You are on page 1of 627

Autism and Child Psychopathology Series

Series Editor: Johnny L. Matson

Justin B. Leaf
Joseph H. Cihon
Julia L. Ferguson
Mary Jane Weiss
Editors

Handbook of
Applied Behavior
Analysis
Interventions for
Autism
Integrating Research into Practice
Autism and Child Psychopathology
Series

Series Editor
Johnny L. Matson, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Brief Overview
The purpose of this series is to advance knowledge in the broad
multidisciplinary fields of autism and various forms of psychopathology (e.g.,
anxiety and depression). Volumes synthesize research on a range of rapidly
expanding topics on assessment, treatment, and etiology.
Description
The Autism and Child Psychopathology Series explores a wide range of
research and professional methods, procedures, and theories used to enhance
positive development and outcomes across the lifespan. Developments in
education, medicine, psychology, and applied behavior analysis as well as
child and adolescent development across home, school, hospital, and
community settings are the focus of this series. Series volumes are both
authored and edited, and they provide critical reviews of evidence-based
methods. As such, these books serve as a critical reference source for
researchers and professionals who deal with developmental disorders and
disabilities, most notably autism, intellectual disabilities, challenging
behaviors, anxiety, depression, ADHD, developmental coordination disorder,
communication disorders, and other common childhood problems. The
series addresses important mental health and development difficulties that
children and youth, their caregivers, and the professionals who treat them
must face. Each volume in the series provides an analysis of methods and
procedures that may assist in effectively treating these developmental
problems.
Justin B. Leaf  •  Joseph H. Cihon
Julia L. Ferguson  •  Mary Jane Weiss
Editors

Handbook of Applied
Behavior Analysis
Interventions for Autism
Integrating Research into Practice
Editors
Justin B. Leaf Joseph H. Cihon
Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal
Beach, California, USA Beach, California, USA 
School of Education, Endicott College School of Education, Endicott College
Beverly, MA, USA Beverly, MA, USA

Julia L. Ferguson Mary Jane Weiss


Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal School of Education
Beach, California, USA Endicott College
School of Education, Endicott College Beverly, MA, USA
Beverly, MA, USA

ISSN 2192-922X     ISSN 2192-9238 (electronic)


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series
ISBN 978-3-030-96477-1    ISBN 978-3-030-96478-8 (eBook)
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8

© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature
Switzerland AG 2022
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the Publisher,
whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation,
reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any
other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation,
computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Dedicated to science, the scientific method, and continually
seeking knowledge through the scientific process.
Preface

The number of autistics/individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder


(ASD) is on the rise. Recent prevalence rates of a child receiving a diagnosis
of ASD are estimated at 1 out of every 54 children living in the United States
with similar prevalence rates internationally. Autistics/individuals diagnosed
with ASD display social communication deficits and engage in repetitive or
restrictive behaviors. Additionally, autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD
may display deficits in cognition, play, and adaptive behaviors and display
aggression or self-injury. Many autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD
require access to quality, effective intervention to develop important, adaptive
behaviors and decrease undesired behaviors. This quality intervention is
comprehensive, individualized, compassionate, progressive, and based on the
best available evidence.
This handbook was developed to provide valuable information on
evidence-­based practices for professionals in the field of behavior analysis
and autism intervention, parents/caregivers of autistic individuals, and autis-
tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. We hope that it serves as a resource for
finding information about interventions likely to be effective and the process
of sorting scientifically supported procedures from procedures that do not
have evidence or merit. The first part consists of eight chapters which provide
an overview of the terms evidence-based and non-evidence-based and the
conditions that may lead one to select an evidence-based or non-evidence-­
based procedure. The second part consists of 22 chapters highlighting several
approaches, procedures, and interventions that are considered to be evidence
based for autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. We wish to thank all the
authors who contributed to this book, without whom this book would not
have been possible.

Beverly, MA, USA Justin B. Leaf


December 2021 Joseph H. Cihon
 Julia L. Ferguson
 Mary Jane Weiss

vii
Contents

1 Introduction to the Handbook of Applied Behavior


Analysis Interventions for Autism��������������������������������������������������   1
Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon, Julia L. Ferguson,
and Mary Jane Weiss

Part I An Overview of Evidence-Based Practice

2 Defining Evidence-Based Practice in the Context


of Applied Behavior Analysis and Autism Intervention����������������  11
Julia L. Ferguson, Mary Jane Weiss, Joseph H. Cihon,
and Justin B. Leaf
3 History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices
for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD����������������������������������������������  23
Elizabeth M. Kryszak and James A. Mulick
4 Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices����������  47
Videsha G. Marya, Victoria D. Suarez, and David J. Cox
5 Evidence-Based Practices for Students with Autism
Spectrum Disorder and the Individuals with Disabilities
Education Improvement Act ����������������������������������������������������������  71
Melissa L. Olive
6 Evidence-Based Practice in Schools ����������������������������������������������  81
Ilene S. Schwartz, Alice Bravo, Robin Finlayson,
Jessica Flaherty, and Adriana Luna
7 Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement
Evidence-­Based Procedures������������������������������������������������������������  99
Thomas Zane, Robin M. Kuhn, Samantha R. Volpe,
Mariah Mussetter, and Jessica F. Juanico
8 Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism
Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model ���������������������������������� 125
Mary Jane Weiss, Lisa Tereshko, Kristin Bowman,
Kimberly Marshall, and Karen Rose

ix
x Contents

9 Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive


Evidence-­Based Practice ���������������������������������������������������������������� 151
Shahla Alai-Rosales, Malika Pritchett, April Linden,
Isabel Cunningham, and Noor Syed

Part II Evidence-Based Practices in Autism Intervention

10 Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward a Progressive Model ���������������� 171


Justin B. Leaf, Julia L. Ferguson, and Joseph H. Cihon
11 Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings
Through Recent Innovations���������������������������������������������������������� 185
Gail G. McGee
12 Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings
Over 30 Years������������������������������������������������������������������������������������ 207
Lynn Kern Koegel, Elizabeth Ponder, Katie Stolen Nordlund,
and Brittany L. Koegel
13 Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals
with Autism Spectrum Disorder ���������������������������������������������������� 227
Ruth M. DeBar, Courtney L. Kane, and Jessica L. Amador
14 Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure
and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills
for Individuals with Autism: An Evidence-Based Approach�������� 251
Ashley N. Creem, Sacha K. G. Shaw, Callie Plattner,
and Jennifer Posey
15 Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral
Intervention for Individuals with Autism�������������������������������������� 267
Christine M. Milne and Ashley Creem
16 Parent Implementation Interventions�������������������������������������������� 289
Sarely Licona, Lauren Bush, Victoria Chavez, Emily Dillon,
and Allison L. Wainer
17 Overview of the Early Start Denver Model ���������������������������������� 317
Melissa Mello and Sally J. Rogers
18 PEAK Relational Training System ������������������������������������������������ 341
Mark R. Dixon, Zhihui Yi, and Amanda N. Chastain
19 The Picture Exchange Communication System���������������������������� 361
Rocío Rosales and Yaimarili Marin-Avelino
20 Augmentative and Alternative Communication
(AAC) Systems���������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 375
Hayley Neimy and Brenda Fossett
21 Shaping: A Brief History, Research Overview,
and Recommendations�������������������������������������������������������������������� 403
Joseph H. Cihon
Contents xi

22 Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices


and Considerations�������������������������������������������������������������������������� 417
Claudia L. Dozier, Adam M. Briggs, Kathleen M. Holehan,
Nicole A. Kanaman, and Jessica F. Juanico
23 Practical Functional Assessment���������������������������������������������������� 443
Joshua Jessel
24 Treating Problem Behaviors Through Functional
Communication Training���������������������������������������������������������������� 465
Faris R. Kronfli, Courtney Butler, Christeen Zaki-­Scarpa,
and SungWoo Kahng
25 Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement���������������������� 479
Ashley Bagwell, Monique Barnett, and Terry S. Falcomata
26 The Token Economy������������������������������������������������������������������������ 497
Patrick M. Ghezzi and Ainsley B. Lewon
27 Activity Schedules and Script-­Fading Procedures:
Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism
Independence and Social Interaction Skills���������������������������������� 513
Amanda S. Freeman, Christine M. Fry, and
Gregory S. MacDuff
28 Extinction and Differential Reinforcement ���������������������������������� 539
Timothy R. Vollmer, Janelle K. Bacotti,
and Lindsay A. Lloveras
29 Response Interruption and Redirection���������������������������������������� 555
Catia Cividini-Motta, Hannah MacNaul,
Haley M. K. Steinhauser, and William H. Ahearn
30 Building Independence: Self-­Management
for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder�������������������������� 577
Kimberly B. Marshall and Jessica L. Rohrer
31 Evidence-Based Practices: What Does the Future Hold?������������ 603
Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon, Julia L. Ferguson,
and Mary Jane Weiss

Index��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������  611
Introduction to the Handbook
of Applied Behavior Analysis 1
Interventions for Autism

Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon , Julia L. Ferguson ,


and Mary Jane Weiss

1.1 Introduction Throughout the years, the diagnostic criterion


to the Handbook of Applied of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has changed
Behavior Analysis (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 2000,
Interventions for Autism 2013), yet the hallmarks of the disorder have
remained relatively consistent. That is, individu-
In 1938, Leo Kanner began the study of 11 chil- als diagnosed with ASD commonly have qualita-
dren who displayed similar behaviors that would tive impairments in reciprocal
later be described in his 1943 article entitled, social-communication interaction (e.g., joint
“Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact.” attention, friendship development, tolerating oth-
Specifically, Kanner (1943) detailed descriptions ers) and engage in restricted or repetitive behav-
from the parents of the 11 children including ior (e.g., hand flapping, scripting, body rocking).
birth conditions, observations from the clinic, In addition to these deficits, it is common for
any available case history, and common themes individuals diagnosed with ASD to have qualita-
across the 11 children. Following this in-depth tive impairments in language and communication
description, Kanner made the case for distin- (Matson et  al., 2013), engage in challenging or
guishing between schizophrenia and what would dangerous behavior (e.g., aggression, self-­
later be called autism (e.g., onset of symptoms, injurious behavior; Jang et  al., 2011), and have
impervious to people, rigidities). As a result, deficits in self-help/leisure skills (Flynn & Healy,
Kanner is often credited for being the first to cod- 2012). Recent prevalence research from the
ify the concept of autism as a syndrome and not a Center for Disease Control and Prevention has
symptom of some other disorder (e.g., childhood indicated that the number of diagnoses has
schizophrenia). increased across the years with 1 out of every 54
children living in the United States receiving an
ASD diagnosis (Baio et al., 2018), which has also
J. B. Leaf (*) · J. H. Cihon · J. L. Ferguson been reported globally (Christensen et al., 2016).
Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, With prevalence rates of ASD increasing, it is
USA more imperative than ever that access to quality
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA, intervention is widely available for those that
USA need it. This intervention should be comprehen-
e-mail: jbleaf@APFmail.org sive, individualized, compassionate, progressive,
M. J. Weiss and based on the best available evidence. This is
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA, a common value among behavior analysts that
USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 1


J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_1
2 J. B. Leaf et al.

find themselves working in the fields of behav- areas in which skill development is necessary
ioral intervention and autism. As Van Houten that requires intensive intervention (e.g., Lovaas,
et  al. (1988) stated, “behavior analysts have an 1987). As such, implementing interventions with
obligation to only use techniques that have been little to no effectiveness may limit the number of
demonstrated by research to be effective, to skills that could be developed using otherwise
acquaint consumers and the public with the effective, evidence-based interventions.
advantages and disadvantages of these tech- Relatedly, a second potential risk in implement-
niques, and to search continuously for the most ing non-evidenced based interventions is the pos-
optimal means of changing behavior” (p.  383). sibility of diluting or negating the effectiveness
Although Van Houten and colleagues were of an evidence-based procedure. This is likely to
describing the right to effective treatment pro- be the case with non-evidenced-based interven-
vided by behavior analysts, we believe that autis- tions that are based upon competing ideologies or
tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD deserve the philosophies of behavior (e.g., opposed perspec-
right to effective treatment by any helping profes- tives on the nature of behavior; Kimberly et al.,
sional (e.g., occupational therapist, speech lan- 2016). These competing ideologies or philoso-
guage pathologist, psychologist). phies of behavior are likely to lead to different
Unfortunately, the services available for autis- approaches of when, how, and if to address vari-
tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD have long ous behaviors or skillsets. With two, or more, dif-
been delivered along with non-evidence-based ferent philosophical approaches to treatment, it is
procedures (e.g., chelation therapy; Singer & probable that the effects of an evidence-based
Ravi, 2015), interventions with a lack of or lim- intervention may be diluted or, worse, negated.
ited empirical support (e.g., Social Thinking™; A third potential risk of implementing a non-­
Leaf et al., 2018), interventions which would be evidence-­based practice is the cost for the con-
considered pseudoscientific (e.g., Son-Rise sumer (Zane et al., 2008). It is not uncommon for
Program®; Moran, 2014), and antiscientific (e.g., pseudoscientific interventions to cost an exorbi-
facilitated communication; Lillenfeld et  al., tant about of money, despite a lack of effective-
2014). As Normand (2008) stated, “one would be ness, yet the promise of effectiveness with little
hard pressed to find an area more widely affected effort keeps consumers dedicated, continually
by rampant pseudoscience than that of autism wasting time and money. The costs associated
treatment” (p.  42). Perhaps even more unfortu- with these interventions are not only monetary, as
nate is that even in 2022 these interventions are they may also take an enormous emotional toll on
still promoted, disseminated, and implemented as consumers and their families. Astronomical
effective treatments for autistics/individuals promises of effectiveness such as effectively
diagnosed with ASD (Lerman et  al., 2008; communicating, developing meaningful relation-
National Autism Center, 2015). ships, an immediate end to any and all problem-
Although these non-evidence-based proce- atic/dangerous behavior, and living a “normal”
dures often make promises of effectiveness with life create expectations that are never met. Across
little to no effort, and in some cases a “cure,” the time, the realization sets in that these expecta-
reality greatly differs (Zane et al., 2008). In fact, tions were promised in malice taking almost
many of these non-evidence-based procedures, or immeasurable emotional toll on consumers and
fad treatments (Zane et al., 2008), often put the their families. This emotional toll can be devas-
autistic/individual diagnosed with ASD in imme- tating and could even result in depression
diate and long-term risk of harm (Freeman, (Maurice, 1993). It can also reduce the likelihood
2008). One potential risk involved in the imple- of hope for the future effectiveness of evidence-­
mentation of these procedures is that doing so based interventions and may make caregivers and
may take away valuable time from more effec- individuals impacted hesitant to embrace treat-
tive, evidence-based interventions. Autistics/indi- ment in the future.
viduals diagnosed with ASD may have multiple
1  Introduction to the Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism 3

A fourth potential risk relates to the limited Researchers and professionals have also
research on the possible long-term side effects of helped to evaluate the evidence and quality of
non-evidence-based procedures. It may be possi- research supporting the effectiveness of an inter-
ble that the known short-term effects of the use of vention or procedure through the publication of
non-evidence-based procedures are continually literature reviews (e.g., Park et  al., 2019) and
exacerbated. Is it possible that non-evidence-­ meta-analysis (e.g., Eldevik et al., 2009). These
based procedures lead to increased reports of reviews and/or meta-analysis commonly involve
anxiety, depression, or post-traumatic stress an examination of the conceptual underpinnings,
symptoms? Is it possible that non-evidence-based theory, and/or data behind any given procedure.
procedures lead to harm in an individual’s physi- For example, there have been several literature
cal or medical health? Is it possible that exposure reviews related to Social Stories™ that have
to ineffective intervention(s) over time, with the resulted in warnings against their use due to weak
concomitant reduction in hope, leads to lowered empirical evidence and methodological rigor
investment in and enthusiasm for intervention? (e.g., Leaf et  al., 2015; Milne et  al., 2020;
Without additional research on the long-term side Reynhout & Carter, 2011; Styles, 2011).
effects of non-evidence-based procedures, these One of the most substantial contributions to
questions will remain unanswered. While the assist with identifying the evidence to support the
research on the long-term side effects of non-­ use of an intervention or procedure has been the
evidence-­ based procedures is lacking, the creation of standards projects which evaluate a
research is clear that eclectic approaches that plethora of interventions (e.g., National Autism
include non-evidence-based procedures are inef- Center, 2009, 2015). These projects are typically
fective or less effective than the use of solely done with the help of many professionals. They
evidence-based procedures (Howard et al., 2014). create a set of criteria to determine if an interven-
Due to the continued growth, popularity, and tion would be considered evidence based and
proliferation of non-evidence-based procedures conduct searches in a systematic and method-
for autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD, the ological manner. The conclusion of this process
increase in the number of ASD diagnoses, the usually results in a list of interventions which
need for quality and effective intervention, and would be considered evidence based and a list of
the risks of implementing non-evidence-based interventions which would not be considered evi-
intervention many professionals have discussed dence based. These reports are immensely help-
non-evidence-based practices within scholarly ful for consumers to identify which interventions
works. For example, Horner et  al. (2005) pro- to implement and which interventions to avoid.
vided parameters to determine if an intervention Although there have been numerous reviews
would be considered evidence based which (e.g., Odom et  al., 2010), chapters (DiGennaro
included: (a) the practice being clearly defined, Reed et  al., 2017), books (e.g., Reichow et  al.,
(b) the outcomes of the practice being defined, 2011), and standards projects (e.g., National
(c) the use of treatment fidelity, (d) a functionally Autism Center, , 2015) dedicated to identifying
related change, (e) the intervention being imple- and listing evidence-based practices as it relates
mented and demonstrated to be effective across to autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD, the
five studies, (f) the intervention being conducted continual proliferation of non-evidence-based
by at a least three different research labs, and (g) practices and emerging research makes it crucial
the intervention being evaluated across at least 20 to continue to publish materials about evidence-­
participants. Additionally, numerous chapters based and non-evidence-based procedures. As
exist to help identify the quality of an interven- such, the editors of this handbook sought to
tion explored within a specific study. For exam- develop a book in which leaders in the field of
ple, DiGennaro Reed et  al. (2017) provided autism and behavior analysis were invited to
professionals with a checklist/questionnaire of write chapters on various topics related to
how to determine the quality of any intervention. evidence-­based practices and autistics/individu-
4 J. B. Leaf et al.

als diagnosed with ASD.  Our hope is that this Thomas Zane, Robin M.  Kuhn, Samantha
book will function as a guide for practitioners in R.  Volpe, Mariah Mussetter, and Jessica
the selection and implementation of procedures F. Juanico and discusses the reasons why a pro-
based on their research evidence and ­effectiveness, fessional may choose to implement evidence-­
rather than unsubstantiated claims. As such, this based or non-evidence-based practices. Chapter 8
handbook consists of two parts: (I) an overview was written by Mary Jane Weiss, Lisa Tereshko,
of evidence-based practices and (II) procedures Kristin Bowman, Kimberly Marshall, and Karen
and interventions which would be considered Rose who outline how to work collaboratively
evidence based. with other professionals when it comes to imple-
menting evidence-based practices and how to
manage circumstances when other team mem-
1.2  art I: An Overview
P bers or families recommend the use of non-­
of Evidence-Based Practice evidence-­based procedures. The final chapter in
the first part of the book is Chap. 7, written by
The first part is meant to provide a discussion of Shahla Ala’i-Rosales, Malika Pritchett, April
what we mean when using the terms evidence Linden, Isabel Cunningham, and Noor Syed.
based and non-evidence based and the conditions This chapter discusses important cultural consid-
that may lead one to select an evidence-based or erations within the context of evidence-based
non-evidence-based procedure. The second chap- practices.
ter of this handbook was written by the editors
who operationally define what constitutes an
evidence-based practice, provide detail on the 1.3  art II: Evidence-Based
P
three criteria of evidence-based practices, and Practices in Autism
compare and contrast how evidence-based prac- Intervention
tices have been evaluated and defined from other
resources and entities. The third chapter was The second part of this handbook highlights sev-
written by Elizabeth M.  Kryszak and James eral approaches, procedures, and interventions
A.  Mulick who describe the history of non-­ that are considered to be evidence based. These
evidence-­based procedures that have been imple- chapters are the bulk of the content of this hand-
mented for autistics/individuals diagnosed with book. Each chapter discusses how the approach,
ASD and why implementing non-evidence-based procedure, or interventions meet standards devel-
practices may be harmful. Chapter 4 was written oped to be considered evidence based. This part
by Videsha G.  Marya, Victoria D.  Suarez, and includes a chapter on discrete trial teaching
David J.  Cox who discussed various decision (DTT) written by Justin B.  Leaf, Julia
models on how to proceed when making ethical L. Ferguson, and Joseph H. Cihon that describes
decisions about interventions. The fifth chapter DTT, the research that has been conducted using
was written by Melissa Olive and provides the DTT to teach various skills, and how DTT meets
context for how evidence-based practices fit into the standards of an evidence-based practice. Gail
the law. Chapter 6 was written by Ilene McGee provided a chapter on incidental teaching
S.  Schwartz, Alice Bravo, Robin Finlayson, that describes some of the research behind inci-
Jessica Flaherty, and Adriana Luna who provided dental teaching as well as future areas of research.
the context on how evidence-based practices fit Lynn Kern Koegel, Elizabeth Ponder, Katie
into the Individuals with Disabilities Education Stolen Nordlund, and Brittany L. Koegel wrote a
Improvement Act, how evidence-based practices chapter discussing pivotal response training
fit into services within a school system, and how (PRT), the research that has been conducted on
evidence-based practice should be implemented PRT to improve behavior, the strengths and limi-
as part of the Individualized Education Program tations found in the research, areas in need of
process. The seventh chapter was written by future research, and clinical implications. Ruth
1  Introduction to the Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism 5

M.  DeBar, Courtney L.  Kane, and Jessica shaping, and research and clinical recommenda-
L.  Amador provided a chapter describing video tions related to the use of shaping. Claudia
modeling and the different variations that are L.  Dozier, Adam M.  Briggs, Kathleen
commonly implemented and evaluated the M.  Holehan, Nicole A.  Kanaman, and Jessica
research, future directions of the research, and F.  Juanico wrote a chapter on the use of func-
clinical implications related to video modeling. tional analysis methodology that described best
Ashley Creem, Sacha Shaw, Callie Plattner, and practice considerations related to the use of func-
Jennifer Posey wrote a chapter dedicated to the tional analyses.
teaching interaction procedure (TIP) and behav- Joshua Jessel provided a chapter on the use of
ioral skills training (BST). In this chapter the the practical functional assessment that outlines
authors examined the evidence base for both pro- the research, to date, on its use as well as clinical
cedures, highlighted the similarities and differ- recommendations when considering the use of a
ences between the two procedures, provided practical functional assessment. Faris Kronfli,
areas of need in the literature, described how Courtney Butler, Christeen Zaki-Scarpa, and
research can relate to practice, and how TIP and SungWoo Kahng wrote a chapter discussing the
BST meet the definition of an evidence-based use of functional communication training (FCT)
practice. Christine M. Milne and Ashley Creem to teach replacement behaviors. Ashley Bagwell,
provided a chapter on social skills groups that Monique Barnett, and Terry S.  Falcomata pro-
described the research and provided clinical rec- vided a chapter on the use of time-out and
ommendations for implementing social skills response cost in which authors evaluated the
groups. research on time-out and response cost as well as
Sarely Licona, Lauren Bush, Victoria Chavez, discussing ethical and clinical considerations
Emily Dillon, and Allison Wainer wrote a chapter with their use. Patrick M.  Ghezzi and Ainsley
evaluating the research related to parent-­mediated B.  Lewon wrote a chapter that discusses the
interventions that described the research and pro- research and, more importantly, the ethical con-
vided clinical recommendations. Melissa Mello siderations related to the use of token economies.
and Sally J.  Rogers provided a chapter on the Amanda S.  Freeman, Christine M.  Fry, and
Early Start Denver Model (ESDM). In this chap- Gregory S.  MacDuff provided a chapter on the
ter, the authors evaluated the research on ESDM, use of activity schedules and script fading that
provided clinical recommendations, and sug- evaluated the research for these two procedures,
gested areas for future research. Mark Dixon, highlighted areas for future research, and pro-
Zhihui Yi, and Amanda N.  Chastain provided a vided clinical implications and best practices for
chapter on PEAK that described the background their integration into an ABA approach. Timothy
of PEAK, the research on PEAK, and clinical R.  Vollmer, Janelle K.  Bacotti, and Lindsay
recommendations. Rocío Rosales and Yaimarili A. Lloveras wrote a chapter on the use of differ-
Marin-Avelino wrote a chapter on the Picture ential reinforcement and extinction. The authors
Exchange Communication System (PECS) that evaluated the relevant research, noted areas for
described the research related to PECS and pro- future research, and provided clinical implica-
vided several clinical recommendations related tions. Catia Cividini-Motta, Hannah MacNaul,
to its use. Hayley Neimy and Brenda Fossett pro- Haley M. K. Steinhauser, and William H. Ahearn
vided a chapter on Augmentative and Alternative wrote a chapter that discussed the use of response
Communication (AAC) that included a descrip- interruption and redirection (RIRD) which
tion of the research on AAC, assessment consid- included relevant research and ethical and clini-
erations for the use of AAC, and several clinical cal considerations with the use of RIRD. Kimberly
recommendations when considering the use of B.  Marshall and Jessica L.  Rohrer provided a
AAC. Joseph H. Cihon provided a chapter on the chapter that evaluated the use of self-­management
use of shaping that provided a brief history of the and monitoring procedures for autistics/individu-
discovery of shaping, the research related to als diagnosed with ASD. The authors highlighted
6 J. B. Leaf et al.

the relevant research, discussed research and Lee, L.  C., Harrington, R., Lopez, M., Fitzgerald,
R. T., Hewitt, A., … Dowling, N. F. (2018). Prevalence
clinical implications, and described how self-­ of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8
monitoring and self-management meet the defini- years-autism and developmental disabilities monitor-
tion of an evidence-based practice. Finally, the ing network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. Morbidity
editors provided a chapter that summarized and and Mortality Weekly Report: Surveillance Summaries,
67(6), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1
provided a general overview of the state of Christensen, D. L., Baio, J., Braun, K. V. N., Bilder, D.,
evidence-­based practices in the field of Charles, J., Constantino, J.  N., Daniels, J., Durkin,
ASD.  Overall strengths, future research direc- M.  S., Fitzgerald, R.  T., Kurzius-Spencer, M., Lee,
tions, and clinical needs are discussed. L. C., Pettygrove, S., Robinson, C., Schulz, E., Wells,
C., Wingate, M. S., Zahorodny, W., & Yeargin-Allsopp,
M. (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism
spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—
1.4 Conclusion Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring
network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. Morbidity and
Mortality Weekly Report: Surveillance Summaries,
Evidence-based intervention is crucial to achiev- 65(3), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6503a1
ing meaningful outcomes for autistics/individu- DiGennaro Reed, F.  D., Novak, M.  D., Henley, A.  J.,
als diagnosed with ASD. Time, money, and hope Brand, D., & McDonald, M. E. (2017). Evidence based
are wasted on interventions that offer promise but interventions. In J. B. Leaf (Ed.), Handbook of social
skills and autism spectrum disorder (pp.  139–153).
do not deliver results. In this book, we explore Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­62995-­7
the foundational need for evidence-based prac- Eldevik, S., Hastings, R.  P., Hughes, J.  C., Jahr, E.,
tices, how these practices are vital to outcomes, Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2009). Meta-analyssi
and how they are woven into effective treatment, of early intensive behavioral intervention for chil-
dren with autism. Journal of Clinical Child and
along with strong collaboration and cultural Adolescent Psychology, 38(3), 439–450. https://doi.
humility. We also discuss state-of-the-art imple- org/10.1080/15374410902851739
mentation of a variety of evidence-based inter- Flynn, L., & Healy, O. (2012). A review of treatments for
ventions for autistics/individuals diagnosed with deficits in social skills and self-help skills in autism
spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum
ASD and provide suggestions for ensuring their Disorder, 6(1), 431–441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
effective implementation. It is our hope that this rasd.2011.06.016
book serves as a guide for the consideration of Freeman, B. J. (2008). Alternative treatments for autism
intervention approaches and assists clinicians in spectrum disorders: What is the science? In R. Leaf,
J. McEachin, & M. Taubman (Eds.), Sense and non-
providing maximally effective, empirically sup- sense in the behavioral treatment of autism. New York,
ported, individualized treatment plans to their NY. Different Roads to Learning. 
clients with autism. Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom,
S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject
research to identify evidence-based practice in spe-
cial education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 165–179.
References https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203
Howard, J. S., Stanislaw, H., Green, G., Sparkman, C. R.,
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic & Cohen, H.  G. (2014). Comparison of behavior
and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). analytic and eclectic early interventions for young
Washington, DC. American Psychiatric Association. children with autism after three years. Research in
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic Developmental Disabilities, 35(12), 3326–3344.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.021
text rev.). Washington, DC.  American Psychiatric Jang, J., Dixon, D.  R., Tarbox, J., & Granpeesheh, D.
Association. (2011). Symptom severity and challenging behavior
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic in children with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Disorders, 5(3), 1028–1032. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 rasd.2010.11.008
Baio, J., Wiggins, L., Christensen, D.  L., Maenner, Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con-
M.  J., Daniels, J., Warren, Z., Kurzius-Spencer, M., tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
Zahorodny, W., Rosenberg, C. R., White, T., Durkin, Kimberly A., Schreck Y., Karunaratne T., Zane Heather,
M.  S., Imm, P., Nikolaou, L., Yeargin-Allsopp, M., W. (2016). Behavior Analysts’ Use of and Beliefs
1  Introduction to the Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism 7

in Treatments for People with Autism: A 5-Year Rise Program® intervention effect. Science in Autism
Follow-up. Behavioral Interventions 31(4), 355–376. Treatment, 11(1), 23–24.
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1461 National Autism Center. (2009). Findings and conclu-
Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Taubman, M., sions: National standards project, phase 1. Randolph,
Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (2018). Social thinking®, MA. Author.
pseudoscientific, not empirically supported, and National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu-
non-evidence based: A reply to Crooke and winner. sions: National standards project, phase 2. Randolph,
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11(4), 456–466. https:// MA. Author.
doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0241-­0 Normand, M. P. (2008). Science, skepticism, and applied
Leaf, J. B., Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L., Leaf, R., Taubman, behavior analysis. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(2),
M., McEachin, J., Parker, T., Waks, A. B., & Mountjoy, 42–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391727
T. (2015). What is the proof now? A methodologi- Odom, S.  L., Collet-Klingenberg, L., Rogers, S.  J., &
cal review of studies that have utilized social stories. Hatton, D.  D. (2010). Evidence-based practices in
Education and Training in Autism and Developmental interventions for children and youth with autism spec-
Disorders, 50(2), 127–141. trum disorders. Preventing School Failure: Alternative
Lerman, D.  C., Sansbury, T., Hovanetz, A., Wolever, Education for Children and Youth, 54(4), 275–282.
E., Garcia, A., O’Brien, E., & Adedipe, H. (2008). https://doi.org/10.1080/10459881003785506
Using behavior analysis to examine the outcomes Park, J., Bouck, E., & Duenas, A. (2019). The effect
of unproven therapies: An evaluation of hyperbaric of video modeling and video prompting interven-
oxygen therapy for children with autism. Behavior tions on individuals with intellectual disability:
Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 50–58. https://doi. A systematic literature review. Journal of Special
org/10.1007/BF03391728 Education Technology, 34(1), 3–16. https://doi.
Lillenfeld, S.  O., Marshall, J.  M., Todd, J.  T., & Shane, org/10.1177/0162643418780464
H. C. (2014). The persistence of fad treatments in the Reichow, B., Doehring, P., Cicchetti, D.  V., & Volkmar,
face of negative scientific evidence: Facilitated com- F. R. (2011). Evidence-based practices and treatments
munication for autism as a case example. Evidence-­ for children with autism. Springer.
Based Communication Assessment and Intervention, Reynhout, G., & Carter, M. (2011). Evaluation of the
8(2), 62–101. https://doi.org/10.1080/17489539.2014 efficacy of social stories™ using three single subject
.976332 metrics. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(2),
Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor- 885–900. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.10.003
mal educational and intellectual functioning in Singer, A., & Ravi, R. (2015). Complementary and
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting alternative treatments for autism part 2: Identifying
and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi. and avoiding non-evidence-based treatments. AMA
org/10.1037/0022-­006X.55.1.3 Journal of Ethics, 17(4), 375–380. https://doi.
Matson, J.  L., Hess, J.  A., & Mahan, S. (2013). org/10.1001/journalofethics.2015.17.4.sect2-­1504
Moderating effects of challenging behaviors and com- Styles, A. (2011). Social stories™: Does the research evi-
munication deficits on social skills in children diag- dence support the popularity? Educational Psychology
nosed with an autism spectrum disorder. Research in in Practice, 27(4), 415–436. https://doi.org/10.1080/0
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(1), 23–28. https://doi. 2667363.2011.624312
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.07.002 Van Houten, R., Axelrod, S., Bailey, J.  S., Favell, J.  E.,
Maurice, C. (1993). Let me hear your voice: A family’s Foxx, R.  M., Iwata, B.  A., & Lovaas, O.  I. (1988).
triumph over autism. New York, NY. Knopf. The right to effective behavioral treatment. Journal
Milne, C.  M., Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21(4), 381–384. https://
McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2020). What is the proof doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1988.21-­381
now? An updated methodological review of research Zane, T., Davis, C., & Rosswurm, M. (2008). The cost
on social stories. Education and Training in Autism of fad treatments in autism. Journal of Early and
and Developmental Disabilities, 55(3), 264–276. Intensive Behavior Intervention, 5(2), 44–51. https://
Moran, K. (2014). Review of: Promoting child-initiated doi.org/10.1037/h0100418
social communication in children with autism: Son-­
Part I
An Overview of Evidence-Based Practice
Defining Evidence-Based Practice
in the Context of Applied Behavior 2
Analysis and Autism Intervention

Julia L. Ferguson , Mary Jane Weiss,
Joseph H. Cihon , and Justin B. Leaf

2.1 Defining Evidence-Based tion of resources to interventions likely to effect


Practice in the Context change.
of Applied Behavior Analysis Within the field of psychology, evidence-­
and Autism Intervention based practice has been highlighted for decades
(e.g., American Psychological Association, 2005;
2.1.1 T
 he Quest to Identify Levant, 2005). Task forces within the field of
Evidence-Based Practice psychology have defined evidence-based prac-
tices and empirically supported treatments by
Evidence-based practice is frequently cited as a focusing on the breadth of evidence (e.g., number
value and a goal across professions (e.g., of studies, quality of data, research design), the
American Psychological Association, 2005; independence of published researchers, the man-
American Speech-Language Hearing ualization of the approach, the specificity of
Association, 2021; Behavior Analyst Certification treatment, and the effectiveness of treatment
Board, 2020; Institute of Medicine, 2001; (American Psychological Association
National Association of Social Workers). It is the Presidential Task Force of Evidence-Based
foundation of effective treatment and is associ- Practice, 2006). Each of these factors is relevant
ated with maximizing outcomes of an interven- to confidence in the findings, consistency of
tion (e.g., Howard et  al., 2014). In the helping results, and implications for practice. These
professions, evidence-based practice has been themes have remained and continue to be empha-
emphasized for several decades (e.g., Levant, sized in analyses of existing approaches to inter-
2005), as there has been an increased sensitivity vention (e.g., American Psychological
to the use of effective procedures and the alloca- Association, n.d.).
Other fields (e.g., American Speech-­
Language-­Hearing Association, applied behavior
J. L. Ferguson (*) · J. H. Cihon · J. B. Leaf analysis, American Occupational Therapy
Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, Association) have similarly articulated strong
USA values on evidence-based practice and have
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA, worked to provide specific guidelines to practi-
USA tioners for the use of such procedures. The medi-
e-mail: JFerguson@APFmail.org cal field has characterized evidence-based
M. J. Weiss practice as the integration of research-based pro-
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA, cedures with clinical expertise and patient values
USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 11


J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_2
12 J. L. Ferguson et al.

(Institute of Medicine, 2001). Within social work, This qualitative element has become increas-
there is an emphasis on locating and evaluating ingly emphasized in terms of evidence-based
evidence applicable to an issue, applying the evi- practice (i.e., incorporation of client input and
dence to implement a solution, and then evaluat- sensitivity to client values). While this has always
ing the success of the solution (Drake et  al., been part of partnering with the client in treat-
2001). There is also an emphasis on integrating ment, it is now more explicitly emphasized in
clinical expertise, client preference, culture, eth- behavior analytic intervention. The new Ethics
ics, and values into the intervention plan (National Code for Behavior Analysts (Behavior Analyst
Association for Social Work, 2021). The Certification Board, 2020) emphasizes that ser-
American Speech-Language-Hearing vices are delivered in the context of core princi-
Association (ASHA) has called for the use of ples, which serve as a framework for
evidence-based procedures, which are seen as compassionate service delivery. The four core
integrating evidence (including research evi- principles are (a) benefit others; (b) treat others
dence and observational data), practitioner exper- with compassion, dignity, and respect; (c) behave
tise, and client/caregiver perspective (American with integrity; and (d) ensure competence. The
Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2021). compassion, dignity, and respect core principle
Finally, the American Occupational Therapy focuses on providing humane care that upholds
Association (AOTA) has called for the use of the dignity and choice of each client. “Behavior
evidence-based practices, emphasizing the inte- analysts respect and actively promote clients’
gration of “critically appraised research results self-determination to the best of their abilities,
with the practitioner’s clinical expertise, and the particularly when providing services to vulnera-
client’s preferences, beliefs, and values” (AOTA, ble populations” (Behavior Analyst Certification
2021, para 1). Board, 2020, p.  4). In addition, the code states
In autism intervention, where fad treatments that behavior analysts acknowledge that “per-
have been a common challenge (Schreck et  al., sonal choice in service delivery is important”
2016; Zane et al., 2008), the need for the identifi- (p. 4) and provide “clients and stakeholders with
cation of evidence-based practices and empiri- needed information to make informed choices
cally supported treatments is high. Clinicians about services” (p.  4). Like other professions,
from multiple disciplines are intensely interested in behavior analysis is working to empower clients
helping practitioners and families select interven- to make informed choices about treatment and
tions with merit. Wasted time reduces outcomes, and recognizes that client input and comfort are of
effective treatment is essential (Zane et al., 2008). paramount importance.
Across disciplines, the addition of clinical
judgment is an interesting element that is often
included. Indeed, clinical judgment is commonly 2.2  ommonly Used Evidence-­
C
emphasized and may at times be at odds with Based Practice Standards
other indices of effectiveness (e.g., client prefer-
ences, caregiver preference). Nevertheless, given Evidence-based practice is generally defined as
the complexity of clinical presentations and that practices that are used based on: (a) the best sci-
experience creates more expertise, clinical judg- entific evidence, (b) the practitioner’s or clini-
ment is generally highly regarded. This integra- cian’s experience and expertise, and (c) client
tion of practitioner judgment does introduce a values (Slocum et  al., 2014). This three-prong
combinatorial effect; that is, research evidence is approach to selecting interventions helps guide
weighed along with clinical judgment to permit clinicians when selecting interventions to help
an individualized decision about the direction of teach skills to the clients with whom we work. In
treatment. Similarly, client values are also high- addition to this general definition of evidence-­
lighted and are accommodated in the selection of based practice, individuals and entities have out-
treatment. lined specific criteria and guidelines that would
2  Defining Evidence-Based Practice in the Context of Applied Behavior Analysis and Autism Intervention 13

qualify interventions to be considered an intervention should specify the conditions under


evidence-­based practice regarding studies using which it should be used, the types of individuals
single-subject research designs and group designs the procedure should be used with, and the types
and in the context of autism service provision. of practitioners or clinicians that are qualified to
Outlined next are some commonly used sources implement the intervention and outline the spe-
to designate if an intervention, practice, or proce- cific outcomes that will be affected by the inter-
dure is considered an evidence-based practice. vention. Third, the body of research supporting
the practice or intervention should have data sup-
porting that the intervention has been imple-
2.2.1 Horner et al. (2005) mented with fidelity across studies. Fourth,
across the body of research, functional control
Horner et al. (2005) outlined criteria to help prac- should be demonstrated between the intervention
titioners and educators determine if an interven- and the corresponding change in the measured
tion is an evidence-based practice when the dependent variable(s). Finally, for a practice or
evidence to support the intervention is evaluated intervention to be considered an evidence-based
through single-subject research designs. Horner practice, their needs to be at least five high-­
et  al. first outlined quality indicators of a pub- quality single subject research design studies
lished research study using single-subject designs published in peer-reviewed journals. Those stud-
across several variables: (a) description of par- ies need to have been conducted by at least three
ticipants and settings, (b) dependent variable, (c) different research groups and across at least three
independent variable, (d) baseline, (e) experi- different geographical locations. Additionally,
mental control/internal validity, (f) external those studies need to include at least 20 partici-
validity, and (g) social validity. Across these vari- pants to be considered an evidence-based prac-
ables, Horner et al. further detailed what to exam- tice (Horner et al., 2005).
ine that would indicate that the study in question
was of high quality. For example, quality indica-
tors for the dependent variable would include a 2.2.2 Gersten et al. (2005)
description with operational definitions and mea-
surement that is valid and quantifiable, dependent Published alongside Horner et  al. (2005) was a
variable is measured repeatedly over time, and corresponding article on quality indicators for
interobserver agreement and reliability data are research using group designs (i.e., Gersten et al.,
collected on the dependent variables (Horner 2005). Similar to Horner et  al., Gersten et  al.
et al., 2005). (2005) outlined quality indicators to examine
After outlining quality indicators of a single-­ when analyzing a research article that utilized a
subject research design study, Horner et  al. group design and outlined guidelines for how to
(2005) indicated how a body of research com- evaluate a body of research using group designs
prised of high-quality single subject designs in the context of evidence-based practice. Gersten
would be analyzed in the context of evidence-­ et  al. outlined quality indicators of a published
based practice. The guidelines Horner and col- research study using a group or quasi-­
leagues provided for considering an intervention experimental design across: (a) description of
to be an evidence-based practice consisted of five participants, (b) implementation of the interven-
criteria. First, the practice or intervention needs tion and description of comparison conditions,
to be operationally defined and described so that (c) outcome measures, and (d) data analysis.
other individuals can replicate the procedures Across these variables, Gersten and colleagues
with fidelity. Second, the intervention or practice posed questions for the reader to analyze with
should specify the context in which the interven- respect to indicators of quality. For example,
tions should be used and the corresponding out- Gersten and colleagues posed questions about
comes. More specifically, the description of the outcome measures regarding if multiple ­measures
14 J. L. Ferguson et al.

were used and if the measures were used at the States’ educational system. The goal of WWC is
appropriate time to capture the intervention’s to assess the available scientific evidence of inter-
effect. After outlining quality indicators for group ventions in order to be the trusted source for
experimental or quasi-experimental research “what works” when it comes to educational inter-
designs, Gersten and colleagues outlined how ventions and practices. To accomplish this goal,
multiple research articles comprised of random- the WWC develops a review protocol, identifies
ized group designs and quasi-experiments on a relevant literature, screens and reviews the eligi-
specific subject should be evaluated in the con- ble studies, and summarizes and reports their
text of evidence-based practice. findings (What Works Clearinghouse, 2014).
When it comes to interventions for children This is a rigorous and detailed process in which
diagnosed with ASD or other developmental dis- each research study is reviewed carefully using
abilities, Gersten et  al. (2005) noted that one specific procedures that apply depending on the
main issue is the extent to which results found research design implemented.
from group designs can be generalized across WWC categorizes a study’s findings into one
individuals diagnosed with ASD with varying of five categories: (a) statistically significant pos-
skill sets and profiles. Gersten et  al. noted that itive effect, (b) substantively important positive
determining if an intervention is evidence based effect, (c) intermediate effect, (d) substantively
is nuanced and will vary depending on the important negative effect, and (e) statistically sig-
research available for each intervention. With nificant negative effect (What Works
that said, Gersten and colleagues did provide Clearinghouse, 2014). When combining multiple
some general criteria for determining if a practice study findings for an intervention brief or report,
is evidence based: (a) an intervention has at least WWC summarizes the data on an intervention
four published studies that are of acceptable across the average improvement index, the statis-
quality or at least two published studies that are tical significance of an effect, the amount of sup-
of high quality, and (b) across those studies the porting evidence, and the generalizability of the
weighted effect size is significantly greater than findings across studies. This level of review and
zero. The definitions of “acceptable quality” and data analysis of the available research allows
“high quality” provided by Gersten and col- WWC to provide information to consumers on
leagues relate back to the quality indicators out- the effectiveness of interventions or curriculum
lined throughout the article. Additionally, Gersten and if they should be considered to be effective
et  al. proposed this criterion for considering a evidence-based practices.
practice to be promising: (a) an intervention has
at least four published studies that are of accept-
able quality or two published studies that are of 2.2.4 National Standards Project
high quality, and (b) across those studies there is
a 20% confidence interval for the weighted effect The National Standards Project was created by
size greater than zero. the National Autism Center to provide informa-
tion to practitioners and consumers regarding
evidence-based practices for individuals diag-
2.2.3 What Works Clearinghouse nosed with ASD (National Autism Center, 2009,
2015). The National Standards Project sought to
What Works Clearinghouse (WWC) is an organi- summarize the strength of the evidence available
zation that was created by the United States for existing educational and behavioral interven-
Department of Education’s National Center for tions for those diagnosed with ASD, describe the
Educational Evaluation and Regional Assistance. individuals with whom the interventions have
The creation of WWC was an important initiative shown to be effective, identify the limitations of
created so that rigorous, current, and evidence-­ the current research for autism intervention, and
based research would be used within the United provide recommendations for practitioners using
2  Defining Evidence-Based Practice in the Context of Applied Behavior Analysis and Autism Intervention 15

evidence-based practices. To accomplish this ing of 1 for research design meant that the study
goal, they launched the National Standards only had two comparisons of the control and
Project that consisted two phases. Phase 1 of the intervention conditions, only included one par-
National Standards Project began in 2005 with ticipant, and had significant data loss. This rating
the report being published in 2009 (National and evaluation process made it possible for the
Autism Center, 2009), and Phase 2 began in National Autism Center to provide two reports to
2011 with the report being published in 2015 consumers and practitioners identifying interven-
(National Autism Center, 2015). To begin the tions with high-quality research as beneficial and
project, the National Autism Center convened an established evidence-based practices for individ-
expert panel of scholars, researchers, and leaders uals diagnosed with ASD (National Autism
in the field to conduct their comprehensive anal- Center, 2009, 2015).
ysis of the available research. The expert panel
developed a coding manual and a rating scale
(i.e., Scientific Merit Rating Scale) to use when 2.2.5 National Clearinghouse
evaluating the articles/research available, identi- on Autism Evidence
fied individuals to rate the articles, conducted a and Practice
search of the literature, trained the article review-
ers to establish reliability, and then began the The National Clearinghouse on Autism Evidence
article review process. Once the article review and Practice (NCAEP) is another organization
process was completed, the interventions were that has developed criteria and reports to deter-
categorized and the analysis was completed. mine what interventions and practices are evi-
First, interventions were categorized by their dence based for individuals diagnosed with ASD
effects: (a) beneficial, (b) ineffective, and (c) (Steinbrenner et  al., 2020). The purpose of the
unknown. From there, the interventions were group and the reports produced (e.g., Odom
then classified based on the strength of the avail- et  al., 2010; Wong et  al., 2013, 2015) is to
able evidence and categorized as either estab- describe and analyze sets of practices or interven-
lished, emerging, or unestablished. These tions that have clear evidence and positive effects
classifications all stemmed from the reviewer’s for children diagnosed with ASD. To accomplish
analysis of the available research using the this purpose, the NCAEP created a process to
Scientific Merit Rating Scale. search the literature, evaluate research studies,
The Scientific Merit Rating Scale was devel- identify focused (i.e., practices designed to
oped for reviewers to objectively evaluate each address a single skill or goal) and comprehensive
research article based on their scientific rigor. practices (i.e., set of practices designed to teach
The scale had five dimensions that articles were broad learning and target the core features of
rated on: (a) research design, (b) measurement of ASD), and synthesize the outcomes in published
the dependent variable, (c) measurement of the reports. The NCAEP seeks to continuously
independent variable, (d) participant ascertain- update their findings on evidence-based practices
ment, and (e) generalization and maintenance. for children and young adults diagnosed with
The scale further broke down each category with ASD because many “treatments” for autism exist
definitions and applied a numerical value on a and many claim to improve the lives or cure chil-
scale from 0 to 5 based on the outlined criteria. dren and adults diagnosed with ASD in the
For example, for the research design category, a absence of data to support their effectiveness
rating of 5 meant that for a study implementing a (Steinbrenner et  al., 2020). The prevalence of
single-subject design, a minimum of three com- anti-science and pseudoscientific (Green, 1996)
parisons of control and intervention conditions treatments for ASD highlights the need for
were conducted, the number of data points per evidence-­based practices and entities that iden-
condition was 5 or greater, the study had at least tify programs and interventions as evidence
3 participants, and there was no data loss. A rat- based.
16 J. L. Ferguson et al.

According to the NCAEP, a practice is consid- Table 2.1 Indicators of support for evidence-based
practice
ered evidence based if they: (a) have two or more
high-quality group design studies, conducted by Indicators
at least two different researchers or research Individual Objective measurement
studies Clear identification of dependent and
groups; (b) have five or more high-quality single-­
independent variables
case design research studies, conducted by at Experimental design demonstrating a
least three different researchers or research functional relation
groups, and have at least 20 participants across Generalization and maintenance
those studies; and (c) have at least one high-­ assessment
quality group design study and at least three Measures of social validity
Procedural integrity checks
high-quality single-case research design studies
Interobserver agreement/reliability of
conducted by at least two different research measurement
groups. To determine if the articles evaluated Across Replication
were high quality, the NCAEP uses the quality literature Repeated single case studies
indicators outlined by Horner et  al. (2005), (multiple)
Gersten et  al. (2005), and What Works Group studies (multiple)
Clearinghouse. After evaluating individual arti- Randomized control trials
cles to assess their quality, the NCAEP catego- Component analyses
rizes interventions as evidence-based practices or
practices with some evidence. Additionally, the
NCAEP denotes age groups for whom the inter-
ventions have been found to be effective.
Although the NCAEP and their published between what each entity looks for to qualify a
reports (e.g., Odom et  al., 2010; Steinbrenner study as high quality, many similarities exist
et  al., 2020; Wong et  al., 2013, 2015) can be a between the groups. Across these groups, there is
useful tool to help practitioners and consumers a reliance on objective measurement, clearly out-
identify if an intervention or practice is evidence lined dependent and independent variables,
based, it should be noted that concerns have been experimental design, and replication across
raised regarding the methods used by the NCAEP research groups and participants (See Table 2.1).
to identify evidence-based practices (e.g., Leaf
et  al., 2021). Some of the criticisms of the
NCAEP’s procedures have included: (a) missing 2.3 Differences in Definitions
relevant research articles due to their search and Terminology
terms, (b) excluding research articles that use
nonconcurrent multiple baseline designs, (c) Although similarities exist for identifying proce-
broad categorization of interventions (e.g., social dures as evidence-based practices for individuals
skills training) in which procedures may greatly diagnosed with ASD, debate remains regarding
differ, and (d) categorizing components of inter- how the term evidence-based practice should be
ventions as practices (Leaf et al., 2021). used in the field of applied behavior analysis
(ABA). Additionally, other terminology such as
evidence-based practice in psychology (EBPP)
2.2.6 Summary and empirically supported treatment (EST) are
sometimes used to identify and categorize inter-
Several articles and organizations have worked to ventions. This section will overview the differ-
develop specific guidelines and definitions to ences in philosophy when it comes to the
help consumers identify interventions as definition of evidence-based practice in ABA and
evidence-­based practices for individuals diag- also go over the definitions of EBPP and EST for
nosed with ASD.  Although differences exist interventions and treatments.
2  Defining Evidence-Based Practice in the Context of Applied Behavior Analysis and Autism Intervention 17

2.3.1 Smith (2013) Evaluating the packages in group designs is the


best way to accomplish this goal (Smith, 2013).
Smith (2013) defined evidence-based practice for
behavior analysts first as a service that aims to
help solve a consumer’s problem. Due to this 2.3.2 Slocum et al. (2014)
definition, Smith stated that it is likely that
evidence-­based practices would be a combined Slocum et al. (2014) also defined evidence-based
package of procedures that would be defined and practice for ABA. Slocum and colleagues’ defini-
manualized. The package would be validated tion of evidence-based practice more closely
through research studies with socially significant aligns with a decision-making model. Slocum
outcomes (Wolf, 1978), and those studies would and colleagues agreed with Smith (2013) in that
typically be group designs (Smith, 2013). Smith current organization’s definitions of evidence-­
noted that this definition is much more restrictive based practice were not helpful to practitioners
than what other organizations have proposed, but due to the lack of nuance in terms of success
he believed that this stricter definition is more across participants, settings, and skill level of cli-
appropriate and aligns with the technological ents but disagreed with Smith on his definition.
dimension of ABA (Baer et  al., 1968, 1987). Slocum et  al. found that Smith’s definition of
Smith created and advocated for this definition evidence-based practice was what in the field of
over other common definitions (e.g., Horner psychology is referred to as an empirically sup-
et  al., 2005; National Autism Center, 2009) ported treatment (EST). An EST is a treatment in
because he did not find that the lists created by psychology that has been demonstrated to be
these entities were very useful for behavior ana- effective through the use of rigorous randomized
lytic practitioners. Additionally, the evaluation of controlled or clinical trials (American Presidential
a practice through both single-subject research Task Force of Evidence-Based Practice, 2006).
design and group design studies leads to flexible To Slocum and colleagues, “evidence-based
definitions and criteria which can put the field at practice of applied behavior analysis is a decision-­
risk of overestimating the accomplishments of making process that integrates (a) the best avail-
our research and how helpful the results are to able evidence with (b) clinical expertise and (c)
consumers and practitioners. In Smith’s revised client values and context” (p. 44). This definition
definition of evidence-based practice, he advo- more closely relates to the definition of EBPP
cates for discussing the evidence in terms of which is the “integration of the best available
packages instead of procedures. The rationale for research with clinical expertise in the context of
this approach is that a single component or proce- patient characteristics, culture, and preferences”
dure of an intervention is usually not enough to (American Psychological Association, 2005,
fix a problem (Smith, 2013). Instead, treatment p. 147). Slocum and colleagues’ proposed defini-
packages are developed that contain many strate- tion highlights that evidence is not the sole factor
gies and techniques to change behavior. Since for making a decision as a practitioner or clini-
packages are what are needed to solve problems cian. ABA is complicated, and practicing the sci-
in behavior analysis, these are what should be ence requires clinical expertise and judgment
evaluated in terms of evidence-based practice when it comes to identifying problems, analyzing
(Smith, 2013). Additionally, when choosing an the problem, and then deciding on a solution
evidence-based practice to implement, the prefer- (Slocum et al., 2014). Without a decision-making
ences of the individual being served and abilities framework or process, behavior analysts would
of the interventionist should be taken into consid- be considered technicians instead of analysts
eration. Smith called for more research testing of (Slocum et al., 2014).
the generality of a treatment packages’ effective- In terms of the first component of Slocum
ness across settings, providers, and clients. et al.’s (2014) definition of evidence-based practice
18 J. L. Ferguson et al.

(i.e., best available evidence), the authors pro- decisions, and (g) ongoing professional develop-
vided guidance for deciding the quality of the ment (Slocum et al., 2014).
evidence in terms of the relevance and certainty As these three components are evaluated
of the available research. This highlights that together, a decision-making framework is fol-
some research will be more relevant to the age of lowed to make evidence-based practice decisions
your client, skill level of your client, or the types in the field of ABA. Slocum et al. (2014) advo-
of targeted skills for your client. It also highlights cated for this definition of evidence-based prac-
that some research on interventions will have tice over others in ABA as it aligns more closely
stronger support for its claims than others. The to the dimensions and tenets of ABA outlined by
certainty and relevance of the available research Baer et al. (1968, 1987)
should be assessed on a continuum as behavior
analysts use the evidence-based practice decision-­
making framework. 2.3.3 Summary
Client values and context should also be con-
sidered using the evidence-based practice Disagreement remains in the field of ABA over
decision-­making framework (Slocum et  al., the definition of evidence-based practice. For
2014). This relates to the applied dimension of some entities, it is a list of procedures and pack-
ABA (Baer et al., 1968, 1987) and to social valid- ages that meet qualifications regarding the avail-
ity (Wolf, 1978). Behavior analysts target behav- able evidence (e.g., National Autism Center,
ior that has practical importance and value to 2009, 2015). Others define evidence-based prac-
society. Additionally, behavior analysts aim to tice in terms of manualized treatment packages
target goals that are deemed meaningful to our that have high-quality randomized control trials
clients, use procedures that are deemed appropri- to support their effectiveness (i.e., Smith, 2013).
ate by our clients, and produce effects that are For others, evidence-based practice is a decision-­
meaningful and lasting for our clients (Wolf, making model that draws upon the best available
1978). Assessing and measuring social validity is scientific research, client values and context, and
important in the context of the evidence-based a behavior analyst’s clinical expertise (Slocum
practice decision-making process (Slocum et al., et al., 2014). Continued discourse on this topic is
2014). It allows behavior analysts to assess client warranted as behavior analysts and practitioners
values and the context of the intervention by also in the field of autism intervention strive to
asking relevant stakeholders their opinions on the improve the interventions and practices used to
interventions in question (Slocum et al., 2014). teach individuals diagnosed with ASD skills that
Clinical expertise is the final component of the will hopefully improve their quality of life. A
evidence-based practice decision-making model. unified definition that is agreed upon in the field
After evaluating the best available research evi- of behavior analysis is needed, and similar to the
dence and assessing client values and context, American Psychological Association (2005), a
behavior analysts use their clinical expertise and specific task force on this topic could be created.
judgment to make an evidence-based decision. In the meantime, the entities and other organiza-
According to Slocum et al. (2014), clinical exper- tions that strive to define and promote evidence-­
tise in ABA includes seven components: (a) based practices in the field of ABA and autism
knowledge of the research and how it applies to intervention still have much in common despite
your specific clients, (b) incorporating the con- some differences in terminology. For now, the
ceptual systems of ABA, (c) the comprehensive- exact/precise definition of evidence-based prac-
ness of the behavior analysts’ clinical and tice may not matter as much as the progress our
interpersonal skills, (d) the integration of the clients make when evidence-based practice inter-
assessed client values and context, (e) recogniz- ventions or decisions are being made. Individuals
ing the need for outside consultation and collabo- making progress based on objective measure-
ration when needed, (f) making data-based ment and data, using interventions that are
2  Defining Evidence-Based Practice in the Context of Applied Behavior Analysis and Autism Intervention 19

p­ referred, and using clinical judgment to make Academy of Pediatrics (Committee on Children
informed decisions are at the heart of evidence-­ with Disabilities, 1998). The American Academy
based practice. of Pediatrics has also issued position statements
about Auditory Integration Training (Committee
on Children with Disabilities, 1998) and Sensory
2.4 Practitioner Resources Integration (Section on Complementary and
Integrative Medicine et  al., 2012). ASHA has
Across disciplines, resources are needed to assist position statements on many different interven-
practitioners to identify evidence-based prac- tions, including auditory integration training and
tices. Especially in multidisciplinary settings, it rapid prompting method (American Speech-­
is important to ensure that opinions expressed are Lanugage-­Hearing Association, 2004; American
backed up with credible resources. Referencing Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2018a,
such resources can reduce interprofessional ten- 2018b). ASHA has also provided follow-up posi-
sion and ensure that the conversations are focused tion statements to update practitioners. In 2018,
on available recommendations rather than differ- they updated the Facilitated Communication
ences in opinion across fields or between position statement (American Speech-Language-­
professionals. Hearing Association, 2018a). Additionally, some
In this context, it is important for practitioners organizations are specifically calling for practi-
to identify available evidence and the categoriza- tioners to practice within the confines of
tions of suggested treatments in timely ways. evidence-­ based practice, and several organiza-
Many of the resources previously cited in this tions are identifying pseudoscience as a threat to
chapter share emphases that are commonly cited effectiveness. For example, Volkers (2019) wrote
as indicators of empirical support. These are about the continuance of non-speech oral motor
listed in Table 2.1. To the extent that available lit- exercises in speech and language pathology inter-
erature has these elements, practitioners can be vention and cited it as evidence that clinicians’
more confident in the findings. In addition to recommendations reflect state-of-the-art scien-
going to the published literature, practitioners tific understanding. Volkers also spoke broadly of
may contact sources known for identifying and the threats to scientific intervention recommen-
categorizing interventions based on available evi- dations, including the dramatic increase in misin-
dence. Several of these have been previously formation, the presence of bias, and the tendency
described, such as the National Standards Project to value our own experiences over data. Within
and What Works Clearinghouse. ASHA and other professional guild organiza-
In addition to these compendiums of informa- tions, the importance of adhering to scientifically
tion about the accrual of research evidence, other validated procedures has been emphasized.
practitioner resources also exist. Perhaps the Several of the position statements across profes-
most useful of these are position statements. sions cite ethical mandates to engage in evidence-­
Position statements are created by professional based practices, including ASHA’s rapid
organizations to guide their members and con- prompting method and Facilitated
sumers about treatment interventions. At times, Communication statements.
these position statements are created to address Another vital resource is the Treatment
interventions that have been shown to be ineffec- Guidelines from Autism New Jersey (Autism
tive and/or produce harm. For example, multiple New Jersey, 2021). This user-friendly resource
organizations have created position statements presents a framework based on the amount of evi-
about Facilitated Communication, including the dence available for interventions, using a red
ASHA (American Speech-Language-Hearing light, yellow light, green light categorization (see
Association, 2018a), the Association for Behavior https://www.autismnj.org/understanding-­autism/
Analysis International (Association for Behavior treatment/). Red light interventions have been
Analysis International, 1995), and the American shown unequivocally to be ineffective and/or
20 J. L. Ferguson et al.

harmful. Examples include chelation therapy, 2.5 Conclusion


Facilitated Communication, and auditory inte-
gration training. Yellow light interventions are Evidence-based practice is highly valued across
those that await more data, and which may be fields and is associated with improved interven-
considered for trial in individual cases, with data tion and improved outcomes. Different methods
to support continuance. Examples include music exist to categorize treatments, but all models rate
therapy, art therapy, and animal-assisted therapy. interventions based primarily on the accrual of
Green light interventions are those that have been objective, well-designed research. Additional
shown to be efficacious and are considered to be considerations include clinician expert opinion
supported by research. Many procedures within and client values. Across disciplines, these addi-
ABA are listed in this category, such as discrete tional themes figure prominently into clinician
trial instruction, the Picture Exchange decisions about treatment. Within ABA, there has
Communication System, and functional commu- been a renewed interest toward the integration of
nication training. client values and the empowerment of clients to
Practitioners are often in a position to provide participate in treatment decisions (similar to the
recommendations for the course of treatment interests and statements of the founders in the
and/or to respond to a recommendation made by field, e.g., Baer et al., 1968, 1987; Wolf, 1978).
a consumer or other professional. It is imperative Clients can also be empowered with information
in these contexts for all professionals to exude about research evidence and about the evaluation
respect and to collaborate in the best interests of of such research by trusted bodies and organiza-
the client even when sources of contention are tions. The analysis of position statements can
present (see Table 2.2). In this context, it is often ensure that the evaluation of interventions can be
possible to open a dialogue within the team about examined from the perspective of multiple disci-
the potential intervention. Published literature plines, which may lead to a broader understand-
may provide some context about the level of evi- ing of an intervention’s merit and relevance.
dence that is known about the treatment. Strength of evidence must be considered in the
Resources that periodically track the available selection of intervention, as wasted time leads to
evidence and categorize interventions by the vol- poorer outcomes (e.g., Howard et al., 2014; Zane
ume and quality of accrued data should also be et  al., 2008). Practitioners must be trained to
consulted. In addition, resources by guild organi- review evidence, to consult resources, to guide
zations, such as position statements, provide clients in the evaluation of extant evidence, and
information about what practitioners are being to individualize the application of interventions
advised to do regarding specific interventions. to clients, based on their profiles, preferences,
Finally, some organizations have created tools for and values.
consumers that may be of use in helping to navi-
gate treatment options.
References
Association for Behavior Analysis International. (1995).
Statement on facilitated communication.. ABAI.
Table 2.2 Sources of contention for evidence-based https://www.abainternational.org/about-­us/policies-­
practice and-­positions/facilitated-­communication,-­1995.aspx
Sources of contention American Psychological Association. (n.d.). Research
Scope of practice, shared practice, or overlapping supported psychological treatments. Clinical
scopes of practice Psychology Division 12 APA. https://div12.org/
psychological-­treatments/
Different worldviews (i.e., philosophical differences)
American Psychological Association. (2005). Policy
Differing definitions of evidence statement on evidence-based practice in psychology.
Interpersonal injury (e.g., hurt feelings) https://www.apa.org/practice/resources/evidence/
evidence-­based-­statement.pdf
2  Defining Evidence-Based Practice in the Context of Applied Behavior Analysis and Autism Intervention 21

American Psychological Association Presidential Task Howard, J.  S., Stanislaw, H., Green, G., Sparkman,
Force of Evidence-Based Practice. (2006). Evidence-­ C. R., & Cohen, H. G. (2014). Comparison of behav-
based practice in psychology. American Psychologist, ior analytic and electic early interventions for young
61, 271–285. children with autism after three years. Research in
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2021). Developmental Disabilities, 35(12), 3326–3344.
Evidence-based practice (EBP). ASHA. https://www. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.021
asha.org/research/ebp/ Institute of Medicine. (2001). Crossing the quality chasm:
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. A new health system for the 21st century. Washington,
(2018a). Facilitated communication [Position DC. National Academies Press.
Statement]. https://www.asha.org/policy/ Leaf, J. B., Sato, S. K., Javed, A., Arthur, S. M., Creem,
ps2018-­00352/ A.  N., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., & Oppenheim-­
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Leaf, M. L. (2021). The evidence-based practices for
(2018b). Rapid prompting method [Position Statement]. children, youth, and young adults with autism report:
https://www.asha.org/policy/ps2018-­00351/ Concerns and critiques. Behavioral Interventions,
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2004). 36(2), 457–472. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1771
Auditory integration training [Position Statement]. Levant, R.  F. (2005). Report of the 2005 presidential
https://www.asha.org/policy/PS2004-­00218/ task force on evidence based practice. Washington,
American Occupational Therapy Association. DC. American Psychological Association.
(2021). Practice essentials: Evidence-­ National Association of Social Workers. (2021).
based practice and knowledge translation. Evidence-based practice: NASW practice snap-
American Occupational Association.  https:// shot. https://www.socialworkers.org/News/
w w w. a o t a . o rg / p r a c t i c e / p r a c t i c e - e s s e n t i a l s / Research-­D ata/Social-­Work-­P olicy-­R esearch/
evidencebased-practiceknowledge-translation Evidence-­Based-­Practice
Autism New Jersey. (2021). Treatment. https://www. National Autism Center. (2009). Findings and conclu-
autismnj.org/understanding-­autism/treatment/ sions: National standards project, phase 1. Randolph,
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some MA. Author.
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 91–97. sions: National standards project, phase 2. Randolph,
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1.91 MA. Author.
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1987). Some Odom, S.  L., Collet-Klingenberg, L., Rogers, S.  J., &
still current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Hatton, D.  D. (2010). Evidence-based practices in
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20(4), 313–327. interventions for children and youth with autism spec-
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1987.20-­313 trum disorders. Preventing School Failure, 54(4), 275–
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020). Ethics code 282. https://doi.org/10.1080/10459881003785506
for behavior analysts. Littleton, CO. Author. Schreck, K. A., Karunaratne, Y., Zane, T., & Wilford, H.
Committee on Children with Disabilities. (1998). Auditory (2016). Behavior analysts’ use of and beliefs in treat-
integration training and facilitated communication ments for people with autism: A 5-year follow up.
for autism. American Academy of Pediatrics, 102(2), Behavioral Interventions, 31(4), 355–376. https://doi.
431–322. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.102.2.431 org/10.1002/bin.1461
Drake, R.  E., Goldman, H.  H., Leff, H.  S., Lehman, Section on Complementary and Integrative Medicine,
A.  F., Dixon, L., Mueser, K.  T., & Torrey, W.  C. Council on Children with Disabilities, American
(2001). Implementing evidence-based practices in Academy of Pediatrics, Zimmer, M., & Desch, L.
routine mental health service settings. Psychiatric (2012). Sensory integration therapies for children with
Services, 52(2), 179–182. https://doi.org/10.1176/ developmental and behavioral disorders. Pediatrics,
appi.ps.52.2.179 129(6), 1186–1189. https://doi.org/10.1542/
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Compton, C. M., Greenwood, peds.2012-­0876
C., & Innocenti, M. S. (2005). Quality indicators for Slocum, T.  A., Detrich, R., Wilcyznski, S.  M., Spencer,
group experimental and quasi-experimental research in T.  D., Lewis, T., & Wolfe, K. (2014). The evidence-­
special education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 149– based practice of applied behavior analysis.
164. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100202 The Behavior Analyst, 37(1), 41–56. https://doi.
Green, G. (1996). Evaluating claims about treatments org/10.1007/s40614-­014-­0005-­2
for autism. In C.  Maurice, G.  Green, & S.  C. Luce Smith, T. (2013). What is evidence-based behavior analy-
(Eds.), Behavioral intervention for young children sis? The Behavior Analyst, 36(1), 7–33. https://doi.
with autism: A manual for parents and professionals org/10.1007/BF03392290
(pp. 15–28). Austin, TX. Pro-Ed. Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin,
Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, K.  L., Nowell, S.  W., Tomazewski, B., Szendrey, S.,
S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject McIntyre, N. S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage, M. N.
research to identify evidence-based practice in spe- (2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth,
cial education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 165–179. and young adults with autism. The University of North
https://doi.org/10.1177/00144029057100203 Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child
22 J. L. Ferguson et al.

Development Institute, National Clearinghouse on tices for children, youth, and young adults with autism
Autism Evidence and Practice Review Team. spectrum disorder. The University of North Carolina,
Volkers, N. (2019). Does truth have a future? The growing Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute,
movement to reject expertise and evidence has hear- Autism Evidence-Based Practice Review Group.
ing and speech professionals scrambling to show why Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig,
facts matter. The ASHA Leader, 24(7), 42. https://doi. A., Kucharczyk, S., Brock, M.  E., Plavnikc, J.  B.,
org/10.1044/leader.FTR1.24072019.42 Fleury, V.  P., & Schultz, T.  R. (2015). Evidence-­
What Works Clearinghouse. (2014). Procedures and stan- based practices for children, youth, and young
dards handbook, Version 3.0. Author. adults with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehen-
Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjec- sive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
tive measurement or how behavior analysis is finding Disorders, 45, 1951–1966. https://doi.org/10.1007/
its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11(2), s10803-­014-­2351-­z
203–214. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1978.11-­203 Zane, T., Davis, C., & Rosswurm, M. (2008). The cost
Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K., Cox, A. W., Fettig, A., of fad treatments in autism. Journal of Early and
Kucharczyk, S., Brock, M. E., Plavnick, J. B., Fleury, Intensive Behavior Intervention, 5(2), 44–51. https://
V. P., & Schultz, T. R. (2013). Evidence-based prac- doi.org/10.1037/h0100418
History of Non-Evidence-Based
Practices for Individuals 3
Diagnosed with ASD

Elizabeth M. Kryszak and James A. Mulick

3.1 Introduction accuracy of something presented as true without


additional information. The result has been called
Few readers will be surprised by our asserting a fracturing of shared reality in modern society,
that the information environment in which we in which people gravitate to media and people
live is full of contradictions and sources of infor- tending to match their preferences and prior
mation that vary in credibility (Mihailidis & beliefs to the exclusion of other or contradictory
Foster, 2021). The modern information age is information and people who disagree. The result
defined by a bewildering array of communication is a self-reinforcing and self-­imposed formation
options. People, corporations, governments, of insular groups that share a reality that is nar-
organizations, interest groups, and individuals row and not necessarily based on facts.
use communication tools for many purposes. We Science is a bulwark against competing truth
have been, nevertheless, cheerfully informed that claims about material nature. This is not because
the Internet makes us all heirs to nearly the sum science contains truth but because science uncov-
of human knowledge. Two problems make this ers truth by its methods and the strong motives of
bounty a gift that bears a dose of caution. First, its practitioners. The scientific method involves
communicators communicate for reasons that go testing ideas against observable facts from every
beyond simply sharing knowledge. The purposes possible direction; from studies that confirm
for which people communicate include selling observations or the results of tests, repeatedly;
things, motivating specific actions, discouraging and by independent scientists. The secret ingredi-
other actions, and exerting forms of social control. ent is the culture of science, in which scientists
Sometimes these purposes conceal selfish or even are trained to question everything offered as true,
malevolent intent. Second, none of us are able to especially by other scientists who set themselves
tell the true motivation behind an assertion or the up as authorities (Rauch, 2013).
Scientific studies rely on repeatedly finding
E. M. Kryszak (*) confirming evidence that a fact or an outcome is
The Child Development Center, Nationwide true no matter how it is tested, by whom, or under
Children’s Hospital, Columbus, OH, USA what circumstances. If some set of conditions
Department of Pediatrics and Psychology, The Ohio gives different results, then attention turns to just
State University, Columbus, OH, USA how those new conditions could have accounted
e-mail: Elizabeth.Kryszak@nationwidechildrens.org for the different results. As boundary conditions
J. A. Mulick for a set of observations are discovered by
Department of Pediatrics and Psychology, The Ohio repeated tests, reliable prediction or control of
State University, Columbus, OH, USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 23


J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_3
24 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

nature emerges. Science makes progress by these spreading falsehoods, but it is rather good in
systematic attempts to show how something determining whether or not they are falsehoods.
changes or stops working as expected, and every Scientists are not immune from such conflicts of
unexpected result leads to finding out if the unex- interest, but the encouragement of routine disclo-
pected will recur under similar conditions and so sure of funding in scientific publication as well as
on until it all finally makes sense. the culture of independent replication and peer
Science works because evidence builds up. If review tend to compensate.
evidence from every approach is consistent such Science-based evidence is the outcome of the
that no serious methodological attempt to dis- application of scientific methods, and the
credit it is offered, then scientists conclude that evidence-­based analyses of autism and of treat-
they are dealing with the truth. In health sciences, ments for problems related to autism are found in
treatments are developed based on ideas that have research publications, journal articles that have
been suggested by the methods that have worked been subjected to rigorous peer review. Peer
consistently in the past with similar problems. review by other scientists makes sure that the
New treatments do not emerge out of the blue methods reported are sound and that conclusions
independent of the whole body of relevant sci- are warranted based on the evidence. Finally,
ence. The corpus of confirmed evidence in biol- each published finding is verified to be consistent
ogy, chemistry, physics, and so on, both applied with related previous research and makes sense
and theoretical, is not going to be overturned by a with accumulated established facts. Non-­
new effective treatment. When effective treat- evidence-­based assertions are just that, assertions
ments are understood, they will be found to be that have not been verified, often because they are
consistent with the facts of established science asserted recklessly for an ulterior motive.
even if they do lead to new understanding of how
those established facts relate to each other. So,
science can yield treatments that work because 3.1.1 W
 hy Do Non-evidence-Based
they are tested extensively, are consistent with Practices Exist?
how nature works generally, and have withstood
the aggressive attempts of eager scientific peers Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is considered a
who worked hard to find something wrong with lifelong neurodevelopmental disorder
them. Scientists doing science communicate (Steinhausen et  al., 2016). While there is much
about facts and about testing them. known about this disorder, there is still much
There are other reasons that people communi- which we are still working to understand.
cate, and some of these reasons can lead to a dis- Monumental gains have been made in the devel-
tortion of reality, sometimes very much on opment of interventions for this disorder, but so
purpose. Advertising is one venue that relies on far, the interventions that work best come with a
communication, and its content is not always heavy cost in resources including time, effort,
limited to facts. New and improved products and money. Gaps in knowledge and absence of a
might be pretty much the same but packaged dif- “quick cure” provide openings for the inception
ferently. Politicians might appeal to values and of “bad” ideas about the mechanisms of ASD and
emotions that have little to do with their past how to best address these symptoms. This leads
actions or future plans in order to gain votes. to the creation and consumption of non-evidence-­
Public figures might hope for financial gain as a based practices, despite concerted efforts by pro-
result of what they promote. Support for a plan in fessionals to debunk these practices and better
an organization might be motived by a desire for educate consumers. There are now over 400
a promotion and not because the plan is a good different treatments for ASD, most of which
one. People may assert things because they want have little to no support (Frame & Casey, 2019;
to fit in or gain membership in a group. Science is Matson et  al., 2013), and less than half of
rarely devoted to discovering the true motives for families with a child with ASD choose a research-­
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 25

supported treatment (Miller et  al., 2012). This quality to confidently say that this intervention
chapter will next explore the history of has a high likelihood of leading to meaningful
non-­
­ evidence-­ based practices and the theories change if the necessary time, energy, and cost are
behind them, by first reviewing what it means to invested into its implementation. If the interven-
show an intervention truly works and why having tion does not have a body of evidence of suffi-
some evidence that an intervention could work cient quality to meet this standard, then we would
does not mean that the intervention is “evidence say it is non-evidence based. But how do we
based.” We will then explore how misunderstand- define sufficient quality?
ing the mechanisms behind a disorder can lead to
misinformed ideas on how to treat it and then
how these interventions can spread despite little 3.1.3 Q
 uality Evidence Is Based
quality science to support them. This concept in the Scientific Method
will be illustrated by reviewing examples of three
different types of misunderstandings about It is human nature to attempt to understand our
autism and the non-evidence-based interventions world. We look for possible connections between
they spawned, including misunderstanding how events and then, hopefully, attempt to test whether
the environment effects behavior, extreme beliefs, these explanations, or theories, hold true. Over
and biological pseudoscience. The chapter will time, scientists have developed many methods to
conclude with an overview of why families are so best test theorized connections. The scientific
susceptible to non-evidence-based treatments approach seeks to develop statements that are
and how providers can attempt to correct these objective, testable, and replicable (Newsom &
misperceptions. Hovanitz, 2015). When applying this approach to
assessing a treatment, “objective” refers to
describing explicitly and unambiguously what is
3.1.2 What Is Evidence? being done and what is the expected outcome. A
“testable” statement is one that can be verified or
Before we can define what a “non-evidence-­ falsified by conducting an experiment. Variables
based” practice is, we must first define evidence. with easily observed effects can be studied using
The Oxford Dictionary defines evidence as “The single-case designs, where an intervention is
available body of facts or information indicating applied and removed in a systematic fashion to
whether a belief or proposition is true or valid” show its effect on a defined behavior (e.g., ABAB
(Lexico, 2021a), while the Merriam-Webster def- designs). When a variable is theorized to have a
inition is simply “Something that furnishes weaker effect or there is more interest in the aver-
proof” (Merriam Webster, 2021). Neither of these age result across many participants, randomized
definitions touches on the quality of the evidence control designs are typically implemented, where
being weighed. Based on these definitions alone, participants are randomly assigned to a treatment
the majority of treatments out there actually or control group. Statistical analyses are then
would not qualify as having no evidence. Almost used to assess whether the treatment group dif-
every intervention being marketed can at least fered significantly from the control group on an
boast a parent testimonial, a case study or two, or objectively defined outcome measure. In both
a poor-quality (but published) research study pro- methods, steps are taken to control other variables
viding some support for the treatment being so that any changes can be attributed only to the
pushed. Therefore, when formally referring to treatment. The effects must then be replicable,
“evidence-based” versus “non-evidence-based” meaning that there should be evidence that inde-
interventions, we are not looking at the presence pendent groups can conduct the same test, in the
or absence of evidence. Rather, we are judging if same way, and get the same result.
the body of evidence available is of sufficient
26 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

Efforts should be taken to use objective and waitlist control; (2) one or more experiments that
direct outcome variables. Ideally the person mea- meet criteria for a “well-established treatment”
suring the outcome variable should be blind to the but that have not yet been replicated by an inde-
condition the participant is in. For example, if pendent team; (3) a small series of quality single-­
measuring the effect of a medication on cognitive case design experiments. Finally, experimental
skills, the person completing the IQ test should treatments would include all other treatments
not know if the child they are testing is taking the under study. Most treatments start as experimen-
medication or a placebo. There are times where tal, often based on theory and first described in
blinding raters is not possible. For example, when case studies or in a less-controlled experimental
comparing two different behavior interventions design. Further steps must be taken ultimately to
for decreasing self-injurious behavior, the person reach the “well-established” criteria.
measuring the self-injurious behavior is likely Unfortunately, many widely used treatments
going to be able to tell which intervention is being never take these steps.
implemented. In these cases, it is important for
the outcome measure to be clearly defined (e.g.,
number of times child hits head) to ensure the 3.1.4 W
 hy Do Non-evidence-Based
rater can stay as objective as possible. Parent and Treatments Exist and Persist?
clinician report measures can provide valuable
insight into whether others perceive an interven- Our understanding of autism is incomplete.
tion is working and into factors such as how palat- While we have some understanding of the genetic
able an intervention is and ease of implementation and developmental risk factors behind its etiol-
in a real-world setting. Using parent and clinician ogy, we still do not have a good explanation for
measures solely to look at outcome of an inter- why many children develop autism. In addition,
vention, particularly when the raters are not able while evidence-based behavioral interventions
to be blinded, can cause bias in results. Human have been created that can meaningfully improve
observation is prone to bias (Mulick & Butter, learning and adaptive functioning, these inter-
2015). Unfortunately, when parents, clinicians, ventions are resource intensive, take a long
and even scientists put a large amount of time, amount of time, and in many cases do not lead to
effort, and money into implementing a treatment, full recovery or “typical” functioning (Jacobson
they are highly susceptible to seeing change, even & Mulick, 2000; Jacobson et al., 1998; Lovaas,
when that change is not there. 1987). Therefore, the pursuit of knowledge con-
Criteria have been offered to define when a tinues in order to better understand the etiologi-
treatment is considered evidence based cal underpinnings of ASD and develop
(Chambless & Ollendick, 2001). Well established interventions that are more palatable and acces-
treatments (i.e., evidence based) are those that sible to the public. Unfortunately, good science
either have (1) two quality between group design seems slow, especially when loved ones are
experiments demonstrating that the treatment in involved. It takes time to develop and adequately
question is better than a placebo or other treat- test theories using rigorous experimental meth-
ment, or equivalent to an already established ods. Scientific nicety, however, does not stop
treatment, or (2) a large series of well-controlled eager practitioners and families from grasping
single-case design experiments demonstrating onto new theories that sound good and then
efficacy compared to a placebo or another treat- widely disseminating untested interventions.
ment. These experiments must also be conducted Widespread use can then lead to interventions
with well-defined treatment manuals or proto- being seen as “standard of care” despite the lack
cols; the characteristics of the sample must be of quality supporting evidence. Then, when sup-
well defined, and an independent group must be porting evidence fails to materialize or when evi-
able to replicate the effects. Probably efficacious dence showing a lack of effectiveness is
treatments are those that have: (1) two experi- published, it becomes difficult for people to
ments that show the treatment is superior to a change course.
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 27

It is imperative that scientists hold true to the emotional factors in the early environment rather
fact that ideas that seem plausible at first may not than neurobiological causes as we understand it
be supported by scientific investigation. This can today. Autism was first categorized as a subtype
be difficult when a fair amount of time, effort, of schizophrenia, and Leo Kanner described his
and money have been put into devising and test- theory of “infantile autism” as “children’s inabil-
ing a theory and when attainment of professional ity to relate themselves in the ordinary way to
goals such as tenure or promotion depend on people and situations from the beginning of life”
whether an idea is supported. There may be great (Cook & Willmerdinger, 2015, pg 3). Given the
reluctance to move on even when the evidence popularity of psychodynamic theory at the time,
being gathered is not promising, leading many these extreme social deficits were misattributed
scientists with good intentions to cling to a theory to poor parenting styles. Particularly popular at
or intervention despite the lack of support. Other the time was the theory that autism was caused by
practitioners may have had less noble intent from “refrigerator mothers,” who were seen as inca-
the beginning, recognizing that families with a pable of providing appropriate emotional warmth
child with an incurable developmental disability to their children. Bruno Bettelheim did much to
are particularly vulnerable to the promise of a popularize this theory in the general public and
miracle cure. created treatments meant to rescue these children
from parents who he likened to guards in a con-
centration camp (Cook & Willmerdinger, 2015).
3.2 Misunderstanding Autism He advocated removing children with autism
and the Non-evidence-Based from their parents and putting them in schools
Practices That Follow and institutions that he thought would be better
suited to providing needed care. Parents seeking
The following section outlines several historical help for children with autism in the 1950s and
and current examples of how misunderstandings 1960s were often sent to psychoanalytic therapy
of autism have led to the creation and dissemina- to discover how they had somehow unconsciously
tion of a host of non-evidence-based interven- rejected their child, causing their child to with-
tions. Each section outlines a different set of draw from the world (Baker, 2010).
theories about ASD that often seem initially plau- Other interventions were created to more
sible but upon further examination are not objec- directly attempt to repair this “deficit” in attach-
tively stated, testable, or reproducible. This has ment including play therapy and holding therapy.
not stopped these theories from becoming the The theory behind holding therapy posited that
basis for a range of “promising” interventions the lack of bond between mothers and their autis-
that become widely used, eating up valuable tic children created an emotional imbalance,
resources despite being non-evidence based. which stopped the child being able to learn from
interactions with others and led to social with-
drawal (Barth et  al., 2005). Holding therapy
3.2.1 Misunderstanding How seeks to repair the mother-child bond by forcing
Environment Affects Behavior a new emotional connection by provoking dis-
tress in the child, which then allows the mother to
3.2.1.1 Getting It Wrong provide the needed comfort (Mercer, 2013b).
From the Start: The Scourge Early iterations of this therapy provoked distress
of the Refrigerator Mother in a number of inhumane and dangerous ways
Misunderstanding autism started at its concep- including having multiple people forcibly hold a
tion. Initial theories of autism put forth in the child down, wrapping the child in blankets or
1940s when psychodynamic theory dominated other material, and withholding food and access
psychology, suggested an etiology based in psy- to bathroom facilities (Mercer, 2013b). Following
chogenic factors. Therefore, autism was origi- several deaths and reports of lasting trauma, this
nally thought to be caused by psychological or form of holding therapy unsurprisingly fell out of
28 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

favor. The next iterations of the therapy advo- and external societal expectations, turning chil-
cated for the holding to be carried out by parents dren into mindless robots and opening them up to
with a therapist coaching. Parents are instructed be taken advantage of by others (Total Autism,
to either hold the face of young children or lie on 2020). Others have posited that ABA causes psy-
older children to encourage prolong and direct chological trauma and can lead to post-traumatic
eye contact. There is no scientific evidence to stress disorder (Kupferstein, 2018), although the
support the use of any type of holding therapy, single published support for this misconception is
beyond a few case studies, and reports of riddled with methodological flaws (Leaf et  al.,
­emotional distress continue even for this “gen- 2018). The backlash of this misunderstanding of
tler” form of forced eye contact (Koocher & Gill, behavioral principles and ABA led many in the
2015). Given the documented physical and psy- field of autism intervention to turn away from
chological dangers of using holding therapy, the proven principles of behavior change and instead
American Psychological Association, the to create a host of interventions focused on build-
American Psychiatric Association, the National ing skills through more “natural interactions.”
Association of Social Workers, and other groups Gentle teaching and developmental social-­
have formally stated that they do not support its pragmatic (DSP) models are two examples of
use (Mercer, 2013b). Several studies have also these types of therapies.
shown that, despite deficits in social skills, chil- Gentle teaching, developed in the 1980s by
dren with autism typically show a strong attach- Dr. John McGee, is a nonaversive strategy to
ment to their parents, which discounts the reduce challenging behavior that purportedly
underlying theory behind holding therapy and focuses on making individuals feel safe, loved,
other psychodynamic-based interventions and connected to their caretakers, not on chang-
(Rutgers et al., 2004). ing behavior (McGee et al., 2009). It is thought
that by creating these strong bonds with others in
3.2.1.2 Making Robots: safe environments, individuals will be motivated
Misunderstanding ABA to naturally make better choices. Interestingly,
and Misguided gentle teaching does still employ a number of
Counter-Interventions behavioral techniques including errorless teach-
The rise of behaviorism and applied behavior anal- ing, task analysis, environmental management,
ysis (ABA) in the 1960s and 1970s provided a new prompting, choice making, and fading assistance,
understanding of how the environment could yet they are sold in a package of promoting
shape learning and behavior (Arnold-­ Saritepe “bonding” and “valuing” which leads them to be
et al., 2015). While the neurobiological origins of more palatable to many who find the language
autism were beginning to be better understood, describing ABA to be too harsh (Arnold-Saritepe
those attempting to help individuals with autism et al., 2015). Unfortunately, because the focus is
also began to discover that the core social deficits more on the bonding and less on the behavioral
and restricted and repetitive behaviors that were outcomes, the behavior principles often are not
interfering with a child’s ability to learn effectively explained in enough detail or applied with the
could be changed through behavior modification consistency needed to work. A review of several
principles. Unfortunately, like all technologies, single-case design studies assessing the effective-
when ABA techniques are misapplied, it can lead ness of gentle teaching showed little positive
to poor or even adverse outcomes. The language change in behavior or even in measures of the
used by behavior analysts can often be confusing “bond” between the child and their caregivers
or sound harsh to families and other professionals (Arnold-Saritepe et al., 2015). While few scien-
working with children with autism (Critchfield tific studies of gentle teaching have been pub-
et al., 2017). Misapplications and misunderstand- lished in the twenty-first century, use of the
ings of ABA have led to several false attributions method continues, with a few organizations con-
including that ABA focuses only on compliance tinuing to promote use and provide training
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 29

including Gentle Teaching International (https:// behaviors must still be relied upon to know
gentleteaching.com/us/) and the Center for whether a capacity has been developed. Within
Education and Caregiving (https://www.gentle- DSP interventions, the clinician follows the
teaching.nl/gentle/en/). child’s lead and looks for intervention opportuni-
Developmental social-pragmatic (DSP) mod- ties based on the child’s interests or attentional
els (also called relationship-based, developmen- focus. Manipulations of the child’s environment
tal, interactive, transactional, or interpersonal to create interaction opportunities are allowed but
models) are another group of interventions meant targeting a specific response is not. Instead, all
to counteract the supposed sterile and impersonal attempts at communication are responded to as if
nature of ABA. As the name implies, DSP mod- they were purposeful including “nonconven-
els are based in developmental models and tional” actions such as echolalia and jargon and
research exploring how mothers’ interactions “preintentional” actions such as crying, reaching,
with their typically developing infants and tod- and grabbing (Ingersoll et al., 2005). Using exag-
dlers during early communication and social gerated emotional expression, adjusting lan-
skills acquisition shapes learning (Kryszak et al., guage, and indirect communication behavior
2018). These ideas reach back to early (e.g., vocal imitation, descriptive modeling, self-­
psychodynamic-­ based object relations theory, talk, parallel talk, expansion) to match the child’s
which posits that humans have instincts that must developmental level are used to build reciprocity
be fulfilled and, as infants, dependent on caregiv- between the parent and child (Ingersoll et  al.,
ers such as our mother to meet these needs (Zane 2005). It is theorized that this more naturalistic
et al., 2015). Our ability to learn and adapt to the teaching style will promote spontaneous and gen-
environment is then based on these early relation- eralized communication skills (Ingersoll et  al.,
ships. While originators of DSP interventions 2005). This assumes that somehow the child will
have moved passed ideas of the “refrigerator develop the functional communication without
mother” to acknowledge that autism is biologi- any direct instruction of these skills and in the
cally based, they suggest that deficits in the indi- absence of any prompting to use more functional
vidual’s ability to develop the self in relation to communication skills. This model also assumes
others is the core deficit that must be corrected that the child would attend to caregivers without
(Zane et  al., 2015). Furthermore, they suggest additional intervention in order to benefit from
that language is developed through “affect-­ the exaggerated emotional expression and other
ladened” interactions within strong relationships modeled skills, which seems contradicted by
with caregivers, with a focus on the “function” of research that shows that difficulties with imita-
communication (e.g., requests, protests, sharing) tion and observational learning are a hallmark
rather than the “form” (e.g., eye contact, gestures, deficit in ASD (Rogers et al., 2003). Further, the
vocalizations, verbal language) (Ingersoll et  al., neurodevelopmental factors associated with ASD
2005). Instead of deliberately focusing on teach- itself have already prevented the child from ben-
ing a set of skills or tasks based on the normal efiting from the normal experiences associated
developmental trajectory as in ABA-based inter- with normal rates and types of learning (Lovaas,
ventions, these interventions focus on “helping 2003). Development is the culmination of experi-
children to develop various capacities related to ence and growth. Development itself as an inter-
social communication in a pragmatically appro- vention concept is irrelevant because it merely
priate social context rather than targeting the describes a sequence of changes that happen, that
behaviors themselves” (Casenhiser et  al., 2013, is, in normal development in normal contexts and
p. 220). For example, instead of focusing on eye that result in normal development, all as statisti-
contact and pointing, DSP-based interventions cally defined. While understanding a typical
focus on developing the “capacity” for joint sequence of development is important for devel-
attention (Casenhiser et al., 2013). Unfortunately, oping a sequence of appropriate treatment targets
“capacities” are not observable, so specific based on the child’s age and current abilities, it
30 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

does little otherwise to inform an intervention son with the disability (Albrecht et  al., 2001).
method. It also ignores the neurodevelopmental Mental illness and developmental disabilities
deficits that underlie the emergence of autism in have also been historically misinterpreted as pos-
otherwise normal family contexts. session by spirits or devils or related to witchcraft
There are several DSP-based models, with (Hemphill, 1966). These early extreme beliefs
some of the most well-known being Developmental led to a number of horrible interventions from
Individualized Relationships-­based burning people at the stake to traumatic exorcism
(DIR)/Floortime-based interventions (Greenspan practices meant to “heal” the person (Mercer,
& Wieder, 2006), Relationship Development 2013a).
Intervention (RDI) program (Gutstein, 2009), and Into the 1900s, those with disabilities were
Play and Language for Autistic Youngsters often seen as of “unfit” moral character (Conrad,
(PLAY) Project Home Consultation model 2020). Disability was framed as something intrin-
(Solomon et  al., 2014). The research body sup- sically and unchangeably wrong with the person,
porting DSP-based interventions, however, is and therefore there was no point in attempting to
unimpressive. A few randomized control trials intervene. While some institutions focused on
(RCTs) and several smaller studies have shown educating those with disabilities, many primarily
differences between children receiving DSP- existed for the confinement and management of
based interventions versus those receiving treat- those with developmental disabilities to keep
ment as usual (TAU), but only on highly specific them separated from the rest of society (Conrad,
parent report or observational measures of social/ 2020). Children with disabilities including autism
emotional abilities often created specifically to were often removed from their families at a
measure the changes of DSP-­based interventions young age and placed in institutions and segre-
(Carter et al., 2011; Casenhiser et al., 2013; Green gated from society. They were also barred from
et  al., 2010; Pajareya & Nopmaneejumruslers, public education because they were believed to
2011; Solomon et al., 2014). No changes on stan- be “uneducable.” Lack of education compounded
dardized measures of language, adaptive skills, or developmental disability to result in further learn-
IQ/cognitive abilities have been found with DSP ing deficit. Fortunately, with the deinstitutional-
interventions (Green et  al., 2010; Smith & ization movement in the 1970s and creation of
Iadarola, 2015; Solomon et al., 2014; Zane et al., laws such as the Individuals with Disabilities
2015). This lack of evidence has not discouraged Education Act (IDEA), attitudes toward those
their use, however, with a recent survey indicating with intellectual disabilities have generally
that about 25% of the study sample were using improved, leading to broad public support for
Floortime or RDI (Becerra et al., 2017). more inclusionary educational practices and bet-
ter recognition of human rights for those with
intellectual disabilities (Scior et al., 2013).
3.2.2 E
 xtreme Beliefs Lead
to Extreme Interventions 3.2.2.2 A Road Paved with Good
Intentions: Unrealistic Positive
3.2.2.1 Unclean to Unfit: Early Negative Beliefs Lead to Poor
Beliefs About Disabilities Led Intervention
to Mistreatment Not all extreme beliefs about children with
Although ASD was first described in the 1930s autism are negative, but hopeful, positive mis-
and 1940s (Kanner, 1943), misperceptions about conceptions can be even more insidious because
developmental disabilities have existed for centu- they are often harder to discredit. We all want to
ries. As far back as Ancient Greece, evidence believe in miracles at times. This is especially
exists of cultures attributing disability as a pun- true for families told that their child has a lifelong
ishment from a higher power for sins either in a neurodevelopmental disability that could signifi-
past life or perpetrated by the family of the per- cantly impair prospects for achievement and
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 31

independence. People with autism vary signifi- then miraculously allows the person with autism
cantly in their cognitive and adaptive functioning to type out full sentences, complete college, write
as adults, ranging anywhere from successful pro- elaborate poems, and express political opinions.
fessionals living independently to having a All this was sometimes said to occur without any
marked intellectual disability and needing one-­ history or opportunity of prior learning by the
on-­one support for the majority of daily tasks. individual, as if such skills just appear instead of
While many factors play a role in how severely a resulting through gradual learning and practice
person is affected, it can seem unfair to families (Biklen, 1990). Like many non-evidence-based
who are trying to create the best life possible for interventions, support was initially provided
their child. This has led some to grasp on to the through a set of papers by a limited number of
belief that all children with autism have high authors describing the miraculous results of FC
underlying cognitive and verbal abilities, but that through emotionally charged accounts of nonver-
some people’s abilities are somehow “locked” bal individuals suddenly communicating in full
away, a belief shamelessly promoted by purvey- sentences (Biklen, 1990, 1992; Biklen et  al.,
ors of miracle cures. According to this belief, the 1992, 1995). What the articles did not include
person with autism has the same abilities as was any convincing scientific evidence. The
everyone else but is unable to communicate their authors explained that the only way the therapy
thoughts and abilities in the same way as a typi- worked was if the individual felt supported by the
cal person. Some have gone so far as to suggest facilitator and that the best facilitators were those
that those with ASD not only have average abili- who had an unshakable core belief that every
ties but that they actually have paranormal pow- individual could communicate. This suggests that
ers such as telepathy. Initially, it may seem FC only works for those who believe it works, a
harmless to allow people to think that their chil- position that many found suspect (Jacobson &
dren can do more than they are actually capable Mulick, 1994).
of, and it may even feel cruel to crush these Several studies followed debunking FC using
dreams by challenging such beliefs. Such extreme experimental methods such as asking people to
beliefs, however, are fertile breeding grounds for name pictures that their facilitator could not see
the creation and promotion of non-evidence-­ or showing the person with ASD and the facilita-
based treatments. tor a series of picture sets that sometimes matched
Introduced in Australia during the 1980s and sometimes did not (Jacobson et  al., 1995;
(Crossley et al., 1980), Facilitated Communication Wheeler et al., 1993). The only “correct” answers
(FC) has become one of the most well-known typed were for the pictures the facilitator could
controversial treatments for ASD. Starting from also see, showing clearly that the facilitators were
the idea that all autistic people want and are able unknowingly determining what was being typed
to communicate, proponents of FC vaguely (Jacobson et  al., 1995; Wheeler et  al., 1993).
described that autistic people suffered from a Proponents of FC were not deterred by such glar-
“praxis” that stops them from being able to com- ing evidence indicating the therapy was a sham,
municate both verbally and nonverbally (Biklen, arguing that those using FC were unable to per-
1990; Jacobson et  al., 1995). The intervention form in these “confrontational naming” tasks
then is to “facilitate” the person’s communication because they challenged the person to prove their
by taking their hand or arm and guiding them to ability to communicate. Some went even further
spell out what they want to say on a keyboard, to suggest that people with autism were confused
letter board, or adapted communication device. during these studies because they were really
The facilitator is supposedly not moving the indi- reading the minds of their facilitators at the same
vidual or cuing them in any way. Somehow time as they were attempting to type (Haskew &
(through a mechanism never fully explained) the Donnellan, 1993). This argument continued, with
facilitator is “supporting” the person’s hand or several more quality experimental studies pub-
wrist or touching them on the shoulder, which lished that showed no support for FC being coun-
32 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

tered by a handful of qualitative and mixed behind AAI is that animals are a source of calm-
methods studies attempting to support FC valid- ing, nonjudgmental support. Therefore, interac-
ity. A review of FC studies from 1995 to 2000 tion with animals can help with communication
(Mostert, 2001) comes to the firm conclusion that and social interaction. AAI includes both animal-­
there was never any valid evidence to support FC. assisted therapy, where an animal is employed in
Despite clear evidence indicating it is not a a treatment protocol with set therapeutic goals,
real treatment, FC continues to live on. The and animal-assisted activities, which involves
Inclusion and Communication Initiatives (for- pairing an animal with a person with the goal of
merly the Institute on Communication and general positive benefit but with no set activities
Inclusion, which was formerly the Facilitated or goals. It has been difficult to compile a unified
Communication Institute), housed in the Syracuse set of results regarding AAI, as the treatment is
University’s Center on Disability and Inclusion, difficult to quantify due to a large amount of vari-
continues to provide training for those who would ation in practice (Davis et  al., 2015). It can be
like to be facilitators of FC (https://disabilityin- provided by a range of clinicians (e.g., psycholo-
clusioncenter.syr.edu/). Articles also continue to gists, occupational therapists, speech therapist)
be published in support of FC, although still and non-clinicians (e.g., animal trainers, riding
grossly lacking in any empirical evidence to back instructors, “dog-guides”) using a range of differ-
these claims. Instead they focus on case studies ent animals including dogs, horses, dolphins,
(Faure et al., 2021), facilitator opinion (Sipilä & guinea pigs, llamas, and rabbits. How often the
Määttä, 2011), and the facilitated testimonials of therapy takes place and what activities are done
the people with ASD (McKee & Gomez, 2020). also vary widely, with durations varying from a
This persistence of FC illustrates an all too com- few weeks to several years. Reviews of the litera-
mon pattern with unsupported interventions, ture report some positive and many mixed find-
where they continue to live on despite a lack of ings (Davis et  al., 2015; Marino & Lilienfeld,
actual evidence supporting their use. Therefore, 2007; O’Haire, 2013; Trzmiel et al., 2019). More
there continues to be a need to publish updated importantly, the vast majority of studies were
research papers (Saloviita, 2018) and profes- found to have a host of methodological flaws
sional statements, such as those by the American including nonexperimental designs, use of anec-
Psychological Association (1994), American dotal evidence, outcome measures designed for
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry the specific intervention, lack of control for the
(2008), and American Psychiatric Association effect of other concurrent interventions, no mea-
and the American Speech-Language-Hearing surements of treatment fidelity, and lack of detail
Association (2018), continuing to debunk and describing the independent variables. Concerns
denounce the use of such treatments to help keep were also noted for safety risks to both partici-
families and clinicians from falling for treatments pants and animals.
making promises too good to be true. This mixed bag of poor evidence purporting
Believing that animals can cure autism is positive outcomes leads to a conundrum seen
another extreme belief that sounds so nice on the with many unsupported treatments. A lay person
surface. Many people like animals. Many people looking at the research literature would see
with autism also like animals. Being around ani- numerous studies suggesting that AAI has posi-
mals causes many people, including many autis- tive to “mixed” results. Significant methodologi-
tic people, to feel good. This has unfortunately cal flaws, however, mean that none of these
led to the leap in logic that animals can be used as studies show convincing evidence that AAI leads
a way of treating autism. Animal-assisted inter- to any objective changes in social or communica-
vention (AAI) is a broad term to refer to any tion variables related to ASD. Therefore, despite
intervention using an animal to provide therapeu- decades’ worth of research resulting in no con-
tic benefit based on a positive relationship clusive evidence supporting AAI, this treatment
between the client and the animal. The theory continues to be referred to as “unproven” (Marino
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 33

& Lilienfeld, 2007), in need of more evidence some of the most well-known examples of pseu-
(Davis et al., 2015; Trzmiel et al., 2019), or even doscience in practice.
“promising” (O’Haire, 2013) because there are
also no published experimental studies showing 3.2.3.1 Sensory Processing Disorder
firmly that it does not work. As a result, many and Related Pseudoscience
families continue to invest their limited time and Interventions
money into this unsupported intervention, which A large and prevalent group of pseudoscience
makes sense, as one conclusion that can be drawn interventions revolve around the idea of “sensory
from parent and clinician reports is that most integration” or “sensory processing” problems.
children like interacting with animals (Marino & Sensory processing broadly (and vaguely) refers
Lilienfeld, 2007) even if this interaction does not to the neurobiological organization and interpre-
lead to any meaningful change in symptoms tation of sensory stimuli coming from the body
related to ASD. and the environment (Borkowska, 2017).
Therefore, atypical sensory processing involves
inappropriate responses to sensory stimulation.
3.2.3 Biological Pseudoscience Sensory processing disorders (SPDs) are then
defined as a lack of ability to use information
Pseudoscience is defined as “a collection of received by the senses in order to efficiently func-
beliefs or practices mistakenly regarded as being tion in everyday life. This can include any combi-
based on scientific method” (Lexico, 2021b). nation of an extremely wide array of possible
Pseudoscience explanations often appear when symptoms including: being overly sensitive or
there is an incomplete understanding of a disor- under-reactive to touch, movement, sights, or
der. Our current understanding of ASD indicates sounds; being easily distracted; having social
that the disorder is caused by a combination of and/or emotional problems; having an activity
genetic and environmental factors affecting neu- level that is unusually high or unusually low;
rodevelopment (Loke et  al., 2015), but we still showing physical clumsiness or apparent care-
lack understanding into the biological processes lessness; being impulsive and lacking in self-­
that are behind this interaction. In pursuit of these control; having difficulty making transitions
answers, researchers turn to exploring all the pos- from one situation to another; having the inability
sible ways people with ASD vary from typically to unwind or calm oneself; having a poor self-­
developing people and then develop theories on concept; and/or having delays in speech, lan-
how these biological differences may explain guage, or motor skills (Jacobson et  al., 2015).
ASD. These theories then are tested often starting While diagnosing SPDs is common in the field of
with animal models and human studies with very occupational therapy, it remains unrecognized in
small sample sizes and then in controlled experi- the most well-known and widely used medical,
ments with larger human samples to aid in gener- psychiatric, or psychological nomenclatures
alization. This process takes a long time. In the including the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual,
meantime, these theories often get taken up pre- fifth edition (DSM-5), and the International
maturely by those overeager for answers or look- Classification of Diseases, tenth edition (ICD-­
ing to make money off a vulnerable population, 10). This is because, while the long list of symp-
who go on to develop treatments based on these toms captured under SPD often do occur and can
unproven possibilities. These theories and their be quite debilitating, they are already well
related treatments often sound plausible and “sci- explained by other diagnoses, such as attention
entific,” leading to families desperate for a “cure” deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), ASD, or
to try them out, despite the lack of any convinc- anxiety disorders. SPD is rarely diagnosed inde-
ing scientific evidence that they are worth the pendently of another diagnosis (Borkowska,
investment. The next few sections will outline 2017). There is also no empirical evidence to
34 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

suggest that most of these behaviors are “sen- AIT attempts to reduce hypersensitive or hyper-
sory” in nature or have anything to do with “inte- acute hearing of children with autism through
gration,” either within or of the nervous system exposure to recorded music played at loud vol-
(Jacobson et  al., 2015). Unfortunately, that has umes, from which the sound frequencies suppos-
not prevented the creation of a host of interven- edly identified as being associated with
tions meant to treat these sensory problems. “hyperacuity” have been removed (Sinha et  al.,
Sensory integration therapy (SIT) was first put 2011). The Tomatis Method attempts to achieve
forth by Jean Ayres in the 1960s and 1970s (Ayres, the same aim using a device called the “electronic
1963), and it has gained significant popularity in ear” to deliver an electrically modified human
school settings and with occupational therapists. voice (Sinha et  al., 2011). Finally, Samonas
SIT suggests that physical activities and exercises Sound Therapy uses a combination of modified
can help children learn to interpret and use sen- human voices, music, and sounds of nature to
sory information more effectively. The focus is reduce sensory difficulties (Sinha et  al., 2011).
not on changing learned associations by modify- While this theory and treatment mechanism make
ing the environment, as in behavioral interven- no sense in the light of present knowledge of
tions, but on modifying how the central nervous auditory functioning, a few controlled studies
system processes all problematic sensory input. have attempted to illustrate therapeutic benefits
Specific interventions include deep brushing, from the use of AIT (Edelson et al., 1999; Edelson
swings for vestibular input, access to different & Rimland, 2001), although outcome measures
textures, bounce pads, scooter boards, weighted have been primarily behavior checklists com-
vests, and generally increasing or decreasing the pleted by parents or therapists. A few case studies
“sensory diet,” depending on the presumed needs have reported changes in language and behavior
of the child. Many studies have been published functioning, although there was no control to
attempting to show the effectiveness of SIT thera- account for other therapies or maturation
pies, although systematic reviews continue to be (Gerritsen, 2010). Comprehensive reviews of the
published that conclude that there is little to no AIT literature concluded that AIT showed no
evidence supporting actual changes in behavior meaningful change in any outcome including
(Hyatt et  al., 2009; Lang et  al., 2012; Weitlauf language, cognitive skills, or behavior (Sinha
et  al., 2017). Direct comparisons of SIT and et al., 2011; Villasenor et al., 2018). Furthermore,
behavior therapy show that behavioral interven- there were some concerns that the therapy could
tions reliably decrease challenging behavior, actually harm hearing if done inappropriately
while SIT has no demonstrable effects on the (Sinha et  al., 2011). Like many non-evidence-­
same behaviors (Addison et al., 2012; Cox et al., based treatments, however, AIT continues to be
2009; Devlin et  al., 2011; Devlin et  al., 2009; repackaged and presented as something new,
Lydon et  al., 2017). Despite a vast body of evi- with the most recent iteration titled Integrated
dence showing little effect, sensory integration Listening Systems (Schoen et  al., 2015). This
remains one of the most frequently used interven- treatment combines both the hallmark modified
tions available for children with ASD, with an sounds with other movement-based sensory
estimated 95% of occupational therapist using interventions. So far only one uncontrolled pilot
this intervention (Schaaf et al., 2018). study has been published looking at the use of
Another group of treatments with roots in the this treatment in ASD (Schoen et al., 2015), but
sensory differences seen in ASD are audio inte- that has not stopped it from already being widely
gration treatments (AIT), which posit that behav- marketed to families as a possible intervention.
ior can be influenced by how we hear, and
hypersensitive hearing can limit people’s ability 3.2.3.2 Neuroinflammation Theory
to learn and pay attention. AIT was first devel- and Related Pseudoscience
oped in France in the 1960s and became popular Treatments
in the United States for the treatment of ASD in Neuroinflammation theory of ASD suggests that
the 1990s (Mudford & Cullen, 2015). Classic autism is caused by the prolonged inflammation
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 35

of the central nervous system that leads to accounting for the effect of other interventions
changes in brain functioning (Matta et al., 2019). that the child may have been receiving during the
Support for this theory remains slim at this time, time each study was conducted or even the effects
with proponents primarily relying on data from of natural maturation. Even in the RCT, the only
postmortem studies in people with autism and groups compared were two different chiropractic
animal models (Matta et al., 2019). Despite this treatments, and there was no objective measure
paucity in support, several treatments applied to of behavior change.
children with ASD focused on decreasing this A third treatment with the goal of reducing
supposed inflammation. neuroinflammation is hyperbaric oxygen therapy
Supporters of cranial osteopathy claim that (HBOT). HBOT involves enclosing an individual
through gentle manipulation of the bones of the in a pressurized chamber, which allows the per-
skull, cranial osteopathy can restore the “rhythm” son to inhale up to 100% oxygen at a pressure
of the cerebrospinal fluid and help its circulation, greater than one atmosphere. This pressurization
which will in turn reduce inflammation (Levy & of oxygen allows high concentrations of oxygen
Hyman, 2008). What and where this “disruption to be delivered deeper into tissues than would
in rhythm” is varies across people and is suppos- normally occur. HBOT is validated for use in
edly determined as the therapist becomes famil- treating a variety of conditions including decom-
iar with each client’s various tensions, vibrations, pression sickness, as well as inflammatory condi-
and natural rhythms through touching and mas- tions such as chronic diabetic ulcers. Advocates
saging the person’s head. While the therapy has of using HBOT to treat ASD theorize that HBOT
been around since the early 1900s, it was intro- can decrease neuroinflammation and therefore
duced as a treatment for ASD in the 1970s despite reduce symptoms of ASD. As with many pseudo-
very little research looking into its safety and science treatments, several initial case studies
effectiveness. In the four decades that this treat- and uncontrolled pilot studies indicated some
ment has been used with countless children with possible benefits (Rossignol et  al., 2012). As
ASD, there have been no randomized control tri- more rigorous studies were published, however,
als showing any definitive support for this treat- no support for this therapy was found in the treat-
ment reducing symptoms related to autism (Kratz ment of autism symptoms (Goldfarb et al., 2016).
et al., 2017; Levy & Hyman, 2008). There have Furthermore, the US Food and Drug
been a number of case studies, however, summa- Administration has put out consumer updates
rizing that the families and practitioners who warning families that HBOT is not approved for
invest their money and energy into doing the treating autism and could be potentially harmful
therapy like it (Kratz et  al., 2017), despite the (United States Department of Health and Human
lack of evidence of actual behavior change. Services, Food and Drug Administration, 2019).
Another therapy focused on reliving neuroin- Despite the lack of evidence, concern for harm,
flammation, this time by relieving irritation to the and the high cost, HBOT is relatively easy to find
spinal nerves through proper spine alignment, is through an Internet search and continues to be
chiropractic care. This treatment also remains pushed as a miracle cure for autism and a host of
unsupported for treating symptoms of other developmental disabilities.
ASD. While there are several case studies, cohort
studies, and even a supposed “randomized con- 3.2.3.3 Poor Gut Health, ASD,
trol” study stating that chiropractic care led to and Related Pseudoscience
positive changes in children with ASD (Alcantara For the last several decades, various theories have
et al., 2011), these studies were all significantly come about attempting to link ASD to poor gut
flawed. The primary outcome measure in the health. Gastrointestinal (GI) distress appears to
majority of studies was parent or clinician be a common complaint in children with ASD,
unblinded report and none of the studies included with almost half of individuals with ASD report-
a control group. Therefore, there was no way of ing some level of GI distress (Holingue et  al.,
36 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

2018). Given this high occurrence, researchers Digestive enzyme therapy also has its roots in
have begun exploring this connection, and mul- opioid excess theory. Supporters of digestive
tiple theories attempting to link various GI con- enzyme therapy believe that taking particular
cerns as possible causes of ASD have appeared. enzymes, which reportedly break down exor-
One well-known pseudoscience theory based phins into smaller peptides that do not have opi-
on poor gut health in autistic people is the opioid oid activity, can improve the digestion of proteins
excess theory of ASD.  This theory posits that like gluten and casein (Saad et al., 2015). This in
ASD could be caused by excess opioid activity in turn could reduce the characteristics of autism.
the brain, which is related to digestive problems So far, two RCTs have focused on evaluating the
with particular proteins in food (Mari-Bauset effectiveness of digestive enzyme therapy. The
et al., 2014). It is known that certain types of pro- first found no evidence of improvement in symp-
teins are related to opioid activity in the intes- toms and skills related to ASD (Munasinghe
tines. Based on this, it has been theorized that the et al., 2010). The second did find a small but sig-
intestinal mucosa in autistic people could be nificant improvement in scores on the childhood
more permeable. Having such a “leaky gut” could autism rating scale (Saad et al., 2015), although
possibly allow proteins associated with opioid both articles have been noted to have a moderate
activity to move intact into the bloodstream. If risk for bias (Sathe et al., 2017). Given this and
these proteins were then transported across the the mixed results of these two trials, there is little
blood-brain barrier in large enough quantities, it support for the wide use of these enzymes.
could affect brain functioning. Multiple studies Several other vitamins and dietary supple-
have shown no support for this theory by high- ments have also been put forth as possible treat-
lighting that people with ASD do not have a ments for ASD under even vaguer theories related
higher concentration of opioid peptides in to correcting poor gut health, including secretin,
plasma, the nervous system, or urinary excretion Vitamin D, Vitamin B6 and magnesium, antifun-
(Mari-Bauset et al., 2014). Despite lack of sup- gal agents, and probiotics. These treatments fol-
port for the underlying mechanisms of the opioid low a similar cycle often seen in
excess theory, multiple popular dietary interven- non-evidence-based interventions. They tend to
tions have been created around this pseudosci- gain popularity based on a few initial studies,
ence explanation. often case studies done by a small group of
Elimination diets are one such intervention authors documenting miraculous results. Use
that has grown particularly popular. In particular then becomes widespread, long before quality
the gluten-free and casein-free diet continues to studies have been completed. Years later, con-
be a popular alternative medicine option, with a trolled, double-blinded research studies with
recent study indicating that about 20% of fami- quality outcome measures show no support for
lies with ASD are using this intervention (Akins the treatment of ASD, but by then it is difficult to
et  al., 2014). This diet involves not eating any battle the flood of testimonial evidence and mar-
food containing gluten (e.g., wheat, oats, barley, keting touting the intervention as the miracle cure
rye flours, bread, cereals, pasta) and eliminating families have been waiting for.
the intake of dairy products, which all contain One example of this cycle is the rise and fall of
casein. The concern is that gluten and casein secretin supplementation to treat ASD. Secretin
could be sources of “exorphins,” the peptides is an endogenous gastrointestinal polypeptide
with opioid activities that concern proponents of composed of 27 amino acids that stimulates the
opioid excess theory. While there is plenty of tes- secretion of digestive fluids from the pancreas.
timonial evidence from families and providers Porcine secretin injections are used by gastroen-
purporting changes in children after the introduc- terologists during diagnostic tests to determine
tion of such diets, these diets remain unsupported the etiology of intestinal complaints. Secretin
in RCTs using objective measures of behavioral receptors have also been found in the brain,
change (Mari-Bauset et al., 2014; Millward et al., although their function is not well understood.
2008; Piwowarczyk et al., 2018). Secretin supplementation to treat ASD became
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 37

popular after a case study was published in 1998 et al., 2019). Unfortunately, this retraction came
documenting large improvements in the language after a review of the connection between vitamin
and social skills of three children with ASD after D and ASD was published by some of the same
they received porcine secretin injections as part authors in which they cite this article as key sup-
of routine gastrointestinal diagnostic tests port for this intervention (Jia et al., 2018), illus-
(Sokolski & Wachtel, 1998). Sokolski and trating how bad science can continue to proliferate
Wachtel (1998) theorized that these improve- even when steps are taken to correct it.
ments were related to a brain-gut interaction Prevalence of elimination diets and the use of
involving the secretin receptors in the brain, vitamins and dietary supplements to attempt to
although they did indicate that more research was treat ASD remains high despite the lack of evi-
needed to better understand this mechanism. Use dence supporting these interventions, with 25% of
of secretin supplementation quickly became a recent sample of families endorsed using some
widespread following this study, with estimates sort of dietary supplement to treat their child’s
indicating that thousands of children were receiv- autism (Akins et al., 2014). Continued high use is
ing secretin a year later (Esch & Carr, 2004). likely due to a feeling of “what’s the harm in try-
Unfortunately several years later, the completion ing?” Diet is something many families feel like
of multiple randomized controlled studies they have more control over, and diet-­based inter-
showed no evidence of symptom improvement ventions have no waitlist. Unfortunately, these
on several measures of core ASD symptoms, as interventions are not cost-­free. Supplements and
well as no improvement in communication, special foods are expensive, and money wasted on
behavior, affect, and visuospatial skills these interventions is money that cannot be more
(Krishnaswami et  al., 2011; Williams et  al., effectively invested in other supported treatments.
2012). The evidence was so compelling that the Second, given that many children with ASD are
reviews concluded that there was no need for fur- already picky eaters, they can be at an even higher
ther study of secretin as a treatment for ASD bar- risk for malnutrition when large groups of foods
ring any drastically new evidence. It does appear are eliminated from their diet (Mari-Bauset et al.,
that this evidence has also been compelling 2014). Similarly, the side effects of many dietary
enough to actually curb use of this supplement supplements are not well understood, particularly
for now with only one family (i.e., 0.2% of the on the developing brain of a child. Finally, special
sample) indicating use in a recent survey (Akins diets could further limit certain social opportuni-
et al., 2014). ties or lead to further stigmatization from peers
Systematic reviews show a similar lack of (Millward et  al., 2008). The ease of access to
quality evidence for the use of probiotics (Ng these interventions, however, makes them irresist-
et  al., 2019), Vitamin B6 and magnesium (Li ible to some families, at least to try initially. Then,
et  al., 2018; Nye & Brice, 2005), and omega 3 given the amount of effort and resources that go
fatty acids (James et al., 2011; Li et al., 2018) to into sticking to a difficult diet or following a daily
treat the core symptoms of ASD.  Vitamin D supplement regimen, it is not surprising that fami-
seems to be making its way currently through this lies would be susceptible to placebo effects, see-
cycle with a small group of researchers showing ing change when it is not actually there.
initially highly promising results in a case study
and further studies using parent report or 3.2.3.4 Heavy Metal Poisoning
unblinded clinician ratings of behavior. This and the Vaccine Controversy
group also attempted to publish a randomized Perhaps one the most insidious pseudoscience
control study; however, several inconsistencies in theories related to autism is the idea originally
the results led to an examination of the data by put forth by Andrew Wakefield that autism is
the journal’s editors. The results found in the caused by common childhood vaccines
paper were unable to be replicated by the editors, (Wakefield et al., 2010). Wakefield’s exact theory
and so the article was officially retracted (Saad of how vaccines cause ASD has changed over
38 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

time. His initial research was an extension of the Reviewing the research studies looking at chela-
opioid excess theory, positing that the measles tion also shows that it is unsupported. The
part of the measles mumps, and rubella (MMR) research body consists of a few case studies and
vaccine was causing ASD by creating a “leaky poorly designed experimental studies (i.e., no
gut” (Wakefield et al., 2010). This original work control group, no control for additional interven-
was shown to be largely falsified, and the original tions such as behavior therapy), which primarily
article was retracted due to severe methodologi- used anecdotal parent report as an outcome mea-
cal flaws including not having approval of an sure, and even the majority of these poor studies
Institutional Review Board (Caplan, 2009). The showed mixed results (Davis et al., 2013; James
idea that autism was caused by the vaccines has et al., 2015). Given the lack of evidence and the
persisted, however, with concern then turning to high potential for danger, the FDA has issued
thimerosal, a preservative contained in several statements warning consumers away from using
common childhood vaccines. The worry was that chelation to treat autism (United States
the thimerosal would somehow stay in a child’s Department of Health and Human Services, Food
body after the vaccine was given and cause mer- and Drug Administration, 2019). It appears that
cury poisoning. One of the most likely reasons use of this therapy is now minimal, with only
for this persistence is timing of childhood vac- 4.4% of families endorsing using this treatment
cines with the presentation of ASD symptoms. A for their child with autism (Akins et  al., 2014),
subgroup of children with ASD goes through a although given the dangers even this seems like
period of apparently typical development and far too many.
then loses skills such as language and eye contact
around 18  months of age, which happens to be
when the MMR vaccine is given. While there 3.3  topping the Use of Non-­
S
have been several studies done showing that this evidence-­Based Practices
timing is an unfortunate coincidence (related to
the fact that clear symptoms of ASD are difficult The battle against non-evidence-based practices
to discern until then) and that vaccines do not is ongoing. It is the duty of all practitioners who
cause autism (Madsen et al., 2003), this fear has work with individuals with ASD and their fami-
led to a sizable minority of people choosing not lies to continue this fight. In order to do this, we
to vaccinate their children. must first understand why families might choose
Despite the lack of evidence connecting heavy non-evidence-based treatments. Next, we have to
metal poisoning and autism, enterprising indi- develop strategies for successfully disseminating
viduals have devised interventions focused on information of evidence-based treatment while
treating this fake problem. One such treatment is combating misinformation in a way that changes
chelation. Chelation treatment uses various opinions rather than alienates those we are trying
chemical substances injected into the blood- to help.
stream for the purpose of binding and then with-
drawing specific metals from the person’s body,
which are then excreted in the urine (Davis et al., 3.3.1 W
 hy Do Families Choose
2013; James et  al., 2015). Unfortunately, these Non-evidence-Based
chemical substances that are supposed to rid the Practices?
body of potential poisonous metals are also asso-
ciated with several potential serious side effects, Research is mixed on whether there are specific
including fever, vomiting, diarrhea, loss of appe- factors that influence families’ choice of non-­
tite, hypertension, hemorrhoid symptoms, metal- evidence-­based practices, although much sug-
lic taste, hypotension, cardiac arrhythmias, and gests that parents from a variety of financial,
hypocalcemia, the latter of which can in turn educational, and familial backgrounds are all
cause fatal cardiac arrest (Davis et  al., 2013). equally likely to choose unsupported treatment
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 39

options (Miller et al., 2012). Families report that culty determining what is actually working
the amount of information on autism and related (Matson et al., 2013).
interventions is overwhelming and often conflict- All of these factors create space for non-­
ing (Frame & Casey, 2019), and the average par- evidence-­based treatments, particularly those
ent does not have the time, resources, or expertise offering quick fixes for much less effort.
to review all available information to make good Unfortunately, this leads to families wasting
decisions (Matson et al., 2013). Families often do precious resources including their time, effort,
not weigh information in the same way as clini- and money on intervention that at the very least
cians or researchers. Families frequently rely on will lead to little to no positive change. At their
testimonials and recommendations from other worst, these interventions can lead to harm.
parents and parent support groups more than sci- Therefore, it is the responsibility of all those
entific sources (Matson et al., 2013; Miller et al., who work with families with children with ASD
2012). For example, a parent doing research on to take steps to advocate for better access to
ABA may be weighing a research study in a peer-­ treatment and help counter misinformation to
reviewed journal showing the effectiveness of help families make the best choices for their
ABA against a video posted on social media of children.
another parent saying ABA causes a child to be
“a robot” or caused their child to have PTSD
symptoms. Such accounts would lead many par- 3.3.2 How to Address
ents to see the evidence for ABA to be mixed at Misinformation
best. Further adding to the confusion, profession-
als including physicians, educators, psycholo- We have explained the difference between plau-
gists, and speech, occupational, and physical sible, science-based information on autism and
therapists often recommend unsupported treat- approaches that have either not panned out or that
ments (Frame & Casey, 2019; McCormack et al., never made sense to begin with. There are many
2020; Miller et al., 2012). more that we have not mentioned. Families and
Good treatment is also hard to get, and out- even non-specialist professionals are often
comes are incremental, long term, and unpredict- tempted to attend to the promise of new, attrac-
able. Accessing ABA often takes incredible tive ideas that seem promising. There is a saying
resources and organizational skills to identify worth remembering, “if something seems too
possible providers and work out funding possi- good to be true, it probably isn’t.” Obviously, that
bilities. A full ABA program is expensive, and is not enough to protect us from well-meaning
insurance coverage is often suboptimal, leading but wrong-headed or outright fraudulent offers of
to families having to cobble together funding ineffective help. But where should people look
from scholarships and waivers or even having to for information about approaches we have not
fund treatment out of pocket. Waitlists are also covered or those yet to come? And how do we
often very long and provider turnover is high correct misinformation and best guide families to
(Frame & Casey, 2019), and there are many areas make informed decisions? Given our current cul-
of the county, such as more rural areas, where tural climate of mis- and dis-information, many
access to ABA is nonexistent (Matson et  al., national organizations are putting together guide-
2013). These barriers leave many families feeling lines on how to identify and correct misinforma-
understandably frustrated and lead to them giv- tion (e.g., https://ethicalleadership.nd.edu/news/
ing up on accessing quality treatment. Even for how-­t o-­s top-­t he-­s preadof-­m isinformation/,
those families who can access ABA, the lure of https://www.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/surgeon-­
non-evidence-based treatments is often too general-­misinformation-­advisory.pdf, https://
tempting. Families want to do everything they www.npr.org/2021/07/21/1018874736/how-­to-­
can to help their child. Therefore, they often start correct-­misinformation). Here is a summary of
multiple treatments at once and then have diffi- some of the main points.
40 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

Inoculate families before they encounter mis- Misinformation is not easy to identify short
information. This is ideally done close to when a of access to a university library and the skills
child receives a diagnosis but can also be used as necessary to understand how to check claims
an introduction at any time when treatment against the relevant background and method-
options are being discussed. Give families a ological standards of valid evidence. There are
shortened overview of what constitutes good evi- some watchdog groups that do try to assist con-
dence. It can also be helpful to give an overview sumers of autism information in this regard. The
of some of the common misperceptions they will Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies
likely hear as they explore social media or talk to (https://behavior.org/) has extensive informa-
other families and professionals and then evi- tion about autism in the form of Internet post-
dence for why these treatments are unsupported. ings and publications. A similar group devoted
Start with listening. When families state incor- specifically to autism-related information is the
rect information or suggest that they are about to Association for Science in Autism Treatment
make a bad choice on treatment, we naturally (https://asatonline.org/).
want to correct them as swiftly as possible.
Unfortunately, immediately arguing with ­families
or discounting misinformed treatment choices References
will often lead to them doubling down on their
beliefs rather than being open to change. Instead, Addison, L. R., Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Bachmeyer,
M. H., Rivas, K. M., Milnes, S. M., & Oddo, J. (2012).
ask them to explain why they have chosen a par- A comparison of sensory integrative and behavioral
ticular non-evidence-based treatment or why therapies as treatment for pediatric feeding disorders.
they might not be interested in an evidence-­based Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(3), 455–471.
treatment option. Once you understand their rea- https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-­455
Akins, C.  R. S., Krakowiak, P., Angkustsiri, K., Hertz-­
soning, you can better present correct informa- Picciotto, I., & Hansen, R. L. (2014). Utilization pat-
tion that targets those specific concerns. terns of conventional and complementary/alternative
Give families better options to find informa- treatments in children with autism spectrum disorders
tion. Much of the vast resource of the Internet is and developmental disabilities in a population-based
study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
devoted mainly to marketing of products, whether Pediatrics, 35(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1097/
or not any specific item of content is labeled as DBP.0000000000000013
such. YouTube, Twitter, and Facebook are not Albrecht, G.  L., Seelman, K.  D., & Bury, M. (Eds.).
good places to do medical research. Public health (2001). Handbook of disability studies. Sage.
Alcantara, J., Alcantara, J. D., & Alcantara, J. (2011). A
and other government agencies like the Center systematic review of the literature on the chiroprac-
for Disease Control and the National Institutes of tic care of patients with autism spectrum disorder.
Health often have a great deal of information Explore, 7(6), 384–390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
about the things that are well understood and sci- explore.2011.08.001
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.
entifically validated. These agencies take some (2008). Facilitated communication [Position
time to consider a given topic or treatment, nec- Statement]. https://www.aacap.org/AACAP/Policy_
essary time needed to accumulate information Statements/2008/Facilitated_Communication.aspx
based on solid and repeated scientific findings, American Psychological Association. (1994). Resolution
on facilitated communication by the American
and will not be found to have reacted to the very Psychological Association. Adopted in Council.
latest fads or claims. Similarly, main line scien- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2018).
tific and professional societies like the National Facilitated communication [Position Statement].
Academy of Science, the American Academy of www.asha.org/policy/
Arnold-Saritepe, A.  M., Mudford, O.  C., & Cullen, C.
Pediatrics, the American Psychological (2015). Gentle teaching. In Controversial therapies
Association, or the Association for Behavior for autism and intellectual disabilities (pp. 359–366).
Analysis International all present useful evidence-­ Routledge.
based information on their web sites and in Ayres, A. J. (1963). The development of perceptual-motor
abilities: A theoretical basis for treatment of dysfunc-
publications.
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 41

tion. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 52(1), 685–716. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.


17(6), 221–225. psych.52.1.685
Baker, J.  P. (2010). Autism in 1959: Joey the mechani- Conrad, J.  A. (2020). On intellectual and developmen-
cal boy. Pediatrics, 125(6), 1101–1103. https://doi. tal disabilities in the United States: A historical per-
org/10.1542/peds.2010-­0846 spective. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities, 24(1),
Barth, R. P., Crea, T. M., John, K., Thoburn, J., & Quinton, 85–101. https://doi.org/10.1177/1744629518767001
D. (2005). Beyond attachment theory and therapy: Cook, K. A., & Willmerdinger, A. N. (2015). The history
Towards sensitive and evidence-based interventions of autism. Narrative Documents. Book 1. http://schol-
with foster and adoptive families in distress. Child arexchange.furman.edu/schopler-­about/1
& Family Social Work, 10(4), 257–268. https://doi. Cox, A. L., Gast, D. L., Luscre, D., & Ayres, K. M. (2009).
org/10.1111/j.1365-­2206.2005.00380.x The effects of weighted vests on appropriate in-seat
Becerra, T. A., Massolo, M. L., Yau, V. M., Owen-Smith, behaviors of elementary-age students with autism and
A. A., Lynch, F. L., Crawford, P. M., Pearson, K. A., severe to profound intellectual disabilities. Focus on
Pomichowski, M. E., Quinn, V. P., Yoshida, C. K., & Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 24(1),
Croen, L.  A. (2017). A survey of parents with chil- 17–26. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357608330753
dren on the autism spectrum: Experience with services Critchfield, T.  S., Doepke, K.  J., Kimberly Epting, L.,
and treatments. The Permanente Journal, 21, 16–009. Becirevic, A., Reed, D. D., Fienup, D. M., Kremsreiter,
https://doi.org/10.7812/TPP/16-­009 J.  L., & Ecott, C.  L. (2017). Normative emotional
Biklen, D. (1990). Communication unbound: Autism responses to behavior analysis jargon or how not
and praxis. Harvard Educational Review, 60(3), 291– to use words to win friends and influence people.
315. https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.60.3.013h502286 Behavior Analysis in Practice, 10(2), 97–106. https://
2vu732 doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­016-­0161-­9
Biklen, D. (1992). Typing to talk: Facilitated com- Crossley, R., McDonald, A., & Molloy, S.  P. A. (1980).
munication. American Journal of Speech-­ Annie’s coming out. Penguin New York.
Language Pathology, 1(2), 15–17. https://doi. Davis, T.  N., O’Reilly, M., Kang, S., Lang, R., Rispoli,
org/10.1044/1058-­0360.0102.15 M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Copeland, D., Attai, S.,
Biklen, D., Morton, M.  W., Gold, D., Berrigan, C., & & Mulloy, A. (2013). Chelation treatment for autism
Swaminathan, S. (1992). Facilitated communica- spectrum disorders: A systematic review. Research in
tion: Implications for individuals with autism. Topics Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(1), 49–55. https://doi.
in Language Disorders, 12(4), 1–28. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.06.005
org/10.1097/00011363-­199208000-­00003 Davis, T.  N., Scalzo, R., Butler, E., Stauffer, M., Farah,
Biklen, D., Saha, N., & Kliewer, C. (1995). How teachers Y.  N., Perez, S., Mainor, K., Clark, C., Miller, S.,
confirm the authorship of facilitated communication: Kobylecky, A., & Coviello, L. (2015). Animal assisted
A portfolio approach. The Journal of the Association interventions for children with autism spectrum dis-
for Persons With Severe Handicaps, 20(1), 45–56. order: A systematic review. Education and Training
https://doi.org/10.1177/154079699502000105 in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 50(3),
Borkowska, A. R. (2017). Sensory processing disorders– 316–329.
diagnostic and therapeutic controversies. Current Devlin, S., Leader, G., & Healy, O. (2009). Comparison
Issues in Personality Psychology, 5(3), 196–205. of behavioral intervention and sensory-integration
https://doi.org/10.5114/cipp.2017.70140 therapy in the treatment of self-injurious behavior.
Caplan, A.  L. (2009). Retraction—Ileal-lymphoid-­ Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(1), 223–
nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and perva- 231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.06.004
sive developmental disorder in children. The Weekly Devlin, S., Healy, O., Leader, G., & Hughes, B.  M.
Epidemiological Record, 84, 301–308. https://doi. (2011). Comparison of behavioral intervention
org/10.1016/S0140-­6736(97)11096-­0 and sensory-integration therapy in the treatment
Carter, A. S., Messinger, D. S., Stone, W. L., Celimli, S., of challenging behavior. Journal of Autism and
Nahmias, A.  S., & Yoder, P. (2011). A randomized Developmental Disorders, 41(10), 1303–1320. https://
controlled trial of Hanen’s ‘More Than Words’ in tod- doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­010-­1149-­x
dlers with early autism symptoms. Journal of Child Edelson, S., & Rimland, B. (2001). The efficacy of audi-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(7), 741–752. https:// tory integration training: Summaries and critiques of
doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-­7610.2011.02395.x 28 reports (January, 1993—May, 2001). Accessed at
Casenhiser, D. M., Shanker, S. G., & Stieben, J. (2013). www.up-­to-­date.com/saitwebsite/aitsummary
Learning through interaction in children with autism: Edelson, S.  M., Arin, D., Bauman, M., Lukas, S.  E.,
Preliminary data from asocial-communication-based Rudy, J.  H., Sholar, M., & Rimland, B. (1999).
intervention. Autism, 17(2), 220–241. https://doi. Auditory integration training: A double-blind study
org/10.1177/1362361311422052 of behavioral and electrophysiological effects in
Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically people with autism. Focus on Autism and Other
supported psychological interventions: Controversies Developmental Disabilities, 14(2), 73–81. https://doi.
and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, org/10.1177/108835769901400202
42 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

Esch, B.  E., & Carr, J.  E. (2004). Secretin as a treat- tic spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and Other
ment for autism: A review of the evidence. Journal Developmental Disabilities, 20(4), 213–222. https://
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 543– doi.org/10.1177/10883576050200040301
556. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­004-­2549-­6 Jacobson, J. W., & Mulick, J. A. (1994). Facilitated com-
Faure, P., Legou, T., & Gepner, B. (2021). Evidence of munication: Better education through applied ideol-
authorship on messages in facilitated communica- ogy. Journal of Behavioral Education, 4(1), 93–105.
tion: A case report using accelerometry. Frontiers https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01560511
in Psychiatry, 11, 1560. https://doi.org/10.3389/ Jacobson, J. W., & Mulick, J. A. (2000). System and cost
fpsyt.2020.543385 research issues in treatments for people with autis-
Frame, K.  N., & Casey, L.  B. (2019). Variables influ- tic disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
encing parental treatment selection for children Disorders, 30(6), 585–593. https://doi.org/10.102
with autism spectrum disorder. Children and Youth 3/a:1005691411255
Services Review, 106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Jacobson, J.  W., Mulick, J.  A., & Schwartz, A.  A.
childyouth.2019.104464. (1995). A history of facilitated communica-
Gerritsen, J. (2010). The effect of Tomatis therapy tion: Science, pseudoscience, and antiscience sci-
on children with autism: Eleven case studies. The ence working group on facilitated communication.
Intl. Journal of Listening, 24(1), 50–68. https://doi. American Psychologist, 50(9), 750–765. https://doi.
org/10.1080/10904010903466378 org/10.1037/0003-­066X.50.9.750
Goldfarb, C., Genore, L., Hunt, C., Flanagan, J., Handley-­ Jacobson, J.  W., Mulick, J.  A., & Green, G. (1998).
Derry, M., Jethwa, A., … Anagnostou, E. (2016). Cost–benefit estimates for early intensive behav-
Hyperbaric oxygen therapy for the treatment of ioral intervention for young children with autism—
children and youth with autism spectrum disorders: General model and single state case. Behavioral
An evidence-based systematic review. Research in Interventions: Theory & Practice in Residential
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 29–30, 1–7. https://doi. & Community-Based Clinical Programs, 13(4),
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.05.004 201–226. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-­
Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., 078X(199811)13:4<201::AID-­BIN17>3.0.CO;2-­R
Slonims, V., Howlin, P., … Pickles, A. (2010). Parent-­ Jacobson, J. W., Mulick, J. A., Foxx, R. M., & Kryszak, E.
mediated communication-focused treatment in chil- (2015). History of fad, pseudoscientific, and dubious
dren with autism (PACT): A randomised controlled treatments in intellectual disabilities: From the 1800s
trial. The Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160. https://doi. to today. In Controversial therapies for autism and
org/10.1016/S0140-­6736(10)60587-­9 intellectual disabilities (pp. 65–90). Routledge.
Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (2006). Engaging autism: James, S., Montgomery, P., & Williams, K. (2011).
Using the floortime approach to help children relate, Omega-3 fatty acids supplementation for
communicate, and think. Da Capo Lifelong Books. autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Cochrane
Gutstein, S. E. (2009). Empowering families through rela- Database of Systematic Reviews, (11). https://doi.
tionship developmental intervention: An important org/10.1002/14651858.CD007992.pub2
part of the biopsychosocial management of autism James, S., Stevenson, S.  W., Silove, N., & Williams,
spectrum disorders. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, K. (2015). Chelation for autism spectrum disorder
21(3), 174–182. (ASD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,
Haskew, P., & Donnellan, A. M. (1993). Emotional matu- 9(11). https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD010766.
rity and well-being: Psychological lessons of facili- pub2
tated communication. DRI Press. Jia, F., Shan, L., Wang, B., Li, H., Miao, C., Xu, Z., Lin,
Hemphill, R. (1966). Historical witchcraft and psychiatric C.  P., & Saad, K. (2018). Bench to bedside review:
illness in western Europe. Proceedings of the Royal Possible role of vitamin D in autism spectrum disor-
Society of Medicine, 59(9), 891–901. https://doi. der. Psychiatry Research, 260, 360–365. https://doi.
org/10.1177/003591576605900947 org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.005
Holingue, C., Newill, C., Lee, L.-C., Pasricha, P.  J., & Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con-
Daniele Fallin, M. (2018). Gastrointestinal symptoms tact. Nervous Child, 2(3), 217–250.
in autism spectrum disorder: A review of the literature Koocher, G. P., & Gill, E. (2015). Pet me, sniff me, squeeze
on ascertainment and prevalence. Autism Research, me: Quack treatment for autism. In Controversial
11(1), 24–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1854 therapies for Autism and intellectual disabilities
Hyatt, K.  J., Stephenson, J., & Carter, M. (2009). A (pp. 367–376). Routledge.
review of three controversial educational practices: Kratz, S.  V., Kerr, J., & Porter, L. (2017). The use of
Perceptual motor programs, sensory integration, and CranioSacral therapy for Autism Spectrum Disorders:
tinted lenses. Education and Treatment of Children, Benefits from the viewpoints of parents, clients,
32, 313–342. and therapists. Journal of Bodywork and Movement
Ingersoll, B., Dvortcsak, A., Whalen, C., & Sikora, D. Therapies, 21(1), 19–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
(2005). The effects of a developmental, social— jbmt.2016.06.006
Pragmatic language intervention on rate of expressive Krishnaswami, S., McPheeters, M.  L., & Veenstra-­
language production in young children with autis- VanderWeele, J. (2011). A systematic review of
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 43

secretin for children with autism spectrum disor- Mari-Bauset, S., Zazpe, I., Mari-Sanchis, A., Llopis-­
ders. Pediatrics, 127(5), e1322–e1325. https://doi. González, A., & Morales-Suarez-Varela, M. (2014).
org/10.1542/peds.2011-­0428 Evidence of the gluten-free and casein-free diet in
Kryszak, E.  M., Scherr, J.  F., & Mulick, J.  A. (2018). autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review.
Autism Spectrum. Child and adolescent psycho- Journal of Child Neurology, 29(12), 1718–1727.
therapy: Components of evidence-based treat- https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073814531330
ments for youth and their parents, 30. https://doi. Marino, L., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Dolphin-assisted
org/10.1017/9781316717615 therapy: More flawed data and more flawed conclu-
Kupferstein, H. (2018). Evidence of increased PTSD sions. Anthrozoös, 20(3), 239–249. https://doi.org/10
symptoms in autistics exposed to applied behavior .2752/089279307x224782
analysis. Advances in Autism, 4(1), 19–29. https://doi. Matson, J. L., Adams, H. L., Williams, L. W., & Rieske,
org/10.1108/Aia-­08-­2017-­0016 R. D. (2013). Why are there so many unsubstantiated
Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Healy, O., Rispoli, M., Lydon, treatments in autism? Research in Autism Spectrum
H., Streusand, W., Davis, T., Kang, S., Sigafoos, Disorders, 7(3), 466–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
J., Lancioni, G., Didden, R., & Giesbers, S. (2012). rasd.2012.11.006
Sensory integration therapy for autism spectrum Matta, S. M., Hill-Yardin, E. L., & Crack, P. J. (2019). The
disorders: A systematic review. Research in Autism influence of neuroinflammation in Autism Spectrum
Spectrum Disorders, 6(3), 1004–1018. https://doi. Disorder. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 79, 75–90.
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.01.006 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2019.04.037
Leaf, J. B., Ross, R. K., Cihon, J. H., & Weiss, M. J. (2018). McCormack, G., Dillon, A.  C., Healy, O., Walsh, C., &
Evaluating Kupferstein’s claims of the relationship of Lydon, S. (2020). Primary care physicians’ knowl-
behavioral intervention to PTSS for individuals with edge of autism and evidence-based interventions for
autism. Advances in Autism, 4(3), 122–129. https:// autism: A systematic review. Review Journal of Autism
doi.org/10.1108/aia-­02-­2018-­0007 and Developmental Disorders, 7(3), 226–241. https://
Levy, S.  E., & Hyman, S.  L. (2008). Complementary doi.org/10.1007/s40489-­019-­00189-­4
and alternative medicine treatments for children with McGee, J., Menolascino, F., Hobbs, D., & Menousek,
autism spectrum disorders. Child and Adolescent P. (2009). Gentle teaching. Gentle Teaching
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17(4), 803–820. International.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2008.06.004 McKee, A., & Gomez, A.  S. (2020). The voices of typ-
Lexico. (2021a). Evidence. https://www.lexico.com/ ers: Examining the educational experiences of indi-
definition/evidence viduals who use facilitated communication. Disability
Lexico. (2021b). Pseudoscience. https://www.lexico.com/ Studies Quarterly, 40(4). https://doi.org/10.18061/
definition/pseudoscience dsq.v40i4.6981
Li, Y.-J., Li, Y.-M., & Xiang, D.-X. (2018). Supplement Mercer, J. (2013a). Deliverance, demonic possession, and
intervention associated with nutritional deficiencies mental illness: Some considerations for mental health
in autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review. professionals. Mental Health, Religion and Culture,
European Journal of Nutrition, 57(7), 2571–2582. 16(6), 595–611. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.20
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-­017-­1528-­6 12.706272
Loke, Y.  J., Hannan, A.  J., & Craig, J.  M. (2015). The Mercer, J. (2013b). International concerns about hold-
role of epigenetic change in autism spectrum dis- ing therapy. Research on Social Work Practice, 24(2),
orders. Frontiers in Neurology, 6(107). https://doi. 188–191. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049731513497518
org/10.3389/fneur.2015.00107 Merriam Webster. (2021). Evidence. https://www.
Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor- merriam-­­webster.com/dictionary/evidence
mal educational and intellectual functioning in Mihailidis, P., & Foster, B. (2021). The cost of disbelief:
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting Fracturing news ecosystems in an age of rampant
and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi. media cynicism. American Behavioral Scientist, 65(4),
org/10.1037//0022-­006x.55.1.3 616–631. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049731513497518
Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals with develop- Miller, V. A., Schreck, K. A., Mulick, J. A., & Butter, E.
mental delays: Basic intervention techniques. Pro-ed. (2012). Factors related to parents’ choices of treat-
Lydon, H., Healy, O., & Grey, I. (2017). Comparison of ments for their children with autism spectrum disor-
behavioral intervention and sensory integration ther- ders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(1),
apy on challenging behavior of children with autism. 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.03.008
Behavioral Interventions, 32(4), 297–310. https://doi. Millward, C., Ferriter, M., Calver, S. J., & Connell-Jones,
org/10.1002/bin.1490 G. G. (2008). Gluten-and casein-free diets for autistic
Madsen, K. M., Lauritsen, M. B., Pedersen, C. B., Thorsen, spectrum disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic
P., Plesner, A.-M., Andersen, P. H., & Mortensen, P. B. Reviews, (2). https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.
(2003). Thimerosal and the occurrence of autism: CD003498.pub3
Negative ecological evidence from Danish population-­ Mostert, M.  P. (2001). Facilitated communication since
based data. Pediatrics, 112(3), 604–606. https://doi. 1995: A review of published studies. Journal of Autism
org/10.1542/peds.112.3.604
44 E. M. Kryszak and J. A. Mulick

and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 287–313. https:// (2004). Autism and attachment: A meta-­ analytic
doi.org/10.1023/A:1010795219886 review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Mudford, O. C., & Cullen, C. (2015). Auditory integration 45(6), 1123–1134. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
training. In Controversial therapies for autism and ­7610.2004.t01-­1-­00305.x
intellectual disabilities: Fad, fashion, and science in Saad, K., Eltayeb, A.  A., Mohamad, I.  L., Al-Atram,
professional practice (pp. 270–282). Routledge. A.  A., Elserogy, Y., Bjørklund, G., El-Houfey,
Mulick, J.  A., & Butter, E.  M. (2015). Positive behav- A.  A., & Nicholson, B. (2015). A randomized,
ior support: A paternalistic utopian delusion. In placebo-controlled trial of digestive enzymes in
Controversial therapies for autism and intellectual children with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical
disabilities (pp. 323–341). Routledge. Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience, 13(2), 188–
Munasinghe, S.  A., Oliff, C., Finn, J., & Wray, J.  A. 193. https://doi.org/10.9758/cpn.2015.13.2.188
(2010). Digestive enzyme supplementation for autism Saad, K., Abdel-Rahman, A., Elserogy, Y., Al-Atram,
spectrum disorders: A double-blind randomized con- A., El-Houfey, A., Othman, H., Bjoklund, G., Jia,
trolled trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental F., Urbina, M.  A., Abo-Elela, G.  M., Ahmad, F.  A.,
Disorders, 40(9), 1131–1138. https://doi.org/10.1007/ El-Baseer, K.  A. A., Ahmed, A.  E., & Abdel-Salam,
s10803-­010-­0974-­2 A. (2019). Retraction: Randomized controlled trial of
Newsom, C., & Hovanitz, C.  A. (2015). The nature vitamin D supplementation in children with autism
and value of empirically validated interventions. In spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and
Controversial therapies for autism and intellectual Psychiatry, 60(6), 711–711. https://doi.org/10.1111/
disabilities (pp. 37–48). Routledge. jcpp.13076
Ng, Q. X., Loke, W., Venkatanarayanan, N., Lim, D. Y., Saloviita, T. (2018). Does linguistic analysis confirm
Soh, A.  Y. S., & Yeo, W.  S. (2019). A systematic the validity of facilitated communication? Focus on
review of the role of prebiotics and probiotics in Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 33(2),
autism spectrum disorders. Medicina, 55(5), 129. 91–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357616646075
https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina55050129 Sathe, N., Andrews, J. C., McPheeters, M. L., & Warren,
Nye, C., & Brice, A. (2005). Combined vitamin Z.  E. (2017). Nutritional and dietary interven-
B6-magnesium treatment in autism spectrum dis- tions for autism spectrum disorder: A systematic
order. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, review. Pediatrics, 139(6), e20170346. https://doi.
(4), CD003497. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858. org/10.1542/peds.2017-­0346
CD003497.pub2 Schaaf, R.  C., Dumont, R.  L., Arbesman, M., & May-­
O’Haire, M.  E. (2013). Animal-assisted intervention Benson, T.  A. (2018). Efficacy of occupational
for autism spectrum disorder: A systematic litera- therapy using Ayres Sensory Integration®: A sys-
ture review. Journal of Autism and Developmental tematic review. American Journal of Occupational
Disorders, 43(7), 1606–1622. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Therapy, 72(1), 7201190010p7201190011–
s10803-­012-­1707-­5 7201190010p7201190010. https://doi.org/10.5014/
Pajareya, K., & Nopmaneejumruslers, K. (2011). A pilot ajot.2018.028431
randomized controlled trial of DIR/Floortime™ par- Schoen, S. A., Miller, L. J., & Sullivan, J. (2015). A pilot
ent training intervention for pre-school children with study of integrated listening systems for children with
autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(5), 563–577. sensory processing problems. Journal of Occupational
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361310386502 Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention, 8(3), 256–
Piwowarczyk, A., Horvath, A., Łukasik, J., Pisula, E., & 276. https://doi.org/10.1080/19411243.2015.1055418
Szajewska, H. (2018). Gluten-and casein-free diet and Scior, K., Addai-Davis, J., Kenyon, M., & Sheridan,
autism spectrum disorders in children: A systematic J. (2013). Stigma, public awareness about intel-
review. European Journal of Nutrition, 57(2), 433– lectual disability and attitudes to inclusion among
440. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-­017-­1483-­2 different ethnic groups. Journal of Intellectual
Rauch, J. (2013). Kindly inquisitors. University of Disability Research, 57(11), 1014–1026. https://doi.
Chicago Press. org/10.1111/j.1365-­2788.2012.01597.x
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, Sinha, Y., Silove, N., Hayen, A., & Williams, K. (2011).
E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with Auditory integration training and other sound thera-
autism and those with other developmental disorders. pies for autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Cochrane
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(5), Database of Systematic Reviews, (12), CD003681.
763–781. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-­7610.00162 https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD003681.pub3
Rossignol, D.  A., Bradstreet, J.  J., Van Dyke, K., Sipilä, A.-K., & Määttä, K. (2011). Can the facilitated
Schneider, C., Freedenfeld, S. H., O’Hara, N., Cave, communication method support autistic people,
S., Buckley, J.  A., Mumper, E.  A., & Frye, R.  E. according to facilitators’ opinions? Psychology of
(2012). Hyperbaric oxygen treatment in autism spec- Language and Communication, 15(1), 1–26. https://
trum disorders. Medical Gas Research, 2(1), 1–13. doi.org/10.2478/v10057-­011-­0001-­5
https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-­9912-­2-­16 Smith, T., & Iadarola, S. (2015). Evidence base update for
Rutgers, A.  H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.  J., van autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical Child &
Ijzendoorn, M.  H., & van Berckelaer-Onnes, I.  A.
3  History of Non-Evidence-Based Practices for Individuals Diagnosed with ASD 45

Adolescent Psychology, 44(6), 897–922. https://doi. consumer-­updates/be-­aware-­potentially-­dangerous-­


org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1077448 products-­and-­therapies-­claim-­treat-­autism
Sokolski, K.  N., & Wachtel, R. (1998). Improved social Villasenor, R.  F., Smith, S.  L., & Jewell, V.  D. (2018).
and language skills after secretin administration in A systematic review of sound-based intervention
patients with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of programs to improve participation in education for
the Association for Academic Minority Physicians, children with sensory processing and integration chal-
9(1), 9–15. lenges. Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, &
Solomon, R., Van Egeren, L.  A., Mahoney, G., Quon Early Intervention, 11(2), 172–191. https://doi.org/10.
Huber, M. S., & Zimmerman, P. (2014). PLAY Project 1080/19411243.2018.1432444
Home Consultation intervention program for young Wakefield, A.  J., Murch, S.  H., & Anthony, A. (2010).
children with autism spectrum disorders: A random- Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific
ized controlled trial. Journal of developmental and colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in chil-
behavioral pediatrics : JDBP, 35(8), 475–485. https:// dren (Retraction of vol 351, pg 637, 1998). Lancet,
doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000096 375(9713), 445–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Steinhausen, H.  C., Mohr Jensen, C., & Lauritsen, S0140-­6736(10)60175-­4
M. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis Weitlauf, A. S., Sathe, N., McPheeters, M. L., & Warren,
of the long-term overall outcome of autism spec- Z.  E. (2017). Interventions targeting sensory chal-
trum disorders in adolescence and adulthood. Acta lenges in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 133(6), 445–452. https:// review. Pediatrics, 139(6). https://doi.org/10.1542/
doi.org/10.1111/acps.12559 peds.2017-­0347
Total Autism. (2020, February 18). Demystifying Wheeler, D.  L., Jacobson, J.  W., Paglieri, R.  A., &
ABA: The myth of robotic respond- Schwartz, A. A. (1993). An experimental assessment
ing. https://www.totalspectrumcare.com/ of facilitated communication. Mental Retardation,
demystifying-­aba-­the-­myth-­of-­robotic-­responding/ 31(1), 49–59.
Trzmiel, T., Purandare, B., Michalak, M., Zasadzka, Williams, K., Wray, J.  A., & Wheeler, D.  M. (2012).
E., & Pawlaczyk, M. (2019). Equine assisted activi- Intravenous secretin for autism spectrum disorders
ties and therapies in children with autism spectrum (ASD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,
disorder: A systematic review and a meta-analysis. (4), CD003495. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.
Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 42, 104–113. CD003495.pub3
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2018.11.004 Zane, T., Weiss, M. J., & Kari, D. (2015). Relationship-­
United States Department of Health and Human Services, based therapies for autism spectrum disorders. In
Food and Drug Administration. (2019). Be aware of Controversial therapies for autism and intellectual
potentially dangerous products and therapies that disabilities (pp. 377–391). Routledge.
claim to treat autism. https://www.fda.gov/consumers/
Ethical Decision-Making
and Evidenced-Based Practices 4
Videsha G. Marya, Victoria D. Suarez,
and David J. Cox

4.1 Introduction the chain of behaviors that alter the probability of


a terminal response; Skinner, 1953), ethics (i.e.,
To implement an evidence-based practice (EBP), what is the right decision), and choice (i.e., the
behavior analysts practicing within the autism act of selecting the EBP).
intervention field (hereafter referred to as applied There are many ways that researchers attempt
behavior analysts) must first select one EBP from to aggregate empirical findings and theoretical
all available EBPs. Selecting an EBP requires at arguments spanning multiple areas. A common
least three sets of behaviors. One set of behaviors approach to aggregating research findings and
are those that produce information about the cur- theory is to develop models of the phenomenon
rent context and the relevant EBPs. A second set of interest. Many different types and approaches
of behaviors are those related to evaluating the to model building exist; however, models typi-
obtained information to determine what is “best” cally allow the model user to describe, predict,
or “right” for the client in the current context. A and control some phenomenon of interest more
final type of behavior is selecting the EBP to succinctly, efficiently, and parsimoniously than if
implement. Thus, to understand how an applied the model components were considered indepen-
behavior analyst selects the right EBP for a client dently (e.g., Frigg & Hartmann, 2020). Here, we
requires that we understand theoretical and are interested in models related to decision-­
empirical work spanning decision-making (i.e., making, ethics, and choice from available EBPs.
The content of models varies depending on
V. G. Marya the function of the modeler’s behavior (e.g., Cox,
Village Autism Center, Mesa, AZ, USA 2019a). In this chapter, we focus on two types of
Department of Applied Behavior Analysis, Endicott practical models of ethical decision-making with
College, Beverly, MA, USA EBPs: causal models and decision models
V. D. Suarez (Table 4.1). Causal models help the model user to
Department of Applied Behavior Analysis, Endicott describe, predict, and control a phenomenon of
College, Beverly, MA, USA interest. Causal models do this by explicitly
Centria Autism, Mesa, AZ, USA labeling the variables that influence the phenom-
D. J. Cox (*) enon of interest as well as the relationships
Department of Applied Behavior Analysis, Endicott between those variables. Practically, causal mod-
College, Beverly, MA, USA els describe why the phenomenon occurs, the
Behavioral Health Center of Excellence, independent variables that can be manipulated to
Los Angeles, CA, USA influence the dependent variable, and the
e-mail: dcox@endicott.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 47


J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_4
48 V. G. Marya et al.

Table 4.1  Purpose and components of causal and decision models for decision-making
Model
type Purpose Components Use
Causal Identify and outline how and Variables known to influence decision-­ Describe, predict, or
why behavior is evoked, making (e.g., rules, experienced control
maintained, increased, or contingencies, available alternative decision-making
decreased behaviors, effort, delay, and likelihood of
reinforcement/punishment)
Decision Identify and outline what Questions and statements that guide the user Help the user make an
behaviors the user should emit through the chain of behaviors necessary to optimal decision
to answer a question the model make a decision based on available
user is not fluent in answering information

expected direction of change in the dependent causal model would need to be accounted for in
variable when those independent variables are the decision model, otherwise the decision model
manipulated. would likely fail to meet its function – to help the
Decision models help the model user to make decision-maker avoid a suboptimal decision.
a decision. Decision models help the model user The purpose of this chapter is to provide and
to choose between the available alternatives in a describe models of ethical decision-making
situation so that all potentially relevant informa- related to implementing EBPs. To do this, we
tion is considered before making a choice. The start by reviewing a causal model of ethical
findings of researchers in behavioral economics decision-­making. After outlining some of the
over the past several decades suggest that many variables known to influence ethical decision-­
organisms fail to consistently make the optimal making, the chapter reviews decision models as
decision (e.g., Fantino et  al., 1997; Pattison & they relate to EBP and the points wherein ethics
Zentall, 2014; Sofis et al., 2015; Zentall, 2016). likely plays a role in shaping decision-making
The purpose of decision models, then, is to help and which EBP is selected. We then combine the
the model user avoid making a suboptimal deci- causal and decision models to create an ethical
sion. Stated differently, accurate decision models decision model for implementing EBPs. The
reduce the probability of bias decisions and chapter closes by demonstrating how the reader
increase our sensitivity to relevant environmental can use the combined causal and decision models
variables that indicate what consequences are to analyze and make ethical decisions related to
likely to result from our decision. EBPs.
Causal models and decision models are inter-
related when ethically making decisions about
EBPs. To accurately predict and control which 4.2  ausal Model of Ethical
C
EBP is selected, the causal modeler likely needs Decision-Making
to know whether the decision-maker has access
to a decision model as well as the decision-­ The goals of scientific research are often descrip-
maker’s learning history with the decision model. tion, prediction, and control. Specifically, scien-
That is, the presence and learned history relative tists seek to: accurately describe the phenomenon
to decision models would be an independent vari- of interest, identify principles and processes that
able in the causal model. Similarly, to reduce the allow for the prediction of the phenomenon of
probability that a suboptimal decision is made, interest, and then use the understanding of the
the decision modeler would need to understand principles and processes to control the phenome-
what current and past contingencies influence non of interest (Cooper et  al., 2020). The ideal
decisions with EBPs and what contingencies may outcome of research, then, is to understand the
need to be arranged to avoid a suboptimal deci- cause of a phenomenon so that researchers can
sion. That is, the independent variables from the control the presence, absence, or degree to which
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 49

the phenomenon occurs in the future. Researchers making relative to an applied behavior analyst’s
often use causal models to succinctly describe decision about EBPs. One categorization is rela-
the variables that are causally related to the phe- tive to perspective: that of the applied behavior
nomenon of interest. analyst compared to that of the profession-labeled
Causal models are legion in the experimen- applied behavior analysts. Below we discuss how
tal and applied analysis of behavior. For exam- perspective plays a causal role in ethical decision-­
ple, researchers in the experimental analysis of making using the distinction between morality
behavior often use models to describe and pre- and ethics. A second categorization is relative to
dict behavior such as the Rescorla-Wagner the ethical behaviors under consideration: claims
model of respondent conditioning (e.g., about what is right compared to claims about
Rescorla & Wagner, 1972), the generalized why it is right. We discuss these components of
matching equation for response allocation (e.g., ethical decision-making in the second part of this
Baum, 1974; McDowell, 1989), delay or prob- section on descriptive and normative ethical
ability discounting for reinforcer value (e.g., behavior. A final categorization can be made
McKerchar & Renda, 2012; Rachlin et  al., regarding the environmental variables and behav-
1991), or the demand equation for the impact of iors involved in decision-making more generally.
effort on reinforcer consumption (Hursh & We discuss these components in the final part of
Silberberg, 2008). this section titled Decision-Making.
The above-referenced causal models of behav-
ior consist of precise mathematical relations
between independent and dependent variables 4.2.1 Morality and Ethics
(e.g., Dallery & Soto, 2013), but not all models in
behavior analysis use math. For example, the Behavior analysts are required to make ongoing
three-term contingency is a model that describes decisions about the treatments they implement
covariance relationships between stimuli and for individuals with autism spectrum disorder
behavior (e.g., Ribes-Inesta, 1997; Skinner, (ASD). The Ethics Code for Behavior Analysts
1953). Thousands of applied behavior analysts (hereafter referred to as the Code; BACB, 2020)
have used this verbal causal model to describe, provides guidance on ethical behavior and the
predict, and control behavior (e.g., Iwata et  al., use of effective treatments. However, ambiguity
1982; Paclawskyj et  al., 2000). Similarly, the in situations inevitably arises wherein an applied
four-term contingency is a model that allows the behavior analyst still must select between treat-
user to describe, predict, and control behavior ments. Each applied behavior analyst has a
that changes dynamically as a function of context unique learning history that is somewhat similar
and time since contacting various reinforcers and to other applied behavior analysts as well as dif-
punishers (Michael, 1993). This four-term con- ferent from other members of the profession.
tingency has also been used by applied behavior Included in these similar and different learning
analysts to effectively predict and control behav- histories are claims about what is right and wrong
ior (e.g., Endicott & Higbee, 2007; Sundberg professional behavior. For example, the behavior
et  al., 2001). In sum, causal models are used of applied behavior analysts is likely to be influ-
extensively in behavior analysis, and, regardless enced by their individual history with rules about
of whether the model uses mathematics or words right or wrong (e.g., from school, local lab or
alone, causal models are instances of human clinic lore, religion, culture, familial) as well as
behavior (Baum, 2018; Marr, 2015). their shared history with other applied behavior
To develop a causal model of ethical decision-­ analysts with rules about right or wrong (i.e., the
making relative to EBPs, we must understand the rules in the Code). Historically, the difference
components that go into the model. There are between right or wrong at the individual level
several ways to categorize and identify the com- versus the professional level has been captured
ponents for a causal model of ethical decision-­ by a distinction between morality and ethics.
50 V. G. Marya et al.

At its root, morality comes from the Latin tacted as “compassion,” “respect,” and “self-­
word moralis which refers to the proper behavior control” (Haidt et al., 2009). In addition, writers
of a person and one’s individual disposition (Cox, often assume that emitting moral and ethical
2020; MacIntyre, 2003). When many people behavior includes the ability to anticipate the
adopt the same pattern of “proper individual consequences of one’s actions (i.e., verbally state
behavior,” moralis takes the plural form and the probability of specific consequences), to
becomes mores  – customs and manners of a make value judgments (i.e., accurately tact the
group of people. Thus, in total, morality can be stimulus relations between an event, behavior, or
defined as focusing broadly on what individuals outcome, and defined values), and to choose
should consider to be right and wrong behavior between alternative courses of action based on
and why an individual person should believe that those anticipated consequences and value judg-
is true for them in their daily lives. Note that this ments (Ayala, 1987).
leaves the door open for individuals to develop Morality and ethics also include behaviors
their own unique systems of moral rules that they that are established, maintained, increased, or
live by and of which everyone else may disagree reduced through the same processes as all other
(e.g., Borum, 2010; Harper, 2009; Monestes behaviors (e.g., Baum, 2005; Skinner, 1953,
et al., 2017). 1971). Past writers have typically theoretically
In contrast, ethics comes from the Greek word extended research in the experimental analysis of
ethos which refers to the proper behavior that behavior to argue that morality and ethics are
characterizes a culture, era, community, or pro- broadly maintained by socially mediated conse-
fession (Cox, 2020). Ethics aims to answer the quences delivered based on the similarity of a
question of, as a group of people with some group members’ behavior with the cultural stan-
shared goal or skill set, what are the rules about dards of what is right versus wrong for that group
right and wrong that are relevant to us all? Even of people (Skinner, 1953). More recently,
though ethics is defined as rules relative to what research has begun to emerge that demonstrates
is right or wrong for individuals behaving as these functional relations empirically (Cox,
members of a collective group, this does not 2020).
mean that the group is the ones who decide the More specific functions of moral and ethical
rules. For example, the Pope and the Cardinals of behavior have been discussed in greater detail in
the Catholic church largely decide what is right other areas of the psychology literature. For
or wrong for the group of people known as example, Kohlberg & Kramer (1969) identified
Catholics, the Supreme Court decides the right six potential functions of moral or ethical behav-
way to interpret the law, and small working com- ior which we can translate behaviorally (Skinner,
mittees often determine the rules espoused in 1945). One function might be avoidance. When
professional codes of ethics such as the American young, humans may contact punishment for
Medical Association (AMA) Code of Ethics engaging in immoral or unethical behavior and
(AMA, 2016), the American Psychological thus learn to emit moral or ethical behavior to
Association (APA) Code of Ethics (APA, 2017), avoid punishment. A second function could be
and the BACB Code (BACB, 2020). Stated suc- socially mediated reinforcement wherein one
cinctly, morality is concerned with what is right learns that behaving morally or ethically leads to
or wrong for me as an individual separate from higher rates of reinforcement for the individual
the profession to which I belong, and ethics is and for others (i.e., social reciprocity; Carpenter
concerned with what is right or wrong for all & Matthews, 2004). A third function might be
members of a group or profession. generalized social reinforcement for adhering to
Morality and ethics as behaviors are similar in rules specifying societal norms about what is
many ways. For example, both involve behaviors acceptable versus unacceptable. A fourth func-
labeled as values, emitting behavior relative to tion might be generalized social reinforcement
labels of right and wrong, and emitting behaviors for adhering to rules claimed by perceived author-
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 51

ity figures. A fifth function follows recognition only scientifically supported treatments
that groups and authorities often differ on what is (Guideline 2.01, 2.13, 2.14; BACB, 2020). Here,
claimed to be right behavior, and, without an failing to implement a lesson on sexual behavior
objective way to decide between the two, societal may violate their moral rules, whereas imple-
rules and authority claims are viewed relatively. menting a lesson on sexual behavior may violate
Once viewed relatively, Kohlberg (1971) argued their ethical rules given the limited experimental
that personal values and individually derived literature on teaching sexual behavior for indi-
rules play a larger role in what is claimed as right. viduals with ASD (Solomon et al., 2019).
Finally, a sixth function of ethical behavior out- Conflicts between a clinician’s morality and
lined by Kohlberg might be considered as adher- ethics are referred to as conflicts of conscience in
ing to a defined response class tacted as “ethical the bioethics and medical ethics literatures (e.g.,
principles.” That is, for some, ethical principles Adams, 2007; Ford & Austin, 2018). Conflict of
such as justice, honesty, and compassion become consciences are rarely discussed in the applied
the primary method for deriving and ranking behavior analysis (ABA) literature despite the
rules for what is considered right behavior. Each common reported occurrence of conflicts of con-
of these ethical principles, when followed, leads science in other areas of healthcare and education
to consistent patterns of behavior and changes in (e.g., American College of Obstetricians and
the environment. For example, justice leads to the Gynecologists, 2007; Curlin, 2008; Ford &
equitable allocation of opportunity and resources Austin, 2018). Whether the applied behavior ana-
which can be observed and measured, honesty lyst is more justified to follow their moral rules or
can be captured with say-do correspondence, and to follow the ethical rules of the profession to
compassion might be observed and measured as which they belong could be a chapter itself. Here,
behaviors leading to the reduction in behavioral the main takeaway is that morality and ethics can
patterns indicative of pain or suffering in others. be distinguished, the potential for conflict
Despite many similarities, the distinction between morality and ethics exists, and the
between morality and ethics is practically impor- potential for conflict between morality and ethics
tant as one’s personal rules (morality) can con- is an important variable that may influence ethi-
flict with the rules of one’s profession (ethics). cal decision-making with EBPs.
For example, consider an applied behavior ana-
lyst who adheres to personal, moral rules that dis-
courage handling pork. However, this same 4.2.2 D
 escriptive vs. Normative
applied behavior analyst works with a client Ethical Behavior
whose most preferred edible reinforcer is bacon.
In this example, handling bacon would violate Historically, philosophers have made a topo-
the applied behavior analyst’s moral rules, but graphical distinction between what is considered
refusing to handle the bacon would violate the to be right behavior (descriptive ethical behavior)
applied behavior analyst’s ethical rules to advo- and why that behavior is considered to be right
cate for the services “designed to maximize behavior (normative ethical behavior). For exam-
desired outcomes” (Guideline 2.01; BACB, ple, one might argue that using Behavior Skills
2020). As another example, consider an applied Training (i.e., BST; Miltenberger, 2012) is the
behavior analyst who has the education and right approach to train Registered Behavior
supervised clinical experiences to teach and pro- Technicians (RBTs). Phrased differently, we can
vide training surrounding sexual behavior. This ask, “What is the right way to train RBTs?”
applied behavior analyst may personally believe Answer: “Using BST.” Note here that our dia-
that their designed interventions are in the best logue with you  – the reader  – is restricted to
interests of a client and feel morally obligated to words on a page. So, “the right way to train
help their client in this area. However, applied RBTs” is not simply by someone saying, “Use
behavior analysts also have the obligation to use BST.” Rather, “the right way to train RBTs” is by
52 V. G. Marya et al.

actually engaging in the behavioral patterns that that are required by applied behavior analysts,
an observer might tact as, “you are using BST.” generally, as well as specific to EBP. For exam-
Summarizing this example, the descriptive ethi- ple, the Code states that applied behavior ana-
cal behaviors for training RBTs are the behav- lysts should educate clients and stakeholders on
ioral patterns one might tact as “BST.” effective, evidence-based treatments (Guideline
We can also ask the question, “Why is imple- 3.12; BACB, 2020). When applied behavior ana-
menting BST the right thing to do?” The behav- lysts must choose between multiple scientifically
iors involved in answering this question would be supported treatments or teaching procedures, the
historically labeled normative ethical behavior. Code states that other factors should be consid-
Different people may answer this question differ- ered such as client preference and clinician expe-
ently. For some, we should use BST because the rience or training (Guideline 2.14; BACB, 2020).
Code says we should use EBPs, and BST has the As another example of descriptive ethical behav-
most published evidence to support its effective- ior related to EBP, the Code states that applied
ness (Kirkpatrick et al., 2019; Schaefer & Andzik, behavior analysts should provide the appropriate
2020). Others may view a particular author in the amount and level of supervisory hours necessary
behavior analytic literature as an expert in staff to meet treatment goals (Guideline 3.12; BACB,
supervision and training and, if that author says 2020).
BST is the right way to train RBTs, then that is Applied behavior analysts can also learn
the right way to train RBTs. As a final example, descriptive ethical behaviors from other sources
others might argue that BST is the right way to such as mentors, organizational policies, clinical
train RBTs because it leads to the fewest errors settings, and the published literature. For exam-
when the RBT subsequently implements an inter- ple, rules might be passed down from supervisors
vention, and fewer errors are more likely to lead or organizations in the form of policies and pro-
to the best client outcomes. cedures for the right way to conduct assessments
Justifying why someone should implement and implement services. Also, rules about what is
BST to train RBTs is verbal behavior and is topo- right and wrong may not always be formally out-
graphically different from the behaviors of actu- lined or written down. Instead, they might be
ally implementing BST.  More generally, passed down via conversations and interactions
descriptive ethical behavior is topographically with colleagues via so-called lab-lore or
distinct from normative ethical behavior. Though clinic-lore.
historically treated topographically in the philo- Formal and informal rules about what is right
sophical literature, recent research suggests that can contribute functionally to what applied
descriptive and normative ethical behaviors are behavior analysts claim as ethical assessment and
also functionally distinct (Cox, 2020). Thus, the implementation of ABA services. For example, a
distinction between descriptive and normative service provision organization may train behav-
ethical behavior is important to consider within a ior analysts to use an interview-informed synthe-
causal model of ethical decision-making related sized contingency analysis (IISCA; Hanley et al.,
to the implementation of EBP as accounting for 2014), while another organization may provide
both is needed. training in functional analyses (FA; Iwata et al.,
1982). Similarly, a behavior analyst may be
4.2.2.1 Descriptive Ethical Behavior encouraged by their organization to specifically
and Decision-Making with EBPs conduct indirect functional assessments (e.g.,
If descriptive ethical behavior is what the right Iwata & DeLeon, 1996; Paclawskyj et al., 2000),
thing to do in a given situation, a question that whereas another behavior analyst may be
naturally follows is where the applied behavior encouraged to conduct direct functional assess-
analysts learn descriptive ethical behavior. The ments (e.g., Hanley et al., 2014; Iwata et al., 1982).
most likely influence on descriptive ethical In turn, behavior analysts working at different
behavior is the Code, as it outlines the behaviors organizations and with different educational his-
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 53

tories may disagree on what is considered the (IEP) team in a particular school district. Though
right EBP based on their organizational policies, never explicitly stated, the applied behavior ana-
training, and prevailing contingencies (Cox, lyst may derive a rule that it will take more effort,
2020). be a more aversive experience, and hinder the
Conflict between statements about correct implementation of treatment if they recommend
implementation of EBP may occur within the that a direct functional assessment be conducted
same person as well. Behavior analysts who hold before intervention design as opposed to begin-
other professional titles (e.g., licensed psycholo- ning intervention more quickly following an indi-
gist, speech-language pathologists, medical doc- rect functional assessment. Here, the right
tors, teacher certification) have additional approach to recommending which EBP to choose
experience with claims about what is right via might be influenced by an individually derived
those professions’ ethical standards, education rule.
and training, and published literature. All of In sum, descriptive ethical behaviors are the
which may influence descriptive ethical state- patterns of behavior which are considered to be
ments about what might be the right thing to do in right. For applied behavior analysts, descriptions
a given situation. For example, the Association of about what is the right thing to do may come
American Educators (AAE) explicitly recognizes from the Code, mentors, colleagues, textbooks,
the important role of educators in the “moral edu- published research literature, organizational poli-
cation” of students (AAE, 2013). Additionally, cies and procedures, other scientific literatures,
applied behavior analysts are obligated to con- or we may create them ourselves. These descrip-
duct assessments before developing behavior-­ tions about what is the right thing to do can play
change programs (Guideline 2.13; BACB, 2020), a causal role in the behavior we emit. Also, this
involve clients in the planning of the behavior-­ includes the decisions we make about which
change program (Guideline 2.09; BACB, 2020), evidence-­based assessment or intervention we
and practice only within the boundaries of their choose to implement.
competence (Core Principle 4; Guideline 3.03;
BACB, 2020). However, as discussed in more 4.2.2.2 Normative Ethical Behavior
detail below, interesting questions quickly arise Behavior analytic writings have primarily been
as to how moral behavior will be assessed, whose explicit in what the right thing to do might be.
moral framework should be taught, and what However, historically, why those behaviors are
competence or expertise in moral education looks the right thing to do has been discussed more
like (e.g., Brummett & Ostertag, 2018; Ho, 2016; implicitly. Verbal behavior tacted as “justifying
Iltis & Rasmussen, 2016; McClimans & Slowther, why a specific behavior is right” can be grouped
2016). In these situations, it seems plausible the into theories of normative ethical behavior – pat-
prevailing contingencies will determine which terns of verbal behavior that provide rationale or
code of ethics predominates (Cox, 2019b) – but justification for why something is right. Theories
we can also ask whether this is right. of normative ethics provide benchmarks for mea-
Lastly, behavior analysts have individual and suring whether one’s choices are justified as
personal experiences that contribute to descrip- being right versus wrong. That is, when we agree
tive ethics. Basic research suggests that verbally on why something is right, we can measure
competent humans can derive rules for what to do whether the results of our behavior meet the
in situations and then adhere to those self-derived agreed-upon benchmark.
rules (e.g., Matthews et  al., 1985; Ninness & In ABA, there currently is not an agreed-upon
Ninness, 1999; Rosenfarb et al., 1992). There is approach for how applied behavior analysts
no reason to suspect ethical behavior is unique in should justify claims about right or wrong behav-
this regard. For example, an applied behavior ior. However, understanding the normative
analyst may have heard about a colleague’s expe- ethical theory one uses as a benchmark for cor-
rience with an individualized education plan rect ethical decision-making is important for
54 V. G. Marya et al.

resolving ethical dilemmas (Brodhead et  al., another example, the principle of justice (i.e.,
2018), making ethical decisions in contexts not treating everyone fairly based on established
covered by existing ethical rules (Brodhead et al., criteria for comparison) is often considered a
2018; Cox, 2020), and because different ethical virtuous behavior (e.g., Huang, 2007). Extended
theories lead to different behaviors labeled as to ABA service delivery, each client a behavior
right (e.g., Brodhead et al., 2018; Cox, 2020). To analyst agrees to provide services for has a cer-
aid applied behavior analysts in identifying an tain number of hours for which the behavior
ethical theory they can use to consistently make analyst receives reimbursement. The principle
ethical decisions, we briefly outline five ethical of justice might require that applied behavior
theories commonly found in modern Western analysts only work the agreed-­upon hours for
medical ethics literature and how they causally that individual so as not to unfairly provide
relate to ethical decision-making for the imple- more time to one client compared to others.
mentation of EBPs. Alternatively, the applied behavior analyst may
One theory of normative ethics is consequen- develop a set of criteria under which they would
tialism (a.k.a. utilitarianism). Consequentialism work more hours than specified in the contract
argues that labels of right or wrong are determined with the understanding that all clients would
by the consequences that follow the ethical deci- have access to that potential for extra attention.
sions, and behaviors that result in the greatest good Contract theory holds that certain behaviors
for the greatest number of people are considered are right or good because they adhere to a social
right (Alexander & Moore, 2016). For example, a contract (Dienstag, 1996). For example, an
person justifying EBP using consequentialism applied behavior analyst may sign a contract with
might argue conducting an indirect functional a client and a third-party payer wherein they
assessment is better than descriptive functional promise to reduce the self-injurious behavior of a
assessments because there is more evidence to client, teach them to communicate their wants
indicate an effective intervention can be imple- and needs in a socially acceptable manner, and
mented more quickly (e.g., Tarbox et al., 2009). accomplish both using 10 hr per week at a reim-
Another theory of normative ethics is deontol- bursement of $85 per hour.1 The behaviors that
ogy. Unlike consequentialism, deontology argues are ethically right in this scenario are the set of
that behaviors cannot be assessed by the conse- behaviors that allow the behavior analyst to effi-
quences that follow from an ethical decision but ciently reduce self-injurious behavior and teach
are determined as right or wrong based on con- functional communication while increasing the
formity to a group’s ethical norm (e.g., a code of probability of generalization and maintenance.
ethics; Alexander & Moore, 2016). For example, Lastly, feminist approaches to normative eth-
a person justifying EBP using deontology might ics argue that determining right or wrong through
justify using edible reinforcers because the Code mathematical adherence to logical principles is
states behavior analysts should use effective misguided. Rather, natural caring relationships
treatments, rather than justifying using edible between humans should provide the basis for
reinforcers because edible reinforcers result in right and wrong behavior (e.g., Gilligan, 1982;
quicker and greater behavior change
(consequentialism).
Virtue theory holds that some behaviors are Note that under this arrangement the client (and their
1 

caregivers) and the third-party payer also have ethical


right or good simply because they are right/ obligations resulting from the agreed-upon contract. The
good by nature, regardless of the consequences client is ethically obligated to show up for the agreed-­
that follow or any rules espoused by authority upon number of hours each week, to participate in their
figures (Annas, 2006). For example, honesty is sessions, and to engage in the behaviors necessary to gen-
eralize the skills to the home environment. The third-party
typically considered a virtuous behavior and payer is ethically obligated to pay the provider at the des-
people should always emit (e.g., Carr, 2014; ignated rate and for a maximum amount of $850 per week
Gachter & Schulz, 2016; Wang et al., 2011). As (10 hr × $85/hr).
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 55

Larrabee, 1993; Noddings, 1984). In our context, making with EBPs, we need to understand the
applied behavior analysts have a responsibility to interaction between choice, verbal behavior,
care for the clients or students on their caseload. ethics, and optimality.
Also, through their interactions with clients or
students, applied behavior analysts identify ways 4.2.3.1 Choice
they can best care for their clients or students in Choice and decision-making have been exten-
the particular social circumstances in which they sively researched within behavior analytic basic
live. EBPs then become a vehicle for which we research (e.g., Fantino, 1997; Herrnstein, 1970;
can demonstrate our care for clients or students. Williams, 1994). Concurrent schedules of rein-
The history of writings on ethics and morality forcement used in basic research have demon-
provides definitions of ethical and moral behav- strated that organisms will allocate more behavior
ior that are useful for claiming what is right to responses that result in increased contact with
behavior and why this may be the case. Moreover, reinforcement and less behavior to the responses
theories of normative behavior are useful for that result in less contact with reinforcement (i.e.,
understanding ways in which behaviors are clas- matching law). For example, when considering
sified as right or wrong to create measures of suc- whether to include a response cost (punishment)
cess. Nevertheless, definitions of ethics and component in a token economy intervention, an
ethical theories are stated at a general and high applied behavior analyst’s decision will be influ-
level. Thus, by themselves, definitions of ethics enced by their past experiences achieving behav-
and ethical theories do not always lead to practi- ior reduction in the presence and absence of
cal solutions when making ethical decisions including response cost.
regarding EBPs. To make ethical decision-­ Choice research has also focused on self-­
making related to EBPs more tractable, it may control (i.e., delay discounting). Self-control is
help to include operant and respondent behav- typically studied by having participants choose
ioral processes and principles that play a causal between a smaller, more immediate reinforcer
role in ethical decision-making. and a larger, more delayed reinforcer (Fisher &
Mazur, 1997; Green & Myerson, 2013). The
general findings are that the relative comparison
4.2.3 Decision-Making between amount and delay to two alternatives
will uniquely determine which response humans
Decision-making involves engaging in behav- make. Translated to our current context, con-
iors to manipulate relevant variables needed to sider a situation where an applied behavior ana-
evoke a decision (Skinner, 1953). As with past lyst must decide whether to use punishment
definitions of ethics, the definition of decision- procedures with a client displaying severe self-­
making provides a general, high-level descrip- injurious behavior (SIB). Using punishment
tion of behavior but does not necessarily allow may decrease the delay to minimal rates of SIB
for the description, prediction, and control of but also includes the use of an aversive stimulus.
decision-­making. A more practical approach is In contrast, refraining from using punishment
to consider the behavioral processes involved. may increase the delay to minimal rates of SIB
Decision-­making appears to involve the inter- but does not include the use of an aversive stim-
action between behavioral processes that con- ulus. Importantly, a robust finding from the self-­
trol choice in addition to verbal behavior. control literature is that individuals are
Further, to evaluate whether people make the differentially influenced by delay (e.g., Green &
right decision, it is necessary to include the Myerson, 2013). Thus, different applied behav-
notion of optimality toward a stated goal. Thus, ior analysts are likely to choose differently in
to understand what causes ethical decision- the above scenario when considering only how
56 V. G. Marya et al.

delay to minimal rates of SIB should determine 4.2.3.4 Causal Model of Ethical
the right EBP. Behavior
In sum, the previous sections have outlined how
4.2.3.2 Verbal Behavior causal models can be used to describe and iden-
The relationships between stimuli and verbal tify variables known to control choice and
behavior are important in ethical decision-­ decision-­making in humans. If we assume that
making for several reasons. First, as noted above, ethical behavior is just behavior, then those same
descriptive ethical behavior is often described variables would also apply to ethical behavior
using verbal behavior, and normative ethical and ethical decision-making. Figure 4.1 provides
behavior is verbal behavior. Furthermore, differ- an example visual depiction of a causal model of
ent ethical situations involve the presence and ethical behavior and ethical decision-making.
absence of a variety of stimuli within the
­environment, relations between those stimuli and
behavior, and relations between those stimuli and 4.3 Decision Models
the likelihood of various consequences occur-
ring. Many decision-making models require the In this section we focus on decision models rela-
model user to verbally tact these relations so as to tive to ethical decision-making, clinical decision-­
make the “right” decision based on all available making with EBPs, and how ethical and clinical
information in the decision context (more details decision-making with EBPs can interact.
below). Thus, understanding how verbal stimuli Throughout, we assume that ethical decision-­
interact to control decision-making is important making is behavior and is susceptible to the same
for describing, predicting, and controlling ethical laws and principles that apply to all other behav-
decision-making with EBPs. ior. In particular, we focus on the choice point
faced by applied behavior analysts dozens, or
4.2.3.3 Optimality perhaps hundreds, of times every week: Which
The outcomes that result from a decision are intervention is the right choice in this situation
rarely binary (i.e., yes/no). Rather, the outcomes and for this client?
that result from a decision often occur at some Decision-making models are prevalent across
level of a continuous gradient. For example, a wide range of scientific and professional disci-
applied behavior analysts are unlikely to claim an plines. For example, decision models have been
intervention successfully reduced SIB if it published in physics related to quantum rein-
decreased from 100 times per day to 98 times per forcement learning (e.g., Li et al., 2020), biology
day. Technically, it decreased, but a simple “yes” related to decision-making in animals (e.g.,
or “no” as to whether the behavior reduced is McFarland, 1977), political theory related to
likely insufficient for determining whether our public administration (e.g., Simon, 1977), and
decision to use an EBP was the right choice. even in behavior analysis related to risk assess-
Instead, the applied behavior analyst would ment in functional analyses (e.g., Deochand
determine whether their decision was the right et  al., 2020). Broadly, decision-making models
choice by measuring how much the SIB reduced are designed to help the model user make the best
(i.e., between 100 and 0 times per day) and how decision in the situation toward meeting a pre-­
quickly the reduction occurred. Measuring and defined goal. Decision-making models accom-
determining the “right” or the “best” ethical deci- plish this by serving as a textual prompt to help
sion with EBPs therefore requires measurement the model user consider the relevant variables
of transitions between stable responding during a and options which may have gone unconsidered
baseline period and during intervention and the without the model prompt. The use of decision-­
analysis of how to most efficiently transition making models results in a chain of behaviors
between the two. that (ideally) leads to an outcome as close to the
pre-defined goal as possible.
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 57

Fig. 4.1  Causal model of ethical behavior and ethical an ethical decision with evidence-based practices. Arrows
decision-making related to evidence-based practice. indicate interactions between variables
Underlined text indicates the behavioral chain of making

4.3.1 F
 unction of Ethical Decision behavior and ethical decision-making might then
Models be used to identify the intervention that leads to
the best outcomes for the patient (consequential-
Decision models help the model user engage in a ism); fulfills our duties to the client and the field
chain of behaviors that leads to an outcome as of behavior analysis as outlined in the Code
close to the pre-defined goal as possible. For (deontology); is aligned with accepted virtuous
example, Geiger et al. (2010) outlined a decision behaviors (virtue theory); meets the goals of
model to help applied behavior analysts decide behavior change set forth in the contract the cli-
between function-based interventions for chal- ent, the payer, and the behavior analysts agreed to
lenging behavior maintained by escape (Geiger (contract theory); or displays genuine care for the
et al., 2010). The goal of the model user might be client and their situation (feminist ethics). All of
to identify and tailor an intervention that effi- which may center on the behavioral definitions of
ciently and effectively reduces the escape-­ what the client or their caregiver states leads to a
maintained behavior. The function of the decision meaningful life. The function of ethical decision
model is to prompt the model user to consider models, then, is the state of environment-­behavior
variables relevant to current environment-­ relations that meet the predetermined goal
behavior relations that allow the model user to derived from the interaction between client val-
identify an intervention that efficiently and effec- ues and the normative theory an applied behavior
tively reduces the challenging behavior (i.e., to analysts uses to determine their ethical claims.
meet their goal).
But what is the goal of applied behavior ana-
lysts who are using an ethical decision model? 4.3.2 Ethical Decision-Making
Ethical behavior is defined as the set of rules for Models
conduct that all applied behavior analysts should
follow, and ethical decision-making refers to the Hundreds of ethical decision-making models
chain of behaviors that lead the model user to have been published and span dozens of profes-
emit ethical behavior. Thus, the consequences sional disciplines and specific ethical decisions
that maintain ethical decision-making might be (e.g., Ford & Richardson, 1994; Lau, 2003).
attaining the right or desired consequence Outside behavior analysis, scholars and research-
wherein right or desired is defined by the norma- ers have published on ethical decisions in fields
tive theory one uses to justify ethical behavior. such as medicine (e.g., Greipp, 1992; Meyer-­
Specifically for EBPs, the function of ethical Zehnder et  al., 2017), education (e.g., Green &
58 V. G. Marya et al.

Walker, 2009; Shapiro & Stefkovich, 2016), psy- action that is “best” and implements it. Finally,
chology (e.g., Cottone & Claus, 2000; Grace some ethical decision-making models recom-
et al., 2020), and even astronomy (e.g., American mend the decision-maker follow-up to evaluate
Astronomical Society, 2017; Hoeppe, 2018). In whether action taken was, in fact, the “best”
behavior analysis, ethical decision-making mod- action or if further action is warranted.
els have been proposed to help the user identify
key stakeholders and Code guidelines relevant to
the ethical decision (Bailey & Burch, 2016), to 4.3.3 Evidence-Based Practice
navigate professional collaboration related to Decision Models
interventions published outside the behavior ana-
lytic literature (Brodhead, 2015), and to Historical accounts of EBP can be traced back to
­incorporate experiences and variables outside the the 1800s and a nurse named Florence Nightingale
Code into the decision-making process who used evidence to promote reform in health-
(Rosenberg & Schwartz, 2019). care (Aravind & Chung, 2010). More recently,
Ethical decision models published within and the concept of EBP was introduced to the field of
outside behavior analysis often have similar com- medicine in the 1990s by Sackett and colleagues.
ponents. Figure  4.2 shows the generally promi- Sackett et al. (1996) proposed a definition of EBP
nent components of ethical decision-making to be practice that integrates “individual clinical
models. The first step to ethical decision-making expertise with the best available external clinical
involves recognition that a situation involves eth- evidence from systematic research” (p. 71). This
ical behavior. Once a situation is identified to definition was later refined to “Evidence-based
involve ethical behavior, the second step is for the practice is the integration of best research evi-
applied behavior analyst to identify the precise dence with clinical expertise and patient values”
ethical problem. Once the specific problem is (Sackett et  al., 2000, p.  170). The American
known, ethical decision models often recom- Psychological Association (APA, 2006) adapted
mend the decision-maker gather information the definition proposed by Sackett et  al. (2000)
about professional, organizational, or personal and stated that EBP in psychology (EBPP) is the
rules of right conduct that are relevant to the cur- integration of the best available research with
rent situation and what response options are plau- clinical expertise in the context of patient charac-
sible. Next, decision models often recommend teristics, culture, and preferences.
that the decision-maker prioritize and clarify the Several authors within the field of behavior
gathered information and conducts a risk-benefit analysis have also offered definitions of
analysis for the different response options that EBP.  Kazdin (2008) describes EBP as clinical
are plausible. Once the information has been syn- practice based on the integration of the best avail-
thesized and the values and potential options able evidence regarding interventions, clinical
ranked, the decision-maker then decides the expertise, and patient values, needs, and prefer-

Fig. 4.2  Decision model for ethical decision-making


4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 59

ences. Smith (2013) defined EBP as “a service captured in the Code which has several subsec-
that helps solve a consumer’s problem...[that] tions directly related to the three elements of EBP
integrates a package of procedures, operational- such as best available evidence (e.g., Guidelines
ized in a manual, and validated in studies of 2.13, 2.14; BACB, 2020), consideration for client
socially meaningful outcomes” (p.  27). Finally, values and context (e.g., Guideline 2.14; BACB,
Slocum et al. (2014) defined EBP in ABA to be 2020), and clinical expertise (e.g., Core Principle
“a decision-making process that integrates (a) the 4; BACB, 2020). Additionally, the interrelated-
best available evidence with (b) clinical expertise ness of ethics and EBP was implicitly captured
and (c) client values and context” (p.  44). The by Slocum et al. (2014) who referenced the Code
definition presented by Slocum and colleagues to justify their claims.
stresses that EBP is a process of decision-making Descriptive and normative ethical behaviors
that involves the integration of three components are influenced by the process of decision-making
which are also present in the definitions used by with EBPs. For example, consider an applied
other fields like medicine and psychology. Even behavior analyst who is considering verbal
before the concept of EBP was introduced within behavior interventions for a minimally vocal
the field of ABA, the tenets of the field empha- child with autism. To make the “right” decision
sized the importance of relying on interventions on intervention selection (descriptive ethical
based on evidence and creating goals and imple- behavior), the applied behavior analyst would
menting treatments which have high social valid- evaluate the research literature to identify inter-
ity (Baer et al., 1968; Slocum et al., 2014). ventions with the best evidence for individuals
One framework for EBP has been suggested similar to the client and their presenting skills.
by Spencer et al. (2012) in which the authors sug- Additionally, the applied behavior analyst would
gest that in the process of selecting, adapting, and likely consult the family for their preference (i.e.,
implementing interventions, practitioners have to incorporate client values) of alternative and aug-
continually make decisions, and the three compo- mented communication systems (e.g., sign lan-
nents of EBP are influencing the decisions guage or picture based) to ensure the designed
throughout the process. Although the framework intervention will be implemented and lead to
is presented in a linear form, the authors suggest optimal intervention outcomes. Lastly, the
that the process might not always be linear. For a applied behavior analyst would tailor the inter-
full treatment of the history of EBP and EBP vention procedures and behavior change targets
decision models, we refer the reader to Chap. 2 of based on their past history with successful behav-
this book. ior change (i.e., incorporate clinical expertise
within their scope of competence).
The process of decision-making with EBPs is
4.3.4 An Ethical-EBP Decision influenced by descriptive and normative ethical
Model behavior. For example, applied behavior analysts
are currently ethically obligated to implement
Despite different authors treating ethical assessments and interventions with empirical
decision-­making and decisions with EBP sepa- backing (Guidelines 2.01, 2.13, 2.14; BACB,
rately, some authors from medicine have argued 2020). Additionally, applied behavior analysts
that EBPs are integral to clinical ethical decision-­ have an obligation to avoid interventions that
making and vice versa (e.g., Borry et al., 2006; may have empirical backing based on potential
Tyson, 1995). That is, clinical ethical decision-­ short-term or long-term harm to the individuals
making requires careful consideration of current they serve (e.g., Core Principle 1; BACB, 2020).
best-available evidence to make the “right” deci- For a child with life-threatening challenging
sion about how to treat individual clients. In behavior, conducting a gold standard functional
ABA, ethical decision models and EBP decision analysis (best available evidence) may not mini-
models also are interrelated. This is implicitly mize short-term harm to the client. In this situa-
60 V. G. Marya et al.

tion, different descriptive ethical claims suggest terminal environment should be (e.g., to reduce
incompatible behavior with simply “choosing the SIB in the classroom to near zero rates), decision
intervention with the most evidence.” Depending models can help the applied behavior analyst
on the normative ethical theory one prefers, emit a chain of behaviors that will increase the
choosing among available EBPs might mean likelihood of reaching that ideal terminal state.
placing greater weight on the clinical expertise For applied behavior analysts, ethical behavior
component or the client preferences component and the implementation of EBP are intertwined.
of EBP decision models. Also, because ethical behavior, ethical decision-­
In sum, ethical decision-making and decision-­ making, and decision-making relative to EBP are
making with EBPs are necessarily interrelated. all just behaviors, they can be described, pre-
“There is nothing in a methodology which deter- dicted, and controlled by the same laws and prin-
mines the values governing its use” (Skinner, ciples as all other behavior: principles such as
1971, p. 148). Figure 4.3 highlights how ethical those described in the research literature on
decision-making and decision-making with EBPs choice, verbal behavior, and optimality. Thus, in
might interact by combining the common com- total, ethical decision models for implementing
ponents ethical decision-making models with the EBP in ABA should involve prompts regarding
common components of EBP decision models. the variables that should be considered when
The main steps of a clinical decision-making pro- selecting an EBP for a client or student and the
cess are shown in rectangles. Between each step, many ways that our learning history can bias us
we highlight how components of ethical decision-­ into choosing a suboptimal EBP.
making (circles) and components of decision-­ Figure 4.4 shows a decision model for ethi-
making with EBPs (triangles) factor into the cally selecting from multiple EBPs while
clinical decision-making process. accounting for known causes of descriptive and
normative ethical behavior and ethical decision-­
making. For practical use, we have turned the
4.4  pplying Ethical Causal
A decision model into a checklist that applied
and Decision Models to EBP behavior analysts can use to ethically decide
in ABA between two EBPs in situations where multiple
options exist (see Fig.  4.5 for an example of a
To summarize the chapter to this point, ethics for completed worksheet). In the final section below,
applied behavior analysts can be defined as the we discuss how all that we have covered to this
descriptive and normative rules about right and point might practically coalesce into ethical deci-
wrong behavior analytic professional and sions to implement EBP in ABA.  As Engels
research practices. Decision-making can be reportedly stated, “an ounce of action is worth a
defined as the chain of behaviors that lead to a ton of theory” (Bohan & Kennedy, 2002). We
defined terminal environmental state. When an have discussed the ton, now we can get to the
applied behavior analyst has a sense of what the ounce.

Fig. 4.3  Decision model showing the interaction between ethical behavior, ethical decision-making, and the imple-
mentation of evidence-based practices in clinical and educational settings
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 61

STEPS IN ETHICALLY NAVIGATING EBP DILEMMA


DESIRED OUTCOME Identify goal of behavior-change program
State goal of behavior-change program
Rule out any medical causes
Is the desired behavior amenable to behavioral treatment?

TARGET BEHAVIOR Consider the target outcome, target setting, client/caregiver values

Define target behavior


Conduct assessment to inform behavior-change program
ASSESSMENT (Obtain consent, consider contextual variables)

Identify relevant assessment

Obtain consent to conduct assessment

Implement assessment and synthesize results


Conduct a literature review of interventions and evaluate the results
SEARCH AVAILABLE EVIDENCE using professional judgement

Intervention 1 Intervention 2 Intervention 3 Intervention 4

Is intervention based on science and behavior analysis


Is the intervention commensurate with education, training, or supervised experience
Is the intervention suited for the client based on their values and context? If not, is
there an opportunity to obtain training, supervision, and/or consultation from
someone who is competent?
Long-term and short-term benefits to the clients
Efficiency and cost-effectiveness

Risks and side-effects

Consider any potential biases


Does the intervention include punishment or other restrictive procedures?
Are there any environmental conditions that would prevent implementation of the
intervention

Are there opportunities for collaboration?

Consider client preferences

Is there evidence supporting likelihood of behavior maintenance and generalization?

SELECTED INTERVENTION
The intervention < insert intervention name > was chosen because...
This intervention will best help us meet our intervention goals
JUSTIFICATION STATEMENT because...

ADAPT INTERVENTION
Tailor intervention based on client's unique behavior, environment, and goals

Discuss with client/client's caregivers

IMPLEMENT INTERVENTION

Training for those who will implement intervention

Ongoing monitoring and evaluation

EVALUATE OUTCOME

Fig. 4.4  Worksheet to aid ethical decision-making when selecting among EBP in clinical and educational ABA
settings

4.4.1 D
 iscrete Trial Teaching vs. trial teaching (DTT) and natural environment
Natural Environment Training training (NET)? Consider the two cases of Athina
and Gia. Athina and Gia are 4-year-old children
So how does an applied behavior analyst ethi- with an ICD-10 CM diagnosis of F84.0 – autistic
cally choose between the two EBPs of discrete disorder. Both were referred for services due to
62 V. G. Marya et al.

STEPS TO ETHICALLY NAVIGATING EBP DILEMMA


DESIRED OUTCOME Identify goal of behavior-change program
State goal of behavior-change program Teach Athina to follow one-step instructions

Rule out any medical causes Consulted with medical doctor; no biological issues found

Is the desired behavior amenable to behavioral treatment? Yes


TARGET BEHAVIOR Consider the target outcome, target setting, client/caregiver values
Athina will independently and willingly follow 10 varying discriminated instructions. She will initiate following the
Define target behavior presented instruction within 5 seconds of SD presentation.
ASSESSMENT Conduct assessment to inform behavior-change program (Obtain consent, consider contextual variables)
Identify relevant assessment VB–MAPP and parent interview
Obtain consent to conduct assessment Parent consent obtained
Implement assessment and synthesize results VB–MAPP results and parent report showed absence of skill
SEARCH AVAILABLE EVIDENCE Conduct a literature review of interventions and evaluate the results using professional judgement
Intervention 3:Word
Intervention 1: NET Intervention 2: DTT
Retrieval
Yes (Smith, 2001; Tarbox & Najdowski, Not based on behavior
Is intervention based on science and behavior analysis Yes (Sundberg & Partington, 1998; Weiss, 2014). 2008; Lerman et al., 2016), analysis

Is the intervention commensurate with education, No prior experience, but has the opportunity for
supervision and guidance from experienced Yes.
training, or supervised experience Clinical Director.
Is the intervention suited for the client based on their No, discussed intervention with the family.
values and context? If not, is there an opportunity to Intervention is not suitable for the client based Yes, discussed intervention with the
on their values and context (parents report a family. Intervention suitable for the client
obtain training, supervision, and/or consultation from more structured approach is best for Athina’s based on their values and context.
someone who is competent? learning).
Long-term and short-term benefits to the clients Both long-term and short-term benefits Both long-term and short-term benefits
Based on parent report, Athina will not be as Based on parent report, Athina will be
Efficiency and cost-effectiveness efficient with learning via a more naturalistic / more efficient with learning via a more
play-based approach. structured approach.

Given parent report, a naturalistic teaching A more structured approach presentes a


approach poses a risk for not achieving optimal benefit to achieving optimal learning based
Risks and side-effects on parent reports of Athina’s learning
outcomes. style.
Consulted with a colleague about biases and Consulted with a colleague about biases
Consider any potential biases biases have been ruled out. and biases have been ruled out.
Does intervention include punishment/other restrictive No. No.
procedures?
Are there any environmental conditions that would Environmental conditions support intervention. Environmental conditions support
prevent implementation of the intervention intervention.

Are there opportunities for collaboration? Yes. Yes.

Consider client preferences A more structured approach is preferred. A more structured approach is preferred.
Is there evidence supporting the likelihood of behavior Yes, there is literature to support this. Yes, there is literature to support this.
maintenance and generalization?
SELECTED INTERVENTION DTT
The intervention DTT was chosen because it better suits the client’s preferences. Also, the applied behavior analyst has
experience using DTT.
JUSTIFICATION STATEMENT
This intervention will best help us meet our intervention goals because it will suit Athina’s unique learning needs and be
acceptable to caregivers.
ADAPT INTERVENTION
Tailor intervention based on client's unique behavior,
This intervention will be tailored as needed.
environment, & goals
Discuss with client/client's caregivers Client and caregivers consented to intervention

IMPLEMENT INTERVENTION

Training for those who will implement intervention Behavior technicians are trained
Ongoing monitoring and evaluation Continuous assessment will be completed
EVALUATE OUTCOME Data are monitored to evaluate outcome

Fig. 4.5  Example completed worksheet for Athina

concerns regarding their current levels of lan- The first step toward determining which EBP
guage and social skills. Specifically, both did not to choose is for Amir to more specifically define
respond to their names when called, were not the target behavior relative to the desired out-
observed to follow simple instructions, and had come for both Athina and Gia (i.e., target behav-
limited language skills (i.e., defining the desired ior; Fig. 4.4). Once defined, Amir then needs to
outcome; Fig. 4.4). conduct an appropriate assessment to inform
more specifically what responses need to be
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 63

taught and what stimuli should be included in using a DTT approach differs from the timing of
training. Based on the information to this point, antecedent and consequence stimulus delivery
Amir chooses to conduct the Verbal Behavior using NET. In NET, learning trials may vary in
Milestones Assessment and Placement Program duration, what defines the beginning and end of a
(VB-MAPP; Sundberg, 2008), and Athina and trial, and may involve one-to-many learning trials
Gia both score in Level 1 for all skills. before moving to something else. Word retrieval
Next, Amir discusses the results of the assess- strategies differ from DTT and NET in the timing
ment with the families and recruits their priorities of antecedent and consequence stimulus delivery.
regarding goals and outcomes for their children. Here, the individual working with the client may
Both families express that they would like for have the participant say or read aloud 5–10 words
their children to respond when called, follow phonologically similar to the tact they want the
instructions (e.g., go get your shoes, sit down, student to emit when finally asked the question,
close the door), and expand their language reper- “What color is this?” (e.g., James & Burke, 2000;
toire. Based on the results of the assessment and Linebaugh et al., 2011).
the parents’ preferences, Amir plans to design DTT, NET, and word retrieval strategies have
acquisition programs to: (a) teach tacts of com- extensive empirical support as a successful
mon items relevant to the children’s daily lives, method for teaching new skills to individuals
(b) respond to their name when emitted by others with ASD and related developmental disabilities.
(e.g., parents, teachers, peers), and (c) follow For example, DTT has been shown to effectively
basic instructions commonly encountered in their teach new forms of behavior and new discrimina-
daily lives (e.g., “come here,” “pick up toy,” and tions and to manage disruptive behavior (e.g.,
“put in box”). Smith, 2001). Similarly, NET has been shown to
The next step is for Amir to search the empiri- effectively teach manding and other motivation-
cal literature to understand what available inter- ally dependent behavior skills (e.g., Sundberg &
ventions have empirical support (i.e., search Partington, 1999; Weiss, 2014). Lastly, word
available evidence; Fig.  4.4). People working retrieval strategies have also been shown to suc-
directly with clients or students can arrange the cessfully result in improved naming performance,
presentation and covariation of environmental generalization to untrained stimuli, and emit
stimuli to teach a new response in many different recall behaviors (e.g., James & Burke, 2000;
ways. After searching the archives of the Journal Linebaugh et al., 2011). Further, in reviewing this
of Applied Behavior Analysis, Behavior Analysis literature, Amir finds that DTT and NET are
in Practice, and Behavioral Interventions, Amir based in science and behavior analysis. However,
finds that DTT (e.g., Lerman et al., 2016; Smith, word retrieval strategies  – though scientifically
2001; Tarbox & Najdowski, 2008), NET (e.g., based – are not based on an operant analysis of
Sundberg & Partington, 1998; Weiss, 2014), and verbal behavior. Thus, Amir (via Guideline 2.01;
word retrieval strategies (e.g., McGregor & BACB, 2020) no longer considers this interven-
Leonard, 1989; Wing, 1990) are used in the lit- tion in the selection process.
erature to teach the target behaviors defined for The next step is for Amir to consider whether
Athina and Gia. the identified EBPs are commensurate with
In DTT, the individual working directly with Amir’s education, training, and supervised expe-
the client or student explicitly arranges instruc- rience. Amir remembers reviewing DTT and
tional trials of similar duration and that com- NET in the Intervention Design class from grad-
prises a definite beginning and end (Leaf et al., uate school and fondly remembers the excited
2016). These discrete trials are often delivered group discussion in class on the conditions to
repeatedly in sets of 3 or more trials while focus- choose DTT over NET and vice versa. Amir also
ing on a specific set of responses (Cummings & remembers his supervised practicum training
Carr, 2009; Najdowski et al., 2009). The timing experience binder and the many documented
of antecedent and consequence stimulus delivery instances of implementing and supervising other
64 V. G. Marya et al.

staff-implemented DTT procedures and NET Similarly, Amir learned from the completed lit-
procedures. However, those clients were a bit erature review that NET has the benefits of
older, and, since the practicum finished, the orga- increased engagement with the learning trials and
nization that hired Amir requires new applied greater generalization of learned skills across
behavior analysts to follow a set of programs contexts, but NET also has the drawbacks of
designed by the clinical director. For individuals potentially less efficient rates of acquisition for
aged similar to Athina and Gia, this has involved non-motivationally related targets and special-
only DTT.  Nevertheless, Amir also knows that ized training to capture naturally occurring,
the clinical director wrote her dissertation on momentary changes in motivation.
NET, and NET is widely used with other clients At this point, Amir has likely identified what
in the company. So, Amir can get guidance from the ideal EBP would be for Athina and Gia. In
a competent applied behavior analyst, if needed. total, the best EBP for each client was determined
In total, Amir is confident he has the education, based on the desired outcome, the assessment
training, and supervised experience for DTT and results, a review of the empirical literature, client
NET, though less so for NET. values and preferences, Amir’s past experience and
The next step is to determine whether DTT training, and a consequentialist analysis of inter-
and NET are suited for the client based on their vention efficiency and safety. Amir has one final
values and context. Given that Athina’s and Gia’s step to complete before turning the ideal interven-
performance on the VB-MAPP indicated their tion into a practical intervention – self-reflection.
receptive language skills are limited, their par- Ethical decision-making about EBPs is a
ents can help inform Amir of their likely values. chain of behaviors shaped and determined by
In conversations with the parents of Athina, Amir respondent and operant processes. Thus, it is pos-
discovered that they prefer the ABA sessions to sible that the interventions Amir has determined
be heavily structured as Athina would get easily as being ideal for Athina and Gia is the result of
distracted with past RBTs who used more of a Amir’s bias for those approaches. In behavior
play-based approach. In contrast, conversations analysis, bias refers to a pattern of choice that
with the parents of Gia indicated that Gia has cannot be predicted by the schedules of rein-
struggled in the past with RBTs who tried to forcement and punishment specific to the choice
make her sit for long stretches at a time and have context (e.g., Baum, 1974). For ethical decision-­
read stories that too much rote learning trial pre- making between EBPs, we might assume that
sentations can lead to “robot-like behavior” in what ought to reinforce Amir’s choice is what-
children who receive ABA-based interventions. ever leads the client to reach their desired out-
Thus, Gia’s parents would prefer a more natural come. Thus, bias toward one intervention would
and play-based approach to ABA-based be any preference in choice for one EBP that dif-
intervention. fers from the optimal intervention that allows the
The next step in the process for ethically client to reach their desired outcome. To test for
deciding between two EBPs might be considered potential bias, Amir can ask a trusted colleague,
the consequentialist steps. Here, Amir must eval- mentor, or supervisor to play “devil’s advocate”
uate DTT and NET based on their long-term and and review his ethical decision-making process
short-term benefits to the clients, the larger com- and to question all assumptions.
munity, and society as a whole (Guidelines 2.09a After testing for biases, Amir is likely confi-
and 2.09c; Fig. 4.4). From his literature review, dent that he has ethically chosen the ideal EBP
Amir learned that some of the benefits of DTT for Athina and Gia. Now, Amir can shift toward
are efficient rates of response acquisition under practically implementing the EBP.  Here, Amir
tight stimulus control (Smith, 2001). However, must determine how environmental conditions
DTT has also been shown to have the drawbacks may hinder implementation of the EBP
of lower generalization across settings and spe- (Guideline 2.16; BACB, 2020). When entering
cialized training to implement (Smith, 2001). this stage of ethical decision-making with EBPs,
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 65

Amir should decide how much hindrance would suited for Athina and NET is best suited for Gia,
lead him to either abandon this EBP or switch to Amir then (a) writes adapted EBP programs for
a different EBP or that would potentially require each client, (b) supervises the implementation of
training to those implementing the interventions those behavior change programs, and (c) evalu-
that are not covered by the payer. Stated differ- ates how close the programs are resulting in
ently, there is likely a point wherein the environ- behavior change toward the originally stated
mental conditions are such that the EBP cannot desired outcome.
be implemented effectively as supported by the
empirical literature. Understanding what this
point looks like will help Amir identify when 4.5 Summary
those conditions have been met and a new
approach taken. Before one can implement EBPs, applied behav-
Once the cutoff point for reconsidering the ior analysts must choose among the available
chosen EBP is known, Amir can begin assessing interventions with empirical evidence that are
the environmental conditions that may prevent related to the desired behavior change outcome
implementing the intervention. These conditions for which they have been contracted. Choosing
might be the amount of training and supervision among EBPs necessarily involves ethics – state-
required for the intervention to be implemented ments about the right or wrong way applied
with fidelity (e.g., Smith, 2001), requirements for behavior analysts go about choosing among
collaborating with other professionals based on EBPs. Thus, to ethically make decisions about
the service-delivery context (e.g., Brodhead, EBPs, applied behavior analysts would need to
2015; Cox, 2012, 2019b), the ease with which consider (a) the variables that influence ethical
behavior change can be generalized and main- decision-making (captured by causal models),
tained (e.g., Sundberg & Partington, 1999; Weiss, (b) the information and potential points of error
2014), and what empirical support may exist to that may lead to “wrong” decisions with EBPs
support that any tailoring required for Athina or (captured by decision models), and (c) how
Gia would not mitigate the effectiveness of the causal models and decision models can be practi-
intervention. Succinctly, Amir must determine cally combined for use in the contexts and set-
that the intervention continues to be supported by tings within which applied behavior analysts
evidence once sufficiently tailored to meet the practice.
client’s unique situation. The first section of this chapter discussed
Once the above steps have been completed, causal models of ethical behavior and ethical
Amir can select the EBP ethically justified based decision-making. In the first section, we dis-
on the client’s values, preferences, and desired cussed how morality differs from ethics and how
outcome; the available empirical evidence, con- each may influence ethical decision-making. We
sidering his own clinical expertise, training, and then discussed two types of ethical behavior in
past experiences; and considering the context descriptive ethical behavior (what is the right
within which the intervention must be imple- thing to do) and normative ethical behavior (why
mented. Though not required, it may help with is it the right thing to do). For our conversation,
case-review and record-keeping for Amir to doc- that would be the difference between the behav-
ument the result of the above steps as a justifica- ior of selecting and implementing the right EBP
tion statement. This will allow anyone to quickly (descriptive ethical behavior) and the behaviors
understand why the EBP was chosen and how the of providing evidence and argument to support
EBP aligns with the desired outcome and profes- why that EBP was chosen (normative ethical
sional standards of applied behavior analysts. behavior). Finally, we closed the first section by
Finally, come the multiple steps which applied highlighting some of the many areas of basic
behavior analysts are probably most familiar. behavioral research that are known to influence
Once, Amir has determined that DTT is best decision-making generally and how these com-
66 V. G. Marya et al.

bine into a causal model of ethical decision-­ tool put forth in the current chapter. This does not
making with EBPs. mean that existing tools and decision models
The second section of this chapter discussed should not be used. However, it does suggest that
decision models. Specifically, we discussed how the models might need to be modified and
the function of decision models is typically to improve to avoid model users from making less
help the model user avoid making the wrong than optimal decisions. As a model user, this also
decision. Making the wrong decision means that suggests they should: understand why they are
we have a desired outcome that we are optimiz- using the model, objectively define the client out-
ing for and ethical benchmarks against which we come they are trying to achieve, and consistently
can measure our effectiveness. Many decision collect data and critically examine their decision-­
models have been published specific to ethical making processes to ensure that their clients
decision-making and decision-making with receive the best care possible.
EBPs. In the second section, we reviewed the pri-
mary characteristics of these models and how
they combine into a single ethical decision-­ References
making model for EBP.
Finally, we closed the chapter by showing Adams, M.  P. (2007). Conscience and conflict. The
American Journal of Bioethics, 7(12), 28–29. https://
how the theoretical and basic research on causal doi.org/10.1080/15265160701710105
and decision models of ethical decision-making Alexander, L., & Moore, M. (2016). Deontological ethics.
with EBPs can be collapsed into a practical deci- In E.N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of phi-
sion tool. We also walked through the use of this losophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2020/
entries/ethics-­deontological/
tool in a hypothetical situation wherein an applied American Astronomical Society (AAS). (2017). AAS
behavior analyst had to choose between imple- Code of Ethics. https://aas.org/policies/ethics
menting DTT and NET for two clients with simi- American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists.
lar clinical presentation and intervention contexts. (2007). The limits of conscientious refusal in repro-
ductive medicine. ACOG committee opinion no. 385.
In short, applied behavior analysts can practically Obstetrics and Gynecology, 110(5), 1203–1208. https://
leverage a functional ethical approach to doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195325911.003.0019
decision-­making with EBPs. In so doing, applied American Medical Association. (2016). AMA prin-
behavior analysts are likely to systematically ciples of medical ethics. https://www.ama-­assn.org/
delivering-­care/ama-­principles-­medical-­ethics
identify the variables that influence ethical American Psychological Association. (2017). Ethical
decision-­making with EBPs, control for biases in principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
decision-making, and implement EBPs that opti- https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/
mize the likelihood of obtaining the positive out- American Psychological Association (APA. (2006).
American Psychological Association, presidential
comes for which clients sought our help. task force on evidence-based practice, evidence-based
This chapter highlights several areas ripe for practice in psychology. American Psychologist, 61,
future research related to the topic of ethical 271–285.
decision-­making with EBPs. First, only a handful Annas, J. (2006). Virtue ethics. The Oxford handbook of
ethical theory, 515–536.
of experiments have directly examined the vari- Aravind, M., & Chung, K.  C. (2010). Evidence-based
ables that predict and control ethical decision-­ medicine and hospital reform: Tracing origins back
making or clinical decision-making in applied to Florence Nightingale. Plastic and Reconstructive
behavior analysts (e.g., Cox, 2021; Cox & Surgery, 125(1), 403–409. https://doi.org/10.1097/
PRS.0b013e3181c2bb89
Brodhead, in press). Second, though many deci- Association of American Educators (AAE). (2013).
sion models have been put forth in the published Code of ethics for educators. Retrieved from:
behavior analytic literature, few have been rigor- https://www.aaeteachers.org/index.php/about-­u s/
ously tested to determine whether they lead the aae-­code-­of-­ethics
Ayala, F.  J. (1987). The biological roots of morality.
model user to consistently optimize the outcomes Biology and Philosophy, 2(3), 235–252. https://doi.
for which the model is being used – including the org/10.1007/BF00128831
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 67

Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some Role for a Code of Ethics. Journal of Autism and
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2729–2738. https://
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97. doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1530-z
Bailey, J., & Burch, M. (2016). Ethics for behavior ana- Cox, D.  J. (2019a). The many functions of quantitative
lysts. Routledge. modeling. Computational Brain & Behavior, 2, 166–
Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the 169. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42113-­019-­00048-­9
matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Cox, D.  J. (2019b). Ethical considerations in interdisci-
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22(1), 231–242. plinary treatments. In R.  D. Rieske (Ed.), Handbook
Baum, W.  M. (2005). Understanding behaviorism: of interdisciplinary treatments for autism Spectrum
Behavior, culture, and evolution (2nd ed.). Blackwell disorder (pp. 49–61). Springer.
Publishing. Cox, D. J. (Aug, 2020). Moral philosophy, ethical theories,
Baum, W.  M. (2018). Three laws of behavior: alloca- and applied ethics. 8th Annual Ethics in Professional
tion, induction, and covariance. Behavior Analysis: Practice Conference. https://doi.org/10.13140/
Research and Practice, 18, 239–251. https://doi. RG.2.2.20697.75361
org/10.1037/bar0000104 Cox, D.  J. (2021). Descriptive and normative ethical
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020). Ethics code behavior appear to be functionally distinct. Journal
for behavior analysts. Author. of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(1), 168–191. https://
Bohan, H., & Kennedy, G. (2002). Reviewed work: doi.org/10.1002/jaba.761
Redefining roles and relationships: Our society in the Cox, D. J., & Brodhead, M. T. (in press). A proof of con-
new millennium. Papers from the third rural resource cept analysis of decision-making with time-series data.
development conference. The Furrow, 53(4), 253–255. The Psychological Record. https://doi.org/10.1007/
https://www.jstor.org/stable/27664516 s40732-­020-­00451-­w
Borry, P., Schotsmans, P., & Dierickx, K. (2006). Cummings, A. R., & Carr, J. E. (2009). Evaluating prog-
Evidence-based medicine and its role in ethi- ress in behavioral programs for children with autism
cal decision-making. Journal of Evaluation in spectrum disorders via continuous and discontinuous
Clinical Practice, 12(3), 306–311. https://doi. measurement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
org/10.1111/j.1365-­2753.2006.00548.x 42(1), 57–71. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­57
Borum, R. (2010). Understanding terrorist psychology. Curlin, F.  A. (2008). Conscience and clinical practice:
The psychology of counter-terrorism, 19–33. https:// Medical ethics in the face of moral controversy.
doi.org/10.1002/9780470672532.wbepp275 Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics, 29(3), 129–133.
Brodhead, M.  T. (2015). Maintaining professional rela- https://doi.org/10.1007/s11017-­008-­9071-­3
tionships in an interdisciplinary setting: Strategies Dallery, J., & Soto, P. L. (2013). Quantitative description
for navigating nonbehavioral treatment recommenda- of environment-behavior relations. In APA handbook
tions for individuals with autism. Behavior Analysis of behavior analysis, Vol. 1: Methods and principles
in Practice, 8(1), 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/ (pp. 219–249). American Psychological Association.
s40617-­015-­0042-­7 Deochand, N., Elridge, R.  R., & Peterson, S.  M.
Brodhead, M.  T., Cox, D.  J., & Quigley, S.  P. (2018). (2020). Toward the development of a functional
Practical ethics for effective treatment of autism spec- analysis risk assessment decision tool. Behavior
trum disorders. Academic Press. Analysis in Practice, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Brummett, A., & Ostertag, C.  J. (2018). Two trou- s40617-­020-­00433-­y.
bling trends in the conversation over whether Dienstag, J. F. (1996). Between history and nature: Social
clinical ethics consultants have ethics expertise. contract theory in Locke and the founders. The Journal
HEC Forum, 30, 157–169. https://doi.org/10.1007/ of Politics, 58(4), 985–1009. https://www.jstor.org/
s10730-­017-­9321-­8 stable/2960146
Carpenter, J.  P., & Matthews, P.  H. (2004). Social reci- Endicott, K., & Higbee, T. S. (2007). Contriving motivat-
procity (No. 1347). IZA Discussion Papers. https:// ing operations to evoke mands for information in pre-
www.econstor.eu/bitstream/10419/20617/1/dp1347. schoolers with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum
pdf Disorders, 1(3), 210–217.
Carr, D. (2014). The human and educational signifi- Fantino, E., Kulik, J., Stolarz-Fantino, S., & Wright, W.
cance of honesty as an epistemic and moral vir- (1997). The conjunction fallacy: A test of averaging
tue. Educational Theory, 64(1), 1–14. https://doi. hypotheses. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(1),
org/10.1111/edth.12047 96–101.
Cooper, J.  O., Heron, T.  E., & Heward, W.  L. (2020). Fisher, W. W., & Mazur, J. E. (1997). Basic and applied
Applied behavior analysis (3rd ed.). Pearson. research on choice responding. Journal of Applied
Cottone, R. R., & Claus, R. E. (2000). Ethical decision-­ Behavior Analysis, 30(3), 387–410. https://doi.
making models: A review of the literature. Journal of org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­387
Counseling & Development, 78(3), 275–283. https:// Ford, N. J., & Austin, W. (2018). Conflicts of conscience
doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-­6676.2000.tb01908.x in the neonatal intensive care unit: Perspectives of
Cox, D.J. (2012). From Interdisciplinary to Integrated Alberta. Nursing Ethics, 25(8), 992–1003.
Care of the Child with Autism: the Essential
68 V. G. Marya et al.

Ford, R.  C., & Richardson, W.  D. (1994). Ethical deci- Huang, X. (2007). Justice as a virtue: An analysis of
sion making: A review of the empirical literature. Aristotle’s virtue of justice. Frontiers of Philosophy
Journal of Business Ethics, 13(3), 205–221. https:// in China, 2(2), 265–279. https://www.jstor.org/
doi.org/10.1007/BF02074820 stable/27823292
Frigg, R., & Hartmann, S. (2020). Models in science. In Hursh, S. R., & Silberberg, A. (2008). Economic demand
E.N.  Zalta (Ed.) The stanford encyclopedia of phi- and essential value. Psychological Review, 115(1), 186–
losophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2020/ 198. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-­295X.115.1.186
entries/models-­science/ Iltis, A.  S., & Rasmussen, L.  M. (2016). The “ethics”
Gachter, S., & Schulz, J. F. (2016). Intrinsic honesty and expertise in clinical ethics consultation. Journal of
the prevalence of rule violations across societies. Medicine and Philosophy, 41, 363–368. https://doi.
Nature, 531(7595), 496–499. https://doi.org/10.1038/ org/10.1093/jmp/jhw013
nature17160 Iwata, B. A., & DeLeon, I. G. (1996). The functional anal-
Geiger, K.  B., Carr, J.  E., & LeBlanc, L.  A. (2010). ysis screening tool. The Florida Center on Self-Injury,
Function-based treatments for escape-maintained The University of Florida.
problem behavior: A treatment-selection model for Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
practicing behavior analysts. Behavior Analysis in & Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional anal-
Practice, 3(1), 22–32. ysis of self-injury. In Analysis and Intervention in
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological Developmental Disabilities, Vol. 2 (pp. 3–20).
theory and women’s development. Harvard University James, L. E., & Burke, D. M. (2000). Phonological prim-
Press. ing effects on word retrieval and tip-of-the-tongue
Grace, B., Wainwright, T., Solomons, W., Camden, J., & experiences in young and older adults. Journal
Ellis-Caird, H. (2020). How do clinical psychologists of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
make ethical decisions? A systematic review of empir- and Cognition, 26(6), 1378–1391. https://doi.
ical research. Clinical Ethics, 15(4), 213–224. https:// org/10.1037/0278-­7393.26.6.1378
doi.org/10.1177/1477750920927165 Kazdin, A. E. (2008). Evidence-based treatment and prac-
Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2013). How many impul- tice: New opportunities to bridge clinical research and
sivities? A discounting prospective. Journal of the practice, enhance the knowledge base, and improve
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99(1), 3–13. patient care. American Psychologist, 63(3), 146–159.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.1 https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-­066X.63.3.146
Green, J., & Walker, K. (2009). A contingency model Kirkpatrick, M., Akers, J., & Rivera, G. (2019). Use of
for ethical decision-making by educational leaders. behavioral skills training with teachers: A systematic
International Journal of Educational Leadership review. Journal of Behavioral Education, 28, 344–
Preparation, 4(4), n4. 361. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­019-­09322-­z
Greipp, M.  E. (1992). Greipp’s model of ethical deci- Kohlberg, L., & Kramer, R. (1969). Continuities and
sion making. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 17(6), discontinuities in childhood and adult moral develop-
734–738. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-­2648.1992. ment. Human development, 12(2), 93–120. https://
tb01972.x www.jstor.org/stable/26761853
Haidt, J., Graham, J., & Joseph, C. (2009). Above and Kohlberg, L. (1971). Form is to ought. In T. Mischel (Ed.)
below left-right: Ideological narratives and moral Cognitive Development and Epistemology (pp.164–
foundations. Psychological Inquiry, 20, 110–119. 165). Academic Press.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10478400903028573 Larrabee, M.  J. (1993). An ethic of care: Feminist and
Hanley, G.  P., Jin, C.  S., Vanselow, N.  R., & Hanratty, interdisciplinary perspectives. Routledge.
L.  A. (2014). Producing meaningful improvements Lau, R. R. (2003). Models of decision-making. In D. O.
in problem behavior of children with autism via syn- Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook
thesized analyses and treatments. Journal of Applied of political psychology (pp. 19–59). Oxford University
Behavior Analysis, 47, 16–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Press.
jaba.106 Leaf, J. B., Cihon, J., Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (2016). A
Harper, S. J. (2009). Ethics versus morality: A problematic progressive approach to discrete trial teaching: Some
divide. Philosophy & Social Criticism, 35(9), 1063– current guidelines. International Electronic Journal of
1077. https://doi.org/10.1177/0191453709343388 Elementary Education, 9(2), 361–372.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of Lerman, D. C., Valentino, A. L., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2016).
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 243–266. Discrete trial training. In R.  Lang, T.  Hancock, &
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1970.13-­243 N.  Singh (Eds.), Early intervention for young chil-
Ho, D. (2016). Keeping it ethically real. Journal of dren with autism Spectrum disorder. Evidence-based
Medicine and Philosophy, 41, 369–383. practices in behavioral health. Springer. https://doi.
Hoeppe, G. (2018). Tensions of accountability: Scientists, org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­30925-­5_3
technicians and the ethical life of data production Li, J.  A., Dong, D., Wei, Z., Liu, Y., Pan, Y., Nori, F.,
in astronomy. Science as Culture, 27(4), 488–512. & Zhang, X. (2020). Quantum reinforcement learn-
https://doi.org/10.1080/09505431.2018.1510909 ing during human decision-making. Nature Human
4  Ethical Decision-Making and Evidenced-Based Practices 69

Behaviour, 4(3), 294–307. https://doi.org/10.1038/ during second-order response-independent schedules.


s41562-­019-­0804-­2 The Psychological Record, 49, 221–243.
Linebaugh, C.  W., Shisler, R.  J., & Lehner, L. (2011). Noddings, N. (1984). Caring: A feminine approach to eth-
Cueing hierarchies and word retrieval: A therapy ics & moral education. University of California Press.
program. Aphasiology, 19(1), 77–80. https://doi. Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y.,
org/10.1080/02687030444000363 & Vollmer, T. R. (2000). Questions about behavioral
MacIntyre, A. (2003). A short history of ethics: A history function (QABF): A behavioral checklist for func-
of moral philosophy from the Homeric age to the 20th tional assessment of aberrant behavior. Research in
century. Routledge. Developmental Disabilities, 21(3), 223–229.
Marr, M.  J. (2015). Reprint of “Mathematics as verbal Pattison, K.  F., & Zentall, T.  R. (2014). Suboptimal
behavior”. Behavioural Processes, 114, 35–40. https:// choice by dogs: When less is better than more. Animal
doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.03.008 Cognition, 17, 1019–1022. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Matthews, B.  A., Catania, A.  C., & Shimoff, E. (1985). s10071-­014-­0735-­2
Effects of uninstructed verbal behavior on nonverbal Rachlin, H., Raineri, A., & Cross, D. (1991). Subjective
responding: Contingency descriptions versus per- probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental
formance descriptions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 233–244. https://doi.
Analysis of Behavior, 43, 155–164. org/10.1901/jeab.1991.55-­233
McClimans, L., & Slowther, A. (2016). Moral expertise in Rescorla, R.  A., & Wagner, A.  R. (1972). A theory of
the clinic: Lessons learned from medicine and science. Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effective-
Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 41, 401–415. ness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H.
https://doi.org/10.1093/jmp/jhw011 Black & W.  F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical condition-
McDowell, J.  J. (1989). Two modern developments ing II: Current research and theory (pp.  64–99).
in matching theory. The Behavior Analyst, 12(2), Appleton-Century-Crofts.
153–166. Ribes-Inesta, E. (1997). Causality and contingency: Some
McFarland, D.  J. (1977). Decision making in ani- conceptual considerations. The Psychological Record,
mals. Nature, 269(5623), 15–21. https://doi. 47(4), 619–635. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395249
org/10.1038/269015a0 Rosenberg, N. E., & Schwartz, I. S. (2019). Guidance or
McGregor, K., & Leonard, L. (1989). Facilitating word-­ compliance: What makes an ethical behavior analyst?
finding skills of language-impaired children. Journal Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(2), 473–482. https://
of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 141–147. doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00287-­5
McKerchar, T.  L., & Renda, C.  R. (2012). Delay and Rosenfarb, I.  S., Newland, M.  C., Brannon, S.  E., &
probability discounting in humans: An overview. The Howey, D.  S. (1992). Effects of self-generated rules
Psychological Record, 62(4), 817–834. https://doi. on the development of schedule-controlled behavior.
org/10.1007/BF03395837 Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58,
Meyer-Zehnder, B., Schleger, H. A., Tanner, S., Schnurrer, 107–121. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1992.58-­107
V., Vogt, D. R., Reiter-Theil, S., & Pargger, H. (2017). Sackett, D.  L., Rosenberg, W.  M., Gray, J.  M., Haynes,
How to introduce medical ethics at the bedside-factors R.  B., & Richardson, W.  S. (1996). Evidence based
influencing the implementation of an ethical decision-­ medicine: What it is and what it isn’t. https://doi.
making model. BMC Medical Ethics, 18(1), 16. org/10.1136/bmj.312.7023.71
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12910-­017-­0174-­0 Sackett, D. L., Straus, S. E., Richardson, W. S., Rosenberg,
Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior W., & Haynes, R.  B. (Eds.). (2000). Evidence-­
Analyst, 16(2), 191–206. based medicine: How to teach and practice EBM.
Miltenberger, R.  G. (2012). Behavioral skills training Livingstone.
procedures. In Behavior modification: Principles Schaefer, J.  M., & Andzik, N.  R. (2020). Evaluating
and procedures (pp.  251–269). Wadsworth, Cengage behavioral skills training as an evidence-based
Learning. practice when training parents to intervene with
Monestes, J.  L., Greville, W.  J., & Hooper, N. (2017). their children. Behavior Modification. https://doi.
Derived insensitivity: Rule-based insensitivity to con- org/10.1177/0145445520923996.
tingencies propagates through equivalence. Learning Shapiro, J. P., & Stefkovich, J. A. (2016). Ethical leader-
and Motivation, 59, 55–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. ship and decision making in education: Applying theo-
lmot.2017.08.003 retical perspectives to complex dilemmas. Routledge.
Najdowski, A.  C., Chilingaryan, V., Bergstrom, R., Simon, H. (1977). Administrative behavior (original pub-
Granpeesheh, D., Balasanyan, S., Aguilar, B., … lication date 1945) (4th ed.). The Free Press.
Roane, H. (2009). Comparison of data-collection Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psycho-
methods in a behavioral intervention program for chil- logical terms. Psychological Review, 52(5), 270–277.
dren with pervasive developmental disorders: A repli- https://doi.org/10.1037/h0062535
cation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(4), Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. The
827–832. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­827 Free Press.
Ninness, H. A., & Ninness, S. K. (1999). Contingencies Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. Knopf
of superstition: Self-generated rules and responding Publishing.
70 V. G. Marya et al.

Smith, T. (2001). Discrete trial training in the treat- Behavior, 18(1), 15–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/
ment of autism. Focus on Autism and Other BF03392968
Developmental Disabilities, 16(2), 86–92. https://doi. Tarbox, R. F., & Najdowski, A. C. (2008). Discrete trial
org/10.1177/108835760101600204 training as a teaching paradigm. In J.  K. Luiselli,
Smith, T. (2013). What is evidence-based behavior anal- D.  C. Russo, W.  P. Christian, & S.  M. Wilczynski
ysis? The Behavior Analyst, 36, 7–33. https://doi. (Eds.), Effective practices for children with autism:
org/10.1007/BF03392290 Educational and behavior support interventions that
Sofis, M.  J., Jarmolowicz, D.  P., Hudnall, J.  L., & work (pp. 181–194). Oxford University Press.
Reed, D.  D. (2015). On sunk costs and escalation. Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles,
Psychological Record, 65, 487–494. https://doi. R.  S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A.  C., Chilingaryan,
org/10.1007/s40732-­015-­0124-­5 V., King, D. M., Niehoff, S. M., Slease, K., & Tia, B.
Slocum, T.A., Detrich, R., Wilczynski, S.M., Spencer, (2009). Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experi-
T.D., Lewis, T., & Wolfe, K. (2014). The evidence- mental functional assessments of challenging behav-
based practice of applied behavior analysis. The ior in children with autism. Journal of Developmental
Behavior Analyst, 37(1), 41–56. https://dx.doi.org/10 and Physical Disabilities, 21(6), 493. https://doi.
.1007%2Fs40614-014-0005-2 org/10.1007/s10882-­009-­9154-­8
Solomon, D., Pantalone, D. W., & Faja, S. (2019). Autism Tyson, J. (1995). Evidence-based ethics and the care
and adult sex education: A literature review using the of premature infants. The Future of Children, 5(1),
information–motivation–behavioral skills framework. 197–213. https://doi.org/10.2307/1602515
Sexuality and Disability, 37(3), 339–351. https://doi. Wang, C.  S., Leung, A.  K. Y., See, Y.  H. M., & Gao,
org/10.1007/s11195-­019-­09591-­6 X. Y. (2011). The effects of culture and friendship on
Spencer, T.  D., Detrich, R., & Slocum, T.  A. (2012). rewarding honesty and punishing deception. Journal
Evidence-based practice: A framework for mak- of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 1295–1299.
ing effective decisions. Education and Treatment of https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.04.011
Children, 35(2), 127–151. https://doi.org/10.1353/ Weiss, M.  J. (2014). Expanding ABA intervention in
etc.2012.0013 intensive programs for children with autism: The
Sundberg, M.  L. (2008). Verbal behavior milestones inclusion of natural environment training and flu-
assessment and placement program: A language and ency based instruction. The Behavior Analyst Today,
social skills assessment program for children with 2(3), 182–186. https://psycnet.apa.org/fulltext/2014-­
autism or other developmental disabilities. Guide, 44007-­005.pdf
AVB Press. Williams, B.  A. (1994). The role of prob-
Sundberg, M.  L., & Partington, J.  W. (1998). Teaching ability of reinforcement in models of choice.
language to children with autism or other develop- Psychological Review, 101, 704–707. https://doi.
mental disabilities. Behavior Analysts, Inc.. org/10.1037/0033-­295x.101.4.704
Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1999). The need for Wing, C. (1990). A preliminary investigation of general-
both discrete trial and natural environment language ization to untrained words following two treatments
training for children with autism. In P. M. Ghezzi, W. of children with word finding problems. Language,
L. Williams & J. E. Carr (Eds.) Autism: Behavior ana- Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 21, 151–
lytic perspectives (pp. 139–156). Context Press. 156. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-­1461.2103.151
Sundberg, M.  L., Loeb, M., Hale, L., & Eigenheer, P. Zentall, T.  R. (2016). Resolving the paradox of subop-
(2001). Contriving establishing operations to teach timal choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
mands for information. The Analysis of Verbal Animal Learning and Cognition, 42(1), 1–14. https://
doi.org/10.1037/xan0000085
Evidence-Based Practices
for Students with Autism 5
Spectrum Disorder
and the Individuals
with Disabilities Education
Improvement Act

Melissa L. Olive

5.1 Evidence-Based Practices Regulations Commentary which elaborates on


for Students with Autism the implementation of the IDEIA law (USDOE,
Spectrum Disorder 2006). In these regulations, peer-reviewed
and the Individuals research was defined and elaborated upon.
with Disabilities Education Specifically, “Peer-reviewed research generally
Improvement Act refers to research that is reviewed by qualified
and independent reviewers to ensure that the
In December of 2004, President George W. Bush quality of the information meets the standards of
signed into law the Individuals with Disabilities the field before the research is published” (IDEA,
in Education Act (Samuels, 2004). This reautho- 2006). However, the regulations went on to note
rization of the 1997 law added an extra word, that due to many definitions of peer-reviewed
improvement, in the title resulting in the research (PRR), a specific definition would not
Individuals with Disabilities Education be included.
Improvement Act (IDEIA, 2004). One of the Fast forward to 2008  in the No Child Left
improvements in the 2004 reauthorization Behind Act when the term scientifically based
included § 300.320 (4) “a statement of the special research (SBR) appeared. IDEIA Final
education and related services and supplemen- Regulations occurred in 2008, and while these
tary aids and services, based on peer-reviewed regulations referenced both PRR and SBR, no
research to the extent practicable, to be provided definitions were provided. Please see Zirkel and
to the child, or on behalf of the child” (IDEIA, Rose (2009) for a complete discussion of these
2004, p.  63). In 2006, the US Department of terms, including the term evidence-based prac-
Education published the Code of Federal tices (EBPs), as well as the definitions and use of
those terms within various education laws.
For the purposes of this chapter and based on
Dr. Olive is not an attorney and nothing in this chapter the work of Zirkel and colleagues (c.f., Zirkel &
should be misconstrued as legal advice.
Rose, 2009; Zirkel, 2013) and that of Yell et al.,
(2016a), I will use the term EBPs throughout this
M. L. Olive (*) chapter as defined by the Every Student Succeeds
Cultivate Behavioral Health & Education, New Act (ESSA, 2015). Note that ESSA replaced the
Haven, CT, USA No Child Left Behind Act. ESSA uses tiers to
e-mail: Missy.Olive@cultivatebhe.com

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 71


J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_5
72 M. L. Olive

establish evidence with the terms strong, moder- their families. The major difference between Part
ate, and promising. Strong evidence includes the C services and Part B services is that focus on the
use of at least one peer-reviewed study with ran- family. In fact, the family is such a focus that the
domized controlled trials. Moderate evidence document that is developed for very young chil-
includes at least one peer-reviewed study with a dren is called the Individualized Family Service
quasi-experimental design. Promising research Plan (IFSP). Eligibility criteria for Part C are also
includes at least one well-designed correlational very different than Part B as evaluators may use
study. For readers who appreciate single-subject their professional judgment to determine if a very
design and establishing evidence with single-­ young child requires services. The other differ-
subject research, please refer to the papers by ence is that the timelines are different because
Horner et al. (2005) and Odom and Strain (2002) very young children change quickly as they
for a thorough understanding of the rigor required develop. Specifically, very young children may
when designing single-subject research studies. begin receiving services immediately, even if the
Now that the main terms have been defined, IFSP is not finalized. Very young children must
the remaining portion of this chapter will be be evaluated every year, and their IFSP must be
organized by first providing an overview of reviewed every 6  months. Very young children
IDEIA law and its basic procedural requirements. begin transition services at least six months prior
This will be followed by a description of areas in to their third birthday. Services for very young
special education where evidence has already children must be offered in the natural environ-
been established. Case law regarding EBP will be ment which has been defined as settings that are
reviewed, and, finally, thoughts regarding ser- natural for very young children without disabili-
vices provided by behavior analysts practicing in ties (IDEIA, 2004). This may include home or
schools will be provided. community settings but could also include the
hospital for children born prematurely or with
other complications. Procedures for very young
5.2 Overview of IDEIA children, however, must occur in the same order
as the procedures under Part B.  For example,
The IDEIA is comprised of four parts: A, B, C, children should be identified and evaluated and
and D. Part A consists of definitions of terms and eligibility determined in that order. Finally, under
established the Office of Special Education Part C, children must receive services that meet
Programs. Part D has a list of national activities the definition of scientifically based research
to improve services for students. These activities (SBR; USDOE, 2011).
include training grants, research grants, and fund-
ing for technical assistance centers where EBP
may be employed through training and dissemi- 5.2.2 Part B
nation. Parts B and C will be described in greater
detail below. Part B of the IDEIA provides educational ser-
vices for students ages 3  years up through
21 years. In order to be eligible for services, stu-
5.2.1 Part C dents must meet the classification criteria for at
least one of 14 conditions defined by each state.
Part C of the IDEIA focuses on very young chil- These classifications include autism, develop-
dren ages birth through two years. Some also mental delay (up to age 9), and intellectual dis-
refer to it as services for birth up to three years of abilities. Multiple disabilities may be used when
age. Services in this age group have a stronger 2 or more classifications apply. It is important
emphasis on the family due to the unique needs that students with a medical diagnosis of ASD be
of serving very young children, as it is difficult to classified appropriately to ensure that their IEP is
serve very young children without also serving designed to meet their unique needs. However, in
5  Evidence-Based Practices for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder and the Individuals… 73

addition to having a disability classification, the case, the court ruled that schools must enable
student must also have a need for services in special education students to make meaningful
order to benefit from the education being pro- progress. This ruling overturned a standard that
vided. The major principles of IDEIA will be was previously referred to as de minimis benefit,
briefly discussed below. meaning that students only had to make minimal
progress. Prince et  al. (2018) summarized the
Endrew F case and identified implications for
5.2.3 Major Principles of IDEIA educators. Specifically, they noted that IEP team
members should take care to address all areas of
All services under IDEIA are based on Free need for eligible students. For example, children
Appropriate Public Education. Services must be with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may have
free for families and appropriately based on the grade level academic skills, but due to their diag-
Individualized Educational Program (IEP). nosis, they may have delays in social skills. IEP
Students may not be rejected from services if teams should address the social skills needs of
they are eligible; this is often referred to as zero the student, even though the student’s academic
reject. Services must be provided in the least skills are appropriate. Prince and colleagues also
restrictive environment (LRE) with the appropri- noted that IEP teams should develop ambitious,
ate supports and services required prior to place- measurable goals for both academic and func-
ment in a more restrictive educational setting. tional areas and that the services within the IEP
Students have a right to be identified and then be targeted to ensure improved performance in
evaluated to determine eligibility for services. those areas. Finally, they noted that IEP teams
Once eligibility has been determined, a team of should include strategies for monitoring student
individuals, including the parents, develop the progress and methods for reporting that progress
IEP.  Other principles of IDEIA include the to parents. Endrew F, when combined with the
requirement for confidentiality and technology-­ IDEIA requirement for students to receive ser-
related assistance. IDEIA includes detailed infor- vices based on EBP, ensures that students should
mation regarding discipline, but this will be be making meaningful progress year after year.
discussed in more detail later in the chapter.
Finally, both Part B and Part C of IDEIA include
procedural safeguards. These safeguards were 5.3 Areas of Established
designed to protect the rights of the students and Evidence Base
their parents. These rights include the right to be
notified timely of meetings; the right to attend the What do we currently know about EBP? The
meetings at a date, time, and location of mutual answer is that it is complicated. Readers are
agreement; and the right to examine all the edu- encouraged to read the entire second issue of
cational records for their child. If the parent dis- Volume 2 of Exceptional Children (2013) for a
agrees with the team, additional rights are thorough discussion of the issues around EBP in
afforded such as the right to an independent edu- Education. In particular, Cook and Odom (2013)
cational evaluation, mediation, and even due pro- discuss the issues facing special educators, and
cess. Readers who are interested in this topic in they note, “no practice will work for every single
more detail are encouraged to see Laviano and student” (p.  137). However, they also note that
Swanson (2017) for additional information. EBP must be combined with effective implemen-
tation. Measures of fidelity of implementation
will be needed to ensure that the EBPs are being
5.2.4 Supreme Court Case Endrew F implemented as developed (c.f., Sutherland et al.,
2019; Sutherland et  al., 2013; Yell & Rozalski,
In 2017, the US Supreme Court heard a case now 2013). Educators and related service providers
commonly referred to as Endrew F (2017). In this such as behavior analysts must attend to each stu-
74 M. L. Olive

dent’s IEP and ensure that meaningful, ambitious to just under 4 years when children over 10 years
goals are written and that services based on EBP are excluded from the analysis (van’t Hof et al.,
are provided (Sayeski et al., 2019). When a stu- 2021).
dent fails to make progress, the team should first A second area of focus is on early identifica-
assess the fidelity of implementation (Sam et al., tion of students once they begin services at
2021). If the intervention is being implemented school, if they were not diagnosed prior to school.
as planned, then the team should modify the EBP Zirkel (2017) notes that educators must take
used until the student begins making meaningful action when they have reasonable suspicion that
progress. the child may be eligible for IDEIA services.
Yell and Rozalski (2013) identified tips for Educators may not look the other way when the
educators during the IEP process. First, educators red flags of ASD appear in students in their class-
should remain current in research related to aca- rooms. In fact, once an educator has reasonable
demic and behavioral interventions. Second, dur- suspicion, then the evaluation must be initiated
ing the IEP, educators should be prepared to within a reasonable amount of time (Zirkel,
discuss EBPs and the science behind their pro- 2017). Note that individual states may have defi-
posed instructional strategies. Third, if parents nitions or timelines specified for the evaluation
propose EBPs during the meeting, the educators process. For example, in Texas, the evaluation
should acknowledge and discuss the practices, must be completed within 60  days of obtaining
including being able to refute any research or to written parent consent (Texas Education Agency,
describe a lack of research on the practice. 2017).

5.3.1 Child Find 5.3.2 Assessment and Evaluation

One area validated by research for many years is Once the student has been identified and referred
the need for early identification and early inter- for evaluation, educational teams need to ensure
vention for children with disabilities. For exam- that the initial evaluation is individualized and
ple, it is widely known that early intervention for assesses every area of need (IDEA, 2014).
children with behavior disorders (Conroy & Because the assessment drives the development
Brown, 2004) and children with learning disabili- of the IEP, it is essential for teams to have a full
ties (Lange & Thompson, 2006) will result in a understanding of each student’s unique strengths
decrease of challenges associated with the dis- and needs.
ability. This is especially true for young children In addition to the initial and full evaluation
with autism (e.g., Filipek et  al., 2000; Koegel required by law, IEP teams should ensure that
et al., 2014; Shaw et al., 2020). they complete appropriate evidence-based assess-
Two areas of focus are needed to ensure that ments. Arguably, one of the most important
children with ASD are identified as early as pos- assessments for children with ASD is the func-
sible. First, we must have coordinated tional behavior assessment (FBA). An FBA is
community-­based developmental monitoring to needed prior to antecedent-based interventions,
ensure that very young children are screened, prior to implementation of interventions to
identified, and evaluated for ASD (Barger et al., address challenging behavior, and prior to the
2018). For example, agencies such as Autism implementation of Functional Communication
Speaks have been sharing the early warning signs Training (FCT). Additional information r­ egarding
through commercials and websites. Pediatricians evidence-based approaches to the FBA will be
will continue to need training to recognize the red discussed later in this chapter.
flags of ASD during well-checks because the Preference assessment is also an established
average age of an ASD diagnosis is just over EBP (Chazin & Ledford, 2016). Preference
4 years (Lord et al., 2006). This number decreased assessments consist of direct observations and or
5  Evidence-Based Practices for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder and the Individuals… 75

trial-by-trial measures of a student’s preferences. lications is an identification of the practices that


Many types of preference assessments exist, but appear to be deemed as EBPs by more than one
they most commonly result in a hierarchy of pref- publication (e.g., Chazin & Ledford, 2016;
erences for the student. These include multiple National Autism Center, 2015; Steinbrenner
stimulus with (MSW) and without replacement et  al., 2020). These include antecedent-based
(MSWO; DeLeon & Iwata, 1996), paired stimu- interventions, augmentative and alternative com-
lus (Fisher et  al., 1992), single stimulus (Pace munication, behavioral momentum, cognitive
et  al., 1985), free operant (Roane et  al., 1998), behavior therapy, direct instruction, and various
and in the moment reinforcer analysis (Leaf strategies of applied behavior analysis (ABA)
et al., 2015). such as chaining, differential reinforcement, dis-
Another assessment that is needed to ensure crete trial training, extinction, FBA, FCT, model-
that EBPs may be implemented effectively is the ing and video modeling, prompting,
Autism Program Environment Assessment reinforcement, response interruption and redirec-
Rating Scale (Odom et al., 2018). In this assess- tion, self-management, task analysis, and time
ment, the environment is first evaluated to deter- delay.
mine the overall quality of the classroom.
Classroom modifications may be necessary first
before EBPs are implemented effectively. 5.3.4 Functional Behavior
Ongoing assessment and evaluation of prog- Assessments, Behavior
ress is likely the most important assessment that Intervention Plans,
should be done for students. Each and every and Positive Behavior
domain in the IEP should be monitored for prog- Interventions and Supports
ress (Yell et  al., 2016b). Moreover, the assess-
ment should include a measurement of baseline An area that has been well-established in EBP is
performance (Yell et  al., 2016a). This requires within assessment and treatment of challenging
that the goal be written as a measurable goal, behaviors. In the 1997 reauthorization of the
measured, and then monitored (Bateman & IDEA  (IDEA, 1997), educators first learned of
Linden, 2012). The IEP team should identify who requirements regarding FBA.  This was only
will be responsible for progress monitoring for 3 years after the Iwata et al. (1994) seminal arti-
each component of the IEP to determine if prog- cle on functional analysis was reprinted follow-
ress is being made (Etscheidt, 2006; Sayeski ing its first publication in 1982. As policy
et al., 2019). Finally, changes are made based on requirements moved faster than research to prac-
student progress or the lack thereof (Yell et  al., tice, educators scrambled to better understand the
2016a). science behind FBAs and positive behavior inter-
ventions and supports (PBIS). This resulted in a
number of legal cases regarding appropriate
5.3.3 Intervention assessment and intervention. Specifically, in
2001, Drasgow and Yell summarized 14 case
A number of formal reports on EBPs for inter- decisions regarding functional behavior assess-
vention have been published by a variety of agen- ment. In 13 of those cases, the hearing officer
cies and authors. A comprehensive review of ruled in favor of the parents. In 11 of those cases,
those publications is beyond the scope of this school districts simply failed to conduct an FBA
chapter. Each review has strengths and limita- when it was required by IDEA. Drasgow and Yell
tions. For example, one of the most commonly went on to note that school teams should initiate
cited EBP report is that of Steinbrenner et  al. an FBA at the first sign of serious problem
(2020). However, Leaf et al. (2021) published a behavior.
paper summarizing their concerns and critiques In 2020, Zirkel reported on a review of 46
of that report. What we can glean from these pub- cases regarding FBAs and BIPs. Zirkel
76 M. L. Olive

described a case wherein the school developed 5.4 Behavior Analysts in Schools
a BIP outside of the IEP process resulting in the
parent being unable to participate. The court The field of behavior analysis has seen an explo-
sided with the family due to this procedural sive increase of board-certified behavior analysts
error. Zirkel also described a case wherein the (BCBA) in recent years (BACB, 2021). This has
district failed to collect high-quality data as resulted in a concomitant increase in behavior
part of the FBA. The court ruled against the dis- analysts practicing in public schools (McMahon
trict in this example as well. In summarizing, et  al., 2021). For example, the Pennsylvania
Zirkel noted that educators should go beyond Department of Education (2018) created a docu-
the minimum legal requirement and instead ment on the role of behavior analysts in public
lean on professional norms of EBPs. Finally, education. More specifically, between 2012 and
Zirkel noted that educators should not just com- 2014, 28% of the jobs for BCBAs specifically fell
plete FBAs and BIPs to comply with legal stan- in education (Burning Glass Technologies, 2015).
dards but rather that by completing quality Because the demand for behavior analysts in
FBAs and BIPs that students would benefit schools is a recent trend, additional commentary
from effective intervention services. is needed to fully understand the application of
Losinski et  al. (2014) outlined the mini- EBPs and IDEIA Law.
mum requirements of an FBA. First, the evalu- The Council of Autism Service Providers
ator should include a clear description of the (CASP) produced a document called Applied
behavior. Second, they should identify the Behavior Analysis Treatment of Autism Spectrum
antecedent and consequences that surround Disorder: Practice Guidelines for Healthcare
the challenging behavior. Next, a hypothesis Funders and Managers (CASP, 2020; hereafter
or hypotheses should be developed to describe CASP ASD Guidelines). This document
why the challenging behavior is occurring. describes tiered intervention services as well as
Finally, data should be included in the FBA to direct and indirect services. The document also
support the hypothesis statement. Drasgow discusses focused and comprehensive interven-
and Yell (2001) recommended procedural tion. Of utmost importance is the discussion of
steps of the FBA that should be completed. caseloads for behavior analysts. In the CASP
This included interviews of teachers, parents, ASD Guidelines, it is recommended that BCBAs
and others, multiple direct observations of the supervising comprehensive caseloads without
student, and experimental manipulation of assistance of an assistant behavior analyst should
variables, if necessary. have between 6 and 12 students, while the casel-
Once a quality FBA is completed, the oad for focused students without the help of an
FBA is then used to develop an appropri- assistant would include 10–15 students.
ate BIP.  At a minimum, the BIP should One limitation of the CASP ASD Guidelines
include antecedent strategies for prevent- is that they were not written to address services in
ing challenging behavior, a plan to teach the schools. Another limitation is that the title of
replacement behaviors, and a plan for the document specifically states that the guide-
reinforcing replacement behaviors and lines are for healthcare funders. Therefore, some
responding to challenging behaviors argue that those guidelines are not appropriate for
(Hirsch et al., 2017). Effective EBPs to be school-based services. Finally, the document is
included in the BIP may consist of ante- specifically called “guidelines” which means
cedent-based interventions, behavioral they are merely recommendations and not neces-
momentum/high probability response sarily enforced.
sequences, FCT, non-contingent rein- On the other hand, on the ABA Ethics Hotline,
forcement, and reinforcement (Chazin & Syed (n.d.) has described appropriate caseloads
Ledford, 2016; National Autism Center, for behavior analysts practicing in schools. While
2015; Steinbrenner et al., 2020). the ABA Ethics Hotline is also not enforceable, it
5  Evidence-Based Practices for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder and the Individuals… 77

is led by Dr. Jon Bailey, a leading expert on ethi- considered EBPs. Universities need to continue
cal issues for behavior analysts. Syed does not to train teachers and paraeducators to fidelity in
provide a specific headcount of students for the implementation of EBPs. Schools need to
behavior analysts in schools. Instead, Syed rec- ensure that teachers are implementing EBPs
ommends that a percentage of supervision hours and ensure that teachers are engaging in con-
be provided based on the total hours of ABA ser- tinuing education, so teachers stay abreast of
vices the student receives. Therefore, it is up to new EBPs as they emerge in research. BCBAs
the individual behavior analyst to determine if need to maintain appropriate caseloads to
they have the time to take on new case assign- ensure they are delivering EBP to their stu-
ments based on existing case load. It is also up to dents. Parents need to demand that their chil-
the individual behavior analysts to self-advocate dren receive EBPs, and they should see weekly
when their caseloads are too high. The only way graphs of their child’s progress toward annual
students with ASD will obtain EBPs in schools is goals. Those goals need to be lofty, consistent
to ensure that behavior analysts have caseloads with Endrew F. When students with ASD have
that allow them to provide appropriate quantity access to EBPs, they are more likely to make
and quality of services. progress resulting in them being active partici-
The other area of importance for behavior pants in the communities where they live and
analysts practicing in schools is the BACB Code work.
of Ethics for Behavior Analysts (BACB, 2020).
So as not repeat what is covered in other chapters
in this book in more detail (e.g., Chapter 4), only References
a brief summary is provided here. Readers are
encouraged to review the information with more Barger, B., Rice, C., Simmons, C. A., & Wolf, R. (2018). A
systematic review of part C early identification studies.
detail in the chapter on Ethical Considerations Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 38(1),
and Dignity for Adults with ASD. Under #2.01, 4–16. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121416678664
#2.13, and #2.14, BCBAs must provide assess- Bateman, B.  D., & Linden, M.  A. (2012). Better IEPs:
ment and intervention services that are effective How to develop legally correct and educationally use-
ful programs. Attainment Company.
and conceptually consistent with behavior analy- Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020). Ethics code
sis and based on scientific evidence. Practicing in for behavior analysts. Author.
schools must be in the BCBA’s scope of compe- Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2021). US employ-
tence (#1.05). The IFSP and the IEP serve as the ment demand for behavior analysts: 2010–2020.
Author.
contract for behavior analytic services (#3.04). Burning Glass Technologies. (2015). US behavior analyst
As with adults, BCBAs should rule out underly- workforce: Understanding the national demand for
ing medical conditions (#2.12). Consistent with behavior analysts. Author.
IDEIA law, BCBAs should include parents and Chazin, K.  T., & Ledford, J.  R. (2016). An overview of
evidence-based instructional practices (EBIPs). In
students throughout the assessment and interven- Evidence-based instructional practices for young
tion planning (#2.09), and they should keep all children with autism and other disabilities. Retrieved
client information confidential (#2.03 and #2.04). from http://ebip.vkcsites.org/
Also consistent with IDEIA law, BCBAs should Conroy, M.  A., & Brown, W.  H. (2004). Early iden-
tification, prevention, and early intervention with
collect and graph data regularly (#2.17). Finally, young children at risk for emotional or behav-
under #2.18, BCBAs continually evaluate the ioral disorders: Issues, trends, and a call for action.
effectiveness of their interventions, and they Behavioral Disorders, 29(3), 224–236. https://doi.
make changes to those interventions based on org/10.1177/019874290402900303
Cook, B. G., & Odom, S. L. (2013). Evidence-based prac-
data to ensure the student makes progress. tices and implementation science in special education.
Making adequate progress is consistent with Exceptional Children, 79(2), 135–144.
Endrew F requirements as well. Council of Autism Service Providers. (2020). Applied
In summary, students with ASD under behavior analysis treatment of autism spectrum dis-
order: Practice guidelines for healthcare funders
21 years of age have a right to services that are
78 M. L. Olive

and managers. https://casproviders.org/wp-­content/ Laviano, J., & Swanson, J. (2017). Your special educa-
uploads/2020/03/ABA-­ASD-­Practice-­Guidelines.pdf tion rights: What your school district isn’t telling you.
DeLeon, I.  G., & Iwata, B.  A. (1996). Evaluation of a Skyhorse Publishing.
multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing Leaf, J. B., Leaf, R., Alacalay, A., Leaf, J. A., Ravid, D.,
reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Dale, S., Kassardjian, A., Tsuji, K., Taubman, M.,
Analysis, 29(4), 519–533. https://doi.org/10.1901/ McEachin, J., & Oppenheim-Leaf, M.  L. (2015).
jaba.1996.29-­519 Utility of formal preference assessments for indi-
Drasgow, E., & Yell, M.  L. (2001). Functional behav- viduals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.
ioral assessments: Legal requirements and challenges. Education and Training in Autism and Developmental
School Psychology Review, 30(2), 239–251. Disabilities, 50(2), 199–212.
Endrew F.  V. (2017). Douglas County School District Leaf, J. B., Sato, S. K., Javed, A., Arthur, S. M., Creem,
RE-1, 137 S. Ct. 988. A.  N., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., & Oppenheim-­
Etscheidt, S.  K. (2006). Progress monitoring: Legal Leaf, M. L. (2021). The evidence-based practices for
issues and recommendations for IEP teams. Teaching children, youth, and young adults with autism report:
Exceptional Children, 38(3), 56–60. Concerns and critiques. Behavioral Interventions,
Every Student Succeeds Act, 20 U.S.C. § 6301 (2015). 36(2), 457–472. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1771
Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek, G. T., Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P. S., Shulman, C., Thurm,
Cook, E. H., Jr., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., Gravel, J. S., A., & Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from 2 to 9 years of
Johnson, C.  P., Kallen, R.  J., Levy, S.  E., Minshew, age. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 694–701.
N.  J., Ozonoff, S., Prizant, B.  M., Rapin, I., Rogers, Losinski, M.  L., Katsiyannis, A., & Ryan, J.  B. (2014).
S. J., Stone, W. L., Teplin, S. W., Tuchman, R. F., & Recent case law regarding functional behavioral
Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Practice parameter: Screening assessments: Implications for practice. Intervention in
and diagnosis of autism. Neurology, 55(4), 68–79. School and Clinic, 49(4), 251–254.
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, McMahon, M.  X. H., Feldberg, Z.  R., & Ardoin, S.  P.
L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison (2021). Behavior analysis goes to school: Teacher
of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for per- acceptability of behavior-analytic language in behav-
sons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of ioral consultation. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 14,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. 131–140.
Hirsch, S. E., Bruhn, A. L., Lloyd, J. W., & Katsiyannis, National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu-
A. (2017). FBAs and BIPs: Avoiding and addressing sions: National standards project, phase 2. Author.
four common challenges related to fidelity. Teaching http://www.autismdiagnostics.com/assets/Resources/
Exceptional Children, 49(6), 369–379. NSP2.pdf
Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, No Child Left Behind Act, 20 U.S.C. § 11 6301 et. seq.
S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject (2008).
research to identify evidence-based practice in special Odom, S. L., & Strain, P. S. (2002). Evidence-based prac-
education. Exceptional Children, 71, 165–179. tice in Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special
IDEA Final Regulations, 34 C.F.R. Part 300 (2008). Education: Single-subject design research. Journal of
IDEA Final Regulations Commentary, 71 Fed. Reg. Early Intervention, 25, 151–160.
46540–46753 (August 14, 2006). Odom, S. L., Cox, A., Sideris, J., Hume, K. A., Hedges, S.,
IDEA regulations, 34 C.F.R. §§ 300.301 (2014). Kucharczyk, S., et al. (2018). Assessing quality of pro-
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Amendments gram environments for children and youth with autism:
of 1997, 20 U.S.C. ξ 1400 et seq. Autism Program Environment Rating Scale. Journal
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act, of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(3), 913–
20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq. (2004). 924. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­017-­3379-­7
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, B. A.,
Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis & Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stimulus prefer-
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, ence and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded
27, 197–209. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27- individuals. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18,
­197. (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in 249–255.
Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). Pennsylvania Department of Education. (2018).
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Ashbaugh, K., & Bradshaw, Board certified behavior analysts and pub-
J. (2014). The importance of early identification and lic education: Rationale and guidelines. Author.
intervention for children with or at risk for autism Retrieved on 11/22/21 from https://www.pattan.
spectrum disorders. International Journal of Speech-­ net/assets/PaTTAN/39/39d62345-­a e5f-­4 888-­9 a42-­
Language Pathology, 16(1), 50–56. https://doi.org/10 2d368ce60eb3.pdf
.3109/17549507.2013.861511 Prince, A.  M. T., Yell, M.  L., & Katsiyannis, A. (2018).
Lange, S. M., & Thompson, B. (2006). Early identification Endrew F v Douglas county school district (2017): The
and interventions for children at risk for learning dis- US Supreme Court and special education. Intervention
abilities. International Journal of Special Education, in School and Clinic, 53(5), 321–324.
21(3), 108–119.
5  Evidence-Based Practices for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder and the Individuals… 79

Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, Syed, N. (n.d.). Focus on…ethical caseloads and practice
B.  A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus pref- in schools. ABA Ethics Hotline. Retrieved November
erence assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior 21, 2021 from https://www.abaethicshotline.com/
Analysis, 31(4), 605–620. https://doi.org/10.1901/ ethical-­caseloads-­and-­practice-­in-­schools/
jaba.1998.31-­605 Texas Education Agency. (2017). Special education
Sam, A.  M., Odom, S.  L., Tomaszewski, B., Perkins, rules and regulations: Individuals with disabili-
Y., & Cox, A.  W. (2021). Employing evidence-­ ties education act, state board of education rules,
based practices for children with autism in elemen- Commissioner’s rules, Texas state laws. Author.
tary schools. Journal of Autism and Developmental U.S.  Department of Education, 71 Fed. Reg. 46,665
Disorders, 51, 2308–2323. https://doi.org/10.1007/ (2006).
s10803-­020-­04706-­x U.S.  Department of Education, 76 Fed. Reg. 60,140
Samuels, C. A. (2004, December 3). President signs reau- (2011).
thorized IDEA. Education Week. Retrieved 11/21/21 van’t Hof, M., Tisseur, C., van Berckelear-Onnes, I., van
from https://www.edweek.org/teaching-­learning/ Nieuwenhuyzen, A., Daniels, A. M., Deen, M., Hoek,
president-­signs-­reauthorized-­idea/2004/12 H. W., & Ester, W. A. (2021). Age at autism spectrum
Sayeski, K.  L., Bateman, D.  F., & Yell, M.  L. (2019). disorder diagnosis: A systematic review and meta-­
Re-envisioning teacher preparation in an era of analysis from 2012 to 2019. Autism, 25(4), 862–873.
Endrew F: Instruction over access. Intervention in https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320971107
School and Clinic, 54(5), 264–271. Yell, M. L., & Rozalski, M. E. (2013). The peer reviewed
Shaw, K.  A., Maenner, M.  J., Baio, J., Washington, J., research requirement of the IDEA: An examination
Christensen, D.  L., Wiggins, L.  D., Pettygrove, S., of law and policy. In B. Cook, M. Tankersly, & T. E.
Andrews, J.  G., White, T., Rosenberg, C.  R. R., Landrum (Eds.), Advances in learning and behavioral
Constantino, J.  N., Fitzgerald, R.  T., Zahorodny, W., disabilities: Evidence-based practices (pp. 149–172).
Shenouda, J., Daniels, J. L., Salinas, A., Durkin, M. S., Emerald Press.
& Dietz, P. M. (2020). Early identification of autism Yell, M.  L., Katsiyannis, A., Losinski, M., & Marshall,
spectrum disorder among children aged 4 years: K. (2016a). Peer-reviewed research and the IEP:
Early autism and developmental disabilities monitor- Implications of Ridley School District v M.  R. and
ing network, six sites, United States, 2016. MMWR J. R. ex rel. E. R. (2012). Intervention in School and
Surveillance Summary, 69(No. SS-3), 1–11. https:// Clinic, 51(4), 253–257.
doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6903a1 Yell, M.  L., Katsiyannis, A., Ennis, R.  P., Losinski,
Steinbrenner, J. R., Hume, K., Odom, S. L., Morin, K. L., M., & Christle, C. A. (2016b). Avoiding substan-
Nowell, S.  W., Tomaszewski, B., … Savage, M.  N. tive errors in individualized education program
(2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth, development. Teaching Exceptional Children,
and young adults with autism. The University of North 49(1), 31–40. Retrieved from https://doi.
Carolina, Frank Porter Graham Child Development org/10.1177/0040059916662204
Institute, National Clearinghouse on Autism Evidence Zirkel, P. A. (2013). Recent legal developments of inter-
and Practice Review Team. est to special educators. Intervention in School and
Sutherland, K. S., McLeod, B. D., Conroy, M. A., & Cox, Clinic, 48(5), 319–322.
J. R. (2013). Measuring implementation of evidence-­ Zirkel, P. A. (2017). Child find under the IDEA: An empir-
based programs targeting young children at risk for ical analysis of the judicial case law. Communiqué,
emotional/behavioral disorders: Conceptual issues 45(7), 4–6.
and recommendations. Journal of Early Intervention, Zirkel, P. A. (2020). FBAs and BIPs: An updated case law
35, 129–149. analysis. Communiqué, 49(3), 20–21.
Sutherland, K. S., Conroy, M. A., Mcleod, B. D., Granger, Zirkel, P.  A., & Rose, T. (2009). Scientifically based
K., Nemer, S.  L., Kunemund, R.  L., Johnson, A., & research and peer reviewed research under the IDEA.
Miles, C. (2019). Adapting an evidence-based early Journal of Special Education Leadership, 22(1),
childhood tier 2 program for early elementary school. 36–50.
The Elementary School Journal, 119(4), 542–561.
Evidence-Based Practice in Schools
6
Ilene S. Schwartz, Alice Bravo, Robin Finlayson,
Jessica Flaherty, and Adriana Luna

6.1 Evidence-Based Practice and 1970s and is supported by many advocacy


in Schools groups, governmental agencies, and journalists.
Many in the ASD community advocate for the
In 1975, President Gerald Ford signed PL 94-142 use of “identity-first” language, that is, using the
into law, now referred to as the Individuals with terms “autistic,” “autistic person,” or “autistic
Disabilities Education Act (IDEA, 2004), chang- individual,” rather than the person-first usage of a
ing the lives of people with disabilities in the person with ASD. Identity-first language is
United States forever. This law entitles all stu- aligned with the identity model of disability,
dents with disabilities to a free appropriate public which asserts that disability is a typical human
education (FAPE). Although IDEA has never experience and an intrinsic part of one’s identity.
been fully funded or implemented, the promise of Advocates for identity-first language see ASD as
IDEA is alive and well in public schools across an inherent part of an individual’s identity—the
the country. In this chapter, we will review the same way one refers to “African-Americans,”
use of evidence-based practice and the compo- “lesbian/gay/bisexual/transgender/queer,”
nents of IDEA and discuss how the context of “gifted,” “athletic,” or “Jewish”—and suggest
public schools impacts the delivery of services that identity-first language celebrates the neuro-
and the practice of applied behavior analysis and diversity of all members of society. As non-­
the development and implementation of IEPs for disabled people, we believe that we need to use
students with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). person-first language until we are invited by indi-
Before we discuss evidence-based practice, viduals with disabilities to use identity-first lan-
we want to discuss person-first language, which guage. Once an individual with a disability
we will use throughout this chapter. Person-first expresses a preference about the type of language
language, the practice of identifying a person they prefer, we honor that request. In general,
before their disability, was an outgrowth of the however, and in this chapter, we continue to use
disability rights political movement of the 1960s person-first language.

