You are on page 1of 33

Changing the World and Changing the Self:

A Two-Process Model of Perceived Control


Fred Rothbaum John R. Weisz
Tufts University University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Samuel S. Snyder
North Carolina State University

There is extensive evidence that people strongly value and are reluctant to re-
linquish the perception of control. Yet, both helplessness and locus of control
theorists interpret various "inward" behaviors (passivity, withdrawal, and sub-
missiveness) as signs of relinquished perceived control. As evidence, they note
that inward behavior frequently is accompanied by causal attributions to limited
ability, chance, and powerful others—all attributions that suggest uncontrolla-
bility. In contrast, we claim,that these attributions and the behaviors to which
they relate may often reflect a type of perceived control that is generally over-
looked. People attempt to gain control not only by bringing the environment into
line with their wishes (primary control) but also by bringing themselves into line
with environmental forces (secondary control). Four manifestations of secondary
control are considered: (a) Attributions to severely, limited ability can serve to
enhance predictive control and to protect against disappointment. Passive and
withdrawn behaviors reflect the attempt to inhibit unfulfillable expectations, (b)
Attributions to chance can reflect illusory control, since people often construe
chance as a personal characteristic akin to an ability ("luck"). Individuals who
make attributions to chance may exhibit passivity and withdrawal in skill situ-
ations, reserving energy and emotional investment for situations that allow them
to capitalize on their perceived strength—that is, being lucky, (c) Attributions
to powerful others permit vicarious control when the individual identifies with
these others. Submission to a powerful leader, a group, or a deity sometimes
enables the individual to join in their power, (d) All of the preceding attributions
may foster interpretive control, in which the individual seeks to understand and
derive meaning from otherwise uncontrollable events in order to accept them.
When perceived control is recognized in both its primary and secondary forms,
a broad range of inward behaviors can be seen as efforts to sustain rather than
relinquish the perception of control.

Factor analytic studies with children (re- set of interrelated problem behaviors includ-
viewed in Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1978) ing passivity, withdrawal, and submissive-
and systematic observations of adults (e.g., ness (hereafter referred to as inward be-
Beck, 1967; Marks, 1977) have identified a havior). The popularity of the learned
helplessness model (Abramson, Seligman,
This article was funded in part by National Institute
& Teasdale, 1978; Seligman, 1975) is due
of Mental Health Grant 1-RO1-MH-34210-01. largely to its lucid account of at least some
The authors are indebted to Camille Wortman for of these seemingly maladaptive behaviors
her unusually thorough review of a draft of this article. (especially passivity and withdrawal). Ad-
Her comments added depth and clarity to many of the herents of this model maintain that percep-
Central points.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Fred Roth- tions of uncontrollability are frequent an-
baum, Department of Child Study, Tufts University, tecedents of inward behavior. In support of
Medford, Massachusetts 02155. this view, a number of studies indicate that
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1982, Vol. 42, No. 1, 5-37
Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/82/4201-0005$00.75
F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

experiences designed to induce perceptions to control stimulation. Theorists from a wide


of uncontrollability lead to decreased learn- variety of perspectives have been emphatic
ing, decreased persistence, and depressed about the importance of control: Adler
affect—key components of inward behavior. (1956) depicted control as an intrinsic "ne-
The locus of control literature also seems cessity of life"; Malinowski (1955) noted
to link perceived uncontrollability and in- among primitives a "desperate" need to be-
ward behavior. Persons with an external lo- lieve they could master the world; White
cus of control—those who see circumstances, (1959), in a now classic review of the liter-
chance, and powerful others as controlling ature on "effectance motivation," refers to
outcomes—are, in general, more likely to a pervasive, intrinsic need to exercise control
manifest such inward behaviors as passivity, over the environment; DeCharms (1968)
withdrawal, compliance, conformity, and argued that the urge to be effective in chang-
depressive symptomatology than are persons ing the environment is "man's primary mo-
with an internal locus of control—those who tivational propensity." Evidence of motiva-
see their own effort or ability as controlling tion for perceived control can be found in a
outcomes (for reviews see Lefcourt, 1976, wide variety of research areas. It is promi-
1980; Phares, 1976; Rothbaum, Wolfer, & nent, for example, in research on (a) help-
Visintainer, 1979).1 Also relevant here are lessness and locus of control (briefly re-
studies in which perceptions of uncontrol- viewed in this article), (b) perceived "con-
lability have been manipulated and locus of tingency" in infancy (e.g., Finkelstein &
control has been assessed (Cohen, Rothbart, Ramey, 1977; Gunnar-Vongnechten, 1978;
& Phillips, 1976; Diener & Dweck, 1978; Watson, 1967), (c) effectance and mastery
Dweck, 1975; Gregory, Chartier, & Wright, motivation in childhood (reviewed by Harter,
1979; Hiroto, 1974; Pittman & Pittman, 1978), (d) intrinsic motivation, both in chil-
1979). While the findings entail complexities dren (e.g., Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973)
which will be dealt with later in the article, and adults (Deci, 1975), (e) the self-serving
in general they indicate that externality and bias in children (Weisz, 1980, 1981) and
manipulations designed to decrease per- adults (Snyder, Stephan, & Rosenfield, 1978;
ceived control are both associated with in- Zuckerman, 1979), (f) feelings of compe-
ward behavior (also see Coyne, Metalsky, tence in the elderly (Langer & Rodin, 1976;
& Lavelle, 1980; Janoff-Bulman & Brick- Schultz, 1980), and (g) the attractiveness
man, 1980). Because of their considerable and adaptive value of different types of per-
overlap in this respect, the helplessness and ceived control, such as predictability (Av-
locus of control positions are referred to col-
lectively as the uncontrollability model of
inward behavior. 1
In addition to their focus on perceptions Rotter (1966), in original formulation of the locus
of control construct, defined internality in terms of both
of control, uncontrollability theorists also self-contingency and self-causality. This definition leads
emphasize the motivation for perceived con- to confusion in the case of certain attributions, such as
trol. The general assumption, supported by attributions to limited ability, which imply an absence
research, is that perceptions of uncontrol- of self-contingency (independence between the self's re-
sponse and the environmental outcomes) but the pres-
lability and decreased motivation for control ence of self-causality (the self s inability is the ascribed
fuel one another in a downward spiral cause of the environmental outcome). In this article we
(DuCette & Wolk, 1973; Roller & Kaplan, equate internality with contingency and externality with
1978). The resulting inward behavior is seen, noncontingency. This position conflicts with the position
in part, as a motivational deficit, that is, as of the locus of control theorists who depict limited ability
as an internal attribution (e.g., Abramson et al., 1978;
a consequence of relinquishing the motiva- Weiner, 1974) but is consistent with the position of other
tion for control (Seligman, 1975). locus of control theorists who see limited ability attri-
Uncontrollability theorists' emphasis on butions as contributing to the belief that effort does not
the motivation for perceived control has make a difference—a belief that exemplifies externality
(e.g., Cohen, Rothbart, & Phillips, 1976; Diener &
many roots in earlier theorizing, dating at Dweck, 1978; Lefcourt, 1976). For a fuller discussion
least from Groos's (1901) observation that of this topic, see Janoff-Bulman (1979) and Rothbaum
children derive pleasure from opportunities (1981).
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL

erill, 1973; Burger & Arkin, 1980; Lefcourt, speculate about deeper motives and to label
1973).2 the behavior "neurotic" (e.g., Kessler, 1966;
Motivation for perceived control and its Wolman, 1965). Such persistence suggests
corollary, aversiveness of perceived uncon- that the behavior does satisfy some under-
trollability, are cornerstones of the uncon- lying purpose and that it does not merely
trollability formulation (Lefcourt, 1976; Se- reflect a "deficit" in motivation .
ligman, 1975). Yet ironically, uncontrol- These phenomena, we suspect, can best be
lability theorists may be shortchanging the understood by broadening our conception of
generality and potency of this motive. In this control and how it is manifest in everyday
article we argue that the motivation to feel life. Simply stated, our proposal is this: Be-
"in control" may be expressed not only in cause control is so valued, the quest for it
behavior that is blatantly controlling but is rarely abandoned; instead, individuals are
also, subtly, in behavior that is not. In some likely to shift from one method of striving
cases, inward behavior may reflect a relin- for control to another. The uncontrollability
quishing of the powerful motive for per- model and the present formulation, then,
ceived control. In other cases, however, such disagree as to how frequently motivation for
behavior may be initiated and maintained control is abandoned. This disagreement
in an effort to sustain perceptions of control. goes hand in hand with a related disagree-
This effort is particularly likely when the ment about the pervasiveness of perceived
inward behavior helps prevent disappoint- uncontrollability. Many situations that ac-
ment, when it leads to a perception of align- cording to uncontrollability theorists indi-
ment with forces such as chance or powerful cate a perception of uncontrollability are
others, and when it is accompanied by at- situations in which we believe perceived con-
tempts to derive meaning from a situation. trol is actually a major dynamic (cf. Janoff-
The uncontrollability model does not explain Bulman & Brickman, 1980). In large part,
any of these phenomena. these disagreements regarding motivation
A related problem is that the uncontroll- and perception hinge on different concept-
ability model does not account for the per- ualizations of control. A reasonable next
sistence of some inward behavior. This per- step, then, is to contrast uncontrollability
sistence is evident in research on depressives' theorists' definition of control with the def-
negative response set (Beck, 1967) and in inition we propose.
research on obsessive compulsives' persev-
erative thoughts and actions (Rackman & Definitions of Control
Hodgson, 1980). There is even evidence of Helplessness and locus of control theorists
persistence in the research on helplessness. conceptualize perceived control similarly.
For example: dogs exposed to uncontrollable Both place a major emphasis on the contin-
shock subsequently have to be dragged into gency between action and outcome (Rotter,
a safety area (Seligman, 1975); individuals 1966; Seligman, 1975). Related constructs
subjected to uncontrollable social situations such as perceived choice and competence
manifest increased perseverative behavior also figure prominently in both helplessness
(Goetz & Dweck, 1980); and individuals theory (e.g., Langer & Benevento, 1978)
exposed, to unsolvable tasks go out of their
way to find reasons why they cannot succeed
2
(Diener & Dweck, 1978; Seligman, 1975). Recently, some authors claimed that the motivation
These findings, and others reviewed later in for control is not as pervasive as has typically been as-
sumed (Brehm & Brehm, 1981; Rodin, Rennert, &
the article, indicate that persons sometimes Solomon, 1980). They take issue with the notion of a
actively undermine their own chances of suc- generalized motivation to seek control and with the no-
cess. In all of these cases, inward behavior tion that control is always desirable. As already noted,
persists well beyond the time during which we believe there is considerable formal and informal
it is adaptive by rational standards, and it evidence of a generalized motivation to seek control, but
we also recognize that much more research is needed
persists despite attempts by others to alter to resolve this matter. Later in this article we briefly
it. It is largely this quality of persistence that consider the evidence indicating that control sometimes
prompts analytically oriented theorists to appears undesirable (see pp. 15-16).
F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

and locus of control theory (e.g., Lefcourt, extreme cases of both assimilation and pri-
1976; Rotter, 1966; see also Weisz & Stipek, mary control, the individual, in a sense,
in press). In each case the individual's ability places his or her desires above the demands
to change the environment to fit the self s of reality. In complementary fashion, ac-
needs is emphasized. Seligman's definition commodation is the tendency to modify one's
of contingency, for example, makes it clear cognitive structures in an attempt to effect
that the individual's actions must lead to a better fit with reality. As is true of sec-
change in the environment. Even White, who ondary control, in extreme accommodation
has identified myriad expressions of control, the demands of reality overwhelm the self s
emphasizes throughout his formulation the desires (cf. Lazarus & Launier, 1978, p.
individual's ability to affect the environ- 314). Averill (1973) highlights this accom-
ment. modative process when he notes that control
Unlike most uncontrollability theorists, increases to the extent that the individual is
we conceive of control as a two-process able to "accommodate to necessity" (cf.
rather than a one-process construct. The first Chein, 1972; Kelly, 1955). Piaget and other
process involves attempts to change the cognitive developmental theorists argue that
world so that it fits the self s needs. When the goal of development is equilibration—
this process is salient, we use the expression the coordination of assimilation and accom-
primary control. It is our belief that uncon- modation—so as to optimize the individual's
trollability theorists have equated control adaptation to the environment. Similarly, in
with primary control and have all but ig- the model proposed here, optimal adaptation
nored a second process we wish to high- is defined as the coordination of primary and
light—attempts to fit in with the world and secondary control.
to "flow with the current." We refer to this The terms primary and secondary are
process as secondary, and when it is salient, adopted for several reasons. First, they re-
we employ the expression secondary control. flect the fact that one process (primary) has
The attempt to change things that offer generally received more attention than the
resistance, which characterizes primary con- other (secondary). Second, the terms distin-
trol, is apt to produce some satisfying suc- guish between the presence and absence of
cesses but also some disappointing failures. more powerful controlling agents: If the self
The attempt to adjust to resistances, which is the most powerful agent, then control is
characterizes secondary control, is apt to be primary; if more powerful agents are ac-
experienced as safer and to lead to less ex- knowledged (e.g., a task that one's sev-
treme highs and lows. Neither process is erely limited ability makes insurmountable,
thought to exist in pure form; often both chance, or a powerful other person), the
processes are intertwined, as when persons self s control is secondary. Another virtue of
negotiate and compromise (Lazarus, 1981). using thb terms primary and secondary is
Often there is vacillation between the two that they capture differences in the temporal
processes, as when persons shift between sequence of the control processes they delin-
phases of encounter and retreat in coping eate. Secondary control is most likely to oc-
with a physical disability (Shontz, 1975; cur after attempts at primary control have
Silver & Wortman, 1980). Thus, the differ- failed. Naturalistic evidence on the temporal
ences between primary and secondary con- sequence of the two types of control indicates
trol should be thought of as differences in that behaviors reflecting secondary control
emphasis. (passivity, withdrawal, and submissiveness)
A rough analogy can be drawn between are a late stage of reaction to stress; typically
this primary/secondary relationship and the they are preceded by a stage of anger and
complementary processes of assimilation and protest—behaviors that we associate with
accommodation as described by Piaget primary control (for evidence regarding
(1970) in his theory of cognitive develop- stages see Baum, Aiello, & Calesnick 1978;
ment. Assimilation denotes the tendency to Klinger, 1975; Mann & Dashiell, 1975;
perceive the environment in ways consistent Rothbaum, 1980; Wortman & Brehm, 1975).
with one's existing cognitive structures. In However, the evidence is far from clear-cut
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL

