of a therapeutic summer camp for chil-
155). New York: Cambridge University Press. dren with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders,
Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R. R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). 16(7), 610–617.
The assessment of intention-cue detection skills in Harms, M. B., Zayas, V., Meltzoff, A. N., & Carlson,
children: Implications for developmental psychopa- S. M. (2014). Stability of executive function and pre-
thology. Child Development, 55(1), 163–173. dictions to adaptive behavior from middle childhood
Dodge, K. A., & Newman, J. P. (1981). Biased decision-­ to pre-adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–9.
making processes in aggressive boys. Journal of Heinze, J. E., Miller, A. L., Seifer, R., Dickstein, S., &
Abnormal Psychology, 90, 375–379. Locke, R. L. (2015). Emotion knowledge, loneli-
Dodge, K. A., & Tomlin, A. M. (1987). Utilization of ness, negative social experiences, and internalizing
self-schemas as a mechanism of interpretational bias symptoms among low-income preschoolers. Social
in aggressive children. Social Cognition, 5, 280–300. Development, 24, 240–265.
Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). Understanding other, Holsen, I., Smith, B. H., & Frey, K. S. (2008). Outcomes
and individual differences in friendship interactions in of the social competence program Second Step in
young children. Social Development, 8(2), 201–219. Norwegian elementary schools. School Psychology
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, International, 29(1), 71–88.
R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of Humphreys, K. L., Galán, C. A., Tottenham, N., &
enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A Lee, S. S. (2016). Impaired social decision-making
meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. mediates the association between ADHD and social
Child Development, 81(1), 405–432. problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.  A., Guthrie, I.  K., & Reiser, M. 44(5), 1023–1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-
(2000). Dispositional emotionality and regulation: 015-0095-7
Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Lalonde, C.  E., & Chandler, M.  J. (1995). False belief
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional
136–157. consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of
Elias, M. J., & Haynes, N. (2008). Social competence, mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2/3), 167–185.
social support, and academic achievement in minority, Ialongo, N., Poduska, J., Werthamer, L., & Kellam, S.
low-income, urban elementary school children. School (2001). The distal impact of two first-grade preven-
Psychology Quarterly, 23(4), 474–495. tive interventions on conduct problems and disor-
Ellis, M. L., Weiss, B., & Lochman, J. E. (2009). der in early adolescence. Journal of Emotional and
Executive functions in children: Associations with Behavioural Disorders, 9(3), 146–160.
aggressive behavior and appraisal processing. Journal Integra Program, Child Development Institute. (2016).
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 945–956. Integra social awareness, competence, engagement,
Forness, S. R., & Kavale, K. A. (1996). Treating social and skills (ACES) program. Unpublished manual.
skill deficits in children with learning disabilities: A Kimber, B., Sandell, R., & Bremberg, S. (2008). Social
meta-analysis of the research. Learning Disability and emotional training in Swedish schools for the
Quarterly, 19, 2–13. promotion of mental health: An effectiveness study of
Galway, T. M., & Metsala, J. L. (2011). Social cognition 5 years of intervention. Health Education Research,
and its relation to psychosocial adjustment in chil- 23(6), 931–940.
dren with nonverbal learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities association of Ontario (LDAO).
Learning Disabilities, 44(1), 33–49. (2001). Learning disabilities: A new definition.
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 319

Retrieved fromhttp://www.ldao.ca/documents/ Ornaghi, V., Grazzani, I., Cherubin, E., Conte, E., &
Definition_and_Suporting%20Document_2001.pdf Piralli, F. (2015). ‘Let’s talk about emotions!’ The
Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated effect of conversational training on preschoolers’ emo-
model of emotion processes and cognition in social tion comprehension and prosocial orientation. Social
information processing. Child Development, 71(1), Development, 24, 166–183.
107–118. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and
Liddle, B., & Nettle, D. (2006). Higher-order theory of later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children
mind and social competence in school-age children. at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102(3), 357–389.
Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 4, Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive func-
231–246. https://doi.org/10.1556/JCEP.4.2006.3-4.3 tions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of
Lillvist, A., Sandberg, A., Bjorck-Akesson, E., & Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.
Granlund, M. (2009). The construct of social com- Petti, V. L., Voelker, S. L., Shore, D. L., & Hayman-­
petence – how preschool teachers define social com- Abello, S. E. (2003). Perception of nonverbal emotion
petence in young children. International Journal of cues by children with nonverbal learning disabilities.
Early Childhood, 41(1), 51–68. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
Lyons, G. L., Huber, H. B., Carter, E. W., Chen, R., & 15(1), 23–36.
Asmus, J. M. (2016). Assessing the social skills Quinn, M. M., Kavale, K. A., Mathur, S. R., Rutherford,
and problem behaviors of adolescents with severe R. B., & Forness, S. R. (1999). A meta-analysis of
disabilities enrolled in general education classes. social skill interventions for students with emotional
American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental or behavioral disorders. Journal of Emotional and
Disabilities, 121(4), 317–345. Behavioral Disorders, 7(1), 54–64.
Milich, R., & Dodge, K. A. (1984). Social information Rantanen, K., Eriksson, K., & Nieminen, P. (2012). Social
processing in child psychiatric populations. Journal of competence in children with epilepsy – a review.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 12(3), 471–489. Epilepsy & Behavior, 24(3), 295–303.
Miller, A. L., Gouley, K. K., Seifer, R., Zakriski, A., Rassovsky, Y., Satz, P., Alfano, M. S., Light, R. K., Zaucha,
Eguia, M., & Vergnani, M. (2005). Emotion knowl- K., McArthur, D. L., & Hovda, D. (2006). Functional
edge skills in low-income elementary school children: outcome in TBI II: Verbal memory and information
Associations with social status and peer experiences. processing speed mediators. Journal of Clinical and
Social Development, 14, 637–651. Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 581–591. https://
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). doi.org/10.1080/13803390500434474
Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the Rao, P. A., Beidel, D. C., & Murray, M. J. (2008). Social
relation between language ability and false-belief skills interventions for children with Asperger’s syn-
understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. drome or high-functioning autism: A review and rec-
Milligan, K., Badali, P., & Spiroiu, F. (2015). Using mind- ommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental
fulness martial arts to address self-regulation challenges Disorders, 38, 353–361.
in youth with learning disabilities: A qualitative explora- Reich, S. M. (2016). Connecting offline social com-
tion. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 562–575. petence to online peer interactions. Psychology of
Milligan, K., Phillips, M., & Morgan, A. S. (2016). Popular Media Culture. Advance online publication.
Tailoring social competence interventions for children https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000111
with learning disabilities. Journal of Child and Family Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002).
Studies, 25, 856–869. Risky families: Family social environments and the
Mishna, F. (2003). Learning disabilities and bullying: mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological
Double jeopardy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, Bulletin, 128(2), 330–366.
336–347. Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusché, C. A., & Pentz,
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, M. A. (2006). The mediational role of neurocogni-
A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity tion in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional
of executive functions and their contributions to com- prevention program in elementary school students:
plex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science,
Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. 7, 91–102.
Nigg, J. T., Quamma, J. P., Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche, Rose-Krasner, L. (1997). The nature of social compe-
C. A. (1999). A two-year longitudinal study of neu- tence: A theoretical review. Social Development, 6,
ropsychological and cognitive performance in relation 111–135.
to behavioral problems and competencies in elemen- Rose, A.  J., & Asher, S.  R. (2017). The social tasks of
tary school children. Journal of Abnormal Child friendship: Do boys and girls excel in different tasks?
Psychology, 27, 51–63. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 3–8. https://doi.
Nowicki, S., & Duke, M.  P. (1992). Helping the child who org/10.1111/cdep.12214
doesn’t fit in. Atlanta: Peachtree Publishers. Rubin, K. H., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1992). Interpersonal
O’Hearn, T. C., & Gatz, M. (1999). Evaluating a psycho- problem solving. In V. B. Van Hasselt & M. Hersen
social competence program for urban adolescence. (Eds.), Handbook of social development (pp. 283–
The Journal of Primary Prevention, 20(2), 119–144. 323). New York: Plenum.
320 K. Milligan et al.

Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1981). Teaching cogni- Taylor, C. A., Liang, B., Tracy, A. J., Williams, L. M.,
tive and social skills to high school students. Journal & Seigle, P. (2002). Gender differences in middle
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49(6), school adjustment, physical fighting, and social skills:
908–918. Evaluation of a social competency program. The
Schultz, D., Tharp-Taylor, S., Haviland, A., & Jaycox, L. Journal of Primary Prevention, 23(2), 259–272.
(2009). The relationship between protective factors Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in
and outcomes for children investigated for maltreat- search of a definition. Monographs of the Society for
ment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 684–698. Research in Child Development, 59(2/3), 25–52.
Shek, D. T. L., & Leung, J. T. Y. (2016). Developing Tlustos, S. J., Kirkwood, M. W., Taylor, H. G., Stancin,
social competence in a subject on leadership and T., Brown, T. M., & Wade, S. L. (2016). A random-
intrapersonal development. International Journal ized problem-solving trial for adolescent brain
on Disability and Human Development, 15(2), injury: Changes in social competence. Rehabilitation
165–173. Psychology, 61(4), 347–357.
Slomkowski, C., & Dunn, J. (1996). Young children's Van der Oord, S., Prins, P. J., Oosterlaan, J., &
understanding of other people’s beliefs and feelings Emmelkamp, P. M. (2008). Efficacy of methylpheni-
and their connected communication with friends. date, psychosocial treatments and their combination
Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 442–447. in school-aged children with ADHD: A meta-analysis.
Stanton-Chapman, T., Walker, V., & Jamison, K. R. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 783–800.
(2014). Building social competence in preschool: The Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., & Barkley, R. A. (2010).
effects of a social skills intervention targeting children Social and emotional impairment in children and ado-
enrolled in head start. Journal of Early Childhood lescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life.
Teacher Education, 35, 185–200. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(3), 209–217.
Stevahn, L., Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., Oberle, K., Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement
& Wahl, L. (2000). Effects of conflict resolution train- motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4),
ing integrated into a kindergarten curriculum. Child 548–573.
Development, 71(3), 772–784. Wheeler, J., & Carlson, C. L. (1994). The social func-
Stichter, J. P., Herzog, M. J., Visovsky, K., Schmidt, C., tioning of children with ADD with hyperactivity and
Randolph, J., Schultz, T., & Gage, N. (2010). Social ADD without hyperactivity. Journal of Emotional and
competence intervention for youth with Asperger Behavioral Disorders, 2, 2–11.
syndrome and high-functioning autism: An initial White, S. W., Oswald, D., Ollendick, T., & Scahil, L. (2009).
investigation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spec-
Disorders, 40(9), 1067–1079. trum disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(3),
Stichter, J. P., O’Connor, K. V., Herzog, M. J., Lierheimer, 216–229.
K., & McGhee, S. D. (2012). Social competence inter- Wiener, J., & Schneider, B. (2002). A multisource explo-
vention for elementary students with Aspergers syn- ration of friendship patterns of children with learning
drome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism disabilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
and Developmental Disorders, 42, 354–366. 30, 127–141.
Stiller, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Perspective-taking Wiener, J., & Sunohara, G. (1998). Parents’ perceptions of
and memory capacity predict social network size. the quality of friendship of their children with learn-
Social Networks, 29(1), 93–104. ing disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and
Swaim, K. F., & Morgan, S. B. (2001). Children’s attitudes Practice, 13, 242–257.
and behavioral intentions toward a peer with autis- Zelazo, P. D., & Lyons, K. E. (2012). The potential ben-
tic behaviors: Does a brief educational intervention efits of mindfulness training in early childhood: A
have an effect? Journal of Autism and Developmental developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspec-
Disorders, 31(2), 194–205. tive. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 154–160.
Headache and Migraine

Tiah Dowell, Paul R. Martin, and Allison M. Waters

Contents  revalence, Burden, and Costs


P
Prevalence, Burden, and Costs of Headaches of Headaches in Children
in Children..........................................................  321
The Headache Classification System....................  322
Headache represents the most common manifes-
tation of pain in childhood and adolescence, with
Course of Headache Disorders in Children.........  323
estimates indicating that as many as 75% of
Mechanisms of Migraine and Tension-Type young people will have experienced a significant
Headache.............................................................  323 headache by the age of 15 years (Bille, 1962;
Genetic and Environmental Factors.....................  324 Kabbouche & Gilman, 2008). Whilst it is com-
The Functional Model of Primary Headaches.......   325 monly accepted that the majority of the popula-
tion will experience infrequent headaches from
Psychological Assessment of Primary
Headaches...........................................................  327
time to time, a significant proportion of youth
experience recurrent and disabling headaches,
Psychological Treatment of Headaches
impacting on many aspects of their lives.
in Children..........................................................  329
Prevalence rates for headache vary considerably
Variations in the Psychological Treatment according to research methodology and diagnos-
of Headaches.......................................................  333
tic criteria used. It is estimated that between 3.2%
Future Directions for the Psychological and 14.5% of children and adolescents suffer
Treatment of Headaches in Children...............  335
from recurrent migraines (Özge et al., 2011) and
References...............................................................  336 between 10% and 25% suffer from frequent
tension-­ type headaches (Anttila, 2006), with
research indicating a concerning trend towards an
increase in incidence (Albers, von Kries, Heinen,
& Straube, 2015). Research consistently indi-
cates that the prevalence of headaches increases
with age and is slightly more common in males in
prepubertal years, transitioning to a clear
predominance in females during adolescence
­
T. Dowell • P.R. Martin • A.M. Waters (*)
(Hershey, 2010).
School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University,
Mt Gravatt, QLD, Australia The impact of headaches on sufferers varies
e-mail: a.waters@griffith.edu.au greatly due to the heterogeneity of headache con-

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 321


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_19
322 T. Dowell et al.

ditions, whereby significant variations may occur medication overuse. Primary headaches are
within individual attacks in terms of headache divided into four categories: migraine, tension-­
frequency, duration, and severity. Headache dis- type headache, trigeminal autonomic cephalal-
orders have the potential to have a negative gias, and other primary headache disorders.
impact on the lives of young people, spanning Migraine and tension-type headache are the most
across their physical, academic, and social func- commonly occurring headache disorders in chil-
tioning and at times causing severe disability dren and adolescents and have received the larg-
(Holden, Levy, Deichmann, & Gladstein, 1998). est proportion of empirical research. They are
Research indicates that many young people also the headache types for which patients are
experience difficulties in coping with their head- most likely to seek psychological treatment, and
aches (Wöber-Bingöl et al., 2014), and the qual- as such will be the focus of the current chapter.
ity of life in those affected is found to be lower Migraine is subdivided into six sections in the
than that of healthy counterparts across all ICHD-3 beta, of which the two major sub-­
domains (Powers, Patton, Hommel, & Hershey, conditions are migraine without aura and
2003). Furthermore, the negative personal impact migraine with aura. Migraine without aura is
of recurrent headaches has been found to be com- defined as a recurrent headache disorder mani-
parable with serious health conditions afflicting festing in attacks lasting from 4 to 72 h, with at
children, including cancer, sickle cell disease, least two of the following: unilateral location,
and rheumatoid arthritis (Peterson & Palermo, pulsating quality, moderate or severe pain inten-
2004; Powers et al., 2003). The societal costs of sity, or aggravation by, or causing avoidance of,
headaches in children are poorly understood, routine physical activity. Additionally, the patient
though research indicates that headaches account must present with at least one of the following:
for a significant number of days missed at school nausea and/or vomiting or photophobia and/or
(Connelly & Rapoff, 2006; Kernick & Campbell, phonophobia. Aura is defined as recurrent attacks
2009) and a significant number of parents miss- of unilateral, fully reversible symptoms charac-
ing days at work (Wöber-Bingöl et al., 2014). terised by visual, sensory, or other central ner-
The high prevalence rates and burden of headache vous system disturbances that develop gradually
in children indicate a serious need for effective and are usually followed by a headache and asso-
diagnosis and treatment. ciated migraine symptoms. Other subtypes of
migraine include chronic migraine, complica-
tions of migraine, probable migraine, and epi-
The Headache Classification System sodic syndromes that may be associated with
migraine. Episodic syndromes that may be
The current headache classification system is the associated with migraine occur exclusively in
International Classification of Headache children and are usually considered a precursor
Disorders, 3rd Edition Beta version (ICHD-3 to the development of a migraine disorder. These
beta; Headache Classification Committee, 2013), conditions are understudied and poorly under-
which is used as the basis for clinical diagnosis stood, though research indicates that up to half of
and research in both adults and children. The children who experience migraines may also
ICHD-3 beta is comprised of three parts: primary meet criteria for an associated episodic syndrome
headaches, secondary headaches, and painful (Rothner & Parikh, 2016). The ICHD-3 beta
cranial neuropathies, other facial pains, and other identifies three episodic syndromes: recurrent
headaches. Primary headaches are thought to be gastrointestinal disturbance (cyclical vomiting
intrinsic to the nervous system, whilst secondary syndrome or abdominal migraine), benign par-
headaches are those which are attributable to a oxysmal vertigo, and benign paroxysmal
separate cause, such as a tumour, infection, or torticollis.
Headache and Migraine 323

There has been considerable debate surround-  ourse of Headache Disorders


C
ing the applicability of the ICHD criteria in diag- in Children
nosing primary headache disorders in children
and adolescents, particularly in regard to migraine The course of a headache disorder may vary con-
disorders. The issues of unilaterality and duration siderably between individuals, though common
of headaches are thought to predominantly trends have been observed in the literature.
account for a general lack of sensitivity, specific- Research indicates that more than half of chil-
ity, and predictive value of the criteria in children dren diagnosed with a headache disorder will
(McAbee, Morse, & Assadi, 2016). The ICHD-3 show significant improvement or even be
beta recognises that characteristics of migraine headache-­free at long-term follow-up. However,
may manifest differently in children, conceding 20–40% of children remain unchanged or experi-
that pain may occur bilaterally in children, and ence a worsening of their symptoms, carrying the
headache duration may occur for a minimum of disorder into adulthood (Antonaci et al., 2014;
two, rather than 4 h. However, research has indi- Brna, Dooley, Gordon, & Dewan, 2005; Guidetti
cated that as many as 11–81% of children with a et al., 1998; Hernandez-Latorre & Roig, 2000). A
diagnosis of migraine may experience headache review of headache trajectories in children con-
durations of less than 2 h, with a further 8–25% cluded that transformations of headaches
reporting headache durations of less than 1 h (migraine becoming tension-type headache and
(Maytal, Young, Shechter, & Lipton, 1997; vice versa) are common, occurring in approxi-
Mortimer, Kay, & Jaron, 1992; Winner & mately one third of children, which is further
Hershey, 2006). Furthermore, research conducted likely to complicate diagnostic issues (Antonaci
by Lima and colleagues (2015) indicated a 58% et al., 2014). Research consistently indicates that
sensitivity of the criteria to accurately diagnose remission rates of headache disorders are more
migraine headaches in children, which rose to favourable for boys compared to girls and for
94% when attacks lasting for less than 2 h were tension-type headache compared to migraine
included. (Brna et al., 2005; Guidetti et al., 1998;
The ICHD-3 beta defines a tension-type Kienbacher et al., 2006). Other potential risk fac-
headache as having two of the following quali- tors for poor outcomes that have been identified
ties: bilateral location, pressing or tightening include having a higher headache severity at
(non-­pulsating) quality, mild or moderate inten- diagnosis (Brna et al., 2005) and a longer time
sity, and not aggravated by routine physical period between headache onset and seeking pro-
activity. Nausea and/or vomiting does not fessional help (Kienbacher et al., 2006).
accompany the headache, and only one of pho-
tophobia or phonophobia may be present. The
duration of the headache may occur from  echanisms of Migraine
M
30 min to 7 days. Tension-type headache is and Tension-Type Headache
divided into four categories, of which the three
main subtypes are infrequent tension-type The mechanisms involved in primary headache
headache (occurring less than once per month), disorders are not well understood and have been
frequent tension-type headache (occurring the subject of much debate within the literature.
between 1 and 14 days per month), and chronic Furthermore, little research has examined the
tension-type headache (occurring on, or more mechanisms of headache in children and adoles-
than, 15 days per month). To date, only one cents, though it is presumed that the mechanisms
study has examined the applicability of ICHD-3 that underlie headaches in adults are similar to
beta criteria to children with tension-type head- that of youth (Hershey, 2010). Traditionally,
ache and found a 69.7% predictive value migraine has been considered a disorder of the
(Albers, Straube, Landgraf, Heinen, & von vascular system. This predominant theory was
Kries, 2014). proposed by Harold Wolff, who argued that a
324 T. Dowell et al.

two-stage model of vasoconstriction caused by (Lipchik et al., 2000). It is hypothesised that


cortical spreading depression (a wave of electro- peripheral activation or sensitisation of myofas-
physiological hyperactivity followed by inhibi- cial nociceptors is involved in the development of
tion), followed by extracranial vasodilation, this muscle pain and acute episodes of tension-­
formed the underlying physiology of pain in type headaches (Bendtsen et al., 2016).
migraine (Shevel, 2011). However, recent years Furthermore, it is thought that prolonged noci-
have seen a transition of focus from peripheral ceptive stimuli from pericranial tissues lead to
mechanisms to central mechanisms, which has sensitisation of pain pathways in the central ner-
been largely facilitated by advances in neuroim- vous system, which is hypothesised to be respon-
aging (Tedeschi, Russo, & Tessitore, 2013). sible for the conversion from episodic to chronic
tension-type headache in sufferers (Bendtsen
Migraine  Mechanisms of migraine are now et al., 2016; Bendtsen & Jensen, 2006).
thought to be based on an interaction between
neural and vascular systems and include cortical
spreading depression and trigeminal vascular Genetic and Environmental Factors
activation with transmission through the thala-
mus to higher cortical structures (Hershey, 2010). When considering the relative influence of
Cortical spreading depression is considered to be genetic and environmental factors in predispos-
the likely cause of migraine aura, with neuroim- ing individuals to headaches, much of the
aging techniques indicating a slowly spreading research has been conducted in adult populations
area of abnormal blood flow in the occipital lobe and is extrapolated to children. Several popula-
during migraine aura, likely caused by height- tion and twin-based studies have indicated that
ened neuronal excitability (Hadjikhani et al., the risk of developing a migraine disorder is
2001). Central neuronal hyperexcitability has dependent on a combination of genetic influence
also been proposed as a key mechanism, with and environmental effects contributing to pheno-
evidence indicating that neuronal excitability in typic expression. Research has indicated major
the occipital cortex is elevated in individuals who genetic components in both migraine with aura
suffer from migraine, and furthermore, this excit- (Ulrich, Gervil, Kyvik, Olesen, & Russell, 1999)
ability determines the threshold for triggering and migraine without aura (Gervil, Ulrich, Kyvik,
attacks (Bussone, 2004). This same hyperexcit- Olesen, & Russell, 1999), though a lack of co-
ability has been demonstrated in a study with occurrence observed in twin studies indicates
children and adolescents suffering from migraine, that the two disorders are genetically distinct
which indicated that sensitivity levels changed (Russell, Ulrich, Gervil, & Olesen, 2002). In
during the migraine cycle, supporting the notion addition to genetic influences, individual envi-
that altered sensitivity in the brain of migraine ronmental factors are indicated to account for at
sufferers may be involved in the initiation and least 50% of an individual’s susceptibility (Ulrich
propagation of migraine in this age group et al., 1999).
(Siniatchkin et al., 2009). Research investigating the genetic component
of susceptibility to developing frequent tension-­
Tension-type  Less research has been conducted type headache has indicated an evident, though
in the area of mechanisms responsible for smaller, genetic component, with individual
tension-­
type headache, and underlying patho- environmental factors deemed to play a larger
physiology is considered to be complex and role, accounting for approximately 81% of indi-
poorly understood (Bendtsen, Ashina, Moore, & vidual’s susceptibility (Ulrich, Gervil, & Olesen,
Steiner, 2016). Evidence suggests that sufferers 2004). Research has indicated that genetic factors
of tension-type headache experience more peri- may play a larger role in chronic tension-type
cranial myofascial tenderness, which is positively headache, with a threefold risk of developing the
correlated with intensity and frequency of attacks disorder observed between first-degree relatives
Headache and Migraine 325

(Ostergaard, Russell, Bendtsen, & Olesen, 1997; stress, negative affect (the tendency to experience
Russell, Østergaard, Bendtsen, & Olesen, 1999). high negative emotional reactivity), problems
related to sleep (too little, poor quality), weather
(humidity, temperature, precipitation), light,
 he Functional Model of Primary
T odours, noise, hunger, and playing video games
Headaches (Fraga et al., 2013; Neut et al., 2012). These trig-
gers are similar to those that are reported in the
A psychological approach to the assessment and adult literature (Kelman, 2007).
treatment of a primary headache disorder should Currently, there is a paucity of research sur-
entail an investigation of the unique factors con- rounding triggers in the paediatric population,
tributing to each individual’s case. This involves with much data relying on retrospective, self-­
attempting to answer a number of questions report methodology. This hinders the ability to
which may include the following: Why the head- establish clear temporal relationships between
aches began when they did, why the individual triggers and headaches, and the data is vulnera-
experiences headaches at certain times and not ble to recall bias (Connelly & Bickel, 2011).
others, and what made the individual susceptible Despite these limitations, a small number of
to developing headaches in the first place? These studies have been conducted which have utilised
questions are not easily answered and in many a prospective headache diary design to investi-
cases have no definitive answers. However, gate triggers in the paediatric population. This
attempting to understand such questions allows research has indicated associations between
for a specialised mapping of factors contributing headaches and factors including increased stress
to the individual’s headache disorder, which is levels, too little sleep, negative mood, and
integral in informing treatment. This approach changes in weather (Connelly & Bickel, 2011;
should involve conducting a functional analysis, Connelly, Miller, Gerry, & Bickel, 2010;
whereby the antecedents and consequences of Karlson et al., 2013).
headaches are explored. Martin has developed a
functional model of primary headaches in adults Setting antecedents  Setting antecedents
(Martin, 1993, 2013; Martin, Milech, & Nathan, acknowledge that headaches occur within and are
1993), which is depicted in Fig. 1. Whilst no such affected by a broader psychosocial context.
model has yet been developed for children and Individual setting antecedents are largely depen-
adolescents, the adult model is likely applicable dent on the triggers that precipitate their head-
to the younger population and provides a useful aches. For example, if stress is identified as a
framework for understanding individual factors headache trigger, it is important to ascertain the
contributing to headaches. main sources of stress in the individual’s life, as
well as their coping style and social support.
Immediate factors  Central to the model are Whilst research has been somewhat inconsistent
headache events and their associated symptoms, within this area, associations have been reported
as well as the underlying central and peripheral between setting factors, such as negative family
mechanisms. The immediate antecedents of environments, difficulties at school, and peer
headaches are commonly referred to as triggers, relational problems, and a higher rate of head-
which are factors that precipitate or aggravate ache occurrence in children (Anttila,
headache attacks. It is thought that at up to 91% Metsähonkala, Helenius, & Sillanpää, 2000;
of children suffering from recurrent headaches Aromaa, Rautava, Helenius, & Sillanpää, 1998).
report having at least one identifiable trigger, Setting antecedents may also incorporate
with an average of approximately seven triggers comorbid conditions that have the potential to
reported in some studies (Fraga et al., 2013; mediate or moderate an individual’s exposure to
Neut, Fily, Cuvellier, & Vallée, 2012). Triggers certain triggers. For example, a comorbid mood
most commonly reported by children include or anxiety disorder may result in an individual
326 T. Dowell et al.

Fig. 1  Functional model of primary headaches (Martin, 1993, 2013; Martin et al., 1993)

experiencing more frequent episodes of negative is little research into onset factors within the pae-
affect or stress, which may act as a trigger for their diatric population, one study examining head-
headaches. Research has consistently indicated ache histories in adolescents suffering from
that children who suffer from headaches are also chronic headaches indicated that personal loss,
more likely to suffer from a variety of psychologi- such as the death of a family member, separation
cal and emotional problems, particularly difficul- or divorce in the family, or change of residence
ties related to anxiety and depression (Fielding, from a familiar area, had occurred within
Young, Martin, & Waters, 2016; Mazzone, et al., 12 months of headache onset in many cases
2006; Milde-Busch et al., 2010; Powers, Gilman, (Kaiser & Primavera, 1993). Childhood maltreat-
& Hershey, 2006). Despite their common preva- ment has also been associated with an earlier age
lence, these relationships are poorly understood of migraine onset and is considered a risk factor
and are thought to be complex and bidirectional. for the chronification of migraines (Tietjen et al.,
Sleeping difficulties represent another important 2010). Hormonal factors may play a role in the
comorbid condition that should be considered, onset of the disorder in females and are thought
given that sleep is commonly cited as a trigger. to play a role in the increase in prevalence of
Children suffering from migraine disorders have headaches in girls following the onset of puberty.
been found to be at higher risk of suffering from a A population-based longitudinal study found that
range of sleeping disturbances including bruxism, the onset of menarche was associated with an
co-sleeping, and snoring (Miller, Palermo, Powers, increased risk of recurrent headaches (Kröner-­
Scher, & Hershey, 2003). Herwig & Vath, 2009). Furthermore, headache
has been found to be more prevalent amongst
Onset factors  Any event or situation occurring adolescents and adult females with early men-
at the time when the individual first began to arche, though it remains unclear if early men-
experience headaches and that is thought to have arche increases headache susceptibility or is a
played a role in the initial development of the dis- consequence of a common pathogenic factor
order is considered as onset factors. Whilst there (Aegidius et al., 2011).
Headache and Migraine 327

Predisposing factors  These factors contribute to Immediate- and long-term responses of


an individual’s vulnerability to developing a head- others  It is important to recognise the complex-
ache disorder. The aforementioned genetic compo- ity inherent in the relationship between the child’s
nent of headaches plays an important role in an pain experience and the reactions and responses
individual’s susceptibility, particularly in the case of those around them, particularly their caregiv-
of migraine headaches. It is speculated that certain ers. Caregivers are likely to be personally affected
temperamental traits may also predispose individu- by their children’s headaches, and their response
als to developing headache disorders. In children, to their child’s expression of pain is likely to be
traits such as anxiety sensitivity and hypersensitiv- part of a complex and bidirectional relationship
ity to somatosensory stimuli have been consistently whereby the parent and child appraisals influence
linked with the development of headache disorders each other (Asmundson, Noel, Petter, &
later in life (Kröner-­Herwig & Gassmann, 2012). Parkerson, 2012). Research indicates that chil-
However, research in this area remains limited and dren with pain conditions experience greater lev-
is hindered by its reliance on cross-sectional and els of functional disability when their parents
retrospective methodology. A number of health consistently respond to their pain in maladaptive
variables including asthma, allergies, obesity, ways (Claar, Simons, & Logan, 2008).
and epilepsy have also been associated with an Maladaptive responses include responding with
increased risk of developing a headache disorder, criticism or discounting of pain but also include
though the relationships between these disorders paying increased attention to the child’s pain or
remain unclear (Hershey, 2010). granting special privileges. Whilst these latter
responses may be perceived as positive, they
Immediate- and long-term responses of suffer- have the potential to act as positive reinforce-
ers  The model recognises that the experience of ment for sick role behaviours, resulting in an
headache is complex and subjective and is influ- ongoing cycle.
enced not only by its antecedents but also its con-
sequences. These consequences pertain to the
short- and long-term reactions and responses of Psychological Assessment
both the headache sufferer and those around of Primary Headaches
them. It is important to consider the individual’s
appraisals of their headaches and associated pain, It is recommended that the starting point in head-
as these are likely to play an important role within ache assessment involves consultation with a
the model. Negative appraisals of pain that are medical practitioner, ideally a paediatric neurolo-
characterised by anxiety and catastrophic think- gist, in order to obtain an accurate diagnosis, par-
ing are associated with an exacerbation of pain ticularly with a view to ruling out secondary
severity and higher levels of disability (Vervoort, headaches. Within the realm of psychological
Goubert, Eccleston, Bijttebier, & Crombez, treatment for headaches, it is argued that func-
2006). These responses also have the potential to tional analysis, regardless of the specific diagnos-
create negative feedback loops within the model. tic type of primary headache, will provide more
For example, if the child’s headaches are trig- useful information pertaining to a treatment plan,
gered by stress, a reaction to headaches charac- as this takes into account the individual factors
terised by stress is likely to play a role in maintaining the headache disorder. Interviewing,
triggering future attacks. In the long term, this self-monitoring, and standardised measures and
response may also influence setting factors such questionnaires should be utilised within this
as comorbid disorders, creating a vicious cycle assessment, in order to obtain a clear picture of
that maintains the headache disorder. the individual’s difficulties.
328 T. Dowell et al.

Interviewing mation regarding the frequency, duration, and


intensity of headaches, usually completed at mul-
Gathering information pertaining to the personal tiple time points on a daily basis. Headache dia-
and social history of the child forms an essential ries also have the potential to collect additional
part of the assessment process, in order to eluci- information including medication consumption
date the context in which the headache problem and exposure to suspected triggers of headaches.
occurs. It is then necessary to gather information A 3–4-week period has been suggested as the
relating to the headache problem itself, as well as optimal length to accurately assess headache pat-
the antecedents and consequences of headache terns (Heyer, Perkins, Rose, Aylward, & Lee,
attacks. Parents play an integral role in the provi- 2014; Osterhaus & Passchier, 1992). Traditionally,
sion of this information. It is widely recognised paper diaries have been used; however the recent
that young children may have difficulties in artic- burgeoning of technology use has seen a move-
ulating the symptoms of their headaches to prac- ment towards electronic diaries, which are com-
titioners and their parents, and it is recommended pleted via computers, specialised handheld
that parents are asked about behaviours that their devices, or via specific applications on mobile
children demonstrate, for example, a child phones. A growing body of evidence suggests the
requesting to lie in a dark room may be indicative electronic diaries are favourable in terms of accu-
of experiencing photophobia. A small but racy and credibility and are suitable for usage in
increasing body of research has investigated the children as young as 8 years old (Krogh, Larsson,
utility in asking children to visually depict their Salvesen, & Linde, 2015; Palermo, Valenzuela,
headaches via drawings, which have been found & Stork, 2004). However, the psychometric prop-
to have a high specificity, sensitivity, and predic- erties of electronic headache diaries are yet to be
tive value in differential headache diagnoses and rigorously assessed and therefore should be inter-
may also allow for a deeper understanding of the preted cautiously (Larsson & Stinson, 2011).
child’s pain experience (Mazzotta et al., 2015;
Stafstrom, Rostasy, & Minster, 2002).
Furthermore, these drawings have been found to Questionnaires and Inventories
be useful in depicting the clinical course of a
headache condition when used longitudinally Measurement of the experience of headaches
(Stafstrom, Goldenholz, & Dulli, 2005). It is themselves forms an important part of the assess-
worth noting that research has indicated that rely- ment process. This entails measuring the inten-
ing solely on parental reports of children’s head- sity of headaches, as well as their frequency and
aches may not provide an accurate depiction of duration. It is recognised that young children
the problem, as parents have been found to con- may not have the verbal capabilities necessary to
sistently underestimate the extent of their chil- describe the intensity of their headaches accu-
dren’s headaches (Lundqvist, Clench-Aas, rately. The usage of visual measures in this area
Hofoss, & Bartonova, 2006; Sasmaz et al., 2004), has been recommended, with devices such as
which highlights the importance of utilising addi- visual analogue scales, which allow children to
tional assessment methods. rate their pain on a visual line, anchored at varia-
tions of ‘no pain’ and ‘most possible pain’
(Stinson, Kavanagh, Yamada, Gill, & Stevens,
Self-monitoring 2006). The Facial Affect Scale has also been used
to measure the level of discomfort associated
Self-monitoring of headaches provides important with recurrent headaches in children as young as
information contributing to the assessment of the 5 years old (McGrath et al., 1996).
individual and is also integral in the measurement Measurement of disability and quality of life
of treatment outcomes. This usually occurs in the associated with headaches allows for an under-
form of a headache diary, which collects infor- standing of the impact of headaches on the
Headache and Migraine 329

i­ ndividual and also provides useful information in ation training, and cognitive behavioural therapy
terms of outcome measures. An emerging consen- (CBT). These treatments may be administered as
sus exists that it is necessary to utilise both generic stand-alone interventions or in combination with
and disease-specific quality of life measures for one another. It is worth noting that research in
chronic health conditions (Varni, Burwinkle, & this area has predominantly focused on adult
Lane, 2005). The pedMIDAS (Hershey et al., populations, and empirical evidence for the
2001) is a migraine-specific measurement of dis- efficacy of these treatments in paediatric popula-
ability across different domains of children’s tions is very much in its infancy.
lives, which has been widely used in the literature
and has sound psychometric properties.
Additionally, the Quality of Life Scale (Langeveld, Pharmacological
Koot, Loonen, Hazebroek-Kampschreur, &
Passchier, 1996) has been developed specifically Pharmacological treatment has traditionally been
to measure quality of life in adolescents suffering the cornerstone of treatment for headaches and
from chronic headaches. Generic measures of remains a common treatment option for both
quality of life and disability include the Paediatric adults and children. Despite this treatment falling
Quality of Life Inventory (Peds QL 4.0; Varni, within the medical realm, it is nonetheless an
Seid, & Kurtin, 2001) and the Functional important area for other professionals to have at
Disability Inventory (Walker & Greene, 1991), least a modest understanding of, given that many
both of which are commonly used. headache patients will receive pharmacological
Comorbid conditions, particularly anxiety and treatment concurrently with psychological treat-
mood disorders, are highly prevalent in children ment. The pharmacological treatment of head-
with headaches and are likely to play an impor- aches can be separated into two broad categories,
tant role in the maintenance of headaches, as well acute and preventive.
as the child’s life more broadly. As such, it may Acute pharmacological treatment aims to treat
be useful to routinely assess for these using psy- headache attacks rapidly, with the intention to
chometrically sound measures, such as the restore the individual’s ability to function.
Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS; Spence, Analgesics and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory
1998) and the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale drugs are commonly used in this endeavour
for Children (March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, (Bonfert et al., 2013). The efficacy of acute medi-
& Conners, 1997) for anxiety and the Children’s cations has predominantly been evaluated in
Depression Inventory (Kovacs & Beck, 1977) for adults thus far, though they are considered to
mood disorders, which have frequently been uti- have putative benefits for paediatric populations
lised within the headache literature. for both migraines and infrequent tension-type
headaches. Ibuprofen is considered the first-line
treatment, followed by acetaminophen, due to
Psychological Treatment their favourable side effect profiles (Bonfert
of Headaches in Children et al., 2013). However, ‘over-the-counter’ medi-
cations are thought to be of minor value in at least
Treatment of headaches at an early age is impor- 30–60% of paediatric migraineurs (Ho et al.,
tant, as it may allow for the prevention of the con- 2012), and triptan agents are recommended when
dition’s evolution into a chronic adulthood children suffer severe episodes that remain unre-
disorder. Additionally, longer periods of untreated sponsive to analgesics. Currently triptan agents
headaches have been linked with poorer out- have been approved for use in paediatric migraine
comes (Osterhaus, Lange, Linssen, & Passchier, sufferers only. Whilst acute therapies may shorten
1997). Current treatment methods for headaches the duration or lessen the intensity of attacks,
in children are varied and may include pharmaco- they are unlikely to affect the frequency of
logical treatment, biofeedback therapy, relax- attacks. Furthermore, the use of acute medication
330 T. Dowell et al.

becomes problematic when children suffer from on understandings of underlying mechanisms


frequent headaches, as consumption on more developed in the 1960s and 1970s, whereby ther-
than three occasions per week poses a risk of mal biofeedback has been used for migraines and
headache transformation to analgesic overuse electromyographic biofeedback has commonly
headaches (Hershey, Kabbouche, & Powers, been used in the treatment of tension-type head-
2010). aches. Whilst physiological control is the focus
Preventive pharmacological treatment is con- of biofeedback training, research indicates that
sidered when headaches begin to seriously inter- other therapeutic processes such as cognitive
fere with the individual’s life, either through their changes (such as increased self-efficacy and
frequency or the level of disability caused. internal locus of control) account for a significant
Preventive medication may also be beneficial portion of changes in headaches (Holroyd et al.,
when acute options are found to be ineffective, 1984).
not tolerated, contraindicated, or regularly over- Biofeedback may be administered as a stand-­
used, in children suffering from migraines or fre- alone therapy or form a component of treatments
quent episodic tension-type headache. that also incorporate cognitive strategies. Usually
Prophylaxis including antidepressants, antihy- several treatment sessions are required, with
pertensives, antihistamines, and antiepileptics is daily home practice involved. It is often facili-
regularly prescribed for children, though evi- tated by teaching relaxation or autogenic exer-
dence of efficacy is limited and effective dose cises. Biofeedback training is comprised of
levels are rarely established (Bonfert et al., 2013); multiple phases, including a baseline phase, a
additionally, evidence in this area tends to focus feedback phase, and a self-control phase where
on children suffering from migraine, with little feedback is not provided. Over time, reliance on
research addressing the efficacy of these treat- the feedback is gradually eliminated as the indi-
ments in cases of tension-type headache. Due to vidual’s skills in self-regulation become
the frequency of side effects, contraindications, consolidated.
and a general lack of consensus on pharmaco- Whilst there are a limited number of high
logical strategies in the paediatric population, it quality studies investigating the efficacy of bio-
is frequently recommended that preventive phar- feedback training as a solitary treatment for
macological treatment is indicated only when headaches in children, current evidence is indica-
non-pharmacological and lifestyle modification tive of positive effects. Bussone and colleagues
strategies have been found to be ineffective (1998) compared EMG biofeedback training
(Bonfert et al., 2013; Termine et al., 2011). with a placebo relaxation condition in children
aged 11–15 suffering from episodic tension-type
headache. Whilst initially both conditions were
Biofeedback Training associated with sizeable headache reductions,
after 12 months, children in the biofeedback con-
Biofeedback training aims to assist individuals to dition were found to show continuous improve-
exert control over biological processes that are ments superior to the control condition.
usually involuntary or modulated outside of con- Biofeedback training for skin temperature con-
scious awareness. Such processes may include trol in children and adolescents aged 7–18 diag-
muscle tension, pulse rate, and peripheral blood nosed with migraine has also been associated
flow. Feedback concerning these processes is with significant improvements in headache activ-
provided by audio or visual feedback devices, ity, which appear to be maintained over time
which are driven by electrodes or transducers, (Labbé, 1995; Labbé & Williamson, 1984).
which detect the signals and pass them onto Interestingly, research conducted by Labbé and
amplifiers, which results in an amplification of Williamson (1984) indicated that the treatment
these processes. The most common forms of bio- condition achieved significant results compared
feedback training in treating headaches are based to a control condition, despite evidence that
Headache and Migraine 331

c­ hildren in the treatment condition did not suc- or tension-type headache and found that 50% of
cessfully learn to regulate hand temperature. adolescents achieved clinically significant
Scharff and colleague (2002) attempted to improvement, though results appeared to be more
explore this placebo effect in children, by com- favourable for migraine sufferers, compared to
paring hand-­warming biofeedback with a control those with tension-type headache. Larsson and
condition involving hand-cooling biofeedback. colleagues (1987b) also found support for the
They found that children in the treatment condi- efficacy of a relaxation programme which was
tion were more likely to achieve clinical improve- taught during school hours and was found to be
ment, though a decrease in headache activity in effective in reducing headache activity for ado-
both groups also indicates that there are likely to lescents with recurrent headaches (mostly
be nonspecific treatment effects. It is noted that tension-type).
the treatment group in this study also included
additional components such as relaxation tech-
niques, stress management, and education sur- Cognitive Behavioural Therapy
rounding stress as a trigger for headaches, which
were not included in the control condition and The earliest forms of psychological treatment for
may have confounded the results. Whilst bio- headaches in children were primarily behavioural
feedback appears to be efficacious in the treat- in nature and included contingency management-­
ment of headaches, the mechanisms of change based interventions. These interventions
remain poorly understood, and further research in acknowledged that headache reports in some
the area is required. patients may be conceptualised as operant behav-
iour that is controlled by the consequences of
headache behaviour. This treatment was explored
Relaxation Training in multiple case studies. Yen and McIntire (1971)
successfully reduced headaches in a 14-year-old
Relaxation training in the treatment of headaches girls suffering from constant headache com-
embodies a number of techniques, aiming to plaints using a response-cost contingency plan.
allow the individual to modify headache-related Other case studies have indicated substantial
physiological responses, decreasing their levels decreases in the reporting of headaches in a
of sympathetic arousal. Techniques that are com- 6-year-old girl (Ramsden, Friedman, &
monly implemented in headache treatment Williamson, 1983) and 11-year-old male (Lake,
include progressive muscle relaxation, diaphrag- 1981) following contingency plans whereby
matic breathing, autogenic training, and medita- healthy behaviours were consistently positively
tive or passive relaxation. Relaxation training reinforced. Results of these case studies suggest
may be utilised as a stand-alone treatment or as that headache reporting in some children may be
part of a multi-faceted treatment plan and usually strongly influenced by the environmental conse-
entails six or more hourly sessions. There is usu- quences of reporting pain. However, despite these
ally a daily home practice component, facilitated initial results, contingency management as a
using an audio guide or script. stand-alone treatment did not progress beyond
The efficacy of using relaxation training alone these limited case studies.
to treat headaches in children has been empiri- CBT is becoming increasingly utilised in the
cally supported, with research indicating that treatment of headache disorders in children. This
children are able to achieve clinically significant treatment combines behavioural and cognitive
change following treatment (Larsson & Carlsson, elements of therapy, aiming to modify unhelpful
1996; McGrath et al., 1988; Passchier et al., thoughts, assumptions, and behavioural patterns
1990; Richter et al., 1986). Fichtel and Larsson that are considered to be influencing the head-
(2001) explored the effects of relaxation treat- ache disorder. Usually in headache treatment,
ment in adolescents suffering from migraine and/ CBT focuses on aiding the individual to gain
332 T. Dowell et al.

awareness of the role of their thought processes was not solely due to therapist support, educa-
in the headache cycle and to gain insight into the tion, or nonspecific intervention effects. Sartory
relationships between stress, coping, and and colleagues (1998) compared children aged
headaches. 8–16 with recurrent headaches who received
CBT usually occurs over 8–12 sessions and either pharmacological treatment, stress manage-
may occur in individual, group, or minimal thera- ment combined with progressive muscle relax-
pist contact formats, typically involving daily ation, or vasomotor feedback. Results indicated
home practice. Initial sessions focus on providing that relaxation and stress management were
psycho-education surrounding the role of CBT in favourable in reducing headache activity, with
treating headaches, followed by behavioural 80% of children experiencing clinical improve-
techniques, which may include relaxation tech- ment in this condition, compared to 53.3% in the
niques, biofeedback, pleasant activity schedul- vasomotor feedback group and 41.7% in the
ing, and activity pacing. Cognitive components group receiving pharmacological treatment.
are likely to include problem-solving strategies, A study conducted by Wicksell and colleagues
exploration of the impact of thoughts and feel- (2009) was the first to utilise acceptance and
ings on headache occurrence and pain, strategies commitment therapy in the treatment of chronic
to challenge and modify unhelpful thought pro- pain conditions in children, which included
cesses, and the development of appropriate cop- recurrent headaches. This intervention was based
ing strategies. The development of a maintenance on behavioural and cognitive strategies, although
plan at the conclusion of treatment is considered it emphasised exposure and acceptance strate-
integral to lessen the likelihood of future relapses. gies. The study compared this treatment to a stan-
Current empirical evidence indicates that CBT dard multidisciplinary approach combined with
produces favourable outcomes for children suf- amitriptyline use. Results indicated that the treat-
fering from recurrent headaches, and clinically ment showed substantial and significant improve-
significant changes appear to be durable through- ments across all measures, indicating that it may
out follow-up periods (Griffiths & Martin, 1996; be an efficacious treatment option for headaches
Kröener-Herwig & Denecke, 2002; McGrath in children, though additional research is required
et al., 1992; Osterhaus et al., 1997). Cognitive in this area.
therapy as a stand-alone treatment has been com-
pared to relaxation training by Richter and col-
leagues (1986) to treat children suffering from  verall Efficacy of Psychological
O
migraine. Both treatments were found to be Treatments
equally efficacious and superior to a placebo
condition. The aforementioned psychological treatments for
Research by Powers and colleagues (2013) headaches in children are gaining a base of
explored the value of CBT when administered empirical evidence that supports their efficacy.
concurrently with pharmacological treatment in However, it remains evident that whilst some
children aged 10–17 suffering from migraine. A children do benefit from these treatments, many
treatment group receiving CBT and amitriptyline do not. Osterhaus and colleagues (1997) indi-
was compared with a control group who received cated that only half of the children in their treat-
amitriptyline and headache education. The study ment group achieved clinical improvement in
revealed a greater reduction in headache days and terms of reductions in headache frequency.
disability in children who received CBT in Similarly, Larsson and Carlsson (1996) found
conjunction with amitriptyline, indicating that that a 34% improvement from baseline could be
psychological intervention has important value seen in children who benefited from treatment,
as an adjunct to pharmacological treatment. indicating that headaches still remained a problem
Furthermore, the addition of headache education for this group. Research consistently indicates
in the control group indicates that the effect found that whilst headache frequency is improved by
Headache and Migraine 333

psychological treatment, intensity and duration incorporated information about headaches, the
tend to remain unchanged (Osterhaus et al., 1997; role of stress and other triggers, and lifestyle
Richter et al., 1986), suggesting that whilst these changes and also included a brief guided practice
treatments appear to have value in terms of head- in relaxation techniques. The education session
ache prevention, they are not overly effective was followed by an immediate consultation with
when headaches do occur. a neurologist. This was compared to a standard
A Cochrane review conducted by Eccleston neurologist consultation. Both conditions were
and colleagues (2014) evaluated currently avail- found to be equally effective in terms of informa-
able evidence pertaining to the efficacy of psy- tion received by patients, and the authors con-
chological treatments in treating children with cluded that the intervention was likely too brief
recurrent headaches. Overall, current research is for a significant difference to emerge. Barry and
indicative of a beneficial effect of psychological von Baeyer (1997) explored the efficacy of abbre-
treatment on the reduction of headache pain, viated cognitive therapy for children with head-
which is maintained over time. Whilst improve- aches. This included two 90-min sessions
ments are seen in terms of disability, effect sizes focusing on relaxation, distraction, visualisation,
are generally small (Eccleston et al., 2014). and stress management skills. Results did not
Additionally, the effects of treatment on comor- support this treatment method as efficacious.
bid anxiety and depression were evaluated when Passchier and colleagues (1990) evaluated a
available. In summary, no beneficial effects were relaxation training programme which consisted
found for comorbid depression, and whilst some of four 20 min sessions conducted over 2 weeks
small effects were found for anxiety reduction, and found efficacy in terms of reduction in
these effects were not found to be maintained at tension-­type headaches but limited efficacy for
follow-up (Eccleston et al., 2014). It is noted that reduction in migraines. Whilst evidence is lim-
evidence in this area is currently limited by a pau- ited in this area, preliminary studies suggest that
city of research; however it does appear that cur- interventions of standard length are necessary to
rent treatments do little to alleviate common achieve clinically significant improvements.
comorbid conditions in children with recurrent
headache.
Modality

 ariations in the Psychological
V A rise in minimal therapist contact and self-help
Treatment of Headaches treatments also acknowledges the potential barri-
ers that families may face in receiving treatment
Time Period for headaches. These interventions may utilise
manuals provided to patients or, increasingly, are
Whilst evidence indicates that psychological delivered using technology, which includes
treatment is efficacious in the treatment of recur- computer-­based programmes, internet-based pro-
rent headaches in children, it is recognised that grammes, smartphone apps, audiotapes, and ther-
treatment is often resource intensive and may be apist contact via telephone. These alternative
costly for families in terms of both finances and formats of treatment may be beneficial in terms
time. For these reasons, research has begun to of being widely accessible and self-paced. Whilst
explore the utility of brief interventions in the research investigating the efficacy of these treat-
treatment of headaches, compared to standard ment formats is growing, the empirical database
treatment timeframes that may span over a num- remains small.
ber of months. Self-administered manuals have been utilised
The possibility of a 1-h education session was to administer CBT for a number of years. These
explored by Abram and colleagues (2007), which are usually supplemented by auditory stimuli for
334 T. Dowell et al.