I. S. Schwartz (*)
Haring Center, University of Washington, Seattle, 6.1.1 Evidence-Based Practice
WA, USA
e-mail: ilene@uw.edu Evidence-based practice (EBP) plays an impor-
A. Bravo · R. Finlayson · J. Flaherty · A. Luna tant role in the services for students with ASD in
College of Education, University of Washington, public schools and behavior analysts working in
Seattle, WA, USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 81


J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_6
82 I. S. Schwartz et al.

schools. The use of EBP is not only mandated by ing which interventions have enough research
IDEA; it is also mandated by the Ethics Code for evidence to suggest that they are most likely to be
Behavior Analysts (BACB, 2020). Sackett et al. effective, with most of the students, most of the
(1996) defined the practice of evidence-based time. They do not, and cannot, determine a priori
medicine as “the conscientious, explicit, and that a specific EBP will be an effective interven-
judicious use of current best evidence in making tion for a specific individual at a specific time to
decisions about the care of individual patients” teach a specific skill. Therefore, EBP should be
(p. 71). This practice involves the integration of considered as a starting point for intervention.
individual practitioner expertise with the best Once an educational team decides to implement
available evidence from systematic research. an EBP, the team needs to evaluate and document
In the past two decades, many definitions of the effects of treatment. If an EBP is implemented
EBP have emerged, and although there are differ- and the anticipated change in behavior is docu-
ences in the definitions across professional disci- mented, fantastic; continue with the treatment
plines, there is general agreement that EBPs are and progress monitoring. A behavior analyst may
an attempt to use replicated, quality research out- then add this example to their professional expe-
comes to make decisions about what intervention rience about the effectiveness of the practice. If,
strategies should be used with students (e.g., however, an EBP is implemented with fidelity
Slocum et  al., 2014; Smith, 2013; Steinbrenner and does not result in a positive change in behav-
et  al., 2020). Most professional organizations ior, it is incumbent upon the behavior analyst
include practitioner experience, client prefer- and/or the educational team to change the inter-
ences, client characteristics, and client values vention. The change may be in regard to the
into the rubric that is used when identifying and intensity of the intervention, the type of rein-
evaluating EBP. As behavior analysts, we would forcer used, or perhaps an entirely different inter-
like to add another characteristic to the defini- vention strategy altogether. The important lesson
tion—consideration of the context in which the is that an instructional strategy cannot be said to
intervention is implemented and the effectiveness be evidence-based for an individual until we have
of the intervention within that setting (Fig. 6.1). collected data demonstrating its effectiveness in
By definition, EBPs are actuarial. They are this specific situation, with this specific behavior,
developed by compiling research and determin- and in this specific context. To be accountable to
our students, we need to conduct these treatment
evaluations, one learner at a time, one behavior at
a time. For behavior analysts working in schools,
the practice of frequent data collection and analy-
sis can help educational teams implement and
document the effectiveness of EBPs selected for
Research Clinical
individual students.
Evidence Experience

6.1.2 Individuals with Disabilities


Education Act (IDEA)
Family
Contextual
Culture,
Fit and Public education is the one great service to which
Value, and
Effectiveness all children and youth in the United States are
Preferences
entitled. Public education is charged with provid-
ing access to education and promoting equal
opportunities for all students while preparing
individuals for civic participation and post-school
Fig. 6.1  Conceptualization of evidence-based practice life (Kober, 2007). Public education provides
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 83

opportunities for students to access dent learning and social engagement within the
evidence-­
­ based instruction and intervention same instructional frameworks and settings
regardless of race, ethnicity, socioeconomic sta- designed for the whole school community”
tus, gender, or disability status. Until 1975, how- (p. 11). Access to effective instruction and access
ever, almost 1.8 million children with disabilities to the general education classroom are not syn-
were excluded from public schools (Duncan, onymous; one can sit in the general education
2015). Many of these students were placed in classroom, experience isolation, and not receive
segregated learning settings, residential centers, the specially designed instruction to which they
or educated at home. IDEA is a comprehensive are entitled. Learners should not need to earn
federal law comprising six major components, their entry into general education. All learners are
each of which are outlined below. general education students first, with special edu-
cation serving a function of supporting access to
6.1.2.1 Free Appropriate Public learning and to success with the general educa-
Education (FAPE) tion curriculum. Student placement and partici-
The foundation of IDEA is the entitlement of all pation in general education should be based on
students with disabilities to a free appropriate meeting the student’s individual needs for opti-
public education (FAPE) that is designed to meet mal growth and meaningful development. This
student’s unique needs, support them to access highlights the importance of using high-leverage
the general education curriculum and environ- EBPs within classrooms and schools to create
ment, and prepare them for further education, systems designed for the whole school
employment, and independent living (Yell et al., community.
2020). The primary measure for providing a stu-
dent with a FAPE is through an Individualized 6.1.2.3 Appropriate Evaluation
Education Program (IEP) that establishes a stu- Under IDEA, every student suspected of having a
dent’s present levels of achievement and perfor- disability has the right to receive an appropriate
mance, the impact of the student’s disability on evaluation via an Evaluation Team Report (ETR)
participation and learning, and the student’s in order to establish the presence of a qualifying
progress within the general education curricu- disability, or disabilities, for special education
lum. A FAPE mandates that IEP goals be aligned services. The evaluation must:
with grade-level content standards.
• Evaluate all areas of suspected disability
6.1.2.2 Least Restrictive Environment • Be completed by a team trained in the use of
(LRE) the relevant and selected assessment
IDEA mandates that students with disabilities measures
access a FAPE in the least restrictive environ- • Employ evaluation materials that are neither
ment (LRE), meaning that all students should racially nor culturally discriminatory
participate and receive instruction within inclu- • Avoid subjecting a student to unnecessary
sive, general education classrooms alongside tests and assessments
typically developing peers to the greatest extent • Include the gathering of relevant information
possible. For students with disabilities, this from a variety of sources
includes opportunities to participate and succeed
in learning communities with their typically The written ETR must include a summary of
developing peers and to engage in meaningful information obtained during the evaluation pro-
learning in inclusive settings where all students’ cess; the names, titles, and signatures of each
identities are welcomed, affirmed, and supported. team member (including the parent); and an indi-
The National Council on Disability (2018) cation of whether they are in agreement with the
defines inclusion as “…not a place, but rather a eligibility determination. The school district must
systemic approach to uniquely addressing stu- provide a copy of the ETR and the documenta-
84 I. S. Schwartz et al.

tion of eligibility or continued eligibility to the Table 6.1  Components of an IEP


parents prior to the IEP meeting and no later than Student’s The IEP specifies the results of the
14 days from the date of eligibility present level of initial or most recent evaluation
performance and the academic, developmental,
determination. and functional needs of the student.
An evaluation will determine whether a stu- It should state the student’s current
dent receives an educational classification within level of performance, student’s
1 of the 13 disability categories: specific learning strengths, and caregiver concerns
for enhancing the student’s
disability, other health impairment, ASD, emo- education
tional disturbance, speech or language impair- Ambitious and Short- and long-term goals are
ment, visual impairment (including blindness), measurable operationally defined, contain
deafness, hearing impairment, deaf-blindness, goals appropriate objective criteria,
orthopedic impairment, intellectual disability, address individualized student
needs, address access to
traumatic brain injury, or multiple disabilities appropriately ambitious education
(IDEA, 2004). Educational classifications for a plans, and specify criterion for
disability are different from medical diagnoses, change
as they are solely used to determine eligibility for Functional If warranted, an IEP will include an
behavior FBA and a BIP. The school district
special education services and are unrelated to assessment and is tasked with assigning a
services received outside of public school set- behavior practitioner with behavioral
tings. If a student arrives at school with a medical intervention expertise to conduct the FBA (e.g.,
diagnosis of ASD, they will still need to partici- plan school psychologist, behavior
specialist/BCBA, special educator).
pate in the school evaluation process to qualify BIPs specify intervention agents,
for special education services. measurement systems, and criterion
for change
6.1.2.4 Individualized Education Description of The IEP defines individualized
Program (IEP) special instruction, supports, and services
education and required for the student to benefit
Once the ETR is complete, the student, family, related services from their public education. It
and educational team participate in a collabora- defines services provided, team
tive process to develop an IEP. The IEP is a docu- members responsible for IEP
ment that describes a student’s strengths and implementation, and the time and
place instruction will be delivered
needs and the specially designed instruction, Coordination of Providers should work as an
accommodations, modifications, and special ser- care interdisciplinary team to
vices (e.g., services from a Board Certified collaborate on creation,
Behavior Analyst [BCBA] or speech-language implementation, and assessment of
student goals. These teams may
pathologist [SLP]) a student needs to optimize include, but are not limited to,
their education and learning. See Table 6.1 for a occupational therapists, SLPs,
description of the required components of an BCBAs, special education
IEP. A collaborative team is developed to identify coordinators, general education
teachers, social work services, etc.
the services and supports that will allow the stu-
Teacher/staff If warranted, the IEP may include a
dent to best participate in the school environ- training plan for additional teacher and staff
ment. The team should consist of the student’s training
caregivers; at least one general educator; at least Placement The IEP specifies the extent to
one special educator; a representative of the which the student will participate
in the general education classroom,
school system who is knowledgeable about spe-
and if it is less than 100% of the
cialized instruction, the general education cur- time, an explanation is required
riculum, and the availability of school resources; (continued)
an individual who may interpret evaluation
results (e.g., school psychologist); the student
themselves, as appropriate; and other individuals
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 85

Table 6.1 (continued) by the US Supreme Court, significantly impacts


Transition plan Once a student turns 16, the IEP how IEPs are planned, written, and evaluated for
includes a transition plan that students with disabilities and especially students
details the transition out of the
public education system and into
with ASD. In this decision, a unanimous supreme
the next stage of their life court ruled that IDEA must provide students with
Plan for The IEP specifies the date services an education program that is “reasonably calcu-
continuous are to begin and states the lated” for the student to make progress on their
evaluation of anticipated frequency, location, and individualized goals (Yell & Bateman, 2019).
IEP duration of services. This includes
a plan to meet yearly, or more The family at the center of this case argued that
frequently if needed, to their son, “Drew,” should be making more prog-
continuously evaluate progress and ress than he was on his IEP goals. The court
make necessary changes agreed, stating that a student’s IEP should be
Caregiver The IEP must be agreed to and
approval signed by the student’s caregiver.
made “appropriately ambitious.” This standard
Caregivers may request an IEP aims to be individualized to each student entitled
team meeting at any time and to an IEP and extends beyond meeting the bare
reserve the right to request minimum requirements, thus making the Endrew
amendments to the proposed plan
decision a landmark case in the field of special
education.
with knowledge of the student as desired by the
caregivers and/or school, such as related service 6.1.2.5 Parent and Student
providers (Yell et  al., 2020). Interestingly, Participation
­students are required to participate in their own IDEA (2004) mandates that parents have the
IEP planning process once they turn 14 years of right to participate in decisions about their child’s
age, but including them earlier is an opportunity education and the IEP process. The importance
to teach self-advocacy and listen to advocates of parental participation has been emphasized in
with ASD who suggest that the phrase “Nothing courtrooms since the seminal Rowley decision
about us without us” should guide intervention. (Board of Education v. Rowley, 1982). For
Each team member brings important information instance, the US Court of Appeals for the Ninth
about the student to the collaboration process. Circuit in Amanda J. v. Clark City School District
The resulting IEP not only mandates the ser- (2001) stated that “procedural violations that
vices and supports allocated to the student but interfere with parental participation in the IEP
also specifies how collaboration is going to hap- formulation process undermine the very essence
pen within the delivery of those services. The ser- of the IDEA” (p.  878). Indeed, parents have a
vices specified within the IEP are not implemented unique and important perspective on their child’s
in a vacuum or in one predetermined location but strengths and needs and are a critical part of the
instead follow a student throughout their school IEP team. The IEP team must ensure that a stu-
day across settings and educators. The IEP plan- dent’s parents, educators, and administrators
ning process, described below, encourages the work together to make important decisions for
educational team to ask “How are we going to eligible students with disabilities throughout the
work together to implement and target all goals?”. special education process (Yell et al., 2020).
It offers educators, caregivers, and related service The student must be invited to attend IEP team
providers a space to consider how they will col- meetings if the meeting will include transition
laborate, request help and support, and learn from planning and consideration of post-secondary
one another. IEPs are written annually, and every goals, which should begin by the age of 16 years
student needs to have a comprehensive evalua- (IDEA, 2004). At the same time, students can be
tion every 3 years to determine if they continue to invited to attend IEP meetings at any age, and
qualify for special education services. there are immense benefits in doing so. Student
The Endrew case (Endrew F. v. Douglas participation in IEP meetings supports the devel-
County School District, 2017), ruled on in 2017 opment of self-determination and self-advocacy
86 I. S. Schwartz et al.

skills (Diegelmann & Test, 2018). These skills across academic and functional areas is the foun-
are important across the lifespan and are particu- dation upon which the IEP stands (Bateman,
larly relevant as students consider post-secondary 2017). Assessment should refrain from relying
options, wherein the student will have to request on a single instrument to identify a disability and
their own accommodations. to inform educational programming and, as such,
should include a variety of strategies and mea-
6.1.2.6 Procedural Safeguards sures (Yell et al., 2020). For a student identified
Parents and students have legal protections dur- as having ASD, the assessment may include tools
ing the evaluation and IEP process, known as focused on the core characteristics of ASD, such
procedural safeguards. Districts are required to as social skills, communication skills, and execu-
share a written copy of these safeguards with tive functioning. However, measures included in
families annually. These safeguards include the the assessment process should be individualized
legal right to be notified of and participate in all to the student to address their specific academic
meetings, access to educational records, transla- and functional needs (Yell et  al., 2020). Within
tion and interpretation services provided free of the context of the general education environment,
charge, documents that are jargon-free, the confi- assessment is ongoing and occurs for all students
dentiality of information as outlined by the regardless of ability labels. Formative and sum-
Family Educational Rights and Privacy Act mative assessments evaluate students’ progress
(Privacy Act, 1974), and a specified process of within the general education curriculum and con-
how to state their disagreement on issues of eval- tribute to ongoing student progress monitoring.
uation, placement, and other provisions of the These are imperative assessments to consider
IEP (Yell et al., 2020). during the IEP planning process as access to and
progress within the general education curriculum
should always be a primary consideration. A
6.1.3 T
 he IEP and Students functional behavior assessment (FBA; discussed
with ASD later in this chapter) should be incorporated for a
student exhibiting challenging behavior so that
6.1.3.1 IEP Planning functionally based supports can be included in
The goal of the IEP planning process is to ensure the student’s plan. The school district is tasked
that the IEP team develops a document that out- with assigning a practitioner with behavioral
lines the specially designed instruction, supports, expertise to conduct the FBA (e.g., school psy-
and accommodations that a student with a dis- chologist, behavior specialist/BCBA, special
ability needs to access learning opportunities and educator). Finally, family priorities and student
succeed at school. As a result of the Endrew input (when possible) should be solicited to
Supreme Court case (Endrew F. v. Douglas inform the selection of goals and objectives.
County School District, 2017), IEPs must result Families may also obtain outside evaluation and
in an educational plan that is appropriately ambi- bring that information to the IEP team for consid-
tious and will result in meaningful learning for eration (Yell et al., 2020).
the student. Assessment, the use of an IEP plan-
ning form, and IEP meetings are all components
of the IEP planning process. IEP Planning Form  The IEP planning form
(see Table 6.2) can be used by all IEP team mem-
Assessment  Assessment is a broad term refer- bers to brainstorm and document their knowledge
ring to the systematic gathering of information to of the student’s current levels of performance and
inform decisions. A formal assessment is admin- ideas for individualized goals and instructional
istered via the school district prior to the creation strategies across multiple domains, including
of an IEP, and accurate and up-to-date informa- academics, motor, communication, social inter-
tion on a student’s current level of performance action/relationships, executive functioning, self-­
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 87

Table 6.2  IEP planning form


Current status (please include levels of Proposals for IEP goals,
independence for different skills and instructional strategies,
behaviors) programming
Academics
 Literacy
 Math
 Other subjects
Motor
Communication
Social interaction/relationships
Executive function (e.g., organization,
planning, problem-solving)
Self-determination
Recreation/leisure
Self-care
Participation in inclusive environment
Barriers to participation/learning
Transition issues (e.g., move to new
school, post-school planning,
employment)
Other

determination, recreation/leisure, self-care, IEP Meeting  IEP meetings can be stressful and
participation in general education, barriers to par- intimidating for parents (Tucker & Schwartz,
ticipation/learning, and transition issues. Under 2013). Some parents report that it can be scary to
“Current Status,” team members may include for- enter a conference with professionals that they do
mal assessment data (e.g., an SLP may include not know and listen to those professionals tell
data from a communication assessment) and/or them about their child’s skills and behavioral
informal data (e.g., caregivers may include data challenges. Rather, the IEP meeting should be a
from observations at home). Using this knowl- time when the educational team, including the
edge, team members may then complete the family and student, take time to celebrate the stu-
“Proposals” column by considering priority areas dents’ accomplishments over the past year and
for instruction, strategies to promote authentic make plans for the next year. All members of the
participation within inclusive settings, and areas educational team should have had the opportu-
of strength or need that are not represented within nity to review the IEP prior to the meeting. The
the domains specified on the form. purpose of the actual meeting should be to ask
questions about the document and how it will be
implemented (Table 6.3).
We recommend that all members of the IEP
team come to the first meeting with the form
completed. This facilitates a productive team The IEP is an important document, and family
meeting in which all team members are prepared input and opinion are valuable. The IEP should be
to share their knowledge and recommendations. a living document, used to inform curricular deci-
Following this meeting, the IEP case manager sions and individualized programming for a stu-
should take all planning forms and team meeting dent. The selected meeting time should aim to
feedback and write the IEP.  Following this, the accommodate as many team members as possible;
case manager distributes the draft IEP to all team full team collaboration is a key component of a
members, including the family, for review prior strong IEP.  Families may invite whomever they
to the formal IEP meeting. want to an IEP meeting, and many advocates rec-
88 I. S. Schwartz et al.

Table 6.3  Suggestions for implementing an IEP in inclu- Table 6.3 (continued)


sive settings
Themes Guiding questions
Themes Guiding questions Training and Has there been ongoing training
Participation in an Does the program provide the coaching for staff and coaching between the
inclusive learner with ASD opportunities educator and a qualified coach?
environment to interact with typically Did coaching entail the coaching
developing peers? “cycle” (i.e., didactic
Does the program staff use instruction, modeling,
appropriate classroom activities, observation, feedback,
room arrangement, and reflection)?
instructional strategies to Technical and Has the family’s input during the
promote positive social social support for assessment process been
interactions between children families solicited?
with ASD and typically Has the family’s need for
developing children? technical support (e.g., parenting
Does the program use master classes) been assessed and have
schedule and activity matrices to these services been provided?
optimize instructional Have the family’s needs
opportunities in general regarding social support been
education settings? assessed and suggestions
Access to an Are family members involved in provided?
appropriately setting the goals for
ambitious and intervention?
QoL-influenced Do intervention goals align with ommend that parents bring someone they trust
curriculum parents’ values, preferences, and
who can act as a notetaker and provide moral sup-
identity?
How will the targeted skills and port during their child’s IEP meetings. Another
behaviors increase the child’s idea to support families in this process is to encour-
independence and increase his or age them to bring a picture of their child, if the
her overall quality of life?
child is not attending, to put in the middle of the
Use of EBPs and Is the data collection system
data-based likely to be implemented and table to remind the team about the real purpose of
decision-making does it provide a measure of the meeting. Others remind parents that they
progress for the particular skill? should not attend an IEP meeting if they have not
Are data being collected on a had the opportunity to review the documentation
frequent enough basis to
determine progress on a ahead of time. Interestingly, a positive outcome of
particular skill? the COVID-19-related school shutdowns is that
Are the data reviewed frequently most IEP meetings have been moved to an online
(at least twice monthly) and are format. Parents report (Bateman & McKittrick,
instructional decisions made,
communicated to the team, and 2021) that they prefer the convenience of online
implemented as a result of the meetings and feel more comfortable participating
data reviews? in IEP meetings using this format. Hopefully,
Functional Has there been a functional school districts will adopt this strategy as an option
approach to behavior assessment (FBA)
to support family engagement.
challenging conducted to determine the
behavior function of the challenging
behavior?
Is there a behavior intervention 6.1.4 IEP Implementation
plan (BIP) in place?
(continued)
The IEP team is collectively responsible for imple-
menting the  IEP.  While IEP goals are generally
domain-specific and may be drafted or suggested
by team members with relevant expertise, this does
not mean that the goals themselves must be imple-
mented by a specific team member. IEP implemen-
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 89

tation should be collaborative with professionals 6.1.4.1 Participating in the Inclusive


from different disciplines working together to Environment
select the most appropriate EBP to address the Education teams must ensure that students are
skill/behavior and the best time of the day to pro- receiving instruction within the LRE. The basic
vide the specially designed instruction. An exam- premise of LRE is that all students with disabili-
ple of this includes supporting a student in the ties are general education students first and must
development of communication skills. While an be assigned a seat in general education. The
SLP may draft goals targeting communication important question to ask is what is happening
based on their discipline-specific knowledge, other while the student is sitting in their general educa-
professionals such as special educators, BCBAs, tion seat and what accommodations, modifica-
and paraprofessionals who have relevant expertise tions, and specially designed instruction  do
may draft goals or contribute additional input and students need to be successful. Being in the LRE
feedback about where and when instruction will be enables students to have access to typically devel-
provided. Once IEP goals are finalized, it is the oping peers and the general education curricu-
entire team’s responsibility to implement the IEP lum. IDEA requires that students with disabilities
and ensure all goals are being consistently targeted be educated alongside their typically developing
by determining who will address each goal, when, peers to the maximum extent possible (IDEA, 20
and how often. School professionals may contrib- U.S.C. 1412). It is incumbent on education and
ute to targeting goals across multiple developmen- IEP teams to decide what settings will be most
tal domains. For example, a student who has appropriate for the student’s learning. Specifically,
communication goals targeting the use of an aug- the team must specify how the student will access
mentative and alternative communication (AAC) the general education curriculum and their typi-
device or behavioral goals surrounding the reduc- cally developing peers. Supporting interactions
tion of challenging behavior will likely work on across ability levels and supporting students with
these goals across the school day. It is unrealistic to and without disabilities to interact meaningfully
have only one IEP team member responsible for promotes key social and communication skills
implementing the communication goal targeting (Schwartz & Davis, 2014).
AAC device use or implementing the behavior Students with disabilities have the right to be
intervention plan. Rather, the entire team is respon- educated in the LRE alongside their typically
sible for determining when, how often, and by developing peers. For the most successful plan-
whom each goal will be addressed. ning, educational teams should not ask if students
The support that facilitates success for one can be included in general education; the more
student with ASD will look different from those appropriate question is what supports and ser-
needed for another student. It is axiomatic that vices do students need to be successful in general
there is not an “average” student with ASD and education?
that the diagnosis or educational determination of
ASD is a description, not a prescription. The indi- Scheduling  Scheduling is one type of structural
vidualized planning associated with writing and support that enables instruction in the general
implementing an IEP is not a suggestion; rather, education environment to occur. Schools are
it is a legal requirement upon which public complex environments with many adults and stu-
schools must educate and support their students. dents moving at different times and going to dif-
Strategies that support students with ASD in pub- ferent places. In order for students with
lic schools include student participation in an disabilities to participate in and benefit from the
inclusive environment, access to a quality of life-­ interactions and instruction in the general educa-
influenced curriculum, the use of EBPs and data-­ tion environment, they need to spend time in that
based decision-making, a functional approach to environment. One strategy that many schools use
challenging behavior, training and coaching for is to develop a master schedule (Villa &
staff, and technical and social support for fami- Thousand, 2016). The purpose of a master school
lies (Schwartz et al., 2017). schedule is to ensure that services and instruc-
90 I. S. Schwartz et al.

tional time across the school are coordinated to come variable of applied behavior analysis and
provide the most efficient and effective instruc- special education (Carr & Horner, 2007; Schwartz
tion for students of all abilities. A well-designed & Kelley, 2021). This, along with the mandate
master schedule will ensure that students are not from the Endrew court decision requiring that
removed from general education during core IEPs be appropriately ambitious, must be consid-
instructional time and that the services that stu- ered when planning and implementing educa-
dents need, to the greatest extent possible, are tional programs for students with ASD.  A
provided in the general education environment QoL-influenced curriculum focuses on functional
alongside their typically developing peers. and culturally relevant skills. Functional skills
are those skills that, if a student cannot do inde-
This type of scheduling also provides grade-­ pendently, someone has to do for them.
level teams (e.g., general and special education Frequently, educational teams limit their list of
teachers, related service providers) time to assess functional skills to activities of daily living
student needs and plan together. This joint plan- (ADL) such as dressing, tooth brushing, and toi-
ning and frequent progress monitoring is key to leting. Although ADL skills are important and
supporting student achievement in general necessary for independence, functional skills
education. extend far beyond this domain. Schalock (2004)
identified eight critical domains that impact QoL:
Planning/Activity Matrices  Activity matrices emotional well-being, interpersonal relations,
guide personnel in determining by whom, when, material well-being, personal development,
and where supports and services will be deliv- physical well-being, self-determination, inclu-
ered. They help ensure that all goals within a stu- sion, and rights (Schalock, 2004; Schalock &
dent’s IEP are being targeted across educational Verdugo, 2002). This breadth of programming
activities over the course of the school day or and the interaction of selected programming with
school week (Sandall et al., 2019; Schwartz et al., student and family values and priorities must be
2017). To create an activity matrix for an indi- considered when implementing an educational
vidual student, an educator develops a table that program.
lists the academic schedule on the left-hand side
and IEP domains in which the student has goals 6.1.4.3 Selecting Appropriate
across the top of the table. The matrix is then Evidence-Based Practice
filled in with the students’ goals within domains to Meet the Needs of Students
indicating where in the academic schedule each and Families
goal will be specifically targeted. To create an Selecting an EBP to support an IEP goal should
activity matrix for a group of students, an educa- consider individual student characteristics and
tor develops a table that lists the academic sched- implementation factors. Student characteristics
ule on the left-hand side and student names across include aspects such as the student’s age, learn-
the top of the table. The matrix is then filled in to ing history, family priorities, gender, cultural
indicate where in the academic schedule various background, whether or not the student is an
student goals will be targeted. Activity matrices English language learner, and disability status
increase the likelihood that each IEP goal will be (Slocum et  al., 2014). Implementation factors
worked on during at least one school activity by include aspects such as the context in which the
serving as a visual reminder for staff. instruction will be provided; the size of the
instructional group (e.g., 1:1 instruction, small
6.1.4.2 Access to an Appropriately group instruction, whole class instruction,
Ambitious and Quality of Life-­ embedded across the school day); the length, fre-
Influenced Curriculum quency, and location of instructional sessions
Improving the quality of life (QoL) of students incorporating the EBP; characteristics of the
with whom we work should be the primary out- individual(s) implementing the EBP; and the
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 91

amount of training required to implement the decision-making are critical components to inter-
EBP with fidelity (Schwartz & Davis, 2014). vention for students with ASD. Without this, pro-
These student and implementation factors help fessionals are unable to determine an
guide EBP selection as IEP team members con- intervention’s effectiveness for an individual stu-
sider for whom and under what circumstances dent (Schwartz & Davis, 2014). To support prog-
intervention strategies have shown meaningful ress monitoring, IDEA requires that students’
gains for students. Using this information, team IEPs contain measurable annual goals, a descrip-
members can identify the research behind (or tion of how these goals will be measured and
lack thereof) practices that fit or approximate the monitored, and a statement indicating when
aforementioned characteristics. reports on the student’s progress will be provided
When selecting EBPs, IEP team members (Yell et al., 2020).
should consult reputable information sources Data should be collected, evaluated, and ana-
such as the What Works Clearinghouse, the lyzed frequently. The frequency is individually
National Center on Intensive Intervention, and determined by the skill and student. In other
the National Professional Development Center words, the data should be collected with enough
on Autism Spectrum Disorder. The What Works frequency that progress is monitored at an appro-
Clearinghouse (https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/) is priate pace (Schwartz et  al., 2017). Collecting
funded by the Institute of Education Sciences and graphing data are associated with increased
within the US Department of Education and student academic achievement, providing educa-
reviews and disseminates research on educational tors with the information they need to make data-­
programs, products, practices, and policies to based decisions (e.g., Fuchs & Fuchs, 1986;
support educators in making evidence-based Sandall et al., 2004). In order to collect and ana-
decisions. The National Center on Intensive lyze data effectively, IEP team members must
Intervention (https://intensiveintervention.org/) determine whose responsibility it is to collect
is also funded by the US Department of Education data for a particular goal and how often these data
through the Office of Special Education Programs will be collected. These data inform the IEP team
(OSEP) and works to build the capacity of educa- whether the specified interventions, instructional
tion agencies, universities, practitioners, and rel- practices, and accommodations are allowing the
evant stakeholders in the implementation of student to make reasonable progress. If it is deter-
academic and behavioral interventions by shar- mined that the student is not making adequate
ing research-based information, professional progress toward their IEP goals, it is the respon-
development, and implementation support. sibility of the IEP team to make changes to the
Finally, the National Professional Development student’s instruction to ensure progress is
Center on Autism Spectrum Disorder (https:// attained.
a u t i s m p d c . f p g . u n c . e d u / ev i d e n c e -­b a s e d -­ Although educators and related service pro-
practices) releases reports that review the litera- viders believe that data collection is essential to
ture to date and identifies EBP specifically for student progress, research suggests that data col-
individuals with ASD. lection and evaluation are inconsistent and/or
considered cumbersome by educators (Sandall
6.1.4.4 Data-Based Decision-Making et  al., 2004). BCBAs and other professionals
While the rigor of the research supporting an have extensive training in data collection but
EBP should be considered when selecting need to be aware of the contextual demands of
instructional strategies to use in practice, contin- collecting data in a classroom setting as c­ ompared
ual progress monitoring is required to determine to a clinic or treatment room. It is recommended
whether or not the practice is effective and result- that the intervention team determine a data col-
ing in positive outcomes for a specific student. lection system that is functional, realistic, and
Current best practice indicates that ongoing data useful. There are many suggestions for collecting
collection and analysis to inform data-based data, using both high-tech and low-tech tools.
92 I. S. Schwartz et al.

Data collection strategies can include applica- In the example of pointing, the child points in
tions on tablets and smartphones, paper and pen- order to request the toy that they want—a clear
cil data sheets, golf counters, 3x5 cards clipped to example of the communicative aspect of
therapist’s waistband, and even masking tape on behavior.
the pants of the therapist to keep track of behav- In the context of schools, IDEA mandates that
ior (e.g., Dunlap et  al., 2019; Hojnoski et  al., an FBA be conducted when a student’s challeng-
2009; Lingo et al., 2011; Schwartz et al., 2017). ing behavior significantly interferes with their
The best data sheet is the data sheet that gets used learning (IDEA, 2004). Conducting an FBA
frequently and yields information that can be refers to the process of gathering behavioral data
used to monitor student progress. Once the data from the student that will inform the develop-
are gathered, they must be graphed and analyzed ment of a functional hypothesis (i.e., why the
in a timely manner (e.g., at least weekly). behavior is occurring). Different school districts
Engaging in these behaviors will allow the edu- may have various required forms for their FBAs;
cational team to determine which behaviors are however, it is considered best practice to have a
responding positively (or not) to intervention. An clear, observable definition of the behavior,
activity matrix, discussed above, can also be use- observe multiple instances of the behavior in
ful in determining when and where data will be context, and interview multiple stakeholders
collected and provide a simple strategy to keep (e.g., teacher, parents, student if possible). This
track of which data have been collected every process will involve determining patterns in the
week. reinforcing consequences of that behavior (Lewis
et al., 2017; Steege et al., 2019), as well as envi-
6.1.4.5 Functional Approach ronmental variables such as the antecedents.
to Challenging Behavior From there, the school team, including a behavior
Over the past 30 years, there has been a plethora analyst or school psychologist, will develop a
of research demonstrating that challenging behavior intervention plan (BIP) that outlines the
behavior serves a communicative function (e.g., proposed method of responding to the challeng-
Andzik et al., 2016; Chezan et al., 2018; Durand ing behavior and how the team will teach replace-
& Moskowitz, 2015). In developing interventions ment behaviors. At the core of an FBA is the
for a particular behavior, extant research indi- behavior analytic assumption that this student’s
cates that the intervention must target the same behavior meets one of four functions: escape,
function as the challenging behavior (Carr, 1988; attention, tangible, or automatic (e.g., sensory).
Carr & Durand, 1985). To do this, behavior must Within school settings, school psychologists fre-
be analyzed in the context that it occurs in order quently conduct FBAs. Other professionals with
to determine a pattern between what happens behavioral expertise and knowledge (such as spe-
before and after the behavior (Carr, 1994; cial educators or behavior analysts) may also be
Heckaman et al., 2000). There are multiple ways the professional leading the team to conduct an
to describe challenging behavior. For example, FBA.  Given that behaviors may serve different
one may describe what form the behavior takes, functions in different contexts, communication
or what it looks like (e.g., reading, jumping, play- and data sharing within the team are crucial.
ing). One may also take a functional approach to Increasingly, schools are implementing
describing behavior, or what effect that behavior School-Wide Positive Behavior Interventions
has on the environment and people around them and Supports (SWPBIS) by using systems-wide
(e.g., pointing to receive a toy). Behavior analysts changes in their expectations, culture, and over-
approach challenging behavior by determining all school environment (Sugai & Horner, 2006).
the function, or the “why” behind the student SWPBIS is a multi-tiered system of support
engaging in that behavior, through an FBA. In (MTSS) designed to provide a preventative
this functional approach, our goal is to describe approach to challenging behavior in schools by
what is motivating, or maintaining, the behavior. creating support at the primary (school-wide),
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 93

secondary (class-wide), and tertiary (individual depends upon the needs of the educators and stu-
student) levels (Sugai & Horner, 2009). Students dents within the school.
with ASD, because they are general education Behavioral skills training (BST; Kirkpatrick
students first, must be included in SWPBIS and et al., 2019) is one of the most commonly cited
any other school side interventions and and practiced models for coaching in applied
supports. behavior analysis. It is important for BCBAs
working in schools to remember that BST is one
6.1.4.6 Training and Coaching for Staff type of coaching and training and that other effi-
The educational team should assess team mem- cacious coaching models exist and are used in
bers’ needs for implementing EBP specified school settings, such as instructional coaching,
within the student’s IEP.  Training and coaching practice-based coaching, and peer coaching
should be provided to any staff member who (Desimone & Pak, 2017; Scheeler et  al., 2010;
works with the student who does not have ade- Snyder et  al., 2015). BCBAs in schools should
quate experience, knowledge, confidence, or avoid becoming stuck on the coaching model or
skills regarding the selected EBP(s). Training and terminology used but rather should emphasize
coaching may be provided by qualified team the importance of coaching across disciplines to
members, other members of the school commu- ensure accurate and effective implementation of
nity, or outside consultants. The need for training evidence-based instructional practice.
and/or coaching should not stand in the way of
implementing an EBP that is a necessary part of 6.1.4.7 Technical and Social Support
a student’s educational program. for Families
Coaching is a broad term constituting an The IEP team must ensure that caregivers, and, to
ongoing process intended to improve perfor- the extent possible, students, are active members
mance. While models of coaching may differ and decision-makers throughout the IEP plan-
slightly, common components making up a ning and implementation processes. IEP team
coaching “cycle” include didactic instruction on members are required to gain consent and input
the identified practice, educator observation of from caregivers in order to proceed with the IEP
the coach engaging in (i.e., modeling) the prac- assessment process; but the quality of the process
tice, coach observation of the educator engaging and product of the IEP can be improved when
in the practice while simultaneously delivering families feel like authentic partners in the process
constructive and positive feedback, an opportu- (Drasgow et al., 2001; Kurth et al., 2019). Further,
nity for the coach and educator to jointly reflect the IEP team should consider caregivers as equal
on the use of the practice (e.g., how the student partners, rather than as passive participants. IEP
responded to the practice, the educator’s percep- meetings often contain large amounts of techni-
tion of implementation), and an opportunity for cal jargon and leave caregivers outnumbered as
the coach and educator to plan next steps (Jewett team members. Helping parents access informa-
& MacPhee, 2012; Rush & Shelden, 2011; tion about educational terms, processes, and
Snyder et al., 2015). Coaching may be delivered interventions is one type of technical support that
by professionals with relevant expertise in the schools can provide for families (Schwartz et al.,
identified practice. BCBAs have knowledge of 2017). For parents of culturally and linguistically
behavioral and effective teaching strategies and diverse learners, ensuring that materials are trans-
are thus good candidates for coaching related to lated, interpreters are present at meetings, and
behavior intervention and skill-based instruc- programs use “cultural brokers” (Conners &
tional strategies. General educators have exper- Capell, 2020, p. 209) such as advocates, religious
tise in academic content such as mathematics, leaders, or others with relevant cultural experi-
literacy, and science, while special educators ences to help to share important information with
have knowledge regarding curriculum modifica- families can improve the parent experience and
tions and adaptations. Coaching in schools the overall program for the student. In addition to
94 I. S. Schwartz et al.

this technical support for caregivers in IEP meet- newly acquired skills and behaviors. When com-
ings, support groups or parenting classes should pared with other intervention settings for indi-
also be provided (Schwartz & Davis, 2014). viduals with ASD, public schools also contain
Caregivers of students with developmental dis- unique features that impact teaching and learn-
abilities are at increased risk of experiencing ing, including interdisciplinary teams working
stress, particularly when a student engages in together within the same setting, a diverse and
challenging behaviors (Woodman et  al., 2015), inclusive environment, and access to the general
and caregivers of students with ASD may experi- education curriculum.
ence higher levels of stress than those with other
developmental disabilities (Estes et  al., 2009). 6.1.5.1 Collaboration
Accordingly, meaningful participation in the IEP and Interdisciplinary Support
process includes finding support and managing Providing comprehensive and effective services
stress. to students with ASD requires a team effort. All
students, including students with ASD, are het-
erogeneous in behavior, strengths, interests, and
6.1.5 Context of Schools support needs (Dunlap and Fox 2007) and may
benefit from provider support across disciplines.
Professionals who spend time in schools know For instance, one student with ASD may require
the breadth of both the official and hidden curri- individualized instruction from a special educa-
cula (Myles & Simpson, 2001; Sulaimani & Gut, tor and speech therapy from an SLP. Another stu-
2019) that students experience across the school dent with ASD may benefit from services that
day. Schools play a vital role in students’ devel- support their identity as a dual-language learner
opment and present many opportunities that stu- in addition to gifted education strategies. Schools
dents do not have elsewhere, including provide a unique opportunity for collaboration
opportunities for social interaction, independent across a diverse, interdisciplinary team of educa-
management of materials, and community build- tors, professionals, and community members to
ing throughout childhood and adolescence. meet the various needs of students. All members
Schools are also the setting in which the majority of a school community can play an important role
of students with ASD and related disabilities in supporting students with ASD, including edu-
receive specially designed instruction and thera- cators, related service providers, office staff,
peutic services to which they are entitled under paraprofessionals, school nurses, classmates,
IDEA. The school setting provides unique oppor- transportation team members, custodial staff, and
tunities and challenges. One of the primary ben- the larger school community of families and
efits of the school setting for students with ASD students.
is that schools provide the greatest opportunity
for inclusion within a student’s community of 6.1.5.2 Diverse and Inclusive
peers—school is where most children and ado- Schools are a great equalizer; all students have
lescents are during the day! Providing support the right to attend. Public school settings provide
and interventions within the school setting brings opportunities for students with ASD to establish
opportunities for generalization and maintenance diverse and meaningful relationships with mem-
of skills, immediate access to peers to develop bers of their community. IDEA mandates that
relationships, and opportunities to access a wide every student have access to the LRE to the maxi-
range of services and supports in one setting. mum extent possible throughout their school day
Much of the intervention provided in schools is (IDEA, 2004). This means that every effort
embedded into valued routines and rituals across should be made to deliver individualized sup-
the school day (Staub et  al., 1994), providing ports alongside typically developing peers in the
multiple opportunities to learn in the natural general education setting. At the same time, sim-
environment and optimizing the generalization of ply being present alongside typically developing
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 95

peers is not enough and is not inclusion. Inclusion In addition to meeting academic goals, IDEA
is not a place or an instructional strategy, but a requires consideration of and planning for every-
cultural shift that promotes all community mem- day functioning beyond the classroom setting.
bers’ active participation in and sense of belong- For this reason, the IEPs are required to include
ing to that community (Sandall et  al., 2019). transition planning information for students by
Classrooms that use high-leverage inclusionary their 16th birthday so that students are prepared
practices create diverse community spaces that for life after school (IDEA, 2004). Planning and
allow for meaningful social connections in addi- programming in an IEP should reflect this and
tion to emotional and academic learning (Agran incorporate students’ dreams, strengths, and pri-
et  al., 2020; National Council on Disability, orities into the transition plan. Further, the educa-
2018). tional team should consider that quality of life
Unfortunately, school and program placement will look different for every student and family.
decisions (i.e., locations within which a student This highlights the importance of considering
will receive their education, such as general or caregivers as equal members of an IEP team,
special education classrooms) have often been rather than as passive participants. Family and
based on perceptions of student competence and student voices need to be heard, especially in
resulting placement policies, economic and transition planning. They are the experts on what
demographic stratification, biases, teacher prepa- their student needs to live a happy adult life
ration and experience, and school resources and beyond the classroom. Culture, language, values,
capacity. None of these factors relate to student and preferences should all be recognized in the
learning needs (Agran et al., 2020). The opportu- implementation and planning of an IEP. A family-­
nity to participate in a classroom alongside peers centered approach should be used to teach the
without disabilities should not be based on a stu- individual the skills they will need to live a high
dent’s zip code, race, or diagnostic label. Rather, quality of life on their own terms.
placement decisions should be based on meeting
the student’s individual needs for optimal growth,
as federal law defines it. Though many ASD-­ 6.2 Conclusion
specific and special education classrooms exist,
they are not necessarily the best fit for every stu- Access to a FAPE is the right of every student
dent with ASD. with a disability in the United States. Part of a
FAPE is having access to high-quality instruction
6.1.5.3 Access to the General Education (i.e., EBP) in the least restrictive environment. In
Curriculum order to ensure that students with ASD achieve
In order to provide students with appropriately meaningful educational outcomes, they also need
ambitious IEPs, educational teams must draw to have access to appropriately ambitious educa-
from the general education curriculum. The goal tion plans, high-quality instruction, ongoing per-
of special education is to support students to be formance monitoring, and data-based
successful in the general education curriculum. decision-making.
That means that every student, regardless of abil- Students with ASD are students first, and so it
ity, should have academic goals and receive follows that in public schools students with ASD
instruction in core requirements such as literacy are general education students first. Therefore,
and math (e.g., Agran et  al., 2020). Although the default placement for students with ASD and
some students, often including students with other disabilities should be the general education
ASD, will need instruction in additional curricu- environment. For students with disabilities,
lum areas such as ADL, organizational skills, and ­additional services and supports are layered on
social skills, these should supplement, not sup- top of the foundation of general education or Tier
plant, access to the general education 1 supports (i.e., in the terms used by multi-tiered
curriculum. systems of supports) to ensure success in the gen-
96 I. S. Schwartz et al.

eral education curriculum and learning environ- demonstrate meaningful student progress. Only
ment. If and only if the educational team can the data can tell whether an EBP is truly effective
demonstrate that they cannot provide a student for an individual student with ASD, and the data
access to high-quality instruction in the general must be analyzed and the analysis must be
education setting can a more restrictive setting applied to make the power of EBP a reality in the
(e.g., a segregated classroom) be considered. It is lives of students with ASD and related
important to remember that if a student receives a disabilities.
diagnosis of ASD, this is simply a description—
not a prescription—for specific supports and ser-
vices. There is not a specific learning program References
that will fit all students with ASD; rather, indi-
vidualized programming is needed to meet stu- Agran, M., Jackson, L., Kurth, J.  A., Ryndak, D.,
Burnette, K., Jameson, M., Zagona, A., Fitzpatrick,
dent needs. It is a good reminder for behavioral H., & Wehmeyer, M. (2020). Why aren’t students
analysts and all members of the educational team with severe disabilities being placed in general
that student failure is instructional failure. If a education classrooms: Examining the relations
student is not making meaningful progress among classroom placement, learner outcomes,
and other factors. Research and Practice for
toward important educational outcomes, it is on Persons with Severe Disabilities, 45(1), 4–13.
the shoulders of the educational team, not the stu- https://doi.org/10.1177/1540796919878134
dent. The instruction must be changed to support Amanda J. v. Clark County, 267 F.3d 877 (9th Cir. 2001).
the student and her success. Andzik, N.  R., Cannella-Malone, H.  I., & Sigafoos,
J. (2016). Practitioner-implemented functional
The IEP planning and implementation pro- communication training: A review of the lit-
cesses require interdisciplinary teaming and erature. Research and Practice for Persons with
appropriately ambitious goals while recognizing Severe Disabilities, 41(2), 79–89. https://doi.
and accommodating the multiple identities, org/10.1177/1540796916633874
Bateman, B. D. (2017). Individualized education programs
strengths, and areas of need that students bring for children with disabilities. In J. M. Kauffman, D. P.
with them to school. Educating students with Hallahan, & P.  Pullen (Eds.), Handbook of special
ASD is a team sport—no professional can imple- education (2nd ed., pp.  91–108). Taylor & Francis/
ment a high-quality IEP alone. Interdisciplinary Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315517698
Bateman, K.  J., & McKittrick. (2021, April 28). Virtual
teaming includes promoting caregivers as active IEPs Should Stay. https://www.crpe.org/thelens/
members of the team by incorporating caregiver virtual-­ieps-­should-­stay
input and providing for caregiver support. Behavior Analysis Certification Board. (2020). Ethics
Caregiver knowledge of and long-term goals for code for behavior analysts. Author.
Board of Education of the Hendrick Hudson Central
their child contribute to the development of School District v. Rowley, 458 U.S. 176 (1982).
appropriately ambitious goals, and the team as a Carr, E.  G. (1988). Functional equivalence as a mecha-
whole is responsible for providing the specific nism of response generalization. In R.  H. Horner,
support and services needed to meet those goals. G. Dunlap, & R. L. Koegel (Eds.), Generalization and
maintenance: Life-style changes in applied settings
Working toward and achieving meaningful edu- (pp. 221–241). Brookes Publishing.
cational outcomes require acknowledging the Carr, E.  G. (1994). Emerging themes in the func-
individuality of students. The “I” in IEP stands tional analysis of problem behavior. Journal of
for individualized; this cannot be overstated. Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 393–399. https://doi.
org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­393
Acknowledging and incorporating the breadth of Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior
a student’s identity is imperative for a truly indi- problems through functional communication training.
vidualized and meaningful IEP. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(2), 111–126.
Finally, we acknowledge the importance of https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­111
Carr, E. G., & Horner, R. H. (2007). The expanding vision
EBP while simultaneously highlighting that EBP of positive behavior support: Research perspectives
is simply the starting place for selecting appro- on happiness, helpfulness, hopefulness. Journal of
priate and effective practices. Implementation of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9(1), 3–14. https://
EBPs requires data-based decision-making to doi.org/10.1177/10983007070090010201
6  Evidence-Based Practice in Schools 97

Chezan, L.  C., Wolfe, K., & Drasgow, E. (2018). A Exceptional Children, 12(3), 32–44. https://doi.
meta-analysis of functional communication training org/10.1177/1096250609333025
effects on problem behavior and alternative com- Individuals With Disabilities Education Act, 20
municative responses. Focus on Autism and Other U.S.C. § 1400 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/
Developmental Disabilities, 33(4), 195–205. https:// springerreference_69963
doi.org/10.1177/1088357617741294 Jewett, P., & MacPhee, D. (2012). Adding collaborative
Conners, B. M., & Capell, S. T. (2020). Multiculturalism peer coaching to our teaching identities. The Reading
and diversity in Applied Behavior Analysis: Bridging Teacher, 66(2), 105–110. https://doi.org/10.1002/
theory and application. Routledge. https://doi. trtr.01089
org/10.4324/9780429263873 Kirkpatrick, M., Akers, J., & Rivera, G. (2019). Use of
Desimone, L., & Pak, K. (2017). Instructional coaching behavioral skills training with teachers: A systematic
as high quality professional development. Theory Into review. Journal of Behavioral Education, 28(3), 344–
Practice, 56(1), 3–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/004058 361. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­019-­09322-­z
41.2016.1241947 Kober, N. (2007). Why we still need public schools: Public
Diegelmann, K.  M., & Test, D.  W. (2018). Effects of a education for the common good. Center on Education
self-monitoring checklist as a component of the self-­ Policy. https://cepr.harvard.edu/
directed IEP. Education and Training in Autism and Kurth, J.  A., McQueston, J.  A., Ruppar, A.  L., Toews,
Developmental Disabilities, 53(1), 73–83. S.  G., Johnston, R., & McCabe, K.  M. (2019). A
Drasgow, E., Yell, M.  L., & Robinson, T.  R. (2001). description of parent input in IEP development
Developing legally correct and educationally appropri- through analysis IEP documents. Intellectual and
ate IEPs. Remedial and Special Education, 22(6), 359– Developmental Disabilities, 57(6), 485–498. https://
373. https://doi.org/10.1177/074193250102200606 doi.org/10.1352/1934-­9556-­57.6.485
Duncan, A. (2015). Forty years of the Individuals with Lewis, T.  J., Hatton, H.  L., Jorgenson, C., & Maynard,
Disabilities Education Act (IDEA). U.S.  Department D. (2017). What beginning special educators need to
of Education. know about conducting functional behavioral assess-
Dunlap, G., & Fox, L. (2007). Parent–professional part- ments. Teaching Exceptional Children, 49(4), 231–
nerships: A valuable context for addressing challeng- 238. https://doi.org/10.1177/0040059917690885
ing behaviours. International Journal of Disability, Lingo, A.  S., Barton-Arwood, S.  M., & Jolivette, K.
Development and Education, 54(3), 273–285. (2011). Teachers working together: Improving
Dunlap, G., Iovannone, R., Kincaid, D., Wilson, K., learning outcomes in the inclusive classroom-
Christiansen, K., Strain, P., & English, C. (2019). practical strategies and examples. Teaching
Prevent-teach-reinforce: The school-based model of Exceptional Children, 43(3), 6–13. https://doi.
individualized positive behavior support (2nd ed.). org/10.1177/004005991104300301
Brookes. Myles, B. S., & Simpson, R. L. (2001). Understanding the
Durand, V.  M., & Moskowitz, L. (2015). Functional hidden curriculum: An essential social skill for chil-
communication training: Thirty years of treating dren and youth with Asperger syndrome. Intervention
challenging behavior. Topics in Early Childhood in School and Clinic, 36(5), 279–286. https://doi.
Special Education, 35(2), 116–126. https://doi. org/10.1177/105345120103600504
org/10.1177/0271121415569509 National Council on Disability. (2018). IDEA Series: The
Endrew F. v. Douglas County School District Re-1, 137 Segregation of Students with Disabilities. https://ncd.
S. Ct. 988 (2017). gov/sites/default/files/NCD_Segregation-­SWD_508.
Estes, A., Munson, J., Dawson, G., Koehler, E., Zhou, pdf
X.  H., & Abbott, R. (2009). Parenting stress and Privacy Act of 1974, 5 U.S.C. Prelim. § 552a (2012 Ed.
psychological functioning among mothers of pre- Supp. I, 2013).
school children with autism and developmen- Rush, D. D., & Shelden, M. L. (2011). The early childhood
tal delay. Autism, 13(4), 375–387. https://doi. coaching handbook. Brookes Publishing Company.
org/10.1177/1362361309105658 Sackett, D.  L., Rosenberg, W.  M., Gray, J.  M., Haynes,
Fuchs, L.  S., & Fuchs, D. (1986). Effects of sys- R.  B., & Richardson, W.  S. (1996). Evidence based
tematic formative evaluation: A meta-analysis. medicine: what it is and what it isn't. British Medical
Exceptional Children, 53(3), 199–208. https://doi. Journal, 312, 71–72. https://doi.org/10.1136/
org/10.1177/001440298605300301 bmj.312.7023.71
Heckaman, K., Conroy, M.  A., Fox, J., & Chait, A. Sandall, S.  R., Schwartz, I.  S., & Lacroix, B. (2004).
(2000). Functional assessment-based interven- Interventionists’ perspectives about data collection
tion research on students with or at risk for emo- in integrated early childhood classrooms. Journal
tional and behavioral disorders in school settings. of Early Intervention, 26(3), 161–174. https://doi.
Behavioral Disorders, 25, 196–210. https://doi. org/10.1177/105381510402600301
org/10.1177/019874290002500307 Sandall, S. R., Schwartz, I. S., Joseph, G. E., & Gauvreau,
Hojnoski, R.  L., Gischlar, K.  L., & Missall, K.  N. A. N. (2019). Building blocks for teaching preschool-
(2009). Improving child outcomes with data-­ ers with special needs (3rd ed.). Brookes Publishing
based decision making: Collecting data. Young Company.
98 I. S. Schwartz et al.

Schalock, R.  L. (2004). The concept of quality of life: The Guilford Practical Intervention in the Schools
what we know and do not know. Journal of Intellectual Series. Guilford Press.
Disability Research, 48(3), 203–216. https://doi. Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin,
org/10.1111/j.1365-­2788.2003.00558 K. L., Nowell, S. W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey, S.,
Schalock, R., & Verdugo, M. (2002). Handbook on qual- McIntyre, N. S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage, M. N.
ity of life for human service practitioners. American (2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth,
Association on Mental Retardation. https://doi. and young adults with Autism. The University of
org/10.1002/dei.156 North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham
Scheeler, M.  C., Congdon, M., & Stansbery, S. (2010). Child Development Institute, National Clearinghouse
Providing immediate feedback to co-teachers through on Autism Evidence and Practice Review Team.
bug-in-ear technology: An effective method of peer Sugai, G., & Horner, R. R. (2006). A promising approach
coaching in inclusion classrooms. Teacher Education for expanding and sustaining school-wide positive
and Special Education, 33(1), 83–96. https://doi. behavior support. School Psychology Review, 35(2),
org/10.1177/0888406409357013 245–259. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2006.120
Schwartz, I. S., & Davis, C. A. (2014). Best practices in 87989
early identification and early services for children with Sugai, G., & Horner, R.  H. (2009). Responsiveness-­
autism spectrum disorder. Best practices in school to-­intervention and school-wide positive behav-
psychology. In P. Harrison & A. Thomas (Eds.), Best ior supports: Integration of multi-tiered system
practices in school psychology VI (pp.  405–416). approaches. Exceptionality, 17(4), 223–237. https://
National Association of School Psychology. doi.org/10.1080/09362830903235375
Schwartz, I.  S., & Kelley, E. (2021). Quality of life for Sulaimani, M. F., & Gut, D. M. (2019). Hidden curricu-
people with disabilities: Why applied behavior ana- lum in a special education context: The case of indi-
lysts should consider this a primary dependent vari- viduals with autism. Journal of Educational Research
able. Research and Practice Severe Disabilities. and Practice, 9(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.5590/
Schwartz, I., Ashmun, J., McBride, B., Scott, C., & jerap.2019.09.1.03
Sandall, S.  R. (2017). The DATA model for teach- Tucker, V., & Schwartz, I. S. (2013). Parents’ perspectives
ing preschoolers with autism. Brookes Publishing of collaboration with school professionals: Barriers
Company. and facilitators to successful partnerships in planning
Slocum, T.  A., Detrich, R., Wilczynski, S.  M., Spencer, for students with ASD. School Mental Health, 5, 3–14.
T.  D., Lewis, T., & Wolfe, K. (2014). The evidence-­ https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-­012-­9102-­0
based practice of applied behavior analysis. Villa, R., & Thousand, J. (2016). The inclusive education
The Behavior Analyst, 37(1), 41–56. https://doi. checklist: A self-assessment of best practices. National
org/10.1007/s40614-­014-­0005-­2 Professional Resources, Inc./Dude Publishing.
Smith, T. (2013). What is evidence-based behavior analy- Woodman, A.  C., Mawdsley, H.  P., & Hauser-Cram, P.
sis? The Behavior Analyst, 36(1), 7–33. https://doi. (2015). Parenting stress and child behavior prob-
org/10.1007/bf03392290 lems within families of children with developmental
Snyder, P.  A., Hemmeter, M.  L., & Fox, L. (2015). disabilities: Transactional relations across 15 years.
Supporting implementation of evidence-based prac- Research in Developmental Disabilities, 36C, 264–
tices through practice-based coaching. Topics in Early 276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.10.011
Childhood Special Education, 35(3), 133–143. https:// Yell, M. L., & Bateman, D. F. (2019). Free appropriate pub-
doi.org/10.1177/0271121415594925 lic education and Endrew F. v. Douglas County School
Staub, D., Schwartz, I.  S., Gallucci, C., & Peck, C.  A. System (2017): Implications for personnel prepara-
(1994). Four portraits of friendship at an inclu- tion. Teacher Education and Special Education, 42(1),
sive school. Journal of the Association for Persons 6–17. https://doi.org/10.1177/0040059920906592
with Severe Handicaps, 19(4), 314–325. https://doi. Yell, M.  L., Collins, J., Kumpiene, G., & Bateman,
org/10.1177/154079699401900407 D. (2020). The individualized education program:
Steege, M.  W., Pratt, J.  L., Wickerd, G., Guare, R., & Procedural and substantive requirements. Teaching
Watson, T. S. (2019). Conducting school-based func- Exceptional Children, 52(5), 304–318. https://doi.
tional behavioral assessments: A practitioner’s guide. org/10.1177/0040059920906592
Factors Influencing to Implement
or Not to Implement 7
Evidence-­Based Procedures

Thomas Zane, Robin M. Kuhn, Samantha R. Volpe,


Mariah Mussetter, and Jessica F. Juanico

The percentage of children diagnosed with devel- Despite the heterogeneity of ASD symptom
opmental disabilities continues to increase, with manifestation, interventions based upon applied
current estimates suggesting nearly 18% of chil- behavior analysis (ABA) have proven effective at
dren in the United States aged 3–17 are diag- building behavioral repertoires that improve the
nosed with a developmental disability (Zablotsky quality of life for individuals diagnosed with
et al., 2019). As of 2016, the Centers for Disease ASD; this claim is supported by experimental
Control and Prevention estimated 1 in 64 4-year-­ evidence demonstrating the effectiveness of
old children (Maenner et al., 2020) and 1 in 54 ABA-based interventions with children diag-
8-year-old children (Shaw et  al., 2020) in the nosed with ASD (Leaf et  al., 2020a; Reichow
United States are diagnosed with autism spec- et  al., 2012), endorsement by the US Surgeon
trum disorder (ASD). ASD is characterized by General (United States Public Health Service,
impairments in social communication as well as 1999), and mandated insurance coverage of ABA
by presentation of repetitive and restricted behav- in most states (National Conference of State
ior, with severity levels ranging from 1 to 3 Legislatures, 2018). Adding to the evidence accu-
according to the least to most support required mulated to date, Makrygianni et  al. (2018)
for daily functioning (American Psychiatric recently published a meta-analysis examining the
Association, 2013). While the diagnostic criteria effectiveness of ABA for children diagnosed with
reflect the homogeneity of ASD symptoms, there ASD.  Their search of the literature revealed 29
is great heterogeneity in their manifestation studies meeting their extensive inclusion and
within and across individuals on the spectrum exclusion criteria. The results of their analyses of
(Lombardo et al., 2019). the included studies indicated that ABA interven-
tion is highly effective at improving intellectual
functioning, moderately to highly effective at
T. Zane (*) · J. F. Juanico improving communication (specifically, highly
Department of Applied Behavioral Science, effective at improving expressive language and
University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA moderately effective at improving receptive lan-
e-mail: tzane@ku.edu guage), and less effective at improving daily liv-
R. M. Kuhn ing skills (perhaps due to the young age of
University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA participants or the increased focus of intervention
S. R. Volpe in other domains). The results of this recent meta-­
Department of Applied Behavior Analysis, Endicott analysis largely mirror findings reported in
College, Beverly, MA, USA
reviews of single-case research designs (e.g.,
M. Mussetter Odom & Strain, 2002).
Sadowsky Autism Services, Platte City, MO, USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 99


J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_7
100 T. Zane et al.

Most individuals diagnosed with autism expe- educating on treatments that are not based on sci-
rience challenges across several domains, includ- ence as often fad treatments are embraced within
ing difficulties with communicating effectively, the treatment of ASD.
developing relationships and socializing, playing
and engaging in recreational activities, participat-
ing in daily living activities, and acquiring aca- 7.1.1 Definitions of Evidence-Based
demic skills, among others (American Psychiatric Practice
Association, 2013). Given the extent of the
domains impacted by autism, it is not surprising There are several definitions of evidence-based
that approaches to autism intervention are preva- practice (EBP). In general, EBP refers to inte-
lent among various scientific disciplines in addi- grating empirically based practices with clinical
tion to ABA including speech language pathology, expertise while considering the context and the
occupational therapy, physical therapy, and individual being served (e.g., American
psychology. Psychological Association, 2006; Gast &
Ledford, 2018; Institute of Medicine, 2001;
Ioannidis, 2016). Practices include curricula,
7.1 Evidence-Based Practice interventions, treatments, and system-level inter-
ventions aimed to change behavior (Horner et al.,
Although most fields acknowledge the impor- 2005). Typically, evidence-based practices
tance of incorporating research into practice, a require measurement of treatment effectiveness
research-to-practice gap is often cited (e.g., men- and maintenance, include fidelity and reliability
tal health, Kazdin, 2000; education, LeRoy, 2017; measures (Dunkel-Jackson et  al., 2012), and
human resources, Rynes et  al., 2002; behavior demonstrate replications of positive treatment
analysis, Slocum et  al., 2014). The research-to-­ outcomes across multiple iterations (Cook &
practice gap is a concern in that the quality-of-­ Cook, 2013; LeRoy, 2017).
service provision may suffer due to practitioners Horner et al. (2005) proposed five guidelines
implementing outdated or irrelevant procedures for assessing evidence-based practices in special
(Valentino & Juanico, 2020). Given the gap education using single-subject designs. The
between research and practice, many fields began guidelines included (a) an operational definition
to develop and encourage evidence-based prac- of the practice, (b) a description of the context,
tice to improve and enhance decision-making by (c) measures of treatment fidelity, (d) demonstra-
practitioners (Dunkel-Jackson et  al., 2012; tion of a functional relationship between the
Slocum et  al., 2014). In fact, many fields have dependent and independent variables, and (e)
created task forces to formally define, identify, replication of outcomes across multiple studies,
and disseminate their field’s evidence-based researchers, and participants. Additional stan-
practices (Chambless & Ollendick, 2001). dards have been established by the What Works
Several organizations promote and disseminate Clearinghouse (2020) to guide reviewers when
evidence-based practices. For example, the evaluating educational practices using single-­
American Academy of Pediatrics (2020) is dedi- case research. To evaluate a single-case research
cated to encouraging basic and applied research study, a reviewer would evaluate the study to
within all facets of healthcare. To meet this goal, determine the (a) availability of data in graphical
they publish a manual of evidence-based prac- or tabular format for visual analysis, (b) extent to
tices to help guide medical professionals in cur- which systematic manipulation of the indepen-
rent best practices (American Academy of dent variable occurred, (c) confidence in mea-
Pediatrics, 2020). Similarly, the Association for sures of reliability (i.e., at least 20% of data
Science in Autism Treatment’s (n.d.) mission is points within each condition have a second,
to promote and disseminate empirically based ­independent observer collect data, and reliability
treatments for individuals with ASD while also measures are at least 80%), (d) presence or
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 101

absence of residual treatment effects (e.g., carry- the single-most agreed-upon criterion. Other
over effects) and confounding factors (e.g., than that, most source documents agreed upon
changes in therapists across conditions), and (e) only up to three of the criteria. Zane and Hanson
extent to which treatment effects were replicated concluded that although many different organi-
across time and phases based on specific criteria zations support the concept and importance of
related to the type of design and number of data evidence-based practice, there is little agree-
points. ment as to what actually constitutes the criteria
Additional tools for assessing evidence- for determining whether a practice actually
based practices (e.g., The Single Case Analysis should be labeled as evidence-based. Regardless
and Review Framework; Ledford et  al., 2020; of differences between the standards developed,
The Risk of Bias in N-of-1 Trials [RoBiNT] the use of these tools and reports generated by
Scale; Tate et  al., 2013) and standards for the standards allows practitioners to select
evidence-­ based practice using single-subject appropriate evidence-based practices (LeRoy,
research designs have been developed by 2017), thus enhancing their clinical practice
researchers (Chambless et al., 1996) and orga- and outcomes for the individuals they serve.
nizations (e.g., National Clearinghouse on In the field of ABA, Slocum et al. (2014) pro-
Autism Evidence and Practice; Steinbrenner posed a definition of evidence-based practice for
et al., 2020). Similarly, many professional orga- evaluating practices. In developing the definition,
nizations have recommendations for evaluating they considered how the definition aligned with
evidence-based practices. Although there are behaviorism’s philosophical tenets while also
similarities across tools, standards, and organi- ensuring the definition would support the use of
zations, there are some differences that further the most effective practices within the field,
lead to confusion about what constitutes an encourage advancements of behavior-analytic
evidence-based practice (Ledford et  al., 2018; practices, and facilitate understanding and sup-
Odom et  al., 2010). For example, Zane and port for individuals outside of behavior analysis.
Hanson (2008) systematically reviewed the Thus, they defined evidence-based practice
diversity of criteria different organizations used within the field of ABA as “… a decision-making
to define “minimal standards” for quality evi- process that integrates (a) the best available evi-
dence. The authors selected several sources that dence with (b) clinical expertise and (c) client
published criteria for what constituted evidence values and context” (Slocum et al., 2014, p. 44).
(e.g., American Psychological Association, This definition suggests that a practitioner should
Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based primarily use the best available evidence; how-
Practice, US Department of Health, US ever, it takes into consideration the complexity of
Education Department, What Works behavioral programming by providing other vari-
Clearinghouse). They then identified the sepa- ables that should be considered such as the con-
rate criterion each organization included as part text and client preferences. The inclusion of best
of the overall criteria for labeling an approach available evidence rather than empirically based
or strategy as either meeting the standards for practice acknowledges that there are some clini-
quality evidence. A total of 19 separate criteria cal problems that have not been extensively
were noted, including reasonable effect size, researched, allowing practitioners greater flexi-
use of experimental-control group, use of sin- bility in their practice when research evidence is
gle-case designs, statistical significance, ran- limited. When faced with a limited evidence
domized controlled trials (RCT), use of base, practitioners should evaluate the evidence
treatment manuals, replications, and scientific that is available for that problem (or similar prob-
results. The results showed a wide range across lems) and evaluate and weigh the relevancy (i.e.,
the sources incorporating these various criteria. formal and functional similarity of evidence with
For example, the criterion of replication was client characteristics, target behaviors, and other
incorporated into nine of the different sources, contextual variables) and certainty (i.e.,
102 T. Zane et al.

­ ethodological rigor, internal validity, quantity


m implement empirically supported interventions,
of replications) of the evidence when thus enhancing the decision-­making processes of
programming. practitioners and the efficacy of treatment for an
For example, in using Slocum and colleagues’ individual (Slocum et al., 2014). Second, the use
(2014) definition as a guide to programming for of evidence-based practices decreases the likeli-
reductions in self-injurious behavior, a practitio- hood of wasted resources (e.g., time, money;
ner would first review the research literature to Zane et al., 2008) on practices that are ineffective
identify interventions that have been used previ- or countertherapeutic (e.g., Beutler, 1998; Nelson
ously to reduce these behaviors. In their review, et al., 1999). Fad treatments are common, partic-
the authors identified weighted vests and func- ularly when working with individuals with ASD
tional communication training. The evidence and other intellectual and developmental disabili-
supporting weighted vests should result in the ties (Vyse, 2016). Therefore, the dissemination
practitioner being wary of this intervention (e.g., of evidence-based practices may help caregivers
Carter, 2005; Quigley et al., 2011; Stephenson & identify empirically supported interventions
Carter, 2009); however, the extensive literature quicker, thereby decreasing money and time
supporting functional communication training spent on ineffective interventions. Third, use of
should result in the practitioner coming to the evidence-based practices may promote quality
conclusion that functional communication train- services and reduce the variability in treatment
ing is effective in reducing self-injurious behav- and programming errors (Carnett, 1999; Handley
ior across a variety of individuals, functions of et  al., 1994). This, in turn, would increase pro-
behavior, and settings (e.g., Durand & Carr, gramming effectiveness of practitioners and out-
1991; Hagopian et al., 1998; Rooker et al., 2013). comes for an individual. Fourth, funders are more
Thus, the definition of evidence-based practice in likely to cover the costs of empirically supported
ABA should guide a practitioner in making the interventions (Rogers & Vismara, 2008), decreas-
best programming decisions for the individuals ing the cost of and increasing access to effective
with whom they work. services for more individuals.
There are many reasons why practitioners Despite the unprecedented advantages of EBP,
should adopt evidence-based practices (Foxx & the field of autism treatment is continually being
Mulick, 2016). In addition to government legisla- flooded with alternative or non-evidence-based
tion (e.g., Individuals with Disabilities Education practices (NEBPs; Weiss et  al., 2008) that (a)
Improvement Act, 2005; No Child Left Behind, lack empirical support, (b) are pseudoscientific in
2002) that mandates their use, professional and nature, and (c) are ineffective (Schreck & Miller,
certifying entities often include evidence-based 2010; Simpson, 2005; Vyse, 2016). Fad treat-
practice in their standards (Gast & Ledford, ments are best characterized as those whose pop-
2018). For example, in the Professional and ularity rapidly increases despite little to no
Ethical Compliance Code for Behavior Analysts scientific evidence, gains wide recognition or
(Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2014), use, and then fades away as a result of disproving
behavior analysts are held to relying on scientific research or, more often than not, the emergence
knowledge in clinical practice, and clients have a of a new fad (Vyse, 2016). For example, Ayres’
right to practices that are validated and based on (1972) created the theory of sensory integration,
empirical evidence (e.g., Van Houten et  al., in which behavior (all types, including learning,
1988). Thus, there are several advantages to the social skills, etc.) is directly related to an indi-
use of evidence-based practices. First, they allow vidual’s ability to process sensory input from the
for accountability of practitioners (Gast & environment (Lang et  al., 2012; Roley et  al.,
Ledford, 2018). That is, rather than solely relying 2007; Smith et al., 2016). Thus, sensory integra-
on clinical practice or previous experiences in tion therapy (SIT) seeks to enhance an individu-
which decisions may be made quickly al’s ability to process input from the environment
(McKibbon, 1998), practitioners are expected to by restoring neurological functioning (i.e.,
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 103

n­ ervous system; Roley et al., 2007; Smith et al., vices have sought guidance from a wide variety
2016). This improved functioning of the nervous of professionals including behavior analysts,
system (e.g., vestibular, tactile, and propriocep- teachers, occupational therapists, and physicians
tive systems) is believed to promote ideal (Schreck & Mazur, 2008). Due to the differing
responding (Lang et al., 2012). However, there is theoretical beliefs of these professionals, a prolif-
a substantial lack of scientific evidence support- eration of treatment options emerged, creating an
ing the philosophical underpinning of SIT (Lang almost buffet-like approach to autism treatment,
et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2016). Moreover, Devlin including scientifically supported and unsup-
et al. (2010) conducted a comparative analysis of ported treatments (Schreck & Mazur, 2008). An
a behavioral intervention and SIT in the treat- unfortunate result is the continual implementa-
ment of challenging behavior. Results indicated tion and promotion of NEBP treatments (Schreck
that the behavioral intervention was superior in & Mazur, 2008; Vyse, 2016).
reducing challenging behavior for all four par-
ticipants (Devlin et al., 2010). Furthermore, there
was little to no change in the frequency of chal- 7.1.2 Use of Evidence-Based
lenging behavior during the SIT condition, sug- Treatments by Service
gesting its ineffectiveness. Providers
Many consider the “ultimate fad treatment” to
be facilitated communication (FC). FC is a teach- In an effort to identify the procedures used by
ing strategy created in the late 1980s that utilizes Board Certified Behavior Analysts (BCBAs) and
augmentative and alternative communication Board Certified Assistant Behavior Analysts
(AAC) methods to assist individuals with intel- (BCaBAs), Schreck and Mazur (2008) surveyed
lectual and developmental disabilities (Hudson, 469 professionals nationwide. The online ques-
1995; Jacobson et  al., 2016). A fundamental tionnaire addressed various issues including (a)
belief of FC is that individuals with disabilities treatments currently being used for individuals
possess linguistic competence, that of which is with ASD, (b) treatments known to have been
unknown to the outside world, but becomes used by other professionals, (c) beliefs regarding
apparent with the assistance of a facilitator the principles underlying various treatments, and
(Jacobson et  al., 2016). More specifically, FC (d) professionals’ views on treatment effective-
requires the physical support of a facilitator to ness, ease of implementation, cost-effectiveness,
help an individual communicate via pointing to and empirical support. Results indicated that
or pressing letters, pictures, or objects on an aug- both BCBAs and BCaBAs have or are currently
mentative device (Hemsley et al., 2018). The end implementing all treatments, scientifically vali-
result, being a well-written document, or sophis- dated and unvalidated, included with the ques-
ticated poem, was believed to be the direct tionnaire. While it may seem encouraging that
expressions/thoughts of the person being assisted. ABA and discrete trial instruction (DTI) are
However, research over the past couple of included within the top five treatments used
decades has repeatedly demonstrated that FC (a) (98.7% and 91%, respectively), the continual use
lacks replicability across well-designed studies of NEBP treatments cannot go without notice.
(Eberlin et al., 1993; Jacobson et al., 2016), (b) Examples of NEBPs included floor time (14.9%),
relies on facilitator control instead of communi- sensory integration (16.4%), auditory integration
cation instead of client producing the responding training (1.1%), gentle teaching (2.6%), and
(Hemsley et al., 2018; Jacobson et al., 2016), and facilitated communication (6.4%). Schreck and
(3) has negative behavioral and social effects Mazur further noted an interesting correlation
(Boynton, 2012; Jacobson et al., 2016). between treatment selection and ease of imple-
As autism prevalence increases, the need for mentation. That is, “BCBAs decisions to use
evidence-based practice becomes imperative. [NEBP] treatments seemed to be dependent upon
Consequently, those in need of behavioral ser- the ease of implementation and cost effective-
104 T. Zane et al.

ness” (Schreck & Mazur, 2008, p. 210). Schreck procedure in coordination with behavioral ser-
et  al. (2016) conducted a 5-year follow-up to vices already being administered or attempting to
assess current treatment selections. This time, provide services independently, parents are often
participants included BCaBAs (n = 66), BCBAs left with the daunting task of treatment imple-
(n = 848), and Board Certified Behavior Analyst-­ mentation (Miller et  al., 2012). Green et  al.
Doctorals (n  =  136). Data again supported that (2006) surveyed 552 parents nationwide to iden-
notion that certified behavior analysts continue to tify current and previous treatments used with
use an assortment of fad treatments, all of which their child(ren) with ASD. Speech therapy was
lack empirical support, have been deemed inef- identified as the most commonly used interven-
fective, and have the potential to cause harm to tion (70%), followed by visual aids/schedules
those receiving the interventions (Schreck et al., (43.2%), sensory integration therapy (38.2%),
2016). and ABA (36.4%), respectively. Interestingly,
Unfortunately, comparable results were researchers noted a differentiation in the percent-
obtained following an analysis of instructional age of parents who implemented ABA based on
practices utilized within the education system the severity of the autism diagnosis. That is, 24%
(Burns & Ysseldyke, 2009; Hess et  al., 2007). of respondents implemented ABA with children
Hess et  al. (2007) administered surveys to 185 diagnosed with Asperger’s, while 80.5% imple-
teachers across the state of Georgia to identify mented the same procedures with children who
which treatments they were using with students have been diagnosed with severe autism (Green
with ASD.  Results suggested that fewer than et al., 2006). Data also showed that parents were
10% of the interventions used by respondents implementing an average of seven different inter-
were EBPs. The top five strategies used by this ventions at any given time, with 47 being the
population included (1) gentle teaching, (2) sen- highest number of treatments currently imple-
sory integration, (3) cognitive behavioral modifi- mented (Green et al., 2006). These results were
cation, (4) auditory integration training, and (5) also supported by Goin-Kochel et al. (2007) who
social stories (Hess et  al., 2007). Burns and noted that parents were, at the time of investiga-
Ysseldke (2009) noted similar results after sur- tion, implementing an average of four to six
veying several members of the National interventions simultaneously. Results obtained
Association of School Psychologists (i.e., 500 by Goin-Kochel and colleagues also indicated a
special education teachers and 1000 school psy- preference for alternative treatments such as sen-
chologists). More specifically, ABA was ranked sory integration (46.9%) over behavior-analytic
the fifth most-employed treatment for both pro- services (40.2%). Moreover, data showed a
fessions, proceeding less-preferred strategies decrease in the percentage of parents who cur-
including modality instructions, social skills rently used ABA. That is, data specify that while
training, and formative assessment (Burns & 55.2% of parents have tried ABA, only 40.2%
Ysseldke, 2009). Overall, the results suggested a currently utilize ABA interventions.
lack of commitment to EBP. Instead, the public
education system seemed to rely on a variety of
strategies, scientifically supported and 7.2  hy Do Parents Continue
W
unsupported. to Explore Non-evidence-­
Based Practices?

7.1.3 Use of Evidence-Based It is clear that parents and service providers con-
Procedures by Parents tinue to use and recommend procedures that are
of Children with Autism not supported to be effective through quality
research. To reach the goal of a wider use of
Parents are at the forefront of autism treatment; evidence-­based procedures, it may be important
whether or not they are asked to implement a to try to identify the conditions under which peo-
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 105

ple continue to select and implement fad (Schreck & Ramirez, 2016). These different con-
­treatments. Choosing to use a procedure, whether notations may make parents lean more toward
it be considered evidence-based or not, is influ- using an NEBP, in part because the language
enced by antecedent and consequent variables, paired with NEBPs is associated with cure, fast
some of which will be explored next. results, and effectiveness, in contrast to the words
paired with ABA interventions, such as no cure,
intensive, and 40 hours. In addition, parents have
7.2.1 Antecedent Variables that reported choosing treatment for ASD based on
Influence The Use of Fad media portrayal of treatment in movies and tele-
Treatments vision (Miller et al., 2012). In recent years, there
has been an increase in characters diagnosed with
7.2.1.1 Media autism in movies and television (e.g., Murray,
The influence of popular media could be one of 2016). Often, these characters are portrayed as
the possible explanations for why parents and loving, competent, intelligent, and able to have a
others continue to select NEBPs. ABA is rarely social and emotional connection with their loved
referred to in commonly accessible literature or ones. These characteristics are positive and
in non-behavior-analytic professional literature desired by parents of children with autism who
(Maurice et al., 2001). While a Google search for may not present these ways. Thus, parents who
“autism treatment” will yield promising results are exposed to these movies and television may
with pro-ABA sources (e.g., CDC, Autism make a connection between the strengths of the
Speaks, the Mayo Clinic, the Autism Science individual and any method, strategy, or tactic
Foundation), it will not take long to contact infor- noticed in the media. This influence on main-
mation about NEBPs. Following credible sources stream media can promise outcomes that are not
listed in a Google search, websites advertising always achievable for every individual with
fad treatments emerge. Another problem that autism. In addition to fiction characters portrayed
arises in analyzing commonly consumed media on television and movies, the news also reports
is that if ABA is listed as an effective treatment, autism treatments and spotlights “success sto-
then it may be alongside other non-evidence-­ ries.” Schreck and Ramirez (2016) found that
based procedures. For example, in an “autism over a 12-year period, airtime related to autism
treatment guide” published by Healthline (2018), treatment on two major news networks were
ABA is listed as the first treatment option; how- devoted more to NEBP than to EBP. Not only is
ever, it is then followed by NEBPs such as sen- the flood of information on NEBPs a problem;
sory integration therapy. Similarly, WebMD the verbiage used to describe effective treatments
(2019) promoted ABA in their “6 Therapies that and non-effective treatments is a cause for con-
Can Help Treat Autism” article; however, also cern, which may promote selection and use of
included in the list was therapeutic horseback rid- these procedures. In print media, newspapers and
ing, an NEBP.  This can make selecting an magazines have been found to recommend
evidence-­based procedure more difficult for par- NEBPs more frequently and in a more positive
ents when the information is presented in a con- light than EBPs. NEBPs received four positive
tiguous manner. comments (e.g., powerful healing, life altering)
The media undoubtedly plays a role in influ- to every one negative comment (e.g., controver-
encing society’s beliefs about autism treatments. sial, flawed, inconclusive; Schreck et al., 2013).
Positive comments for NEBPs consisted of state- These statements can have a powerful motiva-
ments such as “cure/heal,” “powerful tool,” “hold tional influence with parents who are trying to
hope,” and “near miraculous” (Schreck & select a treatment.
Ramirez, 2016). In contrast, less powerful There are also public figures that use the
descriptors were for EBPs including “getting bet- media to push their own agenda of supposed
ter,” “scientifically proven,” and “made progress” “treatments” for autism. Jenny McCarthy, famous
106 T. Zane et al.

anti-vaxxer, claims that her son’s autism was successful at addressing (Levy & Hyman, 2005).
cured through a combination of diet modifica- This could make NEBPs more appealing to fami-
tions (i.e., gluten-free, casein-free), supplemental lies if a “cure” is the family’s goal, as opposed to
vitamins, a detox of metals, and the use of anti-­ acquiring adaptive skills or addressing the core
fungal medication to treat overgrown yeast in his diagnostic characteristics of ASD.  Even if it is
intestines. In her words, these treatments enabled not a familial goal, it may be an underlying or
him to learn skills that he was unable to do unspoken wish; it is natural for families, espe-
because he was “frozen in autism” (McCarthy & cially in the early years after diagnosis, to hope
Carrey, 2008). None of these treatments that she that autism could be eradicated.
cites as the reasons for her son’s progress are Dardennes et  al. (2011) found that parental
considered evidence-based practices, but they do belief about what caused their child’s autism
draw on families’ hope that “recovery” from diagnosis impacted the decision of treatment
autism is possible and point parents to interven- options. Parents who believed autism was caused
tions with no evidence of effectiveness. by a traumatic childhood experience were less
likely to implement behavioral-based interven-
7.2.1.2 Lack of Knowledge About tions, while those who believed an illness during
Etiology pregnancy was the issue were more likely to turn
Families may seek alternative and NEBPs to prescription medication as a treatment option.
because of a lack of information on why ASD Dardennes and colleagues also found that parents
occurs (e.g., Matson et al., 2013). To date, there is who believed their child’s diagnosis was caused
no known cause of autism (e.g., Hodges et  al., by an allergen or a chemical imbalance were
2020). There are certain risk factors that have more likely to use diet and vitamin therapies as a
been identified, but the relationship is mainly treatment. Surveys (e.g., Lay-Beliefs about
correlational, not causal (Newschaffer et  al., Autism Questionnaire; Furnham & Buck, 2003;
2007). Some risk factors include certain genetic Revised Illness-Perception Questionnaire; Moss-­
or chromosomal conditions, having a sibling who Morris et al., 2002) have also found that parental
has autism, and advanced maternal or paternal decision-making may be influenced by the seri-
age at birth (Centers for Disease Control and ousness of the disorder. Parents of individuals
Prevention, 2020). Additionally, autism has been who are more severely impacted by autism have
demonstrated to affect all racial, ethnic, gender, been found to be more likely to use behavioral-­
and socioeconomic groups (Centers for Disease based therapy, while those who believe their
Control and Prevention, 2020). This lack of child suffers from cyclical behavior patterns are
understanding of causality may create an avenue more likely to turn to medication (Al Anbar et al.,
for families to seek as many treatment options as 2010). Those who attribute autism to hereditary
possible. Some parents may be quite upset if they patterns were more likely to turn to vitamins and
believed that autism could be prevented. Other other metabolic treatments (Al Anbar et  al.,
parents may, in some ways, find it easier to cope 2010).
with the diagnosis of autism if they identify with
a specific cause that could not be avoided or that 7.2.1.3 Promises of Cure
can be treated. The lack of a known cause of One of the hallmark characteristics of antiscience
autism may create the conditions for a family is the promotion of quick, fast, and easy results
member to identify unproven causes for regres- (Green, 1996). Parents have reported seeking out
sion such as an external toxic exposure rather complementary and alternative medicine due to
than concluding it is a symptom of autism (Levy the emotional comfort and support from these
& Hyman, 2005). EBPs are effective at treating providers (Hemsley et  al., 2018). Another hall-
the symptoms of autism; however, these treat- mark of pseudoscientific and antiscience
ments do not eradicate the cause of the autism approaches is to provide extensive testimonials
diagnosis, which many NEBPs may claim to be about success stories (Green, 1996). The diagno-
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 107

sis of ASD is undoubtedly a stressor for parents, invasive for the individual and is seen as a quick
and one way of coping may be to seek out provid- fix. The problem with this “it can’t hurt” mental-
ers who appear more in tune with parental feel- ity is young children with autism do not have
ings (Hanson et  al., 2007). Finding others who time to waste on ineffective treatments. The real-
feel the same way (stressed, anxious, etc.) may ity is that there are data showing that NEBPs
result in parents connecting with those who pro- often result in wasted time, financial loss, and
vide such testimonials, and this reliance that pull pain (e.g., Mitka, 2008; Zane et al., 2008). This
at heart strings can be seen across several exam- time spent on NEBPs can delay access to services
ples of NEBPs. Son-Rise therapy’s website that are able to help (Zane et al., 2008). There is
(Autism Treatment Center of America, 2021) a plethora of data and research to support the use
features four pages of testimonials from parents of early intensive behavioral intervention to
whose children have “recovered” from autism. achieve the best possible outcomes for children
The videos all tout “recovery” and talk about how with autism (Zane et al., 2008). By pushing lim-
the Son-Rise program saved their child’s life and ited resources into ineffective interventions, par-
drastically reduced, if not eliminated, all signs of ents can be doing more harm than good (Smith &
autism with their child. One video even acknowl- Antolovich, 2000). Smith and Antolovich (2000)
edges that prior to the use of Son-Rise, the par- found that parents were more likely to indicate
ents had no emotional connection with their that any treatment is more beneficial than harm-
child. Son-Rise is an NEBP with no research evi- ful, even when there has been little to no change
dence demonstrating its effectiveness. However, in behavior. This could be due to a response bias,
the creators of the program appeal to the emo- in which parents are not willing to say that they
tional side of an autism diagnosis and offer hope have wasted time, money, and resources, on inef-
and a cure. fective treatment. Nonetheless, parents often
continue these treatments, sometimes because
7.2.1.4 Lack of Knowledge About they already have funding for alternate treat-
Quality of Evidence ments and sometimes because they feel that any
When researching treatments for autism, it can be benefits derived from the treatment are worth-
difficult and overwhelming to identify what is while. This is a cause for concern given the delay
and is not evidence-based, particularly when a that can be associated with using ineffective
treatment is recommended by another profes- treatments (Smith, 2016).
sional. There are numerous NEBPs that are rec-
ommended by other professionals, such as 7.2.1.5 Lack of Access to Services
therapeutic touch (Rosa et al., 1998; Vyse, 2016), Based on Evidence
sensory integration therapy (e.g., Williams & A reason why parents may use any treatment
Shellenberger, 1996), and facilitated communi- available may be difficulties related to accessing
cation (e.g., Cardinal et al., 1996). quality services. Maurice et  al. (2001) docu-
Parents and service providers may hold onto mented three parents’ perspectives on the inade-
the belief that anything is worth a try and even if quacies of ABA-based interventions. The most
benefits are unlikely, there is benefit in trying daunting task from these parental perspectives
every available option to help their child (Levy & was the quest to find a qualified professional. A
Hyman, 2005; Smith et al., 2016). This seems to parent who is meeting with various professionals
be especially true of treatments such as sensory to hopefully build a team of providers must be
integration and sensory diets where the “treat- able to sift through what is opinion and anecdote
ment” itself is rather benign (Smith et al., 2016). versus what is evidence-based (Maurice et  al.,
For example, Levy and Hyman (2005) found the 2001). This requires a parent to have knowledge
gluten-/casein-free diet to be a popular treatment of various levels of scientific evidence and the
option for the same reason. The treatment was time and capability to comb through a myriad of
viewed as a healthy approach to living that is not information. In the opinion of some parents,
108 T. Zane et al.

placement and services are based on what is factor that could account for a lack of profes-
­readily available to the school district, and often sionals in each area is the proximity to a col-
fails to meet the needs of the learner, but instead lege and/or to a supervision site which is
promises that the school professionals will necessary to complete certification (Deochand
attempt to fit the student into whatever services & Fuqua, 2016). The lack of access to a quali-
and programs currently exist at the school fied behavior analyst could lead to the use of
(Maurice et  al., 2001). There is a documented alternative treatment options, including non-
disparity between what is an appropriate service evidence-­based or fad treatments.
and what is an optimal service (Dymond et  al.,
2007). Thus, having the right professional to
mentor and guide the parent through the maze of 7.2.2 Consequent Variables
services is of utmost importance. Without a pro- Influencing the Use of Fad
fessional whose thinking is based in science and Treatments
the worldview of behaviorism, a parent may be
less likely to select quality services based on There are consequent variables that could defi-
empirical evidence. Through the qualitative and nitely influence parents and other caregivers to
quantitative use of open-ended surveys, Dymond use NEBP.  One is response effort. Some thera-
et al. (2007) found that parents have cited a need pies take more time and effort to implement than
for an increase in the availability and accessibil- others. For example, if a parent believed in the
ity of services. effectiveness of aromatherapy (e.g., Solomons,
A lack of accessible ABA-based services 2005), they would purchase special oils and have
could be due to various factors including the their child smell them throughout the day. This
increased population of individuals with ASD, approach is much less effortful than implement-
the lack of qualified behavior analysts, and a lack ing, say, a 40-hour per week intensive discrete
of coordinated efforts in terms of service delivery trial approach based on the principles of behavior
(Dymond et  al., 2007). These issues become analysis. Mackintosh et al. (2012) found that par-
more pronounced in rural areas where children ents cited both time and effort as negative detri-
are less likely to have access to pediatric and ments to using EBPs, specifically ABA.  One
mental health services. Children in these areas parent stated, “ABA seems to be the only viable
are also less likely to receive an early or efficient option, but it is an extremely rigorous and
diagnosis of ASD (Murphy & Ruble, 2012). A demanding form of schooling... And the expense
lack of an early diagnosis may lead to parents and is, of course, astronomical” (Mackintosh et  al.,
service providers, who are unfamiliar with the 2012, p. 57). While another was concerned about
availability of EBPs, to choose from among time and effort, “I wish the behavior system we
available services, even those with little to no used wasn’t so time intensive on my part”
empirical support of effectiveness. (Mackintosh et al., 2012, p. 57). These are note-
Within the United States, there are huge worthy concerns raised by parents that should be
disparities from state to state in the number of taken into consideration by behavior analysts.
available certified behavior analysts. For Another variable that could function as a posi-
example, in Idaho, there are 74 BCBAs; in tive consequence for implementing NEBP is the
Louisiana, there are 398; and in New Jersey, attention provided to the implementor of the
there are 2081 (Behavior Analyst Certification NEBP.  Parents could receive individualized
Board, 2020a). Historically, rural areas have attention and displays of sympathy for using
difficulty recruiting mental health profession- NEBP. Furthermore, the promise of possibly con-
als. This could be due to several factors such necting socially with the child with autism and
as lower pay rates, higher caseloads, and promises of progress and hope offered by the
potential for dual relationships inside a smaller proponents of NEBP all could reinforce the par-
community (Murphy & Ruble, 2012). Another ent for selecting that particular NEBP.
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 109

7.3  hy Do Behavior Analysts


W ates the degree of scientific integrity of research.
Continue to Use Non-­ Without competence is experimental design and
evidence-­Based Practices? all that entails (e.g., measurement reliability,
internal and external validity, etc.), an individual
The Professional and Ethical Compliance Code may be more likely to interpret results of a study
for Behavior Analysts clearly outlines that all as causally related to the independent variables
BCBAs must use science when delivering ser- manipulated. In autism research, that suggests a
vices or engaging in other professional behavior possibility of a Type I error (Martella et  al.,
(e.g., section 2.09, Behavior Analyst Certification 1999), meaning that treatments that appear to be
Board, 2014). The code also states that behavior working are in fact not. At minimum, quality
analysts do not implement non-behavior-analytic research must include objective definitions of the
interventions (Behavior Analyst Certification dependent and independent variables, an experi-
Board, 2014). Even with a clear outline of expec- mental design that adequately controls for threats
tations for responsible delivery of behavior anal- to both internal and external validity, the ability
ysis, behavior analysts have strayed from the to replicate the study, and technological and con-
compliance code resulting in ethical violations. ceptually systematic procedures (Zane et  al.,
In questionnaires completed by BCBAs, Schreck 2008). Behavior analysts need to have repertoires
and colleagues (2008, 2016) found that BCBAs that permit appropriately interpreting what is
reported to use auditory integration training, quality research when reviewing literature to
facilitated communication training, gentle teach- make treatment decisions. Kubina et  al. (2017)
ing, and music therapy. Although these fad treat- reviewed 4313 line graphs from 11 different
ments were not reported to be used as commonly behavior-analytic journals to analyze the quality
as ABA-based interventions, the fact remains that of the graphical representation of the data. The
this is concerning for the field. authors found that there was a high degree of
variability in how the graphs were constructed
and labeled. Differences in aspects of a line graph
7.3.1 Antecedent Variables that in particular the scale, the labels, and the axis can
Influence The Use of Fad skew the visual interpretation of the graph.
Treatments Behavior analysts should keep this in mind when
critically reviewing research and analyzing the
7.3.1.1 Lack of Sufficient Education visual display of data. Visual interpretation of
and Training in Behavior data has been recognized as a subjective behav-
Analysis and Science ior, and research has been conducted to develop
A lack of comprehensive education and training more structured criteria for interpretation of data.
in behavior analysis could point to one possible Hagopian et al. (1997) used a panel of experts to
reason why behavior analysts select and imple- create structured criteria for how to interpret
ment NEBPs. Although there is accreditation and functional analysis data. These criteria consist of
verified course sequences through the Association how to place criterion lines, how to check for
for Behavior Analysis International, there is less trends, and how to interpret data to ascertain
structure around more concentrated aspects of function. Additionally, close consideration of
graduate school courses (Pastrana et  al., 2018). appropriate use of experimental design is war-
As an example, consider training in research ranted. Group designs are not widely used in
methodology. Behavior analysis is based on sci- behavior analysis, in part due to lack of focus on
ence and the scientific approach toward the study individual change and a greater focus on change
of behavior. An important component of the of the average. However, behavior analysts need
coursework for becoming a behavior analyst is to be able to analyze all types of experimental
studying experimental design. This needs to be design in order to effectively determine if
the basis for which every behavior analyst evalu- research is valid and appropriate (Smith, 2012).
110 T. Zane et al.

Being able to critically analyze experimental begin practicing in ways that involve skills and
design is one way to avoid believing that an competence currently not in their repertoire. With
NEBP is responsible for changes in behavior or the increasing incidence of autism, there is a con-
learning. comitant increase in the demand for services and
In addition to coursework, behavior analysts treatment options. There is reliable documenta-
must accrue supervision hours to qualify for cer- tion that parents and other consumers have strong
tification (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, opinions about the usefulness and preference for
2021). Within the supervision context, behavior different therapeutic approaches (e.g., Green
analysts demonstrate competency in a number of et  al., 2006). A behavior analyst may consider
areas important to clinical competence, such as offering treatment options (for which adequate
case conceptualization, problem-solving, and training may not have been completed) outside of
decision-making repertoires. A major area of behavior analysis with the goal of providing more
competence focuses on practicing as a scientist, treatment for more individuals who otherwise
determining what constitutes an EBP, and using might face very long delays before accessing
and advocating for these types of treatments. treatment (Brodhead et al., 2018). For example, a
Behavior analysts in a supervisory context have behavior analyst, realizing there is a demand for
their skills shaped and acquired through quality solving sleep disorders, may attempt sleep inter-
supervision, which must include consistent feed- ventions even though they had no specific study
back and development of skills to a mastery crite- or training in this particular area. Another poten-
rion (Brodhead et  al., 2018). Some of the key tial factor is the perception of the behavior ana-
components of quality supervision include focus- lyst feeling more qualified than other professionals
ing on collaboration and ethics, outlining perfor- to deliver any service, due to having certification
mance expectations, evaluating performance in behavior analysis (Brodhead et al., 2018).
consistently, and maintaining confidentiality.
There is a general assumption that when super- 7.3.1.3 Perceived Permission
visees acquire quality supervision, they will in to Use NEBP
turn become effective practitioners in the field of Another factor that could lead to the use of
behavior analysis. Turner et  al. (2016) also NEBPs is the allowance by the BACB for BCBAs
emphasized conducting baseline assessments of to use a disclaimer when engaging in NEBPs. In
trainee’s skills and through various teaching the Professional and Ethical Compliance Code
methods honing those skills. The intersection of for Behavior Analysts, section 8.01 states:
NEBP and supervision is this – poor supervision Behavior analysts do not implement non-behavior-­
may increase the chances of behavior analysts analytic interventions. Non-behavior-analytic ser-
not being sufficiently trained in science and vices may only be provided within the context of
science-­based practices, thus more potentially non-behavior-analytic education, formal training,
and credentialing. Such services must be clearly
influenced by the flimflam of NEBP. distinguished from their behavior-analytic prac-
tices and BACB certification by using the follow-
7.3.1.2 Practicing Outside Scope ing disclaimer: “These interventions are not
of Competence behavior-analytic in nature and are not covered by
my BACB credential.” The disclaimer should be
Behavior analysts who practice outside of their placed alongside the names and descriptions of all
scope of competence may also be at greater risk non-behavior-analytic interventions. (Behavior
for implementing NEBPs. While the Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2014)
Analyst Certification Board (BACB) dictates in
the Professional and Ethical Compliance Code Arguably, this disclaimer allows behavior ana-
for Behavior Analysts that all BCBAs must prac- lysts to engage in non-behavior-analytic treat-
tice within their scope of competence (Behavior ments, including NEBPs if it is included in their
Analyst Certification Board, 2014), there are fac- service description and if they possess the appro-
tors that could influence behavior analysts to priate training. This disclaimer is in conflict with
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 111

section 6 of the Professional and Ethical considering behavior analysts who admit using
Compliance Code for Behavior Analysts, which NEBP (Schreck et  al., 2016). When looking at
mandates that behavior analysts must put the allocation of response effort across evidence-­
field of applied behavior analysis above all other based treatment plans and non-evidence-based
training (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, treatment plans, it is likely that there is a greater
2014). Allowing behavior analysts to add the dis- response effort in the implementation of ABA
claimer could create dual worldviews and treat- procedures. As such, if response effort is a criti-
ment that is governed under non-scientific cal variable, parents may select NEBPs over
principles. EBPs that may require more response effort for
all parties involved.
7.3.1.4 Consumer Preference
A final variable to discuss as an influence on 7.3.2.2 Reinforcement for Using NEBP
behavior analysts using NEBP is consumer pref- Since parents and caregivers continue to use
erence. Many behavior analysts offer themselves NEBP, there must be some payoff for doing so.
as consultants to families to assist in the educa- That is, the users of NEBP must obtain some sort
tion and development of their children. As noted of reinforcement for trying and continuing such
above, parents often had specific notions as to procedures. What might that be? One possible
what type of therapy they want. It is logical to source of reinforcement would be perceived
assume that the more therapies and approaches progress on the part of the individual receiving
one can offer to parents, the more marketable a the NEBP.  The emphasis here is on perceived
behavior analyst is likely to be. To put it another progress. As noted in other sources (e.g., Smith,
way, the more therapies a behavior analyst can 2016), the quality of evidence varies a great deal.
offer to a prospective client, the more likely it is Often, if there are in fact positive changes in
that the client will hire that behavior analyst. behavior once an NEBP has been implemented, it
Thus, behavior analysts can be influenced to offer is not clear whether that procedure is solely
more than just evidence-based practices. responsible for the improvement or if there could
be other potentially explanatory factors. In edu-
cation and clinical work, often multiple treat-
7.3.2 Consequent Variables ments are used simultaneously, and the degree to
Influencing the Use of Fad which each resulted in individual progress
Treatments remains to be determined. Thus, consumers of
NEBP may not be able to discriminate as to
7.3.2.1 Response Effort where the improvement is coming from.
Allen and Warzak (2000) considered the rein- Additionally, improvement noted by caregivers,
forcing consequences of parents and service pro- teachers, or others may not be actual improve-
viders selecting treatments based on the effort ments. One part of scientific rigor is the careful
involved in providing that treatment. For exam- measurement of behavior. Studies have shown
ple, a parent of a child with autism is most likely that without careful operational definitions and
managing a complex situation filled with multi- well-planned measurement systems, the wrong
ple professionals, financially difficult decisions, impressions and results of the data could result
different intervention recommendations, various (Freeman, 2016). Thus, implementors of NEBP
medications, and an influx of concerns that arise may attribute real progress to NEBP when in fact
on a frequent basis. Parents may seek solutions such progress could be due to other variables, or
for their child’s behavior that require less effort implementors may not actually see progress, but
than other solutions. In fact, response effort has believe it to be so erroneously.
been found to be one of the most critical elements Other potential sources of reinforcement for
of parental adherence to a treatment plan (Allen using NEBP could include the alleged savings of
& Warzak, 2000) and is partly explanatory when time and effort. For example, purchasing gluten-­
112 T. Zane et al.

free foods could be considered simpler than many menting an NEBP (Brodhead, 2015; Chadwell
hours of ABA therapy each week. Parents and et al., 2019; DiGennaro Reed et al., 2017; Leko
caregivers may also find using NEBP to be rein- et al., 2019; McDonald & Reed, 2018; Walmsley
forced with supportive feedback and attention. & Baker, 2019). Brodhead (2015) proposed a
For example, if a professional suggests using model for making collaborative intervention
some type of intervention, and the parent in fact decisions within interdisciplinary teams. Both
implement the said intervention, then the parent DiGennaro Reed et al. (2017) and McDonald and
will likely be supported by that professional, and Reed (2018) provided lists of questions practitio-
perhaps others will approve as well. ners can ask to help guide decision-making sur-
These perceived reinforcers for using NEBP rounding the implementation of interventions,
often compete with incentives for using scientifi- including NEBPs. Questions addressed goodness
cally validated treatments. For example, ABA-­ of fit for the client, degree of evidence, resources
based interventions and procedures used in available, and implementation-specific details,
effective treatment plans rarely produce immedi- among others. Leko et  al. (2019) proposed that
ate change in behavior and cannot often compete similar factors be considered by special educa-
with other more immediate reinforcers for ser- tors when selecting interventions. Walmsley and
vice providers and parents (Allen & Warzak, Baker (2019) suggested thoroughly evaluating
2000). Undoubtedly, negative reinforcement the state of the evidence for non-behavioral inter-
plays a key role in adherence to ABA-based treat- ventions and advanced one formula for how to do
ment plans, as in the moment it may seem more so that involves “searching the literature,” “rec-
appealing to reinforce challenging behavior to ognizing and evaluating the common properties
escape the aversive consequences the behavior of fad treatments,” “distinguishing quality of evi-
has on the parent. dence,” and “ascertaining behavioral mecha-
Using NEBPs may also be reinforcing for behav- nisms of action” (p.  680). Data reported by
ior analysts for several reasons. Schreck et  al. Chadwell et al. (2019) reminded practitioners to
(2016) identified verbal praise, ease of use/cost- consider consumer preferences pertaining to the
effectiveness, monetary gain, research results, intervention process relative to treatment effec-
and observed success to all be possible reinforc- tiveness. Assuming a practitioner is up to date on
ers that maintain the use of NEBPs by behavior the literature regarding EBPs (Behavior Analyst
analysts. In this study, behavior analysts indi- Certification Board, 2014; Walmsley & Baker,
cated they primarily used unestablished treat- 2019) and has a working understanding of what
ments or ineffective or harmful treatments questions to ask to guide their clinical decision-­
because of verbal praise from the client’s family making surrounding the use of NEBPs both
or school/teacher or because they witnessed suc- within and outside of interdisciplinary teams
cess during the use of the said treatments. (Brodhead, 2015; DiGennaro Reed et al., 2017;
McDonald & Reed, 2018), what are some of the
conditions under which they may consider imple-
7.4  hat Parents and Providers
W menting an NEBP?
Should Do When There Is Perhaps the most likely occasion for which
a Lack of Access to EBPs? practitioners may consider implementing NEBPs
is when there are insufficient EBPs to inform
Despite the growing body of EBPs available to treatment for a particular individual, but there is
autism service providers, most will experience strong reason to believe practices deemed
conditions under which implementation of evidence-­based with other populations will be
NEBPs may be inevitable (Shawler et al., 2018; effective. For example, when providing interven-
Weiss et al., 2008). Of late, several scholars have tion to a child with Rett syndrome, a provider
offered guidance applicable to situations in which might draw from evidence-based practices for
a behavioral practitioner is considering imple- children diagnosed with ASD. Similarly, practi-
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 113

tioners may consider implementing NEBP when


Step 1:
all existing EBPs have been demonstrated to be
Assess competence
ineffective for a specific behavior exhibited by a
particular individual in a particular environment.
For instance, if a service provider is targeting
reduced vocal stereotypy that occurs at high rates Competent Not competent
at school and impedes client and peer learning,
and all evidence-based interventions designed for
a school environment produced insufficient Accept the case Decline the
decrease in the target behavior, they may con- and proceed to case (and report
sider implementing more restrictive procedures Step 2 if needed) or
consider other
considered evidence-based within clinical set- intervenons
tings. In this scenario, an interventionist would
have conducted comprehensive assessments
aimed at identifying function-based interventions Find a mentor or
as well as thorough reviews of the literature at obtain training
each decision point during treatment planning
and progress monitoring to ensure no evidence-­ Fig. 7.1  Step 1 of the decision tree to guide ethical evalu-
based interventions were overlooked. ation of non-evidence-based practices
When consent for a particular EBP is not pro-
vided, but consent is provided for a treatment with important to note that steps presented in the deci-
emerging evidence (i.e., an NEBP), some practi- sion trees represent but a particular configuration
tioners may consider implementing the treatment of presumedly many paths. Beginning with Step
with emerging evidence. Service provision for 1 of Fig. 7.1, prior to engaging in any practice of
individuals diagnosed with autism typically behavior analysis, service providers assess
involves coordinated care among service provid- whether they are competent to do so (Behavior
ers such as behavior analysts, teachers, speech Analyst Certification Board, 2014; see PECCs
and language pathologists, occupational thera- 1.02 and 2.05). Assessing competence involves
pists, physical therapists, and physicians, to name self-reflection and questioning to determine
just a few, to promote skill generalization and whether one has received sufficient education,
improve outcomes. As discussed by Brodhead training, mentoring, and/or supervised experi-
(2015), collaboration among ancillary service ence in a particular subject area, target behavior,
providers may result in the need to evaluate population, or technique to intervene success-
NEBPs (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, fully (Alligood & Gravina, 2020; Brodhead et al.,
2020a, b). In any of the above cases, it is assumed 2018). When an individual lacks competence,
that practitioners have conducted assessments to they should either obtain supervision by a com-
guide their search for an appropriate intervention petent mentor or develop competence through
(Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2020a, b; documented training prior to proceeding
see PECC 3.01) and have thoroughly reviewed the (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2014; see
evidence (or lack of evidence) in support of the PECCs 1.02 and 1.03). If competence is not
NEBP they are considering implementing (and, obtained through a supervising mentor or train-
thus, evaluating) prior to developing an imple- ing, the practitioner is encouraged to either pur-
mentation plan (Behavior Analyst Certification sue other interventions for which they meet
Board, 2014; see PECCs 1.03 and 2.09). minimum competency requirements or ethically
There are some additional steps that should decline the case or that aspect of it, instead refer-
also be taken to ethically evaluate an NEBP, as ring the client to a competent service provider
illustrated in the decision trees presented in (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2014; see
Figs. 7.1, 7.2, and 7.3 and discussed below. It is PECCs 2.15 and 4.11).
114 T. Zane et al.

Fig. 7.2  Step 2 of the


Step 2:
decision tree to guide
ethical evaluation of Assess the risk of
non-evidence-based benefit and harm
practices

Moderate to high risk Low risk of harm to


of harm to client or client or others and
others likely to benefit

Decline the case (and


Assess whether the non
report if needed) or
evidence-based
consider other
intervenon is
intervenons
conceptually systemac

Not conceptually Conceptually


systemac and/or systemac and socially
socially valid valid

Develop a plan for if


the intervenon is
unsuccessful

Document concerns
and proceed
to Step 3

After the interventionist is deemed competent, assess the extent to which the NEBP is conceptu-
a plausible next step is to assess the likelihood of ally systematic with a behavioral worldview
benefit and risk of harm to the client and others in (Leaf et  al., 2020b). Interventions that can be
the environment, as depicted in Step 2 of Fig. 7.2 operationalized and described in terms of the
(Bailey & Burch, 2016). If the NEBP is only underlying behavioral mechanisms may be con-
likely to produce modest benefit to the client, it sidered conceptually systematic (Baer et  al.,
may be best to focus resources elsewhere. When 1968; Walmsley & Baker, 2019). Behavior ana-
an NEBP poses a moderate to high risk of harm lysts are obligated to provide conceptually sys-
to the client or others, considering other interven- tematic interventions, and practitioners of
tions with lower risk of harm or ethically declin- behavior analysis are strongly encouraged to do
ing the case and referring to a qualified provider so; thus, when considering implementing NEBPs,
may be the responsible course of action. If it is service providers should either evaluate the effec-
probable a harmful intervention will be imple- tiveness of NEBPs that are conceptually system-
mented or is already being implemented, the atic for which they are minimally competent to
behavior analyst has an ethical obligation to implement or ethically decline the case, referring
report the case to the relevant authorities the client to a competent practitioner (Behavior
(Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2014; see Analyst Certification Board, 2014; see PECCs
PECC 7.02b). When the likelihood of benefit is 2.15 and 4.11).
considered high and the risk of harm is consid- A crucial element of any effective behavior
ered low, the behavior analyst may proceed to plan is having feasible alternative plans available
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 115

Fig. 7.3  Step 3 of the


Step 3:
decision tree to guide
ethical evaluation of Request informed
consent to evaluate
non-evidence-based
practices

Consent to evaluate Consent to evaluate


not provided provided

Decline the case (and Employ single-case


report if needed) or research design
consider other methodology to
intervenons rigorously assess the
non evidence-based
intervenon

Request informed
consent to
disseminate

Consent to
Consent to
disseminate not
disseminate provided
provided

Do not disseminate Disseminate

if needed (Zimmerman, 2020). Such an approach assessments that could be conducted or experts
is borrowed from the medical model, and who could be consulted if needed.
although there is likely variability among behav- Documenting concerns is the next critical
ior analysts in how treatment recommendations step, even for conceptually systematic non-­
are provided to relevant stakeholders, one method evidence-­based interventions within a practitio-
involves discussing a few suggested treatment ner’s area of competence. First, service providers
options, perceived goodness of fit, likelihood of should implement effective, evidence-based
success, possible negative effects, and implemen- procedures and evaluate any variables that could
tation considerations specific to each and decid- affect intervention (Behavior Analyst
ing together which to implement (Shepherd et al., Certification Board, 2014, PECCs 2.09a and
2011). When evaluating an NEBP, it is essential 2.09d, respectively) as well as meticulously doc-
to have contingency plans in place to prevent ument their work (Behavior Analyst Certification
interruption of services if the NEBP being evalu- Board, 2014, PECC 2.10). Second, service pro-
ated is deemed ineffective (e.g., Cihon et  al., viders should obtain informed consent prior to
2020), particularly given the possibility that assessing (Behavior Analyst Certification Board,
implementation of NEBPs may disrupt continued 2014, PECC 3.03) and intervening (Behavior
use of EBPs (Pellecchia et al., 2020). Contingency Analyst Certification Board, 2014, PECC 4.02).
plans may not be constrained to alternative inter- By documenting concerns regarding an NEBP,
ventions, but may include, for example, further not only is the practitioner meeting ethical stan-
116 T. Zane et al.

dards of best practice, but they may also be more be discontinued if ineffective (Behavior Analyst
likely to communicate all potential concerns to Certification Board, 2014; see PECC 2.15, 4.11).
relevant stakeholders and obtain better informed Regardless of whether the NEBP was effective or
consent. ineffective, it is important to disseminate the
As illustrated in Step 3 of Fig. 7.3, obtaining results (with consent from the consumer)
consent to evaluate an NEBP for research pur- (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2014; see
poses is also required (Behavior Analyst PECC 6.02). Publishing findings on the results of
Certification Board, 2014, PECC 9.03). studies examining the effectiveness of NEBP,
Practitioners may want to obtain written consent even those with questionable demonstration of
not only for evaluating the NEBP but also for dis- experimental control (Tincani & Travers, 2018),
seminating the results obtained; however, they will aid practitioners’ efforts to implement EBPs
may wait to request consent to disseminate after while decreasing publication bias and its down-
the evaluation of the NEBP has concluded. Once stream effects within the service delivery sector
consent to evaluate an NEBP is obtained (Tincani & Travers, 2019).
(Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2014; see
PECC 9.03), along with review board approval
where necessary (Valentino & Juanico, 2020), 7.5  ow to Further Promote
H
the practitioner may proceed to evaluate the Implementation of EBPs
NEBP using a single-case research design in Autism Treatment
(SCRD; Kazdin, 2011).
As it is beyond the scope of this chapter to Given the critical importance of using procedures
discuss details pertaining to single-case research and practices that are based on scientifically
methodology, interested readers are encouraged derived, evidence-based procedures in autism
to examine the abundance of resources on SCRD treatment, considerations must be given to ways
(e.g., Dallery & Raiff, 2014; Kazdin, 2011; to further spread the adoption of such strategies
Ledford et  al., 2018; Perone & Hursh, 2013). and approaches. The information presented
Having said that, certain features of SCRD that above discussed factors influencing the selection
are particularly well suited for initial investiga- of different treatment options, both evidence-­
tions of the effects of NEBPs warrant mention. based and non-evidence-based. There are several
First, SCRDs can detect causal relations between specific strategies that can be put into place that
variables within a single individual, making them increase the selection of empirically derived
ideal to assess the effectiveness of a particular practices.
NEBP for a specific consumer (Perone & Hursh,
2013). Second, SCRDs can be implemented rela-
tively quickly with few to any additional 7.5.1 Consensus on What
resources beyond those already required for Constitutes Quality Evidence
intervention implementation and can be moni-
tored using visual analysis of data collected Currently, there are differing criteria various
(Barton et  al., 2018; Wolfe et  al., 2019). Third, organizations set for what constitutes scientifi-
SCRDs can be flexibly combined and adapted as cally based evidence (see Ledford et  al., 2018;
needed in real time in response to data collected Odom et  al., 2010; Zane & Hanson, 2008). It
(Ledford et  al., 2020; Perone & Hursh, 2013). would be helpful if these organizations could
There are numerous exceptional examples of the agree upon a specific set of criteria adopted uni-
use of SCRD to assess the effectiveness of NEBP, versally by all. Professionals and scientists must
many of which are discussed above as well as in agree to what constitutes rigorous and science-­
Foxx and Mulick (2016). based standards for quality evidence, standards
After the NEBP has been evaluated, the inter- for research designs that control for internal and
vention may be continued if effective and must external validity, and procedures for measure-
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 117

ment and data collection that promote accuracy Part of this information campaign should
in measurement. If such standards for quality evi- include detailed presentations about the poten-
dence were adopted, then perhaps disciplines tial dangers and disadvantages of using treat-
would be influenced by these common standards ments not based on science and evidence. Much
and thus promote a more widely adopted set of has been written about the emotional, financial,
criteria, resulting in more common agreement as and time costs of ineffective treatments (e.g.,
to procedures that are and are not Kay, 2016; Smith, 2016; Smith & Antolovich,
evidence-based. 2000; Vyse, 2016; Zane et  al., 2008). These
variables need to be considered when choosing
from numerous autism treatments. More aware-
7.5.2 Broadly Disseminate ness of these potential issues could decrease the
Evidence-Based Information selection of treatments that seem promising,
on Autism helpful, and not harmful and sway parents and
service providers toward evidence-based
The myths about why autism occurs and the solu- approaches.
tions to this condition need to be effectively
countered and neutralized through education
based on science and current research. 7.5.3 Increase Availability
Professionals who give advice to parents of of Services Based on Evidence
newly diagnosed children – such as pediatricians,
social workers, psychologists, and educators  – If there were more access to services based on
need to be given up-to-date information, based on science and evidence, parents and service provid-
science, about what is known concerning the eti- ers would be more likely to select such services.
ology and prognosis. Most importantly, factual The Behavior Analyst Certification Board
information regarding the importance of treat- (2020a, b) promotes the training and certification
ments based on objective data and quality evi- of professionals who can offer such quality ser-
dence (hence, the importance of the criteria for vices. Although the number of people certified
evidence-based approaches) must be dissemi- continues to increase, clearly there is a need for
nated to these professional groups, because they more. In some geographical areas, such as rural
are in the position to be providing advice and communities, there remains a dearth of service
counsel to parents who will be making decisions providers. As the number of professionals trained
on treatment for their children. Enhancing the in evidence-based approaches increases, perhaps
quality (i.e., science-based) of information that this will alleviate this particular problem.
goes to parents and service providers should However, due to the COVID-19 pandemic, there
increase the probability that treatment and pro- has been an explosion of telehealth services
gram choices will be made toward evidence-­ around the world, and this has included the deliv-
based treatment and programs and less toward ery of ABA-based interventions (e.g., Baumes
fad treatments and programs based upon misin- et al., 2020; Crockett et al., 2020; Frederick et al.,
formation and anti- or pseudoscience. Informing 2020). Treatment providers should strive to
parents and service providers of the importance become skilled at the delivery of clinical services
of science and quality evidence with regard to (certainly, delivering only those clinical services
treatment decisions should further blunt the mis- that can be done competently through a telehealth
information presented through the media about delivery mode), and this would extend the avail-
flimflam approaches and weak thinking by non- ability and opportunity for all to obtain quality
scientists and popular figures in the news. services based upon science.
118 T. Zane et al.

7.6 Conclusion Behavior Analysis, 33(3), 373–391. https://doi.


org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­373
Alligood, C. A., & Gravina, N. E. (2020). Branching out:
The incidence of autism continues to increase. Finding success in new areas of practice. Behavior
Unfortunately, although the concept of evidence-­ Analysis in Practice, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/
based treatment is widely adopted across disci- s40617-­020-­00483-­2
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2020). Evidence-­
plines, the reality is that much of autism treatment based clinical practice guidelines: Development and
is not based on science and quality research find- implementation manual. https://downloads.aap.org/
ings. As described in this chapter, there are many DOCCSA/CPGManual20190628.pdf
reasons for the persistence of fad treatments; American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®).
these treatments delay improvement of the indi- American Psychiatric Pub.
vidual with autism, provide parents and service American Psychological Association, Presidential
providers with false hope of cures, and, in some Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice. (2006).
cases, cost more financially and waste financial Evidence-based practice in psychology. American
Psychologist, 61(4), 271–285. https://doi.
resources that could have been invested into org/10.1037/0003-­066X.61.4.271
approaches with a proven track record of Association for Science in Autism Treatment. (n.d.).
effectiveness. Mission. https://asatonline.org/what-­we-­do/mission/
The scientific approach has been shown to be Autism Treatment Center of America. (2021). Retrieved
May 1, 2021 at https://autismtreatmentcenter.org/
powerfully effective in learning about the world. autism-­success-­stories/
In whatever discipline the scientific approach has Ayres, A. J.. (1972). Sensory integration and praxis tests
been adopted, researchers begin yielding results manual. Western Psychological Services.
and findings that are replicable and valid. This Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
scientific approach has been applied to get effect Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 91–97.
in autism treatment. It is the major impetus for https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1987.20-­313
the importance of EBPs. Service providers have Bailey, J., & Burch, M. (2016). Ethics for behavior ana-
the responsibility of being informed about the lysts (3rd ed.). Routledge.
Barton, E.  E., Lloyd, B.  P., Spriggs, A.  D., & Gast,
criteria for quality evidence and applying that D.  L. (2018). Visual analysis of graphic data. Single
information to the vetting of their services and Case Research Methodology, 179–214. https://doi.
approaches toward supporting individuals with org/10.4324/9781315150666-­8
autism. The more a scientific attitude and Baumes, A., Colic, M., & Araiba, S. (2020). Comparison
of telehealth-related ethics and guidelines and a
approach is adopted by service providers, the checklist for ethical decision making in the midst
more the goal of evidence-based approaches will of the COVID-19 pandemic. Behavior Analysis
be realized. This, in turn, will improve the quality in Practice, 13, 736–747. https://doi.org/10.1007/
of services, better educator parents and consum- s40617-­020-­00475-­2
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2014).
ers, and  – ultimately  – benefit individuals with Professional and ethical compliance code.
autism to live more independently, make more Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020a). Ethics
choices for themselves, and allow them to live the code for behavior analysts. Author.
types of lives that they choose to live. Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020b).
Recommendations for respecializing in a new ABA
practice area. Author.
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2021). BCBA 2022
References eligibility requirements. Retrieved May 1, 2021 at
https://www.bacb.com/bcba/
Al Anbar, N.  N., Dardennes, R.  M., Prado-Netto, A., Beutler, L. (1998). Identifying empirically supported
Kaye, K., & Contejean, Y. (2010). Treatment choices treatments: What if we didn’t? Journal of Consulting
in autism spectrum disorder: The role of paren- and Clinical Psychology, 66(1), 113–120. https://doi.
tal illness perceptions. Research in Developmental org/10.1037/0022-­006x.66.1.113
Disabilities, 31(3), 817–828. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Boynton, J. (2012). Facilitated communication  – What
ridd.2010.02.007 harm it can do: Confessions of a former facilitator.
Allen, K.  D., & Warzak, W.  J. (2000). The problem of Evidence-Based Communication Assessment and
parental nonadherence in clinical behavior analysis: Intervention, 6(1), 3–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/1748
Effective treatment is not enough. Journal of Applied 9539.2012.674680
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 119

Brodhead, M.  T. (2015). Maintaining professional rela- Dallery, J., & Raiff, B.  R. (2014). Optimizing behav-
tionships in an interdisciplinary setting: Strategies ioral health interventions with single-case designs:
for navigating nonbehavioral treatment recommenda- From development to dissemination. Translational
tions for individuals with autism. Behavior Analysis Behavioral Medicine, 4(3), 290–303. https://doi.
in Practice, 8(1), 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/ org/10.1007/s13142-­014-­0258-­z
s40617-­015-­0042-­7 Dardennes, R.  M., Al Anbar, N.  N., Prado-Netto, A.,
Brodhead, M.  T., Quigley, S.  P., & Wilczynski, S.  M. Kaye, K., Contejean, Y., & Al Anbar, N.  N. (2011).
(2018). A call for discussion about scope of Treating the cause of illness rather than the symp-
­competence in behavior analysis. Behavior Analysis toms: Parental causal beliefs and treatment choices in
in Practice, 11(4), 424–435. https://doi.org/10.1007/ autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental
s40617-­018-­00303-­8 Disabilities, 32(3), 1137–1146. https://doi.
Burns, M. K., & Ysseldyke, J. E. (2009). Reported preva- org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.01.010
lence of evidence-based instructional practices in spe- Deochand, N., & Fuqua, R.  W. (2016). BACB certifica-
cial education. The Journal of Special Education, 43(1), tion trends: State of the states (1999-2014). Behavior
3–11. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022466908315563 Analysis in Practice, 9(3), 243–252. https://doi.
Cardinal, D.  N., Hanson, D., & Wakeham, J. (1996). org/10.1007/s40617-­016-­0118-­z
Investigation of authorship in facilitated communica- Devlin, S., Healy, O., Leader, G., & Hughes, B. (2010).
tion. Mental Retardation, 34(4), 231–242. Comparison of behavioral intervention and sensory-­
Carnett, W. G. (1999). Clinical practice guidelines: A tool integration therapy in the treatment of challeng-
to improve care. Journal of Nursing Care Quality, ing behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental
16(3), 60–70. 10.1097/00001786-200204000-00008. Disorders, 41(10), 1303–1320. https://doi.
Carter, S. L. (2005). Empirical analysis of the effects of org/10.1007/s10803-­010-­1149-­x
a possible sinus infection and weighted vest on func- DiGennaro Reed, F.  D., Novak, M.  D., Henley, A.  J.,
tional analysis outcomes of self-injury exhibited by Brand, D., & McDonald, M.  E. (2017). Evidence-­
a child with autism. Journal of Early and Intensive based interventions. In Handbook of social skills and
Behavior Intervention, 2(4), 252–258. https://doi. autism Spectrum disorder (pp.  139–153). Springer.
org/10.1037/h0100318 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­62995-­7_9
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020). What Dunkel-Jackson, S.  M., Dixon, M.  R., & Szekely, S.
is Autism. https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/facts. (2012). Portable data assistants: Potential in evidence-­
html based practice autism treatment. Research in Autism
Chadwell, M.  R., Sikorski, J.  D., Roberts, H., & Allen, Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 65–72. https://doi.
K.  D. (2019). Process versus content in delivering org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.06.004
ABA services: Does process matter when you have Durand, V.  M., & Carr, E.  G. (1991). Functional com-
content that works? Behavior Analysis: Research munication training to reduce challenging behavior:
and Practice, 19(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Maintenance and application in new settings. Journal
bar0000143 of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(2), 251–264. https://
Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically doi.org/10.1901/jaba.24-­251
supported psychological interventions: Controversies Dymond, S.  K., Gilson, C.  L., & Myran, S.  P. (2007).
and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 685– Services for children with autism spectrum disorders.
716. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.685 Journal of Disability Policy Studies, 18(3), 133–147.
Chambless, D.  L., Sanderson, W.  C., Shoham, V., https://doi.org/10.1177/10442073070180030201
Johnson, S. B., Pope, K. S., Crits-Christoph, P., Baker, Eberlin, M., McConnachie, G., Ibel, S., & Volpe, L.
M., Johnson, B., Woody, S.  R., Sue, S., Beutler, L., (1993). Facilitated communication: A failure to
Williams, D. A., & McCurry, S. (1996). An update on replicate the phenomenon. Journal of Autism and
empirically validated therapies. Clinical Psychology, Developmental Disorders, 23(3), 507–530. https://doi.
49, 5–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/e555332011-­003 org/10.1007/BF01046053
Cihon, J.  H., Milne, C.  M., Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  B., Foxx, R. M., & Mulick, J. A. (Eds.). (2016). Controversial
& Leaf, R. (2020). Fad treatments in autism interven- therapies for autism and intellectual disabilities:
tion: An evaluation of fidget spinners. Education and Fad, fashion, and science in professional practice.
Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, Routledge.
55(4), 466–475. Frederick, J. K., Raabe, G. R., Rogers, V. R., & Pizzica,
Cook, B. G., & Cook, S. C. (2013). Unraveling evidence-­ J. (2020). Advocacy, collaboration, and interven-
based practices in special education. The Journal tion: A model of distance special education sup-
of Special Education, 47(2), 71–82. https://doi. port services amid COVID-19. Behavior Analysis
org/10.1177/0022466911420877 in Practice, 13, 748–756. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Crockett, J. L., Becraft, J. L., Phillips, S. T., Wakeman, M., s40617-­020-­00476-­1
& Cataldo, M. F. (2020). Rapid conversion from clinic Freeman, S. (2016). A map through the minefield: A
to telehealth behavioral services during the COVID-19 parent’s primer to find autism treatment that works.
pandemic. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13(4), 725– In R.  M. Foxx & J.  A. Mulick (Eds.), Controversial
735. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00499-­8 therapies for autism and intellectual disabilities:
120 T. Zane et al.

Fad, fashion, and science in professional practice. Hess, K. L., Morrier, M. J., Heflin, L. J., & Ivey, M. L.
Routledge. (2007). Autism treatment survey: Services received
Furnham, A., & Buck, C. (2003). A comparison of lay-­ by children with autism spectrum disorders in
beliefs about autism and obsessive-compulsive disor- public school classrooms. Journal of Autism and
der. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 49(4), Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 961–971. https://doi.
287–307. org/10.1007/s10803-­007-­0470-­5
Gast, D.  L., & Ledford, J.  R. (2018). Research Hodges, H., Fealko, C., & Soares, N. (2020). Autism spec-
approaches in applied settings. In J.  R. Ledford & trum disorder: Definition, epidemiology, causes and
D.  L. Gast (Eds.), Single case research methodol- clinical evaluation. Translational Pediatrics, 9(Suppl
ogy: Applications in special education and behavioral 1), S55–S65. https://doi.org/10.21037/tp.2019.09.09
sciences (3rd ed., pp.  1–26). Routledge. https://doi. Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom,
org/10.4324/9781315150666-­1 S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject
Goin-Kochel, R.  P., Myers, B.  J., & Mackintosh, V.  H. research to identify evidence-based practice in spe-
(2007). Parental reports on the use of treatments and cial education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 165–179.
therapies for children with autism spectrum disorders. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(3), 195– Hudson, A. (1995). Disability and facilitated communica-
209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2006.08.006 tion. In T. H. Ollendick & R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances
Green, G. (1996). Early behavioral intervention for in clinical child psychology (p. 197). Springer. https://
autism: What does research tell us? In C.  Maurice, doi.org/10.1007/978-­1-­4757-­9044-­3_5
G. Green, & S. C. Luce (Eds.), Behavioral interven- Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement
tions for young children with autism: A manual for Act of 2004, 70, Fed. Reg. (2005). https://uscode.
parents and professionals. Pro-Ed. house.gov/view.xhtml?path=/prelim@title20/
Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly, chapter33&edition=prelim
M., & Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treatments Institute of Medicine. (2001). Crossing the quality chasm:
used by parents of children with autism. Research in A new health system for the 21st century. National
Developmental Disabilities, 27(1), 70–84. https://doi. Academies Press.
org/10.1016/j.ridd.2004.12.002 Ioannidis, J.  P. A. (2016). Evidence-based medicine has
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Owen-­ been hijacked: A report to David Sackett. Journal
DeSchryever, J., Iwata, B. A., & Wacker, D. P. (1997). of Clinical Epidemiology, 73, 82–86. 10.1016/j.
Toward the development of structured criteria for jclinepi.2016.02.012.
interpretation of functional analysis data. Journal of Jacobson, J.  W., Foxx, R.  M., & Mulick, J.  A. (2016).
Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(2), 313–326. https:// Facilitated communication. In R.  M. Foxx & J.  A.
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­313 Mulick (Eds.), Controversial therapies for autism and
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto, intellectual disabilities (pp. 282–302). Routledge.
J., & LeBlanc, L.  A. (1998). Effectiveness of func- Kay, S. (2016). Helping parents separate the wheat
tional communication training with and without from the chaff: Putting autism treatments to the test.
extinction and punishment: A summary of 21 inpatient In R.  M. Foxx & J.  A. Mulick (Eds.), Controversial
cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(2), therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion,
211–235. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-­211 and science in professional practice (pp.  196–208).
Handley, M. R., Stuart, M. E., & Kirz, H. L. (1994). An Routledge.
evidence-based approach to evaluating and improving Kazdin, A.  E. (2000). Psychotherapy for children and
clinical practice: Implementing practice guidelines. adolescents: Directions for research and practice.
HMO Practice, 8(2), 75–93. Oxford University Press.
Hanson, E., Kalish, L. A., Bunce, E., Curtis, C., McDaniel, Kazdin, A.  E. (2011). Single-case research designs:
S., et al. (2007). Use of complementary and alternative Methods for clinical and applied settings (2nd ed.).
medicine among children diagnosed with autism spec- Oxford University Press.
trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Kubina, R.  M., Kostewicz, D.  E., Brennan, K.  M., &
Disorders, 37(4), 628–636. https://doi.org/10.1007/ King, S.  A. (2017). A critical review of line graphs
s10803-­006-­0192-­0 in behavior analytic journals. Educational Psychology
Healthline. (2018, June 27). Autism treatment guide. Review, 29(3), 583–598. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Retrieved December 1, 2020. From: https://www. s10648-­015-­9339-­x
healthline.com/health/autism-­treatment#takeaway Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Healy, O., Rispoli, M., Lydon,
Hemsley, B., Bryant, L., Schlosser, R. W., Shane, H. C., H., Streusand, W., Davis, T., Kang, S., Sigafoos,
Lang, R., Paul, D., Banajee, M., & Ireland, M. (2018). J., Lancioni, G., Didden, R., & Giesbers, S. (2012).
Systematic review of facilitated communication 2014-­ Sensory integration therapy for autism spectrum
2018 finds no new evidence that messages delivered disorders: A systematic review. Research in Autism
using facilitated communication are authored by the Spectrum Disorders, 6(3), 1004–1018. 10.1016/j.
person with disability. Autism and Developmental rasd.2012.01.006.
Language Impairments, 3, 1–8. https://doi. Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C.  M.,
org/10.1177/2396941518821570 Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (2020a). Advances in our
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 121

understanding of behavioral intervention: 1980 to Matson, J. L., Adams, H. L., Williams, L. W., & Rieske,
2020 for individuals diagnosed with autism spec- R. D. (2013). Why are there so many unsubstantiated
trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental treatments in autism? Research in Autism Spectrum
disorders. Advanced online publication. https://doi. Disorders, 7(3), 466–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
org/10.1007/s10803-­020-­04481-­9 rasd.2012.11.006
Leaf, R., Zane, T., Weiss, M. J., Leaf, J. B., & Cihon, J. H. Maurice, C., Mannion, K., Letso, S., & Perry, L. (2001).
(2020b). One worldview to rule them all in a flat earth Parent voices: Difficulty in accessing behavior
or behavioral full worldview: The need for behavior intervention for autism; working toward solutions.
analysts to rely upon the fundamentals of our science. Behavioral Interventions: Theory & Practice in
Presented at the Association for Behavior Analysis Residential & Community- Based Clinical Programs,
International Annual Convention, virtual. 16(3), 147–165. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.89
Ledford, J. R., Lane, J. D., & Tate, R. (2018). Evaluating McCarthy, J., & Carrey, J. (2008, April 4). Jenny
quality and rigor in single case research. In J.  R. McCarthy: My son’s recovery from autism. CNN.
Ledford & D.  L. Gast (Eds.), Single case research http://www.cnn.com/2008/US/04/02/mccarthy.
methodology: Applications in special education and autsimtreatment/
behavioral sciences (3rd ed., pp. 365–392). Routledge. McDonald, M. E., & Reed, F. D. D. (2018). Distinguishing
https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315150666-­13 science and pseudoscience in the assessment and treat-
Ledford, J.  R., Lane, J.  D., Zimmerman, K.  N., Chazin, ment of autism spectrum disorder. In S. Goldstein &
K. T., & Ayres, K. A. (2020). Single Case Analysis and S. Ozonoff (Eds.), Assessment of autism spectrum dis-
Review Framework (SCARF). https://ebip.vkcsites. order (2nd ed., pp. 415–441). Guildford Press.
org/scarfv2/ McKibbon, K.  A. (1998). Evidence-based practice.
Leko, M. M., Roberts, C., Peyton, D., & Pua, D. (2019). Bulletin of the Medical Library Association, 86(3),
Selecting evidence-based practices: What works for 396–401.
me. Intervention in School and Clinic, 54(5), 286– Miller, V. A., Schreck, K. A., Mulick, J. A., & Butter,
294. https://doi.org/10.1177/1053451218819190 E. (2012). Factors related to parents’ choices of
LeRoy, A.  S. (2017). Identifying evidence-based prac- treatments for their children with autism spec-
tices for behavior: Analysis of studies reviewed by trum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum
the What Works Clearinghouse. The Clearing House, Disorders, 6(1), 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
90(4), 125–128. https://doi.org/10.1080/00098655.20 rasd.2011.03.008
17.1318026 Mitka, M. (2008). Chelation therapy trials halted. JAMA:
Levy, S. E., & Hyman, S. L. (2005). Novel treatments for The Journal of the American Medical Association,
autistic spectrum disorders. Mental Retardation and 300(19), 2236–2236.
Developmental Research Reviews, 11(2), 131–142. Moss-Morris, R., Weinman, J., Petrie, K.  J., Horne,
https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20062 R., Cameron, L.  D., & Buick, D. (2002). The
Lombardo, M.  V., Meng-Chuan, L., & Baron-Cohen, revised illness perception questionnaire (IPQ-R).
S. (2019). Big data approaches to decomposing het- Psychology and Health, 17(1), 1–16. https://doi.
erogeneity across the autism spectrum. Molecular org/10.1080/08870440290001494
Psychiatry, 24, 1435–1450. https://doi.org/10.1038/ Murphy, M.  A., & Ruble, L.  A. (2012). A comparative
s41380-­018-­0321-­0 study of rurality and urbanicity on access to and satis-
Mackintosh, V.  H., Goin-Kochel, R.  P., & Myers, B.  J. faction with services for children with autism spectrum
(2012). “What do you like/dislike about the treat- disorders. Rural Special Education Quarterly, 31(3),
ments you’re currently using?”: A qualitative study 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1177/875687051203100302
of parents of children with autism Spectrum disorder. Murray, N. (2016). More than rain man: Autism
Focus on Autism and Other Disabilities, 27(1), 51–60. portrayals on screen. Retrieved May 3, 2021
https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357611423542 at https://www.pbs.org/independentlens/blog/
Maenner, M.  J., Shaw, K.  A., Baio, J., et  al. (2020). more-­than-­rain-­man-­autism-­portrayals-­on-­screen/
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among chil- National Conference of State Legislatures. (2018).
dren aged 8 years  — Autism and Developmental Retrieved December 20, 2020 at https://www.ncsl.org/
Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United research/health/autism-­and-­insurance-­coverage-­state-­
States, 2016. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 69(SS-­ laws.aspx
4), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1 Nelson, R., Roberts, M., Mathur, S., & Rutherford, R.
Makrygianni, M.  K., Gena, A., Katoudi, S., & Galanis, (1999). Has public policy exceeded our knowledge
P. (2018). The effectiveness of applied behavior base? A review of the functional behavioral assess-
analytic interventions for children with autism spec- ment literature. Behavioral Disorders, 24(2), 169–
trum disorder: A meta-analytic study. Research in 179. https://doi.org/10.1177/019874299902400207
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 51, 18–31. https://doi. Newschaffer, C. J., Croen, L. A., Daniels, J., Giarelli, E.,
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.03.006 Grether, J. K., Levy, S. E., Mandell, D. S., Miller, L. A.,
Martella, R. C., Nelson, R., & Marchand-Martella, N. E. Pinto-Martin, J., Reaven, J., Reynolds, A.  M., Rice,
(1999). Research methods: Learning to become a crit- C.  E., Schendel, D., & Windham, G.  C. (2007). The
ical consumer. Allyn and Bacon. epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annual
122 T. Zane et al.

Review of Public Health, 28, 235–258. https://doi. Behavior Analysis, 46(4), 708–722. https://doi.
org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.28.021406.144007 org/10.1002/jaba.76
No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub. L.  No. 107– Rosa, L., Rosa, E., Samer, L., & Barrett, S. (1998). A close
110. (2002). https://www2.ed.gov/policy/elsec/leg/ look at therapeutic touch. JAMA, 279(13), 10005–
esea02/107-­110.pdf 11010. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.279.13.1005
Odom, S.  L., & Strain, P.  S. (2002). Evidence-based Rynes, S., Colbert, A., & Brown, K. (2002). HR profes-
practice in early intervention/early childhood special sionals’ belief about effective human resource prac-
education: Single-subject design research. Journal tices: Correspondence between research and practice.
of Early Intervention, 25(2), 151–160. https://doi. Human Resource Management, 41(12), 149–174.
org/10.1177/105381510202500212 https://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.10029
Odom, S.  L., Collet-Klingenberg, L., Rogers, S.  J., & Schreck, K.  A., & Mazur, A. (2008). Behavior analyst
Hatton, D.  D. (2010). Evidence-based practices in use of and belief in treatments for people with autism.
interventions for children and youth with autism spec- Behavioral Interventions, 23(3), 201–212. https://doi.
trum disorders. Preventing School Failure: Alternative org/10.1002/bin.264
Education for Children and Youth, 54(4), 275–282. Schreck, K. A., & Miller, V. A. (2010). How to behavior
https://doi.org/10.1080/10459881003785506 ethically in a world of fads. Behavioral Interventions,
Pastrana, S. J., Frewing, T. M., Grow, L. L., Nosik, M. R., 25(4), 307–324. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.305
Turner, M., & Carr, J. (2018). Frequently assigned Schreck, K.  A., & Ramirez, J.  E. (2016). Television’s
readings in behavior analysis graduate training pro- mixed messages: Choose the best and mute the rest.
grams. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11(3), 267–273. Behavioral Interventions, 31(3), 251–264. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­016-­0137-­9 org/10.1002/bin.1449
Pellecchia, M., Beidas, R.  S., Lawson, G., Williams, Schreck, K.  A., Russell, M., & Vargas, L.  A. (2013).
N. J., Seidman, M., Kimberly, J. R., Cannuscio, C. C., Autism treatment in print: Media’s coverage of sci-
& Mandell, D.  S. (2020). Does implementing a new entifically supported and alternative treatments.
intervention disrupt use of existing evidence-based Behavioral Interventions, 28(4), 299–321. https://doi.
autism interventions? Autism, 24(7), 1713–1725. org/10.1002/bin.1370
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320919248 Schreck, K. A., Karunaratne, Y., Zane, T., & Wilford, H.
Perone, M., & Hursh, D.  E. (2013). Single-case experi- (2016). Behavior analysts’ use of and beliefs in treat-
mental designs. In G.  J. Madden, W.  V. Dube, T.  D. ments for people with autism: A 5-year follow-up.
Hackenberg, G. P. Hanley, & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), APA Behavioral Interventions, 31(4), 355–376. https://doi.
handbooks in psychology®. APA handbook of behav- org/10.1002/bin.1461
ior analysis, Vol. 1. Methods and principles (pp. 107– Shaw, K.  A., Maenner, M.  J., & Baio, J. (2020). Early
126). American Psychological Association. https://doi. identification of autism spectrum disorder among chil-
org/10.1037/13937-­005 dren aged 4 years—Early Autism and Developmental
Quigley, S. P., Peterson, L., Frieder, J. E., & Peterson, S. Disabilities Monitoring Network, six sites, United
(2011). Effects of a weighted vest on problem behav- States, 2016. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 69(3),
iors during functional analyses in children with per- 1. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6903a1
vasive developmental disorders. Research in Autism Shawler, L. A., Blair, B. J., Harper, J. M., & Dorsey, M. F.
Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 529–538. https://doi. (2018). A survey of the current state of the scientist-­
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.019 practitioner model in applied behavior analysis.
Reichow, B., Hume, K., Barton, E.  E., & Boyd, B.  A. Education and Treatment of Children, 41(3), 277–297.
(2012). Early intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.2018.0014
for young children with autism spectrum disorders Shepherd, H. L., Barratt, A., Trevena, L. J., McGeechan,
(ASD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, K., Carey, K., Epstein, R. M., Butow, P. N., Del Mar,
5, 1465–1858. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858. C. B., Entwistle, V., & Tattersall, M. H. (2011). Three
cd009260.pub3 questions that patients can ask to improve the qual-
Rogers, S.  J., & Vismara, L.  A. (2008). Evidence-based ity of information physicians give about treatment
comprehensive treatments for early autism. Journal of options: A cross-over trial. Patient Education and
Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37(1), 8–38. Counseling, 84(3), 379–385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410701817808 pec.2011.07.022
Roley, S.  S., Mailloux, Z., Miller-Kuhaneck, H., & Simpson, R. L. (2005). Evidence-based practices and stu-
Glennon, T. (2007). Understanding Ayres’ sen- dents with autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism
sory integration. Occupational Therapy Faculty and Other Developmental Disorders, 20(3), 140–149.
Publications, 12(17), 1–8. https://www.researchgate. https://doi.org/10.1177/10883576050200030201
net/publication/289758466_Understanding_Ayres_ Slocum, T.  A., Detrich, R., Wilczynski, S.  M., Spencer,
Sensory_IntegrationR T.  D., Lewis, T., & Wolfe, K. (2014). The evidence-­
Rooker, G. W., Jessel, J., Kurtz, P. F., & Hagopian, L. P. based practice of applied behavior analysis.
(2013). Functional communication training with and The Behavior Analyst, 37(1), 41–56. https://doi.
without alternative reinforcement and punishment: org/10.1007/s40614-­014-­0005-­2
An analysis of 58 applications. Journal of Applied
7  Factors Influencing to Implement or Not to Implement Evidence-Based Procedures 123

Smith, T. (2012). Evolution of research on interven- United States Public Health Service. (1999). Mental
tions for individuals with autism spectrum disorder: health: A report of the surgeon general (chap. 3, sec-
Implications for behavior analysts. The Behavior tion on autism). Retrieved May 13, 2009, from http://
Analyst, 35(1), 101–113. https://doi.org/10.1007/ www.surgeongeneral.gov/library/mentalhealth/chap-
BF03392269 ter3/sec6.html#autism
Smith, T. (2016). The appeal of unvalidated treatments. In Valentino, A.  L., & Juanico, J.  F. (2020). Overcoming
R. M. Foxx & J. A. Mulick (Eds.), Controversial thera- barriers to applied research: A guide for practitioners.
pies for developmental disabilities: Fad, f­ashion, and Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13, 894–904. https://
science in professional practice (pp. 29–42). Routledge. doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00479-­y
Smith, T., & Antolovich, M. (2000). Parental percep- Van Houten, R., Axelrod, S., Bailey, J.  S., Favell, J.  E.,
tions of supplemental interventions received by Foxx, R.  M., Iwata, B.  A., & Lovaas, O.  I. (1988).
young children with autism in intensive behav- The right to effective behavioral treatment. Journal
ior analytic treatment. Behavioral Interventions, of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21(4), 381–384. https://
15(2), 83–97. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-­­ doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.21-­381
078X(200004/06)15:2<83::AID-­BIN>3.0.CO;2-­W Vyse, S. (2016). Where do fads come from? In R. M. Foxx
Smith, T., Mruzek, D., & Mozingo, D. (2016). Sensory & J.  A. Mulick (Eds.), Controversial therapies for
integration therapy. In R.  M. Foxx & J.  A. Mulick autism and intellectual disabilities: Fad, fashion, and
(Eds.), Controversial therapies for autism and intel- science in professional practice (2nd ed., pp.  3–28).
lectual disabilities (pp. 247–269). Routledge. Routledge.
Solomons, S. (2005). Using aromatherapy mas- Walmsley, C., & Baker, J. C. (2019). Tactics to evaluate
sage to increase shared attention behaviours in the evidence base of a nonbehavioral intervention in
children with autistic spectrum disorders and an expanded consumer area. Behavior Analysis in
severe learning disabilities. British Journal of Practice, 12(3), 677–687. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Special Education, 32(3), 127–137. https://doi. s40617-­018-­00308-­3
org/10.1111/j.0952-­3383.2005.00385.x Webmd. (2019). What therapies, besides play therapy,
Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin, also help with autism? Retrieved February 23,
K. L., Nowell, S. W., Tomaszewaki, B., Szendrey, S., 2021 at https://www.webmd.com/brain/autism/
McIntyre, N. S., Yucesoy-Ozkan, S., & Savage, M. N. therapies-­to-­help-­with-­autism
(2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth, Weiss, M. J., Fiske, K., & Ferraioli, S. (2008). Evidence-­
and young adults with Autism. The University of North based practice for autism spectrum disorders. In
Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child Clinical assessment and intervention for autism spec-
Development Institute, National Clearinghouse of trum disorders (pp. 33–63). Academic Press.
Autism Evidence and Practice Review Team. https:// What Works Clearinghouse. (2020). Retrieved December
ncaep.fpg.unc.edu/sites/ncaep.fpg.unc.edu/files/imce/ 3, 2020 at https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/PracticeGuides
documents/EBP%20Report%202020.pdf Williams, M.  S., & Shellenberger, C.  R. (1996). How
Stephenson, J., & Carter, M. (2009). The use of weighted does your engine run? A leader’s guide to the Alert
vests with children with autism spectrum disor- Program for self-regulation. Therapy Works, Inc.
ders and other disabilities. Journal of Autism and Wolfe, K., Barton, E. E., & Meadan, H. (2019). Systematic
Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 105–114. https://doi. protocols for the visual analysis of single-case research
org/10.1007/s10803-­008-­0605-­3 data. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(2), 491–502.
Tate, R.  L., Perdices, M., Rosenkoetter, U., Wakim, D., https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00336-­7
Godbee, K., Togher, L., & McDonald, S. (2013). Zablotsky, B., Black, L. I., Maenner, M. J., Schieve, L. A.,
Revision of a method quality rating scale for single-­ Danielson, M.  L., Bitsko, R.  H., Blumberg, S.  J.,
case experimental designs and n-of-1 trials: The Kogan, M. D., & Boyle, C. A. (2019). Prevalence and
Risk of Bias in N-of-1 Trials (RoBiNT) Scale. trends of developmental disabilities among children in
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 23(5), 619–638. the US: 2009–2017. Pediatrics, 144(4), e20190811.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2013.824 https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2019-­0811
Tincani, M., & Travers, J. (2018). Publishing single-case Zane, T., & Hanson, J. (2008). Evidence based practice:
research design studies that do not demonstrate experi- A review of the criteria that constitutes evidence.
mental control. Remedial and Special Education, 39(2), Presented at the Florida Association for Behavior
118–128. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932517697447 Analysis conference, Daytona Beach, Florida.
Tincani, M., & Travers, J. (2019). Replication research, Zane, T., Davis, C., & Rosswurm, M. (2008). The cost
publication bias, and applied behavior analysis. of fad treatments in autism. Journal of Early and
Perspectives on Behavior Science, 42(1), 59–75. Intensive Behavior Intervention, 5(2), 44–51. https://
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-­019-­00191-­5 doi.org/10.1037/h0100418
Turner, L.  B., Fischer, A.  J., & Luiselli, J.  K. (2016). Zimmerman, K. (2020, October 16). Ethically evaluat-
Towards a competency-based, ethical, socially valid ing interventions with minimal or nonexistent eviden-
approach to the supervision of applied behavior ana- tiary support. Professional seminar in the Department
lytic trainees. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9(4), of Applied Behavioral Science at the University of
287–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­016-­0121-­4 Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States.
Effective Collaboration:
Maximizing Outcomes in Autism 8
Intervention in an Interdisciplinary
Model

Mary Jane Weiss, Lisa Tereshko, Kristin Bowman,


Kimberly Marshall, and Karen Rose

8.1 Effective Collaboration: that are within their boundary of competence and
Maximizing Outcomes their available resources. When faced with a case
in Autism Intervention that presents a challenge that is novel to the
in an Interdisciplinary Model behavior analyst, the behavior analyst should
seek advice, training, and recommendations from
One goal of therapy informed by applied behav- others who have expertise in the area. The Ethics
ior analysis (ABA) is to make socially significant Code for Behavior Analysts explicitly states the
changes in the lives of individuals with autism need for effective and respectful collaboration as
spectrum disorder (ASD). The effectiveness of a core principle for all behavior analysts, which
the use of ABA-based therapy with individuals includes collaborating within the field of behav-
with ASD was initially demonstrated by Lovaas ior analysis and with professionals from other
in 1987. Lovaas (1987) showed that children with fields. The collaboration with other behavior ana-
ASD could make significant gains in cognitive lysts can assist in building the behavior analyst’s
abilities and reductions in inappropriate behav- skill set to better serve individuals with ASD and
iors, when exposed to intensive hours of therapy improve the outcomes of intervention.
at early ages. This pivotal study increased the Oftentimes, the case necessitates expertise not
demand for ABA-based therapy for individuals commonly possessed by behavior analysts and
with ASD. requires interdisciplinary collaboration. The
One of the most significant challenges facing Ethics Code for Behavior Analysts states that
behavior analysts is the diversity of the needs of consultation across disciplines is to occur as nec-
individuals with ASD. Each individual with ASD essary, given the specific skills and challenges of
presents a different set of skills and challenges, a client, and to promote the best interests of the
which increases the complexity of the case for client (Behavior Analyst Certification Board,
the behavior analyst. The Ethics Code for 2020a). Individuals with ASD have highly com-
Behavior Analysts (Behavior Analyst plex needs, and every individual presents differ-
Certification Board, 2020a) states that behavior ently, further increasing the skill set needed to
analysts should only accept and work with clients appropriately serve the client. For example, some
individuals with ASD speak fluently, while others
M. J. Weiss (*) · L. Tereshko · K. Bowman do not emit any vocal language. Also, some indi-
K. Marshall · K. Rose viduals engage in aggression or self-injurious
Department of Applied Behavior Analysis, behaviors, while others do not. To further add to
Endicott College, Beverly, MA, USA the challenges of each individual case, the indi-
e-mail: mweiss@endicott.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 125
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_8
126 M. J. Weiss et al.

viduals with ASD may also present with varied include the lack of direct training on collabora-
degrees of language and behavioral challenges tion, lack of training on empathy and relationship
that change based on situational contexts or over building, overreliance on behavioral jargon,
time. reluctance to incorporate recommendations from
An additional challenge results from comor- other fields, and negative perceptions of behavior
bid diagnoses. Comorbid diagnoses occur when analysts. This chapter will further identify and
two or more diagnoses are made for the same describe the challenges of interdisciplinary col-
individual. Individuals with ASD have been laboration, the variables directly affecting inter-
noted to have a high rate of comorbid diagnoses disciplinary collaboration, and the possible
(Matson & Nebel-Schwalm, 2007). These comor- solutions to enhance collaboration for providers
bid diagnoses include psychological diagnoses, of services to individuals with ASD.
such as depression, anxiety, and attention deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (Matson & Nebel-­
Schwalm, 2007). The comorbidity may also 8.2 Definition and Concept
include medical diagnoses, such as gastrointesti- of Collaboration in ASD
nal dysfunction (McElhanon et  al., 2014), Intervention
unhealthy weight (Ranjan & Nasser, 2015), and
nutrient deficiencies (Ranjan & Nasser, 2015). Professionals may agree with needing mutual
The complexity of individual profiles may respect of different professionals and the desire
lead to the need for collaboration with other pro- to do what is best for the client, but different dis-
fessionals to ensure that all issues are compre- ciplines do not always agree on what collabora-
hensively addressed and to ensure that best tion is and how it should occur. Within treatment
practice treatments are implemented for the cli- for individuals with ASD, professionals from
ent. Medical, psychological, psychiatric, allied special education, education, behavior analysis,
health, and educational professionals are com- speech therapy, occupational and physical ther-
mon partners in treatment that behavior analysts apy, and medical disciplines work collaboratively
will ally with when treating an individual with but may all have different expectations of how to
ASD. Each professional plays a vital role in the do so.
treatment of the individual with ASD. In special education, collaboration empha-
To best treat an individual with ASD, the sizes interactions between the general education
behavior analyst is required to work with various teacher and the special education teacher, while
disciplines collaboratively. Each professional education emphasizes the interactions between
brings different skills and knowledge to the case, the consultant and consultee, such as between the
and it is essential that a collaborative relationship speech pathologist and the teacher (Kelly &
is built among the professionals to ensure consis- Tincani, 2013). Behavioral consultation further
tency and efficacy of treatment. All professionals defines collaboration as a process that involves
of an interdisciplinary team bring the desire to identifying the problem, analyzing the problem,
best help the individual client that they share with implementing treatment, and evaluating the treat-
other professionals. This shared desire to do what ment (Kelly & Tincani, 2013). In order for effec-
is best for the client needs to remain at the fore- tive collaboration to occur, one must understand
front of collaboration to ensure effective collabo- the model of collaboration in effect. There are
ration can occur and can lead to a mutual respect several models that have been used in cross-­
across professionals on the team. disciplinary treatment of ASD (e.g., Gerenser &
Interdisciplinary collaboration can sometimes Koenig, 2019). Multidisciplinary treatment
be difficult for various reasons, leading to reduced maintains more separateness of the individual
effective collaboration. Some of the barriers to disciplines, but members share assessment results
effectiveness for behavior analysts specifically and intervention outcomes with members of the
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 127

other disciplines on the team. Interdisciplinary communication, but demonstrated an increased


collaboration approaches aim to provide more influence on the use of ABA-based therapy
integrated and less fragmented services and (Schreck et  al., 2016). Across these studies, it
cooperate to identify goals and priorities. seems clear that behavior analysts are often influ-
Transdisciplinary collaboration is a fully inte- enced by parents and professionals from other
grated model, in which members of different dis- disciplines when incorporating non-evidence-­
ciples often work alongside one another and based interventions. It may be that they lack the
engage in co-assessment and co-treatment. For skills to nimbly navigate these discussions and to
the purposes of this chapter, we will be viewing assist the team in selecting interventions that are
the interdisciplinary model as ideal and will pri- evidence-based. This may indicate a need for
marily reference the process with this term. more training in collaborative work and in con-
flict resolution.

8.2.1 Why Collaboration Is Essential


8.3 Challenges
When multiple interventions are recommended with Collaboration
across disciplines, professionals need to collabo- in the Field of Behavior
rate to determine which aspects of the recom- Analysis
mended treatments would most benefit the client
and how it will be measured to ensure the desired 8.3.1 L
 ack of Information About
behavior change is occurring. An important Other Disciplines
aspect of determining how these collaborations
occur is also examining the behavior of the staff It is common for behavior analysts to lack a
making the recommendations and what the func- detailed understanding of the roles and contribu-
tion of their behavior and their recommendations tions of other members of the team (LaFrance
may be (Frykman et al., 2014). et  al., 2019). Unfortunately, this is often not
Many factors influence the interdisciplinary emphasized in graduate education or in supervi-
team’s treatment recommendations, which sion. While some models of training in interpro-
increases the need for effective collaboration fessional collaboration are emerging, this is a
between all team members. A survey by Kelly relatively new emphasis within behavior analysis
and Tincani (2013) found that behavior analysts (Boivin et  al., 2021). Working to increase the
are less likely to make recommendations to a understanding of the contributions of other team
non-behavior analyst than to other behavior ana- members can be achieved by transdisciplinary
lysts and are more likely to accept recommenda- training, joint treatment experiences, and content
tions from other behavior analysts than from knowledge regarding different fields (Koenig &
non-behavior analysts. To further examine what Gerenser, 2019).
is affecting treatment decisions of behavior ana-
lysts, a survey by Schreck et  al. (2016) high-
lighted that the clients and parents of clients 8.3.2 Fundamental Differences
influence treatment decisions only 38% of the Across Disciplines
time when choosing to implement ABA-based
therapy, but 69% of the time when choosing to Disciplines approach intervention from unique
implement the unestablished procedure of sen- vantage points that reflect the fundamental
sory integration and 63% of the time for the inef- assumptions, historical progress, and founda-
fective and potentially harmful treatment of tional values of their fields of study. These differ-
facilitated communication. Furthermore, the ences have both advantages and disadvantages
influence of coworkers and supervisors had simi- for interdisciplinary team processes. Differences
lar patterns for sensory integration and facilitated can be difficult to navigate, especially if they
128 M. J. Weiss et al.

involve foundational principles such as a world- 8.3.3 P


 references for One’s Own
view or definition of evidence-based practice. Discipline
Much has been written about the fundamental
assumptions and practices associated with closely People naturally have an affinity for their own
allied disciplines (e.g., Koenig & Gerenser, 2019; discipline and prefer it to other professions.
LaFrance et  al., 2019; Ottenbacher et  al., 2002; While this is natural and expected, it does lead to
Schell & Gillen, 2019). The potential for collabo- some unintended consequences that can pose dif-
ration is based on the ability to identify both the ficulty within a team. At its worst, this preference
unique and intersecting skill sets of different dis- for one’s own discipline may come across as
ciplines (Frost & Bondy, 2019). Furthermore, arrogant and as superior. Behavior analysts seem
developing skills in understanding how different most vulnerable to this error, as their commit-
disciplines approach elements of intervention can ment to the science may be conveyed as an intol-
help bridge between recommendations. For erance for any other perspective.
example, it may help to understand how speech When behavior analysts raise concerns about
and language pathologists and behavior analysts the lack of evidence for a proposed intervention,
each approach assessment, the use of antecedents they are doing so out of a commitment to
and consequences in terms of building skills, and evidence-­based intervention and out of a core
the development of goals (Gerenser & Cicero, obligation to use empirically verified treatments.
2019; Koenig & Murphy, 2019; Vail & Koenig, While they have often been exceptionally well
2019). Understanding the manner in which inter- trained in this commitment to science, they may
vention is conceptualized and enacted may help not have been as well trained in the professional-
build an appreciation for the contributions of the ism skills associated with delivering their mes-
other professional. Similarly, much can be gained sages of concern. The delivery of these messages
from occupational therapy and ABA profession- requires compassion, respect, and the ability to
als learning from one another and working resolve conflict. When such concerns are voiced
together to address issues in motor planning, the in the absence of these characteristics, it may
execution of daily living skills, and strength result in interpersonal injury to other members of
training (e.g., Case-Smith & Arbesman, 2008; the team.
Dorsey et al., 2019; Swinth, 2019).
Each discipline approaches their scope of
practice from the lens of their own discipline. In 8.3.4 Lack of Training
that context, they view evidence-based practice, in Collaboration
effective treatment, data collection, efficiency,
and effectiveness differently, and these elements Collaboration comprises a very large percentage
of intervention are distinct across disciplines. of professional time and is a defining feature of
The worldview that is the foundation of the disci- most behavior analytic jobs. Most professionals
pline is also unique and may differ from the agree that working in an interdisciplinary team
worldviews of other disciplines. when treating individuals with ASD is beneficial
In addition, each discipline approaches the and important, yet the same professionals iden-
assessment of outcomes with methods specific to tify collaboration as a challenge to their daily
their field of study. Data may be defined and col- jobs (Frykman et al., 2014). Unfortunately, most
lected in radically different manners, and one dis- behavior analysts are under-prepared for the col-
cipline’s approach may be completely antithetical laboration challenges they will face as profes-
to another discipline’s approach. This can make it sionals. If most professionals are reporting that
difficult for the team to align on goals, on prog- collaborative interactions occur frequently as a
ress reports, and on overall evaluation of the piece of their behavioral practice, and the major-
impact of treatment. ity (i.e., 78%) of professionals collaborated with
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 129

behavior analysts on a regular basis, why is the empathy, and engage in active listening and other
importance of effective collaboration still not a prosocial behaviors (e.g., Baron-Cohen &
focus in training for behavior analysts (Kelly & Wheelwright, 2004; Harris et  al., 1990; Reeve
Tincani, 2013)? et al., 2007; Schrandt et al., 2009; Sigman et al.,
While the importance of collaboration has 1992; Yirmiya et al., 1992). However, while com-
been clearly articulated here and elsewhere (e.g., passionate care was emphasized by early found-
Brodhead, 2015; Kelly & Tincani, 2013), it is less ers and leaders of the field (e.g., Baer et al., 1968;
clear that behavior analysts and behavior analysis Foxx, 1996; Wolf, 1978), it was not discussed
training programs prioritize collaboration and much in the literature again until recently (e.g.,
collaborative skills. Conversely, there is evidence Taylor et al., 2019). In the last few years, several
that collaboration may not be focused on the articles have identified deficits in the skill sets of
training of behavior analysts (Kelly & Tincani, current behavior analysts in the area of compas-
2013; LeBlanc, Taylor, & Marchese, 2020; Taylor sionate care, and a call to action has been issued.
et al., 2019). In a survey of 320 behavior analysts, Specifically, LeBlanc, Taylor, and colleagues
Kelly and Tincani (2013) found that participants (LeBlanc, Taylor, & Marchese, 2020) found that
highly rated the importance of collaboration (i.e., behavior analysts lacked training in compassion,
4 on a scale of 1 to 5) and most participants (i.e., empathy, and therapeutic relationships. Of the
62%) indicated that they collaborated with other 225 survey participants, only 28% reported that
professionals daily. However, despite the per- they encountered lectures or assigned readings
ceived importance and regularity of collaboration on these skills in their behavior analytic course-
in behavior analysts’ job roles, 67% of partici- work, and only 50% reported that these skills
pants had not taken any courses in their behavior were addressed in their supervision experience.
analytic training with the word collaboration in While these skills do not encompass the full
either the title or description, and 45% reported range of responses associated with collaboration
that they had not attended any workshops or and the focus of their study was on working with
trainings with the word collaboration in either families, it can be argued that these skills are also
the title or description. important prerequisite skills to effective collabo-
This deficiency of training in collaborative ration with colleagues. For example, Taylor et al.
skills is concerning, particularly considering the (2019) defined empathy as “involving both a cog-
collaborative nature of many behavior analysts’ nitive component (identifying the emotion being
job roles. Many behavior analytic training pro- displayed) and an affective component (appreci-
grams are designed based on the BACB® task ating and experiencing the person’s emotional
list (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2017) response)” (p. 655).
to ensure that skills that will be required on the The skills noted above are nuanced and would
job are adequately trained, shaped, and practiced. likely not develop in the absence of targeted
However, it appears that despite task items related training. Unfortunately, most behavior analysts
to collaboration (e.g., H-9 Collaborate with oth- do not receive such training. LeBlanc, Taylor
ers who support and/or provide services to cli- et  al. (Taylor et  al., 2019) surveyed Board
ents), training programs may not be effectively Certified Behavior Analysts about the curriculum
addressing these important skills. and training they received in the areas of soft
skills. The results indicated that of the 221
respondents, 72% reported they received no in-­
8.3.5 L
 ack of Training in Empathy class instruction in the area of soft skills, 78%
and Relationship Building reported no assigned readings in the area of soft
skills, 82% reported no formal training or instruc-
In the past several decades, a number of articles tion during supervision in the area of soft skills,
have been written about teaching individuals and 45% reported being exposed to some type of
with ASD to make empathic statements, express training in soft skills outside of their behavior
130 M. J. Weiss et al.

analysis training. Furthermore, 59% of empathic responding may decrease the likelihood
respondents reported pursuing professional
­ of collaborative opportunities. If the behavior
development in soft skills on their own through analyst is not invited to the table to discuss the
practicum settings, trainings provided by employ- client, it will be impossible for the behavior ana-
ers, or contacting the literature or at professional lyst to make recommendations that best support
development and conference opportunities. client outcomes. As such, a lack of focus on
Understandably, since they reported not empathy and other relationship building skills in
encountering formal training, LeBlanc, Taylor graduate and undergraduate behavior analysis
et al. (LeBlanc, Taylor, & Marchese, 2020) found programs may contribute to the challenges that
that 82% of the respondents reported to some- behavior analysts face in collaborating with non-­
times or often feel unprepared or not trained to behavioral colleagues.
deal with the emotional responses of the families
with whom they work. LeBlanc, Taylor et al. also
found that 92% of the respondents felt that their 8.3.6 Problems in Translating
colleagues also often struggle with soft skills. the Science into Everyday
Furthermore, 91% felt that soft skills are impor- Language
tant, while 83% thought that master’s programs
that produce behavior analysts should have soft Related to practitioner preparation for collabora-
skills as a component of their teaching. The tion, another concern that has been raised about
majority of respondents agreed that most training behavior analysis coursework is the strong focus
seems to be provided by supervisors in applied on the precise use of technical terminology
settings, but question the qualifications and expe- (Taylor et al., 2019). While the use of technical
rience of those responsible for providing such terminology can assist in more precise communi-
training, as they often are relatively new behavior cation (Neuman, 2018; Schlinger et al., 1991), it
analysts themselves. It is noteworthy that this is can be detrimental to collaboration with non-­
identified as a weakness by so many practitioners behavioral colleagues (Neuman, 2018). Becirevic
and that there is widespread recognition of the et  al. (2016) suggested that “collaboration
need for more expertise in this area within the requires conversation, a core prerequisite of
field. which would seem to be reliance on mutually
While empathy may not be directly related to acceptable and understandable terms” (p.  312).
collaborative problem-solving that furthers client However, behavior analysts are taught to avoid
outcomes, empathic behavior can be directly cor- non-technical language that is commonly used by
related with relationship building. In the intro- non-behavioral colleagues because these terms
duction of a new relationship, the behavior often attribute behavior to inner causes or hypo-
analyst may be a neutral stimulus. Empathic thetical constructs (Hineline, 1980). For exam-
responses that are reinforcing for colleagues can, ple, an instructor may be teaching the child to
through stimulus pairing, condition the behavior request a snack and may describe the instruc-
analyst as a reinforcing stimulus. On the other tional context as “arranging motivating opera-
hand, responses perceived as unemotional are tions to develop a manding repertoire related to
likely to be experienced as aversive, and any snack time.” While this accurately represents the
stimulus (i.e., the behavior analyst) consistently motivating operation in this situation, this may be
paired with an aversive stimulus will become a perceived by non-behavioral colleagues as a
conditioned aversive stimulus. As Skinner (1953) strange way of speaking.
importantly stated, “Any behavior which reduces Many publications have described the detri-
this conditioned aversive stimulation will be rein- mental effect of behavior analysis technical ter-
forced” (p.  188). In other words, any behavior minology on the marketing of behavior analysis
that results in avoiding or escaping the behavior and the ability for behavior analysts to effectively
analyst will be reinforced. Therefore, a lack of speak to and collaborate with those outside of the
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 131

field (Bailey, 1991; Carr, 1996; Doughty et  al., 2008). Jarmolowicz et  al. (2008) trained new
2012; Friman, 2004; Lindsley, 1991; Morris, therapists with limited experience with behavior
2014). One of the concerns is that behavior ana- analytic terminology using instructions with high
lysts’ focus on language that precisely describes and low amounts of jargon. The therapists with
the relationship between behavior and the envi- low jargon instructions were more successful in
ronment stands in opposition with the more implementing the procedure and indicated greater
broadly held view of free will. People who believe approval of the procedure than those therapists
that they control their own behavior and life may who received high jargon instructions. Banks
find behavior analytic terms, such as control and et al. (2018) found that technical instructions did
contingent, uncomfortable. In addition, this ter- not impact acceptability ratings or evaluation of
minology can lead to beliefs that behavior ana- the therapist presenting the instructions for par-
lysts have different priorities and values than ents of clinically referred children, but did
other professions and the general public (Carr, decrease comprehension. The parents provided
1996). Mainstream goals often include indepen- with technical instructions recalled fewer steps of
dence and self-esteem, and while most behavior the procedure than parents who contacted non-­
analysts would assert that they are working technical instructions. Reductions in procedural
toward those same goals, the terms used to repre- fidelity can be especially problematic when
sent these aspirations may differ greatly between behavior analysts work on interdisciplinary
behavior analysts and their non-behavioral col- teams, in which interventions are typically imple-
leagues (Carr, 1996). mented by numerous team members (Neuman,
Initial empirical research has shown the cost 2018). Interventions are more effective when
of using behavior analytic technical terminology implemented with fidelity, and, as such, ensuring
with non-behavior analysts. Rolider and col- all team members have access to language that
leagues (i.e., Rolider et  al., 1998; Rolider & increases their likelihood of accurate implemen-
Axelrod, 2005) provided instructions using tech- tation of procedures is imperative. This is partic-
nical terminology, conversational language, and ularly relevant for members of other professions,
conversational language with a rationale and who, like parents, would be unfamiliar with the
asked individuals from the general public (with- technical terms. Like parents in these studies,
out training in behavior analysis) to rate the they may implement procedures shared by the
instructions. These individuals rated the conver- behavior analyst with less precision.
sational instructions with a rationale as most Furthermore, it has been shown that behavior
understandable and acceptable and the technical analytic technical terminology elicits negative
instructions as least understandable and accept- emotional responses in non-behavior analysts
able. Becirevic et al. (2016) similarly found that (Critchfield, Becirevic, & Reed, 2017; Critchfield
in an online survey of individuals without behav- & Doepke, 2018; Critchfield, Doepke, et  al.,
ior analytic training, non-technical terms for 2017). Using publicly available ratings of the
behavior analysis procedures were rated as sig- emotional effect of terms, Critchfield and col-
nificantly more acceptable than the technical leagues found that behavior analytic terms were
terms for these same procedures. Decreased per- rated as more unhappy than the majority of terms
ceptions of the acceptability of behavior analytic in English and five other languages and were
procedures could significantly limit behavior rated as more unhappy than other technical sci-
analysts’ ability to have the procedures and pro- ence terms (Critchfield & Doepke, 2018;
tocols they recommend implemented by non-­ Critchfield, Doepke, et  al., 2017). Even when
behavioral colleagues. looking at overall communication, rather than
In addition to the possible negative impact on individual terms, the researchers found that a
adherence, technical terminology may have del- sample of behavior analytic writing was rated as
eterious effects on the implementation of inter- more negative (i.e., more unhappy at a higher
ventions (Banks et al., 2018; Jarmolowicz et al., intensity of emotion) than a sample of writing
132 M. J. Weiss et al.

from a non-behavioral psychologist (Critchfield, recommendations that are not evidence-based or


Becirevic, & Reed, 2017). Engagement in verbal may pose some harm to the client, the behavior
behavior that evokes negative emotional analyst becomes faced with the ethical dilemma
responses will make it difficult for behavior ana- due to conflicting ethical standards.
lysts to have effective collaborative interactions Often, the skills involved in reviewing the
with their non-behavioral colleagues. non-behavioral recommended treatments are not
specifically targeted in training novice behavior
analysts (Brodhead, 2015). Yet, it is a complex
8.3.7 Reluctance to Incorporate process that requires careful evaluation. Research
the Suggestions of Other has suggested that some aspects of non-­behavioral
Team Members treatments that require further review include cli-
ent safety, translation into behavioral principles,
As discussed above, behavior analysts may and if the treatment will interfere with the goals
become conditioned aversive stimuli as a result of the client or compromise the professional rela-
of uncompassionate behavior and the use of tech- tionship (Brodhead, 2015). Other research also
nical terminology. Similarly, a lack of respon- suggests the behavior analyst gather a better
siveness to and implementation of non-behavioral understanding of what led the other professional
colleagues’ recommendations may also be aver- to make these recommendations by further exam-
sive. Behavior analysts have reported that they ining their scope of practice and training and
are more likely to provide recommendations than their definitions and philosophical underpinnings
to implement recommendations provided by (LaFrance et al., 2019). As noted above, these are
other professionals within an interdisciplinary multilayered skills that may not have been
team (Kelly & Tincani, 2013). If behavior ana- addressed in coursework or supervision. Junior
lysts are not evaluating and implementing inter- behavior analysts (e.g., those with less than
ventions recommended by non-behavior 5 years of independent professional experience)
colleagues, they may miss out on the opportunity may be overwhelmed by the suggestions and
to conceptualize these recommendations through unsure of how to proceed when such interven-
a behavior analytic lens and either benefit from tions are suggested.
their potential value or determine their unaccept- Behavior analysts that are ill-prepared to
ableness. Without a consistent process for con- have these discussions may be at risk for simply
ceptualizing non-behavioral treatments presented agreeing in the absence of the skills required to
by colleagues, it is possible that behavior ana- negotiate with interdisciplinary team members.
lysts will not develop this skill set (Brodhead, Indeed, behavior analysts have been reported to
2015). This deficiency may lead to ineffective use interventions that are not conceptually sys-
behavior under circumstances when evaluation of tematic and may even be harmful to clients
other treatments is required (Brodhead, 2015). (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2018;
If effective collaboration occurred, behavior Schreck & Mazur, 2008; Schreck et al., 2016).
analysts would be able to effectively review treat- A highly endorsed rationale for continuing to
ments with those making the recommendation to use harmful treatments is persuasion by col-
better determine a successful and ethical treat- leagues/coworkers and clients/parents (Schreck
ment for their clients. Though the Ethics Code for et al., 2016). Without strategies in collaborative
Behavior Analysts (Behavior Analyst practices, including ways to effectively disagree
Certification Board, 2020a) states that behavior with collaborators, behavior analysts may con-
analysts are to engage in practices that are tinue to be persuaded by non-behavioral col-
evidence-­based and do no harm to clients, it also leagues to implement ineffective and harmful
states that behavior analysts are to engage in pro- treatments that both are damaging to clients and
fessional behavior. When working with non-­ contribute to negative perceptions about the
behavioral professionals that may make field of behavior analysis.
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 133

8.3.8 Perceptions of Behavior 8.4 Strategies to Increase


Analysts’ Collaborative Skills Effective Collaboration

Most recently, Taylor et al. (2019) surveyed fami- 8.4.1 L


 ooking to Other Fields’
lies of children with ASD who received behavior Models
analytic services from behavior analysts and
found that parents rated behavior analysts high in Bosch and Mansell (2015) defined five essential
listening to their concerns in the first meeting and elements of cross-discipline collaboration in
that behavior analysts were good at maintaining medicine: role clarity, trust and confidence, the
confidentiality. Families rated behavior analysts ability to overcome adversity, the ability to over-
low in the areas of caring for the entire family come personal differences, and collective leader-
and acknowledging mistakes or treatment fail- ship. In this model, the authors note the
ures. Parents reported that obstacles to partnering importance of keeping patient care the primary
with behavior analysts include the use of jargon, objective and the need to develop professional-
being distracted during meetings, and having ism and teamwork skills. These seem like essen-
interfering opinions about other disciplines. tial elements of a collaborative context when
Fellow professionals from allied fields often treatment goals and treatment responsibility are
rate behavior analysts as difficult to collaborate shared.
with, which may also impede the initiation and Gerenser and Koenig (2019) highlighted sev-
the success of collaborative efforts. For example, eral similar elements of a successful collabora-
speech and language pathologists have described tive model: create a strong team, establish trust,
behavior analysts as arrogant and as reliant on establish conflict resolution strategies, establish
jargon (McCulloch, 2016). Additionally, Koenig role clarity, establish effective leadership, focus
and Gerenser (2015, 2019) have noted the issues on outcomes, and become an advocate. In this
posed by perception of professional encroach- model, there is also an emphasis on the primacy
ment, which is a particular risk in the shared of the client’s needs, the establishment of a func-
scope of practice of communication skill devel- tional structure for team process, and the demon-
opment. In occupational therapy, behavior ana- stration of competent and respectful interaction.
lysts may be perceived as failing to use naturalistic There is also a need to ensure that members of
interventions or as de-emphasizing generaliza- each profession understand the value and exper-
tion (Welch & Polatajko, 2016). These percep- tise of the other professions. In order to achieve
tions may limit the contexts and goals in which an understanding of the frameworks that define
occupational therapists seek collaborative prac- each discipline, it is important to provide both a
tice opportunities with behavior analysts. formal introduction to the disciplines (e.g., in
Similarly, behavior analysts may hold mispercep- coursework) and informal opportunities to share
tions of other professions. They may perceive information with professionals from other disci-
occupational therapists as using non-evidence-­ plines (LaFrance et  al., 2019). Koenig and
based interventions or as primarily implementing Gerenser (2006, 2019) make several suggestions
sensory integration techniques (Welch & for increasing understanding between behavior
Polatajko, 2016). They may also underemphasize analysts and speech and language pathologists,
the precision and data collection utilized in including sharing articles from their fields, dem-
speech interventions (Koenig & Gerenser, 2015, onstrating intervention techniques, and creating
2019). Such misperceptions intensify the gaps forums for the development of personal and pro-
between professional fields and reduce the likeli- fessional connections between the disciplines. In
hood that practitioners will seek, engage in, and addition, these authors have made suggestions
succeed in collaboration across disciplines. for creating strong teams including establishing
134 M. J. Weiss et al.

trust, using conflict resolution strategies, estab- certification (Behavior Analyst Certification
lishing role clarity, and focusing on outcomes Board, 2020b; Shook & Johnston, 2011).
(Gerenser & Koenig, 2019). In essence, these dis- However, collaboration is often not included in
ciplines are the primary providers for individuals this training, as evident by the majority of indi-
with ASD and share many values and goals for viduals stating they had not taken courses on col-
their shared clientele. Working together and in laboration (Kelly & Tincani, 2013). Furthermore,
tandem improves outcomes, which is highly val- upon review of the Behavior Analyst Certification
ued by both professions. Finding common ground Board’s requirements for certification of course-
and appreciating the contribution of each disci- work content requirements, there is no training
pline allow for more comprehensive care, coordi- hour requirement on the topic of collaboration
nated interventions, and maximal outcomes. (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2020b).
Though some colleges and universities may offer
electives for individuals to learn collaboration
8.4.2 Increased Training skills, those courses are generally not mandatory
and Education for graduation or certification.
Offering courses with a focus on effective col-
The growing number of behavior analysts that laboration within behavior analysis and other
participate in interdisciplinary teams necessitates professions would increase behavior analysts’
a focus on training and educating behavior ana- understanding of other professions and their
lysts to do so successfully. The majority of par- roles. Knowledge of the other professionals’ dis-
ticipants of a survey stated that no course or ciplines assists in creating an effective collabora-
official training on collaboration was provided to tive partnership across disciplines (Cox, 2012;
them during their education (Kelly & Tincani, LaFrance et al., 2019; Welch & Polatajko, 2016).
2013). In the same survey, the majority of the Information about the scope and practice and
same participants stated they collaborate daily code of ethics for all involved disciplines may
with other disciplines but that the collaboration increase the behavior analyst’s acceptance of
only resulted in minor changes for the client other professions through acknowledgment of
(Kelly & Tincani, 2013). areas other disciplines would contribute to the
Staff training and education on collaboration course of treatment (Cox, 2012; LaFrance et al.,
can occur across stages in the behavior analysts’ 2019; LeBlanc, Sellers, & Ala’i, 2020).
development. Behavior analysts receive training Integrating this information into coursework and
and education throughout their career. This supervision would further prepare the behavior
begins with their coursework and fieldwork analyst to be an effective member of an interdis-
supervision and continues with workshops, con- ciplinary team.
tinued education events, and onsite training. To further supplement coursework, students of
Increasing the focus of collaboration during behavior analysis should seek training on effec-
coursework and fieldwork supervision provides tive collaboration from their fieldwork supervi-
the behavior analyst with a collaborative skill set sor. One of the first professional collaborative
prior to entering the field (Shook & Johnston, relationships that the student of behavior analysis
2011). Upon starting their career, the behavior is faced with is the collaborative relationship
analyst would then be immediately able to par- with their fieldwork supervisor (LeBlanc, Sellers,
ticipate in interdisciplinary collaboration. The & Ala’i, 2020). Building this relationship
behavior analyst could then also benefit from between supervisor and supervisee as a collabor-
increased training onsite with their employer and ative relationship provides opportunities for the
from professionally offered workshops and con- student to engage in activities that develop their
tinued education events. effective communication, treatment planning col-
Behavior analysts are required to engage in laboration, and professional relationship skills
coursework and supervised fieldwork prior to that may then generalize to collaborative rela-
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 135

tionships in their careers (LeBlanc, Sellers, & staff implementation of mand training (Nigro-­
Ala’i, 2020). Bruzzi & Sturmey, 2010), proficiency with teach-
Since many behavior analysts work in inter- ing the Picture Exchange Communication System
disciplinary teams, fieldwork supervisors from (PECS)® to students (Rosales et al., 2009), and
those sites can involve their students of behavior compliance with dental care (Graudins et  al.,
analysis during collaborative activities to enhance 2012), and to teach gun safety to small children
their supervision experience, which will also (Himle et  al., 2004). With the success of the
serve to increase understanding of other disci- evidence-­based approach of BST, it is possible to
plines (LeBlanc, Sellers, & Ala’i, 2020). Some of add soft skills to the repertoires of pre-­
these experiences could include observations of credentialed behavior analysts.
Individualized Education Plan (IEP) meetings, Another competency-based training method
overlaps with allied health team members, par- that has been shown to be effective is the teach-
ticipation in group consultation of other behavior ing interaction procedure (TIP; Ferguson et al.,
analysts, and reviews of reports from other disci- 2021; Green et  al., 2020; Leaf et  al., 2015).
plines. Each of these activities increases the The TIP includes identifying the skill to be
future behavior analyst’s experiences and pro- taught, providing rationales as to why the skill
vides a model of how to successfully engage in needs to be taught, breaking down the skill into
these activities (LeBlanc, Sellers, & Ala’i, 2020). smaller steps that the individual is able to state,
Training professionals continues to be an area demonstrating the steps completed correctly
of growing research (Parsons et  al., 2012). and incorrectly while the individual being
Didactic training procedures involve the use of trained identifies which model was the accu-
lectures to convey the novel skills to those being rate model, having the individual engage in
trained. Research demonstrates this method may role-play of the skill until all steps are accu-
not be the most effective method to train staff rate, and providing feedback throughout the
skills that involve the application of the skill process (Leaf et al., 2015).
(Parsons et  al., 2012; Williams et  al., 2012). Employers should also provide opportunities
Active responding by those being trained has for training in this crucial area. This can take a
demonstrated an increase in performance of variety of forms and might include messages
novel skills (Williams et al., 2012). Competency-­ about role definition, the value of collaborative
based training further ensures active participation work, and the model of interdisciplinary coordi-
of all individuals being trained and is supported nation used at that company. Competency-based
as an evidence-based training procedure (Parsons training at the workplace can be enhanced with
et al., 2012). It is important that the training pro- increased offerings of workshops for all employ-
grams used incorporate active engagement of the ees. Collaboration within the company is essen-
trainees and focus on competency-based assess- tial at all levels, and the use of BST or the TIP can
ment of the targeted outcomes. be implemented with multiple staff simultane-
Competency-based training involves active ously within a pyramidal approach (Parsons
responding by the individuals being trained until et  al., 2013). Specifically, training using
performance reaches a mastery criterion. competency-­based methods to teach collabora-
Behavior skills training (BST) involves providing tion would include defining what collaboration is
written and verbal instructions, modeling the and what each individual’s role is, providing a
trained skill, having the individuals practice the rationale as to why collaboration is important and
skills, and providing feedback on performance can increase client gains, breaking down the steps
(Parsons et al., 2012). This is then repeated until of effective collaboration, modeling each step of
mastery is achieved. BST is an evidence-based the process, role-playing effective collaboration
procedure that has been used to teach not only until mastery of each step is complete, and pro-
children but also adults valuable skills, such as viding feedback throughout the training.
136 M. J. Weiss et al.

8.4.3 Soft Skills Development Educators are also trained in the area of com-
passion and collaboration. Through coursework,
Many human service professions, including the readings, supervision, and professional develop-
medical, psychological, and nursing professions, ment, teachers have been required to develop
include a focus on collaboration by including the these skills to better serve students and families.
development of soft skills, including active lis- The First Principle in their ethics code,
tening, compassion, and empathy within their Commitment to Student, states that educators
training. With the direct teaching of soft skills help students realize potential by emphasizing
including active listening, making empathic the development of knowledge, inquiry, and goal
statements, and asking clarifying questions, pro- development (National Education Association,
fessionals develop the skills necessary to work 1975). The need for soft skills can also be found
collaboratively with patients, families, and pro- under the heading of the six characteristics of
fessions from other disciplines. ethical teaching which, along with compassion,
Studies have shown that when physicians have includes appreciation for moral deliberation,
contacted teaching of compassionate care through empathy, knowledge, reasoning, courage, and
coursework, professional development, and read- interpersonal skills (Lynch & Forde, 2016).
ing assignments, patients report being heard and In behavior analysis, evidence-based methods
understood, feeling better about the treatment, exist for training and can be applied to teaching
and adhering to treatment recommendations soft skills to behavior analysts. BST has been
(Bonvicini et  al., 2009; Coulehan et  al., 2001; shown to be effective for training a wide variety
Kelm et al., 2014; Strauss et al., 2016). Physicians of skills (simple and complex) across a wide vari-
who engage in compassionate care are reported ety of populations (including children and adults
as having lower burnout rates, higher rates of per- with and without disabilities) (e.g., Parsons et al.,
sonal well-being, higher ratings of clinical com- 2012). Implementation of BST in coursework
petence, and much less medical-legal risk (Kelm and fieldwork supervision for future behavior
et al., 2014). analysts may assist in the development of these
Historically, nurses have had compassion skills.
emphasized as part of their training and profes-
sional development (e.g., Buckley et  al., 2004;
Moscato et al., 2007). In their code of ethics, the 8.4.4 Understanding Roles
first of nine provisions states, “The nurse prac- and Contributions
tices with compassion and respect for the inher-
ent dignity, worth and unique attributes of every Overlapping scopes of practice and role delinea-
person” (American Nurses Association, 2015, p. tion are commonly cited sources of conflict in
V). This is similar to the value embraced in phy- interprofessional collaboration (Brown et  al.,
sician training and is explicitly taught as part of 2011; Suter et  al., 2009). Interdisciplinary
nurse education. By including soft skills into approaches to treatment aim to provide more
their training repertoire, nurses are prepared and comprehensive services by capitalizing on the
equipped with the necessary tools to effectively unique knowledge and skills of each individual
collaborate with multiple physicians, hospital discipline. However, these joint efforts may
departments, support personnel, patients, and expose the intersecting competencies across pro-
families. fessions, leaving the discipline-specific expertise
This is also true in the psychology field where seemingly less exclusive. Thus, the boundaries
psychologists, social workers, and mental health that traditionally separate the disciplines become
workers are specifically trained in the area of less discernible resulting in “role blurring” (Suter
compassion. Through reading, coursework, train- et  al., 2009, p.  44), professional “turf wars”
ing, and supervision, these professionals are (Chung et  al., 2012, p.  37), and a resistance to
trained in the soft skills necessary for work cooperatively (Hall, 2005; Suter et  al.,
collaboration. 2009). To preserve the harmony of interprofes-
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 137

sional collaboration and promote effective prac- [or]…what the organism is doing” (Skinner,
tices, it is crucial that each team member 1938, p. 6). This broad definition encompasses a
understand not only their own role on the treat- range of responses and activities including both
ment team but the scope of practice, expertise, observable, external events and private, internal
and contributions of their colleagues as well experiences (Skinner, 1945). As such, these defi-
(Hall, 2005; LaFrance et al., 2019; Strunk et al., nitions of behavior and our practice support the
2017). widespread application of our science and its
LaFrance et  al. (2019) summarized the roles technologies to a multitude of socially significant
and responsibilities of four key professionals challenges. Consequently, the potential for inter-
who commonly provide services to individuals professional conflict persists as the scope of our
with ASD under a collaborative model: ABA, discipline is likely to impinge the practice pur-
speech-language pathology, occupational ther- views of our non-behavioral colleagues. In turn,
apy, and psychology. Specifically, LaFrance et al. our colleagues may propose non-behavioral
examined professional documents from each interventions to enact desired behavior changes
field that outlined each discipline’s scope of prac- and treat deficits that are also within the scope of
tice and training. Their purpose was to clarify ABA.
roles and identify practice and/or training areas
which overlap with applied behavior analysis and
may, therefore, cause contention in collaborative 8.4.5 N
 avigating a Shared Scope
relationships. The descriptions of training and of Practice
practice from each field were analyzed and
directly compared to the Behavior Analyst The core characteristics and comorbid conditions
Certification Board’s Model Act for Licensing/ of ASD often serve as the junction where profes-
Regulating Behavior Analysts (BACB Model sionals from different disciplines intersect and
Act; Behavior Analyst Certification Board, their overlapping practice areas are manifested.
2012). Areas of overlap in practice and/or train- For example, sleep disturbances of children with
ing were noted if the content or practice activities ASD are quite prevalent (Rana et al., 2021) and
of the discipline included an analysis of func- span the practice domains of ASD service provid-
tional relations as described by the BACB Model ers including pediatric medicine (Malow et  al.,
Act. While they initially identified a small num- 2016), occupational therapy (Ho & Siu, 2018),
ber of overlapping areas in training and practice, and behavior analysis (Jin et al., 2013). Suppose
a more detailed review of educational standards a client receiving services from these profession-
for each discipline revealed greater distinctions. als reports abnormalities in sleep behavior and
Hence, they concluded more differentiation than requests treatment. Each member of this treat-
overlap between ABA and other disciplines com- ment team will likely recommend an intervention
mon to ASD treatment. according to their discipline-specific clinical per-
While these distinctions highlight the value spective, although each treatment may be
and unique expertise of each discipline, the intended to produce the same outcomes (e.g.,
expansive application of ABA may indeed over- increased duration of sleep, reduced latency).
lap with the practice areas of other professions. That is, each professional will draw on their dis-
The BACB Model Act defines the practice of cipline’s distinct academic training, available sci-
ABA as the application of scientifically sup- entific evidence, and underlying theoretical
ported interventions “to help people develop new foundations, resulting in three separate interven-
behaviors, increase or decrease existing behav- tions each designed to yield the desired behavior
iors, and emit behaviors under specific environ- changes.
mental conditions” (Behavior Analyst There are many disorders, in addition to sleep,
Certification Board, 2012, p. 3). Behavior is rec- that fall within the boundaries of competence and
ognized as “the movement of the organism… practice for both behavior analysts and other
138 M. J. Weiss et al.

collaborating professionals. Additional examples and establish a good rapport with their non-­
include speech, language, social interactions, behavioral colleagues, it benefits both the client
feeding, self-care, leisure activities, household and the field of ABA. Clients are afforded more
tasks, motor coordination, academic skills, safety, comprehensive services that may be able to
maladaptive behaviors, and caregiver training, address a wider array of needs with increased
among others. Since deficits in these areas are treatment integrity. Additionally, through the
often the focus of treatment for individuals with interprofessional relationships, behavior analysts
ASD, collaborating professionals will quite often have the opportunity to disseminate the science
be met with multiple, independent recommenda- of ABA and demonstrate the cooperative efforts
tions derived from a specific discipline base. of the field (see Behavior Analyst Certification
Circumstances such as these potentially threaten Board, 2020a). However, as Brodhead explained,
the cooperative nature of interprofessional col- our ethical obligation to disseminate the science
laboration. When discussing the available treat- does not imply that we should criticize non-­
ment options to determine the best course of behavioral treatment recommendations by ques-
action, non-collaborative practices such as ques- tioning a colleague’s proposal or offering
tioning the validity of the proposed treatments, alternative interventions that we believe to be
responding with skepticism, or suggesting modi- superior. Such contentious actions incite conflict,
fications or alternatives may lead to disagree- eventually causing termination of the relation-
ments among team members (Brodhead, 2015). ship and thereby excluding the behavior analyst
Such responses may be interpreted as profes- from future collaborative care. Since interprofes-
sional impudence which fosters mistrust, discord, sional approaches offer many advantages to cli-
and eventual dissolution. Although it may avoid ent care and the field of behavior analysis, and
interprofessional conflict, simply permitting each non-collaborative practices present such risks,
professional to implement their own intervention Brodhead encourages careful assessment of non-­
in a parallel manner moves the team from a truly behavioral treatments before addressing any
collaborative approach to a mere eclectic pack- concerns.
age with no scientific support (Dillenburger,
2011). Furthermore, these “intervention med-
leys” may be composed of contradictory treat- 8.4.6 Decision-Making Models
ments and procedures that impede progress or
produce unwanted side effects (Cox, 2019). 8.4.6.1 Brodhead’s Model
Behavior analysts in particular are ethically obli- Brodhead (2015) developed a systematic means
gated to evaluate the effects of treatment proce- of appraising non-behavioral treatments which
dures which may impact current objectives and guides behavior analytic practitioners through a
behavior change programs and endorse only the comprehensive analysis of the intervention while
most effective, scientifically supported interven- protecting the professional relationship from any
tions (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, unnecessary conflict (Behavior Analyst
2020a). As such, it is critical that behavior ana- Certification Board, 2020a). His decision-­making
lysts and all members of the interprofessional model begins with an assessment of client safety.
team learn to navigate challenges such as these to The behavior analyst must evaluate any threat to
avoid unnecessary conflict and ensure their cli- safety posed by the non-behavioral treatment by
ents receive the most efficacious treatment considering both short-term and long-term physi-
(Brodhead, 2015). cal and psychological harm. Threats to safety
Brodhead (2015) recognized the value of may also include interventions that will limit
cooperative relationships in interprofessional access to more effective, empirically supported
collaboration and warned against the detrimental treatments. Any safety risks require the behavior
effects of non-collaborative practices. When analyst to immediately address the proposed
behavior analysts demonstrate professionalism intervention with the non-behavioral colleague.
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 139

Once safety concerns are eliminated, the behav- time is caused by a proprioceptive dysfunction.
ior analyst should gain a greater understanding of She recommends use of a small, child-size ther-
the specific procedures and evidence validating apy ball with ring stabilizer to help him maintain
use of the recommended treatment. A variety of a state of arousal (Bagatell et al., 2010). According
resources should be explored to gather informa- to the occupational therapist, sitting on the ther-
tion needed to assess the potential effects of the apy ball during circle time will provide additional
intervention. Examples include scientific litera- proprioceptive input, promote balance and pos-
ture outside of behavior analysis, consultations tural control, and increase Timmy’s attention and
with other professionals from the colleague’s dis- engagement during the circle time activities.
cipline, and reviews provided by organizations A behavior analyst’s initial reaction may be to
that compile and summarize scientific evidence refute this non-behavioral treatment on the
for ASD treatments. Details of the underlying grounds that it lacks empirical support and
mechanisms, procedures, empirical support, and instead propose a behavioral intervention to
philosophical assumptions can help the behavior decrease the elopement. However, by analyzing
analyst understand the recommendation from the this non-behavioral treatment according to the
non-behavioral colleague’s perspective. With this model developed by Brodhead (2015), we can
information, the behavior analyst can translate carefully and systematically evaluate the inter-
the non-behavioral treatment into behavioral vention before we contest it and risk compromis-
principles and terminology to further evaluate its ing the relationship with our colleague. Beginning
potential effects. As Brodhead (2015) explained, with an assessment of safety, we see no threats to
a behavioral interpretation of the treatment the client’s physical or psychological well-being.
removes any discipline-specific jargon, hypothet- We consider that this intervention might prevent
ical constructs, or dualistic theories of causation implementation of a more effective alternative,
that impede a behavior analytic perspective. The but for the sake of a thorough analysis, we pro-
specific variables and methods are analyzed ceed through the model. Next, we gather addi-
according to the principles and procedures of tional information on the use of therapy balls and
behavior analysis to identify conceptual consis- proprioceptive input for decreasing elopement
tencies that may explain its potential success and increasing attention. We review publications
(Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 2020a). from the field of occupational therapy, speak with
As an example, let us consider a fictitious case an occupational therapist from a neighboring
where Timmy, a 6-year-old child with ASD, is school, and visit organizations such as the
enrolled in an inclusive kindergarten program Association for Science in Autism Treatment to
and is being treated by an interprofessional col- read recommendations based on currently avail-
laboration team consisting of a special education able research. As we suspected, there is little
teacher, speech-language pathologist, occupa- empirical support for sensory integration therapy
tional therapist, and behavior analyst. The special (Zimmer & Desch, 2012). Yet, through this
education teacher expresses concerns regarding review, we have come to understand our col-
Timmy’s ability to remain seated during the league’s clinical perspective, sensory integration
morning circle time, stay within the circle, and theory, and the procedures involved in this non-­
attend to the various activities. The behavior ana- behavioral intervention. With this information,
lyst conducts a functional behavior assessment we can translate the non-behavioral treatment
which indicates the elopement from circle time is into behavioral principles. The function of
primarily maintained by access to preferred toys Timmy’s behavior was access to tangible items
located at the back of the classroom, specifically that allowed movements such as jumping, bounc-
the mini trampoline, inflatable rocking horse, and ing, and rocking. Making the therapy ball freely
foam pogo stick. The occupational therapist available during circle time is an antecedent-­
explains that Timmy is hyposensitive to environ- based intervention which will provide reinforce-
mental stimuli and his elopement from circle ment (bouncing movements) non-contingently.
140 M. J. Weiss et al.

Thus, the therapy ball may function as an abol- considerations when collaborating with profes-
ishing operation which may decrease the value of sionals from other disciplines. According to
the trampoline, rocking horse, and pogo stick and Newhouse-Oisten et al., to appraise the effects of
abate the elopement behaviors. a proposed treatment, the behavior analyst should
The behavioral translation identified compo- first review the available scientific evidence using
nents in the non-behavioral treatment that are many of the same resources recommended by
conceptually consistent with behavior analytic Brodhead. Next, the behavior analyst must assess
principles and procedures, namely, non-­compatibility with both the goals of the program
contingent reinforcement, abolishing operations, and the existing treatment procedures. The goal
and abative effects. While the occupational thera- of the proposed treatment is considered compati-
pist referenced sensory processing dysfunction as ble if it is consistent with the current goals of the
a cause for the problem behaviors and attributed program and is intended to produce desirable
their expected decrease to the internal changes outcomes. In the same way, the procedures are
brought on by the therapy ball, the procedures considered compatible if they can be imple-
actually translated into a potentially effective mented concomitantly and without impeding cur-
behavioral intervention. rent methods. This analysis results in dichotomous
According to Brodhead (2015), if the behav- distinctions of evidence-based/non-evidence-­
ioral translation reveals conceptual similarities based interventions and compatible/incompatible
that can explain its potential success, then there is interventions.
no need to address concerns with the non-­ Under the Newhouse-Oisten et  al. (2017)
behavioral colleague. The intervention may be decision-making model, the classification of the
accepted and monitored to measure its effects. In proposed treatment across the dichotomous cate-
our above example, the translation demonstrated gories then dictates the behavior analyst’s deci-
how an unsupported treatment recommendation, sion regarding acceptance of the treatment.
based on unproven theory, could be an effective Proposed interventions classified as both
practice. Thus, we were able to avoid unneces- evidence-­based and compatible with the current
sary conflict and preserve our relationship with treatment program should be adopted. For exam-
the occupational therapist and other team mem- ple, suppose the special education teacher on an
bers. If, however, the success of a non-behavioral interprofessional team recommends computer-­
treatment cannot be achieved through a behav- assisted instruction to teach literacy skills to a
ioral translation, then the behavior analyst must 9-year-old child with autism. The efficacy of this
evaluate its compatibility with the current objec- instructional method has been demonstrated and
tives of the treatment program and consider if the offers advantages over conventional approaches
level of interference is significant such that it such as improved consistency and generalization
warrants the risk of conflict by addressing it with (Root et al., 2017). Given the client’s preference
the non-behavioral colleague (Behavior Analyst for computers and current reading objectives, the
Certification Board, 2020a; Brodhead, 2015). team classifies this intervention as evidence-­
based and compatible and opts to implement.
8.4.6.2 Newhouse-Oisten’s Model Interventions identified as evidence-based but
for Use with Prescribing incompatible should be further evaluated with
Professionals consideration of stakeholder preferences and the
Issues of compatibility and scientific support are advantages and disadvantages of both the new
central to an additional decision-making model and existing treatments. As an example, a speech-­
developed by Newhouse-Oisten et  al. (2017). language pathologist on an interprofessional
While their model is presented in the context of team recommends the Lidcombe Program to treat
pharmacological interventions, we find that it speech disfluencies exhibited by a 5-year-old
aligns with the model described by Brodhead child with ASD. The current treatment program
(2015) and is applicable to a variety of treatment includes differential reinforcement and demand
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 141

fading procedures to decrease escape-maintained (PECS; Frost & Bondy, 2002). The special edu-
tantrum behaviors primarily evoked by adult cation teacher recommends the rapid prompting
demands. The Lidcombe Program is an effective method to allow the child to demonstrate more
procedure for decreasing stuttering behaviors advanced language and literacy skills through
(Nye et al., 2013). However, components of this textual communication (Schlosser et  al., 2019).
intervention include parental requests for self-­ The rapid prompting method lacks empirical evi-
evaluation of speech fluency and requests for cor- dence to support its use and is dangerously simi-
rection of stuttered utterances (Onslow et  al., lar to the debunked and harmful method of
2020). These requests will interfere with the cur- facilitated communication. It would therefore be
rent demand fading procedures. Consequently, classified as not evidence-based and incompati-
this approach is classified as evidence-based, but ble (American Speech-Language-Hearing
incompatible, and the interprofessional team will Association, 2018; Schlosser et al., 2019). Hence,
need to determine which of these interventions the interprofessional team should reject this
should be adopted. intervention.
Interventions deemed not evidence-based but
compatible may either be rejected due to insuffi-
cient scientific support or (under some condi- 8.4.7 Consulting Resources
tions) be considered for implementation if
beneficial to the client with respect to their values A fundamental aspect of the decision-making
and preferences (see also BACB, 2020a, section models developed by Brodhead (2015) and
2.0; Brodhead, 2015). Again, the behavior ana- Newhouse-Oisten et  al. (2017) is reviewing the
lyst and other team members should evaluate the available scientific evidence for a proposed treat-
benefits and disadvantages of accepting the inter- ment. Unfortunately, this crucial step is a daunt-
vention and weigh factors such as client safety, ing task for a practitioner. Empirical investigations
time commitments, cost, and ease of implemen- must be assessed for both quality and quantity,
tation. As an example, consider a case where a that is, the methodological rigor, strength of
nutritionist recommends camel milk to treat the experimental design, replication, and consistency
symptoms of autism based on reports of decreased of findings (DiGennaro Reed et  al., 2018;
oxidative stress and improved behaviors National Autism Center, 2015; Reichow, 2011).
(AL-Ayadhi & Elamin, 2013). This treatment According to the National Autism Center XE
entails replacing the child’s daily intake of two "National Autism Center" (2015), evidence is
cups of reduced fat cow milk with pasteurized sufficient to establish an intervention as effective
camel milk. While substituting the dairy source when at least 2 high-quality group designs or 4
will not interfere with any aspects of the child’s high-quality single-subject designs are conducted
current treatment program, the evidence for with at least 12 participants consistently replicat-
camel milk as an effective autism treatment is ing the effects without conflict. Alternatively,
lacking (Williamson et  al., 2017). As such, this Reichow (2011) graded interventions as estab-
proposed treatment would be classified as not lished when 5 methodologically sound, single-­
evidence-based, but compatible. The team may subject designs with at least 15 participants, or 2
refute the requested changes based on the lack of group designs, were conducted across different
evidence. research groups and in different geographic loca-
Finally, when an intervention is classified as tions. More replications are necessary to demon-
not evidence-based and incompatible, it should strate sufficient evidence when experimental
be rejected, and the behavior analyst may con- designs are weak, findings are inconsistent, or
sider proposing an alternative treatment. Suppose methodology is imprecise. In any case, it can be
current interventions for a 6-year-old with ASD difficult for practitioners to keep up with the
include manding with complete sentences using accrual of evidence, especially for interventions
the Picture Exchange Communication System that come from many different fields.
142 M. J. Weiss et al.

Many reputable organizations have compiled from publications within and beyond the fields of
and summarized the available research for autism speech-language pathology and audiology as
treatments, which may offer practitioners a more well as ASHA’s position statements and policies
efficient and reliable means of examining the sci- on harmful, yet controversial, treatments such as
ence. Notably, the Association for Science in facilitated communication and rapid prompting.
Autism Treatment, the National Autism Center at Summaries and quality ratings are provided for
May Institute, and Autism New Jersey have cat- many of the systematic reviews and meta-­
egorized treatments as established, emerging, analyses, with conclusions regarding the efficacy
untested, and ineffective according to the strength of the intervention.
and quantity of available evidence (Association ASHA’s position statements are particularly
for Science in Autism Treatment, n.d.; Autism helpful and are focused on the extent to which
New Jersey, n.d.; National Autism Center, 2015). procedures have been empirically verified to be
Established interventions are those with substan- effective and the extent to which they pose risk.
tial scientific support and may generally be ASHA has position statements on a number of
accepted by practitioners pending an assessment commonly suggested interventions, including
of compatibility. Emerging interventions have auditory integration training, oral motor exer-
some preliminary empirical evidence supporting cises, facilitated communication, and rapid
their use, but need additional research. Untested prompting method. In all of these statements,
treatments are also in need of empirical investiga- there are a summary of existing evidence, an out-
tion as their effects are unknown. Interprofessional line of known ineffectiveness or documented
teams should further analyze proposed treat- harm, and guidelines for practitioners regarding
ments classified as emerging and untested before the incorporation of the procedure. There is util-
implementing. Those interventions classified as ity for the practitioner in these documents, as
ineffective or harmful should, of course, be they are reminded of their obligations to use and
addressed with the colleague who is recommend- recommend only empirically supported,
ing them, regardless of the risks for conflict, and evidence-­based interventions. There is also util-
an alternate intervention should be identified in ity for consumers, as they are helping to avoid
these instances. interventions that do not have merit and are likely
Another resource recommendation is the to be an unwise investment of time, resources,
American Speech-Language-Hearing and hope.
Association (ASHA). While this is the profes- Other professional guild organizations also
sional organization for speech-language patholo- have position statements that can be extremely
gists and audiologists and is not exclusively useful in collaborative contexts. For example, the
focused on effective ASD treatments, their online American Academy of Pediatrics has position
evidence maps serve as a valuable tool for review- statements on auditory integration, facilitated
ing empirical research in ASD treatments, par- communication, and sensory integration therapy.
ticularly as it relates to the scope of practice for In all of these cases, the statements recommend
speech-language pathology and audiology against the use of these procedures and indicate
(American Speech-Language-Hearing that there is a lack of evidence to support their
Association, n.d.). Their website provides an evi- use.
dence map specifically for ASD spectrum disor- The value of position statements from other
ders that can be filtered to generate treatments professions is great, as they provide external and
from domains such as feeding and language or independent advice on proposed paths of treat-
more specific approaches such as augmentative ment. A behavior analyst may be viewed as
and alternative communication or joint attention biased for their own profession and for interven-
interventions. Results of the search include sys- tions that originated within behavior analysis.
tematic reviews, meta-analyses, and guidelines When sources outside of behavior analysis are
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 143

cited, it becomes a broader dialogue, and the ple, it is important that attention be given to the
focus is on effective intervention, and not on target behaviors; in other words, the team should
field-specific assessments of individual identify and systematically track the behavioral
interventions. changes expected with the intervention. Similarly,
It is helpful for behavior analysts to have a the procedure should be specifically defined, and
broad understanding of the positions that allied measures of procedural fidelity should be taken
fields have on commonly implemented interven- to ensure that it is implemented as planned. Inter-­
tions. Staying abreast of these positions by guild observer agreement measures should also be
organizations can enlarge the list of resources obtained, so that an objective assessment of
one consults and shares with the interdisciplinary impact is assured. Additionally, when possible,
team. Diverse and user-friendly resources can aid applying the intervention in a single-case design
consumers and professionals in understanding format is ideal. In this way, there can be confi-
the current state-of-the-science when it comes to dence that the procedure was implemented in a
evidence for or against a particular intervention. way that allows for assessment of functional con-
trol. For example, the use of an alternating treat-
8.4.7.1 Treatment Classifications ments design may help to identify whether
Autism New Jersey has separated empirically attention was improved on days in which a sen-
supported interventions from those lacking suffi- sory diet was implemented or whether the perfor-
cient evidence with a simple traffic light analogy mance on days in which the sensory diet was not
(Autism New Jersey, n.d.). Green light treatments utilized was equivalent or better. Finally, the team
signal efficacy. Yellow light treatments are those should agree to a data-based decision on continu-
that should be implemented with caution and ance or discontinuance of the procedure. This
need additional research regarding their potential should be discussed before the intervention is
impact. Red light treatments are those that have begun, and there should be an explicit commit-
been proven to be ineffective and/or harmful and ment to “listen to the data” regarding the ultimate
that should not be implemented. decision about the intervention’s inclusion in the
To illustrate the use of these classification sys- individual’s treatment plan.
tems in the evaluation of treatments presented in At times, a yellow light procedure’s assess-
collaborative contexts, consider a recommenda- ment process might yield useful data to guide
tion for music therapy. A review of the informa- treatment. For example, a behavior analyst could
tion presented by the organizations listed be asked to participate in an assessment of the
previously indicates this treatment is a yellow impact of brushing for an individual learner. An
light procedure, an emerging intervention with outside occupational therapist may recommend
preliminary evidence suggesting it may be effec- the use of a brushing protocol to reduce stereo-
tive for individuals with ASD (Association for typy and to increase attention and performance
Science in Autism Treatment, n.d.; Autism New on programs. The behavior analyst would work
Jersey, n.d.; National Autism Center, 2015). A with the occupational therapy and the team to
behavioral translation and compatibility assess- design a comparison of the brushing interven-
ment should be conducted to further analyze the tions with two other conditions: free play and
potential effects, but given this information, the intensive social play. In all three conditions, the
practitioner may opt to avoid unnecessary con- child can be removed from the classroom and be
flict and agree to closely monitor the impact of sent to the gym to engage in one of the three con-
this treatment. ditions (i.e., brushing, free play, intensive social
Yellow light procedures are those that might play). Educational staff would not know which
be individually applied while impact is closely condition was experienced prior to instruction.
assessed. Certain conditions are important to put Over the course of several weeks, multiple data
in place for these trial interventions. For exam- points can be accrued for each condition, and the
144 M. J. Weiss et al.

rates of stereotypy and indices of attention and 8.4.8 Summary and Future


mastery can be evaluated. Potentially, results Directions
could indicate that the brushing condition was
associated with lower rates of stereotypy and Autism is a complex disorder that presents in
higher attention and engagement than the free highly variable ways. For this reason, treatment
play (control) condition. However, the lowest requires the expertise of multiple disciplines, to
levels of stereotypy and the highest levels of ensure that assessment and treatment are compre-
attention and instructional performance were hensive and individually tailored.
seen in the intensive social play condition. The Interdisciplinary models of treatment are well
data could then be reviewed by the entire team, suited to the unique profiles and challenges asso-
and all might agree that intensive social play was ciated with autism, and coordinated intervention
the most effective intervention to achieve these across disciplines leads to better outcomes.
goals. Going forward, the team could precede Challenges in the implementation and coordi-
instructional sessions with intensive social play. nation of an interdisciplinary model are many
In this case and outcome, the team was able to and include differing definitions of evidence-­
“let the data speak.” When it is possible and safe based practice, discipline-specific worldviews
to do so, such comparative intervention trials can and intervention approaches, lack of training in
shed light on the potential utility of an interven- collaborative models of care and in soft skills that
tion. As in the brushing example, it might even increase the success of collaboration, the use of
lead to the identification of an alternative inter- jargon that impedes understanding across disci-
vention that might actually work better than the plines, the reluctance of behavior analysts to
proposed yellow light procedure. incorporate the suggestions of other team mem-
Some procedures are not safe or even poten- bers, and the negative perception of behavior
tially therapeutic, and every effort must be made analyst hold by members of other professions.
to ensure that they are not implemented. A number of strategies can be used to bridge
Accordingly, red light procedures are harmful these divides and to equip practitioners with
and should immediately be addressed with the skills to be more effective in interdisciplinary
prescribing colleague regardless of the risks of contexts. Several professions have created mod-
interpersonal conflict. For example, Autism New els and resources that can assist in this process,
Jersey’s treatment guidelines list a number of most notably, nursing (e.g., Fewster-Thuente &
procedures as red lights, including facilitated Velsor-Friedrich, 2008), medicine (e.g.,
communication, auditory integration training, Gabrielová & Veleminsky, (Gabrielová &
and chelation. All red light procedures have been Veleminsky, 2014), speech and language pathol-
definitively documented to be either ineffective ogy (e.g., Koenig & Gerenser, 2006), and occu-
or harmful, or both. In all instances, these proce- pational therapy (e.g., Scheibel & Watling, 2016).
dures have been vetted by science and found to Certainly, achieving a workforce that is skilled in
be without benefit and to possibly introduce interprofessional collaboration will require an
harm. These designations have been given increased focus on teaching and training skills
because scientific evidence shows a lack of thera- relevant to interdisciplinary collaboration, espe-
peutic impact, because there may be evidence of cially with regard to the development of soft
harm, because they may have an anti-science skills.
basis, and because they are likely to waste time, It is important for behavior analysts to under-
energy, resources, and hope. stand both the roles and contributions of every
Green light procedures are those whose effects member of the interdisciplinary team.
have been well established through empirical Professional training should incorporate more
investigation. These efficacious interventions information about allied field’s training and
may generally be implemented and are likely to expertise and should focus on navigating shared
provide benefit to the individual. scopes of practice. Specifically, skills in making
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 145

clinical decisions about courses of treatment Bagatell, N., Mirigliani, G., Patterson, C., Reyes, Y., &
Test, L. (2010). Effectiveness of therapy ball chairs on
need to be a focus of training. Helping practitio- classroom participation in children with autism spec-
ners to understand processes to use in team trum disorders. American Journal of Occupational
decision-­making is helpful. It is also extremely Therapy, 64(6), 895–903. https://doi.org/10.5014/
helpful for practitioners to be familiar with ajot.2010.09149
Bailey, J.  S. (1991). Marketing behavior analysis
resources that can help individuals and teams to requires different talk. Journal of Applied Behavior
assess potential risks and benefits and to evaluate Analysis, 24(3), 445–448. https://doi.org/10.1901/
the existing evidence for a given procedure. jaba.1991.24-­445
All decisions made must be in the context of Banks, B. M., Shriver, M. D., Chadwell, M. R., & Allen,
K.  D. (2018). An examination of behavioral treat-
evidence-based intervention, and behavior ana- ment wording on acceptability and understanding.
lysts are obligated to uphold a commitment to Behavioral Interventions, 33(3), 260–270. https://doi.
effective treatment. Hence, behavior analysts org/10.1002/bin.1521
must be trained to navigate these circumstances Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empa-
thy quotient: An investigation of adults with asperger
with interpersonal finesse and with science-based syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex
facts. Familiarity with resources that rank treat- differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ments in terms of evidence and with the position Disorders, 34(2), 163–175. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:j
statements issued by organizations regarding add.0000022607.19833.00
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
specific interventions can help behavior analysts current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
to decide when and how to express strong opin- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 91–97.
ions about potential harm or limited benefit. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­91
The benefits of training behavior analysts Becirevic, A., Critchfield, T.  S., & Reed, D.  D. (2016).
On the social acceptability of behavior-analytic terms:
more thoroughly and methodically in interdisci- Crowdsourced comparisons of lay and technical lan-
plinary collaboration extend beyond the benefits guage. The Behavior Analyst, 39(2), 305–317. https://
for individual clients. As behavior analysts col- doi.org/10.1007/s40614-­016-­0067-­4
laborate with more success, the experiences of Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2012). Model
act for licensing/regulating behavior analysts.
colleagues in other disciplines will also improve. Retrieved from https://www.bacb.com/wp-­content/
This will likely increase outcomes on the level of uploads/2020/05/BACB_Model_Act.pdf
the individual, at the level of the team, and for the Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2017). BCBA task
profession as a whole. list (5th ed.). Author.
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2018). A summary
of ethics violations and code-enforcement activities:
2016–2017. Author.
References Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020a). Ethics
code for behavior analysts. Author.
AL-Ayadhi, L. Y., & Elamin, N. E. (2013). Camel milk as Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020b). BCBA
a potential therapy as an antioxidant in autism spec- 2022 eligibility requirements. Author.
trum disorder (ASD). Evidence-based Complementary Boivin, N., Ruane, J., Quigley, S. P., Harper, J., & Weiss,
and Alternative Medicine, 2013, 1–8. https://doi. M.  J. (2021). Interdisciplinary collaboration: An
org/10.1155/2013/602834 example of a preservice training series. Behavior
American Nurses Association. (2015). Code of ethics for Analysis in Practice. Advance online publication.
nurses.. American Nurses Association. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­021-­00561-­z
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Bonvicini, K. A., Perlin, M. J., Bylund, C. L., Carroll, G.,
(2018). Rapid prompting method [Position Statement]. Rouse, R.  A., & Goldstein, M.  G. (2009). Impact of
Available from www.asha.org/policy/ communication training on physician expression of
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (n.d.). empathy in patient encounters. Patient Education and
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). https://www2.asha. Counseling, 75(1), 3–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
org/EvidenceMapLanding.aspx?id=8589943678&rec pec.2008.09.007
entarticles=false&year=undefined&tab=all Bosch, B., & Mansell, H. (2015). Interprofessional
Association for Science in Autism Treatment. (n.d.). Learn collaboration in health care. Canadian
more about specific treatments. https://asatonline.org/ Pharmacists Journal, 148(4), 176–179. https://doi.
for-­parents/learn-­more-­about-­specific-­treatments/ org/10.1177/1715163515588106
Autism New Jersey. (n.d.). Treatment. https://www.autis- Brodhead, M.  T. (2015). Maintaining professional rela-
mnj.org/understanding-­autism/treatment/ tionships in an interdisciplinary setting: Strategies
for navigating nonbehavioral treatment recommenda-
146 M. J. Weiss et al.

tions for individuals with autism. Behavior Analysis Springer International Publishing. https://doi.
in Practice, 8(1), 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/ org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­62995-­7_9
s40617-­015-­0042-­7 Dillenburger, K. (2011). The Emperor’s new clothes:
Brown, J., Lewis, L., Ellis, K., Stewart, M., Freeman, Eclecticism in autism treatment. Research in Autism
T.  R., & Kasperski, M.  J. (2011). Conflict on inter- Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1119–1128. https://doi.
professional primary health care teams  – Can it be org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.008
resolved? Journal of Interprofessional Care, 25(1), Dorsey, J., Ehrenfried, H., Finch, D., & Jaegers. (2019).
4–10. https://doi.org/10.3109/13561820.2010.497750 Work. In B. A. Schell & G. Gillen (Eds.), Willard &
Buckley, K. M., Tran, B. Q., & Prandoni, C. M. (2004). Spackman’s occupational therapy (13th ed.). Williams
Receptiveness, use and acceptance of telehealth by & Wilkins.
caregivers of stroke patients in the home. Online Doughty, A. H., Holloway, C., Shields, M. C., & Kennedy,
Journal of Nursing Informatics, 9(3), 9. L.  E. (2012). Marketing behavior analysis requires
Carr, E. G. (1996). The transfiguration of behavior analy- (really) different talk: A critique of Kohn (2005) and
sis: Strategies for survival. Journal of Behavioral a(nother) call to arms. Behavior and Social Issues,
Education, 6(3), 263–270. https://doi.org/10.1007/ 21(1), 115–134. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v21i0.3914
BF02110128 Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C.  M., Cihon, J.  H., Leaf, J.  B.,
Case-Smith, J., & Arbesman, M. (2008). Evidence-based McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2021). Using the teaching
review of interventions for autism used in or of rel- interaction procedure to train interventionists to imple-
evance to occupational therapy. American Journal of ment the Cool vs. Not Cool procedure. Behavioral
Occupational Therapy, 62(4), 416–429. https://doi. Interventions, 36, 211–227.
org/10.5014/ajot.62.4.416 Foxx, R. M. (1996). Translating the covenant: The behav-
Chung, C. L. R., Manga, J., McGregor, M., Michailidis, ior analyst as ambassador and translator. The Behavior
C., Stavros, D., & Woodhouse, L.  J. (2012). Analyst, 19(2), 147–161. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Interprofessional collaboration and turf wars how BF03393162
prevalent are hidden attitudes? The Journal of Friman, P.  C. (2004). Up with this I shall not put: 10
Chiropractic Education, 26(1), 32–39. reasons why I disagree with Branch and Vollmer on
Coulehan, J. L., Platt, F. W., Egener, B., Frankel, R., Lin, behavior used as a count noun. The Behavior Analyst,
C.-T., Lown, B., & Salazar, W. H. (2001). “Let me see 27(1), 99–106. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392095
if I have this right …”: Words that help build empathy. Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (2002). The picture exchange com-
Annals of Internal Medicine, 135(3), 221–227. https:// munication system training manual (2nd ed.). Pyramid
doi.org/10.7326/0003-­4819-­135-­3-­200108070-­00022 Educational Products.
Cox, D.  J. (2012). From interdisciplinary to inte- Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (2019). Verbal behavior. In ABA
grated care of the child with autism: The essen- for SLPs: Interprofessional collaboration for autism
tial role for a code of ethics. Journal of Autism and support teams. Brookes Publishing.
Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2729–2738. https:// Frykman, M., Hasson, H., Muntlin Athlin, Å., & Von Thiele
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­012-­1530-­z Schwarz, U. (2014). Functions of behavior change
Cox, D. J. (2019). Ethical considerations in interdisciplin- interventions when implementing multi-professional
ary treatments. In R.  D. Rieske (Ed.), Handbook of teamwork at an emergency department: A compara-
interdisciplinary treatments for autism spectrum dis- tive case study. BMC Health Services Research, 14(1).
order (pp. 49–61). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-­6963-­14-­218
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­3-­030-­13027-­5_4 Fewster-Thuente, L., & Velsor-Friedrich, B. (2008).
Critchfield, T.  S., Becirevic, A., & Reed, D.  D. (2017). Interdisciplinary collaboration for healthcare profes-
On the social validity of behavior-analytic commu- sionals. Nursing Administration, 32(1), 40–48. https://
nication: A call for research and description of one doi.org/10.1097/01.NAQ.0000305946.31193.61
method. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 33(1), 1–23. Gabrielová, J., & Veleminsky, M. (2014). Interdisciplinary
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-­017-­0077-­7 collaboration between medical and non-medical pro-
Critchfield, T. S., & Doepke, K. J. (2018). Emotional over- fessions in health and social care. Neurology and
tones of behavior analysis terms in English and five Endocrinology Letters, 35(1), 59–66.
other languages. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11(2), Gerenser, J.  E., & Cicero, F.  R. (2019). Assessing and
97–105. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0222-­3 treating challenging behavior within and beyond
Critchfield, T. S., Doepke, K. J., Epting, L. K., Becirevic, speech therapy sessions. In J. E. Gerenser & M. Koenig
A., Reed, D. D., Fienup, D. M., Kremsreiter, J. L., & (Eds.), ABA for SLPs: Interprofessional collaboration
Ecott, C. L. (2017). Normative emotional responses to for autism support teams. Brookes Publishing.
behavior analysis jargon or how not to use words to Gerenser, J.  E., & Koenig, M.  A. (2019). Strategies to
win friends and influence people. Behavior Analysis enhance SLP-ABA collaboration: Working toward
in Practice, 10(2), 97–106. https://doi.org/10.1007/ interprofessional practice. In J.  E. Gerenser &
s40617-­016-­0161-­9 M.  Koenig (Eds.), ABA for SLPs: Interprofessional
DiGennaro Reed, F., Novak, M.  D., Henley, A.  J., collaboration for autism support teams. Brookes
Brand, D., & McDonald, M.  E. (2018). Evidence Publishing.
based interventions. In J.  B. Leaf (Ed.), Handbook Graudins, M.  M., Rehfeldt, R., DeMattei, R., Baker,
of social skills and autism spectrum disorder. J. C., & Scaglia, F. (2012). Exploring the efficacy of
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 147

behavioral skills training to teach basic behavior ana- Koenig, M., & Gerenser, J. (2019). Interprofessional col-
lytic techniques to oral care providers. Research in laboration. In ABA for SLPs: Interprofessional collab-
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(3), 978–987. https:// oration for autism support teams. Brookes Publishing.
doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.12.010 Koenig, M., & Murphy, C. (2019). Assessment in SLP and
Green, D. R., Ferguson, J. L., Cihon, J. H., et al. (2020). ABA. In J. E. Gerenser & M. Koenig (Eds.), ABA for
The teaching interaction procedure as a staff training SLPs: Interprofessional collaboration for autism sup-
tool. Behav Analysis Practice, 13, 421–433. https:// port teams. Brookes Publishing.
doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00357-­2 LaFrance, D.  L., Weiss, M.  J., Kazemi, E., Gerenser,
Hall, P. (2005). Interprofessional teamwork: J., & Dobres, J. (2019). Multidisciplinary teaming:
Professional cultures as barriers. Journal of Enhancing collaboration through increased under-
Interprofessional Care, 19(sup1), 188–196. https:// standing. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(3), 709–
doi.org/10.1080/13561820500081745 726. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00331-­y
Harris, S. L., Handleman, J. S., & Alessandri, M. (1990). Leaf, J.  B., Townley-Cochran, D., Taubman, M., Cihon,
Teaching youths with autism to offer assistance. J. H., Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L., Kassardjian, A., Leaf,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(3), 297– R., McEachin, J., & Pentz, T. G. (2015). The teaching
305. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-­297 interaction procedure and behavioral skills training for
Himle, M.  B., Miltenberger, R.  G., Flessner, C., & individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder:
Gatheridge, B. (2004). Teaching safety skills to chil- A review and commentary. Review Journal of Autism
dren to prevent gun play. Journal of Applied Behavior and Developmental Disorders, 2(4), 402–413. https://
Analysis, 37(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1901/ doi.org/10.1007/s40489-­015-­0060-­y
jaba.2004.37-­1 LeBlanc, L. A., Sellers, T. P., & Ala’i, S. (2020). Building
Hineline, P. N. (1980). The language of behavior analy- and sustaining meaningful and effective relationships
sis: Its community, its functions, and its limita- as a supervisor and mentor. Sloan Publishing.
tions. Behavior, 8(1), 67–86. https://www.jstor.org/ LeBlanc, L. A., Taylor, B. A., & Marchese, N. V. (2020).
stable/27758952 The training experiences of behavior analysts:
Ho, E. C. M., & Siu, A. M. H. (2018). Occupational ther- Compassionate care and therapeutic relationships with
apy practice in sleep management: A review of con- caregivers. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13, 387–
ceptual models and research evidence. Occupational 393. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00368-­z
Therapy International, 2018, 1–12. https://doi. Lindsley, O.  R. (1991). From technical jargon to plain
org/10.1155/2018/8637498 English for application. Journal of Applied Behavior
Jarmolowicz, D.  P., Kahng, S., Ingvarsson, E.  T., Analysis, 24(3), 449–458. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Goysovich, R., Heggemeyer, R., & Gregory, M.  K. jaba.1991.24-­449
(2008). Effects of conversational versus techni- Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor-
cal language on treatment preference and integrity. mal educational and intellectual functioning in
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 46(3), young autistic children. Journal of Consulting
190–199. https://doi.org/10.1352/2008.46:190-­199 and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi.
Jin, C. S., Hanley, G. P., & Beaulieu, L. (2013). An indi- org/10.1037//0022-­006x.55.1.3
vidualized and comprehensive approach to treating Lynch, D., & Forde, C. (2016). ‘Moral distress’ and the
sleep problems in young children. Journal of Applied beginning practitioner: Preparing social work students
Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 161–180. https://doi. for ethical and moral challenges in contemporary
org/10.1002/jaba.16 contexts. Ethics and Social Welfare, 10(2), 94–107.
Kelly, A., & Tincani, M. (2013). Collaborative training https://doi.org/10.1080/17496535.2016.1155634
and practice among applied behavior analysts who Malow, B. A., Katz, T., Reynolds, A. M., Shui, A., Carno,
support individuals with autism spectrum disorder. M., Connolly, H.  V., Coury, D., & Bennett, A.  E.
Education and Training in Autism and Developmental (2016). Sleep difficulties and medications in children
Disabilities, 48(1), 120–131. with autism spectrum disorders: A registry study.
Kelm, Z., Womer, J., Walter, J. K., & Feudtner, C. (2014). Pediatrics, 137(Supplement), S98–S104. https://doi.
Interventions to cultivate physician empathy: A sys- org/10.1542/peds.2015-­2851H
tematic review. BMC Medical Education, 14(1), 219. Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. S. (2007). Comorbid
https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-­6920-­14-­219 psychopathology with autism spectrum disorder in
Koenig, M., & Gerenser, J. (2006). SLP- ABA: children: An overview. Research in Developmental
Collaborating to support individuals with commu- Disabilities, 28(4), 341–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
nication impairments. The Journal of Speech and ridd.2005.12.004
Language Pathology  – Applied Behavior Analysis, McCulloch, E. (2016). The seven deadly sins of behav-
1(1), 2–10. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100180 ior analysts. Retrieved from https://bsci21.org/
Koenig, M., & Gerenser, J.  E. (2015). SLP-ABA col- the-­seven-­deadly-­sins-­of-­behavior-­analysts/
laboration for children with autism: Developing har- McElhanon, B. O., McCracken, C., Karpen, S., & Sharp,
monious interprofessional relationships. Short course W.  G. (2014). Gastrointestinal symptoms in autism
presented at the annual convention of the American spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 133(5),
Speech and Hearing Association. Denver, CO. 872–883. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-­3995
148 M. J. Weiss et al.

