Goetghebeur 2016 PDF

You might also like

You are on page 1of 9

645269

research-article2016
JOP0010.1177/0269881116645269Journal of PsychopharmacologyGoetghebeur and Swartz

Commentary

True alignment of preclinical and


clinical research to enhance success in
CNS drug development: a review of the Journal of Psychopharmacology

current evidence
1­–9
© The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0269881116645269
jop.sagepub.com

Pascal JD Goetghebeur1 and Jina E Swartz2

Abstract
Central nervous system pharmacological research and development has reached a critical turning point. Patients suffering from disorders afflicting
the central nervous system are numerous and command significant attention from the pharmaceutical industry. However, given the numerous failures
of promising drugs, many companies are no longer investing in or, indeed, are divesting from this therapeutic area. Central nervous system drug
development must change in order to develop effective therapies to treat these patients. Preclinical research is a cornerstone of drug development;
however, it is frequently criticised for its lack of predictive validity. Animal models and assays can be shown to be more predictive than reported
and, on many occasions, the lack of thorough preclinical testing is potentially to blame for some of the clinical failures. Important factors such as
translational aspects, nature of animal models, variances in acute versus chronic dosing, development of add-on therapies and understanding of the
full dose–response relationship are too often neglected. Reducing the attrition rate in central nervous system drug development could be achieved by
addressing these important questions before novel compounds enter the clinical phase. This review illustrates the relevance of employing these criteria
to translational central nervous system research, better to ensure success in developing new drugs in this therapeutic area.

Keywords
CNS, translation, U-shape

Date received: 6 November 2015; revised: 15 March 2016; accepted: 28 March 2016

Introduction
Research into effective pharmacological treatments to alleviate are shared between species at the CNS level, among them cogni-
psychiatric and neurological disorders has, especially in recent tive constructs and processes including executive function, spa-
years, suffered from a critical lack of success, resulting in expen- tial memory, working memory, impulsivity and attention.
sive and often spectacular failures in phase III clinical trials One example, among many, of a cognitive mechanism pre-
(Carroll, 2013; Kidwell et al., 2001; Kresge, 2014; Sadeghi-Nejad, served across species, is that of sensorimotor gating, the ability to
2012). A spate of recent costly unsuccessful development path- filter out irrelevant from relevant information. This phenomenon,
ways in central nervous system (CNS) indications, including present in humans (Hashimoto, 2005; Witten et al., 2016), rodents
Alzheimer’s disease and schizophrenia, have resulted in smaller (Ma et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2015) and zebrafishes (Burgess and
companies leaving this therapeutic area, while many larger ones Granato, 2007; Knogler and Drapeau, 2014), can be disrupted in
have dramatically downscaled their CNS development pro- all three species with a dopamine agonist (Burgess and Granato,
grammes or pulled out of the arena altogether (Abbott, 2011; 2007; Larrauri et al., 2015; Talledo et al., 2009), thus mimicking
Kaitin and Milne, 2011; Miller, 2010). Tellingly, GSK, AstraZeneca the deficit seen in patients with schizophrenia. The abnormal
and Novartis have closed their neuroscience divisions, while response can further be reversed by antipsychotic agents in these
Pfizer, Sanofi, Janssen and Merck have significantly downsized three species (Burgess and Granato, 2007; Csomor et al., 2014;
their CNS facilities and programmes (Skripka-Serry, 2013). Zangrando et al., 2013). On the other hand, doubts are raised
One of several of the significant contributors to this high attri-
tion rate is purportedly a potential lack of predictive validity of
1Cognition Research Group, Takeda Cambridge Ltd, Cambridge, UK
the animal models used in early phase CNS drug development
2CNS Therapeutic Area Unit, Takeda Development Centre Europe Ltd,
(Akhtar, 2015; Matthews, 2008; Witten et al., 2016). Although it
London, UK
is simultaneously recognised that animal research is an essential,
necessary and mandatory component of the development path- Corresponding author:
way, it is self-evident that humans and animals, rodents in par- Pascal Goetghebeur, SynAging, 2 rue du Doyen Marcel Roubault, TSA
ticular, are different biological entities at many levels. However, 70605 54518 Vandoeuvre-les-Nancy, France.
a relatively high number of complex mechanisms and circuitries Email: pascal.goetghebeur@synaging.com

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


2 Journal of Psychopharmacology

about human to rat cross-species translational capabilities in this (ASST) paradigm, which aims to evaluate alterations in a spe-
assay (Witten et al., 2016) because drugs reversing sensorimotor cific aspect of executive control and cognitive flexibility, namely
gating deficit in rodents are inefficacious clinically. However, as attentional set-shifting abilities. This assay has successfully dem-
explained below, assessing the effect of a single dose of a drug in onstrated improvement of the extra-dimensional shift with sev-
such an assay is not informative enough and a full dose response eral pharmacological interventions in patients with schizophrenia
is necessary in order to make a valid comparison of like for like. or schizophrenia-like rodent models including erythropoietin
More importantly, for truly valid comparison the same endpoint (Ehrenreich et al., 2007; Goetghebeur et al., 2010), sertindole
should be evaluated in both species along with assessment of the (Gallhofer et al., 2007; Goetghebeur and Dias, 2009) and
circuitries involved. As pointed out in the paper there is no con- modafinil (Goetghebeur and Dias, 2009; Morein-Zamir et al.,
sensus among scientists for the rodent equivalent of the human 2007; Scoriels et al., 2012, 2013; Turner et al., 2004).
P50, therefore no definite conclusion about its cross-species However, as a counterpoint, the potential failure of the pre-
translational capabilities can be drawn. dictive validity of this assay has been demonstrated by the recent
In order best to compare animal and human research it is development programme of CX516 (Goff et al., 2008), an
important to ensure that, in both instances, the same ‘signal’ α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid recep-
(assay) is measured, using an analogous or comparable ‘protocol’ tor positive allosteric modulator for schizophrenia and a small
(methodology) including the use of multiple disease model(s) clinical trial with modafinil (Clinical Trial ID: NCT00057707,
and that several doses are tested, given the potential Yerkes– NCT00573417). The authors try not to argue that animal
Dodson effect (also known as the inverted U-shape model). All research is always tightly correlated to human research.
these components should be commonly applied to animal and However, in the case of CX516, only its acute effects in rodents
human investigation, to ensure the translatability of the overall have been investigated and no combination studies with antipsy-
research paradigm. chotics have been reported, as in the human context. Both
chronic treatment effects and the use of CX516 as an add-on to
antipsychotic therapies must be investigated before any conclu-
Signal measured sions about the predictive validity (the ability of the preclinical
assay to predict a modulation of the behaviour that can be trans-
If a potential new drug shows efficacy pre-clinically in a specific
posed to humans) of the assay can be drawn. The same applies to
cognition test like the novel object recognition test (investigating
modafinil, which is efficacious acutely in the ASST in patients
visual memory), then a true comparison in the clinical investiga-
with schizophrenia (Turner et al., 2004) as well as in a rodent
tion process must be a similar visual memory task that engages
schizophrenia disease model (Goetghebeur and Dias, 2009). Yet,
the same brain area(s), using a comparable range of memory
this effect was not replicated in a small clinical trial in patients
retention intervals that is species-specific. In these instances, a
with chronic schizophrenia, as mentioned above. It is therefore
composite cognitive assessment tool in which visual memory is a
important to pay close attention to the differences between pre-
contributory subcomponent of the overall scale (e.g. as in the
clinical and clinical studies for any given compound. Firstly, just
Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale-cognitive subscale, also
like the CX516 studies, no preclinical studies were conducted to
known as the ADAS-Cog, a well-established tool used in the
investigate chronic treatment; neither were antipsychotic combi-
assessment of mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease and some
nation studies reported preclinically. Secondly, the small clinical
disorders of cognition) is an inappropriate outcome measure
trial used the (–)-(R)-enantiomer of the compound, unlike the
(Cano et al., 2010), not least because the signal generated by the
rodent (Goetghebeur and Dias, 2009) and clinical (Turner et al.,
relevant cognitive subdomain assessed is subsumed within the
2004) studies, which could further account for the observed dis-
‘noise’ of the total score. In this respect preclinical back-trans-
crepancies between the outcome in the animal and human schiz-
lated assays, that is, preclinical assays derived from human ones
ophrenia studies. Finally, modafinil has been shown to
in a specific animal (i.e. rodent), simultaneously measuring the
demonstrate an inverted U-shape dose–response curve on cogni-
same cognitive subdomain as in the human and showing the
tion in humans (Finke et al., 2010) and rodents (Shuman et al.,
involvement of the same circuitry(ies) are the best options for use
2009), but this effect pattern has never been investigated in the
in the drug development pathway, as these will ensure optimal
attentional set-shifting paradigm. Hence, we do not know
and relevant translation into the clinic. A potential comparable
whether such a hormetic response can be used to explain the
human version of the spontaneous object recognition test could
observed difference between the animal and human studies. To
be the visual paired comparison (Wallace et al., 2015) and this
date the closest, and the only, comparison to be made between
test seems to be of relevance for Alzheimer’s disease (Crutcher
preclinical and clinical investigation of modafinil are between
et al., 2009; Zola et al., 2013). These two paradigms do not only
the Turner et al. (2004) and Goetghebeur and Dias (2009) stud-
share face validity but also construct validity because they are
ies. These two studies showed that preclinical assays can be of
temporal lobe-mediated in many species, including humans
good predictive validity.
(Buffalo et al., 1999; Crutcher et al., 2009; Pihlajamaki et al.,
2004; Winters et al., 2004). It is, however, striking that no drug
studies have been published thus far using this paradigm in
humans. These studies would help to unveil the true potential
Clinical versus preclinical protocol
translational value of spontaneous object recognition. The second factor for consideration when comparing animal and
Back-translated assays are few and far between in CNS drug human studies is that of the methodology used in both. Preclinical
discovery and it is essential that more of them are successfully investigational set ups cannot always be devised to mimic clini-
developed and utilised, to facilitate better chances of success cal ones; however, it is sometimes possible to ensure similarity
in the clinic. One such assay is the attentional set-shifting task between such assessment and investigational protocols. This is

