You are on page 1of 5

Stuttering: incidence

Annotation
and causes
Neil Gordon MD FRCP HonFRCPCH, Huntlywood, 3 Styal
Road, Wilmslow SK9 4AE, UK.

Correspondence to author at above address.


E-mail: neil-gordon@doctors.org.uk

Definition
Stuttering, or stammering as it is also referred to, is a devel- likely to have lost their stutter than were right-handed males.
opmental speech disorder, which usually appears in chil- All had a family history of stuttering and, at that time, there
dren between the ages of 3 and 8 years. More often than not was no evidence of a decline of the disorder over the previous
it remits before puberty, but it can persist into adult life.1 few decades in spite of an increased number of speech clinics
Stuttering is characterized by involuntary syllable repeti- in the area. In 1984 no decreasing trend was reported.13
tions, syllable prolongations, or interruptions (blocks) in the In a survey of children in junior and senior schools in
smooth flow of speech. There may sometimes be difficulty in Alabama, USA, twice as many black students had speech disor-
differentiating infants who definitely stutter from those who ders, such as stuttering and articulatory problems, as white
show the dysfluency often seen in infancy as a normal stage students,15 and among 793 adults in the UK with significant
of speech development. However, the two are distinct.2 The learning disorders, 6.3% were dysfluent for no definite rea-
overall frequency of dysfluency, the proportion and duration son. These included examples of stuttering, cluttering, and
of dysfluency types, and the associated behaviours not atypical dysfluencies – the first group being the largest.16
directly related to speech such as eye, head, and body move- Another study involved 9930 pupils from 150 schools for
ments, can help to distinguish between the two.3 There has children with hearing impairments. Only 12 of these children
certainly been controversy over the exact definition of stut- were said to stutter: three in the oral mode only, six in manual
tering when selecting participants for research studies.4 communication only with effortless repetition using sign lan-
Wingate5 criticized Yairi and coworkers6–8 for not being guage, and three in both modes. Of those dysfluent in the
strict enough in their selection of affected children and for manual mode, only one child was reported to repeat the first
including those who were classified as having stutter-like syllable and show perseverations and incoordinate manipula-
dysfluencies, especially whole-word repetitions. However, tions. There was also blocking on some signs with difficulty in
Yairi and colleagues9 refuted these claims, stating that continuing through the block. One child had weakness in
although syllable repetitions are the most common compo- motor skills, which caused signs to be jerky and hesitant, and
nent of stuttering, whole-word repetitions do occur in stut- another child was described as beginning a sign and then
tering and therefore this is not a criteria for exclusion. In fact, stopping and repeating it. The findings, although intriguing,
whole-word repetitions seem to be more common when the are open to criticism: those on manual dysfluency were not
onset of stuttering is early in life.10 based on any particular standard and the language level of the
children was not given so that some of the results may have
Incidence of stuttering been within the normal limits of a child in the early stages of
It is usually accepted that the overall incidence of stuttering is language learning, or be due to other neuromotor deficits.17
about 1%, but in the preschool and school populations it is
around 4%, and at all ages seems to be more common among Possible causes of stuttering
males than females. About 80% of those who stutter will out- The exact cause of stuttering remains unproven but it is, no
grow their disability, but there is no way of guaranteeing that doubt, multifactorial. Some of the possible reasons for the
this will happen.11 Young12 concluded that the 80% recovery condition will be considered with particular reference to the
rate is an overestimate as retrospective studies are not likely results of neuroradiological studies. It has long been held
to yield verifiable data. that people who stutter have a greater right cerebral hemi-
According to recent American reports, stuttering affects sphere involvement in speech than do fluent speakers, but
more than 15% of children in the age range of 4 to 6 years, the two conditions cannot be clearly contrasted as stuttering
dropping to 1 to 2% among adults.14 A study of stuttering in a is an intermittent condition and fluent speech occurs in both
university population in Boston, USA, found prevalence to be groups.1 The advent of neuroimaging has meant that more
2.1%. 11 Also, 3.4% of those questioned had previously experi- detailed investigations of this condition can now be carried
enced a problem with stuttering. More males than females out.18 It may well be that the causes are organic, psychologi-
stuttered and right-handed females who stuttered were less cal, and social, but PET studies favour an organic cause with

