You are on page 1of 1

108 N. Carbonneau et al.

/ Body Image 33 (2020) 106–114

regarding their attitudes and behaviors related to body and eating self-compassion, body esteem, and emotional eating), mothers and
(e.g., Elfhag & Linné, 2012). However, it is not yet clear to what daughters only rated themselves, not each other. All measures were
extent these associations persist when daughters reach adulthood. in French. To thank participants for the time spent answering the
Studying dyads of mothers and their adult daughters will increase questionnaire, they were offered the opportunity to take part in a
the understanding of whether mothers have a long-lasting influ- prize draw to win one of 12 cash prizes of $50. Informed consent
ence on their daughter or whether similarities between them are was obtained from all participants. Ethical approval for this study
less evident once daughters become more independent from their was granted by the Research Ethics Committee of the Université du
mother and are most likely exposed to other sources of social influ- Québec in Trois-Rivières.
ence (e.g., romantic partners).
In the present research, we examined whether daughters of 2.2. Measures
self-compassionate mothers were likely to have a more positive
relationship with their body and food. This research proposed 2.2.1. Demographic variables
to test an integrative model, positing that higher levels of self- Participants completed a demographic information section that
compassion in mothers may be associated with lower levels of included questions on age, education, employment, ethnicity, and
daughters’ emotional eating through (1) mothers’ more positive current living arrangement.
body attitude and eating behavior and (2) daughters’ more positive
self- and body-related attitudes. This research investigated both the 2.2.2. Body mass index
intrapersonal and intergenerational associations between mothers’ Mothers and daughters’ BMI (kg/m2 ) were calculated using self-
and daughters’ accepting attitudes toward themselves and their reported height and weight. The average BMI was 26.53 (SD = 4.79)
bodies (i.e., self-compassion and body esteem) and eating behav- for mothers and 24.47 (SD = 5.26) for daughters.
iors (i.e., emotional eating). First, given that mothers’ attitudes
and behaviors related to food and body image are often trans- 2.2.3. Self-compassion
mitted to their daughters (e.g., Arroyo et al., 2017), we expected Mothers and daughters completed the short (12-item) Self-
some degree of concordance between mothers’ and daughters’ lev- Compassion Scale (Raes, Pommier, Neff, & Gucht, 2011). A sample
els of self-compassion, body esteem, and emotional eating. For item is “When I’m going through a very hard time, I give myself
the same reason, we expected that mothers’ characteristics (i.e., the caring and tenderness I need.” Responses were scored on a 5-
self-compassion and body esteem) would better predict daughters’ point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Almost never) to 5 (Almost
emotional eating than the reverse (i.e., daughters’ characteristics always), with higher mean scores indicating more self-compassion.
predicting mothers’ eating). Second, we hypothesized that women Cronbach’s alpha was .85 for mothers and .83 for daughters.
who are compassionate toward themselves would report higher
body esteem, which in turn should be related to less emotional eat- 2.2.4. Body esteem
ing. We expected to find this intrapersonal pattern of associations Mothers and daughters completed the appearance and weight
for both generations of women (i.e., that body esteem would medi- subscales of the Body Esteem Scale (Mendelson et al., 2001). A sam-
ate the relationship between self-compassion and emotional eating ple item is “I like what I see when I look in the mirror.” Responses
for both mothers and daughters). Third, we expected that daugh- were scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Never) to
ters of self-compassionate mothers would report less emotional 5 (Always). All items were averaged into a single score, with higher
eating. Both mothers’ (body esteem, emotional eating) and daugh- mean scores indicating higher body esteem. Cronbach’s alpha was
ters’ (self-compassion, body esteem) characteristics were expected .96 for both mothers and daughters.
to mediate this association. In addition, these associations were
expected to be found while controlling for mothers’ and daughters’ 2.2.5. Emotional eating
BMI. A model illustrating the hypothesized relations is presented Mothers and daughters completed the three-item Emotional
in Fig. 1. Eating subscale from the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ-
R18; de Lauzon et al., 2004; Stunkard & Messick, 1985). A sample
item is “When I feel lonely, I console myself by eating.” Responses
2. Method were scored on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Definitely
false) to 4 (Definitely true), with higher mean scores indicating more
2.1. Participants and procedure emotional eating. Cronbach’s alpha was .89 for mothers and .86 for
daughters.
Participants were 191 dyads of mothers and their daughters
from the province of Quebec, Canada. Participants were recruited 2.3. Statistical analyses
by a professional survey firm. Mean age was 57.37 for mothers
(SD = 7.54 years) and 28.76 for daughters (SD = 6.63 years). Almost Means and standard deviations (see Table 1) as well as corre-
all participants (99 % of mothers and 94.2 % of daughters) identified lations between the model variables (see Table 2) were obtained
as White. Most dyads (73.3 %) were not living together at the time using SPSS software (version 25). To measure the degree of
of the study. Participants were part of a larger study on the social resemblance among dyad members, we used intraclass correlation
factors that influence women’s attitudes and behaviors related to coefficients (ICC; Kashy & Kenny, 2000). An ICC of zero implies that
eating and body image (see also Roberts, Carbonneau, Goodman, two members of the same dyad are no more similar to one another
& Musher-Eizenman, 2020). The complete list of measures can be on a given characteristic than two members of different dyads are.
found on the Open Science Framwork. Even though research usually assumes that the direction of
To take part in the study, women had to be aged between 18 influence should go from mothers to daughters more so than the
and 40 years, or to have a daughter within this age range. Those other way around (e.g., Arroyo et al., 2017), we explicitly tested
who agreed to participate were asked to invite their mother or this assumption by examining whether mothers’ characteristics
daughter, respectively, to take part in the study. Only complete (i.e., self-compassion and body esteem) are more strongly related
dyads were analyzed in the present research. Women interested to their daughters’ eating behavior than daughters’ characteristics
in participating in the study were directed to an online survey to their mothers’ eating behaviors. To do so, we first analyzed the
website that contained the questionnaires. For all measures (i.e., data using the Actor-Partner Interdependence Model (APIM; Kashy

You might also like