(Silver & Wortman, 1980), and we suspect lability on this evidence alone. More com-
that vacillation between primary and sec- pelling, they believe, are the causal at-
ondary control is common. tributions that are frequently found in
The primary/secondary distinction we conjunction with these inward behaviors.
propose resembles in certain respects the Both helplessness theorists (Hiroto & Selig-
offensive/defensive distinction frequently man, 1975; Klein & Seligman, 1976; Miller
employed in the psychological literature. & Seligman, 1975) and locus of control the-
One problem with the latter is that it has orists (Lefcourt, 1976; Phares, 1976; Rotter,
been used to refer to such a broad variety 1966) claim that the tendency to attribute
of phenomena that its meaningfulness is di- failure to severe limitations in ability,3 to
minished. Secondary control differs from chance, and to powerful others relates to the
defensiveness in that it does not refer to all tendency to engage in inward behavior (cf.
attempts to neutralize instinctual urges (as Wortman & Brehm, 1975), especially when
does use of the term defensive in psycho- the attributions are global and stable
analytic writings), and it does not refer to (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978).
attempts to maintain self-esteem by forget- Because these theorists equate control with
ting or denying the occurrence of failure (as the ability to change the environment, they
does use of the term defensive in attribution maintain that these attributions reflect per-
theory literature). In fact, we would gener- ceived uncontrollability.
ally regard the protection of self-esteem Probably the most widely cited evidence
through denial as an attempt to maintain of perceived uncontrollability is that the
belief in the eventual effectiveness of pri- above attributions and inward behaviors
mary control—the ability to change the en- both seem to result from manipulations de-
vironment. signed to induce perceived uncontrollability.
The demonstration of these causal connec-
Proof of Uncontfollability tions is a major objective of helplessness re-
search. Manipulations such as noncontin-
Differences between our definition of con- gent aversive events (e.g., loud noise, electric
trol and that advocated by uncontrollability shock) and unsolvable problems have re-
theorists lead to differences in the criteria peatedly been associated with the above at-
used to infer uncontrollability. In this section tributions. Zuckerman (1979), for example,
we attempt to explain these differences. found that failure experiences led to an in-
creased occurrence of external attributions
in 27 of the 38 studies he reviewed. Simi-
The Uncontrollability Model larly, persons high in externality on locus of
control measures tend to have socialization
Uncontrollability theorists rely on several histories indicating repeated experiences of
kinds of information in deciding whether or failure (Lefcourt, 1976; Crandall, Note 1).
not a given situation is perceived as uncon- Manipulations of uncontrollability have
trollable. First, the individual's behavior it- been shown to lead to inward behavior, par-
self may indicate perceived uncontrollabil- ticularly passivity (e.g., Benson & Kennelly,
ity. Passive, withdrawn, and to a lesser 1976; Cohen, Rothbart, & Phillips, 1976;
extent, submissive behaviors often do not Glass & Singer, 1972; Griffith, 1977; Hir-
serve instrumental ends (e.g., to obtain a
desired goal, solve a problem, or terminate
an aversive event). In such cases, uncon- 3
Although locus of control theorists refer to attri-
trollability theorists typically infer an aban- butions to circumstances rather than to attributions to
doning of attempts to control. They are par- limited ability, the major circumstance they consider is
ticularly likely to draw this inference when task difficulty. Thus, there is substantial overlap in these
there is a cluster of the above behaviors. formulations. The qualifier "severe" is added because
of findings that attributions to slight to moderate lim-
However, uncontrollability theorists recog- itations in ability lead to noninward behavior (Pittman
nize a variety of exceptions to this rule, and & Pittman, 1979; Roth & Kubal, 1975; Tennen & Eller,
they do not hinge their proof of uncontrol- 1977).
10 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

oto, 1974; Hiroto & Seligman, 1975; Roth ure experiences (Floor & Rosen, 1975;
& Kubal, 1975; Tennen & Eller, 1977; Weisz, 1979, in press; Yando & Zigler,
Thornton & Jacobs, 1971; for a review see 1971).
Roth, 1980). There is also evidence that To summarize, uncontrollability theorists
withdrawn and submissive behaviors are as- rely on several types of evidence in substan-
sociated with the aforementioned attribu- tiating their claims: (a) The inward behav-
tions and manipulations. Because previous iors in and of themselves suggest perceived
reviews of the uncontrollability model have uncontrollability, especially when they co-
given less attention to the latter behaviors, occur, (b) These behaviors are associated
a brief consideration of them is appropriate with causal attributions indicating perceived
here. uncontrollability. (c) Both the behaviors and
Most uncontrollability theorists would the attributions have been shown to result
probably regard withdrawal as a manifes- from experiences designed to induce per-
tation of perceived uncontrollability (e.g., ceived uncontrollability. Although uncon-
Goetz & Dweck, 1980). As Carver (1979) trollability theorists to date have focused
suggested, the "impulse to withdraw is basic more on passivity than on withdrawal and
to a wide variety of helplessness effects" (p. submissiveness, the evidence reviewed here
1276). However, Carver also noted that demonstrates that the model has been used
many helplessness paradigms have failed to to explain the latter behaviors as well.
include opportunities for physical with-
drawal. When physical withdrawal is pre- A Two-Process Model
vented, he argued, subjects may engage in
cognitive withdrawal—a mental dissociation The uncontrollability model and the po-
from the task. The decreased learning found sition we propose lead to different criteria
in many helplessness studies could be re- of uncontrollability. We claim that attempts
garded as evidence of this cognitive with- to fit more effectively with the world con-
drawal (Carver, 1979). stitute a process of secondary control; if we
Submissiveness is another behavior that are right, attributions and behaviors cited
has been linked to perceived uncontrollabil- above do not necessarily provide proof of
ity. Locus of control theorists explicitly draw perceived uncontrollability. There is a defi-
connections between submissiveness and per- nitional difference here, but its importance
ceived uncontrollability (Lefcourt, 1976). should not be exaggerated. Whether one
Although helplessness theorists have failed chooses the term control when the secondary
thus far to elaborate this connection, the process is salient is not crucial. Rather, at
helplessness model is relatively new and open issue is how the attributions and behaviors
to extension in this direction. Seligman reviewed above may be most usefully ex-
(1975) seemed favorably inclined toward plained. We maintain that underlying these
just such an elaboration of the helplessness attributions and behaviors is a powerful mo-
model when he reported anecdotal evidence tivation to fit in with the environment. In
of a connection between helplessness and contrast, uncontrollability theorists high-
submissiveness (p. 25). In at least one study, light the absence of attempts to change the
helplessness was operationalized as a rela- world and the absence of motivation for con-
tively submissive "going to get the experi- trol (cf. Wortman & Brehm, 1975). This is
menter" as opposed to taking more indepen- the point of contention.
dent action (Roth & Bootzin, 1974). Findings In four subsequent sections we review re-
from this study indicated greater submis- search involving causal attributions to se-
siveness following manipulations of uncon- verely limited ability, to chance, and to pow-
trollability (see also Langer & Benevento, erful others, along with the respective inward
1978). A link between submissiveness, help- behaviors to which they frequently relate.
less behavior, and perceived uncontrollabil- These combinations of attribution and be-
ity has also been noted in studies involving havior, we argue, often reflect a perception
retarded children exposed to recurring fail- of secondary control. To support our two-
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 11

process model, we will attempt to show that in order to enhance a sense of close associ-
these attributions and behaviors are accom- ation with a more powerful entity.
panied by forms of persistence (e.g., under- Finally, all of the above attributions en-
mining success, increasing effort on chance able the self to derive meaning from other-
tasks, seeking out powerful others, and in- wise uncontrollable experiences. We refer to
creased attributional activity) that indicate the ability to interpret events so as to better
a striving for secondary control. To intro- understand and accept them as interpretive
duce the two-process model, we first provide control (Averill, 1973; Frankl, 1963; Gar-
a brief overview. ber, Miller, & Abramson, 1980).
Attributions to severely limited ability can The predictive, illusory, vicarious, and in-
afford the individual control in the form of terpretive control described above are all in-
making the best of a situation that is per- stances of what we label secondary control.
ceived as difficult or impossible to alter. This In each case, a tendency to change the self
control stems from the ability to predict the so as to fit more effectively with the envi-
uncontrollable outcome (or its recurrence) ronment overshadows a tendency to change
and from the adjusting of the self s expec- the environment so that it fits the self s
tations accordingly. Adjustment of expec- needs. We believe that failure to consider
tations is particularly important because un- these and other instances of secondary con-
fulfilled expectations undermine perceived trol underlies uncontrollability theorists'
control; obversely, fulfilled expectations mistaken conclusion that perceived uncon-
heighten perceived control. Because Averill trollability and abandoning the motive to
(1973), Burgess and Holmstrom (1979), control are so prevalent.
Lazarus (1966), Miller and Grant (1980), Although uncontrollability theorists might
and other theorists concerned with this kind acknowledge the existence of predictive, il-
of control all emphasize predictability, we lusory, vicarious, and interpretive control,
refer to it as predictive control. they would probably emphasize their pri-
Attributions to chance permit what is re- mary aspects. Most likely, predictive control
ferred to here as illusory control. Persons would be defined as a belief in the ability to
sometimes attempt to associate themselves predict events and thereby plan a successful
with chance so as to share in the power of task outcome; illusory control would be de-
this larger force. Chance is viewed as a prop- fined as an irrational belief that persons can,
erty of the individual; often persons speak through effortful action, influence chance-
of luck as an ally that they can "rely on," determined (as well as skill-determined) out-
and of being "born lucky," seemingly re- comes (e.g., superstitious behavior through
garding luck as a type of control. Through which they erroneously believe their actions
various means—superstitious behavior, rit- determine chance outcomes); vicarious con-
uals, and, more simply, investing energy in trol would be defined as a belief that one can
chance-determined situations rather than in gain control through powerful others, either
skill-determined situations—people attempt by manipulating their actions or by imitating
to align themselves with chance. them; and interpretive control would be de-
Attributions to powerful others are similar fined as a belief that one can find the mean-
to attributions to chance in that they permit ing of a problem and thereby solve it. (See
association with a more powerful force. Al- Table 1 for a summary of the primary and
though such vicarious control may some- secondary manifestations of predictive, il-
times serve instrumental ends, it often ap- lusory, vicarious, and interpretive control.)
pears to function as an end in its own right. The key difference between primary and sec-
That is, persons sometimes associate with ondary control is that in the former case the
others simply for the sake of sharing psy- goal is to change the environment whereas
chologically in the others' control. Vicarious in the latter case the goal is to fit in with the
control, then, is similar to the phenomena environment. The above instances of pri-
of identification and deindividuation, in mary control are dissimilar to instances of
which individuals submerge a sense of self secondary control in that they are unlikely
12 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

, to accompany attributions to severely lim- three factors will be treated in each section.
ited ability, to chance, or to powerful others To support our claims we attempt to provide
and they are unlikely to accompany nonin- behavioral and self-report evidence of striv-
strumental behaviors of a passive, with- ing for secondary control in situations in
drawn, or submissive kind. which persons (a) manifest passive, with-
In the following four sections, we attempt drawn, and submissive behavior; (b) attrib-
to show that many situations previously ute outcomes to severely limited ability,
thought to involve perceived uncontrollabil- chance, or powerful others; and (c) have just
ity are ones in which persons strive for sec- experienced manipulations designed to in-
ondary control. These sections are organized crease perceived uncontrollability.
by the four kinds of secondary control just
reviewed—predictive, illusory, vicarious, and Predictive Control
interpretive. Because we agree with uncon-
trollability theorists that consideration of When an attribution to severely limited
behaviors, attributions, and precipitating ability occurs in conjunction with passive of
events is essential, evidence regarding all withdrawn behavior, and when the situation

Table 1
Primary and Secondary Processes
Type of Salient
control process Description

Predictive Primary Attempt to predict events so as to succeed at them.


Active behavior and attributions to the self s effort
and ability are likely, especially in tasks of moderate
difficulty.
Secondary Attempt to predict events so as to avoid disappointment.
Passive and withdrawn behavior, especially in tasks of
moderate difficulty. Active, persistent behavior in
extremely easy or difficult situations. Attributions to
severely limited ability are likely.
Illusory Primary Attempt to influence chance-determined outcomes.
Active behavior in chance and skill situations and
attributions to the self s effort and ability are likely.
Secondary Attempt to associate with chance. Active behavior in
chance situations but passive and withdrawn behavior
in skill situations and attributions to chance are likely.
Vicarious Primary Attempt to manipulate powerful others or imitate their
power or ability. Instrumental submissive behavior or
manipulative behavior and attributions to the self s
effort and ability are likely.
Secondary Attempt to associate with powerful others.
Noninstrumental, submissive behavior and attributions
to others are likely.
Interpretive Primary Attempt to understand problems so as to be able to
solve them or otherwise master them. Active behavior
and attributions to the self s effort and ability are
likely.
Secondary Attempt to understand problems so as to derive meaning
from them and to accept them. Passive, withdrawn,
and submissive behavior and attributions to severely
limited ability, chance, and powerful others are likely.
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 13

is one in which effort would likely produce ceived to increase the probability of unful-
a desired outcome, uncontrollability theo- fillable expectations and thus of eventual
rists typically conclude that the individual disappointment. To test this notion, Mettee
has given up—has abandoned the attempt exposed subjects first to failure, then to suc-
to exercise control. In contrast, we believe cess, and finally gave them the opportunity
that in certain situations the combination of to accept or reject the success—that is, he
an attribution to severely limited ability and readministered the test at which the subjects
passive or withdrawn behavior allows for had just succeeded, thus providing them the
predictive control. In the present context, the opportunity to change their correct answers.
expression predictive control refers to the Between failure and success experiences,
ability to predict aversive events to avoid some subjects were informed that an inter-
disappointment (cf. Averill, 1973; Lazarus, view very likely confirming their initial fail-
1966; Miller & Grant, 1980). ing performance would occur at the conclu-
Individuals who have repeatedly experi- sion of the study; other subjects were assured
enced failure sometimes avoid tasks com- that no further information was forthcom-
pletely or participate with minimal effort, ing. As predicted, the former group, for
even when they realize that they have a good whom acceptance of success implied a greater
chance of success, In justifying their seem- vulnerability to disconfirmed expectations^
ingly self-defeating behavior, these individ- was more rejecting of the success experience.
uals often maintain that their ability to avoid To create a third condition, some of the sub-
disappointment is their last line of defense. jects in the "future jnterview" condition
The av6rsiveness of disappointment was viv- were told that the forthcoming feedback
idly demonstrated on a national scale in the would inevitably confirm their prior failure.
United States during the Iranian hostage Mettee hypothesized that when future fail-
crisis of 1980-1981. Many Americans would ure was assured, disappointment would not
have echoed the feelings of a parent of one be possible and that, as a consequence, sub-
of the hostages: "We've been disappointed jects would accept success (i.e., not change
so many times. Now I won't allow myself their correct answers). His findings sup-
to believe in the eventual success of the ne- ported this hypothesis. Subjects accepted
gotiations until I see them [the hostages] success in this condition, reasoned Mettee,
step off the plane." Disappointment is so because it was irrelevant to their future ex-
aversive, we believe, because it adds the in- pectations. Subsequent studies in which dif-
sult of secondary uncontrollability (not being ferent manipulations were employed to make
prepared for the outcome) to the injury of success irrelevant have yielded similar find-
primary uncontrollability (not being able to ings (Brickman, Linsenmeier, & McCareins,
alter the outcome). We believe that individ- 1976; Maracek & Mettee, 1972).
uals actively strivfe to avoid disappointment Additional evidence for the view that per-
and that a major objective in doing so is to sons actively strive to avoid disappointment
gain an increased sense of secondary control. comes from Wortman, Costanzo, and Witt
To support our claim, we will provide evi- (1973), who gave subjects a "social percep-
dence that individuals exert effort and make tiveness task" with predetermined feedback.
sacrifices to avoid disappointment and that Regardless of whether feedback was positive
their objective is an increased sense of ef- or negative, subjects who thought they might
fectiveness. We will rely primarily on three be tested further were less likely to perceive
bodies of literature: research directly con- themselves as capable than were subjects led
cerned with disappointment, research on to believe there would be no further testing.
achievement motivation, and research on re- One interpretation of these findings is that
actions to unpredictable stress. subjects wanted to mitigate the possibility
Mettee (1971) provided the clearest em- of disappointment in the new situation. At-
pirical support for the position that disap- tributions to severely limited ability in these
pointment is actively avoided. He argued cases offer a distinct advantage: They min-
that persons avoid success when it is per- imize the possibility of the kind of discon-
14 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