relaxation components and often include weekly Parental Involvement


therapist check-ins via phone. When compared to
identical interventions delivered by a therapist in The impact of parents and caregivers in the
face-to-face formats, the self-administered pro- maintenance of headaches in children is empha-
grammes have been found to be equally effica- sised in the aforementioned functional model of
cious in producing clinically significant headaches, whereby parental responses to their
improvements in children with recurrent head- children’s headaches play an important role in
aches (Griffiths & Martin, 1996; Kröener-Herwig determining outcomes. As such, it follows natu-
& Denecke, 2002; McGrath et al., 1992). rally that parents and caregivers are likely to play
Relaxation training has also been administered in an important role in the intervention process, the
self-administered formats, with results indicating extent of which is likely to vary depending on the
that it is beneficial (Larsson et al., 1987a). developmental stage and needs of the child.
Connelly, Rapoff, Thompson, and Connelly There are two ways in which parents may be
(2006) conducted the first known study investi- involved in the treatment of childhood headaches.
gating the use of a CD-ROM as a medium for One is to assist and support the child in working
paediatric headache treatment delivery (cogni- through the treatment programme. The other rec-
tive behavioural strategies). They found that the ognises that the parent may be playing a role in
treatment produced clinically significant the perpetuation of headaches, and hence treat-
improvements compared to a control group, ment may include modifying parental behaviours
which were maintained at follow-up, though no and responses considered to be dysfunctional.
differences in disability scores were found. The latter form of involvement necessitates an
Rapoff and colleagues (2014) also examined a assessment of parental responses to headaches
cognitive behavioural self-management and has the possibility of being threatening to
CD-ROM intervention and found that whilst the parents. Many existing psychological interven-
intervention resulted in lower headache intensity tions have acknowledged the necessity of paren-
and disability scores, headache frequency and tal involvement in treatment, with varying
quality of life did not differ from an educational degrees of parental incorporation noted within
control group. Cottrell, Drew, Gibson, Holroyd, the current literature.
and O’Donnell (2007) examined the utility of a At the most basic level, parental involvement
telephone-delivered behavioural management may be limited to assisting the child to complete
programme for adolescents with migraine. tasks associated with the intervention. This was
Treatment effects were large in terms of reduc- utilised in research by Rapoff and colleagues
tions in the number of migraines and disability (2014), where parental involvement had the pri-
equivalent hours, indicating that this is likely to mary goal of assisting the child to complete a
be an efficacious format of treatment for adoles- self-help manual and to aid with any difficulties
cents. Web-based manuals and interventions encountered. Whilst parents play a passive role in
have been examined in treating children with a this form of treatment, there exists an opportunity
multitude of pain problems, including headache, to become educated about the child’s programme,
and have been shown to be significantly superior which is likely to be beneficial. Parents may also
to waitlist control groups (Hicks, Von Baeyer, & be involved on a partial level, where they are
McGrath, 2006; Palermo, Wilson, Peters, required to attend a number of the child’s treat-
Lewandowski, & Somhegyi, 2009; Trautmann & ment sessions, in order to become informed about
Kröner-Herwig, 2010). A Cochrane review con- the material and skills being taught and to facili-
ducted by Fisher, Law, Palermo, and Eccleston tate the practice of these skills in the home envi-
(2015) concluded that remotely delivered treat- ronment (Powers et al., 2003). Additionally,
ment for headaches is a promising area, though parents may be assigned entire sections of the
additional empirical evidence is necessary in child’s manual to complete (Cottrell et al., 2007;
order to be confident of its efficacy. Hicks et al., 2006).
Headache and Migraine 335

Perhaps the most extensive incorporation of scale studies are warranted. Furthermore, much
caregivers in treatment to date has occurred in research to date has focused on the outcomes of
research conducted by Palermo and colleagues headache frequency, duration, and intensity;
(2009), which utilised a family CBT model, however, comorbid conditions such as anxiety
acknowledging the key role that parents may play and depression are common in this population,
in maintaining children’s pain conditions. and the research available indicates that current
Treatment involved eight modules to be com- treatments do little to address these conditions. It
pleted by parents, which covered topics such as would likely be beneficial for future research to
adaptive communication and interaction patterns, focus on developing interventions that success-
education surrounding operant procedures and fully treat such comorbid conditions concur-
appropriate reinforcement, modelling appropriate rently. There are also many possible variations to
behaviours, and supporting the child’s indepen- treatment that are worthy of exploration, particu-
dence. Despite the inclusion of caregivers in inter- larly in terms of minimal therapist contact meth-
ventions, there is currently no literature comparing ods, for which there is a need for additional
these varying levels of involvement, and as such it quality research.
is difficult to empirically comment on the effect When considering the function model of head-
that this has on treatment outcomes. aches in children, it is evident that precipitating
factors, or triggers, are rarely addressed in cur-
rent psychological treatments. Medical advice
Future Directions has traditionally counselled an avoidance of trig-
for the Psychological Treatment gers in order to reduce headache frequency
of Headaches in Children (Friedman & De Ver Dye, 2009). However, an
emerging trigger avoidance model of headaches
Whilst the assessment and treatment of head- has indicated that avoidance of triggers could
aches in children are gaining traction, research in result in a sensitising effect, whereby the trig-
the area remains limited, with many areas war- ger’s capacity to elicit a headache in an individ-
ranting further attention. In terms of the classifi- ual becomes stronger (Martin, 2000, 2001).
cation system, there is a need for additional Laboratory studies have investigated the relation-
research to ascertain how the current diagnostic ship between exposure to triggers and headaches
system, which has been developed for adults, and have provided confirmatory evidence for this
applies to children and adolescents. There is also theory for stress (Martin, Lae, & Reece, 2007),
an ongoing need for additional research to better visual disturbance (Martin, 2000, 2001), and
understand the mechanisms that underlie head- noise (Martin, Reece, & Forsyth, 2006). In
aches, as well as the influences from a broader response to this research, a novel approach to
psychosocial context. Additionally, assessment treating headache called learning to cope with
of headaches in children warrants further triggers has been developed. This approach uti-
research, particularly in order to ensure that elec- lises graduated exposure techniques to promote
tronic measurement methods that are commonly individual desensitisation to triggers when
used are psychometrically sound. deemed appropriate. A randomised control trial
The treatment of headaches in children is per- conducted by Martin and colleagues (2014)
haps the realm warranting the most empirical found this approach to be superior to the avoid-
attention, due to a general lack of research con- ance of triggers. Despite promising gains in this
ducted in this area. Pharmacologically, there is a area, research is yet to evaluate the efficacy of
need for trials to establish the efficacy of current this approach in the paediatric population.
treatments in the paediatric population. Despite growing research interest in the treat-
Psychological treatments including relaxation, ment of headaches in children, relatively little is
biofeedback, and CBT appear to be efficacious known about why treatments work for some
within this population, though additional large-­ ­children and not others. Research by Osterhaus
336 T. Dowell et al.

and colleagues (1997) indicated that the most Aromaa, M., Rautava, P., Helenius, H., & Sillanpää, M. L.
(1998). Factors of early life as predictors of headache
important predictor of treatment success was
in children at school entry. Headache: The Journal of
length of headache history, whereby those with Head and Face Pain, 38(1), 23–30.
shorter periods of time spent experiencing head- Asmundson, G. J., Noel, M., Petter, M., & Parkerson,
aches were more likely to benefit from treatment. H. A. (2012). Pediatric fear-avoidance model of
chronic pain: Foundation, application and future
Additionally, maternal rewarding of illness behav-
directions. Pain Research and Management, 17(6),
iours and positive mother-child relationships were 397–405.
indicated as risk factors for poorer outcomes Barry, J., & von Baeyer, C. L. (1997). Brief cognitive-­
­following treatment. These findings highlight the behavioral group treatment for children’s headache.
The Clinical Journal of Pain, 13(3), 215–220.
importance of early intervention, assessing the
Bendtsen, L., & Jensen, R. (2006). Tension-type head-
possible role of parent behaviours in a child’s ache: The most common, but also the most neglected,
headache cycle, as well as including parents in headache disorder. Current Opinion in Neurology, 19,
treatment plans as necessary. Considering the huge 305–309.
Bendtsen, L., Ashina, S., Moore, A., & Steiner, T. J.
cost and increasing incidence of childhood head-
(2016). Muscles and their role in episodic tension-­
ache disorders, it is thought that ongoing research type headache: Implications for treatment. European
and the development of effective treatment inter- Journal of Pain, 20(2), 166–175.
ventions are of critical importance to headache Bille, B. O. (1962). Migraine in school children. Acta
Paediatrica, 51(5), 614–616.
sufferers, their families, and the wider society.
Bonfert, M., Straube, A., Schroeder, A. S., Reilich, P.,
Ebinger, F., & Heinen, F. (2013). Primary headache
in children and adolescents: Update on pharma-
cotherapy of migraine and tension-type headache.
References Neuropediatrics, 44(01), 003–019.
Brna, P., Dooley, J., Gordon, K., & Dewan, T. (2005). The
Abram, H. S., Buckloh, L. M., Schilling, L. M., Wiltrout, prognosis of childhood headache: A 20-year follow-
S. A., Ramírez-Garnica, G., & Turk, W. R. (2007). A ­up. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine,
randomized, controlled trial of a neurological and psy- 159(12), 1157–1160.
choeducational group appointment model for pediatric Bussone, G. (2004). Pathophysiology of migraine.
headaches. Children’s Healthcare, 36(3), 249–265. Neurological Sciences, 25(3), 239–241.
Aegidius, K. L., Zwart, J. A., Hagen, K., Dyb, G., Holmen, Bussone, G., Grazzi, L., D’Amico, D., Leone, M., &
T. L., & Stovner, L. J. (2011). Increased headache Andrasik, F. (1998). Biofeedback-assisted relaxation
prevalence in female adolescents and adult women training for young adolescents with tension-type
with early menarche. The Head-HUNT Studies. headache: A controlled study. Cephalalgia, 18(7),
European Journal of Neurology, 18(2), 321–328. 463–467.
Albers, L., Straube, A., Landgraf, M. N., Heinen, F., & Claar, R. L., Simons, L. E., & Logan, D. E. (2008).
von Kries, R. (2014). High diagnostic stability of con- Parental response to children’s pain: the moderating
firmed migraine and confirmed tension-type headache impact of children’s emotional distress on symptoms
according to the ICHD-3 beta in adolescents. Journal and disability. Pain, 138(1), 172–179.
of Headache and Pain, 15(1), 1. Connelly, M., & Bickel, J. (2011). An electronic daily
Albers, L., von Kries, R., Heinen, F., & Straube, A. (2015). diary process study of stress and health behavior trig-
Headache in school children: Is the prevalence increas- gers of primary headaches in children. Journal of
ing? Current Pain and Headache Reports, 19(3), 1–9. Pediatric Psychology, 36(8), 852–862.
Antonaci, F., Voiticovschi-Iosob, C., Di Stefano, A. L., Connelly, M., & Rapoff, M. A. (2006). Assessing health-­
Galli, F., Ozge, A., & Balottin, U. (2014). The evolution related quality of life in children with recurrent head-
of headache from childhood to adulthood: A review of ache: Reliability and validity of the PedsQL™ 4.0 in
the literature. The Journal of Headache and Pain, 15(1), a pediatric headache sample. Journal of Pediatric
1–11. Psychology, 31(7), 698–702.
Anttila, P. (2006). Tension-type headache in childhood and Connelly, M., Rapoff, M. A., Thompson, N., & Connelly,
adolescence. The Lancet Neurology, 5(3), 268–274. W. (2006). Headstrong: A pilot study of a CD-ROM
Anttila, P., Metsähonkala, L., Helenius, H., & Sillanpää, intervention for recurrent pediatric headache. Journal
M. (2000). Predisposing and provoking factors in of Pediatric Psychology, 31(7), 737–747.
childhood headache. Headache: The Journal of Head Connelly, M., Miller, T., Gerry, G., & Bickel, J. (2010).
and Face Pain, 40(5), 351–356. Electronic momentary assessment of weather changes
Headache and Migraine 337

as a trigger of headaches in children. Headache: The Hershey, A. D., Powers, S. W., Vockell, A. L., LeCates,
Journal of Head and Face Pain, 50(5), 779–789. S., Kabbouche, M. A., & Maynard, M. K. (2001).
Cottrell, C., Drew, J., Gibson, J., Holroyd, K., & PedMIDAS development of a questionnaire to assess
O’Donnell, F. (2007). Feasibility assessment of disability of migraines in children. Neurology, 57(11),
telephone-­ administered behavioral treatment for 2034–2039.
­adolescent migraine. Headache: The Journal of Head Hershey, A. D., Kabbouche, M. A., & Powers, S. W.
and Face Pain, 47(9), 1293–1302. (2010). Treatment of pediatric and adolescent
Eccleston, C., Palermo, T. M., Williams, A. C. D. C., migraine. Pediatric Annals, 39(7), 416–423.
Lewandowski Holley, A., Morley, S., Fisher, E., & Heyer, G. L., Perkins, S. Q., Rose, S. C., Aylward, S. C.,
Law, E. (2014). Psychological therapies for the man- & Lee, J. M. (2014). Comparing patient and parent
agement of chronic and recurrent pain in children and recall of 90-day and 30-day migraine disability using
adolescents. The Cochrane Library, 5, CD003968. elements of the PedMIDAS and an internet headache
Fichtel, Å., & Larsson, B. (2001). Does relaxation treat- diary. Cephalalgia, 34(4), 298–306.
ment have differential effects on migraine and tension-­ Hicks, C. L., Von Baeyer, C. L., & McGrath, P. J. (2006).
type headache in adolescents? Headache: The Journal Online psychological treatment for pediatric recurrent
of Head and Face Pain, 41(3), 290–296. pain: A randomized evaluation. Journal of Pediatric
Fielding, J., Young, S., Martin, P. R., & Waters, A. M. Psychology, 31(7), 724–736.
(2016). Headache symptoms consistent with migraine Ho, T. W., Pearlman, E., Lewis, D., Hämäläinen, M.,
and tension-type headaches in children with anxiety Connor, K., Michelson, D., …, Bachman, R. (2012).
disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 40, 67–74. Efficacy and tolerability of rizatriptan in pediatric
Fisher, E., Law, E., Palermo, T. M., & Eccleston, C. migraineurs: Results from a randomized, double-­
(2015). Psychological therapies (remotely delivered) blind, placebo-controlled trial using a novel adap-
for the management of chronic and recurrent pain tive enrichment design. Cephalalgia, 32, 750.
in children and adolescents. The Cochrane Library, 0333102412451358.
2014, CD011118. Holden, W. E., Levy, J. D., Deichmann, M. M., & Gladstein,
Fraga, M. D. B., Pinho, R. S., Andreoni, S., Vitalle, M. S. J. (1998). Recurrent pediatric headaches: Assessment
D. S., Fisberg, M., Peres, M. F. P., ..., Masruha, M. R. and intervention. Journal of Developmental &
(2013). Trigger factors mainly from the environmen- Behavioral Pediatrics, 19(2), 109–116.
tal type are reported by adolescents with migraine. Holroyd, K. A., Penzien, D. B., Hursey, K. G., Tobin,
Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 71(5), 290–293. D. L., Rogers, L., Holm, J. E., ..., Chila, A. G. (1984).
Friedman, D., & De Ver Dye, T. (2009). Migraine and the Change mechanisms in EMG biofeedback training:
environment. Headache, 49, 941–952. Cognitive changes underlying improvements in ten-
Gervil, M., Ulrich, V., Kyvik, K. O., Olesen, J., & Russell, sion headache. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
M. B. (1999). Migraine without aura: A population-­ Psychology, 52(6), 1039.
based twin study. Annals of Neurology, 46, 606–611. Kabbouche, M. A., & Gilman, D. K. (2008). Management
Griffiths, J. D., & Martin, P. R. (1996). Clinical‐ver- of migraine in adolescents. Neuropsychiatric Disease
sus home‐based treatment formats for children and Treatment, 4(3), 535.
with chronic headache. British Journal of Health Kaiser, R. S., & Primavera, J. P. (1993). Failure to mourn
Psychology, 1(2), 151–166. as a possible contributory factor to headache onset
Guidetti, V., Galli, F., Fabrizi, P., Giannantoni, A. S., in adolescence. Headache: The Journal of Head and
Napoli, L., Bruni, O., & Trillo, S. (1998). Headache Face Pain, 33(2), 69–72.
and psychiatric comorbidity: Clinical aspects and Karlson, C. W., Litzenburg, C. C., Sampilo, M. L.,
outcome in an 8-year follow-up study. Cephalalgia, Rapoff, M. A., Connelly, M., Bickel, J. L., ..., Powers,
18(7), 455–462. S. W. (2013). Relationship between daily mood and
Hadjikhani, N., del Rio, M. S., Wu, O., Schwartz, D., migraine in children. Headache: The Journal of Head
Bakker, D., Fischl, B., ..., Sorensen, A. G. (2001). and Face Pain, 53(10), 1624–1634.
Mechanisms of migraine aura revealed by functional Kelman, L. (2007). The triggers or precipitants of the
MRI in human visual cortex. Proceedings of the acute migraine attack. Cephalalgia, 27(5), 394–402.
National Academy of Sciences, 98(8), 4687–4692. Kernick, D., & Campbell, J. (2009). Measuring the impact
Headache Classification Committee of the International of headache in children: A critical review of the litera-
Headache Society. (2013). The international classi- ture. Cephalalgia, 29(1), 3–16.
fication of headache disorders, 3rd edition (beta ver- Kienbacher, C. H., Wöber, C. H., Zesch, H. E., Hafferl-­
sion). Cephalagia, 33(9), 629–808. Gattermayer, A., Posch, M., Karwautz, A., ..., Wöber-­
Hernandez-Latorre, M. A., & Roig, M. (2000). Natural Bingöl, Ç. (2006). Clinical features, classification
history of migraine in childhood. Cephalalgia, 20(6), and prognosis of migraine and tension-type headache
573–579. in children and adolescents: A long-term follow-up
Hershey, A. D. (2010). Current approaches to the diag- study. Cephalalgia, 26(7), 820–830.
nosis and management of paediatric migraine. The Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1977). An empirical-clinical
Lancet Neurology, 9(2), 190–204. approach toward a definition of childhood depression.
338 T. Dowell et al.

In J. G. Schulterbrandt & A. Raskin (Eds.), Depression Lipchik, G. L., Holroyd, K. A., O’Donnell, F. J.,
in children: Diagnosis, treatment, and concept mod- Cordingley, G. E., Waller, S., Labus, J., ..., French,
els. New York, NY: Raven. D. J. (2000). Exteroceptive suppression periods and
Kröener-Herwig, B., & Denecke, H. (2002). Cognitive– pericranial muscle tenderness in chronic tension-type
behavioral therapy of pediatric headache: Are there dif- headache: Effects of psychopathology, chronicity and
ferences in efficacy between a therapist-­administered disability. Cephalalgia, 20, 638–646.
group training and a self-help format? Journal of Lundqvist, C., Clench-Aas, J., Hofoss, D., & Bartonova,
Psychosomatic Research, 53(6), 1107–1114. A. (2006). Self-reported headache in schoolchildren:
Krogh, A. B., Larsson, B., Salvesen, Ø., & Linde, M. Parents underestimate their children’s headaches. Acta
(2015). A comparison between prospective internet-­ Paediatrica, 95(8), 940–946.
based and paper diary recordings of headache among March, J. S., Parker, J. D., Sullivan, K., Stallings, P., &
adolescents in the general population. Cephalalgia, Conners, C. K. (1997). The Multidimensional Anxiety
36, 335. 0333102415591506. Scale for Children (MASC): Factor structure, reliabil-
Kröner-Herwig, B., & Gassmann, J. (2012). Headache ity, and validity. Journal of the American Academy of
disorders in children and adolescents: Their asso- Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 554–565.
ciation with psychological, behavioral, and socio-­ Martin, P. R. (1993). Psychological management of
environmental factors. Headache: The Journal of chronic headaches. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Head and Face Pain, 52(9), 1387–1401. Martin, P. R. (2000). Headache triggers: To avoid or not
Kröner-Herwig, B., & Vath, N. (2009). Menarche in girls to avoid, that is the question. Psychology and Health,
and headache–a longitudinal analysis. Headache: The 15(6), 801–809.
Journal of Head and Face Pain, 49(6), 860–867. Martin, P. R. (2001). How do trigger factors acquire
Labbé, E. E. (1995). Treatment of childhood migraine the capacity to precipitate headaches? Behaviour
with autogenic training and skin temperature biofeed- Research and Therapy, 39(5), 545–554.
back: A component analysis. Headache: The Journal Martin, P. R. (2013). Psychological management of the
of Head and Face Pain, 35(1), 10–13. common primary headaches. In M. L. Caltabiano
Labbé, E. L., & Williamson, D. A. (1984). Treatment of & L. A. Ricciardelli (Eds.), Applied topics in
childhood migraine using autogenic feedback train- health psychology (pp. 476–462). Chichester, UK:
ing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Wiley-Blackwell.
52(6), 968. Martin, P. R., Milech, D., & Nathan, P. R. (1993). Towards
Lake, A. E. (1981). Behavioral assessment consider- a functional model of chronic headaches: Investigation
ations in the management of headache. Headache, 21, of antecedents and consequences. Headache, 33,
170–178. 461–470.
Langeveld, J. H., Koot, H. M., Loonen, M. C. B., Martin, P. R., Reece, J., & Forsyth, M. (2006). Noise as a
Hazebroek-Kampschreur, A. A. J. M., & Passchier, trigger for headaches: Relationship between exposure
J. (1996). A quality of life instrument for adolescents and sensitivity. Headache: The Journal of Head and
with chronic headache. Cephalalgia, 16(3), 183–196. Face Pain, 46(6), 962–972.
Larsson, B., & Carlsson, J. (1996). A school-based, nurse-­ Martin, P. R., Lae, L., & Reece, J. (2007). Stress as a trig-
administered relaxation training for children with ger for headaches: Relationship between exposure
chronic tension-type headache. Journal of Pediatric and sensitivity. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 20(4),
Psychology, 21(5), 603–614. 393–407.
Larsson, B., & Stinson, J. N. (2011). Commentary: On Martin, P. R., Reece, J., Callan, M., MacLeod, C., Kaur,
the importance of using prospective diary data in the A., Gregg, K., & Goadsby, P. J. (2014). Behavioral
assessment of recurrent headaches, stressors, and management of the triggers of recurrent headache: A
health behaviors in children and adolescents. Journal randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and
of Pediatric Psychology, 36, 863. jsr034. Therapy, 61, 1–11.
Larsson, B., Daleflod, B., Håkansson, L., & Melin, L. Maytal, J., Young, M., Shechter, A., & Lipton, R. B.
(1987a). Therapist-assisted versus self-help relax- (1997). Pediatric migraine and the International
ation treatment of chronic headaches in adoles- Headache Society (IHS) criteria. Neurology, 48(3),
cents: A school-based intervention. Journal of Child 602–607.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(1), 127–136. Mazzone, L., Vitiello, B., Incorpora, G., & Mazzone, D.
Larsson, B., Melin, L., Lamminen, M., & Ullstedt, F. (2006). Behavioural and temperamental character-
(1987b). A school-based treatment of chronic head- istics of children and adolescents suffering from pri-
aches in adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, mary headache. Cephalalgia, 26(2), 194–201.
12(4), 553–566. Mazzotta, S., Pavlidis, E., Cordori, C., Spagnoli, C.,
Lima, M. M. F., Bazan, R., Martin, L. C., Martins, A. S., Pini, L. A., & Pisani, F. (2015). Children’s headache:
Luvizutto, G. J., Betting, L. E. G. G., & Zanini, M. A. Drawings in the diagnostic work up. Neuropediatrics,
(2015). Critical analysis of diagnostic criteria (ICHD-3 46(04), 261–268.
beta) about migraine in childhood and adolescence. McAbee, G. N., Morse, A. M., & Assadi, M. (2016).
Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 73(12), 1005–1008. Pediatric aspects of headache classification in the
Headache and Migraine 339

International Classification of Headache Disorders—3 ioral therapy intervention for children and adolescents
(ICHD-3 beta version). Current Pain and Headache with chronic pain. Pain, 146(1), 205–213.
Reports, 20(1), 1–6. Passchier, J., Bree, M. B. M., Emmen, H. H., Osterhaus,
McGrath, P. J., Humphreys, P., Goodman, J. T., Keene, D., S. O. L., Orlebeke, J. F., & Verhage, F. (1990).
Firestone, P., Jacob, P., & Cunningham, S. J. (1988). Relaxation training in school classes does not reduce
Relaxation prophylaxis for childhood migraine: A headache complaints. Headache: The Journal of Head
randomized placebo-controlled trial. Developmental and Face Pain, 30(10), 660–664.
Medicine & Child Neurology, 30(5), 626–631. Peterson, C. C., & Palermo, T. M. (2004). Parental rein-
McGrath, P. J., Humphreys, P., Keene, D., Goodman, J. T., forcement of recurrent pain: The moderating impact of
Lascelles, M. A., Cunningham, S. J., & Firestone, P. child depression and anxiety on functional disability.
(1992). The efficacy and efficiency of a self-­administered Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 29(5), 331–341.
treatment for adolescent migraine. Pain, 49(3), 321–324. Powers, S. W., Patton, S. R., Hommel, K. A., & Hershey,
McGrath, P. A., Seifert, C. E., Speechley, K. N., Booth, A. D. (2003). Quality of life in childhood migraines:
J. C., Stitt, L., & Gibson, M. C. (1996). A new ana- Clinical impact and comparison to other chronic ill-
logue scale for assessing children's pain: An initial nesses. Pediatrics, 112(1), e1–e5.
validation study. Pain, 64(3), 435–443. Powers, S. W., Gilman, D. K., & Hershey, A. D. (2006).
Milde-Busch, A., Boneberger, A., Heinrich, S., Thomas, Headache and psychological functioning in children
S., Kühnlein, A., Radon, K., ..., Von Kries, R. (2010). and adolescents. Headache: The Journal of Head and
Higher prevalence of psychopathological symptoms in Face Pain, 46(9), 1404–1415.
adolescents with headache. A population-based cross-­ Powers, S. W., Kashikar-Zuck, S. M., Allen, J. R.,
sectional study. Headache: The Journal of Head and LeCates, S. L., Slater, S. K., Zafar, M., ..., Hershey,
Face Pain, 50(5), 738–748. A. D. (2013). Cognitive behavioral therapy plus ami-
Miller, V. A., Palermo, T. M., Powers, S. W., Scher, M. S., triptyline for chronic migraine in children and adoles-
& Hershey, A. D. (2003). Migraine headaches and cents: A randomized clinical trial. The Journal of the
sleep disturbances in children. Headache: The Journal American Medical Association, 310(24), 2622–2630.
of Head and Face Pain, 43(4), 362–368. Ramsden, R., Friedman, B., & Williamson, D. (1983).
Mortimer, M. J., Kay, J., & Jaron, A. (1992). Childhood Treatment of childhood headache reports with con-
migraine in general practice: Clinical features and tingency management procedures. Journal of Clinical
characteristics. Cephalalgia, 12(4), 238–243. Child & Adolescent Psychology, 12(2), 202–206.
Neut, D., Fily, A., Cuvellier, J. C., & Vallée, L. (2012). Rapoff, M. A., Connelly, M., Bickel, J. L., Powers, S. W.,
The prevalence of triggers in paediatric migraine: A Hershey, A. D., Allen, J. R., ..., Belmont, J. M. (2014).
questionnaire study in 102 children and adolescents. Headstrong intervention for pediatric migraine head-
The Journal of Headache and Pain, 13(1), 61–65. ache: A randomized clinical trial. The Journal of
Ostergaard, S., Russell, M. B., Bendtsen, L., & Olesen, Headache and Pain, 15(1), 1–10.
J. (1997). Comparison of first degree relatives and Richter, I. L., McGrath, P. J., Humphreys, P. J., Goodman,
spouses of people with chronic tension headache. J. T., Firestone, P., & Keene, D. (1986). Cognitive
BMJ: British Medical Journal, 314(7087), 1092. and relaxation treatment of paediatric migraine. Pain,
Osterhaus, S. O. L., & Passchier, J. (1992). The optimal 25(2), 195–203.
length of headache recording in juvenile migraine Rothner, A. D., & Parikh, S. (2016). Migraine variants
patients. Cephalalgia, 12(5), 297–299. or episodic syndromes that may be associated with
Osterhaus, S. O., Lange, A., Linssen, W. H., & Passchier, migraine and other unusual pediatric headache syn-
J. (1997). A behavioral treatment of young migrainous dromes. Headache: The Journal of Head and Face
and nonmigrainous headache patients: Prediction of Pain, 56(1), 206–214.
treatment success. International Journal of Behavioral Russell, M. B., Østergaard, S., Bendtsen, L., & Olesen,
Medicine, 4(4), 378–396. J. (1999). Familial occurrence of chronic tension-type
Özge, A., Termine, C., Antonaci, F., Natriashvili, S., headache. Cephalalgia, 19(4), 207–210.
Guidetti, V., & Wöber-Bingöl, C. (2011). Overview Russell, M. B., Ulrich, V., Gervil, M., & Olesen, J. (2002).
of diagnosis and management of paediatric headache. Migraine without aura and migraine with aura are
Part I: Diagnosis. The Journal of Headache and Pain, distinct disorders. A population-based twin survey.
12(1), 13–23. Headache: The Journal of Head and Face Pain, 42(5),
Palermo, T. M., Valenzuela, D., & Stork, P. P. (2004). A 332–336.
randomized trial of electronic versus paper pain dia- Sartory, G., Müller, B., Metsch, J., & Pothmann, R.
ries in children: Impact on compliance, accuracy, and (1998). A comparison of psychological and pharma-
acceptability. Pain, 107(3), 213–219. cological treatment of pediatric migraine. Behaviour
Palermo, T. M., Wilson, A. C., Peters, M., Lewandowski, Research and Therapy, 36(12), 1155–1170.
A., & Somhegyi, H. (2009). Randomized controlled Sasmaz, T., Bugdayci, R., Ozge, A., Karakelle, A., Kurt,
trial of an internet-delivered family cognitive–behav- O., & Kaleagasi, H. (2004). Are parents aware of their
340 T. Dowell et al.

schoolchildren’s headaches? The European Journal of for recurrent headache in childhood and adolescence.
Public Health, 14(4), 366–368. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(1), 28–37.
Scharff, L., Marcus, D. A., & Masek, B. J. (2002). A Ulrich, V., Gervil, M., Kyvik, K. O., Olesen, J., & Russell,
controlled study of minimal-contact thermal biofeed- M. B. (1999). Evidence of a genetic factor in migraine
back treatment in children with migraine. Journal of with aura: A population-based Danish twin study.
Pediatric Psychology, 27(2), 109–119. Annals of Neurology, 45, 242–246.
Shevel, E. (2011). The extracranial vascular theory of Ulrich, V., Gervil, M., & Olesen, J. (2004). The rela-
migraine—a great story confirmed by the facts. tive influence of environment and genes in episodic
Headache: The Journal of Head and Face Pain, 51(3), tension-­type headache. Neurology, 62, 2065–2069.
409–417. Varni, J. W., Seid, M., & Kurtin, P. S. (2001). PedsQL™
Siniatchkin, M., Reich, A. L., Shepherd, A. J., van Baalen, 4.0: Reliability and validity of the pediatric quality
A., Siebner, H. R., & Stephani, U. (2009). Peri-ictal of life inventory™ version 4.0 generic core scales in
changes of cortical excitability in children suffering healthy and patient populations. Medical Care, 39(8),
from migraine without aura. PAIN®, 147(1), 132–140. 800–812.
Spence, S. H. (1998). A measure of anxiety symptoms Varni, J. W., Burwinkle, T. M., & Lane, M. M. (2005).
among children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Health-related quality of life measurement in pediatric
36(5), 545–566. clinical practice: An appraisal and precept for future
Stafstrom, C. E., Rostasy, K., & Minster, A. (2002). The research and application. Health and Quality of Life
usefulness of children’s drawings in the diagnosis of Outcomes, 3(1), 1.
headache. Pediatrics, 109(3), 460–472. Vervoort, T., Goubert, L., Eccleston, C., Bijttebier, P.,
Stafstrom, C. E., Goldenholz, S. R., & Dulli, D. A. (2005). & Crombez, G. (2006). Catastrophic thinking about
Serial headache drawings by children with migraine: pain is independently associated with pain severity,
Correlation with clinical headache status. Journal of disability, and somatic complaints in school children
Child Neurology, 20(10), 809–813. and children with chronic pain. Journal of Pediatric
Stinson, J. N., Kavanagh, T., Yamada, J., Gill, N., & Stevens, Psychology, 31(7), 674–683.
B. (2006). Systematic review of the psychometric prop- Walker, L. S., & Greene, J. W. (1991). The functional dis-
erties, interpretability and feasibility of self-report pain ability inventory: Measuring a neglected dimension of
intensity measures for use in clinical trials in children and child health status. Journal of Pediatric Psychology,
adolescents. Pain, 125(1), 143–157. 16(1), 39–58.
Tedeschi, G., Russo, A., & Tessitore, A. (2013). Wicksell, R. K., Melin, L., Lekander, M., & Olsson, G. L.
Relevance of functional neuroimaging studies for (2009). Evaluating the effectiveness of exposure and
understanding migraine mechanisms. Expert Review acceptance strategies to improve functioning and qual-
of Neurotherapeutics, 13(3), 275–285. ity of life in longstanding pediatric pain–a randomized
Termine, C., Özge, A., Antonaci, F., Natriashvili, S., controlled trial. Pain, 141(3), 248–257.
Guidetti, V., & Wöber-Bingöl, C. (2011). Overview Winner, P., & Hershey, A. D. (2006). Diagnosing
of diagnosis and management of paediatric headache. migraine in the pediatric population. Current Pain and
Part II: Therapeutic management. The Journal of Headache Reports, 10(5), 363–369.
Headache and Pain, 12(1), 25–34. Wöber-Bingöl, Ç., Wöber, C., Uluduz, D., Uygunoğlu,
Tietjen, G. E., Brandes, J. L., Lee Peterlin, B., Eloff, A., U., Aslan, T. S., Kernmayer, M., ..., Steiner, T. J.
Dafer, R. M., Stein, M. R., ..., Recober, A. (2010). (2014). The global burden of headache in children and
Childhood maltreatment and migraine (part II). adolescents–developing a questionnaire and method-
Emotional abuse as a risk factor for headache chroni- ology for a global study. The Journal of Headache and
fication. Headache: The Journal of Head and Face Pain, 15(1), 1.
Pain, 50(1), 32–41. Yen, S., & McIntire, R. W. (1971). Operant therapy for
Trautmann, E., & Kröner-Herwig, B. (2010). A random- constant headache complaint: A simple response-cost
ized controlled trial of internet-based self-help training approach. Psychological Reports, 28, 267–270.
Eating Disorders

Juliet K. Rosewall, Janet D. Latner,
Suman Ambwani, and David H. Gleaves

Contents Introduction
Introduction   341
Eating problems or irregularities are common
Anorexia Nervosa (AN)   342
among children and adolescents. When the
Bulimia Nervosa (BN)   343 problems reach the point of being gross distur-
Binge-Eating Disorder (BED)   343 bances in eating behavior and when accompa-
Atypical Eating Disorder Variants.......................  344
nied by some form of body image disturbance,
we enter the realm of the eating disorders
Epidemiology .........................................................  345 (EDs). The current Diagnostic and Statistical
Interventions and Empirical Evidence.................  349 Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American
Concluding Remarks ............................................  359 Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013) includes
a chapter called “Feeding and Eating Disorders,”
References ..............................................................  359
which comprises eight disorders: pica, rumina-
tion disorder, avoidant/restrictive food intake
disorder, anorexia nervosa (AN), bulimia ner-
J.K. Rosewall vosa (BN), binge eating disorder (BED), and
Population Health Research Institute, St. George’s, two residual diagnoses. In this chapter, we
University of London, London, UK review AN, BN, BED, and their variants (which
J.D. Latner may fall into the two residual categories). See
Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii, Hong and Dixon (this volume) for a discussion
Honolulu, HI, USA of pica and Penrod and Fryling (this volume) or
S. Ambwani Bryant-Waugh (2013) for a discussion of other
Department of Psychology, Dickinson College, feeding disorders. Another eating-related prob-
Carlisle, PA, USA
lem among children is obesity, but a discussion
D.H. Gleaves (*) of obesity and related problems is beyond the
School of Psychology, Social Work and Social Policy,
University of South Australia, scope of the current chapter. See Altman and
Adelaide, SA, Australia Wilfley (2015) for a review of the literature on
e-mail: david.gleaves@unisa.edu.au treatment of childhood obesity.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 341


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_20
342 J.K. Rosewall et al.

Anorexia Nervosa (AN) somewhat arbitrary, weight criteria (e.g., body


mass index <=17.5; BMI; weight in kilograms/
The central feature of AN is a persistent “restric- height in meters2); however, with the DSM-5,
tion of energy intake relative to requirements, there is no minimum. Severity of the disorder can
leading to a significantly low body weight” (APA, be specified based on current BMI (see Table 1).
2013, p. 338). The full DSM-5 diagnostic criteria For this chapter, it is critical to note that absolute
are listed in Table 1. Significantly low body BMIs may not be suitable to use with children
weight is defined as “less than minimally and younger adolescents given that they are still
expected” (p. 338) for children and adolescents. growing, and BMI does not account for sex and
Past versions of the DSM have used specific, and age expected norms. Assessing deviation from
expected body weight, through looking at age-
Table 1  DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for anorexia nervosa and gender-adjusted BMI percentiles, may be
A. Restriction of energy intake relative to requirements,
more appropriate for this age group (Le Grange
leading to a significantly low body weight in the et al., 2012). A young person may be at a minimal
context of age, sex, developmental trajectory, and body size through having lost weight or through
physical health. Significantly low weight is defined never having gained the weight that would be
as a weight that is less than minimally normal or, for
children and adolescents, less than that minimally
expected with normal development. The latter
expected may be more common among children and young
B. Intense fear of gaining weight or of becoming fat, or adolescents, although it would not be uncommon
persistent behavior that interferes with weight gain, for older adolescents to lose a significant amount
even though at a significantly low weight of weight. Persons diagnosed as having AN may
C. Disturbance in the way in which one’s body weight use a variety of weight control mechanisms
or shape is experienced, undue influence of body
weight or shape on self-evaluation, or persistent lack including intentional starvation, excessive exer-
of recognition of the seriousness of the current low cising, or purgative behaviors (self-induced vom-
body weight iting and misuse of laxatives, diuretics, or
Restricting type: during the last 3 months, the enemas).
individual has not engaged in recurrent episodes of
In addition to the persistent restriction of
binge eating or purging behavior (i.e., self-induced
vomiting or the misuse of laxatives, diuretics, or energy intake, individuals with AN also experi-
enemas). This subtype describes presentations in which ence an intense fear of becoming fat or some-
weight loss is accomplished primarily through dieting, times of gaining any weight (APA, 2013). This
fasting, and/or excessive exercise
fear does not seem to diminish, and may even
Binge eating/purging type: during the last 3 months, the
individual has engaged in recurrent episodes of binge
worsen, as the individual loses weight. Individuals
eating or purging behavior (i.e., self-induced vomiting with AN may experience their bodies in a dis-
or the misuse of laxatives, diuretics, or enemas) torted way, may feel fat or extremely dissatisfied
Specify current severity: The minimum level of severity with their body shape/weight, and may base their
is based, for adults, on current body mass index (BMI) total self-worth on their body size. Such individu-
(see below) or, for children and adolescents, on BMI
percentile. The ranges below are derived from World als may closely monitor their body weight and
Health Organization categories for thinness in adults; shape and experience strong negative emotional
for children and adolescents, corresponding BMI reactions if they gain weight. Another common
percentiles should be used. The level of severity may be feature of AN (in postmenarchal girls), although
increased to reflect clinical symptoms, the degree of
functional disability, and the need for supervision no longer a diagnostic criterion, is the presence
Mild: BMI > 17 kg/m2 of amenorrhea. In young girls with AN, the onset
Moderate: BMI = 16–16.99 kg/m2 of menstruation may be delayed.
Severe: BMI = 15–15.99 kg/m2 Two subtypes of AN are described in the cur-
Extreme: BMI < 15 kg/m2 rent DSM-5 (APA, 2013), and the typology is
Reprinted with permission from the Diagnostic and based on the presence or absence of binge eating
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, as well as on the principal method of weight con-
(Copyright © 2013). American Psychiatric Association trol. Individuals with restricting anorexia restrict
Eating Disorders 343

only, whereas those with binge eating/purging Table 2  DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for bulimia nervosa
anorexia engage in purging which may or may A Recurrent episodes of binge eating. An episode of
not be associated with binge eating. However, binge eating is characterized by both of the
following
excessive dieting and exercising may occur with
1. Eating in a discrete period of time (e.g., within
either type of AN. any 2-h period) an amount of food that is definitely
Recognizing AN in children and adolescents larger than what most individuals would eat in a
presents many challenges. First, in a prepubes- similar period of time under similar circumstances
cent girl, amenorrhea may not be present. Second, 2. A sense of lack of control over eating during the
there may not be a noticeable weight loss but episode (e.g., a feeling that one cannot stop eating
or control what or how much one is eating)
rather a failure to achieve normal weight or to
B Recurrent in appropriate compensatory behaviors in
gain weight at a normal rate. As noted above, order to prevent weight gain, such as self-induced
normal weight is also sometimes a challenge to vomiting; misuse of laxatives, diuretics, or other
determine and quantify. A related concern is that medications; fasting; or excessive exercise
severe anorexia may inhibit normal skeletal C The binge eating and compensatory behaviors both
occur, on average, at least once a week for 3 months
development; thus, height may be affected.
D Self-evaluation is unduly influenced by body shape
Finally, a young person may be less likely to will- and weight
ingly seek help or discuss their symptoms (Lask E The disturbance does not occur exclusively during
and Bryant-Waugh (2013), placing the responsi- episodes of anorexia nervosa
bility on the parents/caregivers. Reprinted with permission from the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition,
(Copyright © 2013). American Psychiatric Association
Bulimia Nervosa (BN)
“Self-evaluation is unduly influenced by body
According to the current DSM-5 (APA, 2013), shape and weight” (APA, p. 345).
BN is characterized by repeated episodes of In earlier versions of the DSM, it was possible
binge eating followed by an inappropriate com- for a person to be diagnosed with both AN and
pensatory response to prevent weight gain, with BN. With the current system, BN cannot be diag-
binge episodes and compensatory behaviors nosed if it occurs only in the context of AN. Such
occurring on average at least once a week for an individual would be diagnosed as having the
3 months (APA). Full criteria are listed in Table 2. binge eating/purging subtype of AN. There is
As with AN, there is some sort of body image evidence that BN occurs on a continuum with the
disturbance. Although the DSM-5 defines a binge binge eating/purging subtype of AN, whereas the
as “Eating, in a discrete period of time (e.g., restricting subtype is qualitatively different from
within any 2-h period), an amount of food that is both other disorders (Gleaves, Lowe, Green,
definitely larger than what most individuals Cororve, & Williams, 2000).
would eat in a similar period of time under simi-
lar circumstances” (APA, p. 345), the require-
ment that binges be necessarily large has been Binge-Eating Disorder (BED)
questioned by some researchers (e.g., Latner,
Hildebrandt, Rosewall, Chisholm, & Hayashi, BED has recently been accepted as a formal
2007). A perceived loss of control during the diagnosis in the DSM-5 (APA, 2013) (Table 3).
binge episodes must also be present and may be BED is characterized by the presence of recur-
the more important factor (rather than size; rent binge eating (as seen with BN) but in the
Latner et al., 2007). Binges are triggered by a absence of the compensatory behaviors that
variety of factors including hunger, stress, bore- occur with BN. Persons with BED may not be as
dom, negative mood, feelings and thoughts asso- restrictive in their eating as persons with BN,
ciated with body image, and food cravings and a large percentage of individuals are conse-
(APA). The DSM-5 body image criterion for BN quently obese. Persons with BED are very often
is less specific than for AN and worded only as dissatisfied with their bodies. The shape and
344 J.K. Rosewall et al.

weight c­ oncerns of obese individuals with BED, Atypical Eating Disorder Variants
as well as their eating-related and general psy-
chopathology, quality of life, and even physical In the previous editions of the DSM, the term for
health, are significantly more impaired than atypical variants was eating disorder not other-
those of obese individuals without BED (Wilfley, wise specified (EDNOS), and much of the avail-
Wilson, & Agras, 2003) with overvaluation of able research uses this term. Although EDNOS
shape and weight considered a marker of symp- was a residual diagnostic category, it was particu-
tom severity in BED (Grilo, Ivezas, & White, larly noteworthy because it appeared to be the
2015). Moreover, results from a recent system- most common ED encountered in clinical prac-
atic review highlighted the role of negative emo- tice (Fairburn & Bohn, 2005), including among
tion in precipitating binge eating for individuals children and young adolescents (Nicholls, Chater,
with BED but not for obese individuals without & Lask, 2000), and this was not simply due to
BED (Leehr et al., 2015). The range and fre- BED being formerly categorized as EDNOS.
quency of comorbid psychopathology for BED Furthermore, it may have been more common
is similar to that for BN, and the mortality rates among adolescents than among adults (Fisher,
for BED may actually be higher than with BN Schneider, Burns, Symons, & Mandel, 2001).
because the former is associated with obesity With the DSM-5 (APA, 2013), atypical EDs that
(Agras, 2001). do not meet the criteria for AN, BN, or BED can
now be classified as other specified feeding or
Table 3 DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for binge-eating eating disorder (OSFED) and unspecified feeding
disorder or eating disorder (UFED). The first includes
A Recurrent episodes of binge eating. An episode of some recognized syndromes such as purging dis-
binge eating is characterized by both of the order (when an individual engages in purging but
following not binge eating) or cases of AN or BN that do
1. Eating in a discrete period of time (e.g., within not meet the duration criterion. UFED would be
any 2-h period) an amount of food that is definitely
larger than what most individuals would eat in a reserved for cases where a clinician chooses not
similar period of time under similar circumstances to indicate why specific criteria are not met or
2. A sense of lack of control over eating during the where there is not enough information to make a
episode (e.g., a feeling that one cannot stop eating more definitive diagnosis.
or control what or how much one is eating)
With the DSM-5 changes to the diagnostic cri-
B The binge eating episodes are associated with three
(or more) of the following
teria for AN and BN, and the formal inclusion of
1. Eating much more rapidly than normal BED, one goal was to reduce the prevalence of
2. Eating until feeling uncomfortably full atypical EDs. More recent prevalence studies
3. Eating large amounts of food when not feeling (Allen, Byrne, Oddy, & Crosby, 2013; Stice,
physically hungry Marti, & Rohde, 2013) have started to determine
4. Eating alone because of feeling embarrassed by the impact of the new diagnostic criteria on the
how much one is eating prevalence of atypical EDs. For both practitio-
5. Feeling disgusted with oneself, depressed, or ners and researchers, it is important to note that a
very guilty afterward
large percentage of individuals who report disor-
C Marked distress regarding binge eating is present
D The binge eating occurs, on average, at least once a
dered eating may not neatly fit into one of the
week for 3 months existing diagnostic categories. Furthermore,
E The binge eating is not associated with the recurrent although the EDs each have distinct clinical fea-
use of inappropriate compensatory behavior as in tures, they also share many features, which have
bulimia nervosa and does not occur exclusively led to a “transdiagnostic” theory of EDs (Fairburn,
during the course of bulimia nervosa or anorexia
nervosa Cooper, & Shafran, 2003). Research suggests
Reprinted with permission from the Diagnostic and
that certain characteristics such as overvaluation
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, of eating, weight and shape control, and prob-
(Copyright © 2013). American Psychiatric Association lems with self-esteem and affect are indeed trans-
Eating Disorders 345

diagnostic, whereas other features such as Greece and Germany (1.26–1.18% (AN), 3.15–
perfectionism, interpersonal problems, and 3.54% (BN), and 13.84–19.45% (EDNOS)
dietary restraint likely vary across distinct forms Fichter, Quadflieg, Georgopoulou, Xepapadakos,
of eating psychopathology (Lampard, Tasca, & Fthenakis (2005)).
Balfour, & Bissada, 2013). It is expected that new estimated prevalence
rates of AN and BN would be higher, and that
rates of OSFED (formerly EDNOS) would be
Epidemiology lower, because the recent version of the DSM has
relaxed some of the diagnostic criteria (e.g., pres-
Prevalence ence of amenorrhea in AN and symptom fre-
quency in BN). Applying the DSM-5, prevalence
Many of the studies on ED prevalence are based rates among adolescent girls interviewed over
on former DSM-IV (APA, 2000) criteria. For 8 years were reported to be 0.8% (AN), 2.6%
example, Hoek and van Hoeken’s (2003) review (BN), and 3.0% (BED), and 11.5% experienced a
of the ED literature (based on DSM-IV criteria) feeding or ED not elsewhere classified (an earlier
reported average prevalence rates of 0.3% (AN) conceptualization of OSFED and UFED) (Stice
and 1% (BN) for young women and 0.1% (BN) et al., 2013). In a larger study that also included
for young men in the general population. Among adolescent males, Allen and colleagues (2013)
at-risk women, however, prevalence estimates compared the prevalence rates of DSM-IV-TR
typically range from 3% to 10% (i.e., ages and DSM-5 EDs at ages 14, 17, and 20 years. The
15–29 years; Polivy & Herman, 2002). Research authors reported significantly greater ED preva-
conducted with children and adolescents sug- lence rates among female adolescents across all
gests varying prevalence rates. For instance, a US ages using DSM-5 criteria (8.5–15.2%) than
National Comorbidity Survey of 10,123 adoles- when applying DSM-IV-TR criteria (5.8–13.4%)
cents aged between 13 and 18 reported that 2.7% as well as fewer unspecified diagnoses when
of the sample met criteria for an ED (AN, BN, or using the DSM-5 criteria. Among adolescent
BED). Reported prevalence rates were more than males, significantly higher rates were only found
double among females (3.8%) relative to males at age 17 when applying the DSM-5 criteria
(1.5%) and slightly increased with age (13– (2.6%) compared to the DSM-IV-TR criteria
14 years = 2.4%, 15–16 years = 2.8%, and (1.2%) (Allen et al., 2013).
17–18 years = 3.0%) (Merikangas et al., 2010). In addition to studies of the prevalence of ED
Similarly, a study with community adolescents diagnoses, several researchers have examined the
reported that 3% met criteria for an ED (not prevalence of eating-disordered behaviors among
including EDNOS), of which 33.3% were male adolescents. For instance, Croll, Neumark-­
and 66.7% were female (Zaider, Johnson, & Sztainer, Story, and Ireland’s (2002) research on
Cockell, 2000). disordered eating behaviors (i.e., binge eating or
Studies of female adolescents in Italy (Cotrufo, using any of the following to control or lose
Gnisci, & Caputo, 2005) and Spain (Ruiz-Lázaro, weight: fasting/skipping meals, using diet pills or
Alonso, Comet, Lobo, & Velilla, 2005) have esti- amphetamines, laxatives, vomiting, and/or smok-
mated the following prevalence rates for EDs: ing cigarettes) among 9th and 12th grade students
0.55–0.77% (BN), 3.47% (partial BN), 0.38% in Minnesota (N = 81,247) revealed a high preva-
(partial BED), 0.14% (AN), 5.79% (subclinical lence of such behaviors. For instance, among 9th
AN), and 3.83% (EDNOS). Similar lifetime prev- graders, 56% of the girls and 28% of the boys
alence rates have also been reported among ado- reported engaging in disordered eating behaviors.
lescent girls in Iran (0.9% (AN), 3.2% (BN),1.84% The estimates were slightly higher among 12th
(partial AN), and 4.79% (partial BN) Nobakht & graders, as 57% of the girls and 31% of the boys
Dezhkam (2000)) and among adolescent girls in reported disordered eating. One limitation of this
346 J.K. Rosewall et al.