Morris, E.  K. (2014). Stop preaching to the choir, pub- Ranjan, S., & Nasser, J. A. (2015). Nutritional status of
lish outside the box: A discussion. The Behavior individuals with autism spectrum disorders: Do we
Analyst, 37(2), 87–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/ know enough? Advances in Nutrition, 6(4), 397–407.
s40614-­014-­0011-­4 https://doi.org/10.3945/an.114.007914
Moscato, S.  R., Valanis, B., Gullion, C.  M., Tanner, C., Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., Townsend, D. B., & Poulson,
Shapiro, S.  E., & Izumi, S. (2007). Predictors of C. L. (2007). Establishing a generalized repertoire of
patient satisfaction with telephone nursing services. helping behavior in children with autism. Journal of
Clinical Nursing Research, 16(2), 119–137. https:// Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), 123–136. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1054773806298507 doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.11-­05
National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu- Reichow, B. (2011). Development, procedures, and
sions: National standards project, phase, 2. https:// application of the evaluative method for determining
www.nationalautismcenter.org/national-­s tandards- evidence-based practices in autism. In B.  Reichow,
­project/phase-­2/ P. Doehring, D. V. Cicchetti, & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.),
National Education Association. (1975). Code of ethics Evidence-based practices and treatments for children
for educators. Retrieved from, https://www.nea.org/ with autism (pp.  25–39). Springer US. https://doi.
resource-­library/code-­ethics-­educators org/10.1007/978-­1-­4419-­6975-­0_2
Neuman, P. (2018). Vernacular selection: What to say and Rolider, A., & Axelrod, S. (2005). The effects of
when to say it. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 34(1– “behavior-­speak” on public attitudes toward behav-
2), 62–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-­018-­0097-­y ioral interventions: A crosscultural argument for using
Newhouse-Oisten, M. K., Peck, K. M., Conway, A. A., & conversational language to describe behavioral inter-
Frieder, J. E. (2017). Ethical considerations for inter- ventions to the general public. In W. L. Heward, T. E.
disciplinary collaboration with prescribing profession- Heron, N. A. Neef, S. M. Peterson, D. M. Sainato, &
als. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 10(2), 145–153. G.  Cartledge (Eds.), Focus on behavior analysis in
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­017-­0184-­x education: Achievements, challenges, and opportuni-
Nigro-Bruzzi, D., & Sturmey, P. (2010). The effects of ties (pp. 283–294). Pearson Press.
behavioral skills training on mand training by staff Rolider, A., Axelrod, S., & Van Houten, R. (1998). Don’t
and unprompted vocal mands by children. Journal of speak behaviorism to me: How to clearly and effec-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 757–761. https:// tively communicate behavioral interventions to the
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­757 general public. Child & Family Behavior Therapy,
Nye, C., Vanryckeghem, M., Schwartz, J. B., Herder, C., 20(2), 39–56. https://doi.org/10.1300/J019v20n02_03
Turner, H. M., & Howard, C. (2013). Behavioral stut- Root, J. R., Stevenson, B. S., Davis, L. L., Geddes-Hall, J.,
tering interventions for children and adolescents: A sys- & Test, D. W. (2017). Establishing computer-assisted
tematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, instruction to teach academics to students with autism
Language, and Hearing Research, 56(3), 921–932. as an evidence-based practice. Journal of Autism and
https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-­4388(2012/12-­0036) Developmental Disorders, 47(2), 275–284. https://doi.
Onslow, M., Webber, M., Harrison, E., Arnott, S., org/10.1007/978-­1-­4614-­6435-­8_102212-­1
Bridgman, K., Carey, B., Sheedy, S., O’Brian, S., Rosales, R., Stone, K., & Rehfeldt, R.  A. (2009). The
MacMillan, V., & Lloyd, W. (2020). The Lidcombe effects of behavioral skills training on implementa-
program treatment guide.. Lidcombe Program tion of the picture exchange communication system.
Trainers Consortium. https://www.lidcombeprogram. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(3), 541–
org/helpful-­resources/helpful-­downloads/ 549. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­541
Ottenbacher, K. J., Tickle-Degnen, L., & Hasselkus, B. R. Schell, B. A., & Gillen, G. (2019). Willard & Spackman’s
(2002). Therapists awake! The challenge of evidence-­ occupational therapy (13th ed.). Williams & Wilkins.
based occupational therapy. The American Journal of Scheibel, G., & Watling, R. (2016). Collaborating with
Occupational Therapy, 56(3), 247–249. https://doi. behavior analysts on the autism service delivery team.
org/10.5014/ajot.56.3.247 OT Practice, 21(7), 15–19.
Parsons, M.  B., Rollyson, J.  H., Carolina, N., & Reid, Schlinger, H.  D., Blakely, E., Fillhard, J., & Poling,
D. H. (2012). Evidence-based staff training: A guide A. (1991). Defining terms in behavior analysis:
for practitioners. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 5(2), Reinforcer and discriminative stimulus. The Analysis
2–11. of Verbal Behavior, 9(1), 153–161. https://doi.
Parsons, M.  B., Rollyson, J.  H., & Reid, D.  H. (2013). org/10.1007/BF03392869
Teaching practitioners to conduct behavioral skills Schlosser, R. W., Hemsley, B., Shane, H., Todd, J., Lang,
training: A pyramidal approach for training multiple R., Lilienfeld, S. O., Trembath, D., Mostert, M., Fong,
human service staff. Behavior Analysis in Practice, S., & Odom, S. (2019). Rapid prompting method and
6(2), 4–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03391798 autism spectrum disorder: Systematic review exposes
Rana, M., Kothare, S., & DeBassio, W. (2021). The lack of evidence. Review Journal of Autism and
assessment and treatment of sleep abnormalities in Developmental Disorders, 6(4), 403–412. https://doi.
children and adolescents with autism spectrum disor- org/10.1007/s40489-­019-­00175-­w
der: A review. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Schrandt, J. B., Townsend, D. L., & Poulson, C. U. (2009).
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(1), 25–35. Teaching empathy skills to children with autism.
8  Effective Collaboration: Maximizing Outcomes in Autism Intervention in an Interdisciplinary Model 149

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(1), 17–32. Swinth, Y. (2019). Education. In B.  A. Schell & Gillen
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­17 (Eds.), Willard and Spackman’s occupational therapy
Schreck, K. A., Karunaratne, Y., Zane, T., & Wilford, H. (13th ed.). Williams & Wilkins.
(2016). Behavior analysts’ use of and beliefs in treat- Taylor, B.  A., LeBlanc, L.  A., & Nosik, M.  R. (2019).
ments for people with autism: A 5-year follow-up. Compassionate care in behavior analytic treatment: Can
Behavioral Interventions, 31, 355–376. https://doi. outcomes be enhanced by attending to relationships
org/10.1002/bin.1461 with caregivers? Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(3),
Schreck, K. A., & Mazur, A. (2008). Behavior analyst use 654–666. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00289-­3
of and beliefs in treatments for people with autism. Vail, T., & Koenig, M. (2019). Integrating ABA and SLP
Behavioral Interventions, 23, 201–212. https://doi. for successful intervention. In J. Gerenser & M. Koenig
org/10.1002/bin.264 (Eds.), ABA for SLPs: Interprofessional collaboration
Shook, G.  L., & Johnston, J.  M. (2011). Training and for autism support teams. Brookes Publishing.
professional certification in applied behavior analysis. Welch, C.  D., & Polatajko, H.  J. (2016). The issue is
In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, & H. S. Roane (Eds.), applied behavior analysis, autism, and occupational
Handbook of applied behavior analysis (pp.  498– therapy: A search for understanding. American Journal
510). The Guilford Press. of Occupational Therapy: Official Publication of The
Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). American Occupational Therapy Association, 70(4),
Responses to the negative emotions of others by autis- 1–5. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2016.018689
tic, mentally retarded and normal children. Child Williams, W. L., Weil, T. M., & Porter, J. C. K. (2012).
Development, 63(4), 796–807. The relative effects of traditional lectures and guided
Skinner, B.  F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An notes lectures on university student test scores. The
experimental analysis. Appleton-Century. Behavior Analyst Today, 13(1), 12–16. https://doi.
Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psycho- org/10.1037/h0100713
logical terms. Psychological Review, 52(5), 270–277. Williamson, E., Sathe, N., Andrews, J., Krishnaswami,
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0062535 S., McPheeters, M., Fonnesbeck, C., Sanders, K.,
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. The Weitlauf, A., & Warren, Z. (2017). Medical thera-
MacMillan Company. pies for children with autism spectrum disorder—An
Strauss, C., Taylor, B.  L., Gu, J., Kuyken, W., Baer, R., update (comparative effectiveness review no. 189).
Jones, F., & Cavanagh, K. (2016). What is compassion Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. https://
and how can we measure it? A review of definitions doi.org/10.23970/AHRQEPCCER189
and measures. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 15–27. Wolf, M.  M. (1978). Social validity: The case for sub-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.004 jective measurement or how applied behavior analy-
Strunk, J., Leisen, M., & Schubert, C. (2017). Using sis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior
a multidisciplinary approach with children diag- Analysis, 11(2), 203–214. https://doi.org/10.1901/
nosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of jaba.1978.11-­203
Interprofessional Education & Practice, 8, 60–68. Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M.  D., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.xjep.2017.03.009 (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning
Suter, E., Arndt, J., Arthur, N., Parboosingh, J., Taylor, children with autism. Child Development, 63(1), 150–
E., & Deutschlander, S. (2009). Role understand- 160. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130909
ing and effective communication as core com- Zimmer, M., & Desch, L. (2012). Sensory integration
petencies for collaborative practice. Journal of therapies for children with developmental and behav-
Interprofessional Care, 23(1), 41–51. https://doi. ioral disorders. Pediatrics, 129(6), 1186–1189. https://
org/10.1080/13561820802338579 doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-­0876
Be Humble, Learn, and Care:
Culturally Responsive 9
Evidence-­Based Practice

Shahla Alai-Rosales, Malika Pritchett, April Linden,


Isabel Cunningham, and Noor Syed

9.1  e Humble, Learn, and Care:


B including the child, should see value in the pro-
Culturally Responsive cess and outcomes and, also, contribute to the
Evidence-Based Practice efforts. Cultural differences oftentimes produce
tensions, avoidance, or exclusion. These difficul-
In the midst of our culturally abundant and com- ties set the occasion for close examination of
plicated world, our goal as interventionists is to intervention practices and growth, as individual
contribute to the betterment of the lives of all the practitioners and as a field.
children and families we serve. As the world Culture is a way of describing the common
changes through migration and the lifting of learning histories, behavior patterns, and values
oppressive structures, greater numbers of people that groups of people share; these are differenti-
who are different from one another will interact ated from groups who have other sets of histories,
and negotiate the process of living together and behavior patterns, and values (Sugai et al., 2012).
supporting one another. Interventions to help For any given person, cultural identity is fluid and
people with autism are highly interactive acts that intersectional (Osborne, 2015). That is, each of
enter into some of the most central parts of living: us will identify with multiple cultural groups, and
communication, social interactions, and activity that identification is porous and dynamic.
engagement. Furthermore, the process of behav- Furthermore, some cultural groups will experi-
ior change involves tremendous collective effort ence more oppression, hardship, trauma, and
over time in each of these areas of life. Everyone, greater disparities in access and distribution of
resources than others. Conversely, some cultural
groups will experience more privilege, greater
S. Alai-Rosales (*) · A. Linden · I. Cunningham
access to resources, and affluence than others.
Department of Behavior Analysis, University of
North Texas, Given these subcultures and intersectionalities,
Denton, TX, USA cultural groups overlap. It would not be uncom-
e-mail: Shahla.Alai@unt.edu mon or unheard of, depending on cultural groups
M. Pritchett (*) in which one identifies, to experience both privi-
Center for Community Health and Development, lege and oppression. Cultural groups are delin-
University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
eated in many ways: by race, ethnicity, religion,
e-mail: malikap@ku.edu
economics, gender, neurology, physicality, abil-
N. Syed
ity, sexuality, and more. As the world shifts and
Center for Autism Inclusivity, SUNY Empire State
College, the voices of people from different groups are
Saratoga, Springs, NY, USA able to participate in societal discourse, we find

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 151
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_9
152 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

that we are interfacing in hard and evolutionary moil, we also see the advent of new ways of
ways, including in our intervention programs. addressing our human condition. A scientific
There are many, many cultures in the world. Each approach to human behavior, the systematic
cultural context will bring a range of different study of interactions and patterns between our
responses in relation to research evidence and behavior and our physical and social environ-
interventions, particularly given the overlapping ments, is an advancement in the evolution of our
metacontingencies which surround each culture. species (Skinner, 1953). There is no doubt that
The greater the differences between the cul- this science has led to a considerable amount of
tures of the people involved – the interventionist, evidence that suggests ways to accelerate and
the child, the family, and the community  – the decelerate behavior that improves the lives of
greater the potential for misunderstandings, con- children with autism (e.g., Leaf et al., 2022, this
flicts, and harm. At the same time, if we bring a volume). The cultural tensions lie in the condi-
posture of humility, learning, and care, there are tions under which we change behavior and what
also opportunities for deeply meaningful and behaviors are changed and when; in other words,
progressive outcomes for everyone involved. sought-after behavior change may or may not be
As professionals interact with ever-widening a contextual (e.g., cultural) fit.
cultural groups, we strive to respond in ways that And, above all, there is tension around who is
produce betterment for all. For years, many of the directing the changes and why and how they are
helping professions have struggled with how to making decisions. The difficulties we see in
learn to do this and how to talk about it in both intervention and practice are part of the broader
research and practice (Miller et al., 2019). For the conflicts we see in the world all around us; cul-
purpose of this chapter, we will rely on the broad tural values about change are colliding in hard
term cultural responsiveness (Ladson-Billings, and evolutionary ways.
1995; Miller et  al., 2019; Wlodkowski & The challenge is to evolve effective, care-­
Ginsberg, 1995). The concept of cultural respon- based interventions that foster wellbeing for all
siveness involves an emphasis on transformation. without excluding, hurting, alienating, or margin-
That is, we expect to be changed and to change alizing. This will involve imagining and shaping
the structures of our practices and how we a broader and encompassing set of values that
approach intervention and training so that our contain an ethic of compassion for people from a
combined efforts produce greater progress, inclu- variety of cultural contexts (Ala’i & Re Cruz,
sion, equity, and social justice for all. 2021; Pritchett et al., 2021). This includes, but is
Responsiveness should be a foundational ele- not limited to, the agreement that there is merit
ment of our assessments, our procedures, our and wisdom in recognizing our growing interde-
measures, our training, and our interactions and pendence as a species on a shared biosphere, that
the foundation for organizational structures that each child born into the world is a responsibility
support and give access to those activities. of the collective of humanity, that each child has
Learning to provide interventions that respond to the right to education and happiness, and that we
the unique preferences, needs, and values of an are in the process of learning how to value and
individual child and their families in their spe- nurture one another in equitable and just ways
cific cultural context is the work of a lifetime. (e.g., Karlberg & Farhoumand-Simms, 2006;
Guided by postures of humility and learning Maparyan, 2012, b; McGoldrick & Hardy, 2019;
(Wright, 2019), it can also be a life-affirming UN General Assembly, 1948). These are the val-
process for children, families, therapists, and ues that direct an examination of how we engage
supervisors. in evidence-based practice across cultural
The purpose of this chapter is to explore ways contexts.
in which behavior analysts can be culturally For the interventionist, the task is to consider
responsive and inclusive in clinical practice and research evidence in the context of a specific
research. As we feel the tremors of a world in tur- child and their cultures. Generally, this involves
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 153

careful consideration of the child’s needs, responsible and responsive care (e.g., Slocum
strengths, and preferences, the family’s et al., 2014; Smith, 2013). In the seminal paper
­preferences and valued outcomes, and, also, cul- on evidence-based practice, scholars from some
tural risk and protective factors that are likely to of the most respected medical institutions in the
affect that child’s quality of life in their present western world wrote about the increasing ten-
conditions and across the lifespan. The growing sions in the medical community regarding the
and collective wisdom of the interventionist, role of evidence and the role of clinical expertise
family, and community can help shape a satisfy- (Sackett et al., 1996). They emphasized the rela-
ing future in which the child has an increasingly tionship and importance of both sources:
better quality of life (Schwartz & Kelly, 2021). Good doctors use both individual clinical expertise
To this task, teams can bring an ever-evolving and the best available external evidence, and nei-
research and wisdom base in the design and ther alone is enough. Without clinical expertise,
implementation of interventions. Both the wis- practice risks becoming tyrannised by evidence,
for even excellent external evidence may be inap-
dom and evidence base are influenced by the plicable to or inappropriate for an individual
interventionists’, families’, and societies’ patient. Without current best evidence, practice
responses to culture. risks becoming rapidly out of date, to the detriment
Our hope is to share methods to integrate evi- of patients. (Sackett et al., 1996, p 72)
dence and wisdom to inform culturally respon-
sive practice. As we touch upon the labor in They also discuss the interaction between the
diverse areas of scholarship, we will consider two:
central concepts as they relate to culture, such as Evidence based medicine is not “cookbook” medi-
evidence-based practice, responsivity, perspec- cine. Because it requires a bottom up approach that
tive, reflection, transformation, social justice, and integrates the best external evidence with individ-
equity. We would also like to make it clear that ual clinical expertise and patients’ choice, it cannot
result in slavish, cookbook approaches to individ-
the research base that we rely on in applied ual patient care. External clinical evidence can
behavior analysis is produced and regulated by inform, but can never replace, individual clinical
one dominant culture. Our overall aim is to expertise, and it is this expertise that decides
describe how an evidence-based practice whether the external evidence applies to the indi-
vidual patient at all and, if so, how it should be
approach can increase possibilities and opportu- integrated into a clinical decision. Similarly, any
nities for valued progress even in the midst of a external guideline must be integrated with indi-
research base developed primarily by one group vidual clinical expertise in deciding whether and
of people. how it matches the patient’s clinical state, predica-
ment, and preferences, and thus whether it should
We will do this by exploring the notion of be applied. Clinicians who fear top-down cook-
evidence-based practice as it relates to culture books will find the advocates of evidence based
and highlighting a few important emerging les- medicine joining them at the barricades. (Sackett
sons for increasing culturally responsive inter- et al., 1996, p 72)
ventions. This chapter may seem challenging. It
will not be prescriptive, and it will focus on creat- Evidence-based practice is a balance and synthe-
ing conditions that allow for increased clinical sis of research evidence and clinical expertise.
wisdom and cultural responsivity. Slocum et  al. (2014) wrote about how this par-
ticular perspective on evidence-based practice
applies to practice of behavior analysis. They
9.2 Evidence-Based Practice also highlighted the importance of considering
in the Context of Culture the client values and context:
Evidence-based practice of applied behavior anal-
There are many ways of describing evidence-­ ysis is a decision-making process that integrates
based practice in applied behavior analysis, and (a) the best available evidence with (b) clinical
all are an attempt to describe how we provide expertise and (c) client values and context. This
154 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

definition positions EBP as a pervasive feature of 9.2.1 The Research Evidence Base
all professional decision-making by a behavior
analyst with respect to client services; it is not lim-
ited to a narrowly restricted set of situations or Generally, research in autism is conducted in uni-
decisions. The definition asserts that the best avail- versities and research centers. Researchers
able evidence should be a primary influence on all engage in a lengthy process of identifying
decision-making related to services for clients
(e.g., intervention selection, progress monitoring,
research questions, obtaining human rights
etc.). It also recognizes that evidence cannot be the approval for the research, obtaining participant
sole basis for a decision; effective decision-making consent, conducting the research, preparing a
in a discipline as complex as ABA requires clinical report of the research findings, and submitting
expertise in identifying, defining, and analyzing
problems, determining what evidence is relevant,
the findings to scientific journals. If peers find the
and deciding how it should be applied. In the research meets current scientific standards and
absence of this decision-making framework, prac- has merit to contribute to our understanding of
titioners of ABA would be conceptualized as intervention, it is published and becomes part of
behavioral technicians rather than analysts.
Further, the definition of EBP of ABA includes cli-
the research base. Journals, libraries, and institu-
ent values and context. (Slocum et al., 2014, p. 44) tions make this information available to the prac-
ticing interventionist in different ways (e.g.,
How do we develop clinical expertise that helps through subscriptions, courses, workshops).
us be responsive to client needs, values, and con- Within the last two decades, concerted efforts
text? How do we relate culture to research evi- have increased to make the research evidence
dence? This is a question being asked in many more accessible and to provide a context for syn-
areas of health care (e.g., DelVecchio Good & thesizing a great deal of information. For exam-
Hannah, 2015). This tension is perhaps most ple, there are articles that prepare practitioners to
apparent at the intersections of behavioral inter- locate and find relevant research (e.g., Carr &
ventions and culture. The answers to these ques- Briggs, 2010; Gillis & Carr, 2014), research
tions are complicated. It will take more than clearinghouses (e.g., National Autism Center,
making sure that stimuli include representative 2015; National Professional Development
pictures, that specific holidays are respected and Center, 2014), and textbooks and volumes that
honored, that correct names are used, or that consolidate and summarize findings for both pro-
translators are available. It is each of those things fessionals and parents (e.g., Leaf et al., 2022, this
and much more. volume).
To begin the process of integrating client val- Part of our ethical and professional responsi-
ues, preferences, and contexts with evidence and bility is to demonstrate knowledge of the basic
clinical expertise, we offer a twofold examina- principles of behavior change and to keep abreast
tion. First, it is helpful to understand the research of research developments as they relate to our
base and how it is developing, how the base is specific areas of practice. To ensure that there is a
being established, who is involved in knowledge minimum level of competence, interventionists
production, and how it is regulated. Second, it is are required to have degrees, certifications, and,
important to examine the development of clinical sometimes, licenses. All of this requires diligence
wisdom in relation to culture. That is, what are and a tremendous effort on the part of the
the sources and types of clinical wisdom? What practitioner.
are the specific areas of tension and potential? In relation to culturally responsive interven-
How does one know if expertise is progressing? tions, there are several issues to consider about
Finally, how do we increase the probability of this research base. First, it is only within the past
continued growth and responsiveness? few years that professional organizations have
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 155

released data regarding professional membership Pritchett et  al., 2021). The second point is the
demographics (Association for Behavior Analysis degree to which the research base has been par-
International, n.d.; Behavior Analysis ticipatory is limited (Fawcett, 1991; Pritchett
Certification Board, n.d.). These data, limited in et al., 2021). That is, research participants are not
content, breadth, and duration, indicate that the collaborators in the process of setting and carry-
leadership (i.e., researchers, editorial board ing out the research agendas. Furthermore, nei-
reviewers, professors who teach courses, direc- ther the Association for Behavior Analysis
tors who oversee intervention programs, scholars International nor the Behavior Analyst
who compile research reviews and write books) Certification Board set disciplinary pre- or post-­
is primarily of the dominant culture, that is, west- educational requirements regarding cultural
ern, white, Global North inhabitants. This repre- diversity and practice for professionals in behav-
sentation is also seen in recent surveys of ior analysis in order to teach leaders, researchers,
practitioners. In one of the more comprehensive or practitioners how to interact with culture and
and earliest surveys, the results indicated that a context, a core feature of evidence-based practice
majority of practitioners are white females work- in behavior analysis (Slocum et  al., 2014).
ing with children with autism and have little or no Finally, translational research is not always con-
training about culture and diversity (e.g., Beaulieu ducted before publication; in fact, it is often con-
et  al., 2019). The process of systematically sidered a separate research type. While this is
including research participant demographics has understood from the perspective of researching
only recently begun, and, even then, demographic scholars, one may argue that research conducted
data about researchers and participants appears to in laboratory settings, as brief designs, etc., lacks
be largely absent (Pritchett et al., 2021). It should cultural contextual fit or at the least has not been
also be noted that most of the research publica- assessed beyond these specific contexts. In other
tions are in English and are products of the Global words, generalizability of such research is not
North. All of this is to say that the research base truly present.
has largely been developed and created by a In summary, the research base is primarily
homogenous cultural group. This is similar to created, produced, and implemented by a rela-
other sciences and helping professions (e.g., tively homogeneous group, and there are no for-
Adams et  al., 2015; Dirth & Adams, 2019; mal training requirements for behavior analysts
Henrich et al., 2010). regarding cultural values and context. From this,
There are two important points here. First, the we can conclude that we are only seeing a portion
homogeneity of the research foundation does not of the questions, methods, opportunities, frame-
negate the relevance or utility of the knowledge. works, and outcomes possible.
On the one hand, we have made tremendous
progress in understanding how to accelerate
meaningful change; many children with autism 9.2.2 Developing a Cultural
have made measurable progress, and the out- Wisdom Base
comes have been valued (e.g., Leaf et al., 2022,
this volume). On the other hand, emerging schol- Fortunately, a practitioner with clinical expertise
arship within and outside of the field tells us that is in a unique and important position and can
there are important voices that have not been part bring wisdom to the process of contextualizing
of the process and that the cultural positionality, the research base with a given child and their
the identity, of the researcher will influence the family’s needs, priorities, goals, and values
research questions, participant inclusion, the (Slocum et al., 2014; Leaf et al., 2016). To under-
experimental methods, the data interpretations, stand the expansiveness and implications of the
and the suggested applications of research in a practitioner’s role in contributing to the progres-
myriad of ways (Ala’i & Re Cruz, 2021; England, sion of the evidence-based practice, we will share
1994; Jacobson & Mustafa, 2019; Jafar, 2018; a description of a paradigm shift occurring within
156 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

the technology industry and relate this to the case and action for a common product or purpose that
of intervention. Boland and Tenkasi (1995) paradigmatic shifts occurred and innovations
described the phenomena of “knowledge-­ were able to surpass all previous technologies.
intensive firms” that exist to produce new Boland and Tensaki also described those knowl-
understandings that, in turn, produce valued edge groups that are unsuccessful; such teams are
­
innovations. They give the example of cellular characterized by a lack of skills in self-reflection
phone development that required 5 disciplines in and narrative, difficulty reconciling one another’s
the first development phase and at least 14 dis- priorities and projections, frequent ignoring, dis-
tinct disciplinary technologies in the later phases. counting, and a lack of appreciation for the per-
In the tech industry, the cell phone is considered spective of one another as well as certitude that
a “killer app,” meaning that it made previous everything is known and correct within their own
technologies irrelevant and archaic. Of course, particular frameworks. These barriers limit cre-
previous technologies were essential for the ative problem-solving and advancement.
foundations of the cell phone, but before today’s At present, the desired outcomes are increased
current version, few could have imagined a quality of life and enhanced wellbeing of the
device that intensively combined and synergized children and families we serve, now and across
the knowledge of so many disciplines and tech- time. The perspective taking, sharing, and mak-
nologies to create a new way of interacting with ing should occur within our intervention teams of
the world. In some ways, this is similar to what is diverse people with different lived experiences
required of the interventionist. There is an obliga- and vantage points. Each interventionist comes
tion to deeply understand the evidence base and with a particular set of learning histories and,
to understand the processes involved in honor- hopefully, a deep knowledge of the evidence base
ably serving children and families that had no and its contextual strengths and limitations. Each
voice in the development of that evidence base. family and child come with a set of learning his-
This requires the interventionist to develop skills tories developed within their cultural communi-
and knowledge that exist in other disciplines, to ties. All cultures have developed funds of
reflect deeply on their own positionality and knowledge, values, and priorities and have had
biases, to work collaboratively with the child and particular positions within the social structures of
family, and to generate a series of programs that society (e.g., Moll et al., 1992). Each of the posi-
create a new way for each child. tions is the product of generations of societal
Boland and Tenkasi (1995) posited that an structures and individual and group learning his-
essential part of the successful development of tories that place people of different cultural
advanced technologies is based on a process of groups in relative advantage and disadvantage.
the disciplines engaged in both perspective tak- These conditions create a compelling need for
ing and making. This applies not only across dis- increased cultural responsiveness and clinical
ciplines but within disciplines. That is, each wisdom in interventions, especially with cultures
disciplinary branch learns to narrate, shape, and often at a disadvantage, that is, people that are not
reflect upon their own perspectives and contribu- from white, affluent, Global North, Christian cul-
tions and to listen, integrate, and reflect upon the tures. This point is amplified in social and aca-
contributions of the other disciplinary groups. In demic discourse as well as systematic qualitative
this process, both data and narrative share impor- studies with people of other cultures.
tance. Data allowed the participants to under- Neither interventionists nor the families know
stand conditional truths under specific conditions, the outcome of the intervention journey. Each is
and narratives gave context and meaning to the part of a dynamic system in which cultural
data. When the groups worked together toward a boundaries and positions are always in motion
common mission, they developed new perspec- (DelVecchio Good & Hannah, 2015). Each
tives and new bodies of knowledge. It is within comes in with knowledge, values, priorities, and
this intensive knowledge sharing, production, contingencies that are to be discovered, acted
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 157

upon, and synergized in a context. The context is the history in the context of humility). The call
the child’s quality of life (current and future) infor responsive care was introduced in behavior
their specific cultural context, the research base,analysis (e.g., Fong & Tanaka, 2013; Iwamasa,
and the team’s willingness to learn and care 1997; Iwamasa & Smith, 1996) and has increased
(Schwartz & Kelly, 2021). The goal is for each in number (e.g., Fong et al., 2017; Miller et al.,
member to learn about their own skills, knowl- 2019; Zarcone et al., 2019) and has become more
edge, values, and contingencies and how they fit specialized since the March 2020 uprisings (e.g.,
within the context of the child’s life. Fortunately,
Ardila Sánchez et  al., 2020; Esquierdo-Leal &
there are scholars outside of the intervention con-Houmanfar, 2021; Gingles, 2021a; Sadavoy &
text that are studying the ways culture interacts Zube, 2021). Wright (2019) published the first
with societal structures and patterns of interac- work specifically addressing cultural humility in
tions (e.g., Annamma et  al., 2016; Benjamin, behavioral practice.
2019; Love, 2019; McGoldrick & Hardy, 2019; We start specifically with humility for several
Project Implicit, n.d.). On the one hand, this is reasons. First, it is foundational in developing
daunting because there are as many sources, if “third ways” in situations of uncertainty and ten-
not more, than were related to something like cell sion. Second, it is a prime example of how non-
phone development. On the other hand, our disci- western frameworks can expand our way of
pline is working fast and furiously to access, navigating our shared world. It is both a useful
unpack, and apply much of the learning from out- concept for this particular situation and a way to
side the field. For example, Behavior Analysis understand that concepts and practices can differ
and Practice published a special issue on Diversityacross cultures; it is a concept that both teaches
and Equity (Zarcone et al., 2019), and another is and exemplifies how cultural responsiveness can
in progress on Racism and Police Brutality improve our ability to act toward the common
(Gingles, 2021b). good.
Our labor in creating culturally responsive Within a western context, the notions of
evidence-based practice then comes to create humility are related to intellectual reasoning and
what Barrera and Kramer (2009) called a “third decision-making and “… the correctness or
way,” an outcome that is a process of taking wrongness of one’s beliefs, truth seeking, accep-
seemingly divergent and sometimes contentious tance of one’s fallibilities as knower/believer, and
positions to create a new, provisional approach. the nature of open mindedness occupy the center
That is, in the case of evidence-based practice stage of discussions about humility at present”
and culture, it means crafting a new way that har- (Li, 2016, p.150). Within health care, including
monizes the family cultural values and contin- behavior analysis, there are also the added dimen-
gencies, the child’s immediate and long-term sions of personal and institutional accountability
growth and happiness, the research evidence, and for power imbalances and the need to address
the sources of knowledge from other areas, such these imbalances and inequities through critical
as sociology, anthropology, public health, and analysis as well as the central role of openness to
education (Miller et al., 2019). We can do this, incultural diversity and ongoing learning about
part, by perspective taking and making, a set of how to improve practice (Wright, 2019).
skills that is intertwined with humility, ongoing In an exploration of the concept of humility,
learning, and care. Li (2016) noted that the concept may run con-
trary to other western cultural values, creating an
9.2.2.1 Humility ambivalence related to and in contrast with val-
The concepts of cultural competence, responsive- ues related to concepts such as assertiveness and
ness, and humility have relatively long histories self-confidence. Li contrasts this with eastern
in fields such as social work, education, psychol- cultures influenced by Confucianism and high-
ogy, and health care (see Tervalon & Murray-­ lights some of the possible cultural differences.
Garcia, 1998, and Foronda, 2020, for reviews of She looked at this across several quantifiable and
158 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

qualitative dimensions. For example, controlled environments for a person or group of people.
word searches across the samples of western and Oftentimes, the people who are the receivers of
Confucian-heritage cultures (CHC) indicate that interventions are vulnerable, and other people
the word “humility” appears in much higher are making decisions about what should be
usage in everyday frequency in CHCs; in con- changed and how. When people express objec-
trast, the word “pride” is used at a much higher tions to the status quo, there are discussions,
frequency in western cultures than in CHCs. The tensions, divisions, and sometimes reconcilia-
more a word appears, the more relevance it has to tions. Some of the controversies that directly
the culture, “it is likely that in a culture where intersect with culture are discussed in the
humility is more commonly emphasized, people emerging literature. For example, discussions
may be more apt to think, feel, behave and judge relate to the use of aversive control (Morris &
each other accordingly. Likewise, parents and Hollins, 2021; Sidman, 2001; Singer et  al.,
teachers socialize their children to do the same” 1999; Van Houten et  al., 1988), gender (e.g.,
(Li, 2016, p. 151). Li went on to suggest that hon- Donovan, 2021; LeBlanc et  al., 2020; Leland
oring ongoing self-cultivation and learning is et  al., 2021; Nordyke et  al., 1977), race (e.g.,
emphasized throughout all CHC societal rela- Gingles, 2021a; Gingles, 2021b; Li, 2021; Čolić
tionships and that there is an implicit duty to oth- et  al., 2021; Pritchett et  al., 2021), indigenous
ers in each of these relationships, a required peoples (e.g., Busch & Levasseur, 2021), inter-
reciprocity, to humbly continue learning for the sectionality (e.g., Cirincione-Ulezi, 2020), eth-
good of the collective. She introduces the CHC nicity (e.g., DuBay et al., 2018), religion (e.g.,
concept of “liability of self-fullness,” that every Aljohani, 2021), language (e.g., Baires et  al.,
time one learns something it is wise to try and 2021), neurodiversity (e.g., Friedman, 2021;
return to the stance of knowing it is not enough. Iland, 2021: Kirkham, 2017), poverty (e.g.,
The “liability of self-fullness” is an important Uwayo et al., 2021), and commodification (e.g.,
part of practicing cultural humility. It encourages Keenan et al., 2010). Sometimes the discussions
us to try and do our best with what we know and are in other forms, such as podcasts. For exam-
realize we do not know enough to serve the well- ple, Beautiful Humans (Gingles & Donovan,
being of the collective and continue developing n.d.) and Shades of ABA (Bradley & Moore,
as human beings. In the present case, it requires n.d.) are two active venues that address specific
holding the tension of a research base that has challenges and biases in practice and research.
suggested that children can make genuine and Such venues have expansive representation and
valued progress and know that the research base content that is not controlled by the dominant
is incomplete and that it is not enough. The majority culture.
research base has not accounted for or included It is important to note that the degree of
the perspectives of many cultures and ways of gatekeeping, scrutiny, and peer review varies
being. Our training programs, research, and prac- considerably in this emerging discourse. That is
tice do not contain an explicit responsibility and both the beauty and the tension. Social media
method for humble learning in relationship to and non-­refereed publications have welcomed
one another, to the families we serve, and to the voices that are barred or inadvertently excluded
collective wellbeing of people from many from the discourse. The intention of peer review
cultures. is to increase confidence that scientific commu-
Fortunately, some of the specific areas for nication is credible and meets the standards of
increased learning are being explored within quality for discourse and experimentation in a
and outside of our field. Most of these topics are given field (Kelly et al., 2014). It is difficult to
uncomfortable. The process of intervention will have peer review in the area of cultural congru-
always be controversial; intervening means that ence of research practices because there are few
there is an intentional and explicit series of “peers” in relation to cultural diversity. That is,
actions to change or not change responses and the majority of the scientific “peers” are also
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 159

cultural peers from the dominant majority. As a 9.2.2.2 Learning


field, we have not systematically addressed Learning to be responsive to the unique needs of
methods to reduce cultural bias, expand diver- people requires comfort in provisional spaces
sity, increase transparency, and further collab- and accessing bodies of knowledge production
orative research methods so that the voices of within and outside of the research evidence in
participants are consistently included (Pritchett intervention. This will require interacting with
et  al., 2021). Although some advances have texts and articles on the topics from within the
been made, refereed publications have strug- discipline (Ala’i & Re Cruz, 2021; Conners &
gled in expanding the base of diverse research- Capell, 2021; Mathur & Rodriguez, 2021;
ers and reviewers with knowledge and lived Sadavoy & Zube, 2021) and outside of the disci-
experiences that are divergent from the status pline on topics such as critical race theory and
quo or majority. Several people in leadership disabilities (e.g., Annamma et  al., 2016) and
have begun to publish discussions of these ten- technology (e.g., Benjamin, 2013). It will also
sions in an attempt to tact the problems and involve learning from other disciplines that have
develop solutions (Leaf et  al., 2021). While had a longer focus in this area (e.g., Lynch &
continued dialogue is welcomed by many lead- Hanson, 2011; McGoldrick & Hardy, 2019).
ers, “non-traditional” spaces (e.g., social The topics to study and learn about will be
media) often remain the sole avenue for some directed by understanding the community you
groups of people to be part of such conversa- serve. That is, who are the members of the com-
tions and to offer differing perspectives. munity? Do all members of the community have
Without understanding the larger context and a voice? Are there disparities or incongruencies
broader discussions taking place, both peer- reported in the literature about populations served
reviewed publications and social media venues in your communities? Are there groups barred
can sometimes appear to be binary and one- from receiving services in your communities?
sided conversations. This process of disruption, Who receives services later? Do the demograph-
fissure, and tension is also characteristic of our ics of your staff match the demographics of the
times. Part of our humble responsibility is to populations served? What are the concerns and
navigate these terrains and develop productive tensions being voiced by and on behalf of the
methods and venues for dialogue, arbitration, people you serve? How do those concerns inter-
and mediation so that we can nurture relation- act with the research base?
ships and welcome diverse perspectives and Ideally, such questions and learning should be
knowledge while creating new paradigms part of our initial training (Mathur & Rodriguez,
(Maparyan, 2012). 2021; Najdowski et al., 2021) and part of ongo-
The range of considerations and tensions is ing practice. In the meantime, as a way of orga-
astonishing, and one could feel overwhelmed and nizing, communities of practice can be formed to
paralyzed by many of the binary and seemingly study and systematically understand the complex
opposing conditions. It is compounded by the information on cultural responsiveness and to
lack of formal cultural training for intervention- make transformative changes in the way we
ists in behavior analytic practice (Fong et  al., engage in evidence-based practice (Anderson-­
2017) and that many of the injustices, exclusions, Carpenter et  al., 2014; LeBlanc et  al., 2020;
and tensions are normalized to the degree that Miller et al., 2019; Wenger, 2000). Specific strat-
they are not acknowledged or recognized in the egies are being developed to address approaches
published research. The path forward involves to training and culture that are specific to behav-
learning with great humility, to engage in the ior analytic interventions (Mathur & Rodriguez,
“liability of self-fullness” with a sense of respon- 2021) and address the distinct feature of a com-
sibility to the collective to do the best you can munity of practice committed to increasing cul-
with great care. tural responsiveness (Miller et al., 2019).
160 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

Time and Resources Have to Be Allocated to expand your understanding, learn the parameters
Learn  That means portions of agendas and of social importance, and help you learn ways to
work days should have designated time to include and tailor research evidence to meet the
improve responsiveness to culture: to review and needs of the people you serve.
reflect on practices; discuss new sources of infor-
mation; map the needs, strengths, and shortcom- Another area emphasized is the necessity of
ings of the community; decide necessary actions; improving our interaction skills across cultures.
and reflect and evaluate those actions taken. It How to communicate and have safe and progres-
also means that resources should be allocated to sive conversations with clients and peers of
meeting times, additional training, bringing in diverse backgrounds, values, and learning histo-
outside experts, and assessing the effects for ries is a complex skill and not an easy one to
both  the people being served and not those develop (e.g., Baires et  al., 2021; Barrera &
not served in our communities. And it means that Kramer, 2019). Because culture is ever evolving
time has to be allocated to act. This involves time and the learning process is ongoing, it will be
allocations for entering and nurturing genuine perpetually uncomfortable. Conditions have to be
relationships with the people that are part of the created to practice the components of effective
interventions, to have extended conversations, intercultural communication, to receive feed-
and to learn who they are and what they value, back, and to improve. Essential to this process is
both now and in the future. It also means spend- understanding the power differentials that exist in
ing time observing and considering desired and the provider-receiver relationship and that these
current ecologies, including shared goals and val- conditions require a humble posture and acknowl-
ues, such that interventions begin to sustainably edgement that the learning will be eternal (Baires
shape current climates toward these preferred et  al., 2021; Wright, 2019). The community of
environments (e.g., Bernal et al., 1995; Schwartz practice is the group that can help create increased
& Kelly, 2021). Finally, both time and resource fluency and courage. Members can provide simu-
allocation should be made for formal evaluations, lated practice and feedback of component skills,
to be discussed ahead. provide a forum for reflecting upon and generat-
ing ideas to improve actual interactions, explore
discomforts with the process, and generally nur-
Create Conditions for Progress  This involves ture these developing skills by acknowledging
identifying and creating opportunities for learn- effort and progress. The community can also
ing. Some of the central areas of opportunity identify intervention points that require improved
relate to self-reflection, communication, valued communication to increase responsiveness to
outcomes, and measures of progress. Cultural context and values.
awareness and self-reflection are key components Conditions can also be created to better under-
in any helping profession’s approach to culture stand valued outcomes and how these relate to
and interventions. This involves a process of cultural context and the evidence base. We start
understanding your own identity and positional- with creating conditions for the people we serve
ity, that is, your cultural context and how it relates to feel comfortable in the perspective sharing and
to the people around you in terms of experiences, making process. Among other things, it is impor-
privilege, access, and power. A community of tant to remember that everyone is in the process
practice is an ideal place to start this process. of learning. Mistakes are part of the learning pro-
There are a variety of training opportunities and cess. The hope is that mistakes are as benign as
articles (many cited in the reference list of this possible, do minimal harm, and are quickly
chapter) that can serve as a basis for learning. repaired. Intentionality is necessary to learn in
Each of these can be part of a community of prac- order to prevent similar mistakes from reoccur-
tice with group discussions that tie back to the ring moving forward. Each of us understanding
populations you work and serve with and that can and articulating our values and perspectives is a
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 161

work in progress that is affected by our environ- Nurture Progress  Here, the focus is on estab-
ments. The degree to which we feel safe doing lishing and developing ongoing methods to sup-
that will depend on where we are in relation to port progress. The central considerations are
privilege and oppression in our communities. In clear and evolving goals, safe and progressive
all cases, it is a matter of developing skills. When interactions, and multi-dimensional and multi-­
any person is learning how to exert agency, there sourced measures of progress. One of the first
is an increased responsibility to create conditions steps is to create a format for program evaluation,
for choices and preferences to be expressed, in a widely underutilized mechanism for assessing
both research and practice (Morris et al., 2021). organizational health in relation to goals and
The centrality of choices and balancing responsi- societal context, that  includes various people
bilities and liberties has been an ongoing dia- from varied cultures within that subset of society
logue in our field and is heightened during (Miller, 2017). The second step is to have ongo-
conditions of cultural differences (Bannerman ing discussions and planning around the goals of
et al., 1990). the community of practice. What are the values
The tension and learning lie in how to develop of your community? Do your program evalua-
strong interventions around those preferences tions incorporate indicators of these values in
and responsibilities within cultural groups, across action, in outcomes (Binder, 2016)? How are
cultural groups, and in ways that will serve chil- they changing over time? What are you learning?
dren with autism in the present cultural context How does it relate to the research? And most
and in a cultural context that will change over the importantly, are the children making better prog-
lifetime. Ultimately, this means how we under- ress? Are the families more involved? How did
stand and work with individuals to increase qual- they participate in the process? Is there more
ity of life, now and in the future. One of the main variation in the way programs are designed that
considerations is how our understanding of this reflects cultural values and happy progress? Do
interacts with an evidence base that is largely the people you work with feel that you care about
molecular in nature (Zarcone et  al., 2019) and their identities and values? Do they feel the out-
does not have often concentration on dependent comes reflect that care?
measures related to quality of life across the lifes-
pan within the context of behavior change inter-
ventions or research (Ala’i-Rosales et al., 2019; 9.2.3 Expansive Care
Fawcett, 1991; Pritchett et al., 2021; Schwartz &
Kelly, 2021). Again, it does not mean to say val- we are each other’s harvest:
we are each other’s business:
ued improvement is not demonstrated in the we are each other’s magnitude and bond.
research base; it means that this research base (Brooks, 1970)
does not often include these issues, which
increases the responsibility of the practitioner to We offer a portion of Pulitzer Prize poet
consider procedures across ecologies and time Gwendolyn Brooks’ work as the introduction to
(Lutzker & Campbell, 1994) and to create assess- care for several reasons. One of the ways to
ments of social importance and valued change understand people is through talking to them,
(Ferguson et al., 2019). Social validity can cover hearing their poetry and music, seeing their art,
a wide range of formats, from measures of affect listening to their stories, understanding how they
to choices in free and restricted operant contexts spend their days and prioritize their activities,
to interviews. The important thing for all social finding out what values they hold dear and what
validity is that the data collection methods are teachings guide them and their perspectives on
culturally informed and interpreted and not just life and the afterlife, and asking what gives their
arranged to make the programs look good or as lives meaning and substance. In fact, as authors,
an afterthought after failing to include the family we share this is as part of our poetry, as part of
and child throughout the entire process (Schwartz what informs our perspectives. By virtue of our
& Baer, 1991; Wolf, 1978). differing and uniquely combined positionalities
162 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

(women, brown, black, raised in poverty, immi- Kramer, 2019; Blell et al., 2010; LeBlanc et al.,
grant, and children of immigrants), many sources 2020; LeBlanc et al., 2021; McLaughlin & Carr,
of wisdom, in addition to research evidence, 2005; Rohrer et al., 2021; Tarbox & Rodriguez,
inform our perspectives and direct the guiding 2021; Taylor et  al., 2019; Walser & O’Connell,
principles and focus of this chapter (Jafar, 2018). 2021). For example, both Rohrer et al. (2021) and
For us, Brooks’ poetry reflects some core values; Taylor et al. (2019) discuss approaches that may
we find meaning and hope in acknowledging and be likely to establish and enhance therapeutic
acting on humanity’s essential unity and respon- relationships, including improving these skills to
sibility to one another. Brooks writes of humani- understand the different ways of expressing care
ty’s maturity, born of generational suffering, that and understanding meaning across cultures (e.g.,
declares that we are all interconnected and will Hurn & Tomlin, 2013; Zoch et  al., 2018) and
affect one another, that those effects will be in learning to change behaviors to adapt, to “sway,”
direct proportion to our efforts, and that we are in our interactions across cultures (Lynch &
bonded in our responsibility to one another. An Hanson, 2011).
ever-expanding number of voices across the
world echo these sentiments in many different 9.2.3.2 Procedures and Outcomes:
arenas, from global policies to scholarship in the Show You Care
humanities to the ethics of the  helping profes- Here, the emphasis is on developing methods to
sions (e.g., Birdsong, 2020; Karlberg, 2008; systematically include participants and cultural
Maparyan, 2012; McGoldrick & Hardy, 2019; context (e.g., Fawcett, 1991; Fong et  al., 2016;
Pritchett et  al., 2021; UN General Assembly, Fong et al., 2017; Fong & Tanaka, 2013; Morris
1948). & Hollins, 2021; Pritchett et al., 2021; Schwartz
Viewing our interdependency is the first part & Kelly, 2021). As practitioners, our role is to
of genuine and mature care. If interdependency understand these dimensions and to facilitate the
and equitable wellbeing is acknowledged as a family’s voice in the design of interventions. This
core value, the next step is to examine how this involves sharing and creating perspective and to
value translates to different areas of practice. do so for people of different cultural groups in
Caring and ways to show genuine care for all in equitable ways.  That is to say, we should care-
applied behavior analysis were part of our gene- fully consider the evidence base and to what
sis (Baer et  al., 1968; Wolf, 1978). The discus- degree it shows evidence of increased quality of
sions centered around care and culture have life and of including people of diverse cultures in
increased in the last few decades in at least a few the research. It also means learning about the
specific areas related to practice: (1) relationship family’s life and values across many important
development and communication, (2) procedures dimensions in which we humans can vary (Lynch
and outcomes, and, perhaps the most difficult & Hanson, 2011). This includes, but is not lim-
area, (3) structural oppression and biased deliv- ited to, the privileges and oppressions we experi-
ery systems. We will briefly discuss some of the ence; the degrees of affluence and poverty in our
caring actions interventionists can take in each of life circumstances; who we rely on, trust, and
these areas. turn to for help and support; how, what, and when
we communicate; how we interact and respond to
9.2.3.1 Attention to Relationship time; how we respond to hierarchies of power;
Development refugee and/or immigration conditions; family
and Communication: Show constellations; gender identification; languages
You Care spoken; religious and spiritual practices; and
There are an increasing number of scholars that trauma related to cultural identity. Time should
direct our attention to the importance of the ther- be dedicated and methods employed to under-
apeutic relationship in behavior analytic treat- stand the participants’ cultural perspective and
ment and the dimensions of how to improve those values along these dimensions and for the inter-
relationships (e.g., Baires et al., 2021; Barrera & ventionist to consider how this contrasts with the
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 163

research  and one's own personal experiences. the injustices in our own communities; welcom-
The family and the interventionist begin the ing and creating spaces for diverse voices within
­process of perspective taking and making as they our organizational structures; developing com-
integrate the best available research for the child’s munities of practice and strategic organizational
wellbeing in the present and in the future. Of plans specifically addressing values, goals, and
course, over time, the child becomes a more and outcomes related to equity and justice; and
more active participant in this process. Behavior encouraging solidarity, because of and in spite of
analytic evidence-based practice includes the our differences, for the wellbeing of all. The sug-
participant context and values, by welcoming and gested efforts can be studied and considered as
listening to the voices of the participants in we act and learn as individuals, as a discipline,
selecting goals, determining dependent mea- and as our world evolves.
sures, and producing satisfaction with outcomes
that address cultural and quality dimensions of
importance (Schwartz & Baer, 1991; Slocum 9.3 Conclusion and Onward
et al., 2014; Wolf, 1978).
As the world undergoes social transformation,
9.2.3.3 Structural Oppression we can better our interventions for all children
and Biased Delivery Systems: with autism in the context of their cultures, we
Show You Care can create and nurture genuine and caring com-
Oppressive and biased systems are perhaps the munities of practice, we can center voices and
most difficult and overwhelming area to navigate expressed dreams and life outcomes of the people
(Broder-Fingert et  al., 2020; Cihon & Mattaini, we serve, and we can learn in those communities
2019; Levy et  al., 2021; Mathur & Rodriguez, with humility. The children and families we serve
2021). Our interventions require moving beyond depend on it. We are doing hard work; there is a
molecular analysis and improvements (Ardila large evidence base we should learn and many
Sánchez et  al., 2020; Cihon & Kazaoka, 2021; skills we should master. And still, it is not enough.
Zarcone et  al., 2019). The pandemic and the We have to place what we are doing in the larger
resulting disruptions of all our societal structures context of culture and the human condition. We
are giving us a chance to hear the voices of many recognize that, on the one hand, we now know
oppressed people (e.g., Hill, 2020; Roy, 2020). In more than ever and can facilitate powerful
our own field, the pandemic and uprisings high- changes and, on the other hand, they are not
lighted injustice, made difficult discussions with always the right changes, nor are they always
unfamiliar words and concepts more “permissi- accessible. That is the liability of our self-­
ble,” and laid the groundwork for a series of arti- fullness. It is also our way forward.
cles and social media forums that specifically
address socio-political concerns. Vigorous and
heated explorations about what we can learn and References
what we might do during these times have
emerged. These dialogues have laid the ground- Adams, G., Dobles, I., Gómez, L. H., Kurtiş, T., & Molina,
L.  E. (2015). Decolonizing psychological science:
work for us to specifically address cultural Introduction to the special thematic section. Journal
responsiveness at a systemic level. As behavior of Social and Political Psychology, 3(1), 213–238.
analysts, we are exploring what we can learn and https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v3i1.564
what we might do during these times. Several Ala’i, S., & Re Cruz, A. (2021). The pedagogy of
nepantla. In J.  A. Sadavoy & M.  L. Zube (Eds.),
authors have suggested how individual behavior Scientific framework for compassion and social jus-
analysts and behavior analytic organizations can tice (1st ed., pp.  204–211). Routledge. https://doi.
take action (e.g., Ardila Sánchez et  al., 2020; org/10.4324/9781003132011-­32
Levy et  al., 2021; Mathur & Rodriguez, 2021; Ala’i-Rosales, S., Cihon, J. H., Currier, T. D. R., Ferguson,
J. L., Leaf, J. B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Weinkauf,
Miller et al., 2019). They include learning about S.  M. (2019). The big four: Functional assessment
164 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

research informs preventative behavior analysis. Benjamin, R. (2019). Assessing risk, automating racism.
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1), 222–234. https:// Science, 366(6464), 421–422. https://doi.org/10.1126/
doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00291-­9 science.aaz3873
Aljohani, W. A. (2021). Islamophobia. In J. A. Sadavoy Bernal, G., Bonilla, J., & Bellido, C. (1995). Ecological
& M.  L. Zube (Eds.), Scientific framework for com- validity and cultural sensitivity for outcome research:
passion and social justice (1st ed., pp.  168–173). Issues for the cultural adaptation and development of
Routledge. psychosocial treatments with Hispanics. Journal of
Anderson-Carpenter, K. D., Watson-Thompson, J., Jones, Abnormal Child Psychology, 23(1), 67–82. https://doi.
M., & Chaney, L. (2014). Using communities of prac- org/10.1007/BF01447045
tice to support implementation of evidence-based pre- Binder, C. (2016). Integrating organizational-cultural
vention strategies. Journal of Community Practice, values with performance management. Journal of
22(1–2), 176–188. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705422. Organizational Behavior Management, 36(2–3), 185–
2014.901268 201. https://doi.org/10.1080/01608061.2016.1200512
Annamma, S.  A., Connor, D.  J., & Ferri, B.  A. (2016). Birdsong, M. (2020). How we show up: Reclaiming fam-
Dis/ability critical race studies (DisCrit): Theorizing ily, friendship and community. Hachette Book Group.
at the intersections of race and dis/ability. In D.  J. Blell, Z. D., Alai-Rosales, S., & Rosales-Ruiz, J. (2010).
Connor, B.  A. Ferri, & S.  A. Annamma (Eds.), The effects of a supportive communication training
DisCrit: Disability studies and critical race theory in workshop on the verbal behavior of behavior analysts.
education (pp. 9–32). Teachers College Press. Behavior and Social Issues, 19(1), 144–166. https://
Ardila Sánchez, J. G., Cihon, T. M., Malott, M. E., Mattaini, doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v19i0.3088
M. A., Rakos, R. F., Rehfeldt, R. A., Richling, S. M., Boland, R. J., & Tenkasi, R. V. (1995). Perspective mak-
Roose, K. M., Seniuk, H. A., & Watson-Thompson, J. ing and perspective taking in communities of know-
(2020). Collective editorial: Ten guidelines for strate- ing. Organization Science, 6, 337–507. https://doi.
gic social action. Behavior and Social Issues, 29(1), org/10.1287/orsc.6.4.350
15–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42822-­020-­00038-­8 Bradley, A., & Moore, T. (Hosts). (n.d.). Shades of ABA.
Association for Behavior Analysis International. (n.d.). [Audio podcast]. https://anchor.fm/shadesofaba
2020 Membership Diversity Data. https://media. Broder-Fingert, S., Mateo, C.  M., & Zuckerman, K.  E.
abainternational.org/dei/DEI%20Data%202019-­ (2020). Structural racism and autism. Pediatrics,
2020%20Membership.pdf 146(3), e2020015420. https://doi.org/10.1542/
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some PEDS.2020-­015420
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Brooks, D.  C. (1970). The emancipation of women.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97. Macmillan.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­91 Busch, L., & Levasseur, A. (2021). Indigenous rights:
Baires, N. A., Catrone, R., & May, B. K. (2021). On the Finding the trail: Indigenous considerations for decol-
importance of listening and intercultural communi- onizing research and clinical work. In J. A. Sadavoy
cation for actions against racism. Behavior Analysis & M. L. Zube (Eds.), A scientific framework for com-
Practice. Advanced online publication. https://doi. passion and social justice (1st ed., pp.  159–167).
org/10.1007/s40617-­021-­00629-­w Routledge.
Bannerman, D.  J., Sheldon, J.  B., Sherman, J.  A., & Carr, J. E., & Briggs, A. M. (2010). Strategies for mak-
Harchik, A. E. (1990). Balancing the right to habilita- ing regular contact with the scholarly literature.
tion with the right to personal liberties: The rights of Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3(2), 13–18. https://
people with developmental disabilities to eat too many doi.org/10.1007/BF03391760
doughnuts and take a nap. Journal of Applied Behavior Cihon, T. M., & Kazaoka, K. (2021). Culture: A cultural
Analysis, 23(1), 79–89. https://doi.org/10.1901/ behavioral systems science perspective on the strug-
jaba.1990.23-­79 gle for social justice. In J. A. Sadavoy & M. L. Zube
Barrera, I., & Kramer, L. (2009). Using skilled dialogue (Eds.), A scientific framework for compassion and
to transform challenging interactions: Honoring iden- social justice (1st ed., pp. 100–106). Routledge.
tity, voice, & connection. Paul H. Brookes. Cihon, T.  M., & Mattaini, M.  A. (2019). Editorial:
Barrera, I., & Kramer, L. (2019). Skilled dialogue: Emerging cultural and behavioral systems science.
Authentic communication and collaboration across Perspectives on Behavior Science, 42, 699–711.
diverse perspectives. Balboa Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-­019-­00237-­8
Beaulieu, L., Addington, J., & Almeida, D. (2019). Cirincione-Ulezi, N. (2020). Black women and bar-
Behavior analysts’ training and practices regarding riers to leadership in ABA. Behavior Analysis in
cultural diversity: The case for culturally competent Practice, 13(4), 719–724. https://doi.org/10.1007/
care. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(3), 557–575. s40617-­020-­00444-­9
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00313-­6 Čolić, M., Araiba, S., Lovelace, T.  S., & Dababnah,
Behavior Analysis Certification Board. (n.d.) S. (2021). Black caregivers’ perspectives on rac-
BACB Certificant Data. https://www.bacb.com/ ism in ASD services: Toward culturally respon-
bacb-­certificant-­data/ sive ABA practice. Behavior Analysis in Practice.
Benjamin, R. (2013). People’s science: Bodies and rights Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/
on the stem cell frontier. Stanford University Press. s40617-­021-­00577-­5
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 165

Conners, B. M., & Capell, S. T. (2021). Multiculturalism Friedman, C. (2021). Reducing ‘challenging’ behav-
and diversity in applied behavior analysis: Bridging ior by training support staff to promote dignity and
theory and application. Routledge. https://doi. respect. Journal of Developmental and Physical
org/10.4324/9780429263873 Disabilities, 33(3), 449–458. https://doi.org/10.1007/
DelVecchio Good, M.  J., & Hannah, S.  D. (2015). s10882-­020-­09757-­7
“Shattering culture”: Perspectives on cultural com- Gillis, J. M., & Carr, J. E. (2014). Keeping current with the
petence and evidence-based practice in mental health applied behavior-analytic literature in developmen-
services. Transcultural Psychiatry, 52(2), 198–221. tal disabilities: Noteworthy articles for the practicing
https://doi.org/10.1177/1363461514557348 behavior analyst. Behavior Analysis Practice, 7(1),
Dirth, T. P., & Adams, G. (2019). Decolonial theory and 10–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­014-­0002-­7
disability studies: On the modernity/coloniality of Gingles, D. (2021a). Black lives matter. In J. A. Sadavoy
ability. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, & M. L. Zube (Eds.), Scientific framework for compas-
7(1), 260–289. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v7i1.762 sion and social justice (1st ed., pp. 38–44). Routledge.
Donovan, A.  R. (2021). Gendered language: Moving Gingles, D. (2021b). Igniting collective freedom: An
toward a compassionate gender expansive society. integrative behavioral model of acceptance and com-
In J.  A. Sadavoy & M.  L. Zube (Eds.), Scientific mitment toward Black liberation. Behavior Analysis
framework for compassion and social justice (1st ed., Practice. Advance online publication. https://doi.
pp. 122–127). Routledge. org/10.1007/s40617-­021-­00584-­6.
DuBay, M., Watson, L.  R., & Zhang, W. (2018). In Gingles, D. & Donovan, A. (Hosts). (n.d.). Beautiful
search of culturally appropriate autism interventions: Humans [Audio podcast]. Beautiful Humans Change.
Perspectives of Latino caregivers. Journal of Autism https://www.beautifulhumanschange.com/podcast
and Developmental Disorders, 48, 1623–1639. https:// Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­017-­3394-­8 weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain
England, K.  V. L. (1994). Getting personal: Reflexivity, Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/
positionality, and feminist research. The s0140525x0999152x
Professional Geographer, 46(1), 80–89. https://doi. Hill, M. L. (2020). We still here: Pandemic, policing, pro-
org/10.1111/j.0033-­0124.1994.00080.x test, and possibility. Haymarket Books.
Esquierdo-Leal, J.  L., & Houmanfar, R.  A. (2021). Hurn, B. & Tomlin, B. (2013). Cross-cultural communi-
Creating inclusive and equitable cultural practices cation: Theory and practice. Palgrave Macmillan UK.
by linking leadership to systemic change. Behavior Iland, T. (2021). Neurodiversity and ableism. In J.  A.
Analysis in Practice, 14, 499–512. https://doi. Sadavoy & M.  L. Zube (Eds.), Scientific framework
org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00519-­7 for compassion and social justice (1st ed., pp.  212–
Fawcett, S.  B. (1991). Some values guiding community 216). Routledge.
research and action. Journal of Applied Behavior Iwamasa, G.  Y. (1997). Behavior therapy and a cultur-
Analysis, 24(4), 621–636. https://doi.org/10.1901/ ally diverse society: Forging an alliance. Behavior
jaba.1991.24-­621 Therapy, 28(3), 347–358. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Ferguson, J. L., Cihon, J. H., Leaf, J. B., Van Meter, S. M., S0005-­7894(97)80080-­9
McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2019). Assessment of social Iwamasa, G. Y., & Smith, S. K. (1996). Ethnic diversity in
validity trends in the journal of applied behavior anal- behavioral psychology. Behavior Modification, 20(1),
ysis. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 20(1), 45–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/01454455960201002
146–157. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2018.15 Jacobson, D., & Mustafa, N. (2019). Social identity
34771 map: A reflexivity tool for practicing explicit posi-
Fong, E. H., & Tanaka, S. (2013). Multicultural alliance of tionality in critical qualitative research. International
behavior analysis standards for cultural competence in Journal of Qualitative Methods, 18, 1–12. https://doi.
behavior analysis. International Journal of Behavioral org/10.1177/1609406919870075
Consultation and Therapy, 8(2), 17–19. https://doi. Jafar, A.  J. N. (2018). What is positionality and should
org/10.1037/h0100970 it be expressed in quantitative studies? Emergency
Fong, E. H., Catagnus, R. M., Brodhead, M. T., Quigley, Medicine Journal, 35(5), 323–324. https://doi.
S., & Field, S. (2016). Developing the cultural aware- org/10.1136/emermed-­2017-­207158
ness skills of behavior analysts. Behavior Analysis Karlberg, M. (2008). Discourse, identity, and
in Practice, 9(1), 84–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/ global citizenship. Peace Review: A Journal
s40617-­016-­0111-­6 of Social Justice, 20(3), 310–320. https://doi.
Fong, E. H., Ficklin, S., & Lee, H. Y. (2017). Increasing org/10.1080/10402650802330139
cultural understanding and diversity in applied Karlberg, M., & Farhoumand-Simms, C. (2006). Global
behavior analysis. Behavior Analysis: Research and citizenship and humanities scholarship. International
Practice, 17(2), 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Journal of the Humanities, 2(3), 2189–2198.
bar0000076 Keenan, M., Dillenburger, K., Moderato, P., & Röttgers,
Foronda, C. (2020). A theory of cultural humility. Journal H.-R. (2010). Science for sale in a free market econ-
of Transcultural Nursing, 31(1), 7–12. https://doi. omy: But at what price? ABA and the treatment of
org/10.1177/1043659619875184
166 S. Alai-Rosales et al.

autism in Europe. Behavior and Social, 19, 126–143. Lynch, E. W., & Hanson, M. J. (2011). Developing cross-­
https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v19i0.2879 cultural competence: A guide for working with chil-
Kelly, J., Sadeghieh, T., & Adeli, K. (2014). Peer review dren and their families (4th ed.). Paul H.  Brookes
in scientific publications: Benefits, critiques, & A sur- Publishing.
vival guide. EJIFCC, 25(3), 227–243. Maparyan, L. (2012). The womanist idea. Routledge.
Kirkham, P. (2017). ‘The line between intervention and Mathur, K., & Rodriguez, K. A. (2021). Cultural respon-
abuse’–autism and applied behaviour analysis. History siveness curriculum for behavior analysts: A mean-
of the Human Sciences, 30(2), 107–126. https://doi. ingful step towards social justice. Behavior Analysis
org/10.1177/0952695117702571 in PracticeAdvance online publication. https://doi.
Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). Toward a theory of cul- org/10.1007/s40617-­021-­00579-­3
turally relevant pedagogy. American Educational McGoldrick, M., & Hardy, K.  V. (2019). Re-visioning
Research Journal, 32(3), 465–491. https://doi. family therapy: Addressing diversity in clinical prac-
org/10.2307/1163320 tice (3rd ed.). Guilford Press.
Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M., McLaughlin, D.  M., & Carr, E.  G. (2005). Quality of
Ala’i-Rosales, S., Ross, R.  K., Smith, T., & Weiss, rapport as a setting event for problem behavior:
M.  J. (2016). Applied behavior analysis is a science Assessment and intervention. Journal of Positive
and, therefore, progressive. Journal of Autism and Behavior Interventions, 7(2), 68–91. https://doi.org/1
Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 720–731. https://doi. 0.1177/10983007050070020401
org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2591-­6 Miller, K.  L. (2017). The use of evaluation in treatment
Leaf, J. B., Cihon, J. H., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Liu, N., programs for children with autism. Behavior Analysis
Russell, N., Unumb, L., Shapiro, S., & Khosrowshahi, in Practice, 10, 35–44. https://doi.org/10.1007/
D. (2021). Concerns about ABA-based intervention: s40617-­016-­0130-­3
An evaluation and recommendations. Journal of Autism Miller, K.  L., Re Cruz, A., & Ala’i-Rosales, S. (2019).
and Developmental Disorders, Advance online publi- Inherent tensions and possibilities: Behavior anal-
cation. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­021-­05137-­y ysis and cultural responsiveness. Behavior and
Leaf, J. B., Cihon, J. H., Ferguson, J. L., & Weiss, M. J. Social Issues, 28, 16–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/
(Eds.). (2022). Handbook of applied behavior analysis s42822-­019-­00010-­1
intervention for autism. Springer. Moll, L. C., Amanti, C., Neff, D., & Gonzalez, N. (1992).
LeBlanc, L. A., Sellers, T. P., & Ala’i, S. (2020). Building Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualita-
and sustaining meaningful and effective relationships tive approach to connect homes and classrooms.
as a supervisor and mentor. Sloan Publishing. Theory Into Practice, 31(2), 132–141. https://doi.
LeBlanc, L.  A., Gingles, D., & Byers, E. (2021). org/10.1080/00405849209543534
Compassion: The role of compassion in social jus- Morris, C., & Hollins, N.  A. (2021). On the uncanny
tice efforts. In J.  A. Sadavoy & M.  L. Zube (Eds.), similarities between police brutality and cli-
Scientific framework for compassion and social justice ent mistreatment. Behavior Analysis in Practice.
(1st ed., pp. 60–65). Routledge. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Leland, W., Stockwell, A., & Vaidya, J. (2021). Supporting s40617-­021-­00576-­6
ontogenic and cultural compassion building for the Morris, C., Detrick, J.  J., & Peterson, S.  M. (2021).
LGBTQ2IA community. In J.  A. Sadavoy & M.  L. Participant assent in behavior analytic research:
Zube (Eds.), Scientific framework for compassion and Considerations for participants with autism and devel-
social justice (1st ed., pp. 180–186). Routledge. opmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior
Levy, S., Siebold, A., Vaidya, J., Truchon, M.  M., Analysis, 54(4), 1300–1316. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Dettmering, J., & Mittelman, C. (2021). A look in jaba.859
the mirror: How the field of behavior analysis can Najdowski, A.  C., Gharapetian, L., & Jewett, V. (2021).
become anti-racist. Behavior Analysis in Practice. Toward the development of antiracist and multicul-
Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/ tural graduate training programs in behavior analysis.
s40617-­021-­00630-­3 Behavior Analysis in Practice, 14(2), 462–477. https://
Li, J. (2016). Humility in learning: A Confucian perspec- doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00504-­0
tive. Journal of Moral Education, 45(2), 147–165. National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and con-
https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2016.1168736 clusions: National standards project, phase 2.
Li, A. (2021). Solidarity: The role of non-black people of Author. https://www.nationalautismcenter.org/
color in promoting racial equity. Behavior Analysis national-­standards-­project/
in Practice, 14, 549–553. https://doi.org/10.1007/ National Professional Development Center. (2014).
s40617-­020-­00498-­9 Evidence-based practices for children, youth, and
Love, B.  L. (2019). We want to do more than survive: young adults. https://autismpdc.fpg.unc.edu/national-­
Abolitionist teaching and the pursuit of educational professional-­development-­center-­autism-­spectrum-­
freedom. Beacon Press. disorder
Lutzker, J.  R., & Campbell, R. (1994). Ecobehavioral Nordyke, N.  S., Baer, D.  M., Etzel, B.  C., & LeBlanc,
family interventions in developmental disabilities. J.  M. (1977). Implications of the stereotyping and
Brooks/Cole. modification of sex role. Journal of Applied Behavior
9  Be Humble, Learn, and Care: Culturally Responsive Evidence-Based Practice 167

Analysis, 10(3), 553–557. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Tarbox, J., & Rodriguez, K. (2021). Acting to support
jaba.1977.10-­553 compassion-focused applied behavior analysis. In
Osborne, N. (2015). Intersectionality and kyriarchy: J. A. Sadavoy & M. L. Zube (Eds.), Scientific frame-
A framework for approaching power and social work for compassion and social justice (1st ed.,
justice in planning and climate change adapta- pp. 8–15). Routledge.
tion. Planning Theory, 14(2), 130–151. https://doi. Taylor, B.  A., LeBlanc, L.  A., & Nosik, M.  R. (2019).
org/10.1177/1473095213516443 Compassionate care in behavior analytic treat-
Pritchett, M., Ala’i-Rosales, S., Cruz, A.  R., & Cihon, ment: Can outcomes be enhanced by attending to
T.  M. (2021). Social justice is the spirit and aim of relationships with caregivers? Behavior Analysis in
an applied science of human behavior: Moving from Practice, 12(3), 654–666. https://doi.org/10.1007/
colonial to participatory research practices. Behavior s40617-­018-­00289-­3
Analysis in Practice. Advance online publication. Tervalon, M., & Murray-Garcia, J. (1998). Cultural humil-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­021-­00591-­7 ity versus cultural competence: A critical distinction in
Project Implicit. (n.d.). https://www.projectimplicit.net defining physician training outcomes in multicultural
Rohrer, J., Marshall, K., Suzio, C., & Weiss, M. J. (2021). education. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and
Soft skills: The case for compassionate approaches Underserved, 9(2), 117–125. https://doi.org/10.1353/
or how behavior analysis keeps finding its heart. hpu.2010.0233
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 14, 1135–1143. https:// United Nations (UN) General Assembly (1948).
doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­021-­00563-­x Universal declaration of human rights.. UN General
Roy, A. (2020). The pandemic is a por- Assembly. https://www.un.org/en/about-­us/
tal. Financial Times. https://www.ft.com/ universal-­declaration-­of-­human-­rights
content/10d8f5e8-­74eb-­11ea-­95fe-­fcd274e920ca Uwayo, M., Hernandez, M., & Ross, D. (2021).
Sackett, D.  L., Rosenberg, W.  M., Gray, J.  A., Haynes, Understanding the needs of economically disadvan-
R.  B., & Richardson, W.  S. (1996). Evidence based taged children and families. In J. A. Sadavoy & M. L.
medicine: What it is and what it isn’t. BMJ (Clinical Zube (Eds.), Scientific framework for compassion and
research ed.), 312(7023), 71–72. https://doi. social justice (1st ed., pp. 151–158). Routledge.
org/10.1136/bmj.312.7023.71 Van Houten, R., Axelrod, S., Bailey, J.  S., Favell, J.  E.,
Sadavoy, J. A., & Zube, M. L. (2021). A scientific frame- Foxx, R.  M., Iwata, B.  A., & Lovaas, O.  I. (1988).
work for compassion and social justice. Routledge. The right to effective behavioral treatment. Journal
Schwartz, I.  S., & Baer, D.  M. (1991). Social valid- of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21(4), 381–384. https://
ity assessments: Is current practice state of the art? doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1988.21-­381
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(2), 189– Walser, R., & O’Connell, M. (2021). Acceptance and
204. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-­189 commitment therapy and the therapeutic relationship:
Schwartz, I.  S., & Kelly, E.  M. (2021). Quality of life Rupture and repair. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
for people with disabilities: Why applied behav- 77(2), 429–440. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23114
ior analysts should consider this a primary depen- Wenger, E. (2000). Communities of practice and social
dent variable. Research and Practice for Persons learning systems. Organization, 7(2), 225–246.
with Severe Disabilities, 46(3), 159–172. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1177/135050840072002
org/10.1177/15407969211033629 Wlodkowski, R. J., & Ginsberg, M. B. (1995). A frame-
Sidman, M. (2001). Coercion and its fallout (2nd ed.) work for culturally responsive teaching. Educational
Authors Cooperative. Leadership, 53(1), 17–21.
Singer, G.  H. S., Gert, B., & Koegel, R.  L. (1999). A Wolf, M.  M. (1978). Social validity: The case for sub-
moral framework for analyzing the controversy jective measurement or how applied behavior analy-
over aversive behavioral interventions for people sis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior
with severe mental retardation. Journal of Positive Analysis, 11(2), 203–214. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Behavior Interventions, 1(2), 88–100. https://doi. jaba.1978.11-­203
org/10.1177/109830079900100203 Wright, P.  I. (2019). Cultural humility in the practice
Skinner, B.  F. (1953). Science and human behavior. of applied behavior analysis. Behavior Analysis in
Macmillan. Practice, 12(4), 805–809. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Slocum, T.  A., Detrich, R., Wilczynski, S.  M., Spencer, s40617-­019-­00343-­8
T.  D., Lewis, T., & Wolfe, K. (2014). The evidence-­ Zarcone, J., Brodhead, M., & Tarbox, J. (2019). Beyond a
based practice of applied behavior analysis. call to action: An introduction to the special issue on
The Behavior Analyst, 37(1), 41–56. https://doi. diversity and equity in the practice of behavior analy-
org/10.1007/s40614-­014-­0005 sis. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(4), 741–742.
Smith, T. (2013). What is evidence-based behavior analy- https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00390-­1
sis? The Behavior Analyst, 36(1), 7–33. https://doi. Zoch, M., Alarcón, J., Bettez, S., & Hardin, B. (2018).
org/10.1007/BF03392290 Textured dialogues: A community project about
Sugai, G., O’Keeffe, B.  V., & Fallon, L.  M. (2012). immigrants’ multimodal perspectives on the mean-
A contextual consideration of culture and school-­ ing of education. Multicultural Perspectives, 20(1),
wide positive behavior support. Journal of Positive 39–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/15210960.2018.140
Behavior Interventions, 14(4), 197–208. https://doi. 8346
org/10.1177/1098300711426334
Part II
Evidence-Based Practices in Autism
Intervention
Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward
a Progressive Model 10
Justin B. Leaf, Julia L. Ferguson ,
and Joseph H. Cihon

10.1 D
 iscrete Trial Teaching: autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD a vari-
Toward a Progressive Model ety of skills including, but not limited to, play and
social skills (e.g., Nuzzolo-Gomez et  al., 2002;
Discrete trial teaching (DTT) is one of the most Shillingsburg et  al., 2014), conversational skills
commonly implemented behavior analytic tech- (e.g., Ingvarsson & Hollobaugh, 2010), and ques-
niques for autistics/individuals diagnosed with tion asking (e.g., Ingvarsson & Hollobaugh,
autism spectrum disorder (ASD; e.g., Lovaas, 2010). In addition to this research, DTT has been
1981, 1987). Within DTT an interventionist described in numerous curriculum books (e.g.,
breaks down complex sills into smaller compo- Leaf & McEachin, 1999), commentaries (e.g.,
nents and teaches these components one at a time Grow & LeBlanc, 2013), book chapters (e.g.,
(e.g., Leaf & McEachin, 1999). Therefore, DTT Weiss et al., 2017), professional conferences, and
commonly consists of a sequence of several dis- webinars. Thus, it is safe to say that DTT is a
crete trials to teach new skills. There are three widely researched, clinically implemented, and
primary components to each discrete tria1: (a) disseminated procedure.
the interventionist providing an instruction, (b) Leaf et  al. (2016) discussed two general
providing the learner an opportunity to respond approaches to the implementation of DTT. More
to the instruction, and (c) the interventionist pro- specifically, Leaf et  al. suggest that the imple-
viding consequence (i.e., reinforcement or pun- mentation of DTT occurs on a continuum with a
ishment) based upon the learners’ response. conventional approach on one end and a progres-
There are additional steps commonly associated sive approach on the other. The main difference
with DTT including arranging establishing oper- between a conventional and a progressive
ations (EO; Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950; Michael, approach to DTT is the main source of control for
1988), providing and fading prompts, and manip- the interventionist’s behavior. Within a more con-
ulating the inter-trial interval. ventional approach to DTT, the main source of
DTT has been extensively researched and has control for the interventionist’s behavior is a pro-
been demonstrated to be effective for teaching tocol that informs the interventionist what to do
and when to do it. This could be referred to as
J. B. Leaf (*) · J. L. Ferguson · J. H. Cihon rigid adherence to a protocol and requires less, or
Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, no, in-the-moment analysis from the interven-
USA tionist based on the learner’s responding. A more
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA, progressive approach to DTT differs in that the
USA main source of the interventionist’s behavior is
e-mail: jbleaf@APFmail.org

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 171
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_10
172 J. B. Leaf et al.

the learner’s behavior and other environmental variables that may contribute to the selection of a
variables. This has been referred to as a struc- more naturalistic environment (i.e., one that more
tured, yet flexible, approach (Leaf, Leaf et  al., closely resembles the terminal/desired environ-
2016) that requires almost constant in-the-­ ment). For instance, this environment may be
moment analysis from the interventionist. desired for learners for whom attending with dis-
Researchers have demonstrated the effectiveness tractions is the terminal goal or for learners who
of both approaches for teaching autistics/individ- have a history of skill acquisition within a more
uals diagnosed with ASD a variety of skills. naturalistic environment. Approaching interven-
However, some comparative studies have demon- tion with this environment in mind may help
strated the benefits of a progressive approach to learners learn with distractions in place and
DTT such as efficiency, effectiveness, and flexi- resembles how they would learn in school or
bility (e.g., Garvey et  al., 2021; Leaf, Cihon, work settings. This may result in better general-
Townley-Cochran et al., 2016; Wong et al., 2020). ization and maintenance of the targeted skills for
The purpose of this chapter is to describe vari- the learner. Ultimately, the environmental
ous components associated with DTT, provide a arrangement selection should not be made a pri-
brief analysis of the research on DTT, provide an ori but should instead be based on the specific
analysis of misconceptions often associated with needs of each learner (i.e., individualized) and
DTT, discuss the evidence base supporting the should be assessed frequently for possible
claim that DTT is an evidence-based practice, changes and needed adaptations.
and provide suggestions for future research and
clinical practice. 10.2.1.2 Target Selection
Prior to a block of teaching trials, an interven-
tionist should consider what skills will be tar-
10.2 Some Components of DTT geted within a block of trials as well as what
specific skill will be targeted on each trial. Within
10.2.1 Pre-trial Arrangement a more conventional approach to DTT, the target
for each trial across a trial block is commonly
10.2.1.1 Environment pre-planned. In other words, the target for each
One of the first decisions an interventionist must individual trial would be pre-determined and out-
make is about the environment in which they will lined on a protocol/data sheet for the interven-
implement DTT. This can be viewed as a contin- tionist. This contrasts with a more progressive
uum with a distraction-free environment on one approach in which the target on each specific trial
end and a more naturalistic environment on the within a teaching block is not pre-determined.
other. Several variables may contribute to the Rather, the interventionist develops a plan for
selection of the environmental arrangement. For what will be targeted within a teaching block and
instance, a distraction-free environment may be assesses the learner’s behavior within and across
desired for learners without a well-developed trials and adjusts the plan as needed. As a result,
attending repertoire. This may lead to better an interventionist can place focus on multiple tar-
attending, create opportunities to target attending gets within a teaching block or could conduct an
within the DTT format, and result in quicker skill entire teaching block dedicated to one target.
acquisition. Typically, when starting with a There has been limited research directly eval-
distraction-­free environment, the interventionist uating and comparing different methods to deter-
should gradually fade in distractions until the mine what to target on each trial across a trial
environment resembles the naturalistic/terminal block. In one of the few studies directly examin-
environment (e.g., school classroom). On the ing this, Wong et al. (2020) compared three dif-
other end of the continuum is the more naturalis- ferent approaches (i.e., predetermined,
tic environment. Similar to the selection of a constrained, and unconstrained) to the order and
distraction-­free environment, there are several number of presentations of target stimuli during a
10  Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward a Progressive Model 173

receptive language task with three ­autistics/indi- (Green 2001; Grow & LeBlanc, 2013). In an
viduals diagnosed with ASD. The predetermined effort to ensure this occurs, data sheets are devel-
condition involved targeting each stimulus three oped with arrays showing the interventionist
times each across a trial block in a counterbal- where to place target and non-target stimuli on
anced order (i.e., target and nontarget stimuli every trial within a trial block. Recommendations
equally distributed between all positions across a for counterbalancing can be found within the lit-
trial block). The constrained condition involved erature. For example, Grow and LeBlanc (2013)
targeting each stimulus a total of three times, but stated, “The instructor should rotate the auditory
not in a predetermined or counterbalanced order. and/or visual stimuli across trials in a balanced
The unconstrained condition involved assessing manner” and “when presented horizontally,
participant behavior to determine the order and visual comparisons should be presented and tar-
number of times each stimulus was targeted. geted proportionally in the left, middle, and right
Wong et al. found that the unconstrained condi- positions” (p.  64). This greatly differs from
tion required fewer sessions for the participants selecting target and non-target stimulus location
to reach the mastery criterion and minimal differ- within a progressive approach to DTT in which
ences across the conditions with respect to par- the interventionist is not directed by a counterbal-
ticipant responding during teaching and the ancing protocol or data sheet. Rather, the inter-
assessment of maintenance. ventionist uses clinical judgement that includes
Wong et al. (2020) also evaluated the interven- assessing the learner’s performance in the
tionists’ rationales within the unconstrained con- moment to determine the placement of target and
dition. This was done by having the interventionist non-target stimulus location. As a result, target
select or note the reason(s) for the selection of the and non-target stimuli might be placed equiva-
trial order of target stimuli as well as how many lently across all locations or may appear in the
times each stimulus was targeted. Wong et  al. same location across trials, depending on what is
found that the interventionists most frequently best for the learner on each trial.
selected “child’s attending behavior during teach- While researchers have demonstrated the
ing sessions,” followed by “current responding effectiveness of using counterbalancing (e.g.,
during current teaching session/condition” and Grow et  al., 2014) as well as in-the-moment
“correct responding to targets during probes” assessment of stimulus placement (e.g., Leaf
(p. 554). The results also indicated that the inter- et  al., 2018), it was not until recently that
ventionists were likely to select more than one researchers began to compare the two approaches.
variable that contributed to their decision-­ Specifically, Leaf et  al. (2018) compared three
making. While more research is needed in this methods of stimulus rotation when teaching
area, it is clear that there are several variables that receptive labels with five individuals diagnosed
influence the interventionist’s behavior within a with ASD. The three methods included counter-
progressive approach to DTT. balancing (as outlined by Grow & LeBlanc,
2013), fixed (i.e., no rotation across trials), and
10.2.1.3 Target Location in-the-moment assessment of stimulus placement
Another decision that an interventionist must within a DTT teaching context. The results were
make prior to DTT is the placement of targets, idiosyncratic across participants and, sometimes,
especially in the case of teaching receptive tar- within each participant. Two of the participants
gets (i.e., listener behavior). One method that has reached the mastery criterion with all three
been developed to assist interventionists in approaches, one participant reached the mastery
addressing the placement of targets is counterbal- criterion with the counterbalanced and in-the-­
ancing. Counterbalancing ensures that each tar- moment assessment of stimulus placement
get and non-target stimulus appears in each approaches, one participant reached the mastery
position in an array (e.g., left, center, and right) criterion only using a counterbalanced approach,
an equal number of times across a trial block and one participant reached mastery criterion
174 J. B. Leaf et al.

only using an in-the-moment assessment of attending, the type of array that may occur in a
stimulus placement approach. Taken together,
­ more naturalistic/the terminal environment (e.g.,
these results suggest interventionists should not a larger array for selecting from a vending
default to the use of counterbalancing, or any machine or a smaller array when selecting a writ-
approach, and should instead constantly assess ing utensil from a pencil box), and the terminal
and select the approach that will be best for each goals.
learner and context.
Like all clinical decisions related to DTT, the
decision as to where to place target and non-­ 10.2.2 Instructions
target stimuli should be informed by the analysis
of multiple variables. For instance, if a learner Following all pre-trial considerations, the first
displays a pattern of responding indicating a pos- component of DTT is the interventionist provid-
sible side bias (e.g., selecting stimuli that appear ing an instruction. There are two main variables
in the same location across trials), counterbalanc- that must be considered with respect to providing
ing might not be the best approach. If a side bias instructions: (a) the complexity of the instruction
is occurring and the interventionist is using coun- on each trial and (b) variations of instructions
terbalancing, the learner may still access rein- across trials.
forcement on a third of all trials for simply
selecting any stimulus that appears in one loca- 10.2.2.1 Complexity of Instructions
tion across all trials. In situations in which the When providing an instruction on each individual
learner has demonstrated learning with the stim- trial, the complexity of the instruction must be
uli changing locations on each trial, counterbal- considered. For example, should a more com-
ancing may be appropriate. This may be plex/natural language instruction be used (e.g.,
especially the case with newer staff as training on “Where is the red ball?”) or should a less com-
in-the-moment assessment of stimulus placement plex instruction be used (e.g., “Ball”). Defaulting
is likely to require more time and effort. to a less complex instruction on each trial is com-
mon with more conventional approaches to DTT
10.2.1.4 Field Size (Green, 2001; Grow & LeBlanc, 2013). For
Another decision that an interventionist must example, Green (2001) stated, “another desirable
make prior to teaching receptive labels or match- practice is to limit the auditory stimulus to start
ing is the size of the comparison array (e.g., 2, 3, each trial to the word to which one of the com-
4, or 8 stimuli). Green (2001) suggested that “for parison is to be matched (e.g., ‘spoon,’ ‘fork,’ or
most purposes, it is preferable to have at least ‘knife’) rather than starting each trial with a nom-
three comparisons on every trial” (p.  76). inal instruction like ‘Touch ________’ or ‘Point
However, it is likely that selecting the size of the to _______.’)” (p.  77). Defaulting to the most
comparison array should be based on a variety of complex/natural language instruction that can be
variables and should not be determined a priori, used while still ensuring learning and striving
which would more closely align with a progres- toward more natural language instructions is
sive approach to DTT (i.e., learner responding common with more progressive approaches to
being the main source of control for the interven- DTT. As such, it may be the case that for some
tionist’s behavior). This also aligns with the learners who are just beginning intervention,
research that has demonstrated the effectiveness simple instructions are used initially with the
of teaching receptive labels using a variety of goal to move to more complex/natural language
comparison array sizes (e.g., Gutierrez et  al., instructions as quickly as possible. For example,
2009). Some variables to consider when deter- Leaf and McEachin (1999) noted that “as the stu-
mining the size of the comparison array may dent progresses, instructions should become
include, but are not limited to, the learner’s more complex, and may be more wordy” (p. 133).
responses on previous trials, current and past There are many benefits of moving to more com-
10  Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward a Progressive Model 175

plex/natural language instructions as soon as learner may be ready for more varied instructions
­possible, with the possibility of preparing learn- across trials. Conversely, if the interventionist
ers for a more natural learning environment (e.g., probes a trial with a varied instruction and learner
school) being the biggest benefit. responding is negatively affected, the learner
Ultimately, the complexity of instructions may not be ready for varied instructions. Varied
should be based on an analysis of the learner’s instructions are also important to consider with
responding and overall goals and should not be respect to programming for generality (Stokes &
selected outside of this context (e.g., based on Baer, 1977). That is, progressing to varied
practice recommendations in isolation). For instructions as quickly as learner responding
example, if a learner is acquiring targets slowly allows is likely to lead to generalized skills and
with the use of complex/natural language instruc- better performance in the terminal environment.
tions, it may be fruitful to probe the use of less It may also be helpful for the interventionist to
complex instructions. It may be the case that less put themselves in the learner’s shoes. Hearing the
relevant parts of the instruction are interfering same instruction across many trials is likely to
with the learning process and less complex become monotonous and boring, which may lead
instructions could ameliorate that problem. It to unwanted behavior (e.g., not attending to the
could also be the case that the learner does not instructional materials, property destruction).
have a well-developed attending repertoire and Varied instructions, on the other hand, may keep
the length of a complex instruction prevents the learner more engaged and mitigate any
attending to the relevant parts of the instruction. unwanted behavior.
Alternatively, for learners who have sophisticated
verbal repertoires or if sustaining attention or
generality is the main goal, then more complex/ 10.2.3 Prompting
natural language instructions may be necessary
and appropriate. Although not a compulsory step of DTT, prompt-
ing likely becomes one of the most important
10.2.2.2 Variety of Instructions components of DTT for many interventionists.
Another consideration related to instructions is Prompting can be defined as any behavior that an
the variety of the topography of the instruction interventionist engages in to increase the likeli-
across trials. Some have advocated for keeping hood the learner will respond correctly (Green,
the topography of the instruction the same from 2001; Grow & LeBlanc, 2013; Krantz &
trial to trial (e.g., saying “Touch the [target]” on McClannahan, 1998; MacDuff et al., 2001). For
each trial; Ghezzi, 2007), which is commonly the purposes of this chapter, we will distinguish
associated with a more conventional approach to between prompt types and prompt systems.
DTT. Others have advocated for the intervention- Prompt types refer to the specific prompt used on
ist to have the flexibility to vary the instruction any discrete trial such as gestural, auditory, posi-
when necessary and appropriate (e.g., saying tional, partial physical, full physical, or reduction
“Touch the [target]” on one trial and “Find the of the field (see MacDuff et  al., 2001 for a
[target]” on another; Leaf & McEachin, 1999), review). Prompting systems refer to frameworks
which is commonly associated with a more pro- designed to help interventionists know when to
gressive approach. provide a prompt, when to fade a prompt, and
When and how to vary instructions requires what prompt type to provide.
the interventionist to analyze and respond to a There are many prompting systems that have
variety of variables. For example, if a learner is been researched and clinically implemented with
responding correctly on several trials with the autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD, many
same instruction, the interventionist may probe a of which more closely align with a conventional
trial with a different instruction. If the learner approach to DTT. Some of these prompting sys-
responds correctly with the varied instruction, the tems (e.g., least-to-most prompting, most-to-least
176 J. B. Leaf et al.

prompting) are based upon providing and fading 0 s delay which is progressively increased (e.g.,
prompts along a hierarchy. In a least-to-most 0 s, 1 s, 2 s, 5 s, 10 s) to a terminal duration (e.g.,
prompting system (e.g., Yanardag et  al., 2011), 10 s) based on learner responding. Both constant
the interventionist first provides an instruction and progressive time delay systems have been
alone without a prompt which gradually increases demonstrated to be effective in teaching a wide
to more assistive prompt types until the learner variety of skills for autistics/individuals diag-
engages in the correct response. Researchers nosed with ASD. For example, Ault et al. (1988)
have demonstrated that least-to-most prompting compared progressive and constant time delay to
can be effective in teaching a variety of skills for teach community signs to three students with
autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD.  For intellectual disability. The progressive time delay
example, Yanardag et  al. (2011) evaluated the ranged from 1 to 8  s, while the constant time
effects of least-to-most prompting in teaching delay was 5 s. Ault et al. utilized a parallel treat-
four autistics/children diagnosed with ASD to ment design and found both procedures to be
play tennis. Specifically, Yanardag et  al. taught effective.
the participants how to do a ball dribble, an air The third time-based prompting system is
dribble, and a dribble the lines drill. The results simultaneous prompting, which involves provid-
demonstrated that least-to-most was effective for ing a controlling prompt (i.e., a prompt that guar-
all four participants. antees a correct response) with a 0  s delay on
The inverse of least-to-most prompting is every teaching trial. Since all trials involve a con-
most-to-least prompting (e.g., Libby et al., 2008). trolling prompt, acquisition is assessed on probe
Most-to-least prompting starts with the interven- trials without a prompt at the beginning of each
tionist providing the most-assistive prompt type session. Simultaneous prompting has been used
possible and systematically fading to less-­ to teach a wide variety of skills including hand-
assistive prompt types as the learner responds washing (Parrott et al., 2000), expressive labeling
correctly. Researchers have also demonstrated (Akmanoglu-Uludag & Batu, 2005), leisure
that most-to-least prompting can be effective in skills (Kurt & Tekin-Iftar, 2008), and dressing
teaching a variety of skills for autistics/individu- (Sewell et  al., 1998). Akmanoglu-Uludag and
als diagnosed with ASD. For example, Fentress Batu (2005), for example, evaluated the effec-
and Lerman (2012) compared most-to-least tiveness of simultaneous prompting to teach
prompting system to a no-no prompting to teach receptive labeling for three individuals diagnosed
matching, receptive instructions, and imitation to with ASD. The controlling prompts were model-
four participants diagnosed with ASD.  Fentress ing and an auditory prompt. The results demon-
and Lerman demonstrated that both prompting strated that simultaneous prompting was effective
systems were effective, no-no prompting was for all three participants.
more efficient, and most-to-least prompting A final prompting system commonly associ-
resulted in fewer errors. ated with more conventional approaches is no-no
There are three prompting systems based on prompt (sometimes referred to as wrong-wrong
the passage of time: constant time delay, progres- prompt; Leaf et al., 2010). No-no prompt involves
sive time delay, and simultaneous prompting. initiating a trial without the use of a prompt. If
Constant time delay involves providing a prompt the learner responds correctly, the interventionist
after two periods of time (e.g., Walker, 2008). provides the corresponding  consequence (e.g.,
Within this system an interventionist starts with praise) and moves to the next trial. If the learner
providing a prompt with a 0  s delay which is responds incorrectly, the interventionist responds
increased to a fixed amount of time (e.g., 5  s) with “no” and repeats the trial without the use of
based on leaner responding. A progressive time a prompt. If the learner responds incorrectly
delay involves providing a prompt on a progres- again, the interventionist responds with “no” and
sive time scale (e.g., Walker, 2008). Like constant repeats the trial again but this time with the use of
time delay, a progressive time delay starts with a a prompt. No-no prompting was designed for use
10  Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward a Progressive Model 177

within a two-choice discrimination task to create receptive labels) for five participants diagnosed
learning opportunities deductive reasoning (e.g., with ASD. Soluaga et al. found that both prompt-
if it [target 1] this then it must be [target 2]) but ing systems were effective, but flexible prompt
has been used outside of two-choice discrimina- fading resulted in fewer trials for the participants
tion tasks. For example, Leaf et al. (2010) com- to reach the mastery criterion. Leaf et al. (2014)
pared no-no prompt to simultaneous prompting also evaluated the effectiveness of flexible prompt
to teach receptive labels to three individuals diag- fading by comparing it to error correction to
nosed with ASD.  The results demonstrated that teach labels of cartoon characters for four partici-
no-no prompt was more effective and efficient pants diagnosed with ASD.  Similar to Soluaga
within the experimental context for all three et al. (2008), Leaf et al. (2014) found that both
learners. systems were effective, but flexible prompt fad-
Flexible prompt fading (Leaf, Cihon, Leaf ing was more efficient. Most recently, Cihon
et  al., 2016; Leaf et  al., 2014; Soluaga et  al., et al. (2020) examined the relative effectiveness
2008) is the prompting system that most closely and efficiency of flexible prompt fading, constant
aligns with a progressive approach to DTT. time delay, and most-to-least prompting using a
Flexible prompt fading requires the intervention- group design (i.e., a randomized clinical trial). A
ist to assess the learner’s behavior and the context total of 27 participants were randomly assigned
on a moment-by-moment basis to determine if, to one of three conditions, each correlated with a
when, and how to prompt. There are some gen- different prompting system. The results showed
eral guidelines to flexible prompt fading, but it no significant differences during post probes
should be noted that these are guidelines, and not across the three prompting systems. The number
rules to be invariably followed. First, interven- of sets the participants reached the mastery crite-
tionists should attempt to keep the learner rion was also similar across the three prompting
responding correctly (prompted or unprompted) systems. However, participants in the flexible
on about 80% of all trials. Second, if the inter- prompt fading condition required fewer sessions
ventionist identifies conditions under which the to reach the mastery criterion, engaged in more
learner is likely to respond correctly without a independent correct responding, and less
prompt, no prompt should be provided. Third, if prompted incorrect responses.
the interventionist identifies potential conditions
under which the learner is likely to respond incor-
rectly, a prompt should be provided. Third, the 10.2.4 The Consequence
interventionist should strive to use the least-­
assistive, but still effective, prompt when possi- The final component of DTT is an interventionist
ble and fade the use of prompts as quickly as delivered consequence. This commonly means
possible. Fourth, if the learner is responding providing a reinforcing consequence following
incorrectly with a less assistive prompt, a more correct responses and some form of error correc-
assistive prompt should be provided. These tion following incorrect responses. This is con-
guidelines can be used to develop tools to assist sistent across more conventional and progressive
in the training of interventionists, an example of approaches to DTT. However, consequences
which can be seen in Fig. 10.1. involving the use of edible items are more com-
Flexible prompt fading has been implemented monly associated with a conventional approach
clinically for decades, dating back to the UCLA to DTT (Graff & Karsten, 2012; Kodak et  al.,
Young Autism Project (Leaf, Cihon, Leaf et al., 2012). This differs from a more progressive
2016; Lovaas, 1987). Soluaga et al. (2008), how- approach to DTT in which the goal is to condi-
ever, were the first to specifically evaluate flexi- tion and provide an array of putative reinforcers
ble prompt fading in an empirical study. In this including tangibles, privileges, social interac-
study, Soluaga et  al. compared flexible prompt tions, and the occasional use of edible items
fading to a time delay procedure to teach a vari- (Leaf, Cihon, Leaf et al., 2016; Leaf, Leaf et al.,
ety of skills (e.g., sight words/letters, math facts, 2016).
178 J. B. Leaf et al.

Fig. 10.1  Prompting decision-making tool

Within a more progressive approach to DTT, progressive approaches to DTT such as ignoring,
the interventionist provides reinforcing conse- providing corrective feedback (e.g., saying “No
quences for behaviors other than the acquisition that is not it”), providing corrective and informa-
target (e.g., receptive label, expressive label, tive feedback (e.g., saying “No that is not it, it is
matching). For example, reinforcing conse- a ball), and response repetition. Researchers have
quences may be provided for attending or refrain- demonstrated that a variety of error correction
ing from interfering behavior (e.g., stereotypy). procedures are effective for autistics/individuals
Thus, the amount, quality, and duration of rein- diagnosed with ASD (Leaf et  al., 2010; Leaf
forcement might differ from trial to trial. For et al., 2014; Worsdell et al., 2005). For example,
example, if a learner is exhibiting more effort Smith et al. (2006) compared three types of error
than on previous trials but is responding incor- correction procedures (i.e., saying no to a partici-
rectly, the interventionist may still provide a rein- pant after an incorrect response, modeling the
forcing consequence. correct response, and extinction) for six
There are many ways in which error correc- ­participants diagnosed with ASD.  Results were
tion may be provided in more conventional and idiosyncratic across the six participants, but
10  Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward a Progressive Model 179

showed that the error correction procedures were have shown that instructive feedback can be a
effective. Worsdell et  al. (2005) conducted a useful tool as part of DTT.
series of evaluations of the effects of response For example, Delmolino et  al. (2013) evalu-
repetition to teach 11 adults with developmental ated the effects of instructive feedback across two
disabilities sight words. The results indicated that experiments. In the first experiment, instructive
response repetition was effective. More recently, feedback was provided in a one-to-one instruc-
Leaf et  al. (2020) compared error correction to tional format for four individuals diagnosed with
errorless learning to teach tacting for 28 individu- ASD, while the second experiment evaluated the
als diagnosed with ASD.  The results demon- effects of instructive feedback in a group instruc-
strated that both were effective; however, error tional format. The results were mixed. In the first
correction was significantly more effective and experiment, only one of the four participants
resulted in low rates of aberrant behavior. acquired the instructive feedback targets, and
Although research and clinical practice have both participants acquired the instructive targets
demonstrated that error correction is an effective in the second experiment. In another experiment,
strategy within a DTT context, some have argued Leaf et al. (2017) evaluated the effects of instruc-
against its use (e.g., Burk, 2008; Gast, 2011). tive feedback within a group instructional format
Concerns about the use of error correction include for nine individuals diagnosed with
error correction resulting in undesired behavior, ASD.  Specifically, Leaf and colleagues taught
more errors occurring, and making therapy aver- the participants to expressively label sports play-
sive (Burk, 2008; Gast, 2011). Many of these ers or superheroes. As such, the instructive feed-
concerns have limited to no evidence within the back was the team the sports players played for
empirical literature. As such, interventionists or the superheroes’ powers. The results demon-
should be cautious of blanket statements about strated that all participants learned the expressive
avoiding the use of error correction procedures. label targets and the instructive feedback targets.
Nonetheless, there are several variables that must Furthermore, the participants acquired the targets
be considered when using error correction proce- from the other participants through observation.
dures. First, corrective feedback should not be More recently, Ferguson et  al. (2020) evaluated
provided in a harsh or excessive tone. However, the effects of instructive feedback using DTT
the corrective feedback must differ from the tone within a dyad format delivered via telehealth for
of the instruction and praise. Second, if the cor- six children diagnosed with ASD. Similar to pre-
rective feedback is ineffective (i.e., does not vious research, the participants learned the skills
result in less incorrect responses and more cor- taught directly and through instructive feedback.
rect responses), the interventionist should change
their strategy. Third, if corrective feedback results
in undesired behavior (e.g., emotional respond- 10.2.5 Data Collection
ing), then the interventionists should find a differ-
ent strategy. The final component of DTT is the intervention-
Finally, more progressive approaches to DTT ist taking objective data on the leaner’s response
commonly include the use of instructive feed- and/or other pertinent behavior (e.g., attending,
back during reinforcing consequences and error emotional responding). There are many types of
correction. Instructive feedback has been defined data collection systems used within conventional
as “consistently presenting extra, non-target and progressive approaches to DTT including
stimuli during the consequent events of instruc- trial-by-trial (e.g., Taubman et  al., 2013), time
tional trials” (Werts et al., 1995, p. 56). For exam- sampling (e.g., Repp, Deitz et  al., 1976), and
ple, if the targeted response was expressively probe data (e.g., Repp, Roberts, & Slack, 1976).
labeling a picture of a ball, instructive feedback A more progressive approach makes use of any
may be “Yes, it is a ball; you can bounce it” or of these data systems in addition to estimation
“No, it is a ball; you can throw it.” Researchers data (Taubman et al., 2013). Within a more pro-
180 J. B. Leaf et al.

gressive approach to DTT, the interventionist fashion and robotic-like responding does not
uses whichever data system is necessary to col- result in reinforcement, then robotic-like respond-
lect data to inform decisions and not disrupt the ing will not be the result. A fourth, and related,
learning process. misconception is that other procedures (e.g., inci-
dental teaching or mand model) are more natural
and effective than DTT (Chicago ABA Therapy,
10.3 Misconceptions of DTT n.d.). While it is possible that DTT can be imple-
mented in less naturalistic ways, not all DTT
Despite the plethora of evidence on the efficacy approaches do so. For instance, a progressive
of DTT, there are several commonly noted mis- approach to DTT is likely to be viewed as more
conceptions about DTT. First, DTT is sometimes natural as a result of varied and natural language
considered to be a procedure that is only imple- instructions.
mented in a one-to-one instructional format A fifth misconception is that DTT is only use-
(Lerman et  al., 2016). Although DTT is clearly ful for young children or for children who are new
effective in one-to-one instructional formats, to intervention (Honsberger, n.d.). This miscon-
research has found it to be effective in dyads ception is not supported by the empirical literature
(e.g., Ferguson et al., 2020), small groups (e.g., that has shown DTT to be effective for a wide vari-
Leaf et al., 2017), and large groups (e.g., Taubman ety of learners. That is, research has documented
et al., 2001). Furthermore, the implementation of the effectiveness of DTT for a variety of ages (e.g.,
DTT in a group instructional format may offer Kurt 2011), children who are new to intervention
several benefits such as observational learning, (e.g., Lovaas, 1987), children who have had more
preparing learners for other environments (e.g., experience with behavioral intervention (e.g.,
school), and assisting with generality. Ferguson et al., 2020), learners who display lower
A second misconception is that DTT and levels of language (Markham et al., 2020), learners
ABA-based intervention are synonymous. Dating who display higher levels of language (Leaf et al.,
back to the UCLA Young Autism Project, DTT 2010), learners who display lower levels of cogni-
has only been one teaching approach/procedure tive skills (e.g., Ferguson et  al., 2020), learners
that is commonly used as a part of a comprehen- who display higher levels of cognition (Leaf et al.,
sive program. Other procedures within ABA-­ 2013), learners who display lower levels of aber-
based interventions include, but are not limited rant behavior (Leaf et al., 2013), and learners who
to, shaping (e.g., Cihon, Ferguson, Leaf et  al., display higher levels of aberrant behavior (Leaf
2019), token systems (e.g., Cihon, Ferguson, et  al., 2010). Essentially, research and clinical
Milne et  al., 2019), incidental teaching (e.g., practice have clearly demonstrated that DTT is
McGee & Daly, 2007), the teaching interaction effective for individuals diagnosed with ASD
procedure (e.g., Green et  al., 2020), and video across the spectrum.
modeling (e.g., Rudy et  al., 2014). Simply put,
DTT is not synonymous with ABA.
A third misconception is that DTT is a robotic 10.4 Evidence-Based Practice
procedure that only results in robotic-like
responding in children (Tobenski Behavior The literature base makes it undeniable that DTT
Analysis Services, n.d.). While it is possible that is an evidence-based practice. This literature base
DTT can result in robotic-like responding in chil- is composed of studies that have clearly described
dren, the problem lies in the implementation of the participants and independent and dependent
DTT as opposed to DTT itself. If DTT is deliv- measures. Furthermore, this literature based is
ered in a robotic-like fashion and robotic-like composed of high-quality research designs with
responding results in reinforcement, then robotic-­ controls in place for threats to external and inter-
like responding will be the result. However, if nal validity. Taken together, the literature base for
DTT is implemented in a flexible, naturalistic DTT goes beyond the minimum standards that
10  Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward a Progressive Model 181

are commonly used to determine if an interven- across a variety of skills and contexts (e.g.,
tion should be considered an evidence-based Jennett et al., 2008). These comparisons could be
practice (e.g., Hume et al., 2021). It is also worth helpful in informing practitioners what approach
noting that the components of DTT themselves may be best for specific goals, clients, and con-
are comprised of behavioral principles with a texts. For instance, it may be possible that DTT is
plethora of research documenting the conditions more preferred and effective for some skills in
under which they result in behavior change. For various contexts while other approaches may be
example, DTT involves the use of reinforcement more preferred and effective in other contexts.
and punishment, both of which have been dem- Relatedly, research should continue to evaluate
onstrated as resulting in corresponding behavior methods to enhance the effectiveness and effi-
change across a wide variety of contexts, organ- cacy of DTT.  This research could continue to
isms, and demographics. Research evaluating the evaluate the effectiveness of the use of instructive
effectiveness of DTT has also included evalua- feedback (e.g., Ferguson et al., 2020) and imple-
tions of social validity, which have indicated that ment DTT within a group setting (e.g., Taubman
DTT is an acceptable approach and consumers et al., 2001).
have indicated that it results in favorable out- Perhaps the most fruitful and important area
comes (e.g., Jennett et al., 2008). for future work is evaluating efficient and effec-
tive methods to train individuals to implement a
progressive approach to DTT. There have been
10.5 Future Directions numerous studies developing and evaluating
effective methods to train individuals to imple-
Despite the documented effectiveness of and the ment DTT (see Leaf et al., 2019, for a review);
size of the literature base for DTT, there are sev- however there are no studies, to date, that have
eral fruitful areas for future work. DTT consists developed and evaluated methods to train indi-
of several components (e.g., prompting systems, viduals to implement a progressive approach to
complexity of instruction, inter-trial interval), DTT. Given the emerging literature on the effec-
and there can be differences within and across tiveness of a progressive approach to DTT, effec-
these components within practice and research. tive methods to train others in this approach are
Despite this, there has been limited research required. This may be a daunting task for
directly comparing differences within and across researchers as the progressive approach to DTT
these components. While there have been some requires analysis of many variables, often in-the-­
notable examples of this research (e.g., Aljohani moment, and changes in responding based on the
et al., 2021; Garvey et al., 2021; Leaf et al., 2018; outcome of that analysis. Identification of those
Leaf et al., 2020; Leaf, Cihon, Townley-Cochran variables and the effect on interventionists’
et al., 2016; Leaf et al., 2014; Leaf et al., 2010; behavior will be essential in this endeavor.
Wong et  al., 2020), most of these comparisons
have come from the same research group, and
more research is needed. Future research should 10.6 Conclusion
directly compare and manipulate these compo-
nents of DTT to determine the conditions under DTT has a long history within the field of ABA
which variations in these components are more or and autism intervention. Its evidence base is con-
less effective. The results from these comparative siderable, and its implementation is widespread.
studies will help practitioners to identify the con- Its long history and common implementation
ditions under which one approach may be more have likely contributed to the development of
effective for the learners and contexts in which protocols for the ease of implementation by prac-
they provide behavioral interventions. titioners with little to no training. This is a useful
Another area for future research is continuing approach when attempting to meet the demand
to compare the use of DTT to other approaches for more practitioners in a short amount of time.
182 J. B. Leaf et al.

Unfortunately, a potential side effect may be pro- Fentress, G. M., & Lerman, D. C. (2012). A comparison
of two prompting procedures for teaching basic skills
tocols becoming the main source of control for to children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum
the interventionist’s behavior as opposed to the Disorders, 6(3), 1083–1090. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
learner’s behavior and other relevant contextual rasd.2012.02.006
variables. More training of interventionists in a Ferguson, J.  L., Majeski, M.  J., McEachin, J., Leaf, R.,
Cihon, J. H., & Leaf, J. B. (2020). Evaluating discrete
progressive approach to DTT may help amelio- trial teaching with instructive feedback delivered in a
rate this practice and lead to more intervention- dyad arrangement via telehealth. Journal of Applied
ists providing meaningful, flexible, effective Behavior Analysis, 53(4), 1876–1888. https://doi.
DTT that lead to meaningful improvements for org/10.1002/jaba.773
Garvey, C.  C., Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C., Cihon, J.  H.,
autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Schulze, K.
(2021). Comparing in-view to out-of-view stimu-
lus arrangements when teaching receptive labels for
References children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.
Behavior Analysis in Practice. Advance online pub-
lication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­021-­00596-­2.
Akmanoglu-Uludag, N., & Batu, S. (2005). Teaching
Gast, D. L. (2011). An experimental approach to selecting
naming relatives to individuals with autism using
a response-prompting strategy for children with devel-
simultaneous prompting. Education and Training in
opmental disabilities. Evidence-based Communication
Developmental Disabilities, 40(4), 401–410.
Assessment and Intervention, 5(3), 149–155. https://
Aljohani, W.  A., Javed, A., Ferguson, J.  L., Cihon,
doi.org/10.1080/17489539.2011.637358
J.  H., Milne, C.  M., & Leaf, J.  B. (2021).
Ghezzi, P. M. (2007). Discrete trials teaching. Psychology
Comparison of simultaneous prompting to error
in the Schools, 44(7), 667–679. https://doi.
correction for children with autism spectrum dis-
org/10.1002/pits.20256
order. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
Graff, R. B., & Karsten, A. M. (2012). Assessing prefer-
Disabilities. Advance online publication. https://doi.
ences of individuals with developmental disabilities:
org/10.1177/10883576211036429.
A survey of current practices. Behavior Analysis
Ault, M. J., Gast, D. L., & Wolery, M. (1988). Comparison
in Practice, 5(2), 37–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/
of progressive and constant time-delay procedures in
BF03391822
teaching community-sign word reading. American
Green, D.  R., Ferguson, J.  L., Cihon, J.  H., Torres, N.,
Journal of Mental Retardation: AJMR, 93(1), 44–56.
Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Rudrud, E., Schulze, K.,
Burk, C. (2008). Errorless learning. Retrieved from http://
& Leaf, J.  B. (2020). The teaching interaction pro-
christinaburkaba.com
cedure as a staff training tool. Behavior Analysis in
Chicago ABA Therapy. (n.d.). Differences between
Practice, 13(2), 421–433. https://doi.org/10.1007/
natural environment training (NET) and dis-
s40617-­019-­00357-­2
crete trial training (DTT). Chicago ABA Therapy.
Green, G. (2001). Behavior analytic instruction for
https://chicagoabatherapy.com/articles/differences-­
learners with autism advances in stimulus con-
between-­n atural-­e nvironment-­t raining-­n et-­a nd-­
trol technology. Focus on Autism and Other
discrete-­trial-­training-­dtt/
Developmental Disabilities, 16(2), 72–85. https://doi.
Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R.,
org/10.1177/108835760101600203
McEachin, J., & Taubman, M. (2019). Use of a level
Grow, L., & LeBlanc, L. (2013). Teaching receptive
system with flexible shaping to improve synchronous
language skills: Recommendations for instructors.
engagement. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1),
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 6(1), 56–75. https://
44–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0254-­8
doi.org/10.1007/BF03391791
Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  B., Milne, C.  M.,
Grow, L. L., Kodak, T., & Carr, J. E. (2014). A comparison
Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (2020). A randomized clini-
of methods for teaching receptive labeling to children
cal trial of three prompting systems to teach tact rela-
with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic repli-
tions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(2),
cation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(3),
727–743. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.617
600–605. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.141
Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C.  M., Leaf, J.  B.,
Gutierrez, A., Hale, M. N., O’Brien, H. A., Fischer, A. J.,
McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2019). A preliminary evalu-
Durocher, J. S., & Alessandri, M. (2009). Evaluating
ation of a token system with a flexible earning require-
the effectiveness of two commonly used discrete
ment. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(3), 548–556.
trial procedures for teaching receptive discrimination
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00316-­3
to young children with autism spectrum disorders.
Delmolino, L., Hansford, A.  P., Bamond, M.  J., Fiske,
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(3), 630–
K. E., & LaRue, R. H. (2013). The use of instructive
638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.12.005
feedback for teaching language skills to children with
Honsberger, T. (n.d.). Fact sheet- discrete trial. Florida
autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(6),
Atlantic University. https://www.fau.edu/education/
648–661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.02.015
centersandprograms/card/documents/discretetrial.pdf
10  Discrete Trial Teaching: Toward a Progressive Model 183

Hume, K., Steinbrenner, J. R., Odom, S. L., Morin, K. cal trial. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 36(1), 1–20.
L., Nowell, S. W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey, S., https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-­019-­00124-­y
McIntyre, N. S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage, M. Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., &
N. (2021) Evidence-based practices for children Taubman, M. (2016). A progressive approach to
youth and young adults with Autism: Third genera- discrete trial teaching: Some current guidelines.
tion review. Journal of Autism and Developmental International Electronic Journal of Elementary
Disorders, 51(11) 4013–4032. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Education, 9(2), 361–372.
s10803-020-04844-2 Leaf, J. B., Cihon, J. H., Townley-Cochran, D., Miller, K.,
Ingvarsson, E.  T., & Hollobaugh, T. (2010). Acquisition Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Taubman, M. (2016). An
of intraverbal behavior: Teaching children with evaluation of positional prompts for teaching receptive
autism to mand for answers to questions. Journal of identification to individuals diagnosed with autism
Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(1), 1–17. https://doi. spectrum disorder. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9(4),
org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­1 349–363. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­016-­0146-­8
Jennett, H.  K., Harris, S.  L., & Delmolino, L. (2008). Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M.,
Discrete trial instruction vs. mand training for teaching Ala'i-Rosales, S., Ross, R.  K., Smith, T., & Weiss,
children with autism to make requests. The Analysis of M.  J. (2016). Applied behavior analysis is a science
Verbal Behavior, 24, 69–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/ and, therefore, progressive. Journal of Autism and
BF03393058 Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 720–731. doi:https://
Keller, F.  S., & Schoenfeld, W.  N. (1950). Principles of doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2591-­6.
psychology. Appleton Century Crofts. Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., Taubman, M., McEachin, J., &
Kodak, T., Fuchtman, R., & Paden, A. (2012). A compari- Delmolino, L. (2014). Comparison of flexible prompt
son of intraverbal training procedures for children with fading to error correction for children with autism
autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(1), spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and
155–160. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-­155 Physical Disabilities, 26(2), 203–224. https://doi.
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1998). Social inter- org/10.1007/s10882-­013-­9354-­0
action skills for children with autism: A script-fading Leaf, J.  B., Sheldon, J.  B., & Sherman, J.  A. (2010).
procedure for beginning readers. Journal of Applied Comparison of simultaneous prompting and no-no
Behavior Analysis, 31(2), 191–202. https://doi. prompting in two-choice discrimination learning with
org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-­191 children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
Kurt, O. (2011). A comparison of discrete trial teaching Analysis, 43(2), 215–228. https://doi.org/10.1901/
with and without gestures/signs in teaching recep- jaba.2010.43-­215
tive language skills to children with autism. Kuram ve Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (1999). A work in progress:
Uyglamada Egitim Bilmleri, 11, 1436–1144. Behavior management strategies and a curriculum for
Kurt, O., & Tekin-Iftar, E. (2008). A comparison of con- intensive behavioral treatment of autism. DRL Books.
stant time delay and simultaneous prompting within Leaf, J.  B., Tsuji, K.  H., Lentell, A.  E., Dale, S.  E.,
embedded instruction on teaching leisure skills to Kassardjian, A., Taubman, M., McEachin, J., Leaf,
children with autism. Topics in Early Childhood R., & Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L. (2013). A comparison
Special Education, 28(1), 53–64. https://doi. of discrete trial teaching implemented in a one-to-one
org/10.1177/0271121408316046 instructional format and in a group instructional for-
Leaf, J. B., Aljohani, W. A., Milne, C. M., Ferguson, J. L., mat. Behavioral Interventions, 28(1), 82–106. https://
Cihon, J.  H., Oppenheim-Leaf, M.  L., McEachin, J., doi.org/10.1002/bin.1357
& Leaf, R. (2019). Training behavior change agents Lernman, D.  C., Valentino, A.  L., & LeBlanc, L.  A.
and parents to implement discrete trial teaching: (2016). Discrete trial training. In R. Lang, T. Hancock,
A literature review. Review Journal of Autism and & N. Singh (Eds.), Early intervention for young chil-
Developmental Disorders, 6(1), 26–39. https://doi. dren with autism spectrum disorder: Evidence-based
org/10.1007/s40489-­018-­0152-­6 practices in behavioral health. Springer. https://doi.
Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Alcalay, A., Mitchell, E., org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­30925-­5_3
Townley-Cochran, D., Miller, K., Leaf, R., Taubman, Libby, M. E., Weiss, J. S., Bancroft, S., & Ahearn, W. H.
M., & McEachin, J. (2017). Instructive feedback (2008). A comparison of most-to-least and least-to-­
embedded within group instruction for children most prompting on the acquisition of solitary play
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of skills. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 37–43.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(2), 304–316. https:// https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03391719
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.375 Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally disabled
Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., McEachin, J., children: The me book. PRO-ED Books.
Leaf, R., & Taubman, M. (2018). Evaluating three Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor-
methods of stimulus rotation when teaching receptive mal educational and intellectual functioning in
labels. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11(4), 334–349. young autistic children. Journal of Consulting
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0249-­5 and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi.
Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C.  M., org/10.1037//0022-­006x.55.1.3
Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (2020). Comparing error Markham, V., Giles, A., & May, R. (2020). Evaluating
correction to errorless learning: A randomized clini- efficacy and preference for prompt type during dis-
184 J. B. Leaf et al.

crete trial teaching. Behavior Modification, 44(1), Soluaga, D., Leaf, J.  B., Taubman, M., McEachin,
49–69. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445518792245 J., & Leaf, R. (2008). A comparison of flexible
MacDuff, G.  S., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E. prompt fading and constant time delay for five
(2001). Prompts and prompt-fading strategies for children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum
people with autism. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & R. M. Disorders, 2(4), 753–765. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Foxx (Eds.), Making a difference behavioral interven- rasd.2008.03.005
tion for autism (1st ed., pp. 37–50). Pro Ed. Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol-
McGee, G.  G., & Daly, T. (2007). Incidental teaching ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior
of age-appropriate social phrases to children with Analysis, 10(2), 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1901/
autism. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe jaba.1977.10-­349
Disabilities, 32(2), 112–123. https://doi.org/10.2511/ Tobenski behavior analysis services. (n.d.). Common
rpsd.32.2.112 misconceptions. Woolf center for ABA. https://
Michael, J. (1988). Establishing operations and the mand. tobenskibehavior.com/applied-­b ehavior-­a nalysis/
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 6, 3–9. https://doi. common-­misconceptions/
org/10.1007/BF03392924 Taubman, M., Brierley, S., Wishner, J., Baker, D.,
Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., Leonard, M.  A., Ortiz, E., Rivera, McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. B. (2001). The effectiveness
C. M., & Greer, R. D. (2002). Teaching children with of a group discrete trial instructional approach for pre-
autism to prefer books or toys over stereotypy or pas- schoolers with developmental disabilities. Research in
sivity. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4(2), Developmental Disabilities, 22(3), 205–219. https://
80–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/109830070200400203 doi.org/10.1016/s0891-­4222(01)00068-­3
Parrott, K. A., Schuster, J. W., Collins, B. C., & Gassaway, Taubman, M. T., Leaf, R. B., McEachin, J. J., Papovich,
L. J. (2000). Simultaneous prompting and instructive S., & Leaf, J.  B. (2013). A comparison of data col-
feedback when teaching chained tasks. Journal of lection techniques used with discrete trial teaching.
Behavioral Education, 10(1), 3–19. https://doi.org/10. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(9), 1026–
1023/a:1016639721684 1034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.05.002
Repp, A.  C., Roberts, D.  M., & Slack, D.  J. (1976). Walker, G. (2008). Constant and progressive time delay
A comparison of frequency, interval, and time-­ procedures for teaching children with autism: A lit-
sampling methods of data collection. Journal of erature review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Applied Behavior Analysis, 9(4), 501–508. https://doi. Disorders, 38(2), 261–275. https://doi.org/10.1007/
org/10.1901/jaba.1976.9-­501 s10803-­007-­0390-­4
Repp, A. C., Deitz, D. E., Boles, S. M., Deitz, S. M., & Weiss, M.  J., Hilton, J., & Russo, S. (2017). Discrete
Repp, C. F. (1976). Differences among common meth- trial teaching and social skills training: don’t throw
ods for calculating interobserver agreement. Journal the baby out with the bath water. In J. B. Leaf (Ed.),
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9(1), 109–113. https:// Handbook of social skills and autism Spectrum disor-
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1976.9-­109 der: Assessment, curricula, and intervention (pp. 155–
Rudy, N.  A., Betz, A.  M., Malone, E., Henry, J.  E., & 170). Springer International Publishing.
Chong, I.  M. (2014). Effects of video modeling on Werts, M. G., Wolery, M., Holcombe, A., & Gast, D. L.
teaching bids for joint attention to children with (1995). Instructive feedback: Review of parameters
autism. Behavioral Interventions, 29(4), 269–285. and effects. Journal of Behavioral Education, 5(1),
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1398 55–75. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02110214
Sewell, T. J., Collins, B. C., Hemmeter, M. L., & Schuster, Wong, E., Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C.  M., Cihon, J.  H.,
J.  W. (1998). Using simultaneous prompting within Leaf, J.  B., McEachin, J., Leaf, R., Schulze, K., &
an activity-based format to teach dressing skills to Rudrud, E. (2020). Evaluating three methods of the
preschoolers with developmental delays. Journal presentation of target stimuli when teaching recep-
of Early Intervention, 21, 132–145. https://doi. tive labels. Behavioral Interventions, 35(4), 542–559.
org/10.1177/105381519802100206 https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1744
Shillingsburg, M. A., Bowen, C. N., Valentino, A. L., & Worsdell, A. S., Iwata, B. A., Dozier, C. L., Johnson, A. D.,
Pierce, L.  E. (2014). Mands for information using Neidert, P. L., & Thomason, J. L. (2005). Analysis of
"who?" and "which?" in the presence of establish- response repetition as an error-correction strategy dur-
ing and abolishing operations. Journal of Applied ing sight-word reading. Journal of Applied Behavior
Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 136–150. https://doi. Analysis, 38(4), 511–527. https://doi.org/10.1901/
org/10.1002/jaba.101 jaba.2005.115-­04
Smith, T., Mruzek, D.  W., Wheat, L.  A., & Hughes, C. Yanardag, M., Birkan, B., Yılmaz, İ., & Konukman, F. K.
(2006). Error correction in discrimination training for (2011). The effects of least-to-most prompting pro-
children with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 21(4), cedure in teaching basic tennis skills to children with
245–263. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.223 autism. Kinesiology, 43(1), 44–55.
Incidental Teaching Research:
Early Beginnings Through Recent 11
Innovations

Gail G. McGee

11.1 Incidental Teaching in more varied and complex contexts, which


Research: Early Beginnings inevitably produces the use of more elaborate
Through Recent Innovations language” (Hart & Risley, 1980, p. 407). In a nut-
shell, when teaching is provided in everyday situ-
Incidental teaching (IT) is a systematic protocol ations, in which the naturally occurring cues are
of instruction that is implemented in the natural constantly changing, a learner benefits by using
environment where newly acquired skills are their new skills in ways that adapt to future
needed and helpful to the learner (McGee, 2005). circumstances.
By teaching new responses in the context of natu- IT procedures were not developed in isolation
rally occurring environmental stimuli, the proba- of other evidence on how children (and adults)
bility of generalized and durable learning is learn. To the contrary, IT builds on the “shoulders
enhanced. of giants” in the experimental and applied analy-
What IT is not is random chatter from a sis of behavior (e.g., Baer et al., 1968; Sidman,
friendly teacher. Given the definition of “inciden- 1988; Skinner, 1948, 1957). IT is an operant con-
tal” in Merriam-Webster’s (n.d.) dictionary … ditioning procedure in which preferred teaching
“occurring merely by chance or without intention materials are both the discriminative stimuli for a
or calculation” …, there is not much that is inci- learner’s initiations that begin IT episodes and
dental about IT. Hart and Risley (1980), the origi- the consequent stimuli for responding to instruc-
nal developers of IT, used the term “incidental” tion (Ala’i-Rosales et al., 2017). When applied to
teaching to describe what were initially unantici- the most challenging learning goals, such as
pated side effects of the instructional procedures speech shaping in nonverbal children with
they were developing to improve children’s use autism, IT also incorporates errorless learning
of language. Thus, early findings were that IT procedures that gradually fade discriminative
yielded flexible use of language beyond the con- stimuli across temporal or physical dimensions
fines of the stimulus conditions in which specific (Halle et  al., 1981; Touchette, 1968, 1971). In
vocabulary words had been directly taught. They sum, IT procedures are firmly grounded upon
proposed that “ … talking more involves talking past and accumulating empirical research in
behavior analysis. When applied to young chil-
G. G. McGee (*) dren, IT research is also informed by empirical
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, findings from other fields (e.g., developmental
Emory University School of Medicine, psychology, social psychology, early childhood
Atlanta, GA, USA education).
e-mail: gmcgee@emory.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 185
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_11
186 G. G. McGee

There are other wonderful “naturalistic” Table 11.1  Components of classic IT episode
behavioral procedures that “go by several differ- Child’s verbal
ent names” (Schreibman et  al., 2015, p.  2417) or gestural
initiation and
other than IT, but this chapter will focus on Setting response Teacher instruction
research that specifically uses the term and/or Natural
procedural components of IT. More than 50 years environment is
of systematic and carefully planned IT research arranged to
have been conducted by a wide variety of inde- attract a child to
desired
pendent investigators who have extended applied materials and
behavior analysis in interesting and creative activities
ways. Recurring themes will be the importance Initiates to the
of systematic environmental engineering to teacher to
request or
ensure reinforcer potency, provider preparation,
discuss an
and program replication. item/topic of
interest
Responds to Assists child in
11.2 O
 riginal Development of IT the teacher’s elaborating an
guidance by initiation by
Procedures elaborating blending prompts
with Neurotypical Children the original for a skill the
initiation child needs to
Betty Hart and Todd Risley initially developed IT learn into the
child’s interests
as a method to narrow the “meaningful” differ-
Praises and
ences in language development of children from provides the child
economically disadvantaged versus professional access to the
families (Hart & Risley, 1995). Their first con- desired play
material, activity,
ceptualization of IT began by observing
or information
Montessori, Head Start, and university-based
preschool teachers who had been recommended
by parents and other teachers as the best teachers response to the teacher’s instruction, the child
in their respective centers; those “naturally-born also accesses reinforcement for having success-
super teachers” were observed to discover com- fully engaged in a social interaction with their
monalities in how they interacted with their teacher. It has been suggested that each IT epi-
young students (T. R. Risley, personal communi- sode should be brief, although a teacher may add
cation, November 17, 1988). Their goal was to up to two more quick prompts as needed to ensure
identify the precise conditions under which chil- the child’s success (e.g., when a child does not
dren will best learn to use language. This early answer the teacher’s first prompt to expand their
analysis of successful teaching exchanges led to initiation, the teacher may provide an easier clue
conclusions that the key components of effective and then [if still needed] offer gentle physical
teaching interactions with young children include guidance to “touch the block that you want”). IT
instruction in natural settings, child initiations, episodes should also be interspersed with play to
and response-produced reinforcement (Hart & ensure that learning continues to be fun for the
Risley, 1982). These components continue to be child.
considered the essential ingredients of IT, as The first applications of IT were aimed at
illustrated in Table 11.1. improving language use by preschoolers who
It is important to note that IT provides at least attended Head Start and lived in economically
a two-step interaction between a child and their disadvantaged neighborhoods (i.e., Hart &
teacher (Hart & Rogers-Warren, 1978). In addi- Risley, 1968; Reynolds & Risley, 1968). In a
tion to accessing reinforcement for a correct study of children’s use of descriptive adjectives
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 187

(Hart & Risley, 1968), it was found that the chil- idly increased and eventually overlapped with
dren learned colors in a traditional circle time those of children in the university preschool.
activity (e.g., the teacher displayed a blue toy car Throughout the course of the IT studies, recur-
and prompted the children to say, “It’s a blue ring findings were as follows:
car.”); however, the children did not use the newly
learned color adjectives during free play later in 1. Toys must be chosen and displayed in ways
the day. Hence, limitations of the initial instruc- that create opportunities for IT (Hart & Risley,
tional procedures served as the impetus for devel- 1978, 1980).
oping a novel approach to enhance children’s use 2. Children played more with toys that were
of language. The IT format taught the children only available through initiation and interac-
color adjectives during their play activities. For tions with teachers, and they played less with
example, when a child pointed to a toy fire truck toys that were freely available (Hart & Risley,
visible on a high shelf, the teacher would ask, 1974, 1980).
“What color is the truck?” If the child responded, 3. IT yielded not only unprompted use of skills that
“I want the red truck,” the teacher praised them had been directly taught, but children enrolled in
and gave them access to the truck. When colors IT classrooms also showed impressive improve-
were taught using IT, the children immediately ments in the “spontaneous” or flexible variety of
began to use color adjectives in their everyday how they used language (Risley, 1972; Hart &
vocabularies, and they continued to use them Risley, 1968, 1975, 1982).
after teaching was completed. In the next two
studies (i.e., Hart & Risley, 1974, 1975), more Especially pertinent to future application of IT
complex language was targeted in sequential as an early autism intervention were childcare
phases (i.e., noun, then color adjective/noun, then “models” developed by Risley and colleagues
color/noun/how the child planned to use the associated with the University of Kansas Living
desired toy). The children in these studies learned Environment Groups. Specifically, university-­
the new language as teaching was introduced for based childcare centers were developed for neu-
each language target, and they generalized their rotypical infants (Herbert-Jackson et  al., 1977),
use of compound sentences to use in unprompted toddlers, (O’Brien et al., 1979), and preschoolers
situations with new teachers. (Allen & Hart, 1984). Research was conducted to
An extensive study next compared the lan- plan how to organize the staff in environments for
guage use of three groups of 4-year-old children dependent populations in ways that maximized
across an entire school year (Hart & Risley, opportunities for interaction and incidental teach-
1980). Specifically, data were collected on the ing (Doke & Risley, 1972; Krantz & Risley,
total number of words per hour that participating 1977; McClannahan & Risley, 1975; Twardosz
children used during free play. Comparison et al., 1974). Also studied were appropriate nutri-
groups included (a) children enrolled in a tion for young children (Herbert-Jackson &
university-­based IT preschool and whose parents Risley, 1977; Twardosz et al., 1975) and how to
were upper-middle-class professionals, (b) chil- use differential attention to promote good behav-
dren enrolled in an IT Head Start classroom and ior (Porterfield et  al., 1976). A hallmark of the
whose parents were economically disadvantaged, models was that multiple teaching zones were
and (c) children enrolled in a traditional (non-IT) arranged in an overlapping activity schedule
Head Start classroom and whose parents were (LeLaurin & Risley, 1972). Multi-faceted for-
economically disadvantaged. Children in the uni- mats were developed to ensure ongoing program
versity preschool had large spontaneous vocabu- quality (Risley & Favell, 1979), and program
laries at the first of the year, yet both groups of evaluation included a Planned Activity Check
Head Start children rarely used language during (PLA-Check) observational system that mea-
free play. As the year progressed, the language sured levels of engagement (Cataldo & Risley,
levels of children in the IT Head Start class rap- 1974).
188 G. G. McGee

11.3 Applications of IT taught with IT during sessions in which reinforc-


with Children with Autism ers were arranged on a bookshelf in varying posi-
tions (e.g., on/under, inside/next to) in relation to
An interest in the application of IT to individuals plastic shoeboxes (McGee et  al., 1985).
with autism arose due to the long-recognized dif- Participants (who had been trained to wait for a
ficulty of establishing functional language in teacher’s prompt) had to be directly encouraged
nonverbal children, along with frequently to “initiate” or say what they wanted. Traditional
encountered problems that children with autism DTT occurred at a table, and teaching stimuli dif-
have in generalizing skills learned in traditional fered from reinforcers for correct responses.
discrete-trial training (DTT) settings to use in Highly preferred and systematically selected (cf.
everyday life (Lovaas et al., 1973; Stokes & Baer, Shafer et  al., 1984) candy and toy reinforcers
1977). In one of the first applications of IT with were used in both conditions. Results showed
children with autism, sign word requests were similar rates of acquisition, but generalization of
taught to nonverbal children (Carr & Kologinsky, correct preposition use was far greater for prepo-
1983). Results showed better acquisition of signs sitions that had been taught with IT (i.e., post-­
than had occurred during previous DTT, and teaching transfer to a different classroom, with
there were collateral decreases in the occurrence different teachers, under prompted and
of challenging behaviors during IT sessions. The unprompted conditions, as well as for teaching
students’ new skills maintained after IT sessions stimuli arranged in same and novel positions than
ended, and the students generalized their use of had been used during initial instruction).
signs acquired during IT to unprompted A procedural comparison was also conducted
situations. in a study of sight word reading with school-aged
Receptive object labels were taught to three children with autism who had been progressing
nonverbal children who had been raised in an slowly in the well-programmed Edmark Reading
institution but recently moved to a community-­ Program (McGee et al., 1986). IT was combined
based group home (McGee et  al., 1983). The with stimulus fading procedures, which began
children had been unresponsive to DTT aimed at with presentation of one sight word card on
teaching them the names of common household which the label of a preferred toy was printed;
items (e.g., spoon, plate). However, IT seemed to additional sight word cards were added (or
pose a plausible alternative because the children removed) depending on a child’s accuracy on the
did appear to be learning the names of items that previous IT episode. Both participants showed
interested them (e.g., “Please go get the brownies faster acquisition and retention of sight words
from the kitchen”). Brief interruption of a highly learned during IT than during concurrent instruc-
preferred daily routine (making school lunches) tion in DTT sessions, and only the sight words
was a substitute for unprompted initiations to acquired during IT generalized across multiple
teachers. Acquisition of labels of lunch-making dimensions (e.g., words printed in different type
supplies occurred quickly across sets of items styles, differing print sizes, and oral reading of
(e.g., relish, baggy, lettuce), and correct identifi- newly learned words in a book during both cued
cation of the new item labels was generalized to a and unprompted conditions). These findings sug-
different activity conducted in the dining room. gested IT may prevent over-selectivity, or atten-
There have been several controlled compari- tion to irrelevant stimulus features, which had
sons between IT and DTT procedures when been previously reported to interfere with learn-
applied to teaching specific skills to children with ing by children with autism (Lovaas et al., 1971).
autism. Two such comparisons were conducted Anecdotal observations also confirmed earlier
with school-aged children who attended the reports that children seemed to enjoy incidental
Princeton Child Development Institute (PCDI), a teaching (Hart & Risley, 1974, 1980); specifi-
specialized school for children with autism cally, students often returned toys used as teach-
(McGee et  al., 1985, 1986). Prepositions were ing and consequent stimuli before their play time
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 189

ended and then immediately re-initiated for the one child decreased his use of newly learned
same toy. mands at follow-up, a second child retained skills
Linne and Melin (1992) compared IT and at levels achieved during teaching but showed
DTT as methods for teaching children with limited generalization, and the third child retained
autism to use phrases containing descriptive and generalized use of his new skills across mate-
adjectives to request preferred toys and foods. rials, places, and people).
Initial instruction took place at school, and main-
tenance and generalization were later assessed in
children’s homes. Initial findings favored DTT in 11.4 E
 xtensions of IT to Various
terms of efficiency. However, at follow-up, both Target Responses
procedures yielded retention, IT produced better and Populations
generalization, and IT yielded equal or better use
of unprompted language. Conclusions were that In a study of IT of sight word reading with stu-
children who have formerly been taught with tra- dents (who had not been diagnosed with autism),
ditional DTT may have to “learn” how to learn adolescents and young adults with moderate to
from IT, but there may be benefits when they do severe mental retardation were taught to read
so. In an examination of ten studies that offered words printed on tokens that had been earned
controlled comparisons of DTT and IT (and other during earlier DTT sessions (Fabry et al., 1984).
naturalistic teaching procedures), conclusions The procedure was effective with five of the six
were that more naturalistic instruction was most participants; conclusions were that IT during
effective (Delprato, 2001). periodic token exchange periods (which had pre-
IT has also been successfully applied to pro- viously not been used as instructional time)
moting social skills in inclusive settings. Thus, increased overall learning opportunities provided
preschoolers with autism and typical peers were throughout the day.
taught how to interact with one another (Brown One study used a group design to evaluate the
et al., 1991). In a study aimed at increasing the use of IT to teach Spanish to eight neurotypical
frequency with which a high-school student with mono-lingual (English-speaking) preschoolers
autism (and another student with moderate devel- (Valdez-Menchaca & Whitehurst, 1989). The
opmental delays) received social bids, Haring preschool environment was arranged to evoke
and Breen (1992) established two small group children’s initiations by displaying boxes labelled
social networks of high-school students without in Spanish words that matched the preferred toys
disabilities. The peer networks met weekly to inside each box. The toy boxes were visible but
discuss and schedule when (lunch and certain out of reach of the children, who were taught to
class transition times) and who would make request the toys in Spanish. Eight different chil-
social contacts with the students with disabilities. dren participated in a control condition, in which
Results showed that inclusive reciprocal interac- the children requested the out of reach toys in
tions developed among the students and friend- English and the teacher simply labeled the toys in
ship activities extended beyond planned activities Spanish as they provided them. IT procedures
at school and even to community outings. yielded more use of Spanish words both during
More recently, IT research was conducted in a and following instruction.
special education preschool in Istanbul, Turkey In a study of differences between IT and more
(Horasan & Birkan, 2015). Three boys with highly structured programs that served 35
autism were taught to use verbal demands for preschool-­aged children (i.e., McWilliam et  al.,
help finding hidden objects that were usually 1985), findings were that higher levels of child
present during art, academic, and leisure activi- engagement occurred in the IT programs.
ties. All the children met initial acquisition crite- Similarly, another study (i.e., Dunst et al., 1986)
ria, but results varied across children during evaluated 20 preschool programs in Western
follow-up and generalization assessments (i.e., North Carolina and found a significant positive
190 G. G. McGee

correlation between the extent to which IT was ducted 5–17 weeks after teaching (Schepis et al.,
used and children’s levels of engagement with 1982). These lead investigators, highly experi-
teachers. The quality of engagement displayed by enced in personnel preparation research, pub-
individual preschool children with developmen- lished an additional IT study that prepared
tal delays was found to be best predicted by IT, support staff for children with disabilities in
developmental quotient, and levels of peer inter- inclusive preschools (Schepis et al., 2001).
actions. However, IT was the strongest predictor When Head Start teachers of preschoolers
of time spent in sophisticated engagement (Casey with language delays participated in a large group
et al., 2012). workshop on IT (Mudd & Wolery, 1987), the
IT procedures have also been extended to teachers learned and later used some steps of IT;
adults with disabilities. In an Indiana group however, the workshop did not prepare teachers
home, peer tutors with developmental delays to use all the steps needed to implement IT effec-
used incidental teaching to increase verbal tively. In a second phase, the teachers were pro-
requests from their peers (Farmer-Dougan, vided both verbal and written feedback on their
1994). A package of instructions, prompts, and use of IT in the classroom, and the frequency of
modelling was used to prepare peer tutors to (a) their complete IT episodes increased markedly.
watch for initiations (reaches for desired items Two IT preparation studies reported positive ben-
kept out of reach), (b) prompt requests from peer efits of brief workshop training; interventionists
learners, and (c) provide the requested items. were able to use IT with group home residents
New requesting skills were generalized to inter- with autism (MacDuff et al., 1988), and teachers’
actions with other peers and group home staff in use of some steps of IT led to increased student
situations that varied from the teaching setting. initiations to their teachers (Ryan et  al., 2008).
Conclusions were that increased initiations were Casey and McWilliam (2008) prepared preschool
maintained due to naturally occurring reinforce- teachers (i.e., 21 lead and assistant teachers from
ment contingencies of fulfilled requests and 10 schools) to use IT with children who had
social interactions with peers. developmental delays. Specifically, provision of
A unique application of incidental teaching graphical feedback on the amount of IT that chil-
was to teach verbal requests to an adult patient dren received on the previous day was effective
with a traumatic brain injury, who presented with in increasing the teachers’ use of IT.
severe challenges in generalization of language The importance of preparing providers in how
(Lennox & Brune, 1993). Evaluated in a multiple-­ to arrange IT environments in ways that encour-
baseline design across three settings/activities/ age initiations has also been explored in several
locations, the patient quickly mastered verbal studies. Haring et al. (1987) prepared teachers of
requesting skills learned via IT. Within a few ses- older students with autism (and students with
sions, they began to initiate with verbal requests severe developmental delays) to use four differ-
rather than waiting for their requests to be cued. ent strategies to evoke the initiations needed to
begin incidental teaching. The teachers learned to
(a) provide students with opportunities to choose
11.5 P
 reparing Providers in How teaching materials, (b) block student access to
to Do IT desired materials and/or events, (c) place desired
materials out of student reach, and (d) offer items
An early provider preparation study involved out of context. Results showed that teachers
hands-on coaching of institutional staff to pro- increased the number of opportunities they pro-
vide modified incidental teaching of sign lan- vided for their students to use communication
guage to four youths with autism and five youths skills, although the teachers varied in terms of
with profound mental retardation; results showed whether they pre-planned environmental arrange-
that all participants increased their use of signs ments as had been recommended. The students
and continued to use them at follow-up con- were interested in the objects and events used to
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 191

structure IT episodes, and their responsiveness to self-advocacy statements both when prompted
teachers increased. Another study prepared nurs- and independently. The paraprofessionals gave
ery schoolteachers in Great Britain to use IT by positive feedback on a social validity survey
emphasizing the importance of how classroom (e.g., confidence in their increased abilities to use
materials were arranged (Dolley & Whendell, IT); one noted that the BIE device was initially
1987). In a later study, these same investigators distracting but said they became used to it over
(Dolley & Whendell, 1988) first prepared teach- time. The researchers suggested the importance
ers of children from predominantly Punjabi- of relationship building prior to preparing class-
speaking families in the how-to steps of IT; room providers from a remote location, and they
results showed that children’s initiations and the offered helpful tips and future research ideas
average number of words that children spoke to regarding remote technology.
teachers increased as a function of the teachers’ Neely et al. (2016) carefully evaluated another
use of IT. In a second phase of this study (Dolley remote telepractice package designed to prepare
& Whendell, 1988), the teachers received addi- various providers to do IT. The package consisted
tional preparation in how to arrange classroom of (a) an online didactic module, (b) self-­
environments to elicit children’s requests for evaluations of videotaped teacher-child interac-
classroom materials; results showed even further tions, and (c) delayed feedback via
increases in both the number of IT episodes pro- videoconference during which the trainer and
vided and children’s initiations to teachers. trainee discussed their independent videotaped
Hsieh et  al. (2011) prepared caregivers reviews. Trainees were future behavior analysts
(including a group of home respite worker and (one with a B.A. and two undergraduate students
two different parents of children with autism and who majored in Special Education, Psychology/
severe developmental delays) in how to do IT Sociology, and Community Health, respectively);
during individualized sessions in their homes. All all were inexperienced in the use of IT proce-
three caregivers were successful in increasing dures. Results showed that the future behavior
their resident’s/children’s use of mands; after analysts increased the number of communication
their preparation was completed, the caregivers opportunities that they provided to children with
were also able to use IT to teach their children autism within the context of ongoing routines and
new skills. Rittenhouse-Cea and Cho (2018) activities. The children with autism also increased
reported similar results in a recent study in which the frequency of using their new mands as a func-
four instructors were provided specific individu- tion of increased communication opportunities.
alized performance feedback on their use of IT, An extension of the previous methodology
and both the providers and the children with was used in a pyramidal approach to preparing
autism showed acquisition and generalization of interventionists to do incidental teaching (Neely
targeted skills. et  al., 2018). Two doctoral-level students in
A series of especially timely studies have Educational Psychology were taught how to do
examined various forms of remote coaching of IT IT, and they were then prepared to coach four
skills. For example, Rosenberg et al. (2020) used additional master’s-level or undergraduate stu-
a “bug-in-ear” (BIE) device to coach four para- dents. Each of the six participants, all of whom
professionals (in an inclusive school system) to were previously unfamiliar with IT, was paired
use IT to teach self-advocacy statements to four with a child with autism who was receiving
students with autism. The paraprofessionals university-­based clinic services to permit evalua-
increased the frequency and accuracy of their tion of their IT skills. Both coaches and interven-
new IT skills, and all children participating tionists acquired IT skills as a function of their
learned to use their individualized self-advocacy provider preparation, and all six children
statement(s). The paraprofessionals also main- increased their use of mands when their providers
tained and generalized their IT skills to new situ- implemented IT.  Another replication of this
ations, and their students continued to use their approach was accomplished by first preparing a
192 G. G. McGee

bilingual coach in Japan, who then taught three throughout the day, including during routine
interventionists how to use IT to effectively childcare activities (Allen & Hart, 1984; Herbert-­
increase use of mands by three students with Jackson et al., 1977; O’Brien et al., 1979).
autism (Neely et al., 2020). Responses to a social
validity survey were highly positive, despite
some comments pertaining to the technology and 11.6.1 Description of the Walden
the challenge of scheduling feedback sessions Classroom
across widely different international time zones.
Comments on the IT literature reviewed above To provide an abundance of teachable moments,
will be presented in Conclusions at the end of this traditional early childhood activities (e.g., free-­
chapter. The research presented next will describe play, snack, art, outdoor recess) are scheduled in
the use of IT as a comprehensive approach to multiple teaching zones. Teaching zones are
early autism intervention. defined by the activity and physical area of the
classroom, which are opened in overlapping time
sequences to provide children with a choice of
11.6 Comprehensive Application three to four activities throughout most of the
of IT: All Behavioral day. A schedule of overlapping zones minimizes
Intervention and Instruction the occurrence of inpatient behaviors that young
Provided Exclusively in IT children sometimes display during large group
Formats transitions. Thus, a child may join a small group
tabletop game whenever space becomes avail-
In 1985, Walden opened as a lab school at the able, and they may leave when they ask to go to a
University of Massachusetts-Amherst with the different activity. At Walden, no matter what time
goal of advancing IT research in a manner that of day or where the child goes, there has been
promotes broad positive change in children’s advanced planning to ensure the classroom envi-
developmental trajectories. It seemed like seren- ronment provides opportunities for each child to
dipitous timing to launch a research program that learn what they need to know and a cheerful and
combined (a) the generalization benefits of IT, energetic teacher is available to make learning
(b) the benefits of beginning intervention when easy and fun.
children with autism were as young as possible To make it possible for children to have a vari-
(cf. Lovaas, 1987), and (c) the benefits of social ety of activity choices, zone teachers must adhere
learning opportunities afforded by inclusion of strictly to detailed teaching routines. A classroom
young children with autism among a majority of schedule (posted prominently on the wall) alerts
typically developing peers (cf. Odom et  al., teachers to which activity zone for which they are
1985). responsible, and every zone teacher is prepared to
Walden began and continues as a systematic mastery on each zone routine to which they are
replication of the previously described model assigned. Virtually all Walden staff preparation
programs created by Todd Risley (and the Living consists of hands-on coaching using zone check-
Environment Group at the University of Kansas). lists, which are task analyses of how a teacher
With inclusion, there comes an obligation to pro- needs to interface between a child and the zone
vide typical peers an enjoyable and enriched environment in a manner that ensures child
early childhood education, and the Kansas mod- engagement and IT.
els offered considerable evidence that IT class- Space is arranged so that a lead teacher can
rooms met that need (Doke & Risley, 1972; constantly oversee the safety of every child, as
LeLaurin & Risley, 1972; Twardosz et al., 1974). well as provide ongoing assistance and feedback
Detailed manuals were available to describe how to teachers. The lead teacher also acts as the
to arrange the furniture, toys, activities, and classroom conductor by facilitating children’s
teachers so that IT can take place continually transitions across zones (e.g., eliciting a verbal
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 193

request from a child who wishes to move across Research on Reinforcer Potency to Empower
zones or marketing an open activity zone to pre- Learning  Systematic assessment of reinforcer
vent overcrowding in other areas). potency is essential when using IT with children
Walden has been visited by many behavioral with autism because (this bears repeating) a key
colleagues, and (especially during the early procedural variable is that children’s interests
years) our visitors were often shocked by the lack serve as the teaching materials as well as the rein-
of pristine and distraction-free learning condi- forcers for correct responses to teaching prompts.
tions that are standard in many behavioral pro- One of the first studies conducted at Walden was
grams for children with autism. Rather, on the conceptualized as a message piece, “reinforce-
surface, Walden appears to operate more like a ment works” … [but] … “carefully selected rein-
three- or four-ring circus. All children move forcement works best” (Mason et  al., 1989,
about as they please, amid the hustle and bustle p.  179). Conditions were compared in which
of ever-changing distractions that are normally teachers of three children with autism selected
present in the real world. To paraphrase Alice in the items to be used as reinforcers for correct
Wonderland, “Everything was not as it should responding during 1:1 sessions (Mason et  al.,
be” … but we propose … “everything was as it 1989). Baseline consisted of a Teacher Selection
could be.” condition, in which experienced teachers were
asked to identify what toys a child liked the most.
During a Child Selection condition, a package
11.6.2 Programmatic Adaptations combined systematic assessment of the chil-
of Kansas Models dren’s preferred sensory stimuli (adapted from
Pace et al., 1985) with a “mini-assessment” con-
Walden did, however, become more organized ducted immediately prior to each teaching ses-
and systematically planned over the years as sev- sion; the child selected a desired toy from a
eral adaptations in the Kansas models were made basket of toys that featured their preferred sen-
to better meet the needs of children with autism. sory qualities (e.g., auditory, gustatory, olfactory,
Thus, several brief (i.e., 15 min), one-to-one (1:1) tactile, thermal, vestibular, and/or visual). Not
IT sessions were added to teach children with only did the systematic assessment of toys used
autism the language and social skills that are dif- in the Child Selection condition increase correct
ficult to blend into everyday classroom activities responding, but the package also virtually elimi-
(e.g., speech shaping, pronoun reversals). In nated off-task behavior and an array of maladap-
addition, overall classroom goals and individual- tive behaviors (e.g., eye-poking, waving arms in
ized objectives for children with autism are the air) during teaching sessions. The sensory
embedded into zones in which the environment preference assessment is now conducted monthly
best supports instruction of various skills. Cue at Walden with every child with autism until they
cards are posted in zones to remind busy teachers reach nearly normal levels of engagement and
of the content of both classwide goals and indi- expressive verbal language and have neither
vidualized objectives (see example in Table 11.2). behavioral nor learning difficulties. Information
on children’s sensory preferences, which change
and usually expand across time in treatment, are
then incorporated into classroom materials and
Table 11.2  Snack zone cue card posted on wall facing used in 1:1 IT sessions.
teacher
Snack goal: requests Child
Contingent vocalizations RS Environmental Engineering of the Free-­
1 – Word BP  JG Play Area  The most challenging zone to man-
3 words AK  SI  TD age is free-play, where the teacher must
Complete sentence w/ please Everyone else continuously circulate among children with
194 G. G. McGee

wide-ranging abilities and interests. Specifically, Table 11.3  Toy rotations that support IT of animal or
the free-play teacher preparation checklist vehicle vocabulary
requires at least 13 contacts with individual chil- Toy rotation set 1 Toy rotation set 2
dren within a 5 min period, during which the Farm play set w/ barn and Jungle animals train and
tractor tracks
teacher (a) praises toy engagement, (b) com-
Farm animals that make Pop up pets
ments about play activities, and (c) provides five noises
IT opportunities (at least three of which secure Farm animal puppets Dress me teddy bear
successful responses). A set of practical proce- Llama jack-in-the-box Fire truck dressing frame
dures were developed to ensure that the class- Rubber wagon Pull cart w/ colorful
room environment contains play materials that alphabet blocks
evoke children’s interests, initiations, and Pound a ball car ramps Vehicle puzzle
interactions. Numbers block train Push/go cars
Smooth/sanded wood Bristle blocks
latches board
First, hobby boxes were created to make it Kaleidoscope Dimple sensory toy
easier to keep children with autism productively Scented Coco doll Xylophone
engaged with toys (McGee, Daly et  al., 1991). CAT Tough Truck Wooden Train Toy Set
Five toys are selected according to the Premack (plastic)
principle (i.e., toys that a child is observed to play FP Farm Set Flashing Word Computer
Toy
with for relatively sustained amounts of time)
and stored in small baskets on teacher-height
shelves. Children can see, but not access, their formal study evaluating environmental engineer-
individual hobby box until they gesture or ver- ing in the free-play zone (i.e., McGee & Daly,
bally request it from a teacher. Neurotypical chil- 1999). Dependent variables were children’s ini-
dren eventually asked to have their own hobby tiations to teachers and both positive peer interac-
boxes, and their requests were easily accommo- tions (e.g., sharing, cooperative play, interactive
dated by a less systematic practice of holding a play) and negative peer-related behaviors (e.g.,
bi-weekly school store in which individual toys tattling, sharing disputes, sad/angry facial affect,
are picked from an array of choices (e.g., small and verbal or physical hostility). Certain play
boxes of crayons, party favors). materials remained available in the free-play
A toy rotation system was also developed to zone throughout all conditions (i.e., a cozy book
ensure the novelty of toys that are freely available area, housekeeping area, and large blocks). Three
on toy display shelves. Toy rotation sets of ten strategies for selecting free-play toys were com-
toys were put together in such a way that included pared using an ABCBC single-subject design. In
“something for everyone.” That is, toys that pro- Condition A (Systematic Toy Selection), hobby
vide opportunities to teach vocabulary associated boxes were on teacher-height shelves, and two
with weekly “themes of the week,” toys that toy rotation sets were rotated weekly to provide
match the play skills of children at different 20 toys that were freely available on child-size
developmental levels, toys that feature various shelves (e.g., toys from set 1 and set 2 were on
preferred sensory stimuli, toys of an array of display on toy shelves for a week, at which time
sizes, and a limited number of toys that are com- the set 1 toys were returned into storage, set 2
posed of many small pieces (i.e., to prevent toys remained in free-play, and set 3 toys were
teachers from having to constantly clean rather introduced to the classroom). Condition B
than teach). At any given time, two rotation sets (Enhanced Toy Selection) was identical to
of 10 manipulative toys (i.e., a total of 20 toys) Condition A, except that toy rotations occurred
are on display in the classroom (see Table  11.3 twice weekly (i.e., Wednesday and Friday morn-
for an example). ings). In Condition C (Conventional Toy
Fifteen children (i.e., eight neurotypical peers Selection), toys were selected based on input
and seven children with autism) participated in a from 20 experienced teachers who worked at
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 195

accredited preschools in the community, and all Table 11.4  IT of farm animal labels in free-play
hobby box toys were added to the child-size Materials: The classroom theme of the week is farm
shelves in free-play. Children’s initiations to animals, and a cue card is posted in free-play to
remind teachers that the week’s objective for Dan is to
teachers were high in both Systematic and learn easy animal labels (i.e., cow, chicken, and pig).
Enhanced Toy Selection conditions compared to Activity: Dan is playing with a toy barn with tractor
relatively low levels of child initiations during and farmer. The teacher grabs a basket of noise-­
the Conventional Toy Selection conditions. making farm animals from the toy display shelf; they
Conversely, negative peer-related behaviors were approach Dan by bending down and lowering the
basket so Dan can see the animals, and wait:
substantially lower during Systematic and IT:
Enhanced Toy Selection conditions compared to Dan points to the cow. Teacher pulls a
Conventional conditions. Positive peer interac- cow out of the
tions were substantially higher during the basket and asks,
biweekly Enhanced Toy Selection conditions “Which animal is
this?”
than in either the Systematic or Conventional
Dan says, “Moo!” Teacher says,
conditions. Identical results were found when a “Cows say moo.
group design was used to compare the statistical Do you want a
significance of differences in the three conditions cow?”
(Morrier et al., 2009). Dan says, “Want cow.” Teacher cheers,
“Terrific! Here’s
The toy rotation system takes time to initially your cow!”
sort classroom toys into approximately 12 toy Teacher circulates among other children to briefly
rotation sets, which are stored in plastic tubs. All praise or comment on their play and then returns to
toys, tubs, and hobby boxes are labeled w/ indel- Dan. Teacher stoops down to where Dan is playing,
pulls out another cow, and waits for him to initiate:
ible ink to make it easy to maintain the system.
Once established, teachers need not be specially
prepared to arrange the environment in creative versus parenting time, keeping a child with
ways, and children are guaranteed a continual autism engaged while helping a sibling with
choice of toys that seem new and interesting. In homework). This array of options ensures that
short, the environmental arrangements in families are helped to address the issues that are
Walden’s classrooms have been systematically individually valued and likely to make a differ-
planned to support teachers with ready opportu- ence in the context of their family’s needs.
nities to teach the skills that children need to Table 11.5 provides an example of how a parent
learn (as described in Table 11.4). may blend IT into one of their family’s everyday
home activities.
Preparing Parents of Children with Autism to
Do IT  Another adaptation was needed to address There is empirical support for ensuring that
the needs of parents of children with autism. goals are individually selected by the family and
Although the exact format changed across the that developmental progress occurs for the child
years, the most intensive preparation of parents in this format. Evaluation of an early version of
of children with autism occurs weekly during the the parent program (i.e., McGee, Jacobs et  al.,
first year of a child’s enrollment. Parents and 1993) found that children progressed an average
other family members are provided with a wide of 1.6  months on the Vineland for each of
range of options from which to choose. 9 months in which parents participated. Analyses
Specifically, they are introduced to a menu of 50 of videotapes of the children during regular fam-
modules that address (a) language (e.g., during ily activities revealed that children made the most
meals, bath time, outdoor play), (b) social skills progress in skills that their parents had selected to
(e.g., games with siblings, how to host a play teach at home rather than in skills that parents did
date), (c) daily living skills, and (d) other family not choose.
issues (e.g., grandparents, balancing personal
196 G. G. McGee

Table 11.5  IT of action words while viewing home ers. We ensured the external validity of target
movie
phrases by surveying the opinions of 15 teachers
Materials: Because Sue is fascinated by Star Wars in other community preschools to determine
movies on DVD, the remote control has become a
powerful teaching tool.
what cute or funny phrases their children often
Activity: Dad decides in advance what action words said (McGee & Daly, 2007). The top two answers
he wants Sue to practice (ideally there will be multiple were “All right.” and “You know what?” Teaching
examples of the same action). Dad lets Sue watch and children with autism, who at the time had mild to
get interested in the movie, and he comments moderate language delays, to comment was an
occasionally about what the characters are doing.
When there is a clear-cut action, Dad uses the remote anticipated challenge; teaching them to solicit
to pause the movie. social attention was an even bigger challenge. IT
IT: took place during a small group tabletop activity
Sue says, “Turn on the movie.” Dad asks a attended by typical peers (McGee & Daly, 2007).
question that Systematic fading procedures were used to trans-
requires an action
word in the fer stimulus control from use of the phrases as
answer (“What is mands to use of the phrases as comments and
Luke Skywalker queries in everyday social situations (i.e., most-­
doing?”). to-­
least prompting was introduced across five
Sue: “Luke is jumping.” Dad responds,
phases). By the final phase, the teacher used no
“Correct! Let’s
see what happens verbal prompts, and the children had access to
after Luke their preferred toys. A multiple baseline across
jumps.” He three children with autism showed that all chil-
quickly resumes
dren acquired appropriate use of the targeted
the movie.
social phrases during teaching sessions, and they
maintained use of the phrases during unprompted
IT of Conversational Language at conditions at the table and with the same teacher
Walden  Expressive verbal speech shaping during free-play. Two of the three boys used the
begins a month after entry to Walden by teaching phrases with a free-play teacher who was unin-
a nonverbal child to use discrete sounds (to mean formed about the study. Fidelity of procedural
“I want anything”). The next targets are word implementation was assessed throughout all con-
approximations and discriminations among ten ditions via verbatim transcription of videotaped
desired items (i.e., nouns beginning with differ- sessions, and children’s use of target phrases was
ent developmentally early consonants). removed from the data when few inadvertent
Experience suggests that any child who learns at prompting errors occurred (primarily when the
least ten discriminated words will continue to uninformed teacher used a target phrase). The
expand their language. A developmentally sensi- child with the mildest language delay showed the
tive sequence of IT language objectives has been most flexible use of the target phrases (“All right
established to continue language elaborations by everybody, it’s time to go.” or “Know what I
adding descriptors, action words, etc. until the have?”), but all three boys continued to use the
child’s vocabulary is diverse enough to directly phrases in varied situations (including at home
target a variety of conversational skills. for at least 6 months, according to parent report
about two of the boys). Walden now targets one
Children with autism need social language to “child-culture” phrase or gesture per week during
interact in age-appropriate ways with their neuro- lunch, during which all children are encouraged
typical peers; however, teaching children with to practice and laugh at one another while build-
autism to use conversational language is far more ing behavioral momentum (e.g., “Why did the
complicated than teaching functional language. chicken cross the road?” All funny answers
The first step is to identify colloquial phrases that accepted).
are commonly used by neurotypical preschool-
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 197

From the outset, IT proved to be powerful in children’s social interactions throughout the day.
teaching children with autism to talk (McGee When Walden relocated to Emory University in
et al., 2001). At the time the first 34 children with Atlanta in 1992, the social intervention curricu-
autism entered Walden, 20 (59%) were com- lum was enriched by targeting peer-related social
pletely nonverbal, 13 (38%) had random echola- objectives during approximately half of the pre-
lia (1-word syllables that were usually spoken out school day. We also changed the ratio of neuro-
of context), and one child used three functional typical peers to children with autism from a bare
words (they had participated in a pilot IT pro- majority to two neurotypical peers for every child
gram). By the time these children graduated with autism. More neurotypical peers provide the
18 months later, four (12%) remained nonverbal children with autism with additional models of
(although they had learned between one and three age-appropriate social behavior, ensure a larger
words or word approximations), 18 (41%) had pool of enthusiastic peer tutors, and preserve an
functional verbal language (defined as more than “early childhood” atmosphere in the classroom
10 unprompted meaningful words, with a range (i.e., neurotypical peers are more likely to stay
of mild to moderate speech delays), and 12 (35%) home when a grandparent visits or the family
were using functional verbal language through- takes a long vacation, while parents of children
out the preschool day within the ranges of the with autism tend to be more diligent about their
amount of time that their neurotypical peers spent children’s attendance in intervention). All chil-
talking. dren (with and without autism) attend large group
social interaction games and peer imitation exer-
Pack the Day with Social Intervention  A cise sessions during a 15 min period of recess
descriptive study of how children with autism (Morrier & Ziegler, 2018), and each child with
spent their time at Walden (i.e., McGee, Paradis autism is invited to participate with two neuro-
et  al., 1993) found significantly lower levels of typical peers in a 15 min dramatic play session
“autistic behavior” (e.g., repetitive body move- (e.g., acting out a camping trip, flying in a space
ments or motions with objects that serve no shuttle; modified from Odom & Strain, 1986).
apparent play function) when children with Two small group social sensory games are also
autism were within 3 ft. of a neurotypical child as offered every day (see Table  11.6 for an
compared to when they were alone or near other example).
children with autism. These results were trans- By the age of three, each child with autism
lated to everyday practice at Walden by routinely also attends at least one daily peer incidental ses-
ensuring that a child with autism sits next to a sion with a neurotypical peer. The peer tutor’s
neurotypical peer at tabletop activities such as adult coach provides the peer tutor with a basket
snack or art. Popular toys (e.g., a marble maze of their buddy’s toys and uses a simple checklist
toy that requires turn-taking) are also used to to teach steps of IT (i.e., hold up a toy and wait to
attract children with autism to play close to neu- see if your buddy wants it, ask “What do you
rotypical peers. want?”, and give them the toy when they ask for
it). Adult coaching and physical presence are
Unfortunately, reduced self-stimulatory withdrawn gradually. In a formal study of peer IT
behavior was the only “free effect” of inclusion (McGee et al., 1992), a multiple baseline across
discovered at Walden. Rather, the potential ben- three dyads of peer tutors and their buddies with
efits of inclusion with neurotypical peers required autism showed quick increases in reciprocal
a great deal of carefully planned social interven- interactions as a function of preparing the peer
tion along with lots of practice. Although the tutors. Peer interactions continued in the absence
children with autism had done well during peer of direct coaching and transferred to lunch in a
interaction sessions during which they were different part of the classroom. Also, neurotypi-
directly prompted, we were disappointed in early cal peers rated their buddies with autism as “more
results from our videotaped database that tracked likeable” on a Likert-type rating scale.
198 G. G. McGee

Table 11.6  IT of peer proximity at social sensory games Table 11.7  IT of handwriting in 1:1 session
Materials: Wooden rocking boat w/ space for 4 Materials: Large Barbie house; small table nearby
children to sit packed closely together. Boat is located holds an easily grasped pencil and paper available for
near light switch on wall, and teacher has a cup of child to write the word wolf or pig. Handwriting task
water. is kept as easy as possible by providing words to trace,
Activity: Teacher leads a dramatic pretend game about or dot-to-dots of the words, and eventually a blank
a boat in a storm, which provides sensory stimuli for paper.
children who like auditory, tactile, vestibular, and Activity: Teacher pre-teaches child in steps of Three
visual stimuli (and/or a fun game). Two children with Little Pigs game.
autism are invited to ride in the “boat storm,” along  1. Pig hides behind house.
with the first two neurotypical peers to arrive.  2. Wolf knocks on door and says, ”Little Pig, Little
IT: Pig, let me come in.”
Teacher rocks boat  3. Pig says, “Not by the hair of my chinny chin,
vigorously while chin.”
announcing a big storm  4. Wolf growls, “Then I’ll huff, and I’ll puff, and I’ll
is coming. Rain drops blow your house down.” Wolf then shakes the
are sprinkled from the house.
cup onto the boaters,  5. Pig runs away squealing and laughing as Wolf
lightning flashes as light chases him. After coaching the game, the teacher
switch is flipped, and prepares to do IT of handwriting.
teacher makes thunder IT:
noises. Child with autism asks to play Teacher asks, “Do
All children laugh. Teacher falls on floor, the game. you want to be a
kicking feet, and roars wolf or a pig?”;
like a pretend shark. they show child
All children squeal. Teacher asks children to where to write the
take turns if new children word wolf or pig.
have arrived or provides Child writes the word for the Teacher says, “Yay,
choice of rocking role they want to play (i.e., wolf you wrote wolf, so
through another or pig). I’ll be the pig.”
incoming storm. Game proceeds
with lots of
laughter.
A pre-kindergarten classroom was added a
few years after the program moved to Atlanta, to re-location), 26 (79%) were fully included in
because children with and without autism need to regular kindergarten classrooms (with varying
learn the conventional classroom behaviors that levels of support, but most often with little or no
ensure success in kindergarten. Across the last support). By 2014, our program evaluation data
year at Walden, children are gradually taught to recorded that 92% of Walden graduates with
wait in a line to go to the playground, to raise autism were successfully included in regular kin-
hands before answering questions at story, and to dergartens (McGee et al., 2020).
cooperate during large group activities.
Independent social interactions and academic Everything Works Better When You Begin IT
readiness are also strongly emphasized in the with Toddlers  In 1994, Walden opened the first
pre-K (see Table 11.7 for an example of how IT inclusive toddler center in Atlanta. The initial
may be used to teach kindergarten readiness goal for toddlers with autism is addressed during
skills). the first month of entry. Each child with autism is
Enrichment of the social intervention curricu- taught to orient toward (and not avoid) teachers
lum produced the desired social gains (i.e., by pairing teacher approach with quick delivery
increases in the amount of time that neurotypical of a preferred and consumable piece of food or
peers gave unprompted social bids to the children sensory stimulus (e.g., flickering light). When the
with autism and vice versa) (McGee et al., 2001). child begins to look at a desired item in the teach-
Of the first 33 children with autism who gradu- er’s hand, on successive approaches the teacher
ated from Walden (one was lost to follow-up due gradually raises the item toward their eyes. This
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 199

procedure, which has been tested in pilot research interaction partners until approximately 3  years
and practice, almost always stabilizes engage- of age. Our youngest children with autism are
ment at higher levels than children presented taught to tolerate proximity to peers, to play in
with at entry, and some children develop eye the same or similar activity as peers, to watch
­contact with teachers. If a child’s eye contact other children, and to imitate the gestures of other
remains inconsistent after the orienting proce- children.
dure, then another pilot-tested procedure blends Progress of the first 28 children with autism
IT with time delay in a 1:1 session (i.e., it is eas- who entered the first Toddler Model was evalu-
ier to precisely time prompt fading adjustments ated through an analysis of Walden’s videotaped
during 1:1 sessions than during group activities). database that randomly sampled time segments
When a child looks at and initiates for a desired throughout the day (McGee et al.,1999). At entry,
toy, the teacher raises the toy to their eye level, the amount of time that children with autism
and the child tracks the toy upward toward the were talking was 35% of the day (including echo-
teacher’s eyes. On subsequent IT episodes, the lalia and perseverative speech). After 6–12 months
amount of time between the child’s initiation and in the toddler center, 82% of the children with
the tracking prompt increases (in 1-sec incre- autism were verbalizing meaningful words (func-
ments) until the child eventually looks at the tional expressive language). By the time of grad-
teacher when initiating for the toy (i.e., stimulus uation to preschool, 96% of the children with
control of eye contact transfers from the toy to autism spent their time in close physical proxim-
initiations to the teacher). An advantage of these ity to typical peers. A decade later, improvements
approaches to establishing eye contact is that arti- in the age at which autism can be diagnosed and
ficial prompts (i.e., “look at me” or manipulation greater space availability made it possible for
of a child’s chin) are unnecessary. Walden to open a new younger toddler classroom
(and the first toddler program became the “Early
Daily living skills are taught at the same ages Preschool”).
that neurotypical children usually learn them, and
an abundant ABA literature supports this goal
(e.g., toilet training procedures were modified 11.6.3 Summary of Program
slightly to remove overcorrection, so that bladder Replications of Walden’s IT
control conditioning usually takes approximately Model
a week at Walden [Azrin, & Foxx, 1971; Azrin &
Foxx, 1981]). Daily living skills such as hand- University-based lab schools are almost a thing
washing are task analyzed and taught with back- of the past, but they offer certain advantages such
ward chaining; the process is transformed into an as (a) capacity for conducting comprehensive
IT format by teaching at times when children are intervention research, (b) opportunities for prep-
eager to get to a highly desired activity (e.g., chil- aration of large numbers of young professionals,
dren must change from classroom slippers into and (c) the potential to achieve widespread com-
shoes to go to the playground, and they get out- munity impact via program replications (McGee
side most quickly when they become indepen- et al., 2020). It was described above how internal
dent). The Kansas Toddler Center provided an Walden replications were used to develop a con-
“Ask-Say-Do” faded guidance procedure to teach tinuum of four IT classrooms, which have served
independence to neurotypical toddlers, and 821 children (including 270 children with autism
Walden modified the procedure to add an oppor- and 551 neurotypical peers). The hour intensity
tunity for independence and a gestural prompt has varied across time, but 35  hours per week
(i.e., “Wait, Ask, Say, Show, Do” or WASSD). (plus daycare and home intervention) are now
The major adaptation of Walden Preschool for provided across 12 months per year, up to 4 years.
toddlers is in teaching peer-related social goals, The biggest change in outcomes for children with
because typical peers are not fully competent autism has been accomplished by lowering the
200 G. G. McGee

age of entry into intervention (children with dency). Moreover, there were numerous observa-
autism were an average of 44  months of age in tions that both children with autism and
the first preschool at UMass, they were an aver- neurotypical children seemed to enjoy IT.
age of 29 months when the first toddler program Especially heartening is the renewed focus on
opened at Emory, and the newest toddler program how to prepare personnel to do IT. We previously
accepts children as young as 12  months (and conducted a review of approximately 100 studies
walking). In sum, Walden provides a very hour-­ in which behavioral researchers prepared provid-
intensive IT intervention that emphasizes engage- ers to either implement various ABA interven-
ment in language and social interactions. tions or to simply do their jobs better (McGee &
Replications of Walden’s IT model have been Morrier, 2005). Many were large-scale efforts
established in external programs located in eight aimed at improving the quality of life for benefi-
states across the USA, and week-long IT speech ciaries of the providers, while others addressed
shaping (and provider preparation) clinics have motivational systems that improve and maintain
served children from five continents. One of the provider performance (cf. Greene et  al., 1978;
first external replications of Walden’s toddler Iwata et al., 1976). Most studies (including those
model classroom was accomplished by research- reviewed earlier and those reported in this review)
ers at Children’s Hospital in San Diego, have found that some form of behavior-specific
California, who wished to get their own “lab performance feedback system is essential to
school” open quickly to accommodate their obtaining lasting positive change in provider
research programs. This program mixed IT with behavior.
other naturalistic interventions and traditional Many of these IT provider preparation studies
behavioral interventions, and they reported simi- were impressive in their attention to current
lar outcomes to those of the children at Walden essential features of ABA single-subject research
(Akshoomoff et al., 2010; Stahmer & Ingersoll, methodology (e.g., specifying dependent vari-
2004). The longest continuously operating exter- ables for both providers and their beneficiaries,
nal replication (19 years) is a full Walden replica- measuring fidelity of implementation and social
tion in North Dallas, Texas. Two years ago, this validity). Various strategies for provider prepara-
program added a classroom for infants at risk for tion yield repeated findings that providers
autism and neurotypical peers. increase their use of IT, and increased IT by pro-
viders yields concurrent increases in the fre-
quency with which learners initiate and respond
11.7 Conclusions to their providers.
The remote telehealth technologies are not
A definite strength of the IT studies reviewed only timely during the age of learning to work
above is in the collective demonstration that IT from home, but these studies clearly offer the
yields positive benefits for many dependent pop- potential to prepare increased numbers of provid-
ulations, of wide-ranging ages, and when applied ers to serve children who live in geographic
via diverse groups of providers. Thus, there are regions that do not have access to specialized
repeated findings that IT is effective in teaching a autism interventions. It may be useful to know
large variety of skills that maintain after instruc- whether these technologies are best applied to
tion has ended, and most learners show general- certain skills, while other skills might be better
ization to use of new skills in situations (i.e., prepared via hands-on coaching and/or a combi-
people and settings) that were not associated with nation of remote and live performance feedback.
initial IT. The literature reviewed also repeatedly Based on my experience in training dozens of
reported beneficial side effects of IT that directly college students to do IT, I suggest that incidental
address learning challenges associated with teachers will do best if you first have a conversa-
autism (e.g., decreased behavioral difficulties, tion with them about their big picture to deter-
less social avoidance, reduced prompt depen- mine how your preparation may be relevant to
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 201

their interests. Whenever possible, assign them to were usually interacting with teachers or other
a teaching routine that blends with their interests children. We once had a Walden child with autism
(e.g., a college student athlete may be most graduate to regular kindergarten, where they got
enthusiastic about designing fun outdoor running into difficulty by following their neurotypical
games, while an aspiring author of children’s peers on one last slide before lining up to go
books might enjoy developing a checklist that inside (we secretly cheered); however, the princi-
prepares other teachers to be great story readers). pal was not amused when they entered the office
When you are preparing an experienced behavior for discipline with a broad grin on their face. In
analyst who is accustomed to DTT, advise them short, these and other population differences in
to be patient because the timing of “waiting” for social behavior might be usefully explored as
a learner’s initiation may initially seem uncom- progress indicators across time in early interven-
fortable. Assure them that all their good ABA tion (McGee et al., 1997).
skills will kick back in and be useful once they There is also a need for more intervention
have mastered a new prompting sequence. research on the content of peer-related social
communication in the highest-achieving 4- to
5-year-old children with autism, because they
11.7.1 Suggestions for Future will likely receive little if any systematic social
Research in Early Autism intervention after entering regular kindergartens
Intervention (e.g., “I like to play with super-heroes. What are
your favorite toys?”). One of the important social
Having already suggested potential research competencies taught at Walden is the same rule
directions in preparing IT providers, we turn now most of us use: “When in doubt in a social situa-
to suggestions for research that may advance tion, do what everyone else is doing” (Okay, that
early autism intervention. There are large litera- wouldn’t have helped our graduate on the play-
tures on developmental milestones for young ground, but it may have worked if we had taught
neurotypical children and on the characteristic them to look sheepish when appropriate to do
differences presented by young children with so).
autism; however, the body of intervention
research that addresses the social irregularities of
the very youngest toddlers with autism is rela- 11.7.2 IT Is an Evidence-Based
tively sparce. It may also be useful to examine Practice
systematic ways in which to shape babbling into
discrete sounds or to discover how to promote IT procedures have been primarily developed and
babbling in children who seldom vocalize. evaluated using single-subject research designs.
By the age of preschool, we found that chil- To use a current analogy during the days of a pan-
dren with autism are delayed in their ability to demic, there are some researchers who develop
understand even basic emotional expressions of vaccines, others who compare vaccinations in
others (Feldman et  al., 1993). Although some randomized controlled trials, and still others who
clever preschool teachers address the issue of figure out how to distribute vaccines. ABA
affective decoding, the few intervention studies researchers are analogous to the intervention
in this area have been conducted with school-­ developers.
aged children. A descriptive analysis also found One set of standards (Horner et al., 2005) that
subtle differences in the emotional facial displays may be used to qualify a body of ABA research
of children with autism and their neurotypical as an evidence-based practice include the
peers (McGee, Feldman et  al., 1991). Early in following:
treatment, when 3-year-old children with autism
looked happy, they were usually playing alone, 1. A minimum of five single-subject studies that
but when neurotypical peers looked happy, they meet minimally acceptable methodological
202 G. G. McGee

criteria and document experimental control rated language by waiting for another person to
and have been published in peer-reviewed initiate conversation about a topic and then
journals. responding in ways that ask for more information
2. The research should have been conducted by from that person” (Hart & Risley, 1982, p.5). The
at least three research groups who are in at goal of IT is to provide learners (whether they be
least three different geographical locations. children, parents, teachers, or persons with dis-
3. The peer-reviewed studies should have had at abilities of any age) with interesting opportunities
least 20 participants. Cumulatively, the IT to learn needed skills.
studies presented in this literature review far Specific to early intervention for children with
exceed these criteria. autism, key variables that contribute to successful
outcomes are (a) the age at which intervention
IT procedures have been designated as an begins, (b) the content of social communication
“established” and/or “evidence-based” practice between a child and their caregivers, and (c) the
by associations and research groups using objec- amount of time a child spends engaged in lan-
tive criteria and known standards (cf. National guage and social interactions within increasingly
Autism Center, 2015; Wong et al., 2015). IT pro- complex environmental contexts. IT procedures
cedures are also considered one of the empiri- are highly compatible with efforts to influence
cally validated early autism interventions now each of these variables. A comprehensive IT
known as Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral approach addresses the rights of children with
Interventions, which include approaches devel- autism to receive early intervention that is suffi-
oped from the formerly very divergent concep- ciently intensive to provide them with skills that
tual frameworks of developmental psychology will improve their future lives. At the same time,
and behavior analysis (Schreibman et al., 2015). a comprehensive IT approach addresses the rights
Finally, Walden was presented as one of ten of children with autism to enjoy the only early
evidence-­ based model programs for children childhood that they will ever have.
with autism after review by an interdisciplinary
team of experts (National Research Council, Conflict of Interest  There is no known conflict of inter-
2001). Certification that Walden provides “best est to report.
practices” established for the education of all
children is confirmed in repeated accreditation
evaluations conducted by the National References
Association for Education of Young Children
Akshoomoff, N., Stahmer, A. C., Corsello, C., & Mahrer,
(NAEYC). N.  E. (2010). What happens next? Follow-up from
the Children’s Toddler School program. Journal of
Positive Behavioral Interventions, 12(4), 245–253.
11.7.3 Enjoy Your Interesting IT https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300709343724
Ala’i-Rosales, S. A., Toussaint, K. A., & McGee, G. G.
Conversations (2017). Incidental teaching: Happy progress. In J. B.
Leaf (Ed.), Handbook of social skills and autism
Spending most of my career at Walden has been a spectrum disorders: Assessment, curricula, and
fascinating adventure, which was made more intervention (Autism and psychopathology series)
(pp.  171–185). Springer International Publishing.
interesting by the generous time and advice pro- https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­62995-­7_11
vided by Todd Risley. Perhaps the most important Allen, K.  E., & Hart, B.  M. (1984). The early years:
aspect of “incidental,” although not accidental, Arrangements for learning. Prentice-Hall.
teaching is that the process is an interaction Azrin, N.  H., & Foxx, R.  M. (1971). Toilet training the
retarded: A rapid method of toilet training the insti-
between two people that ends in an event desired tutionally retarded. Journal of Applied Behavior
by the learner and the teacher. Todd Risley loved Analysis, 2, 89–99.
to talk about the social dance between a child and Azrin, N. H., & Foxx, R. M. (1981). Toilet training in less
their interaction partner. “IT is used to get elabo- than a day. Pocket Books.
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 203

Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some Analysis, 27(3), 533–544. https://doi.org/10.1901/
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. jaba.1994.27-­533
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 91–97. Feldman, R.  S., McGee, G.  G., Mann, L., & Strain, P.
Brown, W.  H., McEvoy, M.  A., & Bishop, N. (1991). (1993). Nonverbal affective decoding ability in chil-
Incidental teaching of social behavior: A naturalistic dren with autism and in typical preschoolers. Journal
approach for promoting young children's peer inter- of Early Intervention, 17(4), 341–350. https://doi.
actions. Teaching Exceptional Children, 24(1), 35–38. org/10.1177/105381519301700401
https://doi.org/10.1177/004005999102400109 Greene, B.  F., Willis, B.  S., Levy, R., & Bailey, J.  S.
Carr, E. G., & Kologinsky, E. (1983). Acquisition of sign (1978). Measuring client gains from staff implemented
language by autistic children. II: Spontaneity and programs. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11,
generalization effects. Journal of Applied Behavior 395–412.
Analysis, 16(3), 297–314. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Halle, J.  W., Baer, D.  M., & Spradlin, J.  E. (1981).
jaba.1983.16-­297 Teachers' generalized use of delay as a stimulus con-
Casey, A.  M., & McWilliam, R.  A. (2008). Graphical trol procedure to increase language use in handicapped
feedback to increase teachers’ use of incidental teach- children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14(4),
ing. Journal of Early Intervention, 30(3), 251–268. 389–409. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1981.14-­389
https://doi.org/10.1177/1053815108319038 Haring, T.  G., & Breen, C.  G. (1992). A peer-mediated
Casey, A.  M., McWilliam, R.  A., & Sims, J. (2012). social network intervention to enhance the social
Contributions of incidental teaching, developmental integration of persons with moderate and severe dis-
quotient, and peer interactions to child engagement. abilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(2),
Infants & Young Children, 25(2), 122–135. https://doi. 319–333. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­333
org/10.1097/IYC.0b013e31824cbac4 Haring, T.  G., Neetz, J.  A., Lovinger, L., Peck, C., &
Cataldo, M. F., & Risley, T. R. (1974). Evaluation of liv- Semmel, M. I. (1987). Effects of four modified inci-
ing environments: The MANIFEST description of dental teaching procedures to create opportunities
ward activities. In P. O. Davison, E. W. Clark, & C. A. for communication. The Journal of the Association
Hammerlynck (Eds.), Evaluation of behavioral pro- for Persons With Severe Handicaps, 12(3), 218–226.
grams in community, residential, and school settings https://doi.org/10.1177/154079698701200308
(pp. 201–222). Research Press. Hart, B.  M., & Risley, T.  R. (1968). Establishing use
Delprato, D.  J. (2001). Comparisons of discrete trial of descriptive adjectives in the spontaneous speech
and normalized behavioral language intervention for of disadvantaged preschool children. Journal of
young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(2), 109–120. https://doi.
Developmental Disorders, 31, 315–325. https://doi. org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­109
org/10.1023/A:101747303957 Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1974). Using preschool materi-
Doke, L.  A., & Risley, T.  R. (1972). The organization als to modify the language of disadvantaged children.
of day-care environments: Required versus optional Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7(2), 243–256.
activities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5(4), https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1974.7-­243
405–420. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1972.5-­405 Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1975). Incidental teaching of lan-
Dolley, D., & Whendell, K. (1987). Talking to teacher – guage in the preschool. Journal of Applied Behavior
Using incidental teaching to encourage child initia- Analysis, 8(4), 411–420. https://doi.org/10.1901/
tions in the nursery classroom: A case study. Child jaba.1975.8-­411
Language Teaching and Therapy, 3(3), 277–292. Hart, B., & Risley, T.  R. (1978). Promoting productive
https://doi.org/10.1177/026565908700300305 language through incidental teaching. Education
Dolley, D., & Whendell, K. (1988). Developing func- and Urban Society, 10(4), 407–429. https://doi.
tional language with young children from English-­ org/10.1177/001312457801000402
speaking and Punjabi-speaking home backgrounds: Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1980). In vivo language inter-
Incidental teaching and contingent access to materials. vention: Unanticipated general effects. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 8(1–2), 101–116. https://doi. Applied Behavior Analysis, 13(3), 407–432. https://
org/10.1080/0144341880080109 doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1980.13-­407
Dunst, C.  J., McWilliam, R.  A., & Holbert, K. (1986). Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. (1982). How to use incidental
Assessment of preschool classroom environ- teaching for elaborating language. H & H Enterprises.
ments. Diagnostique, 11(3–4), 212–232. https://doi. Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in
org/10.1177/073724778601100306 the everyday experience of young American children.
Fabry, B.  D., Mayhew, G.  L., & Hanson, A. (1984). Brookes .
Incidental teaching of mentally retarded students Hart, B.  M., & Rogers-Warren, A.  K. (1978). A milieu
within a token system. American Journal of Mental approach to teaching language. In R. L. Schiefelbusch
Deficiency, 89(1), 29–36. (Ed.), Language intervention strategies (Vol. 2,
Farmer-Dougan, V. (1994). Increasing requests by pp. 192–235). University Park Press.
adults with developmental disabilities using inciden- Herbert-Jackson, E., O’Brien, M., Porterfield, J., & Risley,
tal teaching by peers. Journal of Applied Behavior T. R. (1977). The infant center: A complete guide to
204 G. G. McGee

organizing and managing infant day care. University Psychology, 77(3), 211–222. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Park Press. h0031015
Herbert-Jackson, E., & Risley, T.  R. (1977). Behavioral MacDuff, G.  S., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E.
nutrition: Consumption of foods of the future by (1988). Providing incidental teaching for autistic
toddlers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, children: A rapid training procedure for therapists.
407–413. Education and Treatment of Children, 11(3), 205–217.
Horasan, M. M., & Birkan, B. (2015). The effects of inci- Mason, S.  A., McGee, G.  G., Farmer-Dougan, V., &
dental teaching on teaching children with autism spec- Risley, T. R. (1989). A practical strategy for ongoing
trum disorders to demand their lost objects verbally. reinforcer assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior
International Journal of Early Childhood Special Analysis, 22(2), 171–179. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Education, 7(2), 361–383. https://doi.org/10.20489/ jaba.1989.22-­171
intjecse.65771381.361 McClannahan, L.  E., & Risley, T.  R. (1975). Design of
Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, living environments for nursing-home residents:
S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject Increasing participation in recreation activities.
research to identify evidence-based practice in spe- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8(3), 261–268.
cial education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 165–179. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1975.8-­261
https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203 McGee, G. G. (2005). Incidental teaching. In M. Hersen,
Hsieh, H.  H., Wilder, D.  A., & Abellon, O.  E. (2011). G.  Sugai, & R.  H. Horner (Eds.), Encyclopedia of
The effects of training on caregiver implementation behavior modification and cognitive behavior therapy:
of incidental teaching. Journal of Applied Behavior Educational applications (Vol. 3, pp.  1359–1362).
Analysis, 44(1), 199–203. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Sage Publications.
jaba.2011.44-­199 McGee, G.  G., Almeida, M.  C., Sulzer-Azaroff, B., &
Iwata, B.  A., Bailey, J.  S., Brown, K.  M., Foshee, T.  J., Feldman, R. S. (1992). Promoting reciprocal interac-
& Alpern, M. (1976). A performance-based lottery tions via peer incidental teaching. Journal of Applied
to improve residential care and training by institu- Behavior Analysis, 25(1), 117–126. https://doi.
tional staff. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­117
417–431. McGee, G.  G., & Daly, T. (1999). Prevention of prob-
Krantz, P. J., & Risley, T. R. (1977). Behavioral ecology lem behaviors in preschool children. In A. C. Repp &
in the classroom. In K. D. O’Leary (Ed.), Classroom R.  H. Horner (Eds.), Functional analysis of problem
management: The successful use of behavior modifi- behavior: From effective assessment to effective sup-
cation (pp. 349–367). Pergamon. port (pp. 171–195). Wadsworth Publishing Company.
LeLaurin, K., & Risley, T.  R. (1972). The organization McGee, G.  G., & Daly, T. (2007). Incidental teaching
of day-care environments: "Zone" versus "man-to-­ of age-appropriate social phrases to children with
man" staff assignments. Journal of Applied Behavior autism. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe
Analysis, 5(3), 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Disabilities, 32(2), 112–123. https://doi.org/10.2511/
jaba.1972.5-­225 rpsd.32.2.112
Lennox, D. B., & Brune, P. (1993). Incidental teaching for McGee, G.  G., Daly, T., Izeman, S.  G., Mann, L., &
training communication in individuals with traumatic Risley, T.  R. (1991). Use of classroom materi-
brain injury. Brain Injury, 7(5), 449–454. https://doi. als to promote preschool engagement. Teaching
org/10.3109/02699059309029688 Exceptional Children, 23(4), 44–47. https://doi.
Linne, F.  M., & Melin, L. (1992). Acquisition, general- org/10.1177/004005999102300410
ization, and spontaneous use of color adjectives: A McGee, G.  G., Feldman, R.  S., & Chernin, L. (1991).
comparison of incidental teaching and discrete-trial A comparison of emotional facial display by chil-
procedures for children with autism. Research in dren with autism and typical preschoolers. Journal
Developmental Disabilities, 13(3), 191–210. https:// of Early Intervention, 15(3), 237–245. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1016/0891-­4222(92)90025-­2 org/10.1177/105381519101500303
Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor- McGee, G. G., Feldman, R. S., & Morrier, M. J. (1997).
mal educational and intellectual functioning in Benchmarks of social treatment for children with
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi. Disorders, 27(4), 353–364. https://doi.org/10.102
org/10.1037//0022-­006x.55.1.3 3/A:1025849220209
Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S. McGee, G. G., Jacobs, H. A., & Regnier, M. C. (1993).
(1973). Some generalization and follow-up measures Preparation of families for incidental teaching and
on autistic children in behavior therapy. Journal of advocacy for their children with autism. Office of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(1), 131–165. https://doi. Special Education and Rehabilitative Services News
org/10.1901/jaba.1973.6-­131 in Print, 5, 9–13.
Lovaas, O.  I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm, McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., Mason, D., & McClannahan,
R. (1971). Selective responding by autistic chil- L.  E. (1983). A modified incidental-teaching proce-
dren to multiple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal dure for autistic youth: Acquisition and generalization
11  Incidental Teaching Research: Early Beginnings Through Recent Innovations 205

of receptive object labels. Journal of Applied Behavior Division for Early Childhood, 11(2), 124–134. https://
Analysis, 16(3), 329–338. https://doi.org/10.1901/ doi.org/10.1177/105381518701100204
jaba.1983.16-­329 National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu-
McGee, G.  G., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E. sions: National standards project phase 2. Author.
(1985). The facilitative effects of incidental teaching National Research Council (2001). In C.  Lord & J.  P.
on preposition use by autistic children. Journal of McGee (Eds.), Educating children with autism.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(1), 17–31. https://doi. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­17 https://doi.org/10.17226/10017
McGee, G.  G., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E. Neely, L., Hong, E. R., Kawamini, S., Umana, I., & Kurz,
(1986). An extension of incidental teaching procedures I. (2020). Intercontinental telehealth to train Japanese
to reading instruction for autistic children. Journal of interventionists in incidental teaching for children with
Applied Behavior Analysis, 19(2), 147–157. https:// autism. Journal of Behavioral Education, 29(2), 433–
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1986.19-­147 448. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­020-­09377-­3
McGee, G.  G., & Morrier, M.  J. (2005). Preparation of Neely, L., Rispoli, M., Boles, M., Morin, K., Gregori, E.,
autism specialists. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, Ninci, J., & Hagan-Burke, S. (2018). Interventionist
& D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and perva- acquisition of incidental teaching using pyramidal
sive developmental disorders, volume 2.: Assessment, training via telehealth. Behavior Modification, 43(5),
interventions, and policy (3rd ed., pp.  1123–1160). 711–733. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445518781770
Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939352.ch19 Neely, L., Rispoli, M., Gerow, S., & Hong, E. R. (2016).
McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Ala'i-Rosales, S. (2020). Preparing interventionists via telepractice in inci-
Contributions of university lab schools to behavior dental teaching for children with autism. Journal of
analysis. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, Behavioral Education, 25(4), 393–416. https://doi.
21(1), 74–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2019 org/10.1007/s10864-­016-­9250-­7
.1616988 O’Brien, M., Porterfield, P.  J., Herbert-Jackson, E., &
McGee, G.  G., Morrier, M.  J., & Daly, T. (1999). An Risley, T.  R. (1979). The toddler center manual: A
incidental teaching approach to early intervention practical guide to day care for one- and two-year-­
for toddlers with autism. Journal of the Association olds. University Park Press.
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24(3), 133–146. Odom, S.  L., Hoyson, M., Jamieson, B., & Strain, P.  S.
https://doi.org/10.2511/rpsd.24.3.133 (1985). Increasing handicapped preschoolers' peer
McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Daly, T. (2001). Walden social interactions: Cross-setting and component
early childhood program. In J. S. Handleman & S. L. analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(1),
Harris (Eds.), Preschool education programs for chil- 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­3
dren with autism (2nd ed., pp. 157–190). PRO-ED. Odom, S.  L., & Strain, P.  S. (1986). A comparison of
McGee, G. G., Paradis, T., & Feldman, R. S. (1993). Free peer-initiation and teacher-antecedent interventions
effects of integration on levels of autistic behavior. for promoting reciprocal social interaction of autistic
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 13(1), preschoolers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
57–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/027112149301300107 19(1), 59–71. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1986.19-­59
McWilliam, R. A., Trivette, C. M., & Dunst, C. J. (1985). Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, B. A., &
Behavior engagement as a measure of the efficacy Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stimulus preference
of early intervention. Analysis and Intervention in and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded indi-
Developmental Disabilities, 5(1–2), 59–71. https:// viduals. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(3),
doi.org/10.1016/S0270-­4684(85)80006-­9 249–255. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­249
Merriam-Webster. (n.d.). Incidental. In Merriam-­Webster. Porterfield, J.  K., Herbert-Jackson, E., & Risley, T.  R.
com dictionary. Retrieved May 30, 2021, from https:// (1976). Contingent observation: An effective and
www.Merriam-­Webster.com/dictionary/incidental. acceptable procedure for reducing disruptive behav-
Morrier, M. J., McGee, G. G., & Daly, T. (2009). Effects ior of young children in a group setting. Journal of
of toy selection and arrangement on the social behav- Applied Behavior Analysis, 9(1), 55–64. https://doi.
iors of an inclusive group of preschool-aged children org/10.1901/jaba.1976.9-­55
with and without autism. Early Childhood Services: Reynolds, N. J., & Risley, T. R. (1968). The role of social
An Interdisciplinary Journal of Effectiveness, 3(2), and material reinforcers in increasing talking of a cul-
157–177. turally deprived preschool child. Journal of Applied
Morrier, M.  J., & Ziegler, S.  M. (2018). I wanna play Behavior Analysis, 1, 253–262.
too: Factors related to changes in social behavior for Risley, T. R. (1972). Spontaneous language and the pre-
children with and without autism spectrum disorder school environment. In J. C. Stanley (Ed.), Preschool
after implementation of a structured outdoor play programs for the disadvantaged (pp.  92–110). Johns
curriculum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Hopkins Press.
Disorders, 48(7), 2530–2541. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Risley, T.  R., & Favell, J. (1979). Constructing a living
s10803-­018-­3523-­z environment in an institution. In: L. A. Hammerlynck
Mudd, J.  M., & Wolery, M. (1987). Training head start (Ed.), Behavioral systems for the developmentally dis-
teachers to use incidental teaching. Journal of the abled (Vol. II, pp. 299–318). Brunner-Routledge.
206 G. G. McGee

Rittenhouse-Cea, H., & Cho, S.-J. (2018). Effects of Sidman, M. (1988). Tactics for scientific research (New
instructor training on language outcomes of stu- edition). Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies.
dents with autism spectrum disorders. The Journal Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden two. Macmillan.
of Special Education, 53(2), 108–118. https://doi. Skinner, B.  F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Prentice-Hall.
org/10.1177/0022466918807701 https://doi.org/10.1037/11256-­000.
Rosenberg, N. E., Artman-Meeker, K., Kelly, E., & Yang, Stahmer, A. C., & Ingersoll, B. (2004). Inclusive program-
X. (2020). The effects of a bug-in-ear coaching pack- ming for toddlers with autism spectrum disorders:
age on implementation of incidental teaching by para- Outcomes from the Children’s toddler school. Journal
professionals in a K-12 school. Journal of Behavioral of Positive Behavior Interventions, 6, 67–82. https://
Education, 29(2), 409–432. https://doi.org/10.1007/ doi.org/10.1177/10983007040060020201
s10864-­020-­09379-­1 Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol-
Ryan, C. S., Hemmes, N. S., Sturmey, P., Jacobs, J. D., ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior
& Grommet, E.  K. (2008). Effects of a brief staff Analysis, 10(2), 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1901/
training procedure on instructors’ use of inciden- jaba.1977.10-­349
tal teaching and students’ frequency of initiation Touchette, P. E. (1968). The effects of graduated stimulus
toward instructors. Research in Autism Spectrum change on the acquisition of a simple discrimination
Disorders, 2(1), 28–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. in severely retarded boys. Journal of the Experimental
rasd.2007.02.002 Analysis of Behavior, 11(1), 39–48. https://doi.
Schepis, M. M., Reid, D. H., Fitzgerald, J. R., Faw, G. D., org/10.1901/jeab.1968.11-­39
van den Pol, R. A., & Welty, P. A. (1982). A program Touchette, P.  E. (1971). Transfer of stimulus control:
for increasing manual signing by autistic and pro- Measuring the moment of transfer. Journal of the
foundly retarded youth within the daily environment. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15(3), 347–354.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15(3), 363–379. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1971.15-­347
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1982.15-­363 Twardosz, S., Cataldo, M. F., & Risley, T. R. (1974). Open
Schepis, M.  M., Reid, D.  H., Ownby, J., & Parsons, environment design for infant and toddler day care.
M.  P. (2001). Training support staff to embed teach- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7(4), 529–546.
ing within the natural routines of young children https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1974.7-­529
with disabilities in an inclusive preschool. Journal of Twardosz, S., Cataldo, M.  F., & Risley, T.  R. (1975).
Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(3), 313–327. https:// Menus for toddler day care: Food preference and
doi.org/10.1177/0022466918807701 spoon use. Young Children, 30, 129–144.
Schreibman, L., Dawson, G., Stahmer, A. C., Landa, R., Valdez-Menchacha, M.  C., & Whitehurst, G.  J. (1989).
Rogers, S. J., McGee, G. G., Kasari, C., Ingersoll, B., The effects of incidental teaching on vocabulary
Kaiser, A. P., Bruinsma, Y., McNerney, E., Wetherby, acquisition by young children. Child Development,
A., & Halladay, A. (2015). Naturalistic developmental 59(6), 1451–1459. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
behavioral interventions: Empirically validated treat- ­8624.1988.tb03673.x
ments for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig,
and Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2411–2428. A., Kucharczyk, S., Brock, M.  E., Plavnick, J.  B.,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2407-­8 Fleury, V.  P., & Schultz, T.  R. (2015). Evidence-­
Shafer, M. S., Egel, A. L., & Neef, N. A. (1984). Training based practices for children, youth, and young
mildly handicapped peers to facilitate changes in the adults with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehen-
social interaction skills of autistic children. Journal of sive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Applied Behavior Analysis, 17(4), 461–476. https:// Disorders, 45(7), 1951–1966. https://doi.org/10.1007/
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1984.17-­461 s10803-­014-­2351-­z
Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT):
Research Findings Over 30 Years 12
Lynn Kern Koegel, Elizabeth Ponder,
Katie Stolen Nordlund, and Brittany L. Koegel

12.1 Pivotal Response Treatment underlying ABA strategies of choosing meaning-


(PRT): Research Findings ful target behaviors, carefully defining target
Over 30 Years behaviors, monitoring progress through system-
atic data collection, and coordinating across pro-
This chapter will discuss Pivotal Response viders and settings, specific motivational
Treatment (PRT), also called Pivotal Response components have been researched individually
Therapy. We will provide a background and and as a package. The PRT package has been
description of early studies, discuss various piv- documented in both single-subject design studies
otal areas, and describe the adaptation of PRT to and randomized clinical trials by the original
different target behaviors and age groups. researchers within the clinic where it was devel-
Included are several tables for reference. To start, oped, as well as replicated by researchers in dif-
the importance of evidence-based practice cannot ferent research settings (Koegel & Openden,
be understated as many non-scientific interven- 2019; Mohammadzaheri et  al., 2014; Verschuur
tions for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are et al., 2016; Wong et al., 2015). Again, providing
prevalent. Unscientific and untested interventions evidence-based practices assures that individuals
result in delays in evidence-based interventions. will receive effective interventions in a timely
For children with ASD, time is of the essence, manner, thereby increasing the likelihood of
particularly in regard to verbal communication. more positive outcomes.
The younger the child, the more likely they will
learn verbal spoken communication (Koegel,
2000). Assuring the best possible outcomes 12.2 B
 ackground and Early PRT
necessitates implementing evidence-based Studies
practices.
PRT, an evolution of early applied behavior Kanner’s (1943) initial descriptions of 11 chil-
analysis (ABA) intervention, has a strong evi- dren, which he labeled as having infantile autism,
dence base (Wong et al., 2015). In addition to the included references to parents who were coldly
intellectual and interpersonally distant. This
work set the stage for psychoanalytic interven-
L. K. Koegel (*) · E. Ponder · K. S. Nordlund ·
B. L. Koegel tions focused on repairing the parent-child rela-
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, tionship, which often included a “parentectomy”
Stanford University School of Medicine, wherein children were removed from their par-
Palo Alto, CA, USA ents and placed in an environment where,
e-mail: lynnk@stanford.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 207
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_12
208 L. K. Koegel et al.

t­heoretically, they would feel safe and protected research in the field of ABA has come to under-
in order to provide them with an opportunity to stand that many interfering behaviors are com-
develop (Bettelheim, 1967; Roser, 1996). municative. Understanding the functions and
Although in later writings Kanner would go on to teaching replacement behaviors, rather than
suggest a physiological cause, this initial work response consequences, results in more long-­
implying a parental etiology resulted in many lasting changes. As well, creating inviting teach-
years of ineffective intervention based on faulty ing environments using evidenced-based
theoretical constructs. Prior to the 1960s, before motivational procedures can be considered an
behavioral interventions emerged, the majority of antecedent intervention, thereby increasing
children diagnosed with autism were institution- responsiveness and engagement.
alized by adolescence or adulthood (Henninger Further, starting in the 1970s, studies relating
& Taylor, 2013). Due to lack of effective inter- to parent personality profiles have continuously
ventions and prevailing parental causation senti- demonstrated that parents of children with autism
ment, few remained at home. are not aloof and exceedingly brilliant, nor are
Beginning in the 1960s, a positive change they “refrigerator parents” (Wolff & Morris,
occurred with the shift from the parental causa- 1971). Over the years, accumulating studies have
tion theory to learning theory, which resulted in a established that although specific areas of stress
transformation in the intervention for children may be elevated in parents of children with
with autism. That is, studies began to emerge autism (Moes et al., 1992), personality character-
showing that children with autism could indeed istics do not differ from parents of children not
learn (Lovaas, 1966; Lovaas et  al., 1965). The diagnosed with autism (Koegel et  al., 1983).
programs described in these early studies were Additionally, in contrast to the psychodynamic
largely based on direct intervention with the chil- interventions that separated the parent and child,
dren focusing on teaching nonverbal imitation research shows that parents are essential and nec-
skills and then imitation of sounds, followed by essary components of the habilitation process if
the shaping of words from the imitated sounds generalization and maintenance are to be
(Lovaas, 1966; Lovaas et  al., 1967). Behavior achieved (Lovaas et al., 1973).
management and teaching trials were generally Early ABA interventions used repeated drill
based on rewards for appropriate behavior and exercises presented in a distraction-free environ-
correct responding and punishers administered ment (Pope, 1999). These carefully orchestrated
for inappropriate and incorrect responding Stimulus-Response-Consequence (SRC) trials,
(Lovaas et al., 1965; Risley, 1968). In regard to with repeated presentation until the child reached
these behavioral interventions developed and specific criterion, were very effective, especially
implemented in the 1960s and 1970s, now fre- considering many children at that time did not
quently referred to as “traditional ABA,” many improve with the available psychodynamic inter-
now view the use of punishment as inhumane and ventions. It is important to consider the fact that
unnecessary (Carr et  al., 2002). However, non-­ autism was a low incidence disability, and this
aversive positive behavior support strategies to initial work was focused on demonstrating that
decrease interfering behaviors (including aggres- with proper instruction children with autism
sion and self-injury) were not yet developed and could learn. However many children demon-
researched at that time (Horner et  al., 1990). It strated interfering behaviors during intervention
should also be noted that while feedback and (Mohammadzaheri et  al., 2015), appeared
other consequences that decrease unwanted “unmotivated,” and resisted coming to the ses-
behaviors are important and a part of everyday sions (Koegel, Koegel, & Surratt, 1992). In other
life, positive consequences for desired behaviors words, many children did not appear enthusiastic
have always been preferred, and punishment about participating in the learning sessions and
(sometimes severe punishment for dangerous engaged in avoidance and escape motivated
behavior) is now infrequent. Importantly, behaviors. Thus, while the interventions based on
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 209

learning theory resulted in greatly improved During this time of exploration of motiva-
progress relative to other interventions (Lovaas, tional variables, our research found that combin-
1966, 1981), researchers began a search for areas ing the various individual components into a
that could be incorporated into the trials that package appeared to be particularly effective. We
would lead to improved motivation that may first assessed whether the package of procedures
result in faster treatment gains and greater would improve verbal communication in nonver-
generalization. bal children with ASD. Following the implemen-
Initial studies, focusing on the construct of tation of the package intervention, the participants
motivation, revealed that specific variables could improved their imitative and spontaneous verbal
be adjusted during the teaching sessions to communication. Because these intervention ses-
improve responsiveness and decrease interfering sions looked more like natural, play-based inter-
behaviors. For example, child choice in regard to actions than the traditional ABA sessions, the
preferred activities, topics, and materials helped initial motivational package was described as
improve social engagement and decrease escape “the Natural Language Paradigm” or “NLP”
and avoidance behavior during intervention (Koegel, Dyer, et al., 1987). The package also led
(Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, & Frea, 1992). Other to lower levels of escape and avoidance moti-
studies showed that, rather than providing the vated interfering behaviors (Koegel, Koegel, &
commonly used social rewards paired with food Surratt, 1992). As time progressed, additional
treats, using direct and natural rewards improved research showed that the package was also effec-
learning (Koegel, O’Dell, et  al., 1987; tive when targeting behaviors other than commu-
Mohammadzaheri et al., 2015). A simple exam- nication, and thus it was re-labeled “Pivotal
ple was teaching a child to open a container. Response Treatment” or “PRT.” Our intention in
Putting a snack inside the container was much using the word “pivotal” was that we were search-
more effective than providing the reinforcer ing for core areas that, when targeted, would
extrinsically after the child opened the container. result in widespread positive changes in untreated
In short, arranging direct and functional response- behaviors. The idea was that behaviors could be
reinforcer relationships created higher levels of learned more rapidly and efficiently if we tar-
responding (Koegel & Williams, 1980; Williams geted key core areas, such as motivation, instead
et  al., 1981). Other studies addressed the drill of focusing on teaching individual behaviors that
type practice and showed that interspersing main- may be numerous for many diagnosed with
tenance trials (targets that the individual has mas- ASD.  In short, “pivotal” areas were targeted to
tered) with acquisition (new target behaviors) speed up the learning process and result in posi-
resulted in more efficient learning and improved tive improvements in untargeted areas.
child affect (Dunlap, 1984). Similarly, repeatedly Several points are important when consider-
presenting the same task was compared to vary- ing the roots of PRT. First are the general prin-
ing the task with different activities from the ciples of behavioral learning theory. That is,
child’s curriculum. This research showed that target behaviors must be clearly defined and
task variation resulted in improved responding measurable. Rewards are important and should
with fewer interfering behaviors. As well, greater be natural, whenever possible, as an integral part
child interest, enthusiasm, and happiness (objec- of the trial. In addition, positive affect on the part
tively rated on Likert scales) were observed dur- of the child and interventionist is essential. If
ing the sessions (Dunlap & Koegel, 1980). children are motivated, their measured affect
Finally, when targeting expressive verbal com- should be high, demonstrated by smiles, interest
munication in nonverbal children diagnosed with in the activity, and engagement. Teachers and
ASD, reinforcing all communicative attempts, interventionists should maintain a positive atti-
rather than using a strict shaping paradigm, was tude, and when motivation is considered, the
greatly more effective for teaching first words need for negative consequences is greatly dimin-
(Koegel et al., 1988). ished. Socially significant goals are developed
210 L. K. Koegel et al.

using a “top-down” age-appropriate construct. goal, they may cease to engage in these important
For example, expressive verbal communication behaviors. In other words, under such conditions,
is targeted if a child’s first words are delayed, children behave as if they are helpless, because
regardless of whether the child is able to point to they are unable to escape controlled events or
target items receptively or demonstrate other negative situations, or they do not connect impor-
related skills. Next, data are collected regularly tant behaviors with the outcome (Maier &
to inform intervention and track progress. Seligman, 1976). Thus, the concept of learned
Generalization and maintenance are essential helplessness, as it applies to children with autism,
and are systematically tracked. Thus, although was theorized. For example, well-meaning adults
PRT sessions sometimes resemble play, system- may dress a child who is taking too long to get
atic trials are created using the antecedent- ready. Similarly, a child may get needs met with-
behavior-consequence (ABC) format (or SRC) out having to verbally communicate. For exam-
so that clear learning trials can be evaluated. ple, a peer may intend to be helpful by opening a
Finally, it is also helpful to understand that prior difficult container for the child at lunchtime with-
to the development of PRT that began in the out requiring the child to request “Help” or open
1970s, naturalistic strategies were not commonly it independently. In situations where children
used with this population (Pope, 1999). It should have no control, they may not attempt to respond,
be also noted that this is not a dichotomous rela- or they may engage in interfering behaviors,
tionship. Some important behaviors that are dif- which appears as lack of motivation. However,
ficult or nonpreferred may benefit from the this apparent lack of motivation may be “learned”
addition of secondary rewards. This is similar to because of environmental influences in teaching
accepted practices of getting a good grade in a and other settings that created a situation where
class or a paycheck. However, while the early the child is emitting few responses with low
ABA procedures resulted in improvements in effort. This theory highlights the importance of
responsiveness, more naturalistic strategies, incorporating components that improve motiva-
such as PRT (previously called the “Natural tion into the intervention, which is a key compo-
Language Paradigm” or “NLP”; Dunlap & nent to PRT.
Koegel, 1980; Koegel, Dyer, et al., 1987; Koegel
& Egel, 1979) and incidental teaching (McGee
et al., 1983; McGee et al., 1985, 1986), incorpo- 12.2.2 Pivotal Areas
rated into the intervention, particularly early on,
led to a greater response-reinforcer relationship, As mentioned above, our goal in identifying
improved child affect, and thus more rapid gains. “pivotal” areas has been to discover which key
behaviors will result in collateral gains in untar-
geted areas of functioning and development
12.2.1 Learned Helplessness (Koegel, Koegel, et al., 2010). To date, research
has shown that several key areas appear to be
Another important theoretical foundation of PRT especially helpful in the process of teaching and
is the concept that children with ASD are more learning. Motivation is particularly important, as
capable than they appear, but may be affected by it is an essential underlying component of all
what has been described in the literature as PRT intervention. The motivational components
“learned helplessness.” That is, in order to under- can be implemented across the age span and with
stand motivation, it is helpful to understand the many different target behaviors. In addition to
apparent lack of motivation in children with motivation, this chapter will discuss other pivotal
autism. For example, when children get their areas, including initiations, self-management,
needs met without having to make communica- and empathetic responses.
tive or behavioral efforts to accomplish the end
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 211

12.2.3 PRT Motivational a highly desired item) and the positive outcome
Components of that behavior (receiving the desired item)
improves responsiveness and decreases learned
The five strategies that have been proven to helplessness.
increase motivation in children with autism
include (1) providing choice and incorporating 12.2.3.2 Intersperse Maintenance
preferred interests; (2) interspersing maintenance Tasks
tasks and acquisition tasks; (3) implementing Completing the same task over and over quickly
task variation; (4) providing natural reinforce- becomes monotonous for learners, particularly
ment; and (5) reinforcing all reasonable attempts. when targeting difficult tasks. Furthermore,
repeatedly failing the same task each time may
12.2.3.1 Child Choice cause learners to lose interest and become frus-
Presenting the learner with a choice of instruc- trated, leading to a lack of motivation to continue.
tional tasks improves engagement, responsive- A more effective strategy involves teaching a stu-
ness, and on-task behavior while also decreasing dent through a varied task condition, where the
behaviors that interfere with learning (Dyer et al., challenging target behavior is interspersed with a
1990; Koegel, Dyer, et  al., 1987; Ulke- variety of relatively easy tasks that the child has
Kurkcuoglu & Kircaali-Iftar, 2010). There are already mastered. Research has shown that var-
several ways to assess preference. The learner ied task conditions result in an increase in the
can be observed directly, and parents or caregiv- number of correct responses, cause the student to
ers can help identify various toys, games, books, show more interest, exhibit a happier demeanor,
foods, and activities that are highly preferred. and display fewer interfering behaviors (Dunlap,
Having a variety of options available is helpful as 1984; Dunlap & Koegel, 1980).
children’s interests may change frequently, even
within a single session. The influence of choice is 12.2.3.3 Task Variation
quintessential, as the literature has shown that Task variation involves revising the interaction
when intervention is conducted within a play such that a single task is not repeated until crite-
context and choices are permitted, there are con- rion is met. That is, rather than presenting repeti-
siderably fewer interfering behaviors, increased tive opportunities in a drill-type manner, different
levels of appropriate social interaction, and targets and activities are incorporated into the
improved pragmatic skills (Carter, 2001). The intervention session. For example, instead of
combination of reduced interfering behaviors and repeatedly prompting a child to label the colors
increased appropriate social interactions results of markers during an art activity, varied opportu-
in reduced interventionist redirection. Thus, the nities for different targets can be provided, such
child has more opportunities to independently as choosing between different art materials,
interact socially in the context of positive behav- requesting help to open difficult to open contain-
ioral support. ers, turn taking, identifying colors, and, when
Providing choice by following a child’s lead indicated, changing to a different activity. A lit-
also involves engaging the learner in preferred erature review corroborated these findings that
tasks and correctly identifying how to gain con- variation and stimulus novelty increase respon-
trol of the reinforcing item or action. Lei et  al. sivity, which is exhibited by students’ increased
(2017) found that using choice may also help attempts to perform both target behaviors and
children with autism feel a sense of control, or other less preferred tasks (Clinton & Clees,
predictability, over some uncertainty associated 2015).
with the task at hand, since they are more familiar
with the sequence of events during their preferred 12.2.3.4 Natural Rewards
activity. Thus, strengthening the connection Another essential component of PRT interven-
between the behavior emitted (such as requesting tion is the delivery of reinforcers that are directly
212 L. K. Koegel et al.

related to the task at hand. In contrast, it was child is rewarded for responses that are attempts
common practice in traditional ABA therapy to at the target behavior, persistence follows. In
provide children with edibles, a more desired contrast, a strict shaping paradigm may result in
activity, or other reinforcers that were not directly effort not being rewarded and in turn may extin-
related to the task following a correct response guish the important behavior of trying. Rewarding
(Fisher et  al., 2020; Shvarts et  al., 2020). For attempts are particularly helpful for children with
example, a child may be given an M&M or a ASD who may experience more challenges with
sticker for correctly labeling five pictures of com- learning and may respond well to the higher fre-
mon household items. While these types of exter- quency of rewards.
nal reinforcers can be effective, they do not Table 12.1 shows the specific motivational
provide learning opportunities with a connection procedures that were added to improve engage-
between the response and the reward, and thus ment and decrease interfering behavior (Koegel,
generalization may be moderated and acquisition Koegel, & Surratt, 1992; Koegel, Dyer, et  al.,
slower (Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987). While 1987).
certainly tokens and edibles are useful and can
improve behavior, using naturally rewarding con-
tingencies such as directly enabling the child to Table 12.1  PRT research has compared an adult-driven
model with motivational components added, as shown
gain access to a desired item or activity following
a correct attempt or response appears to improve PRT motivational
Adult-driven components
learning. For example, when targeting expressive
Stimulus Chosen by the Chosen by the
verbal communication, if a child responds appro- items adult or clinician child
priately to a prompt for the word “ball,” the child Repeated until Varied every few
is immediately and naturally rewarded with criterion is met trials
access to the ball. This functional relationship Age-appropriate
items that can be
between the child’s response and the reinforcer found in the
results in more rapid acquisition of target behav- child’s natural
iors (Williams et al., 1981). Natural rewards have environment
been shown to have a broad impact in many Prompts Manual or physical Clinician models
(e.g., touch tip of the response,
areas, such as first words, language, self-help,
tongue or hold lips particularly
and academics, and are increasingly being used together), hand when working
in many intervention programs. over hand on first words
Interaction Clinician holds up Clinician and
12.2.3.5 Reward Attempts stimulus item; child play with
stimulus item not stimulus item
While every target goal has a specific criterion functional within that are
that must be met in order to be considered mas- interaction functional
tered, it is beneficial for the child to be positively within the
reinforced for making an appropriate, earnest interaction
Response Only correct Looser shaping
attempt to produce the target behavior. This way,
responses or contingency so
the child is rewarded for their effort, therefore successive that attempts or
increasing motivation to continue responding to approximations are “good tries” are
subsequent opportunities. Koegel et  al. (1988) reinforced reinforced
found that reinforcing attempts, rather than fol- Consequence Edible or token Natural
reinforcers paired reinforcer (e.g.,
lowing a strict shaping paradigm, was more with social opportunity to
effective with respect to the children’s interest, reinforcers (e.g., engage with the
general behavior, and the targeted area of speech “good job!”) item) is paired
(word) production. Rewarding attempts may be with social
reinforcers
analogous to behavioral momentum. When the
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 213

12.3 A
 dapting PRT for Different to engagement in infant-preferred activities. Task
Age Groups and Target variation is incorporated, in that activities are
Behaviors changed approximately every 10 s before social
breaks. Interspersal of maintenance and acquisi-
Most of the initial PRT research studies were tion is implemented with the interspersal of pre-
implemented with preschool and elementary-­ ferred and neutral activities identified by
aged children, but other research has shown that observable signs of infant enjoyment during
the motivational procedures can be adapted to baseline. Finally, reinforcement is based on clas-
wider age ranges and different target behaviors. sical conditioning, rather than an operant condi-
tioning paradigm. Table  12.2 outlines these
modifications from the PRT motivational compo-
12.3.1 Adapting the PRT nents used for infants compared with preschool-
Motivational Components ers and beyond when verbal communication is
for Infants present or expected. Again, the parent involve-
ment component of the PRT methodology is
The literature suggests that early identification of essential to this modified approach, as parents
autism and early intervention are linked to more serve as the interventionists.
positive outcomes. While an increasing number Specifically, the first step of this modified PRT
of children are being diagnosed and receiving approach involves categorizing activities as either
intervention in the preschool years, by that time “preferred” or “neutral” during play observations
the developmental gap has already begun to with the parent. “Preferred” activities are those
widen, in some cases, quite significantly. In that elicit positive reactions from the infant, such
recent years, research has focused on determin- as smiles, laughing, or eye contact. Activities
ing behavioral markers in infancy that later lead where the infant displays flat to low affect and
to a diagnosis of autism. With the development of avoids eye contact would be considered “neu-
early screening tools, such as the Autism tral.” During the initial phase, parents engage
Observation Scale for Infants (AOSI; Bryson with the infant in a variety of preferred social
et  al., 2007), that assess prelinguistic behaviors interactions, such as peek-a-boo, tickles, and
such as social smiles, visual tracking, social making silly faces, which are individualized
interest, and eye contact, along with known risk based on the interest of the particular infant.
factors (e.g., having an older sibling diagnosed These activities are incorporated into interven-
with autism), there is a rising need for interven- tion sessions that consist of alternating short
tions that can be effectively implemented within intervals of approximately 5–7 min when the par-
the first year of life. Although the bulk of research ent engages in a variety of preferred activities.
showing the effectiveness of PRT has been During the intervals of engagement, the parent
focused on preschool-aged children and older, implements task variation by switching the activ-
the core motivational components have also been ity approximately every 10 s. By varying the task
adapted to target developmentally appropriate often and taking breaks in between short interac-
behaviors for infants as young as 6  months. tion intervals, both the parent and infant are more
Rather than having the focus of intervention be likely to maintain high levels of engagement.
increasing functional verbal communication, as After the interactive intervals, a 5–10-min break
children are preverbal during the first year of life, is provided, during which time the parent and
PRT target goals are related to social engagement infant can take a walk or engage in another relax-
and include responsiveness, positive affect, and ing activity that does not involve providing an
interest in parent-child social interactions. opportunity for the infant to engage in a non-­
Koegel, Singh, et al. (2013) described a modi- social or restricted and repetitive behavior, such
fied PRT approach in which the traditional PRT as staring at a ceiling fan. In regard to interspers-
motivational component “child choice” translates ing acquisition tasks, at the start of intervention,
214 L. K. Koegel et al.

Table 12.2  Motivational component adapted for pre-linguistic infants and preschoolers
Modified PRT
PRT motivational motivational
component Example for preschooler or older component Example for pre-linguistic infant
Child choice “Do you want to play train or Preferred activities Engaging in preferred activity,
bubbles?” or modeling the word such as peek-a-boo
“train”
Task variation Varying opportunities for the child Task variation Varying between different
to request “cookie, please” with preferred activities, such as
“open the box” (of cookies) peek-a-boo, tickles, and making
silly faces every 10 seconds
Interspersal of Providing three easy opportunities Interspersal of Engaging in several preferred
maintenance and for one-word utterances (e.g., preferred and activities (e.g., peek-a-boo,
acquisition “blue,” “block,” “on”), followed by neutral activities tickles, making faces), followed
a more difficult opportunity for a by a neutral activity (e.g.,
two-word utterance (e.g., “red singing, pull up to parent, air
block”) kisses)
Contingent Immediately providing the child Reinforcement Once affect is continuously high
natural access to a red block after the child (classical with preferred activities, neutral
reinforcement makes a verbal request, “red block” conditioning) activities are paired with the
(operant conditioning) preferred activities
(classical conditioning)

the parent engages in only preferred activities. guish, modeling word production, and encourag-
Once the infant is consistently displaying posi- ing high affect activities. Again, these programs
tive affect during the engagement intervals, neu- are largely implemented through parent educa-
tral activities are gradually and systematically tion and, in addition to showing promise, are
incorporated. Results of Koegel et  al. (2013) highly cost-efficient (Table 12.2).
indicated that this modified PRT approach led to
decreased avoidance of eye contact and increased
positive affect, as measured by Likert scales indi- 12.3.2 Adapting the PRT
cating levels of interest and happiness. As a col- Motivational Components
lateral gain, the infants’ responses to their name for Adults
also increased, which occurs around 5 to 7 months
in neurotypical infants. The bulk of the intervention literature, as well as
While interventions for infants are in the early support services, focuses on preschool- and
stages of development, pilot studies suggest opti- school-aged children with ASD (Levy & Perry,
mism for the possibility of targeting pre-­linguistic 2011). Thus, there is a need for effective inter-
social areas (Bradshaw et al., 2015; Brian et al., ventions that support the different challenges that
2015). While there are challenges in the area of come with adolescence and adulthood. PRT strat-
intervention for infants, due to temperament dif- egies have been adapted to target areas that are
ferences across those who will not later receive a meaningful to this population, such as social
diagnosis of ASD, targeting parental concerns interaction, time management, and daily living
that are consistently shown on early development skills. Traditional PRT motivational components,
measures may provide parents with helpful tools such as choice, task variation, interspersal of
and improve social engagement and responsive- maintenance and acquisition, and contingent and
ness in the first year of life. These may include natural reinforcement, are incorporated into the
pre-linguistic activities, such as working with programs, but with modifications to be more
parents to attend to preferred activities to increase appropriate and relevant to the age of the indi-
engagement, pairing activities and items with vidual and the goal of intervention. For example,
response to name so that infants do not extin- choice may be incorporated by having the
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 215

i­ndividual choose what social club to attend or Table 12.3  Motivational components adapted for ado-
lescents and adults
which day to go grocery shopping. For instance,
choice was implemented in Ashbaugh et  al. PRT
motivational Implementation Implementation
(2017) with college students diagnosed with ASD component with a child with an adult
who engaged in few to no extracurricular social Choice Presenting an Having the
activities. During weekly intervention sessions, opportunity for adolescent or
the participants were provided with a menu of the child to adult with ASD
social activities around their unique interests that choose between choose which
playing a game social club to
existed in the community and on campus. These or eating snack attend and what
included campus clubs and classes, community days of the week
activities, events in the dormitories, dining to schedule social
events, and study sessions with peers. Peer men- activities
Task variation Presenting Varying different
tors were recruited to attend these clubs and
opportunities types of social
activities each week and provide support, if for the child to opportunities
desired (all participants requested peer mentors), request different around the
such as introducing them to others, modeling, preferred items, individual’s
engaging in a interests and
and helping them ask for phone numbers.
variety of targeting different
Following intervention, all participants increased different games, goals. For
the number of social activities in which they par- targeting colors, example, an
ticipated each week and further maintained the shapes, and individual who
item labels enjoys dancing
social activities following the completion of the
may attend
intervention. These types of structured social different types of
planning, which involve incorporating specific dance classes.
social activities into the person’s weekly and Target behaviors
are varied, such as
monthly schedule with peer support, have been
social, academic,
shown to improve overall reported satisfaction time management,
and quality of life for young adults on the autism daily living skills
spectrum (Koegel, Ashbaugh, et  al., 2013). In Interspersal of Incorporating Including easy
addition, concomitant increases in untargeted maintenance easy tasks, such tasks, such as
and as high-fives, checking email,
areas, including improvements in unstructured acquisition with more on an adult with
social engagement, grade point average, and difficult tasks, ASD’s daily living
employment, have been observed (Koegel, such as checklist along
Ashbaugh, et al., 2013). Other areas that are par- turn-taking with more difficult
tasks, such as
ticularly relevant to adults are time management attending social
and daily living skills. Much like the structured events
social planning described above, these targets Contingent Giving the child Checking in with
can be incorporated into daily, weekly, and reinforcement access to a a peer mentor and/
preferred item or self-­
monthly schedules and self-­ managed (Palmen
or activity management and
et al., 2012). Table 12.3 shows examples of how immediately scheduling a
the PRT motivational components can be used following the special event after
with adults with ASD. desired behavior completion of
self-help chores
Natural Rewarding a Being able to
reinforcement child with engage in a
12.3.3 Adapting PRT for Academics access to a preferred activity,
favorite toy such as Dungeons
Many children engage in interfering avoidance train after she and dragons, at
says, “train” the social club
and escape-related behaviors during difficult aca-
demic assignments. These behaviors can influ-
216 L. K. Koegel et al.

ence teachers, leading them to reduce the of time the child engaged in disruptive and inter-
demands of the interaction, thereby providing a fering behaviors. Also, incorporating motiva-
less challenging curriculum for their students tional variables increased the children’s interest
with ASD (Carr et  al., 1991). Behaviors that in the academic assignment (based on Likert
interfere with learning can range from lethargy scale adapted from Koegel & Egel, 1979).
and inattention, or “zoning out,” to full melt- Further, these gains were maintained post inter-
downs (Vivar, 2016). Research shows that over vention and generalized to in-class assignments.
time, these behaviors are likely to worsen without In addition, after intervention, parents reported
direct intervention (Horner et al., 2002). In addi- observing low levels of interfering behaviors and
tion, a lack of interest in academic assignments high levels of on-task behavior during homework
may lead to low levels of participation in school assignments.
and during homework (Ochs et al., 2001). Many To be specific, at the beginning of interven-
common strategies used in traditional teaching, tion, child-preferred topics are identified
such as time-out, being sent to the principal’s through observation and/or parent or caregiver
office, or having a chat with a teacher, are typi- interview and then embedded into the task. For
cally not successful for students with ASD example, if a child who enjoys trains is working
because, although these strategies are commonly on writing letters, the teacher could have the
considered “punishers” by school personnel, they child write “T” for train. A child working on
actually may function as rewards for children more advanced writing skills could write “I
with ASD, as the students are able to avoid aca- want to play with the train engine” or a story
demic tasks while being excluded from the about a train. Once the children successfully
activity. complete the task, their behavior is immediately
A lack of motivation to engage in schoolwork reinforced with the corresponding desired item,
has been identified as one of the root causes of action, or activity, such as an opportunity to
avoidance behaviors and low levels of interest. play with the train as a natural reward. During a
Therefore, from a theoretical point of view, using math intervention, the child who likes trains can
the PRT principles to target motivation during add the train cars (2 cars +1 car = 3 cars), and
academics should increase a child’s interest in after completion of the problem(s), they are pro-
both learning and the academic material itself, as vided with an opportunity to play with the trains
well as leading to long-term gains at school and as a natural reward, or a child working on more
home (Heimann et  al., 1995; Koegel, Singh, advanced skills can determine what percent of
et  al., 2010). Specific variables, such as using the cars are green, and so on. Consistent with
choice and incorporating the student’s interests, the PRT motivational components, task varia-
interspersing easy and difficult tasks, and includ- tion and including both easy and challenging
ing natural reinforcers, can increase interest in problems are important during academic activi-
academic materials, improve performance, and ties. In addition to incorporating preferred top-
decrease interfering behaviors during academic ics into the curriculum, other choices can be
activities. provided throughout the assignment to increase
For example, Koegel, Singh, et  al. (2010) motivation. For example, the child can be pro-
demonstrated that incorporating motivational vided with an opportunity to choose what type
variables into writing and math during homework or color of paper to write on, whether to use a
assignments decreased response latency (i.e., pen or pencil, where to sit, and so on. Again,
time between the instruction and when the child incorporating these PRT motivational variables
began the task), increased the rate of work com- into academics has been shown to improve
pletion (i.e., number of math problems completed engagement, correct responding, and overall
or letters written), and decreased the percentage behavior.
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 217

12.4 T
 he Pivotal Behavior 12.4.2 Teaching Question Asking
of Initiations (Question
Asking) Shortly after children begin using first words
(around 15–18 months), they will typically start
12.4.1 Importance of Initiations to imitate parents who frequently ask them
“What’s this?” or “What’s that?” The question is
Question asking plays an important role in simplified to their language level and emitted as a
learning and socialization. Children who one-word utterance: “dis?” or “dat?” Frequently,
develop language typically begin asking pointing will accompany this utterance. Question
questions at a very young age, often within asking is both social and educational, as the back-­
their first lexicon. By preschool, they are and-­forth interactions inherent in a question also
quite sophisticated at asking questions, and result in the acquisition of vocabulary or other
when given a task to solve, ask an average 75 linguistic information elicited by the question.
questions per hour (Chouinard et  al., 2007). During the preschool years, a variety of questions
When children ask questions, they simulta- are acquired and used frequently. The importance
neously gain a better understanding of the of question asking cannot be underestimated, as
social world around them, learn information questions provide an important role in cognitive
from their environment, and expand their and linguistic development (Chouinard et  al.,
verbal repertoires. Thus, these initiations are 2007). Therefore, as discussed above, if question
crucial in overall cognitive and social asking is absent or used infrequently, it becomes
development. an important pivotal goal.
In contrast to children who learn language Incorporating motivational components into
without difficulty, individuals with ASD use question asking intervention is essential. Our pre-
very few to no initiations. Most of the verbal liminary pilot data suggested that if motivational
communication of children with ASD is lim- components were not incorporated, the children
ited to behavior regulation functions, includ- did not exhibit success with the acquisition or
ing requesting items (e.g., “want milk”) and generalization of the target question. Using the
protesting (e.g., “no,” “down,” “all done;” motivational PRT components, our first area of
Maljaars et  al., 2011). By teaching children research (Koegel et al., 1997) focused on teach-
with ASD to initiate questions, their commu- ing “What’s that?”, the first question used by
nicative functions are expanded. However, typically developing children. Our goal in teach-
early research in this area reported difficulty ing “What’s that?” was to add an additional lan-
with generalization of question asking to nat- guage function (outside of behavior regulation),
ural settings as well as the lack of acquisition with the end result being acquisition of large
of the information provided after the question numbers of vocabulary words. Children who par-
was asked (Hung, 1977). Various procedures, ticipated in our question asking studies were able
such as time delay (Taylor & Harris, 1995), to verbally request items, had a vocabulary of at
video modeling (Charlop & Milstein, 1989), least 50 words, were beginning to combine
and self-management (Doggett et  al., 2013), words, but did not use questions in their commu-
have been used to encourage generalized nication. In order to teach and promote general-
question asking. Of interest, the research ization of question asking, the PRT motivational
suggests that when motivational procedures components were added. The specific steps are
are incorporated into the intervention ses- summarized below.
sions with young children, spontaneous gen- In an initial study (Koegel et  al., 1997), the
eralization of question asking to natural children’s favorite (child choice) items were
environments and acquisition of the response placed in an opaque bag, and they were prompted
following the question appears to improve to ask, “What’s that?” This preliminary step was
(Koegel et al., 1997). incorporated only to improve the children’s
218 L. K. Koegel et al.

­ otivation to ask the question; at this point it was


m linguistic and other information from their envi-
not a concern whether or not they could label ronment. A recent study found that initiating
these preferred items. After the children asked, questions in children with ASD not only had
“What’s that?”, the item was taken out of the bag positive effects on their language skills but also
and labeled, and after the children repeated the had positive effects on their overall affect
label, they were given the item as a natural (Popovic et al., 2020). Thus, initiations seem to
reward. Again, it was important when first teach- be another important pivotal area and are crucial
ing “What’s that?” to use highly and naturally for language development, social engagement,
reinforcing items so that the children continue to and improved long-term outcomes (Koegel et al.,
engage and ask questions. Once the children 1999).
were consistently responding to the model for
“What’s that?”, the verbal prompt was faded.
Then, when the children asked, “What’s that?” 12.5 Self-Management
independently and consistently, neutral items
were added into the bag, startings with every Self-management is a technique that is used to
fourth item, third, and so on. The final step was to teach individuals to be aware of, and monitor,
fade the preferred items completely, so that the their own behaviors to either increase the fre-
children only asked “What’s that?” in reference quency of desired behaviors or decrease the fre-
to items they did not know how to label. quency of undesired behaviors (Chai et al., 2018).
Additionally, the opaque bag was faded out, with Self-management is another pivotal area, as it
the goal being that the children would ask empowers individuals to take an active role in
“What’s that?” in reference to items in their natu- controlling and managing their own behaviors
ral environment to which they did not know the and thereby reduces the need for an intervention-
label. Our research showed that incorporating ist in natural settings. For example, through self-­
motivational components into the intervention management procedures, the occurrence of a
resulted in generalized and appropriate use of the newly learned behavior can be programmed to
question “What’s that?” at home and school. occur in settings beyond where intervention is
Other questions, including “Where is it?”, implemented (Koegel, Koegel, & Surratt, 1992).
“Whose is it?”, “What’s happening?”, and “What There are several steps to consider when set-
happened?”, can also be taught using motiva- ting up a self-management program. First, the
tional strategies to improve their use during social target behavior must be clearly defined and mea-
communication (Koegel et  al., 2003; Koegel, sured. Baseline data are important, as it serves as
Singh, et al., 2010, 2014). Similar motivational a guide for the initial goals within the self-­
components are added, such as hiding the child’s management system. Second, the individual must
favorite items, prompting the question “Where is be taught to discriminate between the occurrence
it?”, and then providing the targeted prepositions. and absence of the specific target behavior. This
Children can be prompted to ask, “Whose is it?” discrimination is important so that the individual
using favorite items and “What happened?” using self-records desired behaviors and not undesired
favorite pop-­up books and manipulating the tabs (or irrelevant) behaviors. Once the individual can
so that an action is shown. successfully differentiate between the desired
Most children with ASD use verbal communi- and undesired behaviors, the self-management
cation that is limited to requesting items (e.g., system is introduced, and the individual is taught
“want milk”) or protesting (e.g., “No,” “down,” how to track or monitor these behaviors. For
“all done”; Koegel, et al., 2014; Wetherby, 1986). example, wrist counters were used by Koegel
When these children are taught to initiate in the et al. (1992) to improve responsiveness to ques-
form of question asking, the function of their tions of children with ASD, ages 6  years
communication expands beyond behavior regula- 10 months to 11 years 2 months, in community
tion and provides them with a mechanism to gain settings. For behaviors that are appropriately
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 219

monitored using time intervals, a system that social communication, academics, play, interfer-
notifies the individual to record the presence or ing behaviors, self-help, and daily living, among
absence of the behavior during the previous time others. Below are a few additional examples.
period is used. For instance, Koegel et al. (1999)
taught children in full inclusion classes to mark
an “x” on a series of boxes printed on a sheet of 12.5.1 Self-Management and Social
paper if on-task behavior occurred during the Communication
previous interval using a timer to signal the end
of the interval. The next step was to provide a In a study by Koegel and colleagues (2014), a
reward for the individual’s engagement in the multiple-baseline design was used to investigate
desired behavior and monitoring. Initially, the effectiveness of self-management interven-
rewards were provided frequently and then sys- tion targeting on-topic responsiveness during a
tematically faded. The length of the initial time conversation, expansion of the conversational
interval or number of responses the individual topic, and on-topic question asking. Participants
needs before earning a reward depends on the were between the ages of 4 years 10 months and
baseline data. That is, if a child engages in on-­ 14  years 11  months and diagnosed with
task behavior for 20 s during baseline, a shorter ASD. Data were collected during 10-min conver-
interval (such as 10 s) is chosen initially, so that sation probes, and the conversational partners
the child experiences success. Once the child has were instructed to ask at least ten open-ended
mastered tracking appropriate behavior at 10  s, questions and provide a delay of 3–5  s before
the interval can be increased to 20 s, then 30 s, asking another open-ended question, to assess
and so on, until the child can independently track whether the participants would respond, further
the behavior for the amount of time the interven- elaborate on topics, and ask questions. The inter-
tion team deems appropriate, such as an entire vention phase consisted of providing the partici-
class period. Some children can also learn to self-­ pant with a visual schematic which included
administer rewards (Koegel & Koegel, 2018), but “answer a question,” “add information,” and “ask
others may need assistance with turning in points a question,” with additional empty boxes below,
for rewards. The final step of a self-management so that participants could self-manage when they
program is fading reinforcement and the struc- engaged in the social conversation behaviors out-
tured monitoring system. This can be accom- lined in the schematic and earn “conversation
plished by adding time to each interval and points.” Once the individuals earned the predeter-
requiring increasing numbers of intervals before mined number of points, they were able to access
a reward. For event recording, such as each time a self-chosen reward. During the first interven-
the individual asks a question or a child raises his tion sessions, the clinician prompted the partici-
hand, the number of points needed before a pants to follow the visual schematic and
reward can be increased. Generally, as the new self-manage conversation points. After several
behavior becomes more established and prac- successful sessions, both prompting and the
ticed, the individuals will begin to engage in the structured self-management system were faded.
self-recording less frequently while still exhibit- After the self-management program was com-
ing the desired behavior. In other cases, the pletely faded (by systematically increasing the
reward can be provided at more natural times, number of points before reinforcement), the gen-
such as at home at the end the school day. For eralization phase began by introducing new con-
some individuals, completely fading the program versational partners in novel settings, using the
is not possible; some sort of self-management same data collection model as used during the
system must stay in place. baseline phase. Results from this study showed
Self-management has been used with individ- that a visual conversational framework paired
uals with ASD with a wide variety of behaviors. with self-management led to increases in elabo-
There are positive impacts in the domains of ration of responses and reciprocal question
220 L. K. Koegel et al.

a­sking during conversation. This points to the Meta-analyses have shown that self-­
importance of teaching self-management tech- management is an efficacious procedure for
niques and the positive effects it has for a variety improving socially desirable behaviors in indi-
of different behaviors. viduals with ASD (Lee et al., 2007). It is a versa-
tile technique that can increase the independence
of individuals with autism and can positively
12.5.2 Self-Management improve the quality of lives of individuals with
and Academics ASD (Lee et al., 2007).

Self-management can also be useful within an


academic context. Roberts et  al. (2019) investi- 12.6 Empathy
gated the effects of a self-management program
with a peer training intervention on academic Research suggests that empathy may be a partic-
engagement for high school students with ularly challenging area for individuals diagnosed
ASD. Their research found that the peer trainer with ASD (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004).
implemented the peer training component with Empathy is a complex social construct, so it fol-
fidelity, suggesting the intervention was accept- lows that individuals with ASD experience diffi-
able and effective. Thus, this provides another culties in this area, given that challenges in social
way for children with ASD to learn from their communication and social interaction are diag-
peers. Not only can self-management be taught nostic characteristics (American Psychiatric
by parents and interventionists, but new research Association, 2013). In fact, Wheelwright et  al.
supports that it can be effectively taught by peers (2006) determined that where an individual’s
in academic settings. score falls on the Autism Spectrum Quotient
(AQ) can be predicted by their Empathy Quotient
(EQ) score. Challenges in this area include diffi-
12.5.3 Self-Management culty understanding and expressing interest in
and Interfering Behaviors peers, which can make it difficult to develop and
maintain meaningful friendships (Baron-Cohen
Interfering behaviors, such as protests, tantrums, & Wheelwright, 2004; Laugeson et  al., 2009).
and self-injurious behavior (SIB), are sometimes Without intervention, these challenges are likely
exhibited by children with ASD, particularly if to continue throughout the lifespan and impact
they are not able to effectively communicate their the individual’s quality of life. However, recent
needs or desires, when academics are challeng- research suggests that empathy and empathetic
ing, and when presented with non-preferred responses may be learned, and once demon-
activities (Koegel, Singh, et al., 2010; LaBelle & strated, improvements in other areas are noted
Charlop-Christy, 2002). Recent research by (Koegel et al., 2016). Therefore, empathy appears
Singh et al. (2018) focused on verbal and physi- to be another pivotal area.
cal aggression in children with ASD and the While empathy as a whole is complex, some
effects of teaching self-management to control aspects can be simplified and broken down into
their behavior. In this study, the adolescents with two specific behaviors that can be taught to indi-
ASD were taught to engage in breathing exer- viduals diagnosed with ASD: active listening and
cises and self-control, as opposed to engaging in asking on-topic questions (Hill, 2009; Nugent &
physical or verbal aggression. The results showed Halvorson, 1995). These two areas combined
that following the self-management training and help the individual show interest and engagement
practice, the adolescents showed statistically sig- with the conversational partner. Furthermore,
nificant changes with a decrease in both verbal individuals with ASD may have a strength in
and physical aggression. visual perception, so pairing these areas with a
visual cue has been found to be especially
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 221

b­ eneficial (Ayres & Langone, 2007; Koegel et al., however, as experience is gained, assistance can
2015; Rayner et al., 2009). be faded. Practice during conversations with sim-
Koegel et al. (2016) implemented an interven- ilarly aged peers can also be helpful, particularly
tion that utilized a visual schema to support the if the peer provides opportunities for empathetic
individual’s empathic responses, combined with responding. Video modeling has also been a help-
video feedback. The schematic consisted of three ful intervention, wherein the practice conversa-
boxes. The first box represented a statement tions are recorded and then reviewed, discussing
made by the conversational partner that consisted responses for improving future conversations
of an emotion or physical state. This provided (Koegel & Koegel, 2018). During the video mod-
support for the individual to recognize when eling sessions, it is helpful to begin by reviewing
there was an appropriate opportunity to express positive examples, wherein the individual
empathy. For example, if the conversational part- responded with appropriate empathy, then move
ner said, “I have been really stressed lately,” that to the examples that could use some improve-
was a statement that reflected an emotion (i.e., ment, and finally review additional positive
stress) and would therefore be an opportunity for examples. By sandwiching the “needs improve-
an empathetic response. The second box in the ment” between successful examples, a positive
schematic represented the first part of the indi- learning environment is created.
vidual’s expressive spoken response, which was a These strategies have been shown to improve
statement that expressed understanding of the empathetic listening and expressive verbal state-
conversational partner’s emotion or physical ments. Measurement has calculated the percent
state. For example, if the conversational partner of opportunities when the individual responds
said, “I’m so happy I talked with my brother with the on-topic empathic statement and asks an
today,” and the individual responded with, “That empathetic question (i.e., on-topic questions
sounds nice,” it showed recognition that the event when given an empathetic opportunity). Social
was a positive experience for the conversational validation has also been demonstrated after
partner. However, if the individual responded acquiring this communicative skill, as individu-
with, “That doesn’t sound like fun,” it was not als with ASD self-report improved confidence
clear the individual correctly picked up on the levels during their conversations with peers
conversational partner’s emotion. Lastly, the (Koegel et  al., 2015). Lastly, some individuals
third box in the schematic represented an oppor- have also improved on a measured empathy quo-
tunity to express additional empathic responding tient following support in this area (Baron-Cohen
and to continue the conversation with a cue to ask & Wheelwright, 2004).
a relevant question. In the above example, an
appropriate question might be, “Where does your
brother live?” or “Do you get to talk with him 12.7 Summary
often?” (Fig. 12.1).
After the visual schema is presented and Empirical evidence has shown that PRT is an
explained, responses can be practiced by provid- effective, evidence-based intervention for indi-
ing a statement of emotion or physical state and viduals with ASD. A key component to the suc-
then guiding the individual through the frame- cess of PRT is utilizing a variety of motivational
work. It is important to provide statements that strategies which can be applied to a variety of dif-
cover a variety of emotion and physical states ferent target areas. When used in combination,
(e.g., stressed, sick, excited, bored, happy, scared) these components capitalize on the individual’s
so that the individual has opportunities to prac- motivation to respond, creating an environment
tice responding to these different types of emo- for increased performance and decreased inter-
tional statements. At the beginning of intervention, fering behaviors (Koegel, Singh, et al., 2010). To
it may be necessary to help some individuals date, several pivotal areas have been researched,
come up with appropriate empathetic responses; including the important core areas of motivation,
222 L. K. Koegel et al.

Fig. 12.1  This figure shows the schematic used to with adults to improve empathetic responding during social
conversation

Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empa-


initiations, self-management, and empathy. No thy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger
doubt, other important pivotal areas will be iden- syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex
differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental
tified, and the relative importance of areas will be Disorders, 34(2), 163–175. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:j
further researched. However, at this point, the add.0000022607.19833.00
goal of PRT is to speed up the habilitation pro- Bettelheim, B. (1967). The empty fortress: Infantile
cess, with a focus on the individual with ASD’s autism and the birth of the self. Free Press.
Bradshaw, J., Steiner, A.  M., Gengoux, G., & Koegel,
strengths, to ensure that the intervention is enjoy- L.  K. (2015). Feasibility and effectiveness of very
able for both the individuals providing and early intervention for infants at-risk for autism spec-
receiving the intervention, and to make meaning- trum disorder: A systematic review. Journal of Autism
ful and widespread differences. While there is and Developmental Disorders, 45, 778–794. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­014-­2235-­2
still a great need for research to further improve Brian, J. A., Bryson, S. E., & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2015).
the interventions for individuals with ASD, there Autism spectrum disorder in infancy: Developmental
has progress in defining attainable and measur- considerations in treatment targets. Current Opinion
able goals, coordinating across environments in Neurology, 28, 117–123. https://doi.org/10.1097/
wco.0000000000000182
particularly with respect to involving families, Bryson, S.  E., McDermott, C., Rombough, V., Brian,
providing inclusive environments, and imple- J., & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2007). The autism observa-
menting effective and efficient naturalistic inter- tion scale for infants: Scale development and reli-
ventions, such as PRT (Koegel et al., 1983). ability data. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 38(4), 731–738. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10803-­007-­0440-­y
Carr, E.  G., Dunlap, G., Horner, R.  H., Koegel, R.  L.,
References Turnbull, A.  P., Sailor, W., Anderson, J.  L., Albin,
R. W., Koegel, L. K., & Fox, L. (2002). Positive behav-
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic ior support: Evolution of an applied science. Journal
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4(1), 4–16. https://
ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi. doi.org/10.1177/109830070200400102
org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 Carr, E.  G., Taylor, J.  C., & Robinson, S. (1991). The
Ashbaugh, K., Koegel, R.  L., & Koegel, L.  K. (2017). effects of severe behavior problems in children on
Increasing social integration for college students with the teaching behavior of adults. Journal of Applied
autism spectrum disorder. Behavioral Development Behavior Analysis, 24(3), 523–535. https://doi.
Bulletin, 22, 183–196. https://doi.org/10.1037/ org/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-­523
bdb0000057 Carter, C.  M. (2001). Using choice with game play to
Ayres, K.  M., & Langone, J.  A. (2007). Comparison of increase language skills and interactive behaviors in
video modeling perspectives for students with autism. children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior
Journal of Special Education Technology, 22(2), Interventions, 3(3), 131–151. https://doi.org/10.1901/
15–30. https://doi.org/10.1177/016264340702200202 jaba.1991.24-­523
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 223

Chai, G.  L. C., Anderson, A., & McLean, L.  A. (2018). Disorders, 32(5), 423–444. https://doi.org/10.102
Use of technology to support self management in 3/A:1020593922901
individuals with autism: Systematic review. Journal Horner, R.  H., Dunlap, G., Koegel, R.  L., Carr, E.  G.,
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 5, 142–155. Sailor, W., Anderson, J., Albin, R. W., & O'Neill, R. E.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-­018-­0129-­5 (1990). Toward a technology of “nonaversive” behav-
Charlop, M. H., & Milstein, J. P. (1989). Teaching autistic ioral support. Journal of the Association for Persons
children conversational speech using video modeling. with Severe Handicaps, 15(3), 125–132. https://doi.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22(3), 275–285. org/10.1177/154079699001500301
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1989.22-­275 Heimann, M., Nelson, K.  E., Tjus, T., & Gillberg, C.
Chouinard, M. M., Harris, P. L., & Maratsos, M. P. (2007). (1995). Increasing reading and communication skills
Children's questions: A mechanism for cognitive in children with autism through an interactive mul-
development. Monographs of the Society for Research timedia computer program. Journal of Autism and
in Child Development, 72(1), 1–122. https://www. Developmental Disorders, 25(5), 459–480. https://doi.
jstor.org/stable/30163594 org/10.1007/bf02178294
Clinton, E., & Clees, T. (2015). High-preference strate- Hung, D.  W. (1977). Generalization of “curios-
gies and other interspersal procedures for learners ity” questioning behavior in autistic children.
with disabilities: A review of the literature. Journal of Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Special Education Apprenticeship, 4(2), 1–27. https:// Psychiatry, 8(3), 237–245. https://doi.
scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/josea/vol 4/iss2/4 org/10.1016/0005-­7916(77)90061-­1
Doggett, R. A., Krasno, A. M., Koegel, L. K., & Koegel, Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbance of affective con-
R.  L. (2013). Acquisition of multiple questions tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
in the context of social conversation in children Koegel, L. K. (2000). Interventions to facilitate communi-
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental cation in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 43(9), 2015–2025. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Disorders, 30(5), 383–391. https://doi.org/10.102
s10803-­012-­1749-­8 3/A:1005539220932
Dunlap, G. (1984). The influence of task variation Koegel, L. K., Ashbaugh, K., Koegel, R. L., Detar, W. J., &
and maintenance tasks on the learning and affect Regester, A. (2013). Increasing socialization in adults
of autistic children. Journal of Experimental with Asperger's syndrome. Psychology in the Schools,
Child Psychology, 37(1), 41–64. https://doi. 50(9), 899–909. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.21715
org/10.1016/0022-­0965(84)90057-­2 Koegel, L. K., Ashbaugh, K., Navab, A., & Koegel, R. L.
Dunlap, G., & Koegel, R.  L. (1980). Motivating autis- (2016). Improving empathic communication skills
tic children through stimulus variation. Journal of in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 13(4), 619–627. https:// Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 921–
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1980.13-­619 933. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2633-­0
Dyer, K., Dunlap, G., & Winterling, V. (1990). Effects of Koegel, L.  K., Camarata, S.  M., Valdez-Menchaca,
choice making on the serious problem behaviors of M., & Koegel, R.  L. (1997). Setting gen-
students with severe handicaps. Journal of Applied eralization of question-­ asking by children
Behavior Analysis, 23(4), 515–524. https://doi. with autism. American Journal on Mental
org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-­515 Retardation, 102(4), 346–357. https://doi.
Fisher, W. W., Luczynski, K. C., Blowers, A. P., Vosters, org/10.1352/0895-­80517(1988)102<0346:sgoqbc>2
M.  E., Pisman, M.  D., Craig, A.  R., Hood, S.  A., .0.co;2
Machado, M.  A., Lesser, A.  D., & Piazza, C.  C. Koegel, L.  K., Carter, C.  M., & Koegel, R.  L.
(2020). A randomized clinical trial of a virtual-­training (2003). Teaching children with autism self
program for teaching applied-behavior-analysis skills initiations as a pivotal response. Topics in
to parents of children with autism spectrum disorder. Language Disorders, 23(2), 134–145. https://doi.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(4), 1856– org/10.1097/00011363-­200304000-­00006
1875. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.778 Koegel, L.  K., Koegel, R.  L., Green-Hopkins, I., &
Henninger, N.  A., & Taylor, J.  L. (2013). Outcomes in Barnes, C.  C. (2010). Brief report: Question ask-
adults with autism spectrum disorders: A histori- ing and collateral language acquisition in children
cal perspective. Autism, 17(1), 103–116. https://doi. with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
org/10.1177/1362361312441266 Disorders, 40(4), 509–515. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Hill, C. E. (2009). Helping skills: Facilitating, exploration, s10803-­009-­0896-­z
insight, and action (3rd ed.). American Psychological Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Hurley, C., & Frea, W. D.
Association. (1992). Improving social skills and disruptive behav-
Horner, R.  H., Carr, E.  G., Strain, P.  S., Todd, A.  W., ior in children with autism through self-management.
& Reed, H.  K. (2002). Problem behavior interven- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(2), 341–353.
tions for young children with autism: A research https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­341
synthesis. Journal of Autism and Developmental
224 L. K. Koegel et al.

Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Shoshan, Y., & McNerney, E. characteristics of parents of autistic children. Journal
(1999). Pivotal response intervention II: Preliminary of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51(5), 683.
long-term outcome data. Journal of the Association https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-­006x.51.5.683
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24(3), 186–198. Koegel, R.  L., & Williams, J.  A. (1980). Direct versus
https://doi.org/10.2511/rpsd.24.3.186 indirect response-reinforcer relationships in teaching
Koegel, L.  K., Navab, A., Ashbaugh, K., & Koegel, autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
R.  L. (2015). Using reframing to reduce nega- 8(4), 537–547. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00916505
tive statements in social conversation for adults LaBelle, C.  A., & Charlop-Christy, M.  H. (2002).
with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Positive Individualizing functional analysis to assess multiple
Behavior Interventions., 18, 133–144. https://doi. and changing functions of severe behavior problems
org/10.1177/1098300715596136 in children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior
Koegel, L.  K. & Openden, D. (2019). Pivotal response Interventions, 4(4), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/
treatment. In Gerenser, J., & Zampardi, M. (Eds). SLP 10983007020040040601
guide to ABA: Principles to support inter-professional Laugeson, E.  A., Frankel, F., Mogil, C., & Dillon,
collaboration on autism support teams (pp. 109–132). A.  R. (2009). Parent-assisted social skills training to
Brookes Publishing: Baltimore. improve friendships in teens with autism spectrum
Koegel, L.  K., Singh, A.  K., & Koegel, R.  L. (2010). disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Improving motivation for academics in children Disorders, 39(4), 596–606. https://doi.org/10.1007/
with autism. Journal of Autism Developmental s10803-­008-­0664-­5
Disorders., 40, 1057–1066. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Lee, S.  H., Simpson, R.  L., & Shogren, K.  A. (2007).
e608662010-­001 Effects and implications of self-management for stu-
Koegel, L. K., Singh, A. K., Koegel, R. L., Hollingsworth, dents with autism: A meta-analysis. Focus on Autism
J. R., & Bradshaw, J. (2013). Assessing and improving and Other Developmental Disabilities, 22(1), 2–13.
early social engagement in infants. Journal of Positive https://doi.org/10.1177/10883576070220010101
Behavior Interventions, 16(2), 69–80. https://doi. Lei, J., Sukhodolsky, D. G., Abdullahi, S. M., Braconnier,
org/10.1177/1098300713482977 M.  L., & Ventola, P. (2017). Reduced anxiety fol-
Koegel, L., Singh, A., Koegel, R., Hollingsworth, J., & lowing pivotal response treatment in young chil-
Bradshaw, J. (2014). Assessing and improving early dren with autism spectrum disorder. Research in
social engagement in infants. Journal of Positive Autism Spectrum Disorders, 43–44, 1–7. https://doi.
Behavior Interventions, 16(2), 69–80. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.rasd.2017.09.002
org/10.1177/1098300713482977 Levy, A., & Perry, A. (2011). Outcomes in adolescents
Koegel, R. L., Dyer, K., & Bell, L. (1987). The influence of and adults with autism: A review of the literature.
child-preferred activities on autistic children’s social Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1271–
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20(3), 1282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.023
243–252. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1987.20-­243 Lovaas, O.  I. (1966). A program for the establishment
Koegel, R. L., & Egel, A. L. (1979). Motivating autistic of speech in psychotic children. In J.  Wing (Ed.),
children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 418– Childhood autism. Pergamon Press.
426. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-­843x.88.4.418 Lovaas, I. (1981). The ME book. Pro-Ed.
Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (2018). Pivotal response Lovaas, O.  I., Freitas, L., Nelson, K., & Whalen, C.
treatment for autism spectrum disorders (2nd ed.). (1967). Building social and preschool behaviors in
Brookes Publishing. schizophrenic and autistic children through non-verbal
Koegel, R.  L., Koegel, L.  K., & Surratt, A.  V. (1992). imitation training. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
Language intervention and disruptive behavior in pre- 5, 171–181.
school children with autism. Journal of Autism and Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S.
Developmental Disorders, 22, 141–153. https://doi. (1973). Some generalization and follow-up measures
org/10.1007/bf01058147 on autistic children in behavior therapy. Journal of
Koegel, R. L., O’Dell, M., & Dunlap, G. (1988). Producing Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(1), 131–165. https://doi.
speech use in nonverbal autistic children by reinforc- org/10.1901/jaba.1973.6-­131
ing attempts. Journal of Autism and Developmental Lovaas, O.  I., Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J.  Q. (1965).
Disorders, 18(4), 525–538. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Building social behavior in autistic children by use of
bf02211871 electric shock. Journal of Experimental Research in
Koegel, R.  L., O'Dell, M.  C., & Koegel, L.  K. (1987). Personality, 1(2), 99–109.
A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal Maier, S.  F., & Seligman, M.  E. P. (1976). Learned
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental helplessness: Theory and evidence. Journal of
Disorders, 17(2), 187–200. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 3–46. https://
bf01495055 doi.org/10.1037/0096-­3445.105.1.3
Koegel, R. L., Schreibman, L., O'Neill, R. E., & Burke, Maljaars, J., Noens, I., Jansen, R., Scholte, E., & van
J.  C. (1983). The personality and family interaction Berchelaer-Onnes, I. (2011). Intentional commu-
12  Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT): Research Findings Over 30 Years 225

nication in nonverbal and verbal low-functioning autism spectrum disorder through a short-term
children with autism. Journal of Communication parent-mediated program. Journal of Autism and
Disorders, 44(6), 601–614. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Developmental Disorders, 50(10), 3728–3738. https://
jcomdis.2011.07.004 doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­020-­04426-­2
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., Mason, D., & McClannahan, Rayner, C., Denholm, C., & Sigafoos, J. (2009). Video-­
L.  E. (1983). A modified incidental teaching proce- based intervention for individuals with autism: Key
dure for autistic youth: Acquisition and generalization questions that remain unanswered. Research in Autism
of receptive object labels. Journal of Applied Behavior Spectrum Disorders., 3(2), 291–303. https://doi.
Analysis, 16(3), 329–338. https://doi.org/10.1901/ org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.09.001
jaba.1983.16-­329 Risley, T. R. (1968). The effects and side effects of pun-
McGee, G.  G., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E. ishing the autistic behaviors of a deviant child. Journal
(1985). The facilitative effects of incidental teaching of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 21–34. https://doi.
on preposition use by autistic children. Journal of org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­21
Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(1), 17–31. https://doi. Roberts, G.  J., Mize, M., Reutebuch, C.  K., Falcomata,
org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­17 T., Capin, P., & Steelman, B. L. (2019). Effects of a
McGee, G.  G., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E. self-management with peer training intervention on
(1986). An extension of incidental teaching procedures academic engagement for high school students with
to reading instruction for autistic children. Journal of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Behavioral
Applied Behavior Analysis, 19(2), 147–157. https:// Education, 28(4), 456–478. https://doi.org/10.1007/
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1986.19-­147 s10864-­018-­09317-­2
Moes, D., Koegel, R. L., Schreibman, L., & Loos, L. M. Roser, K. (1996). A review of psychoanalytic theory
(1992). Stress profiles for mothers and fathers of chil- and treatment of childhood autism. Psychoanalytic
dren with autism. Psychological Reports, 71(3), 1272– Review, 83(3), 325–341.
1274. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1992.71.3f.1272 Shvarts, S., Jimenez-Gomez, C., Bai, J. Y., Thomas, R. R.,
Mohammadzaheri, F., Koegel, L.  K., Rezaei, M., & Oskam, J.  J., & Podlesnik, C.  A. (2020). Examining
Bakhshi, E. (2015). A randomized clinical trial stimuli paired with alternative reinforcement to miti-
comparison between pivotal response treatment gate resurgence in children diagnosed with autism
(PRT) and adult-driven applied behavior analysis spectrum disorder and pigeons. Journal of the
(ABA) intervention on disruptive behaviors in public Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 113(1), 214–231.
school children with autism. Journal of Autism and https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.575
Developmental Disorders, 45(9), 2899–2907. https:// Singh, N.  N., Lancioni, G.  E., Karazsia, B.  T., Myers,
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2451-­4 R. E., Kim, E., Chan, J., Jackman, M. M., McPherson,
Mohammadzaheri, F., Koegel, L.  K., Rezaee, M., & C. L., & Janson, M. (2018). Surfing the urge: An infor-
Rafiee, S. M. (2014). A randomized clinical trial com- mal mindfulness practice for the self-management of
parison between pivotal response treatment (PRT) and aggression by adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
structured applied behavior analysis (ABA) interven- order. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 12,
tion for children with autism. Journal of Autism and 170–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.10.003
Developmental Disorders, 44(11), 2769–2777. https:// Taylor, B. A., & Harris, S. L. (1995). Teaching children
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­014-­2137-­3 with autism to seek information acquisition of novel
Nugent, W., & Halvorson, H. (1995). Testing the information and generalization of responding. Journal
effects of active listening. Research on Social of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(1), 3–14. https://doi.
Work Practice, 5(2), 152–175. https://doi. org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-­3
org/10.1177/104973159500500202 Ulke-Kurkcuoglu, B., & Kircaali-Iftar, G. (2010). A com-
Ochs, E., Kremer-Sadlik, T., Solomon, O., & Sirota, K. parison of the effects of providing activity and mate-
(2001). Inclusion as social practice: Views of children rial choice to children with autism spectrum disorders.
with autism. Social Development, 10(3), 399–419. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 717–721.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-­9507.00172 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­717
Palmen, A., Didden, R., & Lang, R. (2012). A systematic Verschuur, R., Didden, R., Lang, R., Sigafoos, J., &
review of behavioral intervention research on adap- Huskens, B. (2016). Pivotal response treatment for
tive skill building in high-functioning young adults children with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic
with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental
Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 602–617. https://doi. Disorders, 1(1), 34–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2001.10.001 s40489-­013-­0008-­z
Pope, K. L. (1999). Autism: An overview and investigation Vivar, L. A. (2016). Frustration and task avoidance in stu-
into the Lovaas technique (ED432078). ERIC. https:// dents with autism spectrum disorder.
files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED432078.pdf Wetherby, A.  M. (1986). Ontogeny of communica-
Popovic, S.  C., Starr, E.  M., & Koegel, L.  K. (2020). tive functions in autism. Journal of Autism and
Teaching initiated question asking to children with
226 L. K. Koegel et al.

Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 295–316. https://doi. Analysis, 14(1), 53–60. https://doi.org/10.1901/


org/10.1007/BF01531661 jaba.1981.12-­53
Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, Wolff, W.  M., & Morris, L.  A. (1971). Intellectual and
N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., Weil, L., & personality characteristics of parents of autistic chil-
Wakabayashi, A. (2006). Predicting autism spec- dren. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77(2), 155–
trum quotient (AQ) from the systemizing quotient-­ 161. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030754
revised (SQ-R) and empathy quotient (EQ). Brain Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig,
Research, 1079(1), 47–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. A., Kucharczyk, S., … Schultz, T. R. (2015). Evidence-­
brainres.2006.01.012 based practices for children, youth, and young adults
Williams, J.  A., Koegel, R.  L., & Egel, A.  L. (1981). with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehen-
Response-reinforcer relationships and improved learn- sive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ing in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Disorders, 45(7), 1951–1966. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10803-­014-­2351-­z
Video Modeling Instruction
for Individuals with Autism 13
Spectrum Disorder

Ruth M. DeBar, Courtney L. Kane,
and Jessica L. Amador

13.1 V
 ideo Modeling Instruction 13.1.1  Advantages of VM
for Individuals with Autism
Spectrum Disorder There are several advantages that VM may offer
when instructing individuals with
Video modeling (VM) is a well-established ASD.  Instruction incorporating VM may lessen
instructional strategy that has been used to effec- attending and language requirements, may be
tively teach individuals with autism spectrum provided in the absence of social interactions
disorder (ASD) diverse skills from commenting with clinicians or educators, and may increase
while playing games (e.g., Ezzedine et al., 2020) motivation for participation as it includes a visu-
to prosocial skills like helping (e.g., Reeve et al., ally preferred medium (i.e., videos; Sherer et al.,
2007), to abduction prevention skills (Abadir 2001) for individuals with ASD.  Additionally,
et al., 2021), to adaptive skills like mobile device video models may more readily capture mean-
usage (e.g., Horn et  al., 2021) and appropriate ingful aspects of the environment (e.g., sound,
social communicative skills (e.g., making small movement) than is permissible through vocal
talk and accepting criticism) while simultane- descriptions or static pictures, can be produced
ously teaching vocational skills (Stauch & by caregivers and practitioners, and can be used
Plavnick, 2020), and to increasing appropriate across a range of settings and situations
transitions (Cihak et al., 2010). (Schreibman et  al., 2000). VM has also been
The purpose of this chapter is to review termi- found to be resource and time efficient compared
nology of VM; review VM research across play, to in  vivo modeling (Charlop-Christy et  al.,
social communication, safety skills, functional 2000). It also helps to standardize modeling pro-
living skills, role in reducing problem behavior, cedures (Gardner & Wolfe, 2013). VM may also
caregiver-implemented VM, and evaluations prove especially advantageous to teach certain
exploring sufficient and necessary prerequisites skills where learning opportunities are limited,
to VM; and review comparative research. Areas like safety skills. For example, VM may permit
of future research will be discussed as well as safe simulation of potentially dangerous scenar-
support for VM as an evidence-based practice. ios without directly placing the participant in
harm’s way. As it is especially important for
safety skills, VM may also easily allow practitio-
R. M. DeBar (*) · C. L. Kane · J. L. Amador ners to program for generalization using strate-
Department of Applied Behavior Analysis, Caldwell
University, Caldwell, NJ, USA gies including programming common stimuli
e-mail: rdebar@caldwell.edu (Abadir et al., 2021; Carlile et al., 2018).

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 227
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_13
228 R. M. DeBar et al.

13.1.2  Terminology and/or vocalizations) and then providing the


opportunity to emit the modeled response
As discussed by Rayner et al. (2009), the efficacy (MacDonald et  al., 2015). Although VM may
of video-based instruction (VBI) including video include different types of models (e.g., peers,
modeling (VM) may best be accounted for via adults), VSM involves viewing a video recording
observational learning and imitation. VBI is a of the participant as the model who is engaged in
broad term used to encompass a range of proce- some form of adaptive behavior (Dowrick, 1999).
dures involving interventions that present videos It has been suggested that VSM may enhance
as the independent variable (Rayner et al., 2009). motivation to view the video as participants may
As shown in Table  1, subtypes of VBI may prefer viewing themselves rather than another
include video modeling (VM), video self-­ (Sherer et al., 2001). VSM can be further catego-
modeling (VSM), and video prompting (VP). rized as feedforward or as positive self-review
VM involves presenting participants with a video (Dowrick, 1999). Feedforward VSM typically
recording of a model (e.g., adults or peers) involves component skills that are in the partici-
engaged in specific scripted behaviors (actions pant’s repertoire albeit arranged in a new
sequence or context to form a new behavior. The
Table 1  Definitions of Procedural Variations of Video- behavior video recorded maybe prompted or
based Instruction reinforced. Contrastingly, positive self-review
Video-based A range of instructional procedures entails creating a video recording during which
instruction involving videos as the primary the participant’s behavior is edited so that desired
(VBI)a independent variable (Rayner et al., performance present in the repertoire is demon-
2009)
strated. Positive self-review involves presenting
Video An instructional procedure that
modeling involves presenting a video of a an exemplary performance of behavior that may
(VM)b model engaging in target behaviors be occurring below criterion level or that has
and then providing an opportunity failed to maintain. Feedforward VSM may
for the participant to emit those involve additional technological support as the
behaviors (MacDonald et al., 2015)
video is edited such that the participant views
Video An instructional procedure that
self-modeling involves presenting a video of the themselves emitting a future target not readily
(VSM)c participant themselves as the model demonstrated (Bellini & Akullian, 2007).
engaging in the target behavior and If not using self as a model, VBI may include
then providing an opportunity for
others as models (e.g., peers, adults, familiar
the participant to emit those
behaviors (Dowrick, 1999) adults; Abadir et al., 2021; Ezzedine et al., 2020;
Feedforwardc A subtype of VSM that includes Kourassanis et al., 2015). Using others as models
component skills in the participant’s may offer advantages over VSM. One, it may be
repertoire arranged into a new less effortful and efficient to use others as models
sequence or context
and then create successful performance via video
Positive A subtype of VSM that includes an
self-review c
edited video recording of editing or prompting (Sherer et  al., 2001).
participant’s responding to depict Additionally, VM may vary from perspective.
exemplary performance of the target Subjective point of view (or first-person; per-
behavior
spective of the viewer) involves presenting vid-
Video An instructional procedure in which
prompting the participant views a brief portion eos from the perspective of the participant.
(VP)a of the video and then providing an Videos presented from a subjective point of view
opportunity for the participant to typically omit a model or may show just the
engage in the target behavior before hands or relevant body parts relevant to the target
viewing the next video segment
(Rayner et al., 2009) behavior (Rayner et  al., 2009). Contrastingly,
videos filmed from a third-person perspective
a
Rayner et al. (2009)
b
MacDonald et al. (2015) (also known as the perspective of the spectator;
c
Dowrick (1999) Cannella-Malone et  al., 2006) show models of
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 229

others or of the participant engaged in a scripted skill research (e.g., Ezzedine et  al., 2020;
behavior to be imitated as if the viewer was an MacDonald et al., 2005; Palcehcka & MacDonald,
on-looker. Lastly, instead of viewing the entire 2010). Using an activity schedule with video
video recording of the targeted response, VP models, one study that targeted playing Guitar
refers to interventions during which the partici- Hero™ required participants to match colors, tol-
pant views a brief segment of the video and then erate manual prompting, and have a history with
is permitted to demonstrate the behavior modeled activity schedules (Blum-Dimaya et  al., 2010).
(Rayner et al., 2009). Although VP may at times Additional prerequisites included attending to a
be discussed in this chapter, by in large, the focus television monitor for a specified duration (Besler
will be restricted to VM. If interested more about & Kurt, 2016; Neff et  al., 2017; Sancho et  al.,
VP, please see the review conducted by Domire 2010) and delayed object imitation (Palcehcka &
and Wolfe (2014). MacDonald, 2010).

13.1.3.1 Types of Play


13.1.3  Play A variety of play skills have been taught using
VM including solitary play (Blum-Dimaya et al.,
For some children with ASD, play may not 2010; Sherrow et al., 2016), imaginative or sym-
emerge as it does for typically developing chil- bolic play (e.g., Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010;
dren; it may be ritualistic and repetitive and may D’Ateno et  al., 2003; Dupere et  al., 2013; Lee
lack imaginative themes (Boudreau & et al., 2017; MacDonald et al., 2005; MacManus
D’Entremont, 2010). VM has been found to be an et  al., 2015), and social play with peers (e.g.,
effective instructional strategy to address deficits Ezzedine et  al., 2020; Kourassanis et  al., 2015;
in play across solitary play (e.g., Blum-Dimaya MacDonald et al., 2009). An early demonstration
et  al., 2010; Sherrow et  al., 2016), imaginative of VM to address play skills with children with
play (e.g., MacDonald et al., 2005; Palcehcka & ASD was conducted by MacDonald et al. (2005).
MacDonald, 2010; Reagon et  al., 2006), and Using adults as models and third-person perspec-
social play (e.g., Ezzedine et  al., 2020; tive, two preschool boys with ASD were taught to
Kourassanis et al., 2015). engage in pretend play (motor actions and vocal-
VM has effectively taught play skills to a izations with figurines) across three play sets
diverse range of participants including preschool- (i.e., a town, a ship, and a house). Results sug-
ers (ages 3–5 years; Centers for Disease Control, gested that VM was successful at teaching
2020; e.g., Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010; sequences of imaginative play with scripted
D’Ateno et al., 2003; Hine & Woolery, 2006; Lee statements and actions across participants and
et  al., 2017, 2020; Palcehcka & MacDonald, that behaviors maintained during follow-up
2010; Reagon et  al., 2006) and young children probes. Using VM, symbolic or imaginary play
(ages 6–8  years) (e.g., Besler & Kurt, 2016; has been effectively targeted using a wide range
Dupere et  al., 2013; Ezzedine et  al., 2020; of play sets including a farm and farm animals, a
Kourassanis et al., 2015; MacDonald et al., 2005, doctor’s clinic (Lee et al., 2017), a veterinary set
2009; MacManus et al., 2015; Neff et al., 2017; (Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010), and a kitchen
Paterson & Arco, 2007). Fewer studies have set (D’Ateno et al., 2003). Additionally, VM has
included middle childhood (ages 9–11  years) effectively established video game playing of
(e.g., Blum-Dimaya et  al., 2010; Macpherson Guitar Hero™ (Blum-Dimaya et  al., 2010) and
et  al., 2015), teenagers (ages 14–17  years), or bowling via the Wii™ (Sherrow et al., 2016).
adults (18  years and over; Centers for Disease Social play has successfully been established
Control, 2020) (Sherrow et al., 2016). using VM and has included a dyad or a small
Requirements for inclusion varied across stud- group. Kourassanis et al. (2015) taught two chil-
ies. Generalized imitation has been identified as a dren with ASD to play Duck, Duck, Goose, and
prerequisite for participating in some VM play The Hokey Pokey. Generalization was assessed
230 R. M. DeBar et al.

with an untrained game, Ring Around the Rosie. 2010; D’Ateno et al., 2003; Ezzedine et al., 2020;
VM was found to be effective for teaching both MacDonald et al., 2005, 2009), peers of typical
games although the skill did not generalize to an development (e.g., Kourassanis et al., 2015; Lee
untrained game. et  al., 2020; Reagon et  al., 2006), participants
Ezzedine et al. (2020) evaluated the effects of (e.g., Lee et  al., 2017), and caregivers (i.e.,
VM across dyads of individuals with ASD across Sunyoung, 2016) have served as models.
on-task behavior and scripted statements during Most studies have included videos generated
previously mastered board games (i.e., by the experimenters (e.g., Ezzedine et al., 2020;
Candyland™, Memory™, Zingo™). Lee et al., 2017; MacDonald et al., 2005, 2009;
Generalization of skills by an untrained peer with Macpherson et al., 2015; Sancho et al., 2010) or
ASD was assessed. It was found that VM alone a parent (Besler & Kurt, 2016). To date, one
was effective for three of the six participants and study has evaluated the comparative effective-
that tangible reinforcement and prompts were ness of commercially available videos to
required for the other participants to achieve instructor-­created videos using a third-person
mastery. Further, positive outcomes were main- perspective (Palcehcka & MacDonald, 2010).
tained, and generalization was observed to Results found that instructor-created videos led
increase over baseline levels. to acquisition while the commercially available
Because they are often the most frequently videos did not and that results were maintained in
available play partner, siblings may especially be the absence of the video.
valuable as play partners for individuals with Research has found that VM alone has been
ASD (e.g., Reagon et  al., 2006; Taylor et  al., effective in establishing play skills including
1999). For example, Reagon et al. (2006) evalu- solitary (e.g., Blum-Dimaya et  al., 2010;
ated imaginative play across play sets with a sib- MacDonald et  al., 2005, 2009; Sherrow et  al.,
ling as a play partner. Generalization of skills was 2016) and social play (e.g., Ezzedine et  al.,
assessed with the participant’s mother and 2020; Kourassanis et  al., 2015). To enhance
another sibling. VM was found to be effective, teaching procedures, some studies have redi-
and skills were generalized to both the mother rected participants’ attending to the video (e.g.,
and sibling. Besler & Kurt, 2016; Paterson & Arco, 2007),
whereas others have provided a least to most
13.1.3.2 Procedural Variations of VM prompting procedure (i.e., gestural to verbal, to
Targeting Play partial physical, to full physical contingent upon
When targeting play using VM, research has var- errors; Kourassanis et al., 2015). Another study
ied across complexity of behavior, perspective, included an error correction procedure of
and type of model. The length of behavioral prompting the correct action, representing the
chains targeted has ranged from 4–6 steps (e.g., model, and providing 2  s to imitate the action
Hine & Woolery, 2006; Reagon et  al., 2006), independently (Sancho et  al., 2010). Although
10–12 actions (Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010; some studies have excluded reinforcement (e.g.,
Sancho et  al., 2010), 14–17 actions (e.g., D’Ateno et  al., 2003; Dupere et  al., 2013; Lee
MacDonald et al., 2005, 2009) to 28–40 scripted et  al., 2017; MacDonald et  al., 2005, 2009;
actions and vocalizations (e.g., MacManus et al., Palcehcka & MacDonald, 2010; Sunyoung,
2015; Palcehcka & MacDonald, 2010; Sherrow 2016), others have included praise and physical
et al., 2016). Studies have presented video mod- contact for correct responding of the target
els using the third-person perspective (e.g., behavior (e.g., Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010;
Ezzedine et al., 2020; Palcehcka & MacDonald, Kourassanis et  al., 2015; Lee et  al., 2020;
2010; Sherrow et al., 2016; Spriggs et al., 2016) Paterson & Arco, 2007). Others have provided
and first-person perspective (e.g., Sancho et  al., edibles for attending and on-task behavior (e.g.,
2010). Adults (e.g., Boudreau & D’Entremont, Blum-Dimaya et al., 2010).
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 231

13.1.3.3 Generalization A robust number of studies have explored the


and Maintenance of Play effectiveness of VM on the maintenance of play
Across play research, stimulus generalization has skills. Maintenance has been assessed as early as
widely been assessed and has included assess- 7  days post-mastery (e.g., Paterson & Arco,
ments across settings (e.g., Besler & Kurt, 2016; 2007), while other studies assessed it up to a
Blum-Dimaya et  al., 2010; Hine & Woolery, month later (e.g., Blum-Dimaya et  al., 2010;
2006; Reagon et al., 2006), people (e.g., Dupere Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010; MacDonald
et al., 2013; Ezzedine et al., 2020), and untrained et al., 2009). Most studies have reported positive
toys (e.g., Lee et al., 2017, 2020; Sancho et al., outcomes of maintenance (e.g., Besler & Kurt,
2010; Spriggs et al., 2016). Overall, studies have 2016; Ezzedine et  al., 2020; Lee et  al., 2020;
reported an increase in generalization measures Sani-Bozkurt & Ozen, 2015) though few studies
when compared to baseline (e.g., Besler & Kurt, have reported mixed outcomes of maintenance
2016; Blum-Dimaya et al., 2010; Ezzedine et al., (e.g., Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010).
2020; Hine & Woolery, 2006; Reagon et  al.,
2006). 13.1.3.4 Social Validity
Few studies targeting play skills with VM Video modeling has been found to be a socially
have directly discussed strategies to enhance gen- valid strategy to teach children with ASD play
eralization. One arrangement used to promote skills. A range of social validity measures have
generative outcomes, matrix training, has been been included in the VM play skills literature
combined with VM to establish scripted actions (e.g., Besler & Kurt, 2016; Blum-Dimaya et al.,
and vocalizations (MacManus et  al., 2015). 2010; Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010;
Matrix training is a method to organize and select Kourassanis et  al., 2015; Macpherson et  al.,
targets where some are directly taught while oth- 2015; Reagon et al., 2006; Sherrow et al., 2016)
ers are not (Curiel et al., 2020). Untrained combi- though there have been many studies that have
nations of targets are assessed to evaluate whether excluded social validity measures (e.g., D’Ateno
they have recombined in a novel way. MacManus et  al., 2003; MacDonald et  al., 2005, 2009;
et  al. (2015) evaluated the effectiveness of VM MacManus et al., 2015; Palcehcka & MacDonald,
and matrix training for teaching children with 2010). Of the studies that have included social
ASD scripted actions and vocalizations across validity measures, many have been conducted
three play scenarios and play sets. After VM with parents (e.g., Besler & Kurt, 2016; Boudreau
across three play scenarios, scripted actions and & D’Entremont, 2010; Kourassanis et al., 2015),
vocalizations recombined in novel ways across teachers or staff (e.g., Ezzedine et  al., 2020;
play sets and materials across participants. Sancho et  al., 2010), and children directly
Researchers have also evaluated the efficacy involved in the study (e.g., Sherrow et al., 2016;
of suitable loops to enhance generalization. By Spriggs et al., 2016). Respondents reported pro-
providing multiple exemplars of play to promote cedures, goals, or outcomes to be socially valid
generalization of skills to untrained play materi- (e.g., Besler & Kurt, 2016; Blum-Dimaya et al.,
als (i.e., characters), suitable loops are similar to 2010; Boudreau & D’Entremont, 2010; Ezzedine
multiple-exemplar training (Dupere et al., 2013). et al., 2020; Hine & Woolery, 2006; Kourassanis
Dupere et al. (2013) assessed the effects of video et al., 2015; Lee et al., 2020; Reagon et al., 2006;
models with scripted suitable loops with trained Sancho et al., 2010).
and untrained characters. The suitable loop per-
mitted participants to perform the same actions 13.1.3.5 Reliability
and vocalizations with trained characters that Reliability measures of the dependent and
could be appropriately performed with untrained independent variables have been robustly
characters. Results indicated that participants included in VM and play research. Studies
incorporated untrained characters into their play, have included interobserver agreement for at
but to varying degrees. least 30% of sessions (e.g., Besler & Kurt,
232 R. M. DeBar et al.

2016; Blum-Dimaya et al., 2010; Macpherson 13.1.4  Social Communication


et  al., 2015; Palechka & Arco, 2010), while
others have included 95% of sessions (e.g., Lee Deficits in social communication and social
et  al., 2017). Interobserver agreement mea- interaction include limited reciprocal conversa-
sures have been reported to be acceptable tion, reduced sharing of interest, limited initia-
(exceeding 80%, e.g., Besler & Kurt, 2016; tion and response to social interactions, deficits
Blum-Dimaya et  al., 2010; Ezzedine et  al., in nonverbal communicative behavior, and diffi-
2020; Hine & Woolery, 2006; Kourassanis culties developing and maintaining relationships
et  al., 2015; Lee et  al., 2017, 2020; Reagon which are major diagnostic criteria for ASD
et al., 2006; Sancho et al., 2010). (American Psychological Association [APA],
Similar findings have been reported for proce- 2013). VM has been used to address the core def-
dural integrity (PI). PI data have been collected icits of ASD since the late 1980s (Charlop &
for a minimum of 25–30% of sessions (e.g., Hine Milstein, 1989).
& Woolery, 2006; Kourassanis et al., 2015; Sani-­ In one of the earliest studies, Charlop and
Bozkurt & Ozen, 2015), 33–50% of sessions Milstein (1989) taught three boys with ASD to
(e.g., Blum-Dimaya et al., 2010; Ezzedine et al., engage in conversational speech using brief
2020; Lee et al., 2017, 2020) to a maximum of (45 s) video models in combination with an error
100% of sessions (Sunyoung, 2016) and were correction procedure (i.e., re-presentation of vid-
reported to be acceptable (e.g., Besler & Kurt, eos contingent upon incorrect conversational per-
2016; Blum-Dimaya et al., 2010; Ezzedine et al., formance). Subsequently, conversation skills
2020; Hine & Woolery, 2006; Kourassanis et al., were generalized to untrained conversation part-
2015; Lee et  al., 2017, 2020). Several studies ners, settings, toys, and topics of conversation.
have omitted PI measures (i.e., D’Ateno et  al., Skills were maintained up to 15  months
2003; Dupere et  al., 2013; MacManus et  al., post-acquisition.
2015; Neff et al., 2017; Paterson & Arco, 2007; Since this study, researchers have used VM to
Reagon et al., 2006). teach social initiations (Buggey, 2012), sharing
(Cardon et al., 2019; Jones et al., 2013), helping
13.1.3.6 Future Research (Reeve et al., 2007), social responsiveness (Jones
While the corpus of research supporting the et al., 2013), greetings (Kouo, 2019), social inter-
use of VM and play skills is well established, actions (Maione & Mirenda, 2006; Nikopoulos &
additional research is needed. Research explor- Keenan, 2003, 2004, 2007; Tetreault & Lerman,
ing play with siblings and peers with ASD and 2010), verbal commenting (Charlop et al., 2010),
of typical development would be valuable. joint attention (Ho et  al., 2019), and social
Given the lack of research that has explored engagement (Ho et  al., 2019). More recently,
response generalization, additional research is Kouo (2019) successfully taught five boys with
needed. There is a dearth of research that has ASD to engage in a three-step greeting skill (e.g.,
evaluated unscripted responses. Research orienting toward person, engaging in vocal greet-
should continue to explore teaching play in ing, maintaining attention toward person) using a
dyad and in small groups and consider teach- packaged intervention of VM plus reinforcement
ing games on iPads®. Lastly, when using a and error correction.
video model, it may be beneficial to fade the
video from the teaching procedures (Ezzedine 13.1.4.1 Procedural Variations of VM
et  al., 2020). Although studies have included Social Communication Skills
no-video probes during follow-up or mainte- VM to teach social communication skills has
nance probes (i.e., Boudreau & D’Entremont, included adults (e.g., Charlop et  al., 2010;
2010; Dupere et  al., 2013; Hine & Woolery, Charlop & Milstein, 1989; Maione & Mirenda,
2006; Lee et al., 2020), few studies have incor- 2006; Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003; Reeve et al.,
porated procedures to fade the video. 2007; Tetreault & Lerman, 2010), peers (e.g.,
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 233

Cardon et al., 2019; Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003, ness for individuals with ASD. Generalization of
2004, 2007), and self-models (SVM; e.g., social communication skills has been evaluated
Buggey, 2012). Ho et al. (2019) reported to match in untrained contexts (e.g., Cardon et al., 2019),
models to physical descriptions of participants with peers (e.g., Charlop et al., 2010; Jones et al.,
although they did not specify whether this 2013; Kouo, 2019; Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003,
included matching across age. Both point of view 2007), in untrained activities (e.g., Nikopoulos &
(POV; e.g., Kouo, 2019; Tetreault & Lerman, Keenan, 2003, 2004; Tetreault & Lerman, 2010),
2010) and third-person (e.g., Buggey, 2012; in untrained settings (e.g., Ho et al., 2019; Kouo,
Charlop et al., 2010; Charlop & Milstein, 1989; 2019; Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003), with
Ho et  al., 2019) video models have been used untrained people (e.g., Ho et  al., 2019; Jones
successfully. Video models ranged in duration et  al., 2013), and with untrained people in
from 18  s (e.g., Cardon et  al., 2019) to 3  min untrained locations (e.g., Charlop et  al., 2010).
(e.g., Buggey, 2012). Nikopoulos and Keenan (2007) evaluated the
VM has been effective when used in isolation generalization of social initiations, reciprocal
(e.g., Charlop et al., 2010) or as a part of an inter- play, and object engagement with a peer not
vention package (e.g., Reeve et  al., 2007). included in video models. Participants engaged
Charlop et al. (2010) were effective in teaching in similar levels of behavior with the untrained
verbal commenting to three boys with ASD using peer as they did during VM training sessions.
VM in isolation. Similarly, Jones et  al. (2013) Reeve et al. (2007) used multiple exemplars of
were effective in establishing sharing, offering verbal, nonverbal, and affective discriminative
help, or responding to requests using VM in iso- stimuli presented in video models showing an
lation. However, overall, VM in isolation has pro- adult and child of typical development to estab-
duced mixed outcomes in teaching targeted social lish a helping repertoire by children with ASD. It
communication skills (e.g., Buggey, 2012; Ho was found that helping occurred in the presence
et al., 2019; Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003, 2004). of untrained discriminative stimuli, in an
Procedural modifications including the addition untrained setting, and with an untrained
of prompts (e.g., Maione & Mirenda, 2006; instructor.
Tetreault & Lerman, 2010), error correction (e.g., Maintenance of social communication skills
Charlop & Milstein, 1989), video feedback (e.g., established via VM has been evaluated from
Maione & Mirenda, 2006), or modified videos 7  days (e.g., Maione & Mirenda, 2006) to
(e.g., Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003, 2004) have 15  months (Charlop & Milstein, 1989) post-­
improved participant performance when VM pro- mastery. Some studies have reported positive
cedures were limited in their effectiveness. For maintenance outcomes for social communication
example, Maione and Mirenda (2006) added skills taught via VM (e.g., Charlop & Milstein,
video feedback when frequencies of verbaliza- 1989; Maione & Mirenda, 2006; Nikopoulos &
tions did not increase for one activity with VM Keenan, 2004). For example, Charlop and
alone. When variability and perseverative behav- Milstein (1989) found that conversational speech
iors continued, vocal prompts and video feed- maintained up to 15  months post-acquisition.
back were added. Subsequently, responding Similar to acquisition data, some researchers
increased and prompts were faded (Maione & reported mixed outcomes of maintenance (e.g.,
Mirenda, 2006). Kouo, 2019; Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003, 2007;
Tetreault & Lerman, 2010) or that additional
13.1.4.2 Generalization prompts were needed during maintenance ses-
and Maintenance of Social sions (e.g., Cardon et al., 2019).
Communication
It is important that social communication skills 13.1.4.3 Future Research
occur in a variety of contexts and maintain over Future research should continue to evaluate best
extended periods of time to be of optimal useful- practices for using VM to teach social
234 R. M. DeBar et al.

c­ommunication skills. Due to mixed outcomes may increase the risk of getting lost, experienc-
(e.g., Buggey, 2012; Maione & Mirenda, 2006) ing an abduction attempt, or drowning, which is
and procedural modifications (e.g., Nikopoulos one of the leading causes of death in children
& Keenan, 2003; Tetreault & Lerman, 2010) with ASD (Autism Speaks, 2017; Guan & Li,
needed to establish social repertoires, it would be 2017). Further, 65% of school-aged or teenagers
valuable for researchers to identify for which with ASD report experiencing bullying (Autism
skills (e.g., greetings, sharing) and for whom Speaks, 2017). Safety skill instruction including
(e.g., learning history, characteristics) VM would abduction prevention and responding to bullying
be optimal. When additional procedures (e.g., may help prevent mitigate risks and teach chil-
prompts, error correction) are necessary for VM dren with ASD how to respond when faced with
to be successful when teaching social communi- such scenarios.
cation skills, it will be important for researchers
to identify variables or participant characteristics 13.1.5.1 Procedural Variations of VM
during which additional support is needed. To do Targeting Safety Skills
so, component evaluations of treatment pack- Researchers have successfully taught safety skills
ages, of procedural modifications, and assess- using VM since the 2000s (e.g., Akmanoglu &
ment of participant characteristics may help Tekin-Iftar, 2011). In one of the earliest studies,
facilitate decision-making. Future research Akmanoglu and Tekin-Iftar (2011) evaluated the
should continue to explore strategies that enhance effects of VM, graduated guidance, and
generalization and maintenance of social com- community-­ based instruction (CBI) to teach
munication skills to promote positive, lifelong abduction prevention skills. Participants first
social interactions across any number of social viewed the video model, and then the teacher left
situations (e.g., work, school, community events). the area, while a probe session was conducted
during which a stranger participant (i.e., univer-
sity students, graduate students, lectures, and
13.1.5  Safety Skills researcher’s friends) attempted to lure the partici-
pant. If an appropriate response did not occur, the
An emerging area that has incorporated VM with teacher returned to implement graduated guid-
individuals with ASD is safety skill instruction. ance to occasion the correct targeted abduction
Safety skill instruction with VM has effectively prevention response. This and later studies (e.g.,
established a range of skills including abduction Abadir et  al., 2021; Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar,
prevention (e.g., responding to lures; Abadir 2011; Carlile et  al., 2018; Ergenekon, 2012;
et  al., 2021; Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Godish et  al., 2017; Morgan & Miltenberger,
Godish et al., 2017), help-seeking when lost (e.g., 2017; Rex et al., 2018) successfully taught par-
Carlile et al., 2018), responding to bullying (e.g., ticipants targeted safety responses. More recently,
Rex et  al., 2018), use of first aid skills (e.g., Abadir et  al. (2021) used VM to successfully
Ergenekon, 2012), and promoting gun safety teach four individuals with ASD to differentially
(e.g., Morgan & Miltenberger, 2017). respond to lures of strangers, known persons who
Addressing safety skills for individuals with presented an incorrect safety code word, and
ASD is important as they may be especially known persons who presented a correct code
prone and susceptible to danger. Characteristic word. Although effective, procedural modifica-
features of ASD like language and communica- tions were required and included an error correc-
tion deficits (APA, 2013) may make responding tion procedure for one participant and vocal
and reporting dangerous situations to caregivers instructions for two participants. Responding
challenging. Second, it has been reported that was generalized to untrained locations, lures, and
about half of children with ASD engage in elope- people.
ment (Autism Speaks, 2017; Carlile et al., 2018; Across safety skills using VM, the number of
Centers for Disease Control, 2019). Elopement steps targeted in task analyses varied from two
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 235

(Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011) to nine (Carlile Carlile et al., 2018) to 14 years old (Carlile et al.,
et al., 2018). Using a three-step behavior chain, 2018). The youngest participants (under age 6;
Morgan and Miltenberger (2017) taught a gun Carlile et  al., 2018) learned to seek help when
safety response of not touching the gun, leaving lost using VM and simulated community envi-
the room, and telling an adult. Regardless of the ronments. Research has focused on participants
number of steps targeted, all abduction preven- in the middle childhood age group (ages
tion studies taught participants to say “No” and 6–11 years; CDC, 2020). Participants in this age
leave the area (Abadir et al., 2021; Akmanoglu & group learned to respond to abduction attempts
Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Godish et  al., 2017). Godish (e.g., Abadir et  al., 2021; Akmanoglu & Tekin-­
et  al. (2017) added the step of telling an adult; Iftar, 2011; Godish et  al., 2017), help-seeking
Abadir et al. (2021) were the first to include ask- when lost (e.g., Carlile et al., 2018), first aid skills
ing for and responding to correct and incorrect (e.g., Ergenekon, 2012), responses to bullying
code words. Carlile et al. (2018) used technology (e.g., Rex et al., 2018), and firearms safety (e.g.,
across skills targeted by teaching participants to Morgan & Miltenberger, 2017). Although safety
either answer or make a FaceTime™ call and skill research included a range of participant
show their location. They also taught low-tech ages, inclusionary criteria for participation varied
help-seeking behaviors which included handing across studies. Several studies did not specify
an identification card to a store employee. The what criteria participants had to meet prior to par-
range of steps targeted across studies demon- ticipation (e.g., Godish et  al., 2017). Most
strates that VM can be used effectively in teach- research frequently required some level of imita-
ing relatively short (e.g., Akmanoglu & tion skills for participation (e.g., Abadir et  al.,
Tekin-Iftar, 2011) to complex safety responses 2021; Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Carlile
(e.g., Carlile et al., 2018). et al., 2018). Attending to videos (e.g., Ergenekon,
VM instruction has been conducted in a range 2012), waiting (e.g., Abadir et  al., 2021;
of settings including participant’s homes (e.g., Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011), discrimination
Morgan & Miltenberger, 2017), classrooms (e.g., skills (e.g., Abadir et  al., 2021; Akmanoglu &
Abadir et  al., 2021), and community settings Tekin-Iftar, 2011), and direction following (e.g.,
(e.g., Godish et al., 2017). Safety skills taught via Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011) were each also
VM in classrooms were then evaluated in reported.
untrained settings (e.g., community playground; Video characteristics (e.g., model, voice-over,
e.g., Abadir et al., 2021) or trained in situ in the duration) have varied with successful outcomes.
community (e.g., Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, Studies have almost exclusively included peer
2011). Some researchers have conducted ses- models (e.g., Abadir et al., 2021; Akmanoglu &
sions in the relevant natural setting. For example, Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Ergenekon, 2012; Godish
Morgan and Miltenberger (2017) trained gun et  al., 2017; Morgan & Miltenberger, 2017).
safety responses in the home setting, a place When targeting bullying skills, Rex et al. (2018)
likely where a child might encounter a gun. included both teenage and adult models within
Settings were extended to untrained rooms or their video models. The duration of videos ranged
community settings to evaluate stimulus general- from 7 s (Abadir et al., 2021) to 6 min (Godish
ization as well (e.g., Carlile et al., 2018). et  al., 2017). Some studies included voice-over
Individuals with ASD (e.g., Abadir et  al., (e.g., Godish et al., 2017; Morgan & Miltenberger,
2021; Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Carlile 2017); however, most studies reviewed did not
et  al., 2018; Ergenekon, 2012; Godish et  al., specify whether voice-over or text captions were
2017; Morgan & Miltenberger, 2017; Rex et al., included.
2018) and multiple disabilities (e.g., Akmanoglu Procedural components (e.g., prompts, error
& Tekin-Iftar, 2011) were represented across correction) have also varied across studies. VM
studies targeting safety skills. Participants were in isolation was effectively used (Rex et al., 2018)
mostly male and ranged in age from 3 (e.g., to teach assertive responses in response to
236 R. M. DeBar et al.

b­ ullying scenarios. Error correction procedures, confederate or researcher within the line of sight
if included, have involved representing the video (e.g., Carlile et  al., 2018; Godish et  al., 2017).
model and/or prompts to complete steps. For Few studies have evaluated potential negative
example, Carlile et  al. (2018) interrupted the side effects of participation (Godish et al., 2017;
error, presented the video model demonstrating Rex et al., 2018). One study reported terminating
the correct step performance, and provided sessions when negative side effects were
another opportunity to complete the step. Studies observed; however, no sessions were terminated
that included reinforcement during VM or in situ (Rex et al., 2018). Another study asked parents to
probes often used praise for correct responding rate whether any negative side effects were expe-
(e.g., Ergenekon, 2012). Akmanoglu and Tekin-­ rienced by participants at the end of the study
Iftar (2011) provided praise and an edible for cor- (Godish et  al., 2017). Outcomes indicated that
rect responding which was faded to the end of participants did not demonstrate any negative
session upon achieving mastery criterion. Abadir side effects. In fact, no studies have reported neg-
et al. (2021) specifically indicated that contrived ative side effects from the use of VM to teach
reinforcers were omitted, while remaining stud- safety skills; however, it is ethically important
ies did not specify the use of reinforcement. that these effects are monitored so modifications
Multiple component treatment packages con- can be made if participants demonstrate them.
sisted of VM plus prompts and reinforcement Confederates are individuals recruited by the
(e.g., Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Morgan & experimenter who are trained to implement spe-
Miltenberger, 2017). For example, when teaching cific portions of the study, while participants are
gun safety, 10 s delays were incorporated within unaware that this individual is a part of the study.
video models to allow participants the opportu- Within the safety skill VM research, confederates
nity to respond and caregivers to provide prompts have been used to help ensure safety of partici-
and praise for correct responding (Morgan & pants. For example, Carlile et al. (2018) had con-
Miltenberger, 2017). POV VM was used to suc- federates positioned where they could monitor
cessfully teach a high- and low-tech help-seeking participant safety during all community-based
response when lost in a store (Carlile et al., 2018). sessions. Within abduction prevention studies,
No other studies were reported to use POV VM. confederates were trained to act as “strangers” or
Few studies reported procedural modifications “known persons” to present lures to participants
when VM alone or VM and in situ training were during in situ assessment or training sessions
not effective in teaching or maintaining safety (e.g., Abadir et  al., 2021; Akmanoglu & Tekin-­
skills. Godish et  al. (2017) first added in situ Iftar, 2011; Godish et  al., 2017). Abadir et  al.
training for participants who did not engage in (2021) recruited 35 adults, whom they trained via
safety responses during follow-up in situ assess- behavior skills training to serve as both “strang-
ments during in situ training. For one participant, ers” and “known persons” to deliver lures across
an edible was presented when correct responding participants and conditions. Akmanoglu and
to lures did not occur. The limited need for proce- Tekin-Iftar (2011) recruited 4 adults to partici-
dural modifications suggests that VM procedures pate in video models and 27 adults to serve as
as described were successful in teaching safety “strangers” during instruction, generalization,
responses without additional modifications. and maintenance. Confederates were limited to
Given that safety skill instruction involves the two non-consecutive sessions per week, seem-
presentation of potentially dangerous situations, ingly to maintain unfamiliarity. Godish et  al.
safety monitoring and evaluation of negative side (2017) reported that men and women in their 30s
effects are important considerations. VM studies participated during in situ assessment sessions.
have reported to monitor the safety of partici- No confederates were reported across gun safety,
pants during community sessions by having a fire safety, bullying, or first aid skills studies.
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 237

13.1.5.2 Generalization VM sessions, provided prompts and reinforce-


and Maintenance of Safety ment, and set up all in situ probes during gun
Skills safety instruction (Morgan & Miltenberger,
Safety skill instruction incorporating VM has 2017). Godish et  al. (2017) trained parents to
robustly programmed for and assessed stimulus implement in situ training for participants who
generalization. Generalization of safety responses did not engage in correct abduction prevention
has been assessed across locations (e.g., responses during in situ probes. Caregivers were
Akmanoglu & Tekin-iftar, 2011), materials (e.g., also included in post-intervention sessions (e.g.,
Ergenekon, 2012), untrained people (e.g., Abadir Carlile et  al., 2018). Carlile et  al. (2018) asked
et  al., 2021), and in situ probes (e.g., Godish caregivers to implement one community-based
et al., 2017). Across studies, generalization was post-intervention session during which they
programmed via multiple exemplar training (e.g., walked or looked away from the participant to
Godish et al., 2017) and programming common evaluate whether the participant would use the
stimuli (e.g., Carlile et  al., 2018). To be func- high- or low-tech help-seeking behaviors taught.
tional, skills must be demonstrated under
untrained situations including across settings, 13.1.5.3 Social Validity
with people, and variations of relevant features of The social acceptability of goals for safety stud-
the environment. We were unable to identify any ies, procedures to teach safety skills, and out-
studies that have included strategies for program- comes of these studies is important when
ming for or assessing response generalization. considering how to apply studies clinically or
Safety skills must maintain over the course an identify avenues for future research. Social
individual’s life, especially as opportunities to validity of procedures has been overwhelmingly
practice skills acquired in the natural setting will positive indicating that stakeholders value this
presumably be infrequent. The evaluation of method for teaching safety skills to individuals
maintenance using VM to target safety skills has with ASD (e.g., Abadir et al., 2021; Akmanoglu
been included across the majority of safety skill & Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Carlile et al., 2018; Godish
research and has occurred 1 (e.g., Morgan & et al., 2017). Parents (e.g., Abadir et al., 2021;
Miltenberger, 2017) to 11  weeks post-mastery Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Carlile et  al.,
(e.g., Godish et  al., 2017). Ergenekon (2012) 2018; Ergenekon, 2012; Godish et  al., 2017),
evaluated the maintenance of first aid skills 2, 4, school staff (Carlile et  al., 2018), teachers
and 6 weeks post-mastery with positive outcomes (Abadir et  al., 2021), first responders (Carlile
across participants and skills. Results of mainte- et  al., 2018), and Board Certified Behavior
nance evaluations suggest that abduction preven- Analysts™ (Abadir et al., 2021) have served as
tion responses (e.g., Abadir et  al., 2021; respondents assessing studies’ procedures,
Akmanoglu & Tekin-Iftar, 2011; Godish et  al., goals, and outcomes. For example, Akmanoglu
2017), gun safety responses (e.g., Morgan & and Tekin-Iftar (2011) asked parents to evaluate
Miltenberger, 2017), first aid skills (e.g., goals, procedures, and outcomes using a
Ergenekon, 2012), and help-seeking when lost researcher-developed questionnaire. Parents
(e.g., Carlile et  al., 2018) were maintained at rated procedures, goals, and outcomes posi-
mastery criteria levels when assessed. tively. Ergenekon (2012) included normative
Although the inclusion of caregivers or peers data as social comparisons and included 20 first
may facilitate generalization or maintenance as and third graders who completed the targeted
these individuals may provide natural communi- first aid skills. Performance of these children
ties of reinforcement or may serve as common was compared to that of study participants.
stimuli in the generalization environment, few Study participants performed better than the
studies included caregivers (e.g., Carlile et  al., control group on targeted first aid skills;
2018; Godish et al., 2017; Morgan & Miltenberger, researchers concluded participant performance
2017). When present, caregivers implemented was socially acceptable.
238 R. M. DeBar et al.