firmed expectation that is most disappoint- issue. In general, the findings indicate that
ing. To expect success but instead to fail is persons low in self-esteem and persons ex-
a double defeat; one has failed not only as posed to failure show low preference for and
a performer of the task but also as a pre- low effort on tasks that offer moderate
dictor of the outcome. chances of success but high preference for
In an earlier pair of studies similar to and high effort on extremely easy or ex-
Mettee's except that the possibility of sub- tremely difficult tasks. Before considering
sequent feedback was not mentioned, Aron- how avoidance of disappointment may use-
son and Carlsmith (1962) also found that fully explain these findings, a brief review
subjects undermine unexpected success. They of the relevant literature is in order.
interpret these results as evidence for a de- Atkinson's (1957, 1964) findings on
sire to avoid inconsistency. Although there achievement motivation are the best known
have been a few successful replications of in this area. He found an overall tendency
the above results (Brock, Edelman, Ed- for subjects to prefer moderately difficult
wards, & Schuck, 1965; Cottrell, 1965; Hai- tasks because they maximize the possibility
mowitz & Haimowitz, 1966), most of the of a meaningful success experience. Atkin-
replication attempts have not been support- son noted, however, that low self-esteem
ive (see Archibald, 1974, for a list of 10 such ("failure avoidant") persons tend to avoid
studies). Mettee's model provides an alter- moderate-level tasks (see also Kukla, 1974).
native explanation for both the effect and The low self-esteem person, Atkinson be-
its equivocal support: Subjects sometimes lieved, seeks to attenuate failure, and this
reject success in order to avoid disappoint- can best be accomplished by maximizing the
ment, but the degree to which success is re- probability of success (very easy tasks) or
jected depends upon the degree to which fac- by minimizing the shame of failure (very
tors contributing to fears of disappointment difficult tasks). Subsequent research by Kar-
(e.g., threats of later feedback) are present. abenick and Yousseff (1968) showed that
Other consistency theorists have shown persons chronically concerned about failure
that subjects with low self-esteem prefer perform better at tasks described as very
evaluators who provide mostly negative feed- difficult. Similarly, Shaban and Jecker
back to evaluators who provide mostly pos- (1968) showed that subjects exposed to a
itive feedback (e.g., Backman & Secord, failure manipulation prefer evaluators who
1959; Deutsh & Solomon, 1959). Here evaluate almost everyone positively or neg-
again we take issue with the desire-for-con- atively to more moderate evaluators (who
sistency explanation and instead suggest that evaluate about half of the people positively).
low self-esteem persons fear disappointment. Following in this line of research, Frankel
Consistent with our view, Jones's (1973) ex- and Snyder (1978) exposed subjects to un-
amination of the relevant literature suggests solvable problems and subsequently assessed
that subjects in these studies often expect to their passivity and withdrawal on either ex-
be given another test in the future. tremely difficult or moderately difficult tasks.
The reliance on avoidance of disappoint- According to Frankel and Snyder, helpless-
ment as a conscious strategy was investi- ness theorists maintain that passivity and
gated more directly in a study by Sidle, withdrawal result from perceived uncontroll-
Moos, Adams, and Cady (1969). Persons ability, and, by this logic, more of these be-
who reported that they expect the worst as haviors would be expected on the extremely
a means of coping tended to view themselves difficult tasks. Their own self-esteem/defen-
as passive, slow, changeable, and low in self- sive-effort model, in contrast, leads to the
esteem. These are precisely the perceptions conclusion that passivity and withdrawal re-
that have led previous researchers to infer flect an attempt to excuse failure and that
an absence of perceived control. the need for such excuses is greater with
A number of other findings, which do not moderately difficult tasks. Their results, in-
directly address the issue of disappointment, dicating that passivity and withdrawal were
can nonetheless be seen as relevant to this greatest in the moderately difficult task con-
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 15

dition, supported the self-esteem/defensive- quency and intensity of aversive events iden-
effort model. tical across conditions (for reviews of related
Thus, there have been numerous studies, studies see Averill, 1973; Burger & Arkin,
spanning over 20 years, indicating that per- 1980). Although aversiveness per se (inten-
sons with low self-esteem and persons ex- sity of noise) has some influence on perfor-
posed to failure tend to avoid tasks of mod- mance, its effect pales in comparison with
erate difficulty. Frankel and Snyder's that of unpredictability. A number of other
explanation that these subjects are seeking investigators also report that specific, accu-
an excuse for possible failure is an appealing rate expectations can reduce the adverse ef-
one (see Snyder & Wicklund, 1981, for an fects of stressful procedures (Epstein, 1973;
elaboration of this position). We believe, Janis, 1958; Johnson, 1973, 1975; Johnson
however, that avoidance of disappointment & Leventhal, 1974; Leventhal, Brown,
provides an equally appealing explanation. Shacham, & Engquist, 1979). In interpret-
Subjects are unlikely to be disappointed by ing some of these findings, Lefcourt (1973,
performance at very easy tasks because they 1976) pointed out that prediction allows the
will almost definitely succeed and are un- individual to steel him- or herself to the aver-
likely to be disappointed by performance at sive event and thus to not be caught off
very difficult tasks because their expecta- guard—that is, prediction protects against
tions of success are very low.4 Moreover, disappointment. In a similar vein, Miller
there are two pieces of evidence that cast (1980) espoused an "internality view of pre-
doubt on Frankel and Snyder's interpreta- diction" according to which preparing for
tion: (a) The subjects who exerted little ef- the worst enables the individual to "match
fort on the moderately difficult tasks scored his or her internal state with external events"
low in self-reports of esteem, (b) These sub- (cf. research on sensitization cited in Averill,
jects did not make a greater number of effort 1973; Janis's, 1958, research on the "work
attributions for their failure (e.g., "I didn't of worry"). These authors would probably
try"). Neither finding fits with Frankel and agree with our conclusion that predictability
Snyder's hypothesis; in contrast, both find- allows the individual to adjust to the uncon-
ings are consistent with our avoidance of trollable event, thus making it, in a second-
disappointment hypothesis. We believe that ary sense, "controllable."
the emphasis on the self-esteem/defensive- Also relevant here are studies showing
ness explanation reflects psychologists' per- that in some instances increased perception
sistent focus on people's desire for primary of opportunities for primary control can be
control (clinging to the belief that the self stress inducing. Stress occurs when the avail-
can, ultimately, change the environment) able methods of control are ineffective, for
and their perception that primary control is example, when responses intended to ter-
an unmitigated good (cf. Janoff-Bulman & minate shock yield inconsistent or negative
Brickman, 1980). Rather than clinging to feedback (Weiss, 1968; 1971a, 1971b,
a belief in primary control, we believe that 1971c). In other cases, in which subjects are
persons sometimes actively oppose such be-
liefs, especially when these beliefs are seen "Trope (1975) and Trope and Brickman (1975)
as opening the floodgates of disappointment found evidence indicating that the major virtue of mod-
and, thus, as jeopardizing secondary control. erately difficult tasks is that they are typically high in
There is other evidence of the aversiveness "diagnosticity" (i.e., they are especially able to distin-
of disappointment and of people's desire to guish the competent from the incompetent). These au-
thors suggested that very difficult tasks are appealing
avoid it through accurate prediction. Glass to failure-avoidant individuals because of their desire
and Singer (1972) showed that a predictable to avoid diagnosticity. This idea resembles our sugges-
(fixed interval) aversive event (loud noise) tion that very difficult tasks enable failure-avoidant in-
leads to fewer errors on a frustration task dividuals to avoid disappointment. Consistent with the
diagnosticity view as well as with ours is recent evidence
and fewer omissions on a task of caution and that persons made anxious about failure subsequently
attentiveness than does an unpredictable show less preference for highly diagnostic tasks (Schulz
(random) aversive event, even with fre- & Hanusa, Note 3).
16 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

given choices that do affect outcomes, but contain evidence of the existence of second-
they lack the information necessary to in- ary control in situations previously thought
crease the likelihood of a beneficial result, to entail perceived uncontrollability. Several
increased stress is also noted (Rodin, Ren- of the studies cited here (e.g., Frankel &
nert, & Solomon, 1980). In the latter cir- Snyder, 1978; Glass & Singer, 1972; Mettee,
cumstances, increased control (choice) also 1971) employed manipulations of uncon-
leads to decreased self-esteem. It is inter- trollability similar or identical to those em-
esting that when subjects are told that they ployed in helplessness studies. It is possible
are not expected to do well (a manipulation that some subjects who appear to be giving
that is likely to mitigate perceived disap- up are in fact abandoning control, whereas
pointment), the aversive effects of choice are other subjects who appear to be giving up
not obtained. We maintain that in both of are attempting to control disappointment.
the above studies there is an inaccurate per-
ception of opportunity for primary control Illusory Control
that leads to an increase in unfulfillable ex-
pectations and thus to the likelihood of dis- The treatment of illusory control and of
appointment. The advantage of secondary attributions to chance is situated between
control is the avoidance of this disappoint- consideration of attributions to severely lim-
ment. ited ability and to powerful others because
it bears an essential similarity to both.
Summary Chance attributions resemble attributions to
severely limited ability (treated in the last
The evidence reviewed here suggests that section) in that both give rise to passivity
attributions to severely limited ability and and withdrawal in situations in which activ-
the passive and withdrawn behavior that ity is likely to lead to success. Moreover,
often accompanies these attributions some- depression, which is commonly regarded as
times reflect the individual's striving for pre- a behavioral manifestation of perceived un-
dictive control. Individuals who can accu- controllability (Beck, 1967; Seligman, 1975),
rately predict uncontrollable events can has been linked both to severely limited abil-
thereby avoid unfulfillable expectations (i.e., ity attributions (see Journal of Abnormal
disappointment) that fuel the. perception of Psychology, February 1978) and to chance
uncontrollability. This tendency to adjust attributions (Janoff-Bulman, 1979). Chance
the self s expectations and behaviors to make attributions are also similar to attributions
them compatible with a difficult environ- to powerful others (treated in the next sec-
ment is an example of secondary control. tion) in that both involve association with
The evidence reviewed here indicates that a powerful external agent (in the case of
there is an increased desire for predictive chance attributions, the agent is fate). One
control following uncontrollable experiences indication of the close connection between
and that the striving for predictive control attributions to chance and attributions to
often takes the form of inward behavior. powerful others is the variety of phenomena
Moreover, there is evidence that this striving incorporating both—belief in supernatural
for predictive control, as shown by the avoid- powers, witches, demons, fortune-tellers,
ance of moderately difficult tasks, is likely psychics, mystics, and astrologers. As a con-
to accompany attributions to severely lim- sequence of their relation to attributions in-
ited ability (Kukla, 1974; Maracek & Met- volving severely limited ability and powerful
tee, 1972). What is not yet available is ev- others, chance attributions are often re-
idence that persons in the situations described garded as particularly indicative of per-
above actually perceive themselves as gain- ceived uncontrollability.
ing predictive control. This evidence is es- Our claim here, as in the preceding sec-
pecially needed because of the competing tion, is that the attributions involved allow
explanations for the phenomena in question. for a type of perceived control, referred to
This section and the sections that follow here as illusory control. Persons seek to align
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 17

themselves with the force of chance so that ing their interpretation are other findings
they may share in the control exerted by that that subjects perceive causal relationships in
powerful force. In addition, we maintain that the absence of objective contingency (Bruner
individuals who perceive illusory control in & Revusky, 1961; Chapman & Chapman,
a situation nonetheless recognize the chance 1967; Golding & Rorer, 1972; Smedslund,
determination of that situation. What we are 1963; Starr & Katkin, 1969; Ward & Jen-
concerned with, then, is not simply the trans- kins, 1965; Hamilton & Gifford, Note 2).
formation of a perception of chance into a Whereas Langer and her colleagues did not
, perception of skill. Rather, we believe that differentiate between subjects' sense of con-
people are often aware that chance is op- trol in skill situations and their sense of con-
erating and that they continue to refer to trol in chance situations (except to say that
luck and fate in explaining outcomes, but the latter is inaccurate), we maintain that
that they perceive chance as a force with the former is a perception of primary control
which they can align themselves. Moreover, and that the latter is, in at least some cases,
persons who are most familiar with chance a perception of secondary control—a sense
events (e.g., gamblers) are often most sus- of participation in the more powerful force
ceptible to perceiving them as controllable of chance.
(Langer, 1977). Thus, the perception of The studies described above provide some
chance as a type of control cannot be dis- support for the interpretation we propose.
missed simply as reflecting a lack of famil- First, intelligent and well-educated subjects
iarity with chance. (Duke University undergraduates in one
study, Yale undergraduates in another) were
The Relationship Between Skill , employed in this research, and the chance
and Chance nature of the tasks was made explicit. Such
subjects are likely to perceive the chance
One of the proofs of illusary control is nature of the situation accurately. Second,
evidence that persons perceive control in pre- included among the factors that stimulate
dominantly chance-determined situations. the illusion of control is passive involvement:
Research by Ayerhoff and Abelson (1976), The longer people wait for the outcome of
Langer (1975, 1977), Langer and Roth a lottery to be announced, the greater their
(1975), and Wortman (1975) provides the confidence that they will win. Langer (1975)
necessary evidence. These investigators have reasoned that in skill situations passive in-
shown that under certain conditions subjects volvement permits time for thought and thus
respond as if a chance-determined situation for the development of strategies that en-
were controllable. Specifically, when factors hance one's likelihood of success. Such strat-
normally associated with skill situations egies, however, are unlikely to emerge when
(e.g., choice, effort, task familiarity) are in- one contemplates a lottery in which the out-
troduced into chance situations in which come will be determined via blind drawing
subjects cannot influence the outcome, an by unknown people in an unknown location.
illusion of control is created (Langer, 1977), Why then does confidence increase the longer
Manifestations of this illusory control in- one waits for the outcome? We believe the
clude increased confidence in success, in- reason is that, over time, "the ticket holder
creased risk taking, and self-reported per- feels increasingly associated with the forces
ceptions of control. Naturalistic observations of chance that will operate in the situation.
of gambling lead to similar conclusions The very fact of passivity underscores the
(Langer, 1977). secondary nature of this perceived control.
The above authors, especially Langer, A similar argument applies to Langer's ma-
suggested that under these circumstances, nipulation of subjects' familiarity with the
subjects are impressed by the superficial sim- task. If anything, increased familiarity with
ilarity between chance and skill situations a chance task should sharpen one's aware-
and, as a consequence, they fail to distin- ness of its chance-determined nature and
guish adequately between the two. Support- reduce the likelihood that it will be confused
18 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