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


Goetghebeur and Swartz 3

very important for translation from animals to humans, as ele- does not ensure clinical success. In addition, rodent disease mod-
gantly presented by Professor Grace’s group (Gill et al., 2014). els must be carefully selected with respect to the patient popula-
This group investigated the impact of short antipsychotic with- tion. However, these are only models of the disorders or, indeed,
drawal, as performed in the clinical set up, in an animal model of a component aspect of the complete syndrome. This could be the
schizophrenia, and demonstrated the lack of efficacy of the main reason for lack of translatability. Therefore, testing a drug
investigational treatment when antipsychotic withdrawal is per- in different models of the target disease is important, to increase
formed before treatment is initiated. The likely hypothesis is that probability of success (Jucker, 2010). The scope of this review is
the dopamine hypersensitivity resulting from antipsychotic with- not to debate the modelling of CNS diseases in animals as these
drawal results in difficulties in developing and successfully test- are reviewed elsewhere (McGonigle, 2014; Nestler and Hyman,
ing novel antipsychotics in isolation, and renders the development 2010); however, selecting a disease model displaying some face,
of new drugs as an add-on strategy for either negative or cogni- construct and predictive validity and sharing similarities, not
tive symptoms of schizophrenia more attractive. However, for only behavioural ones, but also at a cellular and biochemical
add-on therapy, this would require preclinical testing of new level, with the targeted patient population is important for subse-
compounds as an add-on to a relevant antipsychotic subchronic quent clinical testing. Patient stratification then becomes an
or chronic treatment. Publications describing this approach are important factor, yet it is very often neglected. For successful
currently very scarce or non-existent. drug development one may consider the benefit derived from
One of the main and recurrent issues in preclinical testing is investigating drug effect in multiple disease models for a given
the lack of complete understanding as to what the assays really disease from which positive results will then guide a clinical trial
measure and the potential impact of other behaviour(s) on the in a specific patient population(s) – not only for phase II, but also
measure of interest in the considered assay. This is why: (a) sin- phase III, clinical studies.
gle use of behavioural assays (e.g. marble burying test; forced Notwithstanding, the RDoC initiative is paving the way to a
swim test) potentially responding to a specific mechanism of different approach towards diseases, investigating symptoms and
action should be avoided for proof of efficacy; and (b) thorough associated circuitries rather than diseases as defined by the DSM-
behavioural investigation of the effect of a drug in relevant V. Thus modelling of specific symptoms in animals rather than a
behavioural assays is necessary to rule out any other masking disorder (see Fernando and Robbins, 2011, for review) is becom-
behaviour(s). Obviously behavioural homology between humans ing more attractive and more realistic at the preclinical level. This
and animals, namely behavioural similarities observed across approach could bring preclinical and clinical research closer
species manifesting as a similar behavioural pattern, resulting together, making the preclinical approaches more translational
from outputs from a specific shared brain structure (Roberts, and thereby proving to be a better approach for clinical success.
1996) or gene/gene pathway (Reuame and Sokolowski, 2011) is Another important aspect to consider is that the clinical meas-
a prerequisite. Moreover, one could be even more stringent and urement of functional outcome is frequently achieved by using
use only tasks in which learning is similar (Mennenga et al., questionnaires or test batteries that generate a composite score.
2014, Possin et al., 2016; Woolley et al., 2013). Indeed, it is obvious that a better overall functional outcome is
One of the major initiatives in the neuroscience field for achievable if multiple impaired behavioural domains are
enhancing translational research has been the cognitive neuro- addressed rather than just one. In addition, the present regulatory
science treatment research to improve cognition in schizophre- framework dictates that drugs cannot be approved on the basis of
nia (CNTRICS) (Carter et al., 2008). This has led to the selection a positive or significant impact on one, or even several, cognitive
of defined preclinical tools (imaging, biomarker and behavioural domains. However, the recent US Food and Drug Administration
assays) aligned with the clinical initiative equivalent: measure- approval of vortioxetine for certain aspects of cognitive dysfunc-
ment and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophre- tion in major depressive disorder (Fiore, 2016) may signal a new,
nia (MATRICS) (Green et al., 2004; Marder and Fenton, 2004). and more encouraging, regulatory approach, that may further
The CNTRICS initiative gives a theoretical preclinical road map encourage the use of cognitive and/or behavioural stratification
for the development of drugs designed to improve cognition in in phase III or pivotal studies.
schizophrenia; however, in the authors’ view, it is very unlikely Historically, a licence is only granted if a significant and rel-
that the industry, in the current climate, will adhere preclinically evant improvement in patients’ global outcome can be shown,
to this road map to facilitate cutting down drug development with a clear and definite demonstration of clinical meaningful-
time. Although the MATRICS initiative has successfully pro- ness and impact on quality of life measures. These complex per-
vided a brief battery of cognitive assessment tools to evaluate formance and functional outcomes are not measured by any
key cognitive domains in schizophrenia, several additional single assay. It is therefore necessary preclinically to assess the
domains of function impaired in patients with schizophrenia potential effect of a drug on various subdomains as one given
could not be categorised according to the MATRICS domains. In drug will have different effects on different behavioural domains,
addition, these initiatives only cover specific Diagnostic and with an efficacious dose in one given domain being potentially
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-V-defined dis- very different from another one.
eases; therefore, initiatives covering other diseases or symptom In this respect, Canal and Imbimbo (1996) investigated the
complexes are needed, but remain elusive. Rather, initiatives effect of the acetylcholinesterase (AchE) inhibitor eptastigmine
looking at behavioural domains in a disease agnostic way, like on different cognitive tests in patients with probable Alzheimer’s
the research domain criteria (RDoC) initiative, may prove to be disease, and reported a differential effect of AchE inhibition on
more relevant. these domains. In that study, an effect on logical memory and
This scrupulous alignment of assays and protocols between semantic word fluency is seen at 30% AchE inhibition, whereas
the clinical and preclinical context is a prerequisite; yet it still an effect on trail making B is seen only at 40% inhibition. Hence,