research is that it included a fairly broad defini- highest for females aged 15–19 and for boys aged
tion of disordered eating. However, as the authors 10–14 (Micali, Hagberg, Petersen, & Treasure,
noted, engaging in any of the above behaviors 2013). In their review on ED incidence, Smink,
would constitute risk factors for the subsequent van Hoeken, and Hoek (2012) reported that,
development of EDs. although overall incidence of AN has remained
In practice, recognizing and detecting EDs stable, there has been an increase in this high-risk
may often be restricted by the secrecy associ- group of 15–19-year-old girls. They suggest that
ated with binge eating and purging behaviors. this could be due to thorough and early detection
Moreover, some have suggested that the preva- of AN or an earlier age at onset. Regarding BN,
lence of some EDs (particularly BN) may be Smink et al.(2012) reported incidence rates to be
lower among children and adolescents than decreasing since the early 1990s.
adults for practical reasons, such as not having With regard to time trends, Van Son, van
access to money or privacy required for binge Hoeken, Bartelds, van Furth, and Hoek (2006)
eating (Netemeyer & Williamson, 2001). assessed the incidence of EDs in the Netherlands
Similarly, although AN may be more obviously during two time periods, 1985–1989 and 1995–
detectable because of patients’ extreme low 1999. The authors reported that although the inci-
weight, such detection may be more difficult dence of AN was fairly stable for the general
when the low weight is a manifestation of a population (i.e., 7.4–7.7 per 100,000), the inci-
failure to gain weight (Campbell & Peebles, dence for the 15–19-year-old female age group
2014). Overall, we view proper assessment as significantly increased from 56.4 to 109.2 per
critical for diagnosis and treatment, and there 100,000. Of the individuals diagnosed with AN
are many issues specific to assessment of chil- between 1995 and 1999, only one (2%) was male.
dren and adolescents with eating problems. See In contrast, they reported that the incidence of
Lask and Bryant-Waugh (2013), Rosen (2010), BN decreased somewhat (nonsignificantly) from
or Weaver and Liebman (2011) for a more in- 8.6 to 6.1 per 100,000, consistent with other
depth discussion of assessment of EDs among research (Keel, Heatherton, Dorer, Joiner, &
children and adolescents. Zalta, 2006). An additional time trend is that age
of onset for both AN and BN may be decreasing
over time (Favaro, Caregaro, Tenconi, Bosello, &
Incidence and Time Trends Santonastaso, 2009).
Regarding trends in ED-related symptomatol-
Lewinsohn, Striegel-Moore, and Seeley (2000) ogy, Neumark-Sztainer et al. (2012) examined
reported the incidence of EDs to be less than trends in weight and weight-related attitudes and
2.8% by age 18, and 1.3% for individuals aged behaviors among adolescents in 1999 and 2010.
19–23 years. In contrast, Rastam, Gillberg, and Among girls, there was no increase in obesity,
Garton (1989) screened the entire population of and dieting and unhealthy and extreme weight
school children in an urban region of Sweden control behaviors both decreased. Body dissatis-
(N = 4291) and reported 3 cases of BN, 17 cases faction did not change over time. The trends were
of AN, and 3 cases of a partial AN syndrome. In less consistent among boys: extreme weight con-
their review of the research, Hoek and van trol behaviors decreased; however, dieting and
Hoeken (2003) reported the incidence of AN to unhealthy weight control behaviors remained
be 8 cases per 100,000 population per year and constant. There were also significant increases to
noted that the incidence rates for AN are the obesity, perceptions of being overweight, and
highest for females in the 15–19 age group. They body dissatisfaction in 2010 than in 1999. Fichter
estimated the incidence of BN to be 12 cases per et al. (2005) reported significant increases in
100,000 population per year. In the United weight phobia and bulimic behaviors from
Kingdom, data gathered from primary care approximately 1979 to 1998 among Greek girls
records indicated the incidence of EDs was also in Germany but, interestingly, observed the
Eating Disorders 347

reverse for the male adolescents, whose weight present to ED treatment programs (e.g., Geist,
phobia scores decreased from the first to second Heinmaa, Katzman, & Stephens, 1999; Peebles,
assessment. Wilson, & Lock, 2006), but there may be some
gender differences in the presentation of these
disorders. For instance, Geist et al. (1999)
Gender Differences reported that male adolescents presented with
significantly lower drive for thinness and body
As reflected by the incidence and prevalence dissatisfaction than their female counterparts.
rates, EDs typically occur less frequently among However, the authors noted that in the absence of
males than among females (Mitchison & Hay, adolescent male norms on the instrument used,
2014). One possibility is that the prevalence of their results may be difficult to interpret.
AN is higher among boys than it appears to be Among patients younger than age 13, males
but is not readily recognized due to its reputation and females may be equally affected (Rosen,
as a stereotypically female disorder. Alternately, 2010). In comparing a large sample (N = 959) of
extant assessment instruments may be better at children and adolescents ages 8–19 years in an
detecting ED symptomatology among women ED treatment program, Peebles et al. (2006)
than men due to variability in symptom presenta- reported that compared with older adolescents
tion. Thus, research examining EDs among men (mean age = 15.6 years, SD = 1.4), younger
and boys may have been limited by the tendency patients (mean age = 11.6 years, SD = 1.2) were
toward misdiagnosis, although greater attention more often male, presented at a lower percentage
has been devoted to this problem in recent years. of ideal body weight, and lost weight more rap-
In general, data suggest that boys with EDs idly. Specifically, in the younger sample, 16.5%
typically strive for a more muscular body ideal, was male, whereas 7.8% of the older sample was
rather than the thin ideal typically pursued by male. In the entire sample, most of the patients
girls (McCreary & Sasse, 2000; see Labre, 2002, were female (91.1%) and presented with EDNOS
for a review on adolescent boys and the muscular (51.3%), although there were also large propor-
ideal). Although EDs have been diagnosed tions presenting with AN (35.8%) and BN
among individuals of all sexual orientations, (12.9%).
bisexual and homosexual orientation may be
particular risk factors for developing EDs
(Austin et al., 2004). The prevalence of homo- Comorbidity
sexuality and bisexuality is higher among men
with BN than in the general population (43% EDs are often accompanied by a wide range of
versus 10%; Carlat, Camargo, & Herzog, 1997); medical and/or psychological problems. Perhaps
however, it is not clear whether this applies to the greatest attention has been devoted to the co-­
adolescents. Furthermore, athletes (e.g., wres- occurrence of EDs with mood disorders (Stice,
tlers, gymnasts) and other individuals for whom Hayward, Cameron, Killen, & Taylor, 2000;
physical appearance and body shape are espe- Stice, Presnell, & Bearnman, 2001; Swanson,
cially important (e.g., body builders) are at a Crow, Le Grange, Swendsen, & Merikangas,
higher risk of developing BN because they need 2011) and substance abuse disorders (Dansky,
to maintain their weight at or below specific Brewerton, & Kilpatrick, 2000). Among female
thresholds (Beals, 2004). adolescents with first onset AN, 47.3% were
For boys, the following estimates are available found to meet criteria for an additional psychiat-
for lifetime prevalence rates: 6.5% (any ED), ric disorder, most commonly mood (major
0.2% (AN), 0.4% (BN), and 0.9% (BED) depression or dysthymia) and anxiety disorders
(Kjelsås, Bjørnstrøm, & Götestam, 2004). (particularly, social phobia and obsessive-­
Among children and adolescents, consistently compulsive disorder). Those diagnosed with
higher proportions of female than male patients binge–purge subtype experienced a higher
348 J.K. Rosewall et al.

f­requency of comorbid diagnoses as well as psychopathology predicted increases in sub-


increased suicidal ideation and self-harm behav- stance abuse symptoms over a 5-year period.
ior compared to those with restrictive AN In addition to comorbid psychological condi-
(Bühren et al., 2014). Of note, the large US tions, individuals with EDs are prone to experi-
National Comorbidity Survey (N = 10,123) ence a host of significant medical consequences
reported that AN was only associated with oppo- and correlates, such as gastrointestinal complica-
sitional defiant disorder. However, BN and BED tions, dangerously low body weight, and dental
were significantly associated with most disor- caries (Campbell & Peebles, 2014). Specifically,
ders assessed, particularly mood and anxiety dis- individuals with AN are susceptible to experienc-
orders. Among those with BN, more than 1/2 ing osteoporosis and osteopenia, cardiovascular
reported suicidal ideation and more than 1/3 problems, and orthopedic problems due to the
reported having made a suicide attempt (Swanson combined effects of excessive exercise and nutri-
et al., 2011). In another study with adolescents, tional deficiencies (Brambilla & Monteleone,
Zaider et al. (2000) reported that individuals 2003). Individuals with BN are likely to experi-
with dysthymia, panic, and major depressive dis- ence various medical complications including
order were significantly more likely than those electrolyte imbalances, dental problems, and car-
without these disorders to have an ED. Even diovascular problems (Brambilla & Monteleone,
after controlling for the effects of other Axis I 2003).
and Axis II psychopathology, dysthymia inde-
pendently predicted EDs.
EDs also appear to be highly comorbid with Course and Outcome
substance use problems (Bulik et al., 2004), and
approximately 20–46% of women with EDs In evaluating the longitudinal course of EDs,
report a history of problems with alcohol and/or Kotler, Cohen, Davies, Pine, and Walsh (2001)
drugs (Bulik et al.; Conason, Brunstein Klomek, reported that BN during early adolescence is
& Sher, 2006). Researchers have suggested that associated with a 9-fold and 20-fold increase in
the powerful drive for thinness that is central to risk for BN in late adolescence and adulthood,
EDs may increase the likelihood of abusing stim- respectively. Moreover, BN in late adolescence
ulant drugs for weight loss reasons (Measelle, was found to be associated with a 35-fold increase
Stice, & Hogansen, 2006). Moreover, if binge in risk for BN in adulthood. In another longitudi-
eating and subsequent compensatory behaviors nal study on female adolescents (aged
engender feelings of guilt, the individual may 12–15 years at initiation of study), Measelle et al.
turn to substance use to modulate his or her nega- (2006) examined the course of co-occurring dis-
tive affect. orders, including EDs, over a 5-year period. They
In their study of 290 adolescents, Mann et al. reported that ED symptoms increased substan-
(2014) reported a lifetime prevalence of sub- tially over time, at fairly constant rates.
stance use of 24.6% for those with AN, 48.7% for Steinhausen (2009) reported that for 20% of AN
those with BN, and 28.6% for those with cases, across all ages of onset, the course is
EDNOS. A diagnosis of BN was strongly associ- chronic, highlighting the treatment challenges
ated with regular alcohol consumption, and often associated with AN. Overall, the course and
tobacco use was significantly more frequent in outcome for EDs varies as a function of the disor-
those with BN compared with AN. Consistent der and a host of other predictive factors. Among
with these data, individuals with restricting patients with AN, deaths are due to either physi-
anorexia reported less substance use than the cal complications or suicide. Of all psychiatric
general (nonclinical) population (Stock, disorders, AN appears to have the highest mortal-
Goldberg, Corbett, & Katzman, 2002). Finally, in ity rate, approximately 5.6% per decade (Agras,
their longitudinal study with adolescent girls, 2001). In a meta-analysis of mortality rates in
Measelle et al. (2006) reported that initial eating patients with EDs, Arcelus and colleagues
Eating Disorders 349

reported that one in five adults with AN who died u­ p, about 60% of individuals are in full or partial
had committed suicide (Arcelus, Mitchell, Wales, remission from the disorder, but between 30%
& Nielsen, 2011). and 50% continue to have a clinical ED. Consistent
In terms of ED outcome, Steinhausen (2002) with these findings, Steinhausen and Weber
reviewed data from 119 outcome studies with (2009) reviewed 79 BN outcome studies con-
follow-ups of greater than 10 years and reported ducted over the past 25 years and reported that
mean values of 73.2% for recovery, 8.5% for 45% of patients showed full recovery from BN,
improvement, 13.7% for chronicity, and 9.4% for 27% improved considerably, and nearly 23% had
mortality. Factors contributing to poor prognosis a chronic course. The mortality rate was 0.32%,
included vomiting, purgative behaviors, illness crossover to another ED at follow-up was 22.5%
chronicity, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. (EDNOS, followed by AN, and then BED; how-
Patients who were followed up for a longer dura- ever, the latter may be underreported given the
tion tended to have better outcomes, highlighting age of some studies when BED was less under-
the extended time it can take for full recovery. stood), and reporting other psychiatric disorders
Adolescent-onset AN tends to have better out- at the outcome assessment was common.
comes than adults (Rosen, 2010; Steinhausen, In comparison to AN and BN, the course and
2009); however, those very young (typically pre- outcome for BED appears to be more positive.
pubertal) tend to have poorer outcomes (Wentz, Among young women with BN or BED, Fairburn,
Gillberg, Anckarsäter, Gillberg, & Råstam, Cooper, Doll, Norman, and O’Connor (2000)
2009). There is a strong evidence base supporting observed that over 5 years the outcome of those
the necessity of early treatment and weight gain with BN was relatively poor but that the majority
in adolescent AN to maximize positive outcomes of the BED group made a full recovery despite
(e.g., Le Grange, Accurso, Lock, Agras & not having received treatment. However, that
Bryson, 2014). In a Swedish study, Wentz and sample might have been younger and slimmer
colleagues (2009) followed up 51 individuals than is often typical of BED. Obesity may be an
with adolescent-onset AN over an 18-year period. outcome in a proportion of cases (e.g., 28%;
At follow-up, there were no deaths; 12% still had Wade et al., 2006). Finally, less is known about
an ED (AN, EDNOS, or BN), 39% experienced the course and outcome of EDNOS. Among a
another psychiatric disorder, and 25% were sample of ED patients enrolled in the
unable to work. Poor prognosis was associated Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders
with being a younger age at diagnosis, premorbid Study, Grilo et al. (2003) reported that the 2-year
obsessive-compulsive disorder and the presence course for EDNOS was better than for BN (40%
of autistic traits. These findings are similar to remitted for BN versus 59% for EDNOS).
those of Saccomani, Savoini, Cirrincione, Notably, Grilo et al. (2003) also found that the
Vercellino, and Ravera (1998) who also reported course for both BN and EDNOS appeared to be
zero deaths and a negative outcome in 14% of unrelated to the presence, severity, or change in
cases. The authors noted that poor outcome was comorbid personality disorder or other Axis I
associated with greater severity of the disorder at disorder. Given the paucity of research in this
initial presentation, the length of inpatient treat- area, further research is needed to better under-
ment, and comorbidity with mood and personal- stand the course and outcome of BED and
ity disorders. EDNOS.
Although longer-term outcome seems to be
better for individuals with BN than for AN, BN is
still associated with a considerable amount of I nterventions and Empirical
relapse and chronicity (Agras, 2001). In his Evidence
review of the literature, Agras surmised that only
10% of individuals with BN continue to experi- In the following sections, we review the literature
ence the full syndrome at 10-year follow-up and on psychologically based interventions for EDs.
that less than 1% develop AN. At 10-year follow- Pharmacological interventions, although beyond
350 J.K. Rosewall et al.

the scope of this chapter, are also used in some weight and metabolic problems (Patel, Pratt, &
situations. See Van den Heuvel and Jordaan Greydanus, 2003). For instance, patient symp-
(2014) for a review. toms such as dehydration, electrolyte and fluid
imbalances, hypotension, cardiac dysrhythmias,
and seizures require urgent medical care, and,
Inpatient Treatment thus, the basis of the medical management
approach to EDs is nutritional rehabilitation
Outpatient treatment is the norm for children and (Patel et al., 2003).
adolescents with EDs; however, a relatively small Anzai et al. (2002) suggested that the first
proportion of these patients require inpatient components of AN treatment, refeeding and
treatment in psychiatric or pediatric units. The weight restoration, may be best administered in
admission criteria, goals, treatment methods, and inpatient settings alongside close monitoring of
duration of stay vary widely across inpatient set- physical risk. Moreover, they noted that recov-
tings, and such treatment decisions are based on ered AN patients who were never hospitalized
limited research evidence. Anzai, Lindsey-­ often reported that if they could start again, they
Dudley, and Bidwell (2002) suggested the fol- would choose to begin treatment via hospitaliza-
lowing admission criteria for inpatient psychiatric tion, as it would facilitate sooner recovery with
care for individuals with AN: (1) poor medical less suffering. However, given the dramatic
status but not so severe as to warrant medical changes in inpatient psychiatric services and
hospitalization (low pulse, temperature, blood managed care in the United States, the previously
pressure, or potassium; dehydration), (2) low typical 3–6-month admission for AN treatment
body weight and refusal to eat (BMI <17 or has changed substantially. For instance, Anzai
weight <75% of expected for height/weight or, et al. reported that AN patients typically stay in
for children and adolescents, food refusal or rapid an acute inpatient unit for 7–10 days, after which
weight loss), (3) low motivation and compliance they are transferred to a partial hospital program
(denial of problems, refusal to eat more than min- for 1–3 weeks and then, finally, transitioned to
imum amount), (4) poor family support (absent outpatient treatment. In most cases, patients with
or not sufficient to make progress), (5) purging AN can receive oral refeeding, with the objective
behavior (to the point of jeopardizing health, of gaining 1–3 lbs per week of inpatient treat-
with an inability to stop or decrease behavior), ment (Patel et al., 2003).
and (6) comorbid psychiatric complications (sui- Not surprisingly, those who receive inpatient
cidality or severe comorbid disorders warranting treatment tend to gain weight faster than those
hospitalization). Anzai et al. (2002) also noted who receive outpatient treatment (Hartmann,
that individuals with AN require hospitalization Weber, Herpetz, & Zeeck, 2011). Davies and
more often than BN patients, and whereas treat- Jaffa (2005) assessed weekly weight gain among
ment for AN emphasizes refeeding and weight adolescents with AN in a United Kingdom inpa-
gain, BN inpatient treatment focuses on provid- tient unit (N = 53) and reported that the average
ing a structured setting for patients to eat ade- weight gain was 0.82 kg/week. Patients did not
quate meals without engaging in binge eating and differ in average weight gain based on whether
purging. Thus, guidelines for inpatient treatment they had received prior inpatient treatment, but
are mostly relevant for AN, as most patients with those with an initial lower percentage of expected
BN can be treated on an outpatient basis, and out- body weight were faster to gain weight. Research
come may be comparable to inpatient treatment has demonstrated that, among adolescent and
(Zeeck et al., 2009). adult females admitted for inpatient treatment for
A primary goal of inpatient treatment for EDs AN, the only predictor of outcome at 1-year fol-
is medical and nutritional management, particu- low-­ up was rate of weight gain. Those who
larly in cases where patients present for treatment gained less than ≤0.8 kg per week were more
at a late stage of their ED and have multiple likely to deteriorate after discharge (Lund et al.,
Eating Disorders 351

2008). Similarly, low weight at discharge is a risk weight restoration (to 90% expected body weight)
factor for rehospitalization (Steinhausen, or a shorter admission for medical stabilization.
Grigoroiu-Serbanescu, Boyadjieva, Neumärker, Post discharge, all of the adolescents received 20
& Metzke, 2008). sessions of outpatient family-based treatment and
Although inpatient treatment is typically more were followed up at 6 months and at 12 months.
appropriate for patients with AN than BN, there Other than expected differences in higher body
are some instances in which hospitalization for weight after hospitalization, the authors reported
BN is necessary. Some reasons for the hospital- no differences between the groups in terms of
ization of children or adolescents with BN may hospital days needed after admission, readmis-
include (1) severe cardiac or physiological distur- sion rates, or rates of remission. They concluded
bances caused by binge eating and purging; (2) that prolonged hospital admissions provided no
persistent suicidal ideation/attempts, self-harm, added benefit to clinical outcomes when effective
or psychosis; (3) intractable binge eating and outpatient treatments are available (Madden
purging that have not responded to outpatient et al., 2015). Gowers, Weetman, Shore, Hossain,
treatment or partial hospitalization; or (4) serious and Elvins (2000) reported that outpatients dem-
comorbid conditions that interfere with treatment onstrated a better outcome 2–7 years after initial
(Robin, Gilroy, & Dennis, 1998). For BN patients, presentation, and the primary predictor of poorer
the main treatment objective is to establish nor- outcome was admission to inpatient care.
mal nutritional intake without purging, binge eat- Although this study was not randomized and
ing, or restricting. inpatient treatment may have simply reflected
A limitation of extant research on inpatient greater severity, its results suggest that caution is
treatment outcome is that there have been few necessary in prescribing inpatient care (Gowers
controlled investigations on outcome for adoles- & Bryant-Waugh, 2004).
cents with AN. The Trial of Outcome for Child To understand the experience of adolescents
and Adolescent Anorexia Nervosa (TOuCAN) in undergoing inpatient treatment for AN, Colton
the United Kingdom compared inpatient treat- and Pistrang (2004) conducted semi-structured
ment with specialist outpatient care and treat- interviews with young women (N = 19) in inpa-
ment as usual (TAU) in a general community tient ED units. The authors reported that the
child and adolescent mental health service. patients maintained positive as well as negative
Overall, the researchers found no statistically sig- views about their treatment, characterized by
nificant differences between the three settings in themes of confusion about their illness, readiness
terms of AN treatment outcome at 1-, 2-, and for treatment as a key to recovery, advantages and
5-year follow-up, providing little support for disadvantages of social support from other
lengthy, and costly, inpatient admissions. patients, individual treatment from staff, and the
Specialist outpatient treatment was considered value of being a collaborator in treatment.
most cost-effective, and patients and caregivers In summary, most hospitalization programs
were most satisfied with specialist treatment. for EDs are multidisciplinary and include a mix-
However, poor adherence to initial randomiza- ture of treatment components. The foremost goal
tion (65% across the sample and 49% in the inpa- is to achieve medical and nutritional stabiliza-
tient group) limits the strength of these findings tion, weight restoration, and regular eating, while
(Gowers et al., 2010). These findings, and study closely monitoring physical risk. Nasogastric
limitations, are similar to earlier research con- feedings are infrequently required but may be
ducted with adolescent and adult AN patients needed when the patient is unable to tolerate food
(Crisp et al., 1991). orally to gain sufficient weight. Treatment also
In another recent randomized controlled study focuses on facilitating fundamental change to
(Madden et al., 2015), 82 medically unstable attitudes about weight, shape, and appearance
adolescents with AN were assigned to either a and also disrupting the binge–purge cycle. In
longer inpatient admission for the purpose of addition to focusing on specific ED symptoms,
352 J.K. Rosewall et al.

inpatient treatment aims to promote individual (Herpertz-Dahlmann et al., 2014). In that study,
change and growth (affect regulation, self-­ 179 adolescents received either day-patient care
identity) and assists with the acquisition of skills after a period of brief medical stabilization or
needed to deal with life issues and social engage- continued inpatient care and were followed up at
ment (e.g., communication, conflict resolution). 12 months after admission. At follow-up, there
Although inpatient admissions facilitate prompt were no significant differences between the two
and necessary weight restoration, inpatient treat- groups in terms of weight gain and maintenance,
ment remains expensive, and there is conflicting readmission rate, or number of serious adverse
evidence as to the long-term benefits of pro- events, suggesting that day-patient care can be an
longed admissions. effective and safe alternative to inpatient care.
In a small uncontrolled study of adolescents
with AN (N = 26), Goldstein et al. (2011) exam-
Partial Hospitalization ined the effectiveness of a day program on both
weight and eating pathology. Results indicated
In a stepped-care framework, treatment that con- clinically significant weight gain and improve-
stitutes the least restrictive alternative, but is still ments in eating-disordered behavior, and trends
believed to be helpful, is the first treatment indicated that these improvements were main-
attempted (Davison, 2000). A form of treatment tained at 6-month follow-up. Similarly, Danziger,
that is more intensive than outpatient treatment Carcl, Varsano, Tyano, and Mimouni (1988)
but less intensive, less restrictive, and less costly described a follow-up of 32 girls with AN in a
than inpatient treatment is partial hospitalization pediatric day treatment program that involved
or day treatment programs. parents as participants and providers in the ther-
Partial hospital programs often use the same apy. Nine months after treatment, the majority of
treatment strategies and have the same treatment cases showed a healthy restoration of weight,
goals as inpatient programs. A descriptive report menstruation, body image, eating and exercise
noted that in three typical day treatments for habits, and social functioning.
eating-­disordered patients of all ages, these pro- Two of the 3-day programs described by
grams regularly use group meals, nutrition and Zipfel and colleagues (2002) have been exam-
cooking education groups, body image and coun- ined in uncontrolled research trials. These pro-
seling groups, and groups that address social grams have shown preliminary evidence of
skills, assertiveness, family issues, and relation- efficacy in a range of age groups. Among 51
ships (Zipfel et al., 2002). However, because adult women with AN, BN, or subthreshold vari-
patients return home in the evening, they spend ants of these disorders, treatment outcomes and
less time on the unit. Thus, such programs permit direct costs of inpatient and partial day hospital
patients to remain in their natural environments treatment were compared (Williamson, Thaw, &
during the course of treatment. Staying in the Varnado-Sullivan, 2001). Based on disorder
natural environment may facilitate more rapid severity, patients were assigned to either inpa-
learning and generalization of therapeutic skills tient or day treatment. Although the outcome of
to home and school settings. These programs also the two treatments was similar, the day hospital
allow patients to continue to function in their program was substantially less costly. Savings
everyday social roles and to have continued fam- per patient in the day hospital program were
ily contact and support (Herpertz-Dahlmann $9645 (43% of the cost of inpatient cases). The
et al., 2014; Zipfel et al., 2002). proportion of patients classified as recovered
Outcome research on day treatment programs across the two treatments was 63%.
for children or adolescents with EDs, and even These findings from a small number of studies
for adults, is limited. One randomized controlled suggest that partial hospitalization programs
trial to date has compared day-patient treatment might be an effective and less costly alternative to
with inpatient treatment in adolescent AN inpatient care. However, more randomized con-
Eating Disorders 353

trolled studies are needed to compare the efficacy apist aims to strengthen the bond between the
of day treatment with other treatment modalities, two parents in their joint refeeding efforts and
inpatient, and outpatient treatments. This is a par- between the patient and any siblings available to
ticular research priority for the treatment of chil- provide support. During the second phase of
dren, as day programs allow patients greater time treatment, family issues that may be interfering
with their family and more opportunity to partici- with refeeding are identified and addressed. The
pate in normal activities outside of hospital, as third treatment phase is initiated only after
well as being potentially more cost effective. healthy weight and eating patterns have been
achieved. At this final stage, treatment centers on
building a healthy relationship between the ado-
Outpatient Treatment lescent and the family that is not focused primar-
ily around the ED (Lock & Le Grange, 2015).
In this section we highlight four forms of outpa- FBT has been tested in a number of random-
tient treatment for childhood and adolescent eat- ized controlled trials. These studies have investi-
ing disturbances. Certain caveats should be gated its efficacy as well as its ideal length and
noted, however. Although growing, the research format. In a meta-analytic review of 12 random-
base concerning these treatments is limited, due ized controlled trials of FBT for adolescent AN,
to factors such as the rarity of these disorders and Couturier et al. (2013) reported that FBT tended
the difficulty in recruiting and retaining patients to demonstrate a similar outcome to individual
in treatment trials (Couturier, Kimber, & treatment; however, FBT was superior at both
Szatmari, 2013). In addition, several studies on 6-month and 12-month follow-up. It is still
AN that have found no differences between unclear what components of family therapy
groups have had small sample sizes. In such stud- account for its efficacy. Research has demon-
ies, it is important not automatically to interpret a strated that parental self-efficacy in FBT is pre-
lack of significant differences across conditions dictive of adolescent outcomes in FBT (Robinson,
as treatment equivalence (Fairburn, 2005). Strahan, Girz, Wilson, & Boachie, 2013). Le
Grange et al. (2012) also identified that those
Family-Based Treatment adolescents with more severe eating psychopa-
Most children and adolescents with EDs are thology and low levels of family conflict tend to
treated on an outpatient basis. The most widely perform better in FBT than with individual ther-
researched form of outpatient treatment for child- apy. However, other components could include
hood EDs is family-based therapy (FBT). Clinical parental control over eating, changes in the fam-
researchers at the Maudsley Hospital in the United ily dynamics, or other unknown factors (Lock &
Kingdom developed FBT, and it is based on a Le Grange, 2005).
model of mobilizing family resources to help the In their randomized controlled trial comparing
family refeed the patient (Lock & Le Grange, FBT and adolescent-focused individual therapy
2015). This treatment has support from well-con- (AFT), Lock et al. (2010) examined 121 adoles-
ducted clinical studies (Lock, 2015). The APA cents with AN over a 12-month follow-up period.
(2006) and National Institute for Health and Care They found that although there was no significant
Excellence (NICE) (2017) guidelines for the difference between the groups at the end of treat-
treatment of EDs recommend family treatment ment, the FBT group had significantly higher
for the treatment of child and adolescent EDs. remission rates than the AFT group at 6-month
Early in treatment, the therapist emphasizes (40% versus 18%) and 12-month follow-ups
that the family is not at fault for the illness but (49% vs. 23%). The authors followed up a conve-
that they must take responsibility for helping to nience subgroup of the original sample (N = 79)
overcome it. Treatment consists of three major after 4 years and found that the outcomes had
phases. First, it focuses on the primary goal of remained stable, irrespective of treatment group
refeeding the patient. During this phase, the ther- (Le Grange, Lock et al., 2014).
354 J.K. Rosewall et al.

Subsequent research has investigated the opti- eating-­related obsessional thinking gained more
mal format and length of family therapy for weight in the longer treatment. Similarly, those
AN. Eisler et al. (2000) compared two forms of from non-intact families experienced greater
family therapy: conjoint family therapy (CFT) improvements in eating psychopathology in the
and separated family therapy (SFT). In CFT, 19 longer treatment. Across the two groups at 1 year,
adolescents with AN were seen together with 96% of patients no longer met criteria for AN,
their parents, and, in SFT, 21 adolescents with and 67% achieved a healthy BMI (>20). Thus, for
AN were seen separately from their parents. SFT the majority of AN patients (especially those
parents had regular sessions with the same thera- from intact families and those who are not excep-
pist. The goals and techniques used in both ther- tionally high on eating-related obsessionality), a
apy types were similar between the groups. The short form of FBT is likely to be as effective as
SFT group showed small and nonsignificant dif- standard-length treatment. These findings were
ferences in ED symptoms. However, more sub- maintained at a long-term follow-up (on average,
stantial benefits in general psychopathology 4 years), when no significant differences between
(mood, obsessionality, and psychosexual adjust- the groups were found and 89% of all patients
ment) followed CFT. SFT might be more appro- were at a healthy weight (Lock, Couturier, &
priate in families with high levels of conflict. In Agras, 2006).
families where frequent criticism from mothers Researchers have also started to explore adap-
was directed at the patient, SFT was significantly tive approaches for those who do not respond ini-
superior. Only four patients in this study required tially to FBT. Given the importance of early
concurrent hospitalization. In a smaller study, weight gain for symptom remission (Le Grange
these treatment formats were also compared et al., 2014), Lock et al. (2015) examined the fea-
among 18 adolescents with AN randomly sibility of using a novel three-session treatment
assigned to CFT or SFT (Le Grange, Eisler, Dare, called intensive parental coaching to enhance
& Russell, 1992). Inpatient treatment was also parental self-efficacy for those who were not
required during the course of treatment. Both gaining the weight expected in the early stages of
treatments brought about clinically significant FBT. The authors observed that the additional
improvements in weight and psychological func- coaching improved the weight recovery rates of
tioning, with few differences between the treat- the poor early responders to the level of those
ment formats. who responded early in treatment. Further
Lock, Agras, Bryson, and Kraemer (2005) research is needed to corroborate these findings.
examined the ideal length and dose of family Similarly, preliminary research into skills train-
therapy. These investigators compared the stan- ing and psychoeducation for parents/caregivers
dard therapy length of 20 sessions over 12 months of adolescents with AN related to the interper-
to a short form of therapy offering 10 sessions sonal challenges in the family that may maintain
over 6 months. Whereas the standard-length ther- AN has shown promising results. A randomized
apy covered all three phases of treatment, the controlled trial (the ECHO trial; Experienced
short form of therapy primarily focused on the Carers Helping Others) comparing guided self-­
first and second phases with less time for general help ECHO (in addition to TAU), self-help
adolescent concerns and building the family rela- ECHO (in addition to TAU), and TAU only is cur-
tionship. In this randomized controlled trial, 86 rently underway (Rhind et al., 2014).
adolescents with AN showed similar gains in Additional forms of family therapy have been
BMI, ED psychopathology, and general psycho- examined as well. A version of family therapy
pathology across both the short and long treat- called behavioral systems family therapy (BSFT)
ment conditions at 12 months. Although 19 has also been compared with an individual treat-
patients required hospitalization during treat- ment, ego-oriented individual treatment (EOIT,
ment, these were distributed evenly across the described below under psychodynamic treat-
two treatments. Patients with high levels of ment). BSFT was similar to the Maudsley model
Eating Disorders 355

of FBT, with a few subtle differences. Robin and were few hospitalizations in the FBT condition.
colleagues (1999) compared these treatments Of note, by 12 months, less than half of all of the
among 37 adolescents with AN, 16 of whom participants were recovered regardless of treat-
required concurrent hospitalization (11 BSFT ment condition suggesting that further research is
and 5 EOIT patients). Immediately after treat- needed to refine treatments in order to increase
ment and at a 12-month follow-up, patients in the recovery rates. (For a description of the treatment
BSFT group had gained more weight. A greater strategies used in FBT for adolescent BN, see Le
proportion of BSFT patients resumed menstrua- Grange, Lock, & Dymek, 2003).
tion after treatment (94% vs. 64%). This differ- In an earlier study, Le Grange, Crosby,
ence was no longer statistically significant at Rathouz, and Leventhal (2007) compared FBT to
follow-up, when both groups had similar rates of supportive psychotherapy (SPT), a nonspecific
menstruation recovery. However, the more rapid supportive treatment, for adolescents with
response of menstruation and weight gain to fam- BN. Eighty adolescents were randomly assigned
ily therapy suggests that BSFT was quicker act- to receive either 20 sessions of FBT or SPT over
ing than individual treatment. In a disorder as a 6-month period. The authors reported signifi-
medically compromising as AN, speed of recov- cantly higher binge and purge abstinence for the
ery can be an important consideration, and a FBT group, compared to SPT at end of treatment
faster-acting treatment would generally be more and at 6-month follow-up. FBT was also a faster-­
advisable. acting treatment than SPT and resulted in signifi-
Although AN in younger children is rare, a cantly more improvement on behavioral and
large case series also provided support for the use attitudinal aspects of eating pathology.
of FBT in this population. Thirty-two children Conversely, a case series described eight adoles-
(average 11.9 years) showed clinically significant cents with BN treated with FBT (Dodge, Hodes,
improvements in eating-disordered thinking pat- Eisler, & Dare, 1995). Standard FBT for AN was
terns and body weight gain following family modified to address compensatory behaviors and
therapy (Lock, Le Grange, Forsberg, & Hewell, shifted the focus from weight gain to regular eat-
2006). These patients closely resembled those in ing. At 12 months after the start of treatment,
a comparable adolescent sample before and after there were significant reductions in eating pathol-
treatment. This study suggested that efficacy of ogy and in the level of self-harm behaviors,
FBT did not depend on addressing issues of ado- which were initially present in half of the patients.
lescent development, and these issues may not be However, only one patient achieved a good out-
crucial to treatment, even with adolescents. come as defined by the Morgan-Russell criteria
There have been few randomized controlled listed earlier (Morgan & Russell, 1975).
trials of family therapy for adolescents with
BN. In a recent randomized controlled trial com- Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy
paring FBT with cognitive behavioral therapy CBT focuses on identifying and modifying dys-
(CBT), Le Grange, Lock, Agras, Bryson, and Jo functional thoughts and behaviors related to eat-
(2015) examined 130 adolescents with BN. ing, weight, and body shape. Therapists challenge
Participants were given treatment over 18 ses- patients’ thoughts in treatment through cognitive
sions (6 months in total) and followed up at 6 and restructuring and behavioral experiments.
12 months. The authors observed that greater Another primary goal of treatment is to establish
symptomatic improvement (abstinence from regular eating patterns, with the assistance of
binge eating and purging for a period of 4 weeks) self-monitoring and dietary planning (see Waller
occurred in the FBT group than the CBT group at et al., 2007 for a comprehensive CBT treatment
end of treatment and at 6-month follow-up. manual for EDs). Gowers (2006) argued that EDs
However, at 12-months, there were no significant are a classic example of a problem in which
differences between the two treatments. Along abnormal thoughts and behaviors combine to
with FBT being a faster-acting treatment, there result in physical and social disability; thus, he
356 J.K. Rosewall et al.

argued that CBT should, in theory, be effective. therapy (BT) and a low-contact treatment admin-
In their abovementioned randomized controlled istered by psychiatrists. Not surprising consider-
trial comparing specialist outpatient, generalized ing the small sample, the three treatments did not
outpatient care and inpatient treatment for ado- statistically differ from each other on outcome.
lescent AN, Gowers et al. (2007) reported that All patients improved significantly on nutritional
those in the specialist outpatient setting received status, menstrual functioning, and body weight.
a package of care which included up to 6 months However, patients had better treatment atten-
of CBT. Although the authors did not find any dance with CBT than with BT. CBT also resulted
significant differences between the treatments, in fewer early dropouts in a 12-month compari-
CBT was considered more cost effective (Byford son of CBT and nutritional counseling in adult
et al., 2007). AN patients following hospitalization. CBT
Enhanced CBT (CBT-E), developed by patients remained significantly longer without
Fairburn et al. (2003), is a transdiagnostic per- relapsing (44 vs. 27 sessions); 22% versus 53%
sonalized treatment for EDs that focuses on mod- of patients relapsed in CBT versus nutritional
ifying the mechanisms thought to maintain all counseling (Pike, Walsh, Vitousek, Wilson, &
forms of ED psychopathology. Given that not all Bauer, 2003). Similarly, a comparison of CBT
adolescents recover from AN or BN after receiv- and dietary counseling found a much lower drop-
ing FBT, and that it may not be suitable for all out rate with CBT; indeed, all patients dropped
families (Lock et al., 2010), researchers are start- out of dietary counseling by 3 months (Serfaty,
ing to investigate the effectiveness of CBT-E for Turkington, Heap, Ledsham, & Jolley, 1999). In
adolescent EDs. addition, all patients refused to provide data for a
In an uncontrolled study, Dalle Grave, Calugi, 6-month follow-up. This study dramatically illus-
Conti, Doll, and Fairburn (2013) examined 46 trates some of the difficulties encountered in con-
adolescent girls who received 40 sessions of ducting research with those with AN. In addition,
CBT-E as a sole treatment. Participants also Fairburn (2005) argued that nutritional counsel-
received two 60-min preparatory sessions and ing without concurrent psychotherapy is not a
one review session 20 weeks after the end of sufficiently rigorous comparison group against
treatment. Parents were intermittently involved in which to test CBT.
the therapy (an initial 1-h assessment within the In an uncontrolled study, Fairburn and col-
first 2 weeks and eight 15-min sessions through- leagues (2013) examined the immediate and
out therapy). The authors reported that 63% com- long-term outcome for 99 adults with AN follow-
pleted the treatment, 19.6% were classed as ing CBT-E. For those who completed the treat-
nonresponders (e.g., needed additional support ment (64%), there was a reasonable increase in
post therapy or failed to make sustained prog- weight (M = 7.47 kg) with 62% achieving a BMI
ress), and 17.4% dropped out. Among the treat- over 18.5. These gains tended to be maintained
ment completers, there was a substantial after a 60-week follow-up period, although those
improvement in ED psychopathology and weight, with a BMI over 18.5 reduced to 55%. ED psy-
although only 32.1% of completers gained weight chopathology also improved. Similar to the Dalle
to reach 95% of their expected weight. Changes Grave et al. (2013) study, these findings provide
were maintained after a 60-week follow-up support for the use of CBT-E; however, random-
period, and 44.8% had reached 95% of expected ized controlled trials are needed to confirm its
weight. The authors argued that these promising therapeutic efficacy for AN.
findings indicate the need for randomized con- Interestingly, the results of one study cast
trolled trial comparing CBT-E and FBT in AN. doubt on the superiority of CBT in a comparison
CBT for adult AN has been tested in a small to another manualized psychotherapy, interper-
number of clinical trials. For example, in 24 adult sonal therapy (IPT), and to a nonspecific clinical
AN patients, Channon, de Silva, Hemsley, and management condition providing supportive psy-
Perkins (1989) compared CBT to both behavior chotherapy (McIntosh et al., 2005). Patients were
Eating Disorders 357

55 women (aged 17–40) diagnosed with AN treatment for BN in their evidence-based guide-
using a slightly higher than usual weight criterion lines for the treatment of EDs. The efficacy of
to define the disorder (BMI <19). Thirty percent CBT for BN has been supported by strong evi-
of all patients were considered much improved or dence from randomized controlled trials. CBT
had minimal symptoms after treatment. However, involves weekly individual sessions over
despite the authors’ predictions, the nonspecific 4–5 months and typically results in complete
control treatment was superior to CBT and IPT remission in about 40% of cases (Wilson &
on global measures of ED symptoms. Thus, there Fairburn, 2002). Treatment does not typically
is not yet strong support for the use of any spe- affect patients’ body weight. The majority of
cific individual psychotherapy for adult therapeutic gains occur in the first few sessions of
AN. Although CBT has been associated with treatment, significantly sooner than in compari-
improved psychological and physical outcomes son treatments (Wilson et al., 1999). This finding
as well as reduced dropout rates, it has yet to suggests that CBT is relatively fast acting.
show superiority to other treatments for AN (see Similarly, more patients achieved remission by
Galsworthy-Francis & Allan, 2014, for a review). the end of CBT than by the end of IPT, although
In addition to AN research, Schmidt et al. this difference leveled off by a 12-month follow-
(2007) compared the effectiveness of CBT guided ­up (Agras, Walsh, Fairburn, Wilson, & Kraemer,
self-help and FT in adolescent BN. In their sam- 2000; Fairburn et al., 2015). Randomized con-
ple of 85 participants, binge eating significantly trolled trials studying a wide range of EDs
reduced in the CBT group after 6 months; how- together have also demonstrated the efficacy for
ever, no significant difference was seen between CBT-E for those above and below a BMI of 17.5
the two treatments at 12 months. The authors (Byrne, Fursland, Allen, & Watson, 2011;
attributed the initial superiority of CBT to the Fairburn et al., 2009).
modality focus on reduction of binges for recov- Although experts recommend CBT for ado-
ery. Of note, almost a third of potential partici- lescents with BN when FBT is ineffective or
pants who refused to join the study reported not unacceptable (NICE, 2017), it is important that
wishing to have family involved in their treat- age-related modifications be made to fit the ado-
ment. This finding suggests that adolescents may lescent patient’s level of development and cir-
be more motivated to receive CBT rather than cumstances. It is also essential that the patient’s
FBT and highlights the importance of also offer- family be included as appropriate. Lock (2015)
ing individualized appointments to adolescents recommended that treatment for adolescent BN
(as per NICE guidance; NICE, 2017). Similar to considers normal developmental tasks during
the Le Grange et al. (2015) study also comparing adolescence, involving families where possible
CBT and FT mentioned earlier, there were no to enhance skills generalization and being aware
significant differences between the treatments at of expected emotional limitations given their age.
12 months; however, this study had the opposite Again, although inferences can only be made
outcome at the end of treatment and at 6 months. from research with adults, CBT is also estab-
The findings of Schmidt and colleagues may lished as a treatment for BED, efficacious in
have been impacted by a smaller sample size. Put reducing binge eating and associated psychopa-
together, the research suggests that both modali- thology, even over long-term follow-up (Vocks
ties may be viable treatment options for adoles- et al., 2010). Generally, CBT does not produce
cents with BN but CBT may be more suitable for clinically significant weight loss (Berkman et al.,
those older adolescents who do not wish to have 2015) and thus does not effectively treat the obe-
their families involved in their treatment. sity often associated with BED. Descriptive
For adults with BN, CBT is considered the research has now documented the presence of
treatment of choice (Hay, 2013). For example, BED among a proportion of children presenting
both the APA (2006) and the NICE (2017) rec- for obesity treatment (Decaluwe & Braet, 2003).
ommended CBT as the leading evidence-based Therefore, evaluating the effect on childhood
358 J.K. Rosewall et al.