278 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2002, 44: 278–282


Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Bibiliothèque interuniversitaire de Santé, on 05 May 2020 at 10:28:24, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162201002067
involvement of both the motor and perceptual elements of ference of stimuli from peripheral sense neurons to the cen-
speech. However, psychosomatic aspects of the condition tral analyzing systems, or the very analysis, synthesis, and
must also be considered as they will influence how the patient selection on filtration, are impaired in those who stutter.
copes with the disability.19 Understandably, the anxiety of Executive dysfluency occurs due to the fact that the signals are
people who stutter is mainly restricted to their attitude abnormal and masking reduces such malfunction either by
towards communication situations, for example, having to providing additional encoding activity or by altering the dura-
speak in public.20 tion, frequency, and intensity of the physical qualities of the
perceived stimuli. These findings obviously have consider-
Genetic factors able implications when prescribing treatment. Likewise,
Hereditary factors also appear to contribute to stuttering. To delayed auditory feedback, in which affected individuals hear
investigate this, Drayna and coworkers14 assembled more what they are saying several milliseconds after it has been pro-
than 100 families with multiple cases of persistent stuttering nounced, often prevents stuttering, as does singing. However,
and compared them with families with cases of sporadic stut- delayed auditory feedback causes non-stutterers to become
tering. Males predominated among this latter group, but dysfluent. Zanini and coworkers,24 compared the effect of
among families where persistent stuttering occurred, males delayed auditory feedback in one group of 12 students with
and females were affected more equally. It has been noted the effect of normal auditory feedback in 12 control individu-
that among monozygotic twins there is a 90% chance that if als. Results showed that, in the group with delayed auditory
one twin stutters the other sibling will stutter, whereas there feedback, there was disruption when speaking at a normal
is only a 20% chance of stuttering in dizygotic twins; and there rate. However, as the rate of speaking increased, the number
is frequently a family history of the disorder.11 of errors decreased in this group but increased in the control
To try and analyze the genetic contribution to stuttering, group with normal auditory feedback. This difference was
Ambrose and colleagues21 studied the immediate and extend- more marked when the auditory input was applied to the
ed families of 66 children who stuttered. It was found that right ear. This may be explained by the left hemisphere being
females had a better prognosis. Also, persistence and recov- less resistant to the disruptive effect of the feedback because
ery were both familial, with families tending to express either of its extensive involvement with language function, while
persistent or recovered stuttering. There was statistical evi- the better performance of the right hemisphere, with its many
dence for the multifactorial nature of the disorder contribut- connections with the reticular activating system, may be due
ing to both types, which were unlikely to be genetically to its critical role in attention. These findings stress the role of
independent. The type of stuttering which recovers does not sensory contribution to the causation of stuttering.11
appear to be a milder form of that which persists, and the lat- In their research, Peters and coworkers25 found no sup-
ter appears to be due, in part, to other transmitted genetic fac- port for the claim that those who stutter differed from those
tors, in addition to underlying abnormalities which may make who do not in assembling motor plans for speech, although
a child more at risk of stuttering. physiological data suggest that those who stutter may have
different ways of initiating and controlling speech. It may be
Developmental stuttering that the disability results from a deficiency of speech motor
The complex nature of speech production with the participa- skills, so that they reach the maximum movement rate (i.e.
tion of auditory, motor, and linguistic systems makes it difficult the rate at which dysfluencies begin) at a lower speech rate
to distinguish between causes and consequences, and devel- than those who do not stutter. In choral reading and singing,
opmental stuttering present since childhood may result from which markedly reduce stuttering, movement rate is usually
disturbances in any one of these systems. In a study by slower, less accurate articulation is required, and external
Salmelin and coworkers,22 it was shown that in some affected timing may provide a signal that enhances coordination.
individuals, the functional organization of the auditory cortex
differed from that in normally developing control individuals. PET and other studies
They recorded neuromagnetic responses to monoaural tones Neuroimaging research data and the effectiveness of
in nine participants who stuttered and 10 fluent speakers dopamine receptor antagonists in the treatment of develop-
while the participants read silently with mouth movements mental stuttering, has raised the possibility of a hyper-
only, aloud, or in chorus with another person. In the group dopaminergic origin for the condition, with an overactive
which stuttered there was an altered interhemispheric bal- presynaptic dopamine system in regions of the brain that
ance affected by speech production as a result of changes in modulate verbalization.26
the left auditory cortical representation. This was more severe To distinguish the neural systems of normal speech from
in self-paced than accompanied speech. This could lead to those of stuttering, Fox and colleagues27 used PET combined
transient non-optimal interpretation of the auditory input and with choral reading to induce fluency in order to investigate a
affect speech fluency. It did not occur in fluent speakers. group with stuttering of varying severity. It was found that
Brown and colleagues23 examined the effects of reduced stuttering induced widespread overactivation of the motor
auditory feedback on stuttering. No differences in hearing system in both the cerebrum and cerebellum, with right cere-
thresholds were found between those who stuttered and bral dominance. Stuttered reading lacked left-lateralized acti-
those who did not, but those who stuttered had a lower vations of the auditory system which was thought to support
threshold for auditory discomfort. The study also showed the self-monitoring of speech. There was also selective deacti-
that fluency is inversely related to auditory feedback. Masking vation of a fronto-temporal system implicated in speech
the auditory feedback of the affected individuals’ own voice production. Induced fluency decreased or eliminated overac-
by a sound transmitted through earphones had a dramatic tivity in most motor areas and largely reversed the auditory
effect in improving stuttering.23 This suggests that the trans- system underactivations and the deactivation of the speech