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


4 Journal of Psychopharmacology

a different dose response curve can be delineated for each differ- Mishkin, 1998; Arnsten et al., 1994; Castner et al., 2014; Eddins
ent cognitive subdomain in response to the same drug therapy at et al., 2014; Matsuoka and Aigner, 1996; Vijayraghavan et al.,
the same efficacious dose. Müller and colleagues (2012) 2007) and rodents (Braida et al., 1996; Clark et al., 1998;
described the effect of the antipsychotic amisulpiride on several Hosseini-Sharifabad et al., 2012; Salehi et al., 2010; Stoiljkovic
symptoms and similarly demonstrated differential modulation. et al., 2015; Uslaner et al., 2009). It is important to note that the
Recently Fallon et al. (2015) demonstrated the inverted U-shape described inverted U-shape dose–response curves are not due to
nature of two different cognitive tasks (investigating two cogni- the appearance of other drug-related competitive behaviours or
tive domains) in Parkinson’s disease patients and, of equal toxic effects.
importance, a differential optimal dopamine levels for these two These biphasic curves are not assay or domain dependent
tasks. The same phenomenon can be found preclinically. Berridge because they have been described for executive function in an
and colleagues (2012) demonstrated differential effects of a dose attentional set-shifting paradigm (Jones et al., 2014), working
of methylphenidate in rats on two cognitive domains, namely memory (Berridge et al., 2012), spatial memory (Park et al.,
sustained attention and working memory. Therefore, drug devel- 2010; Wise and Lichtman, 2007), social investigation (Gacsályi
opment should take into consideration not only one, but several, et al., 2013), anxiety (Foreman et al., 2009; Hendrie et al., 1993;
domains impacted in a given disease when considering optimum Vaidya et al., 2005) and visual memory (Horiguchi et al., 2013;
dose selection. Uslaner et al., 2009), among many others.
Various classes of drugs including dopamine agonists
(Horiguchi et al., 2013; Khroyan et al., 1995), acetylcholine ester-
Dose selection: the Yerkes–Dodson ase inhibitors (Braida et al., 1996; Van Dam et al., 2005), alpha7
nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonists (Jones et al., 2014), neu-
principle ropeptide Y (Flood et al., 1987), metabotropic glutamate receptor
To complicate the picture further, the dose–effect relationship is 5 positive allosteric modulators (Uslaner et al., 2009), NMDA
very often non-linear within the CNS, as well as in the periphery. receptor potentiators (Matsuoka and Aigner, 1996), histamine H3
The concept of the inverted U-shape comes from animal studies receptor antagonists (Esbenshade et al., 2012), interleukin-1
and was first demonstrated in 1908 by Yerkes and Dodson (1908) (Goshen et al., 2007), pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating poly-
in their ‘dancing mice’ experiment, in which they established the peptide hormone (Sacchetti et al., 2001), phosphodiesterase type
biphasic effect of arousal on cognition. This principle established 5 inhibitors (Prickaerts et al., 2005) and others (Quevedo et al.,
a relationship between stress and performance whereby stress 1998) have demonstrated a similar effect in rodents.
improves performance to a certain level, after which performance These biphasic dose–response curves can further be explained
then decreases. Clinical investigation has continued to demon- at the cellular or molecular level. For example, AchE inhibitors
strate this effect in humans (de Veld et al., 2014; Ossewaarde modulate the cognitive signal in a biphasic manner in humans
et al., 2011; Schilling et al., 2013) and this paradigm is now well (Canal and Imbimbo, 1996) and rodents (Godley et al., 2014). At
recognised. Further clinical evidence has shown that numerous a cellular level donepezil modulates long-term potentiation of
drugs can exert a biphasic effect on different and various behav- population spike in the CA1 region of rat hippocampal slices in a
ioural domains, although these data are scarce given the cost of similar fashion (Kapai et al., 2012), providing a rationale for this
testing multiple drug doses of a drug in experimental settings phenomenon at the supra-behavioural level.
(Canal and Imbimbo, 1996; Fallon et al., 2015). Antihypertensive Glycine transporter 1 (GlyT1) inhibitors are also shown to
medication (Peng et al., 2014; Razay et al., 2009), cholinergic exert an inverted U-shape dose–response curve upon cognition in
modulators (Canal and Imbimbo, 1996; Poltavski et al., 2012; rhesus monkeys (Castner et al., 2014; Eddins et al., 2014) and
Soncrant et al., 1993; Wilens et al., 2006), antipsychotics (Müller rodents (Harada et al., 2012). To the authors’ knowledge, limited
et al., 2012; Santos et al., 1989), dopaminergic agents (Chowdhury cognitive clinical data with GlyT1 inhibitors have been published
et al., 2012; Tipper et al., 2005) and antidepressants (Ericksen, (Tsai et al., 2014); only the effect on negative symptoms of schiz-
1979) are all shown to modulate various clinical endpoints ophrenia indicating an inverted U-shape dose–response curve has
according to an inverted U-shape dose–response curve. been demonstrated. Recently Spiros et al. (2014) investigated the
Bridging clinical and preclinical research, Soncrant et al. effect of glycine upon negative symptoms of schizophrenia using
(1993) tested the effect of the non-selective muscarinic acetyl- a computer-based quantitative system pharmacological approach
choline receptor agonist arecoline, administered as an infusion, and found an inverted U-shape dose response, with low dose gly-
in Alzheimer’s disease patients and demonstrated an inverted cine being optimal. However, and in disagreement with the view
U-shape relationship between the dose of the drug and verbal of this paper’s authors on the lack of predictive validity of animal
memory. A biphasic effect of arecoline was later demonstrated models, GlyT1 inhibition showed an inverted U-shape in animal
(Bartolomeo et al., 1997) in the rat in a spatial working memory models as referenced above. Moreover, at a non-behavioural
assay. In a similar fashion, the AchE inhibitor eptastigmine level GlyT1 inhibitors do modulate excitatory postsynaptic cur-
showed a biphasic effect upon the mini mental state examination rent in a biphasic manner, again providing a rationale for the
(MMSE) score and several cognitive domains (Canal and observed behaviour (Alberati et al., 2012). The same could apply
Imbimbo, 1996). Others have demonstrated such a biphasic for the putative cognition enhancer H3 antagonist, as it displays
curve preclinically with other AchE inhibitors in primates (Ou biphasic modulation of rodent cognition (Esbenshade et al.,
et al., 1997) and rodents (Braida et al., 1996, 1998; Flood et al., 2012) and the hippocampal neural cell adhesion molecule poly-
1981; Waite and Thal, 1995; Wanibuchi et al., 1994). sialylation state (Foley et al., 2009).
Indeed, this phenomenon has been shown preclinically in a Preclinically, other biological entities or phenomena such as
number of species including non-human primates (Aigner and neurotransmitter modulation (Huang et al., 2014), brain-derived

