You are on page 1of 16

Issues in Research on Children With

Early Language Delay


Irina Tsybina
Alice Eriks-Brophy
University of Toronto, Toronto, ON

T he role of evidence-based practice is becom-


ing increasingly important. Understanding
current research findings and clarifying
research issues have become critical in helping speech-
language pathologists (SLPs) to make important clinical
achieve effective treatment for children with language
impairments, research on early language delay tackles
important and timely, yet complex, topics. A large body of
literature reflecting the findings of this research has been
created over the past two decades.
decisions. In light of the urgent need to identify preschool The purpose of the current article is to provide a
children who are at risk for academic difficulties, and to synopsis of the literature that has been generated on early
language delay in the past 15 years and to highlight the
ABSTRACT: Some children acquire new words more consistencies and inconsistencies in the literature in the
slowly and start to combine words into phrases later than following areas: (a) characteristics of children who are slow
their typically developing peers, while having no obvious to talk, (b) terminology, (c) definitions and inclusion
cognitive or sensory disabilities. These children’s criteria, (d) issues of spontaneous recovery, and (e)
language deficits are typically restricted to language
effectiveness of intervention. The current review is re-
production; the receptive language abilities in most,
stricted in scope and temporally to exclude the earlier
although not all, of these children appear to develop
normally. Therefore, these children are often character- studies of the 1980s, which were mostly epidemiological
ized as late talkers, children with early language delay, (for review of the prevalence studies, see Law, Boyle,
or children with expressive language delay. The current Harris, Harkness, & Nye, 2000). We have also chosen not
article reviews research on these children in five areas: to include studies on the validation of assessment instru-
(a) characteristics of late talkers (children with early ments, studies on children whose language delay continued
language delay), (b) terminology, (c) definitions and after reaching the age of 4 years (with the exception of
inclusion criteria, (d) issues of spontaneous recovery, and follow-up publications to the key studies), studies on
(e) effectiveness of intervention. The review makes it phonological characteristics, and studies connecting early
apparent that the evidence that has so far been generated
language deficits to further reading problems.
by the research on early language delay is mixed or
The information reviewed in this article is organized
lacking in many areas of the field, such as predicting
language outcomes for late-talking children, defining chronologically, where possible. The Appendix includes the
communication patterns of these children, and determin- studies that were reviewed for this article. The review
ing the necessity for and effectiveness of early interven- begins with the “classic” studies of the early 1990s, which
tions. Some possible directions for future research are provided the basis for subsequent research in outlining
suggested; for example, extending research to bilingual inclusion criteria and determining definitions and most of
late-talking children. the measures and methodology used in the field. These
studies have also generated quite extensive, albeit some-
KEY WORDS: expressive language delay, late talkers, what mixed, findings on the statistics and predictors of
spontaneous recovery, early language intervention,
spontaneous recovery. The information on the conversa-
caregiver–child interaction, early language delay
tional patterns of children with language delay and their

118 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION


CONTEMPORARY ISSUESSIN AND D ISORDERS
COMMUNICATION
CIENCE SCIENCE• AND
Volume 34 • 118–133
DISORDERS • Volume• 34
Fall 2007 • Fall 2007 © NSSLHA
• 118–133
1092-5171/07/3402-0118
parents has also been provided in part by some of the disabilities as well as emotional and behavioral disorders
studies of the 1990s. The intervention studies began mainly (Rescorla et al., 1997).
in the mid-1990s. Some authors of the more recent studies
have approached all of these issues as well, although the
authors of the “classic” studies of the 1990s are now
focusing on determining academic outcomes for children TERMINOLOGY
who had early language delay when they were younger, Despite the apparent wealth of literature in the field of
because their participants have now grown up and are early language delay, research on this topic has been
enrolled in school. conducted mainly by only a few groups of researchers,
each working with their own group of children who were
originally recruited mostly in the late 1980s and early
1990s and were followed up over the years. The Appendix
CHARACTERISTICS OF CHILDREN WITH provides definitions and inclusion criteria, as well as the
EARLY LANGUAGE DELAY research focus, of these groups. The studies of each group
are organized chronologically starting with the earliest.
“Late-talking” toddlers began to receive the attention of Most of these researchers have introduced their own
child language researchers in the mid-1970s and early terminology to refer to children with early language delay.
1980s. It is estimated that 10% to 15% of 2-year-olds These various terms appear quite similar yet are used to
acquire new words more slowly and start to combine words refer to groups of children who were recruited using
into phrases later than their typically developing peers, somewhat different inclusion criteria: specific expressive
showing obvious delays in language in contrast to seem- language delay, specific expressive language impairment,
ingly typical development of sensory and cognitive systems delayed expressive language, early expressive delay, slow
(Law et al., 2000). In addition, these late-talking children expressive language acquisition (development), late-
normally show gross and fine motor and self-help skills developing language, developmental expressive language
comparable to peers, and the perception of their affect and disorder, delayed onset of lexical skills, and delayed onset
mood by their parents is similar to that of parents of of expressive oral vocabulary. Almost every researcher that
typically developing children (Paul, Spangle- Looney, & has proposed his or her own term also has used the term
Dahm, 1991). According to Thal and Tobias (1992), such “late talker” synonymously with their term (as in, e.g.,
children may use the same or a higher frequency of Paul’s work); some have used this term exclusively (e.g.,
gestures than do typically developing children to compen- Girolametto). Third, although acronyms are often used, it is
sate for their limited verbal abilities. important to note that the same acronym may stand for
The acquisition of receptive vocabulary of most, although different terms. For example, SELD represents “specific
not all, late talkers is often comparable to that of normally expressive language delay” for Rescorla and her colleagues
functioning children, in contrast to remarkably slower rates and “slow expressive language development” for Paul and
of acquisition of expressive vocabulary. These children her team.
produce only a few words at a time, whereas their peers say In this article, we will use the term late talkers in its
hundreds of words and combine them into phrases broadest sense to include children under the age of 3 who
(Whitehurst, Fischel, et al., 1991). By age 2, the expressive have been identified with language delay manifested
vocabulary size of late talkers is often in the range of 20 primarily but not exclusively in the expressive domain.
words, whereas their typically developing peers normally
have expressive lexicons of approximately 200 words (Paul,
1996). Thus, the overall profile of late talkers’ language
skills does not resemble that of the typically developing INCLUSION CRITERIA
child (Paul, 2001) and is generally believed to be delayed
rather than disordered (Leonard, 1989; Miller, 1991). It is difficult to diagnose a child with a language delay
The syntactic difficulties of late talkers are first mani- before the age of 24 months because of high variability in
fested by their limited ability to produce two-word combina- the rate of vocabulary acquisition at early ages (Dale, Price,
tions at age 2 (Paul, 1996; Rescorla, 1989; Rescorla, Bishop, & Plomin, 2003; Rescorla, 1989). Whitehurst and
Roberts, & Dahlsgaard, 1997). As these children grow older, colleagues (Whitehurst, Fischel, et al., 1991) proposed that
many of them continue to manifest problems with the 24 months is the minimum age at which such a diagnosis
grammaticality of their utterances that may have an appropri- becomes possible, because at that age, the gap between
ate semantic focus but lack elaboration using appropriate children’s receptive and expressive abilities becomes
grammatic morphemes (Rescorla & Schwartz, 1990). apparent. The authors contend that another reason for the
Even in the absence of other significant cognitive, motor, validity of this age for diagnosis is that parents rarely begin
sensory, or socioemotional disabilities, an early delay in to express concerns about their children’s language at earlier
language development that is left untreated until school age ages. Therefore, most of the research in the field of early
may put a child at a very substantial risk for long-term language delay studied children who were between 1;6
language, academic, and social difficulties (Paul, 2000; (years;months) and 3;6 in age.
Rescorla, 2002; Thal & Katich, 1996). Such young children The researcher groups included in the Appendix worked
are at risk for persisting language problems and learning with essentially the same population of late-talking children,