Annotation 279
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Bibiliothèque interuniversitaire de Santé, on 05 May 2020 at 10:28:24, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162201002067
production system. It was concluded that stuttering was a dis- function, compared with control individuals. Also, those who
order affecting the multiple neural systems used for speaking. stutter lack the strong left cerebral dominance for speech gen-
Fox and colleagues carried out a further study28 using PET erally seen among those who do not,18 although the role of
images of brain blood flow. Two groups were studied: 10 right- cerebral dominance in causing this disability is certainly not as
handed males who stuttered and 10 right-handed, age- and strongly supported as it used to be.31 An important study by
sex-matched control individuals who did not. Ninety PET Foundas and colleagues32 using volumetric MRI has shown evi-
blood-flow images were obtained in each group and striking dence that cerebral processes regulating the speech-motor
differences were found in studies of stutter rate and syllable control system are disrupted and although emotional stress
rate. Their findings supported the theories that implicate the will aggravate a stutter, it is not the cause. Their investigations
speech-motor regions of the non-dominant cerebral hemi- on adults showed that many of those who stutter have an
sphere, usually on the right, in those who stutter, and also enlarged planum temporale and less asymmetry of this region,
added evidence that the non-dominant cerebellar hemisphere, which is a component of Wernicke’s area, and abnormal gyral
usually the left, is also affected. The cerebellum seems to have a patterns in the cortex involved with language function, such as
specific role in the fluent utterances of those who stutter and the frontal opercular diagonal sulcus and superior bank of the
who also have auditory processing problems. sylvian fossa. However, there is no specific disruption common
Using PET during new and practice performances of a sim- to all who stutter. It is suggested that developmental stuttering
ple verbal response selection task, such as saying an appro- may result from an instability between an outer linguistic loop
priate verb for a visually presented noun, Raichle and and an inner phonatory loop served by these structures.33
coworkers 18,29 found that different brain circuits were recruit- Perkins and coworkers34 discuss a number of theories,
ed for learned and automatic language processing tasks. which have been put forward to explain the dysfluency. They
Activation of Broca’s area occurred during learned language theorize that two variables, speech disruption and time pres-
processing, but diminished as the task became more auto- sure, are necessary and together are sufficient to account for
matic; and this occurred rapidly even after short periods of stuttered and non-stuttered dysfluency, if stuttering is defined
practice. Therefore, two distinct circuits can be used for ver- as a disruption of speech that is experienced by the speaker as
bal response selection, and a critical factor in determining the loss of control under time pressure, and, when relatively
circuitry used seems to be the degree to which a task is unaware of the cause of the dysfluency. For example, when in
learned or automatic. It is reasonable to assume that the dif- command of the situation, stuttering is unlikely to occur, and
ference between those who stutter and those who do not is this fits in with the old adage that children who stutter are
that in the former, the circuitry normally activated during speaking faster than they can think. Time pressure to continue
learned language tasks does not function properly, and that an utterance when it is already disrupted is of obvious causal
the disability will be improved by the use of tasks which mini- importance.
mize learned speech production. Acquired stuttering among people over the age of 18 years is
Braun and coworkers,30 using regional cerebral blood most frequently due to neurogenic causes: e.g. vascular lesions
flow measured with H2150 PET, confirmed that during the and trauma affecting areas of the brain, such as the internal and
production of stuttered speech the anterior forebrain external capsules, the thalamus, and the basal ganglia; or the
regions, which play a role in the regulation of motor function, causes are drug related. Such stuttering seems to appear
are disproportionately active. In contrast, the post-rolandic throughout the utterance, and not just at the start, and fails to
regions, which are involved in perception and decoding of respond to typical fluency-evoking manoeuvres. Those affect-
sensory information, are relatively silent. Comparison with ed are seldom disturbed by their disordered speech33and most
control individuals in the same situations suggests mecha- of these patients improve, although few make a complete
nisms by which fluency-evoking manoeuvres may affect the recovery.35
anterior and posterior cerebral areas in different ways and
may facilitate speech production in those who stutter. Conclusions
Activation of the left cerebral hemisphere seems to be related Although it is not entirely clear to what extent the incidence of
to the production of stuttered speech, while activation of the stuttering has changed over the years, there is, today, a better
right may represent compensatory processes linked to the understanding of the condition. Incomplete lateralization of
reduction of the disability. The normal pattern of left-hemi- speech, and other motor functions, is no longer accepted as
sphere dominance for language is not seen in affected indi- the definite cause of stuttering.28 The investigations by Braun
viduals who do not activate the relevant areas or activate them and coworkers 30 and the comparable studies by Fox and col-
bilaterally. Also, the posterior regions may fail to provide the leagues 28 suggest that the right and left cerebral hemispheres
sensory feedback on which the anterior regions depend for seem to play distinct and opposing roles in the generation of
coordination of speech output. Then, activities which stuttering. They indicate that the symptoms are associated with
improve stuttering, such as singing, may reduce demands on activation of anterior forebrain regions almost exclusively in
the frontal areas of the brain and may enhance the sensory the left hemisphere, while both anterior and posterior perisyl-
processing in the posterior areas. vian areas of the right hemisphere are activated as speech
So far, there is little evidence of gross structural abnormali- became more fluent. This suggests that this improvement may
ties of the brain among individuals who stutter, or of consistent possibly be due to coupling of motor and sensory areas within
differences in cerebral blood flow in the resting state between the right hemisphere. This model can integrate a number of
adults who stutter and those who do not. PET has shown that previous theories of stuttering such as hemisphere asymmetry,
during tasks which invoke dysfluent speech, those who stutter disorders of language processing, motor planning or sequenc-
show hypoactivity in cortical areas associated with language ing, and defective auditory feedback.
processing but hyperactivity in areas associated with motor Investigations have been carried out on the prognosis for