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


Goetghebeur and Swartz 5

neurotrophic factor levels (Mamounas et al., 2000), electrophysi- Finally, it is also crucial that the specific biological abnormal-
ological assessments (Hiyoshi et al., 2014; Stoiljkovic et al., ity at a given stage in a disease process is considered. As neuro-
2015), long-term depression (Zhang et al., 2008), long-term degenerative disorders progress, the neuronal architecture is
potentiation (Kroker et al., 2011; Otani et al., 2015), oestrogens progressively eroded, such that the potential target upon which a
(Luine and Frankfurt, 2012) and N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor drug acts may change in structure or biochemical make-up, or
subunits (Uslaner et al., 2009) display a biphasic modulation in even entirely disappear. Hence, it is important to consider that a
relation to the dose of a drug. drug effect may interact with, or be dependent upon, pathological
In summary, evaluating a drug effect upon a specific behav- processes during the progression of a disease and vary according
ioural domain between species or between laboratories in the to the disease stage or severity. The dose–response curve of a
same species should carefully take into consideration the dosage given agent may therefore change as a disease progresses, such
being assessed and preferably test a broad range of doses. It is that therapeutic benefit is only observable at a given severity or
especially important to be cognisant that, as a consequence of pathological stage of the disease. Parkinson’s disease, and the
this effect, higher drug concentrations or doses do not always change in responsiveness to dopamine replacement therapies, is a
(and indeed, rarely) mean a better response. This inverted good example of such a disorder demonstrating this effect.
U-shape could therefore explain many of the contradictory find- However, it is not only the change in response to l-dopa respon-
ings reported in the scientific literature. siveness, but evidence of differential frontostriatal and visuospa-
The preceding discussion has concentrated on describing the tial cognitive impairment at differing disease stages, which
inverted U-shape relative to one specific domain. However, over- exemplifies this (Owen et al., 1992, 1993). Hence, the need for a
all individual functionality is composed of many behavioural specific therapeutic intervention will change during the course of
domains that could be potentially impacted in a given disease. the disorder, depending on the disease stage.
Neurotransmitters or drugs could modulate various behavioural This review has mainly focused on behavioural aspects of
domains, but not in a similar way or at the same dosage. The preclinical and clinical research because the endpoint(s) of clini-
interrelations between the different inverted U-shape curves cal studies are ultimately amelioration of the symptoms.
should be taken into detailed consideration when effectively tar- However, in addition to the discussion above, differences in phar-
geting a positive functional outcome in the clinic. Very few pre- macokinetic parameters between animals and humans, target
clinical studies investigate the effect of a drug upon various engagement measures (e.g. electroencephalogram, magnetic res-
cognitive domains using the same species, strain and disease onance imaging, positron emission tomography occupancy data
model across a wide range of doses. Modulation of only one spe- and/or other imaging parameters and biochemical readouts) in
cific behavioural domain is often the rule for compound progres- both animals and humans whenever possible must also be part of
sion in the clinic and, if several domains are investigated, the the decision process to progress a drug further into the clinic.
relationship between them is rarely further assessed. A full Indeed, it is essential (but somehow often remains elusive in the
behavioural profile of the drug is necessary preclinically to current state of knowledge of numerous CNS diseases) to obtain
ensure increased likelihood of clinical success. a reliable biomarker and to show engagement of the target and
A positive functional outcome in the clinic may only be circuitry(ies). This could be further linked to the optimal effec-
observed if the potential for multiple (and non-aligned) inverted tive dose if the drug showed an inverted U-shape dose response.
U-shapes are taken into consideration. As elegantly demon- All these aspects need to be taken into account when optimising
strated by Hills and Hertwig (2011), an optimum gain on behav- dose selection in the clinic. For example, Bakker et al. (2015)
ioural domain A could have a high impact on behavioural recently showed that levetiracetam enhanced cognition in mild
domain B if the two biphasic dose–response curves are not cognitive impairment patients consistent with an inverted
overlapping. Methylphenidate shows a biphasic modulation of U-shape dose–response curve, while the functional MRI signal
both these cognitive subdomains (Berridge et al., 2012). followed the same pattern.
However, these curves do not overlap. While the optimal dose
of the drug for working memory is 0.5 mg/kg, the optimum
dose for sustained attention is 2 mg/kg. If this drug is to be used
From phase II to phase III
to improve the overall symptoms of a disease in which both The genesis of a drug starts several years, if not decades, before
working memory and attentional processes are perturbed, dose its potential launch and should be based on a strong scientific
selection would need to be tightly defined in order not to exceed hypothesis. Preclinical research brings together all the scientific
the dose at which working memory is no longer positively disciplines in order to establish proof of mechanism and, ulti-
responsive. mately, proof of concept (POC) in animals, thereby understand-
Furthermore, it is not only the positive modulation of dys- ing the biology of the target while, potentially, finding relevant
functional behavioural domain(s) by the drug that should be biomarkers that can be taken into the clinic to define the respon-
addressed, but also the potential of a novel compound to impact sive patient population or aid in predicting a therapeutic
negatively on some domains. There could feasibly be a dose response. As the compound is progressed into humans, tradition-
range above which a drug would negatively modulate one or sev- ally from healthy volunteers into the targeted patient population,
eral behavioural domains, thus impacting on the overall benefit POC must be demonstrated by the end of phase II to facilitate
of the treatment. A primary example of such complex inverted confidence in moving into the larger, registration studies of
U-shape interrelations has been demonstrated with the neuro- phase III. As discussed above, a selected compound that is likely
transmitter dopamine: dopamine modulates circuitries differen- to demonstrate a biphasic dose response preclinically and clini-
tially with different optimum responses (Fallon et al., 2015; cally, potentially impacts positively upon deficiencies in one or
Fern-Pollak et al., 2004; Metha et al, 2001). several specific behavioural domains (but not all) and is linked