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay 119


having the characteristics described in the first section of months) in expressive vocabulary development according to
this article. However, the inclusion criteria of some authors maternal report. In that study, this meant that late talkers
were broader than others. The population must be defined produced fewer than 65 different words and only a few
by exclusion; that is, by the absence of other mental, two-word combinations. Alternatively, when using the LDS
sensory, or emotional problems in contrast to the presence (Rescorla, 1989), one can simply count the number of
of language problems. Nonverbal intelligence measures vocabulary items that children can produce, which may be
have been used quite consistently. However, the language more efficient than computing a score. Moreover, the LDS
component of the definition has not been used with the (Rescorla, 1989) has a section in which parents may write
same degree of consistency. This has led in part to existing any words the child produces in addition to those listed.
differences in inclusion criteria, language outcomes, and, Rescorla and colleagues define children as having an
consequently, variation in the reported statistics of preva- expressive vocabulary delay if their vocabulary contains
lence and recovery, all of which are discussed below. fewer than 50 words and/or they do not use two-word
The first and main difference in the use of language utterances at 24 months of age. This assessment is roughly
criteria in defining late talkers is including children who equivalent to having scores within the 15th percentile on
presented with delays in both receptive and expressive the MCDI (Paul, 1993; Rescorla, 1989). This means that
language in contrast to children with expressive delay only. studies by Rescorla and colleagues used a more stringent
It is noteworthy that in many studies in the field, the criterion than those of Ellis Weismer and colleagues’ (10th
groups that were referred to as “children with specific percentile) and Thal and Tobias’s (65 words). The criteria
expressive language delay” included children with both used by Rescorla and colleagues seems to be useful because
types of language delays. Moreover, children with either or it was found to yield the best results in terms of sensitivity
both types of delay constituted one group, the average (minimizing “false alarms” and “misses”), as described in
outcomes of which were compared with those of typically Rescorla (1989), and because of its ease of use.
developing children. In summary, and taking into account the criteria used in
A second difference is in defining the expressive delay. a variety of research studies, to be identified as a late
There are two primary methods used to assess language and talker, children should have normal hearing, age-appropriate
vocabulary development, and, consequently, two types of global development, and a lexicon below the 10th percen-
inclusion criteria in order for a child to be diagnosed with tile for their chronological age (or fewer than 50 words).
expressive language delay. These methods differ in strin- They should also produce few or no two-word combina-
gency. The first method is based on standardized tests, tions and may or may not have age-appropriate receptive
several of which have been used in the field. In the early language abilities. When the population of late talkers is
1990s, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised defined according to these criteria, the reported prevalence
(Dunn & Dunn, 1981) was used by Whitehurst and of this language disability is 10% to 15% of all 2-year-olds
colleagues. Rescorla used the Reynell Expressive Language (Hrncir, Goldfarb, Scarr, & McCartney, 1985; Rescorla,
Scale (Reynell, 1977) in most of her studies, in addition to 1989) and 8% of all 3-year-olds (Silva, 1980).
the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (Gardner, Regardless of what are deemed to be the best criteria to
1981) in Rescorla et al. (1997). The Preschool Language define this group of children, it is important to consider the
Scale (Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 2002) was used by definitions given by the researchers when comparing the
Robertson and Weismer (1999). A child was deemed to findings of the existing studies. For future research, it may
have expressive language deficits if the child scored below be important to reach a consensus on what criteria should
the 10th percentile on any of these tests. be used to define early language delay so as to facilitate
The second method used to assess language and vocabu- the ease of comparisons across studies and to develop
lary development relies on parent report instruments. consistency in the identification of the disorder.
Although some groups of researchers used the MacArthur As for including children with receptive language delays,
Communicative Developmental Inventories (MCDI; Fenson it may be important to consider the clinical significance of
et al., 1993) as a measure of vocabulary development (e.g., separating children with early language delays into sub-
Dale group, Ellis Weismer group, Girolametto group), the groups according to the presence of both or only one type
Language Development Survey (LDS), developed by of a delay. More studies are needed to determine to what
Rescorla (1989), was used by Rescorla and Paul. The LDS extent the receptive delay affects the outcomes of treatment
has been reported to have good concurrent validity with and overall prognosis of these children. If these effects are
expressive measures on the Reynell Expressive Language limited, there may be no need for the subgroups and we
Scale (Reynell, 1977) and is proposed to be a reliable, may use the broad definition as it is has been proposed in
sensitive, and specific indicator of language delay (Paul & clinical practice.
Shiffer, 1991; Paul et al., 1991).
Unlike the administered tests listed above, parent reports Conversational Patterns
use varying methods to determine expressive delays. Based
on the norms of the MCDI (Fenson et al., 1993), a score In addition to the characteristics of late-talking children
below the 10th percentile has been used as an indicator of summarized above, it is important to consider their
a delay. For example, Thal and Tobias (1992) defined communicative patterns. However, less information is
children as having expressive language delay if their scores available in this area of research in contrast to that of the
were in the lowest 10% of their age range (18 to 28 linguistic abilities of late talkers.

120 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION S CIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 34 • 118–133 • Fall 2007
The research on the communicative patterns of late- motor routines in play, using household objects in play, and
talking children and their caregivers provides valuable smiling appropriately. Finally, Rescorla et al. (2001) found
information about the communication style of these that late-talking children and their peers produced a similar
children, which may impact the choice of language proportion of responses to their mothers’ utterances and
intervention approach. For example, an entirely child- maintained the semantic relatedness of the responses
centered approach, which may employ the provision of high equally. The only difference observed between the two
frequencies of caregivers’ responsive language strategies, groups of children was in the late talkers’ less frequent use
may not be feasible for a child who talks very rarely and of questions.
does not present enough opportunities to provide such Several studies have also examined the use of various
verbal input. In addition, research on these issues examines discourse forms by the parents of late-talking children. The
the reality of having a child with this disability in the evidence in support of the similarity between caregiver–child
family, the nature of parent–child interaction that impacts interactions of such children and those of their peers seems
on quality of life, and the amount of stress experienced by to be substantial. Paul and Ellwood (1991) demonstrated that
both the child and the parents. mothers of late talkers and mothers of typically developing
The research on conversational patterns of late-talking children were similar in their use of a variety of discourse
children and their caregivers has not been extensive and features such as declaratives, questions, commands, requests
has produced mixed results (Rescorla & Fechnay, 1996). for information, comments, topic initiations, responses to the
Although it seems that the pragmatic skills of such children child’s initiation, and topic continuations. Rescorla and
may be relatively better than their expressive skills due to Fechnay (1996) and, subsequently, Rescorla et al. (2001),
the fact that their level of comprehension is similar to that who worked with a subset of children of the former study,
of their peers (Paul, 2001), some studies have provided also discovered that mothers and children in the two groups
findings to support this assumption whereas others have did not differ in maintaining topic synchrony.
generated evidence to the contrary. However, Pearce, Girolametto, and Weitzman (1996)
Initiations (or expressions of communicative intention) suggested that although mothers of late-talking children and
are the discourse feature of late-talking children and their mothers of their peers seem to provide similar conversa-
caregivers that has been studied most extensively. This is tional opportunities to their children, these similarities may
an important strategy for a child to acquire and use because be artifacts of face-to-face interactions in laboratory
it is believed to be the foundation for all expressive settings versus natural settings. The only study exploring
communication (Paul & Shiffer, 1991). Paul and Shiffer, the communicative environment of late talkers that was
Paul et al. (1991), and Rescorla, Bascome, Lampard, and conducted in their homes is that of Whitehurst et al.
Feeny (2001) examined initiations made by late talkers and (1988). Contrary to the laboratory findings, the mothers in
their mothers. this study were more directive and used more noncontin-
Paul and Shiffer (1991) observed that late-talking gent labels. If more studies demonstrate similar findings, it
children initiated joint attention primarily through nonverbal may be important to consider optimizing parent–child
means, but not as often as their typical peers. The authors interactions in designing a family-oriented intervention
proposed that these children’s less frequent expressions of program. However, at present, it is not clear whether or not
communicative intent could be accounted for by a differ- it is necessary to focus early interventions on the interac-
ence in their expression of joint attention. The activities of tion features of caregivers.
joint attention may be important for future language Another important issue that so far has not received
development because, through them, children may learn much attention is the question of whether or not late
how adult speakers can share a topic and thus receive new talkers and their caregivers demonstrate various communi-
information (Bruner, 1975). This could be a potentially cative patterns depending on their home languages. The
important area for early intervention with late-talking only study that examined the communicative patterns of
children and merits further research. late talkers across cultures was that of Girolametto, Wiigs,
However, according to Rescorla and Fechnay (1996), late Smyth, Weitzman, and Pearce (2002). This study examined
talkers communicated as often as their peers. Similarly, cross-cultural variation in the responsiveness of English-
according to Rescorla et al. (2001), late talkers and speaking and Italian-speaking mothers of children with
typically developing children did not differ in the number early language delay. The results of the study pointed to
of topic initiations or in their willingness and desire to differences in discourse patterns that may be culturally
interact with others. It is important to note, however, that based. For example, Italian mothers used more utterances,
most of this communication took a nonverbal form, and spoke more quickly, and used more diverse vocabulary than
these findings thus converge with those of Paul and did Canadian mothers, and their children mirrored this
Ellwood (1991), who observed a similar number of pattern. Mothers in both language groups had interactive
nonverbal initiation produced by late-talking and typically styles with similar responsiveness with respect to using
developing children. responsive labels and expansions, although the Italian
Paul et al. (1991) also concluded that late-talking mothers used fewer imitations and interpretations of their
children and their peers were comparable on the number of children’s utterances. Clearly, more research is needed to
initiations they produced, although the late-talking group make cross-cultural comparisons across different language
had poorer socialization skills, which included such groups. Any existing differences would have significant
nonverbal items as playing social games, imitating complex implications for clinical practice with late-talking children