with a skill task. But increased familiarity DuCette and Wolk (1973) found that exter-
might well increase one's sense of partici- nals showed a greater preference for an ex-
pation in, or alignment with, the task—that trasensory perception task than did internals.
is, one's perception of secondary control. These authors interpreted their findings as
Support for our view would be provided by demonstrating externals' greater preference
more direct evidence that manipulations for situations that are congruent with their
such as passive involvement and task famil- generalized expectations for control.
iarity increase illusory control (perceived Support for the preceding view comes also
probability of success) under conditions in from findings that externals exhibit "reac-
which people are fully aware that no direct tance" in situations entailing an impersonal
manipulation is possible and in which no threat, but not in situations entailing a per-
"strategies" have been developed to influ- sonal threat to control (Cherulnik & Citrin,
ence outcomes. 1974).5 Impersonal threat, operationalized
Although the dynamic we propose is dif- as denying the subjects a choice of reward
ferent from that proposed by Langer and her due to circumstances beyond the control or
colleagues, the two are not incompatible. intent of the experimenter, was designed to
Illusory control may involve both an asso- foster attributions to "chance, fate, or the
ciation with chance (a secondary process) action of another over which [the subject]
and a misperception of chance-determined has no control" (p. 399). A significant in-
situations as skill-determined (a primary teraction between locus of control and the
process). Because illusory control is perva- way in which control was eliminated indi-
sive in the laboratory and in everyday life, cated that externals, compared with inter-
both influences are possible. nals, reported greater liking for the elimi-
nated reward ("reactance") following the
Locus of Control and Behavior in Chance impersonal, chance-like elimination of the
Situations reward (it did not arrive in the mail on time)
but less liking for the reward when it was
Research on locus of control has repeat- eliminated in a personal way (the experi-
edly established a link between externality menter decided it was not appropriate for
and various forms of inward behavior (see the subject). In explaining their findings,
p. 6). These links take on added significance Cherulnik and Citrin suggested that exter-
itr light of evidence on the behavior of ex- nals expect greater control over impersonal,
ternals in chance situations. In .considering chance-like rewards than do internals and
this evidence, it is important to keep in mind that it is the violation of this expectation that
that the chance situations are clearly iden- leads to reactance.
tified as such and that externals, who man- Karabenick and Srull (1978) and Srull
ifest the most illusory control, are also the and Karabenick (1975) reported findings
most likely to attribute outcomes to chance. similar to those above. In both studies, ex-
Indeed, externality is operationalized, in ternals cheated more than internals on a task
part, as a tendency to attribute outcomes to described as chance determined, but an op-
chance, luck, and fate (see Lefcourt, 1976; posite pattern held when the task was de-
Phares, 1976; Rotter, 1966). scribed as measuring skill (see also Aronson
Whereas internals, compared with exter- & Mettee, 1968). In both skill and chance
nals, have consistently been found to prefer tasks, insoluble problems were presented,
and perform better on skill-determined tasks, thereby inducing a loss of control similar to
externals have been shown to express a that induced in the Cherulnick and Citrin
greater preference for chance tasks than do study. Because cheating is a common reac-
internals and even to prefer chance tasks to tion to perceived loss of control (Brehm,
skill tasks (e.g., DuCette & Wolk, 1973; 1966; Wicklund, 1974), these findings sug-
Kahle, 1980; Schneider, 1968, 1972). Rotter 5
Cherulnik and Citrin, like most reactance theorists,
and Mulry (1965) found that externals con- use the term freedom rather than control, but these two
sult fortune-tellers and have their horoscopes terms are freely substituted for one another in their
read more often than do internals, and writings.
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 19

gest that externals perceive their failure at particularly likely to perceive contingency
chance tasks as a loss of control; that is, they and thus to feel confident in chance situa-
perceive chance as controllable. Further, tions. Evidence contrary to this view comes
self-report findings from the Karabenick and from Benassi, Sweeney, and Drevno's (1979)
Srull study indicate that externals, but not finding that internals, compared with exter-
internals, perceived the chance task as mea- nals, give higher estimates of their success
suring their "ability." This result, the au- and perceive more control when presented
thors noted, is consistent with Rotter and with a chance-determined task that is intro-
Mulry's (1965) suggestion that externals re- duced as being skill determined (a psycho-
gard luck as a personal characteristic and kinesis' task in which subjects are directed
are concerned about being lucky. In a more to concentrate so as to affect the outcome).
recent study, Karabenick and Addy (1979) Thus, it appears unlikely that externals over-
found that in chance situations externals estimate the role of skill in chance-deter-
were much more likely than internals to rate mined situations. Rather, it is something
outcomes as informative of their "disposi- about the perception of chance per se that
tions to be lucky or unlucky" (p. 227). motivates their performance.
Interactions between locus of control and Finally, Maracek and Mettee (1972) rep-
type of task (chance vs. skill) have also been licated the findings, discussed earlier, that
found with performance level (effort and persons who expect future failure resist
quality) as the dependent variable. The gen- seeing themselves as successful. If success
eral pattern in these studies is that externals, is an indication of ability, Maracek and
compared with internals, try harder and do Mettee reasoned, then the individual ac-
better in tasks described as chance deter- cepting success is susceptible to elevated ex-
mined. In two studies (Davis & Phares, pectations and subsequent disappointment.
1967; Rotter & Mulry, 1965), the superior On the basis of this reasoning, they hypoth-
performance of externals on chance tasks esized that subjects would not discount suc-
failed to reach significance; in three others cess if they could attribute it to luck. Luck,
(Lefcourt, Lewis, & Silverman, 1968; Petzel they argued, is an unstable and uncontrol-
& Gynther, 1970; Watson & Baumal, 1967) lable characteristic that will not affect sub-
the difference was significant (but see sequent expectations. Although their find-
DuCette & Wolk, 1973, for conflicting re- ings supported this hypothesis, there is an
sults). In a related vein, Pittman and Pitt- alternative interpretation. We realize that
man (1979) reported that externals per- many subjects may see luck as unstable and
formed better than internals under conditions uncontrollable; but some (externals and per-
of high helplessness (60 problem-solving sons who have experienced acute failure)
trials with noncontingent feedback) but per- may be motivated to see it as at least some-
formed worse under conditions of low help- what stable and controllable (cf. Weisz &
lessness (20 trials with noncontingent feed- Stipek, in press). In the Karabenick and
back) and no helplessness (see Gregory, Srull study cited earlier, externals were
Chartier, & Wright, 1979, for similar find- found to consider themselves "luckier peo-
ings). Because the high-helplessness condi- ple" (1978, p. 85). These individuals, we sus-
tion in this study is intended to accentuate pect, perceive primary control as unlikely or
noncontingency, subjects probably perceived impossible and so adopt stable chance attri-
it as one in which outcomes were chance butions (e.g., "I am a lucky person") that
determined. If so, these findings are consis- permit secondary control.
tent with those described above: Externals
perform better in situations perceived to be Summary
chance determined.
The preceding findings, we maintain, are We have argued in this section that cer-
due to externals' greater preference for and tain individuals neither seek out skill situa-
feelings of competence in chance situations. tions nor strive to manipulate the environ-
An alternative explanation, which merits ment; instead, they attempt to align with and
brief comment, is that externals might be rely upon the powerful forces of chance. The
20 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

evidence reviewed here indicates that these attributions and behavior indicate helpless-
individuals tend to manifest a variety of in- ness in the sense that individuals do not per-
ward behaviors, especially passivity and ceive themselves as able to alter the envi-
withdrawal in skill situations, they tend to ronment to fit their desires, but individuals
seek out chance situations, and they tend to are not helpless in that they derive a per-
be more reactant when adverse events occur ception of secondary, vicarious control from
because of chance (their perceived ally) than association with powerful others. This as-
when adverse events occur because of their sociation is neither a means to an end nor
own actions. Taken together, this evidence a method of fulfilling other objectives; rather,
suggests that objectively uncontrollable sit- the association is desired for its own sake.
uations, for these individuals, facilitate the By aligning themselves with more powerful
perception of and desire for secondary con- others, individuals can share in their victo-
trol. Further, this illusory control is revealed ries and in their accomplishments—in short,
in attributions to chance, attributions that in their control. In a sense they can ride the
satisfy a criterion of proof of the two-process coattails of these more powerful others. But
model outlined earlier. in order to share in this control, the individ-
Secondary control, as noted repeatedly in uals must become aligned with the more
this section, is closely related to external lo- powerful other or become a part of the more
cus of control. The differences that do exist powerful group. In other words, they must
are, in part, historical ones. Originally, ex- submit. That such enhanced control can re-
ternal locus of control was assumed to entail sult vicariously from the association with
a perception of uncontrollability. However, more powerful others is evidenced in such
numerous research findings have discon- everyday situations as the child who proudly
firmed the perceived uncontrollability as- asserts "my daddy is stronger than your
sumption, and many locus of control theo- daddy," the adolescent cult member whose
rists now espouse a position close to the one perception of control rests squarely in the
advocated here. According to the more re- hands of his or her demagogue leader, the
cent congruence hypothesis, people reserve ultratraditional housewife who derives a
energy for activities that match the form of sense of power from her union with her hus-
control they feel best able to exercise (with band, and the regional, chauvinistic follower
externals, for example, focusing on chance of sports heroes. (For example, millions of
activities). Cherulnik and Citrin (1974) put Americans felt a rush of pride when the U.S.
it well: "Externals do not feel powerless, but Olympic hockey team won the 1980 gold
simply pursue rewards in different avenues" medal). Most significantly, vicarious control
(p. 404). Despite the similarity between this is evident in religious phenomena, as, for
hypothesis and ours, the fact remains that example, when persons speak of gaining
external control was initially defined and in "strength through the Lord."
some quarters continues to be thought of as The notion of vicarious control is related
an absence of perceived control. For this rea- to two existing bodies of theory and research.
son we prefer the primary/secondary dis- "One involves the process of identification.
tinction. Indeed, we believe that the confu- The other involves Fromm's theorizing re-
sion between the different definitions of garding individuation and also the social-
externality mirrors the confusion between psychological research on self-awareness and
perceived uncontrollability and secondary deindividuation. Both literatures are re-
control. viewed in this section.

Vicarious Control Identification


Our explanation of attributions to pow- The construct of identification is similar
erful others and submissive behavior is at in several respects to the present notion of
once similar to and different from the ex- vicarious control. Kelman (1958) described
planation provided by uncontrollability the- identification as having the following prop-
orists. According to the proposed model, the erties: (a) It reflects desire for a self-defining
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 21

relationship to another person or a group to "extract subservience from others" (p.


that is intrinsically desired (rather than 232)—a clear reference to submissive be-
being desired because it leads to specific re- havior, which is of primary concern here.
wards or approval or to the avoidance of spe- Direct support for Bandura's "vicarious
cific punishments or disapproval), (b) It is control" hypothesis was provided in a now
not attributable to the value of the behavior classic study of the determinants of imita-
or attitude identified with, (c) It leads to tion. Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1963) ex-
submissiveness, even in the absence of sur- amined situations, designed to simulate those
veillance. occurring in the nuclear family, in which
Another description of identification that children could choose to identify with an
clarifies its relevance to the construct of vi- adult who controlled rewards or an adult
carious control is provided by Goldschmidt who was the envied competitor for the re-
(1974): wards. Because children showed greater im-
In every society, each individual identifies himself with itation of the former adult, Bandura et al.
one or more groups of individuals.and finds not merely concluded that there is greater support for
material advantage through such identification, but, the social power explanation than for the
more significantly, finds ego gratification (or loss) in status envy explanation favored by Whiting.
response to the fortunes of such groups, (p. IS)
That is, it is the control of rewards, rather
The notion of identification has its origins than their consumption, that is most desired.
in psychoanalytic theory. According to Freud While Bandura's addition of the social
(1923/1961), the individual takes on char- power notion to the construct of identifica-
acteristics of others in order to share in their tion serves to link it more closely to the pres-
experiences. The psychoanalytic model is so ent construct of vicarious control, his for-
often used to explain character formation mulation differs from ours in one critical
and sex-role development that this under- respect. Although rarely explicitly, Bandura
lying function—the ability to share in the at times suggested that individuals who en-
desirable experiences of others—is some- gage in identification do so for instrumental
times neglected. It is perhaps most evident purposes—that is, as a means of primary
in Freud's own description of the Oedipal control (see especially Bandura, 1977). (This
conflict: The son's "identification with his is consistent with his preference for the term
father . . . permits the affectionate rela- imitation over the term identification, which
tionship to the mother to be in a measure he felt has "excess meaning.") Even so, Ban-
retained" (p. 32). dura did, to some extent, depart from the
In Freud's view and in related formula- instrumental conceptualization of identifi-
tions (e.g., Bettelheim's, 1943, work on cation. This is /evident in the research he
"identification with the aggressor"; Whit- cited to support his claims. For example, he
ing's, 1959, 1960, work on "status envy"), cited Hetherington and Frankie's (1967)
the importance ascribed to control is unclear. finding that children are most likely to im-
Bandura (1969), however, reinterpreted these itate the picture preferences of their domi-
formulations in a manner consistent with our nant parent (i.e., the parent rated as more
position. He maintained that the incentives powerful by independent observers) even
desired in each case are control and power when it is made clear that their parents will
(see also Maccoby, 1959; Mussen & Distler, remain unaware of their choices. Such be-
1959; Parsons, 1955). In support of his havior hardly appears instrumental. Rather,
claim, Bandura cited evidence indicating the individual seems to be striving for as-
that people are more likely to identify with sociation with the more powerful other and
others who possess characteristics suggesting for a form of control that can only be sec-
control—competence, expertise, power, and ondary.
dominance (for a review of these studies, see i
Bandura, 1969). Another noteworthy fea- Individuation
ture of Bandura's model is its mention of
identificatory behavior other than imitation. Fromm's position. One of the most in-
He observed that powerful models are able fluential psycho-philosophical books of the
22 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

20th century is Erich Fromm's (1941) Es- dividual escape his unbearable feeling of
cape From Freedom.6 In it, Fromm explored aloneness and powerlessness" (p. 151). The
the process of individuation, taking as the preponderance of self-blame among de-
prototypical case the separation of infant pressed individuals, who also perceive ex-
from caretaker. The key dynamics in this tremely little control over outcomes (Abram-
process, according to Fromm, are an in- son & Sackeim, 1977; Beck, 1967), suggests
creased sense of freedom (similar to the pres- that they too may be attempting to align
ent notion of increased primary control) and themselves with more powerful others.7
an increased sense of loneliness and a di- Fromm summarized his thesis in this way:
minished sense of power (similar to the pres-
ent notion of diminished secondary control). The annihilation of the individual self and the attempt
to overcome thereby the unbearable feeling of power-
Although the separated individual is more lessness are only one side of the masochistic strivings.
independent, at the same time the psycho- The other side is the attempt to become a part of a
logical benefits derived from association with bigger and more powerful whole outside of oneself, to
more powerful others are lost. Fromm submerge and participate in it. This power can be a
person, an institution, God, the nation, conscience, or
claimed that persons flee from the individ- a psychic compulsion. By becoming part of a power
uation process in order to regain a sense of which is felt as unshakably strong, eternal and glamo-
power. He saw evidence of flight from in- rous, one participates in its strength and glory . . . one
dividuation in neurotics' submissiveness and gains a new security and a new pride in the participation
conformity and in historical phenomena in the power in which one submerges, (p. 155)
such as the Reformation. In the latter, Finally, it should be noted that Fromm
Fromm observed an "emphasis on the did not view alignment with others as an
wickedness of human nature, the insignif- effective long-term means of. increasing per-
icance and powerlessness of the individual ceived power: "The escape into symbiosis
and the necessity of the individual to sub- can alleviate the suffering for a time but it
ordinate himself to a power outside of him- does not eliminate it" (p. 238). Rather, dein-
self (p. 38). In capitalism and fascism, too, dividuation leads to a self-perpetuating cycle
Fromm saw the individual as attempting to of greater dependence on others, increased
escape from powerlessness through sacrific- fear of powerlessness, increased conformity,
ing the individual self, "either by submission
to new forms of authority or by a compulsive
conforming to accepted patterns" (p. 135). 6
The ever-present "shoulds" and "oughts" of Fromm's concept of freedom corresponds closely to
submissive individuals reflect their attempt what we refer to here as primary control. It is a notion
of a lack of external constraints and an opportunity to
to subordinate themselves. Motivating this function independently. The reason that people escape
submission is "the tendency to give up the it is that they lack the wherewithal to succeed on their
independence of one's individual self and to own. Averill (1973) contrasts this with another type of
fuse one's self with somebody or something freedom: "agreement of the individual with whatever
constraints do exist" (p. 299). This type of freedom cor-
outside of oneself in order to acquire the responds to what we refer to as secondary control.
strength which the individual self is lacking" 7
Steinbeck (1963) created a character, Mrs. Trask,
(p. HI). who beautifully portrays these dynamics. She is de-
Fromm viewed masochism as an extreme scribed as "an inside-herself woman" who
form of the desire to lose the self. The aim used religion as a therapy for the ills of the world
of masochism is symbiosis, "the union of one . . . Her . . . god was an expert in punishment. He
individual self with another self (or any other demanded of her a sacrifice. She searched her mind
power outside of the own self)" (p. 158). for some proper egotistical humility and almost hap-
Persons desire "to feel inferior and insignif- pily arrived at the sacrifice—herself. It took her two
weeks to write her last letter with revisions and cor-
icant" out of all proportion to their actual rected spelling. In it she confessed to crimes she could
shortcomings. They "belittle themselves," not possibly have committed and admitted to faults
engaging in "self accusation and self criti- far beyond her capacity. And then, dressed in a se-
cism" in their aim to make themselves weak cretly made shroud, she went on a moonlight night
and drowned herself in a pond so shallow that she had
and thereby deserving of the association they to get down on her knees in the mud and hold her
so ardently seek: "The goal of masochism head under water. This required great will power,
is not suffering. . . . [It is] to help the in- (p. 15)
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 23

even greater dependence, and so on (pp. avoidance of self-awareness and individua-