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


6 Journal of Psychopharmacology

to a strong scientific rationale. All these factors define the spe- later, clinical studies, rather than (as is so often the case cur-
cific clinical population that the drug should target. rently) ignoring or overlooking the signals generated by such
Yet many companies may prematurely or inappropriately work. The RDoC approach, in which ‘dimensionality’ of an ill-
embark on the phase IIb/III clinical trials setting based on mar- ness characterises its definition, rather than the use of standard-
ginal efficacy readouts, inappropriate subgroup findings, ineffi- ised and, in some instances, out-dated diagnostic features, is one
cient data analyses and questionable conclusions generated by that many in the field are beginning to advocate (Adam, 2013).
both preclinical and preceding clinical studies. It is also worrying In conclusion, preclinical research plays an important part in
that many CNS development programmes do not truly benefit facilitating the progress of a compound towards final marketing
from the POC studies that are run. POC studies are often designed as an approved and effective drug. The failure of clinical trials in
and powered as potential back-up registration trials, but should CNS drug development may not always reside in the lack of pre-
take the form of a precise innovative experiment that specifically dictive validity of animal models and assays, but in the minimal
addresses one or two major objectives in a rigorous manner and, preclinical package provided prior to entering the clinical phase.
often, in enriched patient groups. The use of targeted patient Back-translation is obviously an important part of preclinical
population/s with a specific pattern of cognitive (and/or behav- research but focus should be on better understanding the dose
ioural) deficits, should be selected in these early studies, to assess effect of a given drug on multiple behaviours for a given disease.
whether an effective signal can be generated, confirming proof of In addition, patient stratification, which is currently uncommon
drug mechanism and giving confidence to allow progression into particularly in phase III clinical studies, becomes a greater neces-
larger registration studies. However, care should be taken that the sity. Finally, measuring specific behavioural domains in the
relevant positive efficacy signal/s observed in such POC studies clinic, and potentially in a disease agnostic way, by selecting a
are not subsumed in a broader, significantly more heterogeneous relevant patient population according to deficiencies in specific
population, in which the deficits targeted by the drug are no behavioural domains and not according to the diagnostic criteria
longer present or are not seminal characteristics of the population of the DSM-V, much like the way in which preclinical research
being studied. The mechanistic targeting of a specific symptom assesses behaviour in its animal models, might be the way for-
in a particular patient subgroup should guide the clinical approach ward. In this respect, approaches like those advocated by the
(and, indeed, the regulatory pathway), rather than the current and RDoC initiative from the NIMH is a highly relevant exemplar
historical model in which very large, expensive phase III studies and might steer CNS drug discovery into a new and more suc-
are conducted, in which the link between intended drug response cessful era.
and clinical outcome measure is likely to be lost or diluted.
This means that in many phase III programmes in CNS clini- Acknowledgements
cal development, few distinctions are made when selecting
The authors would like to thank Dr Sarah Almond, Dr Rebecca Dias and
patients for study participation in terms of specific behavioural Dr Gavin Kilpatrick for their critical reviews of the manuscript.
domains impacted, the extent of the deficit or the duration of the
disease. The ‘one size fits all’ approach that has traditionally
driven phase III trials in the CNS area aligns similarly to devel-
Declaration of conflicting interests
oping a cancer drug to cure all cancers, irrespective of their geno- The authors declared the following potential conflicts of interest with
type, size and location. Indeed, this is not the approach adopted respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: both
in oncology research. Therefore, the progression of a drug from authors were employees of Takeda Pharmaceutical Company Ltd. when
this paper was written.
phase II to phase III can result in a dramatic dilution of the origi-
nal underlying scientific rationale with consequent loss of the
measured ‘signal’, which might help to explain some of the fail- Funding
ures of these drugs to reach the market. The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship,
Perhaps the best approach from a clinical development per- and/or publication of this article.
spective, CNS should embrace the use of more concrete and rel-
evant endpoints in clinical trials. Thomas Insel, the director of the References
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) in the United States Abbott A (2011) Novartis to shut brain research facility. Nature 480:
recently stated: ‘Unlike our definitions of ischaemic heart dis- 161–162.
ease, lymphoma or AIDS, the DSM diagnoses are based on a Adam D (2013) On the spectrum. Nature 496: 416–418.
consensus about clusters of clinical symptoms, not any objective Aigner TG and Mishkin M. (1998) Improved recognition memory in
laboratory measure. In the rest of medicine, this would be equiva- monkeys following naloxone administration. Psychopharmacology
lent to creating diagnostic systems based on the nature of chest 94: 21–23.
pain or the quality of fever’ (Insel, 2013). It is telling that, despite Akhtar A (2015) The flaws and human harms of animal experimentation.
the advancements made over decades of successful pharmaceuti- Camb Q Health Ethics 24: 407–419.
cal research and drug development, clinical trial assessment in Alberati D, Moreau JL, Lengyel J, et al. (2012) Glycine reuptake inhibi-
tor RG1678: a pharmacologic characterization of an investigational
CNS studies still relies on symptom manuals, some of which
agent for the treatment of schizophrenia. Neuropharmacology 62:
were written nearly half a century ago. A compelling and urgent 1152–1161.
need for more discriminatory tools is driving progress in these Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Murphy BL, et al. (1994) Dopamine D1 receptor
neuropsychiatric disorders – but we still have to make significant mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged
progress (Skripka-Serry, 2013). It is essential that clinicians use monkeys. Psychopharmacology 116: 143–151.
the data generated from their preclinical colleagues and the ani- Bakker A, Albert MS, Krauss G, et al. (2015) Response of the medial
mal models they employ to guide both small and large, early and temporal lobe network in amnestic mild cognitive impairment to