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay 121


due to the increasingly multi-cultural and multilingual However, it should be noted that the statistics of the
modern society. recovery varied depending on the language measure chosen
Although the findings of the research summarized above to determine progress. In Paul (1993), 34%–40% of
are mixed, several conclusions may be made. First, late children were still delayed on the Developmental Sentence
talkers’ interactive style seems to be similar to their peers Score (Lee, 1974), though they had attained age-appropriate
in many ways, and principally in the nonverbal domain. mean length of utterances (MLUs) and receptive and
Second, the interactive style of late talkers’ mothers may expressive language. Rescorla and Schwartz (1990) sug-
have some implications for interventions involving parents, gested that late talkers show their disability in delayed
such as, for example, to reduce the nonresponsiveness of vocabulary acquisition in their earlier years but may reach
parental speech. However, additional research is required in normal levels of lexical development long before attaining
order to provide enough evidence to clarify these areas and age-appropriate syntactic or morphological skills. Some
to be able to draw implications for intervention with children’s difficulties with syntactic development may be
greater certainty. manifested in their tendency to construct long phrases that
consist of word combinations that are lacking in grammatic
morphemes (Rescorla & Schwartz, 1990). Rescorla,
Dahlsgaard, and Roberts (2000) stated that late-talking
SPONTANEOUS RECOVERY
children improve in vocabulary acquisition in their third
Another controversial area in the research on early lan- year of life and may attain a lexicon similar to that of their
guage delay concerns the statistics regarding the spontane- typically developing peers by age 4. As children with early
ous recovery of these children. It is known that some late- language delay age, their single-word vocabulary increases,
talking children will continue to experience language although development of syntax does not appear to progress
impairment or learning disability later in their lives (Rescorla at the same rate. These late-talking children improve their
et al., 1997), whereas others will not. Children who continue vocabulary from age 2 to 3; however, they still show
to experience problems are referred to as “children with grammatic delays in the preschool years. The most recent
persisting language delay” by Rescorla et al. (1997) and study, Dale et al. (2003), showed that 40% of 4-year-olds
Rescorla et al. (2001). Children displaying a transient maintained this delay.
language delay and whose language difficulties resolve by In a study reported by Whitehurst, Arnold, et al. (1991),
school age are referred to as “late bloomers” (Paul & 96% of late talkers recovered by age 5;6. However, other
Alforde, 1993; Thal, Tobias, & Morrison, 1991). Consider- studies reported the percentage of children who continue to
able attention has been devoted to this difference because it have persisting language delays at age 5 to be as high as
has important implications for the issue of the necessity of one quarter to one half of all children with the diagnosis of
early intervention, as will be discussed later in the review. early language delay (Leonard, 1998; Rescorla, Mirak, &
The statistics of spontaneous recovery that are provided Singh, 2000). It should be noted that children who continue
in the literature vary as a result of differences in defini- to exhibit delays by age 5 are likely to continue to have
tions and inclusion criteria as noted above, as well as language and learning problems for many years (Rescorla,
differences in the measures used by the researchers to Mirak, & Singh, 2000). Nevertheless, it appears that most
assess outcome data. Moreover, there is a strong view in children who have recovered by roughly 3 years of age will
the literature that the proportion of children who recover have roughly normal vocabulary skills by 5 or 6 years of
spontaneously also varies with age. Thus, the concerns for age (Paul, 1996; Rescorla & Lee, 1999).
younger children’s disability are suggested to have less Unfortunately, then, the findings with respect to recovery
weight than those for older children. Therefore, a child who of a substantial proportion of children with early language
passes the age of 30 months and is still not talking delay may not indicate that the child’s disorder has been
engenders more concern than that accorded to a 24-month- resolved by an early age. There is evidence that these
old child with similar difficulties (Rescorla et al., 1997). children may perform in the normal range at some point in
It has been established that 2-year-old late talkers may their development, which would be taken for recovery.
be at risk for continuing expressive language delay up to Nevertheless, their skills may still be generally lower than
and beyond age 3 (Rescorla et al., 1997). Of 2-year-old late those of their peers without a history of language disorder
talkers, Rescorla and Schwartz (1990) found only 50% of (Paul, 2000; Rescorla, 2002; Thal & Katich, 1996). Paul
children in their study to have recovered by the age of 3 (1996) found that late talkers perform normally in school
years. Whitehurst et al. (1991) found 88% of children in on most language measures but may nevertheless have
their study to have recovered by 3;6. In Thal and Tobias lower scores than their peers. Furthermore, by the age of
(1992), 60% of 18–32-month-old children appeared to have 13, the reading skills of many children who have previ-
caught up in 1 year. Dale et al. (2003) found that 44% of ously been diagnosed with an early language delay may fall
children in their study had persistent language delay at 3 into the lower end of the normal range while still falling
years of age. It should be noted, however, that in the significantly lower than those of their peers (Rescorla,
studies reporting higher rates of recovery, the sample sizes 2002). Similarly, Law et al. (2000) reported reading
were smaller. In all of the above studies, recovery was problems persisting at the age of 8 years for 41%–75% of
assessed by evaluating performance on vocabulary tests. children in their study.
The proportion of children who are delayed in their In summary, at present, it is not clear what should be
vocabulary acquisition appears to decrease with age. deemed to constitute “recovery” if, as described above,

122 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION S CIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 34 • 118–133 • Fall 2007
children improve in their vocabulary scores but not in other In addition, consistent with the perspective of many other
aspects of their linguistic competence. It is probable that researchers (Rescorla & Schwartz, 1990; Rice, Hadley, &
vocabulary tests may not be the most appropriate measures Wexler, 1998), Bishop, Price, Dale, and Plomin (2003) and
to assess the development of older children with a history Lyytinen, Poikkeus, Laakso, Eklund, and Lyytinen (2001)
of language disorders such as early language delay. Two suggested that a late-talking 2-year-old with no accompany-
important conclusions that can be drawn from the research ing internal or environmental risk factors should not be an
are that the older the children with persisting language immediate concern for speech-language pathologist atten-
problems are, the more attention they should be given, and tion, but if the child has a family history of the disorder,
second, recovery is a relative term, and children need to be there is a greater likelihood of persisting difficulties.
monitored throughout their school-age years when initial Furthermore, Paul (1993) suggested that females had a
concerns with vocabulary, phonology, and syntax may give lower probability of moving into the normal range in
way to concerns with literacy and overall academic syntactic development following a diagnosis of a late
performance. talker, whereas Rescorla et al. (1997) concluded that gender
was not a predictor. As for the age of diagnosis, Paul
Predictors of Spontaneous Recovery (1993) suggested that the older children are at the time of
diagnosis, the less positive the prospect of recovery. This
Predictors of spontaneous recovery are one of the most finding is supported by Rescorla and Schwartz (1990).
controversial issues in the literature on early language Another group of researchers (Casby, 2001; Lonigan,
delay. The predictors of spontaneous recovery that have Fischel, Whitehurst, Arnold, & Valdez-Menchaca, 1992;
received the most attention in the literature include the Shriberg, Friel-Patti, Flipsen, & Brown, 2000) hypothesized
severity of the initial language impairment, the degree of that a subgroup of late talkers may have this diagnosis
impairment in gestural communication, consonant reper- subsequent to early and persistent middle ear infections, but
toire, family history, gender, age of diagnosis, and a history not all researchers concur with this hypothesis (Paul et al.,
of presenting with otitis media (Dale et al., 2003; Kelly, 1991).
1998; Rescorla et al., 1997). The conflicting findings
regarding each of these predictors are discussed below.
The first category of predictors of eventual recovery
includes intake language measures. Capirci, Iverson, INTERVENTION
Pizzuto, and Volterra (1996) and Thal and Tobias (1992)
suggested that the lack of sequenced gestures can predict As a result of the lack of clear predictors of spontaneous
poorer outcomes of early language delay. Thal and Tobias recovery, as well as the uncertainty with respect to how
suggested that reduced receptive language and gesture different the communicative environments of late-talking
production were both predictive of persistent language children are from those of their peers, there remains
delay; however, neither vocabulary at intake nor MLU were considerable controversy with respect to the appropriate
significant predictors. Ellis Weismer, Murray-Branch, and clinical management of late talkers (e.g., Olswang & Bain,
Miller (1994) agreed that the latter two were not good 1996; Paul, 1996, 1997; Whitehurst & Fischel, 1994). It is
predictors of outcome, but questioned the use of receptive still debated whether late-talking children should be
language as a predictor because in their study, the child provided with treatment once a possible language delay
with the poorest outcome manifested the largest vocabulary becomes apparent, and, if so, which components of their
and the best receptive language at intake. Olswang and language delay should be treated, using which approaches
Bain (1996) share this viewpoint. to language intervention, and in which settings. The studies
Pharr, Bernstein Ratner, and Rescorla (2000) suggested below summarize the research findings pertaining to each
that vocabulary at the age of 3 can be predicted by earlier of these questions.
vocabulary abilities, and Rescorla and Schwartz (1990)
established that the magnitude of the expressive language To Intervene or Not To Intervene
delay rather than an absolute language level at intake was a
predictor of continued language delay. That is, a child who It has been generally agreed that, if a child is diagnosed
has a vocabulary of 30 words at the age of 2 years has a with specific language impairment at the age of 4 years or
higher chance of recovery than does a 3-year-old child with older, he or she should be provided with treatment.
the same vocabulary size because relative to the expecta- However, an important question remains regarding what
tions for the age of 3, this vocabulary size is substantially should be done with younger children.
smaller. It has also been suggested that, in addition to Paul (1996) advocated for a “watch and see” approach,
vocabulary size, vocabulary composition may be an in which identified children’s development is monitored
important predictor. For example, Rice and Bode (1993) closely instead of direct intervention being provided. In her
suggested that poor outcomes are likely for children who study, intervention was administered only if the child’s
have a small number of verbs in their receptive vocabulary. condition worsened over time. Under this approach,
As for the consonant repertoire, a low proportion of children were evaluated at different intervals depending on
consonants to vowels and a small consonant inventory in their age: every 3 to 6 months for 2-year-olds and subse-
the children’s babble are reported to be predictors by Mirak quently every 6 to 12 months until the age of 5. Paul
and Rescorla (1998) and Pharr et al. (2000). (1997) argued that clinical intervention comes at a high