BED of weight control treatment and other thera- (emphasizing psychoeducational techniques),
pies should be a research priority. administered to 30 adult AN patients. This study,
as well as that of Dare et al. (2001), may have
Psychodynamic Therapy been underpowered. In an adult study comparing
One randomized trial has examined a form of 2 years of psychoanalytic psychotherapy and
psychodynamic therapy for adolescents with 5 months of CBT for BN (Poulsen et al., 2014),
AN. The study by Robin et al. (1999), described those who received CBT had higher remission
earlier, compared a version of family therapy to rates at 5 months (42%) than those who received
ego-oriented individual treatment (EOIT). EOIT psychoanalytic psychotherapy (6%) as well as at
emphasized developing ego strength, learning 2-year follow-up (44% versus 15%). Although
coping skills, individuating from the family, and patients might get better with psychoanalytic
identifying and modifying any dynamics that psychotherapy, this study again suggests that
may be blocking eating. EOIT led to decreases CBT is faster acting and more effective over time.
similar to family therapy in conflicts during fam- Based on the research so far, there is no compel-
ily interactions even though sessions were indi- ling evidence that psychodynamic therapy is
vidually conducted; however, EOIT took effect more effective than alternative specialized treat-
less immediately than family therapy. ments for AN or BN with adolescents or adults.
Time-limited versions of psychodynamic
treatment for AN have also been tested with Interpersonal Therapy
adults. A randomized controlled trial investigated IPT is a specific, time-limited form of psychody-
three specialized treatments and a low-contact namic treatment that focuses on resolving inter-
control treatment in 84 women with AN (Dare, personal difficulties that contribute to the onset or
Eisler, Russell, Treasure, & Dodge, 2001). Focal maintenance of the disorder. Four potential prob-
psychoanalytic therapy addressed the meaning of lem areas typically constitute the focus of treat-
the patients’ symptoms in light of their history ment: grief, interpersonal disputes, role
and family relationships, as well as the effect of transitions, and interpersonal deficits. The study
these symptoms on their relationships (including discussed above, which compared CBT, IPT, and
the relationship with the therapist). This treat- nonspecific clinical management in adult women
ment was compared to cognitive analytic treat- with AN, found IPT to be the least efficacious of
ment (CAT), in which components of cognitive these three treatments (McIntosh et al., 2005).
therapy were integrated with components of psy- IPT has shown similar efficacy to CBT in adults
chodynamic therapy such as interpersonal and with BN, but its benefits may be more delayed
transference issues. Family therapy was the third (Agras et al., 2000; Fairburn et al., 2015).
specialized treatment tested in this investigation. Research also supports the use of IPT for BED in
After 12 months, the three specialized treatments adults (Wilfley et al., 2002) as well as its use with
were similar in outcome, and both focal psycho- other problems (i.e., depression) among adoles-
analytic therapy and family therapy were supe- cents (O’Shea, Spence, & Donovan, 2015).
rior to the control treatment. However, patients However, the lack of research specifically on IPT
did poorly in all treatments. Only 30% of patients for EDs in this age group suggests that this treat-
in the three treatment groups no longer met crite- ment should not be considered an optimal first-­
ria for AN (compared to 5% of patients in the choice intervention.
control treatment). The study may have had
insufficient power to detect differences among
the specialized treatments, and patients had a Summary of Treatment Literature
long history of illness (6.3 years on average),
indicating poor prognosis. Similarly, Treasure Overall, the state of the research base varies
et al. (1995) found no differences between CAT depending on which disorder and which age
and another specialized therapy, behavior therapy group is being considered. The prognosis is bet-
Eating Disorders 359

ter for children and adolescents than for adults, BED may have the best prognosis, although
but there is still limited research with younger research with children and adolescents is clearly
ages. One limitation with the current literature on needed. More research with other atypical vari-
treatment of child and adolescent EDs is that ants of EDs, particularly given how common they
much of it has been based on adults rather than have been in clinical practice, is also necessary.
children and/or adolescents. Extrapolating from More research on the treatment of various sub-
the adult literature may or may not be appropri- types of OSFED as well as more controlled
ate. Gowers and Bryant-Waugh (2004) listed four research on the transdiagnostic approach
arguments in favor of such extrapolation and five described by Fairburn et al. (2003) and how this
reasons why such extrapolation may not be model applies to children and adolescents would
warranted. help fill these gaps in the literature. Finally,
In the last 10 years, there has been an increase research could consider novel approaches to
in controlled trials for child and adolescent EDs. working with adolescents, such as web-based and
In a recent systematic review of psychosocial technologically enhanced guided self-help
interventions for adolescents EDs, Lock (2015) approaches, as well as looking at ways to enhance
concluded that, for adolescents with AN, the existing family-based interventions to improve
only well-established treatment is FBT. Recent outcomes.
randomized controlled studies have confirmed
the efficacy of this approach; however, not all
families or adolescents with AN benefit from Concluding Remarks
FBT, suggesting the need for continued research
into effective and novel interventions for adoles- The EDs are potentially life-threatening condi-
cent AN. There is growing support for interven- tions that are also treatable. Early detection and
tions that are offered to an adolescent (e.g., intervention is crucial, and expertise in assessment
CBT-E), with NICE (2017) recommending these and treatment of EDs is valuable for those working
if FBT is contraindicated or ineffective, and with children and adolescents (Campbell &
although these may be more acceptable to the Peebles, 2014). Although the treatment literature
young person, further randomized controlled has increased in the past decade, more robust
studies are necessary. research is needed with these age groups to
For BN, there is also a notable lack of research enhance current treatments and to explore novel
specifically with adolescents or children, treatment approaches in order to improve treat-
although the randomized trials that have been ment outcomes for adolescents with EDs.
completed (Le Grange et al., 2007, 2015; Schmidt
et al., 2007) suggest that FBT and CBT might
both be effective. However, little is known about References
which type of patient would benefit from which
therapy. The efficacy of CBT for BN among Agras, W. S. (2001). The consequences and costs of
the eating disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North
adults (whose samples often include adolescents) America, 24, 371–379. https://doi.org/10.1016/
has been well established, and NICE (2017) rec- S0193-953X(05)70232-X
ommended CBT as the leading evidence-based Agras, W. S., Walsh, B. T., Fairburn, C. G., Wilson, G. T.,
treatment for BN. Among children and adoles- & Kraemer, H. C. (2000). A multicenter comparison
of cognitive-behavioral therapy and interpersonal psy-
cents, they recommend FBT as the first line treat- chotherapy for bulimia nervosa. Archives of General
ment for BN; however, for those where FBT is Psychiatry, 57, 459–466. https://doi.org/10.1001/
unacceptable, contraindicated or ineffective, archpsyc.57.5.459
CBT should be offered. IPT for BN has been Allen, K. L., Byrne, S., Oddy, W. H., & Crosby, R. D.
(2013). DSM-IV-TR and DSM-5 eating disorders in
studied among adults and also among adoles- adolescents: Prevalence, stability, and psychosocial
cents for different problems but not for BN. correlates in a population-based sample of male and
360 J.K. Rosewall et al.

female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Bulik, C. M., Klump, K. L., Thornton, L., Kaplan, A. S.,
122, 720–732. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034004 Devlin, B., Fichter, M. M., … Kaye, W. H. (2004).
Altman, M., & Wilfley, D. E. (2015). Evidence update Alcohol use disorder comorbidity in eating disorders:
on the treatment of overweight and obesity in chil- A multicenter study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
dren and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and 65, 1000–1006. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v65n0718
Adolescent Psychology, 44, 521–537. https://doi.org/1 Byford, S., Barrett, B., Roberts, C., Clark, A., Edwards,
0.1080/15374416.2014.963854 V., Smethurst, N., & Gowers, S. G. (2007). Economic
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic evaluation of a randomised controlled trial for
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th. Text anorexia nervosa in adolescents. The British Journal
Revision ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric of Psychiatry, 191, 436–440. https://doi.org/10.1192/
Association. bjp.bp.107.036806
American Psychiatric Association. (2006). Practice Byrne, S. M., Fursland, A., Allen, K. L., & Watson, H.
guidelines for the treatment of patients with eating (2011). The effectiveness of enhanced cognitive
disorders (3rd ed.). http://psychiatryonline.org/pb/ behavioural therapy for eating disorders: An open
assets/raw/sitewide/practice_guidelines/guidelines/ trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 219–226.
eatingdisorders.pdf https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.01.006
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic Campbell, K., & Peebles, R. (2014). Eating disorders
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). in children and adolescents: State of the art review.
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. Pediatrics, 134, 582–592. https://doi.org/10.1542/
Anzai, N., Lindsey-Dudley, K., & Bidwell, R. J. (2002). peds.2014-0194
Inpatient and partial hospital treatment for adolescent Carlat, D. J., Camargo, C. A., & Herzog, D. B. (1997).
eating disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Eating disorders in males: A report on 135 patients.
Clinics of North America, 11, 279–309. https://doi. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1127–1132.
org/10.1016/S1056-4993(01)00015-3 https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.154.8.1127
Arcelus, J., Mitchell, A., Wales, J., & Nielsen, S. (2011). Channon, S., de Silva, P., Hemsley, D., & Perkins, R.
Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and (1989). A controlled trial of cognitive-behavioral and
other eating disorders: A meta-analysis of 36 studies. behavioral treatment of anorexia nervosa. Behaviour
Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 724–731. https:// Research and Therapy, 27, 529–535. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.74 org/10.1016/0005-7967(89)90087-9
Austin, S. B., Ziyadeh, N., Kahn, J. A., Camargo, C. A., Colton, A., & Pistrang, N. (2004). Adolescents’ expe-
Colditz, G. A., & Field, A. E. (2004). Sexual orienta- riences of inpatient treatment of anorexia nervosa.
tion, weight concerns, and eating-disordered behaviors European Eating Disorders Review, 12, 307–316.
in adolescent girls and boys. Journal of the American https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.587
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Conason, A. H., Brunstein Klomek, A., & Sher, L. (2006).
43, 1115–1123. https://doi.org/10.1097/01. Recognizing alcohol and drug abuse in patients with
chi.0000131139.93862.10 eating disorders. QJM: An International Journal
Beals, K. A. (2004). Disordered eating among athletes: of Medicine, 99, 335–339. https://doi.org/10.1093/
A comprehensive guide for health professionals. qjmed/hcl030
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Cotrufo, P., Gnisci, A., & Caputo, I. (2005). Psychological
Berkman, N. D., Brownley, K. A., Peat, C. M., Lohr, characteristics of less severe forms of eating disorders:
K. N., Cullen, K. E., Morgan, L.C, …, Bulik, C. M An epidemiological study among 259 female adoles-
(2015). Management and outcomes of binge-eating cents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 28, 147–154.
disorder. Comparative Effectiveness Review, 160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.07.006
Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Couturier, J., Kimber, M., & Szatmari, P. (2013). Efficacy
Quality. of family-based treatment for adolescents with eat-
Brambilla, F., & Monteleone, P. (2003). Physical com- ing disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
plications and physiological aberrations in eating International Journal of Eating Disorders, 46, 3–11.
disorders. In M. Maj, K. Halmi, J. J. Lopez-Ibor, https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22042
& N. Sartorius (Eds.), Evidence and experience in Crisp, A., Norton, K., Gowers, S., Halek, C., Bowyer, C.,
psychiatry: Vol. 6. Eating disorders (pp. 139–192). Yeldham, D., … Bhat, A. (1991). A controlled study of
Chichester, England: Wiley. the effect of therapies aimed at adolescent and family
Bryant-Waugh, R. (2013). Feeding and eating disorders in psychopathology in anorexia nervosa. British Journal
children. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 26, 537–542. of Psychiatry, 159, 325–333. https://doi.org/10.1192/
https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e328365a34b bjp.159.3.325
Bühren, K., Schwarte, R., Fluck, F., Timmesfeld, N., Krei, Croll, J., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., & Ireland,
M., Egberts, K., … Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2014). M. (2002). Prevalence and risk and protective fac-
Comorbid psychiatric disorders in female adolescents tors related to disordered eating behaviors among
with first-onset anorexia nervosa. European Eating adolescents: Relationship to gender and ethnicity.
Disorders Review, 22, 39–44. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 166–175. https://
erv.2254 doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00368-3
Eating Disorders 361

Dalle Grave, R., Calugi, S., Conti, M., Doll, H., & Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H. A., Norman, P., &
Fairburn, C. G. (2013). Inpatient cognitive behaviour O’Connor, M. (2000). The natural course of bulimia
therapy for anorexia nervosa: A randomized con- nervosa and binge eating disorder in young women.
trolled trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 82, Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 659–665. https://
390–398. https://doi.org/10.1159/000350058 doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.7.659
Dansky, B. S., Brewerton, T. D., & Kilpatrick, D. G. Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H. A., O’Connor,
(2000). Comorbidity of bulimia nervosa and alcohol M. E., Bohn, K., Hawker, D. M., … Palmer, R. L.
use disorders: Results from the National Women’s (2009). Transdiagnostic cognitive-behavioral ther-
study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, apy for patients with eating disorders: A two-site
27, 180–190. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098- trial with 60-week follow-up. American Journal of
108X(200003)27:2<180::AID-EAT6>3.0.CO;2-Z Psychiatry, 166, 311–319. https://doi.org/10.1176/
Danziger, Y., Carcl, C. A., Varsano, I., Tyano, S., & appi.ajp.2008.08040608
Mimouni, M. (1988). Parental involvement in treat- Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H. A., O’Connor, M. E.,
ment of patients with anorexia nervosa in a pediatric Palmer, R. L., & Dalle Grave, R. (2013). Enhanced
day-care unit. Pediatrics, 81, 159–162. Retrieved from cognitive behaviour therapy for adults with anorexia
https://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/81/1. nervosa: A UK–Italy study. Behaviour Research
Dare, C., Eisler, I., Russell, G., Treasure, J., & Dodge, and Therapy, 51, R2–R8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
E. (2001). Psychological therapies for adults with brat.2012.09.010
anorexia nervosa: Randomised controlled trial of out-­ Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003).
patient treatments. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A
216–221. “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behaviour
Davies, S., & Jaffa, T. (2005). Patterns of weekly weight Research and Therapy, 41, 509–528. https://doi.
gain during inpatient treatment for adolescents with org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00088-8
anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, Favaro, A., Caregaro, L., Tenconi, E., Bosello, R., &
13, 273–277. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.652 Santonastaso, P. (2009). Time trends in age at onset
Davison, G. C. (2000). Stepped care: Doing more with less? of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, Clinical Psychiatry, 70, 1715–1721.
580–585. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.580 Fichter, M. M., Quadflieg, N., Georgopoulou, E.,
Decaluwe, V., & Braet, C. (2003). Prevalence of binge-­ Xepapadakos, F., & Fthenakis, E. W. (2005). Time
eating disorder in obese children and adolescents trends in eating disturbances in young Greek migrants.
seeking weight-loss treatment. International Journal International Journal of Eating Disorders, 38, 310–
of Obesity, 27, 404–409. https://doi.org/10.1038/ 322. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20187
sj.ijo.0802233 Fisher, M., Schneider, M., Burns, J., Symons, H., &
Dodge, E., Hodes, M., Eisler, I., & Dare, C. (1995). Mandel, F. S. (2001). Differences between adolescents
Family therapy for bulimia nervosa in adolescents: and young adults at presentation to an eating disorders
An exploratory study. Journal of Family Therapy, program. Journal of Adolescent Health, 28, 222–227.
17, 59–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6427.1995. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(00)00182-8
tb00004.x Galsworthy-Francis, L., & Allan, S. (2014). Cognitive
Eisler, I., Dare, C., Hodes, M., Russell, G., Dodge, E., & behavioural therapy for anorexia nervosa: A system-
Le Grange, D. (2000). Family therapy for adolescent atic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 54–72.
anorexia nervosa: The results of a controlled com- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.11.001
parison of two family interventions. Journal of Child Geist, R., Heinmaa, M., Katzman, D., & Stephens, D.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 727–736. https://doi. (1999). A comparison of male and female adolescents
org/10.1111/1469-7610.00660 referred to an eating disorder program. Canadian
Fairburn, C. G. (2005). Evidence-based treatment Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 374–378. https://doi.
of anorexia nervosa. International Journal of org/10.1177/070674379904400408
Eating Disorders, 37(Suppl), S26–S30. https://doi. Gleaves, D. H., Lowe, M. R., Green, B. A., Cororve,
org/10.1002/eat.20112 M. B., & Williams, T. L. (2000). Do anorexia and
Fairburn, C. G., Bailey-Straebler, S., Basden, S., Doll, bulimia nervosa occur on a continuum? A taxometric
H. A., Jones, R., Murphy, R., … Cooper, Z. (2015). analysis. Behavior Therapy, 31, 195–219. https://doi.
A transdiagnostic comparison of enhanced cogni- org/10.1016/S0005-7894(00)80012-X
tive behaviour therapy (CBT-E) and interpersonal Goldstein, M., Peters, L., Baillie, A., McVeagh, P.,
psychotherapy in the treatment of eating disorders. Minshall, G., & Fitzjames, D. (2011). The effec-
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 70, 64–71. https:// tiveness of a day program for the treatment of ado-
doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.04.010 lescent anorexia nervosa. International Journal of
Fairburn, C. G., & Bohn, K. (2005). Eating disorder Eating Disorders, 44, 29–38. https://doi.org/10.1002/
NOS (EDNOS): An example of the troublesome eat.20789
“not otherwise specified” (NOS) category in DSM– Gowers, S., & Bryant-Waugh, R. (2004). Management
IV. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 691–701. of child and adolescent eating disorders: The current
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.06.011 evidence base and future directions. Journal of Child
362 J.K. Rosewall et al.

Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 63–83. https://doi. Keel, P. K., Heatherton, T. D., Dorer, D. J., Joiner, T. E.,
org/10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00309.x & Zalta, A. K. (2006). Point prevalence of bulimia
Gowers, S. G. (2006). Evidence based research in nervosa in 1982, 1992, and 2002. Psychological
CBT with adolescent eating disorders. Child and Medicine, 36, 119–127. https://doi.org/10.1017/
Adolescent Mental Health, 11, 9–12. https://doi. S0033291705006148
org/10.1111/j.1475-3588.2005.00348.x Kjelsås, E., Bjørnstrøm, C., & Götestam, K. G. (2004).
Gowers, S. G., Clark, A., Roberts, C., Griffiths, A., Prevalence of eating disorders in female and male ado-
Edwards, V., Bryan, C., … Barrett, B. (2007). Clinical lescents (14–15 years). Eating Behaviors, 5, 13–25.
effectiveness of treatments for anorexia nervosa in ado- https://doi.org/10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00057-6
lescents. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 427– Kotler, L. A., Cohen, P., Davies, M., Pine, D. S., & Walsh,
435. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.107.036764435 B. T. (2001). Longitudinal relationships between
Gowers, S. G., Clark, A. F., Roberts, C., Byford, S., childhood, adolescent, and adult eating disorders.
Barrett, B., Griffiths, A., … Roots, P. (2010). A ran- Journal of the American Academy of Child and
domised controlled multicenter trial of treatments for Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1434–1440. https://doi.
adolescent anorexia nervosa including assessment org/10.1097/00004583-200112000-00014
of cost-effectiveness and patient acceptability – the Labre, M. P. (2002). Adolescent boys and the mus-
TOuCAN trial. Health Technology Assessment, 14, cular male body ideal. Journal of Adolescent
1–98. https://doi.org/10.3310/hta14150 Health, 30, 233–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Gowers, S. G., Weetman, J., Shore, A., Hossain, F., & S1054-139X(01)00413-X
Elvins, R. (2000). Impact of hospitalisation on the out- Lampard, A. M., Tasca, G. A., Balfour, L., & Bissada, H.
come of adolescent anorexia nervosa. British Journal (2013). An evaluation of the transdiagnostic cognitive-­
of Psychiatry, 176, 138–141. https://doi.org/10.1192/ behavioral model of eating disorders. European
bjp.176.2.138 Eating Disorders Review, 21, 99–102. https://doi.
Grilo, C. M., Ivezas, V., & White, M. A. (2015). Evaluation org/10.1002/erv.2214
of the DSM-5 severity indicator for binge eating dis- Lask, B., & Bryant-Waugh, R. (Eds.). (2013). Eating
order in a clinical sample. Behaviour Research and disorders in childhood and adolescence. New York:
Therapy, 71, 110–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Routledge Press. https://doi.org/10.1080/10640266.2
brat.2015.05.003 013.828529
Grilo, C. M., Sanislow, C. A., Shea, M. T., Skodol, A., Latner, J. D., Hildebrandt, T., Rosewall, J. K., Chisholm,
Stout, R. L., Pagano, M. E., … McGlashan, T. H. A. M., & Hayashi, K. (2007). Loss of control over eat-
(2003). The natural course of bulimia nervosa and ing reflects eating disturbances and general psychopa-
eating disorder not otherwise specified is not influ- thology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2203–2211.
enced by personality disorders. International Journal https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.12.002
of Eating Disorders, 34, 319–330. https://doi. Le Grange, D., Accurso, E. C., Lock, J., Agras, S., &
org/10.1002/eat.10196 Bryson, S. W. (2014). Early weight gain predicts out-
Hartmann, A., Weber, S., Herpetz, S., & Zeeck, A. (2011). come in two treatments for adolescent anorexia ner-
Psychological treatment for anorexia nervosa: A meta-­ vosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47,
analysis of standardized mean change. Psychotherapy 124–129. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22221
and Psychosomatics, 80, 216–226. https://doi. Le Grange, D., Crosby, R. D., Rathouz, P. J., & Leventhal,
org/10.1159/000322360 B. L. (2007). A randomized controlled comparison of
Hay, P. (2013). A systematic review of evidence for psy- family-based treatment and supportive psychotherapy
chological treatments in eating disorders: 2005–2012. for adolescent bulimia nervosa. Archives of General
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 46, 462– Psychiatry, 64, 1049–1056. https://doi.org/10.1001/
469. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22103 archpsyc.64.9.1049
Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Schwarte, R., Krei, M., Egberts, Le Grange, D., Doyle, P. M., Swanson, S. A., Ludwig,
K., Warnke, A., Wewetzer, C., … Dempfle, A. K., Glunz, C., & Kreipe, R. E. (2012). Calculation
(2014). Day-patient treatment after short inpatient of expected body weight in adolescents with eating
care versus continued inpatient treatment in adoles- disorders. Pediatrics, 129, e438–e446. e438–e446.
cents with anorexia nervosa (ANDI): A multicentre, https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1676
randomised, open-label, non-inferiority trial. The Le Grange, D., Eisler, I., Dare, C., & Russell, G. F. M.
Lancet, 9924, 1222–1229. https://doi.org/10.1016/ (1992). Evaluation of family therapy in anorexia
S0140-6736(13)62411-3 nervosa: A pilot study. International Journal
Hoek, H. W., & van Hoeken, D. (2003). Review of of Eating Disorder, 12, 347–357. https://doi.
the prevalence and incidence of eating disorders. org/10.1002/1098-108X(199212)12:4<347::AID-
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34, 383– EAT2260120402>3.0.CO;2-W
396. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10222 Le Grange, D., Lock, J., Accurso, E. C., Agras, W. S.,
Hong, E., & Dixon, D. (this volume). Pica. In J. L. Matson Darcy, A., Forsberg, S., & Bryson, S. W. (2014).
(Ed.), Handbook of childhood psychopathology and Relapse from remission at two- to four-year follow-
developmental disabilities: Treatment. Springer. ­up in two treatments for adolescent anorexia nervosa.
Eating Disorders 363

Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Lock, J., Le Grange, D., Forsberg, S., & Hewell, K.
Psychiatry, 53, 1162–1167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. (2006). Is family therapy useful for treating chil-
jaac.2014.07.014 dren with anorexia nervosa? Results of a case series.
Le Grange, D., Lock, J., Agras, W. S., Bryson, S. W., & Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Jo, B. (2015). Randomized clinical trial of family-­ Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 1323–1328. https://doi.
based treatment and cognitive-behavioral therapy for org/10.1097/01.chi.0000233208.43427.4c
adolescent Bulimia Nervosa. Journal of the American Lund, B. C., Hernandez, E. R., Yates, W. R., Mitchell,
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, J. R., McKee, P. A., & Johnson, C. L. (2008). Rate
886–894. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.08.008 of inpatient weight restoration predicts outcome in
Le Grange, D., Lock, J., & Dymek, M. (2003). Family-­ anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating
based therapy for adolescents with bulimia nervosa. Disorders, 42, 301–305. https://doi.org/10.1002/
American Journal of Psychotherapy, 57, 237–251. eat.20634
Leehr, E. J., Krohmer, K., Schag, K., Dresler, T., Zipfel, Madden, S., Miskovic-Wheatley, J., Wallis, A., Kohn,
S., & Giel, K. (2015). Emotion regulation model in M., Lock, J., Le Grange, D., … Touyz, S. (2015). A
binge eating disorder and obesity: A systematic review. randomized controlled trial of in-patient treatment for
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 49, 125– anorexia nervosa in medically unstable adolescents.
134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.008 Psychological Medicine, 45, 415–427. https://doi.
Lewinsohn, P. M., Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Seeley, J. R. org/10.1017/S0033291714001573
(2000). Epidemiology and natural course of eating Mann, A. P., Accurso, E. C., Stiles-Shields, C., Capra,
disorders in young women from adolescence to young L., Labuschagne, Z., Karnik, N. S., & Le Grange, D.
adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child (2014). Factors associated with substance use in ado-
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1284–1292. https:// lescents with eating disorders. Journal of Adolescent
doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200010000-00016 Health, 55, 182–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Lock, J. (2015). An update on evidence-based psycho- jadohealth.2014.01.015
social treatments for eating disorders in children and McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. K. (2000). An exploration
adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent of the drive for muscularity in adolescent boys and
Psychology, 44, 707–721. https://doi.org/10.1080/153 girls. Journal of American College Health, 48, 297–
74416.2014.971458 304. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448480009596271
Lock, J., Agras, W. S., Bryson, S., & Kraemer, H. C. McIntosh, V. V. W., Jordan, J., Carter, F. A., Luty, S. E.,
(2005). A comparison of short- and long-term fam- McKenzie, J. M., Bulik, C. M., … Joyce, P. R.
ily therapy for adolescent anorexia nervosa. Journal (2005). Three psychotherapies for anorexia nervosa:
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 44, 632–639. https://doi.org/10.1097/01. Psychiatry, 162, 741–747. https://doi.org/10.1176/
chi.0000161647.82775.0a appi.ajp.162.4.741
Lock, J., Couturier, J., & Agras, W. S. (2006). Comparison Measelle, J. R., Stice, E., & Hogansen, J. M. (2006).
of long-term outcomes in adolescents with anorexia Developmental trajectories of co-occurring depres-
nervosa treated with family therapy. Journal of sive, eating, antisocial, and substance abuse
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent problems in female adolescents. Journal of
Psychiatry, 45, 666–672. https://doi.org/10.1097/01. Abnormal Psychology, 115, 524–538. ­https://doi.
chi.0000215152.61400.ca org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.524
Lock, J., & Le Grange, D. (2005). Family-based treat- Merikangas, K. R., He, J., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A.,
ment of eating disorders. International Journal of Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., … Swendsen, J. (2010).
Eating Disorders, 37(Suppl), S64–S67. https://doi. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US ado-
org/10.1002/eat.20122 lescents: Results from the National Comorbidity
Lock, J., & Le Grange, D. (2015). Treatment manual for Study-Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal
anorexia nervosa: A family-based approach (2nd ed.). of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
New York: Guilford Press. Psychiatry, 49, 980–989. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Lock, J., Le Grange, D., Agras, W. S., Fitzpatrick, K. K., jaac.2010.05.017
Jo, B., Accurso, E., … Stainer, M. (2015). Can adap- Micali, N., Hagberg, K. W., Petersen, I., & Treasure,
tive treatment improve outcomes in family-based ther- J. L. (2013). Incidence of eating disorders in the UK
apy for adolescents with anorexia nervosa? Feasibility in 2000–2009: Findings from the general practice
and treatment effects of a multi-site treatment study. research database. BMJ Open, 3, e002646. https://doi.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 73, 90–95. https:// org/10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002646
doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.07.015 Mitchison, D., & Hay, P. (2014). The epidemiology of
Lock, J., Le Grange, D., Agras, W. S., Moye, A., Bryson, eating disorders: Genetic, environmental, and societal
S. W., & Jo, B. (2010). Randomized clinical trial com- factors. Clinical Epidemiology, 6, 89–97. https://doi.
paring family-based treatment with adolescent-focused org/10.2147/CLEP.S40841
individual therapy for adolescents with anorexia ner- Morgan, H. G., & Russell, G. F. M. (1975). Value of
vosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 1025–1032. family background and clinical features as predic-
https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.128 tors of long-term outcome in anorexia nervosa: Four
364 J.K. Rosewall et al.

year follow-up study of 41 patients. Psychological randomized controlled trial of psychoanalytic psycho-
Medicine, 5, 335–371. https://doi.org/10.1017/ therapy or cognitive-behavioral therapy for bulimia
S0033291700056981 nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 109–
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2017). 116. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12121511
Eating Disorders: Recognition and Treatment. (NICE Rastam, M., Gillberg, C., & Garton, M. (1989). Anorexia
Guideline 69). Retrieved from https://www.nice.org. nervosa in a Swedish urban region: A population based
uk/guidance/ng69. study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 642–646.
Netemeyer, S. B., & Williamson, D. A. (2001). Assessment https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.155.5.642
of eating disturbance in children and adolescents with Rhind, C., Hibbs, R., Goddard, E., Schmidt, U., Micali,
eating disorders and obesity. In J. K. Thompson & N., Gowers, S., … Treasure, J. (2014). Experienced
L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image, eating disorders, and Carers Helping Others (ECHO): Protocol for a pilot
obesity in youth: Assessment, prevention, and treat- randomised controlled trial to examine a psycho-­
ment (pp. 215–233). Washington, DC: American educational intervention for adolescents with anorexia
Psychological Association. nervosa and their carers. European Eating Disorders
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Wall, M., Larson, N., Story, M., Review, 22, 267–277. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2298
Fulkerson, J. A., Eisenberg, M. E., & Hannan, P. J. Robin, A. L., Gilroy, M., & Dennis, A. B. (1998).
(2012). Secular trends in weight status and weight-­ Treatment of eating disorders in children and ado-
related attitudes and behaviors in adolescents from lescents. Clinical Psychology Review, 18, 421–446.
1999 to 2010. Preventive Medicine, 4, 77–81. https:// https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00013-0
doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.10.003 Robin, A. L., Siegel, P. T., Moye, A. W., Gilroy, M., Dennis,
Nicholls, D., Chater, R., & Lask, B. (2000). Children A. B., & Sikand, A. (1999). A controlled comparison of
into DSM don’t go: A comparison of classifica- family versus individual therapy for adolescents with
tion systems for eating disorders in children and anorexia nervosa. Journal of the American Academy
early adolescence. International Journal of Eating of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1482–1489.
Disorders, 28, 317–324. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098- https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199912000-00008
108X(200011)28:3<317::AID-EAT9>3.0.CO;2-# Robinson, A. L., Strahan, E., Girz, L., Wilson, A.,
Nobakht, M., & Dezhkam, M. (2000). An epidemiologi- & Boachie, A. (2013). ‘I Know I Can Help You’:
cal study of eating disorders in Iran. International Parental self-efficacy predicts adolescent outcomes in
Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 265–271. https:// family-based therapy for eating disorders. European
doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(200011)28:3<265::AID- Eating Disorders Review, 28, 108–114. https://doi.
EAT3>3.0.CO;2-L org/10.1002/erv.2180
O’Shea, G., Spence, S. H., & Donovan, C. L. (2015). Rosen, D. (2010). Identification and management
Group versus individual interpersonal psychotherapy of eating disorders in children and adolescents.
for depressed adolescents. Behavioural and Cognitive Pediatrics, 126, 1240–1253. https://doi.org/10.1542/
Psychotherapy, 43, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/ peds.2010-2821
S1352465814000216 Ruiz-Lázaro, P. M., Alonso, J. P., Comet, P., Lobo, A., &
Patel, D. R., Pratt, H. D., & Greydanus, D. E. (2003). Velilla, M. (2005). Prevalence of eating disorders in
Treatment of adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Spain: A survey on a representative sample of ado-
Journal of Adolescent Health, 18, 244–260. https:// lescents. In P. I. Swain (Ed.), Trends in eating disor-
doi.org/10.1177/0743558403018003004 ders research (pp. 85–108). Hauppauge, NY: Nova
Peebles, R., Wilson, J. L., & Lock, J. D. (2006). How Biomedical Books.
do children with eating disorders differ from ado- Saccomani, L., Savoini, M., Cirrincione, M., Vercellino,
lescents with eating disorders at initial evaluation? F., & Ravera, G. (1998). Long-term outcome of
Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 800–805. https:// ­children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa: Study
doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.05.013 of comorbidity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
Penrod, B., & Fryling, M. (this volume). Feeding disor- 44, 565–571.
ders. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Handbook of childhood Schmidt, U., Lee, S., Beecham, J., Perkins, S., Treasure,
psychopathology and developmental disabilities: J., Yi, I., … Eisler, I. (2007). A randomized controlled
Treatment. Springer. trial of family therapy and cognitive behavior therapy
Pike, K. M., Walsh, B. T., Vitousek, K., Wilson, G. T., & guided self-care for adolescents with bulimia nervosa
Bauer, J. (2003). Cognitive behavior therapy in the and related disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry,
posthospitalization treatment of anorexia nervosa. 164, 591–598. https://doi.org/10.1176/foc.7.4.foc512
American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 2046–2049. Serfaty, M. A., Turkington, D., Heap, M., Ledsham, L.,
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.2046 & Jolley, E. (1999). Cognitive therapy versus dietary
Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disor- counselling in the outpatient treatment of anorexia
ders. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 187–213. https:// nervosa: Effects of the treatment phase. European
doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135103 Eating Disorders Review, 7, 334–350.
Poulsen, S., Lunn, S., Daniel, S. I. F., Folke, S., Mathiesen, Smink, F. R. E., van Hoeken, D., & Hoek, H. W. (2012).
B. B., Katznelson, H., & Fairburn, C. G. (2014). A Epidemiology of eating disorders: Incidence, preva-
Eating Disorders 365

lence and mortality rates. Current Psychiatry Reports, the incidence of eating disorders: A primary care study
14, 406–414. in the Netherlands. International Journal of Eating
Steinhausen, H. C. (2002). The outcome of anorexia Disorders, 39, 565–569. https://doi.org/10.1002/
nervosa in the 20th century. American Journal of eat.20316
Psychiatry, 159, 1284–1293. Vocks, S., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Pietrowsky, R.,
Steinhausen, H. C. (2009). Outcome of eating disorders. Rustenbach, S. J., Kersting, A., & Herpertz, S. (2010).
Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North Meta-analysis of the effectiveness of psychological
America, 18, 225–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. and pharmacological treatments for binge eating dis-
chc.2008.07.013 order. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 43,
Steinhausen, H. C., Grigoroiu-Serbanescu, M., 205–217. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20696
Boyadjieva, S., Neumärker, K. S., & Metzke, C. W. Wade, T. D., Bergin, J. L., Tiggemann, M., Bulik, C.
(2008). Course and predictors of rehospitalization M., & Fairburn, C. G. (2006). Prevalence and long‐
in adolescent anorexia nervosa in a multisite study. term course of lifetime eating disorders in an adult
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41, 29–36. Australian twin cohort. Australian and New Zealand
https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20414 Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 121–128.
Steinhausen, H. C., & Weber, S. (2009). The outcome of Waller, G., Cordery, H., Corstorphine, E., Hinrichsen,
bulimia nervosa: Findings from one-quarter century of H., Lawson, R., Mountford, V., & Russell, K. (2007).
research. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 1331– Cognitive behavioral therapy for eating disorders:
1341. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040582 A comprehensive treatment guide. Cambridge, NY:
Stice, E., Hayward, C., Cameron, R., Killen, J. D., Cambridge University Press.
& Taylor, C. B. (2000). Body image and eat- Weaver, L., & Liebman, R. (2011). Assessment of
ing related factors predict onset of depression in anorexia nervosa in children and adolescents.
female adolescents: A longitudinal study. Journal Current Psychiatry Reports, 13, 93–98. https://doi.
of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 438–444. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11920-010-0174-y
org/10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.438 Wentz, E., Gillberg, C., Anckarsäter, H., Gillberg, C.,
Stice, E., Marti, C. N., & Rohde, P. (2013). Prevalence, & Råstam, M. (2009). Adolescent-onset anorexia
incidence, impairment, and course of the proposed nervosa: 18-year outcome. British Journal of
DSM-5 eating disorder diagnoses in an 8-year pro- Psychiatry, 194, 168–174. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.
spective community study of young women. Journal bp.107.048686
of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 445–457. https://doi. Wilfley, D. E., Welch, R. R., Stein, R. I., Spurrell, E. B.,
org/10.1037/a0030679 Cohen, L., Saelens, B., … Matt, G. L. (2002). A ran-
Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Bearnman, S. K. (2001). Relation domized comparison of group cognitive-behavioral
of early menarche to depression, eating disorders, sub- therapy and group interpersonal psychotherapy for
stance abuse, and comorbid psychopathology among treatment of overweight individuals with binge-eating
adolescent girls. Developmental Psychology, 37, 608– disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 713–
619. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.608 721. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.8.713
Stock, S. L., Goldberg, E., Corbett, S., & Katzman, D. K. Wilfley, D. E., Wilson, G. T., & Agras, W. S. (2003).
(2002). Substance use in female adolescents with eat- The clinical significance of binge eating disorder.
ing disorders. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 176– International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34(Suppl),
182. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00420-2 S96–S106. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10209
Swanson, S. A., Crow, S. J., le Grange, D., Swendsen, Williamson, D. A., Thaw, J. M., & Varnado-Sullivan, P. J.
J., & Merikangas, K. R. (2011). Prevalence and cor- (2001). Cost-effectiveness analysis of a hospital-based
relates of eating disorders in adolescents: Results cognitive-behavioral treatment program for eating dis-
from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication orders. Behavior Therapy, 32, 459–477. https://doi.
Adolescent Supplement. Archives of General org/10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80031-9
Psychiatry, 68, 714–723. https://doi.org/10.1001/ Wilson, G. T., & Fairburn, C. G. (2002). Treatments for
archgenpsychiatry.2011.22 eating disorders. In P. E. Nathan & J. M. Gorman
Treasure, J., Todd, G., Brolly, M., Tiller, J., Nehmed, A., & (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work (2nd ed.,
Denman, F. (1995). A pilot study of a randomised trial pp. 559–592). New York: Oxford University Press.
of cognitive analytical therapy vs educational behav- Wilson, G. T., Loeb, K. L., Walsh, B. T., Labouvie,
ioral therapy for adult anorexia nervosa. Behaviour E., Petkova, E., Liu, X., & Waternaux, C. (1999).
Research and Therapy, 33, 363–367. https://doi. Psychological versus pharmacological treat-
org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)00070-Z ments of bulimia nervosa: Predictors and pro-
Van den Heuvel, L. L., & Jordaan, G. P. (2014). The psy- cesses of change. Journal of Consulting and
chopharmacological management of eating disorders Clinical Psychology, 67, 451–459. https://doi.org/
in children and adolescents. Journal of Child and 10.1037/0022-006X.67.4.451
Adolescent Mental Health, 26, 125–137. https://doi. Zaider, T. I., Johnson, J. G., & Cockell, S. J. (2000).
org/10.2989/17280583.2014.909816 Psychiatric comorbidity associated with eating disor-
Van Son, G. E., van Hoeken, D., Bartelds, A. I. M., van der symptomatology among adolescents in the com-
Furth, E. F., & Hoek, H. W. (2006). Time trends in munity. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
366 J.K. Rosewall et al.

28, 58–67. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098- Zipfel, S., Reas, D. L., Thornton, C., Olmsted, M. P.,
108X(200007)28:1<58::AID-EAT7>3.0.CO;2-V Williamson, D. A., Gerlinghoff, M., … Beumont,
Zeeck, A., Weber, S., Sandholz, A., Wetzler-Burmeister, E., P. J. (2002). Day hospitalization programs for eat-
Wirsching, M., & Hartmann, A. (2009). Inpatient versus ing disorders: A systematic review of the literature.
day clinic treatment for bulimia nervosa: A randomized International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 105–
trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 78, 152–160. 117. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10009
Feeding Disorders

Jonathan K. Fernand, Krista Saksena,
Becky Penrod, and Mitch J. Fryling

Contents  ediatric Feeding Disorders: Clinical


P
Pediatric Feeding Disorders: Clinical Presentation and Diagnosis
Presentation and Diagnosis...............................  367
Multidisciplinary Assessment: The  Role
Feeding problems are prevalent within the pedi-
of Applied Behavior Analysis............................  370 atric population, occurring in up to 45% of typi-
cally developing children and as many as 80% of
Assessment..............................................................  370
children with disabilities (Ahearn, Castine, Nault,
Intervention............................................................  374 & Green, 2001; Linscheid, 2006; Williams,
Parent Training......................................................  382 Gibbons, & Schreck, 2005). A wide-ranging
Programming for Generalization and  spectrum of issues exist among reported feeding
Maintenance.......................................................  383 difficulties, from mild problems like picky eating
Treatment Considerations and 
to more severe problems like total food refusal
Recommendations..............................................  384 and liquid or tube dependence. Although some
difficulties fall within the scope of typical child
Concluding Remarks.............................................  386
development, often resolving in the absence of
References...............................................................  387 formal treatment, pediatric feeding disorders
occur when such difficulties result in impair-
ments in social functioning, irrespective of nutri-
tional deficiencies or loss of weight (Murphy &
Zlomke, 2016). Common feeding problems
J.K. Fernand (*)
encountered in children include problems related
Department of Psychology, University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL, USA to feeding skill delay and deficits such as lack of
e-mail: jkfernand@ufl.edu self-feeding, failure to advance texture, oral
K. Saksena • B. Penrod motor dysfunction, swallowing problems, and
Department of Psychology, California State respondent choking, gagging, and vomiting, as
University, Sacramento, CA, USA well as maladaptive and disruptive mealtime
e-mail: krista.saksena@gmail.com; bpenrod@csus.edu
behavior including aggression, throwing food,
M.J. Fryling tantrums, food refusal and food selectivity, pack-
Division of Special Education & Counseling,
ing or pocketing food, and eating too much or too
California State University, Los Angeles,
Los Angeles, CA, USA little in addition to eating too fast or too slow
e-mail: Mitchell.Fryling2@calstatela.edu (Berlin, Davies, Lobato, & Silverman, 2009).

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 367


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_21
368 J.K. Fernand et al.

In the current Diagnostic and Statistical food refusal and liquid dependence are two
Manual of Mental Disorders, feeding disorders severe forms of food refusal where the child
have been classified under a new category either refuses oral consumption altogether or
referred to as Avoidant/Restrictive Food Intake only consumes fluids while refusing to consume
Disorder (ARFID). Diagnostic criteria include solid food, respectively. Enteral feedings are a
restricted feeding and lack of interest or avoid- common medical intervention aimed at address-
ance of food based on sensory properties of the ing the nutritional deficits associated with severe
food or fear of consequences that may result from cases of food refusal where a nutritionally insig-
eating (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). nificant amount of food is consumed orally.
Although some of the aforementioned feeding Enteral tube feedings (ETF) can be used alone or
problems might not necessarily be captured in conjunction with oral and intravenous nutri-
under the ARFID diagnostic category, all of them tional supplementation and require that a nutri-
are likely to cause functional impairment to some tionally complete food be pumped directly into
extent, thereby warranting intervention. the stomach, duodenum, or jejunum on a set
Some common feeding disorders, discussed in schedule to provide requisite nutrition. Although
more detail below, include food selectivity, food ETF is often medically necessary to stabilize
refusal, and lack of developmentally appropriate children whose health has been compromised by
feeding skills. severe weight loss or nutritional deficiencies
(e.g., iron deficient anemia), there are a number
of potential physical and developmental concerns
Food Selectivity that can result from the procedure. For example,
children can experience local wound infections,
Food selectivity is a common feeding difficulty tube dislodgement, and tube blockage or fracture
typically characterized as the chronic failure to (Holmes, 2012); furthermore, children may
meet recommended nutritional needs due to the become tube dependent even when ETF is no
consumption of a limited variety of foods (Shore longer medically necessary which might inhibit
& Piazza, 1997). Food selectivity can be further the development of oral feeding (Morris, 1989).
delineated as selectivity by type (e.g., refusal to
consume vegetables), texture (e.g., refusal of
tabletop textures such as apple slices in favor of Feeding Skill Deficits
pureed textures such as apple sauce), or even
brand (e.g., only eating fries from one restaurant Skill deficits related to feeding are often multi-
and refusing store-bought fries or fries from other factorial in etiology. Examples of potential con-
restaurants). Though selective eaters might sus- tributing factors include structural abnormalities
tain normal weight and growth, they lack requi- (e.g., cleft lip/palate, mandibular hypoplasia),
site nutrition for healthy development as a result medical complications such as food allergies and
of prolonged dietary restrictions. gastroesophageal reflux disease (GERD), genetic
disorders (e.g., cerebral palsy, Down syndrome),
neuromuscular and developmental delays (e.g.,
Food Refusal hypotonia, lack of communication), and environ-
mental factors. These organic and nonorganic
The term “food refusal,” when used to describe a determinants might produce a variety of skill
type of feeding disorder, typically refers to chil- deficits including swallowing disorders, packing,
dren who reject consuming most if not all foods chewing deficits, and a lack of self-feeding.
when presented (Field, Garland, & Williams, Swallowing dysfunction (i.e., dysphagia) is
2003) and can also be used to describe children characterized by difficulties or pain when swal-
who consume some foods yet at inadequate vol- lowing. There are several voluntary and reflexive
umes (Williams, Field, & Seiverling, 2010). Total behaviors that contribute to swallowing, i­ ncluding
Feeding Disorders 369

bolus formation, bolus transfer, initiation of the lead to impairments in intellectual, emotional,
swallow, and passage of the bolus through the and academic development, (Hoch et al., 2001) in
esophageal sphincter. Difficulty in completing addition to familial stress (Greer, Gulotta, Masler,
any one of these behaviors can put a child at risk & Laud, 2008), while children who engage in
for aspiration, pneumonia, gagging, choking, and food selectivity and consume a large volume of
vomiting (Arvedson, 2008). Packing occurs their preferred foods may experience unhealthy
when an accepted bite of food is held or “pock- weight gain when those foods are rich in calories
eted” in the mouth in the absence of swallowing. and fat. Obesity in childhood has been linked to
Much like dysphagia, there is also a risk of aspi- an increased prevalence of type II diabetes, high
ration. Chewing deficits constitute a variety of cholesterol, hypertension, and a number of social
problems and, like dysphagia and packing, can consequences (e.g., ostracism, bullying, weight
result from a range of organic factors. Treatment preoccupation; Dietz, 1998). Although this sub-
of chewing and swallowing deficits should be population does not experience the immediate
based on the specific type of deficit(s) exhibited biomedical concerns associated with marked
(Kadey, Roane, Diaz, & Merrow, 2013). In some weight loss, developmental delays, or need for
cases, chewing deficits might not have any iden- enteral feedings, they are likely to experience
tifiable organic origin. For example, deficits with drastic long-term outcomes as a result of their
chewing can develop from a lack of learning feeding problem and would likely benefit from
opportunities resulting from prolonged use of interventions targeting improved nutrition.
tube feedings in which oral consumption is infre- Malnutrition that results from feeding disor-
quent or does not occur (Morris, 1989). Self-­ ders can often lead to delays with young children,
feeding or independence with feeding, similar to including cognitive impairment, in particular
chewing deficits, can be delayed as a result of during the sensitive period of development
environmental factors (e.g., lack of opportunities between birth and 5 years of age. Children can
to practice due to prolonged bottle dependence). also display delays in speech, social responses,
However, these delays can also result from some motor development, and might fail to reach other
of the genetic and developmental etiologies men- major developmental milestones within an appro-
tioned previously, especially when motor move- priate window of time. These symptoms, in con-
ments to reach for, pick up, bite, and chew are junction with weight loss or stagnation, are
effortful. In general, difficulty eating might sometimes referred to as “failure-to-thrive,” a
reduce the motivation to eat, and this lack of condition that describes the child’s decelerated or
motivation should be considered when develop- arrested development (Heffer & Kelley, 1994).
ing a comprehensive intervention plan, by teach- As eating is typically a social activity, feeding
ing appropriate feeding skills, and thereby difficulties can also limit the number of social
decreasing response effort (Kadey et al.). Prior to opportunities for children as they no longer par-
intervention, each of these deficits should be ticipate in mealtimes (e.g., family dinner, school
evaluated for potential organic causes, and medi- lunch). In fact, the child’s participation in such
cal clearance must be obtained. settings might be an unpleasant experience for
everyone involved. For example, a child who
engages in total food refusal, and as a result
 edical, Developmental, and Social
M receives feedings via a gastrostomy tube, might
Outcomes miss important learning opportunities to engage
in social behaviors most common at mealtimes.
The aforementioned feeding disorders usually Attempts to include the child might lead to inap-
require some form of intervention to address a propriate mealtime behaviors (e.g., gagging,
variety of negative biomedical, developmental, vomiting, crying, aggression, self-injury), behav-
and social outcomes. Untreated feeding disorders iors commonly exhibited by children with feed-
that result in malnutrition and weight loss can ing difficulties. These experiences likely make
370 J.K. Fernand et al.