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


Goetghebeur and Swartz 7

therapeutic intervention assessed by fMRI and memory task perfor- Ericksen SE (1979) Recent advances in antidepressant drug treatment.
mance. Neuroimage Clin 7: 688–698. West J Med 131: 104–113.
Bartolomeo AC, Morris H and Boast CA (1997) Arecoline via minios- Esbenshade TA, Browman KE, Miller TR, et al. (2012) Pharmacological
motic pump improves AF64A-impaired radial maze performance in properties and procognitive effects of ABT-288, a potent and selec-
rats: a possible model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Learn Mem tive histamine H3 receptor antagonist. J. Pharmacol Exp Ther 343:
68: 333–342. 233–245.
Berridge CW, Shumsky JS, Andrzejewski ME, et al. (2012) Differen- Fallon SJ, Smulders K, Esselink RA, et al. (2015) Differential optimal
tial sensitivity to psychostimulants across prefrontal cognitive tasks: dopamine levels for set-shifting and working memory in Parkinson’s
differential involvement of noradrenergic α1- and α2-receptors. Biol disease. Neuropsychologia 77: 42–51.
Psychiatry 71: 467–473. Fernando AB and Robbins TW (2011) Animal models of neuropsychiat-
Braida D, Paladini E, Griffini P, et al. (1996) An inverted U-shaped curve ric disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 7: 39–61.
for heptylphysostigmine on radial maze performance in rats: com- Fern-Pollak L, Whone AL, Brooks DJ, et al. (2004) Cognitive and motor
parison with other cholinesterase inhibitors. Eur J Pharmacol 302: effects of dopaminergic medication withdrawal in Parkinson’s dis-
13–20. ease. Neuropsychologia 42: 1917–1926.
Braida D, Paladini E, Griffini P, et al. (1998) Long-lasting antiamnesic Finke K, Dodds CM, Bublak P, et al. (2010) Effects of modafinil and
effect of a novel anticholinesterase inhibitor (MF268). Pharmacol methylphenidate on visual attention capacity: a TVA-based study.
Biochem Behav 59: 897–901. Psychopharmacology 210: 317–329.
Buffalo EA, Ramus SJ, Clark RE, et al. (1999) Dissociation between the Fiore K (2016) FDA Advisors Support New Indication for Brintellix
effects of damage to perirhinal cortex and area TE. Learn Mem 6: – Panel debates vortioxetine for treating cognitive dysfunction in
572–599. major depression. Available at: http://www.medpagetoday.com/Psy-
Burgess HA and Granato M. (2007) Sensorimotor gating in larval zebraf- chiatry/Depression/55999.
ish. J Neurosci 27: 4984–4994. Flood JF, Landry DW and Jarvik ME. (1981) Cholinergic receptor inter-
Canal N and Imbimbo BP (1996) Relationship between pharmacody- actions and their effects on long-term memory processing. Brain Res
namic activity and cognitive effects of eptastigmine in patients with 215: 177–185.
Alzheimer’s disease. Eptastigmine Study Group. Clin. Pharmacol Flood JF, Hernandez EN and Morley JE. (1987) Modulation of memory
Ther 60: 218–228. processing by neuropeptide Y. Brain Res 421: 280–290.
Cano SJ, Posner HB, Moline ML, et al. (2010) The ADAS-cog in Foley AG, Prendergast A, Barry C, et al. (2009) H3 receptor antagonism
Alzheimer’s disease clinical trials: psychometric evaluation of the enhances NCAM PSA-mediated plasticity and improves memory
sum and its parts. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 81: 1363–1368. consolidation in odor discrimination and delayed match-to-position
Carroll J (2013) Updated: Baxter adds Gammagard to growing roster of paradigms. Neuropsychopharmacology 34: 2585–2600.
PhIII Alzheimer’s failures. Available at: http://www.fiercebiotech. Foreman MM, Hanania T and Eller M. (2009) Anxiolytic effects of
com/story/baxter-adds-gammagard-growing-roster-phiii-alzheim- lamotrigine and JZP-4 in the elevated plus maze and in the four plate
ers-failures/2013-05-07. conflict test. Eur J Pharmacol 602: 316–320.
Carter CS, Barch DM, Buchanan RW, et al. (2008) Identifying cognitive Gacsályi I, Nagy K, Pallagi K, et al. (2013) Egis-11150: a candidate anti-
mechanisms targeted for treatment development in schizophrenia: an psychotic compound with procognitive efficacy in rodents. Neuro-
overview of the first meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Treat- pharmacology 64: 254–263.
ment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia Initiative. Gallhofer B, Jaanson P, Mittoux A, et al. (2007) Course of recovery of
Biol. Psychiatry 64: 4–10. cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia: a randomised
Castner SA, Murthy NV, Ridler K, et al. (2014) Relationship between double-blind study comparing sertindole and haloperidol. Pharma-
glycine transporter 1 inhibition as measured with positron emission copsychiatry 40: 275–286.
tomography and changes in cognitive performances in nonhuman Gill KM, Cook JM, Poe MM, et al. (2014) Prior antipsychotic drug treat-
primates. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 2742–2749. ment prevents response to novel antipsychotic agent in the methyl-
Chowdhury R, Guitart-Masip M, Bunzeck N, et al. (2012) Dopamine azoxymethanol acetate model of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 40:
modulates episodic memory persistence in old age. J Neurosci 32: 341–350.
14193–14204. Godley A, Havolli E, Hill M, et al. (2014) Alzheimer’s disease therapeu-
Clark KB, Smith DC, Hassert DL, et al. (1998) Posttraining electrical tics in a pre-clinical spatial recognition paradigm: further evidence
stimulation of vagal afferents with concomitant vagal efferent inac- for cognition-related bell-shaped dose response curve. J Psycho-
tivation enhances memory storage processes in the rat. Neurobiol pharmacol 28: A16, MA09.
Learn Mem 70: 364–373. Goetghebeur P and Dias R (2009) Comparison of haloperidol, risperi-
Crutcher MD, Calhoun-Haney R, Manzanares CM, et al. (2009) Eye done, sertindole, and modafinil to reverse an attentional set-shifting
tracking during a visual paired comparison task as a predictor of impairment following subchronic PCP administration in the rat – a
early dementia. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 24: 258–266. back translational study. Psychopharmacology 202: 287–293.
Csomor PA, Preller KH, Geyer MA, et al. (2014) Influence of aripip- Goetghebeur P, Lerdrup L, Sylvest A, et al. (2010) Erythropoi-
razole, risperidone, and amisulpride on sensory and sensorimotor etin reverses the attentional set-shifting impairment in a rodent
gating in healthy ‘low and high gating’ humans and relation to psy- schizophrenia disease-like model. Psychopharmacology 212:
chometry. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 2485–2496. 635–642.
de Veld DM, Riksen-Walraven JM and de Weerth C (2014) Acute psy- Goff DC, Lamberti JS, Leon AC, et al. (2008) A placebo-controlled add-
chosocial stress and children’s memory. Stress 17: 305–313. on trial of the Ampakine, CX516, for cognitive deficits in schizo-
Eddins D, Hamill TG, Puri V, et al. (2014) The relationship between phrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 465–472.
glycine transporter 1 occupancy and the effects of the glycine Goshen I, Kreisel T, Ounallah-Saad H, et al. (2007) A dual role for inter-
transporter 1 inhibitor RG1678 or ORG25935 on object retrieval leukin-1 in hippocampal-dependent memory processes. Psychoneu-
performance in scopolamine impaired rhesus monkey. Psychophar- roendocrinology 32: 1106–1115.
macology 231: 511–519. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Gold JM, et al. (2004) Approaching a
Ehrenreich H, Hinze-Selch D, Stawicki S, et al. (2007) Improvement of consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: the
cognitive functions in chronic schizophrenic patients by recombinant NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test
human erythropoietin. Mol Psychiatry 12: 206–220. criteria. Biol Psychiatry 56: 301–307.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