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay 123


cost in both financial and emotional terms, especially children more proficient in certain linguistic behaviors at a
because, she claims, the majority of children attain age- certain point in time. Paul et al. (1991) emphasized the
appropriate norms by preschool age without any interven- need to improve socialization skills. Rescorla, Mirak, and
tion. Similarly, Dale et al. (2003) considered it inefficient Singh (2000) suggested that intervention involving labeling
to target young children because a high proportion of them can help to initiate the acquisition of new words. Rescorla
will eventually achieve normal language without treatment. et al. (2001) suggested that, for a late-talking child, it is
Others, including van Kleeck, Gillam, and Davis (1997), difficult to learn to formulate questions.
advocated for early intervention because employing a watch In terms of optimizing the children’s communicative
and see approach requires predicting with certainty which environment, Whitehurst et al. (1988) proposed that
children will improve spontaneously versus those for whom caregivers of late-talking children become less directive and
the language delay will persist. This may not be feasible reduce their use of noncontingent labeling. Similarly, Paul
because, as mentioned above, no concrete evidence with and Ellwood (1991) emphasized optimizing the children’s
respect to the predictors of spontaneous recovery is communicative environment through the use of expansions
currently available. and imitations. Although these authors also found that the
Moreover, waiting until a child is old enough to rule out speech of late talkers and their mothers had a greater MLU
the possibility of a spontaneous recovery may deprive him gap than that of typically developing children and mothers,
or her of intervention at a time when it might be the most they did not consider this finding to merit equal clinical
effective (Rescorla, 1989). Unfortunately, the age when importance.
intervention is most effective has not yet been determined, These findings raise the question of whether or not it is
and is yet another facet of the problem of when and how possible for the caregivers of late-talking children to alter
to intervene, and whether or not it is necessary. Further- their interactional style. It may not, in fact, be an easily
more, children who do recover do not necessarily attain the achieved goal if, as suggested by Whitehurst et al. (1988),
desired age norms in all aspects of the linguistic compe- the parents’ directive interactive style is a maladaptive
tence, as described above. For example, in Rescorla et al. strategy of relying on children’s receptive rather than
(1997), children with no expressive speech at 2 years of expressive abilities. To date, only one study has addressed
age performed significantly lower than their peers up to 8 this question. In Pearce et al. (1996), 16 mothers and their
years of age in almost every area of expressive language, children with early language delay were randomly assigned
even though for these children, factors such as family to treatment and control groups. The treatment adhered to a
status, receptive language, and nonverbal ability warranted focused stimulation approach, which will be discussed in
no concern (Rescorla et al., 1997). more detail below. Pearce et al. found that, in the course of
Another important argument in support of early interven- intervention, mothers could learn to use shorter, less
tion is that, even though the child’s language skills may complex utterances; repeat target words more often; and
eventually improve, in the meantime, other skills that become less directive with their children. However, the
depend on language proficiency may be delayed as a mothers did not become more responsive. The authors
consequence. For example, skills central to literacy, such as provided two competing explanations for this finding. First,
metalinguistic and phonological awareness, depend on late talkers may not provide enough opportunities to
vocabulary knowledge, as do social skills, self-esteem, and increase contingent input due to the paucity of their talk.
psychosocial attitudes (Adams, 1990). These skills may be Alternately, mothers could have already reached an optimal
negatively affected by the watch and see approach. level of responsiveness before the intervention. The
Furthermore, social and language deficiencies may objective of reducing mothers’ talkativeness was also not
interact with each other because a child with low communi- achieved, possibly due to the fact that the children used
cative abilities may be rejected by peers and may therefore fewer interpretable verbal social utterances and the mothers
have fewer opportunities to practice their language skills, needed to maintain a certain level of child-directed talk to
as suggested by Rice (1993). Paul and Shiffer (1991) continue the social exchange.
argued that late talkers’ utterances may not be sufficiently
intelligible, presumably due to lack of practice, which Intervention Approaches
decreases their chance of receiving parent and peer
reinforcement in response to their production. A scarcity of Different approaches have been proposed with respect to
reinforcement may in turn decrease these children’s the types of intervention to be used with late-talking
motivation to practice, resulting in slow improvement of toddlers, including clinical versus home based. We will first
their speech intelligibility. review the techniques used with late talkers in clinical
settings and then turn to the extensions of these methods to
Intervention Targets other models.
Clinical methods. Some methods assumed to be success-
Researchers have proposed various domains of focus of ful in clinical settings are used frequently; however, there
early intervention with late-talking children. Olswang and are no experimental studies demonstrating their efficacy.
Bain (1996) suggested targeting single words, word Among such techniques are milieu teaching, joint action
combinations, early grammatic morphemes, and simple routines, and inductive teaching (Olswang & Bain, 1996).
syntactic constructions. They stated, however, that interven- Milieu teaching involves shaping behavior by arranging the
tion would not cure the disorder, but rather would make the setting to elicit certain communication behaviors (Hart &