mealtimes difficult for families if the caregivers’ relations. In other words, maladaptive feeding
attention is consumed with managing such behaviors are learned behaviors often maintained
behaviors, preventing them from enjoying their by negative reinforcement contingencies
own meal and interacting with other members of (Clawson & Elliott, 2014), and regardless of
the family (Greer et al., 2008). underlying structural, neurological, cardiorespi-
ratory, and/or metabolic features of feeding dis-
orders, there is often a significant behavioral
Multidisciplinary Assessment: component. In fact, a study by Burklow, Phelps,
The Role of Applied Behavior Schultz, McConnell, and Colin (1998) identified
Analysis significant behavioral contributions to feeding
disorders in as many as 80% of children referred
As previously mentioned, feeding disorders have to a multidisciplinary feeding clinic.
been categorized as feeding skill delays and defi- Consequently, applied behavior analysis is
cits and maladaptive feeding behaviors and dis- uniquely important in multidisciplinary assess-
ruptive mealtime behaviors (Berlin et al., 2009). ment and treatment because even after underly-
It is important to note that the feeding problems ing medical, developmental, sensory, or
within these broad categories are not always dis- psychosocial challenges have been addressed,
tinct from one another and generally occur within maladaptive feeding behaviors are likely to per-
a relational context. Thus, Berlin et al. proposed sist if the reinforcement contingencies that main-
a third grouping of feeding problems character- tain such behavior are not identified and
ized as relational or family difficulties and disrupted. Further, treatments that include behav-
unpleasant mealtime environments, in which ioral interventions may also act to increase the
feeding problems are identified in the context of child’s compliance with treatments focused on
family difficulties such as an antagonistic envi- oral-motor skill development used by speech
ronment, parents’ own aversions surrounding pathologists or occupational therapy (Clawson &
mealtimes, and mealtime interactions that are Elliott, 2014). In order to develop a comprehen-
coercive in nature. In other words, parental mis- sive treatment that addresses the function of mal-
management may be responsible for the develop- adaptive mealtime behavior, a functional
ment and/or maintenance of feeding problems. behavioral assessment, as described below, is
Given the complexity of pediatric feeding disor- critical.
ders in clinical presentation and diagnosis, bio-
logical, behavioral, and psychosocial factors
must all be considered and addressed (Berlin Assessment
et al.). As such, a multidisciplinary approach is
the suggested model of care for the assessment Various types of questionnaires, observations,
and treatment of severe feeding disorders and other assessment methodologies can be used
(Cornwell, Kelly, & Austin, 2010), including to gather information on the severity of feeding
speech and/or occupational therapy, medical and problems and other qualitative aspects regarding
dietary interventions, psychosocial support pro- mealtimes, as well as types of inappropriate
vided by a social worker or psychologist, and a mealtime behavior the child exhibits and events
strong emphasis on behavioral interventions. that might influence their persistence.
Though the etiology of feeding problems var-
ies widely and can include medical complica-
tions such as GERD allergies, and structural Indirect Assessment
abnormalities that impact chewing and swallow-
ing, maladaptive feeding behaviors exhibited by Studies evaluating indirect methods as a means to
children, regardless of initial precipitating events, obtain information regarding the function of
can all be attributed to environment-behavior problem behavior have been largely unsuccessful
Feeding Disorders 371

in identifying behavioral functions as compared Descriptive Assessment


to functional analysis methodology (Smith,
Smith, Dracobly, & Peterson Pace, 2012; In general, studies have shown descriptive assess-
Zarcone, Rodgers, Iwata, Rourke, & Dorsey, ments to be insufficient for obtaining accurate
1991). However, use of initial surveys for obtain- information regarding the function of problem
ing crucial information pertaining to medical behavior when comparing the outcomes obtained
concerns (e.g., physical abnormalities, allergies), from descriptive assessments with functional
past interventions, and food preferences can not analysis (see description below; e.g., Pence,
only be useful in ensuring the safety of the child Roscoe, Bourret, & Ahearn, 2009). However, the
but helpful in guiding potential intervention initial process for identifying environment-­
approaches as well. behavior relationships for subsequent manipula-
Questionnaires such as the Brief Autism tion in functional analysis might hinge on the
Mealtime Behaviors Inventory (BAMBI; Lukens careful observation of these relationships by cli-
& Linscheid, 2008), Children’s Eating Behavior nicians. For example, in a seminal functional
Inventory (CEBI; Archer, Rosenbaum, & analysis study conducted by Piazza, Fisher, et al.
Streiner, 1991), and Screening Tool of Feeding (2003), the experimenters utilized contingencies
Problems (STEP; Matson & Kuhn, 2001) can be in the functional analysis that were informed by
used as assessment tools to identify whether a the prior descriptive observations of caregiver-­
child has a feeding problem or as the dependent conducted meals. Despite the lack of research
measure in determining efficacy of interven- comparing the obtained function of inappropriate
tions. For example, the BAMBI has been short- mealtime behavior from descriptive and func-
ened to a 15-item survey in which parental report tional analysis methods, the outcomes from
on questions pertaining to food selectivity, dis- descriptive assessment studies have been crucial
ruptive mealtime behavior, food refusal, and in showing that a number of environmental events
mealtime rigidity is obtained (DeMand, Johnson, such as caregiver attention (e.g., coaxing, repri-
& Foldes, 2015; Lukens & Linscheid). These mands), delivery of preferred items (e.g., toys,
types of assessments are useful for specific pur- preferred foods), and escape from bite presenta-
poses such as identifying whether intervention is tions are events that likely take place during
warranted for an individual but will likely fall meals (Borrero, Woods, Borrero, Masler, &
short when used as the sole method for obtaining Lesser, 2010; Piazza, Fisher, et al.). Thus,
accurate information regarding the function of descriptive methods could be useful in conveying
inappropriate mealtime behavior. Nonetheless, to caregivers and other professionals how these
indirect assessments such as interviews are valu- events might be playing a role in the maintenance
able in a number of ways. For example, assess- of a feeding problem.
ing the types of foods a child currently consumes
via questionnaire could inform their inclusion in
a direct assessment (e.g., preference assess- Preference Assessments
ment), examining dietary patterns via food logs
might inform the types of foods necessary to Generally, preference assessments are often used
include in an intervention to better nutritional in research and clinical practice to identify poten-
outcomes, and obtaining information regarding tial reinforcing stimuli, both leisure and edible.
problems related to cross-discipline expertise With respect to assessing foods for individuals
might inform the need for including alternate with feeding problems, the process or outcome of
professionals (e.g., dental problems warranting a conducting a preference assessment can serve
dentist, oral motor deficits warranting a speech several additional purposes such as confirming
and language pathologist) in cases where behav- caregiver report of the topographies of problem
ior analysts are providing treatment outside of a behavior their child exhibits and which foods are
multidisciplinary team. consistently refused. For the sake of assessment
372 J.K. Fernand et al.

of feeding problems prior to a treatment evalua- practical utility in using different preference
tion, preference assessments are typically used to assessment arrangements dependent on various
identify foods that are not consumed altogether. types of feeding problems. For example, as noted
Although, it is possible preference assessments above, the SSPA is likely most useful when want-
could be utilized to identify foods that are incon- ing to obtain information about whether a child
sistently accepted or consumed relative to foods will or will not consume a specific food. In con-
that are not accepted at all. It is possible those trast, the MSWO arrangement might be most
foods that are consumed more frequently, albeit similar to a typical meal in that multiple foods are
not to a sufficient level, might be more easily presented at the same time. Thus, this arrange-
incorporated into the child’s diet with less intru- ment would make it possible to examine the
sive interventions than foods the child completely sequencing of bites within a meal. Likewise,
refuses. Although more research needs to be con- assessment of bite sequencing could occur if bites
ducted using such an assessment, it is likely that are replaced as in the multiple stimulus with
these procedures would only be applicable for replacement preference assessment (Windsor,
cases of picky eating rather than children who Piché, & Loche, 1994), if free access is provided
exhibit more severe forms of feeding problems. similar to that of the free operant preference
The utility of different preference assessments assessment (Roane, Vollmer, Ringdahl, & Marcus,
might depend on what function they are being 1998) or if foods are restricted as exclusive con-
used for. For example, a paired stimulus prefer- sumption occurs, as in the response-­restriction
ence assessment (PSPA; Fisher et al., 1992) is preference assessment (Hanley, Iwata, Lindberg,
typically used to obtain a relative hierarchy of & Conners, 2003). In addition, if several foods
preference across foods. However, given that chil- (with a range of preference) are available to the
dren with feeding problems often refuse to con- child to consume in any order, one would be able
sume foods when presented, the typical outcome to measure at what point problem behavior occurs.
of a PSPA is that the majority of foods are not For example, the SSPA and MSWO presentation
consumed even when preferred foods are used in methods might be useful to distinguish whether
combination with non-preferred foods during the the child will engage in inappropriate mealtime
assessment. The result of the PSPA being that behavior when non-preferred foods are presented
consumption occurs for a few of the preferred in isolation (SSPA) or when presented simultane-
foods whereas consumption does not occur for ously with preferred foods (MSWO). In addition,
any other foods. Further, consumption is not likely both might be important assessments if the rec-
to occur during the PSPA if the child engages in ommendation is to provide exposure to a variety
total food refusal or preferred foods are not incor- of foods as a possible least intrusive intervention
porated into the preference assessment. Thus, a where caregivers or therapists might present non-
hierarchy of preference cannot be obtained, preferred foods on a time-based schedule.
defeating the original purpose of using the However, applying preference assessments in this
PSPA. Alternatively, a single stimulus preference way has yet to be empirically validated and, like
assessment (SSPA; Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, any direct assessment, will only prove to be useful
& Page, 1985) might be the most practical prefer- if they provide valuable information in guiding
ence assessment and yield the resulting informa- the design of subsequent treatments and predict-
tion if the goal is to corroborate caregiver report of ing successful applications or outcomes.
foods their child likely refuses and identify what As an example, Munk and Repp (1994) uti-
foods the child will or will not consume. lized an assessment procedure in which 10–12
Other preference assessments such as the mul- types of food (e.g., pears, chicken) were presented
tiple stimulus without replacement (MSWO; at up to four possible textures (e.g., ground,
DeLeon & Iwata, 1996) have not been employed chopped) for five individuals with feeding prob-
as frequently as the PSPA when assessing prefer- lems. The experimenters recorded acceptance,
ences for this population; however, there might be refusal, expulsion, and inappropriate mealtime
Feeding Disorders 373

behavior in an effort to categorize each child’s Functional Analysis


feeding problem as total food refusal, selectivity
by type, selectivity by texture, or selectivity by Functional analysis methodology (e.g., Iwata,
both type and texture. The pattern of behavior that Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982/1994)
would be expected for each category would be has been used as a means to identify variables
refusal to consume all foods when presented that influence the occurrence of problem behavior
across all textures, only consuming a few foods and are considered the only assessment method
regardless of texture but refusing other types of that reliably identifies a functional relationship
foods, consuming foods at one texture but not between two variables, typically between envi-
another, and only consuming some foods at some ronmental events and a response class. Girolami
textures but refusing other foods, respectively. and Scotti (2001) were one of the first to extend
Subsequent research has demonstrated that the functional analysis procedures to the assessment
assessment procedures proposed by Munk and of inappropriate mealtime behavior in an effort
Repp can identify functional relationships to identify the circumstances under which those
between dimensions of foods and behavior such behaviors are likely to occur; however, since then
as accepting, consuming, or expelling as well as a number of studies have utilized similar meth-
predict successful intervention strategies (e.g., odology by arranging variations of attention,
Patel, Piazza, Santana, & Volkert, 2002). escape, tangible (toy and edible), and control
In addition to the possibility of preference conditions (e.g., Bachmeyer et al., 2009; Piazza,
assessments being used to inform treatment Fisher, et al., 2003). Typically in the attention
development, preference assessments might also condition of a functional analysis, a bite of food
be used in a pre- and posttreatment format to is presented for a predetermined amount of time
measure both generalization and maintenance. and remains fixed in position until that interval
As we noted previously, the PSPA can be limited lapses, regardless of problem behavior. However,
in assessing the hierarchy of preference between inappropriate mealtime behavior results in the
foods for this population; however, researchers delivery of attention (e.g., consoling, coaxing,
have begun utilizing pre- and posttreatment reprimands). Increased problem behavior in this
PSPAs to measure changes in preferences follow- condition would suggest the child’s inappropriate
ing intervention (e.g., Fernand, Penrod, Brice Fu, mealtime behavior is sensitive to social-positive
Whelan, & Medved, 2015; Penrod & VanDalen, reinforcement in the form of attention. In the
2010). The pre- and posttreatment PSPA allows escape condition, a bite of food is presented for
for measurement of both generalization and the same interval of time as decided upon in the
maintenance. Generalization is measured if foods attention condition. However, the bite of food is
are included in the PSPA that the child was not removed, and the demand to eat the bite is ter-
exposed to during intervention, and consumption minated for a brief amount of time (e.g., 20 s)
occurs with those foods during the posttreatment contingent upon inappropriate mealtime behav-
PSPA. Finally, the posttreatment PSPA allows for ior, and no other programmed consequences are
an assessment of the possibility for fading treat- provided for problem behavior, that is, attention
ment in that the PSPA is typically implemented is not provided in the escape condition. Elevated
under baseline contingencies (i.e., escape is avail- problem behavior in this condition would suggest
able and no programmed consequences are deliv- that the child’s inappropriate mealtime behavior
ered). Thus, treatment components are removed is sensitive to social-negative reinforcement in
during the PSPA, and one possible variable main- the form of escape from eating or bite presenta-
taining consumption is the change in preference tions. During the tangible condition, the arrange-
that occurred during treatment, indicating that ment is the same as the previous conditions;
consumption might be likely to occur in the however, attention and escape are not provided.
future (i.e., maintain) without formal intervention Instead, a preferred item (either a toy or food)
components as a result of the shift in preference. is presented contingent upon the ­occurrence of
374 J.K. Fernand et al.

problem behavior. Increased problem behavior in For ­example, Babbitt et al. (1994) suggested that
this condition would suggest the child’s inappro- there are two general issues, which might over-
priate mealtime behavior is sensitive to social- lap, that often contribute to the presence of a
positive reinforcement in the form of access to feeding disorder: (1) motivational problems and
preferred items. Lastly, during the control condi- (2) skills deficits. Along these lines there are a
tion, either the bite of food used in the other con- number of interventions that might be pursued to
ditions is presented (e.g., Piazza, Fisher, et al.) target both motivation and skill deficits.
or a preferred food is presented (e.g., Najdowski The effectiveness of behavioral interventions
et al., 2008). Typically in this condition, noncon- in the treatment of pediatric feeding disorders
tingent access to attention is provided, and in has been well documented. Specifically, behav-
some cases noncontingent access to toys is pro- ioral interventions have been used to address
vided (if assessing a tangible function), and no inappropriate mealtime behaviors (Bachmeyer
programmed consequences are provided if inap- et al., 2009), food selectivity (Najdowski,
propriate mealtime behavior occurs. A number Wallace, Doney, & Ghezzi, 2003; Piazza et al.,
of studies have presented variants of the afore- 2002), packing (Gulotta, Piazza, Patel, & Layer,
mentioned conditions using a variety of designs 2005; Patel, Piazza, Layer, Coleman, &
including reversal (e.g., Piazza, Fisher, et al.) and Swartzwelder, 2005), total food refusal (Gulotta
multielement designs (e.g., Najdowski et al.), as et al., 2005; Mueller, Piazza, Patel, Kelley, &
well as pairwise (LaRue et al., 2011) and brief Pruett, 2004; Shore, Babbitt, Williams, Coe, &
analyses (e.g., Wilder, Normand, & Atwell, Snyder, 1998), swallowing problems (Greer,
2005). Together, previous research has identi- Dorow, Williams, McCorkle, & Asnes, 1991;
fied inappropriate mealtime behavior is likely Lamm & Greer, 1988), and self-feeding (Collins,
to serve an escape function (e.g., Piazza, Fisher, Gast, Wolery, Holcombe, & Leatherby, 1991;
et al.) pointing to the importance of interventions Luiselli, 2000). This section considers less
focused on decreasing the aversive properties of intrusive interventions, namely, those that
foods, feeding apparatus, or mealtime context as involve positive reinforcement and antecedent
well as those which terminate the relationship interventions and those that strengthen skills
between the escape contingency for engaging in involved in feeding.
inappropriate mealtime behavior. Importantly, in our clinical experience the
extent to which less intrusive interventions are
successful in the absence of extinction is often
Intervention related to the severity of inappropriate mealtime
behavior, and this is consistent with patterns in
Although there is a range of medical conditions the research literature as well (Seubert, Fryling,
that can influence the development of feeding Wallace, Jiminez, & Meier, 2014). That is to say,
disorders, the previous section has highlighted less intrusive interventions, those that don’t
how behavioral factors can often play a large involve extinction, seem more likely to be suc-
role in both the development and maintenance of cessful for less severe feeding problems. To be
feeding problems. Indeed, the functional analy- sure, there are a great variety of interventions that
sis literature has highlighted how both negative might be characterized as less intrusive. Given
reinforcement (i.e., the removal or avoidance of this, our aim is to provide an overview of some of
non-­preferred foods) and positive reinforcement the strategies that have been evaluated within the
(e.g., Bachmeyer et al., 2009; Najdowski et al., research literature, but at the same time we
2008; Piazza, Fisher, et al., 2003) can participate acknowledge that our review is selective and that
in the development and maintenance of inap- more thorough reviews might be pursued. We
propriate mealtime behavior. There are several begin by reviewing perhaps one of the most
other factors to consider when pursuing a broad straightforward behavioral interventions, differ-
functional analysis of feeding disorders, though. ential reinforcement of alternative behavior.
Feeding Disorders 375

Differential Reinforcement non-contingent reinforcement and escape extinc-


of Alternative Behavior (DRA) tion in the treatment of four children with food
refusal. Results showed that non-contingent rein-
DRA consists of providing access to a reinforcer forcement (access to toys and attention through-
contingent upon a target behavior and not provid- out meals) did not increase consumption of foods,
ing access to this reinforcer in the absence of the but that it did reduce inappropriate mealtime
target behavior. Within the context of a feeding behavior for some of the participants. Wilder
intervention, DRA consists of providing reinforc- et al. (2005) studied the effects of non-­contingent
ers contingent upon acceptance and consumption reinforcement on the self-injurious behavior and
of non-preferred foods and withholding those food refusal of a 3-year-old child with autism and
reinforcers in the absence of acceptance or con- feeding difficulties. A pretreatment functional
sumption. Interestingly, while this intervention analysis confirmed that the child’s self-injury was
may seem to be the most straightforward behav- maintained by escape from food. Results showed
ioral intervention, only a handful of studies have that non-contingent reinforcement (access to a
demonstrated its effectiveness in isolation. A video throughout meals) resulted in a reduction
study by Riordan, Iwata, Finney, Wohl, and in self-injury and an increase in bites accepted.
Stanley (1984) demonstrated how positive rein- Thus, there are somewhat mixed results related to
forcement in the form of access to preferred items the effectiveness of NCR in the treatment of feed-
and social praise could be used to improve the ing problems. Future researchers should continue
number of bites accepted with three children to try to understand the contexts in which NCR
admitted to an inpatient feeding disorders pro- is effective.
gram. Brown, Spencer, and Swift (2002)
described the successful use of DRA with a
7-year old who was a picky eater. In this study, Antecedent Interventions
parents were instructed to give their child a rule
(e.g., “if you eat X you can have some Y”), and A number of interventions target the motivation
this contingency was successful at increasing to engage in negatively reinforced inappropriate
consumption across three foods. Other research- mealtime behavior by specifically altering some
ers have examined DRA in combination with aspect of the stimulus properties of non-preferred
other interventions, suggesting that it may be a foods. These interventions generally a) target the
useful, though not necessarily critical, compo- stimulus properties of the avoided food itself or
nent of behavioral treatment packages (e.g., b) attempt to change the context in which the
Najdowski et al., 2010). However, other research non-preferred food is presented and experienced.
has shown that DRA is not successful at improv- We will first review those interventions that have
ing feeding behavior in isolation (e.g., Najdowski focused on changing the stimulus functions of
et al., 2003; Patel, Piazza, Martinez, Volkert, & non-preferred foods directly.
Santana, 2002; Penrod, Wallace, Reagon, Betz,
& Higbee, 2010), but that it may be helpful Fading and Texture Manipulations  Stimulus
toward reducing challenging behavior associated fading and texture manipulations consist of
with mealtimes (Piazza, Patel, Gulotta, Sevin, & changing the characteristics of the non-preferred
Layer, 2003). foods directly. In a stimulus fading intervention,
target foods (i.e., non-preferred foods) are
blended with preferred foods (e.g., 70% preferred
Non-contingent Reinforcement food and 30% non-preferred food). Mueller et al.
(2004) improved the variety of foods two chil-
Non-contingent reinforcement has also been stud- dren with food refusal consumed by using a
ied within the feeding disorders research. Reed blending intervention that involved blending pre-
et al. (2004) assessed the relative ­effectiveness of ferred and non-preferred foods at various ratios.
376 J.K. Fernand et al.

After the blending treatment, probes were con- with preferred foods, a strategy called simultane-
ducted with both treated and untreated target ous presentation in the research literature. Piazza
foods. Results showed that consumption et al. (2002) conducted an evaluation of the
improved for all foods, but that consumption of effects of simultaneous presentation of non-pre-
foods that had not been exposed to the blending ferred and preferred foods relative to the sequen-
treatment only improved after several foods had tial presentation of non-preferred and preferred
been treated. Patel, Piazza, Kelley, Ochsner, and foods (i.e., only providing preferred foods after
Santana (2001) evaluated a fading procedure with consuming non-preferred foods). Results of this
a child who would only consume water. Initially, study showed that two third of the participants
the intervention involved systematically increas- consumed more food when non-preferred and
ing the amount of Carnation Instant Breakfast ® preferred foods were provided simultaneously
(CIB) added to the water. When consumption was relative to sequentially. A final participant also
high with all of the CIB in the water, the amount consumed more in the simultaneous condition,
of milk added to the water and packet of CIB was but only when it was combined with escape
also systematically increased. Ultimately, the extinction procedures. Ahearn (2003) also evalu-
child consumed glasses of milk with a packet of ated the simultaneous presentation condition
CIB. Tiger and Hanley (2006) pursued a similar with a 14-year old, mildly selective child, to
intervention with a child who would not drink improve the acceptance of three vegetables.
milk. Chocolate syrup was added to the milk and Acceptance of all three vegetables improved
systematically faded, as consumption remained when condiments (preferred food) were added to
high. At the end of the treatment, the child drank the vegetables (non-preferred foods). While these
glasses of milk with no syrup added. These stud- studies suggest that simultaneous presentation
ies demonstrate that stimulus fading can be used may be effective, other research has been less
to systematically transfer stimulus control to ini- supportive. VanDalen and Penrod (2010) found
tially non-­preferred foods and liquids. that neither simultaneous nor sequential presen-
Texture manipulations are similar to fading tation methods were effective at increasing the
interventions in that the non-preferred food is consumption of bites with two children with
directly altered in some way. Patel, Piazza, autism spectrum disorder and that both methods
Santana, et al. (2002) evaluated a texture manipu- were equally effective when combined with
lation on the number of expulsions and grams escape extinction. Given these mixed findings,
consumed with a child who consumed foods at more research is needed which identifies the cir-
low textures (e.g., puree) but expelled foods at cumstances in which the simultaneous presenta-
higher textures. The authors specifically reduced tion method is likely to be effective in the absence
the textures of meats in this study, with results of escape extinction.
showing that expulsions decreased and grams
consumed increased. This evaluation demon- High-P Sequence  Other interventions aim to
strates that the texture of foods may be a variable improve consumption by altering some feature of
to consider in the treatment of feeding problems the feeding context. One of these interventions
and that texture manipulations may be pursued to involves the high-probability instructional
improve consumption. Importantly, food texture sequence. Generally, the high-p sequence
is related to chewing and swallowing skills, and involves providing a series of instructions that
efforts to systematically increase texture after it the individual has a very high probability (i.e.,
is decreased may need to be combined with inter- history) of complying with prior to providing an
ventions aimed at developing and strengthening instruction that they have a low probability of
chewing and swallowing behavior (see below). complying with. The high-p sequence has also
had mixed effects within the feeding literature.
Simultaneous Presentation  Another ­antecedent For example, Dawson et al. (2003) found that the
strategy involves providing non-preferred foods high-p sequence did not improve consumption or
Feeding Disorders 377

inappropriate mealtime behavior and that it also Volkert, Piazza, Vaz, and Frese (2013) examined
did not add to the effectiveness of escape extinc- two relatively straightforward behavioral inter-
tion. Patel et al. (2006) also found that the high-p ventions to improve chewing skills. Their first
sequence did not improve acceptance or con- study involved examining the effects of a least-­
sumption, but that it did reduce inappropriate to-­most prompting procedure and praise on the
mealtime behavior when combined with extinc- number of chews per bite with a typically devel-
tion interventions relative to extinction alone. oping 4-year-old child. Results showed that the
Others have found the high-p sequence to be intervention was successful at increasing the
effective in the absence of escape extinction number of chews per bite across various foods
(Ewry & Fryling, 2016; Meier, Fryling, & (green beans, apricots, peaches, carrots, potatoes,
Wallace, 2012; Patel et al., 2007). It is notewor- fish sticks, and chicken). In a refinement of the
thy that these studies employed a variation of the first experiment, the researchers evaluated the
high-p sequence wherein the high-p instruction effects of a descriptive verbal prompt (i.e., “Chew
was topographically similar to the low-p instruc- 10 times”) and praise on chewing with a 14-year-­
tion (both involved taking bites from spoons) and old child with a developmental disability. Results
involved participants that were generally compli- showed that both chews per bite and mastication
ant. Finally, Penrod, Gardella, and Fernand improved as a result of the intervention. These
(2012) combined the high-p sequence with initial evaluations suggest that simple behavioral
demand fading (i.e., progressively increasing the interventions might be used to improve skills
demand requirement) to improve consumption associated with chewing and swallowing.
with children with food selectivity. The results of More recently, Volkert, Peterson, Zeleny, and
Penrod et al. demonstrate that the high-p Piazza (2014) evaluated a protocol involving a
sequence may be successful when combined chew tube to improve chews per bite, mastica-
with other interventions. tion, and to decrease early swallows with three
As we have mentioned before, a great number young children with feeding problems (aged 2, 3,
of interventions might fall within the purview of and 4 years). Specifically, using mothers as thera-
antecedent interventions for feeding problems. pists, participants were taught to first bite a chew
Interested readers are encouraged to consult tube, then, using least-to-most prompting, to
reviews on the topic (e.g., Bachmeyer, 2009; chew the tube with a bite in it, then to chew a half
Seubert et al., 2014). Interestingly, although tube with a bite, and finally least-to-most prompt-
appetite has been mentioned in some of the ing with a bite was used alone. Target behaviors
behavioral feeding literature (Linscheid, 2006), improved for each of the three participants
the role of appetite manipulation has not been involved in the study. These studies suggest that
systematically evaluated in the behavior analytic behavioral interventions may be used to improve
feeding literature. However, interventions such as chewing skills with children with feeding prob-
developing a structured feeding schedule and lems. Given the importance of skills such as
reducing the extent to which individuals eat small chewing, it is hoped that more research focuses
amounts of food (or “snack”) throughout the day on this area.
seem to be part of standard behavioral recom-
mendations for feeding intervention (e.g., Self-Feeding  An additional skill related to feed-
Williams & Foxx, 2007). ing is that of self-feeding. As we have described
throughout the chapter thus far, children with
feeding difficulties often have histories of avoid-
Developing Related Skills ing foods for various reasons, and, as a conse-
quence, having caregivers feed them. This history
Chewing  Relatively fewer published behavioral may result in a lack of self-feeding skills in chil-
interventions have focused specifically on dren with a history of feeding difficulties.
strengthening target skills related to eating. Behavioral researchers have examined a number
378 J.K. Fernand et al.

of procedures that may be used to improve self-­ less likely to be effective in the absence of extinc-
feeding. Like all skills, self-feeding skills may be tion. In the meantime, we recommend that clini-
deficient due to skill deficits or motivational cians consider a pretreatment assessment of
problems. Luiselli (1991, 1993, 2000) reported a various treatments rather than pursue what may
number of case studies where individuals with become a rather lengthy trial and error process.
various disabilities were taught self-feeding skills Consistent with the functional analysis logic
using prompting and prompt fading, differential described earlier in the chapter, this involves test-
reinforcement, and demand fading. These studies ing out the effects of various interventions on
demonstrated that when self-feeding skills are important target behaviors, most often accep-
weak or absent, they can be taught using common tance, consumption, and inappropriate mealtime
behavioral acquisition procedures. behavior. Such an assessment may be relatively
Self-feeding can also be deficient with indi- brief; there are often notable differences that can
viduals who have a self-feeding repertoire. be seen very quickly.
Recently, behavioral researchers have evaluated For example, after verifying that a child is
the use of negative reinforcement contingencies ready and able to participate in a feeding interven-
(i.e., avoidance) to increase self-feeding with tion (i.e., ruling out medical causes, assessing for
individuals who have self-feeding skills. prerequisite skills), a therapist might discuss sev-
Specifically, participants were given choices eral intervention options with a caregiver.
between self-feeding a bite of a target food and Collaboratively, it may be decided that two to
having someone else feed them a bite of the tar- three interventions are of interest and fit within
get food, multiple bites of the target food, or mul- the context of the child’s situation. Then, using an
tiple bites of less preferred foods (Rivas et al., alternating treatment design, therapists can
2014; Vaz, Volkert, & Piazza, 2011). Results “probe” the effects of different interventions (e.g.,
demonstrated that self-feeding increased when DRA and non-contingent reinforcement) on con-
children could avoid having someone feed them sumption and inappropriate behavior (see Fig. 1).
multiple bites of the target food or multiple bites This way, potential differences between the two
of less preferred foods. Given this, it seems pos- interventions will be identified rather quickly, and
sible that once self-feeding skills are established, if not, a parent could choose which intervention
motivation to self-feed, perhaps especially with they find to be more preferable, and this can be
children, who have a history of feeding difficul- pursued while planning for generalization and
ties, can be improved with avoidance contingen- maintenance (more on generalization and mainte-
cies. Given the importance of self-feeding, much nance below). While careful pretreatment assess-
research remains to be done in this area. ments might be pursued in future research, we
also recommend them as good clinical practice
given the likely idiosyncratic responses different
Implications children will have to various feeding treatments.
Of course, as we have described above, there
As we have described, there are many interven- are situations in which less intrusive interventions
tions that have been evaluated to improve feeding are not effective in isolation, when something
behavior. While having options is often consid- more intrusive is required. Again, it is our experi-
ered a good thing, perhaps the largest problem ence that these situations are closely related to the
with having so many interventions is determining severity of the inappropriate mealtime behavior
when a particular intervention should or should (e.g., yelling, pushing the spoon away, attempting
not be used. Clearly, for practitioners, simply to leave the feeding context) that children engage
having a very large menu of possible interven- in. It is also possible that individuals with more
tions is not all that is needed. Future research lengthy histories of reinforcement for challeng-
should focus on identifying the specific contexts ing behavior could require more intrusive inter-
where less intrusive interventions are more or ventions, at least initially. Much more research is
Feeding Disorders 379

100

Percentage of Bites Consumed


NCR

80

60
DRA
40

20

0
1 2 3 4 5 6
Session

Fig. 1  Example of a brief pretreatment assessment of the effects of two interventions

needed to explore these possibilities. The follow- altogether. Extinction-based procedures involve
ing section provides a detailed overview of what withholding functional reinforcers when the
we know about extinction procedures. target behavior is emitted (Cooper, Heron, &
Heward, 2007). When applied to feeding diffi-
culties, such as food refusal or food selectivity,
 scape Extinction and Procedural
E extinction procedures typically include prevent-
Variations ing escape from non-preferred foods or from the
mealtime situation. It is speculated that escape
As noted, the use of antecedent and some extinction procedures are the most efficient means
consequence-­based environmental manipulations of addressing food refusal behavior because the
to address feeding difficulties can often prove to negative reinforcement contingency maintain-
be inefficient when dealing with more persistent ing such behavior (often escape or avoidance
problem behavior (e.g., noncompliance, continual of non-preferred foods) is disrupted (Riordan,
expulsions), warranting a more intensive treat- Iwata, Wohl, & Finney, 1980). Although the effi-
ment model. In such cases, the incorporation of cacy of escape extinction ­procedures for feeding
escape extinction within the existing intervention difficulties has been demonstrated repeatedly
should be considered. At times, feeding disorders in the literature (Ahearn, Kerwin, Eicher, &
are so severe that extinction procedures can be Lukens, 2001; Cooper et al., 1995; Piazza,
immediately warranted as part of an initial treat- Patel, et al., 2003), there are numerous clinical
ment package in an effort to resolve imminent considerations to make regarding whether and
threats to health or well-being (e.g., prevention of how to incorporate them into a comprehensive
further declines in weight and surgical placement intervention plan. With careful consideration of
of a gastrostomy tube). In any case, it is crucial these variables, escape extinction procedures
that extinction procedures be implemented with can be both an efficient and effective treatment
integrity and only by individuals who have been component for remediating feeding difficulties
trained to implement the procedure and recognize when other treatment methods prove ineffective
potential safety risks or under close supervision or inefficient.
of someone with extensive experience in the pro- Escape extinction procedures for feeding prob-
vision of this type of treatment. lems typically involve repeated exposure to, and
Placing any behavior on extinction will continued presentation of, non-preferred or novel
reduce the future frequency of the behavior or foods, as well as escape prevention, and shaping
may stop the occurrence of the targeted behavior consumption of target foods through differential
380 J.K. Fernand et al.

reinforcement. Application of the escape extinc- that have been evaluated include FT-30s (Allison
tion procedure occurs repeatedly (i.e., across sev- et al.; Patel, Piazza, Martinez et al., 2002) and
eral sessions) until the child meets a termination FT-45s (Riordan et al., 1984). It may also be
criterion (e.g., is independently consuming the useful to employ quasi-fixed time schedules in
target food). The most common application of which bites are presented every 30 s, unless the
escape extinction involves what is referred to as child engages in packing (Allison et al.; Patel,
nonremoval of the spoon (NRS). A variation of Piazza, Martinez et al., 2002), or the child’s rate
escape extinction that has received relatively little of acceptance changes in which case the rate of
attention in the literature (yet may seem more presentation can be modified accordingly
acceptable to parents) is nonremoval of the meal (Cooper et al., 1995). A maximum number of
(NRM; Tarbox, Schiff, & Najdowski, 2010). bite presentations per session should be based
on the average number of bites the child has
Nonremoval of the Spoon (NRS)  NRS is the been observed to consume consistently while
most experimentally evaluated escape extinction eating their preferred foods (if relevant) so as to
procedure in the treatment of pediatric feeding avoid presenting an excessive number of bites
disorders. Implementation of NRS may be lik- and ensure satiation is not affecting rates of
ened to feeding an infant as the therapist holds acceptance or inappropriate mealtime behavior.
the feeding apparatus (e.g., spoon) directly in
front of the child’s mouth and deposits each bite Bite Insertion  The manner in which bites are
upon the child opening their mouth. Escape from deposited into the child’s mouth varies across
the bite presentation is prevented and no longer studies that have evaluated NRS. In some cases,
provided for inappropriate mealtime behavior. bites have been inserted at any time the child’s
However, escape is provided in the form of brief mouth was sufficiently open – including yawn-
breaks from bite presentations or termination of ing, crying, and accepting the bite (Anderson &
the meal/treatment session, contingent on the McMillan, 2001; Hoch, Babbitt, Coe, Krell, &
emission of a previously identified alternative Hackbert, 1994; LaRue et al., 2011). In other
behavior or behavior product (e.g., acceptance, cases, the bite was held in close proximity to the
swallowing, mouth-clean). Variables to consider child’s mouth (e.g., within 1 in.), until the child
before implementing NRS include the frequency opened to accept the bite independent of physi-
and rate of bite presentations, the manner in cal or partial physical prompting (Ahearn, 2002;
which bites are deposited, and whether or not Coe et al., 1997; Hoch et al., 1994). The fre-
expelled bites will be re-presented. quency with which the child is exposed to the
target food may depend on the manner in which
Bite Presentation  The number of bites pre- bites are deposited during NRS. For example, if
sented (i.e., opportunities to accept/consume) in the bite is held in front of the child’s lips until
a single treatment session may vary with respect she/he accepts the bite, there could be fewer
to both frequency and rate. The reported num- opportunities for the child to taste and subse-
ber of bites accepted per treatment session have quently consume the bite as compared to an
ranged from 1 (Allison et al., 2012; Sharp, NRS procedure in which bites are inserted at
Jaquess, Bogard, & Morton, 2010) to 51 (Penrod any opportune time. However, consideration
et al., 2010) with a mode of 20 (Ahearn, Kerwin should be given to any problem behaviors emit-
et al., 2001; Ahearn, Kerwin, Eicher, Shantz, & ted by the child when making this selection. For
Swearingin, 1996; Kerwin, Ahearn, Eicher, & example, children who engage in active inappro-
Burd, 1995). In general, bites are presented as priate mealtime behavior (e.g., aggression, self-
discrete trials, and in addition to being pre- injury, operant vomiting) might be less likely to
sented for a fixed number of opportunities, bites emit said behaviors when bites are deposited
are often presented on a set schedule, regardless only when they independently open their mouth
of the behavior emitted by the child. Schedules to accept.
Feeding Disorders 381

Re-presentation of Expelled Bites  NRM may be implemented in conjunction with


Re-presentation is a treatment component requir- bolus and volume fading (Hoch et al., 2001) to
ing expelled bites to be scooped up and re-­ increase the likelihood that the child will contact
presented as a consequence for expulsion (Coe reinforcement.
et al., 1997). Inclusion of this component might
depend largely on the idiosyncrasies of the
child’s behaviors during the application of  onsiderations for the Use of Escape
C
extinction-­based feeding procedures. Prior to Extinction
including this procedure, it is recommended that
an assessment of the child’s eating skills be con- The use of extinction-based procedures can often
ducted to ensure the presented texture can be complement treatment packages including ante-
consumed safely. This might include assess- cedent- and reinforcement-based interventions;
ments conducted by a speech and language however, not every child will respond favorably
pathologist to rule out any physiological abnor- to escape extinction even when implemented as
malities or skill deficits, followed by the type part of a treatment package. Moreover, caregivers
versus texture assessment referred to earlier in must be comfortable with the procedure; other-
this chapter (Munk & Repp, 1994) as previous wise, treatment integrity and generalization of
research has reported texture can affect the rate treatment effects are likely to be compromised.
of expulsion (Patel, Piazza, Santana, et al., 2002), Extinction-based procedures have been docu-
in which case texture fading would also be mented to evoke what Ahearn (2002) describes
required as part of the child’s treatment package as interfering corollary behaviors including neg-
as opposed to re-presentation. ative vocalizations, disruption of the bite presen-
tation, and self-injurious behavior. Whereas the
Nonremoval of the Meal (NRM)  Some chil- occurrence of such behavior typically decreases
dren will not require an intrusive trial-based pro- in frequency and duration as compliance
cedure like NRS. Furthermore, caregivers might increases, it can be distressing for both the child
not feel comfortable consenting to NRS with and parent. The likelihood and range in severity
their child. Older children, for example, who can of emotional responses, as well as the potential
feed themselves and who do not engage in per- for extinction bursts, should be explained to
sistent expulsion, might respond to less intrusive caregivers prior to intervening so that their con-
variations of escape extinction – specifically sent to implement such an intervention is fully
nonremoval of the meal (NRM). Much like informed. This is especially important, as the
NRS, NRM provides repeated exposure to and application of extinction must be consistent to
prevents escape from non-preferred food while avoid unintentional intermittent reinforcement.
reinforcing an alternative behavior. This proce- A single instance of reinforcement for a previ-
dure differs from NRS in regard to the frequency ously extinguished behavior may make it diffi-
of food presentation; the food or bite is pre- cult for the child to discriminate whether
sented as a meal, and the entire mealtime situa- reinforcement will continue to be available in the
tion is terminated following the emission of the future, contributing to a resistance to extinction
alternative behavior (Tarbox et al., 2010). NRM (i.e., strengthening the problem behavior against
closely resembles a common mealtime contin- extinction in the future; Cooper et al., 2007,
gency enforced by parents who require their p. 463–465). If there are concerns about whether
child to clear their plate before being excused procedures would be implemented with fidelity,
from the table; however more research is needed it might be more efficacious to postpone using
to substantiate the effectiveness of this proce- extinction-based procedures, or exclude them
dure. When selecting this method of escape altogether, until additional training produces the
extinction, special consideration should be given high treatment fidelity necessary for such treat-
to the volume of food presented. Furthermore, ments to be successful.
382 J.K. Fernand et al.

Parent Training are implemented one at a time to determine the


necessary resources for training caregivers to
Parent training is a critical component in the suc- implement intervention (e.g., Mueller et al.,
cess of a feeding intervention especially consid- 2004). However, parent and child behavior is not
ering caregivers will ultimately be the ones always measured and reported together regard-
presenting meals and feeding their child follow- less of method used to increase caregiver skills.
ing the identification and, often, implementation For example, Mueller et al. measured parent
of effective treatments. Although it is often the implementation of differential reinforcement
case that parents are not used as the initial with nonremoval of the spoon or noncontingent
behavior-­change agents during intervention, their reinforcement with nonremoval of the spoon pro-
training is crucial in the continued success of cedures, yet to what extent the caregiver’s level
their child’s progress following treatment. of implementation and treatment integrity
Some research has incorporated caregivers impacted their child’s behavior remains unknown
within the assessment process by having them because child behavior was not reported.
serve as therapists when conducting functional Conversely, Anderson and McMillan (2001)
analysis of inappropriate mealtime behavior (e.g., measured and displayed child behavior (i.e.,
Najdowski et al., 2008). One advantage to having acceptance and inappropriate mealtime behavior)
caregivers serve as therapists during the assess- and demonstrated a therapeutic effect of differen-
ment process is that they are the primary individu- tial reinforcement with nonremoval of the spoon
als that serve as feeders within the home and have on child behavior when caregivers implemented
acquired the most history with respect to the feed- the intervention, yet the caregiver’s implementa-
ing process relative to a novel therapist in the tion was not reported. Fluctuations on a session-­
clinic. However, research has yet to be conducted by-­session basis when both caregiver and child
on examining the differences in outcomes between behaviors are measured and depicted can allow
caregiver-conducted and therapist-­ conducted for careful scrutiny over what treatment variables
functional analyses with respect to inappropriate are influencing child behavior and vice versa. For
mealtime behavior, so it remains tentative on example, parents might not need to implement all
whether starting with caregivers as feeders pro- of the treatment components for cases in which it
duces better outcomes. Given the published litera- is demonstrated that parents have poor treatment
ture on functional analysis of inappropriate fidelity, yet consumption persists and inappropri-
mealtime behavior producing clear outcomes ate mealtime behavior remains low. However,
across studies thus far, the presence of a caregiver additional components might be warranted even
or a therapist currently seems unsupported. In if a caregiver is implementing the protocol per-
addition, given that most individuals engage in fectly, yet their child’s acceptance, consumption,
inappropriate mealtime behavior as a function to problem behavior, or other targeted responses do
terminate the bite requirement, it is likely that it not improve. Thus, measuring both parent and
does not matter in terms of who is presenting the child behavior and analyzing their patterns
non-preferred food during sessions. together could serve as important stimuli in guid-
One of the most empirically researched proce- ing the decision-making process when imple-
dures on improving parent-implemented inter- menting behavioral interventions to address
ventions is behavioral skills training (BST; e.g., feeding problems.
Marcus, Swanson, & Vollmer, 2001). BST is a Little work has been conducted examining
treatment package in which instructions, model- caregiver or child preferences for treatments. One
ing, rehearsal or role-play, as well as feedback notable study conducted by Ahearn et al. (1996)
are implemented to increase treatment integrity. compared physical guidance and a nonremoval of
Sometimes these components are implemented the spoon to treat food refusal in three children
as a package (e.g., Seiverling, Williams, Sturmey, using an alternating treatments design. The
& Hart, 2012), whereas other times the components experimenters showed both treatments resulted
Feeding Disorders 383

in similar increases in acceptance and decreases uating the components needed to assess for the
in expulsion. However, the physical guidance prerequisite skills necessary to conduct proto-
treatment was correlated with slightly lower lev- cols with high fidelity and increase those skills
els of problem behavior than the nonremoval of if they are not sufficient via telehealth while
the spoon procedure. When caregivers viewed ensuring the safety of the children undergoing
graphs and video of sessions and were provided those protocols.
with a session-by-session description of their
child’s progress, they reported a preference for
the physical guidance over the nonremoval of the  rogramming for Generalization
P
spoon procedure. Ahearn et al. provided self-­ and Maintenance
report statements from caregivers that implied
caregiver preference for treatment might be influ- Programming for generalization and mainte-
enced by the efficiency and effectiveness of the nance is paramount to the success of any of the
preferred intervention. Future work in caregiver aforementioned interventions. Although mea-
and child preferences for different types of feed- sures of generalization and maintenance have not
ing treatments remains imperative as behavioral been commonly reported in the feeding literature,
feeding procedures are disseminated to other a number of study characteristics have been iden-
fields and become selected for based on their per- tified that are commensurate with strategies for
ceived acceptability. promoting generalization and maintenance,
Conducting sessions via telehealth (e.g., including reinforcing instances of generalization
Barretto, Wacker, Harding, Lee, & Berg, 2006) (e.g., Anderson & McMillan, 2001; Cooper et al.,
seems to be a cost-effective method in which 1995; Galensky, Miltenberger, Stricker, &
specialized behavioral services can be imple- Garlinghouse, 2001), training skills that contact
mented in the child’s natural environment and natural contingencies (e.g., Galensky et al.,
when families live in remote areas relative to 2001; Najdowski et al., 2003), incorporating a
where those services are housed. One advantage variety of relevant stimulus situations in training
of this training option is that caregivers serve as (e.g., Hoch et al., 2001; LaRue et al., 2011), and
feeders in their home, where the child’s meals incorporating common stimuli (e.g., Najdowski
typically take place. However, safety precau- et al.; Mueller et al., 2004).
tions prior to conducting clinical treatments via In a notable study, Najdowski et al. (2010)
telehealth should be taken into consideration. specifically measured generalization and main-
Thus, currently this method of conducting ses- tenance of participants’ consumption following
sions might be most useful as follow-up after a treatment evaluation in which parents served
initial treatments have already been validated as the primary behavior-change agents in their
and caregivers have been thoroughly trained as own homes. Results indicated that caregivers
little research has been conducted on the types trained to implement a treatment package con-
of feeding problems that might be addressed in sisting of demand fading, differential rein-
this manner. For example, Peterson, Volkert, forcement, and NRS successfully increased
and Zeleny (2015) conducted sessions via tele- their child’s consumption of both foods tar-
health with caregivers as therapists with one geted during the intervention as well as
participant’s sessions to increase self-feeding untrained foods. Further, the schedule and
following a previous intervention to increase magnitude of reinforcement were systemati-
consumption. In another study, Wilkins et al. cally thinned to mirror more natural contingen-
(2014) taught a child to close her mouth around cies of reinforcement, and behavior change was
a spoon using a three-step prompting procedure maintained over time. Some notable features of
via telehealth as a follow-up procedure after an this study that map onto recommended strate-
inpatient clinical evaluation of those procedures. gies for the promotion of generalization and
Thus, future research remains warranted in eval- maintenance include reinforcing instances of
384 J.K. Fernand et al.

generalization (providing training in the target ber of exemplars needed for response general-
situation and teaching parenting skills in the ization. The nature of the child’s feeding
home), training skills that contact natural con- disorder could also serve as a factor in the deci-
tingencies (systematically thinning the magni- sion to target a single food versus multiple
tude and schedule of reinforcement), and foods. For example, if a child is engaging in
incorporating common stimuli (using parents total food refusal, it might be beneficial to estab-
as the primary behavior-change agents and lish consumption of a single food as an initial
foods regularly prepared at home). treatment goal to increase oral intake, whereas a
When the provision of treatment cannot be child with several foods already included in
carried out in the child’s home (e.g., inpatient their diet (albeit within restricted food catego-
treatment), generalization strategies must be spe- ries) might benefit from more effective general-
cifically incorporated into the treatment plan. ization programming.
Such strategies may include selecting foods to Incorporating stimuli from the natural envi-
target what the child is likely to encounter in their ronment into the treatment setting may also pro-
natural environment and training skills likely to mote generalization. For example, asking parents
contact natural contingencies of reinforcement to bring their child’s favorite plate and utensils,
such as targeting snacks commonly provided at targeting foods parents bring from home, and
the child’s school or focusing on age-appropriate arranging the treatment setting to resemble the
purees for children who are selective with respect mealtime setting at home as closely as possible.
to the texture of foods (e.g., transitioning from Lastly, stimuli from the treatment setting might
jarred baby food to age-appropriate purees also be incorporated in the child’s natural envi-
[yogurt, applesauce, mashed potatoes, etc.]). ronment (e.g., visual timers, mealtime rules), and
Attention should be given to contingencies in the in some situations self-generated mediators may
natural environment that may need to be modi- be incorporated into the child’s natural environ-
fied; for example, family members may need to ment; for instance, if a child learned to pace their
be trained to not make negative comments about eating using a vibrating pager, the vibrating pager
food or provide attention for inappropriate meal- can transition with the child back to the mealtime
time behaviors. setting at home.
Consideration should also be given to the
number of foods (exemplars) targeted. The
number of foods targeted during a single treat- Treatment Considerations
ment session has varied widely in the literature and Recommendations
from a single food (Bachmeyer et al., 2009;
Freeman & Piazza, 1998) to as many as three to This chapter has reviewed several treatment
five different foods (Hoch et al., 1994, 2001; options to remediate feeding disorders, including
LaRue et al., 2011). The total number of foods reinforcement-based interventions, antecedent
targeted during the span of treatment may be interventions, interventions to address skill defi-
directly related to the likelihood of generaliza- cits, and escape extinction. We have also
tion as well as caregiver independence with described assessment methods that can be used to
treatment implementation. Findings from inform the selection of variations within each of
Ahearn (2002) suggest that selecting a single these treatment categories as well as other con-
target food to present during sessions may textual factors that should be considered when
increase the speed with which independent con- designing an intervention. Some additional con-
sumption of the food occurs, whereas selecting siderations germane to each of the aforemen-
and presenting multiple foods may lead to more tioned treatments are discussed below, namely,
efficient rates of generalization. The child’s identification of target foods, identification of
generalization of previously acquired skills target behaviors, and the frequency and duration
should also be considered in terms of the num- of treatment sessions.
Feeding Disorders 385

Identifying Target Foods Identifying Target Behaviors


to Strengthen Through Differential
When designing an intervention, target foods Reinforcement
should be selected in consultation with caregiv-
ers. Including caregivers in this decision could Differential reinforcement is typically provided
increase caregiver compliance with treatment, as both for acceptance and the behavior product of
well as increase the likelihood that foods consumption, mouth-clean. Consideration
included in treatment will continue to be pre- should be given to the child’s existing skill set
sented in the child’s home environment follow- and history of inappropriate mealtime behavior
ing termination of the intervention. It might also when selecting appropriate alternatives to
be beneficial to collect several days of data refusal. For example, a child with a long his-
regarding the child’s existing diet, as initial tar- tory, absent of oral acceptance, might benefit
get foods that more closely resemble the existing from reinforcement of acceptance initially as
diet could increase the likelihood of success. opposed to mouth-­clean, so as to bring the child
Further, assessing the child’s preference for into contact with reinforcement more consis-
foods caregivers would like to target in treatment tently and efficiently. Regardless of the behav-
may reveal some foods to be more preferred than ior selected to replace inappropriate mealtime
initially reported by caregivers. Data collection behavior, the operational definition should ref-
could reveal that high rates of acceptance are erence with clarity the required volume, latency,
occurring with foods that also have a low rate of and level of independence necessary to occa-
presentation. Therefore, training caregivers to sion reinforcement.
increase the presentation of certain foods might A reinforcement contingency for acceptance
benefit the child in the absence of direct inter- has been cited most frequently in the literature;
vention. Measures of preference (approach however, the way in which acceptance has been
responses, interaction with the foods presented operationally defined varies across publica-
[smelling, licking, etc.]) may also be used to tions. The most frequently reported definition
identify relative preferences of non-preferred of acceptance appears to have initially been
foods, from most to least disliked. Selecting provided by Hoch et al. (1994), “...only those
those non-preferred foods that are the least aver- instances in which the [participant] opened his
sive may be advantageous, though this remains mouth such that the food was deposited within 5
an empirical question as previously noted. seconds of instructing him to do so…” (p. 110).
In addition to caregiver preference for target It should be noted that reinforcement based on
foods, the types and textures should vary with this definition has been documented to increase
respect to the child’s dietary needs, restrictions, the persistent expulsion of target foods (Coe
and oral motor skill set. For example, a child who et al., 1997). Observed increases are possibly
consumes several fruits but no vegetables would related to the provision of reinforcement prior to
benefit from targeting vegetables specifically. the emission of the terminal link in the behavior
Continuing, special diets and food allergies may chain of consumption. In some studies, research-
in contrast preclude interventions with specific ers have reinforced both acceptance and reten-
types of foods. Lastly, children with structural tion (Riordan et al., 1984) or have transitioned
abnormalities or delayed oral motor skills, spe- from reinforcement of acceptance to swallowing
cifically immature chewing and swallowing pat- (Coe et al.). Continuing, bite re-presentation has
terns, require intervention with less advanced been demonstrated to be an effective treatment
textures (e.g., pureed or ground) while learning component for addressing persistent expul-
to chew and swallow, with the ultimate goal sions that have resulted from reinforcement of
being to advance to tabletop textures or the tex- acceptance alone (Coe et al.). In contrast, sev-
ture deemed appropriate, which will be specific eral studies have documented that the reinforce-
to each child. ment of acceptance alone produces consumption
386 J.K. Fernand et al.