254-255). This cycle is related to our claim tion relates to Fromm's belief that persons
that individuals think they must maintain seek to escape freedom and to associate with
their submissiveness in order to maintain powerful others when they perceive them-
vicarious control. It helps explain the per- selves as lacking the wherewithal to function
sistence of this form of inward behavior. independently, as would be expected to fol-
Self-awareness and deindividuation. low from heightened attention to individual
Fromm's case for vicarious control is accom- limitations. Consistent with these claims,
panied by a kaleidescope of historical, po- persons do not escape self-awareness and in-
litical, economic, religious, sociological, and dividuation when they see their independent
psychological evidence. However, research efforts as likely to be successful (Carver,
is needed to substantiate his claims more 1979).
adequately. The clearest empirical evidence Further support for the above claims
that submissiveness results from a desire to comes from research on the consequences of
fit in with others can be found in the liter- decreased individuation. Manipulations de-
ature on self-awareness and deindividuation. signed to decrease individuation (increase
Manipulations of self-awareness, such as "deindividuation") include having subjects
placing subjects in rooms with mirrors or wear uniforms, addressing them collectively
cameras, exposing them to tape recordings by a group name, or having them engage in
of their voices, and having them write au- group activities. These manipulations have
tobiographical sketches, have been found to been found to facilitate an increase in un-
lead t<y increased submissiveness (Carver, inhibited behavior, which suggests greater
1974; Diener & Wallbom, 1976; Duval, perceived "freedom" (Baron, 1971; Diener,
1976; Rule, Nesdale, & Dyck, 1975; Scheier, 1979; Festinger, Pepitone, & Newcomb,
Carver, & Gibbons, 1979; Wicklund & Du- 1952; Singer, Brusch, & Lubin, 1965; Zim-
val, 1971) and feelings of individuation bardo, 1970). We believe that this is often
(Diener, 1979; Ickes, Layden, & Barnes, a secondary form of freedom, in which in-
1978). The fact that self-awareness also dividuals feel able to fit in with their envi-
leads to perceptions of decreased choice, low- ronment, rather than a primary form of free-
ered self-esteem, and increased helplessness dom, in which they feel able to change their
and that these effects are ^particularly strong environment. Johnson and Downing (1979)
following failure (see Brockner, 1979; provided evidence of deindividuated sub-
Scheier, Carver, & Gibbons, 1979; Wick- jects' desire to fit in and be responsive to
lund, 1975, for reviews) suggests that self- environmental cues: Deindividuated people
awareness serves to focus attention on per- increase shocks administered to a stranger
sonal limitations and to increase perceived when in the presence of cues supporting
uncontrollability. aggression (wearing a robe resembling a Ku
We believe that another dynamic opera- Klux Klan uniform), but they decrease
tive in the above studies is the desire for shocks administered in the presence of cues
secondary control. Because primary control supporting nonaggression (wearing a robe
is perceived as likely to lead to failure, at- resembling a nurse's uniform). Moreover,
tempts at secondary control are more ap- deindividuated subjects report that they are
pealing. The individuation induced by self- better able to "flow with the environment
awareness serves as a reminder that the in- and make spontaneous choices" (Diener,
dividual is separated from powerful others, 1979). In terms of the proposed model, these
but it does not act as a barrier to realigning subjects seek to fit in and thereby share in
oneself with them. A well-replicated finding the control of the more powerful group.
is that people exposed to self-awareness ma-
nipulations seek to avoid self-awareness, es- Summary
pecially following failure (Steenbarger &
Aderman, 1979; Wicklund, 1975). A related The findings just reviewed suggest the ex-
finding is that persons high in fear of failure istence of secondary control (in this case a
prefer working in a group to working alone vicarious type) in situations that have in the
(Willerman, Lewit, & Tellegen, 1960). This past been interpreted as reflecting perceived
24 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

uncontrollability. The submissive behavior them. The claim that interpretation consti-
that was noted is not instrumental, and it is tutes a type of control for which people ac-
likely to be accompanied by the perception tively strive has been eloquently made by
of others as more powerful than the self. Frankl (1963), who referred to the will to
Moreover, self-awareness, which typically meaning, the achievement of meaning, the
focuses attention on negative discrepancies sense of courage derived from meaning, and
between standards and behavior, has been the freedom to choose to accept or reject a
likened to a manipulation of uncontrollabil- particular meaning. Frankl's writings make
ity (Wicklund, 1975). Although there is ev- it clear that the search for meaning is par-
idence that a type of primary uncontrolla- ticularly intense under conditions in which
bility is operative here, we agree with Fromm there exists minimal primary control.
and with self-awareness and deindividuation Janoff-Bulman and Brickman (1980) pro-
theorists that other dynamics are often more vided an excellent example of what is re-
influential. In particular, we point to the ferred to here as interpretive control: Many
self s strong desire to submit to others' ex- European Jews in the early 1930s who had
. pectations (as opposed to the lack of desire the opportunity to escape from Europe in-
to assert the self) and to increased enjoyment stead "clung" to the interpretations offered
following deindividuation, which can be at- by the Nazis. What looked like "giving up"
tributed, at least in part, to an increase in and "helplessness," Janoff-Bulman and
secondary control. However, we acknowl- Brickman argued, was actually "persistence
edge that the experimental evidence of vi- at accommodation" (p. 24). People work
carious, secondary control is mostly indirect. hard at interpreting events so that they can
To strengthen our claims, more direct evi- accept them; in so doing, they may appear
dence, including self-reports of increased to be giving up, but their persistence suggests
perceptions of secondary control following otherwise. Because considerable energy is
deindividuation, is needed. devoted to interpretation and because a sense
Further support for our claims may be of mastery is associated with the understand-
found in the literature on support systems. ing of and ability to accept aversive events,
Evidence indicating that the family and we maintain that interpretation constitutes
groups of a religious, fraternal, ideological, a secondary type of perceived control (cf.
social, or political nature offer their mem- Averill, 1973; Frankl, 1963; Garber, Miller,
bers "strength," "buttressing," and "in- & Abramson, 1980).
creased self confidence" (cf. Silver & Wort- Several laboratory studies in the area of
man, 1980) suggests the existence of the attribution theory have indicated that per-
kind of vicarious, secondary control with sons who perceive a lack of primary control
which we are concerned here. However, the seek secondary control in the form of reasons
groups may also provide increased primary and explanations. When exposed to failure,
control by making the individual more con- "helpless" children (those who attribute fail-
fident about himself as an independent agent, ure to severely limited ability) have been
so it cannot be assumed that secondary con- shown to engage in a set of interpretations
trol will prove to be the key-dynamic. very different than those of "mastery-ori-
ented" children (those who attribute failure
Interpretive Control to motivational factors such as effort). Ac-
cording to Diener and Dweck (1978), these
Interpretive control refers to the search helpless children tend to look to the causes
for meaning and understanding. Because it of their failure, typically doubt their own
relates to each of the other kinds of control ability, and are likely to concern themselves
already considered, an analysis of interpre- with "unmodifiable factors" such as chance.
tive control serves as a summary for the, ar- Mastery-oriented children, in contrast, are
ticle. All of the attributions treated in the much more concerned with monitoring their
previous sections are treated again here. behavibr via solution-directed verbalizations
These attributions, we argue, help individ- and self-instructions. Their interpretations
uals find meaning in events and thus accept concern the possible solutions, not the causes
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 25

or reasons, for the problem. Looking to comes from experimental laboratory studies,,
causes (which entails the perplexing phe- much of the evidence for interpretive control
nomenon of self-castigation) has the subtle, comes from naturalistic field studies (Silver
secondary benefit of providing the individual & Wortman, 1980). Bulman and Wortman
with a reason and thus an acceptable mean- (1977), for example, showed that paralyzed
ing for his or her condition. Looking to so- accident victims exert considerable effort to
lutions has the obvious, primary benefit of make sense of what has happened to them.
increasing the probability of task success. All 29 persons interviewed mentioned that
The distinction between interpretations that they had asked the question, "Why me?"
explain the event and thereby enable the in- and all but one had developed explanations
dividual to accept it and those that are so- for their accidents. The six types of expla-
lution oriented neatly parallels the distinc- nations they identified—predetermination
tion between secondary control and primary (fate), probability, chance, God had a rea-
control. son, deservedness, and reevaluation—par-
The Diener and Dweck results also help allel in interesting ways the attributions
to clarify other findings in the attribution treated in this article. Similarly, Burgess and
literature. For example, they may explain Holmstrom (1979) found that many rape
why Alloy and Abramson (1979) found that, victims had dealt with the "Why me?" ques-
following failure, subjects in problem-solv- tion. They concluded that "coming up with
ing situations engaged in fewer complex, so- an explanation gives some understanding to
lution-oriented hypotheses. The Diener and the bizarreness of the act and aids in re-
Dweck results suggest that subjects in the turning some degree of control to the victim"
Alloy and Abramson study generated few (p. 1270). In contrast to Burgess and Holms-
complex solution-oriented hypotheses be- trom's focus on control, Bulman and Wort-
cause they were busy generating many cause- man concluded that their data indicate a
oriented hypotheses (speculations regarding need for an orderly and meaningful world
factors contributing to failure). This sug- (p. 362). These two positions, however, can
gestion closely resembles the conclusion be seen as compatible within the present for-
reached by Coyne et al. (1980), whose re- mulation: The striving for secondary con-
sults indicate that "helpless" subjects "be- trol—understanding and thereby accepting
come preoccupied with the explanation of the event—sometimes overshadows the striv-
their inability" (p. 352). Coyne et al. cited ing for primary control—maintaining that
numerous other studies in the literature on the event is or was changeable. That is, both
failure, repression, anxiety, stress, and frus- positions may be emphasizing the phenom-
tration, which they believe are consistent enon of secondary, interpretive control.
with this claim. Perhaps most relevant to One of the most common explanations
Diener and Dweck's findings and to the pres- found in the previously cited study on pa-
ent hypothesis are Pittman and Pittman's ralysis victims was to reevaluate the victim-
(1980) results showing increased "attribu- ization in a more positive light (Bulman &
tional activity" following failure manipula- Wortman, 1977). These reevaluations were
tions. In Pittman and Pittman's study, sub- regarded as explanations because they al-
jects who had experienced failure and who ways incorporated a theory as to why the
were subsequently administered an attribu- accident had occurred. For example, a fo-
tion task unrelated to the failure experience cusing on positive outcomes, such as slowing
were more likely to probe for causes under- down, learning about other people and life,
lying the behavior of persons described in and choosing their faith, might be accom-
the attribution task than were persons not panied by the thought "The accident may
exposed to the failure manipulation. This have been for the best." This search for rea-
generalization of the desire for interpretive sons and a purpose following tragic events
control is a phenomenon warranting further is also evident in Burgess and Holmstrom's
investigation. (1979) finding that victims of rape con-
Whereas most of the evidence reviewed sciously minimize their anxiety. One com-
thus far in support of secondary control mon strategy is to compare their own rape
26 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

with those of others who were killed, sub- isolation (Holmes & Houston, 1974; Laza-
jected to perversions, or suffered "greater vi- rus & Alfert, 1964; Lazarus, Opton, Nom-
olence. Through this "cognitive strategy," ikos, & Rankin, 1965; Speisman, Lazarus,
Burgess and Holmstrom argued, rape vic- Mordkoff, & Davison, 1964). This research
tims are able to render the event more tol- showed that when people are given a new
erable and acceptable. Similarly, Langer, meaning for an event—for example, if they
Janis, and Wolfer (1975) found that when interpret a painful circumcision rite as some-
patients were coached to interpret their hos- thing desired by the participants (Lazarus
pitalization experience as an opportunity for & Alfert, 1964) or interpret a painful elec-
such positive outcomes as rest, diet, and re- tric shock as an interesting new type of phys-
flection, they rated themselves and were iological sensation (Holmes & Houston,
rated by hospital staff as manifesting less 1974)—they experience less stress, as mea-
stress than did a control group. Like us, these sured by self-reports and physiological in-
authors emphasize the increased sense of dexes. The two-process model suggests that
control gained by the coached patients via the diminished stress reflects the successful
their ability to reinterpret events. attainment of one type of secondary con-
A related aspect of interpretive control is trol—the finding of meaning and thereby the
suggested by evidence that people are par- ability to accept potentially aversive events.
ticularly unlikely to select positive incentives The studies on interpretive control indi-
that they previously believed were difficult cate that, rather than changing the world,
to obtain, and similarly, they are particularly persons sometimes attempt to change them-
likely to select negative incentives that they selves (specifically their views) so as to better
previously believed were inevitable (Brehm, accept the world. Unfortunately, the re-
Stires, Sensenig, & Shaban, 1966; Ebbesen, search does not make it possible to differ-
Bowers, Phillips, & Snyder, 1975; Ham- entiate the extent to which subjects are seek-
mock & Brehm, 1966; Mann & Dashiell, ing meaning per se (which we regard as a
1975; also see Silver & Wortman, 1980). type of control) from the extent to which
For example, Mann and Dashiell found that they are seeking the positive thoughts and
males receiving a low draft number (indi- positive mood that often accompany the
cating a high probability of being drafted) achievement of meaning. Some authors, es-
were more likely to derogate lost alternatives pecially Bulman and Wortman (1977) and
(job, graduate school, travel). Similarly, Eb- Langer et al. (1975), emphasize the former;
besen et al. (1975) found that children were others, especially Lazarus and Launier
likely to decrease their evaluation of toys, (1978), emphasize the latter. In the light of
as shown by verbal choice and actual play- Lazarus and Launier's emphasis, it is ironic
time, when the toys were forbidden. The that they provided some of the most eloquent
decreased evaluation, they argued, makes descriptions of secondary control dynamics.
the inevitable loss of the toy less frustrating. They referred to individuals "trying to mod-
It is consistent with the two-process model ify [themselves] to improve the situation,"
of perceived control that externals, whom we to "changing one's own characteristics," and
believe are prone to secondary control, are to "accommodation" (Lazarus & Launier,
especially likely to devalue unattainable in- 1978, p. 314). However, Lazarus and Lau-
centives (Lefcourt, 1976). This tendency to nier appeared to regard these interpretive
alter individual desires and values so that efforts as means to an end—positive thoughts
they correspond more closely to the likely and positive mood—rather than as an end
outcome may eventually constitute some of in their own right. In contrast, we believe
the most compelling behavioral evidence of that people often regard interpretive control
striving for secondary control of the in- as intrinsically rewarding. Teasing apart the
terpretive type. desire for perceived control and the desire
Our notion of interpretive control is based, for positive incentives to which this control
in part, on research exploring coping strat- often leads poses methodological difficulties.
egies such as denial, intellectualization, and To some extent, these problems have already
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 27