13.1.5.4 Future Research surveys, interviews, parental report, or evalua-


Although the VM literature has covered a large tions of behavioral indicators (e.g., crying) dur-
breadth of safety skills, additional research is ing sessions. Finally, response generalization
needed to replicate and extend research across should also be considered in future research and
additional participants, stimuli, and behaviors instruction as there may be multiple effective
(e.g., safely responding to dangerous substances). behaviors that would result in ultimate safety in
Research should seek to include participants any given situation. For example, after learning
from other age groups as safety skills are likely to apply a bandage to a small cut on the arm, it
still an important skill for those individuals. would be functional to apply bandages to differ-
Specifically, adults may require training in safety ent body parts when injured or to cuts of different
skills as they gain increased independence in the sizes.
home and community. Given the dearth of
research across these areas and the need to
enhance external validity, future research should 13.1.6  Functional Living Skills
replicate and extend research exploring the effec-
tiveness of video modeling on bullying, first aid, A broad range of functional living skills have
and dangerous stimuli. Future research should been addressed through VM including cleaning
also seek to evaluate the use of VM interventions (e.g., Aldi et al., 2016), vocational and employ-
to teach responses to cyber bullying, swimming ment skills (e.g., Allen et al., 2010; Bross et al.,
skills or water safety, fire safety, how to call 911, 2020; English et  al., 2017), self-help skills like
sexual abuse prevention, on-line predatory snack making (Shrestha et  al., 2013), toileting
behavior, street crossing, and other dangerous skills (Drysdale et  al., 2015), oral hygiene
stimuli (e.g., poison, chemicals, medicine, suspi- (Popple et  al., 2016), and skills that provide
cious packages). These areas are lacking from the access to the greater community (e.g., teaching
current VM literature but remain important for a mobile device use, Horn et  al., 2021, and com-
functional repertoire of safety skills for individu- pleting exercise behaviors in a community fitness
als with ASD and to reduce risk of harm or injury. center, Pinter et  al., 2021). Examples of voca-
If individuals can safely respond to dangerous tional skills targeted include acting as a mascot
situations in the absence of caregivers, they may and engaging with customers (e.g., waving, shak-
require less supervision and have greater oppor- ing hands, giving high-fives; Allen et al., 2010),
tunities for independent living, travel, or work. customer service skills (Bross et al., 2020), and
Limited information regarding pre-experimental vocational gardening (English et al., 2017).
assessments or participant history with VM leads
to additional questions regarding sufficient and 13.1.6.1 Procedural Variations of VM
necessary prerequisite skills for successfully Targeting Functional Living
responding to VM. Prerequisite skill assessments Skills
will allow for clinicians to determine whether Participants have included young children of 4
their clients would potentially learn this skill. and 5 years old (Drysdale et al., 2015; Shrestha
This information may be increasingly important et al., 2013) and participants up to young teenag-
as duration of videos and lengths of chained ers (Popple et  al., 2016) although most studies
behaviors to be taught may vary greatly across included young adults (Pinter et al., 2021; Stauch
the type of safety skills; thus, the necessary pre- & Plavnick, 2020) and adults (Aldi et al., 2016;
requisite skills may also vary. Future research Allen et  al., 2010; Bross et  al., 2020; English
should seek to include additional evaluations of et  al., 2017; Horn et  al., 2021). Settings have
possible negative effects resulting from study included participant’s homes (e.g., Aldi et  al.,
participation. These could include ongoing mea- 2016; Drysdale et al., 2014; Popple et al., 2016),
sures of potential side effects (e.g., aversive reac- employment settings (e.g., Allen et  al., 2010;
tions, undesirable generalization) via formal Bross et  al., 2020; English et  al., 2017), a
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 239

u­ niversity (Horn et  al., 2021), and a healthcare device usage, assessed generalization in an
agency (Stauch & Plavnick, 2020). Video models untrained setting and with an untrained mobile
have largely been presented on mobile devices— device. Few studies have explicitly identified
iPad®, iPods®, or iPhones® (e.g., Aldi et  al., strategies to program for generalization. Using
2016; English et  al., 2017; Horn et  al., 2021; VM to target vocational and social skills (i.e.,
Pinter et al., 2021), which enhances community small talk, accepting criticism, accepting a com-
access and social validity. Further, most studies pliment) for two adolescents with ASD, Stauch
have included voice-over and/or text (e.g., Aldi and Plavnick (2020) incorporated multiple exem-
et  al., 2016; Bross et  al., 2020; Drysdale et  al., plars of models, social partners, materials, and
2014; English et  al., 2017; Horn et  al., 2021; vocal statements by creating three to five varia-
Pinter et al., 2021). tions of videos per skill. Although both partici-
Researchers have taught functional living pants acquired skills, vocational skills were
skills using POV VM (Aldi et al., 2016; English acquired more readily than the social skills.
et  al., 2017; Shrestha et  al., 2013), third-person
perspective (e.g., Bross et al., 2020; Pinter et al., 13.1.6.3 Reliability
2021; Stauch & Plavnick, 2020), or a combina- Reliability of the dependent variables has been
tion of both (Allen et al., 2010; Drysdale et al., robustly included in the research (e.g., Aldi et al.,
2015; Horn et al., 2021). For example, Aldi et al. 2016; Allen et  al., 2010; Bross et  al., 2020;
(2016) used POV VM with voice-over instruction English et  al., 2017; Horn et  al., 2021; Pinter
presented on an iPad® to teach three different et al., 2021). Data have been collected minimally
activities of daily living (e.g., making a snack, for 20% of sessions (e.g., Bross et  al., 2020;
setting the table, and folding jeans) for two Drysdale et  al., 2014) often exceeding minimal
18-year-old males with ASD.  Video models standards (e.g., Horn et  al., 2021; Stauch &
showed hands and arms of family members com- Plavnick, 2020); data have been appropriate.
pleting tasks. Across participants, tasks were suc- Reporting of procedural integrity data has been
cessfully mastered. One participant maintained under-reported (e.g., Aldi et  al., 2016; Allen
responding in the absence of the video model. et  al., 2010; English et  al., 2017; Pinter et  al.,
Horn et al. (2021) employed a combination of 2021; Popple et al., 2016). When included, data
first- and third-person perspectives combined have been collected across an appropriate per-
with voice- and text-over instruction to teach centage of sessions (e.g., Shrestha et  al., 2013;
receiving a phone call, sending a text message, Stauch & Plavnick, 2020), and the data have been
and initiating a phone call to adults with ASD. The acceptable.
intervention effectively led to the acquisition of
the targeted skills across participants. 13.1.6.4 Future Research
Although intervention outcomes have been Although there have been some diverse areas
positive, few studies have evaluated VM in isola- addressed within functional living skills, contin-
tion (e.g., Aldi et  al., 2016; Allen et  al., 2010; ued research is needed. Vocational research is
Popple et  al., 2016). Many have included error needed with participants who are prevocational
correction and prompts (e.g., Horn et  al., 2021; (e.g., at least 14 years of age). Future researchers
Pinter et al., 2021), while others have incorporated may wish to explore conditions during which
forward chaining (e.g., Shrestha et al., 2013). voice-over or text-over are most effective.
Additionally, it will be important for future
13.1.6.2 Generalization research to explore optimal treatment compo-
Generalization measures have mainly included nents that produce best outcomes and how treat-
stimulus generalization (e.g., Bross et al., 2020; ment components may be informed by participant
English et al., 2017; Horn et al., 2021; Shrestha characteristics. Also, it will be important that
et al., 2013; Stauch & Plavnick, 2020). For exam- reliability of the independent variable is
ple, Horn et  al. (2021), who targeted mobile collected.
240 R. M. DeBar et al.

13.1.7  Addressing Problem pets) and/or background effects (e.g., applause)


Behaviors to enhance attending. It was concluded that the
VSM produced strong outcomes for reducing
An innovative application of VM with individu- aggressive behavior and moderate support for
als with ASD has been addressing problem increasing a replacement behavior.
behaviors including aggression (Buggey, 2005; Research findings exploring the use of VM to
Sadler, 2019a, b), disruptions (Schreibman et al., decrease problem behavior and/or increasing
2000), off-task behaviors (Coyle & Cole, 2004), compliance and on-task behaviors have several
transitions (Schreibman et  al., 2000; Taber-­ areas of strength. Overall, findings indicate that
Doughty et  al., 2013), and increasing on-task VM can lead to positive outcomes related to
behaviors (Schatz et  al., 2016) and compliance reducing problem (e.g., Sadler, 2019a, b) or
(Diorio et  al., 2019). Participants have been as increasing appropriate behaviors (e.g., Diorio
young as 3  years old (Schreibman et  al., 2000) et  al., 2019) across school (e.g., Buggey, 2005)
and have included elementary-aged participants and community settings (e.g., Taber-Doughty
(e.g., Cihak et  al., 2010), middle-school-aged et al., 2013). Positive outcomes were observed in
participants (Buggey, 2005), and high schoolers the absence of reinforcement for the targeted
(Taber-Doughty et al., 2013). Interventions have response although few did incorporate it (praise;
been implemented in variety of school (e.g., Cihak et al., 2010). Several researchers provided
Diorio et al., 2019; Sadler, 2019a, b; Schatz et al., praise, a tangible (e.g., Schreibman et al., 2000),
2016) and community settings (e.g., Schreibman or an edible for video viewing (Diorio et  al.,
et  al., 2000; Taber-Doughty et  al., 2013). For 2019), whereas others embedded preferred songs,
example, Taber-Doughty et al. (2013) evaluated animated characters (Sadler, 2019b), or praise
the effectiveness of a self-operated (i.e., partici- (e.g., Buggey, 2005) in the videos. Moreover,
pant initiated) VM with voice-over presented on positive outcomes were achieved with VM alone
an iPad® on independent task completion, dura- (Schatz et  al., 2016; Schreibman et  al., 2000).
tion of task transitions, and independent transi- Some research incorporated error correction pro-
tions with four high school students implemented cedures involving representing the video (e.g.,
in a schoolwork room, grocery store, and bowl- Diorio et al., 2019), use of a modified system of
ing alley. least prompts (Taber-Doughty et al., 2013), or a
combination of representing the video with least
13.1.7.1 Procedural Variations of VM to most prompts (e.g., Cihak et  al., 2010). No
Targeting Functional Living studies, to our knowledge, made procedural mod-
Skills ifications to achieve favorable outcomes.
Research addressing problem behaviors has pre- Maintenance of the intervention effects had been
dominately included VSM (e.g., Sadler, 2019a, assessed across several days (Sadler, 2019b) to
b), while few researchers have used a third-­ 2  weeks (e.g., Coyle & Cole, 2004), 3  weeks
person perspective (e.g., Taber-Doughty et  al., (Buggey, 2005), 1  month (Schreibman et  al.,
2013; Schatz et al., 2016). Sadler (2019b) evalu- 2000), and 9  weeks (e.g., Cihak et  al., 2010).
ated the efficacy of using VSM to reduce aggres- There is preponderance of support for problem or
sion and establish replacement behaviors (e.g., adaptive behaviors established through VM to be
requesting a break or an item) with three children maintained (e.g., Schreibman et  al., 2000) fol-
with ASD. The video self-model showed the par- lowing the termination of the intervention
ticipant engaged in a socially appropriate behav- although a single study reported mixed findings
ior (or a replacement behavior) during conditions (Schatz et al., 2016). Measures of reliability for
that typically evoked problem behavior. the dependent variable(s) were robustly reported.
Additionally, the videos included a title page and Across students, interobserver agreement data
a participant-specific preferred song with an ani- were collected for an acceptable percentage of
mated character (i.e., Baby Einstein sock pup- sessions, and mean IOA was at least 80%.
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 241

13.1.7.2 Future Research 13.1.8  Caregiver Implemented


Although this is an interesting application of
VM, there are areas that future researchers Some VM research has focused upon caregiver
may wish to explore. One, it would be impor- implementation (e.g., Besler & Kurt, 2016;
tant to incorporate best practices when assess- Cardon, 2012; Clark et al., 2020). Cardon (2012)
ing problem behavior including the evaluated whether caregivers could be taught to
incorporation of functional behavioral assess- implement an imitation protocol, Video Modeling
ments. None of the aforementioned studies Imitation Training (VMIT), targeting five one-­
included an experimental functional analysis step imitative actions with young children with
to inform interventions or replacement behav- ASD. It was found that caregivers could learn to
iors targeted, whereas few studies referred to create video models and could implement the
the inclusion of functional behavioral assess- protocol with integrity (i.e., at least 95% of ses-
ments (Sadler, 2019a, b), even though these sions). With VMIT, imitation skills increased
components are best practices for treating (two met mastery criteria), and during follow-up
challenging behaviors. Most of the research probes, skills were maintained and generalized to
has been conducted with children (e.g., Diorio targets modeled in vivo.
et  al., 2019; Schreibman et  al., 2000). It Targeting solitary play, Besler and Kurt (2016)
behooves researchers to explore the effective- trained parents to prepare a task analysis, create
ness of VM with additional participants, espe- materials, select the model, choose the location,
cially adolescents and adults. Further, it create the video, and transfer it to a computer. It
would be important to explore VM across was found that parents implemented VM with a
additional targets behavior (e.g., self-injuri- level of high integrity and successfully taught
ous behaviors, stereotypy) and across addi- building a Lego™ train.
tional settings (e.g., homes, residential In another study implemented by parents,
placements). Several studies included porta- written instructions and VM were evaluated on
ble technology like the use of iPad® and parent-implemented feeding interventions for
iPods® (e.g., Cihak et al., 2010; Diorio et al., children with ASD to address food selectivity
2019; Taber-Doughty et  al., 2013). Use of (Clark et al., 2020). Written instructions and VM
portable technology should be further were effective for one caregiver to achieve mas-
explored and may offer additional advantages tery, while two caregivers required in vivo mod-
including accessibility in community settings eling and feedback.
and increased social validity. Most research
reviewed did not actively include parents
(e.g., Buggey, 2005; Cihak et al., 2010); none 13.1.9  Prerequisite Skill
included siblings or peers (except as a model;
Buggey, 2005). Incorporating caregivers, sib- Evaluating prerequisite skills sufficient and nec-
lings, and peers should be a research priority, essary for acquisition of skills through VM has
as they can help maintain favorable outcomes more recently been explored (MacDonald et al.,
in the natural environment. Because most of 2015; Tereshko et  al., 2010). It has been sug-
the research did not assess nor program for gested that attending to a video or model and an
generalization, it is important that this be an imitative repertoire may be an important prereq-
area of focus for future researchers. Reliability uisite to acquiring skills through VM (McCoy &
measures for the independent variables were Hermansen, 2007). As discussed by Tereshko
lagging in the aforementioned studies (e.g., et  al. (2010), remembering (or successive
Buggey, 2005; Sadler, 2019a, b; Schatz et al., discriminations) viewed behaviors modeled in
­
2016) although, when included, data were the video prior to the opportunity to emit the
appropriate (e.g., Cihak et al., 2002; Coyle & scripted behaviors likely impacts acquiring skills
Cole, 2010). from a video model. Use of delayed match-to-
242 R. M. DeBar et al.

sample procedures permits assessment of this 13.1.10  Comparative Research


repertoire and has been used in past research to
assess memory (MacDonald et al., 2015). The comparative effectiveness of VM has been
To further explore the role of prerequisite explored across procedural variations of VM
skills, Tereshko et  al. (2010) assessed skills (e.g., Miltenberger & Charlop, 2015; Sancho
deemed important to learning from VM. Second, et  al., 2010) and types of models incorporated
they evaluated a segmented VM procedure (e.g., Cihak & Schrader, 2008; Marcus & Wilder,
involving forward chaining on teaching an 2009; Sherer et  al., 2001) and compared to VP
eight-­step behavioral chain to participants who (e.g., Cannella-Malone et  al., 2006, 2011;
were unable to learn from traditional Mechling et al., 2014; Thomas et al., 2020) and
VM. During the segmented VM procedure, the other instructional strategies (Cardon & Wilcox,
full video model was broken down into seg- 2011; Cihak, 2011; Charlop-Christy et al., 2000;
mented steps. Across sessions, the first step of McDowell et al., 2015).
the model was presented, and then the partici- Procedural variations of VM have been
pant was provided the opportunity to complete explored across the timing of a video model and
that step. Once mastered, steps systematically the use of reinforcement and prompts (Sancho
increased in a forward chain progression. et  al., 2010) and display of the video model
Regarding prerequisite skills, they pre-experi- (Miltenberger & Charlop, 2015). Sancho et  al.
mentally evaluated motor imitation, action with (2010) compared the effectiveness of two proce-
objects, delayed and simultaneous match-to- dural variations of VM on play skills with two
sample performance (i.e., picture to object, 5-year-old children with ASD: one that presented
computer screen to object, delayed picture to the video model and then permitted the partici-
object, delayed computer screen to object), pant to engage in the scripted behavior with no
motor skills, and attending to videos. It was prompting or reinforcement (priming) and the
found that three of the four participants demon- other involved presenting the video model while
strated deficits in delayed match-to-sample per- the opportunity to emit the scripted behavior was
formance (i.e., delayed picture to object, simultaneously provided and included prompts
delayed computer screen to object) although for the targeted response and reinforcement
they demonstrated mastery across motor and (simultaneous). Although both conditions lead to
object imitation and attending to a video. While acquisition and maintenance of scripted behav-
these participants were unable to learn from tra- iors across both participants, one participant
ditional VM, the use of a segmented video acquired scripted responses more efficiently in
model was effective at establishing an eight- the simultaneous condition, whereas, for the
step chain. other participant, comparable outcomes were
MacDonald et  al. (2015) extended Tereshko observed across both variations.
et al. (2010) by further exploring the role of pre- Miltenberger and Charlop (2015) evaluated
requisite skills on the acquisition of VM across the effectiveness of VM across five children with
29 children with ASD.  Results indicated that ASD across a range of skills (e.g., pretend play,
delayed object imitation was positively corre- asking questions, interactive play) when pre-
lated with success with an eight-step VM task. In sented on a television (20  inches) compared to
addition, participants attended to the video irre- presented on an iPad® (9.5 × 7.31 × 0.37). It was
spective of their success with VM; this suggests found that both procedures were effective in
attending to a video is not sufficient albeit neces- establishing the targeted skills, that skills gener-
sary for learning through VM (e.g., McCoy & alized across people and settings, and were
Hermansen, 2007). As discussed by MacDonald ­maintained across participants; however, for four
and colleagues, future researchers may wish to of the five participants, VM presented on the tele-
explore which aspects of delayed object imitation vision was marginally more efficient than when
may be critical to learning through VM.
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 243

presented on the iPad® but generalization and videos at home by the parents three times a day
maintenance outcomes favored VM on the iPad®. for 2 consecutive days prior to intervention.
These viewings were implemented by the parent
13.1.10.1 Model Type in the absence of independent observers. The
There has been interest in whether the type of extent to which these presession viewings
model (e.g., adult, peers, self) may impact acqui- impacted the outcomes remains unknown.
sition with individuals with ASD when using VM
(Cihak & Schrader, 2008; Marcus & Wilder, 13.1.10.2 V  ideo Modeling and Video
2009; Sherer et  al., 2001). One of the first to Prompting
explore this area, Sherer et al. (2001) compared Few researchers have evaluated the efficacy of
the efficacy of self as a model compared to a peer VM with VP for individuals with ASD (Cannella-­
model on answering questions within a conversa- Malone et al., 2006, 2011; Mechling et al., 2014;
tion with five male children with autism. The Thomas et  al., 2020). Cannella-Malone et  al.
researchers concluded that use of self as a model (2006) were the first to explore this question on
is as effective as using another as a model. Three daily living skills (i.e., setting a table and putting
participants demonstrated comparable acquisi- away a groceries) with six adults with develop-
tion across both conditions; one participant mental disabilities, the majority of which were
acquired skills more readily during the self as a also diagnosed with an ASD.  Videos were cre-
model, whereas another participant acquired the ated from the perspective of the spectator and
skill more efficiently in the peer video modeling included voice-over instruction. For VP, duration
condition. Participants who acquired the skill ranged from 10 to 42 s, whereas it ranged from
also generalized skills in the presence of an 1 min 37 s to 2 min 42 s during VM. Results indi-
untrained peer and in an untrained setting. cated the VP led to acquisition across both tasks
Cihak and Schrader (2008) evaluated the effi- and participants; contrastingly, VM did not lead
cacy and efficiency of learning vocational chains to mastery across any tasks or participants.
via VSM as compared to other as a model (adult) Extending and replicating Cannella-Malone
with four young adults with ASD.  Tasks were et al. (2006, 2011) compared the efficacy of VP
vocational or prevocational tasks and included and VM on daily living skills (i.e., washing
10–12 steps. Across conditions, voice-over was dishes and doing the laundry) with seven adoles-
included in the videos, prompts were provided cents with severe intellectual disabilities, six of
contingent upon errors, and praise was delivered whom were dually diagnosed with autism.
for independent correct responding. Results Results again favored VP over VM for six of the
found that both model types were effective, and seven participants. For one participant, neither
skills were maintained during a 3- and 6-week procedure was effective in teaching the skills,
follow-up. Further, results slightly favored VSM although in  vivo modeling led to acquisition. It
for three of four participants, but differences should also be noted that generalization, mainte-
were marginal. nance, and social validity were omitted across
Marcus and Wilder (2009) evaluated the effi- both studies.
cacy of peer modeling compared to VSM with Other researchers have compared the effec-
three children with autism on correct responding tiveness of VM, VP, and a variation of VM
to Greek and Arabic letters. Across participants, described as continuous video modeling (CVM)
targets in the VSM were acquired first; only one with young adults with moderate intellectual dis-
participant met mastery in the peer modeling abilities, including one participant with autism,
condition. Despite these findings, there are some across daily living tasks (i.e., putting items away,
limitations of the study that future researchers cleaning, and folding tasks) using videos with a
may wish to explore. Maintenance and general- subjective point of view and voice-over instruc-
ization were omitted. Further, to acclimate par- tion (Mechling et  al., 2014). During CVM, the
ticipants to the videos, participants were shown video was continuously looped until the task was
244 R. M. DeBar et al.

completed. Participants could complete the step in that in  vivo modeling with prompting was
simultaneously with the video or wait until it found to be effective for three of the four partici-
replayed to complete the step if previously omit- pants. However, measures of generalization and
ted. Across participants and tasks, all procedures maintenance were omitted.
increased performance although VP was superior
to both VM and CVM although CVM was found 13.1.10.4 V  ideo Modeling and Other
to be more effective than VM. Instructional Procedures
In extending past research comparing the effi- Besides comparing types of modeling proce-
ciency and efficiency of VP and VM, Thomas dures, the effectiveness of activity schedules with
et  al. (2020) compared POV VP and POV VM static pictures compared to an activity schedule
presented on an iPad® to young adults with ASD with videos has also been explored on appropri-
on meal preparation skills. Differing from past ate transitions with middle-school-aged partici-
research (i.e., Cannella-Malone et  al., 2006, pants with ASD (Cihak, 2011). Cihak (2011)
2011), results supported VM for three of the four found that for two of the three participants, the
participants although both procedures resulted in static picture activity schedule resulted in more
acquisition across both meals. Additionally, VP independent transitions than the video-based
evoked substantially more errors than activity schedule. One participant’s transitioning
VM. Further, both interventions lead to general- improved more with the video-based activity
ization of skills to an untrained setting and were schedule.
found to maintain during a 3-week follow-up. Lastly, the effectiveness of reciprocal imita-
tion training (RIT), the focus of which aims to
13.1.10.3 V  ideo Modeling and In Vivo establish imitation through naturalistic social
Modeling interactions instead of in response to a directive,
In exploring the comparative efficacy of VM and has been compared to VM on the acquisition of
other instructional strategies, some researchers object imitation in young children with ASD
have evaluated in vivo modeling to VM (e.g., (Cardon & Wilcox, 2011). Cardon and Wilcox
Charlop-Christy et  al., 2000; McDowell et  al., (2011) concluded that both procedures increased
2015) and compared static prompts to VM (e.g., responding and that results were maintained and
Cihak & Schrader, 2008), and others have com- produced generalized outcomes to untrained toys
pared VM to a naturalistic imitation procedure and to caregivers at 1- and 3-week follow-up.
(e.g., Cardon & Wilcox, 2011).
Charlop-Christy et al. (2000) were the first to 13.1.10.5 Future Research
compare VM to in vivo modeling across a range Despite the aforementioned studies, there are
of diverse skills (e.g., expressive labeling, inde- opportunities to better understand the variables
pendent play, spontaneous greetings, social play) that contribute to the effectiveness of procedural
for children with ASD using others as a model. variations, types of models, and when one
Interventions were implemented in the absence instructional strategy proves more effective.
of prompts or reinforcement. Across four of the Additional research is needed to assess the exter-
five participants, VM was more efficient than nal validity across additional participants and
in vivo modeling, required far less time to imple- behaviors. Although there is support for the use
ment, and was more cost-efficient than in  vivo of others as models being as effective as self as a
modeling. For one participant, both procedures model (e.g., Sherer et al., 2001), there are oppor-
were equally effective. tunities for future research. The conditions under
McDowell and colleagues (2015) evaluated which various types of models may produce
the comparative effectiveness of in  vivo model- faster acquisition should be explored. There are
ing with prompting compared to VM with young likely participant characteristics that impact the
children with ASD on imitation skills. Their find- efficacy of VSM. It has been suggested that pref-
ings contrast with Charlop-Christy et al. (2000) erence for watching one-self (Sherer et al., 2001)
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 245

and self-recognition (Buggey, 2005) impact the et  al., 2013), matrix training (e.g., McManus
efficacy of VSM.  Future research may wish to et al., 2015), and multiple exemplar training (e.g.,
empirically explore these variables. Additionally, Reeve et  al., 2007), there are opportunities to
the role of voice-over instructions, screen size, extend this research. Systematic evaluations of
perspective, continuous or simultaneous VM, and procedural variations, such as additional prompts,
factors that inform optimal use of VM or VP error correction, reinforcement, and/or in situ
should continue to be explored. Few studies con- training, should be explored to determine best
ducted pre-experimental assessments (e.g., practices and for whom which variations would
Sancho et al., 2010; Thomas et al., 2020) on imi- be most beneficial. Further, it is unclear the con-
tative repertoires. Exploring repertoires that are ditions under which voice- and text-over should
sufficient and necessary for VBI should be be included in VM. It would be important to eval-
explored. Additionally, assessment and strategies uate the conditions under which this is needed.
to enhance generalization have been omitted Additionally, more research incorporating care-
across a number of studies that involved a com- givers, siblings, and peers is needed. In addition,
parison of some sort (e.g., Cannella-Malone across domains, we consistently found that PI
et  al., 2006, 2011; Marcus & Wilder, 2009; data were under-reported. This will be important
McDowell et  al., 2015). Further, especially for to address as these data (or lack thereof) impact
comparative research, it is important that future believability of outcomes. Further, additional
research includes measures of maintenance comparative research is needed.
including assessment of long-term maintenance.
Few studies evaluated performance maintenance
longer than a month (Miltenberger & Charlop, 13.1.12  Evidenced-Based Practice
2015; Sherer et al., 2001).
There is a large corpus of research supporting
VM as an evidenced-based practice. Wong et al.
13.1.11  Future Research (2013) identified 27 evidenced-based practices
via a systematic review of research including
When using VM with individuals with ASD, both single-subject research designs and group
there are several areas that future researchers designs of participants diagnosed with ASD. VM
may wish to explore. We have aimed to provide was identified as an evidenced-based practice
more specific suggestions throughout this chapter producing positive outcomes across multiple
and will highlight areas that have emerged. There developmental and skill areas. According to their
is a continued need to further evaluate sufficient findings and across areas reviewed in this chap-
and necessary perquisite skills. Although the ter, VM was found to be effective across social,
existing research (MacDonald et  al., 2015; communication, behavior, play, motor, adaptive,
Tereshko et al., 2010) has provided a foundation school-readiness, and vocational domains.
upon which this area could be further explored, Several domains were found to be effective
this is still a relatively new research area espe- across all three age categories (i.e., 0–5  years,
cially when balanced with the corpus of VM 6–14  years, and 15–22  years old) and include
research by individuals with ASD.  There is a social, play, and adaptive skills. Further, although
dearth of research exploring strategies to enhance not directly reviewed in this chapter, VM was
response generalization. Given the support for found to be effective establishing joint attention,
establishing scripted behaviors, research is cognitive, and academic skills.
needed to enhance generative repertoires. Further support for VM as an evidenced-based
Although some researchers have explored this practice is found in past meta-analyses. Bellini
area and have incorporated strategies like pro- and Akullian (2007) conducted a meta-analysis
gramming for common stimuli (e.g., Carlile of VM and VSM with children and adolescents
et al., 2018), use of suitable loops (e.g., Dupere with ASD across intervention, maintenance, and
246 R. M. DeBar et al.

generalization. Results support that both VM and tional safety skills, vocational research with var-
VSM were effective at establishing social-­ ied age groups) remain, overwhelmingly, research
communication skills, functional skills, and supports the use of VM to effectively teach par-
behavioral functioning and that skills were main- ticipants with ASD a variety of skills. Practitioners
tained and generalized across people and set- are encouraged to incorporate VM into their
tings. A meta-analysis was also conducted on instruction and continue to explore questions
POV VM, and its findings support the efficacy of related to participant characteristics and optimal
POV VM on independent living skill with indi- instructional arrangements for efficacy, general-
viduals with ASD and developmental disabilities ization, and maintenance.
(Mason et  al., 2013). Future research should
explore the efficacy of POV VM across play,
social communication, increasing academic References
behavior, and decreasing problem behavior. More
recently, Qi et al. (2018) conducted a systematic Abadir, C.  M., DeBar, R.  M., Vladescu, J.  C., Reeve,
S. A., & Kupferman, D. M. (2021). Effects of video
review of single-subject research on VM and modeling on abduction-prevention skills by individu-
social communication skills for individuals with als with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Applied
ASD using What Works Clearing House Single Behavior Analysis. Advanced online copy. https://doi.
Subject Research Design Standards. Based upon org/10.1002/jaba.822
Akmanoglu, N., & Tekin-Iftar, E. (2011). Teaching
their findings, VM is an evidenced-based practice children with autism how to respond to the lures
for social communication skills for individuals of strangers. Autism, 15(2), 205–222. https://doi.
with ASD.  Despite the support of VM as an org/10.1177/136236309352180
evidenced-­based practice, future research should Aldi, C., Crigler, A., Kates-McElrath, K., Long, B., Smith,
H., Rehak, K., & Wilkinson, L. (2016). Examining
continue to evaluate the efficacy of VM and VM the effects of video modeling and prompts to teach
variations (e.g., POV, VSM) across populations activities of daily living skills. Behavior Analysis
(e.g., young children, adults) and domains (e.g., Practice, 9(4), 384–388. https://doi.org/10.1007/
play, reducing problem behavior) as this body of s40617-­016-­0127-­y
Allen, K., Wallace, D., Renes, D., Bowen, S., & Burke,
research is vast and is continuously evolving. R. (2010). Use of video modeling to teach vocational
skills to adolescents and young adults with autism
spectrum disorders. Education and Treatment of
13.1.13  Summary Children, 33(3), 339–349. https://doi.org/10.1353/
etc.0.0101
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
VM is an evidence-based instructional procedure and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
effective for teaching individuals with ASD a https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
diverse range of skills. The purpose of this chap- Autism Speaks. (2017). Autism statistics and facts. https://
www.autismspeaks.org/autism-­statistics-­asd
ter was to review current research on VM. Skills Bellini, S., & Akullian, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of
that have been targeted with VM include play video modeling and video self-modeling interventions
(e.g., MacDonald et al., 2009), social communi- for children and adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
cation (e.g., Kouo, 2019), vocational (e.g., orders. Exceptional Children, 73(3), 264–287. https://
doi.org/10.1177/001440290707300301
English et  al., 2017), safety (e.g., Rex et  al., Besler, F., & Kurt, O. (2016). Effectiveness of video mod-
2018), and functional living skills (e.g., Popple eling provided by mothers in teaching play skills to
et  al., 2016). Another interesting application of children with autism. Educational Sciences: Theory
VM has been on the reduction of problem behav- & Practice, 16(1), 209–230. https://doi.org/10.12738/
estp.2016.1.0273
iors (e.g., Sadler, 2019a). Comparisons of VM Blum-Dimaya, A., Reeve, S.  A., Reeve, K.  F., & Hoch,
have been conducted within procedural varia- H. (2010). Teaching children with autism to play
tions (e.g., type of model, VM and VP) and across a video game using activity schedules and game-­
instructional strategies (e.g., VM and in  vivo embedded simultaneous video modeling. Education
and Treatment of Children, 33(3), 351–370. https://
modeling). Although many areas for future doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0103
research (e.g., inclusion of peers, targeting addi-
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 247

Boudreau, E., & D’Entremont, B. (2010). Improving the eling. Education and Treatment of Children, 33(3),
pretend play skills of pre-schoolers with autism spec- 371–393. https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0104
trum disorders: The effects of video modeling. Journal Charlop, M. H., & Milstein, J. P. (1989). Teaching autistic
of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22(4), children conversational speech using video modeling.
415–431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­010-­9201-­5 Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22(3), 275–285.
Bross, L.  A., Travers, J.  C., Wills, H.  P., Huffman, https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1989.22-­275
J.  M., Watson, E.  K., Morningstar, M.  E., & Boyd, Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A. (2000).
B.  A. (2020). Effects of video modeling for young A comparison of video modeling with in  vivo mod-
adults with autism in community employment set- eling for teaching children with autism. Journal of
tings. Career Development and Transition for Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(6), 537–
Exceptional Individuals, 43(4), 209–225. https://doi. 552. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005635326276
org/10.1177/2165143420941488 Cihak, D. F. (2011). Comparing pictorial and video mod-
Buggey, T. (2005). Video self-modeling applications with eling activity schedules during transitions for students
students with autism spectrum disorder in a small with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
private school setting. Focus on Autism and Other Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 433–441. https://doi.
Developmental Disabilities, 20(1), 52–63. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.006
org/10.1177/10883576050200010501 Cihak, D., Fahrenkrog, C., Ayres, K.  M., & Smith, C.
Buggey, T. (2012). Effectiveness of video self-modeling (2010). The use of video modeling via a video iPod
to promote social initiations by 3-year-olds with and a system of least prompts to improve transitional
autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and Other behaviors for students with autism spectrum disorders
Developmental Disabilities, 27(2), 102–110. https:// in the general education classroom. Journal of Positive
doi.org/10.1177/1088357612441826 Behavior Interventions, 12(2), 103–115. https://doi.
Cannella-Malone, H.  I., Fleming, C., Chung, Y.  C., org/10.1177/1098300709332346
Wheeler, G.  M., Basbagill, A.  R., & Singh, A.  H. Cihak, D. F., & Schrader, L. (2008). Does the model mat-
(2011). Teaching daily living skills to seven individu- ter? Comparing video self-modeling and video adult
als with severe intellectual disabilities: A compari- modeling for task acquisition and maintenance by
son of video prompting to video modeling. Journal adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13(3), 144–153. of Special Education Technology, 23(3), 9–20. https://
https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300710366593 eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ861031
Cannella-Malone, H., Sigafoos, J., O'Reilly, M., de la Clark, R.  J., Wilder, D.  A., Kelley, M.  E., & Ryan, V.
Cruz, B., Edrisinha, C., & Lancioni, G.  E. (2006). (2020). Evaluation of instructions and video modeling
Comparing video prompting to video modeling for to train parents to implement a structured meal proce-
teaching daily living skills to six adults with devel- dure for food selectivity among children with autism.
opmental disabilities. Education and Training in Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13(3), 674–678. https://
Developmental Disabilities, 344–356. http://www. doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00419-­w
jstor.org/stable/23879661 Coyle, C., & Cole, P. (2004). A videotaped self-modelling
Cardon, T. A. (2012). Teaching caregivers to implement and self-monitoring treatment program to decrease
video modeling imitation training via iPad for their off-task behaviour in children with autism. Journal
children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 29(1),
Disorders, 6(4), 1389–1400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/089270204100016626
rasd.2012.06.002 42
Cardon, T., Wangsgard, N., & Dobson, N. (2019). Video Curiel, E.  S., Axe, J.  B., Sainato, D.  M., & Goldstein,
modeling using classroom peers as models to increase H. (2020). Systematic review of matrix training for
social communication skills in children with ASD in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Focus on
an integrated preschool. Education and Treatment of Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 35(1),
Children, 42(4), 515–536. https://doi.org/10.1353/ 55–64. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357619881216
etc.2019.0024 D’Ateno, P., Mangiapanello, K., & Taylor, B.  A.
Cardon, T. A., & Wilcox, M. J. (2011). Promoting imitation (2003). Using video modeling to teach complex play
in young children with autism: A comparison of recip- sequences to a preschooler with autism. Journal of
rocal imitation training and video modeling. Journal Positive Behavior Interventions, 5(1), 5–11. https://
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 654– doi.org/10.1177/10983007030050010801
666. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­010-­1086-­8 Diorio, R., Bray, M., Sanetti, L., & Kehle, T. (2019). Using
Carlile, K. A., DeBar, R. M., Reeve, S. A., & Reeve, K. F. video self-modeling to increase compliance to class-
(2018). Teaching help-seeking when lost to individu- room requests in students with autism spectrum dis-
als with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Applied order. International Journal of School & Educational
Behavior Analysis, 51(2), 191–206. https://doi. Psychology, 7(sup1), 145–157. https://doi.org/10.1080
org/10.1002/jaba.447 /21683603.2018.1443857
Charlop, M.  H., Dennis, B., Carpenter, M.  H., & Domire, S.  C., & Wolfe, P. (2014). Effects of video
Greenberg, A. L. (2010). Teaching socially expressive prompting techniques on teaching daily living
behaviors to children with autism through video mod- skills to children with autism spectrum disorders:
248 R. M. DeBar et al.

A review. Research and Practice for Persons with Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 58, 83–95.
Severe Disabilities, 39(3), 211–226. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.09.004
org/10.1177/1540796914555578 Horn, A.  L., Layden, S.  J., & Bobzien, J. (2021).
Dowrick, P.  W. (1999). A review of self modeling Where are you? Young adults with autism call-
and related interventions. Applied & Preventive ing and texting location following video modeling
Psychology, 8(1), 23–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/ instruction. Career Development and Transition for
S0962-­1849(99)80009-­2 Exceptional Individuals, 44(1), 17–27. https://doi.
Dupere, S., MacDonald, R. P. F., & Ahearn, W. H. (2013). org/10.1177/2165143420953908
Using video modeling with substitutable loops to Jones, J., Lerman, D. C., & Lechago, S. (2013). Assessing
teach varied play to children with autism. Journal of stimulus control and promoting generalization via
Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(3), 662–668. https:// video modeling when teaching social responses to
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.68 children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
Drysdale, B., Lee, C.  Y. Q., Anderson, A., & Moore, Analysis, 46(4), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.81
D.  W. (2015). Using video modeling incorporat- Kouo, J.  L. (2019). The effectiveness of a packaged
ing animation to teach toileting to two children with intervention including point-of-view video model-
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental ing in teaching social initiation skills to children with
and Physical Disabilities, 27(2), 149–165. https://doi. autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and Other
org/10.1007/s10882-­014-­9405-­1 Developmental Disabilities, 34(3), 141–152. https://
English, D. L., Gounden, S., Dagher, R. E., Chan, S. F., doi.org/10.1177/108835761885887
Furlonger, B.  E., Anderson, A., & Moore, D.  W. Kourassanis, J., Jones, E.  A., & Fienup, D.  M. (2015).
(2017). Effects of video modeling with video feedback Peer-video modeling: Teaching chained social
on vocational skills of adults with autism spectrum game behaviors to children with ASD. Journal of
disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 20(8), Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 27, 25–36.
511–524. https://doi.org/10.1080/17518423.2017.12 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­014-­9399-­8
82051 Lee, G.  T., Hu, X., Liu, Y., & Ren, Y. (2020). Effects
Ergenekon, Y. (2012). Teaching basic first-aid skills of video modeling on the acquisition, mainte-
against home accidents to children with autism nance, and generalization of playing with imagi-
through video modeling. Educational Sciences: nary objects in children with autism spectrum
Theory and Practice, 12(4), 2759–2766. https://eric. disorder. Behavior Modification, 00(0), 1–29. https://
ed.gov/?id=EJ1002874 doi.org/10.1177/0145445520939856
Ezzedine, E.  W., DeBar, R.  M., Reeve, S.  A., & Lee, S.  Y., Lo, Y., & Lo, Y. (2017). Teaching functional
Townsend, D.  B. (2020). Using video modeling to play skills to a young child with autism spectrum dis-
teach play comments to dyads with ASD. Journal of order through video self-modeling. Journal of Autism
Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(2), 767–781. https:// and Developmental Disorders, 47(8), 2295–2306.
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.621 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­017-­3147-­8
Gardner, S., & Wolfe, P. (2013). Use of video modeling MacDonald, R., Clark, M., Garrigan, E., & Vangala, M.
and video prompting interventions for teaching daily (2005). Using video modeling to teach pretend play to
living skills to individuals with autism spectrum dis- children with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20(4),
orders: A review. Research and Practice for Persons 225–238. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.197
with Severe Disabilities, 38(2), 73–87. https://doi. MacDonald, R.  P., Dickson, C.  A., Martineau, M., &
org/10.2511/027494813807714555 Ahearn, W.  H. (2015). Prerequisite skills that sup-
Godish, D., Miltenberger, R., & Sanchez, S. (2017). port learning through video modeling. Education and
Evaluation of video modeling for teaching abduc- Treatment of Children, 33–47. https://www.jstor.org/
tion prevention skills to children with autism spec- stable/44683850
trum disorder. Advances in Neurodevelopmental MacDonald, R., Sacramone, S., Mansfield, R., Wiltz, K.,
Disorders, 1, 168–175. https://doi.org/10.1007/ & Ahearn, W.  H. (2009). Using video modeling to
s412252-­017-­0026-­4 teach reciprocal pretend play to children with autism.
Guan, J., & Li, G. (2017). Injury mortality in indi- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(1), 43–55.
viduals with autism. American Journal of Public https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­43
Health, 107(5), 791–793. https://doi.org/10.2105/ MacManus, C., MacDonald, R., & Ahearn, W. H. (2015).
AJPH.2017.303696 Teaching and generalizing pretend play in children
Hine, J.  F., & Woolery, M. (2006). Using point-of-view with autism using video modeling and matrix training.
video modeling to teach play to preschoolers with Behavioral Interventions, 30(3), 191–218. https://doi.
autism. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, org/10.1002/bin.1406
26(2), 83–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121406026 Macpherson, K., Charlop, M. H., & Miltenberger, C. A.
0020301 (2015). Using portable video modeling technology to
Ho, T. Q., Gadke, D. L., Henington, C., Evans-McCleon, increase the compliment behaviors of children with
T. N., & Justice, C. A. (2019). The effects of animated autism during athletic group play. Journal of Autism
video modeling on joint attention and social engage- and Developmental Disorders, 45(12), 3836–3845.
ment in children with autism spectrum disorder. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­014-­2072-­3
13  Video Modeling Instruction for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 249

Maione, L., & Mirenda, P. (2006). Effects of video mod- Palcehcka, G., & MacDonald, R. (2010). A comparison of
eling and video feedback on peer-directed social lan- the acquisition of play skills using instructor-­created
guage skills of a child with autism. Journal of Positive video models and commercially available videos.
Behavior Interventions, 8(2), 106–118. https://doi.org Education and Treatment of Children, 33(3), 457–474.
/10.1177/10983007060080020201 https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0100
Marcus, A., & Wilder, D.  A. (2009). A comparison of Paterson, C., & Arco, L. (2007). Using video modeling
peer video modeling and self video modeling to teach for generalizing toy play in children with autism.
textual responses in children with autism. Journal of Behavior Modification, 31(5), 660–681. https://doi.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(2), 335–341. https:// org/10.1177/0145445507301651
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­335 Pinter, E., Johnson, J.  W., & Boden, T. (2021). Using
Mason, R. A., Davis, H. S., Boles, M. B., & Goodwyn, F. video modeling to facilitate students’ independent
(2013). Efficacy of point-of-view video modeling: A use of a community fitness center. Education and
meta-analysis. Remedial and Special Education, 34(6), Treatment of Children, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/
333–345. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932513486298 s43494-­021-­00040-­8
McCoy, K., & Hermansen, E. (2007). Video modeling for Popple, B., Wall, C., Flink, L., Powell, K., Discepolo,
individuals with autism: A review of model types and K., Keck, D., Mademtzi, M., Volkmar, F., & Shic, F.
effects. Education and Treatment of Children, 183– (2016). Brief report: Remotely delivered video model-
213. https://www.jstor.org/stable/42899952 ing for improving oral hygiene in children with ASD:
McDowell, L.  S., Gutierrez, A., Jr., & Bennett, K.  D. A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental
(2015). Analysis of live modeling plus prompting and Disorders, 46(8), 2791–2796. https://doi.org/10.1007/
video modeling for teaching imitation to children with s10803-­016-­2795-­4
autism. Behavioral Interventions, 30(4), 333–351. Qi, C. H., Barton, E. E., Collier, M., & Lin, Y.-L. (2018).
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1419 A systematic review of single-case research studies
Mechling, L.  C., Ayres, K.  M., Bryant, K.  J., & Foster, on using video modeling interventions to improve
A. L. (2014). Comparison of the effects of continuous social communication skills for individuals with
video modeling, video prompting, and video model- autism spectrum disorder. Focus on Autism and Other
ing on task completion by young adults with moder- Developmental Disabilities, 33(4), 249–257. https://
ate intellectual disability. Education and Training in doi.org/10.1177/1088357617741282
Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 491–504. Rayner, C., Denholm, C., & Sigafoos, J. (2009). Video-­
https://www.jstor.org/stable/24582346 based intervention for individuals with autism: Key
Miltenberger, C.  A., & Charlop, M.  H. (2015). The questions that remain unanswered. Research in Autism
comparative effectiveness of portable video model- Spectrum Disorders, 3(2), 291–303. https://doi.
ing vs. traditional video modeling interventions with org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.09.001
children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Reagon, K.  A., Higbee, T.  S., & Endicott, K. (2006).
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 27(3), 341– Teaching pretend play skills to a student with autism
358. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­014-­9416-­y using video modeling with a sibling as a model and
Morgan, K., & Miltenberger, R. (2017). Evaluation of play partner. Education and Treatment of Children, 29,
video modeling and in situ training to teach firearm 517–528. http://www.jstor.org/stable/42899899
avoidance skills to individuals with autism spec- Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., Townsend, D. B., & Poulson,
trum disorder. Advances in Neurodevelopmental C. L. (2007). Establishing a generalized repertoire of
Disorders, 1, 122–128. https://doi.org/10.1007/ helping behavior in children with autism. Journal of
s41252-­017-­0024-­6 Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), 123–136. https://
Neff, E.  R., Betz, A.  M., Saini, V., & Henry, E. (2017). doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.11-­05
Using video modeling to teach siblings of children Rex, C., Charlop, M.  H., & Spector, V. (2018). Using
with autism how to prompt and reinforce appropriate video modeling as an anti-bullying intervention for
play. Behavioral Interventions, 32(3), 1–13. https:// children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
doi.org/10.1002/bin.1479 Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48, 2701–2713.
Nikopoulos, C.  K., & Keenan, M. (2003). Promoting https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­018-­3527-­8
social initiation in children with autism using video Sadler, K.  M. (2019a). Video self-modeling and func-
modeling. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 87–108. tional behavior assessment to modify aggressive
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.129 behaviors in students with autism spectrum disorder
Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2004). Effects of video and intellectual disabilities. Education and Training in
modeling on social initiations by children with autism. Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 54(4), 406–
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37(1), 93–96. 419. https://www.proquest.com/openview/356fbf113
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2004.37-­93 b942b6981f54d4454a69235/1?pq-­origsite=gscholar&
Nikopoulos, C.  K., & Keenan, M. (2007). Using video cbl=2032023
modeling to teach complex social sequences to children Sadler, K.  M. (2019b). Video self-modeling to treat
with autism. Journal of Developmental Disorders, 37, aggression in students significantly impacted
678–693. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0195-­x by autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Special
250 R. M. DeBar et al.

Education Technology, 34(4), 215–225. https://doi. Developmental Disorders, 46(9), 2845–2858. https://
org/10.1177/0162643418822070 doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­016-­2824-­3
Sancho, K., Sidener, T. M., Reeve, S. A., & Sidener, D. W. Stauch, T.  A., & Plavnick, J.  B. (2020). Teaching voca-
(2010). Two variations of video modeling interven- tional and social skills to adolescents with autism using
tions for teaching play skills to children with autism. video modeling. Education and Treatment of Children,
Education and Treatment of Children, 33(3), 421–442. 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s3494-­020-­00020-­4
https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0097 Sunyoung, K. (2016). Use of video modeling to teach
Sani-Bozkurt, S., & Ozen, A. (2015). Effectiveness and developmentally appropriate play with Korean
efficiency of peer and adult models used in video mod- American children with autism. Research and Practice
eling in teaching pretend play skills to children with for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 41(3), 158–172.
autism spectrum disorder. Education and Training in https://doi.org/10.1177/1540796916658015
Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 50(1), 71–83. Taber-Doughty, T., Miller, B., Shurr, J., & Wiles, B.
http://www.jstor.org/stable/24827502 (2013). Portable and accessible video modeling:
Schatz, R. B., Peterson, R. K., & Bellini, S. (2016). The Teaching a series of novel skills within school and
use of video self-modeling to increase on-task behav- community settings. Education and Training in
ior in children with high-functioning autism. Journal Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 147–163.
of Applied School Psychology, 32(3), 234–253. https:// https://www.jstor.org/stable/23880636
doi.org/10.1080/15377903.2016.1183542 Taylor, B. A., Levin, L., & Jasper, S. (1999). Increasing
Schreibman, L., Whalen, C., & Stahmer, A.  C. (2000). play-related statements in children with autism toward
The use of video priming to reduce disruptive tran- their siblings: Effects of video modeling. Journal of
sition behavior in children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 11(3), 253–
Positive Behavior Interventions, 2(1), 3–11. https:// 264. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021800716392
doi.org/10.1177/109830070000200102 Tereshko, L., MacDonald, R., & Aheam, W.  H. (2010).
Sherer, M., Pierce, K.  L., Paredes, S., Kisacky, K.  L., Strategies for teaching children with autism to imitate
Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2001). Enhancing response chains using video modeling. Research in
conversation skills in children with autism via video Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 479–489. https://doi.
technology: Which is better, “self” or “other” as org/10.1016/j.rasd.2009.11.005
a model? Behavior Modification, 25(1), 140–158. Tetreault, A. S., & Lerman, D. C. (2010). Teaching social
https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445501251008 skills to children with autism using point-of-view
Sherrow, L., Spriggs, A., & Knight, V. (2016). Using video modeling. Education and Treatment of Children,
video models to teach students with autism to play 33(3), 395–419. https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0105
the Wii. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Thomas, E.  M., DeBar, R.  M., Vladescu, J.  C., &
Disabilities, 31(4), 312–320. https://doi. Townsend, D. B. (2020). A comparison of video mod-
org/10.1177/1088357615583469 eling and video prompting by adolescents with asd.
Shrestha, A., Anderson, A., & Moore, D. W. (2013). Using Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13(1), 40–52. https://
point-of-view video modeling and forward chaining to doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00402-­0
teach a functional self-help skill to a child with autism. Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K., Cox, A. W., Fettig, A.,
Journal of Behavioral Education, 22(2), 157–167. Kucharczyk, S., Brock, M. E., Plavnick, J. B., Fluery,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­012-­9165-­x V. P., & Schultz, T. R. (2013). Evidence-based prac-
Spriggs, A. D., Gast, D. L., & Knight, V. F. (2016). Video tices for children, youth, and young adults with autism
Modeling and observational learning to teach gaming spectrum disorder. The University of North Carolina,
access to students with asd. Journal of Autism and Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute,
Autism Evidence-Based Practice Review Group.
Using the Teaching Interaction
Procedure and Behavioral Skills 14
Training to Develop Skills
for Individuals with Autism:
An Evidence-Based Approach

Ashley N. Creem, Sacha K. G. Shaw, Callie Plattner,


and Jennifer Posey

14.1 Using the Teaching language, and gestures (Williams White et  al.,
Interaction Procedure 2007). Unsupported skill acquisition in this area
and Behavioral Skills can lead to difficulties in forming relationships
Training to Develop Skills and loneliness (Bauminger & Kasari, 2000), anx-
for Individuals with Autism: iety and depression (Ghaziuddin, 2002; Sterling
An Evidence-Based et al., 2008), behavioral challenges (Cowen et al.,
Approach 1973), and suicidal ideation (Mayes et al., 2013).
Moreover, failure to develop adequate skills in
The prevalence of autism spectrum disorder social communication is associated with an
(ASD) has grown over the past 20 years, ascend- increased risk of being victimized by bullying
ing from 2 in 10,000 in 1990 to 1 in 59 in 2020 (Ashburner et  al., 2019). Restrictive and repeti-
(Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, tive behavior may manifest through fixations on
2020). ASD is diagnosed by meeting several specific topics or routines, as well as stereotypic
diagnostic characteristics including impairments sounds or movements (Esbensen et  al., 2009).
in social communication, and the existence of Without effective intervention, these characteris-
restricted and repetitive behaviors (American tics can impact individuals’ participation at
Psychiatric Association, 2013). According to the home, school, or in the community. Thus, an
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental effective intervention that remedies these impair-
Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-V), children and ments is critical for increasing the individual’s
adults with a diagnosis of ASD must display defi- overall quality of life.
cits in two of three areas of social communication Teaching skills to individuals diagnosed with
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013). These ASD requires knowledge of evidence-based
include deficits in social initiations and reciproc- practices. DiGennaro et  al. (2017) defined
ity as well as a limited understanding of nonver- evidence-­based practice as “the process of using
bal behavior such as facial expressions, body results from high-quality research to inform clin-
ical practice, while also taking into consideration
A. N. Creem (*) · S. K. G. Shaw · C. Plattner clinical experience and expertise, and the indi-
J. Posey vidual characteristics, culture, and preferences of
School of Education, Endicott College, a client” (p.  142). Evidence-based practices are
Beverly, MA, USA determined by criteria including operationally
e-mail: acreem@endicott.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 251
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_14
252 A. N. Creem et al.

defined procedures, a specific context for use, the form of answer-volunteering and non-verbal
procedural integrity, evidence of a functional language improved for all participants. Since its
relation, and repeated outcomes (Horner et  al., early validation in the 1970s, the TIP has been
2005). applied to benefit individuals diagnosed with
The teaching interaction procedure (TIP) and ASD (Dotson et al., 2010, 2013; Harchik et al.,
behavioral skills training (BST) are evidence-­ 1992; Leaf et al., 2009; Peters et al., 2016), and
based interventions used to teach skills to indi- has functioned to guide educators in curricula
viduals diagnosed with ASD (Dotson et al., 2010; (Taubman et  al., 2011), and provide training
Kornacki et  al., 2013; Leaf et  al., 2009; guidelines (Dowd et al., 1994; Hazel et al., 1983).
Nuernberger et  al., 2013; Palmen et  al., 2008;
Peters et al., 2016). Both procedures manipulate
antecedent and consequent variables to system- 14.1.2 Components of the TIP
atically teach skills and measure learner compe-
tency. The TIP and BST utilize a series of steps, The TIP includes six components (Leaf et  al.,
some of which are shared. This chapter aims to 2015). First, the instructor describes the skill to
describe these procedures, summarize supporting the learner. Second, the learner is provided with a
research, and underscore their differences. meaningful rationale for using the skill. Third,
Considerations for practice and areas for future the instructor describes smaller parts of the skill,
research are also provided. which is followed by the learner stating each
component. The fourth component consists of the
instructor modeling the skill with the learner or
14.1.1 The Teaching Interaction another person. Within this component, the
Procedure instructor models the skills correctly and incor-
rectly. The learner must identify correct and
The term and procedure, teaching interaction, incorrect examples. Fifth, the learner role-plays
first appeared in The Teaching-Family Handbook the target skill with the instructor. The sixth com-
(Phillips, 1968). The process was initially evalu- ponent includes the instructor delivering feed-
ated as a component of the Teaching-Family back to the learner following the emission of
Model (Phillips et al., 1971), an applied behavior each response. Feedback consists of positive
analysis (ABA)-informed teaching model used to reinforcement for correct responses (i.e., vocal
decrease rates of recidivism in youth. Early facil- responses and physical imitation of the skill), and
itators of the program videotaped their interac- corrective feedback for incorrect responses.
tions with youth and the analysis of such videos Although the TIP typically consists of six com-
assisted in operationalizing the TIP. Features of ponents, the instructor is encouraged to use the
interactions included labeling and modeling of components as guidelines and adjust the sequence
skills, descriptions as to why the skills are valu- when necessary (Leaf et al., 2015). Imitation and
able, the practice of the skill by youth, and feed- feedback are provided until the skill is demon-
back regarding the accuracy of the rehearsed strated to mastery. A flowchart illustrating the
skills (Cihon et al., 2017). Since the publication usual sequence for the TIP is displayed in
of Phillips et al. (1971), several more demonstra- Fig. 14.1.
tions have been completed. Minkin et al. (1976)
employed the TIP to increase conversation skills 14.1.2.1 Label/Identify Skill
with 10 typically developing females ranging Suppose you are teaching a learner to share cray-
from ages 12–20  years old. The intervention ons with peers using the TIP. First, the instructor
increased conversational behaviors in all partici- labels or identifies the skill being taught and then
pants. Similarly, Maloney et al. (1976) provided asks the learner to re-state the skill that was iden-
conversation skills training to four predelinquent tified. (e.g., “Today, we are going to talk about
females aged 13–15 years. Conversation skills in sharing. What are we going to learn about
14  Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills… 253

Label Provide Describe Demonstrate Role-Play Feedback


Rationale
Identify the 1. When the Provide The learner 1. Praise for
skill for the Explain behavior examples of is asked to correct
learner and why the should both correct imitate the 2.
ensure they skill is occur and incorrect skill Corrective
ae also able important 2. Each step models of the feedback
to label the explained target skill for
target skills in detail inaccurate
steps

Imitation and feedback are provided until the skill is demonstrated to mastery.

Fig. 14.1  A flowchart illustrating the usual sequence for the TIP

today?”). If the learner accurately states the skill instructor can elaborate by describing specific
being taught, the instructor provides praise (e.g., situations where the skill is appropriate or useful.
“Yes, very good. We will be talking about sharing For example, the instructor states: “If a friend
today.”). If the learner does not restate the skill, asks you for the red crayon during art class, and
or does so inaccurately, then the instructor pro- you let them use it, they might want to color with
vides corrective feedback (e.g., “That is not quite you more.” The rationale should not include far-­
right.”). The sequence starts over, beginning with reaching outcomes (e.g., “If you share, then
the instructor stating the skill to be taught until everyone in the class will invite you to their birth-
the learner accurately labels the skill. day party.”). It is important that rationales are
individualized to the learner. Suppose the learner
14.1.2.2 Provide Rationale has difficulty sharing crayons during free time
In the second step, the instructor provides a but not during instructor-led activities. In this
meaningful rationale for why the learner should case, the rationale should be illustrative of this
demonstrate the behavior. A number of reasons context-specific deficit.
are linked to the importance of providing this
rationale. Rationales may function to explain 14.1.2.3 Description
how one’s own behavior can impact the future The third component includes deconstructing the
and remind the learner why they should engage skill into smaller steps. Each step should include
in the target behavior during non-contrived situa- all critical components involved. In the case of
tions (Leaf et  al., 2015). Said another way, the sharing, the smaller steps can be broken down as
rationale should emphasize that the learner’s follows: “When a friend asks to use something
behavior impacts the environment rather than an that you have, you can say ‘sure’ and pass it to
arbitrary event. In the case of sharing, the instruc- them. If you are using the crayon when they ask,
tor should describe why sharing is a meaningful then you can say ‘hang on,’ wait a few seconds
skill as it pertains to uncontrived outcomes pro- while you finish coloring, and then pass the
vided by the environment. Instead of saying, crayon to them. You could also say ‘I’m just fin-
“Sharing crayons is good because you will get ishing this part, then you can have the crayon.’
tokens,” a meaningful rationale might include, Then you can hand it to them when you are
“If you share crayons with friends, then they done.” The list of ways to explain the skill need
might want to color at your table with you.” Or not be exhaustive. While some learners will ben-
“You can help someone by sharing your crayon efit from several descriptions, others might ben-
when their crayon breaks. Friends usually like to efit from one or two. For learners who can read
color with those who share their crayons.” The but are having difficulty remembering the steps
254 A. N. Creem et al.

in a skill, the TIP provides the flexibility to write requesting it was handed a crayon by the person
out the skill steps and then fade. Ensuring only who was asked.
the critical variables are presented within the
instruction will also help learners remember 14.1.2.5 Role-Playing
complex skills with many steps. Additionally, it The fifth component includes the learner role-­
will permit flexibility in responding (e.g., saying playing the target behavior and receiving feed-
“hang on” or saying “one moment” would both back for the components demonstrated correctly
be considered correct). The end of this compo- and incorrectly. This component is arguably the
nent is achieved when the learner restates the most important component in the TIP as it allows
steps of the skill. the individual to contact contingencies to estab-
lish the behavior in the learner’s repertoire (Cihon
14.1.2.4 Demonstration et  al., 2017). Role-plays can occur with the
Instructor demonstration of the skill can occur instructor or another peer. In the TIP sequence
during the description or afterward. Within this used to establish sharing, the instructor or peer
component, the instructor models the target would request to use an item that the learner is
behavior accurately and inaccurately. Doing so using. Contingent upon correct demonstration of
allows the learner to observe multiple exemplars the skill (i.e., the learner passes the item to the
of correct and incorrect versions of the skill. It person who requests it), the instructor gives
may be beneficial to select models based on the praise. Contingent upon incorrect demonstration
learner’s performance. For example, if earlier in of the skill, the instructor gives corrective feed-
the day the learner said, “No! It’s mine!” when a back. These practice sessions continue until all of
peer asked to borrow their crayon, the instructor the steps are demonstrated correctly (Leaf et al.,
might model that same scenario during the incor- 2015) and are shaped up over time such that they
rect demonstration part of the intervention. This eventually mimic the terminal environment in
would be followed by a correct demonstration of which the learner must demonstrate the skill
this scenario such as handing the peer the crayon (Cihon et al., 2017).
while saying, “Sure! You can use it.” Often,
exemplars range from obvious to less obvious. In 14.1.2.6 Feedback
this case, a correct model might include someone Throughout the sequence, the instructor provides
asking the instructor to share and then saying the learner with feedback on their performance
“sure, here you go” while passing the crayon while practicing the skill. It is crucial to embed
immediately to the requesting person. An obvi- this component in the sequence, as role-play
ous incorrect demonstration of sharing would without feedback will likely result in less prog-
include someone asking the instructor to share ress. Contingent on accurate responses, feedback
followed by the instructor walking away with the can take several forms (e.g., specific praise, point,
crayons. The instructor should select example token, preferred activity). If praise is used, phras-
demonstrations thoughtfully and ensure that ing should be specific to the skill demonstrated
there are salient features of each, increasing the correctly. For instance, if the learner accurately
likelihood that the instructor labels the skill cor- shares the crayon during role-play, then the
rectly and receives reinforcement. Less obvious instructor should say “Nice work passing your
examples of sharing could include someone ask- crayon to me right when you were finished color-
ing the instructor for the crayon, the instructor ing in the sky,” versus “Great job, coloring in the
throwing a nub of a crayon in the trash, and then sky.” In addition, corrective feedback follows
handing the person who requested it a new incorrect responses. It is important the forms of
crayon. Although this might be more difficult to feedback are specific to the learner (e.g., activity-­
discern, technically this is sharing. The instructor based reinforcers are age-appropriate, the time
was in possession of the crayons and the person between the correct or incorrect demonstration of
the skill is adequate, tokens are used only after
14  Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills… 255

proper conditioning has taken place). Role-plays/ Empirical studies have been extended to adults
practice and feedback are repeated until the diagnosed with ASD. Dotson et al. (2013) used a
learner achieves the designated mastery criteria. multiple-probe across behaviors design to evalu-
ate the efficacy of the TIP to teach six adults
employment skills at recycling jobs. Skills were
14.1.3 The TIP and ASD divided into three categories: worker skills (e.g.,
checking the recycling bins), supervisory skills
To date, there are a number of empirical applica- (e.g., gathering weekly pick-up schedules), and
tions of the TIP (e.g., Dotson et al., 2010, 2013; office skills (e.g., entering employee timecard
Harchik et  al., 1992; Leaf et  al., 2009; Peters information onto spreadsheets). Results indicated
et  al., 2016). In the first empirical investigation that the TIP was an effective method to teach the
with individuals diagnosed with ASD, Leaf et al. adult participants relevant job skills in the natural
(2009) used the TIP to successfully develop environment.
social skills with three children with ASD. Four While the majority of recent research demon-
domains of social skills (i.e., play, social-­ strating the efficacy of the TIP has been with
communication, emotion skills, and choice/ individuals diagnosed with ASD, the literature is
selection skills) were targeted across three par- not limited to populations with disabilities. Using
ticipants in a multiple baseline design. Before the a multiple-baseline design across behaviors,
intervention, participants displayed selected Harchik et  al. (1992) assessed the efficacy of a
skills at near-zero levels, and following the inter- slightly modified version of the TIP to increase
vention, skills were demonstrated consistently. In staff’s use of token economies in a group home.
addition, play and communication skills were The modification included a seventh component,
generalized to peer interactions. A group format where the staff was presented with the opportu-
is also an effective modality for use of the TIP. For nity to ask questions or make comments before
example, Peters et al. (2016) evaluated the effec- ending the TIP session. Results indicated that the
tiveness of the procedure to teach four social TIP could be an effective means to increase the
skills to a group of four young children diagnosed frequency of exchanging tokens for backup rein-
with ASD using a multiple-probe design. The forcers, social behaviors during token delivery,
group took place during daily school instruction, and social behaviors during the exchange. As
where all participants were taught the same social demonstrated in research and practice, the TIP is
skill. The TIP was effective in teaching all the tar- a well-established method for teaching skills to a
geted social skills to the participants and the par- variety of learners with ASD, as well as those
ticipants maintained the skills at least 2  weeks working in the human service industry.
after the intervention ended. The efficacy of the Despite the TIP’s long history in the field of
TIP has also been demonstrated to teach adoles- ABA and the convincing evidence demonstrating
cents diagnosed with ASD conversational skills. its effectiveness as a method for teaching a myr-
Dotson et al. (2010) assessed the efficacy of the iad of skills to individuals with ASD, research on
TIP to teach five adolescents conversational skills the TIP is still limited (Leaf et al., 2015). Partially
(i.e., answering and asking open-ended ques- responsible for this limitation is the tendency to
tions, extending positive feedback to a conversa- confuse the TIP and BST (Leaf et al., 2015). The
tion partner). A multiple-probe design across clinical implications of TIP and BST procedures
behaviors indicated that mastery of skills are discussed later. First, we provide an overview
occurred as a result of the TIP for the participants of BST.
diagnosed with ASD.  Although three skills did
not fully generalize to a more natural setting, all
five participants showed some generalization to
naturalistic situations with peers.
256 A. N. Creem et al.

14.2 Behavioral Skills Training description of the target behavior and the context
in which it should occur (Miltenberger, 2016).
BST (Bornstein et al., 1980; Sarakoff & Sturmey, For example, when teaching a child to share, the
2004) is an evidence-based and empirically vali- instructor may say to the learner, “Today we are
dated method for teaching skills to individuals going to practice sharing. When a friend asks for
with disabilities (Kornacki et  al., 2013; your toy (i.e., the context) you should hand it to
Nuernberger et  al., 2013; Palmen et  al., 2008; them (i.e., the target behavior).” Miltenberger
Peters & Thompson, 2015; Ryan et  al., 2019). et al. (2017) included a number of key points to
BST utilizes the three-term contingency in a consider while using BST. To begin, instructions
­role-­play scenario. This arrangement allows the should be delivered by an authority figure, such
learner to repeatedly practice the target behavior as a parent or instructor, and only delivered after
in the presence of the relevant stimulus condi- attaining the learner’s attention. Additionally,
tions until fluency is achieved (Miltenberger instructions should be clear and match the recep-
et al., 2017). In order to promote generalization, tive capabilities of the learner so that they are
BST systematically utilizes four components understandable. Finally, to ensure instructions
(Miltenberger, 2008, 2016): instruction, model, are understood, Miltenberger et  al. (2017) sug-
role-play, and feedback (Bornstein et  al., 1980; gested the learner repeats the instruction immedi-
Himle et al., 2004; Miltenberger, 2016; Parsons ately after it is delivered. Instructions can be
et al., 2012). The components of BST are similar delivered either verbally (Miltenberger, 2008) or
to those used in the TIP, with two major differ- in written form (Parsons et al., 2012).
ences. First, while implementing the TIP, the
instructor provides a rationale for learning the 14.2.1.2 Model
skill. In contrast, BST does not include a ratio- To further aid in the understanding of the instruc-
nale component. A second major difference is tion, the instruction is subsequently followed
that BST includes a correct demonstration only, with a correct model of the target behavior. This
while the TIP includes a correct and incorrect allows the learner to observe the target behavior
demonstration. As in the TIP, BST begins with after it is described. Modeling provides a correct
the instructor describing the target behavior to demonstration of the target behavior in the pres-
the learner. This is then followed by the instructor ence of relevant stimulus conditions. For exam-
providing the learner with a correct model only. ple, the instructor might model sharing his blue
The remainder of the components are identical to crayon in the presence of a child wanting to color
the components of the TIP. After the model, the a blue car but not having a blue crayon (i.e., the
learner is provided with an opportunity to prac- relevant stimulus condition). This can be done in-­
tice the target behavior in a role-play scenario vivo (e.g., a live real-time model) or in a simula-
and the instructor provides feedback during or tion of the natural context. For example, when
after the role-play. The role-play and feedback teaching sharing, an instructor may use a live
components are typically repeated until the model if another child approaches the instructor
learner is able to demonstrate the skill accurately. and asks for a toy the instructor is using. In this
A comprehensive description of these compo- instance, the instructor may model sharing by
nents is to follow as well as a review of the effi- saying “Sure!” and allowing the child to use the
cacy of BST across populations and skills. toy. However, a natural opportunity may not
always be available. If a live model is not possi-
ble, the instructor may create a simulation and
14.2.1 Components of BST model the target behavior using a video (Nigro-­
Bruzzi & Sturmey, 2010) or computer simulation
14.2.1.1 Instruction (Vanselow & Hanley, 2014), as well as using
The first component of BST is the instruction, another adult, or being creative with other stim-
which provides the learner with a detailed uli, such as dolls (Miltenberger et  al., 2017).
14  Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills… 257

Miltenberger et  al. (2017) provided recommen- include practicing the behavior in a simulation of
dations for effective modeling. The first recom- the natural context and in the presence of the rel-
mendation was that the model should match the evant stimulus condition, following each practice
learner’s ability (i.e., the model should not be too with an immediate and appropriate consequence
lengthy or complex for the learner). Additionally, (i.e., reinforcement for correct responses and cor-
Miltenberger and colleagues suggested that the rective feedback for incorrect responses), and
model be completed by an individual that shares repeated practice (i.e., practicing until the target
similar characteristics with the learner (e.g., a behavior has been demonstrated several times).
child of similar age). If this is unavailable, then
an authority figure (e.g., a parent or instructor) 14.2.1.4 Feedback
should provide the model in a simulation of a Feedback includes (a) reinforcement for any part
context that matches the natural context in which of the target behavior that is emitted correctly
the relevant stimulus condition would actually and/or (b) corrective feedback for any incorrect
occur (Miltenberger et  al., 2017). Miltenberger responses (e.g., If while sharing the child throws
and colleagues further recommended the learner the toy, the instructor might say, “Remember to
be oriented toward the instructor before the give the toy to your friend gently.” The instructor
behavior is demonstrated and the instructor might then model how to hand over a toy gently).
should model as many times as necessary for the Feedback should be delivered immediately after
learner to correctly demonstrate the target behav- the role-play (Miltenberger et  al., 2017).
ior. Each time, the target behavior should be Feedback may be followed by further practice
modeled in a variety of ways to increase the like- until the learner emits the target behavior cor-
lihood of generalization (Miltenberger et  al., rectly several times (Stocco et al., 2017). A flow-
2017). For example, when a child is being taught chart illustrating the usual sequence for BST is
to engage in sharing, the instructor might model displayed in Fig. 14.2.
asking someone if they want some of their snack,
giving someone a toy they are playing with, or
giving someone a crayon from their crayon box. 14.2.2 BST and ASD
Final recommendations included describing
important components of the model (e.g., After BST procedures have been demonstrated effec-
modeling sharing, the instructor might say, “I tive to teach a variety of skills including increas-
used a kind voice and gave her the toy gently”) ing appropriate social behaviors (Matson &
and if the learner has the verbal ability, the learner Stephens, 1978), teaching child management
should describe the modeled behavior. A positive skills to parents (Forehand et  al., 1979), and
consequence should be provided following cor- teaching emergency safety skills (Jones &
rect responding by the learner and this can either Kazdin, 1980; Wurtele et al., 1986). BST has also
be provided by the instructor or, at times, it can been used to teach neurotypical adults (Hogan
be a naturally occurring consequence as part of et al., 2015; Sarakoff & Sturmey, 2004) and chil-
the interaction (Dogan et al., 2017). dren (Johnson et al., 2005, 2006; Wurtele et al.,
1986), as well as adults and children with various
14.2.1.3 Rehearsal disabilities (Elder et  al., 1979; Haseltine &
BST provides the learner with an opportunity to Miltenberger, 1990; Matson & Stephens, 1978;
practice the target behavior in the presence of the Palmen & Didden, 2012; Sanchez & Miltenberger,
relevant stimulus condition immediately after it 2015).
is modeled (Parsons et al., 2012). The role-play is BST has also been demonstrated effective in
completed in a simulation of the natural context promoting skill development for individuals with
and allows for the learner to receive feedback for ASD (Kornacki et al., 2013; Nuernberger et al.,
correct and incorrect performance(s) of the target 2013; Palmen et al., 2008; Peters & Thompson,
behavior. Considerations for effective role-play 2015; Ryan et al., 2019). Ryan et al. (2019) used
258 A. N. Creem et al.

Instruction Model Rehearsal Feedback

Provide a verbal Provide a correct Rehearse and/or Provide


and/or written demonstration of role-play the target descriptive praise
description of the the target behavior behavior in a and/or corrective
target behavior natural context feedback
with the relevant
stimulus condition

Rehearsal and feedback are repeated until mastery is demonstrated.

Fig. 14.2  A flowchart illustrating the usual sequence for BST

BST to increase appropriate conversation skills Studies have also shown that BST is effective
for six adults with ASD. Targeted interaction in developing job skills for individuals diagnosed
skills included approaching, greeting, posing a with ASD, such as mascot performance skills
question or comment, waiting for a response, and (Allen et  al., 2010; Burke et  al., 2010), task
ending the conversation. All six participants engagement (Palmen & Didden, 2012), and
demonstrated low levels of accurate responding behavior therapist skills (Lerman et  al., 2015).
in the baseline condition. Following baseline, Allen et al. (2010) evaluated the effects of BST to
BST was implemented, which began with partici- teach three young adults with ASD to exhibit the
pants receiving verbal or written instruction on skills necessary to perform in a mascot costume.
how to have an appropriate conversation (e.g., Skills included waving, handshaking, moving the
“Say hello to your friend.”). Next, an appropriate costume’s tongue up and down, wagging the cos-
conversation was modeled for the participants. tume’s tail, winking the costume’s eye, and wig-
Following the model, the participants practiced gling the costume’s ears. Additional skills
engaging in a conversation and were provided included jumping, shaking the body of the cos-
with feedback in relation to their performance of tume, and pulling the hands and arms into the
conversation interactions. If the participants costume. In baseline, the participants put on the
demonstrated an appropriate conversation (i.e., costume, were brought to the main aisle of a
accurate response), verbal praise was delivered. store, and were instructed to “Do whatever you’d
If the participants demonstrated an inappropriate like.” The training was then implemented wherein
conversation (i.e., inaccurate response), correc- the instruction and model were delivered using a
tive feedback in the form of verbal and gestural video model demonstrating scripted and natural
prompts was delivered. The participants then mascot performances. Following the video
completed another practice with the researcher model, instructors offered the opportunity to
and verbal instruction and model prompts were practice. The participants were brought back to
used until the participants performed an appro- the main aisle of the store while wearing the cos-
priate conversation. The training continued until tume and were once again instructed to “Do
the participants completed three consecutive ses- whatever you’d like.” Following the intervention,
sions with 80% accuracy or one session with all participants were able to complete the skills
100% accuracy. Through the use of BST, several necessary to perform in a mascot costume.
other studies have demonstrated improved social BST has also been demonstrated in the litera-
skills for individuals with ASD (Kornacki et al., ture as an effective intervention for teaching
2013; Nuernberger et  al., 2013; Palmen et  al., safety skills to individuals living with ASD. Some
2008; Peters & Thompson, 2015). of these skills include firearm safety, (Gatheridge
et al., 2004; Rossi et al., 2017), abduction preven-
14  Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills… 259

tion (Gunby & Rapp, 2014), and fire and poison tice opportunities that allow the learner to contact
safety (Rossi et  al., 2017). Rossi et  al. (2017) the reinforcing contingencies to strengthen skills
examined the effects of BST in developing a gen- in their repertoire. It is a proactive approach to
eralized safety skills repertoire for three children teaching important skills such as safety skills, job
with ASD.  Participants were taught to avoid skills, and social skills. Its effects have been
touching a dangerous stimulus, leaving the area, empirically demonstrated with typically develop-
and reporting the dangerous stimulus to an adult. ing adults and children, as well as with adults and
Dangerous stimuli included firearms, fire-starting children with ASD.
agents, and liquid poisons. In baseline, the par-
ticipants were provided the instruction, “Go play
in the [name of classroom center or room]. I will 14.3 Considerations for Using
be right back” and were left alone with a danger- the TIP and BST
ous stimulus. Researchers recorded if the partici-
pants engaged in an inaccurate response (i.e., Several considerations should be made when
touched the dangerous stimulus) or accurate implementing the TIP and BST. First, it is recom-
response (i.e., left the area and/or reported the mended that the practitioner assesses the litera-
dangerous stimulus to an adult). In baseline, all ture base as well as the learner’s prerequisite
of the participants touched at least one dangerous skills prior to selecting intervention procedures
stimulus and none of the participants left the area to ensure the most effective intervention is
to report the dangerous stimuli to an adult. selected for the learner. Given the components of
Following baseline, BST was implemented using the TIP and BST, there are several prerequisite
multiple exemplars within each category. The skills that may increase the likelihood of their
experimenter began by providing an instruction effectiveness. We will highlight four. First, the
(e.g., “don’t touch, move away, tell an adult”) learner must have a well-developed instruction-­
while showing the participant the dangerous following repertoire. Second, in order to cor-
stimulus. The participants were asked to repeat rectly role-play the model, the learner must have
the instruction. After the instruction, the experi- a well-developed generalized imitative reper-
menter modeled approaching a dangerous stimu- toire. Additionally, because practice does not
lus, stopping before touching it, and stating always occur just prior to times where the learner
“don’t touch.” The participants then practiced, is presented with an opportunity to demonstrate
and behavior-specific praise was delivered for the skill in real-time, delayed imitative reper-
correct responses. If the participants emitted an toires should be considered. Third, it may be ben-
incorrect response, the experimenter corrected eficial for the learner to engage in rule-governed
the error and modeled a correct response. This behavior. This might help the behavior generalize
procedure continued until the participant demon- to the natural environment in the absence of rein-
strated the skill accurately in two consecutive forcement. Finally, it is essential for the learner to
practices. The same procedure was used to teach have well-established attending skills. For these
the participants to move away and tell an adult. reasons, the TIP and BST may be more appropri-
BST was continued until the participants inde- ate to use with individuals with more well-­
pendently demonstrated the full safety response developed skill repertoires. These
(i.e., stopping before touching, moving away, and recommendations are general and should not be
telling an adult) for two consecutive role-play tri- substituted for thorough consideration of a learn-
als. Following BST, all three children demon- er’s current skill repertoire. It is useful for practi-
strated the complete safety response for two tioners to assess learners’ skills on an individual
exemplars in each stimulus category. basis prior to choosing intervention methods.
The extant literature indicates that BST is an If the learner demonstrates these prerequisite
effective, evidence-based approach. BST manip- skills, further considerations must be made. Prior
ulates antecedent conditions by providing prac- to implementation, it must be determined if the
260 A. N. Creem et al.

pertinent context can be modeled in-vivo and, if the likelihood of generalization by allowing the
not, how that context will be simulated. For learner to practice the skill and for the target to be
example, the instructor must determine if they reinforced in the presence of these stimuli.
will use a video, audio, or computer simulation or The TIP and BST are effective interventions
if they will use another person or other materials, for teaching a variety of skills and are ideal inter-
such as a doll, to model the skill. Furthermore, ventions for teaching skills where in-the-moment
generalization may occur more reliably when the training is difficult due to limited opportunities or
training context shares adequate similarity to the stigmatization. To illustrate, there is usually just
terminal context (Stokes & Baer, 1977; Stokes & one opportunity to interview for a job.
Osnes, 1989). Therefore, the instructor must also Additionally, it may be stigmatizing to have
consider who will provide the model and in what someone with you on a worksite telling you what
setting they will do so. The similarity between to do, or to be prompted or receive feedback
the model and the learner and the setting the while engaging in social interaction. The TIP and
training is complete may increase the likelihood BST facilitate the teaching of skills outside of
that the learner will imitate the behavior. limited or delicate situations. With in-the-­
Therefore, it is best if a model is similar to the moment training, the individual can successfully
learner and the setting is similar to the natural demonstrate the correct behavior in their natural
setting the behavior should occur. environment.
Further, in the area of generalization, multiple
exemplars should be embedded into the model
and role-play components (e.g., teach the learner 14.4 Conclusions
to respond to multiple interview questions). The and Recommendations
learner should also be provided with opportuni-
ties to engage in the target behavior in the pres- The increasing prevalence of ASD (Maenner
ence of a variety of stimuli that would be present et al., 2020) necessitates innovative and socially
in the natural context. Additionally, the instructor significant interventions. The use of evidence-­
is encouraged to model and allow for practice of based treatment is critical to the effective treat-
the skill in its environment-specific context. For ment of deficits associated with ASD.  The
example, when using the TIP or BST to target application of non-evidence-based treatment can
interview skills, then teaching opportunities result in harm to individuals diagnosed with
should occur in a variety of office-like settings. It ASD, wasting valuable time, money, and other
is also recommended that the instructor allows resources (Zane et al., 2008). The TIP and BST
for repeated modeling and practice of many dif- are two evidence-based interventions that have
ferent models of the target behavior, as well as been reliably shown to develop a variety of skills
immediate feedback in the presence of the dis- for individuals diagnosed with ASD (e.g., Allen
criminative stimuli outside of the natural context. et al., 2010; Dotson et al., 2010; Harchik et al.,
The instructor should teach multiple variations of 1992; Kornacki et  al., 2013; Leaf et  al., 2009;
a skill (i.e., teach several different responses to an Ryan et al., 2019). These skills include, but are
interview question) and many different stimuli not limited to, play skills (Leaf et  al., 2009),
from the natural environment (e.g., a desk, desk social skills (Leaf et al., 2009; Nuernberger et al.,
chair, notepad) should be incorporated into the 2013; Ryan et  al., 2019), communication skills
model and role-play. It is also important to select (Leaf et  al., 2009), conversation skills (Dotson
target behaviors that are likely to be reinforced in et al., 2010; Kassardjian et al., 2013; Nuernberger
the natural environment (e.g., shaking the inter- et  al., 2013), employment skills (Allen et  al.,
viewer’s hand at the end of an interview). Finally, 2010; Burke et  al., 2010; Harchik et  al., 1992),
when possible, any instances of the target behav- and safety skills (Gatheridge et al., 2004; Gunby
ior in the natural environment should be rein- & Rapp, 2014; Rossi et al., 2017). The research
forced. The use of these strategies will increase and results described in this chapter have demon-
14  Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills… 261

strated the effectiveness of the TIP and BST pro- authors suggested, “When rationales are included
cedures across settings, individuals, and skills. with labeling, demonstration, role-play, and feed-
Both procedures can also be implemented in the back, researchers and professionals should label
group or one-to-one setting (Leaf et  al., 2009; the procedure as a TIP. When rationales are not
Nuernberger et  al., 2013; Palmen et  al., 2008; included with labeling, demonstration, role-play,
Peters et al., 2016). The generality of the TIP and and feedback, the procedure should be labeled as
BST makes them valuable teaching procedures, BST” (Leaf et al., 2015, p. 410). Important com-
particularly when in-the-moment teaching would parisons and component analyses are made pos-
be stigmatizing or is not possible, but repeated sible by accurately labeling procedures and
practice is necessary. relative components.
The benefits of TIP and BST are well estab- To date, no studies have completed a compo-
lished but further areas of research remain. To nent analysis to determine the effectiveness of
begin, while both the TIP and BST can be imple- each component within the TIP or BST. Doing so
mented in the one-to-one or group format, no lit- may improve the efficiency of the interventions
erature has compared the efficacy and and determine if there is a need for a distinction
effectiveness of implementation in each format. between the two procedures. For example, it may
It is possible that teaching in one format may lead be possible that the rationale and incorrect dem-
to more efficient skill acquisition or greater gen- onstration of the skill are not necessary compo-
eralization and maintenance. Additionally, there nents within the TIP procedure. When doing so,
is limited research on the use of the TIP or BST researchers should evaluate a variety of outcome
in populations with significant intellectual delays measures. For example, it would not only be nec-
or minimal vocal language. An evaluation of the essary to look at the acquisition of the skill but
TIP and BST that includes participants with more also the generalization and maintenance of the
severe impairments and/or minimal vocal lan- skill. It is possible certain components are less
guage may increase the generality of these proce- important for the acquisition of the skill but are
dures. To do so, it may be necessary to evaluate necessary for generalization and maintenance.
the prerequisite skills needed for the TIP and Additionally, while there is evidence of effective
BST to be an effective intervention as well as any outcomes through the use of in-vivo and video-­
intervention modifications (e.g., modality of based modeling, there has not yet been a com-
instruction delivery) that can effectively be made parison of these delivery methods in the TIP or
to the procedures. BST literature. To improve the efficacy and effec-
As previously stated, the TIP and BST closely tiveness of the procedures, it should be evaluated
resemble one another, with the only difference which modality of instruction delivery demon-
being that in BST the instructor does not provide strates the most promising results.
the learner with a rationale, and the instructor Finally, as another measure of effectiveness,
provides a correct demonstration only (Leaf further social validity measures should be com-
et  al., 2015; Miltenberger et  al., 2017). As a pleted. While it is suggested that the TIP and
result, there is confusion between the two proce- BST limit stigmatization by allowing for teach-
dures (Leaf et  al., 2015). Additionally, there is ing to be implemented in simulations of a context
inconsistent use of components within both pro- rather than in front of others in the actual context,
cedures. For example, Allen et al. (2010) did not limited social validity measures currently exist
include a practice and feedback portion in their (Carr et  al., 1999; Cihon et  al., 2017). Social
use of the TIP procedure. Not only could these validity measures would identify the extent to
inconsistencies hinder the precision of imple- which the procedures are socially acceptable and
mentation, but they also risk decreasing the over- improve the meaningfulness of intervention.
all effectiveness and efficacy of the procedures. Furthermore, if component analyses reveal that
Leaf et al. (2015) provided a recommendation for both the TIP and BST are equally effective at
differentiating between the TIP and BST.  The teaching skills, the intervention can be selected
262 A. N. Creem et al.

by using social validity measures to determine for teaching more simple social skills
which procedure may be more preferred by the (Kassardjian, 2013; Ryan et  al., 2019) such as
consumer. greetings or more complex social skills (Allen
Although areas for future research remain, the et al., 2010; Dotson et al., 2013) such as complet-
evidence supporting the use of the TIP and BST ing a job interview. While more research is
to develop skills in individuals living with ASD is required to evaluate the prerequisites needed to
prominent. The generality of the TIP and BST benefit from the TIP or BST, it is likely a learner
allows for flexibility of teaching. This generality, with a well-developed instruction following rep-
in conjunction with the large base of existing evi- ertoire, well-developed generalized imitative rep-
dence, makes the TIP and BST eminent ertoire, delayed imitative repertoire,
­interventions in the treatment of ASD.  Further rule-governed behavior, and well-established
research would only strengthen the evidence and attending skills will learn best from the TIP and
generality of these procedures, advancing their BST. To increase the likelihood of generalization,
effectiveness and application. clinicians should embed multiple exemplars
The TIP and BST can be used for a variety of within their instruction when using the TIP and
learners (Dotson et  al., 2013; Johnson et  al., BST.  Modeling multiple appropriate responses,
2005; Kassardjian, 2013; Ryan et  al., 2019) to incorporating a variety of relevant stimuli that
teach a variety of social skills (Dotson et  al., would be present in the natural context, teaching
2010; Kornacki et  al., 2013; Leaf et  al., 2009; in the environment-specific context or a setting
Nuernberger et  al., 2013; Palmen et  al., 2008; that closely resembles the natural setting, select-
Ryan et  al., 2019). The TIP and BST allow for ing target behaviors that will likely be reinforced
flexible teaching which yields generalization in the natural setting, and providing reinforce-
(Dotson et al., 2010; Rossi et al., 2017)). For this ment when the learner displays the skill in the
reason, the TIP and BST are especially helpful natural environment will all help increase the
for teaching social skills that only allow one likelihood of generalization. Finally, to increase
opportunity for practice in the natural environ- learner buy-in, it is recommended that instruction
ment (e.g., a job interview) or for complex skills is collaborative and interactive for the learner.
that require flexibility in instruction delivery or For example, the instructor might use open-ended
responding (e.g., saying “hang on,” “one questions such as, “What do you think are impor-
moment,” “I need a second” to tell someone you tant things to do in a job interview?” or “why
are busy). Furthermore, the TIP and BST are use- might it be important for you to learn interview
ful for teaching skills that may be embarrassing skills?” rather than just telling the learner what
or stigmatizing to teach in the natural setting. For they should do. It is possible this may allow the
example, teaching social skills in the natural learner to feel more involved in their learning and
environment while peers are present may feel will make teaching more individualized to the
embarrassing for the learner. The TIP and BST learner. With consideration for the learners’
allow for social skills to be practiced in a simula- needs, the TIP and BST can be effectively used
tion of the natural setting in the presence of rele- within the clinical practice to develop social
vant stimuli. This provides the ability for a social skills for individuals with ASD.
skill to be taught in a more private setting while
allowing for generalization to the natural setting.
Given the tremendous clinical utility of the References
TIP and BST, clinicians should consider the use
of these technologies when teaching social skills. Allen, K. D., Wallace, D. P., Greene, D. J., Bowen, S. L.,
& Burke, R. V. (2010). Community-based vocational
The TIP and BST can be used with learners with instruction using videotaped modeling for young
ASD ranging in age from preschool (Johnson adults with autism spectrum disorders performing
et al., 2005; Kassardjian, 2013) to adult (Dotson in air-inflated mascots. Focus on Autism and Other
et al., 2013; Ryan et al., 2019) and can be used
14  Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills… 263

Developmental Disabilities, 25(3), 186–192. https:// Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4(2), 199–209. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1088357610377318 doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2009.09.005
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic Dotson, W. H., Richman, D. M., Abby, L., Thompson, S.,
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). & Plotner, A. (2013). Teaching skills related to self-­
Author. employment to adults with developmental disabili-
Ashburner, J., Saggers, B., Campbell, M.  A., Dillon-­ ties: An analog analysis. Research in Developmental
Wallace, J.  A., Hwang, Y.-S., Carrington, S., & Disabilities, 34(8), 2336–2350. https://doi.
Bobir, N. (2019). How are students on the autism org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.009
spectrum affected by bullying? Perspectives of stu- Dowd, T., Czyz, J.  D., O’Kane, S.  E., & Elofson, A.
dents and parents. Journal of Research in Special (1994). Effective skills for child-care workers: A train-
Education Needs, 19(1), 27–44. https://doi. ing manual from boys town. The Boys Town Press.
org/10.1111/1471-­3802.12421 Elder, J.  P., Edelstein, B.  A., & Narick, M.  M. (1979).
Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (2000). Loneliness and Adolescent psychiatric patients: Modifying
friendship in high-functioning children with autism. aggressive behavior with social skills training.
Child Development, 71(2), 447–456. https://doi. Behavior Modification, 3(2), 161–178. https://doi.
org/10.1111/1467-­8624.00156 org/10.1177/014544557932002
Bornstein, M., Bellack, A.  S., & Hersen, M. (1980). Esbensen, A. J., Seltzer, M. M., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish,
Social skills training for highly aggressive chil- J. W. (2009). Age-related differences in restricted repet-
dren: Treatment in an inpatient psychiatric setting. itive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Journal
Behavior Modification, 4(2), 173–186. https://doi. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 57–66.
org/10.1177/014544558042002 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­008-­0599-­x
Burke, R.  V., Andersen, M.  N., Bowen, S.  L., Howard, Forehand, R., Sturgis, E. T., McMahon, R. J., Aguar, D.,
M.  R., & Allen, K.  D. (2010). Evaluation of two Green, K., Wells, K. C., & Breiner, J. (1979). Parent
instruction methods to increase employment options behavioral training to modify child noncompliance:
for young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Treatment generalization across time and from home
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6), 1223– to school. Behavior Modification, 3(1), 3–25. https://
1233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.07.023 doi.org/10.1177/014544557931001
Carr, J.  E., Austin, J.  L., Britton, L.  N., Kellum, K.  K., Gatheridge, B.  J., Miltenberger, R.  G., Huneke, D.  F.,
& Bailey, J. S. (1999). An assessment of social valid- Satterlund, M. J., Mattern, A. R., Johnson, B. M., &
ity trends in applied behavior analysis. Behavioral Flessner, C. A. (2004). Comparison of two programs
Interventions, 14(4), 223–231. https://doi.org/10.1002/ to teach firearm injury prevention skills to 6-and
(SICI)1099-­0 78X(199910/12)14:4<223::AID-­ 7-year-old children. Pediatrics, 114(3), e294–e299.
BIN37>3.0.CO;2-­Y https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2003-­0635-­L
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020). Ghaziuddin, M. (2002). Asperger syndrome: Associated
Autism spectrum disorder. Retrieved from https:// psychiatric and medical conditions. Focus on Autism
www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/index.html and Other Developmental Disabilities, 17(3), 138–
Cihon, J. H., Weinkauf, S., & Taubman, M. (2017). Using 144. https://doi.org/10.1177/10883576020170030301
the teaching interaction procedure to teach social skills Gunby, K.  V., & Rapp, J.  T. (2014). The use of behav-
for individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disor- ioral skills training and in situ feedback to protect
der. In J. B. Leaf (Ed.), Handbook of social skills and children with autism from abduction lures. Journal of
autism spectrum disorder (pp.  313–324). Springer. Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(4), 856–860. https://
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­62995-­7 doi.org/10.1002/jaba.173
Cowen, E. L., Pederson, A., Babigian, H., Isso, L. D., & Harchik, A. E., Sherman, J. A., Sheldon, J. B., & Strouse,
Trost, M.  A. (1973). Long-term follow-up of early M.  C. (1992). Ongoing consultation as a method of
detected vulnerable children. Journal of Consulting improving performance of staff members in a group
and Clinical Psychology, 41(3), 438–446. https://doi. home. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(3),
org/10.1037/h0035373 599–610. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­599
DiGennaro, R. F. D., Novak, M. D., Henley, A. J., Brand, Haseltine, B., & Miltenberger, R.  G. (1990). Teaching
D., & McDonald, M. E. (2017). Evidence-based inter- self-protection skills to persons with mental retarda-
ventions. In J. B. Leaf (Ed.), Handbook of social skills tion. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 95(2),
and autism spectrum disorder (pp. 139–154). Springer. 188–197.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­62995-­7_9 Hazel, J. S., Schumaker, J. B., Sherman, J. A., & Sheldon-
Dogan, R. K., King, M. L., Fishetti, A. T., Lake, C. M., Wildgen, J.  A. (1983). Social skills training with
Matthews, T.  L., & Warzak, W.  J. (2017). Parent-­ court-adjusted youths. In C. LeCroy (Ed.), Social skills
implemented behavioral skills training of social skills. training for children and youth (pp. 117–137). Haworth
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(4), 805–818. Press. https://doi.org/10.1300/J024v05n03_08
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.411 Himle, B.  H., Miltenberger, R.  G., Flessner, C., &
Dotson, W.  H., Leaf, J.  B., Sheldon, J.  B., & Sherman, Gatheridge, B. (2004). Teaching safety skills to chil-
J. A. (2010). Group teaching of conversational skills dren to prevent gun play. Journal of Applied Behavior
to adolescents on the autism spectrum. Research in
264 A. N. Creem et al.

Analysis, 37(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1901/ with autism: Outcomes of a behavioral skills training
jaba.2004.37-­1 program. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(2),
Hogan, A., Knez, N., & Kahng, S. W. (2015). Evaluating 233–256. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.196
the use of behavioral skills training to improve school Maenner, M.  J., Shaw, K., Baio, J., Washington, A.,
staffs’ implementation of behavior intervention plans. Patrick, M., DiRienzo, M., Christensen, D., Wiggins,
Journal of Behavioral Education, 24(2), 242–254. L., Pettygrove, S., Andrews, J. G., Lopez, M., Hudson,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­014-­9213-­9 A., Baroud, T., Schwenk, Y., White, T., Robinson
Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, Rosenberg, C., Lee, L., Harrington, R., Huston, M.,
S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject & Dietz, P. (2020). Prevalence of autism spectrum dis-
research to identify evidence-based practice in spe- order among children aged 8 year-- autism and devel-
cial education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 165–179. opmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites,
https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203 United States, 2016. (2020, March 26). Retrieved from
Johnson, B.  M., Miltenberger, R.  G., Egemo-Helm, K., https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/volumes/69/ss/ss6904a1.
Jostad, C. M., Flessner, C., & Gatheridge, B. (2005). htm?s_cid=ss6904a1_w
Evaluation of behavioral skills training for teaching Maloney, D. M., Harper, T. M., Braukmann, C. J., Fixsen,
abduction prevention skills to young children. Journal D. L., Phillips, E. L., & Wolf, M. M. (1976). Teaching
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(1), 67–78. https:// conversation-related skills to predelinquent girls.
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.26-­04 Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9(3), 371–371.
Johnson, B.  M., Miltenberger, R.  G., Knudson, P., https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1976.9-­371
EgemoHelm, K., Kelso, P., Jostad, C., & Langley, Matson, J.  L., & Stephens, R.  M. (1978). Increasing
L. (2006). A preliminary evaluation of two behav- appropriate behavior of explosive chronic psychi-
ioral skills training procedures for teaching abduc- atric patients with a social-skills training package.
tion prevention skills to school children. Journal of Behavior Modification, 2(1), 61–76. https://doi.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 39(1), 25–34. https://doi. org/10.1177/014544557821004
org/10.1901/jaba.2006.167-­04 Mayes, S.  D., Gorman, A.  A., Hillwig-Garcia, J., &
Jones, R.  T., & Kazdin, A.  E. (1980). Teaching chil- Syed, E. (2013). Suicide ideation and attempts in
dren how and when to make emergency telephone children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum
calls. Behavior Therapy, 11(4), 509–521. https://doi. Disorders, 7(1), 109–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
org/10.1016/S0005-­7894(80)80067-­0 rasd.2012.07.009
Kassardjian, A., Rudrud, E., Taubman, M., Leaf, J.  B., Miltenberger, R. G. (2008). Teaching safety skills to chil-
Edwards, A., Schulze, K., McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. dren: Prevention of firearm injury as an exemplar of
(2013). Utilizing teaching interactions to facilitate best practice in assessment, training, and generaliza-
social skills in the natural environment. Education and tion of safety skills. Behavior Analysis in Practice,
Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 1(1), 30–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391718
48(2), 245–257. Miltenberger, R.  G. (2016). Behavior modification:
Kornacki, L.  T., Ringdahl, J.  E., Sjostrom, A., & Principles and procedures (6th ed.). Cengage
Nuernberger, J. E. (2013). A component analysis of a Learning.
behavioral skills training package used to teach con- Miltenberger, R.  G., Zerger, H.  M., Novotny, M., &
versation skills to young adults with autism spectrum Livingston, C. P. (2017). Behavioral skills training to
and other developmental disorders. Research in Autism promote social behavior of individuals with autism. In
Spectrum Disorders, 7(11), 1370–1376. https://doi. J. B. Leaf (Ed.), Handbook of social skills and autism
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.07.012 spectrum disorder: Assessment, curricula, and inter-
Leaf, J. B., Taubman, M., Bloomfield, S., Palos-Rafuse, L., vention. Springer.
Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Oppenheim, M. L. (2009). Minkin, N., Braukmann, C.  J., Minkin, B.  L., Timbers,
Increasing social skills and pro-social behavior for G. D., Timbers, B. J., Fixsen, D. L., Phillips, E. L., &
three children diagnosed with autism through the use Wolf, M. M. (1976). The social validation and training
of a teaching package. Research in Autism Spectrum of conversational skills. Journal of Applied Behavior
Disorders, 3(1), 275–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Analysis, 9(2), 127–139. https://doi.org/10.1901/
rasd.2008.07.003 jaba.1976.9-­127
Leaf, J.  B., Townley-Cochran, D., Taubman, M., Cihon, Nigro-Bruzzi, D., & Sturmey, P. (2010). The effects of
J. H., Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L., Kassardjian, A., Leaf, behavioral skills training on mand training by staff
R., McEachin, J., & Pentz, T. G. (2015). The teaching and unprompted vocal mands by children. Journal of
interaction procedure and behavioral skills training for Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 757–761. https://
individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­757
A review and commentary. Review Journal of Autism Nuernberger, J.  E., Ringdahl, J.  E., Vargo, K.  K.,
and Developmental Disorder, 2(4), 402–413. https:// Crumpecker, A. C., & Gunnarsson, K. F. (2013). Using
doi.org/10.1007/s40489-­015-­0060-­y a behavioral skills training package to teach conversa-
Lerman, D. C., Hawkins, L., Hillman, C., Shireman, M., tion skills to young adults with autism spectrum dis-
& Nissen, M. A. (2015). Adults with autism spectrum orders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(2),
disorder as behavior technicians for young children 411–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.09.004
14  Using the Teaching Interaction Procedure and Behavioral Skills Training to Develop Skills… 265

Palmen, A., & Didden, R. (2012). Task engagement in Sarakoff, R.  A., & Sturmey, P. (2004). The effects of
young adults with high-functioning autism spec- behavioral skills training on staff implementation of
trum disorders: Generalization effects of behav- discrete-trial teaching. Journal of Applied Behavior
ioral skills training. Research in Autism Spectrum Analysis, 37(4), 535–538. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Disorders, 6(4), 1377–1388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jaba.2004.37-­535
rasd.2012.05.010 Sterling, L., Dawson, G., Estes, A., & Greenson, J.
Palmen, A., Didden, R., & Arts, M. (2008). Improving (2008). Characteristics associated with presence of
question asking in high-functioning adolescents with depressive symptoms in adults with autism spec-
autism spectrum disorders: Effectiveness of small-­ trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
group training. Autism, 12(1), 83–98. https://doi. Disorders, 38(6), 1011–1018. https://doi.org/10.1007/
org/10.1177/1362361307085265 s10803-­007-­0477-­y
Parsons, M.  B., Rollyson, J.  H., & Reid, D.  H. (2012). Stocco, C. S., Thompson, R. H., Hart, J. M., & Soriano,
Evidence-based staff training: A guide for practi- H.  L. (2017). Improving the interview skills of col-
tioners. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 5(2), 2–11. lege students using behavioral skills training. Journal
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391819 of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(3), 495–510. https://
Peters, L. C., & Thompson, R. H. (2015). Teaching chil- doi.org/10.1002/jaba.385
dren with autism to respond to conversation partners’ Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol-
interest. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(3), ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior
544–562. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.235 Analysis, 10(2), 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Peters, B., Tullis, C., & Gallagher, P. (2016). Effects of jaba.1977.10-­349
a group teaching interaction procedure on the social Stokes, T. F., & Osnes, P. G. (1989). An operant pursuit
skills of students with autism spectrum disorders. of generalization. Behavior Therapy, 20(3), 337–355.
Education and Training in Autism and Developmental https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-­7894(89)80054-­1
Disabilities, 51(4), 421–433. Taubman, M. T., Leaf, R. B., McEachin, J., & Driscoll, M.
Phillips, E.  L. (1968). Achievement place: Token rein- (2011). Crafting connections: Contemporary applied
forcement procedures in a home-style rehabilita- behavior analysis for enriching the social lives of per-
tion setting for “predelinquent” boys. Journal of sons with autism spectrum disorder. DRL Books.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(3), 213–223. https://doi. Vanselow, N. R., & Hanley, G. P. (2014). An evaluation of
org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­213 computerized behavioral skills training to teach safety
Phillips, E.  L., Phillips, E.  A., Fixsen, D.  L., & Wolf, skills to young children. Journal of Applied Behavior
M.  M. (1971). Achievement place: Modification of Analysis, 47(1), 51–69. https://doi.org/10.1002/
the behaviors of pre-delinquent boys within a token jaba.105
economy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 4(1), Williams White, S., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007).
45–59. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1971.4-­45 Social skills development in children with autism spec-
Rossi, M. R., Vladescu, J. C., Reeve, K. F., & Gross, A. C. trum disorders: A review of the intervention research.
(2017). Teaching safety responding to children with Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
autism spectrum disorder. Education and Treatment 37(10), 1858–1868. https://doi.org/10.1007/
of Children, 40(2), 187–208. https://doi.org/10.1353/ s10803-­006-­0320-­x
etc.2017.0009 Wurtele, S.  K., Saslawsky, D.  A., Miller, C.  L., Marrs,
Ryan, G., Brady, S., Holloway, J., & Lydon, H. (2019). S.  R., & Britcher, J.  C. (1986). Teaching personal
Increasing appropriate conversation skills using a safety skills for potential prevention of sexual abuse:
behavioral skills training package for adults with A comparison of treatments. Journal of Consulting
intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorder. and Clinical Psychology, 54(5), 688–692. https://doi.
Journal of Intellectual Disabilities, 23(4), 567–580. org/10.1037/0022-­006X.54.5.688
https://doi.org/10.1177/1744629517750744 Zane, T., Davis, C., & Rosswurm, M. (2008). The cost
Sanchez, S., & Miltenberger, R.  G. (2015). Evaluating of fad treatments in autism. Journal of Early and
the effectiveness of an abduction prevention program Intensive Behavior Intervention, 5(2), 44–51. https://
for young adults with intellectual disabilities. Child & doi.org/10.1037/h0100418
Family Behavior Therapy, 37(3), 197–207. https://doi.
org/10.1080/07317107.2015.1071178
Developing Social Skills Groups
for Behavioral Intervention 15
for Individuals with Autism

Christine M. Milne and Ashley Creem

15.1 Social Skills Groups leads to the benefits of teaching complex social
behaviors, or social skills, in a group setting.
Social behavior, which includes anything from Given that social contingencies mitigate or
sitting in a public space to engaging in interper- strengthen social behavior, it is important to rec-
sonal conversations, is shaped by our verbal com- ognize the variables that contribute to the com-
munity (Skinner, 1953). Within that verbal plexity of social interactions, especially when
community, there are contingencies arranged for attempting to develop a new skill or decrease the
which social behaviors will result in reinforcing probability of an existing one. When teaching
or punishing1 consequences, but individuals will social skills in a group setting, there is an
continue to experience variability and variation, increased number of instructors and peers, which
or even exceptions, within and across those con- immediately allows individuals within the group
tingencies (Skinner, 1953). Therefore, in order to to experience and/or observe contingencies
teach social skills that will result in desirable out- across more people. This opportunity for interac-
comes for the individual, it is important to teach tion with a variety of individuals may be limited
the variable contingencies that the individual is in most 1:1 therapeutic sessions. Not only is the
more likely to encounter. Initially, it may be nec- mere number of individuals with whom to inter-
essary to use an arbitrary contingency to develop act greater in a group setting, but there is more
a skill or reduce the probability of a particular likely to be variability in the types of responses
behavior, but it would be advantageous to transi- one individual will encounter. Therefore, within a
tion to more naturally occurring contingencies as group setting, an individual is more likely to
quickly as possible (Taubman et al., 2011). This experience a spectrum of contingencies that one
will more likely encounter in the terminal setting
The term punishment, in this case, describes the contin-
1 
(Ellingsen et  al., 2017; Taubman et  al., 2011).
gencies that result in a reduction in the likelihood of a With that, it is also important to consider the vari-
particular response or response class. This includes peers
ignoring certain responses to telling them to “stop.”
ability in the rate of consequences when working
in a group setting versus an individual setting.
With the higher ratio of students to instructors,
C. M. Milne (*) the rate of reinforcement or punishment is likely
Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, to differ from the rate within a low ratio or one on
USA
e-mail: cmilne@apfmail.org
one setting. Of course, this rate may be manipu-
lated (and should be manipulated) through the
A. Creem
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA,
development of skill acquisition, but overall, it is
USA more likely to be intermittent and resemble the
© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 267
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_15
268 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

ratio they are likely to encounter outside of the responsive to their attempts at engagement. After
teaching setting. repeated exposure to these contingencies, indi-
Finally, when addressing variability in social viduals on the spectrum may reduce attempts to
contingencies, it is paramount to recognize and engage with peers, and immediately initiate inter-
analyze the potential effects of those contingen- actions with surrounding adults. However, this
cies over time. Individuals that engage in social could be minimized if the complexity of contin-
skills groups for a limited period of time will ide- gencies with peers is addressed, and the rein-
ally benefit from the short-term effects of devel- forcement from adults is minimized. The
oping social skills. They are likely to experience presentation of a peer should act as a stimulus for
immediate consequences such as engaging in a a variety of consequences, given a particular
few conversations, playing with peers in certain response. For example, the presence of a peer
activities, and so on. However, it is important to may allow for a conversation about a preferred
consider the potential effects of engaging in a topic, or engagement in a preferred activity that
social skills group for longer periods of time. requires more than one person. In the absence of
With continued engagement with a number of a peer, conversation on a preferred topic, or
familiar peers for longer periods of time, indi- engagement in a particular activity is not avail-
viduals are not only able to develop social skills able. Within social skills groups, a variety of
but hopefully increase the likelihood of develop- salient social stimuli and consequences can be
ing social relationships (Taubman et  al., 2011). introduced for certain behaviors. For example,
Shared experiences and social skills maintained when engaging in a particular conversation topic,
over time help build those social relationships, one peer may simply smile while another peer
and while true, reciprocal friendships cannot be provides continuous statements about the topic
forced, it is important to at least provide increased without displaying behaviors associated with
opportunities to do so (Leaf, 2017; Taubman positive affect (e.g., smile). While both peers dis-
et al., 2011). An additional benefit to prolonged play different behaviors (i.e., positive affect with
interactions within a social skills group is that no comments vs. commenting without positive
instructors may point out complexities of social affect), an individual continuing to engage in dis-
behavior that typically occur over time. For cussion on the particular topic may result in simi-
example, there may be a student that typically lar consequences (e.g., those peers seeking out
shares play items, a behavior likely associated the individual to interact in the future).
with “being a good friend.” However, for what- Alternatively, both peers may respond similarly
ever reason, the student might not share that day. (e.g., smile), then by continuing the conversation,
The isolated incident should not change that the may result in different consequences (e.g., one
student is considered a “good” friend. Instead, it peer seeks the individual for future conversation
may highlight that the student has occasional while the other does not). One of the benefits of
lapses in friendly behavior. With a limited num- social skills groups is that it allows individuals to
ber of interactions, it may be more difficult to experience the complexity of social interactions,
highlight patterns of behavior. With sufficient and opportunities to learn the nuances of social
sessions, such patterns may be helpfully observed, behavior and the skills needed to adapt based on
and may lead to more targeted interventions or the history of consequences within the social
may identify changes in interactional patterns skills group.
that need attention. Social skills groups offer the opportunity to
Another benefit to teaching social behaviors in pair peers with reinforcing events. For some indi-
social skills groups is that it allows teaching peers viduals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD),
as a social stimulus. Often peers may ignore the opportunity to interact with peers may not
social initiation attempts by individuals with motivate them to engage in appropriate social
autism, therefore the individual with ASD might skills, and therefore, pairing may be necessary
turn to the surrounding adults that may be more (Leaf, 2017). In these cases, social skills groups
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 269

provide an opportunity to pair, or condition peers address skills in acquisition and maximize skill
with other reinforcing activities. For example, in strengths embedded within a setting that requires
order to engage in a preferred activity like freeze interaction with peers that will ideally parallel
dance, they must do so with peers. In other cases, experiences they will encounter in the future. The
some individuals with ASD are socially moti- outcomes are more easily manipulable so that all
vated though may not have the skills to interact individuals within the group access reinforce-
appropriately (Leaf, Leaf, et al., 2016). Therefore, ment for engaging in specific behaviors, espe-
one may use peers as a motivating factor for indi- cially when those behaviors are in acquisition. As
viduals to engage in certain behaviors in order to the target behavior is strengthened, those same
access peers and maintain positive interactions variables that were manipulated should eventu-
with those peers. In addition, peers may be used ally more closely resemble those in the terminal
to introduce socially motivated individuals to environment.
engage in activities that they may have previ- In order to produce the most meaningful out-
ously considered neutral or even aversive (Leaf, come, social skills groups must also improve
Oppenheim-Leaf, et al., 2016). Therefore, using social skills performance (Wolstencroft et  al.,
peers to condition common activities to be more 2018). Social skills performance differs from
preferred or at least less aversive. social skill acquisition in that social skill acquisi-
Skinner (1953) discusses how individuals may tion mitigates social behavior deficits resulting
behave as a unit, or behave together, to meet a from a lack of knowledge to perform a social
common goal. Within a social skills group, there behavior, whereas social skills performance
are more opportunities to manipulate the environ- allows an individual to apply the social skills
ment in such a way to practice behaviors that knowledge already acquired during naturally
require group cooperation. For example, perhaps occurring situations (Wolstencroft et  al., 2018).
a desired game is too high to reach, and a box is Therefore, it may be necessary to train toward
needed to climb on to reach the game. However, generalization so that the targeted behavior is
the box is too heavy for one student to move it by brought under the control of the terminal stimu-
themselves. Therefore, more students work lus and this control spreads to other stimuli
together to push the box toward the shelf in order (Skinner, 1953). Generalization and maintenance
to reach the game. For a more complex example, strategies such as using natural contingencies,
in order to engage in an exciting activity, like a training diversely, and embedding functional
bubble party, they may have to solve a series of mediators (Stokes & Baer, 1977; Stokes & Osnes,
puzzles to access items for that activity. By 1989) may be more easily applied within a social
assigning roles to individuals in the group, one skills group format, as opposed to a 1:1 format.
can address individual targets (e.g., initiating For example, since peers are present, natural con-
comments, responding to others) within the over- tingencies can easily be programmed for by
all group target (e.g., communication). For exam- implementing teaching during naturally occur-
ple, one individual can see a completed block ring social situations such as having the individ-
structure but cannot access blocks and another ual ask to join an ongoing game of tag. The
individual has the blocks but cannot see the com- therapist can coach the peers in the social skills
pleted block structure. The individual who sees group to accept this initiation so it is followed by
the completed block structure must tell the other the consequence of being able to participate in
individual with the blocks how to imitate the the game of tag. Training diversely can also be
block structure. Upon building the structure easily applied to the social skills group format.
accurately, they will earn bubble wands. Activities Again, by having peers present, the individual
such as these continue until they acquire all items may now practice the skill of initiating to join
needed to engage in the group activity, a bubble play across different peers and activities. The
party. By creating opportunities like these, individual can also initiate using different
instructors can manipulate the activities to best responses such as “Can I play with you?” or
270 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

“Hey, I’d like to join!” All of which will be rein- overall results were positive; however, not all
forced by natural consequences and promote studies had strong effects, or there were inconsis-
response generalization and maintenance. tent results. Additionally, most of the studies
Finally, by incorporating stimuli similar to those included individuals with more advanced skill-
that may be present in the natural environment sets or typical cognitive functioning levels; there-
(i.e., functional mediators) such as a playgroup or fore, the efficacy of a social skills group for
circle area, or cues such as gesturing, can be eas- individuals with an atypical cognitive function-
ily embedded to transfer stimulus control from ing level is still unknown. Finally, they recom-
programmed stimuli to stimuli that may be pres- mend the evaluation of social skills groups
ent in the natural environment. Since social implemented in applied settings such as class-
behavior is socially mediated (Skinner, 1953), rooms to determine the social validity of this
having peers present to deliver a variety of dis- intervention procedure. These results and recom-
criminative stimuli and consequences allows for mendations are consistent with a previous meta-­
the strategies for generalization and maintenance analysis conducted by White et al. (2007).
to be easily programmed. In 2012, Reichow et al. did a follow-up review
analyzing studies that used randomized control tri-
als (RCTs) to assess the effects of social skills
15.2 Literature Overview groups for individuals aged 6–21 years with autism
on social competence, social communication, and
The effects of group-based social skills training quality of life. A total of five studies met the search
have and continue to be evaluated (Atkinson-­ criteria for analysis that specifically required a
Jones & Hewitt, 2018; Reichow et  al., 2012; treatment group (i.e., social skills group) and a no-
Reichow & Volkmar, 2010; White et  al., 2007; treatment or waitlist treatment group (i.e., no social
Wolstencroft et al., 2018). While there have been skills group). Sessions occurred across 5–20 weeks,
inconsistent results regarding the efficacy of with most sessions occurring weekly for 60–90 min.
group teaching of social skills, the research gen- Different standardized assessment scales were
erally has shown positive results, though future used across different social skills measures. Social
research is needed (Gates et al., 2017; Reichow competence was measured through the Social
& Volkmar, 2010; White et  al., 2007). Group Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham & Elliott,
teaching formats are typically referred to as 1990), Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS;
social skills groups (Reichow & Volkmar, 2010; Constantino & Gruber, 2012), or Social
Reichow et al., 2012) or group social skills inter- Competency Inventory (SCI; Rydell et al., 1997).
ventions (GSSI; Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; Social communication was measured using the
Gates et  al., 2017; Wolstencroft et  al., 2018). Idiomatic Language subtest of the Comprehensive
These terms may be used interchangeably as they Assessment of Spoken Language (Carrow-
were described in the literature, though outside Woolfolk, 1999). Emotion regulation was mea-
the literature review, these will be referred to as sured using Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal
social skills groups. Accuracy 2 (DANVA-2; Nowicki, 1997). Quality
Reichow and Volkmar (2010) conducted a of life was reported differently across studies,
review to evaluate the research for social skills depending on different aspects of quality of life.
interventions that include the use of social skills Friendship was measured using The Friendship
groups for individuals with autism. In this review, Qualities Scale (Bukowski et al., 1994) and a popu-
Reichow and Volkmar not only synthesized the larity subscale from the Piers-Harris Self-Concept
results of interventions but the quality of the Scale (Piers, 1984). Loneliness was tracked using
studies such as the methodological rigor and Loneliness Scale (Asher et al., 1984) and depres-
design of each study. Of the 66 studies examined, sion was measured using Beck Depression
only five evaluated teaching social skills within a Inventory (Beck et al., 1996). None of the studies
social skills group. Within these five studies, the reported individual behaviors for participants.
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 271

When comparing the treatment group to the they should do so, and if the results can be gener-
no treatment group, improvements were found in alized to the natural environment and maintained
the treatment group when it came to social com- over time. After screening, 15 RCTs were
petence and friendship quality (Reichow et  al., included in the review.
2012). However, no significant differences were In Jonsson et  al.’ (2016) review, participants
noted between the groups when measuring emo- were children (6–13  years of age for 11 trials)
tional recognition or social communication and adolescents (13–18 years of age for four tri-
related to idioms (Reichow et al., 2012). In addi- als), mostly male, and all diagnosed with ASD or
tion, there was some report of decreased loneli- a related diagnosis (e.g., pervasive developmen-
ness, but no effects on child or parent depression tal disorder—not otherwise specified). While two
(Reichow et al., 2012). Though there were results studies did not include how the diagnoses were
that showed improvement from social skills confirmed, all other participant diagnoses were
groups in areas like social competence and confirmed using standardized diagnostic tools.
decreased loneliness, the overall quality of evi- Participants had a minimum intelligence quotient
dence was considered low. This is because of fac- (IQ) or verbal IQ level of 60–85  in all but one
tors such as inconsistency in data, varying trial. Participants were recruited from academic
assessments used across studies, and varying centers, clinics, schools, or local organizations
interventions used within social skills groups. A (i.e., 13 trials), or through a public announcement
major component that compromises the quality (i.e., two trials). The trials were completed in sev-
of evidence is that the reliance of parent and eral countries (i.e., Australia, Canada, France,
interventionist reports for standardized assess- Netherlands, South Korea, USA) and across dif-
ments could result in biased results if they are ferent settings (e.g., universities, clinics, schools).
aware of which interventions are implemented Information on treatment providers was often not
and when these interventions are implemented. provided; however, for studies that did provide
Based on Reichow et al. (2012), future research treatment provider information, trials had up to
on social skills group intervention is needed to four providers with varied qualifications through-
strengthen the reliability of the results. Additional out trials (i.e., post-graduate, graduate, or under-
research is also required before conclusions can graduate students, psychologists,
be generalized and recommendations can be psychotherapists, psychiatrists, social workers,
made, especially since research has typically registered nurses, and speech and language
been conducted with individuals of a standard pathologists) and had varied experience conduct-
cognitive function level. Furthermore, studies ing social skills groups for individuals with
evaluating social skills groups report varying ASD.  All trials collected data on the provider’s
results across children and adolescents, limiting treatment fidelity. Information provided on the
the generality of these data. type of social skills group used (e.g., PEERS;
Jonsson et  al. (2016) completed a literature Program for the Education and Enrichment of
review to investigate the extent to which social Relational Skills for Young Adults) was also lim-
skills group intervention results gathered in RCTs ited, and the type of interventions varied greatly.
can be generalized to other settings and individu- Social skills groups also focused on different
als. Jonsson and colleagues evaluated aspects social skills areas such as verbal and non-verbal
such as the source population (i.e., those individ- interaction, social interactions, conversation
uals that met eligibility criteria for the study), skills, and social problem-solving. Similar to the
including population (i.e., those who actually type of social skills group used, outcome mea-
participated in the study), context, treatment pro- sures varied across trials. Outcome measures
vider, type of intervention used, and outcome to used included blinded observation, blind
identify for whom and what settings the results of ­assessment, follow-up assessments, and pre- and
social skills group interventions can be applied, post-­test measures. Some studies measured anxi-
who can implement social skills groups and how ety and depression using either a global assess-
272 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

ment of everyday functioning or an assessment of mean standardized verbal ability of 100.01


the change in the clinical global impression. (range, 86.3–106.26). Overall, Gates et al. found
Jonsson et  al. (2016) identified several areas that there was a medium effect for those who par-
for future research. To begin, there is an overall ticipated in a treatment group from those within
need for more rigorous investigations on the the control group. Across assessment measures,
external validity of social skills group research. regardless of informant, were considered to eval-
Studies should specifically evaluate the ability to uate social competence. Sixteen studies used par-
clinically apply and implement social skills ent reports. Within these 16 studies, there was a
groups. Studies should also include information small difference in effect for those who were in
such as the characteristics of the source popula- the treatment group when compared to those who
tion to allow for the generalizability of the results were in the control group. The four studies that
to be more effectively assessed. Future studies included teacher reports also reported a small
should be less selective with their populations to effect, but overall did not report a difference from
better represent realistic clinical populations. For GSSIs. In comparison, the 10 studies that used
example, populations most commonly included self-report measures resulted in a large effect on
white males. This is not representative of indi- social competence for those in the treatment
viduals of varied genders or ethnicities. To repre- group than those in the control group. The five
sent an even broader population, individuals with studies using observer reports ranged from no
a variety of skillsets and social-economic statuses effect to large effects, but overall resulted in a
should also be included. Jonsson and colleagues small effect for those within the treatment groups
acknowledge this may weaken the internal valid- compared to control groups. When using task-­
ity of the study, it will strengthen the external based measures, within the eight studies, effect
validity. Similarly, since individual differences sizes ranged from small to large, but overall had
can impact the generalizability of results, studies a medium effect.
should include participant descriptors of charac- Overall, there were positive effects in improv-
teristics such as comorbidities to better assess ing social competence for individuals who par-
who the results can be applied to and how these ticipated in GSSIs versus those within a control
characteristics impact treatment efficacy. Future group. Interestingly, those who used self-report
research is also needed to assess the efficacy of assessments mentioned that participants did
social skills groups with cultural modifications. report gains in social knowledge, but did not nec-
Finally, Jonsson et al. recommend studies include essarily change their social behaviors. This infor-
more blind observations in everyday environ- mation would be consistent with the smaller
ments, more data on follow-up and long-term effect sizes from parent and teachers reports,
effects, and reports on the client and caregiver where they may observe small to moderate effects
treatment preference to determine its impact on in their social behavior. Gates et al. mention that
treatment outcome. it may be beneficial to consider opportunities for
Gates et al. (2017) conducted a meta-analysis participants to practice implementing the social
to evaluate the effects of GSSIs within well-­ behaviors taught within GSSIs, which may have
designed RCTs while considering the assessment an effect on performance in general settings. In
measures (i.e., parent report, teacher report, self-­ addition, it would be important to assess if the
report, task-based assessment, observer report) strategies used in the studies that utilized task-­
used to evaluate efficacy. In addition, they com- based measures aimed to improve skills or are
pared if these effects differed across assessments merely “teaching to the test.”
within the study, as well as across different fea- Wolstencroft et al. (2018) completed a meta-­
tures of the intervention. The participants within analysis of 593 articles, which examined the
this meta-analysis ranged from 5.30 to 20.42 years effectiveness of GSSIs in developing social per-
of age, with a mean overall standardized cogni- formance. Wolstencroft et  al. reviewed articles
tive ability of 102.27 (range, 87.55–112.45), and measuring participant social performance using
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 273

the parental report and analyzed measures of out- a significant effect (p  <  0.0001). On the SSRS,
come based on the degree of change in the SRS the GGSI also showed improvement with a mod-
and SSRS scores. Wolstencroft and colleagues erate effect size. Moderator analysis of the SRS
also evaluated the effect of specific intervention was completed to find differences in total SRS
factors to determine their impact on the improve- scores between groups by separating studies
ment of social knowledge and performance skills. based on the GSSI program used (e.g., SENSE
The participants in the studies ranged from 6 to Theater). No significant difference was detected
25  years of age. Five different programs were between the SENSE Theater and CBT social
used across the 593 articles examined. These skills groups and the controls; however, summer-
included PEERS, Children’s Friendship Training, MAX and PEERS resulted in large and signifi-
summerMAX and SENSE Theatre, and an cant positive effect sizes. Group differences on
unnamed manualized Cognitive Behavioral total SRS scores were also evaluated by analyz-
Therapy (CBT) social skills program. SENSE ing parent involvement and intensity and dura-
Theatre was the only program to utilize a perfor- tion of the GSSI program. Programs with more
mance teaching strategy. All of the studies parent involvement and greater intensity and
included child groups and most included parent duration achieved larger effect sizes. Overall,
groups. The programs included in the studies Wolstencroft et al. found that GSSI studies often
ranged in intensity and duration, with summer- rely on informant reports for outcome measures.
MAX and the SENSE Theatre employing the Future research should utilize more objective,
most intensive models. The GSSIs included in blind measures and also include participant social
the meta-analysis targeted different social skills validity measures.
domains which included social knowledge, social Spain and Blainey (2015) completed the first
communication, social cognition, and social systematic review to evaluate the effectiveness of
emotions. While all studies included needed to social skills groups for adults with ASD who had
have used the SRS and/or the SSRS, other assess- more well-developed skill sets, or adults diag-
ment measures may have been included and all nosed with ASD with an IQ within the typical
studies used a parent informant as well as a par- range. The review included 5 articles that met the
ticipant, staff, or teacher informant. inclusion criteria. Three of these studies described
Wolstencroft et al. (2018) completed a “risk of a single-arm intervention and two included quasi-­
bias” analysis for all RCTs in seven different experimental methods using a treatment as usual
areas. These areas included sequence generation, control or a waitlist control. In terms of the qual-
allocation concealment, baseline measurements, ity of these articles, all had small sample sizes
blinding of participants and personnel, blinding (i.e., 6–10 participants). Furthermore, only two
of outcome assessors, incomplete outcome data, had control groups and Spain and Blainey
and selective outcome reporting. Notable is that described them as “perhaps best considered pilot
all studies obtained a “high risk” rating due to studies” (p. 876). Most of the participants in the
incomplete blinding of the outcome by partici- studies were young adult males with 85% of the
pants, personnel, and outcome assessors. participants being male with a mean age of
Additionally, two-thirds of the studies were rated 25.8 years. Though, the range of the participants
high risk for incomplete outcome data. However, was 18–55 years of age. Four of the groups met
selective outcome reporting was rated low-risk weekly and one met monthly. The studies
across all of the evaluated studies. Two studies included 8–18 sessions which lasted between
received a “high risk” rating in four or more of 50 min to 2.5 hr., with at least two staff leading
the seven criteria, receiving more “high risk” rat- each group. One study included a parent treat-
ings than the other RCTs. In terms of the SRS ment group in addition to the participant group.
scores, a large effect size was shown for GSSIs, The goal of this group was to teach the parents to
and GSSIs produced greater participant improve- support the participants’ skill development. One
ments than participants in the control group with other study included a parent group but this group
274 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

was self-directed and optional, although partici- ing scale or role-play demonstration. Self-report
pation was encouraged. indicated decreased measures in the area of lone-
In Spain and Blainey’s (2015) review, the liness and increased positive attitude toward
studies analyzed targeted a variety of skills which peers, as well as a perceived improvement in
included emotional understanding and under- social communication skills. Performance on
standing situations, as well as providing informa- role-plays varied across studies but also reported
tion on friendships, social understanding, and overall improvement. Participants within the
social problem-solving. Some studies aimed to studies also reported lower levels of anxiety and
integrate these skills into real-life scenarios. The depression but with small effect sizes and large
methods for developing these skills included ranges. Finally, the satisfaction with the interven-
didactic teaching (e.g., discussing social prob- tion was rated positive overall across participants.
lems and encouraging participants to develop It is important to note that only one study reported
solutions to the problem), small and large group on fidelity.
discussions, practical tasks (e.g., role plays or In summary, the results of Spain and Blainey’s
evaluating videos), and a supportive group model. (2015) review suggest that social skills groups
Two studies gave homework to encourage may be effective for adults with ASD within a
between-session learning. Spain and Blainey typical IQ range, yet areas for future research
noted some concerns regarding outcome mea- exist. Spain and Blainey expressed an overall
sures. For one, no studies included baseline or need for more social skills groups studies with
follow-up measures. There was also a lack of individuals with high functioning ASD, some of
consistent outcome measures across studies, which should specifically examine the ability of
making comparison difficult. Finally, some out- an intervention to decrease levels of anxiety,
come measures were unavailable due to reasons depression, and other comorbidities. These stud-
such as participant refusal, lack of participant ies should be completed across participants with
motivation, or attrition. varying levels of symptom severity and clinical
The studies in the review assessed the quality presentation (e.g., male, female, non-binary,
and quantity of the social skills developed and all white, Hispanic, African American). Future stud-
revealed overall positive effects. However, Spain ies should also assess the effect of different group
and Blainey (2015) reported concerns regarding sizes and compare the different techniques used
the reliability and validity of some results and with social skills groups. Finally, Spain and
none of the studies reported the clinical signifi- Blainey recommended future research should use
cance of change for outcome measures. Spain consistent outcome measures and incorporate
and Blainey grouped the results of the studies more social validity measures.
based on the areas measured. These areas Atkinson-Jones and Hewitt (2018) reviewed
included social knowledge and cognition, social the social skills groups literature to evaluate the
functioning, anxiety and depression, and satisfac- effect of social skills groups in developing posi-
tion with the intervention. In the area of social tive social behaviors in adults with ASD and a
knowledge and cognition, results demonstrated mild or moderate intellectual disability (ID). After
significant improvement. However, two studies a screening process, Atkinson-Jones and Hewitt
did not report the overall mean cognitive scores. identified 10 studies. Four of these studies were
This made it unclear whether there was a clini- RCTs, one was a quasi-experimental non -ran-
cally relevant change or just a general trend. domized control study, and the remaining five
Overall, the social skills groups in the review studies utilized quasi-experimental pre- and post-
resulted in improvement in empathy, emotion test designs without a control group. Eight of the
recognition, and Theory of Mind (ToM). The area studies included participants with an ID and most
of social functioning evaluated the participants’ participants were white males. The participants
performance of the skill in their natural setting. within the studies were all diagnosed with ASD
In all studies, this area was assessed using a rat- and ranged in age from 18 to 55 years old. The 10
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 275

studies reviewed included a variety of social skills ple size. One study evaluated the Social Skills
programs including Program for the Education Programme, an 18-session monthly program
and Enrichment of Relational Skills for Young developed for individuals with ASD aimed to
Adults (PEERS-YA; Laugeson & Frankel, 2010), improve conversation skills, assertiveness, social
Aspirations (Hillier et al., 2007), Social Cognition problem solving and social understanding skills,
and Interaction Training for Adults (SCIT-A; and interview skills. The study utilized a pre-post
Turner-Brown et  al., 2008), Social Skills group design without a control group. Findings
Programme (Howlin & Yates, 1999), Social Skills indicated improved social and conversational
Group (Ashman et  al., 2017), and Workplace skills which generalized to other settings; how-
Training Programme (Liu et al., 2013). None of ever, weaknesses in methodology such as a lack
the studies in the review compared any of these of control and standardized outcome measures
programs. Four studies incorporated PEERS-YA, yield caution when considering results (Atkinson-­
a 16-week program for individuals with high Jones & Hewitt, 2018).
functioning ASD utilizing 90  min small group Atkinson-Jones and Hewitt (2018) found one
sessions targeting a variety of social skills. Three RCT that evaluated the Social Skills Group, a
of these studies were rigorously designed RCTs. 16-week program for adults with ASD utilizing
PEERS-YA resulted in positive effects including weekly 60-min sessions targeting social and rela-
improved social knowledge and performance, and tionship skills. No differences were found
increased empathy. Results also maintained. between groups in the study, but the treatment
PEERS-YA was also the most efficient interven- showed trends toward greater improvement in the
tion, taking the least amount of time to complete. areas of ToM and social functioning. Finally, one
Two studies in Atkinson-Jones and Hewitt’s study assessed the effects of the Workplace
(2018) review assessed Aspirations, an eight-­ Training Programme. The Workplace Training
week program for individuals with ASD that Programme is an intensive 6-month program
aims to develop social and vocational under- designed for individuals with ASD and ID to
standing involving weekly hour-long small group develop vocational, communication, and emo-
sessions. Both studies were quasi-experimental tional skills. Reliability validity measures were
pre- and post-test designs without a control group good (i.e., r = 0.80–0.90 for reliability and cor-
and were conducted by the same research group. rectly identifying more than 80% of work reha-
One of these studies confirmed IQ and diagnoses. bilitation placements). Post interventions
No significant differences were found in attitudes demonstrated significant improvement in com-
or feelings toward peers, but one study decreased munication, emotional control, and workplace
levels of anxiety and depression and improved social behavior. The study was completed outside
empathy as evident during the post-test. of the USA and UK which broadened the popula-
Participants of one study demonstrated improved tions studied. However, some factors limit the
interpersonal skills. Neither study completed fol- validity and generality of the findings of the
low-­up measures. study. To begin, measures were not completed by
Atkinson-Jones and Hewitt (2018) found that blind observers, participants, or stakeholders.
the SCIT-A program was evaluated using a quasi-­ Additionally, a small heterogeneous group was
experimental pre- and post-design with a used, intervention was implemented in a con-
treatment-­as-usual (TAU) control group. trolled setting, and the intervention was evaluated
Treatment involved 18 sessions weekly, lasting using a single-arm intervention with no control
50  min each as targeted emotional and ToM group and only one standardized measure for use
development. The post-intervention treatment with people with ID.
group showed improvement in ToM skills but did Based on the review, Atkinson-Jones and
not demonstrate improvement in emotional or Hewitt (2018) identified areas for future research
social communication skills. The study also including more objective outcome measures such
lacked internal validity and included a small sam- as measures of real-life performance in more
276 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

applied settings, evaluating the effectiveness of these designs are strengthened when compared to
social skills groups across a varied population, a control group. In addition, including observers
specifically other than white males. Finally, blind to the treatment groups would further
Atkinson-Jones and Hewitt recommended col- strengthen the results as it limits the possibility of
lecting participant satisfaction ratings. observer bias. Other designs such as a multiple
baseline design across skills or participants may
provide results that more clearly control for vari-
15.2.1 Strengths of Current Research ables outside of the social skills group. In addi-
tion, these types of designs provide direct and
There is a growing body of research in the area of observable data on the behavior being measured.
social skills groups (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, This allows for the identification of any variable
2018; Gates et  al., 2017; Jonsson et  al., 2016; influencing the behavior change. There is an
Reichow et al., 2012; Reichow & Volkmar, 2010; overall need for consistent and unbiased outcome
Spain & Blainey, 2015; Wolstencroft et al., 2018). measures such as blind observations and assess-
The social skills group literature includes evalua- ments (Jonsson et al., 2016; Reichow et al., 2012;
tions of the efficacy of social skills groups across Wolstencroft et  al., 2018), as well as follow up
several different factors such as diagnosis, age, and maintenance measures (Jonsson et al., 2016;
intervention type, and many social skills areas Spain & Blainey, 2015).
(Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; Jonsson et al., Another limitation is minimal evidence sup-
2016; Spain & Blainey, 2015; Wolstencroft et al., porting the generality of social skills groups
2018). There has also been an increase in the (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; Jonsson et al.,
strength of the research designs and measures in 2016; Reichow et al., 2012; Reichow & Volkmar,
which progress is monitored (Atkinson-Jones & 2010; Spain & Blainey, 2015). Rigorous evalua-
Hewitt, 2018; Reichow & Volkmar, 2010). tions should be completed on the external valid-
Research has also begun to assess the impact of ity and generality of social skills groups. The
social skills groups on loneliness, depression, efficacy of social skills groups should be assessed
and anxiety (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; using a larger and less selective population with a
Reichow et al., 2012; Spain & Blainey, 2015) as variety of skill sets, IQ scores, diagnoses, gen-
well as the areas of ToM and emotional regula- ders, races, and ages. As Reichow et  al. (2012)
tion (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; Spain & mentioned, future research must expand its
Blainey, 2015). Research supports the use of examination of the effects of social skills groups
social skills groups to develop social competence across demographics. Current research has lim-
and knowledge (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; ited its efficacy to individuals aged 7–12 years of
Gates et al., 2017; Reichow et al., 2012; Spain & average to above-average intelligence within the
Blainey, 2015; Wolstencroft et al., 2018). USA. Thus, it would be beneficial to evaluate the
effectiveness of social skills groups across differ-
ent ages, IQ scores, and cultures. It will also be
15.2.2 Areas for Future Research valuable to determine the effect of the makeup of
the social skills group (i.e., autism only or with
There are many limitations to the current body of peers) on acquisition.
research which leads to areas for future research. Current research displays weak to no infor-
First, future researchers should consider using mation on the efficacy of skills learned to be
stronger experimental designs and measurement generalized across settings (Atkinson-Jones &
procedures to control for variables such as his- Hewitt, 2018; Gates et  al., 2017; Jonsson
tory, maturation, and observer bias (Atkinson-­ et al., 2016; Reichow & Volkmar, 2010). It is
Jones & Hewitt, 2018;Jonsson et  al., 2016 ; important to evaluate if skills taught within a
Reichow et al., 2012 ; Wolstencroft et al., 2018). social skills group generalize to applied set-
If a pretest–posttest design is used, the results of tings, and if these effects maintain over time
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 277

(Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; Gates et al., 15.3 Clinical Implications


2017; Jonsson et  al., 2016; Reichow &
Volkmar, 2010; Spain & Blainey, 2015). Since individuals with autism are a heteroge-
Further research must also be done across spe- neous group, it is up to the clinician to examine
cific curricula, as well as the procedures used the research to determine under which conditions
to cover the curricula to determine which the interventions may result in substantial behav-
would be most beneficial with certain types of ior change. By better understanding the varying
groups (Jonsson et  al., 2016; Reichow et  al., components and their effects from each study,
2012). There also remains a limitation in mea- clinicians are better able to individualize the
suring skill acquisition from skill performance structure of their social skills group to make it the
(Gates et al., 2017; Wolstencroft et al., 2018). most appropriate for their clients. In addition, cli-
While it is important for individuals to learn nicians have the benefit of making adjustments
skills, the acquisition may be useless if not more freely than when conducting research.
performed when the opportunity is presented. Clinicians may want to track the type of adjust-
Therefore, future research should continue to ments and why adjustments were made in order
measure the performance of skills acquired. It to determine some variables that influenced
is possible teaching in a more natural setting behavior change. As research should inform clin-
will help develop this skill (Stokes & Baer, ical work, clinicians may inform the research of
1977). other strategies that are effective in an applied
Even further, it would be beneficial to exam- setting which may improve the external validity
ine if certain procedures used within a social of future research (Jonsson et al., 2016).
skills group are more efficacious than others Current research may inform clinicians on
within this measure (Jonsson et al., 2016). Future how to measure participant progress. Clinicians
research should compare the efficacy and effec- may want to consider taking direct measures of
tiveness of different types of social skills groups observable behavior to ensure behavior change is
across a variety of factors including different occurring. Even further, it would be beneficial for
social skills and varying populations, settings, clinicians to measure if progress occurs as the
and providers. Additionally, future studies should variables within the structured social skills group
specifically examine the ability of social skills to more closely mimic the terminal environment.
decrease loneliness, depression, and anxiety Those implementing social skills groups may
using objective and rigorous measurement (Spain also consider the curriculum taught to the group.
& Blainey, 2015). Rather than following a manualized curriculum,
Finally, future researchers must continue to perhaps obtaining information from parents and
evaluate the social validity of social skills groups. teachers of which social skill deficits seem to be
This could be done by taking long-term measures most impeding the individual’s daily life may
that evaluate the quality of life, long-lasting help prioritize skills to be taught in the group.
friendships, reciprocal relationships, and profes- Not only will there more likely be an observed
sional relationships. Do participants within these change in behavior but there may also be an
groups feel as though the skills taught are mean- increase in the likelihood of social validity of the
ingful for improving their daily life? In addition, intervention and skills.
do caretakers and other relevant persons in that
person’s life feel as though the individual has
learned meaningful skills and uses them in a way 15.4 Experiences from Clinical
that improves their quality of life? Not only is it Practice
important to measure if the skills themselves are
meaningful, but also if the procedures used to Social skills groups can be constructed for a vari-
teach the skills are acceptable or enjoyable. ety of ages and skills sets (Atkinson-Jones &
Hewitt, 2018; Gates et al., 2017; Reichow et al.,
278 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

2012). There are major components to consider tions), intervention procedures (e.g., discrete trial
when establishing a social skills group to maxi- teaching [DTT], The Cool versus Not Cool™
mize effectiveness. When determining the overall [CNC] procedure, behavior skills training
objectives of the group, it would be important to [BST]), and the types of environments (e.g.,
consider the age of the participants, the current playgrounds vs. shopping malls) they must per-
language skills of the participants, the current form these skills. In addition, the specific skills
social skill deficits of the participants, and the and curriculum used within each group will
problem behaviors of the participants. depend upon the ages of the participants, their
language skills, social skill deficits, and problem
behaviors.
15.4.1 Developing the Group For one social skills group, the primary objec-
tive may be to develop social skills to function
There are two different approaches to developing through daily living. This might include skills
an effective social skills group. An objective for a needed when grocery shopping like asking for
group may be created then participants with help, waiting in line, and interacting with the
appropriate strengths and deficits are recruited store clerk. Other skills might include those
that fit best within that objective, or potential par- needed for interviewing for a job, like greetings
ticipants will be already available and the objec- and responding to questions. Another could be
tive of the group will be developed based on the those needed for working as a grocery clerk, such
current strengths and deficits of the given partici- as greeting customers, responding to questions,
pants. If a group is being developed given the lat- and referring them to someone if they are not sure
ter approach, it is essential that these participants of the answer. Given the objective of improving
are compatible and complementary as peers. daily living, a combination of DTT, BST, and the
Variables to consider are further described below. CNC procedure might be used to develop these
Putting individuals together who vary in age, skills, then practiced across environments and
skills, problem behaviors, and social skill deficits people so that these skills are used within the
for the sake of a group may not necessarily be appropriate opportunities.
better than nothing. It will be incredibly difficult For another social skills group, the primary
to find a common objective and similar teaching objective may be to develop social skills to
strategies that are effective to teach in a group enhance interactions with peers to create oppor-
setting, which ultimately defeats the purpose of a tunities for friendships and relationships. This
group setting. Therefore, despite the approach in might include skills from initiating and engag-
which the group is developed, it is paramount ing in conversation to identifying ways to prob-
that time and evaluation are devoted to the par- lem solve when reaching a disagreement. Given
ticipants that make up the group to ensure they the objective of developing skills to increase
are compatible. the likelihood of friendships, the CNC proce-
dure or the Teaching Interaction Procedure
15.4.1.1 Objective of the Group (TIP) may be best suited to develop the skills,
The main objectives of a social skills group then practiced across environments where they
should include developing appropriate social are likely to encounter opportunities to engage
behaviors, decreasing inappropriate social behav- in the skill.
iors, and creating an environment that allows for There may be a number of different objectives
performance practice that leads to generalization that may be selected for any given social skills
and maintenance. Depending on the participants group, but it is important to determine those
in the group, variation in overall objectives occur objectives to better determine the skills needed to
in the type of social behaviors to be addressed meet those objectives, as well as the environment
(e.g., basic interaction skills vs. complex interac- in which participants will need to perform them.
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 279

15.4.2 Participants safety of their child when an older individual


approaches and the interaction could end in
Prior to implementation, some strategies to deter- unfortunate ways. Therefore, it would be benefi-
mine if participants are compatible could include cial to keep individuals within the same age
interviews and assessments completed by par- range, to not only teach age-appropriate skills but
ents, teachers, case supervisors, and/or the indi- also better familiarize individuals with others
vidual themself. It is important to understand the within their same-age peer set. Finally, if indi-
deficits of the individuals in the group to ensure viduals are paired in groups of similar-aged
the skills targeted are more likely to be socially peers, they are more likely to be exposed to gen-
valid for the participants in their social eral knowledge appropriate for that age group,
environments. and the curriculum within the social skills group
It is recommended that individuals within a could focus on topics appropriate for that age
group fall within a similar range in the categories group. If individuals within the group vary too
of age, language skills, social deficits, or problem much in terms of age, there is the potential of
behaviors. While not all deficits or strengths must teaching general knowledge information that
be the same, it may be more beneficial to have does not apply or be appropriate for the age group
participants with complementary social skills tar- (e.g., action movies for a 5-year-old, Sesame
gets. For example, when it comes to conversa- Street for a 15-year-old). While an older individ-
tion, it would be ideal if there were participants in ual may enjoy topics such as Sesame Street, it
the group that had a target skill of asking ques- will be important to not solely rely on these as
tions, while another participant in the group had topics of conversation and expose more age-­
a target skill of responding to peers. While not typical topics. This will increase the likelihood
always possible, this is another consideration that they can interact with other individuals their age.
may be beneficial when building a social skills Also, when an individual is perceived as older,
group. the expectations for that individual are commonly
higher. If a 15-year-old is watching Sesame Street
15.4.2.1 Age on a mobile device, there is a high likelihood that
In order to maximize effectiveness, it would be others will interact with the 15-year-old as if they
beneficial to find participants of similar age since are much younger. While the 15-year-old may
the developmental expectations are more likely have more advanced skills, others may not realize
to be similar. In addition, it would promote par- and inhibit the potential for independence.
ticipants engaging with peers of a similar age.
Even if there are older individuals who have the 15.4.2.2 Language Skills
cognitive age of the other individuals, it is not When selecting individuals for a social skills
recommended that they be in the same social group, it is also beneficial to determine the cur-
skills group for several reasons. First, the social rent language skills of the individuals. While they
expectation for both age groups would differ. For must not all be exactly similar, it would be bene-
example, the way a 5-year-old would join in an ficial if they are similar in terms of receptive and
activity would look very different than a 10-year-­ expressive language understanding. This way,
old. Second, if the older individual is accustomed when using teaching procedures, they will be
to playing with younger individuals, they are able to follow the same lesson with limited modi-
more likely to gravitate to younger individuals fications. This would not only make the interven-
outside of the structured teaching sessions or vice tion effective but would allow it to function more
versa. This might limit the number of interactions efficiently as well. Of course, there may be an
the older individual could have with similar-aged occasional need to modify language or teaching
peers, and may put the individual into harm’s procedures, as this is expected when implement-
way if outsiders are not aware of the diagnosis of ing a quality intervention. However, in order to
the individual. Parents may worry about the maintain the most efficient group, it is recom-
280 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

mended to have most, if not all, peers functioning 5  min, the instructor can continue to provide a
at a similar level. Otherwise, it would be critical rate of reinforcement appropriate for the student
to provide enough staffing to pull students out for to be successful without interfering with teaching
more individualized teaching; however, it is rec- the rest of the group.
ommended to immediately incorporate the stu-
dent back into the group so the purpose of
teaching in a group setting is not lost. 15.4.3 Curriculum

15.4.2.3 Social Skill Deficits Since current research provides a spectrum of


While social skill deficits will be individualized curricula to choose from, the topic of the curricu-
for each participant, there will likely be common lum is complex. Rather than arbitrarily selecting
deficits within the group, especially if developing a curriculum to use for a social skills group, it
social skills groups for individuals with ASD. For should be determined based on which skills are
example, one participant may have a skill deficit required to function across a variety of social set-
within carrying a conversation, while another tings, and which do or will apply to the partici-
participant may have difficulty taking turns pants within the objective of the group. It will be
within a conversation. While both have different important to recognize broader topics to address
deficits, there is a common deficit which is skills in the group, such as playing with others or
within a conversation. It is beneficial to find indi- engaging in conversation, but, more importantly,
viduals that have similar, or even complementary, the actual teaching and task analysis that com-
deficits in order to maximize the benefits of a prise these skills are individualized based on the
social skills group. Another example is if one par- deficits of the individuals within that group.
ticipant tends to be overly passive or does not ini- Beyond curriculum, it is important to use pro-
tiate and another participant does not share cedures empirically supported to be effective in
materials. While these are very different skill teaching social skills within group settings. Some
deficits, they both fit under the umbrella skill of procedures include the teaching interaction pro-
appropriate interactive play. cedure (TIP; Leaf et  al., 2010), cool versus not
cool (CNC; Milne et  al., 2017), and behavior
15.4.2.4 Problem Behaviors skills training (BST; Palmen et  al., 2008). In
The rate and topography of problem behaviors of order to determine which procedure or proce-
individuals in the group is another thing to con- dures is most appropriate for the social skills
sider when developing a group. Ideally, if indi- group, it is important to understand the benefits
viduals are being selected to participate in a and difficulties of each procedure, the skills that
social skills group, there should not be a high rate will be taught, the skill set of the staff that will
of behaviors that interfere with learning. implement the procedure, as well as the skill set
Participants should be able to refrain from prob- of the participants in the group.
lem behaviors with contingencies that are realis-
tic for the group setting. If a replacement or
alternative behaviors for problem behaviors are 15.4.4 Staffing
not yet established, it may be beneficial to
develop these in a more individualized setting. To run an effective group, it is ideal to have
For example, if reinforcement is needed every skilled staff that understand the overall objective
10  s for refraining from stereotypy, the partici- of the group, understand the current and l­ ong-­term
pant may not be ready to learn in a group setting goals of individual participants, and can imple-
as the instructor will not be able to provide a high ment the teaching procedures with fidelity. By
frequency of reinforcement while also teaching having a clear understanding of the overall objec-
the rest of the group. However, if the participant tives within the group, staff can better prioritize
can refrain from engaging in stereotypy for up to and maximize teaching opportunities, including
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 281

those that are planned and those that come up who is responsible for delivering reinforcement
organically. Similarly, while maintaining a clear and feedback. If too many staff are providing
objective, staff can better manipulate variables prompts, feedback, and reinforcement at any
within the environment that can help participants given moment, participant attention can become
be successful with certain goals or slightly chal- divided. The lead instructor should provide the
lenge them in a way that will better prepare them primary instructions and feedback to participants
for generalized settings. With a good understand- and direct shadow support staff on when to pro-
ing of the teaching procedures, they can manipu- vide prompts or additional intervention. Shadow
late the components to best meet the needs of the support is present to provide additional support
group, then make the adjustments necessary to for the lead instructor. Shadow support should
teach toward generalization (Stokes & Baer, not reissue an instruction or provide feedback
1977). without the approval of the lead instructor and
Staff should be organized and prepared for should not physically position themself in a way
each session. Staff should be prepared with the that is intrusive (e.g., hovering over or sitting
specific lesson plan and all necessary materials directly next to participants). If the shadow sup-
for whichever activity they will be leading. They port notices that a participant missed an instruc-
must ensure they have established clear goals and tion and reissues that instruction, the participant
objectives for their particular lesson, as well as is likely to learn to not attend to the lead staff.
individual goals embedded for each participant. Rather, the shadow should notify the lead that the
Staff will prepare other staff for their role when participant missed the instruction so that the lead
assistance is needed. If staff are not well prepared instructor may decide to provide feedback or
for their given lesson, it is time wasted for all the reissue the instruction. When the shadow must
participants within that group. gain the attention of the lead, ideally they provide
Staff must also be able to communicate clearly non-vocal social cues (e.g., thumbs up or thumbs
with other staff during each session, as well as down) to communicate to not distract the partici-
prior to and after each session. While communi- pants from the lead. The shadow serves as “extra
cation should be consistent throughout a session, eyes” for the lead and informs the lead of the
this should not be confused with engaging in con- behavior and responses of participants that the
stant conversation. When participants are pres- lead might have missed. For example, if the lead
ent, all focus should be on maximizing learning provides a group instruction and is looking at the
opportunities for participants to learn and prac- left side of the room, the shadow should observe
tice skills. Therefore, during sessions, quick this, and look at the participants on the right side
instructions or social cues are used to maintain of the room. When the lead provides feedback
communication among staff and to minimize dis- following the instruction, the lead may look to
ruption. Thorough communication among staff the shadow to determine if praise or corrective
should occur prior to and after sessions to mini- feedback is needed for the participants on the
mize disruptions to teaching. Debriefing follow- right side of the room. If the shadow is not needed
ing each session allows all staff to fill everyone in for immediate assistance, the shadow may also
on what they may have missed and where each take data and prepare materials for the upcoming
student ended at the end of the session, so they activity. These tasks may also be passed on to a
may revisit antecedents, increase or fade prompts, third staff member if available.
or increase or fade reinforcement for the follow-
ing session.
Effective staff in a group must also know and 15.4.5 Schedule
follow their role. When multiple staff are present,
it is important to establish a clear lead instructor. When balancing staff, multiple participants, and
Establishing a clear lead helps the participants group and individual goals, it could be difficult to
understand who they should be attending to and keep track of everyone’s progress, and smoothly
282 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

transition through activities. By having a detailed will be individualized based on the need of each
schedule, more time can be spent implementing participant, but should be at a rate that can be
the intervention. A typical schedule should list managed by the lead instructor in a group setting.
time blocks, the main activity within that time If the rate of reinforcement or intervention is so
block, the objective of that activity, the subphase frequent that 1:1 support is continuously needed,
(if applicable), the roles of each staff member, it should be reassessed if the group setting is the
and individual participant targets (if applicable). most appropriate learning environment for the
Table  15.1 displays an example of the level of individual. Occasional pull-outs for individual
detail that may be included in a daily schedule. learning may occur, but it should not be some-
These should be updated prior to every session to thing frequently needed.
ensure group and individual targets are up to
date, and staff have time to prepare. It also allows 15.4.6.2 Group Contingency
time for staff to ask any questions if they are Given the context of a social skills group, a group
unclear about any task they must lead and for contingency system can target an appropriate
staff to analyze and discuss if there needs to be social behavior in order to access a reinforcer. A
any adjustments to current teaching procedures group contingency could occur as a dependent
or behavior management techniques. contingency in which the behavior of one or more
individuals results in the reinforcement for the
group, an independent contingency in which only
15.4.6 Behavior Management those that engage in a certain behavior or meet
Strategies certain criteria may access the reinforcer, or an
interdependent contingency in which each indi-
The type of behavior management system used vidual must engage in a behavior or meet the cri-
will depend on the participants and objectives of teria in order for the group to access the reinforcer
the group. There can be systems used for indi- (Deshais et al., 2018; Strain & Schwartz, 2001).
vidual behavior targets such as a token economy Within a social skills group, there may be several
(Gillis & Pence, 2015) or self-evaluation systems group contingencies occurring simultaneously,
(Barry & Haraway, 2005) that targets behavior, each targeting a different behavior and accessing
communication, or social skill goals. Another a different reinforcer. For example, when partici-
system could be used based on a group contin- pants are given “free time” and all begin engag-
gency in which one or all members of the group ing in conversation with another peer, the
engage in a behavior that earns the entire group a instructor may decide to provide a quick dance
preferred activity. Ideally, given the setting of a break for the entire group, using an interdepen-
social skills group, the group contingency would dent contingency. Upon observing one partici-
be based on engaging in one or more targeted pant helping another participant in the group, the
social skills. instructor might reward the group with a token
that goes toward earning a group party, employ-
15.4.6.1 Individual Behavior ing a dependent contingency. An independent
Management contingency can occur when participants are
Several behavior management systems could be instructed to play on the playground, and the
used to address individual behavior goals. These majority of the participants are interacting with
include a level system (e.g., Cihon et al., 2019), one another, those participants who interacted
token economy (e.g., Gillis & Pence, 2015), time with another participant earn a gummy bear.
out system (e.g., Donaldson & Vollmer, 2011), or Using one or a combination of group
self-evaluation (e.g., Barry & Haraway, 2005). ­contingencies may be an efficient way to increase
The system used and the rate of reinforcement appropriate social skills in a group setting.
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 283

Table 15.1  Sample of Social Skills Group Schedule


Activity Objective Staff 1 Staff 2 Staff 3 Notes
2:45– Staff meet Go over goals for the
3:00 day and set up
materials
3:00– Opening Learning how to learn Lead Shadow Data/prep Joint
3:30 circle  Raising hands Increase rate Continue rate materials for attention
 Observational reinforcement of tokens for next activity probes:
learning for Jamie Taylor Set up two  Bubble
 Joint attention Fade feedback joint attention machine
for sitting for probes  Play
Alex −1:1 for Jamie dance
if needed music
3:30– Cool vs. not Joining Shadow Data/prep Lead whole Materials:
4:00 cool in -demonstrations: Lead group B materials for group  Train set
 Not cool: too quiet, Erin: Loud next activity teaching  Legos
joining rudely voice Provide Lead group A  Coloring
 Roleplay Max: Refrain additional Jamie: pages
from rude tone support as Assertiveness
Kate: needed Alex:
Flexibility with Flexibility with
play play
Chris: Refrain
from SSB
4:00– Freeze dance Positive social Data/prep Lead Shadow Materials:
4:15 engagement materials for Chris: Over  Music
next activity excitement  Colored
spots
4:15– Outdoor Sustained play with Lead line Trail line/ Bring stickers
4:45 playground peers Shadow group shadow and data
-walking in a Individual goals: Shadow Shadow group
line until  Jamie: Novel ideas group
playground  Alex: Flexibility
with play
 Chris: Staying near
peers
 Erin: Making
comments
 Max: Responding
to peers with a
friendly tone
 Kate: Flexibility
with play
4:45– Closing Checking in with Lead Clean up Shadow
5:00 circle behavior chart and then greet
stickers parents at
the door for
debrief
5:00– Debrief Review group
5:15 performance,
individual
performance, goals
and lessons for next
session
Clean up
284 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

15.4.7 Data Collection that may be needed to improve behaviors that


allow the individual to learn in a more natural
There are a few methods to evaluate progress environment. However, having more frequent
throughout the social skills group that involve sessions allows for more consistent exposure and
either subjective or objective measures. Social more frequent trials to ideally promote success.
skills are typically dynamic skills that fluctuate As the participant’s behavior continues to prog-
across different environmental variables; there- ress, it is recommended to assess and adjust to
fore, it is beneficial to track progress across dif- longer sessions and possibly a more complex cur-
ferent assessments (e.g., SSRS, SRS) completed riculum as needed. To the authors’ knowledge,
by multiple informants, including the participants there is no clear research that analyzes different
themselves, across a period of time. These assess- intensities of one method of group intervention
ment scores may be helpful in determining social across skill sets. Therefore, not only is there an
skills to target to increase social validity. Since opportunity for future research, but clinically it
some assessments may result in observer bias, it will require continuous analysis of data to ensure
will be beneficial to also obtain data on observ- the sessions are effective for the participants.
able behavior, or obtain data from observers who If the objective of the group is focused on a
are blind to the intervention; however, using moderate or more complex social skills curricu-
observers blind to the intervention is not always lum such as conversation, engagement with
practical in a clinical setting. peers, or winning and losing graciously, it may be
Data may inform the acquisition of social beneficial to have less frequent sessions (e.g., 1–3
knowledge as well as the acquisition of behavioral days a week) with a longer duration (e.g.,
performance. While social knowledge may be one 90–120 min). Longer durations of sessions pro-
step to improving social skills, the emphasis of vide more time to teach skills that typically have
behavior change should primarily focus on behav- more steps or more nuances to the behavior
ioral performance. In addition, it is imperative to which naturally require more time to teach.
measure if the effect of these behavior changes is Longer sessions also allow for longer practice
socially valid to the individuals themselves. opportunities since these types of social skills
need to be sustained for longer periods of time.
Longer sessions permit spending more time on
15.4.8 Intensity the lesson, multiple opportunities to practice in a
variety of settings, and time to teach two different
Determining the appropriate number of sessions skills simultaneously. As individuals get older,
and the duration of each session for the social they are often more involved in extracurricular
skills group could be challenging. The range of activities such as soccer, theater, or science club.
sessions can vary from 1 to 5 days a week With such busy schedules, it also becomes more
(Reichow et  al., 2012) and range from 60 to difficult to attend frequent social skills classes.
120  min each session (Reichow et  al., 2012). Having less frequent sessions increases the likeli-
Given the range, it is beneficial to evaluate the hood of high attendance and allows more time for
overall objective of the group as well as the skill the participants to engage in social activities out-
sets of the participants within the group. If the side of therapeutic sessions where they can ide-
overall objective is focused on a more basic ally practice the target skills. It also allows staff
social skills curriculum such as exposure to peers, time to adjust and prepare the next lesson based
awareness of peers, tolerance of peers, or wait- on how the previous session ended. Because indi-
ing, it may be beneficial to have more frequent viduals learning moderate or complex social
sessions (e.g., 5 days a week) with a shorter dura- skills may learn the skills more quickly, the
tion (e.g., 30–60 min). A shorter duration of ses- ­lessons may need to be adapted day to day to
sions allows for successful exposure without ensure the efficiency of teaching as well.
taking away time for more individual services
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 285

15.5 Conclusion munity where social skills are taught and learned,
the environment itself allows for a more expan-
The efficacy of social skills groups continues to sive repertoire of contingencies an individual will
be researched and the growing body of literature encounter than when taught in a one-to-one set-
has increasingly shown social skills groups can ting. Social skills group interventions allow the
result in the development of social skills for indi- practitioner to capitalize on this and develop and
viduals with autism (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, use social skills interventions that are effective in
2018; Gates et al., 2017; Spain & Blainey, 2015; promoting the development and generalization of
Wolstencroft et al., 2018). However, research on critical social skills that will make socially sig-
the efficacy and effectiveness of social skills nificant changes in the lives of the individuals
groups is still limited, and future research is they work with.
needed. For example, future research should use
stronger experimental designs and more consis-
tent measures (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; References
Wolstencroft et al., 2018). It is also important to
continue to evaluate the effectiveness and effi- Asher, S.  R., Hymel, S., & Renshaw, P.  D. (1984).
Loneliness in children. Child Development, 55, 1456–
cacy of social skills groups when it comes to 1464. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130015
applied skill performance and generality across a Ashman, R., Banks, K., Philip, R.  C., Walley, R., &
variety of populations and settings (Atkinson-­ Stanfield, A. C. (2017). A pilot randomised controlled
Jones & Hewitt, 2018; Gates et al., 2017; Jonsson trial of a group based social skills intervention for
adults with autism spectrum disorder. Research in
et al., 2016; Reichow et al., 2012). Finally, social Autism Spectrum Disorders, 43, 67–75. https://doi.
skills groups should be compared to other inter- org/10.1016/j.rasd.2017.08.001
ventions in order to determine the most effica- Atkinson-Jones, K., & Hewitt, O. (2018). Do group inter-
cious procedure for developing social skills for ventions help people with autism spectrum disorder
to develop better relationships with others? A critical
individuals with autism (Jonsson et al., 2016). review of the literature. British Journal of Learning
The growing literature provides implications Disabilities, 47(2), 77–90. https://doi.org/10.1111/
for practitioners. It would be beneficial for prac- bld.12258
titioners to analyze the variables that have been Barry, L. M., & Haraway, D. L. (2005). Behavioral self-­
control strategies for young children. Journal of Early
effective, or even those variables that are interfer- and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 2(2), 79. https://
ing with skill performance and generality, and to doi.org/10.1037/h0100302
modify and individualize teaching procedures to Beck, A.  T., Steer, R.  A., & Brown, G. (1996). Beck
increase the effectiveness of these procedures for depression inventory–II. Psychological Assessment.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­1-­4419-­1005-­9_441
their clients. Practitioners should select measures Bukowski, W.  M., Hoza, B., & Boivin, M. (1994).
that best depict the client’s performance and Measuring friendship quality during pre-and early
allow for analysis of this progress. Finally, rather adolescence: The development and psychometric
than adhering to a manualized curriculum, it may properties of the Friendship Qualities Scale. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 11(3), 471–484.
be beneficial for practitioners to develop a social https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407594113011
skills curriculum that prioritizes skills that are Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive assessment
most socially significant for the individuals. of spoken language. American Guidance Services.
Social skills groups are an evidence-based https://www.wpspublish.com/casl-­2-­comprehensive-­
assessment-­of-­spoken-­language-­second-­edition
method for developing social skills for individu- Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R.,
als with autism (Atkinson-Jones & Hewitt, 2018; McEachin, J., & Taubman, M. (2019). Use of a level
Reichow et al., 2012; Reichow & Volkmar, 2010; system with flexible shaping to improve synchronous
White et al., 2007; Wolstencroft et al., 2018). If engagement. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1),
44–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0254-­8
constructed correctly, social skills groups can Constantino, J.  N., & Gruber, C.  P. (2012). Social
offer an opportunity to efficiently teach individu- responsiveness scale: SRS-2. Western Psychological
als with autism a variety of social skills while Services. https://www.wpspublish.com/
with their peers. By expanding the verbal com- srs-­2-­social-­responsiveness-­scale-­second-­edition
286 C. M. Milne and A. Creem

Deshais, M.  A., Fisher, A.  B., & Kahng, S. (2018). A Leaf, J. B., Dotson, W. H., Oppenheim, M. L., Sheldon,
preliminary investigation of a randomized dependent J.  B., & Sherman, J.  A. (2010). The effectiveness
group contingency for hallway transitions. Education of a group teaching interaction procedure for teach-
and Treatment of Children, 41(1), 49–63. https://doi. ing social skills to young children with a pervasive
org/10.1353/etc.2018.0002 developmental disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum
Donaldson, J. M., & Vollmer, T. R. (2011). An evaluation Disorders, 4(2), 186–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
and comparison of time-out procedures with and with- rasd.2009.09.003
out release contingencies. Journal of Applied Behavior Leaf, J. A., Leaf, J. B., Milne, C., Townley-Cochran, D.,
Analysis, 44(4), 693–705. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L., Cihon, J. H., Taubman, M.,
jaba.2011.44-­693 McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2016). The effects of the
Ellingsen, R., Bolton, C., Laugeson, E. (2017). Evidence-­ cool versus not cool procedure to teach social game
based social skills groups for individuals with autism play to individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum
spectrum disorder across the lifespan. In Leaf, J. B., disorder. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9(1), 34–49.
(Ed.). (2017). Handbook of social skills and autism https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­016-­0112-­5
spectrum disorder: Assessment, curricula, and inter- Leaf, J.  B., Oppenheim-Leaf, M.  L., Townley-Cochran,
vention. Springer International Publishing. https:// D., Leaf, J.  A., Alcalay, A., Milne, C., Kassardjian,
www.springer.com/gp/book/9783319629940#aboutB A., Tsuji, K., Dale, S., Leaf, R., Taubman, M., &
ook McEachin, J. (2016). Changing preference from tan-
Gates, J. A., Kang, E., & Lerner, M. D. (2017). Efficacy of gible to social activities through an observation pro-
group social skills interventions for youth with autism cedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(1),
spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-­ 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.276
analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 52, 164–181. Liu, K.  P., Wong, D., Chung, A.  C., Kwok, N., Lam,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.01.006 M.  K., Yuen, C.  M., Arblaster, K., & Kwan, A.  C.
Gillis, J. M., & Pence, S. T. (2015). Token economy for (2013). Effectiveness of a workplace training pro-
individuals with autism spectrum disorder. In Autism gramme in improving social, communication and
service delivery (pp.  257–277). Springer. https://doi. emotional skills for adults with autism and intellectual
org/10.1007/978-­1-­4939-­2656-­5_9 disability in Hong Kong–a pilot study. Occupational
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rating Therapy International, 20(4), 198–204. https://doi.
system (SSRS). American Guidance Service. https:// org/10.1002/oti.1356
pearsonclinical.in/solutions/social-­s kills-­r ating-­ Milne, C., Leaf, J.  A., Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Torres,
system-­ssrs/ N., Townley-Cochran, D., Taubman, M., Leaf, R.,
Hillier, A., Fish, T., Cloppert, P., & Beversdorf, D.  Q. McEachin, J., & Oppeheim-Leaf, M. (2017). Teaching
(2007). Outcomes of a social and vocational skills joint attention and peer to peer communication using
support group for adolescents and young adults on the cool versus not cool procedure in a large group
the autism spectrum. Focus on Autism and Other setting. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Developmental Disabilities, 22, 107–115. https://doi. Disabilities, 29(5), 777–796. https://doi.org/10.1007/
org/10.1177/10883576070220020201 s10882-­017-­9556-­y
Howlin, P., & Yates, P. (1999). The potential effec- Nowicki, S. (1997). Instructional manual for the receptive
tiveness of social skills groups for adults tests of the diagnostic analysis of nonverbal accuracy
with autism. Autism, 3, 299–307. https://doi. 2. Peachtree.
org/10.1177/1362361399003003007 Palmen, A., Didden, R., & Arts, M. (2008). Improving
Jonsson, U., Choque Olsson, N., & Bölte, S. (2016). question asking in high-functioning adolescents with
Can findings from randomized controlled trials of autism spectrum disorders: Effectiveness of small-­
social skills training in autism spectrum disorder group training. Autism, 12(1), 83–98. https://doi.
be generalized? The neglected dimension of exter- org/10.1177/1362361307085265
nal validity. Autism, 20(3), 295–305. https://doi. Piers, E.  V. (1984). Piers-Harris children's self-concept
org/10.1177/1362361315583817 scale. Western Psychological Services.
Laugeson, E.  A., & Frankel, F. (2010). Social skills for Reichow, B., Steiner, A. M., & Volkmar, F. (2012). Social
teenagers with developmental and autism spec- skills groups for people aged 6 to 21 with autism spec-
trum disorders: The PEERS treatment manual. trum disorders (ASD). Campbell Systematic Reviews,
Routledge. https://www.routledge.com/Social-­ 8(1), 1–76. https://doi.org/10.4073/csr.2012.16
Skills-­f or-­Teenagers-­w ith-­D evelopmental-­a nd-­ Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F.  R. (2010). Social skills
Autism-­S pectrum-­D isorders/Laugeson-­F rankel/p/ interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation
book/9780415872034 for evidence-based practices within a best evi-
Leaf, J. B. (2017). An introduction to the handbook (Ed.). dence synthesis framework. Journal of Autism and
(2017). Handbook of social skills and autism spec- Developmental Disorders, 40(2), 149–166. https://doi.
trum disorder: Assessment, curricula, and interven- org/10.1007/s10803-­009-­0842-­0
tion. Springer International Publishing. https://www. Rydell, A.  M., Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (1997).
springer.com/gp/book/9783319629940#aboutBook Measurement of two social competence aspects in
15  Developing Social Skills Groups for Behavioral Intervention for Individuals with Autism 287

middle childhood. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), behavior analysis for enriching the social lives of per-
824. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-­1649.33.5.824 sons with autism spectrum disorder. DRL books: New
Skinner, B.  F. (1953). Science and human behavior. York, NY.
Macmillan. Turner-Brown, L. M., Perry, T. D., Dichter, G. S., Bodfish,
Spain, D., & Blainey, S.  H. (2015). Group social skills J. W., & Penn, D. L. (2008). Brief report: Feasibility
interventions for adults with high-functioning autism of social cognition and interaction training for adults
spectrum disorders: A systematic review. Autism, 19(7), with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and
874–886. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315587659 Developmental Disorders, 38(9), 1777–1784. https://
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol- doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­008-­0545-­y
ogy of generalization 1. Journal of Applied Behavior White, S. W., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social skills
Analysis, 10(2), 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1901/ development in children with autism spectrum disor-
jaba.1977.10-­349 ders: A review of the intervention research. Journal of
Stokes, T. F., & Osnes, P. G. (1989). An operant pursuit Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(10), 1858–
of generalization. Behavior Therapy, 20, 337–355. 1868. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0320-­x
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-­7894(89)80054-­1 Wolstencroft, J., Robinson, L., Srinivasan, R., Kerry,
Strain, P. S., & Schwartz, I. (2001). ABA and the devel- E., Mandy, W., & Skuse, D. (2018). A systematic
opment of meaningful social relations for young review of group social skills interventions, and meta-­
children with autism. Focus on Autism and Other analysis of outcomes, for children with high func-
Developmental Disabilities, 16(2), 120–128. https:// tioning ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental
doi.org/10.1177/108835760101600208 Disorders, 48(7), 2293–2307. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Taubman, M. T., Leaf, R. B., McEachin, J., & Driscoll, M. s10803-­018-­3485-­1
(2011). Crafting connections: Contemporary applied
Parent Implementation
Interventions 16
Sarely Licona, Lauren Bush, Victoria Chavez,
Emily Dillon, and Allison L. Wainer

16.1 Introduction Clearinghouse on Autism Evidence and Practice


(NCAEP) Review classifying sets of practices
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a life-long that have clear evidence of positive outcomes for
developmental disorder characterized by core children, youth, and young adults with ASD
deficits in communication, social abilities, and (Steinbrenner et al., 2020).
the presence of restricted and repetitive behaviors One of the areas of research that has seen dra-
that can be observed within the first 3 years of life matic growth over the last several decades is that
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013). of parent/caregiver training. Importantly, parent
Individuals with ASD often require intensive and involvement in intervention has a long history in
comprehensive intervention in core symptom the ASD-intervention field, with Lovaas and col-
domains, as well as in additional areas of func- leagues’ (Lovaas, 1987; Lovaas et al., 1973) sem-
tioning (e.g., adaptive skills, behavior manage- inal findings supporting a better response to
ment), starting as early as toddlerhood when a intervention for children whose parents were
reliable diagnosis of ASD can be made (Kim trained in intervention strategies relative to chil-
et al., 2013). In fact, there is increasing research dren whose parents did not receive this training.
to suggest that ASD-specific early intervention These findings were soon replicated and expanded
can have a significant impact on brain develop- upon with further formative studies, including
ment and later adult outcomes (Dawson, 2008; those showing that parents could learn to use
Dawson et al., 2012). As a result, the last several basic behavioral strategies with high levels of
decades have seen the development and dissemi- fidelity (Anderson et  al., 1987; Baker, 1984;
nation of intervention programs aimed at decreas- Harris, 1984). These studies from the 1970s and
ing core symptoms and improving daily 1980s underscored the need for collaboration
functioning in individuals with ASD. In addition, between home and the educational environment
recent years have seen scoping efforts to quantify and active partnership with parents for successful
and interpret the mounting scientific data on programming, and set the foundation for what
these intervention approaches and outcomes, cul- has become known as ASD parent training or
minating in the most recent National parent-mediated intervention research (Lovaas
et al., 1973; Schopler & Reichler, 1971). Building
S. Licona · L. Bush · V. Chavez · E. Dillon · from this strong foundation, efforts over the last
A. L. Wainer (*) three decades have interrogated complimentary
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, questions such as (1) how do parent learning and
Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, USA use of different intervention techniques impact
e-mail: Allison_Wainer@rush.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 289
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_16
290 S. Licona et al.

child and family-level outcomes? (2) what are 2007). It requires fewer resources and is less
effective strategies for supporting parent learning expensive to deliver compared to other types of
and use of intervention techniques? and (3) which early intervention (Matson et al., 2009).
intervention techniques and programs are best Research indicates that parents can be taught
suited for parent learning and use? While the to successfully use ASD-specific intervention
field still seeks answers to these nuanced ques- strategies. Parent participation in ASD-specific
tions, data accumulated over the last 50  years intervention programs has been associated with
offer a strong rationale for continued investment improvements in child language skills (Charlop
in the development, study, and dissemination of & Trasowech, 1991; Rogers et al., 2006), imita-
parent and caregiver training to optimize inter- tion (Ingersoll & Gergans, 2007), joint attention
ventions and maximize positive outcomes for and joint engagement (Drew et  al., 2002), and
children with ASD and their families (Bearss, play skills (Stahmer, 1995), as well as a decrease
Johnson, et  al., 2013; Bearss, Lecavalier, et  al., in problem behavior (Moes & Frea, 2002). In
2013; Gerow et al., 2018; Mahoney et al., 1999; addition, children whose parents receive training
Oono et al., 2013). show increased generalization and maintenance
There are several theoretical reasons for the of targeted skills across settings (Remington
long-standing support for parent involvement in et al., 2007).
ASD intervention. First, child learning occurs The benefits of parent training extend beyond
largely through daily routines and, as parents are the target child with ASD. For example, Koegel
often at the center of creating and enacting these et al. (1996) found that parent training resulted in
routines, they have the opportunity to maximize decreased parent stress and overall increases in
developmental learning in everyday activities. As positive family communication (Koegel et  al.,
parents are also often the most consistent pres- 1996). Similarly, parent training is associated
ence in a child’s life, they have numerous oppor- with improvements in family functioning, spe-
tunities throughout the day to implement the cifically in marital, parent–child, and sibling rela-
intervention techniques. In addition, increased tionships (Dunlap & Fox, 1999). Further, parent
parental knowledge and skills for engaging their training provides psychoeducational opportuni-
child with complex neurodevelopmental difficul- ties for parents to increase their ASD-related
ties allows for continued opportunities for the knowledge, which can result in improved confi-
child to learn in a range of different situations and dence when raising a child with ASD (Karst &
environments (Mahoney & Wiggers, 2007). van Hecke, 2012). Additional positive parental
Importantly, parent involvement in interven- outcomes include a decrease in parent mental
tion tends to be acceptable and desirable to fami- health concerns and enhanced parent understand-
lies. For example, almost three-quarters of ing of their child’s developmental strengths and
parents reported parent training to be the most weaknesses (Matson et al., 2009).
effective contributor to their child’s growth rela- Taken together, there is a compelling theoreti-
tive to other types of interventions, such as occu- cal and empirical rationale for continued invest-
pational, speech, and physical therapy (Hume ment in parent implementation interventions for
et al., 2005). Parent training is favored for posi- ASD. Indeed, parent involvement in ASD inter-
tively influencing a child’s development and vention was identified by the most recent NCAEP
reducing the risk of severe ASD-related symp- Review as one of the 28 evidence-based practices
toms (Maglione et al., 2012). It also rates highly for children, youth, and young adults with ASD
among parents for carrying out its intended pur- (Steinbrenner et al., 2020). From 1990 to 2017,
pose and providing relevant and useful informa- there were 55 empirical demonstrations of the
tion (Thomson & Carlson, 2017). Additionally, efficacy of parent implementation interventions
parent training expands the availability of inter- in high-quality, peer-reviewed journals, con-
vention services to children with autism by mak- ducted by several independent research groups.
ing it more accessible for families (Bryson et al.,
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 291

While the rapidly growing literature base in discuss various approaches to parent implemen-
this area has resulted in an expanded and more tation interventions in ASD.
sophisticated understanding of parent involve- The goal of the current chapter is to provide an
ment in intervention, it has also created a land- overview of parent-mediated and parent training
scape where multiple terms and definitions are intervention programs designed for use with chil-
used across different studies and stages of dren with ASD.  For each type of parent imple-
research. This, in turn, can complicate the inter- mentation intervention, we first provide the
pretation of research findings and slow the theoretical rationale for treatment targets and
­process of translation from research to clinical offer a brief historical context. With an eye
practice. Fortunately, Bearss et al. (2015) offer a toward supporting the deployment of parent
taxonomy of parent training to help define differ- implementation within clinical practice, we
ent models of parent involvement in intervention selected several exemplar manualized programs
and better interpret findings from this diverse lit- within each parent implementation approach and
erature (Bearss et al., 2015). Within this frame- offer a description and brief overview of the evi-
work, there are two broad categories of parent dence base for these specific interventions. We
involvement in intervention: (1) parent support will then discuss the nascent, but growing,
and (2) parent implementation. Parent support research base for improving access to parent
includes psychoeducation to increase parental implementation interventions via telehealth and
understanding of the ASD diagnosis and associ- related technology. Finally, we end with a discus-
ated needs. Parent implementation involves the sion of the clinical implications of this literature
parent as an active participant in the treatment, and recommendations for continuing to advance
such that parents learn to use intervention strate- research in this area. It is important to reiterate
gies directly with their child. that parents as active participants in ASD inter-
Parent implementation interventions can fur- vention have served as a longstanding keystone
ther be divided into those that target skill building for the field. While a comprehensive review of all
with the child and those that target behavior parent implementation interventions is beyond
reduction with the child. Parent implementation the scope of this chapter, it seeks to offer impor-
interventions that focus on skill-building are tant context by describing the evolution and cur-
referred to as parent-mediated interventions for rent state of the field with respect to actively
core symptoms. Such parent-mediated interven- engaging parents as a way to optimize and maxi-
tions target core deficits in ASD with the goal of mize ASD intervention outcomes.
increasing pivotal behaviors such as social com-
munication, imitation, and play, as these early
skills are fundamental to long-term social com- 16.2 Parent-Mediated
munication development (Greenslade et  al., Interventions: Improving
2019). Parent implementation interventions Social Communication
focused on behavior reduction are referred to as in ASD
parent training interventions for maladaptive
behaviors. Such approaches aim to minimize As noted above, there is a long history of parent
undesired or challenging behaviors such as involvement in intervention for children with
aggression, non-compliance, and task avoidance, ASD, particularly those emphasizing supporting
as well as problematic behaviors related to feed- the development of core deficits as the treatment
ing, sleeping, and toileting, as these behaviors targets, such as social communication. Indeed,
can be disruptive to learning for children with difficulties with social communication are
ASD.  The above terms and definitions put for- observed across the entirety of the autism spec-
ward by Bearss et al. (2015) will be used through- trum, regardless of intelligence and co-occurring
out the remainder of this chapter to organize and disorders, making them a critical target for inter-
vention. Social communication skills are com-
292 S. Licona et al.

posed of verbal (e.g., language, tone) and intervention approaches that combine best prac-
nonverbal (e.g., eye contact, gestures) abilities, tices from developmental science and the science
with the goal of enabling clear and effective com- of applied behavior analysis (ABA). Such
munication with others (Swineford et al., 2014). approaches emphasize strategies from develop-
The process of successful communication, while mental sciences to promote engagement, social
innate in typical development, is very complex, motivation, and synchrony between the parent
involving a unification of multiple neural net- and child, and utilize operant learning strategies
works involved in language production, social from ABA to teach specific new skills.
understanding, language comprehension, and Individualized and developmentally appropriate
others (Catani & Bambini, 2014; Landa et  al., treatment goals are guided by developmental
1992). Early social communication skills, such as sequences, with a strong emphasis on embedding
the use of a point, or following another person’s teaching within natural routine and play interac-
eye contact to find something of interest, predict tions to enhance generalizability and mainte-
social and language development in later child- nance of skills. NDBI treatment targets often
hood, suggesting that early interventions in these include early pivotal social communication skills
core social communication skills can set the such as joint attention, pointing, imitation, and
foundation for social communication develop- social routines. Common elements among NDBIs
ment later in life (Greenslade et al., 2019). are described in detail by Schriebman and col-
Indeed, given the well-documented and life- leagues (2015) and include a three-part contin-
long nature of social communication impair- gency (antecedent–response–consequence)
ments in ASD, research aimed at identifying fundamental to all ABA therapies, manualized
evidence-based strategies to improve social com- practice, the fidelity of implementation criteria,
munication early in development has been long- individualized treatment goals, ongoing mea-
standing, with parent-mediated interventions surement of progress, child-initiated teaching
representing a promising approach. Published episodes (e.g., using the child’s focus or interests
reports of parents learning and implementing to teach concepts), environmental arrangement
strategies to support their child’s social commu- (e.g., placing items in sight but out of reach to
nication functioning date back to the 1970s prompt requesting and child initiation), natural
(Schopler & Reichler, 1971), yet structured and reinforcement, use of prompting and prompt fad-
widely available curricula for evidence-based, ing, balanced turns (which allows for some
parent-mediated ASD interventions historically access control to materials, maintains engage-
had been lacking. As a result, recent efforts have ment, and teaches social interactions), modeling
seen an active shift toward standardized and man- (demonstrating the desired behavior), adult imi-
ualized parent-mediation interventions (Matson tation, and broadening the attentional focus of the
et al., 2009), in part driven by the recognition that child. Naturalistic Interventions, including
parents are the agent of change in parent-­ NDBIs, are considered evidence-based practice
mediated approaches. Given the central role of categories by the NCAEP, with 75 high-quality
parent learning in parent-mediated intervention, efficacy and effectiveness studies showing posi-
the majority of research has been on programs tive outcomes for children with ASD between
that involve parents working closely with a thera- 1990 and 2017 (Steinbrenner et al., 2020). While
pist (or “coach”) to learn and use the intervention there are many similarities across parent-­
strategies in their everyday lives. mediated NDBIs, the methods of intervention
In this regard, the last two decades have seen development, implementation, and evaluation
the emergence of a growing evidence base for have varied across these studies. To highlight
parent-mediated interventions falling under the work being done with different parent-mediated
category of naturalistic developmental behav- NDBIs, we describe and review the evidence
ioral interventions (NDBIs; Schreibman et  al., base of three of these programs below.
2015). NDBI is an umbrella term used to describe
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 293

16.2.1 Pivotal Response Treatment ing sessions for parents; Coolican et  al., 2010).
Some studies of parent-mediated PRT used a
Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT; Koegel et al., general curriculum based on a standard set of
1989) is one NDBI approach that was developed PRT-specific technical materials (e.g., handouts
to address deficits in core “pivotal” skills for chil- and video examples; Hardan et al., 2015), while
dren with ASD and has been adapted for a parent-­ other studies took a more individualized approach
mediated approach. PRT is recognized as a based on the specific needs of the parent and the
comprehensive teaching model for children with child (Bradshaw et  al., 2017). However, across
autism (Lord & McGee, 2001). The goals of PRT the majority of studies, active parent coaching
are to build social and educational skills that will (e.g., parents receiving feedback on their use of
allow children to engage with others and thereby PRT strategies with their child) was a central
increase opportunities for learning (Koegel et al., component of the intervention. Parent-mediated
1999). Indeed, the authors define the “pivotal” PRT has also been delivered in a variety of group
areas that their interventions address as domains, formats: Minjarez et  al. (2011) delivered a
“that, when changed, generally produce large 10-week, group-based program consisting of
collateral improvements in other areas” (Koegel 90 min sessions, while Hardan et al. (2015) deliv-
et  al., 1999, pg. 174), such as responsivity to ered eight 90-min group sessions and four 60-min
cues, motivation to initiate (with others), environ- individual parent-child dyad sessions over the
mental responsivity, and self-regulation. In this course of 12 weeks.
way, the intervention targets a few core areas of There is empirical support for the efficacy of
development, with the understanding that these parent-mediated PRT, with some data even sug-
core areas have cascading influences to improve gesting longer-term social communication
other domains, or prevent future delays, not improvements (Koegel et al., 1996, 2003, 2010).
directly targeted in the intervention. Additionally, For example, a 12-week randomized control trial
the PRT model addresses an entire domain at a demonstrated that children whose parents partici-
time (e.g., motivation to initiate), rather than pated in group-based PRT demonstrated greater
focusing on singular behaviors. PRT has emerged improvements in language and adaptive commu-
as an evidence-based practice in its own right, nication relative to children whose parents were
under the larger category of Naturalistic in a psychoeducation group. In addition, 3-month
Interventions, and is considered by the NCAEP follow-up data from families who completed the
to be a Manualized Intervention Meeting Criteria parent-mediated PRT program indicated mainte-
(MIMC) for Evidence-Based Practice nance of these language and adaptive communi-
(Steinbrenner et al., 2020). This means that PRT cation gains, as well as additional gains in early
is manualized, has a unique “intervention iden- cognition after treatment (Hardan et  al., 2015).
tity,” and shares common features with other Importantly, the benefits of parent-mediated PRT
approaches in the Naturalistic Intervention cate- appear to extend beyond child outcomes.
gory, yet also has sufficient data from high-­ Research suggests that parents trained to use PRT
quality studies and replications to be considered demonstrate significant increases in family
its own evidence-based practice. empowerment and decreases in parenting
Parent-mediated PRT has been successfully stress—most notably reductions in stress related
delivered in both individual and group formats to parent–child interactions—as a result of a
(e.g., Bradshaw et  al., 2017; Coolican et  al., group-based parent-mediated PRT program
2010; Hardan et al., 2015; Randolph et al., 2011). (Minjarez et  al., 2011). There has also been an
Most research on individual parent-mediated emphasis on determining the best way to inte-
PRT involved weekly (e.g., 45 min to 1 h) parent grate parent-mediated PRT into more compre-
coaching sessions over the course of 12–24 weeks. hensive programs and community settings. For
A handful of studies have explored “brief” mod- example, a 24-week randomized control trial
els of parent-mediated PRT (e.g., three 2-h train- examined a combination of parent-mediated and
294 S. Licona et al.

clinician-delivered PRT with data suggesting that gies into their daily interactions (Dawson et al.,
children in the PRT group showed greater 2010). More recent adaptations of the interven-
improvements in social communication skills tion have seen parents as the main intervention
and language, as well as improvements on a clini- providers, and research has explored a number of
cal global impressions rating scale (Gengoux different formats and structures to support parent
et al., 2019). Preliminary data also indicate that learning and the use of ESDM (Ryberg, 2015;
less intensive formats of parent-mediated PRT, Zhou et  al., 2018). Indeed, research on parent-­
with greater potential for dissemination to mediated ESDM has been at the forefront of
­community settings, may be effective for teach- determining the optimal delivery of the parent-­
ing parents to use PRT and enhancing child lan- mediated intervention in the United States and
guage and communication skills (Coolican et al., globally (Zhou et al., 2018; Rogers et al., 2019).
2010). Together, the literature supports a parent-­ For example, investigators in China trialed a
mediated model of PRT for teaching parents to 6-month, high-intensity, parent-mediated ESDM
use intervention strategies that bolster language model, consisting of initial self-directed learning
and social communication development in young via a parent manual, followed by an 8-hr group
children with ASD. parent-training course, and then a 90-min indi-
vidual parent coaching session each week over a
period of 26  weeks (Zhou et  al., 2018).
16.2.2 The Early Start Denver Model Researchers in the United States have typically
studied parent-mediated ESDM delivered in
The Early Start Denver Model (ESDM) is an 90-min, clinic-based individual parent coaching
intervention model developed for toddlers with sessions across 12  weeks (Rogers et  al., 2019).
ASD and designed to be implemented in the Efforts have also been made to identify an
home setting (Rogers & Dawson, 2010). “enhanced” parent-mediated ESDM model that
Specifically, ESDM utilizes the routines built builds on the traditional format by adding moti-
into a child’s day for opportunities for social vational interviewing, multimodal learning tools
learning and engagement. The ESDM model (e.g., web-based learning), and an additional
considers development as a whole, and therefore 90-min home-based individual parent coaching
has a broad focus to improve functioning across session each week for 12  weeks (Rogers et  al.,
all child domains: motor, cognitive, language, 2019).
play, and self-care skills. It aims to reduce autism The majority of research on ESDM has
symptoms and address delays in development focused on therapist-implemented ESDM or a
(e.g., social communication). However, consider- combination of parent-mediated and therapist-­
ing the developmental level of the intervention implemented ESDM. The growing body of high-­
and the particular needs of children with ASD, quality research indicates that such approaches
there is a greater focus on improving child out- increase a child’s spontaneous language use, imi-
comes in imitation, nonverbal communication tation skills, social initiations, and scores on stan-
(joint attention), verbal communication, social dardized developmental measures (Dawson et al.,
development, and pretend play (Rogers & Talbott, 2010, 2012; Estes et  al., 2015; Rogers et  al.,
2016). Although ESDM itself was not yet consid- 2019). There is also promising data indicating
ered a MIMC in the NCAEP review as a unique normalization of brain activity in response to this
evidence-based practice, it falls under the cate- model of ESDM (Dawson et  al., 2012), with
gory of Naturalistic Interventions, and studies additional longitudinal findings supporting
examining ESDM are pivotal to the Naturalistic improved child outcomes up to the age of 6 years
Intervention evidence base. and after 2 years from the end of the intervention
Initially, ESDM was delivered by providers (Dawson et al., 2010).
trained in the model, with parents serving a sup- Beyond this, data suggests that parents can
porting role by incorporating the model’s strate- effectively learn the ESDM strategies and that
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 295

children show improvements in social communi- risk of developing ASD (i.e., younger siblings of
cation and cognition as families engage in these children with ASD). Specifically, the intervention
programs (e.g., Rogers et  al., 2012, 2019; targets child social engagement, language, imita-
Waddington et  al., 2019; Zhou et  al., 2018). tion, and play by encouraging parents to use
Initial single-subject design studies showed that strategies during daily routines and play (Stadnick
parents increased the number of ESDM strategies et al., 2015).
used as they progressed through the parent coach- Project ImPACT was developed through an
ing curriculum and that their use of ESDM iterative process using the insights of parents,
­strategies was associated with improvements in teachers, and service providers for use within the
child functioning, including child engagement community setting (Ingersoll & Dvortcsak,
and expressive language use (Waddington et al., 2010). This close collaboration with stakeholders
2019). While the first randomized control trial of led to the development of both an individual and
parent-mediated ESDM compared to community-­ group-based Project ImPACT model, which
treatment as usual failed to find differences on included materials and supports for parents (e.g.,
child outcomes after 12  weeks of intervention, PowerPoint slides) and providers (e.g., tutorial
children in the community group received signifi- videos to assist providers as they train and coach
cantly more hours of intervention, suggesting parents). The individual and group models are
that lower intensity (e.g., fewer hours) parent-­ both delivered over a 12-week period. The indi-
mediated ESDM may be an efficient alternative vidual Project ImPACT format includes
to higher intensity community-based treatment 45–60  min parent coaching sessions once or
approaches (Rogers et  al., 2012). A follow-up twice per week across the 12 weeks. The Project
comparative efficacy trial indicated that relative ImPACT group format alternates between 1 week
to the traditional parent-mediated ESDM format, of group (2  h) and 1  week of individual parent
an “enhanced” version (as described above) pro- coaching sessions (1  h). Additional academic-­
duced greater improvement in parent skills, community partnerships have adapted the tradi-
although improvements in child social communi- tional Project ImPACT curriculum for use within
cation, cognition, and adaptive outcomes were specific community settings. For example,
comparable across formats (Rogers et al., 2019). Project ImPACT for Toddlers is an adaptation
Another randomized control trial of Chinese tod- that retained the traditional 12-week structure of
dlers found that 6  months of high-intensity, Project ImPACT but made enhancements to the
parent-­mediated ESDM was associated with intervention and training materials to better align
greater improvement in language, social commu- with the Part C Early Intervention systems’ val-
nication, and play relative to a community com- ues and structures (Stahmer et al., 2019).
parison condition (Zhou et  al., 2018). Taken Project ImPACT is one of only two interven-
together, these findings offer strong support for a tions within the NCAEP Parent-Implemented
parent-mediated ESDM intervention for improv- Intervention category to be considered its own
ing cognitive, language, and adaptive skills in MIMC as an evidence-based practice
young children with ASD. (Steinbrenner et  al., 2020). There have been a
handful of lab-based efficacy studies examining
outcomes from Project ImPACT, with results
16.2.3 Project ImPACT indicating parents can learn the ImPACT strate-
gies successfully and that their children demon-
Like the previously discussed interventions, strate improvements in social communication in
Project ImPACT (Improving Parents as response to the intervention (Ingersoll & Wainer,
Communication Teachers) is a parent-mediated 2013; Yoder et al., 2020). For example, a recent
intervention targeting social communication out- randomized control trial (RCT) comparing indi-
comes for young children with ASD or a high vidual Project ImPACT to treatment as usual in
296 S. Licona et al.

high-risk younger siblings of children with ASD 16.2.4 Summary


found that parent use of Project ImPACT strate-
gies improved children’s imitation and social Research on these three NDBI parent-mediated
communication skills, which in turn, improved intervention models demonstrates the breadth and
overall expressive language abilities (Yoder depth of study in this area. For example, research
et al., 2020). Although the only lab-based RCT on parent-mediated PRT has focused heavily on
of Project ImPACT, Yoder and colleagues’ understanding outcomes at both the parent and
research meets a high threshold of methodologi- child level; a critical area of further work as par-
cal rigor with a relatively large sample size, ents are truly integral to such intervention
multi-method assessment approach, and approaches. Research on parent-­mediated ESDM
research staff and outcome evaluators blind to has focused on high-quality and well-­controlled
participant status. randomized trials, as well as identifying the opti-
Although the research base for Project mal structure, format, and dose of such interven-
ImPACT includes relatively less efficacy data, tion approaches. Project ImPACT, on the other
most of its evidence base comes from effective- hand, has been evaluated in a variety of commu-
ness trials which is critical that this program was nity settings with an eye toward understanding
specifically designed for delivery within the com- adaptation, implementation, and sustainability of
munity. Ingersoll and Wainer (2013) worked with parent-mediated NDBIs in real-world practice
13 teachers representing three intermediated settings. It is critical that researchers continue to
school districts to implement the group-based approach the study of parent-mediated NDBIs in
Project ImPACT curriculum; results from this such complimentary fashions, as together this
pilot study indicated children showed improve- work provides different, but equally important,
ments in parent and teacher reports of child social types of evidence to support the rationale for
communication skills and parents reported engaging parents to use NDBI strategies with
decreased stress after participation (Ingersoll & children with or at risk for ASD. Important future
Wainer, 2013). Another community trial of the directions for this work include longitudinal stud-
group format found that children whose parents ies to examine longer-­term intervention outcomes,
learned and implemented Project ImPACT strate- a better understanding of family and child-level
gies from three community providers showed variables that may influence treatment engage-
greater improvements in child social communi- ment and outcomes, and continued exploration of
cation skills relative to those children in treat- strategies to best support parent learning and use
ment as a usual control condition (Stadnick et al., of NDBI approaches.
2015). Recently, Project ImPACT for toddlers
was delivered within the Part C Early Intervention
system with preliminary data suggesting that 16.3 Parent Training
children who received Project ImPACT demon- Interventions: Reducing
strated greater improvements in positive parent– Disruptive Behaviors in ASD
child interactions relative to treatment as usual
families (Stahmer et al., 2019). While a relatively Disruptive behaviors occur in approximately
newer intervention model, Project ImPACT has 50–70% of children with ASD and significantly
been shown to be effective for improving child interfere with aspects of daily functioning
expressive language, imitation skills, and social (Bearss, Lecavalier, et  al., 2013; Gadow et  al.,
communication outcomes, including when 2004; Lecavalier, 2006), peer socialization
implemented within community settings where (Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, & Frea, 1992), and
such programs will be most accessible to young learning (Koegel, Koegel, & Surratt, 1992), mak-
children with ASD. ing the treatment of co-occurring disruptive
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 297

behaviors of high clinical significance for the ing behaviors with children and adolescents with
family and the child. These behaviors often con- disruptive behaviors without ASD to inform the
sist of concerns such as irritability, anger out- development of ASD-­specific programs and pro-
bursts, tantrums, oppositionality, noncompliance, tocols (Brestan & Eyberg, 1998; Briegel, 2016;
property destruction, self-injury, and aggression Costin & Chambers, 2007; Dretzke et  al., 2009;
(Burke et  al., 2002; Hartley et  al., 2008). Postorino et al., 2017; Urquiza & Timmer, 2012),
Importantly, disruptive behaviors may operate making it an important and efficacious treatment
through motivating functions by which a child for disruptive behavior disorders in children with
can escape a challenging situation (e.g., learning, ASD (Kaat & Lecavalier, 2013).
sensory overload) or communicate a want or Thus, in the more recent past, a series of par-
need (Kaat & Lecavalier, 2013; Koegel, Koegel, ent training programs based on ABA principles
Hurley, & Frea, 1992; Yang et al., 2017). Despite have been developed to address disruptive behav-
this, if left untreated, challenging or problematic iors for children with ASD. These programs pro-
behaviors have the tendency to persist across set- vide parents with important behavioral
tings and impair the child’s ability to regulate management strategies and emphasize the parent
once the behavior is established (Oliver et  al., as the primary agent of change for the child
2012), which can significantly impact function- (Postorino et al., 2017). Consistent with an ABA
ing across domains (Bearss et al., 2015). approach, functional assessment/analysis is a
As with the descriptions of NDBIs above, it core component of these interventions, such that
has not been until recently that more formalized clinicians have the opportunity to help parents
and manualized parent training interventions understand antecedents and consequences that
have been developed to reduce and improve co-­ may drive their child’s behaviors (Hanley et al.,
occurring maladaptive and disruptive behaviors 2003). In this framework, intervention models
(e.g., aggression, tantrum behaviors, non-­ often consist of psychoeducation, didactic
compliance, self-injury; Burrell et  al., 2020; instruction, direct modeling, observation, and
Edwards, 2018; Edwards et  al., 2019; Scahill interactive coaching techniques.
et al., 2016). Historically, parents and clinicians Below, we highlight a few of the current
were provided with a series of self-guided evidence-­ based parent training programs
resources to target maladaptive behaviors in chil- designed specifically for children with ASD, rec-
dren with ASD (e.g., No More Meltdowns; Baker, ognizing the ongoing need for continued clinical
2008) and research on the efficacy or effective- and research investigation in this area.
ness of these specific self-guided approaches was
limited (Bearss et al., 2015).
Moreover, the earliest research on parent-­ 16.3.1 Functional Communication
training programs was limited by inconsistent use Training
of standardized manuals, individualized treatment
approaches that lacked generalizability, and small Functional communication training (FCT) is a
sample sizes (Anderson & McMillan, 2001; well-established behavioral approach designed to
Bearss et  al., 2015; Ducharme & Drain, 2004; reduce problematic behaviors with children, such
Moes & Frea, 2002; Wahler et al., 2004). Despite as aggressive and destructive behaviors, self-­
these methodological weaknesses, these early harm, and tantrums (Falcomata & Wacker, 2013;
studies were important in establishing founda- Gerow et  al., 2018; Tiger et  al., 2008). While
tional efficacy for specific parent training tech- FCT is thought to be most effective in early child-
niques in treating disruptive behaviors for children hood and during the elementary years, there is
with ASD (Bearss et  al., 2015). Fortunately, reason to suspect that it is appropriate even for
researchers within the ASD field were able to pull older children (Franzone, 2009). Additionally,
from a longstanding and strong evidence base for FCT can be used with children regardless of their
parent training interventions that reduce challeng- cognitive and/or expressive language abilities
298 S. Licona et al.

(Franzone, 2009). Importantly, FCT was identi- found to generate an accurate implementation of
fied as a broad category evidence-based practice the FCT intervention during a trained routine.
in the most recent NCAEP review, with 31 high-­ The findings for these methods during novel rou-
quality efficacy and effectiveness studies demon- tines (i.e., generalization of the skill from trained
strating positive effects on behavior and routine to other settings/contexts) were less con-
communication for children with ASD from 1990 sistent, although notably the study was limited by
to 2017 (Steinbrenner et al., 2020). a small sample size (n = 3; Gerow et al., 2018).
The overarching goals of FCT are to (1) iden- Thus, ongoing studies to determine the best train-
tify the functions of challenging behaviors, (2) ing models and/or development of a more stan-
teach the child replacement behaviors that dardized, manualized treatment approach to
include more effective, communicative responses, training parents in FCT would be warranted to
and (3) provide reinforcement for the replace- support these findings.
ment response (Gerow et  al., 2018; Mancil & Concerns that parent-implemented, compared
Boman, 2010; Muharib & Wood, 2018; Tiger to therapist-implemented, FCT may produce dif-
et  al., 2008). Additionally, reinforcement in the ferent outcomes in child behavior and have var-
context of challenging behaviors is withheld ied implementation fidelity have been expressed
(Gerow et  al., 2018; Mancil & Boman, 2010; (Gerow et al., 2018). For example, there may be
Tiger et al., 2008). To accomplish this, functional increased rates of challenging child behaviors
behavior assessments (FBAs), a key component during parent-implemented sessions (English &
of FCT, are conducted. Indeed, FBAs are used to Anderson, 2004; Hanley et  al., 2003; Huete &
identify the variables that maintain or reinforce Kurtz, 2010; Ringdahl & Sellers, 2000).
challenging behaviors (e.g., attention, escape) Individual parent differences, such as differences
and help guide the intervention plan (Muharib & in training approaches, time restrictions, and
Wood, 2018). Following this, replacement behav- types of reinforcement (Gerow et al., 2018), may
iors can be taught to the child that produce the also interfere with parent FCT sessions (Feldman
same individual end-goal (Muharib & Wood, et al., 2004; Moes & Frea, 2000, 2002; Sloman
2018). Notably, given the significant relationship et al., 2005).
between impairments in communication and dis- Despite these concerns, Gerow et  al. (2018)
ruptive behaviors (Kaiser et al., 2002; Park et al., conducted a systematic review of the existing lit-
2012), interventions such as FCT are particularly erature on parent training in FCT.  Across peer-­
useful in improving communication skills, and reviewed studies, FCT conducted by parents was
subsequent behavioral problems, among children indeed effective in reducing challenging behav-
with ASD with severe language deficits. iors of children (Gerow et al., 2018). For exam-
Historically, FCT has been most commonly ple, a single-subject design study by Mancil et al.
delivered by clinicians with training in behav- (2006) revealed a clinically significant reduction
ioral principles. In the more recent past, however, in challenging behaviors for a young boy follow-
there has been an attempt to increase parental ing the completion of FCT with his mother.
involvement in FCT interventions, adapting a Furthermore, gains in spontaneous communica-
parent training approach to this intervention. tion were also reported. Parent-implemented
Reports regarding training models for parents FCT intervention outcomes have also been shown
in FCT have varied across research studies, with to, on average, maintain over time and generalize
different methods described for instructional pro- across new environments (Gerow et  al., 2018).
cedures and performance feedback (Barton & Parents were described as active participants in
Fettig, 2013; Gerow et al., 2018; Ward-Horner & the FBA process and often implemented all of the
Sturmey, 2012). In a study by Gerow et al. (2018), required FCT intervention sessions, which were
verbal and written instructions for parents, along- relative strengths of the current literature in this
side performative feedback regarding their ability area and suggest that parent-implemented FCT
to effectively deliver the FCT strategies, were represents a promising intervention approach
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 299

(Gerow et al., 2018). However, this comprehen- sions, seven supplemental sessions, a home visit,
sive review also suggested that parents were and follow-up telephone booster sessions as
inconsistently involved in the development of the needed (Bearss et al., 2018; Edwards et al., 2019).
FCT intervention planning process (such that this The program is designed for children aged three
was typically conducted by the therapist), despite to ten and typically spans a six-month interven-
the fact that this could help improve parent sus- tion period (Bearss et  al., 2015). Clinicians are
tainability of the intervention (Moes & Frea, provided with scripts for each session, as well as
2002) and address important individual-level parent activity sheets and handouts (Bearss et al.,
variables (e.g., contextual family variables; 2018). The content of early RUBI sessions
culturally and linguistically sensitive interven-
­ focuses on teaching parents the different func-
tion plans; Gerow et  al., 2018; Koegel, 2000; tions of behavior, such as understanding behav-
Moes & Frea, 2000, 2002). ioral antecedents (i.e., the situation or action that
Taken together, there remains an ongoing precedes problem behavior) and consequences
need for future research to examine ways to more (Bearss et  al., 2018). For example, if a parent
effectively include parents in the development of identifies that problem behaviors predictably
FCT and examine the impacts of this on both occur following times of transition, especially
child and parent outcomes. Identifying individual away from preferred activities (i.e., antecedent),
and dyadic-specific variables, environmental fac- yet after the behavior happens the parent does not
tors, and adequate supports that can better sup- require the child to transition (the child stays on
port parent learning and implementation to the preferred activity; i.e., consequence), they are
potentially increase overall effectiveness and unintentionally or unknowingly reinforcing the
acceptability of parent-implemented FCT are problematic behavior that successfully functions
important next steps. In particular, future work as an escape or avoidance mechanism. In this
clarifying how, when, and via which methods to intervention, parents would subsequently learn
include parents as FCT interventionists is war- new strategies for preventing these behaviors and
ranted to enhance outcomes and support clinical better preparing the child for transitions.
decision-making in the practice settings (Gerow Parents are provided with support from the
et al., 2018). therapist as they learn to better identify the ante-
cedents of the problem behavior and develop a
series of preventative strategies. Early sessions
16.3.2 Research Units in Behavioral also introduce parents to the use of daily visual
Intervention (RUBI) Autism schedules that are aimed at decreasing their
Network child’s behavior problems. Parents learn the con-
cept of reinforcers as a way to increase compli-
Similar to FCT, the Research Units in Behavioral ance and prosocial behaviors. There is also an
Intervention (RUBI) parent training intervention emphasis on helping parents teach play and social
is based on an ABA framework and recognizes skills through child-directed play, particularly in
that problematic behaviors (e.g., disruptive, non- the context of providing positive reinforcement.
compliant, aggressive behaviors) serve an impor- Toward the latter half of the intervention, ses-
tant function for the child. Like FCT, the RUBI sions begin to emphasize compliance training
program aims to address these behaviors in the (e.g., increasing effective parental requests and
context of the child’s daily activities (e.g., getting commands), functional communication training,
dressed, preparing for bed, managing trips to the task analysis and chaining, prompting proce-
store), which generally represent a significant dures, and generalization of skills. Supplemental
source of the daily struggle for families of chil- sessions are also available and may include top-
dren with ASD (Bearss et al., 2015). ics such as token economy systems, imitation
The RUBI program follows a manualized skills, time out, sleep and/or feeding problems,
intervention approach, consisting of 11 core ses- toilet training, and crisis management.
300 S. Licona et al.

Although RUBI itself was not yet considered 16.3.3 Parent–Child Interaction
a MIMC in the NCAEP review as a unique Therapy (PCIT)
evidence-­ based practice, it falls under the
evidence-­based category of Parent-Implemented Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) is a
Interventions, and studies examining RUBI are parent–child intervention originally developed
pivotal to the Parent-Implemented Interventions for children aged two to seven with disruptive
evidence base (Steinbrenner et  al., 2020). behaviors without ASD (Funderburk & Eyberg,
Indeed, research examining the effectiveness of 2011). In this population, it is highly effective in
the RUBI parent program has found that it reducing disruptive and oppositional behaviors
reduces ­problematic behaviors in children based and strengthening parent–child relationships
on parent and clinician reports (Bearss, Johnson, (Briegel, 2016; Funderburk et al., 1998; García &
et  al., 2013). Indeed, initial studies examining Velasco, 2014; Urquiza & Timmer, 2012; Zisser
the effectiveness of the RUBI model found sig- & Eyberg, 2010). While PCIT has not yet been
nificant reductions across problematic behav- validated for individuals with ASD, there is
iors, including irritability, hyperactivity, emerging literature highlighting the potential
stereotypy, social withdrawal, and inappropriate effectiveness of PCIT for individuals with ASD
speech (Bearss, Johnson, et  al., 2013). (Lesack et al., 2014; Masse et al., 2016; Solomon
Improvements in aspects of daily functioning et al., 2008), particularly when adaptations to the
were also reported (Bearss, Johnson, et  al., intervention are made (Lesack et al., 2014).
2013). Follow-up studies have continued to sup- Broadly, PCIT for children without ASD con-
port the efficacy of this program, with more sists of two treatment phases: (1) child-directed
recent research indicating significant gains rela- interaction (CDI) and (2) parent-directed interac-
tive to a parent education program (e.g., sessions tion (PDI). The CDI phase is considered relation-
aimed at providing parents with information ship enhancement and emphasizes parents
about ASD without behavior management strat- engaging in playtime with their child and learn-
egies; Bearss et  al., 2015). When compared to ing to follow their child’s lead. In this phase, the
parent education programs, the RUBI model therapist’s focus is to help the parent master posi-
remains effective at increasing activities of daily tive skills such as labeled praises, reflections,
living, with the most notable gains in daily liv- behavior descriptions, and imitation, while
ing skills among children with higher baseline simultaneously avoiding negative talk, such as
cognition (Scahill et  al., 2016). There is also commands, questions, and criticism. The PDI
additional benefit when RUBI is paired with phase extends on CDI by teaching parents how to
pharmacological intervention (Aman et  al., use effective commands and to implement struc-
2009; Bearss et  al., 2015; Bearss, Lecavalier, tured timeout sequences in response to non-­
et  al., 2013; Scahill et  al., 2016). Notably, compliance. Across both phases, PCIT sessions
although the RUBI program was designed to be incorporate 1-h, weekly sessions and include a
delivered to parents on an individual basis dur- combination of didactics and live coaching.
ing weekly outpatient sessions, a recent commu- Studies have recently started to examine
nity study adapted the RUBI program to be whether PCIT is effective for children with dis-
applied in a group-based format, finding prelim- ruptive behaviors with ASD. Masse et al. (2016)
inary support for the delivery of RUBI to parent found that PCIT was able to increase child com-
groups (Edwards, 2018; Edwards et  al., 2019). pliance, reduce disruptive behavior, and improve
While continued studies remain warranted, this parenting skills in a small sample (n = 3) of chil-
preliminary work suggests a potentially cost- dren with ASD. Ginn et al. (2017) similarly found
effective approach that could maximize the that among a larger group of children with ASD
availability of this intervention. (n = 30), eight sessions of the CDI phase of treat-
ment were effective in reducing disruptive behav-
ior and increasing child social awareness. There
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 301

were also reported reductions in maternal dis- implementation procedures and may facilitate
tress, and parents learned new strategies for pro- community providers’ deployment of evidence-­
viding positive attention to appropriate social and based approaches across the myriad of patients
play behaviors in their children (Ginn et  al., who walk in their doors, including children with
2017). Other studies have similarly started to rep- ASD. This is particularly important when consid-
licate findings that PCIT is effective in improving ering that many youths with ASD receive ser-
disruptive behaviors of children with ASD and vices across usual care settings (e.g., community
across language and developmental levels mental and behavioral health clinics) from non-­
(Scudder et  al., 2018; Scudder et  al., 2019), specialist providers with diverse training and
although specific findings related to changes in educational backgrounds (Christon et  al., 2015;
parental stress and autism severity have been Cidav et  al., 2013; McLennan et  al., 2008).
inconsistent, with some studies reporting Further, a focus on training and implementation
improvements in these areas (Agazzi et al., 2017; of transdiagnostic behavioral approaches across
Ginn et al., 2017) and others suggesting no sig- service settings could help reduce long waitlists
nificant differences (Scudder et al., 2019). While for families who are referred to ASD-specialist
ongoing research remains warranted, early services due to significant behavioral needs
research suggests that PCIT represents a promis- (Kanne & Bishop, 2021). On the other hand, a
ing and important intervention for children with program like RUBI that includes a formal thera-
ASD and co-morbid disruptive behaviors, with a pist training protocol and technical supports may
need for continued studies to focus on identifying offer a structured way for generalist providers to
the effectiveness across samples and the specific become proficient in the delivery of ASD-specific
types of clinical adaptations needed to best intervention. Overall, there has been tremendous
accommodate the unique needs of children with development in parent training programs over the
ASD. last two decades; however, this remains a highly
relevant area of clinical and research investiga-
tion (e.g., determining individual factors that
16.3.4 Summary of Parent Training may predict best treatment outcomes and subse-
Interventions for Disruptive quently triage families into these services
Behaviors in ASD accordingly).

The programs and data reviewed above offer evi-


dence that parent training interventions targeting 16.4 Telehealth
co-occurring behavioral difficulties are also
highly effective at reducing disruptive behaviors, As highlighted in the previous sections, there is
non-compliance, and aggression. It is promising an already robust and growing body of literature
that even across these different parent training indicating that parents can be successfully trained
approaches (i.e., FCT, RUBI, PCIT), parents in strategies to support social communication and
appear able to learn the intervention strategies behavioral functioning in their children with
and their children show corresponding behav- autism. Unfortunately, long-standing and signifi-
ioral improvements. There is a continued need cant barriers impede on the dissemination of
for research to understand which programs will these evidence-based intervention programs,
fit best in a given service delivery setting and be including a shortage of trained professionals,
most effective and for which children and fami- limited financial resources and transportation,
lies. For example, PCIT and FCT are transdiag- lack of childcare, geographic isolation, lengthy
nostic, meaning that they are appropriate for use waitlists, and extensive time commitments
with children with ASD, as well as children with (Stahmer & Gist, 2001; Symon, 2001; Taylor
other clinical presentations. Transdiagnostic et  al., 2008). As a result, autism intervention
interventions allow for efficient training and researchers, informed by innovative health ser-
302 S. Licona et al.

vices work and dissemination and implementa- 16.4.1 Telehealth for Parent-­


tion sciences, have started to examine Mediated Interventions
non-traditional strategies, such as telehealth, for
delivery of parent implementation interventions. There is a burgeoning body of literature examin-
Telehealth, or providing health care remotely ing the effectiveness of telehealth-delivered
through a variety of telecommunication tools parent-­mediated NDBIs, including the three spe-
(e.g., video conferencing platforms), is a rapidly cific programs discussed earlier in the chapter,
growing service delivery method for health care for enhancing core social communication skills
workers (Dorsey & Topol, 2016). Telehealth in children with ASD.
technology has been utilized to provide training PRT has been delivered via telehealth using a
and coaching to parents with children diagnosed primarily self-guided program; researchers used
with ASD and other neurodevelopmental disor- DVDs to educate and train parents through self-­
ders (Benson et al., 2018; Falcomata & Wacker, directed modules focusing on specific PRT strat-
2013). The use of telehealth technology has egies (Nefdt et al., 2010). An initial study on this
research supporting its effectiveness to help par- approach found that the majority of parents com-
ents teach their children imitation skills, play, and pleted the program and demonstrated the ability
social communication skills, and to support par- to use PRT strategies effectively during interac-
ents as they mitigate problem behavior. Aside tions with their children. Additional results
from telehealth’s promising effectiveness, it fur- showed that the children increased their func-
ther provides benefits to those parents with sig- tional communication/utterances, and parents
nificant barriers in their environment that limit rated the program highly satisfactory (Nefdt
their ability to access early intervention and et al., 2010).
behavior support services. Vismara et  al. (2012) examined the remote
The literature generally describes three dif- delivery of parent-mediated ESDM via a hybrid
ferent types of telehealth approaches used for approach. They provided parents with learning
parent implementation interventions in ASD. modules on DVD and remote coaching over video
Self-­guided telehealth programs give parents an conferencing. The parents in this study achieved
online platform, allowing them to access lessons fidelity in the ESDM intervention skills and main-
and modules to undertake at their own pace. tained these gains across a six-week follow-up
Therapist-assisted telehealth programs provide period. The children demonstrated corresponding
parents with consultation, feedback, and support increases in social communication and social
from trained clinicians. Finally, hybrid telehealth engagement with their parents (Vismara et  al.,
programs often integrate a self-directed compo- 2012). An additional study examined the effec-
nent with opportunities for feedback and support tiveness of a hybrid telehealth program, with
from a therapist. Research has utilized all three online modules and therapist coaching, in the
of these methods to remotely deliver the differ- ESDM intervention across 12  weeks, and the
ent types of parent implementation interventions results suggested that parents were able to learn
previously discussed in this chapter, although it ESDM strategies and then use them effectively in
is important to note that research on the efficacy, interactions with their children (Vismara et  al.,
effectiveness, and sustainability of these pro- 2013). A larger randomized control trial com-
grams is just beginning. Below is a presentation pared this hybrid telehealth ESDM approach to
of the evidence supporting the use of telehealth treatment as usual; social communication
technology to successfully implement parent-­ improved in both groups, although larger gains
mediated interventions for skill-building and were observed for those in the telehealth ESDM
parent training interventions for maladaptive condition (Vismara et al., 2018).
behaviors. Project ImPACT has also been adapted for
both a self-guided and hybrid telehealth approach,
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 303

referred to as ImPACT Online (Ingersoll et  al., majority of these studies tend to involve a
2016). An initial randomized control trial of therapist-­assisted approach to supporting parents
ImPACT Online compared the self-guided to a as they learn and implement functional assess-
hybrid (additional parent coaching from a thera- ments necessary for the appropriate application
pist) model and found that while parents in both of FCT (e.g., understanding the function of the
groups improved in their overall use of Project behavior and creating meaningful behavior and
ImPACT strategies, those in the hybrid condition communication targets within that context) with
showed greater improvements in the fidelity of their children with autism. Initial single-subject
implementation. Furthermore, parents in the designs found that, with guidance from a thera-
hybrid condition reported that support from par- pist, parents were able to learn how to engage in
ent coaching was an essential part of their ability a functional behavior assessment and implement
to learn the material (Pickard et al., 2016). FCT to increase their children’s communication
One of the latest updates in telehealth technol- and decrease challenging or maladaptive behav-
ogy is the use of mobile apps to deliver evidence-­ iors including self-injury (e.g., Benson et  al.,
based practices, which has been applied to 2018; Machalicek et  al., 2016; Simacek et  al.,
interventions for parents of children with ASD. 2017). A 12-week randomized control trial com-
Map4speech, a mobile application based on an paring telehealth-delivered FCT to a waitlist con-
adaptation of Project ImPACT, has been piloted trol group for children with ASD and moderate to
with promising results (Law et al., 2018). Parents severe behavior problems found that FCT led to
in this study had the opportunity to access a greater overall reductions in challenging behav-
hybrid model on their personal cell phones. In ior (Lindgren et al., 2020). Importantly, compari-
addition to accessing learning modules, parents sons between in-person and remote coaching for
had the ability to upload videos of their interac- functional assessment and FCT found no signifi-
tions and receive feedback from trained thera- cant differences between their effectiveness to
pists. Even through the use of mobile apps, reduce problem behavior (Lindgren et al., 2016).
parents maintained high fidelity and built up their The RUBI program has also been adapted to a
skills based on the intervention. Furthermore, the therapist-assisted telehealth delivery format that
children’s functional communication increased closely mirrors the standard, in-person RUBI
compared to baseline (Law et al., 2018). parent training model (Bearss et al., 2018). In an
Overall, these smaller-scale studies of initial feasibility trial, parents were provided with
telehealth-­
delivered, parent-mediated NDBIs a RUBI treatment manual and met virtually with
have provided initial evidence that parents can a therapist over 16 weeks to learn 11 “core” strat-
learn and use intervention strategies in response egies and up to two “supplemental” strategies
to telehealth programs and that children demon- depending on the needs of the family and child
strate corresponding increases in key social com- (Bearss et al., 2018). Session attendance and sat-
munication skills as their parents participate in isfaction with the telehealth delivery were high,
these interventions. with all parents who completed the program
endorsing that they would recommend this
approach to others. Further, parents reported
16.4.2 Telehealth for Parent Training increases in confidence to manage their child’s
Interventions current and future challenging behaviors, and
children showed decreases in parent-reported
There is also evidence for the delivery of parent noncompliance and irritability over the course of
training approaches for ASD via telehealth, the study (Bearss et al., 2018).
including some of those reviewed in this chapter. Lastly, while Parent–Child Interaction
Indeed, there is a quickly evolving literature Therapy (PCIT) has emerging literature to sug-
examining telehealth delivered FCT to address gest that it is effective in the treatment for chil-
challenging behaviors of children with ASD. The dren with ASD, there has not been any literature
304 S. Licona et al.

examining the effectiveness of PCIT for children well as the lack of privacy and/or the presence of
with ASD conducted via telehealth. However, other siblings or family members. If the behav-
there is promising research to suggest that iors that are the target of the intervention are
Internet-delivered PCIT (I-PCIT) can be effec- physically dangerous, the practitioner is not able
tively administered, in a feasible and cost-­ to be physically present to help mitigate the
effective manner (Comer et  al., 2015; Elkins & behaviors (Lerman et al., 2020).
Comer, 2014). In the studies where I-PCIT was Importantly, not all parents may benefit
being used, parents completed sessions in their equally from telehealth. For example, initial data
homes and received direct coaching from the have revealed that certain family characteristics
therapist via a Bluetooth headset (Comer et  al., such as self-report parental depressive symptoms
2015, 2017). There has been one RCT comparing are negatively correlated with success (Ingersoll
I-PCIT versus the standard PCIT with 40 chil- & Berger, 2015). Concurrently, it has been sug-
dren with disruptive behavior disorder between gested that a subgroup of parents may require
the ages of 3 and 5  years. The results demon- more support than online video conferencing is
strated that I-PCIT was relatively well received able to provide, in which case they may not ben-
and the children showed treatment response; fur- efit as greatly from telehealth (Schieltz et  al.,
thermore, the children in the I-PCIT group 2018). Additionally, problem behavior main-
showed an excellent response posttreatment tained by automatic reinforcement may be diffi-
(Comer et al., 2017), suggesting a future need to cult to address fully over telehealth, as well as
assess this model within ASD. behaviors that change in function over time
(Schieltz et al., 2018).
Overall, individual studies, randomized con-
16.4.3 Barriers and Limitations trolled trials, and systematic reviews of telehealth
of Telehealth Services practice for parents of children with ASD suggest
that across parent implementation approaches
While telehealth has shown immense promise (e.g., parent-mediated NDBIs, FCT, RUBI) and
over the past decade or so in terms of its effec- formats (self-guided, therapist-assisted, or a
tiveness and feasibility for families to use, imple- hybrid model), telehealth delivery can be an
ment, and learn, it is not without limitations. effective and promising approach for disseminat-
Barriers to accessing telehealth services for fami- ing evidence-based practices (Boisvert et  al.,
lies and practitioners often include not having a 2010; Ferguson et  al., 2019; Johnsson et  al.,
reliable Internet connection in the home to have 2016; Knutsen et  al., 2016; Neely et  al., 2017;
consistent conversations and sessions (De Los Parsons et  al., 2017; Sutherland et  al., 2018;
Rios Perez, 2018; Lerman et  al., 2020; Reese Tomlinson et  al., 2018; Unholz-Bowden et  al.,
et al., 2012). Difficulty finding a reliable Internet 2020). Indeed, the use of telehealth has the poten-
connection can lead to audio and video issues, tial to decrease barriers typically faced by rural
which can limit the quality of service that practi- and underserved areas by increasing the ability to
tioners are able to provide (Reese et al., 2012). access evidence-based services (Ashburner et al.,
Additional barriers include the parents’ com- 2016; Dorsey & Topol, 2016; Mello et al., 2016;
fort and capability of accessing telehealth ser- Murphy & Ruble, 2012) at reduced costs (Horn
vices and technology (Salomone & Maurizio et al., 2016; Jennett et al., 2003; Knutsen et al.,
Arduino, 2017). Further, when telehealth ses- 2016) and without placing an undue burden on
sions are provided within the context of the home, these families to travel to centers far away from
there are environmental variables that can impact their homes (Heitzman-Powell et al., 2014; Mello
the sessions, including limited control of the et al., 2016). Importantly, the COVID-19 global
environment, the child having access to toys and pandemic has led to the rapid and wide-scale
reinforcers within the home that would be limited adoption and implementation of telehealth pro-
within a clinic setting (Lerman et  al., 2020), as grams, including parent implementation inter-
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 305

ventions for ASD.  It is expected that data provider training protocols that support the dis-
collected from both research and practice settings semination of parent implementation interven-
during this time will be critical for increasing the tions in practice and community settings.
understanding of the effectiveness of these Notably, as with any clinical decision-making
approaches, as well as the larger public health process, it is important for clinicians to carefully
significance of telehealth interventions including, weigh the pros and cons of when to deliver such
but not limited to, the extent to which these pro- parent implementation models, considering pro-
grams address or potentially contribute to dis- gram type and delivery structure in the context of
parities in care. each family and the child’s particular needs
(Siller & Morgan, 2018). For example, despite
the fact that families often indicate an urgent
16.5 Conclusions need to start comprehensive intervention pro-
grams, including parent-mediated NDBIs, recent
The delivery of parent implementation interven- evidence suggests that the timing of when a fam-
tions is associated with a number of important ily starts a parent-mediated intervention may
clinical outcomes for children, both in terms of impact participation and attrition rates (Pickard
reducing the severity of core deficits in ASD and et  al., 2016). Further, there are data to suggest
improving co-occurring behavioral challenges. that previous experiences with services can drive
Parent-mediated interventions targeting core interest in enrolling in these programs and stay-
symptoms of ASD such as PRT, ESDM, and ing engaged throughout (McCurdy & Daro,
Project ImPACT are associated with increases in 2001). As a result, it may be important for clini-
child’s spontaneous language, imitation, and cians to first establish rapport and trust with a
communication skills (Dawson et  al., 2010; family, prior to offering a parent implementation
Duifhuis et  al., 2017; Minjarez et  al., 2011). intervention in order to set up families to be as
Similarly, parent training programs focusing on successful as possible. Finally, while one of the
behavior reduction such as FCT, RUBI Autism most important strengths of parent-implemented
Network, and PCIT report an overall reduction in interventions is the role of the parents and their
challenging behaviors, increase in child compli- ability to incorporate evidence-based strategies
ance, and improvements in parent training skills in the context of their child’s day-to-day life and
(Bearss, Johnson, et al., 2013; Bearss, Lecavalier, routines, this may not always be feasible for par-
et  al., 2013; Gerow et  al., 2018; Masse et  al., ents given other demands in their personal lives
2016). (e.g., professional obligations, other caregiving
responsibilities; McConnell & Savage, 2015).
This may be particularly true for families from
16.5.1 Clinical Implications underrepresented, lower-income communities
who are more likely to face additional challenges
Given these promising outcomes, it is imperative with financial stability, transportation, and child-
for caregivers and providers to be able to find and care (Stahmer & Gist, 2001; Symon, 2001; Taylor
access parent implementation interventions et al., 2008). Importantly, this does not mean that
within community settings. Providers and organi- families with competing priorities should not be
zations are encouraged to seek out formal train- offered opportunities to engage in parent imple-
ing in evidence-based parent implementation mentation interventions; rather, it is critical to
approaches and to work with program trainers consider how best to structure programs so that it
and developers to consider how best to deploy is easier for families to participate and be suc-
these programs within their unique practice set- cessful considering these barriers (e.g., offering
tings. Fortunately, the formalization and manual- childcare during sessions, offering evening and
ization of parent implementation interventions weekend sessions). In summary, providers must
have resulted in the development of prescribed use careful clinical judgment to determine when
306 S. Licona et al.

and what parent implementation intervention is 2016). Given that parents take on a large respon-
most appropriate on an individual basis, with a sibility by assuming the role of “therapist” in
need to consider child’s unique profile of these interventions, a better understanding of out-
strengths and weaknesses, family’s goals for comes such as parental stress, parental compe-
intervention, and individual family factors (e.g., tence, and family quality of life is necessary
dynamics of parent home, family stressors, cul- (Estes et  al., 2015; Ginn et  al., 2017;
tural factors that may impact participation or out- Schwichtenberg & Poehlmann, 2007; Stainbrook
comes, etc.). et al., 2019).
Relatedly, research has started to examine
how certain interventions might work to produce
16.5.2 Limitations & Future observed changes in child functioning. One line
Directions of research has focused on identifying active
ingredients and mechanisms of change in parent
Despite the important promise of parent imple- implementation interventions. For example, ini-
mentation interventions for ASD, there are limi- tial research found that increases in parent use of
tations worth discussing and important directions Project ImPACT strategies were directly associ-
for future research. Indeed, while there is clear ated with improvements in child language
data that parent implementation approaches work (Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013); later work supported
on average for improving functioning for chil- this contention by demonstrating that parent use
dren and families, the field’s understanding of of Project ImPACT strategies improved chil-
how, why, and for whom these interventions dren’s later language abilities via improvement
work is still limited. There are several future motor imitation and intentional social communi-
directions that can be explored concurrently to cation (Yoder et al., 2020). Another approach to
help build a more sophisticated and nuanced understanding how interventions work has been
understanding of parent implementation to examine objective and neurobiological mea-
approaches for children with ASD. sures in response to treatment (e.g., Dawson
To date, very few studies have explored indi- et  al., 2012; Voos et  al., 2013). Research on
vidual family/parent/child variables that may clinician-­administered ESDM demonstrated that,
best predict family involvement and response to in addition to improvements in social communi-
intervention (Gerow et  al., 2018; Tarver et  al., cation, adaptive functioning, and cognition, chil-
2019; Wade et al., 2008). However, identification dren in the ESDM group showed increased EEG
of such variables could support clinical decision-­ activation in brain areas associated with social
making processes that can often be challenging behavior (Dawson et  al., 2012). However,
for providers given the availability of many paral- research has yet to apply these innovative out-
lel therapies. Having more predictive data in come measurement approaches to parent imple-
terms of which families will respond best to spe- mentation interventions in ASD. Additional data
cific intervention types would thus help clinicians related to mechanisms of change, as well as neu-
maximize limited resources and better support robiological outcomes, of parent implementation
children and their families. Additionally, a better interventions is a critical next step.
understanding of the optimal sequence of inter- Questions about the long-term impact of par-
ventions (e.g., which should be first: parent train- ent implementation on child developmental tra-
ing to address challenging behaviors or jectories also remain. Additional longitudinal
parent-mediated intervention to improve commu- research is necessary to determine the effect of
nication?) would further enhance the efficiency parent implementation interventions on child
and effectiveness of service delivery. In addition, social communication, behavior, and adaptive
it is critical that research continues to expand the functioning in later childhood and adulthood as
understanding of outcomes beyond parent fidel- improvements in pivotal developmental skills,
ity and child-level functioning (Wainer et  al., such as those that comprise social communica-
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 307

tion, have long-term developmental implications evant and important to both clinical practice and
(Greenslade et al., 2019). Further research should research in autism spectrum disorders.
establish the cost-effectiveness and public health
significance of parent implementation approaches
over time. References
Finally, the majority of research on parent
implementation interventions for ASD continues Agazzi, H., Tan, S. Y., Ogg, J., Armstrong, K., & Kirby,
R.  S. (2017). Does parent-child interaction therapy
to include families from similar cultural and reduce maternal stress, anxiety, and depression among
socio-economic backgrounds. Frequently, fami- mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder?
lies coming from underrepresented communities Child and Family Behavior Therapy. https://doi.org/1
face challenges that not only impact participation 0.1080/07317107.2017.1375622
Aman, M.  G., Mcdougle, C.  J., Scahill, L.,
in the intervention but also impact interest and Handen, B., Arnold, L. E., Johnson, C., Stigler,
ability to participate in research studies (Carr & K.  A., Bearss, K., Butter, E., Swiezy, N.  B.,
Lord, 2016). Active efforts are underway to Sukhodolsky, D. D., Ramadan, Y., Pozdol, S. L.,
engage underrepresented populations in research Nikolov, R., Lecavalier, L., Kohn, A. E., Koenig,
K., Hollway, J. A., Korzekwa, P., … Wagner, A.
and study parent implementation for ASD inter- (2009). Medication and parent training in chil-
vention in more diverse populations (e.g., Carr & dren with pervasive developmental disorders and
Lord, 2016; Carr et  al., 2015; Pickard et  al., serious behavior problems: Results from a ran-
2017). Pickard et al. (2017) note that it is critical domized clinical trial. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.
to engage underrepresented families in the devel- https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181bfd669
opment and adaptation of parent implementation American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
interventions in order to improve the fit and and statistical manual of mental disorders. American
increase the likelihood of sustainability and Psychiatric Association. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.
books.9780890425596
effectiveness of such programs. Overall, while it Anderson, C.  M., & McMillan, K. (2001). Parental use
is encouraging that current work in this area is of escape extinction and differential reinforcement
underway, there remains a long way to go in the to treat food selectivity. Journal of Applied Behavior
field to develop a more representative and equi- Analysis. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-­511
Anderson, S. R., Avery, D. L., DiPietro, E. K., & Edwards,
table research base for parent implementation G. L. (1987). Intensive home-based early intervention
interventions in ASD. with autistic children. Education and Treatment of
Children, 10(4), 352–366.
Ashburner, J., Vickerstaff, S., Beetge, J., & Copley,
J. (2016). Remote versus face-to-face delivery
16.5.3 Summary of early intervention programs for children with
autism spectrum disorders: Perceptions of rural
Over the last several decades, parent-mediated families and service providers. Research in Autism
and parent training interventions have come to Spectrum Disorders, 23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
rasd.2015.11.011
the forefront of intervention research in ASD, Baker, B. L. (1984). Steps to independence: A skills train-
particularly for young children and families. ing guide for parents and teachers of children with
These treatment approaches are extremely prom- special needs. Brookes Publishers.
ising and are gaining a strong evidence base. Baker, J. (2008). No more meltdowns: Positive strategies
for managing and preventing out-of-control behavior.
Indeed, as described in this chapter, studies have Future Horizons.
consistently documented that the inclusion of Barton, E.  E., & Fettig, A. (2013). Parent-
parents in the treatment of their child, particu- implemented interventions for young chil-
larly when provided with appropriate in-person dren with disabilities: A review of fidelity
features. Journal of Early Intervention. https://doi.
or telehealth support from trained therapists with org/10.1177/1053815113504625
experience in ASD, has the potential to signifi- Bearss, K., Burrell, T.  L., Stewart, L., & Scahill, L.
cantly improve and maximize a child’s outcomes (2015). Parent training in autism spectrum disor-
across critical developmental and behavioral der: What’s in a name? In Clinical child and family
psychology review. (Vol. 18, Issue 2, pp.  170–182).
domains, making parent interventions highly rel-
308 S. Licona et al.

Springer New  York LLC. https://doi.org/10.1007/ ior: A demonstration pilot. Journal of Autism and
s10567-­015-­0179-­5 Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Bearss, K., Johnson, C., Handen, B., Smith, T., & s10803-­020-­04427-­1
Scahill, L. (2013). A pilot study of parent train- Carr, T., & Lord, C. (2016). A pilot study promot-
ing in young children with autism spectrum disor- ing participation of families with limited resources
ders and disruptive behavior. Journal of Autism and in early autism intervention. Research in Autism
Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Spectrum Disorders, 25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
s10803-­012-­1624-­7 rasd.2016.02.003
Bearss, K., Johnson, C.  R., Handen, B.  L., Butter, E., Carr, T., Shih, W., Lawton, K., Lord, C., King, B., &
Lecavalier, L., Smith, T., & Scahill, L. (2018). Parent Kasari, C. (2015). The relationship between treat-
training for disruptive behavior: The RUBI Autism ment attendance, adherence, and outcome in a
Network, clinician manual. caregiver-­mediated intervention for low-resourced
Bearss, K., Lecavalier, L., Minshawi, N., Johnson, C., families of young children with autism spec-
Smith, T., Handen, B., Sukhodolsky, D.  G., Aman, trum disorder. Autism, 20(6), 643–652. https://doi.
M.  G., Swiezy, N., Butter, E., & Scahill, L. (2013). org/10.1177/1362361315598634
Toward an exportable parent training program for Catani, M., & Bambini, V. (2014). A model for social
disruptive behaviors in autism spectrum disorder. communication and language evolution and develop-
Neuropsychiatry. https://doi.org/10.2217/npy.13.14 ment. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 165–171.
Benson, S. S., Dimian, A. F., Elmquist, M., Simacek, J., https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.018
McComas, J.  J., & Symons, F.  J. (2018). Coaching Charlop, M.  H., & Trasowech, J.  E. (1991). Increasing
parents to assess and treat self-injurious behaviour via autistic children’s daily spontaneous speech. Journal
telehealth. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(4), 747–761. https://
62(12). https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12456 doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-­747
Boisvert, M., Lang, R., Andrianopoulos, M., & Christon, L.  M., Arnold, C.  C., & Myers, B.  J. (2015).
Boscardin, M.  L. (2010). Telepractice in the assess- Professionals’ reported provision and recommenda-
ment and treatment of individuals with autism spec- tion of psychosocial interventions for youth with
trum disorders: A systematic review. Developmental autism spectrum disorder. Behavior Therapy, 46(1).
Neurorehabilitation, 13(6). https://doi.org/10.3109/17 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2014.02.002
518423.2010.499889 Cidav, Z., Lawer, L., Marcus, S.  C., & Mandell, D.  S.
Bradshaw, J., Koegel, L.  K., & Koegel, R.  L. (2017). (2013). Age-related variation in health service use and
Improving functional language and social motiva- associated expenditures among children with autism.
tion with a parent-mediated intervention for toddlers Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and 43(4). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­012-­1637-­2
Developmental Disorders, 47(8), 2443–2458. https:// Comer, J.  S., Furr, J.  M., Miguel, E.  M., Cooper-Vince,
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­017-­3155-­8 C.  E., Carpenter, A.  L., Elkins, R.  M., Kerns, C.  E.,
Brestan, E.  V., & Eyberg, S.  M. (1998). Effective psy- Cornacchio, D., Chou, T., Coxe, S., DeSerisy, M.,
chosocial treatments of conduct-disordered children Sanchez, A.  L., Golik, A., Martin, J., Myers, K.  M.,
and adolescents: 29 years, 82 studies, and 5,272 kids. & Chase, R. (2017). Remotely delivering real-time
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. https://doi. parent training to the home: An initial randomized
org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2702_5 trial of Internet-delivered parent–child interaction
Briegel, W. (2016). Parent-child interaction therapy. therapy (I-PCIT). Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Zeitschrift fur Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychology, 89(9), 909–917.
Psychotherapie. https://doi.org/10.1024/1422-­4917/ Comer, J.  S., Furr, J.  M., Cooper-Vince, C., Madigan,
a000453 R. J., Chow, C., Chan, P. T., Idrobo, F., Chase, R. M.,
Bryson, S.  E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Brian, J., Roberts, McNeil, C.  B., & Eyberg, S.  M. (2015). Rationale
W., Szatmari, P., Rombough, V., & McDermott, and considerations for the Internet-based delivery
C. (2007). A prospective case series of high-risk of parent–child interaction therapy. Cognitive and
infants who developed autism. Journal of Autism and Behavioral Practice, 22(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 12–24. https://doi. cbpra.2014.07.003
org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0328-­2 Coolican, J., Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (2010). Brief
Burke, J.  D., Loeber, R., & Birmaher, B. (2002). parent training in pivotal response treatment for pre-
Oppositional defiant disorder and con- schoolers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology
duct disorder: A review of the past 10 years, and Psychiatry, 51(12), 1321–1330. https://doi.
part II. Journal of the American Academy of org/10.1111/j.1469-­7610.2010.02326.x
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. https://doi. Costin, J., & Chambers, S. M. (2007). Parent management
org/10.1097/00004583-­200211000-­00009 training as a treatment for children with oppositional
Burrell, T.  L., Postorino, V., Scahill, L., Rea, H.  M., defiant disorder referred to a mental health clinic.
Gillespie, S., Evans, A.  N., & Bearss, K. (2020). Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. https://doi.
Feasibility of group parent training for children with org/10.1177/1359104507080979
autism spectrum disorder and disruptive behav-
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 309

Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain intervention. Dissertation Abstracts International:
plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum dis- Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
order. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 775– Edwards, G.  S., Zlomke, K.  R., & Greathouse, A.  D.
803. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000370 (2019). RUBI parent training as a group intervention
Dawson, G., Jones, E.  J. H., Merkle, K., Venema, K., for children with autism: A community pilot study.
Lowy, R., Faja, S., Kamara, D., Murias, M., Greenson, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 66. https://
J., Winter, J., Smith, M., Rogers, S. J., & Webb, S. J. doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101409
(2012). Early behavioral intervention is associated Elkins, R.  M., & Comer, J.  S. (2014). Internet-based
with normalized brain activity in young children with implementation: Broadening the reach of parent-child
autism. Journal of the American Acadamy of Child interaction therapy for early child behavior problems.
Adolescence Psychiatry, 51(11) www.jaacap.org. In R. S. Beidas & E. S. Kendell (Eds.), Dissemination
www.jaacap.org and implementation of evidence-based practices in
Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, child and adolescent mental health (pp.  336–355).
J., Greenson, J., Donaldson, A., & Varley, J. (2010). Oxford University Press.
Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention English, C.  L., & Anderson, C.  M. (2004). Effects of
for toddlers with autism: The early start Denver familiar versus unfamiliar therapists on respond-
model. Pediatrics, 125(1). https://doi.org/10.1542/ ing in the analog functional analysis. Research in
peds.2009-­0958 Developmental Disabilities. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
De Los Rios Perez, C. (2018). Adaptable user interfaces ridd.2003.04.002
for people with autism: A transportation example. Estes, A., Munson, J., Rogers, S.  J., Greenson, J.,
Proceedings of the Internet of Accessible Things, 12, Winter, J., & Dawson, G. (2015). Long-term out-
1–2. https://doi.org/10.1145/3192714.3196318 comes of early intervention in 6-year-old chil-
Dorsey, E. R., & Topol, E. J. (2016). State of telehealth. dren with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
New England Journal of Medicine, 375(2), 154–161. the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmra1601705 Psychiatry, 54(7), 580–587. https://doi.
Dretzke, J., Davenport, C., Frew, E., Barlow, J., Stewart-­ org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.04.005
Brown, S., Bayliss, S., Taylor, R. S., Sandercock, J., & Falcomata, T. S., & Wacker, D. P. (2013). On the use of
Hyde, C. (2009). The clinical effectiveness of different strategies for programming generalization during
parenting programmes for children with conduct prob- functional communication training: A review of the
lems: A systematic review of randomised controlled literature. Journal of Developmental and Physical
trials. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Disabilities, 25, 5–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Health, 3(7). https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-­2000-­3-­7 s10882-­012-­9311-­3
Drew, A., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Slonims, Feldman, M.  A., Atkinson, L., Foti-Gervais, L., &
V., Wheelwright, S., Swettenham, J., Berry, B., & Condillac, R. (2004). Formal versus informal inter-
Charman, T. (2002). A pilot randomised control trial ventions for challenging behaviour in persons with
of a parent training intervention for pre-school chil- intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual
dren with autism: Preliminary findings and method- Disability Research, 48(1), 60–68. https://doi.
ological challenges. European Child and Adolescent org/10.1111/j.1365-­2788.2004.00578.x
Psychiatry, 11(6), 266–272. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Ferguson, J., Craig, E.  A., & Dounavi, K. (2019).
s00787-­002-­0299-­6 Telehealth as a model for providing behaviour ana-
Ducharme, J.  M., & Drain, T.  L. (2004). Errorless lytic interventions to individuals with autism spectrum
academic compliance training: Improving gen- disorder: A systematic review. Journal of Autism and
eralized cooperation with parental requests Developmental Disorders, 49(2) Springer US. https://
in children with autism. Journal of the doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­018-­3724-­5
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Franzone, E. (2009). Overview of functional com-
Psychiatry, 43(2), 163–171. https://doi. munication training (FCT). National Professional
org/10.1097/00004583-­2 00402000-­0 0011 Development Center on Autism Spectrum Disorders,
Duifhuis, E. A., den Boer, J. C., Doornbos, A., Buitelaar, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin.
J. K., Oosterling, I. J., & Klip, H. (2017). The effect Funderburk, B.  W., & Eyberg, S. (2011). Parent–child
of pivotal response treatment in children with autism interaction therapy. In History of psychother-
spectrum disorders: A non-randomized study with apy: Continuity and change (2nd ed.). https://doi.
a blinded outcome measure. Journal of Autism and org/10.1037/12353-­021
Developmental Disorders, 47(2), 231–242. https://doi. Funderburk, B. W., Eyberg, S. M., Newcomb, K., McNeil,
org/10.1007/s10803-­016-­2916-­0 C.  B., Hembree-Kigin, T., & Capage, L. (1998).
Dunlap, G., & Fox, L. (1999). A demonstration of behav- Parent-child interaction therapy with behavior prob-
ioral support for young children with autism. Journal lem children: Maintenance of treatment effects in the
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 1(2), 77–87. school setting. Child and Family Behavior Therapy,
https://doi.org/10.1177/109830079900100202 17–38. https://doi.org/10.1300/J019v20n02_02
Edwards, G. (2018). Behavioral parent training in children Gadow, K.  D., Devincent, C.  J., Pomeroy, J., &
with autism: Feasibility and effectiveness of a group Azizian, A. (2004). Psychiatric symptoms in pre-
310 S. Licona et al.

school children with PDD and clinic and compari- ioural telemedicine for rural, Native American popu-
son samples. Journal of Autism and Developmental lations in New Mexico. Journal of Telemedicine and
Disorders, 34, 379–393. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:J Telecare, 22(1), 47–55. https://doi.org/10.1177/13576
ADD.0000037415.21458.93 33X15587171
García, R. F., & Velasco, L. A. (2014). Parent-child inter- Huete, J. M., & Kurtz, P. F. (2010). Therapist effects on
action therapy (PCIT). Papeles Del Psicologo. https:// functional analysis outcomes with young children.
doi.org/10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp115 Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(3), 804–
Gengoux, G.  W., Schapp, S., Burton, S., Ardel, C.  M., 810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.02.005
Libove, R.  A., Baldi, G., Berquist, K.  L., Phillips, Hume, K., Bellin, S., & Pratt, C. (2005). The usage and
J.  M., & Hardan, A.  Y. (2019). Effects of a parent-­ perceived outcomes of early intervention and early
implemented developmental reciprocity treat- childhood programs for young children with autism
ment program for children with autism spectrum spectrum disorder. Topics in Early Childhood Special
disorder. Autism, 23(3), 713–725. https://doi. Education, 25(4), 195–207. https://doi.org/10.1177/02
org/10.1177/1362361318775538 711214050250040101
Gerow, S., Hagan-Burke, S., Rispoli, M., Gregori, E., Ingersoll, B., & Dvortcsak, A. (2010). Teaching social
Mason, R., & Ninci, J. (2018). A systematic review communication: A practitioner’s guide to parent train-
of parent-implemented functional communication ing for children with autism. The Guilford Press.
­training for children with ASD. Behavior Modification. Ingersoll, B., & Berger, N. I. (2015). Parent engagement
https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517740872 with a telehealth-based parent-mediated intervention
Ginn, N.  C., Clionsky, L.  N., Eyberg, S.  M., Warner-­ program for children with autism spectrum disor-
Metzger, C., & Abner, J.  P. (2017). Child-directed ders: Predictors of program use and parent outcomes.
interaction training for young children with autism Journal of Medical Internet Research, 17(10). https://
spectrum disorders: Parent and child outcomes. doi.org/10.2196/jmir.4913
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Ingersoll, B., & Gergans, S. (2007). The effect of a
46(1), 101–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.20 parent-­implemented imitation intervention on sponta-
15.1015135 neous imitation skills in young children with autism.
Greenslade, K.  J., Utter, E.  A., & Landa, R.  J. (2019). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(2), 163–
Predictors of pragmatic communication in school-­ 175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2006.02.004
age siblings of children with ASD and low-risk Ingersoll, B., & Wainer, A. (2013). Initial efficacy of proj-
controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental ect ImPACT: A parent-mediated social communication
Disorders, 49(4), 1352–1365. https://doi.org/10.1007/ intervention for young children with ASD. Journal of
s10803-­018-­3837-­x Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(12), 2943–
Hanley, G., Iwata, B., & McCord, B. (2003). Functional 2952. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­013-­1840-­9
analysis of problem behavior: A review. Journal of Ingersoll, B., Wainer, A. L., Berger, N. I., Pickard, K. E., &
Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. https://doi. Bonter, N. (2016). Comparison of a self-directed and
org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-­147 therapist-assisted telehealth parent-mediated interven-
Hardan, A. Y., Gengoux, G. W., Berquist, K. L., Libove, tion for children with ASD: A pilot RCT. Journal of
R.  A., Ardel, C.  M., Phillips, J., Frazier, T.  W., & Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(7), 2275–
Minjarez, M. B. (2015). A randomized controlled trial 2284. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­016-­2755-­z
of Pivotal Response Treatment Group for parents of Jennett, P.  A., Hall, L.  A., Hailey, D., Ohinmaa, A.,
children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Anderson, C., Thomas, R., Young, B., Lorenzetti,
Psychiatry, 56(8), 884–892. https://doi.org/10.1111/ D., & Scott, R.  E. (2003). The socio-economic
jcpp.12354 impact of telehealth: A systematic review. Journal of
Harris, S.  L. (1984). The family of the autis- Telemedicine and Telecare, 9(6), 311–320. https://doi.
tic child: A behavioral-systems view. Clinical org/10.1258/135763303771005207
Psychology Review, 4(3), 227–239. https://doi. Johnsson, G., Lincoln, M., Bundy, A., & Costley, D.
org/10.1016/0272-­7358(84)90001-­1 (2016). A systematic review of technology-delivered
Hartley, S.  L., Sikora, D.  M., & McCoy, R. (2008). disability training and support for service providers:
Prevalence and risk factors of maladaptive behaviour Implications for rural and remote communities. Review
in young children with autistic disorder. Journal of Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3(4),
Intellectual Disability Research, 52(10), 819–829. 387–398. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-­016-­0091-­z
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-­2788.2008.01065.x Kaat, A.  J., & Lecavalier, L. (2013). Disruptive behav-
Heitzman-Powell, L. S., Buzhardt, J., Rusinko, L. C., & ior disorders in children and adolescents with autism
Miller, T. M. (2014). Formative evaluation of an ABA spectrum disorders: A review of the prevalence, pre-
outreach training program for parents of children with sentation, and treatment. Research in Autism Spectrum
autism in remote areas. Focus on Autism and Other Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.08.012
Developmental Disabilities, 29(1), 23–38. https://doi. Kaiser, A.  P., Cai, X., Hancock, T.  B., & Foster, E.  M.
org/10.1177/1088357613504992 (2002). Teacher-reported behavior problems and
Horn, B. P., Barragan, G. N., Fore, C., & Bonham, C. A. language delays in boys and girls enrolled in head
(2016). A cost comparison of travel models and behav-
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 311

start. Behavioral Disorders, 28(1), 23–39. https://doi. Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M. M., Gayle, J. O., Chase,
org/10.1177/019874290202800103 G. A., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Social language use
Kanne, S.  M., & Bishop, S.  L. (2021). Editorial per- in parents of autistic individuals. Psychological
spective: The autism waitlist crisis and remembering Medicine, 22(1), 245–254. https://doi.org/10.1017/
what families need. Journal of Child Psychology and S0033291700032918
Psychiatry, 62(2). https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13254 Law, G.  C., Neihart, M., & Dutt, A. (2018). The use of
Karst, J. S., & van Hecke, A. V. (2012). Parent and fam- behavior modeling training in a mobile app parent train-
ily impact of autism spectrum disorders: A review and ing program to improve functional communication of
proposed model for intervention evaluation. Clinical young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism,
Child and Family Psychology Review, 15(3), 247–277. 22(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316683887
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-­012-­0119-­6 Lecavalier, L. (2006). Behavioral and emotional problems
Kim, S. H., Thurm, A., Shumway, S., & Lord, C. (2013). in young people with pervasive developmental disor-
Multisite study of new autism diagnostic interview-­ ders: Relative prevalence, effects of subject character-
revised (ADI-R) Algorithms for toddlers and young istics, and empirical classification. Journal of Autism
preschoolers. Journal of Autism and Developmental and Developmental Disorders, 36, 1101–1114. https://
Disorders, 43(7), 1527–1538. https://doi.org/10.1007/ doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0147-­5
s10803-­012-­1696-­4 Lerman, D.  C., O’Brien, M.  J., Neely, L., Call, N.  A.,
Knutsen, J., Wolfe, A., Burke, B.  L., Hepburn, S., Tsami, L., Schieltz, K.  M., Berg, W.  K., Graber, J.,
Lindgren, S., & Coury, D. (2016). A systematic review Huang, P., Kopelman, T., & Cooper-Brown, L.  J.
of telemedicine in autism spectrum disorders. Review (2020). Remote coaching of caregivers via tele-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3(4), health: Challenges and potential solutions. Journal of
330–344. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-­016-­0086-­9 Behavioral Education, 29(2). https://doi.org/10.1007/
Koegel, L. K. (2000). Interventions to facilitate communi- s10864-­020-­09378-­2
cation in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Lesack, R., Bearss, K., Celano, M., & Sharp, W. G. (2014).
Disorders, 30, 383–391. https://doi.org/10.102 Parent-child interaction therapy and autism spec-
3/A:1005539220932 trum disorder: Adaptations with a child with severe
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, developmental delays. Clinical Practice in Pediatric
C.  M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: Psychology, 2(1), 68–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Overview of approach. Journal of the Association for cpp0000047
Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24(3), 174–185. Lindgren, S., Wacker, D., Suess, A., Schieltz, K., Pelzel,
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Hurley, C., & Frea, W. D. K., Kopelman, T., Lee, J., Romani, P., & Waldron, D.
(1992). Improving social skills and disruptive behav- (2016). Telehealth and autism: Treating challenging
ior in children with autism through self-management. behavior at lower cost. Pediatrics, 137(Supplement).
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(2), 341–353. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-­2851O
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­341 Lindgren, S., Wacker, D., Schieltz, K., Suess, A., Pelzel,
Koegel, R.  L., Schreibman, L., Good, A., Cerniglia, L., K., Kopelman, T., Lee, J., Romani, P., & O’Brien, M.
Murphy, C., & Koegel, L.  K. (1989). How to teach (2020). A randomized controlled trial of functional
pivotal behaviors to children with autism: A training communication training via telehealth for young chil-
manual. University of California Press. dren with autism Spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
Koegel, R.  L., Koegel, L.  K., & Surratt, A. (1992). and Developmental Disorders, 50(12). https://doi.
Language intervention and disruptive behavior in pre- org/10.1007/s10803-­020-­04451-­1
school children with autism. Journal of Autism and Lord, C., & McGee, J. P. (2001). Educating children with
Developmental Disorders, 22, 141–153. https://doi. autism. Committee on Educational Interventions for
org/10.1007/BF01058147 Children with Autism. Division of Behavioural and
Koegel, R.  L., Bimbela, A., & Schreibman, L. (1996). Social Sciences and Education.
Collateral effects of parent training on family interac- Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor-
tions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, mal educational and intellectual functioning in
26(3), 347–359. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172479 young autistic children. Journal of Consulting
Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., & Brookman, L. I. (2003). and Clinical Psychology, 55(1). https://doi.
Empirically supported pivotal response interventions org/10.1037/0022-­006X.55.1.3
for children with autism. In Evidence-based psycho- Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S.
therapies for children and adolescents (pp. 341–357). (1973). Some generalization and follow-up measures
The Guilford Press. on autistic children in behavior therapy. Journal of
Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., Vernon, T. W., & Brookman-­ Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(1), 131–165. https://doi.
Frazee, L.  I. (2010). Empirically supported pivotal org/10.1901/jaba.1973.6-­131
response treatment for children with autism spectrum Machalicek, W., Lequia, J., Pinkelman, S., Knowles,
disorders. In Evidence-based psychotherapies for C., Raulston, T., Davis, T., & Alresheed, F. (2016).
children and adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 327–344). The Behavioral telehealth consultation with families of
Guilford Press. children with autism spectrum disorder. Behavioral
312 S. Licona et al.

Interventions, 31(3), 223–250. https://doi.org/10.1002/ program for parents of children with autism. Journal of
bin.1450 Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(1), 92–101.
Maglione, M. A., Gans, D., Das, L., Timbie, J., & Kasari, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­010-­1027-­6
C. (2012). Nonmedical interventions for children with Moes, D.  R., & Frea, W.  D. (2000). Using family con-
ASD: Recommended guidelines and further research text to inform intervention planning for the treat-
needs. Pediatrics, 130(2). https://doi.org/10.1542/ ment of a child with autism. Journal of Positive
peds.2012-­0900O Behavior Interventions, 2(1), 40–46. https://doi.
Mahoney, G., Kaiser, A., Girolametto, L., Macdonald, org/10.1177/109830070000200106
J., Robinson, C., Safford, P., & Spiker, D. (1999). Moes, D.  R., & Frea, W.  D. (2002). Contextualized
Parent education in early intervention: A call behavioral support in early intervention for children
for a renewed focus. Topics in Early Childhood with autism and their families. Journal of Autism and
Special Education, 19(3), 131–140. https://doi. Developmental Disorders, 32, 519–533. https://doi.
org/10.1177/027112149901900301 org/10.1023/A:1021298729297
Mahoney, G., & Wiggers, B. (2007). The role of par- Muharib, R., & Wood, C.  L. (2018). Evaluation of the
ents in early intervention: Implications for social empirical support of functional communication
work. Children and Schools, 29(1), 7–15. https://doi. training for children with autism spectrum disor-
org/10.1093/cs/29.1.7 ders. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental
Mancil, G. R., & Boman, M. (2010). Functional commu- Disorders, 5, 360–369. https://doi.org/10.1007/
nication training in the classroom: A guide for success. s40489-­018-­0145-­5
Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education Murphy, M.  A., & Ruble, L.  A. (2012). A comparative
for Children and Youth, 54(4), 238–246. https://doi. study of rurality and urbanicity on access to and sat-
org/10.1080/10459881003745195 isfaction with services for children with autism spec-
Mancil, G.  R., Conroy, M.  A., Nakao, T., & Alter, P.  J. trum disorders. Rural Special Education Quarterly,
(2006). Functional communication training in the nat- 31(3). https://doi.org/10.1177/875687051203100302
ural environment: A pilot investigation with a young Neely, L., Rispoli, M., Gerow, S., Hong, E. R., & Hagan-­
child with autism spectrum disorder. Education and Burke, S. (2017). Fidelity outcomes for autism-focused
Treatment of Children, 29(4), 615–633. http://www. interventionists coached via telepractice: A system-
jstor.org/stable/42900556 atic literature review. Journal of Developmental and
Masse, J. J., McNeil, C. B., Wagner, S., & Quetsch, L. B. Physical Disabilities, 29(6). https://doi.org/10.1007/
(2016). Examining the efficacy of parent–child inter- s10882-­017-­9550-­4
action therapy with children on the autism spectrum. Nefdt, N., Koegel, R., Singer, G., & Gerber, M. (2010).
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 2508–2525. The use of a self-directed learning program to pro-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-­016-­0424-­7 vide introductory training in pivotal response treat-
Matson, M. L., Mahan, S., & Matson, J. L. (2009). Parent ment to parents of children with autism. Journal of
training: A review of methods for children with autism Positive Behavior Interventions, 12(1). https://doi.
spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum org/10.1177/1098300709334796
Disorders, 3(4), 868–875. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Oliver, C., Petty, J., Ruddick, L., & Bacarese-Hamilton,
rasd.2009.02.003 M. (2012). The association between repetitive, self-­
McConnell, D., & Savage, A. (2015). Stress and resilience injurious and aggressive behavior in children with
among families caring for children with intellectual severe intellectual disability. Journal of Autism
disability: Expanding the research agenda. Current and Developmental Disorders, 42(6). https://doi.
Developmental Disorders Reports, 2(2), 100–109. org/10.1007/s10803-­011-­1320-­z
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-­015-­0040-­z Oono, I.  P., Honey, E.  J., & McConachie, H. (2013).
McCurdy, K., & Daro, D. (2001). Parent involvement Parent-mediated early intervention for young children
in family support programs: An integrated theory. with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Evidence-­
Family Relations: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Based Child Health: A Cochrane Review Journal,
Applied Family Studies, 50(2), 113–121. https://doi. 8(6). https://doi.org/10.1002/ebch.1952
org/10.1111/j.1741-­3729.2001.00113.x Park, C.  J., Yelland, G.  W., Taffe, J.  R., & Gray, K.  M.
McLennan, J. D., Huculak, S., & Sheehan, D. (2008). Brief (2012). Brief report: The relationship between language
report: Pilot investigation of service receipt by young skills, adaptive behavior, and emotional and behavior
children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of problems in pre-schoolers with autism. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(6). https:// Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2761–2766.
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­007-­0535-­5 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­012-­1534-­8
Mello, M., Goldman, S., Urbano, R., & Hodapp, R. Parsons, D., Cordier, R., Vaz, S., & Lee, H.  C. (2017).
(2016). Services for children with autism spectrum Parent-mediated intervention training delivered
disorder: Comparing rural and non-rural communities. remotely for children with autism spectrum disor-
Education and Training in Autism and Developmental der living outside of urban areas: Systematic review.
Disabilities, 51(4), 355–365. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 19(8). https://
Minjarez, M.  B., Williams, S.  E., Mercier, E.  M., & doi.org/10.2196/jmir.6651
Hardan, A. Y. (2011). Pivotal response group treatment
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 313

Pickard, K. E., Wainer, A. L., Bailey, K. M., & Ingersoll, Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/
B.  R. (2016). A mixed-method evaluation of the bs.irrdd.2016.05.0044
feasibility and acceptability of a telehealth-based Rogers, S.  J., Estes, A., Lord, C., Vismara, L.,
parent-mediated intervention for children with Winter, J., Fitzpatrick, A., Guo, M., & Dawson, G.
autism spectrum disorder. Autism. https://doi. (2012). Effects of a brief early start Denver model
org/10.1177/1362361315614496 (ESDM)-based parent intervention on toddlers at
Pickard, K., Rowless, S., & Ingersoll, B. (2017). risk for autism spectrum disorders: A randomized
Understanding the impact of adaptations to a controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy
parent-mediated intervention on parents’ rat- of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(10), 1052–
ings of perceived barriers, program attributes, and 1065. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.003
intent to use. Autism, 23(2), 338–349. https://doi. Rogers, S. J., Hayden, D., Hepburn, S., Charlifue-Smith,
org/10.1177/1362361317744078 R., Hall, T., & Hayes, A. (2006). Teaching young
Postorino, V., Sharp, W. G., McCracken, C. E., Bearss, K., nonverbal children with autism useful speech: A
Burrell, T.  L., Evans, A.  N., & Scahill, L. (2017). A pilot study of the Denver model and PROMPT inter-
systematic review and meta-analysis of parent train- ventions. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ing for disruptive behavior in children with autism Disorders, 36(8), 1007–1024. https://doi.org/10.1007/
spectrum disorder. In Clinical child and family psy- s10803-­006-­0142-­x
chology review. (Vol. 20, Issue 4, pp.  391–402). Ryberg, K. (2015). Evidence for the implementation of
Springer New  York LLC. https://doi.org/10.1007/ the early start Denver model for young children with
s10567-­017-­0237-­2 autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American
Randolph, J. K., Stichter, J. P., Schmidt, C. T., & O’Connor, Psychiatric Nurses Association, 21, 327–337. https://
K. V. (2011). Fidelity and effectiveness of PRT imple- doi.org/10.1177/1078390315608165
mented by caregivers without college degrees. Focus on Salomone, E., & Maurizio Arduino, G. (2017). Parental
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 26(4), attitudes to a telehealth parent coaching intervention
230–238. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357611421503 for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Telemedicine
Reese, R. J., Slone, N. C., Soares, N., & Sprang, R. (2012). and Telecare, 23(3). https://doi.org/10.1177/13576
Telehealth for underserved families: An evidence-­ 33X16642067
based parenting program. Psychological Services, Scahill, L., Bearss, K., Lecavalier, L., Smith, T.,
9(3), 320–322. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026193 Swiezy, N., Aman, M.  G., Sukhodolsky, D.  G.,
Remington, B., Hastings, R.  P., Kovshoff, H., Degli McCracken, C., Minshawi, N., Turner, K., Levato,
Espinosa, F., Jahr, E., Brown, T., Alsford, P., Lemaic, L., Saulnier, C., Dziura, J., & Johnson, C. (2016).
M., & Ward, N. (2007). Early intensive behavioral Effect of parent training on adaptive behavior in
intervention: Outcomes for children with autism and children with autism spectrum disorder and dis-
their parents after two years. American Journal on ruptive behavior: Results of a randomized trial.
Mental Retardation, 112(6), 418–438. https://doi. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
org/10.1352/0895-­8017(2007)112[418:EIBIOF]2.0 Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(7), 602–609. https://doi.
.CO;2 org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.001
Ringdahl, J. E., & Sellers, J. A. (2000). The effects of dif- Schieltz, K.  M., Romani, P.  W., Wacker, D.  P., Suess,
ferent adults as therapists during functional analyses. A.  N., Huang, P., Berg, W.  K., Lindgren, S.  D., &
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(2), 247–250. Kopelman, T. G. (2018). Single-case analysis to deter-
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­247 mine reasons for failure of behavioral treatment via
Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). Early start Denver telehealth. Remedial and Special Education, 39(2),
model for Young children with autism: Promoting lan- 95–105. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932517743791
guage, learning, and engagement. The Guilford Press. Schopler, E., & Reichler, R. J. (1971). Parents as cothera-
Rogers, S.  J., Estes, A., Vismara, L., Munson, J., pists in the treatment of psychotic children. Journal
Zierhut, C., Greenson, J., Dawson, G., Rocha, M., of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1(1), 87–102.
Sugar, C., Senturk, D., Whelan, F., & Talbott, M. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537746
(2019). Enhancing low-intensity coaching in parent Schreibman, L., Dawson, G., Stahmer, A. C., Landa, R.,
implemented early start Denver model intervention Rogers, S. J., McGee, G. G., Kasari, C., Ingersoll, B.,
for early autism: A randomized comparison treat- Kaiser, A. P., Bruinsma, Y., McNerney, E., Wetherby,
ment trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental A., & Halladay, A. (2015). Naturalistic developmental
Disorders, 49(2), 632–646. https://doi.org/10.1007/ behavioral interventions: Empirically validated treat-
s10803-­018-­3740-­5 ments for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
Rogers, S.  J., & Talbott, M.  R. (2016). Early identifica- and Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2411–2428.
tion and early treatment of autism spectrum disorder. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2407-­8
In R. M. Hodapp & D. J. Fidler (Eds.), International Schwichtenberg, A. J., & Poehlmann, J. (2007). Applied
review of research in developmental disabilities behaviour analysis: Does intervention intensity relate
(International review of research in developmental to family stressors and maternal well-being? Journal
disabilities: Fifty years of research in intellectual and of Intellectual Disability Research, 51(8), 598–605.
developmental disabilities) (Vol. 50, pp.  233–275). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-­2788.2006.00940.x
314 S. Licona et al.