(Ahearn, 2002; Ahearn, Kerwin, et al., 2001; predetermined session duration was reached
Cooper et al., 1995; Vaz et al., 2011). Thus, the (Allison et al., 2012; Coe et al., 1997; McCartney,
child’s learning history with each of the required Anderson, & English, 2005). Others have used
behaviors in the behavior chain (e.g., accepting, either/or criteria in which session termination
retaining, chewing, swallowing) should be con- was contingent on the participant either consum-
sidered when selecting an alternative response for ing a specified number of bites or the session was
reinforcement (Patel, Piazza, Santana, et al., 2002). terminated after a maximum duration was
reached (Ahearn, Kerwin, et al., 2001; Cooper
et al., 1995; Freeman & Piazza, 1998), whichever
Treatment Sessions occurred first. Still others required that the ses-
sion continue until the bite presented just prior to
Repeated exposure is an inherent component of the end of the session was consumed. For exam-
all feeding interventions, but particularly ple, Cooper et al. (1995) had a maximum dura-
extinction-­based feeding procedures. Multiple tion of 20 min per session for two participants.
sessions are required over a period of time so that Refusal to consume the bite presented just prior
a sufficient number of learning opportunities to the culmination of the session duration resulted
occur to disrupt the preexisting contingency of in the continued presentation of that bite, regard-
escape from the non-preferred or novel foods less of the effects on duration of treatment, until
being presented and establish a history of rein- consumption occurred. Following consumption
forcement for acceptance and consumption. The of that particular bite, the session was terminated.
number of treatment sessions required will vary The shortest and longest durations identified in
across children based on individual characteris- the literature range from 5 min (Allison et al.,
tics and efficacy of treatment. 2012; Patel et al., 2002) to 60 min (Coe et al.;
Patel et al., 2006). It should be noted that while
Frequency  Sessions may be held several times session duration is best predetermined, in prac-
each day or as little as two to three times each tice these durations can be altered throughout the
week. Attempting to implement procedures for course of treatment to suit the particular child’s
feeding with a frequency less than two to three needs and progression of treatment, and the same
times a week will not only increase the duration is true of session frequency.
of treatment considerably, but it may not be suf-
ficient for maintaining progress. The frequency
of sessions should reflect the child’s age and skill Concluding Remarks
set, levels of satiation (i.e., latency of most recent
meal), as well as treatment model (e.g., inpatient When designing interventions to address feeding
treatment will be more intense with a more fre- difficulties, the initial use of antecedent interven-
quent schedule of sessions). Furthermore, fre- tions and less intrusive consequence-based inter-
quency of sessions will depend on the duration of ventions is recommended. This is due to the ease
each session and should negatively correspond with which such interventions might be faded, the
with increases in duration. It may be beneficial to ethical preference for least intrusive means of
determine a maximum duration of time for a 24-h remediation, as well as the decreased likelihood of
period and then break that down into the desired corollary problem behavior. That said, escape
number of sessions so that sufficient breaks are extinction remains the most empirically supported
provided in between treatments. intervention for feeding disorders and thus should
not be ruled out in spite of a myriad of factors to
Duration  There are several options for defining consider when designing a treatment package that
session durations when implementing feeding includes an escape extinction component.
procedures. Some successful interventions have As previously noted, it can be difficult to
terminated session regardless of progress, once a ­determine when and when not to use particular
Feeding Disorders 387

interventions given the limited number of studies Ahearn, W. H., Kerwin, M. E., Eicher, P. S., & Lukens, C. T.
(2001). An ABAC comparison of two intensive inter-
that have provided information on how contextual
ventions for food refusal. Behavior Modification, 25,
variables and participant characteristics may be 385–405. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445501253002
correlated with positive (or negative) treatment Ahearn, W. H., Kerwin, M. E., Eicher, P. S., Shantz, J.,
outcomes (Silbaugh et al., 2016). However, using & Swearingin, W. (1996). An alternating treatments
comparison of two intensive interventions for food
functional analysis logic, we can make informed
refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29,
treatment selections by conducting pretreatment 321–332. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-321
assessments in which we quickly compare varia- Allison, J., Wilder, D. A., Chong, I., Lugo, A., Pike, J.,
tions of treatment to determine differential effec- & Rudy, N. (2012). A comparison of differential rein-
forcement and noncontingent reinforcement to treat
tiveness. Additionally, we should also consider
food selectivity in a child with autism. Journal of
parental preference and aptitude for implement- Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 613–617. https://doi.
ing certain interventions. For example, a caregiver org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-613
who has a long history of parental mismanage- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5.
ment (e.g., using bribery or threats as opposed to
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
contingent reinforcement) may be more success- Anderson, C. M., & McMillan, K. (2001). Parental use
ful implementing noncontingent reinforcement as of escape extinction and differential reinforcement
opposed to differential reinforcement. In short, to treat food selectivity. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 34, 511–515. https://doi.org/10.1901/
practitioners should use all of the information at
jaba.2001.34-511
hand to determine the best treatment options, Archer, L. A., Rosenbaum, P. L., & Streiner, D. L.
including relevant information from other disci- (1991). The children’s eating behavior inventory:
plines that can be extremely informative when Reliability and validity results. Journal of Pediatric
Psychology, 16(5), 629–642. https://doi.org/10.1093/
making decisions. This includes appropriate
jpepsy/16.5.629
foods to target that are both safe for the child to Arvedson, J. C. (2008). Assessment of pediatric dys­
consume and beneficial in terms of meeting nutri- phagia and feeding disorders: Clinical and instru­
tional requirements as well as the selection of the mental approaches. Developmental Disabilities
Research Reviews, 14, 118–127. https://doi.org/
most appropriate utensils and cups to facilitate
10.1002/ddrr.17
oral motor skill development. Provision of treat- Babbitt, R. L., Hoch, T. A., Coe, D. A., Cataldo, M. F.,
ment in the context of a multidisciplinary team is Kelly, K. J., Stackhouse, C., & Perman, J. A. (1994).
ideal, and for those practitioners working outside Behavioral assessment and treatment of pediat-
ric feeding disorders. Journal of Behavioral and
of a multidisciplinary team, we recommend con-
Developmental Pediatrics, 15, 278–291. https://doi.
sultation with professionals from relevant disci- org/10.1097/00004703-199408000-00011
plines to ensure the best possible care. Bachmeyer, M. H. (2009). Treatment of selective and
inadequate food intake in children: A review and prac-
tical guide. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 2, 43–50.
Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/
References articles/PMC2854063/
Bachmeyer, M. H., Piazza, C. C., Fredrick, L. D., Reed,
Ahearn, W. H. (2002). Effect of two methods of introduc- G. K., Rivas, K. D., & Kadey, H. J. (2009). Functional
ing foods during feeding treatment on acceptance of analysis and treatment of multiply controlled inappro-
previously rejected items. Behavioral Interventions, priate mealtime behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
17, 111–127. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.112 Analysis, 42, 641–658. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Ahearn, W. H. (2003). Using simultaneous presentation jaba.2009.42-641
to increase vegetable consumption in a mildly selec- Barretto, A., Wacker, D. P., Harding, J., Lee, J., & Berg,
tive child with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior W. K. (2006). Using telemedicine to conduct behav-
Analysis, 36, 361–365. https://doi.org/10.1901/ ioral assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior
jaba.2003.36-361 Analysis, 39, 333–340. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Ahearn, W. H., Castine, T., Nault, K., & Green, G. (2001). jaba.2006.173-04
An assessment of food acceptance in children with Berlin, K. S., Davies, W. H., Lobato, D. J., &
autism or pervasive developmental disorder-not other- Silverman, A. H. (2009). A biopsychosocial model
wise specified. Journal of Autism and Developmental of normative and problematic pediatric feeding.
Disorders, 31, 505–551. https://doi.org/10.102 Children’s Healthcare, 38, 263–282. https://doi.
3/A:1012221026124 org/10.1080/02739610903235984
388 J.K. Fernand et al.

Borrero, C. S., Woods, J. N., Borrero, J. C., Masler, E. A., time behaviors inventory. Journal of Autism and
& Lesser, A. D. (2010). Descriptive analyses of pedi- Developmental Disorders, 45(9), 2667–2673. https://
atric food refusal and acceptance. Journal of Applied doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2435-4
Behavior Analysis, 43, 71–88. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Dietz, W. H. (1998). Health consequences of obesity
jaba.2010.43-71 in youth: Childhood predictors of adult disease.
Brown, J. F., Spencer, K., & Swift, S. (2002). A parent train- Pediatrics, 101(2), 518–525. https://doi.org/10.1542/
ing programme for chronic food refusal: A case study. peds.101.3.s1.518
British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 118–121. Ewry, D., & Fryling, M. J. (2016). Evaluating the high-­
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1468-3156.2002.00128.x probability instructional sequence to increase the
Burklow, K. A., Phelps, A. N., Schultz, J. R., acceptance of foods with an adolescent with autism.
McConnell, K., & Colin, R. (1998). Classifying Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9, 380–383. https://doi.
complex pediatric feeding disorders. Journal of org/10.1007/s40617-015-0098-4
Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 27, Fernand, J. K., Penrod, B., Brice Fu, S., Whelan, C. M., &
143–147. Retrieved from: http://journals.lww.com/ Medved, S. (2015). The effects of choice between non-
jpgn/Abstract/1998/08000/Classifying_Complex_ preferred foods on the food consumption of individu-
Pediatric_Feeding_Disorders.3.aspx als with food selectivity. Journal of Applied Behavior
Clawson, E. P., & Elliott, C. A. (2014). Integrating Analysis, 31, 87–101. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1423
evidence-­based treatment of pediatric feeding disor- Field, D., Garland, M., & Williams, K. (2003). Correlates
ders into clinical practice: Challenges to implemen- of specific childhood feeding problems. Journal of
tation. Clinical Practice in Pediatric Psychology, Paediatrics and Child Health, 39, 299–304. https://
2(312), 321. https://doi.org/10.1037/cpp0000076 doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1754.2003.00151.x
Coe, D. A., Babbitt, R. L., Williams, K. E., Hajmihalis, Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P.,
C., Snyder, A. M., Ballard, C., & Efron, L. A. (1997). Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of
Use of extinction and reinforcement to increase two approaches for identifying reinforcers for per-
food consumption and reduce expulsion. Journal of sons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 581–583. https://doi. Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. https://doi.
org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-581 org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-491
Collins, B. C., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Holcombe, A., & Freeman, K. A., & Piazza, C. C. (1998). Combining stim-
Leatherby, J. G. (1991). Using constant time delay to ulus fading, reinforcement and extinction to treat food
teach self-feeding to young students with severe/pro- refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(4),
found handicaps: Evidence of limited effectiveness. 691–694. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-691
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, Galensky, T. L., Miltenberger, R. G., Stricker, J. M., &
3, 157–179. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01045931 Garlinghouse, M. A. (2001). Functional assessment
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). and the treatment of mealtime behavior problems.
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 3, 211–
River, NJ: Pearson. 224. https://doi.org/10.1177/109830070100300403
Cooper, L. J., Wacker, D. P., McComas, J. J., Peck, S. M., Girolami, P. A., & Scotti, J. R. (2001). Use of analog
Richman, D., Drew, J., & Brown, K. (1995). Use of functional analysis in assessing the function of meal-
component analyses to identify active variables in time behavior problems. Education and Training in
treatment packages for children with feeding disor- Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
ders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 139– 36(2), 207–223. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/
154. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-139 stable/23879736
Cornwell, S. L., Kelly, K., & Austin, L. (2010). Pediatric Greer, A. J., Gulotta, C. S., Masler, E. A., & Laud, R. B.
feeding disorders: Effectiveness of multidisciplinary (2008). Caregiver stress and outcomes of children
inpatient treatment of gastrostomy-tube dependent with pediatric feeding disorders treated in an inten-
children. Children’s Health Care, 39, 214–231. https:// sive interdisciplinary program. Journal of Pediatric
doi.org/10.1080/02739615.2010.493770 Psychology, 33(6), 612–620. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Dawson, J. E., Piazza, C. C., Sevin, B. M., Gulotta, jpepsy/jsm116
C. S., Lerman, D., & Kelley, M. L. (2003). Use of the Greer, R. D., Dorow, L., Williams, G., McCorkle, N., &
high-probability instructional sequence and escape Asnes, R. (1991). Peer-mediated procedure to induce
extinction in a child with food refusal. Journal of swallowing and food acceptance in young children.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 105–108. https://doi. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 783–790.
org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-105 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-783
DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a multi- Gulotta, C. S., Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., & Layer, S. A.
ple-stimulus presentation format for assessing reinforcer (2005). Using food redistribution to reduce packing in
preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, children with severe food refusal. Journal of Applied
519–533. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-519 Behavior Analysis, 38, 39–50. https://doi.org/10.1901/
DeMand, A., Johnson, C., & Foldes, E. (2015). jaba.2005.168-03
Psychometric properties of the brief autism meal-
Feeding Disorders 389

Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., Lindberg, J. S., & Conners, Luiselli, J. K. (2000). Cueing, demand fading, and posi-
J. (2003). Response-restriction analysis I: Assessment tive reinforcement to establish self-feeding and oral
of activity preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior consumption in a child with chronic food refusal.
Analysis, 36, 47–58. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. Behavior Modification, 24, 348–358. https://doi.
2003.36-47 org/10.1177/0145445500243003
Heffer, R. W., & Kelley, M. L. (1994). Nonorganic fail- Lukens, C. T., & Linscheid, T. R. (2008). Development
ure to thrive: Developmental outcomes and psychoso- and validation of an inventory to assess mealtime
cial assessment and intervention issues. Research in behavior problems in children with autism. Journal
Developmental Disabilities, 15(4), 247–268. https:// of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 342–
doi.org/10.1016/0891-4222(94)90006-X 352. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0401-5
Hoch, T. A., Babbitt, R. L., Coe, D. A., Krell, D. M., Marcus, B. A., Swanson, V., & Vollmer, T. R. (2001).
& Hackbert, L. (1994). Contingency contacting: Effects of parent training on parent and child behav-
Combining positive reinforcement and escape extinc- ior using procedures based on functional analyses.
tion procedures to treat persistent food refusal. Behavioral Interventions, 16, 87–104. https://doi.
Behavior Modification, 18, 106–128. Retrieved from org/10.1002/bin.87
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8037643 Matson, J. L., & Kuhn, D. E. (2001). Identifying feeding
Hoch, T. A., Farrar-Schneider, D., Babbitt, R. L., problems in mentally retarded persons: Development
Berkowitz, V. I., Snyder, A. L., Rizol, L. M., … Wise, and reliability of the screening tool of feeding prob-
D. T. (2001). Empirical analysis of a multicomponent lems (STEP). Research in Developmental Disabilities,
pediatric feeding disorder treatment. Education and 22(2), 165–172.
Treatment of Children, 24, 176–198. Retrieved from McCartney, E. J., Anderson, C. M., & English, C. L.
www.jstor.org/stable/42899653 (2005). Effect of brief clinic-based training on the
Holmes, S. (2012). Enteral nutrition: An overview. ability of caregivers to implement escape extinction.
Nursing Standard, 26, 41–46. https://doi.org/10.7748/ Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 7(1),
ns2012.05.26.39.41.c9133 18–32. https://doi.org/10.1177/10983007050070010
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & 301
Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis Meier, A. E., Fryling, M. J., & Wallace, M. D. (2012).
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Using high-probability foods to increase the accep-
27(2), 197–209. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27- tance of low-probability foods. Journal of Applied
197. (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in Behavior Analysis, 45, 149–153. https://doi.
Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-149
Kadey, H. J., Roane, H. S., Diaz, J. C., & Merrow, J. M. Morris, S. E. (1989). Development of oral-motor skills
(2013). An evaluation of chewing and swallow- in the neurologically impaired child receiving non-­
ing for a child diagnosed with autism. Journal of oral feedings. Dysphagia, 3, 135–154. Retrived from
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 25, 343– https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2517923
354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-012-9313-1 Mueller, M. M., Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Kelley,
Kerwin, M. E., Ahearn, W. H., Eicher, P. S., & Burd, D. M. M. E., & Pruett, A. (2004). Increasing variety of
(1995). The costs of eating: A behavioral economic food consumed by blending nonpreferred foods
analysis of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior into preferred foods. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 28(3), 245–260. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Analysis, 37, 159–170. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
jaba.1995.28-245 2004.37-159
Lamm, N., & Greer, R. D. (1988). Induction and main- Munk, D. D., & Repp, A. C. (1994). Behavioral assess-
tenance of swallowing responses in infants with dys- ment of feeding problems of individuals with severe
phagia. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27,
143–156. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1988.21-143 241–250. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-241
LaRue, R. H., Stewart, V., Piazza, C. C., Volkert, V. M., Murphy, J., & Zlomke, K. R. (2016). A behavioral parent-­
Patel, M. R., & Zeleny, J. (2011). Escape as reinforce- training intervention for a child with avoidant/restric-
ment and EE in the treatment of feeding problems. tive food intake disorder. Clinical Practice in Pediatric
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(4), 719– Psychology, 4, 23–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/
735. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-719 cpp0000128
Linscheid, T. R. (2006). Behavioral treatments for pedi- Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Doney, J. K., &
atric feeding disorders. Behavior Modification, 30(1), Ghezzi, P. M. (2003). Parental assessment and treat-
6–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445505282165 ment of food selectivity in natural settings. Journal of
Luiselli, J. K. (1991). Acquisition of self-feeding in a child Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 383–386. https://doi.
with Lowe’s syndrome. Journal of Developmental org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-383
and Physical Disabilities, 3, 181–189. https://doi. Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Penrod, B., Tarbox,
org/10.1007/BF01045932 J., Reagon, K., & Higbee, T. S. (2008). Caregiver-­
Luiselli, J. K. (1993). Training self-feeding skills conducted experimental functional analyses of inap-
in children who are deaf and blind. Behavior propriate mealtime behavior. Journal of Applied
Modification, 17, 457–473. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Behavior Analysis, 41, 459–465. https://doi.org/
01454455930174003 10.1901/jaba.2008.41-459
390 J.K. Fernand et al.

Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Reagon, K., Penrod, Penrod, B., Wallace, M. D., Reagon, K., Betz, A., &
B., Higbee, T. S., & Tarbox, J. (2010). Utilizing a Higbee, T. S. (2010). A component analysis of a
home-based parent training approach in the treat- parent-­conducted multi-component treatment for food
ment of food selectivity. Behavioral Interventions, 25, selectivity. Behavioral Interventions, 25, 207–220.
89–107. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.298 https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.3 07
Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, B. A., Peterson, K. M., Volkert, V. M., & Zeleny, J. R. (2015).
& Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stimulus prefer- Increasing self-drinking for children with feeding
ence and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48,
individuals. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 436–441. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.210
249–255. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-249 Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Brown, K. A., Shore, B. A.,
Patel, M., Reed, G. K., Piazza, C. C., Mueller, M., Patel, M. R., Katz, R. M., … Blakely-Smith, A. (2003).
Bachmeyer, M. H., & Layer, S. A. (2007). Use of a Functional analysis of inappropriate mealtime behav-
high-probability instructional sequence to increase iors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(2),
compliance to feeding demands in the absence of 187–204. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-187
escape extinction. Behavioral Interventions, 22, 305– Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Gulotta, C. S., Sevin, B. M.,
310. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.251 & Layer, S. A. (2003). On the relative contributions
Patel, M. R., Piazza, C. C., Kelley, M. L., Ochsner, C. A., of positive reinforcement and escape extinction in the
& Santana, L. M. (2001). Using a fading procedure treatment of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior
to increase fluid consumption in a child with feeding Analysis, 36, 309–324. https://doi.org/10.1901/
problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, jaba.2003.36-309
357–360. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-357 Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Santana, C. M., Goh, H.,
Patel, M. R., Piazza, C. C., Layer, S. A., Coleman, R., & Delia, M. D., & Lancaster, B. M. (2002). An evalu-
Swartzwelder, D. M. (2005). A systematic evaluation ation of simultaneous and sequential presentation of
of food textures to decrease packing and increase oral preferred and non-preferred food to treat food selec-
intake in children with pediatric feeding disorders. tivity. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 259–
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 89–100. 270. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-259
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.161-02 Reed, G. K., Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Layer, S. A.,
Patel, M. R., Piazza, C. C., Martinez, C. J., Volkert, V. M., Bachmeyer, M. H., Bethke, S. D., & Gutshall, K. A.
& Santana, C. M. (2002). An evaluation of two dif- (2004). On the relative contributions of noncontingent
ferential reinforcement procedures with escape extinc- reinforcement and escape extinction in the treatment
tion to treat food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Analysis, 35, 363–374. https://doi.org/10.1901/ 37, 24–42. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2004.37-27
jaba.2002.35-363 Riordan, M. M., Iwata, B. A., Finney, J. N., Wohl, M. K.,
Patel, M. R., Piazza, C. C., Santana, C. M., & Volkert, & Stanley, A. E. (1984). Behavioral assessment and
V. M. (2002). An evaluation of food type and texture treatment of chronic food refusal in handicapped chil-
in the treatment of a feeding problem. Journal of dren. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 327–
Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 183–186. https://doi. 341. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1984.17-327
org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-183 Riordan, M. M., Iwata, B. A., Wohl, M. K., &
Patel, M. R., Reed, G. K., Piazza, C. C., Bachmeyer, Finney, J. N. (1980). Behavioral treatment of food
M. H., Layer, S. A., & Pabico, R. S. (2006). An evalu- refusal and selectivity in developmentally disabled
ation of a high-probability instructional sequence to children. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 1,
increase acceptance of food and decrease inappropri- 95–112.
ate behavior in children with pediatric feeding dis- Rivas, K. M., Piazza, C. C., Roane, H. S., Volkert, V. M.,
orders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27, Stewart, V., Kadey, H. J., & Groff, R. A. (2014).
430–442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2005.05.005 Analysis of self-feeding in children with feeding
Pence, S. T., Roscoe, E. M., Bourret, J. C., & Ahearn, disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47,
W. H. (2009). Relative contributions of three descrip- 710–722. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.170
tive methods: Implications for behavioral assessment. Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 425–446. B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus preference
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-425 assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31,
Penrod, B., Gardella, L., & Fernand, J. (2012). An evalu- 605–620. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-605
ation of a progressive high-probability instructional Seiverling, L., Williams, K., Sturmey, P., Hart, S., &
sequence combined with low-probability demand Wallace, M. (2012). Effects of behavioral skills train-
fading in the treatment of food selectivity. Journal of ing on parental treatments of children’s food selectiv-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 527–537. https://doi. ity. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 197–203.
org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-527 doi: 10.1901/jaba.2012.45-197
Penrod, B., & VanDalen, K. H. (2010). An evaluation of Seubert, C., Fryling, M. J., Wallace, M. D., Jiminez, A., &
emer­ging preference for non-preferred foods targeted in Meier, A. (2014). Antecedent interventions for pedi-
the treatment of food selectivity. Behavioral Interventions, atric feeding problems. Journal of Applied Behavior
25, 239–251. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.306 Analysis, 47, 449–453. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.117
Feeding Disorders 391

Sharp, W. G., Jaquess, D. L., Bogard, J. D., & Morton, feeding in a child with food selectivity. Journal of
J. F. (2010). Additive, multi-component treatment of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 915–920. https://doi.
emerging refusal topographies in a pediatric feeding org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-915
disorder. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 32, Volkert, V. M., Peterson, K. M., Zeleny, J. R., & Piazza,
51–61. https://doi.org/10.1080/07317100903539931 C. C. (2014). A clinical protocol to increase chewing
Shore, B., & Piazza, C. C. (1997). Pediatric feeding dis- and assess mastication in children with feeding disor-
orders. In E. Konarski & J. Favell (Eds.), Manual for ders. Behavior Modification, 38, 705–729. https://doi.
the assessment and treatment of the behavior disor- org/10.1177/0145445514536575
der of people with mental retardation (pp. 65–89). Volkert, V. M., Piazza, C. C., Vaz, P. C. M., & Frese,
New York: The Guilford Press. J. (2013). A pilot study to increase chewing in children
Shore, B. A., Babbitt, R. L., Williams, K. E., Coe, D. A., with feeding disorders. Behavior Modification, 37,
& Snyder, A. (1998). Use of texture fading in the treat- 391–408. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445512474295
ment of food selectivity. Journal of Applied Behavior Wilder, D. A., Normand, M., & Atwell, J. (2005).
Analysis, 31, 621–633. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Noncontingent reinforcement as treatment for
jaba.1998.31-621 food refusal and associated self-injury. Journal of
Silbaugh, B. C., Penrod, B., Whelan, C. M., Hernandez, Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 549–553. https://doi.
D. A., Wingate, H. V., Falcomata, T. S., & Lang, R. org/10.1901/jaba.2005.132-04
(2016). A systematic synthesis of behavioral inter- Wilkins, J. W., Piazza, C. C., Groff, R. A., Volkert, V. M.,
ventions for food selectivity of children with autism Kozisek, J. M., & Milnes, S. M. (2014). Utensil
spectrum disorders. Review Journal of Autism and manipulation during initial treatment of pediatric feed-
Developmental Disorders, 3, 345–357. doi: 10.1007/ ing problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
s40489-016-0087-8 47, 694–708. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.169
Smith, C. M., Smith, R. G., Dracobly, J. D., & Peterson Williams, K. E., Field, D. G., & Seiverling, L. (2010).
Pace, A. (2012). Multiple-respondent anecdotal Food refusal in children: A review of the literature.
assessments: An analysis of interrater agreement and Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 625–633.
correspondance with analogue assessment outcomes. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.01.001
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 779–795. Williams, K. E., & Foxx, R. M. (2007). Treating
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-779 eating problems of children with autism spectrum dis-
Tarbox, J., Schiff, A., & Najdowski, A. C. (2010). Parent-­ orders and developmental disabilities. Austin, TX:
implemented procedural modification of escape Pro-Ed.
extinction in the treatment of food selectivity in a Williams, K. E., Gibbons, B. G., & Schreck, K. A. (2005).
young child with autism. Education and Treatment of Comparing selective eaters with and without devel-
Children, 33(2), 223–234. Retrieved from http://eric. opmental disabilities. Journal of Developmental
ed.gov/?id=EJ882719 and Physical Disabilities, 17, 299–309. https://doi.
Tiger, J. H., & Hanley, G. P. (2006). Using reinforcer pair- org/10.1007/s10882-005-4387-7
ing and fading to increase the milk consumption of a Windsor, J., Piché, L. M., & Loche, P. A. (1994).
preschool child. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Preference testing: A comparison of two pre-
39, 399–403. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2006.6-06 sentation methods. Research in Developmental
VanDalen, K. H., & Penrod, B. (2010). A comparison of Disabilities, 15(6), 439–455. https://doi.
simultaneous versus sequential presentation of novel org/10.1016/0891-4222(94)90028-0
foods in the treatment of food selectivity. Behavioral Zarcone, J. R., Rodgers, T. A., Iwata, B. A., Rourke,
Interventions, 25, 191–206. https://doi.org/10.1002/ D. A., & Dorsey, M. F. (1991). Reliability analysis of
bin.310 the motivation assessment scale: A failure to replicate.
Vaz, P. C. M., Volkert, V. M., & Piazza, C. C. (2011). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12(4), 349–
Using negative reinforcement to increase self-­ 360. https://doi.org/10.1016/0891-4222(91)90031-m
Toilet Training: Behavioral
and Medical Considerations

Pamela McPherson, Claire O. Burns,
Mark J. Garcia, Vinay S. Kothapalli,
Shawn E. McNeil, and Timothy Thompson

Medical Specialty Consideration:


Contents Gastroenterological............................................   413
Toilet Training: Behavioral and Medical
Recommendations..................................................   414
Considerations....................................................   393
Conclusion..............................................................   414
Theories of Toilet Training....................................   394
References...............................................................   415
Learner-Oriented Toilet Training Methods.........   395
Trainer-Centered Toilet Training Methods.........   396
Readiness Factors...................................................   397
Toilet Training: Behavioral
Trainer Readiness..................................................   402
and Medical Considerations
Training Challenges...............................................   403
Toilet Training Techniques....................................   404 Toilet training is an area of human development
Medical Considerations.........................................   407 where the research has not kept pace with the
abundance of popular-culture information pro-
Medical Specialty Consideration:
Neurological........................................................   412
duced. This is for good reason; the vast majority
of children learn this skill at some point without
P. McPherson clinical intervention, largely negating the need
Northwest Louisiana Human Services District, for research. There is limited evidence to suggest
Shreveport, LA, USA that this might change. Despite the abundance of
C.O. Burns information related to toilet training, there is no
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State universal data driven support for any one method
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA nor has expert consensus established operational
M.J. Garcia (*) definitions such as toileting, toileting success,
Northwest Resource Center, Bossier City, LA, USA and toileting failure (Klassen et al., 2006; Kroeger
e-mail: Mark.Garcia@LA.GOV
& Sorensen-Burnworth, 2009). This limits effi-
V.S. Kothapalli • S.E. McNeil cacy based research as key metrics are not well
Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center,
Shreveport, LA, USA defined; how many voidings constitute successful
toileting and how many constitute failed toileting
T. Thompson
Department of School Psychology, University of (Vermandel, Van Kampen, Van Gorp, &
Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, MS, USA Wyndaele, 2008).

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 393


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9_22
394 P. McPherson et al.

Limits in expert consensus or even operational bers are also at risk for communicable infections
definitions have not hampered the teaching of toi- from the presence of feces (Miura, Watanabe,
leting skills. Yet there are important challenges to Takemoto, & Fukushi, 2016).
the practical application of toileting information. Researchers have studied toilet training and
Most families in developed countries agree that offer insight into the relevant factors related to
toileting is voiding in a commode and achieved the development of this skill. Yet there is vari-
when the learner voids independently although, ability for all learners of this skill for example,
those in developing countries agree that voli- several researchers have studied when toilet
tional control is the end point. Important differ- training should start and found that accepted
ences like this have relevance on practical norms for toilet training is related more too cul-
application such as when a learner is started in tural differences than scientific evidence (Kiddoo,
training or what is considered developmentally 2012). Similarly, research as to the type of train-
appropriate for the learner. Interestingly, several ing to be used found that the type is influenced by
researchers suggest that when parents are search- social, economic, educational, and cultural con-
ing for information regarding toilet training, they siderations within the family (Brazelton et al.,
will seek advice from friends and family mem- 1999). Thus, parents are unclear about teaching
bers, whereas professionals seek advice from toileting or even when to initiate toilet training
books and research (Ritblatt, Obegi, Hammons, (Vermandel et al., 2008) however; researchers
Ganger, & Ganger, 2003). Related to this, Schum have demonstrated that the ideal age to initiate
et al. (2001), cautions that most toileting based toilet training is unique to each individual and the
information and advice is derived from theory family (Brazelton et al., 1999; Kiddoo, 2012).
and common experience rather than resulting Since the variables to learning toileting skills are
from scientific knowledge. This calls attention to unique to each learner it is important to account
the difficulties families and clinicians have sort- for the relevant factors related to learning this
ing through an abundance of information, as it is skill, i.e., the behavioral and medical consider-
difficult to compare scientific studies or deter- ations related to toilet training.
mine what is high quality evidenced based
research.
What is agreed upon is that toileting compe- Theories of Toilet Training
tence is an important milestone in an individual’s
physical, emotional, and social development All individuals require training in order to acquire
(Stadtler, Gorski, & Brazelton, 1999). Failure to toileting skills and the method of training differs
learn successful continence affects daily func- by individual. In 2006, Klassen and colleagues
tioning including the ability to live indepen- published an evidence-based report on toilet
dently, to be included in community activities, training for the Agency for Healthcare Research
and increases the risk of abuse (Cicero & Pfadt, and Quality (AHRQ). Their objective was to
2002). Learning to maintain continence eases the evaluate the effectiveness of various toilet-­
need to purchase diapers, provides an opportu- training methods and the factors related to
nity for the individual to communicate about ­successful toileting. The AHRQ report was the
physical needs, and decreases hygiene risks asso- first published systematic review of toilet train-
ciated with diaper use (Simon & Thompson, ing; unfortunately, meta-analysis and direct com-
2006). Furthermore, it is a skill that is routinely parisons of the various toilet-training methods
followed by pediatricians at wellness checkup were not possible due to critical design differ-
since delaying toilet training increases the risk of ences. In the AHRQ report, four distinct toilet
the individual developing infection or lower uri- training methods were identified (Klassen et al.,
nary tract dysfunction (Barone, Jasutkar, & 2006). They were the child-centered methods of
Schneider, 2009). In addition to the individual’s Brazelton (1962) and Spock (1946) and the
risk of infection, family and community mem- parent-­centered methods of Azrin and Foxx/Foxx
Toilet Training 395

and Azrin (1971, 1973, 1974) and the Early main psychological readiness sign emphasized
Elimination/Elimination Communication meth- by Brazelton is impulse control. He argued that
ods. Both the Brazelton and the Dr. Spock meth- the motivation for this control is also influenced
ods are designed to train typically developing by the desire to please caregivers and to be like
children of about 2-years of age who demonstrate them, as well as the desire to achieve autonomy.
physiological and or psychological readiness. Further, the learner must be able to understand
Brazelton posited that readiness was a vital part some verbal instructions in order to follow the
of training and that lasting problems can occur sequence of actions necessary for successful toi-
from poor experiences with toilet training. The let use (Brazelton).
focus on the parent-centered methods by Azrin Training based on this method typically begins
and Foxx is on autonomous toileting. around 18 months of age, the average age of
The first three methods are expert derived emergence of requisite readiness skills.
training methods based on experience, practice, Specifically, by this time voluntary control over
and research, while the final method, Early bowel and bladder typically has emerged around
Elimination/Elimination Communication, is 9 months, development of necessary motor skills
derived from cultural practices of countries with such as walking begins to emerge around
limited resources. Generally, these types of train- 18 months, and the learner’s capacity to under-
ing are often categorized as child-centered or stand and follow directions related to training
parent-centered approaches however; current begins to occur between 18 and 24 months. In
person-centered norms suggest a need to identify Brazelton’s original study the average age at
these methods as “learner”-centered or “trainer”- completion of daytime toilet training was
centered approaches as adolescents or adults 28.5 months, and the average age of all training
maybe learning these skills and other individuals was 33.3 months (Brazelton, 1962). The focus of
maybe training these skills. Below is a brief learner-centered training is “autonomous
description of each of these four district methods achievement” by the learner, and Brazelton rec-
divided by the orientational focus on learner or ommended that negativity be minimized, as a
trainer, aka child and parent, respectively. learner-oriented training may help individuals to
avoid any negative emotions related to toileting
challenges. This training denoted a shift away
 earner-Oriented Toilet Training
L from earlier and more structure methods by
Methods focusing on physiological and psychological
maturational processes, i.e., readiness, that must
The Brazelton Toilet Training Method be present prior to training. By acknowledging
that training should not commence until these
This learner-oriented approach is most associated prerequisite skills have been met, the trainer
with the work of pediatrician T. Berry Brazelton. focuses more on the learner’s abilities and allows
The idea behind this method is that learning to for a more gradual training approach.
use the toilet is an important developmental task
where learner readiness best ensures mastery of
toileting skills. Training starts when the learner is The Spock Toilet Training Method
“ready” and progresses slowly to promote a
negativity-­free experience. Readiness on the part The Spock Toilet Training Method is also a
of the learner includes both physical and emo- learner-centered toilet training method and is
tional maturation (Brazelton, 1962). Physically, designed to train without force. This approach is
the learner must possess some voluntary control based on pediatrician Benjamin Spock’s (1946)
over the sphincter as well as specific motor devel- book, The Common Sense Book of Baby and
opment (e.g., walking, have some autonomy Child Care. In this method, training for most
regarding getting on and off the toilet, etc.). The children begins at about 24–30 months and the
396 P. McPherson et al.

goal is to provide optimal learning, which occurs pendently, pick up objects easily, and follow
when the learner is ready and trained without instructions (e.g., point to body parts, imitate
force. Here learning occurs later than the simple tasks, or place one object inside another;
Brazelton method but has a similar objective: the Azrin & Foxx, 1974). This training method also
training process is made relaxed and pleasant to includes a pre-training component, training sup-
avoid power struggles and negative emotions. plies, and a particular training setup. Important to
Toileting is presented as a straightforward matter-­ this training protocol are both positive reinforce-
of-­fact/fact-of-life task that is not dirty, shameful, ment and punishment. Positive reinforcement is
secretive, or mysterious. Training begins when given in the form of verbal and edible praise for
the learner decides to gain control over bowel and toileting behaviors. Punishment is used when
bladder; the trainer must be patient and trust the accidents occur. Here a verbal reprimand is given,
learner’s decision to learn. Once training begins, reinforcement is absent, and the learner preforms
the trainer encourages and reinforces the learner, ten “positive practice” training repetitions. A ver-
while avoiding criticism and anger if there is bal reprimand includes a statement of the fact
refusal or accidents (Spock). (e.g., “You wet your pants,” “your pants are
wet,”) and a statement of why the trainer is dis-
pleased (e.g., “Wetting is bad,” “Mommy doesn’t
 rainer-Centered Toilet Training
T like wet pants”; Azrin & Foxx). An example of
Methods positive practice includes the learner observing a
doll, capable of simulating urination, use the
 zrin and Foxx Toilet Training in Less
A potty chair to urinate. Once the doll urinates, the
Than a Day Method learner will manually guide the doll through the
toileting process of emptying the removable res-
The Azrin and Foxx “Toilet Training in Less ervoir into the toilet, flushing, and returning the
Than a Day” method (1974) is designed to teach pot to the chair (Klassen et al., 2006). Additional
toileting without reminders or assistance. This is researchers evaluating modified versions of this
an operant learning method based on Drs. Azrin protocol have reported a substantial degree of
and Foxx’s research with the toilet training of 34 success using this protocol (Ardic & Cavkaytar,
institutionalized adults with intellectual disabil- 2014; Butler, 1976; Hanney, Jostad, LeBlanc,
ity (Azrin & Foxx, 1971). Later research with Carr, & Castile, 2012; Kroeger & Sorensen,
typically developing learners replicated the origi- 2010; LeBlanc, Carr, Crossett, Bennett, &
nal findings with toilet training learned in an Detweiler, 2005; Matson & Ollendick, 1977).
average of 3.9 h and has been successfully
adapted for bowel control (Foxx & Azrin, 1973).
This method is the most commonly researched Early Elimination/Elimination
protocol of all the toilet training methods (Polaha, Communication Toilet Training
Warzak, & Dittmer-McMahon, 2002). Method
Training begins at about 20 months of age and
starts with an assessment of physiological readi- The Early Elimination Toilet Training Method is
ness, which includes bladder control, physical also an operant learning trainer-centered method.
development, and ability to follow instructions. It is designed to promote infant control of elimi-
The learner must have bladder control including nation by the age of 1 year. This method is dis-
the ability to empty the bladder fully without tinct from the other methods in that volitional
dribbling, and to stay dry for several hours. The control is the endpoint and not functional inde-
learner must appear to be aware of the urge to pendence as with the other methods. Independence
urinate and communicate the urge verbally or would not be a reasonable goal due to the infant’s
behaviorally (e.g., facial expression or posture limited neuromuscular development. Use of this
changes). The learner must be able to walk inde- method is typical in developing nations such as
Toilet Training 397

China, Africa, India, and South and Central Readiness Factors


America (Vermandel et al., 2008). In North
America, this method is commonly related to Learner Readiness
Elimination Communication. Choby and George
(2008) note that this method has been developing In 2016, the American Academy of Pediatrics
an appeal in the United States since 2005. (AAP) published a guide to toilet training that
Proponents of this method cite the medical ben- outlined particular readiness factors and the ages
efit of reduced or eliminated diaper rash and later at which they usually emerge. According to these
in life urinary tract infections, financial benefits guidelines, physical, cognitive, motor, emotional,
from no longer buying disposable undergar- and verbal skills are necessary to learning toilet
ments, and ecological benefits of reduced energy skills. Specifically, the physiological and motor
consumption from washing as well as less, non-­ skill associated with the awareness of the need to
biodegradable, disposable diapers in landfills eliminate and the motor milestone of walking
(Kelley, 2005). typically emerges between 12 and 18 months of
Training begins at birth with the trainer age. Followed by the ability for brief control of
learning to identify the elimination patterns of sphincter muscles, begins to develop around
the infant via body movement, muscle tension, 18 months, and the skills necessary to manipulate
facial gestures, and noises, e.g. grunts and cries. clothing, around 24–36 months. Lastly, the matu-
After elimination patterns are identified, at ration of the digestive system (and subsequently
2–3 weeks of age, the trainer will simultane- fewer toileting accidents) develop at 3 years and
ously begin training bowel and bladder control. older (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2016).
Initially, the trainer will assume all responsi­ Milestones in cognitive and verbal develop-
bility by identifying the elimination patterns ment are also relevant to training. Between birth
and placing the infant in the voiding position. In and 12 month infants, begin to understand the
North America, the infant is placed over a sink, relation between cause and effect. By
toilet, or potty-chair. In developing nations, the 12–18 months, toddlers start to relate the feeling
typical location for voiding is anywhere in the associated with the need to eliminate with the act
immediate area but outside of the home. If elim- of elimination. Verbal abilities also begin to
ination occurs, then the infant is reinforced with emerge around this age. Goal-oriented behavior
feeding, physical comfort, or pleasurable activ- necessary for toilet training become more devel-
ity. If no elimination occurs, then the infant is oped between 18 and 24 months, as does the abil-
returned to his or her previous activity. Similar ity to understand language and instructions. The
to the Dr. Spock method, infant toileting is not AAP also mentions the role of improved memory
regarded as private or unclean therefore while at this age. By 36 months of age, children have an
the infant voids, social activity carries on. When enhanced ability to focus and follow through on
accidents occur, a caregiver cleans the infant tasks without becoming sidetracked, which aids
immediately and without comment. By 1 year, in their ability to complete the toileting process
the infant is expected to eliminate away from all (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2016).
living areas: accidents that do occur in the living Finally, emotional and social factors have
area are first given a warning, and later acci- been posited as relevant to successful training.
dents are followed by a physical punishment Young infants begin to enjoy social praise, and
(Klassen et al., 2006). after 12 months, their ability to mimic the behav-
ior of others improves. At 18–24 months, tod-
dlers begin to desire “self-mastery” and further
Toilet Training Comparison Table develop the desire to receive praise and apprecia-
tion from their parents. Toddlers age 24–36 are
Presented below is a comparison table and more internally motivated by competency and
accompanying protocols (Tables 1 and 2). autonomy, and by 36 months become more moti-
398 P. McPherson et al.

Table 1  Toilet training comparisons


Learner-centered toilet training Trainer-centered toilet training
Early elimination/
elimination
Method Brazelton Spock Azrin and Foxx communication
Features Prevent problems Relaxed and Toilet without Controlled elimination
for learner pleasant with fewer reminders or
power struggles assistance
Learning control Toileting is a straight Competence in
forward fact of life communicating the need to
void and assuming position
to void by 4–5 months
Proper timing Toileting is not dirty, When able to walk, learner
shameful, secret, or is expected to
mysterious independently eliminate
Training slowly Avoid negative away from the living areas
Allow for periods of comments to avoid
negativity learner feeling
If challenges, stop criticized
and reassure, but
wait till ready
Goal Readiness Train without force Autonomous toileting Early controlled
elimination
Age of learner Around 18 months Around Around 20 months Around 2–3 weeks
24–30 months
Duration of About 9 months About 9 months About 4 h About 4–6 months
training
Learner Able to sit and walk N/A Walks without N/A
physiological assistance
readiness skills Understand some External sphincter
verbal commands control
External sphincter Can follow 10
control instructions
Picks up objects
easily
Can urinate all at
once (not dribbling)
Stay dry for several
hours
Appears to know
when they are about
to void
Learner Desire to develop Desires to gain N/A N/A
psychological autonomy and control of bowel and
readiness skills self-mastery bladder
Feels secure with
parent figures and
desire to please
them
A wish to identify
with and imitate
important
role-models
(continued)
Toilet Training 399

Table 1 (continued)
Learner-centered toilet training Trainer-centered toilet training
Early elimination/
elimination
Method Brazelton Spock Azrin and Foxx communication
Trainer readiness Ready to deal with Must trust the N/A Parent senses the learner
outside pressures learner’s desire to needs to eliminate by
and anxieties learn continence movements, muscle
tension, and vocalizations
Aiming for a Regards elimination as not
relaxed pressure private or unclean
free approach
Reinforcement/ Positive Positive Positive Positive reinforcement
punishment reinforcement reinforcement reinforcement
method Positive punishment Positive punishment
(verbal reprimand)
Negative punishment
(withhold praise or
preferred items/
activities)
Modeling by other No Yes No Yes
Empty diaper in Yes No Yes N/A
toilet
Address accidents Later, if not No Yes Yes
diurnal/nocturnal spontaneous during
day training
Separate bowel Yes, for males No Yes No
and bladder
training

vated by peer approval (American Academy of Kaerts, Van Hal, Vermandel, and Wyndaele
Pediatrics, 2016). (2012) described similar signs often discussed in
Many of the specific readiness signs and asso- the literature, such as the ability to imitate behav-
ciated age ranges described by the AAP are also ior, sit independently, pick up small objects, con-
consistent with those suggested by Brazelton trol sphincter muscles, understand and respond to
(1962), Azrin and Foxx (1974), and other directions, understand potty words, and shows
researchers. In studies review by Berk and Friman interest in toilet training, among others.
(1990), there was some evidence that prior to Overall, the AAP estimates that, on average,
18 months of age, children are not able to delay children in the United States usually become toi-
urination, and that they may not be able to delay let trained between 24 and 36 months. As previ-
for long periods of time until later ages. The age ously mentioned, the average age at daytime
at which children begin to report the need to uri- toilet training found by Brazelton (1962) was
nate was estimated to be around 24 months. Other 27.7 months, which is consistent with Bloom,
researchers have investigated more specific read- Seeley, Ritchey, and McGuide’ (1993) finding of
iness abilities. For example, Schum et al. (2002) 28 months. Although the age at which different
considered readiness factors such as; “under- readiness skills are achieved varies from child to
stands potty words, shows an interest, tells during child, there seems to be a consensus that certain
or after having a bowel movement, stays dry for physiological and psychological milestones are
two hours, and indicates a physical need to go.” important precursors to toilet training.
400 P. McPherson et al.

Table 2  Protocols summaries


Summary of training protocols
Brazelton protocol
Materials: potty chair
Pre-training: N/A
Training set up: place learner’s potty-chair in vicinity of the trainer’s potty
Procedure:
1. Introduce learner to their potty chair
2. Pair association of learner’s potty chair with trainer’s commode
3. Implement daily clothed potty sits when trainer uses toilet
 Praise learner during sits
4. After 1–2 weeks remove bottom clothes during sits
5. Empty diaper contents into potty demonstrating to learners where urine/feces go
6. Introduce scheduled guided potty sits throughout the day
 Base this schedule upon times learners soils diaper
 Praise potty use
7. After successful guided potty sits, encourage independent use by providing verbal prompts using the previous
schedule to go sit on the potty
 Praise compliance with sits
For males, if desired, once bowel/bladder control is demonstrated to be successful an appropriate male model should
demonstrate standing to urinateAdapted from Brazelton (1962) and Brazelton et al. (1999)
Spock protocol
Materials: potty chair, step stool, hand soap (for washing hands), and preferred items available near potty
Pre-training: watches others with no expectation to imitate, teach to wash hands, teach the toileting words (potty,
toilet, urine, feces, pee-pee, poo-poo, etc.)
Training set up: N/A
Procedure:
1. Learner sits on the potty fully clothed and chooses when to get up
2. Once the learner accepts the seat, suggest to use it for bowel movements like role models
3. Allow learner to leave potty chair anytime
4. Do not urge or pressure the learner if unwilling
5. Once interest is shown take the learner to the potty 2–3 times per day or when signals are present
6. Praise for being dry like role-model but do not over praise
7. When ready to be more independent, remove all lower clothing and place the potty nearby
8. Explain that the toilet can be used by self and may give occasional reminders
9. If accident, show how to put in the potty and explain this is where they should void
 Do not flush while the learner is watching
 Put the learner back in diapers
 Do not scold the learner
Once control is achieved teach wiping Adapted from Spock (1946)
Azrin and Foxx protocol
Materials: potty chair, preferred items including snacks, doll model that wets, list of persons (real or fictional) the
learner admires, at least 8 pairs of training pants, clothing that will not interfere with training pants and toileting,
and training reminder sheet
Pre-training: teach learner to assist in his/her own dressing and undressing, model toileting and explain the steps,
teach the toileting words, and teach compliance when given instructions
Training set up: conduct training in one room and remove any distracting items
Procedure:
1. Learner wears regular underpants
2. Tell learner how happy (role-model) will be that the learner is learning to use the potty and to keep pants dry
(continued)
Toilet Training 401

Table 2 (continued)
3. Take learner to bathroom in intervals
4. Only give reinforcement for correct toileting skills
5. Do not reinforce non-toileting acts
6. For accidents, deliver verbal reprimand, omit reinforcement, have learner change wet clothes by self, and deliver
positive practice
7. Use doll that wets to imitate the processes of toileting
8. Teach specific steps; manually guide learner, then learner guides the doll through the steps
9. When doll urinates in potty, learner will empty potty into the toilet, flush and return the potty to the chair
10. Once this is learned begin training learner
11. Teach to check for dry pants
12. Reward/praise dry pants
13. Check pants every 3–5 min using a training reminder sheet
14. Give as much to drink as desired to create an urge to urinate
15. Every 15 min learner walks to potty, lower pants, sit down for 10 min, stand up, and raise pants
 If urination begins, then praise/reward immediately
 Learner wipes self and empties potty
16. Decrease frequency of potty visits as learner acquires skill
17. Learner and parent check for “dry pants” every 5 min
18. Gradually change from directing “go potty” to asking “go potty?” to “are your pants dry?”
19. As steps are successful, only reinforce at the end of the routine
20. Fade to praising only dry pants
21. Do checks at meals, naps, bedtimes, and praise dry pants
22. No reminders to toilet are given
 If there is an accident, learner is reprimanded, changes by self and performs positive practice sessionsAdapted
from Azrin and Foxx/Foxx and Azrin (1971, 1973, & 1974)
Early elimination communication (EEC)
Materials: none
Pre-training: none
Training set up: none
Procedure:
1. Urination and defecation are trained separately
2. Urination
 a. After feeding and waking infant sits on trainer’s legs facing away from trainer
 b. Trainer makes “shuss” sound
 c. Do frequently over 24 h
 d. When successful provide reinforcement
3. Defecation
 a. Infant sits on trainers feet, facing trainer, leaning on trainer’s shins
 b. No prompts or sounds given
 c. When signaled, the trainer may gently pat the buttocks to relax the sphincter reflex
 d. When successful provide reinforcement
 e. When not successful return learner back to activity with minimal attention
 f. At 3–5 months female family members, 5–12 years old, participate in identifying the infants signals to void and
models the elimination position
 i. If accident, warn first, subsequent accidents use physical punishmentAdapted from deVries and deVries (1977)
and Gross-Loh, (2007) 
402 P. McPherson et al.