124 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION S CIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 34 • 118–133 • Fall 2007
Rogers-Warren, 1978). Joint attention routines involve an of twenty-one 21–30-month-old late talkers. A script
interaction unified by a theme or goal, following a logical encouraging communicative attempts was used, which
sequence, and in which each participant plays a role from included scaffolding by adults. Children’s speech improved
which a certain response is expected (Snyder-McLean, in complexity and verbal output in the intervention group
Salomonson, McLean, & Sack, 1984). Finally, during but not in the control group. Children’s socialization scores
inductive teaching, input is structured during meaningful improved as well, whereas parent stress scores decreased,
interactions (Connell, 1989), which involves creating a even though this was not specifically targeted.
pattern of stimuli to highlight specific linguistic features Caregiver-administered intervention. Taking into account
for a child. various arguments against formally providing early inter-
Other approaches to language intervention in clinical vention for late-talking toddlers, it has been proposed that
settings have been documented in experimental studies. the techniques described above for use by clinicians may
Ellis Weismer, Murray-Branch, and Miller (1993) compared also be adopted effectively by parents or educators as a
the effectiveness of the techniques used in milieu teaching preventative measure for toddlers who are at risk for
(modeling plus evoked production) in combination with language delays. This would increase the cost effectiveness
modeling alone, as administered by clinicians, to a group and naturalness of early intervention. For example,
of 3 children. One of 3 children participating in the study Whitehurst, Fischel, et al. (1991) investigated the effective-
responded to the first type of treatment (that is, had overall ness of intervention derived from milieu teaching (Warren
improvements in vocabulary use and language development & Keiser, 1986) but implemented by parents in a home
after intervention by 3 years of age), but another child setting. The home setting for intervention was chosen
responded better to the second type of treatment. The third because in such environments, the intervention is constant,
child did not respond differentially to either condition. takes advantage of the child’s natural interests, and
Results of the study seem to indicate that modeling and minimizes conflicts between the goals and the child’s
evoked production may have similar effectiveness; however, everyday environment. Whitehurst et al. concluded that
the findings are inconclusive. Thus, children may require their intervention was effective based on the finding that
different approaches to treatment based on their individual children increased their production of specified targets from
learning styles. However, the sample size of this study was 8% to 50% during mealtime interactions. However,
too small to make any definite conclusions. As this study Girolametto and colleagues (1996b) criticized this study for
used a single-subject design, more studies are needed in its quasi-experimental design, the lack of randomization,
order to assess the relative effectiveness of the various and the lack of a control group, and therefore suggested
treatment options, employing a group design with random- cautious interpretation of its findings.
ized assignment of the participants to groups. The studies of Girolametto’s group have used stronger
The final clinically based study to be discussed is that of experimental design in using focused stimulation. In a pilot
Robertson and Weismer (1999). These researchers employed study (Girolametto et al., 1996a), 16 late-talking children
an interactive model of language intervention, a model that and their mothers were randomly assigned to a treatment
has been commonly used with children with language delay group and a delayed treatment group. Following the
since the early 1980s (Girolametto, Weitzman, Wiigs, & focused stimulation intervention, which was provided by
Pearce, 1999). It focuses on training parents to use natu- the mothers in the course of 11 weeks, children in the
rally occurring strategies that have been shown to be experimental group started to use more target words and
associated with language gains in typically developing learned more symbolic play gestures, although their overall
children (Girolametto, Pearce, & Weitzman, 1996a, 1996b). vocabulary size was not affected. In a subsequent study,
There are two types of interactive language intervention: Girolametto et al. (1996b) employed a similar design with
focused stimulation and general stimulation. Although both some important adjustments such as recruiting 9 more
versions of the approach train parents to provide contingent children and using more stringent measures of vocabulary
responsive input to child’s talk, in focused stimulation, and language development including parent report, direct
parents use frequent presentations of the preselected targets observations, and semistructured probes. This study
that have been individually chosen for each child, taking examined mothers’ use of language modeling strategies
into consideration his or her phonetic and lexical repertoire (e.g., talkativeness, linguistic complexity, and labeling) in
(Girolametto et al., 1996b). These are economical ap- addition to documenting children’s outcomes. Results
proaches that require few centralized resources and include showed that, following treatment, the mothers’ input
three main techniques: fostering joint activity around the decreased in complexity and speed as compared to mothers’
child’s interests, promoting interactions, and language input in the control group. Children in the experimental
modeling (Girolametto et al., 1996a, 1996b). The use of group used more target words in probes and in free-play
this type of intervention may be warranted given the interactions, had larger vocabulary sizes overall, and used
assumption of Paul and Shiffer (1991) that late-talking more multiword utterances and early morphemes than those
children have less of a developed drive for interaction for in the control group, which could be attributed directly to
its own sake, preventing them from interacting for the sake the effects of the intervention.
of interaction and hence practicing initiating conversations. With respect to the question of what exactly is the
Robertson and Weismer (1999) employed general component of focused stimulation that seems to have
stimulation emphasizing vocabulary development and the facilitatory effects on language acquisition, Girolametto et
use of two- to three-word combinations in a social context al. (1999) tested the validity of both responsivity and

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay 125


structural hypotheses. According to the responsivity Socialization gains and the reduction of parent stress and
hypothesis, it is the semantic contingency of the input that anxiety are also evident (Robertson & Weismer, 1999), as
is facilitative to language acquisition; according to the are long-term gains in expressive grammar and vocabulary
structural hypothesis, providing language models within the (Girolametto et al., 2001). However, continued monitoring
child’s zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978) is and intervention for higher level linguistic abilities may
important. The results of the study provide support for the still be necessary as the children mature (Girolametto et al.,
responsivity hypothesis and, more importantly, demonstrate 2001). Therefore, it may be important to emphasize early
a direct link between children’s language gains at posttest literacy in addition to learning vocabulary and improving
and maternal language variables (such as imitations and socialization skills in the course of early intervention.
expansions), which have previously been shown to be Finally, it is also important to note that the participants in
facilitative with typically developing children (Baker & the studies discussed above were parents of middle-class
Nelson, 1984). Thus, Paul and Ellwood’s (1991) initial socioeconomic status who may have been highly educated
conclusion that providing more contingent feedback may be and more motivated to participate in parent programs than
more important than making structural changes appears to parents from lower socioeconomic status. Research investi-
be supported. gating the efficacy of interventions for families from lower
socioeconomic status and other cultural and linguistics
Long-Term Implications backgrounds is still needed.

The results of the studies discussed above indicate short-


term effectiveness of intervention with late-talking toddlers.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
However, there is not much evidence available with respect
to their long-term effectiveness. A group of studies by The research in the area of early childhood language
Rescorla and Paul (see Appendix) examined the outcomes disorders is important because children with delayed
of cohorts of late talkers. However, their samples were expressive language development have been shown to be
mixed, including children who were not provided interven- affected later in their life in their language, social, emo-
tion in addition to children who were provided various tional, and academic performance, regardless of whether or
types of intervention. The only study that explored the not they have spontaneously recovered. The scope of this
long-term effect of a consistent intervention model was research has grown notably in the past two decades. The
Girolametto, Wiigs, Smyth, Weitzman, and Pearce (2001). research has been quite extensive in some more established
The authors followed up the outcomes of children who areas of the field, such as defining the population of children
were provided with the focused stimulation approach, the with expressive language delay, establishing assessment
short-term effects of which were demonstrated in their measures, determining inclusion criteria and prevalence, and
earlier studies. In this study, parents were trained to determining the statistics and predictors of spontaneous
administer preventive intervention when the children were recovery. Cohort studies examining the outcomes for these
between 2 and 3 years of age. Subsequently, direct treat- children at various ages constitute the bulk of this research.
ment on aspects of phonology and language was provided Although this research has not always led to concurring
to half of the children whose gains in these abilities evidence, or used consistent definitions and measures, it
continued to be slow. As indicated by standardized mea- appears to have provided a wealth of evidence for these
sures, 86% of children in the experimental group reached domains. Additional research may or may not clarify issues
age-appropriate levels in expressive language and grammar. such as how it may be determined which among the late-
However, the children’s abilities remained weaker in talking children are most at risk for persisting language
teacher–child discourse, the use of pragmatic cues for delay, or what outcome measures should be used to indicate
anaphora resolution of ambiguous sentences, and narrative spontaneous recovery. However, it seems that the field may
tasks. These findings are consistent with previous studies benefit more from research in other directions.
that indicated that late-talking children continue to have One direction that the field may take is to examine in
difficulties with higher level linguistic tasks that increase more detail the characteristics of effective interventions
their risk for learning and academic difficulties during the with late talkers. It will be important to follow children
early school years (Kelly, 1998; Paul, 1996; Whitehurst, who have been provided with preventative intervention
Arnold, et al., 1991; Whitehurst, Fischel, et al., 1991). longitudinally to determine their long-term prognoses and
Girolametto and colleagues (2001) concluded that close outcomes. In general, well-designed studies employing
monitoring and intervention in these key areas of weakness group designs with as much randomization as possible,
is necessary for late talkers as they reach school age. larger sample sizes, and clear-cut selection criteria are
In summary, the findings of the intervention studies needed to provide higher quality evidence.
indicate that both clinician- and caregiver-administered It would also be beneficial to generate additional
types of treatment may be effective in achieving the short- evidence to support methods that are currently believed to
term goal of improving the vocabulary and grammar of be clinically effective using well-designed experimental
late-talking children. In addition, as suggested by studies. Their effectiveness should be compared, and
Girolametto et al. (1996b), parent-administered programs existing models should be taken to new contexts, including
may at the very least improve parent–child interaction and home and preschool settings. By exploring the combined
give children practice in using words and structures. effects of intervention that is provided at home, preschool,