Scudder, A. T., Wong, C. B., Mendoza-Burcham, M., & A., & Warren, Z. (2019). Measuring the service
Handen, B. (2018). Summary of lessons learned from system impact of a novel telediagnostic ser-
two studies: An open clinical trial and a randomized vice program for young children with autism
controlled trial of PCIT and young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 23(4), 1051–1056.
spectrum disorders. In C. B. McNeil, L. B. Quetsch, https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318787797
& C. M. Anderson (Eds.), Handbook of parent-child Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin,
interaction therapy for children on the autism spec- K.  L., Nowell, S.  W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey,
trum (pp. 443–456). Springer International Publishing. S., Mcintyre, N.  S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­3-­030-­03213-­5_24 M. N. (2020). Evidence-based practices for children,
Scudder, A., Wong, C., Ober, N., Hoffman, M., Toscolani, youth, and young adults with autism children, youth,
J., & Handen, B.  L. (2019). Parent–child interaction and young adults with autism evidence-based prac-
therapy (PCIT) in young children with autism spec- tices for national clearinghouse on autism evidence
trum disorder. Child and Family Behavior Therapy. and practice review team 2 evidence-based practices
https://doi.org/10.1080/07317107.2019.1659542 for children, youth, and young adults with autism
Siller, M., & Morgan, L. (2018). In M.  Siller & evidence-based practices for children, youth, and
L.  Morgan (Eds.), Handbook of parent-implemented young adults with autism spectrum disorder ©2020
interventions for very young children with autism. Evidence-Based Practices for Children, Youth, and
Springer International Publishing. https://doi. Young Adults with Autism.
org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­90994-­3 Sutherland, R., Trembath, D., & Roberts, J. (2018).
Simacek, J., Dimian, A.  F., & McComas, J.  J. (2017). Telehealth and autism: A systematic search and review
Communication intervention for young children of the literature. International Journal of Speech-­
with severe neurodevelopmental disabilities via Language Pathology, 20(3). https://doi.org/10.1080/1
telehealth. Journal of Autism and Developmental 7549507.2018.1465123
Disorders, 47(3), 744–767. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Swineford, L.  B., Thurm, A., Baird, G., Wetherby,
s10803-­016-­3006-­z A.  M., & Swedo, S. (2014). Social (pragmatic)
Sloman, K.  N., Vollmer, T.  R., Cotnoir, N.  M., Borrero, communication disorder: A research review of
C. S. W., Borrero, J. C., Samaha, A. L., Peter, C. C., this new DSM-5 diagnostic category. Journal of
& St. (2005). Descriptive analyses of caregiver repri- Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6(1), 1–8. https://doi.
mands. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. https:// org/10.1186/1866-­1955-­6-­41
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.118-­04 Symon, J. B. (2001). Parent education for autism: Issues
Solomon, M., Ono, M., Timmer, S., & Goodlin-Jones, in providing services at a distance. Journal of Positive
B. (2008). The effectiveness of parent-child interac- Behavior Interventions, 3(3), 160–174. https://doi.
tion therapy for families of children on the autism org/10.1177/109830070100300304
spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Tarver, J., Palmer, M., Webb, S., Scott, S., Slonims, V.,
Disorders, 38, 1767–1776. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Simonoff, E., & Charman, T. (2019). Child and par-
s10803-­008-­0567-­5 ent outcomes following parent interventions for
Stadnick, N.  A., Stahmer, A., & Brookman-Frazee, L. child emotional and behavioral problems in autism
(2015). Preliminary effectiveness of project ImPACT: spectrum disorders: A systematic review and meta-­
A parent-mediated intervention for children with analysis. Autism, 23(7), 1630–1644. https://doi.
autism spectrum disorder delivered in a commu- org/10.1177/1362361319830042
nity program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Taylor, T. K., Webster-Stratton, C., Feil, E. G., Broadbent,
Disorders, 45(7), 2092–2104. https://doi.org/10.1007/ B., Widdop, C.  S., & Severson, H.  H. (2008).
s10803-­015-­2376-­y Computer-based intervention with coaching: An
Stahmer, A.  C. (1995). Teaching symbolic play skills to example using the incredible years program. Cognitive
children with autism using Pivotal Response Training. Behaviour Therapy, 37(4), 233–246. https://doi.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, org/10.1080/16506070802364511
25(2), 123–141. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02178500 Thomson, R.  N., & Carlson, J.  S. (2017). A pilot study
Stahmer, A.  C., & Gist, K. (2001). The effects of an of a self-administered parent training intervention for
accelerated parent education program on technique building preschoolers’ social–emotional competence.
mastery and child outcome. Journal of Positive Early Childhood Education Journal, 45(3), 419–426.
Behavior Interventions, 3(2), 75–82. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-­016-­0798-­6
org/10.1177/109830070100300203 Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional
Stahmer, A.  C., Rieth, S.  R., Dickson, K.  S., Feder, J., communication training: A review and practical guide.
Burgeson, M., Searcy, K., & Brookman-Frazee, Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23. https://doi.
L. (2019). Project ImPACT for toddlers: Pilot out- org/10.1007/bf03391716
comes of a community adaptation of an intervention Tomlinson, S.  R. L., Gore, N., & McGill, P. (2018).
for autism risk. Autism, 24(3), 617–632. https://doi. Training individuals to implement applied behavior
org/10.1177/1362361319878080 analytic procedures via telehealth: A systematic review
Stainbrook, J.  A., Weitlauf, A.  S., Juárez, A.  P., of the literature. Journal of Behavioral Education,
Taylor, J. L., Hine, J., Broderick, N., Nicholson, 27(2). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­018-­9292-­0
16  Parent Implementation Interventions 315

Unholz-Bowden, E., McComas, J.  J., McMaster, K.  L., lectual disability. Journal of Applied Research in
Girtler, S. N., Kolb, R. L., & Shipchandler, A. (2020). Intellectual Disabilities, 21(4), 351–366. https://doi.
Caregiver training via telehealth on behavioral pro- org/10.1111/j.1468-­3148.2008.00449.x
cedures: A systematic review. Journal of Behavioral Wahler, R.  G., Vigilante, V.  A., & Strand, P.  S. (2004).
Education, 29(2), 246–281. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Generalization in a child’s oppositional behav-
s10864-­020-­09381-­7 ior across home and school settings. Journal of
Urquiza, A.  J., & Timmer, S. (2012). Parent-child inter- Applied Behavior Analysis, 37(1), 43–51. https://doi.
action therapy: Enhancing parent-child relationships. org/10.1901/jaba.2004.37-­43
Psychosocial Intervention, 21(2), 145–156. https:// Wainer, A.  L., Hepburn, S., & McMahon Griffith, E.
doi.org/10.5093/in2012a16 (2016). Remembering parents in parent-mediated
Vismara, L.  A., McCormick, C.  E. B., Wagner, early intervention: An approach to examining impact
A.  L., Monlux, K., Nadhan, A., & Young, G.  S. on parents and families. Autism, 21(1), 5–17. https://
(2018). Telehealth parent training in the early doi.org/10.1177/1362361315622411
start Denver model: Results from a random- Ward-Horner, J., & Sturmey, P. (2012). Component analy-
ized controlled study. Focus on Autism and Other sis of behavior skills training in functional analysis.
Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 67–79. https://doi. Behavioral Interventions, 27(2), 75–92. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1088357616651064 org/10.1002/bin.1339
Vismara, L.  A., McCormick, C., Young, G.  S., Nadhan, Yang, Y.  J. D., Sukhodolsky, D.  G., Lei, J., Dayan, E.,
A., & Monlux, K. (2013). Preliminary findings of Pelphrey, K.  A., & Ventola, P. (2017). Distinct neu-
a telehealth approach to parent training in Autism. ral bases of disruptive behavior and autism symp-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, tom severity in boys with autism spectrum disorder.
43(12), 2953–2969. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 9(1).
s10803-­013-­1841-­8 https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-­017-­9183-­z
Vismara, L.  A., Young, G.  S., & Rogers, S.  J. (2012). Yoder, P.  J., Stone, W.  L., & Edmunds, S.  R. (2020).
Telehealth for expanding the reach of early autism Parent utilization of ImPACT intervention strategies
training to parents. Autism Research and Treatment, is a mediator of proximal then distal social com-
2012, 12. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/121878 munication outcomes in younger siblings of chil-
Voos, A.  C., Pelphrey, K.  A., & Kaiser, M.  D. (2013). dren with ASD. Autism, 25(1), 44–57. https://doi.
Autistic traits are associated with diminished neu- org/10.1177/1362361320946883
ral response to affective touch. Social Cognitive and Zhou, B., Xu, Q., Li, H., Zhang, Y., Wang, Y., Rogers, S. J.,
Affective Neuroscience, 8(4), 378–386. https://doi. & Xu, X. (2018). Effects of parent-implemented early
org/10.1093/scan/nss009 start Denver model intervention on Chinese toddlers
Waddington, H., van der Meer, L., Sigafoos, J., & with autism spectrum disorder: A non-randomized
Whitehouse, A. (2019). Examining parent use of controlled trial. Autism Research, 11(4), 654–666.
specific intervention techniques during a 12-week https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1917
training program based on the Early Start Denver Zisser, A., & Eyberg, S. M. (2010). Parent-child interac-
Model. Autism, 24(2), 484–498. https://doi. tion therapy and the treatment of disruptive behavior
org/10.1177/1362361319876495 disorders. In Evidence-based psychotherapies for
Wade, C., Llewellyn, G., & Matthews, J. (2008). Review children and adolescents (2nd ed.).
of parent training interventions for parents with intel-
Overview of the Early Start Denver
Model 17
Melissa Mello and Sally J. Rogers

17.1 Introduction systems, while maintaining a focus on the use of


everyday activities and the importance of sensi-
The Early Start Denver Model (ESDM) made its tive, responsive relationships as well as carrying
appearance in the press just over a decade ago. out the first rigorous efficacy trials. This last
Yet, its origins in clinical practice and research decade has focused on the further development of
extend back more than 35  years, and its princi- varying applications of ESDM—with infants,
ples originated in science and theory, in both with parent implementation, in groups, in low-­
domains, accumulated over the past 40  years at resource settings, among others—and many
least. Unlike most of the interventions described research studies using controlled group designs
in this text, the origins of ESDM began in the to assess outcomes.
1980s by developing practices and procedures The goal of this chapter is to provide an intro-
informed by developmental science, applying duction and theoretical orientation to the ESDM,
them to the developmental needs of young chil- followed by a clinical description of the practices
dren with autism—needs in virtually all domains used, a review of current published work, and a
of development—and monitoring learning rates dive into NDBI’s and their characteristics. A
over time. In the next decade, we sought to inte- review of its strengths and weaknesses and the
grate the principles of operant learning and posi- need for further work, both scientific and clinical,
tive behavior supports with the developmental will follow.
principles, content, and practices already estab-
lished in this naturalistic developmental behav-
ioral intervention (NDBI). 17.1.1  Origin and Theoretical Bases
In our third decade of work, we partnered with
Geraldine Dawson and her colleagues at the The ESDM is a naturalistic developmental
University of Washington to complete the inte- behavioral intervention for young children with
gration of developmental and behavioral princi- autism spectrum disorder (ASD) aged
ples, treatment techniques, and data collection 12–48  months. Aiming to target the key early
characteristics of autism—delays and differ-
ences in social communication and language,
M. Mello · S. J. Rogers (*) social interactions, joint attention, play and
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, imitation skills, problem behaviors, and adap-
University of California Davis MIND Institute,
Sacramento, CA, USA tive behavior—the intervention seeks to
e-mail: sjrogers@ucdavis.edu enhance the developmental trajectory for chil-

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 317
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_17
318 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

dren with ASD in all affected domains by 17.2 C


 linical Description of ESDM
embedding needed learning in everyday con- Practices
texts, activities, and interactions.
The ESDM is a product of the theories, 17.2.1  The ESDM Curriculum
studies, and models that came before it, par-
ticularly the Denver Model (Rogers et  al., The ESDM curriculum targets ten developmental
1986), a developmental, play- and interaction- domains, follows developmental sequences in
based model emphasizing both developmental each, and aims to accelerate development in
growth and, particularly, the triad of difficul- those domains in which children show delays.
ties involving imitation, joint attention, and Embedded in the curriculum are communicative
symbolic play (Rogers & Pennington, 1991); tools for expressing needs, emotions, desires, and
the Social Motivation Hypothesis of autism interests. Developmental skills on the ESDM
(Dawson et  al., 2004; Mundy, 1995); and the curriculum checklist are grouped into the follow-
applied behavior intervention Pivotal Response ing domains: social skills, cognitive skills, play
Training (Koegel et  al., 1999; Schreibman skills, gross motor skills, fine motor skills, imita-
et al., 1991). tion, self-care skills, joint attention, expressive
These models each contributed a specific communication, and receptive communication.
aspect to the intervention techniques and foci The ESDM curriculum checklist allows for the
that define the ESDM.  They share the general development of individually customed treatment
premise that early social learning is uniquely objectives by evaluating the child’s current skill-
impaired in ASD and that, without specific set and knowledge via play-based activities and
interventions to support the development of interactions during everyday routines. Once writ-
social learning, social communication, and ten, objectives are broken down into 5–6 small
social interest and enjoyment, a specific learn- steps, beginning with the child’s current level of
ing profile develops in children with ASD that performance and ending with the objective at its
appears to prioritize non-social experiences mastery level. New learning goals are written at
over social experiences—one that loses malle- 12-week intervals based on family priorities and
ability as children move beyond the infant, tod- developmental needs based on the ESDM
dler, and preschool years. Development in the curriculum.
domains of social learning and language tend to These steps focus the therapist on practicing
happen particularly slowly, creating a gap that the child’s current level of performance and the
hinders most young ASD children’s participa- next levels of mature performance, arranged hier-
tion in the social-communicative domain. This archically in small steps that lead to the mastery
social communication delay, in turn, hinders of the learning objective, with mastery defined as
other areas of development, especially educa- a high level of performance generalized to adults,
tional progress, language and literacy progress, locations, and materials maintained via natural
peer relationships, and the flow of social infor- reinforcers—those embedded in the situation and
mation in both formal and informal interactions similar to those that support the skill in young
that teaches most young children how to fully children without ASD.  Thus, teaching consis-
participate in their communities. In order to tently occurs in what Vygotsky (1978) identified
address this cascading effect of autism symp- as the zone of proximal development, the next
toms on children’s developmental and behav- small steps in the development of a skill that
ioral trajectories, the ESDM maximizes builds from the child’s present level of mastery.
coordinated, interactive social communication, Several studies examining quite different domains
social play, and interactive daily living activi- of development have supported this concept,
ties throughout the child’s waking hours, to fill demonstrating the most rapid learning occurs at
in for past “missed” opportunities through the edge of the child’s current skills (Lifter et al.,
intensive learning opportunities. 1993; Taumoepeau & Ruffman, 2008).
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 319

17.2.2  Developmental Framework which the child and partner are engaged in a
goal-directed activity with several repetitions; (3)
In the ESDM, ASD is considered a developmen- variations, in which either partner adds variations
tal difference/disorder (i.e., a brain-based disor- to the original actions that both partners engage
der that affects early childhood development in in; and (4) closing, in which the activity is losing
virtually all domains but particularly in social its learning value and the adult and child close
communication and in repetitive action patterns). down the activity and transition to another. The
The ESDM is based on a developmental frame- multiple repetitions embedded in both the theme
work, with principles and concepts, including and the variations phases can help consolidate
adult–child relationships and the approach to lan- immediate learning into long-term memory
guage, all coming from research in developmen- (Horst, 2013; Zhan et al., 2018).
tal psychology focused both on conditions that Child initiation of novel acts and exploration
enhance learning in young children and on key of novel stimuli and novel acts on objects, sup-
developmental skills that influence the develop- ported by the child’s interest and desire to con-
ment of social communication, language, imita- tinue the activities, are key tools for self-directed
tion, play, and joint attention. For example, learning that most young children use but are less
infant/toddler learning and participation are prevalent in ASD (Jarrold et al., 1996; Pierce &
affected by the quality of relationship that exists Courchesne, 2001) and provide many learning
with the adult partner, enhanced by those who are opportunities across each activity of the child’s
sensitive and responsive to child cues and who day. Thus, the use of four-step joint activities
follow the child’s lead to objects and activities of addresses two fundamentals of toddler learning
interest (Tomasello, 1992). While typically that are affected in ASD: (1) initiative and variety
developing toddlers easily engage in back-and-­ in play and (2) social scaffolding of child learn-
forth activities with adults, allowing for practice ing within interactive exchanges (Rogers, 2016).
and imitation of the partner’s actions, children The ESDM is constructed to address the fact
with autism often need scaffolding to learn the that autism limits early childhood learning via
back and forth of dyadic exchange, hence the decreased social interest and social initiative
need for partners to take active turns rather than (Dawson et al., 2004), immature imitation skills
only follow child leads. This adult scaffolding of (Rogers et al., 2003), and delayed and infrequent
more mature performance as a critical learning joint attention acts (Carpenter et al., 2002), result-
tool is part and parcel of Vygotsky’s (1978) the- ing in delayed verbal and non-verbal communi-
ory of early learning (and well supported by the cation and immature play skills (Sigman &
studies mentioned above). Ungerer, 1984). Since most toddlers rely on these
skills to learn from others, especially in the
period before new learning is accessible through
17.2.3  Child Initiative and Learning language, these aspects of ASD limit social learn-
ing opportunities in the child’s earliest years with
ESDM teaching capitalizes on the exploratory cascading effects over time. The relative lack of
nature of toddlers and the number of novel learn- attention that 2-year-old’s with ASD pay to mark-
ing opportunities that result. The “joint activity” ers of social communication in interactions
framework of ESDM activities fosters and rein- (Chawarska et al., 2012) results in far fewer novel
forces child initiation and exploration within the social learning opportunities than typically devel-
interactive dyad in order to maximize the number oping children experience (Rogers, 2016). ESDM
of novel learning opportunities occurring during teaching techniques optimize the frequency of
activities. This framework involves four phases: social learning opportunities by their careful
(1) set-up, in which the child activity emerges planning of developmentally and age-appropriate
from the child’s initial interest and engagement motivating learning materials and activities
with materials or social games; (2), theme, in embedded in an interactive social framework,
320 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

with the goal and result that children with ASD in data-informed instruction, and skilled prompting
ESDM treatment demonstrate much less prefer- and prompt reduction strategies (scaffolding) to
ence for object stimuli over social stimuli than accelerate new learning that is also quickly gen-
those in other interventions (Dawson et al., 2012; eralized and maintained. Thus, ESDM embraces
Gale et al., 2019; Sasson & Touchstone, 2014). both developmental and behavioral approaches
and incorporates them seamlessly into learning
supports for children.
17.2.4  Integrated Approach Third, it integrates early childhood profes-
to Intervention sionals from several different disciplines into an
interdisciplinary team that follows each child
One of the most important characteristics of the and has input into the child’s intervention plan.
ESDM is its focus on merging developmental Speech pathology, behavior analysis, occupa-
domains, varying evidence-based foundations for tional therapy, psychology, early childhood or
teaching, and different areas of professional infant special education, social work, and pedi-
intervention practice into an integrated whole. It atrics play prominent roles, with input from any
merges learning in various developmental other professional needed to help with a child’s
domains by using whole activities rather than dis- particular difficulties also included. This is car-
crete trials as the basis for teaching. Young chil- ried out in one of two main ways. In some set-
dren’s activities with others typically involve tings, ESDM is delivered as part of a larger
visual motor skills, fine motor skills, gross motor pediatric or early intervention setting where
skills, cognitive skills, attention and interest, these disciplines are all part of a general clinic
information processing, and communicative/lan- team. In this case, various disciplines conduct
guage exchanges, carried out within a positive an interdisciplinary team assessment, determine
emotional valence. By using a whole activity as and discuss critical aspects of the child’s inter-
the teaching frame, and by inserting learning vention needs in terms of the domains (e.g.,
objectives in all developmental domains, the motor, communication, cognitive, behavior,
adult has the opportunity to scaffold and rein- family challenges), ending with a set of short-
force new learning in many domains within a term intervention goals that comprehensively
single activity. address the child’s needs.
Second, it integrates several foundations for The person who will be the team lead for this
teaching. It follows principles of constructivist child (see Rogers & Dawson, 2010, for a com-
development in early childhood by the use of plete description of how an intervention is orga-
evidence-based hierarchies of skill development nized and delivered in ESDM) then constructs a
characterizing typical development in its devel- set of intervention goals, broken down into small
opment of learning goals, in its use of Vygotsky’s steps, for the interventionists (parents, parapro-
(1978) zone of proximal development and Bruner fessionals, other professionals) to implement.
(1975) in terms of skill teaching and social scaf- This is signed off on by the interdisciplinary team
folding in a specific activity, and its use of members and carried out by every team member
Piagetian levels of teaching imitation and sym- delivering services to the child, regardless of
bolic play (Piaget, 1952). It follows the develop- their professional training. Progress data are
mental sciences of social communication and reviewed by the various members, and every
language development in its curriculum and 12 weeks, after the child’s recurring curriculum
embeds its language-learning techniques in all evaluation, the team lead drafts a set of new
activities (Bates et  al., 1988). The ESDM also objectives and holds a progress meeting with the
follows the principles of applied behavior analy- team to review progress and assure that the new
sis in the use of ongoing assessment, task analy- plan represents the child’s needs across all
sis, skilled use of reinforcement strategies, domains.
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 321

In other settings, the intervention team con- 17.3 Multiple Methods


sists solely of one or two professionals, and the of Delivery
other disciplines are represented by professionals
at other agencies who are also involved with the The ESDM was constructed as an intervention
child. The team lead assembles assessment data that could be delivered in any setting or any activ-
from other professionals who have been involved ity with any available materials in order to maxi-
with the child, conducts the curriculum assess- mize the amount of learning available for a child.
ment, drafts the learning goals and steps incorpo- It has also produced evidence to demonstrate the
rating recommendations made by other fidelity of implementation and positive outcomes
professionals in their reports, shares them with in varied delivery settings. Many different deliv-
the other professionals and asks for their input, ery methods have demonstrated positive results;
and delivers the intervention for 12 weeks, after these include one-to-one intervention in clinics,
which the curriculum re-assessment occurs, natural community settings, and homes, parent-­
progress data are shared with the other profes- implemented intervention at home and in other
sionals along with new learning goals and steps, community activities and settings, group inter-
and intervention continues. vention in daycare and both specialized and
inclusive or typical preschools, delivery by pub-
lic service programs in remote and low-resource
17.2.5  Interdisciplinary Team areas guided by ESDM-credentialed staff through
telehealth, 12-week models, 1-year models, and
In settings in which various disciplines are 2-year models (Fuller & Kaiser, 2020). The
all on staff, the ESDM uses a generalist, ESDM delivery approach thus fits flexibly into
transdisciplinary team approach (Rogers, the various environments, care, and educational
2016). All intervention staff, regardless of situations that serve young children across many
professional discipline, are trained to fidelity different cultures and countries including those
in ESDM and address the child’s comprehen- with resources and systems of care that differ
sive learning goals in their sessions. Thus, greatly from the higher-income countries. Its
both child and family are experiencing con- emphasis on everyday activities and materials
sistency across professionals in their treat- and naturalistic interactions allows for flexible
ment style as well as their content. Given that delivery that can be adapted to many different
the model uses an entire learning activity as contexts and frameworks with fidelity (Rogers,
the medium for teaching, and that any activ- 2016).
ity involves communication, motor perfor-
mance, cognitive performance, and behavioral
skills, the individual professional can support 17.4 ESDM Teaching Strategies
all developmental domains involved in the
activity while paying particular attention to By combining techniques from the aforemen-
the child’s needs in the area of specific exper- tioned models and theories, ESDM teaching
tise. This model protects parents from experi- strategies aim to create learning opportunities
encing differences of opinion or differing and experiences similar in kind and frequency to
advice from the various members of the those of typically developing toddlers and to fill
child’s team since any differences have in for previously “missed” social and communi-
already been worked through by the team at cative learning opportunities. Scaffolding these
various team meetings. The team lead role for typical toddler/preschool experiences allows for
a specific child may be assigned to the pro- reinforcing social learning and interaction pat-
fessional with the greatest expertise in the terns via introducing social stimuli, teaching the
child’s greatest areas of need, to guide the appropriate responses to these stimuli and mak-
team of interventionists more closely. ing the learning experience highly rewarding,
322 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

ensuring positive emotional experiences during 17.4.2  P


 lay and Everyday Activities
interactions with others, and supporting child ini- as the Frame for Learning
tiative and motivation to initiate and continue and Teaching
such activities. Various teaching strategies to
accomplish these are outlined below. The ESDM method emphasizes joint activity
routines, one of the most important vehicles of
early childhood learning (Bruner, 1975; Siegel,
17.4.1  Creating Positive Emotion 2020)—a type of play in which both participants
Inside the Learning are attending to each other and interactively
Experience engaged in shared play activities, with the more
mature partner scaffolding the participation of
Creating positive emotional states allows chil- the less mature partner. These activities involve
dren to experience pleasure in social interaction. those that would have taken place naturally in
Pleasurable social games (“sensory social rou- different circumstances.
tines [SSRs]” in ESDM terminology) are typi- In ESDM, the child has considerable control
cally accompanied by reciprocal eye contact, within joint activity routines. Carried out with
vocal exchanges, and nonverbal communication. and without objects, these activities establish
Routines involving preferred objects and activi- children as key players in the routine.
ties reinforce communicative and social learning Children’s choices are taken into consider-
targets embedded in the interactions. Repeating ation; while the adult provides a specific range
pleasurable, interactive social exchanges allows of objects available as guided by the child’s
time for children to process the social-­ learning objectives and decides the actions to
communicative learning involved. The emphasis be reinforced and some aspects of the sequence
on positive affect goes beyond behavioral rein- of the activities, it is the children’s choices of
forcement. It involves emotional engagement in or responses to objects and materials used and
pleasurable social learning activities with a to some extent the sequence of activities car-
trusted partner, which activates the social brain ried out.
structures including the neurotransmitters, syn- Joint activities are routines-based activities,
apses, and gene activation that accompanies such meaning they can occur inside all daily routines a
experiences (Kandel, 2006; Santamaria et  al., child engages in. This can be various play activi-
2019; Siegel, 2020). ties, both object-based and social, self-care rou-
The social reward system is stimulated in two tines involving meals, dressing, toileting, learning
unique ways: (1) to like and (2) to want to con- to carry out simple chores, and a wide range of
tinue it. Some children with ASD may respond activities based on parental and societal expecta-
positively to social engagement but they will not tions for children of the same age.
necessarily initiate actions to pursue it. Some Thus, the range and style of activities used in
children may respond in neither way. Some chil- ESDM treatment allow the interventionist to
dren will respond in both ways. The ESDM tar- address a child’s full range of learning objec-
gets both facets by boosting the social engagement tives, a variety of developmental and adaptive
reward value and by carefully using pauses, skills, including constructive and symbolic
anticipation, and prompting as needed to encour- play, imitation, communication (expressive and
age child responses that request repetition. receptive), social skills, cognitive skills, and
Operant learning helps establish these connec- even fine and gross motor development. This
tions by ensuring that children have to work style of teaching also allows the parent to inte-
toward the reward—it is not simply provided to grate teaching into their daily lives, as they do
them. This makes them actively learn and use not have to make monumental changes to their
communicative and social acts and reinforces the family structure in order to incorporate
behavior further. teaching.
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 323

17.4.3  Intensive Teaching tive social interactions. Since the first 5 years of
children’s lives are especially sensitive periods
The social orienting/social motivation hypothesis for language learning (Friedman & Rusou, 2015;
suggests that one of the main reasons for delayed Kuhl, 2000), it stands to reason that those with
development in early autism is due to the number autism require at least the same level of interac-
of learning opportunities that young children tions and experiences as children without devel-
with autism miss due to the differences in social opmental delays in order to develop language,
orienting and social attention, and due to their social communication, and other skills that are
low rates of social initiation and responses to oth- dependent on social interaction for learning.
ers; efforts to engage-characteristic of most
young children with ASD (Dawson et al., 2004;
Mundy, 1995). To fill in these “missed” experi- 17.4.4  Positive Approaches
ences, naturalistic teaching is embedded in every for Unwanted Behavior
social exchange. When this is carried out at fidel-
ity, children are receiving scaffolding learning Inside ESDM, positive behavior supports for
opportunities every 10–30  seconds—a rate of unwanted behavior are implemented and the pro-
teaching intensity that rivals discrete trial teach- cess for this will be discussed here. It is important
ing. In ESDM, intensive teaching refers to this to note, however, that often by following the
frequency of 1:1 scaffolding learning opportuni- ESDM teaching principles described, children
ties, whether delivered in a group, by a parent, or tend to make progress with their unwanted behav-
by a therapist of any discipline, and it is mea- ior. Many behavior issues are addressed simply
sured with the fidelity tool. Activities, materials, through the process of teaching the child’s objec-
and interactions are based on “age-appropriate” tives using the full range of ESDM techniques
experiences so that children we are treating are as including following children’s leads, reinforcing
ready as possible to interact with peers and adults their communication attempts, ensuring they
in typical settings with young children without enjoy the teaching materials and co-constructing
developmental differences. the activities with their interests, giving choices,
Research in developmental psychology has using related reinforcers, and teaching within the
shown that children with caregivers who are child’s zone of proximal development. If we do
responsive and sensitive to their children’s sig- not see changes in problem behaviors, or if the
nals and needs, use rich language, and follow behaviors prevent learning activities from occur-
children’s leads rather than directing most activi- ring, we conduct a functional analysis in multiple
ties stimulate their children’s learning. These environments to determine function and guide
characteristics have multiple impacts on develop- intervention strategies. Based on the results of
ment. Sensitive, responsive caregiving promotes the functional analysis, positive behavior sup-
secure attachment relations, which positively ports are developed.
affect social relations with peers (Bohlin et  al., For very young children, these tend to include
2000), learning (De Ruiter & Van Ijzendoorn, the use of reinforcement to teach adaptive, con-
1993), and behavior (Greenberg et  al., 1991). ventional behaviors that functionally meet the
Parents’ use of rich and varied language spoken needs of the child. Once the new target, or
in response to children rather than primarily replacement behavior, has been identified, the
directing children has large effects on children’s child is taught to use the behavior in the situa-
language learning (Ramirez et al., 2020). Parents tions that elicit the problem behavior and to use
who tend to follow their children’s lead by imitat- the replacement behavior with prompting as
ing and expanding rather than directing have needed prior to the problem behavior. The new
positive effects on language development behavior is reinforced, while the unwanted
(Girolametto et al., 1999), including secure social behavior is placed on extinction and no longer
relations, better language development, and posi- receives reinforcement.
324 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

If a behavior plan is developed, these changes terns (Bauminger et al., 2010). Family participa-
are conveyed to parents, and parents are taught to tion in the planning and implementation of the
use the strategies at home. The behavior plan is child’s intervention plan in their natural environ-
not “taught” separately from teaching within ment throughout the course of a normal day is an
activities during sessions. We teach in every envi- inherent aspect of ESDM.  According to
ronment and the behavior plan is embedded in Brookman-Frazee (2004), active parental
the teaching activities carried out by staff and involvement in ASD intervention provides par-
everyday activities that parents carry out with ents with feelings of empowerment, motivates
their children. Both the replacement behavior and parents to use intervention strategies to aid their
the unwanted behavior are tracked and monitored children’s development, and decreases depres-
closely to ensure progress. Instead of focusing on sion; thus, facilitating early action is imperative
suppression of unwanted behavior like aggres- for children with ASD.  Finally, a randomized
sive, disruptive, and repetitive behavior via pun- controlled trial (Rogers et  al., 2012) demon-
ishment techniques or time out, we use behavior strated that parent coaching using ESDM signifi-
substitution or replacement, prompting children cantly enhanced parental alliances with their
to use a behavior that is as easy, as fast, and as team leads.
efficient as was the unwanted behavior for attain- In ESDM, parents share leadership roles with
ing their goals. We select replacement behaviors the team lead and also share intervention roles
already in the child’s repertoire in some nascent with others, because their provision of high-­
form, behaviors that are age-appropriate for the quality learning activities during the times that
child, culturally acceptable, and readily under- interventionists are not present is crucial for the
stood by others. We prompt the child to use the goal of engaging children in learning opportuni-
replacement behavior to attain their goals (we ties throughout their waking hours. Parents have
assure that they do!), especially intentional com- separate ongoing parent coaching sessions with
munication, and increase the use of age-­ their team lead and learn to deliver ESDM with
appropriate behaviors for expressing wants, fidelity and manage child behavior according to
needs, and negative emotions. We also prompt behavior plans within their everyday activities at
the replacement behavior earlier in the identified home and in community locations with their
chain than the problem behavior would occur child. Although parents of children with ASD
which constitutes a proactive, antecedent tend to interact with their children in the same
approach. way that those with typically developing or chil-
dren with other types of delays do, the response
of children with ASD differs by initiating interac-
17.4.5  Family Involvement tions with less frequency (Chiang et  al., 2008;
Kasari et al., 1988; Kasari & Sigman, 1997).
Autism interventions for young children often Therefore, despite parents interacting with
incorporate family involvement and consider this their children in typical ways, children tend to
to be an important focus of intervention given the not sustain these interactions or initiate them at
large amount of time young children are with the level of frequency seen in other developmen-
their families in the early years, as well as the tally matched groups of children (Kasari &
privileged position that parents have in terms of Sigman, 1997), resulting in fewer interactions,
young children’s attention and responsivity. less communication, and fewer opportunities for
Studies have demonstrated that secure attach- children to learn. In addition to reducing child
ment relations with parents are present in young learning opportunities, this reduces opportunities
children with autism in early childhood (Capps for parents to respond sensitively to their chil-
et al., 1994; Oppenhein et al., 2009; Rogers et al., dren’s cues as well as reducing the reinforcement
1991), as well as in older children with ASD, for to parents that come from successful parent–child
whom attachment security affects friendship pat- exchanges.
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 325

As described throughout this chapter, the sion tree walks sequentially through several
ESDM addresses these characteristics of early steps: (1) adding reinforcer strength, (2) adding
ASD by providing many supported opportunities more structure, and finally (3) use of visual com-
for child initiations and contingent responses, munication systems. While many may question
and by supporting and shaping natural gestures this placement of visuals as the last adaptation to
and sounds into conventional communications make, ESDM prioritizes spoken language and
recognizable by parents that, in turn, allow them natural gestures over pictorial representations of
to respond contingently and sensitively, building language for two reasons. First, developmental
and reinforcing all child communicative efforts. research has clearly demonstrated that it takes
Parents experience the success of their interac- typically developing young children a long time
tions and gain confidence in themselves and in to grasp the representational value of pictures—
their children as a result. well into age 3 (DeLoache, 1991; DeLoache &
Burns, 1994). Second, the benefit of the choices
we have made to prioritize children’s use of and
17.4.6  W
 hen Children Receiving understanding of speech is clearly represented in
ESDM Are Not Making Rapid our replicated outcome data, in which language is
Progress the area of a greatest positive effect of ESDM
(Dawson et al., 2010; Rogers et al., 2019a, b).
Even when ESDM is delivered intensively and
with appropriate fidelity, some children do not 17.4.6.1 Increasing Reinforcer
progress quickly. Individual variability in autism Strength
has been seen in all aspects: cognitive, language, The ESDM typically relies on reinforcers that are
temperament, attention, and activity level intrinsic to the preferred activity or materials and
(Wozniak et  al., 2017). Just as individuals with in the careful arrangement of tasks that surround
autism experience the world in different ways, a preferred activity. When an activity does not
they learn in different ways as well. Thus, teach- have an intrinsic reinforcer, for example, self-­
ing strategies need to consider individual differ- help skills, external reinforcers are added and
ences. A child who is not progressing is not one used to motivate and support learning. We often
who cannot progress, but one who is not being begin with Premack’s principle—placing the
taught in their learning style (Vivanti et al., 2017), activity to be carried out just before a highly pre-
and his or her learning program must be reorga- ferred activity so that the flow of activities con-
nized to fit the child’s needs. tains the reinforcers. As changes are made, data is
During ESDM delivery, data are taken at monitored to assess progress and if the data do
15-minute intervals based on behaviors and not show rapid progress, additional changes to
objectives that have occurred within specific reinforcer strength are made by moving up a hier-
activities. Data sheets are designed to reflect archy and continuing to make adjustments to
progress on objectives involving all areas of reinforcer value, moving next to unrelated toys,
development, with a separate data sheet for spe- tokens, and social reinforcers, then to non-social
cific behavior plans. If a child is making consis- toys (electronics) and perhaps to edibles. Social
tent progress, seen within 1  week of data, the reinforcers are always included to assist with the
approach continues as per the basic model eventual fading of the extrinsic reinforcers. If
(Rogers & Dawson, 2010). new reinforcers are put in place, the data are fol-
Should the data show a child not making prog- lowed for the next 2 weeks to assess progress. If
ress on one or more objectives quickly, the super- there was no problem with reinforcer strength, or
visor consults a decision tree to guide how to if the new reinforcers are not leading to the
optimize the basic teaching approach of the desired progress, the next step is to add additional
objective in question for a particular child. Using structure to learning approaches for the objective
a flowchart involving yes/no questions, the deci- under examination.
326 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

17.4.6.2 Increasing Structure agreed upon by experts, establishing an interven-


and Repetitions tion’s efficacy requires strong evidence that an
When modifying teaching structure and repeti- intervention demonstrates greater benefit than
tions, the ESDM therapist continues to maintain standard care. While intervention studies are
ESDM fidelity, even with the changes made. Child plentiful, many do not demonstrate this level of
choice, child preferred materials and activities, methodological rigor. All the papers chosen for
and social communication objectives in every review here involve well-controlled trials, either
activity are all maintained even while structure and by matched groups or in randomized controlled
repetitions are added. Increasing structure and rep- trials. Dawson et  al. (2010) conducted the first
etitions generally begins with having the child randomized controlled trial of ESDM using a
seated at a table, rather than on the floor or in vari- randomized design that compared the interven-
ous places of the child’s choosing, in order to add tion against community treatment. Additional
some physical boundaries and support for atten- methodological strengths included high retention
tion. The next step would involve additional teach- rates, the use of naive examiners, and measures
ing trials, first distributed within the activity, but if of fidelity of implementation. Participants
needed, also carried out with more tasks that received either community intervention or ESDM
require massed practice. This structuring process delivered 1:1 for a planned 20  hours per week
may also include a reduction in the play variations and were regularly assessed to measure progress.
that typically occur within the joint activity. The results from this study demonstrated ESDM-­
specific benefits in the areas of language, cogni-
17.4.6.3 Visual Supports tion, and adaptive behavior in relation to the
The final level of change made to the program is to comparison group.
add visual supports. In this phase, visual anteced- A follow-up study published by Estes and col-
ents, visual schedules, timers, picture schedules, leagues in 2015 where the children were a mean
and Picture Exchange Communication System age of 6  years demonstrated continued ESDM
(PECS) can all be implemented to support the advantages in core autism symptoms and adap-
child’s learning. There is no hierarchy, and any tive skills in communication, daily living skills,
mode of evidence-based visual communications and social skills compared to the community
that could support the child’s learning can be cho- treatment group. In addition, both groups main-
sen, including video modeling, play scripts, tactile tained the rates of development they had achieved
or kinesthetic information, work baskets, picture during the intervention period, even though the
or word symbols, work schedules, and more. ESDM group received significantly less treat-
To summarize, in each of these three domains, ment than the community group in the 2  years
the decision tree moves from the least amount of following the end of the intervention study,
support through to significantly more support with resulting in an overall benefit per cost advantage
the expectation to see progress begin to occur of the ESDM intervention over community inter-
quickly, within a week. Whatever alterations are vention (Cidav et al., 2017). One outcome mea-
made for teaching as a result of this process are sure involved an examination of brain response to
maintained throughout the 12-week teaching social and non-social preferences to photos of
period, until the next curriculum evaluation occurs, women’s faces or toys (Dawson et  al., 2012).
and a new teaching plan is generated. Two comparison groups were used, the commu-
nity treatment sample and an additional sample
of typically developing agemates. Findings
17.5 Evidence Base ­demonstrated that both the typically developing
group and the ESDM-treated group responded
The ESDM is an evidence-based intervention and more strongly and more rapidly to the social ver-
has been studied in detail. While early behavioral sus the non-social photos, while the community
intervention for children with autism is widely sample demonstrated the opposite response. This
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 327

was interpreted as demonstrating preferential ori- skills continued to show gains during the 3-month
entation to people versus objects in the ESDM follow-up following the end of intervention ses-
group, and it addresses the social orienting/moti- sions. Thus, both parents showed gains during
vational hypothesis underlying the rationale for the intervention that were well-maintained for
many aspects of the ESDM that address social 3  months following the end of the intervention
motivation directly, described earlier. (Vismara et al., 2009).
A multisite, randomized, intent-to-treat repli- A randomized, intent-to-treat study of 115
cation of the Dawson et al. (2010) was published children developed from the original 2009
in 2020. The ESDM study involved 118 children P-ESDM study did not demonstrate significant
and replicated the finding of the significant ben- effects of P-ESDM coaching over community
efit of ESDM for young children in the language intervention on 1-year-olds with ASD (Rogers
domain, though the performance was not consis- et  al., 2012). Both groups of children made
tent across sites for the ESDM group (Rogers marked gains in developmental quotient (DQ)
et al., 2020). and language abilities and decreased autism
symptoms involving social affect over the
3-month period. The children in the community
17.5.1  Parent-Implemented ESDM group averaged more than double the interven-
tion hours of the P-ESDM group, who averaged
Given that parents have the most expertise on only 1.5  hours of intervention per week. While
their children and spend the greatest amount of there were no significant differences in child
time with them, they are well-positioned to take progress, there was a significant effect on parent
advantage of everyday activities and turn those stress in this brief study. By the end of the study,
activities into learning opportunities for their parents in the P-ESDM group reported signifi-
young children with autism. Furthermore, if par- cantly lower levels of parental stress than did
ents understand how to manage difficult behav- those in the community group, and the difference
iors and teach new skills, family life can be less remained significant even when controlled for the
stressful and more rewarding. In many situations, number of negative life events occurring in that
parent-implemented intervention may be the only 3-month period.
source available to the child, given parent sched- Why were there no significant group differ-
ules, intervention costs, and lack of available ences, either in parent fidelity scores or child
intervention resources in some communities progress, in this study? We hypothesized three
(Vismara et al., 2009). reasons: (1) the intensity was just too low for
Research on parent-implemented ESDM effects to show up, (2) the fact that the parents
(P-ESDM) has shown some significant gains in were just learning the intervention during the
both parents’ acquisition of ESDM technique and time that we were measuring child and parent
children’s social-communicative behavior change demonstrates that the P-ESDM children
(Vismara et al., 2009). In the initial single-subject were not getting a “full dose” of the intervention,
study of P-ESDM involving 1 hour per week of and (3) standard measures being used were not
1:1 parent coaching with their child for 12 weeks, sensitive enough to detect small changes occur-
all but one parent mastered the ESDM techniques ring over such a short period.
measured via the ESDM fidelity tool (Rogers & A similar randomized study involving 32
Dawson, 2010) at a minimum of 85% criterion by young children directed by Vismara et al. (2018)
the completion of 5–6 sessions (Vismara et  al., examined the use of the Internet as a delivery
2009), which were maintained over time both method for P-ESDM for learning materials (e.g.,
during and after the ending of intervention. videos, lesson synopses, learning modules, and a
Children in both groups increased their scores in variety of relevant information and aids). In this
verbal production and imitation markedly by the study, significantly more parents receiving
end of the study (Vismara et  al., 2009). These P-ESDM reached criteria for the fidelity of
328 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

implementation than did the community group, acceleration; although, child outcomes did not
demonstrated significantly greater fidelity of differ by group on either proximal or distal mea-
implementation skills than did the control group. sures. However, individual child rate of progress
P-ESDM parents also reported significantly was significantly related to parent fidelity scores,
greater satisfaction with their intervention than demonstrating that the key practices of ESDM
did the community group. Children in the intervention predicted the amount of child prog-
P-ESDM group showed significantly higher rates ress as measured by the proximal measure.
of imitation than did controls. Parents in both groups reported satisfaction with
Rogers et  al. (2019b) and a multi-site team the intervention.
conducted a randomized, intent-to-treat trial of
P-ESDM, with a sample of 45 children to test two
hypotheses from the Rogers et  al. 2012 study. 17.5.2  E
 SDM in Group Settings
The hypotheses concerned (1) enhancing the (G-ESDM)
intensity and parent coaching approaches within
the P-ESDM intervention and (2) testing the sen- While the most rigorous studies of ESDM have
sitivity of proximal versus distal measures for involved 1:1 intervention conducted by interven-
detecting child change. Instead of using a tionists or parents, in some cultures and contexts
treatment-­as-usual comparison group, both 1:1 intervention is not available or preferred.
groups were assigned to a P-ESDM treatment, ESDM grew out of a group model for young chil-
with one receiving an enhanced version of the dren with autism developed by Rogers and col-
training (Rogers et  al., 2019b) and the other leagues at the University of Colorado Health
receiving the standard version. Enhancements Sciences Center. Group ESDM, or G-ESDM, is a
included, first, an extra weekly session conducted current ESDM adaptation, described by Rogers
at home to help generalize techniques to the chil- and Dawson (2010) and Vivanti et  al. (2017),
dren’s everyday environment (Rogers et  al., intended for specialized, inclusive, or community
2019b). Additionally, parents were provided with childcare and early educational group settings.
different materials to target their specific learning The intervention design process follows the basic
styles (e.g., visual, audio; Rogers et al., 2019b). ESDM process of assessment using the ESDM
Lastly, parent coaching was implemented using curriculum checklist completed for each child
Motivational Interviewing techniques (Miller & every 12-weeks and the development of individu-
Rollnick, 2002), to improve parental motivation alized learning goals on all the developmental
and engagement through identifying the parent domains. An interventionist targets several of
goals and aligning treatment with them (Rogers each child’s objectives in each group activity
et al., 2019b). using brief 1:1 exchanges that involve several
The second methodological addition was the learning opportunities delivered every 30  sec-
use of a proximal measure involving a develop- onds or so while other children carry out their
mental checklist administered at several points in own play or wait for a turn. Staff to child ratio
the 6-month trial (i.e., 3 months of intervention, was 1:3 and all staff were trained to fidelity. Data
3  months of follow-up) in addition to the stan- are taken on each objective taught in each activity
dardized, distal measures of child skills adminis- and drive decisions about how to best deliver the
tered at three timepoints (i.e., pretest, end of intervention to accomplish objectives, using the
active treatment, and end of the follow-up decision tree as described previously.
period). In terms of parent change, the parents in Parents receive coaching at regular intervals.
the enhanced group demonstrated significantly Vivanti et  al.’ (2014) outcome study compared
greater gains in parent fidelity of implementation two groups of children, those receiving the spe-
skills than did parents in the non-enhanced group. cialized ASD group services made available
In terms of child progress, both interventions through the public health system and those
were associated with significant developmental receiving group-delivered ESDM carried out in a
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 329

university-based community childcare service son of ESDM intervention outcomes compared


with a specialized autism wing. Children in the to Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention (EIBI)
ESDM group attended at least 15 hours per week outcomes following the treatment manual, A
while children in the community settings typi- Work in Progress, written by Leaf and McEachin
cally attended all day, 5 days a week. At the end (1999) with the EIBI delivery trained and over-
of 1 year of intervention, the ESDM group dis- seen by McEachin. The study also tested two dif-
played greater improvement than the comparison ferent levels of treatment intensity. Planned
group on gains in their developmental quotients, delivery of 15 and 25 hours per week of 1:1 inter-
receptive language, and adaptive behavior using vention resulted in the actual delivery of 12 and
standardized measures and naive raters (Vivanti 22  hours per week. Eighty-seven children from
et al., 2014). The performance had increased in three sites were randomized into one of four
the domains of visual perception, receptive lan- cells: (1) ESDM 15 hours, (2) ESDM 25 hours,
guage, and expressive language. Communication (3) EIBI 15  hours, and (4) EIBI 25  hours. All
skills and gross motor skills had also improved, cells received 12 months of intervention at home
and parents’ questionnaire responses indicated or other care setting from highly trained interven-
reduced autism-specific features. Although the tionists working under the supervision of a pro-
results were not as drastic as those found by fessional supervisor. All families received
Dawson et al., 2010, this was not surprising since monthly coaching twice from their team lead. We
earlier intervention is more beneficial for chil- predicted that the severity of children’s develop-
dren with ASD, and Dawson et  al.’s was con- mental and autism symptoms would moderate
ducted significantly with younger children. the effects of intensity and treatment style in four
domains: autism symptom severity, expressive
communication, receptive language, and nonver-
17.5.3  Other Research bal ability. Examiners and coders were naive to
group assignments. Data analysis revealed that
The strength and consistency of the ESDM find- neither treatment style nor treatment intensity
ings were recently documented in a careful meta-­ had overall effects on outcomes in any of the four
analysis published by Fuller et al. in 2020. The areas. Furthermore, children’s initial severity of
study included data from 12 ESDM studies, symptoms did not predict a better response to one
included data from 640 children, and involved 44 intervention compared to the other. And there
unique effect sizes. The studies included data was very little evidence that severity predicted a
from all three delivery types: one-to-one inter- better response to one intensity level compared to
vention, parent-implemented intervention, and the other. All four groups of children made simi-
group intervention. Data were obtained from lar and marked levels of progress on all four vari-
measures of IQ/DQ, language, adaptive behavior, ables with effect sizes ranging from 0.5 to 2.2.
joint attention, repetitive behaviors, and autism
severity scores. Most measures involved assessor-­
implemented measures, though parent-report 17.6 Naturalistic Developmental
measures were also used. Children who received Behavioral Interventions
ESDM made greater progress than controls in the
areas of receptive and expressive language, and The Early Start Denver Model falls into a cate-
cognition; and the overall aggregated effect size gory of autism treatment known as Naturalistic
across all 44 unique effect sizes was moderate Developmental Behavioral Interventions.
and statistically significant, indicating an overall Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral
advantage for children receiving ESDM com- Interventions, or NDBI’s, are the product of
pared to those in comparison interventions. merging the sciences of applied behavior analysis
Finally, a recent multisite intent-to-treat RCT and child development (Schreibman et al., 2015).
(Rogers et al., 2020) conducted a direct compari- Prior to the development of NDBI’s, these two
330 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

fields existed separately; however, the increasing spontaneity and made children with autism
emphasis on treating autism early in childhood dependent on prompts, which decreased their
has led to the blending of these two fields spontaneous behaviors (Schreibman et al., 2015).
(Schreibman et al., 2015). NDBI’s represent the The inclusion of new developmental theories in
integration of ABA and developmental sciences, learning aimed to target these criticisms and offer
which ideally allows for accelerated child learn- an alternative teaching approach for children
ing and more substantial gains while being par- with autism and their families (Schreibman et al.,
ticularly well-suited for the infant and toddler 2015).
population (Schreibman et al., 2015). The focus
of NDBI’s is to address the core symptoms in
autism including delays in face-to-face reciprocal 17.6.2  Developmental Perspectives
interactions, imitation, play, social communica- and Autism
tion, and joint attention (Schreibman et al., 2015).
Research on infant and child development in the
1980s and 1990s contributed to advanced models
17.6.1  Pre-NDBI of development which, in turn, contributed to
research on autism. Developmental research was
Before the 1960s, interventions for autism were especially useful since the development of com-
minimal, since it was believed that those with munication, social learning, and language are
ASD could not be treated. The early 1960s saw some of the key domains targeted when treating
this belief fade with proof of children with ASD those with ASD. As autism diagnosis and autism
learning new skills through an operant learning intervention moved downward chronologically
paradigm (Lovaas, 1987). The operant learning from 1980 to 2000, autism interventionists were
paradigm was adapted to teach autistic children beginning to be faced with children as young as
various skills such as language, social, and aca- 15–18 months old, children too immature to par-
demic skills (Schreibman et al., 2015). This also ticipate in standard DTT interventions. These
proved to be effective in reducing unwanted children are very close to the age at which the
behavior and was taught to parents so that they skills so affected by autism—reciprocal interac-
could integrate it at home. tion, joint attention, imitation, use of gestures,
In 1987, Lovaas’ work on autism treatment receptive communication, and preverbal commu-
demonstrated the benefits of early intervention nication—are just emerging in typical develop-
using applied behavior analysis. Lovaas’ meth- ment. Developmental studies of ASD
ods resulted in higher IQ scores and adjustments demonstrated that many skills thought to be com-
to mainstream schooling (Lovaas, 1987). This led pletely atypical in ASD were better characterized
parents to favor early intensive behavioral inter- as immature and that progress of children with
vention, advocate for it, and popularized discrete autism tended to follow similar patterns like
trial teaching (DTT). DTT involves breaking those with typical or delayed development—
skills down into different components and using hence the concept that a developmental approach
discrete trials to teach components one at a time to their intervention might be helpful as charac-
(Schreibman et  al., 2015). This was done until terized by studies of the Denver Model as early as
behavioral changes were seen. While DTT has 1986.
been incredibly successful in leading to powerful Because of this, autism treatment began to
behavioral changes for children with autism, cri- focus on skills like joint attention (Mundy et al.,
tiques of the model are that DTT did not allow 1990), which would then aid in the development
children to generalize skills to different environ- of other skills like social engagement, language,
ments and promoted unwanted changes such as and imitation (Rogers & Lewis, 1989). Research
avoidance challenging behavior (Schreibman also showed that children being active partici-
et al., 2015). Other critiques are that DTT reduced pants during learning led to better results than the
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 331

respondent learner approaches, since infants are of engagement with partners; and (6) learn the
hypothesis-testers who learn from testing predic- routines and regularities of their life experiences
tions on their environments (Saffran et al., 1996). (Schreibman et  al., 2015). Children are encour-
Communication and imitation were also found to aged to initiate interactions and engage in sponta-
be dependent on social relationships and learning neous behaviors, both of which are rewarded,
on affective exchange (Charman et  al., 2001). leading to the promotion of the child’s own learn-
Since those affected by ASD have deficits in ing (Schreibman et al., 2015).
social motivation and affective sharing, these Research in developmental psychology also
were incorporated into new and improved inter- examined environmental factors that promoted
ventions (Prizant et al., 2003). Lifter et al. (1993) learning, social cognition, and play, which has
concluded that atypical and typical children fol- been applied to the development of interventions
lowed similar developmental paths in many for individuals with autism (Schreibman et  al.,
domains and that those with ASD simply devel- 2015). One such example is that of a child learn-
oped at different rates, leading to an investigation ing in the context of affectively rich social inter-
of this blended form of teaching individuals with actions that involve play with both objects and
ASD. people. The same information taught in another
context, without the rich affect, lacks the same
quality of learning (Kuhl, 2007). Ratner and
17.6.3  Developmental Perspectives Bruner (1978) found that the daily routines that
and NDBI children participate in offer rich learning contexts
for young children. Not only does this ensure
Early developmental psychologists such as learning takes place in everyday life but it also
Piaget, Vygotsky, and Gibson (among others) helps with adaptive functioning. Teaching chil-
concluded that children flourish when they are dren inside their daily routines showed increased
engaged as active participants in a developmen- generalizability of their skills, addressing one of
tally appropriate learning context, and in contexts the critiques of DTT (McGee et al., 1983).
that are meaningful and relevant to the child Further efforts to improve the effectiveness of
(Schreibman et  al., 2015). Vygotsky (1978) autism treatment led to the development and
found that children most readily learn skills that incorporation of strategies to help improve the
are scaffolded by caregivers and just beyond their child’s motivation for engaging in an activity
current knowledge, therefore assessing the child’s (Schreibman et al., 2015). Some of the strategies
current abilities as well as finding skills that rep- implemented that vary from previous forms of
resent the “zone of proximal development” in treatment based on ABA include the use of rein-
every developmental domain enables success forcement that is embedded in the goal-directed
(Lifter et al., 1993). activity rather than external to it, child-led inter-
NDBIs implement a constructivist approach, actions, and treatment occurring in natural con-
meaning that children’s learning experiences are texts. Bates et al. (1988) showed that skills such
intentionally developed to (1) actively engage as gestures, shared affect, and joint attention led
children’s attention; (2) foster child initiation and typically developing children into language
maintenance of both interactions and goal-­ acquisition (see also Charman et al., 2001), and
directed activities; (3) foster flexible, varied play, Mundy et al. (1990) demonstrated that this was
to help them connect new experiences with what also true for children with autism. This led to a
they know now; (4) teach children guided by change in how language was taught to children
developmental sequences by systematically with autism. Ingersoll and Schreibman (2006)
increasing the complexity of their learning expe- used a treatment study to demonstrate the inter-­
riences; (5) allow them to construct their own related effects of changes in imitation, joint atten-
knowledge, learn language from their own topics tion, language, and pretend play in early ASD.
332 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

17.6.4  NDBIs and Autism 17.7.1  Nature of Intervention Targets

NDBIs have been shown to be successful in chil- Targets include a variety of developmental
dren with autism when children are extremely domains, including social, language, cogni-
young and have less established patterns of disrup- tion, play, and motor systems (Dawson et  al.,
tive behavior. Not only does this help with gener- 2010). NDBIs also ensure that there is an inte-
alization but it also reduces children’s dependence gration of skills across the various domains
on prompts, results in more natural language, and prioritization of generalization. This devel-
teaches language along with the meaning, and opmental system’s approach makes sure that
results in habituation to the distractions that are skills are not taught in isolation or chosen
there in daily life (Schreibman et al., 2015). without regard to the child’s current develop-
NDBIs are also inherently social since they mental accomplishments but that new skills
involve interactions with peers and adults, mak- fall within Vygotsky’s zone of proximal devel-
ing them likely to promote social development opment and are taught within everyday activi-
(Morrier et  al., 2009). Additionally, they are ties by a variety of people rather than in
family-­friendly and can be carried out by parents artificial teaching situations with artificial
in the context of daily routines, increasing the materials and instructions. Skills that form the
quantity and quality of interactions and learning foundation for learning other skills include
experiences (Schreibman et al., 2015). sharing emotions, attending to others, imitat-
The surge in popularity for NDBIs took place ing others, and understanding that meanings
just as researchers studying autism were under- are communicated via sounds, expressions,
standing the importance and the benefits of early gestures, and words. With core components
intervention in autism (Schreibman et al., 2015). established, additional skills can be learned
Learning goals and plans were chosen based on (Schreibman et al., 2015).
developmental readiness, which included both Examples of skills that aid in the acquisition
developmental and chronological age. Children of other skills include joint attention and imita-
with autism, therefore, were taught skills in tion. Joint attention refers to gestures, gaze, and
accordance with their developmental age, which language that is used to aid in sharing informa-
resulted in better generalization, maintenance, tion with others. Joint attention has proven to
and acquisition (Lifter et al., 1993). be linked to greater language skills in both
NDBIs also reduced unwanted and disruptive autistic and typically developing children
behavior in autism by focusing on development (Mundy et al., 1990). Imitation is crucial when
as a whole and considering challenging behavior it comes to social acceptance and learning since
a normal part of development in both autistic and it allows children to engage with others and
typically developing children (Schreibman et al., learn from others before they develop the abil-
2015). This was done by teaching children how ity to speak. Not only does it facilitate social
to regulate their own behavior. These interven- interaction, but it also allows children to experi-
tions also focused on grabbing a child’s attention, ence others’ states by synchronizing their expe-
which was done by making use of items and rience with others, and thus, helps in the
events that are preferred by the child. Because of development of the theory of mind (Gopnik &
this, escape- and avoidance-motivated behavior Meltzoff, 1993). Imitation is especially impor-
is reduced (Koegel et al., 1987). tant since it allows children to learn by observ-
ing others instead of experiencing things
themselves, and helps them to grasp concepts
17.7 NDBI Components such as games, language, and symbols.
Interventions designed for children with ASD
There are a variety of components that make up have proven to be effective in teaching them to
the NDBI strategies. These will be discussed imitate in a socially engaged manner (Ingersoll,
below. 2010).
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 333

17.7.2  Nature of Learning Contexts times per minute. Learning occurs inside the
three-part contingency (i.e., antecedent-response-­
Research has indicated that children’s neurobio- consequence). This creates clarity for the child
logical development is affected by their experi- around when and how to respond and comes
ences and that these experiences affect from the field of applied behavior analysis.
development as a whole (Dawson et  al., 2012). However, unlike discrete trial teaching, this triad
Meaningful social interactions aid learning is not necessarily separate from the next triad,
(Topál et  al., 2008) and allow children to learn and one often sees that the consequence of one
about their environment and the social landscape round is also the antecedent for the next.
surrounding them (Spelke et  al., 2013). NDBIs
focus on this by placing learning and engagement
within daily routines and play, which helps with 17.7.5  Manualized Practice
contingency-based skill-building. These contexts
are characterized by the activities used, the rela- While not specific to NDBIs, treatment manuals
tionship between the adult and child, and the are expected parts of treatment research, and
emotional valence of the interaction and activity maintaining fidelity to a specific model generally
(Schreibman et al., 2015). includes following the treatment manual. Having
a manual that clearly details the practices of
intervention allows for more accurate implemen-
17.7.3  Nature of Instructional tation (Durlak & DuPre, 2008). A manualized
Strategies practice also assists with training and consistency
of implementation among providers (Schreibman
Strategies used in NDBIs are both successful and et al., 2015).
ecologically valid and use simple action sequences
along with rewarding experiences to create motivat-
ing activities. A variety of strategies are used, 17.7.6  Fidelity of Implementation
including modeling, chaining, shaping, prompting,
and differential reinforcement. With these, the child Fidelity of implementation documents that the
learns to expand language and action sequences, intervention is being delivered as intended and
and the adult gradually increases the complexity of this is essential in order to achieve the effects of
the activities to further this learning. Children’s an evidence-based intervention. Fidelity of
communication and social, reciprocity, and play implementation refers to how well a treatment is
skills are expanded while motor, cognitive, and implemented as intended. Fidelity measures may
adaptive skills are scaffolded. Because the activities also serve to determine therapist competence
are child-centered and increase their motivation, (Durlak & DuPre, 2008; Gresham et al., 2000).
unwanted and disruptive behaviors are reduced and
replaced with more acceptable alternatives
(Schreibman et  al., 2015). Incidental Teaching, 17.7.7  Child-Initiated Teaching
Pivotal Response Treatment, and Reciprocal
Imitation Training are all examples of NDBIs, as is A critical difference between NDBI and DTT
the ESDM (Schreibman et al., 2015). While the dif- interventions is the principle of following the
ferent interventions vary in their individual treat- child’s lead, which is the process of allowing the
ments, they have a host of common features. child to have a choice in which activity or materi-
als they will engage in, as well as choices
throughout the interaction. When a child initiates
17.7.4  Three-Part Contingency an activity, they are more likely to engage in the
particular activity, allowing for increased learn-
NDBI’s focus on a high rate of teaching, ideally ing opportunities (Schreibman et al., 2015). Not
with learning opportunities occurring multiple only does this increase the child’s motivation, but
334 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

it also uses achievement of the goal as a positive approaches and begins to handle the blocks, the
consequence for the skill being targeted therapist may stack one block on one of the child’s
(Schreibman et al., 2015). Furthermore, language and offer one to the child. The two go back and
development studies have clearly demonstrated forth co-constructing the tower for the pleasure of
the positive role of parent speech that follows up seeing how high it will build before it falls over.
on child initiations compared to parental direc- The child’s enjoyment in the process is the natural
tive speech for language development (Hirsh-­ reinforcer for engaging reciprocally with the ther-
Pacek et al., 2015). apist in this activity as well as for the fine motor
and cognitive skills involved. No other reward is
needed. If a child is having trouble completing the
17.7.8  Environmental Arrangement task at the level the objective requires, an NDBI
therapist will simplify the task so that the child
The environmental arrangement plays an impor- has a sense of goal achievement. Also known as
tant role in supporting interactions between reinforcing attempts and loose shaping, this main-
adults and children. In NDBI’s, the natural envi- tains the child’s motivation and encourages them
ronmental setting is typically arranged to support to keep trying (Koegel et al., 1988).
the child’s attention and interaction. This usually Rather than discrete trial drilling of acquisi-
involves clearing out spaces, using furniture, tion tasks after failures, NDBI therapists alternate
rugs, and other cues and natural boundaries to one or two acquisition tasks with maintenance
support the child staying with the therapist, orga- tasks. This results in increased motivation, gener-
nizing materials so that the child is not distracted alization, decreased frustration (when a failure
by multiple objects lying around in a chaotic occurs), practicing already learned skills, and
fashion, and reducing distractions in the environ- ultimately in the acquisition of new skills
ment. Often, the arrangement can be such that (Schreibman et al., 2015). Since varying degrees
preferred items are in sight, but some are out of of complexity also occur in learning with typi-
reach to encourage the child to interact in order to cally developing children, this keeps interactions
gain access to the preferred object or activity. as close to natural as possible (Koegel et  al.,
Controlling access to preferred materials or 1988).
blocking child access encourages the child to ini-
tiate interactions to obtain the materials and
allows for the presentation of multiple communi- 17.7.10  Balanced Turns Within
cation learning opportunities. Object or Social Play
Routines

17.7.9  Natural Reinforcement Known as reciprocal interactions, shared control,


and Related Methods and turn-taking, this serves to reinforce social
for Enhancing Motivation reciprocity, develop nonverbal communication,
and give adults opportunities to control access to
Natural reinforcement contrasts with external the materials used and opportunities to model a
reinforcement and refers to reinforcement that is target skill. The back-and-forth structure used is
related to the child’s goal. Instead of having a commonplace in early learning which is why it is
child complete a task and receive an unrelated often incorporated in NDBI’s (Harrist & Waugh,
reward, this procedure uses natural reinforce- 2002). The idea of balanced interactions has to do
ment/intrinsic rewards. For example, instead of with who is initiating and who is responding.
telling a child to stack the blocks and then provid- This idea that the “leader” of an interactive round
ing a token, a break, or an edible when complete, should be the child as often as the adult leads to
the NDBI interventionist may place a box of balanced interactions and supports child
blocks near the child, and when the child learning.
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 335

17.7.11  A
 dult Imitation of Child 17.8 Limitations and Future
and Modeling Directions of the ESDM

Known as reciprocal imitation, mirroring, and The ESDM is in use around the world, and the
contingent imitation, this strategy aims to number of certified therapists and trainers grows
increase the child’s responsivity to the adult daily. It has been examined in upwards of 30 or
and to continue the interaction. Children with more studies, authored by a wide range of scien-
ASD have been shown to pay more attention to tists, and carried out in many nations using sim-
adults when being imitated (Dawson & Adams, ple and complex designs. We have reviewed the
1984). Domains such as imitation, language; findings from the controlled, methodologically
cognitive, motor skills; social, play, and self- rigorous studies here and the intervention is con-
care skills can all be taught using modeling. tinuously being shaped and tweaked to address
Modeling, as it is used here, refers to children new findings both from our own studies and from
learning via imitation from the adult demon- other developmental, behavioral, and treatment
stration of behavior, and is an essential compo- findings.
nent of learning in early childhood. Skills While the ESDM has been implemented in
targeted during modeling are chosen carefully different countries and community settings, there
with the child’s developmental progress in remains much to learn. Future research should
mind. also include subsets of autistic children so as to
determine whether some treatments are more
effective than others for different children and
17.7.12  Broadening Attentional families (Zwaigenbaum et  al., 2015). Further
Focus research is required to learn how to adapt the
model to best meet the needs of different cultures
Children with autism have been known to have worldwide. For example, research should exam-
deficits in attention and are prone to the stimu- ine sociocultural beliefs, cultural factors, and
lus over selectivity (Lovaas et  al., 1971). economic capability since this can greatly affect
Stimulus over selectivity is when a child’s intervention outcomes. Lack of access is often a
behavior is either under a limited range of factor of race, low SES (Liptak et  al., 2008),
stimuli or irrelevant stimuli. Overly restricted insurance (Wang et  al., 2013), living in non-­
attention hinders learning and is affected by metropolitan areas (Thomas et al., 2007), paren-
over selectivity (Ploog, 2010; Reed et  al., tal advocacy, and limited resources, such as
2013). Research by Reed et al. (2013) demon- shortage of professionals (Murphy & Ruble,
strated that typically developing children with 2012).
a mental age of under 36 months also have dif- Not only are lower SES families less likely to
ficulty with over selectivity and that it is likely have access to interventions but they may also
a developmental delay rather than an autism- have different cultural views that hinder develop-
specific impairment. It is not unique to ASD, ment. Families with children with ASD experi-
and attentional flexibility can be supported ence more barriers to interventions than do
and addressed in an intervention (Koegel & families involved with other disabilities (Vohra
Schreibman, 1977). NDBIs target this by using et al., 2014). This can be due to misunderstand-
multiple stimuli that are varied, which helps ings, translation errors in case of poor language
with generalization and thus broadens atten- abilities, or insufficient training for at-home
tional focus. This is also taught in natural interventions (Zwaigenbaum et  al., 2015).
environments for easier adaptation (Dawson Culturally appropriate program materials are
et al., 2012). needed for all families, as are trained local ser-
336 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

vice providers. We need to continue our studies References


investigating how to adapt autism treatment to
best meet the needs of ethnically and culturally Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. S. (1988). From first
words to grammar: Individual differences and disso-
diverse families and how to best educate and ciable mechanisms. Cambridge University Press.
guide providers who do not fit the Western-­ Bauminger, N., Solomon, M., & Rogers, S.  J. (2010).
culture professional mold (Zwaigenbaum et  al., Predicting friendship quality in autism spectrum dis-
2015). orders and typical development. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 40(6), 751–761. https://doi.
Additionally, we need to be extremely sensi- org/10.1007/s10803-­009-­0928-­8
tive to the number of burdens and stressors shoul- Bohlin, G., Hagekull, B., & Rydell, A. (2000).
dered by underrepresented families, economically Attachment and social functioning: A longitudinal
stressed families, families stressed by physical study from infancy to middle childhood. Attachment
and Social Functioning, 9(1), 24–39. https://doi.
and mental illnesses and multiple adverse life org/10.1111/1467-­9507.00109
events. Assuring that we are listening deeply to Brookman-Frazee, L. (2004). Using parent/clinician
family needs and requests and ensuring we are partnerships in parent education programs for chil-
doing our best to meet them rather than imposing dren with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior
Interventions, 6(4), 195–213. https://doi.org/10.1177
our own agenda is a necessary aspect of working /10983007040060040201
with families. It is critical to assure that providing Bruner, J.  S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts.
“help” for families does not add yet another layer Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19. https://doi.
of burden, another experience of failure, or org/10.1017/S030500090000086
Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). Attachment
another experience at the hands of insensitive security in children with autism. Development
professionals. The best intervention for a child is and Psychopathology, 6(2), 249–261. https://doi.
one that works for the child’s family needs. org/10.1017/S0954579400004569
Future ESDM research needs to focus much Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002).
Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young
more on adaptations that make the intervention children with autism. Journal of Developmental
feasible for a very wide range of families. The Disorders, 32(2), 91–106. https://doi.org/10.102
outcomes research that we have published has 3/A:1014836521114
involved a mid-SES American group. Our inter- Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird,
G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2001). Testing joint atten-
national colleagues will help us understand the tion, ­imitation, and play as infancy precursors to lan-
effects of ESDM in differing cultures and the guage and theory of mind. Cognitive Development,
adaptations needed. Since parental involvement 15(4), 481–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/
is key in the ESDM and other early interventions, S0885-­2014(01)00037-­5
Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2012). Context
research should take into account factors or char- modulates attention to social scenes in tod-
acteristics of the family that affect treatment dlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology
fidelity, intensity, and results. This includes fam- and Psychiatry, 53(8), 903–913. https://doi.
ily stressors, parental involvement, cultural dif- org/10.1111/j.1469-­7610.2012.02538.x
Chiang, C., Soong, W., Lin, T., & Rogers, S.  J. (2008).
ferences, and the quality of intervention, among Nonverbal communication skills in young children
others. with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
All of these directions will help us to under- Disorders, 38, 1898–1906. https://doi.org/10.1007/
stand the effects of ESDM on a wide range of s10803-­008-­0586-­2
Cidav, Z., Munson, J., Estes, A., Dawson, G., Rogers,
children and families, and will also help us S.  J., & Mandell., D. (2017). Cost offset associated
improve the intervention’s efficacy, lower its with Early Start Denver Model for children with
cost, simplify it, reduce needed training time, and autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child
increase our competence as therapists and scien- and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(9), 777–783. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.06.007
tists as we continue to provide children with the Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and social
widest and best developing array of early child- responsiveness in autistic children. Journal of
hood knowledge and skills possible. Abnormal Child Psychology, 12(2), 209–225. https://
doi.org/10.1007/BF00910664
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 337

Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 521–523.
Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00031368
social attention impairments in autism: Social ori- Greenberg, M., Speltz, M., DeKlyen, M., & Endriga, M.
enting, joint attention, and attention to distress. (1991). Attachment security in preschoolers with and
Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283. https:// without externalizing behavior problems: A replica-
doi.org/10.1037/0012-­1649.40.2.271 tion. Development and Psychopathology, 3(4), 413–
Dawson, G., Rogers, S. J., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, 430. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007604
J., Greenson, J., Donaldson, A., & Varley, J. (2010). Gresham, F. M., MacMillan, D. L., Beebe-Frankenberger,
Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for M.  E., & Bocian, K.  M. (2000). Treatment integrity
toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. in learning disabilities intervention research: Do
Pediatrics, 125(1), e17–e23. https://doi.org/10.1542/ we really know how treatments are implemented?
peds.2009-­0958 Learning Disabilities Treatment and Practice, 15(4),
Dawson, G., Jones, E.  J. H., Merkle, K., Venema, K., 198–205. https://doi.org/10.1207/SLDRP1504_4
Lowy, R., Faja, S., Kamara, D., Murias, M., Greenson, Harrist, A. W., & Waugh, R. M. (2002). Dyadic synchrony:
J., Winter, J., Smith, M., Rogers, S.  J., & Webb, S. Its structure and function in children’s development.
(2012). Early behavioral intervention is associated Developmental Review, 22(4), 555–592. https://doi.
with normalized brain activity in young children org/10.1016/S0273-­2297(02)00500-­2
with autism. Journal of the American Academy of Hirsh-Pacek, K., Adamson, L.  B., Bakeman, R., Owen,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(11), 1150–1159. M.  T., Golinkoff, R.  M., Pace, A., Yust, P.  K. S., &
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.018 Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early commu-
De Ruiter, C., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1993). Attachment nication quality to low-income children’s language
and cognition: A review of the literature. International success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1071–1083.
Journal of Educational Research, 19(6), 525–540. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615581493
DeLoache, J.  S. (1991). Symbolic functioning in very Horst, J. S. (2013). Context and repetition in word learn-
young children: Understanding of pictures and mod- ing. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 149. https://doi.
els. Child Development, 62(4), 736–752. https://doi. org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00149
org/10.2307/1131174 Ingersoll, B. (2010). Pilot randomized controlled trial of
DeLoache, J.  S., & Burns, N.  M. (1994). Early under- Reciprocal Imitation Training for teaching elicited and
standing of the representational function of pic- spontaneous imitation to children with autism. Journal
tures. Cognition, 52(2), 83–110. https://doi. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(9), 1154–
org/10.1016/0010-­0277(94)90063-­9 1160. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­010-­0966-­2
Durlak, J.  A., & DuPre, E.  P. (2008). Implementation Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2006). Teaching recip-
matters: A review of research on the influence of rocal imitation skills to young children with autism
implementation on program outcomes and the fac- using a naturalistic behavioral approach: Effects on
tors affecting implementation. American Journal of language, pretend play, and joint attention. Journal
Community Psychology, 41(3–4), 327–350. https:// of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 487–
doi.org/10.1007/s10464-­008-­9165-­0 505. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0089-­y
Friedman, N., & Rusou, D. (2015). Critical period for first Jarrold, C., Bucher, J., & Smith, P. K. (1996). Generativity
language: The crucial role of language input during deficits in pretend play on autism. Developmental
the first year. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 35, Psychology, 14(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/j,2044-­
27–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.003 835X.1996.tb00706.x
Fuller, E. A., & Kaiser, A. P. (2020). The effects of early Kandel, E.  R. (2006). In search of memory: The emer-
intervention on social communication outcomes for gence of a new science of mind. Norton.
children with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-­ Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1997). Linking parental per-
analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental ceptions to interactions in young children with autism.
Disorders, 50(5), 1683–1700. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 27(1),
s10803-­019-­03927-­z 39–57. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025869105208
Gale, C. M., Eikeseth, S., & Klintwall, L. (2019). Children Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1988).
with autism show atypical preference for non-social Caregiver interactions with autistic children. Joinral
stimuli. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 10355. https://doi. of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 45–56. https://doi.
org/10.1038/s41598-­019-­46705-­8 org/10.0007/BF00910499
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wilgs, M., & Pearce, P. S. Koegel, R. L., & Schreibman, L. (1977). Teaching autis-
(1999). The relationship between maternal language tic children to respond to simultaneous multiple cues.
measures and language development in toddlers with Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24(2), 299–
expressive vocabulary delays. American Journal of 311. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-­0965(77)90008-­x
Speech Language Pathology, 8(4), 364–374. https:// Koegel, R.  L., O’Dell, M.  C., & Koegel, L.  K. (1987).
doi.org/10.1044/1058-­0360.0804.364 A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1993). Imitation, cul- autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
tural learning and the origins of “theory of mind”. Disorders, 17(2), 187–200. https://doi.org/10.1007/
BF01495055
338 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

Koegel, R. L., O’Dell, M., & Dunlap, G. (1988). Producing Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 63–82.
speech use in nonverbal autistic children by reinforc- https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006349
ing attempts. Journal of Autism and Developmental Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longi-
Disorders, 18(4), 525–538. https://doi.org/10.1007/ tudinal study of joint attention and language devel-
BF02211871 opment in autistic children. Journal of Autism and
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128. https://doi.
C.  M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: org/10.1007/BF02206861
Overview of approach. Journal of the Association Murphy, M.  A., & Ruble, L.  A. (2012). A comparative
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24(3), 174–185. study of rurality and urbanicity on access to and satis-
https://doi.org/10.2511/rpsd.24.3.174 faction with services for children with autism spectrum
Kuhl, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. disorders. Rural Special Education Quarterly, 31(3),
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1177/875687051203100302
the United States of America, 97(22), 11850–11857. Oppenhein, D., Koren-Karie, N., Dolev, S., & Yirmiya,
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.22.11850 N. (2009). Maternal insightfulness and resolution
Kuhl, P.  K. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the of the diagnosis are associated with secure attach-
social brain? Developmental Science, 10(1), 110–120. ment in preschoolers with autism spectrum disor-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-­7687.2007.00572.x ders. Child Development, 80(2), 519–527. https://doi.
Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (Eds.). (1999). A work in prog- org/10.1111/j.1467-­8624.2009.01276.x
ress; behavior management strategies and a curricu- Piaget, J. (1952). Play, dreams, and imitation in child-
lum for intensive behavioral treatment of autism. DRI hood. Norton & Co..
Books. Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Evidence for a cer-
Lifter, K., Sulzer-Azaroff, B., Anderson, S.  R., & ebellar role in reduced exploration and stereotyped
Cowdery, E. (1993). Teaching play activities to behavior in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 49(8), 655–
preschool children with disabilities: The impor- 664. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-­3223(00)01008-­8
tance of developmental considerations. Journal Ploog, B. O. (2010). Stimulus overselectivity four decades
of Early Intervention, 17(2), 139–159. https://doi. later: A review of the literature and its implications for
org/10.1177/105381519301700206 current research in autism spectrum disorder. Journal
Liptak, G.  S., Benzoni, L.  B., Mruzek, D.  W., Nolan, of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(11), 1332–
K.  W., Thingvoll, M.  A., Wade, C.  M., & Fryer, 1349. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­010-­0990-­2
G.  E. (2008). Disparities in diagnosis and access to Prizant, B., Wetherby, A., Rubin, E., & Laurent, A. (2003).
health services for children with autism: Data from The SCERTS model. Infants & Young Children, 16(4),
the National Survey of Children’s Health. Journal of 296–316.
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 29(3), 152– Ramirez, N. F., Lytle, S. R., & Kuhl, P. K. (2020). Parent
160. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e318165c7a0 coaching increases conversational turns and advances
Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor- infant language development. Proceedings of the
mal educational and intellectual functioning in National Academy of Sciences, 117(7), 3484–3491.
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1921653117
and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi. Ratner, N., & Bruner, J. (1978). Games, social exchange
org/10.1037//0022-­006x.55.1.3 and the acquisition of language. Journal of Child
Lovaas, O.  I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm, Language, 5(3), 391–401. https://doi.org/10.1017/
R. (1971). Selective responding by autistic chil- s0305000900002063
dren to multiple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal Reed, S.  R., Stahmer, A.  C., Suhrheinrich, J., &
Psychology, 77(3), 211–222. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Schreibman, L. (2013). Stimulus overselectivity in typ-
h0031015 ical development: Implications for teaching children
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., Mason, D., & McClannahan, with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
L.  E. (1983). A modified incidental-teaching proce- Disorders, 43(6), 1249–1257. https://doi.org/10.1007/
dure for autistic youth: Acquisition and generalization s10803-­012-­1658-­x
of receptive object labels. Journal of Applied Behavior Rogers, S. (2016). Early Start Denver Model. In R.  G.
Analysis, 16(3), 329–338. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Romanczyk & J.  McEachin (Eds.), Comprehensive
jaba.1983.16-­329 models of autism spectrum disorder treatment:
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational inter- Points of divergence and convergence (pp.  45–62).
viewing: Preparing people for change (2nd ed.). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.
Guilford Press. org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­40904-­7
Morrier, M., McGee, G., & Daly, T. (2009). Effects of Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). Early Start Denver
toy selection and arrangement on the social behav- Model for young children with autism: Promoting
iors of an inclusive group of preschool-aged children. language, learning, and engagement. Guilford
Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 3(2), 157–177. Publications.
Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social emo- Rogers, S. J., & Lewis, H. (1989). An effective day treat-
tional approach behavior in children with autism. ment model for young children with pervasive devel-
opmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy
17  Overview of the Early Start Denver Model 339

of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(2), 207–214. Saffran, J.  R., Aslin, R.  N., & Newport, E.  L. (1996).
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-­198903000-­00010 Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science,
Rogers, S.  J., & Pennington, B. (1991). A theoreti- 274(5294), 1926–1928. https://doi.org/10.1126/
cal approach to the deficits in infantile autism. science.274.5294.1926
Development and Psychopathology, 3(2), 137–162. Santamaria, L., Noreika, V., Georgieva, S., Clackson, K.,
https://doi.org/10.1017/S09545794000000043 Wass, S., & Leong, V. (2019). Emotional valence mod-
Rogers, S.  J., Herbison, J.  M., Lewis, H.  C., Pantone, ulates the topology of the parent-infant inter-brain net-
J., & Reis, K. (1986). An approach for enhanc- work. NeuroImage, 116341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ing the symbolic, communicative, and interper- neuroimage.2019.116341
sonal functioning of young children with autism or Sasson, N. J., & Touchstone, E. W. (2014). Visual attention
severe emotional handicaps. Journal of the Division to competing social and object images by preschool
for Early Childhood, 10(2), 135–148. https://doi. children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
org/10.1177/105381518601000205 Autism and Developmental Disorder, 44(3), 584–592.
Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. (1991). A https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­013-­1910-­z
comparative study of attachment behavior in young Schreibman, L., Kaneko, W. M., & Koegel, R. L. (1991).
children with autism or other psychiatric disorders. Positive affect of parents of autistic children: A com-
Journal of the American Academy of Child and parison across two teaching techniques. Behavior
Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(3), 483–488. https://doi. Therapy, 22(4), 479–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/
org/10.1097/00004583-­199105000-­00021 S0005-­7894(05)80340-­5
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. Schreibman, L., Dawson, G., Stahmer, A. C., Landa, R.,
(2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism Rogers, S. J., McGee, G. G., Kasari, C., Ingersoll, B.,
and those with other developmental disorders. The Kaiser, A. P., Bruinsma, Y., McNerney, E., Wetherby,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(5), A., & Halladay, A. (2015). Naturalistic developmental
763–781. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-­7610.00162 behavioral interventions: Empirically validated treat-
Rogers, S.  J., Estes, A., Lord, C., Vismara, L., Winter, ments for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
J., Fitzpatrick, A., Guo, M., & Dawson, G. (2012). and Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2411–2428.
Effects of a brief Early Start Denver model (ESDM)- https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2407-­8
based parent intervention on toddlers at risk for Siegel, D. J. (2020). The developing mind: How relation-
autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled ships and the brain interact to shape who we are (3rd
trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and ed.). Guilford Press.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(10), 1052–1065. https:// Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and lan-
doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.003 guage skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal
Rogers, S. J., Estes, A., Vismara, L., Munson, J., Zierhut, children. Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 293–302.
C., Greenson, J., Dawson, G., Rocha, M., Sugar, https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-­1649.20.2.293
C., Senturk, D., Whelan, F., & Talbott, M. (2019a). Spelke, E. S., Bernier, E. P., & Skerry, A. E. (2013). Core
Enhancing low-intensity coaching in parent imple- social cognition. In M.  R. Banaji & S.  A. Gelman
mented Early Start Denver Model intervention (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants,
for early autism: A randomized comparison treat- children, and other species can teach us (pp. 11–16).
ment trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acpr
Disorders, 49(2), 632–646. https://doi.org/10.1007/ of:oso/9780199890712.003.0003
s10803-­018-­3740-­5 Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones
Rogers, S.  J., Estes, A., Lord, C., Munson, J., Rocha, to others’ minds: Maternal talk relates to child mental
M., Winter, J., Greenson, J., Colombi, C., Dawson, state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24,
G., Vismara, L., Sugar, C., Helleman, G., Whelan, and 33 months. Child Development, 79(2), 284–302.
F., & Talbott, M. R. (2019b). A multisite randomized https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-­8624.2007.01126.x
controlled two-phase trial of The Early Start Denver Thomas, K.  C., Ellis, A.  R., McLaurin, C., Daniels,
Model compared to treatment as usual. Journal of J., & Morrissey, J.  P. (2007). Access to care for
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent autism-related services. Journal of Autism and
Psychiatry, 58(9), 853–865. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Developmental Disorders, 37(10), 1902–1912. https://
jaac.2019.01.004 doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0323-­7
Rogers, S. J., Yoder, P., Estes, A., Warren, Z., McEachin, Tomasello, M. (1992). The social bases of language
J., Munson, J., Rocha, M., Greenson, J., Wallace, L., acquisition. Social Development, 1(1), 67–87. https://
Gardner, E., Dawson, G., Sugar, C.  A., Helleman, doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-­9507.1992.tb00135.x
G., & Whelan, F. (2020). A multisite randomized Topál, J., Gergely, G., Miklósi, A., Erdohegyi, A., &
controlled trial comparing the effects of interven- Csibra, G. (2008). Infants’ perseverative search errors
tion intensity and intervention style on outcomes of are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science
young children with autism. Journal of the American (New York, N.Y.), 321(5897), 1831–1834. https://doi.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. https:// org/10.1126/science.1161437
doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2020.06.013 Vismara, L. A., Colombi, C., & Rogers, S. J. (2009). Can
one hour per week of therapy lead to lasting changes
340 M. Mello and S. J. Rogers

in young children with autism? Autism, 13(1), 93–115. Vygotsky, L.  S. (1978). In M.  Cole, V.  John- Steiner,
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361307098516 S. Scribner, & E. Souberman (Eds.), Mind in society.
Vismara, L.  A., McCormick, C.  E. B., Wagner, Harvard University Press.
A.  L., Monlux, K., Nadhan, A., & Young, G.  S. Wang, L., Mandell, D. S., Lawer, L., Cidav, Z., & Leslie,
(2018). Telehealth parent training in the Early D.  L. (2013). Healthcare service use and costs for
Start Denver Model: Results from a random- autism spectrum disorder: A comparison between
ized controlled study. Focus on Autism and Other Medicaid and private insurance. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 67–79. https://doi. Developmental Disorders, 43(5), 1057–1064. https://
org/10.1177/1088357616651064 doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­012-­1649-­y
Vivanti, G., Paynter, J., Duncan, E., Fothergill, H., Wozniak, R.  H., Leezenbaum, N.  B., Northrup, J.  B.,
Dissanayake, C., & Rogers, S. (2014). Effectiveness West, K.  L., & Iverson, J.  M. (2017). The develop-
and feasibility of the Early Start Denver Model imple- ment of autism spectrum disorders: Variability and
mented in a group-based community childcare set- causal complexity. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews.
ting. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Cognitive Science, 8(1–2). https://doi.org/10.1002/
44(12), 3140–3153. https://doi.org/10.1007/ wcs.1426
s10803-­014-­2168-­9 Zhan, L., Guo, D., Chen, G., & Yang, J. (2018). Effects
Vivanti, G., Duncan, E., Dawson, G., & Rogers, S. of repetition learning on associative recognition over
(2017). Implementing the group-based Early time: Role of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex.
Start Denver Model for preschoolers with autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 277. https://doi.
Springer International Publishing. https://doi. org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00277
org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­49691-­7 Zwaigenbaum, L., Bauman, M., Choueiri, R., Kasari,
Vohra, R., Madhavan, S., Sambamoorthi, U., & St C., Carter, A., Granpeesheh, D., Mailloux, Z.,
Peter, C. (2014). Access to services, quality of care, Smith-Roley, S., Wagner, S., Fein, D., Pierce, K.,
and family impact for children with autism, other Buie, T., Davis, P., Newschaffer, C., Robins, D.,
developmental disabilities, and other mental health Wetherby, A., Stone, W., Yirmiya, N., Estes, A., …
conditions. Autism: The International Journal of Natowicz, M. (2015). Early intervention for chil-
Research and Practice, 18(7), 815–826. https://doi. dren with autism spectrum disorder under 3 years
org/10.1177/1362361313512902 of age: Recommendations for practice and research.
Pediatrics, 136(supplement1), S60–S81. https://doi.
org/10.1542/peds.2014-­3667E
PEAK Relational Training System
18
Mark R. Dixon, Zhihui Yi, and Amanda N. Chastain

18.1 PEAK Relational Training from an array (i.e., an array with three flashcards,
System “Apple,” “Orange,” and “Peach”) when shown an
apple (Stimulus B) and asked to put with same. In
Since the landmark study by Sidman (1971), the the second task, the fruit becomes the sample
phenomenon of stimulus equivalence has been stimulus, and the flashcard becomes the compari-
known to behavior analysts for many decades. In son stimulus. In other words, once the learner is
short, stimulus equivalence can be captured via taught A = B, the learner is able to derive B = A.
three main processes. Reflexivity, or generalized Transitivity describes the derived relation
identify matching, describes the learner’s ability between two stimuli due to their existing rela-
to match a stimulus to its identical copy without tions to other stimuli. In the example described
a direct history of reinforcement. Symmetry above, we would now present a flashcard with the
describes the ability to engage in a conditional word “Apple” (Stimulus A) and an array of three
discrimination without a history of direct rein- flashcards with the Mandarin characters of these
forcement due to existing learning history of the three fruits, “苹果,” “橙子,” and “桃子”
stimulus-stimulus relationships, where the role of (Stimulus C) and ask the learner to “put with
sample stimuli and comparison stimuli switches same.” Once reaching the mastery criteria, we
place. For example, when shown an array of an would see that now the learner can select the fruit
apple, an orange, and a peach (Stimulus B) and apple (Stimulus B) from an array when shown
presented with a flashcard with the word “Apple” the flashcard with the Mandarin character “苹果”
(Stimulus A) and the instruction, “put with same,” (Stimulus C) on it. In this example, the learner
the learner will select the apple and contact a learned that A = B and A = C. Without a direct
reinforcer. Here, the flashcard (Stimulus A) is the history of reinforcement, the learner is now able
sample stimulus, and the array of fruit (Stimulus to demonstrate C  =  B.  Instructional procedures
B) is the comparison stimulus. What symmetry that leverage on stimulus equivalence is termed
describes is that once the learner can reliably equivalence-based instruction (EBI), and its ben-
complete the task described above, without spe- efits are clear. By arranging teaching in specific
cific programing, the learner is now able to select ways, the learner will be able to derive new
the flashcard with the word “Apple” (Stimulus A) knowledge. With a three-member equivalence
class, teaching two conditional discrimination
tasks can lead to the emergence of seven new
M. R. Dixon (*) · Z. Yi · A. N. Chastain stimulus-stimulus relations. With a four-member
Department of Disability and Human Development, equivalence class, teaching three conditional
University of Illinois Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 341
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_18
342 M. R. Dixon et al.

d­ iscrimination tasks can lead to the emergence of analysis (ABA). In addition to the PEAK mod-
13 new stimulus-stimulus relations. As more and ules and assessment, a variety of supplemental
more class members are introduced into an exist- resources have been available to users of the pro-
ing relational network, the growth is exponential. gram for the past 8  years. A PEAK YouTube
Fast forward 30 years, the development in rela- channel has hundreds of demonstrations on con-
tional frame theory (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & ducting the assessments, running curriculum pro-
Roche, 2001) expands the benefit of such instruc- grams, and constructing materials. There is also a
tional paradigm, as the relation is no longer voluntary PEAK Certification program that con-
restricted by equalness. Comparison, opposition, tains three levels of distinction, ranging from
hierarchical, etc., all come together and form a exposure to a set training curriculum (Level 1),
complex relational network. The procedure demonstrated mastery of targeted competencies
through which to produce such networks is often (Level 2), and the ability to deliver and train oth-
coined as relational training. Topographically the ers on PEAK (Level 3). Supplemental assets exist
relational training procedure may resemble that on Facebook, Instagram, and other social media
of EBI, but it can also take other forms such as outlets which allow for peer-to-peer community
requiring the learner to  complete a memory support.
matching game after teaching and deriving coor- The Direct Training Module (Dixon, 2014a)
dination between two arbitrary stimuli stimuli was the first available PEAK module and empha-
(i.e., after teaching ♋ is the same is ♌, and ♌ is sized a direct contingency teaching approach
the same as ♍, asking the learning to play a heavily rooted in B. F. Skinner’s 1957 text Verbal
matching game, where ♋ and ♍ are considered Behavior. In the Direct Training Module, the
as a pair). At the same time, instead of the rela- skills which were targeted ranged from basic
tionship of sameness, other nonequivalent rela- vocal imitation, picture and word discrimina-
tionships are utilized. However, despite such tions, object selecting, and conversation skills.
a phenomenon demonstrated in behavior research These sorts of skills fall within the Skinnerian
for many years, there are limited attempts in vernacular of “verbal operants,” yet such techni-
incorporating this scientific development in clini- cal vocabulary is only lightly used within PEAK
cal practices. In this chapter, we will discuss a to expand accessibility to non-behavior analytic
comprehensive treatment model and its assess- users. More complex forms of such operants,
ment tools that are specifically designed to lever- along with a wide variety of listener skills, are
age on this form of learning, synthesizing existing contained within the Direct Training Module,
evidence, evaluating its clinical utilities, and pro- and prior research has suggested that in contrast
viding a brief review of the discourse around this to the Verbal Behavior Milestone and Placement
curriculum since its introduction. Program (VB-MAPP; Sundberg, 2008), or the
The Promoting the Emergence of Advanced Assessment of Basic Language and Learning
Knowledge, or PEAK Relational Training Skills  – Revised (ABLLS-R; Partington, 2010),
System, was released into the applied behavior which are criterion referenced to neurotypical
analytic  community in a series of four install- ages 4 and 5 years, respectively, this first PEAK
ments between the years of 2014 and 2016. module approximates a total score around neuro-
Approximately every 6 months, a new “module” typical age 8 years (Dixon et al., 2015; Malkin,
appeared which contained a brief assessment Dixon, Speelman, & Luke, 2017). A wider range
tool, data sheets, and 184 curriculum items that of skills that rely on contingency learning are
focused on a specific learning modality. In 2019, included in the Direct Training Module, such as
a standardized evaluation tool entitled the PEAK basic perspective taking skills, social skills (e.g.,
Comprehensive Assessment (PCA; Dixon, 2019) telling jokes), and more advanced verbal oper-
was added to the system, which, to date, remains ants, such as metonymical tacts.
the only standardized assessment of language The Generalization Module (Dixon, 2014b)
and cognition in the field of applied behavior expanded upon the prior installment through the
18  PEAK Relational Training System 343

utilization of a training-testing approach to inter- has shown a range of correlations between pre-­
vention whereby certain stimulus targets were assessment scores and other indexes such as
designed to be trained with prompting and error intelligence (Dixon, Belisle, & Stanley, 2018),
correction, while other similar, yet nonidentical, challenging behavior (Belisle et al., 2017), and
targets were to be concurrently probed in hopes other established assessment tools (Belisle,
of establishing generalization across stimuli, Dixon et al., 2021). A variety of outcome studies
responses, or contexts. The Generalization have documented gains for individual curriculum
Module of PEAK included an additional 184 cur- skills (Dixon, Belisle, Stanley, Speelman, et al.,
riculum items which ranged from more complex 2017; McKeel & Matas, 2017) and the compre-
forms of verbal operants, basic problem-solving, hensiveness of the entire module as proof of
mental and visual rotation tasks, short-term derived relational responding as a generalized
memory challenges, and advanced mathematics. operant (Dixon et al., 2021).
Technical language was again kept to a minimum The Transformation Module (Dixon, 2016)
to encourage usage beyond behavior analysts. was the first and remains the only tool for the
Research on the Generalization Module has dem- assessment of and intervention for deficits in
onstrated both effectiveness of the train-test relational framing (Hayes et  al., 2001). This
teaching approach to produce both new skills and PEAK module also contains a pre-assessment
the generalization of those same skills (Dixon, which is separated into two subsections for
Peach, Daar, & Penrod, 2017; Dunkel-Jackson & expressive and receptive skills. Within each,
Dixon, 2018), and there is also research suggest- there are questions pertaining to six different
ing that the module contains skills advancing to relational frame families: coordination, opposi-
around neurotypical age 11  years (Dixon, tion, distinction, comparison, hierarchy, and
Rowsey, et al., 2017). deictic. Beginning with intervention program-
The Equivalence Module (Dixon,  2015) ming that attempts to establish nonarbitrary rela-
served as a radical departure from the first two tionships among stimuli that are based on formal
PEAK modules in its exclusive reliance on characteristics and advancing to higher-order
equivalence-­based instruction (Sidman, 1971) as transformations of arbitrary stimulus functions,
the sole means of teaching new skills. Included in the PEAK Transformation Module contains a
the Equivalence Module is an expanded assess- comprehensive path to building what is termed
ment, entitled a “Pre-assessment” which utilized “arbitrary applicable relational responding”
a series of mostly arbitrary tasks to determine the (AARR; Hayes et al., 2001). Research has shown
degree of derived relational responding that a cli- that programming within this module can teach
ent would be capable of doing. Such evaluation perspective taking skills (Belisle, Dixon, et al.,
items range across basic identity matching, sym- 2016), recognizing emotions in others (Schmick,
bolic symmetric recall, and higher-level deduc- et al., 2018), establishing temporal relationships
tive tasks. An interesting characteristic of this (Barron et al., 2019), and comparative relations
assessment process was that it incorporates all (Belisle, Stanley et  al., 2020). Furthermore,
five sensory modalities, requiring the client to assessment scores appear to be correlated with
taste, smell, and touch a variety of items, as an measures of intelligence and adaptive behavior
attempt to capture derived relational responding (May & Flake, 2019).
abilities across all sensory modalities, catering to The PCA (Dixon, 2019) serves as an alterna-
the learner’s preference. The 184 curriculum tive and likely eventual replacement to the in-­
items move from an easy-to-hard hierarchy in module assessment tools that were used up until
which early items focus on simple matching to this time. This standardized tool contains a verba-
sample, while later items take the form of com- tim script, all necessary stimulus materials, scor-
plex problem-solving, often times involving mul- ing guide, administration integrity checklist, and
tiple sensory modalities and abstract stimuli. evaluation of challenging behaviors and of autism
Research on the Equivalence Module of PEAK symptomology. Therefore, such enhancements
344 M. R. Dixon et al.

make the PCA a much more comprehensive and such as standard special educational practices,
easier to use apparatus for assessment of language interventions that are primarily based on contin-
and cognition skills than prior installments in the gency learning, and those that do not incorporate
PEAK modules. Although an extremely signifi- EBI or relational training procedures. McKeel,
cant correlation exists between the PCA and the Dixon et  al. (2015) compared the effectiveness
prior assessments of PEAK (Moore et al., 2020), between traditional special education practices
approximately 67% of the items have been modi- and ABA interventions based on the PEAK cur-
fied to improve clarity, global adoptability, and riculum. Their study found that those who
reduce false positives. Additional research on the received interventions based on the PEAK cur-
PCA has also shown correlations between IQ test riculum made more gains in their language skills
scores, adaptive behavior, and autism symptomol- than those in the control group (p  =  0.005).
ogy (Sutton et  al., 2021). A higher score on the Dixon, Belisle, Stanley et al. (2018) compared 34
PCA is statistically correlated with higher IQ test students’ outcomes from three different schools,
scores, better performance during adaptative two of which were implementing PEAK-based
behavior assessments, and decreased autism interventions. The results showed that students
symptom severity (Table 18.1). from these two schools made more gains during
To date there has been considerable empirical the PEAK-DT assessment after one academic
support for the PEAK system; however, future year (p  <  0.05). May and St. Cyr (2021) also
additions to this body of work will strengthen compared the effectiveness between interven-
knowledge of the relative strengths and weak- tions based on the PEAK curriculum and stan-
nesses of the existing studies. What follows is an dard special education practices. Their results
exhaustive review of the current scientific litera- showed that, after one semester, those who
ture that pertain to PEAK. The methods used to received interventions based on the PEAK cur-
source content for this task were the same from riculum made more gains during the PEAK-PA
Dixon, Belisle, McKeel, et  al. (2017), with the (p = 0.04).
addition of all articles and book chapters pub- These studies also suggest that instructions
lished between April 2017 and April 2021. A based on PEAK can produce changes in partici-
total of 61 articles were included in our analysis. pants’ global level of functioning, as measured
The following section summarizes evidence sup- by their performance during intelligence tests,
porting the utilization of PEAK as a structured while traditional interventions focusing on con-
curriculum and as an assessment tool for lan- tingency learning and programing for generaliza-
guage and cognitive skills. tion fail to produce such results. In the study by
May and St. Cyr (2021), participants receiving
PEAK interventions showed greater improve-
18.1.1 Clinical Outcomes of the PEAK ments in their raw score on the vocal (p = 0.02)
Curriculum among Children and the information (p  =  0.04) sub-scale of the
and Adolescents Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fifth
Edition (WISC-V), as well as their full-scale IQ
To date, four studies (see Table  18.2) have (p = 0.04). The results of Dixon, Paliliunas, et al.
evaluated the effectiveness of using PEAK as a (2019) further highlight the importance of incor-
structured curriculum using group-based designs, porating instructions that promote relational
with three conducting randomized controlled tri- responding. In their study, 17 children with
als and one using a quasi-experimental design. autism were randomly placed in the traditional
Overall, their results suggest that PEAK can pro- ABA group (T-ABA) or the comprehension ABA
duce changes in specific behavior skills more group (C-ABA). Additionally, Dixon, Paliliunas,
effectively (Dixon, Belisle, Stanley et al., 2018; et al. included 11 children with autism to serve as
May & St. Cyr, 2021; McKeel, Dixon et  al., a convenient waitlist control. Participants in the
2015) than traditional evidence-based methods, T-ABA group received interventions focusing on
18  PEAK Relational Training System 345

Table 18.1  Comparison between PEAK Pre-assessment and the PCA


DT G E Tr Te
FLS 12* LLS 3 CPM 1 REF 1 COR 1 OPP 1* HIR 1* COR 10 HIR 2
FLS 13* LLS 5 CPM 2 REF 2 COR 2 OPP 2* HIR 2* COR 13 HIR 3*
FLS 14 LLS 6 CPM 4* REF 3 COR 3 OPP 3* HIR 3* COR 15 HIR 4*
FLS 15 LLS 8 CPM 8* REF 4 COR 4 OPP 4 HIR 4* COR 16 HIR 5*
FLS 16 LLS 11* CPM 11* REF 5 COR 5 OPP 5 HIR 5* COM 1 HIR 6*
PLS 1 LLS 12 CPM 12 REF 6 COR 6 OPP 6 HIR 6* COM 2 HIR 7
VCS 10 LLS 14 CPM 13* SYM 1 COR 7 OPP 7 HIR 7* COM 7 HIR 8*
VMS 3 CMS 2 CPM 14* SYM 2 COR 8 OPP 8 HIR 8* COM 9 HIR 9*
VMS 11* CMS 3* CPM 15 SYM 3 COR 9 OPP 9 HIR 9* COM 11 HIR 10*
VMS 12 CMS 6* CPM 16 SYM 4 COR 10 OPP 10 HIR 10* COM 12 HIR 11*
VMS 14 CMS 7 RPR 1 SYM 5 COR 11 OPP 11* HIR 11* COM 14* HIR 12*
VMS 16* CMS 8 RPR 2 SYM 6 COR 12 OPP 12* HIR 12* COM 15* HIR 13*
CMS 9 RPR 3 TRS 1 COR 13 OPP 13* HIR 13* COM 16 HIR 14
CMS 10 RPR 5* TRS 2 COR 14 OPP 14* HIR 14* OPP 1* HIR 15*
CMS 11 RPR 7 TRS 3 COR 15 OPP 15* HIR 15* OPP 4* HIR 16*
CMS 12* RPR 9 TRS 4 COR 16 OPP 16 HIR 16* OPP 5 DTC 1*
CMS 14* RPR 10* TRS 5 COM 1 DIS 1 DTC 1* OPP 10 DTC 2*
CMS 16 RPR 12* TRS 6 COM 2* DIS 2 DTC 2* OPP 12* DTC 3
RPR 13 EQU 1 COM 3* DIS 3 DTC 3* OPP 15 DTC 4
EQU 2 COM 4* DIS 4 DTC 4* OPP 16 DTC 5*
EQU 3 COM 5* DIS 5* DTC 5* DIS 1* DTC 6*
EQU 4 COM 6* DIS 6* DTC 6* DIS 2* DTC 7*
EQU 5 COM 7 DIS 7* DTC 7* DIS 6* DTC 8*
EQU 6 COM 8 DIS 8* DTC 8* DIS 7* DTC 9*
COM 9 DIS 9* DTC 9* DIS 8* DTC 10*
COM 10 DIS 10 DTC 10* DIS 10* DTC 11*
COM 11* DIS 11 DTC 11* DIS 14 DTC 12*
COM 12* DIS 12 DTC 12* DIS 15 DTC 13*
COM 13* DIS 13* DTC 15 DTC 14*
COM 14 DIS 14* DTC 15*
COM 15* DIS 15* DTC 16*
COM 16* DIS 16
Note: Replaced items are noted with *All other items are revised (increase the number of distractions in comparison
stimuli, change the phrasing or include additional instructions in SD to improve clarify, and correcting grammatical
errors)

contingency learning and generalization, while Together, these studies represent some of the
those in the C-ABA group received interventions recent attempts of using group-based random-
focusing on developing their skills in relational ized controlled trials in evaluating the overall
responding. The results showed that, although effectiveness of an ABA-based curriculum,
participants in both groups mastered similar which is somewhat scarce within the field of
number of programs during the 12-week period behavior analysis. These results show that
(p = 0.45), participants were able to make more interventions based on the PEAK curriculum
gains during the intelligence test (p = 0.001) by can not only successfully teach specific skills
including components of relational training but also produce large-scale changes in areas
found in the PEAK curriculum (i.e., PEAK-E and that are not directly targeted, such as perfor-
PEAK-T), compared with those who only mance in intelligence tests, which is commonly
received contingency-based instructions from the used in both educational and clinical settings
PEAK curriculum (i.e., PEAK-DT and PEAK-G). for predicting long-­ term outcomes and as a
346 M. R. Dixon et al.

Table 18.2  Clinical outcomes of the PEAK curriculum among children and adolescents
Authors Title Participants/design Results
McKeel, Evaluating the efficacy of 27 children with pervasive Participants in the treatment
Dixon et al. the PEAK relational developmental disorders (aged group made statistically more
(2015) training system using a 5–10, 25 males and 2 females) significant gains in language
randomized controlled Randomized controlled trial design, skills than those in the control
trial of children with 13 in control group (treatment as group as measured by the
autism usual per participants’ IEP), 14 in PEAK-DT assessment
treatment group (instructions based
on PEAK-DT)
Dixon, Student outcomes after 34 children with autism (aged Participants in the PEAK
Belisle, 1 year of frontline staff 5–15, 30 males and 4 females) group made statistically more
Stanley et al. implementation of the Quasi group design, 19 in PEAK gains during the PEAK-DT
(2018) PEAK curriculum group (instructions based on assessment
PEAK-DT), 15 in control group
(treatment as usual per participants’
IEP)
Dixon, Randomized controlled 28 children with autism and No statistically significant
Paliliunas trial evaluation of ABA language delay (aged 3–13, 24 differences between the
et al. (2019) content on IQ gains in males and 4 females) number of programs mastered
children with autism Randomized controlled trial design, between those in C-ABA and
8 in comprehensive ABA (C-ABA; T-ABA
interventions based on all 4 PEAK Participants in the C-ABA
modules), 9 in traditional ABA group made statistically more
(T-ABA; interventions based on significant gains in their
PEAK-DT and PRAK-G), 11 in performance during
waitlist control intelligence tests than those in
the T-ABA group and the
waitlist control
May and St. The impact of the PEAK 52 participants with autism (aged Compared with the control
Cyr (2021) curriculum on 5–20, 38 males and 14 females) group, PEAK group produced
standardized measures of Randomized controlled trial design, statistically more significant
intelligence: A systems 26 in PEAK group (interventions improvements in participants’
level randomized control based on all 4 PEAK modules), performance in PEAK-PA
trial 26 in control group (treatment as Participants in the PEAK
usual per participants’ IEP) group made more gains on the
vocal and information
sub-scales of the WISC-V test
Only participants in the
PEAK group showed
statistically significant
improvements in their IQ after
removing those demonstrating
a floor effect

proxy for overall problem-solving skills and curriculum were more effective in producing
executive functioning. Rooted in the relational these changes exceeds the scope of this chapter,
frame theory, the structure of the PEAK cur- but interested readers are suggested to explore
riculum focuses on promoting the development the relational frame theory and its implication
of relational framing, or AARR, as a high-order on human language and intelligence (e.g.,
operant via targeting specific behavior skills Cassidy et al., 2010), as well as procedures
that are of social significance to the learner. The such as multiple exemplar training (Hayes
reason why interventions based on the PEAK et al., 2001).
18  PEAK Relational Training System 347

Table 18.3  Psychometric properties of PEAK-based assessments


Authors Title Sample size Results
Dixon, Assessing the relationship 50 participants Statistically significant correlation
Whiting between intelligence and the receiving special between participants’ IQ score and
et al., (2014) PEAK relational training system education services PEAK-DT score
(aged 5–22, 46 males
and 4 females)
Dixon, Normative sample of the PEAK 206 neurotypical Established the normative data of
Belisle relational training system: Direct participants (aged the PEAK-DT assessment
et al.,(2014) training module and subsequent 1–21, 93 males and Positive correlations between age
comparisons to individuals with 113 females) and PEAK-DT score among
autism 70 participants with nonclinical sample
autism (aged 5–22, 58 Participants with autism obtained a
males and 12 females) statistically lower score than their
neurotypical peers
Dixon, PEAK relational training system 13 children with Positive correlation between
Carman et al. for children with autism and developmental PEAK-DT assessment score and
(2014) developmental disabilities: disability (aged 3–8, Peabody picture vocabulary test and
Correlations with Peabody picture 10 males and 3 the Illinois early learning standards
vocabulary test and assessment females) test
reliability
Rowsey, Principal component analysis of 98 participants with Established a four-factor model for
et al., (2015) the PEAK relational training developmental or the PEAK-DT assessment
system intellectual disability Excellent internal consistency
(aged 5–22, 81 males within each factor and among all
and 17 females) items in the PEAK-DT assessment
High interrater reliability
Dixon et al. Toward a behavior analysis of 40 participants with A strong correlation between PEAK
(2015) complex language for children autism (aged 5–21, 35 total scores (DT and G) and the
with autism: Evaluating the males and 5 females) VB-MAPP and that a logarithmic
relationship between PEAK and regression model provided a good
the VB-MAPP fit for the data
Celling for VB-MAPP was
observed with a PEAK total score
of 138
McKeel, Correlation between PEAK 27 participants with A strong correlation between the
Rowsey, relational training system and developmental PEAK-DT assessment and the
Dixon et al. one-word picture vocabulary tests disability (aged 5–22, receptive one-word picture
(2015) 23 males and 4 vocabulary test – Fourth edition
females) (ROWPVT-4) and the expressive
one-word picture vocabulary
test – Fourth edition (EOWPVT-4)
Dixon, The test-retest and interrater 39 participants with High test-retest reliability for both
Stanley, reliability of the promoting the developmental the PEAK-DT raw score and the
et al., (2016) emergence of advanced disabilities (aged 6–22, equivalent developmental age
knowledge-direct training 32 males and 7 (age-referenced score)
assessment for use with females) High interrater reliability for
individuals with autism and PEAK-DT assessment
related disabilities
Malkin et al. Evaluating the relationships 21 children with A significant correlation between
(2017) between the PEAK relational autism (aged 4–8, 18 scores on the PEAK-DT assessment
training system-direct training males and 3 females) and ABLLS-R, PEAK-DT
module, assessment of basic assessment, and VABS-II, as well as
language and Learning skills – the ABLLS-R and VABS-II
Revised, and the Vineland
adaptive behavior scales II
(continued)
348 M. R. Dixon et al.

Table 18.3 (continued)
Authors Title Sample size Results
Dixon, Normative sample of the PEAK 183 neurotypical Established the normative data for
Rowsey et al. relational training system: participants (aged the PEAK-G assessment
(2017) Generalization module with 1–21, 98 males and 85 Positive correlations between age
comparison to individuals with females) and PEAK-G score among
autism 84 participants with nonclinical sample
autism (aged 5–21, 75 Participants with autism obtained a
males and 9 females) statistically lower score than their
neurotypical peers
Belisle et al. The relationship between derived 47 participants with Participants with the ability to
(2017) mutually entailed relations and developmental engage in DRR, as measured by
the function of challenging disability (aged 5–19, PEAK-E-PA, obtained a
behavior in children with autism: 41 males and 6 significantly lower score on
Comparing the PEAK-E-PA and females) QABF. QABF was significantly less
the QABF likely to identify a single behavior
function for this group
The ability to engage in DRR did
not predict specific challenging
behavior topography
Dixon, Derived relational responding and 64 children with Significant positive correlation
Belisle, and intelligence: Assessing the developmental or between PEAK-E pre-assessment
Stanley relationship between the PEAK-E intellectual disability scores with raw IQ and full-scale IQ
(2018) pre-assessment and IQ with (aged 4–16, 54 males Significant positive correlation
individuals with autism and and 10 females) between all subtests of the PEAK-E
related disabilities pre-assessment and raw-IQ scores
Belisle The mediating effects of derived 64 children with Significant positive correlations
et al.,(2018) relational responding on the disability (59 with among PEAK-DT assessment,
relationship between verbal autism, aged 5–16, 55 PEAK-E pre-assessment, and
operant development and IQ males and 9 females) WISC-IV
PEAK-E pre-assessment score has
stronger predictive power on IQ
than PEAK-DT assessment
Ackley et al. A review of language N/A, literature review Among the 18 protocols reviewed,
(2019) development protocols for PEAK is one of the few protocols
individuals with autism that had data supporting its
psychometric properties and
effectiveness
May and PEAK pre-assessments: 18 participants (16 Statistically significant correlations
Flake (2019) Preliminary evidence establishing with autism and 2 with between PEAK-PAs with measures
internal consistency and construct AHDH, aged 3–18, 12 of intelligence and adaptive
validity males and 6 females) behavior
No significant correlations were
found between PEAK-PA and age,
autism diagnostic instruments, and
aggressive scales
Moore et al. An initial evaluation of an 16 children with For PEAK-DT module, indirect
(2019) assessment method for the PEAK autism (aged 2–8, 12 assessment showed moderate
relational system direct training males and 4 females) correlation with the pre-assessment
module result
PEAK-DT pre-assessment offers
the strongest predictor in terms of
participants’ PEAK-DT assessment
scores
(continued)
18  PEAK Relational Training System 349

Table 18.3 (continued)
Authors Title Sample size Results
Barron et al., Evaluation of the PEAK-DT and 31 children (26 with Parental reports of PEAK-DT and
(2020) PEAK-G pre-assessments: autism, aged 3–13, 26 PEAK-G are both significantly
Comparing directly implemented males and 5 females) correlated with PEAK-DT and
and indirect assessments of verbal PEAK-G pre-assessment score
abilities Indirect assessment by parents can
more reliably report deficits in
advanced skills than more basic
skills
Moore et al. Examining the convergent validity 22 children (18 with Strong correlation between PCA
(2020) between the PEAK relational autism, aged 3–13, 12 and PEAK-PA on all levels
training System’s semi-­ males and 10 females) The PCA might assess a wider
standardized and standardized range of skills compared with the
skill assessments PEAK-PA
Belisle, The convergent validity of the 23 participants with Significant correlation between
Dixon et al. PEAK-E-PA and two common autism (aged 3–22, 24 PEAK-E-PA and ABLLS-R and
(2021) assessments of language males and 9 females) TOLD 1:4
development: The ABLLS-R and
the TOLD 1: 4

erties. Dixon, Stanley, et  al. (2016) evaluated


18.1.2 Psychometric Properties of the test-retest reliability and the interrater reli-
PEAK-Based Assessments ability of the PEAK-DT assessment and reported
high test-retest reliability for both the PEAK-DT
To date, 18 studies (see Table 18.3) have exam- raw score (p < 0.001) and the equivalent devel-
ined the psychometric properties of various opmental age (age-referenced score; p < 0.001).
PEAK-based assessments, including the PEAK May and Flake (2019) also reported excellent
Directing Training Module Assessment internal consistency between all subtests of the
(PEAK-DT assessment), the PEAK PEAK-PA.  In two separate studies by Rowsey
Generalization Module Assessment (PEAK-G et al. (2015) and Rowsey et al. (2017), research-
assessment), PEAK Pre-assessments ers conducted two principle component analy-
­(PEAK-­PAs) that accompany each PEAK mod- ses for the PEAK-DT and PEAK-G assessment
ules, and the PCA. As reported by Ackley et al. and established two four-factor models. The
(2019), very few instruments developed by the normative samples of the Direct Training and
field of behavior analysis have data supporting the Generalization modules were also reported
their utility as a clinical assessment tool, which, by Dixon, Belisle, et  al. (2014) and Dixon,
to some extent, limits their external validity. The Rowsey et al. (2017), which provide additional
different PEAK-based assessment tools are some references for clinicians when deciding inter-
of the most heavily researched instruments within vention goals. At the same time, these assess-
the field of behavior analysis. Studies have ments have shown excellent convergent validity
reported favorable outcomes in areas such as the with established measures of adaptive behavior
convergent validity (Dixon et  al., 2015; Dixon, (e.g., the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale II;
Whiting, et al., 2014), factorial structure (Rowsey Malkin et  al., 2017), language assessments
et al., 2015; Rowsey et al., 2017), internal consis- based on Skinner’s account (e.g., VB-MAPP;
tency (Rowsey et  al., 2015), and test-retest and Dixon et  al., 2015), and common standardized
interrater reliability (Dixon, Stanley, et al., 2016). language assessments in clinical and educa-
As a series of semi-standardized, or in the tional settings (e.g., the Peabody Picture
PCA’s case a standardized assessment, multiple Vocabulary Test; Dixon, Carman, et  al., 2014).
studies have reported robust psychometric prop- Moreover, studies have found the obtained
350 M. R. Dixon et al.

results from PEAK-based assessments to be sig- 18.1.3 Skill Specific Gains Produced
nificantly correlated with measures of IQ, which by PEAK-based Instructions
provide additional evidence that supports RFT’s
account of human language and intelligence Twenty-six different studies (see Table  18.4)
(Belisle et al., 2018; Dixon, Belisle, & Stanley, have examined the effectiveness of PEAK-based
2018; Dixon, Whiting, et al., 2014). interventions in producing skill-specific gains
Besides the many evidence supporting these among children, teenagers, and adults with intel-
assessments as valid clinical tools, multiple lectual and developmental disabilities. These
studies have compared the validity of assess- interventions targeted a variety of skills in differ-
ment results obtained from different assess- ent contexts, such as basic and complex language
ment formats, such as the indirect assessment skills (Daar et  al., 2015; McKeel, Rowsey,
completed by caregivers (Barron et  al., 2020) Belisle, et  al., 2015), social skills in group set-
and ­comparison between PEAK-PA and indi- tings (Dixon, Blevins, et  al., 2019), academic
rect assessments (Moore et  al., 2019). The skills (Dixon, Belisle, et al., 2016; Dixon, Stanley,
overall positive outcomes indicate that clini- et  al., 2017; Stanley et  al., 2018), leisure skills
cians can choose the assessment modality that (Dixon, Speelman, et  al., 2016), creativity
best fits their unique clinical settings and still (Dixon, Belisle, Rowsey et  al., 2017), perspec-
expecting reliable outcome. Recently, the PCA tive-taking skills (Belisle, Dixon, et  al., 2016),
was introduced as the newest addition to and advanced skills that require inductive and
PEAK-based assessments. As a standardized deductive reasoning (Belisle et  al., 2019).
assessment, the PCA offers many unique ben- Together, these studies supplement the result of
efits over existing indirect assessments and group-based studies investigating the effective-
semi-standardized PEAK-PAs, as it ensures ness of PEAK as a curriculum and show that the
consistent administration across settings and instructional design found within PEAK can reli-
providers, eliminates the ambiguity in terms of ably produce significant behavior change in mul-
queries, provides uniformed manipulatives, tiple domains.
specifies the termination criterion, and allows Many of the studies also highlight the impor-
comparison across age group. Moore et  al. tance of incorporating relational training proce-
(2020) compared the results obtained from the dures in skill development. For example, Belisle,
PCA and the PEAK-PA and found that they Paliliunas et al. (2021) taught four children with
were highly correlated. Their analysis also a diagnosis of autism how to correctly engage in
showed that 67% of the items on the PCA a selection-based response when presented with a
involved some levels of revision compared 2 x 2 matrix. In the top two cells of the matrix, the
with corresponding items on the researcher presented one colored picture in each
PEAK-PA.  Among the test items that were cell. The two cells were either filled with the
replaced, around half of the items yielded the same color, the opposing color (i.e., black and
same score, while 33% of the items received a white), or different colors (i.e., black and blue).
higher score on the PEAK-PA. Together these In the lower two cells, the researcher would place
results showed a high degree of agreement one picture in one cell and ask the participant to
between the PEAK-PA and the PCA, indicat- select the correct picture that should go into the
ing that existing findings on PEAK-PAs were other cell. For example, if the two cells in the top
likely to be able to be generalized into the row were filled by the same color, and the
PCA. At the same time, the PCA seems to be researcher placed a picture of a boy in the second
able to assess a boarder range of skills that are row, the participant should select another picture
not captured by the PEAK-PA.  Practitioners of a boy, since the relation indicated by the first
should take these findings into consideration row is “same.” However, if the two cells in the
when choosing the optimal assessment top row were filled by the opposing color, the
modality. correct response would be selecting a picture of a
18  PEAK Relational Training System 351

Table 18.4  Skill-specific gains produced by PEAK-based instructions


Authors Title Participants/design Results
Daar et al. Derived emergence of WH 3 children with autism Instructions based on PEAK-E 12R
(2015) question-answers in children (aged 10–11, 1 male and successfully taught intraverbal
with autism 2 females) skills of answering wh-questions
Multiple baseline across among all three participants
participants For two of the three participants,
mastery of these relations was
functionally related to the
emergence of accurate responding
to untrained intraverbal
wh-questions
McKeel, Teaching complex verbal 3 children with autism PEAK-DT-based curriculum was
Rowsey, operants with the PEAK (aged 9–11, 2 males and effective in training complex verbal
Belisle, et al. relational training system 1 female) operants (autoclitics, metonymical
(2015) Multiple probe across tact) among all participants
skills and participants
Dixon, Derived equivalence relations 2 males with autism Instructions based on PEAK-E 5E
Belisle, et al. of geometry skills in students (aged 13 and 15) successfully taught geometry skills
(2016) with autism: An application of Concurrent multiple between both participants
the PEAK-E curriculum probe across participants Derivation of untrailed relations
occurred after mastery of one
trained relation
Belisle, Teaching foundational 3 males with autism Instructions based on PEAK-T 11 J
Dixon, et al. perspective-taking skills to (aged 12–18) successfully promoted basic
(2016) children with autism using the Multiple baseline across perspective taking skills among all
PEAK-T curriculum: Single-­ participants with participants
reversal “I–you” deictic frames embedded multiple All three participants demonstrated
probes transfer of stimulus functions to
untrailed stimuli
Two participants showed transfer
of stimulus functions to untrained
single-reversal “you” relationships
Dixon, Derived rule-following and 3 male children with Instructions based on PEAK-E
Speelman, transformations of stimulus developmental disability 12 M successfully promoted the
et al. (2016) function in a children’s game: (aged 7–9) derivation among unfamiliar
An application of PEAK-E Multiple baseline across synonymous anatomical terms
with children with participants during the game twister
developmental disabilities
Dixon, Establishing derived categorical 3 male children with Instructions based on PEAK-E 14B
Belisle, responding in children with intellectual or successfully promoted the
Stanley, disabilities using the PEAK-E developmental disability emergence of categorical
Speelman, curriculum (aged 8–9) responding
et al. (2017) Multiple probe across Two of the three participants
participants demonstrated the emergence of
additional intraverbal responding
without prior training
McKeel and Utilizing PEAK relational 3 male participants with Instructions based on PEAK-E
Matas (2017) training system to teach visual, autism (aged 23, 24, and 10 K successfully promoted the
gustatory, and auditory 69) emergence of derived relational
relations to adults with Multiple probe across responding across visual, gustatory,
developmental disabilities participants and auditory stimuli among all
participants
(continued)
352 M. R. Dixon et al.

Table 18.4 (continued)
Authors Title Participants/design Results
Dixon, Evaluating emergent naming 3 male children with Instructions based on PEAK-E 8F
Belisle, relations through developmental disability successfully promoted the
Rowsey et al. representational drawing in (aged 7–10) derivation of reflexive responding
(2017) individuals with developmental Multiple baseline with of representational drawing
disabilities using the PEAK-E embedded multiple probe following the training of tacting
curriculum across participants unfamiliar animal blends
Dixon, Establishing derived 3 male children (aged Instructions based on PEAK-E
Belisle, coordinated symmetrical and 10–11, 2 with autism, 1 10 K promoted the derivation of
Stanley et al. transitive gustatory-visual-­ with cognitive delay) reflexive and transitive stimulus-­
(2017) auditory relations in children Multiple baseline across stimulus relationships across
with autism and related stimulus class with different modalities
intellectual disabilities using embedded multiple probe
the PEAK-E curriculum
Dixon, Peach, Teaching complex verbal 3 children with autism Instructions based on PEAK-G
et al. (2017) operants to children with (aged 4–5, 2 males and 1 curriculum successfully taught and
autism and establishing female) established generalization of
generalization using the PEAK Concurrent multiple distorted tacts, autoclitic tacts, and
curriculum baseline across behavior creative path finding among all
replicated across 3 three participants
participants
Dixon, Establishing derived 2 children with autism Instructions based on PEAK-E 7E
Stanley, et al. equivalence relations of basic (one 12-year-old male promoted the derivation of
(2017) geography skills in children and one 9-year-old untrained geography skills on both
with autism female) participants
Multiple probe across Generalization between paper-map
participants and computer-map was observed
Mullen et al. Establishing auditory-tactile-­ 2 male children with Instructions based on PEAK-E 9C
(2017) visual equivalence classes in autism (aged 4 and 5) and 9 K promoted the cross-modal
children with autism and Nonconcurrent multiple derivation of untrailed stimulus-­
developmental delays baseline across stimulus relationships
participants
Stanley et al. Equivalence-based instruction 3 male participants with Three different PEAK-E programs
(2018) of academic skills: Application autism (aged 13–18) were successful in promoting
to adolescents with autism Nonconcurrent multiple academic skills among three
baseline across teenagers with autism in a school
participants with setting
embedded probe
Dunkel-­ Promoting generalized 4 male children with Instructions based on multiple
Jackson and advanced language skills of autism (aged 6–7) PEAK-G programs successfully
Dixon (2018) children in intensive behavioral Concurrent multiple increased advanced language skills
intervention with promoting the baseline across behavior that were directly taught among all
emergence of advanced replicated among all participants
knowledge generalization participants Different degrees of generalization
(PEAK-G) module were observed among three of the
four participants
Schmick et al. Teaching children with autism 3 male participants with Instructions based on PEAK-T 11H
(2018) to identify private events of disabilities (aged 13–17) successfully taught two participants
others in context Concurrent multiple to recognize other’s emotions
baseline across under different context
participants The remaining participant reached
the mastery criteria after a multiple
exemplar training procedure
(continued)
18  PEAK Relational Training System 353

Table 18.4 (continued)
Authors Title Participants/design Results
Dixon, The effectiveness of the PEAK 3 male participants with PEAK-DT-based training was
Wiggins, et al. relational training System and autism (aged 19–21) effective on improving and
(2018) corresponding changes on the Multiple baseline across maintaining an improved
VB-MAPP for young adults participants with VB-MAPP score
with autism embedded assessment
probe
Dixon, A demonstration of higher-­ 1 neurotypical female Instructions based on PEAK-G
McCord, et al. order response class (8 years old) and 1 male 10 M successfully established a
(2018) development in children with autism (13 years higher-order operant of
old) descrambling the work when
Concurrent multiple presented in isolation or when
baseline across contexts presented within a sentence
Belisle et al. Abstraction of tactile properties 2 male participants with Instructions based on PEAK-G 6F
(2019) by individuals with autism and autism (aged 14 and 16) successfully established stimulus
down syndrome using a Multiple baseline across control of abstract tactile properties
picture-based communication response Participants also demonstrated
system generalization using novel
untrained stimuli
Barron et al. Teaching “then-later” and 2 6-year-old children Instructions based on PEAK-T 9P
(2019) “here-there” relations to with autism and 10A successfully establish the
children with autism: An Multiple baseline across here-there and then-later deictic
evaluation of single reversals behavior relations
and transformation of stimulus Participants demonstrated mutually
function entailed single-reversal responding
using novel stimuli
Dixon, Teaching children with autism 3 10-year-old boys with Instructions based on PEAK-DT
Blevins, et al. extended verbal utterances autism 14Q successfully brought extended
(2019) under audience control in the Multiple baseline across verbal utterances under appropriate
context of show-and-tell participants stimulus control within the context
of show-and-tell
O’Connor Establishing multiple control 3 participants with The configuration of PEAK-E was
et al. (2020) responding of children with autism (aged 12–17, 2 able to identify and establish
autism to people and emotions males and 1 female) derived relational responding
in context by utilizing derived Multiple baseline across among two participants in their
stimulus relations participants with ability to identify someone’s
embedded multiple probe emotion under a context using a
multiple exemplar training
procedure
The multiple exemplar training
procedure was able to facilitate the
emergence of untrained relations in
the third participant
Belisle, Generalized reflexive Study 1: 2 children with Match-to-sample was able to
Huggins, et al. responding and cross-modal autism (aged 4, 1 male, 1 establish identity reflexive
(2020) tactile transfer of stimulus female), multiple responding as a generalize operant
function in children with baseline across Among the two participants with
autism participants moderate ASD intensity, such
Study 2: 2 boys with operant was able to be generalized
autism (aged 6 and 7), across modal (tactile)
multiple baseline across
participants with
embedded probes
(continued)
354 M. R. Dixon et al.

Table 18.4 (continued)
Authors Title Participants/design Results
Belisle, Teaching children with autism 3 boys with autism (aged Instructions based on PEAK-DT
Dixon, et al. to tact the private events of 5–10) 14R using a most-to-least
(2020) others Multiple baseline across prompting procedure produced the
participants correct independent responding
among three ASD children to tact
private events of other when shown
a picture
Belisle, Establishing arbitrary Study 1and 2: 2 male Instructions based on PEAK-T 12C
Stanley, et al. comparative relations and participants with autism promoted the emerged
(2020) referential transformations of (aged 14 and 19), combinatorial relation between
stimulus function in individuals multiple baseline across stimuli and the transformation of
with autism behavior with embedded stimulus function to novel stimuli
multiple probe on both participants
Dixon et al. Evidence from children with Study 1: 11 children with Instructions based one PEAK-E
(2021) autism that derived relational autism (aged 4–15, 8 promoted the development of DRR
responding is a generalized males, 3 females), mixed as a generalized operant
operant design Acquisition rate of PEAK-DT and
Study 2: 3 boys with PEAK-G programs is different
autism (aged 10–11), from that of PEAK-E programs
multiple baseline across
participants with
embedded multiple probe
Belisle, Emergent entailed analogical 4 boys (aged 5–7, 3 with Instructions based on PEAK-T 7 L
Paliliunas, reasoning of “same,” autism) successfully promoted the
et al. (2021) “different,” and “opposite” in Multiple baseline across emergence of analogical reasoning,
children with disabilities participants with while contingency-based training
embedded multiple probe failed to produce changes in
untrained analogical reasoning
tasks

girl, since the relation indicated by the first row promoting the development of a high-order oper-
was “opposite.” If the two cells in the top row ant that allows relational and behavior
were filled by different colors, the correct flexibility.
response would be selecting a picture of a com- Among the 26 studies identified above, some
puter, since the relation indicated by the first row of them also demonstrated the effectiveness of
was “different.” After completing this phase of remediation strategies provided within the PEAK
teaching, two of the four participants went curriculum, such as multiple exemplar trainings
through additional relational trainings targeting (Schmick et  al., 2018) and different prompting
derived relational responding along the frame of strategies (Belisle, Dixon, et  al., 2020). For
distinction and opposition. Their results showed example, in the study by Schmick et al. (2018),
that contingency-­based instruction successfully the researcher used a multiple baseline across
improved all four participants’ response score. participants design to investigate the effective-
However, when the researcher replaced the pic- ness of instructions based on PEAK-T: 11H in
ture used in the matrix task, only those who teaching children with autism how to identify
received relational training procedures main- emotions of others. After relational training, two
tained their performance. This is only one of the of the three participants successfully reached the
many examples that highlight the importance of mastery criteria for the directly trained relations,
incorporating relational training procedures in demonstrating derivation of untrained stimulus-­
skill acquisition, as it moves beyond learning via stimulus relationships, as well as transformation
direct contingency and memorization, but rather of stimulus-stimulus relationships toward novel
18  PEAK Relational Training System 355

Table 18.5  Referencing PEAK in other areas Table 18.5 (continued)


Authors Title Context/findings Authors Title Context/findings
Barnes-­ Relational frame Referenced the Padilla Global A survey on the use
Holmes theory: PEAK relational (2020) assessment use of different
et al. Implications for training system as and practices in assessment
(2016) education and one of the applied behavior instruments used in
developmental commercially analysis: applied behavior
disabilities available products Surveying the analytic settings,
to promote field where 14% of the
relational learning respondents
Belisle, The use of in situ Behavioral skills indicated the use of
Rowsey, behavioral skills training improved PEAK-based
et al. training to staff assessments in
(2016) improve staff implementation of clinical practices
implementation PEAK and resulted Belisle, Synthesizing the A technical guide
of the PEAK in a corresponding Clark, et al. multiple-probe on embedding a
relational improvement in (2021) experimental multiple-probe
training system selected language design with the design within the
skills across two of PEAK relational programing of
the three learners training System PEAK
with autism in applied
Hahs and Targeting staff A 2-hour settings
Jarynowski treatment behavioral skill
(2019) integrity of the training session on
PEAK relational PEAK successfully
training system improved staff’s
using behavioral treatment fidelity stimulus class. However, one participant failed to
skills training and improved the show such derivation. The researcher then imple-
clinical outcome of mented a multiple exemplar training procedure,
six staff-client pairs which successfully prompted the emergence of
Belisle, Derived A comprehensive
untrained stimulus-stimulus relations.
Paliliunas, relational review of existed
et al. responding and evidence from
(2020) transformations behavior analytic
of function in journals 18.1.4 Referencing PEAK in Other
children: A demonstrating the
review of applied phenomenon of
Areas
behavior-analytic derived relational
journals responding and Six articles and one book chapter have been pub-
transformation of lished since 2014 that referenced the PEAK
stimulus functions
Relational Training System to various degrees
which referenced
multiple studies (see Table 18.5). Overall, these articles focus on
utilizing PEAK-­ the conceptualization behind PEAK (Barnes-­
based procedures Holmes et  al., 2016; Dixon & Stanley, 2020),
Dixon and PEAK relational A book chapter staff training procedures of implementing the
Stanley training system within the
(2020) encyclopedia of PEAK curriculum (Belisle, Rowsey, et al., 2016;
autism Spectrum Hahs & Jarynowski, 2019), the use of PEAK-
disorders, which based assessments (Padilla, 2020), and conduct-
provides an ing applied research during the implementation
overview of the
PEAK relational of PEAK (Belisle, Clark, et  al., 2021; Belisle,
training system Paliliunas, et  al., 2020). These articles extend
(continued) beyond applied research on PEAK’s clinical out-
356 M. R. Dixon et al.

Table 18.6 Reviews and critiques of the PEAK Table 18.6 (continued)


Relational Training System
Authors Title Context/findings
Authors Title Context/findings Belisle Rational A response to
Reed and Promoting the A book review of and Dixon skepticism: A Witts’s (2018)
Luiselli emergence of the PEAK-DT (2020) scientific review critique that
(2016) advanced module and of Witts’ (2018) examined the 30
knowledge: A emerging evidence criticisms of the major criticisms in
review of peak on its clinical PEAK relational the original article,
relational outcome, supporting training system which concluded
training system: its underlying that the majority of
Direct training principles of the criticisms were
module by Mark behavior science, not valid and that
R. Dixon acknowledging its many of Witts’s
effort of translating suggestions would
relational training potentially hinder
methodologies into the field from
applied practices, moving toward
and recognizing the large-scale research
potential impact of Beaujean Conceptual and A critique of the
PEAK in advancing and methodological Dixon, Paliliunas,
existing clinical Farmer concerns: A et al. (2019)
approaches to verbal (2020) commentary on research on the
behavior “randomized impact of relational
Dixon, An internal and A comprehensive controlled trial training on IQ gains,
Belisle, critical review of literature review of evaluation of listing seven major
et al. the PEAK available evidence ABA content on methodological
(2017) relational published on IQ gains in concerns that
training system peer-reviewed children with undermined the
for children with journals about autism” reported effect in the
autism and PEAK between original paper
related 2014 and 2017 Yi et al. P < 0.05 is in the A response to
intellectual (2021) eye of the Beaujean and
disabilities: beholder: A Farmer (2020) that
2014–2017 response to examined the seven
Witts An external A critique on the Beaujean and concerns voiced by
(2018) review of the Reed and Luiselli Farmer (2020) the author, which
conclusions (2016) article and on concluded that the
regarding the existing research on outcome reported by
peak direct the PEAK-DT, the authors served a
training module listing conceptual cautionary tale of
and methodological categorical
issues with existing interpretation of
evidence on p-values, restrictive
PEAK-DT, calling pre-analytic
for a more rigorous assumptions, and
examining of invalid arguments
previous research favoring certain
findings statistical methods
(continued)

come and provide an overview of many concep- Jarynowski, 2019). At the same time, the PEAK
tualizations behind PEAK and its implementation curriculum could potentially provide a context
from an organizational behavior management for applied researchers to explore many empirical
perspective. Overall, studies have shown that cli- questions regarding clinical applications of the
nicians can significantly improve treatment fidel- relational frame theory among individuals with
ity following behavioral skill training procedures intellectual and developmental disability. As
(Belisle, Rowsey, & Dixon, 2016; Hahs & shown by Belisle, Clark, et  al. (2021), the con-
18  PEAK Relational Training System 357

figuration of PEAK’s training and testing para- the curricula and assessments with the largest
digm across trial blocks allows the researcher to body of research and its utilization of methodolo-
embed a multiple probe design into daily clinical gies beyond traditional approaches in the field of
practices. Given the overall scarcity of empirical ABA, PEAK has the potential to impact services
evidence demonstrating complex transform of beyond traditional ABA interventions and pro-
nonequivalent stimulus-stimulus relations, it is of mote dialog outside this field. Furthermore,
both scientific and applied value for practitioners PEAK’s embracing of randomized trials, conver-
to utilize this opportunity to further advance the gent validity with other nonbehavioral measures,
science of human behavior. and normative referencing positions itself within
broad arenas of autism research that have often
excluded more traditional behavior analytic con-
18.1.5 Reviews and Critiques of the tributions. The content of PEAK, the inclusion of
PEAK Relational Training a wide range of ages and ability levels, and a set
System of easy-to-follow directions have combined to
move this program to the forefront of attention in
Since the publication of the first module of PEAK ABA language interventions. Continued curricu-
in 2014, the content of PEAK has sparked many lum research such as dosages, outcome impact
healthy discourses within the scientific commu- across disability level, global functioning
nity. Six reviews, critiques, and responses have changes, and parent implementation adherence
been published, synthesizing existing evidence on all are in need of empirical-based answers. Larger
research related to the PEAK Relational Training randomized trials, neurological markers of treat-
System (see Table  18.6). Although PEAK has ment success, and large group treatments will
acquired many supporters and positive reviews answer even more questions about the potential
(e.g., Reed & Luiselli, 2016), a few have taken the robustness of PEAK on improving the deficits
position that the data on PEAK are not sufficient and enhancing the strengths of a person with
(e.g., Witts, 2018). When placing a careful eye on disabilities.
the debates around specific findings, it is clear that
the majority of comments extend beyond the cur-
riculum itself, to the broader scientific enterprise References
of ABA. With PEAK being by far the most empir-
ically documented ABA curriculum, skeptics of Ackley, M., Subramanian, J. W., Moore, J. W., Litten, S.,
Lundy, M.  P., & Bishop, S.  K. (2019). A review of
relational framing, ABA interventions, and alter- language development protocols for individuals with
natives to Skinnerian approaches to autism treat- autism. Journal of Behavioral Education, 28(3), 362–
ment have sometimes taken a critical eye. 388. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­019-­09327-­8
However, such questioners have failed to produce Barnes-Holmes, Y., Kavanagh, D., & Murphy, C. (2016).
Relational frame theory: Implications for education
any significant data which supports their own and developmental disabilities. In the Wiley handbook
position on ABA content efficacy. of contextual behavioral science (pp. 227–253). Wiley
In conclusion, the PEAK Relational Training Blackwell.
System has made an impact on behavioral assess- Barron, B.  F., Paliliunas, D., & Dixon, M.  R. (2020).
Evaluation of the PEAK-DT and PEAK-G pre-­
ment and treatment for persons with autism and assessments: Comparing directly implemented and
related disorders. PEAKs adoption of post-­ indirect assessments of verbal abilities. Journal of
Skinnerian philosophy and techniques has Behavioral Education, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/
resulted in a fair amount of research and implica- s10864-­019-­09362-­5
Barron, B. F., Verkuylen, L., Belisle, J., Paliliunas, D., &
tions for best practices in intervention. Existing Dixon, M. R. (2019). Teaching “then-later” and “here-­
research to date has overwhelmingly supported there” relations to children with autism: An evalua-
its effectiveness and user-friendliness as a com- tion of single reversals and transformation of stimulus
prehensive treatment model derived from function. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1), 167–
175. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0216-­1
evidence-­based scientific discoveries. As one of
358 M. R. Dixon et al.

Beaujean, A.  A., & Farmer, R.  L. (2020). Conceptual Belisle, J., Stanley, C. R., Alholail, A. M., Galliford, M. E.,
and methodological concerns: A commentary on & Dixon, M. R. (2019). Abstraction of tactile proper-
“randomized controlled trial evaluation of ABA con- ties by individuals with autism and down syndrome
tent on IQ gains in children with autism”. Journal using a picture-based communication system. Journal
of Behavioral Education. https://doi.org/10.1007/ of Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(2), 467–475. https://
s10864-­020-­09408-­z doi.org/10.1002/jaba.526
Belisle, J., Clark, L., Welch, K., & McDonald, N.  R. Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., & Dixon, M.  R. (2017). The
(2021). Synthesizing the multiple-probe experimental relationship between derived mutually entailed rela-
design with the PEAK relational training system in tions and the function of challenging behavior in
applied settings. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1–12. children with autism: Comparing the PEAK-E-PA
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00520-­0 and the QABF. Journal of Contextual Behavioral
Belisle, J., & Dixon, M. R. (2020). Rational skepticism: Science, 6(3), 298–307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
A scientific review of Witts' (2018) criticisms of the jcbs.2017.07.004
PEAK relational training system. Journal of Applied Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., Schmick, A., Dixon, M.  R.,
Behavior Analysis, 53(2), 620–634. https://doi. Alholail, A., Galliford, M.  E., & Ellenberger, L.
org/10.1002/jaba.654 (2020). Establishing arbitrary comparative relations
Belisle, J., Dixon, M.  R., Alholai, A., Ellenberger, L., and referential transformations of stimulus function in
Stanley, C., & Galliford, M. (2020). Teaching chil- individuals with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
dren with autism to tact the private events of others. Analysis, 53(2), 938–955. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13(1), 169–173. https:// jaba.655
doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00334-­9 Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational
Belisle, J., Dixon, M. R., Munoz, B. E., & Fricke-Steuber, frame theory and human intelligence. European
K. (2021). The convergent validity of the PEAK-E-PA Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 37–51. https://
and two common assessments of language develop- doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2010.11434333
ment: The ABLLS-R and the TOLD 1: 4. Journal of Daar, J. H., Negrelli, S., & Dixon, M. R. (2015). Derived
Behavioral Education, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/ emergence of WH question–answers in children with
s10864-­020-­09426-­x autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19,
Belisle, J., Dixon, M.  R., & Stanley, C.  R. (2018). The 59–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.06.004
mediating effects of derived relational responding on Dixon, M. R. (2014a). PEAK relational training system:
the relationship between verbal operant development Direct training module. Shawnee Scientific Press.
and IQ. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11(4), 411– Dixon, M. R. (2014b). PEAK relational training system:
416. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0215-­2 Generalization module. Shawnee Scientific Press.
Belisle, J., Dixon, M.  R., Stanley, C.  R., Munoz, B., & Dixon, M.  R. (2015). PEAK relational training system:
Daar, J. H. (2016). Teaching foundational perspective-­ Equivalence module. Shawnee Scientific Press.
taking skills to children with autism using the Dixon, M.  R. (2016). PEAK relational training system:
PEAK-T curriculum: Single-reversal “I–you” deictic Transformation module. Shawnee Scientific Press.
frames. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(4), Dixon, M.  R. (2019). PEAK comprehensive assessment.
965–969. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.324 Shawnee Scientific Press.
Belisle, J., Huggins, K., Doherty, M., Stanley, C.  R., & Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., Hayes, S.  C., Stanley, C.  R.,
Dixon, M.  R. (2020). Generalized reflexive respond- Blevins, A., Gutknecht, K. F., Partlo, A., Ryan, L., &
ing and cross-modal tactile transfer of stimulus func- Lucas, C. (2021). Evidence from children with autism
tion in children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal that derived relational responding is a generalized
Behavior, 36(2), 233–250. https://doi.org/10.1007/ operant. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1–29. https://
s40616-­020-­00137-­y doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00425-­y
Belisle, J., Paliliunas, D., & Dixon, M.  R. (2021). Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., McKeel, A., Whiting, S.,
Emergent entailed analogical reasoning of “same,” Speelman, R., Daar, J. H., & Rowsey, K. (2017). An
“different,” and “opposite” in children with disabili- internal and critical review of the PEAK relational
ties. Journal of Behavioral Education, 1–12. https:// training system for children with autism and related
doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­021-­09438-­1 intellectual disabilities: 2014–2017. The Behavior
Belisle, J., Paliliunas, D., Lauer, T., Giamanco, A., Lee, Analyst, 40(2), 493–521. https://doi.org/10.1007/
B., & Sickman, E. (2020). Derived relational respond- s40614-­017-­0119-­4
ing and transformations of function in children: A Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., Rowsey, K.  E., Speelman,
review of applied behavior-analytic journals. The R. C., Stanley, C. R., & Kime, D. (2017). Evaluating
Analysis of verbal behavior, 36(1), 115–145. https:// emergent naming relations through representational
doi.org/10.1007/s40616-­019-­00123-­z drawing in individuals with developmental dis-
Belisle, J., Rowsey, K.  E., & Dixon, M.  R. (2016). The abilities using the PEAK-E curriculum. Behavior
use of in situ behavioral skills training to improve Analysis: Research and Practice, 17(1), 92. https://
staff implementation of the PEAK relational train- doi.org/10.1037/bar0000055
ing system. Journal of Organizational Behavior Dixon, M. R., Belisle, J., Stanley, C., Rowsey, K., Daar,
Management, 36(1), 71–79. https://doi.org/10.1080/0 J. H., & Szekely, S. (2015). Toward a behavior analy-
1608061.2016.1152210 sis of complex language for children with autism:
18  PEAK Relational Training System 359

Evaluating the relationship between PEAK and the trial evaluation of ABA content on IQ gains in children
VB-MAPP. Journal of Developmental and Physical with autism. Journal of Behavioral Education. https://
Disabilities, 27(2), 223–233. https://doi.org/10.1007/ doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­019-­09344-­7
s10882-­014-­9410-­4 Dixon, M. R., Peach, J., Daar, J. H., & Penrod, C. (2017).
Dixon, M. R., Belisle, J., & Stanley, C. R. (2018). Derived Teaching complex verbal operants to children with
relational responding and intelligence: Assessing the autism and establishing generalization using the PEAK
relationship between the PEAK-E pre-assessment curriculum. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
and IQ with individuals with autism and related dis- 50(2), 317–331. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.373
abilities. The Psychological Record, 68(4), 419–430. Dixon, M. R., Rowsey, K. E., Gunnarsson, K. F., Belisle,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-­018-­0284-­1 J., Stanley, C.  R., & Daar, J.  H. (2017). Normative
Dixon, M. R. (2015). PEAK relational training system: sample of the PEAK relational training system:
Equivalence module. Shawnee Scientific Press. Generalization module with comparison to individuals
Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., Daar, J.  H., & with autism. Journal of Behavioral Education, 26(1),
Williams, L. A. (2016). Derived equivalence relations 101–122. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­016-­9261-­4
of geometry skills in students with autism: An applica- Dixon, M. R., Speelman, R. C., Rowsey, K. E., & Belisle,
tion of the PEAK-E curriculum. The Analysis of Verbal J. (2016). Derived rule-following and transforma-
Behavior, 32(1), 38–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/ tions of stimulus function in a children's game: An
s40616-­016-­0051-­9 application of PEAK-E with children with develop-
Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., Munoz, B.  E., mental disabilities. Journal of Contextual Behavioral
& Speelman, R. C. (2017). Establishing derived coor- Science, 5(3), 186–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
dinated symmetrical and transitive gustatory-visual-­ jcbs.2016.05.002
auditory relations in children with autism and related Dixon, M.  R., Stanley, C., Belisle, J., Galliford, M.  E.,
intellectual disabilities using the PEAK-E curricu- Alholail, A., & Schmick, A.  M. (2017). Establishing
lum. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6(1), derived equivalence relations of basic geography
91–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.11.001 skills in children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal
Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., & Rowsey, Behavior, 33(2), 290–295. https://doi.org/10.1007/
K. (2018). Student outcomes after 1 year of front s40616-­017-­0084-­8
line staff implementation of the PEAK curriculum. Dixon, M. R., & Stanley, C. R. (2020). PEAK relational
Behavioral Interventions, 33(2), 185–195. https://doi. training system. In F. R. Volkmar (Ed.), Encyclopedia
org/10.1002/bin.1516 of autism Spectrum disorders. Springer. https://doi.
Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., Speelman, org/10.1007/978-­1-­4614-­6435-­8_102280-­1
R. C., Rowsey, K. E., Kime, D., & Daar, J. H. (2017). Dixon, M. R., Stanley, C. R., Belisle, J., & Rowsey, K. E.
Establishing derived categorical responding in chil- (2016). The test-retest and interrater reliability of the
dren with disabilities using the PEAK-E curriculum. promoting the emergence of advanced knowledge-­
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(1), 134– direct training assessment for use with individu-
145. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.355 als with autism and related disabilities. Behavior
Dixon, M.  R., Belisle, J., Whiting, S.  W., & Rowsey, Analysis: Research and Practice, 16(1), 34. https://
K.  E. (2014). Normative sample of the PEAK rela- doi.org/10.1037/bar0000027
tional training system: Direct training module and Dixon, M.  R., Whiting, S.  W., Rowsey, K., & Belisly,
subsequent comparisons to individuals with autism. J. (2014). Assessing the relationship between intel-
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(11), 1597– ligence and the PEAK relational training system.
1606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.07.020 Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(9), 1208–
Dixon, M.  R., Blevins, A., Belisle, J., & Bethel, B. 1213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.05.005
(2019). Teaching children with autism extended ver- Dixon, M. R., Wiggins, S. H., & Belisle, J. (2018). The
bal utterances under audience control in the context of effectiveness of the PEAK relational training system
show-and-tell. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1), and corresponding changes on the VB-MAPP for
194–198. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0250-­z young adults with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
Dixon, M.  R., Carman, J., Tyler, P.  A., Whiting, S.  W., Analysis, 51(2), 321–334. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Enoch, M. R., & Daar, J. H. (2014). PEAK relational jaba.448
training system for children with autism and develop- Dunkel-Jackson, S.  M., & Dixon, M.  R. (2018).
mental disabilities: Correlations with Peabody picture Promoting generalized advanced language skills of
vocabulary test and assessment reliability. Journal of children in intensive behavioral intervention with
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 26(5), 603– promoting the emergence of advanced knowledge
614. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­014-­9384-­2 generalization module (PEAK–G). Behavior Analysis
Dixon, M. R., McCord, B. E., & Belisle, J. (2018). A dem- in Practice, 11(4), 289–306. https://doi.org/10.1007/
onstration of higher-order response class development s40617-­017-­0204-­x
in children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Hahs, A. D., & Jarynowski, J. (2019). Targeting staff treat-
51(3), 590–595. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.456 ment integrity of the PEAK relational training system
Dixon, M.  R., Paliliunas, D., Barron, B.  F., Schmick, using behavioral skills training. Behavior Analysis
A. M., & Stanley, C. R. (2019). Randomized controlled
360 M. R. Dixon et al.

in Practice, 12(1), 209–215. https://doi.org/10.1007/ delays. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 33(2), 283–
s40617-­018-­00278-­6 289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-­017-­0092-­8
Hayes, S.  C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). O’Connor, M.  T., Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., & Dixon,
Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of M. R. (2020). Establishing multiple-control respond-
human language and cognition. Springer Science & ing of children with autism to people and emotions
Business Media. in context by utilizing derived stimulus relations.
Malkin, A., Dixon, M.  R., Speelman, R.  C., & Luke, Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13(1), 192–196. https://
N. (2017). Evaluating the relationships between the doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00339-­4
PEAK relational training system–direct training Padilla, K.  L. (2020). Global assessment use and prac-
module, assessment of basic language and learning tices in applied behavior analysis: Surveying the field.
skills–revised, and the Vineland adaptive behavior Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 79, 101676.
scales–II. Journal of Developmental and Physical https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2020.101676
Disabilities, 29(2), 341–351. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Partington, J.  W. (2010). The assessment of basic lan-
s10882-­016-­9527-­8 guage and learning skills - revised. Behavior Analysts.
May, B. K., & Flake, L. (2019). PEAK pre-assessments: Reed, D.  D., & Luiselli, J.  K. (2016). Promoting the
Preliminary evidence establishing internal consis- emergence of advanced knowledge: A review of peak
tency and construct validity. Behavior Analysis in relational training system: Direct training module by
Practice, 12(3), 627–631. https://doi.org/10.1007/ mark R. Dixon. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
s40617-­018-­00318-­1 49(1), 205–211. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.281
May, B.  K., & St. Cyr, J. (2021). The impact of the Rowsey, K.  E., Belisle, J., & Dixon, M.  R. (2015).
PEAK curriculum on standardized measures of intel- Principal component analysis of the PEAK rela-
ligence: A systems level randomized control trial. tional training system. Journal of Developmental
Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. https:// and Physical Disabilities, 27(1), 15–23. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1007/s41252-­021-­00199-­6 org/10.1007/s10882-­014-­9398-­9
McKeel, A., Dixon, M.  R., Daar, J.  H., Rowsey, K.  E., Rowsey, K.  E., Belisle, J., Stanley, C.  R., Daar, J.  H.,
& Szekely, S. (2015). Evaluating the efficacy of the & Dixon, M.  R. (2017). Principal component analy-
PEAK relational training system using a randomized sis of the PEAK generalization module. Journal of
controlled trial of children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 29(3), 489–
Behavioral Education, 24(2), 230–241. https://doi. 501. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­017-­9539-­z
org/10.1007/s10864-­015-­9219-­y Schmick, A. M., Stanley, C. R., & Dixon, M. R. (2018).
McKeel, A., & Matas, J. (2017). Utilizing PEAK rela- Teaching children with autism to identify private
tional training system to teach visual, gustatory, and events of others in context. Behavior Analysis in
auditory relations to adults with developmental dis- Practice, 11(4), 400–405. https://doi.org/10.1007/
abilities. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 10(3), 252– s40617-­018-­0214-­3
260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­017-­0194-­8 Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equiva-
McKeel, A., Rowsey, K., Dixon, M.  R., & Daar, J.  H. lences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
(2015). Correlation between PEAK relational train- 14(1), 5–13. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1401.05
ing system and one-word picture vocabulary tests. Stanley, C.  R., Belisle, J., & Dixon, M.  R. (2018).
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 12, 34–39. Equivalence-based instruction of academic skills:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.007 Application to adolescents with autism. Journal of
McKeel, A., Rowsey, K. E., Belisle, J., Dixon, M. R., & Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(2), 352–359. https://
Szekely, S. (2015). Teaching complex verbal operants doi.org/10.1002/jaba.446
with the PEAK relational training system. Behavior Sundberg, M.  L. (2008). VB-MAPP: Verbal behavior
Analysis in Practice, 8(2), 241–244. https://doi. milestones assessment and placement program. AVB
org/10.1007/s40617-­015-­0067-­y Press.
Moore, J.  L., Yi, Z., Hinman, J.  M., Barron, B.  F., & Sutton, A., Pikula, A., Yi, Z., & Dixon, M.  R. (2021).
Dixon, M. R. (2020). Examining the convergent valid- Evaluating the convergent validity of the PEAK com-
ity between the PEAK relational training System’s prehensive Assessment (PCA): Intelligence, behavior
semi-standardized and standardized skill assessments. challenges, and autism symptom severity. Journal of
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities. Developmental and Physical Disabilities. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­020-­09771-­9 org/10.1007/s10882-­021-­09814-­9
Moore, J. W., Rizer, H. B., Warren, L. K., Yeager, M. G., Witts, B. N. (2018). An external review of the conclusions
Brewer, R.  R., Barker, L.  K., Newborne, B., Russo, regarding the peak direct training module. Journal of
K. L., & Potter, M. (2019). An initial evaluation of an Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(3), 719–737. https://
assessment method for the PEAK relational system doi.org/10.1002/jaba.491
direct training module. Behavior Analysis in Practice, Yi, Z., Schreiber, J.  B., Paliliunas, D., Barron, B.  F.,
1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00387-­w & Dixon, M.  R. (2021). P < .05 is in the eye of
Mullen, S., Dixon, M. R., Belisle, J., & Stanley, C. (2017). the beholder: A response to Beaujean and Farmer
Establishing auditory-tactile-visual equivalence (2020). Journal of Behavioral Education. https://doi.
classes in children with autism and developmental org/10.1007/s10864-­021-­09435-­4
The Picture Exchange
Communication System 19
Rocío Rosales and Yaimarili Marin-Avelino

19.1 T
 he Picture Exchange quasi-experimental group designs conducted by
Communication System two (or more) different researchers; (2) five
single-­subject design studies conducted by three
The Picture Exchange Communication System (or more) different researchers with at least
(PECS; Bondy & Frost, 2001) was derived from twenty participants across studies; (3) one high-­
Skinner’s (1957) analysis of verbal behavior and quality randomized or quasi-experimental group
developed to teach learners with autism spectrum design study and three high-quality single-­
disorder (ASD), a form of functional communi- subject design studies conducted by three (or
cation (Bondy & Frost, 1994). Since its develop- more) different researchers or research groups
ment in the early 1990s, outcomes of PECS (Sam et  al., 2020; Steinbrenner et  al., 2020).
implementation have been evaluated in numerous PECS is classified as a focused intervention and a
research studies that demonstrate its effective- manualized intervention. Focused interventions
ness in teaching communication skills to children are defined as individual instructional practices
with ASD, along with other ancillary benefits. To that are used to teach specific skills or concepts to
date, over 200 peer-reviewed articles have been children with ASD (Odom et  al., 2010).
published on PECS (Pyramid Education Communication goals have been successfully
Consultants, n.d.), and several well-recognized addressed using PECS including increased initia-
organizations have deemed PECS an evidence-­ tions from the learner (e.g., Carr & Felce, 2007),
based practice including the National Professional increased requests (e.g., Ali et  al., 2011;
Development Center (NPDC) on ASD (Sam Angermeier et  al., 2008; Dogoe et  al., 2010),
et al., 2020) and the National Clearinghouse on increased speech and spontaneous vocalizations
Autism Evidence and Practice (NCAEP; (e.g., Charlop-Christy et  al., 2002; Greenberg
Steinbrenner et al., 2020). et al., 2013; Jurgens et al., 2009), and increased
The NPDC and NCAEP specify criteria for an social interactions with peers (e.g., Paden et al.,
intervention to be deemed evidence based. This 2012). PECS has been demonstrated to be an
includes meeting one of three minimum stan- effective intervention for preschoolers (3–5 years)
dards: (1) two high-quality experimental or to middle school learners (12–14  years) with
ASD.
R. Rosales (*) · Y. Marin-Avelino PECS comprises six phases that are designed
Department of Psychology, University of to be taught in sequence (Frost & Bondy, 2002).
Massachusetts Lowell,
Lowell, MA, USA Briefly, the goal in Phase 1 is to teach the initial
e-mail: Rocio_Rosales@uml.edu steps of communication by teaching a learner to

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 361
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_19
362 R. Rosales and Y. Marin-Avelino

independently pick up and deliver a single 19.1.1 Learner Outcomes


­picture or icon to a communicative partner (e.g.,
teacher, parent, or another caregiver). The devel- The primary benefit for learners who success-
opers of PECS recommend that two adults be fully use PECS is the development of core com-
available for this initial training phase (Frost & munication skills (e.g., joint attention, initiation,
Bondy, 2002). One adult serves as the communi- requesting; Charlop-Christy et  al., 2002).
cative partner (i.e., the person who receives the Additional collateral benefits have been reported,
picture/icon and gives access to the indicated namely, in the form of the development of speech
item), and the second adult serves as a prompter and reduction of unwanted behaviors (e.g., tan-
to assist the learner in picking up and exchang- trums, aggressive, self-harming behavior; Bondy,
ing the picture. In Phase 2, the learner demon- 2001; Ganz et  al., 2012). Learners who begin
strates this same communicative behavior using PECS before 6 years of age and continue
(exchange of single-picture card) but learns to using this communication system for at least
travel to the communication book and then to a 1 year are more likely to develop speech as their
communicative partner across various settings. sole communicative modality following mastery
Phase 2 is named “distance and persistence” of 80–120 icons (Bondy, 2001). Other learners
because the learner gains important skills in may need a much larger icon representation
using the communication binder as their voice. before vocalizations begin to emerge, if they
In Phase 3, the learner begins to discriminate emerge at all.
between pictures/icons via a four-step error cor- In an early demonstration of the PECS for
rection procedure and correspondence checks. three boys (ages 3–12) with ASD, Charlop-­
During Phase 3A, the learner discriminates Christy et  al. (2002) demonstrated systematic
between a preferred and a non-­preferred item, implementation of all six phases of PECS. Results
while in Phase 3B the learner discriminates of this study showed participants acquired a
between two preferred items. In Phase 4, the range of functional communicative skills follow-
learner constructs simple sentences using an “I ing proficiency in PECS including spontaneous
want” sentence strip. In Phase 5, the learner speech during Phase 4, imitation in play and aca-
responds to the question “What do you want?” demic settings, social-communicative behaviors
Finally, in Phase 6 the learner begins to expand (i.e., joint attention), and reduction of unwanted
their communication by commenting with vari- behaviors.
ous expressions (e.g., “I see,” “I hear,” and “I Other researchers have reported similar out-
feel”) and use of attributes (e.g., adjectives, comes in speech development following profi-
verbs, and prepositions; Frost & Bondy, 2002). ciency in Phase 4 of PECS (Bondy & Frost, 1994;
PECS has benefitted from a robust line of Ganz & Simpson, 2004; Tincani et  al., 2006;
research showing its efficacy in teaching func- Whitby et al., 2019). For example, Tincani et al.
tional communication skills (Ganz et al., 2012). (2006) examined the effects of PECS on request-
The remainder of this chapter will provide an ing behaviors and speech development in two
overview of key research areas that have been school-aged children with ASD. The researchers
evaluated over the years including direct learner varied the training by delivering verbal feedback
outcomes (e.g., acquisition of the various when the child emitted a vocalization in one con-
phases of PECS, development of speech, reduc- dition and no verbal feedback for vocalizations
tion of unwanted behavior), interactions with emitted by the child in a second condition. The
peers as communicative partners, training care- verbal feedback condition resulted in differenti-
givers to implement PECS with high levels of ated outcomes for speech production (following
treatment integrity, and adaptations for bilin- proficiency in Phase 4). These results suggest that
gual learners and learners with multiple dis- the outcome was a function of explicit prompting
abilities. The chapter will conclude with an and reinforcement provided by the researchers,
overview of recommendations for clinical and indicating this is a necessary component of PECS
future research. implementation.
19  The Picture Exchange Communication System 363

A larger outcome study by Carr and Felce nication partners (Tincani et  al., 2006). For
(2007) evaluated the impact on learner outcomes example, Greenberg et  al. (2012) evaluated the
for 41 children with ASD (ages 3–7 years) who effectiveness of a train and probe generalization
received 15  hours of PECS instruction (Phases assessment technique following each of the first
1–3) compared to a group of children who did not four phases of PECS.  Generality of PECS use
receive instruction. Results showed an increase in was measured across various settings (i.e., a cen-
persistence, spontaneity, and generality of com- ter playroom area, a convenience store found in
municative initiations for children who received the nearby community, and the living room and
the PECS training. The generalizability of PECS other central in-home areas for each participant).
outcomes (e.g., across objects, activities, envi- The train and probe procedure was deemed effec-
ronmental settings, and people) was a particu- tive for three of four participants. Some proce-
larly noteworthy outcome of this study. An dural details of the probes may have contributed
essential component of a functional communica- to these positive outcomes. First, access to the
tion system is its generality to situations that dif- highly preferred items was limited to the duration
fer from the original training context. The goal of of the study. Second, the preferred items were
PECS is to create a functional communication visible to the participants on a timed-interval
system that learners can effectively use in a vari- schedule to evoke a response. Future research
ety of contexts and with a variety of communica- should evaluate how these contextual variables
tive partners, including peers in natural settings. may contribute to positive generalization out-
comes. Although limiting access to highly pre-
Generality of PECS  A smaller number of stud- ferred items to increase motivation for learner
ies have evaluated the generality of PECS outside use of PECS is recommended, it is not clear how
of the training context (e.g., in other settings and often this practice is implemented.
maintained use following initial training). Carré
et  al. (2009) taught 5- and 6-year-old children Peers as communicative partners  An impor-
with ASD to use PECS (Phases 1–3) in their tant learner outcome that has received more
classroom and then evaluated the use of PECS in attention in recent years is how PECS can be used
each child’s home. The training was modified to to promote peer interaction. This is an essential
program for generalization. Specifically, teachers skill for learners with ASD who may often expe-
and paraprofessionals were briefed at regular rience difficulties in communication with peers.
intervals throughout the study to promote high Teaching functional communication skills with
levels of treatment integrity among communica- peers may lead to more complex social interac-
tive partners. Despite these efforts, child partici- tions and eventually the development of friend-
pants showed minimal communicative acts using ships. To maximize opportunities and generality
PECS at home even though they were consistent of training outcomes, peers can be involved in
in their use of PECS in the classroom. These PECS training. For example, Kodak et al. (2012)
results indicate the need for intervention that paired peers with access to learner-preferred
incorporates features of a new communicative items to increase the reinforcing value of peer
environment (e.g., typical communication part- interactions. Peers were directly instructed to
ners and contexts) to systematically program for deliver preferred items to target participants con-
generalization. tingent on a picture exchange (following the
Phase 1 protocol). This training established peer
Other studies have reported successful gener- interactions as conditioned reinforcers.
alization outcomes in various forms including
learners using PECS to communicate access to In a similar study, Paden et al. (2012) increased
items that were not directly trained (Marckel peer-directed requests for preferred items using
et  al., 2006), using PECS in novel settings PECS by teaching two boys with ASD to
(Greenberg et al., 2012), and with novel commu- exchange picture icons with one another
364 R. Rosales and Y. Marin-Avelino

f­ollowing direct training with an adult as the Beside direct learner outcomes, numerous
communicative partner. Following proficiency in studies have evaluated training systems to teach
picture exchange with adults, these communica- implementation of PECS with high levels of
tive acts were placed on extinction, while picture treatment integrity. Given the established rela-
exchange to peers was directly prompted and tionship between treatment integrity and learner
reinforced. This training resulted in an increase outcomes in behavior analytic interventions more
in peer-­directed requests and brief social interac- generally (Brand et  al., 2019), this area of
tions in the form of playing with the same toy. research is critical for the continued effective use
The results of this study were replicated by of PECS.  The next section of the chapter will
Doherty et al. (2018) who used a systematic fad- provide a summary of studies that have evaluated
ing procedure to increase rates of independent systems to train parents, staff, and other caregiv-
requests to peers using PECS. Participants were ers how to implement PECS with a variety of
six boys with ASD (ages 3–5). Results of this learners.
study showed some participants initiated com-
munication with a peer and responded appropri-
ately to communication bids by a trained peer. 19.1.2 Teaching Implementation
Importantly, the results generalized and main- of PECS
tained for up to 1 month following the end of for-
mal training sessions. Although initial training in PECS via workshops
Thiemann-Bourque et  al. (2016) also is widely available (Pyramid Educational
recruited peers as communicative partners in a Consultants, n.d.) and is recommended as a first
formal peer-mediated intervention that incorpo- step for parents and professionals who will
rated PECS. Participants in this study were non- implement PECS with children under their care,
verbal or minimally verbal preschool children didactic training alone is insufficient to promote
with ASD and typically developing peers (ages long-term and consistent use of behavioral inter-
3–5  years). Participants with ASD had experi- ventions (Parsons et al., 2012), and this includes
ence using PECS in Phases 3–5 at the start of implementation of PECS in various settings
the study. Typically developing peers were (Ganz et al., 2013). A study conducted by Jurgens
trained to emit responsive social skills such as et  al. (2012) exemplified the need to explicitly
turning toward a peer who initiates communica- train correct implementation of PECS to teachers
tion, showing preferred items to entice commu- and other caregivers. Jurgens et al. recruited fam-
nication, and giving access to preferred items ilies who were using PECS and asked them to
contingent on a picture exchange. Each trained upload a series of videos on YouTube showing
peer was paired with one child with ASD during their implementation at home with their child
10- to 15-min activities 1–2 times per day for with ASD.  Results of treatment integrity scores
2 days per week. During all sessions the PECS showed high levels of errors in implementation.
binder was placed between the child with ASD Specifically, 61% of all observed exchanges
and the typically developing peer. If no interac- included one or more errors such as the use of a
tion occurred following 30  seconds, the child vocal or gestural prompt by the communicative
with ASD was prompted to initiate a picture partner (no prompts should be delivered by this
exchange with their peer. Results of this study individual during training), lack of timely rein-
showed an increase in communicative behaviors forcement (reinforcement should be immediately
including gaining attention, commenting, following a picture exchange), and/or incorrect
requesting, and sharing toys. In addition, implementation of the recommended four-step
engagement in dyadic play increased, which error correction procedure during Phase 3.
was defined as staying within 2  feet of one Fortunately, several studies have evaluated sys-
another and participating in the same activity tematic training approaches to teach implementa-
for at least 45 consecutive seconds. tion of PECS to education professionals (Ganz
19  The Picture Exchange Communication System 365

et  al., 2013; Hill et  al., 2014; McCoy & also showed generalized use of PECS.  These
McNaughton, 2018), student trainees (Martocchio results show promise for training programs
& Rosales, 2016, 2017; Rosales et al., 2009), and that incorporate aspects of BST for teacher
parents of children with ASD (Alsayedhassan training.
et al., 2020; Park et al., 2011). In a related study, Ganz et al. (2013) evalu-
ated the effects of self-monitoring and instruc-
Teacher and university student train- tional coaching on the frequency of PECS
ing  Teachers and other school paraprofessionals opportunities provided by three practitioners
interact with children with ASD on a regular who worked with children with ASD (ages
basis. Similarly, university students in undergrad- 3–4  years). Training was focused on Phase 3
uate and graduate training programs work in vari- and included a review of the steps for this
ous training settings to gain experience working phase, review of the treatment integrity check-
with this population. Training accurate imple- list created for the purpose of the study, prac-
mentation of PECS with these groups is essential tice implementing Phase 3 with the researcher
but may be challenging to implement in a sys- until a criterion was met with the researcher
tematic fashion due to the time constraints school acting as the child confederate, defining “PECS
and other professionals experience daily. opportunities” for the participant, and demon-
strating how to collect data on these opportuni-
Rosales et  al. (2009) used behavioral skills ties throughout the day. Participants set a goal
training (BST) consisting of written and verbal for the number of opportunities they would pro-
instructions, modeling, rehearsal, and feedback vide to target students each day. They were then
to teach implementation of PECS Phases 1–3 to taught to graph and review data on the number
university students. The training was conducted of opportunities provided each week. Results of
with a confederate learner followed by general- this study showed an increase in the number of
ization probes with an adult with developmen- opportunities provided to child participants,
tal disabilities. Results of this study showed the and child participants showed a corresponding
BST package led to high levels of accurate increase in the use of PECS.  However, the
implementation across all participants and the results did not generalize in a novel context due
skills acquired during training generalized to a to the lack of opportunities provided by the
new setting. The results of this study were also school professionals and/or access to the com-
replicated by Homlitas et al. (2014) with three munication book.
teachers who worked directly with young chil-
dren with ASD. Parent training  Teaching parents to use
In a similar study with pre-service teachers, PECS with their children is vital to the mainte-
Hill et al. (2014) taught PECS data collection nance of this functional communication sys-
and implementation for Phases 1–4 in the con- tem. Some studies have recruited parent
text of an extended school year setting. participants to demonstrate the effects of vari-
Training consisted of modeling the procedures ous training approaches. For example, Park
to be used during six 3-hr sessions with oppor- et  al. (2011) evaluated the effects of teaching
tunities to practice and receive feedback from mothers to implement Phases 1–3B on the
the researchers. Two teachers were paired to number of independent picture exchanges for
work with one child participant (one teacher their children with ASD (2–3  years old). The
served as the communicative partner and the training consisted of a 40- to 60-min session
second as the physical prompter during Phase that included written instructions, video mod-
1). Teachers showed successful implementa- eling, rehearsal, and feedback. Once parents
tion of PECS and communicated student prog- met the established criteria for PECS imple-
ress to parents using graphs at the conclusion mentation (90% accuracy across three consec-
of the summer program. Student participants utive trials), they were asked to implement
366 R. Rosales and Y. Marin-Avelino

PECS with their child. Results showed that the 19.1.3 Caregiver and Learner
child participants increased their use of inde- Preference
pendent picture exchanges, generalized this
skill with a different communicative partner, An important aspect of any behavioral interven-
and the skills were ­maintained at 1-month fol- tion program is the consumers’ perception of the
low-up. Importantly, mother participants intervention. Consumers’ perception of services
reported high levels of satisfaction with the is correlated with recommendations for the use of
training, but the primary dependent measure behavioral analytic strategies and follow-through
for this study was the child’s use of PECS. with implementation of such strategies. Social
validity has been at the heart of behavior analytic
In a more recent study, Alsayedhassan et al. interventions since its inception (Wolf, 1978),
(2020) demonstrated the effects of a systemati- and this topic has continued to receive attention
cally implemented BST package on PECS in the field over the years (Hanley, 2010). In the
implementation by parents with their children context of PECS implementation and training,
(3 and 8 years old). The training package con- some studies have systematically evaluated pref-
sisted of written and verbal instructions, model- erence for PECS by both caregivers and learners
ing, role play, and feedback. Parents received (Lorah, 2016; van der Meer et  al., 2012).
formal training in a university setting with a Systematic evaluation of preference for commu-
graduate student playing the role of a child. nication modalities is an objective measurement
Following this initial training, parents imple- of social validity (Hanley, 2010).
mented PECS with their own child in the same Collectively, studies that have systematically
setting. Researchers used a bug-in-ear device to evaluated preference for a communication modal-
provide immediate feedback to parents during ity including PECS have result in idiosyncratic
PECS implementation with their children. outcomes (e.g., Couper et  al., 2014; Hill et  al.,
Results showed quick acquisition of PECS 2014; LaRue et  al., 2016). This indicates that a
implementation by parents (Phases 1–3) and choice evaluation may be important to integrate
increased use of PECS by child participants. into functional communication programs. There
These results generalized to effective use of are advantages to starting with a traditional tan-
PECS in the home setting and maintained gible icon-based system, namely, the systematic
1 month following the end of training. implementation that promotes development of
Although the use of BST to teach imple- critical communication skills (e.g., joint attention
mentation of PECS is effective, the time com- and initiation). Disadvantages of exclusively
mitment may not be feasible for practitioners using a communication book is that learners may
and school professionals. To address this limi- acquire hundreds of pictures/icons in their binder
tation, follow-­up studies have evaluated the use in a short period of time, and this makes storing
of pyramidal training (Martocchio & Rosales, the binder and traveling with the binder impracti-
2016), voice-­over video modules (Martocchio cal. At this point in the training, the learner may
& Rosales, 2017), and computer-based training benefit from a careful and gradual transition to an
(Rosales et al., 2018). The use of asynchronous electronic device to avoid skill degradation or
training helps to address the challenge associ- loss (Bondy, 2001).
ated with methods that require the presence of Although some formal evaluations of pre-
an expert trainer and may help increase acces- ferred communication modalities show a prefer-
sibility to systematic training on the imple- ence for technology-enhanced communication
mentation of PECS and other behavioral systems (e.g., use of Proloqu2Go on an iPad or
intervention procedures (Gerencser et  al., tablet), this may be due in part to a general pref-
2020), but additional research is needed to erence for the device itself and not how it is used
evaluate the effectiveness of this approach to as a tool for functional communication. For
training. example, the auditory stimulus generated by
19  The Picture Exchange Communication System 367

electronic devices may serve as a reinforcer for unique feature of PECS is its flexibility and
selecting an icon on a device by the learner. adaptability for a variety of learners (Frost &
Teachers’ preference for technology-based com- Bondy, 2002). The developers of PECS outline
munication in lieu of traditional PECS systems is many ways this communication system can be
important given their position as the stakeholders changed to meet the needs of learners with a vari-
of this technology. The nature of these devices ety of disabilities, but demonstrations of these
with appealing voice-output features and applica- adaptations have not been widely published. The
tions that serve functions other than communica- next section of the chapter will outline various
tion needs to be considered as potential confounds adaptations that have been reported for learners
in studies that evaluate preference for one com- with multiple disabilities and learners from bilin-
munication modality over another (Couper et al., gual or multilingual backgrounds.
2014). On the other hand, there are advantages of
adopting the use of electronic devices for func- Adaptations for learners with multiple dis-
tional communication. For example, there is a abilities  Adaptations to the PECS protocol have
decrease in the social stigma attached to carrying been demonstrated for learners who have hearing
a communication binder since much of the gen- impairments (Malandraki & Okalidou, 2007) and
eral population always carries at least one elec- visual impairments (Bracken & Rohrer, 2014;
tronic device. Ivy et  al., 2014; Lund & Troha, 2008), and at
In general, PECS is considered an accessible least one study has demonstrated that a visually
option because of its portability and low cost. If impaired therapist was successful with PECS
low-tech intervention can be as effective as high-­ implementation with a learner with ASD (Charlop
tech intervention during the first stages of com- et  al., 2008). To accommodate the therapist’s
munication development, more research is blindness, PECS cards were slightly modified by
needed to evaluate whether a transition from low-­ adding Braille labels to the cards and having
tech to high-tech is effective, and if so, when it another therapist accompany her when she inter-
should be implemented. Bondy (2012) suggested acted with the children.
this transition may be ideal during Phase 4 of
PECS, since this is usually also when vocal ver- Malandraki and Okalidou (2007) adapted
bal behavior is achieved. Alternatively, users may PECS for a 10-year-old boy with ASD and bilat-
benefit from a combination of the two modes of eral sensorineural profound hearing loss (i.e.,
communication. deafness). The participant was taught to use
PECS up to Phase 4 in an intensive training pro-
gram, followed by continued training for an addi-
19.1.4 Adaptations of PECS tional 4-month period and a 6-month follow-up.
Modifications accounted for the participant’s
One of the major advantages of PECS is that this hearing loss and potential writing abilities. For
is a low-tech form of functional communication example, because he emitted spontaneous writ-
is also generally low-cost. That is, caregivers and ing and fingerspelling during an informal assess-
practitioners working with families need not pur- ment, PECS picture cards were replaced with
chase items from the official developers of PECS written cards. The participant emitted spontane-
to create a communication binder so long as the ous vocalizations during the generalization of
implementation of PECS follows the protocol Phases 4 and 5, and these behaviors maintained at
outlined by Frost and Bondy (2002). For exam- follow-up in both written English and Greek sign
ple, a communication binder can be created using language.
digital photos taken with a camera or phone, Lund and Troha (2008) used tactile symbols
printed, and laminated, and a hook-and-loop tape (i.e., three-dimensional objects) to teach Phases
can be purchased to add to a three-ring binder to 1–3A of PECS to three blind adolescents with
create a basic communication binder. Another ASD (12–17  years old). Referents included
368 R. Rosales and Y. Marin-Avelino

c­ omputers, rain sticks, and crash pillows repre- For example, despite the flexibility of picture
sented with a combination of craft supplies communication systems, there are features that
(e.g., plastic, fabric, and masking tape) and require special attention and consideration.
household items (e.g., grains of rice and mar- Nakamura et al. (1998) discussed the difficulties
bles) and placed on 3  ×  3  in. squares of card- of picture-based systems for Japanese speakers
board. The items were attached to a 4  ×  8  in. because these systems tend to be based on English
Plexiglas board to create a modified PECS sentence formation. Chompoobutr et  al. (2013)
communication board. One participant success- also described the importance of evaluating the
fully completed all three phases of the modified choice of graphic icon symbols as these may
PECS instructional program in under a month, have different meanings depending on the learn-
while the other two showed notable improve- er’s cultural background. These researchers
ment from baseline. All participants’ rates of emphasized the cultural factors that decide the
progress resembled those of sighted children efficacy of graphic symbols in picture communi-
using PECS.  This demonstration shows that cation systems. For instance, apples are not tradi-
tactile symbols can be successfully incorpo- tionally consumed in Thailand; therefore, using
rated into a PECS protocol for children with an image of an apple to stand for a food concept
ASD who are also blind. can create unnecessary confusion to the learner.
In a similar demonstration, Bracken and Similarly, Dukhovny and Kelly (2015) outlined
Rohrer (2014) assessed the effectiveness of an basic guidelines for effectively designing and
adapted form of PECS to increase independent implementing functional communication sys-
requesting in deafblind adults with intellectual tems such as PECS to multilingual and multicul-
disabilities. PECS cards consisted of enlarged tural users with limited functional speech (e.g.,
photographs and swelled images on raised line age-appropriate picture symbols, gender- and
drawing paper. Participants learned to communi- language-appropriate voice options, and multi-
cate with PECS up to Phase 3 with these modifi- linguistic keyboards).
cations and their responding generalized to novel
settings and with multiple communicative
partners. 19.2 Limitations and Future
Directions
Adaptation for bilingual learners  A recent
area of increased interest in the field is how As noted throughout earlier sections of this chap-
approaches used by behavior analysts can be ter, there is ample empirical support showing the
adapted for learners from culturally and linguisti- benefits of PECS for learners with ASD.  The
cally diverse backgrounds (Lim et  al., 2018; research to date has focused on the first three
Wang et al., 2019). These adaptations necessarily phases of PECS with few exceptions (e.g.,
apply to the use of PECS. The flexibility of PECS Charlop-Christy et  al., 2002). This paucity of
allows for incorporation of multiple languages, research on implementation and outcomes for
and the communication system has been imple- Phases 4–6 may be due in part to learners transi-
mented across the globe since its development tioning to electronic devices (e.g., tablets and
(Al-dawaideh & Al-Amayreh, 2013; Hu & Lee, iPads). Another factor that may contribute to this
2019; Odluyurt et al., 2016; Sulzer-Azaroff et al., lack of research is that when vocalizations begin
2009). Surprisingly, there are no empirical stud- to emerge in learners following PECS implemen-
ies on the adaptation of PECS for families from tation, there tends to be a shift of focus to shaping
diverse cultures (Medina & Salamon, 2012). vocalizations (speech) and pause the use of
Although the assumption may be that implemen- PECS. This interruption is not recommended as it
tation of PECS may be the same across languages may lead to slowed progress in the development
and cultures, current research on this topic shows of increased length of utterances and complex
that this assumption is risky. verbal repertoires (Bondy, 2001, 2019).
19  The Picture Exchange Communication System 369

Although collective results for direct learner adult learners, but additional research is needed
outcomes are promising, more research is needed with this population. Future research should not
on the generalization and maintenance of these be restricted to ASD/ID in children and adoles-
outcomes for participants who learn to communi- cents and instead evaluate the collateral effects
cate effectively with both peers and adults. Future and other unique aspects of PECS implementa-
research should incorporate more learner out- tion in adults with various disabilities. It is impor-
comes (e.g., measures of emerging vocalizations, tant to note that this paucity of research for adult
spontaneous initiations, and reduction of learners is not unique to PECS.
unwanted behaviors). As noted in a section above, Although the use of speech-generating devices
this line of research is important given the social and other forms of technology may be preferred
validity and direct potential benefits of increased for this age group, there is limited research to
social interactions for both the learner and the show its efficacy, and the transition to exclusive
peers learning to communicate with children use of an electronic device should be done in a
with ASD.  Future research on this topic should systematic manner. Future studies may evaluate
replicate and extend the procedures outlined in rates of acquisition when PECS is implemented
previous studies. For example, demonstrations using electronic devices compared to the tradi-
for peer-directed mands that expand into later tional communication binder. Bondy (2001) cau-
phases of PECS (e.g., beyond Phase 4) are tions that the transition to an electronic device
needed, as well as evaluation of the components may result in the loss of current communication
necessary to support these skills in the absence of skills. To avoid this ethical dilemma, the first two
adult mediation. Future studies should also eval- phases of PECS that teach the learner to initiate
uate other forms of requests with peers (e.g., if communication should be followed regardless of
another child is playing with a preferred item, the communication modality (e.g., learner
child with ASD could be taught a socially accept- approaches a communicative partner, gets their
able response to request access to said item). attention, and then proceeds to use the device to
Additional research is also needed on optimal make a request or statement).
training strategies to implement PECS with
learners from diverse backgrounds. Findings
from the literature to date suggest that ethical 19.3 Clinical Recommendations
practice demands practitioners be informed and
capable of understanding, respecting, and col- Many variables must be considered in selecting a
laborating with the wants and needs of their cli- communication modality including the response
ents’ families in developing more linguistically effort required of the learner, learning histories
fluid and culturally relevant interventions. There correlated with presence of a specific communi-
is surprisingly limited research to date on this cation modality, the likelihood that communica-
topic given the popularity of PECs worldwide. tive acts will be reinforced in the learner’s natural
Finally, in comparison to the evidence base of environment, and prerequisite skills and learner
PECS with children with ASD, there is little preference (Valentino et al., 2019). To date there
research on adult learner outcomes. Hughes-Lika is little experimental research to guide modality
and Chiesa (2020) reviewed the literature of selection by practitioners. Although more
PECS implementation for adult learners and research is needed in this area, there is a large
found only five empirical studies with a total of body of evidence to support the widespread use
18 participants (ages 19–52  years old). The of PECS for learners with ASD.  The following
review included only one participant with ASD are recommendations to prepare for and imple-
(other participants had a wide range of disabili- ment PECS in practice.
ties including intellectual disability, Down syn- First, implementers of PECS should receive
drome, and deaf-blindness). Results of the review formal training from other professionals with
provide initial support for the use of PECS with expertise in this area. As mentioned above,
370 R. Rosales and Y. Marin-Avelino

­ orkshops that provide novice PECS implemen-


w pictures or icons are needed during the first few
tors with the necessary foundational training phases of PECS and no specific pictures or sym-
needed are widely available. If this training is bols are required, although these are available for
completed, it should be followed up with addi- purchase on various websites (e.g., Mayer
tional booster sessions and frequent check-ins to Johnson Co. Boardmaker®). Selection of the pic-
increase the likelihood of adequate levels of ture or symbols that require the least amount of
treatment integrity. response effort  is recommended. As learners
Second, implementers of PECS should  con- progress through the training phases, a desig-
duct a brief assessment to determine if PECS is nated communication book will be needed to
the most appropriate communication modality store all commonly used pictures/icons. A com-
for the learner. Valentino et al. (2019) evaluated munication book can be a small three-ring binder
the use of a brief prerequisite assessment to pre- with hook-and-loop tape placed on the cover and
dict the effectiveness and rate of acquisition of inside of the book. Pictures/icons should be orga-
requesting using three modalities (i.e., sign, pic- nized by theme to make the book user-friendly and
ture exchange, and vocalizations) in 13 young to ease transition to an electronic device or sys-
children with ASD.  A brief assessment such as tem. Images should be saved  in a digital folder
that described by this group of researchers can for easy access (e.g., if icons need to be replaced
save training time and result in optimal outcomes or duplicated).
for the learner. Sixth, during PECS implementation model
Third, implementers of PECS should identify vocalizations, implementers of PECS should dif-
preferred items that will be used during PECS ferentially reinforce vocal approximations by the
training using a formal procedure. Preferred learner and create learning opportunities to
items that will function as reinforcers are an increase the likelihood of initiations. Seventh,
essential ingredient for effective PECS training. data should be collected on learner use and prog-
A stimulus preference assessment or reinforcer ress with changes made as needed. If the learner
sampling can be used to help implementers iden- gets stuck in any phase, the implementer of PECS
tify reinforcers for individual learners. If a learner should  reevaluate steps 1–3 to determine if
stops showing interest in an item that is used dur- refresher training may be needed or if the crite-
ing training, new items should be evaluated for rion to move to the next phase of training was met
use. before advancing to the next phase or before
Fourth, once PECS is selected as the modality switching to a different form of communication.
for functional communication, implementers of Implemters should also evaluate learner  prefer-
PECS should identify two adults to participate in ence, and use multiple modalities if the learner
the initial phases of training. One adult is the indicates preference for more than one form of
prompter and the other is the “communicative communication.
partner.” During Phase 1, these roles should be Finally, implemeters of PECS should view the
consistent, but once the learner demonstrates the communication book/binder as the learners’
selected mastery criterion, the roles can be voice. That is, the communication book should
reversed, and additional communicative partners always be with the learner, and multiple opportu-
(both adults and peers) should be recruited to nities should be created to practice using the
assist with the training to program for generaliza- communication system early in training. Consider
tion. Parents should also be integrated into the adding a strap for easier transport during transi-
training as early as possible to promote further tion periods. Finally, if PECS is the preferred
generalized use of PECS in various settings. form of communication, implementers
Fifth, implementers of PECS should  prepare should  consider a gradual transition to an elec-
the communication book or binder. Only a few tronic device to promote long-term use.
19  The Picture Exchange Communication System 371

19.4 Conclusion spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum


Disorders, 2(3), 430–446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
rasd.2007.09.004
This chapter reviewed evidence for the Picture Bondy, A. (2001). PECS: Potential benefits and risks.
Exchange Communication System. This mode of The Behavior Analyst Today, 2(2), 127–132.
functional communication boasts a large body of https://doi.org/10.1037/h0099924. https://doi.
org/10.1055/s-­2008-­1064055
empirical support when implemented with learn- Bondy, A. (2012). The unusual suspects: Myths and mis-
ers with ASD.  Although it is considered an conceptions associated with PECS. Psychological
evidence-­based practice for children with ASD, Record, 62(4), 789–816. https://doi.org/10.1007/
the research to date is insufficient to make the BF03395836
Bondy, A. (2019). Issues related to AAC and SGD use by
same recommendation for adult learners with adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder.
ASD. Best practice suggests that an initial assess- Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 3, 351–
ment be conducted during the intake process to 362. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41252-­019-­00127-­9
help identify the ideal mode of communication Bondy, A.  S., & Frost, L.  A. (1994). The pic-
ture exchange communication system. Focus
for all learners (Valentino et  al., 2019). While on Autistic Behavior, 9(3), 1–19. https://doi.
there is a large body of support for PECS for org/10.1177/108835769400900301
learners with ASD, limitations of the existing Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (2001). The picture exchange com-
research include a lack of systematic applications munication system. Behavior Modification, 25(5),
725–744. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445501255004
for all six phases of PECS, inadequate demon- Bracken, M., & Rohrer, N. (2014). Using an adapted form
strations of PECS with adult learners, limited of the Picture Exchange Communication System to
evaluation of the protocol with bilingual learners increase independent requesting in deafblind adults
with ASD, and systematic evaluation of with learning disabilities. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 35(2), 269–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
approaches to use or transition to using an elec- ridd.2013.10.031
tronic device for communication. Despite these Brand, D., Henley, A.  J., DiGennaro Reed, F.  D., Gray,
limitations, PECS should continue to be consid- E., & Crabbs, B. (2019). A review of published stud-
ered when learners with ASD do not have an ies involving parametric manipulations of treatment
integrity. Journal of Behavioral Education, 28, 1–26.
established form of functional communication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­018-­09311-­8
Carr, D., & Felce, J. (2007). The effects of PECS teaching
to phase III on the communicative interactions between
References children with autism and their teachers. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 724–
737. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0203-­1
Al-dawaideh, A. M., & Al-Amayreh, M. M. (2013). The
Carré, A., Le Grice, B., Blampied, N., & Walker, D.
effectiveness of Picture Exchange Communication
(2009). Picture Exchange Communication (PECS)
System on learning request skills and the development
training for young children: Does training transfer to
of speech in Arabic-Speaking children with autism.
school and to home? Behaviour Change, 26, 54–65.
Life Science Journal, 10(2), 2139–2148.
https://doi.org/10.1375/bech.26.1.54
Ali, E., MacFarland, S.  Z., & Umbreit, J. (2011).
Charlop, M.  H., Malmberg, D.  B., & Berquist, K.  L.
Effectiveness of combining tangible symbols with the
(2008). An application of the Picture Exchange
Picture Exchange Communication System to teach
Communication System (PECS) with children with
requesting skills to children with multiple disabilities
autism and a visually impaired therapist. Journal of
including visual impairment. Education and Training
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 20(6), 509–
in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 46(3), 425–
525. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­008-­9112-­x
435. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23880596
Charlop-Christy, M. H., Carpenter, M., Le, L., LeBlanc,
Alsayedhassan, B.  T., Banda, D.  R., & Griffin-Shirley,
L. A., & Kellet, K. (2002). Using the picture exchange
N. (2020). Training parents of children with autism
communication system (PECS) with children with
to implement the picture exchange communication
autism: Assessment of PECS acquisition, speech,
intervention. Clinical Archives of Communication
social-communicative behavior, and problem behav-
Disorders, 5(1), 31–41. https://doi.org/10.21849/
ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(3), 213–
cacd.2019.00171
231. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­213
Angermeier, K., Schlosser, R.  W., Luiselli, J.  K.,
Chompoobutr, S., Potibal, P., Boriboon, M., & Phantachat,
Harrington, C., & Carter, B. (2008). Effects of ico-
W. (2013). Perception and multimeaning analysis of
nicity on requesting with the Picture Exchange
graphic symbols for Thai picture-based communica-
Communication System in children with autism
tion system. Disability and Rehabilitation: Assistive
372 R. Rosales and Y. Marin-Avelino

Technology, 8(2), 102–107. https://doi.org/10.3109/1 Disabilities, 24(6), 539–558. https://doi.org/10.1007/


7483107.2012.737531 s10882-­012-­9288-­y
Couper, L., van der Meer, L., Schӓfer, M.  C. M., Greenberg, A., Tomaino, M., & Charlop, M. (2013).
McKenzie, E., McLay, L., O’Reilly, M. F., Lancioni, Adapting the Picture Exchange Communication
G.  E., Marschik, P.  B., Sigafoos, F., & Sutherland, System to elicit vocalizations in children with autism.
D. (2014). Comparing acquisition of and preference Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities.
for manual signs, picture exchange, and speech-­ https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­013-­9344-­2
generating devices in nine children with autism spec- Hanley, G.  P. (2010). Toward effective and preferred
trum disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, programming: A case for the objective measurement
17(2), 99–109. https://doi.org/10.3109/17518423.201 of social validity with recipients of behavior-change
3.870244 programs. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3, 13–12.
Dogoe, M.  S., Banda, D.  R., & Lock, R.  H. (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391754
Acquisition and generalization of the Picture Hill, D.  A., Flores, M.  M., & Kearley, R.  F. (2014).
Exchange Communication System behaviors across Maximizing ESY services: Teaching pre-service
settings, persons, and stimulus classes with three stu- teachers to assess communication skills and imple-
dents with autism. Education and Training in Autism ment picture exchange with students with autism spec-
and Developmental Disabilities, 45(2), 216–229. trum disorder and developmental disabilities. Teacher
Doherty, A., Bracken, M., & Gormley, L. (2018). Teaching Education and Special Education, 37(3), 241–254.
children with autism to initiate and respond to peer https://doi.org/10.1177/0888406414527117
mands using picture exchange communication system Homlitas, C., Rosales, R., & Candel, L. (2014). A further
(PECS). Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11(4), 279– evaluation of behavioral skills training for implemen-
288. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00311-­8 tation of the picture exchange communication system.
Dukhovny, E., & Kelly, E. B. (2015). Practical resources Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 198–203.
for provision of services to culturally and lin- https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.99
guistically diverse users of AAC. Perspectives on Hu, X., & Lee, G. (2019). Effects of PECS on the emer-
Communication Disorders and Sciences in Culturally gence of vocal mands and the reduction of aggres-
and Linguistically Diverse Populations, 22(1), 25–39. sive behavior across settings for a child with autism.
https://doi.org/10.1044/cds22.1.25 Behavioral Disorders, 44(4), 215–226. https://doi.
Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (2002). The picture exchange com- org/10.1177/0198742918806925
munication system training manual (2nd ed.). Pyramid Hughes-Lika, J., & Chiesa, M. (2020). The picture
Educational Products. exchange communication system and adults lack-
Ganz, J.  B., & Simpson, R.  L. (2004). Effects on com- ing functional communication: A research review.
municative requesting and speech development of the European Journal of Behavior Analysis. https://doi.
picture exchange communication system in children org/10.1080/15021149.2020.1815507
with characteristics of autism. Journal of Autism and Ivy, S., Hatton, D., & Hooper, J. (2014). Using the
Developmental Disorders, 34(4), 395–409. https://doi. Picture Exchange Communication System with stu-
org/10.1023/b:jadd.0000037416.59095.d7 dents with visual impairment. Exceptional Children,
Ganz, J. B., Davis, J. L., Lund, E. M., Goodwyn, F. D., & 80(4), 474–488. https://doi-­org.umasslowell.idm.oclc.
Simpson, R.  L. (2012). Meta-analysis of PECS with org/10.1177/0014402914527239
individuals with ASD: Investigation of targeted versus Jurgens, A., Anderson, A., & Moore, D. W. (2009). The
non-targeted outcomes, participant characteristics, and effect of teaching PECS to a child with autism on ver-
implementation phase. Research in Developmental bal behaviour, play, and social functioning. Behaviour
Disabilities, 33(2), 406–418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Change, 26(1), 66–81. https://doi-­org.umasslowell.
ridd.2011.09.023 idm.oclc.org/10.1375/bech.26.1.66
Ganz, J. B., Goodwyn, F. D., Boles, M. M., Hong, E. R., Jurgens, A., Anderson, A., & Moore, D. W. (2012). Parent-­
Rispoli, M. J., Lund, E. M., & Kite, E. (2013). Impacts implemented picture exchange communication sys-
of a PECS instructional coaching intervention on prac- tem (PECS) training: An analysis of YouTube videos.
titioners and children with autism. AAC: Augmentative Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 15(5), 351–360.
& Alternative Communication, 29(3), 210–221. https://doi.org/10.3109/17518423.2012.692125
https://doi.org/10.3109/07434618.2013.818058 Kodak, T., Paden, A., & Dickes, N. (2012). Training and
Gerencser, K.  R., Akers, J.  S., Becerra, L.  A., Higbee, generalization of peer-directed mands with non-vocal
T. S., & Sellers, T. P. (2020). A review of asynchronous children with autism. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 28,
trainings for the implementation of behavior analytic 119–124. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393112
assessments and interventions. Journal of Behavioral LaRue, R.  H., Pepa, L., Delmolino, L., Sloman, K.  N.,
Education, 29(1), 122–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Fiske, K., Hansford, A., Liebross, S., & Weiss,
s10864-­019-­09332-­x M.  J. (2016). A brief assessment for selecting com-
Greenberg, A.  L., Tomaino, M.  A. E., & Charlop, munication modalities for individuals with Autism
M.  H. (2012). Assessing generalization of the pic- Spectrum Disorders. Evidence-Based Communication
ture exchange communication system in children Assessment and Intervention, 10(1), 32–43. https://
with autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical doi.org/10.1080/17489539.2016.1204767
19  The Picture Exchange Communication System 373

Lim, N., O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos, J., & Lancioni, G. E. Odluyurt, S., Aldemir, O., & Kapan, A. (2016). An investi-
(2018). Understanding the linguistic needs of diverse gation on the effects of PECS and observational learn-
individuals with autism spectrum disorder: Some ing in initiating and maintenance of communication
comments on the research literature and suggestions among children with Autism. International Journal of
for clinicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Early Childhood Special Education, 8(2), 151–164.
Disorders, 48(4), 2890–2895. https://doi.org/10.1007/ https://doi.org/10.20489/intjecse.284658
s10803-­018-­3532-­y Odom, S.  L., Collet-Klingenberg, L., Rogers, S.  J., &
Lorah, E. R. (2016). Comparing teacher and student use Hatton, D.  D. (2010). Evidence-based practices in
and preference of two methods of augmentative and interventions for children and youth with autism spec-
alternative communication: Picture exchange and a trum disorders. Preventing School Failure, 54(4), 275–
speech-generating device. Journal of Developmental 282. https://doi.org/10.1080/10459881003785506
and Physical Disabilities, 28(5), 751–767. https://doi. Paden, A.  R., Kodak, T., Fisher, W.  W., Gawley-
org/10.1007/s10882-­016-­9507-­z Bullington, E.  M., & Bouxsein, K.  J. (2012).
Lund, S. K., & Troha, J. M. (2008). Teaching young peo- Teaching children with autism to engage in peer-
ple who are blind and have autism to make requests directed mands using a picture exchange com-
using a variation on the picture exchange communi- munication system. Journal of Applied Behavior
cation system with tactile symbols: A preliminary Analysis, 45(2), 425–429. https://doi.org/10.1901/
investigation. Journal of Autism and Developmental jaba.2012.45-­425
Disorders, 38(4), 719–730. https://doi-­org.umasslow- Park, J.  H., Alber-Morgan, S.  R., & Cannella-Malone,
ell.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/s10803-­007-­0439-­4 H. (2011). Effects of mother-implemented picture
Malandraki, G.  A., & Okalidou, A. (2007). The appli- exchange communication system (PECS) training on
cation of PECS in a deaf child with autism: A case independent communicative behaviors of young chil-
study. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental dren with autism spectrum disorders. Topics in Early
Disabilities, 22(1), 23. https://doi.org/10.1177/10883 Childhood Special Education, 31(1), 37–47. https://
576070220010301 doi.org/10.1177/0271121410393750
Marckel, J.  M., Neef, N.  A., & Ferreri, S.  J. (2006). A Parsons, M.  B., Rollyson, J.  H., & Reid, D.  H. (2012).
preliminary analysis of teaching improvisation with Evidence-based staff training: A guide for practi-
the picture exchange communication system to chil- tioners. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 5(2), 2–11.
dren with a autism. Journal of Applied Behavior https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391819
Analysis, 39(1), 109–115. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Pyramid Educational Consultants, Inc. (n.d.). Retrieved
jaba.2006.131-­04 from: https://pecsusa.com/
Martocchio, N., & Rosales, R. (2016). An evalua- Rosales, R., Stone, K., & Rehfeldt, R.  A. (2009). The
tion of pyramidal training to teach implementation effects of behavioral skills training on implementa-
of the picture exchange communication system. tion of the picture exchange communication system.
Behavioral Interventions, 31(3), 265–282. https://doi. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(3), 541–
org/10.1002/bin.1448 549. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­541
Martocchio, N., & Rosales, R. (2017). Video modeling Rosales, R., Eckerman, D. A., & Martocchio, N. (2018).
with voice-over instructions to teach implementa- An evaluation of train-to-code to teach implementa-
tion of the picture exchange communication system. tion of PECS©. Journal of Organizational Behavior
Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice, 17(2), Management, 38(2–3), 144–171. https://doi.org/10.10
142–154. https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000069 80/01608061.2018.1454873
McCoy, A., & McNaughton, D. (2018). Training educa- Sam, A.  M., Cox, A.  W., Savage, M.  N., Waters, V., &
tion professionals to use the picture exchange commu- Odom, S.  L. (2020). Disseminating information on
nication system: A review of the literature. Behavior evidence-based practices for children and youth
Analysis in Practice, 12(3), 667–676. https://doi. with Autism Spectrum Disorder: AFIRM. Journal of
org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00296-­4 Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50, 1931–1940.
Medina, A. M., & Salamon, J. T. (2012). Current issues https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­019-­03945-­x
in teaching bilingual children with autism spectrum Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Prentice-Hall.
disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Special Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin,
Education Professionals, 69–75. http://www.eric. K. L., Nowell, S. W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey, S.,
ed.gov/?id=EJ1135508 McIntyre, N. S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage, M. N.
Nakamura, K., Newell, A., Alm, N., & Waller, A. (1998). (2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth,
How do members of different language communities and young adults with Autism. The University of North
compose sentences with a picture-based communica- Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child
tion system? A cross-cultural study of picture-based Development Institute, National Clearinghouse on
sentences constructed by English and Japanese speak- Autism Evidence and Practice Review Team.
ers. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, Sulzer-Azaroff, B., Hoffman, A., Horton, C., Bondy,
14(2), 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/074346198123 A., & Frost, L. (2009). The Picture Exchange
31278226 Communication System (PECS): What do the data
374 R. Rosales and Y. Marin-Avelino

say? Focus on Autism, 24, 89–103. https://doi. Comparing three augmentative and alternative com-
org/10.1177/1088357609332743 munication modes for children with developmental
Thiemann-Bourque, K., Brady, N., McGuff, S., Stump, K., disabilities. Journal of Developmental and Physical
& Naylor, A. (2016). Picture Exchange Communication Disabilities, 24(5), 451. https://doi.org/10.1007/
System and pals: A peer-mediated augmentative and s10882-­012-­9283-­3
alternative communication intervention for minimally Wang, Y., Kang, S., Ramirez, J., & Tarbox, J. (2019).
verbal preschoolers with autism. Journal of Speech, Multilingual diversity in the field of applied behav-
Language, and Hearing Research, 59(5), 1133–1145. ior analysis and autism: A brief review and dis-
https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-­L-­15-­0313 cussion of future directions. Behavior Analysis in
Tincani, M., Crozier, S., & Alazetta, L. (2006). The picture Practice, 12(4), 795–804. https://doi.org/10.1007/
exchange communication system: Effects on manding s40617-­019-­00382-­1
and speech development for school-aged children with Whitby, P., Kucharczyk, S., & Lorah, E. (2019). Teaching
autism. Education and Training in Developmental object exchange for communication to a young girl
Disabilities, 41(2), 177–184. https://doi.org/10.1007/ with autism spectrum disorder and visual impairment.
s10882-­017-­9564-­y Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 113(4),
Valentino, A. L., LeBlanc, L. A., Veazey, S. E., Weaver, 372–380. https://doi-­org.umasslowell.idm.oclc.org/10
L. A., & Raetz, P. B. (2019). Using a prerequisite skills .1177/0145482X19869521
assessment to identify optimal modalities for mand Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjec-
training. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1), 22–32. tive measurement or how applied behavior analysis is
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0256-­6 finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
van der Meer, L., Didden, R., Sutherland, D., O’Reilly, 11(2), 203–214. https://doi-­org.umasslowell.idm.oclc.
M.  F., Lancioni, G.  E., & Sigafoos, J. (2012). org/10.1901/jaba.1978.11-­203
Augmentative and Alternative
Communication (AAC) Systems 20
Hayley Neimy and Brenda Fossett

20.1 Augmentative increased rates and severities of inappropriate


and Alternative behaviors (e.g., aggression, self-injury, disrup-
Communication (AAC) tion, and destructive behaviors). These behaviors,
Systems while often highly socially unacceptable, are
adaptive for these individuals in that they serve
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex, socially communicative purposes, such as obtain-
neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by ing specific wants and needs, in the absence of
specific behavioral deficits and excesses, includ- more socially acceptable communication skills
ing significant limitations in communication (Nam et al., 2018). These severe communication
skills, as a whole, and vocal speech, in particular challenges, paired with problematic behavior,
(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental often serve as significant barriers to successful
Disorders, DSM-5; American Psychiatric participation and inclusion in a variety of natural-
Association, 2013). The rate of ASD diagnoses istic settings, including home, school, work, and
has increased over the past three decades, from other community-based environments
approximately 1 in 2500 individuals in 1990 to, (Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013; Holyfield et  al.,
currently, 1  in 59 (United States) and 1  in 66 2017).
(Canada) (Center for Disease Control Interventions targeting various symptoms
and  Prevention, 2014). Research suggests that associated with ASD, grounded in the principles
approximately 50% of all individuals diagnosed of applied behavior analysis (ABA), have pro-
with ASD cannot engage in fluent spoken com- duced efficacious outcomes over nearly 50 years
munication; 25–61% have limited functional (Baer et al., 1968; Reichow, 2012; Virués-Ortega,
communication skills (Aydin & Diken, 2020; 2010). These behavior-analytic interventions
Hart & Banda, 2010; Nam et al., 2018; National ubiquitously focus on incorporating empirically
Research Council, 2001; Schlosser & Wendt, supported methods for improving a wide variety
2008). These profound difficulties in communi- of skills, including functional communication
cating contribute to the etiology of other corre- skills (Carr & Durand, 1985; Durand, 1999).
sponding characteristics of ASD, specifically However, despite advances in comprehensive,
early intensive behavioral interventions (EIBI),
H. Neimy (*) · B. Fossett as many as 35% of children with ASD still enter
Applied Behaviour Analysis-Autism, Capilano school unable to speak in multiword utterances or
University, communicate basic wants and needs via speech
North Vancouver, BC, Canada
or other methods (Light et  al., 2003; Sievers
e-mail: hneimy@endicott.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 375
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_20
376 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

et  al., 2018). Therefore, it is critical that using one or more techniques, including ges-
­researchers and practitioners refocus their efforts tures and/or manual signs, pointing to or
on targeted interventions and strategies to pro- selecting items, or activating speech-generat-
mote strong communication repertoires that ing technology.
maintain over time and generalize across envi- For individuals with severe speech and/or
ronments, throughout the life span. physical impairments, AAC may serve as an
The use of AAC has become a therapeutic alternative to vocal speech. For those with devel-
response to address the unique communicative opmental disabilities, including ASD, AAC may
goals of individuals with complex communica- serve as an alternative to vocal speech, or it may
tion needs (CCNs), many of whom present with augment an individual’s spoken communication
an ASD diagnosis (Light et  al., 1998; Light & skills. For some, AAC may be used temporarily,
McNaughton, 2012). Seventy years of publica- while speech develops, or long-term, if neces-
tions document and describe the use of AAC sary. Some individuals may require AAC for all
interventions with diverse populations of chil- communication, while others may require AAC
dren and adults, across a variety of disciplines at specific times or under specific conditions
(e.g., speech and language therapy, special edu- (ASHA, 1989, 2005; Mirenda & Fossett, 2011).
cation, and ABA-based therapy). Specifically, It is important to emphasize that the use of AAC
AAC interventions have been applied with typi- interventions does not interfere with the develop-
cally developing toddlers and children; neurodi- ment of spoken language skills (Millar et  al.,
verse children, adolescents, and adults, including 2006; Schlosser & Wendt, 2008), and, as such,
those diagnosed with ASD, developmental dis- these interventions should not be withheld while
abilities (DDs), intellectual delays (IDs), and targeting the development of speech. For indi-
Down syndrome; individuals with physical dis- viduals with ASD, specifically, rather than wait-
abilities, such as cerebral palsy (CP); individuals ing for a young child with ASD to “fail” in
who have sustained traumatic brain injury (TBI); developing functional communication skills via
and geriatric populations, including those diag- speech before considering and implementing
nosed with dementia and aphasia (Creer et  al., AAC, early AAC intervention will help to (a) pre-
2016; Harris & Reichle, 2004; Light & Drager, vent the development of communication-based
2011). For the purposes of the present chapter, problem behavior, (b) facilitate the development
discussion will focus primarily on the application of functional and social communication skills,
of evidence-based AAC interventions with indi- and (c) support the development of conceptual
viduals with ASD. and learning skills that will further enhance suc-
cessful outcomes in a variety of environmental
settings (Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013; Mirenda,
20.2 Overview of AAC 1997).
There are two primary elements that comprise
20.2.1 What Is AAC? any individual’s AAC system: (a) the symbols
that represent words or messages and (b) the
As a field of clinical practice, AAC focuses techniques used to communicate words or mes-
on meeting the needs of those with signifi- sages (Mirenda, 2017). Various instructional
cant communication disorders, characterized strategies, many derived from ABA, are used to
by impairments in speech-language produc- teach individuals to use these elements to engage
tion and/or comprehension (ASHA, 2005). in functional, independent communication
AAC interventions incorporate the use of a (Mirenda, 2017). To better understand these two
wide variety of symbols (e.g., objects, primary elements and, ultimately, make thera-
images, and text) to represent language. peutic decisions, it is necessary to explore a num-
Individuals may be taught to communicate ber of individual AAC components.
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 377

20.3 AAC Systems body language (e.g., crossing your arms and put-
ting your hands on your hips), and facial expres-
AAC systems are comprised of multiple, inte- sions (e.g., smiling and grimacing) are culturally
grated components, including (a) symbols, (b) specific methods of communicating that occur
strategies or techniques, and (c) aids or naturally during communicative interactions of
equipment. neurotypically developing individuals. Although
nonvocal, these behaviors are not “nonverbal,” as
they involve sending a message between a
20.3.1 Types of Symbols speaker (i.e., the individual emitting the gesture)
and a listener (i.e., the individual receiving and
Generally speaking, symbols are used to repre- interpreting the gesture) (Skinner, 1957).
sent something else; in the context of AAC, they
represent words and/or messages. Symbols are Advantages of gestures, body language, and
further classified as either unaided or aided facial expressions  A primary advantage relates
(Mirenda, 2003; Mueller, 2014; Sennott & to the overall ease with which the community at
Mason, 2016). It is important to note that both large understands gestures, body language, and
unaided and aided symbols can be used to facial expression. Given that there are culture-­
enhance communication input (i.e., understand- specific (i.e., gestures and body language) and
ing or “receptive” skills) and output (i.e., “expres- cross-cultural (i.e., facial expressions) aspects to
sive” skills). As well, an individual’s AAC system this modality, individuals who use this form of
may incorporate the use of one or more types of communication are likely to be readily under-
symbols. For example, individuals may use both stood by others within their own community.
unaided (e.g., gestures and/or manual signs) and Additionally, for individuals with significant and/
aided (e.g., tangible, graphic, and/or text-based) or multiple impairments that interfere with the
symbols as part of their overall AAC system. The use of more formalized, symbolic communica-
following section will provide a description of tion (e.g., manual signs and picture-based sys-
each category of symbols in general, as well as tems), gestures and body language may be an
specific information regarding commonly used appropriate alternative, provided they possess the
symbol sets. necessary motor and imitative skills (Nam et al.,
2018). Lastly, without the need for external
20.3.1.1 Unaided Symbols equipment, communication exchanges can occur
Unaided symbols do not require any external or with relatively low effort (Mirenda & Fossett,
ancillary materials or equipment; they are pro- 2011). This reduced response effort may be par-
duced by the individual’s body (Aydin & Diken, ticularly advantageous for the initial stages of
2020; Mirenda, 2003; Mirenda & Fossett, 2011; specific interventions, such as functional com-
Sennott & Mason, 2016). Forms of unaided sym- munication training (FCT) (Tiger et al., 2008).
bols include (a) vocalizations, (b) gestures, body
language, and facial expression, (c) manual signs, Disadvantages of gestures, body language, and
and (d) eye gaze. A key advantage to unaided facial expressions  This form of communication
symbols, in general, relates to this lack of exter- is significantly limited with regard to the number
nal equipment. As unaided symbols utilize a per- of messages and communicative functions one
son’s body, cost and portability are not issues. can convey. For those with profound physical
These symbols can be generated quickly and and/or cognitive impairments, gestures may be
with relative ease across all environments and idiosyncratic and less discernable to non-familiar
contexts. communication partners. This may result in
inconsistent reinforcement, which will affect
Gestures, body language, and facial expres- ongoing acquisition, maintenance, and general-
sions  Gestures (e.g., pointing and reaching), ization of communication skills (Nam et  al.,
378 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

2018). Further, given that many individuals with responses (intraverbals), these have not been
ASD experience challenges with social skill-­ researched as extensively.
based repertoires (e.g., emotion recognition,
identifying facial expressions, and understanding Advantages of MS  There are several advantages
nonverbal nuances in communication) (Kanner, to support the use of MS as part of an AAC inter-
1943), reliance on gestures and facial expressions vention. A primary advantage of MS relates to
for communication purposes may be less than portability and cost. Because MS requires no
ideal. additional materials or equipment, it can be
incorporated into daily interactions and activities
Manual signing  Manual signing (MS) refers to with relative ease (Mirenda, 2003; Mirenda &
the use of unique, individual signs borrowed Fossett, 2011; Nam et  al., 2018; Sundberg &
from a formalized sign language (e.g., American Partington, 1998). Another advantage relates to
Sign Language; ASL). As an AAC intervention, speed and efficiency of communication.
MS is typically used for keywords that follow the Messages via MS can be delivered more quickly
grammatical and syntactical rules of the spoken and with greater fluency, compared to communi-
language (e.g., English), rather than the formal- cation boards or devices, which typically require
ized sign language from which they are derived. time to scan and discriminate between symbols.
Particularly for those with developmental dis- Therefore, MS can lead to faster access to rein-
abilities, MS may be modified such that it is idio- forcers, which is a critical consideration when an
syncratic to the individual (e.g., “home signs”). AAC intervention is required to replace problem-
Generally, these modifications are based on the atic behavior; such a replacement behavior must
learner’s fine motor and imitation repertoires. be more efficient and effective in producing the
To support language comprehension, indirect desired reinforcer, and, in most circumstances,
consumers (e.g., parents, teachers, and service MS fits the bill (Ganz et  al., 2019; Nam et  al.,
providers) can use MS to augment their speech, 2018; Sundberg & Partington, 1998). In addition,
thereby enhancing communication input and, there is some research to suggest that MS, a
effectively, modeling the use of MS. For exam- topography-based system (i.e., the output of each
ple, a therapist may simultaneously sign the unique MS symbol is different in form), has dis-
words “stand up,” “outside,” and “play,” when tinct advantages to selection-based systems (i.e.,
saying to the learner, “It’s time to stand up, so we the output involves a single response, such as
can go outside and play!” Combining visual input pointing). Specifically, topography-based sys-
(i.e., MS) with auditory input (i.e., speech) tems are more similar to, and may better facili-
increases the saliency of critical components of tate, the emergence of vocal speech, another
the message. MS can also enhance language out- topography-based communication modality. It is
put. Some learners may combine MS with vocal- important to note, however, that data regarding
izations and vocal speech, or they may use MS this issue are limited.
alone. MS has been used over several decades to
support both receptive and expressive communi- Disadvantages of MS  Despite apparent advan-
cation skills in children, youth, and adults with tages of MS-based AAC interventions, they must
developmental disabilities, including be considered in relation to some very significant
ASD. Investigations of MS as a communication-­ disadvantages. It is important to note that the use
based replacement behavior have focused pri- of MS as an AAC intervention is not equivalent to
marily on teaching requests, or mands, for desired the use of ASL (or other formal signed l­ anguages);
items, activities, or breaks (Ganz et  al., 2012; users do not belong to the signing deaf commu-
Holyfield et al., 2017; Morin et al., 2018; van der nity. Related to this is the topographical nature of
Meer & Rispoli, 2010). While MS can be used MS. Each signed word involves a different hand-
for other communicative purposes, such as label- shape, location, and movement, making the tar-
ing (tacting) or engaging in socially interactive get response different across vocabulary and
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 379

messages. Not only does this increase the com- 20.3.1.2 Aided Symbols
plexity of responding for the learner, it requires Aided symbols require the use of external items
specific expertise on the part of the practitioner. and commonly include (a) three-dimensional
Practitioners must know how to produce individ- (3D) tactile or tangible symbols, (b) two-­
ual signs accurately, yet most lack formal train- dimensional (2D) photographs and/or line-­
ing in the use of MS and, instead, acquire signed drawing symbols, and (c) text (including Braille).
vocabulary from print or online media (e.g., A general advantage across all types of aided
video). While the availability of online video symbols relates to the role these symbols can
models increases the likelihood that teachers pro- play in prompting communication behaviors. The
duce individual signs correctly, there are issues physical presence of aided symbols may serve as
regarding the selection of conceptually accurate a salient prompt or visual discriminative stimulus
signs. For example, whereas in English the spo- (SD) (Nam et  al., 2018), thereby facilitating
ken word, “break” can refer to multiple mean- spontaneous or independent communication
ings, there are different ASL signs to reflect responses.
individual meanings (e.g., the sign for “break” a
toy is different from the sign for take a “break”). Tangible or tactile symbols  This approach
Those without sufficient knowledge of and train- involves the use of real objects, miniature objects,
ing in the use of signed languages often use signs and partial objects (Beukelman & Mirenda,
that are conceptually inaccurate, further decreas- 2013) to represent items, activities, and so on.
ing the degree to which learners will be under- These items may be unaltered, such as using a
stood by members of a signing community. spoon to represent “mealtime” or altered, such as
Given that there is little knowledge of MS using a piece of metal chain to represent “swings.”
among the hearing population, individuals using Further, tangible symbols may be created from
MS for AAC purposes will likely have a limited parts of items within the environment. For exam-
verbal community, comprised only of individuals ple, a piece of rubber cut from an exercise ball
who know them well. As the verbal community may be used to communicate “OT” or “bounce
plays an important role in effectively and consis- on the ball.” Tangible or tactile symbols can, and
tently reinforcing language and communication, should, be individualized based on the learner’s
this is a critical shortcoming (Ganz et al., 2019; unique needs and environment. These symbols
Mirenda & Fossett, 2011; Nam et  al., 2018; can be used to provide augmented input (i.e.,
Sundberg & Partington, 1998). In addition, those showing/giving the object to the learner to give
who exhibit fine motor deficits or motor imitation information) or for augmented output (i.e., the
challenges may have pronounced difficulty emit- learner points to, picks up, and/or gives the object
ting signs accurately (Sundberg & Partington, as a communicative act).
1998), which could result in the adoption of mod-
ified signs. Those using such idiosyncratic “home Advantages of tangible or tactile symbols  The
signs” are even less likely to have a robust verbal primary advantage of this approach is the high
community. While the use of modified MS could degree of accuracy and point-to-point correspon-
be considered a short-term option (e.g., an appro- dence with objects in the learner’s environment.
priate functional communicative response (FCR) Further, and particularly for individuals with
to temporarily replace severe problematic behav- ASD who may present with comorbid visual
ior as a functionally equivalent, socially appro- impairments and/or profound intellectual impair-
priate behavior), it is highly unlikely that these ments, the use of tangible or tactile symbols in
signs would generalize across settings and novel combination with graphic stimuli (i.e., photo-
individuals. In fact, research suggests that trained graphs and line drawings) may facilitate individ-
MS rarely generalizes to other environments, uals in making connections between tangible
individuals, or contexts (Mirenda, 2003). items and two-dimensional (2D) representations,
380 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

thereby promoting their ability to use 2D repre- communication partners, including non-familiar
sentations for communication purposes. partners in community settings. This ease in
comprehension is likely to produce more consis-
Disadvantages of tangible or tactile sym- tent responding and reinforcement by those
bols  The main drawback to this approach relates working with and interacting with the learner,
to portability. Tangible and/or tactile symbols, thereby facilitating the acquisition, maintenance,
being 3D, require space. The more symbols an and generalization of communication across indi-
individual uses, the more space that is needed for viduals and environments (Beukelman &
storage and display. While it is possible to incor- Mirenda, 2013).
porate these symbols across environments such
as home, school, and community, it certainly Disadvantages of photographs and line-­
requires careful planning and the development of drawing symbols  Although it is relatively easy
systems to transport and display symbols. It can to develop AAC systems that incorporate the use
also be difficult to identify or create tangible or of photographs or line-drawing symbols, a key
tactile symbols, particularly for more abstract or disadvantage relates to cost, both monetary and
large items, places, or activities. Further, if a tan-
with regard to time. Producing 2D representa-
gible or tactile symbol is unavailable for a desired tions will require one or more of the following: a
item or activity, the communication response digital camera, access to the Internet, and/or spe-
cannot occur; this may result in frustration and cialized software. Additional materials such as a
confusion on behalf of the learner. Given the printer and ink, laminator, and Velcro® are
complexity of tangible or tactile symbols, consul- required to produce nonelectronic materials.
tation with experts in the field of deafblindness, Time is also required, not only to develop the ini-
who have a wealth of experience in designing tial set of materials but to maintain those materi-
such systems, is recommended. als in the long term. Ongoing maintenance
typically requires replacing print-based materials
Photographs and line-drawing symbols  Two-­ that degrade over time or are lost, and updating
dimensional stimuli in the form of pictures and vocabulary items as the learner acquires new
line drawings are common within AAC interven- terms.
tions. Generic or individualized photographs and/ Another disadvantage relates to the use of
or commercially produced graphic symbols (e.g., nonelectronic systems, whereby photographs or
Picture Communication Symbols, PCS®) are line-drawing symbols are organized on commu-
used to represent various vocabulary items and nication displays and/or in books. Space is
messages, including people, places, activities, required to provide a robust vocabulary; the
items, actions, and so on. larger the vocabulary, the more pages and/or
space required. Particularly for young children,
Advantages of photographs and line-drawing such systems can become heavy and cumber-
symbols  There are a number of advantages that some. Finally, while such symbols can serve as a
make these symbols attractive. First, stimuli are cue to communicate, users may also make
relatively easy to develop or create. The availabil- requests for items or activities that are not avail-
ity of digital cameras, including those on smart- able at a given time. Declining such requests can
phones, allow practitioners to take high-quality lead to problematic behavior; thus, instruction
photos of relevant items, places, activities, etc. and/or information regarding availability and
There are also various applications that include nonavailability is essential. Further, while the
libraries of over 50,000 graphic symbols (e.g., presence of photographs and/or line-drawing
Boardmaker® and SymbolStix PRIME®). symbols can serve as an SD for engaging in a
Produced correctly, with corresponding text-­ communicative response, it may be difficult to
based labels, photographs and line-drawing sym- determine whether that response is under the
bols are relatively easy to understand by most control of the motivating operation (MO) or sim-
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 381

ply a function of stimulus control (i.e., was the 20.3.2 Strategies or Techniques
request made based on actual desire or simply
because the symbol was visible?). For the purposes of this chapter, the following
discussion will focus on AAC systems that incor-
Text  Text and alphabet-based systems focus on porate the use of aided symbols, with a particular
the use of written words to communicate. focus on nonelectronic and electronic systems
Individuals may produce text via writing/print- that incorporate pointing or exchanging as com-
ing, typing on an electronic device, or pointing municative behaviors. Therefore, we will review
to/selecting printed words on a communication AAC systems that involve (a) exchange-based
display or speech-generating devices (SGDs). techniques, (b) point-based techniques, and (c)
Text may also be used to enhance receptive com- alphabetic techniques. Embedded within this dis-
munication (e.g., written schedules and social cussion will be information on the use of non-
narratives). electronic vs. electronic aids, as appropriate.

Advantages of text  For those with the ability 20.3.2.1 Exchange-Based Techniques
to read and write, the use of text for communi- Exchange-based communication systems, such
cation purposes results in ease of communica- as the Picture Exchange Communication System
tion across most environments and (PECS®) (Bondy & Frost, 1994), are among the
communication partners, especially given the most well-researched and widely implemented
increased use of text-based communication in aided AAC systems. When using an exchange-­
the nondisabled population (e.g., e-mail and based technique, the learner is taught to give one
text messaging; Ganz et al., 2014b). Certainly, or more symbols to a communicative partner
those who are able to independently produce (Flores et  al., 2012). Exchange-based systems
text have the potential to say anything, com- can include the use of tangible and/or tactile sym-
pared to those who are reliant on systems where bols, photographs, line-drawing symbols, and/or
facilitators select and provide vocabulary word cards. While AAC systems incorporating
(Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013). For those who exchange-based techniques have traditionally
are unable to produce text (i.e., spell) yet have utilized nonelectronic materials (i.e., laminated
sufficient sight-word reading or decoding skills, symbols), it is possible to develop exchange-­
multiple words can be placed on a single page based techniques using handheld technology
or display; as such, the vocabulary presented (Wendt et al., 2019). A comprehensive discussion
can be more expansive than photographs or of PECS® (Bondy & Frost, 1994) can be found
graphic symbols. Finally, the availability of in this textbook, in the corresponding chapter.
word-predictive software across consumer
devices (e.g., smartphones and tablets) can Advantages of exchange-based techniques  A
assist learners in producing text during commu- key advantage to exchange-based systems is the
nicative interactions. clarity and ease with which communication part-
ners, including young peers, are able to compre-
Disadvantages of text  The overarching disad- hend the learner’s messages (Mirenda & Fossett,
vantage to text-based AAC systems is the reli- 2011; Nam et al., 2018). As these systems tend to
ance on literacy skills. At the very least, the user produce timely and consistent responses, there is
must be able to recognize a large number of a greater consistency of reinforcement in the nat-
printed words. As well, communication partners ural setting, thereby facilitating maintenance and
must be able to read and/or write. This require- generalization of communication responses.
ment precludes the use of text-based systems Additionally, exchange-based systems can be
with young children or those who have learning designed to be relatively portable, with little
disabilities that impact decoding, comprehen- expense (Mirenda & Fossett, 2011). Given that
sion, and/or spelling skills. these systems are selection-based, the response
382 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

required by the learner (i.e., selecting a symbol “What do you want?” and being prompted to say
and giving it to a communication partner) is static “break” following an indication of motivation for
across all messages; the reduced complexity and a “break,” the learner would need to scan between
response effort may be particularly helpful for symbols (while subvocally echoing, “break”),
those with significant intellectual and develop- find the correct symbol (tact), select the correct
mental delays. Additionally, with the act of symbol (listener behavior), and give the symbol
obtaining joint attention is embedded within the to the communication partner (mand). While
act of communicating (i.e., the symbol is placed these complex verbal behaviors are not necessar-
directly in the hand of the communication part- ily prerequisites to the successful use of
ner), limited joint attention skills do not impede exchange-based systems, the absence of these
communication. repertoires may result in challenges to acquisi-
tion and spontaneous independent usage, requir-
Disadvantages of exchange-based tech- ing systematic prompting and prompt-fading
niques  One disadvantage of exchange-based methods for teaching accordingly.
systems is the reliance on external equipment; in
order for communication to occur, the symbols, 20.3.2.2 Point-Based Techniques
usually housed in a communication book, must Point-based techniques can be applied to non-
be present and readily available at all times electronic communication displays (e.g., com-
(Mirenda, 2003; Sundberg & Partington, 1998). munication boards and books), “light” tech SGDs
In addition, learners must seek out such materials (e.g., devices or apps with limited messages), or
in order to communicate. While there are “high”-tech SGDs (e.g., dedicated SGDs, smart-
research-supported procedures targeting the phones or tablets with AAC apps). All of these
search for and retrieval of materials (Sigafoos systems involve the learner pointing to a or
et  al., 2004), this may present challenges for touching specific areas of a non-technological
those with comorbid physical impairments who display that is “read” by a communication part-
lack the ability to move freely about their envi- ner or by touching specific symbols on a SGD.
ronment. As well, at least in some cases, the Nonelectronic communication displays con-
response effort required for exchange-based tain symbols (e.g., photographs and line-drawing
communication may be incommensurate with the symbols) to represent vocabulary and/or mes-
response effort for problematic behaviors. For sages. They are typically individualized to the
example, to request a “break,” a learner may need learner; vocabulary is selected and organized
to locate and/or go to the communication book, based on the learner’s needs, as well as the con-
flip through and visually scan several pages of texts and settings in which the learner partici-
symbols, locate and select the desired symbol, pates, etc. Displays may be created around
and travel to the communication partner, all specific topics (e.g., a communication board for
before making the exchange. These steps can individual activities), or communication books
delay access to reinforcement and may result in may contain vocabulary organized in other ways
the learner reverting to a more efficient, yet prob- (e.g., by parts of speech).
lematic, response (such as throwing work materi- “Light”-tech SGDs include single-message
als). Further, the selection of a picture from a devices (e.g., BIGMack; AbleNet, Inc.), sequen-
display of options requires both simple and con- tial message devices (e.g., StepbyStep; AbleNet,
ditional discrimination skills (i.e., being able to Inc.), or static-display SGDs (e.g., GoTalk;
select a given symbol in an array of other sym- Attainment Co.). These devices all use digitized,
bols), as well as some relative form of mand, or recorded, speech and can be “reprogrammed”
echoic, tact, and listener behavior repertoires easily by re-recording new messages. Single
which serve as the building blocks of more com- message devices allow the learner to convey one
plex verbal behaviors (e.g., bidirectional naming message only (e.g., “break, please”). Sequential
skills) (Miguel, 2016). For example, when asked, message devices allow the user to convey a stan-
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 383

dard sequence of messages (e.g., calling out “go,” and/or program. Dynamic display SGDs, in par-
“go,” “go,” “stop” during a game of “Go, Go, ticular, present with several additional advan-
Stop!”). Static-display devices contain a limited tages. Given that the use of handheld technologies
number of messages, ranging from as few as 2 to is well-established within society, the use of
just over 100 messages, depending on the device. SGDs in general, and SGD apps on handheld
With the advent of technologies such as smart- devices, in particular, is ostensibly more norma-
phones and tablets, there are currently a multi- tive and socially valid (Lorah et al., 2013). The
tude of apps that perform the same functions as vocal speech produced by SGDs is more easily
single message, sequential message, and static-­ understood by individuals who interact with the
display SGDs. learner, and the communicative partner does not
“High”-tech SGDs employ touch screen and need to be directly attending to the individual in
dynamic display technology, thereby allowing order to receive messages (Lorah et  al., 2013).
for a larger, albeit infinite, vocabulary. These Furthermore, SGDs are more adaptable, flexible,
devices utilize digitized (recorded) or synthetic and efficient than physical exchange (Aydin &
(computerized) speech. Dynamic display SGDs Diken, 2020). SGDs are modified with greater
present vocabulary and messages via, in essence, ease and can include an unlimited number of
a menu-based system, similar to folders, subfold- words and messages; this is in contrast to non-
ers, and files on personal computers. The “main electronic and “light”-tech systems, which are
page” may display a number of topic areas (e.g., limited with regard to physical space and capac-
“school,” “chit chat,” and “games”). When one ity (Nam et al., 2018). Overall, the user-friendly
topic is selected, the screen changes to show a nature of SGDs, along with the high degree of
secondary menu and/or vocabulary related to that acceptability and social validity, may help pro-
topic. For example, if the user selects “school” mote maintenance and generalization of commu-
from the main menu, the secondary page may nication skills across a variety of environments,
present school-related menu items (e.g., “math,” communication partners, and con-
“reading,” and “recess”). If the user then selects texts (McNaughton & Light, 2013).
“recess,” vocabulary and messages related to
recess would appear on the screen. Such systems Disadvantages of point-based tech-
are significantly more complex and, therefore, niques  Despite these advantages, there are
more expensive. To address the complexity of important considerations with regard to point-­
multiple menus, there are some dynamic display based techniques. A key concern relates to issues
SGDs whereby messages are accessed via com- obtaining and maintaining joint attention. Point-­
binations of symbols, rather than by categories. based, nonelectronic systems require joint visual
This can reduce the cognitive demand and time attention; the learner must engage in a response
required to access individual messages. Whereas to gain the communication partner’s attention
dedicated, dynamic display AAC devices can prior to producing the communication response.
cost several thousand US dollars, the popularity This can be particularly difficult for learners with
and accessibility of smartphones and tablets have ASD and was, in fact, a primary driver in the
led to a plethora of dynamic display SGD apps, development of PECS® (Bondy & Frost, 1994).
such as Proloquo2Go, TouchChat, and Speak for Even when using point-based electronic systems,
Yourself, that can be purchased for, at most, a few the learner must ensure that a communication
hundred US dollars. partner is nearby and able to hear the device,
prior to activation.
Advantages of point-based techniques  Overall, Availability and portability are also important
point-based techniques are fairly simple to use. considerations. Whether nonelectronic or elec-
Nonelectronic communication boards and books, tronic, aided AAC systems need to be present
and light-tech SGDs, are relatively inexpensive across all environments in order for communica-
and portable. They are also fairly easy to develop tion to occur. In the case of electronic SGDs, not
384 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

only do they need to be physically present, they menus to arrive at a message after several “hits,”
have to work; issues with battery depletion, inter- such apps require a maximum of two “hits” to
net connectivity, and app function can interfere access any message within the system.
with effective communication. Additionally,
SGDs are generally more expensive and vulner-
able to wear and tear than nonelectronic AAC 20.4 A
 AC Assessment and System
systems (Mirenda & Fossett, 2011). There are Selection
also some settings or activities that could damage
technology (e.g., swimming pool and skiing) or Selecting an appropriate AAC system for a given
specific topographies of problematic behavior learner can be a daunting task. In order to effec-
(e.g., self-injury and aggression) that may dam- tively design individualized AAC systems, prac-
age or destroy devices. Actions can be taken, titioners must be well-versed in AAC assessment
however, to prevent and/or mitigate damage to approaches and possess sufficient knowledge and
SGDs, including the use of cases and/or screen expertise regarding available AAC options and
covers to reduce overall wear and tear, the pur- interventions in order to select the most appropri-
chase of device protection plans that include ate AAC system for a given learner. As well, one
device repair. must stay abreast of AAC-related technological
There is an additional drawback specific to advances, which occur rapidly. Practitioners need
handheld technologies, which embed access to to assess (a) communication needs, (b) opportu-
leisure, entertainment, academic tools, calendars/ nity barriers (i.e., policies and/or practices that
visual schedules, and the Internet within a single may interfere with AAC intervention; knowledge
device (Lorah et al., 2013). The presence of these and/or skill deficits that may interfere with AAC
additional device functions can interfere with the intervention), (c) current capabilities, and (d)
reliable use of the device for communication. As cognitive abilities, particularly in relation to sym-
such, general clinical recommendations are that a bolic understanding, and language and literacy
single device be used solely for AAC. This ensures skills (Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013). If there are
that the user always has access to communica- comorbid diagnoses, such as hearing, vision, and/
tion, regardless of the activity, and addresses or motor impairments, assessment regarding
issues regarding preference for “leisure” apps these functional areas will also be necessary.
(e.g., games) over communication. If the use of Practitioners also need to consider many contex-
additional apps for reinforcement and leisure tual variables that will also impact the degree to
activities are desired, these should be provided on which a specific AAC system and/or intervention
a secondary device. approach is successful. These can include ­cultural
Lastly, the sheer amount pages and symbols factors, environments in which the learner par-
can be overwhelming, particularly when using ticipates, parent comfort with technology, access
dynamic display SGDs; the complexity of navi- to specific resources, and so on. Practitioners
gating through many pages and symbols is com- then need to integrate assessment information to
pounded when an individual does not demonstrate inform the development of an appropriate, indi-
fluency with simple and/or conditional discrimi- vidualized AAC system. Considerations include,
nation. To address this issue, some dedicated but are not limited to, (a) the most appropriate
SGDs and SGD apps allow for the modification type(s) of symbol(s) for the learner/context, (b)
of display settings while the learner is acquiring the manner in which symbols are organized, (c)
specific vocabulary items, including “blackout” the type of message output, (d) the visual display
options to simplify the display and reduce the features (e.g., color, spacing, and size of sym-
number of symbols available. Further, some apps bols), and (e) whether or not to include a key-
(e.g., Speak for Yourself) are designed to require board (Abbot & McBride, 2014).
fewer “hits” to access target messages. Rather A number of formal and informal assessment
than navigating through complex menus and sub-­ methods have been used across several decades
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 385

as part of the AAC assessment and system selec- on the belief that they were “too disabled” to ben-
tion process; given that the use of AAC interven- efit from AAC (Hourcade et al., 2004). From both
tions for individuals with ASD is relatively new, a behavior analytic and ethical perspective, can-
much of the research and practice has historically didacy criteria were highly subjective and pre-
focused on those with severe speech and physical vented many from accessing communication
impairments. Overall, there is limited research on interventions that could have been pivotal to the
clinical decision-making processes related to the development of their functional communication
selection and design of individualized AAC sys- skills (Mirenda, 2017).
tems (Schlosser & Raghavendra, 2004); even
highly skilled AAC practitioners find the 20.4.1.2 Communication Needs
decision-­ making process complicated (Boesch Model
et al., 2016). In order to better understand the fac- In the 1980s, the communication needs model
tors both relevant and irrelevant to AAC assess- (Beukelman & Mirenda, 1998) emerged, with a
ment and system identification, it is important to specific focus on addressing the gap between cur-
review the history of AAC assessment practices. rent abilities and presenting needs of the learner.
Throughout the 1980s and 1990s, this model
guided practitioners in identifying candidates for
20.4.1 AAC Assessment Models AAC, determining which AAC approach would
best address the learner’s unmet communication
20.4.1.1 Candidacy Model needs and planning for implementation in home,
Prior to the 1970s, communication interventions school, and/or community settings. While some-
commonly focused on the development of what less restrictive, this approach to assessment
speech, rather than AAC.  As such, only those continued to make predictions regarding AAC
persons able to produce, or imitate, vocalizations, success based on cognitive ability; as such, ser-
who possessed sufficient cognitive capacity to vice provision was limited to those with, at worst,
understand and produce verbal language were mild cognitive impairments  (Kangas & Lloyd,
selected for treatment (Kent et  al., 1972; 1988).
Hourcade et  al., 2004). Throughout the 1970s
and 1980s, AAC interventions emerged that were 20.4.1.3 Participation Model
guided by decision-making models, including the The participation model (Beukelman & Mirenda,
candidacy model. The goal of the candidacy 2013), which surfaced in the 1990s, presumed
model was to (a) identify individuals likely to that all individuals with disabilities, regardless of
benefit from AAC and (b) determine when it severity, could benefit from appropriate, individ-
would be most appropriate to provide AAC inter- ualized AAC intervention. The participation
vention. Because it was believed that AAC would model stresses (a) learner strengths, (b) access to
interfere with the development of speech, practi- quality AAC interventions, and (c) sufficient
tioners were cautious regarding the selection of opportunities to communicate during meaningful
recipients for AAC services. Typically, individu- and motivating activities in the natural environ-
als needed to demonstrate “failure” to develop ment (Iacono et  al., 2016;  Mirenda & Iacono,
vocal-verbal behavior following intensive speech 1990). As illustrated in Fig.  20.1, this model
therapy before they could be considered for AAC guides clinical decision-making by first consider-
services. Candidates for AAC had to have rela- ing discrepancies between the learner’s commu-
tively equivalent chronological and developmen- nication and participation and that of their peers.
tal/intellectual “ages” (i.e., measures of Once discrepancies are identified, assessment
intelligence within the normal range of function- focuses on both the learner and the context(s). As
ing); those with intellectual and/or developmen- with prior models, numerous learner skills and
tal disabilities, including those with ASD, were challenges are identified, in order to determine
typically excluded as candidates for AAC, based which AAC methods will likely be effective.
386 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

Within this model, cognitive skills are assessed, determined based on an analysis of individual
but not for the purpose of “weeding out” those capabilities and constraints; necessary environ-
with more significant challenges; instead, such mental adaptations are put in place to facilitate
assessment assists the team in identifying the and encourage communication; and additional
most appropriate form(s) of symbols, instruc- interventions are identified to increase natural
tional strategies, and other related factors to abilities that may further facilitate the develop-
ensure communication success. Interventions are ment of communication skills (e.g., ongoing

Fig. 20.1  Participation model (Beukelman & Light, 2020)


20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 387

speech therapy). An examination of the context(s) 2018; Sievers et al., 2020). Predictors associated
in which the learner participates reveals opportu- with positive AAC outcomes for individuals with
nity barriers and supports that may help or hinder ASD include (a) cognitive ability, (b) severity of
the implementation of AAC interventions. An ASD symptoms, (c) language comprehension,
awareness of these barriers and supports allow (d) language use, and (e) communication com-
practitioners to (a) address barriers (e.g., provid- plexity/competence (Vandereet et  al., 2011;
ing training and information to improve facilita- Pasco & Tohill, 2011). Further, moderators of
tor skill and knowledge) and/or (b) take advantage AAC success that have been identified include (a)
of opportunity supports (e.g., beginning interven- joint attention, (b) object exploration, (c) verbal
tion with a staff member who is enthusiastic imitation (i.e., echoics), (d) motor imitation, and
regarding AAC intervention). (e) matching skills (Gregory et al., 2009; Layton,
From a participation model framework, there 1988; Yoder & Compton, 2004). While predictors
is the understanding that an individual’s AAC use and moderators are comprised of within-learner
is ever evolving; thus, the team plans AAC inter- characteristics, mediators are entirely comprised
ventions and supports for “today” as well as for of factors outside of the learner. These include (a)
“tomorrow.” Instruction and support to the learner AAC knowledge of the communication partner,
and facilitators (e.g., parents, school staff, and (b) perception of AAC, (c) frequency of AAC
peers) is provided by a practitioner with AAC exposure, and (d) adult input (Sievers et  al.,
expertise, and ongoing evaluation is conducted to 2018).
determine if AAC interventions result in increased While knowledge of these predictors, modera-
participation and communication. If not, partici- tors, and mediators may have some utility in
pation supports and barriers are re-evaluated, matching AAC interventions to individual learn-
with changes made accordingly. ers, it is critical to recognize that current research
in this area is primarily correlational; factors such
as cognitive ability or severity of ASD symptoms
20.4.2 Additional AAC Assessment should not function as gatekeepers or prevent
Considerations access to AAC interventions. Instead, awareness
of these factors should guide the team in select-
20.4.2.1 Predictors, Moderators, ing AAC interventions most likely to support the
and Mediators development of communication skills via AAC;
Increasingly, researchers have attempted to iden- cognitive impairments should never impede
tify the predictors, moderators, and mediators access to AAC interventions.
that correspond with positive AAC outcomes.
Predictors have a main effect on the overall out- 20.4.2.2 Response Efficiency
comes; they include participant characteristics Another important consideration when selecting
associated with success. Moderators relate to the an AAC intervention for a given learner relates to
differential responding to various interventions; the concept of response efficiency. Within the
they can help identify the conditions and/or inter- behavior analytic literature, selection of a given
ventions most likely associated with success for a AAC system has been evaluated in direct relation
given learner. Mediators refer to specific factors to the matching law, concurrent schedules of
that are assessed during the delivery of an inter- reinforcement, and any problem behavior the
vention to help explain why and how individuals FCR is intended to replace (e.g., Ringdahl et al.,
respond. 2016). Response efficiency can be analyzed with
Analyses of results from group-based longitu- respect to the specific schedules of reinforcement
dinal experimental studies of AAC interventions provided for problem behavior(s) vs. an appro-
for learners with ASD have revealed a number of priate alternative; as long as problem behavior is
predictors, moderators, and mediators (Ganz reinforced on a less-frequent schedule than the
et  al., 2011, 2012, 2014a; 2015;  Sievers et  al., appropriate alternative, the matching law predicts
388 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

that the individual will allocate responding to the vidual’s preference regarding one or more AAC
response that most efficiently accesses the rein- system components, particularly when working
forcer (i.e., the FCR). Therefore, regardless of the with individuals who have difficulty discriminat-
symbol(s) and/or techniques used, practitioners ing or effectively communicating choices or
must ensure that communication via AAC is preferences.
more efficient with regard to accessing reinforce- Researchers have evaluated methods to incor-
ment when compared to the problem behavior(s); porate individual preference when selecting AAC
doing so will aid in both promoting the acquisi- components and devising AAC systems (Lund
tion and maintenance of the appropriate FCR et al., 2017). Over the past decade, meta-analyses
(Horner & Day, 1991; Johnston, 2006; Kelley have focused on comparing learner responses to
et al., 2002). different AAC approaches, in order to identify
clear preferences. While some evidence suggests
20.4.2.3 Response Effort that individuals with ASD may prefer exchange-­
Directly related to response efficiency is the based techniques over MS, and SGDs over both
overall effort required when using AAC. Behavior-­ exchange-based techniques and MS, significant
analytic research suggests that the allocation of variability and inconsistency in findings leave the
learners’ responses consistently match lowest question unanswered (Alzrayer et  al., 2014;
effort conditions (Ghaemmaghami et  al., 2018; Couper et  al., 2014; Flores et  al., 2012; Ganz
Horner & Day, 1991). In other words, for learners et al., 2012; Gevartner et al., 2013; Lorah et al.,
to acquire and maintain new communicative 2013; Muharib & Alzrayer, 2018).
responses via AAC, those responses must be eas- Further rigorous studies have evaluated pref-
ier to perform, compared to idiosyncratic and/or erence by arranging concurrent choice-making
problematic responses. Furthermore, response experiments where, following FCT and
effort and response efficiency are interconnected. communication-­ based training across different
If the response effort required to access rein- multiple AAC systems, the systems are simulta-
forcement via AAC is so high that it produces neously offered to the learner and “available” for
ratio strain (i.e., the “tipping point” between how usage. In these arrangements, the selection/utili-
much effort is needed to access a given rein- zation of the given AAC system does suggest
forcer), the individual will reallocate responding learner preferences (Cannella-Malone, 2018;
to the least effortful response (e.g., problem Kunnavatana et al., 2018; Ringdahl et al., 2016;
behavior). Therefore, it behooves practitioners to Sigafoos et  al., 2005, 2009; Son et  al., 2006;
pay particular attention to response effort and Torelli et  al., 2016; Winborn et  al., 2002;
response efficiency as it relates to any potential Winborn-Kemmerer et  al., 2009; Winborn-­
AAC intervention. Kemmerer et al., 2010); however preferences are
consistently idiosyncratic to each learner and
20.4.2.4 Preferences their presenting skills, making any overarching
It is well-established that interventions are more claims about preferences for all learners with
likely to be accepted and adopted when the indi- ASD moot (van der Meer et al., 2011). Further,
vidual preferences of the learner are incorpo- given that preferences may change over time,
rated. Methods for establishing “buy-in” and ongoing evaluation is necessary, particularly as
assent of the learner are critical components of the learner’s skills evolve and the presenting
behavior analytic interventions; thus, consider- environment(s) change (Lorah, 2016).
ation of learner preferences when selecting an Given that each learner with ASD will present
AAC intervention will substantially increase the with their own unique preferences, individual-
likelihood that AAC use will maintain long-term ized evaluation of preferences must drive the
(DeCarlo et al., 2019; Hanley et al., 1997; Tiger selection of AAC interventions. Furthermore,
et al., 2008; van der Meer et al., 2014). It may, recognition that AAC users with ASD comprise a
however, prove challenging to determine an indi- heterogenous group means that there will never
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 389

be a “one-size-fits-all” approach to AAC system (Donato et al., 2018). There are also barriers that
design (e.g., type of symbol, technique(s) used, are somewhat context-specific. The time required
and nonelectronic vs. electronic). Ultimately, for system development and maintenance and/or
decisions about AAC systems should not be a lack of facilitator training and implementation
based on diagnosis; rather, such decisions must support can interfere with AAC implementation.
be grounded in data on presenting behaviors, When working with electronic AAC systems, in
skills, needs, and preferences of the learner, in particular, additional barriers may include (a) a
relation to the individualized goals and the lack technical competence, (b) mechanical and
environment(s) within which the individual software issues, and (c) complexities of AAC
operates. software and apps, particularly for SGDs (Donato
et al., 2018). When implementing AAC in family
20.4.2.5 Assessment of Barriers settings, practitioners may encounter (a) parental
to and Facilitators of AAC perceptions that AAC will prevent oral speech
Intervention and associated resistance to AAC, (b) parental
Contextual fit, a relatively new construct that difficulty implementing AAC interventions
expands social and ecological validity, is an within the context of a busy, family life, (c)
important consideration when implementing any parental discouragement that results from the
type of intervention, including AAC interven- child’s lack of motivation, attention, and/or initi-
tions. Albin and colleagues (2002) suggested that ation, and (d) the presence of challenging
behavior support plans were more likely to be behaviors.
implemented with fidelity if strategies, proce- While an awareness of barriers can be daunt-
dures, and elements of the intervention aligned ing to the practitioner, it’s important to recognize
with the values, knowledge, skills, resources, and that thorough assessment and thoughtful inter-
supports available to implementers. Aspects vention planning can assuage the negative impact
related to contextual fit include factors that serve of these, and other, barriers.
as barriers and facilitators to intervention suc- The research and practice literature also iden-
cess. Research on contextual fit within the field of tifies a number of facilitators that can counteract
AAC has identified factors that impede or pro- barriers to successful AAC implementation. To
mote the implementation of AAC interventions increase the likelihood of contextual fit and, ulti-
(Johnston, 2006). Ultimately, the degree to which mately, successful AAC implementation, research
AAC interventions fit the broader context will recommends that services be (a) family-centered,
impact the overall effectiveness of AAC interven- (b) delivered by those with sufficient knowledge,
tions and must be considered as part of the assess- training, and expertise, and (c) collaborative in
ment process (Donato et al., 2018). nature (Donato et  al., 2018). To increase “buy-
Many barriers to successful AAC implementa- in,” research recommends that AAC interven-
­
tion relate directly to the systems in which learn- tions utilize technologies and methodologies that
ers receive educational and other service-based (a) are user-friendly, (b) require as little response
supports. These may include policies that restrict effort as possible, both for users and facilitators,
access to and funding for AAC services, materi- (c) embed additional features, including photo
als, and/or professionals with expertise in AAC and video technology and access to the Internet,
assessment and intervention. As well, a lack of (d) are used by the population at large (e.g.,
access to ongoing professional development smartphones and tablets), and (e) are multimodal
opportunities directly related to AAC assessment (e.g., incorporate aided and unaided approaches)
and intervention can serve as a further barrier to (Donato et  al., 2018). In the end, practitioners
high-quality AAC service provision. Finally, will need to evaluate a number of factors not
inconsistent and contradictory advice and infor- directly related to the learner in order to under-
mation provided to families and other consumers stand the broader contextual barriers and identify
of AAC services is an additional impediment relevant facilitators. If a given AAC intervention
390 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

is not feasible for the majority of the people in the they are socially, culturally, and ecologically
learner’s life, then compromises in the selection valid. These considerations thereby set the stage
of an AAC intervention, such that contextual fit for successful AAC intervention and, ultimately,
can be achieved, will significantly increase the the learner’s acquisition, maintenance, and gen-
likelihood of that intervention being adopted and eralization of communication skills.
implemented successfully.