Trainer Readiness A ready trainer will need competence and


will need to feel confident in their ability to
Trainer readiness is also a necessary factor to train toileting skills, as the outcome in chil-
successful toilet training. This is especially appli- dren’s growth and development is dependent
cable for trainers using a trainer-centered on parent knowledge and confidence (Coleman
approach that requires significant trainer involve- & Karraker, 1998). For parents, knowledge and
ment (i.e., teaching a learner prior to interest and confidence are important determinants of their
aptitude being evidenced; Vermandel et al., likelihood to undertake new actions, their ideas
2008). Parental readiness for North American about the kinds of activities they might under-
parents is important because in despite of the take, and their motivation to engage in new
increased prominence of childcare providers activities (Green, Walker, Hoover-Dempsey, &
teaching this skill, parents and mostly mothers Sandler, 2007). Researchers have linked paren-
still play a role in training their child toileting tal self-efficacy or confidence to child devel-
skills (Klassen et al., 2006). Typically, the focus opmental outcomes (Coleman & Karraker; De
of readiness is on the learner and the trainer’s Montigny & Lacharité, 2005; Jones & Prinz,
readiness is assumed. This is not typically a prob- 2005). Bandura (1993) reported that parents with
lem because when toilet training goes well, the a strong perception of self-efficacy put much
parent trainer is not met with overwhelming chal- effort, perseverance, and persistence into the
lenges and they are able to persist to success. tasks associated with parenting their children. In
Unfortunately, when challenges do occur and turn, parents who are responsive to communica-
things do not go smoothly, readiness bears a con- tive behavior, provide enriched environments,
siderable portion of the fault. support the child’s freedom for exploration, and
Readiness for parents is specifically related facilitate varied mastery experiences help the
to the timing of training and the competence development of their child’s sense of self-effi-
of the trainers. The timing of training is impor- cacy (Bandura, 1993).
tant and is affected by the trainers’ familial and De De Montigny and Lacharité (2005) identi-
extrafamilial obligations. A significant determi- fied factors that influence parent trainer self-­
nant of parental readiness is determining when efficacy. In their review of the literature, they
it is a good time in the family’s life to begin found that parent trainer’s self-efficacy is affected
training. Generally, stable and low stress peri- by; (1) previous childcare experiences prior to
ods of the family’s life are ideal. It is not recom- parenting, (2) opportunities for observation and
mended that training begin while the family is modeling of experts via parent training programs,
experiencing a major life stressor such as new and (3) meaningful social support with positive
sibling, new home, new school, divorce, poor reinforcement. Parent trainer self-efficacy can
health, or financial stress. Likewise, readiness also be diminished. Factors that disrupt parent
is influenced by the competence of the trainer. trainer’s sense of self-efficacy or confidence dur-
A significant determinant of parental readiness ing toilet training include both personality traits
and thus competence is knowledge and confi- and behaviors as well as factors related to the
dence. Often these are assumed, however there learner such as illness, personality trait, idiosyn-
is a great deal of information related to toilet cratic behaviors, and disability. Gross and Fogg
training with little direction to help the trainer to (2001) add that parent trainer self-efficacy can be
choose a method given that there is no universal developed by training opportunities such as pro-
data driven support for any one method (Klassen viding information, verbal support, reinforce-
et al., 2006; Kroeger & Sorensen-Burnworth, ment, and vicarious learning i.e., watching others
2009). This leaves families susceptible to con- and discussing what was observed. De Montigny
fusion and frustration from the conflicting and Lacharité also recommended that future
advice of friends, relatives, and popular media study should focus on enhancing parent trainer
(Vermandel et al., 2008). self-efficacy by providing anticipatory guidance,
Toilet Training 403

offering training program on parenting roles, this population include impaired communication
offering opportunities to model, role-play, and skills, reduced ability for mobility, and difficulty
practice skills. with changes in routine, or introduction to unfa-
miliar surroundings (i.e. the bathroom). Klassen
et al. (2006) cite communication delays, sensory
Training Challenges process difficulties, sensitivity to stimulation,
limited ability to imitate, compromised motor
Much of the literature on toilet training assumes planning, and preference for routine.
the learner will have typical development with Behavioral challenges can create difficulty
only minor behavior challenges. Of course, these when toilet training. As with autism spectrum
are not the learners mental health professionals disorder (ASD), sometimes behavioral chal-
encounter. Trainers may seek professional guid- lenges are present when the learner has other dis-
ance when the learner has physical and or devel- orders such as fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS),
opmental challenges. For example, neurological, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) or attention
gastrointestinal, or urological difficulties such as deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Wolraich
constipation, diarrhea, enuresis, encopresis, (2016), explains that individuals with these disor-
among others, may interfere with training ders may present with poor motivation or poor
(Issenman, Filmer, & Gorski, 1999). Other learn- ability to respond to the reinforcements that help
ers may have behavior challenges such as non- the majority of learners to tolerate raining activi-
compliance, aggression, or even self-injury. ties such as frequent doffing clothing. The physi-
Individuals with intellectual disability and devel- cal closeness of the trainer can be problematic for
opmental disability (ID/DD) may have cognitive, some of these individuals. Additionally, learners
motor, or sensory impairments that make attain- with behavioral challenges may have problems
ment of readiness skills challenging and there- related to the limited understanding of the
fore subsequent toileting success challenging. abstract concept of using the commode, the ben-
efits of using the commode, and or difficulty imi-
tating the trainer.
 earners with Sensory and Behavioral
L The vast majority of individuals do learn
Challenges ­continence; this even includes individuals with
ID/DD. Schonwald, Sherritt, Stadtler, and
Persons with sensory perception deficits or Bridgemohan (2004) point out that individuals
behavioral challenges may have difficulties with with ID/DD are capable of acquiring skills that
successful toilet training. Most problematic is others do, just at a slower rate. Most individuals
failure to register or process the urge to urinate or with physical disability also achieve continence.
defecate in a timely manner. Such sensory chal- Those who have disabilities that directly affect
lenges have been most commonly reported in their physical ability to independently toilet may
individuals with ASD. Luiselli (1997), suggested require occupational therapy and toilet modifica-
that fear and anxiety is associated with voiding in tions. Still, professionals will encounter some
the toilet. However, Keen, Brannigan, and individuals who have the potential for mastery
Cuskelly (2007), explain that some learners with but encounter challenges with learning this skill
ASD may not understand the sensory input of the set. Yet, the diagnosis of disability does not nec-
urge to void and this leads to delays or failure to essarily include deficits in toileting. It merely
acquire toileting skills. Others with ASD can be leaves individuals exposed to this skill deficit.
more sensitive to touch, uncomfortable with the Despite these challenges common for individuals
physical proximity of the trainer, or simply resist with, which can accompany disorder like, ID/
the frequently donning and doffing of clothing DD, ASD, FAS, ODD, and ADHD training can
required for training. Additional challenges for usually be accomplished.
404 P. McPherson et al.

Toilet Training Techniques and rapid fading of prompts to sustain indepen-


dent voiding (Kroeger & Sorensen-Burnworth).
Pre-training Requirements Reinforcement-based training is also used fre-
quently. Positive reinforcement was an integral
As previously discussed, certain prerequisite part of the original Foxx and Azrin studies (Azrin
skills are required before, toilet training can be & Foxx, 1971; Foxx & Azrin, 1973). Presently,
effective. Azrin and Foxx (1971) indicated that researchers are beginning to use negative rein-
some bladder control is necessary for training forcement as an alternative to punishment pro-
toileting skills as it includes both the ability to cedures such as restitutional overcorrection. One
recognize the need for voiding and the ability to such technique is response restriction or response
maintain continence to some degree. Lott and deprivation (see Ardic & Cavkaytar, 2014; van
Kroeger (2004) state that the individuals must be Oorsouw, Duker, Melein, & Averink, 2009). Here
able to recognize the sensation to eliminate the individual is permitted to perform only one
before they can accomplish the entire chain of behavior, such as sitting on the toilet, and all other
toileting behaviors. Similarly, Schonwald et al. behaviors are blocked. Punishment procedures
(2004) suggested that the learner must be (1) are also used and the most common punishment
aware of the urge to void, (2) have the ability to procedure used with toilet training is overcor-
maintain continence, (3) communicate the need rection, i.e., positive practice and restitutional
to void when assistance is needed, (4) relax the overcorrection. These include either practicing
appropriate muscle groups while sitting on the toileting behaviors in sequence or cleaning after
toilet, (5) be aware of when the bowel or bladder each accident or inappropriate voiding. However,
is empty, in addition to (6) knowing how to don several researchers have questioned the utility of
and doff pants and appropriate hygiene. physical punishment versus verbal punishment
such as reprimands (e.g., Cicero & Pfadt, 2002).
Kroeger and Sorensen-Burnworth make the
Empirically Driven Techniques point that some current researchers report ver-
bal reprimands as “corrective feedback” and are
Researchers and clinicians have advanced sev- not classifying these reprimands as punishment.
eral techniques for teaching toileting. Kroeger Scheduled sits are used as either predetermined
and Sorensen-Burnworth (2009) reviewed the schedules for sitting on the toilet based on the
literature of training protocols modeled after likelihood of voiding or regular intervals where
the original Foxx and Azrin method and report the learner sits on the toilet followed by positive
on nine techniques most used with individuals reinforcement if voiding occurs which completes
with ID/DD and ASD, the first seven are based the scheduled sit (e.g., Bainbridge & Myles, 1999;
on Azrin and Foxx; (1) graduated guidance, (2) Kroeger & Sorensen, 2010; LeBlanc et al., 2005).
reinforcement-­based training, (3) punishment Elimination schedules are another technique pro-
procedures, (4) scheduled sittings, (5) elimi- moted by Azrin and Foxx designed to predict
nation schedules, (6) hydration, (7) nighttime when the learner will void so that timely training
training for diurnal continence, (8) priming and can occur. This is achieved by either a mechani-
video modeling, and (9) manipulation of stimu- cal or manual detection method using a moister
lus control. Graduated guidance is hierarchal detection device or manual dryness check (e.g.,
prompting and is among the most commonly LeBlanc et al., 2005; Luiselli, 1994). Hydration
used techniques in toilet training protocols. The and increasing fluid intake is a technique also
Azrin and Foxx method is an oft cited refer- used by Azrin and Foxx and is used to increase
ence to this behavioral shaping technique and the likelihood the learner will void (Azrin &
most training protocols use a combination of Foxx, 1971; Cicero & Pfadt, 2002). An impor-
prompting hierarchies, least restrictive prompts, tant caveat that Kroeger and Sorensen-Burnworth
Toilet Training 405

cite is the risk of water toxicity where excessive Specifically, Taylor, Cipani, and Clardy (1994)
water intake can lead to hyponatremia, an imbal- explain that disposable undergarments develop
ance of electrolytes, and more importantly, they stimulus control over the elimination urge and
indicate a need to consider medical contraindica- commodes inhibit voiding because of its dissimi-
tions. Lastly, Kroeger and Sorensen-Burnworth lar stimulus attributes. Manipulation or transfer
included a reference to nighttime training for of control from the disposable undergarments to
diurnal continence based on the Saloviita (2000) the commodes is made via use of behavioral
case report of spontaneous generalization from techniques, i.e. shaping, fading, response preven-
nocturnal to diurnal training. Saloviita attempted tion, and negative reinforcement. Kroeger and
to treat nocturnal enuresis for a woman with Sorensen-Burnworth cite that since the original
profound ID using the Azrin, Sneed, and Foxx work by Azrin and Foxx/Foxx and Azrin more
(1973) Dry Bed protocol. While this was inef- recent literature focuses on intervention designs
fective for nocturnal enuresis, diurnal continence that include both the core basics of the their pro-
was gained. As an explanation, Saloviita noted tocols and additional or varied training compo-
that Dry Bed training included training for inde- nents (see Dalrymple & Angrist, 1988; Hagopian,
pendent use of the toilet, practice for prolonged Fisher, Piazza, & Wierzbicki, 1993; Luiselli,
continence, and punishment for accidents, which 1997; Taylor et al., 1994).
is believed to have generalized to daytime dry-
ness. In all, no other researchers have influenced
the field of toilet training as much as the Foxx Punishment Procedures
and Azrin studies (Azrin & Foxx, 1971; Foxx &
Azrin, 1973). As mentioned above, there is growing concern
Priming and video modeling and manipula- with the use of punishment procedures. While
tion of stimulus control are two additional tech- these procedures have been shown to be effective
niques that Kroeger and Sorensen-Burnworth the current philosophical zeitgeist, research, and
(2009) included in their review. Priming and practice for ID/DD populations focuses on posi-
video modeling is described as an empirically tive behavioral interventions and consequently,
evidenced cognitive-behavioral method of prim- overcorrection and punishment procedures have
ing behavior. Bainbridge and Myles (1999) are fallen out of use (Kroeger & Sorensen-Burnworth,
among the first to use video for toilet training and 2009). Several researchers have indicated punish-
report increased initiation, successful voids in the ment procedures such as restitutional overcorrec-
toilet, along with decreased incontinence. Later tion are difficult for caregivers to implement
researchers report that custom-made videos are effectively (Cicero & Pfadt, 2002; Kırcaali-İftar,
more effective than commercial made videos Ülke-Kürkçüoğlu, Çetin, & Ünlü, 2009; Post &
(Palechka & McDonald, 2010; Rosenberg, Kirkpatrick, 2004). Brazelton et al. (1999) sug-
Schwartz, & Davis, 2010). The model demon- gest that because punishment is contingent on a
strating the task to be imitated can be adult mod- toileting accident and the delivery of a conse-
els, peer models, a self-model, a point-of-view quence to the learner there is a potential for phys-
perspective (McLay, Carnett, van der Meer, & ical abuse to the learner if the amateur trainer is
Lang, 2015), or animated models (Drysdale, Lee, unsupervised. For this reason, research has been
Anderson, & Moore, 2015). Manipulation of conducted using modified procedures with
stimulus control is a technique used in conjunc- greater social validity such as removing overcor-
tion with traditional protocols, e.g., graduated rection (Cicero & Pfadt; Kroeger & Sorensen,
guidance, scheduled sitting, and reinforcement 2010), including fading procedures (Cicero &
for appropriate voiding where toileting skill is Pfadt, 2002), priming and video modeling
trained by first changing the reinforced stimulus (Bainbridge & Myles, 1999), and manipulation
to be one related to successful toileting. of stimulus control (Taylor et al., 1994).
406 P. McPherson et al.

 iaper and Disposable-­
D Clearly, there is a need to provide services for
Undergarments Use individuals with disabilities who are not able to
void in a commode. The typical means of follow-
Families and providers of care to adults rely on ing up on toileting concerns use frequency
freedom of diapers and disposable-­undergarments counts, ratings of stool consistency, and medical
to facilitate daily functioning, mobility, and par- referral when the concern becomes a serious
ticipation in social activities (Simon & Thompson, medical condition. A formalized assessment may
2006). However, clinicians cite several adverse be attempted with the Adaptive Behavior Scales,
effects to long-term diaper and disposable-­ 2nd edition (VABS-II; Sparrow, Cicchetti, &
undergarment usage for all individuals including Balla, 2005), using the four items related to toi-
foregoing toilet training, extending the use of leting contained in the larger section of personal
diapers or disposable-undergarments, diminish- self-help. However, no normative information
ing personal hygiene, decreasing self-confidence, can be provided about an individual’s toileting
weakening bladder control, as well as increasing behavior using this method.
physical discomfort, stigmatism, and creating a To provide a formalized assessment Matson,
barrier to participating in community life (Cicero Dempsey, and Fodstad (2010) developed the
& Pfadt, 2002; Joinson et al., 2009; Kroeger & Profile of Toileting Issues (POTI). The POTI is a
Sorensen-Burnworth, 2009; Tarbox, Williams, & 56-item checklist used for three goals. To screen
Friman, 2004). For individuals with extended for toileting issues, to identify potential functions
diaper and disposable-undergarment usage, there of toileting challenges, and to help target treat-
is the concern that the diaper or disposable-­ ment in populations of individuals with ID from
undergarments will become a discriminative age 4 years through adulthood. It allows the care-
stimulus for urination and may create toilet train- giver to report on the diagnostic criteria for
ing resistance. Researchers have found that indi- enuresis and encopresis as well as potential func-
viduals with ASD are at risk for the diaper and tions for toileting accidents, toileting consistency,
disposable-undergarment gaining stimulus con- and related challenging behaviors (i.e., pain,
trol over the urge to urinate (Tarbox et al., 2004). avoidance, social difficulties, non-compliance,
This interferes with training as the learner with internal cues, shame/deception, peer rejection,
ASD may avoid using a toilet in favor of waiting aversive parenting, and medical problems).
to don the diaper or disposable-undergarment in Higher scores on this checklist indicate greater
order to void. toileting challenges. Examining the utility of the
POTI, Matson, Neal, Hess, and Kozlowski (2011)
found that individuals who used no verbal com-
Assessment of Toilet Training munication scored significantly higher POTI
Concerns scores than those who used verbal skills.
Similarly, Belva et al. (2011) found that toileting
Research on toileting training assessments for challenge and adaptive function are inversely
individuals with developmental disabilities is related for individuals with ID. Specifically, com-
needed, yet lacking (Matson, Horovitz, & Sipes, paring scores on the VABS-II, they concluded
2011). The need to address toileting concerns for that higher adaptive functioning is associated
individuals with disabilities has been established with significantly fewer toileting problems.
without question. Individuals with disabilities Matson, Neal, et al. (2011) reported that the POTI
may experience challenges with toileting and checklist has sound internal consistency and reli-
incontinence into their adult years (Matson & ability, with a Cronbach alpha coefficient of 0.83
LoVullo, 2009). Additionally, individuals with and interrater reliability of r = 0.44. Matson et al.
greater developmental challenges have signifi- (2010) have been the first researchers to system-
cantly more voiding related challenges (Belva, atically identify toileting skill deficits in this pop-
Matson, Barker, Shoemaker, & Mahan, 2011). ulation and urge further research in this area.
Toilet Training 407

Medical Considerations Lee, & Hsieh, 2002). Persons with genetic disor-
ders have increased risk of gastrointestinal
Medical Consultation defects including anorectal malformations
(ARM) and Hirschsprung’s Disease (Gariepy,
Toilet training may be complicated by medical 2004). The ARMs are associated with high rates
conditions (Brazelton et al., 1999; von Gontard, of constipation and fecal incontinence, and to a
2013). While basic toilet-readiness guide- lesser degree urinary incontinence (Springford,
lines are universal, persons with congenital or Connor, Jones, Kapetanakis, & Giuliani, 2016).
acquired physical anomalies, neurological con- Hamid and colleagues reported 44% of persons
ditions, sensory issues, or chronic illness may with anorectal defects had a urological anomaly
require individualized training strategies and/ and post-repair 66% experienced soiling and
or environmental accommodations to master 30% urinary incontinence (Hamid, Holland, &
toilet training. Increased incidence of inconti- Martin, 2007). These examples highlight the
nence is common in many genetic disorders, importance of medical consultation.
including Rett, Angelman, Fragile-X, Prader-
Willi, Noonan, and Williams syndromes (von
Gontard et al., 2016). Successful toilet training Seeking Medical Consultation
requires goals informed by realistic expectations
that consider developmental and physical needs. Challenges with toilet training or the loss of skills
Consultation with primary care providers and in persons who have mastered toileting may
medical specialists may be necessary to evaluate prompt a medical referral. Painful urination,
fully the toilet-readiness of persons with medical incontinence during laughter, incontinence while
conditions. Occupational and physical therapists running to the toilet, and persistently damp under-
may offer valuable direction for specific accom- wear may all be signs that a medical cause is pres-
modations (AAP, 2016). ent. In addition, diarrhea and/or constipation
The etiology of an individual’s developmental during toilet training should be worked up in the
disability may alert the trainer to specific training medical setting (Schmitt, 2004). Between 2% and
challenges. With prenatal screening, newborn 3% of children have problems with toilet training
assessments and well-child pediatric evaluations and this difficulty is associated with certain traits.
many children with developmental disabilities Children with training difficulties are often “less
will have undergone assessment before they adaptable, have a more negative mood, and are
reach the age of 2–3; the average age of toilet less persistent than easy-to-train children” (Choby
training in the United States (Wolraich & & George, 2008). Trainers should document spe-
American Academy of Pediatrics, 2016). cific details of challenges including the longest
Cerebral palsy, urinary tract birth defects, and period of bladder and bowel continence during
gastrointestinal defects are often identified in wake and sleep, all medical conditions and sen-
infancy. Urinary tract birth defects occur in sory issues, family history of illness and toileting
approximately 1% of newborns and may involve challenges, and interventions attempted. A bath-
the kidney, ureters, bladder, and external genita- room journal documenting frequency of urination
lia (Rasouly & Lu, 2013). Such defects are more and bowel movements as well as associated pain
common in persons with developmental disor- or behavioral issues before, during, and/or after
ders. For example, persons with Down’s syn- will be valuable to medical caregivers (Wolraich
drome are at increased risk for urogenital defects & American Academy of Pediatrics, 2016). A
leading to dysfunctional voiding and urinary description of the volume, color, and odor of urine
retention (Mercer et al., 2004). In a study of the and feces should be included as well. Lastly, toilet
most common congenital defect of the penis, trainers should attend medical appointments if
hypospadias, 5.9% of children were found to possible in order to communicate details of train-
have developmental delay (Wu, Chuang, Ting, ing methods and challenges.
408 P. McPherson et al.

The Initial Medical Evaluation  edical Specialty Consideration:


M
Urology
The initial medical evaluation of toileting chal-
lenges includes a detailed history and physical As a learner matures from infancy into child-
examination. For many children with develop- hood, voluntary bladder control and urinary con-
mental disabilities treatment history will include tinence is slowly achieved. Daytime urinary
surgeries, chronic disease management, genetic continence is usually achieved by age 4 and
consultation, and psychological assessment as nighttime continence is usually achieved between
well as speech, occupational and physical thera- the ages of 5 and 7 (Jansson, Hanson, Sillen, &
pies. Based on the history and physical findings, Hellstrom, 2005). A problem with the filling and/
blood tests, urinalysis, stool analysis, and imag- or emptying of the bladder after the age of five is
ing studies may be ordered. Trainers should pre- referred to as lower urinary tract dysfunction
pare learners for the physical examination and (Austin et al., 2014) and can be a challenge to
tests. For example, the medical caregiver will toilet training or to a previously toilet trained
visually inspect and touch the perineal area, individual. Lower urinary tract dysfunction
palpate the abdomen, and if indicated perform symptoms are a common problem in childhood,
a rectal examination. Urinalysis may require accounting for up to 40% of outpatient urological
catheterization. Trainers and medical caregivers visits (Farhat et al., 2000). Bladder and bowel
may need to consider the possibility that some dysfunction (BBD) is a term used to describe a
learners may not be able to tolerate the exami- combined problem in bowel and bladder func-
nation and will require sedation for a successful tion. The International Children’s Continence
physical examination. After the initial evalua- Society (ICCS), an international organization of
tion, referral for specialty evaluations may be leading experts in the treatment of BBD, have
considered. subcategorized BBD into lower urinary tract dys-
function and bowel dysfunction (Austin et al.).
The ICCS has specifically defined several
Medical Specialty Evaluations symptoms associated with lower urinary tract
dysfunction encompassing frequency and tempo-
Learners with neurological, gastrointestinal, or ral parameters as well as effort and sensation
urological conditions identified in infancy may (Austin et al., 2014). For example, according to
have an established relationship with a medical the ICCS, urinary incontinence refers to the invol-
specialist prior to toilet training. If this is not untary leakage of urine and the ICCS has further
the case, a referral from the primary medical classified urinary incontinence into the major cat-
caregiver will be necessary. A release should be egories of daytime incontinence and nighttime
signed to provide the specialist with the records incontinence or enuresis (Austin et al.). Awareness
of previous assessments, laboratory studies, and of ICCS terminology will allow the trainers to
imaging studies. Trainers should provide the communicate more effectively with medical spe-
bathroom journal along with a list of questions cialists. For reference, common terminologies are
to the specialist prior to the appointment. As summarized in the chart below (Table 3).
with the initial evaluation, the trainer should pre-
pare the learner for the appointment and attend
if possible. Trainers will find that the vocabu-  rology and Developmental
U
lary of medical specialists extends far beyond Disability
the “enuresis” and “encopresis” of the DSM5
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013). While Daytime urinary incontinence and enuresis are
clearly not exhaustive, an introduction medical common bladder dysfunctions among individuals
specialty evaluation is offered below. with severe intellectual or motor disability and
Toilet Training 409

Table 3  ICCS common voiding terminology Carson, and Malone (2007) it was found that
ICCS voiding terminology bladder dysfunction was present in 77% of study
Term Definition subjects with Down syndrome. In another study
Increased Voiding eight or more times per day about toilet training and voiding habits among
daytime children with Down syndrome, it was found that
frequency
learners with Down syndrome tend to have an
Decreased Voiding three times or less per day
voiding increased prevalence of incontinence after toilet
frequency training when compared with a control group
Nocturia Waking up at night to void (Powers et al., 2015). In those with developmen-
Urgency The need to void suddenly and tal disability, urinary incontinence is associated
unexpectedly with medical complications so it is essential that
Hesitancy A problem initiating the process of bladder dysfunction be properly addressed and
voiding
treated and efforts to achieve successful toileting
Straining The need to increase intra-abdominal
pressure in order to initiate or continence (Laecke et al.).
maintain the voiding process While persons with birth defects or metabolic
Weak stream When the stream of urine is of low disorders may be referred for urological evalua-
force during voiding tion prior to toilet training, referrals for bladder
Intermittency The process of voiding when it dysfunction are considered when a school age
occurs in discrete bursts rather than a
continuous flow of urine
child has daytime urinary incontinence or other
Dysuria A sensation of burning or discomfort
persistent urinary symptoms or when these symp-
during voiding toms occur in a previously toilet trained individ-
Holding Observable physical posturing ual. In addition to the previously described
maneuvers behaviors used in order to postpone symptoms of bladder dysfunction, which can be
voiding or to suppress urgency associated with urinary incontinence, several
Post-micturition The involuntary leakage of urine
medical conditions can cause daytime inconti-
dribble after completion of voiding
Urinary The sensation of being unable to
nence, including overactive bladder, underactive
retention void despite a feeling of a full bladder, voiding postponement and dysfunctional
bladder or a feeling of incomplete voiding (Austin et al., 2014).
emptying with the sensation that the
bladder does not feel empty despite
completion of voiding
Spraying/ When urine flow sprays or splits Common Urological Dysfunction
splitting of the during voiding, rather than flowing
urinary stream in a discrete stream Overactive bladder (OAB) is a common blad-
Adapted from Austin et al. (2014) der dysfunction disorder resulting from abnor-
mal bladder contraction while the bladder is
filling. OAB is especially associated with the
are often associated with small bladder capacity symptom of urgency and also characterized by
(Laecke et al., 2001). Inadequate fluid intake is incontinence, frequency and holding maneuvers
associated with poor bladder capacity and consti- (Austin et al., 2014). Underactive bladder is
pation and therefore, proper fluid intake is an associated with a low frequency of voiding and
important consideration for learners with devel- straining during voiding (Nevéus et al., 2006).
opmental disability experiencing urinary inconti- Voiding postponement is used to describe the
nence (Laecke, Raes, Walle, & Hoebeke, 2009). behavior of intentionally delaying voiding and
It is also theorized that the pathophysiology of holding maneuvers are commonly observed.
daytime incontinence and enuresis may be linked Postponement of voiding can result in overflow
to detrusor over activity, which can be very com- incontinence due to an overly distended bladder
mon among individuals with an intellectual dis- (Austin et al.). Dysfunctional voiding refers to an
ability (Laecke et al.). In a study by Hicks, abnormality during the voiding process that can
410 P. McPherson et al.

cause problems with urinary flow and may be tive in reducing daytime incontinence in 45% of
associated with incomplete bladder emptying and study participants (Allen, Austin, Boyt, Hawtrey,
urinary tract infection (Chase, Austin, Hoebeke, & Cooper, 2007). Another study that utilized
& Mckenna, 2010). conservative measures such as timed voiding,
Vaginal reflux occurs when urine is temporar- regulation of fluid intake, pelvic floor exercises,
ily retained in the vagina during the process of and positive reinforcement in treating daytime
voiding and can result in urine leakage as well as urinary incontinence found that 74% of partici-
irritation of skin (Nevéus et al., 2006). Treatment pants had improvement of symptoms in the first
typically consists of altering leg placement and year after therapy and over the long term, 59.4%
posture during voiding and an estrogen cream had improved daytime urinary control (Wiener
can be used to treat labial adhesions (Bernasconi et al., 2000). Education about ideal voiding pos-
et al., 2009). ture, discouragement of holding maneuvers,
Urologists will identify the underlying abnor- maintaining a bathroom journal, and utilizing a
mality causing bladder dysfunction and initiate reward system for the trainee are all-important
proper treatment. While trainers may seek an end components of voiding behavior modification.
to urinary incontinence, the urologist evaluates This approach may be more effective for older
the potential for bladder dysfunction to cause children between the ages of 9 and 12 whom are
kidney damage (Hicks et al., 2007). Routine lab- more receptive to instruction and more acutely
oratory work may include urinalysis and urine aware of social stigma associated with urinary
culture. Urologic imaging studies (ultrasound, incontinence (Heilenkötter et al., 2006). In learn-
MRI, and or voiding cystourethrogram) may be ers with developmental disability, proper fluid
ordered to detect possible anatomic and neuro- intake, healthy diet, and environmental supports
logical abnormalities (Hoebeke, Bower, Combs, take on added significance when implementing
Jong, & Yang, 2010; Wraige, 2002). In addition, behavioral modifications. Learners who have
the urologist may order urinary flow measure- developmental disabilities may have a delayed
ments and urodynamic studies (Nevéus et al., response to sensory stimuli like the sensation of
2006). Trainers should alert the urologist to full bladder so environmental supports such as
trainee characteristics that may interfere with the access to the toilet as well as adaptations that aid
evaluation. stable positioning on the toilet are important fac-
tors in the achievement of continence (Laecke
et al., 2009).
Urology and Conservative Measures

The management of bladder dysfunction aims to Urology and Constipation


improve symptoms and prevent damage to kid-
neys. With the goal of achieving normal voiding Considering the strong association between blad-
habits, management typically begins with con- der and bowel function, an important consider-
servative measures and behavioral modification ation in addressing bladder dysfunction is the
(Thom, Campigotto, Vemulakonda, Coplen, & treatment of constipation. One particular study
Austin, 2012) and progresses as warranted to found that constipation was present in 30% of
more directed approaches such as pharmacologic individuals with symptoms of bladder dysfunc-
intervention and biofeedback therapy. tion (Schulman, Quinn, Plachter, & Kodman-­
Voiding behavior modification is a conserva- Jones, 1999). Another study showed an 89%
tive approach that focuses on trainee and family improvement of daytime incontinence after treat-
education on normal voiding while also facilitat- ment of constipation (Chase et al., 2010).
ing behavioral interventions to improve voiding Additionally, in a study of urinary incontinence
habits. In one study, utilizing a voiding schedule experienced by individuals with developmental
with fixed voiding times was found to be effec- disability it was found that 27% of participants
Toilet Training 411

also had constipation (Laecke ​ et al., 2009). antagonists are also used to treat bladder dys-
Laxative use has been found to be helpful in the function in children and although there is data to
treatment of constipation and in one particular suggest their efficacy (Thom et al.). However,
study of individuals with BBD, treatment with these agents are not currently FDA approved to
polyethylene glycol resulted in improvement of treat bladder dysfunction in children.
constipation and daytime urinary continence
(Pashankar & Bishop, 2001). However, the use of
laxatives, enemas, or suppositories should not be Nocturnal Enuresis
initiated without medical consultation. Hygiene
education is also helpful in preventing bladder Enuresis is a common problem in childhood and
infections that can contribute to toileting chal- is defined as episodic urinary incontinence dur-
lenges. Trainers should consider dietary contri- ing sleep in an individual aged 5 or older (Franco,
bution to toileting challenges. Avoidance of foods von Gontard, & Gennaro, 2013). As mentioned
that can increase bladder activity like caffeine, above, when compared to individuals without
orange juice, tomato, and spicy foods may be special needs, those with developmental disabil-
considered (Herndon & Joseph, 2006). ity have higher rates of both daytime urinary
Biofeedback therapy is an alternate treatment incontinence and enuresis. Furthermore, there is
that may be effective in the treatment of bladder a greater likelihood for incontinence to persist
dysfunction and is used to help a trainee gain bet- into adulthood for individuals with developmen-
ter control of pelvic floor musculature in order to tal disability when compared with those without
achieve bladder control. Biofeedback therapy special needs (von Gontard, 2013).
involves noninvasive monitoring to give real-time Enuresis in children without any history of
visual or audio feedback about bladder function other bladder dysfunction or major lower urinary
(Schulman, 2004). tract symptoms is known as monosymptomatic
enuresis (Neveus et al., 2010). Monosymptomatic
enuresis is further divided into primary enuresis
Urology and Pharmacology and secondary enuresis. Primary enuresis is more
common and refers to individuals who have never
When other methods are unsuccessful, anticho- achieved nighttime urinary continence (Nevéus
linergic medication may be considered (Allen et al., 2006). Secondary enuresis is less common
et al., 2007). There is evidence of the effective- and refers to individuals who previously achieved
ness of anticholinergics in treating the urinary nighttime urinary continence for at least 6 months
symptoms associated with bladder dysfunction before the onset of nighttime urinary inconti-
(Arendonk, Austin, Boyt, & Cooper, 2006). nence (von Gontard, Mauer-Mucke, Pluck,
Anticholinergic agents aid the proper filling of Berner, & Lehmkuhl, 1999). Secondary enuresis
the bladder by decreasing the frequency of detru- is also more likely to be associated with a major
sor muscle contraction (Finney, Andersson, life stressor or psychiatric comorbidity than pri-
Gillespie, & Stewart, 2006). Oxybutynin is a mary enuresis.
commonly used anticholinergic agent that is It has been suggested that maturational delay
FDA approved to treat bladder disorders in chil- may play a role in monosymptomatic enuresis
dren (Lazarus, 2009). The most common side because most cases tend to resolve on their own
effects of oxybutynin include constipation, dry over time (Neveus et al., 2010). There is also evi-
mouth, flushing, and heat intolerance. In one par- dence that genetic, neurobiological, and psycho-
ticular study of the management of bladder dys- logical factors may influence the development of
function, 45% of participants did not respond to enuresis (Joinson, Sullivan, von Gontard, &
first-line measures and of these individuals, 80% Heron, 2016). One study found that identical
improved after being started on medication twins have approximately double the rate of con-
(Thom et al., 2012). Alpha-adrenergic receptor cordance for enuresis as fraternal twins and sug-
412 P. McPherson et al.

gests that genetics may play a significant role in stated, toileting challenges lead to problems with
enuresis (Bakwin, 1971). Low self-esteem, sleep hygiene, comfort, self-confidence, independence
disturbance (Nevéus, 2009), and high stress lev- and socialization for the learner and stigma and
els (Joinson et al.) among learners are well asso- stress for the trainer (Keen et al., 2007).
ciated with enuresis. There is also evidence that Neurological symptoms may include but are not
neuropsychiatric disorders such as ADHD may limited to seizures, fatigue, weakness, pain, sen-
play a role in the development of enuresis sory impairment, tremor, dystonia, gait distur-
(Neveus et al.; Baeyens et al., 2004; Mellon et al., bance, dizziness, speech or swallowing
2013). A major risk factor for incontinence is difficulties, and visual or auditory impairment
intellectual disability and rates of incontinence (Stone, Carson, & Sharpe, 2005). Some neuro-
increase with decreasing IQ (von Gontard, 2013). logical conditions may cause or exacerbate toilet-
Additionally, a study by Joinson et al. (2007) ing challenges.
found an association between nocturnal enuresis Primary medical caregivers may treat uncom-
and lower IQ scores. plicated neurological disorders. However, com-
Medical management of enuresis involves plex or refractory disorders will be referred to a
a variety of treatment modalities including neurologist. Qualitative and quantitative infor-
behavioral therapies, enuresis alarms, and phar- mation will be necessary to convey to the neu-
macotherapy (Neveus et al., 2010). Treatment rologist and a data collection system such as a
initiation should consider both trainer and bathroom journal will help. When preparing the
learner motivation, and is influenced by the bathroom journal, the neurological symptoms
degree to which the trainers and learner view (seizures, fatigue, weakness, pain, sensory
enuresis as a problem. Enuresis alarms have impairment, tremor, dystonia, gait disturbance,
shown long-term efficacy but require a high dizziness, speech or swallowing difficulties, and
degree of motivation on the part of the trainer visual or auditory impairment) should be
and learner. Pharmacotherapy with desmo- included, with special attention to note the onset,
pressin has proven effective in the treat- frequency, and progression of symptoms as well
ment of enuresis. Desmopressin is generally as associated behavioral issues. The neurological
well tolerated and has few major side effects. evaluation and subsequent imaging studies may
Anticholinergic medications like Oxybutynin include magnetic resonance imaging (MRI),
are sometimes considered in the treatment of computed tomography (CT), and or electroen-
enuresis when standard treatment is ineffective. cephalogram (EEG), all require the cooperation
Tricyclic antidepressant (TCA) pharmacother- of the individual being evaluated. For individuals
apy may also be used to treat enuresis but due to likely to have sensory or behavioral concerns
a less favorable side effect profile, is considered accommodations can be provided. This should be
only when other therapies have proven ineffec- communicated to the neurologist prior to the
tive. Imipramine has been shown to be effective evaluation as some instance may allow sedation
in treating enuresis and is the most commonly to be used.
used TCA to treat enuresis (Neveus et al.).

Central Nervous System Lesions


 edical Specialty Consideration:
M
Neurological Lesions of the central nervous system (CNS) may
impair motor function and result in lower urinary
Neurological conditions that are commonly asso- tract dysfunction, which may impair toilet train-
ciated with toilet training difficulties often ing (Guerra, Leonard, & Castagnetti, 2014).
involve damage to the nervous system. The dam- Cerebral palsy (CP) and symptomatic neurogenic
age to the nervous system may impair sensory bladder (SNB) are conditions resulting from
function, motor function, or both. As previously CNS lesions.
Toilet Training 413

Cerebral palsy manifests as a motor dysfunc-  edical Specialty Consideration:


M
tion following damage to the central nervous sys- Gastroenterological
tem, often involving a hypoxic event (Guerra
et al., 2014). Specific symptoms will be general- The terminology for failure to master bowel
ized to the area of the brain deprived of oxygen. control varies across disciplines. Symptoms
Motor cortex injury may manifest as an inability classified as primary encopresis by the DSM 5
to suppress the impulse to void. This may be may be called “fecal incontinence” by gastroen-
complicated by injury to the frontal lobe, which terologists (American Psychiatric Association,
may result in decreased executive functioning or 2013). The gastroenterologist assesses fecal
temporal lobe with impaired ability to communi- incontinence to determine if it is due to struc-
cate (Wu, 2010). The treatment of cerebral palsy tural, metabolic, or functional abnormalities of
may include medications, surgery, and even deep the digestive system. Structural and metabolic
brain stimulation to address the spasticity, clo- abnormalities may be identified with traditional
nus, and hyperreflexia. Botulinum toxin can be medical diagnostic testing. Structural malfor-
used in patients with increased detrusor muscle mations of the lower digestive tract are typically
tone. Oral and intrathecal antispastic drugs may identified before toilet training begins. Some
be helpful for spasticity (Patterson, 2016). malformations may require corrective surgery,
Additionally, physical therapy and occupational delaying toilet training and creating neurologi-
therapy may be helpful in training. cal or psychological barriers to toilet training.
Symptomatic Neurogenic Bladder (SNB) is Metabolic abnormalities include inflamma-
bladder dysfunction that can be caused by a tory and malabsorption diseases of the bowel.
lesion at any level of the nervous system Functional gastrointestinal disorders (FGIDs),
(Verpoorten & Buyse, 2008). This condition can those without biological markers, are diagnosed
also be present with other conditions including using the ROME IV protocols. The fourth gen-
cerebral palsy. One large study identified a preva- eration of this classification system, the Rome
lence of SNB in 16.4% of individuals with cere- IV criteria, was released in 2016. Rome IV
bral palsy (Murphy, Boutin, & Ide, 2012). With separates disorders by age (neonate/toddler,
SNB, the delay in voiding causes a strain on the child/adolescent, and adult) and by structure
bladder and causes uninhibited bladder contrac- (esophagus, stomach/duodenum, bowel, and
tions (Hodges, Richards, Gorbachinsky, & Krane, gallbladder). Neurogastroenterology, microen-
2014). This condition may eventually result in vironment, cultural and biopsychosocial aspects
decreased renal function in children who are of gastrointestinal disorders are explored in
found to have this condition (Guerra et al., 2014). Rome IV (Rome Foundation, Inc., 2016). The
The goal in the treatment of SNB is typically Rome Foundation publishes questionnaires for
to identify and intervene early before secondary the evaluation of FGID’s. Common FGIDs in
damage to the upper urinary tract occurs. persons with ASD include functional constipa-
Catheterizations along with an anticholinergic tion, functional diarrhea, and functional pain.
medication (oxybutynin) are the standard in treat- The ROME protocols utilize thorough but con-
ment for those with neurogenic bladder crete interview questions to ensure that termi-
­co-­occurring with detrusor hyperactivity and/or nology and concepts discussed mean the same
detrusor sphincter dyssynergia. Although around thing to both the clinician and the informant.
90% of patients can be treated with this gold-­ In the absence of structural or biochemical GI
standard therapy, treatment can also include other abnormalities, the symptom of fecal incon-
bladder-relaxant medications or even injection of tinence would be categorized as a functional
botulinum toxin into the detrusor muscle. As a defecation disorder; further categorized as func-
treatment of final resort, augmentation cysto- tional constipation and nonretentive fecal incon-
plasty (a type of surgery) can be considered when tinence (Hyams et al., 2016). Expert consensus
other treatments fail (Verpoorten & Buyse, 2008). (Buie et al., 2010) recommends that the gastro-
414 P. McPherson et al.

enterological evaluation include (1) a medical Successful treatment of functional constipation is


history and physical examination, (2) an anal achieved for approximately 60% of children
examination, assessment of the back and spine, (Pijpers, Mej, Benninga, & Berger, 2010).
(3) analysis of a stool specimen included an
assessment for parasites, (4) enteric pathogens,
stool guaiac, (5) electrolytes/osmolarity, (6) Recommendations
serum electrolytes, (7) liver function tests, (8)
assessment of nutritional status, (9) an abdomi- Clinical recommendations for toilet training
challenges
nal roentgenogram to assess bowel gas pattern
 Fully assess readiness
and (10) the possible retention of stool.
 Trainee’s readiness across multiple domains
-physical, cognitive, motor, emotional, verbal- must
be thoroughly assessed
Functional Constipation  Trainer’s expectations and skill set should be
evaluated
In a notable study comparing parental reports of  Use physiology to your advantage
GI dysfunction relative to those by pediatric gas-  Healthy diet and fluid intake is necessary for proper
bowel and bladder functioning
troenterologists, constipation was the most fre-
 Increasing fluids during the training period will
quently occurring GI symptom at 85% (Gorrindo increase opportunities for voiding
et al., 2012). Constipation is a frequent challenge  Consider the gastrocolic reflex, which stimulates the
for individuals who have poor nutrition and bowel approximately 15–30 min after the stomach is
hydration. A recent meta-analysis by McElhanon, stretched, when scheduling toileting
McCracken, Karpen, and Sharp (2014) found  Sometimes it takes a village
 Psychological consultation will be necessary to
that constipation was three times more likely for
implement an individualized training plan in
individuals with ASD than their peers. In addi- complex situations
tion, Gorrindo et al. (2012) found that constipa-  Occupational therapy consultation may assist with
tion was associated with younger age, increased sensory training, skill building, and the use of
social impairment, and limited expressive lan- adaptive equipment
guage. Prompted by these findings and the need  Medical consultation may be necessary to fully
assess toileting challenges and obtain orders for
to accurately identify GI dysfunction, researchers diapers, toileting orders for school or community
suggest that the expertise of a gastroenterologist settings, and/or orders for a personal care attendant
is needed to determine the nature of the GI disor- to assist with training
der, as parents tend to be poor at discriminating
symptom variability, so practitioners must evalu-
ate the efficacy of parent-child communication
regarding toileting needs to help clarify the extent Conclusion
that limited expressive language contributes to
constipation by thwarting appropriate toileting The mastery of toileting skills is a developmen-
skills. The ROME IV criterion advises screening tal milestone eagerly awaited by parents. For
for “alarm features.” Alarm features include most it is achieved with minimal effort; however,
severe abdominal distention, absent anal reflex, persons with intellectual or developmental dis-
decreased lower extremity strength or tone, and abilities, physical challenges or chronic medical
physical abnormalities of the gluteal region conditions may require professional assistance.
(Hyams et al., 2016). Functional constipation is For example, individuals with ID/DD may have
treated with education regarding toileting, diet, cognitive or motor deficits that hinder the acqui-
and bowel function in conjunction with imple- sition of toilet training milestones. For these indi-
menting a toileting schedule and diary. In some viduals, training procedures based on behavior
cases, laxatives may be prescribed (Hyams et al.). analytic principles such as reinforcement and
Toilet Training 415

punishment may be effective in teaching appro- Ardic, A., & Cavkaytar, A. (2014). Effectiveness of the
modified intensive toilet training method on teaching
priate toileting skills (Azrin & Foxx, 1971).
toilet skills to children with autism. Education and
Some children may also have medical concerns, Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities,
such as enuresis, encopresis, neurological condi- 49(2), 263–276.
tions, or gastrointestinal problems that impede Arendonk, K. J., Austin, J. C., Boyt, M. A., & Cooper,
C. S. (2006). Frequency of wetting is predictive of
the acquisition or maintenance of toileting skills.
response to anticholinergic treatment in children
In these cases, behavior modification, medica- with overactive bladder. Urology, 67(5), 1049–1053.
tion, medical procedures, or dietary changes may https://doi.org/10.1016/j.urology.2005.11.060
improve the symptoms of medical conditions Austin, P. F., Bauer, S. B., Bower, W., Chase, J., Franco,
I., Hoebeke, P., … Yang, S. S. (2014). The standardiza-
that are influencing toilet training (Hyams et al.,
tion of terminology of lower urinary tract function in
2016). Comprehensive behavioral and/or medical children and adolescents: Update report from the stan-
evaluations are warranted to identify the cause of dardization Committee of the International Children’s
any difficulties and to plan an appropriate and Continence Society. The Journal of Urology, 191.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.juro.2014.01.110
comprehensive treatment approach. Fortunately,
Azrin, N. H., & Foxx, R. M. (1971). A rapid method of
trainers can draw upon scientific literature and toilet training the institutionalized retarded. Journal of
national organizations for guidance regarding Applied Behavior Analysis, 4(2), 89–99.
toilet training difficulties. The research of Azrin Azrin, N. H., Sneed, T. J., & Foxx, R. M. (1973). Dry bed:
A rapid method of eliminating bedwetting (enuresis)
and Foxx, Brazelton, Spock and others highlights
of the retarded. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 11,
the importance of trainee/trainer readiness. The 427–434.
trainee’s physical, cognitive, motor, emotional, Azrin, N. H., & Foxx, R. M. (1974). Toilet training in less
and verbal development must be considered than a day. New York: Pocket Books.
Azrin, N. H., Sneed, T. J., & Foxx, R. M. (1973). Dry bed:
in conjunction with trainer’s abilities. Trainers
A rapid method of eliminating bedwetting (enuresis)
are also encouraged to consult with clinicians of the retarded. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 11,
regarding toilet training difficulties in order to 427–434.
accurately identify and address any medical or Baeyens, D., Roeyers, H., Hoebeke, P., Verté, S., Hoecke,
E. V., & Walle, J. V. (2004). Attention deficit/hyper-
behavioral considerations (Stadtler et al., 1999).
activity disorder in children with nocturnal enuresis.
When medical concerns are suspected, clini- The Journal of Urology, 171(6), 2576–2579. https://
cian expertise may be required to guide trainers doi.org/10.1097/01.ju.0000108665.22072.b2
and learners through unfamiliar medical spe- Bainbridge, N., & Myles, B. S. (1999). The use of priming
to introduce toilet training to a child with autism. Focus
cialty evaluations. The American Academy of
on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 14,
Pediatrics, Autism Speaks, The Kennedy Krieger 106–109.
Institute and other national organizations offer Bakwin, H. (1971). Enuresis in twins. Archives of
additional valuable expertise should these chal- Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 121(3), 222. https://
doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.1971.02100140088007
lenges arise. With knowledge and perseverance,
Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cogni-
trainers can assist learners in the mastery of toi- tive development and functioning. Educational
leting skills. Psychologist, 28(2), 117–148. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15326985ep2802_3
Barone, J. G., Jasutkar, N., & Schneider, D. (2009). Later
toilet training is associated with urge incontinence in
References children. Journal of Pediatric Urology, 5(6), 458–461.
Belva, B., Matson, J. L., Barker, A., Shoemaker, M. E.,
Allen, H. A., Austin, J. C., Boyt, M. A., Hawtrey, C. E., & & Mahan, S. (2011). The relationship between adap-
Cooper, C. S. (2007). Initial trial of timed voiding is tive behavior and specific toileting problems accord-
warranted for all children with daytime incontinence. ing to the profile on toileting issues (POTI). Journal
Urology, 69(5), 962–965. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 23(6),
urology.2007.01.049 535–542.
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2016). The American Berk, L. B., & Friman, P. C. (1990). Epidemiologic aspects
Academy of pediatrics guide to toilet training: revised of toilet training. Clinical Pediatrics, 29(5), 278–282.
and updated second edition. Bantam. https://doi.org/10.1177/000992289002900505
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). DSM 5. Bernasconi, M., Borsari, A., Garzoni, L., Siegenthaler,
American Psychiatric Association. G., Bianchetti, M. G., & Rizzi, M. (2009). Vaginal
416 P. McPherson et al.