126 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION S CIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 34 • 118–133 • Fall 2007
and clinic, the benefits of increasing the amount of Bishop, D. V., Price, T. S., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2003).
intervention and providing it in several contexts for one Outcomes of early language delay: II. Etiology of transient and
child may be established. Finally, given older children’s persistent language difficulties. Journal of Speech, Language,
difficulties with higher level linguistic skills, it is likely to and Hearing Research, 46, 561–575.
be important to include early literacy components in these Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of
intervention programs in addition to targeting vocabulary Child Language, 2, 1–19.
and syntax development. Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V. (1996).
Other areas of the field are less well developed and may Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech.
warrant more attention in future research. Interactions of Journal of Child Language, 23, 645–673.
late talkers and their caregivers in general have not been Casby, M. W. (2001). Otitis media and language development: A
investigated thoroughly. In addition, there is a paucity of meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
research with late talkers who speak languages other than 10, 65–80.
English or come from nonmainstream cultural backgrounds Connell, P. (1989). Facilitating generalization through induction
or low socioeconomic status families. Such research is teaching. In L. McReynolds & J. Spradlin (Eds.), Generalization
important and is urgently needed in the increasingly strategies in the treatment of communication disorders (pp. 44–
multicultural North American society. Because early 62). Philadelphia: B. C. Decker.
language delay has been found to affect children speaking Dale, P. S., Price, T. S, Bishop, D. V., & Plomin, R. (2003).
first languages other than English, clinicians will find a Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and
great number of such children among their clients. Inter- transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. Journal of
vention with these children may be challenging because Speech and Hearing Research, 46, 544–560.
many important questions remain unanswered. For example, Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary
it is still not clear whether intervention should be carried Test—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: AGS.
out in the language in which the child is more proficient or Ellis Weismer, S., Murray-Branch, J., & Miller, J. F. (1993).
in the majority language only (for review, see Gutierrez- Comparison of two methods for promoting productive vocabu-
Clellen, 1999). Moreover, cultural differences may be lary in late talkers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
present in the conversational patterns of bilingual late 36, 1037–1050.
talkers and their caregivers (van Kleeck, 1994). Therefore, Ellis Weismer, S., Murray-Branch, J., & Miller, J. F. (1994). A
the interactive language stimulation approaches that are prospective longitudinal study of language development in late
used in clinical and home settings, as well as other clinical talkers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1037–1062
methods found to be efficacious for monolingual English- Fenson, L., Reznick, S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J.,
speaking late talkers, may not be viable for children from Pethnick, S., & Reilly, J. (1993). The MacArthur Communica-
other cultural backgrounds. Furthermore, more research is tive Developmental Inventories. San Diego, CA: Singular.
needed with children from low socioeconomic status Gardner, M. F. (1981). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary
families because, unfortunately, language difficulties often Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.
go hand in hand with poverty (Paul, 2001). The interven- Girolametto, L., Bonifacio, S., Visini, C., Weitzman, E.,
tion models that benefit these children may or may not Zocconi, E., & Pearce, P. S. (2002). Mother–child interactions
prove to be different from those that have been demon- in Canada and Italy: Linguistic responsiveness to late-talking
strated to be effective with their monolingual peers and toddlers. International Journal of Language and Communication
children from families with higher socioeconomic status. Disorders, 37, 153–171.
As illustrated through this review, there continue to be Girolametto, L., Pearce, P. S., & Weitzman, E. (1996a). The
many issues surrounding research involving children with effects of focused stimulation for promoting vocabulary in
early language delay. Areas of inconsistency include young children with delays: A pilot study. Journal of Children’s
differences in terminology, differences in definitions and Communication Development, 17, 3–9.
criteria for inclusion, questions regarding eventual outcomes Girolametto, L., Pearce, P. S., & Weitzman, E. (1996b).
for these children, and essential characteristics of therapy Interactive focused stimulation for toddlers with expressive
approaches. The future research directions outlined above vocabulary delays. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39,
may assist in answering some of these unresolved questions 1274–1283.
and in creating appropriate and effective intervention Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P. S.
models that will permit these children to develop to the (1999). The relationship between maternal language measures
best of their potential. and language development. American Journal of Speech-
Language Pathology, 8, 364–374.
Girolametto, L., Wiigs, M., Smyth, R., Weitzman, E., & Pearce,
P. S. (2001). Children with a history of expressive language
REFERENCES delay: Outcomes at 5 years of age. American Journal of Speech-
Language Pathology, 10, 358–369.
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F. (1999). Language choice in intervention
about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. with bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language
Baker, N., & Nelson, K. E. (1984). Recasting and related Pathology, 8, 291–302.
conversational techniques for triggering syntactic advances by Hart, B., & Rogers-Warren, A. (1978). A milieu approach to
young children. First Language, 5, 3–22. teaching language. In R. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Language

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay 127


intervention strategies (pp. 193–235). Baltimore: University Park Paul, R. (2001). Language disorders from infancy through
Press. adolescence: Assessment and intervention (2nd ed.). Toronto,
Hendrick, D. L., Prather, E. M., & Tobin, A. R. (1984). Canada: Mosby.
Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development Revised Paul, R., & Alforde, S. (1993). Grammatical morpheme acquisi-
Edition. Seattle: University of Washington Press. tion in 4-year-olds with normal, impaired, and late-developing
Hrncir, E., Goldfarb, A., Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1985). language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36,
Report of a screening assessment and treatment programme for 1271–1275.
high-risk two-year-olds in Bermuda. In W. M. Frankenberg & G. Paul, R., & Ellwood, T. (1991). Maternal linguistic input to
Emde (Eds.), Early identification of children at risk: An toddlers with slow expressive language development. Journal of
international perspective (pp. 289–302). New York: Plenum Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 982–988.
Press.
Paul, R., & Fountain, R.(1999). Predicting outcomes of early
Kelly, D. J. (1998). A clinical synthesis of the “late-talker” expressive language delay. Infant–Toddler Intervention: The
literature: Implications for service delivery. Language, Speech, Transdisciplinary Journal, 9, 123–135.
and Hearing Services in Schools, 29, 76–84.
Paul R., & Kellogg, L. (1997). Temperament in late talkers.
Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2000). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 803–811.
Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language
Paul, R., Hernandez, R., Taylor, L., & Johnson, K. (1996).
delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature.
Narrative development in late talkers: Early school age. Journal
International Journal of Language and Communication Disor-
of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1295–1303.
ders, 35, 165–168.
Lee, L. L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis: A grammati- Paul, R., Murray, C., Clancy, K., & Andrews, D. (1997).
cal assessment procedure for speech and language clinicians. Reading and metaphonological outcomes in late talkers. Journal
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1037–1047.
Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Leonard, L. (1989). Language learnability and specific language Paul, R., & Shiffer, M. E. (1991). Communicative initiations in
impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179–202. normal and late-talking toddlers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12,
419–431.
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Paul, R., & Smith, R. L. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds
with normal, impaired, and late-developing language. Journal of
Lonigan, C. J., Fischel, J. E., Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S.,
Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 592–598.
& Valdez-Menchaca, M. C. (1992). The role of otitis media in
the development of expressive language disorder. Developmental Paul, R., Spangle- Looney, S., & Dahm, P. S. (1991). Communi-
Psychology, 28, 430–440. cation and socialization skills at ages 2 and 3 in “late-talking”
young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34,
Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A., Laakso, M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen,
858–865.
H. (2001). Language development and symbolic play in children
with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Pearce, P. S., Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (1996). The
Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 873–885. effects of focused stimulation on mothers of late-talking
toddlers. Journal of Infant–Toddler Intervention, 6, 213–228.
Manhardt, J., & Rescorla, L. (2002). Oral narrative skills of late
talkers at ages 8 and 9. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 1–21. Pharr, A., Bernstein Ratner, N., & Rescorla, L. (2000). Syllable
structure development of toddlers with expressive specific
Miller, J. (1991). Research on language disorders in children: A
language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 429–449.
progress report. In J. Miner (Ed.), Research on child language
disorders: A decade of progress (pp. 3–22). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A
screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of
Mirak, J., & Rescorla, L. (1998). Phonetic skills and vocabulary
size in late talkers: Concurrent and predictive relationships. Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587–599.
Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 1–17. Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in
Olswang, L. B., & Bain, B. (1996). Assessment information for late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
predicting upcoming change in language production. Journal of Research, 45, 360–371.
Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 414–423. Rescorla, L. (2005). Language and reading outcomes of late-
Paul, R. (1991). Profiles of toddlers with slow expressive language talking toddlers at age 13. Journal of Speech, Language, and
development. Topics in Language Disorders, 11, 1–13. Hearing Research, 48, 1–14.
Paul, R. (1993). Patterns of language development in late talkers. Rescorla, L., Bascome, A., Lampard, J., & Feeny, N. (2001).
Journal of Children’s Communication Development, 15, 7–14. Conversational patterns in late talkers at age 3. Applied
Psycholinguistics, 22, 235–251.
Paul, R. (1996). Clinical implications of the natural history of
slow expressive language development. American Journal of Rescorla, L., Dahlsgaard, K., & Roberts, J. (2000). Late-talking
Speech-Language Pathology, 5, 5–21. toddlers: MLU and IPSyn outcomes at 3;0 and 4;0. Journal of
Child Language, 27, 643–664.
Paul, R. (1997). Understanding language delay: A response to van
Kleeck, Gillam, & Davis. American Journal of Speech-Language Rescorla, L., & Fechnay, T. (1996). Mother–child synchrony and
Pathology, 6(2), 40–49. communicative reciprocity in late-talking toddlers. Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 200–208.
Paul, R. (2000). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language
delay: Ethical implications. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard Rescorla, L., & Lee, E. C. (1999). Language impairments in
(Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, young children. In T. Layton & L. Watson (Eds.), Handbook of
characters, intervention and outcome (pp. 195–209). Hove, UK: early language impairment in children: Volume I: Nature. New
Psychology Press. York: Delmar.