20.4.2.6 Cultural Validity 20.5 B


 ehavior Analysis and AAC
Last, but certainly not least, we must consider Intervention
broader cultural- and diversity-related influ-
ences  (Hetzroni & Harris, 1996). Many of our AAC intervention practices have evolved tremen-
learners come from homes that include cultural dously over the last 70  years. While initially,
practices and languages different from our own. interventions focused solely on the development
Families may hold different beliefs and values of speech, a gradual shift in values and priorities
with regard to disability, communication, and the led to the emergence of methods to augment and/
role of professionals. It is imperative that practi- or replace speech for those unable to develop reli-
tioners recognize and incorporate diverse cultural able communication via speech. In the 1960s,
perspectives and influences into the assessment with the establishment of a behavioral approach
and intervention process. While beyond the scope to language, methodologies were devised to teach
of this chapter, readers are encouraged to engage speech and other verbal behavior (i.e., MS) to
in ongoing professional development related to children with language deficits, including those
working with families from cultural and linguis- with ASD.  This approach utilized a number of
tically diverse backgrounds. As it relates to AAC behavior analytic instructional components,
assessment and intervention, practitioners will including systematic prompting and fading meth-
need to be responsive and humble when pre- ods, shaping procedures, and reinforcement for
sented with differing values and beliefs; in par- target behaviors. Systematic data collection
ticular, assessment and intervention planning guided the selection and evaluation of specific
should account for a given learner’s cultural and instructional components for individual learners
linguistic background (e.g., inclusion of cultur- while emphasizing communication skills relevant
ally relevant vocabulary and inclusion of multiple for a learner’s functional environment. Based on
languages on AAC displays and/or SGDs) the erroneous belief that aided AAC systems
would prevent the acquisition of speech, this
20.4.2.7 A  Summary of AAC early behavioral approach to language tended to
Assessment Considerations exclude the use of aided symbols and, instead,
While much attention has focused on the identifi- promoted the use of MS and gestures (Hourcade
cation of appropriate symbol(s) and technique(s), et  al., 2004; Ogletree & Harn, 2001; Zangari
it is ultimately important to attend to these and et al., 1994).
other essential factors, including (a) predictors, During the 1970s and 1980s, the focus of lan-
moderators, and mediators of favorable out- guage interventions shifted further to consider
comes, (b) overall response effort, efficiency, and the pragmatic function of language; interventions
history in relation to reinforcement, (c) individ- focused primarily on the ways in which commu-
ual and family preferences, and (d) social, cul- nicative behavior operated within naturally
tural, and ecological validity. A thorough and occurring social contexts (Bryen & Joyce, 1985;
thoughtful assessment, conducted by a knowl- Guess et al., 1976). Interventions focused on the
edgeable and skilled practitioner, in collaboration use of unaided and/or aided AAC methods, within
with families and other professionals, will commonly occurring routines, to improve func-
increase not only the likelihood that AAC tional communication skills (Zangari et  al.,
intervention(s) match the individual but also that 1994). Over the ensuing decades, to the present
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 391

day, the field of AAC evolved to include (a) indi- While beyond the scope of this chapter to pro-
viduals who, in the past, were deemed ineligible vide an in-depth overview of NaTS, essentially,
for AAC interventions due to intellectual impair- this approach involves careful planning and envi-
ments and (b) technologies that became increas- ronmental arrangement to provide increased
ingly available to consumer populations and motivation and opportunities to communicate.
lower cost. Particularly with the emergence of Based on learner interests, opportunities are con-
handheld devices in the twenty-first century, trived by withholding, blocking, and/or disrupt-
along with a plethora of AAC apps that can be ing access to preferred items, activities, and/or
purchased by anyone for, at most, a few hundred interactions, which increases motivation to com-
dollars, the use of SGDs has exploded. municate (Cosbey & Johnston, 2006; Ganz et al.,
While speech and language pathologists (SLP) 2019; Sennott & Mason, 2016). Modeling,
have traditionally provided AAC-related ser- prompting and prompt fading, and shaping are
vices  (Clarke & Williams, 2020), the use of utilized to promote FCRs, which are followed by
behavior analytic interventions to target commu- specific reinforcing consequences (Hart & Risley,
nication skills, along with the increased accessi- 1978).
bility to handheld devices and AAC apps, and the
wide adoption of ABA-based interventions for
individuals with ASD has resulted in behavior 20.5.2 Discrete Trial Teaching
analysts becoming increasingly responsible, at
least in part, for the design and implementation of While NaTS are consistently utilized for teaching
AAC interventions. AAC interventions, increas- functional and social communication skills via
ingly incorporated into comprehensive ABA pro- AAC within natural settings, the use of DTT can
gramming, are typically provided within the also be advantageous for teaching specific AAC-­
context of naturalistic teaching strategies (NaTS) related skills to learners with ASD (Rabideau
and/or discrete trial training (DTT) methods. et  al., 2018). DTT is an empirically supported,
behavior analytic instructional procedure consist-
ing of five key components: (a) the presentation
20.5.1 Naturalistic Teaching of both an establishing operation (EO) and an SD
Strategies (e.g., instruction), (b) a specific prompt (as
needed), (c) the response from the learner, (d) the
The use of NaTS to promote functional commu- delivery of a consequence (e.g., reinforcer), and
nication within natural environments has become (e) a brief intertrial interval (Smith, 2001). DTT
central to AAC interventions over the past is commonly implemented under controlled,
40 years (Bondy & Frost, 1994; Charlop-Christy distraction-­free environments; however, these
et  al., 2002). Akin to Milieu teaching (Hart & procedures have evolved to become increasingly
Rogers-Warren, 1978; McDuffie, 2013), inciden- more natural, such that they can be implemented
tal teaching (Hart & Risley, 1978; McGee et al., within the context of NaTS.
1999), naturalistic environment teaching (NET) DTT may be most useful when targeting a
(Sundberg & Partington, 1998), and FCT (Carr & specific AAC-related responses, such as exchang-
Durand, 1985), NaTS target functional commu- ing a symbol, discriminating between symbols,
nication skills using activities that are reinforcing or activating a specific location on an SGD. While
and frequently accessible to learners within their DTT can result in relatively rapid acquisition of
everyday environments. Although not specific to target skills, generalization is not guaranteed
AAC, NaTS may target mands (i.e., requests), without systematically incorporating such skills
tacts (i.e., labels), and/or intraverbals (i.e., con- within the natural environment (Sundberg &
versations and social questions and responses) Partington, 1998). Therefore, when teaching
relevant to the learner’s presenting environment AAC-related skills via DTT, it is especially
and routines. important to program for generalization at the
392 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

onset of intervention. This may include engaging US Department of Education, 2016). While AAC
in multiple trials using multiple different stimuli interventions have been implemented increas-
(i.e., multiple exemplar training), across multiple ingly with learners who have ASD over the past
individuals, in multiple environments. five decades, one must consider the degree to
which the use of AAC methodologies is empiri-
cally supported, effective, and evidence-based.
20.5.3 Additional Instructional Definitions of evidence-based vary depending
Considerations upon the field of study and research methodol-
ogy. In single-subject research, which guides
Perhaps the most important aspect of AAC much of the work in both ABA and AAC, inter-
instruction relates to the infusion of communica- ventions are deemed to be evidence based when a
tion via AAC throughout the day, across contexts minimum of 5 high-quality studies, across at
and environments. Consider a typically develop- least 3 different research teams, and including at
ing baby. Parents, and others, speak thousands of least 20 participants all show evidence of a strong
words each day; language and communication functional relationship between the independent
are modeled daily, for approximately 1  year, variable (i.e., intervention) and the dependent
before it is expected that a child will begin speak- variable (i.e., target behavior) (Kratochwill et al.,
ing. Compare that to individuals with develop- 2013).
mental disabilities, including those with ASD, Research regarding AAC practices follow one
provided with an AAC system. In many circum- of two paths: research-to-practice and practice-­
stances, no one in that learner’s environment to-­research (Mirenda, 2017). Research that fol-
models language and communication using the lows a research-to-practice path focuses on
same AAC system. Despite this, there are expec- determining efficacy (i.e., the degree to which
tations that the learner will somehow become a behavior changes when intervention procedures
fluent AAC user. In conjunction with high-­quality are implemented in a highly controlled environ-
instructional practices, implementation of AAC ment by highly skilled intervention agents) and/
should mirror the way in which typically devel- or effectiveness (i.e., degree to which ­intervention
oping infants are exposed to language and com- procedures produce good outcomes when imple-
munication. Others in the environment should mented in the natural environment by natural
use the AAC system to communicate, thereby intervention agents). Research that follows a
providing a model; in addition, the AAC system practice-to-research path focuses on validating
should be available at all times, during all activi- interventions commonly applied in natural set-
ties. Given that we would not expect a typically tings. According to the Association for Science in
developing child to acquire vocal-verbal commu- Autism Treatments (ASAT), a definitive source
nication skills in the absence of intense modeling for disseminating autism-specific, evidence-­
by its verbal community, we should not expect based practice, data supporting the benefits of
that those with ASD will develop AAC fluency in AAC are “limited,” with the exception of those
the absence of robust AAC modeling. interventions that blend and incorporate ABA
methods within the context of AAC intervention
(ASAT, 2021; National Research Council, 2001).
20.6 AAC and Evidenced-Based
Practice
20.6.1 Effectiveness and Efficacy
Identifying methodologically sound studies and of AAC Interventions
evaluating the quality of research on a given topic
is critical to ensuring the credibility of recom- In the past 20  years, a plethora of studies have
mendations regarding effective interventions for investigated the overall effectiveness and efficacy
use within applied settings (Horner et al., 2005; of an assortment of AAC interventions, imple-
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 393

mented across different settings and contexts, implemented within the context of behavior-­
with a variety of learners. As well, researchers analytic programming to improve specific com-
have conducted meta-analyses of AAC research, munication skills and/or verbal repertoires.
particularly in relation to learners with ASD, to Specific components of AAC systems should be
identify practices that can be considered selected based on the individualized needs of the
evidenced-­based (Aydin & Diken, 2020; Ganz user, family, and relevant context(s) rather than
et  al., 2012;  Ganz et  al.  2015; Gevartner et  al., the notion that one specific AAC approach is
2013; Holyfield et al., 2017; Logan et al., 2017; “appropriate” for all learners with ASD.
Lorah et  al., 2015; Muharib & Alzrayer, 2018; It is also important to recognize that much of
Nam et  al., 2018; Schlosser & Raghavendra, the AAC literature, particularly in relation to
2004; Son et al., 2006; van der Meer & Rispoli, those with ASD, has focused primarily on estab-
2010; van der Meer et al., 2011). These compre- lishing FCRs often as a replacement for problem
hensive reviews reveal that, in general, (a) behavior. There is significantly less research on
selection-­ based systems (e.g., SGDs; systems the combination of AAC and ABA interventions
that incorporate physical exchange) are slightly to establish and/or expand other communication
more effective than MS in the acquisition of skills, such as those related to social interaction,
functional communication, social interaction literacy, and/or complex verbal behavior. As
skills, academic behaviors, and the reduction of such, ongoing research must focus on the ways in
problematic behavior and (b) systems incorporat- which behavior-analytic instruction can effec-
ing physical exchange and SGDs are equally tively incorporate AAC interventions to increase
effective (Barlow et  al., 2013; Boesch et  al., these, and other, important skills.
2013; Couper et al., 2014; Holyfield et al., 2017;
Nam et  al., 2018; Tincani, 2004; van der Meer
et al., 2012). While it is important to note that not 20.7 F
 uture Directions of AAC
all published AAC research meets the high-­ for Individuals with ASD
quality research standards required to be consid-
ered an EBP (Aydin & Diken, 2020; Banda, AAC methods have evolved dramatically over
2018; Ganz et  al., 2017; Logan et  al., 2017; the past 70 years; the future for those who require
Morin et  al., 2018), there are sufficient studies AAC is seemingly bright and full of opportuni-
that do meet these criteria and provide direction ties. As AAC-related technologies and research
for current practice and ongoing continue to advance, the application of these
research (Schlosser & Sigafoos, 2009). interventions becomes more readily accessible,
It is also important to recognize that the spe- culturally sensitive, socially acceptable, and
cific AAC methodologies or interventions identi- empirically supported. At the same time, there
fied as EBPs are such only when provided within are some important considerations to keep in
the context of behavior-analytic interventions. mind as we work to improve and expand upon
This begs the question, then, if the primary focus current AAC services for individuals with
of our attention should be on (a) the effectiveness ASD (Light & McNaughton, 2012).
and/or efficacy-specific AAC modalities or sys- We are in desperate need for high-quality
tems (e.g., MS, PE, and SGDs) alone or (b) the research on all of the components of AAC inter-
effectiveness and/or efficacy of combined AAC ventions (e.g., types of symbols and types of sys-
and ABA interventions. From a behavior-analytic tems), as well as assessment approaches that
point of view, it would appear that any AAC assist in identifying the most appropriate AAC
modality or system is potentially effective, if interventions for individual learners. We also
taught using established, evidence-based, need to expand our research beyond the use of
behavior-­ analytic instructional practices. behavior-analytic interventions to target AAC use
Therefore, our research should target the ways in for the purpose of manding and increase our
which AAC interventions can be effectively focus on the use of such tactics to target all verbal
394 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

operants within natural settings. Given that there facilitators, the consideration and incorporation
is a rich body of literature to support the develop- of these variables into AAC-based programming
ment of complex vocal verbal behavior via is paramount. Researchers and practitioners will
behavior-analytic interventions, it stands to rea- benefit from formally evaluating social validity,
son that the same can be accomplished using treatment integrity, and the unique preferences of
AAC methodologies. That said, we do need to individuals who are part of the AAC interven-
better understand this through the execution of tions. This may also include more formalized
well-designed studies designed to meet the rigor- professional preparation and training programs
ous criteria for establishing practices as evidence-­ for family members and practitioners who will be
based. We also need to better understand the working with individuals who use AAC devices
relationship between predictors, moderators, and (Mirenda, 1997).
mediators and the acquisition, maintenance, and As well, it is important to recognize that, in
generalization of AAC by those with ASD. most circumstances, there is not one individual
There is also a significant need to expand on the team who possesses all of the relevant
AAC research and service provision beyond chil- knowledge, skills, and expertise to effectively
dren. While there has been some increased atten- implement AAC interventions for individuals
tion in AAC interventions for very young children with ASD. Historically falling within the skill set
(i.e., infancy through toddlerhood) identified of SLPs, the complexities of AAC are not well-­
with intellectual and/or developmental disabili- understood by behavior-analytic professionals.
ties, there is very little research focused on AAC Similarly, most SLPs are unlikely to possess an
interventions for adolescents or, especially, adults in-depth understanding of the behavior-analytic
and elderly persons (Holyfield et  al., 2017; tactics that facilitate the acquisition, mainte-
Trembath et  al., 2014). Given the increasing nance, and generalization of verbal behavior. In
degree to which adults with developmental dis- order to serve our learners best, it is crucial for
abilities participate in social, community, and SLPs and behavior analysts to respect one anoth-
vocational contexts, along with their increased er’s knowledge, skill, and expertise and
life expectancy, it is especially important to collaborate effectively throughout the AAC
­
determine effective approaches for providing assessment and intervention process. This means
AAC services to this group. that behavior analysts must (a) be open to differ-
With increased access to various AAC tech- ing perspectives and approaches and (b) dissemi-
nologies, questions have emerged regarding the nate information regarding behavior analytic
design of dynamic display systems, particularly practices in a way that is accessible to others
in relation to the organization of and access to (e.g., avoiding jargon). Remembering that one
vocabulary and messages. The manner in which barrier to AAC intervention, particularly for par-
dynamic display AAC systems can be organized ents, relates to conflicting opinions and recom-
is varied and vast. This is likely another aspect of mendations, it behooves us to develop the
AAC system selection that will need to be deter- necessary soft skills for effective collaborative
mined based on individualized assessment; there- consultation (e.g., humility, empathy, compas-
fore, assessment approaches and decision-making sion, and interpersonal skills). These skills may
standards should be established, such that teams position us to be better able to work in conjunc-
can accomplish this task efficiently. tion with SLPs and other professionals to design
Knowing that the degree to which any AAC the best, individualized AAC interventions pos-
intervention is successful is inextricably linked to sible, thereby increasing the likelihood that our
the acceptability and feasibility of that interven- learners, their families, and those who support
tion from the perspective of parents and other them experience success.
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 395

20.8 Conclusion grounded in respect and a desire to work in the


best interests of our learners. For individuals
The selection, design, and implementation of with ASD, communicating effectively, regard-
AAC interventions plays an integral role in facili- less of the modality, is both their right as indi-
tating the development of a learner’s communica- viduals and our ethical responsibility as
tion skills and, by proxy, that learner’s practitioners.
participation in a variety of other contexts,
including social, educational, and vocational.
Each individual learner deserves an individual- References
ized approach to AAC intervention, led by pro-
fessionals with sufficient expertise who Abbot, M., & McBride, D. (2014). Decision mak-
ing in mobile technology: Points to ponder. SIG
collaborate effectively. Such interventions must 12 Perspectives on Augmentative and Alternative
facilitate the learner’s ability to communicate in a Communication, 23, 104–111.
variety of contexts using any and all modalities Albin, R.  W., Dunlap, G., & Lucyshyn, J.  M. (2002).
that are appropriate and effective for that learner. Collaborative research with families on positive
behavior support. In J.  M. Lucyshyn, G.  Dunlap, &
This requires an astute assessment, not only of R.  W. Albin (Eds.), Families and positive behavior
individual strengths, needs, and preferences, but support: Including people with difficult behavior in
of the broader environment(s) and context(s) in the community (pp. 81–98). Brookes.
which the individual participates. When support- Alzrayer, N., Banda, D., & Koul, R. (2014). Use of iPad/
iPods with individuals with autism and other devel-
ing those with ASD, particular attention must opmental disabilities: A meta-analysis of communi-
focus on attending skills, motivation, and sponta- cation interventions. Review Journal of Autism and
neous initiations. Developmental Disorders, 1, 179–191.
Given the wealth of research supporting the American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
use of behavior-analytic interventions to promote https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
the development of vocal-verbal repertoires, in American Speech Language Hearing Association. (2005).
general, combined with emerging research that Roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathol-
supports the use of similar procedures to teach ogists with respect to augmentative and ­alternative
communication: Position statement. https://doi.
specific verbal operants (i.e., mands) via AAC org/10.1044/policy.PS2005-­00113
(e.g., MS and PE), it seems prudent to apply sim- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.
ilar tactics to (a) teach additional verbal operants (1989). Competencies for speech-language patholo-
(e.g., tacts and intraverbals), (b) use additional gists providing services in augmentative communica-
tion. ASHA, 31(3), 107–110.
AAC strategies and/or materials (e.g., SGDs), Association for Science in Autism Treatment. Autism
and (c) implement these across a wide variety of Treatments. Retrieved January 15, 2021 from https://
contexts and settings. asatonline.org/for-­­parents/learn-­more-­about-­specific-
Finally, there is one critical thing that we, as ­treatments/augmentative-­communication/
Aydin, O., & Diken, I.  H. (2020). Studies comparing
behavior analysts, must remember. The field of augmentative and alternative communication systems
AAC, like the field of ABA, is vast and com- (AAC) applications for individuals with autism spec-
plex; no single professional can meet all of the trum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
needs of a given learner. While we certainly Education & Training in Autism & Developmental
Disabilities, 55(2), 119–141.
bring a wealth of knowledge, skills, and exper- Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
tise to the table, there is so much more that we current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
do not know. We must concede that we also Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97.
lack knowledge and skills and appreciate the https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­91
Banda, D. R. (2018). The results of this systematic review
wealth of knowledge, skills, and expertise that on AAC with adolescents and adults with autism spec-
other professionals, including SLPs, bring to trum disorder has promising implications, despite
the table. Such professional humility will serve there being a limited number of studies. Evidence-­
us well in establishing and maintaining collab- Based Communication Assessment & Intervention,
orative professional partnerships that are
396 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

12(3), 81–84. https://doi.org/10.1080/17489539.2018 autism: Assessment of PECS acquisition, speech,


.1504423 social communicative behavior, and problem behavior.
Barlow, K. E., Tiger, J. H., Slocum, S. K., & Miller, S. J. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 213–231.
(2013). Comparing acquisition of exchange-based https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­213
and signed mands with children with autism. The Clarke, K.  A., & Williams, D.  L. (2020). Instruction
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 29(1), 59–69. https://doi. using augmentative and alternative communica-
org/10.1007/BF03393124 tion supports: Description of current practices by
Beukelman, D. R., & Mirenda, P. (1998). Communication speech-language pathologists who work with children
options of persons who cannot speak: Assessment with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of
and evaluation. In C. A. Coston (Ed.), Proceedings of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(2), 586–596. https://
the national planners’ conference on assistive device doi.org/10.1044/2019_AJSLP-­19-­00045
service delivery (pp.  151–165). Association for the Cosbey, J.  E., & Johnston, S. (2006). Using a single-­
Advancement of Rehabilitation Technology. switch voice output communication aid to increase
Beukelman, D. R., & Light, J. C. (2020). Augmentative & social access for children with severe disabilities in
alternative communication: Supporting children and inclusive classrooms. Research & Practice for Persons
adults with complex communication needs. with Severe Disabilities, 31, 144–156. https://doi.
Beukelman, D.  R., & Mirenda, P. (2013). Augmentative org/10.1177/154079690603100207
and alternative communication: Supporting children Couper, L., van der Meer, L., Schäfer, M. C., McKenzie,
& adults with complex communication needs (4th ed.). E., McLay, L., O’Reilly, M.  F., & Sutherland, D.
Brookes. (2014). Comparing acquisition of and preference
Boesch, M.  C., Wendt, O., Subramanian, A., & Hsu, N. for manual signs, picture exchange, and speech-­
(2013). Comparative efficacy of the picture exchange generating devices in nine children with autism spec-
communication system (PECS) versus a speech-­ trum disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation,
generating device: Effects on requesting skills. 17(2), 99–109. https://doi.org/10.3109/17518423.201
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(3), 480– 3.870244
493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.12.002 Creer, S., Enderby, P., Judge, S., & John, A. (2016).
Boesch, M.  C., Shukla Mehta, S., & Da Fonte, M.  A. Prevalence of people who could benefit from augmen-
(2016). Comparing multiple communication modali- tative and alternative communication (AAC) in the
ties yields mixed results indicating that a broad focus UK: Determining the need. International Journal of
may lead to narrow recommendations. Evidence-­ Language & Communication Disorders, 51(6), 639–
Based Communication Assessment & Intervention, 653. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-­6984.12235
10(2), 108–114. https://doi.org/10.1080/17489539.20 DeCarlo, J., Bean, A., Lyle, S., & Cargill, L. P. M. (2019).
11.701383 The relationship between operational competency,
Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1994). The picture exchange com- buy-in, and augmentative and alternative communica-
munication system. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 9, tion use in school-age children with autism. American
1–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/108835769400900301 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(2), 469–
Bryen, D.  N., & Joyce, D.  G. (1985). Language inter- 484. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-­17-­0175
vention with the severely handicapped: A decade of Donato, C., Spencer, E., & Arthur-Kelly, M. (2018). A
research. The Journal of Special Education, 19, 7–39. critical synthesis of barriers and facilitators to the use
https://doi.org/10.1177/002246698501900103 of AAC by children with autism spectrum disorder
Cannella-Malone, H.  I. (2018). Augmentative and alter- and their communication partners. Augmentative and
native communication interventions are effective for Alternative Communication, 34(3), 242–253. https://
adolescents and adults with autism spectrum dis- doi.org/10.1080/07434618.2018.1493141
order, but more work is needed. Evidence-Based Durand, V. M. (1999). Functional communication training
Communication Assessment & Intervention, 12(4), using assistive devices: Recruiting natural communi-
132–134. https://doi.org/10.1080/17489539.2018.15 ties of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior
45400 Analysis, 32(3), 247–267. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Carr, E., & Durand, M. (1985). Reducing behavior prob- jaba.1999.32-­247
lems through functional communication training. Flores, M., Musgrove, K., Renner, S., Hinton, V.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. Strozier, S., Franklin, S., & Hil, D. (2012). A com-
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­111 parison of communication using the Apple iPad and
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014). a picture-based system. Augmentative and Alternative
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among chil- Communication, 28, 74–84. https://doi.org/10.3109/0
dren aged 8 years-autism and developmental dis- 7434618.2011.644579
abilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, Ganz, J.  B. (2015). AAC interventions for individuals
2010. Morbidity and mortality weekly report. MMWR with autism spectrum disorders: State of the science
Surveillance Summaries, 63, 1–21. and future research directions. Augmentative and
Charlop-Christy, M., Carpenter, M., Le, L., LeBlanc, Alternative Communication, 31(3), 203–214. https://
L., & Kellet, K. (2002). Using the Picture Exchange doi.org/10.3109/07434618.2015.1047532
Communication System (PECS) with children with
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 397

Ganz, J. B., Earles-Vollrath, T. L., Mason, R. A., Rispoli, Guess, D., Sailor, W., Keogh, B., & Baer, D. M. (1976).
M. J., Heath, A. K., & Parker, R. I. (2011). An aggre- Language development programs for severely handi-
gate study of single-case research involving aided capped children. In N. G. Haring & L. J. Brown (Eds.),
AAC: Participant characteristics of individuals Teaching the severely handicapped (Vol. I, pp.  301–
with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism 324). Grune & Stratton.
Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1500–1509. https://doi. Hanley, G., Piazza, C., Fisher, W., Contrucci, S., &
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.02.011 Maglieri, K. (1997). Evaluation of client preference
Ganz, J., Earles-Vollrath, T., Heath, A., Parker, R., for function-based treatment packages. Journal of
Rispoli, M., & Duran, J. (2012). A meta-analysis of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 459–473. https://doi.
single case research studies on aided augmentative org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­459
and alternative communication systems with individu- Harris, M.  D., & Reichle, J. (2004). The impact of
als with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism aided language stimulation on symbol compre-
and Developmental Disorders, 42(1), 60–74. https:// hension and production in children with moderate
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­011-­1212-­2 cognitive disabilities. American Journal of Speech-­
Ganz, J.  B., Mason, R.  A., Goodwyn, F.  D., Boles, Language Pathology, 13, 155–167. https://doi.
M.  B., Heath, A.  K., & Davis, J.  L. (2014a). org/10.1044/1058-­0360(2004/016)
Interaction of participant characteristics and type Hart, S.  L., & Banda, D.  R. (2010). Picture Exchange
of AAC with individuals with ASD: A meta-­ Communication System with individuals with devel-
analysis. American Journal on Intellectual and opmental disabilities: A meta-analysis of single sub-
Developmental Disabilities, 119(6), 516–535. https:// ject studies. Remedial and Special Education, 31,
doi.org/10.1352/1944-­7558-­119.6.516 476–488. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932509338354
Ganz, J. B., Rispoli, M. J., Mason, R. A., & Hong, E. R. Hart, B., & Risley, T.  R. (1978). Promoting productive
(2014b). Moderation of effects of AAC based on set- language through incidental teaching. Education
ting and types of aided AAC on outcome variables: An and Urban Society, 10, 407–429. https://doi.
aggregate study of single-case research with individu- org/10.1177/001312457801000402
als with ASD. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, Hart, B., & Rogers-Warren, A. (1978). A milieu approach
17(3), 184–192. https://doi.org/10.3109/17518423.20 to teaching language. In R.  Schiefelbusch (Ed.),
12.748097 Language intervention strategies (pp.  193–235).
Ganz, J.  B., Morin, K.  L., Foster, M.  J., Vannest, K.  J., University Park Press.
Genç Tosun, D., Gregori, E. V., & Gerow, S. L. (2017). Hetzroni, O. E., & Harris, O. L. (1996). Cultural aspects
High-technology augmentative and alternative com- in the development of AAC users. Augmentative and
munication for individuals with intellectual and devel- Alternative Communication, 12, 52–58. https://doi.org
opmental disabilities and complex communication /10.1080/07434619612331277488
needs: A meta-analysis. Augmentative and Alternative Holyfield, C., Drager, K.  D. R., Kremkow, J.  M. D., &
Communication, 33(4), 224–238. https://doi.org/10.10 Light, J. (2017). Systematic review of AAC interven-
80/07434618.2017.1373855 tion research for adolescents and adults with autism
Ganz, J. B., Hong, E. R., Leuthold, E., & Yllades, V. (2019). spectrum disorder. Augmentative and Alternative
Naturalistic augmentative and alternative communica- Communication, 33(4), 201–212. https://doi.org/10.1
tion instruction for practitioners and individuals with 080/07434618.2017.1370495
autism. Intervention in School and Clinic, 55(1), Horner, R., & Day, H. (1991). The effects of response
58–64. https://doi.org/10.1177/1053451219833012 efficiency on functionally equivalent competing
Gevartner, C., O’Reilly, M.  F., Rojeski, L., Sammarco, behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24,
N., Lang, R., Lancioni, G. E., & Sigafoos, J. (2013). 719–732. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-­719
Comparisons of intervention components within aug- Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom,
mentative and alternative communication systems S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject
for individuals with developmental disabilities: A research to identify evidence-based practice in special
review of the literature. Research in Developmental education. Exceptional Children, 71, 165–179. https://
Disabilities, 34(12), 4404–4414. https://doi. doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203
org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.018 Hourcade, J., Pilotte, T. E., West, E., & Parette, P. (2004).
Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G. P., Jessel, J., & Landa, A history of augmentative and alternative communi-
R. (2018). Shaping complex functional communica- cation for individuals with severe and profound dis-
tion responses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, abilities. Focus on Autism & Other Developmental
51, 502–520. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.468 Disabilities, 19(4), 235–244. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Gregory, M. K., DeLeon, I. G., & Richman, D. M. (2009). 10883576040190040501
The influence of matching and motor-imitation Iacono, T., Trembath, D., & Erickson, S. (2016). The role
abilities on rapid acquisition of manual signs and of augmentative and alternative communication for
exchange-based communicative responses. Journal children with autism: Current status and future trends.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(2), 399–405. https:// Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 2349–
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­399 2361. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S95967
398 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

Johnston, S.  S. (2006). Considering response efficiency and Alternative Communication 33(1) 51-64
in the selection and use of AAC systems. Journal of 10.1080/07434618.2016.1267795
Speech-Language Pathology and Applied Behavior Lorah, E. R. (2016). Comparing teacher and student use
Analysis, 1(3), 193–206. and preference of two methods of augmentative and
Kangas, K., & Lloyd, L. (1988). Early cognitive skills alternative communication: Picture exchange and a
prerequisites to augmentative and alternative commu- speech-generating device. Journal of Developmental
nication use: What are we waiting for? Augmentative and Physical Disabilities, 28, 751–767. https://doi.
and Alternative Communication, 4, 211–221. https:// org/10.1007/s10882-­016-­9507-­z
doi.org/10.1080/07434618812331274817 Lorah, E. R., Tincani, M., Dodge, J., Gilroy, S., Hickey,
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con- A., & Hantula, D. (2013). Evaluating picture exchange
tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. and the iPad as a speech generating device to teach
Kelley, M., Lerman, D., & Van Camp, C. (2002). The communication to young children with autism. Journal
effects of competing reinforcement schedules on the of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 25(6),
acquisition of functional communication. Journal of 637–650. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­013-­9337-­1
Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 59–63. https://doi. Lorah, E., Parnell, A., Whitby, P., & Hantula, D. (2015).
org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­59 A systematic review of tablet computers and portable
Kent, L. R., Klein, D., Falk, A., & Guenther, H. (1972). A media players as speech generating devices for indi-
language acquisition program for the retarded. In J. E. viduals with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
McLean, D.  E. Yoder, & R.  L. Schiefelbusch (Eds.), Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 3792–3804.
Language intervention with the retarded (pp.  151– https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­014-­2314-­4
190). University Park Press. Lund, S. K., Quach, W., Weissling, K., McKelvey, M., &
Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J. H., Horner, R. H., Levin, Dietz, A. (2017). Assessment with children who need
J. R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., & Shadish, W. R. augmentative and alternative communication (AAC):
(2013). Single-case intervention research design stan- Clinical decisions of AAC specialists. Language,
dards. Remedial and Special Education, 34, 26–38. Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48(1),
https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932512452794 56–68. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_LSHSS-­15-­0086
Kunnavatana, S., Wolfe, K., & Aguilar, A. (2018). McDuffie,A. (2013). Milieu teaching. In F. R.Volkmar (Ed.),
Assessing mand topography preference when Encyclopedia of autism spectrum disorders. Springer.
developing a functional communication training. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­1-­4419-­1698-­3_1679
Behavior Modification, 42(3), 364–391. https://doi. McGee, G. G., Morrier, M., & Daly, T. (1999). An inci-
org/10.1177/0145445517751437 dental teaching approach to early intervention for tod-
Layton, T.  L. (1988). Language training with autistic dlers with autism. The Journal of the Association for
children using four different modes of presentation. Persons With Severe Handicaps, 24, 133–146. https://
Journal of Communication Disorders, 21, 333–350. doi.org/10.2511/rpsd.24.3.133
https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-­9924(88)90037-­8 McNaughton, D., & Light, J. (2013). The iPad and mobile
Light, J., & Drager, K. (2011). Early intervention for technology revolution: Benefits and challenges for
young children with autism, cerebral palsy, Down individuals who require augmentative and alterna-
syndrome, and other disabilities. Website retrieved tive communication. Augmentative and Alternative
October 12, 2011, from http://aackids.psu.edu Communication, 29, 107–116. https://doi.org/10.3109
Light, J., & McNaughton, D. (2012). Supporting the com- /07434618.2013.784930
munication, language, and literacy development of Miguel, C.  F. (2016). Common and intraverbal bidirec-
children with complex communication needs: State tional naming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32(2),
of the science and future research priorities. Assistive 125–138. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-­016-­0066-­2
Technology, 24, 34–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400 Millar, D.  C., Light, J.  C., & Schlosser, R.  W. (2006).
435.2011.648717 The impact of augmentative and alternative com-
Light, J.  C., Roberts, B., Dimarco, R., & Greiner, N. munication intervention on the speech produc-
(1998). Augmentative and alternative communication tion of individuals with developmental disabilities:
to support receptive and expressive communication A research review. Journal of Speech, Language,
for people with autism. Journal of Communication and Hearing Research, 49(2), 248–264. https://doi.
Disorders, 31, 153–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/ org/10.1044/1092-­4388(2006/021)
S0021-­9924(97)00087-­7 Mirenda, P. (1997). Supporting individuals with challeng-
Light, J.  C., Beukelman, D.  R., & Reichle, J. (2003). ing behavior through functional communication train-
Communicative competence for individuals who ing and AAC: Research review. AAC: Augmentative &
use AAC: From research to effective practice. Paul Alternative Communication, 13(4), 207–225. https://
H. Brooke. doi.org/10.1080/07434619712331278048
Logan, K., Iacono, T., & Trembath T. (2017) A sys- Mirenda, P. (2003). Toward functional augmentative and
tematic review of research into aided AAC to alternative communication for students with autism:
increase social-communication functions in chil- Manual signs, graphic symbols, and voice output
dren with autism spectrum disorder. Augmentative communication aids. Language, Speech, and Hearing
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 399

Services in Schools, 34(3), 203–216. https://doi. Ringdahl, J., Berg, W., Wacker, D., Ryan, S., Ryan, A.,
org/10.1044/0161-­1461(2003/017 Crook, K., & Molony, M. (2016). Further demonstra-
Mirenda, P. (2017). Values, practice, science, and tion of individual preference among mand modalities
AAC. Research & Practice for Persons with during functional communication training. Journal
Severe Disabilities, 42(1), 33–41. https://doi. of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 28(6),
org/10.1177/1540796916661163 905–917. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­016-­9518-­9
Mirenda, P., & Fossett, B. (2011). Augmentative commu- Schlosser, R.  W., & Raghavendra, P. (2004). Evidence-­
nication. In A.  Bondy & L.  Frost (Eds.), A picture’s based practice in augmentative and alternative
worth: PECS and other visual communication strate- communication. Augmentative and Alternative
gies in autism (2nd ed., pp. 43–64). Woodbine House. Communication, 20, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/07
Mirenda, P., & Iacono, T. (1990). Communication options 434610310001621083
for persons with severe and profound disabilities: State Schlosser, R.  W., & Sigafoos, J. (2009). Navigating
of the art and future directions. The Journal of the evidence-based information sources in augmentative
Association for Persons With Severe Handicaps, 15, and alternative communication. Augmentative and
3–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/154079699001500102 Alternative Communication, 25, 225–235. https://doi.
Morin, K.  L., Ganz, J.  B., Gregori, E.  V., Foster, M.  J., org/10.3109/07434610903360649
Gerow, S. L., Genç-Tosun, D., & Hong, E. R. (2018). Schlosser, R.  W., & Wendt, O. (2008). Effects of aug-
A systematic quality review of high-tech AAC inter- mentative and alternative communication interven-
ventions as an evidence-based practice. Augmentative tion on speech production in children with autism:
and Alternative Communication, 34(2), 104–117. A systematic review. American Journal of Speech-­
https://doi.org/10.1080/07434618.2018.1458900 Language Pathology, 17(3), 212–230. https://doi.
Mueller, V.  T. (2014). Alternative communication. In org/10.1044/1058-­0360(2008/021
F. Volkmar, S. Rogers, R. Paul, & K. Pelphrey (Eds.), Sennott, S.  C., & Mason, L.  H. (2016). AAC modeling
Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental dis- with the iPad during shared storybook reading pilot
orders (pp. 813–825). Wiley. study. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 37, 242–
Muharib, R., & Alzrayer, N.  M. (2018). The use of 254. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740115601643
high-tech speech-generating devices as an evidence-­ Sievers, S.  B., Trembath, D., & Westerveld, M. (2018).
based practice for children with autism spectrum A systematic review of predictors, moderators, and
disorders: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and mediators of augmentative and alternative commu-
Developmental Disorders, 5, 43–57. https://doi. nication (AAC) outcomes for children with autism
org/10.1007/s40489-­017-­0122-­4 spectrum disorder. Augmentative and Alternative
Nam, S., Kim, J., & Sparks, S. (2018). An overview of Communication, 34(3), 219–229. https://doi.org/10.1
review studies on effectiveness of major AAC sys- 080/07434618.2018.1462849
tems for individuals with developmental disabili- Sievers, S. B., Trembath, D., & Westerveld, M. F. (2020).
ties including autism. Journal of Special Education Speech-language pathologists’ knowledge and con-
Apprenticeship, 7(2), n2. sideration of factors that may predict, moderate,
National Research Council, & Committee on Educational and mediate AAC outcomes. Journal of Autism and
Interventions for Children with Autism. (2001). Developmental Disorders, 50(1), 238–249. https://doi.
Educating children with autism. National Academy org/10.1007/s10803-­019-­04217-­4
Press. Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Seely-York, S., & Edrisinha,
Ogletree, B. T., & Harn, W. E. (2001). Augmentative and C. (2004). Teaching students with developmental
alternative communications for persons with autism: disabilities to locate their AAC device. Research in
History, issues, and unanswered questions. Focus on Developmental Disabilities, 25(4), 371–383. https://
Autism & Other Developmental Disabilities, 16(3), doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2003.07.002
138. https://doi.org/10.1177/108835760101600301 Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Ganz, J. B., Lancioni, G. E., &
Pasco, G., & Tohill, C. (2011). Predicting progress in Schlosser, R. W. (2005). Supporting self-­determination
Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) in AAC interventions by assessing preference for
use by children with autism. International Journal communication devices. Technology & Disability, 17,
of Language & Communication Disorders, 46, 120. 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3233/TAD-­2005-­17302
https://doi.org/10.3109/13682822.2010.484851 Sigafoos, J., Green, V. A., Payne, D., Son, S. H., O’Reilly,
Rabideau, L.  K., Stanton-Chapman, T.  L., & Brown, M., & Lancioni, G. E. (2009). A comparison of picture
T.  S. (2018). Discrete trial training to teach alterna- exchange and speech-generating devices: Acquisition,
tive communication: A step-by-step guide. Young preference, and effects on social interaction.
Exceptional Children, 21(10), 34–47. https://doi. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 25,
org/10.1177/1096250615621357 99–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/07434610902739959
Reichow, B. (2012). Overview of meta-analyses on early Skinner, B. F. (1957). Century psychology series. Verbal
intensive behavioral intervention for young children behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts. https://doi.
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism org/10.1037/11256-­000
and Developmental Disorders, 42, 512–520. https:// Smith, T. (2001). Discrete trial training in the treat-
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­011-­1218-­9 ment of autism. Focus on Autism and Other
400 H. Neimy and B. Fossett

Developmental Disabilities, 16, 86–92. https://doi. in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 1247–1257. https://
org/10.1177/108835760101600204 doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.04.005
Son, S.-H., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., & Lancioni, G. E. van der Meer, L., Sigafoos, J., Sutherland, D., McLay,
(2006). Comparing two types of augmentative and L., Lang, R., Lancioni, G., & Marschik, P. (2014).
alternative communication systems for children with Preference-enhanced communication intervention
autism. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 9(4), 389–395. and development of social communicative func-
https://doi.org/10.1080/13638490500519984 tions in a child with autism spectrum disorder.
Sundberg, M.  L., & Partington, J.  W. (1998). Teaching Clinical Case Studies, 13(3), 282–295. https://doi.
language to children with autism or other develop- org/10.1177/1534650113508221
mental disabilities. Behavior Analysts, Inc.. Vandereet, J., Maes, B., Lembrechts, D., & Zink, I.
Tiger, J., Hanley, G., & Bruzdek, J. (2008). Functional (2011). Expressive vocabulary acquisition in children
communication training: A review and practical guide. with intellectual disability: Speech or manual signs?
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23. https://doi. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability,
org/10.1007/bf03391716 36, 91–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/13668250.2011.
Tincani, M. (2004). Comparing the picture exchange 572547
communication system and sign language training Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic inter-
for children with autism. Focus on Autism and other vention for autism in early childhood: Meta-analysis,
Developmental Disabilities, 19(3), 152–164. https:// meta-regression and dose–response meta-analysis of
doi.org/10.1177/10883576040190030301 multiple outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 30,
Torelli, J. N., Lambert, J. M., Da Fonte, M. A., Denham, 387–399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.008
K.  N., Jedrzynski, T.  M., & Houchins-Juarez, N.  J. Wendt, O., Hsu, N., Simon, K., Dienhart, A., & Cain, L.
(2016). Assessing acquisition of and preference for (2019). Effects of an iPad-based speech-­generating
mand topographies during functional communication device infused into instruction with the picture
training. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9, 165–168. exchange communication system for adolescents and
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­015-­0083-­y young adults with severe autism spectrum disorder.
Trembath, D., Iacono, T., Lyon, K., West, D., & Johnson, Behavior Modification, 43(6), 898–932. https://doi.
H. (2014). Augmentative and alternative commu- org/10.1177/0145445519870552
nication supports for adults with autism spectrum Winborn, K., Wacker, D., Richman, D., Asmus, J., &
disorders. Autism: The International Journal of Geier, D. (2002). Assessment of mand selection for
Research & Practice, 18(8), 891–902. https://doi. functional communication training packages. Journal
org/10.1177/1362361313486204 of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 295–298. https://
U.S.  Department of Education, Institute of Education doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­295
Sciences, What Works Clearinghouse. (2016). What Winborn-Kemmerer, L., Ringdahl, J., Wacker, D., &
Works Clearinghouse: Procedures and Standards Kitsukawa, K. (2009). A demonstration of individual
Handbook (Version 3.0). Retrieved 15 Jan 2021. preference for novel mands during functional com-
http://whatworks.ed.go munication training. Journal of Applied Behavior
van der Meer, L. A., & Rispoli, M. (2010). Communication Analysis, 42, 185–189. https://doi.org/10.1901/
interventions involving speech-generating devices jaba.2009.42-­185
for children with autism: A review of the literature. Winborn-Kemmerer, L., Wacker, D., Harding, J., Boelter,
Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 294–306. E., Berg, W., & Lee, J. (2010). Analysis of mand selec-
https://doi.org/10.3109/1751842100-­3671494 tion across different stimulus conditions. Education
van der Meer, L., Sigafoos, J., O'Reilly, M. F., & Lancioni, and Treatment of Children, 33, 49–64. https://doi.
G. E. (2011). Assessing preferences for AAC options org/10.1353/etc.0.0086
in communication interventions for individuals with Yoder, P., & Compton, D. (2004). Identifying predic-
developmental disabilities: A review of the literature. tors of treatment response. Mental Retardation and
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1422– Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10,
1431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.02.003 162–168. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20013
van der Meer, L., Sutherland, D., O’Reilly, M.  F., Zangari, C., Lloyd, L.  L., & Vicker, B. (1994).
Lancioni, G.  E., & Sigafoos, J. (2012). A further Augmentative and alternative communication: An
comparison of manual signing, picture exchange, and historic perspective. Augmentative and Alternative
speech generating devices as communication modes Communication, 10, 27–59. https://doi.org/10.1080/0
for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research 7434619412331276740
20  Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Systems 401

Resources Exchange-Based Communication

Tangible and Tactile Symbols PECS: www.pecs.com


SPEAKall!®: Speak MODalities: http://www.speakmod.
com/speakall/
Tangible Symbols Webcast by Elizabeth Torrey, Perkins
School for the Blind: https://www.perkinselearning.
org/videos/webcast/tangible-­symbols
Tangible Symbol Systems Primer, Dr. Charity Rowland Professional Development and Learning
& Dr. Philip Schweigert: https://www.designtolearn.
com/uploaded/pdf/Tangible-­Symbols-­Primer-­07-­09x.
AAC Learning Center Moodle: https://aac-­learning-­
pdf
center-­­moodle.psu.edu/
Using Tactile Symbols to Support Communication,
AAC-RERC Webcasts: https://aac-­rerc.psu.edu/index.
Angela Powell: https://bit.ly/3fJD8CS
php/pages/show/id/44
Speech-Generating Devices: Autism Internet Modules:
https://autisminternetmodules.org/mod_intro.
Photographs and Line-Drawing Symbols php?mod_id=35
AAC Assessment – Adults with DD: Assistive Technology
Internet Modules: https://atinternetmodules.org/mod_
Picture Communication Symbols/Boardmaker 7®:
intro.php?mod_id=135
https://goboardmaker.com/pages/boardmaker-­7
SymbolStix Prime®: https://www.n2y.com/symbolstix-­
prime/
PICTO4me: https://picto4.me/site?lang=en
Visuals Engine, Connectability.ca: https://connectability.
ca/visuals-­engine/
LessonPix: https://lessonpix.com/
Shaping: A Brief History, Research
Overview, and Recommendations 21
Joseph H. Cihon

21.1 S
 haping: A Brief History, those historical descriptions of Skinner’s day of
Research Overview, great illumination in discovering the shaping pro-
and Recommendations cess (later published in the Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior; Peterson,
Shaping is one of those skills within the field of 2004).
applied behavior analysis (ABA) that is com-
monly discussed, under-researched, and can take
many years to attain fluency and effectiveness. 21.2 A
 Historical Journey: A Day
Many professionals commonly have their own of Great Illumination
take on how to describe shaping. Some might be
familiar with the analogy of shaping behavior Peterson (2000)1 was bothered for years about
resembling a sculptor shaping a lump of clay Skinner’s description of his amazement when he
(Skinner, 1951), while others may be more famil- first saw behavior being shaped in 1943.
iar with other analogies (e.g., an engineer build- Specifically, Skinner (1958) stated,
ing a house). Regardless of the analogy, the most In 1943 Keller Breland, Norman Guttman, and I
frequently used description the reader is likely to were working on a wartime project sponsored by
have encountered involves the use of differential General Mills, Inc. Our laboratory was the top
reinforcement of successive approximations to a floor of a flour mill in Minneapolis, where we
spent a good deal of time waiting for decisions to
terminal response. While this may be common be made in Washington. All day long, around the
and useful as a quick account of the shaping pro- mill, wheeled great flocks of pigeons. They were
cess, it is not a comprehensive description of the
shaping process and strays a bit from historical The reader is strongly encouraged to contact the original
1 

descriptions. Peterson (2000) provided one of manuscript.

Author NoteThe contents of this chapter would not have


been possible without the mentorship of Shahla Ala’i and
Jesus Rosales-Ruiz to whom I’d like to express my deep-
est gratitude.

J. H. Cihon (*)
Institute for Behavioral Studies, Endicott College,
Seal Beach, CA, USA
e-mail: jcihon@APFmail.org
© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 403
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_21
404 J. H. Cihon

easily snared on the window sills and proved to be Skinner training his lab rat, Pliny, to perform a
an irresistible supply of experimental subjects. We
built a magnetic food-magazine, which dispensed
rather extensive behavior chain (“This smart,”
grain on the principle of an automatic peanut ven- 1937). The training described made it seem as
dor, and conditioned pigeons to turn at the sound it though Skinner had used shaping, but, as Peterson
made and eat the grain it discharged into a cup. We noted, this also came years before 1943.
used the device to condition several kinds of
behavior. For example, we built a gauge to mea-
What, then, was so illuminating about that day
sure the force with which a pigeon pecked a hori- in 1943? Peterson (2000) narrowed it down to
zontal block, and by differentially reinforcing two main points:
harder pecks we built up such forceful blows that
the base of the pigeon’s beak quickly became (1) the efficacy of implementing a program of suc-
inflamed. This was serious research, but we had cessive approximation by simply watching the ani-
our lighter moments. One clay we decided to teach mal and operating the reinforcement-delivery
a pigeon to bowl. The pigeon was to send a wooden device by hand, rather than making small mechani-
ball down a miniature alley toward a set of toy pins cal adjustments of the physical environment, as he
by swiping the ball with a sharp sideward move- had always done before, and (2) the rapidity with
ment of the beak. To condition the response, we which dramatic changes in response topography
put the ball on the floor of an experimental box and can be brought about when one does this (p. 8)
prepared to operate the food-magazine as soon as
the first swipe occurred. But nothing happened. Herein lies the part of problem with describ-
Though we had all the time in the world, we grew
tired of waiting. We decided to reinforce any
ing shaping as the use of differential reinforce-
response which had the slightest resemblance to a ment of successive approximations to a terminal
swipe  – perhaps, at first, merely the behavior of response. The emphasis on the source of rein-
looking at the ball – and then to select responses forcement delivery, the effect of reinforcement
which more closely approximated the final form.
The result amazed us. In a few minutes, the ball
delivery, and the quickness in changes is lost. In
was caroming off the walls of the box as if the addition to these omissions, there are at least two
pigeon had been a champion squash player. The other challenges worth discussion with using this
spectacle so impressed Keller Breland that he gave common definition for shaping.
up a promising career in psychology and went into
the commercial production of behavior. (p. 94).

The reasons this bothered Peterson helps to 21.3 C


 hallenge One: A Linear
illuminate the problems with describing shaping Perspective
as the differential reinforcement of successive
approximations to a terminal response. The first Defining shaping as the use of differential rein-
reason Peterson provided stems from Skinner’s forcement of successive approximations to a ter-
(1938) chapter entitled “The Differentiation of a minal response may lead practitioners to view the
Response.” In this chapter, Skinner described shaping process and, in turn, implement shaping
experiments using successive approximations to from a linear perspective, that is, viewing how to
develop lever pressing with rats. It seemed odd to get the learner from their current responding to
Peterson that Skinner would be amazed in 1943 the desired responding as a series of unvarying,
when he seemed to be discussing shaping all the discrete steps. This may lead to the development
way back in 1938. Second, Skinner seemed to of an ordered list of responses for the practitioner
describe shaping a rat’s lever press in an exchange to reinforce each step until mastery prior to rein-
with Konorski and Miller (see Konorski and forcing the next response in the list. While behav-
Miller, 1937; Miller and Konorski, 1928, 1969; ior analysis is built upon the assumption of
and Skinner 1935, 1937, for the exchange) even determinism that all behavior is lawful and
before the publication of Skinner (1938). Peterson orderly, shaping behavior is unlikely to follow a
concluded that this description must have been series of unvarying, discrete steps in which the
speculative rather than empirical if Skinner had, practitioner moves from reinforcing only one
in fact, not discovered shaping until 1943. Third, topography to the next until the terminal response
in 1937, Life magazine published a story about occurs. Approaching shaping from this perspec-
21  Shaping: A Brief History, Research Overview, and Recommendations 405

tive is likely to prevent practitioners from analyz- happens to the learner without an effect on the
ing moment-to-moment changes in the learner’s person implementing the shaping procedure.
behavior and delivering access to reinforcement That is, the learner is the focus of the shaping
or withholding reinforcement as a result of this procedure, the learner’s behavior is the only
analysis (i.e., clinical judgement, see Leaf et al., behavior of interest, and the practitioner operates
2018; Leaf et al., 2016). Ultimately, a restricted outside of the effects of shaping. It is the case,
view of responses that may be candidates for however, that shaping is a reciprocal process in
reinforcement may slow or, worse, halt the shap- which the behavior of the learner and the practi-
ing process. tioner are mutually related and affected. The
Approaching shaping from a nonlinear per- learner’s behavior sets the occasion for the prac-
spective (for a more detailed discussion that is titioner’s behavior, which then affects the learn-
beyond the scope of this chapter, see Goldiamond er’s behavior; the learner’s behavior then affects
1984, 2002) permits the practitioner to focus on the practitioner’s behavior and sets the occasion
expanding response classes, which, in turn, for the practitioner’s behavior; and so on. Just as
increases the number of responses that may be the learner’s behavior is lawful, so is the practi-
candidates for reinforcement. This more closely tioner’s behavior, and both are shaped during the
aligns with the effect of reinforcement  – an shaping process. It should also be noted that
increase in the probability of similar responses identification of this reciprocal process of socially
(i.e., expanding response classes) in future simi- meditated consequences is what made Skinner’s
lar situations. In this sense, delivering access to analysis of verbal behavior possible (Peterson,
reinforcement increases the probability of similar 2004).
responses – some that may be on the path to the
terminal goal and some that may not be on that
path. Nonetheless, shaping should increase the 21.4.1 Shaping Defined2
number of responses the individual is emitting,
giving the practitioner more behavior with which So then, how may shaping be defined in a way
to shape. Therefore, within this nonlinear view of that avoids these and other possible challenges?
shaping, the practitioner can determine when and Peterson (2000) provided a definition of shaping
if to deliver access to reinforcement across a that encapsulates what was illuminating for
larger number of responses and response topog- Skinner back in 1943. She described shaping as
raphies. The likelihood that these responses and “a word that would suggest a distinction between
response topographies could be determined a the process of behavioral elaboration directed by
priori in an effort to develop a series of discrete constraints in the physical environment with
steps is low. mechanical connections to sources of reinforce-
ment from behavioral elaboration directed by
another organism” (Peterson, 2000, p.  9). This
21.4 Challenge Two: A Lack definition addresses challenges with respect to a
of Reciprocity linear perspective as well as the reciprocity of
shaping. Cooper et al.’ (2020) updated definition
Defining shaping as the use of differential rein- of shaping (from previous editions), a “three-part
forcement of successive approximations to a ter- process whereby the analyst (a) detects a change
minal response may also prevent practitioners in the learner’s environment, (b) makes a dis-
from developing an understanding of the recipro- criminated judgment about whether that change
cal process that is shaping. This common defini- is a progressively closer approximation to a ter-
tion of shaping permits mechanical descriptions
of shaping (e.g., programming a machine to grad- It is important to  note that this chapter focuses
2 

ually increase the effort required to access rein- on response shaping as opposed to stimulus control shap-
forcement) and implies shaping is something that ing (McIlvane & Dube, 1992).
406 J. H. Cihon

minal behavior of interest, and then (c) differen- fruit) outside of shaping sessions was limited.
tially reinforces that closer successive These changes resulted in more rapid progress,
approximation” (p.  541), also helps address the and Dicky was “soon wearing his glasses contin-
aforementioned challenges. What both of these uously during the meal sessions in his room”
definitions have in common is an emphasis on the (Wolf et al. 1964, p. 310). As Dicky continued to
reciprocity of the shaping process and analyzing be successful, supplemental reinforcement was
the learner’s behavior to determine which gradually faded, and wearing glasses was paired
responses are, or are not, candidates for with preferred activities (e.g., snacks, going for
reinforcement. walks). Upon his release from the hospital, Dicky
was wearing his glasses for approximately 12 h
each day.
21.4.2 Research Examples Although not conducted with human partici-
pants, Schaefer (1970) offered an early example
Although shaping creates unique challenges for of how shaping can develop unwanted, danger-
the researcher, there are examples of research ous behavior. Specially, Schaefer examined if
evaluating shaping and the behavioral processes head banging in primates (i.e., two rhesus mon-
underlying shaping. Wolf et al. (1964) represents keys), which was previously thought to be
one of the earliest demonstrations (if not the ear- reflexive or physiologically occasioned, could
liest demonstration) of shaping within the experi- be shaped and maintained by environmental
mental literature with autistics/individuals variables (i.e., operant control). During the first
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder3 (ASD). session, raising a paw first occasioned reinforce-
Specifically, shaping was used to teach Dicky, a ment delivery, and positioning the paw above
3-year-old autistic boy who was at risk of perma- the head and then bringing it down upon the
nent vision loss, to wear glasses. It is important to head was then shaped by delivering reinforce-
note that prior to this intervention and involve- ment for approximations. During the second
ment of the researchers, wearing glasses had session, a stimulus (e.g., “Poor boy!,” “Don’t do
been paired with attempts to physically force that!,” “You’ll hurt yourself!”) was presented
wearing the glasses. Following the establishment continuously, and the delivery of reinforcement
of clicks of a toy noisemaker as a conditioned was gradually faded. This was followed by dis-
reinforcer, several empty glasses frames (i.e., crimination training in which 30-second inter-
without lenses) were placed around the room. vals were presented during which head banging
Reinforcement was first delivered anytime Dicky occasioned the previously delivered stimulus
picked up, held, or carried the frames then for (e.g., “Don’t do that!”) and the delivery of rein-
approximations of bringing the frames closer to forcement or no response from the researcher.
his eyes. However, the “shaper met with consid- This study confirmed the results of previous
erable difficulty in getting Dicky to wear the studies in demonstrating that self-­ destructive
glassless frames in the proper manner” (Wolf behavior, such as head banging, can be shaped
et al. 1964, p. 309). As a result, Wolf et al., who and maintained by environmental variables. It is
had not previously directly intervened, spent a also important to note that the condition that
day directing the shaping procedure; changes to included statements, such as “You’ll hurt your-
the frames were made (e.g., larger ear pieces, a self,” may have been viewed as compassionate
roll bar); and access to reinforcers (e.g., candy, from the outside but was actually responsible
for the maintenance of head banging.
This terminology was selected to adhere to the seventh
3 
Bernal (1972) trained parents of a child with
edition of the American Psychological Association severe food selectivity to implement a multicom-
Publication Manual and to be inclusive of those who pre-
fer person-first and identity-first language. When discuss-
ponent intervention that involved providing access
ing prior research, the terminology used within that to different foods without requiring consumption,
research is used. changing the portions of preferred foods, and
21  Shaping: A Brief History, Research Overview, and Recommendations 407

reinforcing approximations related to sampling ered for engaging in a criterion response which
several foods rather than eating one target food was determined based on the participants previ-
(i.e., shaping). Prior to any intervention, the ous responses. The tickets could then be
child’s current repertoire, with respect to meal- exchanged for a surprise gift. In addition to shap-
times, was analyzed, and the consumption of a ing with the use of tickets, prompting was also
variety of food groups was determined to be the used when training the diaphragmatic breathing
terminal goal. The first stage of intervention, fol- response. The results demonstrated that the pro-
lowing establishing self-feeding of preferred cedures were effective in teaching the use of the
foods, involved presentation of foods without intermittent positive-pressure breathing appara-
requiring consumption. This stage of intervention tus. In the second experiment, the results were
resulted in the child sampling three foods she had replicated when nurses were trained to imple-
never eaten. The following stage of intervention ment the procedures with two additional
involved providing a preferred food contingent participants.
upon sampling new foods during mealtimes. Mathews et  al. (1992) evaluated the use of
Mealtimes were not modified in that the full meal shaping to teach four children, all under 5 years
available to the rest of the family was also avail- of age, to wear contact lenses. The shaping pro-
able for the child. The child’s responding was the cedure involved the researchers providing access
basis for progression through the intervention. to reinforcement for the child following instruc-
Following a 32-week intervention, the child was tions permitting the researchers to touch the
consuming 50 foods that she had previously child’s face, pull open the child’s eyelid, place
never eaten, and the parents reported that the drops in the child’s eyes, approach the child’s eye
child’s weight and diet were no longer a with one finger, and touch the child’s eye with the
concern. finger. A brief time-out was also used when the
Butterfield and Parson (1973) provided an children did not follow the instruction. The par-
early example of the use of modeling and shaping ents were then trained on lens care and insertion.
for mealtime behavior. More specifically, the The procedures were effective at increasing
treatment plan for the 8-year-old child involved cooperation with instructions, a decrease in the
avoiding attention (e.g., shouting at the child) for time required for lens removal or insertion, and
not chewing, modeling and shaping chewing, and three of the four participants demonstrated high
fading reinforcement. All procedures were imple- levels of cooperation with lens removal or inser-
mented by the child’s family members. The shap- tion during 3- to 10-month follow-ups.
ing procedure was described as “the mother was Although not conducted with human partici-
instructed to reinforce any approximation of a pants, Ferguson and Rosales-Ruiz (2001) pro-
“crunch.” [sic] She was then to successively vided a unique example of the use of shaping
increase the response requirement until the child procedures as an alternative to standard proce-
bit completely through the cracker” (Butterfield dures using aversive techniques when using
& Parson, 1973, p. 286). The results showed the shaping to train five horses to trailer load. The
parents stopped scolding their child, the child’s procedures involved first training the horse to
chewing increased, and the child was eating sev- approach a target (i.e., a red cloth potholder tied
eral new foods prior to the termination of the to a string), during which the horse accessed rein-
treatment program. forcement following touching the target. The tar-
Renne and Creer (1976) explored the use of get was then moved to different locations within
shaping to train children with asthma to use inha- the trailer (e.g., forward little by little into the
lation therapy equipment (i.e., an intermittent trailer) based on successful trials. If the horse did
positive-pressure breathing apparatus). In the not touch the target on a trial, the target was not
first experiment, eye fixation, facial posturing, moved on the next trial. The procedures (i.e., tar-
and diaphragmatic breathing were sequentially get training and shaping) were effective in train-
trained for four participants. Tickets were deliv- ing all of the horses to load into a trailer and at no
408 J. H. Cihon

point in time were procedures based upon pun- forcement (i.e., a linear shaping approach). In
ishment or negative reinforcement required. general, the participants acquired the targeted
Ricciardi et al. (2006) evaluated the effective- responses more rapidly in the shaping condition.
ness of a shaping procedure to increase approach While the reasonable attempts condition appears
responses to animatronic objects (e.g., dancing to align closer to the aforementioned definition
Elmo doll, dancing Santa Claus figure) with an and description of shaping, Newman et  al.
8-year-old boy with autistic disorder. The proce- hypothesized the discrepancy across the condi-
dure involved first providing noncontingent tions may be due to challenges in defining and
access to preferred items placed 6  m from the standardizing what constitutes a reasonable
animatronic objects. The distance the preferred attempt.
items were placed from the animatronic objects Koegel et al. (2012) explored the effectiveness
was then gradually decreased from 6  m to 1  m of a hierarchical shaping intervention to increase
(i.e., the terminal criterion). Access to the pre- the level of acceptance of new foods and sponta-
ferred items was available at each of the distance neous requests for new with three children diag-
criteria. If the participant remained at the current nosed with autism. Specifically, Koegel et  al.
distance for at least 90% of intervals across two developed a eight-level hierarchy of acceptance
consecutive sessions, the distance was reduced. which involved (1) refusing a food, (2) touching
The results indicated that the procedures were and motioning a food to the mouth, (3) putting a
effective at increasing the participants’ proximity food on lips, (4) biting a food, (5) biting a food
to the previously avoided animatronic objects. and putting in mouth but not swallowing, (6)
Treating phobias, as demonstrated by the partici- chewing a food but not swallowing, (7) swallow-
pant, commonly involves preventing escaping or ing a food reluctantly, and (8) accepting a food
distancing oneself from the items that occasion without displeasure or disruptive behavior. Once
phobic responses. Ricciardi et  al. demonstrated a potential reinforcer was identified, the partici-
that shaping without the use of escape extinction pants were informed what behavior (i.e., step on
can be a viable, effective option when treating the hierarchy) was required to access the rein-
phobias. forcer. Once a participant was successful on a
Newman et  al. (2009) compared what they level of the hierarchy during three consecutive
referred to as reinforcing reasonable attempts to probes without disruptive behavior, they were
shaping with three preschoolers diagnosed with advanced to the next level of the hierarchy. The
ASD.  The dependent measure involved vocal results indicated that the intervention was suc-
responding within discrete trial teaching (DTT) cessful in increasing the number of new foods
programs (e.g., answering “wh” questions, consumed by the three participants. Furthermore,
answering personal information questions, filling all three participants were observed requesting
in missing words, expressive identification of new foods during generalization probes.
simple shapes). In the reasonable attempts condi- Hodges et  al. (2017) extended the results of
tion, “reinforcement was not contingent on Koegel et al. (2012) by examining the effective-
equivalent or successively more accurate ness of shaping to increase the number of differ-
responses” (Newman et al., 2009, p. 69). Rather, ent foods consumed two children with
reinforcement was provided contingent upon the ASD. Hodges et al. differed from Koegel et al. in
participant directing any response “that might that there were only four levels in the hierarchy
have been reinforced at any point if shaping were (i.e., refusal, touches food to lips, puts food in the
used to teach this skill (i.e., responding within a mouth and does not swallow, and swallows food).
broader class of correct responses)” (Newman The first phase of intervention involved the
et al., 2009, p. 69) toward the interventionist or simultaneous presentation of four foods, with
training materials. In the shaping condition, only each food targeted individually. Once a partici-
responses that were the same or better quality pant reached the highest level of acceptance for
than previous responses set the occasion for rein- the first food across three consecutive sessions,
21  Shaping: A Brief History, Research Overview, and Recommendations 409

the next food was targeted. Following this first the children’s behavior. In describing what
phase, multiple foods were required to be con- resulted in movement Cihon, Ferguson, Leaf,
sumed per trial to access reinforcement. This et al. noted,
began with targeting the consumption of two If the behavior exhibited by a participant during
foods, followed by three, and, finally, all four the interval represented a general improvement,
foods from Phase 1 of the intervention. The the marker would be moved up (i.e., the children’s
results showed that both participants accepted the markers moved independent of each other).
Likewise, if the general quality of the behavior was
four targeted foods and consumed all four foods below what was reasonable to expect of the child
in the final phase of the intervention when given a number of variables, the marker would be
­reinforcement was contingent upon consumption moved down. (p. 48).
of all four foods.
Fonger and Malott (2018) explored the effec- Said differently, there was no a priori hierar-
tiveness of a shaping procedure, in the absence of chy developed to guide the shaping approach.
a vocal cue and prompting, to increase eye con- Instead, the participants’ behavior set the occa-
tact during pauses in instruction for three sion for the interventionist’s behavior, which then
preschool-­ aged children diagnosed with affected the participants’ behavior and so on. The
ASD.  The shaping procedure involved removal results demonstrated that the level system used
of a preferred item and waiting until the partici- within a flexible shaping approach was effective
pant made an orienting response before returning at increasing the percentage of intervals in which
the preferred item. The duration the participants synchronous engagement was observed for both
had access to the preferred item was differential dyads.
based on the latency between removal of the pre- Turner et al. (2020) provided another example
ferred item and the orienting response. Orienting of the use of shaping to improve the acceptance
within 5  s resulted in an edible reinforcer and of foods for two children diagnosed with
access to the preferred item for 15 s, while orient- ASD.  Specifically, Turner et  al. evaluated the
ing after 5  s resulted in access to the preferred effectiveness of a shaping package, which
item for 5 s. The response requirement was grad- included modeling and prompting, while also
ually increased (e.g., orienting to brief eye con- comparing the use of small-constant food sets
tact to eye contact for 1, 2, and 3  s) as the and large-rotating food sets. In one condition, the
participants were successful. The results indi- same 3 foods were presented each session, and in
cated that all three participants acquired sus- the other condition, 3 of 15 foods were presented
tained eye contact, and the effects maintained up each session. The intervention remained the same
to 1 month following the shaping procedure. across both food set conditions which consisted
Cihon, Ferguson, Leaf, et  al. (2019b) evalu- of a five-step response sequence (i.e., touch,
ated the effectiveness of a level system with a taste, lick, mouth, and eat). Following stating the
flexible shaping approach to improve synchro- contingency (e.g., If you taste the strawberry, you
nous engagement with two dyads of children can play with the toy), reinforcement was deliv-
diagnosed with ASD.  Four children diagnosed ered contingent upon engaging in the targeted
with ASD who were randomly assigned to two response stated in the contingency. The results
different dyads participated in the study. indicated that the shaping package was effective
Synchronous engagement differed from engage- at improving food acceptance for both partici-
ment in that for synchronous engagement to be pants; however, results related to the size of the
scored; both children were required to be engaged targeted foods sets were idiosyncratic across the
in the same activity while also displaying favor- two participants.
able affect. The flexible shaping approach Most recently, Sivaraman et al. (2021) evalu-
involved a three-tiered level system that the chil- ated the effectiveness of coaching caregivers via
dren moved up and down, within and across lev- telehealth to implement graduated exposure and
els, based on the interventionists’ assessment of shaping to teach mask wearing for six children
410 J. H. Cihon

diagnosed with ASD. Sivaraman et al. developed ior analysts with confidence in using shaping
a 15-step exposure hierarchy starting at the face- with autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD.
mask being within 30 cm for 5 s to wearing the Another entity that was created to help dis-
facemask for 10  min before removing the face- seminate information regarding evidence-based
mask using the loops. However, it should be practices for autistics/individuals diagnosed with
noted that this hierarchy was modified for two of ASD is the National Clearinghouse on Autism
the participants. The caregiver delivered inter- Evidence and Practice (NCAEP; Steinbrenner
vention involved conducting a multiple- stimulus et  al., 2020). The most recent resource made
without replacement (MSWO) preference assess- available from the NCAEP was the third iteration
ment, stating it was time to wear a mask, provid- of the “Evidence-Based Practices for Children,
ing a rationale for why the mask was important, Youth, and Young Adults with Autism” report
modeling putting on the mask, and then present- that “describe[d] a set of practices that have clear
ing the mask to their child. When the child com- evidence of positive effects with autistic children
pleted a new step in the hierarchy without and youth” (Steinbrenner et  al., 2020, p.  7).
engaging in problem behavior, the caregiver pro- Shaping is not directly referred to in the Evidence-­
vided praise and access to a preferred item. Based Practices for Children, Youth, and Young
Following two successful occurrences at a step, Adults with Autism report. However, several
the next step in the exposure hierarchy was tar- mechanisms, principles, and procedures involved
geted. All six participants reached the final steps in shaping are listed as evidenced-based (e.g.,
within the hierarchy (i.e., wearing the facemask antecedent-based interventions, differential rein-
for 10 min before removing the facemask using forcement, extinction, naturalistic intervention,
the loops), and the caregivers reported the inter- reinforcement, task analysis). Therefore, while
vention favorably. shaping is not directly referred to in the Evidence-­
Based Practices for Children, Youth, and Young
Adults with Autism report, this report still pro-
21.4.3 Shaping as an Evidence-Based vides support that shaping is an evidence-based
Practice for ASD practice for autistics/individuals diagnosed with
ASD.
One goal of the National Standards Project Despite the resource, report, or project, shap-
(National Autism Center, 2015) was to develop ing and the underlying behavioral mechanisms
evidence-based practice guidelines for autistics/ and principles responsible for the behavior
individuals diagnosed with ASD.  Phase 2 (i.e., change observed when using shaping have a rich
the most recent iteration) of the National literature base demonstrating their effectiveness.
Standards Project (National Autism Center, Shaping relies on providing access to reinforce-
2015) provided more recent information on the ment for some responses and not for others (i.e.,
effectiveness of a broad range of interventions for extinction). There is a copious number of experi-
autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. Phase mental studies that have documented the effec-
2 includes shaping as an established intervention tiveness of reinforcement to increase the
under the broad categories of Behavioral likelihood of future similar responses in similar
Interventions and Comprehensive Behavioral situations. A quick Google Scholar search for the
Treatment for Young Children. While Phase 2 of terms “reinforcement” and “behavior analysis”
the National Standards Project (National Autism yields over 2.2 million results, and the same
Center, 2015) notes the challenges with broad search for the terms “extinction” and “behavior
categories such as Behavioral Interventions and analysis” yields over 1.6 million results. Though
Comprehensive Behavioral Treatment for Young anecdotal, these cursory searches illustrate the
Children, shaping being included under two of breadth of the literature base behind the behav-
these categories should provide practicing behav- ioral principles responsible for the effectiveness
21  Shaping: A Brief History, Research Overview, and Recommendations 411

of shaping. Given this vast literature base, the the shapee’s behavior can occasion) is likely to
onus of effectiveness of shaping ultimately falls create challenges for researchers evaluating shap-
on those implementing shaping. Nonetheless, ing (Cihon, Ferguson, Leaf, et al., 2019b; Cihon,
shaping may, and should, be considered an Ferguson, Milne, et  al., 2019a). While flexible
evidence-­based practice. procedures likely align more closely to what
occurs within practice, providing a technological
description to aid in replication efforts for proce-
21.4.4 Recommendations dures with flexibility will be challenging, espe-
for Research and Practice cially when attempting to publish in behavior
analytic journals. Efforts to describe shaping
21.4.4.1 Research with respect to the shaper’s and shapee’s behav-
As evident from the preceding section, there have ior will likely be useful in addressing this chal-
been several studies evaluating the effectiveness lenge, especially if analyzed similar to Skinner’s
of shaping in isolation and in combination with (1957) analysis of verbal episodes. This analysis
other procedures. Nonetheless, given the histori- would permit identifying conditional probabili-
cal foundations and illuminating discovery of ties with respect to the shaper’s and shapee’s
shaping, the research related to the use of shaping behavior. For instance, we may be able to exam-
with human participants is still somewhat scant ine the probability the shaper responds with the
when compared to other foundational procedures putative reinforcer based on specific topogra-
(e.g., standard functional analysis; Beavers et al., phies of responses emitted by the shapee as well
2013; Hanley et  al., 2003). While there may be as the probability the shaper responds with the
several reasons for this, one likely reason is the putative reinforcer in the absence of specific
aforementioned reciprocal nature of the shaping topographies of responses emitted by the shapee.
process. This is likely the reason for the sole One criticism of the use of shaping is the
inclusion of data related to the length of time sometimes required to shape an
participant’s/“shapee’s” behavior and the exclu- organism’s behavior (Cooper et  al., 2020). The
sion of the interventionist’s/“shaper’s” behavior validity of such claims notwithstanding, future
within research. Each of the previously described research could evaluate this criticism in several
shaping studies are examples of this phenomenon. ways. Much of the research using shaping has
While the shapee’s behavior may be of most inter- involved combining shaping with other
est within applied research, including an analysis approaches (e.g., prompting; Turner et al., 2020),
of the reciprocal relationship of the shapee’s and which prevents evaluation of the effectiveness
shaper’s behavior will be invaluable for replica- and efficiency of shaping alone. Moving forward,
tion purposes as well as a fine-grained analysis of researchers should make concerted efforts to con-
the shaping process. This fine-grained analysis tinue to evaluate the effectiveness and efficiency
will offer researchers and practitioners a better of shaping in isolation. Additionally, attempts to
understanding of the shaping process. Therefore, replicate and extend previous research that have
it is recommended that future research on the use combined procedures could include component
of shaping includes data with respect to the behav- analyses of the combined procedures. This
ior of both parties involved in the shaping process. research would permit direct evaluation if the
This could be done in a similar method used by commonly included additional procedures (e.g.,
Skinner (1957) when analyzing and diagraming modeling, prompting) are just additional or nec-
the relationship between the speaker and listener essary for the effectiveness of the intervention. If
within a verbal episode. No matter the approach, the additional procedures are found to be just
the reciprocal nature of the shaping process is that, additional, they could be removed, yielding
likely the biggest challenge facing researchers a more efficient intervention. Future research
evaluating the effectiveness of shaping. could also compare shaping alone to other proce-
Relatedly, the flexibility inherent to shaping dures to identify the conditions under which
(e.g., the vast number of shaper’s responses that shaping is more or less effective and/or efficient
412 J. H. Cihon

than other procedures. This could be done using dent measures within any study evaluating a
commonly employed single-case comparative method to train others to shape. What exactly will
designs (e.g., alternating treatments design; be measured to evaluate the effectiveness of a
Barlow and Hayes, 1979) as well as randomized training method? It is likely that to determine
clinical trials. mastery, any measurement of the trainee’s behav-
The majority of the research evaluating the ior will need to be evaluated in the context of the
use of shaping procedures for autistics/individu- learner’s behavior, thus, highlighting the recipro-
als diagnosed with ASD has involved mealtime cal nature of shaping. It will also be essential to
challenges (e.g., food selectivity). This is likely ask, “What is the necessary criteria that will need
due to the continual documented success of shap- to be met in which context(s) to determine mas-
ing as an intervention approach for mealtime tery?” It is likely that common mastery criteria
challenges (e.g., Bernal, 1972; Hodges et  al., (e.g., 80% correct across three consecutive ses-
2017; Koegel et al., 2012; Tanner and Andreone, sions) will be insufficient. How many responses
2015). Researchers should continue to examine across how many learners and contexts will be
the effectiveness and efficiency of shaping to necessary for one to conclude that the trainee has
address a variety of mealtime challenges, espe- mastered shaping? All of these questions are ripe
cially as an alternative to more invasive or aver- for research related to training practitioners to
sive approaches (Cihon et  al., 2021). However, become fluent, effective shapers.
researchers should also allocate efforts to evalu-
ating the use of shaping procedures with autis- 21.4.4.2 Practice
tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD across a There are many challenges related to the use of
variety of responses outside of the mealtime con- shaping within practice. Perhaps the biggest chal-
text. While there have been some examples of lenge relates to training practitioners to shape.
this research (e.g., Cihon, Ferguson, Leaf, et al., The reciprocal nature of shaping prevents the use
2019b; Cihon, Ferguson, Milne, et  al., 2019a; of otherwise effective methods. For example, it
Fonger and Malott, 2018; Wolf et  al., 1964), seems unfeasible to be able to provide a thorough
additional research efforts will continue to be protocol with a series of if-then statements for
valuable and necessary especially considering how to respond based on the learner’s response.
some concerns with commonly used approaches While this method may be useful for procedures
to mealtime challenges (e.g., escape extinction; such as discrete trial teaching, where responding
see Cihon et al., 2021; Riordan et al., 1980). is often categorized as correct, incorrect, or
Finally, there is a dearth of research on identi- prompted, it becomes less useful when learner
fying and evaluating effective methods to train responding is less constrained. Even if a thor-
practitioners to shape. Given its foundational and ough protocol may be developed, it is undesired
historical underpinnings of our science, future due to the sources of stimulus control developed.
research should identify and evaluate effective That is, the practitioner would be responding to
training methods. This could begin by evaluating the protocol, as opposed to the learner’s behavior,
current best practices with respect to training therefore, preventing the development of the
(e.g., behavioral skills training; Kirkpatrick et al., reciprocal shaping process. Furthermore, focusing
2021, video modeling; Mulqueen et al., 2021) as on a protocol is likely to develop a rule-­governed
well as other emerging training methods (e.g., the repertoire as opposed to a contingency-shaped
teaching interaction procedure; Green et  al., repertoire, preventing the practitioner from per-
2020). Other less common or new training meth- forming well when there is no previously learned
ods should also be explored given the complex rule. Therefore, training practitioners on how to
reciprocal process that is shaping. This would shape becomes a much more involved task for
include examining how many sessions, exem- the trainer.
plars, and the like will be necessary for someone Training practitioners to become fluent, effec-
to become a fluent, effective shaper. It will also tive shapers will likely require ample time
be essential to pay careful attention to the depen- observing a model with in  vivo narration.
21  Shaping: A Brief History, Research Overview, and Recommendations 413

Someone who is already a fluent, effective shaper of Skinner’s discovery of shaping. Reflecting on
could model the process while narrating the these implications should help to inform practice
rationale or learner behavior that is occasioning and research related to shaping. This chapter also
their response. This may also be accomplished described several select research examples with
with one person providing the model while autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD as well
another discusses what is occurring in vivo with as humans with other challenges and nonhuman
the trainee. However, simply watching a model, animals. While the selected research examples do
with or without narration, is not likely to be effec- not represent a comprehensive literature review
tive in isolation. If it were, imagine how many related to shaping, they provide examples of the
sports fans would have acquired the skills of effectiveness of shaping across several different
those they watch each week. Therefore, any use populations and responses. Furthermore, there
of modeling will also need to be paired with have been several empirical demonstrations of
opportunities to shape while receiving feedback the effectiveness of shaping, and it, as well as its
from the learner and the trainer. This places an components, is considered an evidence-based
emphasis on the development of a practitioner’s approach for autistics/individuals diagnosed with
shaping repertoire as contingency shaped as ASD. Recommendations for future research and
opposed to rule governed. With this in mind, practice were also provided in this chapter.
training practitioners to become fluent, effective Perhaps the most important recommendation for
shapers will likely take considerable time and those seeking to become fluent, effective shapers
effort. Efforts to train practitioners to become flu- is to shape often. Happy shaping!
ent, effective shapers within the clinical setting
could be used to help inform research on effec-
tive training methods yet to be explored in the References
research.
For those practitioners and trainers seeking Barlow, D. H., & Hayes, S. C. (1979). Alternating treat-
ments design: One strategy for comparing the effects
guidance within the literature on shaping, it is of two treatments in a single subject. Journal of
recommended here to examine the nonhuman Applied Behavior Analysis, 12(2), 199–210. https://
animal training literature (e.g., Ferguson and doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1979.12-­199
Rosales-Ruiz, 2001; Pryor, 1999; Schaefer, Beavers, G.  A., Iwata, B.  A., & Lerman, D.  C. (2013).
Thirty years of research on the functional analysis
1970). The additional procedures commonly of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
employed by those providing intervention for Analysis, 46(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.30
humans are often unavailable to those working PMID-24114081
with nonhuman animals. For example, physical Bernal, M.  E. (1972). Behavioral treatment of a child's
eating problem. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
prompting is likely unfeasible when working Experimental Psychiatry, 3(1), 43–50. https://doi.
with elephants due to their size. Therefore, an org/10.1016/0005-­7916(72)90032-­8
emphasis on shaping in the absence of additional Butterfield, W.  H., & Parson, R. (1973). Modeling and
procedures is required. As a result, many behav- shaping by parents to develop chewing behavior in
their retarded child. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
ior analysts providing services for nonhuman Experimental Psychiatry, 4(3), 285–287. https://doi.
animals are likely to develop fluent, effective org/10.1016/0005-­7916(73)90090-­6
shaping repertoires. Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R.,
McEachin, J., & Taubman, M. (2019b). Use of a level
system with flexible shaping to improve synchronous
engagement. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1),
21.5 Conclusion 44–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0254-­8
Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C.  M., Leaf, J.  B.,
Shaping is engrained within the history of behavior McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2019a). A preliminary evalu-
ation of a token system with a flexible earning require-
analysis and ABA (Peterson, 2000, 2004; Skinner, ment. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(3), 548–556.
1951). This chapter provided a brief overview https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00316-­3
and implications of Peterson’s (2000) exploration
414 J. H. Cihon

Cihon, J.  H., Weiss, M.  J., Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (2018). Clinical
Zane, T., & Ross, R. K. (2021). Observational effects judgment. Different Roads to Learning.
on the food preferences of children with autism Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M.,
spectrum disorder. Focus on Autism and Other Ala'i-Rosales, S., Ross, R.  K., Smith, T., & Weiss,
Developmental Disabilities, 36(1), 25–35. https://doi. M.  J. (2016). Applied behavior analysis is a science
org/10.1177/1088357620954368 and, therefore, progressive. Journal of Autism and
Cooper, J.  O., Heron, T.  E., & Heward, W.  L. (2020). Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 720–731. https://doi.
Applied behavior analysis (3rd ed.). Pearson. org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2591-­6.
Ferguson, D.  L., & Rosales-Ruiz, J. (2001). Loading Mathews, J.  R., Hodson, G.  D., Crist, W.  B., &
the problem loader: The effects of target training LaRoche, G.  R. (1992). Teaching young children
and shaping on trailer-loading behavior of horses. to use contact lenses. Journal of Applied Behavior
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(4), Analysis, 25(1), 229–235. https://doi.org/10.1901/
409–423. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-­409 jaba.1992.25-­229
PMID -­ 11800182 McIlvane, W.  J., & Dube, W.  V. (1992). Stimulus con-
Fonger, A.  M., & Malott, R.  W. (2018). Using shaping trol shaping and stimulus control topographies.
to teach eye contact to children with autism spectrum The Behavior Analyst, 15(1), 89–94. https://doi.
disorder. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 36(2), 139. org/10.1007/BF03392591
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0245-­9 Miller, S., & Konorski, J. (1928). On a particular type
Goldiamond, I. (1984). Training parent trainers and of conditioned reflex. Proceedings of the Biological
ethicists in nonlinear analysis of behavior. In Parent Society (Polish Section, Paris), 99, 1155–1157.
training: Foundations of research and practice Miller, S., & Konorski, J. (1969). On a particular form
(pp. 504–546). Guilford. of conditioned reflex. Journal of the Experimental
Goldiamond, I. (2002). Toward a constructional approach Analysis of Behavior, 12(1), 187–189. https://doi.
to social problems: Ethical and constitutional issues org/10.1901/jeab.1969.12-­187
raised by applied behavior analysis. Behavior and Mulqueen, D., Crosland, K. A., & Novotny, M. A. (2021).
Social Issues, 11(2), 108–197. https://doi.org/10.5210/ Using video modeling and video feedback to improve
bsi.v11i2.92 olympic weightlifting technique. Behavior Analysis:
Green, D.  R., Ferguson, J.  L., Cihon, J.  H., Torres, N., Research and Practice, 21(3), 282–292. https://doi.
Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Rudrud, E., Schulze, K., org/10.1037/bar0000211
& Leaf, J.  B. (2020). The teaching interaction pro- National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu-
cedure as a staff training tool. Behavior Analysis in sions: National standards project, phase 2. Author.
Practice, 13(2), 421–433. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Newman, B., Reinecke, D., & Ramos, M. (2009). Is a rea-
s40617-­019-­00357-­2 sonable attempt reasonable? Shaping versus reinforc-
Hanley, G.  P., Iwata, B.  A., & McCord, B.  E. (2003). ing verbal attempts of preschoolers with autism. The
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 25, 67–72. https://doi.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(2), org/10.1007/BF03393070
147–185. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-­147 Peterson, G.  B. (2000). The discovery of shaping: BF
PMID -­ 12858983 Skinner's big surprise. The Clicker Journal: The
Hodges, A., Davis, T., Crandall, M., Phipps, L., & Weston, Magazine for Animal Trainers, 43, 6–13.
R. (2017). Using shaping to increase foods consumed Peterson, G.  B. (2004). A day of great illumination:
by children with autism. Journal of Autism and B.F.  Skinner's discovery of shaping. Journal of the
Developmental Disorders, 47(8), 2471–2479. https:// Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 82(3), 317–328.
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­017-­3160-­y https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2004.82-­317
Kirkpatrick, M., Rehfeld, D. M., Akers, J. S., Rivera, G., Pryor, K. (1999). Don’t shoot the dog: The new art of
& Sulak, T. N. (2021). Using behavioral skills training teaching and training (rev. ed.). Bantam.
with preservice teachers in the university classroom. Renne, C.  M., & Creer, T.  L. (1976). Training children
Behavioral Interventions, 36(1), 145–158. https://doi. with asthma to use inhalation therapy equipment.
org/10.1002/bin.1764 Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9(1), 1–11.
Koegel, R. L., Bharoocha, A. A., Ribnick, C. B., Ribnick, https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1976.9-­1
R. C., Bucio, M. O., Fredeen, R. M., & Koegel, L. K. Ricciardi, J.  N., Luiselli, J.  K., & Camare, M. (2006).
(2012). Using individualized reinforcers and hierar- Shaping approach responses as intervention for spe-
chical exposure to increase food flexibility in children cific phobia in a child with autism. Journal of Applied
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Behavior Analysis, 39(4), 445–448. https://doi.
Developmental Disorders, 42(8), 1574–1581. https:// org/10.1901/jaba.2006.158-­05
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­011-­1392-­9 Riordan, M. M., Iwata, B. A., Wohl, M. K., & Finney, J. W.
Konorski, J., & Miller, S. (1937). On two types of con- (1980). Behavioral treatment of food refusal and selec-
ditioned reflex. The Journal of General Psychology, tivity in developmentally disabled children. Applied
16(1), 264–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.19 Research in Mental Retardation, 1(1–2), 95–112.
37.9917950 https://doi.org/10.1016/0270-­3092(80)90019-­3
21  Shaping: A Brief History, Research Overview, and Recommendations 415

Schaefer, H. H. (1970). Self-injurious behavior: Shaping Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin,
"head banging" in monkeys. Journal of Applied K. L., Nowell, S. W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey, S.,
Behavior Analysis, 3(2), 111–116. https://doi. McIntyre, N. S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage, M. N.
org/10.1901/jaba.1970.3-­111 (2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth,
Sivaraman, M., Virues-Ortega, J., & Roeyers, H. (2021). and young adults with autism. The University of North
Telehealth mask wearing training for children with Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child
autism during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Development Institute, National Clearinghouse on
Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(1), 70–86. https://doi. Autism Evidence and Practice Review Team.
org/10.1002/jaba.802 Tanner, A., & Andreone, B.  E. (2015). Using graduated
Skinner, B. F. (1935). Two types of conditioned reflex and exposure and differential reinforcement to increase
a pseudo type. The Journal of General Psychology, food repertoire in a child with autism. Behavior
12(1), 66–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1935 Analysis in Practice, 8(2), 233–240. https://doi.
.9920088 org/10.1007/s40617-­015-­0077-­9
Skinner, B. F. (1937). Two types of conditioned reflex: A This smart University of Minnesota rat works slot machine
reply to Konorski and Miller. The Journal of General for a living. (1937, May 31). Life, 80–81.
Psychology, 16(1), 272–279. https://doi.org/10.1080/0 Turner, V. R., Ledford, J. R., Lord, A. K., & Harbin, E. R.
0221309.1937.9917951 (2020). Response shaping to improve food acceptance
Skinner, B.  F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An for children with autism: Effects of small and large
experimental analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. food sets. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 98,
Skinner, B.  F. (1951). How to teach animals. Scientific 103574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103574
American, 185(6), 26–29. Wolf, M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. (1964). Application
Skinner, B.  F. (1957). Verbal behavior. of operant conditioning procedures to the behav-
Appleton-Century-Crofts. iour problems of an autistic child. Behaviour
Skinner, B. F. (1958). Reinforcement today. The American Research and Therapy, 1(2), 305–312. https://doi.
Psychologist, 13(3), 94–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/ org/10.1016/0005-­7967(63)90045-­7
h0049039
Functional Analysis Methodology:
Best Practices and Considerations 22
Claudia L. Dozier, Adam M. Briggs,
Kathleen M. Holehan, Nicole A. Kanaman,
and Jessica F. Juanico

22.1 Functional Analysis Gitlesen, 2004; Kahng et  al., 2002). Risks to
Methodology: Best Practices caregivers also include potential injury, as well as
and Considerations financial costs associated with property destruc-
tion and enhanced care or intervention and care-
Commonly reported challenging behavior dis- giver stress (Arora et al., 2019; Doehring et al.,
played by individuals with autism spectrum dis- 2014; Einfeld et  al., 2010; Kurtz et  al., 2020;
order (ASD) and related disabilities include Manan et  al., 2018; Pisula, 2007). Given these
physical aggression (e.g., hitting, kicking, pinch- and other risks associated with the occurrence of
ing, or pulling hair of others), self-injurious challenging behavior in individuals with ASD
behavior (SIB; e.g., biting, scratching, or hitting and related disorders, an important goal of clini-
oneself), property destruction (e.g., breaking, cal practice and research is the assessment and
throwing, or defacing items), pica (i.e., ingestion treatment of challenging behavior.
of inedible objects such as paperclips and dirt), Functional behavioral assessment (FBA) is a
elopement (i.e., running away or leaving an area term used to describe various methods to deter-
of supervision), and disruptive repetitive behav- mine environmental events that influence the
ior (Condillac, 2007; Didden et al., 2012; Matson occurrence of challenging behavior. The out-
et al., 2009; McClintock et al., 2003). These chal- comes of an FBA are used to derive interventions
lenging behaviors may lead to various risks for to decrease and prevent the occurrence of chal-
the individual and their caregivers (e.g., family lenging behavior and have been promoted as a
members, teachers, staff; Hagopian, Dozier, humanistic approach to deriving interventions
et  al., 2013). Risks for the individual include (Hanley, 2012). Common FBA methods include
injury or harm, interference with learning skills indirect assessments (e.g., interviews and ques-
and accessing less restrictive environments, and tionnaires; Gadaire et  al., 2021), descriptive
increased use of psychotropic medication and assessments (i.e., direct observation and mea-
physical restraint, all of which may influence surement of target behavior and relevant anteced-
quality of life (Doehring et al., 2014; Holden & ent and consequences surrounding the behavior;
Thompson & Borrero, 2021), and experimental
C. L. Dozier (*) · K. M. Holehan · N. A. Kanaman (functional) analysis (FA; i.e., direct observation
J. F. Juanico and measurement of target challenging behavior
University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA while antecedents and consequences are system-
e-mail: cdozier@ku.edu
atically manipulated; Beavers et al., 2013; Hanley
A. M. Briggs et  al., 2003; Iwata et  al., 1982/1994). Although
Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti, MI, USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 417
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_22
418 C. L. Dozier et al.

indirect assessments and descriptive assessments vations) are used to determine antecedents and
are commonly used in lieu of FAs in practice (see consequences that are manipulated in FA condi-
Oliver et al., 2015; Roscoe, Phillips, et al., 2015), tions (Dracobly et  al., 2018; Neidert, Rooker,
there are limitations to their use for determining et  al., 2013; Rooker et  al., 2015). During a test
the function of challenging behavior and deriving condition, a potential reinforcement contingency
interventions. Indirect assessments are often is programmed for target challenging behavior,
unreliable in that outcomes across caregiver which includes (a) an establishing operation (EO)
informants often do not match and thus suggest programmed to increase the value of the putative
different functions (e.g., Dracobly et  al., 2018; reinforcer, (b) a discriminative stimulus (Sd) that
Saini, Ubdegrove, et  al., 2020). Furthermore, signals the availability of the putative reinforcer,
indirect assessments may not provide clear out- and (c) the delivery of the putative reinforcer
comes regarding potential functional variables or contingent on the occurrence of target challeng-
may hypothesize all functional variables (Rooker ing behavior (Hanley et al., 2003; Iwata & Dozier,
et  al., 2015). Descriptive assessments provide 2008). During a control condition, the potential
only correlational data and often suggest func- reinforcement contingency is absent. That is, the
tions (e.g., attention) that mask relevant functions EO, Sd, and putative reinforcer are not present
(e.g., Camp et al., 2009; St. Peter et al., 2005) and (Iwata & Dozier, 2008; Thompson & Iwata,
thus may produce false-positive or false-negative 2005). Higher levels of challenging behavior in a
outcomes. Functional analysis is the only FBA test condition as compared to a control condition
method that has almost 40 years of empirical sup- suggest there is a functional relationship between
port regarding its reliability and validity. the environmental event manipulated in the test
Furthermore, it is the only FBA method that condition and the occurrence(s) of the challeng-
allows for determination of a functional (cause-­ ing behavior. The development of FA methodol-
effect) relation between an environmental event ogy has allowed clinicians to derive more
and the occurrence of challenging behavior and effective and socially valid interventions and
thus is the gold standard in FBA methodology. In reduce reliance on punishment procedures and
fact, researchers have suggested FAs are a critical pharmacological interventions (Axelrod, 1987;
step in the assessment and effective intervention Kahng et  al., 2002; Mace et  al., 1991). That is,
of challenging behavior (Beavers et  al., 2013; once the function of a challenging behavior is
Hanley et al., 2003; Slaton & Hanley, 2018). determined via an FA, function-based interven-
The purpose of the current chapter is to pro- tions can be derived to (a) decrease the motiva-
vide an overview of FA methodology as an tion to engage in the challenging behavior, (b)
evidence-­ based procedure, describe important eliminate the reinforcer for the challenging
aspects for designing and conducting FAs, briefly behavior, and/or (c) provide the reinforcer for an
discuss the analysis of FA outcomes, and review alternative response (Beavers et  al., 2013;
modifications of FA methodology that may be Hagopian, Dozier, et  al., 2013; Hanley et  al.,
useful in various contexts or situations. 2003). Furthermore, large-scale studies have sug-
gested that FAs have allowed for determination
of the antecedent and consequent conditions in
22.2 Overview of FA Methodology which challenging behavior within and across
populations occurs, which has been suggested to
Functional analyses involve direct observation be useful for designing environments to prevent
and measurement of the occurrence of target the occurrence of challenging behavior (Ala’i-­
behavior while manipulating relevant anteced- Rosales et al., 2019; Hanley, 2011).
ents and consequences under at least one test The first comprehensive FA methodology was
condition and a control condition (Iwata & developed and evaluated by Iwata et  al.
Dozier, 2008). Often, information from indirect (1982/1994) to determine the influence of com-
and descriptive assessments (or informal obser- mon environmental events on the occurrence of
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 419

SIB displayed by nine individuals diagnosed for more information on the generality of FA
with intellectual and developmental disabilities methodology.
(IDD). The researchers measured the occurrence In addition to the generality of FA methodol-
of SIB during 15-min sessions that included three ogy, research has shown the flexibility of FA
test conditions and one control condition con- methodology in determining the influence of not
ducted using a multielement design. Test condi- only common variables manipulated in Iwata
tions were designed to concurrently assess et  al. (1982/1994) (i.e., attention, escape from
whether SIB was maintained by social positive demands, and sensory reinforcers) but also vari-
reinforcement in the form of attention (i.e., brief ous other variables such as access to tangibles
reprimands and physical attention), social nega- (i.e., preferred items and activities, preferred edi-
tive reinforcement in the form of escape from bles), access to attention within the context of
demands (i.e., academic, self-care, or medical diverted-attention antecedent situations (i.e.,
demands), or automatic reinforcement in the when attention is delivered to someone else),
form of sensory stimulation or pain attenuation. social escape (i.e., escape from interactions with
The control condition was designed to address all others), as well as more complex contingencies
variables manipulated during the three test condi- such as escape to a preferred activity or attention
tions. Following repeated exposure to the four (e.g., conversation about preferred topics), escape
conditions, results showed consistently higher to access automatic reinforcement (e.g., ritualis-
levels of SIB in specific test conditions as com- tic behavior or stereotypy), access to self-­
pared to the control condition for six of the nine restraint, and access to compliance with
participants. These results suggested there is util- participant mands (Hanley et al., 2014). Below is
ity in a comprehensive operant methodology for a description of some of the more common FA
determining the variables maintaining challeng- test conditions and the omnibus control condition
ing behavior. used in traditional FA methodology.
Due to the success of the FA methodology
developed by Iwata et  al. (1982/1994), many
researchers have replicated and extended this 22.2.1 Tests for Social Positive
“traditional” FA methodology suggesting the Reinforcement
generality of the methodology. That is, research-
ers have shown FA methodology to be useful in Tests for maintenance of challenging behavior by
assessing the function of other challenging social positive reinforcement have included an
behavior including physical aggression, pica, attention test condition (e.g., Fischer et al., 1997;
property destruction, disruptive repetitive behav- Richman & Hagopian, 1999), a diverted-­attention
ior, elopement, food refusal, and inappropriate test condition (e.g., Fahmie, Iwata, Harper, et al.
sexual behavior. In addition, FAs have shown to 2013; O’Reilly et  al., 2000; Strohmeier et  al.,
be useful in determining the function of challeng- 2014), and a tangible test condition (e.g., Mace &
ing behavior in various populations including West, 1986; Reed et al., 2009). During an atten-
individuals with ASD and related disabilities, tion test condition, the EO is deprivation from
children with attention deficit hyperactivity dis- attention (i.e., the therapist ignores the partici-
order, typically developing children, individuals pant). The programmed Sd is the presence of the
with Tourette syndrome, and individuals with therapist in the room while engaged in a task
Prader-Will syndrome. Finally, FAs have been activity (e.g., reading a magazine). The pro-
conducted in various settings including hospital grammed reinforcer is brief delivery of attention
settings, schools or classrooms, homes or resi- that has been reported or observed to be delivered
dences, the community (e.g., playground), and to in the natural environment (e.g., verbal repri-
individuals in remote locations using telehealth. mand, statements of concern, various types of
See Beavers et al. (2013) and Hanley et al. (2003) physical attention, preferred conversation).
During a diverted-attention test condition, the EO
420 C. L. Dozier et al.

is deprivation from attention, as well as therapist The programmed Sd is the presence of the thera-
delivery of attention to another person present in pist and task materials. The programmed rein-
the room (e.g., peer or another adult). The pro- forcer is brief removal of demands and materials
grammed Sd is the presence of the two individu- (i.e., escape). Higher levels of challenging behav-
als interacting. The programmed reinforcer is ior in this condition as compared to the control
brief delivery of attention as in the attention test condition suggest maintenance by social negative
condition. Furthermore, in attention conditions, reinforcement in form of escape from demands.
best practice involves providing the individual During a social-escape test condition, the EO is
access to a low or moderately preferred item to continuous social interaction (i.e., the therapist
compete with automatically reinforced challeng- interacts with the individual in close proximity).
ing behavior but not the occurrence of attention-­ The programmed Sd is the presence of the thera-
maintained challenging behavior (Roscoe et al., pist near the individual. The programmed rein-
2008). Higher levels of challenging behavior in forcer is brief removal of social interaction (i.e.,
one of these test conditions as compared to the the therapist stops interacting with the individual
control condition suggest maintenance by social and moves away). Higher levels of challenging
positive reinforcement in the form of attention. behavior in this test condition as compared to a
During a tangible test condition, the EO is control condition suggest maintenance by social
removal of a preferred item or activity (as deter- negative reinforcement in the form of escape
mined by indirect assessment, direct observation, from social interactions (Harper et  al., 2013;
or a systematic preference assessment). The pro- Taylor & Carr 1992a, 1992b).
grammed Sd is the presence of preferred items
and activities. The programmed reinforcer is
brief delivery of preferred items or activities. 22.2.3 Test for Automatic
Higher levels of challenging behavior in this test Reinforcement
condition as compared to a control condition sug-
gest maintenance by social positive reinforce- Tests for maintenance of challenging behavior by
ment in the form of tangibles. It is important to automatic reinforcement include an alone or
note that tests for a tangible function should only ignore test condition (e.g., Querim et  al., 2013;
be included if strong evidence from indirect and Vollmer et al., 1995). In the no interaction/alone
direct assessments suggest a potential functional condition, the EO is deprivation from stimulation
relation to avoid false-positive outcomes (Rooker (i.e., a barren environment). The Sd is the pres-
et al., 2011). ence of a barren environment either while the
individual is alone in the room or in the room
with the therapist to ensure safety. The reinforcer
22.2.2 Tests for Social Negative is not mediated by others, and thus not pro-
Reinforcement grammed, but is produced by engaging in the
challenging behavior (e.g., hand flapping may
Tests for maintenance of challenging behavior by produce visual stimulation; hand-to-head SIB
social negative reinforcement have included an may attenuate pain). Higher levels of challenging
escape from demands test condition (e.g., Iwata, behavior in this condition that persists over time
Pace, Kalsher, et  al., 1990; McComas et  al., suggest maintenance by automatic
2000) and an escape from social interaction test reinforcement.
condition (e.g., Harper et  al., 2013; Taylor &
Carr, 1992a, 1992b). During an escape from
demands test condition, the EO is presentation of 22.2.4 Control Condition
learning trials for tasks that have been reported or
observed to be nonpreferred or difficult (e.g., A common control condition used in FA method-
self-care tasks, academic tasks, vocational tasks). ology is an omnibus control condition, often
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 421

referred to as the play condition (Fahmie, Iwata, (e) session duration, (f) experimental design of
Querim, et al., 2013; Iwata et al., 1982/1994). In FA, and (g) other best practices for promoting
this condition, all variables manipulated in test clear and efficient FA outcomes.
conditions are controlled for by programming for
the absence of relevant EOs and no longer pro-
gramming reinforcers for challenging behavior. 22.3.1 Determine Target Challenging
Thus, the antecedents programmed in the play Behavior
condition include continuous access to preferred
items and activities to interfere with the occur- One of the first steps in designing an FA is to
rence of automatically reinforced challenging determine which challenging behavior(s) will be
behavior, continuous access to preferred atten- assessed. To determine this, clinicians should use
tion to decrease the motivation to engage in chal- indirect assessments and descriptive assessments
lenging behavior to access attention, and no (or informal direct observations) to identify chal-
presentation of demands (or other aversive lenging behavior displayed by the target individ-
events). Furthermore, there are no programmed ual and to derive operational definitions of the
reinforcers for challenging behavior. Thus, low behavior. Once challenging behavior and their
levels of target challenging behavior should be definitions are determined, a clinician must deter-
observed in this condition, which make it an ideal mine the relative priority of the individual’s chal-
control condition under most situations (Betz & lenging behavior (i.e., the order with which
Fisher, 2011; Iwata & Dozier, 2008). However, behavior should be assessed and treated).
the play condition may not be the best control Challenging behavior that poses a risk to the
condition for challenging behavior potentially health and safety of the individual or those around
maintained by escape from social interaction them or interferes with the individual’s learning
because it involves dense schedules of attention or quality of life (i.e., access to long- and short-­
delivery (Fahmie, Iwata, Querim, et  al., 2013; term reinforcers) should be prioritized for assess-
Harper et al., 2013). Therefore, an ignore or alone ment and treatment (Neidert, Rooker, et  al.,
control condition may better control for the vari- 2013). Further, challenging behavior that occurs
ables manipulated in this test condition (Fahmie, at high frequencies should be prioritized over
Iwata, Querim, et al., 2013). behavior that occurs infrequently. However, if a
low-frequency challenging behavior is of con-
cern, various modifications to FA methodology
22.3 Designing Functional may be necessary (e.g., extend the session dura-
Analyses tion or conduct under more naturalistic condi-
tions; Kahng et al., 2001) as it may be difficult to
In addition to the generality and flexibility of FA identify the behavioral function (Davis et  al.,
methodology, research has suggested refinements 2012). Additional information regarding these
to traditional FA methodology, which has culmi- and other modifications is discussed below.
nated in suggestions for best practice, particu- In many cases, an individual may engage in
larly with respect to increasing the efficacy, multiple challenging behaviors that require
efficiency, and safety of the methodology assessment and intervention (i.e., each behavior
(Beavers et al., 2013; Hanley et al., 2003; Saini, poses risks or barriers for the individual). In these
Fisher, et al., 2020). Thus, the information in this situations, a clinician may have difficulty assign-
section outlines best practices in designing FAs, ing priority and may opt to assess multiple topog-
which includes discussion of determining (a) tar- raphies of challenging behavior simultaneously
get challenging behavior, (b) procedural prac- (i.e., place contingencies on multiple response
tices and safeguards in conducting FAs of severe topographies during the FA). Although this is a
challenging behavior, (c) FA conditions, (d) set- common practice that continues to increase in the
ting, therapists, and modality to conduct an FA, literature (Beavers et al., 2013), it is important for
422 C. L. Dozier et al.

clinicians to consider the implications this prac- rately, which may lead to more differentiated out-
tice may have on the outcomes of the assessment. comes (Derby et al., 2000).
That is, combining challenging behavior topog-
raphies into one assessment may mask the true
function of individual topographies of behavior 22.3.2 Determine Procedural
(Asmus et al., 2013). For example, as shown by Safeguards and Practices
Asmus et  al. (2013), when stereotypic behavior
and disruptive behavior were combined in one Because FAs are designed to evoke and reinforce
FA the occurrence of stereotypic behavior the occurrence of challenging behavior, the target
occurred at high levels across conditions (sug- individual and therapist may be at risk for injury
gesting an automatic function), which masked or harm (Hanley et al., 2003; Fisher et al., 2013).
the function of disruption that occurred at lower Thus, once the target challenging behavior has
levels. It was not until the two behaviors were been identified and objectively defined, it is
assessed in separate analyses that a clear function important for clinicians to conduct a risk assess-
was identified for the participant’s disruption, ment to determine whether the benefits associ-
which was maintained by social variables. In ated with conducting an FA of challenging
addition to masking, the function of a single behavior outweigh the risks (Deochand et  al.,
response topography, including multiple topogra- 2020; Fisher et al., 2013; Iwata & Dozier, 2008;
phies of behaviors in one FA, may increase the Wiskirchen et al., 2017). Furthermore, based on
likelihood of obtaining results that suggest the the results of this assessment, clinicians can
behaviors are maintained by multiple sources of develop appropriate procedural safeguards to
reinforcement (i.e., multiply controlled; Beavers reduce risks and maximize benefits in conducting
& Iwata, 2011). This outcome may result in inter- an FA with a particular individual (Saini et  al.,
ventions that are ineffective or countertherapeu- 2021; Deochand et  al., 2020; Iwata & Dozier,
tic for some functions (e.g., Smith et al., 1993). 2008; Weeden et  al., 2010). Wiskirchen et  al.
However, assessing several target behaviors (2017) proposed a risk assessment as a formal
simultaneously in a FA may be more efficient, clinical decision-making model and recom-
which is an important consideration for clini- mended assessing and managing risk across four
cians. One option is to target the most problem- primary domains: (a) clinical experience of those
atic challenging behavior in the FA (i.e., place conducting the FA, (b) intensity of the target
contingencies on this behavior) while simultane- challenging behavior, (c) suitability of the physi-
ously collecting data on graphing and analyzing cal environment in which the FA will be con-
the occurrence of other relevant topographies ducted, and (d) the number and composition of
(Bell & Fahmie, 2018). However, if a clinician support staff available to assist in the FA.  We
decides to place contingencies on multiple chal- review considerations for each of these domains
lenging behaviors in an FA, it is recommended below.
that behaviors are limited to only a few topogra- Various professionals should be involved in
phies (Hanley et al., 2003) that are likely to be in the design, implementation, and oversight of FAs
the same response class (Saini et al., 2020). That of severe challenging behavior. First, to the extent
is, including challenging behavior that is likely to possible, clinicians should consider medical eval-
be maintained by social reinforcement or chal- uation and professional oversight to (a) deter-
lenging behavior likely to be maintained by auto- mine whether behavioral assessment should be
matic reinforcement, rather than including all conducted, (b) provide ongoing evaluation of
response classes, will decrease the likelihood of potential risk or injury, and (c) determine when
masking relevant functions or suggesting multi- FA sessions should be terminated (Iwata et  al.,
ple control outcomes. Furthermore, when includ- 1982/1994). Although ongoing and day-to-day
ing multiple topographies, it is best practice to medical professional oversight is feasible at inpa-
graph and analyze data for each topography sepa- tient facilities and hospitals, other environments
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 423

do not have medical staff to conduct ongoing greater risk of injury than what occurs outside of
evaluations. Thus, clinicians should exercise the FA.  To empirically evaluate whether chal-
additional procedural safeguards to ensure the lenging behavior is more dangerous inside or out-
safety of the individual. Another professional that side of an FA, Kahng et  al. (2015) reviewed
should be involved in the development and imple- records of 99 participants admitted for the assess-
mentation of an FA is a Board Certified Behavior ment and treatment of SIB to determine the
Analyst (BCBA) with expertise in FA methodol- amount and severity of injuries sustained within
ogy and function-based intervention. In addition, versus outside of the FA context using a severity
the BCBA and therapists associated with the case index scale (i.e., Self-Injury Trauma [SIT] Scale;
should be trained to implement best practices to Iwata, Pace, Kissel, et al., 1990). Results showed
increase the safety of the individual and others that although the rate of injury during the FA was
involved (Hanley, 2012). relatively higher than outside of the FA, levels
When considering the intensity of the target were low regardless of the context. Additionally,
challenging behavior, one should ask questions when injuries did occur, they scored very low on
about (a) the utility of alternative assessment pro- the severity index scale. This is particularly
cedures in lieu of conducting an FA, (b) whether important given the severity of the SIB displayed
the challenging behavior will be more intense in by these participants and may suggest even less
or out of the assessment, and (c) whether the injury for individuals with less severe SIB. It is
occurrence of the challenging behavior will result also important to note that research suggests FAs
in injury (Hanley, 2012). Challenging behaviors are not likely to increase the intensity or occur-
that are life-threatening or may result in hospital- rence of challenging behavior outside of the FA
ization or severe harm should be assessed using (e.g., in the classroom or home; Shabani et  al.,
other FBA methods. Furthermore, modifications 2013).
to standard FA methodology may need to be In addition to considering professional over-
addressed to decrease harm, if it is determined sight and intensity of the behavior, clinicians
that an FA is feasible (see below). However, it is should consider additional safety procedures
important to consider that the reason an FA is when conducting FAs of challenging behavior
considered is because the individual already reg- that may cause harm to the individual or others.
ularly engages in the challenging behavior in These procedures include (a) modifications to the
their daily life and likely contacts contingencies assessment environment to increase the safety of
that are programmed in an FA (Kahng et  al., all parties involved in the FA, (b) a system for
2015). monitoring injuries and preventing escalation of
Determining whether the target individual will challenging behavior, and (c) the presence of
be at greater risk during the FA than they nor- trained staff who can provide first aid for minor
mally experience throughout a typical day pres- injuries (Iwata & Dozier, 2008; Neidert, Rooker,
ents an additional consideration of risk mentioned et al., 2013). First, clinicians should ensure that
by Hanley (2012). Best practice in FA methodol- the assessment environment includes soft stimuli
ogy involves various procedures (e.g., program- (e.g., toys and task materials) and padded floors,
ming Sds to enhance discriminated responding, walls, and tables to reduce the likelihood of
providing reinforcers on a continuous schedule, injury or destruction from throwing items or SIB
and providing consequences for lower-intensity (Hanley, 2012). Second, monitoring of injuries
behavior or attempts at behavior; see additional should be ongoing; clinicians should conduct
information below) that are specifically included routine evaluations of injuries prior to each ses-
to produce lower levels of target challenging sion, following every few sessions if little or no
behavior as those observed in the everyday envi- challenging behavior occurs, and following ses-
ronment (Kahng et  al., 2015). Thus, the occur- sions in which the participant’s physical condi-
rence of the target challenging behavior in an FA tion or level of responding meet a criterion for
may not expose the individual or others to any termination. In addition, clinicians should create
424 C. L. Dozier et al.

session-termination criteria to prevent injury. devices or clothing that are worn by the target
Sessions can be programmed to terminate fol- individual or therapist, as well as materials or
lowing minor tissue damage such as reddening or devices placed in the environment to prevent
breaking of the skin (Betz & Fisher, 2011) or injury and decrease risks. Clinicians should use
based on the frequency of challenging behavior the results of a risk assessment and the topogra-
that occurs in the session or a period of time dur- phy and severity of the challenging behavior to
ing the session (e.g., Iwata et  al., 1982/1994). inform the type of protective equipment to use.
Third, all primary staff should be trained in basic For example, an individual with head-directed
first aid such that they can perform immediate SIB might wear a padded helmet to help prevent
treatment on minor injuries, as well as when they injury to their head. For instances of physical
should call for medical assistance for more aggression, the therapist may wear arm, chest, or
involved injury. Furthermore, additional trained leg pads under their clothing to protect them
staff may be necessary to help ensure safety and from biting, hitting, or kicking displayed by the
implement crisis management procedures (Kahng individual. Despite the potential advantages of
et al., 2015). Finally, all safety procedures should using protective equipment, their use may inter-
be reviewed during the informed consent and fere with determining the function of challeng-
assent process. For this process, the clinician ing behavior and in some situations may
should provide the rationale and method for con- exacerbate challenging behavior. For example,
ducting an FA, explain the results of the risk previous research has shown that wearing pro-
assessment, and describe the safeguards used to tective equipment for SIB may obscure FA
address these potential risks (Iwata & Dozier, results when the challenging behavior is main-
2008; Neidert, Rooker, et al., 2013). tained by automatic reinforcement because pro-
In addition to the procedural safeguards men- tective equipment may function as sensory
tioned above, clinicians should consider other extinction or punishment for engaging in the
strategies for blocking or interfering with the automatically reinforced challenging behavior
occurrence of challenging behavior to protect the (Borrero et al., 2002; Le & Smith, 2002; Moore
individual and others from harm or injury. et  al., 2004). As with response blocking,
Response blocking is a strategy that involves pre- researchers recommend protective equipment be
venting the occurrence of challenging behavior used consistently across all FA conditions
by briefly disrupting the occurrence of a poten- (Neidert, Rooker, et al., 2013).
tially harmful response (Reed et  al., 2013). For In summary, after a risk assessment indicates
example, if an individual were to engage in hand-­ the need for safety procedures, clinicians should
to-­head SIB, the therapist might prevent the indi- consider several procedural safeguards when
vidual’s hand from reaching the head by placing developing and conducting an FA that can help
their hand in the way to block the hand-to-head to ensure the safety of those involved and to
contact. Although response blocking is designed decrease the likelihood of damage to the assess-
to reduce injury, its primary limitation is that it ment environment. Deochand et al. (2020) devel-
might inadvertently function as either a positive oped an FA risk assessment decision-making
punisher or a positive reinforcer, which may tool based on the domains described by
result in false-positive of false-negative outcomes Wiskirchen et  al. (2017) that may assist clini-
(Le & Smith, 2002). If practitioners use response cians when assessing risk and considering proce-
blocking during an FA, researchers recommend it dural safeguards and practices when conducting
be used consistently and across all conditions FAs. Furthermore, researchers have developed
(Neidert, Rooker, et al., 2013). and evaluated methodological modifications to
In addition to response blocking, protective standard FAs to address safety concerns, which
equipment may be used to prevent injury (Fisher involve decreasing the occurrence of severe
et  al., 2013). Protective equipment includes challenging behavior in FAs (see below).
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 425

22.3.3 Design FA Conditions mental events that might evoke and maintain the
target behavior, the clinician may choose to only
Another important consideration in FA method- focus on designing test conditions to assess the
ology is to determine which FA conditions will isolated influence of each of these variables and
be conducted, as well as how those FA conditions need not worry about conducting other test con-
will be conducted. This information is often ditions (Iwata & Dozier, 2008); Holehan et  al.,
derived from information gathered in indirect 2020; Strohmeier et  al., 2014). For example, if
assessments and descriptive assessments. results of these assessments suggest a hypothe-
Although information gained from informant sized automatic reinforcement function, then
responses to indirect assessments are commonly consecutive alone/no interaction test conditions
unreliable (Dracobly et  al., 2018; Saini, may be conducted to determine whether the tar-
Ubdegrove, et al., 2020) and descriptive analyses get behavior persists in the absence of social con-
do not provide information on the functional rela- tingencies. If the target challenging behavior
tion between the events observed (Bijou et  al., persists across alone/no interaction conditions,
1968; Thompson & Iwata, 2007), the results of then the outcomes suggest an automatic rein-
these assessments can be used to generate hypoth- forcement function; if the target challenging
eses about possible maintaining variables, which behavior decreases across conditions, then out-
can guide the development of test conditions and comes suggest maintenance by social contingen-
inform certain aspects about the conditions that cies (Querim et al., 2013), which would require
will be conducted within an FA (Hagopian, further assessment. Furthermore, if results of the
Rooker, et al., 2013). assessments suggest a hypothesized social func-
For instance, the data gathered during indirect tion such as social positive reinforcement in the
and descriptive assessments may be used to form of attention, then an attention test condition
inform specific characteristics of the antecedents can be conducted and rapidly alternated with a
or consequences arranged for each condition. test-specific control condition in which continu-
That is, if an attention test condition is being ous attention is provided (Strohmeier et  al.,
designed, practitioners may look to the results of 2014). Not only does this strategy likely produce
the indirect and descriptive assessments to deter- a more efficient FA given that only the hypothe-
mine the specific type of attention that commonly sized variables maintaining the behavior are
follows the behavior and incorporate this type tested, but this could also minimize the risk of
into the FA condition to recreate the naturally identifying false-positive outcomes due to deliv-
occurring environmental conditions and enhance ering non-indicated, but potent consequences
the ecological validity of the assessment (Kodak (e.g., preferred tangibles) on the occurrence of
et al., 2007). The same can be done for determin- challenging behavior (Retzlaff et  al., 2020;
ing the types of demands or aspects of demand Rooker et al., 2011). However, there are several
delivery (e.g., rate) that are likely to evoke chal- limitations of this strategy, as well as important
lenging behavior and the type of preferred tangi- considerations when using this strategy. One lim-
bles that are withheld or removed that evoke the itation is that additional analyses may be neces-
behavior of concern. Furthermore, researchers sary if clear effects are not observed based on the
have developed methods for systematically iden- hypotheses generated. Another limitation is that
tifying types of attention (e.g., Fisher et al., 1996; other functions of the target behavior may be
Roscoe et al., 2010), types of demands (e.g., Call missed because they were not assessed in the
et  al., 2009; Roscoe et  al., 2009), or tangible FA.  Finally, an important consideration when
items offered (e.g., Fisher et al., 1992) during test conducting only one or two test conditions is
(and control) conditions of an FA. what control condition is best to use (see Fahmie,
In addition, if information gathered during Iwata, Querim, et al., 2013).
indirect or descriptive assessments strongly sug- Finally, if FA outcomes are inconclusive or
gest there are only one or two potential environ- unclear, additional indirect assessments and
426 C. L. Dozier et al.

descriptive assessments may need to be con- setting (Hanley et al., 2003); thus, a practitioner
ducted to determine whether idiosyncratic con- may not be able to approximate the natural condi-
tingencies (antecedents and/or consequences) tions under which a behavior occurs in a con-
should be tested in modified FA conditions trived setting. An intervention derived from FA
(Roscoe, Schlichenmeyer, et al., 2015). This pro- results may only be effective if the contingencies
cess might include conducting a closed- and under which the behavior was observed in the
open-ended indirect assessment for identifying assessment are representative of those contingen-
idiosyncratic events to subsequently test in a cies in the individual’s natural environment (Lang
modified FA (see Schlichenmeyer et al., 2013). et al., 2010; Mace et al., 1991). Thus, it is impor-
tant that clinicians, whether they decide to con-
duct a session in the individual’s natural
22.3.4 Determine Setting, Therapists, environment or in a controlled alternative envi-
and Modality of FA ronment, ensure the setting closely mimics the
conditions under which the individual typically
Although many studies on the use of FAs have engages in challenging behavior.
been conducted in inpatient clinics (Beavers In addition to the setting, another variable that
et al., 2013), research has demonstrated FAs can may influence the outcomes of FAs is who serves
be effectively implemented across various set- as therapist (Saini, Fisher, et al., 2020). Functional
tings. Furthermore, research has shown an analyses have historically included trained clini-
increase in FAs conducted in more naturalistic cians and researchers as therapists resulting in
settings (Beavers et al., 2013; Germansky et al., successful outcomes. However, several compari-
2020). The bias observed in the literature toward son studies (e.g., Kurtz et al., 2013; Ringdahl &
conducting FA sessions in inpatient clinical set- Sellers, 2000; Thomason-Sassi et al., 2013) have
tings may be a product of the increased degree of suggested that who serves as therapist in FAs
control afforded in these environments (Hanley may influence the outcomes. That is, challenging
et al., 2003), particularly with respect to isolating behavior may show a different function based on
the influence of relevant variables. Furthermore, whether a caregiver who has a substantial history
conducting sessions in a controlled setting as with the individual versus an unknown individual
opposed to the natural environment may also serves as therapist. Recent research has shown
allow for greater safety precautions (e.g., padded that caregivers (e.g., teachers, staff, and parents)
session room) to mitigate risk of harm for the can be effectively trained to serve as FA thera-
individual and others (Weeden et  al., 2010). pists (Germansky et  al., 2020) and inclusion of
Finally, conducting sessions within an individu- caregivers as therapists may help to clarify
al’s natural environment may be disruptive to the unclear FA outcomes (Kurtz et al., 2013). Thus,
ongoing activities (e.g., in a school setting or in although including caregivers as therapists may
the community), may require additional staff involve additional planning, training, and coach-
involvement to ensure the safety and adequate ing, it may be one method to enhance the external
supervision of other individuals in the environ- validity of FAs. Furthermore, inclusion of care-
ment (e.g., other children in a classroom), and givers as therapists may increase the likelihood
will likely require upfront planning to control for of increased treatment integrity with their imple-
possible confounding variables present in the set- mentation of the intervention (Germansky et al.,
ting (Iwata & Dozier, 2008; Lang et al., 2010). 2020); however, more research is needed to vali-
Although research has demonstrated that FAs date this possibility.
conducted in controlled environments can effec- With respect to modality of FAs, they have tra-
tively identify contingencies responsible for the ditionally been conducted in person. However,
maintenance of challenging behavior, it is possi- with the rising popularity of telehealth (i.e.,
ble that the relevant stimuli present in the natural health-related services delivered via telecommu-
environment cannot be replicated in the contrived nication technology; Boisvert et  al., 2010) as a
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 427

cost-effective service delivery model in other dis- environment when determining if telehealth is an
ciplines (Morrison et al., 2001), researchers have appropriate modality (Pollard et al., 2017). If the
begun evaluating this modality as a behavioral severity of an individual’s challenging behavior
service delivery modality. Telehealth is a particu- would warrant the use of additional measures in a
larly viable solution for delivering assessment clinical setting to ensure safety (e.g., response
services and behavioral consultation to under- blocking, additional staffing, padding in the
served areas (Bloomfield et  al., 2020) such as room), the clinician should ensure the same
individuals living in rural areas that may have resources are available in the setting in which the
difficulty accessing certain services or may be individual will receive services (e.g., in their
required to travel long distances to receive neces- home, school setting; Lerman et al., 2020). If an
sary support (Bolin et  al., 2015; Pollard et  al., individual engages in severe challenging behav-
2017). Additionally, telehealth may be a service ior and does not have access to the necessary
delivery option for FAs in schools given that they resources to keep them safe, receiving services
may not be consistently implemented in schools via telehealth may not be an appropriate modality
due to constraints in the school setting (e.g., lack for service delivery. For additional information
of FA expertise among school staff; Pennington regarding best practices and problem solving in
et  al., 2017), which may result in challenging telehealth services, we refer the reader to Lerman
behavior being left untreated and, as a result, et al. (2020).
negatively impacting student success. Although using telehealth has been shown to
Thus, given the increased flexibility the tele- cut costs significantly (Wacker et al., 2013), there
communication modality affords, researchers are initial startup costs a practitioner may need to
have evaluated the use of this technology to consider. Wacker et al. (2013) shared that it cost
deliver services effectively and efficiently. With $1800 to set up a clinic-based telecommunication
respect to the use of telehealth for the implemen- system. Although a practitioner may not require
tation of FAs, research has demonstrated the effi- as expensive of a setup as the one used by Wacker
cacy of remote coaching for teachers, parents, et al., a reliable internet connection, a computer
and other staff to implement FA procedures with a webcam, headphones, and HIPPA compli-
across various locations (e.g., in home, at a clinic, ant video software are some of the requirements
in schools; Schieltz & Wacker, 2020). The tele- for delivering services via telehealth successfully
health modality provides clinicians with a cost-­ (Lee et al., 2015).
effective (Wacker et al., 2013) and efficient way
to deliver services even when an individual or
organization (e.g., school, clinical site) has barri- 22.3.5 Session Duration
ers constraining their access to services.
Although telehealth has been demonstrated to Although early studies on FAs involved the use
be a viable modality for behavior analysts to of 15-min sessions (Iwata et  al., 1982/1994),
deliver services, there are several variables to researchers have focused on determining the ses-
consider when determining if this service deliv- sion duration that results in clear FA outcomes.
ery model is right for a change agent or their cli- To determine the influence of shorter session
ent (Lerman et al., 2020). First and foremost, it is durations on the validity of FA outcomes,
recommended that clinicians receive training on researchers have evaluated the extent to which
how to effectively utilize this modality to ensure brief exposure to FA conditions resulted in simi-
competence before delivering services (Cox lar outcomes to FAs containing longer exposure
et al., 2020). Furthermore, clinicians must miti- to conditions (e.g., Griffith et al., 2021; Wallace
gate risk of harm to their client when providing & Iwata, 1999). For example, Wallace and Iwata
services; thus, a clinician must consider the (1999) conducted a retrospective analysis of 46
severity of the individuals challenging behavior FA data sets and analyzed the FA outcomes of the
and the resources available in the individual’s data based on different session durations (5 min
428 C. L. Dozier et al.

vs. 10 min vs. 15 min). Results showed that the similar levels across conditions (i.e., undifferen-
outcomes for 10- and 15-min sessions were iden- tiated), it is unclear whether this pattern impli-
tical, and differences were observed between 5- cates automatic reinforcement as the maintaining
and 15-min sessions in only three cases. Thus, variable or whether the results are due to multiple-­
recommendations for best practice, particularly treatment interference (i.e., carryover effects).
in the assessment of severe challenging behavior, Thus, additional modifications and designs might
are to start with brief, 5-min sessions and extend be necessary to clarify outcomes. If this pattern
session duration if repeated exposure does not does emerge, one way to differentiate automati-
produce clear FA results (Betz & Fisher, 2011). cally reinforced behavior from multiple-­treatment
For example, if the rate of target behavior is low, interference is to conduct a series of extended
it may be necessary to extend session duration to alone/no interaction sessions (Querim et  al.,
allow time for the EO to evoke challenging 2013; Vollmer et al., 1995). If the target response
behavior such that condition-specific conse- does not persist during the extended alone/no
quences can be delivered (Davis et  al., 2012; interaction sessions, automatic reinforcement can
Tarbox et al., 2004). In this case, one way to clar- be ruled out and multiple-treatment interference
ify FA outcomes would be to conduct 10 min or is the likely explanation for the pattern of undif-
longer sessions to increase the likelihood of chal- ferentiated responding. If this is the case, it may
lenging behavior in the FA (Kahng et al., 2001). be that the individual’s behavior is failing to dis-
criminate between the differential antecedent and
consequent variables programmed across each of
22.3.6 Experimental Design the conditions. Therefore, the next step would be
to adjust the design to arrange isolated or
According to Beavers et  al. (2013), most pub- extended exposure to a single test condition using
lished FAs have identified clear behavioral func- a reversal (Vollmer et al., 1993) or a test-control
tions using multielement experimental designs. (pairwise; Iwata et al., 1994) design.
However, there may be situations when a multi-
element design is not feasible or pertinent for 22.3.6.2 Reversal Design
indicating behavioral function and alternative When results are unclear in a multielement
designs may need to be employed such as a rever- design, it may be due to a failure of the individu-
sal design or a sequential, test-control (or pair- al’s behavior to discriminate across several, rap-
wise) design (Iwata et  al., 1994). We describe idly alternating test conditions in the multielement
each of these three approaches below, discuss design (i.e., multiple-treatment interference) or
unique characteristics and considerations for because the EO for engaging in the target behav-
each, and review the pros and cons for each ior is not at strength due to relatively brief expo-
design. sures across alternating test and control
conditions. Therefore, a potential solution might
22.3.6.1 Multielement Design be to arrange the test and control conditions in a
The multielement design is often selected as the more traditional reversal-type design (Vollmer
initial experimental design for an FA because it et al., 1993), in which the test and control condi-
involves rapidly alternating test and control con- tions are presented sequentially. That is, one con-
ditions and is likely to produce more efficient dition (test or control) is presented at a time and
results when compared to using a reversal design. sessions are conducted within each condition
Multielement designs are also less susceptible to until clear trends in responding are observed. By
extraneous variables (e.g., sleep deprivation, ill- programming this extended exposure to each test
ness) given all conditions are equally exposed to condition, it provides an opportunity for the
the variables as they are rapidly alternated. The potential EO to build in strength across consecu-
primary limitation of the multielement design is tive sessions and for the individual’s behavior to
that when levels of challenging behavior occur at experience consistent consequences to clarify the
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 429

target behavior function. The primary limitation approach the design of the initial FA such that it
of this approach is that it is not efficient and is could produce differentiated response patterns as
only recommended when results of the initial quickly as possible without compromising exper-
multielement FA are undifferentiated or when imental precision. However, not all experimental
only a few sessions are required to determine analyses yield conclusive results, and more
behavioral function. extended or complex analyses should be pursued
when behavioral function is not quickly
22.3.6.3 Test-Control (Pairwise) identified.
Design Vollmer et  al. (1995) proposed a decision-­
A sequential, test-control (or pairwise) design making model that was designed to complete an
(Iwata et al., 1994) combines the best features of assessment as quickly as possible while attempt-
the multielement and reversal designs to control ing to establish as much experimental control as
for each of the potential limitations associated possible. For instance, the initial phase (i.e.,
with these approaches. In general, this design Phase 1) consisted of a brief assessment (i.e.,
consists of several phases implemented in a single exposure to each test condition) and only
sequential fashion (like the reversal design); progressed onto a full multielement design in
however, each phase consists of two conditions (a Phase 2 if differentiation was not produced under
test and a control) presented concurrently in a brief exposure to the test and control conditions.
multielement format. By combining these design Next, progression to Phase 3 would only happen
elements, this approach reduces the likelihood of if undifferentiated responding occurred during
interaction effects (limitation of multielement the multielement FA. In this model, Phase 3 con-
design) by alternating between a single test and sisted of exposure to extended alone/no interac-
control condition within each phase and mini- tion sessions to rule out an automatic function.
mizes the primary limitation of the reversal Finally, if behavior did not persist during the
design by programming a control condition Phase 3 test for automatic reinforcement, Phase 4
within each phase (as opposed to it being imple- would consist of experiencing the test and control
mented sequentially in its own phase), which conditions in the reversal-type design described
may be a more efficient method when compared above.
with the reversal design. Although the pairwise Since the publication of Vollmer et al. (1995),
design may not be superior to either the multiele- there have been many advances in FA methodol-
ment or the reversal design, its use might be con- ogy that could be considered and incorporated
sidered when multielement data provide unclear into an FA progression decision-making model.
results or when the effects of multiple variables Hagopian et al. (2013) showed that if initial FAs
cannot be examined efficiently within the context resulted in unclear outcomes, the use of additional
of a reversal design. Therefore, this approach modifications in FA methodology (i.e., experi-
should be considered after the traditional multi- mental design, antecedents, consequences) clari-
element FA outcomes are undifferentiated. fied the outcomes in nearly all cases. Based on
contemporary research on FA modifications, we
22.3.6.4 Progressing from Brief present another FA progression decision model
to More Extended FA that might be used, which is similar to that out-
Methods lined by Henry et al. (2021). First, if it is hypoth-
The overarching goal of FA methodology is to esized that the target behavior could be maintained
employ an approach that accurately identifies the by automatic reinforcement, Phase 1 could con-
maintaining variable(s) of the target challenging sist of consecutive alone/no interaction sessions
behavior in an efficient manner, so the results can to rule this potential function in or out (Querim
be used to inform an effective function-based et al., 2013). Next, if automatic reinforcement is
intervention as soon as possible. Therefore, it not hypothesized or ruled out, Phase 2 could con-
stands to reason that behavior analysts should sist of using a pairwise design to test hypothesized
430 C. L. Dozier et al.

social functions derived from indirect and descrip- different therapists, different rooms or locations,
tive assessments (or other brief methods for test- or different colored shirts worn by therapists
ing common functions such as the trial-based FA) across conditions should be programmed to
and progressing to a traditional (full) FA with best enhance discrimination across conditions
practice recommendations (see Henry et al., 2021) (Conners et al., 2000). Third, presession access to
if results are inconclusive (Phase 3). Following programmed reinforcers should be limited to
Phase 3, if these approaches do not produce dif- decrease the likelihood of satiation effects
ferentiation, Phase 4 could be adopted wherein (O’Reilly & Carey, 1996). Fourth, a period of
session duration could be extended to program time without challenging behavior should be pro-
longer EO exposures or an alternative design grammed before beginning subsequent sessions
could be employed to mitigate potential interac- (McGonigle et al., 1987). Fifth, contingencies for
tion effects associated with alternating between target challenging behavior, as well as less
too many conditions. Finally, if responding is still intense occurrences of challenging behavior (or
undifferentiated in Phase 4, Phase 5 could explore attempts at challenging behavior), should be pro-
the possibility of unique or idiosyncratic environ- grammed on a fixed-ratio (FR1) schedule of rein-
mental variables (e.g., Hagopian, Rooker, et  al., forcement to decrease the severity of challenging
2013; Roscoe, Schlichenmeyer, et  al., 2015; behavior (Hanley, 2012).
Schlichenmeyer et al., 2013) and making modifi-
cations to session conditions (i.e., antecedents,
consequences) or design of the FA to capture 22.4 Analyzing Functional
these unique features of the individual’s everyday Analysis Outcomes
environment.
In FAs, observers collect data on the occurrence
of target challenging behavior, which are then
22.3.7 Additional Considerations summarized (i.e., aggregated for each session)
and graphed for analysis. Visual analysis is com-
Although FAs often result in clear outcomes, monly used to determine the variables maintain-
inconclusive (undifferentiated) outcomes some- ing challenging behavior, which involves
times occur (Hagopian, Rooker, et  al., 2013). decisions based on the level, trend, and variabil-
These outcomes might include (a) low levels of ity of data paths. Results showing higher levels in
behavior across all conditions, (b) high levels of a single test condition as compared to the control
behavior across conditions, or (c) variable levels condition suggest maintenance by the variable.
of behavior across conditions. In addition to the However, results showing higher levels in several
best practices and considerations for designing test conditions as compared to the control condi-
and conducting FAs discussed already (e.g., rule tion suggest multiple controls. Although visual
out medical causes for target behavior, include analysis is the most common procedure for deter-
one or a few target behaviors in FA, determine mining FA outcomes, more structured criteria to
conditions based on pre-FA assessment proce- aid in visual analysis have been proposed (e.g.,
dures, consider alternative designs, program for Hagopian et al., 1997; Roane et al., 2013; Saini
stimuli from the natural the environment, extend et  al., 2018; Standish et  al., 2021) to produce
session duration), several other procedures more reliable and valid interpretations of the
should be included to increase the clarity, safety, data, as well as potentially increasing FA effi-
and efficiency of FAs. First, in traditional FAs, ciency. Furthermore, this approach may serve as
the order of conditions should be programmed in an important aid when training visual analysis
a fixed sequence (i.e., ignore, attention, play, skills to behavior analysts (Saini et  al., 2018).
escape) to capitalize on programmed EOs in pre- However, to date, it is unclear the extent to which
vious conditions (Hammond et  al., 2013). these criteria are used outside of certain research
Second, additional discriminative stimuli such as publications.
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 431

Furthermore, in addition to summarizing and 22.5.1 Trial-Based FA


analyzing aggregate data via visual analysis,
conducting and graphing within-session analy- One modification of FA methodology that may
ses (e.g., Kahng & Iwata, 1999; Roane et  al., address efficiency and safety is the trial-based FA
1999; Vollmer et  al., 1993, 1995) may provide (TBFA; e.g., Austin et  al., 2015; Bloom et  al.,
additional information regarding the function of 2011; Sigafoos & Saggers, 1995). Trial-based
challenging behavior. Within-session analyses FAs are typically conducted within the context of
may involve a minute-by-minute analysis of ongoing activities and tasks and involve a
responding during a session. For example, the discrete-­trial format. That is, practitioners con-
occurrence of challenging behavior across min- duct brief trials (3–7 min in duration) that include
utes in a session may be beneficial for identify- a test segment and a control segment to test
ing potential carryover effects from a previous potential reinforcers for challenging behavior.
session (e.g., initial burst in responding) or During the test segment, the therapist implements
extinction of responding within a session (e.g., programmed antecedents and consequences;
challenging behavior decreases over the session however, contingent upon the occurrence of the
duration). Furthermore, within-session analyses target challenging behavior, the therapist delivers
might involve determining the level of respond- the putative reinforcer (except for no interaction
ing within the EO-on (reinforcer absent) and trials) and then begins the control segment. The
EO-off (reinforcer present) periods of a session, percentage of trials with the target challenging
which may provide additional information behavior in each test condition is compared to the
regarding functional variables (Fahmie & percentage of trials in the relevant control condi-
Hanley, 2008; Kahng & Iwata, 1999; Vollmer tion. Researchers (Bloom et  al., 2011; LaRue
et al., 1993). For example, if most challenging et al., 2010; Rispoli et al., 2013) have compared
behavior occurs when the EO is off during a TBFAs to standard FAs and observed exact cor-
condition, differentiation across conditions may respondence between analyses for 10 out of 17
be irrelevant. Conducting within-session analy- participants (59%; Rispoli et al., 2014). In cases
ses could help to identify the extent to which in which exact correspondence was not attained,
momentary changes in EOs influence the experimenters found partial correspondence
responding. for a subset of other participants (Bloom et  al.,
2011; LaRue et al., 2010).
Some of the primary advantages of imple-
22.5 Variations in FA menting TBFAs is they are easy to implement by
Methodology caregivers (e.g., Bloom et  al., 2013) and more
applicable to assessment during ongoing activi-
Although there is an overwhelming amount of ties in the natural environment (e.g., Bloom et al.,
empirical support for the use of FA methodology 2011; Sigafoos & Saggers, 1995). In addition,
in the assessment and treatment of challenging TBFAs typically require fewer personnel and
behavior, practitioners sometimes avoid its use resources to conduct (Kodak et al., 2013) due to
mainly due to efficiency and safety concerns their relative ease of implementation (Austin
(Oliver et  al., 2015; Roscoe, Schlichenmeyer, et  al., 2015; Dowdy et  al., 2020). Although
et al., 2015). Therefore, researchers have derived TBFAs provide a simple, convenient, efficient,
variations of the methodology as options for and relatively effective method for determining
conducting FAs. These modifications include the function of challenging behavior, several
using trial-based FAs, latency-based FAs, pre- potential disadvantages of this approach exist.
cursor FAs, and synthesized contingency analy- First, the short trial durations of TBFAs result in
ses (SCAs). We provide an overview of these minimized exposure to the putative EO, which
modified FAs and review considerations for their may be insufficient for evoking challenging
use. behavior (Bloom et al., 2011; Dowdy et al., 2020;
432 C. L. Dozier et al.

Rispoli et  al., 2013). Therefore, TBFAs might challenging behavior occurred. Overall, these
require oversight by someone who is able to results showed the precursors were likely to be in
identify appropriate conditions for initiating tri- the same functional response class as the severe
als (i.e., presence of putative EO; Bloom et  al., challenging behavior and suggested this method-
2011; Kodak et al., 2013); otherwise, results may ology might be a way to determine the function
be inconclusive. For example, if the test segment of severe challenging behavior without causing
during the escape test is only 2 min in duration, undue risk to the individual, other therapists, or
this may not be enough time to evoke the occur- damage to the environment. Since this publica-
rence of challenging behavior, thus resulting in a tion, various researchers have replicated and
false-negative outcome. Second, 40% of TBFAs extended this work (e.g., Dracobly & Smith,
conducted in previous studies did not result in 2012; Fritz et  al., 2013; Langdon et  al., 2008;
accurate predictions of the function of challeng- Najdowski et al., 2008).
ing behavior, and extended analyses were Prior to conducting a precursor FA, clinicians
required to determine the variables responsible must first determine which behaviors are precur-
for maintaining challenging behavior (Rispoli sors (i.e., behaviors that are likely to be in the
et al., 2013). same functional response class) to the severe
challenging behavior. To date, researchers have
used several procedures to determine precursors,
22.5.2 Precursor FA which range from simple and efficient methods
such as caregiver interviews (e.g., Najdowski
One method for addressing the safety, and poten- et al., 2008) and direct observations (e.g., Smith
tially the efficiency, of FAs is the precursor FA, & Churchill, 2002) to more time-consuming and
which involves conducting an FA on less severe systematic procedures that include collecting
challenging behavior that reliably precedes the data on the occurrence of potential precursors
occurrence of the target challenging behavior and severe challenging behavior and calculating
(Dracobly & Smith, 2012; Heath & Smith, 2019; various analyses such as conditional probabilities
Smith & Churchill, 2002). This recommendation and lag sequential analyses (e.g., Borrero &
is based on the research on response-class hierar- Borrero, 2008; Fritz et al., 2013).
chies (Harding et  al., 2001; Lalli et  al., 1995; To date, research on precursor FAs suggests it
Shabani et al., 2009), which suggests that poten- is a valid method for determining the function of
tially less severe or problematic behavior (e.g., challenging behavior; however, researchers sug-
whining, crying, fidgeting, yelling) often pre- gest they be reserved for severe challenging
cedes severe challenging behavior and is part of behavior that, if evoked, poses risk to the indi-
the same functional response class. Thus, FAs of vidual or others (Fritz et al., 2013; Saini, Fisher,
the precursor behavior should allow one to infer et al., 2020). Finally, there are two primary limi-
the function of the severe challenging behavior tations associated with precursor FA methodol-
based on the outcome of the FA of the precursor ogy. First, researchers have yet to determine the
behavior. most efficient and effective strategy for identify-
In an early study on precursor FAs, Smith and ing precursors to severe challenging behavior
Churchill (2002) observed that two participants (Lydon et al., 2012). In addition, the current sys-
who engaged in severe challenging behavior also tematic methods suggested for identifying pre-
reliably engaged in less severe behavior prior to cursors include the production of severe
the occurrence of severe challenging behavior challenging behavior (e.g., Smith & Churchill,
(precursor behavior). In separate FAs of the pre- 2002) or are likely too technical and time-­
cursor behavior and severe challenging behavior, consuming for clinicians to implement (e.g., con-
results showed that both behaviors had the same ditional probabilities and lag-sequential analyses;
function, and when the contingency was only Borrero & Borrero, 2008). Thus, Heath and
placed on the precursor, few instances of severe Smith (2019) suggest clinicians use precursor
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 433

FAs only when precursors can be readily identi- each challenging behavior showed correspon-
fied and incorporated into the assessment (i.e., dence on the function of challenging behavior for
not time-consuming or cumbersome to identify). 33 out of 38 data sets (87%). In a second experi-
Second, there may be individuals who display ment, the researchers conducted one latency-­
severe challenging behavior that do not present based FA and one rate-based FA on the
any identifiable precursor behaviors, making this challenging behavior displayed by ten partici-
approach irrelevant for such cases. pants. Results showed correspondence (the same
function) between the two FAs for nine out of ten
participants. Additionally, the results of this study
22.5.3 Latency-Based FA showed that when compared to FAs conducted
using a rate measure, latency-based FAs deter-
Another method for increasing safety and effi- mined the function of challenging behavior with
ciency is the use of a latency-based FA, which fewer instances of challenging behavior. Overall,
involves using a latency measure to the first these data suggest that response latency may be a
occurrence of challenging behavior (e.g., viable measure for target behavior during FAs.
Lambert et  al., 2017; Thomason-Sassi et  al., Furthermore, because this measure requires
2011; Traub & Vollmer, 2019) rather than fewer instances of challenging behavior and
repeated measures (i.e., rate, duration) of the likely results in overall shorter FA duration, this
challenging behavior during FA sessions. For methodology may be quite useful for increasing
instance, during each FA session, the observer the safety of conducting FAs of severe challeng-
records the time that elapses from the beginning ing behavior.
of the session to the first occurrence of challeng- In addition to potentially increasing the safety
ing behavior. In addition, the therapist delivers and efficiency of FAs for severe challenging
the programmed consequence and terminates the behavior, latency-based FAs have several other
session once the challenging behavior occurs. potential advantages. First, the use of a latency
Thus, conditions in which shorter latencies to the measure in FAs may avoid the potential con-
first occurrence of challenging behavior are founds (e.g., extinction of automatically reinforc-
observed as compared to control conditions sug- ing challenging behavior) from the use of
gest variables that maintain challenging blocking or wearing protective equipment during
behavior. FAs (Neidert, Rooker, et  al., 2013). Second, a
Although a common characteristic of FA latency measure may be useful in situations in
methodology involves the repeated occurrence of which the occurrence of the severe challenging
challenging behavior within a session to deter- behavior (e.g., elopement and property destruc-
mine response strength under various environ- tion) makes it difficult to restore the original
mental conditions, previous research (Killeen & environmental condition such that behavior can
Hall, 2001; Thomason-Sassi et  al., 2011) sug- recur within session without introducing a poten-
gests that response latency may also be a good tial confound (Neidert, Iwata, et al., 2013; Traub
measure of response strength. Thomason-Sassi & Vollmer, 2019). For example, elopement can-
et al. (2011) demonstrated that response latency not recur without repeatedly returning an indi-
is a valid measure for challenging behavior dur- vidual to the original location/antecedent context
ing FAs. In one experiment, the researchers cre- each time it occurs, which may introduce extra-
ated two separate graphs for 38 previously neous variables that interfere with determining
conducted FAs using repeated measures. In one the function of challenging behavior. Third, some
graph, the researchers graphed the latency to the challenging behavior (e.g., vomiting) may be
first occurrence of the target behavior using the best assessed using a latency measure because
data streams for each session and used the origi- the occurrence of the behavior is limited due to
nal response repetition measure (e.g., rate) in the physiology.
other graph. Comparisons of the two graphs for
434 C. L. Dozier et al.

Although there are several advantages to the stimulus, and consequence (Beavers et al., 2013;
use of latency-based FAs, there are also some Hanley et al., 2003); however, recent research has
potential limitations. First, because sessions are suggested that differentiated FA outcomes may
terminated contingent upon the first occurrence not be found with isolated contingencies (e.g.,
of challenging behavior, this approach limits an Hanley et al., 2014, Payne et al., 2014) as chal-
individual’s exposure to the number of sessions lenging behavior may be more sensitive to a syn-
and types of session contingencies, which may thesized or combined contingency (Hanley et al.,
interfere with discrimination of session contin- 2014). Specifically, SCA procedures include a
gencies (Thomason-Sassi et al., 2011). However, combination of establishing operations, discrimi-
the use of procedural strategies to enhance dis- native stimuli, and consequences in a single test
crimination of the different conditions (e.g., con- condition. Most recently, researchers have begun
dition signaling stimuli or designs such as the referring to FAs in which contingencies are syn-
pairwise design) might be helpful to address this thesized as Practical Functional Analyses (PFAs;
limitation. Second, because there are no repeated Ferguson et al., 2020; Hanley & Gover, n.d.).
measures within a session, this limits the ability In one of the initial studies on the use of syn-
for within-session analyses that can be useful for thesized contingencies from the beginning of the
clarifying functions of challenging behavior FA process, Hanley et  al. (2014) evaluated
(Vollmer et al., 1993). Third, certain EOs might whether differentiation in challenging behavior
require more exposure than others before their occurred across synthesized test conditions as
influence evokes challenging behavior (e.g., compared to condition-specific control condi-
instructional context may not become aversive tions with three children with ASD and evaluated
until demands are presented over time). Finally, the effects of treatment based on the outcomes of
although the evidence available supports the the FAs. Results showed that all participants’
validity of the latency-based FA, this evidence is challenging behavior occurred at higher levels in
limited in that the generality of the methodology the test condition as compared to the control con-
has not been evaluated and only a handful of dition. Furthermore, interventions derived from
studies with a few participants have validated the synthesized functions of challenging behavior
outcomes of latency-based FAs with function-­ that included functional communication training
based interventions. (FCT) and delay denial training were effective
for decreasing challenging behavior and increas-
ing appropriate behavior. Similar results have
22.5.4 Synthesized Contingency been replicated in additional evaluations of SCAs
Analysis (SCA) (e.g., Jessel et  al., 2016, 2018, 2020; Santiago
et al., 2016).
In addition to synthesizing contingencies to clar- Although research has shown positive out-
ify unclear FA outcomes, practitioners and comes with SCAs, several limitations of the
researchers might conduct an SCA from the methodology have prompted research and dis-
beginning of the FA process when synthesizing cussion (see Slaton & Hanley, 2018; Tiger &
contingencies may be beneficial for situations in Effertz, 2020). The main limitation of SCAs is
which isolating contingencies could pose practi- that contingencies are synthesized in test condi-
cal challenges (e.g., blocking access to toys in the tions, and thus the extent to which isolated con-
home or classroom when a break is delivered in tingencies influence challenging behavior is
the escape condition; Saini et al., 2019; Slaton & unknown (Fisher et  al., 2016; Holehan et  al.,
Hanley, 2018) or when hypotheses following 2020; Jessel et al., 2016; Tiger & Effertz, 2020).
indirect assessment and direct observation Thus, the use of synthesized contingencies with-
strongly suggest an idiosyncratic combined func- out first determining the effects of isolated con-
tion. Typically, FA contingencies have consisted tingencies may lead to interventions based on
of a single establishing operation, discriminative irrelevant variables that could (a) result in more
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 435

complex and resource intensive interventions and may also allow for determination of variables
(b) create additional problems in habilitation and that do not influence challenging behavior or
education of individuals (e.g., delivering escape variables that may be included in intervention
when it is not a maintaining variable for chal- (see Tiger & Effertz, 2020). However, research-
lenging behavior may result in less instructional ers recognize this is not always the case and that
time for the individual; Fisher et al., 2016; Tsami some situations warrant modifications. Therefore,
& Lerman, 2019). To address these limitations, as outlined in various sections above, researchers
researchers have compared the utility of isolated suggest that (a) if the behavior of interest is
versus synthesized contingencies in FA method- severe and there is a high probability the individ-
ology (e.g., Fisher et al., 2016; Greer et al., 2020; ual or therapist will experience injury, a precur-
Holehan et al., 2020; Slaton et al., 2017). Results sor or latency-based FA should be conducted; (b)
indicated synthesizing contingencies are not nec- if the behavior of interest is one that is not easily
essary to produce differentiation, and there are reset after its occurrence or delivery of contin-
little to no differences between treatments gencies might produce a confound (e.g., elope-
informed by isolated and synthesized contin- ment), a latency-based FA or trial-based FA
gency FAs. See Chap. 23 for a more detailed should be conducted; (c) if the environment
review of synthesized contingencies. where the assessment is to be conducted will be
difficult to control or assessment in the natural
environment is preferred, then a trial-based FA
22.6 Conclusions should be conducted; and (d) if a specific func-
tion is suspected via indirect assessment and/or
In summary, almost 40  years of research have direct observation, a consecutive alone/no inter-
shown the efficacy of FA methodology for deter- action series (i.e., suspected automatic function),
mining maintaining variables for challenging pairwise evaluation of the hypothesized social
behavior that are useful for deriving intervention variable, or synthesized contingency analysis
and prevention procedures. Furthermore, from (i.e., suspected combination of functions) should
the moment FA methodology was introduced, be conducted.
there have been consistent efforts to address prac- Although best practices and FA modifications
tical concerns and barriers one may face when have provided support for the flexibility of FA
conducting FAs in clinical environments (Hanley, methodology, it remains unknown whether prac-
2012). Results of this research have culminated titioners are able to independently identify and
in various best practices for conducting FAs as evaluate considerations for which procedure and
outlined in the current paper. In addition, modifi- methodology are most relevant to a specific case.
cations of FA methodology have resulted in pro- Thus, there is a need for a problem-solving tool
cedures to enhance the efficiency and safety of to assist practitioners in choosing the most effi-
FAs. From these modifications, suggestions for cient and effective procedures and methodologies
when to use one methodology over another can under various situations and contexts. Future
be derived. For example, researchers have sug- research is needed to develop and examine the
gested that if there are few constraints with regard degree to which flow charts or problem-solving
to the assessment (e.g., ample amount of time tools may support practitioners in making these
and resources, target behavior is unlikely to result decisions. Furthermore, given what we know
in injury, high degree of control in assessment about the conditions under which challenging
environment), a traditional FA should be con- behavior is evoked and maintained, guidelines
ducted using best practice recommendations for for proactively arranging environments and
developing and implementing various FA condi- establishing systematic prevention procedures in
tions (Tiger & Effertz, 2020; Saini et al., 2020). educational and clinical settings with populations
This not only allows for determination of isolated at risk for developing challenging behavior
functional variables to inform intervention but (Fahmie et  al., 2016; Hanley et  al., 2007) are
436 C. L. Dozier et al.

important next steps. Finally, FA methodology is Bloom, S.  E., Iwata, B.  A., Fritz, J.  N., Roscoe, E.  M.,
& Carreau, A.  B. (2011). Classroom applica-
only as good as the intervention it informs; there- tion of a trial-based functional analysis. Journal of
fore, continued efforts to improve upon methods Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(1), 19–31. https://doi.
for programming durable treatment effects that org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­19
prevent or mitigate relapse are needed. Bloom, S.  E., Lambert, J.  M., Dayton, E., & Samaha,
A.  L. (2013). Teacher-conducted trial-based func-
tional analyses as the basis for intervention. Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 208–218. https://
References doi.org/10.1002/jaba.21
Bloomfield, B. S., Fischer, A. J., King, H., Lehman, E., &
Ala’i-Rosales, S., Cihon, J. H., Currier, T. D. R., Ferguson, Clark, R. R. (2020). Exploring implementor error dur-
J. L., Leaf, J. B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Weinkauf, ing remotely conducted school-based functional anal-
S.  M. (2019). The big four: Functional assessment ysis telehealth training package. Journal of Applied
research informs preventative behavior analysis. School Psychology, 36, 347–375. https://doi.org/10.1
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(1), 222–234. https:// 080/15377903.2020.1749204
doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00291-­9 Bolin, N.  J., Bellamy, G.  R., Ferdinand, A.  O., Vuong,
Arora, S., Goodall, S., Viney, R., & Einfeld, S. (2019). A.  M., Kash, B.  A., Schulze, A., & Helduser, J.  W.
Health-related quality of life amongst primary care- (2015). Rural health people 2020: New decade, same
givers of children with intellectual disability. Journal challenges. Journal of Rural Health, 31, 326–333.
of Intellectual Disability Research, 64(2), 103–116. https://doi.org/10.1111/jrh.12116
https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12701 Borrero, C. S., & Borrero, J. C. (2008). Descriptive and
Asmus, J. M., Franzese, J. C., Conroy, M. A., & Dozier, experimental analyses of potential precursors to prob-
C. L. (2013). Clarifying functional analysis outcomes lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
for disruptive behaviors by controlling consequence 41(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-­83
delivery for stereotypy. School Psychology Review, Borrero, J.  C., Vollmer, T.  R., Wright, C.  S., Lerman,
32(4), 624–630. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.20 D.  C., & Kelley, M.  E. (2002). Further evalua-
03.12086225 tion of the role of protective equipment in the func-
Austin, J.  L., Groves, E.  A., Reynish, L.  C., & Francis, tional analysis of self-injurious behavior. Journal of
L.  L. (2015). Validating trial-based functional analy- Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(1), 69–72. https://doi.
ses in mainstream primary school classrooms. Journal org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­69
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(2), 274–288. https:// Boisvert, M.  K., Russell, L., Andrianopoulos, M.  V., &
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.208 Boscardin, M. L. (2010). Telepresence in the assess-
Axelrod, S. (1987). Functional and structural analy- ment and treatment of individuals with autism spec-
ses of behavior: Approaches leading to reduced trum disorders: A systematic review. Developmental
use of punishment procedures? Research in Neurorehabilitation, 13(6), 423–432. https://doi.org/1
Developmental Disabilities, 8(2), 165–178. https:// 0.3109/17518423.2010.499889
doi.org/10.1016/0891-­4222(87)90001-­1 Call, N. A., Pabico, R. S., & Lomas, J. E. (2009). Use of
Beavers, G. A., & Iwata, B. A. (2011). Prevalence of mul- latency to problem behavior to evaluate demands for
tiply controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied inclusion in functional analyses. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 44(3). https://doi.org/10.1901/ Behavior Analysis, 42(3), 723–728. https://doi.
jaba.2011.44-­593 org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­723
Beavers, G.  A., Iwata, B.  A., & Lerman, D.  C. (2013). Camp, E.  M., Iwata, B.  A., Hammond, J.  L., & Bloom,
Thirty years of research on the functional analysis S. E. (2009). Antecedent versus consequent events as
of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior predictors of problem behavior. Journal of Applied
Analysis, 46(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.30 Behavior Analysis, 42(2), 469–483. https://doi.
Bell, M.  C., & Fahmie, T.  A. (2018). Functional analy- org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­469
sis screening for multiple topographies of problem Condillac, R.  A. (2007). Behavioral intervention and
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(3), intellectual disabilities. In I. Brown & M. Percy (Eds.),
528–537. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.462 A comprehensive guide to intellectual and develop-
Betz, A. M., & Fisher, W. W. (2011). Functional analysis: mental disabilities (pp. 363–372). Brooks.
History and methods. In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, Conners, J., Iwata, B.  A., Kahng, S., Hanley, G.  P.,
& H. S. Roane (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior Worsdell, A.  S., & Thompson, R.  H. (2000).
analysis. The Gulford Press. Differential responding in the presence and absence of
Bijou, S.  W., Peterson, R.  F., & Ault, M.  H. (1968). A discriminative stimuli during multielement functional
method to integrate descriptive and experimental field analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(3),
studies at the level of data and empirical concepts. 299–308. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­299
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(2), 175–191. Cox, D.  J., Plavnick, J.  B., & Brodhead, M.  T. (2020).
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­175 A proposed process for risk mitigation dur-
ing the COVID-­ 19 pandemic. Behavior Analysis
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 437

in Practice, 13(2), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 71–78. https://doi.
s40617-­020-­00430-­1 org/10.1002/jaba.20
Davis, B. J., Kahng, S., Schmidt, J., Bowman, L. G., & Fahmie, T. A., Iwata, B. A., & Mead, S. C. (2016). Within-­
Boelter, E. W. (2012). Alterations to functional analy- subject analysis of a prevention strategy for problem
sis methodology to clarify the functions of low rate, behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(4),
high intensity problem behavior. Behavior Analysis 915–926. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.343
in Practice, 5(1), 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Fahmie, T.  A., Iwata, B.  A., Querim, A.  C., & Harper,
BF03391815 J. M. (2013). Test-specific control conditions for func-
Deochand, N., Eldridge, R. R., & Peterson, S. M. (2020). tional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Toward the development of a functional analysis 46(1), 61–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.9
risk assessment decision tool. Behavior Analysis Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  A., Cihon, J.  H., Milne, C.  M.,
in Practice, 13, 978–990. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Leaf, J. B., McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2020). Practical
s40617-­020-­00433-­y functional assessment: A case study replication and
Derby, K. M., Hagopian, L., Fisher, W. W., Richman, D., extension with a child diagnosed with autism spec-
Augustine, M., Fahs, A., & Thompson, R. (2000). trum disorder. Education and Treatment of Children.
Functional analysis of aberrant behavior through mea- https://doi.org/10.1007/s43494-­020-­00015-­1
surement of separate response topographies. Journal Fischer, S.  M., Iwata, B.  A., & Worsdell, A.  S. (1997).
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 113–117. https:// Attention as an establishing operation and as rein-
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­113 forcement during functional analyses. Journal of
Didden, R., Sturmey, P., Sigafoos, J., Lang, R., O’Reilly, Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(2), 335–338. https://
M.  F., & Lancioni, G.  E. (2012). Nature, preva- doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­335
lence, and characteristics of challenging behavior. In Fisher, W.  W., Greer, B.  D., Romani, P.  W., Zangrillo,
J. Matson (Ed.). Functional assessment for challeng- A. N., & Owen, T. M. (2016). Comparison of synthe-
ing behaviors (pp. 25–44). Springer. sized and individual reinforcement contingencies dur-
Doehring, P., Reichow, B., Palka, T., Phillips, C., & ing functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior
Hagopian, L. (2014). Behavioral approaches to man- Analysis, 49(3), 596–616. https://doi.org/10.1002/
aging intense aggression, self-injury, and destruction jaba.314
in children with autism spectrum and related devel- Fisher, W. W., Ninness, H. A. C., Piazza, C. C., & Owen-­
opmental disorders: A descriptive analysis. Child and DeSchryver, J.  S. (1996). On the reinforcing effects
Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North America, 23(1), of the content of verbal attention. Journal of Applied
25–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2013.08.001 Behavior Analysis, 29(2), 235–238. https://doi.
Dracobly, J. D., Dozier, C. L., Briggs, A. M., & Juanico, org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-­235
J. F. (2018). Reliability and validity of indirect assess- Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian,
ment outcomes: Experts versus caregivers. Learning L.  P., Owens, J.  C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A compari-
and Motivation, 62, 77–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. son of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for
lmot.2017.02.007 persons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal
Dracobly, J.  D., & Smith, R.  G. (2012). Progressing of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(2), 491–498. https://
from identification and functional analysis of precur- doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­491
sor behavior to treatment of self-injurious behavior. Fisher, W.  W., Rodriguez, N.  M., Luczynski, K.  C., &
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(2), 361– Kelley, M. E. (2013). The use of protective equipment
374. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-­361 in the management of severe behavior disorders. In
Dowdy, A., Tincani, M., & Fisher, A. G. (2020). Interrater D. D. Reed, F. D. DiGennaro Reed, & J. K. Luiselli
reliability and convergent validity of the trial-based (Eds.), Handbook of crisis intervention for individu-
functional analysis. Advances in Neurodevelopmental als with developmental disabilities (pp.  87–105).
Disorders, 5, 61–70. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Springer.
s41252-­020-­00174-­7 Fritz, J.  N., Iwata, B.  A., Hammond, J.  L., & Bloom,
Einfeld, S.  L., Ellis, L.  A., Doran, C.  M., Emerson, E., S.  E. (2013). Experimental analysis of precursors to
Horstead, S. K., Madden, R. H., & Tonge, B. J. (2010). severe problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Behavior problems increase costs of care of children Analysis, 46(1), 101–129. https://doi.org/10.1002/
with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Mental Health jaba.27
Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 3(4), 202–209. Gadaire, D.  M., Kelley, M.  E., & LaRue, R.  H. (2021).
https://doi.org/10.1080/19315864.2010.524973 Indirect behavioral assessments: Interviews and rating
Fahmie, T. A., & Hanley, G. P. (2008). Progressing toward scales. In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, & H. S. Roane
data intimacy: A review of within session data analy- (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis (2nd
sis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41(3), 319– Ed.; pp. 193–201). Guilford Press.
331. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-­319 Germansky, S., Reichow, B., Martin, M., & Snyder,
Fahmie, T.  A., Iwata, B.  A., Harper, J.  M., & Querim, P. (2020). A systematic review of caregiver-­
A.  C. (2013). Evaluation of the divided attention implemented functional analyses. Behavior Analysis
condition during functional analyses. Journal of in Practice, 13(3), 698–713. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s40617-­019-­00404-­y
438 C. L. Dozier et al.

Greer, B. D., Mitteer, D. R., Briggs, A. M., Fisher, W. W., Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 16–36. https://doi.
& Sodawasser, A. J. (2020). Comparisons of standard- org/10.1002/jaba.106
ized and interview-informed synthesized reinforce- Harding, J.  W., Wacker, D.  P., Berg, W.  K., Barretto,
ment contingencies relative to functional analysis. A., Winborn, L., & Gardner, A. (2001). Analysis of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(1), 82–101. response class hierarchies with attention-maintained
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.601 problem behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior
Griffith, L. S., Roscoe, E. M., & Cornaglia, R. B. (2021). Analysis, 34(1), 61–64. https://doi.org/10.1901/
A comparison of brief and extended session dura- jaba.2001.34-­61
tion functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Harper, J.  M., Iwata, B.  A., & Camp, E.  M. (2013).
Analysis, 54(3), 1001–1012. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Assessment and treatment of social avoidance. Journal
jaba.820 of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 147–160. https://
Hagopian, L.  P., Dozier, C.  L., Rooker, G., & Jones, doi.org/10.1002/jaba.18
B.  A. (2013). Assessing and treating severe prob- Heath, H., Jr., & Smith, R.  G. (2019). Precursor behav-
lem behavior. In G. J. Madden (Ed.), APA handbook ior and functional analysis: A brief review. Journal of
of behavior analysis (Translating principles into Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(3), 804–810. 10/1002/
practice) (Vol. 2, pp.  353–386). APA. https://doi. jaba.571
org/10.1037/13938-­014 Henry, J. E., Kelley, M. E., LaRue, R. H., Kettering, T. L.,
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Owen-­ Gadaire, D. M., & Sloman, K. N. (2021). Integration of
DeSchryver, J., Iwata, B. A., & Wacker, D. P. (1997). experimental functional analysis procedural advance-
Toward the development of structured criteria for ments: Progressing from brief to extended experimen-
interpretation of functional analysis data. Journal of tal analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(2), 313–326. https:// 54(3), 1045–1061. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.841
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­313 Holden, B., & Gitlesen, J.  P. (2004). The association
Hagopian, L. P., Rooker, G. W., Jessel, J., & DeLeon, I. G. between severity of intellectual disability and psy-
(2013). Initial functional analysis of outcomes and chiatric symptomology. Journal of Intellectual
modifications in pursuit of differentiation: A summary Disability Research, 48(6), 556–562. https://doi.
of 176 inpatient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior org/10.1111/j.1365-­2788.2004.00624.x
Analysis, 46(1), 88–100. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Holehan, K.  M., Dozier, C.  L., Diaz de Villegas, S.  C.,
jaba.25 Jess, R. L., Goddard, K. S., & Foley, B. A. (2020). A
Hammond, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Rooker, G. W., Fritz, J. N., comparison of isolated and synthesized contingencies
& Bloom, S. E. (2013). Effects of fixed versus random in functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior
condition sequencing during multielement functional Analysis, 53(3), 1559–1578. https://doi.org/10.1002/
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), jaba.700
22–30. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.7 Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
Hanley, G. P. (2011). Prevention and treatment of severe & Richman, G.  S. (1982/1994). Toward a functional
problem behavior. In E.  A. Mayville & J.  A Mulick analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
(Eds.), Behavioral foundations of effective autism Analysis, 27(2), 197–209. (reprinted from analysis and
treatment (pp. 233–255). Sloan Publishing. intervention in developmental disabilities, 2(1), 3–20,
Hanley, G.  P. (2012). Functional assessment of problem 1982). https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­197.
behavior: Dispelling myths, overcoming implemen- Iwata, B. A., & Dozier, C. L. (2008). Clinical application
tation obstacles, and developing new lore. Behavior of functional analysis methodology. Behavior Analysis
Analysis in Practice, 5(1), 54–72. https://doi. in Practice, 1(1), 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/
org/10.1007/BF03391818 BF03391714
Hanley, G. & Gover, H. (n.d.) Practical functional assess- Iwata, B.  A., Duncan, B.  A., Zarcone, J.  R., Lerman,
ment: Understanding problem behavior prior to its D.  C., & Shore, B.  A. (1994). A sequential, test-
treatment. Practical Functional Assessment. https:// control methodology for conducting functional
practicalfunctionalassessment.com/ analyses of self-­ injurious behavior. Behavior
Hanley, G.  P., Heal, N.  A., Tiger, J.  H., & Ingvarsson, Modification, 18(3), 289–306. https://doi.
E. T. (2007). Evaluation of a classwide teaching pro- org/10.1177/01454455940183003
gram for developing preschool life skills. Journal of Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery, G. E.,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(2), 277–300. https:// & Cataldo, M.  F. (1990). Experimental analysis and
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.57-­06 extinction of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of
Hanley, G.  P., Iwata, B.  A., & McCord, B.  E. (2003). Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(1), 11–27. https://doi.
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-­11
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(2), 147– Iwata, B.  A., Pace, G.  M., Kissel, R.  C., Nau, P.  A., &
185. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-­147 Farber, J.  M. (1990). The self-injury trauma (SIT)
Hanley, G.  P., Jin, C.  S., Vanselow, N.  R., & Hanratty, scale: A method for quantifying surface tissue dam-
L.  A. (2014). Producing meaningful improvements age caused by self-injurious behavior. Journal of
in problem behavior of children with autism via Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(1), 99–110. https://doi.
synthesized analyses and treatments. Journal of org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-­99
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 439

Jessel, J., Hanley, G.  P., & Ghaemmaghami, M. (2016). hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(4),
Interview-informed synthesized contingency analy- 551–559. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-­551
ses: Thirty replications and reanalysis. Journal of Lambert, J.  M., Lopano, S.  E., Noel, C.  R., & Ritchie,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(3), 576–595. https:// M.  N. (2017). Teacher-conducted, latency-based
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.316 functional analysis as basis for individualized lev-
Jessel, J., Ingvarsson, E. T., Metras, R., Whipple, R., Kirk, els system in a classroom setting. Behavior Analysis
H., & Solsbery, L. (2018). Treatment of elopement in Practice, 10, 422–426. https://doi.org/10.1007/
following a latency-based interview-informed, synthe- s40617-­017-­0200-­1
sized contingency analysis. Behavioral Interventions, Lang, R., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Didden, R., &
33(3), 271–283. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1525 Rispoli, M. (2010). Influence of assessment setting on
Jessel, J., Metras, R., Hanley, G.  P., Jessel, C., & the results of functional analyses of problem behavior.
Ingvarsson, E.  T. (2020). Evaluating the boundaries Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(3), 565–567.
of analytic efficiency and control: A consecutive con- Langdon, N. A., Carr, E. G., & Owen-DeSchryver, J. S.
trolled case series of 26 functional analyses. Journal (2008). Functional analysis of precursors for seri-
of Applied Behavior of Analysis, 53(1), 25–43. https:// ous problem behavior and related intervention.
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.544 Behavior Modification, 32(6), 804–827. https://doi.
Kahng, S., Abt, K.  A., & Schonbachler, H.  E. (2001). org/10.1177/0145445508317943
Assessment and treatment of low-rate high-­intensity LaRue, R.  H., Lenard, K., Weiss, M.  J., Bamond, M.,
problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Palmieri, M., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). Comparison of
Analysis, 34(2), 225–228. https://doi.org/10.1901/ traditional and trial-based methodologies for conduct-
jaba.2001.34-­225 ing functional analyses. Research in Developmental
Kahng, S., Hausman, N.  L., Fisher, A.  B., Donaldson, Disabilities, 31(2), 480–487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
J. M., Cox, J. R., Lugo, M., & Wiskow, K. M. (2015). ridd.2009.10.020
The safety of functional analyses of self-injurious Le, D.  D., & Smith, R.  G. (2002). Functional analysis
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(1), of self-injury with and without protective equip-
107–114. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.168 ment. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Kahng, S., & Iwata, B.  A. (1999). Correspondence Disabilities, 14, 277–290. https://doi.org/10.102
between outcomes of brief and extended functional 3/A:1016028522569
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(2), Lee, J.  F., Schieltz, K.  M., Suess, A.  N., Wacker, D.  P.,
149–160. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­149 Romani, P. W., Lindgren, S. D., Kopelman, T. G., &
Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., & Lewin, A. B. (2002). Behavioral Padilla Dalmau, Y. C. (2015). Guidelines for develop-
treatment of self-injury, 1964-2000. American Journal ing telehealth services and troubleshooting problems
on Mental Retardation, 107(3), 212–221. https://doi. with telehealth technology when coaching parents to
org/10.1352/0895-­8017(2002)107<0212:BTOSIT>2. conduct functional analyses and functional commu-
0.CO;2 nication training in their homes. Behavior Analysis
Killeen, P. R., & Hall, S. S. (2001). The principal compo- in Practice, 8(2), 190–200. https://doi.org/10.1007/
nents of response strength. Journal of the Experimental s40617-­014-­0031-­2
Analysis of Behavior, 75(2), 111–134. https://doi. Lerman, D.  C., O’Brien, M.  J., Neely, L., Call, N.  A.,
org/10.1901/jeab.2001.75-­111 Tsami, L., Schieltz, K.  M., Berg, W.  K., Graber, J.,
Kodak, T., Fisher, W. W., Paden, A., & Dickes, N. (2013). Huang, P., Kopelman, T., & Cooper-Brown, L.  J.
Evaluation of the utility of a discrete-trial functional (2020). Remote coaching of caregivers via tele-
analysis in early intervention classrooms. Journal of health: Challenges and potential solutions. Journal
Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 301–306. https:// of Behavioral Education, 29(2), 195–221. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.2 org/10.1007/s10864-­020-­09378-­2
Kodak, T., Northup, J., & Kelley, M. E. (2007). An evalu- Lydon, S., Healy, O., O’Reilly, M.  F., & Lang, R.
ation of the types of attention that maintain problem (2012). Variations in functional analysis methodol-
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), ogy: A systematic review. Journal of Developmental
167–171. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.43-­06 and Physical Disabilities, 24, 301–326. https://doi.
Kurtz, P.  F., Fodstad, J.  C., Huete, J.  M., & Hagopian, org/10.1007/s10882-­012-­9267-­3
L. P. (2013). Caregiver- and staff-conducted functional Mace, F. C., Lalli, J. S., & Lalli, E. P. (1991). Functional
analysis outcomes: A summary of 52 cases. Journal analysis and treatment of aberrant behavior. Research
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(4), 738–749. https:// in Developmental Disabilities, 12(2), 155–180. https://
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.87 doi.org/10.1016/0891-­4222(91)90004-­C
Kurtz, P.  F., Strohmeier, C.  W., Becraft, J.  L., & Chin, Mace, F.  C., & West, B.  J. (1986). Analysis of demand
M.  D. (2020). Collateral effects of behavioral treat- conditions associated with reluctant speech.
ment for problem behavior on caregiver stress. Journal Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental
of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi. Psychiatry, 17(4), 285–294. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10803-­020-­04694-­y org/10.1016/005-­7916(86)90065-­0
Lalli, J. S., Mace, F. C., Wohn, T., & Livezey, K. (1995). Manan, A.  I. A., Amit, N., Sais, Z., & Ahmad, M.
Identification and modification of a response-class (2018). The influences of parenting stress, children
440 C. L. Dozier et al.

behavioral problems and children quality of life on of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(4), 581–584. https://
depression symptoms among parents of children doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-­581
with autism: Preliminary findings. Journal Sains O’Reilly, M.  F., Lancioni, G.  E., King, L., Lally, G., &
Kesihatan Malaysia, Isu Khas, 137–143. https://doi. Dhomhnail, O. N. (2000). Using brief assessments to
org/10.17576/JKSM-­2018-­19 evaluate aberrant behavior maintained by attention.
Matson, J. L., Wilkins, J., & Macken, J. (2009). The rela- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(1), 109–
tionship of challenging behaviors to severity and symp- 112. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­109
toms of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Mental Payne, S. W., Dozier, C. L., Neidert, P. L., Jowett, E. S.,
Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 2(1), & Newquist, M.  H. (2014). Using additional analy-
29–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/19315860802611415 ses to clarify the function of problem behavior: An
McClintock, K., Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (2003). Risk mark- analysis of two cases. Education and Treatment of
ers associated with challenging behavior in people with Children, 37(2), 249–275. https://doi.org/10.1353/
intellectual disabilities: A meta-analytic study. Journal etc.2014.0017
of Intellectual Disability Research, 47(6), 405–416. Pennington, B., & Pokorski, Kumm, Sterrett, Office of
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-­2788.2003.00517.x Special Education Programs, & National Center for
McComas, J., Hoch, H., Paone, D., & El-Roy, D. (2000). Leadership in Intensive Intervention. (2017). Intensive
Escape behavior during academic tasks: A prelimi- intervention practice guide: School-based functional
nary analysis of idiosyncratic establishing operations. analysis. Office of Special Education Programs, US
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(4), 479–493. Department of Education.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­479 Pisula, E. (2007). A comparative study of stress profiles in
McGonigle, J.  J., Rojahn, J., Dixon, J., & Strain, P.  S. mothers of children with autism and those of children
(1987). Multiple treatment interference in the alternat- with down’s syndrome. Journal of Applied Research
ing treatments design as a function of the intercom- in Intellectual Disabilities, 20(3), 274–278. https://
ponent interval length. Journal of Applied Behavior doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-­3148.2006.00342.x
Analysis, 20(2), 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Pollard, J. S., Karimi, K. A., & Ficcaglia, M. B. (2017).
jaba.1987.20-­171 Ethical considerations in the design and implementa-
Moore, J.  W., Fisher, W.  W., & Pennington, A. (2004). tion of telehealth service delivery model. Behavior
Systematic application and removal of protective Analysis: Research and Practice, 17(4), 298–311.
equipment in the assessment of multiple topographies https://doi.org/10.1037/bar000005
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Querim, A.  C., Iwata, B.  A., Roscoe, E.  M.,
37(1), 73–77. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2004.37-­73 Schlichenmeyer, K.  J., Ortega, J.  V., & Hurl, K.  E.
Morrison, J., Bergauer, N.  K., Jacques, D., Coleman, (2013). Functional analysis screening for problem
S.  K., & Stanziano, G.  J. (2001). Telemedicine: behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement.
Cost-effective management of high-risk pregnancy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 47–60.
Retrieved July 3, 2005, from http://www.managed- https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.26
caremag.com/archives/0111/0111.peer_highrisk.html Reed, D. D., Luiselli, J. K., Miller, J. R., & Kaplan, B. A.
Najdowski, A.  C., Wallace, M.  D., Ellsworth, C.  L., (2013). Therapeutic restraint and protective holding.
MacAleese, A.  N., & Cleveland, J.  M. (2008). In D. D. Reed, F. D. DiGennaro Reed, & J. K. Luiselli
Functional analyses and treatment of precursor behav- (Eds.), Handbook of crisis intervention for individu-
ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41(1), als with developmental disabilities (pp.  107–120).
97–105. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-­97 Springer.
Neidert, P.  L., Iwata, B.  A., Dempsey, C.  M., & Reed, D.  D., Pace, G.  M., & Luiselli, J.  K. (2009). An
Thomason-Sassi, J.  L. (2013). Latency of response investigation into the provision of choice in tan-
during the functional analysis of elopement. Journal gible conditions of a functional analysis. Journal of
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 312–316. https:// Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 21, 485–
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.11 491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-­009-­9159-­3
Neidert, P.  L., Rooker, G.  W., Bayles, M., & Miller, J. Retzlaff, B.  J., Fisher, W.  W., Akers, J.  S., & Greer,
(2013). Functional analysis of problem behavior. In B.  D. (2020). A translational evaluation of poten-
D. D. Reed, F. D. DiGennaro Reed, & J. K. Luiselli tial iatrogenic effects of single and combined con-
(Eds.), Handbook of crisis intervention for individu- tingencies during functional analysis. Journal of
als with developmental disabilities (pp.  147–168). Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(1), 67–81. https://doi.
Springer. org/10.1002/jaba.595
Oliver, A. C., Pratt, L. A., & Normand, M. P. (2015). A Richman, D., & Hagopian, L. (1999). On the effects of
survey of functional behavior assessment methods "quality" of attention in the functional analysis of
used by behavior analysts in practice. Journal of destructive behavior. Research in Developmental
Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(4), 817–829. https:// Disabilities, 20(1), 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.256 S0891-­4222(98)00031-­6
O'Reilly, M. F., & Carey, Y. (1996). A preliminary analy- Ringdahl, J. E., & Sellers, J. A. (2000). The effects of dif-
sis of the effects of prior classroom conditions on per- ferent adults as therapists during functional analyses.
formance under analogue analysis conditions. Journal
22  Functional Analysis Methodology: Best Practices and Considerations 441

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(2), 247–250. Saini, V., Fisher, W.  W., Betz, A.  M., & Piazza, C.  C.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­247 (2021). Functional analysis: History and methods.
Rispoli, M. J., Davis, H. S., Goodwyn, F. D., & Camargo, In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, & H. S. Roane (Eds.),
S. (2013). The use of trial-based functional analy- Handbook of applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.,
sis in public school classrooms for two students pp. 214–233). Guilford Press.
with developmental disabilities. Journal of Positive Saini, V., Fisher, W. W., & Retzlaff, B. J. (2018). Predictive
Behavior Interventions, 15(3), 180–189. https://doi. validity and efficiency of ongoing visual-inspection
org/10.1177/1098300712457420 criteria for interpreting functional analyses. Journal
Rispoli, M., Ninci, J., Neely, L., & Zaini, S. (2014). A of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(2), 303–320. https://
systematic review of trial-based functional analysis doi.org/10.1002/jaba.450
of challenging behavior. Journal of Developmental Saini, V., Fisher, W.  W., Retzlaff, B.  J., & Keevy, M.
and Physical Disabilities, 26, 271–283. https://doi. (2020). Efficiency in functional analysis of prob-
org/10.1007/s10882-­013-­9363-­z lem behavior: A quantitative and qualitative review.
Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Kelley, M. E., Mevers, J. L., Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(1), 44–66.
& Bouxsein, K.  J. (2013). Using modified visual-­ https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.583
inspection criteria to interpret functional analysis out- Saini, V., Kadey, H.  J., Paszek, K.  J., & Roane, H.  S.
comes. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), (2019). A systematic review of functional analysis
130–146. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.13 in pediatric feeding disorders. Journal of Applied
Roane, H. S., Lerman, D. C., Kelley, M. E., & VanCamp, Behavior Analysis, 52(4), 1161–1175. https://doi.
C.  M. (1999). Within-session patterns of responding org/10.1002/jaba.637
during functional analyses: The role of establishing Saini, V., Ubdegrove, K., Biran, S., & Duncan, R. (2020).
operations in clarifying behavioral function. Research A preliminary evaluation of interrater reliability and
in Developmental Disabilities, 20(1), 73–89. https:// concurrent validity of open-ended indirect assessment.
doi.org/10.1016/S0891-­4222(98)00033-­X Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13, 114–125. https://
Rooker, G.  W., DeLeon, I.  G., Borrero, C.  S., Frank-­ doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00364-­3
Crawford, M. A., & Roscoe, E. M. (2015). Reducing Santiago, J.  L., Hanley, G.  P., Moore, K., & Jin, C.  S.
ambiguity in the functional assessment of problem (2016). The generality of interview-informed func-
behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 30(1), 1–35. tional analyses: Systematic replications in school
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1400 and home. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Rooker, G.  W., Iwata, B.  A., Harper, J.  M., Fahmie, Disorders, 46(3), 797–811. https://doi.org/10.1007/
T. A., & Camp, E. M. (2011). False-positive tangible s10803-­015-­2617-­0
outcomes of functional analyses. Journal of Applied Schieltz, K.  M., & Wacker, D.  P. (2020). Functional
Behavior Analysis, 44(4), 737–745. https://doi. assessment and function-based treatment delivered
org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­737 via telehealth: A brief summary. Journal of Applied
Roscoe, E.  M., Carreau, A., MacDonald, J., & Pence, Behavior Analysis, 53(3), 1242–1258. https://doi.
S. T. (2008). Further evaluation of leisure items in the org/10.1002/jaba.742
attention condition of functional analyses. Journal of Schlichenmeyer, K.  J., Roscoe, E.  M., Rooker, G.  W.,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 41(3), 351–364. https:// Wheeler, E. E., & Dube, W. V. (2013). Idiosyncratic
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-­351 variables that affect functional analysis outcomes:
Roscoe, E.  M., Kindle, A.  E., & Pence, S.  T. (2010). A review (2001-2010). Journal of Applied Behavior
Functional analysis and treatment of aggression main- Analysis, 46(1), 339–348. https://doi.org/10.1002/
tained by preferred conversational topics. Journal of jaba.12
Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 723–272. https:// Shabani, D. B., Carr, J. E., Pabico, R. S., Sala, A. P., Lam,
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­723 W. Y., & Oberg, T. L. (2013). The effects of functional
Roscoe, E. M., Phillips, K. M., Kelly, M. A., Farber, R., analysis test sessions on subsequent rates of prob-
& Dube, W. V. (2015). A statewide survey assessing lem behavior in the natural environment. Behavioral
practitioners’ use and perceived utility of functional Interventions, 28(1), 40–47. https://doi.org/10.1002/
assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, bin.1352
48(4), 830–844. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.259 Shabani, D.  B., Carr, J.  E., & Petursdottir, A.  I. (2009).
Roscoe, E. M., Rooker, G. W., Pence, S. T., & Longworth, A laboratory model for studying response-class hier-
L. J. (2009). Assessing the utility of a demand assess- archies. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(1),
ment for functional analysis. Journal of Applied 105–121. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­105
Behavior Analysis, 42(2), 819–825. https://doi. Sigafoos, J., & Saggers, E. (1995). A discrete-trial approach
org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­819 to the functional analysis of aggressive behavior in two
Roscoe, E.  M., Schlichenmeyer, K.  J., & Dube, W.  V. boys with autism. Australia & New Zealand Journal of
(2015). Functional analysis of problem behavior: A Developmental Disabilities, 20(4), 287–297. https://
systematic approach for identifying idiosyncratic vari- doi.org/10.1080/07263869500035621
ables. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(2), Slaton, J. D., & Hanley, G. P. (2018). Nature and scope of
289–314. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.201 synthesis in functional analysis and treatment of prob-
442 C. L. Dozier et al.

lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis (2nd
51(4), 943–973. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.498 ed., pp. 202–213). Guilford Press.
Slaton, J.  D., Hanley, G.  P., & Raftery, K.  J. (2017). Thompson, R.  H., & Iwata, B.  A. (2005). A review of
Interview-informed functional analyses: A compari- reinforcement control procedures. Journal of Applied
son of synthesized and isolated components. Journal Behavior Analysis, 38(2), 257–278. https://doi.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(2), 252–277. https:// org/10.1901/jaba.2005.176-­03
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.384 Thompson, R. H., & Iwata, B. A. (2007). A comparison
Smith, R.  G., & Churchill, R.  M. (2002). Identification of outcomes from descriptive and functional analyses
of environmental determinants of behavior disorders of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
through functional analysis of precursor behaviors. Analysis, 40(2), 333–338. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 125– jaba.2007.56-­06
136. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­125 Tiger, J. H., & Effertz, H. M. (2020). On the validity of
Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R. data produced by isolated and synthesized contingen-
(1993). Experimental analysis and treatment of multi- cies during the functional analysis of problem behav-
ply controlled self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. https://doi.
Analysis, 26(2), 183–196. https://doi.org/10.1901/ org/10.1002/jaba.792
jaba.1993.26-­183 Traub, M. R., & Vollmer, T. R. (2019). Response latency
St. Peter, C., Vollmer, T., Bourret, J., Borrero, C., Sloman, as a measure of behavior in the assessment of elope-
K., & Rapp, J. (2005). On the role of attention in ment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(2),
naturally occurring matching relations. Journal of 422–438. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.541
Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(4), 429–443. https:// Tsami, L. K., & Lerman, D. C. (2019). Transfer of treat-
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.172-­04 ment effects from combined to isolated conditions
Standish, C. M., Bailey, K. M., Lambert, J. M., Copeland, during functional communication training for multi-
B. A., Banerjee, I., & Lamers, M. E. (2021). Formative ply controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied
applications of ongoing visual inspection for trial-­ Behavior Analysis, 53(2), 649–664. https://doi.
based functional analysis: A proof of concept. Journal org/10.1002/jaba.629
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(4), 1526–1540. Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., & Lerman,
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.866 D.  C. (1993). Extensions of multielement functional
Strohmeier, C., Pace, G. M., & Luiselli, J. K. (2014). Brief analyses using reversal-type designs. Journal of
(test-control) functional analysis and treatment evalu- Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 311–325.
ation of aggressive behavior evoked by divided atten- https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01046388
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(4), Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
331–338. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1394 H.  S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to
Tarbox, J., Wallace, M.  D., Tarbox, R.  S., Landaburu, extended experimental analyses in the evaluation
H. J., & Williams, W. L. (2004). Functional analysis of aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
and treatment of low rate problem behavior in indi- Analysis, 28(4), 561–576. https://doi.org/10.1901/
viduals with developmental disabilities. Behavioral jaba.1995.28-­561
Interventions, 19, 187–204. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Wacker, D.  P., Lee, J.  F., Padilla Dalmau, Y.  C.,
bin.158 Kopelman, T.  G., Lindgren, S.  D., Kuhle, J., Pelzel,
Taylor, J. C., & Carr, E. G. (1992a). Severe problem behav- K. E., Dyson, S., Schieltz, K. M., & Waldron, D. B.
iors related to social interaction: I. attention seeking and (2013). Conducting functional communication train-
social avoidance. Behavior Modification, 16(3), 305– ing via telehealth to reduce the problem behavior of
335. https://doi.org/10.1177/01454455920163002 young children with autism. Journal of Developmental
Taylor, J.  C., & Carr, E.  G. (1992b). Severe problem and Physical Disabilities, 25(1), 35–48. https://doi.
behaviors related to social interaction: II.  A systems org/10.1007/s10882-­012-­9314-­0
analysis. Behavior Modification, 16(3), 336–371. Wallace, M. D., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of session
https://doi.org/10.1177/01454455920163003 duration on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of
Thomason-Sassi, J. L., Iwata, B. A., & Fritz, J. N. (2013). Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(2), 175–183. https://
Therapist and setting influences on functional analy- doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­175
sis outcomes. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Weeden, M., Mahoney, A., & Poling, A. (2010). Self-­
46(1), 79–87. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.28 injurious behavior and functional analysis: Where are
Thomason-Sassi, J.  L., Iwata, B.  A., Neidert, P.  L., & the descriptions of participant protections? Research
Roscoe, E. M. (2011). Response latency as an index of in Developmental Disabilities, 31(2), 299–303. https://
response strength during functional analyses of prob- doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2009.09.016
lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Wiskirchen, R.  R., Deochand, N., & Peterson, S.  M.
44(1), 51–67. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­51 (2017). Functional analysis: A need for clinical
Thompson, R. H., & Borrero, J. C. (2021). Direct obser- decision support tools to weigh risks and benefits.
vation. In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, & H. S. Roane Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice, 17(4),
325–333. https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000088
Practical Functional Assessment
23
Joshua Jessel

23.1 Practical Functional While adherence to a specific set of proce-


Assessment dures is not required when relying on functional
analysis methodologies, developing a standard
Functional analysis methodologies for assessing practice can improve dissemination and adoption
environmental contributors to problem behavior of empirically supported methods among clini-
have existed for decades with case studies docu- cians. One of the earliest demonstrations of the
mented in the research literature as early as the functional analysis has been replicated by applied
1960s (Lovaas et al., 1965; Lovaas & Simmons, researchers so frequently over the years that
1969). The functional analysis remains an influ- many proponents have proposed this format to be
ential step in the process of treating problem accepted as the standard practice (Jessel, Hanley,
behavior because of its intuitive appeal in under- & Ghaemmaghami, 2020). Iwata et  al. (1982/
standing the conditions under which problem Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, et al., 1994) implemented a
behavior occurs before any treatment is devel- functional analysis with multiple test conditions
oped. The systematic manipulation of environ- evaluating general classes of isolated reinforce-
mental events, characteristic of the functional ment (i.e., positive social, negative social, auto-
analysis, informs the design of function-based matic) that were rapidly alternated with a play
treatment procedures that result in (a) improved control to identify the environmental contributors
therapeutic outcomes (Campbell, 2003; Heyvaert to self-injurious behavior (SIB) of individuals
et  al., 2014), (b) reduced reliance on aversives diagnosed with intellectual and developmental
(Pelios et al., 1999; Rooker et al., 2013), and (c) disabilities admitted to an inpatient hospital.
a more humane approach that incorporates the These procedures have since been designated the
individual’s circumstances (Hanley, 2012). The traditional approach to functional analysis.
abundance of research to support the use of the The uniformity of the procedures of the tradi-
functional analysis, along with the personalized tional approach and simplicity of identifying a
approach to clinical assessment, comfortably potential of three generals categories of func-
establishes the general functional analysis pro- tional reinforcement likely influenced wide-
cess within the framework of evidence-based spread adoption among applied researchers.
practice (Smith, 2013). However, recent surveys provide a much differ-
ent picture among clinicians, suggesting that the
J. Jessel (*) functional analysis has been sparsely used in
Department of Psychology, Queens College, Queens, practice due to practical barriers to application
NY, USA (Oliver et  al., 2015; Roscoe et  al., 2015).
e-mail: Joshua.Jessel@qc.cuny.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 443
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_23
444 J. Jessel

Furthermore, the efficiency and precision of empirically validated as a means for identifying
functional analysis have historically been lacking individualized and ecologically relevant contin-
with early reports suggesting upward of 16 h to gencies to be evaluated during subsequent func-
conduct a functional analysis (Iwata, Pace, tional analyses (Jessel et  al., 2016). That is,
Dorsey, et al., 1994) and an initial success rate of closed-ended assessments that force the imple-
less than 50% (Hagopian et  al., 2013; Slaton menter to choose between predetermined func-
et al., 2017), which may have led to an erosion in tions of problem behavior tend to lack
public opinion and confidence in the practical correspondence with the outcomes of the more
utility of the functional analysis. Thus, the long-­ empirically rigorous, functional analysis (Fryling
standing efficacy of the functional analysis is & Baires, 2016). Rather than attempting to
somewhat marred by its inability to meet the inte- replace the functional analysis, indirect and
gration goals of evidence-based practice (i.e., descriptive assessments may find greater benefit
widespread approval and use in clinical as a supplemental tool incorporated into the pro-
services). cess with open-ended queries that refine and
Clinicians have specifically noted the lack of identify unique contingencies to be further evalu-
time or resources necessary to conduct a func- ated. This collective process of using open-ended
tional analysis, which has impeded its use, and assessments to create a functional analysis that
have suggested that the benefits often do not out- incorporates a contingency representative of a
weigh the potential risks of establishing an unsafe child’s individualized experiences has come to be
environment during the assessment period termed the practical functional assessment (PFA).
(Hanley, 2012). Considering that the functional The PFA is a specific functional assessment
analysis procedures were originally designed in process directly influenced by elements of clini-
an inpatient hospital with access to highly trained cal significance. The PFA includes three stages
staff members and health professionals, difficul- for guiding clinicians in the design of individual-
ties were bound to arise for the many clinicians ized functional analyses for each client in need of
attempting to conduct assessments of problem assessment and treatment services for problem
behavior in the home, school, or outpatient clinic behavior. In other words, there is no set of generic
settings. It is likely because of these barriers that contingencies to be targeted. Instead, the PFA
clinicians have abandoned the functional analysis references the experiences of each client to probe
and applied researchers have returned to the labo- a potential history of reinforcement that has con-
ratory to evaluate elements of practical concern tributed to problem behavior. The entire PFA pro-
such as efficiency, cost effectiveness, and safety. cess can be completed within a single clinical
This practitioner-informed research has led to the visit, allowing the clinician to quickly advance to
development of comprehensive programming for the far more important stages of implementing
assessing and treating problem behavior intended effective, function-based treatment. To fully
to be of direct value for those in any environment understand the purpose of the PFA, it may be best
in which the behavior analyst has been con- to first describe the relevance of subjective inter-
strained to indirect or descriptive assessments as pretation when drawing conclusions regarding
the sole means for informing the design of behavioral functions (Jessel et al., in press).
function-­ based treatments due to concerns of
practicality.
This is not to say that indirect and descriptive 23.1.1 The Continuum
assessments have no purpose. Many researchers of Interpretation
have pointed out the shortcomings of closed-­
ended approaches for implicating general classes The functional assessment is a category of tools
of reinforcement (e.g., Thompson & Iwata, 2007; for identifying environmental variables to be
Zarcone et al., 1991), but open-ended approaches manipulated and incorporated in subsequent
that obtain qualitative information have been treatments. It does not, and cannot, identify some
23  Practical Functional Assessment 445

sort of “True” function in the absolute sense as if contingencies are systematically manipulated
this function of problem behavior exists inter- and a functional relation is implicated. At this
nally within the individual waiting to be illumi- point the degree of interpretation is minimal,
nated like an X-ray finding a malignant tumor. nothing more than minor apprehension, and the
Instead, the functional assessment can only build clinician is able to predict with confidence what
subjective confidence in an identifiable contin- will reduce problem behavior.
gency’s representation of that which the client
has likely experienced in the past and is currently
contributing to problem behavior. In other words, 23.1.2 Practical Functional
a history of reinforcement for problem behavior Assessment Process
is a construct, and the clinician’s job is to (a) use
the functional assessment to reduce any believ- The PFA follows the logical reasoning of inter-
able alternative interpretations of potential causes pretation in a process that progressively reduces
and (b) reach some decision-making threshold the number of inferences that must be made
that often leads to the implementation of empiri- regarding problem behavior. The PFA thereby
cally supported treatment procedures to success- intuitively pushes the clinician along the interpre-
fully reduce similar problem behavior. tative continuum reaching an ultimate goal of a
The interpretation exists on a continuum. On sufficient display of functional control in an effi-
one side is open speculation with multiple expla- cient manner. What makes the PFA particularly
nations and little evidence to support treatment unique in comparison to more traditional func-
decisions. This is essentially the point at which tional analysis methodology is this interpretation
the clinician is first informed of the family in of the context as a whole that contributes to the
need of services. Fortunately, any clinician’s problem behavior of each client, rather than a less
training in behavior analysis at least limits the precise portrait of isolated classes of generic
interpretations to monistic speculation (there is reinforcement. The latter establishes the larger
no need to open the continuum further to wild assumption that what environmental events
and unrestricted conjecture) of operant contin- impact the problem behavior of the group is
gencies allowing for initial assumptions that the likely to similarly impact the problem behavior
problem behavior is evoked by antecedents (e.g., of the individual.
establishing operations and discriminative stim- Therefore, the purpose of the PFA is to estab-
uli) and sensitive to reinforcement. However, the lish an empirical understanding of the ecologi-
interpretation remains large, and little can be said cally relevant contingencies of reinforcement
about the specifics of the contingency. historically contributing to problem behavior,
The clinician can move along the continuum, thus validating the experiences of the client and
reducing the degree of interpretation, by asking caregivers, while minimizing practical barriers,
questions about the context in which problem and maintaining accountability as health profes-
behavior is said to occur. Any indirect reports sionals strive to implement current evidence-­
obtained from the caregivers allows the clinician based practices. To accomplish this, the PFA
to begin to make hunches, eliminating some now includes three phases (i.e., open-ended interview,
irrelevant experiences the client is unlikely to brief observation, functional analysis), with each
have had and honing in on those specific encoun- phase providing additional support for the next.
ters described by caregivers. The hunches of
functional relations can be informally arranged in 23.1.2.1 Open-Ended Interview
the presence of the clinician crafting direct obser- The PFA begins with an open-ended interview
vations of correlated occurrences between (see practicalfunctionalassessment.com for an
interview-­informed, environmental events and example) with one or two caregivers, preferably
problem behavior. The clinician only reaches the those of whom have the most experience with the
other end of the continuum when those observed client and have witnessed the problematic c­ ontext
446 J. Jessel

first hand on multiple occasions. This could for further evaluation during subsequent phases
include parents and caretakers if the problem of the PFA, as well as for treatment.
behavior occurs in the home, teachers and teach- The next set of questions during the open-­
er’s assistants if the problem behavior occurs in ended interview center on the extent of the prob-
the school, other clinicians and therapists if the lem that is experienced. This includes collecting
problem behavior occurs in the clinic, or a com- information on the topographies of problem
bination of multiple individuals who are experi- behavior while creating hierarchies of response
encing problem behavior in different settings. classes based on the level of reported concern,
Although the open-ended interview is primar- range of intensities, and temporal occurrence of
ily intended to be used to collect qualitatively different responses. Problem behavior may be
rich information about caregiver experiences, cli- reported to be evoked in one, intense, and emo-
nicians are also suggested to use this time to build tional outburst; however, the extended exposure
rapport because this is likely to be the first to the evocative events is far more likely to have
encounter with the client and their family. To do an additive effect causing the client to begin with
so, the open-ended interview includes questions benign low-intensity responses before escalating
strategically asking caregivers to describe what to more dangerous topographies in tandem with
they are going through (see Table 23.1). By con- the percolation of response requirements without
trast, closed-ended interviews constrain caregiv- access to reinforcement (Magee & Ellis, 2000;
ers to a limited set of predetermined conclusions Warner et  al., 2020). During this portion of the
that may or may not be reflective of their direct interview, the clinician is also attempting to oper-
experiences. The open-ended interview is, there- ationally define different topographies of non-­
fore, an opportunity to convince those who are dangerous and dangerous problem behavior to be
experiencing immense stress that you are there to reinforced during the functional analysis.
empathize and help, rather than to criticize and The final questions of the interview complete
change. That is, the clinician, guided by the open-­ the puzzle of the putative contingency by probing
ended interview, provides a platform for the care- for information regarding immediate environ-
givers to detail their personal circumstances that mental influences within the context in which
have led to them seeking out the help of profes- problem behavior is occurring. For example,
sionals. The open dialogue establishes a level of questions are asked regarding the situations in
trust with the clinician and introduces the process which problem behavior is likely to occur, par-
as a collaboration, with caregivers playing a ticular activities that are likely to evoke problem
direct role in informing the personalized assess- behavior, or specific triggers for problem behav-
ment and treatment procedures. ior. These questions on antecedent events are fol-
The open-ended interview begins with the col- lowed by those on consequences asking
lection of background information such as the caregivers to specify how they react, calm down,
client’s language abilities and preferences. or distract the client after problem behavior
Understanding the client’s baseline language occurs. The entirety of these questions is intended
abilities can be used initially to deduce potential to create a holistic understanding of the contin-
difficulties during social interactions (e.g., prob- gency contributing to problem behavior in its
lem behavior after appropriate requests are entirety. Therefore, the qualitative information
denied) and will eventually aid in the operational obtained during the open-ended interview should
definition of communication responses to target broaden the clinician’s perspective to novel, idio-
during treatment. Identifying any preferences syncratic contingencies that impact each client
will help ensure that the clinician is able to estab- individually rather than restrict classification to a
lish a rich environment that is highly motivating limited array of general classes of reinforcement
to teach new skills that replace problem behavior. that leave ample room for interpretative uncer-
In addition, understanding the context in which tainties (see Table 23.2).
the client is happy, relaxed, and engaged is vital
23  Practical Functional Assessment 447

Table 23.1  Information on the open-ended interview


Section Objectives Questions
Background Create an understanding of the client’s 1. Describe his/her language abilities
language abilities to determine difficulties 2. Does he/she attend private/public/home school/no
with social interactions and targets for school/other?
future communication training 3. Describe his/her play skills and preferred toys or
Identify highly motivating context and leisure activities
establish rich reinforcement 4. What else does he/she prefer?
5. Is your child taking any medication(s) for their
problem behavior? If yes, list all medications
Problem Operationally define all functionally 6. What are the problem behaviors? What do they
behavior related topographies of problem behavior look like?
to be targeted in further assessments 7. What is the single-most concerning problem
Establish hierarchy of non-dangerous and behavior?
dangerous problem behavior 8. What are the top three most concerning problem
behaviors? Are there other behaviors of concern?
9. Describe the range of intensities of the problem
behaviors and the extent to which he/she or others
may be hurt or injured from the problem behavior
10. Do the different types of problem behavior tend
to occur in bursts or clusters and/or does any type of
problem behavior typically precede another type of
problem behavior (e.g., yelling preceding hitting)?
Putative Develop function hunches regarding 11. Under what conditions or situations are the
contingency antecedent events that have and will evoke problem behaviors most likely to occur?
problem behavior 12. Do the problem behaviors reliably occur during
Develop function hunches regarding any particular activities?
consequent events that have and will 13. What seems to trigger the problem behavior?
reinforce problem behavior 14. Does problem behavior occur when you break
routines or interrupt activities? If so, describe
15. Does the problem behavior occur when it
appears that he/she won’t get his/her way? If so,
describe things that the child often attempts to
control
16. How do you and others react or respond to the
problem behavior?
17. What do you and others do to calm him/her
down once he/she engaged in the problem behavior?
18. What do you and others do to distract him/her
from engaging in the problem behavior?
19. What do you think he/she is trying to
communicate with his/her problem behavior, if
anything?
20. Do you think this problem behavior is a form of
self stimulation? If so, what gives you that
impression?
Note. Original questions were developed by Dr. Gregory Hanley and published in Hanley (2012)

23.1.2.2 Brief Observation givers may know when they are likely to see
After the open-ended interview, the clinician problem behavior but the explanatory conclu-
must take the time to collect the qualitative infor- sions regarding why problem behavior occurs are
mation replete with anecdotes and colloquialisms drawn by the clinicians using their training in
and then analyze that which is obtained through principles of behavior analysis. The brief obser-
the lens of the three-term contingency. The care- vation includes the unsystematic arrangement of
448 J. Jessel

Table 23.2  General classes of reinforcement and their sors or behaviors that are indicative of the client
interpretative uncertainties
“becoming mad” and potentially escalating. The
General clinician can then create operational definitions
class Interpretative uncertainties
of those previously unreported topographies of
Attention Is physical attention involved? Is it access
to preferred conversation? Does the client non-dangerous behavior. Second, ecological rel-
direct the conversation? Does the client evance of the contingency can be refined as the
initiate and specify the conversation? clinician attempts to recreate the problematic
Does the attention correspond to a context experienced by the caregivers. Therefore,
specific activity? Does the attention
involve singing a song? What about the brief observation is an opportunity for care-
imaginative stories? givers to (a) suggest additional changes to the
Tangible Is the tangible presented as independent contingency in the event that problem behavior
play? Will the client specify the preference does not reliably occur in the presence of the
to play interactively? Does the client
evocative stimuli and eliminate in the presence of
direct play during access to the activity?
Does the caregiver direct play during the reinforcing stimuli or (b) confirm the rele-
access to the activity? Is this period of vance of the contingency with the inverse occur-
free play intended to be enjoyed with rence of predictable change in problem behavior.
peers?
It is important to note that the brief observa-
Escape Escape to what? Is the client attempting
to escape and gain access to attention tion is an intermediary step intended to bridge the
(return above for more clarification)? Is gap between interpretation after the open-ended
the client attempting to escape and gain interview and before the functional analysis.
access to tangibles (return above for more Some researchers have considered forgoing the
clarification)? Is the client attempting to
escape and gain access to stereotypy, brief observation, instead transitioning immedi-
daydreaming, or sleep? Does the client ately to the functional analysis (e.g., Rajaraman
escape from academic instructions? Does et al., 2021; Ward et al., 2021). Although the brief
the client escape from homework with the observation may be identified as an optional step
caregivers? Does the client escape from
chores? Does the client escape from of the PFA, novice clinicians may want to follow
interactive situations with others? Does the PFA process in its entirety, only removing
the client escape from adult directions steps as their level of confidence improves with
during preferred activities? repeated implementation. Circumventing the
Note. Automatic reinforcement is not included because brief observation may result in a more efficient
the level of specificity is unlikely to be measurable in any
functional analysis arrangement
process for those skilled in forming ecologically
relevant contingencies based on caregiver reports;
however, the brief observation’s removal could
those individualized variables and in some cases also result in a less efficient process if the clini-
has been referred to as a contingency probe cian must repeatedly calibrate the functional
(Coffey et al., 2021). That is because the putative analysis procedures by returning to the open-­
contingency and environmental events included ended interview until the contingency is fully,
in the contingency have yet to be evaluated and and properly, developed.
may need calibrating as the clinician directly
observes how the client responds under those cir- 23.1.2.3 Functional Analysis
cumstances. The contingency can be calibrated in The functional analysis is the final phase of the
two potential ways during the brief observation. PFA process. Through the systematic manipula-
First, the open contingency class can be tion of environmental events in two conditions
expanded to include new topographies of prob- (i.e., test, control), the functional analysis empiri-
lem behavior that are observed. When the evoca- cally validates both the caregiver reports from the
tive events are presented during the brief open-ended interview and informal correlations
observation, the caregivers are able to provide from the direct observation. While the functional
further feedback confirming additional precur- analysis is individualized, and therefore a differ-
23  Practical Functional Assessment 449

Open-Contingency Class Synthesized Contingencies


(Component 5) (Component 3)
3 Single Test Condition

and Dangerous Behavior


(Component 1) Escape from adult-directed chores
to child-directed conversation
2 Noncontingent continuous
Precursor

child-directed conversation

1 Informed Procedures
(Component 2)

Matched Control
0 (Component 4)

1 2 3 4 5
Sessions

Fig. 23.1  Example IISCA highlighting the five core components

problem behavior is implicated during the open-­


ent experience for every client, there are a few ended interview, the clinician has two options.
core procedural components that distinguish this
functional analysis format from others (Jessel,
Hanley, & Ghaemmaghami, 2020). The com- First, distinct IISCAs can be conducted simul-
bined use of these core components have come to taneously, each evaluating the disparate contexts
identify the functional analysis as the interview-­ with their own single test condition. For example,
informed, synthesized contingency analysis Hanley et al. (2014) determined that one partici-
(IISCA; Jessel et  al., 2016). An example of the pant’s problem behavior (i.e., Bob) was likely to
IISCA including hypothetical data highlighting occur both when directed by a teacher to com-
each component can be found in Fig. 23.1. plete math problems using a specific teaching
technique or when others interrupted and began
Core Component 1 (Single Test Condition)  The directing play with his tablet. Therefore, two test
first component of the IISCA is an analysis with conditions were evaluated in their own IISCA
a single test condition. Reducing analytic clutter with a respective control condition. In one IISCA,
has multiple benefits including improving dis- the researcher directed math worksheet comple-
crimination of experienced contingencies (Iwata, tion only allowing independent access to the
Duncan, Zarcone, et  al., 1994) and practicality worksheet following problem behavior in the test
(Iwata & Dozier, 2008) by limiting that which condition and allowed noncontingent indepen-
needs to be evaluated. Thus, the IISCA will dent access to the worksheet throughout the con-
always be conducted with one test condition per trol. In the other IISCA, the researcher interrupted
control. That is not to say that multiple IISCAs play with the tablet and returned to child-directed
cannot be implemented with a single individual play contingent on problem behavior. The contin-
(e.g., Hanley et al., 2014; Ghaemmaghami et al., gency was then removed in the control condition
2016; Santiago et al., 2016). It merely refers to a and the child directed play with the tablet regard-
procedure maintaining an intimate relation less of problem behavior.
between that which is included in the test and The second option is to conduct the IISCA
control conditions of the IISCA. In fact, a single evaluating the more problematic context as
IISCA is only implemented when problem reported by the caregivers and delaying further
behavior is said to be sensitive to a single, synthe- IISCAs until the treatment is complete. This
sized contingency. If functionally dissimilar option is more so a probe of the generality of
treatment effects across functionally distinct
450 J. Jessel

c­ onditions. For example, problem behavior may with the inclusion of indirect and descriptive
be reported to occur when completing classwork assessments, Bowman et al. were able to identify
in the school and when screen time with the tab- the problematic situation encountered when
let is restricted in the home. The clinician may denying requests. The informed test condition
choose to first target the problematic context in included honoring any reasonable requests con-
school because of the potential impact on educa- tingent on problem behavior and the informed
tional goals and then probe the in-home context control condition included honoring requests
later. When using this delayed probe option, the noncontingently regardless of problem behavior.
clinician is hoping the effects spread openly to Immediate differentiation was obtained after
reduce the necessity of implementing multiple using the informed procedures during the func-
treatment conditions. That is, this is targeted care tional analysis and a subsequent function-based
with the eventual failure to identify additional treatment nearly eliminated problem behavior for
functions determining when treatment for all both participants.
problem behavior is complete. Continuing to see It is important to point out that informing the
problem behavior during multiple IISCAs would procedures of the IISCA is intended improve the
be indicative of the need to repeat the treatment ecological precision of that which is tested.
process in each disparate context. Problem behavior may be found to be sensitive to
general classes of reinforcement but that does not
Core Component 2 (Informed infer that they have historically been influenced
Procedures)  The second core component of the by those reinforcers. Especially considering the
IISCA specifies the individualized nature of the programming of dense reinforcement during
procedures. Only that which is informed by the functional analysis, there is a chance that prob-
open-ended interview and observation are lem behavior will be strengthened with the con-
included in the test and control conditions. There tingent presentation (or removal) of any highly
is no need to rely on generic contingencies when preferred (or aversive) events. Thus, informing
rich information can be obtained from caregivers procedures of the functional analysis have been
and incorporated into an individualized evalua- recommended to avoid the potential establish-
tion without impacting the efficiency of the ment of novel contingencies supporting problem
functional analysis. To the contrary, individual- behavior with the use of powerful arbitrary con-
izing procedures can improve the efficiency in sequences (Jessel et  al., 2014; Shirley et  al.,
which functional relations can be identified 1999). The closer we are to representing that
(Bowman et  al., 1997; Hagopian et  al., 2007; which the client has experienced in the past dur-
Hausman et  al., 2009) and the IISCA itself is ing the functional analysis, the more likely the
considered a quick alternative to other, more clinician is going to probe a problem rather than
extended functional analysis methods. create one. The IISCA is committed to evaluating
problem behavior’s sensitivity to contingencies
For example, Bowman et  al. (1997) initially of historical relevance by including informed
conducted a functional analysis evaluating procedures.
generic contingencies of reinforcement for the
destructive behavior of two children admitted to Core Component 3 (Synthesized
an inpatient unit. Despite spending upward of Contingencies)  In another attempt to retain eco-
14.5 h conducting sessions of the functional anal- logical relevance, contingencies of reinforcement
ysis evaluating generic classes of reinforcement, are synthesized as they naturally occur. This
the results were inconclusive and could not be includes a combination of multiple variables that
used to support a function-based treatment. In could be synthesized during an IISCA (Slaton &
response, the researchers collected reports from Hanley, 2018). The clinician may find it neces-
caregiver and observational data from staff mem- sary to synthesize different antecedents, such as
bers. By reducing the degree of interpretation establishing operations that are likely to precede
23  Practical Functional Assessment 451

and evoke problem behavior. For example, any is little to be inferred regarding problem behav-
distractions (e.g., activities, games, cellphones) ior’s sensitivity to preferred conversations if the
are going to be simultaneously removed when the test condition isolates the attention and the con-
child is presented with homework. By doing so, trol condition includes a synthesis of play with
the synthesized establishing operation increases tangible activities in addition to the attention.
the value of not only escaping from the home- There is something to be said about suppressing
work but also regaining access to those preferred problem behavior in the control condition by any
items. The consequences could also be synthe- means and eliminating problem behavior in the
sized in the test condition with the simultaneous control condition by the same means.
contingent removal of homework instructions
and return of the preferred items. The control condition could also improperly
differ from the test condition with the inclusion
In the example above, positive and negative of qualitatively dissimilar consequences. In other
reinforcement is combined in the synthesized words, one form of attention could be incorpo-
contingency, but this need not always be the case. rated in the test condition (e.g., reprimands),
Two forms of positive reinforcement are synthe- while another form is incorporated in the control
sized when presenting interactive play (i.e., tan- (e.g., general praise). Each difference between
gible and attention). Two forms of negative the test and control conditions adds a level of
reinforcement are synthesized when removing uncertainty regarding the influence of the tar-
teacher-directed math completion (i.e., verbal geted environmental variables of interest until the
instructions and work material). Furthermore, clinician reaches an inability in determining if
multiple forms of positive and negative reinforce- they understood behavioral change or simply
ment are synthesized when removing teacher-­ caused it. The matched control includes the same
directed math completion and presenting level of synthesis and same variables as is repre-
interactive play. There are an infinite number of sented in the test condition, the only difference
potential combinations in which contingencies between the two conditions being the contin-
can be synthesized. The importance is not to gency. In the test condition, the synthesized rein-
needlessly combine the contingencies into some forcers are presented contingently following
unnatural amalgam to cause problem behavior problem behavior; whereas, those same synthe-
under the worst of circumstances. The same can sized reinforcers are presented noncontingently
be said about any attempt to decouple contingen- throughout the control condition.
cies in analogue arrangements of isolated rein-
forcement the client is unlikely to experience in Core Component 5 (Open-Contingency
their everyday lives. Instead, the level of synthe- Class)  Safety during a functional analysis is
sis is dependent on an accurate representation of always a concern considering that this is the only
the problematic context as a whole. period in the assessment and treatment process in
which putative establishing operations and rein-
Core Component 4 (Matched Control)  Any forcers will be arranged to purposefully evoke
functional analysis attempts to reduce confounds and potentially strengthen problem behavior,
by eliminating extraneous variables that could be respectively. However, that which is experienced
contributing to the effects obtained. The best con- should be a controlled reaction with minimal
trol condition should therefore act as a sort of risk. As an analogy, a stress test is conducted for
reflection of the test condition including the same those who have symptoms of heart disease to
variables while only eliminating the contingent help the physician determine if there are block-
relation (Thompson & Iwata, 2005). ages in arteries. This test typically involves some
Interpretations of effects become obscured when light physical activity (e.g., walking on a tread-
the level of synthesis or isolation in one condition mill, using a stationary bicycle) and measures for
is not represented in the other. For example, there minor abnormalities in heart rate or blood
452 J. Jessel

p­ ressure. The minor abnormalities are indicative on the part of the clinician. Simply put, there is
of more severe, potentially life-threatening events no need for an assessment that is unable to iden-
such as a heart attack or stroke. Clinicians work- tify a unique contingency to be manipulated in a
ing with individuals who exhibit problem behav- treatment with the expressed purpose of reducing
ior must hold themselves to a similar standard of problem behavior and teaching deficit skills. This
safety and care to ensure that “minor abnormali- pragmatic validation process has been termed
ties” in problem behavior are observed during a treatment utility (Hayes et al., 1987; Kratochwill
functional analysis. The extent of severity does & Shapiro, 2000), and a functional assessment is
not need to be caused in order to predict and determined to have treatment utility if the results
reduce the problem from occurring in the future. consistently inform a successful treatment. The
PFA is of no exception, and its treatment utility
For this purpose, when conducting an IISCA, has been validated on multiple occasions (see
the reinforcers are provided for a range of differ- Coffey, Shawler, Jessel, Nye, et al., 2020) includ-
ent topographies of problem behavior that have ing large-n consecutive case series (Jessel,
been reported by caregivers to be functionally Ingvarsson, Metras, Kirk, & Whipple, 2018;
related and co-occur in the same environment Slaton et  al., 2017); however, it is important to
(Warner et  al., 2020). The open-contingency highlight the generality of the procedures in
class is meant to include non-dangerous problem socially relevant contexts.
behavior (e.g., crying, yelling, swearing) that are
believed to occur before or co-occur with any 23.1.3.1 Case Examples (Home)
escalation to other dangerous and severe topogra- Problem behavior in the home can be particularly
phies (e.g., SIB, aggression, property destruc- worrisome for parents who often lack appropriate
tion). These problem behaviors have often been training and resources to implement safe assess-
identified as precursors and are predictive of a ment and treatment procedures on their own. In
worsening in problem behavior (Borrero & addition, it is easy to fall into the habit of elimi-
Borrero, 2008; Smith & Churchill, 2002). nating any behavioral requirements in the home
Therefore, providing the reinforcers contingent in order to avoid unmanageable problem behav-
on these precursors to problem behavior during ior. Rose and Beaulieu (2019) implemented the
the IISCA serves particularly to reduce risk by PFA with two children diagnosed with ASD who
eliminating any motivation to exhibit more dan- exhibited problem behavior such as inappropriate
gerous topographies. This is opposed to the far vocalizations, aggression, and property destruc-
less efficient process of progressively reinforcing tion. All sessions were conducted in the respec-
and extinguishing each topography of problem tive homes and included a combination of parents
behavior in a closed-contingency class, creating and in-home therapists. Following the open-­
the inevitable escalation to dangerous and unsafe ended interview and observation with the care-
levels (Hanley et al., 2003). Finding the level at givers, Rose and Beaulieu identified
which problem behavior can be turned off while individualized contingencies to be evaluated dur-
maintaining a healthy inference regarding the ing the IISCA.  The mother of one child (i.e.,
functional class of behavior will reduce the Anna) reported difficulty terminating interactive
necessity of relying on the occurrence of danger- play, and the test condition involved the mother
ous problem behavior during the IISCA. informing the child that playtime was over while
taking the toys with her to different location in
which the child did not have access. The other
23.1.3 Treatment Utility child (i.e., Owen) reportedly exhibited problem
behavior when preferred items were visible but
Beyond elements of practical relevance, any unavailable. The in-home therapist, therefore,
functional assessment for problem behavior placed items out of reach and blocked or denied
should only be used if it informs effective action
23  Practical Functional Assessment 453

any attempts to access those items in the test behavior, and immediate differentiation was
condition. observed in comparison to the control condition
Differentiation was obtained during the that included noncontingent access to the tablet.
IISCAs for both children with problem behavior The results of the IISCA were used to inform
only being observed in the test condition. The the teaching of multiple skills, which eventually
treatment was conducted in three stages, teaching led to the child being reintegrated back into the
communication skills, tolerance skills, and coop- classroom with the teachers and teaching assis-
eration skills, before extending the procedures to tants implementing the treatment procedures
different rooms with caregiver implementers. In while maintaining near elimination of problem
addition, the therapists administered a social behavior. Taylor et al. (2018) completed the study
validity questionnaire and maintenance probes to by conducting two surveys with both parents and
assess whether the caregivers found the treatment school staff members, once at the end of the treat-
procedures and outcomes to be acceptable and ment and again after the first school term ended.
whether treatment effects continued to maintain All adults rated the PFA and treatment proce-
reductions in problem behavior across time. The dures and outcomes as highly satisfactory.
positive results were validated by the caregivers
and sustained in the home as long as 6  weeks 23.1.3.3 C  ase Example (Outpatient
after the treatment sessions were discontinued. Clinic)
Apart from hospitalization, outpatient clinics
23.1.3.2 Case Example (School) may be the most well-equipped environment to
Problem behavior that occurs in the school pres- conduct a functional assessment on many occa-
ents its own set of difficulties, not the least of sions because the student-to-teacher ratio is typi-
which can result in the interruption of learning cally low, and all therapists are consistently
opportunities and potential exclusion from social trained and supervised by experienced behavior
contexts. In fact, problem behavior in the school analysts. However, this does not infer that a clinic
can result in the necessity for costly one-on-one has unlimited resources and time to specifically
services separate from the classroom or even devote to problem behavior. Especially consider-
expulsion. ing that a clinic providing early intensive behav-
Taylor et al. (2018) extended the PFA process ioral intervention (EIBI) services will focus on
to a 12-year-old boy diagnosed with autism spec- teaching adaptive and functional skills.
trum disorder (ASD), attention-deficit/hyperac- Ferguson et al. (2020) implemented the PFA
tivity disorder (ADHD), and dyspraxia (i.e., a with a young girl who exhibited problem behav-
neurological disorder that impacts coordination). ior (e.g., verbal threats, aggression, property
Not long after transitioning to middle school, the destruction) that was resistant to earlier assess-
child had to be removed from the classroom due ment and treatment services received prior to the
to his aggression, property destruction, and admittance to the EIBI clinic. The open-ended
elopement. Taylor et  al. began the PFA process interview was conducted with both of the child’s
by conducting the open-ended interview twice: parents together at one time. The parents indi-
once with his teacher and former teaching assis- cated that problem behavior often occurred when
tant together, and again with his mother. All care- their child did not get her way and when they
givers indicated the classroom setting to be the tried to control her activities. Therefore, a context
problematic context, and the clinicians evaluated in the test condition was arranged whereby thera-
the reputed arrangement in the IISCA following a pist restricted access to preferred items while
30-min observation. During the test condition, denying any requests and directing her to com-
the child’s tablet was removed, and he was plete other activities that were available. During
instructed to complete classwork at his table. the control condition, no adult direction was pro-
Access to the tablet away from the table was vided, and any reasonable requests were honored
returned following any instances of problem with continuous access to preferred items. The
454 J. Jessel

IISCA implicated a socially mediated, synthe- 23.1.4.1 Full IISCA


sized function when problem behavior was only The original procedures, designated the full
observed in the test condition. In addition to com- IISCA (although often abbreviated to IISCA), are
munication skills, Ferguson et  al. progressively conducted with the informed test condition being
introduced increasingly complex tasks directed rapidly alternated with the matched control in a
by the therapist. By the terminal goal, problem multielement design (Hanley et al., 2014; Jessel
behavior remained low, and the child was engag- et  al., 2016). Problem behavior during the full
ing in more adult-directed EIBI programming IISCA is measured as a rate on the order of min-
throughout the day than before the PFA process utes with the expectation of optimal responding
began. Finally, the parents completed a standard- during the test condition around two responses
ized assessment for measuring stress, which per min if reinforcement is presented in intervals
revealed a decrease in reported stress found of 30  s. That is, the rate of problem behavior
across all domains (i.e., child characteristics, par-
should be sensitive to the immediate fluctuations
ent characteristics, situational/demographic life in reinforcement, and if problem behavior were
stress). to occur at every establishing operation opportu-
nity when the reinforcer is removed, the clinician
would predict a response every 30 s. Responding
23.1.4 Procedural Variations below optimal rates would be indicative of a lack
of motivation and may suggest the need for
It is important to remember that the PFA is a including higher-quality reinforcers when teach-
guide to IISCA development and that which is ing. On the other hand, above optimal perfor-
considered an IISCA need only maintain the five mance would be more problematic and indicate
core components. That is to say that the IISCA is either that problem behavior is likely to occur in
not a functional analysis with a standardized set bursts or that all reinforcers responsible for prob-
of specific procedures. The clinician is free to lem behavior were not properly synthesized into
make changes to the functional analysis while the contingency (i.e., uncontrolled establishing
maintaining the classification as an IISCA.  In operations continue to influence problem behav-
fact, multiple variations to the IISCA currently ior). In any case, clinicians are attempting to
exist (Metras & Jessel, 2021) and have been evoke problem behavior during the full IISCA at
developed to serve different practical purposes the controlled, optimal level.
(see Fig. 23.2 for a flowchart on guided usage). It The most efficient organization of the test-­
is important to point out that these procedural control sessions is presented in the following
variations are not necessarily unique in-and-of-­ order: (1) control, (2) test, (3) control, (4) test, (5)
themselves, but their application to the IISCA test. In addition to efficiency, it is best to start
are. Similar changes have historically been made with the control condition of noncontingent rich
to improve the practicality of more traditional reinforcement to establish rapport and trust with
functional analysis procedures such as changing the client. Two control sessions are often suffi-
the measure of problem behavior (Sigafoos & cient in cases where problem behavior is entirely
Saggers, 1995; Thomason-Sassi et  al., 2011), eliminated; however, more sessions can be inter-
reducing the session duration (Wallace & Iwata, spersed within the IISCA if problem behavior
1999), or reducing the number of sessions occurs and trend, level, or variability need to be
(Northup et  al., 1991). Therefore, variations of visually analyzed. The final consecutive test ses-
the IISCA share related modifications for improv- sions disrupt the rapid alternation between the
ing adoption among clinicians. Each variation of control sessions and provide additional confi-
the IISCA format including hypothetical data is dence that the putative contingency is influencing
presented in Fig. 23.3. problem behavior when it is introduced. Thus,
the full IISCA need only a minimum of five ses-
sions to implicate a functional relation.
23  Practical Functional Assessment 455

Selecting the IISCA Format

Is the problem behavior a discrete response or a free operant?

Discrete Response Free Operant

Are the topographies


of problem behavior Is time of immediate concern?
dangerous?

Yes No Yes No Full IISCA

Is it necessary to conduct
an analysis in the Single-Session IISCA
natural environment?

Latency-Based IISCA
No Yes

Trial-Based IISCA
Note. Discrete response refers to problem behavior that requires the rearranging of the
environment in order for the response to re-occur.

Fig. 23.2  Flowchart of IISCA formats. Note. Discrete response refers to problem behavior that requires the rearrang-
ing of the environment in order for the response to reoccur

Although the duration of the full IISCA can impact interpretations of functional control. The
vary depending on the number of sessions con- authors found minimal detriments to interpreta-
ducted and the session duration, it is often sug- tions of control and obtained the same positive
gested that the entire IISCA can be completed in outcomes with an additional eight full IISCAs
25–35 min in most applications (Coffey, Shawler, using 3-min sessions. Thus, if time is of concern,
Jessel, Nye, et al., 2020; Jessel et al., 2016; Jessel, the clinician could implement a 15-min full
Ingvarsson, Metras, Kirk, & Whipple, 2018). In IISCA with a high level of certainty that differen-
addition, the full IISCA could be as brief as tiation will be achieved in that time. However, if
15 min due to the tendency to obtain immediate any concerns of the problem behavior occurring
differentiation between the test and control con- at low rates were to arise during the brief obser-
ditions when using 30 s access to reinforcement vation of the PFA, this would be indicative of the
in the test condition (Coffey et al., 2021; Jessel, need to extend session duration.
Metras, et al., 2020b). Jessel et al. (Jessel, Metras,
et al., 2020b) conducted a series of 26 full IISCAs 23.1.4.2 Single-Session IISCA
using 10-min sessions and reanalyzed rates of The single-session IISCA was designed to
problem behavior during the first 5 and 3 min to improve the efficiency of the process further,
determine if session brevity would negatively ­specifically for cases in which (a) the interpreta-
456 J. Jessel

Problem behavior per min


Test
Full IISCA
2 Session(s) #: 5
Session Duration: 3 min
Instances of Problem Behavior: 17
1
Total Time Commitment: 15 min
Control

0
1 2 3 4 5
Sessions
1.0
Problem behavior per sec

RAI
0.8 Single-Session IISCA
0.6 Session(s) #: 1
Session Duration: 5 min
0.4 Instances of Problem Behavior: 9
Total Time Commitment: 5 min
0.2 RPI

0
5 10 15
Intervals
Latency to problem behavior (s)

NR
180 Control Latency-Based IISCA
Session(s) #: 5
120 Session Duration: 3 min (max)
Instances of Problem Behavior: 3
60 Total Time Commitment: 7 min
Test

0
1 2 3 4 5
Sessions
Cumulative problem behavior

20

15 Test Trial-Based IISCA


Segment
Trials #: 20
10 Trial Duration: 4 min (max)
Control Instances of Problem Behavior: 17
5 Segment Total Time Commitment: 40-80 min

0
5 10 15 20
Trials
Note. An exact total time commitment cannot be calculated with the trial-based IISCA because
the duration of the segments is dependent on when problem behavior occurs.

Fig. 23.3  Examples of the IISCA variations with hypo- duration of the segments is dependent on when problem
thetical data. Note. An exact total time commitment can- behavior occurs
not be calculated with the trial-based IISCA because the

tion following the open-ended interview and else being equal. Because a single session is con-
observation is minimal or (b) the problem behav- ducted, the rate of problem behavior cannot be
ior is severe and repeated instances could signifi- measured on the order of minutes, and instead a
cantly impact safety for those involved (Jessel within-session analysis of responding is con-
et  al., 2019; Jessel, Metras, et  al., 2020a). The ducted analyzing problem behavior within sec-
single-­session IISCA is defined by its use of only onds. Thus, the control condition is no longer
a single test session from the full IISCA with all used, and interpretations of differentiated
23  Practical Functional Assessment 457

responding are obtained during intervals where conducted a full IISCA.  Therefore, the single-­
the reinforcer is present and absent from the test session IISCA lends itself to extension when
session. necessary.
The test session of an IISCA includes two dis- The popularity of the single-session IISCA as
tinct intervals the client will experience. The a practical and efficient alternative has engen-
reinforcer present interval (RPI) represents the dered a subset variation of its own that has
30 s access to the reinforcers following problem recently been termed the performance-based
behavior. The RPI is analogous to the control IISCA (Metras & Jessel, 2021). The duration of
condition, and problem behavior is predicted to any interval, RPI or RAI, experienced by the cli-
not occur during this time because the client has ent during the performance-based IISCA is
access to the synthesized reinforcers (i.e., the entirely determined by the client’s targeted
value for responding is abolished). The reinforcer behavior. In addition to problem behavior, other
absent interval (RAI) represents the period of positive measures are included such as indica-
time in which the reinforcers are removed and the tions of the client being happy, relaxed, and
establishing operation is in place. The RAI is dis- engaged. That is, the RPI is not a typical fixed
continued only following instances of problem time of 30  s, and the duration is extended until
behavior. Higher rates of problem behavior remnants of the establishing operation are no lon-
should be observed during the RAIs in compari- ger felt, leading to a return in the state of behav-
son to the RPIs. The number of RAIs and RPIs ioral quietude or calm before problem behavior
experienced by the client is dependent on how was exhibited. The procedures of the
quickly problem behavior occurs during the RAI. performance-­based IISCA are likely to improve
At optimal levels of responding, the client may the acceptability of process and ensure that the
experience as many as 12, 20, or 40 total intervals client experiences a far greater ratio of a pre-
if the single-session IISCA was 3, 5, or 10 min, ferred context over aversive evocative events.
respectively. However, an analysis completed
entirely in 3 min may be more susceptible to det- 23.1.4.3 Trial-Based IISCA
riments in interpretations of functional control, The trial-based IISCA converts the measure of
and if clinicians are committed to conducting the problem behavior from a rate to a percentage of
single-session IISCA, they may want to consider trials. The benefit of the trial-based IISCA is the
using 5- or 10-min sessions (Jessel, Metras, et al., ability to seamlessly insert the trial into the natu-
2020a). Devoting a maximum of 10  min to a ral milieu of the classroom or home setting. If
functional analysis is still relatively brief, and problem behavior is believed to occur during aca-
this preparation eliminates the reliance on multi- demic tasks, the teacher can implement a trial
ple sessions of exposure to evocative events and during scheduled work periods rather than having
problem behavior. to set aside time during the busy schedule to
Due to the similarities between the single-­ implement an IISCA in an analogue setting. The
session IISCA and the full IISCA, clinicians same can be said for a parent who often experi-
could also implement a model of progressively ences problem behavior during instructions to
introducing sessions as needed (cf., Vollmer complete chores (i.e., trials can be implemented
et  al., 1995). For example, a control condition during times in which the child is expected to
can be conducted following the single-session complete the chores). The repetitive nature of
IISCA if within-session differentiation is not ini- presenting and removing reinforcers in a full
tially obtained. If the clinicians were to repeat- IISCA could also exacerbate problem behavior
edly alternate between the control condition of with more advanced individuals who may find
the full IISCA and the single-session IISCA (i.e., such interactions as awkward and off-putting.
test condition), the data can then be aggregated as Therefore, the implementation of the trial-based
a representation of responses per minute across IISCA will hardly be noticeable when conducted
sessions, and the clinician would have essentially
458 J. Jessel

correctly because it should be representative of 23.1.4.4 Latency-Based IISCA


the client’s contextually relevant ecology. The latency-based IISCA maintains the entire
Trial-based methods have been incorporated structure of the full IISCA while only changing
into functional analysis technology in the past the measure of rate of responding to latency
(Sigafoos & Saggers, 1995); however, the exten- (Jessel, Ingvarsson, Metras, Whipple, et  al.,
sion to the IISCA was a more recent develop- 2018; Lambert et al., 2017). Sessions of test and
ment. Curtis et al. (2020) evaluated the trial-based control conditions are rapidly alternated; how-
IISCA with three participants diagnosed with ever, the session is terminated after the first
autism. A total of 20 trials were conducted that instance of problem behavior. Therefore, that
included the sequential implementation of con- which is programmed is not necessarily that
trol and test segments. The maximum duration of which is experienced. For example, Boyle et al.
each segment was programmed at 2 min but each (2020) conducted the latency-based IISCA with a
segment could, in theory, be as brief as 1  s if boy diagnosed with ASD who would often run
problem behavior occurred immediately because away from caregivers to engage in stereotypy
segments were discontinued following an with a door. In the test condition, the child was
instance of problem behavior. The client would positioned away from the door and instructed to
experience the noncontingent access to the syn- stay with the adult for 5 min. The latency to the
thesized reinforcers until problem behavior first instance of elopement was recorded, the ses-
occurred or 2 min elapsed (i.e., control segment) sion terminated, and the response consequated
before the establishing operation was introduced with escape from adult supervision to gain free
and continued until the same criteria were met access to stereotypy with the door. The authors
(i.e., test segment). found that differentiation can be obtained with as
Although an improvement in ecological valid- little as a single response per session when
ity, the efficiency of the trial-based IISCA is neg- latency is used as a measure of response strength.
atively impacted by two elements of the It is important to point out that interpretations
procedures. First, the clinician may have to wait of control based on visual inspection of graphed
hours or days for the natural establishing opera- data when using a latency-based IISCA are a mir-
tion to be arranged if they are to avoid contriving ror image of other IISCA formats. Elevated levels
the events in an analogue setting. Second, the of problem behavior during the test condition are
trial-based IISCA is somewhat limited by the fact often associated with response strength when
that a fixed number of trials must be conducted using measures such as rate or frequency. A brief
when data are represented as the traditional latency, on the other hand, represents a response
aggregate percentage across segments. That is, that is immediately evoked under specific occa-
the data cannot be visually analyzed at any point, sions (Thomason-Sassi et  al., 2011). Therefore,
preventing the ability to make ongoing decisions the expectation during the latency-based IISCA
regarding functional control. The ability to con- is an inverse visual depiction of low levels of
duct an ongoing visual analysis of the data can problem behavior during the test condition (i.e.,
improve the efficiency of the functional analysis quick responding) and high levels or, more pref-
(Saini et  al., 2018) by discontinuing the imple- erably, no problem behavior during the control
mentation of trials when a function is implicated. condition (i.e., slow responding).
With some evidence suggesting that differentia- There are multiple practical benefits to con-
tion can be achieved in as little as five trials ducting the latency-based IISCA. First, measur-
(Dowdy et al., 2021), clinicians may want to con- ing latency reduces each condition to a single
sider representing data of the trial-based IISCA instance of problem behavior. This means that on
as a cumulative record (see bottom panel of many cases a functional relation can be estab-
Fig. 23.2). lished in as little as three instances of problem
behavior. This can be a substantial improvement
in the safety of the functional analysis process
23  Practical Functional Assessment 459

when considering that exposure to repeated 23.1.5 Further Considerations


instances of severe or dangerous topographies
can result in physical harm to the client or clini- The PFA serves as an intuitive tool for practitio-
cian. During the latency-based IISCA, the clini- ners; however, as with any other functional
cian can provide the reinforcer immediately assessments, certain difficulties may arise for
following any signs of escalation (i.e., precur- which the original procedures cannot properly
sors) and reestablish a state of calm before either prepare the clinician. In such instances, the clini-
continuing onto the control condition or repeat- cian would do well to consider what to do during
ing the test condition. circumstances where (a) differentiated outcomes
A second practical benefit of the latency-based are not achieved and (b) when automatic rein-
IISCA is that some instances of problem behav- forcement is implicated as a potential source of
ior are not so easily measured as free operants. reinforcement for problem behavior.
That is because the environment needs to be
arranged in a certain manner for that discrete 23.1.5.1 Undifferentiated Outcomes
response to occur. For those whose problem A function-based treatment can only be devel-
behavior consists of disrobing, once the last arti- oped and implemented after the PFA successfully
cle of clothing is removed, they are unable to provides empirical evidence supporting proce-
emit the response again. Those who elope from dures likely to be effective. Any delay to treat-
caregivers can no longer elope again unless they ment can often be a stressful period for parents
are caught and returned to the same level of and teachers as they wait for the results of the
supervision from which they were attempting to assessment without any direction on how to solve
escape. There are multiple examples of discrete the problems they are experiencing. Fortunately,
topographies of problem behavior that do not fit marked improvements have been made in func-
the more common repeated-instances model of tional analysis technology, and high levels of
the functional analysis. In addition, forcing the control are likely to be afforded by the IISCA
measure of these discrete responses as a rate may with initial success rates as high as 73% (Jessel
be detrimental to ecological validity because et  al., 2016) and 85% (Jessel, Metras, et  al.,
repeated instances may not be representative of 2020b). This still infers, however, that in some
the client’s experiences. In the case of elopement, cases the clinician will need to problem solve
returning the client to the original evocative con- when the IISCA fails to identify a socially medi-
text may involve providing confounding physical ated function of problem behavior.
forms of attention antithetical to the putative con- The PFA includes a combination of proactive
tingency attempting to be evaluated (Neidert and reactive strategies for improving the efficient
et  al., 2013). The latency-based IISCA seems identification of socially mediated functions. The
best suited to evaluate such problem behavior PFA process of conducting an interview and
that consists of discrete responses. observation proactively shapes the contingency
Third, the benefit of terminating a session fol- evaluated during the IIISCA to be representative
lowing a single instance of problem behavior of the natural environment, which increases the
results in a more efficient analysis. In one of the probability of obtaining differentiated outcomes.
first evaluations of the latency-based IISCA, In other words, modifications after the IISCA is
Jessel, Ingvarsson, Metras, and Whipple et  al. conducted are less likely to be necessary because
(2018) found that it took less than 10 min to con- those modifications are already implicated by the
duct. Therefore, the clinician should consider interview and observation to be included in the
conducting the latency-based IISCAs when initial IISCA iteration. In addition, the PFA is a
safety, ecological validity, and efficiency are of transparent process that should incorporate care-
concern. giver involvement throughout every step. Beyond
the interview where caregivers are directly asked
specific open-ended questions, they should be
460 J. Jessel

present during the observation and IISCA. That socially mediated functions, the clinician can first
way, the caregivers can continue to (a) consent to attempt to rule out sensitivity to automatic rein-
the procedures, (b) provide additional informa- forcement. The client is either placed in a room
tion based on their own observations, (c) be alone or with an adult present who will ignore all
probed with follow-up questions when needed, behavior and is only present to protect the client
and (d) validate the ecological relevance of the in the case of severe SIB. If problem behavior
test condition. All of which can be assumed to continues to occur in this condition devoid of
improve the success of the IISCA. social reinforcement, automatic reinforcement is
Reactive strategies involve further modifica- inferred and treatment procedures can be devel-
tion to the IISCA when differentiation is initially oped including sensory extinction (Iwata, Pace,
not achieved. When this happens, the clinician Cowdery, & Miltenberger, 1994) or noncontin-
should return to open-ended questions and obser- gent delivery of items that are believed to com-
vations to modify the procedures depending on pete with the source of automatic reinforcement
the further input from caregivers. For example, (Piazza et al., 2000). In addition, a function-based
the caregiver may suggest that the contingency is treatment restricting access to the automatically
correct, but the client has not been exposed to the reinforcing problem behavior to brief intervals
evocative events long enough (e.g., “He is getting contingent on alternative behavior can even be
mad, I can tell”). In which case, the clinician used in cases such as stereotypy, where the topog-
could either continue to implement the contin- raphy of problem behavior is not harmful (Potter
gencies from the initial IISCA but extend session et al., 2013; Slaton & Hanley, 2016).
duration or ask the caregiver to identify other A second possibility exists for those who
potential precursors that are occurring more fre- exhibit automatically reinforced SIB to further
quently. Both modifications are attempting to evince functionally relevant properties. The pro-
address low-rate problem behavior (Hanley, cess involves distinguishing between three cate-
2012). In other cases, the assumption may be that gories of subtypes of SIB based on the different
important variables were overlooked and the con- patterns observed in the alone and control condi-
tingency needs to be recalibrated. For example, tion during the functional analysis (Hagopian
caregivers may further specify individuals who et al., 2015). The alone condition is representa-
are likely to be present when problem behavior tive of a barren environment essentially limiting
occurs (e.g., “It only happens with dad”) or a the only source of reinforcement to the client’s
highly restricted response to only certain items own behavior, if the SIB is, in actuality, automati-
(e.g., “It needs to be with his tablet”). Thus, mod- cally reinforcing. The control condition is repre-
ifications to the contingency are deemed neces- sentative of a rich environment with multiple
sary, and this cyclical process is repeated until sources of reinforcement that could compete with
differentiation is obtained. automatic reinforcement and suppress the occur-
rence of SIB.  Therefore, once the open-ended
23.1.5.2 Automatic Reinforcement interview from the PFA implicates automatically
There is also a chance that problem behavior is reinforced SIB, the clinician can use the results to
not sensitive to socially mediated reinforcement further identify specific items or events to be
and that the behavior itself produces its own included in the enriched environment of the con-
source of reinforcement. The process of the PFA trol condition (Jessel & Metras, in press).
includes questioning on the potential for auto- Based on the alone (i.e., test) and enriched
matically reinforced problem behavior during the (i.e., control) comparison, the clinician can iden-
open-ended interview. When automatic rein- tify three patterns of SIB.  First, differentiation
forcement is implicated, the clinician can forgo between the alone and enriched condition is
any test-control comparison and instead imple- obtained with elevated levels of SIB observed in
ment a screening of extended alone conditions the alone condition and low levels observed in
(Querim et al., 2013). That is, before evaluating the enriched condition. This differentiated
23  Practical Functional Assessment 461

o­ utcome is delineated as Subtype I. Second, the ing improvements in replacement skills. Although
outcome can be undifferentiated with high levels only a recent development, the PFA has grown to
of SIB observed across the alone and enriched inform research on multiple formats and will,
conditions. This undifferentiated outcome is hopefully, continue to advance functional analy-
delineated as Subtype II. Third, the outcome can sis technology toward embodying evidence-­
again be undifferentiated, but SIB is not observed based practice with client-centered values and
with the participant instead exhibiting high levels parent-preferred procedures.
of self-restraint. This undifferentiated outcome is
delineated as Subtype III.  The distinction
between functional properties of automatically References
reinforced SIB is important to make because it
could influence treatment decisions (i.e., treat- Borrero, C. S. W., & Borrero, J. C. (2008). Descriptive and
experimental analyses of potential precursors to prob-
ment utility) with reinforcement alone often lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
proving effective with Subtype I, the necessity to 41(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-­83
combine supplemental treatment components Bowman, L.  G., Fisher, W.  W., Thompson, R.  H., &
(e.g., punishment, response blocking, restraint) Piazza, C. C. (1997). On the relation of mands and the
function of destructive behavior. Journal of Applied
with reinforcement for Subtype II, and the reli- Behavior Analysis, 30(2), 251–265. https://doi.
ance on restraint with Subtype III (Hagopian org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­251
et al., 2015; Hagopian et al., 2016). Boyle, M. A., Bacon, M. T., Brewer, E. M., Carton, S. M.,
& Gaskill, L. A. (2020). Evaluating a treatment with-
out extinction for elopement maintained by access
to stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
23.2 Conclusions 53(3), 1531–1541. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.682
Campbell, J.  M. (2003). Efficacy of behavioral inter-
Functional analysis technology has been around ventions for reducing problem behavior in per-
sons with autism: A quantitative synthesis of
for decades yet is seldom used among clinicians. single-subject research. Research in Developmental
It seems that those working with individuals who Disabilities, 24(2), 120–138. https://doi.org/10.1016/
exhibit problem behavior in the home and school S0891-­4222(03)00014-­3
have overwhelmingly found experimental proce- Cathleen C., Piazza J. D., Adelinis G. P., Hanley H.-L.,
Goh M. D., Delia (2000). An evaluation of the effects
dures developed in the inpatient hospital to be of matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by auto-
cumbersome and unwieldly. Without widespread matic reinforcement.  Journal of Applied Behavior
adoption, traditional functional analysis methods Analysis 33(1), 13–27. https://doi.org/10.1901/
have left the goals of evidence-based practice jaba.2000.33-13
Coffey, A. L., Shawler, L. A., Jessel, J., Bain, T. A., Nye,
somewhat fractured and incomplete. In other M. L., & Dorsey, M. F. (2021). Generality of the prac-
words, the scientific findings supporting tradi- tical functional assessment and skill-based treatment
tional functional analysis methods were at odds among individuals with autism and mental health
with elements of practical utility, and clinicians disorders. Behavioral Interventions, 36(1), 298–314.
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1755
were largely unable to fully integrate the model Coffey, A. L., Shawler, L. A., Jessel, J., Nye, M. L., Bain,
into practice along with caregiver-informed val- T. A., & Dorsey, M. F. (2020). Interview-informed syn-
ues. This has led to the clinician-informed devel- thesized contingency analysis (IISCA): Novel inter-
opment of functional analysis procedures pretations and future directions. Behavior Analysis
in Practice, 13(1), 217–225. https://doi.org/10.1007/
oriented toward elements of practical relevance. s40617-­019-­00348-­3
The PFA was designed to test for problem behav- Curtis, K. S., Forck, K. L., Boyle, M. A., Fudge, B. M.,
ior’s sensitivity to ecologically relevant contin- Speake, H.  N., & Pauls, B.  P. (2020). Evaluation of
gencies in a safe and efficient manner. a trial-based interview-informed synthesized contin-
gency analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Furthermore, the PFA process, including the 53(2), 635–648. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.618
IISCA, has been found to be socially acceptable Dowdy, A., Tincani, M., & Fisher, A. G. (2021). Interrater
among constituents and often leads to successful reliability and convergent validity of the trial-based
reductions in problem behavior with correspond- functional analysis. Advances in Neurodevelopmental
462 J. Jessel

Disorders, 5(1), 61–70. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Hanley, G.  P., Jin, C.  S., Vanselow, N.  R., & Hanratty,
s41252-­020-­00174-­7 L.  A. (2014). Producing meaningful improvements
Ferguson, J.  L., Leaf, J.  A., Cihon, J.  H., Milne, C.  M., in problem behavior of children with autism via
Leaf, J. B., McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2020). Practical synthesized analyses and treatments. Journal of
functional assessment: A case study replication Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 16–36. https://doi.
and extension with a child diagnosed with autism org/10.1002/jaba.106
spectrum disorder. Education and Treatment of Heyvaert, M., Saenen, L., Campbell, J.  M., Maes, B.,
Children, 43(1), 171–185. https://doi.org/10.1007/ & Onghena, P. (2014). Efficacy of behavioral inter-
s43494-­020-­00015-­1 ventions for reducing problem behavior in persons
Fryling, M.  J., & Baires, N.  A. (2016). The practi- with autism: An updated quantitative synthesis of
cal importance of the distinction between open and single-subject research. Research in Developmental
closed-ended indirect assessments. Behavior Analysis Disabilities, 35(10), 2463–2476. https://doi.
in Practice, 9(2), 146–151. https://doi.org/10.1007/ org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.06.017
s40617-­016-­0115-­2 Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G.  P., & Jessel, J. (2016). Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis
Contingencies promote delay tolerance. Journal of of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(3), 548–575. https:// 27(2), 197–209. (Reprinted from Analysis and
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.333 Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3-20,
Hagopian, L. P., Bruzek, J. L., Bowman, L. G., & Jennett, 1982). https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­197
H.  K. (2007). Assessment and treatment of problem Iwata, B. A., & Dozier, C. L. (2008). Clinical application
behavior occasioned by interruption of free-operant of functional analysis methodology. Behavior Analysis
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), in Practice, 1(1), 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/
89–103. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.63-­05 bf03391714
Hagopian, L.  P., Rooker, G.  W., Jessel, J., & DeLeon, Iwata, B.  A., Duncan, B.  A., Zarcone, J.  R., Lerman,
I. G. (2013). Initial functional analysis outcomes and D.  C., & Shore, B.  A. (1994). A sequential, test-
modifications in pursuit of differentiation: A summary control methodology for conducting functional
of 176 inpatient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior analyses of self-­ injurious behavior. Behavior
Analysis, 46(1), 88–100. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Modification, 18(3), 289–306. https://doi.
jaba.25 org/10.1177/01454455940183003
Hagopian, L. P., Rooker, G. W., & Zarcone, J. R. (2015). Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
Delineating subtypes of self-injurious behavior main- R.  G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An
tained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied analysis of procedural form and function. Journal of
Behavior Analysis, 48(3), 523–543. https://doi. Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(1), 131–144. https://
org/10.1002/jaba.236 doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­131
Hagopian, L. P., Rooker, G. W., Zarcone, J. R., Bonner, Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R.,
A.  C., & Arevalo, A.  R. (2016). Further analysis of Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., Rodgers, T. A., … Willis,
subtypes of automatically reinforced SIB: A replica- K. D. (1994). The functions of self-injurious behavior:
tion and quantitative analysis of published datasets. An experimental-epidemiological analysis. Journal of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(1), 48–66. Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(2), 215–240. https://
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.368 doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­215
Hausman, N., Kahng, S., Farrell, E., & Mongeon, C. Jessel, J., Hanley, G.  P., & Ghaemmaghami, M. (2016).
(2009). Idiosyncratic functions: Severe problem Interview-informed synthesized contingency analy-
behavior maintained by access to ritualistic behaviors. ses: Thirty replications and reanalysis. Journal of
Education and Treatment of Children, 32(1), 77–87. Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(4), 576–595. https://
https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0051 doi.org/10.1002/jaba.316
Hayes, S.  C., Nelson, R.  O., & Jarrett, R.  B. (1987). Jessel, J., Hanley, G.  P., & Ghaemmaghami, M. (2020).
The treatment utility of assessment: A func- On the standardization of the functional analysis.
tional approach to evaluating assessment quality. Behavior Analysis and Practice, 13(1), 205–216.
American Psychologist, 42(11), 963–974. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00366-­1
org/10.1037/0003-­066X.42.11.963 Jessel, J., Hanley, G. P., Ghaemmaghami, M., & Metras,
Hanley, G.  P. (2012). Functional assessment of problem R. (2019). An evaluation of the single-session
behavior: Dispelling myths, overcoming implemen- interview-­informed synthesized contingency analysis.
tation obstacles, and developing new lore. Behavior Behavioral Interventions, 34(1), 62–78. https://doi.
Analysis in Practice, 5(1), 54–72. https://doi. org/10.1002/bin.1650
org/10.1007/BF03391818 Jessel, J., Hausman, N. L., Schmidt, J. D., Darnell, L. C.,
Hanley, G.  P., Iwata, B.  A., & McCord, B.  E. (2003). & Kahng, S.  W. (2014). The development of false-­
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. positive outcomes during functional analyses of prob-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(2), 147– lem behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 29(1), 50–61.
185. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-­147 https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1375
23  Practical Functional Assessment 463

Jessel, J., Ingvarsson, E.  T., Metras, R., Kirk, H., & Neidert, P.  L., Iwata, B.  A., Dempsey, C.  M., &
Whipple, R. (2018). Achieving socially signifi- Thomason-Sassi, J.  L. (2013). Latency of response
cant reductions in problem behavior following the during the functional analysis of elopement. Journal
interview-­informed synthesized contingency analysis: of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 312–316. https://
A summary of 25 outpatient applications. Journal of doi.org/10.1002/jaba.11
Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(1), 130–157. https:// Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand,
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.436 K., Cook, J., & DeRaad, A. (1991). A brief func-
Jessel, J., Ingvarsson, E. T., Metras, R., Whipple, R., Kirk, tional analysis of aggressive and alternative behavior
H., & Solsbery, L. (2018). Treatment of elopement in an outclinic setting. Journal of Applied Behavior
following a latency-based interview-informed, synthe- Analysis, 24(3), 509–522. https://doi.org/10.1901/
sized contingency analysis. Behavioral Interventions, jaba.1991.24-­509
33(3), 271–283. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1525 Oliver, A. C., Pratt, L. A., & Normand, M. P. (2015). A
Jessel, J., & Metras, R. (in press). Experimental functional survey of functional behavior assessment methods
analysis. In J.  L. Matson (Ed.), Applied behavior used by behavior analysts in practice. Journal of
analysis: Integrating research into practice. Springer Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(4), 817–829. https://
Nature. doi.org/10.1002/jaba.256
Jessel, J., Metras, R., Hanley, G.  P., Jessel, C., & Pelios, L., Morren, J., Tesch, D., & Axelrod, S. (1999).
Ingvarsson, E. T. (2020a). Does analysis brevity result The impact of functional analysis methodology on
in loss of control? A consecutive case series of 26 treatment choice for self-injurious and aggressive
single-session interview-informed synthesized con- behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(2),
tingency analyses. Behavioral Interventions, 35(1), 185–195. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­185
145–155. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1695 Potter, J.  N., Hanley, G.  P., Augustine, M., Clay, C.  J.,
Jessel, J., Metras, R., Hanley, G.  P., Jessel, C., & & Phelps, M.  C. (2013). Treating stereotypy in ado-
Ingvarsson, E. T. (2020b). Evaluating the boundaries lescents diagnosed with autism by refining the tactic
of analytic efficiency and control: A consecutive con- of “using stereotypy as reinforcement.” Journal of
trolled case series of 26 functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 46(2), 407–423. https://
Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(1), 25–43. https://doi. doi.org/10.1002/jaba.52.
org/10.1002/jaba.544 Querim, A.  C., Iwata, B.  A., Roscoe, E.  M.,
Jessel, J., Rajaraman, A., & Dowdy, A. (in press). ABA Schlichenmeyer, K.  J., Ortega, J.  V., & Hurl, K.  E.
treatment approaches to problem behavior. In J.  L. (2013). Functional analysis screening for problem
Matson & P. Sturmey (Eds.), Handbook of autism and behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement.
pervasive developmental disorders. Springer Nature. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 47–60.
Kratochwill, T. R., & Shapiro, E. S. (2000). Conceptual https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.26
foundations of behavioral assessment in schools. In Rajaraman, A., Hanley, G. P., Gover, H. C., Staubitz, J. L.,
E. S. Shapiro & T. R. Kratochwill (Eds.), Behavioral Simcoe, K.  M., & Metras, R. (2021). Minimizing
assessment in schools (pp. 3–15). The Guilford Press. escalation by treating dangerous problem behavior
Lambert, J.  M., Staubitz, J.  E., Roane, J.  T., Houchins-­ within an enhanced choice model. Behavior Analysis
Juárez, N. J., Juárez, A. P., Sanders, K. B., & Warren, in Practice. Advanced Online Publication. https://doi.
Z.  E. (2017). Outcome summaries of latency-based org/10.1007/s40617-­020-­00548-­2
functional analyses conducted in hospital inpatient Rooker, G. W., Jessel, J., Kurtz, P. F., & Hagopian, L. P.
units. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(3), (2013). Functional communication training with and
487–494. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.399 without alternative reinforcement and punishment:
Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C. An analysis of 58 applications. Journal of Applied
(1965). Experimental studies in childhood schizophre- Behavior Analysis, 46(4), 708–722. https://doi.
nia: Analysis of self-destructive behavior. Journal of org/10.1002/jaba.76
Experimental Child Psychology, 2(1), 67–84. https:// Roscoe, E. M., Phillips, K. M., Kelly, M. A., Farber, R.,
doi.org/10.1016/0022-­0965(65)90016-­0 & Dube, W. V. (2015). A statewide survey assessing
Lovaas, O. I., & Simmons, J. Q. (1969). Manipulation of practitioners’ use and perceived utility of functional
self-destruction in three retarded children. Journal of assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 2(3), 143–157. https://doi. 48(4), 830–844. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.259
org/10.1901/jaba.1969.2-­143 Rose, J. C., & Beaulieu, L. (2019). Assessing the gener-
Magee, S. K., & Ellis, J. (2000). Extinction effects during ality and durability of interview-informed functional
the assessment of multiple problem behaviors. Journal analyses and treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(3), 313–316. https:// Analysis, 52(1), 271–285. https://doi.org/10.1002/
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­313 jaba.504
Metras, R., & Jessel, J. (2021). Adaptations of the Saini, V., Fisher, W. W., & Retzlaff, B. J. (2018). Predictive
interview-­informed synthesized contingency analysis. validity and efficiency of ongoing visual-inspection
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(3), 877-881. criteria for interpreting functional analyses. Journal
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.849 of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(2), 303–320. https://
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.450
464 J. Jessel

Santiago, J.  L., Hanley, G.  P., Moore, K., & Jin, C.  S. Thomason-Sassi, J.  L., Iwata, B.  A., Neidert, P.  L., &
(2016). The generality of interview-informed func- Roscoe, E. M. (2011). Response latency as an index of
tional analyses: Systematic replications in school response strength during functional analyses of prob-
and home. Journal of Autism and Developmental lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Disorders, 46, 797–811. https://doi.org/10.1007/ 44(1), 51–67. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­51
s10803-­015-­2617-­0 Thompson, R.  H., & Iwata, B.  A. (2005). A review of
Shirley, M. J., Iwata, B. A., & Kahng, S. (1999). False-­ reinforcement control procedures. Journal of Applied
positive maintenance of self-injurious behavior by Behavior Analysis, 38(2), 257–278. https://doi.
access to tangible reinforcers. Journal of Applied org/10.1901/jaba.2005.176-­03
Behavior Analysis, 32(2), 201–204. https://doi. Thompson, R. H., & Iwata, B. A. (2007). A comparison
org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­201 of outcomes from descriptive and functional analyses
Sigafoos, J., & Saggers, E. (1995). A discrete-trial approach of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
to the functional analysis of aggressive behaviour Analysis, 40(2), 333–338. https://doi.org/10.1901/
in two boys with autism. Australia & New Zealand jaba.2007.56-­06
Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 20(4), 287– Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
297. https://doi.org/10.1080/07263869500035621 H.  S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to
Slaton, J.  D., & Hanley, G.  P. (2016). Effects of multi- extended experimental analyses in the evaluation
ple versus chained schedules on stereotypy and item of aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
engagement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Analysis, 28(4), 561–576. https://doi.org/10.1901/
49(4), 927–946. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.345 jaba.1995.28-­561
Slaton, J. D., & Hanley, G. P. (2018). Nature and scope of Wallace, M. D., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of session
synthesis in functional analysis and treatment of prob- duration on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of
lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(2), 175–183. https://
51(4), 943–973. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.498 doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­175
Slaton, J.  D., Hanley, G.  P., & Raftery, K.  J. (2017). Ward, S. N., Hanley, G. P., Warner, C. A., & Gage, E. E.
Interview-informed functional analyses: A compari- (2021). Does teaching an omnibus mand preclude
son of synthesized and isolated components. Journal the development of specifying mands? Journal of
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(2), 252–277. https:// Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(1), 248–269. https://
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.384 doi.org/10.1002/jaba.784
Smith, T. (2013). What is evidence-based behavior analy- Warner, C.  A., Hanley, G.  P., Landa, R.  K., Ruppel,
sis? The Behavior Analyst, 36(1), 7–33. https://doi. K.  W., Rajaraman, A., Ghaemmaghami, M., Slaton,
org/10.1007/BF03392290 J. D., & Gover, H. C. (2020). Toward accurate infer-
Smith, R.  G., & Churchill, R.  M. (2002). Identification ences of response class membership. Journal of
of environmental determinants of behavior disorders Applied Behavior Analysis, 53, 331–354. https://doi.
through functional analysis of precursor behaviors. org/10.1002/jaba.598
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 125– Zarcone, J.  R., Rodgers, T.  A., Iwata, B.  A., Rourke,
136. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­125 D. A., & Dorsey, M. F. (1991). Reliability analysis of
Taylor, S.  A., Phillips, K.  J., & Gertzog, M.  G. (2018). the motivation assessment scale: A failure to replicate.
Use of the synthesized analysis and informed treat- Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12(4), 349–
ment to promote school reintegration. Behavioral 360. https://doi.org/10.1016/0891-­4222(91)90031-­M
Interventions, 33(4), 364–379. https://doi.org/10.1002/
bin.1640
Treating Problem Behaviors
Through Functional 24
Communication Training

Faris R. Kronfli, Courtney Butler,
Christeen Zaki-­Scarpa, and SungWoo Kahng

24.1 T
 reating Problem Behaviors increase and impede one’s opportunities to
Through Functional socialize with peers, caregivers, and others in
Communication Training various settings such as school, community, per-
sonal residence, or place of employment
Functional communication training (FCT) is one (Doehring et  al., 2014). Furthermore, academic
of the most common and effective treatments for growth and skill acquisition may be interrupted
problem behaviors exhibited by individuals with with the onset of frequent problem behaviors
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and related dis- (Fragale et al., 2016).
orders (Gerow, Davis, et  al., 2018). Systematic Carr and Durand (1985) first evaluated the use
reviews have concluded FCT is an evidenced-­ of FCT to decrease disruptive behaviors exhib-
based practice for individuals with ASD and ited by four children diagnosed with develop-
related disorders (Gerow, Hagan-Burke, et  al., mental disabilities. Their antecedent assessment
2018; Gregori et  al., 2020; Kurtz et  al., 2011; showed that for two participants, problem behav-
Walker et al., 2018; Wong et al., 2015). FCT is a iors occurred most often when presented with
differential reinforcement of alternative behavior difficult demands. For another participant, prob-
procedure used to teach an appropriate, function- lem behaviors occurred during periods of low
ally equivalent response, which competes with attention. For the final participant, problem
socially mediated problem behavior. Oftentimes, behaviors occurred most often during both diffi-
the target behavior might include severe problem cult demand and low attention situations. Carr
behavior such as aggression, self-injurious and Durand taught the participants to request
behavior, or property destruction. Without effec- assistance with the demand and/or recruit atten-
tive treatment, problem behaviors are likely to tion. FCT resulted in immediate reductions in
disruptive behaviors and increases in the appro-
priate functional communication response (FCR).
F. R. Kronfli
They concluded that teaching individuals who
Department of Psychology, University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL, USA engage in problem behavior a functionally equiv-
alent response is an effective method of reducing
C. Butler · C. Zaki-Scarpa
Rutgers Center for Adult Autism Services, Rutgers problem behavior. Since Carr and Durand’s semi-
University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA nal study, numerous studies have continued to
S. Kahng (*) explore the utility of FCT as a function-based
Department of Applied Psychology, Rutgers intervention for problem behaviors (Gerow,
University, Piscataway, NJ, USA Davis, et  al., 2018; Neely et  al., 2018; Walker
e-mail: S.Kahng@rutgers.edu
et al., 2018).
© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 465
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_24
466 F. R. Kronfli et al.

24.2 Teaching the Functional “please” and exchanging a communication card


Communicative Response for one child with developmental delays whose
problem behavior was maintained by positive
FCT is a function-based treatment typically used reinforcement in the form of access to preferred
to treat problem behaviors maintained by socially toys and possibly parental attention. During the
mediated positive (e.g., access to preferred items first phase, they used a concurrent operants
or attention) and negative reinforcement (e.g., arrangement to provide their participant with
termination of an aversive event). Therefore, a 30-s access to toys contingent on either aggres-
functional analysis must be conducted to ensure sion or handing his mother the communication
that the FCR is functionally equivalent to the card. During the second phase, the participant
problem behavior. After the behavioral function was provided with 30 s of access to toys contin-
is identified, it is important to identify an appro- gent on signing “please” or handing his mother
priate form of FCR for that individual. These the communication card while aggression was
might include topography-based responses such placed on extinction. Their results showed that
as vocal (e.g., “excuse me,” “break please”) or when the communication card and signing
gestural responses (e.g., pointing or reaching for “please” were concurrently reinforced, the par-
an item). Alternatively, they might be selection-­ ticipant allocated most of his responding to the
based responses such as the use of augmentative sign. Richman et  al. hypothesized that the sign
communication devices (e.g., picture cards, required less response effort and time. That is, to
Proloquo2go). The response chosen should be use the communication card, the child needed to
identified on a case-by-case basis and consider orient and move toward the card, pick up the
cultural and other contextual variables (e.g., the card, orient and move toward his mother, and
family’s preferred language) to ensure the most place the card in her hand. Signing “please” only
appropriate form of FCR is identified for that required the child to orient toward his mother and
individual. Behavior analysts should ensure they bring his hand to his chest to sign.
consult with the individual and stakeholders to Appropriate prompting procedures to teach
identify what would be the most appropriate FCR the FCR may differ depending on the individuals.
prior to conducting FCT. For example, if an indi- For example, Libby et  al. (2008) recommended
vidual exhibits vocal speech, it would be appro- that most-to-least with a delay (MTLD) be used
priate to teach an alternative, vocal response in if the individual’s learning history is unknown,
that individual’s preferred language as opposed most-to-least or MTLD is preferred if errors have
to using a picture-exchange system. historically hindered the individuals learning or
The initial implementation of FCT typically evoked problem behavior, and least-to-most is
requires a dense reinforcement schedule for the preferable for individuals who have previously
FCR, which allows for quicker acquisition. This been successful learning using this technique. In
in turn may lead to quicker reductions in problem summary, Libby et  al. recommended that the
behavior. A continuous reinforcement schedule is prompting technique should be tailored to the
typically used during the training of the FCR, and individual.
over time, the reinforcement schedule should be
thinned to allow for periods in which reinforce-
ment is unavailable while maintaining reductions 24.3 Reinforcement Schedule
of problem behavior. Thinning: Why Is It
It is also important to consider the effort or Important and How to Do It
efficiency of FCRs to ensure the individual is
more likely to emit the appropriate response rela- FCT reinforcement schedule thinning has been
tive to problem behavior (Horner & Day, 1991; demonstrated to be an essential component of FCT
Richman et  al., 2001). Richman et  al. (2001) (Durand & Moskowitz, 2015; Hagopian et  al.,
compared the effectiveness of two FCRs, signing 2011; Kurtz et  al., 2011; Muharib et  al., 2019;
24  Treating Problem Behaviors Through Functional Communication Training 467

Tiger et al., 2008). One of the primary purposes for 2016; Kuhn et  al., 2010; Leon et  al., 2010;
schedule thinning is include reducing the rate of Shamlian et al., 2016) to signal the availability or
the FCR and minimizing the resurgence of prob- unavailability of reinforcement. For example,
lem behavior during extinction periods (Betz et al., Shamlian et  al. (2016) taught three participants
2013). Common scheduling thinning procedures with problem behavior reinforced by access to
is the use of multiple schedules, contingency- and preferred items to discriminate between therapist-­
time-based delays, demand fading, or a combina- arranged discriminative stimuli (i.e., the presence
tion of components. or absence of a colored bracelet worn by the ther-
apist) and easy or difficult naturally occurring
activities that signaled the availability (i.e., non-
24.3.1 Multiple Schedules busy activities) or the unavailability (i.e., busy
activities) of reinforcement. Overall, participants
Multiple schedules are commonly used to pro- acquired discriminated responding more rapidly
mote schedule thinning (Greer et al., 2016; Saini when therapist-arranged stimuli were used rela-
et  al., 2016). It includes two distinct schedule tive to the naturally occurring stimuli. These
components for which a unique stimulus signals results and the general paucity of research evalu-
the availability of reinforcement with a discrimi- ating naturally occurring stimuli suggest a need
native stimulus and the unavailability of rein- to shift our focus to the use of naturally occurring
forcement with an SΔ (stimulus delta). After stimuli.
stimulus control is obtained over the target It is important to note that multiple schedules
behavior(s), gradual increases in the duration of might not be an effective method of FCR rein-
the extinction component or gradual reductions forcement schedule thinning for all individuals.
in the duration of the reinforcement component Pizarro et al. (2020) evaluated various skills such
(Hanley et al., 2001) are introduced to establish a as matching and tacting colors, a listener respond-
schedule more likely to be observed in the natu- ing task, and an intraverbal task to determine if
ralistic environment. Often, therapist-arranged their participants’ responding was correlated with
stimuli are used because they are salient and sub- discriminated responding during the multiple-­
stantially different from one another (e.g., red schedule arrangements. Overall, listener respond-
and green boards; Fisher et al., 1998). For exam- ing and tacting colors were strongly correlated
ple, Campos et al. (2020) used FCT to teach three with the likelihood that multiple schedules
participants who engaged in problem behaviors would be effective. Therefore, researchers and
maintained by positive reinforcement in the form practitioners should consider the individual’s
of access to tangible items an appropriate com- prerequisite skills before utilizing multiple
munication response (i.e., vocal responses). This schedules for reinforcement schedule thinning.
resulted in an increase in the FCR and a decrease Most of the research evaluating schedule thin-
in problem behaviors. They then used a multiple ning using multiple schedules includes a rein-
schedule, which included different colored index forcement and an extinction component.
cards or poster boards correlated with reinforce- However, given that perfect treatment integrity is
ment or extinction. After discriminated manding unlikely when the target behavior is problem
was observed, the extinction component was behavior (Fryling et al., 2012; DiGennaro Reed
gradually increased until a final duration of 240 s et  al., 2011), especially in the naturalistic envi-
was reached. Furthermore, Campos et al. taught ronment, future research should evaluate to what
parents to successfully implement the proce- extent multiple schedules can be implemented
dures, and social validity measures showed that with less-than-ideal levels of integrity. It is also
parents were very accepting and willing to imple- possible we should focus on incorporating mul-
ment the procedures. tiple schedules for behaviors that are more likely
Naturally occurring stimuli (e.g., talking on to result in higher levels of integrity. This is
the phone) have also been used (Balka et  al., ­especially relevant given the influx of research
468 F. R. Kronfli et al.

evaluating the resurgence of problem behavior complexity of the FCR; (c) introduced delays,
(Briggs et al., 2018; Muething et al., 2020). denials, and training a specified response to earn
reinforcement after denials and delays; (d) rein-
forced easy responses during tolerance denial
24.3.2 Contingency- and Time-Based and delay training; (e) reinforced more complex
Delays responses during tolerance denial and delay train-
ing; and (f) programmed for generalization across
Another common schedule thinning method various environments. FCRs generalized beyond
includes contingency- and time-based delays. the training environment and maintained after
Mace et  al. (2010) evaluated three methods of 14 weeks. Results showed that these components
implementing contingency- and time-based were effective in reducing problem behavior
delays to reinforcement following the FCR.  A while increasing appropriate behavior. However,
functional analysis and a response class hierar- it was unclear if time- and contingency-based
chy assessment indicated that problem behaviors delay tolerance were necessary to reduce prob-
were maintained by positive reinforcement lem behavior.
(access to a computer). The three methods of Ghaemmaghami et al. (2016) extended Hanley
denied access were (a) the experimenters said et al. (2014) by conducting a comparative analy-
“no” with an explanation; (b) the experimenters sis of time- and contingency-based delay toler-
said “no” with an explanation but provided an ance training with four individuals. Before
option to participate in another preferred activity; introducing FCT, five training trials were imple-
or (c) the experimenters said “yes” to computer mented. Trials included (a) an instruction, (b)
access contingent upon the learner completing a FCR modeled by experimenters, (c) role-playing,
less preferred activity. Reductions in problem (d) gaining access to reinforcement, and (e)
behavior were still evident even when access to praise and/or corrective feedback of the
the reinforcer was denied following the FCR and FCR.  Once FCT was introduced, participants
when contingencies of task completion were in gained access to reinforcement for approximately
place following the response. Results showed 60  s before each session. All reinforcers were
that target behaviors (including aggression) removed once the session started; however, rein-
occurred at higher levels when the participants forcers remained in the room beyond reach (e.g.,
were told “no” with an explanation compared to experimenters paused the DVD player and turned
the other two conditions. No problem behaviors the screen away from the participant). All prob-
occurred when the participants were provided an lem behaviors were placed on extinction, and
option to participate in another preferred activity. FCR resulted in 30-s access to reinforcement.
Finally, less severe problem behaviors, such as Their results showed that contingency-based
loud vocalizations, occurred during 50% of ses- delays were more effective than time-based
sions when the participants were told they could delays in maintaining low rates of problem
have access after they completed a less preferred behavior without suppressing appropriate com-
activity. These results correspond to the effective- munication supporting previous research (Fisher
ness of delayed access to reinforcement for pre- et al., 2000; Hagopian et al., 1998; Hanley et al.,
venting escalating target behaviors when an 2001).
alternative, preferred activity is provided. Although it might be useful to provide a signal
Hanley et al. (2014) showed that contingency- to indicate the availability or absence of rein-
and time-based delays may be effective in thin- forcement (Vollmer et al., 1999), Hagopian et al.
ning the reinforcement schedule with a (1998) found that this may not always be the
supplementary tolerance response following the case. They found that a brief signal at the begin-
delay for three children diagnosed with ning of the delay interval weakened the FCR and
ASD.  Experimenters (a) taught a replacement resulted in increased levels of problem behavior.
behavior with FCT; (b) gradually increased the Other studies have found that stimulus control
24  Treating Problem Behaviors Through Functional Communication Training 469

may also be possible, even when only one sched- by increasing the schedule by ten responses until
uled is signaled (Fisher et al., 1998; Jarmolowicz reaching the terminal schedule of FR 30. Demand
et al., 2009). fading in addition to differential reinforcement
for appropriate responding was effective in
decreasing aggression and increasing the amount
24.3.3 Concurrent Chains Schedule of work.
More recently, Gerow et al. (2020) compared
Another FCR schedule thinning procedure demand fading to a dense reinforcement schedule
involves chained schedules. These are typically during FCT to determine if differential levels in
used to thin FCT reinforcement schedules for task completion and rate of mands would occur
problem behavior sensitive to negative reinforce- across two participants. Although the rate of FCR
ment in the form of escape from aversive stimuli varied between participants, the rate of task com-
(e.g., demands; Berg et  al., 2007; Falcomata pletion was higher for both participants during
et al., 2013; Wacker et al., 2005). One example of the demand fading condition relative to the dense
a chained schedule is demand fading, which schedule condition. This further supports the util-
involves increasing the number of demands ity of demand fading as a schedule thinning pro-
needed before the FCR may be reinforced. This cedure when using FCT as a treatment
method of reinforcement thinning allows clini- component.
cians to increase the number of demands required
before reinforcing the FCR so that they may
match similar schedules of reinforcement in a 24.3.4 Combining FCT
naturalistic environment. Demand fading is often with Additional Interventions
used if the FCR is emitted at high rates such that
reinforcement is provided at an unreasonable When implementing FCT, it is important to con-
extent, resulting in a reduction in opportunities to sider that the reinforcer for the FCR might not
comply with directives. always be immediately available, if at all.
Davis et al. (2018) used FCT and demand fad- Furthermore, it might not be feasible to deliver
ing to reduce severe problem behavior and the reinforcer if it is available (e.g., if manding
increase compliance with a 7-year-old boy diag- for unhealthy food). When delays or withholding
nosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disor- reinforcement occurs, problem behavior might
der. Contingent on compliance with demands, the occur, and it might not be feasible or safe to
participant was provided with a high-quality implement extinction. Therefore, other strategies
break from work which included 30-s access to might be required as part of the schedule thinning
his iPad. Contingent on aggression, a low-quality process to ensure continued low levels of prob-
break from work was provided, which only lem behavior (Fisher et al., 1993). For example,
included a 10-s break from work. If he did not Wacker et  al. (1990) evaluated various compo-
complete the demand, experimenters continued nents of an FCT treatment and found that each
to provide reminders to complete his work if he part of the treatment was necessary for control
wanted his iPad. Demand fading consisted of over the behavior, and withdrawal of the treat-
5-min sessions in which the experimenter ment was not effective in maintaining reduced
increased the number of letters required before levels of problem behavior. The contingency for
presenting the SD for a break (green card). When problem behavior was not enough to maintain the
the green card was present, a request for a break treatment effects. Hagopian et al. (1998) summa-
resulted in a high-quality break, while requests rized 21 inpatient cases and found that additional
for a break with no green card present resulted in treatment components, including extinction and
low-quality breaks. The reinforcement schedule punishment, were necessary to reduce problem
was first thinned by doubling the number of let- behavior. That is, it might not be sufficient to
ters that the participant needed to write, followed include FCT as the sole treatment component.
470 F. R. Kronfli et al.

These findings were later supported by Rooker Stokes and Baer (1977) introduced general-
et al. (2013) who found that FCT in combination ization as an active technology that allows for the
with other treatment components such as alterna- programming of relevant behavior under differ-
tive reinforcement, extinction, or punishment ent, non-training conditions outside of the initial
was necessary to produce at least a 90% reduc- treatment setting. In this preliminary account,
tion in almost 80% of applications. nine strategies were presented to promote gener-
alization: (a) train and hope; (b) sequential modi-
fication; (c) introduce to natural maintaining
24.4 Generalization contingencies; (d) train sufficient exemplars; (e)
train loosely; (f) use indiscriminable contingen-
Generalization of the trained communication cies; (g) program common stimuli; (h) mediate
response is an indicator of success for the inter- generalization; and (i) train “to generalize.”
vention. Although this is the goal, it is not always More recently, Tiger et  al. (2008) recom-
possible to see generalization effects. Schindler mended three of the nine techniques presented by
and Horner (2005) attempted to decrease prob- Stokes and Baer (1977) to promote the generality
lem behavior exhibited by three children with of an FCR.  The suggested techniques included
ASD.  FCT was an effective intervention in the training and incorporating multiple exemplars,
primary setting where initially implemented, but sequentially introducing training in relevant con-
other intervention strategies were necessary once texts, and creating a similar training environment
the children transitioned to new environments. to that of the naturalistic environment by includ-
The FCR occurred at low levels outside of the ing like stimuli.
primary setting and problem behavior increased.
Parents also provided a low rating for the time,
effort, and materials needed to conduct the 24.4.1 Training Multiple Exemplars
intervention.
Although potentially difficult, generalization Training multiple (or sufficient) exemplars
across various domains is important when teach- increases the likelihood of generalization by
ing an FCR.  Clinicians develop interventions reducing control of extraneous stimuli by intro-
with the intent of the individual performing the ducing training in a previously untrained setting
skill across settings, stimuli, and trainers. and measuring responses across all settings
Continued implementation of the FCR in gener- (Diaz-Salvat et al., 2020; Kirby & Bickel, 1988).
alized settings, with novel trainers, and across Durand and Carr (1991) used FCT to increase
modalities is important to allow treatment effects appropriate communication and decrease prob-
to spread beyond the context in which the skill lem behavior across various trainers for three
was taught. boys who displayed problem behavior main-
In the case of FCT, research primarily focuses tained by negative reinforcement in the form of
on the acquisition of the FCR, and less is reported escape from demands. An appropriate FCR was
on generalization (Berg et al., 2007; Macnaul & taught across novel care providers, and the behav-
Neely, 2018; Wacker et al., 2005). Ghaemmaghami ior was generalized and maintained across new
et  al. (2021) found that the generality of treat- tasks, environments, and untrained teachers.
ment effects was only evaluated in 24% (181 out Wacker et al. (2005) programmed for generaliza-
of 744) of the applications assessed. Although tion by conducting posttreatment stimulus condi-
understudied, the generalization of treatment tion probes using stimuli that were not included
effects may allow for long-term suppression of during the training phase for 12 children who
problem behavior over time (Derby et al., 1997; exhibited problem behavior. They found that
Durand & Carr, 1991, 1992). while generalization occurred across persons and
24  Treating Problem Behaviors Through Functional Communication Training 471

settings, generalization was less likely to occur 2020; Gerow, Hagan-Burke, et  al., 2018). Even
across tasks. less research has evaluated the best method of
Berg et  al. (2007) analyzed treatment effects training parents to implement FCT. Generalization
across various stimulus dimensions and depen- may be more difficult to achieve when parents are
dent variables. Results indicated a strong reduc- not well trained in the implementation of a given
tion in problem behavior following FCT along intervention.
with increases in appropriate communicative
responses. Generalization did not occur across all
stimulus dimensions, but the most consistent 24.4.3 Include Like Stimuli
generalization was seen for increased task com-
pletion for all participants. Few studies have eval- Another method to program generalization to
uated generality in terms of task competition, and novel settings is to make the training environ-
future research should analyze the generalization ment similar to the naturalistic environment
effects of FCT in this context. Although used (Tiger et  al., 2008). Establishing antecedent-­
similarly, training multiple exemplars is distinct based procedures to incorporate familiar people
from the generalization procedure in which FCT and tasks into the training environment is useful
is sequentially introduced in relevant contexts. for generality to other settings (Kemp & Carr,
The primary distinction is that subsequent gener- 1995). Few studies have evaluated the effects of
alization training in various settings. FCT on behavior beyond the training context
(Durand & Carr, 1991, 1992; Durand, 1999).
Durand (1999) taught five children who engaged
24.4.2 Sequentially Introduce in problem behavior to use a voice-output device
Training in Relevant Contexts in school to mand for assistance, tangible items,
or brief periods of attention. The results showed
Generalization across environments must occur FCT was effective in reducing problem behavior
in collaboration with training across personnel in while increasing appropriate communication
the various contexts. An important aspect of gen- using the voice-output device across children.
eralization across people is to create opportuni- These results generalized to various community
ties for multiple interactions for the learner to settings, suggesting that the voice-­output device
encounter. Allowing novel trainers to teach the may have functioned as a discriminative stimulus
FCR will allow for greater generalization of the to promote generalization. Maximizing common
FCR across people and settings. stimuli will help to remove conditional control
The FCR should come under the control of and allow for greater generalization across set-
stimuli in various contexts to establish reinforce- tings (Kirby & Bickel, 1988). Creating modifica-
ment in the naturalistic environment (Kuhn et al., tions to the training environment can be possible
2010; Leon et al., 2010). Teachers and practitio- by adapting stimuli in the training environment to
ners should plan opportunities to allow for the match those of the terminal setting.
generalization of the FCR outside of the training Furthermore, previous research has shown
environment. This technique of generalization that continued application of FCT may pro-
may pose difficulties with time and resources. duce withstanding treatment effects. Wacker
When attempting to generalize treatment et al. (2011) evaluated the long-term effects of
effects across trainers, it is important to evaluate FCT and demonstrated quick reductions in
the effects of FCT implemented by novel person- inappropriate behavior and improvement in
nel with minimal involvement from the trained appropriate behavior. The persistence of these
clinician (Durand & Kishi, 1987; Northup et al., effects suggests that FCT may be a durable
1994). Few studies have evaluated the impact of treatment for long-term suppression of prob-
parent-implemented FCT and the sustained use lem behavior while decreasing the potential for
of training over time for parents (Campos et al., resurgence.
472 F. R. Kronfli et al.