8 Journal of Psychopharmacology

Harada K, Nakato K, Yarimizu J, et al. (2012) A novel glycine trans- Luine VN and Frankfurt M. (2012) Estrogens facilitate memory process-
porter-1 (GlyT1) inhibitor, ASP2535 (4-[3- isopropyl-5- (6-phenyl- ing through membrane mediated mechanisms and alterations in spine
3-pyridyl)-4H-1,2,4- triazol-4-yl]-2,1,3-benzoxadiazole), improves density. Front Neuroendocrinol 33: 388–402.
cognition in animal models of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia Ma J, Mufti A and Stan Leung L (2015) Effects of memantine on hip-
and Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Pharmacol 685: 59–69. pocampal long-term potentiation, gamma activity, and sensorimotor
Hashimoto K (2005) Targeting of α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in gating in freely moving rats. Neurobiol Aging 36: 2544–2554.
the treatment of schizophrenia and the use of auditory sensory gating McGonigle P (2014) Animal models of CNS disorders. Biochem Phar-
as a translational biomarker. Curr Pharm Des 21: 3797–3806. macol 87: 140–149.
Hendrie CA, Neill JC, Shepherd JK, et al. (1993) The effects of CCKA Mamounas LA, Altar CA, Blue ME, et al. (2000) BDNF promotes the
and CCKB antagonists on activity in the black/white exploration regenerative sprouting, but not survival, of injured serotonergic
model of anxiety in mice. Physiol Behav 54: 689–693. axons in the adult rat brain. J Neurosci 20: 771–782.
Hills T and Hertwig R (2011) Why aren’t we smarter already: evolution- Marder SR and Fenton W (2004) Measurement and Treatment Research
ary trade-offs and cognitive enhancements. Curr Direct Psycholog to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia: NIMH MATRICS initia-
Sci 20: 373–377. tive to support the development of agents for improving cognition in
Hiyoshi T, Kambe D, Karasawa J, et al. (2014) Differential effects of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 72: 5–9.
NMDA receptor antagonists at lower and higher doses on basal Matsuoka N and Aigner TG (1996) D-cycloserine, a partial agonist at
gamma band oscillation power in rat cortical electroencephalograms. the glycine site coupled to N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors, improves
Neuropharmacology 85: 384–396. visual recognition memory in rhesus monkeys. J Pharmacol Exp
Horiguchi M, Hannaway KE, Adelekun AE, et al. (2013) D(1) receptor Ther 278: 891–897.
agonists reverse the subchronic phencyclidine (PCP)-induced novel Matthews RA (2008) Medical progress depends on animal models –
object recognition (NOR) deficit in female rats. Behav Brain Res doesn’t it? J R Soc Med 101: 95–98.
238: 36–43. Mehta MA, Swainson R, Ogilvie AD, et al. (2001) Improved short-term
Hosseini-Sharifabad A, Ghahremani MH, Sabzevari O, et al. (2012) spatial memory but impaired reversal learning following the dopa-
Effects of protein kinase A and G inhibitors on hippocampal cho- mine D(2) agonist bromocriptine in human volunteers. Psychophar-
linergic markers expressions in rolipram- and sildenafil-induced macology 159: 10–20.
spatial memory improvement. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 101: Mennenga SE, Baxter LC, Grunfeld IS, et al. (2014) Navigating to new
311–319. frontiers in behavioral neuroscience: traditional neuropsychologi-
Huang M, Felix AR, Kwon S, et al. (2014) The alpha-7 nicotinic receptor cal tests predict human performance on a rodent-inspired radial-arm
partial agonist/5-HT3 antagonist RG3487 enhances cortical and hip- maze. Front Behav Neurosci 2014: 294.
pocampal dopamine and acetylcholine release. Psychopharmacology Miller G (2010) Is pharma running out of brainy ideas? Science 329:
231: 2199–2210. 502–504.
Insel T (2013) Director’s Blog: Transforming Diagnosis. Available at: Morein-Zamir S, Turner DC and Sahakian BJ (2007) A review of the
http://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/director/2013/transforming-diagno- effects of modafinil on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull
sis.shtml. 33: 1298–1306.
Jones KM, McDonald IM, Bourin C, et al. (2014) Effect of alpha7 Müller MJ, Wetzel H, Eich FX, et al. (2012) Amisulpride Study Group.
nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonists on attentional set-shifting Dose-related effects of amisulpride on five dimensions of psychopa-
impairment in rats. Psychopharmacology 231: 673–683. thology in patients with acute exacerbation of schizophrenia. J Clin
Jucker M (2010) The benefits and limitations of animal models for Psychopharmacol 22: 554–560.
translational research in neurodegenerative diseases. Nat Med 16: Nestler EJ and Hyman SE (2010) Animal models of neuropsychiatric
1210–1214. disorders. Nat Neurosci 13: 1161–1169.
Kaitin KI and Milne CP (2011) A dearth of new meds: Drugs to treat Ossewaarde L, Qin S, Van Marle HJ, et al. (2011) Stress-induced reduction
neuropsychiatric disorders have become too risky for big pharma. in reward-related prefrontal cortex function. Neuroimage 55: 345–352.
Scientific American 305: 16. Otani S, Bai J and Blot K (2015) Dopaminergic modulation of synaptic
Kapai NA, Bukanova JV, Solntseva EI, et al. (2012) Donepezil in a nar- plasticity in rat prefrontal neurons. Neurosci Bull 31: 183–190.
row concentration range augments control and impaired by beta- Ou LY, Tang XC and Cai JX (2001) Effect of huperzine A on working
amyloid peptide hippocampal LTP in NMDAR-independent manner. memory in reserpine- or yohimbine-treated monkeys. Eur J Phar-
Cell Mol Neurobiol 32: 219–226. macol 433: 151–156.
Khroyan TV, Baker DA and Neisewander JL. (1995) Dose-dependent Owen AM, James M, Leigh PN, et al. (1992) Fronto-striatal cognitive def-
effects of the D3-preferring agonist 7-OH-DPAT on motor behaviors icits at different stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain 115: 1727–1751.
and place conditioning. Psychopharmacology 122: 351–357. Owen AM, Beksinska M, James M, et al. (1993) Visuospatial memory
Kidwell CS, Liebeskind DS, Starkman S, et al. (2001) Trends in acute deficits at different stages of Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia
ischemic stroke trials through the 20th century. Stroke 32: 1349– 31(7): 627–644.
1359. Park SJ, Kim DH, Lee IK, et al. (2010) The ameliorating effect of the
Knogler LD and Drapeau P (2014) Sensory gating of an embryonic extract of the flower of Prunella vulgaris var. lilacina on drug-induced
zebrafish interneuron during spontaneous motor behaviors. Front memory impairments in mice. Food Chem Toxicol 48: 1671–1676.
Neural Circuits 8: 121. Peng J, Lu F, Wang Z, et al. (2014) Excessive lowering of blood pressure
Kresge N (2014) Roche schizophrenia drug at risk after failing in trials. is not beneficial for progression of brain white matter hyperintensive
Available at: http://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2014-01-21/ and cognitive impairment in elderly hypertensive patients: 4-year
roche-schizophrenia-drug-at-risk-after-failing-in-trials. follow-up study. Am Med Dir Assoc 15: 904–910.
Kroker KS, Rast G and Rosenbrock H (2011) Differential effects of sub- Pihlajamaki M, Tanila H, Kononen M, et al. (2004) Visual presentation
type-specific nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonists on early and of novel objects and new spatial arrangements of objects differen-
late hippocampal LTP. Eur J Pharmacol 671: 26–32. tially activates the medial temporal lobe subareas in humans. Eur J
Larrauri JA, Burke DA, Hall BJ, et al. (2015) Role of nicotinic receptors Neurosci 19: 1939–1949.
in the lateral habenula in the attenuation of amphetamine-induced Poltavski DV, Petros TV and Holm JE (2012) Lower but not higher doses
prepulse inhibition deficits of the acoustic startle response in rats. of transdermal nicotine facilitate cognitive performance in smokers on
Psychopharmacology 232: 3009–3017. gender non-preferred tasks. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 102: 423–433.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016