128 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION S CIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 34 • 118–133 • Fall 2007
Rescorla, L., & Merrin, L. (1998). Communicative intent in Thal, D. J., Miller, S., Carlson, J., & Vega, M. M. (2005).
toddlers with specific expressive language impairment (SLI-E). Nonword repetition and language development in 4-year-old
Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 393–414. children with and without a history of early language delay,
Rescorla, L., Mirak, J., & Singh, L. (2000). Vocabulary growth Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48,
in late talkers: Lexical development from 2.0 to 3.0. Journal of 1481–1495.
Child Language, 27, 293–311. Thal, D. J., Reilly, J., Laura Seibert, L., Jeffries, R., & Fenson,
Rescorla, L., & Roberts, J. (2002). Nominal vs. verbal morpheme J. (2004). Language development in children at risk for
use in late talkers at ages 3 and 4. Journal of Speech, Lan- language impairment: Cross-population comparisons. Brain and
guage, and Hearing Research, 45, 1219–1231. Language, 88, 167–179.

Rescorla, L., Roberts, J., & Dahlsgaard, K. (1997). Late-talkers Thal, D. J., & Tobias, S. (1992). Communicative gestures in
at 2: Outcome at age 3. Journal of Speech, Language, and children with delayed onset of oral expressive language use.
Hearing Research, 40, 556–566. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1281–1289.

Rescorla, L., & Schwartz, E. (1990). Outcome of toddlers with Thal, D. J., Tobias, S., & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and
specific expressive language delay. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, gesture in late talkers: A year 1 follow-up. Journal of Speech
393–407. and Hearing Research, 34, 604–612.

Reynell, J. (1977). Reynell Developmental Language Scales. van Kleeck, A. (1994). Potential cultural bias in training parents
Windsor, England: NFER. as conversational partners with their children who have delays in
language development. American Journal of Speech-Language
Rice, M. (1993). “Don’t talk to him, he’s weird.” A social
Pathology, 3, 67–78.
consequences account of language and social interactions. In A.
P. Kaiser & D. B. Gray (Eds.), Communication and language van Kleeck, A., Gillam, R., & Davis, B. (1997). When is watch
intervention issues: Volume 2. Enhancing children’s communica- and see warranted? A response to Paul’s 1996 article, “Clinical
tion: Research foundations for intervention (pp. 139–158). implications of the natural history of slow expressive language
Baltimore: Brookes. development.” American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
6(2), 34–39.
Rice, M. L., & Bode, J. V. (1993). Gaps in the verb lexicons of
children with specific language impairment. First Language, 13, Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
113–131. University Press.
Rice, M. L., Hadley, K. R., & Wexler, K. (1998). Family Warren, S. F., & Keiser, A. P. (1986). Incidental language
histories of children who show extended optional infinitives. teaching: A critical review. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, Disorders, 51, 291–299.
419–432. Whitehurst, G., & Fischel, J. (1994). Early developmental
Robertson, S. B., & Weismer, S. E. (1999). Effects of treatment language delay: What, if anything, should the clinician do about
on linguistic and social skills in toddlers with delayed language it? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 613–648.
development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Whitehurst, G., Fischel, J., Lonigan, C., Valdez-Menchaca, M.
Research, 42, 1234–1248. C., Arnold, D., & Smith, M. (1991). Treatment of early
Shriberg, L. D., Friel-Patti, S., Flipsen, P., & Brown, R. L. expressive delay: If, when, and how. Topics in Language
(2000). Otitis media, fluctuant hearing loss, and speech-language Disorders, 11, 55–68.
outcomes: A preliminary structural equation model. Journal of Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Smith, M., Fischel, J. E.,
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 100–120. Lonigan, C. J., & Valdez-Menchaca, M. C. (1991). Family
Silva, P. A. (1980). The prevalence, stability and significance of history in developmental expressive language delay. Journal of
developmental language delay in preschool children. Develop- Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1150–1157.
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 22, 768–777. Whitehurst, G. J., Fischel, J. E., Lonigan, C. J., Valdez-
Snyder-McLean, L., Salomonson, B., McLean, J., & Sack, S. Menchaca, M. C., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Caulfield, M. B.
(1984). Structuring joint attention routines: A strategy for (1988). Verbal interaction in families of normal and expressive-
facilitating communication and language development in the language delayed children. Developmental Psychology, 24,
classroom. Seminars in Speech and Language, 5, 213–228. 690–699.

Thal, D. J., & Flores, M. (2001). Development of sentence Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002).
interpretation strategies by typically developing and late-talking Preschool Language Scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt
toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 28, 173–193. Assessment.

Thal, D. J., & Katich, J. (1996). Predicaments in early identifica-


tion of specific language impairment: Does the early bird always Contact author: Irina Tsybina, Graduate Department of Speech-
catch the worm? In K. N. Cole, P. S. Dale, & D. J. Thal (Eds.), Language Pathology, University of Toronto, 500 University Avenue,
Assessment of communication and language (pp. 1–28). Room 160, Toronto, ON, Canada M5G 1V7. E-mail:
Baltimore: Brookes. irina.tsybina@utoronto.ca.

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay 129


130
APPENDIX. CHRONOLOGICAL SUMMARY OF REVIEWED STUDIES
Author Year Focus Number, age of children Inclusion criteria, instrument, ELD or E/R-LD? Terminology Sample

Rescorla & 1990 Outcomes after 8m 24–31 months at intake (as for Expressive criterion for all studies of this SELD – Philadelphia
Schwartz all studies in this group): 25 author: minimum 6m delay on Reynell specific expressive
boys of the original 40 late (except for 1 child, 5m delay) language delay
talkers (36 boys and 4 girls) Receptive criterion: within 4m of CA on
Reynell, but based on children included in various
follow-ups, the average was sometimes better

CONTEMPORARY ISSUES
LDS: 2 of 25 > 50 words but no combinations

IN
Rescorla & 1996 Communicative 18 boys of the original 40 late Receptive within 3m Late talkers Philadelphia
Fechnay synchrony reciprocity talkers LDS: none > 45 words

Rescorla, 1997 Outcomes at age 3 33 boys and 1 girl of the Receptive within 4m SLI-E Philadelphia
Roberts, & original 40 late talkers LDS: 1 child > 50 words and no combinations
Dahlsgaard

Mirak & 1998 Phonetic skills 33 boys and 4 girls of the original Receptive within 3m Specific expressive
Rescorla and vocabulary size 40 late talkers LDS: 1 of 33 > 50 words but no combinations language impairment Philadelphia

COMMUNICATION S CIENCE
Rescorla & 1998 Communicative intent 29 late-talking boys and 2 girls Receptive within 3m, except for 2 children Delayed expressive

AND
Merrin with 4m delay language, early
expressive delay

Rescorla, 2000 Patterns of vocabulary 28 of 40 late talkers Receptive within 3m Using “late talkers” from Philadelphia
Mirak, & growth from 2–3 years LDS: none > 50 this study on to mean
Singh “children identified under
the age of 3 with expressive
language delay; though some
may have receptive language
delay, language delay should
be primary”

Rescorla, 2000 Outcomes at 3 and 4 33 boys and 1 girl of the original Receptive within 3m Late talkers Philadelphia
Dahlsgaard, & 40 late talkers
Roberts