280 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2002, 44: 278–282


Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Bibiliothèque interuniversitaire de Santé, on 05 May 2020 at 10:28:24, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162201002067
children who stutter. Yairi and coworkers35 found on a conser- 14. Drayna D, Kilshaw J, Kelly J. (1999) The sex ratio in familial
persistent stuttering. American Journal of Human Genetics
vative estimate that about two-thirds of those who stutter 65: 1473–5.
recover. The risk of chronic stuttering seems to be related to a 15. Gillespie SK, Cooper EB. (1973) Prevalence of speech problems
number of genetic and environmental factors, such as age at in junior and senior high schools. Journal of Speech and
onset, duration of the disorder, family history of persistent Hearing Research 16: 731–8.
16. Stansfield J. (1990) Prevalence of stuttering and cluttering in
and recovered stuttering, and scores on language and non- adults with mental handicaps. Journal of Mental Deficiency
verbal measures. Also, females seem to have a better chance Research 34: 287–307.
of recovery. Research has also shown that linguistic skills do 17. Montgomery BM, Fitch JL. (1988) The prevalence of stuttering in
not differ between children who stutter and those who do the hearing-impaired school-age population. Journal of Speech
not,36 and that a persistent stutter is linked to a child’s articu- and Hearing Disorders 53: 131–5.
18. Sandak R, Fiez JA. (2000) Stuttering: a view from neuroimaging.
latory skills and the mother’s communicative style and lan- Lancet 356: 445–6.
guage complexity.37 When carrying out such research it must 19. Rosanowski F, Hoppe U, Hies Th, Moser M, Pröschel U, Eysholdt
always be noted that especially rapid recovery can occur dur- U. (1998) Auditorische sprachevozierte hirnrindenpotentiale bei
ing the 6 months after the onset of stuttering, and this must be patienten mit stottersyndromen. Laryngo-Rhino-Otology 77:
709–14. (In German).
accounted for when selecting study participants.38 20. Miller S, Watson BC. (1992) The relationship between
Further prospective studies on the incidence of stuttering communication attitude, anxiety, and depression in stutterers
would surely be of interest, and of help in planning services, and non-stutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research
and be of assistance in defining more accurately how urgent it 35: 789–8.
is to start a particular child on treatment. It is not known if the 21. Ambrose NG, Cox NJ, Yairi E. (1997) The genetic basis of
persistence and recovery in stuttering. Journal of Speech,
neurophysiological patterns in adults who stutter are always Language and Hearing Research 40: 567–80.
present in children when they start to stutter. Also, there may 22. Salmelin R, Schnitzler A, Schmitz F, Jäncke L, Witte OW, Freund H-
be other questions about how patterns in children who spon- J. (1998) Functional organization of the auditory cortex is
taneously recover from stuttering differ from those who do different in stutterers and fluent speakers. Neuroreport
9: 2225–9.
not.18 It is possible that these two developmental paths are 23. Brown T, Sambrooks JE, MacCulloch MJ. (1975) Auditory
influenced by genetic factors, especially an association with thresholds and the effect of reduced auditory feedback on
other deficits, which may complicate stuttering and prevent or stuttering. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 51: 297–311.
slow down the recovery process. In addition, phonological 24. Zanini S, Clarici A, Fabbro F, Bava A. (1999) Speaking speed
and language skills may have a role in the recovery and persis- effects on delayed auditory feedback disruption of speech
fluency. Perceptual and Motor Skills 89: 734–52.
tence of stuttering,7 as may environmental factors, such as par- 25. Peters HFM, Hulstijn W, Van Lieshout PHHM. (2000) Recent