voiding: A common cause of daytime urinary leak- tivity and the overactive bladder syndrome: Motor or
age in girls. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent sensory actions? BJU International, 98(3), 503–507.
Gynecology, 22(6), 347–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-410x.2006.06258.x
jpag.2008.07.017 Foxx, R. M., & Azrin, N. H. (1973). Dry pants: A
Bloom, D. A., Seeley, W. W., Ritchey, M., & McGuide, rapid method of toilet training children. Behaviour
G. (1993). Toilet habits and continence in children: An Research and Therapy, 11(4), 435–442. https://doi.
opportunity sampling in search of normal parameters. org/10.1016/0005-7967(73)90102-2
The Journal of Urology, 149(5), 1087–1090. Franco, I., von Gontard, A. V., & Gennaro, M. D. (2013).
Brazelton, T. B. (1962). A child-oriented approach to toi- Evaluation and treatment of nonmonosymptomatic
let training. Pediatrics, 29(1), 121–128. nocturnal enuresis: A standardization document from
Brazelton, T. B., Christophersen, E. R., Frauman, A. C., the International Children’s Continence Society.
Gorski, P. A., Poole, J. M., Stadtler, A. C., & Wright, Journal of Pediatric Urology, 9(2), 234–243. https://
C. L. (1999). Instruction, timeliness, and medi- doi.org/10.1016/j.jpurol.2012.10.026
cal influences affecting toilet training. Pediatrics, Gariepy, C. E. (2004). Developmental disorders of the
103(Supplement 3), 1353–1358. enteric nervous system: Genetic and molecular bases.
Buie, T., Campbell, D. B., Fuchs, G. J., III, Furuta, Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition,
G. T., Levy, J., VandeWater, J., … Winter, H. (2010). 39(1), 5–11.
Evaluation, diagnosis, and treatment of gastrointesti- Gorrindo, P., Williams, K. C., Lee, E. B., Walker, L. S.,
nal disorders in individuals with ASDs: A consensus McGrew, S. G., & Levitt, P. (2012). Gastrointestinal
report. Pediatrics, 125, S1–S18. dysfunction in autism: Parental report, clinical evalu-
Butler, J. F. (1976). The toilet training success of parents ation, and associated factors. Autism Research, 5(2),
after reading toilet training in less than a day. Behavior 101–108.
Therapy, 7(2), 185–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Green, C. L., Walker, T. M., Hoover-Dempsey, K. V.,
S0005-7894(76)80274-2 & Sandler, H. M. (2007). Parents’ motivations for
Chase, J., Austin, P., Hoebeke, P., & Mckenna, P. (2010). involvement in children’s education: An empirical
The management of dysfunctional voiding in chil- test of a theoretical model of parental involvement.
dren: A report from the standardisation committee Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 532–544.
of the International Children’s Continence Society. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.532
The Journal of Urology, 183, 1296–1302. https://doi. Gross, D., & Fogg, L. (2001). Clinical trials in the 21st
org/10.1016/j.juro.2009.12.059 century: The case for participant-centered research.
Choby, B. A., & George, S. (2008). Toilet training. Research in Nursing & Health, 24(6), 530–539.
American Family Physician, 78(9), 1059–1064. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.10010
Cicero, F. R., & Pfadt, A. (2002). Investigation of a Guerra, L., Leonard, M., & Castagnetti, M. (2014).
reinforcement-­ based toilet training procedure for Best practice in the assessment of bladder function
children with autism. Research in Developmental in infants. Therapeutic Advances in Urology, 6(4),
Disabilities, 23, 319–331. 148–164.
Coleman, P. K., & Karraker, K. H. (1998). Self-efficacy Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., & Wierzbicki,
and parenting quality: Findings and future applica- J. J. (1993). A water-prompting procedure for the
tions. Developmental Review, 18(1), 47–85. https:// treatment of urinary incontinence. Journal of Applied
doi.org/10.1006/drev.1997.0448 Behavior Analysis, 26, 473–474.
Dalrymple, N. J., & Angrist, M. H. (1988). Toilet training Hamid, C. H., Holland, A. J. A., & Martin, H. C. O. (2007).
a sixteen year old with autism in a natural setting. The Long-term outcome of anorectal malformations: The
British Journal of Mental Subnormality, 34, 117–130. patient perspective. Pediatric Surgery International,
De Montigny, F., & Lacharité, C. (2005). Perceived paren- 23(2), 97–102.
tal efficacy: Concept analysis. Journal of Advanced Hanney, N. M., Jostad, C. M., LeBlanc, L. A., Carr, J. E.,
Nursing, 49(4), 397–396. Castile, A. J. (2012). Intensive behavioral treatment
Drysdale, B., Lee, C. Y. Q., Anderson, A., & Moore, D. W. of urinary incontinence of children with autism spec-
(2015). Using video modeling i­ncorporating anima- trum disorders: An archival analysis of procedures and
tion to teach toileting to two children with autism outcomes from an outpatient clinic. Focus on Autism
spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and and Other Developmental Disabilities. https://doi.
Physical Disabilities, 27(2), 149–165. org/10.1177/1088357612457987
Farhat, W., Bägli, D. J., Capolicchio, G., O’Reilly, S., Heilenkötter, K., Bachmann, C., Janhsen, E., Stauber,
Merguerian, P. A., Khoury, A., & Mclorie, G. A. T., Lax, H., Petermann, F., & Bachmann, H. (2006).
(2000). The dysfunctional voiding scoring system: Prospective evaluation of inpatient and outpatient
Quantitative standardization of dysfunctional voiding bladder training in children with functional urinary
symptoms in children. The Journal of Urology, 164(3), incontinence. Urology, 67(1), 176–180. https://doi.
1011–1015. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5347(05) org/10.1016/j.urology.2005.07.032
67239-4 Herndon, C. A., & Joseph, D. B. (2006). Urinary incon-
Finney, S. M., Andersson, K., Gillespie, J. I., & Stewart, tinence. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 53(3),
L. H. (2006). Antimuscarinic drugs in detrusor overac- 363–377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcl.2006.02.006
Toilet Training 417

Hicks, J., Carson, C., & Malone, P. (2007). Is there an nyregion/a-fast-track-to-toilet-training-for-those-at-


association between functional bladder outlet obstruc- the-crawling-stage.html
tion and Down’s syndrome? Journal of Pediatric Kiddoo, D. A. (2012). Toilet training children: When to
Urology, 3(5), 369–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. start and how to train. Canadian Medical Association
jpurol.2007.02.003 Journal, 184(5), 511–511.
Hodges, S. J., Richards, K. A., Gorbachinsky, I., & Krane, Kırcaali-İftar, G., Ülke-Kürkçüoğlu, B., Çetin, Ö., &
L. S. (2014). The association of age of toilet training Ünlü, E. (2009). Intensive daytime toilet training of
and dysfunctional voiding. Research and Reports in two children with autism: Implementing and monitor-
Urology, 6, 127–130. ing systematically guarantees success! International
Hoebeke, P., Bower, W., Combs, A., Jong, T. D., & Yang, Journal of Early Childhood Special Education, 1(2),
S. (2010). Diagnostic evaluation of children with day- 117–126.
time incontinence. The Journal of Urology, 183(2), Klassen, T. P., Kiddoo, D., Lang, M. E., Friesen, C.,
699–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.juro.2009.10.038 Russell, K., Spooner, C., & Vandermeer, B. (2006).
Hyams, J. S., Di Lorenzo, C., Miguel, S., Shulman, R. J., The effectiveness of different methods of toilet train-
Annamaria, S., & van Tilburg, M. (2016). Childhood ing for bowel and bladder control. Rockville, MD:
functional gastrointestinal disorders: Child/adoles- Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, US
cent. Gastroenterology, 150(6), 1456–68.e2. Department of Health and Human Services.
Issenman, R. M., Filmer, R. B., & Gorski, P. A. (1999). Kroeger, K., & Sorensen, R. (2010). A parent training
A review of bowel and bladder control development model for toilet training children with autism. Journal
in children: How gastrointestinal and urologic condi- of Intellectual Disability Research, 54(6), 556–567.
tions relate to problems in toilet training. Pediatrics, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01286.x
103(Supplement 3), 1346–1352. Kroeger, K. A., & Sorensen-Burnworth, R. (2009). Toilet
Jansson, U., Hanson, M., Sillen, U., & Hellstrom, A. training individuals with autism and other develop-
(2005). Voiding pattern and acquisition of bladder mental disabilities: A critical review. Research in
control from birth to age 6 years—a longitudinal study. Autism Spectrum Disorder, 3, 607–618.
The Journal of Urology, 174(1), 289–293. https://doi. Laecke, E. V., Golinveaux, L., Goossens, L., Raes, A.,
org/10.1097/01.ju.0000161216.45653.e3 Hoebeke, P., & Walle, J. V. (2001). Voiding disorders
Joinson, C., Heron, J., Butler, R., von Gontard, A. V., in severely mentally and motor disabled children.
Butler, U., Emond, A., & Golding, J. (2007). A The Journal of Urology, 166, 2404–2406. https://doi.
United Kingdom population-based study of intellec- org/10.1097/00005392-200112000-00108
tual capacities in children with and without soiling, Laecke, E. V., Raes, A., Walle, J. V., & Hoebeke, P.
daytime wetting, and bed-wetting. Pediatrics, 120(2). (2009). Adequate fluid intake, urinary incontinence,
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2006-2891 and physical and/or intellectual disability. The
Joinson, C., Heron, J., von Gontard, A. V., Butler, U., Journal of Urology, 182(4), 2079–2084. https://doi.
Emond, A., & Golding, J. (2009). A prospective org/10.1016/j.juro.2009.05.125
study of age at initiation of toilet training and subse- Lazarus, J. (2009). Intravesical oxybutynin in the pediatric
quent daytime bladder control in school-age children. neurogenic bladder. Nature Reviews Urology, 6(12),
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 671–674. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrurol.2009.214
30, 385–393. LeBlanc, L. A., Carr, J. E., Crossett, S. E., Bennett,
Joinson, C., Sullivan, S., von Gontard, A. V., & Heron, C. M., & Detweiler, D. D. (2005). Intensive outpatient
J. (2016). Stressful events in early childhood and behavioral treatment of primary urinary incontinence
developmental trajectories of bedwetting at school of children with autism. Focus on Autism and Other
age. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 41(9), 1002– Developmental Disabilities, 20(2), 98–105. https://
1010. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsw025 doi.org/10.1177/10883576050200020601
Jones, T. L., & Prinz, R. J. (2005). Potential roles of Lott, J. D., & Kroeger, K. A. (2004). Self-help skills in
parental self-efficacy in parent and child adjustment: persons with mental retardation. In J. L. Matson, R. B.
A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(3), 341– Laud, & M. L. Matson (Eds.), Behavior modification
363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.12.004 for persons with developmental disabilities: Treatment
Kaerts, N., Van Hal, G., Vermandel, A., & Wyndaele, and supports (Vol. II). New York: National Association
J.-J. (2012). Readiness signs used to define the proper for the Dually Diagnosed.
moment to start toilet training: A review of the litera- Luiselli, J. K. (1994). Toilet training children with sensory
ture. Neurourology and Urodynamics, 31(4), 437–440. impairments in a residential school setting. Behavioral
https://doi.org/10.1002/nau.21211 Interventions, 9, 105–114.
Keen, D., Brannigan, K. L., & Cuskelly, M. (2007). Toilet Luiselli, J. K. (1997). Teaching toilet skills in a pub-
training for children with autism: The effects of video lic school setting to a child with pervasive develop-
modeling. Journal of Developmental and Physical mental disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Disabilities, 19(4), 291–303. Experimental Psychiatry, 28, 163–168.
Kelley, T. (2005). A fast track to toilet training for Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J. C. (2010). The
those at the crawling stage. The New York Times. profile of toileting issues (POTI). Baton Rouge, LA:
Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2005/10/09/ Disability Consultants, LLC.
418 P. McPherson et al.

Matson, J. L., Horovitz, M., & Sipes, M. (2011). Children’s Continence Society. The Journal of
Characteristics of individuals with toileting problems Urology, 176(1), 314–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/
and intellectual disability using the profile of toileting s0022-5347(06)00305-3
issues (POTI). Journal of Mental Health Research in Palechka, G., & McDonald, R. (2010). A comparison of
Intellectual Disabilities, 4(1), 53–63. the acquisition of play skills using instructor-created
Matson, J. L., & LoVullo, S. V. (2009). Encopresis, video models and commercially available videos.
soiling and constipation in children and adults with Education and Treatment of Children, 33(3), 457–474.
developmental disability. Research in Developmental Pashankar, D. S., & Bishop, W. P. (2001). Efficacy and
Disabilities, 30(4), 799–807. optimal dose of daily polyethylene glycol 3350 for
Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Hess, J. A., & Kozlowski, A. M. treatment of constipation and encopresis in children.
(2011). Assessment of toileting difficulties in adults The Journal of Pediatrics, 139(3), 428–432. https://
with intellectual disabilities: An examination using doi.org/10.1067/mpd.2001.117002
the profile of toileting issues (POTI). Research in Patterson, M. C. (2016). Management and prognosis
Developmental Disabilities, 32(1), 176–179. of cerebral palsy. In T.W. Post, C. Bridgemohan
Matson, J. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (1977). Issues in toilet & C. Armsby (Eds.), UptoDate. Available from
training normal children. Behavior Therapy, 8(4), 549– https://www.uptodate.com/contents/management-
553. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(77)80184-6 and-prognosis-of-cerebral-palsy?source=search_
McElhanon, B. O., McCracken, C., Karpen, S., & Sharp, result&search=cerebral%20palsy%20treatment&sele
W. G. (2014). Gastrointestinal symptoms in autism ctedTitle=1~150
spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, Pijpers, M., Mej, B., Benninga, M. A., & Berger, M. Y.
133(5), 872–883. (2010). Functional constipation in children: A sys-
McLay, L., Carnett, A., van der Meer, L., & Lang, R. tematic review on prognosis and predictive factors.
(2015). Using a video modeling-based interven- Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition,
tion package to toilet train two children with autism. 50(3), 256–268.
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, Polaha, J., Warzak, W. J., & Dittmer-McMahon, K.
27(4), 431–451. (2002). Toilet training in primary care: Current prac-
Mellon, M. W., Natchev, B. E., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, tice and recommendations from behavioral pediatrics.
R. C., Weaver, A. L., Voigt, R. G., & Barbaresi, W. J. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics,
(2013). Incidence of enuresis and encopresis among 23(6), 424–429.
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- Post, A. R., & Kirkpatrick, M. D. (2004). Toilet training
der in a population-based birth cohort. Academic for a young boy with pervasive developmental disor-
Pediatrics, 13(4), 322–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. der. Behavioral Interventions, 19, 45–50.
acap.2013.02.008 Powers, M. K., Brown, E. T., Hogan, R. M., Martin, A. D.,
Mercer, E. S., Broecker, B., Smith, E. A., Kirsch, A. J., Ortenberg, J., & Roth, C. C. (2015). Trends in toilet
Scherz, H. C., Massad, A., & C. (2004). Urological training and voiding habits among children with Down
manifestations of Down syndrome. The Journal of Syndrome. The Journal of Urology, 194(3), 783–787.
Urology, 171(3), 1250–1253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.juro.2015.03.114
Miura, F., Watanabe, T., Watanabe, K., Takemoto, K., Rasouly, H. M., & Lu, W. (2013). Lower urinary tract
& Fukushi, K. (2016). Comparative assessment of development and disease. Wiley interdisciplinary
­primary and secondary infection risks in a norovirus reviews. Systems Biology and Medicine, 5(3), 307–342.
outbreak using a household model simulation. Journal Ritblatt, S. N., Obegi, A. D., Hammons, B. S., Ganger,
of Environmental Sciences, 50, 13–20. T. A., & Ganger, B. C. (2003). Parents’ and child
Murphy, K. P., Boutin, S. A., & Ide, K. R. (2012). Cerebral care professionals’ toilet training attitudes and prac-
palsy, neurogenic bladder, and outcomes of lifetime tices: A comparative analysis. Journal of Research in
care. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Childhood Education, 17(2), 133–146.
54(10), 945–950. Rome Foundation, Inc. (2016). Rome foundation. Retrieved
Nevéus, T. (2009). Diagnosis and management of noctur- from Rome Foundation: http://romecriteria.org/
nal enuresis. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 21(2), 199– Rosenberg, N. E., Schwartz, I. S., & Davis, C. A. (2010).
202. https://doi.org/10.1097/mop.0b013e3283229b12 Evaluating the utility of commercial videotapes
Neveus, T., Eggert, P., Evans, J., Macedo, A., Rittig, S., for teaching hand washing to children with autism.
Tekgül, S., … Robson, L. (2010). Evaluation of and Education and Treatment of Children, 33, 443–455.
treatment for monosymptomatic enuresis: A standard- Saloviita, T. (2000). Generalized effects of dry bed training
ization document from the International Children’s on day-time incontinence. Behavioral Interventions,
Continence Society. The Journal of Urology, 183(2), 15(1), 79–81.
441–447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.juro.2009.10.043 Schmitt, B. D. (2004). Toilet training problems:
Nevéus, T., von Gontard, A., Hoebeke, P., Hjälmås, K., Underachievers, refusers, and stool holders.
Bauer, S., Bower, W., … Djurhuus, J. C. (2006). The Contemporary Pediatrics-Montvale, 21(4), 71–77.
standardization of terminology of lower urinary tract Schonwald, A., Sherritt, L., Stadtler, A., & Bridgemohan,
function in children and adolescents: Report from C. (2004). Factors associated with difficult toilet train-
the standardisation committee of the International ing. Pediatrics, 113(6), 1753–1757.
Toilet Training 419

Schulman, S. L. (2004). Voiding dysfunction in children. urinary tract dysfunction: A stepwise approach.
Urologic Clinics of North America, 31(3), 481–490. Journal of Pediatric Urology, 8(1), 20–24. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ucl.2004.04.019 org/10.1016/j.jpurol.2011.01.004
Schulman, S. L., Quinn, C. K., Plachter, N., & Kodman- van Oorsouw, W. M., Duker, P. C., Melein, L., & Averink,
Jones, C. (1999). Comprehensive management of M. (2009). Long-term effectiveness of the response
dysfunctional voiding. Pediatrics, 103(3), 1353–1358. restriction method for establishing diurnal bladder
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.103.3.e31 control. Research in Developmental Disabilities: A
Schum, T. R., Kolb, T. M., McAuliffe, T. L., Simms, Multidisciplinary Journal, 30(6), 1388–1393.
M. D., Underhill, R. L., & Lewis, M. (2002). Vermandel, A., Van Kampen, M., Van Gorp, C., &
Sequential acquisition of toilet-training skills: A Wyndaele, J. J. (2008). How to toilet train healthy
descriptive study of gender and age differences in nor- children? A review of the literature. Neurourology
mal children. Pediatrics, 109(3), e48–e48. https://doi. and Urodynamics, 27(3), 162–166. https://doi.
org/10.1542/peds.109.3.e48 org/10.1002/nau.20490
Schum, T. R., McAuliffe, T. L., Simms, M. D., Walter, Verpoorten, C., & Buyse, G. M. (2008). The neurogenic
J. A., Lewis, M., & Pupp, R. (2001). Factors associ- bladder: Medical treatment. Pediatric Nephrology,
ated with toilet training in the 1990s. Ambulatory 23(5), 717–725.
Pediatrics, 1(2), 79–86. von Gontard, A. (2013). Urinary incontinence in children
Simon, J. L., & Thompson, R. H. (2006). The effects of with special needs. Nature Reviews Urology, 10(11),
undergarment type on the urinary continence of toddlers. 667–674.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 362–368. von Gontard, A., de Jong, T. P. V. M., Angie, R., Anka,
Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). N.-L., Badawi, J. K., & Linda, C. (2016). Do we man-
Vineland adaptive behavior scales: (Vineland II), age incontinence in children and adults with special
survey interview form/caregiver rating form. Livonia, needs adequately? ICI-RS 2014. Neurourology and
MN: Pearson Assessments. Urodynamics, 35(2), 304–306.
Spock, B. (1946). The common sense book of baby and von Gontard, A. V., Mauer-Mucke, K., Pluck, J., Berner,
child care. New York: Duell, Sloan and Pearce. W., & Lehmkuhl, G. (1999). Clinical behavioral prob-
Springford, L., Connor, M. J., Jones, K., Kapetanakis, lems in day- and night-wetting children. Pediatric
V. V., & Giuliani, S. (2016). Prevalence of active long-­ Nephrology, 13(8), 662–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/
term problems in patients with anorectal malforma- s004670050677
tions: A systematic review. Diseases of the Colon and Wiener, J. S., Scales, M. T., Hampton, J., King, L. R.,
Rectum, 59(6), 570–580. Surwit, R., & Edwards, C. L. (2000). Long-term effi-
Stadtler, A. C., Gorski, P. A., & Brazelton, T. B. (1999). cacy of simple behavioral therapy for daytime wetting
Toilet training methods, clinical interventionstraining in children. The Journal of Urology, 786–790. https://
methods, clinical interventions, and recommenda- doi.org/10.1097/00005392-200009010-00048
tions. Pediatrics, 103(Supplement 3), 1359–1361. Wolraich, M., & American Academy of Pediatrics.
Stone, J., Carson, A., & Sharpe, M. (2005). Functional (2016). American academy of pediatrics guide to toilet
symptoms and signs in neurology: Assessment and training. Bantam.
diagnosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Wolraich, M. L. (Ed.). (2016). Guide to toilet training
Psychiatry, 76, i2–12. (2nd ed.). New York: Bantam Books.
Tarbox, R. S. E., Williams, W. L., & Friman, P. C. (2004). Wraige, E. (2002). Investigation of daytime wetting:
Extended diaper wearing: Effects on continence in When is spinal cord imaging indicated? Archives of
and out of the diaper. Journal of Applied Behavior Disease in Childhood, 87(2), 151–155. https://doi.
Analysis, 37, 97–100. org/10.1136/adc.87.2.151
Taylor, S., Cipani, E., & Clardy, A. (1994). A stimulus con- Wu, H.-Y. (2010). Achieving urinary continence in chil-
trol technique for improving the efficacy of an estab- dren. Nature Reviews Urology, 7(7), 371–377.
lished toilet training program. Journal of Behavior Wu, W.-H., Chuang, J.-H., Ting, Y.-C., Lee, S.-Y., &
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 155–160. Hsieh, C.-S. (2002). Developmental anomalies and
Thom, M., Campigotto, M., Vemulakonda, V., Coplen, disabilities associated with hypospadias. The Journal
D., & Austin, P. F. (2012). Management of lower of Urology, 168(1), 229–232.
Index

A children and adolescents, 47, 215


Academic achievement, 119 childhood, CBT, 232–236
Academic performance cognitive theories, 226, 227
child’s age, 100 developmental and (trans)diagnostic
classroom settings, 106 considerations, 223
student, 98, 101, 103 EBPP, 228–230
Academic support, 99 ESTs, 230–232
Adolescent depression etiology
cognitive behavioral approaches, 171, 174–176 biological differences, 223, 224
evidence-based treatments, 166 environmental differences, 224–226
interpersonal approaches, 176–178 temperamental differences, 223
medication trials, 178, 179 OCD and PTSD, 48, 49
youth, 166 pathogenesis and treatment, 207
Adolescent-focused individual therapy (AFT), 353 separation, 208, 209
Adolescents social, 208, 209
anxiety disorder symptoms, 211 and specific phobias, 211
SCI, 308, 309 systematic evaluations, 237
specific phobias, 207, 209 treatment, 221–223
ADVANCE parent training program, 66 youth depression, 47, 209
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Anxious-fearful symptoms, 15
(AHRQ), 394 Applied behavior analysis (ABA), 149, 258, 370
Aggression and tantrums application, 26
ABA studies, 258 behavior analysis, 25
ASD diagnosis, 258 component, 26
behavioral approach, 259–262 principles, 26, 27
characteristics and prevalence, 258–259 prompting, 28
child, 257 reinforcement, 27, 33, 34
DRA, FCT and DRI procedures, 261 seminal paper, 25
EFA and FBA, 260 subject matter, 25
treatments, 261 Aripiprazole, 199, 200, 262
Aggressive behavior, 258 Asperger’s disorder, 139, 140
American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP), 397 Assessment, 271
American National Mental Health Act, 5 pica, developmental disabilities, 287–289
American Psychological Association (APA), 72, 228 SIB (see Self-injurious behavior (SIB))
Analog functional analysis methodology, 35 Attachment-based family therapy (ABFT), 178
Anorexia nervosa (AN), 342–343 Attention capture, 88, 89
Antecedent interventions, 375–377 Attention deficit disorder (ADD), 33
Antecedent strategies, 114 Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 12, 16,
Antipsychotics, 199–200 19, 20, 70–73, 244, 403
Anxiety disorders in children, 127
ameliorate symptoms, 236 diagnose, 134
behavioral theories, 227, 228 issue of medication, 133, 134
characteristic symptoms, 222 mental health professionals, 134

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 421


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Childhood Psychopathology and Developmental
Disabilities Treatment, Autism and Child Psychopathology Series,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-71210-9
422 Index

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (cont.) indirect functional analysis methods, 290
multimodal treatments, 129–132 limitation, 296
parent training, 132, 133 meta-analyses, 291
SCI, 310, 311 punishment-based interventions, 293, 294, 296
symptoms, 127, 128 QABF, 291
treatment, 128, 129 response-contingent interventions, 292, 293, 296
Auditory processing disorders (APD), 89 SIB
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), 26, 28, 31, 50, 76–78, CSA, 279
99, 105, 109, 110, 112, 115, 152, 153, 258, 310 DRO, 276
behavior therapy, 149, 150 NCE, 277
camels’ milk, 142, 143 NCR, 276, 278
chelation therapy, 146 precursor behavior, targeting, 277
chemical castration, 146 protective equipment, 279, 280
chiropractic manipulation, 147 punishment treatment, 280
comorbid psychopathology, 152–153 safeguards, 281
core symptoms, 138 Behavioral parent training (BPT)
craniosacral therapy, 145 interventions, 78
diets, 143 programs, 67
difference relationship-based model, 148 treatments, 68
dolphin-assisted therapy, 144 Behavioral reward systems, 250
DSM-5, 139, 140 Behavioral systems family therapy (BSFT), 354
early diagnosis, 140 Bilateral subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, 165
empirical support, 140–142 Binge eating disorder (BED), 343–344
equine-assisted activities, 143, 144 Biofeedback training, 330, 331
ESDM, 150 Bipolar disorders, 46–47
facilitated communication, 147, 148 adults, 204
HBOT, 144, 145 antipsychotics, 199, 200
history of, 138, 139 children’s mental health, 204
individuals, 137, 153 in clinical practice, 204
medicinal marijuana, 146, 147 definition, 195, 196
MMS, 145, 146 divalproex sodium, 201
oxytocin inhalation, 151 lamotrigine, 201
PRT, 149 medications and supplements, 199
researchers and clinicians, 153 open-label carbamazepine study, 200
schizophrenic trait, 138 pediatric, 203
SCI, 310 prevalence, 196
social skills interventions, 150, 151 psychological treatments, 202, 203
stem cells, 151 quetiapine, 201
therapies, 143, 144 risk factors, 196–198
vitamin supplements, 142 service models, 198, 199
Autism Spectrum Disorders- Comorbidity for Children supplements, 201, 202
(ASD-CC), 289, 295 Blood-injection-injury, 208
Blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD), 151, 247
Brazelton toilet training method, 395
B Brief Autism Mealtime Behaviors Inventory
Behavior problems inventory (BPI), 289, 295 (BAMBI), 371
Behavior therapy (BT), 356 Brief cognitive behavioral therapy (BCBT), 47
Behavioral approach test (BAT), 209 Bulimia nervosa (BN), 343
Behavioral intervention, 97, 101, 134, 260
ABA-based, 149
DIR, 148 C
evaluation of, 103 Carbamazepine, 200
intensive, 141 Carnation Instant Breakfast® (CIB), 376
meta-analysis, 141 Casomorphin, 142
pica Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale
antecedent modification interventions, 291, 292, 295 (CES-D), 164, 177
direct functional analysis methods, 290 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), 80
experimental functional analysis, 291 Central nervous system (CNS), 412
functional analysis procedures, 290 Challenging Horizons Program, 133
Index 423

Chelation therapy, 146 Conjoint behavioral consultation (CBC), 101, 102


Child/Adolescent Anxiety Multimodal Treatment Study Consultation
(CAMS), 47 CBC, 101, 102
Child-centered therapy (CCT), 183 intervention monitoring, 103–104
Child-directed interaction, 66 problem behavior, 102, 103
Childhood schizophrenia, 139 termination, 104
Children, 270, 287, 322 Content-specificity hypothesis (CSH), 44
ADHD, 98, 115 Coping with Depression Course (CWD), 174, 175
anxiety and depression, 98, 116 Coprophagy, 287, 294, 295
ASD, 99 Corticostriatal-thalamo-cortical (CSTC), 246
clinicians and families, 99 Cost-effective method, 383
depression, 113, 118 Craniosacral therapy, 145
disabilities and ID, 106
disruptive behavior, 115
EBD, 105 D
feeding disorder (see Pica) Daytime urinary incontinence, 408
headaches (see Headache disorders) Depression, 12, 13, 16–19, 45, 46
learning disorders, 100 adolescents
preschool, 99 diagnose, 167–170
SCAS, 329 symptoms, 172–173
school-aged, 99, 104 preadolescents, 180–182
SCI, 308, 309 youth
self-injurious behavior, 109 adolescent-onset and preadolescent-onset, 159,
SIB (see Self-injurious behavior (SIB)) 160, 179–184
social problem-solving training, 113 clinical practice, 185
tension-type headache, 323 comorbidity, 161, 162
Chronic tic disorder (CTD), 242 cultural considerations, 163, 164
Chthonophagy, 287, 295 developmental context, 160, 161
Clinicians familial processes, 162, 163
consultation team, 100 neurobiological factors, 164–166
school setting, 101 psychopharmacologic and psychosocial
school-based consultation, 100 treatments, 159
students’ cognitive and behavioral deficits, 101 research, 184, 185
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), 4, 138, 152, 171, SSRIs, 159
174, 175, 215, 355–358 stress, 163
characteristic session structure, 51 Developmental disabilities, 99, 100, 257–261
clinical applications, 43 Developmental psychopathology
cutting-edge Coping Cat protocol, 45 adaptation, 11
effectiveness, 45 adolescence, 16, 17
learning theory, 43 biological, psychological and socio-contextual
meta-analysis, 44 factors, 20
operant learning paradigms, 44 challenges, 11
problem-solving, 52 continuity and discontinuity, 12
procedures, 44 early sexual abuse, 12
single-case study, 45 equifinality, 12
social learning theory, 44 heterotypic and homotypic continuity, 12
therapeutic style, 51, 52 infancy, 13, 14
young patients, 50 multifinality, 12
Cognitive-Behavioral Intervention for Trauma in Schools normal and abnormal, 12
(CBITS), 48 normal suffering and human experience, 20
Collaborative empiricism (CE), 51 preschool/toddlerhood, 14, 15
Community Parent Education Program (COPE), 133 progression and organization of, 11
Competing stimulus assessment (CSA), 278, 279 in school-aged children, 15, 16
Competing-response training, 249 Diagnosis
Compliance training skills, 65–66 ASD
Comprehensive behavioral intervention for tics children, 140
(CBIT), 244, 249 DSM-III and DSM-III-R, 139
Computerized CBT program (C-CBT), 175 DSM-IV and DSM-IV-TR, 139
Conduct disorder (CD), 98 bipolar disorder, 196–198, 200
424 Index

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders EOIT, 358


(DSM), 208, 368 family therapy, 354
Difference relationship-based model (DIR), 148 FBT, 353
Differential reinforcement (DR), 292 gender differences, 347
Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior hospitalization programs, 351
(DRA), 32, 375 incidence, 346
in pica, 292, 293 IPT, 358
in SIB, 276 longitudinal course, 348
Differential reinforcement of incompatible behavior medical and nutritional management, 350
(DRI), 32 outpatient treatment, 353–358
Differential reinforcement of low rates (DRL), 32, 262 prevalence, 345
Differential reinforcement of other SFT, 354
behavior (DRO), 31, 276 symptomatology, 346
Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs), 14 treatment, 350–352
Discrete trial teaching (DTT), 27 Ego-oriented individual treatment (EOIT), 358
Discrete trial training (DTT), 149 Emotion regulation strategies, 307, 308
Disposable-undergarments, 406 affective education, 112
Disruptive behavior disorders (DBD), 49, 68–70, 72, 245 social problem-solving, 112, 113
Divalproex sodium, 199, 201 somatic relaxation, 112
Dolphin-assisted therapy (DAT), 144 Emotion understanding, 307
Dopamine 2 (D2), 251 Empirically supported treatment (EST), 72, 221, 231, 236
DRD2 gene, 81 Enhanced CBT (CBT-E), 356
Dysthymia, 209 Enuresis, 411
Epidemiology
in infancy, 14
E prevalence and risk factors, 12
Early elimination communication (EEC), 401 Episodic memory, 92, 322
Early elimination toilet training method, 395, 396 Equine-assisted activities and therapies (EAAT), 143, 144
Early history, intellectual disabilities Escape extinction procedures, 379
academic skills, 7 Ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid (EDTA), 146
adaptive behaviors scales, 2 Evidence-based practice in psychology (EBPP), 78, 153,
adult mental health services, 6 228, 230, 231
autism spectrum disorders, 7 Experimental functional analysis (EFA), 260, 305, 306, 309
behavioral and school-based learning problems, 3 Externalizing behavior disorders, 98
child mental health, 2, 5, 6, 8 Extinction-based procedures, 381
child sexual abuse, 7
childhood education and mental health, 6
children’s learning and behavior problems, 1, 2 F
comorbidity, 6 Family School Success (FSS), 131
disabilities, 1 Family-based IPT (FB-IPT), 179, 183
educational and learning-based treatments, 8 Family-focused treatment for childhood depression
ethnic minority community, 6 (FFT-CD), 183, 353–355
functional assessment, 6 Federal education regulations
global child mental health, 6 IEP/504 plan for students, 105
international developments, 6 Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 104, 105
mental health professionals, 7 Section 504, 104
modern mental health movement, 2 Feeding disorders
neurodevelopmental disorders, 3, 4 BAMBI, 371
pediatric depression, 2 behavioral interventions, 370, 374
psychotherapy, 3 chewing, 377
self-mutilation, 6 descriptive assessments, 371
Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), 141, 150 DRA, 375
Eating disorders (EDs) extinction-based procedures, 381
adolescents, 357 fading and texture manipulations, 375
AN, 342 feeding interventions, 386
BED, 343 food refusal and selectivity, 368
BN, 343 frequency, 386
CBT, 355 generalization and maintenance, 383
child and adolescent, 359 indirect methods, 370
comorbidity, 347–348 malnutrition, 369
EDNOS, 344 multidisciplinary assessment, 370
Index 425

obesity, 369 Integra Social ACES (awareness, competence,


pediatric population, 367 engagement and skills) program
preference assessments, 371 childrens and youths, 312
PSPA, 372 component, 312
questionnaires, 370 description, 311
re-presentation, 381 effect sizes, 316
skill deficits, 368 information processing deficits, 314
target foods, 385 LD, 311
treatment options, 384 program content, 312
Feeding skill deficits, 368–369 self-regulation and emotion regulation, 314
Fine-motor skills, 245 silly-serious scale, 315
Fluvoxamine, 152 Intellectual and developmental disabilities (IDD)
Food refusal/selectivity, 368 administrative decision-making, 272
Foxx and Azrin method, 404 CSAs, 279
Functional analysis (FA), 273, 373 functional analysis, 279
Functional assessment observation (FAO), 249, 273 NCCPC, 271
Functional behavior assessment (FBA), 31, 102, 103, prevalence, 270
109, 110, 117, 260 topographies, 270
Functional communication training (FCT), 36, 261, 276 Intellectual disability (InD), 105
Functional gastrointestinal disorders (FGIDs), 413 Internalizing disorders, 98, 99
International Classification of Headache Disorders, 3rd
Edition Beta version (ICHD-3 beta), 322, 323
G Interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT), 176, 356, 358
Gastrointestinal (GI) abnormalities, 143 Inter-professional collaboration, 118
Geophagy, 287, 288, 295 Irrelevant sound effect (ISE), 88
Global Burden of Disease Study (GBD), 14
Goal-directed behavior, 249
Graduated exposures, 114 J
Juvenile delinquency, 3, 7

H
Habit reversal training (HRT), 249 L
Headache disorder Lamotrigine, 201
in children, 321, 322 Learning disability (LD), 307, 311
classification system, 322, 323 Least restrictive environment (LRE), 105
course of, 323 Lithium, 199
genetic and environmental factors, 324–325 Lithophagy, 287, 295
migraine, 324 Long-term memory, 92
primary
functional model, 325–327
psychological assessment, 327–329 M
psychological treatment, 329–336 Major depressive disorder (MDD), 18, 19, 209
tension-type, 324 Marcus autism center, 77
Heart rate variability (HRV), 165 Measurement-based care (MBC), 50, 51
Helping the Noncompliant Child (HNC) program, 66 Medical specialty evaluations, 408
Heterotypic continuity, 12, 16, 17 Medicinal marijuana, 146, 147
High-frequency HRV (HF-HRV), 165 Meta-analysis (MA), 68, 71
Homotypic continuity, 12, 17 Migraine
Hostile-aggressive behavior, 15 abdominal, 322
Human Genome Project’s sequencing, 81 adolescents, 334
Hyperbaric oxygen therapy (HBOT), 144, 145 aura, 322, 324
Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical characteristics of, 323
(HPA) axis, 13, 14 chronification, 326
and electromyographic biofeedback, 330
pedMIDAS, 329
I tension-type headache, 322–324
Incidental teaching, 29 Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR), 113, 114
Incredible Years (IY), 66 Miracle Mineral Solution (MMS), 145, 146
Individualized educational program (IEP), 104–105 Mixed clinical problems, 67
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 105, Modifications
116, 118, 275 adapted workload, 117
426 Index

Modifications (cont.) randomized control trial, 77


alternate assessments, 118 rewarding, 65
alternate grading, 117–118 skills, 64
children’s bipolar disorder, 199 SOE reviews, 73
pediatric bipolar disorder, 198 standing rules, 65
Modular Approach to Therapy for Children with Anxiety, time-out protocol, 65
Depression, Trauma/Conduct Problems treatment category, 67
(MATCH-ADTC), 45 trends, 78
Modular CBT (mCBT), 45 Parent training effects, 74, 75
Mood disorders, 45, 46 Parental anxiety, 224
Parental control, 225
Parent-child interaction therapy (PCIT), 66, 71, 179, 183
N Parent-directed interaction, 66
National Standards Project (NSP), 76 Partial hospital programs, 352
Neuropsychological functioning, 245 Patterson’s model, 64
Non-Communicating Children’s Pain Checklist Pediatric bipolar disorder, 196–203
(NCCPC), 271 Peer-mediated instruction and intervention (PMII), 114,
Noncompliant Child program, 64, 80 115
Noncontingent escape (NCE), 277 Peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs), 151
Noncontingent reinforcement (NCR), 276–278, 291, 292, Pharmacological treatment, headaches
295, 375 acute, 329
Nonremoval of the meal (NRM), 381 adults and children, 329
Nonremoval of the spoon (NRS), 380 ibuprofen, 329
Non-stimulant drugs, 128, 133 prevention, 330
Non-verbal learning disabilities (NLD), 306, 307 prophylaxis, 330
Nurse Family Partnership (NFP) program, 79 triptan agents, 329
Pharmacology, 411
Pharmacotherapy, 251, 262–264
O Phonological similarity effect (PSE), 90
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), 48, 208, 209 Pica
One-session therapy (OST), 47, 215 antecedent modification interventions
oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), 98, 209, 403 discrimination training procedures, 292
Overactive bladder (OAB), 409 environmental enrichment procedures, 292
Oxytocin, 151 food/toys, noncontingent presentation, 291
NCR, 291
response effort manipulations, 292
P assessment, 289
Paired stimulus preference assessment (PSPA), 372 behavioral/biological interventions, 290, 291
Panic disorder, 209, 211 challenging behavior, 287
Parent educational intervention (PEI), 77 by cultural practices, 288
Parent management training (PMT), 250 description, 287
Parent training, 129, 130, 132, 382 etiology, 288, 289
ADHD, 70, 72 factors, affecting, 288
ADVANCE program, 66 inappropriate feeding behavior, 295
attention plans, 65 medical consequences, 288
BPT, 69 nutritional deficiencies, 288, 289, 295
B-S and W-S studies, 70 prevalence, 295
disruptive behavior problems, 73 punishment-based interventions
factors, 74 aversive stimuli, presentation of, 294
features, 75 environmental stimulus, 293
genetic and biological factors, 81–82 overcorrection, 293, 294
health and mental health services, 79 physical and mechanical restraints, 294
ignoring, 65 time-out procedures, 294
instructions/commands, 65 response-contingent interventions
IY training series, 66 DR, 292
MA, 68 DRI and DRA, 292, 293
meta-analytic reviews, 68 response-blocking procedures, 293
PCIT, 66, 79 visual screening procedures, 293
phases, 64 and social interaction, 289
Index 427

symptoms, 287 Risk factors, bipolar disorder, 196–198


types, 287 Risperidone, 152, 199, 200, 262
with ASD and ID, 288
Pivotal response training (PRT), 149
Positive behavior interventions at school S
daily report card, 111 Screening Tool of Feeding Problems (STEP), 289, 295
economies, 109–111 Second Step program, 309
IDEA, 106 Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs)
labeled praise, 106–109 adolescents, 178
planned ignoring, 106–109 CBT, 171, 179
Positive parenting program, 66 and clinical care, 171
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), 48, 49 self-reported depressive symptoms, 178
Preadolescent depression in youth, 159, 178
CBT, 179 Self-injurious behavior (SIB)
CCT, 183 assessment methods
depressive symptoms, 183 categories, 271
FB-IPT, 179, 183 clinical manipulation, variables, 271
PAPA, 184 direct/indirect assessment, 272, 273
PCIT, 179, 183 categorization, 270
in school-aged youth, 183 causes
Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS), 247 automatic reinforcement, 274
Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA), 184 automatically-maintained SIB, 274
Profile of Toileting Issues (POTI), 406 functional analysies data, 275
Progressive muscle relaxation (PMR), 112 function-based intervention, 275
Psychiatric disorders, 160, 162 hypotheses, 274
Psychodynamic therapy, 358 negative/positive reinforcement, social, 274
Psychoeducation, 52, 177, 234 computer-assisted measurement tool, 271
Psychological assessment, headaches definition, 269
biofeedback training, 330, 331 intervening earlier, 269
diagnostic type, 327 measurement, 270, 271
children, 335 prevalence, 270
chronic adulthood disorder, 329 topographies, 270
cognitive behavioural therapy, 331, 332 treatment
diagnostic system, 335 antecedent manipulations, 277
efficacy, 332, 333 CSA, 279
illness behaviours, 336 differential reinforcement, 276
interviews, 328 DRO, 276
modality, 333, 334 FCT, 276
paediatric population, 335 medical and pharmacological, 275
parental involvement, 334, 335 NCE, 277
pharmacological, 329, 330 NCR, 276–278
positive mother-child relationships, 336 precursor behavior, 277, 278
questionnaires, 328, 329 problem behavior, 277
relaxation training, 331 protective equipment, 279, 280
time period, 333 punishment, 280
self-monitoring, 328 and safety, 281
Psychotropic medication, 101 task completion, 277
Semantic memory, 92–93
Separated family therapy (SFT), 354
Q Serotonin noradrenergic reuptake inhibitor (SNRI), 178
Questions About Behavioral Function (QABF), 291 Shapiro TS Severity Scale (STSS), 247
Quetiapine, 201 Silly-serious scale, 315
Social anxiety disorder, 222–224, 226, 227, 232
Social competence
R attentional control, 305
Randomized control trials (RCTs), 45, 161, 228, 231 and cognitive factors, 303
Relaxation training, 331 definition, 302
Response cost, 30, 31 emotion regulation/understanding, 307, 308
Response to intervention (RtI) system, 103, 105 executive functions (EF), 305, 306
428 Index

Social competence (cont.) Symptomatic Neurogenic Bladder (SNB), 413


friendships and social relationships, 301 Systematic desensitization, 213
hostile attributions, 303
processing deficits, 305
protective factor, mental health, 301 T
schemas, 303 Tantrum behaviors, 258
SIP model, 303, 304 Teaching interaction procedure (TIP), 34, 35
social skills, 302 Temperament, 210
speed, processing, 306 Tension-type headache, 322–324, 330, 331
and ToM, 304 Tests of evidence (TOE), 53
visual-spatial processing, 306, 307 Texture manipulations, 376
Social competence enhancement program (SCEP), 310 Theory of mind (ToM), 304–306
Social competence interventions (SCI) Therapeutic Summer Day Camp for Children with
children and adolescents ADHD, 311
emotion regulation and skills, 308, 309 Tic disorder, 242, 244
neuropsychological cognitive processes, 309 Toilet training
physical social actions, 308 AAP, 399
Second Step program, 309 AHRQ report, 394
group content and matching, 312 assessments, 406
group session, 313, 314 Azrin and Foxx method, 404
Integra Social ACES program, 311, 312 behavioral challenges, 403
neurodevelopmental disorders bladder dysfunction, 410
repetitive motor movements, 310 cultural differences, 394
SCEP, 310 diapers, 406
social deficits, 310 disposable-undergarments, 406
summer day camp, 311 emotional and social factors, 397
visualization and breathing, 310 FGIDs, 413
sample activity and skils, 315 initial medical evaluation, 408
Social information processing (SIP) model, 303, 304 literature, 403
Social skills training, 250 medical caregivers, 412
Socially-mediated SIB, 276, 278 medical conditions/consultation, 407
Specific phobia POTI, 406
child, 207 pre-training requirements, 404
epidemiology, 209 protocols, 400–401
etiology, 210–213 punishment procedures, 405
phenomenology, 208, 209 teaching, 394
response systems, 216 theories, 394–395
treatment, 213–216 trainer, 402
Spock Toilet Training Method, 395 video modeling and manipulation, 405
Spreading activation, 93 Token economy, 29, 30
Squiggle game, 313 Tourette syndrome
Strategy utilization deficiency, 89 A-TAC, 248
Strength of evidence (SOE), 72 attention, 247
Stress basal ganglia model, 245
cognitive vulnerabilities, 163 BOLD activity, 247
genetic factors, 163 CBIT, 249
interpersonal, 163 characteristics, 242
life stressor, 163 clinical assessment, 247
youth depression, 163 diagnostic criteria and features, 242
Student accommodations DSM-5, 242
alternate presentation and responses, 116, 117 fMRI study, 246, 247
chunking and scheduled breaks, 117 HRT techniques, 249
extended time, 116 hyperactivation, 247
preferential/limited distraction setting, 116 instruments, 248
student’s 504 plan/IEP, 116 motor tics, 242
Subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC), 165 pathophysiology, 246
Supplementary motor area (SMA), 246 premonitory sensations and urges, 243
Supportive psychotherapy (SPT), 355 psychometric instruments, 247
Swallowing dysfunction, 368 stress and anxiety, 243–244
Index 429

STSS, 247 school-based interventions, 129


treatment, 248 Think Aloud program, 130
TS/CTD, 242 pica (see Pica)
Tourette Syndrome Clinical Global Impression Trial of Outcome for Child and Adolescent Anorexia
(TS-CGI), 247 Nervosa (TOuCAN), 351
Tourette’s Disorder Scale (TODS), 247
Trauma-Focused Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
(TF-CBT), 48 U
Treatment of Resistant Depression in Adolescents UCLA Young Autism Project, 141
(TORDIA), 46 Urology, 408
Treatment
ADHD
adolescents, 132 V
behavioral-based school program, 131 Videoconferencing, 118
children, 128, 132 Vineland Social Maturity Scale, 2
CLAS program, 131 Voiding behavior modification, 410
clinicians, 128
community comparison group, 130
comorbid disorders, 130 W
conventional approach, 131 Working memory (WM), 91
guidelines, 128
HOPS, 131
hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms, 132 Y
instructional methods, 132 Yale Global Tic Severity Scale
issues, 133, 134 (YGTSS), 247
methylphenidate, 129, 131 Years lived with disability (YLDs), 14
noncontingent reinforcement, 129
noncore symptoms of, 129
psychiatry and psychology, 129, 130 Z
school personnel and parents, 128 Ziprasidone, 152

You might also like