Rescorla, 2001 Conversational patterns 30 boys and 2 girls of the original Receptive within 3m Late talkers Philadelphia
Bascome, 40 late talkers
Lampard, &

DISORDERS • Volume 34 • 118–133 • Fall 2007


Feeny

Rescorla 2002 Reading at age 9 34 of 40 late talkers Receptive within 3m Late talkers Philadelphia

Manhardt & 2002 Oral narrative 31 of 40 late talkers Receptive within 3m Late talkers Philadelphia
Rescorla

Rescorla 2002 Nominal vs. verbal 38 of 40 Receptive within 3m Late talkers Philadelphia
& Roberts morpheme use
Author Year Focus Number, age of children Inclusion criteria, instrument, ELD or E/R-LD? Terminology Sample

Rescorla 2005 Reading at age 13 28 of 40 late talkers Receptive within 3–4m Late talkers Philadelphia

Paul 1991 Profiles 18–34 at intake, 30 of the original LDS: Fewer than 10 intelligible words produced Late talkers; Slow expressive Portland
37 late-talking children at 18–23, < 50 or no two-word phrases by 24–34 language acquisition
(for all studies in this group, using the same (development) SELD (note
group selected using this criterion at intake) the same acronym standing
71% within 6m on receptive, 29% worse for different terms)

Paul & 1991 Maternal input At 20–34m, 28 of the original Receptive with approximately 3m (Reynell) Slow expressive language Portland
Ellwood group of 37 development

Paul & 1991 Communicative 22 of the original 37 at 24–34 m 1/4 of LT sample had receptive delays Late talkers Portland
Shiffer initiations (Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales)

Paul, 1991 Communication and 21 of the original group of 37, 18–23 m < 10 words produced on LDS, or < 50 Late talkers Portland
Spangle- socialization also at 18–34 months for 24–34m and/or no word combinations
Looney, &
Dahm

Paul 1993 Outcomes at age As intake Late talkers Portland


3, 4, 5

Paul & 1993 Use of grammatic 34 of 37, at age 4 As intake Slow expressive language Portland
Alforde morphemes development
Late-developing language

Paul & 1993 Narrative at age 4 23 of the original group of 37, Below 10th percentile on DSS Slow expressive language Portland
Smith 70% male. 10 of these had moved development
into normal range for DSS
(above 10th percentile)

Paul, 1996 Narrative: early 17 moved into normal (76% boys) Below 10th percentile on DSS History of expressive Portland
Hernandez, school age and 10 (80% boys) were ELD language delay vs. chronic
Taylor, & Reevaluations at at kindergarten, expressive language delay
Johnson kindergarten, first 22 (73%) vs. 8 (75%) at 1st grade
and second grade 24 (79%) vs. 4 (50%) at 2nd grade

Paul, 1997 Reading and meta- 27 with a history of ELD and 5 Below 10th percentile on DSS History of expressive Portland
Murray, phonological skills with chronic language delay vs. chronic
Clancy, & expressive language delay
Andrews

Paul & 1997 Temperament (at age 6) 6 of 28 who remained in the study Below 10th percentile on DSS History of expressive Portland
Kellogg till age 6 were still ELD, plus 22 language delay vs. chronic
with a history of ELD expressive language delay

Paul & 1999 Predicting outcomes at 16% of 37 original who still Below 10th percentile on DSS SELD Portland
Fountain second grade had ELD

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay


131
continued on next page
Author Year Focus Number, age of children Inclusion criteria, instrument, ELD or E/R-LD? Terminology Sample

132
Whitehurst, 1988 Verbal interactions 17 late talkers, 15 boys and 2 girls EOWPVT: 2.5 SD below; PPVT: Developmental expressive New York
Fischel, in families no more than 1 SD below language disorder (ELD, an
Lonigan, acronym that stands for different
Valdez- terms: does not mean
Menchaca, “expressive language delay”
DeBaryshe, & because delay and disorder
Caulfield are different terms)

Whitehurst, 1991 Family history 62 late talkers (85% boys) EOWPVT 2.33 below norms Developmental expressive New York

CONTEMPORARY ISSUES
Arnold, PPVT–R 85 or higher language disorder

IN
Smith, (no more than 1 SD below)
Fischel,
Lonigan, &
Valdez-
Menchaca

Lonigan, 1992 The role of otitis 65 late-talkers EOWPVT 2.33 below norms Developmental expressive New York
Fischel, media in outcomes PPVT–R 85 or higher language disorder
Whitehurst, (no more than 1 SD below)
Arnold, &
Valdez-

COMMUNICATION S CIENCE
Menchaca

AND
Thal, 1991 Language and gestures 19 children, 18–29 m Lowest 10% on Language and Gesture Inventory Children with delayed onset San Diego
Tobias, & 1 year follow-up (both receptive and expressive), late talkers of lexical skills
Morrison Reevaluation of Thal & understood more than language-matched and
Bates 1988, same data, so comparable to age-matched
not really a “follow up”
as the title suggests

Thal & 1992 Gestures of truly late- 10 children, 18–28 m Lowest 10% on the Language and gesture Delayed onset of expressive San Diego
Tobias talkers & late bloomers inventory for expressive, late bloomers age- oral vocabulary, late talkers
appropriate comprehension vs. truly delayed
poorer comprehension at posttest

Thal & 2001 Development of 17 2.5 year old late talkers Below 10th percentile on the MCDI, confirmed Specific expressive language San Diego
Flores sentence interpretation of the original group by Reynell delay

Thal, 2004 Language development 20 late talkers Below 15th percentile on the MCDI Late talkers San Diego
Reilly, in children at risk, at
Seibert, age 3
Jeffries, &
Fenson

DISORDERS • Volume 34 • 118–133 • Fall 2007


Thal, 2005 At 4 years of age, 11 males, 9 females, late talkers Below 10th percentile on the MCDI; originally Delayed at early stages of San Diego
Miller, nonword repetitions 4 of them had receptive below 10th language development
Carlson, & percentile on the MCDI
Vega
Author Year Focus Number, age of children Inclusion criteria, instrument, ELD or E/R-LD? Terminology Sample

Ellis 1993 Two methods of 3 late talkers, 27-28m Expressive: 25–87 words on Early Language Late talkers Wisconsin
Weismer, intervention Inventory (Bates et al., 1986) – now called
Murray- MCDI, would be below 10th; speech samples
Branch, & with SALT for word combinations (had some),
Miller one had normal receptive, one 5m delay and
one 6m delay

Ellis 1994 Longitudinal, profiles – 4 late talkers, from SICD receptive 5m below comparison group, Late talkers Wisconsin
Weismer, main for this group 2 years onwards expressive early language inventory below
Murray- 10th percentile
Branch, &
Miller

Robertson & 1999 Intervention 21 late talkers, Some only expressive, some receptive/expressive; Late talkers, delayed Wisconsin
Weismer 21–30m below 10th percentile on MCDI for expressive language development
–1.25 SD,

Girolametto, 1996b Focused stimulation 25, 23–33m Below 5th on MCDI on expressive, 4 children Toddlers with ELD Toronto
Pearce, & intervention had receptive more than 6m below (SICD) (expressive language delay)
Weitzman

Girolametto, 1996a Focused stimulation 16, 2;0–3;6 Below 5th on MCDI on expressive, 4 children Toddlers with ELD Toronto
Pearce, & intervention had receptive more than 6m below (SICD)
Weitzman

Girolametto, 1999 Maternal language 12, 25–35m Vocabulary size below 5th percentile for their age Toddlers with expressive Toronto
Weitzman, and children’s on MCDI, 4 of 12 children had receptive 6m vocabulary delays
Wiigs, language development below age (SICD)
& Pearce

Girolametto, 2001 Outcomes 21, 3 years after the original Below 5th on MCDI on expressive, 4 children Toddlers with expressive Toronto
Wiigs, Smyth, assessment and interventionhad receptive more than 6 m below (SICD) vocabulary delays
Weitzman, &
Pearce

Note. CA = chronological age, DSS = developmental sentence score (Lee, 1974), EOWPVT = Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (Gardner, 1981), (E/R) LD = (expressive/
receptive) language delay, LDS = Language Development Survey (Rescorla, 1989), LT = Late talker, MCDI = MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (Fenson et al.,
1993), PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn & Dunn, 1981), SALT = Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (Miller, 1983), SELD = specific expressive language delay
(Rescorla’s group) or slow expressive language development (Paul’s group), SD = standard deviation, SICD = Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development (Hendrick, Prather,
& Tobin, 1984), SLI (-E) = specific language impairment (– expressive).

Tsybina • Eriks-Brophy: Issues in Early Language Delay


133

You might also like