been faced by researchers concerned with limited ability, chance, or powerful others),
primary control (Glass & Singer, 1972; Hir- it is especially important to identify the cri-
oto & Seligman, 1975; Seligman, 1975). teria that distinguish the two phenomena.
There is one study on coping strategies in A major kind of evidence that is relied
which the desire for control and the desire upon here, and that should be relied upon
for positive ideation were distinguished in future tests of the model, is behavior in-
(Neufeld, 1976). Subjects who were led to dicating persistence and effort. Although we
believe they were relying on their own coping agree with uncontrollability theorists that
strategies reported greater "confidence" failure leads to inward behavior, we claim
during stress (viewing a violent film) than that inward behavior is often motivated and
did subjects led to believe they were relying thus that evidence of this motivation is avail-
on an effective technique devised by the ex- able. We have attempted to show that the
perimenter. In fact, both groups of subjects effort to obtain secondary control is char-
were given the same coping strategy (clear- acterized by various forms of persistent be-
ing the mind). The enhanced effectiveness havior. In seeking predictive control, people
of the strategy perceived to be self-generated may undermine their success so as to avoid
as contrasted with the strategy perceived to rising expectations and subsequent disap-
be experimenter derived is consistent with pointment. In seeking illusory control, peo-
our belief that people seek personal control ple may increase their effort on chance tasks
as well as positive ideation. so as to align themselves with the forces of
fate and to see themselves as "riding a lucky
Summary streak." In seeking vicarious control, people
may increase their effort in deindividuated
What we refer to here as interpretive con- (e.g., group, religious) situations to align
trol is most likely to occur following failure themselves with the more powerful others
and in conjunction with attributions typi- and to join in their strength. Finally, in seek-
cally assumed to reflect perceived uncon- ing interpretive control, people may increase
trollability. The increased attributional ac- their attributional activity and may alter
tivity found in these situations, we believe, their selection of incentives to make attri-
is a clear indication that more than perceived butions and incentives conform better to the
uncontrollability is involved. What is needed likely outcome. In each case, the persistent
to further substantiate our claims is addi- behavior occurs within a context of failure
tional self-report evidence that people see and inward behavior and is not itself the kind
themselves as striving for meaning and un- that we immediately associate with the con-
derstanding, and behavioral evidence that struct of control. This is the reason why un-
people revise their selections of incentives to controllability theorists have generally ig-
make them mesh with their new-found in- nored this behavior. We have tried to show,
terpretations. however, that this persistent behavior is
characterized by a pervasive effort to fit in
Summary and Implications effectively with one's environment and to
Distinguishing Between Secondary Control obtain what we refer to as secondary control.
and Uncontrollability Another kind of evidence needed to sup-
port the model developed here is subjective
The central thesis, of this article is that impressions of the benefits of secondary con-
persons perceive and are motivated to obtain trol. The existing self-report data are lim-
secondary control in many situations previ- ited. We believe the reason for this limited
ously assumed to be characterized by per- data is that most previous researchers have
ceived uncontrollability and an absence of not included appropriate probes to elicit the
motivation for control. Because much of the subjective experience of secondary control.
evidence for secondary control is also evi- One example of the type of self-report con-
dence of uncontrollability (e.g., prior failure, firmation we would seek is the finding that
inward behavior, and attributions to severely persons with an external locus of control
28 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

perceive luck as a personal "disposition" and 1976, 1980; Rotter, 1966), or "Type A's"
as an "ability." If subjects in other studies (Brunson & Matthews, 1981; Glass &
cited here had been queried, we suspect that Carver, 1980). When these persons are pre-
they would have depicted the avoidance of sented with obstacles that are of low salience
disappointment, the union with powerful (Glass, 1977) or of brief duration (Pittman
others, and the finding of meaning as making & Pittman, 1980), they typically respond in
them feel effective and, more to the point, a reactant manner, as if they were chal-
as increasing their ability to fit in. Repli- lenged to exert more primary control. It is
cation of these previous studies with the ad- only when the obstacle and the resulting fail-
dition of appropriate self-report and inter- ure are salient and of lengthy duration that
view measures is one promising way to test these individuals' resources are depleted and
the two-process model of perceived control. they are forced to give up. We expect that
their giving up is typically accompanied by
Correlates of Secondary Control perceived uncontrollability; this happens be-
cause their unsuccessful attempts at primary
In addition to providing criteria that dis- control are intense and long-lasting and also
tinguish secondary control from uncontrol- because they lack the time and energy nec-
lability, the model presented here suggests essary for mustering secondary control at-
that there are personality variables that help tempts. Some support for this claim comes
to distinguish those most likely to exercise from Pittman and Pittman's (1979) finding
each type of control. People seeking second- that following prolonged failure, internals
ary control are typically persons who have showed significantly greater performance
experienced recurring prior failure or chronic deficits than did externals whereas internals
disabilities (e.g., the paralysis victims dis- exhibited significantly smaller performance
cussed by Bulman and Wortman, 1977, or deficits in the briefer failure condition. As
persons who are characterized by external noted earlier, secondary control is likely to
locus of control, low self-esteem, or high fail- manifest itself in persistence on extremely
ure avoidance). We especially suspect the difficult and chance-like tasks, such as that
operation of secondary control in persons provided in the prolonged failure condition.
who manifest perseverative behavior. For If, as we suspect, internals lacked both pri-
example, we speculate that persons labeled mary and secondary control in the prolonged
mute or autistic strive to avoid disappoint- failure condition, they would have little rea-
ment and to gain predictive control by cre- son for persisting. (Also see Janoff-Bulman
ating a constant, eventless environment and & Marshal's, Note 4, evidence of depression
that persons labeled obsessive-compulsive among highly competent institutionalized
strive to win the approval of powerful forces elderly.)
and to gain vicarious control by submitting As the above hypothesis suggests, we do
to directives and "shoulds." These specula- not believe that there is a type of individual
tions are supported by clinical descriptions who chronically perceives uncontrollability.
of mutism (Lefcourt, 1973), autism, (Park, We think of perceived uncontrollability not
1967), and obsessive compulsiveness (Rack- as constituting a consistent style but as
man & Hodgson, 1980; Shapiro, 1965). We something that persons occasionally slip into
believe that these individuals are particu- and from which they are highly motivated
larly invested in secondary control, both be- to escape. This view is consistent with the
cause of their recurring inability to obtain notion elaborated earlier (pp. 6-7) that con-
primary control and because of their success trol is a highly desired and pervasive motive.
at secondary control. Besides suspecting the existence of per-
We argue that perceived uncontrollability, sonality characteristics that predispose per-
ironically, is especially likely to occur in per- sons to secondary control rather than to un-
sons who typically rely on primary control: controllability, we suspect the existence of
persons referred to as having a high moti- situational factors that predispose persons
vation for success (Atkinson, 1964), persons to each of these tendencies. Secondary con-
with an internal locus of control (tefcourt, trol, we believe, is particularly likely in cases
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 29

of prolonged failure to obtain highly desired adaptiveness of efforts for primary control;
and important incentives or cases in which we would add that there may also be an
the inability is perceived as permanent (cf. underestimation of the adaptiveness of ef-
Lazarus & Launier, 1978; Silver & Wort- forts for secondary control. As an example
man, 1980). The combination of realizing of the former, Janoff-Bulman and Brickman
that primary control is not possible and the (1980) cited the scientist who persists in his
opportunity to prepare for attempts at sec- efforts to solve an impossible problem; an
ondary control optimizes the likelihood of example of the latter is the individual who
secondary control. Perceptions that we as- does not value the ability to predict when his
sociate with secondary control have been efforts are futile. Underestimations of sec-
noted in such settings as old-age homes ondary control could have important con-
(Felton & Kahana, 1974) and concentration sequences because people are reluctant to
camps (DesPres, 1976). Specifically, it has abandon one type of control (in this case,
been found that institutionalized elderly who primary control) when there are no alter-
make external attributions report more pos- natives besides uncontrollability (Janoff-
itive feelings of adjustment than their inter- Bulman & Brickman, 1980; Lazarus, 1981).
nal counterparts and that concentration Similarly, in the case of people who fail to
camp victims who try to hold out are those persist at a task at which they are able to
who cling to the hope that ultimately luck succeed, our analysis differs from the anal-
will serve them. (Also see Frankl, 1963, for ysis of uncontrollability theorists. We em-
an analysis of the role of secondary control phasize the possibility that the adaptiveness
in concentration camp victims.) of secondary control has been overestimated.
Perceptions of uncontrollability, in con- The shift from a concern with the optimal
trast, should be most likely in cases in which degree of control to a concern with the op-
primary control is anticipated but is not at- timal balance between different processes of
tainable or in which opportunities for sec- control is one of the most significant impli-
ondary control are limited (i.e., situations cations of the two-process model.
that do not lend themselves to avoidance of How can we determine what balance is
disappointment, alignment with chance or most adaptive? To answer this question, we
powerful others, or finding meaning). One need to operationalize adaptiveness, an ex-
of the factors that may seduce people to tremely difficult task due to the subjectivity
wrongly anticipate primary control is that of the values involved. There is considerable
they are not required to test its effectiveness.disagreement as to the relative benefits and
As long as people are not prompted to exert costs associated with different kinds of con-
primary control and to confront its costs and trol (Lazarus, 1981). For example, some
its limitations, they have the luxury of be- people highly value achievement and chal-
lieving in it (Solomon, Holmes, & McCaul, lenge, which are most fostered by primary
1980). They are then prime candidates for control and most jeopardized by secondary
perceived uncontrollability when they are control; in contrast, other people highly
finally tested and fail. value physical health, safety, and content-
ment, for which the opposite is true. Related
Adaptiveness to the issue of values is the issue of temporal
perspective. Different evaluations of adap-
The two-process model differs substan- tiveness will follow from a focus on short-
tially from the uncontrollability model in its term effects than will follow from a focus
conceptualization of adaptiveness. Whereas on long-term effects (Lazarus, 1981; Silver
uncontrollability theorists define adaptive- & Wortman, 1980). For example, consider
ness in terms of the absolute level of (pri- the finding that people "benefit" most (show
mary) control, we define adaptiveness in least .stress) when instructed to use a strategy
terms of the relative levels of primary and that is already dominant in their hierarchy
secondary control. If people persist at an in- (Lazarus & Launier, 1978); this finding
soluble task, uncontrollability theorists would might be questioned by persons opting for
probably suspect an overestimation of the a more long-term measure of benefit, which
30 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

includes the development of new strategies. Brickman (1980), by Lazarus (1981), and
An equally thorny problem in determining by Silver and Wortman (1980). The reader
what constitutes an optimal balance of con- is referred to these authors for a more elab-
trol processes is that judgments of adaptive- orate treatment of the issues involved.
ness depend upon the circumstances in ques-
tion. In extreme circumstances persons are Relationships Between Different Kinds of
likely to perceive extremes in primary and Secondary Control
secondary control as adaptive. If the situa-
tion is one of life or death, one in which the An assumption underlying the two-pro-
person must attack or be attacked, exclusive cess model is that the various forms of con-
primary control may seem appropriate. If, trol subsumed under the rubric secondary
on the other hand, the situation is one of are related to one another. For example, we
living with a series of fortuitous and painful assume that individuals who attribute out-
but irreversible events, then exclusive sec- comes to severely limited ability (and who
ondary control may seem appropriate. Even perceive increased control by avoiding dis-
when circumstances are not so extreme, appointment) are more likely to attribute
some people will tend to favor primary con- outcomes to chance (and to perceive greater
trol and others will tend to favor secondary control through aligning with chance) than
control. For example, in most competitive are individuals who do not attribute out-
and aggressive situations, passivity is detri- comes to severely limited ability. In support
mental; however, in some of these situations of this particular link is the fact that both
(e.g., when naive persons become involved types of persons are especially likely to seek
in con games or when the United States be- out extremely difficult tasks because such
came involved in Vietnam), impulsive, direct tasks minimize disappointment and maxi-
action may fuel the problem and make mize the role of chance.
things worse (Rubin & Brockner, 1975). In general, there is little evidence regard-
The multitude of relevant circumstantial ing the relationship between different forms
factors handicaps the search for general of secondary control. There are, however,
principles (see Silver & Wortman, 1980, for several factor analytic studies on children
a review). that reveal low, but impressively consistent,
Mindful of these complexities, we propose correlations among inward behaviors. These
that adaptiveness or "good adjustment" be low-level relationships hold with both "nor-
conceptually defined as a knowledge of how mal" and "clinic" populations (see Achen-
and when to exert the two processes of con- bach & Edelbrock, 1978, for a review). A
trol and how to integrate them (Janoff-Bui- reasonable assumption is that the relation-
man & Brickman, 1980). The importance ship between these behaviors is similar in
of integration is seen, for example, in the magnitude to the relationship between the
secondary efforts of paralysis victims to find types of secondary control that, we believe,
meaning in and to accept aspects of their motivate these behaviors. Research is needed
situation that are irreversible while still de- to test this assumption.
voting efforts to mastery of new problem- Finally, there is the anecdotal evidence of
solving skills in areas in which primary con- links between the various forms of secondary
trol is still possible (Silver & Wortman, control. We suggested earlier that beliefs in
1980). The effectiveness of integration is supernatural powers and in other mysterious
also evident in the classic aphorism of the forces point to the overlap between illusory
Chinese philosopher Lao Tzu: "Those who and vicarious control. Moreover, we indi-
would conquer must yield; and those who cated that interpretive secondary control re-
conquer do so because they yield." It is not lates to each of the other types of secondary
enough to be proficient at one process of con- control: It embodies the striving for accep-
trol; in fact, proficiency is a handicap if it tance of events and situations which lies at
interferes with the development of the com- the foundation of all attempts for secondary
plementary process. Similar positions have control. Finally, we point to the domain of
been developed by Janoff-Bulman and religion. With its emphasis on fate, mysti-
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 31

cism, powerful other(s), and spiritual mean- creased helplessness following a manipula-
ing, it combines several key manifestations tion of success in a depressed population
of secondary control. The religious sphere (Kilpatrick-Tabak & Roth, 1978)—may be
may ultimately prove to be one of the most due to the unintended undermining of sec-
fruitful for the assessment of the relations ondary control (in this case, the undermining
among secondary control phenomena. of avoidance of disappointment). Kilpatrick-
Tabak and Roth's suggestion that subjects
Therapeutic Implications in their study may be "particularly prone to
the fear of being unable to repeat or sustain
If, as argued here, the two-process model successful performance" (p. 150) is consis-
is an appropriate one for understanding a tent with our interpretation. Because of the
variety of inward behaviors, then certain very different therapeutic implications of the
therapeutic implications follow. Rather than uncontrollability model and the two-process
perceiving individuals manifesting these be- model, further investigation of their relative
haviors as lacking in adaptiveness, in ego merits is imperative.
strength, or, particularly, in control, the em-
phasis shifts to what they possess—a net- Conclusion
work of perceptions, motivations, and be-
haviors herein referred to as secondary The major objective here has been to in-
control. If secondary control is a major dy- troduce an as yet undeveloped position,
namic that is adaptive for many people in rather than to replace an established one.
many situations, then it is a logical starting Because the established position—the un-
point for therapeutic interventions. Ignoring controllability model—has been taken to
this dynamic and implementing techniques task so often in this article, the reader may
that are irrelevant to it, or that actually op- be left with the mistaken impression that the
pose it (e.g., trying to force the individual present authors do not believe in the exis-
to abandon secondary control), can have tence of perceived uncontrollability. This is
negative consequences that might be pre- certainly not the case. What we consistently
vented. It may be important to match ther- maintain throughout the article is that there
apeutic methods to clients along dimensions are different types of perceived control and
suggested by the two-process model. Ther- that when primary control is perceived as
apeutic outcome research indicates, for ex- unattainable, people do not necessarily aban-
ample, that individuals with an external lo- don all efforts at control. As a consequence,
cus of control benefit more from directive stringent criteria should be observed before
interventions whereas individuals with an it is concluded that an individual is com-
internal locus of control profit more from pletely without motivation for or a percep-
nondirective interventions (Abramowitz, tion of control. This is especially true when
Abramowitz, Roback, & Jackson, 1974; the attributions and behaviors that are pre-
Helweg & Gaines, 1977). Similar findings sumed to reflect perceived uncontrollability
have been obtained in research on classroom are highly persistent. In these cases, and in
interventions (Arlin, 1975; Ryback & Sand- others detailed above, a substantial burden
ers, 1980) and in research on weight-reduc- of proof should be felt by those who maintain
tion interventions (Wallston, Wallston, Ka- that perceived control is absent.
plan, & Maides, 1976).
The uncontrollability model of therapeu- Reference Notes
tic intervention operates under assumptions 1. Crandall, V. J. Differences in parental antecedents
that differ from ours. The therapist is seen of internal-external control in children and in young
as working with a cleaner slate—or more adulthood. Paper presented at the American Psy-
properly, with a client relatively lacking in chological Association Convention, Montreal, 1973.
2. Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. K. Illusory corre-
motivation for or in perceptions of control lation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis
on which the therapist might inadvertently of stereotypic judgments. Unpublished manuscript,
trample. The ineffectiveness of some manip- Yale University, 1972.
ulations—for example, the findings of in- 3. Schulz, R., & Hanusa, B. H. Attributional deter-
32 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