24.5 Resurgence resurgence was observed in almost 41% of the


thinning steps and that the rate of problem behav-
Although a treatment might successfully general- ior was seven times higher with the introduction
ize to other settings and individuals, there will of schedule thinning relative to the average five
inevitably be some level of treatment integrity sessions before schedule thinning. Although they
failure, typically as omission and commission described resurgence as transient, it allows more
errors (St Peter Pipkin et al., 2010; Vollmer et al., opportunities to practice errors of commission
2008). Within FCT, we are primarily concerned (i.e., reinforcing problem behavior), which needs
with omission errors, which occur when a rein- to be considered when training novel individuals
forcer is not delivered following the FCR. This (e.g., parents, teachers) in the implementation of
could result in the resurgence of problem behav- the treatment.
ior, which is defined as an increase in a previ- Research has shown that one approach to miti-
ously extinguished behavior (e.g., problem gate resurgence is to use discriminative stimuli
behavior) when a more recently reinforced alter- signaling the availability and unavailability of
native response (e.g., FCR) is not reinforced reinforcement. Fuhrman et  al. (2016) compared
(e.g., low levels of treatment integrity; Greer & resurgence of problem behavior during an extinc-
Shahan, 2019). tion challenge for two children using a traditional
Greer et  al. (2016) used multiple schedules FCT approach (i.e., no signal) and a multiple
and chained schedules to place the FCR on schedule arrangement. Results showed that the
extinction and found that schedule thinning was multiple schedule arrangement was more effec-
effective at maintaining the effects of tive at mitigating resurgence of problem behavior
FCT. Although these findings support the use of relative to the traditional FCT approach.
FCT, they did not evaluate the possibility of Teaching the FCR in a context with little or no
resurgence during the schedule thinning proce- reinforcement history for problem behavior is
dures. Briggs et al. (2018) examined the data col- another approach that could mitigate resurgence.
lected by Greer et al. and found rates of resurgence Suess et al. (2020) used a telehealth model with
during applications of reinforcement schedule four participants diagnosed with ASD. They were
thinning. Results indicated that resurgence taught an FCR in three contexts with little or no
occurred during 19 of the 25 cases or 76% of the history of reinforcement for problem behavior
applications of schedule thinning. before conducting FCT. Overall, resurgence of
Volkert et al. (2009) used FCT to treat prob- problem behavior was low and occurred quickly,
lem behavior exhibited by five individuals diag- supporting previous findings that implementing
nosed with ASD or developmental disabilities. FCT in contexts that are not correlated with prob-
They exposed the FCR to extinction and lean lem behavior mitigates resurgence (Mace et  al.,
reinforcement schedules to evaluate extinction 2010).
induced resurgence. They found that the resur-
gence of problem behavior occurred for all but
one participant. 24.6 Feasibility of Extinction
Muething et  al. (2020) retrospectively ana-
lyzed problem behavior during thinning of mul- Extinction might not be feasible when imple-
tiple schedules of reinforcement following FCT menting FCT for a variety of reasons, including
for 32 patients enrolled in an intensive day treat- ethical and safety reasons and treatment integrity
ment program. There were 239 thinning steps failures (Vollmer et al., 2020). For example, if a
across participants (M  =  7.5 steps), and experi- student engages in attention-maintained aggres-
menters only compared the rate of problem sion, school staff may be obligated to provide
behavior during the last five sessions before the some level of attention (e.g., redirecting) to
thinning step to the first three sessions after the ensure the safety of other students and staff (e.g.,
schedule was thinned. Their results showed that Newcomb et al., 2019). Another example might
24  Treating Problem Behaviors Through Functional Communication Training 473

include a student who engages in attention-­ Therefore, best practice should always include
maintained inappropriate vocalizations. Although maximizing reinforcement for appropriate behav-
a teacher might be able to implement an interven- ior (e.g., high-quality attention, immediate rein-
tion with 100% integrity, other students in the forcer delivery) and minimizing reinforcement
class may reinforce the behavior in the form of for problem behavior (e.g., little to no attention,
attention. Vollmer et al. (2020) suggested modi- delay reinforcement).
fying the operational and procedural definition of Although dense and rich reinforcement sched-
differential reinforcement of alternative behavior ules will shift responding toward the appropriate
procedures such that it did not include response, these schedules are often not feasible
extinction. as terminal goals and schedule thinning must be
Effecting behavior change without extinction considered. For example, it might not be possible
may be possible by manipulating reinforcement for a parent to provide their child with immedi-
parameters such as rate, magnitude, delay, qual- ate, high-quality praise if the caregiver is on the
ity, and duration. For example, Athens and phone or engaged in another activity. Therefore,
Vollmer (2010) implemented FCT with six indi- it is important to consult with those involved
viduals diagnosed with developmental disabili- (e.g., caregivers, teachers) and consider conduct-
ties who engaged in socially mediated problem ing descriptive analyses to identify what might be
behavior. The experimenters manipulated single feasible to implement without sacrificing the
or combined dimensions of reinforcement such integrity of the treatment. These components
that reinforcement favored appropriate behavior often include some combination of delay to rein-
while reinforcement was still available for prob- forcement, chained schedules (or demand fad-
lem behavior. Overall, appropriate behavior was ing), and multiple schedules. In addition to FCT,
sensitive to each dimension in isolation, but the other treatment components often include extinc-
most consistent behavior change was observed tion (Hagopian et al., 1998). However, as previ-
when multiple dimensions were combined (e.g., ously described, extinction might not be feasible
duration, quality, or delay). Despite these find- or safe to implement depending on the topogra-
ings, clinicians and practitioners might be reluc- phy of the behavior or size of the individual.
tant to evaluate sensitivity to these parameters as Therefore, many of the examples below will
it requires problem behavior to be evoked. include research that evaluated DRA without
Kunnavatana et  al. (2018) extended Athens extinction.
and Vollmer (2010) by using arbitrary responses Delay to reinforcement increases the duration
to identify sensitivities to reinforcement parame- between an individual emitting a response and
ters with three individuals diagnosed with devel- the delivery of the reinforcer. However, care must
opmental disabilities. Based on the results of the be taken in identifying when to thin the schedule
parameter sensitivity assessment, they developed to minimize the reemergence of problem behav-
a treatment evaluation that included DRA with- ior. One such example was described by Vollmer
out extinction, specifically, differential magni- et al. (1999) who used FCT to teach two individ-
tudes and quality of reinforcement for appropriate uals diagnosed with developmental disabilities to
behavior relative to problem behavior. appropriately mand for food. They then evaluated
These shifts in response allocation can be both individual’s sensitivity to magnitude using a
described by the matching law, which provides a concurrent-schedules format, during which
quantitative description of response allocation to mands produced a larger reinforcer (e.g., three
two or more behaviors that are concurrently chips) while problem behavior produced a
available (Baum, 1974; Herrnstein, 1961). Within smaller reinforcer (e.g., one chip). Finally, they
the context of interventions that preclude extinc- compared the presence and absence of a signal, a
tion, either due to safety or integrity concerns, hand gesture for one participant, and a timer for
individuals will allocate responding to the choice the other participant, when a delay was intro-
with a denser or richer reinforcement schedule. duced for mands. Although problem behavior
474 F. R. Kronfli et al.

continued to occur despite mands producing References


larger, delayed reinforcers, the addition of a sig-
nal was sufficient to shift responding toward the Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An investigation
of differential reinforcement of alternative behav-
appropriate response (i.e., mands) even as the ior without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior
delay increased to a terminal goal of 10 min for Analysis, 43(4), 569–589. https://doi.org/10.1901/
one participant. jaba.2010.43-­569
More recently, Boyle et al. (2020) used FCT, Balka, K.  E., Hausman, N.  L., Schaller, E., & Kahng,
S. (2016). Discriminated functional communication
tolerance training, and reinforcement thinning for attention: Evaluating fixed and varied durations
(i.e., delay to reinforcement) with an 8-year-old of reinforcer availability. Behavioral Interventions,
boy diagnosed with ASD to reduce elopement 31(2), 210–218. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1440
maintained by access to stereotypy. After teach- Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the
matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the
ing an appropriate response (“Can I play with Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22(1), 231–242.
that door?”) which produced access to stereotypy https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1974.22-­231
for 1 min, the experimenters introduced tolerance Berg, W.  K., Wacker, D.  P., Harding, J.  W., Ganzer, J.,
training during which engaging in an alternative & Barretto, A. (2007). An evaluation of multiple
dependent variables across distinct classes of ante-
appropriate response (“Okay”) was immediately cedent stimuli pre and post functional communica-
reinforced with access to stereotypy. Finally, tion training. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior
reinforcement schedule thinning was imple- Intervention, 4(1), 305–333. https://doi.org/10.1037/
mented, during which the participant was told to h0100346
Betz, A., M., Fisher, W. W., Roane, H. S., Mintz, J. C.,
wait before engaging in stereotypy. The results & Owen, T. M. (2013). A component analysis of
indicated that although there was an extinction schedule thinning during functional communication
burst during schedule thinning, the FCR was training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1),
quickly acquired and elopement was eliminated, 219–241. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.23
Boyle, M. A., Bacon, M. T., Brewer, E. M., Carton, S. M.,
and treatment effects remained through tolerance & Gaskill, L. A. (2020). Evaluating a treatment with-
training. It is important to note that differential out extinction for elopement maintained by access
access to stereotypy was in place throughout the to stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
experiment (i.e., 3-s access contingent on elope- 53(3), 1531–1541. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.682
Briggs, A.  M., Fisher, W.  W., Greer, B.  D., & Kimball,
ment relative to 1-min access contingent on the R. T. (2018). Prevalence of resurgence of destructive
appropriate response). behavior when thinning reinforcement schedules dur-
ing functional communication training. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(3), 620–633. https://
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.472
24.7 Summary Campos, C., Bloom, S. E., Weyman, J. R., & Garcia,
A. R. (2020). Parent-implemented multiple schedules.
FCT is one of the most common and effective Behavioral Interventions, 35(4), 524–541. https://doi.
treatments for problem behaviors exhibited by org/10.1002/bin.1743
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior
individuals with ASD.  Practitioners and clini- problems through functional communication training.
cians must consider many factors before imple- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(2), 111–126.
menting FCT, including the function of the https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­111
behavior and an appropriate FCR. Once FCT has Davis, T.  N., Weston, R., Hodges, A., Uptegrove, L.,
Williams, K., & Schieltz, K.  M. (2018). Functional
been demonstrated to be effective in reducing communication training and demand fading using
problem behavior, plans must be made to develop concurrent schedules of reinforcement. Journal of
an effective and efficient method to thin the rein- Behavioral Education, 27(3), 343–357. https://doi.
forcement schedule, program for generalization, org/10.1007/s10864-­017-­9289-­0
Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W., DeRaad, A., Ulrich,
and develop strategies to mitigate resurgence. S., Asmus, J., Harding, J., Prouty, A., Laffey, P., &
When each of these factors is considered and Stoner, E.  A. (1997). The long-term effects of func-
accounted for, FCT can be an immensely effec- tional communication training conducted in young
tive intervention that can result in long-lasting children’s home settings. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 30(3), 507–531. https://doi.org/10.1353/
behavioral change. etc.2017.0003
24  Treating Problem Behaviors Through Functional Communication Training 475

Diaz-Salvat, C. C., St Peter, C. C., & Shuler, N. J. (2020). ance of delayed reinforcement during functional com-
Increased number of responses may account for munication training. Behavior Modification, 24(1),
reduced resurgence following serial training. Journal 3–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445500241001
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(3), 1542–1558. Fragale, C., Rojeski, L., O’Reilly, M., & Gevarter, C.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.686 (2016). Evaluation of functional communication train-
DiGennaro Reed, F.  D., Reed, D.  D., Baez, C.  N., & ing as a satiation procedure to reduce challenging
Maguire, H. (2011). A parametric analysis of errors behavior in instructional environments for children
of commission during discrete-trial training. Journal with autism. International Journal of Developmental
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(3), 611–615. https:// Disabilities, 62(3), 139–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­611 20473869.2016.1183957
Doehring, P., Reichow, B., Palka, T., Phillips, C., & Fryling, M. J., Wallace, M. D., & Yassine, J. N. (2012).
Hagopian, L. (2014). Behavioral approaches to man- Impact of treatment integrity on intervention effec-
aging severe problem behaviors in children with tiveness. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(2),
autism spectrum and related developmental disor- 449–453. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-­449
ders. A descriptive analysis. Child and Adolescent Fuhrman, A.  M., Fisher, W.  W., & Greer, B.  D. (2016).
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23, 25–40. A preliminary investigation on improving functional
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2013.08.001 communication training by mitigating resurgence of
Durand, V.  M., & Carr, E.  G. (1991). Functional com- destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
munication training to reduce challenging behavior: Analysis, 49(4), 884–899. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Maintenance and applications in new settings. Journal jaba.338
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(2), 251–264. Gerow, S., Davis, T., Radhakrishnan, S., Gregori, E., &
Durand, V. M. (1999). Functional communication training Rivera, G. (2018). Functional communication train-
using assistive devices: Recruiting natural communi- ing: The strength of evidence across disabilities.
ties of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Exceptional Children, 85(1), 86–103. https://doi.
Analysis, 32(3), 247–267. https://doi.org/10.1901/ org/10.1177/0014402918793399
jaba.1999.32-­247 Gerow, S., Hagan-Burke, S., Rispoli, M., Gregori,
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1992). An analysis of main- E., Mason, R., & Ninci, J. (2018). A system-
tenance following functional communication training. atic review of parent-implemented functional
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(4), 777–794. communication training for children with ASD.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­777 Behavior Modification, 42(3), 335–363. https://doi.
Durand, V.  M., & Kishi, G. (1987). Reducing severe org/10.1177/0145445517740872
behavior problems among persons with dual sensory Gerow, S., Radhakrishnan, S., Davis, T. N., Hodges, A.,
impairments: An evaluation of a technical assis- & Feind, A. (2020). A comparison of demand fading
tance model. Journal of the Association for Persons and a dense schedule of reinforcement during func-
with Severe Handicaps, 12(1), 2–10. https://doi. tional communication training. Behavior Analysis
org/10.1177/154079698701200102 in Practice, 13(1), 90–103. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Durand, V.  M., & Moskowitz, L. (2015). Functional s40617-­019-­00403-­z
communication training: Thirty years of treating Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G.  P., & Jessel, J. (2016).
challenging behavior. Topics in Early Childhood Contingencies promote delay tolerance. Journal of
Special Education, 35(2), 116–126. https://doi. Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(3), 548–575. https://
org/10.1177/0271121415569509 doi.org/10.1002/jaba.333
Falcomata, T. S., Muething, C. S., Gainey, S., Hoffman, Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G.  P., & Jessel, J. (2021).
K., & Fragale, C. (2013). Further evaluations of func- Functional communication training: From efficacy to
tional communication training and chained schedules effectiveness. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
of reinforcement to treat multiple functions of chal- 54(1), 122–143. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.762
lenging behavior. Behavior Modification, 37(6), 723– Greer, B.  D., Fisher, W.  W., Saini, V., Owen, T.  M., &
746. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445513500785 Jones, J. K. (2016). Functional communication train-
Fisher, W. W., Kuhn, D. E., & Thompson, R. H. (1998). ing during reinforcement schedule thinning: An analy-
Establishing discriminative control of responding sis of 25 applications. Journal of Applied Behavior
using functional and alternative reinforcers dur- Analysis, 49(1), 105–121. https://doi.org/10.1002/
ing functional communication training. Journal of jaba.265
Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(4), 543–560. https:// Greer, B.  D., & Shahan, T.  A. (2019). Resurgence as
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-­543 choice: Implications for promoting durable behavior
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Cataldo, M. F., Harrell, R., change. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 3(3),
Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com- 816–846. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.573
munication training with and without extinction and Gregori, E., Wendt, O., Gerow, S., Peltier, C., Genc-Tosun,
punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, D., Lory, C., & Gold, Z. S. (2020). Functional com-
26(1), 23–36. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-­23 munication training for adults with autism spectrum
Fisher, W.  W., Thompson, R.  H., Hagopian, L.  P., disorder: A systematic review and quality appraisal.
Bowman, L. G., & Krug, A. (2000). Facilitating toler-
476 F. R. Kronfli et al.

Journal of Behavioral Education, 29(1), 42–63. supported treatment for problem behavior displayed
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­019-­09339-­4 by individuals with intellectual disabilities. Research
Hagopian, L.  P., Boelter, E.  W., & Jarmolowicz, D.  P. in Developmental Disabilities, 32(6), 2935–2942.
(2011). Reinforcement schedule thinning following https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.009
functional communication training: Review and rec- Leon, Y., Hausman, N.  L., Kahng, S., & Becraft, J.  L.
ommendations. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 4(1), (2010). Further examination of discriminated func-
4–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03391770 tional communication. Journal of Applied Behavior
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto, Analysis, 43(3), 525–530. https://doi.org/10.1901/
J., & LeBlanc, L.  A. (1998). Effectiveness of func- jaba.2010.43-­525
tional communication training with and without Libby, M. E., Weiss, J. S., Bancroft, S., & Ahearn, W. H.
extinction and punishment: A summary of 21 inpatient (2008). A comparison of most-to-least and least-to-­
cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(2), most prompting on the acquisition of solitary play
211–235. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-­211 skills. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 37–43.
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001). https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03391719
Reinforcement schedule thinning following treatment Mace, F. C., McComas, J. J., Mauro, B. C., Progar, P.
with functional communication training. Journal of R., Taylor, B., Ervin, R., & Zangrillo, A. N. (2010).
Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(1), 17–38. https://doi. Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior
org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-­17 increases resistance to extinction: Clinical demon-
Hanley, G.  P., Jin, C.  S., Vanselow, N.  R., & Hanratty, stration, animal modeling, and clinical test of one
L.  A. (2014). Producing meaningful improvements solution. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
in problem behavior of children with autism via Behavior, 93(3), 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1901/
synthesized analyses and treatments. Journal of jeab.2010.93-349.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 16–36. https://doi. Macnaul, H. L., & Neely, L. C. (2018). Systematic review
org/10.1002/jaba.106 of differential reinforcement of alternative behav-
Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of ior without extinction for individuals with autism.
response as a function of frequency of reinforcement, Behavior Modification, 42(3), 398–421. https://doi.
12. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, org/10.1177/0145445517740321
4(3), 267–272. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1961.4-­267 Muething, C., Pavlov, A., Call, N., Ringdahl, J., &
Horner, R.  H., & Day, M.  H. (1991). The effects of Gillespie, S. (2020). Prevalence of resurgence during
response efficiency on functionally equivalent compet- thinning of multiple schedules of reinforcement fol-
ing behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, lowing functional communication training. Journal of
24(4), 719–732. Applied Behavior Analysis. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Jarmolowicz, D.  P., DeLeon, I.  G., & Contrucci Kuhn, jaba.791
S.  A. (2009). Functional communication during sig- Muharib, R., Alrasheed, F., Ninci, J., Walker, V.  L., &
naled reinforcement and/or extinction. Behavioral Voggt, A. P. (2019). Thinning schedules of reinforce-
Interventions, 24(4), 265–273. https://doi.org/10.1002/ ment following functional communication training for
bin.288 children with intellectual and developmental disabili-
Kemp, D. C., & Carr, E. G. (1995). Reduction of severe ties: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Autism and
problem behavior in community employment using Developmental Disorders, 49, 4788–4806. https://doi.
an hypothesis-driven multicomponent interven- org/10.1007/s10803-­019-­04191-­x
tion approach. Research and Practice for Persons Neely, L., Garcia, E., Bankston, B., & Green, A. (2018).
with Severe Disabilities, 20(4), 229–247. https://doi. Generalization and maintenance of functional commu-
org/10.1177/154079699602000401 nication training for individuals with developmental
Kirby, K. C., & Bickel, W. K. (1988). Toward an explicit disabilities: A systematic and quality review. Research
analysis of generalization: A stimulus control interpre- in Developmental Disabilities, 79(February), 116–
tation. The Behavior Analyst, 11(2), 115–129. https:// 129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2018.02.002
doi.org/10.1007/bf03392465 Newcomb, E. T., Wright, J. A., & Camblin, J. G. (2019).
Kuhn, D.  E., Chirighin, A.  E., & Zelenka, K. (2010). Assessment and treatment of aggressive behavior
Discriminated functional communication: A proce- maintained by access to physical attention. Behavior
dural extension of functional communication training. Analysis: Research and Practice, 19(3), 222–231.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(2), 249–264. Northup, J., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Kelly, L., Sasso,
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­249 G., & DeRaad, A. (1994). The treatment of severe
Kunnavatana, S. S., Bloom, S. E., Samaha, A. L., Slocum, behavior problems in school settings using a techni-
T. A., & Clay, C. J. (2018). Manipulating parameters cal assistance model. Journal of Applied Behavior
of reinforcement to reduce problem behavior with- Analysis, 27(I), 33–47.
out extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Pizarro, E.  M., Vollmer, T.  R., & Morris, S.  L. (2020).
51(2), 283–302. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.443 Evaluating skills correlated with discriminated
Kurtz, P.  F., Boelter, E.  W., Jarmolowicz, D.  P., Chin, responding in multiple schedule arrangements.
M.  D., & Hagopian, L.  P. (2011). An analysis of Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. https://doi.
functional communication training as an empirically org/10.1002/jaba.759
24  Treating Problem Behaviors Through Functional Communication Training 477

Richman, D.  M., Wacker, D.  P., & Winborn, L. (2001). children with severe behavior disorders. Journal of
Response efficiency during functional communica- Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(4), 451–466. https://
tion training: Effects of effort on response allocation. doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­451
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(1), 73–76. Vollmer, T. R., Peters, K. P., Kronfli, F. R., Lloveras, L.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-­73 A., & Ibañez, V. F. (2020). On the definition of differ-
Rooker, G. W., Jessel, J., Kurtz, P. F., & Hagopian, L. P. ential reinforcement of alternative behavior. Journal of
(2013). Functional communication training with and Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(3), 1299–1303. https://
without alternative reinforcement and punishment: doi.org/10.1002/jaba.701
An analysis of 58 applications. Journal of Applied Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & St Peter Pipkin, C. (2008).
Behavior Analysis, 46(4), 708–722. https://doi. Practical implications of data reliability and treatment
org/10.1002/jaba.76 integrity monitoring. Behavior Analysis in Practice,
Saini, V., Miller, S. A., & Fisher, W. W. (2016). Multiple 1(2), 4–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391722
schedules in practical application: Research trends Volkert, V. M., Lerman, D. C., Call, N. A., & Trosclair-­
and implications for future investigation. Journal of Lasserre, N. (2009). An evaluation of resurgence dur-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(2), 421–444. https:// ing treatment with functional communication training.
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.300 Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(1), 145–160.
Schindler, H.  R., & Horner, R.  H. (2005). https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­145
Generalized reduction of problem behavior of Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Harding, J. W., Barretto, A.,
young children with autism: Building trans- Rankin, B., & Ganzer, J. (2005). Treatment effec-
situational interventions. American Journal of tiveness, stimulus generalization, and acceptability
Mental Retardation, 110(1), 36–47. https://doi. to parents of functional communication training.
org/10.1352/0895-­8017(2005)110<36:GROPBO>2.0 Educational Psychology, 25(2–3), 233–256. https://
.CO;2 doi.org/10.1080/0144341042000301184
Shamlian, K.  D., Fisher, W.  W., Steege, M.  W., Wacker, D.  P., Harding, J.  W., Berg, W.  K., Lee, J.  F.,
Cavanaugh, B.  M., Samour, K., & Querim, A.  C. Schieltz, K. M., Padilla, Y. C., Nevin, J. A., & Shahan,
(2016). Evaluation of multiple schedules with natu- T.  A. (2011). An evaluation of persistence of teat-
rally occurring and therapist-arranged discriminative ment effects during long-term treatment of destruc-
stimuli following functional communication training. tive behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(2), 228–250. of Behavior, 96(2), 261–282. https://doi.org/10.1901/
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.293 jeab.2011.96-­261
St Peter Pipkin, C., Vollmer, T. R., & Sloman, K. N. (2010). Wacker, D. P., Steege, M. W., Northup, J., Sasso, G., Berg,
Effects of treatment integrity failures during differen- W., Reimers, T., Cooper, L., Cigrand, K., & Donn, L.
tial reinforcement of alternative behavior: A transla- (1990). A component analysis of functional commu-
tional model. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, nication training across three topographies of severe
43(1), 47–70. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­47 behavior problems. Journal of Applied Behavior
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol- Analysis, 23(4), 417–429. https://doi.org/10.1901/
ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior jaba.1990.23-­417
Analysis, 10(2), 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Walker, V.  L., Lyon, K.  J., Loman, S.  L., & Sennott, S.
jaba.1977.10-349 (2018). A systematic review of functional communica-
Suess, A. N., Schieltz, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Detrick, J., tion training (FCT) interventions involving augmenta-
& Podlesnik, C.  A. (2020). An evaluation of resur- tive and alternative communication in school settings.
gence following functional communication train- Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 34(2),
ing conducted in alternative antecedent contexts via 118–129. https://doi.org/10.1080/07434618.2018.14
telehealth. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of 61240
Behavior, 113(1), 278–301. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig,
jeab.551 A., Kucharczyk, S., Brock, M.  E., Plavnick, J.  B.,
Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional Fleury, V.  P., & Schultz, T.  R. (2015). Evidence-­
communication training: A review and practical guide. based practices for children, youth, and young
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 16–23. https:// adults with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehen-
doi.org/10.1007/BF03391716 sive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Vollmer, T.  R., Borrero, J.  C., Lalli, J.  S., & Daniel, D. Disorders, 45, 1951–1966. https://doi.org/10.1007/
(1999). Evaluating self-control and impulsivity in s10803-­014-­2351-­z
Response Cost and Time-Out
from Reinforcement 25
Ashley Bagwell, Monique Barnett,
and Terry S. Falcomata

25.1 R
 esponse Cost and Time-Out 25.1.1 Principles and Concepts
from Reinforcement
Response cost entails the removal of earned rein-
Response cost and time-out from reinforcement forcing stimuli (e.g., tokens, time with preferred
(referred to as time-out for the remainder of the activities) contingent on a target behavior (Harris,
chapter) are behavior change tactics that have 1985; Kazdin, 1972; Weiner, 1962). Similarly,
been applied widely as part of intervention efforts time-out entails the removal of an individual’s
to reduce problem behaviors (e.g., aggression, access to positive reinforcement along with the
self-injurious behavior [SIB], property destruc- opportunity to obtain reinforcement (Harris,
tion, noncompliance, disruptive behavior, tan- 1985; Leitenberg, 1965). Nonexclusionary time-­
trums, pica) demonstrated by individuals with out consists of the removal of reinforcing stimuli
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and other devel- from the individual while the individual remains
opmental disorders (e.g., Conyers et  al., 2004; in their current environment. Exclusionary time-­
Nolan & Filter, 2012; Watkins & Rapp, 2014). out consists of the removal of the individual from
Both response cost and time-out procedures have the environment in which the reinforcing stimuli
been demonstrated to be effective in isolation as are present (Harris, 1985; Wolf et al., 2006).
well as in combination with other behavior Any discussion of response cost and time-out
change tactics (e.g., noncontingent reinforcement procedures requires a clear definition of, and con-
[NCR], differential reinforcement of other behav- nection to, the principles from which the two pro-
iors [DRO], differential reinforcement of alterna- cedures are derived. The process that is
tive behaviors [DRA], stimulus control responsible for the effects of both response cost
procedures). In this chapter, we describe the prin- and time-out procedures is operant conditioning;
ciples from which response cost and time-out are and, more specifically, the principle on which
derived, relevant basic findings that predated both procedures are based is negative punish-
applications of the procedures, examples of the ment. Operant conditioning (Skinner, 1953) is the
clinical application of the procedures, and con- process by which behaviors are affected by the
siderations for their use including advantages, consequences they produce such that the behav-
disadvantages, and ethical considerations. iors are either strengthened (i.e., reinforced) or
suppressed (i.e., punished). When a consequence
A. Bagwell · M. Barnett · T. S. Falcomata (*) results in an increased likelihood that a response
Department of Special Education, The University of will happen in the future or an increase in
Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA response rate, reinforcement has occurred. The
e-mail: falcomata@austin.utexas.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 479
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_25
480 A. Bagwell et al.

two types of consequences that strengthen behav- [ABAI]; i.e., Van Houten et  al., 1988; Vollmer
iors are positive reinforcement (i.e., a stimulus is et al., 2011) and (b) the ethical codes of the field’s
presented contingent on a behavior that strength- primary professional certification organization
ens the behavior) and negative reinforcement (Behavior Analysis Certification Board [BACB];
(i.e., a stimulus is removed contingent on a BACB, 2014), subscribes to several positions
behavior that strengthens the behavior). When a pertinent to the use of punishment-based proce-
consequence results in a decreased likelihood dures. First, individuals have the right to effective
that a behavior will happen in the future or a behavioral treatments (Van Houten et  al., 1988;
decrease in response rate, punishment has Vollmer et al., 2011). This principle dictates that
occurred. The two types of consequences that even though a punishment-based procedure may
suppress behaviors are positive punishment (i.e., not be the most desirable approach given its
a stimulus is presented contingent on a behavior potential side effects (Baer, 1971; Iwata, 1988),
that suppresses the behavior) and negative pun- when weighed against the continued occurrence
ishment (i.e., a stimulus is removed contingent on of problem behavior (because of the lack of
a behavior that suppresses the behavior). effects of alternative approaches including ante-
cedent and/or reinforcement-based procedures)
25.1.1.1 Punishment and its own negative side effects, it may be the
Punishment is a naturally occurring phenomenon most appropriate approach to treatment. Second,
that impacts the behavior of all organisms, treatments should be utilized based on the prin-
including humans, on a relatively ongoing basis. ciple of least restrictiveness (Vollmer et  al.,
Examples of the natural occurrence of punish- 2011). This principle does not presuppose that
ment include instances in which behaviors come alternatives to punishment-based procedures will
into contact with painful consequences such as always entail the most “favorable risk-to-benefit
touching hot stoves or drinking hot liquids. ratio” (Vollmer et al., 2011, p. 104) which should
Further, the principle of punishment, both posi- be calculated based on multiple factors including
tive and negative, is applied throughout society the direct effects of behavior (e.g., tissue damage
even though those applications may not be for- to self and others, competition with learning,
mally conceptualized as such by the implement- social isolation), duration of intervention (e.g.,
ers. Examples of common general applications of DRA plus punishment-based procedure may sig-
punishment in society include fines for moving nificantly reduce the length of treatment vs. DRA
violations or loss of driving privileges, demotion alone), and likely success of intervention (e.g.,
for on-the-job performance, and suspension of DRA or NCR alone are not effective in the
posting privileges on social media platforms. absence of punishment-based treatment compo-
Two types of punishment-producing conse- nents). Third, the welfare of the individual is pri-
quences are primary and secondary. Primary pun- oritized, including ensuring their safety (Vollmer
ishers are associated with unlearned responses et al., 2011). Thus, should the clinical team deem
and include consequences such as pain. it to be in the individual’s best interest given the
Secondary punishers are associated with learned characteristics of the clinical case and best prac-
behaviors that rely on previous pairings with pri- tices based on the research literature, punishment-­
mary punishers (Sigafoos et al., 2003). based procedures can/should be utilized (i.e., the
interests and welfare of the individual “must take
25.1.1.2 Punishment-Based precedence over the broader agendas of institu-
Procedures in the Treatment tions or organizations that would prohibit certain
of Problem Behavior procedures regardless of individual’s needs;”
The field of applied behavior analysis (ABA), as Vollmer et al., 2011, p. 104). Last, it should also
reflected by (a) position statements provided by be noted that it is standard and recommended
the field’s primary professional organization practice that when punishment-based procedures
(Association for Behavior Analysis International are utilized, they (a) be used after alternative
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 481

approaches (e.g., antecedent-based procedures, individual with ASD.  Specifically, Falcomata


reinforcement-based procedures, combination of et al. initially implemented NCR in the form of
antecedent and reinforcement-based procedures) enriched environment (EE) in an attempt to treat
are either demonstrated to be inadequately automatically maintained severe pica (Fig.  25.1
­effective (see Baer, 1971 and Iwata, 1988 for fur- shows an X-ray of a safety pin lodged in the par-
ther discussion) or unavailable given the charac- ticipant’s throat prior to treatment) exhibited by a
teristics and/or constraints of the clinical case 12-year-old boy with ASD. After NCR/EE was
and (b) be combined with reinforcement-based demonstrated to be ineffective, Falcomata et  al.
procedures to the extent possible (Vollmer et al., incorporated a time-out procedure along with
2011). NCR/EE and a stimulus control procedure. The
Falcomata et al. (2007) provided an example treatment package was effective at reducing the
in which the above-described principles were pica and clearly met the criteria for justifying the
combined in a case in which non-punishment use of punishment (e.g., effective, evidence-­
(i.e., reinforcement) procedures were not effec- based, favorable in terms of the risk-to-benefit
tive at reducing problem behavior exhibited by an ratio, the welfare of the client was clearly priori-

Fig. 25.1  X-ray showing physical evidence of the severity of participant’s pica in Falcomata et al., (2007; Reprinted
with permission from Roane et al. (2005)). The X-ray also appeared in Falcomata et al. (2007).
482 A. Bagwell et al.

tized given these factors). Bagwell et al. (in prep- animal subjects terminated responding in the
aration) provided an example of a situation in presence of stimuli that were paired with a time-­
which typically recommended evidence-based out procedure that was contingent on responding.
procedures (i.e., blocking, high preferred item-­ Ferster (1958, Experiment IV) and other research-
based NCR) were not available. In this case, ers (e.g., Baer, 1960; Baron & Kaufman, 1966)
automatically maintained bruxism was exhibited also showed, in early basic and translational stud-
by a 14-year-old girl with ASD, and blocking the ies, that animals and humans would engage in
behavior in combination with NCR was not pos- responding that resulted in the avoidance of time-­
sible. Further, only after NCR was demonstrated out periods.
to be ineffective, Bagwell et  al. implemented
response cost which was effective at decreasing
bruxism, albeit with significant side effects which 25.2 Response Cost
were subsequently mitigated (i.e., engagement in
high preferred activities was also suppressed; see 25.2.1 Response Cost in Practice
below for further discussion of the study).
It is recommended that response cost, like other
punishment strategies, be used in combination
25.1.2 Early Basic Research with other procedures (e.g., DRO, DRA, NCR).
on Response Cost However, a few studies exist in which response
and Time-Out cost was the sole intervention strategy. For exam-
ple, Woods (1982) implemented response cost to
Numerous early basic and translational studies decrease the occurrence of severe SIB exhibited
were conducted that investigated negative pun- by an adolescent with ASD in the home setting.
ishment in general, and response cost and time-­ Tickets were provided at the start of each day,
out-­based independent variables specifically. For and engagement in any of the target problem
example, the effects of response cost received a behaviors (i.e., SIB or aggression) resulted in los-
great deal of early attention in the basic literature, ing one ticket per occurrence. As the intervention
specifically, within human operant-based experi- progressed, the number of tickets allotted per day
mental preparations (Kazdin, 1972; e.g., Weiner, decreased such that fewer tickets were available
1962). For example, Weiner (1962) studied the to meet the criterion. Engagement in the target
effects of response cost in the form of loss of problem behaviors decreased to near zero levels,
points with typically developing human partici- and a final criterion of providing only three tick-
pants. Specifically, the participants’ responding ets per day was met. Woods demonstrated that
was reinforced with points via variable interval response cost can be used alone to reduce severe
(VI; Experiment 1) and fixed interval (FI; problem behaviors such as headbanging, biting,
Experiments 2 and 3) schedules of reinforcement and aggression. Furthermore, the participants’
and subsequently lost points for responding dur- family administered consequences in the home
ing a cost condition (Weiner, 1962). The results, setting demonstrating that response cost can be
across experiments, showed suppressing effects, implemented in natural environments. Bartlett
including the elimination of scalloped respond- et al. (2011) demonstrated a decrease in spitting
ing with FI schedules, of the response cost proce- for one child with ASD after reinforcement-based
dures. Likewise, time-out also received a great approaches proved ineffective. A high-preferred
deal of attention in early studies within basic item (i.e., a radio) was removed contingent on
research arrangements (Leitenberg, 1965; e.g., spitting behavior for 10 s. After the 10 s elapsed,
Ferster, 1958; Ferster, 1960). For example, the radio was given back to the child. Bartlett
Ferster and colleagues showed, in a series of et al. observed a decrease in spitting behaviors to
experiments with chimpanzees and pigeons (i.e., near zero levels with findings that generalized
Ferster, 1958, Experiment III; Ferster, 1960), that
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 483

and maintained up to 4  months following the two interventions. McGoey and DuPaul also
intervention. found that teachers considered response cost to
Response cost procedures have been found to be more acceptable than token reinforcement and
be similar or more effective in comparison to continued using it after the study ended. Holland
other interventions. For example, Conyers et al. and McLaughlin (1982) evaluated the effects of
(2004) compared the effects of response cost to response cost, public posting, and group contin-
DRO on disruptive behaviors in a preschool gencies on a variety of problem behaviors with
classroom. Twenty-five students, all of whom 254 primary and secondary grade students across
exhibited disruptive behaviors such as scream- a school. Specifically, Holland and McLaughlin
ing, crying, and throwing, participated in the applied response cost procedures to a several
study. During the response cost condition, 15 behaviors (e.g., gum chewing, entering school
tokens were provided to each of the students via without permission, fighting, running in hall-
stars next to their names on a board. Contingent ways) by recording student’s name as well as
on disruptive behaviors, individual students lost their teacher’s name. Each occurrence resulted in
one token. At the end of the 15-min period, the a 1-point loss for the entire class in which the stu-
students exchanged tokens for access to preferred dent resided; each class started each day with 10
edibles. Engagement in disruptive behaviors points. At the end of the day, point totals were
decreased in both conditions, with a larger summed and publicly displayed outside the class-
decrease observed in the response cost condition. room. The results showed that the response cost
Low rates of disruptive behavior continued to be procedures in combination with public posting
observed when response cost intervals were and group contingencies were effective at
increased (i.e., from 1 to 12  min). Further, decreasing problem behaviors across the school.
Capriotti et al. (2012) compared the effects of a Further, students and teachers reported that the
response cost analogue to DRO on tics exhibited intervention was useful and indicated that they
by four children with Tourette’s syndrome. were able to dedicate more time to learning.
Decreases in rates of tics were observed in both Treatment packages that include response cost
conditions with no differences observed between have been demonstrated to be effective in
the effects of response cost and DRO. decreasing a variety of problem behaviors such
Response cost can also be applied as a group as vocal stereotypy in the form of screaming
contingency. For example, McGoey and DuPaul (e.g., Shillingsburg et  al., 2012), SIB (e.g.,
(2000) compared the effects of token reinforce- Capriotti et al., 2012), aggression (e.g., Nolan &
ment and response cost in a classroom with chil- Filter, 2012), and feeding issues (e.g., Buckley &
dren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder Newchok, 2005). Shillingsburg et al. (2012) eval-
(ADHD) diagnoses. Reinforcement and response uated the effects of response cost on automati-
cost contingencies were implemented, respec- cally maintained screaming behavior exhibited
tively, for off-task behavior, disobeying rules, by a child with ASD.  Initially, the authors pro-
and tantrum behavior. During the token rein- vided the individual with access to a preferred
forcement intervention, the participants earned activity (i.e., access to a computer). Contingent
buttons that were placed on a chart contingent on on the vocal stereotypy, the authors removed
appropriate behavior (i.e., following classroom access to the computer for 20 s. Following a dem-
rules). At the end of class, the participants were onstration of the positive effects of response cost,
provided with reinforcers in exchange for earned Schillingsburg et al. incorporated demand fading
buttons. During the response cost intervention, to imbed instructional tasks during the procedure.
the token reinforcement system remained in After an increase in problem behavior during
place, but contingent on target problem behav- demand fading, the authors incorporated a DRO-­
iors, buttons were removed from the chart. Both based token economy along with instructions.
token reinforcement and response cost were Specifically, the authors provided tokens contin-
effective with little difference in the effects of the gent on abstinence from vocal stereotypy and
484 A. Bagwell et al.

removed tokens contingent (i.e., response cost) response cost for its specific ease of implementa-
on vocal stereotypy. Following predetermined tion (DeJager et al., 2020).
intervals, the individual exchanged tokens for
time with the computer. The combination of
token economy, DRO, and response cost was Response cost can be implemented in the nat-
effective at reducing vocal stereotypy when ural environment  The removal of access to
instructions were implemented and reinforce- highly preferred items (e.g., cell phones, video
ment was faded. Buckley and Newchok (2005) games, internet privileges) contingent on inap-
demonstrated the positive effects of response cost propriate behavior by parents is a common prac-
on packing exhibited by child with ASD during tice (e.g., Borrego et  al., 2007). Likewise, the
feeding. Response cost was first paired with dif- removal of a favorite toy by a parent when sib-
ferential reinforcement followed by the addition lings are fighting over the toy is common (e.g.,
of simultaneous presentation of a highly pre- Richins & Chaplin, 2015). Removal of privileges
ferred food. Specifically, the authors removed (e.g., access to free time, recess, lunch with
access to a preferred video contingent on packing friends, tokens, points, or other privileges) for
and returned access following mouth cleans. violation of classroom rules is common practice
Response cost plus differential reinforcement in educational settings (e.g., Long et al., 2019).
resulted in significant reductions in packing; the State and local governments “remove” our money
addition of simultaneous presentation of a pre- (i.e., fines) when we break traffic laws. Thus,
ferred food further decreased packing behavior. response cost is a commonly occurring conse-
Nipe et  al. (2018) evaluated the effects of quence in many settings for children and adults
response cost, NCR, and blocking on compliance alike, in the home and at a societal level (CCBD,
with hearing aids exhibited by a teenager with 1990). Response cost procedures are possible and
multiple developmental disabilities. The response conducive to use in natural contexts as individu-
cost component consisted of removal of preferred als are typically accessing reinforcing stimuli on
items for 15-s contingent on the individual a relatively ongoing basis. Thus, from a practical
attempting to remove the prostheses. After initial perspective, reinforcing stimuli are typically
positive effects, the response cost and blocking available for removal contingent on undesirable
components were systematically faded and com- behaviors.
pliance remained high and generalized to differ-
ent settings.
Response cost is socially valid  There have been
many studies assessing the social validity of
25.2.2 Considerations response cost. Specifically, response cost proce-
dures are often the second highest rated form of
25.2.2.1 Advantages behavioral intervention, ranking only behind
Response cost can be easy to imple- reinforcement-based procedures by both teachers
ment  Response cost, especially when used in and parents (e.g., DRO, differential attention,
combination with reinforcement-based proce- social praise; Borrego et al., 2007, Curtis et al.,
dures, can take advantage of programmed rein- 2006, Eid et  al., 2019, Heffer & Kelley, 1987,
forcers without requiring much adjustment to Jones et al., 1998, Pisecco et al., 2001).
existing interventions. In some cases, response
cost can even be self-administered by students in
the classroom when class-wide rules are provided Response cost can be readily combined with
(e.g., Briesch et al., 2015). In fact, in a compari- reinforcement-based strategies  When
son of token economy and response cost alone reinforcement-­based procedures are not effective
and in combination, many teachers favored to a degree that is considered clinically signifi-
cant, response cost may be introduced with rela-
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 485

tively simple procedures in combination with result in an increase in the occurrence of escape
reinforcement-based approaches. When and avoidance behaviors, though such effects
reinforcement-­based strategies are in place (with have seldom been observed in the response cost
previously identified, programmed reinforcers literature (Kazdin, 1972; Walker, 1983). Another
and clearly identified target responses) incorpo- potential side effect of response cost is adventi-
rating a response cost component (when the tious punishment. When Bagwell et al. (in prepa-
reinforcement-­based approach has been shown to ration) implemented a response cost procedure
be ineffective) can be relatively simple. for an individual who engaged in bruxism, item
engagement decreased concomitantly with brux-
25.2.2.2 Disadvantages ism, suggesting that item engagement had been
Potential for misuse  Potential for misuse exists adventitiously punished. When response cost was
when implementing response cost procedures. used in combination with NCR with edible rein-
Without carefully programmed contingencies forcers, item engagement increased while brux-
with clear operational definitions and staff train- ism remained low. Side effects associated with
ing, there is a risk that response cost may be failures in treatment integrity can also result from
implemented incorrectly (e.g., misapplied to the implementation of a response cost procedure.
nontarget behaviors). Consider, for example, the Both errors of omission (failure to reinforce
implementation of response cost in conjunction appropriate alternative behaviors) and commis-
with a token economy. Removing tokens based sion (failure to apply response cost to problem
on behaviors other than the intended target behavior) can result in suppressed appropriate
behaviors, whether as a result of issues with treat- responding and increased problem behavior
ment fidelity or for some other reason, might responding (St. Peter et al., 2016).
result in the “incorrect” removal of all available
tokens, creating an establishing operation with
regard to problem behavior (CCBD, 1990) or Punishment and social stigma  Although the
other complications. colloquial understanding of punishment may not
be consistent with the behavioral principle, it can
impact how people outside of the field might per-
Isolated use  When used in isolation, response ceive the term and its subsequent use. Thus, hav-
cost operates to decrease problem behavior in ing the knowledge and expertise necessary to
lieu of specific programming for increasing adap- explain response cost and use it in a socially rel-
tive behavior. In instances in which the problem evant and effective way may be considered a pre-
behavior is so severe as to pose a danger to the requisite to using response cost procedures in
safety of the individual, direct care staff, or peers, many settings.
a singular focus on decreasing the behavior as
quickly as possible may be acceptable when con- 25.2.2.3 Recommendations
sidering the risk-to-benefit ratio (Vollmer et  al., Response cost, as a punishment procedure,
2011). When the behavior has been reduced to should be used with caution. There are several
acceptable levels, however, the focus should issues to consider when response cost is imple-
widen to include adaptive behaviors, and punish- mented alone or within a treatment package.
ment procedures should no longer be used in
isolation. Response cost should be applied consistently
and immediately following the occurrence of
the target behavior  Applying response cast as
Undesirable side effects  Undesirable side closely in time with the problem behavior will
effects can occur as a result of the use of punish- ensure that the loss (e.g., removal of points) will
ment (Hine et al., 2018). Interventions which are be paired with the problem behavior. If the
perceived by the individual to be aversive can latency between the problem behavior and
486 A. Bagwell et al.

response cost is too large, the consequence may Response cost should only be used When rein-
be associated with nontarget behaviors instead of forcement strategies are not available or have
the intended target behavior (Walker, 1983). been shown to be ineffective  As with any pun-
ishment procedure, response cost should be used
only after other strategies have been proven inef-
The penalty or cost should be appropriate and fective, when the severity of the behavior war-
controlled  When response cost is applied, the rants immediate and intensive intervention, or
loss of tokens or reinforcing stimuli should match when the characteristics of the case create con-
the severity or frequency of the behavior. straints that prevent the initial use of alternative
Alternatively, if too few points are available for strategies.
retention, the penalty may be considered too
severe, and additional problem behaviors may
emerge (DeJager et al., 2020). What is removed should be reinforcing  The
delivery of stimuli contingent on behavior that
does not subsequently increase the likelihood of
Penalty arrangements resulting in negative future instances of the behavior cannot be called
values should be avoided  When response cost a reinforcer. Likewise, if the removal of stimuli
is used, a maximum number of points should be contingent on a target behavior does not reduce
preselected (e.g., 30) according to average base- the likelihood of future occurrences of that target
line data (Walker, 1983). The implementer should behavior, the response cost procedure is not func-
avoid removal of more than the preselected num- tioning as negative punishment. Thus, preference
ber resulting in negative values. If an individual and reinforcer assessments are vital in the imple-
engages in more than the preselected number of mentation of a response cost procedure just as
points, an alternative consequence should be pro- they are in the implementation of reinforcement-­
vided. The implementer should provide alterna- based procedures.
tive ways to earn points or implement another
consequence-based procedure until response cost
can be readministered. 25.3 Time-Out

25.3.1 Inclusionary Time-Out


Applying response cost should be objec-
tive  The removal or loss of points or access to Inclusionary time-out involves removing the
preferred items should be contingent on engage- individual from the reinforcing activity or area
ment in the intended target response. This means without eliminating the ability of the individual
that response cost procedures should be based on to observe the time-in area (Harris, 1985; Wolf
behavior rather than the preferences of the imple- et al., 2006). Types of inclusionary time-out have
menter. Walker (1983) noted that care provider been described as (a) contingent observation, (b)
frustration can emerge when addressing problem removal of stimulus conditions, and (c) ignoring.
behaviors; however, frustrations should not bias Contingent observation “refers to a procedure in
how response cost is applied. Thus, it is impor- which the individual is required to sit on the
tant to remain objective and apply response cost periphery of the ongoing activity and observe the
only when target behaviors occur. Development appropriate behaviors of his or her peers for a
of objective protocols, care provider training, and brief period of time” (p.280, Harris, 1985). With
ongoing assessment of treatment integrity is contingent observation, the individual may sit
recommended. adjacent to the reinforcing area and observe their
peers engage in activities. Removal of stimulus
conditions refers to the removal of reinforcing
stimuli (e.g., preferred toys) contingent on
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 487

engagement in a target behavior (Harris, 1985; individual from clearly viewing others in the
Wolf et al., 2006). For example, if a child throws time-in area.
a toy at their peers, the toy can be removed from
the child’s possession. Ignoring, also known as
planned ignoring, refers to the removal of social 25.3.3 Seclusion Time-Out
attention without removing the individual from
the environment contingent on engagement in a Seclusion time-out, also known as isolation, is
target behavior (Harris, 1985; Nelson & the most intrusive form of time-out and is gener-
Rutherford, 1983; Wolf et al., 2006). For exam- ally not recommended for use because of risk of
ple, if a child hits their peer with a toy, the teacher injury and potential effects of isolation the indi-
might prompt the peer to move away without vidual may experience. However, in the interest
engaging in conversation with the child. During of clarity and distinguishing the procedure from
the planned ignoring interval, the teacher and less intrusive forms of time-out and crisis man-
peers would ignore any attempts to interact by agement techniques, we discuss and provide a
the other child. It has been demonstrated that for description of seclusion time-out. Seclusion
inclusionary time-out strategies to be used effec- time-out refers to the removal of an individual
tively, the implementer must ensure that all rein- from the reinforcing area to a separate non-­
forcement is withheld (e.g., social interactions reinforcing environment (e.g., a separate room)
with others) and appropriate time-out behavior with the express purpose of behavior reduction
occurs such as sitting quietly or staying in the (Harris, 1985; Nelson & Rutherford, 1983; Wolf
time-out environment (Nelson & Rutherford, et al., 2006). Seclusion settings are typically bare
1983). and do not include any items or objects with
which the individual can interact. The individual
is transitioned to and placed in the separate set-
25.3.2 Exclusionary Time-Out ting alone without peers, care providers, or clini-
cians. An example of seclusion time-out would
Exclusionary time-out is similar to inclusionary include placing an individual in a room without
time-out in that the individual is removed from supervision for an extended period of time con-
the reinforcing area. However, the individual is tingent on engagement in problem behavior.
unable to observe others during exclusionary During the seclusion time-out interval, the unsu-
time-out. The individual may be required to sit pervised individual may engage in high-risk
facing the opposite direction of the activity, or a behaviors such as SIB or other destructive behav-
barrier (e.g., room divider, screen) is placed iors (Ryan et al., 2007).
between the individual and the activity (Harris, A distinction should be made between seclu-
1985; Wolf et al., 2006). Similar to inclusionary sion time-out and crisis management, which is
time-out, it has been recommended that the not a behavior reduction technique. If an individ-
implementer ensures that the individual does not ual poses a severe risk to those around them (e.g.,
contact reinforcement, appropriate behavior is peers, direct care staff, school-based educational
maintained in the time-out setting, and the length personnel), crisis management procedures might
of time is appropriate (Nelson & Rutherford, include placement of the individual in a safe and
1983). Ensuring that these aspects of the time-out secure environment, under constant and appro-
are implemented requires the implementer to pre- priate supervision (including blocking SIB if
pare the time-out setting prior to use. Thus, stim- necessary), until predetermined safety criteria are
uli (e.g., toys, leisure items) are removed before met and the individual may be transitioned out of
the individual is placed in the time-out setting. A the crisis management protocol/area. The goal of
barrier or screen used to divide the time-in and this type of crisis management is to ensure the
time-out areas is typically used to prevent the safety of the individual and others involved in an
488 A. Bagwell et al.

emergency situation; it is not intended as a treat- appropriate behaviors were exhibited (i.e., release
ment of the dangerous behavior. contingency time-out; Donaldson & Vollmer,
2011). After a release from time-out, parents pre-
sented a different instruction, and the same proto-
25.3.4 Time-Out in Practice col was followed. The procedures for time-out
plus escape extinction were similar; however, the
Time-out has been used to address a variety of parents presented the same instruction following
problem behaviors with considerable evidence the release from time-out. The results showed
demonstrating its effectiveness. Additionally, that time-out alone and time-out plus escape
recent research has provided evidence that mul- extinction produced increases in compliance.
tiple variations of time-out can be used safely and However, the procedure that included escape
effectively. For example, Donaldson and Vollmer extinction was more effective than the alternative
(2011) compared two time-out procedures in procedure.
terms of their effects on problem behavior (i.e., Recent evaluations of time-out have also
crying, aggression) exhibited by preschool-aged examined the long-term effects of time-out pro-
children. Three of four participants were diag- cedures. For example, Iwata et  al. (2009) con-
nosed with autism or developmental delay, and ducted an evaluation of a phased time-out
the fourth participant had no diagnoses. An procedure with individuals at a community-based
exclusionary time-out procedure was used such residential program. The study sought to identify
that participants were removed from the reinforc- if time-out procedures were needed for 34 resi-
ing environment contingent on engagement in dents. Participants were diagnosed with develop-
problem behavior and observed by therapists dur- mental or intellectual disabilities (e.g.,
ing time-out periods. Within this context, Prader-Willi syndrome, cerebral palsy, ADHD)
Donaldson and Vollmer compared a fixed dura- and had histories of engagement in some form of
tion time-out procedure to a release contingency problem behavior (e.g., aggression, property
time-out procedure. The fixed duration time-out destruction). Exclusionary and seclusion time-­
required participants to remain in the time-out out procedures were utilized contingent on target
area for 4 min. The release contingency time-out behaviors and were faded over a 1-year period.
also required participants to remain in the time-­ Iwata et al. successfully faded the time-out pro-
out area for 4 min. However, the time-out dura- cedures for 92% of the participants; in some
tion increased by 30-s intervals if participants cases, alternative strategies were implemented.
engaged in additional aggression or disruption. As time-out was faded, a decrease in problem
The results showed that both time-out strategies behaviors was observed across participants.
reduced problem behaviors and neither strategy These results indicated that time-out does not
was more effective than the other. The results of have to be used indefinitely to produce lasting
Donaldson and Vollmer suggested that using a effects. Also, in some cases, time-out alone was
time-out procedure with or without a release con- sufficient to decrease problem behaviors.
tingency can be useful in reducing problem Other recent studies of time-out procedures
behaviors in similar contexts. have evaluated other procedure-specific varia-
Everett et  al. (2007) evaluated the effects of tions and parameters. For example, Slocum et al.
parent-delivered time-out versus time-out plus (2019) evaluated the effects of delaying the
escape extinction on escape-maintained noncom- implementation of time-out procedures following
pliance exhibited by four typically developing target problem behaviors exhibited by four
preschool-aged children. During both conditions, preschool-­aged children (two of whom had been
parents provided an instruction to their child. identified as developmentally delayed; two of
When time-out was used alone, participants were whom had no diagnoses). The authors first dem-
prompted to a time-out area contingent on non- onstrated that time-out was effective when imple-
compliance and were allowed to leave when mented immediately following occurrences of
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 489

problem behavior. Next, Slocum et  al. imple- procedures have been attempted in isolation, add-
mented a series of conditions in which they ing a time-out component to the treatment pack-
delayed the implementation of time-out (i.e., 5 s, age may allow the care provider to take advantage
10 s, 60 s, 120 s) with three of four participants. of previously programmed reinforcers.
Problem behavior remained low when time-out
was delayed up to 90 and 120 s. Their results sug-
gested that time-out procedures can be effective Time-out can be quick and effective  The
even when the procedures are not immediately effects of time-out procedures can be quick and
implemented following target problem behav- considerable when they are implemented consis-
iors. Donaldson et  al. (2013) also examined tently and for appropriate durations. In a review
procedure-­specific parameters by evaluating the of time-out parameters, Corralejo et  al. (2018)
effects of reducing durations of time-out intervals found that a duration of 5 min or less was indi-
contingent on compliance with time-out instruc- cated based on findings regarding the effects of
tions. After showing the standard time-out proce- duration on time-out periods. Additionally, there
dures were effective at reducing problem behavior was no benefit to time-out periods lasting as long
with six of six participants, Donaldson et  al. as 10 min relative to shorter durations. It should
found that reducing durations of time-out inter- be noted that although the required time for
vals was effective at maintaining low rates of effects may be short for some individuals and
problem behavior with four of six participants. their behavior, more time may be required for
others. Thus, the time parameters of the proce-
dures should always be individualized to the indi-
25.3.5 Considerations vidual case. However, as long as the time-out
procedures are implemented consistently and
25.3.5.1 Advantages appropriately, little time may be needed to be
Social validity  Studies assessing the social spent in the time-out setting to produce signifi-
validity of different behavioral interventions have cant reductions in problem behavior.
shown time-out to be generally acceptable. While
time-out is typically rated by care providers
below reinforcement-based interventions and The individual can stay in the learning/thera-
response cost, time-out is consistently ranked peutic environment  When implementing inclu-
above overcorrection, medication, and spanking sionary time-out procedures, the individual does
(Blampied & Kahan, 1992; Miller & Kelley, not need to leave the physical setting where the
1992; Miltenberger et  al., 1989; Plessy et  al., behavior occurred. For example, under some
2018). Global efforts to eliminate the use of cor- time-out arrangements (e.g., loss of access to pre-
poral punishment in family homes have led ferred stimuli during task demands), loss of
experts and advocates to recommend the use of instructional time would not be a requirement
time-out procedures (Warzak et  al., 2012). during the procedure. Exclusion from time with
Decisions regarding whether or not to incorpo- family would not be a requirement of the proce-
rate time-out in a treatment package should be dures with individuals in home setting. Last, ter-
made on a case-by-case basis with support from mination of time in the therapeutic environment
stakeholders. would not be a requirement of the procedures
with individuals receiving behavioral or other
supports.
Time-out takes advantage of existing reinforc-
ers  Time-out from reinforcement necessitates 25.3.5.2 Disadvantages
the presence of reinforcement prior to the removal Potential for misuse  Potential for misuse exists
of either the reinforcing stimuli (inclusion) or the when time-out is not implemented based on
individual (exclusion). If reinforcement-based evidence-­
based recommendations. If the care
490 A. Bagwell et al.

provider’s implementation of the time-out proce- child in a classroom engages in behavior main-
dures is reinforced by escape from having to tained by peer attention, inclusionary time-out
manage problem behavior (i.e., negative rein- may not remove the relevant reinforcer (Turner &
forcement), misapplication of the procedure and/ Watson, 1999). Similarly, the amount of attention
or in appropriate extension of the duration of required to implement time-out may reinforce
time-out can occur. attention-maintained behavior, especially if
physical guidance is implemented.

Potential loss of instructional time  Potential 25.3.5.3 Recommendations


loss of instructional time is one of the primary Time-in must be reinforcing  As the full term
concerns with time-out in educational settings (i.e., time-out from reinforcement) implies, time-
(Zirpoli, 2012). Extended periods of time outside ­in should be a reinforcing environment and stim-
the classroom can impact students’ academic uli targeted for removal should be reinforcing. If
success as well as their relationships with peers. the individual is removed from a setting or if
If time-out is being used repeatedly with an indi- stimuli are removed that are not reinforcing,
vidual student at high rates and durations, it is negative punishment will likely not have
possible that the intervention is ineffective (i.e., occurred as intended (Turner & Watson, 1999).
response cost is not functioning as a punisher, This could, in some scenarios, overlap with
might be reinforcing the behavior) and would escape or avoidance-­maintained problem behav-
warrant reevaluation. Results of previous reviews ior in which the time-­out setting might function
of time-out usage in school settings have sug- as reinforcement.
gested that implementation of time-out was low
relative to lost instructional time resulting from
absences, suspensions, and truancies (Skiba & Time-out cannot be reinforcing  To be effective
Raison, 1990); though updated assessment of as a form of punishment, the time-out environ-
these effects may be warranted. ment cannot contain stimuli which function as
reinforcers (Turner & Watson, 1999). An alterna-
tive source of reinforcement could compete with
Negative side effects  Negative side effects com- the reinforcement that has been removed, lessen-
monly associated with negative punishment are ing the efficacy of time-out as a punishment pro-
possible when time-out procedures are imple- cedure and potentially establishing a functional
mented. However, reviews of time-out studies relation between the problem behavior and the
have failed to note any evidence of side effects time-out setting.
unique or more pronounced in the context of
time-out procedures (Corralejo et  al., 2018;
Warzak et al., 2012). Reinforcement based procedures should be
used first  Time-out should be used when
reinforcement-­ based alternatives are not avail-
Misapplications of the proce- able or have been attempted and shown to be
dure  Misapplication of time-out procedures can ineffective. In the event that an individual engages
occur when the function of the target behavior is in severe, problem behavior which poses a risk to
not addressed or has not been correctly identified. themselves or those around them, reinforcement
If the target behavior is maintained by escape and punishment-based procedures might be used
from demands, removing the individual from the in combination from the beginning of interven-
environment (and the demand) can reinforce the tion, to achieve a swift reduction in problem
problem behavior (Taylor & Miller, 1997). If a behavior.
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 491

25.4 R
 esponse Cost and Time-­ stress, and/or death; and (c) dehumanization of
Out: Acceptability the individual, through means such as social deg-
radation, social isolation, verbal abuse, tech-
Regarding the implementation of punishment niques inappropriate for the individual’s age, and
procedures generally, there are several issues treatment out of proportion to the target behavior.
relating to acceptability that merit additional con- Such dehumanization is equally unacceptable
sideration. A considerable body of literature and whether or not an individual has a disability
diversity of opinions has been generated around (AAIDD, 2020). AAIDD’s positions are consis-
the topic of social acceptability. There are many tent with the recommendations for implementa-
stakeholders involved in the assessment and tion identified in the current chapter more broadly
treatment of problem behavior. Acceptability of in the behavioral literature.
different behavioral interventions has been In a position paper released by the Council for
assessed from many perspectives including pro- Children with Behavioral Disorders (CCBD;,
fessional organizations (e.g., ABAI), organiza- 1990), the authors identified response cost as a
tions representing individuals with specific procedure used frequently in natural settings
disabilities, parents, and teachers. In this section, including the classroom, the home, and in society
official statements from organizations as well as broadly. They noted that response cost appears to
studies related to social acceptability will be be most effective when used in combination with
reviewed and discussed, along with a variety of a reinforcement-based procedure and recom-
cultural and demographic variables that have mend against using response cost in isolation. In
been studied with regard to the acceptability of a more recent position paper, CCBD (2009) drew
different behavioral interventions. a clear distinction between the use of time-out
procedures and “seclusion,” stating that the two
concepts are often confounded. The authors
25.4.1 Professional Associations acknowledged time-out as a behavior reduction
and Disability-Specific technique and identified three forms of time-out
Organizations procedures including (a) inclusionary, (b) exclu-
sionary, and (b) seclusionary. Regarding restric-
Many position statements released by organiza- tive forms of time-out, they cautioned, “regardless
tions related to the representation of individuals of the name or the purpose, if a student is alone
with disabilities and by professional organiza- and prevented from leaving, this setting consti-
tions (e.g., ABAI) have not specifically addressed tutes seclusion” (CCBD, 2009, p. 3).
the use of response cost or time-out, with the As discussed earlier, ABAI (i.e., Vollmer et al.,
notable exception of exclusionary time-out. 2011) provided a position statement in which the
However, many organizations have officially issues of restraint and seclusion were addressed.
stated positions on the use of aversive stimuli or Specifically, the authors stated that they supported
aversive interventions, generally. While response the use of planned time-out but that it must “(a) be
cost and time-out procedures may be considered derived from a behavioral assessment, (b) incor-
to be aversive in some capacity, these procedures porate reinforcement strategies for appropriate
do not fall under the scope of procedures most behavior, (c) be of brief duration, (d) be evaluated
often identified in such position statements. by objective outcome data, and (e) be consistent
The American Association on Intellectual and with the scientific literature and current best prac-
Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD;, 2020) tices” (Vollmer et al., 2011, pp. 105–106). Across
called for the elimination of aversive procedures all of the position papers detailed above, views on
characterized by one or more of (a) obvious signs the use of time-out procedures are consistent.
of physical pain experienced by the individual; Response cost is rarely specifically addressed but
(b) potential or actual physical side effects, also does not appear to meet the provided defini-
including tissue damage, physical illness, severe tions of aversive interventions.
492 A. Bagwell et al.

25.4.2 Teachers and Parents interventions by mothers of children with


Oppositional Defiant Disorder or Conduct
Many studies have compared different behavioral Disorder. Positive reinforcement was the highest
interventions in terms of teacher and parent rated intervention with response cost second;
acceptability and preference. For example, a sur- however, response cost was not rated signifi-
vey of 1005 elementary school teachers across cantly higher than time-out, differential attention,
the United States found that an intervention con- and overcorrection. A more recent study (i.e.,
sisting of praise and planned ignoring was rated Stary et  al., 2016) included different diagnoses
similarly acceptable and feasible to an interven- (i.e., ADHD, ASD, no diagnosis) in the vignettes
tion consisting of self-managed response cost used to present different behavioral interventions
(Briesch et al., 2015). In addition, Pisecco et al. to parents via a community sample. As can be
(2001) used the Behavioral Intervention Rating seen in Table 25.1 from Stary et al. (2016), posi-
Scale (BIRS; Elliot & Von Brock Treuting, 1991) tive reinforcement and response cost were rated
to assess the acceptability ratings of 159 elemen- as the most acceptable interventions regardless of
tary school teachers specific to diagnosis. Time-out was rated as significantly
ADHD. Interventions included parent feedback/ more acceptable than spanking regardless of
goal setting, response cost, medication, and a diagnosis. However, no studies have assessed the
group contingency. Response cost and feedback/ impact of having a child with unique behavioral
goal setting interventions were deemed compara- challenges on parent perceptions of various
bly acceptable, and significantly more acceptable behavioral interventions.
than medication.
Blampied and Kahan (1992) assessed social
acceptability of punishment procedures specifi- 25.4.3 Cultural and Demographic
cally, including parent views. No significant dif- Variables
ferences were found based on respondents’
demographic variables, including parental status. Some studies have assessed the potential influ-
The interventions included were rated on a ence of different cultural and demographic vari-
5-point scale in terms of acceptability in different ables on social acceptability of different
settings and different child genders. Response behavioral interventions, including response cost
cost procedures and social reprimands were more and time-out (e.g., Borrego et al., 2007; Eid et al.,
preferred than time-out and overcorrection, 2019; Heffer & Kelley, 1987; Mah & Johnston,
which were rated significantly higher than physi- 2012). Borrego et al. (2007) found that response
cal punishment. Overall, punishment procedures cost procedures were rated significantly more
were deemed to be more acceptable in the home acceptable by Mexican-American parents than
than school. any other treatment; and time-out, overcorrec-
It should be noted that many studies assessing tion, and token economy were rated as signifi-
parent perceptions of behavioral interventions cantly more acceptable than differential attention,
have not specifically recruited parents of children spanking, and medication. No differences related
who have specific diagnoses; instead, they have to response cost and time-out procedures were
utilized community samples. The degree to which found when controlling for acculturation. Mah
being a parent of a child with a history of engage- and Johnston (2012) compared perspectives of
ment in severe problem behavior might impact Euro-Canadian and Chinese-immigrant mothers.
one’s knowledge and acceptance of various Techniques were grouped into three categories
behavioral interventions has not been clearly including (a) reward (praise and token economy),
established. One exception, Jones et  al. (1998), (b) withdrawal (response cost and time-out), and
assessed acceptability of different behavioral (c) punishment (overcorrection and spanking).
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 493

Table 25.1  Descriptive statistics for acceptability ratings of techniques and diagnostic status
Technique/diagnosis Mean SD % with positive reaction
Timeout
ADHD 13.26 4.43 60.2
Autistic disorder 13.54 3.33 61.4
No diagnosis 13.75 3.97 58.7
All groups 13.53 3.96 60.0
Response cost
ADHD 14.87 4.03 73.6
Autistic disorder 14.42 3.18 68.4
No diagnosis 15.01 3.60 70.6
All groups 14.80 3.63 71.0
Positive reinforcement
ADHD 15.82 3.45 70.6
Autistic disorder 15.37 4.02 73.7
No diagnosis 14.99 3.91 74.6
All groups 15.38 3.79 73.0
Spanking
ADHD 8.78 4.60 19.1
Autistic disorder 8.86 4.66 19.3
No diagnosis 9.53 4.46 17.4
All groups 9.09 4.55 18.5
Reprinted with permission from Stary et al. (2016)

No differences were found between reward and should both be recommended and used with cau-
withdrawal techniques in terms of acceptability tion given some associated disadvantages and
within or between the two groups of mothers. A ethical considerations. However, given their
similar study was recently conducted with par- advantages, both procedures are viable, and valu-
ents of children with ASD in Saudi Arabia (Eid able options in situations in which (a) non-­
et al., 2019). Eid et al.’s findings were consistent punishment-­ based approaches have been
with studies published with American parents, exhausted or demonstrated to be ineffective, (b)
with the relative acceptability of interventions when the severity of the behavior warrants imme-
placing reinforcement-based interventions first, diate and intensive behavior-reductive interven-
response cost second, and time-out third and tions, and/or (c) when treatment constraints are
ahead of medication and spanking. present that prevent the use of alternative
strategies.

25.4.4 Summary
References
Response cost and time-out are two operant-­
based interventions that have been shown to be AAIDD (2020). Aversive procedures: Position statement
of AAIDD.
effective at the reduction problem behaviors Baer, D.  M. (1960). Escape and avoidance responses of
exhibited by individuals with ASD and other pre-school children to two schedules of reinforcement
developmental disorders. Both procedures are withdrawal. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
derived from basic principles pertaining to nega- Behavior, 3(2), 155–160. https://doi.org/10.1901/
jeab.1960.3-­155
tive punishment and have wide evidentiary sup- Baer, D. M. (1971). Let’s take another look at punishment.
port in both the basic and applied research areas. Psychology Today, 5(5), 32–34.
Although both procedures have a great deal of Bagwell, A., Falcomata, T.  S., Ramirez, A., Vargas
support in terms of clinical application, they Londono, F., Shpall, C., and Swinnea, S. (in prepa-
494 A. Bagwell et al.

ration). Noncontingent access to edible reinforcers ventions for children with ADHD: A cross-cultural
effectively reduces bruxism maintained by automatic comparison of teachers in the United States and New
reinforcement and is related to high preferred item Zealand. School Psychology Quarterly, 21(2), 171–
engagement. 196. https://doi.org/10.1521/scpq.2006.21.2.171
Bartlett, S. M., Rapp, J. T., Krueger, T. K., & Henrickson, DeJager, B., Houlihan, D., Filter, K. J., Mackie, P. F. E.,
M.  L. (2011). The use of response cost to treat spit- & Klein, L. (2020). Comparing the effectiveness and
ting by a child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, ease of implementation of token economy, response
26(1), 76–83. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.322 cost, and a combination condition in rural elementary
Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1966). Human, free-operant school classrooms. Journal of Rural Mental Health,
avoidance of “time out” from monetary reinforcement. 44(1), 39–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/rmh0000123
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9(5), Donaldson, M. J., & Vollmer, T. R. (2011). An evaluation
557–565. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaeb.1966.9-­557 and comparison of time-out procedures with and with-
Blampied, N.  M., & Kahan, E. (1992). Acceptability out release contingencies. Journal of Applied Behavior
of alternative punishments. Behavior Analysis, 44(4), 693–705. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Modification, 16(3), 400–413. https://doi. jaba.2011.44-­693
org/10.1177/01454455920163006 Donaldson, J.  M., Vollmer, T.  R., Yakich, T.  M., & Van
Buckley, S.  D., & Newchok, D.  K. (2005). An evalu- Camp, C. (2013). Effects of a reduced time-out interval
ation of simultaneous presentation and differential on compliance with the time-out instruction. Journal
reinforcement with response cost to reduce packing. of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(2), 369–378. https://
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(3), 405– doi.org/10.1002/jaba.40
409. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.71-­04 Eid, A., Jobeir, A., Alhaqbani, O., AlSaud, A., & Fryling,
Board, B. A. C. (2014). Professional and ethical compli- M. J. (2019). Assessment of parental acceptability and
ance code for behavior analysts. preference for behavioral interventions for childhood
Borrego, J., Ibanez, E. S., Spendlove, S. J., & Pemberton, problem behavior in Saudi Arabia. Child & Family
J. R. (2007). Treatment acceptability among Mexican Behavior Therapy, 41(4), 237–241. https://doi.org/10
American parents. Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 217–227. .1080/07317107.2019.1659548
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.007 Elliot, S. N., & Von Brock Treuting, M. (1991). The behav-
Briesch, A.  M., Briesch, J.  M., & Chafouleas, S.  M. ior intervention rating scale: Development and valida-
(2015). Investigating the usability of classroom man- tion of a pretreatment acceptability and effectiveness
agement strategies among elementary schoolteachers. measure. Journal of School Psychology, 29(1), 43–51.
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 17(1), https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-­4405(91)90014-­I
5–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300714531827 Everett, G.  R., Olmi, D.  J., Edwards, R.  P., Tingstrom,
Capriotti, M.  R., Brandt, B.  C., Ricketts, E.  J., Espil, D. H., Sterling-Turner, H. E., & Christ, T. J. (2007).
F. M., & Woods, D. W. (2012). Comparing the effects An empirical investigation of time-out with and with-
of differential reinforcement of other behavior and out escape extinction to treat escape-maintained non-
response-cost contingencies on tice in youth with compliance. Behavior Modification, 31(4), 412–434.
Tourette syndrome. Journal of Applied Behavior https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445506297725
Analysis, 45(2), 251–263. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., & Pabico, R. R. (2007).
jaba.2012.45-­251 Unintentional stimulus control during the treatment of
CCBD. (1990). Position paper on use of behavior reduc- pica displayed by a young man with autism. Research
tion strategies with children with behavioral disorders. in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(4), 350–359. https://
Behavioral Disorders, 15(4), 243–260. doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2006.12.004
CCBD. (2009). CCBD’s position summary on the Ferster, C. B. (1958). Control of behavior in chimpanzees
use of seclusion in school settings. Behavioral and pigeons by time out from positive reinforcement.
Disorders, 34(4), 235–243. https://doi. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied,
org/10.1177/019874290903400405 72(8), 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093787
Conyers, C., Miltenberger, R., Maki, A., Barenz, R., Ferster, C. B. (1960). Suppression of a performance under
Jurgens, M., Sailer, A., Haugen, M., & Kopp, B. differential reinforcement of low rates by a pre-time-­
(2004). A comparison of response cost and differen- out stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
tial reinforcement of other behavior to reduce disrup- of Behavior, 3(2), 143–153. https://doi.org/10.1901/
tive behavior in a preschool classroom. Journal of jeab.1960.3-­143
Applied Behavior Analysis, 37(3), 411–415. https:// Harris, K.  R. (1985). Definitional, parametric, and pro-
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2004.37-­411 cedural considerations in timeout interventions and
Corralejo, S. M., Jensen, S. A., Greathouse, A. D., & Ward, research. Exceptional Children, 51(4), 279–288.
L. E. (2018). Parameters of time-out: Research update Heffer, R.  W., & Kelley, M.  L. (1987). Mothers’ accep-
and comparison to parenting programs, books, and tance of behavioral interventions for children: The
online recommendations. Behavior Therapy, 49(1), influence of parent race and income. Behavior
99–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2017.09.005 Therapy, 18(2), 153–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Curtis, D.  F., Pisecco, S., Hamilton, R.  J., & Moore, S0005-­7894(87)80039-­4
D. W. (2006). Teacher perceptions of classroom inter-
25  Response Cost and Time-Out from Reinforcement 495

Hine, J.  F., Ardoin, S.  P., & Call, N.  A. (2018). Token Nelson, C.  M., & Rutherford, R.  B. (1983). Timeout
economies: Using basic experimental research to revisited: Guidelines for its use in special education.
guide practical applications. Journal of Contemporary Exceptional Education Quarterly, 3(4), 56–67.
Psychotherapy, 48(3), 145–154. https://doi. Nipe, T.  A., Dowdy, A., Quigley, J., Gill, A., & Weiss,
org/10.1007/s10879-­017-­9376-­5 M. J. (2018). Increasing the wearing of multiple pre-
Holland, E. L., & McLaughlin, T. F. (1982). Using pub- scription prosthetic devices. Education and Treatment
lic posting and group consequences to manage stu- of Children, 41(3), 331–344. https://doi.org/10.1353/
dent behavior during supervision. The Journal of etc.2018.0017
Educational Research, 76(1), 29–34. https://doi.org/1 Nolan, J.  D., & Filter, K.  J. (2012). A function-based
0.1080/00220671.1982.10885419 classroom behavior intervention using non-contingent
Iwata, B.  A. (1988). The development and adoption of reinforcement plus response cost. Education and
controversial default technologies. The Behavior Treatment of Children, 35(3), 419–430. https://doi.
Analyst, 11(2), 149–157. https://doi.org/10.1007/ org/10.1353/etc.2012.0017
BF03392468 Pisecco, S., Huzinec, C., & Curtis, D. (2001). The effects
Iwata, B.  A., Rolider, N.  R., & Dozier, C.  L. (2009). of child characteristics on teachers’ acceptability of
Evaluation of timeout programs through phased classroom-based behavioral strategies and psycho-
withdrawal. Journal of Applied Research in stimulant medication for the treatment of ADHD.
Intellectual Disabilities, 22(2), 203–209. https://doi. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(3), 413–421.
org/10.1111/j.1468-­3148.2008.00489.x https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3003_12
Jones, M.  L., Eyberg, S.  M., Adams, C.  D., & Boggs, Plessy, K. S., Long, A. C. J., & Kelley, M. L. (2018). The
S.  R. (1998). Treatment acceptability of behavioral influence of race and income on community mothers’
interventions for children: An assessment by mothers acceptance of child management methods. Behavior
of children with disruptive behavior disorders. Child Therapy, 49(5), 668–680. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
& Family Behavior Therapy, 20(4), 15–26. https://doi. beth.2017.12.011
org/10.1300/J019v20n04_02 Richins, M.  L., & Chaplin, L.  N. (2015). Material par-
Kazdin, A. E. (1972). Response cost: The removal of con- enting: How the use of goods in parenting fos-
ditioned reinforcers for therapeutic change. Behavior ters materialism in the next generation. Journal of
Therapy, 3(4), 533–546. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Consumer Research., 41(6), 1333–1357. https://doi.
S0005-­7894(72)80001-­7 org/10.1086/680087
Leitenberg, H. (1965). Is time-out from positive reinforce- Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Call, N. A., & Kelley, M. E.
ment an aversive event? A review of the experimental (2005). Self-injury, aggression and pica. In I. L. Rubin
evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 64(6), 428. https:// & A.  C. Crocker (Eds.), Developmental disabilities:
doi.org/10.1037/h0022657 Delivery of medical care for children and adults
Long, A.  C. J., Miller, F.  G., & Upright, J.  J. (2019). (p. 506). Brooks Publishing.
Classroom management for ethnic-racial minor- Ryan, J. B., Peterson, R. L., & Rozalski, M. (2007). State
ity students: A meta-analysis of single-case design policies concerning the use of seclusion timeout in
studies. School Psychology, 34(1), 1–13. https://doi. schools. Education and Treatment of Children, 30(4),
org/10.1037/spq0000305 215–239. https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.2007.0032
Mah, J.  W. T., & Johnston, C. (2012). Cultural varia- Shillingsburg, M. A., Lomas, J. E., & Bradley, D. (2012).
tions in mothers’ acceptance of and intent to use Treatment of vocal stereotypy in an analogues and
behavioral child management techniques. Journal of classroom setting. Behavioral Interventions, 27(3),
Child and Family Studies, 21(3), 486–497. https://doi. 151–163. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1340
org/10.1007/s10826-­011-­9502-­z Sigafoos, J., Arthur, M., & O’Reilly, M. (2003).
McGoey, K.  E., & DuPaul, G.  J. (2000). Token rein- Challenging behavior and developmental disability.
forcement and response cost procedures: Reducing Whurr.
the disruptive behavior of preschool children with Skiba, R.  J., & Raison, J. (1990). Relationship between
attention-­deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School the use of timeout and academic achievement.
Psychology Quarterly, 15(3), 330–343. https://doi. Exceptional Children, 57(1), 36–46. https://doi.
org/10.1037/h0088790 org/10.1177/001440299005700105
Miller, D. L., & Kelley, M. L. (1992). Treatment accept- Skinner, B.  F. (1953). Science and human behavior..
ability: The effects of parent gender, marital adjust- Macmillan.
ment, and child behavior. Child & Family Behavior Slocum, S. K., Vollmer, T. R., & Donaldson, J. M. (2019).
Therapy, 14(1), 11–23. https://doi.org/10.1300/ Effects of delayed time-out on problem behavior
J019v14n01_02 of preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior
Miltenberger, R. G., Parrish, J. M., Rickert, V., & Kohr, Analysis, 52(4), 994–1004. https://doi.org/10.1002/
M. (1989). Assessing treatment acceptability with jaba.640
consumers of outpatient child behavior management St. Peter, C. C., Byrd, J. D., Pence, S. T., & Foreman, A. P.
services. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 11(1), (2016). Effects of treatment-integrity failures on a
35–44. https://doi.org/10.1300/J019v11n01_03 response-cost procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior
496 A. Bagwell et al.

Analysis, 49(2), 308–328. https://doi.org/10.1002/ tions. Exceptional Education Quarterly, 3(4), 47–55.
jaba.291 https://doi.org/10.1177/074193258300300411
Stary, A.  K., Hupp, S.  D. A., Jewell, J.  D., & Everett, Watkins, N., & Rapp, J. T. (2014). Environmental enrich-
G. E. (2016). Parent acceptability of time-out, spank- ment and response cost: Immediate and subsequent
ing, response cost, and positive reinforcement. The effects of stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior
Behavior Therapist, 39(4), 112–117. Analysis, 47(1), 186–191. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Taylor, J., & Miller, M. (1997). When timeout works some jaba.97
of the time: The importance of treatment integrity and Warzak, W. J., Floress, M. T., Kellen, M., Kazmerski, J. S.,
functional assessment. School Psychology Quarterly, & Chopko, S. (2012). Trends in time-out research: Are
12(1), 4–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0088943 we focusing our efforts where our efforts are needed?
Turner, H.  S., & Watson, T.  S. (1999). Consultants The Behavior Therapist, 35(2), 30–33.
guide for the use of time-out in the preschool and Weiner, H. (1962). Some effects of response cost upon
elementary classroom. Psychology in the Schools, human operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental
36(2), 135–148. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-­ Analysis of Behavior, 5(2), 201–208. https://doi.
6807(199903)36:2<135::AID-­PITS6>3.0.CO;2-­3 org/10.1901/jeab.1962.5-­201
Van Houten, R., Axelrod, S., Bailey, J.  S., Favell, J.  E., Woods, T.  S. (1982). Reducing severe aggressive and
Foxx, R.  M., Iwata, B.  A., & Lovaas, O.  I. (1988). self-injurious behavior: A nonintrusive, home based
The right to effective behavioral treatment. Journal approach. Behavioral Disorders, 7(3), 180–188.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21(4), 381–384. https:// https://doi.org/10.1177/019874298200700311
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1988.21-­381 Wolf, T. L., McLaughlin, T. F., & Williams, R. L. (2006).
Vollmer, T.  R., Hagopian, L.  P., Bailey, J.  S., Dorsey, Time-out interventions and strategies: A brief review
M. F., Hanley, G. P., Lennox, D., … Spreat, S. (2011). and recommendations. International Journal of
The Association for Behavior Analysis International Special Education, 21(3), 22–29.
position statement on restraint and seclusion. The Zirpoli, T.  J. (2012). Behavior management: Positive
Behavior Analyst, 34(1), 103–110. https://doi. applications for teachers (6th ed.). Pearson Education
org/10.1007/BF03392238 Limited.
Walker, H.  M. (1983). Applications of response cost in
school settings: Outcomes, issues, and recommenda-
The Token Economy
26
Patrick M. Ghezzi and Ainsley B. Lewon

26.1 Introduction dural fidelity) consistently produce the best out-


comes, as expected (Reed & Codding, 2011).
We begin this chapter where papers on the token Our chapter is written for credentialed applied
economy often end, with ethics. The decision to behavior analysts who work in the field with
implement a token economy, as we shall empha- youthful clients and professional co-workers and
size, is always informed by the relevant course- who supervise aspiring applied behavior ana-
work, training, and supervised experience in the lysts. We take for granted a working knowledge
applied analysis of behavior (ABA). If this of functional behavior assessment and interven-
appears obvious, it is because of the agonizing tions, and we naturally assume an unwavering
lessons learned in the early days of “behavior commitment to manage the token economy
modification.” We open a window on those days according to the principles of behavior and the
in order to comment on ethical matters pertaining best practices of ABA.
to ABA in general and to the token economy in The bulk of our own applied experience is
particular. early intensive behavior intervention (EIBI) and
We turn next to a list of nine key elements in a early childhood autism (e.g., Lovaas, 1987;
token economy. We emphasize “key elements” Lewon & Ghezzi, 2020). This background does
for good reason. The trove of scientific research not commit us to writing a chapter on autism and
and systematic replication on the token economy early intervention per se, and besides, it would be
today is sufficiently rich and informative to say a mistake to assume that a token economy is
with confidence that certain elements had better restricted to a particular person, age, setting, or
be in place in order for the intervention to qualify circumstance. We take this opportunity instead to
as ABA as here conceived. Putting these keys ele- discuss broader matters, beginning with events
ments in motion does not guarantee a successful over 50 years ago that shaped the future of ABA.
outcome, but it does increase the chances of one
when the elements are in place. Successful
behavioral interventions with high scores on 26.2 Ethics and the Token
measures of intervention integrity (a.k.a. proce- Economy

Workers in the field of applied behavior analysis


P. M. Ghezzi (*) · A. B. Lewon might recall reading about the scandal at Sunland
Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Miami Training Center in Jon Bailey and Mary
Reno, Reno, NV, USA Burch’s book, Ethics for Behavior Analysts
e-mail: patrickg@unr.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 497
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_26
498 P. M. Ghezzi and A. B. Lewon

(2016; see also Curry, 2013; McAllister, 1972: the mission statement, “in order to recommend
NARC, 1972). Following an investigation of the effective courses of action to deal with the legal,
residential treatment facility by Florida state offi- ethical, and professional issues raised by these
cials in 1972, the superintendent and several behavior-influencing procedures” (Stolz, 1978,
senior staff members faced civil and criminal p. xiv). As luck would have it, the most promi-
charges of cruel and unusual treatment of several nent “behavior-influencing procedure” at the
youngsters with developmental disabilities living time was the token economy, the ugly centerpiece
there at the time. The incident shocked the nation. of the “superb behavior modification program” at
To make matters worse, the superintendent the Sunland Miami Training Center.
characterized the Sunland Miami facility as a The APA Commission, chaired by Sidney
“superb behavior modification program” (Bailey Bijou, published a final report in 1978 that paid
& Burch, 2016, p.  7). Florida state officials special attention to the ethics of large-scale appli-
strongly disagreed, writing that the program, cations of the token economy (Stolz, 1978). The
which included random beatings, public nudity authors of the report referred to “thousands” of
and humiliation, physical restraint, and solitary undocumented token economies that surfaced in
confinement, was a “bizarre, abusive, and inef- the wake of Ayllon and Azrin’s legendary token
fective system of punishment (p. 2).” economy at Anna State Hospital in the 1960s and
The Sunland Miami program had no credible feared that most of the programs were operating
basis in ABA, concluded the authorities, and yet in state hospitals, nursing homes, detention cen-
reports began to surface from prisons, psychiat- ters, prisons, and schools for students with dis-
ric hospitals, and residential care and treatment abilities. The committee understood that the most
facilities across the nation accusing “behavior vulnerable people in society populate these
modifiers” of exploiting convicts, demeaning places, many too young or too old or weak to
psychiatric patients, mistreating elderly per- speak for themselves, many silenced by virtue of
sons, and abusing people with disabilities (Moya their incarceration or involuntary commitment,
& Achtenberg, 1974). Lawyers were prosecut- and each one unable to exert counter-control over
ing federal and state governments for civil rights a powerful and potentially coercive and oppres-
violations, and the courts began scrutinizing sive system such as a token economy.
therapeutic practices in prisons, psychiatric hos- Fortunately, a token economy is like a magnet
pitals, and other “total” institutions (Wexler, for exposing this type of trouble. As with most
1973). monetary economic systems, a token economy
On top of that, the mainstream media was vili- limits or restricts a person’s access to the things
fying behavior modification as dystopic and events the tokens can buy. Known technically
(Skinner’s Utopia, 1971), and the motion picture as a “motivational operation,” it is possible to
industry was animating the script with a deeply take matters to extreme and even inhumane and
disturbing parody of classical conditioning in deadly lengths, for example, by restricting an
Stanley Kubrick’s A Clockwork Orange. To top it inmate’s access to food, water, shelter, sleep,
all off, Vice President Agnew warned in a speech hygiene, social contact, and so on. The effects are
in 1972 that behavior modification “poses a dire conspicuous.
threat to traditional American values.” For a This, however, is not the only source of trou-
young science striving to gain a toehold in human ble that a token economy attracts. The inmate
services, things could get no worse. earns tokens for performing certain tasks in the
Against this backdrop, the American prison and exchanges the tokens for the things
Psychological Association (APA), led by Albert and events the inmate can afford to buy. The rela-
Bandura, intervened by forming a Commission tionship between tasks and tokens is laden with
on Behavior Modification in 1974. “The potential for gross inequities in workload and
Commission will focus on the area of applied earnings, and the relationship between earnings
behavior analysis in research and practice,” read and the price of the things and events that tokens
26  The Token Economy 499

can buy is equally susceptible to gross tion. “Winging it” seems to capture these
inequities. moments concisely.
Suppose the price for a hot dinner in the prison Design flaws, technical errors, conceptual
commissary is 30 tokens. The inmate works in shortcomings, and so forth are clear sources of
the prison laundromat 8 hours a day for a maxi- trouble for a token economy. Less apparent but
mum of 10 tokens a day, 5  days a week. The just as troublesome is the potential for harm cre-
inmate might earn less than 10 tokens a day and ated by making the mistake to introduce a token
might even lose tokens already earned during the economy in the first place.
day, for instance, for failing to meet the standards We suspect that many token economies in the
set for properly folded clothes. A guard inspects 1970s were adopted prematurely and probably
the inmate’s work periodically in this scenario unnecessarily, given the success of Ayllon and
and dispenses tokens based ostensibly on the Azrin’s token economy at Anna State Hospital in
quality of the inmate’s performance. It is safe to the 1960s. The trouble with introducing a token
say that under these conditions, the inmate would economy needlessly or too soon is that it departs
be eating many cold dinners. from the common practice in ABA to manage an
It is easy to imagine far greater injustices than individual’s behavior as naturally and unobtru-
a cold dinner. A token economy is actually sus- sively as possible. A token economy receives
ceptible to unspeakable abuse; to inconsistent, poor marks on these two dimensions, bringing it
haphazard, and arbitrary use; and for use as a closer to a “last resort” than to a “first resort”
cudgel for punishment, retribution, and revenge. intervention on the continuum of behavior man-
A token economy, under these circumstances, not agement interventions (BACB, 2020)
only spells trouble but can also breed cruelty, The upshot of poor marks on the natural and
contempt, and corruption. unobtrusive dimensions is another common prac-
It is reasonable to suppose, then, that a large tice in ABA; if a less contrived and less intrusive
number of early token economies failed on moral intervention fails to change behavior, then move
and legal grounds, as in the Sunland Miami case. up to an intervention with a comparably higher
It is also reasonable, furthermore, to suppose that level of contrivance and intrusiveness. A decision
many of them failed on technical and conceptual to move up this scale sets the occasion for yet
grounds, which was also the case at Sunland another common practice in ABA, which is to
Miami. It turns out that Florida state officials choose a course of action in the presence of data
found that the token economy at Sunland was in showing little or no change in a target behavior
disarray, that staff training was nonexistent, and over the course of a deliberate and systematic
that the people in charge of the facility simply did progression from the least-to-most intrusive and
not know how to manage behavior within the contrived interventions.
confines of token economy (McAllister, 1972). It In hindsight, it seems inevitable that many
seems safe to conclude that the abusive practices token economies would fail for technical and
at Sunland grew from this inadequacy. conceptual reasons. There were, after all, very
Fortunately, a token economy is like a magnet few colleges and universities with graduate train-
for this type of trouble, too. An inherent feature ing programs in behavior analysis at the time and
of the token economy is that it has many “moving in the era of the Sunland Miami scandal. There
parts” that require constant attention to keep the was no code of ethics, no regulatory controls, and
system running smoothly and in tune with the no professional organizations in behavior ana-
objectives set for a given individual. When the lysts to support the education and training of
parts fail, the intervention fails, and when it fails, future applied behavior analysts.
it is likely that the demands of the token economy In an effort to protect highly vulnerable peo-
exceeded the behavioral skills and abilities of the ple from harm by behavioral interventions such
people responsible for managing the interven- as a token economy, the APA Commission
500 P. M. Ghezzi and A. B. Lewon

strongly recommended that behavior analysts 26.2.1  S


 tar Charts, Point Systems,
adhere to the 1977 edition of APA’s Ethical and the Token Economy
Standards of Psychologists. The recommenda-
tion provided protections not only for the civil Star charts and point systems can be confused
rights of persons subject to behavioral interven- with a token economy and mistaken for ABA. The
tions and research, but it also gave the ABA com- confusion over these types of systems and a token
munity the cover it needed to begin solving the economy is understandable, given the similarities
legal, ethical, and professional problems that they share. Mistaking star charts and point sys-
scandals such as the Sunland Miami affair tems for ABA is a different matter, and under-
exposed and that the commission brought to light standing the difference both clarifies the meaning
in its report. These problems, in a nutshell, boiled of a token economy in the context of ABA and
down to poor academic preparation in the princi- underlines the importance of maintaining a dis-
ples of behavior, poor training in applying the tinction between a token economy and systems
principles of behavior, poor supervision over the that resemble a token economy.
practice of applying the principles of behavior, A token economy is a behavior management
and poor regulatory control over the behavior of intervention based on decades of experimental
applied behavior analysts (Johnstone et  al., research and field studies in the analysis of
2017). behavior. A properly credentialed behavior ana-
How the behavior analysis community lyst (1) selects the intervention for a given indi-
responded to these problems is beyond the scope vidual based on a comprehensive functional
of this chapter. It suffices to say that the group analytic assessment of the individual’s behavior
came together in the 1970s and began building and current circumstances, (2) manages the inter-
the culture and infrastructure necessary for vention with the competence and commitment to
behavior analysis to succeed as a scientific disci- follow the basic principles and best practices of
pline, as a legitimate profession, and as a leader ABA for the duration of the intervention, and (3)
in the human services community. The perma- monitors and evaluates the effects of the inter-
nent products of this continuing pursuit include vention regularly according to directly measur-
the Association for Behavior Analysis able changes in personally and socially
International (ABAI) and the Behavior Analyst meaningful target behavior(s) in the intervention
Certification Board (BACB). ABAI has been setting(s) and, to the maximum extent appropri-
accrediting graduate training programs in behav- ate, in the individual’s natural environment. A
ior analysis since 1993, and the BACB has been star chart or point system has none of these char-
credentialing behavior analysts since 1998. A acteristics, obviously.
license to practice ABA, furthermore, is now While this clearly disqualifies these systems
required in most states and provinces, and many as a token economy or ABA, it does not diminish
licensed behavior analysts working today main- the experience shared by millions of parents and
tain Professional Liability (Malpractice) teachers that a star chart or point system can be
Insurance for protection against complaints, an effective way to promote desirable behavior at
claims, and lawsuits (BACB, 2021). home and in the classroom with children and
Our motive in beginning this chapter with eth- youth. The key to this success is the same key to
ical matters is to be clear from the start that a successful token economy: Maintain a contin-
implementing a token economy, or for that mat- gent relation between responses and
ter, any ABA intervention without the proper reinforcements.
education, training, supervised experience, com- Consider the child who earns points for feed-
mitment, and in most states and providences, a ing the family dog. The youngster enjoys a fam-
license to practice as an applied behavior analyst, ily picnic at the neighborhood park and feeds the
is plainly unethical and rightly so. dog regularly to earn enough points to exchange
for the activity. The points by themselves are ini-
26  The Token Economy 501

tially neutral, but they acquire and maintain a economy in mind when it comes not only to
reinforcing function by virtue of their contingent selecting a token economy as a behavior manage-
relation to the things and events they buy, in this ment intervention but also to encouraging parents
instance, a picnic with the family at the park. to adopt a star or sticker chart.
Assuming the youngster’s parents invest in the We turn next to a discussion of nine key ele-
success of the system, they must commit them- ments in a token economy. Our purpose is not to
selves to maintaining the contingency they cre- review or critique the vast literature on the token
ated for their child. This means that feeding the economy, but instead to cite a few, mostly current
dog earns the child points toward a family picnic studies in support of a key element, and add our
at the park, but only so long as the child earns own practical experience now and then to aug-
enough points to exchange for the activity. It ment the scientific support. Keep in mind that we
means that the child does not go to the park for a discourage people from implementing a token
family picnic noncontingently, that is, without economy on any scale, large or small, without the
earning the correct number of points, and it also proper coursework, training, experience, and
means that the points themselves are not given credential(s) in ABA and that we encourage a
away freely or noncontingently, but are awarded functional analytic assessment of the person(s)
instead just for feeding the dog in this scenario. involved in the token economy before deciding to
A token economy, as with star charts and point build and manage one for them.
systems, takes advantage of a basic learning pro-
cess in nature, operant reinforcement. The simi-
larity ends there, however. The parent who 26.2.2  K
 ey Elements in a Token
awards a point on a ledger contingent on their Economy
child feeding the family dog is taking advantage
of operant reinforcement, but is not thereby prac- A list of the key elements in a token economy
ticing ABA or implementing a token economy appears in Table 26.1. We discuss each element in
per se. For the same reason, the parent who puts turn, offering suggestions along the way on how
a bandage over a small cut on their child’s finger to develop, maintain, troubleshoot, and fade a
is taking advantage of the healing process but is token economy. To reiterate a previous point, we
neither practicing medicine nor implementing a are more concerned with the token economy
medical procedure per se. itself rather than with applications of the inter-
We hasten to add that we have absolutely no vention to certain populations, ages, settings, and
antipathy toward star charts and point systems, so on.
only toward mistaking these systems as ABA and
confusing them with a token economy. Indeed,
we agree with the legions of pediatricians, educa- Table 26.1  Key elements in a token economy
tors, and child psychologists that star charts and Develop objectives and select relevant target responses
point systems can promote good behavior at with clarity and precision
home, in the classroom, and in the community. Measure the target behavior repeatedly, accurately,
In our work with young children and families, and reliably
we sometimes encourage parents to implement a Choose when, where, and with whom the token
economy will operate
simple star or sticker chart at home. We exercise
Pick out tokens
considerable caution in these cases, however. We Stockpile backup reinforcers
understand that parents tend to want to introduce Establish tokens as generalized conditioned reinforcers
these systems needlessly, that the systems are Specify the schedule of reinforcement
susceptible to inconsistent and haphazard use,   Token production schedule
and that they are vulnerable to abuse by strict dis-   Token exchange schedule
ciplinarians as an instrument of punishment. We   Exchange-production schedule
view these as warnings and urge practitioners to Decide when to exchange tokens
keep the early history of ABA and the token Phase out the token economy
502 P. M. Ghezzi and A. B. Lewon

26.2.3  Develop Objectives A clear, concise, and easy-to-follow descrip-


and Select Relevant Target tion of the target response(s) is essential to the
Responses with Clarity success of any ABA intervention, including a
and Precision token economy. A useful description includes
verbs and phrases that depict a person’s actions in
We distinguish between an intervention (or treat- real time. Given a learning goal to wake up in the
ment) objective and the response(s) that will lead morning at 6:30, actually waking up and getting
to achieving the objective for a given person. An out of bed at the appointed time is useful in that it
objective for a youngster in a token economy, for describes the target behavior (awaking, getting
example, is to be ready for school each morning. out of bed) in clear and unmistakable action
The responses that serve or accomplish this terms. Indeed, the whole point of describing
objective might include awaking earlier, groom- behavior as action is to obtain an objective,
ing faster, or dressing quicker. unvarnished description of the behavior, one that
Developing objectives and selecting responses anyone would be able to identify with perfect
relevant to accomplishing the objective is an indi- accuracy, at least in theory. Relying upon ambig-
vidualized, ongoing process guided by an over- uous or vague terms defeats this ideal and under-
riding concern for a person’s health, welfare, and mines the efficacy of the token economy (Moore
happiness. This concern translates into objectives et al., 2001).
and responses that (1) maximize a person’s Giving clear examples and non-examples of
access to contingencies of positive reinforcement the response(s), describing the full range of
and minimize exposure to aversive stimuli, (2) topographies included in the class, and delineat-
promote independence and autonomy over the ing strict rules for recording instances and/or
course of the intervention, (3) open new and pre- non-instances of the response(s) in the class
viously unavailable or restricted contingencies of accomplish the task. Keep in mind that classes
positive reinforcement, and (4) establish appro- that are defined too broadly may fail to capture a
priate replacements for dangerous and undesir- fine-grained but clinically significant change over
able behaviors. time and that classes that are defined too nar-
Engage the person from the start, if possible, rowly may fail to capture instances of behavior
in the process of developing objectives and that relate to accomplishing an objective, thereby
selecting responses with developmentally (and providing an incomplete picture of change over
culturally) appropriate methods and materials, as time (Johnston et al., 2020).
needed. Be mindful of the truism that learning
goals linked to personally important and socially
meaningful objectives keeps the “applied” and 26.2.4  Measure the Target
“analytic” dimensions of ABA in balance Behavior(s) Repeatedly,
throughout the intervention (Baer et  al., 1968; Accurately, and Reliably
Common & Lane, 2017; Leaf et al., 2016).
An important first step in creating important It is one thing simply to observe a response and
and meaningful behavior change is to begin iden- another thing to observe, record, and measure the
tifying instances of the responses that will accom- response repeatedly with a high degree of accu-
plish an objective. Some or all of these responses racy and reliability over extended periods. The
will constitute the “targets” of the intervention. outcome of the process is a measurement system
The challenge is to develop a class of target that both compliments an intervention objective
responses that is not only commensurate with a and captures the responses essential to accom-
given objective but also populated with a suffi- plishing the objective.
cient number of instances to which positive rein- Obtaining a measure of inter-observer agree-
forcement can be applied once the intervention ment (IOA) with respect to the occurrence and
begins in earnest. non-occurrence of a given target response is a
26  The Token Economy 503

long-standing practice in ABA (e.g., Johnston these indirect measures are susceptible to
et al., 2020; Page & Iwata, 1986). Bear in mind observer bias and may under- or overestimate the
that the practice of working toward and obtaining magnitude of the treatment effects. Thus, these
a high degree of IOA is not limited to research in measures supplement, not supplant, objective
ABA, but it also provides practitioners with accu- and direct measures of behavior (Cosper &
rate and reliable information that is necessary to Erickson, 1984; Reitman et al., 2004).
assessing the impact of an intervention over time.
It serves as the basis for modifying the parame-
ters of the token economy, it tracks maintenance 26.2.5  C
 hoose When, Where,
and generalization of behavior change in differ- and with Whom the Token
ent settings and circumstances, and it is Economy Will Operate
­indispensable when phasing-out a token econ-
omy and moving toward less contrived and intru- A person’s behavior is always a matter of time
sive contingencies. Measuring behavior often and place. It is critical, therefore, to be explicit
and monitoring progress frequently have an addi- regarding when, where, and with whom the token
tional benefit of keeping the behavior analyst economy will and will not operate. In some cases,
accountable to the stakeholders and clients they it might operate during all waking hours across
serve (Hawkins & Mathews, 1999). all persons and environments. In others, it may be
Behavior data collection systems are based on restricted to specific parts of the day or week
directly observable and objectively defined (e.g., morning, weekends), to certain activities
dimensions of the target behavior, facilitate data (e.g., evening routine, household chores), to cer-
collection with high IOA, and specify how often tain environments (e.g., stores, parks), or to cer-
data are graphically depicted, reviewed, and eval- tain people (e.g., parents, teachers) who deliver
uated. Behavior analysts design these systems and/or exchange tokens. These factors are indi-
with accuracy, reliability, and validity in mind, as vidualized in a token economy and tailored to the
even the most diligently designed and monitored objective(s) set for a given person. We might add
systems are subject to measurement error that all stakeholders, including the person(s) for
(Johnston et al., 2020). whom the token economy operates, receive
Continuous data collection methods (e.g., fre- instructions concerning the times, settings, and
quency, duration; see Table 4.1 in Cooper et al., circumstances under which the economy is
2020) that capture all occurrences of the target operational.
behavior constitute the gold standard in this
regard (Johnston et al., 2020). Interval recording
and other discontinuous data collection methods 26.2.6  Pick Out Tokens
fail to capture all instances of behavior during an
observation period and therefore provide a rough The “tokens” in a token economy are construed
estimate of the dimensions of the target behavior. as conditioned reinforcers in behavior theory, but
The data collected using discontinuous methods in practice, they function more like generalized
are interpreted with caution given the well-­ conditioned reinforcers (Hackenberg, 2009,
documented variation and measurement error 2018). We shall return to the distinction in a
inherent in these systems (Fiske & Delmolino, moment, but for now, the point is simply that a
2012; Meany-Daboul et al., 2007). token is like a coin in a traditional currency econ-
Subjective or retrospective measures, such as omy, something that someone earns and then
questionnaires where stakeholders report on their exchanges at a certain time and place for things
perceptions of acceptability and efficacy of the and events such as a new toy, a favorite snack, a
token economy, may be helpful in assessing the special outing, and so forth.
social validity of the intervention (Common & A conventional token is a physical possession,
Lane, 2017). Research has shown, however, that light and durable, inexpensive, easy to handle
504 P. M. Ghezzi and A. B. Lewon

and store, hard to deface or forge, and difficult to demands of the token economy, therefore, are
steal. Take steps, as needed, to reduce the poten- vital to achieving the objective(s) set for the
tial for stigma by making tokens as inconspicu- intervention.
ous as possible and by incorporating a person’s There are several well-documented assess-
preferences in the selection of the physical fea- ments available to identify preferred stimuli with
tures of the token(s). reinforcement potential in a contingent relation.
Research indicates that the physical features Individuals with sufficient language abilities can
of the token itself can interact with its functional provide input on likely backups, but bear in mind
properties. Studies conducted with children diag- that self-reported preferences do not always cor-
nosed with ASD, for example, show that incorpo- respond to actual preferences for children and
rating a child’s “perseverative interests” (e.g., adults (e.g., Northup et al., 1996). More formal-
cartoon figures) into a token’s design can aug- ized stimulus preference assessments and/or
ment the reinforcing effects of the token (Carnett caregiver interviews may be required if a person
et al., 2014; Charlop-Christy & Haymes, 1998). has difficulty verbally communicating their pref-
Visually stimulating tokens, according to erences (Piazza et al., 2011).
Hineline (2005), may have the added benefit of Preference assessments do not guarantee that
strengthening the social validity of the a given thing or event will serve a reinforcing
intervention. function, but instead identifies and reveals poten-
Research conducted in educational environ- tial reinforcements. Multiple stimulus without
ments suggests that digital tokens delivered over replacement (MSWO) preference assessments
online school communication platforms may (DeLeon & Iwata, 1996) receives high marks in
offer an effective alternative to physical tokens regard to selecting stimuli that are most likely to
(Horner et  al., 2018; Robacker et  al., 2016; function as reinforcers (Kang et al., 2013).
Williamson & McFadzen, 2020). This feature is The value of a backup changes as a function of
available on smartphones and tablets and could several well-researched variables. They include
become an attractive option in future applications (1) the level of deprivation or restriction of the
of token reinforcement systems. Bear in mind, backup (Ivy et al., 2015; Roane et al., 2005), (2)
however, that designing a collection of visually the effort required to obtain the backup (Reed
stimulating tokens is no replacement for well-­ et al., 2013), (3) the availability of other backups
researched procedures that establish a stimulus as (Foster & Hackenberg, 2004), and (4) the magni-
a conditioned or generalized conditioned tude, quality, and delay to the backup (Mace
reinforcer. et al., 1994; Neef et al., 1994). Evaluate prefer-
ences often, according to this extensive literature,
stay alert to the changing preferences of
26.2.7  Stockpile Backup individual(s), keep a fresh menu of backups
Reinforcements handy, and remember that the value of a token is
proportionate to the value of the backup(s) in a
Backup reinforcements or simply “backups” are token economy.
the preferred toys, items, activities, treats, privi-
leges, and so on that a person can buy in exchange
for the tokens in a token economy. The reinforc- 26.2.8  Establish Tokens
ing value of a token, in the technical sense of as Generalized Conditioned
actually strengthening the behavior on which it is Reinforcers
contingent, is proportional to the value of the
backup(s) with which the token is correlated A token in a token economy is defined in behav-
(Moher et al., 2008). Selecting well-established, ior analysis as a conditioned reinforcer, one that
ethically responsible backups and managing their has acquired the capacity to reinforce “due to its
availability according to the supplies and contingent relation to another reinforcer”
26  The Token Economy 505

(Catania, 1998, p. 391). The function of a token is individual exchanges a token for a backup
seldom restricted to a single reinforcer in prac- appears to accelerate the process of establishing
tice, however, but instead is related to multiple the token as conditioned generalized reinforcer
reinforcers. “Generalized conditioned reinforce- (Hackenberg, 2009, 2018).
ment” is the term given to “a conditioned rein-
forcer that is backed-up by many other sources of
reinforcement” (Pierce & Cheney, 2017, p. 513). 26.2.9  Specify the Schedules
These sources, in a token economy, are the back- of Reinforcement
ups, as previously discussed.
The main advantage of establishing a token as A token economy is composed of three interre-
a generalized conditioned reinforcer is that it lated schedules of reinforcement. The three
augments the value of the token, thereby creating schedules specify (1) the response requirement
a stimulus that is capable of reinforcing countless and conditions under which tokens are delivered,
responses so long as the contingent relations called the token production schedule; (2) the
remain in effect between backups, responses, and exchange rate, or the number of tokens needed to
tokens (Defulio et al., 2014; Russell et al., 2018; trade for backups, called the token exchange
Sran & Borrero, 2010). In a word, a generalized schedule; and (3) the conditions under which an
conditioned reinforcer is a versatile stimulus, one opportunity to exchange tokens for backups is
that can transcend most motivational operations, available, called the exchange-production sched-
settings, objectives, and individual ule (Hackenberg, 2009, 2018). We turn first to the
circumstances. token production schedule, then to the exchange-­
Two methods for establishing a token as a production schedule in the section developing
conditioned reinforcer have emerged in practice exchange rates, and finally to the token exchange
guidelines (Ivy et  al., 2017; Hackenberg, 2009, schedule.
2018). The most common method of the two The token production schedule specifies the
involves a written or spoken description of the contingency between tokens and responses.
value of the token with respect to the prevailing There are numerous options in this regard, includ-
schedule of token reinforcement, e.g., “When ing awarding a token after a fixed or variable
you earn three points, you can chose to play a number of responses (i.e., ratio schedules) or
video game” (Ivy et al., 2017). Incorporating this after the first response following a fixed or vari-
type of rule appears sufficient to establish a con- able amount of time (i.e., interval schedules; see
ditioned reinforcer for language-able people; Cooper et al., 2020). The token production sched-
however, the process is not well understood at ule also specifies the contingency between
this time (Moher et al., 2008; see also Hackenberg, responses and the number of tokens earned,
2009, 2018; Harte & Barnes-Holmes, 2021). which can range from small to large depending
The second method is “stimulus pairing” upon the criteria set for responses and reinforcer
whereby a token is closely associated in time and amounts. On that point, target responses that
space with a backup. Fashioned after the tradi- occur infrequently, that require considerable time
tional “S-S” procedure for establishing a condi- and effort, or that impact an objective in powerful
tioned reinforcer (Hendry, 1969), the protocol and consequential ways are reinforced most fre-
involves repeatedly delivering a token and a quently and most often with a large number of
backup contiguously with, and contingent on, an tokens relative to target responses without these
appropriate, low-effort, and highly probable exceptional qualities (Ghezzi et  al., 2008;
response. Once the response(s) is occurring at an Miltenberger, 2016).
acceptable level, delays to the time between earn- A great deal is known about schedules of rein-
ing a token and exchanging it for a backup are forcement and how different schedules affect the
systematically added until the desired delay is rate, pattern, and other characteristics of respond-
achieved. Incorporating a response whereby the ing (Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Hackenberg, 2009,
506 P. M. Ghezzi and A. B. Lewon

2018). Variable-ratio (VR) schedules often pro- tokens at any time, and a second option is a time-­
duce higher response rates than fixed-ratio (FR) based schedule, which restricts exchanging to
schedules, fixed-interval (FI) schedules, and certain days or times regardless of the number of
variable-­interval (VI) schedules (de Luca & tokens an individual earns (Ivy et al., 2017).
Holburn, 1990, 1992; Mazur, 1983), and individ- A meaningful difference between the two
uals tend to show a preference for VR over FR options is the time delay between awarding
and VI over FI schedules (Mazur, 2004; Repp & tokens and exchanging tokens for backups.
Deitz, 1975). Responses maintained by a rela- Response-based exchanges grant access to the
tively “lean” schedule typically show greater backup(s) the moment a person meets the
resistance to extinction compared to a denser response requirement, while time-based
schedule (Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Kazdin & exchanges add a delay to the backup(s). It is most
Polster, 1973), and a strong preference is usually helpful to know that a response-based schedule
seen for immediate over delayed reinforcers with a short delay to exchange is most appropri-
(Romani et al., 2017). These are just a few of the ate for young children with and without disabili-
many characteristics of token production sched- ties and for persons with limited language (e.g.,
ules, each one offering a great deal of flexibility Hendy et  al., 2005; Klimas & McLaughlin,
in terms of achieving a combination of responses 2007). Long delays to exchange can be difficult
and reinforcers that compliments the objective(s) even for older children and adults and can weaken
set for a given person. the positive effects of a token economy (Field
Managing the number of tokens in circulation et al., 2004; Moore et al., 2001). Indeed, research
is an important consideration when determining suggests that the frequency of exchanges is as
how the token production schedule interacts with vital to the success of a token economy as the
the token exchange and the exchange-production frequency of awarding tokens (Bullock &
schedules. Too many tokens can lead to accumu- Hackenberg, 2006; Webbe & Malagodi, 1978).
lation or “saving,” which can decrease the moti-
vation to earn more (Winkler, 1972). Too few
tokens in circulation can limit the number of 26.2.11  Select Exchange Rates
opportunities to exchange tokens for backup
reinforcers, thereby reducing the conditioned The exchange-production schedule specifies the
reinforcing value of the tokens themselves. The cost in tokens of the backup(s) and functions in
most effective token systems allow many oppor- tandem with the token production and exchange-­
tunities to earn and exchange tokens but arrange production schedules as a significant factor in
the contingencies to keep savings low or nonexis- determining response requirements in a token
tent (Hackenberg, 2009, 2018; Winkler, 1971). economy (Bullock & Hackenberg, 2006;
Resolving the issue of too many or too few tokens Hackenberg, 2009, 2018). A combination of a
in circulation may require adjustments to the thin token production schedule and relatively
token production schedule, or it may involve expensive backups, for example, can limit oppor-
making modifications to the token exchange and tunities to exchange tokens for backups. A dense
exchange-production schedules, described below. token production schedule together with rela-
tively inexpensive backups, in contrast, can lead
to excessive exchanges that may lower the value
26.2.10  Decide When to Exchange of the backup(s) and the tokens (Tarbox et  al.,
Tokens 2006; Ward-Horner et al., 2017).
Several exchange-production configurations
The exchange-production schedule specifies the are available for use. These include (1) a fixed
conditions under which tokens are exchanged for schedule, whereby a person exchanges their
backups. One option is a response-based sched- tokens after earning a given number of tokens;
ule, which stipulates that a person can exchange (2) a variable schedule, whereby a person
26  The Token Economy 507

exchanges their tokens after earning a variable (e.g., Ghezzi & Bishop, 2008; Ghezzi & Rogers,
number of tokens; and (3) a “menu” system, 2011).
whereby an assortment of concurrently available Consider schedule thinning, which is regarded
backups can be purchased in different amounts in ABA as an essential step toward maintaining
for different sums of tokens (Ivy et al., 2017). A the gains made during an intervention after the
token economy appears to operate most intervention is over (Cooper et  al., 2020).
­effectively when variables as opposed to fixed Thinning a schedule of reinforcement in a token
earning requirements are used and when the economy is complicated by the fact that there are
magnitude and quality of the backup(s) vary in three schedules to consider, the token production
cost (Becraft & Rolider, 2015; Cihon et al., 2019; schedule, the token exchange schedule, and the
Madden et  al., 2000; Sran & Borrero, 2010; exchange-production schedule, as previously
Whitney et al., 2018). described (see also Petursdottir & Ragnarsdottir,
Incorporating a written or pictorial “menu” 2019). Changes to the token production schedule
of backup reinforcers may be helpful when are possible by systematically increasing the
multiple backups are available or when manag- response requirement for token delivery and/or
ing the behavior of a person with a language incorporating an intermittent schedule of token
delay (Cooper et  al., 2020; Daley, 1969). A delivery (e.g., Christensen et al., 2004; LeBlanc
visual reminder of the cost of each backup is et  al., 2000). The token exchange schedule is
particularly important when the cost of the modified by gradually increasing the cost of
backups range from “most expensive” to “least backups, particularly for those that are highly
expensive” (Cooper et al., 2020; Ghezzi et al., preferred and those with no functional equivalent
2008). in the natural environment (e.g., Tarbox et  al.,
2006). The exchange-production schedule is
thinned slowly reducing the number of times
26.2.12  P
 hase Out the Token tokens are exchanged for backups (Kazdin,
Economy 1977).
While not specific to phasing out a token
The key elements in a token economy have little economy, there are several procedures available
in common with the naturally occurring contin- that we consider good candidates for this pur-
gencies of reinforcement in the everyday envi- pose. They include (1) incorporating a “level sys-
ronment, as we said before. The demand to tem” in the token economy wherein a person
transition from the conditions and contingencies passes through a hierarchy of tiered levels which
that manage the relevant target responses in a culminate in the termination of contrived contin-
token economy to those that manage the individ- gencies (e.g., Paul & Lentz, 1977; Pritchard
uals’ behavior under more natural and less con- et al., 2018), (2) reducing the number and type of
trived circumstances is greater in a token backups specific to the token economy and
economy compared to other ABA behavior man- replacing them with reinforcers available in the
agement interventions, as we also said before. A natural environment, and (3) establishing a self-­
plan to phase out the token economy, then, is on monitoring repertoire (McLaughlin & Malaby,
a par with the decision to develop and manage a 1975).
token economy to begin with.
It may come as a surprise to discover that the
scientific literature on ending a token economy is 26.3 Conclusion
sparse compared to the vast amount of informa-
tion on starting a token economy. What is avail- Kazdin (1978) credits Joseph Lancaster (1778–
able instead are well-worn recommendations on 1838) with developing the prototype of Ayllon
a range of topics, from promoting stimulus gen- and Azrin’s renowned token economy. A talented
eralization to increasing resistance to extinction promoter and successful businessperson,
508 P. M. Ghezzi and A. B. Lewon

Lancaster designed his “monitorial” system to ence, knowledge, and commitment of a veteran
meet the demand in the nineteenth-century applied behavior analyst.
England to educate the growing population of
poor children and youth at a time when teachers
were in short supply. The solution was simple: References
The older children (the “monitors”) would teach
the younger children in small groups under the Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
direction of an adult “master teacher.” The young Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 91–97.
monitors in Lancaster’s classroom earned merit https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-­91
badges (tokens) for their performance as teachers Bailey, J. S., & Burch, M. R. (2016). Ethics for behavior
and disciplinarians and could exchange their analysts (3rd ed.). Routledge.
Becraft, J. L., & Rolider, N. U. (2015). Reinforcer varia-
tokens (badges) for prizes such as a new toy, tion in a token economy. Behavioral Interventions,
game, book, writing materials, and the like. 30(2), 157–165. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1401
Lancaster advertised the monitorial system in Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2021, August
mostly commercial terms as an “economy of 30). U.S. licensure of behavior analysts. Behavior
Analyst Certification Board. https://www.bacb.
expense, efficiency of instruction, discipline by com/u-­s-­licensure-­of-­behavior-­analysts/.
routine, motivation by competition, and neutral- Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020). Ethics code
ity of religion” (Kaestle, 1973). Ayllon and Azrin, for behavior analysts. Authors.
in stark contrast, viewed the token economy in Bullock, C.  E., & Hackenberg, T.  D. (2006). Second
order schedules of token reinforcement with pigeons:
scientific terms as a “motivating environment Implications for unit price. Journal of Experimental
based upon reinforcement theory, specifically Analysis of Behavior, 85(1), 95–106. https://doi.
operant reinforcement theory” (1968, p. 4). This org/10.1901/jeab.2006.116-­04
change in purpose, from commercial success to Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Prentice Hall.
Carnett, A., Raulston, T., Lang, R., Tostanoski, A., Lee,
applied behavior science, is a major turning point A., Sigafoos, J., & Machalicek, W. (2014). Effects of
in the history of ABA. The token economy played perseverative interest-based token economy on chal-
the leading role, either as the hero or as the vil- lenging and on-task behavior in a child with autism.
lain, depending on your point of view. As the Journal of Behavioral Education, 23, 368–377. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­014-­9195-­7
hero, the token economy exposed the incompe- Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1998). Using
tence and systemic malfeasance to many total objects of obsession as token reinforcers for children
institutions in the USA and abroad in the 1960s; with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
as the villain, it provided the means and pretext Disorders, 28(3), 189–198. https://doi.org/10.102
3/A:102061220171
for unscrupulous people to violate the civil rights Christensen, L., Young, K.  R., & Merchant, M. (2004).
of persons living in total institutions at the time. The effects of peer-mediated positive behavior support
The token economy is understood in ABA program on socially appropriate classroom behavior.
today as one of many behavior management Education and Treatment of Children, 27, 199–234.
Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., Milne, C.  M., Leaf, J.  B.,
interventions. It is a demanding intervention, one McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2019). A preliminary evalu-
that requires a great deal of preparation, plan- ation of a token system with a flexible earning require-
ning, and daily management, as our nine key ele- ment. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12(3), 548–556.
ments show, and one that can be difficult to fade https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­00316-­3
Common, E. A., & Lane, K. L. (2017). Social validity assess-
without a plan in place from the start. These fea- ment. In J. K. Luiselli (Ed.), Applied behavior analysis
tures of a token economy leads practitioners to advanced guidebook: A manual for p­ rofessional prac-
evaluate less intrusive and more natural proce- tice (pp. 73–92). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.
dures before turning to the intervention to accom- org/10.1016/B978-­0-­12-­811122-­2.00004-­8
Cooper, J.  O., Heron, T.  E., & Heward, W.  L. (2020).
plish a given objective for a given person. It is a Applied behavior analysis (3rd ed.). Pearson.
course of action in which “minimally invasive” Cosper, M. R., & Erickson, M. T. (1984). Relationships
interventions are favored over interventions such among observed classroom behavior and three types of
as a token economy that require not only a sig- teacher ratings. Behavioral Disorders, 9(3), 189–195.
nificant amount of time and effort but also experi-
26  The Token Economy 509

Curry, K. (2013). The Sunland Training Centers: Florida’s and managing a token economy. In W. T. O’Donohue
institutes for children with intellectual disabilities. & J.  E. Fisher (Eds.), Cognitive behavior therapy:
Florida Public Health Review, 10, 14–24. Applying empirically supported techniques in your
Daley, M. F. (1969). The “reinforcement menu”: Finding practice (pp. 565–671). John Wiley & Sons.
effective reinforcers. In J.  D. Krumboltz & C.  E. Hackenberg, T. D. (2009). Token reinforcement: A review
Thorsen (Eds.), Behavioral counseling: Cases and and analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
techniques (pp. 42–45). Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. of Behavior, 91(2), 257–286. https://doi.org/10.1901/
DeFulio, A., Yankelevitz, R., Bullock, C., & Hackenberg, jeab.2009.91-­257
T.  D. (2014). Generalized conditioned reinforce- Hackenberg, T.  D. (2018). Token reinforcement:
ment with pigeons in a token economy. Journal of Translational research and application. Journal of
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 102(1), 26–46. Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(2), 393–435. https://
https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.94 doi.org/10.1002/jaba.439
DeLeon, I.  G., & Iwata, B.  A. (1996). Evaluation of a Hawkins, R. P., & Mathews, J. R. (1999). Frequent moni-
multiple stimulus presentation format for assessing toring of clinical outcomes: Research and accountabil-
reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior ity for clinical practice. Education and Treatment of
Analysis, 29(4), 519–533. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Children, 22(2), 117–135.
jaba.1996.29-­519 Harte, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2021). The status of
de Luca, R. V., & Holburn, S. W. (1990). Effects of fixed rule-governed behavior as pliance, tracking, and aug-
interval and fixed ratio schedules of token reinforce- menting within relational frame theory: Middle-level
ment on exercise with obese and nonobese boys. The rather then technical terms. The Psychological Record.
Psychological Record, 40(1), 67–82. https://doi.org/10.1007?s40732-021-00458-x
de Luca, R.  V., & Holburn, S.  W. (1992). Effects of a Hendy, H. M., Williams, K. E., & Camise, T. S. (2005).
variable-­ratio reinforcement schedule with chang- “Kids choice” school lunch program increases chil-
ing criteria on exercise in obese and nonobese boys. dren’s fruit and vegetable acceptance. Appetite, 45(3),
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(3), 671– 250–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.07.006
679. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­671 Hendry, D. P. (1969). Conditioned reinforcement. Dorsey
Reed, F.D.G, & Codding, R.S. (2011). Intervention integ- Press.
rity assessment. In J.K. Luiselli (Ed.), Teaching and Hineline, P.  N. (2005). The aesthetics of behavioral
Behavior Support for Children and Adults with Autism arrangements. The Behavior Analyst, 28(1), 15–28.
Spectrum Disorder, (pp. 38–47). New York: Oxford. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392101
Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of rein- Horner, R., Hew, K., & Tan, C. (2018). Comparing digi-
forcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts. tal badges-and-points with classroom token systems:
Field, C. E., Nash, H. M., Handwerk, M. L., & Friman, Effects on elementary school ESL students’ class-
P. C. (2004). A modification of the token economy for room behavior and English learning. Educational
nonresponsive youth in family-style residential care. Technology & Society, 21(1), 137–151.
Behavior Modification, 28(3), 438–457. https://doi. Ivy, J. W., Neef, N. A., Meindl, J. N., & Miller, N. (2015).
org/10.1177/014544503258995 A preliminary examination of motivating operation and
Fiske, K., & Delmolino, L. (2012). Use of discontinuous reinforcer class interaction. Behavioral Interventions,
methods of data collection in behavioral interven- 31(2), 180–194. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1436
tion: Guidelines for practitioners. Behavior Analysis Ivy, J. W., Meindl, J. N., Overley, E., & Robson, K. M.
in Practice, 5(2), 77–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/ (2017). Token economy: A systematic review of pro-
BF03391826 cedural descriptions. Behavior Modification, 41(5),
Foster, T. A., & Hackenberg, T. D. (2004). Unit price and 708–737. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517699559
choice in a token-reinforcement context. Journal of Johnstone, J. M., Carr, J. E., & Mellichamp, F. H. (2017).
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81(1), 5–25. A history of the professional credentialing of applied
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2004.81-­5 behavior analysts. The Behavior Analyst, 40, 523–538.
Ghezzi, P.  M., & Bishop, M.  R. (2008). Generalized Johnston, J. M., Pennypacker, H. S., & Green, G. (2020).
behavior change in young children with autism. In Strategies and tactics of behavioral research and
J. K. Luiselli, D. C. Russon, W. P. Christian, & S. M. practice (4th ed.). Routledge.
Wilcynzski (Eds.), Effective practices for children Kaestle, C.  F. (1973). Joseph Lancaster and the moni-
with autism: Educational and behavior support inter- torial school movement: A documentary history.
ventions that work (pp. 137–158). Oxford University Teachers College Press.
Press. Kang, S., O’Reilly, M., Lancioni, G., Falcomata, T.  S.,
Ghezzi, P. M., & Rogers, V. R. (2011). Promoting general- Sigafoos, J., & Xu, Z. (2013). Comparison of the
ization. In J. K. Luiselli (Ed.), Teaching and behavior predictive validity and consistency among preference
support for children and adults with autism spectrum assessment procedures: A review of the literature.
disorders: A “how to” practioner’s guide (pp.  179– Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(4), 1125–
186). Oxford University Press. 1133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.12.021
Ghezzi, P.  M., Wilson, G.  R., Tarbox, R.  S. F., & Kazdin, A. E. (1977). The token economy. Springer.
MacAleese, K.  R. (2008). Guidelines for developing
510 P. M. Ghezzi and A. B. Lewon

Kazdin, A.  E. (1978). History of behavior modification: of Behavioural Science, 7(4), 411–419. https://doi.
Experimental foundations of contemporary research. org/10.1037/h0081925
University Park Press. Meany-Daboul, M. G., Roscoe, E. M., Bourret, J. C., &
Kazdin, A.  E., & Polster, R. (1973). Intermittent token Ahearn, W.  H. (2007). A comparison of momentary
reinforcement and response maintenance in extinc- time sampling and partial-interval recording for evalu-
tion. Behavior Therapy, 4(3), 286–391. https://doi. ating functional relations. Journal of Applied Behavior
org/10.1016/S0005-­7894(73)80118-­2 Analysis, 40(3), 501–514. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Klimas, A., & McLaughlin, T.  F. (2007). The effects of jaba.2007.40-­501
a token economy system to improve social and aca- Miltenberger, R.  G. (2016). Behavior modification:
demic behavior with rural primary aged child with dis- Principles and procedures (6th ed.). Cengage
abilities. International Journal of Special Education, Learning.
22(3), 72–77. Moher, C.  A., Gould, D., Hegg, E., & Mahoney, A.  M.
Kubrick, S. (Director). (1971). A clockwork orange (2008). Non-generalized and generalized conditioned
[Film]. Polaris Productions. reinforcers: Establishment and validation. Behavioral
LeBlanc, L.  A., Hagopian, L.  P., & Maglieri, K.  A. Interventions, 23(1), 13–38. https://doi.org/10.1002/
(2000). Use of a token economy to eliminate exces- bin.253
sive inappropriate social behavior in an adult with Moore, J. W., Tingstrom, D. H., Doggett, A., & Carlyon,
developmental disabilities. Behavioral Interventions, W.  D. (2001). Restructuring an existing token econ-
15(2), 135–143. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-­ omy in a psychiatric facility for children. Child and
078X(200004/0615:2<135::AID-­BIN51>3.0.CO;2-­3 Family Behavior Therapy, 23(3), 51–57. https://doi.
Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M., org/10.1300/j019v23n03_04
Ala’i-Rosales, S., Ross, R.  K., Smith, T., & Weiss, Moya, C. B., & Achtenberg, R. (1974). Behavior modifi-
M.  J. (2016). Applied behavior analysis is a science cation: Legal limitations on methods and goals. Notre
and, therefore, progressive. Journal of Autism and Dame Law Review, 50(2), 230–250.
Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 720–731. https://doi. National Association for Retarded Citizens, The Record,
org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2591-­6 Fall, (1972).
Lewon, A. B., & Ghezzi, P. M. (2020). An evaluation of Neef, N. A., Shade, D., & Miller, M. S. (1994). Assessing
the Early Learning Measure as a predictor of outcomes influential dimensions of reinforcers on choice in stu-
in early intensive behavioral intervention. Behavioral dents with serious emotional disturbance. Journal of
Interventions, 36, 388–406. Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(4), 575–583. https://
Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor- doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­575
mal educational and intellectual functioning in Northup, J., George, T., Jones, K., Broussard, C., &
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting Vollmer, T.  R. (1996). A comparison of reinforcer
and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi. assessment methods: The utility of verbal and picto-
org/10.1037/0022-­006X.55.1.3 rial choice procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior
Mace, F.  C., Neef, N.  A., Shade, D., & Mauro, B.  C. Analysis, 29(2), 201–212. https://doi.org/10.1901/
(1994). Limited matching on concurrent-schedule jaba.29-­201
reinforcement of academic behavior. Journal of Page, T. J., & Iwata, B. A. (1986). Interobserver agreement.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(4), 585–596. https:// In A. Poling & R. W. Fuqua (Eds.), Research methods
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­585 in applied behavior analysis (pp.  99–126). Springer.
Madden, G.  J., Bickel, W.  K., & Jacobs, E.  A. (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-­1-­4684-­8786-­2_6
Three predictions of the economic concept of unit Paul, G.  L., & Lentz, R.  J. (1977). Psychosocial treat-
price in a choice context. Journal of the Experimental ment of chronic mental patients: Milieu versus social-­
Analysis of Behavior, 73(1), 45–64. https://doi. learning programs. Harvard University Press.
org/10.1901/jeab.2000.73-­45 Petursdottir, A.  L., & Ragnarsdottir, G.  B. (2019).
Mazur, J. E. (1983). Steady-state performance on fixed-, Decreasing student behavior problems, fostering
mixed-, and random-ratio schedules. Journal of the academic engagement through function-based sup-
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39(2), 293–307. port, and fading of token reinforcement. Behavioral
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1983.39-­293 Interventions, 34(3), 323–337. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Mazur, J. E. (2004). Risky choice: Selecting between cer- bin.1670
tain and uncertain outcomes. The Behavior Analyst Piazza, C.  C., Roane, H.  S., & Karsten, A. (2011).
Today, 5(2), 190–203. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Identifying and enhancing the effectiveness of positive
h0100031 reinforcement. In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, & H. S.
McAllister, J.  W. (1972). Report of resident abuse Roane (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis
investigating committee. Division of Retardation, (pp. 151–164). The Guilford Press.
Department of Health and Rehabilitative Services. Pierce, W. D., & Cheney, C. D. (2017). Behavior analy-
McLaughlin, T. F., & Malaby, J. E. (1975). The effects of sis and learning: A biobehavioral approach (6th ed.).
various token reinforcement contingencies on assign- Routledge.
ment completion and accuracy during variable and Pritchard, D., Penney, H., & Mace, F.  C. (2018). The
fixed token exchange schedules. Canadian Journal ACIEVE! Program: A point and level system for
26  The Token Economy 511

reducing severe problem behavior. Behavioral Stolz, S. B. (1978). Ethical issues in behavior modifica-
Interventions, 33(1), 41–55. https://doi.org/10.1002/ tion. Jossey-Bass Publishers.
bin.1506 Tarbox, R. S. F., Ghezzi, P. M., & Wilson, G. (2006). The
Reed, F.D.G, & Codding, R.S. (2011). Intervention integ- effects of token reinforcement on attending in a young
rity assessment. In J.K. Luiselli (Ed.), Teaching and child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 21(3),
Behavior Support for Children and Adults with Autism 155–164. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.213
Spectrum Disorder (pp. 38–47). New York: Oxford. Time Magazine. (1971, September 20). Behavior:
Reed, D. D., Niileksela, C. R., & Kaplan, B. A. (2013). Skinner’s utopia: Panacea, or path to hell? Time
Behavioral economics: A tutorial for behavior ana- Magazine. http://content.time.com/time/subscriber/
lysts in practice. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 6(1), article/0,33009,909994,00.html
34–54. Webbe, F.  M., & Malagodi, E.  F. (1978). Second-order
Reitman, D., Hupp, S.  D. A., O’Callaghan, P.  M., & schedules of token reinforcement: Comparisons of
Murphy, M.  A. (2004). Behavior change and per- performance under fixed-ratio and variable-ratio
ceptions of change: Evaluating the effectiveness of a exchange schedules. Journal of the Experimental
token economy. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, Analysis of Behavior, 30(2), 219–224. https://doi.
26(2), 17–36. https://doi.org/10.1300/J019v26n02_02 org/10.1901/jeab.1978.30-­219
Repp, A.  C., & Deitz, S.  M. (1975). A comparison of Ward-Horner, J. C., Cengher, M., Ross, R. K., & Fienup,
fixed-ratio and variable-ratio token-production sched- D.  M. (2017). Arranging response requirements and
ules with human subjects. The Psychological Record, the distribution of reinforcers: A brief review of pref-
25, 131–137. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394296 erence and performance outcomes. Journal of Applied
Roane, H. S., Call, N. A., & Falcomata, T. S. (2005). A pre- Behavior Analysis, 50(1), 181–185. https://doi.
liminary analysis of adaptive responding under open org/10.1002/jaba.350
and closed economies. Journal of Applied Behavior Wexler, D.  B. (1973). Token and taboo: Behavior
Analysis, 38(3), 335–348. https://doi.org/10.1901/ modification, token economies, and the law.
jaba.2005.85-­04 California Law Review, 61(1), 81–109. https://doi.
Robacker, C. M., Rivera, C. J., & Warren, S. H. (2016). org/10.2307/3479877
A token economy made easy through ClassDojo. Whitney, T., Cooper, J. T., & Lingo, A. S. (2018). Using
Intervention in School and Clinic, 52(1), 39–43. a token economy combined with a mystery motiva-
https://doi.org/10.1177/1053451216630279 tor for a student with autism exhibiting challenging
Romani, P.  W., Alcorn, A.  S., Miller, J.  R., & Clark, G. behavior. Kentucky Teacher Education Journal, 5(2),
(2017). Preference assessment for dimensions of rein- 1–23.
forcement to inform token economies targeting prob- Williamson, R.  L., & McFadzen, C. (2020). Evaluating
lem behavior. Journal of Behavioral Education, 26, the impact of token economy methods on student on-­
221–237. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­017-­9270-­y task behavior with an inclusive Canadian classroom.
Russell, D., Ingvarsson, E. T., Haggar, J. L., & Jessel, J. International Journal of Technology and Inclusive
(2018). Using progressive ratio schedules to evaluate Education, 9(1), 1531–1541.
tokens as generalized conditioned reinforcers. Journal Winkler, R.  C. (1971). Reinforcement schedules for
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(1), 40–52. https:// individual patients in a token economy. Behavior
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.424 Therapy, 2(4), 534–537. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Sran, S. K., & Borrero, J. C. (2010). Assessing the value of S0005-­7894(71)80099-­0
choice in a token system. Journal of Applied Behavior Winkler, R. C. (1972). A theory of equilibrium in token
Analysis, 43(3), 553–557. https://doi.org/10.1901/ economies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 79(2),
jaba.2010.43-­553 169–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032722
Activity Schedules
and Script-­Fading Procedures: Key 27
Curricula for Teaching People
with Autism Independence
and Social Interaction Skills

Amanda S. Freeman, Christine M. Fry,
and Gregory S. MacDuff

27.1 Activity Schedules prompts, and increased the levels of sustained


engagement or on-task behavior (Duttlinger
According to McClannahan and Krantz (1999, et al., 2013; Kelley et al., 2013; Mechling et al.,
2010), an activity schedule is a set of pictures or 2010; Purrazzella & Mechling, 2013). In fact, in
words that cue someone to engage in a sequence a review of 28 research articles that used activity
of activities. A more extended definition was pro- schedules, Koyoma and Wang (2011) stated that
vided by Hugh et al. (2018), when they described an increase in engagement or on-task behavior
visual activity schedules (VAS) as “an evidence-­ was the most frequently cited outcome from stud-
based type of visual support that provide sequen- ies investigating the effects of activity schedules.
tial organization of the steps for an activity or Other outcomes posited by these authors related
skill” (p.  4). Although the use of visual cues/ to an increase in the levels of independence
activity schedules to evoke sustained engagement included improvements in self-scheduling and
and the completion of target response chains has task initiation following instruction in the use of
a long-standing and well-established place in the activity schedules.
applied behavior analysis literature (Bryan & With the ongoing use of activity schedules as
Gast, 2000; Horton & Taylor, 1989; MacDuff a means of promoting the acquisition and gener-
et  al., 1993; Massey & Wheeler, 2000; Pierce alization of a wide array of target responses, sev-
et  al., 2013), what is considered the best eral comprehensive reviews of the effects of
practice? activity schedules have been published. Some
A primary function of activity schedules is to reviews have focused on the evolution of sched-
increase the levels of independence/unprompted ule formats from notebooks containing photo-
behavior (Johnson et  al., 2016). In this respect, graphs to technological devices (e.g., iPad,
multiple authors have posited that the presence of iPhone, smart phone, Samsung Galaxy Tab),
visual cues displayed within an activity schedule while others have analyzed the effectiveness of
has reduced learners’ reliance on caregivers, activity schedules as a means of teaching social
increased the number of tasks completed without skills, increasing on-task and on-schedule behav-
ior, and promoting higher levels of independence.
A. S. Freeman · C. M. Fry · G. S. MacDuff (*) For example, in a 2006 review of the activity
Princeton Child Development Institute, schedule literature, Stromer and colleagues
Princeton, NJ, USA reported that activity schedules could be used to
e-mail: gmacduff@pcdi.org

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 513
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_27
514 A. S. Freeman et al.

teach generative and functional skills (e.g., social other developmental disorders including severe
initiations, independent play, and academic skills intellectual disabilities and pervasive develop-
such as reading, spelling, and math). They con- mental disorders. These authors also reported
cluded that a combination of computer-based and that 88% of the studies reviewed reported the use
notebook schedules provided an appropriate of photographs or video-enhanced activity sched-
framework for the acquisition of social, play, and ules and 12% described written activity sched-
communication skills for children with autism ules—72% of the investigations that used
spectrum disorder (ASD). photographs presented these materials in a book
Still, other researchers have examined the format. In contrast, 18% of activity schedules
effects of activity schedules on the reduction of were presented in technological formats includ-
problem behavior. Banda and Grimmett (2008) ing, but not limited to, Dell and Microsoft
reviewed 13 studies that investigated the use of PowerPoint, a Windows touch-screen computer,
activity schedules as a means of enhancing social and an iPod Touch.
behavior and decreasing occurrences of problem
behavior. A total of 28 children and 3 adults with
autism served as participants in the reviewed 27.1.1 Mode of Presentation
publications, and the format of the schedules var-
ied (e.g., photographic, video models, textual). Activity schedules consisting of photographs,
The authors reported that, regardless of the for- line drawings, and/or words have all been found
mat of the activity schedule, all 31 participants to be effective in increasing the levels of engage-
showed improvement in the dependent variables ment and promoting the independent completion
measured (e.g., social interactions, transitions, of tasks and activities (Knight et al., 2015; Lequia
on-task behavior, decreased occurrences of prob- et  al., 2012). Kimball et  al. (2004) noted that
lem behavior) following the introduction of activ- “activity schedules have evolved in terms of (a)
ity schedules. the ever more sophisticated media for their deliv-
In an additional summary of the literature per- ery and (b) the ever-broadening range of indepen-
taining to the effects of activity schedules on dent skills they may support” (p. 283). Whereas
challenging behavior, Lequia and colleagues activity schedules were originally designed to
(2011) analyzed 18 studies that included the use cue learners to execute previously acquired skills
of activity schedules. Although they concluded (Stromer et al., 2006) over time, activity sched-
that the acquisition of schedule-following behav- ules have, in some instances, become more tech-
ior resulted in a reduction in problem behavior, nologically elaborate and have evolved as a
they suggested that other variables such as sever- means of expanding the existing repertoires of
ity of diagnosis, communication skills, and set- children and adults with developmental disabili-
ting were also likely to have influenced the extent ties across an array of response classes. For
of the reduction. example, in an effort to present an activity sched-
In 2018, Koos and MacDuff reviewed 34 pub- ule in a more socially acceptable format, Carlile
lished studies that met the definition of activity et  al. (2013) presented activity schedules on an
schedules as posited by McClannahan and Krantz iPod to teach four 8- to 12-year-old boys with
(2010). Unlike previous reviews that summarized autism to engage in age-appropriate leisure skills.
the literature over more limited time spans, these In addition to enhancing the boys’ levels of
authors examined the literature on activity sched- engagement, acquired skills generalized to novel
ules and other visual cues over a 40 year period settings and novel activity schedules and main-
(i.e., the earliest paper included was published in tained over time after instruction had ceased.
1977). Most studies (i.e., 76%) examined the use Cihak (2011) compared the effects of static-­
of activity schedules with children—the majority picture schedules (i.e., digital photographs of par-
of whom were diagnosed with ASD (i.e., 53%). ticipants engaging in activities) and video clips
The remaining participants were diagnosed with that showed learners engaged in activities and
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 515

independently moving from one task to another book- and tablet-based activity schedules—the
(i.e., video models) on the number of independent third participant required additional training ses-
transitions that were completed by four pre-teens sions to reach a criterion level of performance
with autism. Results showed that two of four par- across the two modalities. According to the
ticipants emitted more independent transitions authors, there was no functional difference in the
using video schedules, one participant performed rate of acquisition across the two modalities.
better using the static-picture schedule, and the Giles and Markham (2017) also used a
final participant’s performance was equivalent concurrent-­ chains preference assessment to
across the two formats. Cihak stated that some determine each child’s preferred activity sched-
participants “demonstrated a preference (via bet- ule modality. Although it was not uncommon for
ter performance) with one particular intervention participants to fluctuate in their choice of modali-
over another” (p.  439). According to the author, ties, two participants selected the tablet-based
the results obtained from this study “should schedule more often, and the remaining partici-
heighten educator’s awareness of the need to be pant the book-based schedule. Importantly,
flexible and to incorporate a range of research- according to the authors, the modality preferred
based practices to address individual problems” by a given participant did not necessarily match
(Cihak, 2011, p. 439). the modality that produced more rapidly acquisi-
Does the literature suggest a particular activity tion for that participant.
schedule format as the best practice or as demon- Although some researchers have posited the
strating an advantage over other formats? Reinert possibility of social stigmatization and lack of
et  al. (2020) acknowledged the outcomes pro- mobility as potential deficits of book-based activ-
duced by photographic schedules (e.g., increased ity schedules (Carlile et al., 2013; Stromer et al.,
levels of engagement) but proposed that book-­ 2006), both traditional book-based and technology-­
formatted activity schedules may be cumbersome based activity schedules have been effective in
to manipulate and socially stigmatizing. In citing promoting the acquisition of schedule-­following
potential advantages of digital activity schedules, skills and an increase in the levels of engagement
the authors included that technology-based (Cihak, 2011; Goldsmith & Le Blanc, 2004;
schedules (a) can be shared across devices, (b) Moore & Calvert, 2000; Williams et al., 2002).
reduce potential stigmatization because tablet In 2004, Rehfeldt and colleagues stated “A
computers are pervasive in most settings, and (c) proficient schedule user is an individual whose
lend themselves to the re-sequencing of tasks completion of, and transition between activities
within the schedule. However, these researchers is occasioned by the visual cues presented in his
cautioned that when selecting a schedule modal- or her schedule” (p.  115) and advocated that
ity, instructors and caregivers must also realize learners who are new to activity schedules might
that technology-based activity schedules use benefit from instruction using a “standard note-
equipment that is more expensive than the mate- book schedule” (p.  116) before proceeding to a
rials used in traditional activity schedules and computer-based activity schedule. With these
that these systems require some level of techno- recommendations in mind, because both book-
logical expertise to design, use, and modify. and technology-based activity schedules have
In a comparative study, Giles and Markham been demonstrated to produce desired outcomes,
(2017) compared acquisition rates when partici- perhaps the “best practice” for selecting the most
pants used book- and tablet-based activity sched- appropriate modality for activity schedules
ules and assessed which of the modalities should include exposure to both standard note-
participants preferred to use. Three preschool book schedules and technology-based schedules
boys with ASD served as participants. During so that learners’ levels of on-task and acquisition
instruction, two participants showed a gradual of on-schedule behavior can be used to determine
increase in the percentage of schedule compo- which type of format represents the best practice
nents completed without assistance using both for each individual.
516 A. S. Freeman et al.

27.1.2 Prerequisite Skills Similarly, Kimball et  al. (2004) linked the suc-
cessful acquisition of schedule-following behav-
Rehfeldt’s (2002) review of McClannahan and ior to (a) the selection of tasks that children could
Krantz’s (1999) book, Activity Schedules for competently complete, (b) the consistent imple-
Children with Autism: Teaching Independent mentation of graduated guidance, and (c) an
Behavior, identified figure-background discrimi- ample schedule of reinforcement for schedule
nation, identity matching, and matching three-­ completion.
dimensional objects to their two-dimensional When instructors/caregivers are ready to begin
representations as prerequisites to learners begin- teaching, McClannahan and Krantz (2010) sug-
ning the use of and/or benefiting from an activity gested the use of an initial instruction that is gen-
schedule. Rehfeldt went on to suggest that com- eral in nature to evoke schedule-following
pliance with adult instructions and attending behavior (e.g., “Time for chores,” “Go play,”
behavior (e.g., visually attending to the task at “Please find something to do”). Following the
hand) was also an important prerequisite to the initial instruction, McClannahan and Krantz sug-
acquisition of schedule-following behavior. gested that graduated guidance be used to evoke
Interestingly, in their review of the literature, schedule-following behavior and task completion
Koos and MacDuff (2018) reported that of 34 and that further verbal instructions, gestural
reviewed studies, only 10 (29%) explicitly stated prompts, and models be avoided. In their book,
that picture-object correspondence or other pre- Activity Schedules for Children with Autism:
requisite skills had been established prior to the Teaching Independent Behavior, McClannahan
introduction of an activity schedule. and Krantz (1999) also advocated the use of
Accepting manual guidance was also consid- graduated guidance and advised against the use
ered prerequisite to the acquisition of schedule-­ of least-to-most prompt hierarchies when teach-
following skills by McClannahan and Krantz ing children with autism to complete activity
(1999, 2010) who stated, “The procedures we use schedules “because it permits them to make many
to teach children to follow activity schedules errors. After errors occur, they are likely to be
emphasize manual guidance. If we are to accom- repeated” (p. 38).
plish this teaching, children must permit us to
touch their hands, arms, and shoulders, and must
allow us to guide them” (p. 17). 27.1.4 Selection of Prompt
and Prompt-Fading Strategies

27.1.3 Implementation of an Activity Several researchers (Betz et  al., 2008; Bryan &
Schedule Gast, 2000; MacDuff et al., 1993; McClannahan
& Krantz, 1999; Spriggs et  al., 2007; Torres
Spriggs et  al. (2015) stated, “Consistency in et al., 2018) advocated the use of or used gradu-
schedule implementation is critical for the sched- ated guidance to teach schedule-following behav-
ule to work in increasing desired behaviors as ior and encouraged that these prompts be
well as decreasing undesired behaviors” (p. 30). delivered from behind participants to decrease
According to this publication, the most important the salience of the instructor and to promote the
factors related to the successful implementation activity schedule as a discriminative stimulus.
of an activity schedule is to establish a routine Interestingly, numerous other researchers
based on (a) the type of schedule selected, (b) the have selected prompt and prompt-fading strate-
environments where the schedule will be used, gies other than graduated guidance to promote
(c) and practical aspects of the schedule such as acquisition of targeted dependent variables and
how learners will manipulate schedule materials schedule-following behavior. For example,
to indicate that a task or component of a task has Whatley et al. (2009) successfully increased the
been completed (e.g., turn the page, mark a box). number of independent transitions completed by
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 517

four students with moderate autism using a least-­ of prompts, the influence of supervision and its
to-­most hierarchy of prompts. In another experi- effect on learner’s engagement is well docu-
ment, Carlile et  al. (2013) used a progressive mented. Dunlap and Plienis (1988) stated that
time-delay prompt procedure to teach partici- while many individuals with developmental dis-
pants to complete leisure tasks using an iPod abilities have learned to remain engaged and
Touch. They reported that the time-delay proce- complete tasks in the presence of supervisory
dure used resulted in the rapid fading of prompts staff and “clear contingencies of reinforcement”
and allowed the instructor to successfully fade (p. 121), levels of engagement decrease markedly
their proximity to participants until they were no when supervision is absent. Decreased levels of
longer present. engagement in the absence of supervision are not
In another example, Massey and Wheeler unique to individuals with developmental dis-
(2000) used gestural prompts in addition to ver- abilities. For example, Sariscany et  al. (1995)
bal and manual prompts to promote engagement examined the effects of three supervision strate-
in an inclusive preschool setting. As the authors gies when providing physical education to three
reported, the number of gestures required to pro- junior high school males. Three conditions, (a)
duce the expected levels of on-task behavior close supervision with feedback, (b) distant
showed a pronounced decrease over time. Spriggs supervision with feedback, and (c) distant super-
et al. (2016) suggested that instructors “reflect on vision without feedback, were applied. In feed-
the specific response prompting strategy that back conditions, these adolescents received 0.5
would be most successful with their students” feedback statements per min. Sariscany and col-
(p. 11). Perhaps the success of the studies that did leagues reported that two of three participants
not use graduated guidance exclusively owes showed significantly higher levels of engagement
their success to the fact that the selected prompt when they received active supervision.
and prompt-fading procedures represented the In their review of procedures designed to
intervention variables that were most beneficial increase independence, Hume et  al. (2009)
to those individual participants. emphasized the importance of identifying
Although certain authors have advocated the instructional strategies that result in a shift in
use of particular prompt and prompt-fading pro- stimulus control from continuous adult manage-
cedures (McClannahan & Krantz, 1999, 2010), ment to an alternate stimulus. An activity sched-
the literature suggests that the specific prompt ule could serve such a stimulus if the presence of
type or fading strategy selected may be less criti- the instructor can ultimately be faded. Several
cal than the systematic removal of prompts. Cuvo studies investigating the effects of activity sched-
and Davis (1983) proposed that programming the ules described successfully fading the proximity
transfer of stimulus control “be an integral aspect and/or presence of the instructor. In 1997, Steed
of any behavioral intervention” (p.  378). The and Lutzker used activity schedules to teach an
application of a more diverse number of prompt-­ adult with developmental disabilities to indepen-
fading strategies in the activity schedule litera- dently complete vocational activities. In the third
ture has expanded the possibilities for the phase of teaching, the instructor’s presence was
successful transfer of stimulus control and gradually faded until the participant completed
allowed a greater level of individualization when tasks independent of supervision. Steed and
designing intervention packages for learners. Lutzker noted that their findings showed that pic-
ture prompts facilitated the increase and mainte-
nance of the participant’s task completion
27.1.5 The Presence of the Instructor behavior and represented an alternative to direct,
as a Prompt ongoing supervision.
Pelios et al. (2003) examined the effects of a
Although MacDuff et al. (2001) did not include treatment package that included the systematic
the presence of the instructor in their taxonomy fading of the instructor’s presence on the promo-
518 A. S. Freeman et al.

tion of independent work skills. Fading occurred phrases. When feedback was contingent on an
via shadowing the participant and then gradually incorrect response, incorrect responses predomi-
increasing the physical distance between the stu- nated. Similarly, when the error-correction con-
dent and instructor until the instructor was no sequence followed nonresponding, this behavior
longer present in the room. The description of the prevailed. The authors reported that students
fading process was well defined across ten steps. mastered the target task in fewer trials with the
As stated, the initial fading steps were designed above-mentioned error-correction strategies than
to decrease the instructor’s proximity to the they did in a no prompt and/or trial-and-error
learner. The first component of fading was char- condition but noted that the acquired skill showed
acterized by the instructor shadowing the move- better retention in a 1-week follow-up for partici-
ments of the student at a distance of 5 or 6 inches. pants exposed to trial-and-error procedures.
This distance was increased to 12 and then 24 in., These results caused the authors to suggest that
and finally the instructor moved to and stood in the corrective procedure (i.e., feedback) may
the doorway to the classroom for the duration of have functioned as a reinforcer.
the session. When the proximity of the instructor In a study conducted by Shreiber and col-
had been faded to this level, the remaining fading leagues (n.d.), the authors noted that prompts that
steps were dedicated to making supervision non-­ followed errors functioned as reinforcers for
continuous and less predictable. At this point, the many of their more challenged students. Using a
instructor exited the room and closed the door staff training protocol that included modeling
after instructing the child to begin their activity and feedback, the authors successfully decreased
schedule but returned every minute on the minute the number of prompts teachers provided that fol-
mark. At the next two levels of fading, the length lowed student errors and increased instructor’s
of time the instructor was absent was increased, delivery of tokens. These changes in teacher
and they returned every 2  min and then every behavior resulted in a marked change in the mean
3  min. Two schedules of random supervision levels of engagement displayed by adult learn-
were then introduced in succession with the ers—30% in baseline and 90% following
instructor returning every 2–5 min and then every intervention.
3–5 min. The final level of fading was achieved Touchette and Howard (1984) suggested that
by having the instructor return once during the unlike trial-and-error procedures, intervention
15  min session at a randomly selected time. procedures that produced a low level of errors
Throughout the fading process, the instructor also evoked greater attention to discriminative
remained in the room for approximately 3 s each stimuli. They also stated that transfer of stimulus
time they returned. This level of systematic fad- control was accelerated when the probability of
ing resulted in high levels of on-task and on-­ reinforcement favored unprompted responding.
schedule behavior for each of the boys with a Both observations would appear relevant to
supervising adult present for only brief and inter- teaching learners with disabilities to complete
mittent periods of time. activity schedules in which a goal would be to
have the schedule function as a discriminative
stimulus.
27.1.6 The Influence of Prompts Although the most-to-least prompt sequence
Following Errors described by McClannahan and Krantz (1999,
2010) is designed to prevent errors, they acknowl-
Although several prompt-fading procedures have edged that all individuals will make errors when
been posited as “errorless,” few studies have learning to follow activity schedules. They pos-
evaluated the function of prompts that follow ited further that when an error occurs that the
errors. Magee and Ellis (2006) assessed the func- instructor returns to the previous level of prompt-
tion of errors with undergraduate students learn- ing that had produced successful schedule-­
ing to receptively identify Japanese words and following behavior—if the instructor was
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 519

prompting at the learner’s shoulder, they would on the controlling prompt of the therapist with
return to prompt at the forearm or wrist. They little progress made in fading the prompt.
specified that this level of prompting should con- Comparably, Oppenheimer et  al. (1993) stated
tinue to be provided until the learner has made that it is not uncommon for participants to wait to
one or more correct schedule-following engage in a response until a prompt is provided,
responses. For example, if the learner does not despite having learned the skill.
point to the textual or photographic stimuli in the A stimulus acquires control over a response
schedule return to the previous prompting proce- only when the response emitted in the presence
dure until they have correctly pointed to one or of that stimulus produces reinforcement (Tarbox
two consecutive stimuli, then resume & Tarbox, 2017). To ensure that target responses
prompt-fading. come under stimulus control of relevant aspects
of the environment, it would appear prudent to
select teaching strategies that emphasize environ-
27.1.7 Activity Schedules mental stimuli that will continue to be present
and Reinforcement when instruction and prompt-fading have been
completed.
Although “independence” has been identified as Stimuli are described as criterion related when
a desired outcome produced via the acquisition they cue participants to attend to the dimensions
of schedule-following skills, the New Webster of the stimulus complex that are relevant to mak-
Encyclopedic Dictionary of the English Language ing a criterion discrimination (Etzel et al., 1981;
defines “independent” as behavior that is not sub- Horner & Billingsley, 1988; Smeets et al., 1990).
ject to the control of others or that does not rely Zane et al. (1984) stated that “the success of any
on others. In many activity schedule publications method designed to facilitate performance is
(Bryan & Gast, 2000; Massey & Wheeler, 2000; dependent upon the degree to which it maximizes
Pelios et al., 2003; Steed & Lutzker, 1997), the attention to the critical dimensions of either the
emphasis of teaching schedule-following skills target (correct) and/or comparison stimuli”
appears to be on increasing participants’ levels of (p.  368). When teaching schedule-following
unprompted behavior (i.e., addressing, treating, behavior, instructors endeavor to establish the
and/or minimizing the level of prompt depen- activity schedule as a relevant or criterion-related
dence exhibited by participants). aspect of the learning environment (i.e., discrimi-
As Cameron et  al. (1992) stated, sometimes native stimuli). The success of establishing the
prompting procedures cause people with autism activity schedule as a discriminative stimulus
to “attend to the teacher’s prompt and learn noth- (SD) or series of SDs is dependent on fading
ing about the task” (p.  329). MacDuff et  al. prompts and minimizing pairing of the instructor
(2001) defined prompt dependence as behavior and the motivational system used to reinforce
that is under the stimulus control of irrelevant schedule-following behavior. In their develop-
aspects of the learner’s environment. They repeat- ment of a practice guide for the design and imple-
edly described how prompts (paired with the mentation of visual activity schedules, Hugh
delivery of rewards) presented by supervising et  al. (2018) emphasized the importance of
adults and/or the presence of the supervising ­ individualizing reinforcement systems that are
adult often come to control target responses ver- used in conjunction with activity schedules.
sus having these responses of interest come under A number of authors suggest frequently deliv-
the stimulus control of aspects of the environ- ering reinforcers for new schedule followers and
ment that evoke these same behaviors in non-­ ensuring that reinforcers are delivered from
disabled individuals. Similar definitions of behind the participant to avoid pairing the pres-
prompt dependence have been posited by Clark ence of the instructor with reinforcement
and Green (2004). Dependency was character- (Doenyas, 2014; McClannahan & Krantz, 1999,
ized by the learner’s response being dependent 2010). Dunlap and Plienis (1988) stated that all
520 A. S. Freeman et al.

stimuli associated with contingencies of rein- grams, and books using picture activity schedules
forcement are likely to acquire discriminative and an instructor-managed motivational system.
properties. In addition these authors noted that “Students earned the same token during the study
the continual pairing of instructors with the deliv- that they earned during their routine school day”
ery of rewards is a key variable that results in dif-(Spriggs et al., 2013, p. 213). Tokens were deliv-
ferences noted in a learner’s performance when ered at the conclusion of each 40-min session and
the instructor is present versus absent. were based on the amount of work completed.
Interestingly, in their review of the literature, Token exchanges were also delayed; participants
Koos and MacDuff (2018) reported only 13 of exchanged tokens for backup reinforcers twice
the 34 studies reviewed (38%) described how or each week. Similarly, Carson et al. (2008) used a
when reinforcement was managed within the delayed but socially mediated reinforcement sys-
intervention packages used by researchers to tem in combination with a photographic activity
teach schedule-following behavior. schedule to increase task completion and inde-
pendent movement between tasks for three par-
ticipants with mild to moderate intellectual
27.1.8 Socially Mediated disabilities. Prior to the 65-min sessions, partici-
Motivational Systems pants selected a snack from a reinforcer prefer-
ence menu. They gained access to this reinforcer
Reinforcement is a key variable in the acquisition if they completed four of five changes in task
and maintenance of schedule-following behavior without assistance. Although Carson et al. noted
and in establishing the activity schedule as a dis- that the proximity of the instructor may have
criminative stimulus (Kimball et al., 2004). How influenced participants’ work behavior, they
reinforcers are presented and managed has dif- made no mention of how the use of delayed rein-
fered across studies. Some of the earliest studies forcers may have impacted their performance.
investigating the effects of visual cues on the lev- MacDuff et  al. (1993) taught four boys with
els of engagement and task completion used autism to complete hour-long photographic activ-
teacher-managed rewards systems. For example, ity schedules consisting of leisure, recreation,
Johnson and Cuvo (1981) taught adults with and academic activities. Perhaps one of the most
mental retardation to cook using visual cues and important, yet overlooked, aspects of this study
instructor-delivered stars. Participants received a was the authors’ decision to embed reinforcing
star on a bar graph for each day’s performance activities (i.e., snacks) into the activity schedule
that showed improvement over the previous data to avoid repeated pairing of the instructor with
point. In another study, Thinesen and Bryan the delivery of potential reinforcers. The speed
(1981) stated that instructors delivered edible with which the presence of the instructor was
reinforcers to participants for correctly complet- faded in this study may be attributed to the fact
ing grooming responses. These authors also that they were never paired with the delivery of
described the systematic thinning of edible reinforcement and, therefore, their presence and/
rewards until all edible reinforcers had been or absence did not influence the boys’ schedule-­
faded. As the activity schedule literature has pro- following behavior. For the four boys in this
gressed, so has the manner in which researchers study, it is likely that prohibiting the instructor
have endeavored to provide appropriate levels of from delivering rewards and praise statements at
reinforcement for task completion, engagement, any time not only decreased the salience of the
and on-schedule behavior. instructor’s presence but also helped establish the
Spriggs et al. (2007) increased the level of on-­ activity schedule as a discriminative stimulus.
task and on-schedule behavior displayed by four Similarly, Pierce and Schreibman (1994) used
students with moderate intellectual disabilities photographic activity schedules with three boys
during centers activities that included familiar with autism to evoke the independent completion
tasks such as math worksheets, computer pro- of daily living skills such as making a bed, setting
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 521

a table, and getting dressed. Smiley faces were saved the favored activity until the others had
placed on the last page of each activity schedule been completed. The final participant also dis-
to evoke the self-delivery of reinforcers. The played their activity schedule stimuli on a bulletin
authors reported that the use of the activity sched- board. Although the authors did not explicitly
ule and self-management systems was taught describe the motivational system for this individ-
using praise, verbal prompts, and modeling. The ual, their activity schedules were brief and con-
authors specified that behavior shaping was used sisted of only a few activities—when they
to promote the “transfer of stimulus control from completed their scheduled activities, the authors
prompt to task-related stimuli” (Pierce & stated that they would engage in routine activities
Schreibman, 1994, p. 474). that they had previously completed without assis-
Embedded rewards might also encompass tance. Perhaps the opportunity to engage in their
activity schedules that produce and/or provide daily routine had reinforcement value.
access to potential reinforcers. For example, In a study designed to teach a child with
Mechling and colleagues (2009) taught three autism “generative sociodramatic play” skills,
high school males with ASD to successfully pre- Dauphin et  al. (2004) used a teaching package
pare a series of recipes using a personal digital that combined video-based matrix training and
assistant (PDA) as a “self-prompting” device. notebook activity schedules. The authors embed-
Their description of the motivational system used ded breaks in both types of activity schedules. In
in conjunction with the PDA included verbal the video-based activity schedule, slides that
praise for unprompted and correct prompted played auditory praise and signaled the availabil-
responses on a variable-ratio (VR) three schedule ity of a break were included. When the partici-
of reinforcement and the opportunity to consume pant earned a break, they gained access to
the food they had prepared. Spriggs et al. (2014) preferred activities and snack. Similarly, a token
used visual activity schedules with embedded was mounted on each page of a book-based activ-
video models to assess the acquisition and gener- ity schedule that the student manipulated when
alization of novel tasks such as check writing, they completed an activity. The last picture in the
table setting, and paragraph writing as well as schedule depicted the icon for a break.
independent movement between activities by In their development of a practice guide for
four high school students with ASD.  Spriggs the design and implementation of visual activity
et al. (2014) specified that “no tangible reinforce- schedules, Hugh et al. (2018) suggested that the
ment was given” (p. 3852), but each session was schedule of reinforcement required by learners is
followed by a preferred activity that was a com- likely idiosyncratic and therefore requires that
ponent of students’ daily schedules. the motivational system associated with the activ-
In a study designed to teach adults with mental ity schedule be individualized. That said, if a goal
retardation to complete self-care, housekeeping, of instruction is to ultimately fade the presence of
and leisure activities, Anderson et al. (1997) used the instructor, motivational systems that rely on
pictures and line drawings to design individual- socially mediated access to tokens and other con-
ized activity schedules. For one of the three par- ditioned reinforcers would appear detrimental
ticipants, the target activities were presented in a because they cannot be implemented in the
book format with tokens attached to the schedule absence of the instructor and result in a condition
pages. The learner removed the tokens as they in which previously reinforced behaviors (e.g.,
completed each activity. Another participant in task completion and schedule-following behav-
this study selected a “favorite activity” from an ior) are placed on extinction. Therefore, systems
array of possibilities and placed the picture of that that included embedded reinforcers and that pro-
activity below the other activity schedule pictures mote self-management of reinforcement systems
placed in a column on their bulletin board. The including gaining access to materials or activities
authors noted that although this participant could acquired through token exchange may be of value
complete these tasks in any order, they typically particularly for older learners and adults.
522 A. S. Freeman et al.

27.1.9 Self-Management Systems achieving student was taught to use self-­


monitoring, self-evaluation, and self-delivery of
Neizel and Busick (2009) posited that self-­ rewards to address goals for the accurate comple-
management meets criteria as an evidence-based tion of math problems—the participant self-­
intervention strategy that has been used to reduce administered a gold star each time they achieved
inappropriate behavior (Coyle & Cole, 2004) and or exceeded their goal. Later, they exchanged
increase adaptive responses such as giving com- accumulated stars for items depicted on a self-­
pliments (Lowy Apple et  al., 2005), on-task determined menu.
behavior (Axelrod et  al., 2019; Callahan & Although self-management of reinforcers
Rademacher, 1999), play skills (Lee et al., 2007; would appear to lend itself to use with activity
Newman et  al., 2000), and increasing levels of schedules, only one published study was identi-
independent behavior (Reinecke et  al., 2016). fied that examined the effects of this strategy. In
Although the conceptualization and use of “self-­ a 2017 publication, Beaver and colleagues
reinforcement” have been debated within our compared the levels of on-task behavior and
field for decades (Golddiamond, 1976; Hayes task completion produced by teacher-delivered
et  al., 1985), a few studies from more recent and self-delivered motivational systems for
times were identified that taught participants to adolescents with autism. Participants used text-
self-manage rewards/reinforcers. One example of based activity schedules presented on an iPod
an early study found that college students who to complete vocational, functional living, and
self-reinforced significantly increased the dura- leisure activities. Prior to intervention partici-
tion of their study time, while undergraduates pants were taught to use a golf counter for the
who did not self-manage rewards did not self-delivery of reinforcers. During the teacher-
(Tichenor, 1977). delivered condition, the instructor delivered
Agran et al. (2001) taught six males with vary- reinforcers via clicks on the golf counter begin-
ing disabilities (e.g., learning disabilities, visual ning with an FR2 schedule of reinforcement.
impairments, intellectual disabilities) and self-­ When on-task behavior and task completion
regulatory behavior that included self-­monitoring, increased, the schedule of reinforcement was
self-evaluation, and self-delivery of rewards. The gradually thinned—when thinning was com-
authors specified that the acquisition of the tar- pleted, participants performed using a FR28
geted self-management responses was likely to schedule of reinforcement. In the self-rein-
promote greater levels of independence, compe- forcement condition, pictures of the golf coun-
tence, and acceptance for the participants. In ter were embedded within the activity
their effort to teach the participants to self-­ schedule—participants advanced the counter
reinforce, Agran and colleagues taught the boys each time the picture appeared. The proximity
to praise their own behavior whenever they of the instructor was faded under both rein-
attained a level of performance that met or forcement conditions. The authors reported
exceeded pre-established goals. When partici- high levels of on-task behavior and task com-
pants achieved weekly goals, they selected a pre- pletion under both reinforcement conditions
ferred activity or snack. For example, one and that the proximity of the instructor was suc-
participant was interested in the Titanic. As a cessfully faded in approximately the same
component of their self-evaluation and self-­ number of sessions. The authors concluded that
managed motivational system, they added a letter although both systems for reinforcer delivery
to their daily self-evaluation card whenever their were equally effective, the self-managed sys-
daily performance reached the predetermined tem may have been superior because it permit-
goal. Access to the weekly reward was contingent ted the presence of the instructor to be
on spelling the word “Titanic.” completely faded, while the teacher-­managed
In another study (i.e., Stevenson & Fantuzzo, system required the instructor to be present, at
1984) involving “self-management,” an under- least, occasionally.
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 523

27.1.10 Summary In a review of the script-fading literature con-


ducted by Akers et al. (2016), visual (i.e., textual)
Applied behavior analytic research on interven- scripts were deemed to be slightly more effective
tion with people with developmental disabilities for individuals with autism. The authors hypoth-
may be viewed as an effort to identify and assess esized that because visual scripts provide two
procedures that, it is hoped, will bring target modes of presentation, in that they provide a
responses under the control of relevant, naturally visual support and oral response via the verbal
occurring discriminative stimuli. Activity sched- prompt, there may be more enhanced
ules can come to function as discriminative stim- responding.
uli and continue to be one of the most effective Researchers have provided limited rationales
evidence-based interventions available that reli- for selecting the use of an auditory or written
ably increases levels of engagement, task com- script. Primarily, researchers have identified the
pletion, and movement from one task to another. use of textual scripts for participants that dis-
The research literature also suggests that with played prerequisite reading skills. For example,
systematic fading of prompts and embedded or Argott et al. (2008) indicated that all participants
self-managed reinforcement systems, activity could read and therefore selected the use of writ-
schedules can be completed at a criterion level in ten scripts to teach three individuals with autism
the absence of or with limited supervision. to respond to affective stimuli with an empathetic
statement. This procedure was effective for two
of three participants. However, for the third par-
27.2 Scripts and Script-Fading ticipant, reading the prompt and emitting the
Procedures appropriate empathetic response proved chal-
lenging, despite their success in reading the tex-
A script is an audiotaped or written word, phrase, tual stimuli. When auditory scripts were presented
or sentence that enables people with autism to and slight procedural modifications were made,
start or continue conversation (McClannahan & they began to respond to the affective stimuli
Krantz, 2005). Scripts can be used to teach stu- with appropriate empathetic statements. This led
dents with limited vocal repertoires as well as the authors to suggest that the method of present-
students with rather complex vocal repertoires. ing scripts should be individualized.
The general goal of script-fading is to reduce stu-
dents’ dependency upon verbal prompts so that 27.2.1.1 Factors That Influence
they spontaneously engage in social interactions. the Selection of Scripts
While further research is needed to compare the
effectiveness of textual versus auditory scripts,
27.2.1 Selecting the Type of Script some potential factors to consider should include
To Be Used the student’s particular strengths related to verbal
imitation and reading. Students who do not dis-
Scripts may be presented via an audio recording play prerequisite reading skills will require the
or textual stimuli. In a seminal study by Krantz use of auditory scripts. Some students who have
and McClannahan (1993), scripts were presented acquired prerequisite reading skills struggle with
using textual stimuli to teach students to interact quickly referencing the textual stimuli and ori-
with one another during an art activity. Stevenson enting toward the interaction partner. Additional
et  al. (2000) adapted this procedure to incorpo- teaching in which the student reads a phrase and
rate the use of auditory scripts to teach non-­ orients to an interaction partner may be
readers to initiate interactions. Scripts were necessary.
presented via a magnetic card reader, and faded Other factors to consider when selecting the
one word at a time, from end to beginning, until type of script to be used pertain to the specific
only photographs of activities remained. skill being taught. Auditory scripts may have the
524 A. S. Freeman et al.

distinct advantage of modeling aspects of vocal to imitate these same sounds or short words from
behavior, such as intonation, volume, and inflec- an audio-recording. This has two benefits. First,
tion. If teaching a student to display an empa- it prepares the student for the use of script-fading
thetic response, as Argott et  al. (2008) did, this procedures. Second, it teaches the student to imi-
may be of particular importance. Similarly, when tate vocalizations without the added prompt of
teaching students to ask questions, an auditory observing instructors’ lip movements. Instructors
model of change in inflection may be valuable. may do this by alternating opportunities to imi-
Written scripts, however, can be more easily tate in  vivo vocal responses and responses pre-
attached to items in the environment and are sented on recording devices.
more discreet and portable. Brown et al. (2008) For students who do not display prerequisite
used written scripts to teach students to engage in skills related to receptive labeling, photographs
interactions while shopping in a mock store and should be included on scripts. For example, if a
ultimately in community settings. A student student learns to approach an adult with a
learning to order from a menu may have written recorder and imitate “play tag” but then does not
scripts available in their wallet to reference as attempt to engage in that activity, attaching a
they order. Students learning to complete work- photograph of them running may be useful to
sheets or other written tasks may have scripts associate meaning to the script. For some stu-
incorporated on those pages. Written scripts can dents, this strategy may be sufficient in teaching
also be faded in a more gradual manner, such as picture-object correspondence. For other stu-
by removing one letter at a time or even by cut- dents, direct teaching of picture-object corre-
ting a letter in half. spondence will be necessary. Stevenson et  al.
Some students may be more likely to omit (2000) attached photographs to auditory scripts
words presented via an auditory script due to the and faded these photographs over the course of
fleeting nature of auditory stimuli. These students the study.
may instead benefit from the visual support of a Textual and auditory scripts may also be
written script. Additionally, when using scripts introduced and faded using smart phones and
with multiple students, peers may attend to the tablets. This has been done at the Princeton
recording rather than the individual repeating the Child Development Institute (PCDI) by using a
script. In this format, such as when teaching chil- variety of applications and embedding text or
dren to interact with one another during play, the voice recordings within activity schedules or
use of textual scripts may be beneficial. photographs presented on the device. For exam-
Alternatively, for students with articulation chal- ple, a young adult may learn to swipe through
lenges, it may be advantageous for peers to hear the photographs on their smart phone to talk
the recording as it may serve to supplement the about recent events with peers. Scripts are
child’s vocal speech. recorded within the photographs and ultimately
It is often helpful to teach students to imitate faded. These technological advancements, in
the audio-recording or read the written script addition to the recent popularity of smart
prior to the onset of instruction. At times, even watches, have the potential to increase portabil-
students who display proficiency in vocal imita- ity of scripts and provide additional strategies
tion struggle with imitating from a recorder and for fading scripts, such as by fading the volume
require supplemental instruction. For children of the auditory model.
who approximate vocal models, such as by say-
Auditory scripts Textual scripts
ing “buh” for “ball,” the recorder may include
Format Voice recorders Cards,
that single sound, in an effort to evoke this verbal Smart phones worksheets,
response. Smart watches written cues
Students who have acquired early vocal imita- Tablets Smart phones
tion responses, such as imitating single sounds or Smart watches
Tablets
short words, should immediately begin learning
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 525

Auditory scripts Textual scripts room,” and as they enter adulthood, it may be
Advantages Models various Child can more appropriate for them to say, “Excuse me,
aspects of vocal repeatedly I’ll be right back.”
behavior reference the
Does not script
compete with Portable and 27.2.2.1 Incorporating Content
visual attending discreet Developed by Peers
Disadvantages Auditory stimuli May compete Observing similarly aged peers of typical devel-
are fleeting with visual
opment can be enormously helpful in generating
attending
normative samples of rates of interactions and
Clinicians are encouraged to evaluate each of content of interactions. Garcia-Albea et al. (2014)
these factors prior to determining whether to use observed preschool and kindergarten students of
auditory or written scripts. Further research is typical development playing with toys that were
needed to determine how auditory and written similar to those introduced during script-fading
scripts compare in the acquisition of scripts and sessions. Three categories of scripts were created
the success of script-fading. Additionally, (i.e., identification of the toy, description of the
researchers should evaluate the impact of ancil- toy, function of the toy) based on the results of
lary aspects of vocal behavior, such as appropri- this observation. Similarly, Gomes et  al. (2020)
ate intonation and inflexion. conducted a 30-min observation of five children
of typical development exposed to stimuli used to
evoke a joint attention response during the course
27.2.2 Selecting the Content of the study. Scripts generated were identical or
of the Script highly similar to those emitted during the obser-
vation. In many cases a normative sample may
In addition to determining the type of script to be produce language that exceeds that child’s
used, instructors must carefully select the content expressive and receptive repertoire. Scripts will
of that script. For very early learners, scripts may require modification to ensure that the student
be introduced without a vocal response. A young with autism can imitate the script fluently and is
child may be taught to obtain a script, such as a understandable if there are articulation
card featuring a photograph of a toy. The child challenges.
approach an interaction partner and activate the Researchers have yet to evaluate the influence
recorder that says “Look” while orienting to the of the content of scripts and how this relates to
adult to garner their attention. This allows the ongoing social interaction following the fading of
child to come into contact with social attention scripts. Anecdotally, during clinical observations
and to engage in early reciprocal interactions, there appears to be a correlation between a stu-
prior to learning to emit vocal responses. dents’ understanding of the content of a script
In order for students to come into contact with and the generality and maintenance of that
reinforcement for social interaction, vocal response. If a student learns to say, “My brother
responses must be socially competent. Creating plays lacrosse,” without having a referent for the
scripts that are age-inappropriate or include atyp- word “lacrosse,” it is unlikely that the student
ical use of language has the potential to socially will initiate this interaction in relevant contexts.
stigmatize students. Scripts that might be taught Future research should more carefully assess par-
at a young age should be modified as needed to ticipants’ understanding of content presented via
increase socially acceptability. Perhaps a 2-year-­ scripts to determine how this influences
old is taught to say “potty” prior to using the ­ effectiveness and to identify whether students
bathroom. As they age, scripts may be reintro- acquire new vocabulary during the script-fading
duced to teach them to say, “May I use the bath- process.
526 A. S. Freeman et al.

27.2.2.2 T  eaching Students to Write used to prevent the occurrence of errors. But,
Scripts when errors occur, prompting should resume, and
As students become more proficient in their use a behavioral rehearsal should be completed. The
of scripts and respond to script-fading proce- use of physical prompting strategies has been
dures, alternate stimuli may be used to evoke most commonly used in the literature (Garcia-­
social interactions. For example, students may Albea et  al., 2014; Krantz & McClannahan,
learn to generate conversational exchanges when 1993; Stevenson et  al., 2000; Wichnick-Gillis
given only a topic or may learn to write their own et al., 2019). Gomes et al. (2020) also used physi-
scripts (McClannahan & Krantz, 2005). While cal prompts during teaching and incorporated the
research has not yet been conducted in this area, use of behavioral rehearsals if a participant did
clinically, we have used these strategies at PCDI not initiate a bid for joint attention.
over the years. It is very common for PCDI stu- If a child does not have a history of approach-
dents to learn to prepare for lunch by obtaining a ing adults, a second person is helpful during early
worksheet, selecting topics to discuss with teaching sessions (McClannahan & Krantz,
friends, and writing scripts to bring to lunch. This 2005). The instructor stands behind the student
allows the student to be an active participant in and provides physical prompts for the student to
choosing topics of conversation. For example, obtain the script, approach the interaction part-
perhaps a child is given several categories, such ner, and initiate the script. Students who demon-
as recent events at school, an upcoming holiday, strate prerequisite skills in emitting vocal
or a favorite movie. The student may look through responses and approaching an interaction partner
those options and choose to talk about his favor- can begin learning to use scripts to engage in
ite movie. Differential reinforcement is used to vocal exchanges. McClannahan and Krantz
teach students to select topics that are novel, (2005) recommend beginning by observing a
accurate, and interesting to others. Instructors child’s preferences and identifying toys, foods, or
may remind students that they have already spo- activities that he or she especially enjoys. If these
ken about a topic or include visual supports, such preferences are not obvious, a preference assess-
as having a student cross items off a topic list ment may be valuable. Ideally, early scripts
after discussing them. For some students addi- would result in immediate access to those items.
tional fading steps are introduced so that the The early script-fading literature did not
scripts are not present during lunch. Instead, gen- include delivery of reinforcement by the instruc-
erating and reviewing scripts beforehand is suffi- tor (e.g., Krantz & McClannahan, 1993, 1998).
cient in maintaining social interactions. Since then, many studies have followed suit
(Groskreutz et  al., 2015; Pollard et  al., 2012;
Stevenson et al., 2000; Woods & Poulson, 2006).
27.2.3 Identifying a Teaching Some studies incorporated the use of materials
Strategy that may have reduced the need for instructor-­
delivered reinforcement. For example, Akers
Prior to implementing script-fading procedures, et  al. (2018) and Reagon and Higbee (2009)
clinicians should plan for the use of prompts, taught interactions about play materials that may
error correction, and reinforcement. One of the have functioned as a reinforcer throughout teach-
goals of script-fading is to prevent students from ing. Similarly, in a study by Sarokoff et al. (2001),
becoming reliant upon instructor prompts to participants had access to snacks and video
interact. Specifically, this strategy reduces the games following initiations in which textual
need for verbal prompts, which can be more dif- stimuli were embedded on those items. Other
ficult to fade. Thus, it is critical that prompting studies have incorporated the use of additional
strategies be selected on the basis that they will reinforcers, such as snacks (Garcia-Albea et al.,
be successfully faded. McClannahan and Krantz 2014; MacDuff et  al., 2007) or tokens (Argott
(2005) indicate that physical prompts should be et  al., 2008; Brown et  al., 2008; Dotto-Fojut
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 527

et  al., 2011; Wichnick, Vener, Keating et  al., ple, the script “I like basketball” may be faded to
2010; Wichnick, Vener, Pyrtek et  al., 2010; “I like ____,” “I ____,” and “____.” Additional
Wichnick-Gillis et  al., 2016; Wichnick-Gillis fading steps are then required to remove the
et al., 2019). Both Garcia-Albea et al. (2014) and recording device or the card that displayed the
MacDuff et  al. (2007) thinned the schedules of text. As students become more fluent in their use
reinforcement during the course of the interven- of scripts, it is possible to fade more rapidly, per-
tion until instructor-delivered prompts were no haps by removing multiple words at once. The
longer used. introduction of other modalities in presenting
Prior to determining whether to incorporate auditory scripts may also provide alternate oppor-
the use of instructor-delivered reinforcement, tunities to fade scripts. For example, presenting
Gomes et  al. (2020) conducted a social conse- scripts via a tablet allows for the opportunity to
quence reinforcer assessment to determine the fade scripts by gradually decreasing the volume
reinforcing value of adult social responses (mod- of the auditory model.
ified from Isaksen & Holth, 2009). Based on the For students with more advanced vocal reper-
results of this assessment, social reinforcement toires, gradual fading may not be necessary at all.
was used for two participants, and an additional Yamamoto and Isawa (2020) evaluated the effects
reinforcer (i.e., edibles) was added for two par- of scripts on social interactions. The authors
ticipants but thinned out throughout the course of demonstrated that even when scripts were fully
the study. This is the first study to provide such removed without the use of a gradual script-­
clear rationales pertaining to the introduction of fading procedure, all participants continued to
tangible reinforcers to establish adult-delivered emit scripted, unscripted, and novel initiations.
social consequences as conditioned reinforcers. This study provides preliminary evidence of the
The determination of the use of tangible rein- potential effectiveness of script removal in main-
forcers should be individualized based upon the taining interactions.
student’s responsiveness to social reinforcers and Determining the potential steps for script-­
the task being targeted. It is likely that the content fading should occur very early on in the process.
of the script will impact whether additional rein- Even when first developing scripts, the fading
forcers are needed. For those responses that process must be a consideration, as this should
require ongoing programmed reinforcement, influence the type of script selected and the con-
self-monitoring might be an effective strategy. tent of the script. For example, if scripts all begin
Krantz and McClannahan (1993) had participants with the same word, this may impact the script-­
use check marks to indicate the use of a script. It fading process. A student presented with the
is possible that checking off each script func- scripts “I like to play outside,” “I like to play tag,”
tioned as a reinforcer to maintain this response and “I like to play soccer” may struggle in identi-
and in some ways served as a form of self-­ fying which script to emit when the last word is
monitoring. Similarly, Parker and Kamps (2011) faded.
taught two students with autism to interact during Planning for script-fading must include con-
social activities, games, and cooking activities. siderations regarding what should ultimately
Scripts were embedded within task analyses, and evoke the target response. Several researchers
participants were taught to self-monitor social have attempted to transfer stimulus control to
initiations. natural stimuli in the environment. Sarokoff et al.
(2001) used scripts with embedded textual stim-
uli, via product labels on food and video
27.2.4 Script-Fading ­packaging, to interact with peers (e.g., “Gummi
Savers” on the package began the sentence
Initially, for students first learning to use scripts, “Gummi Savers are my favorite”). Groskreutz
fading may occur one word at a time or one letter et  al. (2015) embedded scripts on sets of toys.
at a time, when using written scripts. For exam- Similarly, Brown et al. (2008) carefully selected
528 A. S. Freeman et al.

stimuli present at a video-tape rental store, con- or not a person is there to greet. Future research-
venience store, and sporting goods store to ulti- ers should more closely evaluate whether the
mately evoke conversational initiations in those location of a script influences participants’ atten-
settings. The use of these items served as a com- tiveness to the interaction partner.
mon stimulus to promote generalization from the Several researchers have more carefully exam-
“mock” store used for teaching to community ined the role of the establishing operation in the
settings. Wichnick-Gillis et al. (2016) replicated use of social initiations acquired via script-fading
this procedure by superimposing scripts on lei- procedures. For example, Dotto-Fojut et  al.
sure activities (e.g., movies, coloring books, toys, (2011) taught participants to request assistance
puzzles). Similarly, Wichnick-Gillis et al. (2019) when completing a vocational task. The authors
placed textual scripts on a laptop, Lego pieces, also probed scenarios in which the participant did
and lunch materials to teach students to initiate not require assistance. Howlett et al. (2011) used
interaction during these activities. script-fading strategies to teach students with
Researchers have attempted to shift stimulus autism to mand for information by asking
control to the natural environment by manipulat- “Where’s [object]” when a high-preference item
ing the location of scripts. Gallant et  al. (2017) was not in its typical location. The authors incor-
more closely examined this variable by compar- porated an abolishing operation (AO) condition
ing the placement of scripts on target stimuli to in which the item was present to evaluate that this
holding a script behind the participant’s head to mand for information was emitted only when an
activate it. The authors assessed the effects of establishing operation (EO) was in effect. These
these differences on acquisition and maintenance strategies should be more consistently incorpo-
of initiations. After the script-fading process was rated into script-fading procedures to ensure that
completed, the frequency of initiations emitted manding does not inadvertently come under
was greater for two participants when the device stimulus control, rather than evocative control.
was visible, greater for another participant when The use of language is a complex discrimina-
the device was not visible, and for the fourth par- tion, and additional teaching may be necessary to
ticipant, no difference was observed as a function help students identify not only when to initiate
of the auditory script location. This suggests that but also when it is inappropriate to initiate.
perhaps the location of the script is less relevant Eventually, students must learn to determine who
than initially suspected or varies based upon the is available to interact and when it is appropriate
individual characteristics of the student. to interact.
Additional research may be useful in deter-
mining whether the location of the script impacts
other variables, such as by more carefully analyz- 27.2.5 Importance of Conversation
ing the stimuli in the environment that ultimately Partner
evoke social interactions. Materials alone should
not serve as a discriminative stimulus for social McClannahan and Krantz (2005) identified strat-
interaction. Instead, the presence and behavior of egies for conversation partners to provide appro-
a recipient should signal the availability of rein- priate models and reinforce attempts at social
forcement for social interactions, perhaps in con- interaction. These strategies are outlined in
junction with the presence of particular stimuli in Table  27.1. Additional research is needed to
the environment. At PCDI it is common for audi- determine how these variables impact acquisition
tory scripts to be affixed to doorways to teach and maintenance of interaction skills.
students to initiate greetings upon entering the Krantz and McClannahan (1998) taught stu-
building. For many students, this strategy is suc- dents to initiate interactions with adults by saying
cessful, but for some, the doorway acquires faulty “Look” or “Watch me” before engaging in an
stimulus control in which students emit a greet- activity included within an activity schedule. The
ing upon seeing a doorway, regardless of whether conversational recipient was instructed to respond
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 529

to interactions with phrases or short sentences materials, it is possible that participants were less
relevant to the activity. If a child showed the attentive to the instructor’s vocal models due to
recipient a recently colored picture of Big Bird their access to toys following an initial
and said “Look,” the recipient might respond initiation.
“It’s Big Bird.” The authors anecdotally reported Differences to consider across these studies
that the unscripted statements emitted were mod- include the vocal repertoire of the participants
eled by the recipient in prior conversations (e.g., and the content of the models presented. For
“Look, it’s Big Bird”). example, Krantz and McClannahan (1998) pro-
Pollard et al. (2012) more carefully evaluated vided very brief statements following an initial
the effects of varied adult responses on the pro- interaction. Pollard et  al. (2012) and Garcia-­
duction of unscripted statements. In an initial Albea et al. (2014) included far more variability
teaching condition, adults provided a single com- within the models presented. Script-fading pro-
ment in response to bids for joint attention (“Yes, cedures are often developed for students who dis-
that’s right”). In a subsequent teaching condition, play profound deficits in making vocal initiations.
adults varied the statement to include one of three It can be assumed that these students are exposed
statements about feature, function, or class of the to vocal models on a regular basis—yet this has
stimulus. While unscripted interactions increased not been sufficient in teaching this repertoire. It is
for two of three participants in the varied adult possible that script-fading procedures, as
response condition, it is difficult to determine if intended, are more effective models for social
this relationship is causal. interaction. Perhaps this is in part due to the high
Garcia-Albea et  al. (2014) directly evaluated amount of variability in in vivo language models
the influence of vocal models on interactions. and students’ particular learning history with
The authors measured scripted and unscripted responding to those models. Scripts may provide
statements modeled after the conversation part- a more static model that more consistently sig-
ner’s responses. Increases in social initiations nals the availability of reinforcement following
were observed using scripts; this same effect did imitation of the stimulus. Subsequent research
not occur in regard to participants’ use of the should more carefully compare acquisition of
instructor’s models, despite delivery of reinforce- social initiations when presented with textual
ment following in  vivo interactions. Because scripts, auditory scripts, in  vivo models, and
teaching occurred with initiations about play alternate models, such as those presented via
video.
Table 27.1  Role of conversation partner (McClannahan
& Krantz, 2005, p. 29)
1. Invite interaction by looking at and smiling at the 27.2.6 How to Measure and Graph
child
2. Respond enthusiastically to the youngster’s attempts Data collection is critical in assessing the effec-
at conversation tiveness of script-fading strategies. The student’s
3. Ensure a language environment that is
representative of the youngster’s language level—use
existing vocal repertoire and history with script-­
words that are likely to be understood fading procedures should dictate the goal of
4. Make conversation as “natural” as possible instruction. McClannahan and Krantz (2005)
5. Make interesting comments specify that for students first learning to approach
6. Model appropriate voice volume and intonation an interaction partner, data collection should
7. Model gestures—for example, pointing when indicate whether the student obtained the script,
making a statement such as “I like that one” or using
approached the interaction partner, oriented to
expansive hand movements when commenting about
big objects the interaction partner, and imitated the script (if
8. Provide powerful rewards for social interaction by applicable).
delivering the preferred activities, toys, or snacks that When students begin to imitate scripts, it is
were the topic of the child’s initiation important that data collection be focused on the
530 A. S. Freeman et al.

correct use of scripts. As scripts are faded, data altered. For younger students, any and all interac-
collection continues to be important in ensuring tions should be scored. As students acquire more
that the use of the scripts maintains, as well as the advanced vocal repertoires, this may be refined to
occurrence of unscripted interactions. only include interactions that are on-topic, and
McClannahan and Krantz (2005) specify that socially competent, with the eventual goal of pro-
data should be collected per opportunity when ducing novel initiations.
measuring the use of scripts and that a frequency
measure be used to collect data on the frequency
of unscripted initiations. It may also be useful to 27.2.7 Programming
record students’ interactions verbatim. This will for Generalization
allow the instructor to subsequently determine
whether the student is recombining scripts or Stimulus control and generalization occur on a
incorporating language models presented by the spectrum. While it is important to ensure that
instructor. vocal interactions come under relevant stimulus
Early research done by Krantz and control, it is equally important to ensure that
McClannahan (1993) defined scripted interac- these interactions generalize across untaught
tions as those that matched the script and defined conditions or result in interactions that have not
unscripted interactions as those that differed from been directly taught. The script-fading literature
the script or occurred after script-fading was includes many examples of stimulus generaliza-
complete. Thus, scripted interactions were no tion. Woods and Poulson (2006) taught three
longer scored following the script-fading pro- children with developmental disabilities to initi-
cess. Stevenson et al. (2000) altered that defini- ate social interactions about recently completed
tion to include a second measure of scripted and future activities to typically developing peers
responses to account for the use of scripts outside using scripts. Teaching occurred during an art
of immediate repetition of the script. Wichnick, activity. When probe data were collected during a
Vener, Keating, et al. (2010) incorporated a defi- novel activity (i.e., lunch), in a novel setting,
nition for novel initiations, defined as initiations without scripts, initiations increased across all
never uttered during the course of the study. participants.
These data were graphed as the cumulative num- Similarly, Wichnick-Gillis et al. (2019) used a
ber of novel unscripted initiations. script-fading package to teach children with
Akers et al. (2016) reviewed script-fading pro- autism to initiate social interaction with peers
cedures and called for future researchers to while playing games on a laptop, building with
address the lack of agreement in the definition of Legos, and eating lunch. When generalization
unscripted initiations and recommended that was assessed in the home setting with a sibling,
responses be defined in a manner that allows using the same activities, social initiations sys-
researchers to determine if participants engage in tematically increased. Gomes et  al. (2020)
not only scripted responses but also novel assessed generalization with untaught categories
responses, following teaching. Garcia-Albea of stimuli when teaching students to initiate joint
et al. (2014) further refined these definitions by attention responses and assessed generalization
categorizing responses as scripted, unscripted, in a novel setting prior to and following
and novel and differentiating those acquired via intervention. There may be several potential
­
in vivo models. explanations for the success of script-fading in
While these areas are important in refining the producing these generalized responses. First,
script-fading literature, in clinical practice the some studies incorporate the use of common
measurement procedure selected must corre- stimuli to promote generalization. For example,
spond to the goal set forth for the student. As stu- Wichnick-­Gillis et al. (2019) used the same mate-
dents become more proficient in their use of rials in the home setting to assess generalization
scripts, the response requirements should be across settings and recipients. The use of natural
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 531

maintaining contingencies may also promote 27.2.8 Social Validity of Script-­


generalization, as social initiations produce Fading Procedures
access to social attention and at times items in the
environment. Teaching individuals with autism to emit appro-
Recent research has focused on producing priate social interactions has the potential to
response generalization, in which participants improve upon social competence. One assess-
emit vocal responses that vary from those directly ment of this critical area pertains to the social
taught. Not only does this improve social compe- validity of script-fading procedures. Woods and
tence, but it also may ensure that social initiations Poulson (2006) used the Lower Elementary Level
are less sensitive to extinction. Wichnick et  al. Acceptance Scale (A-Scale) to assess the atti-
(2010) demonstrated the effectiveness of script-­ tudes of typically developing peers toward the
fading procedures in increasing the production of students with disabilities (Voeltz, 1980). This
novel utterances emitted by people with autism. was administered prior to and after introducing
The authors defined novel utterances as those that script-fading procedures to increase peer interac-
were never before emitted during the course of tions. Sample questions included “I could be
the study. Garcia-Albea et al. (2014) used a more friends with a kid who can’t talk yet,” “I like to
conservative measure of novel initiations, requir- play with special needs kids,” and “I am some-
ing that they not contain any words from the times mean to other kids.” The changes in accep-
scripts besides conjunctions, articles, preposi- tance scores were positive for every typically
tions, pronouns, or the toys’ name. The authors developing peer included in the study, suggesting
observed the production of novel utterances in that it is possible that the intervention may have
some, but not all, participants. Gomes et  al. contributed to increased acceptance of children
(2020) used a similar definition and observed an with disabilities by their typically developing
increase in unscripted initiations, defined as peers. The authors noted anecdotal observations
recombinations of words from the original of other improvements, such as a peer initiating
scripts, but did not observe a marked increase in play dates with a student from the self-contained
novel bids for joint attention, defined as the par- class.
ticipant emitting a contextual vocal response that Garcia-Albea et  al. (2014) and Gomes et  al.
did not contain any words from the original (2020) both incorporated social validity mea-
scripts except for conjunctions, articles, preposi- sures related to the acceptability of the script-­
tions, pronouns, or the object’s name. fading procedure and outcomes. Garcia-Albea
The differences in the definitions of unscripted et  al. had adults view video from baseline and
and novel interactions across these studies make maintenance sessions and rated the appropriate-
it difficult to ascertain the relevant variables that ness of participant’s initiations. Eighty-six per-
influence the production of responses that vary cent of raters scored maintenance sessions as
from scripts. Future studies should more closely including more appropriate initiations. Gomes et
examine variables that impact the success of al. used a similar assessment; raters indicated that
scripts and script-fading procedures on produc- 0% of baseline clips and 100% of post-interven-
ing response generalization. For example, pro- tion clips included a bid for joint attention. The
viding more specific participant information that use of rating scales in both studies also indicated
perhaps includes an assessment of overall rigidity that instructors found the procedures acceptable
across a variety of areas may be helpful. Perhaps for use.
students who are more likely to engage in more While acceptance by peers and teachers is an
varied responding across a wide array of areas important variable to measure, perhaps the most
are also more likely to emit more variable vocal critical assessment of acceptability would be to
responses. Additionally, more information should directly measure responses to initiations taught
be provided to ascertain the extent to which vocal via scripts. It is likely that the maintenance of ini-
initiations vary from the original scripts. tiations taught via scripts and script-fading pro-
532 A. S. Freeman et al.

cedures would be dependent upon positive tutoring, offering assistance, initiating greetings,
responses that follow those initiations. If a stu- recalling information about the school day and
dent is taught to ask a peer to play, and the peer transmitting this to home, joining a conversation,
does not positively respond, these initiations may sustaining a conversation, retelling a storybook,
be extinguished over time. Thus, an area of future and interacting with others about photographs.
research might include measures of the respon- Teaching students to engage in appropriate
siveness of adults and peers to scripted state- initiations may also be used to provide differen-
ments and an assessment of the variables that tial reinforcement for alternative responses to
impact this. For example, it is possible that teach- responses that may be socially stigmatizing, such
ing students to engage in more variable respond- as perseverative language, and socially incompe-
ing or to emit interactions that require a response, tent interactions. Yamamoto and Isawa (2020)
such as mands for information, may be more targeted social initiations during play and
likely to produce positive responses and therefore assessed its effectiveness on reducing inappropri-
maintain. ate responses as well. While a more thorough
research design was required, this serves as pre-
liminary evidence in the effectiveness of using
27.2.9 Instructional Targets Acquired script-fading strategies to increase one repertoire
via Script-Fading while simultaneously decreasing another. For
students who display problem behavior that is
As students learn to respond to the use of scripts, hypothesized to be maintained by attention, the
subsequent teaching can more narrowly focus on introduction of scripts that result in attention
teaching specific target responses. For example, (e.g., joke telling, giving complements, request-
several studies have employed script-fading to ing positive physical contact) can be successful
teaching the use of varied mand frames (Betz in providing a rich schedule of reinforcement for
et al., 2011; Brodhead et al., 2016; Sellers et al., those appropriate responses and can serve as an
2016), mands for information (Howlett et  al., AO for problem behavior.
2011), and mands for assistance (Dotto-Fojut There may also be benefits to teaching script-­
et  al., 2011). Additional social responses have fading procedures in conjunction with related
been targeted, such as the use of empathetic state- social skills. For example, MacDuff et al. (2007)
ments when instructors demonstrated a non-­ taught a pointing response while teaching stu-
verbal change in affect (Argott et al., 2008), and dents to initiate joint attention responses. There
joint attention (Gomes et al., 2020; MacDuff et are many other ancillary responses that might co-­
al., 2007; Pollard et al., 2012). Script-fading has occur with social initiations, such as the use of
also been used to teach initiations during play gestures, appropriate facial expressions, or into-
with siblings (Akers et  al., 2018), peers nation that corresponds to the script. Additional
(Wichnick, Vener, Pyrtek, et al., 2010), and par- research should explore strategies that target
ents (Reagon & Higbee, 2009). alternate pro-social behaviors to enhance the
The effectiveness of this teaching strategy social validity of initiations.
across so many broad areas indicates that script-­
fading can likely be adapted to many other areas.
Some areas that have not yet been formally eval- 27.2.10 Incorporating Scripts
uated but have been used clinically at PCDI and Script-Fading
include teaching specific grammatical responses Procedures Within
(e.g., pronouns, prepositions, adjectives), incor- Activity Schedules
porating listener responses (e.g., using scripts to
ask follow-up questions, engage in reciprocal McClannahan and Krantz (2005) discuss the
interactions, and demonstrate response variabil- value of embedding scripts within activity sched-
ity when answering questions), engaging in peer ules. Both procedures are designed to teach stu-
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 533

dents to engage in more spontaneous responding, evidence-based interventions used to provide a


whether it be social interactions or the comple- child/adolescent with language to successfully
tion of complex response chains. In their 1998 complete an activity or interaction (NAC, 2015).
study, Krantz and McClannahan embedded social The repeated replication of research on the use
interactions within activity schedules. Woods and of script-fading procedures and activity sched-
Poulson (2006) later incorporated a similar strat- ules demonstrates their utility in teaching indi-
egy in teaching social children with developmen- viduals with autism to initiate activities and
tal disabilities to interact with typically initiate and sustain conversations and other vocal
developing peers. Incorporating scripts within interactions. Incorporating these procedures into
activity schedules has several advantages. For behavior-analytic instruction may facilitate main-
many individuals with autism, engaging in social tenance and more independent initiations of skills
interactions is challenging; interspersing these taught in isolation.
interactions with independent and highly pre-
ferred activities may be advantageous. Social ini-
tiations can also be linked to activities in such a References
way to create a sharing of experiences that repli-
cates many of the social interactions of typically Agran, M., Blanchard, C., Wehmeyer, M., & Hughes, C.
(2001). Teaching students to self-regulate their behav-
developing children. A child may learn to com- ior: The differential effects of student- vs. teacher-­
plete a puzzle and then initiate an interaction with directed reinforcement. Research in Developmental
a peer or adult to garner positive attention. Disabilities, 22, 319–332.
Embedding scripts within schedules also has the Akers, J. S., Pyle, N., Higbee, T. S., Pyle, D., & Gerencser,
K. R. (2016). A synthesis of script fading effects with
benefit of providing visual support long after individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A 20-year
adult supervision is faded, ensuring maintenance review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental
of both skills. Disorders, 3, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s40489-­015-­0062-­9
Akers, J. S., Higbee, T. S., Pollard, J. S., & Reinert, K. S.
(2018). Sibling-implemented script fading to pro-
27.2.11 Conclusion mote play-based statements of children with autism.
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11, 395–399. https://
Activity schedules and script-fading procedures doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­018-­0257-­5
Anderson, M.  D., Sherman, J.  A., Sheldon, J.  B., &
take advantage of visual discrimination and imi- McAdam, D. (1997). Picture activity schedules and
tation skills that often serve as areas of strength engagement of adults with mental retardation in a
for individuals with autism. These procedures are group home. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
highly effective in promoting the use of sponta- 18, 231–250.
Argott, P., Townsend, D. B., Sturmey, P., & Poulson, C. L.
neous language and task completion (Brodhead (2008). Increasing the use of empathetic statements
et al., 2016). Without specifically programming in the presence of a nonverbal affective stimulus in
for the spontaneous occurrence of these adolescents with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum
responses, even students who have received Disorders, 2, 341–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
rasd.2007.08.004
effective behavior analytic services may fail to Axelrod, M. I., Zhe, E. J., Hangen, K. A., & Klein, J. A.
initiate activities, including social interactions. (2019). Self-management of on-task homework
The National Standards Report (NAC, 2009) behavior: A promising strategy for adolescents with
identified schedules as an “established” evidence-­ attention and behavior problems. School Psychology
Review, 38, 325–333.
based intervention for individuals with autism. At Banda, D. R., & Grimmett, E. (2008). Enhancing social
that time, they identified scripts and script-fading and transition behaviors of persons with autism
procedures as an “emerging” evidence-based through activity schedules: A review. Education and
practice, indicating that more high-quality Training in Developmental Disabilities, 43, 324–333.
Beaver, B. N., Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., & DeBar, R. M.
research was necessary to support this treatment. (2017). Self-reinforcement compared to teacher-­
In their updated 2015 review, script-fading proce- delivered reinforcement during activity schedules on
dures were reclassified as 1 of 14 “established”
534 A. S. Freeman et al.

the iPad Touch. Education and Training in Autism and Dauphin, M., Kinney, E.  M., & Stromer, R. (2004).
Developmental Diabilities, 52, 393–404. Using video-enhanced activity schedules and matrix
Betz, A. M., Higbee, T. S., Kelley, K. N., Sellers, T. P., & training to teach sociodramatic play to a child with
Pollard, J.  S. (2011). Increasing response variability autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 6,
of mand frames with script training and extinction. 238–250.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 357–362. Doenyas, C. (2014). Activity schedules on iPads for chil-
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­357 dren with autism: How to prepare them and teach their
Betz, A., Higbee, T. S., & Reagon, K. A. (2008). Using independent use. NJ.
joint activity schedules to promote peer engagement in Dotto-Fojut, K., Reeve, K. F., Townsend, D. B., & Progar,
preschoolers with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior P.  R. (2011). Teaching adolescents with autism to
Analysis, 41, 237–241. describe a problem and request assistance during sim-
Brodhead, M. T., Higbee, T. S., Gerencser, K. R., & Akers, ulated vocational tasks. Research in Autism Spectrum
J.  S. (2016). The use of a discrimination-­ training Disorders, 5, 826–833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
procedure to teach mand variability to children with rasd.2010.09.012
autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, Dunlap, G., & Plienis, A.  J. (1988). Generalization and
34–48. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.280 maintenance of unsupervised responding via remote
Brown, J.  L., Krantz, P.  J., McClannahan, L.  E., & contingencies. In R.  H. Horner, G.  Dunlap, & R.  L.
Poulson, C.  L. (2008). Using script fading to pro- Koegel (Eds.), Generalization and maintenance: Life-­
mote natural environment stimulus control of verbal style changes in applied settings. Brookes Publishing.
interactions among youths with autism. Research in Duttlinger, C., Ayres, K. M., Bevill-Davis, A., & Douglas,
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 480–497. https://doi. K. H. (2013). The effects of a picture activity schedule
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2007.08.006 for students with intellectual disability to complete a
Bryan, L. C., & Gast, D. L. (2000). Teaching on-task and sequence of tasks following verbal directions. Focus
on-schedule behaviors to high-functioning children on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 28,
with autism via picture activity schedules. Journal of 32–43.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 553–567. Etzel, B. C., LeBlanc, J. M., Schilmoeller, K. J., & Stella,
Callahan, K., & Rademacher, J. A. (1999). Using M. E. (1981). Stimulus control procedures in the edu-
self-management strategies to incrrease the on- cation of young children. In S.  W. Bijou & R.  Ruiz
task behavior of a student with autism. Journal (Eds.), Contributions of behavior modification to edu-
of Positive Interventions, 1, 117–122. https:// cation (pp. 3–37). Lawrence Eilbaum.
doi:10.1177/109830079900100206 Gallant, E.  E., Reeve, S.  A., Brothers, K.  J., & Reeve,
Cameron, M.  J., Ainsleigh, S.  A., & Bird, F.  L. (1992). K. F. (2017). Auditory script location does not affect
The acquisition of stimulus control of compliance acquisition and maintenance of vocal initiation by
and participation during an AD routine. Behavioral children with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 32,
Residential Treatment, 7, 327–340. 103–120. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1467
Carlile, K. A., Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., & DeBar, R. M. Garcia-Albea, E., Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., & Brothers,
(2013). Using activity schedules on the iPod touch to K. J. (2014). Using audio script fading and multiple-­
teach leisure skills to children with autism. Education exemplar training to increase vocal interactions in
and Treatment of Children, 36, 33–57. children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
Carson, K. D., Gast, D. L., & Ayres, K. M. (2008). Effects Analysis, 47, 325–343. https://doi.org/10.1002/
of a photo activity schedule book on independent task jaba.125
changes by students with intellectual disabilities in Giles, A., & Markham, V. (2017). Comparing book- and
community and school job sites. European Journal of tablet-based picture activity schedules: Acquisition
Special Needs Education, 23, 269–279. and preference. Behavior Modification, 41, 647–664.
Cihak, D. F. (2011). Comparing pictorial and video mod- Golddiamond, I. (1976). Self-reinforcement. Journal of
eling activity schedules during transitions for students Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 509–514.
with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Goldsmith, T. R., & Le Blanc, L. A. (2004). Use of tech-
Spectrum Disorders, 5, 433–441. nology in interventions for children with autism.
Clark, K.  M., & Green, G. (2004). Comparison of two Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention,
procedures for teaching dictated word/symbol rela- 1, 166–178.
tions to learners with autism. Journal of Applied Gomes, S. R., Reeve, S. A., Brothers, K. J., Reeve, K. F.,
Behavior Analysis, 37, 503–507. & Sidener. (2020). Establishing a generalized reper-
Coyle, C., & Cole, P. (2004). A videotaped self-modeling toire of initiating bids for joint attention in children
and self-monitoring treatment program to decrease with autism. Behavior Modification, 44(3), 394–428.
off-task behavior in children with autism. Journal of https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445518822499
Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 29, 3–16. Groskreutz, M. P., Peters, A., Groskreutz, N. C., & Higbee,
Cuvo, A. J., & Davis, P. K. (1983). Behavior therapy T.  S. (2015). Increasing play-based commenting in
and community living skills. Progress in Behavior children with autism spectrum disorder using a novel
Modification, 14, 125–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/ script-frame procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior
B978-0-12-535614-5.50008-4
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 535

Analysis, 48(2), 442–447. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Koos, M., & MacDuff, G.  S. (2018). Using activity
jaba.194 schedules and other visual stimuli to enhance skill
Hayes, S. C., Rosenfarb, I., Wulfert, E., Munt, E. D., Korn, acquisition: A review of the literature. Manuscript in
Z., & Zettle, R.  D. (1985). Self-reinforcement: An preparation.
artifact of social standard setting. Journal of Applied Koyoma, T., & Wang, H. T. (2011). Use of activity sched-
Behavior Analysis, 18, 201–214. ules to promote independent performance of individu-
Horner, R.  H., & Billingsley, F.  F. (1988). The effect of als with autism and other intellectual disabilities: A
competing behavior on the generalization and main- review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32,
tenance of adaptive behavior in applied settings. 2235–2242.
In R.  H. Horner, G.  Dunlap, & R.  L. Koegel (Eds.), Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E. (1993). Teaching
Generalization and maintenance: Life-style changes children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a
in applied settings. Brookes. script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior
Horton, S. V., & Taylor, D. C. (1989). The use of behavior Analysis, 26, 121–132.
therapy and physical therapy to promote independent Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1998). Social inter-
ambulation in a preschooler with mental retarda- action skills for children with autism: A script-fading
tion and cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental procedure for beginning readers. Journal of Applied
Disabilities, 10, 363–375. Behavior Analysis, 31, 191–202.
Howlett, M. A., Sidener, T. M., Progar, P. R., & Sidener, Lee, S.-H., Simpsom, R.  L., & Shogren, K.  A. (2007).
D. W. (2011). Manipulation of motivating operations Effects and implications of self-management for stu-
and use of a script-fading procedure to teach mands dents with autism: A meta-analysis. Focus on Autism
for location to children with language delays. Journal and Other Developmental Disabilities, 22, 2–13.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(4), 943–947. https:// Lequia, J., Machalicek, W., & Rispoli, M. (2012). Effects
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­943 of activity schedules on challenging behavior exhib-
Hugh, M. L., Conner, C., & Stewart, J. (2018). Practice ited in children with autism spectrum disorders: A
guide: Using visual activity schedules to identify aca- systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum
demic interventions for young children with autism Disorders, 6, 480–492.
spectrum disorder. US Department of Education, Lowy Apple, A., Billingsley, F., & Swartz, I.  S. (2005).
Office of Special Education Programs. Effects of video modeling alone and with self-­
Hume, K., Loftin, R., & Lantz, J. (2009). Increasing inde- management on compliment-giving behavior of chil-
pendence in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A review dren with high-functioning ASD. Journal of Positive
of three focused intervention. Journal of Autism and Behavior Interventions, 7, 33–46.
Developmental Disorders, 39, 1329–1338. MacDuff, G.  S., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E.
Isaksen, J., & Holth, P. (2009). An operant approach to (1993). Teaching children with autism to use pho-
teaching joint attention skills to children with autism. tographic activity schedules: Maintenance and gen-
Behavior Intervention, 24, 215–236 eralization of complex response chains. Journal of
Johnson, L., Fleury, V. F., Ford, A., Rudolph, B., & Young, Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 89–97.
K. (2018). Translating evidence-based practices to MacDuff, G.  S., Krantz, P.  J., & McClannahan, L.  E.
usable interventions for young children with autism. (2001). Prompts and prompt-fading strategies for
Journal of Early Intervention, 40, 1–18. people with autism. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & R. M.
Johnson, M., Spriggs, A. D., Shepley, S. B., Allday, R. A., Foxx (Eds.), Making a difference: Behavioral inter-
& Samudre, M. (2016). Video activity schedules to vention for autism. pro-ed.
increase independence for students with disabilities. MacDuff, J. L., Ledo, R., McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz,
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, P. J. (2007). Using scripts and script-fading procedures
31, 73–88. to promote bids for joint attention by young children
Johnson, B.F., & Cuvo, A. J. (1981). Teaching mentally with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
retarded adults to cook. Behavior Modification, 5, 1, 281–290.
187–202. https://doi.org/10.1177/014544558152003 Magee, S.  K., & Ellis, J. (2006). The role of error-­
Kelley, K. R., Test, D. W., & Cooke, N. L. (2013). Effects correction procedures in the reinforcement of errors.
of picture prompts delivered by a video iPod on pedes- Behavioral Interventions, 21, 205–226.
trian navigation. Exceptional Children, 79, 459–474. Massey, G., & Wheeler, J. J. (2000). Acquisition and gen-
Kimball, J. W., Kinney, E. M., Taylor, B. A., & Stromer, eralization of activity schedules and their effects o task
R. (2004). Video-enhanced activity schedules for chil- engagement in a young child with autism in an inclu-
dren with autism: A promising package for teaching sive pre-school classroom. Education and Training in
social skills. Education and Treatment of Children, 27, Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
280–298. 35, 326–335.
Knight, V., Sartini, E., & Spriggs, A. D. (2015). Evaluating McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz, P. J. (1999; 2010). Activity
visual activity schedules as evidence-based practice schedules for children with autism: Teaching indepen-
for individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Journal dent behavior. Woodbine.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 157–178.
536 A. S. Freeman et al.

McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz, P. J. (2005). Teaching con- dents with moderate intellectual disability when lost
versation to children with autism: Scripts and script in community settings. Education and Training in
fading. Woodbine House. Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 48, 178–189.
Mechling, L.  C., Gast, D.  L., & Seid, N.  H. (2010). Reagon, K.  A., & Higbee, T.  S. (2009). Parent-­
Evaluation of a personal digital assistant as a self-­ implemented script fading to promote play-based
prompting device for increasing multi-step task verbal initiations in children with autism. Journal of
completion by students with moderate intellec- Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 659–664. https://doi.
tual disabilities. Education and Training in Mental org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42.659
Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 45, Rehfeldt, R.  A. (2002). A review of McClannahan and
422–439. Krantz’s activity schedules for children with autism:
Moore, M., & Calvert, S. (2000). Brief report: Vocabulary Teaching independent behavior: Toward the inclu-
acquisition for children with autism: Teacher or com- sion and integration of children with diabilities. The
puter instruction. Journal of Autism and Developmental Behavior Analyst, 25, 103–108.
Disorders, 30, 359–262. Rehfeldt, R. A., Kinney, E. M., Root, S., & Stromer, R.
National Autism Center. (2009). National standards (2004). Creating activity schedules using Microsoft
report: The National Standards Project – Addressing Powerpoint. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
the need for evidence-based practice guidelines for 37, 15–128.
autism spectrum disorders. Reinert, K. S., Higbee, T. S., & Nix, L. D. (2020). Creating
National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu- digital activity schedules to promote independence
sions: National Standards Project, phase 2.. Retrieved and engagement. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 13,
from http://www.autismdiagnostics.com/assets/ 577–595.
Resources/NSP2.pdf Reinecke, D., Krokowski, A., & Newman, B. (2016). Self-
Neizel, J., & Busick, M. (2009). Overview of self-­ management for building independence: Research and
management. National Professional Development future directions. International Journal of Educational
Center on Autism Spectrum Disorders, Frank Porter Research, 87, 119–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Graham Child Development Institute, The University jer2016.08.006.
of North Carolina. Sariscany, M. J., Darst, P. W., & van der Mars, H. (1995).
Newman, B., Reinecke, D. R., & Meinberg, D. L. (2000). The effects of three teacher supervision patterns on
Self-management of varied responding in three stu- student on-task and skill performance in secondary
dents with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 15, physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical
145–151. Education, 14, 179–197.
Oppenheimer, M., Saunders, R.  R., & Spradlin, J.  E. Sarokoff, R. A., Taylor, B. A., & Poulson, C. L. (2001).
(1993). Investigating the generality of the delayed-­ Teaching children with autism to engage in conversa-
prompt effect. Research in Developmental Disabilities, tional exchanges: Script fading with embedded textual
14, 425–444. stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34,
Parker, D., & Kamps, D. (2011). Effects of task analy- 81–84.
sis and self-monitoring for children with autism in Sellers, T.  P., Kelley, K., Higbee, T.  S., & Wolfe, K.
multiple social settings. Focus on Autism and Other (2016). Effects of simultaneous script training on use
Developmental Disabilities, 26, 131–142. https://doi. of varied mand frames by preschoolers with autism.
org/10.1177/1088357610376945 The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32, 15–26. https://
Pelios, L. V., MacDuff, G. S., & Axelrod, S. (2003). The doi.org/10.1007/s40616-­015-­0049-­8
effects of a treatment package in establishing inde- Shreiber, P. C., Brothers, K. J., & Machado, E. D. (n.d.).
pendent academic work skills in children with autism. Prompting while teaching children with autism: An
Education and Treatment of Children, 26, 1–21. analysis of prompting procedures. Somerset Hills
Pierce, K.  L., & Schreibman, L. (1994). Teaching daily Learning Institute, Unpublished manuscript.
living skills to children with autism in unsupervised Smeets, P.  M., Striefel, S., & Hoogeveen, F.  R. (1990).
settings through pictorial self-management. Journal of Time-delay discrimination training: Replication with
Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 471–481. different stimuli and different populations. Research
Pierce, J.  M., Spriggs, A.  D., Gast, D.  L., & Luscre, D. in Developmental Disabilities, 11, 217–240.
(2013). Effects of visual activity schedules on inde- Spriggs, A.  D., Gast, D.  L., & Ayres, K.  M. (2007).
pendent classroom transitions for students with autism. Using picture activity schedule books to increase
International Journal of Disability, Development, and on-­schedule and on-task behaviors. Education and
Education, 60, 253–269. Training in Developmental Disabilities, 42, 209–223.
Pollard, J. S., Betz, A. M., & Higbee, T. S. (2012). Script-­ Spriggs, A.  D., Knight, V., & Sherrow, L. (2014).
fading to promote unscripted bids for joint attention Talking picture schedules: Embedding video models
in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior into visual activity schedules to increase indepen-
Analysis, 45, 387–393. https://doi.org/10.1901/ dence for students with ASD. Journal of Autism and
jaba.2012.45-­387 Developmental Disorders, 45, 3846–3861.
Purrazzella, K., & Mechling, L.  C. (2013). Use of an Spriggs, A. D., Mims, P. J., van Dijk, W., & Knight, V. F.
iPhone 4 with video features to assist location of stu- (2016). Examinization of the evidence base for using
27  Activity Schedules and Script-Fading Procedures: Key Curricula for Teaching People with Autism… 537

visual activity schedules with students with intellec- Voeltz, L. (1980). Children’s attitudes toward handi-
tual disability. The Journal of Special Education, 51, capped peers. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,
1–13. 84, 455–464.
Spriggs, A. D., van Dijk, W., & Mims, P. J. (2015). How Whatley, A. D., Gast, D. L., & Hammond, D. L. (2009).
to implement visual activity schedules for students Visual activity schedules: Teaching independent tran-
with disabilities. DADD Online Journal: Research to sitioning during recreation and leisure. Therapeutic
Practice, 2, 21–34. Recreation Journal, 43, 27–42.
Steed, S.  E., & Lutzker, J.  R. (1997). Using picture Wichnick, A. M., Vener, S., & M., Keating, C., & Poulson,
prompts to teach an adult with developmental dis- C. L. (2010). The effect of a script-fading procedure
abilities to independently complete vocational tasks. on unscripted social initiations and novel utter-
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, ances among young children with autism. Research
9, 117–132. in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 51–64. https://doi.
Stevenson, H. C., & Fantuzzo, J. W. (1984). Application org/10.1016/j.rasd.2009.07.006
of the “Generalization Map” to a self-control inter- Wichnick, A.  M., Vener, S.  M., Pyrtek, M., & Poulson,
vention with school aged children. Journal of Applied C.  L. (2010). The effect of a script-fading proce-
Behavior Analysis, 17, 203–212. dure on responses to peer initiations among young
Stevenson, C.  L., Krantz, J.  P., & McClannahan, L.  E. children with autism. Research is Autism Spectrum
(2000). Social interaction skills for children with Disorders, 4, 2090–2299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
autism: A script-fading procedure for nonreaders. rasd.2009.09.016
Behavioral Interventions, 15, 1–20. Wichnick-Gillis, A.  M., Vener, S.  M., & Poulson, C.  L.
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol- (2019). Script fading for children with autism:
ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Generalization of social initiation skills from school
Analysis, 10, 349–367. to home. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 52,
Stromer, R., Kimball, J.  W., Kinney, E.  M., & Taylor, 451–466.
B.  A. (2006). Activity schedules, computer technol- Wichnick-Gillis, A.  M., Vener, S.  M., & Poulson, C.  L.
ogy, and teaching children with autism spectrum dis- (2016). The effect of script-fading procedure on
orders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental social interactions among young children with autism.
Disabilities, 21, 14–24. Research is Autism Spectrum Disorders, 26, 1–9.
Tarbox, J., & Tarbox, C. (2017). Training manual for https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.03.004
behavior technicians working with individuals with Williams, C., Wright, B., Callaghan, G., & Coughlan, B.
autism. Academic Press. (2002). Do children with autism learn to read more
Thinesen, P. J., & Bryan, A. J. (1981). The use of sequen- readily by computer assisted instruction or traditional
tial pictorial cues in the initiation and maintenance of methods? A pilot study. Autism, 6, 71–91.
grooming behaviors with mentally retarded adults. Woods, J., & Poulson, C. L. (2006). The use of scripts to
Mental Retardation, 19, 246–250. increase the verbal initiations of children with devel-
Tichenor, J.  L. (1977). Self-monitoring and self-­ opmental disabilities to typically developing peers.
reinforcement of study by college students. Education and Treatment of Children, 29, 437–457.
Psychological Reports, 40, 103–108. Yamamoto, S., & Isawa, S. (2020). Using a script pro-
Touchette, P.  E., & Howard, S. (1984). Errorless learn- cedure without fading to increase novel behavior
ing: Reinforcement contingencies and stimulus con- in a conversation between children with autism.
trol transfer in delayed prompting. Journal of Applied Behavioral Interventions, 35, 192–199. https://doi.
Behavior Analysis, 17, 175–188. org/10.1002/bin.1699
Torres, R., DeBar, R.  M., Reeve, S.  A., Meyer, L.  S., Zane, T., Handen, B.  L., Mason, S.  A., & Geffin, C.
& Covington, T.  M. (2018). The effects of a video-­ (1984). Teaching symbol identification: A compari-
enhanced schedule on exercise behavior. Behavior son between standard prompting and intervening
Analysis in Practice, 11, 85–96. response procedures. Analysis and Intervention in
Developmental Disabilities, 4, 367–377.
Extinction and Differential
Reinforcement 28
Timothy R. Vollmer, Janelle K. Bacotti,
and Lindsay A. Lloveras

28.1 Introduction empirical evidence, is to maximize reinforcement


in such a way to favor appropriate alternative
In this chapter we describe extinction and differ- behavior while minimizing reinforcement for
ential reinforcement, primarily as elements of dangerous or destructive behavior, even when it
behavioral interventions for behavior disorders. is not technically placed on extinction (Vollmer
In the first section, we will describe extinction as et al., 2020).
an isolated procedure. However, a central theme
of this chapter is that extinction should rarely be
used in isolation. Among many reasons for this 28.2 Extinction
theme include the following: (a) extinction in iso-
lation has potential side effects, (b) pure extinc- 28.2.1 Overview
tion is often difficult or even impossible to
implement with fidelity, and (c) sometimes For the purposes of this chapter, extinction is
extinction does not address a primary variable defined as the withholding of a reinforcer that
associated with occurrence of the behavior (such was previously presented contingent on a
as when a medical or physical problem increases response, such that there is a decreased probabil-
the likelihood of escape behavior). When extinc- ity of that response (Catania, 2013; Cooper et al.,
tion is not implemented with fidelity, problem 2020). In the context of behavioral interventions,
behavior is intermittently reinforced, making it extinction usually involves withholding the
more resistant to change than ever. That leads to reinforcer(s) for problem behavior that has (have)
the second main section of the chapter, on differ- been identified via functional analysis (e.g.,
ential reinforcement. Another central theme of Iwata, Dorsey, et al., 1994), which subsequently
this chapter is that differential reinforcement is a results in a decrease and (ideally) elimination of
more logical behavioral intervention in compari- the problem behavior. Contrary to common
son to extinction in isolation. We describe vari- usage, extinction is not just “ignoring” problem
ants of differential reinforcement in the latter behavior, it is the withholding of the maintaining
section of the paper. A general premise, based on reinforcer for problem behavior. Because the
maintaining reinforcer can take many forms
T. R. Vollmer (*) · J. K. Bacotti · L. A. Lloveras (Kuhn et al., 1999; Richman et al., 1998), simply
Department of Psychology, University of Florida, “ignoring” could be incidental to the functional
Gainesville, FL, USA properties of behavior.
e-mail: vollmera@ufl.edu

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 539
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_28
540 T. R. Vollmer et al.

The two main components of the extinction withholding the particular form of attention. For
definition highlight that extinction is both a treat- example, if problem behavior is maintained by
ment procedure and a behavioral process (Iwata, reprimands, the behavior change agent would
Pace, et  al., 1994). Procedural extinction is the withhold reprimands if problem behavior
withholding of reinforcement previously pre- occurred.
sented contingent on a response. An example of As implied in the definition section, we rec-
procedural extinction is continuing to present an ommend that the term “ignore” should not be
instruction (i.e., not allowing escape) when used in the context of assessment and treatment
escape-maintained problem behavior occurs. of problem behavior, as it may imply to a lay
However, if problem behavior does not decrease audience that the behavior analyst is suggesting
(and is not eventually eliminated), then the that the behavior should not be monitored. To the
behavioral process of extinction did not occur. contrary, all individuals responsible for imple-
An outcome is required to meet the full definition menting an intervention involving extinction
of extinction. should carefully monitor a client who is engaging
in problem behavior, to ensure that everyone in
the environment is safe (including the person
28.2.2 Functional Variations engaging in problem behavior). A more appropri-
of Extinction ate characterization would be to provide minimal
differential consequences for the individual’s
One important feature of extinction as treatment behavior. Minimal differential consequences
for problem behavior is that it requires knowl- means that the problem behavior produces no (or
edge of the reinforcer maintaining the problem as little as possible) change in the therapist’s
behavior. As decades of functional analysis behavior while maintaining safety. For example,
research has shown, the same topography of if a care provider is attending to a household task
problem behavior could be reinforced by (say) when a child throws a toy (suppose that toy
attention for one individual but reinforced by throwing is maintained by reprimands), the care
(say) escape for another individual. The implica- provider would continue to engage in the house-
tions for interventions are significant (Iwata, hold task and would not provide any differential
Pace, et  al., 1994), because interventions based consequences (i.e., reprimands) for the disrup-
on extinction cannot be developed by merely tion. There may be times when the behavior
observing the behavioral topography. requires some sort of physical intervention to
ensure the safety of the individual or others in the
28.2.2.1 S  ocially Mediated Positive environment. However, the reaction to attention-­
Reinforcement maintained behavior should be minimized as
Extinction of behavior maintained by socially much as possible.
mediated positive reinforcement involves with- Another variant of problem behavior main-
holding a positive reinforcer that was previously tained by socially mediated positive reinforce-
presented contingent on a response, such that ment is when behavior is reinforced by access to
withholding it decreases the probability of the tangibles such as toys, snacks, or activities
response. One example of this is extinction of (Beavers et al., 2013). In these cases, extinction
behavior maintained by attention (e.g., Fisher involves withholding the tangible item(s) that
et  al., 2004). Attention that reinforces problem was (or were) previously delivered contingent on
behavior can take many forms, such as soothing problem behavior. For example, if a child dis-
statements from a caregiver (Iwata, Dorsey, et al., plays problem behavior maintained by access to
1994), reprimands from a teacher (Iwata, Dorsey, an electronic tablet, one would withhold access
et al., 1994), peer attention (Northup et al., 1995), to the tablet that was previously given contingent
or even eye contact from a therapist (Kodak et al., upon problem behavior, which will result in a
2007). Extinction, in such cases, would involve decrease in that response (note that emotional
28  Extinction and Differential Reinforcement 541

side effects can be expected to occur, and this At times, instructional activity, self-care activ-
will be discussed shortly). ity, medical activity, and so on are aversive if it is
presented too frequently, for too long a duration,
28.2.2.2 S  ocially Mediated Negative or when the individual does not have a proper
Reinforcement skill set for compliance (Smith et al., 1995). One
Extinction for negatively reinforced problem approach to address this phenomenon is to imple-
behavior, often called escape extinction, involves ment escape extinction along with instructional/
continuing to present the activity or requirement demand fading (e.g., Zarcone et  al., 1993),
from which escape was previously delivered wherein the aversive event is presented gradually
­contingent on problem behavior (Cooper et  al., while extinction of escape behavior is in place.
2020). For example, if a student displays problem Often, a gradual presentation of the functionally
behavior maintained by escape from math aversive stimulation is combined with positive
instructions, math instructions would continue reinforcement, such as in the case of necessary
when instances of the problem behavior occur. medical procedures that cannot be avoided (e.g.,
This can be applied to a variety of contexts that Shabani & Fisher, 2006).
may be functionally aversive, such as academic
(instructional) demands, loud noises, or even the 28.2.2.3 Automatic Reinforcement
physical presence of certain individuals. Extinction can also be used in the treatment of
However, usage of escape extinction requires automatically reinforced problem behavior.
very careful ethical consideration. For example, Automatically reinforced behavior produces its
if a student is engaging in escape behavior in the own source of reinforcement, independent of the
presence of instructional demands, a behavior social environment (Vollmer, 1994). Extinction
analyst should evaluate possible reasons that the in this case involves either altering the properties
instructional demands are aversive (Carr & of the response so that they no longer produce the
Smith, 1995; Kennedy & Meyer, 1996; Smith reinforcer or blocking the stimulation produced
et al., 1995). It is possible the individual does not by the behavior (e.g., Rincover et al., 1979). For
have the skill in their repertoire, in which case example, if disruption in the form of toy throwing
continued presentation of the demand does not is maintained by the sound that the toys make
make sense from an ethical or clinical standpoint, when they hit the wall, one could alter the wall by
unless the skill is being taught in some other way. covering it with a pad so that the toys no longer
Similarly, a loud ambient noise may be distract- make the noise when thrown. Extinction of auto-
ing or even painful to a particular individual. matically reinforced behavior is sometimes more
Keeping the person in the environment for the difficult to implement than extinction of socially
purposes of extinction, then, may not address the reinforced behavior. This difficulty arises from
ultimate cause of the behavior (such as an audi- the fact that, by definition, automatically rein-
tory sensitivity). forced behavior produces its own source of rein-
A notable example of the effectiveness of forcement. Thus, the specific stimulus features of
escape extinction is in the treatment of pediatric the reinforcer(s) may not be detectible or other-
feeding disorders, specifically food refusal. wise controlled.
Escape extinction, or non-removal of the spoon,
has been shown to produce increases in bite
acceptance (e.g., Ahearn et  al., 1996; Peterson 28.2.3 Limitations and Special
et al., 2016; Piazza et al., 2003). It is noteworthy Considerations
that successful escape extinction procedures for
pediatric feeding disorders are an outcome of an Extinction should rarely if ever be presented in
exploratory process wherein potential physical or isolation, without the use of differential rein-
medical impediments are addressed first or in forcement, environmental enrichment, or non-
conjunction with extinction (Ibañez et al., 2020). contingent reinforcement. Extinction is limited
542 T. R. Vollmer et al.

as an isolated procedure because it can produce must be reinforced because it is too dangerous.
side effects that are attenuated when combined This is an example of inadvertent shaping of
with these other (reinforcement-based) proce- problem behavior intensity; the intense problem
dures (Lerman & Iwata, 1995). Further, in some behavior that contacted reinforcement will be
circumstances, extinction is difficult if not impos- more likely to occur in the future (Fahmie et al.,
sible to implement with fidelity, which creates a 2017). A common example of extinction-induced
host of problems, not the least of which is contin- problem behavior is aggression, often toward the
ued (possibly intermittent) reinforcement of the person implementing extinction. Withholding
problem behavior (Vollmer et al., 2020). Also, if reinforcement can be an aversive event, so it is
extinction is implemented without consideration not surprising that aggression occurs toward the
of other contributing variables, the procedure can individual who withheld the reinforcement
be unethical. For example, if someone is required (Lerman et al., 1999). In fact, basic research on
to take a bite of food, but they do not have the aggression has shown that both presentation of
skill to swallow the food, procedural extinction aversive stimulation (as seen in escape extinc-
would be ineffective at the least and harmful in tion) and reinforcer loss/withholding (as seen in
many cases (Ibañez et  al., 2020). We describe extinction of positively reinforced behavior) can
these general limitations and considerations next. induce aggressive behavior, including but not
limited to biting of the self or others (Hutchinson,
28.2.3.1 Side Effects 1977).
One of the common side effects of extinction has Extinction can also produce emotional
been referred to as an extinction burst (Lerman responding (Lerman & Iwata, 1996b). Individuals
et al., 1999; Lerman & Iwata, 1995). An extinc- may cry, scream, or say unkind things to the ther-
tion burst is an increase in the frequency, dura- apist or caregiver. Induced emotional responding
tion, or intensity of behavior that has been placed poses additional challenges, and collectively the
on extinction (Lerman & Iwata, 1995). In some potential side effects of extinction make extinc-
cases, the burst can be relatively minor, but in tion difficult to implement without other treat-
other cases, problem behavior can rise to danger- ment components in place. Further, some
ously high levels. Although the extinction burst is potential implementers of extinction may find it
usually temporary and decreases over time as the unacceptable (Ducharme & Van Houten, 1994),
behavior continues to encounter extinction, the and such unacceptability equates to poor social
initially increased frequency or intensity can put validity (Wolf, 1978). The side effects of extinc-
the client or therapist at significant risk. Extinction tion can be attenuated by combining the proce-
bursts can be difficult or even unacceptable dure with reinforcement-based procedures
depending on the resources of the environment, (Lerman & Iwata, 1996b), which will be dis-
especially if the burst is prolonged. cussed shortly.
Related to extinction bursts, extinction can
also induce other types of responses. These other 28.2.3.2 Feasibility
responses can be desirable (e.g., novel communi- There are several reasons that pure extinction in
cation responses) or undesirable (e.g., other isolation may not be practical or feasible, and
topographies of problem behavior). One can use when the procedure is not practical or feasible,
the desirable effects to advantage when shaping implementers make mistakes and sometimes
new responses and extinguishing previously rein- continue to reinforce problem behavior. As a
forced approximations. However, in cases when result, a schedule intended as extinction may
undesirable responses are induced, problems can actually be an intermittent schedule of reinforce-
arise. It might be the case that new problem ment for the problem behavior. Some of the rea-
behavior occurs that is more intense than the sons that pure extinction may not be feasible
behavior that is placed on extinction and, thus, include (but are not limited to) the following:
28  Extinction and Differential Reinforcement 543

1. The client may be too large and strong, such least one dimension such as higher rate of rein-
that physical guidance is not possible or forcement, shorter delay to reinforcement, greater
potentially dangerous. quality of reinforcement, higher quality of rein-
2. The client may be elusive, such that physical forcement, and greater magnitude of reinforce-
guidance is not possible. ment (e.g., Athens & Vollmer, 2010).
3. It may be too dangerous to withhold response
blocking (such as for some SIB, elopement, or 28.2.3.3 Root Cause
aggression) even when it is known that physi- It is critical to identify the operant contingencies
cal contact is a reinforcer for a given client’s of reinforcement maintaining problem behavior
SIB or aggression. in order to implement extinction. However, it is
4. There may be laws or guidelines against the not enough to simply identify those contingen-
use of physical guidance during escape cies when addressing severe behavior disorders.
extinction. Medical and physical variables can interact with
5. There may be laws or guidelines requiring operant contingencies in such a way that problem
response blocking for SIB. behavior is exacerbated. For example, if a child
6. Even if there are no specific laws, ethical has difficulty swallowing, they may develop
guidelines or personal ethics may lead practi- escape behavior in the context of mealtime or
tioners to opt against physical guidance or to food presentation (Ibañez et al., 2020). Similarly,
protect the individual through response there is some evidence that physiological factors
blocking. such as allergies (Kennedy & Meyer, 1996),
7. The outcome of some behavior (such as fatigue (Smith et al., 2016), menstrual cycle (Carr
observable injury or even unobservable et  al., 2003), and illness (Carr & Owen-­
injury) may require medical consultation, DeSchryver, 2007) could exacerbate dangerous
which is not available in all settings. behavior of the sort that is commonly maintained
8. If the behavior is automatically reinforced, the by operant contingencies. Withholding the source
specific form of the reinforcer may not be of reinforcement without addressing the medical
known or if known may not be easily or physical problem has serious ethical implica-
controlled. tions (Behavior Analyst Certification Board,
9. Even if primary care providers are expertly 2014). Consider an extreme example: a client
coached to implement extinction, the individ- displays escape behavior in the context of instruc-
ual is likely to encounter many other people tions to put on their socks and shoes and to begin
who are not, and therefore the behavior is walking. Suppose that, unbeknownst to the thera-
accidentally reinforced (e.g., by grandparents, pist, the client has a badly bruised (or possibly
siblings, family friends, school personnel). broken) toe. If a therapist moves directly to
Further, even expertly coached care providers extinction and persists with extinction, the actual
will make at least some integrity errors reason for escape behavior is not addressed: put-
(Marcus et al., 2001). ting on footwear and walking are aversive
because there is an underlying medical problem.
Combining extinction with differential rein- At times this “root cause” issue is more subtle.
forcement can reduce some of the above prob- For example, a student may find reading aloud in a
lems, because research on concurrent schedules classroom to be aversive (Hofstadter-Duke &
shows that weighting reinforcement to favor Daly, 2011) and therefore displays severe escape
appropriate behavior versus problem behavior behavior, reinforced by being sent out of the class-
will shift allocation toward appropriate behavior, room. If a functional analysis shows that the severe
even if problem behavior is still reinforced behavior is maintained by escape, a literal inter-
(Athens & Vollmer, 2010; Vollmer et al., 2020). pretation of extinction would involve continuing to
The key strategy is to ensure that appropriate require the student to read aloud in the classroom.
behavior produces greater reinforcement along at However, it is possible that the student does not
544 T. R. Vollmer et al.

know how to read or is several grade levels behind when problem behavior occurs. However, this
other students. The persistent requirement to read intervention alone is unlikely to produce a com-
aloud does not address the core of the problem, plete reduction in problem behavior, especially in
which would require individualized instruction on the absence of skill acquisition procedures
reading, and probably reflects an ethical shortcom- designed to increase the client’s adaptive
ing in application (consider, for example, the repertoire.
humiliation the student might experience). Relating to escape extinction, we have empha-
sized the importance of careful exploration of
why a certain event or set of events functions as
28.2.4 Using Extinction in Practice aversive stimuli. Blanket usage of escape extinc-
tion without detailed exploration and analysis at
Extinction can be an effective and useful tool to multiple levels has serious ethical implications. It
decrease problem behavior. Extinction can is critical to understand why the event or events
make some treatments more effective and can are aversive. Some examples of such consider-
also decrease problem behavior when other ations are listed here, but this list is by no means
treatments have not worked (Rooker et  al., exhaustive: (a) the activity produces some sort of
2013). Extinction has also produced impactful pain state for the individual, (b) the individual
effects on the field in some critical areas such as does not have the necessary skills in their reper-
pediatric feeding disorders (e.g., Peterson et al., toire, or (c) the individual is experiencing physi-
2016). Despite its apparent effectiveness as a cal limitations (such as difficulty swallowing or
treatment for problem behavior, as we have dis- grasping).
cussed, extinction is very rarely used in isola- In short, due to potential side effects, extinc-
tion. Extinction has been used in the context of tion should be combined with procedures involv-
noncontingent reinforcement (Fisher et  al., ing reinforcement. Relatedly, due to feasibility
2004; Reed et  al., 2004; Saini et  al., 2017), concerns wherein it is difficult and sometimes
instructional fading (Zarcone et al., 1993), and impossible to implement extinction perfectly
differential reinforcement of alternative behav- (and, hence, an intermittent schedule of rein-
ior (DRA; Piazza et  al., 2003), among other forcement for problem behavior is in place), it is
procedures. Extinction is almost always used in important to minimize reinforcement for problem
combination with another procedure because of behavior while maximizing reinforcement for
the importance of skill acquisition in the con- alternative behavior along as many dimensions of
text of behavior reduction, the side effects asso- reinforcement as possible (e.g., rate, duration,
ciated with extinction, and the practical immediacy, quality). Further, extinction should
limitations of applying extinction procedures only be considered after other contributing vari-
(and resulting problems associated with extinc- ables have been identified, not only the maintain-
tion failures). ing reinforcers. A functional analysis is a first
We have suggested that when treating problem step, but an evaluation of medical variables,
behavior for individuals diagnosed with autism instructional context, and skill level is equally
spectrum disorder (ASD)/intellectually and critical.
developmental disabilities (IDD), it may be use-
ful to provide minimized differential conse-
quences for problem behavior. As previously 28.3 Differential Reinforcement
described, minimized differential consequences
means that the problem behavior produces no 28.3.1 Overview and Forms
change in the therapist’s behavior (other than of Differential Reinforcement
what is necessary to protect the individual, others
in the environment, or property). To the best of Differential reinforcement is one of the most
the therapist’s ability (and safety permitting), the commonly used behavior change procedures
therapist should minimize environmental changes (MacNaul & Neely, 2018; Petscher et al., 2009;
28  Extinction and Differential Reinforcement 545

Vollmer et  al., 1999; Weston et  al., 2018). involves modifying parameters of reinforcement
Differential reinforcement is typically defined as such that the alternative response receives greater
reinforcing some response(s) and not reinforcing reinforcement than another response (for the pur-
other responses (Catania, 2013; Cooper et  al., poses of this discussion, problem behavior). In
2020; DeLeon et  al., 2013; Vollmer & Iwata, other words, DRA need not be constrained to
1992). When defined in this way, however, dif- explicit reinforcement of a target response and
ferential reinforcement is procedurally con- extinction for the problem behavior (as previ-
strained to the use of reinforcement and ously defined by Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). When
extinction. Although extinction is a common DRA is implemented, even without perfect
component when implementing differential rein- extinction, robust effects can still be obtained
forcement procedures, many successful applica- when treatment integrity failures occur because
tions have occurred without pure extinction the schedule of reinforcement favors appropriate
(review Trump et  al., 2019). Implementing dif- behavior (e.g., Athens & Vollmer, 2010; Brand
ferential reinforcement can be viewed as a et al., 2019).
concurrent-­operant arrangement that involves Sometimes DRA procedures are labeled based
applying different schedules of reinforcement to on the type of alternative behavior that is rein-
two or more responses (Fisher & Mazur, 1997). forced. One such example is differential rein-
In other words, it is accurate to view differential forcement of incompatible behavior (DRI). DRI
reinforcement as any procedure that involves two involves selecting an alternative response that is
or more schedules of reinforcement that vary physically incompatible with the target behavior
along some dimension (e.g., reinforcer duration, selected for decrease (e.g., Young & Wincze,
reinforcer quality, delay to reinforcement) across 1974). Another procedural variant is functional
different responses, whereby response allocation communication training (FCT), wherein the
favors the programmed schedules of reinforce- alternative response is always some form of com-
ment (Athens & Vollmer, 2010). Several proce- munication (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985).
dural variations of differential reinforcement The DRA approach is also a key component
exist; however, the most common differential for establishing new skills in an individual’s rep-
reinforcement procedures are differential rein- ertoire. DRA plays an essential role in shaping
forcement of alternative behavior, differential new responses or differentially reinforcing suc-
reinforcement of other behavior, and differential cessive approximations to a terminal response.
reinforcement of low rate behavior (Cooper et al., For example, a therapist might reinforce succes-
2020; Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). sive approximations to the word “tunes” as a
mand for music (e.g., Bourret et al., 2004). The
28.3.1.1 Differential Reinforcement vocal utterance “t-” is followed by a positive
of Alternative Behavior reinforcer, but then placed on extinction once a
DRA is the most commonly used differential closer approximation “tu-” contacts reinforce-
reinforcement procedure (Petscher et al., 2009). ment. This process would continue until the ter-
Traditionally, as it relates to treating problem minal goal of “tunes” is achieved.
behavior, DRA has been described as reinforcing
some specific alternative behavior, while placing 28.3.1.2 Differential Reinforcement
problem behavior on extinction (Vollmer & of Other Behavior
Iwata, 1992). A more recent definition, which Differential reinforcement of other behavior
takes into account the problems associated with (DRO) involves delivering a reinforcer when a
implementing pure extinction, describes DRA as target response does not occur during a specified
“providing greater reinforcement, along at least observation period (Catania, 2013; Reynolds,
one dimension, contingent on the occurrence of 1961). DRO is sometimes referred to as omission
one form or type of behavior, while minimizing training (Uhl & Garcia, 1969) or differential
reinforcement for another form or type of behav- reinforcement of no responding (e.g., Poling &
ior” (Vollmer et al., 2020, p. 1300). Thus, DRA Ryan, 1982). The contingencies of a DRO may
546 T. R. Vollmer et al.

involve a reset (i.e., timer restart) or no reset (i.e., Kahng, 1999, Lindberg, Iwata, Kahng, &
no timer restart) of the interval when the target DeLeon, 1999) described and compared the
response occurs. If a reinforcer unrelated to the effects of fixed interval, variable interval, and
function of behavior is used, the implementation variable-momentary DRO on rates of self-injury.
of DRO involves a procedural extinction compo- Fixed interval DRO involves a constant interval
nent (i.e., withholding a positive reinforcer duration. Lindberg et  al. withheld functional
­unrelated to the function of problem behavior). If reinforcers when self-injury occurred and pro-
the reinforcer maintaining problem behavior is vided functional reinforcers when self-injury did
used, the implementation of DRO involves func- not occur during a constant time interval speci-
tional extinction (i.e., withholding the reinforcer fied for each session. For example, if self-injury
identified to maintain problem behavior). (the target response) did not occur during a 10 s
At least four potential underlying mechanisms interval, then an edible (positive reinforcer) was
for the effectiveness of DRO have been proposed: delivered; if self-injury occurred during the inter-
(1) repeated delivery of the reinforcer may serve val, then no reinforcer was delivered. Variable
as an abolishing operation that momentarily sup- interval DRO has varied interval durations, based
presses the target response, (2) extinction, (3) on an average value. Lindberg et al. administered
negative punishment (because scheduled rein- the same procedures as described in the fixed
forcers are, in a sense, “lost” contingent on the interval DRO condition, except the interval
occurrence of behavior), and (4) the strengthen- lengths varied. For the variable-momentary DRO
ing of alternative responses due to adventitious condition, Lindberg et al. withheld the functional
reinforcement (Jessel & Ingvarsson, 2016; Poling reinforcer only if self-injury occurred at the end
& Ryan, 1982). A DRO contingency indicates of a specified interval. Thus, the functional rein-
when reinforcement is delivered based on the forcer was delivered if self-injury was not occur-
interresponse times (IRTs) that are either equal to ring at the end of the interval (i.e., self-injury
or greater than the specified interval length (as could occur at other times during the interval).
described by Lindberg, Iwata, Kahng, 1999; All three variations of DRO (fixed interval, vari-
Lindberg, Iwata, Kahng, & DeLeon, 1999). able interval, and variable-momentary) were
Commonly, DRO interval lengths are determined equally effective in reducing self-injury main-
by calculating the mean IRT for a specified num- tained by social-positive reinforcement.
ber of sessions (Poling & Ryan, 1982) to system-
atically establish and then to thin the 28.3.1.3 Differential Reinforcement
reinforcement schedule. There are two primary of Low Rate Responding
procedural variations of DRO: interval DRO and Differential reinforcement of low rate respond-
momentary DRO. ing (DRL) involves delivering a reinforcer for
For both procedural variations of DRO, there low rates of behavior, rather than total response
is a specified interval that requires either continu- suppression (Ferster & Skinner, 1957).
ous (interval) or discontinuous (momentary) Sometimes the goal is to maintain the behavior
observation; a reinforcer is delivered contingent at low rates or slowly decrease the response cri-
on the absence of the target response. Interval terion, rather than the total elimination of the
DRO involves continuous observation of the tar- response. Thus, this procedure is particularly
get response during a specified interval (which useful when targeting behavior that should be
can remain constant, vary, or progressively maintained but is perhaps occurring too fre-
increase) and then delivering the reinforcer if the quently or rapidly. Similar to DRO, IRT is a rel-
target response does not occur at any point during evant measure when implementing DRL (as
the interval. Momentary DRO involves delivering described below). The three primary procedural
a reinforcer if the target response does not occur variations of DRL include full session, interval,
at the end of the interval (or the exact “moment” and spaced responding (Becraft et  al., 2017;
of observation). Lindberg et al. (Lindberg, Iwata, Deitz, 1977).
28  Extinction and Differential Reinforcement 547

For all the procedural variations of DRL, there extinction of self-feeding or classroom participa-
is a specified observation period during which a tion is not the goal. Thus, this procedure’s utility
predetermined criterion of (low) responding must mainly relies on selecting responses that should
be met for a reinforcer to be delivered. Full-­ persist at a socially valid or a medically safe
session DRL involves delivering a reinforcer fol- level.
lowing a full session (e.g., treatment session, The DRL approach is considered a time-­
appointment, observation window) during which intensive procedure (Cooper et  al., 2020).
the target response occurs at or below a predeter- Practitioners can choose this procedure when the
mined criterion. Austin and Bevan (2011) response does not require immediate response
observed elementary school-aged children during suppression and can withstand incremental
20-min classroom sessions and differentially changes. It is appropriate for responses that do
reinforced low rates of requests for attention not require complete elimination (e.g., reducing
from the teacher (e.g., hand raising, calling out rapid eating; Wright & Vollmer, 2002).
for the teachers). For example, on average, one Practitioners should aim to prevent (i.e., imple-
student requested her teacher’s attention nine ment safety procedures) or eliminate the occur-
times during baseline sessions; however, during rence of dangerous behavior that places the
the DRL condition, the teacher only delivered a individual or others at risk. The procedure is not
reinforcer if the student requested the teacher’s designed to gradually wean an individual off of
attention three or fewer times. Interval DRL problem behavior when the aim is complete
involves delivering a reinforcer when the target reduction. Although incremental change when
response occurs at or below a predetermined cri- treating severe behavior disorders is a possible
terion following a specified interval length. For outcome of behavioral treatment, practitioners
example, Deitz et al. (1977) observed disruptive should not deliberately plan for gradual progress
behavior during a 30-min session and divided the in these cases. The type of DRL selected for the
session into 2-min intervals. If disruptive behav- response depends on the terminal goal and sched-
ior occurred one or zero times during a 2-min ule of reinforcement required to produce an
interval, the student received a star. If the disrup- effect. For example, full-session DRL seems
tive behavior occurred more than once during the most useful when individuals can follow instruc-
interval, the interval was reset. The stars were tions (e.g., “if you only raise your hand three
exchangeable for playtime at the end of the ses- times, you can earn playtime.”), and the delivery
sion. Spaced responding DRL involves delivering of the preferred stimulus can be delayed. Interval
a reinforcer based on a predetermined IRT (i.e., a and spaced responding DRL might be useful
predetermined amount of time must pass between when the response necessitates a denser schedule
a response and a subsequent response; Deitz, of reinforcement. Spaced responding DRL, spe-
1977). For example, Lennox et  al. (1987) com- cifically, seems more useful when IRT is particu-
bined response interruption and spaced respond- larly important (e.g., seconds between bites).
ing DRL to increase the time between bites of
food (i.e., to reduce rapid eating). Any attempt to
have a bite of food before 15 s elapsed was inter- 28.3.2 Functional Variations
rupted by blocking; therefore, 15 consecutive of Differential Reinforcement
seconds were required to occur between bites of
food. Additionally, Becraft et  al. (2017) com- Functional variations of differential reinforce-
bined schedule-correlated stimuli and spaced ment include differential positive reinforcement,
responding DRL (and compared this condition to differential negative reinforcement, and differen-
full-session DRL and DRO), which reduced bids tial automatic reinforcement (Cooper et al., 2020;
for attention in a simulated classroom. In both Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). Although these proce-
examples, it is clear that the target responses dures can be applied as either DRA or DRO, we
must occur at some level. For example, complete will use primarily examples of DRA in our dis-
548 T. R. Vollmer et al.

cussion (for reasons that we will subsequently 28.3.2.2 Differential Negative


clarify relating to practical implementation of Reinforcement
differential reinforcement). Differential negative reinforcement is used to
treat behavior maintained by negative reinforce-
28.3.2.1 Differential Positive ment. The logic behind this approach is that by
Reinforcement providing escape or avoidance contingent on
Most commonly, differential positive reinforce- alternative behavior (such as compliance, func-
ment is used to treat behavior maintained by pos- tional communication), the alternative behavior
itive reinforcement, such as attention or tangibles functionally replaces the problem behavior,
(e.g., Pizarro et al., 2021). The logic behind this which would be placed on extinction or other-
approach is that if the alternative behavior pro- wise produce minimal escape. An example of this
duces the reinforcer previously maintaining approach involves providing a 60s break from
problem behavior, the alternative behavior func- instructions contingent on compliance and deliv-
tionally replaces the problem behavior, which ery of another directive contingent on problem
either is placed on extinction or otherwise pro- behavior (e.g., Ringdahl et  al., 2002).
duces a minimal outcome. An example of this Alternatively, differential escape intervals (240 s
approach involves varying the duration, quality, break following compliance, 10 s break follow-
or delay when accessing positive reinforcers con- ing problem behavior) can increase compliance
tingent on problem behavior or alternative behav- and reduce problem behavior (Rogalski et  al.,
ior (Athens & Vollmer, 2010). For example, 2020).
Athens and Vollmer (2010) provided qualitatively
different forms of attention contingent on aggres- 28.3.2.3 Differential Automatic
sion (reprimands) and exchanging a picture card Reinforcement
to obtain an adult’s attention (praise and physical Differential automatic reinforcement is most
interaction). commonly used to treat behavior maintained by
Another application of differential positive automatic reinforcement (reinforcement not
reinforcement is to use positive reinforcement delivered via social mediation). The logic of this
even when behavior is maintained by negative approach is that by bringing alternative behavior
reinforcement (Lalli et  al., 1999). The logic into contact with alternative sources of reinforce-
behind this approach is that the use of positive ment (e.g., toy play, music, activity), it will func-
reinforcement may reduce the aversiveness of tionally replace at least some amount of problem
the instructional context, and the positive rein- behavior. Because the problem behavior pro-
forcement for behavior such as compliance duces its own source of reinforcement, it is some-
with instructional activity might compete with times difficult to minimize that source of
the negative reinforcement in the form of escape reinforcement. As a result, researchers have
(i.e., if it is a higher-quality reinforcer). For examined an approach known as a competing
example, Slocum and Vollmer (2015) compared stimulus assessment (see Haddock & Hagopian,
the effects of providing escape (the reinforcer 2020). In a competing stimulus assessment, one
maintaining problem behavior) and edibles can evaluate (a) whether a stimulus is highly pre-
(reinforcers previously unrelated to problem ferred, as indicated by high levels of engagement,
behavior) contingent on compliance. During and (b) whether engagement with a stimulus sup-
both treatments, the problem behavior contin- presses instances of the problem behavior, as
ued to produce escape. The results demon- indicated by low levels of problem behavior
strated that problem behavior decreased more when the item is available (Haddock & Hagopian,
substantially, and compliance increased more 2020).
substantially in the condition where compli- Differential automatic reinforcement, even
ance was followed by positive reinforcement when based on a competing stimulus assessment,
(edible delivery). may require some additional components. One is
28  Extinction and Differential Reinforcement 549

that some individuals with automatically rein- disorders: those that are problematic only because
forced problem behavior do not have repertoires the behavior occurs too frequently. Thus, DRL is
that bring them into contact with appropriate most commonly used for behavior such as rapid
sources of automatic reinforcement, such as play eating, talking out in class, and other topogra-
skills. As a result, it is sometimes critical to phies that should not be extinguished entirely. As
explicitly teach a skill or set of skills that ulti- a result, most general types of behavior disorders
mately produces automatic reinforcement (e.g., are not treated using DRL.  DRO is limited
Britton et al., 2002; Leif et al., 2020). Another is because it is highly sensitive to treatment integ-
that, for some individuals, engagement with rity failures in the form errors of commission
highly preferred items does not necessarily sup- (e.g., Mazaleski et al., 1993). For example, even
press the occurrence of problem behavior (e.g., if someone refrains from reinforcing problem
review Gover et  al., 2019; Lindberg, Iwata, behavior 95% of the time it occurs (which sounds
Kahng, 1999; Lindberg, Iwata, Kahng, & on the surface like good integrity), the problem
DeLeon, 1999; Piazza et  al., 1998; Ringdahl behavior is still reinforced on a variable ratio
et  al., 1997). As a result, differential automatic (VR) 20 schedule. A VR 20 schedule of rein-
reinforcement is sometimes combined with forcement could easily sustain high levels of
response blocking (e.g., Lerman & Iwata, 1996a; behavior for some individuals. Further, DRO
Lindberg, Iwata, Kahng, & DeLeon, 1999; does not explicitly arrange for reinforcement of
Lindberg, Iwata, Kahng, 1999; Roscoe et  al., alternative behavior, so it is not always clear
2013) or response interruption (e.g., Gibbs et al., which behavior is being reinforced. As a result of
2018; Shawler et  al., 2020). Examples of these these limitations and special considerations,
problems and potential solutions can be seen in implementation in practice would focus largely
Vollmer et al. (1994). Three children participated on DRL in restricted circumstances, DRO prob-
in the study. One child displayed SIB that was ably only in conjunction with reinforcement of
entirely replaced by toy play with a preferred toy. new or alternative skills, and nearly continuous
A second child required explicit reinforcement of application of DRA-like contingencies through-
toy contact to learn play skills that subsequently out an individual’s daily routine (Vollmer et al.,
competed with SIB.  A third child also required 2020).
explicit reinforcement for toy contact but further
required a response blocking procedure to reduce
SIB to acceptable levels. 28.3.4 Using Differential
Reinforcement in Practice

28.3.3 Limitations and Special Our conclusion, based on the literature summa-


Considerations rized above, is that DRL and DRO are valuable
procedures but used in special circumstances and
Differential reinforcement procedures can be as adjuncts to DRA.  To the contrary, DRA is a
limited in ways similar to how extinction proce- general “lifestyle” of interactions between a care
dures are limited (see the list above under limita- provider and an individual. By translating the
tions and special considerations for extinction). interpretation of DRA expressed by Vollmer et al.
However, these limitations brought about by side (2020) into practice, DRA circumvents many of
effects, feasibility, and consideration of root the limitations of extinction and differential rein-
causes are less pronounced when using differen- forcement described previously. DRA is not
tial reinforcement because alternative means of restricted to placing one response on extinction
obtaining reinforcement are explicitly arranged and reinforcing another response. It is possible to
and taught. present greater reinforcement for alternative
It is important to note that DRL is limited to behavior even when problem behavior continues
use with a relatively restricted range of behavior to be reinforced (e.g., greater magnitude, higher
550 T. R. Vollmer et al.

quality, longer duration, more immediate). Also, Presenting DRA as a lifestyle that caregivers can
practitioners need not select one and only one integrate into their daily interactions with their
topography of alternative behavior to reinforce. child, family member, student, or client might
Appropriate behavior of all sorts (e.g., increase acceptability and, therefore, adherence
­communication, play skills, self-care skills, aca- to DRA as a treatment recommendation.
demic skills) can and should be richly reinforced It is also important to consider the use of dif-
to compete with the reinforcement schedules ferential positive reinforcement when treating
maintaining problem behavior. DRA is not only escape-maintained behavior. This approach is
well supported for the treatment of problem notable because a more commonly discussed
behavior but is also essential for establishing route to treating escape-maintained problem
skills (Grow & LeBlanc, 2013; Vladescu & behavior involves the use of differential negative
Kodak, 2010). reinforcement. Although differential escape
Because the DRA procedure does not neces- intervals (e.g., Rogalski et al., 2020) or teaching
sitate perfect execution to maintain treatment an individual to ask for a “break” can reduce
effects (e.g., Brand et al., 2019), treatment integ- problem behavior, there are some less favorable
rity errors become less detrimental as long as implications of adhering strictly to this “func-
DRA is implemented with high levels of integrity tional match” treatment approach. When the
at the onset of treatment (St. Peter Pipkin et al., demand context remains aversive, it precludes
2010; Vollmer et  al., 1999). More specifically, individuals from learning in more favorable con-
errors of omission (i.e., withholding reinforce- ditions and can potentially limit the rate of
ment for an alternative response) are less prob- engagement in learning activities. Further, the
lematic than errors of commission (i.e., arrangement essentially requires an acceptance
reinforcing problem behavior) or both errors in that instructional activity should be aversive,
combination (St. Peter Pipkin et  al., 2010). To which seems counterintuitive to good instruc-
this end, it is clear that DRA produces robust tional practices (e.g., review possible implica-
effects that maintain even in the face of at least tions for practice proposed by Haq & Aranki,
some treatment integrity failures. Thus, DRA is 2019). Thus, as a comprehensive treatment for
flexible enough to operate throughout the day as escape-maintained behavior, (a) features of the
a lifestyle, where differential schedules can be instructional context must be carefully examined
moderated loosely (so long as the schedules gen- to determine why instructional activity is aver-
erally favor appropriate or target responses). sive, (b) the instructional context should then be
Establishing an alternative response may modified or arranged such that it is less aversive,
require a dense schedule of reinforcement at the and (c) the use of differential positive reinforce-
outset of treatment (e.g., Greer et  al., 2016). ment is useful as it has been shown to engender
Thus, practitioners should plan for systematic less escape behavior even when problem behav-
schedule thinning to ensure that the alternative ior is not fully placed on extinction (e.g., Lalli
response is occurring at a rate that is feasible to et al., 1999; Slocum & Vollmer, 2015).
reinforce and to avoid the resurgence of problem Using DRA in combination with other proce-
behavior (e.g., see Hagopian et al., 2011). Finally, dures has also produced favorable results when
DRA does not require additional time expendi- targeting problem behavior maintained by auto-
ture (because it occurs in naturally occurring sit- matic reinforcement. Leif et al. (2020) identified
uations) or the use of gadgets (such as re-setting stimuli that could potentially compete with auto-
timers). Practitioners or caregivers might equate matically reinforced problem behavior (e.g.,
decreased time expenditure and decreased hand mouthing). However, item engagement was
“setup” with decreased response effort. Response relatively low when participants were provided
effort is a factor that practitioners often consider, with noncontingent access to different leisure
as it might impact caregiver’s adherence to treat- items and, therefore, problem behavior persisted.
ment recommendations (Allen & Warzak, 2000). Including prompting (i.e., vocal and physical
28  Extinction and Differential Reinforcement 551

support to interact with items) in conjunction of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 46(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.30
with DRA significantly increased item engage- Becraft, J.  L., Borrero, J.  C., Mendres-Smith, A.  E., &
ment, which permitted the identification of Castillo, M.  I. (2017). Decreasing excessive bids for
­multiple competing stimuli. In this case, simply attention in a simulated early education classroom.
providing positive reinforcers (edibles) contin- Journal of Behavioral Education, 26(4), 371–393.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­017-­9275-­6
gent on 10 s of manipulating an item established Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2014).
a sustained item engagement repertoire, which Professional and ethical compliance code for behav-
permitted identifying stimuli that successfully ior analysts. Author.
suppressed problem behavior. Bourret, J., Vollmer, T. V., & Rapp, J. T. (2004). Evaluation
of a vocal mand assessment and vocal mand train-
ing procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 37(2), 129–144. https://doi.org/10.1901/
28.4 Conclusions jaba.2004.37-­129
Brand, D., Henley, A.  J., DiGennaro Reed, F.  D., Gray,
E., & Crabbs, B. (2019). A review of published stud-
Extinction and differential reinforcement are ies involving parametric manipulations of treatment
central procedures and processes that have been integrity. Journal of Behavioral Education, 28(1),
tested and used effectively for many years. 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-­018-­09311-­8
Extinction presented in isolation can create a Britton, L. N., Carr, J. E., Landaburu, H. J., & Romick,
K. S. (2002). The efficacy of noncontingent reinforce-
range of practical and even ethical problems. By ment as treatment for automatically reinforced stereo-
combining extinction and reinforcement (i.e., dif- typy. Behavioral Interventions, 17, 93–103. https://
ferential reinforcement), many of these problems doi.org/10.1002/bin.110
and limitations associated with extinction can be Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior
problems through functional communication training.
circumvented. We have ultimately concluded that Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(2), 111–126.
a general differential reinforcement approach, in https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­111
which reinforcement for appropriate behavior is Carr, E.  G., & Owen-Deschryver, J.  S. (2007). Physical
presented richly and reinforcement for problem illness, pain, and problem behavior in minimally ver-
bal people with developmental disabilities. Journal
behavior is minimized, is best practice. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 413–
424. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­006-­0176-­0
Carr, E.  G., & Smith, C.  E. (1995). Biological set-
References ting events for self-injury. Mental Retardation and
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 1(2),
94–98. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.1410010204
Ahearn, W.  H., Kerwin, M.  E., Eicher, P.  S., Shantz, J.,
Carr, E. G., Smith, C. E., Giacin, T. A., Whelan, B. M.,
& Swearingin, W. (1996). An alternating treatments
& Pancari, J. (2003). Menstrual discomfort as a bio-
comparison of two intensive interventions for food
logical setting event for severe problem behavior:
refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(3),
Assessment and intervention. American Journal of
321–332. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-­321
Mental Retardation: AJMR, 108(2), 117–133. https://
Allen, K.  D., & Warzak, W.  J. (2000). The problem of
doi.org/10.1352/0895-­8017(2003)1082.0.CO;2
parental nonadherence in clinical behavior analysis:
Catania, A. C. (2013). Learning. Ny Sloan Publishing.
Effective treatment is not enough. Journal of Applied
Cooper, J., Heron, T., & Heward, W. (2020). Applied
Behavior Analysis, 33(3), 373–391. https://doi.
behavior analysis. Pearson. (ISBN-13: 978-­
org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-­373
0134752556; ISBN-10: 0134752554).
Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An investigation
Deitz, S.  M. (1977). An analysis of programming
of differential reinforcement of alternative behav-
DRL schedules in educational settings. Behaviour
ior without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior
Research and Therapy, 15(1), 103–111. https://doi.
Analysis, 43(4), 569–589. https://doi.org/10.1901/
org/10.1016/0005-­7967(77)90093-­6
jaba.2010.43-­569
DeLeon, I., Bullock, C., & Catania, A. (2013). Arranging
Austin, J. L., & Bevan, D. (2011). Using differential rein-
reinforcement contingencies in applied settings:
forcement of low rates to reduce children’s requests
Fundamentals and implications of recent basic and
for teacher attention. Journal of Applied Behavior
applied research. Translating Principles into Practice,
Analysis, 44(3), 451–461. https://doi.org/10.1901/
2, 47–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/13938-­003
jaba.2011.44-­451
Ducharme, J.  M., & van Houten, R. (1994). Operant
Beavers, G.  A., Iwata, B.  A., & Lerman, D.  C. (2013).
extinction in the treatment of severe maladaptive
Thirty years of research on the functional analysis
552 T. R. Vollmer et al.

behavior. Behavior Modification, 18(2), 139–170. Hutchinson, R. R. (1977). By-products of aversive control.
https://doi.org/10.1177/01454455940182001 In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook
Fahmie, T.  A., Macaskill, A.  C., Kazemi, E., & Elmer, of operant behavior (pp. 415–431). Prentice-Hall.
U.  C. (2017). Prevention of the development of Ibañez, V. F., Peterson, K. M., Crowley, J. G., Haney, S. D.,
problem behavior: A laboratory model. Journal of Andersen, A. S., & Piazza, C. C. (2020). Pediatric pre-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(1), 25–39. https://doi. vention: Feeding disorders. Pediatric Clinics of North
org/10.1002/jaba.426 America, 67(3), 451–467. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Ferster, C.  B., & Skinner, B.  F. (1957). Schedules of pcl.2020.02.003
reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts. https://doi. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
org/10.1037/10627-­000 & Richman, G.  S. (1994). Toward a functional
Fisher, W.  W., DeLeon, I.  G., Rodriguez-Catter, V., & analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
Keeney, K.  M. (2004). Enhancing the effects of Analysis, 27(2), 197–209. https://doi.org/10.1901/
extinction on attention-maintained behavior through jaba.1994.27-­197
non-contingent delivery of attention or stimuli identi- Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
fied via a competing stimulus assessment. Journal of R.  G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An
Applied Behavior Analysis, 37(2), 171–184. https:// analysis of procedural form and function. Journal of
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2004.37-­171 Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(1), 131–144. https://
Fisher, W. W., & Mazur, J. E. (1997). Basic and applied doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-­131
research on choice responding. Journal of Applied Jessel, J., & Ingvarsson, E.  T. (2016). Recent advances
Behavior Analysis, 30(3), 387–410. https://doi. in applied research on DRO procedures. Journal of
org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­387 Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(4), 991–995. https://
Gibbs, A.  R., Tullis, C.  A., Thomas, R., & Elkins, B. doi.org/10.1002/jaba.323
(2018). The effects of noncontingent music and Kennedy, C. H., & Meyer, K. A. (1996). Sleep deprivation,
response interruption and redirection on vocal ste- allergy symptoms, and negatively reinforced problem
reotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(4), behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(1),
899–914. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.485 133–135. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-­133
Gover, H. C., Fahmie, T. A., & McKeown, C. A. (2019). A Kodak, T., Northup, J., & Kelley, M. E. (2007). An evalu-
review of environmental enrichment as treatment for ation of the types of attention that maintain problem
problem behavior maintained by automatic reinforce- behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1),
ment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(1), 167–171. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.43-­06
299–314. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.508 Kuhn, D. E., DeLeon, I. G., Fisher, W. W., & Wilke, A. E.
Greer, B.  D., Fisher, W.  W., Saini, V., Owen, T.  M., & (1999). Clarifying an ambiguous functional analysis
Jones, J. K. (2016). Improving functional communica- with matched and mismatched extinction procedures.
tion training during reinforcement schedule thinning: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), 99–102.
An analysis of 25 applications. Journal of Applied https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­99
Behavior Analysis, 49(1), 105–121. https://doi. Lalli, J.  S., Vollmer, T.  R., Progar, P.  R., Wright, C.,
org/10.1002/jaba.265 Borrero, J., Daniel, D., Hoffner Barthold, C., Tocco,
Grow, L.  L., & LeBlanc, L.  A. (2013). Teaching recep- K., & May, W. (1999). Competition between positive
tive language skills: Recommendations for instructors. and negative reinforcement in the treatment of escape
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 6, 56–75. https://doi. behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(3),
org/10.1007/BF03391791 285–296. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999/32-­285
Haddock, J.  N., & Hagopian, L.  P. (2020). Competing Leif, E.  S., Roscoe, E.  M., Ahearn, W.  H., Rogalski,
stimulus assessments: A systematic review. Journal of J. P., & Morrison, H. (2020). Increasing item engage-
Applied Behasummary of 21 inpatient cases. Journal ment and decreasing automatically reinforced prob-
of Appliedior Analysis, 53(4), 1982–2001. https://doi. lem behavior within a modified competing stimulus
org/10.1002/jaba.754 assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Hagopian, L.  P., Boelter, E.  W., & Jarmolowicz, D.  P. 53(3), 1638–1659. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.695
(2011). Reinforcement schedule thinning following Lennox, D.  B., Miltenberger, R.  G., & Donnelly, D.  R.
functional communication training: Review and rec- (1987). Response interruption and DRL for the reduc-
ommendations. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 4(1), tion of rapid eating. Journal of Applied Behavior
4–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391770 Analysis, 20(3), 279–284. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Haq, S.  S., & Aranki, J. (2019). Comparison of tradi- jaba.1987.20-­279
tional and embedded DTT on problem behavior and Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1995). Prevalence of the
responding to instructional targets. Behavior Analysis extinction burst and its attenuation during treatment.
in Practice, 12, 366–400. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(1), 93–94.
s40617-­018-­00324-­3 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-­93
Hofstadter-Duke, K. L., & Daly, E. J. (2011). Improving Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996a). A methodology
oral reading fluency with a peer-mediated interven- for distinguishing between extinction and punishment
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(3), effects associated with response blocking. Journal of
641–646. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­641
28  Extinction and Differential Reinforcement 553

Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(2), 231–233. https:// its reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-­231 Analysis, 31(2), 165–189. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Lerman, D.  C., & Iwata, B.  A. (1996b). Developing a jaba.1998.31-­165
technology for the use of operant extinction in clini- Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., Gulotta, C. S., Sevin, B. M.,
cal settings: An examination of basic and applied & Layer, S.  A. (2003). On the relative contributions
research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(3), of positive reinforcement and escape extinction in the
345–382. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-­345 treatment of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior
Lerman, D.  C., Iwata, B.  A., & Wallace, M.  D. (1999). Analysis, 36(3), 309–324. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Side effects of extinction: Prevalence of bursting jaba.2003.36-­309
and aggression during the treatment of self-injurious Pizarro, E.  M., Vollmer, T.  R., & Morris, S.  L. (2021).
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), Evaluating skills correlated with discriminated
1–8. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­1 responding in multiple schedule arrangements.
Lindberg, J.  S., Iwata, B.  A., & Kahng, S.  K. (1999). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(1), 334–
On the relation between object manipulation and 345. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.759
stereotypic self-injurious behavior. Journal of Poling, A., & Ryan, C. (1982). Differential rein-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), 51–62. https://doi. forcement of other behavior schedules.
org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­51 Behavior Modification, 6(1), 3–21. https://doi.
Lindberg, J. S., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S., & DeLeon, I. G. org/10.1177/01454455820061001
(1999). DRO contingencies: An analysis of variable-­ Reed, G.  K., Piazza, C.  C., Patel, M.  R., Layer, S.  A.,
momentary schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Bachmeyer, M. H., Bethke, S. D., & Gutshall, K. A.
Analysis, 32(2), 123–136. https://doi.org/10.1901/ (2004). On the relative contributions of non-contingent
jaba.1999.32-­123 reinforcement and escape extinction in the treatment
MacNaul, H. L., & Neely, L. C. (2018). Systematic review of food refusal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
of differential reinforcement of alternative behav- 37(1), 27–42. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2004.37-­27
ior without extinction for individuals with autism. Reynolds, G.  S. (1961). Behavioral contrast. Journal of
Behavior Modification, 42(3), 398–421. https://doi. the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4(1), 57–71.
org/10.1177/0145445517740321 https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1961.4-­57
Marcus, B.  A., Swanson, V., & Vollmer, T.  R. (2001). Richman, D. M., Wacker, D. P., Asmus, J. M., & Casey,
Effects of parent training on parent and child behav- S. D. (1998). Functional analysis and extinction of dif-
ior using procedures based on functional analyses. ferent behavior problems exhibited by the same indi-
Behavioral Interventions, 16(2), 87–104. https://doi. vidual. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(3),
org/10.1002/bin.87 475–478. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-­475
Mazaleski, J.  L., Iwata, B.  A., Vollmer, T.  R., Zarcone, Rincover, A., Cook, R., Peoples, A., & Packard, D. (1979).
J. R., & Smith, R. G. (1993). Analysis of the reinforce- Sensory extinction and sensory reinforcement prin-
ment and extinction components in DRO contingen- ciples for programming multiple adaptive behavior
cies with self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior change. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12(2),
Analysis, 26(2), 143–156. https://doi.org/10.1901/ 221–233. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1979.12-­221
jaba.1993.26-­143 Ringdahl, J.  E., Kitsukawa, K., Andelman, M.  S.,
Northup, J., Broussard, C., Jones, K., George, T., Vollmer, Call, N., Winborn, L., Barretto, A., & Reed, G.  K.
T.  R., & Herring, M. (1995). The differential effects (2002). Differential reinforcement with and without
of teacher and peer attention on the disruptive class- instructional fading. Journal of Applied Behavior
room behavior of three children with a diagnosis of Analysis, 35(3), 291–294. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of jaba.2002.35-­291
Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(2), 227–228. https:// Ringdahl, J. E., Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Roane,
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-­227 H. S. (1997). An analogue evaluation of environmental
Peterson, K. M., Piazza, C. C., & Volkert, V. M. (2016). enrichment: The role of stimulus preference. Journal
A comparison of a modified sequential oral sensory of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(2), 203–216. https://
approach to an applied behavior-analytic approach doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­203
in the treatment of food selectivity in children with Rogalski, J.  P., Roscoe, E.  M., Fredericks, D.  W., &
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Applied Behavior Mezhoudi, N. (2020). Negative reinforcer magnitude
Analysis, 49(3), 485–511. https://doi.org/10.1002/ manipulations for treating escape-maintained problem
jaba.332 behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(3),
Petscher, E. S., Rey, C., & Bailey, J. S. (2009). A review 1514–1530. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.683
of empirical support for differential reinforcement Rooker, G. W., Jessel, J., Kurtz, P. F., & Hagopian, L. P.
of alternative behavior. Research in Developmental (2013). Functional communication training with and
Disabilities, 30(3), 409–425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. without alternative reinforcement and punishment:
ridd.2008.08.008 An analysis of 58 applications. Journal of Applied
Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A., Behavior Analysis, 46(4), 708–722. https://doi.
Worsdell, A.  S., Lindauer, S.  E., & Keeney, K.  M. org/10.1002/jaba.76
(1998). Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of
554 T. R. Vollmer et al.