ents’ responses to the disability.8 These and other questions developments in speech motor research into stuttering. Folia
may be answered with the use of investigations such as PET. Phoniatrica Logopaedica 52: 103–19.
26. Costa D, Kroll R. (2000) Stuttering: an update for physicians.
Accepted for publication 26th September 2001. Canadian Medical Association Journal 162: 1849–55.
27. Fox PT, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Hirsch TB, Downs JH, Martin C,
References Jerabek P, Glass T, Lancaster JL. (1996) A PET study of the neural
1. Ludlow CL. (2000) Stuttering: dysfunction in a complex and systems of stuttering. Nature 382: 158–62.
dynamic system. Brain 123: 1983–4. 28. Fox PT, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Zamarripa F, Xiong J-H, Lancaster
2. Ambrose NG, Yairi E. (1999) Normative disfluency data for early JL. (2000) Brain correlates of stuttering and syllable production.
childhood stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing A PET performance-correlation and analysis. Brain
Research 42: 895–909. 123: 1985–2004.
3. Zebrowski PM. (1995) The topography of beginning stuttering. 29. Raichle ME, Fiez JA, Videen TO, MacLeod A-MK, Pardo JV, Fox PT,
Journal of Communication Disorders 28: 75–91. Petersen SE. (1994) Practice-related changes in human brain
4. Wingate ME. (1981) Knowing what to look for: comments on functional anatomy during non-motor learning. Cerebral Cortex
stuttering identification, standard definition, and moment of 4: 8–26.
stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 24: 622–3. 32. Foundas AL, Bollich AM, Corey DM, Hurley M, Heilman KM.
5. Wingate ME. (2001) SLD is not stuttering. Journal of Speech, (2001) Anomalous anatomy of speech-language areas in adults
Language and Hearing Research 44: 381–3. with persistent developmental stuttering. Neurology
6. Paden EP, Yairi E, Ambrose NG. (1999) Early childhood stuttering 57: 207–15.
II: Initial status of phonological abilities. Journal of Speech, 33. Rosenfield DB. (2001) Do stutterers have different brains?
Language and Hearing Research 42: 1113–24. Neurology 57: 171–2.
7. Watkins RV, Yairi E, Ambrose NG. (1999) Early childhood 34. Perkins WH, Kent RD, Curlee RF. (1991) A theory of
stuttering III: initial status of expressive language ability. Journal neuropsycholinguistic function in stuttering. Journal of Speech
of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 42: 1125–35. and Hearing Research 34: 734–52.
8. Yairi E, Ambrose NG. (1999) Early childhood stuttering I: 35 Yairi E, Ambrose NG, Paden EP, Throneburg RN. (1996) Predictive
Persistency and recovery rates. Journal of Speech, Language and factors of persistence and recovery: pathways of childhood
Hearing Research 42: 1097–1112. stuttering. Journal of Communication Disorders
9. Yairi E, Watkins R, Ambrose M, Paden E. (2001) What is 29: 51–77.
stuttering? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 36. Kloth S, Janssen P, Kraaimaat F, Brutten GJ. (1998) Child and
44: 585–97. mother variables in the development of stuttering among high-
10. Bloodstein O, Grossman M. (1981) Early stuttering: some risk children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Fluency Disorders
aspects of their form and distribution. Journal of Speech and 23: 217–30.
Hearing Research 24: 208–302. 37. Kloth S, Kraaimaat F, Janssen P, Brutten GT. (1999) Persistence
11. Rosenfield DB. (2000) Stuttering. Current Problems in and remission of incipient stuttering among high-risk children.
Pediatrics 12: 1–27. Journal of Fluency Disorders 24: 253–65.
12. Young MA. (1975) Onset, prevalence, and recovery from 38. Yairi E, Ambrose MG, Niermann. (1993) The early months of
stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders stuttering: a developmental study. Journal of Speech and
40: 49–58. Hearing Research 36: 521–8.
13. Rustin L, Cook F. (1984) The disappearing stammer. British
Medical Journal 288: 235. (Letter).