minants of information seeking after helplessness cesses. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of social-
training. Unpublished manuscript, Institute on Ag- ization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally,
ing, Portland State University, 1979. 1969.
4. Janoff-Bulman, R., & Marshall, G. Coping and con- Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward an unifying theory
trol in a population of institutionalized elderly. of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 1977,
Unpublished manuscript, University of Massachu- 84. 191-215.
setts, Amherst, 1978. Bandura, A, Ross, D., & Ross, S. A comparative test
of the status envy, social power, and secondary re-
inforcement theories of identificatory learning. Jour-
References nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67,
527-534.
Abramowitz, C. V., Abramowitz, S. F., Roback, H., Baron, R. S. Anonymity, deindividuation and aggression
& Jackson, C. Differential effectivenss of directive (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1970).
and non-directive group therapies as a function of Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971,32, 533 A.
client internal-external locus of control. Journal of (University Microfilms No. 71-18, 681)
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1974, 42, 849- Baum, A., Aiello, J. R., & Calesnick, L. E. Crowding
853. and personal control: Social density and the devel-
Abramson, L., & Sackeim, H. A. A paradox in depres- opment of learned helplessness. Journal of Person-
sion: Uncontrollability and self blame. Psychological ality and Social Psychology. 1978,3(5, 1000-1011.
Bulletin, 1977, 84, 838-851. Beck, A. T. Depression: Clinical, experimental, and
Abramson, L., Seligman, M., & Teasdale, J. Learned theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row, 1967.
helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Benassi, V. A., Sweeney, P. D., & Drevno, G. E. Mind
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1978,57, 49-74. over matter: Percieved success at psychokinesis. Jour-
Achenbach, T., & Edelbrock, C. The classification of
child psychopathology: A review and analysis of em- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37,
1377-1386.
pirical efforts. Psychological Bulletin, 1978, 85,
1275-1301. Benson, J. S., & Kerinelly, K. J. Learned helplessness:
Adler, A. The individual psychology of Alfred Adler The result of uncontrollable reinforcements or un-
(H. C. Ansbacher & R. R. Ansbacher, Eds.). New controllable aversive stimuli? Journal of Personality
York: Harper & Row, 1956. and Social Psychology, 1916,34, 138-145.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. Judgment of contin- Bettelheim, B. Individual and mass behavior in extreme
gency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder situations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy-
chology. 1943, 38, 417-452.
but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 1979, 108, 441-485. Brehm, J. W. A Theory of psychological reactance.
New York: Academic Press, 1966.
Archibald, W. P. Alternative explanations for self-ful-
filling prophecy. Psychological Bulletin, 1974, 81, Brehm, J. W., Stires, L. K., Sensenig, J., & Shaban,
74-84. J. The attractiveness of an eliminated choice alter-
native. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Arlin, M. The interaction of locus of control, classroom 1966, 2, 301-313.
structure, and pupil satisfaction. Psychology in the Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. Psychological reactance:
Schools, 1975, 12, 279-286. A theory of freedom and control. New York: Aca-
Aronson, E., &. Carlsmith, J. M. Performance expec- demic Press, 1981.
tancy as a determinant of actual performance. Jour- Brickman, P., Linsenmeier,. J. A. W., & McCareins,
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962, 65, A. G. Performance enhancement by relevant success
178-182. and irrelevant failure. Journal of Personality and
Aronson, E., & Mettee, D. Dishonest behavior as a func- Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 149-160.
tion of differential levels of induced self-esteem. Jour- Brock, T., Edelman, S., Edwards, D., & Schuck, J.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1968, 9, Seven studies of performance expectancy as a deter-
121-127. minant of actual performance. Journal of Experi-
Atkinson, J. W. Motivational determinants of risk-tak- mental Social Psychology, 1965, /, 295-310.
ing behavior. Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 359- Brockner, J. Self-esteem, self-consciousness, and task
372. performance: Replications, extension, and possible
Atkinson, J. W. An introduction to motivation. Prince- explanations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
ton, N.J.: Van Nostrand, 1964. chology, 1979, 37, 447-461.
Averill, J. R. Personal control over aversive stimuli and Bruner, A., & Revusky, S. H. Collateral behavior in
its relationship to stress. Psychological Bulletin, humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
1973, 80, 286-303. Behavior, 1961, 4, 349-350.
Ayerhoff, F., & Abelson, R. P. ESP and ESB: Belief Brunson, B. I., & Matthews, K. A. The Type A coro-
in personal success at mental telepathy. Journal of nary-prone behavior pattern and reactions to uncon-
Personality and Social Psychology, 1976, 34, 240- trollable stress: An analysis of performance strategies,
247. affect, and attributions during failure. Journal of
Backman, C. W., & Secord, P. F. The effect of perceived Personality and Social Psychology, 1981, 40, 906-
liking on interpersonal attraction. Human Relations, 918.
1959, 12, 379-384. Bulman, R. J., & Wortman, C. B. Attributions of blame
Bandura, A. Social-learning theory of identiflcatory pro- and coping in the "real world": Severe accident vie-
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 33

tims react to their lot. Journal of Personality and Duval, S. Conformity on a visual task as a function of
Social Psychology, 1977, 35. 351-36.3. personal novelty on attitudinal dimensions and being
Burger, J. M., & Arkin, R. M. Prediction, control and reminded of the object status of self. Journal of Ex-
learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and So- perimental Social Psychology, 1976, 12, 87-98.
cial Psychology, 1980, 38, 482-491. Dweck, C. S. The role of expectations and attributions
Burgess, A. W., & Holmstrom, L. L. Adaptive strategies on the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of
and recovery from rape. American Journal of Psy- Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 674-
chiatry, 1979, 136, 1278-1282. 685.
Carver, C. S. Facilitation of physical aggression through Ebbesen, E. B., Bowers, R. J., Phillips, S., & Snyder,
objective self-awareness. Journal of Experimental M. Self-control processes in the .forbidden toy para-
Social Psychology, 1974, 10, 365-370. digm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Carver, C. S. A cybernetic model of self-attention pro- 1975, 31, 442-452.
cesses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Epstein, S. Expectancy and magnitude of reaction to
1979, 37, 1251-1281. noxious UCS. Psychophysiology, 1973,10, 100-107,
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. Genesis of popular Felton, B., & Kahana, E. Adjustment and situationally-
but erroneous psychodiagnostic categories. Journal bound locus of control among institutionalized aged.
of Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 72, 193-204. Journal of Gerontology, 1974, 29, 3, 295-301.
Chein, I. The science of behavior and the image of man. Festinger, L., Pepitone, A., & Newcomb, T. Some con-
New York: Basic Books, 1972. sequences of deindividuation in a group. Journal of
Cherulnik, P. D., & Citrin, M. M. Individual differences Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1952,47, 382-389.
in psychological reactance: The interaction between Finkelstein, N. W., & Ramey, C. T. Learning to control
locus of control and mode of elimination of freedom. the environment in infancy. Child Development,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1974, 1977, 48, 806-819.
29, 398-404. Floor, L., & Rosen, M. Investigating the phenomenon
Cohen, S., Rothbart, M., & Phillips, S. Locus of control ' of helplessness in mentally retarded adults. American
and the generality of learned helplessness in humans. Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1975, 79, 565-572.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1976, Frankel, A., & Snyder, M. Poor performance following
34, 1049-1056, unsolvable problems: Learned helplessness or ego-
Cottrell, N. B. Performance expectancy as a determi- tism? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
nant of actual performance: A replication with a new 1978, 36, 1415-1423.
design. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Frankl, V. E. Man's search for meaning: An introduc-
ogy, 1965, 2, 685-691. tion to logotherapy. New York: Washington Square
Coyne, J. C., Metalsky, G. I., & Lavelle, T. L. Learned Press, 1963.
helplessness as experimenter-induced failure and its Freud, S. The ego and the id. In J. Strachey (Ed. and
alleviation with attentional redeployment. Journal of trans.), Standard edition of the complete psycholog-
Abnormal Psychology, 1980, 89, 350-357. ical works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 19). London:
Davis, W. L., & Phares, E. J. Internal-external control Hogarth Press, 1961. (Originally published, 1923).
as a determinant of information-seeking in a social Fromm, E. Escape from freedom. New York: Reinhart,
influence situation. Journal of Personality, 1967, 35, 1941.
547-561. Garber, J., Miller, S. M., & Abramson, L. Y. On the
DeCharms, R. Personal causation. New York: Aca- distinction between anxiety and depression: Perceived
demic Press, 1968. control, certainty and the probability of goal attain-
Deci, E. L. Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum ment. In J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.),
Press, 1975. Human helplessness: Theory and applications. New
DesPres, T. The survivor. New York: Oxford University York: Academic Press, 1980.
Press, 1976. Glass, D. C. Behavior patterns, stress, and coronary
•Deutsh, M., & Solomon, L. Reactions to evaluations by disease. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977.
others as influenced by self-evaluation. Sociometry, Glass, D. C., & Carver, C. S. Helplessness and the cor-
1959,22,93-112.. onary-prone personality. In J. Garber & M. E. P.
Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. An analysis of learned Seligman (Eds.), Human helplessness: Theory and
helplessness: Continuous changes in performance, applications. New York: Academic Press, 1980.
strategy, and achievement cognitions following fail- Glass, D. C., & Singer, J. E. Urban noise: Experiments
ure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, on noise and social stressors. New York: Academic
1978, 36, 451-462. Press, 1972.
Diener, E. Deindividuation, self-awareness, and disin- Goetz, T. E., & Dweck, C. S. Learned helplessness in
hibition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- social situations. Journal of Personality and Social
ogy, 1919,37, 1160-1171. Psychology, 1980, 39, 246-255.
Diener, E., & Wallbom, M. Effects of self-awareness Golding, S. L., & Rorer, L. G. "Illusory correlation"
on antinormative behavior. Journal of Research in and the 'learning of clinical judgment. Journal of
Personality, 1976, / 0, 107-111. Abnormal Psychology, 1972, 80, 249-260.
DuCette, J., & Wolk, S. Cognitive and motivational Goldschmidt, W. Ethology, ecology, and ethnological
correlates of generalized expectancies for control. realities. In G. V. Coelho, D. A. Hamburg, & J. E.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, Adams (Eds.), Coping and adaptation. New York:
26, 420-426. Basic Books, 1974.
34 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

Gregory, W. L., Chartier, M. C., & Wright, M. H. expectations and behavioral instructions on reactions
Learned helplessness and learned effectiveness: Ef- during a noxious medical examination. Journal of
fects of explicit response cues on individuals differing Personality and Social Psychology, 1974, 29, 710-
in personal control expectancies. Journal of Person- 718.
ality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 1982-1992. Johnson, R. D., & Downing, L. L. Deindividuation and
Griffith, M. Effects of noncontingent success and failure valence of cues: Effects on prosocial and antisocial
on mood and performance. Journal of Personality, behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
1977, 45, 442-458. chology, 1979, 37, 1523-1538.
Groos, K. The play of man. New York: Appleton, 1901. Jones, S. C. Self and interpersonal evaluations: Esteem
Gunnar-Vongnechten, M. R. Changing a frightening theories versus consistency theories. Psychological
toy into a pleasant toy by allowing the infant to con- Bulletin, 1973, 79, 185-199.
trol its actions. Developmental Psychology, 1978,14, Kahle, L. R. Stimulus condition self-selection by males
157-162. in the interaction of locus of control and skill-chance
Haimowitz, M. L., & Haimowitz, N. R. The evil eye: situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Fear of success. In M. L. Haimowitz & N. R, Hai- chology, 1980, 38, 50-56.
mowitz (Eds.), Human development: Selected read- Karabenick, S. A., & Addy, M. M. Locus of control
ings (2nd ed.). New York: Crowell, 1966. and sex differences in skill and chance risk-taking
Hammock, T., & Brehm, J. W. The attractiveness of conditions. Journal of General Psychology, 1979,
choice alternatives when freedom to choose is elimi- 100, 215-228.
nated by a social agent. Journal of Personality, 1966, Karabenick, S. A., & Srull, T. K. Effects of personality
34, 546-554. and situational variation in locus of control on cheat-
Harter, S. Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward ing: Determinants of the "congruence effect." Journal
a developmental model. Human Development, 1978, of Personality, 1978, 46, 72-95.
21, 34-64. Karabenick, S. A., & Yousseff, Z. I. Performance as
Helweg, G. C., & Gaines, L. S. Subject characteristics a function of achievement motive level and perceived
and preferences for different approaches to psycho- difficulty. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
therapy: A multivariate study. Journal of Consulting chology, 1968, 10, 414-419.
and Clinical Psychology, 1977, 45, 963-964. Kelly, G. A. The psychology of personal constructs.
Hetherington, E., & Frankie, G. Effects of parental New York: Norton, 1955.
dominance, warmth, and conflict on imitation in chil- Kelman, H. Compliance, identification, and internal-
dren. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ization: Three processes of attitude change. Journal
1967,6, 119-125. of Conflict Resolution, 1958, 2, 51-60.
Hiroto, D. S. Locus of control and learned helplessness. Kessler, J. Psychopathology of childhood. Englewood
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1974, 102, Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1966.
187-193. Kilpatrick-Tabak, B., & Roth, S. An attempt to reverse
Hiroto, D. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. Generality of performance deficits associated with depression and
learned helplessness in man. Journal of Personality experimentally induced helplessness. Journal of Ab-
and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 311-327. normal Psychology, 1978, 87, 141-154.
Holmes, D. S., & Houston, B. K. Effectiveness of sit- Klein, D. C., & Seligman, M. E. P. Reversal of per-
uation redefinition and affective isolation for reducing formance deficits in learned helplessness and depres-
stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, sion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1916,85, 11-
1974, 29, 212-218. 26.
Ickes, W., Layden, M. A., & Barnes, R. D. Objective Klinger, E. Consequences of commitment to and dis-
self-awareness and individuation: An empirical link. engagement from incentives. Psychological Review,
Journal of Personality, 1978, 46, 146-161. 1975, 82, 1-25.
Janis, I. L. Psychological stress. New York: Wiley, Koller, P. S., & Kaplan, R. M. A two-process theory
1958. of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and
Janoff-Bulman, R. Characterological versus behavioral Social Psychology, 1978,56, 1177-1183.
self-blame: Inquiries into depression and rape. Jounal Kukla, A. Performance as a function of resultant
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, achievement motivation (perceived ability) and per-
1798-1809. ceived difficulty. Journal of Research in Personality,
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Brickman, P. Expectations and 1974, 7, 374-383.
what people learn from failure. In N. T. Feather Langer, E. J. The illusion of control. Journal of Per-
(Ed.), Expectancy, incentive and action. Hillsdale, sonality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 311-328.
N.J.: Erlbaum, 1980. Langer, E. J. The psychology of chance. Journal of the
Johnson, J. E. Effects of accurate expectations about Theory of Social Behavior, 1977,7, 185-207.
sensations on the sensory and distress components of Langer E. J., & Benevento, A. Self-induced dependence.
pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978,
1973, 27, 261-275. 36, 886-893.
Johnson, J. E. Stress reduction through sensation in- Langer, E. J., Janis, I. L., & Wolfer, J. Reduction of
formation. In I. G. Sarason & C. D. Spielberger psychological stress in surgical patients. Journal of
(Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2). Washington, Experimental Social Psychology, 1915,11, 155-165.
D.C.: Hemisphere, 1975. Langer, E. J., & Rodin, J. The effects of choice and
Johnson, J. E., & Leventhal, H. Effects of accurate enhanced personal responsibility: A field experiment
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 35