Goetghebeur and Swartz 9

Possin KL, Sanchez PE, Anderson-Bergman C, et al. (2016) Cross- Tsai G, Lane HY, Yang P, et al. (2014) Glycine transporter I inhibitor,
species translation of the Morris maze for Alzheimer’s disease. N-methylglycine (sarcosine), added to antipsychotics for the treat-
J Clin Invest 126: 779–783. ment of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 55: 452–456.
Prickaerts J, Sik A, van der Staay FJ, et al. (2005) Dissociable effects of Turner DC, Clark L, Pomarol-Clotet E, et al. (2004) Modafinil improves
acetylcholinesterase inhibitors and phosphodiesterase type 5 inhibi- cognition and attentional set shifting in patients with chronic schizo-
tors on object recognition memory: acquisition versus consolidation. phrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 1363–1373.
Psychopharmacology 177: 381–390. Uslaner JM, Parmentier-Batteur S, Flick RB, et al. (2009) Dose-depen-
Quevedo J, Vianna M, Daroit D, et al. (1998) L-type voltage-dependent dent effect of CDPPB, the mGluR5 positive allosteric modulator, on
calcium channel blocker nifedipine enhances memory retention recognition memory is associated with GluR1 and CREB phosphory-
when infused into the hippocampus. Neurobiol Learn Mem 69: lation in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Neuropharmacol-
320–325. ogy 57: 531–538.
Razay G, Williams J, King E, et al. (2009) Blood pressure, dementia Vaidya AH, Rosenthal DI, Lang W, et al. (2005) Oral buspirone causes
and Alzheimer’s disease: the OPTIMA longitudinal study. Dement a shift in the dose–response curve between the elevated-plus maze
Geriatr Cogn Disord 28: 70–74. and Vogel conflict tests in Long–Evans rats: relation of brain levels
Reaume CJ and Sokolowski MB (2011) Conservation of gene of buspirone and 1-PP to anxiolytic action. Methods Find. Exp Clin
function in behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366: Pharmacol 27: 245–255.
2100–2110. Van Dam D, Abramowski D, Staufenbiel M, et al. (2005) Symptomatic
Roberts AC. (1996) Comparison of cognitive function in human and non- effect of donepezil, rivastigmine, galantamine and memantine on cogni-
human primates. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 3: 319–27. tive deficits in the APP23 model. Psychopharmacology 180: 177–190.
Sacchetti B, Lorenzini CA, Baldi E, et al. (2001) Pituitary adenylate Vijayraghavan S, Wang M, Birnbaum SG, et al. (2007) Inverted-U dopa-
cyclase-activating polypeptide hormone (PACAP) at very low dos- mine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working
ages improves memory in the rat. Neurobiol Learn Mem76: 1–6. memory. Nat Neurosci 10: 376–384.
Sadeghi-Nejad N (2012) The Lessons of Failure: What We Can Learn Waite JJ and Thal LJ (1995) The behavioral effects of heptylphyso-
from Bapineuzumab’s Blowup. Available at: http://www.forbes. stigmine on rats lesioned in the nucleus basalis. Neurosci Res 21:
com/sites/natesadeghi/2012/08/07/the-lessons-of-failure-what-we- 251–259.
can-learn-from-bapineuzumabs-blowup/#3d1cad2c5da6. Wallace TL, Ballard TM and Glavis-Bloom C (2015) Animal paradigms
Salehi B, Cordero MI and Sandi C. (2010) Learning under stress: the to assess cognition with translation to humans. Handb Exp Pharma-
inverted U-shape function revisited. Learn Mem 17: 522–530. col 228: 27–57.
Santos JL, Cabranes JA, Vazquez C, et al. (1989) Clinical response and Wanibuchi F, Nishida T, Yamashita H, et al. (1994) Characterization
plasma haloperidol levels in chronic and subchronic schizophrenia. of a novel muscarinic receptor agonist, YM796: comparison with
Biol Psychiatry 26: 381–388. cholinesterase inhibitors in in vivo pharmacological studies. Eur J
Schilling TM, Kölsch M, Larra MF, et al. (2013) For whom the bell Pharmacol 265: 151–158.
(curve) tolls: cortisol rapidly affects memory retrieval by an inverted Wilens TE, Verlinden MH, Adler LA, et al. (2006) ABT-089, a neuro-
U-shaped dose–response relationship. Psychoneuroendocrinology nal nicotinic receptor partial agonist, for the treatment of attention-
38: 1565–1572. deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: results of a pilot study. Biol
Scoriels L, Barnett JH, Soma PK, et al. (2012) Effects of modafinil on Psychiatry 59: 1065–1070.
cognitive functions in first episode psychosis. Psychopharmacology Winters BD, Forwood SE, Cowell RA, et al. (2004) Double dissociation
220: 249–258. between the effects of peri-postrhinal cortex and hippocampal lesions
Scoriels L, Jones PB and Sahakian BJ (2013) Modafinil effects on cogni- on tests of object recognition and spatial memory: heterogeneity of
tion and emotion in schizophrenia and its neurochemical modulation function within the temporal lobe. J Neurosci 24: 5901–5908.
in the brain. Neuropharmacology 64: 168–184. Wise LE and Lichtman AH (2007) The uncompetitive N-methyl-d-as-
Shuman T, Wood SC and Anagnostaras SG. (2009) Modafinil and mem- partate (NMDA) receptor antagonist memantine prolongs spatial
ory: effects of modafinil on Morris water maze learning and Pavlov- memory in a rat delayed radial-arm maze memory task. Eur J Phar-
ian fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 123: 257–266. macol 575: 98–102.
Skripka-Serry J (2013) The great neuro-pipeline brain drain (and why Witten L, Bastlund JF, Glenthøj BY, et al. (2016) Comparing pharma-
Big Pharma hasn’t given up on CNS disorders). Available at: http:// cological modulation of sensory gating in healthy humans and rats:
www.ddw-online.com/therapeutics/p216813-the-great-neuro-pipe- the effects of reboxetine and haloperidol. Neuropsychopharmacol-
line-brain-drain-(and-why-big-pharma-hasn-t-given-up-on-cns- ogy 41: 638–645.
disorders)-fall-13.html. Woolley DG, Laeremans A, Gantois I, et al. (2013) Homologous involve-
Soncrant TT, Raffaele KC, Asthana S, et al. (1993) Memory improve- ment of striatum and prefrontal cortex in rodent and human water
ment without toxicity during chronic, low dose intravenous arecoline maze learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110: 3131–3136.
in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacology 112: 421–427. Yang YY, Lu CL, Lo SM, et al. (2015) Early antipsychotic intervention
Spiros A, Roberts P and Geerts H (2014) A computer-based quantitative and schizophrenia. Med Hypotheses 85: 367–370.
systems pharmacology model of negative symptoms in schizophre- Yerkes RM and Dodson JD (1908) The relation of strength of stimulus
nia: exploring glycine modulation of excitation-inhibition balance. to rapidity of habit formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol 18: 459–482.
Front Pharmacol 5: 229. Zangrando J, Carvalheira R, Labbate G, et al. (2013) Atypical antipsy-
Stoiljkovic M, Leventhal L, Chen A, et al. (2015) Concentration– chotic olanzapine reversed deficit on prepulse inhibition of the acous-
response relationship of the α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor ago- tic startle reflex produced by microinjection of dizocilpine (MK-801)
nist FRM-17874 across multiple in vitro and in vivo assays. Biochem into the inferior colliculus in rats. Behav Brain Res 257: 77–82.
Pharmacol 97: 576–589. Zhang XL, Sullivan JA, Moskal JR, et al. (2008) A NMDA receptor gly-
Talledo JA, Sutherland Owens AN, Schortinghuis T, et al. (2009) cine site partial agonist, GLYX-13, simultaneously enhances LTP
Amphetamine effects on startle gating in normal women and female and reduces LTD at Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapses in hippocam-
rats. Psychopharmacology 204: 165–175. pus. Neuropharmacology 55: 1238–1250.
Tipper CM, Cairo TA, Woodward TS, et al. (2005) Processing efficiency Zola SM, Manzanares CM, Clopton P, et al. (2013) A behavioral task
of a verbal working memory system is modulated by amphetamine: predicts conversion to mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s
an fMRI investigation. Psychopharmacology 180: 634–643. disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 28: 179–184.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at Oakland University on June 5, 2016

You might also like