Annotation 281
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Bibiliothèque interuniversitaire de Santé, on 05 May 2020 at 10:28:24, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162201002067
The Royal Society of Medicine
Feeding pre-term and term babies
Feeding pre-term and term babies
The Royal Society of Medicine
FRIDAY 17 MAY 2002
1 WIMPOLE STREET, LONDON, W1G OAE

9.00 am Registration and coffee


Meetings

Chair: Martin Bax, Imperial College, London

9.25 am Introduction: feeding in babies. Martin Bax, Imperial College, London

9.30 am Physiological markers of abnormal feeding in pre-term infants. Ira Gewolb,


Baltimore, USA

10.15 am Feeding adaptations in respiratory distress. Lorraine Pinnington, University of


Nottingham

10.35 am Psychological aspects of feeding difficulties in infants: mother and child. Gill
Harris, University of Birmingham

11.05 am Coffee

Chair: Lesley Carroll-Few, Chelsea & Westminster, London


Keith

11.35 am Successful breast feeding. Inga Warren, St Mary's Hospital, London

11.55 am Feeding assessment in infancy. Gilly Kennedy, Middlesex Hospital, London

12.25 pm The baby's gut. Peter Sullivan, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford

12.55 pm Lunch

Chair: Richard Morton, Derbyshire Children's Hospital, Derby

2.00 pm Nutrition and development. Carlo Agostini, Italy


Mac

2.40 pm Videofluoroscopy in infants. Annie Bagnall, Hammersmith Hospital, London

3.10 pm Management of difficult feeders. Helen Cockerill, Guy's Hospital, London

3.40 pm Panel Discussion. Annie Bagnall, Gilly Kennedy, Ira Gewolb, Helen Cockerill,
Martin Bax, Lesley Carroll-Few

4.15 pm Close of meeting and tea

This course provides 5 CME/CPD Credits

Registration forms due Tuesday 14th May. For information contact:


Melanie Armitage, Academic Department
The Royal Society of Medicine
1 Wimpole Street, London, W1G 0AE, UK
Tel: +44 (0)20 7290 3934, Fax: +44 (0)20 7290 2989
Email: mackeith@rsm.ac.uk, Website: www.rsm.ac.uk/mackeith

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Bibiliothèque interuniversitaire de Santé, on 05 May 2020 at 10:28:24, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0012162201002067

You might also like