in an institutional setting. Journal of Personality and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1971,
Social Psychology, 1976, 34, 191-198. 17, 332-341.
Langer, E. J., & Roth, J. Heads I win, tails it's chance: Miller, S. M. Why having control reduces stress: If I
The illusion of control as a function of the sequence can stop the roller coaster, I don't want to get off. In
of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Per- J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human help-
sonality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 951-955. lessness: Theory and applications. New York: Aca-
Lazarus, R. S. Psychological stress and the coping pro- demic Press, 1980.
cess. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966. Miller, S. M., & Grant, R. P. The blunting hypothesis:
Lazarus, R. S. The costs and benefits of denial. In S. A theory of predictability and human stress. In P. O.
Breznitz (Ed.), Denial of stress. New York: Inter- Sjoder, S. Bates, & W. R. Dockens (Eds.), Trends
national Universities Press, 1981. in behavior therapy. New York: Academic Press,
Lazarus, R. S., & Alfert, E. The short-circuiting of 1980.
threat. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Miller, W. R., & Seligman, M. E. P. Depression and
1964, 69, 195-205. learned helplessness in man. Journal of Abnormal
Lazarus, R. S., & Launier, R. Stress-related transac- Psychology, 1975, 84, 228-238.
tions between person and environment. In L. A. Pervin Mussen, P. H., & Distler, L. Masculinity, identification,
& M. Lewis (Eds.), Perspectives in interactional psy- and father-son relationships. Journal of Abnormal
chology. New York: Plenum Press, 1978. and Social Psychology, 1959,59, 350-356.
Lazarus, R. S., Opton, E. M., Nomikos, M. S., & Ran- Neufeld, R. W. J. Evidence of stress as a function of
kin, N. O. The principle of short circuiting of threat: experimentally altered appraisal of stimulus aversive-
Further evidence. Journal of Personality, 1965, 33, ness and coping adequacy. Journal of Personality and
622-635. Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 632-646.
Lefcourt, H. M. The function of the illusions of control Park, C. C. The siege. New York: Harcourt, Brace &
and freedom. American Psychologist, 1973, 28, 417- World, 1967.
425. Parsons, T. Family structure and the socialization of the
child. In T. Parsons & R. F. Bales (Eds.), Family,
Lefcourt, H. M. Locus of control: Current trends in socialization, and interaction process. Glencoe, III.:
theory and research. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum,
1976. Free Press, 1955.
Petzel, T. P., & Gynther, M. D. Effects of internal-
Lefcourt, H. M. Personality and locus of control. In J. external locus of control and skill or chance instruc-
Garber & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human help- tional sets on task performance. Journal of General
lessness: Theory and applications. New York: Aca- Psychology, 1970, 82. 87-93.
demic Press, 1980. Phares, E. J. Locus of control in personality. Morris-
Lefcourt, H. M., Lewis, L., & Silverman, I. W. Internal town, N.J.: General Learning Press, 1976.
versus external control of reinforcement and attention Piaget, J. Piaget's theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Car-
in a decision-making task. Journal of Personality, michael's manual of child psychology (3rd ed., Vol.
1968, 36, 663-682. 2). New York: Wiley, 1970.
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E., Under- Pittman, N. L., & Pittman, T. S. Effect of amount of
mining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic re- helplessness training and internal-external locus of
wards: A test of the "overjustiflcation" hypothesis. control on mood performance. Journal of Personality
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 39-47.
28, 129-137. Pittman, T. S., & Pittman, N. L. Deprivation of control
Leventhal, H., Brown, D., Shacham, S., & Engquist, and the attribution process. Journal of Personality
G. Effects of preparatory information about sensen- and Social Psychology, 1980, 39, 377-389.
ations, threat of pain, and attention on cold pressor Rackman, S. J., & Hodgson, R. J. Obsessions and com-
distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- pulsions (Century Psychology Series). Englewood
ogy, 1979, 37, 688-714. Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1980.
Maccoby, E. E. Role-taking in childhood and its con- Rodin, J., Rennert, K., & Solomon, S. Intrinsic moti-
sequences for social learning. Child Development, vation for control: Fact or fiction. In A. Baum, J. E.
1959, 30. 239-252. Singer, & S. Valios (Eds.), Advances in environmen-
Malinowski, B. Magic, science, and religion. New York: tal psychology II. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1980.
Anchor Books, 1955. Roth, S. A revised model of learned helplessness in hu-
Mann, L., & Dashiell, T. Reactions to the draft lottery: mans. Journal of Personality, 1980, 48, 103-133.
A test of conflict theory. Human Relations, 1975, 28, Roth, S., & Bootzin, R. R. The effects of experimentally
155-173. induced expectancies of external control: An inves-
Maracek, J., & Mettee, D. R. Avoidance of continued tigation of learned helplessness. Journal of Person-
success as a function of self-esteem, level of esteem ality and Social Psychology, 1974, 29, 253-264.
certainty, and responsibility for success. Journal of Roth, S., & Kubal, L. Effects of non-contingent rein-
Personality and Social Psychology, 1972,22,98-107. forcement on tasks of differing importance: Facili-
Marks, I. Phobias and obsessions. In J. D. Maser & tation and learned helplessness. Journal of Person-
M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Psychopathology: Exper- ality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 680-691.
imental models. San Francisco: Freeman, 1977. Rothbaum, F. Children's clinical syndromes and gen-
Mettee, D. R. Rejection of unexpected success as a func- eralized expectations of control. In H. W. Reese &
tion of the negative consequences of accepting success. L. P. Lipsett (Eds.), Advances in child development
36 F. ROTHBAUM, J. WEISZ, AND S. SNYDER

and behavior (Vol. 15). New York: Academic Press, Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1963,4, 165-
1980. 173.
Rothbaum, F. The effect of sufficiency and necessity on Snyder, M. L., Stephan, W. G., & Rosenfield, D. At-
perceptions of control and responsibility. Journal of tributional egotism. In J. H. Harvey, W. Ickes, &
Personality, 1981, 49, 85-100. R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution re-
Rothbaum, F., Wolfer, J., & Visintainer, M. Coping search (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978.
behavior and control in children. Journal of Person- Snyder, M., & Wicklund, R. A. Attribution ambiguity.
ality, 1919,47, 118-135. In J. H. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New
Rotter, J. B. Generalized expectancies for internal ver- Directions in attribution research (Vol. 3). Hillsdale,
sus external control of reinforcement, Psychological N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.
Monographs, 1966, 80, 1-28. Solomon, S., Holmes, D. S., & McCaul, K. D. Behav-
Rotter, J. B., & Mulry, R. C. Internal versus external ioral control over ayersive events: Does control that
control of reinforcement and decision time. Journal requires effort reduce anxiety and physiological
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 598- arousal? Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
604. chology, 1980, 39, 729-736.
Rubin, J. Z., & Brockner, J. Factors affecting entrap- Speisman, J. C., Lazarus, R. S., Mordkoff, A. M., &
ment in waiting situations: The Rosencrantz-Guil- Davidson, L. A. The experimental reduction of stress
denstern effect. Journal of Personality and Social based on ego-defense theory. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1975, 32, 1054-1063. and Social Psychology, 1964, 68, 361-380.
Rule, B. G., Nesdale, A. R., & Dyck, R. Objective self- Srull, T. K., & Karabenick, S. A. Effects of personality-
awareness and differing standards of aggression. Rep- situation locus of control congruence. Journal of Per-
resentative Research in Social Psychology, 1975, 6, sonality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 617-628.
82-88. Starr, B. J., & Katkin, E. S. The clinician as an aberrant
Ryback, D., & Sanders, J. J. Humanistic vs. traditional actuary: Illusory correlation and incomplete sentences
teaching styles and student satisfaction. Journal of blank. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 74,
Humanistic Psychology, 1980, /, 87-90. 670-675.
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Gibbons, F. X. Self- Steenbarger, B. N., & Aderman, D. Objective self-
directed attention, awareness of bodily states, and awareness as a non-aversive state: Effect of antici-
suggestibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- pating discrepancy reduction. Journal of Personality,
chology, 1979, 37, 1576-1588. 1979, 47, 330-339.
Schneider, J. M. Skill versus chance activity preference Steinbeck, J. East of Eden. New York: Viking Press,
and locus of control. Journal of Consulting and Clin- 1963.
ical Psychology, 1968, 32, 333-337. Tennen, H., & Eller, S. J. Attributional components of
Schneider, J. M. Relationship between locus of control learned helplessness and facilitation. Journal of Per-
and activity preferences: Effects of masculinity, ac- sonality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 4, 265-
271.
tivity, and skill. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 1972, 38, 225-230. Thornton, J. W., & Jacobs, P. D. Learned helplessness
in human subjects. Journal of Experimental Psy-
Schulz, R. Aging and control. In J. Garber & chology, 1971, 87, 369-372.
M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human helplessness: The-
ory and applications. New York: Academic Press, Trope, Y. Seeking information about one's own ability
1980. as a determinant of choice among tasks. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 1004-
Seligman, M. E. P. Helplessness: On depression, de- 1013.
velopment and death. San Francisco: Freeman, 1975. Trope, Y., & Brickman, P. Difficulty and diagnosticity
Shaban, J., & Jecker, J. Risk preference in choosing an as determinants of choice among tasks. Journal of
evaluator: An extension of Atkinson's achievement- Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 918-
motivation model. Journal of Experimental Social 925.
Psychology, 1968, 4. 35-45. Wallston, B. S., Wallston, K. A., Kaplan, G. D., &
Shapiro, D. Neurotic styles. New York: Basic Books, Maides, S. A.: Development and validation of the
1965. Health Locus of Control (HLC) Scale. Journal of
Shontz, F. C. The psychological aspects of physical Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1976, 44, 580-
illness and disability. New York: Macmillan, 1975. 585.
Sidle, A., Moos, R., Adams, J., & Cady, P. Development Ward, W. C., & Jenkins, H. M. The display of infor-
of a coping scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, mation and the judgment of contingency. Canadian
1969, 20, 226-232. Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psy-
Silver, R. L., & Wortman, C. B. Coping with undesir- chologie, 1965, 19, 231-241.
able life events. In J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman Watson, D., & Baumal, E. Effects of locus of control
(Eds.), Human helplessness: Theory and applica- and expectation of future control upon present per-
tions. New York: Academic Press, 1980. formance. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Singer, J. E., Brusch, C. A., & Lubin, S. C. Some as- chology, 1967, 6, 212-215.
pects of deindividuation: Identification and confor- Watson, J. S. Memory and "contingency analysis" in
mity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, infant learning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1967,13,
1965, /, 356-378. 55-76.
Smedslund, J. The concept of correlation in adults. Weiner, B. (Ed.). Achievement motivation and attri-
A TWO-PROCESS MODEL 37

button theory. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning (Eds.), Personality development in children. Austin:
Press, 1974. University of Texas Press, 1960, 112-126.
Weiss, J. M. Effects of coping responses on stress. Jour- Wicklund, R. A. Freedom and reactance. New York:
nal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, Wiley, 1974.
1968, 65, 251-260. Wicklund, R. A. Objective self-awareness. In L. Ber-
Weiss, J. M. Effects of coping behavior in different kowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
warning signal conditions on stress pathology in rats. chology (Vol. 8). New York: Academic Press, 1975.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol- Wicklund, R. A., & Duval, S. Opinion change and per-
ogy, 1971, 77, 1-13. (a) formance facilitation as a result of objective self-
Weiss, J. M. Effects of coping behavior with and without awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
a feedback signal on stress pathology in rats. Journal chology, 1971, 7, 319-342.
of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1971, Willerman, B., Lewit, D., & Tellegen, A. Seeking and
77, 22-30. (b) avoiding self-evaluation by working individually or in
Weiss, J. M. Effects of punishing and coping response groups. In D. Willmer (Ed.), Decisions, values and
(conflict) on stress pathology in rats. Journal of Com- groups. New York: Pergamon Press, 1960.
parative and Physiological Psychology, 1971,77, 14- Wolman, B. B. (Ed.). Handbook of clinical psychology.
21. (c) New York: McGraw-Hill, 1965.
Weisz, J. R. Perceived control and learned helplessness Wortman, C. B. Some determinants of perceived con-
among mentally retarded and nonretarded children: trol. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychol- 1975, 31, 282-294.
ogy, 1979, 15, 311-319. Wortman, C. B., & Brehm, J. W. Responses to uncon-
Weisz, J. R. Developmental change in perceived control: trollable outcomes: An integration of reactance theory
Recognizing noncontingency in the laboratory and and the learned helplessness model. Advances in Ex-
perceiving it in the world. Developmental Psychology, perimental Social Psychology, 1975, 8, 277-336.
1980, 16, 385-390. Wortman, C. B., Costanzo, P. R., & Witt, R. T. Effect
Weisz, J. R. Illusory contingency in children at the state of anticipated performance on the attribution of cau-
fair. Developmental Psychology, 1981, ; 7, 481-489. sality to self and others. Journal of Personality and
Weisz, J. R. Learned helplessness and the retarded Social Psychology, 1973, 27, 372-381.
child. In E. Zigler & D. Balla (Eds.), Mental retar- Yando, R., & Zigler, E. Outerdirectedness in the prob-
dation: The developmental-difference controversy. lem-solving of institutionalized and noninstitutional-
Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, in press. ized normal and retarded children. Developmental
Wiesz, J. R., & Stipek, D. J, Competence, contingency, Psychology, 1911,4, 277-288.
and the development of perceived control. Human Zimbardo, P. G., The human choice: Individuation, rea-
Development, in press. son, and order versus deindividuation, impulse, and
chaos. In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska
White, R. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of com- Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 17). Lincoln: Uni-
petence. Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 297-333. versity of Nebraska Press, 1970.
Whiting, J. W. M. Sorcery, sin, and the superego. In Zuckerman, M. Attribution of success revisited or: The
M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Moti- motivational bias is alive and well in attribution the-
vation (Vol. 6). Lincoln: University of Nebraska ory. Journal of Personality, 1979, 47, 245-287,
Press, 1959.
Whiting, J. W. M. Resource, mediation and learning Received September 5, 1980
by identification. In I. Iscoe & H. W. Stevenson Revision received May 5, 1981 •

You might also like