Roscoe, E.  M., Iwata, B.  A., & Zhou, L. (2013). acquisition in early intervention. Journal of Applied
Assessment and treatment of hand mouthing. Journal Behavior Analysis, 43(2), 351–355. https://doi.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 181–198. https:// org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-­351
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.14 Vollmer, T.  R. (1994). The concept of automatic rein-
Saini, V., Fisher, W.  W., & Pisman, M.  D. (2017). forcement: Implications for behavioral research
Persistence during and resurgence following noncon- in developmental disabilities. Research in
tingent reinforcement implemented with and without Developmental Disabilities, 15(3), 187–207. https://
extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, doi.org/10.1016/0891-­4222(94)90011-­6
50(2), 377–392. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.380 Vollmer, T.  R., & Iwata, B.  A. (1992). Differential
Shabani, D.  B., & Fisher, W.  W. (2006). Stimulus fad- reinforcement as treatment for behavior disorders:
ing and differential reinforcement for the treatment Procedural and functional variations. Research in
of needle phobia in a youth with Autism. Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 13(4), 393–417. https://
Applied Behavior Analysis, 39(4), 449–452. https:// doi.org/10.1016/0891-­4222(92)90013-­v
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2006.30-­05 Vollmer, T.  R., Marcus, B.  A., & LeBlanc, L. (1994).
Shawler, L.  A., Dianda, M., & Miguel, C.  F. (2020). A Treatment of self-injury and hand mouthing following
comparison of response interruption and redirection inconclusive functional analyses. Journal of Applied
and competing items on vocal stereotypy and appro- Behavior Analysis, 27(2), 331–344. https://doi.org/
priate vocalizations. Journal of Applied Behavior jaba.1994.27-­331
Analysis, 53(1), 355–365. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Vollmer, T. R., Peters, K. P., Kronfli, F. R., Lloveras, L. A.,
jaba.596 & Ibañez, V. F. (2020). On the definition of differen-
Slocum, S. K., & Vollmer, T. R. (2015). A comparison of tial reinforcement of alternative behavior. Journal of
positive and negative reinforcement for compliance to Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(3), 1299–1303. https://
treat problem behavior maintained by escape. Journal doi.org/10.1002/jaba.701
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(3), 563–574. https:// Vollmer, T. R., Roane, H. S., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus,
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.216 B.  A. (1999). Evaluating treatment challenges with
Smith, C.  E., Carr, E.  G., & Moskowitz, L.  J. (2016). differential reinforcement of alternative behavior.
Fatigue as a biological setting event for severe prob- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), 9–23.
lem behavior in autism spectrum disorder. Research in https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-­9
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 23, 131–144. https://doi. Weston, R., Hodges, A., & Davis, T. N. (2018). Differential
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.12.003 reinforcement of other behaviors to treat challenging
Smith, R.  G., Iwata, B.  A., Goh, H.-L., & Shore, B.  A. behaviors among children with autism: A systematic
(1995). Analysis of establishing operations for self-­ and quality review. Behavior Modification, 42(4),
injury maintained by escape. Journal of Applied 584–609. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517743487
Behavior Analysis, 28(4), 515–535. https://doi. Wolf, M.  M. (1978). Social validity: The case for sub-
org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-­515 jective measurement or how applied behavior analy-
St. Peter Pipkin, C., Vollmer, T.  R., & Sloman, K.  N. sis is finding its heart1. Journal of Applied Behavior
(2010). Effects of treatment integrity failures during Analysis, 11(2), 203–214. https://doi.org/10.1901/
differential reinforcement of alternative behavior: jaba.1978.11-­203
A translational model. Journal of Applied Behavior Wright, C.  S., & Vollmer, T.  R. (2002). Evaluation of a
Analysis, 43(1), 47–70. https://doi.org/10.1901/ treatment package to reduce rapid eating. Journal of
jaba.2010.43-­47 Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(1), 89–93. https://doi.
Trump, C. E., Ayres, K. M., Quinland, K. K., & Zabala, org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-­89
K.  A. (2019). Differential reinforcement with- Young, J. A., & Wincze, J. P. (1974). The effects of the
out extinction: A review of the literature. Behavior reinforcement of compatible and incompatible alterna-
Analysis: Research and Practice, 20(2), 94–107. tive behaviors on the self-injurious and related behav-
https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000169 iors of a profoundly retarded female adult. Behavior
Uhl, C.  N., & Garcia, E.  E. (1969). Comparison of Therapy, 5(5), 614–623.
omission with extinction in response elimination Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Jagtiani, S.,
in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Smith, R.  G., & Mazaleski, J.  L. (1993). Extinction
Psychology, 69(3), 554–562. https://doi.org/10.1037/ of self-injurious escape behavior with and without
h0028243 instructional fading. Journal of Applied Behavior
Vladescu, J. C., & Kodak, T. (2010). A review of recent Analysis, 26(3), 353–360. https://doi.org/10.1901/
studies on differential reinforcement during skill jaba.1993.26-­353
Response Interruption
and Redirection 29
Catia Cividini-Motta, Hannah MacNaul,
Haley M. K. Steinhauser, and William H. Ahearn

29.1 Introduction to Response ate behavior when repetitive behavior is interfer-


Interruption and Redirection ing or stigmatizing.
In 1984, Fellner et al. published a case study
Response interruption and redirection (RIRD) is in which a 6-year-old engaged in hand flapping,
an applied behavior analytic procedure com- hyperventilating, hand mouthing, and lip pulling
monly implemented to treat stereotypic behavior that was reported to be significantly hindering
and other responses thought to be maintained by their progress. The responses were presumed to
the sensory consequences that engaging in the be automatically reinforced by the authors, and
response produces (i.e., automatic reinforcement; they compared a treatment package consisting of
see Rapp & Vollmer, 2005). RIRD consists of differential reinforcement for incompatible
interrupting the target behavior and redirecting in behavior (DRI) and differential reinforcement of
the form of prompting alternative behavior other behavior (DRO) to DRI and DRO com-
(Ahearn et  al., 2007). For example, if a child bined with what was referred to as a mild inter-
emits stereotypic vocalizations during a commu- ruption procedure. The child had access to leisure
nity outing, their teacher asks the child to answer materials at all times. Interruption was brief,
questions (e.g., “What’s your name?,” “Where do 1–2  s, and results demonstrated that the
you live?,” “What’s your brother’s name?”) they DRI + DRO was only effective when combined
have readily answered in the past. The general with interruption. The authors reported that sev-
purpose of the procedure is to promote appropri- eral additional topographies emerged during
treatment, and they applied the interruption pro-
cedure to those as well. By the end of the second
C. Cividini-Motta treatment phase involving interruption, problem
University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA behavior occurred at near zero levels and toy play
H. MacNaul had increased substantially. These procedures
University of Texas at San Antonio, were adapted by Ahearn et al. (2007) to use redi-
San Antonio, TX, USA rection to an appropriate response as interruption
H. M. K. Steinhauser for stereotypic vocalizations.
Regis College, Weston, MA, USA In the years following the Ahearn et  al.
Melmark New England, Andover, MA, USA (2007) paper, studies have systematically repli-
W. H. Ahearn (*) cated the Ahearn et  al. procedure, evaluated
New England Center for Children, RIRD procedural variations, assessed methods
Southborough, MA, USA for enhancing treatment effects, and compared
e-mail: bahearn@necc.org

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 555
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_29
556 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

RIRD to other interventions. To summarize the 29.2.1 Inclusion Criteria


RIRD literature, we conducted a literature
review of RIRD studies published from 2007 to The 68 articles were then downloaded and evalu-
2020 as the foundation of this chapter. The pri-ated based on preset inclusion criteria. To be
mary purposes of this chapter were to outline included in the literature review for this chapter,
procedural variations of RIRD, identify areas an article had to meet the following criteria: (a)
warranting further research, and recommend participants were human subjects, (b) peer-­
considerations for practitioners considering reviewed and published in English, (c) used a
RIRD as an intervention for stereotypic single-case research design, (d) evaluated RIRD,
behavior. procedurally defined as redirection to other
behavior contingent on repetitive behavior, and
(e) measured at least one dependent variable per-
29.2 Literature Search Method taining to repetitive behavior. In regard to the
RIRD procedural definition, articles were
Seven databases and behavior analytic jour- included if the intervention(s) included redirec-
nals were searched to identify potential tion to alternative and/or appropriate behavior
studies for this review, including APA (e.g., clap your hands, hands down, hand in lap,
PsycINFO, Academic Search Premier, touch your head)  – procedures referred to as
JSTOR, ERIC, Journal of Applied Behavior hands down were not included. After application
Analysis, Behavioral Interventions, and of these criteria, a total of 41 studies met inclu-
Behavior Modification. The search was con- sion criteria. An extended search was conducted
ducted in September of 2020, and only arti- by reviewing the references of each of these 41
cles published since the Ahearn et al. (2007) articles. Articles identified during the extended
study were selected for review. Results were search were reviewed using the same procedures
limited to English-language and peer-­ as described above. Of the 37 additional articles
reviewed research. All databases and jour- identified via the extended search, 15 were
nals were searched by combining the terms selected for the full inclusion review and 2 met
“automatic reinforcement” and “stereotypy” inclusion criteria. Therefore, in preparation for
with the terms “redirection,” “response this chapter, a total of 43 articles were reviewed,
redirection,” “response interruption and resulting in a total of 118 cases that experienced
redirection,” “RIRD,” “contingent RIRD.
demands,” and “overcorrection” to conduct
advanced searches. These search procedures
yielded a total of 116 articles. The titles and 29.2.2 Interrater Agreement (IRA)
abstracts of the 116 articles were screened
to identify articles for potential inclusion in To ensure accurate application of the search pro-
this review. During the title-abstract review, cess and inclusion criteria, a second independent
articles were excluded if they (a) were not rater coded at least 33% of the articles (M = 35%,
empirical (e.g., case studies, reviews, com- range = 33–40%) across each step of the search
mentary in nature), (b) did not mention ste- process. IRA was calculated by dividing the total
reotypic repetitive behavior as a dependent number of agreements by the sum of the agree-
variable (e.g., aggression, SIB), or (c) were ments and disagreements and multiplying by
off topic to the study of interest (e.g., token 100. The resulting IRA was an average of 94%
economies, picture activity schedules, aneu- agreement across each phase of the search pro-
risms). Following the title-­a bstract review, cess. Any article in which the raters disagreed
68 articles were identified for further was discussed before proceeding to the next step
review. of the search.
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 557

29.3 RIRD Overview erature on RIRD and provides an updated discus-


sion on the existing gaps in the literature.
To understand the evolution in the RIRD
research literature, it is imperative to begin with
a detailed description of RIRD, as implemented 29.3.1 Study Characteristics
by Ahearn et al. (2007). In that study, appropri-
ate vocalizations were reinforced with praise 29.3.1.1 Participants
and access to requested items whenever possi- Similar to Martinez and Betz (2013), all of the
ble, while vocal stereotypy was interrupted and individuals included in the studies reviewed for
redirected to appropriate vocal responses. this chapter were children or young adults.
Specifically, upon engagement in vocal stereo- Specifically, 75.4% (n  =  89) of the participants
typy, the researchers called the participant’s were males, all participants were 24 years old or
name, initiated eye contact, then presented a younger, and 98.3% (n = 116) of the participants
series of social questions or vocalizations to had an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis alone
occasion vocal imitation. Finally, praise for use or in combination with another diagnosis. Studies
of appropriate communication was delivered included information about verbal repertoires for
following completion of the RIRD requirement. 86.4% (n = 102) of the participants, and 79.4%
For all participants, the tasks selected for RIRD (n = 81) of these had a verbal repertoire consist-
implementation were in the participant’s reper- ing of functional vocalizations alone or in combi-
toire. In addition, prompts were presented until nation with at least one augmentative and
the participant complied with the three consecu- alternative communication (AAC) system. In
tive tasks without engaging in stereotypy. In this addition, studies seldomly provided additional
study, RIRD decreased vocal stereotypy for all information about participants’ repertoires, but
participants and increased appropriate vocaliza- 2.5% (n = 3) and 17.8% (n = 21) of the partici-
tions for three of four participants. In addition, pants were described as having a repertoire of
vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations motor imitation and to follow verbal instructions,
that occurred during RIRD implementation respectively.
were excluded from the data analysis because
the experimenter stopped the session timer dur- 29.3.1.2 Target Behavior
ing RIRD. and Functional Assessment
Since the publication of Ahearn et al. (2007), Across studies, the most common target behavior
many iterations of RIRD alone or in combination was vocal stereotypy (61% of the participants;
with other interventions have been evaluated (see n  =  72), followed by motor stereotypy (28% of
Martinez & Betz, 2013, for a brief review), which the participants; n  =  33), the combination of
will be described below. To note, Martinez and vocal and motor stereotypy (5.9% of the partici-
Betz concluded RIRD to be an effective interven- pants; n = 7), and then public masturbation (3.4%
tion in reducing levels of stereotypy across all of the participants; n  =  4). For a single partici-
studies included in their review and also identi- pant, the target behavior was hand and object
fied several gaps in the literature (e.g., social mouthing. Although the function of target behav-
validity measures, treatment integrity, RIRD in ior was not reported for 5.9% (n = 7) of the par-
natural environments) that are still present to ticipants, the target behavior was maintained by
date. Moreover, the review by Martinez and Betz automatic reinforcement alone or in combination
was limited to articles published in the Journal of with social reinforcement (e.g., automatic plus
Applied Behavior Analysis. Past and present attention) for all remaining participants (n = 111).
research on RIRD has been published in many To determine the function of the target behavior,
sources, and as such, we conducted a broader, a functional analysis (e.g., Iwata et al., 1982/1994)
updated review (see search method above). Thus, that included at least one test for social reinforce-
this chapter discusses a much larger range of lit- ment was completed with 65.3% (n = 77) of the
558 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

participants, an automatic screen or repeated no ment analyses. There are various ways in which
consequence sessions (Querim et al., 2013) was data can be collected and factors such as ease of
employed for 17.8% (n = 21) of the participants, implementation, reliability, and correspondence
and descriptive assessments alone were com- of measurement systems may impact the inter-
pleted with a couple participants (n = 2). In fact, pretation of results. Specifically, continuous data
for the two participants whose disruptive behav- collection procedures such as duration or fre-
ior was concluded to be reinforced by both access quency are the most sensitive methods to mea-
to attention and sensory consequences (i.e., auto- sure target behaviors (LeBlanc et  al., 2016).
matic reinforcement), a functional analysis was However, other discontinuous measurement such
completed for one of these cases and an indirect as momentary time sampling (MTS) and partial
assessment for the other. interval recording (PIR) may be more feasible
For cases in which the functional analysis did and/or reliable data collection depending on the
not include tests for social functions (i.e., auto- treatment context and resources.
matic screen only), it is possible that the target Meany-Daboul et al. (2007) conducted a com-
behavior also had a social function that was not parison of measurement methods and demon-
identified due to the lack of test conditions for strated that time sampling procedures, such as
maintaining social consequences. However, MTS and PIR, often yield data similar to continu-
given that RIRD was effective in reducing stereo- ous recording. However, MTS produced data that
typy, and it involves the contingent delivery of more closely aligned to the continuous duration
attention, it is unlikely that the functional rein- recording (CDR) data and PIR tended to match
forcer for the disruptive behavior of these indi- well to continuous frequency recording. In the
viduals was access to attention or, presumably, Martinez and Betz (2013) review, the authors
disruptive behavior would have increased. noted that studies evaluating the impact of RIRD
Moreover, given that the implementation of on stereotypy have used continuous measure-
RIRD results in the immediate delivery of atten- ment methods (e.g., CDR) as well as discontinu-
tion in the form of contingent prompting, it may ous measurements methods (e.g., time sampling),
not be appropriate as an intervention for stereo- and they concluded that discontinuous measures
typy that is also maintained by access to attention appeared to be reliable. Thus, given that discon-
as suggested by Ahearn et al. (2007). To address tinuous methods are easier to use and appear to
this possibility, Cividini-Motta et al. (2020) con- produce data similar to continuous measures
ducted a brief attention screen with one partici- (Meany-Daboul et al., 2007), they are suitable for
pant for whom the target behavior was suspected use in clinical work and research, but continuous
to be reinforced by attention and found that the measurement methods should be used whenever
target response persisted only in the no-­ possible.
interaction condition. On the other hand, RIRD In addition to various measurement methods
involves the contingent presentation of redirec- for the target behavior, session measurement sys-
tion tasks; thus, perhaps it would have a thera- tems (i.e., interrupted or uninterrupted measure-
peutic effect on disruptive behavior maintained ment) have also been evaluated. In the seminal
by escape from demands. However, we did not study evaluating RIRD by Ahearn et al. (2007),
identify any studies in which RIRD was imple- the session timer was paused and data were not
mented for problem behavior maintained by collected on occurrences of the target behavior
escape. during RIRD implementation. Thus, it is not sur-
prising that subsequent RIRD evaluations have
29.3.1.3 Target Behavior employed a similar interrupted measurement sys-
Measurement tem or did not specify whether the data reported
It is important to consider the type of measure- included instances of target behavior that
ment system that will be employed to collect data occurred during RIRD implementation (i.e.,
on the target behavior during baseline and treat- uninterrupted measurement). However, in 2014,
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 559

Carroll and Kodak demonstrated that the use of as stating their name in response to the question,
interrupted measurement of target behavior may “What is your name?” or a motor response such
overestimate treatments outcomes. as touching their head in response to the instruc-
Since the publication of the Carroll and Kodak tion, “Touch your head”). Ahearn et  al. (2007)
(2014) study, approximately 60% of the articles treated vocal stereotypy by redirecting the par-
evaluating RIRD have employed either inter- ticipants to complete tasks that required a vocal
rupted measurement or both interrupted and response. That is, the topography of the target
uninterrupted measurement systems. It is impor- behavior (i.e., vocal stereotypy) matched that of
tant to note that multiples studies have produced the RIRD demands (i.e., vocal tasks). Results of
results similar to those of Carroll and Kodak more recent studies have shown that RIRD is
(e.g., DeRosa et al., 2019; Wunderlich & Vollmer, effective independent of the topography of the
2015), but other studies have demonstrated sig- target behavior or the demands (e.g., Ahrens
nificant RIRD treatment effects using uninter- et  al., 2011; Shawler & Miguel, 2015). Ahrens
rupted measurement (e.g., Toper-Korkmaz et al., et  al. (2011) conducted a series of experiments
2018). Given these findings, uninterrupted evaluating the impact of both vocal RIRD (i.e.,
­measurement methods should be employed when presentation of demands requiring a vocal
possible to portray a more accurate representa- response) and motor RIRD (i.e., presentation of
tion of the occurrence of stereotypy. Supplemental demands requiring a motor response) on vocal
measures of RIRD implementation (e.g., fre- and motor stereotypy. In Experiment 1, the
quency, duration) might also help researchers and researchers compared the relative efficacy of
clinicians interpret the efficacy and practicality motor RIRD and vocal RIRD on vocal stereotypy
of RIRD procedures. and results indicated that they were equally effec-
tive at reducing vocal stereotypy. In Experiment
2, the researchers assessed the impact of vocal
29.3.2 RIRD Evaluations RIRD and motor RIRD on vocal and motor ste-
reotypy. Motor RIRD led to slightly greater
Up to this point, this chapter has focused on the reductions of both vocal and motor stereotypy;
considerations to be made before the implemen- however, the differing prompt hierarchies
tation of RIRD (e.g., participant characteristics, between the motor and vocal RIRD procedures
functional assessment, measurement methods); might have affected these results. Similarly,
however, RIRD is an intervention with multiple motor RIRD and both motor and vocal RIRD
moving parts, and as such, various adaptations were also effective in reducing vocal stereotypy
have been evaluated. These have included proce- in the studies completed by Cassella et al. (2011)
dural variations to the key components of RIRD, and Shawler and Miguel (2015), respectively.
antecedent manipulations, and combining RIRD However, these studies differ in that appropriate
with other interventions (e.g., differential rein- vocalizations did not increase in the Cassella
forcement of alternative behavior, response cost). et al. study. Given that the topography of disrup-
tive behavior and the RIRD demands can differ
29.3.2.1 RIRD Procedural from one another, it is possible for RIRD to be
Modifications implemented with a larger number of partici-
pants. More specifically, many individuals with
Topography of RIRD Demands autism do not have a functional vocal repertoire
One variation in the implementation of RIRD is (CDC, 2019) and some even lack echoic respond-
the topography of demands presented to the par- ing (Carroll & Klatt, 2008), making the imple-
ticipant. These are the demands that are provided mentation of vocal RIRD unviable. Thus, it is
by the implementer to the participant or client important to note that both motor and vocal RIRD
that redirect the individual’s target behavior to an demands can be used to implement RIRD to
alternative response (e.g., a vocal response such reduce either form of stereotypy.
560 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

Type of RIRD Tasks required compliance with three RIRD demands


In addition to the topography of RIRD demands, (RIRD-3), and of these, more than half required
the implementer can also vary the type of task either prompted or independent completion of
presented to the individual during the RIRD the RIRD demands. Saini et al. (2015) and Toper-­
implementation. These can include mastered Korkmaz et  al. (2018) compared the effects of
tasks (i.e., tasks the participant readily and inde- different RIRD requirements (e.g., RIRD-1 vs.
pendently emits), nonmastered tasks (i.e., tasks RIRD-3), and the results suggested that RIRD-1
the individual requires assistance to emit), or a was equally effective as RIRD-3, including when
mix of both. Although we did not identify any RIRD-1 preceded RIRD-3.
studies that have evaluated the effects of RIRD Additional variations related to the termina-
task type on RIRD efficacy, results of previous tion criteria have included whether the authors
studies show that RIRD that includes the presen- required compliance in the absence of problem
tation of mastered (e.g., Ahearn et  al., 2007) or behavior as well as whether a duration-based cri-
nonmastered tasks (e.g., Cook & Rapp, 2020) is terion was used to terminate RIRD. For instance,
effective in reducing stereotypy. However, the Athens et  al. (2008) presented a single RIRD
majority of the studies reviewed reported that the task, but termination of RIRD was contingent on
demands presented consisted of tasks the partici- at least 5  s without stereotypy. Moreover,
pant could readily complete, had previously mas- Cividini-Motta et al. (2020) evaluated the relative
tered, or were selected based on a direct efficacy of RIRD and a brief response interrup-
assessment (e.g., RIRD probes). tion procedure in the treatment of public mastur-
A case in point is the study conducted by Love bation. During RIRD, participants were prompted
et  al. (2012), in which RIRD probes were com- to complete 1  min of physical activity. Both
pleted to identify vocal responses that the partici- RIRD and the brief response interruption reduced
pants consistently responded to correctly (i.e., the target behavior, but RIRD required longer
minimum of 89% accuracy across settings and durations and resources to implement. To date,
experimenters). This study provided only a gen- no studies were found that systematically com-
eral description of the procedures employed in the pared the effects of different duration-based ter-
RIRD probes, but in more recent studies the mination criteria on the efficacy or efficiency of
authors reported repeatedly (e.g., five times each) RIRD. Additionally, it does not appear that any
presenting various tasks that required vocal or studies have evaluated whether compliance (e.g.,
motor responses until they identified a minimum independent vs. prompted) or the absence of ste-
number of tasks the participant readily completed reotypy (e.g., RIRD is terminated independent of
independently (e.g., McNamara & Cividini-­Motta, or contingent on the absence of stereotypy) has
2019). It remains unclear whether the type of task an effect on the efficacy of RIRD.
affects the efficacy or feasibility of RIRD, but it is
likely that RIRD that involves mastered tasks may 29.3.2.2 Components to Enhance
be easier to implement and potentially less aver- RIRD
sive to the individual and the implementer. In addition to the RIRD literature involving
numerous procedural variations of the procedure
RIRD Termination Criteria itself, several studies have packaged other treat-
In an effort to increase the practically of imple- ment components with RIRD. These components
menting RIRD in clinical and naturalistic set- have included antecedent modifications (e.g.,
tings, several studies have evaluated the relative abolishing operation procedures, stimulus con-
efficacy of different RIRD termination criteria in trol procedures), reinforcement components
terms of the number of required RIRD demands. (e.g., competing stimuli, differential reinforce-
Specifically, this refers to the criteria determining ment), and other interventions implemented con-
when each RIRD interval is considered complete. tingent on stereotypy (e.g., verbal reprimands,
The majority of studies reviewed for this chapter response blocking, response cost). Several stud-
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 561

ies have specifically evaluated the enhancing tive stimulus (SD), whereas RIRD was imple-
(i.e., additive) effects of these treatment compo- mented contingent on stereotypy when it occurred
nents packaged with RIRD. in either the absence of the SD or in the presence
of different stimulus (S-delta). Moreover, a rule
Antecedent Modification was commonly provided to the participants dur-
In regard to antecedent modifications, two main ing these evaluations (e.g., “the red card is out;
components have been substantially evaluated, please set the string down”; Brusa & Richman,
including abolishing operation procedures prior 2008). Previous studies have used a variety of
to RIRD implementation and stimulus control stimulus control cues including red cards (e.g.,
procedures. Sloman et al., 2017), red and green cards (Brusa
& Richman, 2008; Falligant & Dommestrup,
Abolishing Operation Procedures 2020; Gould et al., 2019), the presence or absence
Two studies evaluated the use of an abolishing of a wristband worn by the participant (Frewing
operations component (AOC) prior to RIRD et  al., 2015), and large colored poster boards
implementation (Lang et  al., 2009; Lang et  al., placed on the wall behind the therapist (Martinez
2010) to measure the collateral effects on stereo- et al., 2016; Pastrana et al., 2013).
typy during treatment. During the AOC condi- To test for inhibitory control in the absence
tion, the participant was given free access to a of RIRD, Sloman et al. (2017) evaluated a stim-
variety of toys and was permitted to engage in ulus cue only (SCO) condition in which the
stereotypy or any other behavior freely. No con- S-delta was present, but stereotypy was not
sequences were delivered for problem behavior interrupted. Results demonstrated that when the
or stereotypy and the access period lasted until SCO condition was implemented in the environ-
the participant stopped engaging in stereotypy ment where signaled RIRD was previously con-
for 10 s and attempted to leave the room. In the ducted, moderate suppression of stereotypy
no AOC condition, the free-access period was (e.g., inhibitory control) was achieved, but when
omitted, and treatment started immediately at the the SCO condition was conducted in a novel set-
beginning of session. The purpose of this evalua- ting, stereotypy remained at baseline levels.
tion was to determine if the AOC condition would Similarly, other studies evaluating stimulus con-
reduce the reinforcing value of stereotypy during trol procedures with RIRD have found mixed
treatment sessions. Results demonstrated that results. Brusa and Richman (2008) demon-
when the AOC condition was implemented strated that stereotypy was under the control of
before treatment, that stereotypy was lower com- the stimulus conditions, but the S-delta condi-
pared to when the AOC condition was omitted. tion was never evaluated in the absence of RIRD
Additionally, reductions in problem behavior implementation. That is, RIRD was always
were observed in the AOC condition compared to implemented when the S-delta (e.g., red card)
the no AOC condition, suggesting that pre-­ was present. Similarly, Frewing et  al. (2015)
session access to stereotypy may abate the occur- evaluated stimulus control procedures in which
rence of stereotypy and other topographies of RIRD was implemented across three additional
problem behavior during regular treatment settings. During the generalization probes to
conditions. novel settings, stimulus control plus RIRD sup-
pressed stereotypy compared to baseline. Taken
Stimulus Control Procedures together, the use of stimulus control procedures
Another antecedent manipulation that has been may lead to quicker suppression of stereotypy
evaluated is the addition of stimulus control pro- during RIRD implementation compared to when
cedures. In RIRD studies using a stimulus control no stimulus conditions are present. However,
procedure, stereotypy was not interrupted in the when RIRD is not implemented, inhibitory con-
presence of the stimulus used as the discrimina- trol is unlikely to be achieved.
562 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

RIRD Plus Reinforcement Components whether or not this is a worthwhile component to


Lanovaz et  al. (2013) reviewed the effects of enhance the effects of RIRD on stereotypy.
reducing stereotypy on untargeted responses and
concluded that stereotypy interventions can result Differential Reinforcement
in response reallocation to untargeted responses, Other studies have specifically evaluated the
suggesting that practitioners arrange reinforce- degree to which the delivery of a reinforcer con-
ment for appropriate behavior. Within the pub- tingent on alternative responses enhances the
lished RIRD literature, RIRD is commonly effects of RIRD on both stereotypy and alterna-
packaged with noncontingent reinforcement tive responses. Dickman et  al. (2012) evaluated
(NCR; e.g., free access to leisure items) and/or the effects of additional reinforcement for appro-
differential reinforcement of appropriate alterna- priate vocalizations in the treatment of vocal ste-
tive responses (DRA; e.g., praise following reotypy by comparing RIRD with praise
appropriate vocalizations). contingent on appropriate vocalizations (similar
to Ahearn et al., 2007) and RIRD with praise and
Noncontingent Reinforcement tokens exchangeable for edibles contingent on
With respect to noncontingent sources of rein- appropriate vocalizations. Both procedures effec-
forcement, Love et  al. (2012) systematically tively decreased stereotypy engagement, but the
evaluated the effects of RIRD paired with access addition of the token reinforcement enhanced the
to sound-making toys, which were presumed to effects with lower levels of stereotypy and higher
produce matched stimulation (MS), on the vocal levels of appropriate vocalizations.
stereotypy of two participants. Prior to imple- Cividini-Motta et al. (2019) conducted a simi-
menting treatment, authors conducted an MS lar comparative study that evaluated the enhanc-
assessment (i.e., competing items assessment) to ing effects of a DRA component with
demonstrate that the items used in the MS condi- reinforcement contingent on appropriate vocal-
tions competed with vocal stereotypy. RIRD and izations and leisure engagement packaged with
RIRD + MS decreased stereotypy, but RIRD + RIRD. In the DRA component, praise and avail-
MS was slightly more effective for one partici- able requested items were provided contingent
pant and required less time implementing on appropriate vocalizations and a tangible rein-
RIRD.  However, the sound-making toys were forcer was provided contingent on leisure engage-
removed during RIRD implementation in the ment. Both procedures with RIRD (i.e., RIRD,
RIRD + MS condition, which might have func- RIRD + DRA) effectively decreased the targeted
tioned as punishment and affected the observed stereotypy for all participants, but sustained lev-
treatment effects. Similarly, Gibbs et  al. (2018) els of appropriate vocalizations or leisure engage-
conducted a competing stimulus assessment to ment were not observed during any intervention.
identify stimuli that competed with vocal stereo- Cividini-Motta et al. suggested that the tangible
typy and compared RIRD and RIRD + MS. The item delivered contingent on leisure engagement
results suggested that RIRD + MS decreased ste- likely affected the levels of appropriate behavior
reotypy relative to RIRD alone. One limitation, because consumption of the item competed with
however, was that the comparison did not include appropriate responses. Thus, practitioners should
a condition without RIRD or with MS alone. consider whether the reinforcers contingent on
Thus, it is unclear how RIRD alone affected ste- appropriate responses might compete with func-
reotypy engagement, if both components com- tional engagement.
bined were necessary to observe treatment
effects, or if MS alone would have decreased ste- RIRD Plus Contingent Interventions
reotypy. Taken together, NCR may enhance the To enhance treatment efficacy, a few studies have
effects of RIRD, but given that a competing stim- paired RIRD with other behavioral intervention
ulus assessment is warranted to identify appro- implemented contingent on the occurrence of ste-
priate stimuli to be used during MS, it is unclear reotypy. For instance, two studies have evaluated
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 563

the enhancing effects of response cost (RC) tasks. The study involved five participants. Each
paired with RIRD (McNamara & Cividini-Motta, participant experienced a five-phase assessment,
2019; Toper-Korkmaz et al., 2018). Both studies with each phase involving a different behavioral
compared RIRD and RIRD + RC, with the RC intervention for stereotypy with more compo-
component involving contingent toy removal nents than the previous phase and a variation of
during RIRD implementation. RIRD and RIRD + RIRD (i.e., overcorrection) as the last phase. The
RC effectively decreased the targeted vocal ste- earlier phases prior to RIRD sufficiently
reotypy, but RIRD + RC enhanced the treatment decreased the stereotypy of four of the five par-
effects in some cases. Toper-Korkmaz et al. sug- ticipants, suggesting that interventions with
gested that the degree to which a RC component fewer procedural components than RIRD might
affects treatment outcomes is dependent on the effectively decrease stereotypy.
degree to which the removed stimulus functions
as a reinforcer throughout the treatment analysis.
Thus, the degree to which RC enhances RIRD 29.4 RIRD Efficacy and Outcomes
treatment effects is likely idiosyncratic across
individuals and dependent on which stimulus is Taken together, this chapter has provided a discus-
contingently removed. sion on various measurement methods, procedural
variations, and additive effects of RIRD. Overall,
29.3.2.3 R  IRD Added to Other RIRD has been found to be effective at reducing
Interventions both motor and vocal stereotypy for participants
RIRD has also been added following other across a variety of ages and diagnoses. In most
approaches to stereotypy treatment. For example, cases, when RIRD is implemented alone, appro-
Colón et al. (2012) first arranged a DRA in the priate behavior and untargeted challenging behav-
form of verbal operant training to train and rein- ior remain unchanged. Although RIRD alone does
force appropriate vocalizations prior to imple- not reliably produce increases in appropriate
menting RIRD.  Verbal operant training alone behavior, RIRD is often combined with other
increased appropriate vocalizations for all three interventions or various procedural modifications
participants and sufficiently decreased stereotypy (e.g., RIRD + DRA for appropriate vocalizations;
for one participant. RIRD was then added for the Colón et al., 2012) to achieve therapeutic effects
other two participants, and stereotypy decreased for behavior other than stereotypy. However, a
while appropriate vocalizations continued to more thorough discussion on the use of RIRD as
occur. The results suggest that at times an evidence-based practice is introduced in the
reinforcement-­based procedures (e.g., DRA) for next section, followed by a discussion of effects of
appropriate responses can promote response real- RIRD on other behavior, and the relative efficacy
location from stereotypy to appropriate alterna- of RIRD compared to other interventions.
tive behavior. Similarly, the results from
Steinhauser et  al. (in press) suggest that some
contexts with naturalistic stimuli and DRA 29.4.1 RIRD Is an Evidence-Based
arrangements might not warrant the addition of Practice
RIRD.  In contexts in which stereotypy remains
problematic with reinforcement-based proce- An evidence-based practice (EBP) consists of an
dures alone, RIRD can be packaged with DRA to intervention that has been shown to be effective
promote response reallocation to functional through multiple replications of rigorous and well-
engagement and decrease stereotypy. controlled research studies (Kazdin, 2011). The
Cook and Rapp (2020) conducted a similar identification of EBPs should facilitate clinical
study involving a progressive treatment analysis decision-making by ensuring that clinicians imple-
for stereotypy to determine the extent practitio- ment interventions that are likely to have therapeu-
ners need to treat stereotypy during academic tic effects, and thus, improve outcomes for clients.
564 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

There are currently multiple criteria to evaluate 29.4.2.1 Medication


treatments and determine whether they meet spe- Unlike other comparative studies that compared
cific criteria to be considered an EBP (e.g., Council RIRD and other behavior analytic interventions,
for Exceptional Children, 2014; What Works Miguel et al. (2009) compared RIRD and sertra-
Clearinghouse, 2017), all of which take into con- line, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor
sideration variables related to the design employed (SSRI), that had been prescribed for stereotypy.
and results attained. Multiple sources have identi- The results suggested that the SSRI did not affect
fied RIRD as an evidence-­based practice (Akers stereotypy engagement, but RIRD effectively
et al., 2020; Steinbrenner et al., 2020; Tomaszewski decreased stereotypy. Therefore, when possible,
et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2020). behavioral interventions for stereotypy should be
For instance, the National Clearinghouse for evaluated prior to considering medicinal
Autism Evidence and Practice (NCAEP) identified intervention.
RIRD as an evidence-based practice for children to
young adults (i.e., age 22 years or younger) with 29.4.2.2 Reinforcement-Based
autism based on a systematic review of studies Procedures
published from 1990 to 2017 (Steinbrenner et al., In addition to Love et  al. (2012) evaluating the
2020). This review identified 29 studies evaluating enhancing effects of matched stimulation paired
RIRD with individuals between the ages of 3 and with RIRD, the study also evaluated the relative
22  years old. According to the authors, to deter- efficacy of the RIRD and MS. Although all three
mine if studies met methodological acceptability procedures decreased stereotypy, higher levels of
each study was independently reviewed by two appropriate vocalizations were observed during
external reviewers using protocols developed based the procedures with RIRD. These results suggest
on the quality indicators described by Gersten et al. that in addition to considering a treatment’s
(2005; for the group design protocol), Horner et al. effects on stereotypy, practitioners and research-
(2005; single-­case design protocol), and the guide- ers should also consider other outcomes such as
lines described by What Works Clearinghouse engagement in appropriate alternative responses.
(WWC). Then, interventions were rated as an evi- Shawler et  al. (2020) conducted a similar com-
dence-based practice if they met one of the three parative study, comparing RIRD, sound-­
criteria developed by the National Professional producing stimuli, and nonsound-producing
Development Center on ASD (NPDC) team: (a) at stimuli. Shawler et al. concluded that both RIRD
least two high-quality group design studies by dif- and sound-producing stimuli effectively
ferent research teams; (b) at least five high-quality decreased stereotypy with the two participants
single-case design studies, totaling at least 20 par- but neither procedure produced consistently
ticipants, conducted by at least three different higher levels of appropriate vocalizations.
research teams; or (c) at least one high-quality Similarly, results from Gibney et al. (2020) sug-
group design and three high-­ quality single-case gested that both RIRD and presumably matched
design studies conducted by at least two different competing stimuli effectively decreased vocal
research teams. As such, RIRD is an EBP. stereotypy with three of four participants. Carroll
and Kodak (2014), Experiment 2, involved a sim-
ilar comparative analysis following an evaluation
29.4.2 RIRD Relative Efficacy of measurement methods. However, the results
Compared to Other differed from other studies, suggesting that NCR
Interventions was more effective than RIRD when stereotypy
was measured using uninterrupted measurement
The relative efficacy of RIRD compared to other methods. During Experiment 1 and Experiment 2
interventions for stereotypy has been evaluated in with the same two participants, Carroll and
comparative studies, comparing RIRD to ante- Kodak measured stereotypy with both interrupted
cedent and consequent interventions. and uninterrupted measurement systems and sug-
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 565

gested that interrupted measurement overesti- These comparable studies provide considerations
mated RIRD efficacy relative to uninterrupted for practitioners deciding between stereotypy
measurement. Although RIRD resulted in more interventions. For example, Cividini-Motta et al.
modest reductions than NCR in Experiment 2, (2020) compared RIRD involving a 1 min physi-
both RIRD and NCR effectively reduced vocal cal redirection and a brief response interruption
stereotypy. procedure in the treatment of public masturbation
With respect to the relative efficacy of RIRD (i.e., inappropriate sexualized behavior). The
and reinforcement contingent on alternative procedures were equally effective in decreasing
responses (i.e., DRA), Colón et  al. (2012) and the target behavior, but the brief response inter-
Steinhauser et  al. (in press) demonstrated that ruption procedure required considerably fewer
RIRD can be effective when stereotypy persists in resources and less time to implement. Giles et al.
DRA conditions. Cividini-Motta et  al. (2019), (2012) compared the efficacy and participant
however, is the only study identified in the chapter preference of RIRD and response blocking in the
literature review that sought to systematically treatment of motor stereotypy, concluding that
evaluate the relative efficacy of RIRD with respect RIRD was both more effective and preferred rela-
to DRA procedures for appropriate alternative tive to response blocking for all three partici-
behavior. As noted previously, none of the proce- pants. DeRosa et al. (2019) conducted a similar
dures resulted in sustained levels of appropriate comparative study but also evaluated the effects
responses (i.e., appropriate vocalizations, leisure of response measurement methods, concluding
engagement), but the procedures with RIRD were that response blocking was substantially more
consistently more effective for all three partici- effective than RIRD when uninterrupted mea-
pants compared to the DRA alone procedure. surement was used. These conflicting conclu-
sions of Giles et al. and DeRosa et al. suggest that
29.4.2.3 Other Contingent the measurement method likely affects the per-
Interventions ceived relative efficacy. However, it is also impor-
As for the relative efficacy of RIRD compared to tant to note that the RIRD procedural variations
other interventions implemented contingent on and the specific variations of the other contingent
stereotypy, several studies have compared RIRD interventions have likely affected the concluding
to other interventions. Notably, Verriden and relative efficacy of procedures.
Roscoe (2019) compared variations of RIRD Toper-Korkmaz et al. (2018) and McNamara
(i.e., contingent demands, overcorrection) to and Cividini-Motta (2019) evaluated the relative
other contingent interventions (e.g., verbal repri- efficacy of RIRD and RC. Across the two studies,
mands, response blocking, response cost) with RC, which consisted of toy removal contingent
noncontingent and contingent sources of rein- on the target behavior, effectively decreased ste-
forcement simultaneously available in a punisher reotypy of some of the participants. In contrast,
assessment. Across the four participants, all RIRD produced treatment effects for all partici-
interventions effectively decreased stereotypy, pants. These results, again, are consistent with
but they had differing effects on leisure engage- the Verriden and Roscoe (2019) assertion that the
ment and emotional responding. Following this most effective intervention is likely
assessment, Verriden and Roscoe involved the idiosyncratic.
participants’ clinicians in the decisions regarding
which interventions to evaluate and in determin-
ing which intervention was most effective overall 29.4.3 Additional Considerations
for each of the participants. Results suggested with RIRD
that the most effective and socially valid inter-
vention was idiosyncratic across participants. Although RIRD is an EBP and is usually at least
Other comparative studies have evaluated the as effective as other behavioral interventions,
relative efficacy of RIRD and other interventions. there are additional considerations that should be
566 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

made when implementing RIRD in practice. results are similar to those attained by Schumacher
These include the effects on untargeted responses, and Rapp (2011). More recently, Cook and Rapp
the social validity of this type of intervention, (2020) implemented motor RIRD [termed posi-
maintenance and generalization of treatment tive practice overcorrection (PPOC) by the
effects, and how treatment integrity may impact authors] contingent on motor stereotypy but also
results. A discussion is provided on each of these measured levels of vocal stereotypy. During the
topics. motor RIRD phase, an increase in vocal stereo-
typy was observed during one of the activities but
29.4.3.1 I ndirect Effects of RIRD levels of vocal stereotypy did not change during
on Untargeted Responses the other two. Furthermore, levels of vocal ste-
Although results of the studies reviewed indi- reotypy also did not change when motor RIRD
cated that RIRD is effective in reducing automat- was implemented contingent on both vocal and
ically reinforced problem behavior, clinical motor stereotypy.
decisions must also consider the impact of a
behavior intervention on untargeted responses Appropriate Behavior
such as untargeted topographies of stereotypy, For approximately half of the participants
appropriate behavior, and challenging behavior included in the reviewed studies, data were also
(e.g., emotional responding, aggression). reported on the effects of RIRD on appropriate
behavior (e.g., appropriate vocalizations, leisure
Untargeted Stereotypy engagement). In general, RIRD either increased
A few studies have evaluated the effect of RIRD appropriate behavior (e.g., appropriate vocaliza-
on untargeted stereotypy. For instance, Cook tions; Ahearn et  al., 2007; Ahrens et  al., 2011;
et al. (2014) implemented RIRD, termed by the Guzinski et al., 2012), did not have an impact on
authors as verbal reprimand, contingent on the appropriate behavior (e.g., communication;
occurrence of either vocal or motor stereotypy Cassella et  al., 2011), or the effects were not
but also measured untargeted stereotypy. In this reported. Thus, decisions about treatment selec-
study, the impact of RIRD on untargeted stereo- tion should consider the impact of the interven-
typy was idiosyncratic across participants (e.g., tion on target and appropriate behavior.
decreased for two, increased for one of the five
participants). Similarly, Pastrana et  al. (2013) Challenging Behavior
implemented RIRD contingent for motor stereo- Additionally, with some individuals, supplemen-
typy and also measured levels of untargeted vocal tal measures of challenging behavior or emo-
stereotypy. In this study, RIRD resulted in mar- tional responding might be appropriate. For
ginal increases in untargeted vocal stereotypy for example, Verriden and Roscoe (2019) used sup-
one participant and decreases in vocal stereotypy plemental measures of emotional responding,
of the second participant. To evaluate the subse- defined as whining, crying, screaming, aggres-
quent effects of RIRD on targeted motor stereo- sion, self-injury, and attempts to escape from the
typy, Pastrana et al. included no-interaction (NI) procedure or physical resistance, as another mea-
sessions prior to and following each RIRD ses- sure used to evaluate interventions. These mea-
sion and compared the RIRD sequence to an NI sures might also serve as social validity measures
sequence (i.e., three consecutive NI sessions). for participants or clients with limited verbal
Results showed that RIRD resulted in an immedi- repertoires.
ate decrease in levels of motor stereotypy, but
motor stereotypy occurred at levels similar to 29.4.3.2 Social Validity
baseline immediately after RIRD implementa- Given that RIRD can be conceptualized as a pun-
tion ceased. In other words, RIRD did not ishment procedure, the inclusion of social valid-
increase or decrease levels of motor stereotypy ity measures is warranted. However, of the
during the subsequent NI sessions and these studies reviewed in this chapter, the majority of
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 567

evaluations did not assess social validity. When it Thus, if certain items or activities are used during
was measured, the most common social validity RIRD implementation, they may still function as
measure was a questionnaire or a questionnaire reinforcers outside of treatment settings.
plus video review of treatment sessions con-
ducted with a caregiver, teacher, aides, or 29.4.3.3 Maintenance of RIRD
­clinician. For studies involving a questionnaire Treatment Effects
for social validity, the majority of results were Out of the articles reviewed in this chapter, few
favorable, indicating the RIRD was effective, measured the maintenance of treatment effects
acceptable, and likely to be carried out by over time. In the majority of cases in which main-
stakeholders. tenance data were collected, RIRD remained in
Another form of social validity measurement effect, whereas only two studies, which included
was based on participant preference and was evalu- a total of four participants, measured mainte-
ated using a preference assessment for the various nance of effects without RIRD (Schumacher &
treatment components (Cividini-Motta et al., 2019; Rapp, 2011; Sivaraman & Rapp, 2020).
Giles, & St. Peter, C. C., Pence, S. T.,, & Gibson, In all cases in which RIRD remained in effect,
A.  B., 2012). In Giles et  al. (Giles, & St. Peter, treatment effects were sustained (i.e., the thera-
C. C., Pence, S. T.,, & Gibson, A. B., 2012), RIRD peutic effect on stereotypy persisted).
was compared to response blocking and all three Furthermore, when maintenance was assessed in
participants preferred RIRD.  In Cividini-Motta the absence of RIRD, stereotypy increased to lev-
et al. (2019), three treatment conditions were eval- els similar to those observed pretreatment (i.e.,
uated (RIRD, DRA, RIRD + DRA) in a concur- baseline). That is, treatment effects did not per-
rent-chains preference assessment. Based on sist in the absence of RIRD (e.g., Schumacher &
participants’ selections, two of three participants Rapp, 2019). An exception is the study by
preferred RIRD alone, whereas one participant pre- Sivaraman and Rapp (Sivaraman & Rapp, 2020),
ferred RIRD + DRA.  These results suggest that which showed that lower levels of stereotypy per-
although redirection to another task may seem sisted when RIRD was withdrawn and that
aversive, or that it would require higher response repeated exposure to RIRD increased its efficacy.
effort for the participant, oftentimes participants Specifically, authors found that when RIRD was
preferred RIRD to other types of interventions that implemented for 20 min and then removed, post-­
did not include prompting another task. Related to treatment levels of stereotypy were below those
participant preference, but novel in approach, observed in the initial baseline phase. However,
Frewing et al. (2015) implemented RIRD and stim- these results were not achieved when RIRD was
ulus control procedures and measured the partici- implemented for only 5 min. These results sug-
pant’s heart rate as a measure of distress. During gest that as RIRD is implemented repeatedly, and
the generalization probes to novel settings, the par- for longer periods of time, that the intervention
ticipant’s heart rate did not increase when treat- functions more effectively and more efficiently
ment was implemented or immediately after it was as the frequency of RIRD intervals decreased
removed. These results suggest that although RIRD over repeated exposures. However, it is possible
is a punishment procedure, it may not be aversive that over extended periods of time without inter-
based on participant’s physiological responses. vention, stereotypy may steadily increase to
Also related to preference, Peters and baseline levels, and as such, RIRD booster ses-
Thompson (2013) did not assess preference for sions may be required to maintain long-term
the various treatment conditions, but instead treatment effects (e.g., Colón & Ahearn, 2019).
evaluated the relative preference for activities
after they were used during RIRD implementa- 29.4.3.4 Generalization of RIRD
tion. Authors found that the activities used during to Naturalistic Settings
treatment did not acquire aversive properties The original RIRD procedures described by
after RIRD was implemented with the activities. Ahearn et  al. (2007) were conducted in a con-
568 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

trolled setting, absent of anyone other than the RIRD procedures have also been effective in
implementer and any unnecessary materials. community settings such as at a restaurant table
Given that stereotypy may prevent acquisition of with peers and other staff members while waiting
new skills in an educational context or be socially for meals to arrive (Sloman et al., 2017) or in the
stigmatizing in the community (Cunningham & home or clinic setting with a parent implement-
Schreibman, 2008), evaluating the efficacy of ing the intervention (Gibbs et  al., 2018). In
RIRD in less controlled settings is warranted. Sloman et al., staff members implemented RIRD
Since 2007, researchers have evaluated the effec- across three educational activities (morning
tiveness of implementing RIRD in more natural- group, independent activities, and deskwork) and
istic settings, such as classroom, community, and one community setting (restaurant). Results dem-
home settings. onstrated that RIRD was effective for one partici-
One of the earliest evaluations of RIRD in natu- pant across a variety of settings and activities.
ralistic settings was conducted by Liu-Gitz and Similarly, Gibbs et  al. first trained clinicians to
Banda (2010), in which RIRD was implemented implement RIRD with MS in a clinic setting and
by a special education teacher in a classroom set- then conducted generality probes either in the
ting with other students of various abilities pres- clinic (Elizabeth) or in the home setting
ent. Appropriate vocalizations were reinforced (Matthew) with the participants’ mothers imple-
with behavior-specific praise (e.g., “I like it when menting the intervention. Results of the general-
you use your pretty voice”), while vocal stereo- ity probes indicated positive results were obtained
typy was interrupted and redirected with questions when parents implemented the intervention out-
about topics of interest to the student (e.g., “Do side the clinical or instructional context. Given
you like Toy Story?”). The teacher maintained high these results, research supports the use of RIRD
procedural integrity and results suggested that in both controlled and naturalistic settings.
RIRD was effective in decreasing vocal stereotypy
in a classroom context. Similarly, Wells et  al. 29.4.3.5 RIRD Treatment Integrity
(2016) evaluated the effects of RIRD implemented As discussed thus far, RIRD is often used in the
by a teacher and educational assistant with an ado- treatment of automatically reinforced stereotypic
lescent during group instruction with other stu- behavior that is either interfering or stigmatizing.
dents present. Results were similar to those Therefore, research assessing the effects of vary-
obtained by Liu-Gitz and Banda; staff easily ing levels of RIRD treatment integrity is of par-
learned to implement the RIRD procedure with ticular importance for clinicians adopting RIRD
ease and the intervention was successful at procedures during naturally occurring activities.
decreasing vocal stereotypy during classroom Multiple studies have evaluated aspects of RIRD
group instruction. Moreover, Martinez et al. (2016) treatment integrity. For example, Giles et  al.
transferred the implementation of RIRD in a con- (2018) evaluated teacher-implemented RIRD in a
trolled environment to a classroom setting for ses- classroom setting. The study involved dividing
sions lasting up to 30  min. Notably, as sessions the prescribed RIRD procedure into task analysis
progressed, the time spent in RIRD and the num- steps and measuring treatment integrity (i.e., the
ber of instructions delivered during RIRD percentage of correct task analysis steps) before
decreased substantially, indicating that repeated and after BST. Before BST, the teachers imple-
exposure to RIRD had long-term effects in regard menting RIRD received a flowchart outlining the
to treatment efficacy and efficiency. Lastly, Giles RIRD procedure and performed approximately
et al. (2018) used behavior skills training (BST) to 50% of the RIRD steps correctly. The authors
coach novice teaching assistants to implement concluded that initiating RIRD within 3 s and ter-
RIRD with students during ongoing classroom minating RIRD after three responses without ste-
activities. Results of Giles et  al. and the above- reotypy were the two most common integrity
mentioned studies support the use of RIRD in a errors across teachers. However, BST drastically
classroom context. improved treatment integrity and teachers quickly
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 569

met the mastery criterion (i.e., 90% accuracy consistency levels reduced stereotypy across par-
across two sessions). These results support using ticipants but the degree to which stereotypy
BST with RIRD implementers, but the Giles decreased varied across participants. Gauthier
et  al. evaluation does not allow for conclusions et al. concluded that the study adds to the RIRD
with respect to how integrity errors at specific treatment integrity literature by demonstrating
steps of RIRD impact its efficacy. that diminished treatment consistency can main-
Two studies, Colón and Ahearn (2019) and tain low levels of stereotypy in some cases.
Gauthier et  al. (2020), sought to evaluate the Although Ahearn et al. (2007) suggested that low
impact of RIRD implemented at various treat- treatment integrity might negatively affect RIRD
ment integrity levels on stereotypy. In Experiment efficacy, results of Gauthier et al. and Colón and
1 of Colón and Ahearn (2019), the authors first Ahearn suggest that, at least in some cases, treat-
trained staff to implement RIRD and then identi- ment integrity errors might have a lesser impact
fied treatment consistency (i.e., implementing on the efficacy of RIRD than initially expected.
RIRD contingent on each occurrence of stereo-
typy) as the most common treatment integrity
error in a classroom setting. The authors found 29.5 Clinical and Research
that when the staff implemented RIRD contin- Recommendations
gent on stereotypy, they implemented it accu-
rately but they did not implement RIRD following Although decreasing stereotypic repetitive
each occurrence of stereotypy. Therefore, behavior is often the primary objective of RIRD
Experiment 2 was designed to systematically procedures, Rapp and Vollmer (2005) and
evaluate the effects of RIRD when implemented Lanovaz et  al. (2013) suggested that treatment
contingent on 100%, 75%, 50%, and 25% of ste- packages that involve reinforcement components
reotypy occurrences. RIRD remained effective at for appropriate behavior can help promote
50% treatment consistency for all three partici- response reallocation to appropriate alternative
pants. These findings are comparable to those of responses (e.g., appropriate vocalizations, func-
Experiment 3 of Ahrens et al. (2011) that sought tional engagement). In fact, as noted above,
to identify the operant mechanism responsible RIRD in combination with reinforcement-based
for efficacy of RIRD by systematically present- procedures has been shown to increase appropri-
ing RIRD contingent on 0.5, 0.25, and 0.1 pro- ate responses such as appropriate vocalizations
portions of stereotypy occurrences. Additionally, and leisure engagement (e.g., Dickman et  al.,
the last phase of the Colón and Ahearn paramet- 2012; Verriden & Roscoe, 2019). Additionally,
ric analysis involved alternating between 25% the behavior analytic ethical standards require
and 100% treatment consistency conditions and the implementation of reinforcement-based pro-
resulted in reduced levels of stereotypy. These cedures in conjunction with punishment-based
results suggest that occasional booster sessions procedures (Behavior Analyst Certification
with high integrity might facilitate maintained of Board [BACB®], , 2014), and Pokorski and
RIRD effects when implemented following only Barton (2020) suggested that behavior analytic
a proportion of responses (i.e., 25%). research, in addition to clinical applications,
Similar to Colón and Ahearn (2019), the study must be in accordance with these BACB® ethical
conducted by Gauthier et  al. (2020) systemati- standards.
cally evaluated different levels of treatment con- Thus, we recommend that clinicians always
sistency; however, the participants did not combine RIRD with reinforcement-based proce-
experience RIRD implemented with 100% con- dures and also evaluate reinforcement-based
sistency prior to the evaluation. Thus, the evalua- interventions prior to the introduction of
tion involved an ABAB design with an alternating RIRD. For instance, Verriden and Roscoe (2019)
treatments comparison of 33% and 100% treat- evaluated the impact noncontingent access to
ment consistency in the B phase. Both treatment stimuli and DRA prior to evaluating the impact of
570 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

punishment-based interventions implemented the RIRD literature, future research on RIRD


contingent on the targeted stereotypy. In addition, should include measures of social validity to
the reinforcement-based components remained ensure that the intervention being provided is
in effect during the punisher assessment. In some acceptable and socially significant. Furthermore,
cases, similar to what was observed by Colón it would be helpful to determine whether the ver-
et  al. (2012), Cook and Rapp (2020), and sion of RIRD recommended here, which includes
Steinhauser et al. (in press), reinforcement-based prompts and reinforcement of completion of non-
procedures alone might promote appropriate mastered tasks, would be associated with higher
alternative behavior and make the addition of social validity measures.
RIRD unnecessary. If appropriate behavior is not Based on results of previous research, there
occurring at desirable levels during the evalua- are additional procedural modifications that can
tion of reinforcement-based procedures, we rec- be used to increase the feasibility of RIRD. For
ommend using prompting or other instructional instance, RIRD has been found to be effective
procedures (e.g., shaping, chaining) to foster an independent of the number of tasks required
increase in appropriate responses such as func- (e.g., RIRD-1 vs. RIRD-3; Saini et  al., 2015;
tional play. Lastly, if RIRD implementation is Toper-Korkmaz et  al., 2018), in cases in which
warranted, to promote response reallocation and RIRD was terminated based on independent
be in accordance with the BACB® ethical stan- (e.g., Ahearn et  al., 2007) or both independent
dards, clinicians should program noncontingent and prompted compliance (e.g., Gauthier et  al.,
sources of reinforcement and differential rein- 2020) with tasks, and in procedures in which
forcement contingent on appropriate behavior. RIRD termination was based on a certain dura-
One way to easily program reinforcement into tion of RIRD implementation (e.g., Cividini-­
RIRD is through the inclusion of nonmastered Motta et  al., 2020; Cook & Rapp, 2020).
tasks during RIRD. That is, instead of selecting Therefore, in clinical settings where feasibility of
skills that the individual has already acquired, implementation is imperative, we recommend
clinicians can select tasks still in training but that that clinicians consider setting the criterion to
can be easily prompted. Then, during RIRD terminate RIRD implementation as compliance,
implementation, prompts can be used to promote independent or prompted, with one RIRD task or
compliance with the tasks and reinforcers can be a specified duration of RIRD implementation
delivered following task completion. In this case, (e.g., 1  min as done by Cividini-Motta et  al.,
the repeated implemented of RIRD may lead to 2020). Furthermore, given that uninterrupted
skill acquisition. Furthermore, by including non- measurement provides more stringent informa-
mastered tasks, the efficacy of RIRD can be tion about the impact of RIRD on target behavior
increased by omitting the resources required to (e.g., Carroll & Kodak, 2014; Wunderlich &
identify mastered tasks. Although previous Vollmer, 2015), and it is likely easier to imple-
research on RIRD has used both mastered and ment, we recommend that clinicians use this
nonmastered tasks, given that no studies have method instead of the interrupted method used in
compared these iterations of RIRD, future a large number of previously published studies.
research should evaluate the relative efficacy of However, clinicians can improve feasibility by
RIRD with mastered and nonmastered tasks, using discontinuous measures (e.g., MTS) and
investigate whether participants indeed acquire potentially by collecting data only during specific
new skills when nonmastered tasks are presented times when target behavior is more likely to
during RIRD, and assess whether further modifi- occur. Furthermore, the results of Colón and
cations to the RIRD procedure (e.g., inclusion of Ahearn (2019) indicate that RIRD implemented
prompts, reinforcement) make acquisition of at variable levels of treatment integrity remains
these skills likely. Moreover, given that both effective as long as it is implemented with high
Martinez and Betz (2013) and the current chapter integrity at least some of the time. Thus, it is
identified measures of social validity as a gap in plausible that in clinical settings after RIRD has
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 571

been implemented at a high degree of integrity with the least intrusive version of RIRD in con-
and shown to reduce stereotypy, some compo- junction with at least one reinforcement-based
nents of RIRD (e.g., long termination criteria; procedure, we suggest that clinicians consider a
implementing contingent on every response) can systematic comparison of several treatment
be omitted from behavioral plans that are imple- options in cases in which RIRD does not decrease
mented on a daily basis by technicians with lim- the target behavior to therapeutic levels. We rec-
ited training as long as clinicians with more ommend an evaluation similar to the Verriden
expertise implement booster sessions on a regular and Roscoe (2019) punisher assessment and dis-
basis. However, research is needed to determine courage the successive addition of components
which components can be omitted on a regular (e.g., increase number of RIRD tasks; increase
basis and the appropriate schedule of implemen- duration of implementation of RIRD) meant to
tation for booster sessions. Additionally, RIRD increase the aversive properties of RIRD due to
can be faded by gradually increasing the amount the potential for habituation. Future research
of time the implementer is out of the room (e.g., should provide additional information about par-
Athens et al., 2008) or by changing the schedule ticipants’ characteristics and repertoires so that
of RIRD implementation from continuous to an these data can be used to identify RIRD varia-
intermittent schedule (e.g., Sloman et al., 2017). tions that are more likely to be effective for a
For example, Athens et  al. (2008) first imple- given individual. With these data, it may be pos-
mented RIRD throughout an entire session, then sible to develop efficient methods for identifying
faded the presence of the implementer in the the most efficacious intervention in particular
room by leaving the room for 10 s, 20 s, and so on cases rather than continuing to pose general rela-
and returning for 1  min or contingent on the tive efficacy questions. Furthermore, clinicians
occurrence of stereotypy to implement must consider other implementation measures,
RIRD.  Results of the fading procedure demon- such as the frequency or duration of implementa-
strated that time alone increased while stereotypy tion, to determine whether an intervention is
remained low. Sloman et al., however, evaluated effective or feasible. Although it is likely that
the effects of intermittent RIRD on vocal stereo- RIRD may initially be implemented frequently
typy during two other activities after RIRD effec- and potentially require large amounts of time,
tively decreased vocal stereotypy during two research has demonstrated that repeated exposure
other activities. In this study, intermittent RIRD, to RIRD may function more efficiently and
consisting of the implementation of RIRD fol- require fewer RIRD presentations as time passes
lowing each instance of stereotypy during the (Sivaraman & Rapp, 2020).
first minute of the session and subsequently on a Additionally, RIRD might not be necessary in
FI 1 min schedule, was effective at reducing ste- an ongoing basis and can be transferred to other
reotypy. Results of these two studies indicate that interventions for maintenance of effects. For
once treatment effects are attained with full deliv- instance, the study by Cook and Rapp (2020)
ery of RIRD, certain components, time spent in found that at least for some individuals, interven-
the room, or the schedule of RIRD implementa- tions less intrusive than RIRD (e.g., academic
tion may be adjusted to increase feasibility of instruction; academic instruction plus auditory
implementation and maintain suppressed levels stimulation) were effective in reducing stereo-
of stereotypy. typy. In addition, most topographies of stereo-
However, findings from previous research typy do not have the potential to result in injury
evaluating the relative efficacy of RIRD itera- to the individual or persons in their environment.
tions or RIRD packages with other interventions That is, stereotypy is not SIB, and thus, is not
suggest that the most efficacious and socially inherently harmful. In determining whether an
valid intervention may be idiosyncratic across intervention is warranted, it is imperative that cli-
participants and relevant stakeholders. Therefore, nicians determine whether the stereotypy emitted
although we recommend that clinicians begin by their client is posing a negative impact on their
572 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

life such as limiting access to social interactions each. Given that RIRD comprises many variables
or disrupting skill acquisition for the individual that can be individualized, future research is war-
or others in their vicinity. Additionally, the ranted to further our understanding of the effects
­ultimate goal for SIB interventions is to com- of each component on stereotypy and how adap-
pletely eliminate SIB from the individual’s reper- tations may be employed to produce socially sig-
toire; however, it is likely that a more appropriate nificant, lasting treatment effects. For further
goal for stereotypy is to ensure it is not impeding descriptions of each procedure, please consult the
acquisition of skills or access to social situations. empirical sources provided.
Thus, whenever deemed appropriate by the clini-
cal team and caregivers, discrimination training
procedures should be considered so that the indi- References
vidual engages in this response only during times
or locations when it is socially acceptable (e.g., All reviewed studies indicated by
independent leisure, downtime; Frewing et  al., asterisk (*)
2015). Additionally, less intrusive interventions
similar to those described by Cook and Rapp may *Ahearn, W.  H., Clark, K.  M., MacDonald, R.  P. F., &
Chung, B. I. (2007). Assessing and treating vocal ste-
maintain low levels of stereotypy following reotypy in children with autism. Journal of Applied
RIRD implementation. Behavior Analysis, 40(2), 263–275. https://doi.
Many additional areas of research were identi- org/10.1901/jaba.2007.30-­06
fied. For instance, although it is presumed that *Ahrens, E. N., Lerman, D. C., Kodak, T., Worsdell, A. S.,
& Keegan, C. (2011). Further evaluation of response
RIRD is contraindicated as a treatment for interruption and redirection as treatment for stereo-
attention-­maintained problem behavior, we have typy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(1),
not identified any studies that evaluated the use of 95–108. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­95
RIRD with repetitive behavior maintained only Akers, J. S., Davis, T. N., Gerow, S., & Avery, S. (2020).
Decreasing motor stereotypy in individuals with
by access to attention. Given that RIRD is con- autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review.
sidered a punishment-based procedure, perhaps Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 77, 101611.
its aversive property may be more potent than the https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2020.101611
potentially reinforcing effects resulting from the *Athens, E. S., Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & St. Peter
Pipkin, C. (2008). An analysis of vocal stereotypy
attention delivered during its implementation. and therapist fading. Journal of Applied Behavior
Thus, future research should consider evaluating Analysis, 41(2), 291–297. https://doi.org/10.1901/
the effect of RIRD on attention-maintained repet- jaba.2008.41-­291
itive problem behavior that has not decreased *Brusa, E., & Richman, D. (2008). Developing stimulus
control for occurrences of stereotypy exhibited by a
when function-based treatments were imple- child with autism. International Journal of Behavioral
mented. Furthermore, few studies have system- Consultation and Therapy, 4(3) 264–269. https://doi.
atically evaluated the enhancing effects of other org/10.1037/h0100855
interventions implemented contingent on the tar- Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2014).
Professional and ethical compliance code for behavior
get behavior (e.g., verbal reprimands, response analysts. Retrieved from https://www.bacb.com/wp-­
blocking) combined with RIRD, even though content/uploads/2020/05/BACB-­Compliance-­Code-­
these treatment components are regularly pack- english_190318.pdf
aged with RIRD.  Thus, future research should Carroll, R.  A., & Klatt, K.  P. (2008). Using stimulus-­
stimulus pairing and direct reinforcement to teach
consider evaluating the additive effects of these vocal verbal behavior to young children with autism.
procedures packaged with RIRD similar to the The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24(1), 135–146.
evaluations with response cost (McNamara & https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393062
Cividini-Motta, 2019; Toper-Korkmaz et  al., *Carroll, R.  A., & Kodak, T. (2014). An evaluation of
interrupted and uninterrupted measurement of vocal
2018). In conclusion, RIRD is an evidence-based stereotypy on perceived treatment outcomes. Journal
practice to reduce stereotypy. This chapter has of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(2), 264–276. https://
provided an overview of various iterations of doi.org/10.1002/jaba.118
RIRD and discussed clinical implications of
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 573

*Cassella, M. D., Sidener, T. M., Sidener, D. W., & Progar, Analysis of stimulus control with response interrup-
P.  R. (2011). Response interruption and r­edirection tion and redirection. Behavioral Interventions, 35(2),
for vocal stereotypy in children with autism: A sys- 306–314. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1703
tematic replication. Journal of Applied Behavior Fellner, D. J., Laroche, M., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1984).
Analysis, 44 (1), 169–173. https://doi.org/10.1901/ The effects of adding interruption to differential rein-
jaba.2011.44-­169 forcement on targeted and novel self-­ stimulatory
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2019). Signs behaviors. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
and symptoms of autism spectrum disorders.. https:// Experimental Psychiatry, 15(4), 315–321. https://doi.
www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/signs.html org/10.1016/0005-­7916(84)90095-­8
*Cividini-Motta, C., Garcia, A.  R., Livingston, C., & *Frewing, T.  M., Tanner, A., Bonner, A.  C., Baxter, S.,
MacNaul, H. L. (2019). The effect of response inter- & Pastrana, S.  J. (2015). The immediate and subse-
ruption and redirection with and without a differen- quent effects of response interruption and redirection
tial reinforcement of alternative behavior component on vocal stereotypy, motor stereotypy and heart rate.
on stereotypy and appropriate responses. Behavioral European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 16(2), 351–
Interventions, 34(1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/ 361. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2015.1110407
bin.1654 *Gauthier, K. A., Ahearn, W. H., & Colón, C. L. (2020).
*Cividini-Motta, C., Moore, K., Fish, L.  M., Priehs, Further evaluation of treatment integrity for response
J.  C., & Ahearn, W.  H. (2020). Reducing pub- interruption and redirection. Behavioral Interventions,
lic masturbation in individuals with ASD: An 35(4), 571–580. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1738
assessment of response interruption procedures. Gersten, R., Fuchs, L.  S., Compton, D., Coyne, M.,
Behavior Modification, 44(3), 429–448. https://doi. Greenwood, C.  R., & Innocenti, M.  S. (2005).
org/10.1177/0145445518824277 Quality indicators for group experimental and
*Colón, C.  L., & Ahearn, W.  H. (2019). An analysis of quasi-experimental research in special education.
treatment integrity of response interruption and redi- Exceptional Children, 71(2), 149–164. https://doi.
rectionJournal of Applied Behavior Analysis 52(2), org/10.1177/001440290507100202
337–354. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.537 *Gibbs, A.  R., Tullis, C.  A., Thomas, R., & Elkins, B.
*Colón, C. L., Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., & Masalsky, (2018). The effects of noncontingent music and
J. (2012). The effects of verbal operant training and response interruption and redirection on vocal ste-
response interruption and redirection on appropriate reotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(4),
and inappropriate vocalizations. Journal of Applied 899–914. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.485
Behavior Analysis, 45(1), 107–120. https://doi. Gibney, C., Phillips, K. J., Arnold-Saritepe, A., & Taylor,
org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-­107 S. A. (2020). An evaluation of the effects of response
*Cook, J.  L., & Rapp, J.  T. (2020). To what extent do interruption redirection and matched stimulation on
practitioners need to treat stereotypy during academic vocal stereotypy. Behavioral Interventions, 35(1),
tasks? Behavior Modification, 44(2). 228–264. https:// 114–130. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1700
doi.org/10.1177/0145445518808226 *Giles, A.  F., St. Peter, C.  C., Pence, S.  T., & Gibson,
*Cook, J. L., Rapp, J. T., Gomes, L. A., Frazer, T. J., & A.  B. (2012). Preference for blocking or response
Lindblad, T.  L. (2014). Effects of verbal reprimands redirection during stereotypy treatment. Research
on targeted and untargeted stereotypy. Behavioral in Developmental Disabilities, 33(6), 1691–1700.
Interventions, 29(2), 106–124. https://doi.org/10.1002/ https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.05.008
bin.1378 *Giles, A., Swain, S., Quinn, L., & Weifenbach, B. (2018).
Council for Exceptional Children. (2014). Council for Teacher-implemented response interruption and redi-
Exceptional Children standards for evidence-based rection: Training, evaluation, and descriptive analysis
practices in special education. CEC. of treatment integrity. Behavior Modification, 42(1),
Cunningham, A. B., & Schreibman, L. (2008). Stereotypy 148–169. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517731061
in autism: The importance of function. Research in *Gould, K. M., Harper, J. M., Gillich, E., & Luiselli, J. K.
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 469–479. (2019). Intervention, stimulus control, and generaliza-
*DeRosa, N. M., Novak, M. D., Morley, A. J., & Roane, tion effects of response interruption and redirection
H.  S. (2019). Comparing response blocking and on motor stereotypy. Behavioral Interventions, 34(1),
response interruption/redirection on levels of motor 118–126. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1652
stereotypy: Effects of data analysis procedures. *Guzinski, E. M., Cihon, T. M., & Eshleman, J. (2012).
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(4), 1021– The effects of tact training on stereotypic vocaliza-
1033. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.644 tions in children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal
*Dickman, S.  E., Bright, C.  N., Montgomery, D.  H., & Behavior, 28(1), 101–110. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Miguel, C. F. (2012). The effects of response interrup- bf03393110
tion and redirection (RIRD) and differential reinforce- Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom,
ment on vocal stereotypy. Behavioral Interventions, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single subject
27(4), 185–192. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1348 research to identify evidence-based practice in spe-
*Falligant, J.  M., & Dommestrup, A.  K. (2020). cial education. Exceptional Children, 71(2), 165–180.
Decreasing motor stereotypy in leisure contexts: https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290507100203
574 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

Iwata, B., Dorsey, M., Slifer, K., Bauman, K., & Richman, *Miguel, C. F., Clark, K., Tereshko, L., & Ahearn, W. H.
G. (1982/1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-­ (2009). The effects of response interruption and redi-
injury. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental rection and sertraline on vocal stereotypy. Journal of
Disabilities, 2(1), 3–20. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(2), 883–888. https://
jaba.1994.27-­197. doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-­883
Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Evidence-based treatment research: *Pastrana, S.  J., Rapp, J.  T., & Frewing, T.  M. (2013).
Advances, limitations, and next steps. American Immediate and subsequent effects of response inter-
Psychologist, 66(8), 685–698. https://doi.org/10.1037/ ruption and redirection on targeted and untargeted
a0024975 forms of stereotypy. Behavior Modification, 37(4),
*Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G.  E., 591–610. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445513485751
Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M., White, P., & Zarcone, *Peters, L. C., & Thompson, R. H. (2013). Some indirect
J. (2009). Enhancing the effectiveness of a play inter- effects of positive practice overcorrection. Journal of
vention by abolishing the reinforcing value of ste- Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 613–625. https://
reotypy: A pilot study. Journal of Applied Behavior doi.org/10.1002/jaba.63
Analysis, 42(4), 889–894. https://doi.org/10.1901/ Pokorski, E.  A., & Barton, E.  E. (2020). A systematic
jaba.2009.42-­889 review of the ethics of punishment-based proce-
*Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Machalicek, W., dures for young children with disabilities. Remedial
Rispoli, M., Lancioni, G. E., Aguilar, J., & Fragale, C. and Special Education. Advance online publication.
(2010). The effects of an abolishing operation inter- https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932520918859
vention component on play skills, challenging behav- Querim, A.  C., Iwata, B.  A., Roscoe, E.  M.,
ior, and stereotypy. Behavior Modification, 34(4), Schlichenmeyer, K.  J., Ortega, J.  V., & Hurl, K.  E.
267–289. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445510370713 (2013). Functional analysis screening for problem
Lanovaz, M.  J., Robertson, K.  M., Soerono, K., & behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement.
Watkins, N. (2013). Effects of reducing stereotypy Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 47–60.
on other behaviors: A systematic review. Research https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.26
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(10), 1234–1243. Rapp, J. T., & Vollmer, T. R. (2005). Stereotypy I: A review
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.07.009 of behavioral assessment and treatment. Research in
LeBlanc, L. A., Raetz, P. B., Sellers, T. P., & Carr, J. E. Developmental Disabilities, 26(6), 527–547. https://
(2016). A proposed model for selecting measurement doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2004.11.005
procedures for the assessment and treatment of prob- *Saini, V., Gregory, M. K., Uran, K. J., & Fantetti, M. A.
lem behavior. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9(1), (2015). Parametric analysis of response interruption
77–83. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-­015-­0063-­2 and redirection as treatment for stereotypy. Journal of
*Liu-Gitz, L., & Banda, D.  R. (2010). A replication of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(1), 96–106. https://doi.
the RIRD strategy to decrease vocal stereotypy in a org/10.1002/jaba.186
student with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 25(1), *Schumacher, B. I., & Rapp, J. T. (2011). Evaluation of the
77–87. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.297 immediate and subsequent effects of response inter-
*Love, J.  J., Miguel, C.  F., Fernand, J.  K., & LaBrie, ruption and redirection on vocal stereotypy. Journal
J. K. (2012). The effects of matched stimulation and of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(3), 681–685. https://
response interruption and redirection on vocal stereo- doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-­681
typy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(1), *Shawler, L. A., Dianda, M., & Miguel, C. F. (2020). A
549–564. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-­549 comparison of response interruption and redirection
Martinez, C. K., & Betz, A. M. (2013). Response interrup- and competing items on vocal stereotypy and appro-
tion and redirection: Current research trends and clini- priate vocalizations. Journal of Applied Behavior
cal application. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Analysis, 53(1), 355–365. https://doi.org/10.1002/
46(2), 549–554. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.38 jaba.596
*Martinez, C.  K., Betz, A.  M., Liddon, C.  J., & Werle, *Shawler, L. A., & Miguel, C. F. (2015). The effects of
R.  L. (2016). A progression to transfer RIRD to the motor and vocal response interruption and redirection
natural environment. Behavioral Interventions, 31(1), on vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations.
144–162. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1444 Behavioral Interventions, 30(2), 112–134. https://doi.
*McNamara, K., & Cividini-Motta, C. (2019). Further org/10.1002/bin.1407
evaluation of treatments for vocal stereotypy: *Sivaraman, M., & Rapp, J. T. (2020). Further analysis of
Response interruption and redirection and response the immediate and subsequent effect of RIRD on vocal
cost. Behavioral Interventions, 34(2), 181–197. stereotypy. Behavior Modification, 44(5), 646–669.
https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1657 https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445519838826
*Meany-Daboul, M. G., Roscoe, E. M., Bourret, J. C., & *Sloman, K.  N., Schulman, R.  K., Torres-Viso, M., &
Ahearn, W.  H. (2007). A comparison of momentary Edelstein, M. L. (2017). Evaluation of response inter-
time sampling and partial-interval recording for evalu- ruption and redirection during school and community
ating functional relations. Journal of Applied Behavior activities. Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice,
Analysis, 40(3), 501–514. https://doi.org/10.1901/ 17(3), 266–273. https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000061
jaba.2007.40-­501
29  Response Interruption and Redirection 575

Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin, *Verriden, A.  L., & Roscoe, E.  M. (2019). An evalua-
K. L., Nowell, S. W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey, S., tion of a punisher assessment for decreasing auto-
McIntyre, N. S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage, M. N. matically reinforced problem behavior. Journal of
(2020). Evidence-based practices for children, youth, Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(1), 205–226. https://
and young adults with Autism. The University of North doi.org/10.1002/jaba.509
Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child Wang, D., Mason, R. A., Lory, C., Kim, S. Y., David, M.,
Development Institute, National Clearinghouse on & Guo, X. (2020). Vocal stereotypy and autism spec-
Autism Evidence and Practice Review Team. trum disorder: A systematic review of interventions.
Steinhauser, H.  M. K., Ahearn, W.  H., Foster, R.  A., Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 78, 101647.
Jacobs, M., Doggett, C.  G., & Goad, M.  S. (in https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2020.101647
press). Examining stereotypy in naturalistic contexts: *Wells, J., Collier, L., & Sheehey, P. H. (2016). Decreasing
Differential reinforcement and context-specific redi- vocal stereotypy of a youth with autism in a classroom
rection. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(4), setting. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 38(2),
1420–1436 164–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/07317107.2016.11
Tomaszewski, B., Regan, T., & AFIRM Team. (2017). 72887
Response interruption and redirection. National What Works Clearinghouse. (2017). What works clear-
Professional Development Center on Autism inghouse: Standards handbook, version 4.0. Retrieved
Spectrum Disorder, FPG Child Development Center, from Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
University of North Carolina. http://afirm.fpg.unc.edu/ https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Docs/referenceresources/
response-interruption-and-redirection. wwc_standards_handbook_v4.pdf.
*Toper-Korkmaz, O., Lerman, D. C., & Tsami, L. (2018). *Wunderlich, K. L., & Vollmer, T. R. (2015). Data analy-
Effects of toy removal and number of demands on sis of response interruption and redirection as a treat-
vocal stereotypy during response interruption and ment for vocal stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior
redirection. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Analysis, 48(1), 749–764. https://doi.org/10.1002/
51(4), 757–768. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.497 jaba.227
Building Independence:
Self-­Management for Individuals 30
with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Kimberly B. Marshall and Jessica L. Rohrer

30.1 What Is Self-Management? that behavior change tactics or operant control


are behavior (Epstein, 1997; Newman et  al.,
Skinner (1953) used the term self-control to 1997; Skinner, 1953). As such, the two responses
describe the application of techniques of operant defined within self-­management, the behavior
control to one’s own behavior when he said a change tactics and the desired change in behav-
man “controls himself precisely as he would con- ior, can still be traced back to environmental
trol the behavior of anyone else-through the functions (Cooper et  al., 2007; Epstein, 1997;
manipulation of variables of which behavior is a Skinner, 1953). The behavior change tactics
function”. More recently, Cooper et  al. (2007) (e.g., keeping track of one’s completion of
defined self-management as “the personal appli- chores on a checklist) may be reinforced by a
cation of behavior change tactics that produces a social community (e.g., a family) that prioritizes
desired change in behavior” (p. 578). While some self-control. Moreover, the desired change in
behavior analysts have suggested separating behavior (e.g., completing the expected tasks)
these definitions (Newman et  al., 1996), others may be reinforced through the social environ-
have used them synonymously and focused ment (e.g., payment for completion of chores)
instead on the importance of ensuring that these or the physical environment (e.g., access to
terms are used precisely (Epstein, 1997). clean clothes after doing the laundry). This
analysis of self-management stresses the rela-
tionship between environmental stimuli and
30.1.1 Theoretical Basis behavior and, therefore, still maintains the sci-
of Self-Management entific assumptions of determinism. Skinner
(1953) captured this point when he stated that
Inherent in the presented definitions of self-­ self-control “is a proper object of analysis, and
control and self-management is the assumption eventually it must be accounted for with vari-
ables lying outside the individual himself”
(Skinner, 1953, pp. 228–229).
K. B. Marshall (*) In his analysis of self-control, Skinner
University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, USA
(1953) drew upon the same methods of control
J. L. Rohrer that are used in the control of someone else’s
Endicott College, Beverly, MA, USA
behavior. The difference in self-control was
The Center for Children with Special Needs, that the individual manipulates the environ-
Glastonbury, CT, USA
e-mail: Kmarshal1@endicott.edu
mental variables of which their own behavior

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 577
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_30
578 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

is a function. In discussing self-control, whereas Hayes et al. (1985) suggested that pub-
Skinner defined two important responses: (1) a lic goal setting and the resulting social scrutiny
controlling response that manipulates environ- may be the operative process. Newman et  al.
mental variables that impact the likelihood of (1996) discussed the controlling response in
(2) a controlled response. In Cooper et  al.’s terms of Skinner’s (1953) conceptualization of
(2007) definition of self-­management, the con- conditioned aversive stimuli. When a stimulus is
trolling response is akin to the behavior change consistently paired with an aversive stimulus,
tactics and the controlled response is repre- then any behavior paired with that stimulus may
sented by the desired change in behavior. Each in itself become a conditioned aversive stimu-
of these responses is ultimately controlled by lus. “Any behavior which reduces this condi-
the environment with the controlling response tioned aversive stimulation will be reinforced”
evoked by present conditions and a history of (Skinner, 1953, p. 188). Take the example of an
reinforcement, and the controlled response individual who has previously told others about
evoked by those stimuli that resulted from the their New Year’s resolutions. If others have his-
controlling response. torically punished their lack of follow through
Skinner (1953) identified nine methods of self- on their plans, then engaging in a different
control. In one of these methods, the controlling behavior (e.g., eating ice cream) rather than
response results in the addition of an aversive to their planned behavior (e.g., eating healthy
the environment in order to increase behavior that foods) will become a conditioned aversive stim-
results in the removal of the aversive stimulation. ulus as a result of consistent pairing with the
Examples of controlling responses that result in social punishment. Even if their newest New
aversive stimulation include setting an alarm clock Year’s resolution is not shared with others, the
or writing items on a to-do list in which the loud act of engaging in or even thinking about behav-
sound of the alarm and the presence of incomplete ior that conflicts with their resolution (e.g.,
items on the to-do list function as aversive stimuli, spending money) will be aversive. Engaging in
respectively. Engaging in the controlled response, the planned behavior (e.g., saving money) or
turning off the alarm clock, and completing and behavior that evokes the planned behavior (e.g.,
checking off the item are negatively reinforced setting up a direct deposit system) is negatively
through the removal of these aversive stimuli. reinforced as they escape from the aversive con-
Another, changing the stimulus is a controlling dition of engaging in or thinking about the con-
response that either removes or adds a discrimina- flicting behavior. As such, self-­ management
tive stimulus to the environment that may evoke a might be best explained as a negative reinforce-
controlled response. Removing candy from your ment process (Newman et al., 1996).
home to increase the likelihood of healthy eating
or setting a reminder on your phone to increase the
likelihood of attending an important meeting 30.1.2 Importance and Benefit
exemplify self-control resulting from changing the of Self-Management
stimulus.
The changes in the environment that result Epstein (1997) asserted that self-management
from the controlling response are the variables aids in developing citizens who can be most pro-
of which the controlled response is a function. ductive and who engage in behavior most sup-
However, this provides little information about portive of the long-term interests of the society.
the function of the controlling response. The Relatedly, Lovitt (1973) discussed the need for
mechanisms responsible for the controlling educational systems to systematically instruct
response are less well understood, although students in self-management skills to build inde-
many explanations have been suggested. Catania pendence and self-reliance. Skinner (1953) also
(1975) suggested that self-awareness is the discussed the purpose of education to be, most
underlying mechanism of self-management, importantly, the acquisition of skills of self-­
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 579

control. While educational systems may be setting to their new schedule to arrive to work on
those held most responsible for teaching self-­ time, will increase their success in this new role.
management skills, these skills have broad value Self-management may be self-perpetuating in
to the community as a whole (Epstein, 1997). that it leads to further opportunities for reinforce-
The extent to which such skills among citizens ment. Relatedly, it has been suggested that self-­
develop has a large impact on society broadly. management empowers individuals and can
Newman et al. (1996) proposed that the term improve self-determination and quality of life
self-control be used for situations that involve (Lee et al., 2007).
selecting between the types of responses, dis-
cussed by Epstein, that conflict in terms of their
long- and short-term consequences, whereas self-­ 30.1.3 Concerns with Self-­
management should be used when referring more Management in Behavior
generally to the application of operant principles Analysis
to one’s own behavior. Mallott (2004) conceptu-
alized self-management differently and asserted Despite the noted importance of self-control,
that it is more often concerned with competition concerns have been expressed about the term
between immediate consequences and cumula- self-management in behavior analysis because of
tive consequences. For example, the immediate the potential use of such a term as an explanatory
consequence of relaxing may be in competition fiction, its potential to be considered antithetical
with the cumulative consequence of losing to determinism, and long-standing debate about
weight that comes from repeated exercise. the efficacy of the concept of self-reinforcement
Through either conceptualization, there are clear (Epstein, 1997; Newman et  al., 1997; Poppen,
benefits to society of selecting the long-term or 2004). However, if self-management is used by
cumulative reinforcers over the short-term or behavior analysts in the way self-control was dis-
immediate reinforcers. Reinforcement in the cussed by Skinner (1953), as the manipulation of
form of warmth may lead an individual con- variables that evoke further behavior, then the
trolled by their immediate environment to turn up term can continue to be conceptually systematic
the thermostat, while the longer-term conse- and fit solidly into a scientific attitude of deter-
quences on the environment may lead a self-­ minism. As Newman et  al. (1997) stated, “It is
manager to turn it down (Epstein, 1997). not the term that is a threat to the rigor of the
There are also benefits of self-management to discipline, but its inappropriate usage” (p. 89).
the individual. Individuals who exhibit a reper- The term self-reinforcement, in particular, has
toire of self-control behavior are able to more been the subject of many concerns within behav-
independently transition across the lifespan, ior analytic interpretations of self-management.
problem solve, and generalize and maintain skills These concerns have generally fallen into three
across environments (Koegel & Koegel, 1990; categories: (1) the self or individual is not rein-
Lee et al., 2007; Skinner, 1953). These individu- forced, (2) reinforcement is accessible to the
als can access more opportunities for learning individual at any time, and (3) self-reinforcement
and reinforcement as a result of generalizing con- is an explanatory fiction indicative of many pro-
trolling responses that lead to further controlled cesses including self-monitoring and self-­
responses. This generalized repertoire can evaluation (Catania, 1975; Newman et al., 1996).
increase independence across academic, social, As in the control of others’ behavior, reinforce-
and vocational settings. For example, learning to ment increases the probability of similar behav-
set and respond to an alarm clock allows greater ior and should be discussed technically, regarding
opportunity for a student to access an educational its effect on behavior rather than its effect on the
setting. Increased opportunities for education individual. Consequently, while the term self-­
may lead to improved employment skills and, in reinforcement seems to imply one is reinforcing
combination with the ability to generalize alarm themself, self would be better thought of as the
580 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

location of the behavior that is reinforced 30.2.1 Goal Setting


(Newman et al., 1997; Poppen, 2004). It is true
that in self-reinforcement the reinforcer is acces- Goal setting refers to selecting a performance
sible to the individual at any time, yet the indi- standard to achieve in order to change behavior
vidual does not contact the reinforcer when it is (Bandura, 1977). Goal setting is often used in
not earned. This behavior of accessing the avail- conjunction with self-monitoring and self-­
able stimulus only when the conditions are evaluation. Individuals identify a performance
appropriate can be best explained by focusing on standard (i.e., set a goal), track their own behav-
the mechanisms discussed previously that control ior (i.e., self-monitor), and then review whether
the controlling response. If the individual has they achieved the goal (i.e., self-evaluate). Fellner
previously contacted punishment for cheating or and Sulzer-Azaroff (1984) offered a behavior
lying, then that behavior will function as a condi- analytic conceptualization of goal setting. They
tioned aversive stimulus and escape from that described the setting of the goal as an antecedent
aversive condition will negatively reinforce stimulus, and the praise for meeting the goal as a
behavior that is incompatible with dishonesty. reinforcing consequence. Once these are suffi-
Finally, self-­reinforcement does require other ciently paired (e.g., several instances of setting
mediating responses including self-monitoring the goal, achieving the goal, and receiving
and self-­evaluation, and, as such, may be more praise), the goal itself becomes both a discrimi-
indicative of a repertoire of responses rather than native stimulus and a conditioned reinforcer. In
as a single operant (Newman et al., 1997). addition to this contingency-shaped conceptual-
The issue of self-reinforcement is indicative ization, goal setting can also be viewed as rule-­
of the larger challenge of determining the under- governed behavior, in which a verbal chain
lying mechanisms of self-management. In the (rather than past experience) influences future
clinical application of self-management, the use performance (Skinner, 1974).
of treatment packages including multiple com- Goal setting is often incorporated as part of a
ponents of self-management is common. This self-management package. Kazdin (1974) found
furthers the difficulty of discerning the mecha- that providing a performance standard or goal
nisms of change and the effective and necessary augmented the reactive effects of self-­monitoring,
components of self-management treatments. but that goal setting on its own (without self-­
monitoring) did not significantly change behav-
ior. However, Locke and Latham (1990) posited
30.2 Self-Management that there is efficacy in goal setting as an inter-
Interventions vention independent of other procedures. Despite
these differing views on the conditions of effec-
As has been discussed here, the controlling tiveness, goal setting has been shown to be asso-
response in self-management is controlled by the ciated with behavioral improvements in a broad
external environment and a history of reinforce- range of areas including organizational behavior
ment. In self-management interventions, clini- management (Pritchard & Curtis, 1973) and
cians, teachers, or caregivers manipulate sports settings (Brobst & Ward, 2002).
environmental stimuli to teach the controlling Hughes et  al. (2013) trained three typically
response that the individual will use to manipulate developing high school peers to set a goal with
the environment to control their own behavior (the respect to the number of interactions they would
controlled response). These methods of control achieve with an identified peer with autism spec-
can be broken down into various components, trum disorder (ASD). The typically developing
some or all of which may be part of an intervention peer was trained on topics that might be of inter-
package. Package components may include goal est to the peer with ASD, then was asked to set a
setting, self-monitoring, self-­evaluation, self-rein- goal with respect to the number of interactions
forcement, and self-­instruction (Carr et al., 2014; they would have with their identified partner and
Cooper et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2007). to try to achieve their goal during a shared gen-
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 581

eral education class. Initiations from the typical to their behavior beyond identification of occur-
peers increased significantly, and the duration of rences and nonoccurrences. That is, the quality of
interaction time between the peers and the par- responses emitted by the individual is not com-
ticipants with ASD was found to be consistent pared to a particular criterion. However, self-­
with the duration of interactions of other (typi- monitoring in isolation has been shown to be
cally developing) normative peer dyads observed. effective in increasing appropriate behavior.
Importantly, social validity measures were also Holifield et al. (2010) evaluated the impact of a
high. Typically developing partners indicated that self-monitoring intervention on the on-task
they generally found their interactions enjoyable, behavior and accuracy of work completion in two
and participants with ASD stated that they had a elementary school students with ASD.  The stu-
new friend and responded positively when asked dents were provided a verbal cue to identify if
about this relationship. they were on-task every 5 min during a 20 min
work block in language arts and math classes and
instructed to record a yes or no accordingly.
Goal Setting in Mrs. Simon’s Class When the intervention was implemented,
Mrs. Simon wanted to increase the number increases above baseline were immediately
of assignments her students completed dur- observed in both on-task behavior and work
ing morning work in her third-grade class- accuracy across both subject areas. This exempli-
room. On Monday morning, she asked fies how the reactivity that often occurs with self-­
each student to set a goal for the number of monitoring treatments can have clinically
assignments they would complete by the beneficial effects. That is, the act of assessing or
end of morning work each day that week. measuring one’s own behavior often results in
responses that are in the direction of desired
behavior change (Broden et al., 1971).
Self-monitoring interventions are frequently
30.2.2 Self-Monitoring accompanied by preintervention training in
which the individual is taught to accurately
Self-management interventions often rely on identify their own behavior (Carr et  al., 2014;
self-monitoring, which requires an individual to Koegel & Koegel, 1990; Mancina et al., 2000).
systematically observe their own behavior and In the above example, the participants were
respond to the occurrence or nonoccurrence of trained on accurate self-reporting during the
the specified target response (Cooper et  al., first six sessions and data collected during the
2007). Self-monitoring systems often include intervention showed 90% agreement between
self-recording, which involves documenting staff and participant evaluations of on-task
occurrences or nonoccurrences of the target behavior (Holifield et al., 2010). Initial training
behavior as it occurs or after an identified interval may include simple discrimination of the target
(e.g., Harris 1986; Koegel & Koegel, 1990; Lloyd behavior in videos or in the moment. Training
et  al., 1989; Soares et  al., 2009). Figure  30.1 may also include ongoing reinforcement pro-
shows two data sheets that could be used for self-­ vided for accuracy of self-­ monitoring. This
monitoring and self-recording of on-task behav- additional component is sometimes referred to
ior in a classroom or other instructional setting. as self-evaluation and will be discussed further
The self-monitoring systems associated with in the next section.
these data sheets might include the use of an Interestingly, research has shown that even
auditory timer to cue an individual to self-record when individuals’ self-recording is inaccurate,
if they have been on-task throughout the interval positive outcomes of self-monitoring are still
(i.e., whole interval recording) or when the timer observed (e.g., Koegel & Koegel, 1990; Newman
sounds (i.e., momentary time sampling). et  al., 1997). Koegel and Koegel (1990) used a
Importantly, self-monitoring does not require self-management intervention to decrease the
that individuals evaluate their behavior or respond stereotypic behavior of four children with ASD
582 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

At the end of each 5-minute interval, record a check if you are presently on-task and a
minus if you are presently off-task (your behavior prior to the timer going off is not
recorded).

Questions to ask myself:


 Do I know what the teacher just said?
 Am I looking at my work or the teacher?
 Am I working on the present task?

Class 5:00 10:00 15:00 20:00 25:00 30:00 35:00 40:00 45:00

Fig. 30.1  Self-monitoring data sheets for on-task behavior

and severe intellectual disability across clinical


and community settings. In the preintervention Self-Monitoring in Mrs. Simon’s Class
condition, the participants were trained to check During the week in which goal setting was
a box when stereotypic behavior was absent dur- implemented, Mrs. Simon noted that a few
ing the interval. During the self-monitoring inter- students increased the number of assign-
vention, the participants’ accuracy of ments that they completed. However, most
self-monitoring ranged from 18% to 98% across students did not show an improvement.
sessions. Despite inaccuracies in self-recording, Therefore, during the next week, Mrs.
marked decreases in stereotypic behavior were Simon placed a post-it note on each stu-
observed in trained settings across all partici- dent’s desk at the start of morning work
pants, including when the number of boxes and every day. The students were instructed to
length of intervals was increased. record a tally mark on their post-it when
they completed an assignment.
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 583

30.2.3 Self-Evaluation children were taught to indicate whether they had


engaged in a specified target behavior by marking
Self-evaluation or self-assessment involves either a smiling or frowning face next to a picture
monitoring one’s own behavior and making of the behavior following completion of a task.
accurate judgments about the appropriateness of Next, students reviewed their self-­assessment with
that behavior. Self-evaluation typically involves the teacher individually and were praised for both
self-­
recording and comparing the results to appropriate behavior and accuracy of self-assess-
another observer’s record. Within a self-man- ment (i.e., matching with the teacher). Finally, stu-
agement context, individuals are generally dents received a small toy contingent on matching
taught to (1) assess their own behavior and (2) with the teacher. This study included a component
“match” their assessment with those of a teacher analysis that was conducted by systematically
or clinician. Frequently, the individual is then withdrawing each component (i.e., reinforcement,
rewarded contingent on either the accuracy of matching with teacher). All four participants main-
their assessment or the quality of their perfor- tained high levels of appropriate behavior (on-
mance on the task (Sainato et  al., 1990). For task, waiting) with only the self-assessment
example, in an interval-­based self-monitoring component in place.
system, a clinician and student might compare A second use of the term self-evaluation
their data sheets to check for correspondence involves an individual assessing the extent to
following every interval initially. A data sheet which their engagement in the target behavior
that could be used at this stage is shown in meets a specified standard. This relates to goal
Fig.  30.2. Errors would be corrected and rein- setting as an individual may set a goal to attain,
forcement provided for matching responses. and then evaluate their own behavior in relation
Over time, the schedule of checks on accuracy to this goal. This type of self-evaluation may also
would be faded from continuous to intermittent involve comparisons to other-determined goals.
in order to increase the student’s independence Two responses are required for this type of self-­
with the self-monitoring system. Eventually, the evaluation: (1) the individual must recognize
reinforcement contingency would be shifted to when certain behavior has been emitted (i.e.,
engagement in the desired behavior only. self-monitor) and (2) determine whether their
Sainato et  al. (1990) evaluated the effect of a behavior meets a standard (King-Sears, 1999).
self-evaluation treatment package on the appropri- Self-evaluation involving comparison of a
ate classroom work behavior (i.e., on-task and response to a standard has been shown to be asso-
waiting) of preschoolers with disabilities. The ciated with increases in appropriate behavior
treatment package included self-assessment, (e.g., Sainato et al., 1990) and decreases in dis-
match with teacher, and reinforcement. First, the ruptive behavior (e.g., Koegel et al., 1992).

Period Complete Work Stay in Seat Kind Words & Actions

Student Staff Student Staff Student Staff

Period 1 Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N

Period 2 Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N
Period 3 Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N
Period 4 Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N Y / N

Total

Fig. 30.2  Self-monitoring data sheet including self-evaluation


584 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

In an example of self-evaluation toward a pre- after observing that a previously established cri-
set criterion, King-Sears (1999) taught a 7-year-­ terion for the target behavior has been met
old child with multiple disabilities to (Goldiamond, 1976). Self-reinforcement requires
independently transition across settings in her a repertoire of underlying skills including self-­
elementary school. The intervention included monitoring and self-evaluation. An individual
self-monitoring (i.e., checking off a smiley face must first monitor their own behavior (e.g., tally
for each step of the routine she completed appro- each completed math problem), then evaluate if
priately), self-evaluation (i.e., determining if she their behavior met an expected criterion (e.g.,
met the criterion of all three checks), and self-­ compare these tallies to the expectation of 10
reinforcement (i.e., accessing a reward if she met problems), and then provide oneself the rein-
the expected criterion). The intervention was forcer only if the behavior was equal to or above
effective in increasing the number of steps com- the criterion (e.g., 12 problems were completed).
pleted independently during two trained transi- The use of self-reinforcement techniques has
tions and generalization effects were observed resulted in decreased challenging behavior (e.g.,
across a third transition. Newman et al., 1997) and increased appropriate
behavior (e.g., Newman et  al., 1995; Shogren
et al., 2011).
Self-Evaluation in Mrs. Simon’s Class Using self-reinforcement, Koegel et  al.
While some students were observed to regu- (1992) demonstrated extended improvement in
larly record their completed assignments the social interactions of four children with
when provided with the post-it notes and ASD.  The social improvements observed were
instructions, Mrs. Simon noted that other associated with reductions in disruptive behav-
students were not recording tallies on their ior such as self-injury, tantrums, spinning, and
post-it notes. In addition, increases in assign- yelling. The participants ranged from 6 to
ment completion were observed in only a 11 years old and had been described as charac-
subset of the class. Mrs. Simon decided to teristically unresponsive to others’ social initia-
make two changes to her plan to incorporate tions. To promote responsiveness to others’
self-evaluation. First, Mrs. Simon taught her initiations, the participants were first taught to
students to graph the number of assignments discriminate between a correct and incorrect
completed each day. Mrs. Simon also response. They were then taught to self-monitor
graphed the number of assignments handed correct responses, which were continually rein-
in by each student each day and compared forced initially and then reinforced on a thinned
her graph to the students’ graphs. She pro- schedule. Eventually, the children were taught
vided feedback to the students on the accu- to self-reinforce by accessing the reinforcer on
racy of their self-­recording. Second, Mrs. their own (i.e., it was not delivered by the adult).
Simon had the students draw a yellow line The self-management procedure was effective
on their graph each morning to indicate their in improving sustained responding to questions
goal for the day. At the end of morning as well as decreasing disruptive behavior, which
work, Mrs. Simon reminded the students to importantly supports the use of self-manage-
check if they met their daily goal. ment procedures in cases where challenging
behavior may interfere with social opportunities
and where independence from adult support is
desirable.
30.2.4 Self-Reinforcement Similar to self-monitoring, accuracy of self-­
reinforcement has not been observed to be a nec-
Self-reinforcement involves an individual select- essary condition for the effectiveness of
ing and/or accessing desirable consequences self-management interventions. Newman et  al.
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 585

(1997) taught three children with ASD and mild 30.2.5 Self-Instruction
to moderate intellectual disability to implement a
differential reinforcement of other behavior Like other forms of self-management, self-­
(DRO) procedure to decrease their inappropriate instruction involves a controlling response that
behavior including out-of-seat and nail-flicking. manipulates the environment to increase the like-
For all participants, decreases in the target behav- lihood of a controlled response (Cooper et  al.,
ior were observed at the outset of adult-provided 2007; Skinner, 1953). In the context of self-­
contingent reinforcement and were observed to instruction, the controlling response is often overt
maintain in the presence of prompted self-­ or covert verbal behavior that serves as a prompt
management, unprompted self-management, and or additional discriminative stimulus to engage in
a follow-up phase. During self-management, all the controlled response (Cooper et  al., 2007;
participants were taught to provide themselves a Skinner, 1953). Self-instruction may also involve
token for each interval in which they did not controlling responses that increase access to
engage in the target behavior. In both the materials that alter the probability of the con-
unprompted self-management and follow-up trolled response, such as opening and observing
phase, data were collected on participant accu- an online video that provides instructions on how
racy with self-reinforcement. One participant to complete a task. Further self-instruction may
took approximately 50% of the tokens he had ensue following access to materials, such as
earned, another was observed to frequently take overtly or covertly repeating the instructions
tokens prior to the timer going off, whereas the from the video, which increases the likelihood of
third participant showed a greater degree of accu- recalling the steps delineated in the video
racy. Despite this variability, all participants (Epstein, 1997; Skinner, 1953). Humans are gen-
maintained substantial decreases in target behav- erally better at recalling rather than remember-
ior in the unprompted self-management and fol- ing, which is why self-instruction is observed to
low-­up phases. be an effective self-management technique
(Epstein, 1997). Here, recalling refers to behav-
ior evoked by a discriminative stimulus and sup-
Self-Reinforcement in Mrs. Simon’s Class plementary stimuli that the individual provides to
themselves in the form of a self-probe (e.g.,
After adding in self-evaluation, Mrs. When trying to recall the next step in a sequence,
Simon observed a substantial increase in the individual may covertly state, “The second
completed assignments during morning step is”; Skinner, 1953). Whereas, remembering
work. However, a few students were still is behavior evoked by a discriminative stimulus
handing in only one completed assign- and private stimuli that already exist in strength
ment. Mrs. Simon decided to add a self- and require no supplementary stimulation
reinforcement component to her plan to (Skinner, 1953).
see if she could increase task completion Hughes et  al. (1995) used self-instruction
for those last few students. Mrs. Simon combined with multiple exemplar training
placed an empty jar at the front of the (MET) to improve the conversational skills of
room. She informed the students that four individuals with moderate intellectual dis-
when they filled the jar with marbles, they abilities, including one with a comorbid ASD
could choose a class prize (e.g., bring a diagnosis. The self-instruction strategy, which
stuffed animal to class day, watch a movie was based on the steps outlined by Meichenbaum
at lunch). The students were told to put a and Goodman (1971), included (1) stating the
marble in the jar at the end of morning problem (e.g., “I want to talk”), (2) stating the
work if they met their daily goal. solution (e.g., “I need to look and talk”), (3)
evaluating the steps taken (e.g., “I did it, I
586 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

talked”), and (4) self-praise (e.g., “I did a good of a self-­management strategy for individuals
job”). The self-management intervention was with ASD can be described as a pivotal behavior
trained by typically developing peers in a high (Smith et al., 2015).
school setting. Across the conversational skills Palmer (1991) described problem solving as
targeted (e.g., initiation of conversations, eye the condition in which “the required response is
gaze toward conversational partners), all four part of the repertoire of the individual but is not
participants showed increases to levels consis- directly controlled by the nominal discrimina-
tent with typically developing peers. In addi- tive stimulus, the individual must engage in pre-
tion, all participants generalized these skills to current behavior providing himself with
conversations with novel conversational part- supplementary discriminative stimuli until the
ners with and without disabilities, generalized combined effect of the nominal and the supple-
skills to novel settings, and maintained most mentary stimuli are enough to occasion the tar-
skills at the range of expected behavior in a fol- get response” (p. 271). When behavior has not
low-­up 9–11  months later (for the two partici- been previously conditioned to occur in the
pants who continued to attend the school). presence of a discriminative stimulus, self-
In a study involving access to additional stim- instruction has been used as a problem-solving
uli as part of self-instruction, Smith et al. (2015) strategy to supply the needed supplementary
taught three adolescents with ASD to use instruc- stimuli. In this context, self-instruction has been
tional videos on a mobile device to teach them- shown to generalize across problem situations
selves to complete unknown tasks. Then a novel in the context of vocational activities (Hughes
task was presented to evaluate their ability to et  al., 1996). Self-­ management interventions
independently self-instruct. When the materials targeting problem-­ solving could have broad
for the task and the mobile device were made implications in terms of generalizability across
accessible, two of the three participants used the a variety of problem scenarios and domains. In a
device independently, whereas the third partici- series of experiments, Hughes and colleagues
pant required verbal prompting to use the mobile (Hughes et al., 1995, 1996) found that the inclu-
device to self-instruct. While the use of materials sion of MET with self-­instructional strategies
beyond the individual (i.e., instructional videos) improved maintenance and generalization of
may be seen as a limitation in this study, access to skills within social and vocational domains.
technology is commonly used as a method of Further research evaluating the generalizability
self-instruction in our society (Smith et al., 2015). of self-instruction problem-­solving repertoires
Frequently, when the steps to a task are unknown, across domains could be beneficial in identify-
people use the internet to access information ing far-reaching strategies. Table 30.1 provides
that will allow them to self-instruct (Lopez & a summary of the self-management procedures
Wiskow, 2020; Smith et al., 2015). Consequently, discussed, including definitions and examples
teaching this problem-solving repertoire as part of each.
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 587

Self-Instruction in Mrs. Simon’s Class ment a self-­instruction intervention for these


Following the addition of self-­reinforcement, two students. First, she placed the visual
Mrs. Simon observed an increase in assign- below on their desks. Then, Mrs. Simon
ment completion across all but two students reviewed the visual with the students and
in her class. Mrs. Simon decided to imple- modeled its use.

30.3 Self-Management ers (Koegel et  al., 1992; Newman et  al., 1995;
and Autism Spectrum Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992). Prompting is a
Disorder common and evidence-based procedure for
teaching skills (Cooper et  al., 2007), and it is
An advantage of self-management, which may be commonly used to teach children with ASD
particularly important for individuals with ASD, (Wilson et  al., 2014). Although prompts can be
is that this repertoire increases the likelihood that effective while teaching, individuals with ASD
an individual will engage in appropriate behavior may become dependent on prompts, particularly
in the absence of prompting or support from oth- if the prompts are not systematically faded
588 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

Table 30.1  Definitions and examples of the components Table 30.1 (continued)


of self-management
Term Definition Example
Term Definition Example Self-­ Engaging in a The time required
Goal setting Selecting a A student you instruction controlling to sit has
performance work with is response that increased, and
standard to having difficulty manipulates the you find that your
achieve, in order staying seated environment to student is
to change during work increase the frequently getting
behavior sessions. He sets likelihood of a out of his seat
the following controlled again. You
goal, “during response provide him with
work time, I will a flowchart on his
sit for 5 minutes.” desk:
Self-­ Systematically Your student is  1. Do I need
monitoring observing one’s continuing to something?
own behavior have difficulty  2. Yes, I can
and responding staying seated ask for what I
to the occurrence independently, need
or nonoccurrence you give him a  3. No, I need to
of the specified timer to record stay in my seat
target response how long he sits  4. I can get up
for during the when my timer
work session. matches my
Self-­ Monitoring one’s To continue to goal
evaluation own behavior increase the
and making duration of
accurate sitting, you critical to plan for independence and self-­
judgments about provide the management for individuals with ASD. Sustained
the student with a attention and independence with academic tasks,
appropriateness length of time to daily living routines, and social interactions can
of that behavior match his timer
to (e.g., 6:00). promote autonomous and self-driven lifestyles
The student runs and reduce reliance on caregivers and staff. Self-­
the timer up, and management strategies have been used widely as
when it matches a method to promote independence and decrease
the time written,
he is asked to dependence on treatment providers (Stahmer &
identify that he Schreibman, 1992).
met his goal. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses on the
Self-­ Selecting and/or Your student has use of self-management techniques with
reinforcement accessing increased his time
individuals with ASD have shown that self-­
­
desirable sitting to 10 min
consequences and now you management is an effective intervention for this
after observing want him to be population (e.g., Carr et  al., 2014; Lee et  al.,
that a previously able to access his 2007). Lee et al. (2007) identified 11 articles pub-
established own reward. You
lished between 1992 and 2001 that evaluated the
criterion for the have the student
target behavior determine if he use of self-­management interventions to improve
has been met met his criteria, the appropriate behavior of individuals with
and then take out ASD.  The authors analyzed trends across the
a preferred
studies and found that 64% of the studies used
activity.
self-­
management packages that included
(continued)
prompts, self-monitoring, and self-reinforcement
procedures, 64% of the studies provided pretrain-
(Bryan & Gast, 2000). This tendency to respond ing and discrimination training prior to the self-­
to the teaching prompts rather than naturally management intervention, and 73% of the studies
occurring environmental cues makes it especially targeted only increases in appropriate behavior. A
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 589

meta-analysis using the percentage of nonover- Preschool-aged children were the only group that
lapping data (PND) in baseline and intervention did not meet this criterion. This group’s PND
showed that 81.9% of data points did not overlap, averaged 68.4%, below the 70% criteria to be
displaying that self-management is an effective considered effective, which the authors asserted
treatment (Lee et  al., 2007; Scruggs & may have been a result of the experimental proce-
Mastropieri, 1998). dures in one of the two studies that targeted this
Southall and Gast (2011) compared self-­ population. Finally, Carr and colleagues evalu-
management interventions across two groups of ated the findings in reference to established
individuals with ASD: individuals diagnosed parameters for synthesizing single-subject litera-
with autistic disorder (AD) and individuals diag- ture to determine if an intervention is empirically
nosed with high-functioning autism (HFA) or supported. Both the WWC and Kratochwill et al.
Asperger’s syndrome (AS). The authors identi- (2013) recommended the following standards:
fied 16 studies between 1994 and 2008 that used (1) five single-subject design papers published in
a single-subject design to evaluate the effective- a peer-reviewed journal and meeting quality
ness of a self-management intervention for an assessment standards, (2) studies are conducted
individual with AD, HFA, or AS.  Interventions across three separate teams in three different geo-
used for both groups were similar and most com- graphic locations, and (3) the studies include a
monly consisted of self-monitoring, self-­ minimum of 20 total participants. According to
recording, and self-reinforcement. While token these criteria, self-management interventions for
economies were used more frequently with the individuals with ASD exceed the standards
AD group and contracting was used only with the required and can, therefore, be considered an
HFA/AS group, other intervention components effective treatment for individuals with ASD
including peer training, pictures, and videos were between the ages of 3 and 20  years old (Carr
used across groups. Despite these differences, et al., 2014).
Southall and Gast found that self-management
interventions were effective for individuals with
AD and HFA/AS with all participants showing an 30.4 Uses of Self-Management
increase in targeted skills. Social validity mea- for Individuals with Autism
sures across studies also indicated that the inter- Spectrum Disorder
ventions were perceived by parents, teachers, and
peers as effective and easy to implement. For all individuals, self-management may be
As the previous mentioned reviews did not conceptualized as a pivotal skill. This is because
screen studies based on quality, in their 2014 learning to self-manage one’s own behavior
meta-analysis, Carr and colleagues only included ­facilitates opportunities to engage across envi-
self-management research for individuals with ronments, with various people, and independent
ASD that met the quality standards outlined by from a support provider (Koegel & Koegel, 1990;
the What Works Clearinghouse (WWC). The Koegel et al., 1999). Opportunities afforded may
authors identified 23 articles published in peer-­ be social, academic, or adaptive.
reviewed journals between 1992 and 2008. The
mean PND score of all participant and experi-
mental data was 84.3%, which indicates that self-­ 30.4.1 Social Skills
management can be considered an effective
treatment for increasing appropriate behavior for Access to social opportunities may be preceded
individuals with ASD.  The authors calculated by certain foundational or prerequisite skills
mean PND scores across a number of variables coming together. Vernon (2017) noted that “as
including functioning level, target behavior, and social motivation, language sophistication, and
setting and found all but one PND score to be in cognitive development converge, the stage is set
either the effective or highly effective range. for self-monitoring social exchanges. This is the
590 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

developmental precipice of a third pivotal area of Stahmer and Schreibman (1992) taught three
development—the area of self-monitoring, man- children with ASD to appropriately play with
agement, and regulation” (p.  193). As a pivotal toys in the absence of a treatment provider, which
area of development, self-managing one’s own represented an important extension of the exist-
social behavior can be a critical skill in moving ing self-management literature. Specifically, the
toward independent and effective social engage- researchers demonstrated the effectiveness of
ment, including the development of friendships self-management as a procedure to promote gen-
and other relationships. Self-monitoring and self-­ eralization of taught skills in the absence of both
assessment are skills that typically developing treatment providers and self-management materi-
individuals often engage in as a way to modify als. This suggests that appropriate play may have
their behavior in response to social feedback. As been under the control of the individuals’ own
individuals on the autism spectrum often experi- behavior rather than the materials or trainer. The
ence challenges with understanding and engag- children maintained appropriate play 1  month
ing in typical social behavior, developing later during a maintenance follow-up.
independence with navigating social situations is Additionally, self-stimulatory behavior (e.g.,
of critical importance. flapping, spinning) decreased following imple-
Importantly, self-management affords a level mentation of the self-management intervention.
of independence for the training and generaliza- Appropriate and varied play skills are particu-
tion of social skills that other interventions may larly important targets for individuals with ASD
not and is therefore a viable intervention for since children with ASD characteristically
promoting independence for individuals with engage in repetitive, restricted, or inappropriate
ASD (Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992). That is, play that limits their opportunities to engage in
self-­management allows the individual to moni- more typical play with their peers (American
tor and adjust their own responses in the absence Psychiatric Association, 2013). By teaching chil-
of a practitioner’s immediate involvement. Self-­ dren with ASD to self-manage their own appro-
management in academic and community set- priate play responses, they may be more likely to
tings is particularly important for this reason as be invited to social groups and maintain social
social interactions may be hindered by clinician interactions over time. These opportunities are
prompting (Koegel et  al., 1992). Self- ultimately critical to promoting friendships.
management procedures have also been used to Newman et  al. (2000) taught three individuals
increase social interactions of typical students with ASD to vary their play and other social
and their peers with ASD, which aligns with the responses using a self-management intervention.
movement away from adult facilitation. As dis- The participants were taught first to self-monitor
cussed previously, Hughes et al. (2013) imple- their variations in responding, and then
mented a treatment package involving goal ­self-­reinforce (i.e., take a token contingent on
setting and self-­monitoring to increase the social varying their response in relation to previous
interactions between high school students with responses in that session). Participants were
ASD and their typically developing peers. taught to vary their responding in imaginative
Social skills and play continue to be areas of play activities, verbal responding to social ques-
significant interest in ASD research and prac- tions, and drawing/coloring activities. Variation
tice. Self-management interventions have been in social skills may facilitate opportunities for
used to teach a variety of social skills, including more positive social interactions and friendships,
responsiveness to social initiations (Koegel and ultimately lead to a larger repertoire of lei-
et  al., 1992), appropriate play (Stahmer & sure activities.
Schreibman, 1992), appropriate social commu- In addition to play skills, prosocial behavior
nication (Koegel & Frea, 1993), conversation can be targeted with the use of self-management
skills (Koegel et  al., 2014), and compliment- techniques. One prosocial behavior, compliment-­
giving (Apple et al., 2005). giving, can facilitate relationships with greater
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 591

social reciprocity (Attwood, 1998). Apple et  al. ing have resulted in consequent reductions of
(2005) demonstrated an increase in compliment-­ disruptive behavior (e.g., Koegel et  al., 1992;
giving for three preschoolers using a video model Soares et  al., 2009). Educational agencies have
and self-management treatment package. The often been tasked with teaching self-management
children were shown videos of peers engaging in skills (Lovitt, 1973; Skinner, 1953); however, it is
both “initiations” (i.e., compliments given in the also evident that self-management is primary to
absence of a peer approaching such as, “I like students’ success in educational systems, where
your picture!”) as well as “responses” (i.e., com- students are expected to maintain appropriate and
pliments given as a response to a peer’s initiation on-task behavior in both structured (e.g., class-
of, “Look!”). Video modeling improved partici- room) and unstructured (e.g., cafeteria, recess)
pants’ engagement in compliment initiations and settings.
responses, and the self-management component Delano (2007) evaluated the use of the self-­
allowed the preschoolers to independently moni- regulated strategy development model (SRSD) to
tor and maintain their compliment-giving. Two of improve essay writing for three adolescents diag-
the three participants generalized compliment-­ nosed with Asperger’s syndrome. SRSD, which
giving across settings, and both parents and had been previously shown to be effective for stu-
teachers reported improved scores in general dents with learning disabilities, is a combined
social skills as compared to pre-study ratings. package using self-video modeling to instruct
Parent reports also indicated that the participants students on self-management strategies including
generalized their compliment-giving in the goal setting, self-monitoring, and self-evaluation.
absence of self-management materials (i.e., wrist The intervention was effective in increasing the
counter or paper checklist), suggesting that this number of words written, the number of func-
type of intervention may have validity and utility tional essay elements included (e.g., premises,
outside of the preschool setting. reasons, conclusion), and the duration of writing
Self-management of social skills may lead to time for the trained skill of writing a persuasive
increased opportunities for social engagement essay. Increased number of words written was
and improved quality of social relationships. observed to generalize to the writing of an expos-
Various studies discussed here reported social itory essay and to maintain in follow-up essays
validity measures that demonstrated improve- 1 week and 3 months later.
ment in the quality of social interactions. For Asaro-Saddler and Saddler (2010) also used
example, Koegel et  al. (2014) noted that naive SRSD to improve the writing abilities of individ-
observers reported meaningful improvements in uals with ASD. The researchers used a scaffolded
participants’ conversational skills across interest, set of six lessons to teach three elementary-­school
naturalness, and desirability as a conversational children to use goal setting, self-monitoring, self-­
partner following self-management intervention. evaluation, and self-instruction to increase the
number of story elements and words in their fic-
tional stories and to improve holistic story quality
30.4.2 Academic Skills (as measured on an eight-point scale). PND was
analyzed and the treatment was observed to be
Self-management interventions have been used very effective across all dependent measures.
to increase a variety of academic and related Notably, participants were observed to overtly
skills including writing (Asaro-Saddler & use self-management strategies including writing
Saddler, 2010; Delano, 2007), following direc- bullets for the number of story elements required
tions (Agran et  al., 2005), on-task behavior and crossing them out as they included them and
(Harris, 1986), classroom behavior (Shogren stating self-instruction and self-prompts aloud
et al., 2011), and independent work skills (Sainato (e.g., “I know I can do this.”). On a social validity
et al., 1990). Additionally, self-management pro- measure, participants all indicated that they
cedures targeted at improving academic respond- believed the strategies made them better writers.
592 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

In addition to academic skills, classroom to directly target on-task behavior versus those
behavior is an important target for individuals designed to target academic productivity (Harris,
with ASD. Increased independence and appropri- 1986). In an initial experimental evaluation,
ate behavior can lead to increased academic Harris (1986) compared self-monitoring of on-­
opportunities because of the decreased potential task behavior and academic productivity for four
for interruption to classroom activities as a result children with learning disabilities during spelling
of disruptive behavior or prompting from support practice. All participants were taught to record if
staff. Self-management in inclusive classroom they were on-task when a timer sounded and to
settings can consequently promote increased record the number of spelling words they wrote
opportunities for students to engage both with during the session. While consistent increases
peers and with academic content. were observed in on-task behavior under both
Shogren et al. (2011) compared the effective- interventions, during the academic productivity
ness of a token economy and self-management phase, one participant consistently wrote a
intervention for two kindergarten students with greater number of words and two others showed
ASD in an inclusive classroom setting. During variable increased words written compared to the
baseline, the two participants did not consistently on-task phase. When provided a choice of inter-
engage in the classroom rules: stay in your space, vention following the study, three of the students
keep your hands to yourself, and do what the chose academic productivity (although one later
teacher says. These deficits were reported to switched to on-task), and the final participant
interfere with the students’ focus on classroom chose to run a combined approach. Social valid-
activities and task instructions. The children were ity data showed that all participants and the
first taught to discriminate between examples and teacher approved of both interventions.
nonexamples of each rule, and then a token econ- In an effort to control for the limitations of
omy managed by the teacher was introduced. Harris (1986; e.g., differences between the inter-
During the self-management phase, the same vention formats with self-recording of productiv-
token board was utilized, but the students were ity only including a graphical representation),
taught to report on their own rule following Lloyd et  al. (1989) also compared self-­
behavior during three classroom activities. The management interventions targeting on-task
students showed a high level of accuracy in behavior and academic productivity. Lloyd et al.
recording their own behavior, and both the token found that all five students with learning and/or
economy and self-management interventions led emotional disabilities displayed increased com-
to improvements in classroom behavior. Although pletion of math work and on-task behavior as a
the rates of behavior change were comparable in result of both interventions. Unlike the previous
both treatment conditions, the classroom teacher study, the students displayed a preference for the
reported a preference for the self-management on-task self-management intervention. The stu-
procedure. The teacher adopted, maintained, and dents stated that the academic productivity inter-
expanded the self-management intervention into vention was confusing, indicating that these
a class-wide system following the completion of selections may have been reflective of the
the study. The high social validity of this inter- increased complexity of this procedure resulting
vention has implications for the implementation from efforts to make it more consistent with the
of self-management strategies that can improve on-task intervention (Lloyd et al., 1989). Another
access to learning in classroom settings. possibility suggested by the authors was that the
Restricted and repetitive behavior associated graphical representation of academic productiv-
with ASD can interfere with a student’s attention ity in Harris’ study may have functioned as a
to academic tasks, and as such on-task behavior reinforcer. Further evaluation of this potential
is a common concern for individuals with effect of self-graphing is merited.
ASD.  There has been debate about the relative While Harris’ (1986) findings displayed that
value of self-management interventions designed self-management of productivity was marginally
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 593

more effective and preferred, Lloyd et al. (1989) pre-vocational tasks such as packaging, boxing,
found no difference between the two self-­ and assembly tasks (e.g., Mace et al., 1986; Zohn
management interventions and, converse to & Bornstein, 1980). Although most participants
Harris, observed a preference for the on-task self-­ included in this review were diagnosed with
management intervention. As such, it can be developmental disabilities, some were identified
inferred that both intervention types are useful as having ASD (e.g., Koegel & Koegel, 1990;
for improving academic performance and on-task Sainato et al., 1990). Given that self-management
behavior of individuals with disabilities. Notably, procedures have been shown to be effective in
this line of research has focused on individuals addressing a wide range of skills for individuals
with disabilities other than ASD, and empirical with developmental disabilities, these procedures
evaluation of this effect with an ASD population should be considered for use across these same
is needed to determine if similar findings would domains for individuals with ASD.
be observed. Further research could also evaluate In one study that evaluated the use of a self-­
additional variables, such as self-monitoring for- management intervention for teaching individu-
mat, that may impact student preference and als with ASD independent living skills, Newman
effectiveness of self-management interventions. et  al. (1995) implemented a self-management
package with three adolescents and found that
their accurate verbal identification of transition
30.4.3 Independent Living times (as indicated by a schedule and timer)
and Vocational Skills increased following training. Students were
taught to use tokens to self-reinforce their accu-
Self-management interventions to increase inde- rate statements that it was time for a transition.
pendent and adaptive skills for individuals with Consistent with this example of schedule-­
ASD have also been shown to be effective following, self-management procedures have
(Newman et  al., 1995; Pierce & Schreibman, been shown to be successful in increasing perfor-
1994), though the empirical basis for these tar- mance of other important independent and daily
gets is less extensive than for social or academic living skills (Lovett & Haring, 1989).
skills. Task engagement in vocational and inde- Pierce and Schreibman (1994) evaluated the
pendent living skills is paramount to improving use of a self-management package on daily living
successful employment and other community skills in three individuals with ASD between 6
opportunities for individuals with ASD, as reli- and 9 years old. The treatment package included
ance on parents and service agencies is common pictorial task analyses as prompts, self-selection
across the lifespan (Howlin et al., 2004). of reinforcers, and self-reinforcement. All three
While there is less research on the use of self-­ participants mastered their three individualized
management interventions for the improvement target routines, which included tasks such as set-
of daily living skills for individuals with ASD ting the table, getting dressed, and doing laundry.
specifically, there is ample evidence to suggest This study extended the previous literature by
that self-management strategies have been effec- evaluating the efficacy of self-management inter-
tive in this domain across individuals with devel- ventions in young children with ASD and moder-
opmental disabilities (e.g., Carr et  al., 2014; ate to severe intellectual disabilities and by
Harchik et  al., 1992; Pierce & Schreibman, evaluating student completion of daily living
1994). Harchik et al. (1992) reviewed nearly 60 skills in the absence of supervision. In other
studies that used self-management procedures to words, individuals were asked to engage in self-­
address a variety of skills for individuals with selection of reinforcement, self-prompting, and
developmental disabilities. Participants were self-reinforcement without a parent or researcher
taught to use self-management strategies to present. The ability to generalize skill completion
increase on-task behavior (e.g., Hughes & to these conditions illustrates the value of self-­
Petersen, 1989; Sugai & Rowe, 1984), as well as monitoring interventions in increasing the gener-
594 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

alizability of skills, increasing independence, and participation in her public school setting. The
decreasing the need for adult support. Finally, intervention was effective in decreasing vocaliza-
Pierce and Schreibman analyzed the training tions and both the length of the interval and the
time required for each skill and found that for criteria required for reinforcement were able to
each participant, the time required to teach the be increased during training sessions with the
self-management skills decreased progressively special education teacher. However, intervals
across the skills taught. This provides further were only able to be lengthened to a period of
support for self-management as a tool that could 40  s and, despite the student not achieving full
both improve student performance of daily living independence with the self-management proce-
skills and decrease the long-term response effort dures, the training time was significant. The spe-
of families and teachers. cial education teacher indicated the student was
quieter and had more appropriate behavior when
the self-management intervention was in place,
30.4.4 Decreasing Challenging indicating that despite these limitations the inter-
Behavior vention was still considered to be socially valid.
Like Mancina et  al., Singh et  al. (2011) found
While decreasing challenging behavior in indi- that while a self-management intervention using
viduals with ASD has often not been the primary mindfulness strategies was effective for decreas-
target of research on self-management interven- ing the aggressive behavior of three boys with
tions, many researchers have evaluated decreases ASD in their home settings, the training time
in challenging behavior as a secondary measure required for the intervention and the time required
(e.g., Koegel et  al., 1992; Soares et  al., 2009; to achieve notable results were lengthy (i.e.,
Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992). For example, 23–30 weeks before the participants met the cri-
when using self-management to increase aca- teria of zero incidents for 4 weeks).
demic task completion, off-task behavior has Unlike the previous interventions discussed,
been observed to simultaneously decrease Coyle and Cole (2004) observed rapid and con-
(Harris, 1986; Lloyd et al., 1989). In a study with sistent results across participants when an inter-
one individual with ASD, Soares et  al. (2009) vention package including self-monitoring,
increased academic task completion through self-reinforcement, and self-video modeling was
implementation of a self-monitoring package and implemented to decrease the off-task behavior of
observed simultaneous decreases in tantrums and three children with ASD.  The intervention
self-injury when the intervention was applied and included video observations of their own on-task
increases in these challenging behaviors when behavior, self-monitoring using a timer and
the intervention was removed. As discussed pre- visual check sheet, and self-reinforcement when
viously, improvements in challenging behavior criteria were met. Off-task behavior was shown
have also been observed when self-management to vary consistently with the implementation and
interventions are targeted at improving social withdrawal of the intervention including in a fol-
skills (e.g., Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992). low-­up 2 weeks later. In addition, for one partici-
Some studies have more directly addressed pant, an increased interval length of 1 min showed
the use of self-management interventions to more substantial decreases in off-task behavior.
decrease challenging behavior for individuals The ability to successfully increase interval
with ASD. Mancina et al. (2000) implemented a length within self-management systems is prom-
self-management package including self-­ising for generalization of interventions across
monitoring, self-evaluation, and self-­settings and activities. Lengthier intervals are
reinforcement with a 12-year-old girl diagnosed less intrusive and cause less disruption to activi-
with ASD and a moderate intellectual disability ties, making procedures more feasible for use in
to decrease vocalizations (i.e., humming, tongue settings, such as general education classrooms
clicking, echolalia) that were interfering with her and work sites. However, as this was observed
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 595

with only one participant, further evaluation of recording, self-evaluation, and


this effect is needed to determine the potential for self-reinforcement.
increasing interval lengths in self-management Similarly, there is a need to evaluate the sepa-
systems targeting decreases in challenging rate components of self-management programs
behavior. to identify which are effective and necessary
(Carr et  al., 2014; Southall & Gast, 2011). As
Stahmer and Schreibman (1992) indicated, the
30.5 Future Research “problem of separating self-management from
in Self-Management other factors of treatment plagues the self-­
management literature” (p. 457). Future research
The effectiveness of self-management packages should include component analyses to identify
has been attributed to many variables. As with which self-management techniques are most effi-
self-management broadly, the mechanisms cient and effective and which provide little value
underlying self-management interventions are or only repetitious effects. Of particular focus
not well understood or agreed upon (Stahmer & should be less utilized components of self-­
Schreibman, 1992). One possible mechanism is management, including goal setting (Carr et al.,
that the individual becomes a stimulus associated 2014). Identification of effective components
with the controlling and controlled responses could have a significant impact on clinical prac-
removing the need for behavior change agents, tice as self-management packages may be able to
such as parents and therapists, to evoke the target be narrowed in scope. Narrowing the components
behavior. Understanding those mechanisms that of self-management packages could make these
underlie self-management interventions may interventions more attractive to teachers and
allow researchers and practitioners to increase caregivers who may presently perceive these
the effectiveness and efficiency through which interventions as requiring significant response
self-management interventions are used. If the effort due to the multifaceted nature of these
mechanisms were better understood, then self-­ intervention packages.
management interventions that most efficiently Further, a deficiency of procedural integrity
allow individuals to access the necessary contin- data in the research evaluating self-management
gencies could be developed. interventions makes it challenging to ascertain
There is a particular need for research that elu- which components are effective in both the train-
cidates the contingencies that lead to the effec- ing of students to implement these systems and in
tiveness of self-monitoring and self-reinforcement the systems themselves (Southall & Gast, 2011).
procedures in the absence of accuracy. Koegel Future research should attend to the accuracy of
and Koegel (1990) provided some potential implementation of self-management techniques
mechanisms that may have led to the effective- by participants as well as the accuracy of imple-
ness of self-monitoring interventions in the mentation of training procedures by instructors.
absence of accuracy in their study, including This research could lead to more robust findings
reactivity to self-recording and inadvertent rein- about best-practice models for training stakehold-
forcement of appropriate behavior instead of ers (e.g., parents, teachers, peers) to teach indi-
self-management behavior. The authors noted viduals with ASD to self-manage their behavior
that the students’ errors were most commonly in and evidence-based training methods for those
recording the presence of stereotypic behavior stakeholders to use when training individuals with
rather than the absence; as such, it is possible that ASD to implement self-management techniques.
the recording of the absence of stereotypic behav- While the role of peers as trainers has been evalu-
ior was the controlling variable. Further studies ated in previous self-management research (e.g.,
should evaluate these potential explanations of Hughes et al., 1995, 2013), further study of poten-
the effectiveness of self-management systems in tial roles for and effective use of peers in self-
the absence of accurate performance of self-­ management systems could contribute to the
596 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

development of self-management packages that tics of self-management interventions on desired


require fewer adult resources and have an changes in target behavior and social validity. For
increased likelihood of generalizing across set- example, across previous research, self-recording
tings (Carr et al., 2014; Southall & Gast, 2011). and self-evaluation have varied significantly in
The resources used within self-management terms of the length of intervals. Future research
systems may also have a large impact on the like- should continue to work toward the development
lihood of generalization. Southall and Gast of best-practice guidelines for length of intervals,
(2011) suggested making materials and equip- criteria for increasing intervals, and the terminal
ment unobtrusive to increase the potential for length of intervals required for social validity.
generalization. For example, much of the previ- Another variable that has been asserted to impact
ous research used auditory timers to cue self-­ the effectiveness of self-management interven-
monitoring (e.g., Harris, 1986; Koegel & Koegel, tions is the format of self-monitoring (Harris
1990); however, vibrating timers or similarly 1986; Lloyd et al., 1989). DiGangi et al. (1991)
functioning apps on smartphones may be less evaluated the effectiveness of self-­ monitoring
disruptive and stigmatizing. Evaluations are with and without self-graphing for two individu-
needed to determine if results of self-monitoring als with learning disabilities. The researchers
procedures using these technologies are consis- found greater improvements in both students’
tent with previous results. Dynamic technology is academic performance and for one student’s on-
important to consider broadly across self-­ task behavior when graphing was incorporated.
management procedures. The advent of smart- Further evaluation of different self-­ monitoring
phones, smart watches, and other ubiquitous formats is needed to evaluate if these findings
technologies increases the likelihood that compo- would generalize beyond the limited population
nents of self-management strategies can be readily evaluated in this study and if similar results would
incorporated into the academic, vocational, and be observed in individuals with ASD.
social lives of individuals with ASD.  Relatedly, The empirical study of self-management
Lopez and Wiskow (2019) taught individuals interventions for individuals with ASD is robust.
with ASD to respond to tactile and textual However, while the overall effectiveness of self-­
prompts on an Apple Watch® to increase social management interventions for individuals with
initiations. While recent research has begun to ASD has been shown, more research is needed to
evaluate this technology, further study is needed evaluate the use of self-management interven-
to assess the value of such personal devices in tions under specific conditions for this p­ opulation.
improving self-management procedures includ- For example, in their systematic review, Carr
ing self-monitoring, self-­ reinforcement, and et al. (2014) noted a lack of literature evaluating
self-instruction. the use of self-management systems to improve
Videos including self-video modeling (Coyle academic skills for lower functioning individuals
& Cole, 2004), peer video modeling (Apple et al., with ASD and to target social skills in preschool-
2005), and other instructional videos (Smith aged children with ASD. Notably, there is also a
et al., 2015) have been used as part of efficacious lack of literature evaluating the use of self-­
self-management interventions for individuals management techniques to target increases in lei-
with ASD.  While the use of videos and other sure skills for individuals with ASD.
visual prompts has been highly effective, only a
small number of studies have incorporated these
components (Carr et  al., 2014). Researchers 30.6 Clinical Use and Benefits
should continue to evaluate the importance and of Self-Management
relative effectiveness of these materials as part of
self-management packages. One of the primary benefits of self-management
Finally, researchers should continue to evalu- procedures is increased independence. Since
ate the impact of more idiosyncratic characteris- individuals with ASD can become dependent on
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 597

prompts from adults (Bryan & Gast, 2000), it is students (Agran et al., 2005). There is also evi-
critical that interventions systematically transfer dence that teachers value self-management inter-
at least partial control over behavior from the cli- ventions and expand these interventions to other
nician to the individual themselves. The transfer students and responses (Shogren et  al., 2011).
of control that is inherent in self-management Increased independence with behavior manage-
interventions makes them valuable procedures to ment is associated with reduced parental stress;
increase independence (Newman et al., 1995). therefore, similar to teachers, there are benefits to
Self-management interventions have notable self-management procedures from a family per-
advantages over other types of interventions. As spective (Koegel et al., 1992). Additionally, pro-
discussed above, because the individual becomes cedures that involve self-management
a discriminative stimulus or prompt for self-­ components are generally rewarding for the indi-
management behavior, these skills can be used vidual (Ganz & Sigafoos, 2005). Participants
over time in the absence of ongoing support have reported increased confidence in their skills
(Newman et al., 1996). Cooper et al. (2007) iden- following self-management interventions
tified a number of additional benefits of self-­ (Holifield et al., 2010) and noted they would rec-
management interventions including that the ommend self-management procedures to others
individual may be able to catch all occurrences of (Harris, 1986).
a behavior including those that might be missed Relatedly, social benefits are also associated
by a clinician, individuals can observe their own with self-management procedures. As discussed
behavior that is unobservable to others, self-­ above, general social skills ratings (Apple et al.,
management responses can control many topog- 2005) as well as dimensions of conversational
raphies of other behavior, self-management has skills (Hughes et al., 1995; Koegel et al., 2014)
been shown to be effective across populations, improved following interventions using self-­
and self-management helps people “feel free and management. Improvements in these and other
good” (p. 586). Self-management skills also gen- social responses can lead to greater access to a
eralize more readily to new environments because variety of settings and social opportunities for
of the ongoing presence of the individual as a individuals with ASD.
controlling stimulus (Newman et al., 1996). Self-management is a pivotal skill that has
Self-management interventions have led to been associated with generalization of skills
improved behavior across a variety of settings across settings and behaviors (Koegel et  al.,
including special education classrooms (e.g., 1999), resulting in a level of independence that is
Holifield et al., 2010; Lloyd et al., 1989), general unique to these types of systems. As the ­individual
education classrooms (e.g., Shogren et al., 2011), becomes the discriminative stimulus or prompt
unstructured school settings (e.g., Hughes et al., for engagement in target behavior, these systems
2013), home environments (e.g., Singh et  al., do not require extensive materials for implemen-
2011), and community settings (e.g., Koegel & tation (Ganz & Sigafoos, 2005). Control of behav-
Koegel, 1990). The broad variety of environ- ior by the individual also increases the likelihood
ments in which self-management interventions of maintenance of intervention results (Loftin
have been efficacious indicates that self-manage- et al., 2008; Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992) with
ment systems are not confined to one setting and students continuing to perform at high levels
can and should be used broadly by practitioners when interventions are faded (Lloyd et al., 1989).
across the settings in which they work with However, maintenance in the absence of self-
clients. management materials is not consistently
Self-management systems have also been observed (Koegel & Koegel, 1990), indicating
associated with high levels of social validity. that the materials used in training and initial
Teachers who implement self-management sys- implementation of self-­management systems may
tems in their classrooms report that they have also be an important stimulus-­controlling behav-
more time to spend on instruction and across ior. Given the findings related to maintenance
598 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

and materials, practitioners should select self- 2005). In addition, ongoing instruction and inter-
management materials that are nonstigmatizing, mittent feedback are often included components
commonly found in the environment, and can be and may improve self-evaluation (e.g., Koegel &
easily reimplemented if needed. Koegel, 1990).
While many recommendations for best prac- In building self-management systems, it is rec-
tice when implementing self-management sys- ommended that clinicians maintain the assump-
tems can be drawn from the previous literature, tion of parsimony and keep it simple. Notably,
questions still abound regarding the underlying when self-monitoring procedures are overly com-
mechanisms and effective components of self-­ plex (Lloyd et al., 1989) or multiple components
management systems. Despite studies evaluating are implemented simultaneously (DiGangi et al.,
the relative efficacy of self-management inter- 1991), interventions have been observed to
ventions for work completion and on-task behav- become less effective. For example, DiGangi
ior (Harris, 1986; Lloyd et al., 1989), a singular et  al. (1991) observed improvements in on-task
best option has not been identified. As such, it has behavior across two individuals with learning dis-
been suggested that selection of targets for self-­ abilities when self-monitoring and self-graphing
management systems should be determined by were combined but saw subsequent decreases in
individual student need, ongoing data collection, on-task behavior when self-­ reinforcement was
and measures of client preference (Lloyd et  al., added to the package. For one participant,
1989). Notably, while some studies have evalu- increases were again observed when self-evalua-
ated self-management interventions to decrease tion was added, indicating that the decrease may
challenging behavior, these studies have shown have been related to a lack of self-­evaluation skills
mixed results (Mancina et al., 2000; Singh et al., for this individual. However, it has been observed
2011) and a much larger research base supports that complex interventions may detract from stu-
the use of self-management to increase appropri- dents’ success with self-­monitoring systems and,
ate behavior (Carr et al., 2014). As such, it is rec- as such, should be avoided.
ommended that responses to increase are
prioritized when selecting targets for self-­
management interventions. Recommendations for Using
While the evidence is only preliminary, there Self-Management
is some support for self-graphing increasing the
effectiveness of self-monitoring (DiGangi et al., Planning your self-management system:
1991; Harris, 1986). The mechanisms through
which this increase is observed are unclear. Some • Select materials
possibilities have been suggested including self-­ –– Readily available
graphing functioning as a reinforcer and self-­ –– Easy to use
graphing increasing reactivity to self-monitoring –– Easily incorporated into the existing
(DiGangi et  al., 1991). However, practitioners milieu
may want to consider utilizing this component of –– Easily reimplemented if needed
self-monitoring with careful evaluation of the • Recruit teacher or caregiver input
effects, given the limited empirical support for • Select a behavior to increase
and limited understanding of the procedure. –– Collect data on baseline levels to
Inclusion of discrimination training has gener- establish criteria for reinforcement
ally been shown to be beneficial as an initial com- • Create procedures that are simple to
ponent of self-management systems (Koegel & implement
Koegel, 1990). Common training components • Build in steps for the fading of clinician
include behavioral practice (Palmen & Didden, support
2012), instructions (Agran et  al., 2005), and • Consider incorporating into existing
modeling (Koegel et  al., 1986; Agran et  al., treatment package

(continued)
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 599

References
Implementing your self-management
system: Agran, M., Sinclair, T., Alper, S., Cavin, M., Wehmeyer,
M., & Hughes, C. (2005). Using self-monitoring to
• Incorporate discrimination training increase following-direction skills of students with
• Consider reinforcing accurate moderate to severe disabilities in general education.
Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities,
self-recording 40(1), 3–13.
–– Initially reinforce accuracy American Psychiatric Association (Ed.). (2013).
continuously Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
–– Fade reinforcement for accuracy and (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. https://
doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
shift reinforcement to the target Apple, A. L., Billingsley, F., Schwartz, I. S., & Carr, E. G.
behavior (2005). Effects of video modeling alone and with self-­
• Collect and analyze data to determine management on compliment-giving behaviors of chil-
needed changes to the plan dren with high-functioning ASD. Journal of Positive
Behavior Interventions, 7(1), 33–46. https://doi.org/1
0.1177/10983007050070010401
Asaro-Saddler, K., & Saddler, B. (2010). Planning
Self-management interventions are well estab- instruction and self-regulation training: Effects
lished as evidence-based procedures for improv- on writers with autism spectrum disorders.
Exceptional Children, 77(1), 107–124. https://doi.
ing a wide range of skills for individuals with org/10.1177/001440291007700105
ASD as well as other populations. Self-­ Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide for
management interventions may incorporate sev- parents and professionals. Jessica Kingsley.
eral related interventions (e.g., goal setting, Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall.
Brobst, B., & Ward, P. (2002). Effects of public post-
self-monitoring, self-reinforcement) that may be ing, goal setting, and oral feedback on the skills of
used individually or in conjunction with one female soccer players. Journal of Applied Behavior
another. Though there is not a clear consensus Analysis, 35(3), 247–257. https://doi.org/10.1901/
related to the responsible mechanisms for its suc- jaba.2002.35-­247
Broden, M., Hall, R.  V., & Mitts, B. (1971). The effect
cess, generally self-management can be concep- of self-recording on the classroom behavior of two
tualized as an effective behavior analytic eighth grade students. Journal of Applied Behavior
procedure that relies on an individual making Analysis, 4(3), 191–199. https://doi.org/10.1901/
modifications to the environment to effect change jaba.1971.4-­191
Bryan, L. C., & Gast, D. L. (2000). Teaching on-task and
on their own behavior. As self-management inter- on-schedule behaviors to high-functioning children
ventions are implemented by the individual with autism via picture activity schedules. Journal of
themselves, they do not require ongoing involve- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 553–567.
ment of a clinician. Additionally, in some cases https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005687310346
Carr, M.  E., Moore, D.  W., & Anderson, A. (2014).
materials have been successfully faded, leading Self-management interventions on students with
to greater autonomy and integration into social autism: A meta-analysis of single-subject research.
and community settings. The application of self-­ Exceptional Children, 81(1), 28–44. https://doi.
management systems to a wide variety of behav- org/10.1177/0014402914532235
Catania, A.  C. (1975). The myth of self-reinforcement.
iors and environments makes these systems Behaviorism, 3(2), 192–199.
dynamic and versatile interventions. The impor- Coyle, C., & Cole, P. (2004). A videotaped self-mod-
tance of independence and autonomy in the lives elling and self-monitoring treatment program to
of individuals with ASD makes self-­management decrease off-task behaviour in children with autism.
Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability,
procedures valuable additions to treatment 29(1), 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/0892702041000
options. Individuals who can monitor and change 1662642
their own social, adaptive, academic, and voca- Cooper, J.  O., Heron, T.  E., & Heward, W.  L. (Eds.).
tional behavior can more readily and indepen- (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Merrill
Prentice Hall.
dently navigate their communities.
600 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

Delano, M.  E. (2007). Improving written language per- Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 38(1),
formance of adolescents with Asperger syndrome. 1–16. https://doi.org/10.2511/027494813807046999
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(2), 345– Hughes, C., Hugo, K., & Blatt, J. (1996). Self-instructional
351. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.50-­06 intervention for teaching generalized problem-solving
DiGangi, S.  A., Maag, J.  W., & Rutherford, R.  B., Jr. within a functional task sequence. American Journal
(1991). Self-graphing of on-task behavior: Enhancing on Mental Retardation, 100(6), 565–579.
the reactive effects of self-monitoring on on-task Hughes, C., & Petersen, D.  L. (1989). Utilizing a self-­
behavior and academic performance. Learning instructional training package to increase on-task
Disability Quarterly, 14(3), 221–230. https://doi. behavior and work performance. Education and
org/10.2307/1510851 Training in Mental Retardation, 24, 114–120.
Epstein, R. (1997). Skinner as self-manager. Journal of Kazdin, A.  E. (1974). Reactive self-monitoring: The
Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(3), 545–568. https:// effects of response desirability, goal setting, and feed-
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-­545 back. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Fellner, D. J., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1984). A behavioral 42(5), 704–716. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037050
analysis of goal setting. Journal of Organizational King-Sears, M.  E. (1999). Teacher and researcher co-­
Behavior Management, 6(1), 33–51. https://doi. design self-management content for an inclusive
org/10.1300/J075v06n01_03 setting: Research training, intervention, and gener-
Ganz, J. B., & Sigafoos, J. (2005). Self-monitoring: Are alization effects on student performance. Education
young adults with MR and autism able to utilize cog- & Training in Mental Retardation & Developmental
nitive strategies independently? Education & Training Disabilities, 34(2), 134–156.
in Developmental Disabilities, 40(1), 24–33. Koegel, R.  L., & Frea, W.  D. (1993). Treatment of
Goldiamond, I. (1976). Self-reinforcement. Journal of social behavior in autism through the modification
Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 509–514. of pivotal social skills. Journal of Applied Behavior
Harchik, A. E., Sherman, J. A., & Sheldon, J. B. (1992). Analysis, 26(3), 369–377. https://doi.org/10.1901/
The use of self-management procedures by people jaba.1993.26-­369
with developmental disabilities: A brief review. Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1990). Extended reduc-
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 13(3), 211– tions in stereotypic behavior of students with autism
227. https://doi.org/10.1016/0891-­4222(92)90026-­3 through a self-management treatment package.
Harris, K.  R. (1986). Self-monitoring of attentional Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(1), 119–
behavior versus self-monitoring of productivity: 127. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-­119
Effects on on-task behavior and academic response Koegel, R.  L., Koegel, L.  K., & Carter, C.  M. (1999).
rate among learning disabled children. Journal of Pivotal teaching interactions for children with autism.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 19(4), 417–423. https:// School Psychology Review, 28(4), 576. https://doi.org/
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1986.19-­417 10.1080/02796015.1999.12085986
Hayes, S.  C., Rosenfarb, I., Wulfert, E., Munt, E.  D., Koegel, L.  K., Koegel, R.  L., & Ingham, J.  C. (1986).
Korn, Z., & Zettle, R.  D. (1985). Self-reinforcement Programming rapid generalization of correct articula-
effects: An artifact of social standard setting? Journal tion through self-monitoring procedures. Journal of
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(3), 201–214. https:// Speech & Hearing Disorders, 51(1), 24–32. https://
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-­201 doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5101.24
Holifield, C., Goodman, J., Hazelkorn, M., & Heflin, Koegel, L.  K., Park, M.  N., & Koegel, R.  L. (2014).
L.  J. (2010). Using self-monitoring to increase Using self-management to improve the reciprocal
attending to task and academic accuracy in chil- social conversation of children with autism spec-
dren with autism. Focus on Autism and Other trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Developmental Disabilities, 25(4), 230–238. https:// Disorders, 44(5), 1055–1063. https://doi.org/10.1007/
doi.org/10.1177/1088357610380137 s10803-­013-­1956-­y
Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Koegel, R.  L., Schreibman, L., Loos, L.  M., Dirlich-­
Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Wilhelm, H., Dunlap, G., Robbins, F.  R., & Plienis,
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 212–229. A.  J. (1992). Consistent stress profiles in moth-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-­7610.2004.00215.x ers of children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Hughes, C., Harmer, M.  L., Killian, D.  J., & Niarhos, Developmental Disorders, 22(2), 205–216. https://doi.
F. (1995). The effects of multiple-exemplar self-­ org/10.1007/BF01058151
instructional training on high school students' gener- Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J., Horner, R. H., Levin, J.
alized conversational interactions. Journal of Applied R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., & Shadish, W. R.
Behavior Analysis, 28(2), 201–218. https://doi. (2013). Signlecase intervention research design stan-
org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-­201 dards. Remedial and Special Education, 34(1), 26–38.
Hughes, C., Harvey, M., Cosgriff, J., Reilly, C., https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932512452794
Heilingoetter, J., Brigham, N., Kaplan, L., & Bernstein, Lee, S.  H., Simpson, R.  L., & Shogren, K.  A. (2007).
R. (2013). A peer-delivered social interaction interven- Effects and implications of self-management for stu-
tion for high school students with autism. Research and dents with autism: A meta-analysis. Focus on Autism
30  Building Independence: Self-Management for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder 601

and Other Developmental Disabilities, 22(1), 2–13. Behavioral Disorders, 20(3), 190–196. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1177/10883576070220010101 org/10.1177/019874299502000304
Lloyd, J. W., Bateman, D. F., Landrum, T. J., & Hallahan, Newman, B., Reinecke, D.  R., & Meinberg, D.  L.
D.  P. (1989). Self-recording of attention versus pro- (2000). Self-management of varied responding in
ductivity. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22(3), three students with autism. Behavioral Interventions,
315–323. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1989.22-­315 15(2), 145–151. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-­­
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal 078X(200004/06)15:2<145::AID-­BIN50>3.0.CO;2-­3
setting and task performance. Prentice Hall. Newman, B., Tuntigian, L., Ryan, C.  S., & Reinecke,
Loftin, R. L., Odom, S. L., & Lantz, J. F. (2008). Social D.  R. (1997). Self-management of a DRO proce-
interaction and repetitive motor behaviors. Journal of dure by three students with autism. Behavioral
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(6), 1124– Interventions: Theory & Practice in Residential
1135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­007-­0499-­5 & Community-Based Clinical Programs, 12(3),
Lopez, A. R., & Wiskow, K. M. (2019). Teaching children 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-­­
with autism to initiate social interactions using tex- 078X(199707)12:3<149::AID-­BRT173>3.0.CO;2-­M
tual prompts delivered via Apple Watches®. Behavior Palmen, A., & Didden, R. (2012). Task engagement in
Analysis in Practice, 13(3), 641–647. https://doi. young adults with high-functioning autism spec-
org/10.1007/s40617-­019-­00385-­y trum disorders: Generalization effects of behav-
Lopez, A. R. & Wiskow, K. M. (2020). Teaching children ioral skills training. Research in Autism Spectrum
with autism to initiate social interactions using tex- Disorders, 6(4), 1377–1388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
tual prompts delivered via Apple Watches. Behavior rasd.2012.05.010
Analysis in Practice, 13, 641–647. https://doi. Palmer, D. C. (1991). A behavioral interpretation of mem-
org/10.1007/s40617-019-00385-y. ory. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on
Lovett, D. L., & Haring, K. A. (1989). The effects of self- verbal behavior (pp. 261–279). Context Press.
management training on the daily living of adults with Pierce, K. L. & Schreibman, L. (1994). Teaching daily
mental retardation. Education and Training in Mental living skills to children with autism in unsupervised
Retardation, 24(4), 306–323. settings through pictorial self-management. Journal
Lovitt, T.  C. (1973). Self-management projects with of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(3), 471–481. https://
children with behavioral disabilities. Journal of doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-471
Learning Disabilities, 6(3), 138–150. https://doi. Poppen, R.  L. (2004). Some clinical implications of
org/10.1177/002221947300600303 rule-governed behavior. In S.  C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-­
Mace, F. C., Shapiro, E. S., West, B. I., Campbell, C., & governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies &
Altman, J. (1986). The role of reinforcement in reac- instructional control (pp. 325–357). Context Press.
tive self-monitoring. Applied Research in Mental Pritchard, R.  D., & Curtis, M.  I. (1973). The influ-
Retardation, 7, 315–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/ ence of goal setting and financial incentives on
S0270-­3092(86)80004-­2 task performance. Organizational Behavior &
Mallott, R. W. (2004). The achievement of evasive goals: Human Performance, 10(2), 175–183. https://doi.
Control by rules describing contingencies that are not org/10.1016/0030-­5073(73)90011-­1
direct acting. In S.  C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed Sainato, D.  M., Strain, P.  S., Lefebvre, D., & Rapp, N.
behavior: Cognition, contingencies & instructional (1990). Effects of self-evaluation on the independent
control (pp. 269–322). Context Press. work skills of preschool children with disabilities.
Mancina, C., Tankersley, M., Kamps, D., Kravits, T., Exceptional Children, 56(6), 540–549. https://doi.
& Parrett, J. (2000). Brief reports brief report: org/10.1177/001440299005600605
Reduction of inappropriate vocalizations for a child Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1998). Summarizing
with autism using a self-management treatment single-subject research: Issues and applications.
program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Behavior Modification, 22(3), 221–242. https://doi.
Disorders, 30(6), 599–606. https://doi.org/10.102 org/10.1177/01454455980223001
3/A:1005695512163 Shogren, K. A., Lang, R., Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M. J.,
Meichenbaum, D.  H., & Goodman, J. (1971). Training & O’Reilly, M. (2011). Self-versus teacher manage-
impulsive children to talk to themselves: A means ment of behavior for elementary school students with
of developing self-control. Journal of Abnormal Asperger syndrome: Impact on classroom behavior.
Psychology, 77(2), 115–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13(2),
h0030773 87–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300710384508
Newman, B., Buffington, D. M., Hemmes, N. S., & Rosen, Singh, N.  N., Lancioni, G.  E., Manikam, R., Winton,
D. (1996). Answering objections to self-management A. S., Singh, A. N., Singh, J., & Singh, A. D. (2011).
and related concepts. Behavior and Social Issues, 6(2), A mindfulness-based strategy for self-management
85–95. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v6i2.284 of aggressive behavior in adolescents with autism.
Newman, B., Buffington, D.  M., O’Grady, M.  A., Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1153–
McDonald, M.  E., Poulson, C.  L., & Hemmes, 1158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.012
N.  S. (1995). Self-management of sched- Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. The
ule following in three teenagers with autism. MacMillan Company.
602 K. B. Marshall and J. L. Rohrer

Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. Vintage Books. Sugai, G., & Rowe, P. (1984). The effect of self-recording
Smith, K. A., Shepley, S. B., Alexander, J. L., Davis, A., on out-of-seat behavior of an EMR student. Education
& Ayres, K. M. (2015). Self-instruction using mobile and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 19(1), 23–28.
technology to learn functional skills. Research in Vernon, T. (2017). Pivotal response treatment: Empirically
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 11, 93–100. https://doi. supported strategies to target social competencies and
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.001 motivation in individuals with ASD. In J. B. Leaf &
Soares, D.  A., Vannest, K.  J., & Harrison, J. (2009). J.  L. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of social skills and
Computer aided self-monitoring to increase academic autism spectrum disorder: Autism and child psycho-
production and reduce self-injurious behavior in a pathology series (pp. 187–196). Springer. https://doi.
child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 24(3), org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­62995-­7_12
171–183. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.283 Wilson, B., Beamish, W., Hay, S., & Attwood, T. (2014).
Southall, C. M., & Gast, D. L. (2011). Self-management Prompt dependency beyond childhood: Adults with
procedures: A comparison across the autism spectrum. Asperger’s syndrome and intimate relationships.
Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Journal of Relationships Research, 5, e11. https://doi.
Disabilities, 46(2), 155–171. org/10.1017/jrr.2014.11
Stahmer, A. C., & Schreibman, L. (1992). Teaching chil- Zohn, C.  J., & Bornstein, P.  H. (1980). Self-monitoring
dren with autism appropriate play in unsupervised of work performance with mentally retarded adults:
environments using a self-management treatment Effects upon work productivity, work quality, and on-­
package. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(2), task behavior. Mental Retardation, 18, 19–25.
447–459. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-­447
Evidence-Based Practices: What
Does the Future Hold? 31
Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon , Julia L. Ferguson ,
and Mary Jane Weiss

31.1 Evidence-Based Practices: behavioral intervention for autistics/individuals


What Does the Future Hold? diagnosed with ASD. Most notably with the pub-
lication of Lovaas’ (1987) outcome study. In the
With every passing year, the outcomes for autis- 1990s, growth and improvement of high-quality,
tics/individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum evidence-based interventions continued for autis-
disorder (ASD) have and continue to improve. In tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. This
the 1960s and 1970s, many autistics/individuals included the publication of Catherine Maurice’s
diagnosed with ASD were relocated into institu- (1994) book about her experience with applied
tions, with many facing horrible living condi- behavior analysis (ABA). Maurice told the story
tions. Robert Koegel (2015) described one of his of how intensive behavioral intervention led to
early experiences with these institutions: best outcomes for her two children. This parental
He [Lovaas] took me on a tour of a mental hospital memoir catapulted the level of interest in behav-
where all kids with autism were. All these kids tied ior analytic intervention for individuals with
to their beds in four-point restraints. People [not autism and there was a dramatic increase in the
behavior analysts] giving them electric shocks, number and need for practitioners implementing
painful electric shocks for punishment. If the kids
tried to bite somebody they were pulling the teeth behavioral intervention.
out of their head. The 1990s also involved the development of
certification standards (Johnston & Shook, 1993;
The deinstitutionalization movement continu- Moore & Shook, 2001; Weiss & Shook, 2010).
ing into the 1980s led to many adults no longer Growth in the field continued in the 2000s with
being placed in institutions, but rather group many more practitioners entering the field, more
homes. Simultaneously, the field of behavior universities offering behavior analytic course-
analysis was beginning to identify the benefits of work and degrees, and refinements and improve-
ments in commonly used procedures (Leaf,
J. B. Leaf (*) · J. H. Cihon · J. L. Ferguson Cihon, et  al., 2018a). The 2010s gave birth to
Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, changes in insurance law (thanks in large part to
USA the work of Lorri Unumb and Dan Unumb,
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA, among many others), which resulted in children
USA accessing behavior analytic services that were
e-mail: jbleaf@APFmail.org not formerly available. All of these events, and
M. J. Weiss others, have directly led to the improvement of
School of Education, Endicott College, Beverly, MA, the field of ABA, and, more importantly,
USA

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 603
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8_31
604 J. B. Leaf et al.

improved outcomes for autistics/individuals emphasize the power of science and the scientific
diagnosed with ASD. process.
These outcomes are also directly related to the One possible way to stress the importance of
development and refinement of a plethora of pro- science and EBPs is developing more effective
cedures that are considered evidence-based prac- methods to train consumers and professionals on
tices (EBPs). This handbook provides an the value and importance of science and the sci-
excellent resource regarding EBPs for profes- entific method. This training is essential for
sionals, caregivers, and autistics/individuals behavior analysts whose goal is to work in the
diagnosed with ASD.  The early chapters dis- field of ABA and behaviorally based autism ser-
cussed what it means to be an EBP, the history of vices. Training on the scientific method/process
the development of EBPs, and why EBPs are should include the traditional steps, the rationales
important. Those chapters were followed by a for those steps, and other behaviorally oriented,
series of chapters discussing specific procedures/ pragmatic approaches (e.g., Skinner, 1956).
approaches [e.g., discrete trial teaching (DTT), Furthermore, this training will help practicing
teaching interaction procedure, shaping] and behavior analysts discriminate between method-
comprehensive models of intervention [e.g., ologically sound empirical research and less
Pivotal Response Training (PRT), Early Start methodologically rigorous research published in
Denver Model] that have been clinically imple- predatory journals. While this training will be
mented and experimentally evaluated to improve essential for practicing behavior analysts, com-
desired behavior and decrease undesired behav- ponents may also be helpful for consumers  –
ior for autistics/individuals diagnosed with especially with respect to an understanding of
ASD. Although there have been marked improve- EBPs.
ments of our procedures and outcomes for autis- Training others on and disseminating the
tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD, our work is importance of science and the scientific method/
far from complete. As such, the editors dedicated process will not be an easy task. It will likely
the final chapter of this handbook to discussing require collaboration between many profession-
possible future endeavors related to EBPs and als and professional organizations. We recom-
autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. mend that large professional behavior analytic
organizations [e.g., the Association for Behavior
Analysis International (ABAI), the Council of
31.2 The Importance of Science Autism Service Providers (CASP), the
Association of Professional Behavior Analysts
Science, and the tenants thereof, empirical evi- (APBA), the Behavior Analysis Certification
dence, and objective data seem to be under attack Board (BACB)] help lead these initiatives. These
within society. Credible evidence is being organizations can provide continuing education
crowded out by misinformation shared on social opportunities, allocate resources to research
media and fueled by conspiracy theories. This informing the development of policy statements,
phenomenon is apparent worldwide, with large and help guide third-party payers and govern-
members of the population questioning the utility ment officials in the endorsement of EBPs. Peer-­
of masks for COVID-19 and/or vaccines. The reviewed journals, especially those within the
value of science and the scientific process seems field of behavior analysis, could also help in this
to be more easily questioned or disregarded com- endeavor. For example, journal editors could
pletely. Unfortunately, the field of ABA, espe- dedicate special issues to this topic and ensure
cially in the context of autism services and EBPs, reviewers of submitted manuscripts include an
is not immune to this phenomenon. One of the evaluation of scientific merit in their reviews.
most important goals of the future is for profes- Ultimately, behavior analysts cannot adopt a pas-
sionals to change the narrative within the field, sive role and must be actively involved in amelio-
and for individual practitioners and researchers to rating efforts to devalue science and the scientific
31  Evidence-Based Practices: What Does the Future Hold? 605

method/process. This involves having active dis- and any areas of consumer confusion regarding
cussions on the importance of EBPs, promoting EBPs. In addition, these professional groups
peer-reviewed journals, and validating sources could consider and clarify the use of alternate
and data. terms that have emerged including empirically
supported treatment and evidence-based practice
in psychology.
31.3 EBP Definition

Although EBP generally consists of best research 31.4 Research


evidence, clinical expertise, and consumer input,
there are many different criteria that are used to 31.4.1 Expanding Participant
determine if a procedure is an EBP. Further, there Demographics
have been discussions concerning if EBP should
be used to refer to a list of procedures (e.g., A general theme of this handbook is that there
Smith, 2013) or a well-developed professional has been an abundance of research on the many
decision-making skill set used to determine different behaviorally based procedures used
which procedures to implement for specific cli- throughout the course of intervention for autis-
ents in specific contexts (e.g., Slocum et  al., tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. While this
2014). While this is a necessary discussion, until abundance of research is necessary, impressive,
a conclusion is reached, it is likely to lead to con- and methodologically sound, there are numerous
fusion among practitioners and consumers of areas that are ripe for additional research. First,
behavioral intervention. This confusion could, in more research is needed with a wider demo-
part, lead to professionals and consumers not graphic of participants and the necessary demo-
understanding EBPs and minimizing the impor- graphic variables of these participants need to be
tance of EBPs in the context of science. As such, well documented within this research (Jones
it is imperative that future work continues to et al., 2020). For example, adolescents and adults
address these challenges. diagnosed with ASD are largely underrepre-
One possible fruitful venture would be review- sented in much of the research involving autis-
ing the literature for the conditions under which tics/individuals diagnosed with ASD (Gerhardt &
the term “evidence-based practice” is used. Data Lainer, 2011), and future research should actively
collected could then be examined for any general strive to evaluate the effectiveness of different
themes that could assist in efforts to determine an procedures (e.g., DTT, PRT, script fading) with
agreed upon use and description of EBP.  This adolescents and adults diagnosed with ASD. This
data could also be compared to large-scale efforts research should also include participants who
to determine if an approach or procedure should commonly require more supports (e.g., less
be considered an EBP (e.g., Steinbrenner et al., vocal-verbal language, higher rates of aberrant
2020). At the same time, large professional behavior, lower cognitive skills). Furthermore,
behavior analytic organizations (e.g., ABAI, more analysis on the impact of socioeconomic
APBA, CASP, BACB) could help form advisory status, race and ethnic identity, and other demo-
committees to assist in this work. These advisory graphic variables on responsiveness to interven-
committees could assist in reviewing the histori- tion should be examined. By evaluating the
cal use of the term “EBP” as well as discussing effectiveness of behaviorally based procedures
the development of the desired conditions under with a more diverse population, it will be possi-
which “EBP” should be used (e.g., as a list of ble to determine the conditions under which these
procedures or a process used by practicing behav- procedures are helpful for a wider population of
ior analysts). These efforts could prove to be autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD.
invaluable in clarifying the importance of EBP
606 J. B. Leaf et al.

31.4.2 Group Research Methodology 31.4.3 Comparative Research

Second, additional research that embraces the Do we know that DTT can be used to improve
use of group research methodology, where pos- expressive language? Yes, because both research
sible, is needed. Many of the empirical studies and clinical practice have demonstrated it to be
cited in this handbook made use of single-case effective (e.g., Ferguson et  al., 2020). Do we
research methodology. Single-case research know that incidental teaching can also be used
designs (e.g., reversal, changing criterion, mul- to improve expressive language? Yes, because
tiple baseline) are excellent for clear demonstra- both research and clinical practice have demon-
tions of experimental control (i.e., the strated it to be effective (e.g., McGee & Daly,
independent variable is responsible for the 2016). Do we know that the teaching interaction
changes in the dependent variable). The benefits procedure can be effective in improving social
of the use of single-case research designs, which behavior for autistics/individuals diagnosed
permit using individual participants as their own with ASD? Yes, because both research and clini-
baseline, are well known within the field of cal practice have demonstrated it to be effective
behavior analysis. They allow for an evaluation (e.g., Green et al., 2020). Do we know that video
of the effectiveness and efficiency of a procedure modeling can also be effective in improving
at an individual level. Despite the strengths of social behavior for autistics/individuals diag-
single-subject design methodology, they also nosed with ASD? Yes, because both research
come with some limitations and criticism and clinical practice have demonstrated it to be
(mostly from outside of the field of behavior effective (e.g., Rudy et  al., 2014). But we are
analysis). One issue is the extent to which results less confident in the conditions under which
can be generalized to a larger population. That DTT or incidental teaching, comparatively, will
is, the generality of one study using single-­ be more effective or efficient in teaching lan-
subject design methodology is limited. It should guage skills. We are less confident in the condi-
be noted, however, that a core principle of sin- tions under which the teaching interaction
gle-subject design methodology is replication, procedure or video modeling, comparatively,
which increases the generality of the body of are more effective and efficient in teaching
research. Nevertheless, utilizing group research social skills. More importantly, we know little
design methodology can help address concerns about which procedures are more or less pre-
of generality within behavior analysis and, per- ferred for teaching particular goals, for specific
haps more importantly, outside of behavior anal- clients, and under which contexts. Thus, a third
ysis where group designs are viewed as the gold area that we encourage researchers to engage in
standard (Smith, 2012). These efforts will also is comparative studies to help evaluate the con-
likely be helpful for policymakers who are more ditions under which different procedures and
accustomed to the use of group designs, such as approaches are more effective, efficient, and
randomized control trials, when it comes to mak- preferred (Johnston, 1988). This research will
ing important policy decisions regarding funding help assist practitioners to select and implement
options. In sum, producing group design and the most effective, efficient, and preferred pro-
single-case design research within behavior cedures for their clients. Such research is
analysis may increase the extent to which behav- entirely consistent with the foundational tenets
ior analytic research is understood and respected of the science of ABA; an essential research and
by allied disciplines, and may help to elevate clinical practice question is always to ask under
behavior analysis as a field in research circles. what conditions is the procedure best applied.
31  Evidence-Based Practices: What Does the Future Hold? 607

31.4.4 Long-Term Outcomes 2019). Some of these claims have resulted in


responses from the behavior analytic community
Finally, research is desperately needed evaluating to specific claims (e.g., Gorycki et  al., 2020;
the possible long-term outcomes of the use, or Leaf, Ross, et al., 2018b) as well as more general
nonuse, of EBPs. There are several variables that discussions (e.g., Leaf, Cihon, et  al., 2021a;
should be assessed within this area of research Rajaraman et al., 2021). Any claims that proce-
including objective and subjective measures. dures based upon the principles of ABA, or in
More objective measures should include evalua- some cases ABA more generally, have resulted in
tions of maintenance and generalization of skills serious negative or unwanted side effects need to
developed using EBPs. It may be the case that be taken seriously and approached compassion-
some EBPs, in some contexts, are more likely to ately. Furthermore, behavior analytic researchers
lead to better maintenance and generalization. are uniquely able to conduct research to evaluate
Other more objective measures should also any long-term, and short-term, negative or
include an evaluation of collateral behaviors or unwanted effects associated with ABA-based
outcomes. This would include evaluating if the procedures and interventions (i.e., EBPs). The
development of desired behaviors led to the use of single-subject research methodology and
development of more desired, but untargeted, the use of measures of social validity will be
behaviors as well as other possible outcomes invaluable in this line of research. Data resulting
(i.e., generalized behavior change). For instance, from this research can be used to determine the
does the teaching how to initiate and maintain a necessity for any global and contextually specific
conversation lead to the development of mean- changes to ABA-based procedures and interven-
ingful and desired friendships? Subjective mea- tions. Any data collected from this research are
sures must include an evaluation of the also more likely to be more welcomed and con-
acceptability of the goals, procedure, and out- sumed by behavior analysts than anecdotal
comes of those procedures – collectedly known reports that may be found on social media outlets
as social validity (Wolf, 1978). There is a general and blogs. This may accelerate the rate of change
dearth of the assessment of social validity within in adopting or adapting procedures to ensure that
behavior analytic research (Ferguson et  al., service delivery is more compassionate, humane,
2019), and when it is included, the assessment is and tailored to the individual’s preferences and
commonly related to short-term outcomes and the community’s needs.
results. The importance of including the assess-
ment of social validity within behavior analytic
research cannot be understated. This is also the 31.5 Training
case with respect to the assessment of social
validity within behavior analytic research related Over the past 40 years, there has been a drastic
to long-term outcomes. That is, do ratings of the and continual increase in the number of certified
acceptability of the goals, procedures, and out- behavior analysts in the world. To illustrate, when
comes of those procedures change across time? the BACB was first established in 1999, there
This research has always been meant to be at the were only 30 Board-Certified Behavior Analysts
heart of behavior analysis, and future research (BCBAs) and Board-Certified Assistant Behavior
should be no different. Analysts (BCaBAs) in the world  – today that
Perhaps even more importantly, future number is over 55,000 (Behavior Analyst
research must evaluate any instances of negative Certification Board., n.d.). This growth is mir-
or unwanted side effects of any EBPs. There have rored with the Registered Behavior Technician
been claims that procedures based upon the prin- (RBT) credential, with now more than 100,000
ciples of ABA have resulted in serious negative RBTs worldwide since its adoption in 2014
or unwanted side effects (e.g., trauma, anxiety; (Behavior Analyst Certification Board., n.d.).
Kupferstein, 2018; Sandoval-Norton & Shkedy, This does not take into account the number of
608 J. B. Leaf et al.

behavior analysts who are not certified, autism through the development of analytic skills often
processionals who are not behavior analysts, or required by interventionists providing a progres-
the number of teachers who teach autistics/indi- sive approach to ABA-based intervention (Leaf
viduals diagnosed with ASD.  Put simply, there et al., 2016).
are countless numbers of professionals providing
services for autistics/individuals diagnosed with
ASD. 31.6 Increasing and Improving
It is critical that all professionals providing Standards
behavior analytic services for autistics/individu-
als diagnosed with ASD are well trained to help Current ethical standards for certified behavior
ensure the implementation of quality interven- analysts include requirements to implement
tions with the highest degree of fidelity. There EBPs that are consistent with the principles of
have been numerous studies that have demon- behavior analysis (Behavior Analyst Certification
strated how to effectively train professionals on Board, 2020). BACB-certified individuals pro-
implementing the procedures highlighted in this viding nonbehavioral services “must be clearly
handbook (e.g., Cheung et  al., 2020; Green distinguished from their behavioral services and
et al., 2020; Kirkpatrick et al., 2021; Weinkauf BACB certification” (Behavior Analyst
et al., 2011). There have also been several dis- Certification Board, 2020, p. 16) by providing a
cussions within the literature about the impor- disclaimer stating as such. While the purpose of
tance of training and improving standards in this disclaimer, as well as the rationale for it, is
training for certified behavior analysts (e.g., logical on the surface, the possible ramifications
Leaf, Leaf, et al., 2021b; Leaf et al., 2017). With are concerning. For example, is it reasonable to
the increasing need for behavior analysts in the believe that an interventionist is providing qual-
field of autism, effective and efficient training is ity, effective intervention if they use a functional
needed now more than ever. As such, research- communication training approach in one context,
ers and clinicians should continue to develop but implement facilitated communication while
and evaluate comprehensive, effective, and effi- providing a disclaimer in another? We believe the
cient training methods to help ensure the imple- answer is no; however, this remains an empirical
mentation of quality interventions with the question. Future research should examine the
highest degree of fidelity. Some examples can association between the level of analysis in the
be found within the literature (e.g., Cheung repertoires of certified behavior analysts and
et  al., 2020; Weinkauf et  al., 2011), but much technicians and the quality and effectiveness of
more research is needed to help improve stan- their provided interventions. As such, we suggest
dards in intervention as well as training for cer- that certified behavior analysts should not pro-
tified behavior analysts. vide and/or advertise dangerous or ineffective
Comprehensive, effective, and efficient train- nonbehavioral services under any circumstances.
ing methods may not just increase the quality and Practitioners should be encouraged to stay abreast
fidelity of behavioral intervention, but also of emerging research evidence, organization
increase the likelihood of the implementation of position statements, and resources about how
EBPs and decrease the likelihood of non-EBPs. interventions are categorized along the EBP con-
As previously mentioned, if behavior analysts are tinuum. These are moving targets, and adherence
trained in the value and importance of science to this goal requires continued vigilance and
and the scientific process, they may be less sus- awareness of the available evidence. As data are
ceptible to suggestions or temptations to imple- collected from research evaluating the impact of
ment interventions that have little scientific merit certified behavior analysts providing and/or
or support. It may also be the case that this train- advertising nonbehavioral services, these expec-
ing would lead to more effective interventionists tations can be further refined.
31  Evidence-Based Practices: What Does the Future Hold? 609

31.7 Conclusion dyad arrangement via telehealth. Journal of Applied


Behavior Analysis, 53(4), 1876–1888. https://doi.
org/10.1002/jaba.773
The editors of this handbook believe that EBPs, Gerhardt, P.  F., & Lainer, I. (2011). Addressing the
science, and the scientific method are essential to needs of adolescents and adults with autism: A
ensure autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD crisis on the horizon. Journal of Contemporary
Psychotherapy, 41(1), 37–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/
access high-quality, effective, and behaviorally s10879-­010-­9160-­2
based interventions. This handbook provides Gorycki, K. A., Ruppel, P. R., Zane, T., & Navalta, C. P.
ample evidence of the number of procedures and (2020). Is long-term ABA therapy abusive: A response
interventions that have been documented within to Sandoval-Norton and Shkedy. Cogent Psychology,
7(1), 1823615. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.202
the peer-reviewed literature to improve desired 0.1823615
behaviors and decrease undesired behaviors for Green, D.  R., Ferguson, J.  L., Cihon, J.  H., Torres, N.,
autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD. These Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Rudrud, E., Schulze, K.,
procedures should be considered as part of com- & Leaf, J.  B. (2020). The teaching interaction pro-
cedure as a staff training tool. Behavior Analysis in
prehensive programing and should be adjusted to Practice, 13(2), 421–433. https://doi.org/10.1007/
meet the individual needs of any specific learner. s40617-­019-­00357-­2
Despite the numerous procedures and plethora of Johnston, J.  M. (1988). Strategic and tactical limits of
research on these procedures, our work is far comparison studies. The Behavior Analyst Today,
11(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392448
from done. ABA-based interventions and proce- Johnston, J.  M., & Shook, G.  L. (1993). Model for the
dures are based on sound science and constantly statewide delivery of programming services. Mental
evolve. The recommendations provided here Retardation, 31(3), 127–139.
should not be viewed as exhaustive or as a finite Jones, S.  H., Peter, C.  C. S., & Ruckle, M.  M. (2020).
Reporting of demographic variables in the jour-
to-do list for researchers and practitioners. nal of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied
Rather, we offer this as a starting point and hope Behavior Analysis, 53(3), 1304–1315. https://doi.
it functions as a motivational tool for additional org/10.1002/jaba.722
work to keep our science and the application of Kirkpatrick, M., Rehfeld, D. M., Akers, J. S., Rivera, G.,
& Sulak, T. N. (2021). Using behavioral skills training
that science progressing. with preservice teachers in the university classroom.
Behavioral Interventions, 36(1), 145–158. https://doi.
org/10.1002/bin.1764
References Koegel, R.  K. (2015, January, 23-25). Autism, ABA,
and pivotal response treatment [conference session].
Ninth Annual Association for Behavior Analysis
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (n.d.). BACB cer-
International Autism Conference, Las Vegas, NV,
tificant data. Retrieved from https://www.bacb.com/
United States. https://www.abainternational.org/
BACB-­certificant-­data
events/program-­details/event-­detail.aspx?sid=41743
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2020). Ethics code
&by=ByArea#s6_0
for behavior analysts. Author.
Kupferstein, H. (2018). Evidence of increased PTSD
Cheung, Y., Man Kit Cheung, A., Ho Yan Luk, E., Man
symptoms in autistics exposed to applied behavior
Fung, Y., Mountjoy, T., Cihon, J.  H., & Leaf, J.  B.
analysis. Advances in Autism, 4(1), 19–29. https://doi.
(2020). An evaluation of a comprehensive train-
org/10.1108/aia-­08-­2017-­0016
ing package for interventionists providing behav-
Leaf, J.  B., Cihon, J.  H., Ferguson, J.  L., & Weinkauf,
ioral intervention for children with autism spectrum
S.  M. (2018a). An introduction to applied behav-
disorder. International Journal of Developmental
ior analysis. In J.  Matson (Ed.), Handbook of
Disabilities, 66(5), 358–369. https://doi.org/10.1080/
childhood psychopathology and developmental dis-
20473869.2020.1827208
abilities treatment (pp.  25–42). Springer. https://doi.
Ferguson, J. L., Cihon, J. H., Leaf, J. B., Van Meter, S. M.,
org/10.1007/978-­3-­319-­71210-­9_3
McEachin, J., & Leaf, R. (2019). Assessment of social
Leaf, J. B., Cihon, J. H., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Liu, N.,
validity trends in the journal of applied behavior anal-
Russell, N., Unumb, L., Shapiro, S., & Khosrowshahi,
ysis. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 20(1),
D. (2021a). Concerns about ABA-based intervention:
146–157. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2018.15
An evaluation and recommendations. Journal of Autism
34771
and Developmental Disorders. Advance online publi-
Ferguson, J.  L., Majeski, M.  J., McEachin, J., Leaf, R.,
cation. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­021-­05137-­y
Cihon, J. H., & Leaf, J. B. (2020). Evaluating discrete
Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Bondy, A., Cihon,
trial teaching with instructive feedback delivered in a
J. H., Detrich, R., Eshleman, J., Ferguson, J. L., Foxx,
610 J. B. Leaf et al.

R.  M., Freeman, B.  J., Gerhardt, P., Glenn, S.  S., teaching bids for joint attention to children with
Miller, M., Milne, C.  M., Mountjoy, T., Parker, T., autism. Behavioral Interventions, 29(4), 269–285.
Pritchard, J., Ross, R. K., Saunders, M. S., & Streff, https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1398
T. (2021b). The importance of professional discourse Sandoval-Norton, A. H., & Shkedy, G. (2019). How much
for the continual advancement of practice standards: compliance is too much compliance: Is long-term
The RBT® as a case in point. Journal of Autism and ABA therapy abuse? Cogent Psychology, 6(1). https://
Developmental Disorders, 51(5), 1789–1801. https:// doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2019.1641258
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-­020-­04631-­z Skinner, B. F. (1956). A case history in scientific method.
Leaf, J.  B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M., American Psychologist, 11(5), 221–233. https://doi.
Ala'i-Rosales, S., Ross, R.  K., Smith, T., & Weiss, org/10.1037/h0047662
M.  J. (2016). Applied behavior analysis is a science Slocum, T.  A., Detrich, R., Wilczynski, S.  M., Spencer,
and, therefore, progressive. Journal of Autism and T.  D., Lewis, T., & Wolfe, K. (2014). The evidence-­
Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 720–731. https://doi. based practice of applied behavior analysis.
org/10.1007/s10803-­015-­2591-­6 The Behavior Analyst, 37(1), 41–56. https://doi.
Leaf, J. B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M., Smith, org/10.1007/s40614-­014-­0005-­2
T., Harris, S. L., Freeman, B. J., Mountjoy, T., Parker, Smith, T. (2012). Evolution of research on interven-
T., Streff, T., Volkmar, F.  R., & Waks, A. (2017). tions for individuals with autism spectrum disorder:
Concerns about the registered behavior technician Implications for behavior analysts. The Behavior
in relation to effective autism intervention. Behavior Analyst, 35(1), 101–113. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Analysis in Practice, 10(2), 154–163. https://doi. BF03392269
org/10.1007/s40617-­016-­0145-­9 Smith, T. (2013). What is evidence-based behavior analy-
Leaf, J.  B., Ross, R.  K., Cihon, J.  H., & Weiss, M.  J. sis? The Behavior Analyst Today, 36(1), 7–33. https://
(2018b). Evaluating Kupferstein’s claims of the doi.org/10.1007/BF03392290
relationship of behavioral intervention to PTSS for Steinbrenner, J.  R., Hume, K., Odom, S.  L., Morin,
individuals with autism. Advances in Autism, 4(3), K.  L., Nowell, S.  W., Tomaszewski, B., Szendrey,
122–129. https://doi.org/10.1108/aia-­02-­2018-­0007 S., McIntyre, N.  S., Yücesoy-Özkan, S., & Savage,
Lovaas, O.  I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and nor- M.  N. (2020). Evidence-based practices for chil-
mal educational and intellectual functioning in dren, youth, and young adults with autism. In
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. https://doi. National Clearinghouse on Autism Evidence and
org/10.1037//0022-­006x.55.1.3 Practice Review Team.
Maurice, C. (1994). Let me hear your voice: A family’s Weinkauf, S. M., Zeug, N. M., Anderson, C. T., & Ala’i-­
triumph over autism. Ballantine Books. Rosales, S. (2011). Evaluating the effectiveness of a
McGee, G.  G., & Daly, T. (2016). Incidental teaching comprehensive staff training package for behavioral
of age-appropriate social phrases to children with interventions for children with autism. Research in
autism. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(2), 864–871. https://
Disabilities, 32(2), 112–123. https://doi.org/10.2511/ doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.10.001
rpsd.32.2.112 Weiss, M. J., & Shook, G. L. (2010). Resources on train-
Moore, J., & Shook, G. L. (2001). Certification, accredi- ing requirements for applied behavior analysts: The
tation, and quality control in behavior analysis. behavior analyst certification board and the autism
The Behavior Analyst, 24(1), 45–55. https://doi. special interest group consumer guidelines. European
org/10.1007/BF03392018 Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11, 217–222. https://
Rajaraman, A., Austin, J.  L., Gover, H.  C., Cammilleri, doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2010.11434345
A. P., Donnelly, D. R., & Hanley, G. P. (2021). Toward Wolf, M.  M. (1978). Social validity: The case for sub-
trauma-informed applications of behavior analysis. jective measurement or how applied behavior analy-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. Advance online sis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior
publication. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.881 Analysis, 11(2), 203–214. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Rudy, N.  A., Betz, A.  M., Malone, E., Henry, J.  E., & jaba.1978.11-­203
Chong, I.  M. (2014). Effects of video modeling on
Index

A Activity Schedules for Children with Autism: Teaching


ABA-based intervention, 29, 99, 180 Independent Behavior (Book), 516
inadequacies, 107 Adaptive functioning, 331
treatment plans, 112 Adaptive skills, 106
ABA-based services, 108 Adolescents conversational skills, 255
ABA-based therapy, 125, 127 Adult–child relationships, 319
ABA ethics hotline, 76 Adult-delivered social consequences, 527
ABA implementation, 104 Adult management, 517
ABA interventions, 105 Adult supervision, 533
ABA professionals learning, 128 Affect-ladened interactions, 29
ABA program, 39 African-Americans, 81
Abative effects, 140 Aggression/self-injurious behaviors, 125
Abolishing operation (AO), 140, 528 Aided symbols, 379
Academic skills, 100, 591, 592 AIT literature, 34
Acceptable quality, 14 Alternative communication (AAC), 103
Active listening, 136 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Activities of daily living (ADL), 90 32
Activity engagement, 151 American Academy of Pediatrics, 19, 142
Activity schedules, 5 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental
acquisition and generalization, 513 Disabilities (AAIDD), 491
advantages, 533 American Occupational Therapy Association (AOTA), 12
challenging behavior, 514 American Psychological Association (APA), 18, 28, 498
children with ASD, 514 American Sign Language (ASL), 378
definition, 513 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
dependent variables, 514 (ASHA), 12, 19, 32, 142
discriminative stimuli, 523 Anecdotal observations, 531
effects, 513 Animal-assisted intervention (AAI)
format, 514 animal-assisted therapy, 32
implementation, 516 definition, 32
instructor as prompt, 517–518 experimental studies, 33
literature, 520 methodological flaws, 32
presentation modes, 514–515 research literature, 32
primary function, 513 variation in practice, 32
problem behavior, 514 Anna State Hospital, 499
prompt-fading strategies, 516, 517 Antecedent-based intervention, 139
prompt following errors, 518–519 Antecedent-behavior-consequence (ABC), 210
prompt selection, 516, 517 Antecedent variables influencing fad treatments, 108
reinforcement, 519–520 cure promises, 106, 107
review, 513 lack of access to services based on evidence, 107, 108
sustained engagement, 513 lack of knowledge about etiology, 106
teaching individuals, 533 lack of knowledge about quality of evidence, 107
technological formats, 514 media, 105–106
VAS, 513 Anthropology, 157

© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature 611
Switzerland AG 2022
J. B. Leaf et al. (eds.), Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions for Autism, Autism
and Child Psychopathology Series, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96478-8
612 Index

Antiscience, 106 facial expressions, 377


Anxiety, 126 facilitators, 389
Applied behavior analysis (ABA), 16, 28, 75, 125, 153, gestures, 377
252, 342, 375, 403, 480, 603, 604, 606–609 instruction, 392
behavior analysts, 18 mediators, 387
clinical expertise and judgment, 17 moderators, 387
components, 18 MS, 378
decision-making framework, 18 participation model, 385–387
decision-making process, 17 physical impairments, 376
evidence-based practice, 17 predictors, 387
interventions, 99 preferences, 388, 389
practices, 81 primary elements, 376
procedures and packages, 18 response efficiency, 387, 388
scientific disciplines, 100 response effort, 388
technological dimension, 17 SLP, 391
Applied Behavior Analysis Treatment of Autism system selection, 384, 385
Spectrum Disorder, 76 team, 394
Applied behavior analyst, 49, 51–55, 57, 59, 60, 65, 66, technologies and methodologies, 389
523 types of symbols, 377
Applied behavior intervention Pivotal Response Training, Autism, 1, 72, 144, 344, 346–348, 465
318 hereditary patterns, 106
Appropriate conversation, 258 individual variability, 325
Appropriate initiations, 532 intervention researchers, 301
Appropriately ambitious, 85 Autism experience challenges, 100
ASD community, 81 Autism intervention, 12
ASD diagnosis, 106–107 Autism Observation Scale for Infants (AOSI), 213
ASD intervention Autism prevalence, 103
definition and concept of collaboration, 126–127 Autism Program Environment Assessment Rating Scale,
ASD parent training, 289 75
ASD symptom manifestation, 99 Autism Speaks, 74
ASD symptoms, 37 Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), 49, 73, 74, 125, 207,
Asperger’s syndrome (AS), 589 375, 406, 417, 452, 453, 458, 465, 481, 482,
Association for Behavior Analysis International (ABAI), 514, 544, 557, 559, 603
109, 480, 491, 500 autistics/individuals, 2
Association for Science in Autism Treatments (ASAT), behavior analysts, 2
40, 100, 142, 392 BST (see Behavioral skills training (BST))
Astronomical promises of effectiveness, 2 children with, 125
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 126, communication challenges, 375
453 diagnostic characteristics, 251
Audio integration treatments (AIT), 34 diagnostic criterion, 1
Audiology, 142 DTT (see Discrete trial teaching (DTT))
Audio-recording, 524 evidence-based treatment, 260
Auditory integration training, 104 high-quality IEP, 96
Auditory scripts, 523, 524, 527 individual’s requirement, 289
Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC), 5, individuals with, 126
89, 142, 376 intervention programs, 289
ASD, 393, 394 interventions, 1, 2
assessment considerations, 390 life-long developmental disorder, 289
barriers, 389 lifelong neurodevelopmental disorder, 24
behavior analysis, 390, 394 mechanisms, 24
body language, 377 NCR implementation, 481
candidacy model, 385 non-evidence-based treatment, 260
children, 394 prevalence, 260
communication needs model, 385 qualitative impairments, 1
communication skills, 376 response cost and time-out from reinforcement, 479
components, 393 restrictive and repetitive behavior, 251
cultural validity, 390 social communication impairments, 99
disadvantages, 377 speak fluently, 125
effectiveness and efficacy, 392, 393 spectrum disorders, 142
evidence-based, 392 symptoms, 375
Index 613

teaching skills, 251 feedback, 257


TIP (see Teaching interaction procedure (TIP)) instruction, 256
unsupported skill acquisition, 251 model, 256, 257
US, 99 rehearsal, 257
Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ), 220 considerations, 259–260
Autism symptoms, 318 evaluation, 261
Autism treatment guide, 105 evidence-based interventions, 252, 259, 260
Autistic behavior, 197 flexible teaching, 262
Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact (article), 1 future research, 262
Autistic/individuals, 2 instructor, 256
Automatically maintained bruxism, 482 in-the-moment teaching, 261
Automatic reinforcement, 460 learner, 256
Aversive control, 158 measure learner competency, 252
rationales, 261
skills, 260
B social skills, 262
BACB training and certification, 117 social validity measures, 261
BACB® task list (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, teaching skills, 252, 256
2017), 129 teaching social skills, 262
Behavior, 25 three-term contingency, role-play scenario, 256
Behavior analysis, 12, 108, 109, 113, 126 usual sequence, 257, 258
clinician’s training, 445 Behavioral strategies, 289
collaborative skills, 133 Behavior-analytic instruction, 533
effectiveness, 126 Behavior-analytic journals, 109
fundamental assumptions, 127, 128 Behavior-analytic services, 104
individual with ASD, 125 Behaviorism, 28
lack of information about other disciplines, 127 Behaviorism’s philosophical tenets, 101
one’s own discipline, 128 Bilingual learners
skills and knowledge, 126 picture exchange communication system, 368
technical terminology, 130 Biological pseudoscience, 33
training, 129, 130 Blood-brain barrier, 36
Behavior Analyst Certification Board (BACB), 110, 134, Board Certified Behavior Analysts (BCBAs), 76, 77, 129,
155, 500 607
Behavior analytic evidence-based practice, 163 decisions, 103
Behavior analytic services, 533 online questionnaire, 103
Behavior data collection systems, 503 participants, 104
Behavior intervention plans (BIP), 76, 92 scientifically validated and unvalidated, 103
Behavior management system Board-Certified Assistant Behavior Analysts (BCaBAs),
group contingency, 282 607
individual, 282 Bonding, 28
Behavior plan, 324 Book-based activity schedules, 515, 521
Behavior skills training (BST), 51, 52, 135, 278 Book-formatted activity schedules, 515
Behavioral change, 36 Boundary conditions, 23
Behavioral consultation, 126 Brodhead’s model, 138–140
Behavioral functioning, 301 Bug-in-ear (BIE) device, 191
Behavioral intervention program, 366 Building skills, 128
Behavioral Intervention Rating Scale (BIRS), 492
Behavioral interventions, 14, 34, 484, 489, 491, 492,
603, 605, 608 C
acceptability, 491 Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies, 40
Behavioral mechanisms, 114 Candidacy model, 385
Behavioral practitioner, 112 Care-based interventions, 152
Behavioral programming, 101 Caregiver implementation, 241
Behavioral rehearsal, 526 Caregiver knowledge, 96
Behavioral skills training (BST), 5, 93, 365 Caregivers, 409
ASD, 257–259 CASP ASD Guidelines, 76
benefits, 261 Causal models, ethical decision-making and EBPs
clinical practice, 262 behavior analysis, 49
component analysis, 261 categorization, 49
components, 256 decision-making
614 Index

Causal models, ethical decision-making and EBPs (cont.) Clinical practice experiences, social skills groups
choice, 55 behavior management strategies
definition, 55 group contingency system, 282
ethical behavior, 56 individual behavior, 282
verbal behavior, 56 type, 282
descriptive vs. normative ethical behavior curriculum, 280
ABA, 53 data collection, 284
behavior analytic writings, 53 group development
BST, 51, 52 group objectives, 278
consequentialism, 54 intensity, 284
contract theory, 54 participants
and decision-making with EBPs, 52–55 age, 279
deontology, 54 categories of, 279
ethical theories, 54 implementation, 279
ethics and morality, 55 language skills, 279, 280
feminist approaches, 54 problem behaviors, 280
justice, principle of, 54 social skill deficits, 280
RBTs, 51, 52 schedule, 281, 282
verbal behavior, 53 staff, 280, 281
virtue theory, 54 Clinic-based individual parent coaching sessions, 294
ethics, 49–51 Clinician unblinded report, 35
experimental analysis of behavior, 49 Closed-ended assessments, 444
four-term contingency, 49 Coaching, 93
independent vs. dependent variables, 49 Cognitive and verbal abilities, 31
morality, 49–51 Cognitive behavioral modification, 104
phenomenon of interest, 47 Cognitive skills, 26
research outcome, 48 Collaborating professionals, 138
scientific research goals, 48 Collaboration
three-term contingency, 49 interdisciplinary (see Interdisciplinary collaboration)
Cell phone development, 157 transdisciplinary, 127
Center for Disease Control, 40 Collaborative opportunities, 130
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), 99 Collaborative work, 135
Cerebral palsy (CP), 376 Commodification, 158
Certification standards, 603 Common childhood vaccines, 38
Certified behavior analysts, 608 Communication, 151, 160
Challenging behavior, 594, 595 Communication needs model, 385
Challenging/problematic behaviors, 297 Communication purposes, 23
Chelation therapy, 20 Communication skill development, 133
Chelation treatment, 38 Community of practice, 160, 161
Chemical substances, 38 Community-based instruction (CBI), 234
Child functioning, 306 Comorbid diagnoses, 126
Child initiative and learning Comorbidity, 126
interactive social framework, 319 Compassion, 152
joint activity framework, 319 Competency-based assessment, 135
novel acts, 319 Competency-based training, 135
novel learning opportunities, 319 Competing ideologies/philosophies of behavior, 2
social interest and social initiative, 319 Complex communication needs (CCNs), 376
social learning opportunities, 319 Compliance training, 299
Children and adults with developmental Comprehensive education, 109
disabilities, 514 Comprehensive programming, 444
Children with learning disabilities, 74 Computer-based activity schedule, 515
Children’s communication, 333 Conceptually systematic non-evidence-based
Choice research, 55 interventions, 115
Classrooms, 95 Concurrent-chains preference assessment, 515
Client safety, 138, 141 Conflict resolution strategies, 134
Client’s physical/psychological well-being, 139 Conflicts of conscience, 51
Clinical assessment, 443 Confrontational naming, 31
Clinical competence, 110 Confucian-heritage cultures (CHCs), 158
Clinical decision-making process, 305, 306 Consequent variables influencing fad treatments
Clinical ethical decision-making, 59, 60 NEBP reinforcement, 111, 112
Clinical judgment, 12 response effort, 111
Index 615

Consequentialism, 54 cultural risk and protective factors, 153


Constant time delay, 176, 177 cultural tensions, 152
Constructivist approach, 331 EBP (see Evidence-based practice (EBP))
Consulting resources, 141–144 element of assessments, 152
Contacted non-technical instructions, 131 histories, 151
Content knowledge, 127 professionals interact, 152
Content selection, script tensions, avoidance/exclusion, 151
child approach, 525 values, 151
social interaction reinforcement, 525 Cultural tensions, 152
socially acceptability, 525 Cultural values, 152
teaching students to write script, 526 Culture interacts, 157
Contingency plans, 115 Curriculum, 95, 280
Contingent observation, 486
Continuous duration recording (CDR), 558
Continuous reinforcement (CRF) schedule, 466 D
Continuous video modeling (CVM), 243 Dangerous stimulus, 259
Contract theory, 54 Data-based decision, 89, 143
Contradictory treatments, 138 activity matrix, 92
Controversies, 158 continual progress monitoring, 91
Conventional behaviors, 323 critical components, 91
Conversation partners data collection strategies, 92
adult responses, 529 educators, 91
appropriate models, 528 frequency, 91
conversational recipient, 528 information, 92
interactions, 528 responsibility, 91
learning history, 529 service providers, 91
role, 529 team members, 91
script-fading procedures, 529 Decision-making, 100
social initiations, 529 behaviors, 55
vocal models influence, 529 causal and decision models
vocal repertoire, 529 components, 48
Conversation skills, 252, 255, 258 purpose, 48
Cool versus Not Cool™ (CNC), 278 choice, 55
Cooperation skills, 453 definition, 55, 60
Cost condition, 482 ethical behavior, causal models, 56
Council for Children with Behavioral Disorders (CCBD), ethical decision-making and EBPs, 55
484, 485, 491 verbal behavior, 56
Council of Autism Service Providers (CASP), 76 Decision-making models, 429
Counterbalancing, 173, 174 Brodhead’s model, 138–140
COVID-19, 604 Newhouse-Oisten’s model, 140–141
COVID-19-related school shutdowns, 88 Decision-making repertoires, 110
Cranial osteopathy, 35 Decision models, ethical decision-making and EBPs
Crisis management, 487 applied behavior analysts, 56
Critical communication skills, 366 behaviors chain, 56
Critical race theory, 159 choice, 48
Cross-discipline collaboration in medicine, 133 clinical and educational settings, 60
Cultural awareness, 160 decision-maker’s learning history, 48
Cultural congruence, 158 ethical decision models function, 57
Cultural diversity, 157 ethical decision-making models, 57, 58
Cultural groups, 151, 161 ethical-EBP decision model, 59, 60
Cultural humility evidence-based practice decision models, 58, 59
in behavioral practice, 157 pre-defined goal, 56
Cultural identity, 151 suboptimal decision, 48
Cultural positionality, 155 Decreased perceptions, 131
Cultural responsiveness De-emphasizing generalization, 133
and clinical wisdom, 156 Default placement, 95
Cultural responsivity Deficit in attachment, 27
behavior patterns, 151 Degree of scientific integrity of research, 109
concept, 152 Dehumanization, 491
cultural groups, 151 Deinstitutionalization movement, 30, 603
616 Index

Delivery of rewards, 520 Digestive enzyme therapy, 36


Deontology, 54 Digital activity schedules, 515
Dependency, 519 Direct Training Module, 342
Dependent contingency, 282 Disabilities, 30, 159
Dependent variable, 13 Disability categories, 84
Depression, 126 Disciplines approach
Descriptive ethical behavior, 56, 65 fundamental assumptions, 127, 128
Descriptive ethical claims, 60 lack of information, 127
Descriptive vs. normative ethical behavior one’s own discipline, 128
ABA, 53 Discipline-specific clinical perspective, 137
behavior analytic writings, 53 Discrete trial instruction (DTI), 103
BST, 51, 52 Discrete trial teaching (DTT), 4, 278, 330, 391, 408,
consequentialism, 54 604–606
contract theory, 54 behavior analytic techniques, 171
and decision-making with EBPs, 52–55, 59 components, 181
deontology, 54 data collection, 179, 180
ethical decision-making and EBPs, 55 evidence-based practice, 172, 180–181
ethical theories, 54 future research, 181
ethics and morality, 55 implementation, 171
feminist approaches, 54 instructions
justice, principle of, 54 complexity, 174–175
RBTs, 51, 52 variety of topography, 175
verbal behavior, 53 instructive feedback, 179
virtue theory, 54 misconceptions, 180
Developing skills, 128 pre-trial arrangement
Developmental and behavioral principles, 317 counterbalancing, 173–174
Developmental disabilities (DDs), 30, 99, 376 environment, 172
Developmental Individualized Relationships-based field size, 174
(DIR), 30 target selection, 172–173
Developmental psychology, 323, 331 primary components, discrete tria1, 171
Developmental quotient (DQ), 327 progressive approach, 171, 172
Developmental sciences of social communication, 320 prompting (see Prompting)
Developmental social-pragmatic (DSP), 28, 29 reinforcing consequence, 177–179
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, skills, 171
5th Edition (DSM-V), 251 Discrete trial teaching (DTT) vs. natural environment
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, fifth edition (DSM-5), training (NET)
33 applied behavior analyst, 64, 65
Diagnostic criteria, 99 assessment results, 63
Diaphragmatic breathing response, 407 EBP, 62, 64, 65
Didactic training procedures, 135 empirical literature, 63
Diet modifications, 106 environmental conditions, 65
Differential reinforcement ethical decision-making, EBPs, 64
alternative behavior, 545 instructional trials, 63
definition, 544, 545 intervention design, 63
DRA, 550, 551 learning trials, 63
DRI, 545, 549 literature review, 64
DRO, 545, 546 long-term and short-term benefits, 64
functional variations RBTs, 64
automatic reinforcement, 548, 549 VB-MAPP, 63, 64
definition, 547 word retrieval strategies, 63
negative reinforcement, 548 Discrete trial training (DTT), 188, 391
positive reinforcement, 548 Discriminative stimulus (SD), 516, 518–520,
learning, 550 528
limitations, 549 Disruption in rhythm, 35
low rate responding, 546, 547 Disruptive behaviors, 296–297
Differential reinforcement and extinction, 5 Distraction-free environment, 172
Differential reinforcement for incompatible behavior Double-blinded research studies, 36
(DRI), 545, 555 DSP-based interventions, 29, 30
Differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO), 545, DSP interventions, 29
555 Dyspraxia, 453
Index 617

E Emotional and academic learning, 95


Early communication, 29 Empathetic statements, 523
Early developmental psychologists, 331 Empathic responses, 130
Early extreme beliefs, 30 Empathy, 129, 130
Early intensive behavior intervention (EIBI), 329, 375, Empathy Quotient (EQ) score, 220
453, 497 Empirical evidence, 102
Early psychodynamic-based object relations theory, 29 Empirically supported interventions, 102
Early script-fading literature, 526 Empirically supported treatment (EST), 16, 17
Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), 5 Endrew court decision, 90
ASD experience, 335 Enhanced parent-mediated ESDM model, 294
brain activity normalization data, 294 Enriched environment (EE), 481
clinical practice and research, 317 Environmental arrangement, 334
developmental level, 294 Environment-behavior, 57
EBP, 294 Equivalence Module, 343
formats and structures, 294 Equivalence-based instruction (EBI), 341
implemented, 335 Error correction, 177–179, 518
intervention model, 294 ESDM curriculum
intervention techniques, 318 checklist, 318
intervention’s efficacy, 336 child’s performance level, 318
NDBI, 317 communicative tools, 318
parent-mediated, 295 developmental domains, 318
play-and interaction-based model, 318 priorities and developmental needs, 318
practices and procedures, 317 ESDM delivery
providers, 294 data, 325
randomized control trial, 295 decision tree, 325
research, 294, 335, 336 fidelity, 325
routines, 294 increasing reinforcer strength, 325
social communication and cognition development, increasing structure and repetitions, 326
295 individuals with autism, 325
EBP in psychology (EBPP), 58 language and natural gestures, 325
EBP in schools positive effect, 325
effectiveness, 82 visual support, 326
IDEA, 82 ESDM evidence base
intervention strategy, 82 advantages, 326
professional disciplines, 82 behavioral intervention, 326
professional organizations, 82 data analysis, 329
research evidence, 82 EIBI delivery, 329
treatment and progress monitoring, 82 G-ESDM, 328–329
Edmark Reading Program, 188 intensity and treatment style, 329
Education, 157 intent-to-treat replication, 327
Education compounded developmental disability, 30 intervention studies, 326
Educational classifications, 84 meta-analysis, 329
Educational outcomes, 96 multisite intent-to-treat RCT, 329
Educational Psychology, 191 P-ESDM (see Parent-implemented ESDM
Educators, 136 (P-ESDM))
Effective collaboration, 126 preferential orientation, 327
fields’ models, 133, 134 RCT, 326
roles and contributions, 136–137 social and non-social preferences, 326
shared scope of practice, 137–138 ESDM in group settings (G-ESDM), 328, 329
soft skills, 136 ESDM practices, clinical description
training and education, 134, 135 child initiative and learning, 319–320
Effective treatments, 24 curriculum, 318
EIBI programming, 454 developmental framework, 319
Elimination diets, 36, 37 integrated approach to intervention, 320–321
Embedded reinforcers, 521 interdisciplinary team, 321
Embedding scripts, 533 multiple methods of delivery, 321
Emerging interventions, 142 ESDM teaching strategies
Emerging scholarship, 155 aims, 321
Emitting behavior, 50 creating positive emotional states, 322
Emotion regulation, 270 family involvement, 324, 325
618 Index

ESDM teaching strategies (cont.) client values and context, 153


intensive teaching, 323 clinical expertise, 154
joint activity routines, 322 clinician expert opinion, 20
positive approaches, unwanted behavior, 323, 324 concept, 58
stimuli and responses, 321 conceptualization, 82
ESDM-treated group, 326 core principles, 12
Establish conflict resolution strategies, 133 criteria, 14
Establish effective leadership, 133 decision-making models, 18
Establish role clarity, 133 decision-making process, 153
Establish trust, 133 definition, 12, 17, 58, 100, 154, 251
Establishing operation (EO), 391, 528 DTT, 172, 180–181
Establishing role clarity, 134 education, 73
Establishing trust, 133–134 effective treatment, 11
Ethical and clinical considerations, 5 evaluation, 74, 75
Ethical causal and decision models to EBP in ABA expansive care, 161–163
decision-making, 60 external clinical evidence, 153
descriptive and normative ethical behavior, 60 external guideline, 153
DTT vs. NET, 61–65 FBA, 76
Ethical decision-making and EBPs field of psychology, 11
applied behavior analysts, 65 government legislation, 102
causal models (see Causal models, ethical decision-­ guidelines, 100
making and EBPs) identification, 12
decision models (see Decision models, ethical IEP, 74
decision-making and EBPs) implementation, 4, 73
future research, 66 indicators, 16, 19
models, 47 intervention, 75
worksheet, 61, 62 interventions/decisions, 18
Ethical decision-making models, 57, 58 knowledge production, 154
Ethical decision models PBIS, 76
ABA, 59 practitioner resources, 19–20
behavior analysis, 58 process of, 59
decision-maker, 58 professional organizations recommendations, 101
function, 57 qualitative element, 12
Ethical obligation, 114 quality services, 102
Ethical principles, 51 relationship and importance, 153
Ethical teaching, 136 research, 154–155
Ethics, 49–51, 55 comparative research, 606
Ethics Code for Behavior Analysts, 12, 125, 132 ethical standards, 608
Ethics for Behavior Analysts, 497 long-term outcomes, 607
Ethnicity, 158 participant demographics, 605
Evaluation, 83 research evidence and clinical expertise, 153
Evaluation Team Report (ETR), 83 research-based procedures, 11
Ever-evolving research, 153 RIRD, 563, 564
Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA), 71 seminal paper, 153
Evidence-based approaches, 117, 118 sets of behaviors, 47
Evidence-based behavioral interventions, 26 single-subject research designs, 13, 101
Evidence-based information, 117 sources of contention, 20
Evidence-based interventions, 2, 128, 142 specific guidelines, 11
Evidence-based methods, 136 standards, 13–16, 100
Evidence-based practice (EBP), 128, 159, 227, 361, 371, state-of-the-art implementation, 6
410, 443–445, 461, 465 terminology, 16
ABA, 59, 101, 102 tools, 101
advantages, 102 treatment effectiveness, 11
ASD treatment, 6 treatment packages, 17
ASHA, 12 wasted resources, 102
assessment, 74, 75 Evidence-based practice decision model, 58, 59
behavior analysts, 17, 73 Evidence-based practice in psychology (EBPP), 16, 17
behavior analysts, schools, 76–77 Evidence-based procedure, 116, 418
behavioral change, 100 Evidence-based training, 135
BIP, 76 Evidence-based treatment, 111, 117, 118
Index 619

Exaggerated emotional expression, 29 vocal behavior, 524


Exchange-based communication systems, 381 vocal imitation responses, 524
advantages, 381, 382 Fad treatments, 2, 102
disadvantages, 382 Families report, 39
Exchange-production schedule, 505 Family Educational Rights and Privacy Act, 86
Exclusionary time-out, 479, 487, 488, 491 Feedback, 252, 254, 255, 257
Exorphins, 36 Feminist approaches, 54
Expansive care, EBP Fidelity of implementation, 333
humanity’s maturity, 162 Fields’ models, 133, 134
interdependency, 162 Fieldwork supervisor, 134, 135
principles, 162 Fixed-interval (FI) schedules, 506
procedures and outcomes, 162–163 Fixed-ratio (FR) schedules, 506
relationship development and communication, 162 Flexible prompt fading, 177
structural oppression and biased delivery systems, 163 Floortime-based interventions, 30
Experimental studies, 38 Formalized and manualized parent training interventions,
Experimental treatments, 26 297
Expressive language, 606 Fracturing of shared reality, 23
External clinical evidence, 153 Free appropriate public education (FAPE), 73, 81, 83, 95
Extinction/differential reinforcement Friendship Qualities Scale, 270
automatic reinforcement, 541 Frozen in autism, 106
aversive event, 542 Full IISCA, 454–455
behavior disorders, 543 Full recovery/typical functioning, 26
behavioral intervention, 539 Full team collaboration, 87
components, 540 Functional analysis (FA) methodology
definition, 539, 551 ASD, 419
feasibility, 542, 543 automatic reinforcement, test, 420
functional variations, 540 behavior conditions, 430
limitation, 541, 542 challenging behavior, 417, 418, 421, 422
logical behavioral intervention, 539 control condition, 420, 421
root cause, 543 decision-making model, 429
side effects, 542 design, 421
socially mediated negative reinforcement, 541 design FA conditions, 425
socially mediated positive reinforcement, 540 experimental design
tool, 544 multielement, 428
reversal, 428
test-control (pairwise), 429
F FBA, 417
Facilitated Communication (FC), 19 function-based interventions, 418, 429
adapted communication device, 31 latency-based, 433–435
controversial treatments, 31 precursor, 432
core belief, 31 procedural safeguards, determine, 422–424
debunking, 31 SCA, 434, 435
extreme belief, 32 session duration, 427
facilitator, 103 social functions, 430
fundamental belief, 103 social negative reinforcement, 420
nonverbal individuals, 31 social positive reinforcement, 419, 420
persistence, 32 systematic prevention procedures, 435
proponents, 31 target behavior, 435
quality experimental studies, 31 target challenging behavior, 430
research, 103 test condition, 418, 419
teaching strategy, 103 therapists/modaity, 426, 427
Factors influencing script selection trial-based, 431
approximate vocal models, 524 Functional approach
articulation challenges, 524 IDEA, 92
auditory stimuli, 524 Functional assessment, 444
picture-object correspondence, 524 Functional behavior assessment (FBA), 74, 76, 298, 417
prerequisite reading skills, 523 behavior analytic assumption, 92
prerequisite skills, 524 behavioral expertise and knowledge, 92
student learning, 524 challenging behavior, 86, 92
technological advancements, 524 definition, 92
620 Index

Functional communication, 29, 361–363, 365–368 Generalized communication skills, 29


Functional communication response (FCR), 465, 466 Generative sociodramatic play, 521
Functional communication training (FCT), 5, 74, 102, Genetic/chromosomal conditions, 106
377, 545 Gentle teaching model, 28, 29
autism spectrum disorder (ASD), 465 Gestures, 377
challenging behaviors reduction, 298 Goal setting, 580, 581
communication skills development, 298 Good treatment, 39
developmental disabilities, 465 Gradual script-fading procedure, 527
EBP, 298 Graduate education/supervision, 127
effectiveness, 297 Green light interventions, 20
employment, 465 Green light treatments, 143
FBAs, 298 Group-based parent-mediated PRT program, 293
FCR, 466 Group-based Project ImPACT curriculum, 296
feasibility of extinction, 472–474 Group-based PRT, 293
future research, 299 Group contingency system, 282
generalization Group designs, 13, 14, 17, 109
domains, 470 Group experimental/quasi-experimental research designs,
environments, 470 14
stimuli, 471 Group research methodology, 606
strategies, 470 Group social skills interventions (GSSI), 270, 272, 273
technology, 470
training, 470, 471
treatment effects, 470 H
literature review, 298 Health care, 157
manualized treatment approach, 298 Health sciences, 24
overarching goals, 298 Heavy metal poisoning, 38
parent’s training models, 298 High-functioning autism (HFA), 589
parental involvement, 298 Holding therapy, 27, 28
performative feedback, 298 Homogeneity, 155
planning process, 299 Homogenous cultural group, 155
problem behaviors, 465, 474 Honesty, 54
reinforcement, 298 Human behavior, 152
reinforcement schedule thinning Human observation, 26
concurrent chains schedule, 469 Humanity, 152
contingency- and time-based delays, 468, 469 Humility, 157–159
interventions, 469, 470 concept of, 157
multiple schedules, 467 Hybrid telehealth ESDM approach, 302
problem behavior, 467 Hybrid telehealth programs, 302
related disorders, 465 Hyperacuity, 34
resurgence, 472 Hyperbaric oxygen therapy (HBOT), 35
varied implementation fidelity, 298 Hypersensitive/hyperacute hearing, 34
well-established behavioral approach, 297
Functional communicative response (FCR), 379
Functional living skills I
future research, 239 IDEIA law, 77
generalization measures, 239 Identity-first language, 81
procedural variations of, 238, 239 IEP and Students with ASD
reliability, 239 IEP planning process
Functional reinforcement, 443 assessment, 86
Function-based interventions, 113 educational plan, 86
Function-based treatment, 443, 444, 450, 459, 460 form, 86, 87
IEP team, 86
meetings, 87, 88
G IEP components, 84–85
Gastrointestinal (GI), 35 IEP formulation process, 85
Gastrointestinal dysfunction, 126 IEP implementation
General education curriculum, 84, 95–96 activity matrices, 90
General education environment, 86 collaborative with professionals, 89
Generalization Module, 342, 343 communication goal targeting, 89
Index 621

data-based decision-making, 91–92 social intervention, 197, 198


functional approach, 92–93 social sensory games, 198
goals, 88 target responses and populations, 189, 190
inclusive environment, 89 temporal/physical dimensions, 185
inclusive settings, 88 toddlers, 198, 199
QoL, 90 toy rotations, 194
scheduling, 89, 90 university-based lab schools, 199
school professionals, 89 Walden classroom, 192, 193
selecting EBP, 90, 91 Inclusion, 95
staff training and coaching, 93 Inclusionary time-out, 486
team’s responsibility, 89 contingent observation, 486
technical and social support, 93, 94 ignoring, 487
IEP meetings, 87, 88 removal of stimulus conditions, 486
IEP planning form, 86, 87 Incorporating scripts within activity schedules, 533
IEP planning process, 85, 86 Independence, 519
IEP team, 85 Independent contingency, 282
Ignoring, 487 Independent living skills, 593, 594
Imitation, 332 Indigenous peoples, 158
ImPACT Online, 303 Individual behavior management, 282
Imperative assessments, 86 Individual parent-child dyad sessions, 293
In vivo language models, 529 Individualized activity schedules, 521
In vivo modeling, 244 Individualized Education Program (IEP), 72–77, 83–85
Incidental teaching, 4, 333 Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP), 72, 77
analysis of behavior, 185 Individualized planning, 89
applications, 188, 189 Individualized programming, 96
autism, 188, 189, 200 Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 30
behavior-specific performance feedback child’s education, 85
system, 200 components, 81
challenging learning goals, 185 comprehensive federal law, 83
components, 186 education program, 85
conversational language, 196, 197 ETR, 83
definition, 185 FAPE, 83
diverse groups, 200 LRE, 94
early autism intervention, 201, 202 multiple disabilities, 84
evidence-based practice, 201, 202 public education, 82
free-play area, 193–195 students’ IEPs, 91
Kansas models, 193 United States, 81
learning, 185 Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act
naturalistic behavioral procedures, 186 (IDEIA)
neurotypical children, 186, 187 behavior analysts, schools, 76–77
neurotypical peers, 200 No Child Left Behind Act, 71
preparing parents of children, 195 Part B, 72, 73
provider preparation study Part C, 72
caregivers, 191 PRR, 71
classroom environments, 191 special education, 71–73
environments, 190 Individuals with Disabilities in Education Act, 71
institutional staff, 190 Ineffectiveness/documented harm, 142
international time zones, 192 Infant/toddler learning and participation, 319
methodology, 191 Infantile autism, 27
preschools, 190 Inhalation therapy equipment, 407
remote coaching, 191 In-home therapist, 452
remote location, 191 Innovative outcome measurement approaches, 306
remote telepractice package, 191 In-person RUBI parent training model, 303
social validity survey, 191 Insensitive professionals, 336
teachers, 190 Insidious pseudoscience theories, 37
trainees, 191 Instructive feedback, 179
reinforcer potency to empower learning, 193 Instructor, 260
remote telehealth technologies, 200 Instructor-delivered reinforcement, 526, 527
routine childcare activities, 192 Instructor-managed motivational system, 520
622 Index

Integrated approach to intervention standards projects, 3


ABA principles, 320 Intervention medleys, 138
childhood professionals, 320 Intervention strategies, 289
constructivist development principles, 320 Interventionist, 114
curriculum re-assessment, 321 Interventions, 607
goals, 320 Interview-informed, synthesized contingency analysis
motor skills, 320 (IISCA)
pediatric, 320 informed procedures, 450
professional intervention practice, 320 matched control, 451
professionals, 321 open-contingency class, 451
progress data, 320 procedural variations, 454
Integrated Listening Systems, 34 full IISCA, 454–455
Intellectual and developmental disabilities (IDD), 419 latency-based IISCA, 458–459
Intellectual delays (IDs), 376 single-session IISCA, 455–457
Intellectual disability (ID), 274 trial-based IISCA, 456–458
Intellectual functioning, 99 single test condition, 449–450
Intellectually and developmental disabilities (IDD), 544 synthesized contingencies, 450–451
Intelligence quotient (IQ), 271 Intestinal mucosa, 36
Intensive behavioral intervention, 107 IQ/cognitive abilities, 30
Intensive intervention, 2 Isolated reinforcement, 443, 451
Intensive teaching, 323 Isolation, 487, 489, 491
Intercultural communication, 160
Interdependency, 162
Interdependent contingency, 282 J
Interdisciplinary approaches, 136 Joint activity routines, 322
Interdisciplinary collaboration, 134 Joint attention, 332
challenges Joint attention interventions, 142
collaborative skills, 133 Junior behavior analysts, 132
disciplines (see Disciplines approach) Justice, principle of, 54
lack of training (see Lack of training)
other team members, 132
technical terminology, 130–132 K
consulting resources, 141–144 Killer app, 156
decision-making models (see Decision-making Knower/believer, 157
models) Knowledge-intensive firms, 156
definition and concept, 126–127
effective (see Effective collaboration)
interdisciplinary team’s treatment recommendations, L
127 Label/identify skill, 252, 253
treatment classifications, 143–144 Lack of training
Interdisciplinary teaming, 96 in collaboration, 128, 129
International Classification of Diseases, tenth edition in empathy and relationship building, 129–130
(ICD-10), 33 Language, 158
Internet, 40 Language development, 323, 334
Internet-delivered PCIT (I-PCIT), 304 Language skills, 279, 280
Inter-observer agreement (IOA), 502 Large-rotating food sets, 409
Interpretative uncertainties, 448 Latency-based IISCA, 458–459
Interprofessional approaches, 138 Leadership, 159
Interprofessional collaboration, 136 Leaky gut, 36, 38
Interprofessional conflict, 137 Learner role-plays, 254
Interprofessional teams, 142 Learning, 158
Interrater Agreement (IRA), 556 communities of practice, 159
Interresponse times (IRTs), 546 community, 159
Intersectionality, 158 creating opportunities, 160–161
Intervention critical race theory, 159
checklist/questionnaire, 3 demographics, 159
effectiveness, 3 initial training, 159
evidence-based, 3 nurture progress, 161
non-evidence-based practices, 3 provisional spaces, 159
selection and implementation, 4 time and resource allocation, 160
Index 623

Least restrictive environment (LRE), 73, 83 N


Least-to-most prompt hierarchies, 516 National Academy of Science, 40
Least-to-most prompting system, 176 National Association for Education of Young Children
Liability of self-fullness, 158, 159 (NAEYC), 202
Lidcombe Program, 140, 141 National Association of School Psychologists, 104
Life-affirming process, 152 National Autism Center, 15, 141
Loose shaping, 334 National Clearinghouse for Autism Evidence and
Lovaas’ methods, 330 Practice (NCAEP), 289, 564
Lower Elementary Level Acceptance Scale (A-Scale), age groups, 16
531 criteria, 15
criticisms, 16
evidence-based practices, 15, 16
M quality indicators, 16
Magnetic food-magazine, 404 reports, 16
Mainstream media, 105 search process, 15
Making empathic statements, 136 National Council on Disability, 83
Manual signing (MS), 378 National Institutes of Health, 40
advantages, 378 National organizations, 39
disadvantages, 378, 379 National Professional Development Center (NPDC), 361
Manualized Intervention Meeting Criteria (MIMC), 293 National Standards Project, 14, 15, 19
Measles mumps, and rubella (MMR) vaccine, 38 Natural interactions, 28
Mediators, 387 Natural Language Paradigm (NLP), 209, 210
Mental health professionals, 108 Natural reinforcement, 334
Mental health workers, 136 Naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention
Mental illness and developmental disabilities, 30 (NDBI), 202, 292
Meta-analysis, 99 ABA and child development, 329
Metacontingencies, 152 core symptoms in autism, 330
Methodological weaknesses, 297 developmental perspectives, 331
Mid-SES American group, 336 developmental perspectives and autism, 330, 331
Minimal standards, 101 disruptive behavior, 332
Misinformation, 40 escape- and avoidance-motivated behavior, 332
Misinformed treatment choices, 40 learning and positive behavior supports, 317
Misunderstanding autism learning goals and plans, 332
ABA, 28 pre-NDBI, 330
environment effect on behavior, 27 promotes social development, 332
Modeling, 335 reduced unwanted and disruptive behavior, 332
Moderators, 387 Naturalistic environment teaching (NET), 391
Modern information age, 23 Naturalistic interventions, 133, 292, 293
Momentary DRO, 546 Naturalistic teaching strategies (NaTS), 391
Momentary time sampling (MTS), 558 NDBI approaches, 296
Monumental gains, 24 NDBI components
Moral and ethical behavior adult imitation, 335
definitions, 55 balanced turn within object, 334
functions broadening attentional focus, 335
avoidance, 50 child-initiated teaching, 333, 334
ethical principles, 51 environmental arrangement, 334
generalized social reinforcement, 50 fidelity of implementation, 333
recognition, 51 instructional strategies nature, 333
socially mediated reinforcement, 50 intervention target nature, 332
Morality, 49–51 learning context’s nature, 333
Most-to-least prompting system, 176 natural reinforcement, 334
Most-to-least with a delay (MTLD), 466 three-part contingency, 333
Motivating operation (MO), 380 NDBI treatment targets, 292
Motivational systems, 521 NEBPs positive comments, 105
Movement-based sensory interventions, 34 Negative punishment, 479, 480, 482, 486, 490, 493
Multidisciplinary treatment, 126 Negative reinforcement, 480, 490
Multiple exemplar training (MET), 585 Neurodevelopmental deficits, 30
Multiple popular dietary interventions, 36 Neurodevelopmental disability, 30
Multiple stimulus without replacement (MSWO), 410, Neurodevelopmental factors, 29
504 Neurodiversity, 158
624 Index

Neuroinflammation theory, 34, 35 supportive feedback and attention, 112


Newhouse-Oisten’s Model, 140–141 testimonials, 107
Newly diagnosed children, 117 treatment, 36
No Child Left Behind Act, 71 Non-evidence-based treatment plans, 111
Non-behavior analysts, 127, 131 Non-evidence-based treatments, 31, 34
Non-behavior colleagues, 130–132, 137, 138, 140 Non-evidenced-based procedures
Non-behavioral intervention, 112, 139 ASD diagnoses, 3
Non-behavioral professionals, 132 autistics/individuals diagnosed with ASD, 4
Nonbehavioral services, 608 competing ideologies/philosophies, 2
Non-behavioral treatment, 132, 139, 140 consumer cost, 2
Non-behavior-analytic interventions, 109, 110 eclectic approaches, 3
Non-behavior-analytic treatments, 110 emotional toll, 2
Non-clinicians, 32 implementation risk, 2
Non-collaborative practices, 138 individual’s physical/medical health effects, 3
Noncontingent reinforcement (NCR), 479, 481, 482, 484, long-term side effects, 3
485 short-term side effects, 3
Nonconventional actions, 29 No-no prompting, 176
Non-disabled individuals, 519 Non-refereed publications, 158
Non-effective treatments, 105 Non-social experiences, 318
Nonelectronic communication, 382 Non-specialist professionals, 39
Nonelectronic systems, 380 Non-technical language, 130
Non-evidence-based assertions, 24 Non-traditional spaces, 159
Non-evidence-based interventions, 31, 127, 133 Nonverbal behavior, 251
Non-evidence-based practices (NEBPs) Non-verbal change, 532
ABA, 39 Normative ethical behavior, 65
autism diagnosis, 106 Normative ethical theory, 53, 60
autism treatment, 102 Novel initiations, 531
autism, 25 Novel interactions, 531
BACB, 110 Novel utterances, 531
behavior analysts, 109, 111 Nutrient deficiencies, 126
clinical decision-making, 112
conceptually systematic, 114
consumers, 111 O
contingency plans, 115 Occupational and physical therapy, 126
creation and consumption, 24 Occupational therapist, 139
data, 107 Occupational therapy, 128, 133, 137, 139
DTI, 103 Office of Special Education Programs (OSEP), 91
effectiveness, 116 On-schedule behavior, 515
ethical standards, 115–116 On-task and on-schedule behavior, 520
evaluation, 113 Open-ended assessments, 444
experimental design, 110 Open-ended interview, 445–450, 452, 453, 456, 460
families’ choice, 38 Open-ended surveys, 108
implementation and promotion, 103 Operant conditioning, 479
implementation, 112, 113 Operant learning, 322
initial investigations, 116 Opioid excess theory, 36
intervention, 116 Oppressive and biased delivery systems, 163
language, 105 Oppressive structures, 151
non-behavior-analytic professional literature, 105 Optimality, 56
positive comments, 105 Organizational skills, 95
positive consequence, 108
practitioners, 38, 116
print media, 105 P
professionals, 107 Parent education program, 300
quick fixes, 39 Parent implementation interventions
reinforcement, 111 active participants, 291
review board approval, 116 behavior reduction (see Parent-training interventions)
sensory integration therapy, 105 clinical implications, 305
service providers, 113–115 clinical judgment, 305
Son-Rise, 107 clinical outcomes, 305
supervision, 110 clinical practice, 291
Index 625

comprehensive review, 291 definition, 291


development and adaptation, 307 ESDM, 294–295
disadvantages, 306 learning and implementing strategies, 292
efficiency and effectiveness, 306 NDBI, 292
empirical demonstrations, 290 parent learning roles, 292
evidence-based strategies, 305 project ImPACT, 295–296
formalization and manualization, 305 PRT, 293–294
longitudinal research, 306 standardized and manualized, 292
opportunities, 305 targeting core symptoms, 305
parent-mediated NDBIs, 305 Parent-mediated NDBIs, 292, 296
research, 307 Parent-mediated PRT
skill-building (see Parent-mediated interventions) advantages, 293
sustainability and effectiveness, 307 empirical support, 293
telehealth, 291 individual and group formats, 293
theoretical and empirical rationale, 290 intensive formats, 294
Parent involvement in ASD intervention language and adaptive communication gains, 293
acceptable and desirable, 290 literature, 294
advantages, 290 parent and child level outcomes, 296
categories, 291 parent coaching sessions, 293
child language skills improvement, 290 randomized control trial, 293
child’s development, 290 Parents and service providers, 107
family functioning, 290 Parent-training interventions
generalization and maintenance, 290 ABA principles, 297
literature base, 291 behavior reduction, 291, 305
long-standing support, 290 behavioral improvements, 301
NCAEP Review, 290 behavioral management strategies, 297
opportunities, 290 earliest research, 297
positive parental outcomes, 290 evidence-based, 297
resource requirement, 290 FCT, 297–299
taxonomy, 291 intervention models, 297
treatment targets, 291 intervention research, 307
Parental decision-making, 106 PCIT, 300–301
Parental involvement in ASD intervention RUBI, 299–300
empowerment, 324 targeting co-occurring behavioral difficulties, 301
Parental participation, 85 Partial interval recording (PIR), 558
Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT), 303 Participation model, 385–387
child development, 300 PEAK relational training system, 341
co-morbid disruptive behaviors, 301 clinical outcomes of, 346
disruptive behaviors with ASD, 300 clinical tools, multiple studies, 350
effectiveness, 300 direct training module, 342
language and developmental levels, 301 effectiveness, 344
parent–child intervention, 300 equivalence module, 343
treatment phases, 300 generalization module, 342, 343
Parent-directed interaction (PDI), 300 group-based randomized controlled trials, 345
Parent-implemented ESDM (P-ESDM) PCA, 343–345, 349
child progress, 328 procedures, 350, 354
DQ, 327 psychometric properties of, 347–349
fidelity of implementation, 327–328 referencing PEAK in other areas, 355
hypothesis, 327 skill-specific gains, 351–354
intent-to-treat study, 327 traditional ABA group or comprehension ABA group,
intent-to-treat trial, 328 344
lowered parental stress, 327 transformation module, 343
methodological addition, 328 transitivity, 341
parent coaching, 327, 328 Peer-reviewed journals, 39, 604
randomized study, 327 Peer-reviewed research (PRR), 71
research, 327 Percentage of nonoverlapping data (PND), 589, 591
verbal production and imitation, 327 Personal choice, 12
Parent-implemented FCT, 298, 299 Personal digital assistant (PDA), 521
Parent-mediated ESDM, 294, 295, 302 Person-first language, 81
Parent-mediated interventions, 5 Photographic activity schedules, 520
626 Index

Physical environment, 152 social conversation, 222


Physical prompting strategies, 526 socialization, 217
Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS), 5, teachers, 209
141, 326, 361, 362 teaching question asking, 217, 218
adaptations of, 367 technical materials, 293
for bilingual learners, 368 Planned Activity Check (PLA-Check), 187
for learners with multiple disabilities, 367, 368 Planning/activity matrices, 90
caregiver and learner preference, 366, 367 Play
clinical recommendations, 369, 370 diverse range of participants, 229
learner outcomes, 362, 363 future research, 232
generality of, 363 generalization and maintenance, 231
peers as communicative partners, 363, 364 reliability, 231
limitations, 368, 369 social validity, 231
teaching implementation of, 364 targeting play, 230
parent training, 365, 366 types of, 229, 230
teacher and university student training, 365 Play and Language for Autistic Youngsters (PLAY), 30
Pictures and line drawings, 380 Podcasts, 158
advantages, 380 Point-based techniques, 382, 383
disadvantages, 380 advantages, 383
Piers-Harris Self-Concept Scale, 270 disadvantages, 383, 384
Pivotal areas, 293 Police Brutality, 157
Pivotal developmental skills, 306 Porcine secretin injections, 37
Pivotal response training (PRT), 4, 604, 605 Position statements, 19, 20
Pivotal response treatment (PRT), 209, 333 Positive behavior interventions and supports (PBIS), 75,
adolescence/adulthood, 208 76
adult-driven model, 212 Positive parent–child interactions, 296
ASD, 207 Positive punishment, 480
behavior management and teaching trials, 208 Positive reinforcement, 479, 480, 492
comprehensive teaching model, 293 Post-traumatic stress disorder, 28
construct of motivation, 209 Poverty, 158
EBP, 293 Power imbalances, 157
empathy, 220, 221 Practical functional assessment (PFA), 5
evidence-based practices, 207 automatic reinforcement, 460–461
functions and teaching replacement behaviors, 208 brief observation, 447–448
group-based program, 293 functional analysis, 448, 449 (see also Interview-­
infantile autism, 207 informed, synthesized contingency analysis
intervention identity, 293 (IISCA))
interventionists, 209 functional assessment process, 444
learned helplessness, 210 function-based treatment, 459
learning, 217 logical reasoning of interpretation, 445
motivational components marked improvements, 459
academics, 215, 216 open-ended interview, 445–447
adolescents, 215 proactive and reactive strategies, 459, 460
adults, 214, 215 transparent process, 459
child choice, 211 treatment utility, 452
infants, 213, 214 outpatient clinics, 453–454
intersperse maintenance tasks, 211 problem behavior in home, 452–453
natural rewards, 211, 212 problem behavior in school, 453
pre-linguistic infants, 214 Practitioner resources
preschoolers, 214 green light interventions, 20
reward attempts, 212 guild organizations, 20
task variation, 211 interprofessional tension, 19
motivational variables, 209 multidisciplinary settings, 19
NDBI approach, 293 non-speech oral motor exercises, 19
parentectomy, 207 position statements, 19
pivotal areas, 210 recommendations, 20
play-based interactions, 209 red light interventions, 19
psychodynamic interventions, 208 Pragmatic validation process, 452
refrigerator parents, 208 Predictors, 387
social and educational skills building, 293 Preference assessment, 74
Index 627

Preintentional actions, 29 Pseudoscience, 2, 19, 33, 117


Pre-NDBI, 330 Pseudoscience theory, 36
Prerequisite skills, 241 Psychoanalytic therapy, 27
Prescription medication, 106 Psychodynamic theory, 27
Princeton Child Development Institute (PCDI), 188, 524 Psychological nomenclatures, 33
Probably efficacious treatments, 26 Psychological trauma, 28
Problem behavior, 240, 280, 465–474, 555, 560, 561, Psychology, 137, 157
572 PTSD symptoms, 39
history of reinforcement, 445 Public education, 82
future research, 241 Public figures, 24
procedural variations, 240 Public health, 157
Procedural safeguards, 86 Pulitzer Prize poet Gwendolyn Brooks’ work, 161
Professional and Ethical Compliance Code for Behavior Punishment
Analysts, 102, 110, 111 common general applications, 480
Professional behavior analytic organizations, 604, 605 natural occurrence, 480
Professional codes of ethics, 50 negative punishment, 479
Professional goals, 27 primary punishers, 480
Professional organizations, 154 principle, 480
Professionals interact, 152 procedures in problem behavior treatment, 480–482
Program evaluation, 161 secondary punishers, 480
Program for the Education and Enrichment of Relational Punishment-based procedures, 480, 490
Skills for Young Adults (PEERS‐YA), 275
Program placement decisions, 95
Programmed reinforcement, 527 Q
Progressive approach QoL-influenced curriculum, 90
to DTT, 171, 172, 174, 177–179 (see also Discrete Quality evidence, 101
trial teaching (DTT)) Quality indicators, 13
Progressive time delay, 176 Quality interventions, 608
Project Home Consultation model, 30 Quality of life (QoL), 90, 157, 161
Project ImPACT Quality research, 109
academic-community partnerships, 295 Quality supervision, 110
children’s imitation and social communication skills, Quality-of-service provision, 100
296
community trial, 296
development, 295 R
effectiveness, 296 Race, 158
evaluation in community settings, 296 Racism, 157
ImPACT Online, 303 Random supervision, 518
individual and group-based model, 295 Randomized control designs, 25
NCAEP Parent-Implemented Intervention category, Randomized control study, 35
295 Randomized control trial (RCT), 30, 101, 270–275, 295,
parent use, 306 606
RCT, 295 Rationales, 253
research base, 296 Reciprocal imitation, 335
social communication outcomes, 295 Reciprocal imitation training (RIT), 244, 333
targets, 295 Reciprocal interactions, 334
Promote effective practices, 137 Reciprocal process, 405
Prompt dependence, 519 Red light interventions, 19
Prompting, 587 Red light treatments, 143
decision-making tool, 177, 178 Refrigerator mother, 27, 29
definition, 175 Registered Behavior Technician (RBT) credential, 51,
flexible prompt fading, 177 607
prompt types, 175 Regression, 106
prompting systems, 175, 176 Reinforcement, 520
Prompting systems, 175, 176 effect of, 404
least-to-most prompting, 176 quickness in, 404
most-to-least prompting, 176 source of, 404
no-no prompt, 176, 177 Reinforcement systems, 519
passage of time, 176 Reinforcement value, 521
Provider-receiver relationship, 160 Reinforcer absent interval (RAI), 457
628 Index

Reinforcer present interval (RPI), 457 social validity, 566, 567


Reinforcers, 112, 519 treatment integrity, 568, 569
Reinforcing attempts, 334 untargeted stereotypy, 566
Reinforcing consequence, 177–179 evaluations, 559
Relational frame theory, 342, 346, 355, 356 abolishing operation procedures, 561
Relationship Development Intervention (RDI) program, 30 antecedent modification, 561
Religion, 158 differential reinforcement, 562
Removal of privileges, 484 interventions, 563
Removal of stimulus conditions, 486 noncontingent reinforcement, 562
Replacement behaviors, 324 RIRD plus contingent interventions, 562, 563
Replication criterion, 101 RIRD plus reinforcement components, 562
Rescorla-Wagner model, 49 stimulus control procedures, 561
Research evidence, 157 tasks, type of, 560
Research publications, 155 termination criteria, 560
Research Units in Behavioral Intervention (RUBI) topography of RIRD demands, 559
ABA framework, 299 literature search method, 556
aims, 299 inclusion criteria, 556
baseline cognition, 300 Interrater Agreement, 556, 557
content, 299 study characteristics
effectiveness studies, 300 participants, 557
evidence base, 300 target behavior and functional assessment, 557,
follow-up studies, 300 558
formal therapist training protocol, 301 target behavior measurement, 558, 559
group-based format, 300 Responsive care, 157
manualized intervention approach, 299 Responsive caregiving, 323
pharmacological intervention, 300 Restricted and repetitive behavior, 592
reinforcers concepts, 299 Rett syndrome, 112
dupplemental sessions, 299 Reviews/meta-analysis, 3
therapist support, 299 Rhythm, 35
Research-based practices, 515 Rich schedule of reinforcement, 532
Research-to-practice gap, 100 Robotic-like responding, 180
Response cost condition, 483 Role blurring, 136
Response cost procedures Role-plays, 254, 257, 259, 260
advantages Routines, 322
easy to implementation, 484
implementation in natural environment, 484
social validity, 484 S
with reinforcement-based strategies, 484 Safety, 451, 452, 456, 458, 459
behavior change tactics, 479 Safety skill
cultural and demographic variables, 492–493 future research, 238
disadvantages generalization and maintenance, 237
potential for misuse, 485 procedural variations of, 234–236
punishment and social stigma, 485 Samonas Sound Therapy, 34
undesirable side effects, 485 Schedule-following behavior, 516, 518, 520
punishment strategies, 482 Schedule-following skills, 516, 519
removal of earned reinforcing stimuli, 479 Scheduling, 89, 90
treatment packages, 483 Schizophrenia, 1, 27
Response efficiency, 387, 388 Scholarship, 153
Response effort, 388 Schools
Response interruption and redirection (RIRD), 5, 555 advantages, 94
clinical and research recommendations, 569–572 collaboration and interdisciplinary support, 94
efficacy and outcomes, 563 diverse and inclusive, 94, 95
appropriate behavior, 566 general education curriculum, 95
challenging behavior, 566 interdisciplinary teams, 94
contingent interventions, 565 opportunities, 94
evidence-based practice, 563, 564 students with ASD, 94
generalization of, 567, 568 students’ development, 94
maintenance of, 567 Schools, behavior analysts, 76–77
medication, 564 School-Wide Positive Behavior Interventions and
reinforcement-based procedures, 564, 565 Supports (SWPBIS), 92
Index 629

Science, 23, 24, 604 Self-managed motivational system, 522


and EBPs, 604 Self-managed response cost, 492
Science-based evidence, 24 Self-managed system, 522
Science-based information, 39 Self-management, 521
Science-based standards, 116 academics, 220
Scientific approach, 118 ASD
Scientific investigation, 27 academic skills, 591–593
Scientific Merit Rating Scale, 15 decreasing challenging behavior, 594, 595
Scientific method, 23 effectiveness, 589
Scientific sources, 39 independent living skills, 593
Scientific studies, 23 PND score, 589
Script prompting, 587
audiotaped/written word, 523 social skills, 589–591
content selection, 525–527 vocational skills, 593, 594
selections, 523 behavior analysis, 579, 580
teaching students, 523 benefits, 596, 597
Scripted interactions, 530 clinical use, 597, 598
Script-fading literature, 523, 530 community settings, 218
Script-fading strategies conceptualization and use, 522
advanced vocal repertoires, 527, 530 definition, 577
advantages, 533 evidence-based intervention strategy, 522
data collection, 529 FR2 schedule of reinforcement, 522
discriminative stimulus, 528 future research in, 595, 596
embedding scripts within activity schedules, 532 goal setting, 580, 581
generalization programming, 530–531 importance and benefit of, 578, 579
gradual, 527 interfering behaviors, 220
initiations frequency, 528 procedures, 218
instructional targets, 532 recommendations for using, 598, 599
language use, 528 reinforcement conditions, 522
leisure activities, 528 reinforcers, 522
partners (see Conversation partners) self-administer rewards, 219
planning, 527 self-evaluation, 522, 583, 584
scripted interactions, 530 self-instruction, 585–587
social initiations, 528 self-management program, 219
social validity, 531–532 self-monitoring, 522, 581, 582
steps, 527 self-reinforcement, 584, 585
stimuli, 528 social communication, 219, 220
students with autism, 528 target behavior, 218
teaching strategy identification, 526–527 theoretical basis of, 577, 578
unscripted initiations, 530 Self-management and monitoring procedures, 5
Scripts Self-monitoring, 581, 582
factors influencing selection, 523–525 Self-reflection, 113, 160
Seclusion time-out, 487–489 Self-regulated strategy development model (SRSD), 591
Secretin, 36, 37 Self-regulatory behavior, 522
Secretin supplementation, 36, 37 Self-reinforcement, 579, 580, 584, 585, 588, 593–595,
Self-advocacy skills, 85–86 598
Self-confidence, 157 Self-reinforcement condition, 522
Self-control, 33, 55, 577–579 Self-reinforcing, 23
Self-cultivation, 158 Self-reliance, 578
Self-delivery of reinforcers, 521 Self-scheduling, 513
Self-determination, 85 Sensory diet, 34, 107
Self-esteem, 131 Sensory integration, 33, 104, 107, 127
Self-evaluation, 583, 584 Sensory integration therapy (SIT), 34, 102, 139
Self-evaluation of speech fluency, 141 Sensory processing, 33
Self-guided telehealth programs, 302 Sensory processing disorders (SPDs), 33
Self-imposed formation, 23 Sensory social routines (SSRs), 322
Self-injurious behavior (SIB), 26, 54–56, 102, 443, 452, Service providers, 113, 118
460, 461 Service provision organization, 52
Self-Injury Trauma [SIT], 423 Sexual behavior, 51
Self-instruction, 585, 586 Shadow support, 281
630 Index

Shaping, 403 Social communication skills, 291, 292


ASD, 412 Social competence, 270, 271
autism, 408 Social contingencies, 267, 268
caregivers, 409 Social environment, 152
component analyses, 411 Social initiations, 532
definition, 405 Social interactions, 151, 333, 533
effectiveness, 408 Social intervention, 197, 198
evidence-based practice, 410 Social knowledge, 284
examples of research, 406, 407 Social media, 40, 158, 159
five-step response sequence, 409 Social Motivation Hypothesis of autism, 318
food, 408 Social orienting/social motivation hypothesis, 323
historical descriptions, 403 Social reinforcement, 50, 527
level system, 409 Social reward system, 322
linear perspective, 404 Social scaffolding, 320
modeling and prompting, 409 Social skill deficits, 280
nonlinear perspective, 405 Social skills, 255, 262, 277, 284, 589–591
participants’ behavior, 409 Social skills acquisition, 29
practice, 412 Social skills curriculum, 284
practitioners and trainers, 413 Social skills groups
reasons, 404 activities, 269
reciprocal, 405 adults with ASD, 274
reinforcement, 410 aspirations, 275
research, 411 assessment measures, 272
responses, 408 benefits, 268
Skinner’s description, 403 clinical implications, 277
speaker/listener, 411 clinical practice experiences (see Clinical practice
training methods, 412 experiences, social skills groups)
use, 407 current research strengths, 276
vocal cue and prompting, 409 effects, 270
Simultaneous prompting, 176, 177 efficacy, 285
Single-case research design (SCRD), 116, 606 emotion regulation, 270
Single-case research methodology, 116 evaluation, 270
Single-session IISCA, 455–457 friendship, 270
Single-subject research design study, 13 future research, 271–273, 275–277, 285
context specification, 13 future studies, 274
data, 13 game, 269
evidence-based practice, 13 generalization and maintenance strategies, 269
functional control, 13 GSSI studies, 273
operationally defined, 13 individuals, 267, 269
Single-subject research designs, 13 individuals with ASD, 269, 285
SIT behavioral intervention, 103 individuals with autism, 285
SIT philosophical underpinning, 103 instructors, 269
SIT therapies, 34 literature review, 270, 271
Skills, 258, 335 loneliness, 270
Sleep behavior, 137 manualized curriculum, 285
Sleep disturbances, 137 meta-analysis, 272
Sleep interventions, 110 natural contingencies, 269
Small-constant food sets, 409 natural environment, 270
Social acceptability, 491, 492 opportunity to pair peers and reinforcing
Social activities, 215 events, 268
Social and emotional connection, 105 outcome measures, 271
Social behavior, 267, 270, 272 outcomes, 269
Social Cognition and Interaction Training for Adults participants, 271
(SCIT‐A), 275 peers, 268, 269
Social communication, 218–220, 232, 270, 301 PEERS-YA, 275
future research, 233–234 post-intervention treatment, 275
generalization and maintenance, 233 programs, 273
procedural variations of, 232 quality of life, 270
Social communication delay, 318 RCTs, 270, 275
Social communication impairments in ASD, 292 research, 270, 285
Index 631

review, 270, 274 Static-picture schedule, 514, 515


salient social stimuli and consequences, 268 Statistical analyses, 25
SCIT‐A program, 275 Stigmatization, 37
self-report assessments, 272 Stimulus, 519
shares play items, 268 Stimulus control and generalization
skill acquisition development, 267 assessment, 530
social behavior, 267 common stimuli usage, 530
social communication, 270 novel initiations, 531
social competence, 270–272 novel utterances, 531
social contingence, 267, 268 participant information, 531
social functioning, 274 social interactions, 530
social performance, 272 spectrum, 530
social relationships, 268 vocal responses, 531
social skills areas, 271 Stimulus equivalence, 341
social skills interventions, 285 Stimulus-Response-Consequence (SRC) trials, 208
social skills performance, 269 Stress test, 451
social skills programs, 275 Student participation, 85
source population, 271 Sunland Miami Training Center, 498
SRS scores, 273 Supervised experience, 113
standardized assessment scales, 270 Supervision, 517
studies, 270, 272–274 Supervision experience, 129
systematic review, 273 Supervision non-continuous, 518
teaching peers as social stimulus, 268 Supervision strategies, 517
teaching social skills, 267 Synchronous engagement, 409
therapist, 269 Synthesized contingencies, 449–451
treatment group, 271 Synthesized Contingency Analysis (SCA), 434
trials, 271 Systematic fading, 517, 518
variable contingencies, 267 Systematic reviews, 37
Workplace Training Programme, 275 Systematic thinning, 520
Social skills interventions, 270
Social skills performance, 269
Social skills programs, 275 T
Social stigmatization, 515 Tablet-based activity schedules, 515
Social stimulus, 268 Tangible/tactile symbols, 379
Social stories, 104 advantages, 379
Social structures of society, 156 disadvantages, 380
Social validity, 161, 231, 489, 566, 567, 607 Target behavior
acceptability, 531 and functional assessment, 557, 558
assessment, 531 measurement, 558, 559
positive responses, 532 Target maladaptive behaviors, 297
scripted statements, 532 Targeted interaction skills, 258
Socially mediated reinforcement system, 520 Targeted social initiations, 532
Society’s beliefs, 105 Task acquisition and generalization, 521
Sociology, 157 Teacher-delivered condition, 522
Soft skills, 129, 130, 136 Teacher-managed rewards systems, 520
Son-Rise program, 107 Teaching communication skills, 453
Son-Rise therapy’s website, 107 Teaching interaction procedure (TIP), 5, 135, 180, 278
Sources of knowledge, 157 ASD, 255
Special education, 126 benefits, 261
Special education teacher, 139 clinical practice, 262
Speech and language pathologists, 133 component analysis, 261
Speech interventions, 133 components
Speech pathologist, 126 demonstration, 254
Speech pathology, 320 description, 253, 254
Speech therapy, 104, 126 feedback, 252, 254, 255
Speech-generating devices (SGDs), 369, 381 instructor, 252
Speech-language pathologist (SLP), 84, 139 label/identify skill, 252, 253
Speech-language pathology, 137, 142 learner, 252
Staff training and education, 134 learner role-plays, 254
Standard notebook schedule, 515 rationales, 253
632 Index

Teaching interaction procedure (TIP) (cont.) Text and alphabet-based systems, 381
considerations, 259–260 advantages, 381
conversation skills, 252 disadvantages, 381
evaluation, 261 Textual scripts, 523, 524, 528
evidence-based interventions, 252, 260 Textual stimuli, 523, 526
features, 252 Theoretical beliefs, 103
flexible teaching, 262 Theory of Mind (ToM), 274–276
future research, 262 Therapeutic touch, 107
individuals diagnosed with ASD, 252 Therapist-assisted telehealth programs, 302
in-the-moment teaching, 261 Third ways, 157
measure learner competency, 252 Three-part contingency, 333
recommendation, 261 Time-out procedures
social skills, 262 acceptability, 491
social validity measures, 261 advantages
teaching social skills, 262 arrangements, 489
usual sequence, 253 quick and effective, 489
Teaching picture-object correspondence, 524 social validity, 489
Teaching safety skills, 258 behavior change tactics, 479
Teaching schedule-following behavior, 519 cultural and demographic variables, 492–493
Teaching skills, 259, 260 disadvantages
Teaching social skills, 267 misapplication of procedures, 490
BST (see Behavioral skills training (BST)) negative side effects, 490
natural environment, 262 potential for misuse, 489
TIP (see Teaching interaction procedure (TIP)) potential loss of instructional time, 490
Teaching specific grammatical responses, 532 early basic research, 482
Teaching strategy effectiveness, 532 effectiveness, 488
Tech industry, 156 exclusionary time-out, 487
Technical instructions, 131 inclusionary time-out, 486–487
Technical terminology, 130–132 long-term effects, 488
Technological devices, 513 nonexclusionary, 479
Technology-based activity schedules, 515 reinforcing environment, 490
Telehealth seclusion time-out, 487–488
barriers, 304 Token, 520
COVID-19 global pandemic, 304 Token economy
developmental and behavioral domains, 307 accuracy, 502, 503
EBP, 304 analysis of behavior (ABA), 497
effectiveness, 302 behavior management interventions, 508
effectiveness and feasibility, 304 behavior modification, 497
family characteristics, 304 behavior theory, 503
health care, 302 behavioral interventions, 497
parent implementation interventions, 302 continuous data collection methods, 503
parent’s training and coaching, 302 developing objectives, 502
parent-mediated interventions (see Telehealth-­ early childhood autism, 497
delivered parent-mediated interventions) educational environments, 504
parent-training interventions (see Telehealth-­ environments, 503
delivered parent-training interventions) ethics, 497
public health significance, 305 APA Commission, 498
systematic reviews, 304 behavior analysis community, 500
Telehealth-delivered parent-mediated interventions behavior modifiers, 498
ESDM, 302 behavioral interventions, 499
Map4speech, 303 behavior-influencing procedure, 498
Project ImPACT, 302, 303 human services community, 500
PRT, 302 moral and legal grounds, 499
strategies, 303 motivational operation, 498
Telehealth-delivered parent-training interventions point systems, 500, 501
FCT, 303 star charts, 500, 501
PCIT, 303, 304 Sunland Miami program, 498
RUBI program, 303 superb behavior modification program, 498
Telepathy, 31 supervision, 500
Testable statement, 25 training, 499, 500
Index 633

exchange rates, 506, 507 V


exchange-production schedule, 506, 507 Valuing, 28
features, 508 Variable-interval (VI) schedules, 506
generalized conditioned reinforcers, 504, 505 Variable-ratio (VR), 506, 521
intervention, 507 Varied instructions, 175
key elements, 497, 501 Verbal behavior, 53, 56, 367
physical features, 504 Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment
procedures, 507 and Placement Program (VB-MAPP),
reliability, 502, 503 63, 64
response-based exchanges, 506 Video modeling (VM), ASD
schedules of reinforcement, 505, 506 advantages, 227
select relevant target responses, 502 caregiver implementation, 241
stakeholders, 503 comparative research, 242
stockpile backup reinforcements, 504 future research, 244, 245
subjective/retrospective measures, 503 instructional procedures, 244
training, 497, 501 model type, 243
Token reinforcement, 483 video modeling and video prompting, 244
Token systems, 180 video prompting, 243
Tolerance skills, 453 functional living skills, 238
Tomatis Method, 34 future research, 239
Toy rotation system, 195 generalization measures, 239
Traditional functional analysis methods, 445, 454, 461 procedural variations of, 238, 239
Training, 604, 608 reliability, 239
Training and educating behavior analysts, 134, 135 play
Training practitioners, 412 diverse range of participants, 229
Training professionals, 135 future research, 232
Transdiagnostic behavioral approaches, 301 generalization and maintenance, 231
Transdiagnostic interventions, 301 reliability, 231, 232
Transdisciplinary collaboration, 127 social validity, 231
Transdisciplinary team approach, 321 targeting play, 230
Transdisciplinary training, 127 types, 229, 230
Transformation Module, 343 problem behaviors, 240
Transitivity, 341 future research, 241
Traumatic brain injury (TBI), 376 prerequisite skills, 241, 242
Treatment as usual (TAU), 30 procedural variations, 240
Treatment decisions, 20 safety skill
Treatment effectiveness, 112 characteristic features, 234
Treatment objective, 25 future research, 238
Treatment packages, 17 generalization and maintenance, 237
Treatment planning collaboration, 134 procedural variations of, 234–236
Treatment providers, 117 social validity, 237
Treatment utility, 452 social communication, 232
Trial-and-error condition, 518 conversational speech, 232
Trial-and-error procedures, 518 future research, 233, 234
Trial-based IISCA, 456–458 generalization and maintenance, 233
Turf wars, 136 procedural variations of, 232, 233
Video Modeling Imitation Training (VMIT), 241
Video prompting, 243
U Virtue theory, 54
Unaided symbols, 377 Virtuous behavior, 54
Unblinded clinician ratings, 37 Visual activity schedules (VAS), 513, 521
Uncompassionate behavior, 132 Visual interpretation, 109
Uncontrolled pilot studies, 35 Visual scripts, 523
Unhealthy weight, 126 Vitamins and dietary supplements, 36
Unscripted initiations, 530, 531 Vocal imitation responses, 524
Untargeted stereotypy Vocal responses, 526, 531
response interruption and redirection, 566 Vocal stereotypy, 113, 483, 484
US Food and Drug Administration, 35 Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development, 332
634 Index

W practitioner’s role, 155


Wakefield’s exact theory, 37 quality of life, 156, 157
Well established treatments, 26 third way, 157
Western context, 157 Workplace Training Programme, 275
What Works Clearinghouse (WWC), 14, 19, 589 Written scripts, 524, 525
Wisdom-based culture
develop skills and knowledge, 156
disadvantage, 156 Y
humility, 157–159 Yellow light treatments, 143
knowledge groups, 156
knowledge-intensive firms, 156
learning (see Learning) Z
learning histories, 156 Zone of proximal development, 318, 331
perspective taking and making, 156

You might also like